Sei sulla pagina 1di 16

Journal of Consumer Behaviour

J. Consumer Behav. 6: 32–47 (2007)


Published online in Wiley InterScience
(www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/cb.206

Consumer recycling: Role of


incentives, information,
and social class
Easwar S. Iyer 1* and Rajiv K. Kashyap 2
1
Isenberg School of Management, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA
2
Department of Marketing and Management, Cotsakos College of Business,
William Paterson University, Wayne, NJ, USA

 More and more communities have instituted recycling programs and consumer recycling
is no longer a new fad; it is here to stay. However, consumer commitment to recycling and
participation rates have leveled off. Whereas lack of access to recycling facilities was cited
as a key inhibitor to participation in the early days of recycling, that is generally not the
case anymore. Thus there is an imperative to revisit consumer recycling by focusing on
behavioral issues that reflect today’s context. In this study we review the past literature
and propose a comprehensive model of consumer recycling. We identify two intervention
mechanisms – incentives or information – that are believed to increase recycling
participation. We, then, describe a longitudinal field experiment to evaluate the relative
merits of these intervention programs. We conclude that either intervention program is
effective, although informational programs appear to have more long-term effects than
incentive programs. We also create a new measure of social class, one that includes other
influential actors’ characteristics, and show its relationship to recycling attitudes and
behaviors.
Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Introduction looning to 200 million tons in 1990 (Oskamp


et al., 1991). In 2001, it was estimated to be
The visible technical and economic successes 229 million tons (www.epa.gov); this works
in the 20th century have enhanced standards
out to 4.4 pounds of waste per person per day,
of living, while simultaneously placing a heavy up from 2.7 pounds per person per day in
toll on our natural environment (Carson,
1960. Landfilling has been the traditional
1962). One such impact can be seen in the solution, but for a number of reasons its
generation of household waste, which, in the
appeal continues to erode. Household recy-
U.S.A., was only 7 million tons in 1960, cling is an increasingly popular solution, but to
jumping to 160 million tons in 1989 (U.S.
extract its full benefits we need a thorough
House of Representatives, 1989), before bal-
understanding of technical and behavioral
issues. In this study we focus exclusively on
*Correspondence to: Easwar S. Iyer, Isenberg School of the behavioral aspects and try to answer
Management, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA
01003, USA. the central question: How can we get more
E-mail: iyer@mktg.umass.edu people to recycle more frequently and how

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
Consumer recycling 33

can we help them sustain these behaviors?


Our report is sequenced as follows: first, we
review the literature and present a conceptual
model of recycling behavior; second, we
discuss our choice of independent factors
and derive hypotheses; third, we describe
our longitudinal field experiment; fourth, we
present our results; fifth, and finally, we
discuss our findings’ implications and offer
some recommendations.
Figure 1. Factors influencing recycling behavior.

Factors influencing recycling


Researchers from many disciplines have stu- to discriminate recyclers from nonrecyclers
died recycling and depending on their dis- based on their general environmental attitudes
cipline’s orientation, they have selectively (Gray 1985; Vining and Ebreo, 1990; Oskamp
focused on some factors to explain why et al., 1991; Derksen and Gartrell, 1993;
people recycle. For instance, economist Gamba and Oskamp, 1994), the link to
tended to study the role of incentives, recycling values, beliefs, and attitudes is
particularly monetary incentives, whereas tenuous. The partial overlapping of the
social psychologists preferred to see how domains of environmental and recycling
motivational appeals, especially altruism or attitudes led Kashyap and Iyer (2001) to
guilt, influenced this behavior. This has suggest that recycling attitudes are influenced
resulted in a wide and disparate array of by factors other than just environmental
factors without any comprehensive theoretical attitudes.
model of recycling behaviors. Based on a Bratt (1999) has offered two reasons for
meta-analysis of 67 published empirical stu- recycling behavior not being strongly related
dies, Hornik et al. (1995) theorized that there to general environmental behaviors. In one,
were four antecedent factors, that is, extrinsic consumers are hypothesized to compartmen-
incentives, intrinsic incentives, external facil- talize their behaviors into various sectors, that
itators, and internal facilitators, which influ- is, energy, transportation, recycling, etc.
enced recycling behaviors. The modest dis- Behaviors in one sector do not necessarily
tinction between incentives and facilitators led have an impact on attitudes or behaviors in
Kashyap and Iyer (2001) to abbreviate and other sectors because they are only sector
group all the antecedents into two, that is, specific. In the second, consumers are postu-
internal and external motivators. For our lated to behave in a manner that compensates
current research, we have adapted this their excesses in one domain with a deficit in
version, but included a third factor, that is, another domain. Cycling to work instead of
individual characteristics. In summary, we driving, and yet not bothering to recycle might
postulate that internal motivators, external be an example of compensation.
motivators, and individual characteristics (see More recently, McCarty and Shrum (2001)
Figure 1) collectively influence recycling modeled the influence of three values –
intentions and behaviors. individualism, collectivism, and locus of con-
trol – on recycling behaviors. Specifically they
found that individualism negatively influenced
recycling behavior because that relationship
Internal motivators
was mediated by the perceived inconvenience
These include an individual’s environmental of recycling. On the other hand, collectivism
values, beliefs, and attitudes. Given the failure positively influenced recycling behaviors since

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
34 Easwar S. Iyer and Rajiv K. Kashyap

that relationship was mediated by the per- information on the benefits of recycling. On
ceived importance of recycling. Since people the other hand, information that increases a
can simultaneously hold individualistic and consumer’s knowledge about recycling and its
collectivistic values, communicating the need potential impact must positively influence
to recycle becomes a challenge. recycling attitudes and behaviors. This has
received no attention in the literature.

External motivators
Individual characteristics
These include all the external agents that can
be theoretically or empirically linked to We mean to include an individual’s demo-
recycling attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. graphic identifiers and cognitive capabilities.
Access to recycling facilities (Curlee, 1986) and Except gender, most of the demographic
the setting in which recycling behavior is characteristics seem to be unrelated to recy-
either promoted or occurs have been shown to cling behaviors (Mohai and Twight, 1987;
influence recycling propensity. Berger and Stern et al., 1993; Berger, 1997). Many
Kanetkar (1996) found that recycling in the researchers have reported the vital role of
workplace influenced household recycling education in determining whether one
behaviors. Similarly, Kashyap and Iyer (2001) recycles or not (Ostman and Parker, 1988;
documented a significant relationship between Vining and Ebreo, 1990, 1992; Lansana, 1992;
home-based recycling and attitude toward Lakhan and Lavalle, 2002). Recyclers seem to
recycling. Laws and regulations are influential have superior knowledge and awareness of
external agents that increase the pressure recycling programs (Tasady, 1991), suggesting
upon an individual to recycle (Business Week, their importance in influencing recycling
1989, p.154). Societal norms and peer or behaviors.
community pressure are other external agents Roberts (1996, p. 82) suggests that demo-
that increase an individual’s motivation to graphics, taken collectively, account for less
recycle (Granzin and Olsen, 1991; Lord, 1994; than 8 per cent of the variance in socially
Taylor and Todd, 1995). The second most responsible consumer behavior. Part of the
important reason for recycling was the per- reason could be because ‘all aspects of the
ceived pressure from neighbors (Gamba and environmental consciousness construct’ may
Oskamp, 1994), thus validating the success of not have been investigated (Diamantopoulos
publicly visible curbside programs (Reid et al., et al., 2003, p. 467). In other words,
1976; Humphrey et al., 1977). environmental consciousness is complex and
Various forms of monetary incentives serve comprehensive construct, and to better study
as external motivators of recycling behaviors. its antecedents, we need to specify and
Prizes (Luyben and Bailey, 1979) and lotteries measure all its aspects. Besides the complexity
(Diamond and Loewy, 1991) have been of the construct of recycling, single demo-
shown to positively influence recycling rates, graphic indicators are too limited in their
although their long-term impact is unknown. predictive scope. In fact, many decades ago,
The role of information as an agent of Coleman (1960) and Schaninger (1981),
influence is less well studied, although the among others, argued that multi-item
importance of consumer knowledge is well indicators like social class have greater pre-
established (Ellen, 1994; Diamantopoulos dictive power than single item measures like
et al., 2003). Even in the few studies that income. Thus, we feel that a more compre-
manipulated information, it was generally hensive measure, one that includes multiple
restricted to information about the existence socio-demographic measures such as social
of a recycling program (Hopper and Nielson, class, will have better predictive power
1991; Porter et al., 1995) as opposed to (Heath, 1998).

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
Consumer recycling 35

Independent factors used in our similar results, suggesting that individual


study – incentives and information rewards may be a stronger motivator than
group rewards.
Incentives Is the monetary cost of the incentives
In the broadest of senses, the term ‘incentives’ justified through increase recovery? In the
can be used to include positive rewards only study known to us, Porter et al. (1995)
(incentives) or negative rewards (disincen- estimated that only 30 per cent of the cost of
tives); the rewards themselves can be monet- intervention is recovered. Whether more of
ary or psychological in nature. Prizes and the costs will be recovered over a longer
lotteries are examples of positive monetary period or other forms of intervention will
rewards whereas tax would exemplify a prove to be more cost effective is unknown.
negative monetary reward. Similarly, recog- Employing another form of intervention and
nition would be an example of a positive using a longitudinal design, we hope to shed
psychological reward whereas sanctions or some light on these questions.
rebukes would be negative psychological
rewards. We limit our review to positive
Information
monetary rewards.
The importance of monetary incentives in Information can mean two related concepts,
trash management is well established (Burgess that is, communication or knowledge. When
et al., 1971; Clark et al., 1972). In the 1971 used to mean ‘communication,’ the success of
study, children at a movie theater were an informational intervention depends on the
exposed to different litter-reduction treat- interplay among content, media, and format.
ments that included free litterbags, an instruc- We are more interested in ‘information as
tional movie, extra trashcans placed more knowledge’ and its role in influencing recy-
conspicuously, or an incentive. Even modest cling behaviors.
rewards, such as the 10 cents or a free movie Written and verbal informational prompts
ticket, led to a two- to-threefold increase in the have been employed to increase recycling
amount of litter collected. The 1972 sequel behaviors. Jacobs et al. (1984) found that
done at a campground produced identical informational brochures delivered to individ-
results. Luyben and Bailey (1979) structured ual households increased participation rates by
the monetary reward to increase with the 6 per cent. More recently two studies used the
poundage of recycled paper brought to the informational interventions in the form of
center and found that rewards had a signifi- block leaders, although they differed in their
cantly greater impact on recycling than just findings. In one such study, block leaders
providing additional access to recycling. disseminated information and as a result
What is the best way to offer a monetary increased the number of households partici-
reward? Is a definite reward of $1 better or pating by 16 per cent (Burn, 1991). In a
worse than 1/8 chance of winning $8? In spite companion study, Hopper and Nielson (1991)
of an identical expected value, Diamond and found the ‘block leader’ approach to infor-
Loewy (1991) found that probabilistic rewards mation dissemination to be only marginally
led to more attitude change than definite better than an informational brochure. These
rewards. Are individual rewards better than discrepant findings were attributed to the
group rewards? Geller et al. (1975) compared differing socio-economic status in the two
two types of incentives: a raffle for individuals studies (Everett and Peirce, 1991–1992).
versus a contest for the entire group. Whereas Information can enhance declarative
the amount of recycled paper increased in knowledge, that is, what is involved in
both conditions, it was significantly higher in recycling, or procedural knowledge, that is,
the raffle condition as compared to the contest how to recycle (Gagne et al., 1993). In a recent
condition. Diamond and Loewy (1991) found study, women with diabetes were placed in

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
36 Easwar S. Iyer and Rajiv K. Kashyap

various treatment conditions (Miller et al., different locations on the campus of a large
1999). In one condition, subjects participated university served as our experimental sites.
in a 90-minute information session once a These clusters were identified based upon
week for 9 weeks. Compared to a control their similarity in size and composition of the
group, these subjects had greater declarative residents. There were four measurement
and procedural knowledge; as a result they periods – baseline, 1st wave, 2nd wave, and
were more confident in using food label 3rd wave – when weights of recycled paper,
information. Likewise, we expect that provid- glass, and the amount of contamination were
ing individuals with information on what is recorded; these were used as measures of
recycling (declarative knowledge) and how to recycling output. We also distributed a survey
recycle (procedural knowledge) will modify at both sites. Selected items from the survey
their attitude toward recycling and future were used to measure social class, environ-
recycling behaviors. We also predict that the mental attitude and behavior, and recycling
changes resulting from an information inter- attitude and behavior.
vention will last longer than those resulting Our design had five strengths. First, unlike
from an incentive intervention. In other words, most of the prior research (see Roberts, 1995,
the wear out that follows any intervention will 1996; Diamantopoulos et al., 2003 for review
be much less in case of information-based of past studies), we measured actual recycling
intervention programs. output. Second, we triangulated by including
traditional self-reported measures of attitudes
and behaviors as well. Third, the longitudinal
Summary of hypothesis design allowed us to track recycling output
over time. Fourth, our experimental design
enabled us to study the relative impact of
H1: Providing incentives will positively incentives and information. Fifth, and finally,
influence recycling behaviors and recy- by conducting a field experiment, we
cling output. enhanced the ‘realness’ of our study. There
are two limitations. First, the intervention and
H2: Providing information about recycling recycling output measures applied at the
will positively influence recycling beha- group level. Second, being a field study, we
viors and recycling output. had no control over extraneous factors that
could have influenced our results. On balance
H3: Providing information, as compared our design allowed us to compare the relative
to offering incentives, will have a longer merits of incentives and information over
lasting impact on recycling behaviors and time.
recycling output.

H4: Social class will influence recycling Intervention


behaviors.
The two external motivators, that is, incentives
and information, represented our experimen-
tal interventions. After baseline measures were
Methodology: a longitudinal field recorded over a 2-week period, at one site
experiment we publicly announced sponsoring a party for
the residence hall that had the highest amount
Design
of recycled materials over 4 months. We will
Our study’s design is best described as a refer to this site as the ‘Incentive Site.’ At the
longitudinal field experiment lasting over same time, in the other site, we began an
4 months. Two residence hall clusters at two information dissemination program focused on

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
Consumer recycling 37

increasing the declarative and procedural Traditionally social class measures include
knowledge of participants at this site. The three inputs: family income, education, and
program included thrice-a-week demon- occupation of only the respondent. We
strations on recycling and once-a-week mailbox expanded our measure to include parents’
inserts on the how and why to recycle. We will scores on education and occupation as well.
call this as the ‘Information Site.’ The inter- Thus, in all, five ordinal scales – household
ventions lasted for two more time periods that income, level of both parent’s education, and
we have already referred to as 1st and 2nd parent’s occupations1 – were summed; the
waves. minimum score was 6 and the maximum was
32 (See Appendix for details). This score was
used to categorize an individual’s social class.
Measures
We used four dependent measures at the
individual level – recycling attitudes, recycling
Results
behaviors, environmental attitudes, and Sample profile
environmental behaviors. We also had four
dependent measures at the group level – The Information site consisted of two resi-
recycling output for paper and glass, and dence halls accommodating 633 students,
contamination for paper and glass. We made up of 341 males (54%) and 292 females
included demographic measures used to (46%). The Incentive site also contained two
identify social class; this was our independent residence halls with a total of 806 students
variable. made up of 458 males (57%) and 348 females
We measured individual level recycling (43%). Students at both sites were between 20
attitudes using 10 items and individual level and 23 years of age. Overall, the two sites were
recycling behaviors using 5 items. Individual well matched on age and gender.
level environmental attitude was measured
using 15 items and individual level environ-
mental behavior was measured using 13 items. Overview of analysis
The dependent measures that resulted by
Recycling output was measured in terms of the
summing the seven-Likert scale, had relia-
poundage of paper and glass collected in
bilities ranging from 0.88 to 0.95. Recycling
recycling bins at sites. There were four
output was measured in terms of the amount
measurement periods: baseline, 1st wave,
of recycled paper collected in the blue bins and
2nd wave, and 3rd wave; each measurement
recycled glass collected in the red bins at each
period consisted of 2 consecutive weeks. The
site. The bins at the two experimental sites
2-week window helped avoid unusual swings
bore a special hidden marking, not noticeable
in the poundage that may have been caused by
to the participants, but known only to the staff.
parties, shorter workweeks or extended week-
Bins from the experimental sites were taken to
ends, and so forth. We focused on four
the processing facility where, at first, any
comparisons:
contamination was separated and kept aside
for further future analysis. The remaining
1. Inter-period comparisons of the volume of
contents were weighed to the nearest pound;
recycled paper and glass between the treat-
this constituted recycling output. Spot checks
ment sites (Table 1)
revealed no errors in this measurement. Next
2. Comparisons of recycled paper and glass
the already separated contaminants were
relative to baseline at both treatment sites
weighed to the nearest one-half pound. This
(Figures 2 and 3)
represented contamination; there was one
measure for recycled paper and one for 1
We have treated occupation as an interval variable as has
recycled glass. been done by others in the past (Travis and Kohli, 1995).

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
38 Easwar S. Iyer and Rajiv K. Kashyap

Table 1. Pounds of recycled paper and glass at information and incentive sites

Information Site Incentive Site

Paper % Da Glass % Da Total % Da lbs/Student Paper % Da Glass % Da Total % Da lbs/Student

Baseline 147 NA 388 NA 535 NA 0.85 250 NA 318 NA 568 NA 0.70


1st wave 481 227.2 459 18.3 940 75.7 1.48 441 76.4 444 39.6 885 55.8 1.10
2nd wave 505 5.0 476 3.7 981 4.4 1.55 512 16.1 484 9.1 996 12.5 1.24
3rd wave 367 27.3 463 2.7 830 15.4 1.31 545 6.5 185 61.8 730 26.7 0.91

NA: Not Applicable.


a
Inter-period comparisons.

3. Comparisons of total contamination and materials increased dramatically from 535 lbs
contamination rates at both sites (Table 2) (0.85 lb per student) during the baseline to
4. Social class differences and its impact on 940 lbs (1.48 lbs per student) in the first wave,
recycling attitude and behavior (Tables 3 an increase of over 75per cent. The increase
and 4) (55.8%) at the Incentive site was similar,
growing from 568 lbs (0.7 lb per student)
during baseline to 885 lbs (1.1 lb per student)
Inter-period comparison of recycling
at the first wave. It is interesting to note that
output between treatment sites
both the volume of paper and glass recycled
The interventions had positive effects at both showed a remarkable increase at both sites. At
sites, thus supporting H1 and H2. At the the Information site, the volume of paper more
Information site, the amount of recycled than doubled (227.2% increase) from 147 lbs at

Figure 2. Recycled paper and glass at education site.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
Consumer recycling 39

Figure 3. Recycled paper and glass at incentive site.

Table 2. Pounds of contamination and contamination rates at information and incentive sites

Site/Time Paper (lbs) Rate (%) Glass (lbs) Rate (%) Total (lbs) Rate (%)

Information
Baseline 2 1.36 8 2.06 10 1.87
1st wave 11 2.29 16 3.49 27 2.87
2nd wave 5 0.99 21 4.41 28 2.85
3rd wave 8 2.18 8 1.73 16 1.93
Incentive
Baseline 2.5 1.00 6.5 2.04 9 1.58
1st wave 7 1.59 8 1.80 15 1.69
2nd wave 7 1.37 12 2.48 19 1.91
3rd wave 5 0.92 7 3.78 12 1.64

Table 3. Effects of gender and social class on environmental and recycling attitudes and behaviors

Source F values

df Environmental attitude Environmental behavior Recycling attitude Recycling behavior

Effects
Gender 1 7.062 14.036 3.210 4.605
Social class 2 1.293 0.829 3.087 3.651


p < 0.10;

p < 0.05;

p < 0.01.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
40 Easwar S. Iyer and Rajiv K. Kashyap

Table 4. Comparison of means and standard deviations for lower, middle, and high social class groups

N Mean SD SE Min Max

Environmental attitude
Lower 35 87.74 10.05 1.75 65 105
Middle 132 84.17 13.71 1.22 46 105
Upper 38 82.79 15.72 2.55 25 105
Total 205 84.53 13.59 0.95 25 105
Environmental behavior
Lower 35 49.94 10.94 1.90 21 68
Middle 132 46.86 15.88 1.39 13 84
Upper 38 45.26 17.08 2.77 14 91
Total 205 47.09 15.39 1.07 13 91
Recycling attitude
Lower 35 63.23 6.40 1.10 46 70
Middle 132 59.89 11.19 0.94 16 70
Upper 38 56.71 13.50 2.19 11 70
Total 205 59.87 11.14 0.78 11 70
Recycling behavior
Lower 35 29.17 4.99 0.86 19 35
Middle 132 26.34 5.94 0.52 5 35
Upper 38 26.44 4.94 0.80 15 35
Total 205 26.84 5.69 0.40 5 35

baseline to 481 lbs at first wave. At the During the third wave, volume of recycled
Incentive site, the increase was not as materials dropped off at both sites. However
dramatic, but the volume of paper grew the decrease at the Information site (15.4%)
(76.4%) from 250 to 441 lbs. The volume of was smaller relative to the Incentive site,
glass increased from 388 to 459 lbs at the which showed a significant decrease (26.7%)
Information site (18.3%) and from 318 to in volume of materials recycled. At the
444 lbs at the Incentive site (39.6%). Information site, the amount of paper recycled
Total volume of recycled materials contin- declined (27.3%) from 505 to 367 lbs while the
ued to grow modestly at both sites during the amount of glass recycled decreased marginally
second period. At the Information site, the (2.7%) from 476 to 463 lbs. In terms of total
volume increased (4.4%) from 940 to 981 lbs poundage, the amount of recycled material per
during the second wave. At the Incentive site, student dropped from 1.55 to 1.31 lbs. At
the increase was larger (12.5%), going from the Incentive site, recycled paper increased
885 to 996 lbs. On a per student basis this while glass decreased significantly. Volume of
corresponded to an increase from 1.48 to recycled paper showed a modest increase
1.55 lbs at the Information site and from 1.10 to (6.5%) from 512 to 545 lbs. The volume of glass
1.23 lbs at the Incentive site. The volume of recycled was significantly lower (61.8%)
paper recycled increased at both sites, as did going from 484 to 185 lbs. Recycling volume
the volume of glass. Specifically, recycled per student dropped from 1.24 to 0.91 lb.
paper went up (5.0%) at the Information site
from 481 to 505 lbs while the Incentive site
recorded a higher increase (16.1%) growing
Changes in recycling output in
from 441 to 512 lbs. Recycled glass showed a
comparison to baseline
smaller increase (3.7%) at the Information site
going from 459 to 476 lbs but continued to Compared to baseline measures, the growth in
grow at a strong rate (12.5%) from 444 to recycling poundage at both sites was extre-
484 lbs at the Incentive site. mely noteworthy. Consider that by the end of

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
Consumer recycling 41

the experiment, total poundage had increased the second wave, and finally fell to 1.93 per
by over 46 per cent in the Information site and cent in the third wave. At the Incentive site, the
by over 28.5 per cent at the Incentive site. increase in the total contamination was much
Total recycled materials grew by 65 per cent at more gradual; it was 1.69 per cent during the
the Information site from baseline to first wave first wave, climbing to 1.91 per cent during the
and by over 55 per cent at the Incentive site. second wave, before easing off to 1.64 per cent
The increase was comprised of a 227 per cent during the third wave. Between baseline and
increase in recycled paper and a modest the third wave, increase in total contamination
increase of 18 per cent in recycled glass at rates at both sites was negligible (0.05%).
the Information site. Correspondingly, the The paper contamination rate at the
Incentive site recorded a relatively moderate Information site grew from 1.36 to 2.18 per
increase of over 76 per cent in recycled paper cent by the end of the third wave. In the
and close to 40 per cent in recycled glass. interim, the rate increased to 2.29 per cent
During the second wave, recycled materials during the first wave, followed by a substantial
continued to grow by over 72 per cent at the drop to 0.99 per cent during the second
Information site and over 75 per cent at the wave. At the Incentive site, the paper
Incentive site. At the Information site this contamination rate was 1.0 per cent to start
consisted of an increase of over 243 per cent with at baseline. The rate dropped to 0.92 per
in recycled paper and nearly 23 per cent in cent by the time the experiment was con-
recycled glass. By comparison, the increase in cluded. In between, the rate grew to 1.59
recycled paper (104.8%) and glass (52.2%) was per cent during the first wave and then fell to
lower at the Incentive site. In the third wave, 1.37 per cent during the second wave. The
which was 4 weeks after the treatments had glass contamination rates at the Information
been discontinued at both sites, the volume of site (2.06%) and the Incentive site (2.04%)
materials recycled was higher than the baseline were approximately equal during baseline.
volume, but lower than the increase recorded However, at the Information site, following a
during the second wave. Increase in recycled rapid increase during the first wave (3.49%)
paper was almost 150 per cent over baseline at and second wave (4.41%), the rate dropped
the Information site and 118 per cent at the rapidly to below baseline (1.73%) by the
Incentive site. Increase in recycled glass was end of the experiment. Correspondingly, at
over 19 per cent at the Information site, but the Incentive site, the glass contamination
actually dropped by 42 per cent relative to rate dropped to 1.80 per cent during the first
baseline at the Incentive site. These support wave, rose to 2.48 per cent during the second
H3 in all but one case. wave, and continued to climb to 3.78 per cent
during the third wave.

Changes in contamination rates Social class and recycling/


environmental attitudes and behaviors
Next, we focused on the differences in total,
paper, and glass contamination rates at both Items used to construct the social class scale
sites. During the baseline period, the total are reproduced in Appendix. Our measure of
contamination rate at the Information site social class includes family income and the
(1.87%) was higher than at the Incentive site education and occupation of both parents. The
(1.58%). The growth in volume of materials summed scales ranged from 6 to 32. We got
recycled during the experiment was accom- 205 (14.25%) completed and usable surveys
panied by an increase in total contamination from both sites; these were used in this
rates at both sites. At the Information site, the analysis. Based on the 75th, 50th, and 25th
rate increased from to 2.87 per cent during first percentiles on the summed social class scale,
wave, falling slightly to 2.85 per cent during we categorized our sample into three social

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
42 Easwar S. Iyer and Rajiv K. Kashyap

classes, upper (n ¼ 38), middle (n ¼ 132), and measures, that is, environmental attitude,
lower classes (n ¼ 35); the cutoff scores were environmental behavior, recycling attitude,
15 (lower class) and 25 (middle class). and recycling behavior. The mean scores on
In order to investigate the effects of social all the four dependent measures for upper,
class on environmental and recycling attitude middle, and lower classes are reported in
and behavior, we employed a multivariate Table 4. We found a similar pattern for
analysis of variance. Since gender is the only environmental attitude, environmental beha-
demographic variable known to be strongly vior, and recycling attitude. Highest mean
related to recycling attitude and behavior, scores were observed for the lower social class
we did the analysis with gender and social group, followed by the middle class, and then
class as the two independent variables, even the upper class. In the case of environmental
though our primary interest was only in social attitude, the scores were 87.74 for the lower
class. As mentioned earlier, our four depen- class, 84.17 for the middle class, and 82.79 for
dent measures were environmental attitude, the upper class. For environmental behavior,
environmental behavior, recycling attitude, the mean scores were 49.94 (lower), 46.86
and recycling behavior. We have already (middle), and 45.26 (upper). For recycling
provided details on the number of items used attitude, the mean scores were 63.23 (lower),
and the manner in which these measures were 59.89 (middle), and 56.71 (upper). For recy-
constructed. cling behavior, the highest mean score was
Results of this analysis are presented in observed for the lower class (29.17), the
Table 3. It is interesting to note that, as second highest mean score was observed for
expected and consistent with past findings, the upper class (26.44), and the lowest score
gender had a strong significant effect on was for the middle class (26.34). It is clear that
environmental attitude (F ¼ 7.06, p < 0.01), recycling attitude and behavior varies inversely
environmental behavior (F ¼ 14.04, p < 0.01), with social class. Although we are not fully able
and recycling behavior (F ¼ 4.61, p < 0.05), to explain this result, we presume it to imply
although surprisingly its effect on recycl- that the norms and influence of other family
ing attitude (F ¼ 3.21, p < 0.10) was weak. members might be an underlying factor that
Specifically, women had more favorable atti- varies with social class. This would be a matter
tudes toward the environment and recycling for further exploration.
and were more likely to engage in environ-
mentally friendly and recycling behaviors; this
underscores a well-established relationship. Conclusion and implications
However, social class had a significant effect Taken collectively our findings have signifi-
only on recycling attitude (F ¼ 3.09, p < 0.05) cantly added to our understanding of con-
and recycling behavior (F ¼ 3.651, p < 0.05), sumer recycling. Here below we offer six
but not environmental attitude or environ- conclusions and their implications before
mental behavior. Lower social class subjects closing this section with a few recommen-
held significantly more favorable environmen- dations.
tal and recycling attitudes than middle or
upper class respondents and were more likely
Role and value of intervention
to engage in environmentally friendly and
recycling behaviors. Similarly, middle class First, interventions are vital to encourage
respondents showed more favorable environ- recycling. Even though recycling has received
mental and recycling attitudes and were more unprecedented publicity and support, it is still
inclined to engage in recycling behaviors than in its infancy from a consumer behavior
upper class respondents. perspective. By this we mean that recycling
Pursuant to the unexpected direction of may not yet have become central and
results we tabulated scores on all dependent embedded in most of the consumers’ beha-

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
Consumer recycling 43

vioral scripts. Thus, frequent interventions are at the Incentive site. This is suggestive of a
needed to remind consumers about recycling ‘rational knowledge based’ model of human
and its importance in conserving resources. In behavior. The effectiveness of information-
the past, researchers have studied the impact based intervention may be longer lasting
of message content, format, or media on because it tends to influence a more funda-
consumers’ recycling attitudes and behaviors. mental driver of consumer behavior, that is,
Our study also suggests that message fre- values and beliefs. However, since our incen-
quency is an important consideration. Con- tive was structured as a group benefit, we
ceivably in 10–20 years from now, recycling cannot comment on how individually oriented
may have become a part of every consumer’s incentives would work. At the very least
scripted and habitual behaviors. If so, inter- consumer value orientation, for example,
ventions may be needed at a much-reduced individualistic versus collectivistic (see
frequency or may not be needed at all, but until McCarty and Shrum, 2001) needs to be
then frequent interventions are needed to included in future studies. And lastly, it is
motivate consumers. not clear if the added costs incurred in
disseminating information – akin to incentives
– will be offset by the increased output.
Incentives or information
Second, offering incentives appears to have Gender
an immediate and dramatic effect on
Fourth, gender has an important role. Simply
recycling behaviors. The increase in output,
stated, women are more environmentally
especially paper, was remarkable and mani-
friendly than men. Whether it is because
fested within a few weeks of announcing the
women are ‘inherently’ closer to nature or
incentive, strongly implying that people
because they are more closely involved in
adhere to the ‘rational incentive based’ model
managing the day-to-day household activities is
of behavior. The added cost of running an
unclear; that they are more environmentally
incentive based program may not be fully
friendly is definite. We highlight this finding,
offset by the increased recycling output
not because it is new, novel or counter
(Porter et al., 1995). Nonetheless, offering
intuitive, but only because it strongly reaffirms
incentives is an excellent way to instill this
extant knowledge. Clearly women need to be
new behavior, especially if the goal is to boost
significantly involved in any program that
short-term output. It was interesting to find a
promotes recycling. For instance, in the home
marked decrease in recycling rates of glass at
setting, any promotional or educational
the incentive site after the intervention was
message should be targeted at women. More-
discontinued. This suggests that subjects
over, media appropriate to women have to be
exposed to the incentive treatment might
selected in order to maximize the impact of
have been alerted to the possibility of another
any promotional message.
recycling-related incentive, that is, reimburse-
ments for recycling bottles and cans. Such
Social class
second-order effects have rarely been men-
tioned in the literature and provide another Fifth, social class is significant, although its
fruitful avenue for future research. influence is intriguing and warrants close
Third, disseminating information that scrutiny. We will discuss the fact that social
increases consumers’ knowledge has a more class, measured in a novel manner, influences
lasting effect on recycling output than offer- recycling, and follow that with a discussion on
ing incentives. This is clearly evident from the how it influences recycling.
fact that even after the interventions were Single demographic measures are weak
withdrawn, recycling output at the Infor- predictors of recycling attitudes and beha-
mation site was considerably higher than that viors (Roberts, 1996; Diamantopoulos et al.,

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
44 Easwar S. Iyer and Rajiv K. Kashyap

2003), suggesting that composite measures, Environmental attitude/behavior and


such as social class, might have greater recycling attitude/behavior
predictive power. Thus, we included edu-
cation and occupation of both parents besides Sixth, recycling attitudes and behaviors are
only weakly correlated to environmental
family income to create a composite measure
of social class. The significance of our social attitudes and behaviors. We found that
social class was a significant determinant of
class measure suggests that family members,
only recycling attitudes and behaviors and
and by extension, others in the social, work,
not of environmental attitudes and behaviors.
or peer groups, can be effective sources of
What does that imply? For one, trying to
influence. We would recommend that
waste managers, in their efforts to motivate engender a more favorable attitude toward
recycling by exclusively appealing to one’s
individual recycling behavior, expand their
focus to include members of the extended environmental concerns may be insufficient
and inappropriate. Second, it suggests that
family as well.
Berger (1997) has argued that socio- environmentally friendly behaviors may
include an array of behaviors that may not
economic factors may positively influence
necessarily include recycling behaviors. For
recycling behavior only because affluent
some consumers avoiding consumption,
neighborhoods tend to have easier access to
thereby precluding the need to recycle,
recycling. However, our findings are contrary
may be the signature to being environmen-
to this expectation. Why is it the case? We do
tally friendly. A third reason could be that
not have a clear answer but can provide three
speculative explanations. We call the first one consumers do not see their individual recy-
cling having any significant effect on the
the ‘economic payoff’ hypothesis. The logic
goes as follows. Recycling bottles and cans global environmental problems. Clearly
there is a need to better understand the
involves a financial incentive; this incentive,
being stronger for consumers in a lower social linkage between recycling attitude/behavior
and environmental attitude/behavior.
class, they are more likely to recycle glass
bottles and cans. Eventually, by force of habit
and routinization, they begin to recycle other
Recommendations
materials as well. If our economic payoff
hypothesis is true, it speaks to the significance Our study has demonstrated the significant
of the ‘bottle deposit’ bill that was enacted into value of intervention programs. Incentives
legislation more than a decade ago. Our second are a fine way to motivate people to adopt a
explanation, termed ‘defensive NIMBY,’ has new behavior, but information needs to be
the following logic. Most landfills and incin- disseminated in order to help sustain the
erators tend to be located in poorer commu- newly learned behavior. Thus, we could
nities, and since recycling is a way to diminish speculate that programs aimed at promoting
landfilling and incineration, by actively recy- recycling might have to include incentives
cling, lower social class consumers can fore- and information. If so, how do we allocate
stall siting landfills or incinerators in their resources between incentives and infor-
communities. Thus, for them, recycling is a mation? Although not directly addressed in
defensive maneuver that will prevent invasion our study, we feel that a flexible policy, one
of their ‘backyard.’ We base our third expla- that is initially biased toward incentives but
nation on ‘overacculturating.’ Our data were gradually shifts toward information cam-
gathered at a state university, which tends to paigns in the later stages, might be an optimal
be very ‘middle class’ in its composition and choice. The rate at which resources are
values. Those in the lower class, we speculate, shifted needs to be based on monitoring key
were attempting to ‘fit in,’ leading to the outcomes, such as number of new recyclers,
results we observed. values, and beliefs of recyclers, etc. A flexible

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
Consumer recycling 45

combination of incentives and information, responsible investing, environmentalism, and


whose relative weights reflect critical bench- marketing strategy. His research has been pub-
marked outcomes, may be the most effective lished in the Journal of Marketing, Journal of
and efficient way to introduce recycling in Applied Social Psychology, Academy of Man-
the short run and sustain continued participa- agement Learning and Education, Academy of
tion in the long run; this can be the subject of Marketing Science Review, and elsewhere.
future research.
Acknowledgements
Biographical notes The authors would like to acknowledge the
assistance and cooperation of John Pepi at
Easwar S. Iyer (Ph.D. University of Pitts- the Physical Plant Waste Management facility
burgh) is Associate Professor of Marketing at the University of Massachusetts. They
and Head of the Marketing Department at would also like to thank two anonymous
the Isenberg School of Management, Univer- reviewers and the editor for their guidance
sity of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA. He is and suggestions.
very interested in environmental issues as they
apply to business practices and has written
extensively on various aspects of that topic References
such as environmental management, environ-
mental alliances, green advertising, socially Berger I. 1997. The demographics of recycling and
responsible consumer behavior, among others. the structure of environmental behavior.
Rajiv K. Kashyap (Ph.D. University of Mass- Environment and Behavior 28(4): 515–531
achusetts Amherst) is Associate Professor and (July).
Chair, Department of Marketing and Manage- Berger I, Kanetkar V. 1996. Increasing environmen-
tal sensitivity via workplace experiences. Jour-
ment, William Paterson University, NJ. His
nal of Public Policy and Marketing 14(2):
current research interests include socially
205–215.
Bratt C. 1999. Consumers’ environmental behavior:
generalized, sector-based or compensatory? Envir-
Appendix onment and Behavior 31(1): 28–44 (January).
Burgess RL, Clark RN, Hendee JC. 1971.
Social class measures An experimental analysis of anti-litter procedures.

My family income is & <$29 k & $39–39 k & $40–49 k


& $50–74 k & $75–100 k & >$100 k
Which of the following describes your parents’ present or past occupation?
Father Mother
Homemaker & &
Steadily employed, for example, custodian & &
Assembly line worker, bus drive, sales clerk & &
Police, fire fighter, skilled craftsman, office worker & &
Owner of small firm, technician, civil servant & &
Middle manager, school teacher, registered nurse & &
Owner medium size firm, dentist, engineer, professor & &
Doctor, lawyer, owner large business & &
Which of the following describes your parents’ education?
My mother’s highest study level & No high school & High school & Some college
& Undergraduate degree & Graduate degree
My father’s highest study level & No high school & High school & Some college
& Undergraduate degree & Graduate degree

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
46 Easwar S. Iyer and Rajiv K. Kashyap

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 2(Summer): Geller ES, Chaffee JL, Ingram RE. 1975. Promoting
71–75. paper recycling on a university campus.
Burn SM. 1991. Social psychology and the stimu- Journal of Environmental Systems 5(1): 39–57.
lation of recycling behaviors: the block leader Granzin KL, Olsen JE. 1991. Characterizing partici-
approach. Journal of Applied Social Psychology pants in activities protecting the environment: a
21 611–629. focus on donating, recycling and conservation
Business Week. 1989. Snap, Crackle, Stop, Septem- behaviors. Journal of Public Policy and Market-
ber 25, p. 154. ing 10(2): 1–27.
Carson R. 1962. The Silent Spring. Fawcett Publi- Gray D. 1985. Ecological Beliefs and Behaviors:
cations: Greenwich CT. Assessment and Change. Greenwood: Westport,
Clark RN, Burgess RL, Hendee JC. 1972. The devel- CT.
opment of anti-litter behavior in a forest Heath RP. 1998. The new working class. American
campground. Journal of Applied behavior Demographics 20(1): 51–55 (January).
Analysis 1(Spring): 1–5. Hopper JR, Nielson JM. 1991. Recycling as altruistic
Coleman RP. 1960. The significance of social stra- behavior: normative and behavioral strategies to
tification in selling. In Marketing: A Maturing expand participation in community recycling
Discipline, Bell ML (ed.). American Marketing programs. Environment and Behavior 23: 195–
Association: Chicago; 171–184. 220.
Curlee RT. 1986. The Economic Feasibility of Hornik J, Cherian J, Madansky M, Narayana C. 1995.
Recycling. Praeger: New York. Determinants of recycling behavior: a synthesis
Derksen L, Gartrell J. 1993. The social context of of research results. The Journal of Socio-
recycling. American Sociological Review 58: Economics 24(1): 105–128 (Spring).
434–442. Humphrey C, Bord R, Hammond M, Mann S. 1977.
Diamantopoulos A, Schlegelmilch BB, Sinkovics Attitudes and conditions for cooperation in a
RR, Bohlen GM. 2003. Can socio-demographics paper recycling program. Environment and
still play a role in profiling green consumers? A Behavior 9: 107–124.
review of the evidence and an empirical investi- Jacobs HE, Bailey JS, Crews JI. 1984. Development
gation. Journal of Business research 56: and analysis of community-based resource recov-
465–480. ery program. Journal of Applied Behavior
Diamond WD, Loewy BZ. 1991. Effects of probabil- Analysis 17(2): 127–145 (Summer).
istic rewards on recycling attitudes and behavior. Kashyap RK, Iyer ES. 2001. Attitudes and behaviors
Journal of Applied Social Psychology 21: of college students in regards to the environment
1590–1607. and recycling. In Developments in Marketing
Ellen PS. 1994. Do we know what we need Science (Vol. 24), Moore M, Moore R (eds).
to know? Objective and subjective knowl- Academy of Marketing Science; 71–79.
edge effects on pro-ecological behaviors. Lakhan VC, Lavalle PD. 2002. Use of loglinear
Journal of Business Research 30(May): 43– models to assess factors influencing concern
52. for the natural environment. Environmental
Everett JW, Peirce JJ. 1991–1992. Social networks, Management 30(1): 77–87.
socioeconomic status, and environmental collec- Lansana FM. 1992. Distinguishing potential recy-
tive action: residential curbside block leader clers from nonrecyclers: a basis for developing
recycling. Journal of Environmental Systems recycling strategies. Journal of Environmental
21: 65–84. Education 23: 16–23.
Gagne ED, Yekovich CW, Yekovich FR. 1993. The Lord KR. 1994. Motivating recycling behavior: a
Cognitive Psychology of School Learning (2nd quasiexperimental investigation of message and
edn). Harper Collins: New York. source strategies. Psychology and Marketing
Gamba RJ, Oskamp S. 1994. Factors influencing 11(4): 341–358.
community residents’ participation in com- Luyben PD, Bailey JS. 1979. Newspaper recycling:
mingled curbside recycling programs. Environ- the effects of rewards and proximity of containers.
ment and Behavior 26: 587–612. Environment and behavior 11: 539–557.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb
Consumer recycling 47

McCarty JA, Shrum LJ. 2001. The influence of Roberts JA. 1996. Will the real socially responsible
individualism, collectivism, and locus of control consumer please step forward? Business
on environmental beliefs and behavior. Journal Horizons 39(1) (January–February): 79–
of Public Policy & Marketing 20(Spring): 83.
93–104. Schaninger C. 1981. Social class versus income
Miller CK, Jensen GL, Achterberg C. 1999. Evalu- revisited: an empirical investigation. Journal of
ation of food label nutrition intervention for Marketing Research 18(May): 192–208.
women with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Journal Stern P, Dietz T, Kalof L. 1993. Value orientations,
of American Dietetic Association 99(3): gender, and environmental concern. Environ-
323–328. ment and Behavior 25: 322–348.
Mohai P, Twight B. 1987. Age and environmental Tasady L. 1991. Shopping for a Better Environ-
concern: an elaboration of the buttel model using ment. Meadowbrook Press: New York.
national survey evidence. Social Science Quar- Taylor S, Todd P. 1995. Understanding household
terly 68: 798–815. garbage reduction behavior: a test of an inte-
Oskamp S, Harrington MJ, Edwards TC, Sherwood grated model. Journal of Public Policy and
DL, Okuda SM, Swanson DC. 1991. Factors Marketing 14(Fall): 192–204.
influencing household recycling behavior. Travis R, Kohli V. 1995. The birth order factor:
Environment and Behavior 23(4): 494–519. ordinal position, social strata, and educational
Ostman RE, Parker JL. 1988. Impact of education, achievement. The Journal of Social Psychology
age, newspapers, and television on environmen- 135(4): 499–507.
tal knowledge, concerns, and behaviors. Journal U.S. House of Representatives, Committee on Agri-
of Environmental Education 19: 3–9. culture. 1989. Recyclable Materials Science and
Porter BE, Leeming FC, Dwyer WO. 1995. Solid Technology Act of 1989, Hearing before the 101st
waste recovery: a review of behavioral programs Congress, 1st session on H.R. 500, #101-49.
to increase recycling. Environment and Beha- Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
vior 27(March): 122–152. Vining J, Ebreo A. 1990. What makes a recycler? A
Reid D, Luyben P, Rawers R, Bailey J. 1976. News- comparison of recyclers and nonrecyclers.
paper recycling behavior: the effects of prompt- Environment and Behavior 22: 55–73.
ing and proximity of containers. Environment Vining J, Ebreo A. 1992. Predicting recycling beha-
and Behavior 8: 471–481. vior from global and specific environmental atti-
Roberts JA. 1995. Profiling levels of socially respon- tudes and changes in recycling opportunities.
sible consumer behavior: a cluster analytic Journal of Applied Social Psychology 22:
approach and its implications for marketing. 1580–1607.
Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice www.epa.gov2004. Municipal Solid Waste: Basic
3(4) (Fall): 97–117. Facts.

Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, Jan.–Feb. 2007
DOI: 10.1002/cb

Potrebbero piacerti anche