Sei sulla pagina 1di 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/220420019

Contextual correlates of synonymy

Article  in  Communications of the ACM · October 1965


DOI: 10.1145/365628.365657 · Source: DBLP

CITATIONS READS
554 1,091

2 authors, including:

John Goodenough
Carnegie Mellon University
73 PUBLICATIONS   3,069 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Eliminative Argumentation View project

All content following this page was uploaded by John Goodenough on 30 December 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


A. G. OETTINGER, Editor

Contextual Correlates of Synonymy in this paper is the assumI)tion that the degree of semantic
similarity existing between a pair of words is indicated by
the frequency with which they stand in first-order asso-
[[ERBERT [{UB:ENST:EINAND JOHN B. GOODENOUGII ciation to the same words, More specifically we test what
Decision Sciences Laboratory, L. G. Hanscom Field, Bedford, is aetuMly a converse of the assumption; namely, that the
ltIasscl~c~<4ts. more similar in meaning two words are, the greater will
be the mimber of first-order associates they have in eom-
inon.
Experimentol corroboration was obtained for the hypothesis
that the proportion of words common to the contexts of word A I t is not a new notion that words which are similar in
and to the contexts of word B is a function of the degree to meaning occur in similar contexts. Joos [3] defined the
which A and B are similar in meaning. The tests were carried meaning of a morpheme as "the set of conditional prob-
out for variously defined contexts. The shapes of the functions, abilities of its occurrence in context with all other mor-
however, indicate that similarity of context is reliable as cri- phemes." From this definition it was a slight step to Harris'
terion only for detecting pairs of words that are very similar view [2]: " I f we consider words or morphemes A and B to
in meaning. be more different in meaning than A and C, then we will
often find that the [contextual] distributions of A and B
Introduction are more different than the distributions of A and C."
While it seems evident that words which are very similar
This s t u d y is concerned with t:he relationship between in meaning will indeed be very similar in contextual dis-
similarity of context and similarity of meaning (syn- tribution and, contrarily, words which are completely
onymy). /\lore specifieMly, we asked how the proportion dissimilar in meaning will be very dissimilar in their
of words commott to contexts containing word A and to contextual distributions, the nature of the relationship
contexis containing word B was related to the degree to for intermediate values of synonymy cannot be even con-
which A a n d B were similar in meaning. The existence of jectured.
this positive relationship is a basic assumption of statisti- The general approach is to compare contexts from a
cal association methods in information retrieval [6]. These corpus of sentences especially generated for a small set of
methods assume that pairs of words which h a v e m a n y words (hereafter called 'theme words') with human judg-
contexts in c o m m o n are semantically closely related and ments of the degree of s y n o n y m y existing between pairs
consequently that items of information in which either of these theme words. Similarity of contextual distribution
word occurs will tend to be relevant to the s a m e query. is measured by the overlap between the sentence sets of
To the extent that this assumption is invalid statistical the themes being compared. However context is defined
methods ca, nnot be reliable; our purpose here is to investi- in four different ways, each definition specifying a different
gate the w d i d i t y of this assumption. set of conditions under which words in the sentence set
The more sophisticated of these methods distinguish are to be considered first-order associates to the theme
various ordel'S of associalion. Titus if the sentence is taken word. Thus the context is defined as:
as the unit of context any two words A and B which occur (1) all words within a sentence,
in the same sentence are first-order ~tssociates. I f some (2) ,vll content words within a sentence that fall in a
other word C o(.curs with B in a sentence in which A is certain frequency range according to the Lorge Magazine
absem, then A and C are said to be second-order associates Count [8],
siace both h a v e B as a first-order associate. Our concern (3) all content words which stand in closest proxinfity
to the theme word in the grammatical schema of each
This is ESI)-Tt{4;5-21S of the AF Electronic Systems ])ivision,
Air Force Systems (?Olnm~m(l. This research was performed at the Setltence
l)ec[si(m Sciences L~fl)orat.ory as part of Project 2806, Man--(?om-- (4) all words judged to be most closely associated with
I)uter l)ymmdc Inter~ction. Further reproduction is authorized to the theme, where words A and C are considered to be
satisfy the needs (,f the U. S. Government. closely associated if the occurrence of word C is judged
Some of i:he (bata. an~dysis ~mdthe writeup were carried out while ~s strongly implying the occurrence of A and vice versa.
the ~Lu~h(>rswere (m leave ~t Harv~Lr(t University. Since the first
a.u~hor w~s :~ Resea.rch Fellow ~t the Center for Cognitive Studies, Using 65 pairs of words (which range from highly
i financi:flsui)port under AIfPA SD-187 should be ~Leknowledged. synonymous pairs to semantically unrelated pairs) the

} Volume 8 / Number 10 / October, 1965 Communications o f t h e ACM 627


re,lation is shown between s i m i l a r i t y of m e a n i n g atsd the differettt nouas r e p r e s e ~ t e d in {~he theme l)~,]rs T} %, ,~,, ,
a m o u n t of overlap for each definition of context, shown in T a b l e 2.
~Y
T h e 65 word pairs consist of o r d i n a r y English words. T h e t h e m e pairs always eot~taiaed one word ['rolll Iris, ::ii
I~ was fel(; th:~(; since the p h e n o m e n o n under i n v e s t i g a t i o n columrt A. and one from l~he column B. Fif'ly undergra( ~t-
was a general p r o p e r t y of language t h e r e was no necessity al;es were paid {o wriie two sentences HSillg (}aC}l of 11:
to s t u d y technical v o c a b u l a r y . F u r t h e r m o r e , using tech- theme words in e o l u m n A a u d fifty other undcrgradutt<~ :i<.
nical v o e a b u l a r y would raise the p r a c t i c a l difficulty of using t h e theme words ill cob.ram B. (These suhjecls t>d
finding enough c o m p e t e n t people to serve as judges of not I)artieipal,ed in m a k i n g lhe s y n o n y m y ]u(tg~n(~)~i~,~ ':~
s y n o n y m y for such words. T h e s u b j e c t s were (,old that: (:he (heroes were (:o t)~: used ~,< "
nouns only and t h a t each serd;enee was to be ~{ leasi 1() :-
Procedures {:
words long. ( T h e average length a c t u a l l y Ire'ned out ~o ]>~,
Synonymy Judgments. T h e p u r p o s e of (,his procedure
was ~o o b t a i n j u d g m e n t s on how similar i~t m e a n i n g one
TABLE 1. ,iIfDGEI) SYNONY3,IYOF TtIEME PAl ~s
word was to another. T h e r e ware 65 pairs of nouns (theme
pairs) presented for j u d g m e n t . E a c h subje(:t was given a cord smile 0.02 hill WOOdlgtl/d 1. ,"~
shuffled deck of 65 slips of paper, each slip c o n t a i n i n g a rooster voyage 0,04 (;at" journey 1.55 Li
different t h e m e pair. T h e s u b j e c t was given the following noon string 0.04 eemet(:ry mound 1.6!)
instrtmtions : fruit furnace 0.05 glass jewel 1,7S
autograph shore 0.06 magicia~ oravle I.S2
1. After looking through the whole deck, order ~he pairs accord- automobile wizard 0.11 crane inlpleme1~t'2.:~7
ing to amount of "similarity of meaning" so that the slip contain- IDX)/tll(f stoveO. 14 brother lad 2.,I1 {
ing the pair exhibiting the greatest amount, of "similarity ef grin implement 0,18 sage wizard 2. !~;
meaning" is .'-Ltthe toopof the deck and tile pair exhibiting lAaeleast asylum fruit 0.19 oracle sage 2.(;1
amount is on bottom, asylum monk 0.a9 bird cra)~e 2/;3
2. A s s i g n a value from 4.0-0.0 to each pair--the greater the graveyard madhouse 0.42 bird cock 2,1;3
"similariby of meaning," the higher tile number. You may assign glass magician 0.44 food frui{ 2.(;9
the same value to more than one pair. boy rooster 0.44 brother monk 2.7~
cushion jewel 0.45 asylum ma(lhousc :.~0!
Two groups of college m l d c r g r a d u a t e s were paid to serve nlonk slave O.57 f urmu~e st,ove :~.1~
as subjects. G r o u p i, consisting of 15 subjeel~s, m e t for two asylum cemetery 0.79 m~gician wizard '&21
sessions two weeks a p a r t . I a the first session t h e y gave coast forest 0.85 hill mou)t(i ;'; '2! ;.
lad 0.88 cord string :~.,)1
s y n o n y m y j u d g m e n t s on 48 pairs of t h e m e s including 36 grin woodland 0.90 glass tumbler :;.15
shore
of the 65 pairs finally selected for the s t u d y . I n the second monk oracle 0.91 grin smile :~. t~;
sessio~:t t h e y gave syn,o n y m y j u d g m e n t s on t h e 65 pairs boy sage 0.96 serf slave :;. lq;
finally s,.elected." ' Thus t h e r e were 36 t h e m e pairs used in automobile cushion 0.97 journey voyage ~,5"
shoi'e 0.97 autograph sigmtt ~lr(: ;I.59 )
b o t h sessions. These pairs e n a b l e d us to c o m p u t e the intra- mound
wizard 0.99 coast shore :~.i;{) <i::
s u b j e c t r e l i a b i l i t y in j u d g i n g s y n o n y m y . T h e p r o d u c t - lad
forest graveyard 1.00 forest woo(llax~d ::',.(;5
m o m e n t correlation was c o m p u t e d b e t w e e n t h e first and food rooster 1.09 implement tool :~.(;~;
second j u d g m e a t s on these 36 pairs for each subject. De- cemetery woodland 1.18 cock roos1 (?'," { % -
spite t h e fact t h a t (:hese pairs were intermingled a m o n g shore voyage 1.22 boy lad :',.s;2
difi'er(mt pairs in the two sessions t h e correlation turlmd bird woo(lland 1.24 cushion l)illow :~'>!
hill 1.26 (emetery grtv(!v;~rt[ :~Ss
o u t q u i t e high; t;he a v e r a g e ()vet' all 15 subjects was r = coast
ftlrnaee impleinent 1.37 :mlomobile ear ,'.,a-
.85. crane rooster 1.41 midday no(m :~ )
A second group of 36 s u b j e c t s ( G r o u p I I ) front the same gem jewel :~ !)
general l)opulation as G r o u p [ p a r t i c i p a t e d only iu the
second session b u t gave j u d g m e n t s ou all 65 pairs. Ac-
c o r d i n g l y for each of (:he 65 pairs of words judged b y both TABLE 2. LIST OF rFIIEME WOI{DS
groups of subjects in t h e second session a memt j u d g m e n t A
was e a l e u l a t e d for G r o u p s I and i I independeutly. T h e asyhtm gera aut<>nmbile m{<iday
c o r r e l a t i o n b e t w e e n these l~wo sets of means came to ~' autograph glass bird m,,l~k
= .99. T h u s it seemed r e a s o n a b l e t;o pool the j u d g m e n t s of boy graveyard cemetery pi',L,v,
t h e t w o groups. T h e s y n o n y m y w d u e s shown in T a b l e 1., brother grin forest r~, ~stel
motmd fruit sage
a n d used in all the diseussioas below, are the means of the car hill serf
coast I/OOll
j u d g m e n t s collecte,d a t the second experimettt, al session cock oracle implement sh,re
from b o t h groups forming 51 subjects, cord slave jewel siglmtm'e
Generation of the Corpus. T h e purpose of this procedure crane tool journey smile
was to o b t a i n a set of contexts for each of the (heine cushion voyage lad s~,ve
wizard ma~dhouse srril~g
words. E a c h set. consisted of 100 sentences w r i t t e n for food magician ~tmbler
furnace woodland
e~mh different word in the 65 theme p~drs. There are 48

628 C o m m u n i c a t i o n s of tile ACM Vohlme 8 / N u m b e r 10 / October, 19(,5


about 13.5 words long.) Thug sets of 100 sentences were
obtained for erich of the 48 themes. sumption that only the number of different word types in
By h~ving the sentences for the themes in each colunm the overlap is significant and that the frequencies with
written by difl'erent subjects we avoided obtaining the which these types occur in SA or S~ is irrelevant. The token
condition which defines the token measure M~, on the
spuriously high overb~p which might result if the sentences
other hand, does take into account the frequencies of
for both members of a theme pair were written by the same
occurrence of words in SA and S~ by including a given
subjects. Systematic effects of the ordering of the themes
word type in A~ (or Bk) just as often as that type occurred
upon sentences generated by the subjects were avoided by
in Sa (or SB). Thus the token measure M~ reflects the
presenting a different ordering of the themes to each sub-
similarity in the frequency distributions of word types.
jeer and by placing only one theme word on each page of
Table 3 illustrates the calculation of My and 211k for a
the booklet ill which tile sentences were written. The fact
sample of the contextual distributions of the themes gem
that each subject contributed only two sentences to each and jewel.
set made the peculim'ities of any particular subject neg-
Unrestricted Context. The context of a theme word is
ligible.
defined here as all tile words--content words and function
Measures of Overlap. M a n y measm-es have been pro- words--that occur in its sentence set. Figure i shows over-
posed for characterizing the amount of overlap in two
lap versus judged similarity of meaning for this definition
contextual distributions. A summary is given by Giuliano of context. The lower curve is composed of the 11/1~wdues
and Jones [1] and Kuhns [4]. We have chosen a simple and the upper curve is composed of tile il(lk values. The
formula whose interpretation seems intuitively clear. smooth curves, in this and later figures, are third-order
Let Sa be the set of all words used in tile sentences least-square fits, shown simply to characterize the trend
written for theme word A. Let A, he a subset of S , defined of the values.
according to some condition, x. Then the measure of overlap
under condition x is defined as
N(A=B=)
M~ = Min [N(A~), N(B~)]" a-M k
.s I o-My
Expressed verbally M~ is the number of words shared under
condition x divided by the number of items in A~ or in
B~, depending on which is the lesser. (This denominator is ~J Q OD O Da

~he maximtm~ number of items that could be shared under , °o og %° o~


~ondition x.) ~ 8 °
The measures used in this study are derived from two g .4t.¢ °
tonditions: a type eoMition and a token condition. The ,l° °
~ype condition defines a subset A~ consisting of tlle differ-
mt word types which occur in Sa . This condition which ,g ° o: o - o.
o o o
tefines tile type measure M~ is developed from the as-
4°:. .
o / -Oo oo °

TABLE 3. S A M P L E C A L C U L A T I O N OF .~[y AND Mk


Sgem S jjewet O I 2 $ 4
priceless priceless JUDGED SYNONYMY
priceless priceless
priceless FIG. 1. Context unrestricted. Each i)oint represents one of 65
jewelry jewelry pairs of theme words. The eontexts from which the overlap is
jewelry derived are all the content and funetioa words in the sentence sets
ray ray in which the theme words occur. The parameter is the condition
faceting used to calculate overlap.

lem~ N ,lew@j: priceless, jewelry, ray The plots clearly support the hypothesis that the more
N(Gem.) = 3, N(Jewely) = 4 similar words are ill meaning, the more similar they are in
N(Gemv n Jewels) 3 their contextual distributions. It is appare~lt however that
M,, . . . . 1.00
Min [N(Gem~), N(Jewelv)] 3 this relationship is strongest for the highly syn(myn~ous
~em~ N Jewelk: priceless, priceless, jewelry, ray pairs, i.e., those with a judged synonymy greater than
N(Gemk) = 5, N(Jewel~) = 6 3.0. For the intermediate values 1.0-2.7 overlap is almost
N(Gemk rl Jewelk) 4
Mk . . . . . . 80 constant. This constancy may mean that the subjects did
Min [N(Gem~), N(Jewel~)] 5
not react to the distance 1.0--3.0 on the scale as they did
A slightly different formula replaced the denominator of M. to the distance 2.0-4.0. However, subjects' judgments
ith N(A~) + N(B.) - N(A. n B~) and yielded quMitatively were reliable in the range 1.0-3.0 sittce the correlation
milar results. between subjects' judgments in this interval on two trials

olume 8 / Number 10 / October, 1965 Communications o f t h e ACM 629


averaged .67 and the correlation for tile mean judgments that the number of occurrences per type in the overlap i~
of the two groups in this interval was .97. Titus, all:hough greater for pairs of greater synonymy.
we do ~tot know whether we can truly represent the syn- All four measm'es are very closely correlated as ca~l t)e
onymy judgment on a ratio scale, it seems clear that the seen in Table 4.
subjects were at least consistent in the area 1.0-3.0. (Tile Having shown thai) the amount of overlap is indeed a
standard deviation for subiect judgments in this interval function of the semantic similarity existfing betwe<~ a
ranged from .70 to 1.30 attd was greater than at the ex- pah" of words, we must now take up ~he question that i.
tremes of the scale, but this is to be expected.) of primary interest for the associative method of inforn~a.
The picture presented by Figure 1 remained essentially lion retrieval: To what extent cart the degree of synony~y
unchanged when morphological differences among tile existing between a pair of words be inferred from thei,.
words in the sentence sets were leveled; e.g., walk, walks, contextual overlap? I t is quite apparent from Figures t
walking were all considered occurrences of walh; boy, boys, and 2 that one cart reasonably expect to make only a
boy'a', boys' were all considered occurrences of boy; pretty, broad division on the basis of o v e r l a p - m a m c l y , betwe~.~
prettier, prettiest, prettily were all considered occurrences of theme pairs having a degree of synonymy less than ::L0
pretty. The result of such leveling was to reduce tim aver'- and those having a synonylny value greater than 3.'(k
age number of types per sentence set front 478 to an aver- To quantify this obsetwation we used the following (:ri-
age of 420 and to increase the overlap primarily for ~[~ terion.
and only slightly for M~. Figure 2 shows that the effect Inference Power. The overlap measure can be used as a
of morphological leveling was merely to raise the curves statislfic to test the hypothesis that a given word pair has
shown in Figure 1 an approximately constant amount (or would have) a judged synonymy value less than 3.0.
over all synonymy values. i.e., a meditnn or low synonymy pair. We can estimate i lw
Iti both Figm'es 1 and 2 tile slope of M;~ is greater than probability of rejecting this hypothesis when it is in fac?
the slope of M~, for synonymy wdues above 3.0. This im- true (Type I error) by considering the pereenh~ge of km)wt,~
plies that the number of tokens in the overlap increases low synonymy pairs that exceed a particular overlap wthw.
more rapidly than tile number of t y p e s - - i n other words If tile probability of T y p e I error is fixed ( i n o u r Slu(ty w/'
chose 1 percent and 5 percent levels), this determines :,
critical value for the overlap statistic. We now calculate t t=~
=-M k percentage of the known high synonymy pairs whose over-
O-My
lap is greater than this value and take this percentage ~>
%
.6 an estimate of the probability of rejecting the hypothc.<i-
:l,LI
.J
D
.
D O
°~° when it is in fact false. This is the percentage of c(,*~(cg
"inferences (or the inference power) of the overlap llleasklv('
o= =" s~ o .°
O O
2~L.----".
O (at Cite x percent error level) for a particular delinitiol~ o(
~-=.-- ° """"~ o " o context.
.5
.<
013
0
I- o In our study 20 ttmme pairs had a judged synonymy
greater than 3.0 and so the proportion of these pairs th..:l:l
.d..4 o o o o oo o o exceeds the critical overlap value is tile estimate of in-
_J o o
,10g
W o o ,.,~ o ~ , o ference f)ower for a particular test. To estimate 'l'yp~ [
2~
o ~oO o
O o~ o o ° o error we considered all possible theme word pairs such that
.3 % each word of the pair had sentence sets written by differ-
o
eat groups of subjects, i.e., 576 pairs. Judgments wcr('
actually ogtained for only 65 of these pairs. The remaini~g
D•f 0
t

I 2
i I

5
i

4
511 pairs could not be submitted for judgment to subje c~-"
for practical reasons, but it seemed clear that these pair-
JUDGED SYNONYMY were less synonynLous than the pairs which wore judged ao
FIG. 2. Effect of leveling. T h e conditions are the s~me as in have synonymy values greater than 3.0. Thus using the
F i g u r e i except t h a t the c o n t e x t s were morphologically leveled overlap values for 556 low synonymy pairs we determined :t
before t~c~e overlap was calculated. critical value which was exceeded by 1 percent or 5 per-
cent of these pairs and then determined tile proportioll of
T A B L E 4. COI~I{EL.X.TIONS(r) BETWEI,:N OVEI{.I.,,'~PMI,:;tSUItES FOR the 20 high synonymy pairs that exceeded ttLe critic:d
UXa~STmC'rED CON'rEXT value.
Table 5 shows the percentages of correct inferences (:Lad
Mlk~ Myl z}[$d
critical values) for overlap based on unrestricted cor~tcx~
~I~,, .s7 .96 .a7
as well as on the other contexts studied.
Ma,, .87 .98
+II~,~ .88 Note that the percentage of correct, inferences is about
the s~mle when the context is limited to content words.
u = unleve]ed; I = leveled Conte:ct Defined by Word Frequer~cy. I t might tw

630 Commmfications of the AC3[ V o l u m e 8 / N u m b e r 10 / O c t o b e r , 1965


thought t h a i a m o r e se,~sitive rcfle(.t>ion of lhe relation- word has a high fiequeney of occurrence usually as the
ship betwe(m simflariI~y of (',on/ex~, and s y n o n y m y would result of its use in a wide variety of s i t u a t i o n s
be obtained if we (:onsidered o n l y those words of context W e investigated the relationship between word f r e
M~ose oceurre~ee was c o n d i t i o n a l on the occurrence of the quency and a m o u n t of overlap b y partitioning the c o n
theme word. Such w o r d s would t e n d to occur in the over- tent words (nouns, verbs, adjectives and a d v e r b s ) h i t •
lap of two themes o n l y if some signifieattt semantic feature three word fl'equency intervals: 0-150, 117)1.-...1000, fr(>
was common t~o b o l h themes. queneies greater than 1001 (occurrences per <I,.5 million
I:: particular, it seems o b v i o u s t h a t t h e higher the fre- tokens of the Lorge Magazine Count). l,'ut~etion words
quency of oeem'renee of a word in t h e language as a whole (articles, prepositions, conjunctions, prottotlns, etc,) were
or in some r e a s o n a b l y large eorptts, the less likely will its partitioned out as a fom'th set (for list: of function words,
occurrence I)e c o n d i t i o n a l upon t h e occurrence of some see Miller et al. [5].)
1)~trlicular word. T h i s follows from the o b s e r v a t i o n t h a t a Figure 3 shows t h a t there is only a slight te~dency for

TABLE 5. PERCEN'P CORRECT I N F E R E N C E S FOR T W O I!]RROR LEVI' LS

Co~le:ct D e f i n i t i o ~ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . i ......................................................................................................................

Umestricted
(:W & FW 75 (34) .O5 (32) 7'5 (54) 95 (52) 7O (38) 90 (30) 80 (57) 85 ('55)
CW alone 75 (22) 95 (19) 90 (20) 95 (17) ............................................

L(,rgc Frequency I
0 LS0 85 (9.1) 90 (6,6) 85 (8.1) 95 (6.4) 70 (8.5) 95 (6,3) S0 (7.5) 85 (5.(;)
151-1000 70 (22) 80 (20) 75 (20) 95 (16) 50 (25) 70 (23) 85 (22) 95 (17)
10()l and above 5 (59) 15 (56) 35 (49) 60 (-hi) 5 ((;2) 35 (57) 55 (48) 75 (,i5)
FW 0 (82) 10 (79) 45 (8.1) 55 (80) : 5 (82) 5 (79) I 35 (8:~) 50 (81)
i .............................................................................. .................................................................
Grammatical 90 (15) 95 (12) 95 (13) 95 (ll) ii . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
t
Ass )ciation 90 (14) 100 (4.8) 95 (tl) 100 (3.7) { . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . i . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
........................................................................... ! ..................................................... ..........................................

The figures in parentheses are the overlap values associated with the given pereeuh~ges.

1
o 0-150
My • 151-1000 Mk
o EXX)+
FW
a h 5 & a

¢3
w 7 A A []
-I
w []
>
~.S 0 / . 1 . *- 0 0
M 0 ~ 0 O0 0
Z 00 0 0 D
° 1] o 0 0 0 0
~.~ D
O
Z o • •
____._._-e-4 °"
O
ha
o
o° o° ° °°° , 0=
~z o •& & ~ • • •A ¢&• •~ ° o

¢I
r•" • °g
,,~....*---"--," " ,* o o ~o
ha • ~ 0 0 0 gO • o o, o
O ,I a ooo $ o °o _o_,t~,.-----'-"-~_
o"__._.~.-~-
°1° t I '
0 I 2 5 4
O I 2 3 4
JUDGED SYNONYMY
Fro. 3. Effect of partitioning context into frequency classes. Each point represmtts one of 65 pairs of theme words. The contexts were
Partitioned into four classes: function words (FW) and three content-word classes. The content-word classes rc3)rcscnt three intervals
according the Lorge Magazine Count (frequency of occurrence in 4.5 million tokens). Each curve reprcsenls tit(; overlap obtained for one
of the four partitions of context.

Volume 8 / N u m b e r 10 / O c t o b e r , 1965 C o m m u n i c a t i o n s of the ACM 631


overlap consisting of content words of lower frequency to examined the lists for the eolunm A theme words (Table
be more affected by synonymy than content words of 2) and ano{,her j udge examioed the lists for the column B
higher frequency. As for function words, while the M~ themes. (The iwo judges were the (irstm:m~ed n.uthof and
measure shows zero slope (.002 with a st~mdard error of his wife.)
.003) the M~ measure shows a slope which is signific:mtly The judge looking at :~ list of words thai> occurred with a
greater than zero (.017 with a standard error of .004). given theme asked himself: What is (;he likelihood that a
No explanation is readily available for this surprising situalion which evoked the I>heme word would evoke the
result. We guessed that it might have been prinmrily due word type in question, and conversely, what is the likeli-
to the pronouns and prepositions, which of all function
words would seem to have ltxe greatest potential sen, antic

if"
connection to the theme words; however, even Ibis con- 13
jecture was not t)orne out by an analysis of the data.
The results shown in Figure 3 are essentially unchanged
if leveled data are used. > °
The percentages of correct inferences for these four oo o
o o ~ o
Lorge frequency intervals are shown in Table 5. I t is I
t 0 ~e~ t~ = a o
evident that content words with a frequency greater than
<
1000 are relatively useless for discriminating highly syn- :E
onymous pairs. The statistical significance of the 3I~ slope .4 'My
for function words permits as high as 55 percent correct ¢,0
i
inference with 5 percent, error. Apparently the small o ¢1o o o
fleet evident in Figure 3 has some inference power al- g .2 °
W o
though there is no explanation why this is so. o
o o
.I ~ oO~ ° o
Context Gra'mmatically Defined. I n this limitation on 0 0 0 0~
o
context the overlap was restricted to those words which
0
were grammatically nmst closely related to the theme.
Content words which were grammatically dependent upon o i
the theme word, which was always a noun, or upon a JUOGED SYNONYMY
pronoun standing for the theme were considered related. FIG. 4. Effect of limiting the context to words grammatically
Thus the following were included: (1) nouns, adjectives related to the theme. E~tch point represents one of 65 p~irs of
or participles describing the theme, (2) the verb if the theme words.
theme was the subject (and predicate noun if the verb
was the copula or a copula substitute), and (3) the noun
.6 Mk
in a prepositional or possessive phrase modifying the
theme. Also included were the following in the event that .5 Q
0"
the theme word was the grammatically dependent form: .4 a a
(1) the verb of which the theme was the object, (2) the
noun to which the theme was the appositive, and (3) the Q a

verb or noun modified by a prepositional or possessive .2 a a a


phrase of which the theme was the head. UJj 0 00 D 000

Figure 4 shows the relationship between the overlap of


i o o o o o° o o o
contexts defined in this manner and judged synonymy. a 0 ~
It is obvious that the slope is considerably steeper for i i t i

this context definition than for either the unrestricted or < .E My


the frequency<lefined context. This difference is reflected O .5 o
03, o
n the greater discriminating power of grammatically <
!
defined context: percent correct inferences for one percent ~.4
error, Mz, equals 90 percent, M~ equals 95 percent; for ~.~ o o g
five percent error, Mu and 211~ both yield 95 percent (all °° o
O .2 o o o Oo
based on unleveled data).
o oo o
Context D~fined by Association. On the notion that we .I
might obtain a relation between overlap and synonymy o
O o~:~_~oo-.o-~-~o o o o
which was more sensitive to differences in synonymy,
especially at the lower end of the scale, we limited the
context to those words in a sentence set that were judged JUDGED SYNONYMY
to be highly associated to the theme. [i'~G. 5. Effect of limiting the context to words j udged ~s being
To obtain these judgments lists of the word types oc- in high associatioll rel~tionship with the theme. E ~ c h p o i n t repre-
curring in each sentence set were prepared. One judge sents one of 65 pa~irs of theme words.

632 C o m m u n i c a t i o n s o f the ACM V o l u m e 8 / N u m b e r 1.0 / O c t o b e r , 1965


hood that a situ~:,,iion vvt~i(:l~ evoked the word type would been judged highly associated to the theme was calculated
also evoke the ~heme word? Only if both likelihoods were and plotted against position.
judged "high" or "very high" were {)he words considered Figure 6 shows only two positions have a distinctly
highly associ:d,ed. For examp]e, for (~he theme word food, higher proportion of highly associated words: 25 percent
the following words were among those judged to be highly of the content words occurring directly before the theme
associated: appetite, appetizing, ate, bread, breakjb, st, cooked, and 20 percent of the content words occurring directly
delicious, dige,~'ted, digestion, mealtime, nourishment, nu- after belonged to the set of words which had been judged
tritional, r@'igeration, restau~'ant, taste. to be highly associated to the theme words.
It is unfortunate th:.~ only two judges were available for
this experiment. However, having observed a high degree Conclusions
0f reliability among a larger group of judges in a similar 1. The basic hypothesis investigated by the study is
associt~tive task, we are fairly confident that essentially corroborated: there is a positive relationship between the
the same results would have been obtained if a greater degree of synonymy (semantic similarity) existing be-
number of judges had been employed. tween a pair of words and the degree to which their con-
Figure 5 shows that the lower portion of the curves is texts are similar.
still r~ther flat although the curves do rise on the high 2. It may be safely inferred that a pair of words is highly
synonymy end nmch more sharply than they do under the syllonymous if their contexts show a relatively great
other definitions of context. amount of overlap. Inference of degree of synonymy from
The high discriminating power of a,ssociatively defined lesser amounts of overlap, however, is apparently un-
context is clearly shown by the percentage of correct eertain since words of low or medium synonymy differ
inferences possible from examination of the overlap meas- relatively little in overlap.
ures: for one percent ca'or, M~ equals 90 percent, Mk The first of these conclusions may be accepted in its
equals 95 percent while for five percent error both measures full generality since it is, in alt probability, a linguistic
yield 100 percent. universal. The second conclusion, however, together with
In view of trhe fact that the overlap from contexts de- the findings on the effects of various definitions of context
fined this way is more discriminating it was considered and on the particular shape of the overlap versus syn-
whether such contexts could be obtained by computer onymy function should be accepted only tentatively for
processing. One possibility worth exploring seemed to be they may be properties of the language materials and pro-
that highly associated words would tend to occur close to eedures enlployed in this study.
the theme word. Thus all the content words in the sen- Valid generalization of findings like these depends upon
tence sets were partitioned according to their position an increased knowledge of the effects of such factors as
ill the sentence relative to the theme. The proportion of corpus size, degree of homogeneity of content and form
the content words occurring at each position that had and the size of the units in which information is packaged
within the corpus.
25 RECEIVED MAr, 1965.

REFERENCES
¢3
1. GIUIaANO,V. E,, ANDJONES, P.E. Studies for the design of an
~.
<
2c o English command aDd control language system. ESD-TDR-
5 63-673, Arthur D. Little, Inc., Cambridge, Mass., 1963.
0
2. ttAm¢ts, Z.S. Distributional structure. Word 10 (1954), 146-.
162.
>- 3. Joos, M. Description of language design. J. Acoust, Soc.
.J
I Amer. 22 (1950), 701-708.
0 o
o 4. ~Kur~NS, J. L. The continuum of coefficients of association.
:E o
o ° o o oO o oooo in M. E. Stevens, L. Heilprin, & V. E. Giuliuno, (Eds.), Pro-
Z o
o
o
o o oo ceedings of the Symposium on Statistical Association Methods
W
0 o o for Mechanized Documentation, National Bureau of Stand-
o °o °o o o
W ards, US Government Printing Off., Washington, D. C. In
O. o o
press.
5 5. MILLER, G. A., ~NEwMAN, E . g . , :kiD FRIEDMAN, E. 2~k. Length-
frequency statistics for written English. Inform. and Contr. 1
o (1958), 370-389.
6. STEVENS, M. E., HEILPRIN, L., AND GIULIANO, V. E. (EDS.)
i6 i i i I i i |
-I -12 -8 -4 0 4 8 12 16 Proceedings of the Symposium on Statistical Association Meth-
ods for Mechanized Documentation, National Bureau of Stand-
DISTANCE FROM THEME ards, US Government Printing Off., Washington, D. C. In
Fro. 6. Proportion of content words at various positiuns in the press,
sentence that were judged to be in high association relationship 7. STLL~:S,H . E . The association factor in information retrieval.
with the theme. The negative values on the abscissa are the num- J. ACM 8 (1961), 271-279.
ber of word places bqfore the theme while the positive values are 8. THOIeNDIKE, E. L., AND LORGE, [. The Teacher's Word Book of
the number of word places after the theme. 30,000 Words. Columbia U. Press, New York, 1944.

Volume 8 / N u m b e r [0 / Octol)er, 1965 C o m m u n i c a t i o n s of t h e ACM 633

View publication stats

Potrebbero piacerti anche