Sei sulla pagina 1di 223

INFORMATION TO USERS

While the most advanced technology has been used to


photograph and reproduce this manuscript, the quality of
the reproduction is heavily dependent upon the quality of
the material submitted. For example:

• Manuscript pages may have indistinct print. In such


cases, the best available copy has been filmed.

• Manuscripts may not always be complete. In such


cases, a note will indicate that it is not possible to
obtain missing pages.

• Copyrighted material may have been removed from


the manuscript. In such cases, a note will indicate the
deletion.

Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, and charts) are


photographed by sectioning the original, beginning at the
upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in
equal sections with small overlaps. Each oversize page is
also film ed as one exp osu re and is a v a ila b le, for an
additional charge, as a standard 35mm slide or as a 17”x 23”
black and white photographic print.

Most photographs reproduce accep tab ly on p o sitiv e


microfilm or microfiche but lack the clarity on xerographic
copies made from the microfilm. For an additional charge,
35mm slides of 6”x 9” black and white photographic prints
are available for any photographs or illustrations that
cannot be reproduced satisfactorily by xerography.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
8713554

Strouse, Lewis Hillard

FROM ANALYSIS TO GESTURE: A COMPREHENSIVE APPROACH TO


SCORE PREPARATION FOR THE CONDUCTOR

Ball State U niversity D.A. 1987

University
Microfilms
International 300 N. Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, Ml 48106

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
PLEASE NOTE:

In allcases thismaterial has been filmed inthe best possible way from the availablecopy.
Problems encountered with thisdocument have been identified herewith a check mark •/ .

1. Glossy photographs or pages____

2. Colored illustrations, paper or print_____

3. Photographs with dark background____

A. Illustrationsare poor copy_____

5. Pages with black marks, not original copy_____

6. Printshows through as there istext on both sides of page_

7. Indistinct, broken or small print on several pages V

8. Printexceeds margin requirements_____

9. Tightiybound copy with print lost inspine _____

10. Computer printout pages with indistinct print_____

11. Page(s)_________ lacking when material received, and notavailable from school or
author.

12. Page(s)_________ seem to be missing in numbering only as textfollows.

13. Two pages numbered . Text follows.

14. Curling and wrinkled pages_____

15. Dissertation contains pages with printat a slant,filmed aj?received________

16. Other_________________________________________________________

University
Microfilms
International

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
FROM ANALYSIS TO GESTURE:

A COMPREHENSIVE APPROACH TO SCORE PREPARATION

FOR THE CONDUCTOR

A DISSERTATION

SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE EDUCATIONAL POLICIES COUNCIL

IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS

for the degree

DOCTOR OF ARTS

by

LEWIS H . STROUSE

ADVISORS: MR. LEONARD ATHERTON, DR. JOSEPH SCAGNOLI

BALL STATE UNIVERSITY

M U N C I E , INDIANA

APRIL, 1987

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
FROM ANALYSIS TO GESTURE:

A COMPREHENSIVE APPROACH TO SCORE PREPARATION

FOR THE CONDUCTOR

A DISSERTATION

SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE EDUCATIONAL POLICIES COUNCIL

IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS

for the degree

DOCTOR OF ARTS

by

LEWIS H. STROUSE

Approved:

Chairperson Date

rp1 son
MaMLL
Date '
»

Committe'e Date

Committee
vnm
Dat/e

BALL STATE UNIVERSITY

MUNCIE, INDIANA

APRIL, 1987

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ABSTRACT

FROM ANALYSIS TO GESTURE:

A COMPREHENSIVE APPROACH TO SCORE PREPARATION

FOR THE CONDUCTOR

A DISSERTATION

SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE EDUCATIONAL POLICIES COUNCIL

IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS

for the degree

DOCTOR OF ARTS

by

LEWIS H. STROUSE

DISSERTATION CHAIRMEN: MR. LEONARD ATHERTON

and DR. JOSEPH SCAGNOLI

BALL STATE UNIVERSITY

MUNCIE, INDIANA

APRIL 1987

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
FROM ANALYSIS TO GESTURE:
A COMPREHENSIVE APPROACH TO SCORE PREPARATION
FOR THE CONDUCTOR

Lewis H. Strouse
D.A.
Ball State University, 1987

Advisors: Mr. Leonard Atherton and Dr. Joseph Scagnoli

The purpose of this study was to develop a

comprehensive and carefully detailed approach to score

preparation which would be adaptable to all Kinds of music.

As a result of reviewing selected approaches to score

preparation and general' musical analysis techniques, a

Comprehensive Approach to Score Preparation (CASP) based

upon three levels of deductive analysis was created. These

levels are described as Level I— Basal Structure Analysis;

Level II— Review of Supplementary Information; and Level

III— Detailed Structural Analysis. The sequence of Levels

was based upon (1) the importance of formulating

appropriate conducting gestures and (2) the most efficient

method of learning an extended musical work.

Level I, Basal Structure Analysis, explores the score

in terms of musical elements that may be translated directly

into gesture without further study of structural

comparisons. Level II, Review of Supplementary Information,

surveys the historical and stylistic background of the work

and the composer. The final stage of the CASP process,

Level III— Detailed Structural Analysis, identifies the

individual components of the musical structure as each

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
relates to the larger structure and to the conductor’s

emotive interpretation.

Conclusions

1. The musical experience effects various modes of

emotional energy in appreciative listeners as well as non-

appreciative listeners, and musical scores should be

interpreted on the basis of emotional energy.

2. The elements of the cond uct or ’s musical

interpretation are transmitted to the players and to the

audience by means of the c o n duc tor ’s personality.

3. The musical elements of articulation, dynamics, and

tempo are the most important determinants of variations in

musical interpretations.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The author wishes to express deepest appreciation to

Mr. Leonard Atherton for his unselfish generosity with time

and knowledge in guiding the development of this study.

Special thanks is due Dr. Joseph Scagnoli who served as

a principle reader of the manuscript, offered keen insight

into the practical application of the material, and endured

endless discussions on musicians and music-making which

served to continually inspire and motivate.

The contributions of committee members Dr. Philip

Jackson, Dr. Eugene Karjala, and Dr. John Strouse are

recognized with sincere and warmest gratitude. Discussions

with these gentlemen always provided honest and constructive

feedback within a professional atmosphere of mutual support.

L.H.S.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Table of Contents

Page

List of E x a m p l e s ................................................ v

List of F i g u r e s ................................................ vi

Chapter

1. Rationale for the S t u d y .............................. 1

The C o m p o s e r ’s Intent.............................. 8

The Psychophysical Effect of M u s i c .............. 10

S u m m a r y .............................................12

2. Survey of Approaches to Score Preparation......... 14

Historical S u r v e y ................................. 19

Application of Theoretical Techniques.......... 20

The C o m p o s e r ’s Creative Process................. 24

3. The Comprehensive Approach To Score


Preparation For The Cond uc tor ....................32

Level I— Basal Structure A na l y s i s ............... 39

Level II— Review of Supplementary


Information...................................... 82

Level III— Detailed Structural A na l y s i s ........ 83

Application of CASP Results:


The C o nd uct or ’s Interpretation................ 94

4. Application of the CASP Analysis Process:


Analysis For Performance Of Darius M il hau d’s
Suite Francaise For Concert B a n d .............. 98

Level I— Basal Structure A n aly si s............... 98

Level II— Review of Supplementary


Information........ -........................... 138

Level III— Detailed Structural A n al ysi s....... 140

ill

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
5. Basal Structure Analysis Of Palestrina's
Four Voice Motet Sicut Cervus and
Deb uss y’s L'Apres-midi d'un f a u n e ............. 148

Sicut C e r v u s ....................... ..............148

L'Apres-midi d'un f a u n e ......................... 157

Appendixes

I. CASP Forms for Data Collection &Retention....... 174

II. Basal Structure Analysis Charts for


Movements II, III, IV, V of Darius
Milhaud’s Suite F r a nc ais e....................... 192

Bibliography.................................................. 197

iv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Examples

Example Page

1. Milhaud. Suite Fran ca ise : I


(mm. 7 2 - 8 3 ) ........................................ 43

2. Milhaud. Suite Francaise: V


(mm. 3 1 - 3 9 ) ........................................ 48

3. Williams. Symphonic Dance No. 5


(mm. 53 -7 5 ) ........................................ 50

4. Milhaud. Suite Fr ancaise: III


(mm. 6 1 - 7 0 ) ........................................ 53

5. Milhaud. Suite Francaise: - V


(mm. 6 6 - 7 4 ) ................................... 56

6. Milhaud. Suite Fran cai se: II


(mm. 4 2 - 5 3 )....................................... 59

7. Milhaud. Suite Francaise: III


(mm. 4 4 - 5 2 )....................................... 61

8. Milhaud. Suite Francaise: IV


(mm. 14-27)....................................... 64

9A. Milhaud. Suite F rancaise: IV


(mm. 14-27)....... 70

98. Milhaud. Suite Francaise: III


(mm. 2 4- 3 3 ) ................. 73

10. Phrasal Outline S c h e m e ...............................88

11. Phrasal Outline Scheme Using T e x t .................. 89

12. Description of Overall D e s i g n ...................... 91

13. Description of Sectional Design


with D et a i l ............. 92

14. Byrd. Mass For Three V o i c e s : C r e d o ................ 92

15. Phrasal Outline with Thematic


Indications...................................... . .93

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Figures

Figure Page

1. Basal Structure Analysis Chart,


Suite Francaise: I. Norm an die ................. 81A

2. Overview Chart of Suite Fran ca ise ................140A

3. Structural Chart No. 1,


Suite Francaise: I. Normandie ................ 140B

4. Basal Structure Analysis Chart,


Suite F rancaise, I. Norm an die ................. 141A

5. 8asal Structure Analysis Chart,.


Sicut C e r v u s .............. 154A

6. Basal Structure Analysis Chart,


L ’Apres-midi d'un f a u n e ........................ 157A

7. Overall Structure Chart,


L ’Apres-midi d ’un f a u n e ........................ 170A

vi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER 1

Rationale for the Study

Considering all aspects of music theory, the processes

of musical expression and interpretation are among the least

understood.! As a result, young musicians lack ready access

to a standard approach for the interpretive analysis of solo

literature and ensemble scores.

Traditionally, the conductor prepares a score using a

series of four steps: (1) researching pertinent information

concerning the compo ser ’s lifestyle and the events

surrounding the genesis of the composition, (2) analyzing

the score theoretically in order to discover the

relationships between large structural features and much

smaller components, (3) as a result of historical research

and score analysis, the conductor forms a personalized

interpretation of the work, which leads to (4) the selection

of gestures that will best communicate the interpretation to

the ensemble and the listener. Unfortunately, the available

literature on conducting does not present an approach to

score preparation of sufficient breadth and detail to allow

for thorough comprehension through these four steps.

iDonald Barra, The Dynamic Performance: A Performer’s


Guide to Musical Expression and Interpretation (Toronto:
Prentice-Hall Canada Inc., 1983), p. vii.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
2

The essential purpose of musical analysis is to provide

the musician with a systematic method of exploring musical

structures.2 Considering the massive amounts of score

information that the conductor of an extended work must

comprehend and assimilate, it is necessary that a detailed

method for score preparation be offered that can give order

to the exploration of information.

A survey of approaches to score preparation reveals the

most common analytical objectives as, simply, the analysis

of themes, tonal structure, and form. The analytical

techniques involved tend to limit their evaluations to

problems of musical structure and expressions of key and

chord relationships. The translation of theoretical

analyses into musical interpretations, and the

identification of musical passages that may require unusual

conducting gestures, are rarely considered. Furthermore,

the important supplementary topic of the unique

psychological relationship between conductor and ensemble is

addressed only s u p e r f icially.3 in general, concern for the

practicalities of performance seems to be very limited.

The small amount of information on performance

preparation is reflected in Hermann Scherchen’s view that

many concert artists achieve technical mastery of their

2John D. White, The Analysis of M u s i c , 2d ed. (London:


Scarecrow Press, 1984), p. 1.

3Peter Paul Fuchs, The Psychology of Conducting (New


York: MCA Music, 1969), p. vii.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
3

instruments to the exclusion of a thorough understanding of

the works which they perform.4* Concern for the composer's

communicative intentions is considered of little importance

by too many performers. Scherchen further states that the

performer has practically no acquaintance with the technique

of composition (Scherchen, p. 1)— a useful tool for

attempting to understand a c o mpo se r’s approach to

structuring a piece of music.

- Some conductors "feel too frustrated by the too

abstract process of learning scores well, so they merely go

through a few perfunctory motions of preparation” (Fuchs, p.

32). The need for a reasoned methodology of score

preparation, having a direct relationship to the practical

application of gesture technique, would be worthy of serious

consideration.

The Conductor's Purpose

The conductor is a specialist in communication, and the

content of that communication derives from a comprehensive

view of the score. The discussions which follow will

describe the co nductor’s communications with the players and

the listeners. The nature of the communication may be

obvious physical gesture or a more subtle form of contact

which results in a subjective connection.

'•Hermann Scherchen, Handbook of Conducting (1933; rpt.


London: Oxford University Press, 1942), p. 1.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
4

The conductor’s ultimate goal must be to "discover the

composer’s intentions and project them according to his own

conviction,"5 and as a result, "reach the heart and mind of

the listener" (Barra, p. vii). "The art of the interpreter

must never be separated from the warmth of the soul and the

heart..."5 Thus, the cumulative effect of the c o n du cto r’s

interpretation lies in the emotional response of the

listener. It is critical, therefore, that the conductor

understand the emotive impact of music, and the

characteristics of music that produce certain kinds of

emotive responses. The musical interpretation will result

from the conductor’s shaping of these elements.

"The superiority of interpretation is due to the

quality of the conductor’s conviction resulting from an

exceptional musical intuition based upon broad knowledge..."

(Blum, p. v) The secret of musical interpretation, made

manifest through 'conviction’ and 'intuition’, is the secret

of personality" (Scherchen, p. 1), and the effect of the

conductor’s personality is strengthened by a confident

knowledge of the score and mastery of competent rehearsal

and baton technique.

5 Roger Sessions, The Musical Experience of Composer,


Performer, Listener (Princeton: Princeton University Press,
1950), p. 82.

5David Blum, Casals and the Art of Interpretation (Los


Angeles: University of California Press, 1977), p. v.


Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
5

The perception of a conductor’s positive personality,

by the players and the audience, is of paramount importance

to the receptivity of the interpretation. The conductor’s

total preparation, invigorated by the medium of a warm

personality, will encourage inspirational energy from

players and audience alike.

The conductor must be able to infect the ensemble with

an appropriate sense of the m u s i c ’s g r e a t n e s s . 7 "The art of

conducting is governed by the fact that the conductor’s

instrument is a live one..." (Scherchen, p. 1) The

relationship between a conductor and ensemble exemplifies

the dynamic relationship between human beings, and is

replete with emotional cues. This factor is emphasized

throughout the literature on conductors and conducting. The

ability to lead an ensemble through an inspired musical

interpretation is manifested "...in the innermost forms of

the ego, at the very source of feeling..."8 When complete

technical competence marks the ability of conductor and

players, the decisive factor which insures an inspirational

performance is the positive impact of the conductor’s

personality. William Steinberg boldly asserts that most

conductors gradually evolve an effective form of technical

expression, but, in the final analysis, "the only factor

^Daniel Moe, Basic Choral Concepts (Minneapolis:


Augsburg Publishing House, 1972), p. 7.

8Carl Bamberger, The Conductor’s Art (New York:


McGraw-Hill, 1965), p. 306.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
6

that counts is the power of personality" (Bamberger, p.

306).

Conductoi— Player Interaction

The irrationality of conducting is that “one d o e s n ’t do

things by oneself, but makes other people do them— yet the

conductor is held responsible" for the outcome (Bamberger,

p. 303). In addition to the co n d u c t o r ’s technical

excellence, the capacity to effect certain modes of behavior

in others depends upon the creation of a psychological

bridge between the players and the conductor (Fuchs, p. 39).

The amount of accomplishment at rehearsal sessions is

greatly increased when the conductor "is dealing, not with

anonymous orchestra parts, but with [players] he knows as

well as they know hi m. "9 The conductor must come to know

and interact with the players as individuals, on and off the

podium. Toscanini once remarked that "every musician in the

orchestra needs to feel exactly what I feel" (Bamberger, p.

311). By his own admission, he came to realize the need for

this relationship with his players much too late— after more

than a generation of conducting. More recently, Michel

Debost, flutist with the Orchestre de Paris, remarked about

3Frederick Goldbeck, The Perfect Conductor (New York:


Pellegrini & Cudahy, 1951), p. 181.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
7

Sir Georg Solti: "I may not like his music, but I play it

the way he wants because I c a n ’t resist him."1*1

Practicality limits the intimacy of a relationship

between a conductor and all members of a very large

ensemble, and this is obviously true of guest conductors—

yet, all conductors must achieve successful interaction at

some level.

The major focus of commentaries on conductors is the

need for a conductor to be perceived as having "the

emotional impact of a great heart" (Fuchs, p. 59). When the

conductor is so perceived, even without the opportunity for

intimate social interaction, the core of success has been

achieved. The conductor must project his interpretive

feelings through a "current of love" (Bamberger, p. 274) and

thereby, communicate at the deepest levels with the players,

and ultimately, with the audience.

Conductoi— Audience Relationship

The conductor’s great victory is to have created

receptivity on the part of the audience, since conveying the

com p o s e r ’s musical intentions to the audience is the

principal goal of the concert performance.11 Richard

Strauss once admonished conductors to "remember that you are

lOActon Ostling, Jr., "Research on Nonverbal


Communication with Implications for Conductors," Journal of
Band Research 12 (1977): 29.

iiDavid Ewen, The Han with the Baton (New York: Thomas
Y. Crowell Co., 1936), p. 15.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
8

making mu sic not to amuse yourself but to delight your

audience" (Bamberger, p. 117).

The listener depends on the conductor for guidance

through a piece of music. However, sometimes the music may

be so complex that "no amount of [the conductor’s]

interpretative skill can enable the listener to grasp the

work until he has heard it many times . "12

The conductor’s impact as a guide through strange or

familiar territory may take the obvious forms of giving cues

and making large changes in the size or nature of gestures,

or, in more subtle changes of physical bearing. Fuchs

describes the overall impression of the conductor’s impact

as psychological (Fuchs, p. 59). Ultimately, it is through

the listener’s psychological response to the co nductor’s

physical actions that communication, at the deepest level,

exists (Bamberger, p. 274).

The Compos er’s Intent

With the advent of electronic music during the middle

part of the twentieth century, the nature of sounds

generated for musical purposes greatly expanded beyond the

scope of those produced by traditional wind, percussion, and

stringed instruments. While the compo ser ’s sound-choices

have continually expanded through the twentieth century, the

fundamental purpose of these choices has remained as it has

1 2 Robert L. Jacobs, "A Gestalt Psychologist on Music,"


Music Review 17 (1956): 187.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
9

been throughout music history. The composer's intent is to

manipulate the elements of sound and silence within a

structural framework; and, to intend this setting as an

artistic experience.*3

It is vitally important for all musicians to understand

that the vast majority of books about music literature and

music performance underscore an inextricable bond between

emotion and response to music (Lundin, p. 150). Through the

manipulation of musical elements, the composer is actually

having an effect on the listener's mood.*4 Studies have

proven that even non-appreciative listeners respond

emotionally to musical stimuli (Dreher, 1947 and Phares,

1934 ) . 15 While the nature of listener moods elicited by a

particular piece may or may not be similar among a certain

population, "affective changes induced by certain [other]

musics [may be] common to a large majority of listeners,

regardless of training, age, or experience" (Lundin, p.

161). These findings reinforce the observation by Roger

Sessions that "the realm of music is emotional energy rather

than emotion itself" (Sessions, p. 24).

Music which most interests the emotions and the

intellect is judged as the most valuable and lasting.

i^Robert W. Lundin, An Objective Psychology of Music


(New York: Ronald Press, 1967), p. 10.

^ L e o n a r d B - Meyer, Explaining Music (Los Angeles:


University of California Press, 1973), p. 6.

iSLundin, pp. 157-158.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
10

Compositional skill and emotional appeal must be combined

favorably by the composer; and the conductor’s successful

interpretation will manifest the proper relationship between

the two.

The Psychophysical Effect of Music

According to Carl Seashore (1941), "...our taste for

music creates a feeling of physiological well-being"

(Lundin, p. 186). The affective power of music to produce

various emotional moods in human beings has been recorded

since earliest times. Its origins appear linked to the

physiological processes of the human body, e.g., the

regularity of breathing, the regular beating of the heart,

and vocal sounds produced by the release of emotional

tension through sighs, yells, etc. Serge Koussevitsky has

postulated that "musical activity grows steadily because we

have a need for music" (Bamberger, p. 146). Could the "need

for music" be the need to release physiological tension? A

highly sophisticated release of psychophysical tension is

the a u t h o r ’s hypothesis on the origin of music; and while

Bamberger and Lundin strongly imply this origin, the

hypothesis is never explicitly stated.

Leading authorities on music agree that our responses

to musical stimuli are primarily emotional, and that these

responses are shaped through experiences with a variety of

"social and psychological forces” (Lundin, p. 189). Barra

describes our responses to music as "based upon a series of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
11

interlocking tensive reactions, expectations, and

projections which occur in the mind and body of the

listener" (1983, p. 161). When the nature and intensity of

the interpreter and audience tension is the same, a true

line of communication exists. Therefore, we may say that

music listening produces a sequence of alternating tensive

and relaxing responses in the listener through which

emotional release is achieved. While a specific type of

emotion, e.g., fear, joy, surprise, may not be communicated,

emotional energy is always communicated (Sessions, p. 24).

For the majority of listeners, the substantive response

to music is emotional; however, the first step of our

response mechanism is to recognize, cognitively, the

measurable elements of the musical event. Lundin (1967)

cites several studies which relate the effects of various

musical elements, of varying degrees of intensity, to

different categories of emotive reponse. These studies

include Ellis and Brighous (1954), Heinlein and Hevner

(1935), and Mull (1957). The results confirm a measured

comfort range for each element, beyond which an amount of

increased tension is caused in the listener. The increased

tension may take the form of heightened pleasure or anxiety.

While Meyer (1973, p. 6) does draw a relationship

between emotional responses and cognitive patternings ,

Sessions (1950, p. 30) observes that it is possible for

listeners to have "the truest sense of auditory

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
12

discrimination" (in terms of being able to identify causal

musical elements) and still be "unable to love or understand

[music]". In actuality, the listener’s appreciation of

music is dependent upon connections between the social

environment and the musical experience as well as the

quantity of past musical experiences.is

That music is widely appreciated more for emotional

effect than intellectual understanding is well-documented.

As performers and conductors strive to interpret the printed

page, they must not lose sight of music as a fundamentally

emotional experience. Proper performance practices demand

emotional conceptions. The emotional impact of the

intellectually structured events is absolutely essential to

inspired performance and appreciation.

Summary

The conductor must prepare to communicate the

co mp ose r’s intentions by making preparations in two major

areas: (1) development of a theoretical and emotional

rationale for the c omposition’s structure, and (2) the

practical transformation of these analyses results into

conducting gestures that will translate the objective score

information into physical motion, reflect the emotional

energy of the piece, and thus elicit an emotive response

i6i_eon Crickmore, "The Musical Gestalt," Music Review


33 (1972): 287.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
13

from the players and the audience. This entire process

forms the basis of the condu ct or’s interpretation.

As a result of thorough score knowledge and technical

mastery of gestural skills, the conductor is able to display

confidence on the podium that is vital to professional

acceptance by members of the ensemble, resulting in a high

quality of music-making (Fuchs, p. 5). A superficial

knowledge of the score, on the other hand, may be evidenced

in a conductor’s lack of enthusiasm for the compo ser ’s

intent and the m u s i c ’s effect, or an interpretation lacking

in imagination and spontaneity. Positive and constructive

personality traits, combined with technical competence, are

the principal and mutually supportive characterisitics of

the finest conductors. This study will seek to explore one

aspect of this mutually supportive relationship by focusing

on the conductor’s preparation of the score.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER 2

Survey of Approaches to Score Preparation

A survey of principal sources on instrumental and

choral conducting published within the last twenty years

reveals an emphasis on the development of gestural technique

with relatively little attention given to score study. Even

though a cond uc tor ’s gestural technique is based upon the

information printed in- the score,' the actual interpretation

of the score and the transformation of its objective

information into conducting gesture is discussed only

superficially.

In too many instances, conductors do not sufficiently

value the refining of gesture technique in order to

efficiently reflect score information, and often, they do

not appreciate the structural development of the music

itself.i These conductors tend to use very basic gesture

patterns, devoid of the nuance that should attend the subtle

effects in the score. Moreover, they rehearse their

interpretations verbally rather than rely on the more

expedient method of silent gesture. That is, instead of

indicating a strong dynamic accent or a change in tempo by

using the appropriate conducting gesture, the conductor

iCharles Blackman, Behind The Baton (New York: Charos


Enterprises,Inc., 1964), p. 76.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
15

simply tells the ensemble what element to change,, when to

make the change, and how much change to make.

The review of texts and articles on conducting produced

three different means of presenting score preparation: (1)

discussions of individual musical elements to be analyzed;

(2) cursory commentary on score study; and (3) discussions

which offer simple information of ancillary importance to

score preparation, such as concerto rehearsal practices and

the numbering of measures for rehearsal purposes.

The review produced a total of twenty-three (23)

musical elements that were suggested for analysis by the

conductor. The following list cites the twenty-three (23)

suggested elements of score preparation and the source(s) in

which they were found:

Components Source(s)

1. Gain overview Boult, 8usch,

2. Advocates memorization Scherchen

3. Key structure Boult

4. Thematic balance Boult

5. Emotional sequence Boult

6 . Identify climactic points Lamb

7. Dynamic shape Boult

8 . Pattern of colors Boult, Rudolf

9'. Harmonic dstail Boult, Rudolf

Determine tempo Moe


O a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
16

11. Determine phrasings Boult, Green & Malko, Rudolf

12. Listen to recording with


a full score Boult, Bowles

13. Identify musical events


requiring special gesture
preparation Moe

14. Examine historical


context Busch, Lamb

15. Ascertain instrumentation Green & Malko

16. Learn the notes Garretson, Green & Malko

17. Structural content Garretson, Rudolf

18. Analysis of text Garretson

19. Score read at piano Garretson

20. Highlight published score


markings Busch, Garretson

21. Composer and composition


background information Rudolf

22. Determine conductor and


player problems Busch

23. Analyze melody according


to gesture Busch

While a common relationship may be defined between two

or more of the suggested components, e.g., "emotional

sequence"2 and "identify climax points,"3 it is interesting

to note how few phrases describing the musical elements

appear among thetwenty-three elements with exactly the same

name. For example, while one text studies the"pattern of

2Sir Adrian Boult, A Handbook on the Technique of


Conducting (London: Paterson’s Publishing Ltd., 1968).

3Gordon H. Lamb, Choral Techniques (Dubuque: W.C.


Brown, 1974).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
17

colours,"4 another examines the "instrumentation,"5 or,

while one studies "phrasings,"5 another looks at "dynamic

shape."7 This use of unprecise terminology and phrases

causes confusion in music analysis.

The elements of music frund in the score can be defined

in very specific terms, e.g., tempo, rhythm, meter, theme,

dynamics, harmony, etc., yet authors will use ambiguous

phrases such as "dynamic shape," which may refer to either

actual volume levels, or to the melodic contour of phrases

used to ascertain principal climactic points. The reader

must constantly be aware of various terminology having the

same definition.

An analysis of the common elements reveals that nine of

the elements were repeated, at least once, in more than one

text. Ranked in the order of most frequent appearance,

those elements include: (1) gaining an overview of the

composition (four sources), (2) identifying phrase groupings

(three sources), and the following, all of which were tied

for third place— each having been mentioned in two texts—

(3) identifying patterns of colors, harmonic detail,

48oult, 1968; Max Rudolf, The Grammar of Con du cti ng , 2d


ed. (New York: Schirmer, 1980).

5Elizabeth A.H. Green and Nicolai Malko, The Conductor


and His Score (Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, Inc.,
1975).

5Boult, 1968; Green and Malko, 1975; Rudolf, 1980.

7Boult, 1968.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
18

listening to a recording while reading a full score,

examining the historical context of the work, examining

structural content, and the highlighting of published

markings in the score, e.g., changes in tempo, meter, key,

etc.

Absent in every case is a clearly organized and

systematic analytical approach to exploring the structural

use of musical elements throughout a score. Instead,

analysis categories are often presented in unrelated ways

and through such unprecise suggestions as "study the

strucutural content."

The commonalities found among the conducting sources

suggests that score analysis should begin by achieving an

overview of the work through identification of (1) major

structural points, (2) textures, and (3) climactic events.

Once this broad perspective is attained, a conception of the

work as a whole may be maintained as various elements are

studied in more detail and continually related to the whole

work. This continual relating of the phrase-by-phrase

sequence to the whole work will facilitate the conductor's

comprehensive understanding of the work.

The conducting sources did not present the detailed

analysis of score elements in a particular ordered fashion.

Moreover, a systematic routine for the analysis of detailed

structure on a phrasal level was not offered. One text did

suggest that the conductor establish a personalized

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
19

hierarchy of elements for examination by deductive analysis.

However, for the inexperienced conductor, there may be too

much time devoted to trial and error in designing the

hierarchy of elements before a workable order is found.

Historical Survey Of Score Preparation

A review of information on score preparation was drawn

from writings about conductors whose professional careers

spanned the late nineteenth and early twentieth centuries.

The emphases of approaches to interpretation included

analysis for (1) detail and overall compositional design,

and (2) personal emotional involvement, or (3) a combination

of the two. Approaches focused on understanding the overall

design of a work.

The means to attain an overall understanding included

cursory score study with the final interpretive decision

being made at the rehearsals; having a score reader play the

score through several times at the piano; having the

assistant conductor direct a new work while the conductor

sits in the auditorium and shapes an interpretation; and

reading the score through like a book in order to permit an

interpretation to take shape. The survey of available

information revealed that a variety of attitudes had

prevailed simultaneously. There was not one methcd-of-

choice.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
20

Application Of Theoretical Techniques


To Score Preparation

In view of the absence of a well-founded technique of

musical interpretation in general, and score interpretation

in particular, it is useful to apply certain processes of

established techniques of theoretical analysis in order to

produce a method which may examine, systematically, all

kinds of musical composition to reveal interpretive

characteristics. To derive a viable analysis system, a

synthesis of these theoretical techniques, along with the

traditional approach to score preparation cited earlier (p.

1), will be necessary. The resulting analytical process

must be able to engage a mass of musical information and

analyze it in an orderly manner.

The conductor will benefit from the regular application

of a uniform analysis process to each new composition

studied. This will allow for a common basis of comparison

in identifying similarities and contrasts among different

works. Furthermore, the categories of analysis must be

broad enough to allow for the study of a wide range of

compositions, e.g., tonal, atonal, works of completely

indefinite pitch, chance music, improvisatory music,

electronic media, etc. They must address the basic

components of all musics, and, when necessary, the nature

of descriptive terms used to define each category of music

must be permitted to derive from the music itself.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
21

An analysis process used to make interpretive decisions

must describe the overall form of the music and the use of

elements through that form. It is necessary for the process

to explore the elements of the composition and measure their

respective import to evoke meaningful interpretation. An

understanding of the compositional process, as well as

judgments of style and interpretation will be the result

(White, p. 207).

Apart from the results of objective analysis, "...the

instinctive and wordless feelings as to what a piece of

music is really about...can sometimes be more useful than

the most detailed of analyses." (White, p. 209) However,

through the conscientious combination of intuition and

objective analysis, the conductor should arrive at a viable

interpretation.

Many theorists favor a deductive approach to analysis

in order to gain an overview of a work before detailed

analysis is begun (White, 1984). By doing so, one develops

a concept of the whole composition to which discoveries

about the work may be related. The ability to constantly

draw relationships between the components and the whole,

plus relationships among the components themselves, serves

to reinforce comprehension of the entire work. When

studying works of extended length, this approach is a

crucial factor.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
22

The illustration of a deductive process can be seen in

L a R u e ’s (1970) approach to the analysis of musical style, in

which he divides "Part II: Observation"® of his style-

analysis outline into five elements which he considers

critical to the examination of tonal music. These elements

are listed in the following sequence, preferred for analysis

purposes: Sound, Form, Harmony, Rhythm, and Melody. The

rationale for the ordering is given by the following

explanations: (1) Sound (there is a psychological firstness

to sound because color combinations are generally easy to

recognize), (2) Form (in order to retain and compare

structural impressions, the analyst must first achieve a

structural overview), (3) Harmony (chosen as a principal

element that defines texture— placed ahead of rhythm and

melody due to degree of analytical difficulty), (4) Rhythm

(placed before melody due to its greater range of

complexity), and (5) Melody (melody "demands the least

concentration"9 ). L a R u e ’s approach to musical analysis is

organized and systematic.

Another deductive analytical method is W h i t e ’s (1984)

presentation of his "Descriptive Analysis" in three

categories. White recommends music analysis in the

following order: (1) Macroanalysis (general discussion

®Jan LaRue, Guidelines for Style Analysis (New York:


W.W. Norton, 1970), p. 18ff.

9Jan LaRue, "Fundamental Considerations in Style


Analysis," Notes 25, no. 3 (1969); 448.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
23

regarding the piece as a whole), (2) Middle-analysis

(general discussion regarding large sections of the work),

and (3) Microanalysis (essentially a phrasal review of the

compositional process).

LaRue and White both present systems that demonstrate

the importance of understanding the whole, and both systems

present a well-reasoned order to elemental analysis.

The deductive quality of always relating detailed

information to the larger whole is congruent with the

concept of hierarchical ordering presented by Meyer (1973).

He suggests that it is easier even for listeners to hear the

components of a work because of the compos er’s hierarchical

ordering of structure, i.e., the consistent formation of a

progression of structures (each only partially completed)

which combine to form "more extended, higher order

patterns..." Once again, the ability to draw a relationship

between one part of an event and the entire event reinforces

the process of comprehension.

One method of analyzing and comparing analytical data

is to produce a graph by detailing the information along a

continuum that may be scrutinized at a glance (White, 1984).

The use of such graphs should significantly increase

understanding of a particular work by permitting visual

comparisons of the structural features over the course of

one or several movements; and between different works. Due

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
24

to the coded nature of graphed information, graphs may also

have potential for computer application (White, p. 1 S 3 ) .

An analytical process using graphs was developed by

Heinrich Schenker.*0 His analyses illustrate the structural

points of melodic development that he believes shapes the

melodic and harmonic structures of entire compositions, and

explains essential structural relationships within a

composition. However, S c h e n k e r ’s graphs are of little use

to conductors since they present limited information which

concerns the understanding of developing motion at the

phrasal level.

The Composer’s Creative Process

Regarding pre-rehearsal preparation, the final

interpretive act of the conductor is the selection of

appropriate gestures which are derived from the stylistic

and interpretive analyses of the composer’s score.

Since the conductor’s interpretation derives from (1)

an understanding of the compositional process, and (2) the

subsequent cognitive-emotional bond formed between the

conductor and the score, it is useful to further trace the

compositional process to its source of emotional

inspiration. By doing so, the conductor will complete a

personal involvement with the music through some knowledge

of the compos er ’s personality and the reasons for having

lOHeinrich Schenker, Five Graphic Music Analyses (New


York: Dover, 1969), p. 4.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
25

written the composition. Access to this information will

mold the c o ndu cto r’s deepest emotional involvement with the

c o m p o s e r ’s intentions.

Fuchs (1969) reinforces the idea of understanding the

c o m p o s e r ’s creative process by recommending that conductor

training should parallel composer training with regard to

the study of "harmony, counterpoint, composition,

orchestration, score reading, etc." Understanding of the

compositional process permits empathy to develop between

conductor and composer, and assists the conductor in

determining the aural balance, and resulting impact, of

various compositional elements. This should lead to an

honest interpretation of the work, and the conductor may be

better able to find "the basic i d e a " n in the work of each

composer. Koussevitsky believed "the basic idea" to be the

meaning of the c o m p o s e r ’s life and the ideals that he

transmits.

Fundamental composing processes have not changed

significantly for the last several centuries; and most

importantly for the composition student: "These basic

techniques remain technical, written, and learnable..."i 2

The basic process of composition is concerned "with

tension and the relaxation of tension." (Music Review,

11 Review of Tensions in the Performance of Music edited


by Carola Grindea, Music Revie w, 44 (1983): 305.

Ibid.

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
26

1983) Understanding this basic process is of critical

importance to the conductor since the processes of tension

and release are the bases of emotion (Barra, p. 28), and it

is widely held that emotion is the final arbiter of the

c o nductor’s interpretation.

The seminal idea of a composition can be virtually any

musical element developed by the composer’s imagination.

Once an idea is created, the composer immediately begins to

evolve a structure through the development and prolongation

of tension. That is, the compose!— emotion generated as a

result of the intellectual idea further develops musical

events (Sessions, p. 24). Structure and passion are not

mutually exclusive (Sessions, p. 54).

From a rough outline of the musical seed, the composer

must develop integrated form and style. Edward Cone has

stated that "...musical form is basically rhythmic.“1 3

While many composers formulate structure from thematic

ideas, others may, in fact, formulate a structure into which

thematic ideas are molded. For example, Maurice Ravel is

quoted as having said: "I’ve finished my composition. , .all

but .the themes" (Cone, 1968).

As the composition evolves, the composer varies the

musical elements independently according to conceived

patterns of tension with the purpose of producing an

1 2 Edward I. Cone, Musical Form and Musical Performance


(New York: W.W. Norton, 1968), p. 25.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
27

interesting work. An interesting work will follow a

logical, emotional progression. The composer's style will

be revealed through the repetition of a few unique elemental

combinations within the work. A summation of the style

characteristics among the compositions of many composers

then allows commonalities regarding the use of elements to

be drawn. These commonalities result in the major

characterisitics of a particular historical period.

With the advent of open score notation, composers were

able to experiment with varied ways of molding chordal and

key progressions, motivic development, harmonic tension and

release, and various shadings of texture. The use of open

score improved the clarity of communication between composer

and interpreter by enabling the composer to increase the

amount of detail on a page. However, regarding the effect

on performance practice, "...the intangible factors...which

make all the difference between a good performance and a bad

one, cannot conceivably be indicated in the score"

(Sessions, p. 73). The literature on conducting and

interpretation emphasizes that variations in tempi,

dynamics, and articulation are the quintessential factors of

unique interpretations. This point is of vital importance

to the conductor and to all others who interpret music.

The Compositional Process and Score Preparation

The conductor needs to approach the interpretation of

all music with a common process of exploration which results

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
28

from an understanding of how composers communicate through

their music. The best composers communicate a sequence of

expectations to the listener, each of which is resolved in

unique or unexpected, yet appropriate, ways that will tend

to manipulate the mood of the listener and expand the

listener’s view and understanding of humanity. The

conductor must discover these expectations from the

objective notation presented in the score and arrive at a

progression of emotional impact.14

Characteristics of style presented by a particular work

offer insight into the c o m p o s e r ’s creative method, and the

compositional process offers a perception of the

contemporary style of writing. The c onductor’s

comprehensive awareness of the characteristic style of the

period, as well as the unique characteristics of the

composition being prepared, will assist in understanding the

c o m p o s e r ’s intentions, resulting in a convincing

interpretation.

The twentieth century has expanded the definition of

music from the pitch and rhythm content of earlier common

practice to one which replaces pitch and rhythm with a

progression of sound events. In some cases the conductor

merely signals the beginning and duration of these sound

14Stanley Sadie, ed., The New Grove Dictionary of Music


and Musicians (London: Macmillan Publishers, 1980), pp.
599-602.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
29

events, e.g., Penderecki, Pittsburgh Overture (1 9 6 7 )is and

does not utilize traditional gestures based on a metrical

pattern. The conductor’s principal duty is to offer

feedback regarding the balance of various sound sources and

the overall effect of the piece.

In other types of twentieth century music, the composer

might structure events according to a preconceived

framework, e.g., Boulez, Structures^ in which the music is

not determined by emotive rationale, but rather by an

autonomic sequencing of events. Chance music and

compositions using improvisation are not based upon

composei— generated emotive sequence that can be interpreted

and controlled by a conductor. As a result, any type of

emotive interpretation is gained solely by the efforts of

the players. However, creations not based upon an emotive

premise, to whatever degree, do not prevent the audience

from responding emotionally.

A review of repertoire for composers from various style

periods reveals that "...composers...have rarely maintained

consistent awareness of movement in [the] middle and large

dimensions" of their works (LaRue, 1969, p. 456). This

would indicate that composers have been concerned with

compositional process to the exclusion of concern for the

isPenderecki, Pittsburgh Overture (New York: C.F.


Peters, 1967)

ispierre Boulez, Structures (Wien: Universal Edition,


c . 1955-67)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
30

a u d i e n c e ’s emotive reaction, thus, making it more difficult

for the conductor to successfully convey the co mpo s e r ’s

intentions in an emotional way. Stated differently, this

type of music may be evaluated as boring by the listener.

Summary

In order for the theoretical analysis of a score to be

useful to the conductor, a relationship must be drawn

between the type of analytical data sought and the

translation of the data into sound. The medium of the

translation, gesture technique, must be clearly understood.

Therefore, the co n d u c t o r ’s preparation must involve the

inextricably related areas of score analysis and gesture

technique.

As a means of gathering the type of analytical data

that is necessary for translation into gesture, conducting

literature and literature on theoretical analysis stresses

the identification of broadly based score characteristics.

These characteristics include (1) the overview of the work,

(2) the identification of major structural points, textures

(e.g., orchestration, harmonic areas, homophonic or

contrapuntal writing, etc.), and climactic areas, and (3)

the review of historical context. Understanding the broad

view of the musical work will be enhanced by the graphing of

analytical data which is capable of being translated into

conducting gesture. Since the conducting gesture, at any

given moment, is involved with the communication of score

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
31

detail, as opposed to broad areas such as overview or

harmonic development, it follows that score analysis must

also lead to the analysis of score detail at the level of

the immediate musical sound.

The premise which underlies score analysis, subsequent

decisions on interpretation, and finally, determinations on

gestures, will be emotional effect. Most music is conceived

by a compo se r’s emotional intellect, performed by emotional

players, and heard and evaluated by emotional listeners. An

approach to score preparation which presents a means for the

translation of the score into emotional sound events will be

the subject of the next chapter.

(; -

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER III

The Comprehensive Approach To Score Preparation


For The Conductor

Introduction

"A large gap exists between the commonly taught

technique of conducting and the actual demands of leading an

orchestra" (Fuchs, p. viii). While the acquisition of

competent stick technique forms the core of most college

level conducting classes, the professional literature on

conducting and conductors also emphasizes the importance of

physical bearing and personality as vital aspects of the

conducting process (Bamberger, p. 313). Many of the demands

of conducting stem from these interactions between conductor

and players.

The manifestation of personality on the podium develops

as a result of a high degree of familiarity with the score

and gestural technique. We might postulate a basic formula

for competent conducting as: POSITIVE PERSONALITY +

COMPETENT REHEARSAL SKILLS = MEANINGFUL INTERPRETATION.

In most college level conducting texts, a major portion

of the material is devoted to developing stick technique,

and far less time is devoted to actually exploring the

components of the score. Yet, gestural technique must be

chosen according to the events portrayed in the score. This

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
33

lack of attention to gestures may be caused by the prevalent

attitude of professional musicians who say that "conducting

cannot be taught...conducting is instinctive...conductors

are born, not made" (Blackman, pp. 46-47). To project such

ideas lends a mysterious air to music-making in general, and

conducting in particular. Conducting texts do not recognize

the objective materials that compose a musical event, and

refuse to consider that music is composed "according to

human laws" (Scherchen, p. 2). Music is composed by human

beings in ways that are observable and measurable, and the

results of compositional efforts may be altered by other

human beings.

There is no mystery to music-making except "the secrets

of man and of human nature," (Scherchen, p. 2) and these

"secrets" are measurable and communicable. The complexity of

the score may make conducting instruction a detailed and

lengthy process, but it does not make it an impossible one

(Blackman, p. 47).

The Comprehensive Approach To Score Preparation (CASP)

will provide the young conductor with a systematic method of

exploring the complexities of any musical score. Having

mastered the sequence of steps involved with CASP, the

student may later alter the sequence to fit a more

personalized form of analysis.

Through C A S P ’s initial steps, exploration of the score

should yield data that can be immediately transformed into

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
34

gesture. As additional score data is collected through the

remaining steps, the conductor’s insight will broaden and

deepen to produce a better musical understanding, and

ultimately, a convincing interpretation.

For seasoned conductors who begin score study with"

greater knowledge and experience, many of C A S P ’s initial

steps will be assimilated and synthesized very quickly;

however, for the experienced as well as the inexperienced

conductor, the steps through which score learning takes

place remain the same.

While CASP acknowledges the critical importance of (1)

the conductor’s personality in conveying interpretation to

an ensemble and (2) the critical importance of developing a

clear and communicative gesture technique, the scope of CASP

factors presented here will be limited to the pre-rehearsal

preparation of the score.

Definition of CASP Analysis Process

CASP presents score preparation as a deductive process.

This process derives from the comprehension of the musical

work as an intellectually structured, emotion-effecting

entity, which has been produced by a human being responding

to the emotional energy of life experiences.

The CASP process is divided into three levels of data

collection. The levels are presented in their order of

practical importance to the act of conducting a work, and

are labelled as Level I: Basal Structure Analysis ( B S A) ,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
35

Level II: Review of Supplementary Information (RSI), and

Level III: Detailed Structural Analysis (DSA). The

following is an abbreviated description of each level:

Level I: Basal Structural Analysis (BSA)

This level is the critical area of immediate

importance to the conductor. Score information for

the Basal Structure Analysis is acquired by

exploring the score from cover to cover, several

times, according to categories of elemental features.

From the results of the BSA the conductor will select

and choreograph the conducting gestures. In cases of

having to conduct on short notice, the time available

for personal study does not permit the assimilation of

a new score by virtue of thorough analysis at all three

levels, the analysis priority falls to Level I. The

primary focus on a Level I analysis, even to the

virtual exclusion of the other two levels, will permit

the pacing of musical events to be accurate, if not

interpretively convincing.

Level II: Review of Supplementary Information (RSI)

This area highlights the personal chatacteristics

of the composer during the creation of the work, the

specific events which led the composer to compose the

work, and particulars of the environment in which the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
36

work was first performed. An understanding of the

history of the compositional form being prepared is

helpful to complete a comprehensive understanding of

the work. This information may assist with decisions

regarding performance practices.

Level III: Detailed Structural Analysis CDSA)

This area approaches the score through a process

of "re-composition" with a view towards the formulation

of explanation and rationale for the sequence of

musical events. It explores the score in minute detail

in order to discover communicative intentions through

the character and structure of the melodic line; the

relationship of one structural section or feature to

another; the overall structural proportions based upon

thematic development; the influence of the performance

environment; the balance of tone color combinations;

and the contrasts indicated in the score. The results

of this last analysis process should deepen the

conductor’s understanding of the work and expand the

amount of conviction with which the interpretation is

presented.

When the three levels of CASP analysis are completed,

the conductor should have achieved as complete an

understanding of the compo se r’s intentions as possible

without having actually spoken with the composer.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
37

Rehearsal application of the results of the CASP analysis,

combined with a positive personal approach and efficient

rehearsal techniques, should permit the opportunity for

developing a reasonable musical interpretation.

Presented below is a detailed explanation of each of

the three CASP analysis levels. In addition to being

ordered according to immediate usefulness, this sequence of

three levels obliges the conductor to follow a deductive

process of score learning. This should provide continual

means for better understanding the relationships between the

various components of the score. Thorough comprehension of

the score will thereby be steadily reinforced.

It is useful to view a musical work as part of the

c o mpo ser ’s development as a musical craftsman. As a result

of analysis covering several of a co mp o s e r ’s works, one is

able to identify various stylistic features which are

indicative of the c o m p o s e r ’s overall compositional technique

and development as a composer. Knowing the description of a

c o mposer’s general style, it is possible to analyze a work

with a view towards finding innovative features in

compositional structure, thereby noting advances in musical

style achieved in a particular work.

The conductor’s emotional and intellectual involvement

with the work is heightened by the results of comparing

stylistic features of a particular work with a c o m p o s e r ’s

total output. The emotional and intellectual involvement of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
38

the conductor then becomes an inextricable part of the

interpretation.

The following is a summary outline of the three CASP

analysis levels:

I. Level I— Basal Structure Analysis

A. Review of all instructions set forth in the


score regarding performance practice, -
interpretation, stage set-up, etc.

B. Identification of Major Musical Moments (MMM).

C. Identification of significant changes in


texture.

D. Identification of emotional energy sequence.

E. Selection of instruments to be cued.

F. Identification of phrase lengths.

G. Identification of form, thematic content, and


major tonal areas.

H. Identification of conductor problems.

I. Identification of player problems.

J. Synthesis of Level I analysis results (Basal


Structure Analysis Charts)

II. Level II— Review of Surplementary Information

A. Reason for which the work was composed.

B. The lifestyle and cultural environment of the


c o mpo ser .

C. Circumstances surrounding the first


performance of the work.

D. Compositional features of the contemporary


style period.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
39

III. Level III— Detailed Structural Analysis

A. Assessment of overall structure in terms of


thematic development with sectioning by
p h r a s e s a n d clauses.

8. Analysis of phrase structure with


relationships to other musical components
found in the s c o r e v

Level I; BASAL STRUCTURE ANALYSIS

A Level I analysis is the most critical part of the

entire CASP process. From this analysis of structural

elements, the conductor derives decisions on gesture

choreography. Specifically, analyzing a score to translate

certain aspects of its structural features into gesture

enables the conductor to exhibit clear and efficient

conducting.

Basal Structure Analysis is based upon the following

prem is es:

(a) Music derives from human emotion.

(b) The listener responds to musical stimuli through a

number of internal and external responses in terms of

tension and release.

(c) The conduc tor ’s unique interpretation of a musical

work is achieved through the manipulation of tempo,

dynamics, and articulation.

(d) The conductor must seek the c o mp ose r’s intent

regarding the temporal pace of unfolding events within the

composition.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
40

(e) Based upon the c omp ose r’s intent and personal

intuition, the conductor will conclude decisions on

interpretation and communicate this interpretation to the

players through

(1) the use of predetermined conducting gestures

(which may change during rehearsals), resulting

in

(2) a highly refined system of visual' communication

which contributes to

(3) the effective use of rehearsal time, and leads

to

(4) an effective performance of the work.

Leyel I analysis enables the conductor to quickly

comprehend the work as a whole (Boult, 1968). It is of

primary importance that the conductor understand the

unfolding of the work in its immediate sense— as a series of

discreet aural events (Fuchs, 1969). Level I focuses on

textural structures as they unfold, and without regard for

their theoretical derivation, or relationships among the

details of the textures. Thus, the conductor is dealing

with a synthesis of all of the m u s i c ’s elements in order to

make preliminary decisions concerning style, meaning,

design, and pacing (White, 1984).

The Level I process consists of ten (10) separate

analyses, and it is suggested that each analysis be

completed, separately, by a thorough reading of the score.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
41

By reading the score through several times, the

c o n d u c t o r ’s comprehension of the work will gradually

increase (Boult, 1968; Davison, 1968; Lamb, 1974). The

progressive synthesis of the information collected from

these readings will reveal insights into how the music

should be perceived; and, this will suggest various

interpretations of the score (Cone, 1960, p. 174).

According to Blackman (1964) and Moe (1972), the

conductor conveys the following musical messages via silent

gestures: (1) Tempo, (2) Meter, (3) Dyanmics, (4) Rhythm,

(5) Intensity, and (6) Mood. CASP adds the elements of

articulation,, linear writing, and orchestration to this

list. Other factors, such as shape of the phrase and color

balance, are conductoi— controlled through a melding of the

before mentioned messages.

Components of the Basal Structure Analysis

#1■ Review all preliminary instructions set forth in the


score by the composer or editor as regards
instrumentation, stage set-up, interpretation, etc.

As Level I analysis proceeds and comprehension deepens,

it will be useful to compare the published editions of the

score with the original manuscript, if available, in order

to note any changes or structural modifications. Moreover,

a review of articles about the composition may reveal

comments or revisions made by the composer, or with the

c o m p o s e r ’s sanction, that are not included in any published

score. Comments from musicians who worked with the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
42

composer, or comments handed down by the musicians to their

students, should be taken into account. Recorded

performances with the composer conducting, or with the

composer in collaboration, should also be consulted at this

point.

#2. Identification of Major Musical Moments (M M M >, or areas


of climax, throughout the score (Boult, 1968; Jacobs,
1956).

These structural areas provide points of arrival for

the patterns of motion within the work (Barra, 1983, p. 9),

which, in turn, become major factors in determining the

pacing of the work. The MMM may be defined as one or more

phrases, or it may be only a portion of a phrase. Decisions

on M M M ’s are subjective and should be considered changeable.

Mark M M M ’s in the score by selecting those measures

that are perceived as having a unique emotional impact on

the listener, and label the measured section accordingly

(See Example 1).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
U3

hi

M O
PM

■P
O

I §3
Q)
-P 03

03

«- s *

CL O
1

si (
lid
«u< «

•j u

si| aj

d <

d••i •
•i

I S I I I S I | I « ;

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
44

Mm. 7 2 -8 3

Phrases: .78,79

MMM:

Meter:

Tempo: Anime ( J. = 138)

The climax of the first movement is stated during the

last quarter of its length and is developed from m. 79

through a fortissimo restatement of theme (Al) with tutti

orchestration beginning at m. 83. The restatement of Al is

prepared by a four measure use of the repetitive rhythm (/T J ) -

with repeated pitches, simultaneously contrasting rhythmic

textures of the triplet pattern over a sequence, of ( «J. )

note values, tutti orchestration, fortissimo dynamic, and

tonal preparation of the tonic key of Bb major through

alternating inversions of a minor f n chord.

The climactic areas of music composed using common

practice techniques usually consist of a blending of

harmonic and melodic material that is unique in relation to

the surrounding harmonic and melodic texture, and usually

causes a tensional response. In the case of twentieth

century works not composed in this style, an MMM may be

created by an aural event that is noted for its uniqueness

of intensity in relation to the surrounding texture. In any

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
45

event, M M M ’s derive from a musical setting which forms a

unique emotional experience in relation to the surrounding

musical material.

#3. Identification of significant changes in texture.

The listen er ’s perception of musical form comes largely

as a result of contrast and variation in texture,* and the

texture of a section or phrase determines the nature and

size of the conducting gesture.

For the purposes of Level I analysis, the traditional

definition of texture, which refers to linear movement,

e.g., homophonic, polyphonic, etc., is expanded to include

not only this description of linear writing, but also the

other musical elements that can be communicated by the

conductor through silent gesture. Therefore, the sum of

musical elements that will define changes or variation in

textures include (1) tempo, (2) meter, (3) dynamics, (4)

rhythm, (5) linear writing, (6) articulation, and (7)

orchestration.

The definition of the initial textural section followed

by the subsequent changes and variations in textures,

through the elements named above, will serve as the chief

sources for decisions on conducting gestures that will guide

the ensemble from the beginning to the end of a composition.

*Karl Krueger, The Way of the Conductor (New York:


Scribner & Sons, 1958), p. 210; Meyer, 1973, p. 19.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
46

Elemental changes in textural sections must effect

enough change in sound to warrant a change in some aspect of

the conducting gesture. The beginnings of score sections

which offer such changes are marked with a symbol at the top

of the score, such as ( -4-) , above the first measure of the

new texture. This symbol will not describe the nature of

the textural change, but will simply call attention to the

point at which a significant change occurs.

The decision to mark a change in texture will come from

the conductor’s consideration of the score information.

Rules containing absolute criteria for textural changes are

not offered. Whether or not a section is marked as a

textural change will depend upon the conductor’s perception,

which will vary from one conductor to another.

The following discussion describes various changes in

textural definition of enough consequence, in the auth or’s

view, to require a change in the respective conducting

gesture. Following the common practice musical definition

which precedes each example, a basic description of the

related conducting gesture is discussed, followed by the

generic definition of the textural element. The generic

definition describes the element as it may be used with

other kinds of musical composition, particularly of the

twentieth century, in which structural features defy

definition by the use of common practice terminology.

¥ — -----------
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
47

When the various musical textures seen by the players

in their parts are also reinforced by the conductor’s

gestures, these two sets of signals reinforce one another,

and enables the ensemble to present the most unified

performance.

Significant Changes in Texture

(a) T E M PO. Variations will include such factors as

gradual change, sudden change, fermatas, and 'grand’ pauses.

(See Example 2)

Gest ur e: Tempo is reflected in the speed of the

conducting motion.

Generic d e f i n i t i o n : In the cases of unmetered time,

'tempo’ may be recognized as the number of separate events

passing through a particular time frame.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
48

<!
ON
NO
cA
NO

g.
P< X

-=r
ON

a>
at
■3
I►«
£
w n

©
m
O
*3
OJ *3
© |!
£
g.K
©
I*
W 03

■s
3
A

1
©

i
03

5/s T I

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
49

Mm. 3 5 -3 8

Phrases: 34,35,36_

Meter: C (4/4)

Tempo: Anime ( J =144)

The molto ri't. at m. 35 is presented in subito fashion

and must be prepared for between m. 344 and m. 3 5 1 .

Following release of the fermata m. 35« , the conductor must

determine the length of pause before the beginning of the "a

tempo" at m. 36*-

(b) ME TER. Change in conducting pattern according to

the number of beats per measure. (See Example 3)

Gest ur e: Meter is reflected in the conductor's the

beat pattern.

Generic d e f i nit ion : Some type of numerical grouping of

sound events, perceptible or not, which occurs during a

particular time frame. Often, sequences of aural events

will be totally without recognizable groupings in time.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Example 3: Symphonic Dance Ho. 3


Textural Change via METER
Source: Clifton Williams, Symphonic Dance ho. 3 (New York: Sam Pox, 1967)» PP» 6-7*

Condensed Score Condensed Score

Phi mosso

■* 1 p « p » e 1_ 0 v P "p U
»
P' v p * p '_

A lle q ro 152?
tn.r. W.W.I-BkCtl.l

« j a » ^ **"4 a « 4 ♦ j

f " .----- . ~ ~ r ~ f=i . / / r ? , ■" r _ ' _ ■ 1

Tpu. J 4 1. Hat. Tifti. Ju.

----------- f l . f -----------^ 1 : .1 K l « 1 L
■■■■'. p 4 _ P ■■ “ U P ■' V ■ p- - '8 - p H — ' ~*
* * ♦
4il Bum
DkCl*. h ill

I i ^

1 ( P i A lle g re tto c o n g r u U ( . » 116)


US’*
V 1 y jT"
-p-r=— 1J&— _______ —1
'i-j'py f ■
9-
1■
_;79 9 HLJ?'’
b O f” r
f n i hi
'»* 'T— ~'Vl~r~i
\n
o
51

Mm. 55-75

Phrases: 53_______ 55, 56 64, 65 70, 71

Meter: 4/4 5/4 4/4 3/4

Tempo: ( J = 152) C J = 116)

The first change in meter, m. '56— 5/4, results in the

addition of one beat to the melodic phrase which begins at

m. 53. The essential thematic idea does not change, but the

rhythmic character of the idea is simply lengthened by one

beat per measure. The following change to 4/4, m. 65,isa

return to the thematic idea of mm. 53-55. The gesture

differentiation between these three sections will not be

overly evident, but a change in the condu cto r’s physical

bearing must signal the new section, along with the change

in beat pattern.

The metric change to 3/4, m. 71 will present a larger

change in the gestures due to the slower tempo and the

concomitant change to legato articulation.

(c) D Y NAM ICS . Any change in the volume level, or the

use of a dynamic ornament within a particular level, e.g.,

accent, sfp, crescendo, etc. (See Example 4)

G e st u r e : A dynamic change is reflected in the size and

intensity of the conducting motion.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
52

Generic definition; The nature of various dynamic

characteristics remains the same for all music regardless of

the sound source.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
53

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
54

Mm. 63-70

Phrases: 63__ 6 6 , ' h 70,*71_______

Meter: 2/2

Tempo: Vif ( J =112)

The dynamic change from mezzo piano to mezzo forte

between the first two phrases must be reflected by an

increased intensity of the gesture accompanied by raising

the ictus of the beat pattern and using a slightly more

expansive gesture which suggests more of an invitation to

play louder.

The preparation for m. 702 cannot preempt completion of

the mezzo forte phrase of m. 70*, therefore, it must not

appear too early.

(d) RHYTHM. Conducting gestures reflect changes in

rhythmic patterns. As rhythmic structures change within

sections of significant length, the attitude of the gesture

must change to reflect the rhythm, especially where

expressive performance of the rhythm requires an

interpre ive gesture. (See Example 5)

Ges tur e: The long and short relationships of the

rhythmic setting should, on occasion be reflected in the

gesture. This will be especially true with certain

homorhythmic settings.

_______________________

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
55

Generic de fi nition: A progression of sonic events

which occurs in durational values composed in units of time,

e.g., seconds, milli-seconds, etc., without necessarily

containing a common unit of metered division or variously

restated patterns.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
56

&

-3-
ON

1
M
i
—l
•H
s

•H
u
PI

•V
a>
0
1
ca

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
;

57

Mm■ 6 7 —7 4

Phrases: 67___ 68,'69___ 73,73

Meter: C

Tempo: Anime

The constant eighth-note rhythm beginning at m. 69i

provides an abrupt rhythmic contrast to the preceding

syncopated melodic rhythm. The nature of the gesture

preceding m. 69 should be more animated to reflect the

buoyant rhythm. Prior to the eighth-note anacrusis to m.

69, the nature of the gesture must reflect a more focused

and stolid gesture to show the sameness of the upcoming

eighth-note pattern.

M. 733 begins another rhythmic contrast with the return

of the more animated sixteenth-note rhythm in the woodwind

lines. The gesture must reflect the animated attitude of

the rhythm. Additionally, the focus of the gesture may be

in the upper third of the gesture field* in order to reflect

the high woodwind register.

(e) LINEAR W RIT I N G . Change of writing style, in whole

or part, such as from a chordal style to a more linear form

of writing. (See Example 6)

2 "Gesture field" is a CASP term referring to the area


around the c o ndu cto r’s body which is defined by the radius
of the c on d u c t o r ’s gestures.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
58

Ge st ure : The conducting motions will reflect a

homophonic setting by guiding the rhythm of the setting.

The rhythmic independence of lines that characterize a

polyphonic setting will be reflected in gesture which simply

marks the meter and articulation nature of the passage.

Generic d efi nition: The combination of sound that

results from the simultaneous performance of several aural

events, or the presentation of contrasting aural events,

each of which may be heard distinctly when all are combined.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Example 6: Suite Francaise, II


Textural Change via LINEAR WRITING
Source: Darius Milhaud, Suite Francaise (New York: MCA Music, 19U7)» PP* 26-27«

J ---- ,— I-------
■ i-z— ■ hrr ____
:2 ^ ___
j I - ■ .n
1 1 "
> 1 ■■■ - ^ g " > *1
*-%- - - - 1- - - - - - - - - - 1- - - - -
J i| 1 -—
&
|f j ■= ■ =

£o] rc tt.

' ! & = - I. . *
k ^ / I ■ ‘ ' 1 1" ■ 1 1 1 1
t^ T ’ _ -- 1
i2S
1_
V
-/ 1 1 -■ 1-T-----
r ' ------- 1 IJ,
,
____ ^ g ? >g__ ?___,
- -- i
■/-j,--- -...................... ---- ■------- ■
*J 1 ■- -1 ....- ■1' ” "^ vn
vo
60

E lf f l , 4 3 - 5 £ 4

Phrases: 43__________ 50, 51__ 52,

Theme: 82 A2

Meter: 6/8

Tempo: Lent ( =116)

Mm. 43-50 present a widely spaced homophonic setting

which contrasts with the unison line presented by the

woodwinds at mm. 51-52.

(f) A RTI CULATION. Varying degrees of legato and non-

legato articulation. (See Example 7)

Ge s t u r e : The conducting motion conveys note length and

the degree of dynamic and intensity emphasis.

Generic de f i n i t i o n : The nature of various articulation

types remains the same for all music, regardless of the

sound source.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

/
Example Ji Suite Francaise, III
Textural Change via ARTICULATION
Source: Darius Milhaud., Suite Francaise (New York: MCA Music, 1Sh7)» PP* 38-39*
62

Mm. 4 5 -5 2

Phra se s: 45__ 4 8 2 , '~49__5Q , 'si_______

Meter: 2/2

Tempo: Vif ( J =112)

Mm. 45-482 present legato passages in the woodwinds

which contrast with the detached style of mm. 49-50

manifested by the eighth-note and sixteenth-note rhythms. A

milder contrast, closer in style to the legato phrases of

mm. 4 5 - 4 8 2 , i s provided by the detached eighth-note lines of

mm. 51+ .

(g) ORCHESTRATION. Variations will include the color

nature of the instruments used, the number of instruments

used and the doublings employed, the chord spacings, and the

register used within the range of the instrument.3 All of

these before mentioned factors will combine to describe the

density of the sound as light, medium, or heavy. (See

Example 8)

Ge st u r e : The focal area (high, middle, low) of the

conducting gesture within the gesture field should be

reflected in the related registral emphasis of the

orchestration. In cases where the orchestration is written

in two to three distinct registers, the conducting pattern

3 Quentin R. Nordgren, "A Measure Of Textural Patterns


and Strengths," Journal of Music T h e o r y , 4 (1960): 19.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
63

should be located ir> a compromised "middle" area. Thinly

scored passages are conducted with lighter, more delicate

gesture— depending upon the dynamic. As the orchestration

approaches the fullness of tutti scoring, the gestures

become more firm and more intense as compared with the

lighter scorings.

Generic definition: Density is described by the

overall sound structure which is created by the number and

variety of sonic combinations.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
6U

ON h*
-=t
-=r

P4

-=r
ON

O
•H
03
3
S
§
M
EH S
0)
03
•H co
ctf
o
i J* -I I i I i « : I :
£ 1cd s3e
I '

CD ■H
-P >
•H
3 0) < D
CQ
ca
flj
•# ,43
CO o
<1) r—t £
r-f CS

s 1§ -P
ra -P *H
MM 3
W d) 03
EH
»

1
AS
•s

1
s
«
m
0)
o
u
o
C/3

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
65

Mm. 14-27

Phrases: _________16,17______ 24,25________

Meter: 2/4

Tempo: Lent ( J =58-60)

Beginning at m. 17, the color emphasis changes from the

preceding moderate middle register saxophone density to a

higher register and lighter density woodwind sound. This

lightness must be reflected in the attitude of the gesture

and a rise in the area of the beat pattern.

The two measure diminuendo (mm. 23-24) prepares the

following contrast in orchestration which begins at m. 25.

The contrast is characterized by substantial reduction in

the number of instruments correlated with a similar

reduction in dynamic. Since musical elements may act

independently of one another, contrasting sections may be

joined by a dovetailing of different elements. While

certain elements act to achieve closure on one section,

certain other elements may act in contrary fashion to

initiate the following section. At these connective points,

the conductor must decide which textural elements need to be

conducted, and at what point during the juncture the

condu cto r’s attention must be shifted to the next section.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
66

#4. Identification of emotional energy s e q u e n c e .

The emotional nature of the cond uct or ’s interpretation

is founded upon variations in “physiological equilibrium

called homeostasis" (Barra, p. 4), i.e., when the listener

responds to the musical stimuli whose energy levels match

normal levels of physiological function, the listener feels

most comfortable— experiences a state of homeostasis.


\

However, when musical stimuli lie outside the physiological

comfort range, at either end of the scale, tension is

created and the tension seeks resolution.

Musical stimuli consists of energy which elicits

tension and release in the listener, and this internal

combination of tension and release factors becomes the

l i ste ner ’s emotional experience (8oult, p. 21). As the

emotional content of the music changes, the conductor must

also experience that change and be ready to communicate the

change to the players, and subsequently, to the audience.

The fluctuation of emotional energy is a fact of

musical composition that has remained consistent throughout

music history (Music R e v i e w , 1983, p. 305). 8y marking the

entire score in terms of tension and release according to a

subjective evaluation of the w o r k ’s emotional progression,

the conductor will more accurately communicate the

individual musical phrases, and ultimately, the overall

interpretation.

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
67

The conductor should mark the score in terms of tension

and release above the measures which initiate the sections

so described by using the letter (T) for tension producing

sections and (R) for sections producing release or

relaxation. Musical elements whose manipulation will

produce either tension or relaxed emotional energy include

the following: orchestration register and density, tempo,

rhythm, meter, articulation, dynamics, linear writing style

(e.g., homophonic, contrapuntal), degree of harmonic

consonance and dissonance, and the nature of the harmonic

rhythm. While the number of variations in factors producing

either tension or release are infinite, a general

description of the most common characteristic tension and

release factors effecting emotional energy will be helpful:

Tension Traits Relaxation Traits

Emphasis on frequent Emphasis on infrequent


elemental change within a change.
comparatively brief time
frame.

Complexity of linear Simplicity of linear


writing, e.g., polyphony. writing, e.g., homophony.

Increase in the amount, Decrease in the amount,


number, or nature of an number, or nature of an
element, e.g., dynamic, el e men t.
orchestration.

Emphasis on complex Emphasis on simple harmony


harmony and frequent and infrequent harmonic
harmonic change. change.

Fast tempo. Moderate to slow tempo.

High or low register. Middle register.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
68

There are basically three feelingful levels of

emotional energy: (1) Stability— -the feeling caused by a

sustained event; (2) Local Activity— a type of forward

movement not characterized as decisive forward movement; and

(3) Directional'Motion— a feeling of definite directional

movement.

The following symbols are used for indicating these

three types of movement in terms of tension and release

levels and their combinations. The symbols should be placed

along the top of the score above the measures which initiate

the change in emotional energy. These symbols are intended

to define the emotive energy of the motion, but not the

degree of related intensity. The information contained

within the score will indicate these subtle details.

Stability. The unchanging nature of a section.

(See Example 9A)

1. (T— “-) Tension is sustained.

2. (R ) Release or relaxation is sustained.


R m
3. - o r ) Both tension and release elements

are composed to occur concurrently.

When combined in this fashion, a heightened

emotional response usually occurs (Barra,

p. 12), but in these cases a movement of

energy is negligible..

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
69

4. (TR ) Alternating blocks of tension and

release, which occur in linear sequence, and

produce musical events at a constant

energy level.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
70

CT\ m
-3
cA ft
-3-
&
■O P< I
g
o •>
M 1
3
>< ON
^

O
•H
03
H
t> C
«P
S £
<5
§
o> a
CO
I
I 1. ? >'

s
'— — c a a a t^ q ia
&
« T
M s
a)
© 3
•H
CO «H
o
*• q)

<3! ^ Hi-
ON O
+>
a> o
rH Cd
Pt P=« 0)
-p
3 Cd •H
X *H
« > CO
a>
a
05 05
o rH
»H
05
u
+» «i
X
0) 9 m.
6 P
•a
0)
o
§

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
71

Mm. 14-27

Phrases: 16,17______ 24,25______

Emotional Energy: 14_16, 17___21,22_24,25_____

R— T— R->* R---

Meter: 2/4

Tempo: Lent ( J =58-60)

The overall texture which brings the phrase to a close

at m. 16 achieves Release (R ) through the use of a mezzo

forte dynamic, slow tempo, middle register, legato

articulation, arched phrase shape at mm. 15-16, and longer

note values. Beginning0=ai m. 17, a subito tensional effect

(T ) is achieved through the use of a sudden forte

dynamic, widening of the overall register through the

simultaneous orchestration of upper woodwinds and low brass,

and the use of quicker rhythmic values. The increase in

Release (R— » begins in m. 22 with the descending melodic

line, and is then reinforced by the decrescendo of mm. 23-

24. Release is achieved at m. 25+ through substantially

reduced instrumentation, return to longer note values, the

much softer dynamic, and the repeated, descending, ostinato-

like phrase in the lower voices.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
72

B. Local Activity & Directional Motio n. The

difference between Local Activity and Directional

Motion is found in the intensity of forward

movement. Only one symbol (---» is used to

indicate movement. The symbol does not define the

amount or nature of the movement; only that move­

ment towards tension or relaxation is in progress.

(See Example 9A, mm. 22-24)

1. (T----->) Gradual increase in tension.

2. (R----- >) Gradual increase in the amount of

release or relaxation.
R T
3. ( rj — >• » ) Gradual increase in energy of

thevertically combined tension and release

areas.

4. (TR >) Gradual increase in energy through

settings which consist of the alternation of

tension and release.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Example 9B: Suite Francaise, III


Textural Change via Factors of Vertically Combined TENSION and RELEASE
Source: Darius Milhaud, Suite Francaise (New York: MCA Music, 19h7)» PP» 3h-3%»
(L

23 . rr.f t
• j i ■■ ■: _ -A ---------- -
..1
4j»jp -
-
■1 I 1 ■*
f i £ > - P r 7 - ......................
, .1 1■ T > rV - - -
it 1 . * 1 l.t
11 ly ■ HU , 1^-

. ,4 . -
a —1 J -■ J ----- - * 1
* "* ' "■*'' ■*’ r U | r 'r --------

‘S - r r - , . .rW k ' 1 - r ' ^ 1


1 — — * i 1 1 1

g • t ^ m »■ jj. ^ ___________
,1 I 1 " 'f if e | =

-ii-- - ■ ! ■ ■ - h —
•ir ^
00] y
. rt >:

‘ j
■m ■** ** T ~ !r
>Ufc_U^_____ '■'■ * - -- ZZI * "•*
~i 1 1 r

‘ ,4 "■ ■ '■ — 1 1 1
1 T m n i . n ^ f..« -------le-------- - t r ' t F. . J l A s U l L --------------- ,
1 » i|n > ■ ' 1-i-in----- !■■■ :.=
[ I______________ | in I r _ 1______________
J i L?» Ai—jjjiS iZ p S S ti J.I|Jm„ i ■1 .. —
T» j j

-J
V<J
74

Mm. 2 9 -3 3

Phrases: ________ 30, 31 32, 33_________

Emotional Energy: 2 9 ______ 32

Meter: (£,

Tempo: Vif ( J =112)

The superimposition of Release and Tension factors is

created by the legato lines of the woodwinds (R ) which

sound over the detached eighth-note movement of the

trombones and tuba (T ).

#5. Selection of instruments to be c u e d .

A system of marking cues in the score will enable the

conductor to cue the players, and will also assist in the

con d u c t o r ’s reading of the score.4 In fact, in order to

prepare a score for rehearsal or performance on short

notice, the marking of cues in the score will be of critical

importance for the conductor’s confidence and the resulting

effect on the ensemble, which, in turn, should lead to the

best possible interpretation under the circumstances.

Decisions regarding which instruments to cue should be

made while practice-conducting the work through at

performance tempi. The conductor may tend to make

unnecessary cues if the choice of cues is made during silent

4 Rudolph Fellner, "DND: A Better Way Of Marking


Orchestra Scores," Opera J o u r n a l , 25 (1982): 10-14.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
75

score study without consideration for the proper tempo. Too

many cues will result in a cluttered gesture field, and

unnecessary signals with which the players must deal.

'Overcuing’ may help at initial readings of a work but

should be culled so that only those that are absolutely

necessary are left.

Cuing motions should be performed in a physioally

eloquent manner. The cuing motion should reflect the nature

of the m u s i c ’s motion. Cues should not be executed in a

mechanical way, but should be a part of the mood and motion

of the music. Cuing motions must be choreographed at proper

performance tempi. The motions must be smooth and

deliberate. Every effort should be made to avoid looking

awkward. Cues that are performed in an awkward manner, or

amid a cluttered gesture field, will unduly focus the

attention of players and audience on the conducting

gestures, and thereby distract from the aural comprehension.

CASP marks cues in the score by means of a penciled

check mark placed at the beginning of the instrument’s line.

As the conductor reads the page, the check mark immediately

draws attention to the instrument and line to be cued. An

abbreviation for the instrument to be cued may be written on

the extended line of the checkmark. Thus, the instrument

and its notation are seen immediately by the conductor.

As stated in Chapter 1, personal and psychological

rapport between conductor and players is of critical

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
76

importance to the finest performances. Personal contact

between conductor and ensemble members will be enhanced by

the nature of the cuing gestures and by eye contact. When

cuing a section of the ensemble, it is more effective to

look at those members seated in distant chairs, rather than

to continually dwell on first chair players. By looking at

distant chairs, the c onductor’s view will encompass the

entire section. A cuing code, discussed on pp. 167-168, may

be used as an abbreviated method of assigning cue rationale.

#6. Identification of phrase lengths.

The identification of phrase lengths clarifies the

conductor’s understanding of structural groupings on a

foreground level— i.e., the level being immediately

communicated by the conducting gesture as the music unfolds.

Phrasal understanding is critical for learning asymmetrical

groupings. The groupings of various phrase lengths on the

foreground level gradually combine to form larger sections,

or hierarchical groupings, thus enabling the conductor to

learn the overall proportional development of the work.

When combined with the placement of M M M ’s, this technique is

useful for arriving at good performance pacing.5 Of

especial importance is the accurate conducting of the

5Grosvenor Cooper and Leonard B. Meyer, Rhythmic


Structure of Music (Chicago: University of Chicago Press,
1960).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
77

asymmetrical phrase lengths. The structural understanding

of phrase groupings is a major aid in memorization.

As a work is being performed, the level of immediacy

for the conductor, players, and listeners is at the phrase

level. In large measure, substantive differences in musical

interpretation lie in the interpreter’s handling of phrase

interpretation via tempo, dynamic shadings, and

articulation. The numerical indication of measured phrase

lengths is placed at.the top of the score above the initial

measure of each phrase length. Thus, the conductor is

immediately made aware of phrase lengths.

#7. Identification of form, thematic content, and major


tonal a r e a s .

During Level I analysis, the conductor does not

intentionally follow a process of re- composition in order

to determine rationale for various structural relationships.

The process of explaining the measure-by-measure detail of

the work will be the objective of the Level III analysis.

This part of the Level I analysis identifies and marks large

sections of structural material according to formal

structure, thematic origin, and tonal areas. The purpose

here is to realize the conceptual groupings of material that

immediately have an impact on the players, and to use this

material in order to understand the relationship between

structural features previously marked in Level I analyses

and the formal development of the work. The formal outline

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
78

created for this particular area of Level I analysis will

serve as the point of departure for the Level III analysis

process of 're-composition.’

The following content abbreviations are used to label

thematic material: principal themes will be indicated by

capital Arabic letters, while secondary themes derived from

the principal themes will be marked by small case Arabic

letters which correspond to the principal theme letter from

which they are derived: a,b,c, etc. Thematic content is

identified at the bottom of the score below the measure

where each theme begins.

#8. Identification of conductor proble ms.

A reliable, comprehensive, and rationale technique will

allow conductors to arrive at good decisions for accurate

gestural communications. Measures involving some difficulty

of a gestural or interpretive nature should be marked at the

top of the score above the initial measure of the difficult

section. It is critical for the conductor to master areas

of gestural difficulty in order to earn and maintain player

confidence and respect as well as to carry on an efficient

rehearsal.

Gestural difficultues often consist of the need to

combine, simultaneously or in quick juxtaposition, two or

more contrasting types of gestures. The gesture

choreography that results from careful consideration of the

area must be practiced slowly so that the complete physical

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
79

feeling of the gesturing is totally comfortable. Areas of

conductor difficulty should be marked at the top of the

score as study reminders.

#9. Identification of player p r obl em s.

The finest conductors exhibit empathy for the technical

concerns of their players. By reviewing the score to

identify player difficulties, the conductor can consider

possibilities to assist the pla y e r s ’ technique (especially

necessary with young or inexperienced players), or to

legitimately alter the interpretation or retouch the

orchestration in order to assist the execution of a

particular passage. A thorough knowledge of instrumental

playing techniques and orchestration procedures are required

to assure the conductor’s success in this area. Measures

involving player difficulty should be marked at the top of

the score as study reminders.

#10. Synthesis of Level I analysis results.

Each time the conductor reads through the score to

collect Level I data, a synthesis of that information occurs

along with score information not under immediate scrutiny.

Each reading of the work expands the c o ndu ct or’s

comprehensive view. Ultimately, the conductor must achieve

a congruent impression of the whole work in order to make

decisions regarding the various components of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
80

interpretation. The comprehensive view of the whole work is

enhanced by a technique of graphing certain Level I data.

Barra (1983) and White (1984) promote graphing

techniques for the sake of making various structural

comparisons within a composition or among many compositions

of a particular composer, style, or historical period.

Boult (1968, p. 21) suggests constructing "an emotional plot

of the work" by noting the sections of tension and release

as well as climactic moments within the score. The

overviews gained from such graphing techniques assist the

conductor in developing a thorough grasp of structural

evolution, especially for works of considerable length. The

graphing technique presented by CASP includes both objective

structure and emotional progression.

For the most efficient comprehension of overall design,

CASP suggests graphing Level I information along four

separate continuums that define the relative placement of

structural information in the following order: (1) Major

Musical Moments— denoting principal climactic areas, (2) the

progression of emotional energy plotted by marking the areas

of tension and release (T and R, respectively) which will

give the emotional ebb and flow of the work, (3) points of

significant textural change requiring an adjustment in the

nature or size of the conducting gesture, and (4) a formal

outline of the work based upon thematic development and

indicating sectional proportions, and key areas. See Figure

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Figure I * Basal Structure Analysis Chart
I. Normandie
Suite FrancaiBe;

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
82

Level II: REVIEW OF SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

This analysis level addresses the importance of

acquiring background information regarding (1) the reason

for which the work was composed, (2) the lifestyle and

cultural environment of the composer during the creation of

the work, and (3) a review of the circumstances surrounding

the first performance of the work which include the

contemporary instruments employed, the contemporary

acoustical environment of the performance, and performance

practices.

The acquisition of background information concerning

composer and composition serves several functions. It adds

a perceptive measure to music-making by enabling the

conductor to comprehend and interpret the work as an entity

that developed as part of a human life. The exploration of

social, economic, political, intellectual, philosophical,

and emotional factors which surrounded the c om p o s e r ’s

creative process facilitates the c o n d uc tor ’s attempt to

interpret the mood of the work.

It is also useful to view a musical work as one part of

a composer’s developing craftsmanship. By analyzing several

major works of a given composer, the conductor can identify

certain common stylistic features that may assist in

explaining the nature of emotion-effecting events in the

music. Understanding the stylistic descriptors of a

particular composer and the related style period increases

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
S3

the probability of discovering innovative features of

compositional structure, thereby noting new or unique

developments in the contemporary musical style.

The comparison of a work's stylistic features to a

co mp o s e r ’s total output, and to similar compositions of

other periods, heightens the co nductor’s emotional and

intellectual involvement with the work. The personalization

of the cond uc tor ’s inquiry becomes an inextricable part of

the interpretation.

CASP suggests the establishment of reference base

files. These should contain three sections of stylistic

data which stem from the information collected from Level II

analysis: (i) biographical information of composer and

his/her cultural environment, (2) stylistic features of the

composition under study, and (3) stylistic features of the

historical period during which the work was composed. A

complete data collection system to accompany the CASP

analysis format is provided in Appendix A.

Level III: DETAILED STRUCTURAL ANALYSIS

The Detailed Structural Analysis represents the most

detailed approach to score preparation for the conductor.

Thoughtful consideration of musical details and their

relationships within a particular structure will serve to

reinforce the critical performance decisions regarding

interpretation as indicated in Level I analysis. Through

this process of detailed analysis, the conductor must not

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
84

lose the relative concept of the whole work (Boult, p. 20).

For example, conductors may become involved with analytical

details to the exclusion of creating a beautiful melodic

line (Fuchs, p. 22).

As the conductor examines the details of the

composition, the analyses must always include reference to

the larger formal development (Moe, p. 9). Understanding

structural evolution becomes clearer as relationships are

drawn between detail and overall form, as well as between

the individual details. A knowledge of score detail should

no more interfere with conductor comprehension than that of

the sculptor or painter (Fuchs, p. 34). The conductor

should be able to shift from one structural level to another

structural level without "cognitive confusion."

The common juncture between Level I and Level III

analyses is the graph of overall formal structure and

thematic development. This graph becomes the final

consideration of Level I analysis and the first

consideration, or point of departure, for Level III

analysis.

Level III analysis continues the concept of deductive

comprehension that is basic to the CASP process. Level III

analysis asks the conductor to re-create the compositional

process and clarify the interaction of form and process in

the music being studied. This process leads to a reasonable

interpretation of the composition. In a continual effort to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
85

increase the interpreter’s familiarity with the work, there

should be an understanding of the "technique of composition

and the creative forces involved" (8amberger, p. 223).

Through Level I analysis, the conductor deals with the

work as it unfolds for the sake of formulating clear and

precise gesture choreography. Level I analysis does not

demand that a structural rationale be drawn among the formal

sections, although a peripheral awareness of obvious

relationships among phrases and larger sections is

inevitable. Level III analysis, however, asks the conductor

to explain the progression of structural events (Cone, p.

174; White, p. 5).

Level III analysis consists of two sub-divisions: Part

1 is an assessment of the overall structure of the work;

Part 2 constructs a detailed phrasal analysis of the work.

The effect of a Level III analysis will be to structurally

reinforce the comprehensive view gained from the Level I

analysis. The detailed analysis will draw attention to

various unifying factors that create structural cohesion and

lucid expression. This type of detailed analysis helps the

conductor to understand the structural shape at various

hierarchical levels through the energy-intensive emphases

and release of large formal shapes.

Level I emotive structures are explored in more detail

at Level III. Since the appreciation of music consists of

intellectual and emotional responsiveness, it is important

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
86

to translate all structural detail into their emotional

components.

The results of Level III analysis will serve several

functions: (1) to structurally reinforce the performance

rationale of the comprehensive vie-; ; • r<?d from Level I

analysis, (2) to focus on stylistic unifying factors which

result in structural cohesiveness, and (3) to translate

structural events into designs of emotional energy.

Level III: PART 1— Assessment of Overall Structure

(A) A view of the overall form should define the

compo se r’s basic structural approach. Any uniqueness or

peculiarity of formal structures, compared to similar

compositions of the same historical period, should be noted.

(8) The overall structure is defined by the evaluation

of symmetry and unifying factors among movements or

sections. Comparisons should identify large scale

similarities and contrasts among movements such as forms,

key centers, instrumentation, etc.

(C) If the composition employs instrumental and vocal

writing, the relationship betweer- the two must be drawn.

To begin the detailed evaluation of the work, it is

useful to construct a chart of the overall structure which

contains basic elemental material in order to acquire some

sense of progression through the broad component outlines.

A CASP Level III Overview Chart will consist of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
87

following elements (See Example 12): movement, type of

form, tempo, meter, orchestration, key, textual content (if

applicable), and an additional comments section. The

"additional comments" section may note large formal

relationships between movements or sections.

This large overview will give the conductor an

impression of movement through variations in the use of

tempo, density of orchestration, text (if applicable), and

key area. Each of these elements will contribute to the

hierarchical structuring of tension and release, and the

subsequent temporal pacing of the composition being studied.

LEVEL III: Part 2 — Detailed Structural Analysis

This assessment begins with an overview of the movement

or section by the construction of a chart according to

phrase structure. Isolated phrases chosen for analysis from

the overview chart will include tonal movement and indicate

thematic similarities and contrasts between individual

measures or phrases. These isolated representations of

phrase structures are called "schemes." An appropriate

sense of visual proportion should exist when representing

phrase lengths visually. These phrasal charts and schemes

continue to foster comprehensive learning through the

continually relating of detail to overall structure.

For example, the following scheme represents the

initial outline of a phrasal structure:

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
88

Example 10: Phrasal Outline Scheme

1 I---------------- 1
3/4 1 .12,13_____ 22 23 __ 26,27_____ 36

12 10 4 10
(6+6 ) (6+4) (2+2) (5+5)

D A D

13-14=1-2 Orch: 23 - 3 6 W 1-2

15-16W3-4

The above scheme presents two parts of a movement, 36

measures in length, divided into two large structural

sections and performed in 3/4 meter. The opening section is

divided into two phrases of twelve measures and ten

measures, respectively, while the closing section divides

into two phrases of four measures and ten measures,

respectively. Each large phrase is subdivided into two

clauses which are indicated in parentheses below the large

phrase length indication. Note that a comma is used to

separate the phrases within each bracketed section. The

tonal progression is indicated by capital letters (small

case letters would indicate minor modes). Below the tonal

progression, phrase relationships are designated (" = " means

"the same as"; “ £ 3 ," means "similar to” ). Unless otherwise

indicated, designations such as 13-14=1-2 and 15-16£i3-4

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
89

refer to similarities in melodic structure. The designation

"Orch: 23-26££l-2" specifically refers to the similarity in

orchestration of both phrases. These detailed schemes are

generally constructed according to the thematic phraseology

of the work.

The discussions which follow focus on the kinds of

information which the conductor finds unique or of special

interest in relation to the movement being examined. For

example:

Example 11: Phrasal Outline Scheme Using Text

Measures: 433 _50,502 592, 684 792

582 693

L e ng t h : 7 9 11 10

Vo ic ing : AT2B STT AT 28 STTB

Text: Domine Deus Qui tollis Qui tollis Qui sedes.

Miserere suscipe

Cadence

notes

The contrast in content between Example 10 and Example

11 is clear. "Voicing" refers to vocal part, e.g., A =

alto, T2 = second tenor. The "Text” information describes

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
90

the text setting by giving the beginning text of each

phrase. In the "Measures" line, 433 means "measure 43

beginning on beat 3" and the phrase ,r582___________ 693" is

illustrated as overlapping with phrases "502_______ 592" and

"684_______ 792".

In addition to the various shorthand symbols found in

Example 10, i.e., (=), (££), (5 0 2 ), the following phrase

descriptions may be used:

mm. 4, 5 means "anacrusis to m. 5";

mm. 4 ^ 5 means ”m. 4 material ends during m. 5";

mm. 6,7 indicates "a complex overlap of phrase cadences

and beginnings";
•V"
m. 4 indicates "a pivot bai the end of one phrase and

the beginning of the next."

The following two examples provide additional

categories which may serve to illustrate a comprehensive

view. The data for each category is listed across the page

from left to right:

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 12: Description of Overall Design

Composer (dates):

Title of Composition:

Move m e n t :

Tempo:

Meter:

Type or Char ac ter :

Orchestration:

Formal Cont ent :

Ke^:

Additional C omm ent s:

(Broad comparisons between movements)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
92

Example 13: Description of Sectional Design With Detail

Phrase L eng ths :

Voicing:

Measure of Vocal E n t r y :

Entry V o i c e :

Text:

Cadence V o i c e s :

Measure of Cad e n c e :

Cadence Pitches:

Another type of chart may be used to indicate sections

according to text, length of textual section, and tonality.

These items are found in Example 14 and are as follows:

Example 14: Byrd. Mass For Three V o i c e s : Credo

(Final Large Section)

I. Et in spiritum sanctum... Mm. 106-117 C

II. Qui cum patri et filio... Mm. 118-131 C-(G)-D

III. Et unam sanctam Catholicam. Mm. 132-142 d-(C)-C

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
93

In order to clarify and relate thematic and

developmental motives and phrases, letter and number labels

which correspond to inserted manuscript examples, may be

added to the phrase schemes. For example, capital letters

(A,B,C) indicate principal thematic ideas, capital letters

with Arabic numerals indicate the various phrases of a

particular theme section, e.g., A1,A2,A3 (the A theme is

presented in three varied phrases. Variations of principal

thematic ideas are indicated with superscript numbers

(All,A12,atc .), and small Arabic letters (a,b,c) indicate

subordinate or secondary motives or phrases derived from

their related principal thematic ideas. Variation of

secondary thematic material would be indicated by the use of

superscript numerals, e.g., all, al*, etc. (See Example

15. )

Example 15: Phrasal Outline With Thematic Indications

2/4 1____4 , 5___ A _____ Al_____ 20.21 A21 28,29___ a ___ 36

8 + 8 8 + 8

The discussion which follows a particular phrasal

scheme is directed at the 're-composition’ of the phrasal

scheme. The use of musical elements involved in that

particular discussion must address the scheme, or related

elements which occur elsewhere in the piece. The discussion

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
94

following each outline scheme need not explain every phrase

of the outline. Only the phrases of the outline that offer

unique aspects of compositional technique or style need be

presented for discussion.

The detailed discussion of phrasal breakdowns is used

to trace the c o m p o s e r ’s structural process within the phrase

and as compared to other areas of the composition. These

discussions may include a relationship to the c o m p o s e r ’s

general stylistic tendencies, or to the contemporary

historical style period. The order of topic discussion need

not follow any prescribed pattern. As stated above, the

choice of material to be discussed should be drawn from

prominent or unique compositional features. This material

may include the melodic character, the relationship of the

text to the form, the impression of the opening texture, or

the tonal character and its relationship to text and form.

Application of CASP Results: The Cond uct or ’s Interpretation

The results of preparing a score through a CASP

analysis will serve three basic functions for the conductor:

(1) assist in formulating an interpretation of the

c om pos er ’s intentions, (2) enable the communication of

interpretation to the ensemble, and (3) the communication of

the c om p o s e r ’s intentions to the listeners. The basic CASP

formulations -from which these results are derived include:

(1) information regarding the origin of the composition, (2)

comprehension of the w o r k ’s overall structure, (3) the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
95

sequence of emotional moods, and (4) an awareness of how the

audience may react to the interpretation (Boult, p. 20).

For all practical purposes, an audience reacts to the

immediacy of the music (Cone, 196B). The large majority of

listeners do not comprehend the structural evolution of

music, especially of lengthy works. Therefore, when the

c o n duc to r’s preparation time becomes limited, the

c o nd uc tor ’s understanding of how the audience perceives the

music is significant for the establishment of a priority

routine for score preparation. When preparation time is

limited, the Level I analysis is sufficient to render an

interpretation of the score.

Furthermore, it is of paramount importance to realize

that, as a result of a CASP analysis, the conductor will

formulate the interpretation through the selection of

variations in tempo, dynamics, and articulation. These

variations are the three principal means of rendering unique

interpretive expression by a conductor or any performing

musician (Green and Malko, p. 22). Therefore, the conductor

translates the structural features of a work into emotive

events through the manipulation of these three elements.

Arthur Nikisch once commented: "...the details of my

interpretation change at nearly every concert, in accord

with the emotions that are especially aroused in me"

(Bamberger, p. 313). Other famous conductors have also

drawn a relationship between emotions and interpretation.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
96

Therefore, if one were to merge the Nikisch quote and the

Green and Malko information, it appears that there is a very

strong link between emotional involvement and expressivity

through tempo, dynamics, and articulation. Additionally,

Barra (1983) states that the principle of interpretation is

based upon a turn-of-the-century (20th century) theory of

emotions.

Articulation, Dynamics, Tempo

Of all the musical elements at the c o mpo se r’s disposal,

the elements which most often remain beyond his/her control

in performance are the shadings of articulation, dynamics,

and tempo. Consequently, given two interpretations of the

same piece, and assuming that all other musical elements are

performed correctly, a preference for one interpretation

over the other will be defined in terms of the interpreter’s

handling of articulation, dynamics, and tempo.

The varying of elements in the interpretations of

inexperienced musicians are often marked by the same type of

change in all three of these elements. For example, if a

passage increases in tempo, it will often also increase in

dynamic level and in the length of the notes; and if a

passage decreases in tempo, it will often also decrease in

dynamic level and the articulations may become shorter.

These responses are common knowledge, especially among

conductors of younger, inexperienced ensembles, and are

natural human reactions. Therefore, the mark of an

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
97

experienced interpreter must be the ability to vary each of

the three principal expressive elements independently

(Barra, p. 17). Additionally, it is useful for the

conductor to know that when any unnatural combination is

presented, e.g., loud dynamic and slow tempo, there is an

increase in tensional response from the player and the

listener. Any combination of elements that are opposites

tend to heighten tension.

Of the three expressive elements, several major

conductors cite the choice of tempo as the most critical.

The proper articulation depends upon the influence of tempo

on the rhythm. The tempo must allow the rhythmic

development to evolve in a logical and clearly articulated

manner. Reverberation time of the performance site will

also affect the choice of tempo.

The results of the complete CASP analysis process will

include the c o n du cto r’s decisions regarding choice of tempi,

extent of dynamic range, and shading of articulation

lengths; and the selection of gestures to effect these

interpretive ideas. These three areas are the cond uct or ’s

principal means of translating the printed score into a

convincing and meaningful interpretation.

t: '

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER IV

ANALYSIS FOR PERFORMANCE OF DARIUS MILHAUD'S


SUITE FRANCAISE FOR CONCERT BAND

Darius Milhaud (1892-1974), Suite Francaise (New York: MCA


Music, 1947). Full Score.

Level I; BASAL STRUCTURE ANALYSIS

#1. Review of preliminary comments regarding interpretation


and special instructions printed in the s c o r e .

Special instructions which address interpretation or

stage set-up are not indicated in the score.

The instrumentation is that of the standard concert

band as set forth by the College Band Directors' National

Association and remains the same for each of the five

movements. Some variation does occur with the

instrumentation for percussion. The percussion

instrumentation appears as follows:

First Movement: Timpani, snare drum, crash cymbals.

Second M ov eme nt: Timpani.

Third Movement: Timpani, snare drum, cymbals, bass

drum.

Fourth M o vem en t: Timpani, snare drum, cymbals, bass

drum.

Fifth Movement: Timpani, snare drum, military drum

(with timpani sticks).

Special' orchestration effects: II. Muted trombones

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
99

and cornets; IV. Muted cornets.

#2. Identification of Major Musical Moments (MMM)

First Movement: NORMANDIE

Mm. 1-20

The opening of the movement presents the tuneful first

theme alia m a r c i a . Orchestration, the rhythmic nature of

the theme and accompaniment setting, and the nature of the

harmony all combine to cause the majestic excitement of the

opening.

Mm. 79-108

A four measure phrase (mm. 79-82) presenting sudden

rhythmic excitement, fortissimo dynamic, and tutti

orchestration, introduces the final complete statement of


/

the opening theme. The orchestration of this restatement

remains tutti throughout most of its length, unlike the more

milder setting of the moveme nt’s opening.

During mm. 101-108 the orchestration thins to a unison

statement of a phrase derived from the first theme in the

brass. The excitement of the restatement is maintained

through the sudden contrast in orchestration and subsequent

unison soli lines in the cornets and trumpets.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
100

Second Movement: BRETAGNE

Mm. 2 6 -5 6

This section develops climactic contrasts via elevated

dynamic levels and the use of high pitch registers. While

this section does provide the climactic moment of the

movement, the nature of the overall section does not provide

the subjective impression of an MMM.

Third Movement: ILE DE FRANCE

Mm. 24-36, 40-44, 58-70

Each of these three sections presents the m ov e m e n t ’s

second theme in contrasting legato articulation and

contrasting harmonic settings. These settings combine to

form distinctive passages that the listener can compare to.

other contrasting sections of the work in order to discover

the thematic structure of the work.

Mm. 75-79

This last section of the movement presents the most

exciting setting of the second theme. The combination of

elements which result in this MMM include a fortissimo

dynamic, tutti orchestration, the repetitive eighth-note

rhythm on the pitch (g) in the trumpet’s higher register

(mm. 77-79), and the simultaneity of legato woodwind,

detached brass, and accented articulation textures.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
101

Fourth Movement: ALSACE-LORRAINE

Mm. 80-109

This last section of the movement combines a legato,

fortissimo line with theme and harmony, the highest thematic

register in the movement, and tutti orchestration, except

for the omission of the baritone line.

Fifth Movement: PROVENCE

Mm. 1-14. 27-54, 53-68

A unison statement of the first theme presents

orchestration contrast via the clarinets and flutes

accompanied by detached quarter-note chords in trombones and

tubas. The uniqueness of the theme derives from the

rhythmic sequence, intervallic content, and accompanying

ha rm ony .

#3. Identification of significant changes in texture.

The following outline presents only those specific

changes in the s e c t i o n ’s texture whose nature must be

reflected in the conducting gesture. The listing of

elements are derived from those that are communicable by the

conductor: orchestration register and density (0), tempo

(T), meter (M), rhythm (R), dynamic (D), linear writing (L),

and articulation (A).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
102

First Movement: NORMANDIE

Mm. 1-12
The opening of the movement establishes the following
textural characteristics:
Tempo: Anime = 144. J,
Meter: 6/8
Rhythm: Regular quarter and eight-note patterns.
Dynamic: Forte (Loud).
Linear writing: Homophonic, with short counterline
phrases and motives.
Articulation: Detached, with short, legato phrases and
triplet motives.
Orchestration: Light density with variations in
overall tone color via saxophone and horn scoring.

Mm. 15-18
Rhythm: Triplets.
Orchestration: Low register brass and woodwinds.
•* :•

Mm. 19-22
Articulation: Contrasting legato phrases.
Orchestration: Light density including only horns and
saxophones.

Mm. 25-26
Dynamic: Reduced via decrescendo from forte to mezzo
forte (moderately loud).
Rhythm: Second theme presented here uses longer note
values.
Orchestration: A mixture of tone colors which produce
a polarity of registers through scoring for high
woodwinds with low-register note punctuations
following in the low brass.

Mm. 27-50
Rhythm: Increase in rhythmic activity via a repetitive
pattern.
Dynamic: Crescendo through mm. 28-29.

Mm. 31-54
Dynamic: Decrescendo in m. 33 leading to a mezzo forte
level.
Rhythm: At m. 3 4 2 motion is suspended by the dotted-
auarter note before the first restatement of. the
opening theme at m. 35.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
103

Mm. 35-42
Dynamic: Restatement of theme articulated by a sudden
forte.
Articulation: Detached.
Rhythm: Contrasting activity.

Mm. 43-52
Articulation: A variation of the first theme stated by
the alto saxophone line is presented in
contrasting legato.
Rhythm: Slowing of forward motion via dotted-quarter
and dotted-half note chords.

Mm. 53-56
Orchestration: Lighter density of four treble
voicings.

Mm. 57-58
Articulation: Woodwind lines provide legato contrast.

Mm. 59-62
Orchestration: Lower register.
Rhythm: The rhythm ( jJ J
) provides contrast.

M m . 63—66
Rhythm: This phrase is the first of four which is
based on the contrasting rhythm of the second
theme.

Mm. 71-74
Dynamic: This extern-ion ot the preceding rhythmic
phrase begins wi v-h a sudden f o r t e .
Orchestration: Widening of the pitch range.

Mm. 75-78
Orchestration: Upper woodwinds and middle register
trombones.

Mm. 79-82
Rhythm: Contrast provided by the dotted-eighth
patterns ( J, jr - ) . J
Orchestration: Antiphonal-like statements between high
and low register over sustained chords.

Mm. 83-92
Rhythm: (A) theme material returns.

Mm. 922-951
Orchestration: Descending triplet line in low
register.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
104

Mm. 9 5 2 - 1 0 1 1
Rhythm: Expansion of triplet texture.
Orchestration: Low woodwinds and low brass.

Mm. 1012-105
Orchestration: Low register statement of unison
melodic idea from first theme.

Mm. 104-107
Orchestration: Middle register cornets with (A) theme
statement.

Mm. 1072-108
Orhestration: Low and middle brass presenting the
cadential figure.

Second Movement: BRETAGNE

M. 1
The simple presentation of an octave (Bb), suspended in
the low woodwind and low brass registers, establishes a
pianissimo (very soft) dynamic and suspends forward motion
until the entrance of the first theme.

Mm. 1 2 - 4
Following the sustained Bb octave, the first theme is
presented by the horns, and the following characteristics of
the opening are established:
Tempo: Lent = 116.
Meter: 6/8
Dynamic: Pianissimo.
Rhythm: Regular patterns.
Linear writing: Homophonic.
Articulation: Legato and tenuto.
Orchestration: Light woodwind and horn scoring, low
and middle registers, respectively.

Mm. 5-6
Orchestration: Lower middle register of bassoon.

Mm. 7-9
Orchestration: Setting of theme in the middle horn
register.

Mm. 10-121
Orchestration: Scoring of A2* theme in lower register.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
105

Mm. 122-15
Orchestration: Secondary thematic phrase (81)
presented by oboe with chordal punctuations
articulating the ends of phrases in trombones.

Mm. 16-17
Orchestration: The A22 theme is scored in the lower
voices of bassoon and baritone.

Mm. 18-24
Orchestration: 82 theme occurs in oboe.
Articulation: Certain measures present tenuto
markings on beats 1 and 2.
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo f o r t e ; a brief decrescendo
at m. 24 articulates restatement of A2 theme at
m. 251.

Mm. 25-26
Orchestration: Scoring of A2 in middle register.
Dynamic: Return to mezzo forte level.

Mm. 27-561
Orchestration: Presentation of C theme in middle and
upper registers.
Rhythm: Eighth-note motion of C theme contrasts with
A and 8 themes.

Mm. 362-38i
Orchestration: Appearance of A2 in middle register of
alto clarinet and alto saxophone followed by a
middle register horn statement.
Dynamic: Decrescendo at m. 381 articulates the end of
theme A2.

Mm. 382-41
Orchestration: Statement of B1 in oboe.
Rhythm: Triplet motive in baritone on beat 2.

Mm. 42-43
Orchestration: A2 scored in middle register.

Mm. 43-50
Orchestration: 82 continues in oboe.
Rhythm: Triplet motive on every beat.
Dynamic: Crescendo to approx. mezzo forte in m. 45.

Mm. 51-52
Orchestration: Middle register scoring of A2.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
106

Mm. 53-541
Orchestration: (a2) appears in- the lower register.
Tempo: Rallentando.
Dynamic: Pianissimo, m. 541.

Mm. 542-57
Orchestration: A1 scoring in middle horn register.

Mm. 58-59
Orchestration: A2 scored in lower bassoon register.

Mm. 60-62
Orchestration: A1 appears in middle register of horns
accompanied by alto and tenor saxophone counter
melodies.

Mm. 65-66
Orchestration: A2 scored in low register.
Rhythm: Motion begins to slow in m. 64.
Dynamic: Decrescendo to and.

Third Movement: ILE DE FRANCE

Mm. 1-21
Opening characteristics:

Tempo:
Meter: 2/2
Dynamic: Forte.
Rhythm: Regular pattern, rapid, repeated eighth-notes.
Linear writing: Homophonic.
Articulation: Detached.
Orchestration: Middle and low brass.

Mm. 22-7
Orchestration: A1 initially presented in upper
woodwinds and middle register cornets.

Mm. 7-11
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo forte.
Articulation: A 2 , legato.
Orchestration: Middle register clarinets.

M m . 12-13
Dynamic: Sudden fortissimo (very loud); accent.
Articulation: Detached.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
107

Mm. 14-18
Dynamic: Sudden fortissimo.
Orchestration: Low brass.

Mm. 19-201
Orchestration: Continuation of al, i.e., a i s , in the
middle register.

Mm. 202-24
Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e ; m. 24 decrescendo.
Articulation: A2* , legato.
Orchestration: Melding of low (horn, baritone) and
middle (flute) registers.
Linear writing: Overlapping contrapuntal lines of A 2 i .

Mm. 25-28
Articulation: 81, alto saxophone, legato.
Dynamic: Crescendo and decrescendo, mm. 26 and 28,
respectively.

Mm. 29-32
Articulation: Detached.
Orchestration: Low brass.

Mm. 35-35
Articulation: 81, legato.

Mm. 36-38
Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e .
Orchestration: Low, middle register of clarinet.
Articulation: Clarinet, legato; detached and legato
combinations with brass entrance, m. 362 .

Mm. 39-40
Rhythm: Three beat tie resulting in suspended motion.
Orchestration: Middle register brass and high
wo odwinds.

Mm. 41-48
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo forte.
Artioulation: Simultameity of legato (81) and detached
(al«).
Orchestration: Upper register (clarinet and
saxophone).

Mm. 49-50
Orchestration: Middle register, high woodwinds.
Dynamics: Sudden mezzo forte.
Articulation: Detached.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
108

Mm. 51-54
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo piano (moderately soft); m. 53,
crescendo.
Orchestration: Very thin woodwind texture.

Mm. 55-58
Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e ; mm. 57-58, decrescendo.
Orchestration: Greater density in high woodwinds.
Tempo: m. 58, ritard.

Mm. 59-62
Articulation: Legato.

Mm. 63-66
Articulation: Simultaneity, detached accompaniment
with legato theme.
Dynamic: mm. 652-66, crescendo.

Mm. 67-701
Articulation: Legato.

Mm. 702 - 7 5 1
Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e .
Articulation: Deatched, accented.

Mm. 752-79
Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e ; horn accents.
Articulation: Legato.

Fourth Movement: ALSACE-LORRAINE

Mm. 1-8
Opening characteristics:

Tempo:
Meter:
Lent,
2/4
J
= 58-60.

Dynamic: Mezzo p i a n o .
Rhythm: Regular; special attention to ( J J. J )
motive.
Linear writing: Homophonic.
Articulation: Alternating detached, legato, and
tenuto.
Orchestration: Melding of middle and lower registers.

Mm. 9-16
Dynamic: Mezzo forte preceded by one measure
crescendo.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
109

Mm. 17-21
Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e .
Orchestration: Increase in density via addition of
oboes, clarinets, trombones.

Mm. 22-24
Dynamic: Even though a decrescendo is not scored
until mm. 23-24, a decrease in dynamic begins to
be felt at m. 22 with the descending melodic line
in the upper woodwinds.

Mm. 25-36
Dynamic: Pianissimo prepared by a two measure
decrescendo.
Orchestration: Density similar to the mezzo piano
opening.

Mm. 37-381
Dynamic: Mezzo piano prepared by crescendo at m. 34
and established through mm. 35-36.
Orchestration: Returns to the identical opening
density with emphasis on middle register via
saxophone family.

Mm. 382-40
Orchestration: Addition of counter line in flutes
and oboes— upper register.
Rhythm: Constant eighth-note motion.
Articulation: Fully legato in flute and oboe counter
lines.

Mm. 41-421
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo f o r t e .
Articulation: Detached.

Mm. 422-44
Orchestration: Flute and oboe counter line, upper
register.
Articulation: Legato, flute and oboe.

Mm. 45-47
Articulation: Detached.

M. 48
Articulation: Legato.

Mm. 482-52
Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e .
Articulation: Melding of legato and detached styles,
high woodwinds and saxophones, respectively.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
110

M. 55
Orchestration: Middle register, cornets.

M. 54
Orchestration: Middle register, Bb clarinets.
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo piano followed by a
decrescendo.

Mm. 55-60
Orchestration: Middle register, clarinet family.
Dynamic: Piano (soft), preceded by one measure
decrescendo.

Mm. 60-61
Dynamic: F o r t e , prepared by crescendo (mm. 58-59);
accents on strong beats.
Orchestration: Melding of high register woodwinds with
middle register high and low brass.

Mm. 62-671
Dynamic: Sudden pi a n o .
Orchestration: Lower middle register, clarinet
emphasis.

Mm. 672-751
Orchestration: Addition of tuba, m. 67.

Mm. 752-762
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo f o r t e .
Orchestration: Middle register, woodwinds and high
brass.

Mm. 77-80
Orchestration: High register, woodwinds.
Dynamic: m. 80 forte prepared by crescendo at m. 78;
crescendo through m. 80 to prepare the fortissimo
at m. 81.
Rhythm: ( J, "j
j ) pattern, m. 80.

Mm. 81-96
Orchestration: Tutti; alternating addition and
deletion of high woodwinds.
Dynamic: Fortissimo prepared by one measure crescendo.
Rhythm: Alternating contrast between eighth-note
pattern and dotted-eighth & sixteenth-note
pattern.

Mm. 97-102
Rhythm: (
Orchestration:
JUJlnsm associated with thematic line,
Upper register, woodwind.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
I l l

Mm. 1 0 3 -1 0 5
Rhythm:
Dynamic:
cJ- j>lj>
Accents.

Mm. 106-109
Rhythm: Slowing of the note values,

Fifth Movement: PROVENCE

Mm. 1-11
Opening characteristics:

Tempo: Anime, J = 138.


Meter: C
Dynamic: Forte.
Rhythm: Constant variation through a four measure
phrase.
Linear writing: Homophonic.
Articulation: Alternating use of legato and detached.
Orchestration: Melding of middie-upper woodwinds with
low brass.

M. 12
Rhythm: Contrast with introduction of triplets.

Mm. 13-14
Resume all opening specifications.

Mm. 15-191
Orchestration: Effect of pitch polarity between
high woodwind and percussion lines.

Mm. 192-22
Rhythm: Introduction of sixteenth-notes to the
woodwind lines.

Mm. 23-271
Rhythm: Return to quartet— note and eighth-note
patterns.

Mm. 2 7 1—28
Rhythm: Quartet— note.
Or chestration: Melding of uppet— middle and lower
registers.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
112

Mm. 29-54
Rhythm: Return to opening thematic rhythm of
constantly varied four measure phrase.
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo f o rte ; crescendo, m. 32.

M. 35
Tempo: Molto ritard; fermata on beat 4&.
Dynamics: Sudden fortissimo; accents.
Orchestration: Uppei— middle register, cornets and
trumpets.

M m . 36—37
Articulation: Legato.
Dynamic: Sudden p i a n o .
Rhythm: Less activity via emphasis on quartei— note and
eighth-notes.

Mm. 48-49
Rhythm^ Contrast, absence of dotted rhythm.
Orchestration: Low register.

M m . 50-56
Rhythm: Returnv to eighth-note and sixteenth-note
phrases of the opening.
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo f o rte ; crescendo, m. 56.

M. 57
Tempo: Molto ritard; fermata on beat 4&.
Dynamic: F o r t i ssi mo.

M m . 58—68
Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e .
Tempo: "A tempo".
Rhythm: Emphasis on syncopated variation of (Al).
Orchestration: Melding of the polarity of high
woodwinds and middle register low brass.
Articulation: Alternating legato and detached motives.

Mm. 69-732
Orchestration: Effect of pitch polarity between high
woodwind and percussion lines.
Rhythm: Return of the eighth-note rhythm.

Mm. 733-771
Rhythm: Addition of sixteenth-notes.

Mm. 772-78
Rhythm: Quartei— note passage.
Articulation: Detached.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
113

Mm. 7 9 -8 2
Rhythm: Synoopated variation
Dynamic: Sudden fortissimo.

Mm. 83-84
Rhythm: Return of (Al).

Mm. 85-89
Rhythm: Contrasting simple rhythm involving slower
note values.

#4. Identification of emotional energy sequen ce .

The following evaluations have been developed as a

result of the subjective judgment of objective score

information. Those elements that are primary to producing

the indicated emotional energy-type are followed by a colon

and the explanation.

While not precisely communicated through gesture, the

element of harmony is included in this list. For the

purpose of this discussion, pitch is viewed as a part of

orchestration register. The elements which effect emotional

energy are: orchestration register and density (0), tempo

(T), rhythm (R), meter (M) changes, articulation (A),

dynamics (D), linear writing (L), and harmony (H).

First Movement: NORMANDIE

Mm. 1-20 (T ) .
Tempo: Anime, J.
= 138.
Dynamic: Forte.
Rhythm: Regular.
Articulation: Detached.
Or ch estration: Broad range.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
114

Mm. 21-22 (R— »


Dynamic: Natural softening due to descending melodic
line.
Orchestration: Middle register.

Mm. 23—34 (R---)


Harmony: Introduction of the secondary tonality.
Dynamic: Mezzo f o r t e .
Articulation: Legato.
Rhythm: Long note values in accompaniment lines.

Mm. 35-42 (T )
Dynamic: Forte.
Articulation: Detached brass set simultaneously with
legato woodwinds (mm. 35-38); mm. 39-41, legato.

Mm. 43-52 (R-)


Dynamic: Sudden mezzo f o r t e .
Rhythm: Less activity via quartei— notes and eighth-
notes; long note values in accompaniment lines.

Mm. 53-78 (T-)


Dynamic: F o r t e , accents.
Articulation: Detached.
Rhythm: Active writing.
Orchestration: Progression of gradually increasing
score density.

Mm. 79-82 (T-)


Dynamic: Fo rti ss imo .
Rhythm: Dotted-eighth motive.
Pitch: Repeated pitches.

Mm. 85-108 (T )
Rhythm: The increase in tension ceases with the
re-establishment of the regularity of the
opening rhythm and the return of the tonic
key of Bb major.

Second Movement: BRETAGNE

Mm. 1-65 (R )
The entire movement is relaxed in nature, except for
the subtle tensions that are characteristic of the
individual melodic lines and phrases. The opening
Relaxation characteristics include:

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
115

Tempo: Lent, J - 116.


Dynamic: Range = pianissimo to mezzo fo r t e .
Rhythm: Uses values of the beat unit and the first
sub-division. The second sub-division occurs only
twice, mm. 17 and 50.

Third Movement: ILE DE FRANCE

Mm. 1-23 (T ) *
Tempo:
Dynamic:
Vif, J
= 112.
F o r t e ; including mezzo forte and fortissimo
variations.
Rhythm: Fast, unpredictable.
Articulation: Detached, accents.
Orchestration: Tutti.

M. 24 (R — »
Dynamic: Decrescendo.
Articulation: Legato anacrusis to m. 25ff.
Orchestration: Very light; melding of middle (brass)
and upper (woodwinds) registers.

Mm. 25-28 (R )
Dynamic: Piano with slight variations.
Rhythm: Regular, predictable.
Linear writing: Homophonic; vocal character.
Articulation: Legato..
Orchestration: Thematic emphasis in woodwinds; light
accompaniment figures in low brass.

Mm. 29-52 ( ® --- )


Articulation: Legato woodwinds over detached brass.

Mm. 35-561 (R )
Dynamic: Sudden fo r t e .
Rhythm: Unpredictable, syncopated.
Linear writing: Instrumental character.
Articulation: Detached, accents.
Orchestration: Emphasis on high brass and trombone I.

Mm. 362-4Q1 (J ---)


Articulation: Detached.
Rhythm: Fast.

Mm. 402-48 (J ---)


Articulation: Legato woodwinds over detached horns
and low woodwinds.
Dynamic: Sudden forte, m. 45.
Orchestration: Expansion, m. 45.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
116

Mm. 49-50 (T )
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo f o r t e .
Rhythm: Unpredictable.
Linear writing: Instrumental character.
Articulation: Emphasis on detached.
Orchestration: Middle register.

Mm. 51-54 (R— »


Dynamic: Sudden mezzo p i a n o .
Linear writing: Vocal character.
Orchestration: Light density.

Mm. 55-571 (T--- )


Dynamic: Sudden f or te.
Rhythm: Contrasting pattern with that of mm. 51-54.
Articulation: Legato and detached.

Mm. 572-58 (R )
Tempo: Ritard, m. 58.
Dynamic: Decrescendo; mezzo piano in low brass.

Mm. 59-62 (R )
Dynamic: Piano.
Rhythm: Regular, predictable.
Linear writing: Vocal character.
Articulation: Legato.
Orchestration: Light.

Mm. 63-66 ( ® --- )


M. 63: Tension is added to the texture by the
introduction of the trombones with quick rhythms
and detached articulations.

Mm. 67-701 (R--- )


Dynamic: Mezzo f o rt e.
Rhythm: Regular, predictable.
Linear writing: Vocal character.
Articulation: Emphasis on legato.
Orchestration: Very light.

Mm. 7Q2-79 (T--- )


Dynamic: Sudden f or te.
Rhythm: Unpredictable.
Linear writing: Contrapuntal.
Articulation: Detached.
Orchestration: mm. 702-75, moderately dense; mm. 76-
79, tutti.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
117

Fourth Movement: ALSACE-LORRAINE

Mm. 1-16 (R ) .
Tempo: Lent, J
= 58-60.
Meter: Regular.
Dynamic: Piano to mezzo forte.
Rhythm: Regular, predictable.
Linear writing: Homophonic.
Articulation: Emphasis on legato and tenuto effect.
Orchestration: Lightly scored, emphasis on middle
register woodwinds.

Mm. 17-21 (T--- )


Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e .
Rhythm: Intensified via 32nd-note ornament.
Orchestration: Increase in density of woodwinds.

Mm. 22-24 (R— »


Dynamic: Decrescendo.
Rhythm: Gradually longer note values.

Mm. 25-48 (R---)


Dynamic: Pianissimo; subtle increase via mezzo piano
(mm. 37-40), mezzo forte (mm. 41-48). The subtle
increase in dynamic also brings about a subtle
increase in tension.
Rhythm: Regular, predictable.
Linear writing: Homophonic.
Articulation: Legato.
Orchestration: Very light.

Mm. 49-53 (T )
Dynamic: Sudden f o r t e .
Orchestration: Expansion of woodwind density; the
tension is maintained through m. 53 with the two-
part soli for cornets.

M. 54 ( R— >)
Dynamic: Decrescendo.
Orchestration: Clarinets, light density.

Mm. 55-57 (R-- )


Dynamic: Piar.o.
Orchestration: Expanding clarinet family.

Mm. 58-59 (T~»


Dynamic: Crescendo.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
118

Mm. 60-61 (T-- )


Dynamic: Sudden f o rte .
Rhythm: Addition of ornamental figures in low
register to emphasize strong beats.
Orchestration: Expands to include high and low brass.

Mm. 62-76 (R-- )


Dynamic: Sudden p ia no .
Orchestration: Light density.

Mm. 77-80 (T--»


Dynamic: Crescendo; accent, m. 802.
Orchestration: Expansion of texture via trumpets and
trombones.

Mm. 81-109 (T )
Dynamic: F or tissimo; accents.
Orchestration: Tutti.
Rhythm: Contrast offered by 32nd-note ornaments,
mm. 97-102.

Fifth Movement: PROVENCE

Mm. 1-35 (T ) .
Tempo: Anime, J = 132.
Meter: Regular.
Dynamic: Forte.
Rhythm: Fast, unpredictable.
Linear writing: Homophonic.
Articulation: Detached.

Mm. 36-49 (R )
Dynamic: Sudden p i a n o .
Rhythm: Predictable.
Linear writing: Homophonic.
Articulation: Legato.
Orchestration: Emphasis on woodwind scoring.

Mm. 50-55 )
Dynamic: Sudden mezzo f o r t e .
Rhythm: Unpredictable.
Linear writing: Contrapuntal.
Articulation: Legato, detached styles.

Mm. 56-57 (T— >)


Tempo: Molto ritard, m. 57.
Dynamic: Crescendo, accents.
Rhythm: Contrasting rhythmic patterns.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
119

Mm. 58-84 (T )
Dynamic: F o r t e , accents.
Rhythm: Fast, syncopation.
Linear writing: Contrapuntal.
Orchestration: Mm. 58-68, partial scoring with
emphasis on woodwind density; mm. 69-76, maintains
partially through polarity of scoring, i.e.,
flute+piccolo and percussion lines.

Mm. 85-89 ( ® --- )


Rhythm: Use of longer note values to relax the
forward motion.
Linear writing: Homophonic.
Articulation: Emphasis on legato and tenuto effect.

#5. Selection of instruments to be c u e d .

The following descriptions of cues indicate the measure

number of the cue, the instrument being cued, and the reason

for the cue. When more than one instrument is playing a

line to be cued, the cuing gesture of the body movement must

invite all of the instruments on that line to play. The

conductor’s eye contact, however, can only focus in one

direction at a time, in which case, the conductor must

decide where to finally fix eye contact at the moment of the

cued entrance.

When several instruments enter with the cued line, the

conductor must determine the focus of eye contact according

to (1) which instrument color to recognize, or (2) which

instrument has yet to be cued. These decisions are

subjective and will vary from conductor to conductor and

from performance to performance.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
120

First Movement: NORMANDIE

Mm. 1 -2 2

M. 1: Cornet— A theme. M. 3 2 : Horns I/II— prominent

triplet that articulates the end of the opening theme. M.

62: Alto saxophones— a prominent triplet motive lending an

agogic accent to a weak beat. M. 8*: Clarinet II/III—

prominent triplets that articulate the end of the A theme.

M. 82; Trumpets— first entrance, and anacrusis to the

second phrase of A. M. 9: Tuba— first entrance, and

articulates the beginning of A2. M. 1 3 2 ; Baritones— begins

an extended triplet pattern that marks the end of A theme.

M. 19: Alto saxophones— begins the four measure bridge into

the B theme.

Mm. 23-58

M. 23: Flutes— introduce B theme. M. 27: Alto

saxophone— this entrance doubles and reinforces a portion of

the flute on 8. M. 31: Clarinet III— prominent, descending

accompaniment line which articulates the end of B before the

reintroduction of A in variation form (A1 ). M. 35: Horns

III— restatement of A. M. 37: Trumpets— prominent

accompanying triplet figure. M. 43: Alto saxophones—

introduces A 2 . M. 53: Cornet I— prominent melodic line.

M. 55: Eb clarinet— echoes the melodic line from m. 53. M.

57: Flutes— prominent, ascending accompanying triplet line.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
121

Mm. 5 9 -8 2

M. 59: Clarinet I— bridge to syncopated entrance of A

at m. 63. M. 63: Horns— present, along with the clarinets,

the syncopated entrance of A. M. 67: Trombones— strong

accompaniment chords that punctuate the beginning of an

episodic phrase between A statements. M. 71: Horns III/IV-

-syncopated entrance of A material. M. 75: Trombone III—

adds a line to the accompaniment density. M. 79: Cornets

I— a prominent rhythmic idea that initiates a four measure

bridge into the recapitulation of A.

Mm. 83-108

M. 85: Baritone— articulates triplet pattern to close

Al. M. 90: Horns III/XV— extended triplet line that

articulates A2. M. 95: Tubas— initiate extended ascending

triplet pattern which articulates the closing of A theme.

M. 101: Trombone II— initiates the first of two unison

statements that brings the movement to a close. M. 104:

Cornet I— presents a final cornet unison statement of Al.

Second Movement: BRETAGNE

Mm. 1-25

M. I 1 : Tubas— sounds the opening Bb pitch with fermata

in order to set the mood of the movement. M. I2 : Horns—

introduce the A theme material. M. 2: Clarinets I— initial

entrance with harmony pitch. M. 5: Bassoons— introduce A2.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
122

M. 6: Horns— restatement of A. M. 7: Alto saxophone—

initiates a solo counter line. M. 10: Baritones— first

entrance with A2. M. 11: Bassoons— doubles A2 after four

measures rest. M. 12: Oboe— first entrance, solo line. M.

132; Trombones— first entrance. M. 152: Flutes— doubles

trombone chords. M. 16: Bassoon I— statement of A2. M.

17: Oboe— counter-melodic line. M. 182; Trombones— chord

entrance. M. 20: Horns— chord entrance. M. 2 0 2 ; Oboe—

melodic continuation. M. 222 : Flutes— octave entrance

after rest. Mm. 23-24: Trombones— entrances on strong

beats. M. 24: Oboe— solo line. M. 25: Alto clarinet— A2.

Mm. 26-66

M. 26-361: Clarinets— unison melodic line. M. 36:

Alto saxophone— A2. M. 3 7 2 ; Baritones— counterline

entrance. M. 38: Horns— counterline; Oboe— counterline.

M. 3 9 2 : Flutes— chordal entrance. M. 4 1 2 : Cornets— chord

entrance. M. 42: Alto saxophone— A2, prominent timbre: M.

43: Oboe— counterline. M. 44: Flutes— chord entrance. M.

46: Cornet I— chord entrance. Mm. 48-50: Trombones—

chordal entrances on strong beats. M . .51: Alto saxophone—

A2. M. 53: Bassoons— solo melodic line for ending of A2.

M. 54: Tuba— Bb entrance; Horns— A. M. 60: Alto

saxophone— counterline. M. 63: Baritone— A2. M. 64:

Bassoons— A 2 .

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
123

Third Movement: ILE DE FRANCE

Mm. 1-39

M. 1: Horns— shortest accompaniment rhythm. M. 22:

High woodwinds— initial entrance of A theme. M. 8:

Clarinets— A extension. M. 1 1 2 ; Trumpets— accompaniment

passage. M. 132 : Tuba— A. M. 182 : Clarinets—

accompaniment passage. M. 20: Baritones— (A) extension.

M. 21: Flutes— (A) extension. M. 242; Tubas; Alto

saxophone— B. M. 262; Flutes— B. M. 29: Trombones—

eighth-note accompaniment passage. M. 30: Flutes— B

extension. M. 32: Cornet I— B solo. M. 36: Clarinets—

eighth-note passage; Trumpets— A.

Mm. 40-79

M. 40: .Clarinets— B. M. 44: Trombones— eighth-note

accompaniment passage. M. 482: High woodwinds— soli

episodic idea. M. 50: String bass; Oboe— (B) extension.

M. 52: Horns I/II— motive. M. 54: High woodwinds— B* . M.

58: Alto saxophone— B. M. 63: Trombones— eighth-note

passage. M. 64: Oboes— B extension. M. 66: Piccolo— A.

M. 70: Timpani— A. M. 75: Tutti— B.

Fourth Movement: ALSACE-LORRAINE

Mm. 1-16

M. 1: Alto saxophone— A. M. 4: Trombone I— motive

articulating end of Al. M. 5: Alto saxophone— A. M. 7:

Flutes— first entrance, initiates A2. M. 9: Tubas— strong

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
124

beat punctuations, controlling tempo. M. 13: Baritone

saxophone— first entrance, continuation of A2 phrase. M.

16: Timpani— punctuates end of A2.

Mm. 17-80

M. 17: Alto saxophone. M. 19: Tenor saxophone. M.

21: Bassoon. M. 25: Cornet I— B, solo. M. 32: Soli

cornets I. M. 37: Alto saxophone— A. M., 38: Flutes. M.

41: Alto saxophone— A. M. 42: Oboes. M.44: Alto

saxophone— A2. M. 46: Cornet I— A2 solo. M. 48: Upper

woodwinds— 82. M. 50: Trumpet solo— B2. M. 53: Cornets—

soli. M. 54: Clarinets— soli. M. 55:Clarinet tutti. M.

60i: Oboe— melodic motive. M. 602: Tubas— strong beat

punctuation. M. 61: Clarinet I— anacrusis to clarinet

tutti. M. 64: String bass-strong beat punctuation. M. 67:

Tubas— harmonic lead. M. 68: Cornets— B. M. 69: Oboes—

B. M. 72: Cornets— B2. M . 73: Piccolo— B2. M. 75:

Clarinet I— B2. M. 76: Flute— B2. M. 77: Clarinets

II/III— 8. M. 801: Cornets— motive. M. 8 0 2 ; Trombones—

motive.

Mm. 81-109

M. 81: Tubas— strong tone; Trumpets— B2. M. 84:

Trombones— strong-beat punctuation. M. 84: Flutes— B2. M.

B8: Trombones— strong-beat punctuation. M. 89: Oboes— 82.

M. 94: Piccolo— 82. M. 97: Alto saxophone. M. 99:

Horns. M. 101: Trombones I. M. 102: Tubas— strong-beat

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
125

punctuation. M. 103: Horns I/II— -strong motive. M. 104:

Flutes— thematic augmentation of 82. M. 105: Horns II/III-

-strong motive.

Fifth Movement: PROVENCE

Mm. 1-141

M. 1: Clarinets— A. M. 2: Horns— harmonic support.

M. 4: Flutes— A2. h. 5: Trombones— harmonic support. M.

6: Horns— harmonic support. M. 10: Trombones— harmonic

support. M. 11: Horns— harmonic support. M. 12: Upper

woodwinds— contrasting passage of triplets.

Mm. 142-57

M. 15: Percussion— accompaniment; Flutes— 8. M. 21:

Clarinet soli entrance. M. 27: Trombones— prominent

quarter-note passage. M. 29: Upper woodwinds— A; Baritone-

-soli line. M. 35: Cornets— soli. M. 36: Flutes— 8. M.

38: Horns— strong counterline. M. 49: Tuba— syncopated

harmonic close to B. M. 50: Cornets— soli A. M. 51:

Clarinets— counterline phrase. M. 53: Cornets II/III—

prominent motive. M. 54: Clarinets— A. M. 57: Cornets—

sol i .

Mm. 58-68

M. 58: Upper woodwinds— syncopated counterline with A.

M. 61: Trombone— contrasting rhythmic counterline. M. 62:

Cornet I— A. M. 64: Clarinets— A2. M. 67: Cornet I— A.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
126

Mm. 684-89

M. 684; Percussion— anacrusis to melodic

accompaniment. M. 69: Flute— B. M. 73: Eb clarinet—

doubles continuation of B. M. 77: Tutti— quarter note

bridge to A. M. 85: Cornet— soli, slower note values

ritarding forward motion. .

#6. Identification of phrase lengths.

Phrases are indicated on the Form continuum of the

Basal Structure Analysis graphs (See Appendix I) according

to thematic development. For the sake of clarity, the

measure continuum of the graph indicates the number of full

measures of length; the number located underneath each

phrase of the continuum is a more specific indicator of

length, e.g., a phrase graphed as four full measures may, in

actuality, measure only 3-1/2 measures.

The following discussions highlight the unique aspects

of phrase structure for each movement.

I. NORMANDIE

The majority of phrases are symmetrical

(four measures each).

Odd-measure groupings occur at the end of the

movement (mm. 91-108). These last phrases form

three groupings of five measures each. In

contrast, the preceding phrase lengths occur in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
127

duple groupings of 2, 4, or 6 measures each.

II. BRETAGNE

Throughout the movement, the sequences of

individual phrase lengths are unpredictable. Only

once do two phrases of equal length occur side-by­

side (mm. 51-54). For the majority of the

movement, the phrases alternate between lengths of

3-1/2 measures and 2 measures, respectively,

The longest phrase lengths occur in the middle

of the movement (mm. 18-50) and are 9, 10, and

12-1/2 measures in length, respectively. Due to

the slow tempo and soft dynamic throughout the

movement, the unpredictability of the odd-measured

phrases does not create tension in these areas.

III. ILE DE FRANCE

The majority of phrases are symmetrical (four

measures each). The portion of the movement

containing the most odd-measured phrases occurs at

the beginning (mm. 1-24). These opening phrase

lengths are constantly varied. The

unpredictability of the odd-measured lengths

produces tensional energy, and the opening twenty-

four measure portion of the movement does, in fact,

correspond with the Emotional Energy g r a p h ’s

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
123

opening length of tension.

Variation in length occurs between phrases

having the same thematic content, e.g., Al makes

its first and last appearance with a length of

5-1/2 measures, but at mm. 37-40, it is only four

measures in length. The recognition of the

irregular grouping effects tensional energy in the

listener.

IV. ALSACE-LORRAINE

This movement presents the fewest number of

odd-measured phrase lengths with the majority of

them occurring at the center of the movement

(mm. 54-83).

The end of the movement is articulated by an

odd-measured length. Similar types of phrases

occurring at mm. 56, 80, and 105 are used to

heighten tension. Additionally, the loud dynamic

heightens the tensional affect of the odd-measured

length. In contrast, the odd-measured

length at m. 54 actually increases relaxation

through the effect of the set t i n g ’s softer

dynamic.

V. PROVENCE

Odd-measured phrase lengths appear scattered

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
129

throughout the movement.

The elongated middle phrase which presents the

C material (mm. 36-49), is fourteen measures in

length without the occurrence of a strong harmonic

cadence. It subdivides into two phrase groupings

of eight and six measures, respectively.

#7. Identification of form, thematic content, and major


tonal areas (See Appendix I for graph analyses of
thematic development for each movement of Suite
Franc ais e.)

Throughout the entire work, thematic development is

generally achieved by the repetition of whole thematic

phrase lengths, with only minimal use of motivic development

derived from the principal themes. The settings of the

various repetitions are varied by the use of contrasting key

areas and color combinations.

I. NORMANDIE

A B Ai Dev A2

A-theme material, at twenty-two measures in

length, is considerably longer than B material,

measuring eight measures, and is presented via

three principal phrases and one subordinate (a3)

phrase. The A-theme material is the emphasis of

thematic development throughout the movement.

Relative key relationships occur. The

initial A section is in Bb major; the B material

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
130

is presented in the relative key of G minor, and

the return of A material is in Bb major.

II. BRETAGNE

A B C Bi A2

The sectional structure of the movement is

palindrome-like with the C material being

presented in 8 minor. The return of the A theme

(m. 55ff) is presented by the horns in the key of D

major, and the return of the B theme (m. 38ff) is

presented by the oboe in the key of E minor.

In addition to the palindrome-like effect,

the individual phrase lengths throughout the

movement form an almost complete rondo-like

substructure with the alternating return of A2.

The absence of the A2 phrase articulates the

beginning of the middle C section.

III. ILE DE FRANCE

A B Dev B1 Al

The overall structure resembles the

palindrome-like form of the second movement. In

this case, however, the middle section does

develop the first two theme sections, and does not

present new material. Both A sections appear in

F major while the B section is presented in G

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
131

major.

This movement uses more short phrases and

motives derived from the principal themes than any

of the others. As might be expected, they occur

during the development section of the movement.

IV. ALSACE-LORRAINE

A B Al Bl A2

The overall formal design resembles that of a

rondo structure. As in the third movement, the

keys of the sections have a stepwise relationship.

The opening A section is inA-minor followed

by the B section in G major. The first return of A

is in A-minor, however, the final return of the A

section is in A major.

With each of the B sections, the harmonic

support creates a polytonal setting. The B theme-

is stated in G major while the arpeggiated bass

line outlines a half-diminished seventh chord built

on the dominant scale degree of G major.

V. PROVENCE

A B Al C A2 Bl A3

The formal structure of this movement presents

a rondo design with three contrasting themes. Each

return of a theme is clearly stated, but the themes

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
132

return in contrasting keys. The corresponding key

sequence to the above rondo design is (G F c Ab

F# c G).

A unique aspect of this movement is the

simultaneous combination and contrapuntal treatment

of three thematic phrases (Al, a* , Bl) in the last

section of the movement, mm. 79-89.

The phrase structure is characterized by odd-

measured groupings scattered throughout, and a

phrase sequence structured unpredictably.

#8. Identification of conductor problems.

I. NORMANDIE

Significant problems for gestural technique do not

occur. The majority of phrases are four measures in length

and change infrequently.

II. BRETAGNE

M. 1: A decision must be made regarding the length of

the fermata; ..and the subsequent entrance of the horns.

Phrase lengths of the two A sections vary between two

and 4-1/2 measures, and occur in an unpredictable sequence.

III. ILE DE FRANCE

M. 2: The conducting gesture must be clearly executed

so that the woodwinds do not enter too soon. The odd-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
133

measured, unnatural rest period of 1-1/2 measures may cause

players to anticipate this entrance.

The opening section (A) presents several odd-measured

phrases in an unpredictable sequence. This may create

concern due to the fast tempo.

Mm. 57-59: Ritardando; decision regarding the tempo

and gesture execution of the eighth-note anacrusis.

IV. ALSACE-LORRAINE

A principal conducting concern is maintenance of the

full length of the note values in the slower tempo of this

movement. The majority of phrases are symmetrical and occur

in a predictable sequence.

V. PROVENCE

Odd-measured phrase groupings occur frequently

throughout this movement. Coupled with the fast tempo,

these phrases create a special concern for the conductor.

Mm. 35 and 57: The subito molto ritard— decision to

subdivide, or not; the hold and release of the fermata on

the beat "4&"; judgment of the amount of time to elapse

between the release of the fermata and the downbeat of m.

36; preparatory gesture for the "a tempo" at m. 36.

#9. Identification of player p ro ble ms .

The work was written specifically for high school

students. "The parts are not difficult to play either

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
134

melodically or rhythmically and use only the average ranges

for the instruments" (Milhaud, 1945).

Player concerns center on interval tuning, e.g.,

octaves in divisi flutes and trombones, fermata releases and

tempo changes such as occur in V. PROVENCE, and typical

ensemble problems of attacks, releases, and similarity of

playing style.

The following discussions present genres of player

difficulty with representative examples which are unique to

each movement.

I. NORMANDIE

A. Intonation of exposed harmonic interval or chordal

passages of two or more like instruments. Mm. Iff, 67-70,

75-78: Trombones. Mm. 19-22: Saxophones. M. 4 3 f f :

Trombones— chromatic chordal passage.

B. Intonation of exposed unison passages with two or

more instruments of like or unlike timbre. M. Iff: Cornet

I. M. 3 4 f f : Horns. Mm. 55-53: Flutes and oboes. Mm. 59-

66: Clarinets. Mm. 71-82: Flutes. M. 103ff: Cornets and

trump et s.

C. Problems of ensemble p l a y i n g . M. 95ff: Low brass-

-maintaining tempo and attacks of the ascending triplet

line.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
135

II. BRETAGNE

A. Intonation of exposed harmonic intervals or chordal

passages of two or more like instruments. M. 13ff:

Trombones— muted chords, li. 20ff: Horns. Mm. 27-39:

Horns— progression of alternating octave and eleventh

intervals.

B. Intonation of exposed unison passages with two or

more instruments of like or unlike timbr e. M. 1: Opening

Bb unison at pianissimo dynamic. Mm. 51-52: Alto clarinet

and alto saxophone.

C. Problems of ensemble p l a y i n g . M. 1* : Horns—

entrance at pianissimo dynamic.

III. ILE DE FRANCE

A. Intonation of exposed harmonic interval or chordal

passages of two or more like instruments. M. Iff: Horns—

chords via repeated eighth-notes. Mm. 12-13: Flutes and

trumpets— progression of open fifths. M. 29ff: Trombones—

muted chord progression. M. 41ff: Horns, bassoons, tenor

and baritone saxophones.

B. Intonation of exposed unison passages with two or

more instruments of like or unlike t im br e. M. 36ff:

Clarinets.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
136

C. Matching intonation and timbre with antiphonal-like

scoring. M. 44ff: Cornets.

D. Problems of ensemble pl ay i n g . M. 2 2 : The woodwind

entrance following a weak beat may be late. M. 25ff:

Horns— continually syncopated rhythm may be difficult to

maintain a consistent tempo. M. 35: Trumpets. Mm. 58-59:

Ritard and "a tempo" scored without a break.

IV. ALSACE-LORRAINE

A. Intonation of exposed harmonic interval or chordal

passages of two or more instruments of like or unlike

timbre. M. Iff: Alto saxophones. M. 17ff: Flutes. M.

38ff: Flute and oboe— octaves.

8. Matching intonation and timbre with dovetailed

melodic writing scored with two or more different

instrumental p a r t s . M. 32: Cornet I— solo; Cornet I—

tutti.

C. Intonation of exposed unison passages with two or

more instruments of like or unlike timbre. M. 38ff:

Flutes. M. 6Qff: Piccolo, flutes, oboes, cornets I. M.

64: Bassoons.

D. Problems of ensemble p l a y i n g . M. 88: Clarinets I,

alto saxophone I, cornet I— 32nd-note passage. M. 97ff:

Rhythmic differentiation between the 32nd-note duplet,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
137

triplet, and dotted-eighth & sixteenth note figure among the

six instrumental groups.

V. PROVENCE

A. Intonation of exposed harmonic interval or chordal

passages of two or more instruments of like or unlike

timb re , li. 1: Trombones. M. 15ff: Piccolo, flutes— soli

in octaves. Mm. 50-53: Piccolo and oboes— octaves. Mm.

54-56: Piccolo, flutes, and oboes— two octave spacing. M.

60: Trombone— wide spacing of outer parts. Mm. 69-77:

Piccolo and flutes— octaves; m. 73: Eb clarinet— added to

line doubling the flute at the unison.

B. Intonation of exposed unison passages with two or

more instruments of like or unlike tim bre . M. 36ff:

Flutes, alto clarinet, alto saxophone I— in octaves and

unison.

C. Matching intonation and timbre with antiphonal-like

wr it i n g . Mm. 36-47: Horns.

D. Problems of ensemble p l a y i n g . M. Iff: Flutes, Eb

clarinet, Bb clarinets I/II— proper rhythmic execution of

the unpredictable rhythmic setting. Mm. 35 and 57:

Cornets, trumpets— unpredictable rhythmic passage at subito

molto ritard. Mm. 54-56: Clarinets, baritone— articulation

and rhythm.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
138

Level II: REVIEW OF SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

The following discussion presents information regarding

the genesis of the composition and the lifestyle and

cultural environment of the composer during the creation of

the work. This information is meant to provide insight into

the composer and composition beyond the limits of the score

in order for the conductor to further personalize the

musical experience.

Genesis of the Composition

Suite Franoaise was commissioned by the Leeds Music

Corporation to be an extended and accessible contemporary

work for the high school concert band. Its composition was

completed in 1944, Op. 248, and its first performance was

given by The Goldman Band in 1945.1 Following its

successful debut, Milhaud was asked to score the piece for

orchestra. This work was completed, with additional

movements for ballet, in 1945, Op. 254 (Sadie, p. 305).

Environment and Lifestyle of the Composer

The composition was written while Milhaud was composei—

in-residence at Mills College in Oakland, California.

Milhaud had newly arrived in the United States from France

following the German occupation of France in 1940.

iNormand Smith and Albert Stoutamire, Band Music N o t e s ,


2d ed. (San Diego: Kjos Publishers, 1979), p. 161.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
139

Before coming to the United States, Milhaud had

distinguished himself as a composer, pianist, and lecturer,

and was recognized as a major French composer. While a

student at the Paris Conservatory before the outbreak of

World War I, he had won awards in counterpoint, fugue, and

violin.

Suite Francaise reflects M i l h a u d ’s interest in South

American rhythm and the styles of American jazz. He was an

avid travellor, and had first come to hear jazz during a

visit to London in 1920. His interest in South American

rhythm stemmed from his work at Rio de Janiero as a

diplomatic secretary for the French government.

During the composition of Suite Franc ais e, the memories

of his beloved France were still very much alive. He

composed the work in five movements and titled each of the

movements with the name of a French province where Allied

forces, together with the French underground, had fought for

the liberation of France.

His embitterment towards the German invaders reads

quite clearly in his remarks at the beginning of the score:

[Each of the movements contains folk tunes from its namesake

Province.] "I wanted the young American to hear the popular

melodies of those parts of France where their fathers and

brothers fought to defeat the German invaders, who in less

than seventy years have brought war, destruction, cruelty,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
140

torture, and murder, three times, to the peaceful and

democratic people of France."2

Level Ills DETAILED STRUCTURAL ANALYSIS

Part 1— Assessment of Overall Structure

The work is divided into five separate movements. Each

movement is clearly sectioned into a large structural design

resembling rondo form. An unusual occurrence in the design

sequence is the use of a palindrome-like structure for the

second movement.3

The structural designs are clearly based upon the

repetition of principal thematic phrases. The use of

motivic or phrasal development derived from the principal

themes is generally reserved^for the bridge passages between

the larger thematic sections.

The sequence of tempi among the five movements follows

the normal alternating pattern typical of multi-movement

works, i.e., fast-slow-fast-slow-fast. Metrically, the

movements are grouped 2+3, that is, the first two movements

distinguish themselves by a triple division of the beat,

while the last three movements present duple beat divisions.

Orchestration textures within each movement vary

widely, however, the tutti orchestration for all five

2Darius Milhaud, Suite Francaise (New York: Leeds


Music Company, 1945).

3 Refer to Figure 1: Overview Chart of Suite Francaise


and the 8asal Structure Analysis graphs provided as part of
the dicussion of Part 2— Detailed Structural Analysis.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Movement: II III 17

1 2 1
Form: ABA Dev A* A B C B1 A2 A 3 Dev A A B A 1 31 A2 A 3 A1 C A2 B I?

Tempo: (1-'WO Lent 7if ( J =112) Lent ( J =53-60) Anime ( J = 138 )

Metar: 6 6 2
8 8 * U

Orchestration: Piccolo
Flute I, II
Oboe I,II
Bassoon I*II
Eb Clarinet Same Same Same Same
Bb Clarinet I,II,III Woodwinds Woodwinds Woodwinds Woodwinds
Eb Alto Saxophone
Bb Tenor Saxophone
Bb Baritone Saxophone
Bb Bass Saxophone
Horn in F 1,11,111,17
Bb Comet I,II,III
Bb Trumpet I,II Same Same Same Same
Trombone I,II,III Brass Brass BT33S Brass
Baritone
Tuba
String Bass String Bass String Bass String Bass String Bass
Timpani Timpani Timpani Timpani Timpani
Percussion; Percussions Percussion: Percussion:
Snare Drum Snare Drum Snare Drum Snare Drum
Cymbals Cymbals Cymbals Military Drum
Bass Drum Bass Drum

Key: Bb a - A
f
Figure 2: Overview Chart of Suite Francaise
11+OB
CM CM
_4 CO

•C CM
•rl
1=3 PC oa

00
__ UA
V' CM
«4 1-4
.

r- ^ 0)
UA jo •H

5)
m
co
•a
CM .4
id -4 + — o
+ CM
CM
JO !§
UA
~ C'•»A J-
O
rcy. -4
(4
o O 0)
£
®
t'~
*8
CJ IT
to
•H
ol
CM
w o CM | - 4 o
§
t-
CM £
* VO CM
VO «d CM a>
CM VO +>
VO. CM
O
CO
«- i-fl
•*4 Ica

-4
PA
CM
60
e° a
a
co
CM
CM vo 0>
IN
*d
CM
CM UA
UA
Io
fd
jo r-4

& icrv
vo fi
+>
fs> Ov OA (O
UA
UA
m I co
UA
OA
id
H

CM
JO
CM
t
CM *c
UA
I
-4
"I PA
pa OA <1)
UA
o
CM OA
CM UA
-4

-4

Ox f—
* o% , (C)
CO -1
«k
CO VO
-4 CO

°_ s -4
-«S

r» \ r*A X>
i-4 .00 cn
VO CO

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
141

movements remains the same. Exceptions to the tutti

arrangement are noted in the use of percussion

instrumentation.

Key progressions within each movement do not follow

common practice routines and often emphasize stepwise

motion. Exceptions occur in the first m o vem ent ’s use of

third relationships, the second movement’s use of fifth

relationships, and the fifth m o vem ent ’s varied use of

second, third, tritone, and fifth relationships. In keeping

with Milhaud’s preference for using a variety of linear and

polytonal effects, the key sequence for the entire

composition does not achieve closure in its original key.

The sequence of tonalities for the five movements are: Bb-

D-F-(a-A)-G. The sharp-key areas may present performance

problems to high school bands who limit the majority of

their repertoire to works in flat-key areas.

Thematic development is characterized by phrasal

asymmetry throughout the five movements, but is especially

evident in the second movement, the first part of the third

movement, and the fifth movement.

Part 2— Detailed Structural Analysis

These analysis discussions occur for the sake of

intellectual curiosity, structural understanding,and their

contribution to the emotive interpretation of the work. The

emotional and textural results of each section presented

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
r*
a < 1A
S

oo
o
’rt
T“

;ioo)
CM
11*1A

rA 1A

CM
IA
CM
T*
T” J -

<
T“ -3 -
&

,o
oo
o -a S'
T*

CO

3 -a
CM

*S| -a
> CM
-a

Structure Analysis Chart


9 CM
n -Mj

T-J-" -a
CM

Suite Francaiset I. Normandie


o
VO o -a o VO
CM
<a -a
CM

-a
VO

Figure U i Basal
«

s
-a 3
-a .n
M
T”
CM
o -a
m m
CM -a
as m -a OS

o
CM
$
K
d
T” -a o' CM

ra VO
<!

CM
W

■*< at

-a n
o vo oo

1
*4 o
+> g1
X
<D 1
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
142

below may be seen by viewing the corresponding section on

the Basal Structural Analysis graph, Figure 2.

The first movement of Suite F rancaise, Normandie, is

presented as an example of the Detailed Structural Analysis

(DSA) process. Normandie is divided into four large

sections plus a coda. Its thematic material is derived from

two principal ideas, the first of which consists of four

differing phrases (A1,A2,A3,A4) and the second (B) of two

differing phrases (81,B2). Asymmetrical phrase lengths are

use in the presentation of A, while symmetrical lengths are

utilized for the B phrases.

The basis of thematic development stems, generally,

from the complete repetition of the various A and 8 phrases,

with infrequent use of motivic or partial phrase

development. Abrupt harmonic variation is heavily relied

upon to achieve tensional momentum and a sense of change

with later phrase repetitions.

I. Normandie

Mm. 1-22

6/8 1 A 8,9 A2 13, * 1 4 A3 19,


8 4 1/2 6
(4+4)
19 al 22.
4
Bb:

The opening eight measures present a regular and

predictable Al theme. Asymmetry is introduced with (A2)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
143

which simply occurs as the extension of the last note of the

phrase. The third phrase of the A theme, (A3), continues

the stepwise character, adds an ascending stepwise bass line

and dotted-quarter arrival points at mm. 15,17, and 19, and

extends the original four measure phrase scheme by two full

measures. Bar 19 contains a thematic overlap of the A3

ending and the beginning of a l .

The bridge phrase beginning at m. 19 derives its new

melody" from the stepwise character and rhythmic patterning

of Al. The harmonic outline of the unison melodic line

stated by saxophones and horns is that of a majoi— minor 7th

chord built on the cadence pitch Bb (m. 19*).

Mm. 25-34

23 B 26,27 B2 50,31 b2+ai 34


4 4 4
(2 +2 )
g:

The 81 theme introduces a contrasting rhythmic phrase

which contains ( ). Theme B2 is formed by the

repetitive extension of the 6/8 triplet rhythm of the A

theme. The bridge phrase consists of a stepwise scale

passage which derives from the B2 triplet rhythm.

Beginning with B2 at m. 27, a polytonal relationship is

introduced and articulated in the outer parts. The B2 theme

is presented in G minor, while the bass support arpeggiates

an F7 chord. A prolongation of G minor, through descending

scale passages in the woodwinds, cadences on V7/Eb major (m.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
144

33i) and prepares the restatement of Al by the horns at m.

35 in the key of Eb major.

Mm. 35-63

35 A 42. **43 A4 48.49 A4 52.53 a4+b2 58.


8 6 4 6
(4+4) (4+2)
F# (mixolydian)
F# (major)
59 b2 63.
5
8:

The Al theme is restated in Eb major (IV/8b) against

linear writing of the accompaniment derived from the 82 6/8

triplet rhythm. The second statement of the four measure Al

theme comes to an unstable resolution at m. 42 with a

cadence on a 8b chord in the first inversion. The following

statement of A4 begins abruptly in F# major through the

enharmonic relationship (A#-Bb). The introduction of a new,

six measure A phrase, A4, at m. 43, corresponds in length to

the earlier six measure statement of A3. The harmonic

support of A4 gives the suggestion of F# mixolydian with the

use of E-natural in the bass voices. The use of the Ftt

leading tone (E#) does not happen until m. 50. The phrase

beginning in m. 49ff is simply a variation of A4, (A4i), and

provides only one climactic point (instead of the two

provided by A4). The A4* statement is articulated by the

snare drum at m. 502.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
145

The descending melodic contour of the first phrase of

the bridge, m. 53ff, is derived from the last 2-1/2 measures

of the preceding A4* , and is designated as a 4 * . Its

derivation from the latter half of a principal phrase causes

its secondary (small case) status. The following two

J measure ascending triplet phrase, mm. 57-58, is the

inversion of b2, and is so designated as a variation of b2,

(b2i).

At mm. 59-63, the motion of the bridge section is

continued through the use of a triplet melodic motive

derived from 82, b22 (the second figure to be derived from

82). The abrupt harmonic movement results in an unstable

sense of B major caused by the use of a pedal B-natural with

chordal movement in the upper voices providing the

alternating tonalities of F# major and B major. The cadence

of the section at m. 622 elicits a polytonal suggestion by

the use of an established B minor tonality which supports an

F#-C# harmonic interval.

Mm. 64-82

^4 A* 66,67 A2 70.71 A2 74,75 A2 78,79 A2* 82,


3-1/2 4 4 4 4
bill
G: V II VII
IV I bVIb vii7

M. 64 begins the statement of A with a shift of the

rhythmic opening to the second beat of the measure. This

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
146

momentarily alters the listener’s perception of downbeat

until the statement of A2 at m. 67. At m. 67ff, the

rhythmic instability created by the altered anacrusis and

the use of the moving eighth-note pattern, mm. 63-66, gives

way to melodic-rhythmic stability in the woodwinds,

reinforced by the stability of ongoing dotted-quarter note

chords in the low brass.

The low brass chords use an inverted C major chord as a

focal point of departure and arrival (mm. 67-69). The

entire parallel progression, mm. 67-70, creates instability

through the use of inverted chords. The unstable C major

focus occurs simultaneously with the presentation of the

melodic line by the woodwinds in D major.

Mm. 71-74 continually heightens the need for resolution

by repeating the melodic line in a wider register through

the addition of the flutes. The horns present the Al theme

in the tonality of A mixolydian and begin with the

previously heard rhythmic shift to the second beat. Mm. 75-

79 present a repetition of the textural setting of mm. 67-

70, but in different key areas and with the further

expansion of the top register by the piccolo.

At m. 75ff the polytonality of three tonal areas is

suggested with the use of theme A2 stated in Bb major (high

woodwinds), an F major statement of A2 in the clarinets,

alto saxophones, and cornets with support by an unstable

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
147

progression of parallel chords in the trombones which use

an Eb6 major chord as a focal point of departure.

The completion of this development section is finally

articulated with the harmonic arrival of V7/Bb major in a

progression of inversions at m. 79ff and the presentation of

the B1 theme dotted.rhythm ( m ) in the melodic line.

M. 79ff is considered a bridge, and separate from the

Development section, because some.sense of stability has

been achieved through the repetitive melodic rhythm and

slower harmonic rhythm.

Mm. 85-108

85 Al 86,^87 Al 90.91 A2 95.* 9 6 A3 101,


4 4 5-1/2 5-1/2

Bb:

102 ft2 1 0 5 , /104 ftl 108.


2 5

M. 83ff presents the recapitulation of A in' the

original key of Bb major in a more dense orchestration and

with the same sequence of phrasings. At m. lOlff, a coda of

A2 and A material is added with the extension of the 8b

tonal area. In view of this last grand statement of A

material via tutti orchestration, the statement of A at m.

103ff by unison cornets seems comically anti-climactic

before the arrival of the final V-I cadence.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER V

BASAL STRUCTURE ANALYSIS OF


PALESTRINA’S FOUR VOICE MOTET SICUT CERVUS
AND
DEBUSSY’S L ’APRES-MIDI D ’UN FAUNE

Analysis For Performance Of Palestrina’s Four Voice Motet


Sicut Cervus

/
#1. Review of preliminary comments regarding interpretation
and special instructions printed in the s c o r e .

This work was taken from Volume XI (pp. 42-44) of the

complete works of Palestrina. There are no instructions

regarding interpretation or performance practice indicated

in the score. Aside from the music itself, the only

instruction indicated is the assignment of vocal lines,

i.e., cantus, a l t us, tenor, b a s s u s .

42. Identification of Major Musical Moment(s) (MMM).

Mm. 18-20

Until this passage, harmonic intervals created by the

voice parts move quickly through consonance, dissonance, and

open intervals. Listener interest is sustained through the

coincidental contours of the four contrapuntally conceived

lines. Mm. 18-19 present the first contrast in harmonic

characteristic, which is heard apart from the previous

melding of the whole fabric, by the succession of thirds

between the cantus and altus parts. This is followed in m.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
149

20 by the emphasis on the harmonic interval of a sixth which

is created by the cantus and altus during beats one and two,

and the interval of a sixth between the cantus and the tenor

on beat three.

The climax of the phrase, which extends from m. 153 to

m. 23, occurs during this MMM at m. 20 with the placement of

the high "f" in the altus as part of the melodic

progression: 6-5-1, 1 being the "f". This melodic phrase is

written using half-notes which results in a pronounced

agogic accent.

Mm. 25-263

The second MMM also comes as the result of a succession

of four harmonic intervals of a sixth b tween the tenor and

bassus voices. The phrase is articulated by the progression

V6-IV6, in contrast to the recent IV-V (mm. 22-23)

progression. This MMM is further defined by the emphasis on

the lower vocal registers of tenor and b a s s u s .

Mm. 28^-292

A succession of five widely spaced harmonic intervals

of a third articulates this MMM. The beginning of the

section is marked by the deceptive cadence, V-IV. This is

the same type of harmonic punctuation that prepared the last

MMM, mm. 25-263.

Thus far, each of the M M M ’s has been characterized by a

progression of distinctly consonant intervals, i.e., thirds

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
150

and sixths. Each presentation has been within a different

and distinctly unique textural setting. For example, mm.

18-20 presented a mixture of close thirds and close sixths

in the treble voices; mm. 25-26 involved close sixths in the

lower voices; and mm. 28-29 was characterized by the

parallel movement by thirds in the outer voices through open

spacing, i.e., harmonic intervals of a tenth. Harmonic

progressions of the M M M ’s also emphasize the use of IV

chords.

Mm. 59-411

The beginning of this MMM is articulated by the cadence

resolution to IV at m. 3 9 i . The MMM is characterized by a

progression of accented non-harmonic tones and suspensions.

Mm. 48^-511

This MMM is initiated by a 7-6 suspension between tenor

and bassus voices followed by movement to a IV chord,

harmonic intervals of a third, an imitative 7-6 suspension

between cantus and altus voices, and the three voiced

simultaneity of the pitches D-A-D which closes the MMM, m.

511 .

#3. Identification of significant changes in texture.

For the analysis of this vocal piece, the term

"Orchestration" is being replaced by "Voicing". Therefore,

texture descriptions will be discussed in terms of the

following criteria, and will be indicated in outline fashion

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
151

according to the abbreviations indicated in parentheses for

each of the elements: Voicing (V)— density and register

[furthermore, voice abbreviations will occur as follows:

cantus (C), altus (A), tenor (T), bassus (B)]; tempo (T),

rhythm (R), articulation (A), dynamic (D), linear writing

(L).

Mm. 1-2
V: T, mid-register.
R: Long note values, whole and half notes.

Mm. 3-42
V: A ,T
R: Faster note values of quartei— note and eighth-
notes.

Mm. 43-62
V: C,A,T— density begins to increase markedly.
L: Homophonic.
R: Homorhythmic motion between altus and tenor.

Mm. 6 5 - 7 2
V: First appearance of full scoring: C,A,T,B
R: Sustained half-note in ba ss u s .

Mm. 73-82
V: Register emphasis moves to cantus and a l t u s .
R: Quartei— note and eighth-note motion.

Mm. 83-94
V: Four voices.
R: Homorhythmic characteristic of the outer voices.

M. 10
V: Emphasis on tenor and b a s s u s .
R: Quartei— note motion.

M. 11
V: Four voices— middle and upper registers.

M. 12
V: Emphasis on altus and tenoi upper register.
R: Emphasis on sustained half-notes.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
152

» iin ■ 1 rar
j l **t
*• _ A
1Wc?"
V: Emphasis on middle register.
R: Emphasis on quarter-note and eighth-note motion.

Mm. 153-17*
V: Four voices— middle register.
R: Quartei— note emphasis.

Mm. 183-191
V: Cantus and altus emphasis.
R: Homorhythmic ( ~ i J > J ) i
Mm. 192-214
V: Four voices.

M. 22
R: Sustaining whole-notes in A,T,B; solo-like quality
by virtue of being the only moving line.

Mm. 23-24
V: Altus emphasis via moving line.

Mm. 25-27
V: Emphasis on tenor and bassus via rhythmic motion.
R: Articulate the dotted rhythm.

Mm. 28-29
V: Emphasis on cantus and bassus via homorhythmic
scoring— middle register.
R: Articulate the dotted rhythm.

M. 30
R: Predominant quartei— note motion.-

Mm. 513-342
V: A,T,8— middle register emphasis.

Mm. 543-562
V: Four voice emphasis.

Mm. 563-58
V: C ,A ,T— middle register.

Mm. 39-47
V: Four voice emphasis; brief rests occur throughout
the parts causing momentary changes of texture.
M. 3 9 f f : A dense scoring texture— emphasis
on middle register.
R: The gesture must address the registers of greatest
rhythmic motion.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
153

M. 48
V: A ,T ,8
R: Syncopation in b a s s u s .

M. 49
V: Four voices.

M. 50
V: C ,A ,8
R: Syncopation in a l t u s .

Mm. 51-58
V: Four voice emphasis.

#4. Identification of emotional energy s eq u e n c e .

The essence of the emotional energy sequence is

produced by the number of lines used, the rhythmic character

of those lines, the degree of contrapuntal or homorhythmic

nature, and the harmonic results of line coincidence.

Mm. 1-4 (R )
V: Gradual expansion to three voices, T,A,C.
R: Use of longer note values; the fastest motion is
located in the tenor voice.
D: As attends the nature of contrapuntal settings, the
dynamics will vary in relation to the number of
voices in the texture and the subtle variations
which normally follow the contour of the melodic
line.

Mm. 5-62 (T— >)


V: Increase to three voices.
R: Sudden increase in homorhythmic, altus and tenor.

Mm. 63-17 (T )
V: Continual shifting from three to four voices.
R: Independent writing among the parts; homorhythmic
activity minimized. Active writing with quarter
and eighth notes predominating.

Mm. 18-22 (R— >)


V: Extended use of four voice texture.
R: Homorhythmic writing in cantus and altus.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
154

Mm. 25-36 (R )
V: Emphasis on three voice writing.
L: Homophonic.

Mm. 37-38 (T— >)


R: Increase in independent rhythmic activity.
Harmony (H): First use of chromatic alteration,
cadence, m. 38*, a l t u s .

Mm. 39-58 (T— *)


V: Establishes a predominantly four voice texture
throughout.
R: Remains active (quartei— note and eighth-note motion
throughout. Two full measures of rest occur,
mm. 48 and 50, resulting in a three voice
texture, and are contained in the last MMM,
mm. 48-50.

#5■ Selection of voices to be c u e d .

Voice part abbreviations will again be used to indicate

cues.

Mm. 1-17

M. 3: A. M. 4: C. M. 6 : 8 . M. 8: T. M. 11: A. M.
13: B. M. 15: C.

Mm. 18-36

M. 18: B. M. 19: T. M. 23: B. M. 26: C. M. 28: A.


M. 29: T. M. 31: B. M. 34: C. M. 36: A.

Mm. 37-58

M. 37: T. M. 39: B. M. 40: C. M. 41: A. M. 43: T.


M. 44: C. M. 45: B. M. 4 6 4 : a . M. 49: C. M. 51: T. M.
522; C. M. 552; A. M. 554; T.

#6. Identification of phrase lengths.

In general, the musical phrases coincide with phrases

of the text as marked by punctuation. These text phrases

are often defined by rests in the music. (Consult Figure 5

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
15U
at
>>
■a

<D
u
+>
o

■p
co
at co

irv
s
ui

Ph

as
•s «
o
\A &
$ 3*-1
's ls p ' { & *
* ^ l UN
'S <3 I 3 hK
-=t s cm |u \
CM
E* «* xr\
1“
B* CM ■-*»
•« vr\

r$N
CM
«aj (s cm
<4
Mm
1A

O O
SI £■ a5-. JpS-3-
I CA
CM Hn
•a! in
I
(ti
CM HN
-»J pT
W
«4 $
I o
CM r
tf
O
CM
d
5 *13^
m
•<

* 1A

1o\
*
frl
t*

0) 1
5 6
0}
•3p
m
3
+>
8
3
(0
-P •p Pi $
MMM

M o 0) § m
® o
I! U Gc

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
155

for a diagram of phrase lengths and the related thematic

material.)

Due to the contrapuntal nature of the entire

composition, an analysis of the individual vocal lines,

according to phrase lengths and thematic content, is not

helpful to the process of developing gesture choreography.

It is, however, helpful in the determination of pacing that

results in the assignment of emotional energy emphases to

the individual phrases and larger sections. These decisions

are based upon the structural purpose of the phrase or

section.

#7. Identification of form, thematic content, and major


tonal a r e a s .

The thematic development of the piece is based upon the

repetition, variation, and motivic development of three

principal ideas. The tenor voice presents the opening A

phrase of 6-1/2 measures— the initial 4-1/4 measures is

repeated exactly in the first statements of cantus and

bassus voices at mm. 4 and 6, respectively.

The altus voice presents a second melodic idea on which

a small amount thematic development is based, and is a tonal

variation of the A theme (A*).

The third melodic idea (A2) which, in fact, forms the

basis of development for the majority of the work, is

presented initially by the bassus at m. 23. The opening of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
156

A2 is merely a contour inversion of the A theme (Al). Its

rhythmic nature is clearly derived from Al.

The editor of the complete works edition has

transcribed the entire composition in the key of Fmajor,

and with the exception of one cadential, chromatic

alteration (m. 38, a l t u s ) , the entire piece is completely

diatonic. The strongest cadential effect, occurring in all

voices simultaneously, is located at mm. 22-23 and

articulates the ending of the first phrase of text. Other

types of cadences are weaker by virtue of continuous linear

writing in voices other than those containing the cadence

figures, deceptive chord progressions, and cadence

progressions with the fifth scale degree in the top voice.

#8. Identification of conductor p r o b l e m s .

The condu ct or’s principal concern will be one of

achieving balance among the vocal parts and an ease of

gesture choreography used to execute cues without disturbing

the mood of the work.

#9. Identification of player problem s.

The work does not present "typical" problem areas for

the vocalist, such as extensive range, angular thematic

contours, extensive chromatic alterations, or rhythmic

complexity. The s i n g e r s ’ principal concerns are those of

balance, blend, and intonation.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
157

Analysis For Performance Of D ebu ss y’s


L ’Apres-midi d ’un faune for Orchestra

#1 - Review of preliminary comments regarding interpretation


and special instructions printed in the s c o r e .

Special instructions which address interpretation or

stage set-up are not presented in the score.

The instrumentation is that of the standard Romantic

orchestra with two harps and English horn. Special

orchestration effects: muted strings, string tremolos, harp

glissandos, and the cuivre and bouche effects written for

horn.i

#2. Identification of Major Musical Moment(s) (MMM)

In most cases the M M M ’s are created by the use of

melody and harmony. In two instances, M M M ’s focus on short

chordal passages. (See Figure 6 for the graph of M M M ’s in

relation to the w o r k ’s other Level I constructs.)

iNorman Del Mar, Anatomy of the Orchestra (Los Angeles:


University of California Press, 1983), p. 249.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
/■—s
c\to
\A o
O , o
O
*— 157A

t *—
Pi
/-N _
o o
ON ^ ON

~3-
T ps
_PS
Eh
O o
00 m 00 M 4 J
'
bi'(
PS
t
^ Pi
9
c— EH
-* o
Eh
Eh PS T—
a
✓—s Eh
O K s
rJ
,a
’ o n
w"f
Pi c f'f
o o
'-T\ * if\ t1
PS
•a
EH w i
* 01
Eh a>
M

« 01 P-
a}
*— » Eh «
O
-4f EH 3

. w VO
a)

•H
PS o
O Eh
m
CM CM CO

✓->
ON 0 3

CM CO
o S o
CM
C
VJ
w <*«
S'
EH

<
o
T"

VO GO

«sj
PS _ - - m
hh«
T
o» 'A „
U
p
.r,p tsh >1
0) O
P<
fli o at G N
QJ 4Q»
> H|
Q
I: a Ihf.\ £ w s: t*

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
158

Mm. 1 7 -1 9

This first MMM derives from the A melodic idea— the

triplet motive. The triplet rhythm is extended over the

harmonic alternation of a Bb7 chord and a chord built on F

stacked with intervals of a third and one interval of a

second. The use of a pedal E# in m. 19 heightens the ending

of the MMM. This MMM marks the close of approximately half

of the opening A thematic section.

M. 30

This MMM consists of a melodic and harmonic cadence

figure on the dominant chord of E Major. The six measure

section which follows is the last variation section of the

opening A section. The attractive scoring of the Moment is

defined by the use of solo flute set with the low register

string accompaniment which effectively contrasts with the

preceding scoring of 2 flutes, bassoon, and strings.

Mm. 37-50

This section marks the beginning of the second major

thematic section. The oboe presents a diatonic and

rhythmically simple (B) theme which is followed one measure

later by the beginning of a countermelody in the celli and

characterized by contrasting, extended note values. These

melodies are set above sustained E Major and E7 chords in

lower registers. This temporary harmonic pedal effect

creates the charm of the MMM. The pedal effect continues in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
159

the next section, mm. 45-50, with harmonic movement from Bb

Major to Bb minor.

Mm. 55-54

A chromatic passage, caused by the melodic half-step

motion (Violins) written above the sustained minor 7th

harmonic interval, articulates the end of a four measure

transition into the third tneme section (C).

Mm. 67-70

This section is characterized by an extended setting of

majoi— minor seventh chord support for a stepwise melody

which also revolves around the seventh of the alternating

majoi— minor seventh chords of G b a n d E b minor.

Mm. 75-78

This MMM marks the close of the five measure transition

to the closing section (A*) of the work. The basses sustain

the tonic (Db) of the key above which the lines of the

clarinet and oboe weave a melodic focus around the minor

seventh. Additional emphasis on the seventh is heard in the

quartei— note counterline of the clarinet.

M. 105

This very brief chordal MMM is the last measure of

transition to the final section of the work. It presents a

major 8*3 chord which moves to an F# minor chord. This F#

chord creates a 9th chord sonority with the bass note '8’.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
160

An extension of the MMM is heard in the Harp I and II line

as the descending nalf-step line creates seventh sonorities

with the bass note *E’.

#3. Identification of significant changes in texture.

Mm. 1-3 (Flute solo, no conducting. The opening measures


establish the following textural
characteristics.) i
T: Ad libitum within context of J - 44.
M: 9/8
R: Unpredictable.
D: Piano.
L: Solo melodic line.
A: Legato.
0: Solo flute

Mm. 44-5 j
44.
9/8; m. 5: 6/8
Piano.
Two voice writing predominates in the winds while
the strings and Harp II present chordal
accompaniment.
A: Legato.
0: Very light, transparent density; emphasis on wind
color.

M. 6
0: Tacet.

Mm. 7-14
0: Very thin; middle register horn soli.
D: Decrescendo.

M- 15
T: Accelerando due to expressif (expressively)
marking.

Mm. 17-19
0: Tutti.
D: Crescendo.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
161

M. 20
Q: Thinning to clarinet and horn accompaniment.
D: Diminuendo.
T: Ritard.

li. 21
0: Three emphases— low (horn and cellos), middle
(harp), and high (flute).
T: New tempo established.

M. 22
0: Parallel movement with emphasis on high and low
register polarity.

M. 23
0: Middle register focus.

M- 24-26
0: Focus on lower registers of the string
accompaniment.

M. 28
M: 12/8
D: Gradual changes.

M. 29
M: 9/8

Mm. 27,29
0: Focus on lower string accompaniment.

M. 30
T: Slowing of rhythmic motion.
0: Violin II focus.

M. 51
R: Movement of rhythmic focus through the various
lines.
D: Layered effect of piano and pianissimo.
T: Establishes new tempo.

Mm. 32-33
M: 3/4

M. 54
M: 12/8
T: Slowing.

M. 55
M: 3/4
T: Increase.
0: Middle range.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
162

Mm. 57-58
T: M.M. = 72 established.

M. 59
D: Crescendo.

Mm. 44-45
D: Varying activity.
T: Increase in animation effect.

M m . 46—47
D: Arrival at f o r t e .
R: Motion through the sixteenth triplet figures.

Mm. 48-50
R: Regularity of quartei— note rhythm.
D: Beginning diminuendo.

Min. 51-52
R: Regular quartei— note rhythm.
0: High violin register.
T: M.M. = 60.

Mm. 53-54
D: Increase to p i a n o .

Mm. 55-57
0: Tutti.
D: Layered piano and pianissimo effect.
R: Syncopation in strings and winds.
T: M.M. = 56.

M. 58
D: Winds increase to mezzo f o r t e .

Mm. 59-60
0: Focus on strings due to rhythmic activity.
D: Crescendo.

Mm. 61-62
R: After m. 6 1 2 , focus on triplet rhythms of wind
voices.
D: Crescendo continues in winds and strings.

M m . 65~66
R: Shift to slower patterns.
D: Sudden pian is sim o.

Mm. 67-70
0: Focus on strings due to rhythmic activity.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
163

Mm. 68,70
R: Mark change to triplet rhythm.

M. 71
0: Lighter wind density.

M. 72
D: Sudden mezzo forte

M. 73
0: Emphasis on light string density.

Mm. 74-78
0: Emphasis on winds; middle range horn figure.
L: Independent lines established.

Mm. 79-82
0: Harp II line establishes rhythmic regularity;
middle register scoring.
M: 4/4
T: M.M. = 84

Mm. 83-84
0: Focus on middle register woodwind sound.

M. 85
0: Segmented linear passage scored through the
wo odw ind s.
R: Complexity in string movement.

Mm. 86-89
0: Thin, middle to low register.
R: Return to regularity via harp I passage,
D: Gradual changes.
m

Mm. 90-92
R: Combination of regular accompaniment and
unpredictability of English horn line.
A: Staccato.

M. 94
0: Tutti strings except basses.
A: Legato.

Mm. 95
R: Focus on duple subdivision.

Mm. 96-99
R: Regularity in winds via triplets; articulate the
occasional duple subdivision.
T: Interpretive increase in tempo.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
164

Mm. 100-101
R: Subdivision in harp I.

M. 102
M: 9/8
T: Ritard.

M m . 103 -104
R: Homorhythmic string accompaniment.
T: ‘a t e m p o ’

M. 105 -

T- Ritard.
R: Simultaneity of two against three.

M. 106
0: Harp fcous.
D: Pianississimo (Very, very soft).

#4. Identification of emotional energy sequence.

Mm. 1-14 (R--- ) Opening characteristics.


0 Solo flute; middle register.
T Slow.
R Unpr ed ictable.
A Legato.
D Soft.

Mm. 15--19 (T— »


0 Gradual increase; upper register focus.
D Cr e s c e n d i .
H Chromaticism.

M. 20 (R--»
0 Sharp release of all instruments except solo
clarinet and horns; middle register.
D Diminuendo.

Mm. 21--30 (R---)


0 Light; middle.
T Slow.
R Regularity established after the introduction of
each new pattern.
A Legato.
H Repeated, parallel movements.
D Basically soft with only slight, gradual changes.
L Homophonic.

Mm. 31 -38 (TR---)


0: High register, harp I.
R: Alternating changes of fast and slow rhythms.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
165

H: Chromatic Bb7.
D: Quick juxtapositions of gradual changes; sforzato
(forced accent) articulations; layering of forte,
p i a n o , and mezzo f o r t e .
L: Contrapuntal.

Mm. 39-40 (T— >)


0: Additional instruments in m. 40; higher violin
register.
R: Syncopation, agogic accent.
D: Crescendo.
L: Contrapuntal.

Mm. 41-43 (TR )


0: Follow movement of principal lines.
D: Quick alternation of louds and softs within a
gradual change setting.
L: Contrapuntal.

Mm. 44-45 (T— »


0: Tutti; quick change of principal sixtesnth-note
lines to different instruments; wide range
extending into the higher registers of flute,
oboe, and violin..
D: Quick changes via sudden and gradual effects.
L: Homophonic.

Mm. 46-47 (T )
0: Sustained motion in upper register of violins I;
octave leaps in English horn and clarinet lines.
R: Syncopation set against sustained lines.
H: Bb minor chord sustained.
D: F o r t e ; accent.
L: Emphasis on homorhythmic setting.

Mm. 43-50 (R— »


0: Gradually thinning; emphasis on low register except
for violins I.
T: Ritard.
R: Less complexity.
D: Diminuendo.
L: Homophonic.

Mm. 51-58 (R )
0: Descending pitch lines; emphasis on middle
r e gister.
H: Slower harmonic rhythm.
T: Slower.
D: Focus on soft dynamic.
A: Legato.
R: Regular.
L: Homophonic.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
166

Mm. 5 9 -6 2 (T — > )
Contrary motion.
Syncopation.
Legato-staccato blend in violins.
Chromaticism.
Crescendo.

Mm. 63-64 (£ ■— )
0: Tutti.
R: Simultaneity of faster and slower note values.
A: Simultaneity of detached over legato.
D: Simultaneity of quick, gradual changes over a
sustained pianissimo.

Mm. 65-66 ( )
D: Crescendo.

Mm. 67-69 (T— »


0: Increase in high register of flute and harp;
contrary motion.
Strings become .more rhythmically complex.
Large crescendo.
Contrapuntal.

M. 70 (T )
D: Sustained forte and fortissimo.

Mm. 71-73 (R— »


0 Gradual thinning; middle and lower registers.
T Ritard in m. 73
H Diato nic .
D = Decrescendo.

Mm. 74-82 (R )
0: Very transparent; emphasis shifts from wind soli to
string section accompanying the flute solo;
middle register.
Steady.
Legato.
Diatonic emphasis.
Pianissimo emphasis.
Homophonic.

Mm. 85-84 (TR )


R: Alternation of slow and faster rhythms.
A: Alternation of staccato and legato.
H: Alternation of diatonic and chromatic changes.
D: Alternation of steady levels with gradual changes

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
167

M. 85 (R— »
0: Descending pitch line passed from lighter to darker
instruments.
D: Decrescendo.
H: Diatonic.

Mm. 86-89 (R )
0: Very light; middle register.
T: Steady,
ft: Legato.
H: Very slow.
D: Soft.
L: Homophonic.

Mm. 90-91 (TR )


0: Movement from high to low instruments.
R: Alternation of slow and faster values.
D: Alternation ofgradual changes, accent.
L: Alternation of thin and dense writing.

Mm. 92-95 (R— > )


0: Gradual thinning; descending through registers.
T: Ritard.
R: Gradually slower note values.
A: Movement from staccato to legato.
L: Movement from widely spaced to closely spaced.

Mm. 94-110 (R-- )


0: Middle register.
T: Slow with only slight changes.
R: Somewhat unpredictable.
A: Emphasis on legato.
D: Very soft.
H: Basically homophonic.

#5. Selection of instruments to be c u e d .

Instruments were chosen by considering the following

criteria (derived from the music): (a) solo line, (b) soli

line, (c) counterline, (d) prominent line, (e) entrance

following rest period, (f) cuing the player(s) of a

sectional entrance who are located furthest from the

conductor in order to envelop the entire entering body of

musicians in the cue, (g) choice based upon the need to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
168

share the conduc to r’s attention among all the members of the

ensemble, and (h) the rhythm of the entering instrument sets

or controls the tempo.

Following each instrument listed below is the

corresponding letter of the appropriate cue-reason cited

above.

M m . 1-20

M. 1: Flute— a. M. 44 : Oboe— d. M. 4®: Horn— d. M.

51: Horn— c. M. 5 2 : Horn— e. M. 71: Violin I— d. M. 7 2 :

Horn— d. M. 81: HarpII— d. M. 9 2 ; Horn— c. M. Ill: Flute-

-a. Mm. 11-13: Attention to string tremolos. M. 133 : Horn-

-c. M. 144 : Oboe— d. M. 151: Bassoon— c. M. 164 :

Clarinet— c. M. 171: Expansive tutti invitation. M. 1 8 i :

Violins I— c. M. 2 0 i : Clarinet— a.

Mm. 21-43

M. 211: Flute— a; 21?: Harp II— d; 2110: Violin II—

d. M. 237: Harp II— d; 2 3 i o : Celli— c. M. 257; Viola— d.

M. 26i: Flute— a; 267: Harp II— d. M. 284 : Bassoon— d. M.

301°: Clarinet— c; 301 i : Horn— c. M. 31s : Celli— c; 31s :

Violins II— d. M. 323 : Violin I— d. M. 341: Horn I— d;

345: Celli— c; 3 4 8 : Viola— d. M. 353; Celli— d. M. 361:

Flute— a. M. 371: Oboe— a. M. 381: Celli— c. M. 393 :

Clarinet— b,c. M. 401: Violin I— c. M. 4 1 i : Bass— c,d. M.

421; Clarinet— a. M. 43*: Violin I— b.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
169

Mm. 44-73

M. 441: Flute— -b; 443; English horn— c. M. 471: Horn

I— c. M. 501: Flute— h. M. 51: Clarinet— a. M. 52: Flute-

-d. M. 53: Celli— d. M. 55: Flute— b. M. 56: Horn— d. M.

58: Bassoon— d. M. 60: Violins I— c. M. 61: Horn— c. M.

63: Flute— c. M. 67: Horn III— c. M. 712: Clarinet— d. M.

722; Flute— d. M. 733: Harp— d.

Mm. 74-89

M. 74: Horn I— a. M. 75: Clarinet— a. M. 76: Violin—

a. M. 77: Flute— a. M. 79i : Bass— d; 793: Flute— d. M.

82: Clarinet— d. M. 8 3 i : Horn— h; 834 : Clarinet— h. M.

841: Oboe— a; 84*: Bassoon— d. M. 851: Flute— d; 852;

Oboe— d; 853; Clarinet— d; 854 : Horns— d. M. 861: Violins

II— d; 863 : Oboe— a. M. 8 9 i ; Clarinet— d.

Mm. 90-110

M. 901: English horn— a; 904 : Bass— d. M. 914 :

Clarinet— d. M. 92: Flute— d; 923: Horn I— a. M. 9 3 i :

Harp— d; 933; Horn— a. M. 9 4 i : Flute— b; 944 : Antique

cymbals— d. M. 951: Violins— c. M. 964 : Antique cymbals—

d. M. 974 : Antique cymbals— d. M. 98: Horn I— a. M. 100:

Viola— a. M. 102: Oboe— d. M. 103: Oboe— a. M. 104:

Violins I— c. M. 105: Violin I— c. M. 106: Harps— b. M.

107: Horn— b. M. 1081: Flute— a; 1082,4 : Harp— d. M.

1091: Violin I— b; 1094 : Celli— d. M. 1101: Cello— d.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
170

#6. Identification of phrase le ng ths .

While phrases frequently vary in length from one to

five measures, the variations do not pose conducting

concerns because of the slower tempi. Moreover, the

majority of phrases are symmetrical.

#7. Identification of form, thematic content, and major


tonal a rea s.

The overall structure divides into three major tonal

movements,

E Major Mm. 1-54

Db Major Mm. 55-78

E Major Mm. 79-110

and into four sections according to principal thematic

presentations. (See Figure 7.)

The A theme is five measures in length and is

characterized by its initial chromatic, stepwise movement.

A-theme material provides a rounded form by beginning and

ending the work. The second and third themes are

distinguished from the first by their intervallic sequence

and predominantly diatonic structure. Thematic development

proceeds by thematic repetition, and motivic and phrasal

development.

The first and second themes share the tonality of E

major, while a unifying factor exists between the second and

third themes with the emphasis on syncopation in the later

portion of their thematic statements. The first two themes

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CM 170A
in ii

vo
1I ™
-

-a
he ?
m
U
VO
o

fauna
CM
r'Y* in
o

C2

d'un
0

- ' A

Chart, L'Aprea-mldl
« n
o

1
■cm
o

|CM +
m + cm

in
CM
0\
Ov

Structure
■f
i**»
♦ CM
.Q
r> n CM
ci
VO
ON
CM +

Overall
in
o \

O
L
CM
o\
7*
n
+
X CM ov Pigure
CM
C-
T“
^cS
\o
o
o>

CM fd:
o
O CM
+ vo
CM
m -1
. CM
u >
J r*CM♦
VO
CD
3

m
*co
in
-d
^
^ C♦M en ♦
CM CM

j co
m

VO
2
UN
00
CM
in ♦
me*
+ n CM
CM

w M

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
171

are introduced by solo instruments, in contrast to the third

theme's introduction by four instrumental doublings. The

three themes also contrast in their characteristic note

values.

#8. Identification of conductor p rob lem s.

A clear conducting pattern is especially required

throughout the composition because of the frequent meter

changes and beat subdivisions that are necessary to execute

certain passages made complex by the tempo changes and

contrasting rhythmic juxtapositions. The following

conducting suggestions are offered for the effective

communication of certain passages:

M. 4— conducted in '9' due to the slower tempo; m. 15—

conduct the three subdivisions of the first dotted-quarter

beat only, due to the gradual increase in tempo which

follows as a result of the expressif marking; m.20—

conducting subdivisions of the dotted-quarter beat begins

here due to the slowing tempo; m. 31— conduct the dotted-

quarter subdivision due to rhythmic complexity; mm. 32-33—

these two measures can be conducted in '3' due to their

rhythmic simplicity; m. 34— conducted in '12' due to

rhythmic complexity; m. 35ff— conducted in ‘3' due to

rhythmic simplicity.

M. 50— conducted in subdivided '3' due to slowing

tempo; m. 51ff— resume conducting in three due to fa c j r

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
172

tempo; m. 74ff— conduct in six due to rhythmic

juxtaposition; ^1. 73— conduct the last sixteenth count as

the tempo broadens in order to articulate the new section at

m. 79ff; m. 79ff— resume conducting the beat unit; m. 93—

subdivide beats three and four due to slowing tempo; m. 94—

conduct in *12’, and m. 95— conduct in *8’, both due to

rhythmic complexity; mm. 96-99— conducted in '4’ due to

rhythmic simplicity and increase in tempo.

Mm. 100-101: conducted in '12’ due to divisions of

flute and harp dotted-quarter beats; m. 102— conducted in

'9’ due to slowing tempo; mm. 103-104: conducted in '3’ due

to increase in tempo; m. 105— conduct the triple subdivision

of the first and third dotted-quarter beats in order to

assist proper execution of the simultaneous duplet and

triplet rhythms; m. 106— conduct in '8’ due to harp

subdivisions; m. 107— conduct in '12’ due to rhythmic

subdivisions; m. 108— conduct in '4’ due to rhythmic

division; m. 1 0 9 2 — conduct a subdivision of the beat for the

flute movement.

#9. Identification of player p r o b l e m s .

Intonation and blend throughout lightly scored and soft

dynamic sections, e.g., m. 4: Oboe, clarinets, horn; mm. 7-

8: strings; mm. 106-110: strings and horn sustained tones.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
173

Rhythmic complexity, e.g., m. 31: celli rhythm, varied

rhythmic juxtaposition; m. 83: Oboe solo in duple divisions

against the triple rhythms of the horns.

Exposed solo entrances that must occur in the proper

t e m po, e.g., mm. 13-14: Horns and oboe.

Ensemble pl aying, e.g., m. 25: string pizzicato at a

very soft dynamic; m. 67: string section rhythms in four

against the woodwind and horn rhythm in three; m. 85: the

sequence of wind entrances; the execution of the string

passage; m. 107: execution of the strings and horn together,

given the distance in seating between them.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX I

CASP SCORE PREPARATION

FORMS FOR DATA COLLECTION AND RETENTION

1. Preliminary Analysis Card

2. BSA Checklist

3. Bibliographic Information Card

4. Basal Structure Analysis Review

5. Aural Analysis
(Element Data Collection Form)

6. Stylistic Descriptive Summary

7. Summary of Conductor Problems

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
175

The following is a description of the various forms

used with the CASP score analysis process. These forms

permit the efficient collection of various types of data for

immediate as well as later reference by the conductor.

Information of this type is especially useful for the

teacher of conducting as a means of surveying repertoire in

order to select the most appropriate pieces for study at a

particular level of conducting skill.

The forms are listed on separate pages, in numbered

order, immediately following these pages of definition.

Form #1: "Preliminary Analysis Card"

This 4"x6" card is used to record general information

about a piece of music which the conductor has heard

performed in concert or by recording. The intention of the

card is to record aural impressions that have been gained

without the assistance of score analysis.

The card was originally intended to be used for the

collection of pieces that one might feel to be worthy of

study or concert programming; however, it is useful to take

note of music not so evaluated so that, should a particular

piece be discussed at a later time, one might have a record

of having known the piece, thus eliminating a later need to

relisten or re-study. Of course, some aural evaluations may

be more a reflection of our mood at the time of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
176

evaluation than of our most "honest" evaluation, and it is

always best to keep an open mind.

The information on these cards is designed to be useful

until a more thorough evaluation can be made through score

analysis. The results of this latter evaluation are then

recorded on the "Bibliographic Information Card" (Form #3).

Form #2: "8SA Checklist"

This 4"x5-l/2" form is used as a category check-off

list when a Basal Structure Analysis is being performed.

Use of the check-off lists will permit the alternating study

of several scores in an orderly manner.

Form #3: "Bibliographic Information Card"

This 5"x8" card is used to record various items of

bibliographic and musical detail for pieces that are being

seriously studied and prepared for concert performance. The

card provides areas for recording three sets of information,

each with a specific purpose.

Information provided by the line of elements listed

along the top of the card is especially useful when

considering concert programming, or when selecting pieces

for educational purposes. Usually, a concert program

consists of some variety in the nature and content of

repertoire, and this type of information will, for example,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
177

elimina.te redundancies such as having all or most of the

selections in one key, in the same tempo, or in the same

form or style. These elements are described as follows:

Medium (type of ensemble for which the piece is written),

Classification (the form of the piece, e.g., march,

overture, suite), Dynamic (the general volume level of the

entire composition), Tempo (the general pace of the work,

e.g., fast, slow, mixed tempi), Meter (indicates a singular

or mixed metered piece), Time (length of pe rformance), Grade

(the grading of the piece according to level of difficulty),

Style (general style of writing such as Baroque, Romantic,

Jazz), Key (the tonal center of the composition), Preference

(the eval uat or ’s personal grade regarding his enjoyment of

the work).

The information used to describe the musical elements

is meant to be abbreviated along the top of the card for

easy reference. Extended discussions of these and other

unique aspects of the piece are included under the "Remarks"

heading.

Information listed under the top line of musical

elements simply refers to bibliographic data which is used

to order or rent the composition. "Score" refers to whether

a condensed or full score is provided.

The items listed under "Supplementary Information On

File" refer to personal files of the conductor which contain

information on composers and works. These files should be

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
178

kept up-to-date as new works, or works that are new to the

conductor, are studied. "Comp 8io" refers to biographical

information that has affected the composer’s musical

development; "Style Des" refers to information on the

historical style of which the composition cited on the card

is representative; "Aural" refers to information collected

from a synthesis of aural analysis and Level I— Basal

Structure Analysis and is recorded on the "Basal Strcuture

Analysis Review" (Form #4); "Theoretical" refers to

information gained from a CASP: Level III— Detailed

Structural Analysis of the work. Forms are not provided for

the recording of a Level III analysis because of the

normally extensive length.

Form #4: "Basal Structure Analysis Review"

The initial portion of this foui— page form asks simply

for an indication of the information contained on the

"Bibliographic Information Card" and is to be completed as

the Basal Structure Analysis proceeds. This musical element

and bibliographic data will then be transferred to the

"Bibliographic Information Card" for easier reference.

The topics that are addressed in the body of these

sheets require a statement of information gained through the

Level I— Basal Structure Analysis process. Some clarifying

comments may help to explain the following: "A. General

Remarks & Response To The Work"— statements regarding the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
179

evaluator’s enjoyment of the work and any especially unique

aspects of the work; "B. Genesis Of The Composition"— why

was the work composed?; "C. Conductor Difficulty" and "D.

Player Difficulty"— cite specific score areas and types of

difficulty peculiar to conductor and players, respectively;

"E. Composer Life Style"— particular events in the

composer’s life that may have led to and surrounded the

composition of the work; "F. Stylistic Developments"—

discussions of stylistic characteristics of the composer or

style period that are introduced or furthered by this work;

"G. Especial Descriptives"— any special needs in terms of

musicianship or environment that may be necessary to

properly perform the work; "H. Conclusions of the Basal

Structure Analysis"— discussion of conclusisons drawn by

the CASP: Level I process, especially in the areas of major

climactic moments and sequence of emotional energy as they

relate to the overall structural design.

Form #5: "Aural Analysis"

This 8 ”xll" sheet is used to record general stylistic

information gained from the aural analysis of a work, or

part of a work. The evaluator completes the sheet by

entering descriptive comments on the c om pos er ’s use of each

element cited on the sheet. Summary conclusions regarding

stylistic description may then be entered for an entire work

on the "Stylistic Descriptive Summary" sheet (Form #6).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
180

Form #6: "Stylistic Descriptive Summary"

This 8"xll", two-page sheet is used to record the

summary of the co mp o s e r ’s use of musical elements. This

results in the identification of various stylistic

characteristics. Collections of information about composers

allow the conductor to compare stylistic details between

various composers and to other style periods.

Form #7: "Summary of Conductor Difficulty"

This 8"xll" sheet is used to record the nature and

location of specific areas of gesture difficulty that may

attend a particular composition. This information is

especially useful for teachers of literature survey courses

because of the dual nature of such courses to provide

discussion of interpretation rationale and problem areas

pertaining to conducting technique.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
COMPOSER (Dates} COMPOSITION (Pate)
HE I Form, Meter, Tempo, Thematic Development, Harmony, Dynamic,
Texture, Special EffectB, Notable Characteristics."

L H S /2 -6 5

PCBM #1: PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS CARD

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Composer/Composition

BSA CHECKLIST

1. Review of score instructions.

2a. MMM 2b. Time:_

3. Textural Structure.

4. Phrase lengths.

5. Emotional Energy.

6. Cues.

7. Conductor Problems.

8. Player P r o b l e m s .

9. Tonal M o v e m e n t .

10. Theme Sheet.

L H S / R e v . 9-86

PQHM #2: BSA CHECKLIST

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Medium Cl Dyn Tempo Meter Time Gr Style Key Pref

Composer/Dates Composition/Date

Accomp. Medium No. of Movts/Key Arranger/Editor.

Publisher::Year Score Cost/Year

Supplementary Information On File:

Composer Bio: Style Pes: Aural: Theoretical:

REMARKS:

LHs /9 -8 5

FCSM #3 s BIBLIOGRAPHIC INFORMATION CARD

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Medium cl Dyn Tempo Meter Tine Cr Style Key Pref

Composer/Dates Composition/bate

No. of Mort/Key Arranger/Editor

Publishers slear Score Coet/lear

Supplementary Znforaatlon On Filet

Composer Blot Style Pest Auralt Theoreticali

* * »

A. General Remarks & Response to the Works

B. Genesis of the Composition:

FORM & s BASAL STRUCTURE ANALYSIS REVIEW

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
C. Conductor Difficulty!

D. Player Difficulty!

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
8. Cooposer U f a Stylet

P. Styliatlo Davalopaentai

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Go 3*racial PraBcriptiTeei

H« Conclusions of Baetd Structure AnalysiBi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
AURAL ANALYSIS ________ ___________ _____________ ____
Composer/Composition/Movement Page •

INSTRUMENTATION ________________________________________________

COMPOSITION TYPE ________________________________ __________ ____

P O R M ______________________________________ __________

ORCHESTRATION
Theme ____________________
Register ________________________________________________
. Countermelody _________________
Accompaniment

TIME ELEMENTS
Tempo ________________________ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ ______
Meter -
Rhythm .

THEME
Length _ _ _ _ _____________________ ,
_______________
Development _____ _
Character - ■,

HARMONY ________ _______________________________________

DYNAMICS ________ _______________________ ________________

TEXTURE ______________________________________________

REMARKSt

LHS/9-79

FORM #5 * AURAL A1ALTSIS


(Element Data Collection Form)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
STYLISTIC DESCRIPTIVE SUMMARY

• Pagw
Composer/Composition/Movement — — — — — — — —

IA. COMPOSITION TYPE__________________ IB. FORM

II. INSTRUMENTATIONI

III. ORCHESTRATION (theme, register, e-melody, accompanisnat):

17. TIME ELEMENTS (tempo, meter, rhythm):

7. THEMATIC DBVELOfMENT (length, character):

FORM £61 STYLISTIC DESCRIPTIVE SUMMARY

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
VI. HARMOHTl

VII. DTHAMICSi

VIII. TEXTUREI

IX. REMARKS l

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
SUMMARY OF CONDUCTOR PROBLEMS

Composer Composition

I. TEMPO

II. RHYTHM

III. TEXTURE

IV. ARTICULATION

V. DYNAMICS

VI. METER

VII. CUES

VIII. BALANCE

Remarks on reverse side

FORM #7: SUMMARY OF CONDUCTOR PROBLEMS

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
APPENDIX II

BASAL STRUCTURE ANALYSIS CHARTS

FOR MOVEMENTS II, III, IV, V

OF

DARIUS MILHAUD'S SUITE FRANCAISE

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
193

03
+=
a

CM
T“
¥. £

II
o O
vo CM vo
St

Basal Structure Analysis Chart, Suite Francalsei


H£v

CM
CM
orv
i o
xrv

CM
m £
n
cm•’tr
T“
o ffl o
st -3-

^CM4
O m
o CM
CM
w
O
\r\
cn

~ 2t

CVJ o
o
CM
PQ o
pq CM
CM
St
pp
CM
ai
VO
cn
CM CM

c^> a
a>
VO CO

a
+>
•H bo o 0> M o
€ (U
x o a o >» 2 +»
a> si
a
Q)
IH IJ? Pm « eh Eh

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
19U

-c

f t

o o

III. lie De Prance


EH I—
r— T“ -3
pq " "
PS

B2
-3
rq
PS Eh
o T" -3
vo « o
vo
Pn
I
PS
00 -3
,0

CM -3

Basal Structure Analysis Chart, Suite Francalse:


cd
ps
CM c
u\ Eh rQ 1f\
h I
iv
CM r<~i
>
o
p
T*“ -3
PS Eh
o
s Ov|
-3

T— -3
PS pq

CM -3
O
O^v pq pq c
m
^ PS E^
V- -3
pq
r—
o
CM <H|CV
-3

o V O
CM cd CM CM
T“
•Hkv
U\

CM
T— CM
cd
«-Scv *c
S! T— CM

ft
T“ «H
cd CM •H
Pei 0~ >

0)
I
X
0)
5 fe
S 8
<H
>
O O
_» aa>
® , ©
0)
-p
o


E3
o
EH $ pJ Pi M HSa®

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
TV \ \A
-O
CM
cd W
o o 195
o vo o
T*“
CM

x
-a
+
-a
o
ON
o
ON

-a-
o
CO
ON -a
a
pq

CM
m

IV. Alsace-Lorraine
o
c~- -af °
pq
CM
-a a»
&
CM
pq +
pq CM
e* O
s
E-i
< OJ VO

Hw
\JS
*T@

Francaieet
o o
UN -d- v>
CVJ
•<

Chart, Suite
•oS
o
-a- -3* S

c^\
w Analysis
CVJ
o
ON pq PP o

pq
Structure

ffi
CM
* +
« CM
O +
CM
CM CM O
cd + CM
CM w
Basal

Cr\
S! +
-d* s
oA
V\
II
— *%
-p
a
a>
CM - J j ►q

a)
3a '
u
+>
•2& O Qi o
4J U
^ a a>
x
at
o a> n >> Q) +* a1
0) £ a) Q)
El ss t4 Eh S Eh

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
196

O '! U\
< cd PQ CM

O
OO
CM
•Q CM

CM
m tp

cq
o

V. Provence
CM CM
+
OS rr\

O <$
o
VO CM VO
&
t

ffrancalse:
E-i cd

1A
v-
C « E-
sf £ at,
U\ & s

Suite
O
o vo

Chart,
o cS
-3- 3

Analysis
<1
CM
+
-=r
o or\
f
c'l

Structure
CM CM

w
o a c
Basal

CM CM

CM
T™ tr\ co
o! cn

v\

C5 o

o> 1
-p ° CD M o
X
t a <u
0> o >»S9Ji 4d0
d) »
s'
Eh Ph w eh a £

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
197

SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY

Books

Bamberger, Carl. The Conductor's A r t . New York: McGraw-


Hill, 1965.

Barra, Donald. The Dynamic Performance: A Performer's Guide


to Musical Expression and Interpretation. Toronto:
Prentice-Hall Canada Inc., 1983.

Blackman, Charles. Behind the B a t o n . New York: Charos


Enterprises, Inc., 1964.

Blum, David. Casals and the Art of Interpretation. Los


Angeles: University of Cali.fornia Press, 1977.

Boult, Sir Adrian Cedric. A Handbook on the Technique of


Conducting. London: Paterson's Publishing Ltd.,
1968.

Boult, Sir Adrian Cedric. Thoughts on Co ndu ct ing . London:


Phoenix House Ltd., 1963.

Bowles, Michael. The Art of C o n d uct in g. New York:


Doubleday, 1959.

Braithwaite, Warwick. The Conductor's A r t . London:


Williams and Norgate Ltd., 1952.

Busch, Brian R. The Complete Choral Conductor Gesture and


Method. New York: Schirmer Books, 198

Cone, Edward T. Musical Form and Musical Performance.


New York: W.W. Norton, 1968.

Cooper, Grosvenor and Meyer, Leonard B. Rhythmic Structure


of Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960.

Davison, Archibald T. Choral Co nd ucting. 2d ed.


Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1968,.

Del Mar, Norman. Anatomy of the O rchestra. Los Angeles:


University of California Press, 1983.

Ewen, David. The Man with the B a t o n . New York: Thomas


Y. Crowell Co., 1936.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
198

Fuchs, Peter Paul. The Psychology of C ond ucting. New York:


MCA Music, 1969.

Garretson, Robert L. Conducting Choral M u s i c . 4th ed.


8oston: Allyn and Bacon, Inc., 1975.

Goldbeck, Frederick. The Perfect Conductor. New York:


Pellegrini and Cudahy, 1951.

Green, Elizabeth A.H.; and Malko, Nicolai. The Conductor


and His S c o r e . Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, Inc.,
1975.

Kendrie, Frank S': Handbook on Conducting and Orchestral


Routine. New York: The H.W. Gray Co., 1930.

Krueger, Karl. The Nay of the Co nductor. New York:


Scribner and Sons, 1958.

Lamb, Gordon H. Choral Techniques. Dubuque, Iowa: W.C.


Brown, 1974.

LaRue, Jan. Guidelines for Style Analysis. New York: W.W.


Norton, 1970.

Lundin, Robert W. An Objective Psychology of M u s i c . New


York: Ronald Press, 1967.

Marple, Hugo D. The Beginning Conductor. New York:


McGraw-Hill, 1972.

Meyer, Leonard B. Explaining Music: Essays and


Explorations. Los Angeles: University of California
Press, 1973.

Moe, Daniel. Basic Choral Conce pts . Minneapolis:


Augsburg Publishing House, 1972.

Prausnitz, Frederik. Score and Podium. New York: W.W.


Norton, 1983.

Rudolf, Max. The Grammar of Conducting. 2d ed. New


York: Schirmer, 1980.

Schenker, Heinrich. Five Graphic Music A n aly ses . New York:


Dover, 1969.

Scherchen, Hermann. Handbook on Conducting. 2d ed.


London: Oxford University Press, 1942.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
199

Sessions, Roger. The Musical Experience of Composer,


Performer, Li s t e n e r . Princeton: Princeton University
Press, 1950.

Smith, Normand; and Stoutamire, Albert. 2d ed. Band Music


N o t e s . San Diego: Kjos Publishers, 1979.

Stanton, Harry E. Helping Students L e a r n . Washington,


D.C.: University Press of America, 1978.

Stoessel, Albert. The Technic of the Baton. New York:


Carl Fischer, Inc., 1928.

White, John D. The Analysis of M u s i c . 2d ed. London:


Scarecrow Press, 1984.

JournaLs

Clifton, Thomas. "Some Comparisons Between Intuitive and


Scientific Descriptions of Music." Journal of Music
Theory 19 (Spring 1975): 66-111.

Cone, Edward T. "Analysis Today." Musical Quarterly 46


(1960): 172.

Coover, James. "Music Theory in Translation:- A


Bibliography." Journal of Music Theory 3 (April
1959): 70-95.

Crickmore, Leon. "The Musical Gestalt." Music Review 33


(1972): 285.

Fellner, Rudolph. "DND: A Better Way of Marking Orchestra


Scores." Opera Journal 25 (November 1982): 10-14.

Ferrara, Lawrence. "Phenomenology as a Tool for Musical


Analysis." Musical Quarterly 70 (1984): 355-73.

Forte, Allen. "Schenker's Conception of Musical Structure."


Journal of Music Theory 3 (1959): 1-30.

Jacobs, Robert L. "A Gestalt Psychologist on Music." Music


Review 17 (1956): 185.

Kalabin, Fedor. "Score Preparation: Suggestions and


Solutions." Opera Journal 18 (1985): 21-28.

Keller, Hans. "Functional Analysis: Its Pure Application."


Music Review 18 (1957): 202.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
200

Kerr, W.A. "Psychological Effects of Music as Reported by


162 Defense Trainees." Psychological Record 5 (1942):
205-12.

La Rue, Jan. "Fundamental Considerations in Style


Analysis." Notes 25 (1969): 447-464.

____________ . "On Style Analysis." Journal of Music Theory


6 (Spring 1962): 91.

Mann, Michael. "Schenker’s Contribution to Music Theory."


Music Review 10 (1949): 3.

McMullin, Michael. "Musical Analysis and Appreciation: A


Critique of "Pure M u s i c . ’" Music Review 45:1 (1984):
47-60.

Mills, Donn Laurence. "Teaching Artistry." Instrumentalist


40:6 (January 1986): 66-69.'

Nordgren, Quentin R. "A M e a s u r e 1of Textural Patterns and


Strengths." Journal of Music Theory 4 (1960): 19-31.

Oelrich, Jean. "Claudio Abbado— The Human Touch."


Instrumentalist 39:2 (September 1984): 32.

Ostling, Jr. Acton. "Research on Nonverbal Communication


with Implications for Conductors." Journal of 8and
Research 12 (1977): 29.

Schmalfeldt, Janet. "On the Relation of Analysis to


Performance: Beethoven’s Bagatelles Op. 126 Nos. 2,
5." Journal of Music Theory 29 (Spring 1985): 1-32.

Talley, V. Howard. "A Critique of Musical Analysis."


Journal of the American Musicological Society 6 (1953)
87.

Willard, Raymond. "Speak Less, Communicate More."


Instrumentalist 40:6 (January 1S86): 38-41.

Youngblood, Joseph E. "Style as Information." Journal of


Music Theory 2 (April 1958): 24-35.

Reviews

Review of Tensions in the Performance of Music edited by


Carola Grindea. Music Review 44 (1983): 304-305.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
201

Scores

Boulez, Pierre. Structures. Wien: Universal Edition,


c . 1955-67.

Debussy, Claude. Prelude to "Afternoon of ft Faun."


A Norton Critical Score. New York: W.W. Norton, 1970.

Milhaud, Darius. Suite F r anc ai se. New York: MCA Music,


1947.

Penderecki, Krzysztof. Pittsburgh Ov er tur e. New York:


C.F. Peters, 1967.

Scalera, Fratelli, ed. Le Opere Complete Pi Giovanni


Pierluigi Da Palestrina XI:42-44 Roma: Incisoria e
Stamperia Musicals Biagiotti— Via Fos, 1941.

Williams, Clifton. Symphonic Dance No. 3 . New York: Sam


Fox, 1967.

Reference Works

The New Harvard Dictionary of M u s i c . 5th ed.


Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press,
1986.

Sadie, Stanley, ed. "Composition." The New Grove


Dictionary of Music and Musicians 4 (1980): 599-
602.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Potrebbero piacerti anche