Sei sulla pagina 1di 547

FORENSIC NEUROPSYCHOLOGY

This page intentionally left blank


SECOND EDITION

FORENSIC
NEUROPSYCHOLOGY

A Scientific Approach

EDITED BY

G L E N N J. L A R R A B E E

1
1
Published in the United States of America by Oxford University Press, Inc.,
198 Madison Avenue, New York, NY, 10016

United States of America

Oxford University Press, Inc. publishes works that further Oxford University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship,
and education
Oxford is a registered trademark of Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries

Copyright © Oxford University Press, Inc. 2012, 2005

First Edition published in 2005

All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted,
in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, Inc., or as expressly
permitted by law, by licence, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reproduction rights organization. Inquiries
concerning reproduction outside the scope of the above should be sent to the Rights Department,
Oxford University Press, Inc., at the address above
You must not circulate this work in any other form and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer
_______________________________________________________________________
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Forensic neuropsychology : a scientific approach / edited by Glenn J. Larrabee. — 2nd ed.


p. cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978-0-19-538352-2 (hardback : alk. paper)
1. Forensic neuropsychology. I. Larrabee, Glenn J.
RA1147.5.F668 2011
614'.15—dc23 2011036769
_______________________________________________________________________

1 3 5 7 9 10 8 6 4 2
Typeset in Minion
Printed on acid-free paper
Printed in the United States of America

This material is not intended to be, and should not be considered, a substitute for medical or other professional advice. Treatment for
the conditions described in this material is highly dependent on the individual circumstances. And, while this material is designed to
offer accurate information with respect to the subject matter covered and to be current as of the time it was written, research and
knowledge about medical and health issues is constantly evolving and dose schedules for medications are being revised continually,
with new side effects recognized and accounted for regularly. Readers must therefore always check the product information and
clinical procedures with the most up-to-date published product information and data sheets provided by the manufacturers and the
most recent codes of conduct and safety regulation. The publisher and the authors make no representations or warranties to readers,
express or implied, as to the accuracy or completeness of this material. Without limiting the foregoing, the publisher and the authors
make no representations or warranties as to the accuracy or efficacy of the drug dosages mentioned in the material. The authors and
the publisher do not accept, and expressly disclaim, any responsibility for any liability, loss or risk that may be claimed or incurred as a
consequence of the use and/or application of any of the contents of this material.
To Sebastian LaSpina and Andy Vince.

As my son Zack said, “A teacher passes on knowledge about their subject.


A mentor passes on knowledge about life. It is only a great coach
who does both.”

I could not have asked for better coaches for my son. It is with
sincere gratitude that I dedicate this book to you.
This page intentionally left blank
P R E FAC E

The first edition of this book was published in and research. As before, all of the contributors are
2005, at which time the preface identified the recognized scientists and clinicians who have
rapid growth of the field of forensic neuropsy- contributed to the peer-reviewed literature in
chology. In the ensuing six years this growth has neuropsychology and forensic neuropsychology.
accelerated, as reviewed in Chapter 18 in the cur- I gratefully acknowledge the assistance of
rent volume by Sweet and Meyer. These authors Patricia Reynolds, M. L. S. of the Bishopric
show in their Figure 18-1 a more than tripling of Medical Library at Sarasota Memorial Hospital
the mention of “neuropsychology,” “neuropsy- in Florida, who was most helpful in providing
chologist,” or “neuropsychological,” in the legal articles for the three chapters I prepared for the
search engine LexisNexis® Academic for the current volume. I am also grateful to Matthew
decade of 2000 to 2010, in comparison to 1990 to Miliano, B. A., who helped read and proof the
1999. Complimentary data presented in Figure contents of the entire volume. I further appreciate
2-1 in Chapter 2 by Kaufmann shows a near dou- the encouragement and assistance of my editors
bling of legal citations from 2004 to 2009, with a at Oxford University Press, Joan Bossert, Vice
projected accelerated growth in coming years. President/Editorial Director, Medical Division,
Many important developments have occurred and Tracy K. O’Hara, Development Editor,
since the first edition, including the decision of the Medicine. Finally and most important, I appreci-
New Hampshire Supreme Court upholding the ate the love, support, and understanding of my
use of a flexible test battery (Baxter v. Temple, wife, Jan, and our son, Zack, who tolerated the
2008) and the consensus statement of the American additional demands on my time during the
Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology on the use completion of this revision.
of measures of symptom validity and response bias G.J.L.
(Heilbronner, Sweet, Morgan, Larrabee, Millis, & May 2011
conference participants, 2009). The continuing
rapid growth and significant developments in REFERENCES
forensic neuropsychology have resulted in the Baxter v. Temple, 949 A. 2d 167, (N.H., 2008).
need for a revision of the first edition. Heilbronner, R. L., Sweet, J. J., Morgan, J. E., Larrabee,
I was extremely fortunate to have all of the G. J., Millis, S. R., & Conference Participants.
senior authors agree to contribute revisions of the (2009). American Academy of Clinical Neuro-
work they contributed to the first edition. psychology consensus conference statement on the
Additionally, there are five new chapters, includ- neuropsychological assessment of effort, response
bias, and malingering. The Clinical Neuro-
ing a chapter on admissibility of evidence by
psychologist, 23, 1093–129.
Kaufmann, a chapter on perinatal injury by Taylor,
a chapter by Greve, Bianchini, and Ord on foren-
sic assessment of chronic pain, a chapter by
Andrikopoulos and Greiffenstein on posttrau-
matic stress disorder, and a concluding chapter by
Sweet and Meyer on trends in forensic practice
This page intentionally left blank
CONTENTS

Contributors xi 11 Neurotoxic Injury 281


karen i. bolla
1 A Scientific Approach to
12 The Psychological Assessment
Forensic Neuropsychology 3
of Persons with Chronic Pain 302
glenn j. larrabee
kevin w. greve, kevin j. bianchini,
2 Neuropsychology and the Law: and jonathan s. ord
Principles of Productive Attorney-
13 Forensic Assessment of Medically
Neuropsychologist Relations 23
Unexplained Symptoms 336
manfred f. greiffenstein and
laurence m. binder
paul m. kaufmann
14 Something to Talk About? The Status
3 Admissibility of Expert Opinions based
of Post-traumatic Stress Disorder in
on Neuropsychological Evidence 70
Clinical Neuropsychology 365
paul m. kaufmann
jim andrikopoulos and
4 Ethical Practice of Forensic manfred f. greiffenstein
Neuropsychology 101
15 Assessing Civil Competencies in Older
christopher l. grote and Adults with Dementia: Consent
benjamin a. pyykkonen Capacity, Financial Capacity, and
5 Assessment of Malingering 116 Testamentary Capacity 401
glenn j. larrabee daniel c. marson, katina hebert,
and andrea c. solomon
6 Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic
Neuropsychology 160 16 Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology
and Assessment of Competency 438
joseph ricker
robert l. denney
7 Perinatal Brain Injury 179
17 Criminal Responsibility and
h. gerry taylor
Other Criminal Forensic Issues 473
8 Forensic Aspects of Pediatric robert l. denney
Traumatic Brain Injury 211
18 Trends in Forensic Practice
jacobus donders
and Research 501
9 Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 231 jerry j. sweet and
glenn j. larrabee dawn giuffre meyer
10 Moderate and Severe Traumatic Index 517
Brain Injury 260
tresa roebuck-spencer and
mark sherer
This page intentionally left blank
C O N T R I BU TO R S

Jim Andrikopoulos, PhD, ABPP Jacobus Donders, PhD, ABPP


Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology
Ruan Neurology Clinic and Rehabilitation Psychology
Des Moines, IA Psychology Service
Mary Free Bed Rehabilitation Hospital
Kevin J. Bianchini, PhD, ABN Grand Rapids, MI
Board Certified in Professional Neuropsychology
Jefferson Neurobehavioral Group Manfred F. Greiffenstein, PhD, ABPP
Metairie, LA Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology and
Department of Psychology Forensic Psychology
University of New Orleans Psychological Systems, Inc.
New Orleans, LA Royal Oak, MI

Laurence M. Binder, PhD, ABPP Dawn Giuffre Meyer, PhD


Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology National Rehabilitation Hospital
Independent practice, Beaverton, OR Washington, D.C.
Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry
Oregon Health and Science University Kevin W. Greve, PhD, ABPP
Portland, OR Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology
Department of Psychology
Karen I. Bolla, PhD University of New Orleans
Departments of Neurology, Psychiatry and New Orleans, LA
Behavioral Sciences, and Environmental Jefferson Neurobehavioral Group
Health Sciences Metairie, LA
Johns Hopkins University, Bayview
Medical Center Christopher L. Grote, PhD, ABPP
Baltimore, MD Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology
Department of Behavioral Sciences
Robert L. Denney, PsyD, ABPP Rush University Medical Center
Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology Chicago, IL
and Forensic Psychology
U.S. Medical Center for Federal Prisoners Katina Hebert, PhD
School of Professional Psychology at Department of Veterans Affairs
Forest Institute Medical Center
Springfield, MO Tuscaloosa, AL
xii contibutors

Paul M. Kaufmann, JD, PhD, ABPP Tresa Roebuck-Spencer, PhD, ABPP


Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology
Nebraska Department of Health and Department of Psychology
Human Services University of Oklahoma
University of Nebraska Norman, OK
Lincoln, NE
Mark Sherer, PhD, ABPP
Glenn J. Larrabee, PhD, ABPP Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology
Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology TIRR Memorial Hermann
Independent practice, Sarasota, FL Baylor College of Medicine
Houston, TX
Daniel C. Marson, JD, PhD
Department of Neurology and Alzheimer’s Andrea C. Solomon, PhD
Disease Center Solomon Neuropsychology, LLC
University of Alabama at Birmingham Montgomery, AL
Birmingham, AL
Jerry J. Sweet, PhD, ABPP
Jonathan S. Ord, MS Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology and
Department of Psychology Clinical Psychology
University of New Orleans NorthShore University HealthSystem
New Orleans, LA Evanston, IL
Jefferson Neurobehavioral Group University of Chicago Pritzker School of
Metairie, LA Medicine
Chicago, IL
Benjamin A. Pyykkonen, PhD
Department of Behavioral Sciences H. Gerry Taylor, PhD., ABPP
Rush University Medical Center Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology
Chicago, IL Department of Pediatrics
Case Western Reserve University, and
Joseph Ricker, PhD, ABPP Rainbow Babies & Children’s Hospital
Board Certified in Clinical Neuropsychology University Hospitals Case Medical Center
and Rehabilitation Psychology Cleveland, OH
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine
Pittsburgh, PA
FORENSIC NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
This page intentionally left blank
1
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology
G L E N N J. L A R R A B E E

This book emphasizes a scientific approach to the overuse of ad hoc hypotheses to immunize claims
practice of forensic neuropsychology: the applica- from falsification; evasion of peer review; absence
tion of neuropsychology to legal issues that arise of self-correction; and emphasis on confirmation
in both civil and criminal legal proceedings. In rather than refutation. Regarding this last point,
certain circumstances, neuropsychological defi- Lilienfeld et al. (2003) cited the physicist Feynman
cits have a direct bearing on legal issues; for exam- (1985), who maintained that the essence of
ple, in establishment of damages in a personal science is a bending over backward to prove one-
injury case (see chapters 5 and 7–14, this volume). self wrong. Referencing Popper (1959) and Meehl
In other settings, a person may have impaired (1978), Lilienfeld et al. noted that scientists ideally
neuropsychological test scores, but the impair- subject their claims to grave risk of refutation,
ment alone does not provide the answer to the contrasted with pseudoscientists, who tend to
legal issue; for example, an older person may have seek only confirming evidence for their claims.
dementia, but still possess competency to execute Because a determined advocate for a particular
a valid will (chapter 15, this volume); a person pseudoscientific position can find at least some
facing criminal charges may have neuropsycho- supportive evidence for essentially any claim,
logical impairment, but still be found competent Lilienfeld et al. described this confirmatory
to stand trial and be responsible for the criminal hypothesis testing strategy as “an inefficient means
act (chapters 16 and 17, this volume). of rooting out error in one’s web of beliefs” (p. 7).
Greiffenstein and Kaufmann (chapter 2), and Subsequently, Lilienfeld (in press) provided a
Kaufmann (chapter 3) discuss issues related to the basic definition of science as a set of systematic
admissibility of expert neuropsychological testi- safeguards against confirmation bias.
mony related to Frye v. United States (1923) and to The basic aim of science is explanation, and
Daubert v. Merrell Dow (1993), legal standards the explanations offered by science are theories
that emphasize acceptability of a particular scien- (Kerlinger, 1973). More formally specified, theo-
tific methodology by one’s peers and standards ries are sets of concepts, definitions, and proposi-
that mandate a particular methodology meet sci- tions that present a systematic view of phenomena
entific principles, such as possessing a known by specifying relations among variables, with the
error rate, and the potential to be falsified or dis- purpose of explaining and predicting the phe-
credited (cf. Popper, 1959). nomena (Kerlinger, 1973). Kerlinger further
defined research as “systematic, controlled, empir-
W H AT I S S C I E N C E ? ical, and critical investigation of hypothetical
The goals of the empirical natural sciences include propositions about the presumed relations among
exploration, description, explanation, and predic- rational phenomena” (p. 11).
tion of worldly occurrences or phenomena (Badia Science is hypotheticodeductive (Badia &
& Runyon, 1982; Hempel, 1966; Kerlinger, 1973). Runyon, 1982; Hempel, 1966; Kerlinger, 1973).
Science is concerned with things that can be A hypothesis represents a conjectural statement
publicly observed and tested as opposed to meta- or tentative proposition about the relation between
physical explanations (Kerlinger, 1973). two or more variables, for example, between
Science differs from pseudoscience in a duration of coma and memory test performance
number of ways (Lilienfeld, Lynn, & Lohr, 2003). 1-year status post–traumatic brain injury (TBI).
Some of the features of pseudoscience include The scientist then deduces the consequences of
4 forensic neuropsychology

the hypothesis he or she has formulated (e.g., alleged TBI, alternative hypotheses could include
those brain-injured subjects with shorter dura- (a) neuropsychological deficits secondary to TBI;
tions of coma will perform better on memory (b) no evidence for any neuropsychological
testing than will those with longer durations of deficits; (c) neuropsychological deficits secondary
coma). to a preexisting condition such as learning dis-
A long tradition in psychology is the Fisherian ability, hypertension, or dementia; (d) neuropsy-
tradition of specifying a hypothesis for a given chological deficits secondary to a psychiatric
relationship (e.g., between coma and memory test condition that is related or unrelated to the
performance), which is then contrasted with a accident in question; or (e) neuropsychological
hypothesis of no effect, otherwise known as the deficits secondary to malingering.
null hypothesis (Kerlinger, 1973; Meehl, 1978). The appropriate use of logical and scientific
Meehl criticized the null hypothesis testing reasoning in performing a forensic evaluation is
approach as responsible for the slow progress of critical to avoid committing diagnostic errors.
“soft psychology,” resulting in “a disturbing At present, there is a growing problem of overdiag-
absence of that cumulative character that is so nosis of neuropsychological deficits in legal set-
impressive in disciplines like astronomy, molecu- tings because of failure to analyze cases critically
lar biology, and genetics” (p. 807). As Meehl noted, and scientifically (Faust, Ziskin, & Hiers, 1991;
the null hypothesis, taken literally, is always false. Larrabee, 1990, 2000b; Russell, 1990; Wedding &
He recommended the Popperian approach Faust, 1989). This failure frequently results in inad-
(Popper, 1959) of subjecting hypotheses to grave equate differential diagnosis (Binder, 1997).
danger of refutation with modus tollens: “If p, then Faulty logic commonly leads to diagnostic
q; not q, therefore not p.” In this manner, theories error. Miller (1983) noted the argument “if
or hypotheses that survive the most attempts at damage to structure X is known to produce a
refutation are the best-supported theories. decline on Test T, it is tempting to argue that any
In his 1978 article, Meehl did specify “five new subject or group of subjects having a rela-
noble traditions in clinical psychology,” two of tively poor performance on T must have a lesion
which are directly relevant to the practice of at X” is the same as the argument “because a horse
clinical and forensic neuropsychology: (a) descrip- meets the test of being a large animal with four
tive clinical psychiatry (for the discipline of legs, then any newly encountered large animal
neuropsychology, descriptive behavioral neurol- with four legs is a horse” (p. 131). Larrabee (1990,
ogy can also be included) and (b) psychometric 2000b) extended Miller’s (1983) example to
assessment. Arguably, both of these “noble tradi- the diagnostic decision of “brain damage” versus
tions” have led to an accumulation of information “no brain damage”: If brain-damaged subjects
in clinical psychology in general, and neuropsy- perform poorly on neuropsychological tests, then
chology in particular, contributing to the cumula- any new person who performs poorly on neurop-
tive nature of knowledge in these disciplines that sychological tests must be brain damaged.
Meehl found lacking in comparisons of the “soft” As Russell (1990) and Russell and Kolitz-
science of psychology with the “hard” sciences Russell (2003) observed, neuropsychological tests
such as astronomy. are measures of cognitive abilities rather than tests
of brain damage. Consequently, neuropsychologi-
A S C I E N T I F I C A P P ROAC H cal test performance can be poor for many reasons
TO N E U RO P S Y C H O L O G I C A L other than brain damage, including limited coop-
E VA L UAT I O N eration or inattentiveness caused by fatigue, pain,
Conducting a neuropsychological evaluation and discomfort, medication effects, substance abuse,
making a neuropsychological diagnosis regarding learning disability, psychiatric diagnosis, or poor
the presence of brain damage can be conce- motivation (Binder, 1997; Larrabee, 1990).
ptualized as a scientific endeavor. The scientific Faust and colleagues have written extensively
endeavor includes formulation of hypotheses about sources of error and bias in clinical decision
that are then checked for support or, better yet, making (Faust, 1989; Faust & Nurcombe, 1989;
subjected to attempts at refutation. The formula- Faust et al., 1991; Wedding & Faust, 1989). Failure
tion of various different hypotheses corresponds to consider base rate information can be a major
to formulation of differential diagnoses. More factor in misdiagnosis. The term base rate refers to
specifically, in a particular civil forensic case the frequency with which something occurs; for
seeking compensation for damages from an example, if 5 in 100 persons with mild traumatic
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology 5

brain injury (MTBI) suffer persisting neuropsy- reported by 62% of MCs and 88% of non-CNS
chological deficits, the base rate is 5%. Lack of litigants; and “irritability” was reported by 38% of
awareness of base rates and biases such as confir- MCs and 77% of non-CNS litigants.
mation bias or hindsight bias can lead to the forma- The data presented by Lees-Haley and Brown
tion of illusory correlations or “seeing” relationships (1993) highlighted significant problems with the
that do not exist (Wedding & Faust, 1989). sensitivity and specificity of common neuropsy-
The original work on illusory correlations chological symptom checklists. Sensitivity refers
was conducted by the Chapmans (Chapman & to the frequency or rate of occurrence of a finding
Chapman, 1967), who presented psychology among patients with the condition in question,
undergraduates with human figure drawings whereas specificity refers to the frequency of nega-
accompanied by randomly paired symptom tive test results among patients who do not have
statements (e.g., “suspiciousness” would appear in the illness or condition in question (Baldessarini,
association with accented eyes as frequently as it Finklestein, & Arana, 1983). If the base rate of a
appeared in association with nonaccented eyes). symptom used to diagnose concussion such as
Despite the absence of systematic relationships in headache in non-CNS samples is ignored, this
the data, the psychology students attributed would lead to misdiagnosis of brain damage or
diagnostic “signs” to the relationships they had dysfunction in 62% of MCs and 88% of non-CNS
assumed existed in the first place (e.g., associating litigants based on the data compiled by Lees-
accented eyes in human figure drawings with Haley and Brown.
“suspiciousness”). Chapman and Chapman com- Faust et al. (1991) and Wedding and Faust
mented that the erroneously reported correlations (1989) discussed two major types of bias in clini-
corresponded to associative connections between cal judgment and decision making: hindsight bias
symptoms and drawing characteristics (formed and confirmation bias. Hindsight bias is the ten-
by their research subjects), as well as to what their dency to believe, once the outcome of an event is
subjects expect to see before they actually observed. known, that the outcome could have been more
Essentially, the Chapman and Chapman research easily predicted than is actually the case. Thus,
subjects demonstrated both a failure to consider knowing about an event through clinical history
base rates as well as a confirmation bias (i.e., (e.g., a blow to the head in an automobile acci-
seeing what they expected to see). dent) leads clinicians to believe they can predict
Chapman and Chapman (1969) further dem- the outcome of the event and diagnose neuropsy-
onstrated the presence of illusory correlations chological deficits consistent with TBI.
based on the judgments of practicing clinicians. Hindsight bias can be closely associated with
As shown by Kurtz and Garfield (1978), the bias confirmation bias, which has been discussed as the
toward forming illusory correlation could not be tendency to seek confirming evidence at the
overcome, even when subjects were provided with expense of ignoring disconfirming evidence for a
special training against illusory correlations. set of diagnostic hypotheses. In confirmation bias,
The diagnostic significance of base rate data initial hypotheses are subjected to preferential
is further underscored by the investigation by analysis, so that the clinician is much more lenient
Lees-Haley and Brown (1993), who tabulated the or accepting of information supporting the initial
frequency of so-called neuropsychological com- hypothesis and more critical and less accepting of
plaints in two groups of subjects: (a) 50 outpa- information contradicting the initial hypothesis,
tients from a group family practice clinic and demonstrating a propensity toward asymmetric
(b) 170 litigants filing personal injury claims for error costs (Trope, Gervey, & Liberman, 1997).
emotional distress or industrial stress, with no Moreover, Trope et al. observed that persons are
known history of head injury, toxic exposure, more likely to terminate hypothesis testing pre-
seizure disorder, or neuropsychological impair- maturely once they receive information support-
ment and without any claim for central nervous ing their described hypothesis. Although it is
system (CNS) injury (non-CNS litigants). tempting to attribute confirmation bias to the
Symptoms commonly thought of as indicative or emphasis on null hypothesis testing in psycholog-
“diagnostic” of TBI and neurotoxic exposure ical research (cf. Meehl, 1978), confirmation bias
occurred frequently in the medical controls (MCs) may simply represent a common judgment error
and non-CNS litigants. For example, “difficulty in human decision making (cf. Trope et al., 1997).
concentrating” was reported by 26% of MCs and I previously described an example of confir-
78% of non-CNS litigants; “headache” was mation bias in a litigant alleging TBI (Larrabee,
6 forensic neuropsychology

2000b). In this case, it was questionable that Reminding Test (Buschke, 1973; Hannay & Levin,
the litigant ever struck her head (she claimed 1985; Larrabee, Trahan, & Levin, 2000), Trail
she did; records did not substantiate this claim). Making B (Army Individual Test Battery, 1944),
Nonetheless, the litigant had no loss of conscious- and the Grooved Pegboard (Lafayette Instrument,
ness, post-traumatic amnesia, history of acute P.O. Box 5729, Lafayette, IN)?” Indeed, to counter
focal neurological signs, or abnormal neurological the diagnostic bias to find brain impairment when
findings, yet she was diagnosed as suffering brain there is none (cf. Russell, 1990; Wedding & Faust,
damage on the basis of a Wechsler Memory Scale– 1989), the neuropsychologist should first list all
Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987) Attention evidence suggesting no evidence for brain impair-
Concentration Index (AC) of 75. The psychologist ment (e.g., clinical history of no loss of conscious-
in this case selectively ignored the WMS-R ness or post-traumatic amnesia, normal magnetic
General Memory Index (GM) of 129, which not resonance imaging scan, normal performance on
only contradicted the presence of brain damage, sensitive tests such as Trail Making B and Verbal
but also was highly inconsistent with the patient’s Selective Reminding).
AC of 75. It is logically and clinically inconsistent I suggested a four-component consistency
that a person with impaired attention at the 5th analysis in neuropsychological decision making
percentile could have memory function at the (Larrabee, 1990, 1992, 1997, 2000b):
97th percentile. Actually, the 54-point GM minus
AC difference score had a probability of malinger- 1. Are the data consistent within and
ing greater than 0.99 based on Mittenberg, Azrin, between neuropsychological domains?
Millsaps, and Heilbronner’s (1993) research on 2. Is the neuropsychological profile
detection of malingered head trauma using atypi- consistent with the suspected etiologic
cal patterns of performance on the WMS-R. condition?
Wedding and Faust (1989) provided a number 3. Are the neuropsychological data
of strategies that can be employed to reduce biases consistent with the documented severity
in clinical judgment, beginning with the recom- of injury?
mendation that the clinician know the literature 4. Are the neuropsychological data
on human judgment. Trope et al. (1997) provided consistent with the subject’s behavioral
a concise review of human judgment and decision presentation?
making. The Wedding and Faust article also
reviewed relevant literature on judgment, decision The data to be considered in this consistency
making, and clinical versus actuarial prediction. analysis include a detailed and extensive inter-
Wedding and Faust cautioned against prematurely view, detailed record review, and extensive and
abandoning useful decision-making rules by start- redundant neuropsychological test measures
ing with the most valid information, listing alter- within each of several functional domains, includ-
native diagnostic options and seeking evidence for ing language, perception, sensorimotor function,
each, and systematically listing disconfirmatory attention/information processing, psychomotor
information. Consideration of disconfirmatory speed, verbal and visual learning and memory,
information is particularly important for reducing intelligence and problem solving, and motivation
confirmation bias. Wedding and Faust noted that and personality.
neuropsychologists frequently make up lists of The clinical interview is conducted prior to
test findings that support particular hypotheses, testing and yields information about the subject’s
but recommended also listing all data that argue recollection of the original injury or trauma (e.g.,
against these hypotheses. head injury, toxic exposure, hypoxic event); sub-
Along these lines, I (Larrabee, 2000b) found it sequent symptoms and change in symptoms over
useful to frame hypothetical questions such as, time; other health care providers the subject has
“What kind of brain damage causes poor perfor- seen; and the procedures, diagnoses, and treat-
mance on the Category Test (Reitan & Wolfson, ments the subject has received. This information,
1993), California Verbal Learning Test-Second when cross-checked against the medical records
Edition (CVLT-II; Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, for accuracy, provides an informal clinical assess-
2000), and Finger Tapping (Reitan & Wolfson, ment of the subject’s memory function. In the
1993), with above-average performance on the specific case of closed head injury, detailed ques-
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Heaton, Chelune, tioning about the events leading up to and follow-
Talley, Kay, & Curtiss, 1993), Verbal Selective ing the accident (e.g., where the person was
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology 7

headed prior to the accident, the time the accident Arithmetic, or Digit Symbol; although patients
occurred, recall of events from the scene of the with dementia may perform poorly on Digit Span,
accident, diagnostic procedures in the hospital) Arithmetic, or Digit Symbol, they do not perform
can be compared to the medical records and at above-average levels on complex problem-
allows a retrospective determination of the pres- solving tasks such as WAIS-IV Block Design or
ence and duration of posttraumatic amnesia. the Category Test.
Detailed interviewing about the events of the This general principle also applies to awareness
accident also allows the opportunity to evaluate of test profile patterns that could indicate the pres-
for the presence/absence of the arousal or distress ence of a preexisting condition. I (Larrabee, 1990)
that could indicate potential for post-traumatic previously described a case of misdiagnosis in
stress disorder. A background interview covering which a psychologist diagnosed left hemisphere
early development, nuclear family, school experi- brain damage in a patient with an alleged MTBI
ences, occupational history, marital history, prior (no loss of consciousness or post-traumatic amne-
personal and family medical history, substance sia, normal neuroradiological findings) who had
abuse, and prior litigation and criminal history reduced right-hand motor functions, lower Verbal
must also be conducted. The data from this inter- IQ relative to Performance IQ, and poor verbal
view are then checked and validated against med- memory. The psychologist did not consider evi-
ical, school, work, and criminal records. dence of learning disability verified through school
Following completion of the interview, record records and the effects of peripheral injury to the
review, and collection of the neuropsychological right upper extremity (with evidence suggesting
test data, the consistency analysis is conducted. functional overlay). These neglected factors pro-
First, analysis of the consistency within and vided a much more compelling interpretation of
between domains should be conducted. Within the subject’s performance than the original diag-
domains, a person who performs poorly on the nosis of left hemisphere brain damage.
Working Memory Index of the Wechsler Adult The third consistency requirement is that level
Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV; of neuropsychological test performance should be
Wechsler, 2008) should not perform above aver- consistent with the severity of injury. This can be
age on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test considered as biological severity “indexing” or
(Gronwall, 1977); a person who performs poorly “referencing” (Larrabee, 1990, 1997, 2000b).
on Trail Making A should not perform normally Dikmen, Machamer, Winn, and Temkin (1995)
on Trail Making B; a person with very poor per- provided 1-year outcome data for varying degrees
formance on Finger Tapping should not have of head trauma severity, ranging from subjects
normal Grooved Pegboard Performance. Between who could follow a doctor’s commands within
domains, a subject with very poor attention one hour to persons who took longer than one
should not perform normally on memory tests; a month to follow a doctor’s commands following a
person with borderline scores on intelligence and TBI. Consequently, a litigant who was briefly
problem solving should not have superior memory unconscious at the scene of the accident, who
function. recalls transportation to the hospital, and who
Second, the neuropsychological test score pro- has a Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) of 15 (i.e., is
file should be consistent with established patterns oriented, follows commands, eyes open spontane-
for known disorders such as amnesia or dementia. ously), no focal neurological signs, and normal
In my experience, litigants with neuropsychologi- magnetic resonance imaging scan of the brain,
cal deficits typically do not present with test pat- should not perform on neuropsychological tests
terns that suggest focal neurobehavioral disorders at a level equivalent to that produced by subjects
such as aphasia or neglect; rather, litigants either who have suffered two weeks of coma following
present with a pattern of test results suggestive of their TBI.
amnesia (i.e., specific impairment in memory Rohling, Meyers, and Millis (2003) provided a
functions, with other neuropsychological func- statistical methodology, based on the Rohling
tions essentially preserved) or dementia (i.e., interpretive method of deriving an overall test
impairment in memory as well as in other neu- battery mean (OTBM; L. S. Miller & Rohling,
ropsychological functions, typically intelligence 2001), that allows for an analysis of neuropsycho-
and problem-solving skills). Patients with true logical data as a function of head injury severity.
amnestic disturbance do not perform poorly on The methodology proposed by Rohling et al.
the WAIS-IV (Wechsler, 2008) Digit Span, (2003) yields essentially identical results when
8 forensic neuropsychology

based on an expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery different places, circumstances, and times. The
(HRB; Reitan & Wolfson, 1993) such as that used example used by Hill was the increased incidence
by Dikmen et al. (1995), as well as when based on of cancer of the lung and nasal sinuses among
a battery employing standard measures of motor nickel refiners in South Wales, found by Hill as
function, attention, processing speed, verbal and well as by other investigators. When a change was
visual memory, and intellectual and problem- instituted in the refining process, not a single
solving skills (the Meyers Neuropsychological person working after the change was made devel-
Battery; Volbrecht, Meyers, & Kaster-Bundgaard, oped cancer of the nose. An example relevant to
2000). neuropsychology is the repeated observation that
Finally, test performance should be compared memory impairment and slowed information-
to other aspects of a subject’s behavior. A person processing speed are common long-term residual
who has good memory in the clinical interview, effects of severe closed head trauma.
demonstrated by accurate recall of doctors seen, Third is Hill’s (1965) factor of specificity; that
evaluations, and treatments and validated by is, the association between disease and environ-
cross-checking the actual medical records, should ment is only seen in specific subjects exposed to a
not perform in a significantly impaired fashion on specific environment. If the association between
formal memory testing. I previously described the environment and disease is limited to specific
two examples of this type of inconsistency workers and to particular sites and types of dis-
(Larrabee, 2000b). One case of alleged MTBI ease and there is no association between the work
accurately analyzed his current Wechsler Adult and other fatal illnesses, there clearly is a strong
Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, argument supporting causation. Hill’s principle of
1981) Digit Symbol performance as superior to specificity is perhaps the most difficult to satisfy
testing conducted two years earlier, showing evi- in neuropsychology, given the nonspecific nature
dence of excellent memory, yet the subject per- of symptomatic complaints (cf. Lees-Haley &
formed very poorly on all memory tests Brown, 1993) and the fact that deficits in atten-
administered by the author. Another subject with tion, memory, and executive functions can be
alleged MTBI performed on memory tests at a seen in a variety of neurobehavioral disorders.
level similar to that associated with Alzheimer’s The process of careful differential diagnosis
disease (AD), yet on the second day of examina- advocated in this chapter is particularly important
tion noticed that the clock had been removed because of the low specificity of these complaints
from the wall of the examining suite. and performance patterns.
Hill (1965) presented nine factors that should Hill’s (1965) fourth causal factor, temporality,
be considered before moving from the observa- refers to the temporal contingency between the
tion of an association between a particular envi- environmental factor and development of disease.
ronmental condition and a particular disease, to In one recent case of mine, memory complaints
inferring that the environmental condition is were present before the accident in question;
related to causation of that disease. Although the indeed, there was a closer temporal relationship
factors posited by Hill are particularly relevant to between the pre-accident memory complaints
inferring causation in neurotoxicology, certain of and the accident than there was for onset of
those factors are also related to other traumatic memory complaints following the accident.
CNS events. Fifth is Hill’s (1965) biological gradient or
The first of Hill’s (1965) factors is the strength dose–response curve. Of course, this is related to
of the association, which he illustrated by describ- Hill’s first principle of strength, as well as to my
ing the association between lung cancer and recommendation to analyze data, particularly for
smoking. In particular, the strength of association cases of closed head injury, in relation to the
was demonstrated by the dose–response relation- severity of initial trauma, referred to as biological
ship between number of cigarettes smoked indexing or referencing (Larrabee, 1990; also see
per day and increased incidence of lung cancer. Rohling et al., 2003). Bolla discusses the impor-
The paper by Rohling et al. (2003) provides an tance of analyzing dose–response when evaluat-
example of a dose-response relationship between ing neurotoxic injury in chapter 11, this volume.
severity of TBI and neuropsychological outcome. Hill’s (1965) sixth factor, plausibility, refers to
Hill’s (1965) second factor is the consistency of the biological plausibility of the purported causal
the association; that is, if the association has relationship. Bolla provides additional discussion
been repeatedly observed by different persons, in of biological plausibility in chapter 11. As she
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology 9

argues, if several animal studies show that the litigant must be considered, including the
a particularly high level of exposure to a specific possibility of invalid test performance.
chemical does not produce any health effects in
animals, then there is little reason to suspect that C L A S S I F I C AT I O N S TAT I S T I C S
health effects would be produced in humans at a IN NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL
lower level. DIAGNO SIS
The seventh causal factor posited by Hill Similar to clinical psychology research in general,
(1965) is that of coherence. Hill stated that the neuropsychological research has relied primarily
cause-and-effect interpretation of data should not on null hypothesis significance testing. Such
seriously conflict with the natural history and testing can yield statistically significant mean
biology of a particular disease or disorder. An differences between reference groups on a given
example of a failure to consider this principle is dependent variable that do not necessarily either
the interpretation of severe memory impairment reflect a clinically meaningful finding or provide
as indicative of sequelae of uncomplicated clinically useful information (Woods, Weinborn,
MTBI when this is not characteristic of the natu- & Lovejoy, 2003). In contrast, classification
ral history of MTBI (see chapter 9, this volume). accuracy statistics do provide information diag-
Hill’s (1965) eighth factor, experiment, allows nostically important in individual clinical use
for demonstration of a causal relationship (Baldessarini et al., 1983; Glaros & Kline, 1988;
between environment and disease by manipulat- Meehl & Rosen, 1955; Millis, 2009).
ing an environmental factor and then evaluating Traditional classification accuracy statistics
the results. For example, taking some preventive include sensitivity, specificity, hit rates, predictive
action that consequently lowers the incidence values, likelihood ratios, and odds ratios
of the disease, such as reducing dust in the work- (Baldessarini et al., 1983; Glaros & Kline, 1988;
place and finding a reduction in a particular dis- Ivnik et al., 2001; Woods et al., 2003). As defined
ease. Experiment or quasi-experiment can provide in this chapter, sensitivity refers to the proportion
the strongest evidence for the causation hypo- of patients with a given disorder who show a char-
thesis. Chapters 9 and 11 in this volume discuss acteristic of interest (i.e., an impaired neuropsy-
the importance of careful control of competing chological test score), defined as true positives/
variables in research on the outcome of MTBI and (true positives + false negatives). Specificity refers
neurotoxic injury, respectively. to the proportion of control subjects or some
Analogy is Hill’s (1965) ninth factor for dem- other reference sample without the characteristic
onstrating a causal association. As an example, of interest (i.e., who have nonimpaired neuropsy-
Hill noted that, given the known effects of thali- chological test scores), defined as true negatives/
domide and rubella on fetal development, doctors (true negatives + false positives). Sensitivity and
would be prepared to accept slighter but similar specificity serve different diagnostic purposes
evidence regarding effects of another drug or (Millis, 2009; Straus, Richardson, Glasziou, &
another viral disease on pregnancy. Haynes, 2005); that is, when a test cutoff score has
Hill (1965) pointed out that none of his nine a very high sensitivity, a negative test result rules
factors can bring indisputable evidence for or out the diagnosis, whereas conversely, when a test
against a cause–effect hypothesis, and none can be cutoff has a very high specificity, a positive test
required as a sine qua non. Moreover, formal tests result rules in the diagnosis. Straus et al. (2005)
of significance cannot provide the answers to the provide a useful mnemonic for the different
nine factors, although these statistical tests can impacts of sensitivity and specificity: With high
remind the investigator of the effects that chance sensitivity (Sn) a (N)egative test rules (out) the
can create; beyond that, statistics contribute noth- diagnosis, or SnNout; with high specificity (Sp), a
ing to the proof of the cause–effect hypothesis. (P)ositive test result rules (IN) the diagnosis, or
In summarizing Hill’s points as they relate to SpPin. Last, the hit rate index describes the total
neuropsychological decision making as well as proportion of accurately classified cases, (true
the consistency analysis I advocate, everything positives + true negatives)/N.
must make “neuropsychological sense” (Larrabee, It is not uncommon that when cutoff scores
1990; Stuss, 1995). When significant departures are derived to define sensitivity, specificity, and hit
from Hill’s principles are observed or inconsisten- rate, these cutoff scores are based on equal sample
cies appear in the neuropsychological data, diag- sizes of persons (a) with a given disorder, such as
noses other than the injury or illness alleged by TBI, and (b) those without the given disorder,
10 forensic neuropsychology

such as normal or nonneurological medical the LR is the more meaningful statistic for quanti-
orthopedic patient control subjects (cf. Dikmen fying the risk of having a COI after a patient’s test
et al., 1995). This essentially sets the base rate or score has been obtained (italics theirs). By contrast,
frequency of occurrence of the disorder at 50%, Ivnik et al. (2001) highlight that ORs are better for
which may not be the actual base rate in the total comparing the overall diagnostic usefulness of a
population (also see Baldessarini et al., 1983). test’s selected cut-score for group analysis.
Predictive value statistics do take into account the Otherwise stated, LR is preferred for diagnosis of
population base rate of the disorder in question. the individual patient, with ORs preferred for
Theoretically, sensitivity and specificity are inde- group comparisons.
pendent of the base rate or prevalence of illness in Obviously, sensitivity, specificity, LRs and ORs
the population tested (Baldessarini et al., 1983). are dependent on the setting of a particular cutoff
Positive predictive power (PPP; also referred to as score for the determination of abnormality. PPP
positive predictive value, PPV) is the ratio of and NPP are dependent on sensitivity, specificity,
true positive scores to total positive scores: true and prevalence of the condition of interest. The
positive/(true positive + false positive). PPP LR has a unique relationship to the prevalence
reflects the probability of the presence of a disease (base rate) of the COI in that premultiplying the
or disorder given a positive test finding. Negative LR for a particular test cutting score by the base
predictive power (NPP; also referred to as nega- rate odds yields the posttest odds of the COI,
tive predictive value, NPV) is the ratio of true which can be converted back to the posttest
negatives to total negative scores, true negatives/ probability of the disorder by the formula odds/
(true negatives + false negatives), and reflects the odds + 1 (Grimes & Schulz, 2005). For example, if
probability of the absence of a disease or disorder the base rate of malingering is 0.40, and a particu-
given a negative test finding. lar cutoff score for a symptom validity test (SVT)
Ivnik et al. (2001) observe that clinicians need has a sensitivity of 0.50 and specificity of 0.90, the
to quantify the probability that a patient does or base rate odds are 0.40/1–.40 or 0.67, which is
does not have a condition of interest (COI), so then used to multiply the LR, 0.50/.10 or 5.0, to
that the “risk” of having the COI specific to the yield posttest odds of 3.35, which can then be con-
obtained test results can be determined. Ivnik verted to a posttest probability of 0.77 (3.35/4.35).
et al. (2001, citing Fletcher, Fletcher & Wagner, If there is more than one diagnostic test for a COI,
1996) note that risk is expressed as “odds,” and and the diagnostic tests are independent of one
odds are the ratio of two probabilities. Odds that another, LRs can be chained such that the posttest
are associated with a single event are defined as odds following application of one test become the
the probability of the event occurring divided pretest odds by which the LR for the next test is
by the probability of the event not occurring. One multiplied. Continuing with the example above, if
of the risk statistics discussed by Ivnik et al. (2001) a second SVT independent of the first SVT but
is the likelihood ratio (LR), defined as the proba- with the same sensitivity of 0.50 and specificity of
bility of the obtained test result in the presence of 0.90 for the test score achieved by the examinee is
the condition of interest (COI) divided by the obtained, the LR again is 5.0 (0.50/.10) which is
probability of the obtained test result in the multiplied by the posttest odds of 3.35 from appli-
absence of the condition; in other words, the sen- cation of the first SVT, to now yield new posttest
sitivity divided by 1 minus specificity [i.e., (true odds of 16.75, for a posttest probability following
positive)/(false positive)]. Another way to quan- failure of two independent SVTs of 16.75/17.75 or
tify risk is the odds ratio (OR), which really is the 0.94 (see Larrabee, 2008 for additional examples
ratio of two risks: the risk of having a COI for all of the power of chaining of LRs).
people who earn a test score that indicates they Caution is urged when diagnostic indicators
should have the COI (risk 1) in comparison to the are not independent, which can lead to inflated
risk of having the COI for all people who earn a posttest probabilities for chained likelihood ratios
test score that indicates they should not have the (Grimes & Schulz, 2005). Logistic regression
COI (risk 2; Ivnik et al., 2001). Per Woods et al. employing multivariable models is preferred to
(2003) and Ivnik et al. (2001), the odds ratio is chaining of likelihood ratios for correlated indica-
computed as [(true positive)(true negative)]/ tors, since logistic regression accounts for variable
[(false positive)(false negative)]. Ivnik et al. (2001) intercorrelation, as well as allows for differential
note that since the reference group for LRs is weighting of the more salient diagnostic indica-
patients who have the same score as the patient, tors (Millis, 2009). Of course, logistic regression
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology 11

may not always be available for a unique combi- Berry (2007), displays a ROC for discrimination
nation of multiple SVTs. Research of mine of a group of definite/probable malingerers from a
(Larrabee, 2003a) and Victor, Boone, Serpa, group of patients with moderate/severe traumatic
Buehler, and Ziegler (2009), using cutoff scores brain injury with the MMPI-2 Symptom Validity
above/below which SVT failure is determined, Scale (FBS), using combined data from Larrabee
has shown that combinations of any two of five (2003b) and Ross, Millis, Krukowski, Putnam,
failed SVTs (Larrabee, 2003a) or any two of four and Adams (2004). The AUC of 0.96 for the ROC
failed SVTs (Victor et al., 2009) yields essentially in Figure 1.1 shows outstanding discrimination of
the same diagnostic discrimination as when the the definite/probable malingerers and the moder-
individual SVTs are considered altogether as ate/severe TBI cases.
continuous variables in a logistic regression. Baldessarini et al. (1983) provided the formula
By plotting true-positive rates and false- for computing PPP and NPP. Sensitivity is repre-
positive rates for all possible cutoff scores on a sented as x; specificity is represented as y; preva-
particular test, the receiver operating characteris- lence (base rate) is represented as p.
tic (ROC; also known as the relative operating
characteristic) of the test can be determined PPP = [(p)(x)]/[(p)(x) + (1−p)(1−y)]
(Hsiao, Bartko, & Potter, 1989; Swets, 1973). The
area under the ROC generated by the different NPP = [(1−p)(y)]/[(1−p)(y) + (p)(1−x)]
cutoff scores and their associated true-positive
and false-positive rates gives the overall diagnos- Baldessarini et al. (1983) provided an example
tic efficiency of the test, with an upper limit of 1.0 demonstrating the effect of prevalence/base rate
(perfect diagnostic accuracy) and a lower limit of on PPP and NPP from a study employing the dex-
0.50 (chance). Hosmer and Lemeshow (2000) amethasone suppression test as an indicator of the
present guidelines for interpreting the magnitude presence of major depression. In the original
of ROC area under curve (AUC): values of 0.90 or study sample, 100 depressed patients were com-
more reflect outstanding discrimination, 0.80 to pared to 100 neurotic patients, creating a preva-
<0.90 show excellent discrimination, 0.70 to <0.80 lence/base rate of depression of 50%. Of the 100
show acceptable discrimination, and 0.50 shows depressed patients, 70 were identified as depressed
no discrimination. Figure 1.1, from Larrabee and (true positives), and 30 were misidentified (false
negatives), giving a sensitivity of 70. Of the 100
neurotic patients, 5 tested positive for depression
1.0 (false positives), whereas 95 tested negative for
depression, yielding a specificity of 95%. In the
original derivation sample, the PPP was 70 (true
0.8 positive)/(70 true positive + 5 false positive),
which equals 70/75 or 93.3%. Similarly, derived
via the formula, [(0.5)(0.7)]/[(0.5)(0.7) + (1 − 0.5)
Sensitivity

0.6
(1 − 0.95)], PPP is 93.3%. NPP is (95 true nega-
tive)/(95 true negative + 30 false negative), which
0.4 equals 95/125 or 76.0%. Also, derived via the for-
mula, NPP is [(1 − 0.5)(0.95)]/[(1 − 0.5)(0.95) +
(0.5)(1 − 0.7)] = 0.76 or 76%.
0.2 Baldessarini et al. (1983) demonstrated the
effect of change in prevalence/base rate of depres-
sion on PPP and NPP for the test of depression.
0.0
0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0
Moving from the derivation study in which the
base rate of major depressive disorder was 50% to a
1-Specificity
Diagonal segments are produced by ties.
general psychiatric practice in which the base rate
of major depressive disorder was 10%, but keeping
FIGURE 1.1: Receiver operating characteristic for sensitivity at 70% and specificity at 95%, reduces
discrimination of definite and probable malingerers PPP to 12.3%, but increases NPP to 99.7%.
from patients with moderate to severe traumatic Baldessarini et al. (1983) described a recipro-
brain injury using the FBS of the MMPI-2. From cal relationship between PPP and NPP as a func-
Larrabee and Berry (2007) with permission. tion of prevalence/base rate, with sensitivity and
12 forensic neuropsychology

specificity held constant. At low prevalence/base Heaton, Grant, & Matthews, 1991, data, and an
rates, a negative test result is more likely to be true Average Impairment Rating cut score of T score =
than a positive result. This results in a loss of PPP 39), PPP for neuropsychological detection of per-
moving from an artificial setting in test popula- sisting impairment following MTBI was 0.26,
tions with a high prevalence of an illness or condi- with NPP at 0.99, at the base rate of 5%. Thus, a
tion of interest into more realistic clinical settings. clinician is more likely to be correct when diag-
The converse is true of NPP as a function of prev- nosing no persisting impairment in MTBI.
alence/base rate, with sensitivity and specificity I (Larrabee, 2003a) developed a five-variable
held constant. Thus, at high prevalence/base rates, algorithm for detection of malingering based on
a positive test result is more likely to be true than atypical (for moderate or severe TBI) performance
a negative test result. on any two (or three) variables, including Benton
When prevalence/base rate is held constant, Visual Form Discrimination (Benton, Sivan,
PPP is affected more by changes in specificity; Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, 1994), Finger
that is, when the rate of false positives is low. Tapping, Reliable Digit Span (Greiffenstein, Baker,
Conversely, NPP is highest when sensitivity is & Gola, 1994), Wisconsin Card Sorting Failure to
high; that is, when the rate of false negatives is low Maintain Set, and the Lees-Haley Fake Bad Scale
(Baldessarini et al., 1983). When multiple, inde- (Lees-Haley, English, & Glenn, 1991; now known
pendent tests sensitive to a COI are positive, the as the Symptom Validity Scale; Ben Porath,
posttest probabilities increase significantly, so that Graham & Tellegen, 2009). Based on a positive
even with low base-rate conditions, diagnostic score (i.e., in the malingering range) on any two of
probability for the COI can be high. For example, the five indicators, sensitivity for detection of def-
I (Larrabee, 2008) showed, using the assumption inite or probable malingered neurocognitive dys-
of independent SVTs, each with a sensitivity of function (Slick, Sherman, & Iverson, 1999) was
0.50 and specificity of 0.90, that failure of one SVT 0.878 and specificity (for correct detection of
at a base rate of 0.10 yielded a posttest probability moderate/severe TBI, neurological or psychiatric
of 0.56, which increased to 0.74 with failure of two patients) was 0.944. Using a base rate of mali-
SVTs, and to 0.93 with failure of three SVTs. ngering of 40% in litigated minor head injury
The overall accuracy of a test cutting score is a (Larrabee, 2003a; Mittenberg, Patton, Canyock, &
function of the positive and negative base rates in Condit, 2002), PPP for this formula was 0.913,
the population as well as the positive and false- with NPP of 0.921. When a positive score was
positive rates associated with the cutting score obtained on any three of the five indicators, sensi-
(Meehl & Rosen, 1955). The ratio of the positive tivity dropped to 0.542; however, specificity was a
base rate P to the negative base rate Q must be perfect 1.00. Consequently, PPP for failure of any
greater than the ratio of the false-positive rate p2 three indicators was 1.00, since the simplified for-
to the true-positive rate p1, or P/Q > p2/p1. Stated mula for PPP (true positive/true positive + false
otherwise (Gouvier, 1999), the combined error positive) results in 0.542/.542 (with zero false
rate of a test must be smaller than the base rate of positives), yielding 100% probability of malinger-
the condition that the test is designed to detect. ing. This latter finding underscores the important
As reviewed by Woods et al. (2003), classifica- relationship of specificity to PPP.
tion statistics, particularly those related to PPP,
NPP, and odds ratios, are underused in neuropsy- S C I E N T I F I C S TAT U S O F
chological research. Indeed, in their review of five NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL
prominent neuropsychology journals published TESTING
during the years 2000 and 2001, only 31% of neu- This section provides a brief overview of general
ropsychology articles published indices of sensi- issues related to the scientific status of neuropsy-
tivity, with fewer than 3% reporting predictive chological test procedures. This is by no means an
values or risk ratios. exhaustive review, and the reader is directed to
Two studies in which I have been involved more comprehensive reviews, including the
provide a further perspective on PPP and NPP. In widely referenced texts by Lezak, Howieson, and
the first, Binder, Rohling, and Larrabee (1997) Loring (2004) and Strauss, Sherman, and Spreen
determined 5% prevalence of chronic persisting (2006).
neuropsychological deficit following MTBI. The discipline of neuropsychology in America
Assuming sensitivity of 80% and specificity of dates to the 1940s and 1950s with the establish-
88% for neuropsychological tests (using the ment of laboratories by Arthur Benton, Ward
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology 13

Halstead, and Ralph Reitan (Reitan & Wolfson, Some have stated that certain fixed battery
2009; Tranel, 2009). During this same time period, neuropsychological test procedures such as the
laboratories were formed by Zangwill in England HRB or Luria Nebraska Neuropsychological
and Hécaen in France, and Luria had initiated Battery (LNNB; Golden, Purisch, & Hammeke,
his investigations in Russia (Soviet Union) 1985) are the only acceptably validated proce-
(Hécaen & Albert, 1978; Luria, 1966; McFie, dures for use in forensic neuropsychology (Hom,
1975). As currently practiced, neuropsychology 2003; Hom, 2008; McKinzey, 2000; Russell, 2007).
has benefited from psychometric influences (e.g., In contrast, a survey by Lees-Haley, Smith,
principles of test construction, methodology for Williams, and Dunn (1996) found, in a review of
determining reliability and validity), develop- 100 forensic neuropsychological evaluations con-
ments in experimental psychology (e.g., signal ducted in 20 states and in the province of Ontario,
detection methodology; information-processing Canada, that the HRB and LNNB were rarely
models), and behavioral neurology (e.g. neuro- used. Lees-Haley et al. found that only 10% used
behavioral analysis of syndromes of aphasia, the LNNB, and approximately 21% used the HRB,
apraxia, agnosia and amnesia; Larrabee & Crook, contrasted with 75% that used the WAIS-R, 68%
1988; Lezak et al., 2004; Milberg, Hebben, & that used the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
Kaplan, 2009). Inventory (MMPI; Hathaway & McKinley, 1983)/
Psychological and neuropsychological testing MMPI-2 (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen,
has established validity, comparable to the validity & Kaemmer, 1989), and 51% that used the WMS
of tests used in clinical medicine (Meyer et al., (Wechsler, 1945) and WMS-R. These percentages
2001). A meta-analytic review by Zakzanis, Leach, make it difficult to argue that fixed batteries are
and Kaplan (1999) demonstrated varying patterns the standard of practice in forensic settings.
of effect sizes for measures of verbal and perfor- Subsequent surveys further established the
mance skill, attention, memory, cognitive flexibil- primacy of approaches other than fixed batteries
ity/abstraction, and manual dexterity as a function such as the HRB or LNNB. Sweet, Nelson and
of different neurobehavioral disorders such as Moberg (2006) surveyed practicing neuropsy-
AD, frontotemporal dementia, Parkinson’s dis- chologists regarding their basic assessment
ease, multiple sclerosis, Huntington’s disease, and approaches and found that 76% used a flexible
as a function of psychiatric disorders such as battery, defined as variable but routine groups of
schizophrenia and major depressive disorder. tests for different types of patients (e.g., head
Neuropsychological testing of adults is accepted injury, alcoholism, elderly patients), whereas 18%
by neurologists as “established,” with Class II used a purely flexible approach dependant upon
evidence and Type A recommendation, and was the needs of an individual case but not uniform
found by the Therapeutics and Technology across patients, and only 7% used a standardized
Assessment Subcommittee of the American battery such as the HRB. A contemporaneous but
Academy of Neurology (AAN) to be appropriate independent survey by Rabin et al. (2005) found
for a wide range of neurological disorders, that most neuropsychologists frequently assessed
including TBI, cerebrovascular disease, Parkinson’s attention, verbal memory, executive functions,
disease, HIV encephalopathy, multiple sclerosis, visuospatial skills, nonverbal memory, intelli-
epilepsy, and neurotoxic exposure (“Assessment: gence, language, and construction. The most
Neuropsychological Testing of Adults,” 1996). In widely used procedures included the Wechsler
particular, the AAN views neuropsychological intelligence scales (WAIS-R/WAIS-III; 63.1%),
documentation as critical in cases where litigation and Wechsler Memory Scales (WMS-R/Wechsler
concerns the presence of cognitive impairment Memory Scale-Third Edition or WMS-III;
(“Assessment: Neuropsychological Testing,” Wechsler, 1997; 42.7%), with the Trail Making
1996). Although some have questioned certain Test used by 17.6%, and California Verbal
conclusions reached by the Technology and Learning Test (CVLT; Delis, Kramer, Kaplan,
Therapeutics Subcommittee (Reynolds, 2001), Ober, & Fridlund, 1987; CVLT-2; Delis et al.,
“These criticisms aside, the AAN report does 2000) used by 17.3%. Consistent with the Sweet
make a positive contribution to the field of neu- et al. (2006) survey, only 15.5% used the HRB.
ropsychology and concludes by recommending Recommendations for the use of the HRB or
that neuropsychological assessment of adults has LNNB over other neuropsychological procedures
value to neurologists in particular circumstances” are not supported by empirical studies that have
(p. 200). compared both batteries to the WAIS (Wechsler,
14 forensic neuropsychology

1955), WAIS-R, or Auditory Verbal Learning Test hours to 6 days of coma (with coma defined by
(Rey, 1964). Based on WAIS IQ scores and global time to follow commands).
impairment scores for the HRB and LNNB, Kane, Rohling et al. (2003) found that a flexible
Parsons, and Goldstein (1985) found essentially battery, employing a core set of tests of language,
equivalent classification of brain damage versus spatial judgment, motor and tactile function,
control subjects. Reanalysis of the Kane et al. verbal and visual learning and memory, and
(1985) published data, computing effect sizes intelligence and problem solving, showed the
(Cohen’s d) showed d values of 1.63 for an HRB same dose–response association with head injury
average T score and 1.88 for the HRB Average severity found by Dikmen et al. (1995), who used
Impairment Rating, comparable to a d of 1.71 for an HRB augmented by the WAIS and memory
WAIS PIQ (Loring &Larrabee, 2006). These HRB procedures such as the Verbal Selective Reminding
and PIQ effect sizes were all inferior to the effect test. What was particularly impressive is that the
size of 2.40 for WAIS VIQ (Loring & Larrabee, Overall Test Battery Mean (OTBM) average
2008). Sherer, Scott, Parsons, and Adams (1994) T score for the Meyers Neuropsychological Battery
also found that the WAIS-R and HRB were both (a core for a flexible battery) was 39.2, essentially
sensitive to the presence of brain damage. When identical to the expanded HRB OTBM of 38.9;
Cohen’s d was computed for the Sherer et al. additionally, the MNB correlated 0.99 with five
(1994) data, d was 0.92 for WAIS-R FIQ, a value levels of TBI severity (<1 hour time to follow
over double the d of 0.43 computed for the HRB commands [TFC], 1–23 hours TFC, 1–6 days
(Loring & Larrabee, 2006). TFC, 7–13 days TFC, and 14–28 days TFC),
Chelune (1982) attributed the equivalent whereas the HRB OTBM correlated 0.96 with
sensitivity to brain damage of the HRB and these same levels of severity, with no difference in
LNNB to the variance these procedures share slope or intercept from the MNB data. Last, the
with WAIS IQ. The equivalent sensitivity of the two OTBMs correlated 0.97 with one another
WAIS/WAIS-R and HRB for brain damage is across the five levels of trauma severity. These
further understood as a function of shared essentially identical values support not only the
underlying (neuro)psychological constructs of equivalent validity of a core flexible battery to an
(a) visuospatial reasoning and problem solving, expanded HRB, but equally as important, estab-
(b) attention/concentration, and (c) psychomotor lish the comparability of a collection of individu-
speed, as demonstrated in a factor analysis of ally normed tests (the tests comprising the MNB)
the HRB, WAIS-R, and WMS-R (Larrabee, to a co-normed battery of tests comprising the
2000a; Leonberger, Nicks, Larrabee, & Goldfader, Dikmen et al. (1995) expanded HRB.
1992). My colleagues and I (Larrabee, Millis, &
Memory is only weakly assessed on the origi- Meyers, 2008) compared the performance of a
nal LNNB (Larrabee, Kane, Schuck, & Francis, mixed group of patients with neurological dys-
1985) and is not assessed at all on the HRB function to that of a group of pseudoneurologic
(Leonberger et al., 1992). This explains why controls on the seven core measures of the HRB
Powell, Cripe, and Dodrill (1991) found that the (Category Test; TPT Total Time, Memory and
Auditory Verbal Learning Test, particularly Trial Location; Finger Tapping; Seashore Rhythm Test;
5, was more sensitive than any other single test on Speech Sounds Perception Test) and the core of a
the HRB, as well as more sensitive than the HRB flexible battery, which we named an Ability
Impairment Index, in discriminating normal Focused Battery (AFB). The AFB was designed to
subjects from a group of subjects with a variety of capture several core neuropsychological con-
neurological disorders. structs/domains/abilities including language/
Dikmen et al. (1995), in their study of long- verbal skills, perceptual/spatial skills, sensorimo-
term sequence of TBI, found that the two most tor ability, working memory, processing speed,
sensitive tests (i.e., the first tests to pick up differ- verbal and visual memory, and intellectual and
ences in performance 1-year status post-injury, as problem-solving skills, and included eight tests
a function of trauma severity) were the Verbal (H-Words, a written word fluency test; Grooved
Selective Reminding Test and Trail Making B for Pegboard; WMS Delayed Logical Memory and
the head-injured subjects who took between 1 Delayed Visual Reproduction; WAIS-R
and 24 hours to follow commands. The HRB Arithmetic, Digit Symbol, Similarities, and Block
Impairment Index did not show differences in Design). Discrimination of the neurologic and
1-year outcome until TBI severity reached 25 control groups with the AFB yielded a receiver
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology 15

operating characteristic area under curve (AUC) psychomotor speed, verbal and visual learning
of 0.86, compared to the HRB AUC of 0.83. While and memory, intellectual and problem-
this difference was not statistically significant, the solving skills, academic functions, personality,
Bayes Information Criterion (BIC) favored the and motivation (American Academy of Clinical
AFB over the HRB. We noted that the AFB would Neuropsychology, 2007; “Assessment: Neuro-
have yielded even more impressive discrimina- psychological Testing of Adults,” 1996; Larrabee,
tion if more sensitive measures of verbal and 2000a; Lezak et al., 2004; Strauss et al., 2006;
visual learning and memory, and more sensitive Zakzanis et al., 1999). A battery lacking assess-
measures of working memory, had been employed. ment of certain functions such as memory and
Bayesian Model Averaging selected four measures verbal abilities should be augmented by tests
from the combined set of AFB and HRB subtests, covering the missing areas, as many who use the
plus Trail Making B: H-Words, Trail Making B, HRB have done (Dikmen et al., 1995; Heaton
Grooved Pegboard, and Finger Tapping (note: et al., 1991; Powell et al., 1991).
The effect size for Finger Tapping was quite small, Multiple measures are recommended for
and it appeared Finger Tapping was working as a each of the above areas of function; for example,
measure of symptom validity, in that the brain- Finger Tapping, Grip Strength (Reitan & Wolfson,
impaired subjects outperformed the controls, 1993), and the Grooved Pegboard can be used to
despite performing significantly less well than assess manual motor function. This recommenda-
the controls on the Grooved Pegboard Test). tion goes counter to Wedding and Faust’s (1989)
The largest effect size for the individual subtests recommendation to avoid reliance on highly
was d = 1.08 for the Grooved Pegboard Test. intercorrelated measures. Wedding and Faust
A subsequent investigation by Miller, argued that, to the extent tests are redundant,
Fichtenberg, and Millis (2010) found that an AFB using a second test merely remeasures what the
measuring five domains: attention, processing first test does, and diagnostic or predictive
speed, visual-spatial reasoning, language/verbal accuracy is minimally increased. This observation
reasoning, and memory discriminated a mixed is certainly true if and only if the clinician’s sole
neurological group from a pseudoneurologic concern was the reliability and validity of test
control group, with a receiver operating charac- scores. However, this is not the case in clinical and
teristic AUC of 0.89. Moreover, a more parsimo- forensic neuropsychology, for which the reliabil-
nious model based solely on processing speed and ity and validity of the individual participant’s
memory yielded an AUC of 0.90. These values performance is also at issue (see chapter 5 on
show excellent diagnostic discrimination and malingering). Thus, inclusion of multiple mea-
also demonstrate the superior sensitivity to brain sures of each domain, some easier, some more
dysfunction of measures of memory and proce- difficult, allows analysis of within-domain consis-
ssing speed. These data support the results of tency of performance.
Larrabee et al. (2008) who also found that mea- As an example employing multiple measures
sures of processing speed were very sensitive to of motor function, Greiffenstein, Baker, and
brain dysfunction, and confirm our prediction Gola (1996) studied patients with severe TBI who
that more sensitive measures of memory, such as also had unambiguous motor abnormalities on
the CVLT-II used by Miller et al. show better standard neurological examination and found
sensitivity to neurologic dysfunction than the these subjects had different patterns of motor
Logical Memory and Visual Reproduction tests performance on Finger Tapping, Grip Strength,
used in our earlier study. and the Grooved Pegboard test, in comparison to
The investigations by Rohling et al. (2003), the motor performance patterns of litigants
Larrabee, et al. (2008), and Miller et al. (2010) with persistent post-concussion syndrome. The
show that what is important in conducting patients with severe TBI performed less well on
neuropsychological evaluation is that the exami- the Grooved Pegboard relative to Finger Tapping
nation, using standardized and validated tests, and Grip Strength, whereas the litigants with
should cover the key areas of neuropsychological persistent post-concussion syndrome showed
function. These core neuropsychological func- the opposite pattern, performing better on the
tions include language/verbal abilities, percep- Grooved Pegboard in comparison to Finger
tual/spatial ability, sensorimotor function Tapping and Grip Strength.
(particularly manual motor function), attention, Some have argued for predicting expected
working memory and information processing, levels of neuropsychological test performance
16 forensic neuropsychology

based on estimated premorbid IQ (Tremont, the OPIE ignores regression to the mean effects
Hoffman, Scott, & Adams, 1998). This approach and contaminates the predictor (Vocabulary and
is only applicable if there is a correlation of Picture Completion subtests) with the criterion
a particular neuropsychological test with IQ; (IQ; i.e., the Vocabulary and Picture Completion
even then, there can be considerable regression to subtests are used to predict IQ, which is also based
the mean, particularly if there is no preexisting partly on the Vocabulary and Picture Completion
IQ test, and IQ itself must be estimated (Larrabee, subtests).
2000a). For example, Tremont et al. (1998) found Finally, performance on WAIS-R subtests such
that the Category Test correlated 0.5 with Full as Vocabulary or Picture Completion that are
Scale IQ (FIQ). In someone for whom preinjury thought to be less sensitive to the effects of brain
testing was unavailable but who actually had a damage can still be affected by brain dysfunction.
“true” premorbid IQ of 130, the estimated pre- Specifically, Larrabee, Largen, and Levin (1984)
morbid IQ based on a multiple R of 0.60 (Barona, found that using performance on hold or best-
Reynolds, & Chastain, 1984) would be 118 (1.2 performance WAIS subtests to estimate premor-
SD above the mean). If the expected Category Test bid IQ underestimated premorbid level of function
score was then estimated on the basis of this esti- by a full standard deviation or more for subjects
mated IQ, the predicted Category Test score would who had mild-to-moderate AD compared to
be 0.5 × 1.2 or 0.6 SD above the mean, for a T-score WAIS IQ scores produced by normal controls
equivalent of 56 (standard error of estimate of matched on age, education, and gender. Although
8.66) or an IQ-equivalent score of 109 (standard coworkers and I (Larrabee et al., 1984) did not
error of estimate of 12.99). Estimated premorbid employ Vocabulary in our investigation, the
IQ scores themselves have large standard errors of Information subtest mean for the AD patients was
estimate (SEEs) when based on demographic fac- 3.0 age-scaled score points lower than that of the
tors (Barona et al., 1984; range of SEE is 12 to 13 age-, education-, and gender-matched controls,
IQ points). The size of the SEE can be reduced with the Picture Completion subtest mean falling
using current function measures such as the 5.44 age-scaled score points lower for the AD
Advanced Clinical Solutions Test of Premorbid patients.
Functioning (TOPF; Pearson, 2009), in combina- The point of the discussion is that estimation
tion with demographic factors, but the TOPF of premorbid neuropsychological ability is a risky
itself must be estimated based on demographic enterprise. In my opinion, the clinician is better
factors to ensure the score has not been affected off using neuropsychological test procedures that
by brain dysfunction (Pearson, 2009). Although have been carefully standardized on the basis of
the TOPF can be useful for estimating premorbid relevant demographic factors such as age, educa-
IQ, it is not as useful for predicting premorbid tion, and gender (cf. Heaton et al., 1991, but note
memory function (Pearson, 2009). the concerns of Fastenau, 1998). By evaluating
Some have advocated using performance on and adjusting for demographic factors related
current IQ subtests that are resistant to effects of to test performance during test norming and
cerebral dysfunction (i.e., “hold” tests) or picking development, essentially the test procedures are
the highest subtest scores on current IQ testing precorrected for premorbid level of function.
(the “best performance method”; see Lezak et al., The recently published Advanced Clinical
2004) to estimate premorbid IQ. Krull, Scott, and Solutions also provide demographically corrected
Sherer (1995) and Scott, Krull, Williamson, norms for both the WAIS-IV and Wechsler
Adams, and Iverson (1997) have developed a pro- Memory Scale, Fourth Edition (WMS-IV;
cedure for predicting premorbid IQ that combines Wechsler, 2009). To date, these demographically
demographic information with current perfor- corrected norms for WAIS-IV and WMS-IV have
mance on the Vocabulary and Picture Completion not been compared directly to premorbid predic-
subtests of the WAIS-R, the Oklahoma Premorbid tions of ability using the TOPF, in relation to diag-
Intelligence Estimate (OPIE). Powell, Brossart, nostic sensitivity to acquired neurological deficits
and Reynolds (2003) compared the OPIE proce- (e.g. AD; TBI). Thus, the addition of a current
dure to Barona et al.’s (1984) demographic estima- function estimator to demographic factors may
tion of premorbid IQ in both normal and clinical provide a superior benchmark for detection of
patients and found that the OPIE was less sensi- impairment, for the particular case of the WAIS-IV
tive (i.e., predicted lower premorbid IQ) in the Index Scores, although this remains to be demon-
clinical sample. This is not unexpected because strated. Last, review of preinjury standardized
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology 17

test scores is also helpful for establishing the level conclusion of deficit, when the pattern of perfor-
of preinjury function, but provides information mance is actually the result of nonpathological
typically related to academic achievement and less normal variability or random factors.
often to intellectual level of function, although the Binder, Iverson, and Brooks (2009) have
two are definitely correlated. comprehensively reviewed the subject of fre-
As discussed in this chapter, the actual quency of “abnormal” scores in healthy adults.
neuropsychological data must be interpreted in They concluded that abnormal performance on
the context of other data using appropriate some proportion of tests in a neuropsychological
scientific judgment and reasoning. Use of a large battery is psychometrically normal (italics theirs).
number of tests can result in abnormal scores in Binder et al. reviewed several normative data-
entirely normal individuals. Ingraham and Aiken bases, reporting on the frequency of abnormal
(1996) presented an empirical approach to deter- scores using various definitions of abnormality
mine abnormality in test batteries with multiple (bottom 16%, bottom 5%). For example, using a
measures; for example, in a battery with 30 scores cutoff of >1 standard deviation below the mean,
in which the criterion is three tests falling at 1 SD a battery of 40 scores from an expanded HRB
below the mean, the probability is nearly 0.90 that (Heaton et al., 1991) showed that 90% of the
this will occur by chance. Note that Ingraham and normative sample had at least one score that
Aiken based their computations on independent exceeded this cutoff, with a median of 4 scores out
scores, using the binomial theorem, which is of 40 that exceeded the cutoff. Lowering the cutoff
likely not the case in an actual test battery. to >1.5 standard deviations, 64% still had at least
Ingraham and Aiken note that since correlated one score exceeding the cutoff. On the Neuro-
scores reduce the likelihood of abnormal findings, psychological Assessment Battery (NAB; Stern &
the probabilities generated by the binomial theo- White, 2003), at a cutoff of more than 1 standard
rem are likely somewhat overestimated. Indeed, deviation below the mean, 36.9% of the normative
Crawford, Garthwaite and Gault (2007) found sample had an abnormality on at least one of the
that using the binomial theorem with uncorre- five index scores, and 19.3% had an abnormality
lated data yielded the same percentage of abnor- on at least two index scores.
mal scores as did a procedure based on a Monte Binder et al. (2009) comment on several
Carlo method utilizing the standard deviation of clinical implications of their review. One is the
the difference between individuals’ mean scores limitation placed on estimation of premorbid
on the battery and each of the subtests or indexes mental abilities based on current cognitive
contributing to the mean. When the size of the performance (also see earlier comments in this
subtest intercorrelations increased, the binomial section). Specifically, a few scores much higher
procedure overestimated the number of abnormal than other scores do not mean that premorbid
scores; for example, if a battery consisted of six mental abilities were at the same level as the
tests that are uncorrelated, the percentage of highest scores. Also, Binder et al. (2009) noted
tests that are abnormally low (<5th percentile) is that published data demonstrated large discrep-
26.49%, but when the average subtest inter- ancies occur fairly commonly between IQ, index,
correlation is 0.7, only 14.87% demonstrate and subtest scores of the WAIS-R, WMS-R, WAIS-
abnormally low scores. III, and WMS-III, but that no researcher or test
Determination of abnormalities in perfor- publisher has provided information regarding the
mance based on test score variability depends on base rates of large discrepancy scores when all
the range of variability in normal subjects, which discrepancies are considered simultaneously, as is
can be quite large. Schretlen, Munro, Anthony, typical of the practitioner evaluating data on an
and Pearlson (2003) studied the maximum range individual case (italics theirs). Regarding the
between a normal subject’s highest and lowest forensic implications of their review, they observe
scores on 32 z-transformed scores derived from that a full appreciation of the extent of the base
15 neuropsychological tests. The smallest maxi- rates of low scores and large discrepancies in
mum difference was 1.6 SD, with the largest max- normal persons should decrease the frequency of
imum difference 6.1 SD. Again, knowledge of the misdiagnosis of cognitive impairment in forensic
literature pertaining to patterns of performance in evaluations. Consequently, the forensic practitio-
different neurobehavioral disorders and use of the ner should guard against over-interpreting
judgment strategies recommended in this chapter isolated low scores, adopting a more scientific
can reduce the risk of making an erroneous approach to evaluation of test results. Binder and
18 forensic neuropsychology

colleagues close their review by quoting Schretlen litigants/defendants, and the ethics of appropriate
et al. (2003): “the findings reported here under- forensic practice are addressed in chapter 4.
score the importance of basing clinical neuropsy- Chapter 18 provides a perspective on past and
chological inferences about cerebral dysfunction future trends in forensic neuropsychology.
on clinically recognizable patterns of performance
in the context of other historical, behavioral, AC K N OW L E D G M E N T
and diagnostic information, rather than on psy- I acknowledge the assistance of Matthew Miliano
chometric variability alone.” (p. 869, italics added in the preparation of this chapter.
by Binder et al.).
REFERENCES
S U M M A RY American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology.
This chapter reviewed scientific factors relevant (2007). American Academy of Clinical
to the practice of forensic neuropsychology. The Neuropsychology (AACN): Practice guidelines for
scientific method was reviewed, including the neuropsychological assessment and consultation.
hypothetico-deductive approach, with particular The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21, 209–31.
emphasis on the importance of seeking discon- Army Individual Test Battery. (1944). Manual of
firming evidence to strengthen both scientific and directions and scoring. Washington, DC: War
Department. Adjutant General’s Office.
diagnostic hypotheses. Factors related to diagnos-
Assessment: Neuropsychological testing of adults.
tic error were reviewed, including faulty use of
(1996) Considerations for neurologists. Report of
logic, illusory correlation, hindsight bias, and con-
the Therapeutics and Technology Assessment
firmation bias. Recommendations for effective
Subcommittee of the American Academy of
diagnostic formulations aimed at reducing judg- Neurology. Neurology, 47, 592–99.
mental bias were made, and statistics relevant to Badia, P. & Runyon, R. P. (1982). Fundamentals of behav-
diagnostic decision making were reviewed, includ- ioral research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
ing discussion of base rates, sensitivity, specificity, Baldessarini, R. J., Finklestein, S., & Arana, G. W.
and positive and negative predictive power. Finally, (1983). The predictive power of diagnostic tests
general issues relevant to neuropsychological and the effect of prevalence of illness. Archives of
testing in forensic settings were considered. General Psychiatry, 40, 569–73.
The remainder of this book expands on the Barona, A., Reynolds, C. R., & Chastain, R. (1984).
science of forensic neuropsychology. All of the A demographically based index of premorbid
contributors are actively involved in empirical intelligence for the WAIS-R. Journal of Consulting
research in neuropsychology and are respected and Clinical Psychology, 52, 885–87.
and well-known scientist/clinicians. The cumula- Ben Porath, Y. S., Graham, J. R., & Tellegen, Y. S. (2009).
tive aspect of science is represented by chapters The MMPI-2 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
that present up-to-date reviews of major areas of Inventory-2 Symptom Validity (FBS) Scale. Develop-
concern, including assessment of malingering ment, research findings, and interpretive recommen-
(chapter 5), functional neuroimaging (chapter 6), dations. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota
acquired brain damage in children (chapters Press.
7, 8), MTBI (chapter 9), moderate-to-severe TBI Benton, A. L., Sivan, A. B., Hamsher, K. deS., Varney,
N. R., & Spreen, O. (1994). Contributions to neu-
(chapter 10), neurotoxic injury (chapter 11),
ropsychological assessment. A clinical manual
chronic pain (chapter 12), and post-traumatic
(2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
stress disorder (chapter 14).
Binder, L. M. (1997). A review of mild head trauma.
The special circumstances in which the scien- Part II: Clinical implications. Journal of Clinical
tist/clinician must effectively communicate results and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 432–57.
to attorneys and the court are covered in chapter Binder, L. M., Iverson, G. L., & Brooks, B. L. (2009).
2, with admissibility of neuropsychological evi- To err is human: “Abnormal” neuropsychological
dence covered in chapter 3. Issues pertinent to scores and variability are common in healthy
civil and criminal competencies and criminal adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24,
responsibilities are addressed in chapters 15–17. 31–46.
In particular, chapter 15 demonstrates a cutting- Binder, L. M., Rohling, M. L., & Larrabee, G. J. (1997).
edge scientific approach to the assessment of com- A review of mild head trauma. Part I: Meta-analytic
petencies in the elderly. Chapter 13 provides a review of neuropsychological studies. Journal of
perspective on somatoform stress disorders that is Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19,
important in performing differential diagnosis of 421–31.
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology 19

Buschke, H. (1973). Selective reminding for analysis of Glaros, A. G. & Kline, R. B. (1988). Understanding the
memory and learning. Journal of Verbal Learning accuracy of tests with cutting scores: The sensitiv-
and Verbal Behavior, 12, 543–50. ity, specificity, and predictive value model. Journal
Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R., of Clinical Psychology, 44, 1013–23.
Tellegen, A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989). Minnesota Golden, C. J., Purisch, A. D., & Hammeke, T. A. (1985).
Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2). Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery: Forms I
Manual for administration and scoring. Minneapolis: and II. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
University of Minnesota Press. Gouvier, W. D. (1999). Base rates and clinical decision
Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J. P. (1967). Genesis of making in clinical neuropsychology. In J. J. Sweet
popular but erroneous psychodiagnostic observa- (Ed.), Forensic neuropsychology. Fundamentals and
tions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 193–204. practice (pp. 27–37). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets
Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J. P. (1969). Illusory cor- and Zeitlinger.
relation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodi- Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Gola, T. (1994).
agnostic signs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 74, Validation of malingered amnesia measures with a
271–80. large clinical sample. Psychological Assessment, 6,
Chelune, G. J. (1982). A reexamination of the relation- 218–24.
ship between the Luria-Nebraska and Halstead- Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Gola, T. (1996).
Reitan Batteries: Overlap with the WAIS. Journal of Motor dysfunction profiles in traumatic brain
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 578–80. injury and post-concussion syndrome. Journal
Crawford, J. R., Garthwaite, P. H., & Gault, C. B. (2007). of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2,
Estimating the percentage of the population with 477–85.
abnormally low scores (or abnormally large score Grimes, D. A. & Schulz, K. F. (2005). Epidemiology 3.
differences) on standardized neuropsychological Refining clinical diagnosis with likelihood ratios.
test batteries: A generic method with applications. Lancet, 365, 1500–1505.
Neuropsychology, 21, 419–30. Gronwall, D. M. A. (1977). Paced auditory serial addi-
Daubert v. Merrell Dow, 509 U.S. 579 (1993). tion task: A measure of recovery from concussion.
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., Ober, B. A., & Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 367–73.
Fridlund, A. (1987). The California Verbal Learning Hannay, H. J. & Levin, H. S. (1985). Selective Reminding
Test. New York: Psychological Corporation. Test: An examination of the equivalence of four
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. forms. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
(2000). CVLT-II. California Verbal Learning Test. Neuropsychology, 7, 251–63.
Second edition. Adult version. San Antonio, TX: Hathaway, S. R. & McKinley, J. C. (1983). The Minnesota
The Psychological Corporation. Multiphasic Personality Inventory manual. New
Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., Winn, H. R., & Temkin, York: The Psychological Corporation.
N. R. (1995). Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., &
post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 80–90. Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Fastenau, P. S. (1998). Validity of regression-based manual. Revised and expanded. Odessa, FL:
norms: An empirical test of the comprehensive Psychological Assessment Resources.
norms with older adults. Journal of Clinical and Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1991).
Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 906–916. Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-
Faust, D. (1989). Data integration in legal evaluations: Reitan Battery: Demographic corrections, research
Can clinicians deliver on their premises? Behavioral findings, and clinical applications. Odessa, FL:
Sciences and the Law, 7, 469–83. Psychological Assessment Resources.
Faust, D. & Nurcombe, B. (1989). Improving the Hécaen, H. & Albert, M. L. (1978). Human neuro-
accuracy of clinical judgement. Psychiatry, 52, psychology. New York: Wiley.
197–208. Hempel, C. G. (1966). Philosophy of natural science.
Faust, D., Ziskin, J., & Hiers, J. B. (1991). Brain damage Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
claims: Coping with neuropsychological evidence Hill, A. B. (1965). The environment and disease.
(Vol. 1). Los Angeles: Law and Psychology Press. Association and causation. Proceedings of the Royal
Feynman, R. P. (with R. Leighton). (1985). Surely you’re Society of Medicine, 58, 295–300.
joking, Mr. Feynman: Adventures of a curious char- Hom, J. (2003). Forensic neuropsychology: Are we
acter. New York: Norton. there yet? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18,
Fletcher, R. H., Fletcher, S. W., & Wagner, E. H. (1996). 827–45.
Diagnosis. Clinical epidemiology: The essentials (pp. Hom, J. (2008). Commentary. Response to Bigler
42–75). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. (2007): The sky is not falling. Archives of Clinical
Frye v. United States (D.C. Cir. 1923) 293 F. 1013. Neuropsychology, 23, 125–28.
20 forensic neuropsychology

Hosmer, D. & Lemeshow, S. (2000). Applied logistic Larrabee, G. J. (2008). Aggregation across multiple
regression, 2nd ed. New York: Wiley Interscience. indicators improves the detection of malingering:
Hsiao, J. K., Bartko, J. J., & Potter, W. Z. (1989). Relationship to likelihood ratios. The Clinical
Diagnosing diagnoses. Receiver operating charac- Neuropsychologist, 22, 666–79.
teristic methods and psychiatry. Archives of General Larrabee, G. J. & Berry, D. T. R. (2007). Diagnostic
Psychiatry, 46, 664–67. classification statistics and diagnostic validity of
Ingraham, L. J. & Aiken, C. B. (1996). An empirical malingering assessment. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.).
approach to determining criteria for abnormality Assessment of malingered neuropsychological
in test batteries with multiple measures. deficits (pp. 14–26). New York: Oxford University
Neuropsychology, 10, 120–24. Press.
Ivnik, R. J., Smith, G. E., Cerhan, J. H., Boeve, B. F., Larrabee, G. J. & Crook, T. H. (1988). Assessment of
Tangalos, E. G., & Peterson, R. C. (2001). drug effects in age-related memory disorders:
Understanding the diagnostic capabilities of cogni- Clinical, theoretical, and psychometric consider-
tive tests. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 114–24. ations. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24, 515–22.
Kane, R. L., Parsons, O. A., & Goldstein, G. (1985). Larrabee, G. J., Kane, R. L., Schuck, J. R., & Francis,
Statistical relationships and discriminative accu- D. E. (1985). The construct validity of various
racy of the Halstead-Reitan, Luria-Nebraska, and memory testing procedures. Journal of Clinical and
Wechsler IQ scores in the identification of brain Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 239–50.
damage. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Larrabee, G. J., Largen, J. W., & Levin, H. S. (1984).
Neuropsychology, 7, 211–33. Sensitivity of age-decline resistant (“hold”) WAIS
Kerlinger, F. N. (1973). Foundations of behavioral subtests to Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical
research (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 497–504.
Winston. Larrabee, G. J., Millis, S. R.,& Meyers, J. E. (2008).
Krull, K. R., Scott, J. G., & Sherer, M. (1995). Estimation Sensitivity to brain dysfunction of the Halstead-
of premorbid intelligence from combined perfor- Reitan vs. an ability-focused neuropsychological
mance and demographic variables. The Clinical battery. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22, 813–25.
Neuropsychologist, 9, 83–88. Larrabee, G. J., Trahan, D. E., & Levin, H. S. (2000).
Kurtz, R. M. & Garfield, S. L. (1978). Illusory correla- Normative data for a six-trial administration of the
tion: A further exploration of Chapman’s para- Verbal Selective Reminding Test. The Clinical
digm. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Neuropsychologist, 14, 110–118.
46, 1009–1015. Lees-Haley, P. R. & Brown, R. (1993). Neuropsycholo-
Larrabee, G. J. (1990). Cautions in the use of neuro- gical complaint base rates of 170 personal injury
psychological evaluation in legal settings. Neuro- claimants. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8,
psychology, 4, 239–47. 203–209.
Larrabee, G. J. (1992). Interpretive strategies for evalu- Lees-Haley, P. R., English, L. T., & Glenn, W. J. (1991).
ation of neuropsychological data in legal settings. A fake bad scale on the MMPI-2 for personal injury
Forensic Reports, 5, 257–64. claimants. Psychological Reports, 68, 203–210.
Larrabee, G. J. (1997). Neuropsychological outcome, Lees-Haley, P. R., Smith, H. H., Williams, C. W., &
post concussion symptoms, and forensic consider- Dunn, J. T. (1996). Forensic neuropsychological
ations in mild closed head trauma. Seminars in test usage: An empirical survey. Archives of Clinical
Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 2, 196–206. Neuropsychology, 11, 45–51.
Larrabee, G. J. (2000a). Association between IQ and Leonberger, F. T., Nicks, S. D., Larrabee, G. J., &
neuropsychological test performance: Commentary Goldfader, P. R. (1992). Factor structure of the
on Tremont, Hoffman, Scott, and Adams (1998). Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised within a compre-
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 14, 139–45. hensive neuropsychological battery. Neuropsy-
Larrabee, G. J. (2000b). Forensic neuropsychological chology, 6, 239–49.
assessment. In R. D. Vanderploeg (Ed.), Clinician’s Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004).
guide to neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed., Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York:
pp. 301–35). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Oxford University Press.
Larrabee, G. J. (2003a). Detection of malingering Lielienfeld, S. O. (in press). Can psychology become a
using atypical performance patterns on standard science? Personality and Individual Differences
neuropsychological tests. The Clinical Neuro- (2010), doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.024
psychologist, 17, 410–25. Lilienfeld, S. O., Lynn, S. J., & Lohr, J. M. (2003). Science
Larrabee, G. J. (2003b). Detection of symptom exag- and pseudoscience in clinical psychology. In S. O.
geration with the MMPI-2 in litigants with malin- Lilienfeld, S. J. Lynn, & J. M. Lohr (Eds.), Science
gered neurocognitive dysfunction. The Clinical and pseudo-science in clinical psychology (pp. 1–14).
Neuropsychologist, 17, 54–68. New York: Guilford Press.
A Scientific Approach to Forensic Neuropsychology 21

Loring, D. W. & Larrabee, G. J. (2006). Sensitivity of the Pearson (2009). Advanced Clinical Solutions for
Halstead and Wechsler test batteries to brain WAIS-IV and WMS-IV. Administration and
damage: Evidence from Reitan’s original validation scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: Author.
sample. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 221–29. Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery.
Loring, D. W. & Larrabee, G. J. (2008). “Psychometric New York: Basic Books.
Intelligence” is not equivalent to “Crystallized Powell, B. D., Brossart, D. F., & Reynolds, C. R. (2003).
Intelligence,” nor is it insensitive to brain damage: Evaluation of the accuracy of two regression-based
A reply to Russell. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, methods for estimating premorbid IQ. Archives of
22, 524–28. Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, 277–92.
Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. Powell, J. B., Cripe, L. I., & Dodrill, C. B. (1991).
New York: Basic Books. Assessment of brain impairment with the Rey
McFie, J. (1975). Assessment of organic intellectual Auditory Verbal Learning Test: A comparison with
impairment. New York: Academic Press. other neuropsychological measures. Archives of
McKinzey, R. K. (2000). A research update: Neuro- Clinical Neuropsychology, 6, 241–49.
psychological assessment. For the Defense, 23, Rabin, L. A., Barr, W. B., & Burton, L. A. (2005).
62–63. Assessment practices of clinical neuropsycholo-
Meehl, P. E. (1978). Theoretical risks or tabular aster- gists in the United States and Canada: A survey of
isks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the slow progress of INS, NAN, and APA Division 40 members. Archives
soft psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 33–65.
Psychology, 46, 816–34. Reitan, R. M. & Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead-
Meehl, P. E. & Rosen, A. (1955). Antecedent probability Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Theory and
and the efficiency of psychometric signs, patterns, or clinical interpretation (2nd ed.). South Tucson, AZ:
cutting scores. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 194–216. Neuropsychology Press.
Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Moreland, K. L., Reitan, R. M. & Wolfson, D. (2009). The Halstead-
Dies, R. R., Eisman, E. J., et al. (2001). Psychological Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery for adults-
testing and psychological assessment. A review of Theoretical, methodological, and validational bases.
evidence and issues. American Psychologist, 56, In I. Grant & K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological
128–65. assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders (3rd ed.,
Milberg, W. P., Hebben, N., & Kaplan, E. (2009). The pp. 3–24). New York: Oxford University Press.
Boston Process Approach to neuropsychological Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie [The
assessment. In I. Grant & K. M. Adams (Eds.), clinical examination in psychology]. Paris: Presses
Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric Universitaires de France.
disorders (3rd ed., pp. 42–65). New York: Oxford Reynolds, C. R. (2001). Commentary on the American
University Press. Academy of Neurology report on neuropsycho-
Miller, E. (1983). A note on the interpretation of data logical assessment. Archives of Clinical Neuro-
derived from neuropsychological tests. Cortex, 19, psychology, 16, 199–200.
131–32. Rohling, M. L., Meyers, J. E., & Millis, S. R. (2003).
Miller, J. B., Fichtenberg, N. L., & Millis, S. R. (2010). Neuropsychological impairment following trau-
Diagnostic efficiency of an ability-focused battery. matic brain injury: A dose–response analysis. The
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 24, 678–88. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 289–302.
Miller, L. S. & Rohling, M. L. (2001). A statistical inter- Ross, S. R., Millis, S. R., Krukowski, R. A., Putnam,
pretive method for neuropsychological test data. S. H., & Adams, K. M. (2004). Detecting incomplete
Neuropsychology Review, 11, 143–69. effort on the MMPI-2: An examination of the Fake
Millis, S. R. (2009). What clinicians really need to know Bad Scale in mild head injury. Journal of Clinical
about symptom exaggeration, insufficient effort, and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 115–24.
and malingering: Statistical and measurement Russell, E. W. (1990, June). Twenty ways of diagnosing
matters. In J. E. Morgan & J. J. Sweet (Eds.). Neuro- brain damage when there is none. Paper presented
psychology of malingering casebook (pp. 21–37). at the meeting of the Florida Psychological Associ-
New York: Psychology Press. ation, St. Petersburg Beach, FL.
Mittenberg, W., Azrin, R., Millsaps, C., & Heilbronner, Russell, E. W. (2007). Commentary on “A motion to
R. (1993). Identification of malingered head injury exclude and the ‘fixed’ versus ‘flexible’ battery in
on the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Psycholo- ‘forensic’ neuropsychology.” Archives of Clinical
gical Assessment, 5, 34–40. Neuropsychology, 22, 787–90.
Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit, Russell, E. W. & Kolitz-Russell, S. L. (2003). Twenty
D. C. (2002). Base rates of malingering and ways and more of diagnosing brain damage when
symptom exaggeration. Journal of Clinical and there is none. Journal of Controversial Medical
Experimental Neuro-psychology, 24, 1094–1102. Claims, 10, 1–14.
22 forensic neuropsychology

Schretlen, D. J., Munro, C. A., Anthony, J. C., & Tremont, G., Hoffman, R. G., Scott, J. G., & Adams, R.
Pearlson, G. D. (2003). Examining the range of L. (1998). Effect of intellectual level on neuropsy-
normal intraindividual variability in neuropsycho- chological test performance: A response to Dodrill
logical test performance. Journal of the Internatio- (1997). The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 560–67.
nal Neuropsychological Society, 9, 864–70. Trope, Y., Gervey, B., & Liberman, N. (1997). Wishful
Scott, J. G., Krull, K. R., Williamson, D. J. G., Adams, thinking from a pragmatic hypothesis-testing per-
R. L., & Iverson, G. L. (1997). Oklahoma Premorbid spective. In M. S. Myslobodsky (Ed.), The mythom-
Intelligence Estimation (OPIE): Utilization in anias: The nature of deception and self-deception
clinical samples. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, (pp. 105–31). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
11, 146–54. Victor, T. L., Boone, K. B., Serpa, J. G., Buehler, J., &
Sherer, M., Scott, J. G., Parsons, O. A., & Adams, R. L. Ziegler, E. A. (2009). Interpreting the meaning of
(1994). Relative sensitivity of the WAIS-R subtests multiple symptom validity test failure. The Clinical
and selected HRNB measures to the effects of brain Neuropsychologist, 23, 297–313.
damage. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9, Volbrecht, M. E., Meyers, J. E., & Kaster-Bundgaard, J.
427–36. (2000). Neuropsychological outcome of head
Slick, D. J., Sherman, E. M. S., & Iverson, G. L. (1999). injury using a short battery. Archives of Clinical
Diagnostic criteria for malingered neurocognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 251–65.
dysfunction: Proposed standards for clinical prac- Wechsler, D. (1945). A standardized memory scale for
tice and research. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, clinical use. Journal of Psychology, 19, 87–95.
13, 545–61. Wechsler, D. (1955). WAIS manual. New York: The
Stern, R. A. & White, T. (2003). Neuropsychological Psychological Corporation.
Assessment Battery. Lutz, FL: Psychological Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R manual. New York: The
Assessment Resources. Psychological Corporation.
Straus, S. E., Richardson, W. S., Glasziou, P., & Haynes, Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised
R. B. (2005). Evidence-based medicine: How to manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
practice and teach EBM (3rd ed.). New York: Corporation.
Elsevier Churchill Livingstone. Wechsler, D. (1997). WMS-III. Administration and
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
A compendium of neuropsychological tests (3rd ed.). Corporation.
New York: Oxford. Wechsler, D. (2008). WAIS-IV Administration and
Stuss, D. T. (1995). A sensible approach to mild trau- scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson.
matic brain injury. Neurology, 45, 1251–52. Wechsler, D. (2009). WMS-IV. Administration and
Sweet, J. J., Nelson, N. W., & Moberg, P. J. (2006). The scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson.
TCN/AACN 2005 “Salary Survey”: Professional Wedding, D. & Faust, D. (1989). Clinical judgment and
practices, beliefs, and incomes of U. S. neuropsychol- decision making in neuropsychology. Archives of
ogists. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 325–64. Clinical Neuropsychology, 4, 233–65.
Swets, J. A. (1973). The relative operating characteristic Woods, S. P., Weinborn, M., & Lovejoy, D. W. (2003).
in psychology. Science, 182, 990–1000. Are classification accuracy statistics underused in
Tranel, D. (2009). The Iowa-Benton School of neurop- neuropsychological research? Journal of Clinical
sychological assessment. In I. Grant & K. M. Adams and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 431–39.
(Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsy- Zakzanis, K. K., Leach, L., & Kaplan, E. (1999).
chiatric disorders (3rd ed., pp. 66–83). New York: Neuropsychological differential diagnosis. Lisse, The
Oxford University Press. Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.
2
Neuropsychology and the Law
Principles of Productive Attorney-Neuropsychologist Relations

M A N F R E D F. G R E I F F E N S T E I N A N D PAU L M . K AU F M A N N

INTRODUCTION The purpose of this chapter is to outline,


Forensic neuropsychology is defined as the understand, and manage the important compo-
application of neuropsychological evidence to nents of the attorney-neuropsychologist interac-
inform legal questions. There is empirical evi- tion. The reader should be mindful of the
dence that forensic neuropsychology has grown terminology used repeatedly in this chapter. The
from the occasional private practice referral, to a term “forensic neuropsychologist” (FN) simply
major component of assessment and research. means any neuropsychologist who provides
In a comprehensive survey, Sweet, Moberg, and opinions in a legal setting, irrespective of the
Suchy (2000) reported that legal referrals had frequency of this work. The term does not denote
become a major source of private practitioner a practitioner who restricts their practice to foren-
income. Published research addressing empirical sic work. To achieve practice content neutrality,
questions raised by forensic involvement has also the authors strive to extract general principles by
grown steadily. Sweet, King, Malina, Bergman, focusing on elements of professional conduct
and Simmons (2002) compiled a decade of common to both civil plaintiff and defense, and
abstracts from three major neuropsychology jour- government and criminal defense experts. The
nals and reported the number of articles address- terms “retaining” and “opposing,” “examining” and
ing forensic issues more than tripled between “cross-examining” attorneys are used descriptively,
1990 and 2000. And the courts have taken and avoid a value judgment. There are some issues
notice: Kaufmann (2008) reported an average 6% unique to plaintiffs or defense experts. In these
increase/year in published legal cases referencing matters, the terms “plaintiff ” and “defense” will be
neuropsychology during the past five years. used for accuracy’s sake, but they are still applied
Given the proven growth of legal applications without prejudice.
for neuropsychology, it logically follows that neu- We further strive for neutrality on assessment
ropsychologists have increased personal interac- orientation (process/flexible approach, fixed
tions with agents of the legal system. These agents battery, flexible battery), legal context (civil, crim-
include attorneys, judges, hearing officers, and inal, probate, administrative, informal dispute
police, but attorney contacts account for most of resolution), or practice pattern (mostly plaintiff or
these interactions. For this reason, the nature of the defense, fulltime versus occasional forensics).
attorney-neuropsychologist interaction merits Wherever possible, we stress universal aspects
considerable discussion. These interactions can binding all approaches to neuropsychological
have many positive consequences, including testing. The terms “neuropsychological tests,”
enhanced income, inter-professional understand- “neurocognitive methods,” and similar wording
ing, and research opportunities. There can also be are used to denote any procedure relying on
negative consequences, including close public scru- quantitative or qualitative analysis to arrive at
tiny of a neuropsychologist’s cherished methods conclusions about cognitive and perceptual-motor
and fundamental beliefs. Attorney coaching to sab- capacities.
otage assessments is also a risk, as this documented However, our test content neutrality is tem-
practice violates a psychologist’s crucial assump- pered by the existence of generally accepted,
tion of a naïve test taker (Youngjohn, 1995). mainstream neuropsychology practices. The test
24 forensic neuropsychology

selection examples offered are of the flexible Temple, 2008). Indeed, every published legal case
battery approach, as opposed to uniform admin- addressing the validity of a flexible battery has
istration of the same tests to all patients (fixed determined that fixed batteries offer no advantage
battery) and the pure flexible approach (individu- for forensic consulting in civil (Minner v. American
alized test selection for every patient). The flexible Mortg. & Guar., 2000; Chapple v. Granger, 1994)
battery is defined as a core set of tests given to the and criminal proceedings (People v. Sebastianelli,
same class of patients (e.g., dementia or closed 1998).
head injury battery), augmented by additional Irrespective of what test grouping the reader
measures to inform additional questions raised uses, we recognize that even the soundest neurop-
by a case (Barr, 2007; Bauer, 1994). The flexible sychological tests should never be interpreted
battery approach is a sound one because it is blindly in isolation from a context or important
modeled on the medical practice of a core exami- extratest data. This is not a new idea. Matarazzo
nation (e.g., history and neurological examina- and Herman (1985) discussed approaches to
tion) with additional tests ordered to discriminate interpreting Verbal—Performance differences on
between competing hypotheses (e.g., ordering Wechsler IQ tests, and provided a principle that
EEG, CT/MRI). There is no such thing as a “fixed serves as a useful generalization to any clinical or
medical battery.” Even proponents of the so-called forensic endeavor. They note:
fixed battery approach often add measures not
considered or recommended by the battery’s A [VIQ versus PIQ)] difference score is
creator, for example, adding memory tests to merely the initial datum that should stimulate
overcome the deficiencies of the Halstead-Reitan the clinician to search for corroborating,
Battery (Russell & Starkey, 1993), and replacing extra-test evidence from the clinical or social
the Wechsler-Bellevue with updated intelligence history that such a difference is associated
tests (Russell, 1994, 2004). Hence, even fixed bat- with a potentially significant diagnostic finding
tery proponents are actually practicing flexible (p. 928).
battery administration.
Practice pattern surveys show the flexible Replacing “VIQ-PIQ difference scores” with
battery approach is not only a recurrent majority the term “an abnormal neurocognitive score”
preference (Lees-Haley, Smith, Williams & Dunn, provides the important maxim of reliance on a
1996), it is gaining an increasingly greater number multi-method approach to interpretation. The FN
of adherents (Sweet, Moberg & Westergaard, relies on converging lines of evidence to support
1996; Sweet & Moberg, 2000). As shown by the conclusions proffered in court, not on test scores
most recent 2005 The Clinical Neuropsychologist in isolation (see Larrabee’s introductory chapter
survey, (Sweet, Nelson, & Moberg, 2006), the on scientific approaches, this volume). In sum-
flexible battery enjoys broad sponsorship in the mary, your work is not done after you have scored
neuropsychology community, as 76% of clinicians and profiled the tests.
reported reliance on that approach. In contrast,
only 7% relied on a fixed battery, a striking 61% BASIC PRINCIPLES OF
decline in sponsorship since 1989. A preliminary N E U R O P S Y C H O L O G I S T-
result from the most recent survey notes that only AT T O R N E Y I N T E R A C T I O N S
5% of the 1,600 respondents use a fixed battery We promote five basic principles of productive
(Sweet, 2010). Additionally, the flexible battery neuropsychologist-attorney interactions. These
approach has withstood challenges brought in principles are interwoven with each phase of the
litigation (Bigler, 2007), a point conceded by some attorney-neuropsychologist interaction and for
critics of flexible approaches (McKinzey & the remainder of this chapter; we will apply them
Ziegler, 1999). Because neuropsychology’s diag- to these phases although some principles have
nostic reasoning mirrors that of clinical medicine, more weight than others do in a particular phase.
and because fixed medical batteries do not exist, These principles are:
there is no good clinical reason for a fixed cogni-
tive battery.1 M. Greiffenstein made this argument • Understand legal bases
as co-author of an amicus brief submitted to the • Practice competent neuropsychology
New Hampshire Supreme Court, a case in which • Support board certification
that court reversed a trial court and reinstituted • Adhere to ethical principles
an expert’s flexible battery evidence (Baxter v. • Be courtroom familiar.
Neuropsychology and the Law 25

Understanding legal bases means the FN conclusions about causation of current neurocog-
develops a working knowledge of key law and nitive performance. Although empiricism is
legal practices. This is not just a competence issue; important in justifying methodology and conclu-
it is a legal and ethical issue. Being reasonably sions, there will not be a published study for every
familiar with the law is a legal requirement question that arises in legal disputes. Each legal
imposed by states before granting permission case will have unique aspects that can only be
and license to practice psychology, and recent addressed by clinical judgment, that is, extrapola-
publications have emphasized the importance of tion and logic. When confronted with a 58-year-
knowledge and competence in law relevant to old school teacher and asked whether she can
neuropsychological practice (Howe, Sweet, & return to work one year after a mild concussion,
Bauer, 2010; Kaufmann, 2008, 2009). Even the do not bother to look for studies entitled
ethics code advises “when assuming forensic roles, “Prediction of work status in mildly head injured
psychologists are or become reasonably familiar teachers.” FN must always exercise sound judg-
with the judicial or administrative rules govern- ment in generalizing from the data and literature
ing their roles” (Standard 2.01f) and all states to a specific claimant’s (or defendant’s) behavior.
require psychologists to pass a test on laws regu- Even that champion of actuarial approaches, Paul
lating the profession. But preparation for forensic Meehl, recognized the need to use case-specific
neuropsychology does not necessarily require a data to modify conclusions in the absence of the
deep or broad understanding of legal principles. perfect on-point study (Meehl, 1954). The logical
It does require knowing the most applicable path through the data must be made transparent.
evidentiary law, landmark legal cases relevant to The U.S. Supreme Court decision in Joiner requires
psychology, and the basic civil rights of plaintiffs trial courts to separately evaluate the expert’s rea-
and defendants. Even the most elementary foren- soning process that links observation and litera-
sic psychology texts provide a sufficient under- ture to conclusions.
standing of the law and courtroom procedures to The principle of supporting board certification
engage in forensic consulting (Greiffenstein, 2008; in neuropsychology should not be a controversial
Melton, Petrila, Poythress, Slobogin, & Otto, 2007; principle to readers. Our intention is not to pro-
Andrew, Benjamin, & Kazniak, 1991; Glass, 1991). voke or diminish noncertified specialists. We
However, understanding legal bases overlaps with mean only that peer review of one’s work product
appreciating legal culture. The upcoming section is an important means of proving competence in
on conflicting agendas is devoted exclusively to neuropsychology, and certification through a rec-
legal culture. ognized certifying body is acceptable proof of
One author (P. Kaufmann) frequently uses the competence. Professional standards matter. We
phrase practicing competent neuropsychology in do not mean that only psychologists certified by
his legal orientation workshops for neuropsychol- the American Board of Clinical Neuropsychology
ogists. The principle simply means the FN (a specialty board under the umbrella of ABPP)
embraces the scientist-practitioner role by com- should be allowed to step into court, or that such
bining the body of scientific knowledge with psychologist’s opinions be given more weight.
sound clinical judgment while conducting legal However, board certification does help to prove
work involving predictions about individuals. The competence in a legal vacuum and courts rou-
scientific component of the FN’s work requires tinely accept it as some evidence of competence;
placing the legal question and assessment meth- for example, footnoting the 13 member boards of
ods in the context of peer-reviewed scientific lit- the American Board of Professional Psychology
erature. This principle is very general, as it must (Tarver v. State, 2005). Many state licensing laws
recognize there are a number of approaches to only protect the title of psychologist, so there are
neuropsychological testing. Most important is the few safeguards against psychologists who adver-
FN’s ability to defend the chosen method on tise neuropsychology services without the requi-
empirical and logical grounds. For example, con- site training or coursework. Certification allows
sideration of the well-established dose-response consumers to better distinguish between choices
relationship between head injury severity in states that allow liberal self-definition of
and neuropsychological outcome (Dikmen, specialty services.2 As neuropsychologists are ever
Machamer, Winn, & Temkin, 1995; Rohling, more frequently called upon to render expert
Meyers, & Millis, 2003; Volbrecht, Meyers, & opinions, lawyers are increasingly examining the
Kaster-Bundgaard, 2000) is relied upon to justify background, training, and experience of experts.
26 forensic neuropsychology

Ethical questions are more likely to be raised of forensic experience, while understanding legal
by forensic work than in any other setting. First, bases is a precondition for starting forensic work.
more ethical principles are invoked because of the After the section on clashing worldviews, the
larger number of parties involved and the remainder of the chapter describes the four main
protracted nature of litigation. Second, opposing phases of attorney-neuropsychologist interac-
parties may attempt to intimidate witnesses or tions: The pre-assessment, assessment, trial, and
influence opinions by prematurely filing ethical post-trial phases. The five basic principles of
complaints for the purpose of undermining the productive interactions are then applied to each
credibility of an opposing expert.3 Third, ethical phase where appropriate.
issues are embedded in every phase of the forensic
assessment process (Bush, 2007; Bush, Grote, CONFLICTS BETWEEN
Johnson-Greene, & Macartney-Filgate, 2008). The S C I E N T I S T- P R A C T I T I O N E R
FN should be aware of the unique ethical issues AND LEGAL MODELS
associated with each phase of attorney-neuropsy- The most difficult adjustment is to the inevitable
chology interactions. Courtroom experience offers conflict between neuropsychological and legal
constant and subtle pressures to deviate from methods. The law and science are two clashing
sound practices, for example, the pull of advocacy. epistemologies (Greiffenstein, 2008). The FN must
The very nature of the adversarial system poses the recognize and manage these basic conflicts in
following unique challenge to the neuropsycholo- order to develop a productive relationship with
gist: Legitimate courtroom practices and ethical the attorney and the trier of fact. The three
attorney behavior would be considered unethical fundamental conflicts encountered by the FN are
if practiced by a neuropsychologist. (a) conflicting agendas, (b) conflicting methods,
The difficulty lies in recognizing ethical and (c) conflicting relationships.
dilemmas and the appropriate coping response.
Even the most ethically scrupulous neuropsy- Conflicting Agendas
chologist must be prepared to cope with unfair The most basic and pervasive conflict is objectivity
ethical charges raised by either advocates pretend- versus zealous advocacy. In a typical clinical
ing to be neuropsychology experts, or unsavory setting, neuropsychologists pursue a goal of
attorneys. Recognizing the burden of behaving describing a person’s perceptual-motor, cognitive,
ethically is made more difficult by ambiguity in and personality characteristics in an objective and
published ethical guidelines. In cases of ambigu- reasonably certain way. All final conclusions are at
ity, the FN must make an honest attempt to distill least partially grounded in scientific findings and
the gist or “spirit” of the code in ways that are not logical inferences tightly linked to facts. In con-
self-serving (Shapiro, 2000). The reader may trast, criminal and civil proceedings in America
cross-reference Christopher Grote’s chapter in the take place in an adversarial setting. Attorneys are
current volume for a broader and in-depth advocates. Zealous advocacy is required and tem-
exploration of ethical issues. pered only by the ambiguous “candor before the
The principle of being courtroom familiar tribunal” principle. Consequently, winning and
means the FN is able to recognize courtroom losing cases are more important than accuracy
dynamics, trial procedures, interpersonal transac- and objectivity, and justice is more important
tions, and personalities (Brodsky, 1991). There are than truth. Justice provides the opportunity for
many indicia of “courtroom familiarity” (a term truth to be heard, but it does not guarantee that
credited to Brodsky & Robey, 1973). These may the truth shall prevail. The attorney’s goal is to
include knowledge about the presiding judge, present a strong case for their client while simul-
knowing the opposing attorney, suppressing the taneously minimizing and directly challenging
“star witness” mentality, minimal emotional reac- the opposing attorney’s arguments. Attorneys are
tions to aggressive subpoenas, and easy conces- not allowed to lie, even as they strike a balance
sion of minor points during cross-examination. between competing ethical imperatives in the
This principle is similar to the understanding of ABA Model Rules of Professional Conduct in
legal bases principle, but we kept it separate Table 2.1. Balancing rules of professional conduct
because developing courtroom familiarity requires is both the obligation and the prerogative of an
“insider” knowledge, some of it arcane and spe- attorney. That is, lawyers exercise professional
cific to a locale or jurisdiction. Courtroom famil- discretion in how they balance the competing
iarity also differs because it represents an outcome requirements found in these two rules. Attorneys
Neuropsychology and the Law 27

TABLE 2.1 ABA MODEL RULES OF PROFESSIONAL CONDUCT


Relevant Excerpts

Rule 1.3 Diligence – A lawyer shall act with reasonable diligence and promptness in representing a client.
Comment [1] A lawyer must also act with commitment and dedication to the interests of the client and with zeal
in advocacy upon the client’s behalf. A lawyer is not bound, however, to press for every advantage that might be
realized for a client. For example, a lawyer may have authority to exercise professional discretion in determining
the means by which a matter should be pursued. [emphasis added]

Rule 3.3 Candor Toward The Tribunal (a) A lawyer shall not knowingly:

(1) make a false statement of fact or law to a tribunal or fail to correct a false statement of material fact or law
previously made to the tribunal by the lawyer;
(2) fail to disclose to the tribunal legal authority in the controlling jurisdiction known to the lawyer to be directly
adverse to the position of the client and not disclosed by opposing counsel; or

(3) offer evidence that the lawyer knows to be false. If a lawyer, the lawyer’s client, or a witness called by the lawyer,
has offered material evidence and the lawyer comes to know of its falsity, the lawyer shall take reasonable
remedial measures, including, if necessary, disclosure to the tribunal. A lawyer may refuse to offer evidence,
other than the testimony of a defendant in a criminal matter, that the lawyer reasonably believes is false.
Comment [2] This Rule sets forth the special duties of lawyers as officers of the court to avoid conduct that
undermines the integrity of the adjudicative process. A lawyer acting as an advocate in an adjudicative proceeding
has an obligation to present the client’s case with persuasive force. Performance of that duty while maintaining
confidences of the client, however, is qualified by the advocate’s duty of candor to the tribunal. Consequently,
although a lawyer in an adversary proceeding is not required to present an impartial exposition of the law or
to vouch for the evidence submitted in a cause, the lawyer must not allow the tribunal to be misled by false
statements of law or fact or evidence that the lawyer knows to be false. [emphasis added].

exercise professional discretion in how they An accurate, comprehensive, and balanced report
engage in zealous advocacy for their clients. may be harmful to an attorney’s case. Because
However, zealous advocacy must be tempered most FNs would be alarmed at any colleague
with candor before the tribunal. A failure to writing a one-sided report that minimizes incon-
exercise candor during legal proceedings is venient findings and highlights only minor
a violation of professional responsibility. ones, she should realize that it is considered
Failure to acknowledge this basic law-psychol- ethical and fair for an attorney to present his
ogy conflict will ensure a poor working relation- case in just such a partisan form. An attorney’s
ship and quickly sour an interest in forensic work. acceptance or rejection of his FN’s report may
For example, the novice FN becomes dismayed make sense in the context of this adversarial
when the retaining attorney invokes the “work system.
product rule” and insists she not even write a In rare circumstances, the court may retain
report. She concludes that the retaining attorney the FN as an expert. Federal Rule of Evidence
is dishonest or unappreciative of her well- (FRE; 1975, 2003) 706 allows a court to authorize
developed diagnostic skills. If she is asked to write its own expert at the expense of both parties.
a report and makes an appearance at court, she Having the court for your client mitigates the
becomes annoyed that the cross-examining attor- conflicts to some degree, as judges are much more
ney spends time repeatedly asking about a minor interested in everything an FN has to say and
scoring error, but does not affirm her 99% there is less pressure to tailor findings to an advo-
accuracy in all other scoring. She concludes this cate’s needs. However, court appointed expertise
attorney is unfair. These perceptions, while often is uncommon in civil proceedings and perhaps
true, betray her misunderstanding of legal culture. found only in juvenile law. Even in this more
28 forensic neuropsychology

desirable circumstance, the FN is still subject to standard means the trier of fact favors parties
aggressive cross-examination by an advocate. having at least 51% of the evidence in their favor.
This is the evidentiary standard in personal injury
Conflicting Methods cases and worker’s compensation. Criminal courts
Another pervasive conflict is differing standards of rely on a “beyond a reasonable doubt” standard,
proof (Blau, 1998). FNs are trained in the scientist- but as observed by Hess (1999a), even this level of
practitioner model, which relies on two prongs: (1) legal evidence barely approaches the lower limit
Conservative evidentiary standards and (2) repli- of the p < .05 standard common in scientific
cation. Two common standards of proof used by psychology. When asked to assign probabilities to
FNs to justify selection of measures are (a) the specific burdens of proof, many judges refuse
zero-order validity study (Grove & Barden, 1999) to do so (McCauliff, 1982), stating such things as
and (b) the proven classificatory accuracy of a test “percentage or probabilities simply cannot encom-
(Larrabee & Berry, 2007). A zero-order validity pass all the factors, tangible and intangible, in
study is the demonstration of a statistically signifi- determining guilt—evidence cannot be evaluated
cant correlation between a neurocognitive measure in such terms” (Simon & Mahan, 1971). To this
and a neurological criterion. The statistical thresh- day, trial consultants continue to advise that jurors
old for a significant zero-order correlation is will never agree to decide cases on the basis of
conventionally (but not universally) considered to probabilities (Ball, 2008).
be >95% confidence (p < .05) and ideally 99% Another methods conflict is responsivity to
(p < .01. A classificatory accuracy study examines social and political forces. Scientific neuropsychol-
the sensitivity and specificity of a test in predicting ogy, in principle at least, is supposed to be resistant
a criterion, that is, how many hits and errors (false to changing fads, laws, popular beliefs, and politi-
positives and negatives). With either method, how- cal forces. However, court rulings and jury verdicts
ever, many factors can produce artifacts in a single are not only influenced by popular beliefs and
study: small sample size (insufficient power), political trends, lawyers and judges expect out-
number of variables studied (family-wise error), comes to be influenced by changing community
plain serendipity (type I error), and uncontrolled standards. Courts may set aside verdicts that are
between-group differences (internal validity threat, clearly inconsistent with the law under non obstante
the covariate problem). Thus, replication, prefera- veredicto, judgment as a matter of law, or through
bly in another laboratory, is added as a second remittitur.4 But this self-correcting mechanism is
requirement. Scientific standards of proof are uni- rarely invoked in courts; modern science has a
form across settings and are not altered as a func- better track record of self-correction. The assertion
tion of socially desired versus potentially unpopular that the Constitution is a “living document” is an
outcomes. In summary, neuropsychological con- example of social forces influencing legal interpre-
sensus requires conservative statistical evidence tation. This leads to the anomalous situation of
and consistent results across different settings. different evidentiary requirements for the same
In contrast, the courts require less conservative neuropsychological methodology dependent on
evidentiary standards, shaped by a desire to resolve the legal question (Tenopyr, 1999). For example,
cases over shorter time frames (Hess, 1999a). The the use of IQ tests in employment and school
uniqueness of each case and requirement for settings is held to very rigorous standards follow-
speedy justice makes replication of deficit over ing the Griggs et al. v. Duke Power (1971) and Larry
time a practical impossibility. In addition, legal P. v Riles (1984) rulings. In contrast, despite the
evidentiary standards themselves float; that is, they proven insensitivity of IQ tests to remote
depend on the gravity of the legal outcome, and mild head injuries (Binder, 1997; Binder, Rohling,
whether damages (cash) or fundamental rights & Larrabee, 1997; Dikmen, Machamer, Winn, &
(life/liberty) are at stake. Per Kagehiro (1990), Temkin, 1995; Dikmen, Temkin, Machamer,
these evidentiary thresholds (with proposed confi- & Holubkov, 1994) neuropsychologists are not
dence levels in parentheses) are preponderance of enjoined from using “subtle” intelligence subtest
the evidence (51%), clear and convincing evidence differences as evidence for “brain damage.”
(75%), and reasonable doubt (90%). Although
these proposed confidence levels make sense to Conflicting Roles
scientist–practitioners, judges, jurors, and layper- Conflicts in the forensic arena are not necessarily
sons do not apply them systematically (Wright, between the law and neuropsychology. Role
2008). The “preponderance of the evidence” conflicts arise within the FN as they move from a
Neuropsychology and the Law 29

clinical to a legal setting, and this is termed “dual The treater versus expert conflict may be man-
roles.” A dual role means a psychologist has more ageable, but with difficulty. The manageability
than one relationship with a patient or client. Dual depends on the treatment model. Those therapists
roles are not blanketly unethical—it is the nature whose large unpaid bills are part of the legal dis-
of the dual role that requires reflection; it is only a pute may be especially vulnerable to this dual role
problem when the goals associated with the dual conflict. In some cases, the provider may have been
roles conflict. One definite conflict is treater versus hired only to create a traumatic brain injury narra-
expert. In clinical settings, even the most objective tive and perform unneeded treatment to inflate
neuropsychologist treats the patient with empathy injury severity. Other providers more appropriately
and the goal is primary gain: reducing symptoms advocate for the patient’s best interests, which
and improving function. In contrast, the expert is sometimes includes support for disability pay-
supposed to be objective at all times, assert truths ments, to remove secondary gain as an obstacle to
the plaintiff does not want to hear, and evaluates treatment. This situation often applies when a psy-
the influence of secondary gain: maintenance of chologist has treated a plaintiff before their neuro-
symptoms for reward. The conflict is clear: One logical injury. Supporting a patient’s demands for
cannot reduce and maintain symptoms at the compensation has a long tradition in clinical psy-
same time; one cannot be empathic but increase chology and medicine, as secondary gain (the legal
emotional costs by making statements damaging requirement of proving damages) may interfere
to a patient’s legal pursuits. The conflict between with primary gain (the therapeutic goal of reducing
“treater” and attorney’s agent is the most common symptoms). Fenichel (1945) discussed how sec-
form dual role conflict takes, but views differ on ondary gain prolonged the psychoanalytic treat-
whether this specific conflict is inherently unethi- ment of traumatic neuroses and he advised:
cal. Greenberg and Shuman (1997, 2008a, 2008b) “Perhaps the idea of giving a single compensation
provide an exhaustive analysis of all the problems at the right time may be the best way out” (p. 126).
inherent in therapists being asked to be expert Collegiality versus opposition is another con-
witnesses and trial consultants. As one example, flict requiring some adjustment. In pure clinical
Greenberg and Shuman point out that if the treater settings, neuropsychologists operate in a collegial
accepts the role of consultant, then the patient is fashion with their community. They share data
no longer the client but the plaintiff attorney is. with each other, strive for points of agreement in
There may also be circumstances where the civil conceptualization of a patient’s cognitive prob-
defense attorney or prosecutor asks a treater to lems, and collaborate on research. In a forensic
testify. In such cases, the treater should insist on setting, FNs are often asked to forcefully critique
being a reporting (fact) witness only and answer the work of another neuropsychologist who has
questions based on information contained within been hired by the other side.
the “four corners” of their reports. Shuman,
Greenberg, Heilbrun, and Foote (1998) go even MANAGEABLE
further—they make the provocative argument C O N T E M P O R A RY
that therapists should not be allowed to testify at CONFLICTS
all, and attorneys from both sides should be forced The core conflicts discussed to this point are
to retain independent examiners. structural and endemic to every case FNs accept.
We believe these proposals are too restrictive They can only be adjusted to, not changed in any
because they deviate from FRE 702. Kaufmann substantial manner. FNs cannot rewrite law, or
(this volume) describes recent practical alterna- dictate lawyer conduct. However, there are con-
tive positions detailed by Dvoskin (2002), Heltzel flicts that the FNs encounter episodically, where
(2007), and Woody (2009). The criterion should the law and neuropsychology clash, but the FN
be “probable assistance” to the trier of fact, and a may exercise some control or have considerable
therapist (or cognitive rehabilitation specialist) input as to how these conflicts are resolved. The
may provide evidence regarding daily function response to each of these conflicts is a matter of
that an independent examiner could not. controversy, and various solutions are available.
Greenberg and Shuman are correct that inherent
conflicts may contaminate the evidence, but any Access to Raw Data
strong biases (such as financial interest in trial Legal decisions, like scientific ones, are based on
outcome) or zealous advocacy can be addressed evidence. The rules of evidence in every jurisdic-
during cross-examination. tion allow attorneys to obtain the data on which
30 forensic neuropsychology

the opposing expert bases their opinions. This pro- strong prohibitions against data sharing. The term
cess is termed discovery. In civil and criminal set- interpreted is chosen because of the inevitable
tings, all relevant information is discoverable, ambiguous nature of ethical guidelines. Ethics
unless privileged. Cardiologists must provide EKG codes, like American law, are not Napoleonic,
tracings, crime scene scientists must provide meaning not every possibility or decision is laid
bloody clothes, and psychologists must provide out in detail, to eliminate interpretation. Instead,
test data to the opposing expert. However, principles are broadly abstract and applied by
attorneys are not required to disclose conversa- human judges, not administrators.
tions with a client or work product on a case, Psychologists commonly cite the 2002 edition
because of the important social need to protect of the Ethical Principles for Psychologists
the attorney-client relationship, and to encourage (American Psychological Association, 2002;
clients to be forthcoming within that relationship. hereinafter the 2002 Code) as their authority
In some states, psychologists are not allowed to controlling the treatment of raw data requests.
disclose their test results to nonpsychologists. The A number of the 2002 Code’s principles bear
evidentiary base upon which neuropsychologists directly on either forensic activities or the disclo-
rely typically includes test scores, records, inter- sure of raw test data to nonpsychologists.
view notes, and mental status observations. It is Unfortunately, there has been no consensus on
inevitable that most FNs are eventually asked to Code interpretation in responding to subpoenas,
provide a copy of their case file to either retaining and there is a wide range of views on handling raw
or opposing counsel, hereinafter referred to as data requests. Essig, Mittenberg, Peterson,
“raw data.” Strauman, and Cooper (2001) surveyed FNs about
Legal arguments and strategies of managing their raw data practices. They reported 61% of
the “raw data” problem are described extensively FNs did not share data in half or more of their
elsewhere (Kaufmann, 2005, 2009). In developing forensic cases, while 39% reported honoring the
responses to requests for data disclosure, it is request in the majority of cases. Looking at the
important to define terms. What exactly is meant extremes only, 12% of FNs refused to forward raw
by raw data? Matarazzo (1990) defines the several test data at any time and 18% reported sharing
different types of raw data subject to discovery data with the opposition regularly. One reason-
demands. Such data includes (1) written reports, able interpretation of this data is that 30% take the
(2) handwritten interview notes, (3) numerical polarized views reflected in the commentary
scores, for example, raw counts of number correct literature (discussed elsewhere in this chapter)
or standardized scores, (4) test stimuli, (5) the and the middle 70% disclose data on an irregular
participant’s actual responses to test stimuli, and basis, depending on particulars. Essig et al. (2001)
(6) test manuals. As a practical matter, most foren- did not explore the contingencies of this middle
sic case files will not contain stand-alone test group’s practices. Pieniadz and Kelland (2001) did
stimuli and actual manuals.5 The clash between explore these contingencies but restricted their
the law and neuropsychology usually results from survey to the issue of including test scores in the
demands to discover raw data in categories 3 and report body. They found 35.5% regularly included
5: test forms containing verbatim responses, and scores (the “Yes” group) and 64.5% (the “No”
scoring sheets/formulas. group) did not. The main reasons for including
First, we state the law-psychology conflict. scores were “integrity/thoroughness” and “future
Essentially, many psychologists (wrongly) view a comparison” (both 100% of Yes group). The main
raw data request as a negotiation between two reasons for excluding specific scores were “data
equal parties. Perhaps as a reflection of sound protection” (90% of No group) and to “maintain
ethical training or alternatively an evasion of the [Boston] process focus” (50%). Adherents of
public scrutiny, many psychologists approach the the process approach were more likely to refuse
raw data problem as an ethical dilemma that they test score inclusion than flexible battery adher-
should resolve, rather than a legal question that ents. Kaufmann (2009) noted that marked
courts alone will decide. In addition to this legal variance in data release practices persists in his
analysis, further conflict may also arise because workshop participants, in part, due to widely
the law generally allows discovery of raw data varying jurisdictional laws governing the practice.
used to formulate expert opinions, but the ethics Although survey data is limited, it appears
code under which most neuropsychologists neuropsychologists lack uniformity and offer
operate has been interpreted by some as placing many different reasons for including, excluding,
Neuropsychology and the Law 31

or variably supplying the data that serves as the However, the Edison nondisclosure privilege
basis for their opinions. is narrow and only implied. Stated alternatively,
Other reasons for this lack of practice stan- test security interests were presumed by all parties
dard may originate in the ambiguities and frank and the Court, such that the Edison decision did
contradictions of the preceding 1992 Code not confront directly the “raw data” controversy.
(American Psychological Association, 1992). One Consequently, Edison does not contain any test
1992 Code paradox allowed psychologists to give security holdings because appellate courts gener-
their raw test data to their own patients, but also ally do not render opinions on matters not
encouraged withholding data from other nonpsy- certified for determination. The duty of psycholo-
chologists (e.g., courts). The 1992 Code also dis- gists to safeguard test materials from disclosure to
tinguished between forensic and clinical settings, nonpsychologists (e.g., the Illinois statute) does
further encouraging different approaches to han- not exist in most jurisdictions. Following publi-
dling data. Review of commentary articles from cation of Kaufmann (2009), two federal cases
the 1992–2002 eras reveals a wide array of confronted the Edison privilege and commented
opinions, which may be broadly classified into on his legal theory (Taylor v. Erna, 2009; Lumsden
two camps based on the public policy debate: test v. United States, 2010). In Taylor, the district court
security and absolute discovery camps.6 The test agreed Edison’s holding was that test security
security position treats raw test data as distinct trumped the union’s interests, given the particular
from other forms of legal evidence, and urged facts (page 7).7 The absence of a universal test
special treatment by the courts. Tranel (1994) security privilege is also shown in the recent
viewed the 1992 Code as “clear” on barring dis- Lumsden decision. The North Carolina trial court
closure of data by relying solely on 1992 Standard declined to recognize the Edison privilege because
2.02(b) as his authority. He suggested barring it was not properly presented, stating that the
the reporting of test scores in the report to avoid psychologist “does not even appear to be asserting
(a) “potential misuse” and (b) dissemination of any legal privilege of his own against disclosing
test items into the public domain; for example, the materials” (page 8).8
stopping cross-examining attorneys from asking In light of these rulings, Kaufmann (2010)
questions about individual items rather than subsequently advised: 1) Parties (plaintiff, defense)
aggregate scores. Kaufmann (2005) drew atten- cannot assert the psychologist nondisclosure
tion to an important U.S. Supreme Court decision privilege/duty to protect psychological test mate-
and its progeny that favor test security: Detroit rials, because the psychologist (nonparty) is the
Edison Co. v. National Labor Relations Board holder of that privilege; 2) If psychologists do
(NLRB) (1979) (hereinafter Edison). In Edison, not independently assert the psychologist non-
the High Court ruled against the utility union that disclosure privilege/duty to protect psychological
demanded test scores, materials, and manuals for test materials, then they may be ordered to
psychological tests administered by the utility. produce raw data and test protocols to nonpsy-
The USSC spoke to the “strong public policy” of chologists. Essentially, there is a fundamental dif-
test security for standardized psychological ference between asserting a privilege not to release
instruments. Kaufmann (2005) argued the Edison materials and pointing out legal prohibitions
decision creates a narrow, implied nondisclosure against release. Absent a claim of privilege the
privilege for psychologists that imposes a duty to court will not rule on whether it applies in the
safeguard psychological tests from wrongful dis- case at bar.
closure. In reaction to Edison, a number of states The absolute discovery position is best exem-
enacted legislation designed to protect psycho- plified by Lees-Haley and Courtney’s (2000) com-
logical test materials. Subsequently, a federal court mentary in a special issue of Neuropsychology
(Chiperas v. Rubin, 1998) linked the Edison privi- Review. This position asserts civil rights and legal
lege to a clinical case and state courts have recog- due process as the only guide that dictates treat-
nized “the psychological profession’s legitimate ment of psychological test data, making no men-
interest in preserving the security of tests” (p. 776, tion of privilege law. Raw test data should not be
Fla. DOT v. Piccolo, 2007). Kaufmann (2009) afforded special protections, and psychologists
identifies a series of additional federal court and must be subject to the same evidentiary rules as
NLRB administrative decisions that uniformly other experts. According to this argument, broad
recognize that discovery of psychological tests are nondisclosure rights create a special class of
restricted under Edison. experts who are not subject to legally mandated
32 forensic neuropsychology

courtroom practices, a potential violation of manuals and items were discoverable by a party to
due process. Lees-Haley and Courtney (2000) cite the suit. Until higher courts and/or legislatures
1992 Standard 1.23(b) (Documentation of adopt a universal test security privilege, FNs have
Professional Work) as their main authority. How an array of choices for dealing with requests
can one document work performed, yet refuse to (or subpoenas) for raw data and test manuals.
share the documentation for legal scrutiny? The In states that have no unique protections for test
Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychology materials that contain raw data (see Lumsford
(SGFFP) also require complete transparency of 2010 described, supra), one can begin with a pre-
psychological evidence for scrutiny by a court. sumption against disclosing test manuals (and
Greiffenstein and Cohen (2005) observed that if perhaps raw data), much like the initial presump-
in-text test scores were disallowed as proposed by tion against third-party observation (discussed
Tranel (1994), every school psychologist in this later). The presumption can be backed by assert-
country was subject to ethical sanctions for ing a privilege for the court to consider, or request-
reporting WISC-3/4 profiles. The authors do not ing the court issue a protective order (see Table
find that Tranel’s position is supported under the 2.2). Otherwise, Standard 9.04(b) requires psy-
current Ethics Code, nor can Kaufmann find any chologists to provide test data as required by law
legal support for such a broad prohibition of test or court order. Currently, about twenty states pro-
score disclosure. tect psychological tests as a unique methodology,
The new 2002 Code attempts to address the with some states enacting a psychologist nondis-
ambiguities in the earlier Code to better reflect closure privilege/duty to safeguard test materials
the evolving relationship between psychology and from wrongful disclosure. Nevertheless, a valid
law (Fisher, 2003). The major shift from 1992 to subpoena is sufficient for disclosing a test partici-
2002 is from a presumption of withholding raw pant’s scored protocols and history notes.9 The
data to one of providing it under specified condi- 2002 code is imperfect, and it has been criticized
tions (Behnke, 2003). To meet conflicting goals of (Behnke, 2003; Rapp, Ferber, & Bush, 2008). For
test security versus discovery, the 2002 Code example, it does not deal with the situation of
definition of raw data is subdivided into two neuropsychological measures that contain the test
classes in the spirit of Matarazzo (1990), making a stimuli on the scoring forms, for example, the
clear distinction between scored test forms from a Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. Kaufmann
case file versus test manuals. Each is given sepa- (2009) calls for a code revision that recognizes a
rate treatment: Standard 9.04(a) deals with scored uniform psychologist nondisclosure privilege
protocols in the individual case and Standard 9.11 consistent with Edison. Nevertheless, the 2002
deals with test manuals and stimuli. Standard 9.04 Code remains a reasonable advance towards
allows release of scored protocols in response to a resolving perceived conflicts between ethics and
valid subpoena without the ambiguous “reason- the law. We will not belabor the strained ethical
able steps” contingency of the earlier Code, takes analyses in what so many others have attempted
local law into consideration, and allows for more to explain, except to say: Professional psychology
uniform treatment of individual raw data than still has not set forth a workable standard to the
the old parallel standard: Raw test data can still be wider audience of practitioners outside of the
disclosed to a patient/participant, but without the forensic specialists who require regular raw data
language that some have used to justify withhold- access to address legal questions.10 In summary,
ing evidence from attorneys. Code 2002 Standard the present authors favor the legal primacy of test
9.11 protects test security by maintaining restric- security with respect to manuals and test stimuli
tions on disclosure of manuals and stimuli. This because it is most consistent with the law and best
separate treatment of manuals may allay some record policy for psychology practice.
fears of public dissemination, the main reason
usually given to withhold any data (Tranel, 1994; Third-Party Observation (TPO)
Naugle & McSweeny, 1995). The presence of a third-party observer remains a
In the first edition of this chapter, Greiffenstein contentious issue that continues to occupy courts,
and Cohen (2005) argued that to maintain fidelity attorneys, and FNs. There is a legal foundation for
with the 2002 Code, test data should not be TPO requests, as almost all jurisdictions, federal
afforded special protections, but that test manuals and state, allow consideration of attorneys observ-
should be. This opinion is also in accord with ing independent medical examinations (IMEs)
Edison, in which the legal issue was whether test or government-ordered evaluations. The specific
Neuropsychology and the Law 33

TABLE 2.2 CONDITIONS FOR VIDEOTAPING A NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL


EVALUATION
Model Court Order
1. A single stationary wireless video camera with microphone will be located in the examination room, focused
on Plaintiff but outside his/her direct line of vision, to minimize distraction. The video monitor and video
operator will be located in a separate room. The video operator will receive the video and audio feeds from the
two output jacks at the back of the video monitor.
2. The video operator and/or plaintiff attorney will not interfere, interrupt or disrupt the examination at any time,
even if the videotape needs to be changed. The operator and any plaintiff ’s representatives are silent observers
in the separate room. The Court recognizes that any distraction introduced by the video monitoring procedure
may impact scores on attention tests.
3. This Court recognizes that psychological and neuropsychological measures are of no use if the test forms,
administration procedures and test responses are broadly disseminated to the public. Videotapes of
neuropsychological procedures may be used to coach clients to improve performance on tasks designed
to detect exaggerated deficit, or to intentionally underperform on standard neuropsychological measures.
For these reasons, reasonable restrictions must be placed on the use of any videotape. These restrictions are
described in section #4.
4. Any and all videotapes and test materials that are produced pursuant to the examination will be returned to
Dr. Ima Legend at the conclusion of the litigation, including any and all appeals, if necessary. Exhibits and
courtroom records containing test materials will be protected or sealed until destroyed or returned to Dr.
Legend at the conclusion of litigation. Information about the videotape and test materials shall not be disclosed
to any person other than to Plaintiff, Plaintiff ’s attorney, Defendant’s attorney and Plaintiff ’s and Defendants
designated experts. In no event shall any information about the videotape or tests materials be reviewed by
nor disclosed to any person not specifically recited herein. Presentations of test materials or videotape in the
courtroom will he minimized to the extent possible.

wording of these statutes may grant the right legislation to exclude TPO during civil neuro-
for plaintiff ’s personal “physician” or attorney to psychological evaluations.
be present during a “physical examination” in a Case law to date shows a wide range of rulings,
contested matter. It is a matter of judicial discre- from narrow construction to unlimited TPO.
tion whether these statutes broadly cover any Case law can be grouped into various approaches,
third-party examination requested by the civil such as a “presumptive right” or “no presumptive
defendant (or government) or whether the statute right” to TPO, but most rulings allow for trial
should be narrowly construed to the plain lan- court discretion based on case particulars. Much
guage that names purely medical investigations. depends on the legal context (criminal vs. civil),
Trial judges have great latitude in defining the the nature of the evaluation (invasive vs. noninva-
scope of these terms and in most cases in local sive), and the claims made (legal insanity vs.
(but not federal) courts. It is inevitable that every traumatic brain injury). Many appellate decisions
FN will be confronted with this demand at contain language proving that courts recognize
some time. the validity of neuropsychological test responses
Major neuropsychology organizations pub- can be affected by third-party involvement, when
lished strongly worded policies discouraging TPO compared to physical responses in medical exam-
practice, except in very limited circumstances inations. For example, in Cline v. Firestone Tire
(AACN, 2001; Axelrod et al., 2000). But an APA (1988) the plaintiff moved for a protective order
position paper (Assessment, 2008) avoided barring a neuropsychology IME unless strict
blanket resistance, instead offering a variety of conditions were met, but the federal district court
approaches that included minimizations of held that plaintiff was not entitled to have an
intrusion or using assessment procedures less attorney present. In Tomlin v. Holecek (1993), a
affected by an audience, as well as requesting TPO Minnesota district court denied plaintiff ’s motion
withdrawal or refusing it. Most recently, Howe to record or observe the examination, reasoning
and McCaffrey (2010) encouraged advocacy and TPO would influence the plaintiff to “guard, alter
34 forensic neuropsychology

or disguise” test responses during both the history on an examinee’s test performance.” Although the
taking and testing phases. In Ragge v. MCA/ AACN position statement on TPO asserts that
Universal Studios (1995), a USDC denied plain- “involved and uninvolved” are crucial distinctions
tiff ’s request for a third-party observer during a in predicting performance, some studies have not
neuropsychological IME. In a criminal matter shown any evidence that forensic role or social
(United States v. Byers, 1984), the DC Circuit connection makes any difference. Yantz and
Court ruled that a defendant was not allowed an McCaffrey (2005) demonstrated that a neuro-
attorney TPO during a competency evaluation.11 psychology supervisor’s presence had just as
But in child abuse allegations and death penalty impairing an influence as any other observer, and
cases, where much is a stake and there is a greater Greiffenstein, Baker, Fox, Boone, and Tsushima
demand for reliability of psychologist report, TPO (2010) showed forensic role (plaintiff or defense)
may be more strongly enforced. Some states, such had no influence on MMPI-2 validity scales.
as Texas, require audiotaping or videotaping of There is also no support for the idea promoted
interviews and psychological testing whenever by plaintiff attorneys that “supportive” observers
there is child abuse/neglect prosecution. One are necessary to facilitate cooperation during an
factor tying these cases together is that the burden adversarial examination. Using a single case
rests on the movant (the one who petitions for an ABAB experimental design, Binder and Johnson-
observer) to show good cause for potentially Greene (1995) described a patient performed
affecting an evaluation’s validity. This is an easier worse on malingering measures when her mother
hurdle to jump if a lot is at stake. was present then when she was absent. More on
The scientific evidence regarding TPO or point, Butler and Baumeister (1998) reported
recording empirically supports making distinc- that even warm, supportive observers caused
tions between the interview and testing phases of decrements on skilled tasks relative to unmoni-
the assessment. The counseling psychology and tored performance. These studies disprove the
child abuse literature raises concern about the legal assertion that having an “agent” or “support-
accuracy of interviewers when asked to recall ive family member” facilitates the neuropsycho-
questions and answers (Covner, 1944; Gelso, 1974; logical examination. Regarding the interview
Ceci, Bruck, & Battin 2000). phase, research suggests the recording or observa-
On the testing side, there is no reasonable dis- tion may improve the reliability of history, insofar
pute that the presence of an audience influences as mistakes are correctable.
performance (McCaffrey, Lynch, & Yantz, 2005). Unlike the raw data issue, practitioner surveys
There is a rich, 100+ year-old literature showing show stronger attitudes against TPOs, although
TPO affects performance tests differently. Termed actual compliance with observer requests differs
the “social facilitation” literature, social psycholo- along the civil-criminal divide. In mostly civil
gists showed that perceptual-motor performance cases, Essig et al. (2001) found that 88% of FNs
is affected by observation. A typical finding is that never allow third-party observation, 11% allow it
simple perceptual-motor tests are facilitated by an on rare occasion, and none allow on a regular
audience, but more complex tasks are negatively basis. But in a criminal context, Shealy, Cramer,
affected (Bond & Titus, 1983; Constantinou & and Pirelli (2008) reported 75% of forensic psy-
McCaffrey, 2003; McCaffrey et al., 2005). There chologists had conducted examinations with TPO
are many key studies relevant to neuropsychology. presence (11% on the civil side, see Essig et al.
Kehrer, Sanchez, Habif, Rosenbaum, and Townes supra), but 59% expressed worry that observer
(2000) reported performance decrements on tests presence influenced their results in a negative
of attention, speed of information processing, and direction. The different practices and attitudes
verbal fluency, but no effect on simple manual of forensic neuropsychologists versus forensic
speed. Constantinou, Ashendorf, and McCaffrey psychologists are also reflected in commentary
(2002) examined the effects of an audio-recorder papers and official policy statements.
and found a decrement in memory scores, but not Otto and Krauss (2009) proposed a detailed
on motor measures. A similar result was found decision-making framework for coping with TPO
with a video-recorder (Constantinou & McCaffrey, demands. Their approach is shaped by a forensic
2003). Gavett, Lynch, and McCaffrey (2005) psychology viewpoint, which places greater
concluded “the presence of an observer during emphasis on civil rights and Constitutional
a neuropsychological evaluation should be issues, and less emphasis on test standardization
expected to have a clinically meaningful impact and scientific issues. Although we do not agree
Neuropsychology and the Law 35

with their legalistic emphasis, they provide recommend two approaches. First, the FN should
thoughtful guidelines that allow the FN to con- include a paragraph in the report, addressing the
sider many variables, which includes the costs of influence of the TPO on that particular cognitive
potentially unreliable reporting of the interview profile. Second, the FN should take reasonable
data. McCaffrey, Fisher, Gold, and Lynch (1996) steps to limit access to any videotape or audiotape
also offer practical guidelines for decision-making of the test administration. This includes offering a
when responding to attorney requests for obser- model court order. Table 2.2 gives an example of a
vation. Practical responses to common TPO chal- proposed order.
lenges and attorney tactics are reviewed in detail
elsewhere (Howe & McCaffrey, 2010). P H A S E S O F T H E AT T O R N E Y-
In the present authors’ view, we recommend NEUROPSYCHOLOGIST
that FNs start with a presumption against TPO INTERACTION
presence during the testing phase. TPO is often a The FN can expect his interactions with attorneys
legal tactic designed to intimidate neuropsy- to go through a series of typical steps. Each step
chologists, not to protect the rights of the plaintiff requires different preparatory, cognitive, and
(or criminal defendant). We have no objections to ethics-related activities. The typical phases of
attorneys sitting in during the interview portion, attorney-neuropsychologist interaction include
as there is a more compelling interest to protect the pre-assessment, assessment, report writing,
legal rights. For example, discussion of liability discovery, testimony (deposition or trial), and
issues (who caused the accident) is outside the post-trial phases.
purview of neuropsychologists, and discussion of
elements of the crime is inappropriate in a trial Pre-Assessment: Initial Meeting
competency matter (although not in a legal insan- First contact with an attorney contains a number
ity matter). We agree with Otto and Krauss (2009) of crucial elements. The first decisions the FN
that the literature on child abuse allegations makes are (a) competence to answer the legal
shows ample proof that retroactive recollection of question, and (b) the specific role that will be
interview content is sufficiently inaccurate to played. Preliminary data collection to support
warrant concerns. decision making at bare minimum requires four
But, because test administration is standard- pieces of information: (a) a brief case synopsis, (b)
ized, the answers immediately recorded, and no a short list of hypothetical questions to be asked of
case-specific legal issues raised by cognitive test the FN, (c) potential dual role conflicts, and (d)
items, the costs of TPO (negative impact on test availability of the plaintiff (or criminal defendant).
scores) outweighs the minimal benefits (detecting Other requested information may include antici-
minor scoring inaccuracies). As judges like to pated court dates, discovery deadlines, and
say to parties attempting to suppress damaging amount and type of records to be reviewed. This
evidence, “That’s why cross-examination was conversation need not last more than 10 minutes.
invented.” We disagree with APA’s surprisingly The brief case summary is critical for a number
poor advice that selecting tests less-affected by of reasons. The fact set will help determine good-
observation is a viable solution, precisely because ness of fit between your expertise and the case, the
those tests would be less sensitive to brain injury, elements of the assessment approach, and amount
and may have the unintended result of false nega- of time you will spend. More importantly, the case
tive error; that is, not finding cognitive impair- summary represents the first challenge to your
ment when it is present. If this option were chosen, objectivity. The attorney’s synopsis is from the
it would give the de facto right of any party to liti- viewpoint of an advocate, so be alert to one-sided
gation to dictate test battery content. For example, portrayals of the case. First contact is when the
a vocabulary test is unlikely to be affected by third FN starts the process of resisting the “pull of affili-
party observation, but it is also unaffected by most ation” (Brodsky, 1991). Attorneys may contact
brain injury. Should we replace a time-sensitive FNs on the belief that neuropsychological analysis
test like Controlled Oral Word Association with will support their medical-legal theory. Thus, they
an untimed vocabulary measure? may try to prejudice you right from the outset by
Resistance to TPO should be greatest in civil providing a biased summary that omits or distorts
cases, but in criminal cases where liberty interests key facts. A plaintiff attorney may state “I don’t
are at stake, the FN should consider allowing believe in accepting mild concussion cases, but
TPO. If the decision is to allow TPO, we I have a client who got a severe closed head injury
36 forensic neuropsychology

two years ago, and he hasn’t worked since.” Access to the plaintiff (or criminal defendant)
You later find out the injury was only minor is the third key issue to clarify. Neuropsychological
orthopedic strain (a/k/a ‘whiplash’). A civil and psychological testing requires conditions
defense attorney may say, “The plaintiff has a long similar to the standardization groups described in
history of drug and alcohol abuse, he hasn’t the test’s instruction manual. If the case demands
worked in two years, and I’m sure his brain was a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment,
fried before the accident.” You later receive records the FN should insist the plaintiff be examined at
showing a trauma neurosurgeon had to debride the FN’s offices. Some modern prisons do contain
5 cc of destroyed prefrontal lobe tissue. We advise secure, quiet interview rooms. Otherwise, the
giving attorneys broad latitude by accepting there guiding principle is: If the case comes with severe
will be some attempt to prejudice you; acceptance restrictions imposed by a judge or the retaining
or rejection of the case depends on the magnitude attorney, or lacks an appropriate testing room,
of the partisanship and fact distortion. Refuse the FN should refuse, but only if the restriction
involvement only if the attorney grossly misrepre- potentially influences test score validity.
sents the facts, limits your access to key records or In summary, acceptance of the case depends
collaterals, or insists on your prejudging the case. on (1) relevance to one’s expertise, (2) sufficient
The hypothetical questions of the retaining access to data on which to base an opinion, and
attorney determine the activities and roles that the (3) absence of potential conflicts. If the FN has a
FN assumes. The hypothetical question is designed prior relationship with any party to the suit, this
to elicit an opinion that summarizes all the psycho- relationship needs to be considered. “Treating”
logical and neuropsychological issues in a brief neuropsychologists should precede especially gin-
conclusion that is understandable to the typical gerly, as both the plaintiff and the plaintiff attor-
juror (Blau, 1998). The attorney may not yet know ney become clients. But such a dual relationship
the specific questions that she will ask during trial, does not automatically disqualify participation if
but she can gave you a general question that sum- the FN can give probable assistance to the trier of
marizes the issues relevant to a neuropsychologist; fact, but we’ll develop that idea later when dis-
for example, “Did the accident (or failed medical cussing ethics. The remainder of this section will
treatment) cause brain damage?” Feel free to pro- assume the FN’s acceptance of involvement.
pose specific preliminary hypothetical questions
raised by the fact set, so that there is a shared under- Pre-Assessment: Role Selection
standing of your assessment goals, and how they fit The next crucial element during initial contact is
the legal goals. The shared framework also assists role selection. The FN must determine which of
the FN in determining whether he can answer the potential roles they will choose. The basic
questions in a reasonably probable way, or whether choices are (a) fact witness, (b) expert witness,
the question even falls within his expertise. and (c) litigation consulting. The fact (a/k/a
This early conversation provides an opportu- “reporting”) and expert witness roles both involve
nity to candidly discuss your own views and foren- live testimony either in court or by deposition. As
sic history. If a plaintiff attorney contacts you and a fact witness, the FN can only report facts about
you do not believe that common adult whiplash the patient. The testifying expert is a FN who
causes diffuse axonal injury or that minor concus- reports opinions at trial or during a deposition.
sion causes disproportionately severe long-term Two crucial features distinguish the expert
disability, say so. The plaintiff attorney will appre- witness from the fact witness: The expert is
ciate your candor and will look for a neuropsy- allowed to report hearsay (“The plaintiff said….”)
chologist with different views, or maybe drop the and is allowed to offer an opinion (“Smith’s
case because of financial risks to his practice. memory problems were caused by a penetrating
Alternatively, if contacted by a defense attorney, missile wound to the brain”). In contrast, the fact
and you tell her that you believe that five years of witness reports information gleaned through the
cannabis smoking is unlikely to cause major brain senses only, with little interpretation (“I saw Jones
damage, also say so at the outset. Even if the attor- shoot the gun at Smith’s head” or “I saw the plain-
ney decides not to use you, you have established tiff for an examination on July 5, 1999”). Myths to
credibility, and diminished your chances of being the contrary, fact witnesses are allowed to draw
called a hired gun who will testify to support any some conclusions, as long as the conclusion is not
outlandish medical-legal theory that the retaining based on any specialized knowledge that defines
attorney wants to advance. an expert witness (“Jones looked like he wanted to
Neuropsychology and the Law 37

kill Smith”). If an original examiner saw the patient The retaining attorney may ask the testifying
on referral from a physician prior to involvement expert to examine the claimant for evidence of
(assuming an attorney did not start the referral impaired brain functions and disturbed personal-
chain), that neuropsychologist is technically a fact ity. Generally, there is no doctor-patient relatio-
witness. As a practical matter, however, the line nship and such an evaluation is termed an
between a fact witness who is a doctor and a testify- independent medical examination (IME). However,
ing expert is blurred and state rules vary regarding at least one duty of the doctor-patient relationship
these witness designations. A treating doctor, read- exists in some jurisdictions during an IME (Harris
ing from a report, states, “I gave a diagnosis of v. Kreutzer, 2006). In Harris, the Virginia Supreme
closed head injury.” That’s an expert opinion dis- Court ruled that a prima facie malpractice allega-
guised as a factual report. But it is rare for judges to tion against a clinical psychologist during an IME
impose any testimonial limitation on a treating may get to a jury. Although the case was ultimately
doctor, so in practice, a “treating” doctor is viewed settled, the Virginia Supreme Court reaffirmed a
no differently than a testifying expert. “circumscribed duty” to do no harm in the limited
The role of litigation consultant means the FN doctor-patient relationship during an IME. In
works “behind the scenes” to educate the attorney Dyer v. Trachtman (2004), a Michigan appellate
about basic neuropsychological terms and princi- panel ruled that physical injury during an IME
ples, examine test data supplied by another sounded in medical malpractice law, not ordinary
neuropsychologist, offer alternative theories of negligence. IMEs are typically requested by defense
the facts from a neuropsychological perspective, attorneys in civil suits or by claims adjustors in
and design cross-examination questions for the nonlitigated claims, although plaintiff attorneys
opposing neuropsychologist (Derby, 2001; ask for them as well in cases where treating doctors
Greiffenstein, 2008). Any documents developed are unwilling to be involved.
in this role are subject to the attorney “work prod- Testimony can also be based on records review
uct” rule, meaning the FN remains anonymous only, although this places limitations on the scope
and his contributions are not discoverable. of opinions. Both of the Codes state a preference
However, if a treating or testifying neuro- for a direct examination in legal cases, but both
psychologist is also hired as a litigation consultant Codes also make clear that opinions based on
(examine the other side’s raw test data), the work records review only are permissible if (a) the doc-
product rule does not apply and all conversations uments are clearly catalogued, (b) the legal ques-
or documents related to these activities are tion does not require an evaluation in order to be
discoverable. Also, consulting experts should be answered, and (c) one testifies to the limitations of
forewarned that the retaining attorney may decide relying solely on records. The SGFFP (Committee,
to convert the consulting “behind the scenes” 1991) further adds that if records are sufficient to
expert into a testifying expert as the litigation answer the question posed, direct examination
strategy changes; for example, if the consulting becomes unnecessary. The Iowa Supreme Court
expert expresses an opinion that is favorable and upheld neuropsychologists’ qualifications to
the retaining attorney has nobody else who can testify on basis of records alone (Hutchison v.
render that opinion (see Federal Rules of Civil American Family Mutual Ins. Co, 1994).
Procedure, #26, “Disclosures”). The pre-assessment phase is crucial with
The last facet of role selection is determination respect to ethical adherence. It is here that the FN
of the pertinent issues on which the FN will offer may be offered dual or even triple roles. A good
opinions. Neuropsychological evidence is deemed example of a dual role conflict is a treater testify-
helpful in assisting the trier of fact in a broad ing for plaintiff, but also being asked to prepare
range of civil and criminal cases. Table 2.3 sum- the plaintiff attorney for cross-examination of
marizes the kinds of legal settings in which FNs defense expert. To avoid this, the FN should strive
find themselves and the specific issues they are to maintain a single role: testifying expert, treater,
asked to address. The experienced FN should or trial consultant. Some legal journals (e.g.,
have learned how to map legal terminology into Lawyer’s Weekly) publish commentary advising
neuropsychological terminology. Communication the use of the “objective treater to also analyze the
between attorney and FN is facilitated when there defense neuropsychologist’s raw test data and
is a good understanding of what neuropsycho- advice on proper cross-examination questions.”12
logical issues are associated with which jurisdic- Acting as a treater but also testifying and provid-
tional requirement, and vice versa (Grisso, 1988). ing background consulting may turn the treater
38 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 2.3 PARTIAL SUMMARY OF LEGAL SET TINGS AND ISSUES RELEVANT
TO FORENSIC NEUROPSYCHOLOGY

Court Setting Specific Legal Issue Neuropsychological Issues

Probate Testamentary Competence • Post-mortem analyses of cognitive state on critical


legal dates
• Recent and remote memory
• Social perception and delusional status
Capacity for Contracts, Trusts • Vulnerability to persuasion
• Recent memory and intelligence
• Delusional status
Competency to Testify • Usually an issue with children; determine
cognitive developmental stage
Guardianship • IQ and memory
• Functional abilities
• Judgment
Administrative Worker’s Compensation Social • Distinguishing old vs. acquired cognitive
Security Disability deficits
• Prediction of Functional Deficits
• Symptom validity
School district due process • IQ and academic achievement
• Intellectual and learning disabilities
• Qualification for special education services
Civil Causation & Damages • Neurogenic vs. psychosocial factors
• Closed Head Injury
• Low-dose exposure organic solvents
• Black mold causing occult CNS damage
• Acquired versus old cognitive weaknesses
• Post-traumatic versus premorbid psychological
attributes
• Primary and secondary gain, symptom validity
Disability under ADA • Formal diagnosis of cognition-based disability
• Prediction of reasonable accommodations to
remediated deficit
Mental health and commitment • Danger to self or others
• Inability to care for self
Criminal Mens rea or Not guilty by • Transient amnesic disorder
reason of insanity • Dysexecutive Syndrome
• IQ and mental retardation
Competent Waiver Miranda • Auditory Comprehension
Rights • Reading Comprehension
Competence to stand trial • Attention
• Comprehension
• Memory

into an agent of the attorney. To combat this, the As pointed out by Brodsky (1991), bias and ethi-
FN needs to keep in mind the pervasive principle cal conflict occur when a psychologist allows the
running throughout this chapter: What many attor- “pull of affiliation” to subtly enmesh the treater or
neys consider good practice is questionable practice expert into dual advocacy roles.
for a neuropsychologist. The FN does not have to Trial consulting work has special ethical ramifi-
do everything an attorney expects them to do. cations that get limited attention. McSweeny (1997)
Neuropsychology and the Law 39

interpreted the 1992 Code Standard 1.16(a) Pre-Assessment: Compensation


(Misuses of Psychologists Work) to support a for Services
conclusion that trial-consulting work by its very The last step is determining compensation for
nature may be unethical. He reasons that because services. This is usually dealt with in the initial
FN trial consultants are typically anonymous, meeting. The fees should be reasonable; that is,
there is no “corrective mechanism” present as no different from the fees charged for clinical
required by 1.16(a). Although the present authors examinations, assuming similarities in battery
believe that trial consulting is not an inherently composition and testing duration. There is no
unethical activity, we agree with McSweeny (1997) clinical equivalent to deposition or trial testimony,
that such “behind the scenes” work places a spe- so it is customary for experts to charge a higher
cial burden on FN found nowhere else in legal hourly rate and/or charge a minimum fee
work. Anonymity creates a psychological state irrespective of the deposition’s length. This is
with reduced likelihood of negative consequences justifiable because the higher fee is to reserve a
for behavior. This absence of “moral hazard” may block of time; many depositions are cancelled at
uninhibit aggressive tendencies. Examples of the last minute, literally on the courthouse
aggressive behavior include designing cross- steps. The FN has no way of predicting how long a
examination questions that misuse neuropsycho- deposition will take or how long they will sit in a
logical test data, providing irrelevant personal court corridor, so steps are taken to compensate
anecdotes attacking the opposing neuropsycholo- for loss of a day’s income from regular activity.
gist, or biasing neuropsychological interpretation Avoid the temptation to charge more for a foren-
away from the best fitting but inconvenient diag- sic assessment than a clinical test battery of the
nosis. A good example of questionable ethical same size and content.
behavior is for the FN consultant to tell the retain- The FN should maintain a detailed, annually
ing attorney that “fixed batteries such as the updated fee schedule that describes the hourly,
Halstead-Reitan are the only measures accepted bundled, and/or per diem charges. This list can
in the neuropsychology community,” “process easily be faxed or electronically mailed. List
neuropsychology is the only method for detecting elements usually include: The basic hourly rate
deficits,” or “age and education corrections do for records review or conferences; “bundled”
not need to be used.” In other words, anytime a fee for each type of test battery (e.g., neuropsy-
consulting FN replaces accepted practice param- chological or chronic pain); deposition or trial
eters with a polarizing polemic, a potential ethical testimony; and expense policies regarding travel.
violation may be taking place. A FN who regularly Some FN use paralegal or graduate student staff
performs such work should note that anonymity to perform literature searches, review records,
is not guaranteed. In federal law, there are author case synopses, or conduct statistical ana-
exceptions to the anonymous trial consultant rule lyses. Supporting staff ’s time for nontesting activ-
and under certain circumstances an attorney ity should be billed at a lower rate. Do not pretend
must provide the name of the previously secret you did the work yourself. The fee schedule should
consultant (FRCP Rule 26). include an explicit policy for live testimony. Some
The trial consultant’s work product should be experts bill “portal to portal” (the time you leave
shaped by two parameters: (1) Offer advice based home until the time you arrive back home after
on honestly debatable issues of fact, and (2) provide testifying) at the hourly testimony rate, others
consultation as if your identity will eventually be charge less for commuting time. A popular alter-
revealed. For example, reliance on age-based cor- native is a per diem charge if assessment or
rections is a well-established and sound practice testimony involves traveling to a different city, or
parameter. It would be ludicrous for an FN to argue sitting in court for the day. A per diem charge
that age corrections are not needed for neuropsy- is easily defensible for the same reasons as a
chological tests while simultaneously relying on minimum deposition charge: You have to reserve
instruments with age-specific deviation metrics, a large time block during hours you would
for example, Wechsler Intelligence scales. What is normally perform other billable work.
honestly debatable, however, is the particular Practices vary, as do the formality of retention
choice of age-based normative tables to use with agreements. Some FNs ask the attorney to initial
any given neuropsychological measure, as there and date the fee schedule, or sign a brief agreement
are competing normative databases for the same to pay for services rendered. Payment plans can be
measure (Mitrushina, Boone, & D’Elia, 1999). flexible to respond to the role or reputation of the
40 forensic neuropsychology

retaining attorney. Per the advice of Blau (1998), • Managing diagnostic error
do not fall for vague promises such as “Don’t worry, • Incorporating debiasing procedures
you’ll get paid” or “I’ll protect your fee.” In one
author’s experience (M. Greiffenstein), some attor- Assessment Phase: General Contours
neys insist the neuropsychologist bill their clients, What evidence best informs the legal question
on the grounds that paying the FN directly “is posed? Deciding what neuropsychological
unethical for a lawyer to pay you because it is like evidence is necessary to answer the question
loaning money to client.” But that same attorney posed is not an automatic step. This is the point at
will pay investigators directly. To circumvent this, which the principle of “practicing competent
consider demanding retainer fees before initiating neuropsychology” first applies. Some FNs insist
any work from an unfamiliar attorney. This prac- that an examination is inadequate without an
tice dissolves the issue of having a personal finan- extensive battery of tests (particularly “fixed
cial interest in the outcome. Some plaintiff attorneys ‘ones), regardless of forensic question. In reality,
will insist you accept a lien on any settlement or some questions are easily answered by simple
jury verdict. Avoid this arrangement at all costs. records review. The question ‘did the plaintiff sus-
Accepting this arrangement introduces bias and tain a closed head injury on January 1, 2010’ can
is considered unethical under the Specialty be answered just with a review of ambulance run
Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists (SGFFP; sheet and ER records. Records are sufficient to
Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic answer questions regarding the existence of
Psychology, 1991). These ethical guidelines are only a condition, and later neuropsychological testing
binding on members of the American Academy of cannot retrospectively detect brain injury some-
Forensic Psychology (those who earned the ABPP how overlooked by ER trauma specialists. The
in Forensic Psychology), but we accept this docu- question ‘Are there long-term effects of a closed
ment as a best practice guideline for all FNs.13 head injury’ evokes a need for extensive testing,
Accepting a lien creates a financial interest in the however. Competency to stand trial (CST) gener-
outcome of a trial, increasing the “pull of affilia- ally requires a good interview, formal mental
tion” to the retaining side and creating a dual role status, and a specific forensic test. Extensive
conflict (e.g., moneylender vs. treater). Otherwise, testing is not required to answer CST questions.
the 2002 Code is silent on this practice. Do not succumb to the temptation to lend an ‘air
of science and objectivity’ to your work by a large
Assessment Phase battery (Melton et al., 2007, p. 47).
This section describes the data collection process The competent FN relies on a multi-method,
to gather the evidence on which future opinions convergent means of data gathering, if the legal
are based. The general contour of the forensic questions are multifaceted (e.g., damages in
neuropsychological assessment is made up of a personal injury case). Three recommended
these important steps: overlapping methods of the assessment phase are:
Review of outside records (e.g., brain scans, first
• Considering what neuropsychological responder records); direct and collateral inter-
evidence will inform the legal question view, with mental status observations; and
• Reliance on convergent evidentiary method neurocognitive, forensic, and psychological test
wherever possible scores. Lally (2003) refers to these three eviden-
• Recognizing limitations of isolated (single) tiary sources as the “tripod” on which psycholo-
methods gical testimony rests, and further argues this
• Evaluating social influences on approach places psychologists at an advantage
presentation, not just organic factors over other mental health professionals. Kaufmann
• Supplementing forensic instruments most (2009) writes:
suited to legal judgment to be made
• Using normative and scientific bases Psychologists evaluate clinical impressions
sufficient to support test use from interviews, behavioral observations, and
• Reliance on tests in common use informal assessment, with the added benefit of
• Using valid tests, with known sensitivity comparing the individual’s test performance to
and specificity norms. Unlike other mental health profession-
• Including measures of response style als, psychologists use objective psychological
and effort tests to refine clinical impressions when
Neuropsychology and the Law 41

formulating working diagnoses, initial treat- psychopathy. This 59-year-old male claimed
ment plans, and expert opinions. Neuro- irreversible brain damage from a 2008 slip-and-
psychology adds the brain–behavior knowledge fall while selling “pork-belly futures” in a working
base and incorporates neuroimaging, neuro- class Indiana neighborhood. He remained alert
diagnostic, and other neurologic findings to and lucid, but later required surgical drainage of a
the aforementioned evaluation techniques, left subdural hematoma, followed by transient
thereby creating the unique practice of forensic mild aphasia. He recovered fully and neuro-
neuropsychology consulting (p. 1149). psychological testing showed unimpaired scores
in every cognitive and perceptual-motor domain.
A convergent evidentiary model allows greater He pursued three forms of secondary gain:
confidence in one’s opinions and makes rejection permanent workers compensation disability, a
of alternative hypotheses more credible. An opin- premises liability suit against a city, and day-long
ion that a litigated head injury caused cognitive attendant care services from his wife. His wife
impairment is defensible with a convergent dropped a domestic abuse charge against him
pattern of: poor scores on memory tests but shortly after filing suit.
“passing” scores on effort test (test scores), behav- Mental status and interview are fundamental
ior observation of memory retrieval difficulty features of psychological evaluation separate from
during interview (mental status), and acute brain formal tests, and provide a context. It bears
changes and low Glasgow Coma Scale on hospital emphasizing that they are sufficient to render
admission (outside records). Also, the reader is diagnoses per the DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000). The
referred to the Larrabee chapter on scientific context for interpretation is developed by distill-
approaches in this volume. ing historical information into a few areas such as
Records review is important because third- (a) the severity of the initial neurological injury,
party data helps inform diagnostic accuracy in a (b) background variables that help establish
setting known for strong validity threats. We rec- preinjury aptitude and achievement levels, and
ommend the FN create a standing list of recom- (c) establishing the symptom time line. Symptom
mended records for the retaining attorney to evolution also informs accurate diagnosis.
obtain, records most likely to contain relatively Cognitive symptoms that suddenly erupt or evolve
objective data. This includes school records con- long after a head injury are not consistent with the
taining standardized testing, date-of-incident disease of traumatic brain injury (TBI); rather,
records that contain neurological and mental true TBI symptoms appear in maximum intensity
status testing (or arrest reports), neuroimaging immediately following head trauma. Paniak,
studies, routine and 24-hour electroencephalog- MacDonald, Toller-Lobe, Durand, and Nagy
raphy, and pharmacy records. Litigants may give (1998) provide a normative profile for acute injury
inaccurate histories during the interview, in some characteristics of a mild TBI (e.g., anterograde
cases biasing information to the point of frank and retrograde amnesia, loss of consciousness,
deceit (Faust, 1995; Lees-Haley & Brown, 1992; time to first memory) that can be used as
Mittenberg, DiGuilio, Perrin, & Bass, (1992); a template. For this reason, it is important to
Nelson, Drebing, Satz, & Uchiyama, 1998; Schrag, inquire when the symptoms started, and self-
Brown, & Trimble, 2004). One form of reporting report must be correlated with the records.
bias takes is positive inflation of preinjury cogni- A typical approach to interviewing is to develop a
tive and mental health status (Greiffenstein, Baker, structured interview form. This confers standard-
& Johnson-Greene, 2002; Johnson-Greene et al., ization on the interview process (see test adminis-
1997), more recently referred to as the “good old tration section). The mental status examination
days” bias (Iverson, Lange, Brooks, & Rennison, (MSE) component is a systematic monitoring of
2009). Keep in mind that biased reporting is not behavioral cues during the interview, cues that
always conscious. Factors such as normal forget- will be integrated into the total impression and
ting and misremembering also influence present final opinions (Cronbach, 1984; Melton, Petrila,
recall (Barsky, 2002; Schachter, 2001). Poythress, & Slobogin, 2007; Strub & Black, 1988).
The value of third-party data is shown in The MSE includes a description of the claimant’s
Table 2.4. The table is from an author’s (MFG) speech and language, verbal content and discourse
case file, and shows the value of third-party data organization, logic and reasoning, affect, mental
in detecting compelling historical discrepancies trends, nonverbal behavior, and social related-
in a worker’s compensation claimant suspected of ness. These observations are necessary for clinical
42 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 2.4 THIRDPART Y RECORD ANALYSIS IN PLAINTIFF SUSPECTED OF


PREMORBID PSYCHOPATHY

Mr. Jones’s Present Assertion Third-Party Data Implication

1969 car accident, “no injuries” VA records show MVA at age 18 with Inconsistent with current
head trauma, LOC, and neurological report; probable premorbid
residuals of ataxia; but told other neurological problems,
doctors diffuse pain ever since because no incentive to
embellish head injury history
Fabulously wealthy because of SSA records showed most years earned Inconsistent with plaintiff claim
work success below payroll tax cap of business success; implies
grandiose sense of self-
importance
Retired young, in 1994 IRS records show continuous work in Inconsistent with plaintiff claim;
1990s and 2000s, drifted from job to job actually unable to sustain
employment
Intercourse 4-5 times a week up VAMC records show frequent impotence Inconsistent with plaintiff claim
to accident complaint, requests for Viagra, dating of posttraumatic impotence
back to early 2000’s
Gave up drinking 10 years ago VAMC records show ongoing alcohol Inconsistent with plaintiff claim
abuse and dependence, up to and after of abstinence or reduced
the work incident; abstinent periods drinking; currently a heavy
brief and rely on plaintiff ’s self-report drinker
only
Unsteady gait is a problem only Palo Alto records show diagnosis of Inconsistent with plaintiff ’s
since work injury cerebellar ataxia, blamed on presumed causal attribution to the
closed head injury at age 18, although recent accident
plaintiff gave different accounts of that
injury.
Avoided contact with daughter VAMC social worker documented a Inconsistent with plaintiff
through voluntary choice court PPO barring any contact with report; proves judicial notice
daughter of past behavior problems
No prior contact with police Federal indictment for bank fraud in Inconsistent with self-report;
2000, but charges dropped; arrested for aggression while drinking a
domestic violence while intoxicated known feature of psychopathy
in Indianapolis; California and Ohio
records not yet available

correlation, that is, converging lines of evidence Reliance on subjective cognitive complaints has
towards a conclusion. little proven ability to predict cerebral status, and
The limitations of each method should inform symptoms do not correlate with neurocognitive
the reasoning process. A blind interpretation of test scores (Chelune & Heaton, 1986; Satz et al.,
test scores should be avoided when relevant extrat- 1998; Gervais, Ben-Porath, & Wygant, 2009).
est data are available. There can be many reasons Neuropsychological testing, like medical lab stud-
for a low test score, including genuine brain injury, ies, was invented to analyze subjective presenta-
demographics, effort, mood, and medications. In tions. The drawbacks of third party records are
their classic study, Heaton, Smith, Lehman, and manifold, and the fallibilities are outlined by
Vogt (1978) found that expert neuropsychologists Cripe (2002). Pitfalls include lack of universal
could not distinguish the test score summaries of documentation standards; objectivity versus
severe head injury patients from volunteer fakers. subjectivity (CT scan vs. psychiatric diagnosis);
Equally problematic is overweighting self-report unproven assumption of veridicality; unknown
and conducting “diagnosis by clinical history.” reliability and validity of past mental health
Neuropsychology and the Law 43

diagnoses; and overweighting of negative evidence. to the contrary, there is no legal requirement that
The absence of premorbid mental health records “only fixed batteries” are admissible (Baxter v.
does not prove good mental health. Persons with Temple, 2008; Greiffenstein, 2009). Melton et al.
antisocial traits, poor insight, or personality disor- (2007) divide formal psychological tests into three
ders are unlikely to seek mental health counseling. types, and their categorization is generalizable to
Prospects that social (non-neurological) FN practices. Clinical assessment instruments are
influences shaped the data require consideration. tests designed for measurement of clinical con-
Such exploration provides further insights into structs, but can be co-opted for forensic use if
prognosis, separation of neurogenic from psycho- relevant to a legal question. For example, Wechsler
genic influences, and situational and secondary IQ tests can be used in capital eligibility determi-
gain factors. The history is defined as the story nations. Forensically relevant instruments also
(or narrative) of the examinee’s problems, from measure clinical constructs, but are rarely used in
inception to the present (Albers & Schiffer, 2007). mental health settings, and the constructs mea-
Data collected from the claimant should include sured are more prevalent in forensic contexts. The
not only the precipitating event(s) and current Hare Psychopathy Checklist Revised is a good
symptoms, but also the evolution (time line) of example (Hare & Neumann, 2006). Finally, there
complaints, and referral patterns (e.g., saw chiro- are forensically specific instruments. These tests
practor or neurosurgeon first?). Cognitive- measure legal capacities as defined by statute or
attitudinal factors are also important, especially case law, and are used strictly in narrowly defined
with regards to provider feedback in litigated legal settings. Examples include various trial com-
cases. FNs devote some time to exploring the petency measures. Although these are cognitive
claimant’s beliefs regarding the nature of their tests that overlap with many recognizable neurop-
cognitive problems, their expectancies of out- sychological constructs, they are never used clini-
come, and the sources for their beliefs. Greater cally. The training of FNs puts them in a good
scrutiny is necessary because the process of position to use and interpret these instruments
litigation exposes claimants to influences and because of their emphasis on cognitive capacities.
experiences not present in straightforward clini- The normative basis for tests must be a factor
cal situations. in selection. A sound normative base provides an
One source of non-neurogenic influence is objective means of classifying cognitive scores as
symptom suggestion by authority figures. Direct normal, abnormal, deficient, weak or superior.
coaching by attorneys is a documented occur- Commercially available tests with a manual are
rence (Youngjohn, 1995); the majority of trial preferable, and homemade tests and/or norms are
lawyers view it their duty to expose plaintiffs to best avoided. For example, if you assess decision-
psychological test content in preparation for making and impulsivity with the Iowa Gambling
defense scrutiny (Essig et al., 2001), and well- Test, rely on T-scores from the manual. Do not
meaning neuropsychologists may spoil data by rely on z-scores derived from the small “normal
giving inappropriate warnings about symptom control” group of a single study (Manes et al.,
validity detection (Youngjohn, Lees-Haley, & 2002), like one forensic psychologist did in a high-
Binder, 1999). Other social influences include stakes criminal case in which one author (MFG)
education in head injury symptoms through a was involved. Methods are standardized when a
“TBI education group,” Internet research (Ruiz, procedure is described with sufficient clarity to
Drake, Glass, Marcotte, & van Gorp, 2002), and allow different examiners to collect data in the
the mental set known as “diagnosis threat” (Suhr same manner with minimum error (Grisso, 1988;
& Gunstad, 2002). Some plaintiffs may monitor Anastasi & Urbani, 1997).
their every memory act with intense diligence, Reliance on tests in common use means tests
resulting in misinterpretation of normal forget- used by a least a plurality of the neuropsychology
ting as evidence for a closed head injury. community; it does not mean test acceptance by
Adding forensic instruments tailored to the every last neuropsychologist. The best evidence
legal context is often overlooked by FNs. Test for community use is published surveys of test
selection is guided by most of the same general usage. Surveys help the FN in a number of ways.
principles as during a regular clinical neuro- They can be used to get ideas on what the pre-
psychological examination, except for one differ- ferred tests are for a given question, or they can be
ence: FNs select tests that answer the legal used to justify test selection at the testimony
question, not just the clinical one. Despite myths phase. Good examples of general practice surveys
44 forensic neuropsychology

can be found in Butler, Retzlaff, and Vanderploeg where appropriate). In summary, interpret scores
(1991) and Rabin, Barr, and Burton (2005); in a direction of the less damaging error. The
forensic neuropsychology surveys can be found in important concluding maxim is this: The nonspeci-
Lees-Haley, Smith, Williams, and Dunn (1996) ficty of most neuropsychological tests is the main
and Sharland and Gfeller (2007); and good foren- reason for never interpreting scores in isolation from
sic psychology references are Lally (2003) and collateral records and history.
Archer, Buffington-Vollum, Stredny, and Handel The FN should always give symptom validity
(2006). Also useful is a survey of report writing tests (SVTs) and effort tests. Much scholarship
practices by Donders (2001b, 2001d). was published on this issue since the first edition
Evidence for test validity is proven incremental of this book, including Boone (2008), Franzen
ability to detect cognitive deficits in persons and Iverson (2006), Greiffenstein and Baker
known to have them (sensitivity) and ruling out (2006), Larrabee (2008), Larrabee, Greiffenstein,
deficits in persons known to be deficit-free (speci- Greve, and Bianchini (2007), and Sweet and
ficity) when compared to any other available Morgan (2009). It is generally accepted that foren-
method (Larrabee & Berry, 2007). Imperfect mea- sic settings contain strong incentives for distort-
sures (errors in prediction) are fine, as long as ing test scores or interview data (Archer et al.,
there is incremental validity, that is, better overall 2006; Mittenberg, Patton, Canyock, & Condit,
hit rate than with guessing or other methods. 2002; Sharland & Gfeller, 2007; Slick, Tan, Strauss,
Perfection in prediction (or rare errors of predic- & Hultsch, 2004; Sullivan, Lange, & Dawes, 2007).
tion) is not a realistic standard; medical doctors There is no legitimate reason for omitting these
regularly rely on tests with known false positive measures in a forensic context or for avoiding
and negative errors, such as the PSA and chest consideration of the issue during report writing.
X-rays. It follows that there is no neuropsycho- The base rate for invalid response styles in liti-
logical measure (or battery) demonstrating both gated brain damage claims is high, even when
excellent sensitivity and specificity in all imagin- stringent criteria for malingering are applied. For
able clinical situations (Larrabee & Berry, 2007). example, Binder and Willis (1991), using the
Sensitivity and specificity are usually tradeoffs and Portland Digit Recognition Test, reported that
depend on many factors, including the base rate of roughly one-third of patients with remote con-
the target symptom measured and task difficulty. cussion met stringent criteria for malingering.
Diagnostic error management is critical in Larrabee’s (2003) compilation of old data suggests
forensic cases. The FN aims for tests that show the a base rate of 40% for cognitive invalidity. When
best balance between specificity and sensitivity for less stringent but still reasonable criteria are
the construct that is being measured. The pre- applied, elevated base rate of malingering in
ferred type of error depends on the goal; there is litigated post-concussion syndrome is the rule
no such thing as a single gold standard for errors and not the exception (Greiffenstein & Baker,
of prediction. Always choose the less costly of two 2006; Greiffenstein, 2003; Mathias, Greve,
types of diagnostic errors: False positive and false Bianchini, Houston, & Crouch, 2002). Modestly
negative errors. Two practical examples include high base rates for malingering, defined as from
the homeowner who would prefer false positive one-third to two-thirds of a population, provide
errors in selecting a smoke alarm, but the female maximal diagnostic efficiency and incremental
seeking a mate (who will invest in offspring) hit rates for validated SVTs (Mossman, 2000a;
should favor false negatives (Haselton & Buss, 2000b).
2000). Likewise, brain injury diagnosis should There is also good evidence for an inverse
skew towards false positive errors when data is dose-response effect in litigated neurological
mixed/ambiguous, but in effort testing where injury: The more minor the neurological injury,
malingering is considered, false negative errors are the greater the likelihood of response distortion
more desirable. Cutting scores can be adjusted to (Green, Iverson, & Allen, 1999; Greiffenstein,
reflect the desired error mix. Specificity tends to 2003; Greiffenstein & Baker, 2006; Miller &
be variable, as non-neurological conditions can Cartlidge, 1972; Albers, Wald, Garabrant et al.,
affect test scores, for example, abnormal scores in 2000). Finally, one should also take a multistrat-
depressed, older, or intellectually marginal, per- egy approach to examining response distortion
sons. A defensible forensic test battery should (Sweet, 1999). Poor effort and/or malingering are
include both sensitive measures (cognitive perfor- not only seen on memory tests. There are other
mance) and specific measures (e.g., aphasia testing documented forms of noncompliance such as
Neuropsychology and the Law 45

exaggeration of motor deficits (Greiffenstein, Assessment: Report Writing Phase


Baker, & Gola, 1996), overidealized premorbid Neuropsychologists entering the forensic area for
baseline (Greiffenstein, Baker, & Johnson-Greene, the first time may treat written reports like clinical
2002), amplification of somatic problems reports. However, there are numerous differences
(Larrabee, 1998), and excessive subjective com- between the two, including differences in audi-
plaining (Greiffenstein, Baker, Gola, Donders, & ence, objectives, vocabulary, level of detail, source
Miller, 2002). A review of particular SVTs is attribution, and treatment of the unspoken
beyond the scope of this chapter and is covered in (Derby, 2001; Greiffenstein, 2008; Heilbrun,
more detail in another chapter in this volume. Marzcyk, & DeMatteo, 2002; Melton et al., 2007).
Neuropsychologists should employ proce- The differing matters of style and report content
dures to manage potential bias when conducting are summarized in Table 2.5 and discussed in
neuropsychological evaluations, interpreting more detail below.
results, and formulating expert opinions. Self The most obvious difference in report styles
examination and other debiasing procedures have is audience and the related difference of report
been discussed extensively elsewhere (Sweet & function. The clinician writes for physicians and
Moulthorp, 1999a, 1999b; Lees-Haley, 1999). mental health professionals, but the FN writes for

TABLE 2.5 DIFFERENCES BET WEEN CLINICAL AND FORENSIC


NEUROPSYCHOLOGY REPORTS

Report Characteristics Clinical Setting Forensic Setting

Audience Physicians, mental health professionals Attorneys, judges, hearing officers, claims
adjustors
Audience’s Main Medical Issues: Neurocognitive Narrow legal issues: Causation, damages,
Interest diagnosis, prognosis, treatment competency
Level of Detail Report all meaningful cognitive and Report details to establish important facts
personal characteristics to better and support main inferences; avoid
understand patient prejudicial details if not at issue
Vocabulary Freely use trait labels (“egocentric”), Use understandable terms, avoid technical
cognitive deficit terms jargon in narrative, although may be
(“perseveration”), diagnostic terms unavoidable when reporting scores.
(“dementia”), acronyms (“TIA”)
Controlling Mental Set Write as if the patient’s general welfare Both consider general welfare AND write
depends on your opinion as if you are under oath; be careful and
definitive
Causation Rarely considered, unknown for most State whether cause of action is a
mental disorders contributing factor to abnormal
cognitive findings
Fact Reporting Treat history as fact unless proven Stress the attribution of facts, e.g., “The
otherwise, e.g., “The patient became claimant described himself as disabled
disabled after the accident” after the accident” or “He reported
subjective disability after the accident.
Functional Patterns Predict activities of daily living Provide a nexus, i.e., explain the reasoning
linking test scores to predictions of
altered living skills
Diagnosis Fit the person to the diagnostic category, Fit the category to the person; describe in
i.e., formal DSM-IV diagnosis concrete terms (see text for examples)
Behavior Observations Typically brief, limited to factors relevant Longer and more detailed; correlate mental
to administration and scoring of tests status with test scores and legal claims
46 forensic neuropsychology

a lay audience with administrative and/or legal exposure/toxic substances (is/is not) a factor in
backgrounds. The main report function for a cli- the claimant’s neuropsychological presentation”
nician is neurocognitive diagnosis, psychological represents a nice balance between the need for a
diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment. This report simple causation statement and the need to
function is invariant across medical referral consider multiple factors.
sources. In contrast, attorneys, judges, claim Another important difference is characteriza-
adjustors, and juries are concerned with narrow tion of fact status. Clinical report writing requires
issues of law. In disability certification matters, for economy and an assumption of patient self-report
example, contract law dictates the scope of the accuracy. Hence, the reporting style may treat
referral questions, which may not include opin- unsupported statements as fact. A common intro-
ions of treatment. Thus, report functions can vary duction in clinical reports is the following: “The
widely. In general, the primary function of the FN patient comes for neuropsychological testing 8
report is to offer opinions on the link between months after a closed head injury.” This sentence
neuropsychological findings and the issue before assumes “closed head injury” is a valid diagnosis. In
the court. In personal injury cases, FNs address a forensic report, the FN must attribute unsup-
the issue of causation and damages but causation ported “factual sounding” statements to the source
may not be an issue in probate or criminal cases. to make it more factual. A more factual restatement
In criminal cases, there are six different types of of the lead-in sentence in a forensic context would
competence, including competence to plead be “The claimant comes for neuropsychological
guilty, waive right to counsel, and stand trial testing, believing/reporting/asserting/recounting
(Grisso, 1988, p. 3). The reader is referred to she suffered a closed head injury.” This protects you
Denney’s chapter on criminal contexts in the (especially plaintiff witnesses) from the accusation
present volume. In essence, the FN report writer in court that you prejudged brain damage when
needs greater mental flexibility in tailoring the you first laid eyes on the patient. Beginning alleged
report to the specific legal issues. statements of fact with “The claimant states” or
The most important difference between “reports” is more factual and objective.
clinical and forensic reports is the question of Another important difference is the type and
causality. Did the event in question (termed the range of detail. In a clinical report, the focus is on
“cause of action”) cause any alterations in cogni- describing all of the meaningful psychological and
tive (and presumably brain) function? Many juris- cognitive characteristics of the person. In contrast,
dictions rely on a “proximate cause” criterion, the forensic report should focus only on the most
meaning an act, event, or omission that results in relevant factors that contribute to a neuropsycho-
the damage claims made, though it may not nec- logical or psychological opinion. Grisso (1988)
essarily be the nearest or last cause. There is much discusses the different philosophies of forensic
legal controversy about what qualities an agent report writing and recommends “striking a bal-
must possess in relation to an outcome to deserve ance on detail.” The report should include enough
the title “cause” and we will not concern ourselves detail to establish important facts or track work-
with that discussion here. What FN should recog- ing hypotheses; it should not include all of the psy-
nize is that courts are very concerned with simple chological observations that could be made,
causal ideas such as “Did this car accident cause irrespective of how crucial such factors may be in
any cognitive or brain problems”? Most neuro- clinical settings. The mention of a psychologically
psychologists recognize there is rarely a single meaningful but legally irrelevant characteristic
cause for any psychological or neuropsychologi- may introduce bias into the report that is more
cal disorder, barring rare neurobehavioral syn- prejudicial than probative. When dealing with
dromes such as alexia or reduplicative paramnesia. emotionally charged details (e.g., sexual orienta-
Neuropsychologists are taught to think in multi- tion, religiosity, prior crimes), decide whether
factorial terms, and clinicians rarely address single they contribute to understanding of the plaintiff ’s
causes in their reports, if they address it at all. But (or criminal defendant’s) legal claims. If sexual
a forensic setting requires an answer to the ques- orientation is relevant (in a claim of organic per-
tion in a reasonably certain way. Remember the sonality change, for example) note it. If not (pain
standard of proof in personal injury cases is 51% disorder claim), strongly consider not reporting.
or better, meaning the FN needs only to state an FN must strive to communicate in plain lan-
opinion that takes the form of more likely than guage and limit use of technical terms. One mental
not. A FN’s statement that “the accident/mold exercise that may be useful to testifying experts is
Neuropsychology and the Law 47

to develop a “dictionary” of commonly used criminal intent, permanent work disability, and/
jargon that supplies “definitions” that lay people or marital problems. A patient’s conduct in the
can understand. This is especially useful prior to waiting room would not be mentioned in a rou-
trial. Table 2.6 contains a list of common neurop- tine clinical report, but an observation of a crimi-
sychological terms. For example, the phrase “the nal defendant reading a magazine in the waiting
protocol was characterized by strong persevera- room yet scoring at chance levels on the PIAT
tive tendencies in response to feedback” can be Reading Comprehension Test would be relevant
translated as “Mr. K showed an abnormal ten- in a competent waiver of Miranda rights issue. On
dency to repeat the same mistake, even when the other hand, a plaintiff who resists redirection
given clues. This indicates difficulty profiting from during the interview by repeating the same
experience”. topic during interview and also makes many
The clinical and FN reports also differ on the perseverative errors on the WCST shows better
importance of behavior observations. The clinical clinical correlation.
writer briefly comments on the gross neurological The approach to formal diagnosis also differs.
or cognitive functions necessary to support stan- In forensic settings, the reader should avoid strict
dard test administration, for example, does the categorization in the narrative (exclusive of the
respondent have functional vision and hearing? formal axial diagnosis) and use more descriptive,
In forensic settings, behavior observations are probabilistic sounding statements in describing
more comprehensive and detailed in order to pro- neuropsychological characteristics. Fit the cate-
mote convergent reasoning and to evaluate social gory to the person, not the person to the category
influences on behavior. Convergent reasoning is (Weiner, 1999). For example, in the case of find-
promoted through the correlation of mental ings favoring the plaintiff position, a conclusory
status observations with other data such as test statement such as “The accident caused closed
scores and objective injury characteristics. The head injury syndrome of 5 years’ duration” is not
allegation of neuropsychological damages always advisable. Instead, consider a statement such as
include claims of functional deficits, that is, “Mrs. Jones present difficulties in recalling new
disruption of daily activities or attitudes deemed information and subjective complaints resem-
important by society. Examples of functional bling those seen in persons with proven brain
impairments include capacity for forming tissue loss after blunt head trauma. The accident is

TABLE 2.6 PROPOSED LIST OF NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TERMINOLOGY


TRANSLATED INTO LAYMAN’S TERMS
Concept Technical Definition Layman Definition
Cerebral The superiority of one cerebral The right or left-brain is better at one form of
dominance hemisphere for processing specific mental processing than another.
tasks
Perseveration Persistence of response set in response Repeating the same mistake over and over despite
to negative feedback feedback
Executive Cognitive abilities necessary for A group of mental abilities concerned with
Functions goal-directed, error monitoring, decision-making and impulse control. These
perceptual-motor schemata selection, abilities stop you from burping loudly in church.
and adaptation to a range of
environmental conditions and
demands
Constructional A disturbance in visually guided A special difficulty in drawing, copying or building
dyspraxia constructional activity things, that often seen with right brain damage
Dementia A generalized loss of cognitive functions A brain disease in which many mental abilities are
resulting from cerebral disease lost, even though the patient remains alert and
occurring in clear consciousness attentive.
(i.e., absence of confusional state)
48 forensic neuropsychology

a factor in the present results.” If you discover have presented the conclusion as you did. An
findings that favor the defense, consider the fol- important caveat that supports a narrative style is
lowing: Make the negative diagnosis conditional, this: Write about a human being, not a set of
and then add a positive descriptive statement. For scores. The examination should include a vivid
example, “Although it is not possible for any one- description of the examinee as part of a mental
time examination to rule out closed head injury status examination that leaps from the page in
symptoms in the past, the present examination three dimensions.
does not contain any evidence for present neu- Attention to detail means you include those
ropsychological impairment. Mrs. Jones most facts/observations that track the questions, and
closely resembles persons who are neurologically supports the reader’s later appreciation of your
normal and enjoy a wide range of intact cognitive, conclusion. To maintain objectivity, include
perceptual, and motor abilities.” In this way, the details that help to understand the case, whether it
neuropsychologist provides differential certainty supports the side retaining you or not. If you feel
depending on time, that is, higher certainty about you need to account for the problematic points in
the present and lower certainty about the past. the conclusion, do so. Use quotes wherever you
Weiner (1999) describes this language promoting feel it illustrates a particularly informative or
inter-professional convergence of psychologists’ relevant aspect of your evaluation. Importantly,
style of probabilistic statements and attorneys’ make sure that you qualify everything the infor-
“preponderance of the evidence” standards. mant states with words like “reports,” “relates,”
A simple diagnosis and formal axial diagnosis “indicates,” or “notes.” Each is a neutral word
may be sufficient for clinical reports. However, in which neither downplays nor supports an infor-
a forensic report, providing a diagnosis to address mant’s words. This way you are quietly pointing
the causation issue is considered a “mere conclu- out the limits of hearsay in influencing your opin-
sory comment.” In the example given above of ion and supporting the need for corroborating
“closed head injury of 5 years’ duration,” there is data. Avoid using a quote that merely quotes
no stated nexus between the event 5 years earlier another. That is double hearsay and will be struck
and the conclusion. A nexus refers to a network of down by courts. Remember to refer to corrobo-
lucid links between the cause of action, diagnostic rating sources of information whenever possible,
methods, and the conclusion. In simplest terms, from records to deposition testimony, whatever
the FN must explain the reasons the plaintiff still you can utilize as indicative of supporting or
suffers cognitive impairments so many years refuting pertinent history.14
later. Simply diagnosing closed head injury on the Objectivity should be fairly self-evident. An
sole basis of obtaining abnormal results after objective tone is supported by also reporting
the incident is not logically supportable. This is details and scores that do not support hypotheses,
the logical error of post hoc, ergo propter hoc [after by explicitly stating alternative theories you con-
this, therefore because of this] (Larrabee, 1990). sidered, and avoiding extreme descriptors unless
As you prepare your draft, keep these general scores or behaviors are truly extreme. Confirmatory
objectives in mind: bias (discussed later) is your biggest enemy, and is
an issue that can scuttle even the best-reasoned
• Organization opinion. A commonly encountered example is
• Detail the FN who emphasizes weak memory scores
• Objectivity obtained after an accident, and then concludes
• Priorities these represent post-accident cognitive changes.
• Persuasion But the same FN ignores records showing poor
literacy, limited numerical skill, and a ninth grade
Organization requires treating every case as a education. This plaintiff may well have sustained a
story, so your writing should flow with a narrative lingering TBI, but omitting mention of pre-
structure. Begin with an introduction that pres- existing cognitive weaknesses can not only hurt
ents why you were retained in the first place and this neuropsychologist’s credibility, it can also
includes the questions you will answer in the nar- hurt the plaintiff ’s legitimate injury claim.
rative. Word them in such a way as they relate to However, in an effort to be objective, don’t under-
the questions you know you are answering at the mine your decisiveness. Lay out your details;
end. This way, the reader has a sense of closure when it comes time to gathering the material into
about the report he has just read, and why you an interpretation in the conclusion section, be
Neuropsychology and the Law 49

clear and forceful where you can be. If you have memory complaints, if not impaired brain func-
medical certainty, express it. Part of objectivity is tion? Statements about the impact of mood, age,
laying out what information is missing and speci- psychological syndromes, attribution style, physi-
fying what additional collateral information cal status, medication status, and social demand
would help to resolve uncertainty. features can be made if such opinions are grounded
Priorities become a big issue in more compli- in facts. The following opinion is an example of an
cated cases. Never lose sight of what you should alternative theory of complaints: “Mr. Doe’s
really be focusing on in order to educate the reader memory complaints are shaped by his sad mood
about why you came to your conclusions. Work and significant others’ tendency to over-focus on
with the retaining attorney to develop concise any mistake he makes, which results in mistrust of
hypothetical questions at the beginning, to stay his thought processes.” In this case, the FN’s evi-
on track and prevent the report from getting dence for this opinion was a history of remitting/
unwieldy. Complex cases often distract with many relapsing depression predating an accident, a cur-
compelling but peripheral details. Media accounts rent spike-2 MMPI-2 profile, an overly solicitous
of the case may make factual claims that are family, and a speech pathologist that treated low-
untrue, present advocacy as fact, introduce juicy average memory scores as evidence for TBI.
but irrelevant details, or may channel the views of The ethical aspects of the report-writing phase
advocates trying to inflame the jury pool against are controlled by Section 9.0 (Assessment) of the
one side of a case. Media accounts of the MMPI-2 2002 Code (APA, 2002). Ethical standard 9.06
Symptom Validity Scale were partly informed by (APA, 2002) implies an ethical reason for relying
financially interested parties and their lawyers on convergent evidence to interpret test scores.
(Armstrong, 2008). As you read over your report, Please refer to Grote’s chapter on ethics in this
make sure it builds from a neutral stance, so that volume for a more detailed discussion.
by the time the reader gets to the conclusions, it is
obvious not what you are going to say, but why The Trial Phase
you are going to say it. This section describes the process of taking and
Persuasion means marshalling a sufficient giving testimony.
number of facts, and integrating and interpreting
them with such clarity that the reader is convinced Trial Phase: Discovery
you made defensible conclusions. Persuasion does Discovery is a preliminary process for compiling
not mean using sophistry or empty eloquence to facts relevant to the case. All parties to a suit have
bamboozle. Some attorneys use excessive verbiage the statutory right to ask for all documents that
to “muddy the record” and their experts may go form the basis of an opinion, unless those docu-
along. If you write something in your conclusion ments are privileged. Discovery is designed to get
and the facts in your report had not already made an early look at the basis for the FN’s opinions,
it obvious to the reader, then you either have to narrow the dispute into main issues, gather perti-
include better explanations in your narrative or nent personal information, prematurely pin the
you may have come to the wrong conclusion. Both expert down, determine what the FN does not
of the present authors have many examples of know, and sample the witnesses behavior under
diagnostic conclusions that leap from the page scrutiny. Discovery takes one of two forms: an
precisely because nothing in the results section interrogatory (a typed list of questions) or a dis-
prepares the reader. A good example is describing covery deposition. A discovery deposition can
test scores as being within functional limits, but only be used for impeachment purposes at trial,
still concluding the patient is suffering disabling meaning it can be used to develop inconsistencies
brain injury. Another example is reporting poor to address at trial.
memory scores and passed effort tests, but still Neuropsychologists should expect their entire
concluding the patient is malingering. A large file to be subpoenaed. Your complete case file is
mismatch between the report body and the sum- discoverable (unless privileged), regardless if you
mary conclusions means either key details have are a treating or retained neuropsychologist. This
been neglected or the neuropsychologist is not includes interview notes, test forms, computer-
practicing competent neuropsychology. ized scoring reports, electronic correspondence,
Aim to provide a positive theory of the plain- records reviewed, notes, and billing statements.
tiff ’s behavior if your findings are negative for Although practices vary, most states have broad
impairment. What is a reasonable basis for their discovery rules.
50 forensic neuropsychology

Trial Phase: Admissibility injury in the plaintiff or not). FRE represents a


An important element of neuropsychologists’ broad liberalization of the term “expert,” and most
legal involvement is the admissibility of their neuropsychologists would have no difficulty being
data and opinions. Federal, state, and case law qualified under this rule. The judge is given
provide criteria, or “legal tests,” that determine broad discretion in applying admissibility rules.
what evidence is admitted into trial. Courts use Understanding admissibility guidelines is crucial
two analytic tools to determine the admissibility to a productive attorney-neuropsychologist inter-
of scientific opinions: The Frye rule and the action, as you must convince the attorney that
Daubert rule. your methodology and opinions will survive a
The Frye rule is presently applied only in 16 legal challenge. Challenges to neuropsychological
states.15 It is also termed the “general acceptance testimony may take three forms: (a) general com-
rule” and originates in a famous case styled as petence of psychologists to testify, (b) scope of
Frye v. U.S. (1923). For the next 70 years, chal- neuropsychological testimony, and (c) challenges
lenged experts had to show their method was gen- to particular neuropsychological methods and/or
erally relied upon by most practitioners in a tests.
particular professional community. The famous Challenges to general competence to testify
Daubert v. Merrell Dow (1993) Supreme Court are rare. Courts have recognized psychologists as
decision replaced Frye in many jurisdictions and competent experts since the Jenkins v. United
in federal court. Although much has been written States (1962) appellate court decision. In Jenkins
about Daubert, the ruling of law is very simple: the higher court held that mental health experi-
The Federal Rules of Evidence (FRE) are the only ence and training are most probative. A medical
legal basis for judging scientific admissibility in degree or its absence is insufficient grounds for an
federal courts. Under the FRE, in order for evi- admissibility ruling when a mental health fact is
dence to be admissible, scientific testimony must in legal dispute. Jenkins was cited in subsequent
meet two broad criteria: (a) must be scientifically cases involving neuropsychology evidence. In
valid and (b) relevant to the case at hand. In its Simmons v. Mullen (1974), a Pennsylvania appeals
obiter dicta (basically, a “discussion section” for court relied on Jenkins and the FRE to rule that
lawyers), the Daubert court offered trial judges a neuropsychologists could testify because they had
partial list of guidelines to evaluate scientific “reli- specialized knowledge about measuring cognitive
ability” (validity in our language). Currently, 30 change.
states either codified the FRE, or case law recog- Challenges to scope of neuropsychological
nized it as applicable. The reader is referred to testimony are more common but typically involve
Kaufmann’s chapter on evidentiary standards for only one issue: causation. Can the neuropsycho-
an in-depth review. This chapter will only focus logist reasonably infer brain changes from the
on Rule 702, which states: evidence? This body of case law contains mixed
rulings both adverse to and supportive of neuro-
If scientific, technical, or other specialized psychological testimony, depending on the court’s
knowledge will assist the trier of fact to under- location and type of brain damage claim. Case law
stand the evidence or to determine a fact in patterns indicate that closed head injury claims
issue, a witness qualified as an expert by knowl- are considered within the scope of expertise of a
edge, skill, experience, training, or education neuropsychologist (Hutchison v. American Family
may testify thereto in the form of an opinion or Mutual Insurance, 1994; Valiulis v. Scheffeos
otherwise, if (1) the testimony is based on suf- (1989). However, testimonial scope rulings appear
ficient facts or data, (2) the testimony is the more unfavorable to neuropsychologists in a dif-
product of reliable principles and methods, (3) ferent context: claims of neurotoxic brain injury.
the witness has applied the principles and In this body of law, neuropsychologists’ alleged
methods reliably to the facts of the case. ability to infer subtle (i.e., otherwise unobserv-
able) brain changes is regularly challenged, and a
This means an expert has training in areas that few cases of impact bear mentioning. In Schudel v.
the average juror or judge is not expected to General Electric (1995), plaintiffs proffered neu-
understand. The FN assists the juror (or judge) in ropsychological evidence for brain damage caused
either understanding the evidence (e.g., explain- by organic solvents and PCBs. The 9th Circuit
ing closed head injury) or determining a fact in Federal Appeals Court for the 9th Circuit ruled
issue (e.g., whether the car accident caused a brain neuropsychological testimony is limited to
Neuropsychology and the Law 51

damages but cannot address physical causation. cumulative empirical and anecdotal evidence
In Chandler Exterminators v. Morris (1992), the during 16 years of post-Daubert jurisprudence.
Georgia Supreme Court affirmed the exclusion An integrated reading of the Daubert trilogy and
of a neuropsychologist who linked neurotoxicants the FRE (1975) indicates the judge’s gatekeeping
to abnormal test scores. role is a liberal mandate, which encourages accep-
More likely are challenges to specific neuro- tance of even novel and recent methodologies, if
psychological tests and measures. This type of they are defensible on logical and empirical
challenge questions the scientific basis of one or grounds. Consider this: The Daubert elements are
more of the expert’s specific methods, measures, unweighted and polythetic criteria and they are
and/or conclusions. For example, a plaintiff not exclusive. This means no single element is
attorney may challenge the use of a particular necessary, and any single element or combination
malingering detection method, or a defense attor- of elements is sufficient to admit evidence.
ney may challenge reliance on a list of “brain Practically, this means the trial judge can rely on
damage signs.” Challenges to specific methods are only a single element if he chooses, for example,
brought under the Frye or Daubert (FRE) rules. the Frye “general acceptance” criteria alone.16
The psychology and neuropsychology com- Some federal courts have found that evidence is
munity’s initial reception of the “Daubert trilogy” admissible based on factors that are not included
was one of mixed feelings tinged with foreboding. in Daubert.
The earliest commentary articles speculated on There is much empirical evidence for the
Daubert’s impact. Grove and Barden (1999) nonthreat of Daubert to forensic neuropsychol-
warned that a number of diagnoses such as PTSD ogy. Neurocognitive symptom validity tests have
and multiple personality disorder would not sur- survived many Daubert challenges. Mossman
vive scrutiny, and Posthuma, Podrouzek, and (2003) conducted a LexisNexis™ search in
Crisp (2002) opined that Daubert poses a serious December 2002 and retrieved 18 published fed-
challenge to mild head injury cases, and Lees- eral and state cases referring by name to neu-
Haley, Iverson, Lange, Fox, and Allen (2002) rocognitive SVTs. Five of these cases involved
expected “many inadmissibilties” of MMPI-2 application of Daubert reliability factors to use of
validity scales. Taking a different tack, some com- neurocognitive SVTs and the courts found such
mentators saw opportunities to advance narrower tests admissible in all cases. More recently, Hoyt
interests. Reed (1996) interpreted Daubert to (2009) reviewed a series of 12 cases challenging
mean that only the “fixed” batteries (e.g., Halstead- the admissibility of the MMPI-2 symptom valid-
Reitan) would survive, but “flexible” test batteries ity scale (FBS) over the past decade, concluding
would be inadmissible. Reitan and Wolfson (2002) “the fake bad scale is here to stay.” In 2002, there
opined most neuropsychological measures are were 26 federal and state cases in the LexisNexis™
“lacking” in conformity to Daubert criteria, except database using the terms “neuropsychology” and
Reitan’s own test battery. Insofar as most neurop- “malingering.” Except for 2006, there has been a
sychologists adhere to a flexible battery approach pattern of accelerating growth in these cases cul-
and only a small minority advocate fixed batter- minating in a total of 88 published legal cases in
ies, these authors implied most neuropsycholo- 2009.
gists would not receive court work unless they Why the failure of dire predictions to come
bought the recommended test battery. true? The method skeptics or narrow practice
Our view is that Daubert has had minimal to advocates (those who warn that Daubert will only
no impact on neuropsychological practices. favor a narrow range of instruments) seem to
Greiffenstein (2009) and Kaufmann (this volume) confuse the important legal distinctions between
rebutted the idea that “only fixed batteries are admissibility and weight. Admissibility is a judge’s
admissible” through a fairer summary of case law. decision that evidence has probative value, that is,
We agree with Lally (2003) that psychologists is it relevant, can it determine the outcome of the
are better positioned to respond to Daubert case, and should the jury see it? The judge only
challenges than other classes of mental health analyzes the relevance of the evidence and does
professionals. We also agree with Shuman (2001) not make value judgments about relative impor-
that fears of Daubert’s impact on psychology tance. Hence, a test of modest validity has equal
were over-blown, and showed a misunderstand- footing with a test of great validity during an
ing of the legal system. Our optimism is also based admissibility hearing because both are probative.
on textual analysis of Daubert, combined with In contrast, “weight” is a jury decision and refers
52 forensic neuropsychology

to perception of the evidence’s importance and or affidavit) or in oral form through a deposition
believability after it is admitted. The confusion or in the courtroom. A deposition is a form of
between admissibility and weight stems from legal discovery in which litigants question wit-
Reed’s (1996) commentary piece on a bench trial nesses to determine what testimony they will offer
result (Chapelle v. Ganger) as proof of his conclu- at trial. A deposition is a very formal process in
sion, but even cursory study of the judge’s written which a court reporter transcribes questions and
decision revealed all neuropsychological testi- answers into a typed record. A “discovery” depo-
mony was admitted.17 Similarly, Reitan and sition is conducted solely by the opposing counsel
Wolfson’s (2002) and Russell’s (2007) conclusions and represents a preliminary effort to obtain
that only fixed test batteries are admissible are information about opinions, narrow the dispute
incorrect because it is based on such confusion. into the most pertinent issues, and to evaluate
They believe that perceived superiority is relevant potential courtroom demeanor of the FN. A dis-
to admissibility, but their arguments for superior- covery deposition cannot be introduced at trial
ity only go to weight. except to impeach the witness. A de bene esse
The FN must still be prepared to cope with deposition is more commonly referred to as a
occasional challenges to portions of the test “trial deposition,” and it is intended to preserve a
battery. Challenges to specific neuropsychological FN’s testimony if he or she is not available to
tests take the form of Daubert or Frye hearings appear live at trial. Alternatively, but more rarely,
held in limine. This means the court hears evi- the FN’s live testimony can also be offered at trial
dence pro and con away from the jury. The oppos- in front of the judge and jury. Rules defining
ing attorney files a motion asking the court to depositions and governing the use of deposition
exclude a particular test measure or a particular testimony vary among the states and the compe-
conclusion from admission. Most states have tent consultant should seek legal advice within
adopted the Daubert standards into their own the relevant jurisdiction.
rules of evidence, but many states still rely on Frye A trial deposition or live trial testimony is
or Frye hybrids (e.g., Frye-Davis in Michigan until broken into two phases: direct examination and
2005). The FN helps the retaining attorney pre- cross-examination. Direct examination occurs
pare the response (or provisions of the motion) by first and is defined as testimony elicited by the
applying the Daubert criteria to a particular test’s retaining attorney. The direct examiner first estab-
(or inference’s) knowledge base. The validity and lishes the FN’s qualifications to testify as an expert.
falsifiability of the WCST can be shown by citing Questions focus on five broad areas: education,
studies which correlate WCST performance with experience, specialized knowledge, skill, and aca-
frontal activity and/or damage (e.g., Boone et al., demic contribution. Evidence for qualification
1999; Pendleton & Heaton, 1982; Robinson, includes many forms of evidence, including aca-
Heaton, Lehman, & Stilson, 1980; Steinberg, demic degrees, neuropsychology coursework at
Devous, & Paulman, 1996) and the sensitivity and the graduate school level, nature of pre-doctoral
error rate can be shown with Robinson et al. and post-doctoral training, employment history,
(1980). The general acceptance of the WCST can relevant experience, continuing education work-
be shown through its mention in commonly used shops, professional society activity, pro bono ser-
test compendiums (Spreen & Strauss, 1991) or by vices, and publications. FNs should prepare
citing test user surveys. For example, Butler, themselves for this approach by briefly outlining
Retzlaff, and Vanderploeg (1991) reported 73% of the features of their background that support their
respondents use the WCST routinely. The reader self-designation. Greiffenstein (2002) offered an
is referred to Kaufmann’s separate chapter (this objective checklist approach for attorneys to select
volume) on scientific admissibility law. neuropsychologists, but FNs can adopt this same
approach to prepare a list of qualifying attributes
Trial Phase: Deposition or Live Testimony for the retaining attorney to use during direct
Once the admissibility of neuropsychological examination.
methods is established, the neuropsychologist- Opposing counsel may on occasion challenge
attorney interaction moves to the next step: The credentials during direct examination by asking
FN offers oral or written opinions under oath for the court to voir dire the FN. Voir dire means, “to
scrutiny by opposing counsel and the trier of tell the truth” and is a preliminary examination of
fact (judge or jury). Sworn testimony can be competence to testify through additional ques-
offered in written form (interrogatory responses tions about credentials. The trial judge then
Neuropsychology and the Law 53

decides whether your testimony is allowed or not. consistently show that nonverbal communication
Qualification as a witness is rarely a problem for is more persuasive with the jury (Boccacini &
FNs. In addition, the FRE (1975) 702-705 (and Brodsky, 2002; Cramer, Brodsky, & DeCoster,
the related state evidentiary rules) give broad 2009).
latitude to trial judges to consider many factors, The best general style is defining your role as
including experience in the absence of publica- one of educating the jury. The corollary of this is
tion or board certification. In summary, the voir never being an advocate for either party, even as
dire does not pose a major threat in most cases. you defend the neuropsychological methods you
However, opposing attorneys can continue to applied in the case. You are not there to persuade,
emphasize questionable or insufficient credentials entertain, advocate, to crusade for justice, pursue
during cross-examination. quixotic ideals, or to validate your own sense of
The direct examination continues after the FN self-importance. Your objectivity will be mea-
has been qualified. The questions are open ended sured by how you educate the jury in principles of
but designed to elicit brief and simple opinions neuropsychology and how you applied those
favorable to the retaining attorney. Most retaining principles to the issue at hand. You advance this
attorneys use the FN’s report outline and head- style by responding deliberately without excessive
ings to organize questions, which mirror the same qualification, patiently explaining definitions in
temporal sequence of steps taken by the FN before simple terms without using technical words, and
issuing a final report. Cross-examination begins not making gratuitous assumptions about the fact
immediately following the conclusion of direct finder’s technical knowledge. Be honest about
examination. The opposing counsel asks ques- your experience, knowledge, and the neuropsy-
tions from two general categories: (1) questions chological literature. Jurors are likely to come
designed to elicit weakness or flaws in the FN data from more modest backgrounds than the expert,
gathering or logic, and (2) questions designed to but they don’t need an advanced degree to recog-
prove bias or lack of independence. nize puffery, manipulation, and gross violations of
Productive attorney-neuropsychologist inter- laws of physics when they hear it.
actions during trial phase cannot be managed or Another stylistic element is maintaining
controlled in the same way as during any pretrial objectivity. This does not mean you can’t have a
stage. The FN’s interactions with retaining and point of view or that you should never be passion-
opposing attorneys are now public. The attorneys ate. Despite cherished myths to the contrary, do
and court rules govern your behavior. Hence, the not brag that your objectivity is proven by state-
FN should focus on their style of interacting with ments such as “I do 50% work for plaintiffs and
the judge or jury. The cross-examining attorney 50% for defense.” A 50/50 split can easily be
has the right to ask questions and receive respon- evidence for the opposite: You give a favorable
sive answers. Responsive means the answer is rel- opinion to the side that reaches the telephone
evant; it does not mean you have to give the first. If you commonly give opinions favoring
answer desired. There is no legal requirement that certain positions over others, this could actually
FN must answer “yes” or “no,” despite some attor- be more objective if your opinion percentages
neys desire to have you believe this. Brief narra- approximate the base rate for a given condition,
tive answers are allowable. diagnosis, or phenomenon. For example, if you
Productive attorney-neuropsychologist inter- happen to find that 40% of minor head injury
actions depend on the elements of expert witness litigants show positive malingering signs on
control. These are behaviors and attitudes you memory tests, this would be more objective given
have wide latitude in determining. The FN has the high prevalence of malingering in late post-
control over nonverbal behaviors (demeanor and concussive claimants (Binder & Willis, 1991;
gaze); agency (active vs. passive answering), Gianoli, McWilliams, Soileau, & Belafsky, 2000;
speech characteristics (pace and volume), speech Greiffenstein, Baker, & Gola, 1996). For example,
content (scope of answers, definitions offered), Mittenberg, Patton, Canyock, and Condit (2002)
manifest attitude (deliberate, unbiased), and gen- surveyed forensic neuropsychologists represent-
eral style of communication (objectivity, advo- ing a pool of 33,000+ forensic cases and reported
cacy, educator). The general elements of expert a 40% base rate of invalid effort. Given the high
witness control refer to a consistent style of com- prevalence of invalid effort, the FN who never
munication across your entire testimony. While diagnoses or even suspects invalid effort should
verbal content of testimony is important, studies be considered biased. If you find permanent brain
54 forensic neuropsychology

dysfunction in 95% of the mild TBI litigants you but also responds to the latent content, and selec-
examine, these conclusions may be biased, as they tive active responding to neuropsychology con-
go against the base rate for residual deficits in tent-specific questions is what differentiates the
mild head trauma (Binder, Rohling, & Larrabee, effective witness from the less-effective one. This
1997). Conversely, if you never find evidence of interchange between a defense attorney and plain-
permanent brain dysfunction in persons who tiff ’s neuropsychologist shows an active answer in
have taken over 30 days to follow commands, you response to a general question:
may also show bias, as persisting deficits are
common in persons with less periods of coma Q: So abnormal neuropsychological test scores
(Dikmen et al., 1995). The point is that witness are seen in persons with normal brains?
bias is not measured by simply calculating the
number of plaintiff versus defense (or prosecution A: It is true that a few abnormal test scores are
versus defense) referrals. common, but Ms. Jones’ high number and
Nonverbal behaviors are also important for pattern of abnormal scores led to my diagnosis
attorney-neuropsychologist interactions during of brain problems.
the trial phase. Maintain consistency of demeanor
across both the direct and cross-examination In this case, the active answer addressed the
phases. It is helpful to consider the familiar psy- imprecision of the question, namely, ambiguity of
chological concept of “examiner characteristics,” number (How many abnormalities can a normal
in which the tester’s personality, biological traits, person produce?). The latent issue raised by the
and nonverbal behavior influence responses to question was what criteria differentiate abnormal
psychological tests. This applies equally to the brain states from non-neurological explanations.
courtroom and entails the characteristics of the The answer “yes” may leave misimpressions with
questioning attorney influencing your responses. the trier of fact, such as (1) this plaintiff scored
That is because direct and cross-examinations abnormally on only a few measures, (2) the expert
contain entirely different questions, methods, and never considered alternative explanations, or
aims associated with different personalities. The (3) the witness was biased towards finding brain
direct examination elicits evidence to support an damage. The point is that active answers are allow-
opinion and the cross-examination elicits testi- able, control misimpressions, educate the trier of
mony that disproves it. The opposing attorney fact, and provide a lucid link between general
limits you with closed questions. The FN has no questions and the specific matter at hand.
procedural right to dictate the content of direct or Always anticipate difficult questions designed
cross-examination. The FN does have control of to trap, embarrass, or impeach you. There are
demeanor. This means consistency in tone, pos- many popular questions, termed gambits,
ture, gaze, prosody, and movement. For example, designed to make you look ill-informed, stupid,
when retaining attorney’s direct examination is biased, or out of touch with mainstream neurop-
done, turn in the witness chair and squarely face sychology. While such questions can be legitimate
the opposing attorney. This signals you are giving and such characteristics could accurately apply to
as much attention to the opposing side as you did you, this chapter assumes a conscientious and
the retaining side. Continuing to stare in the old knowledgeable NP. The following represents a
direction appears dismissive and inattentive. small list of the most common gambits you should
There is an extensive and evolving scientific always be prepared for. These tactics include the
literature on these topics, for example, Neal and false alternative, learned treatise, upsetting the
Brodsky (2008), that is beyond the scope of this witness, and false conceit gambits.
chapter, The gambit of false alternatives asks the FN to
The FN may also control the scope of answers. choose between two explanations, usually phrased
Two types of answers are relevant: passive and in an either/or manner. For example, after estab-
active answers. Passive answering means respond- lishing that the plaintiff passed effort tests, his
ing only to the overt, literal meaning of the ques- attorney asks, “Now doctor, either my client is
tion. Responding with your name and title when malingering or they are brain damaged, correct?”
asked to introduce you is an example. The effec- This statement assumes (note: latent content) that
tive FN engages in active answering whenever malingering and cerebral dysfunction is mutually
possible to handle more difficult questions. An exclusive, and the courtroom-unfamiliar neurop-
active answer is responsive to the overt question, sychologist may think that courtroom procedure
Neuropsychology and the Law 55

dictates choosing only those alternatives. The best some expert witness guides advise you against
response is one that rejects the underlying this. One general approach is to acknowledge a
premise while simultaneously educating the trier book’s relative importance or personal usefulness
of fact, for example, “The absence of faking does (“It’s an important text that I sometimes rely
not prove brain damage. Good effort only means upon…”), while simultaneously denying over-
the test scores are valid, and Mr. Smith produced arching authority (“…but nothing in neuropsy-
normal memory scores.” Upsetting the witness is chology is authoritative”). The expert may answer,
an attempt to raise the frustration level of the “That text is one authority in the field, but I have
witness. There is a popular maxim that governs not recently read every chapter and section con-
attorney behavior: “If you have the facts on your tained therein.” Another way is to provide answers
side, pound the facts. If you have the law on your that inform the jury that sound scientific and
side, pound the law. If you have neither facts nor clinical practices never rest in any single individ-
law on your aside, pound the table!” Some attor- ual or any single book. This is particularly true in
neys try to provoke expert witnesses with inap- neuropsychology. Hence, in response to questions
propriate assaults in an attempt to elicit an overly about a book or specific journal, the FN witness
emotional response. When an attorney attacks might respond “With over 100 journals in neu-
(“badgering” or improper argumentative ques- ropsychology and neurology coming out every
tioning is legally objectionable by the retaining month, and thousands of articles in general
attorney), assume your opinions are so unass- psychology, no one article is that important.”
ailable that the attorney has nowhere to go. A In response to the “authoritative person” variant,
common problem for experts is that not all retain- reasonable answers may be as follows:
ing attorneys will know to object, nor will all trial
“Thousands of men and women have contrib-
judges sustain an objection to unfair questions.
uted to the field of neuropsychology, and I recog-
The FN must be prepared to cope with situations
nize Dr. Legend as one of those contributors.”
in which attorneys are given unusual latitude. You
Or
cannot offer legal objections yourself.
“Dr. Legend is an important contributor to
The FN must recognize the learned treatise
neuropsychology but I don’t necessarily agree
gambit and its variants. This is a line of question-
with everything he has written.”
ing during which the FN is confronted with neu-
ropsychology texts, journal articles, published In cases where the opposing attorney wants
commentary, and even interview transcripts with you to agree or disagree with a statement he/she
well-known neuropsychologists. The attorney has read, another tactic is to ask for the book/
tries to get the expert to acknowledge this book/ article/document that the attorney is holding. The
article/author/sentence/paragraph as “authorita- opposing counsel usually offers you a document
tive.” A common question is “Don’t you agree that containing a highlighted paragraph, sentence, or
Dr. Ima Legend’s book Neuropsychological Testing in some cases just a half-sentence. Politely demur
for Idiots is authoritative?” or “You have to agree and state that although you are able to read, it
that Dr. Ima Legend is one of the great authorities would have no meaning unless you could read the
in neuropsychology.” The courtroom-unfamiliar whole chapter. It is very common for an attorney
expert does not know the legal ramifications of to read a sentence out of context, even though the
agreeing that a treatise is authoritative: Accepting entire paragraph or chapter may make clear the
a learned treatise means you must agree with every sentence has a meaning other than the one
statement in the document. The eager-to-please intended. Could you get a 60-minute break to
witness who answers “yes” will next be treated to read the whole chapter? In the appropriate con-
a list of inconsistencies between his opinion and text, more courtroom-familiar witnesses can fine
the alleged authoritative text. tune the answer to powerfully demonstrate to the
The general principle is to refuse the gambit. jury that even alleged “authorities” frequently
This is both honest and scientifically defensible change their opinion. Going back to the first ques-
but often difficult to do. Unless the expert is a tion, an answer might be, “Given that Dr. Lezak
polymath and absolute master of a particular has published many different editions of her book
book or paper, you should always refuse to answer and updated her opinions, it can’t be authorita-
with a simple “yes.” To maintain consistency, your tive. Which of the updated editions of Lezak did
must even refuse to acknowledge your own publi- you have in mind?” However, this is a gambit
cations as authoritative in the legal sense, even if where assertiveness does not cause the attorney to
56 forensic neuropsychology

beat a hasty retreat. The attorney may persist in an that the question is unnecessarily tangential and
effort to make the witness look evasive, pompous, likely irrelevant.
or out of touch with scholarship. Unlike other The trial phase poses its own ethical chal-
gambits, the FN should be prepared for persistent lenges. Neuropsychologists must present well-
questions designed to make you look out-of- founded opinions that do not misstate or distort
touch. principles of neuropsychology (Standard 2.04
Courtroom-familiar and knowledgeable of the 2002 Code, Basis for Scientific and
experts face the false conceit gambit. This line of Professional Judgments). It is inevitable that some
questioning implies the plaintiff is special and not opinions will be outliers to mainstream practices,
subject to the general expectations of persons but this is not necessarily unethical. The main-
with the same claimed etiology. Questions may stream or consensus opinion is not automatically
focus on length of time in evaluation (“His the best or most valid one. FRE 702-705 combined
treating doctor saw him 25 times, but you just saw with the Daubert (1993), Kumho (1999), and
him once?”) and the uncertainties of whether a Joiner (General Electric v. Joiner, 1997) court
particular test or principle applies to this person decisions were designed to allow novel but poten-
(“The Test of Memory Malingering has never tially provable (“falsifiable”) approaches into the
been validated in transsexual railroad workers, courtroom.
has it?”). The FN’s strategy is to answer these ques- Sometimes outlying opinions are so
tions by presenting the bases for generalizing neu- unfounded, and in some instances outrageous,
ropsychological techniques to diverse populations. that appellate courts limit neuropsychologists’
Stress the basic commonalities among all patients scope of testimony. In the case of Grenitz v. Tomlian
from a neurological standpoint. For example, in (2003), the Florida Supreme Court withdrew from
the case of memory disorder, you would stress earlier legal precedent that gave neuropsycholo-
that all patients irrespective of social class or race gists’ broad scope in causation testimony. In
or illness share basic commonalities, including Grenitz, plaintiff ’s expert claimed that the Object
the fact they have brains with right and left halves, Assembly subtest of a Wechsler IQ test was able to
and the parts of the brain that control memory differentiate in utero from intrapartum brain
formation are basically invariant from one person damage. Some medical organizations took action
to the next with rare exception. Also stress the against such testimony on the grounds that testi-
representativeness of your test norms. Witness mony is an extension of practicing medicine and
this interchange: thus subject to oversight. Two neurosurgeons (in
separate jurisdictions) were sanctioned for pur-
Q: Isn’t it true that you have never performed portedly misstating practice standards (Albert,
neuropsychological testing on railroad work- 2002). The U.S. Supreme Court upheld the profes-
ers exposed to organic solvents, nor have you sional societies’ right to discipline members for
published anything about it? improper courtroom testimony (Austin v. AANS,
2002; also reported by Adams, 2002). To date,
A: Although it is true this is the first time I saw there is no parallel action by any neuropsychology
a railroad worker, neuropsychological tests are organization.
designed for use with a wide variety of patients
with suspected brain disorders. Post-Trial Phase
An issue that receives little attention in expert
The expert in this case has accomplished witness guidebooks is post-trial considerations.
multiple things: She has answered honestly about Your involvement should not end after verdict.
her lack of experience with railroad workers This should be a time of reflection. First, never
claiming solvent exposure (responsive to the call the retaining attorney to find out the verdict.
surface question). But she also educated the jury Strong interest in the outcome of a trial means
on latent issues such as (a) stressing long and deep you have an emotional stake in winning and
experience with many different kinds of patients, losing. Second, engage in an honest self-appraisal
(b) the generalizability of neurocognitive mea- of your methods and testimony. This requires rec-
sures to different communities, and (c) deflating ollection of the cross-examination. Was the cross-
the question’s false conceit of the “special plaintiff examination skillful? Did the aggressive questions
understood only by my expert.” She has also pro- uncover genuine weaknesses in your reasoning,
vided a professional “so what” answer, indicating or were they merely designed to assassinate your
Neuropsychology and the Law 57

character? An honest self-appraisal not only a closed head injury from a long list of symptoms,
improves future testimony, it can also inform your but ignores the widespread nature of complaints,
routine clinical work. For example, the author is engaging in confirmatory bias by overlooking
(MFG) used to rely heavily on qualitative signs as other possibilities, such as somatization disorder,
evidence for brain impairments. After a few histrionic personality, or malingering. The same
unpleasant depositions, it became evident that applies to a defense-retained neuropsychologist,
these “signs” were sometimes seen in the context who when confronted with the same list, con-
of normal aggregate scores. Another area for cludes only malingering. The DSM-IV-TR (APA,
self-appraisal is reliance on Verbal–Performance 2000) provides a reasonable guide to differential
IQ differences. A typical over-interpretation is diagnosis when the FN is confronted with a long
“Mr. Doe’s PIQ is 15 points lower than his VIQ, list of symptoms or other challenging presenta-
proving traumatic damage to the right brain.” tions.
Actually, Verbal–Performance IQ splits are the Some methods for self-assessment should
rule and not the exception among healthy be avoided. Colbach (1981) recommended
persons (Kaufman, 1990; Matarazzo, 1972; calculating a Validity Quotient (VQ). This is cal-
Matarazzo, Bornstein, McDermott, & Noonan, culated by dividing the total number of court
1986). After that point, I (MFG) relied more on a decisions into the number of court decisions
quantitative approach, using process neuropsy- where opinion and verdict matched. Hence, if
chology methods as an additional tool, not a com- your opinion and the jury’s have matched every
plete approach. time, the VQ would be 100%. The present authors
The post-trial phase is the best time for an strongly advise against using this method. The
“integrity check” (Brodsky, 1999). This is a self- VQ requires calling the retaining attorney to get
appraisal of one’s objectivity and fairness. the verdict. This is nothing more than the pull of
Maintaining objectivity requires a number of affiliation, a wish to be “on the winning side.”
steps, including awareness of the pull to affiliate Second, the VQ assumes that a legal verdict vali-
with the retaining side, and double checking for dates or invalidates a psychologist’s opinion. But
the error of confirmatory bias. A simple ratio of many factors go into a trier-of-fact’s decision-
plaintiff to defense cases is not compelling evi- making processes, including the competency of
dence for objectivity versus partisanship. It is a attorneys, the likability of litigants, other expert
reality that an FN’s career trajectory increasingly witness testimony, liability, and other legal issues
attracts retention by one side more than another. having nothing to do with an FN’s work. Belief in
There are many reasons for this, including word of the VQ may betray underlying grandiosity, that
mouth, aggressive versus conservative neurodiag- somehow the FN’s opinion is always the most
nostic approaches, and scientist-practitioner critical in determining a fact at issue. If you
ethos versus pure clinical orientation. In criminal strongly believe in the VQ, you may be prone to
contexts, many psychologists testify for the the “star witness” mentality.18
defense because psychologists’ sympathies often The best method for estimating objectivity
lie with mentally disturbed offenders. Reject the is to correlate one’s opinions with known
idea that any kind of “bias” is automatically bad. base rates for certain conditions. Of course,
All experts have biases towards a type of conclu- estimating base rates is art as well as science.
sion or method of analysis, and these should be The idea is to determine whether one finds
readily acknowledged. The human mind was not cognitive dysfunction related to the cause of
designed to be neutral, and there are many built- action much more often than the base rates allow
in cognitive biases that serve perception, atten- or less cognitive dysfunction (or psychopathol-
tion, and decision-making (Haselton & Buss, ogy) than one would expect. One could think
2003). There are, however, biases that negatively about this issue in the same way attorneys do:
influence forensic work product. A judge’s basic fairness is not determined if
One problematic bias common in forensic he sustains plaintiff or defense’s objections in
psychology is “confirmatory bias.” This is the ten- a 1:1 ratio. That’s not fairness, that’s just score
dency to search only for evidence consistent with keeping! Instead, fairness is determined how close
a diagnosis, but ignore or minimize evidence the judge’s rulings adhere to the letter of law.
inconsistent with a conclusion (Davies, 2003; Hence, a fair judge could still easily rule 90% in
Iverson, Brooks, & Holdnack, 2008). A plaintiff ’s favor of plaintiff and 10% in favor of defense in a
expert who selects only symptoms consistent with particular case.
58 forensic neuropsychology

CONCLUDING COMMENTS The authors wish to emphasize that an ency-


Practicing competent forensic neuropsychology clopedic knowledge of the law is not necessary to
requires two integrated approaches: aspirational engage in productive and effective expert witness
behavior and avoidance of committing wrongful work. There are, however, key texts and papers
acts (Hess, 1999b). Avoidance of wrongful acts that are indispensable. Melton, Petrila, Poythress,
means the FN provides professional services Slobogin, and Otto’s third edition of Psychological
that are sensitive to jurisdictional law, with Evaluations for the Courts (2007) is a veritable
ethical propriety and moral integrity. Personal encyclopedia of forensic knowledge. The book
preference may invoke analogies based on the provides summaries and analysis of landmark
seven deadly sins, for example, avoiding sloth legal cases, demystifies legal terminology, reviews
by writing timely neuropsychology reports. many forensic assessment instruments that
Aspirational behavior means constant examina- address cognitive capacities, and devotes much
tion of each act or report concerning its implica- attention to the ethical and legal details of assess-
tions for personal development, professional ment. The important teaching from this book is
integrity, and our system of justice in civil society. that the FN’s report does not have to be a compre-
The aspirational approach encourages excellence hensive evaluation of every last aspect of an exam-
in our profession and science. Some attorneys do inee’s cognitive function. For example, it is
a great service to forensic neuropsychology pointless and potentially harmful if you give a
practice with heavy scrutiny of our assessment comprehensive neuropsychological test battery
methods and diagnostic reasoning. A good cross- on a question of competent waiver of Miranda
examination can reveal genuine inadequacies in Rights. Melton et al. (2007) offer no separate
assessment techniques, such as an insufficient chapter on neuropsychology, but the intelligent
normative base for a test, failure to examine symp- reader can use this book to make extrapolations
tom validity, and classification hit rates no better to neuropsychology work.
than base-rate guessing. Cross-examination can Forensic report writing requires sensibilities
only improve our pursuit of good test instruments and formatting different from clinical reports.
and competent practices. Aspirational goals There are many good casebooks to choose from,
include prevention of the misuse of FNs’ work. depending on level of experience and the issue to
Newspapers have been filled with bizarre legal be addressed. Heilbrun, Marzcyk, and DeMatteo
defense theories in sensational crimes, and some (2002) casebook is a primer in case studies for
neuropsychology experts have contributed every known forensic context. This includes
regretfully to such defenses. For example, one FN worker’s compensation, death penalty mitigation,
opined that a mass killer was legally insane competence to be sentenced, and many other con-
because of “subtle frontal lobe damage,” while texts. Each chapter author is strongly associated
simultaneously ignoring the defendant’s well- with the diagnostic question, and in some cases,
organized efforts to cover up the crime and evade developed the specialty. The appendix of Melton
detection for over a year. State insanity statutes et al. (2007) also contains excellent, but briefer
typically require evidence of severe cognitive reports. Heilbronner’s (2005) Forensic Neuro-
dysfunction or severe emotional dysregulation to psychology Casebook data-mines prototypical
support a conclusion of legal insanity. The respon- cases from the files of the country’s best known
sible FN must not only weigh the body of scien- FNs, while Sweet and Morgan (2009) focus solely
tific knowledge, he must apply this knowledge to on questions of malingering and data validity.
all relevant behaviors displayed by a defendant Donders’ (2001a, 2001c) survey details typical
before agreeing to support an unusual medico- report writing practices within professional
legal theory. neuropsychology.
Table 2.7 represents a distillation of the ideas Legal libraries should be limited to cases of
in this chapter. It is similar to the “Rules of Road” impact and pertinent local law. Every neuro-
tables that the late Ted Blau created for forensic psychologist should have copies of the Daubert
psychologists (Blau, 1998). The Table contains trilogy. The Daubert, Joiner, and Kumho cases
both the aspirational and moral underpinnings can be downloaded from Cornell Law School’s
mentioned by Hess (1999b). The reader may wish website: http://supct.law.cornell.edu/supct/index.
to add his or her own ideas. html. If you are interested in researching federal
Both budding and experienced FNs may wish and state appellate cases relevant to neuropsy-
to maintain a basic resource library. chology, the LexisNexis™ service allows relatively
Neuropsychology and the Law 59

TABLE 2.7 SUMMARY OF FIVE PRINCIPLES OF PRODUCTIVE


AT TORNEYNEUROPSYCHOLOGY RELATIONS, WITH EX AMPLES
Knowledge of Legal Bases
• Understand, accept, and adjust to structural conflicts between legal and scientific outlooks.
• Recognize manageable specific conflicts that arise from time to time and develop specific response strategies,
e.g., third-party observation request.
• Be familiar with the essentials of the Federal Rules of Evidence and Civil Procedure, the Daubert Trilogy,
the Frye decision, and case law applicable to neuropsychological testing.
• Hold strong presumption against third-party observation of testing phase and test manual release, but
final decision to proceed/decline depends on nature of legal action and observation conditions.
Practice Competent Neuropsychology
• Adhere to scientist-practitioner model.
• Data collection is ideally a tripod consisting of outside records, history and behavioral observations,
and test scores.
• Recognize the fallibility of each method in isolation.
• Justify selection of all measures on basis of peer-reviewed studies; admissibility law does NOT require
relying on a fixed test battery.
• Design test battery to target both legal and clinical issues raised by a particular case.
• Avoid use of single or homemade tests, and diagnosis of brain damage on symptoms alone.
Support Board Certification
• Complete appropriate board certification (the authors recommend the oldest, largest, and most-widely
recognized boards associated with the American Board of Professional Psychology (ABPP).
• Acknowledge importance of open peer review and practice standards.
• Reject bogus issues such as “elitism” or “academic–clinical” artificial dichotomies as excuses for avoiding
certification.
Adhere To Ethical Principles
• Understand 2002 Ethics Code both generally and the specific principles relevant to forensic applications.
• View the Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychology as a model for desirable behavior.
• Recognize the potential ethical traps associated with each phase of the attorney-neuropsychologist
interaction.
• Avoid “pull of affiliation” by recognizing that you do not have to do everything an attorney
asks you to do.
Be Courtroom Familiar
• Rigorously maintain mental set of “educator to trier of fact” when testifying.
• Maintain consistency of gaze, demeanor, volume, and prosody during both direct and cross-examination.
• Practice “active answering” to questions to educate the jury and avoid misimpressions; no legal authority
requires answering just “yes” or “no.”
• Recognize common legal gambits and develop acceptable means of responding.
• Respond with tactics only when they are genuinely called for; do not overuse.

inexpensive downloading of individual opinions criminal consulting, the 2002 Atkins decision is
at http://www.lexisnexis.com/. The recently important.
published 2003 Federal Rules of Evidence and Neuropsychologists frequently ask the authors
Federal Rules of Civil Procedure are available about specific responses to especially difficult direct
through West Publishing at http://west.thomson. and cross-examination questions. Coping with
com, although there has been little modification cross-examination is partly science, but mostly
over the first publication in 1975. For those doing art. Stanley Brodsky’s three books (Brodsky, 1991,
60 forensic neuropsychology

1999, 2004) on psychological testimony are essen- 3. Personal attacks are part of the territory in
tial tools, especially the first: Testifying in Court: forensic work. There is an old law school maxim: When
Maxims for the Expert Witness. They are powerful you have the law on your side, pound the law; when
tools for providing genuine, responsive, and you have the facts, pound the facts; when you have nei-
respectful answers to the most difficult questions. ther, pound the table.
The Faust, Ziskin, and Hiers (1991) volume Coping 4. Remittitur is designed to cure an award of
with Brain Damage Claims provides guidelines for damages that is grossly excessive without the necessity
litigation consultation, such as developing basic of a new trial or an appeal.
5. Aggressive attorneys are demanding test manu-
cross-examination questions. The appendices
als with greater frequency in some jurisdictions.
contain large libraries of stock cross-examination
Florida is one example.
questions for which every FN should be familiar.
6. In the first edition, Greiffenstein and Cohen used
But the book has it biases, such as being strongly the terms “exceptionalism versus legal primacy” camps.
civil-defense oriented in its advice. The material is 7. However, Judge Woodlock added, “this holding
also too dated because it predates Daubert, leaves open the possibility that a more focused and
appeared prior to the revolution in effort and conditioned disclosure would be acceptable to the
validity testing, and the book focuses on tests that Court. The most common resolution for this type of
are no longer accepted by competent neuropsy- dispute has been some compromise between full,
chologists, for example, Draw-A-Person. In short, unconditioned disclosure and total exemption from
most competent neuropsychologists are skeptics the Federal Rules of Civil Procedure.” (p. 8). Judge
of the methods used in an earlier era. Woodlock issued a protective order that restricted the
Since the first edition, the trend toward neu- release of psychological test materials in Taylor, even
ropsychologists as the preferred brain-behavior though the Edison court expressed reservations about
experts in legal cases has become a presumption. such orders, with the majority noting concern about
Growth in forensic consulting for neuropsy- intentional violations and the minority acknowledging
chologists is outpacing every related brain-behav- problems from inadvertent disclosure.
ior expertise and that growth is accelerating. The 8. The judge appropriately noted that North
authors recommend that you be patient, remain Carolina recognized no test security privilege and that
objective, and let your reports speak to the quality under conflict of law analysis, North Carolina substan-
of your work; do not advertise your forensic ser- tive law controls, absent a specific claim of privilege.
vices. Always be a prepared, competent, credible, Nevertheless, the judge cited Kaufmann (2009) and an
APA editorial, as important considerations. The judge
and professional witness. Know your client, know
ultimately denied plaintiff ’s motion to quash a defense
what they need, know what you can and cannot
subpoena for raw test data, and ordered the plaintiff-
do for them, and communicate directly and forth-
retained psychologists to release data to opposing
rightly, even when you know it is something that counsel.
may not help their case. Always be true to your 9. A commonly held myth is that only subpoenas
scientist–practitioner training. Neuropsychologists signed by a judge need be honored. The reality is that
who practice competently, avail themselves to in most jurisdictions, judges have delegated much of
forensic consulting, and follow the suggestions in their subpoena authority to attorneys. In Michigan for
this chapter, will find that forensic consulting is a example, the typed signature of a court clerk is suffi-
rewarding way to diversify practice and develop cient to render a subpoena valid. That is not to say that
new revenue in an increasingly stagnant/con- the FN has no rights in response to a subpoena. It is
stricted market for general clinical services. In important to respond to every subpoena, but this does
offering reliable services to the legal profession, not mean comply with every demand.
neuropsychology supports the truth-seeking func- 10. In August 2009, the APA Council of
tion of the judiciary, promotes justice, protects the Representatives proposed a change to the Code,
profession, and serves public policy. removing some ambiguous language from the section
addressing the “raw data” problem.
N OT E S 11. The majority opinion was written by Scalia,
1. Human reasoning, unlike computers, is capable who noted “attention may wander where the eyes
of abduction: inference to the best explanation, among don’t.” The APA wrote an amicus brief supporting
many competing ones. defendant. This amicus was unusual for a scientific
2. The Florida Board of Psychology recently tight- organization, because it cited only case law, and did not
ened guidelines for marketing specializations and include any scientific references or psychology practice
board certifications standards.
Neuropsychology and the Law 61

12. Attorney’s perceptions of experts provide good Medical News. Retrieved September 2002 from
examples of advocacy. Plaintiff attorneys uniformly http://www.ama-assn.org/sci-pubs/amnews/
term the neuropsychologists they retain as “treating, pick_02/prse0204.htm.
objective doctors,” but those hired by the defense are Adams, K. H. (2002). Personal communication,
“all biased.” Defense attorney’s view “treating” neurop- September. Dr. Adams was present during the APA
sychologists as the biased ones, because they are advo- Council Meeting vote.
cates by nature, and may also have a financial interest in Albers, J. & Schiffer, R. (2007). Features of the neuro-
the outcome of the trial, e.g., the FN has a lien on the logical evaluation that suggest noncredible perfor-
case. In their view, defense experts are more objective mance. Assessment of malingered neuropsychological
because they are paid irrespective of who wins or loses. deficits. (pp. 312): Oxford University Press:
13. The SGFFP are now under revision and may New York.
be in force when this book goes to press. See http:// Albers, J. W., Wald, J. J., Garabrant, D. H., Trask, C. L.,
www.ap-ls.org/aboutpsychlaw/SpecialtyGuidelines. & Berent, S. (2000). Neurologic evaluation of work-
php for updates. ers previously diagnosed with solvent-induced
14. Your extra-test records may be “Bates stamped” toxic encephalopathy. Journal of Occupational and
(a numerical page reference used by courts), so put the Environmental Medicine, 42, 410–23.
Bates number in parentheses after key facts. This makes Albert, T. (2002, April 8). American Medical News. On
it easier to find supporting documents during testi- the hot seat: Physician expert witnesses. Retrieved
mony, rather than struggling to find pages while in the September 2002 from http://www.ama-assn.org/sci-
hot seat. pubs/amnews/pick_02/prsa0408.htm#rbar_add.
15. States accepting Frye include Alabama, American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology,
Arizona, California, Colorado, the District of Colum- (2001). Special presentation. Policy statement on
bia, Florida, Illinois Kansas, Maryland, Minnesota, the presence of third party observers in neuro-
Mississippi, New Jersey, New York, Pennsylvania, and psychological assessment. The Clinical Neuro-
Washington. psychologist, 15, 435–39.
16. After hearing Daubert, the Supreme Court American Psychiatric Association, (1994). Diagnostic
remanded the case back to the trial judge to apply the and statistical manual of mental disorders (Fourth
new standard to the facts. The trial judge again rejected ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
plaintiff ’s expert testimony on the same grounds: The American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic
testimony was not valid or relevant for lack of any gen- and statistical manual of mental Disorders (Fourth
eral acceptance. Note that the Frye rule is one of the Edition)-Text Revision. Washington DC: Author.
Daubert dicta for the trial court to consider. Dicta are American Psychological Association. (1992). Ethical
like the discussion section of a paper, but not the formal principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
ruling of law. American Psychologist, 47, 1597–1611.
17. Chapelle v. Ganger was a bench trial of a brain American Psychological Association (2002). Ethical
injury claim. The judge’s written decision indicated principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
that even partial Halstead-Reitan battery protocols American Psychologist, 57, 1060–73.
from two other neuropsychologists were admissible. Anastasi, A. & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological testing
This fact alone rebuts Reed and Reitan’s assertions. (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-
Defense counsel moved for a Daubert hearing only to Hall, Inc.
bar the testimony of a vocational specialist who offered Andrew, G., Benjamin, H., & Kazniak, A. (1991). The
speculative testimony that the minor plaintiff would discovery process: Deposition, trial testimony and
never work. hearing testimony. In H. O. Doerr & A. S. Carlin,
18. A defense attorney once called a co-author (Eds.), Forensic neuropsychology (pp. 17–32). New
(MFG) to announce the jury awarded plaintiff a large York: Guilford.
sum of money. He stated, “After verdict, they said they Archer, R. P., Buffington-Vollum, J. K., Stredny, R. V., &
liked your down-to-earth manner, and didn’t believe Handel, R. W. (2006). A survey of psychological
plaintiff had a significant head trauma. But they really test use patterns among forensic psychologists.
hated the claims adjustor and my accident reconstruc- [doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_07]. Journal of
tionist” The point is that doing a competent and ethical Personality Assessment, 87(1), 84–94.
job is not automatically associated with any specific out- Armstrong, D. (2008, March 5, 2008). Malingering test
come. Do not judge your work quality by legal outcome. roils personal injury law. Wall Street Journal Online,
pp. 1–5.
REFERENCES Committee on Psychological Tests And Assessment,
Adams, D. (2002, February 4). U.S. Supreme Court of the American Psychological Association
denies appeal of physician discipline case. American (2008). Statement on Third Party Observers in
62 forensic neuropsychology

Psychological Testing and Assessment: A Frame- Binder, L. M., Rohling, M. L., & Larrabee, G. J. (1997). A
work for Decision Making. Retrieved June 27, Review of mild head trauma: I. Meta-analytic review
2001, from http://www.apa.org/science/programs/ of neuropsychological studies. Journal of Clinical &
testing/third-party-observers.pdf Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 432–57.
Austin v. Am. Ass’n of Neurological Surgeons, 253 F.3d Binder, L. M., & Willis, S. C. (1991). Assessment of
967 (7th Cir. 2001). motivation after financially compensable minor
Axelrod, B., Barth, J., Faust, D., Fisher, J., Heilbronner, head trauma. Psychological Assessment, 3, 175–81.
R., Larrabee, G., et al. (2000). Presence of Blau, T. H. (1998). The psychologist as expert witness
third party observers during neuropsychological (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
testing: Official statement of the National Academy Boccaccini, M. T., & Brodsky, S. L. (2002). Believability
of Neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical Neuro- of expert and lay witnesses: Implications for trial
psychology, 15, 379–80. consultation. Professional Psychology: Research and
Babitsky, S. & Mangraviti, J. J. (1997). How to excel Practice, 33, 384–88.
during cross-examination. Techniques for experts Bond, C. F., & Titus, L. J. (1983). Social facilitation:
that work. Falmouth, MA: S.E.A.K, Inc. Press. A meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychological
Ball, D. (2008). Making preponderance work. Trial, Bulletin, 94, 265–92.
44(3), 35–43. Boone, K. (Ed.). (2008). Assessment of feigned cognitive
Barr, W. (2007). Historical development of the neurop- impairment. New York: Guilford.
sychological test battery. In J. E. Morgan & J. H. Boone, K. B., Miller, B. L., Lee, A., Berman, N., Sherman,
Ricker (Eds.), Textbook of clinical neuropsychology. D., & Stuss, D. T. (1999). Neuropsychological pat-
New York: Psychology Press. terns in right versus left frontotemporal dementia.
Barsky, A. J. (2002). Forgetting, fabricating, and tele- Journal of the International Neuropsychological
scoping. The instability of the medical history. Society, 5, 616–22.
Archives of Internal Medicine, 102, 981–84. Brodsky, S. L. (1991). Testifying in court. Guidelines and
Baxter v. Temple, No.01–c–0567 (N.H. Sup. Ct. Aug. 8, maxims for the expert witness. Washington, DC:
2005) (order granting motion in limine). American Psychological Association Press.
Baxter v. Temple, 949 A.2d 167, (N.H. 2008). Brodsky, S. L. (1999). The expert expert witness.
Bauer, R. (1994). The flexible battery approach to neu- Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
ropsychological assessment. In R. D. Vanderploeg Press.
(Ed.), Clinician’s guide to neuropsychological assess- Brodsky, S. L. (2004). Coping with cross-examination and
ment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. other pathways to effective testimony: Washington,
Behnke, S. (2003). Release of test data and APA’s DC: American Psychological Association.
new Ethics Code. Monitor on Psychology, 34(7), Brodsky, S. L. & Robey, A. (1973). On becoming and
no pagination. expert witness: Issues of orientation and effective-
Benton, A. L. (1992). Clinical neuropsychology: ness. Professional Psychology, 3:173–76.
1960–1990. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Bush, S. S. (2007). Ethical decision making in clinical
Neuropsychology, 14, 407–417. neuropsychology: New York, NY: Oxford University
Bigler, E. D. (2007). A motion to exclude and the ‘fixed’ Press.
versus ‘flexible’ battery in ‘forensic’ neuropsychol- Bush, S. S., Grote, C. L., Johnson-Greene, D. E., &
ogy: Challenges to the practice of clinical neuro- Macartney-Filgate, M. (2008). A panel interview
psychology. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, on the ethical practice of neuropsychology. The
22, 45–51. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22(2), 321–44.
Bigler, E. D. & Ehrfurth, J. W. (1980). Critical limita- Butler, M., Retzlaff, P. D., & Vanderploeg, R. (1991).
tions of the Bender Gestalt Test in clinical neuro- Neuropsychological test usage. Professional
psychology: Response to Lacks. International Psychology: Research & Practice, 22, 510–512.
Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2, 88–90. Butler, J. L. & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The trouble with
Bigler, E. D. & Ehrfurth, J. W. (1981). The continued friendly faces: Skilled performance with a support-
inappropriate singular use of the Bender Visual ive audience. Journal of Personality & Social
Motor Gestalt Test. Professional Psychology: Psychology, 75, 1213–30.
Research & Practice, 12, 562–69. Chandler Exterminators, Inc. v. Morris, 416 S.E.2d 277
Binder, L. M. (1997). A review of mild head trauma: II. (Ga. 1992).
Clinical implications. Journal of Clinical & Chapple v. Granger, 851 F. Supp. 1481 (E.D. Wash.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 421–31. 1994).
Binder, L. M. & Johnson-Greene, D. (1995). Observer Ceci, S. J., Bruck, M., & Battin, D. B. (2000). The
effects on neuropsychological performance: A case suggestibility of children’s testimony. In D. F.
report. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 74–78. Bjorklund (Ed.), False-memory creation in children
Neuropsychology and the Law 63

and adults: Theory, research, and implications Davies, M. F. (2003). Confirmatory bias in the
(pp. 169–201). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum evaluation of personality descriptions: Positive
Associates. test strategies and output interference. [doi:10.1037/
Chelune, G. & Heaton, R. (1986). Neuropsychology 0022-3514.85.4.736]. Journal of Personality and
and personality tests and their relation to patient’s Social Psychology, 85(4), 736–44.
complaints of disability. In Goldstein & Tarter Derby, W. N. (2001). Writing the forensic neuro-
(Eds.), Advances in clinical neuropsychology (Vol. psychological report. In C. G. Armengol, E. Kaplan,
3). New York: Plenum Press. & E. J. Moes (Eds.), The consumer-oriented
Chiperas v. Rubin, No. 96-130, 1998 WL 765126. neuropsychological report (pp. 203–24). Lutz, FL:
Cline v. Firestone Tire, 118 F.R.D. 588 (S.D.W.Va. PAR.
1988). Detroit Edison Co. v. NLRB, 440 U.S. 301 (U.S. 1979).
Colbach, E. M. (1981). Integrity checks on the witness Dikmen, S. S., Temkin, N. R., Machamer, J. E., &
stand. Bulletin of the American Academy of Holubkov, A. L. (1994). Employment following
Psychiatry and Law, 9, 285–88. traumatic head injuries. Archives of Neurology, 51,
Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic 177–86.
Psychology (1991). Specialty guidelines for foren- Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., Winn, H. R., & Temkin,
sic psychologists. Law and Human Behavior, 15, N. R. (1995). Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year
655–665. post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 80–90.
Committee on Psychological Tests and Assessment, Doerr, H.O. (1991). Issues in initial contact:
American Psychological Association. (2007). Neuropsychologist-Attorney-Patient. In H. O.
Statement on third party observers in psychological Doerr & A. S. Carlin (Eds.), Forensic neuropsychol-
testing and assessment: A framework for decision ogy (p. 40). New York: Guilford.
making. Washington, DC: Author. Doerr, H. O. & Carlin, A. S. (1991). Forensic neuropsy-
Constantinou, M., Ashendorf, L., & McCaffrey, R. J. chology. New York: Guilford, p. 242.
(2002). When the third-party observer of a neu- Donders, J. (2001a). A survey of report writing by
ropsychological evaluation is an audio-recorder. neuropsychologists, I: General characteristics and
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16, 407–412. content. [doi:10.1076/clin.15.2.137.1893]. The
Constantinou, M. & McCaffrey, R. J. (2003). The effects Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15(2), 137–49.
of third party observation: When the observer is a Donders, J. (2001b). A survey of report writing by
video camera. Paper presented at the 23rd Annual neuropsychologists, I: General characteristics and
Convention of the National Academy of content. [doi:10.1076/clin.15.2.137.1893]. The
Neuropsychology, Dallas, TX. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15(2), 137–49.
Constantinou, M., Ashendorf, L., & McCaffrey, R. J. Donders, J. (2001c). A survey of report writing by neu-
(2005). Effects of a third party observer during ropsychologists, II: Test data, report format, and
neuropsychological assessment: When the observer document length. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
is a video camera. Journal of Forensic Neuro- 15(2), 150–61.
psychology, 4, 39–47. Drogin, E. Y. & Barrett, C. L. (2003). Substituted judg-
Covner, B. J. (1944). Studies in phonographic ment: Roles for the forensic psychologist. In A. M.
recordings of verbal material: IV. Written reports Goldstein (Ed.), Comprehensive handbook of psy-
of interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, 28(2), chology: Vol. 11. Forensic psychology (pp. 301–312).
89–98. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Cramer, R. J. & Brodsky, S. L. (2007). Undue influence Dvoskin, J. A. (2002). Knowledge is not power –
or ensuring rights? Attorney presence during foren- Knowledge is obligation. Journal of the American
sic evaluations. Ethics and Behavior, 17, 51–60. Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 30, 533–40.
Cramer, R. J., Brodsky, R. L., & DeCoster, J. (2009). Dyer v. Trachtman, 470 Mich 45; 679 NW2d 311
Expert witness confidence and juror personality: (2004).
Their impact on credibility and persuasion in the Essig, S. M., Mittenberg, W., Peterson, R. S., Strauman,
courtroom. Journal of the American Academy of S., & Cooper, J. T. (2001). Practices in forensic neu-
Psychiatry and the Law, 37(1), 63–74. ropsychology: Perspectives of neuropsychologists
Cripe, L. (2002). Limitations of records reviews. and trial attorneys. Archives of Clinical Neuro-
Division of Clinical Neuropsychology Newsletter 40, psychology, 16, 271–91.
20, 7–8. Faust, D. (1995). The detection of deception. Neurologic
Cronbach, L. (1984). Essentials of psychological testing Clinics, 13, 255–65.
(4th ed.). New York: Harper & Row. Faust, D., Ziskin, J, & Hiers, J. B. (1991). Brain damage
Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc., 509 U.S. 579 claims: Coping with neuropsychological evidence.
(1993). Los Angeles: Law and Psychology Press.
64 forensic neuropsychology

Federal rule of civil procedure (1975). St. Paul, MN: Greiffenstein, M. F. (2002). Selecting a neuropsycholo-
West Publishing. gist as an expert witness. Michigan Bar Journal, 81,
Federal rules of evidence for United States courts and 45–46.
magistrates (1975). St. Paul, MN: West Publishing. Greiffenstein, M. F. (2003). Neuropsychology research
Fenichel, O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of out of a private practice setting. In G. L Lamberty
neurosis. New York: W. W. Norton. (Ed.), The practice of clinical neuropsychology
Fisher, C. B. (2003). Release of test data and the new (pp. 125–42). New York: Swets & Zeitlinger.
APA ethics code. American Psychology Law Society Greiffenstein, M. (2008). Basics of forensic neuro-
News, 23, 1–6. psychology. In J. E. Morgan & J. Richter (Eds.),
Florida Dept. of Trans. v. Piccolo, 964 So. 2d 773 (Fla. Textbook of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 905–41).
Cir. Ct. 2nd, 2007). New York: Psychology Press.
Frankle, A. H. (1995). A new method for detecting Greiffenstein, M. F. (2009). Clinical myths of forensic
brain disorder by measuring perseveration in neuropsychology. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
personality responses. Journal of Personality Assess- 23(2), 286–96.
ment, 64, 63–85. Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Gola, T. (1996).
Franzen, M. D. & Iverson, G. L. (2006). Detecting Motor dysfunction profiles in traumatic brain injury
negative response bias and diagnosing malinger- and postconcussion syndrome. Journal of the
ing: The dissimulation exam. In Clinical neuropsy- International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 477–85.
chology: A pocket handbook for assessment, 2nd ed. Greiffenstein, M. F. & Baker, W. J. (2001). Comparison
(pp. 102–119): Washington, DC: American Psycho- of premorbid and postinjury MMPI-2 profiles in
logical Association. late postconcussion claimants. The Clinical Neuro-
Frye v. United States, 293 F. 1013 (D.C. Cir. 1923). psychologist, 15, 162–70.
Gavett, B. E., Lynch, J. K., & McCaffrey, R. J. (2005). Greiffenstein, M. F., & Baker, W. J. (2003). Premorbid
Third party observers: The effect size is greater clues? Preinjury scholastic performance and
than you might think. Journal of Forensic Neuro- present neuropsychological functioning in late
psychology, 4, 49–64. postconcussion syndrome. The Clinical Neuro-
General Electric Co. v. Joiner, 522 U.S. 136 (1997). psychologist, 17, 561–73.
Gelso, C. J. (1974). Effects of recording on counselors Greiffenstein, M. R., & Baker, W. J. (2006). Miller was
and clients. Counselor Education and Supervision, (mostly) right: Head injury severity inversely
14(1), 5–12. related to simulation. Legal and Criminological
Gervais, R. O., Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Wygant, D. B. (2009). Psychology, 11, 131–45.
Empirical correlates and interpretation of the Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Johnson-Greene, D.
MMPI-2-RF Cognitive Complaints (COG) Scale. (2002). Actual versus self-reported scholastic
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23(6), 996–1015. achievement of litigating postconcussion and
Gianoli, G., McWilliams S., Soileau, J., & Belafsky P. severe closed head injury claimants. Psychological
(2000). Posturographic performance in patients with Assessment, 14, 202–208.
the potential for secondary gain. Otolaryngological, Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., Gola, T., Donders, J.,
Head and Neck Surgery, 122, 11–8. & Miller, L. (2002). The Fake Bad Scale in atypical
Glass, L. S. (1991). The legal base in forensic neuropsy- and severe closed head injury litigants. Journal of
chology. In H. O. Doerr & A. S. Carlin, (Eds.), Clinical Psychology, 58, 1591–600.
Forensic neuropsychology (pp. 3–16). New York: Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., Fox, D., Boone, K., &
Guilford. Tsushima, W. (2010). MMPI-2 validity scores in
Green P., Iverson, G. L., & Allen, L. (1999). Detecting defense- versus plaintiff-selected examinations:
malingering in head injury litigation with the Word A repeated measures study of examiner effects.
Memory Test. Brain Injury, 13, 813–9. Published on-line, February 15. The Clinical
Greenberg, S. A. & Shuman, D. W. (1997). Irreconcilable Neuropsychologist.
conflict between therapeutic and forensic roles. Greiffenstein, M. F. & Cohen, L. (2005). Principles of
Professional Psychology: Research & Practice, 28, productive attorney-neuropsychologist interac-
50–57. tions. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.), Forensic neuro-
Greenberg, S. A. & Shuman, D. W. (2008a). Irre- psychology. New York: Oxford.
concilable conflict between therapeutic and foren- Grenitz v. Tomlian, 858 So. 2d 999 (Fla. 2003).
sic roles Ethical conflicts in psychology (4th ed.) Griggs v. Duke Power Co., 401 U.S. 424 (1971).
(pp. 492–98). Washington, DC: American Grisso, T. (1988). Competency to stand trial. A manual
Psychological Association. for practice. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resources
Greenberg, S. A., & Shuman, D. W. (2008b). When worlds Exchange, Inc.
collide: Therapeutic and forensic roles Ethical conflicts Grove, W. M. & Barden, R. C. (1999). Protecting the
in psychology (4th ed.) (pp. 505–506). Washington, integrity of the legal system: The admissibility of
DC: American Psychological Association. testimony from mental health experts under
Neuropsychology and the Law 65

Daubert/Kumho analyses. Psychology, Public Policy, neuropsychology. In Neuropsychology in the court-


& Law, 5(1), 224–42. room: Expert analysis of reports and testimony
Hare, R. D. & Neumann, C. S. (2006). The PCL-R (pp. 243–66). New York: Guilford Press.
assessment of psychopathy: Development, struc- Iverson. G. L., Lange, R. T., Brooks, B. L., & Rennison,
tural properties, and new directions. In Handbook L. A. (2009). “Good old days” bias following mild
of the psychopathy (pp. 58–88). New York, NY: traumatic brain injury. The Clinical Neuro-
Guilford Press. psychologist, 24, 17–37.
Harris v. Kreutzer, 624 S.E.2d 24 (Va. 2006). Jenkins v. United States, 307 F.2d 637 (D.C. Cir.
Haselton, M. G. & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error manage- 1962).
ment theory: A new perspective on biases in cross- Johnson-Greene, D., Dehring, M., Adams, K. M.,
sex mind reading. [doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.81]. Miller, T., Arora, S., Beylin, A., et al. (1997).
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), Accuracy of self-reported educational attainment
81–91. among diverse patient populations: A preliminary
Haselton, M. G. & Buss, D. M. (2003). Biases in social investigation. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
judgment: Design flaws or design features? In J. P. 12, 635–43.
Forgas, K. D. Williams, & W. von Hippel, (Eds.), Kagehiro, D. (1990). Defining the standard of proof in
Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes (pp. jury instructions. Psychological Science, 1, 194–200.
23–43). New York: Cambridge University Press. Kaufman, A. S. (1990). Assessing adult and adolescent
Heaton, R., Smith, H. H., Lehman, R. A., & Vogt, A. T. intelligence. New York: Allyn & Bacon.
(1978). Prospects for faking believable deficits on Kaufmann, P. M. (2005). Protecting the objectivity,
neuropsychological testing. Journal of Clinical and fairness, and integrity of neuropsychological evalu-
Consulting Psychology, 5, 892–900. ations in litigation: A privilege second to none?
Heilbronner, R. L. (2005). Forensic neuropsychology Journal of Legal Medicine, 26(1), 95–131.
casebook: New York: Guilford Press. Kaufmann, P. M. (2008). Admissibility of neuro-
Heilbrun, K., Marzcyk, G. R., & DeMatteo, D. (Eds.). psychological evidence in criminal cases:
(2002). Forensic mental health assessment. A case- Competency, insanity, culpability, and mitigation.
book: Oxford. In Clinical neuropsychology in the criminal forensic
Heltzel, T. (2007). Compatibility of therapeutic and setting (pp. 55–90). New York: Guilford Press.
forensic roles. Professional Psychology: Research Kaufmann, P. M. (2009). Protecting raw data and psy-
and Practice, 38, 122–128. chological tests from wrongful disclosure: A primer
Hess, A. K. (1999a). Defining forensic psychology. on the law and other persuasive strategies. The
In A. K. Hess & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), The handbook Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23(7), 1130–59.
of forensic psychology (Second ed.) (pp. 24–47). Kaufmann, P. M. (2010). Personal communication as
New York: John Wiley & Sons. noted on AACN list serve item “Detroit Edison
Hess, A.K. (1999b). Practicing principled forensic update.”
psychology: Legal, ethical and moral consider- Kehrer, C. A., Sanchez, P. N., Habif, U., Rosenbaum,
ations. In A. K. Hess & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), The J. G., & Townes, B. D. (2000). Effects of a signifi-
handbook of forensic psychology (Second ed.) cant-other observer on neuropsychological test
(pp. 673–99). New York: John Wiley & Sons. performance. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 14,
Hess, A. K. & Weiner, I. B. (1999). The handbook of 67–71.
forensic psychology (Second ed.) (p. 756). New York: Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, 526 U.S. 137 (1999).
John Wiley & Sons. Lacks, P. (1982). Continued clinical popularity of the
Howe, L. L. S. & McCaffrey, R. J. (2010). Third party Bender-Gestalt Test: Response to Bigler and
observation during neuropsychological evaluation: Ehrfurth. Professional Psychology: Research &
An update on the literature, practical advice for Practice, 13, 677–80.
practitioners, and future directions. The Clinical Lally, S. J. (2003). What tests are acceptable for use in
Neuropsychologist, 24, 518–37. forensic evaluations? A survey of experts. Professional
Howe, L. L. S., Sweet, J. J., & Bauer, R. M. (2010). Psychology: Research & Practice, 34, 491–98.
Advocacy 101: A step beyond just explaining: How Larrabee, G. J. (1990). Cautions in the use of neuro-
the individual practitioner can become involved psychological evaluation in legal settings. Neuro-
and make a difference. The Clinical Neuro- psychology, 4, 239–47.
psychologist, 24, 373–90. Larrabee, G. J. (1998). Somatic malingering on the
Hoyt, R. D. (Fall, 2009). Is the Fake Bad Scale test here MMPI and MMPI-2 in personal injury litigants.
to stay? Mass Torts, 8, 14–15, 23. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 179–88.
Hutchison v. American Family Mutual Ins. Co., 514 Larrabee, G. J. (2003). Detection of malingering using
N.W.2d 882 (Iowa 1994). atypical performance patterns on standard neuro-
Iverson, G. L., Brooks, B. L., & Holdnack, J. A. (2008). psychological tests. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
Misdiagnosis of cognitive impairment in forensic 17(3), 410–25.
66 forensic neuropsychology

Larrabee, G. J. (Ed.). (2008). Assessment of malingered Digit Span in traumatic brain injury. Assessment, 9,
neuropsychological deficits. New York: Oxford. 301–308.
Larrabee, G. J. & Berry, D. (2007). Chapter 2. Diagnostic Matarazzo, J. D. (1972). Wechsler’s measurement
classification studies and diagnostic validity. and appraisal of adult intelligence (5th Edition).
In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.), Assessment of malingered New York: Oxford University Press.
neuropsychological deficits (pp. 14–26). New York: Matarazzo, J. D. (1990). Psychological assessment
Oxford. versus psychological testing: Validation from Binet
Larrabee, G. J., Greiffenstein, M. F., Greve, K. W., & to the school, clinic, and courtroom. American
Bianchini, K. J. (2007). Refining diagnostic criteria Psychologist, 45, 999–1017.
for malingering Assessment of malingered neuro- Matarazzo, J. D., Bornstein, R. A., McDermott, P. A., &
psychological deficits (pp. 334–71). New York: Noonan, J. V. (1986). Verbal IQ versus Performance
Oxford University Press. IQ difference scores in males and females from the
Larrabee, G. J., Millis, S. R., & Meyers, J. E. (2009). WAIS-R standardization sample. Journal of Clinical
40 plus or minus 10, a new magical number: Reply Psychology, 42, 965–74.
to Russell. [doi:10.1080/13854040902796735]. The Matarazzo, J. D. & Herman, D. O. (1985). Clinical uses
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23(5), 841–49. of the WAIS-R: Base rates of differences between
Larry P. v Riles, 793 F.2d 969 (9th Cir. 1984). VIQ and PIQ in the WAIS-R standardization
Lees-Haley, P. (1999). Commentary on Sweet and sample. In B. B. Wolfman (Ed.), Handbook of
Moulthrop’s debiasing procedures. Journal of intelligence (pp. 899–932). New York: Wiley.
Forensic Neuropsychology, 1, 43–57. McCaffrey, R. J., Fisher, J. M., Gold, B. A., & Lynch, J. K.
Lees-Haley, P. R. & Brown, R. S. (1992). Biases in (1996). Presence of third parties during neuropsy-
perception and reporting following a perceived chological evaluations: Who is evaluating whom?
toxic exposure. Percept Mot Skills, 75(2), 531–44. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, 435–49.
Lees-Haley, P. R., Smith, H. H., Williams, C. W., & McCaffrey, R. J., Lynch, J. K., & Yantz, C. L. (2005).
Dunn, J. T. (1996). Forensic neuropsychological Third party observers: Why all the fuss?
test usage: An empirical survey. [doi:10.1016/0887- [doi:10.1300/J151v04n02_01]. Journal of Forensic
6177(95)00011-9]. Archives of Clinical Neuro- Neuropsychology, 4(2), 1–15.
psychology, 11(1), 45–51. McCauliff, C. M. A. (1982). Burdens of proof: Degrees
Lees-Haley, P. R. & Courtney, J. C. (2000). Reply to the of belief, quanta of evidence, or Constitutional
commentary on “Disclosure of tests and raw test guarantees? Vanderbilt Law Review, 35, 1293–335.
data to the courts.” Neuropsychology Review, 10, McKinzey, R. K. & Ziegler, T. G. (1999). Challenging a
181–82. flexible neuropsychological battery under Kelly/
Lees-Haley, P. R., Iverson, G. L., Lange, R. T., Fox, D. Frye: A case study. Behavioral Sciences & the Law,
D., & Allen, L. M., III. (2002). Malingering in 17, 543–51.
forensic neuropsychology: Daubert and the MMPI- McSweeny, A. J. (1997). Regarding ethics in forensic
2. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 3(1–2), neuropsychological consultation: A comment on
167–203. Guilmette and Hagan. The Clinical Neuro-
Lees-Haley, P. R., Smith, H. H., Williams, C. W., & psychologist, 11, 291–93.
Dunn, J. T. (1996). Forensic neuropsychological Meehl, P. E. (1954). Clinical vs. statistical prediction:
test usage: An empirical survey. Archives of Clinical A theoretical analysis and a review of the evidence.
Neuropsychology, 11, 45–51. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota
Loring, D. (1999). International Neuropsychology Press.
Society (INS) dictionary of neuropsychology. New Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., Slobogin, &
York: Oxford University Press. Otto, R. (2007). Psychological evaluations for the
Lumsden v. United States, No. 7:06-CV-60-F (E. Dist. courts. A handbook for mental health professionals
N.C., Jan. 15, 2010) (order granting release of raw and lawyers. (3rd edition). New York: Guilford
data and test materials under a protective order). Press.
Manes, F., Sahakian, B., Clark, L., Antoun, N., Robbins, Miller, H. & Cartlidge, N. (1972). Simulation and
T., Aitken, M., et al. (2002). Decision-making pro- malingering after injuries to the brain and spinal
cesses following damage to the prefrontal cortex. cord. Lancet, 1, 445–52.
[doi:10.1093/brain/awf049]. Brain: A Journal of Minner v. American Mortgage & Guaranty Co., 791
Neurology, 125(3), 624–39. A.2d 826 (Del. Super. Ct. 2000).
Mathias, C. W., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Houston, Mitrushina, M.N., Boone, K. B., & D’Elia, L.F. (1999).
R. J., & Crouch, J. A. (2002). Detecting malingered Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological
neurocognitive dysfunction using the Reliable assessment. London: Oxford University Press.
Neuropsychology and the Law 67

Mittenberg, W., DiGiulio, D. V., Perrin, S., & Bass, Category Test. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38,
A. E. (1992). Symptoms following mild head 392–96.
injury: Expectation as etiology. Journal of Neuro- Posthuma, A., Podrouzek, W., & Crisp, D. (2002). The
logy, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 55, 200–204. implications of Daubert on neuropsychological
Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit, evidence in the assessment of remote mild trau-
D. C. (2002). Base rates of malingering and matic brain injury. American Journal of Forensic
symptom exaggeration. Journal of Clinical & Psychology, 20(4), 21–38.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 1094–1102. Ragge v. MCA/Universal Studios, 165 F.R.D. 605
Mossman, D. (2000a). Interpreting clinical evidence (C.D.Cal. 1995).
of malingering: A Bayesian perspective. Journal Rapp, D. L., Ferber, P. S., & Bush, S. S. (2008).
of the American Academy of Psychiatry & Law, 28, Unresolved issues about release of raw test data
293–302. and test materials. In A. M. Horton, Jr. &
Mossman, D. (2000b). The meaning of malingering D. Wedding (Eds.), The neuropsychology handbook
data: Further applications of Bayes’ theorem. (3rd ed.) (pp. 469–97). New York: Springer.
Behavioral Sciences and Law, 18, 761–79. Reed, J. E. (1996). Fixed vs. flexible neuropsychological
Mossman, D. (2003). Daubert, cognitive malingering, test batteries under the Daubert standard for the
and test accuracy. Law and Human Behavior, 27, admissibility of scientific evidence. Behavioral
229–49. Sciences & the Law, 14, 315–22.
NAN Policy and Planning Committee (2000). Test Rabin, L. A., Barr, W. B., & Burton, L. A. (2005).
security: Official position statement of the National Assessment practices of clinical neuropsycho-
Academy of Neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical logists in the United States and Canada: A survey
Neuropsychology, 15, 383–86. of INS, NAN, and APA Division 40 members.
Naugle, R. & McSweeny, A. J. (1995). On the practice of Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(1),
routinely appending neuropsychological data to 33–65.
reports. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 245–47. Reitan, R. M. & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-
Neal, T. M. S. & Brodsky, S. L. (2008). Expert credibility Reitan neuropsychological test battery. Theory and
as a function of eye contact behavior and gender. clinical Interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsycho-
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 35(12), 1515–26. logy Press.
Nelson, L. D., Drebing, C., Satz, P., & Uchiyama, C. Reitan, R. M. & Wolfson, D. (2002). Detection of
(1998). Personality change in head trauma: malingering and invalid test results using the
A validity study of the Neuropsychology Behavior Halstead-Reitan Battery. Journal of Forensic Neuro-
and Affect Profile. Archives of Clinical Neuro- psychology, 3(1–2), 275–314.
psychology, 13, 549–60. Robinson, A. L., Heaton, R. K., Lehman, R. A., &
Oestreicher, J. M. & O’Donnell, J.P. (1995). Validation Stilson, D. W. (1980). The utility of the Wisconsin
of the General Neuropsychological Deficit Scale Card Sorting Test in detecting and localizing
with nondisabled, learning-disabled, and head- frontal lobe lesions. Journal of Consulting & Clinical
injured young adults. Archives of Clinical Neuro- Psychology, 48, 605–614.
psychology, 10, 185–91. Rohling, M. L., Meyers, J. E., & Millis, S. R. (2003).
Otto, R. K. & Krauss, D. A. (2009). Contemplating the Neuropsychological impairment following trau-
presence of third party observers and facilitators in matic brain injury: A dose-response analysis. The
psychological evaluations. Assessment, 16, 362–72. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 289–302.
Paniak, C., MacDonald, J., Toller-Lobe, G., Durand, A., Ruiz, M. A., Drake, E. B., Glass, A., Marcotte, D., & van
& Nagy, J. (1998). A preliminary normative profile Gorp, W. G. (2002). Trying to beat the system:
of mild traumatic brain injury diagnostic criteria. Misuse of the Internet to assist in avoiding the
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro- detection of psychological symptom dissimulation.
psychology, 20, 852–55. Professional Psychology: Research & Practice,
People v. Sebastianelli (1998). Unpublished case. 33(3), 294.
Pieniadz, J. & Kelland, D. Z. (2001). Reporting scores Russell, E. W. (1994). The cognitive-metric, fixed bat-
in neuropsychological assessments: Ethicality, tery approach to neuropsychological assessment.
validity, practicality and more. In C. G. Armengol, In Clinician’s guide to neuropsychological assessment
E. Kaplan, & E. J. Moes (Eds.), The consumer- (pp. 211–58). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
oriented neuropsychology report (pp. 123–40). Associates, Inc.
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Russell, E. W. (1998). In defense of the Halstead Reitan
Pendleton, M. G. & Heaton, R. K. (1982). A compari- Battery: A critique of Lezak’s review. Archives of
son of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and the Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, 365–81.
68 forensic neuropsychology

Russell, E. W. (2004). The operating characteristics of Spreen, O. & Strauss, E. (1991). A compendium of
the major HRNES-R measures. [doi:10.1016/j. neuropsychological tests. New York, NY: Oxford
acn.2003.12.006]. Archives of CLinical University Press.
Neuropsychology, 19(8), 1043–61. Spreen, O. & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of
Russell, E. W. (2007). Commentary on “A motion to neuropsychological tests (Second ed.) (p. 736).
exclude and the ‘fixed’ versus ‘flexible’ battery in New York: Oxford University Press.
‘forensic’ neuropsychology.” [doi:10.1016/j. Steinberg, J. L., Devous, M. D., Sr., & Paulman, R. G.
acn.2007.06.006]. Archives of Clinical Neuro- (1996). Wisconsin Card Sorting activated regional
psychology, 22(6), 787–90. cerebral blood flow in first break and chronic
Russell, E. W. & Starkey, R. I, (1993). Halstead Russell schizophrenic patients and normal controls.
neuropsychological evaluation system (HRNES). Schizophrenia Research, 19, 177–87.
Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Strub, R. & Black, F. W. (1988). Neurobehavioral disor-
Satz, P., Forney, D. L., Zaucha, K., Asarnow, R. R., ders: A clinical approach. Philadelphia, PA: F. A.
Light, R., McCleary, C., et al. (1998). Depression, Davis.
cognition, and functional correlates of recovery Suhr, J. A. & Gunstad, J. (2002). “Diagnosis threat”: The
outcome after traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, effect of negative expectations on cognitive perfor-
12, 537–53. mance in head injury. Journal of Clinical &
Schachter, D. (2001). The seven sins of memory. New Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 448.
York: Houghton Mifflin. Sullivan, K., Lange, R. T., & Dawes, S. (2007). Methods
Schrag, A., Brown, R. J., & Trimble, M. R. (2004). of detecting malingering and estimated symptom
Reliability of self-reported diagnoses in patients exaggeration base rates in Australia. Journal of
with neurologically unexplained symptoms. J Forensic Neuropsychology, 4(4), 49–70.
Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 75(4), 608–611. Sweet, J. J. (1991). Psychological evaluation and testing
Schudel v. General Electric Co., 120 F.3d 991 (9th Cir. services in medical settings. In J. J. Sweet, R. H.
1996) Rozensky, & S. M. Tovian (Eds.), Handbook of
Shapiro, D.L. (2000). Commentary: Disclosure of tests clinical psychology in medical settings (pp. 291–313).
and raw test data to the courts. Neuropsychology New York: Plenum.
Review, 10, 175–76 Sweet, J. J. (1999). Forensic neuropsychology. Fund-
Sharland, M. J., & Gfeller, J. D. (2007). A survey of amentals and practice (p. 535). Lisse, the
neuropsychologists’ beliefs and practices with Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.
respect to the assessment of effort. [doi:10.1016/j. Sweet, J. J. (1999). Malingering: Differential diagnosis.
acn.2006.12.004]. Archives of Clinical Neuro- In J. J. Sweet (Ed.), Forensic neuropsychology.
psychology, 22(2), 213–23. Fundamentals and practice (pp. 255–85). Lisse,
Shealy, C., Cramer, R., & Pirelli, G. (2008). Third party Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.
presence during criminal forensic evaluations: Sweet, J. J., King, J. H., Malina, A. C., Bergman, M. A.,
Psychologists’ opinions, attitudes and practices. & Simmons, A. (2002). Documenting the promi-
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 39, nence of forensic neuropsychology at national
570–72. meetings and relevant professional journals from
Shuman, D. W. (2001). Expertise in law, medicine, and 1990 to 2000. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16,
health care. J Health Polit Policy Law, 26(2), 267– 481–94.
90. Sweet, J. J. & Moberg, P. J. (2000). A survey of practices
Shuman, D. W., Greenberg, S., Heilbrun, K., & Foote, and beliefs among ABPP and non-ABPP clinical
W. E. (1998). An immodest proposal: Should treat- neuropsychologists. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
ing mental health professionals be barred from 4, 101–20.
testifying about their patients? Behavioral Sciences Sweet, J. J., Moberg, P. J., & Suchy, Y. (2000). Ten-year
& the Law, 16, 509–23. follow-up survey of clinical neuropsychologists:
Simmons v. Mullen, 331 A.2d 892 (Pa. Super. Part II. Private practice and economics. The Clinical
Ct. 1974). Neuropsychologist, 14, 479–95.
Simon, J. & Mahan, L. (1971). Quantifying burdens of Sweet, J. J., Meyer, D. G., Nelson, N. W., & Moberg, P. J.
proof: A view from the bench, the jury, and the (2011). The TCN/AACN 2010 “Salary Survey”:
classroom. Law and Society Review, 5, 319–30. Professional Practices, Beliefs, and Incomes of
Slick, D. J., Tan, J. E., Strauss, E. H., & Hultsch, D. F. U.S. Neuropsychologists. The Clinical Neuropsy-
(2004). Detecting malingering: A survey of experts’ chologist, 25, 12–61.
practices. [doi:10.1016/j.acn.2003.04.001]. Archives Sweet, J. J., Moberg, P. J., & Westergaard, C. K. (1996).
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(4), 465–73. Five-year follow-up survey of practices and beliefs
Neuropsychology and the Law 69

of clinical neuropsychologists. The Clinical neuropsychological test performance: A response


Neuropsychologist, 10, 202–21. to Dodrill (1997). The Clinical Neuropsychologist.
Sweet, J. J. & Morgan, J. E. (Eds.). (2009). The neuro- 12, 560–67.
psychology malingering casebook. New York: Taylor United States v. Byers, 740 F.2d 1104 (1984).
and Francis. Valiulis v. Scheffeos, 547 N.E. 2d 1289 (Ill. App. Ct. 1989).
Sweet, J. J. & Moulthrop, M. A. (1999a). Self- Volbrecht, M. E., Meyers, J. E., & Kaster-Bundgaard, J.
examination questions as a means of identifying (2000). Neuropsychological outcome of head
bias in adversarial assessments. Journal of Forensic injury using a short battery. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 1, 73–88. Neuropsychology, 15, 251–65.
Sweet, J. J. & Moulthrop, M. (1999b). Response to Wright, R. W. (2008). Symposium: Liability for possible
Lees-Haley’s commentary: Debiasing techniques wrongs: Causation, probability, and the burden of
cannot be completely curative. Journal of Forensic proof. Loyola of Los Angeles Law Review, 41, 1295–
Neuropsychology, 1, 49–57. 343.
Sweet, J. J., Nelson, N. W., & Moberg, P. J. (2006). The Wolfson, D. & Reitan, R. M. (1995). Cross-validation
TCN/AACN 2005 “salary survey”: Professional of the General Neuropsychological Deficit Scale
practices, beliefs, and incomes of U.S. neuropsychol- (GNDS). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 10,
ogists. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 325–64. 125–31.
Taylor v. Erna, No. 08-10534-DPW, 2009 U.S. Dist. Yantz, C. L. & McCaffrey, R. J. (2005). Effects of a super-
LEXIS 69033 (D. Mass., August 3, 2009). visor’s observation on memory test performance of
Tarver v. State, 940 So.2d 312 (Ala. Crim. App., 2005). the examinee: Third party observer effect confirmed.
Tenopyr, M. L. (1999). A scientist-practitioner’s view- Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 4, 27–38.
point on the admissibility of behavioral and social Youngjohn, J. R. (1995). Confirmed attorney coaching
scientific information. Psychology, Public Policy prior to neuropsychological evaluation. Assessment,
and Law, 5, 194–202. 2, 279–83.
Tomlin v. Holecek, 150 F.R.D. 628 (D. Minn. 1993). Youngjohn, J. R., Lees-Haley, P. R., & Binder, L. M.
Tranel, D. (1994). The release of psychological data to (1999). Comment: Warning malingerers produces
nonexperts: Ethical and legal considerations. more sophisticated malingering. Archives of Clinical
Professional Psychology: Research & Practice, 25, Neuropsychology, 14, 511–15.
33–38. Weiner, I. B. (1999). Writing forensic reports. In A. K.
Tranel, D. (2000). Commentary on Lees-Haley and Hess & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), The handbook of foren-
Courtney: There is a need for reform. Neuro- sic psychology (pp. 501–20). New York: Wiley.
psychology Review, 10, 177–78. Woody, R. H. (2009). Ethical considerations of multi-
Tremont, G., Hoffman, R. G., Scott, J. G., & ple roles in forensic services. Ethics and Behavior,
Adams, R. L. (1998). Effect of intellectual level on 19(1), 79–87.
3
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on
Neuropsychological Evidence
PAU L M . K AU F M A N N

INTRODUCTION opinions may actually be excluded if the expert


Clinical neuropsychology is a specialty practice testimony results in “unfair prejudice, confusion
of psychology that uses unique methods and legally of the issues, or misleading the jury, or . . . undue
protected test materials in conjunction with the delay, waste of time, or needless presentation of
brain-behavior knowledge base to evaluate, diag- cumulative evidence” (Fed. R. Evid. 403). This
nose, and treat individuals with known or sus- chapter begins with a history and description of
pected neurological disease and/or injury. Lawyers key cases that have given rise to the rules for
increasingly seek consultation from neuropsychol- expert witnesses, with a special emphasis on those
ogist experts on an expanding set of legal issues, in cases involving psychologists. These rules are then
part, because clinical neuropsychologists apply a applied to neuropsychological expert opinions in
scientific approach that meets judicial standards a set of recurring challenges to the admissibility of
for expert testimony (Larrabee, this volume). neuropsychological evidence. These challenges
Kaufmann (2009c) recently published a summary include: 1) battery selection (fixed vs. flexible) as
of the growth in forensic neuropsychology con- legally analyzed in Baxter v. Temple (2008),
sulting, showing that the growth in legal cases 2) symptom validity science as taken up in a series
referencing neuropsychology in the Lexis database of cases involving the MMPI-2 Symptom Validity
is outpacing every related area of brain-behavior Scale (FBS, formerly known as the Fake Bad
expertise and this growth is accelerating. This Scale), 3) causation opinions with commentary
author recently noted a 6% average rate of annual from Grenitz v. Tomlian (2003) and its final out-
growth in Lexis cases referencing neuropsychol- come, and 4) nonpsychologists asserting neurop-
ogy from 2005–2009, with an unprecedented 20% sychological opinions. Florida Frye challenges to
increase in 2010. Greiffenstein and Kaufmann FBS, evolving litigation tactics, related jurispru-
(this volume) note the civil, criminal, administra- dence, and the smoldering debate over symptom
tive, probate, and alternative dispute resolution validity test (SVT) application are summarized,
settings in which neuropsychologists are com- with recommendations for how neuropsycholo-
monly asked to render expert opinions. Kaufmann gist experts may testify about response bias, effort,
(2008) recently detailed the complexities of admis- and malingering. The chapter then turns to con-
sibility of neuropsychological evidence in criminal sider rules and case law relevant to tendering
cases.1 With the rapidly increasing use of neuro- expert opinions in criminal cases, carefully distin-
psychology in our courts, practitioners of law and guishing between competency and the affirmative
psychology would benefit from understanding the defense of insanity. The Insanity Defense Reform
nature of neuropsychological evidence and the Act of 1984 and its implications for the scope of
standards for its admissibility. expert testimony are analyzed in a recently
In order for neuropsychologists to render resolved criminal case that relied upon the expert
admissible expert opinions, courts must accept opinions of a clinical neuropsychologist. These
the neuropsychologist as an expert who is capable sections remind professionals that courts
of assisting the trier of fact (see Fed. R. Evid. 702) painstakingly avoid philosophical debate to
in adjudicating an issue or controversy confront- focus on justiciable issues in actual cases. The
ing the court. Experts who do not assist the jury reader is alerted to the persistent controversy
are of no value to the court and poorly formulated about the proper role, if any, of neuroscience and
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 71

neuroimaging in questions of guilt and punish- Scientific advances changed skeptical courts,
ment. The next section examines research evalu- with Nelson v. Nelson (1958) responding to
ating the impact of Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm, Whitaker, writing, “Science of comparing . . . is
Inc. (1993) and its progeny on admissibility of much further advanced today than it was 80 years
expert testimony, with emphasis on neuropsy- ago” (p. 770). As the scientific method became
chological evidence. Finally, this chapter con- more widely understood and accepted, the meth-
cludes with a warning that legislative and judicial odologies of behavioral and social sciences were
processes are underway that might undermine applied increasingly to the study of psychology.
the reliability of neuropsychological evaluations After World War II, training of mental health
and forensic consulting unless neuropsychologists practitioners increased with expanded funding
advocate for reasonable protections (Howe, Sweet, from the U.S. federal government, providing
& Bauer, 2010) and take active steps to legally some support for the Boulder Conference (1949).
intervene (Kaufmann, 2005; 2009c). The Boulder Conference created the scientist–
Courts commonly recognize psychologist practitioner model of training in clinical psychol-
experts, but reliance on the board–certified neuro- ogy that remains at the core of the profession
psychologist expert is a relatively recent develop- (Baker & Benjamin, 2000). The scientist–practi-
ment. The next section reviews the history of tioner model bolsters the credibility of expert psy-
forensic psychology consultation in criminal chologists in our courts.
courts, noting the emergence of neuropsychology Jenkins v. United States (1962) first addressed
experts who address brain–behavior questions at the admissibility of expert testimony by a psy-
issue in litigation. chologist as to the existence of mental defect or
organic brain injury. Three psychologists testified
H I S T O RY that Mr. Jenkins had a mental disease at the time
of the crime. The trial court instructed the jury to
Experience has shown that opposite opinions disregard the testimony of the psychologists
of persons professing to be experts may be because “a psychologist is not competent to give a
obtained to any amount; and it often occurs medical opinion as to a mental disease or defect”
that not only many days, but even weeks, are (p. 643). On appeal, the reviewing court cited the
consumed in cross-examinations, to test the greater weight of legal authority finding that
skill or knowledge of such witnesses and the “some psychologists are qualified to render expert
correctness of their opinions, wasting the time testimony” (p. 644) regarding mental disorders,
and wearying the patience of both court and referring to concurring dicta2 in an earlier case
jury, and perplexing, instead of elucidating, the (People v. Hawthorne, 1940). For further discus-
questions involved in the issue. sion of Jenkins and its progeny, the interested
Winnans v. N.Y. & Erie Railroad Co. reader is referred to Kaufmann (2008).
(1859) (p. 101) In an early case recognizing neuropsycho-
logical expertise in a civil proceeding, Simmons v.
Expert testimony must overcome the courts’ Mullen (1974) found that a psychologist was com-
caution by clearly assisting the trier of fact. petent to testify “as an expert on organic brain
Following Winnans, other courts expressed simi- injury” (p. 897). The following testimony directly
lar hesitations, see Rush v. Megee (1871) (“We are confronts the issue of whether opinions regarding
not enamored with expert testimony, however brain damage are exclusively the domain of
procured or presented” p. 73); Whitaker v. Parker medical professionals.
(1876) (“Evidence of experts is of the lowest order
and of the most unsatisfactory character” p. 587). Q: Doctor, when you formed an opinion that
Clinical psychology did not exist in the nineteenth this girl did have, in addition to some of the
century, but courts would have likely refused emotional problems which you indicated, that
to recognize anyone claiming psychological she did have minimal brain damage.
expertise. Although the science of psychology was
A: Yes.
only emerging, practice was well underway by
those called phrenologists, physiognomists, Q: Can you state with reasonable medical
graphologists, mesmerists, spiritualists, seers, certainty whether you have an opinion as to the
psychics, mediums, mental healers, and psycho- cause of this minimal brain damage which you
logists (Benjamin, 2005). found; and if so, what that cause would be?
72 forensic neuropsychology

A: Yes. I am just stumbling over your term I think that this man is most excellent in his
“medical.” We have to keep in mind that I am field and we rely heavily on what he reports
not a medical person. to us (p. 898).
Q: I think my question was with reasonable
Based in part on the reliance of the medical
professional certainty.
profession on psychologists to make these
A: Okay. Yes, I feel that this youngster’s judgments, Simmons concluded that to adopt the
minimal brain damage results from the trauma appellant’s view that psychologists are not compe-
she received in the accident (p. 897). tent witnesses to testify on physical matters
“would be to ignore present medical and psycho-
Citing Jenkins, the Simmons court noted “it is logical practice” (p. 899).
not essential that an expert witness be a medical Kaufmann (2009c) notes that courts increas-
practitioner to testify on organic problems” ingly rely on neuropsychologists as preferred
(p. 898). Moreover, a neurosurgeon provided brain-behavior-mental-state experts, with growth
the court some evidence for such conclusions, as trends showing that neuropsychologists are out-
follows: pacing every other forensically related psychiatric
or psychological specialty practice (see Figure 3.1).
This unprecedented growth in forensic neuro-
Q: Doctor, I wonder if you would explain for
psychology consulting suggests that practitioners
the Court and Jury the relationship of a clinical
of law and psychology who want to understand
psychologist to a neurosurgeon such as your-
the nature of neuropsychological evidence and
self. What does a clinical psychologist do for
the standards for its admissibility, should learn
yourself [sic]?
more about how courts recognize expertise. The
A: Well, primarily in our business, if we have a next section addresses how courts evaluate the
patient who has complaints, particularly with qualifications of neuropsychologist experts.
those complaints representing the possibility
of emotional disfunction [sic], behavioral dis- Q UA L I F I C AT I O N
turbance, things that we cannot measure on OF EXPERTS
the basis of what we can see, things that we Under Federal Rules, courts must evaluate expert
cannot measure on the basis of our detailed qualifications, and determine the relevance of
examinations — with that particular history, the expert opinions to the issue at bar and the
we then refer those patients for psychometric reliability of the bases for those opinions, before
evaluation and request then of our psycho- those opinions are admitted into evidence and
logist who is doing the examination whether, heard by a jury. Judges must determine whether
by his battery of tests, he can or cannot tell us special experience is required to develop these
whether he believes there is evidence of distur- expert opinions that will assist the jury in resolv-
bance as far as brain function is concerned, ing an issue in the case at bar.3 Therefore, consult-
whether this would be on the basis of possible ing neuropsychologists should understand the
organic disturbance or on the basis of psycho- evolution of court standards for evaluating
logical disturbance. experts as addressed in Frye v. United States
Q: And is the psychologist properly capable of
(1923), Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc. (1993)
determining by his course of tests and his
and its progeny, and Fed. R. Evid. 104 Preliminary
examination, whether the problem is, the prob-
Questions, Fed. R. Evid. 403 Relevance, and
lem that your patient is having, is primarily
Fed. R. Evid. 702 Testimony of Experts.
emotional or primarily organic or having to do
In Frye, a defendant convicted of second
with the brain, itself, or a combination of both?
degree murder appealed, claiming the trial
Is that a fair statement?
court erred because it denied the admission of
expert testimony on a “systolic blood pressure
A: Yes. We feel very strongly that Dr. Romano deception test” (p. 1013). Defense attempts to
can. He has worked with us many, many years admit this expert and his opinions or to conduct
and is one of the two psychologists that, in our the test in the courtroom were denied. The appel-
practice over this period of time, we have come late court affirmed the trial judge, quoting the
down to, and the only one that we use from the government’s brief and thereby creating the
standpoint of this type of testing, because following rule:
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 73

5500

5000
Published Lexis cases using the term
4500

4000 Neuropsycholo! 2009


Forensic Psycholo!
Federal and State Cases

Foresic Psychia!
3500 Neuropsychia!
Neuropsychology projection 2009
3000 Forensic Psychology projection
Forensic Psychiatry projection
2500 Neuropsychiatry projection

2000

1500

1000

500

0
1979 1984 1989 1994 1999 2004 2009
Five year epochs

FIGURE 3.1: Number of United States federal and state cases using the root terms Neuropsycholo!, Forensic
Psycholo!, Forensic Psychia!, and Neuropsychia! in five-year epochs for the past thirty years used as a basis
for polynomial regression projections for the next fifteen years.

. . . when the question involved does not lie reliable evidence of causation in this field of study.
within the range of common experience or The plaintiffs failed to present “statistically signi-
common knowledge, but requires special expe- ficant epidemiological proof that Bendectin causes
rience or special knowledge, then the opinions limb reduction defects” because their expert
of witnesses skilled in that particular science, relied, in part, on in vitro animal and chemical
art, or trade to which the question relates are studies (p. 575). The plaintiffs appealed, arguing
admissible in evidence (p. 1014). the reanalysis of the epidemiological data and the
scientific techniques employed by their experts
In a two page unanimous opinion, the appellate were permissible. The unanimous three-judge
court concluded that the deception test had Ninth Circuit Appellate Court affirmed the trial
not gained “standing and scientific recognition court, again citing Frye and following the prece-
among physiological and psychological authori- dent set in sister courts, referencing
ties as would justify the courts in admitting expert
testimony” (p. 1014). Stated alternatively, the . . . a well-founded skepticism of the scientific
test was denied admission in court because it was value of the reanalysis methodology employed
not generally accepted in the relevant scientific by plaintiffs’ experts; they recognize that “[t]he
community. best test of certainty we have is good science-
In Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm, Inc. (1989), the science of publication, replication, and
infants and their guardians sued a drug company verification, the science of consensus and peer
to recover for limb reduction birth defects caused review. P. Huber, Galileo’s Revenge: Junk Science
by the mother’s ingestion of the antinausea “morn- in the Courtroom 228 (1991) (p. 1131).
ing sickness” drug Bendectin. Merrell Dow won
on summary judgment4 with the trial judge citing The Ninth Circuit suggested in vitro studies
Frye, the “prevailing school of thought” (p. 572) were junk science, affirming the trial court
regarding Bendectin, and controlling legal autho- decision to ignore this new scientific evidence
rity that epidemiological studies are the most because it failed Frye’s general acceptance test.
74 forensic neuropsychology

The plaintiffs appealed and the U.S. Supreme The Court clarified Daubert and broadened its
Court granted certiorari.5 impact in two subsequent cases, General Electric
In a landmark decision that forever changed Co. v. Joiner (1997) and Kumho Tire Co. v.
the scope of expert testimony, the Supreme Court Carmichael (1999). In Joiner, a city electrician,
found that the general acceptance test in Frye who was diagnosed with lung cancer, brought suit
had been superseded by Fed. R. Evid. 702, thereby against the manufacturer of polychlorinated
requiring all federal courts to admit any “scien- biphenyls (PCBs) and manufacturers of electrical
tific, technical, or other specialized knowledge” transformers and dielectric fluid, alleging expo-
that assists the trier of fact to understand the sure caused his cancer based on expert testimony.
evidence.6 “General acceptance” is not a necessary The District Court judge excluded the plaintiff ’s
precondition for the admissibility of scientific expert testimony finding it “subjective belief or
evidence under the Federal Rules of Evidence. unsupported speculation” (p. 140) and Joiner
A 7–2 majority also held that District Court appealed. The Eleventh Circuit Appellate Court
judges (gatekeepers) had the duty to evaluate the applied a stringent standard of review, reversing
admissibility of expert testimony. However, the trial court and finding the judge erred in
the Honorable Chief Justice William Rehnquist excluding the expert testimony. The U.S. Supreme
parted company on the issue of the judge’s role, Court intervened to reverse the Eleventh Circuit,
writing thereby affirming and strengthening the gatekeep-
ing function of the trial court, directing appellate
I do not doubt that Rule 702 confides to the courts not to review a trial judge’s decision regard-
judge some gatekeeping responsibility in decid- ing expert admission unless the judge committed
ing questions of the admissibility of proffered a clear abuse of discretion. Basically, appellate
expert testimony. But I do not think it imposes courts were ordered to show great deference to
on them either the obligation or the authority gatekeeper judges in District Courts and to not
to become amateur scientists in order to disturb the decisions of the trial judge regarding
perform that role (p. 600-01). the admissibility of expert testimony in the
absence of gross error.
With candor, Chief Justice Rehnquist acknowl- In Kumho Tire, a vehicle overturned when a
edged being “at a loss” to understand what is meant right rear tire blew out, killing one passenger and
when it is said that the scientific status of a theory injuring others. The plaintiffs sought to admit the
depends on its “falsifiability” and he predicted testimony of a tire failure analyst regarding his
other federal judges would be too. Nevertheless, visual and tactile inspection of the tire, based on
the Ninth Circuit decision was reversed and the the theory that in the absence of at least two of
case was remanded for further proceedings. four specific physical indicators, tire failure must
In the interests of justice and judicial economy, have been caused by a defect. The defendant
the Ninth Circuit decided to conduct the newly moved to exclude the tire analyst testimony, claim-
required Daubert analysis of Fed. R. Evid. 702, ing the methodology failed to satisfy Fed. R. Evid.
framing the question in this “brave new world” as 702 requirements. The trial court applied Daubert
follows: “How do we figure out whether scientists and the judge excluded the tire analyst after find-
have derived their findings through the scientific ing the methodology employed was insufficiently
method or whether their testimony is based on sci- reliable; plaintiff Carmichael appealed. The
entifically valid principles?” (p. 1316). In the end, a Eleventh Circuit held that the trial court erred in
unanimous Ninth Circuit found that the plaintiff applying Daubert, believing that it only applied to
expert failed Rule 702 requirements and Daubert’s scientific testimony. The U.S. Supreme Court
holding because the plaintiff presented only experts’ reversed the Eleventh Circuit and clarified that
qualifications, their conclusions, and their assur- Daubert factors apply to the testimony of engi-
ances of reliability. The original summary judg- neers and other experts who are not scientists.
ment rendered six years earlier was affirmed and Experts may also be evaluated and admitted to tes-
the Daubert plaintiffs received due process, equal tify based on skill, experience, and other special-
protection, and justice, but no compensation, ized knowledge, not only scientific knowledge.
because the scientific evidence offered by their In 2002, holdings from the Daubert “trilogy”
expert failed to show with a preponderance of the of cases were used to amend Rule 702 and codify
evidence that Bendectin caused the birth defects. these U.S. Supreme Court decisions into the
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 75

current rules governing expert testimony. Rule Battery Selection (Fixed vs. Flexible
702 reads as follows: and the Boston Approach)
Although there have been a number of lower
If scientific, technical, or other specialized court cases that have uniformly supported the
knowledge will assist the trier of fact to under- use of flexible batteries in forensic consulting
stand the evidence or to determine a fact in (Chapple v. Gangar, 1994; People v. Sebastianelli,
issue, a witness qualified as an expert by knowl- 1998; Minner v. American Mortg. & Guar., 2000),
edge, skill, experience, training, or education, fixed battery advocates continue to promote fixed
may testify thereto in the form of an opinion or battery superiority in forensic neuropsychology
otherwise, if (1) the testimony is based upon (Hom, 2008; Russell, 2007). More recent scientific
sufficient facts or data, (2) the testimony is the studies have shown equivalent or superior validity
product of reliable principles and methods, of flexible batteries to fixed battery (i.e., Halstead-
and (3) the witness has applied the principles Reitan) approaches (Larrabee, 2008; Larrabee,
and methods reliably to the facts of the case. Millis, & Meyers, 2008).
(Fed. R. Evid. 702) In Baxter v. Temple (2005), the defense filed a
motion in limine7 to exclude the testimony of a
The test for when expert testimony may Barbara Bruno–Golden, EdD as insufficiently
be used is determined on the basis of assisting reliable under Daubert. During the evidentiary
the trier. Such opinions are excluded when hearing, Dr. Bruno–Golden described the Boston
they are unhelpful, and therefore superfluous and Process Approach (BPA) to hypothesis testing in
a waste of time. (Fed. R. Evid. 702 Comments). the neuropsychological evaluation of a child
The next section shows how these rules are exposed allegedly to lead poisoning. The defen-
applied to a neuropsychologist expert in a recent dants argued successfully that Dr. Bruno–Golden’s
civil case. testimony should be excluded because the BPA
has not been subject to peer review and publica-
B AT T L E O F T H E tion, has no known or potential error rate, and
E X P E RT S OV E R is not generally accepted in the appropriate scien-
ADMIS SIBILITY OF tific community. A review of hearing testimony
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL shows how a trial judge used Daubert factors
EVIDENCE to exclude expert neuropsychological evidence
Within 12 years of Simmons (1975), neuropsy- and the cross–examination provides instruction
chology identified some emerging concerns for how to avoid such outcomes (Desmond,
(Schwartz, 1987; Satz, 1988; Rothke, 1992) regard- 2007).
ing the qualifications of neuropsychologist All three neuropsychologists, Drs. Bruno–
experts and the scope of opinions. Neuropsy- Golden, Sandra Shaheen, and David Faust, testi-
chologists began tracking early decisions regard- fied that the Boston Process methodology
ing admissibility of expert opinions (Richardson employed was untested (Baxter v. Temple (2005),
& Adams, 1992). Despite the clear history of p. 8). Dr. Bruno–Golden added that the BPA that
admissibility of expert opinions based on neuro- she employs in forensic examinations, “has never
psychological evidence, courts have confronted been . . . and cannot be tested, because it varies
cases and controversies in which psychologist from practitioner to practitioner.” (p. 9) [emphasis
experts have taken diametrically opposed posi- added]. In fact, Dr. Bruno–Golden testified that
tions on the admission of certain types of opin- she “could not recall if she had ever administered
ions based on methodological assumptions the same test battery” (p. 9) on the thousands of
underlying the basis for those opinions. These other patients she evaluated during her career.
most common challenges to the admissibility of The Baxter trial court ruled that the BPA as
expert neuropsychologist opinions, include employed by Dr. Bruno–Golden cannot be and
motions to exclude evidence based on: 1) battery has not been tested in this case.
selection (fixed vs. flexible), 2) application of All three neuropsychologists testified that the
symptom validity science in neuropsychological BPA methodology employed was not subjected to
evaluations, 3) causation testimony beyond the peer review, nor described in published articles.
scope of expertise, and 4) expert unqualified to Dr. Bruno–Golden referenced a professional
render neuropsychological opinions. position paper supporting the application of the
76 forensic neuropsychology

BPA that Dr. Faust noted was not in a peer- (Woody, 2009) and a minority even suggest that
reviewed publication. Dr. Shaheen referred to the roles are potentially compatible (Heltzel,
many learned treatises on the general acceptance 2007). Dvoskin (2002) chastised forensic evalua-
of the BPA in clinical neuropsychology practice, tors as “illogical” and “ignorant” for suggesting
yet admitted that Dr. Bruno–Golden’s specific that providing treatment presents some conflict of
methodology had not been subject to peer review interest with the “purity of their objective assess-
or described in published articles. Finally, ments” (p. 537). In the end, neuropsychologists in
Dr. Bruno–Golden admitted she had not previ- forensic practice should always employ objective
ously used the methodology employed and it was methods that allow them to be unbiased truth–
likely that no other clinician had either. Therefore, seekers and must exercise care in all professional
the Baxter trial court found the BPA as employed work, not just forensic consulting
in this case had not been subject to peer review The Baxter trial court held that the evidence
and publication. overwhelmingly showed that Dr. Bruno–Golden’s
No evidence was offered on a known or methodology was not sufficiently reliable for
potential error rate for the BPA. At one point, forensic analysis (Desmond, 2007). The trial court
Dr. Bruno–Golden testified that she ignored judge found the evidence failed to meet any of
standardized instructions regarding time con- Daubert’s flexible factors. Therefore, the defen-
straints on some tests in order to “test the limits” dant’s motion in limine was granted, Dr. Bruno–
of the child’s performance. Neither Drs. Bruno– Golden’s testimony was excluded, and the jury
Golden nor Shaheen offered any evidence on the never heard her opinions in the original trial. The
reliability of testing the limits. Dr. Faust pointed plaintiff appealed and the New Hampshire
out that any variation in the standardized instru- Supreme Court certified three questions for
ctions destroys the normative comparisons of the judicial review, asking whether the trial court
child’s performance to like–aged peers, making it erred when excluding: 1) the neuropsychologist’s
impossible to determine an error rate or interpret testimony based on the Boston Process Approach,
the results. Moreover, although selected individ- 2) the IQ test testimony, and 3) the pediatrician’s
ual tests have known error rates, when Dr. Bruno– testimony that reasonably relied upon the neuro-
Golden modified the BPA she created what psychologist’s report.
Dr. Faust described as an “idiosyncratic combina- In a thorough analysis of neuropsychological
tion, if not hodgepodge of multiple influences.” test administration errors and a unanimous
(p. 11). The court detailed some departures from decision, the New Hampshire Supreme Court
standardized techniques. Dr. Faust concluded the reversed the trial court on the first question and
methodology employed was “not scientifically vacated the subsequent questions as moot. The
validated . . . founded on guesswork, speculation, Baxter court reasoned, “the Daubert test does not
and conjecture, which sometimes flies directly in stand for the proposition that scientific knowl-
the face of scientific literature.” (p. 11). Hence, edge must be absolute or irrefutable” (citing State
the Baxter trial court ruled the methodology v. Dahood, 2002). Referring to the trial court’s
employed by Dr. Bruno–Golden did not have a conclusion, it “focused upon the plaintiff ’s failure
known or potential error rate. to demonstrate that the specific battery–the entire
In concluding its analysis, the Baxter trial series of tests viewed as a whole–employed by
court drew a distinction between appropriate Dr. Bruno-Golden in this case was, or could not
scientific literature for clinical assessment and be, tested, was subject to peer review and publi-
“a ‘forensic’ approach to assessing children with cation, or has a known or potential error rate.”
lead poisoning” (p. 13). Dr. Faust described how (p. 174). In its review, the court found that the
the role of the expert neuropsychologist changes Daubert factors “do not constitute a definitive
depending on whether a case is a clinical or foren- checklist or test” (Kumho Tire v. Carmichael,
sic referral. This important distinction between 1999), but even if they did, “the BPA meets three
the roles of clinical provider and forensic exam- of four Daubert factors” (p. 184). The Baxter court
iner has been frequently described (Greenberg & expressly rejected the battery as a whole argu-
Shuman, 1997; Heilbrun, 2001). Although most ment, finding “that the individual tests he or she
authorities agree that clinical and forensic roles administered as part of the battery, not the battery
are irreconcilable and every effort should be made as a whole, have been tested, have been subject to
to avoid the dual role (Greenberg & Shuman, peer review and publication, and have a known or
2007), others’ approaches are more situational potential error rate” (p. 184). The Baxter court
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 77

wrote, “we reject the defendant’s assertion that allegations of unconstitutional delay of justice
Dr. Bruno-Golden’s methodology, the BPA as a (Baxter v. State, 2010).
flexible battery approach, is not a sufficiently reli-
able methodology to assist the fact finder in under- Application of Symptom Validity Testing
standing the plaintiff ’s neuropsychological status.” Science in Neuropsychological Evaluations
(p. 187). The case was remanded back to the trial The application of symptom validity science in
court with the instruction to admit Dr. Bruno- neuropsychology is not new (Benton & Spreen,
Golden’s opinions that were based on the BPA. 1961) and it will not go away. The modern era of
The Baxter court relied, in part, on an amicus scientific investigation of symptom validity
brief (co-authored by Greiffenstein and Sweet) was introduced with the examination of faking
submitted by the American Academy of Clinical believable deficits in neuropsychological testing
Neuropsychology (AACN). Justices referred to (Heaton, Smith, Lehman, & Vogt, 1978; Pankratz,
the AACN brief frequently during oral arguments, Fausti, & Peed, 1975), but probably began in
raising questions about the standard of care for earnest after descriptions of symptom validity
clinical neuropsychology. In its brief, AACN drew testing (SVT) (Pankratz, 1979; Pankratz, Binder,
an analogy between the specialty practices of & Wilcox, 1987). For the past twenty years,
clinical neuropsychology and clinical neurology, neuropsychology has seen a proliferation of
arguing that neurologists do not “conduct either research designs investigating symptom validity
an invariant exam procedure or order an invariant and an explosion of peer-reviewed scientific
set of diagnostic tests for each and every patient” research on malingering. As exemplified in the
(p. 9). AACN asserted “that administering the Frye, effort to introduce a physiologically based
same set of tests to all patients and litigants, “deception” test some 85 years ago, there is a
regardless of the known or suspected condition is lengthy history of assessment of deception and
uninformed and inappropriate practice” (p. 9). malingering in clinical practice detailed elsewhere
Further, AACN added “a standardized battery (Rogers, 2008). However, the competent forensic
runs counter to an acceptable standard of care in consultant is well advised to carefully compare
neurology” and “we know of no area of specialty and contrast lay opinion about deceit with expert
or subspecialty in clinical medicine in which opinion regarding response bias, effort, and symp-
a routine, invariant battery of tests across all tom validity. Astute comparisons of symptom
medical conditions being evaluated would be validity with deceit yield a deeper appreciation of
acceptable practice” (p. 9). an important boundary between the role of the
Although not entirely clear from the brief, expert and the province of the trier–of–fact. The
AACN was apparently advocating for a flexible scientific evidence supporting SVT application in
battery standard of care for clinical neuro- neuropsychology practice is overwhelming and
psychology. The flexible battery approach is the widely accepted (Boone, 2007; Larrabee, 2007,
predominant form of practice, for certain popula- this volume; Morgan & Sweet, 2008).
tions and referral questions, but the fixed battery The application of SVT science in neuro-
method remains a respectable minority practice psychological evaluations is an increasingly
in clinical neuropsychology. In what is probably common practice and a more recent source of
the best example of the application of Daubert challenge to expert neuropsychological opinions.
factors to neuropsychological methodology, the The problem of symptom validity and effort test-
Baxter case nicely illustrates how federal courts ing has become so prominent that professional
and a majority of state courts would likely address neuropsychological associations have published
questions of admissibility based on battery selec- positions on this topic (NAN, 2005; AACN, 2007)
tion. Baxter also shows that neuropsychology and the AACN recently convened a conference
is in early stages of addressing complex questions (2008) that set forth a Consensus Conference
about standards of care for the profession. Statement on the Neuropsychological Assessment
Although the New Hampshire Supreme Court of Effort, Response Bias, and Malingering
resolved concerns about the admissibility of flexi- (2009). The jurisprudence of SVT science has
ble batteries in forensic practice, the Baxter trial been hotly debated in our courts (detailed
court has not weighed the opposing opinions below), scientific journals (Butcher et al., 2008;
because the second trial has not yet been heard Ben-Porath, Greve, Bianchini, & Kaufmann,
(Hanna, August 8, 2010). Indeed, Baxter has 2009a; Williams, et al. 2009; Ben-Porath, Greve,
been delayed by state budget cuts resulting in Bianchini, & Kaufmann, 2009b), continuing
78 forensic neuropsychology

education workshops (Kaufmann, 2006; 2007; Although relevant, evidence may be excluded
2009a), and a recent American Bar Association if its probative value is substantially outweighed
Conference (Kaufmann, 2009b). by the danger of unfair prejudice, confusion of
Legal opposition to the application of SVT the issues, or misleading the jury, or by consid-
science in neuropsychological practice has evolved erations of undue delay, waste of time, or need-
over the past decade, with early challenges being less presentation of cumulative evidence.
confronted in court (Batzel v. Gault, 2002) and
described in subsequent nonscientific advocacy The judge in Williams v. CSX Transportation,
writings in legal newsletters (Creager, Shea, & Inc. (2007) weighed these concerns and deter-
Larner, 2002), advocacy groups, and blogs. Other mined that the probative value of the MMPI-2
plaintiff attorneys took note and began opposing Symptom Validity Scale (formerly and hereinafter
SVT admissibility, arguing that experts were “FBS”) was outweighed by its prejudicial effect,
inappropriately engaging in determination of the commenting that the term “faking bad” was overtly
truthfulness and credibility of the plaintiff. SVT prejudicial. In balancing the relevance of SVT
science has clearly collided with evidence law and science, Williams placed greater weight on the
the burden is on the expert to demonstrate the name of the scale rather that its reliability. Other
reliability and relevance of its application in Florida judges have disagreed (Nason v. Shafranski,
neuropsychology, and to present results in testi- 2008) allowing expert testimony based on FBS,
mony that does not intrude upon the province of but restricting use of the term malingering.
the court (Kaufmann, 2009c). Symptom validity science involves system-
A common tactic used to restrict application atically designed studies using psychometric tests
of SVT science is a motion in limine, in which an and sophisticated statistical techniques that are
attorney asks the court to exclude SVT evidence beyond the ken of most jurors, demonstrating the
from being heard by a jury. In civil cases, motions need for an expert to assist the trier of fact.
to exclude evidence of malingering are most com- However, if an expert cannot adequately explain
monly filed by plaintiff attorneys, whereas in the science and gets drawn into extended testi-
criminal cases the defendant is usually seeking to mony about, for example, specificity, sensitivity,
keep out evidence of malingering. Attorneys are negative predictive value, likelihood ratios, and
advancing legitimate arguments regarding the other receiver operating characteristic (ROC)
admissibility of SVT science for consideration by curve analysis, a judge may find such testimony
the court. Although arguments to exclude SVT more confusing than helpful. Although it is very
science may take a variety of forms, most are important for an expert to understand his or her
based on the rules of evidence and standards methodology thoroughly, accurate and compre-
for expert testimony, asserting that SVT science hensive explanation all of the subtle complexities
is: 1) more prejudicial than probative; 2) confus- of a science may not necessarily enhance expert
ing, rather than helpful, to the jury; 3) inadmissi- credibility. Courts may exclude testimony that is
ble character evidence; 4) inadmissible hearsay more confusing than helpful to the jury.
evidence; 5) wrongfully intruding into the prov- The inadmissible character evidence argument
ince of the jury; or 6) not generally accepted by derives from the relevance question from Federal
the relevant scientific community. Rule of Evidence 404, as follows:
The first three arguments rely upon Federal
Rule of Evidence 401 and the definition of Evidence of a person’s character or a trait of
relevance, as follows: character is not admissible for the purpose of
proving action in conformity therewith on a
“Relevant evidence” means evidence having particular occasion.
any tendency to make the existence of any fact
that is of consequence to the determination of The complicated exceptions to this rule in
the action more probable or less probable than criminal cases are beyond the scope of this chap-
it would be without the evidence. ter, but in civil proceedings, character evidence is
generally inadmissible unless character is at issue
Essentially, all relevant evidence is admissible, (e.g., defamation). An elevated FBS indicates that
unless privileged. However, Federal Rule of the examinee was likely over-endorsing symp-
Evidence 403 requires courts to balance other toms—a fact that a plaintiff attorney can miscon-
factors when resolving admissibility. strue as the expert calling the plaintiff a fake, a
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 79

fraud, or a liar. One good response to this inten- Having addressed the relevance-based argu-
tionally inflammatory tactic is to answer in the ments used in efforts to exclude SVT science, the
negative and simply point out that an elevated final argument questions the reliability of expert
FBS is just one indicator of symptom invalidity opinions. Again, the best case examples come
(over-reporting) and allow the jury to draw its from Florida involving FBS. This strategy for
own conclusions. excluding FBS uses the standards for evaluating
Some plaintiff attorneys have advocated for experts as described in section III of this chapter.
exclusion of SVT science based on the hearsay Here, the judge plays the key role in determining
rule (Creager, Shea, & Larner, 2002), suggesting reliability of the methods employed by expert wit-
that reliance on another examiner’s test scores nesses. Briefly, a judge may deny the admission of
constitutes hearsay data. This argument fails to evidence in a Frye jurisdiction by simply finding
recognize the common expert exception to the that the methodology is not accepted in the
hearsay rule, allowing the expert to use such relevant scientific community. The judge in
information reasonably relied upon by experts in Vandergracht v. Progressive Express (2005) made
that particular field (Federal Rule of Evidence such a finding and excluded the MMPI-2 FBS
703). Hearsay objections are rarely successful in because there was not “ample evidence that the
excluding expert testimony. test is accepted by his peers.” Surveys of current
Respecting juror conclusions is the basis for SVT science application in neuropsychological
the fifth argument against FBS admissibility. evaluations, not available at the time of the hear-
Judges make decisions about admissibility of ing (e.g. Sharland & Gfeller, 2007), would have
evidence, and generally, juries weigh the credi- effectively addressed the judge’s concern.
bility of that evidence. In the end, the jury decides Attorneys must be cognizant that zealous
the credibility of the plaintiff ’s claim, not an advocacy for their clients should be balanced by
expert witness. Experts must express appropriate their professional responsibility to exercise candor
opinions within the scope of their expertise before the court. Recent examples of these com-
in a manner that is helpful to the jury (Federal peting forces, observed in motion practice from
Rule of Evidence 702). However, experts must forensic neuropsychology, involve challenges to
not state legal conclusions that potentially invade the admissibility of the FBS (Vandergracht v.
the province of the jury. In this regard, Federal Progressive Express, 2005; Williams v. CSX
Rule of Evidence 704 is a source of confusion, as Transportation, Inc., 2007; Davidson v. Strawberry
follows: Petroleum, Inc., 2007; Stith v. State Farm, 2008;
UpChurch v. Broward Co. School Board, 2008;
No expert witness testifying with respect to the Limbaugh-Kirker v. Decosta, 2009) and from those
mental state or condition of a defendant in a cases that were not successful in excluding such
criminal case may state an opinion or inference testimony (Nason v. Shafranski, 2008; Solomon v.
as to whether the defendant did or did not have TK Power, 2008). However, the orders in these
the mental state or condition constituting an Florida cases cannot be neatly categorized. For
element of the crime charged or of a defense example, although the Decosta judge excluded
thereto. Such ultimate issues are matters for the FBS after a lengthy Frye challenge that was with-
trier of fact alone. drawn during pretrial motions then re-introduced
at trial, all other SVT evidence was allowed and
Some attorneys misapply this rule in civil pro- the plaintiff was shown to be exaggerating symp-
ceedings, while others over-extend its reach by toms.
suggesting that experts cannot testify about their In Sharrer & Sharrer v. Sunscape Landscape
data when those data are directly relevant to a Nursery, Inc. et al. (2009), FBS testimony was
matter that a jury must decide. In many respects, excluded, as was all testimony regarding malinger-
the “ultimate issue” rule is abandoned when the ing. The expert neuropsychologist was allowed to
expert witness testimony is demonstrably helpful testify about performance validity, defined as the
to the jury. Expert opinions based on SVT science assessment of whether an accurate measure of
are helpful to the jury considering questions of plaintiff ’s actual abilities was obtained. This testi-
symptom over-reporting (“fake-bad”) in criminal mony included discussion of how SVTs are devel-
and civil proceedings or symptom under-report- oped based on simulation and known groups
ing (“fake-good”), in child custody disputes or investigations, showing how simulators as well as
mental health commitment proceedings. persons with known invalid performance perform
80 forensic neuropsychology

in ways that are atypical for persons with actual and psychological test materials cannot be released
neurologic, psychiatric, or developmental prob- to nonpsychologists. In Sierra, the defense expert
lems. Although testimony about the FBS was pro- neuropsychologist discovered that the plaintiff
hibited, testimony was allowed comparing the expert released the protected material to the
plaintiff ’s standard validity and clinical scale pro- plaintiff counsel in violation of the judge’s order.
file to various clinical groups (depression, chronic Such action can result in contempt findings and
pain, spinal cord injury, moderate and severe TBI, may raise ethical questions. For more information
and multiple sclerosis) and to a profile of examin- regarding the best practice for protecting psycho-
ees with known invalid performance. The expert logical test materials see Greiffenstein and
further testified that when a particular examinee Kaufmann (this volume) and Kaufmann (2005;
shows multiple SVT failures, it is a strong indica- 2009c). The implications of these Florida FBS
tion that test performance is invalid. In such decisions remain hotly debated (Williams et al.,
circumstances, the expert testified that poor per- 2009; Ben-Porath, Greve, Bianchini, & Kaufmann,
formances were, more likely than not, the product 2009b; Kaufmann, Larrabee, & Bigler, 2010).
of invalid performance, whereas normal range However, in response to Hoyt’s (2009) question,
scores were likely an underestimate of actual level “Is the fake bad scale test here to stay?” the answer
of ability. In this fashion, the expert neuropsychol- in almost all jurisdictions is a resounding yes. The
ogist was able to effectively communicate that the same can be said for SVT science in general.
plaintiff was producing invalid data, without dis-
closing the assessment of probable malingering Causation Opinions Beyond the Scope of
made in the original examination report. Neuropsychological Expertise
Generally, SVT evidence is not challenged Generally, psychologists who conduct assess-
(Ben-Porath, Greve, Bianchini, & Kaufmann, ments testify about tests, test results, and their
2009a), but even when it is, SVT science is interpretation of those results (Buckler v. Sinclair
routinely admitted (United States v. Bitton, 2008; Refining Co., 1966; Ross v. State, 1980; Executive
Jackson v. Mason, 2009; Johnson v. Rockwell Car & Truck Leasing, Inc. v. DeSerio, 1985; Minner
Automation, 2009).8 Despite a few isolated Florida v. American Mortgage & Guaranty Co., 2000).
trial court rulings excluding MMPI-2 FBS Nevertheless, psychologists who offer opinions
(reviewed above), Decosta reflects the shifting regarding the physical causes of injury, based on
plaintiff tactics that purportedly belie an underly- their assessments, are sometimes challenged, with
ing debate about timing of SVT challenges (Hsieh, most courts conducting a fact-specific inquiry
2008). With more recent rulings allowing FBS before recognizing expertise and admitting psy-
into evidence on pre-trial motions to exclude, chologist opinions. A majority hold that neurop-
plaintiff attorneys have started withdrawing such sychologists are qualified to render opinions about
motions, only to re-introduce them at trial, a the physical causes of brain injury (Kinsey v. King,
strategy designed to hamper effective defense of 1982; Madrid v. Univ. of Ca., 1987; Valiulis v.
the motion to exclude. A common impeachment Scheffeos, 1989; Fabianke v. Weaver ex rel. Weaver,
strategy used at deposition and trial attempts to 1988; Sanchez v. Derby, 1989; Hutchinson v.
examine the expert on each and every item of the American Family Mut. Ins., 1994; Cunningham v.
MMPI-2 FBS in an effort to use content of the Montgomery, 1995; Huntoon v. TCI Cablevision,
items to discredit the expert. These facial attacks 1998; Landers v. Chrysler Corp., 1998; Adamson v.
may score short–term gains with the court and/or Chiovaro, 1998; Bonner v. ISP Technologies, Inc.,
jury, if the court allows the plaintiff advocate to 2001; Rustenhaven v. American Airlines, Inc., 2003;
use such a strategy and the expert is ill–prepared. Sanders v. Nike, 2004; Wiles v. Dep’t of Educ.,
Recently, experts have successfully used the law to 2008), though a minority restrict or bar neuropsy-
pre-empt these attacks before they can take place. chologists from rendering causation testimony
Some Florida judges recognize that an item-by- (Krevitz v. Savoy Heating and Air Conditioning
item cross examination not only wastes the time Co., 1981; G.I.W. Southern Valve v. Smith, 1985;
of the court and wearies the patience of the jury, Lugo v. Citicorp Mortgage, 1994; Bergeson v. Ray,
but also that Florida regulation bars release of 1998; In re: Breast Implant Litigation, 1998;
psychological test material to nonpsychologists Louderback v. Orkin Exterminating Co., 1998).
(Fla. Admin. Code Ann. r. 64B19–18.004(3); Comparing these two sets of decisions shows that
Fla. Admin. Code Ann. r. 64B19–19.005(3)). courts tend to admit neuropsychological expert
In these cases,9 the judge ordered that raw data opinions on causation in car accident TBI cases,
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 81

but more readily exclude causation opinions from A: I believe that it was after that.
neuropsychologists in cases involving various
Q: And the basis for that opinion, sir?
toxic exposures. Psychologist expert opinions
about damages are commonly accepted without A: The basis is that there appears to be an intact
challenge, but the court’s differential exclusion of brain based on neuropsychological function-
certain causation opinions seems reasonably ing, that there wasn’t a delay in total brain
based on the neuropsychologist’s insufficient development, and that, in fact, rather than a
expertise in toxicology. Expert neuropsychologist delay, there’s an impairment and that would
opinions about causation are also excluded when have come much later in the process.
relying on untested or unproven technologies,
for example, QEEG (In re: Breast Implant Q: More likely than not was Jacob’s injury—did
Litigation, 1998; John v. Im, 2002). When neurop- Jacob’s injury occur during the intrapartum
sychologists offer opinions that fall within their period when Dora was at Bennett Hospital on
scope of expertise, courts generally allow causa- May 11th?
tion testimony. A: Yes. (p. 1011–1012).
A resurgent minority have found that neuro-
psychologists offering opinions about the physical After analyzing the scope of psychology practice
causes of injury are operating outside the scope of under the statute, the Court concluded “Thus, the
psychology practice (John v. Im, 2002; Grenitz v. neuropsychologist was permitted to testify with
Tomlian, 2003; McCarthy v. Atwood, 2005; regard to the etiology (brain damage) of the
Guzman v. 4030 Bronx Blvd. Assoc. L.L.C., 2008). behavior he evaluated. He should not have been
In Grenitz, the Florida Supreme Court confronted permitted to testify as to the medical causation of
the scope of neuropsychology practice when an the organic brain damage itself ” (p. 1003). Despite
expert, Barry Crown, PhD, ABN, proffered opin- a split opinion on some issues, the Court unani-
ions that Jacob Tomlian’s brain damage “was mously noted that Dr. Crown exceeded his area of
brought about by an oxygen deprivation experi- expertise with the dissent writing ‘the field of
ence at the intrapartum level or in the neonatal psychology being related to behavior, and behav-
period” (p. 1003). Although this medical mal- ior is not studied or known in utero.’ The dissent
practice case was complicated by a statutory reasonably argued any error introduced by
change in the definition of psychology practice Dr. Crown’s testimony was harmless because both
that took place during the pendency parties presented expert testimony of pediatric
of the appeal, Dr. Crown’s testimony illustrates neurologists on the causation question. In the
an important boundary of neuropsychologist end, the plaintiff ’s neurologist expert was appar-
expertise, as follows: ently persuasive, with the jury finding the hospital
(85%) and obstetrician (15%) negligent and
Q: [Plaintiff ’s counsel]. Can you rule out injury awarding the plaintiff $30 million–the largest
occurring during the 24- to 34-week period? medical malpractice verdict awarded in Broward
A: [Dr. Crown]. Yes, to the extent that—that County, Florida (Tomlian v. Grenitz, 2008).
his functioning includes those aspects of the Despite what some may view as a setback for
brain that are the last to develop; and, in fact, neuropsychologist causation opinions, even
his highest score on object assembly involves Grentiz disagreed with a “categorical rule that a
that kind of visual motor processing, yes. neuropsychologist can never testify as to the cause
of organic brain damage” (p. 1010). Based on cur-
Q: As a neuropsychologist, how can you rent jurisprudence in this area, courts are much
do that? more willing to admit neuropsychologist brain
A: As I said, there are functions that were tested injury causation testimony in TBI car accident
that he scored—Actually, his highest score on cases.
one test was in that area, and that’s the last area
of the brain that actually develops.
Experts Unqualified to Render
Neuropsychological Opinions
Q: Do you have an opinion within a reasonable Some courts have excluded physicians from
degree of neuropsychological probability as to rendering expert neuropsychological opinions
whether Jacob sustained a hypoxic injury at the (see proffered testimony of Kaye Kilburn, MD in
24- to 34-week period? Downs v. Perstorp Components, Inc., 1999; Goeb v.
82 forensic neuropsychology

Tharaldson, 2000; Ellis v. Appleton Papers, Inc., condition of a defendant as it may relate to con-
2006). However, most courts follow the general siderations of competence, insanity, culpability,
rule that “any person whose profession or voca- and mitigation. Before considering the current
tion deals with the subject at hand may testify as and continuing application of neuropsychology in
an expert” (Hagen v. Swenson, 1975, p. 162). criminal litigation, it is important to understand
A New York judge held that selected social work- some basic concepts of criminal law, and some
ers were qualified to offer expert neuropsychologi- rules of evidence and procedure in criminal
cal opinions, finding the practice of psychology proceedings. In criminal litigation, competency
and social work “wholly equal and the same” must be distinguished from the affirmative defense
(People v. R.R. & G.A., 2005, p. 544). Ultimately, of insanity in order to understand admissibility
most courts admit neuropsychologist expert testi- standards for neuropsychological evidence.
mony and the value of expert evidence is addressed In 1899, the Sixth Circuit determined that the
under cross examination and weighed by the jury. Fourteenth Amendment due process clause pro-
In criminal litigation involving questions of hibited criminal proceedings against a mentally
mental illness, psychologists are recognized as incompetent person (Youtsey v. United States,
experts in matters of competence, insanity, 1899). Although history provides different defini-
criminal culpability, and mitigation, with few tions of insanity that are reviewed elsewhere
exceptions.10 Although the modern trend toward (Denney, chapter addressing criminal responsi-
broader recognition of neuropsychologist expert bility in the current volume; Denney & Sullivan,
opinions in criminal cases is apparent, isolated 2008), after John Hinckley shot President Ronald
courts may still apply the traditional rule that only Reagan, his Press Secretary James Brady, and two
physicians can offer expert medical testimony. law enforcement personnel, Congress changed
Courts remain cautious about the science of the federal standard for an insanity defense,
brain-behavior relations and are skeptical about heightening the defendant’s burden of proof. The
its application to resolve cases and controversies Insanity Defense Reform Act (IDRA) places the
(Brown & Murphy, 2010; Pardo & Patterson, 2010; burden on the defendant to prove that he or she
Appelbaum, 2009; Khoshbin & Shahram, 2007; was unable to appreciate the “nature and quality
Pettit, 2007; Keckler, 2006; Morse, 2006, Redding, or the wrongfulness” of his or her acts as a result
2006; Kulynych, 1997). Although many judges of a “severe mental disease or defect” at the time
may be ignorant of the science, they rightfully of the crime (18 U.S.C. § 17). In order to meet this
are concerned with balancing the probative value burden, the defendant must present clear and
of neuropsychological evidence against its preju- convincing evidence11 (§ 17(b)). In contrast, the
dicial effect, while being mindful of the risks test for competency is whether the party has “suf-
associated with juror confusion (Fed. R. Evid. ficient present ability to consult with his lawyer
403). Criminal courts cannot resolve long-stand- with a reasonable degree of rational understand-
ing philosophical debates about free will and ing—and whether he has a rational as well as fac-
determinism; such matters are nonjusticiable tual understanding of the proceedings against
issues. However, even though the jurisprudence him” (Dusky v. United States, 1960, p. 402). Thus,
of neuropsychological expertise in criminal litiga- while insanity is relevant only to the time of the
tion is not as long as other mental health experts, crime, competency is relevant throughout the
nor as well developed, judges increasingly admit entire legal proceeding, including understanding
the brain–behavior science of neuropsychologist of Miranda warnings, entry of a plea, waiver of
experts into their courts. counsel, nature of trial proceedings, and punish-
Although the rules of evidence for expert wit- ments. Consequently, the consulting expert must
nesses apply in civil and criminal litigation, pro- understand the rules governing and distinguish-
cedural rules and burdens of proof are markedly ing competence and the insanity defense.
different. In civil proceedings, the plaintiff must
prove its case with a “more likely than not” pre- RU L E S G OV E R N I N G
ponderance of the evidence, whereas in criminal COMPETENCY
cases the state carries a much heavier burden to AND THE INSANITY
prove guilt beyond a reasonable doubt. Careful DEFENSE IN CRIMINAL
analysis of modern jurisprudence shows the piv- PROCEEDINGS
otal role of the neuropsychologist expert in assist- Generally, prohibitions against subjecting an
ing the trier of fact to understand the mental incompetent person to trial extend from arrest
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 83

until imposition of the sentence (18 U.S.C. § 4244; Miranda Warnings


United States v. Johns, 1984). Retrospective com- Waiver of the Fifth Amendment right to remain
petency determinations, although disfavored, silent raises the first issue of competency, in that
may be conducted provided that a meaningful such waiver must be made “knowingly, voluntarily,
hearing of the issue remains possible (p. 957). and intelligently” (Miranda v. Arizona, 1966, p.
Moreover, the issue of a criminal defendant’s 444). Moreover, waiver of the Sixth Amendment
mental capacity to proceed may be raised at any right to consult with an attorney must use the
stage of the litigation (Howard v. State, 1985), same standard and the right to counsel can be
whether by the defense, the government, or the invoked at any time during the proceeding. Indeed,
court (State v. Broom, 1995). the government has a high burden to show com-
Once competency is placed at issue, the defen- petent waiver if they intend to use statements
dant constitutionally is entitled to a hearing (Pate made by a suspect during custodian interrogation
v. Robinson, 1966). Competency hearings do not conducted in the absence of an attorney (p. 503).
determine whether the defendant is competent, However, that governmental burden does not nec-
but rather whether there are reasonable grounds essarily require an express written or oral state-
for the court to believe that the defendant may be ment of waiver (North Carolina v. Butler, 1979).
incompetent (Calloway v. State, 1995). However,
the mere fact that a defendant is pleading not Entry of a Plea
guilty by reason of insanity does not require the Competency to enter a plea applies the afore-
court to order a competency hearing (State v. Lee, mentioned Dusky “sufficient present ability”
1983). The fact that competency has been put in standard. No higher standard of competency
doubt also does not require the court to order a applies to entry of a plea than that which is
mental examination (State v. Clemons, 1997). required for standing trial (Miles v. Stainer, 1997).
A defendant must file pretrial motions if However, if evidence presents a substantial
planning to introduce expert testimony relating question of the defendant’s sanity at the time of
to a mental disease or defect, or to any other the offense, then the court must assure that the
mental condition bearing on the issue of guilt defendant: 1) is fully availed of alternative pleas,
(Fed. R. Crim P. 12.2(b)). However, the court 2) comprehends the consequences of failing to
may, in its discretion, allow a defendant to file assert an insanity defense, and 3) exercises a free
notice of intent to rely on the insanity defense at a choice (Frendak v. United States, 1979). Further,
later time (Fed. R. Crim P. 12.2(a)). There is even if counsel indicates that the defendant
a split of authority12 as to whether the Rule 12.2(b) waives a viable insanity defense, the court must
notice is sufficient for the court to order a mental ascertain through independent inquiry that
examination (United States v. Banks, 1991) or waiver is voluntary and intelligent before such
whether the defendant expressly must place waiver can be accepted (People v. Gettings,
competency at issue or file intent to rely on the 1988). Finally, defendants pleading not guilty by
insanity defense (United States v. Marenghi, 1995). reason of insanity must be advised of the likeli-
These jurisdictional variations yield different hood of involuntary commitment for mental
defense strategies. For example, a defendant who illness if the plea is successful (Morrison v. United
claims the presence of a mental condition negates States, 1990).
an element of the crime may not necessarily be
required to undergo a state mental examination. Stand Trial
In some jurisdictions, the defendant may present The Dusky “sufficient present ability” test of com-
expert testimony about his or her mental condi- petency involves the defendant’s ability to com-
tion without being required to undergo an inde- municate with his attorney and to understand the
pendent examination by the state, so long as that nature of the proceedings. Stated alternatively, it is
defendant has not placed competency or insanity fundamental to our system of criminal justice to
at issue. prohibit subjecting to trial a person whose “mental
Recognizing that competency to proceed can condition is such that he lacks the capacity to
be raised by either party or the court at any stage understand the nature and object of the proceed-
of the proceedings, competency may become an ings against him, to consult with counsel, and to
issue at the time of issuance of Miranda warnings, assist in preparing his defense” (United States v.
entry of a plea, standing trial, sentencing, and Renfroe, 1987, p. 766). The capacity to assist in
punishment. preparing a defense includes understanding court
84 forensic neuropsychology

procedures and roles (accused person, attorneys, State v. Korell, 1984). There is no fundamental
judge, jury), and the ability to recall relevant Constitutional right to assert an insanity defense,
events, produce evidence, testify, confront hostile even in capital murder cases (State v. Card, 1991).
witnesses, and project a sense of innocence (Drope However, such statutes do not preclude all consid-
v. Missouri, 1975, p. 172). Essentially, incompetent erations of a defendant’s mental state during
persons are not really present in the courtroom, criminal proceedings. Courts have acknowledged
even though they physically appear. the “constantly shifting adjustment” of tension
between the “evolving aims of the criminal law
Punishment and changing religious, moral, philosophical, and
Although the basic premise that insane individu- medical views of the nature of man” (p. 1085).
als should not be punished while “insane” applies Uncertainty regarding the proper treatment of the
to all punishments (18 U.S.C. § 4245(e)), it is criminally insane has resulted in wavering stan-
rarely invoked except for execution. In yet another dards and inconsistent application by our courts.
inherently confusing choice of terminology, here After Hinkley, Congress passed IDRA defining
the term insane actually refers to competency to the circumstances when an “otherwise culpable
face execution. The Eight Amendment prohibi- defendant is excused for his conduct because of
tion against cruel and unusual punishment bars mental disease or defect” (18 U.S.C. § 17), but it
execution of insane individuals (Ford v. does not limit evidence offered to negate specific
Wainwright, 1986), and also is prohibited under intent13 (United States v. Frisbee, 1985, p. 1220).
state laws. However, the standard for legal insan- Under IDRA, evidence of mental abnormality
ity applied to execution is not the same as suffer- proving lack of mens rea14 is admissible, although
ing from a mental illness (Billiot v. State, 1995). such evidence cannot be used to prove diminished
Although intelligence is a relevant factor that responsibility or diminished capacity (United
courts must weigh, mental impairment and brain States v. Pohlot, 1987). Although “persistent con-
damage (Shaw v. Delo, 1992) do not preclude exe- fusion” remains in the application of terms like
cution necessarily, so long as the inmate possesses diminished responsibility and diminished capac-
the mental awareness required for execution. In ity in determining guilt, IDRA attempted to care-
some jurisdictions, sufficient competence for exe- fully delineate the proper use of mental health
cution only requires that the defendant under- evidence as it relates to legal excuse and criminal
stand execution proceedings, know that it is a culpability (United States v. Cameron, 1990).
punishment, and appreciate why he is being pun- Cameron summarized how IDRA altered the
ished (Rector v. Clark, 1991). use of mental health evidence in federal criminal
Individuals with mental retardation can be trials, as follows: 1) eliminated the volitional
found competent to stand trial, yet a death sen- “diminished capacity” element of the insanity
tence does not pass Constitutional muster and is defense (§ 17(a)), 2) eliminated all other affirma-
barred as cruel and unusual punishment under tive defenses or excuses based on mental disease
the Eighth Amendment (Atkins v. Virginia, 2002). or defect (§ 17(a)), 3) required the defendant to
The Court reasoned that individuals with mental show clear and convincing evidence of insanity
retardation have diminished capacity that reduces (§ 17(b)), 4) limited the use of expert psycholo-
their personal culpability. Similarly, the Court has gical testimony regarding ultimate issues (Fed. R.
held states cannot impose the death penalty on Evid. 704(b)), and 5) linked a “not guilty by reason
juveniles under the age of eighteen years (Roper of insanity” verdict directly to federal civil com-
v. Simmons, 2005), due to the diminished cul- mitment proceedings (§ 4242(b)). Notwithstand-
pability of psychological immaturity. Capital pun- ing the statutory language or congressional intent
ishment is reserved for those offenders who behind IDRA, federal courts have struggled with
commit the most serious crimes and the death statutory interpretation and its application to
penalty is only for those whose extreme culpabil- criminal cases (Cameron, p. 1062).
ity makes them “the most deserving of execution” Cameron suggested that federal court decisions
(Atkins, p. 319). are erratic with no less than three approaches
to diminished capacity. Some courts view “dimin-
THE INSANITY DEFENSE ished responsibility” and “diminished capacity” as
REFORM ACT interchangeable terms meaning that mental
A small minority of jurisdictions abolished the health evidence will be admitted on the issue of
insanity defense by statute (State v. Beam, 1985; specific intent. Other courts agree the terms are
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 85

interchangeable, but they contend that such desig- defendant accused of murder, relying in part on
nations do not narrow the use of psychiatric evi- neuroimaging EEG technology.
dence solely to the question of specific intent. This
second group applies both terms “to excuse, miti- In June 2008, police in Maharashtra, India,
gate, or lesson the defendant’s moral culpability” took 24-year-old student Aditi Sharma into
due to “psychiatric compulsion or inability or fail- custody as a suspect in the murder of her
ure to engage in normal reflection” (p. 1062). former fiancé, Udit Bharati. Sharma and Bharati
Essentially, this approach views these diminish- had been living together in Pune when Sharma
ments as a partial legal excuse. Finally, other courts met another man and eloped with him to Delhi.
distinguish diminished responsibility and dimin- The following year, Sharma returned to Pune,
ished capacity. They claim that only diminished where, according to prosecutors, she asked
capacity aims at negating specific intent, while Bharati to meet her at a McDonald’s and laced
diminished responsibility refers to showing that his food with arsenic. Bharati died from the
“the accused suffered from an abnormality of mind poisoning. Sharma insisted she was innocent.
that substantially impaired his mental responsibil- Police read Sharma her rights and asked
ity” (p. 1062). Current jurisprudence on these mat- her to sit for a brain imaging test. Sharma
ters remains unsettled. agreed to the test. Officers strapped her onto a
Modern neuroscience and neuroimaging high-tech gurney and fastened electrodes to
technology are adding to the confused jurispru- her head. The sensors measured electrical
dence of diminished capacity, in the form of the brainwaves in response to targeted stimuli.
neuropsychological signs and symptoms collec- Investigators read Sharma first-person state-
tively described as frontal lobe syndrome. Frontal ments that corresponded to their theory of the
lobe syndrome has given rise to the frontal lobe crime, as well as neutral statements to help the
defense (FLD) in criminal cases (State v. Rogers, software distinguish between Sharma’s actual
2000), which some predict will play an increasing remembrance and her normal cognition.
role when representing brain-injured defendants Sharma said nothing in reply, but the sensors
(Redding, 2006). A New York Times article (Rosen, were able to measure and display her brainwave
2007) proclaimed, “Neuroscientific evidence will patterns to confirm that she knew some of the
have a large impact not only on questions of guilt statements were true and that some were false.
and punishment, but also on the detection of lies “I bought arsenic,” they said. And, “I met Udit at
and hidden bias, and on the prediction of future McDonald’s.” For an hour, Sharma did nothing.
criminal behavior.” A subsequent Lexis search15 But the parts of her brain where memo-
revealed that functional magnetic resonance ries are thought to be stored lit up on the screen
imaging (fMRI) findings have been raised, but when she heard these statements. At the
not yet admitted in a U.S. criminal trial, even as murder trial, the brain scans were admitted
some speculate the threat of neuroimages has into evidence. These neuroscientific test results
prompted more favorable plea agreements (Rosen, persuaded Judge S.S. Phansalkar-Joshi that
2007). Popular media increasingly speculates Sharma had “experiential knowledge” of having
about how neuroimaging may be “redefining committed the murder. Sharma was convicted
criminal culpability” (Haederle, 2010). These and sentenced to life in prison.16
“Orwellian” theories of brain function and neu-
roimaging techniques are already appearing in In a second case from England alleging
legal cases, as scientists produce enhanced brain Munchausen by proxy, fMRI data were admitted
images that are dramatically highlighted with vivid in an effort to bolster the innocence of a female
hues indicating areas of heightened or suppressed caretaker accused of poisoning a child (Spence
brain activity. Some legal scholars and commenta- et al., 2008). The interested reader is directed to a
tors agree that the influence of neuroimaging is series of papers published after a Neuroscience,
bound to spread and may threaten the underpin- Law & Government Conference (Moriarty,
nings of criminal law, even as most predict that the 2008).17
law will continue to hold individuals accountable Although a number of investigators believe
for wrongful conduct (Erickson, 2010). neuroimaging used in conjunction with neuropsy-
However, expert opinions based on functional chological techniques may be applied increasingly
neuroimaging may be used to by either party to a to assist the trier of fact (Bigler, 2001), courts are
dispute. India is the first country to convict a cautious about admitting expert testimony based
86 forensic neuropsychology

on neuroimages (also see the chapter on neuro- relatively few in number, often include only a
imaging by Ricker in the current volume). The fail- small number of subjects, lack control groups,
ure of magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) lie or find ‘considerable overlap between the values
detection science under Daubert was predicted of patient and control groups in studies of the
because of the gap between experimental findings size, shape, or metabolic activity of different
and practical application (Alexander, 2006). brain regions.’ Moreover, to date, there are no
Alexander’s prediction has been vindicated in standard criteria available for differentiating
U.S. courts thus far. In March, 2009, an attempt to between normal and abnormal scan results or
admit fMRI images to bolster claims of innocence for quantifying the extent of brain damage.
was withdrawn in a San Diego County case with (Redding, 2006, p. 63)
sealed records alleging sexual abuse of a minor.
Figure 3.2 provides an excerpt from the No Lie Others are less diplomatic in their criticism of
MRI report. More recently, fMRI lie detection has neuroimaging evidence, suggesting that “brain
failed to meet the judicial standards under Frye in overclaim syndrome”18 often afflicts those enam-
New York state courts (Wilson v. Corestaff Services, ored by fascinating new theories in the neurosci-
2010) and under Daubert in a Tennessee federal ences (Morse, 2006). Morse concludes, “brains do
court (United States v. Semrau, 2010). Despite not commit crimes, people commit crimes”
these rebuffed attempts to admit expert opinions (p. 397), seemingly unconvinced they are materi-
based on fMRI neuroimaging, civil plaintiffs and ally the same and absolutely unwilling to yield
criminal defendants are likely to persist in efforts personal accountability under the law in what he
to use newly emerging functional neuroimaging describes as a “fundamental psycholegal error”
technology to bolster claims. Admission of expert (p. 397). Prominent cognitive neuroscientists have
testimony based on functional neuroimaging, started to address these questions, asking,
when used to evaluate witness credibility, would
likely draw challenges under the First, Fourth, Should psychopaths, a group that makes up
Fifth, Sixth, Seventh, and Fourteenth Amendments about 20% of our high-security male prison
of the U.S. Constitution. population, be considered as suffering from
Even the most enthusiastic promoters of the a brain disorder that prevents them from
legal applications of functional neuroimaging forming an empathetic response and under-
currently urge caution when using such evidence standing (even comprehending) the feelings of
in criminal cases, because the scientific basis for others? If so, do we want to excuse them under
such is very limited, as follows: insanity or diminished capacity doctrines and
thereby judge them as exculpable and let them
. . . the role of FLD in violent behavior, a note of go? Do we want the state to house them
caution must be sounded. These studies, still in a different kind of facility? The issues seem

FIGURE 3.2: Published excerpt from No Lie MRI report recently contemplated, then withdrawn, from a
San Diego County child custody dispute, illustrating how physiologically based Lie Detection technology is
fundamentally different than the application of SVT science in neuropsychological evaluations.
To illustrate how SVT science is not attempting to detect lies or discern truth as noted above.
Contrast to common language usage of SVT science in neuropsychological evaluation.
“Three independent examiners conducted three separate MMPI-2 administrations that demonstrated profound response bias consistent
with symptom exaggeration. All evaluations demonstrated a pattern of symptom reports that are essentially never seen, except in patients
who are seeking compensation in litigation. Other embedded measures were consistent with exaggerated memory complaints. Reanalysis
of existing neuropsychological raw data demonstrate multiple indications of atypical performance and suboptimal effort during three
neuropsychological evaluations.”
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 87

endless. We are at a major crossroads. expert and rendering expert testimony to the
(Gazzaniga, 2008)19 court. Therefore, the next section will take the rel-
evant case law and rules governing experts, sum-
Workshops at professional conferences are begin- marized in the third section of this chapter and
ning to address these vexing ethical, legal, and apply them to a recently resolved federal case,
social questions (Kaufmann & Wagner, 2010; challenging the scope of a neuropsychologist’s
Blair, 2010; Morse, 2010), even as the scientific expertise.
literature expands rapidly. Legal commentary
continues to identify the “well intentioned, but A P P L I C AT I O N O F
misguided reductionism” (Brown & Murphy, A D M I S S I B I L I T Y S TA N D A R D S
2010, p. 1191) of those who “seem all too willing IN A CRIMINAL CASE
to accept a view of personhood that invariably United States v. Jose Santos-Bueno (2006) illus-
trivialized the ability of individuals to exert con- trates the current standards of admissibility for
trol over their own behaviors, in favor of one neuropsychological evidence as applied in a fed-
which reduces humanity to the indiscriminate eral criminal case. Here, the government filed a
ebb and flow of chemicals between neurons” motion in limine, to exclude the expert testimony
(Erickson, 2010, p. 33). Although neuroscientists of a neuropsychologist regarding the impact of a
may view a person and their brain as one and the brain injury on the defendant’s cognitive abilities.
same for scientific investigation, such a perspec- Mr. Santos-Bueno was charged with transport-
tive is irrelevant to criminal law because describ- ing illegal aliens in violation of the Immigration
ing physiology that may provide explanation of and Nationality Act (8 U.S.C. § 1324(a)(1)(A)(ii)).
multifactorial causes does not excuse conduct. The defense introduced expert neuropsychologist
Nevertheless, Brown and Murphy (2010) include testimony to rebut the government’s evidence that
an appendix “checklist for judges confronted with Mr. Santos-Bueno formed the requisite mental
functional neuroimaging evidence” in full antici- state to commit the crime. The neuropsychologist’s
pation “that the validity and probative value of testimony challenged “the accuracy and reliability
fMRI will improve in the future” (p. 1207). of inculpatory statements” made by the defendant
Definition by exclusion is another way to grasp to law enforcement following his arrest (p. 2).
diminished capacity–by distinguishing it from Essentially, the defense asserted that Mr. Santos did
competency and insanity. Unlike a finding of not have the requisite state of mind to infer that
incompetency, diminished capacity does not delay passengers were aliens and that he was excessively
criminal proceedings, nor is it an affirmative vulnerable to suggestions during questioning, due
defense. However, even when a defendant, who is to his cognitive disabilities. The government sought
found competent to stand trial, fails to show clear to exclude the expert testimony on three grounds:
and convincing evidence of insanity, or cannot 1) preclusion under IDRA, 2) insufficient reliability
prove that diminished capacity negated the mens and relevance under Fed. R. Evid. 702 and Daubert,
rea component of a crime, his mental condition and 3) probative value of the expert testimony is
may still be relevant in mitigating punishment. outweighed substantially by its potential to mislead
Consequently, when the battle of neuropsycholo- and confuse the jury under Fed. R. Evid. 403.
gists experts does not yield a favorable verdict for Pleading in the alternative, the government also
the defendant, testimony used by the defense to sought to have their own expert examine the defen-
argue incompetence, insanity, or diminished dant under Fed. R. Crim. P. 12.2(c)(1)(B),20 should
capacity may be resurrected to reduce a sentence the court decide to admit the defense expert.
or avoid the death penalty. Federal court authority to review this motion
In light of these complex, inconsistent, and derives, in part, from Fed. R. Evid. 104
confusing legal standards, how can neuropsychol- (a) Preliminary Questions of Admissibility, and
ogists provide meaningful expert testimony to (b) Relevancy Conditioned on Fact, as follows:
assist the trier of fact in resolving guilt or inno-
cence? Knowing the differing legal standards for (a) Preliminary questions concerning the
insanity and competency in the relevant jurisdic- qualification of a person to be a witness,
tion is essential for neuropsychological consulting the existence of a privilege, or the
on criminal cases, but such understanding is moot admissibility of evidence shall be
if the clinical neuropsychologist fails to appreciate determined by the court, subject to the
and fulfill the requirements for recognition as an provisions of subdivision (b). In making
88 forensic neuropsychology

its determination it is not bound by the (p. 1061). However, the IDRA does not preclude
rules of evidence except those with respect the use of such evidence “to negate a requisite state
to privileges. of mind” that is an element of the alleged criminal
(b) When the relevancy of evidence offense (United States v. Schneider, 1997, p. 201).
depends upon the fulfillment of a Some appellate courts admit diminished capacity
condition of fact, the court shall testimony only for “specific intent” crimes, because
admit it upon, or subject to, the it is difficult or rare that a mental condition could
introduction of evidence sufficient to negate a “general intent” requirement (United
support a finding of the fulfillment of the States v. Santos-Bueno, 2006, p. 14). However,
condition. there is no agreement whether transportation of
illegal aliens is a specific or general intent crime,
As the gatekeeper of admissibility as outlined nor did the defense attempt to use this distinction.
under Daubert, the Honorable Judge F. Dennis Rather, the defense simply asserted that
Saylor conducted a pre-trial evidentiary hearing Mr. Santos-Bueno did not have the required
and rendered an opinion on January 5, 2006. mental state because he did not draw the neces-
In analyzing the statutes and evidence, Judge sary inferences that the van passengers were
Saylor concluded that only two of four elements illegal aliens. Consequently, the court held that
of 8 U.S.C. § 1324(a)(1)(A)(ii) were at issue, that the IDRA simply did not apply to the neuro-
is: 1) Did Mr. Santos-Bueno act “knowing or in psychologist’s expert opinions.
reckless disregard of the fact that an alien has
come to . . . the United States in violation of Neuropsychologist Testimony
law” and 2) Did Mr. Santos-Bueno act “willfully in meets Daubert and Rule 702 Standards
furtherance of the alien illegal presence” in the for Admissibility
United States? (p. 11). The government attempted Next, the court turned to Daubert and Fed. R.
to prove these two elements and the defense was Evid. 702, which codifies the requirements for
to rely on the testimony of an expert neuropsy- admissibility of expert testimony in federal courts.
chologist to rebut their proof. David Gansler, District courts have broad gatekeeping discretion
PhD, ABPP-CN offered testimony that Mr. Santos- in this inquiry—a process of evaluating the
Bueno suffered a brain injury six months before reliability and relevance of expert testimony
the crime, which caused persistent and significant (Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, 1999, p. 141).
cognitive deficits. Dr. Gansler was prepared to tes- Courts may use a flexible approach and enjoy
tify people with defendant’s condition “are known “substantial discretion” in determining whether
to be vulnerable to suggestion” (p. 10). The defense to admit or exclude expert testimony (General
argued that statements allegedly made by Mr. Electric Co. v. Joiner, 1997). Although the govern-
Santos-Bueno during a police interview were ment neither challenged the scientific reliability of
unreliable and untrue. In evaluating the admissi- the evidence nor Dr. Gansler’s qualifications to
bility of this testimony, Judge Saylor asked whether conduct an appropriate evaluation or render an
such evidence is: 1) precluded under IDRA, expert opinion, the court must conduct a prelimi-
2) admissible under Rule 702 and Daubert, and nary assessment.
3) excluded under Rule 403.
Reliability of Neuropsychologist
Neuropsychologist Testimony Testimony
not Precluded by the Insanity In Daubert, the Supreme Court determined that
Defense Reform Act Fed. R. Evid. 702 superseded the holding in Frye v.
The Insanity Defense Reform Act provides an United States (1923) and set a new standard for
affirmative defense when showing clear and con- admissibility of novel scientific evidence. Federal
vincing evidence of insanity (IDRA at §17), but rules allow expert testimony “if scientific, techni-
precludes “the use of non-insanity” psychiatric cal, or other specialized knowledge” will assist the
evidence pointing toward exoneration or mitiga- trier of fact (p. 589). Rule 702 provides that a wit-
tion of an offense (United States v. Cameron, 1990, ness may qualify as an expert based on “knowledge,
p. 1066). Congress intended to preclude “dimin- skill, experience, training, or education.” However,
ished capacity” testimony by restricting the use of courts must evaluate expert testimony and an
defendant’s supposed “psychiatric compulsion or expert may testify only if: 1) the testimony is based
inability or failure to engage in normal reflection” upon sufficient facts or data, 2) the testimony is the
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 89

product of reliable principles or methods, and abilities of defendant were irrelevant, the balanc-
3) the witness applied the principles and methods ing of relevance was the more essential analysis
reliably to facts in the case (Fed. R. Evid. 702). under Daubert and the Federal Rules. As such, the
Although declining to establish a definitive Santos-Bueno court asked whether the probative
checklist, the Court examined whether the theory value of the expert testimony “to the extent that
and methods used: 1) were generally adopted by any exists—is substantially outweighed by its
the scientific community (Frye “general accep- potential to mislead or . . . confuse the jury”
tance” test), 2) were subject to peer review and (p. 23). However, this legal analysis also relies, in
publication, 3) can be or have been tested, and part, on Fed. R. Evid. 403.
4) have a known and acceptable error rate
(Daubert, p. 597). Neuropsychologist Testimony Does not
Although the government evidence did not Mislead or Confuse the Jury
challenge the scientific reliability of the neuro- Although a court may find neuropsychological
psychological evaluation, Judge Saylor concluded evidence reliable and relevant under Daubert and
that “the reasoning and methodology applied by Rule 702, such evidence may be excluded if its
Dr. Gansler in drawing conclusions” appears reli- value to the court does not outweigh its potential
able (Santos-Bueno, p. 24). Dr. Gansler’s reliance to mislead or confuse the jury. In addressing this
on standardized psychological tests, for example, question, the court applied Fed. R. Evid. 403, as
the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–III, follows:
although not infallible, as a tool for assessing cog-
nitive function “is not seriously disputed in this Although relevant, evidence may be excluded
case” (p. 24). Accordingly, the court held that if its probative value is substantially outweighed
Dr. Gansler’s testimony regarding cognitive abili- by the danger of unfair prejudice, confusion of
ties were reliable sufficiently under Daubert and the issues, or misleading the jury, or by consid-
Rule 702, and were not excluded. erations of undue delay, waste of time, or need-
less presentation of cumulative evidence.
Relevance of Neuropsychologist (Fed. R. Evid. 403)
Testimony
In addition to Fed. R. Evid. 702 governing expert In balancing these factors, the court must
testimony, the criminal court also addressed decide whether the proposed expert testimony
Fed. R. Evid. 704, as follows: “could improperly suggest . . . the abolished
diminished capacity defense is available” or that
No expert witness testifying with respect to the Mr. Santos-Bueno is “entitled to sympathy and
mental state or condition of a defendant in a possible nullification” (p. 26). Psychiatric evidence
criminal case may state an opinion or inference “presents an inherent danger” of distracting the
as to whether the defendant did or did not have jury from their task of evaluating the govern-
the mental state or condition constituting an ment’s proof of each element of the alleged offense.
element of the crime charged or of a defense In this case, there was concern as to whether
thereto. Such ultimate issues are matters for the Dr. Gansler’s testimony may improperly open up
trier of fact alone. the jury to theories of defense that do not exist
under current law or provide an erroneous
Here, all parties stipulated that Dr. Gansler may justification for the alleged crime (p. 26).
not offer any opinions on whether Mr. Santos- Consequently, Judge Saylor limited the scope of
Bueno “actually knew (or recklessly disregard Dr. Ganlser’s testimony to those expert opinions
[ed])” that the van passengers were illegal aliens addressing how Mr. Santos-Bueno’s cognitive
(p. 23). However, because the defense offered disabilities impacted his factual inferences, or lack
Dr. Gansler’s opinion only in rebuttal to the of such inferences, regarding whether the van
government’s proof of actual knowledge, Rule 704 passengers were illegal aliens. Further, the Judge
does not apply. excluded any opinions about “alleged difficulty in
While conceding the reliability of neuro- exercising judgment, his impaired ability to appre-
psychological methodology, the government chal- ciate the consequences of his actions, and . . . to
lenged the relevance of the neuropsychological ‘execute appropriate plans of action’” as being out-
evaluation and expert testimony about cognitive weighed by the potential to mislead or confuse the
abilities. In addition to arguing that the cognitive jury (p. 27).
90 forensic neuropsychology

Then, the court proceeded to apply a similar, pre-trial determinations), apparently because
although briefer, analysis to evaluate the admi- Daubert reminded that judges have an obligation
ssibility of neuropsychological evidence and to do pretrial assessments of the reasoning and
Dr. Gansler’s opinion on the defense claim the methodology of experts. Although there were
Mr. Santos-Bueno was more vulnerable to sugges- increased references to Daubert, Groscup et al.
tion due to his head injury. Judge Saylor expressed (2002) noted no concomitant increase in analysis
“grave concern about both the scientific reliability of its four suggested factors. Increasingly, courts
and the relevance of Dr. Gansler’s statement” about are trying to dispose of admissibility issues in Rule
enhanced vulnerability to suggestion (p. 29). The 104 hearings as a result of Daubert.
court granted the defense an additional week to Not surprisingly, Daubert also caused greater
supplement the record on this question.21 discussion of Frye, if for no other reason than to
Santos-Bueno illustrates current application of distinguish the holding. Even though Daubert
standards for admissibility to neuropsychological and its progeny were superseded by the 2002
evidence in a recent federal criminal case. amendment to Rule 702, the discussion of Frye
Although the court applied Daubert in its analysis remains relevant because a minority of states (16)
of Dr. Gansler’s proposed testimony, the court still use its general acceptance standard, including
does not, nor should it, provide a thorough under- CA, NY, FL, IL, and AZ (Lustre, 2004). Groscup
standing of the significant impact of Daubert on et al. (2002) reported a decreased use of the gen-
the use of expert testimony in criminal cases. The eral acceptance standard in jurisdictions adopting
next section focuses on the broader trends and Daubert, and an increase in reliance on its falsifi-
far-reaching implications of Daubert and its prog- ability, peer review, and error rate criteria. As
eny on the standards for admissibility of expert courts have adjusted to the gatekeeping function,
testimony. courts spent less time analyzing Daubert when
disposing of expert admissibility issues.
T H E I M PA C T O F D AU B E R T In a result with greater implications for clini-
ON ADMIS SIBILITY OF cal neuropsychology, scientific expert evidence is
EXPERT TESTIMONY IN treated differently than medical-mental health,
C R I M I N A L L I T I G AT I O N technical, or business evidence. In addition to
Research demonstrates that greater judicial scru- lengthier discussions of Daubert, court consider-
tiny after Daubert was increasingly fatal to civil ation of Rules 104, 403, and the Frye general
cases, with 90% of summary judgments going acceptance test were much longer and more influ-
against the plaintiff (Dixon & Gill, 2002). That is, ential on admissibility for scientific expert opin-
testimony from plaintiff experts was more readily ion. Obviously, these rules existed before Daubert,
excluded. In contrast, there has been little change but results show greater scrutiny of scientific
in admission rates for expert evidence in criminal expert opinions on key evidentiary rules, in addi-
litigation at the trial and appellate level (Groscup, tion to the Daubert factors of falsifiability, peer
Penrod, Studebaker, Huss, & O’Neil, 2002). review, and error rate. This elevated scrutiny has
Further, the setting of a standard for appellate led to greater exclusion of scientific expert testi-
review in Joiner had little impact on the admissi- mony when compared with medical-mental
bility of expert testimony in criminal litigation. health testimony. Clinical neuropsychologists are
Nonetheless, Daubert and its progeny prompted trained in a scientist-practitioner training model
greater scrutiny of experts by criminal court judges and may be retained to offer expert opinions as
and brought about many changes to the manner in scientists or board-certified providers of medical–
which expert testimony is evaluated. Groscup et al. mental health services. Results show that when
(2002) note a few general trends in criminal appel- the expert’s opinion is viewed as scientific, Daubert
late cases as briefly summarized below. factors must be met in order for the court to admit
Since Daubert, relevance of expert testimony the expert evidence. For most other experts, pre-
was discussed at greater lengths in criminal appel- sentation of information addressing Daubert is
late decisions, suggesting that courts spend more probably unnecessary. Courts tend to rely more
time analyzing the Federal Rules or applying the heavily on whether the expert testimony will assist
Daubert factors. Specifically, judges spent more the trier of fact (Rule 702) and that its probative
time scrutinizing expert evidence under Fed. R. value outweighs its prejudicial effect (Rule 403).
Evid. 702. However, judges also devoted more Medical-mental health practitioners seeking
time to analyzing Fed. R. Evid. 104 (permitting expert qualification should be more prepared to
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 91

discuss their indicia of expertise, for example, cases that commonly incorporate structural
education, experience, specialty, board certifica- neuroimaging studies when rendering clinical
tion, and so forth, rather than their own research formulations. It is routine practice for clinical
or an existing body of research. neuropsychologists to use neuroanatomical find-
Courts increasingly rely on neuropsychologist ings when drawing inferences about brain-behav-
experts to assist the jury in resolving certain legal ior relations and when rendering expert opinions.
claims (Kaufmann, 2005; 2009c). However, legal To date, there are no studies comparing the rela-
scholars generally avoid the legal implications of tive persuasiveness of expert opinions based on
neuroscience in our courts (Greely, 2006), with neuropsychological evaluations presented with
only six published articles using the terms “neuro- and without functional neuroimages, even as it is
science” or “neuroimaging” in their titles (p. 607). widely presumed that neuroimages have a preju-
However, a more recent Lexis search22 revealed 62 dicial impact on jury deliberations (Brown &
law review and bar journal articles, with 52 of Murphy, 2010).
those published in the last 3.5 years. As noted in In summary, Daubert assigned and Joiner
the recent cases from California, New York, and reinforced the gatekeeping role of the trial court
Tennessee (federal), neuroimaging techniques for judge in determining the admissibility of expert
detecting deceit are no better than lie-detectors, testimony, thereby raising the level of scrutiny of
with neither technique being sufficiently reliable experts and the basis for their testimony. Daubert
for admission (Keckler, 2006). Thirteen years ago, also partially defined a road map, in the form of
few neuroimaging findings were considered flexible factors, for experts to follow in order to
specific enough to address legal questions of have their testimony admitted and ultimately
cognitive or volitional impairment (Kulynych, heard by the trier of fact. Santos-Bueno showed
1997). Although neuroimaging has advanced rap- how careful attention to evidentiary and proce-
idly, a carefully conducted neuropsychological dural rules in a criminal proceeding, including
evaluation remains a more reliable and legally rel- Daubert factors, allows expert neuropsychologists
evant source of information about an individual’s to be recognized by the court in a preliminary evi-
level of functioning because neuropsychological dentiary hearing. Ultimately, Dr. Gansler’s expert
tests have much better normative data (also see neuropsychological opinions were admitted as
the chapter by Ricker in the current volume). sufficiently reliable and relevant to assist the jury.
Indeed, it is on the basis of reliable and valid nor-
mative data that neuropsychological tests meet WRONGFUL DISCLO SURE
the requirements of admission under Daubert OF PSYCHOLOGICAL
and the Federal Rules (Kaufmann, 2005; 2008). T E S T S T H R E AT E N S
Lawyers may believe that a neuroimage pic- T H E A D M I S S I B I LT Y O F
ture is worth a thousand words, but the general NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL
public does not seem to carry an inherent bias OPINIONS
toward greater reliance on neuroimages than neu- At the same time that clinical neuropsychology
ropsychological test results (Guilmette, Kennedy, has experienced unprecedented growth in foren-
Weiler, & Temple, 2006). However, students sic consultation, discovery rules and isolated court
ascribe greater explanatory power to neurosci- decisions threaten the validity of neuropsycho-
ence publications that use the phrase “brain scans logical methods (Kaufmann, 2005; 2009c).
indicate” and present neuroimages, masking oth- Specifically, the strong public policy underlying
erwise problematic explanations (Weisberg et al., test security (Detroit Edison Co. v. NLRB, 1979), as
2008). Another recent study noted that under- applied to clinical cases (Chiperas v. Rubin, 1998),
graduate students tend to rate sham scientific is being threatened by the overly zealous discov-
articles as “better written” if the article included ery demands of litigation. Standardized psycho-
neuroimages (McCabe & Castel, 2008). The risk metric tests used during forensic evaluations
of undue prejudice has recently prompted some provide the best technology available to assist the
commentators to suggest that trials using neu- jury in resolving certain legal claims (Kaufmann,
roimages may require special jury instructions or 2005). Wrongful disclosure of psychological test
hiring court-appointed experts to assist judges to materials allows opportunistic litigants and their
“fortify the gate” (Compton, 2010). But these sci- attorneys to “review test protocols, obtain test
entific reports and legal commentary do not com- items, discover answers, and ‘cheat’ on the tests in
pare neuropsychological evaluations from actual the future” (p. 100), thereby risking the validity of
92 forensic neuropsychology

future applications of neuropsychological meth- to amend a protective order requiring the return
ods in our courts (also see Wetter & Corrigan, of a videotape made during a psychological evalu-
1995; Youngjohn, 1995; Youngjohn, Lees-Haley, ation (Fla. Dept. of Trans. v. Piccolo, 2007).
& Binder, 1999; Abeles, 2001; Rüsseler et al., Ultimately, neither of these cases were successful
2008). Recent articles provide specific recommen- in modifying the Broyles v. Reilly rule, but efforts
dations for minimizing the risks associated with continue with the Florida Psychological
wrongful disclosure (Prohaska & Martin, 2007; Association and the legislature (Howe, Rice, &
Kaufmann, 2009a; Morrell, 2009). In keeping Hoese, 2007). For more recent information on the
with the theme of this chapter, such wrongful dis- harmful effects of TPO and the most effective
closure could possibly lead judges to exclude neu- responses for challenging litigation tactics, see
ropsychological evidence in pre-trial hearings, Grieffenstein and Kaufmann (this volume) and
because opinions based on tainted data would not Howe and McCaffrey (2010).
be sufficiently reliable under Daubert. In Detroit In a unanimous decision, a California
Edison, the U.S. Supreme Court ordered that such appellate court held that copyright protection
materials should be released only to psychologists does not preclude a test recipient from obtaining
in order to “insure [sic] the future integrity of the their answers and the test questions after complet-
tests” (p. 308). ing a mental examination using psychometric
Although about 20 states have acted to protect tests (Carpenter v. Yamaha Motor Corp., 2006).
psychological test materials from wrongful dis- The court also found that a test recipient could
closure, potentially harmful court decisions obtain the names of the tests before the examina-
caused concerned professionals to create the tion and—with the assistance of counsel, psychol-
“Group Protecting the Integrity of Psychological ogist, or other expert—could object to certain
Examinations” (hereinafter, G–PIPE) Berman v. tests if deemed inappropriate for the purposes of
Kuckarski, (2006). G–PIPE consists of individual the mental examination. Although the Carpenter
practitioners, neuropsychology credentialing court acknowledged that disclosure of test materi-
boards, and state psychological associations who als before the examination “could affect the integ-
are concerned about negative consequences of rity of the tests” (p. 267), this appellate panel failed
test disclosure on the objectivity, fairness, and to appreciate that test identification before exami-
integrity of neuropsychological evaluations and nation posed a risk. While remanding the case for
practice. G-PIPE also includes test developers, further proceedings due to deficiencies in the
publishers, marketers, and distributors who are record, Carpenter saw no threat in providing test
concerned about copyright infringement, trade names because “actual test questions are a care-
secret protection, and other intellectual property fully guarded secret among the publishers and
rights associated with their tests. G-PIPE has examiners” (p. 268). Yet in Detroit Edison, all of
addressed challenges to the integrity of neuropsy- the justices recognized the risk of test disclosure,
chological evaluations posed by third-party with the majority commenting that protections
observers/recording devices (hereinafter, TPO) afforded by restrictive orders were not persuasive
and compliance with court orders protecting the (p. 314), and the minority commenting on the
integrity of test materials.23 harm associated with inadvertent disclosure
Currently, Florida places the burden of proof (p. 324). In the end, the U.S. Supreme Court has
on the party seeking to avoid a TPO during found a better balance in clarifying the strong
a psychological evaluation (Broyles v. Reilly, 1997). public policy underlying test security, ordering
In Broyles, the court set forth a two-part test that psychometric tests should only be released to
requiring: 1) case-specific reasons why a TPO is psychologists. Subsequent state courts (Fla. Dept.
disruptive to the evaluation, and 2) no other qual- of Trans. v. Piccolo, 2007) and federal courts
ified provider in the area is willing to conduct the (Taylor v. Erna, 2009; Lumsden v. United States of
evaluation in the presence of a TPO. This legal test America, 2010) continue to explore the contours
was developed originally for medical legal exami- of the Detroit Edison psychologist nondisclosure
nations, and G-PIPE argues that psychological privilege/duty to safeguard psychological test
evaluations are distinguishable because they materials. The reader is directed to Greiffenstein
employ psychometric tests. G-PIPE advocates that and Kaufmann (this volume) for solutions to the
the party seeking the TPO have the burden to “raw data” problem.
show case-specific reasons why a TPO should be The Florida and California TPO cases show
allowed. In a related Florida case, G-PIPE sought how easily a court could disclose psychological
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 93

test materials into the public domain, thereby Although the law seeks every rational means
compromising the integrity of neuropsychologi- for ascertaining the truth, public policy must
cal practice and forensic consultation. This promote the truth-seeking function of the judi-
chapter has demonstrated the importance of pro- ciary by protecting psychological test materials
tecting psychological test materials, standardized from wrongful disclosure in a manner that serves
instructions, and normative comparisons in all parties and the profession, while preserving
developing neuropsychological evidence and justice in our courts.
rendering reliable opinions in court. Precedent-
setting court decisions may sway the public
policy debate away from test security, in favor of
N OT E S
1. Extended excerpts from Kaufmann, P. M.
evidentiary discovery rules, such that courts will
(2008). Admissibility of neuropsychological evidence
increasingly find neuropsychological opinions
in criminal cases: Competency, insanity, culpability,
unreliable. Wrongful disclosure of psychological and mitigation. In R. L. Denney & J. P. Sullivan (Vol.
test materials is a looming threat to the admissi- Ed.), Clinical Neuropsychology in the Criminal Forensic
bility of neuropsychological evidence in our Setting. New York: Guilford Press are contained herein
courts, prompting more aggressive legal advocacy with the publisher’s permission.
efforts by G-PIPE and within AACN (Kaufmann, 2. Statements of opinion or belief considered
2009c). authoritative because they are made by the court.
Compare to holding, which is “a court’s determination
CONCLUSIONS of a matter of law pivotal to its decision; a principle
Three out of every four reported cases referencing drawn from such a decision.” Black’s Law Dictionary
(7th Edition). Dicta may be persuasive, but do not set
neuropsychology have been adjudicated in the
precedent. Holdings create binding precedent within
past decade. This rapid increase in use of neuro-
the applicable jurisdiction of the court.
psychology to assist juries shows that neuropsy-
3. Current case being heard by the court.
chologist expert opinions meet the criteria for 4. There is no genuine issue of material fact upon
admissibility under Daubert and the Federal which the plaintiff could prevail as a matter of law. The
Rules. As such, courts are admitting neuropsy- trial judge rendered a verdict for Merrill Dow based on
chologist expert opinions on essential questions briefs without a trial.
in administrative, probate, civil, and criminal 5. The most common mechanism used by the U.S.
cases. Standardized, norm-referenced psycho- Supreme Court, in which it chooses to hear a case by
metric tests provide the most reliable and valid order directing a lower court to deliver the case record.
procedures for addressing questions involving the 6. However, about 16 states still use a Frye type of
mental condition of the civil plaintiff and the standard, including CA, NY, FL, IL, and AZ.
criminal defendant. Unlike other mental health 7. A motion to limit or exclude allegedly prejudi-
professionals, psychologists use objective psycho- cial evidence presented to the judge or during a trial.
logical tests as part of a scientific methodology to 8. See Hoyt, T. D. (Fall, 2009). Is the fake bad scale
refine their impressions when formulating expert test here to stay? Mass Torts, 8(1), 14–15, 23. citing Nejo
opinions (Kaufmann, 2005; 2009c). Applying the v. Tamaroff Buick Honda Isuzu Nissan, 88 Fed. App’x
unique brain-behavior knowledge base, neurop- 881, 885 (6th Cir. Feb. 23, 2004); Thomas v. Sec’y of
sychologists use psychological tests in conjunc- Health and Human Servs., No. 98–1185, 1998 WL
tion with neurologic findings and neuroimaging 516815, at *1 (7th Cir. Aug. 17, 1998) (unpublished);
techniques in a scientific enterprise that assists Carovski v. Jordan, No. 06CV716S, 2008 WL 1805813,
the trier of fact to resolve certain legal claims. at *1 (W.D.N.Y. Apr. 18, 2008); Adams v. Astrue, No.
06–5132-CV-S-JCE-SSA, 2008 WL 508683, at *3 (W.D.
Neuropsychological methods fulfill Daubert and
Mo. Feb. 21, 2008); Eubanks v. Astrue, No. 03–427-
Frye requirements and the board-certified neu-
WDS, 2008 WL 515001, at *6 (S.D. Ill. Feb. 22, 2008);
ropsychologist versed in the constitutional and
Cummins v. Unumprovident Ins. Co., No. 04–339-A,
judicial foundations of forensic consultation shall 2007 WL 4104275, at *11 (M.D. La. Nov. 15, 2007);
continue to provide an important expert service. Contreras v.United States, No. 1:03-CV-360, 2004 WL
That is, the forensic neuropsychologist expert 3457632, at *7 (W.D. Mich. Oct. 26, 2004); Spurgeon v.
shall offer admissible neuropsychological evi- Barnhart, No. 2:02 CV 29 DDN, 2003 WL 25734676,
dence that is more probative than prejudicial of at *5 (E.D. Mo. Sept. 16, 2003); Sochor v. State, 883 So.
the ultimate issues of civil liability and criminal 2d 766, 779 (Fla. 2004); Posey v. Singletary, 795 So. 2d
responsibility. 1249, 1259 (La. App. 2001); Muhammed v. State, 46
94 forensic neuropsychology

S.W.3d 493, 508 (Tex. App. 2001); See also Chuk, S. R. 20. Rule allowing the court to order the defen-
(2009). NOTE: It’s (Not) Bad, It’s (Not Bad), you know dant to be examined by a government expert after
it: The growing acceptance of the “Fake Bad Scale.” the defendant provides notice of intent to intro-
Villanova Law Review, 54, 479–513. duce expert evidence relating to a mental disease or
9. Campbell v. Papell, Florida 12th Circuit, Sarasota defect.
County (March 19, 2009 Order denying release of raw 21. Defense counsel reported that the admission
data and psychological test materials to the plaintiff of neuropsychological evidence was pivotal in the gov-
attorney); see also Sierra v. Reyes Florida 13th Circuit, ernment’s offer to let her client plea guilty to a lesser
Hillsborough County (February 16, 2009 Order deny- offense, that is, conspiracy to transport illegal aliens,
ing release of raw data). being sentenced to time already served and avoiding
10. See Foster, S., Qualification of NonMedical immediate deportation (personal communication).
Psychologist to Testify as to Mental Condition or 22. Conducted July 15, 2011.
Competency, 72 A.L.R.5th 529 (1999); citing Russell v. 23. For more information on G-PIPE, contact
State, 775 So. 2d 866 (Ala. CIM. App. 1997); People v. Laura Lee Shaw Howe, JD, PhD.
Noble, 42 Ill. 2d 425, 248 N.E.2d 96 (1969); State v.
Alexander, 252 La. 564, 211 So. 2d 650 (1968); Saul v. REFERENCES
State, 6 Md. App. 540, 252, A.2d 282 (1969),; Span v. Abeles, N. (2001). Challenges of test coaching in
Bees, 23 Md. App. 313, 327 A.2d 801 (1974); People v. assessment. Testing International, 11(2), 4–6.
Diaz, 51 N.Y. 2d 841, 433 N.Y.S. 2d 751, 413 N.E. 2d Adamson v. Chiovaro, 705 A.2d 402 (N.J. Super.
1166 (1980); Com v. Williams, 270 Pa. Super. 27, 410 App. Div. 1998).
A.2d 880 (1979); State v. Williams, 278 Md. 180, 361 Alexander, A. (2006). Functional magnetic resonance
A.2d 122 (1976); Saul v. State, 6 Md. App. 520, 252 imaging lie detection: Is a “brainstorm” heading
A.2d 282 (1969). toward a “gatekeeper.” Houston Journal of Health
11. Intermediate evidentiary standard that exceeds Law and Policy, 7, 1–56.
the preponderance of the evidence standard used in Am. Jur. 2d Crim. Law §§ 48, 62, 63, 67, 68, 87, 94, 95,
civil litigation, but less than the beyond a reasonable 102, 103, 119, 120 (2007).
doubt standard used in criminal litigation. Therefore, AACN Board of Directors. (2007). American Academy
although the defendant has the burden to show that of Clinical Neuropsychology (AACN) Practice
insanity is highly probable, the defense is not required Guidelines for Neuropsychological Assessment
to prove it beyond the shadow of a doubt. and Consultation. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
12. Courts in different jurisdictions operate under 21, 209–31.
different legal precedent and rules. Atkins v. Virginia, 536 U.S. 304, 318 (U.S. 2002).
13. Specific intent refers to the state of mind Appelbaum, P. S. (2009). Through a glass darkly:
required to accomplish a precise criminal act that can Functional neuroimaging evidence enters the
be negated by intoxication or insanity. courtroom. Psychiatric Services, 60(1), 21–23.
14. Mens rea means guilty mind or the state of Baker, D. B. & Benjamin, L. T. (2000). The affirmation
mind that the prosecution must prove beyond a rea- of the scientist-practitioner: A look back at Boulder.
sonable doubt in order to secure a conviction. American Psychologist, 55, 241–47.
15. Conducted on July 3, 2010. Baxter v. State, No. 217-2010-CV-00683 (N. H.
16. See Anand Giridharadas, Brain Scan a New Sup. Ct., Oct. 26, 2010) (memorandum of law
Wave in Criminal Evidence, INT’L HERALD TRIB., in support of the Hon. John T. Broderick, Jr.’s
Sept. 8, 2008, at 1; Anand Giridharadas, India’s Novel standing to file the October 12, 2010 motion in
Use of Brain Scans in Courts is Debated, N.Y. TIMES, limine).
Sept. 14, 2008, at A8. Baxter v. Temple, No. 01–c–0567 (N.H. Sup. Ct. Aug. 8,
17. See http://www.uakron.edu/law/lawreview/ 2005) (order granting motion in limine).
v42/v42num3.dot, for series of nine papers on Baxter v. Temple, 949 A.2d 167, (N.H., 2008).
neuroimaging applications and the law, last visited Batzel v. Gault, Law No. 195596, Fairfax Circuit Court,
July 5, 2010. 2002 (April 12, 2002 Order).
18. Morse describes the speculative claims of some Ben-Porath, Y. S., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J.,
practitioners about the implications of neuroscience & Kaufmann, P. M. (2009a). The MMPI-2
for criminal responsibility that cannot be conceptually, Symptom Validity Scale (FBS) is an empirically
empirically, or legally sustained; recommending “cog- validated measure of over-reporting in personal
nitive jurotherapy” as the treatment of choice for brain injury litigants and claimants: Reply to Butcher
overclaim syndrome. et al. (2008). Psychological Injury and Law, 2(1),
19. See The Law and Neuroscience Project at www. 62–85.
lawandneuroscienceproject.org; last visited July 5, Ben-Porath, Y. S., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., &
2010. Kaufmann, P. M. (2009b). The MMPI-2 Symptom
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 95

Validity Scale (FBS) is an empirically validated Chuk, S. R. (2009). NOTE: It’s (Not) Bad, It’s (Not Bad),
measure of over-reporting in personal injury liti- you know it: The growing acceptance of the “Fake
gants and claimants: Reply to Williams et al. (2009). Bad Scale.” Villanova Law Review, 54, 479–513.
Psychological Injury and Law, 3(1), 77–80. Compton, E. S. (2010). Not guilty by reason of neu-
Benjamin, L. T. (2005). A history of clinical psychology as roimaging: The need for cautionary jury instruc-
a profession in America (and a glimpse at its future). tions for neuroscience evidence in criminal trials.
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 1–30. Vanderbilt Journal of Entertainment and Technology
Benton, A. L. & Spreen, O. (1961). Visual memory test: Law, 12(2), 333–55.
The simulation of mental incompetence. Archives Creager, R. T., Shea, J. C., & Larner, G. P. (Fall, 2002).
of General Psychiatry, 4, 79–83. Role of defense neuropsychologists should be
Bergeson v. Ray, Middlesex Superior Court, Docket limited under Virginia evidence law. The Journal of
No.: 1998–5696. the Virginia Trial Lawyers Association.
Berman v. Kuckarski, Florida 5th Circuit, Case No. Cunningham v. Montgomery, 921 P.2d 1355 (Or. Ct.
5D06–2053, (2006). App., 1995).
Bigler, E.D. (2001). Neuropsychological testing defines Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc., 727 F. Supp. 570,
the neurobehavioral significance of neuroimaging- 575 (S.D. Cal., 1989).
identified abnormalities. Archives of Clinical Neuro- Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc., 951 F.2d 1128
psychology, 16, 227–28. (9th Cir., 1992).
Billiot v. State, 655 So.2d 1, 17–18 (Miss. 1995). Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc., 509 U.S. 579 (U.S.
Blair, J. (2010). Medicolegal neuroscience: My brain 1993).
made me do it! Scientific overview and frontiers. Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc., 43 F.3d 1311 (9th
Penn Conference on Clinical Neuroscience and Cir., 1995).
Society, July, 2010 Philadelphia, PA. Davidson v. Strawberry Petroleum, Inc., Florida 13th
Bonner v. ISP Technologies, Inc., 259 F.3d 924 (8th Circuit, Hillsborough County (May 30, 2007 Order).
Cir., 2001). Denney, R. L. (2005). Criminal responsibility and other
Boone, K. (2007). Assessment of feigned cognitive criminal forensic issues. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.)
impairment: A neuropsychological perspective. Forensic neuropsychology: A scientific approach (pp.
New York, NY: Guilford Publications. 325–465). New York: Oxford University Press.
Brief for Amicus Curiae Group Protecting the Integrity Denney, R. L. & Sullivan, J. P. (2008). Clinical neuro-
of Psychological Examinations, Berman v. psychology in the criminal forensic setting. Guilford
Kuckarski (Fla. 5th DCA 2006) (No. 5D06–2053). Press: New York.
Brief for Amicus Curiae Group Protecting the Integrity Desmond, J.M. (Winter, 2007). Admissibility of neu-
of Psychological Examinations, Florida Dept. of ropsychological evidence in New Hampshire.
Trans. v. Piccolo (Dist. Ct. App. Fla., 2006) (No. New Hampshire Bar Journal, 47(4), 12–17,
04–11035). Retrieved May 7, 2007 from http://www.nhbar.org/
Brown, T. & Murphy, E. (2010). Through a scanner publications/display-journal-issue.asp?id=347
darkly: Functional neuroimaging as evidence of a Detroit Edison Co. v. NLRB, 440 U.S. 301 (U.S. 1979).
criminal defendant’s past mental states. Stanford Dixon, L. & Gill, B. (2002). Changes in the standards
Law Review, 62, 1119–1207. for admitting expert evidence in federal civil cases
Broyles v. Reilly, 695 So.2d 832, (Dist. Ct. App. Fla., standards since the Daubert decision. Psychology,
1997). Public Policy, and Law, 8, 251–308.
Buckler v. Sinclair Refining Co., 216 N.E.2d 14, 19 Downs v. Perstorp Components, Inc., 126 F. Supp.2d
(Ill. App. Ct., 1966). 1090 (E.D. Tenn., 1999).
Butcher, J. N., Gass, C. S., Cumella, E., Kally, Z., & Drope v. Missouri , 420 U.S. 162, 171–172 (U.S. 1975).
Williams, C. L. (2008). Potential bias in MMPI-2 Dusky v. United States, 362 U.S. 402, 402 (U.S. 1960).
assessments using the Fake Bad Scale (FBS). Dvoskin, J.A. (2002). Knowledge is not power–Knowl-
Psychological Injury and the Law, 1(3), 191–209. edge is obligation. Journal of the American Academy
Calloway v. State, 651 So.2d 752, 754 (Dist. Ct. App. of Psychiatry and the Law, 30(4), 533–40.
Fla., 1995). Ellis v. Appleton Papers, Inc., 2006 U.S. Dist. LEXIS
Campbell v. Papell, Florida 12th Circuit, Sarasota 7164, (N.D.N.Y., 2006).
County (March 19, 2009 Order). Erickson, S. K. (2010). Neuroscience: Blaming the
Carpenter v. Yamaha Motor Corp., 141 Cal. App. 4th brain. Minnesota Journal of Law, Science and
249 (Cal. Ct. App., 2006). Technology, 11. 27–77.
Chapple v. Gangar, 851 F. Supp. 1481, (E.D. Wash., Executive Car & Truck Leasing, Inc. v. DeSerio, 468
1994). So.2d 1027 (Dist. Ct. App. Fla., 1985).
Chiperas v. Rubin, 1998 U.S. Dist. LEXIS 23578 (D.C. Fabianke v. Weaver ex rel. Weaver, 527 So.2d 1253 (Ala.
Cir., 1998). 1988).
96 forensic neuropsychology

Fed. R. Crim. P. 12.2(a), (b), (c)(1)(B) (2009). neuropsychological and CT scan results: Forensic
Fed. R. Evid. 104, 401, 403, 702, 703, 704 (2009). implications. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20,
Frendak v. United States, 408 A.2d 364, 380 (D.C. 1979). 305–314.
Fla. Admin. Code Ann. r. 64B19–18.004(3) (2009). Guzman v. 4030 Bronx Blvd. Assoc. L.L.C., 54 A.D.3d
Fla. Admin. Code Ann. r. 64B19–19.005(3) (2009). 42, (N.Y. App. Div., 2008).
Florida Dept. of Trans. v. Piccolo, 964 So. 2d 773, Haederle, M. (2010, February 23). A mind of crime:
(Fla. Cir. Ct. 2nd, 2007). How brain scanning technology is redefining
Ford v. Wainwright, 477 U.S. 399, 409–410 (U.S. 1986). criminal culpability. Miller-McCune.
Foster, S.K. (2004). Qualification of nonmedical psy- Hagen v. Swenson, 236 N.W.2d 161 (Minn. 1975).
chologist to testify as to mental condition or com- Hanna, M. (August 8, 2010). Eight years and counting.
petency. American Law Reports 5th, 72, 529–595. Concord Monitor.
Frye v. United States, 293 F. 1013 (D.C. Cir., 1923). Heaton, R. K., Smith, H. H., Jr., Lehman, R. A., & Vogt,
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2008). The law and neuroscience. A. J. (1978). Prospects for faking believable deficits
Neuron, 60(3), 412–415. on neuropsychological testing. Journal of Clinical
General Electric Co. v. Joiner, 522 U.S. 136 (U.S. 1997). and Experimental Neuropsychology, 46, 892–900.
G.I.W. Southern Valve v. Smith, 471 So.2d 81 (Dist. Ct. Heilbronner, R. L. (2004). A status report on the prac-
App. Fla. 1985) tice of forensic neuropsychology. The Clinical
Giridharadas, A. (2008, Sept. 8). Brain scan: A new Neuropsychologist, 18, 312–26.
wave in criminal evidence. International Herald Heilbronner, R. L., Sweet, J. J., Morgan, J. E., Larrabee, G.
Tribune, p. 1. J., Millis, S. R., and Conference Participants. (2009).
Giridharadas, A. (2008, Sept. 14). India’s novel use of American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology
brain scans in courts. New York Times, p. A8. consensus conference on the neuropsychological
Goeb v. Tharaldson, 615 N.W.2d 800, (Minn., 2000). assessment of effort, response bias, and malingering.
Goldstein, A. M. (2003). Overview of forensic psycho- The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, 1093–129.
logy. In I. B. Weiner (Series Ed.) and A.M. Goldstein Heilbrun, K. (2001). Principles of forensic mental
(Vol. Ed.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. 11: Forensic health assessment. New York: Kluwer Academic/
psychology (pp. 3–20). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Plenum.
Greely, H. T. (2006). Neuroethics and ELSI: Similarities Heltzel, T. (2007). Compatibility of therapeutic and
and differences. Minnesota Journal of Law, Science, forensic roles. Professional Psychology: Research
& Technology, 7, 599–637. and Practice, 38(2), 122–28.
Greenberg, S. A. & Shuman, D. W. (1997). Irreconcilable Hom, J. (2008). Commentary. Response to Bigler
conflict between therapeutic and forensic roles. (2007): The sky is not falling. Archives of Clinical
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 28, Neuropsychology, 23, 125–28.
50–57. Howard v. State, 698 S.W.2d 23, 25 (Mo. Ct. App., 1985).
Greenberg, S. A. & Shuman, D. W. (2007). When worlds Howe, L. L. S. & McCaffrey, R. (2010). Third party
collide: Therapeutic and forensic roles. Professional observation during neuropsychological evaluation:
Psychology: Research and Practice, 38(2), 129–32. An update on the literature, practical advice for
Greiffenstein, M. F. & Cohen, L. (2005). Neuro- practitioners, and future directions. The Clinical
psychology and the law: Principles of productive Neuropsychologist, 24(3), 518–37.
attorney-neuropsychologist relations. In G. J. Howe, L. L. S., Rice, W. J., & Hoese, V. (Summer 2007).
Larrabee (Vol Ed.), Forensic neuropsychology: A Psychological ethics and third party observers
scientific approach (pp. 29–91). New York: Oxford (TPO): We’ve observed their effect and now need
University Press. to act. Florida Psychologist, 58(2), 18–19, 35.
Grenitz v. Tomlian, 858 So.2d 999 (Fla., 2003). Howe, L. L. S., Sweet, J. J., & Bauer, R. M. (2010).
Grisso, T. (1988). Competency to stand trial evalua- Advocacy 101: A step beyond just explaining: How
tions: A manual for practice. Sarasota, FL: Profe- the individual practitioner can become involved
ssional Resource Exchange. and make a difference. The Clinical Neuro-
Groscup, J. L., Penrod, S. D., Studebaker, C. A., Huss, psychologist, 24(3), 373–90.
M. T., & O’Neil, K. M. (2002). Effects of Daubert on Hoyt, R. D. (Fall, 2009). Is the Fake Bad Scale test here
the admissibility of expert testimony on state and to stay? Mass Torts, 8(1), 14–15, 23.
federal criminal cases. Psychology, Public Policy, Hsieh, S. (2008, April 2). Defense experts using
and Law, 8, 339–72. controversial malingering scale. Lawyers USA,
Guilmette, T. J., Kennedy, M. L., Weiler, M. D., & pp. 1, 32.
R. O. Temple. (2006). Investigation of biases in the Hsieh, S. (2008, May 19). Plaintiff turns tables on “Fake
general public in evaluating mild head injury using Bad Scale.” Lawyers USA, pp. 1, 29.
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 97

Huber, P. W. (1991). Galileo’s revenge: Junk science in on the law and other persuasive legal strategies. The
the courtroom. New York: Basic Books. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23(7): 1130–59.
Huntoon v. TCI Cablevision of Colo., 969 P.2d 681 Kaufmann, P. M., Larrabee, G. J., & Bigler, E. D. (2010).
(Colo. 1998). Admissibility and appropriate use of symptom
Hutchinson v. American Family Mutual Ins. Co., 514 validity science in forensic consulting. Journal of
N.W.2d 882 (Iowa 1994). the International Neuropsychological Society, CE
Immigration and Nationality Act. 8 U.S.C. § 1324(a)(1) Workshop presented at the 38th Annual INS meet-
(A)(ii) (2007). ing, Acapulco, Mexico.
In re: Breast Implant Litigation, 11 F. Supp. 2d 1217, Kaufmann, P. M. & Wagner, A. (2010). Cognitive
(D. Colo., 1998). neuroscience, neuroimaging, and the law: Is a pic-
Insanity Defense Reform Act of 1984. (‘IDRA’) 18 U.S.C. ture worth a thousand words? Journal of the
§§ 17, 4244, 4245 (2007). International Neuropsychological Society, CE
Jackson v. Mason, Case No. 5:08-cv-5267-JLH (W.D. Workshop presented at the 38th Annual INS meet-
Ark, 2009)(October 8, 2009 Order). ing, Acapulco, Mexico.
Jenkins v. United States, 307 F.2d 637 (D.C. Cir. 1962). Keckler, C. N. W. (2006). Cross-examining the brain:
John v. Im, 559 S.E.2d 697, (Va. 2002). A legal analysis of neural imaging for credibility
Johnson v. Rockwell Automation, Inc. 2009 U.S. Dist. impeachment. Hastings Law Journal, 57, 509–56.
LEXIS 57450, (E.D. Ark., 2009). Kinsey v. King, 431 N.E.2d 1316 (Ill. App. Ct. 1982).
Kaufmann, P. M. (2005). Protecting the objectivity, Khoshbin, L. S. & Shahram, K. (2007). Imaging the
fairness, and integrity of neuropsychological evalu- mind, minding the image: A historical introduc-
ations in litigation: A privilege second to none? The tion to brain imaging and the law. American Journal
Journal of Legal Medicine, 26, 95–131. of Law and Medicine, 33, 171–92.
Kaufmann, P. M. (2006). Neuropsychology in the Krevitz v. Savoy Heating and Air Conditioning Co., 396
courtroom: Discovery wars, records management, So.2d 49 (Ala. 1981).
scope of practice, and legal requirements. The Kulynych, J. (1997). Psychiatric evidence: High-tech
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 183. CE Workshop crystal ball? Stanford Law Review, 49, 1249–70.
presented at the 4th Annual Conference of the Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, 526 U.S. 137 (U.S. 1999).
American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology Landers v. Chrysler Corp., 963 S.W.2d 275 (Mo.Ct.App.
(AACN): Philadelphia, PA. 1998).
Kaufmann, P. M. (2007). Scientific evidence in the Larrabee, G. J. (2005). Forensic neuropsychology: A scien-
court room: Legal requirements and ethical dilem- tific approach. New York: Oxford University Press.
mas for the expert neuropsychologist. Journal of Larrabee, G. J. (2007). Assessment of malingered neuro-
the International Neuropsychology Society, 13, psychological deficits. New York: Oxford University
Supplement S1. CE Workshop presented at the Press.
35th Annual Meeting of the International Neuro- Larrabee, G. J. (2008). Flexible vs. fixed batteries in
psychological Society (INS): Portland, OR. forensic neuropsychological assessment: Reply to
Kaufmann, P. M. (2008). Admissibility of neuropsy- Bigler and Hom. Archives of Clinical Neuro-
chological evidence in criminal cases: Competency, psychology, 23, 167–80.
insanity, culpability, and mitigation. In: R. L. Larrabee, G. J., Greiffenstein, M. F., Greve, K. W., &
Denney & J. P. Sullivan (Eds.) Clinical neuropsy- Bianchini, K. J. (2007). Refining diagnostic criteria
chology in the criminal forensic setting. (pp. 55–90). for malingering. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.), Assessment
New York: Guilford Press. of malingered neuropsychological deficits (pp. 334–
Kaufmann, P. M. (2009a). Cases, controversies, and 71). New York: Oxford University Press.
legal authority in neuropsychological practice. The Larrabee, G. J., Millis, S. R., & Meyers, J. E. (2008).
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23(4), 556. CE Sensitivity to brain dysfunction of the Halstead-
Workshop presented at the 7th Annual AACN Reitan vs. an ability-focused neuropsychological
Conference, San Diego, CA. battery. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22,
Kaufmann, P. M. (2009b). Evidence law adapts to new 813–25.
science: Symptom validity techniques in litigation. Limbaugh-Kirker v. Decosta, Case No. 06-CA-000706,
Invited unpublished manuscript presented Florida 20th Circuit, Lee County (February 9, 2009
ABA 2009 Workers’ Compensation Midwinter Order).
Seminar and Conference, March 13, 2009, New Louderback v. Orkin Exterminating Co., 26 F. Supp. 2d
Orleans, LA. 1298, (D. Kan., 1998).
Kaufmann, P. M. (2009c). Protecting raw data and psy- Lugo v. Citicorp Mortgage, No. 925051, 1994 WL
chological tests from wrongful disclosure: A primer 879715, (Mass. Super. Ct., 1994).
98 forensic neuropsychology

Lumsden v. United States of America, Case No. Pankratz, L., Binder, L. M., & Wilcox, L. (1987).
7:06-CV-60-F, (E.D.N.C., 2010) (Jan. 15, 2010 Evaluation of exaggerated somatosensory deficit
order denying motion to quash subpoenas). with symptom validity testing. Archives of Neuro-
Lustre, A. B. (2004). Post-Daubert standards for admis- logy, 44, 798.
sibility of scientific and other expert evidence in state Pankratz, L., Fausti, A., & Peed, S. (1975). A forced
courts. American Law Reports 5th, 90, 453–545. choice technique for evaluating deafness in a hys-
Madrid v. Univ. of Cal., 737 P.2d 74 (N.M. 1987). terical or malingering patient. Journal of Consulting
McCabe, D. P. & Castel, A. D. (2008). Seeing is believ- and Clinical Psychology, 43, 421–22.
ing: The effects of brain images on judgments of Pardo, M. S. & Patterson, D. (2010). Philosophical
scientific reasoning. Cognition, 107, 343–52. foundations of law and neuroscience. University of
McCarthy v. Atwood, 67 Va. Cir. 237 (Va. Cir. Ct., Illinois Law Review, 2010(5), 1211–50.
2005). Pate v. Robinson, 383 U.S. 375, 377 (U.S. 1966).
Miles v. Stainer, 108 F.3d 1109, 1112 (9th Cir. 1997). Pettit, M. (2007). fMRI and BF meet FRE: Brain imag-
Minner v. American Mortgage & Guaranty Co., 791 ing and the Federal Rules of Evidence. American
A.2d 826 (Del. Super. Ct., 2000) Journal of Law and Medicine, 33, 319–39.
Miranda v. Arizona, 384 U.S. 436, 444 (U.S. 1966). People v. Gettings, 530 N.E.2d 647 (Ill. Ap. Ct., 1988).
Morrell, K. R. (2009). Test security in medicolegal People v. Hawthorne, 291 N.W. 205, 208 (Mich.
cases: Proposed guidelines for attorneys utilizing 1940).
neuropsychology practice. Archives of Clinical People v. Sebastianelli (1998). Unpublished case.
Neuropsychology, 24(7), 635–46. People v. R.R.& G.A., 807 N.Y.S.2d 516, 544 (N.Y. Sup.,
Morrison v. United States, 579 A.2d 686, 691 (D.C. Ct. 2005).
1990). Prohaska, M. L. & Martin, D. P. (May, 2007). Obtaining
Morgan, J. & Sweet, J. (Eds.) (2008). Neuropsychology of neuropsychological test data: Why this is so hard?
malingering casebook. New York: Taylor & Francis/ The Alabama Lawyer, 217–23.
American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology Rector v. Clark, 923 F.2d 570, 572 (8th Cir., 1991).
and Psychology Press. Redding, R. E. (2006). The brain-disordered defendant:
Moriarty, J. M. (2008). Foreward to the neuroscience, Neuroscience and legal insanity in the twenty-
law, & government symposium. Akron Law Review, first century. American University Law Review, 56,
42(3), 681–86. With a series of nine papers about 51–123.
neuroimaging and the law at http://www.uakron. Reed, J. E. (1996). Fixed vs. flexible neuropsychological
edu/law/lawreview/v42/v42num3.dot, last visited test batteries under the Daubert standard for
July 5, 2010. admissibility of scientific evidence. Behavioral
Morse, S. J. (2006). Symposium: The mind of a child: Sciences and the Law, 14, 315–22.
The relationship between brain development, cog- Richardson, R. E. L. & Adams, R. L. (1992). Neuro-
nitive functioning and accountability under the psychologists as expert witnesses: Issues of admis-
law: Brain overclaim syndrome and criminal sibility. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6(3),
responsibility: A diagnostic note. Ohio State Journal 295–308.
of Criminal Law, 3, 397–412. Rogers, R. (2008). Clinical assessment of malingering
Morse, S. J. (2010). Medicolegal neuroscience: My brain and deceptions. New York: Guilford Press.
made me do it! Ethical, legal, and social implica- Rosen, J. (2007, March 11). The brain on the stand.
tions. Penn Conference on Clinical Neuroscience New York Times. Retrieved May 6, 2007, from
and Society, July, 2010 Philadelphia, PA. http://www.nytimes.com/2007/03/11/magazine/11
National Academy of Neuropsychology Policy and Neurolaw.t.html?ex=1331269200&en=bbdc98f5f3
Planning Committee Position Paper. (2005). 800cd7&ei=5088
Symptom validity assessment: Practice issues and Ross v. State, 386 So.2d 1191, 1195 (Fla. 1980).
medical necessity. Archives of Clinical Neuro- Rothke, S. (1992). Expert testimony by neuropsycholo-
psychology, 20, 419–26. gists: Addendum to Schwartz and Satz. The Clinical
Nason v. Shafranski, Florida 19th Circuit, St. Lucie Neuropsychologist, 6(1), 85–91.
County (September 11, 2008 Ruling). Roper v. Simmons, 543 U.S. 551, 568 (U.S. 2005).
Nelson v. Nelson, 87 N.W.2d 767 (Iowa 1958). Rush v. Megee, 36 Ind. 69 (Ind. 1871).
North Carolina v. Butler, 441 U.S. 369, 373 (U.S. 1979). Russell, E. W. (2007). Commentary on “A motion to
Pankratz, L. (1979). Symptom validity testing and exclude and the ‘fixed’ versus ‘flexible’ battery in
symptom retraining: Procedures for the assess- ‘forensic’ neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical
ment and treatment of functional sensory deficits. Neuropsychology, 22, 787–90.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, Rüsseler J., Brett A., Klaue U., Sailer M., & Münte T. F.
09–410. (2008). The effect of coaching on the simulated
Admissibility of Expert Opinions based on Neuropsychological Evidence 99

malingering of memory impairment. BMC Sweet, J. J., Nelson, N. W., & Moberg, P. J. (2006). The
Neurology, 8(37):1–14. TCN/AACN 2005 “salary survey”: Professional
Rustenhaven v. American Airlines, Inc., 320 F.3d 802 practices, beliefs, and incomes of U.S. neuropsychol-
(8th Cir., 2003). ogists. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 325–64.
Satz, P. (1988). Neuropsychological testimony: Some Taylor v. Erna, Civil Action No. 08–10534-DPW, 2009
emerging concerns. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, U.S. Dist. LEXIS 69033, (D.C. Mass., 2009).
2(1), 89–100. Thompson v. Bell, No. 1:04-cv-177, 2006 U.S. Dist.
Sanchez v. Derby, 433 N.W.2d 523 (Neb. 1989). LEXIS 27283 (E.D. Tenn. May 4, 2006).
Sanders v. Nike, 2004 U.S. Dist. LEXIS 30756, (D. Vt., Tomlian v. Grenitz, Case No. 94–08121, Broward (FL)
2004). County (April 8, 2008).
Schwartz, M. L. (1987). Limitations on neuropsycho- United State v. Banks, 137 F.R.D. 20, 21 (C.D. Il., 1991).
logical testimony by the Florida Appellate United States v. Bitton Case No. 2:05-CR-661 TS United
Decisions: Action, reaction, and counteraction. States District Court of Utah, Central Division
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 1(1), 51–60. (July 1, 2008 Memorandum Decision and Order).
Sharland, M. J., & Gfeller, J. D. (2007). A survey of neu- United States v. Cameron, 907 F.2d 1051, 1066 (11th
ropsychologists’ beliefs and practices with respect Cir., 1990).
to the assessment of effort. Archives of Clinical United States v. Frisbee, 623 F. Supp 1217, 1220 (N.D.
Neuropsychology, 22, 213–33. Ca., 1985).
Sharrer and Sharrer v. Sunscape Landscape Nursery United States v. Hinkley, 525 F. Supp. 1342 (D.D.C.
Inc., et al. Case No. 07–006429, Florida 13th 1981), clarified 529 F. Supp. 520 (D.D.C., 1981),
Circuit, Hillsborough (11/05/09). aff ’d 672 F.2d 115 (D.C. Cir. 1982).
Shaw v. Delo, 971 F.2d 181, 187 (8th Cir., 1992). United States v. Johns, 728 F.2d 953, 956 (7th Cir.,
Simmons v. Mullen, 331 A.2d 892, 897 (Pa. Super. Ct., 1984).
1974). United States v. Jose Santos-Bueno, No. 04–40023-FDS,
Solomon v. TK Power, Case No. 06-CA-003088, Florida 2006 U.S. Lexis 6275 (Dist Ct. Mass., 2006).
4th Circuit, Duval County. (Plaintiff counsel with- United States v. Marenghi, 893 F. Supp. 85, 99 (D. Me.,
draws FBS motion in limine, September 10, 2008). 1995).
Spence, S. A., Kaylor–Hughes, C. J., Brook, M. L., United States v. Pohlot, 827 F.2d 889 (Dist. Ct. Ca.,
Lankappa, S. T., & Wilkinson, I. D. (2008). 1987).
“Munchausen’s syndrome by proxy” or a “miscar- United States v. Renfroe, 825 F.2d 763, 766 (3d. Cir.,
riage of justice”? An initial application of func- 1987).
tional neuroimaging to the question of guilt versus United States v. Riggleman, 411 F.2d 1190 (4th Cir.,
innocence. European Psychiatry, 23(4), 309–314. 1969).
State v. Beam, 710 P.2d 526 (Idaho 1985). United States v. Schneider, 111 F.3d 197, 201 (1st. Cir.,
State v. Broom, 655 So.2d 705, 708 (La. Ct. App. 1995). 1997).
State v. Card, 825 P.2d 1082 (Idaho 1991). United States v. Semrau, No. 07–10074 Ml/P (W,D,
State v. Clemmons, 946 S.W.2d 206, 222 (Mo. 1997). Tenn., 2010) (May 31, 2010 Order granting motion
State v. Dahood, 814 A.2d 159 (N.H., 2002). to exclude fMRI Lie Detection testimony).
State v. Korell, 690 P.2d 992 (Mont. 1984). Upchurch v. Broward Co. School Bd., (2008) January 24,
State v. Lee, 660 S.W.2d 394, 397 (Mo. Ct. App. 1983) 2008 Deposition of Dr. James N. Butcher.
State v. Rogers, 4 P.3d 1261, 1276 (Ore. 2000) Upchurch v. Broward Co. School Board, Florida 15th
Stith v. State Farm, Case No. 03-CA-010945, Florida Circuit, Broward County.
15th Circuit, Palm Beach County (August 28, 2008 Vandergracht v. Progressive Express Case No. 02-CA-
Order). 04552 Florida 13th Circuit, Hillsborough (Ruling
Sullivan, J. P. & Denney, R. L. (2003). Constitutional to exclude FBS, March 9, 2005).
and judicial foundations in criminal forensic neu- Valiulis v. Scheffeos, 547 N.E.2d 1289 (Ill. App. Ct.,
ropsychology. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 1989).
3, 13–44. Weissberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E.,
Sweet, J. J. (1999). Forensic neuropsychology: Fundamentals & Gray, J. R. (2008). The seductive allure to neuro-
and practice. Royersford, PA: Swets & Zeitlinger. science explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuro-
Sweet, J. J., King, J. H., Malina, A. C., Bergman, M. A., science, 20(3), 470–77.
& Simmons, A. (2002). Documenting the promi- Wetter, M. W. & Corrigan, S. K. (1995). Providing
nence of forensic neuropsychology at national information to clients about psychological tests: A
meetings and relevant professional journals from survey of attorneys’ and law students’ attitudes.
1990 to 2000. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16, Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 26,
481–94. 474–77.
100 forensic neuropsychology

Whitaker v. Parker, 42 Iowa 585, (Iowa 1876). Wilson v. Corestaff Services, 2010 N.Y. Mixc. LEXIS
Wiles v. Dept of Educ, 2008 U.S. Dist. LEXIS 110037, 1044. (N.Y. Sup. Ct., 2010).
(D. Haw., 2008) Woody, R. H. (2009). Ethical considerations of multi-
Winnans v. N.Y. & Erie Railroad Co., 62 U.S. 88 (U.S. ple roles in Forensic Services. Ethics and Behavior,
1859). 19(1), 79–87.
Williams v. CSX Transportation, Inc. Case 04-CA- Youngjohn, J. R. (1995). Confirmed attorney coaching
008892, Florida 13th Circuit, Hillsborough County, prior to neuropsychological evaluation. Assess-
(Sept 19, 2007 Order). ment, 2, 279–83.
Williams, C. L., Butcher, J. N., Gass, C. S., Cumella, E., Youngjohn, J. R., Lees-Haley, P. R., & Binder, L. M.
& Kally, Z. (2009). Inaccuracies about the MMPI-2 (1999). Comment: Warning malingerers produces
Fake Bad Scale in the Reply by Ben-Porath, Greve, more sophisticated malingering. Archives of Clinical
Bianchini, and Kaufmann (2009). Psychological Neuropsychology, 14, 511–515.
Injury and Law, 2(2), 182–97. Youtsey v. United States, 97 F. 937, 940 (6th Cir., 1899).
4
Ethical Practice of Forensic Neuropsychology
CHRISTOPHER L. GROTE AND BENJAMIN A. PYYKKONEN

Neuropsychologists have at least two reasons to offered by commercial insurers or government


be aware of the unique ethical dilemmas that can payers such as Medicare. It is not surprising then
present in the “legal” arena: the increasing that these referrals will increase not only because
percentage and number of forensically related of the valuable information they can bring to the
referrals made to our profession, and the vastly courtroom, but because neuropsychologists will
increased number of resources available to seek out and welcome such cases both for finan-
practitioners seeking out published guidance or cial and research purposes. Kaufmann’s recent
position papers on these topics. This chapter review (2009) points out that 71% of Lexis refer-
reviews: 1) how the number of forensic referrals, ences for “neuropsycholo-” occurred in just the
involvement of neuropsychology in the courts, last ten years, and that the ‘growth of legal cases
and ethics position papers have increased tremen- referencing neuropsychology is outpacing every
dously in just the last few years; 2) how the “ethics related area of brain-behavior expertise and this
of neuropsychology” fits into the larger context of growth is accelerating’ (p. 1131).
assessment guidelines promulgated by organiza- Given that neuropsychologists can increas-
tions, some of which are not exclusively populated ingly expect to have forensic cases constitute a
by neuropsychologists; 3) the continued impor- part of their practice, it might be considered a
tance of issues related to the release of psycholo- relief for most and a “burden” for a few, that “igno-
gical raw data, and; 4) common ethical dilemmas rance of the law” can no longer be offered as an
that can present in forensic neuropsychological excuse for not knowing how to anticipate or
evaluations, as illustrated by actual case examples potentially resolve ethical dilemmas that may
encountered by the authors. arise during such cases. When the senior author
of this chapter was preparing for his ABCN exams
T R E N D S I N AVA I L A B L E in the mid 1990s, there were very few articles,
REFERRALS AND chapters, or books on the ethics of neuropsychol-
RESOURCES ON ETHICAL ogy. Instead, one relied primarily on the APA
PRACTICE ethics code, and just a few relevant publications
Recent neuropsychology practice surveys report that were specific to neuropsychological practice.
continued growth in the practice of forensic neu- Five to ten years later, during the writing of this
ropsychologists. The survey conducted by Rabin, chapter for the first edition of this book, there
Barr, and Burton (2005) of the National Academy were quite a few more resources available to the
of Neuropsychology (NAN), APA Division 40, interested clinician or scientist. Dr. Shane Bush
and International Neuropsychological Society certainly was a leader in this development, and
(INS) members reported that legal work accounts the interested reader would do well to simply seek
for 32% of the referrals to these same neuropsy- out his publications for thorough reviews of a
chologists, a significant increase compared to ear- number of contemporary issues in the ethics of
lier surveys. This same survey reported as many as forensic neuropsychology (e.g., Bush & Drexler,
68.3% of all neuropsychologists reported receiv- 2002). What has transpired in more recent years,
ing referrals from attorneys. Further, reimburse- however, is increased awareness of and efforts by
ment rates in forensic neuropsychology can organizations in neuropsychology to publish posi-
approach 100% of the billed amount and at rates tion papers on topics related to ethics in forensic
which can be considerably higher than those neuropsychology. Reference to the website for the
102 forensic neuropsychology

American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology those issues related to forensic neuropsycholo-


(www.theaacn.org) lists 12 position papers devel- gical practice, such as informed consent for assess-
oped just in the last five years, all of which could ment, release of raw data, or obstacles to presenting
be considered as regarding ethical practice. More unbiased and fully informed opinions in forensic
specifically, AACN has asked experts, either on cases.
their own or in collaboration with others, to
review the literature and to offer guidelines on PLACING THE ETHICS OF
topics such as working with minority populations, NEUROPSYCHOLOGY IN
the proper assessment of effort and bias CONTEXT
(Heilbronner et al., 2009), practice guidelines, and It appears to be that very few “unethical” neurop-
disclosure of psychological raw test data. Of sychologists commit blatantly grievous acts such
particular interest is a compendium prepared as engaging in romantic or sexual liaisons with
by Dr. Bush of neuropsychology-related ethics clients, embezzling from employers, or like poten-
references (Bush, 2007). This 24-page document tial felonies. Instead, most perceived transgres-
lists hundreds of citations, nearly all published sions involve incompetent practice (Grote, Lewin,
in the last decade, arranged over a dozen or so Sweet, & van Gorp, 2000). Of course, “compe-
topics. In addition, the National Academy of tence” can be in the eye of the beholder and may
Neuropsychology (NAN) has also long been active not be easily agreed upon, especially in cases that
in developing and publishing papers related to are being litigated. Nonetheless, the following sec-
competent and ethical practice. Many of these tion reviews ways in which various organizations
were pioneering efforts, in that they date to the have commented on this issue.
late 1990s and early 2000s, but have been added to
in subsequent years, including unique topics such Competency Requirements
as the secret recordings of evaluations. These “Professional competence is the foundation of
papers are also easily accessed via the Internet at ethical practice” (Bush, 2007, page 37). Incom-
www.nanonline.org. petent practice precludes ethical practice.
Given these expanded resources, no practic- Therefore, it is entirely appropriate and useful to
ing neuropsychologist can be absolved for claim- identify the basic competency requirements for
ing lack of awareness of potential ethical problems the ethical practice of forensic neuropsychology.
or how they might be best resolved. These The following will delineate competency require-
increased resources are entirely appropriate given ments for ethical practice in three areas with
the unique demands of the forensic neuropsycho- successively increasing demands: 1) clinical
logical evaluation. This is both appropriate and assessment, 2) neuropsychological assessment,
necessary, for at least two reasons. First, neurop- and finally, 3) forensic neuropsychological
sychologists have to be constantly vigilant of the assessment. Naturally, more space will be spent
need to produce unbiased and appropriately discussing competency demands for forensic
informed opinions if courts can be expected to neuropsychological assessment.
rely on our opinions. Failure to maintain this neu-
trality could instead lead others to view us as mer- Competency Requirements for
cenaries whose opinions might be influenced by Clinical Assessment
the needs of those who retain us in adversarial Basic test user qualifications were most clearly
cases. Second, neuropsychologists may not be identified in official APA policy in a position
fully aware of all the potential ethical and legal paper for the American Psychological Association
implications that arise when their reports are used (APA) published in 2001 (Turner, DeMers, Fox, &
in forensic settings. Their graduate education and Reed, 2001). The basic qualifications represent the
training may have focused only on more general final results of the APA’s Task Force for Test User
ethical issues, such not having sexual relation- Qualifications (TTFUQ) to identify APA policy.
ships with patients (note: the term “patient” will This policy statement identifies two categories of
be used throughout this chapter instead of client, test user qualifications: a) generic psychometric
claimant, or litigant; of course, use of this term is knowledge with widespread application to the
not meant to imply that a treating or doctor- majority of tests, and b) qualifications specific
patient relationship exists during a forensic neu- to the application of particular tests in specific
ropsychological examination). Previous training settings. The basic psychometric knowledge iden-
in ethics may not have specifically focused on tified in this APA policy statement includes an
Ethical Practice of Forensic Neuropsychology 103

understanding of item response theory; descrip- The application of this understanding of brain-
tive statistics; reliability and measurement behavior relationships in concert with the afore-
error; validity and meaning of test scores; norma- mentioned generalist training represents the
tive interpretation of test scores; selection of foundational building blocks for the practice of
appropriate tests; test administration procedures; competent clinical neuropsychology. The Houston
ethnic, racial, cultural, gender, age, and linguistic Conference guidelines then identify specific areas
variables; and the use of tests with individuals of expertise requisite to competent practice of
with disabilities. This policy also recommends clinical neuropsychology, including an under-
that theses skills are best and most ethically devel- standing of neuropsychological assessment, inter-
oped under the supervision of “appropriately vention, research design, professional issues and
experienced professionals.” Unfortunately, the ethics, and the practical implications of neuro-
qualifications of such professionals are not clearly psychological conclusions.
delineated in this policy statement. In addition to A comprehensive update and review of the
these basic psychometric requirements for test APA ethics code (American Psychological
user qualifications, this policy paper identifies Association, 2002) can be found in Ethical Decision
a number of specific qualifications related to Making in Clinical Neuropsychology (Bush, 2007).
specific contexts. Five general categories of test Within this review a chapter is dedicated to com-
use are identified, including: (a) classification, petence. Specific APA ethics codes related to com-
(b) description, (c) prediction, (d) intervention petence in clinical neuropsychology include
planning, and (e) tracking. Although neuropsy- standards related to boundaries of competence,
chological assessment is not identified in this maintaining competence, and understanding the
policy statement, these are the exact applications bases for scientific and professional judgment.
of clinical neuropsychology. Moreover, while neu- The application of these ethical codes can func-
ropsychological assessment is not specifically tion as a useful guide and omnibus in the applica-
listed, the forensic context is specifically delin- tion of the Houston Conference guidelines to
eated in this policy paper and will be discussed competent practice in clinical Neuropsychology.
below.
Additional Requirements for
Specific Requirements for Competent Competency in Forensic Practice
Neuropsychological Assessment Given the unique nature of the forensic practice of
Numerous attempts have been made to identify clinical neuropsychology, certain competency
or describe the qualifications of a competent requirements emerge; namely, competent man-
neuropsychologist. In this vein, training para- agement of raw data and its release, and informed
digms have been developed, with the most com- consent (i.e., the person assessed is not afforded
plete being the Houston Conference guidelines the same rights of confidentiality or the results).
on specialty education and training in clinical The existing APA ethics code is applicable in these
neuropsychology (http://www.theaacn.org/ circumstances and provides some guidance in this
position_papers/Houston_Conference.pdf ). regard.
These guidelines indicate that practice in clinical
neuropsychology requires basic clinical training R E L E A S E O F R AW D ATA
to include proficiencies in statistics, psychopa- No doubt the reader of this chapter has already
thology, psychometric theory, and lifespan devel- been alerted to the fact that the 2002 ethics code
opment, among other critical areas. In addition to has altered policy regarding release of raw data.
this foundational generalist training, these guide- The ethics code change probably most relevant to
lines identify the need for foundational under- the practice of forensic neuropsychology is found
standings of the brain-behavior relationship, in Standards 9.04 (Release of Raw Data) and 9.11
including (a) functional neuroanatomy; (b) neu- (Maintaining Test Security). The current product
rological and related disorders including their is a more liberal interpretation than the 1992
etiology, pathology, course and treatment; code (American Psychological Association, 1992;
(c) non-neurologic conditions affecting central 2003) in determining to whom, and when, a psy-
nervous system functioning; (d) neuroimaging chologist must release “raw data.” The 1992 code
and other neurodiagnostic techniques; (e) neuro- prevented release to others who were ‘not quali-
chemistry of behavior (e.g., psychopharma- fied’ to use the raw data. This was a vague descrip-
cology); and (f) neuropsychology of behavior. tion that led to frequent debate about who was or
104 forensic neuropsychology

was not qualified to examine raw data. There was possible, exhibits and courtroom records contain-
also dissension regarding the 1992 interpretation ing test materials will be protected or sealed, and
of ‘raw data’ and whether this referred to test all test materials will be destroyed or returned
forms, test materials, test scores, or some combi- upon the completion of the case.
nation of these elements. As a result, some neu- A position paper and a review paper were also
ropsychologists reportedly refused to release published by the AACN on the topics of releasing
copies of patient’s test results/forms to other and protecting raw data. These can either be
licensed psychologists because they were not retrieved through their website (www.theaacn.
board certified by a particular organization or org), or through the papers published in The
perhaps even because of some personal enmity Clinical Neuropsychologist (Attix, Donders,
between the two psychologists. Johnson-Greene, Grote, Harris, & Bauer, 2007;
The 2002 code makes it a bit clearer as to what Kaufmann, 2009). These papers indicate the com-
psychologists should do when asked for their files, plexity of these issues, which is further discussed
although these requests will have to be balanced in chapter 2 of the current volume. However, it
against one’s interpretation of HIPAA and appli- does seem to be the case that “test materials” are
cable laws in one’s state of practice. Further, the rarely, or never, requested by nonpsychologists.
2002 code draws a distinction between raw data Second, the standard only requires “reasonable
and test materials. The 2002 Standard 9.11 efforts” to not release these materials, and does
(Maintaining Test Security) states that “psycholo- not make it clear what this means. Some psychol-
gists make reasonable efforts to maintain the ogists may interpret this to mean they only need
integrity and security of test materials,” these to explain their potential concerns about misuse
being defined as “manuals, instruments, proto- of the materials to a nonpsychologist before
cols, and test questions or stimuli and does not releasing them, while other psychologists might
include test data as defined in Standard 9.04.” This instead insist on a court order. Regardless of these
seems to suggest that psychologists should not quibbles, it is clear that Standard 9.04 has greater
freely release things such as the WAIS Block relevance concerning the release of test materials
Design drawings and blocks, the WMS Spatial or raw data. It states:
Span blocks, or test protocols containing the actual
test items (e.g., the word list for the California (a) The term test data refers to raw and
Verbal Learning Test-II). The differentiation of scaled scores, client/patient responses to
test data and materials is discussed at length in a test questions or stimuli, and
2003 position paper from the National Academy psychologists’ notes and recordings
of Neuropsychology (www.nanonline.com), concerning client/patient statements
which is a follow-up to their first paper on Test and behavior during an examination.
Security (National Academy of Neuropsychology, Those portions of test materials that
2000). This update further discusses the confusion include client/patient responses are
arising from Standard 9.04 and Standard 9.11. included in the definition of test data.
True raw test scores or calculated test scores that Pursuant to a client/patient release,
do not reveal test questions do not require test psychologists provide test data to the
security protection, per this position paper update. client/patient or other persons identified
However, the new 2002 revised APA Ethics Code in the release. Psychologists may refrain
does not address the very practical problem of from releasing test data to protect a
releasing data which imply or reveal test ques- client/patient or others from substantial
tions. As recommended in the original test secu- harm or misuse or misrepresentation of
rity position paper (National Academy of the data or the test, recognizing that in
Neuropsychology, 2000), when the court orders many instances release of confidential
release of raw data that include test protocols con- information under these circumstances is
taining test stimuli, the expert neuropsychologist regulated by law
should request a protective order containing the (b) In the absence of a client/patient release,
following language: The test materials will not be psychologists provide test data only as
circulated beyond those directly involved in the required by law or court order.
case, no unauthorized copies or reproductions
will be made, the presentation of the test materials Standard 9.04 is more liberal than what was
in the courtroom will be minimized to the extent in the 1992 code in that it is fairly clear in telling
Ethical Practice of Forensic Neuropsychology 105

psychologists to release copies of test forms proceeding (Fisher, 2003). Federal Law 45 CFR 45
when directed to by patients or their attorneys. 164.508 and 164.524[(a)(1)] is part of the HIPAA
One might attempt to argue that “substantial legislation, and states that “Patients do not have
harm” might result because of this release, but this the right of access to information compiled in rea-
might be a stretch, especially in light of the new sonable anticipation of, or for use in, a civil, crim-
HIPAA regulations. Of course, psychologists who inal, or administrative action, or procedure.”
practice in states such as Illinois and Iowa, which In terms of release of raw data, it has been
have laws restricting the release of raw data to written that state law takes precedence over
nonpsychologists, will not be affected by either HIPAA, and that HIPAA in turn supersedes any
the new APA ethics code or by HIPAA. An article ethical codes (Daw Holloway, 2003a). Figure 4.1
in the January 2003 APA Monitor states that state attempts to provide an overview of steps that
law supersedes HIPAA, and of course both of clinicians may want to consider in determining
these would trump the APA ethics code in those if or when to release copies of their raw data. It
situations where the code conflicts with state or illustrates that a patient, their attorney, or a court
federal law. It is also important to know that must request or order this release; it is difficult to
HIPAA is not applicable when raw data was imagine circumstances under which this release
collected in the process of legal or administrative could otherwise occur. However, if a patient has

When Should Raw Data Be Released?

Has patient or their attorney No


requested release? Do not
Yes release data
Is raw data to be sent to Ye
another psychologist? s
No
Has a court order directed Release data
Yes
you to release raw data?
No
Does state law prohibit
Yes Do not
release of raw data to
non-psychologist? release data

No
Was raw data collected as
part of legal or
administrative proceeding?
No Ye
s
HIPAA is HIPAA is
relevant not relevant

Can you clearly Can you clearly


demonstrate substantial demonstrate substantial Yes Do not
harm will be done if harm will be done if release data
data is released? data is released?

Yes No No

Release Data Release Data

FIGURE 4.1: Flowchart of data release.


106 forensic neuropsychology

requested this release, and a court has not ordered including becoming part of the record of the
the release, the clinician must then determine case. Obviously, this is a complicated situation
whether the state in which they are practicing has and clinicians should proceed cautiously, consult
any legislation regarding this issue. An informal with colleagues, and document their reasons for
survey of colleagues indicated that at least four of responding or failing to respond to requests for
the United States, and one Canadian Province, copies of their raw data. Chapters 2 and 3 provide
each prohibit the release of raw data to non- further discussion of the issue of test security.
psychologists. The cited legislation for each juris-
diction is as follows: Illinois (Section 3 c) of O B S TA C L E S T O T H E
(740 ILCS 110) of the Mental Health and ETHICAL PRACTICE
Developmental Disabilities Act of the State of OF FORENSIC
Illinois); Florida (chapter 490; 64 B19–18.004(3); NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
Iowa (Section 1 228.9); Texas (Title 22, Part 21, It is not enough to be well intentioned, or to view
Rule 465.22); Alberta (Health Information Act oneself as “ethical,” to avoid ethical dilemmas
Section 11(1)(e). Colleagues from other states, and inappropriate outcomes in forensic neuro-
including Utah, Hawaii, Arizona, and Oregon, psychology. One cannot necessarily rely on one’s
responded that their states had no such restric- common sense, or the opinion of a referring party,
tions on the release of raw data to nonpsycholo- as to the correct course of action in ethically
gists. This obviously is not a comprehensive questionable situations. Another problem is that
listing, so it is incumbent upon a practicing psy- many questions or problems can’t be anticipated
chologist to check with their state psychological in advance of their occurrence, and one may have
association, or otherwise be aware of state legisla- little time to seek consultation from colleagues or
tion regarding this issue. Assuming that a psy- to think one’s way through the problem. Therefore,
chologist is not prohibited from releasing raw data the rest of this chapter is intended to serve as
to nonpsychologists, the next step in this process a reference guide for ethical dilemmas that one
would be to determine if the request is related to a reasonably might expect to encounter in forensic
court or administrative proceeding. If it is not neuropsychology cases. These are based on situa-
related, then HIPAA does apply. This is important, tions and actual questions and comments encoun-
because HIPAA does not appear to allow for the tered by the author and colleagues. These are now
concept of “substantial harm or misuse” of the presented in roughly the same order that they
data to be an excuse to not send the data (Fisher, would occur in the evolution of a forensic neurop-
2003). That is, a psychologist’s concern that the sychological evaluation. That is, this discussion
data will somehow be misused cannot serve as a will initiate with dilemmas likely encountered with
reason to not send it in non-legal cases. The excep- the first contact with a referring party, and con-
tion to sending raw data to a patient extends to clude with issues related to deposition or trial tes-
test protocols which contain the actual test stim- timony. References to the relevant sections of the
uli, which are excluded from HIPAA release 2002 Ethical Principles are made for each section.
requirements due to the “trade secret” exemption
provision. On the other hand, if the request for COMEPETENCE
raw data is related to a court or administrative (ES 2 COMPETENCE;
proceeding, then HIPAA does not apply. Should 2 . 0 1 B O U N DA R I E S O F
there be a substantial reason to believe the data COMPETENCE)
would be misused in such a scenario, they would
be justified in attempting to withhold it. Of course, “Will you provide opinions on this child
this refusal may not be the last word, as an attor- custody case?”
ney might be successful in obtaining a court order,
which of course would trump any of the psychol- Psychologists should agree to be retained only on
ogist’s reasons for not sending the data along. those cases where their education, training, and
Again, if such a court order for release is obtained, knowledge are sufficient for them to offer expert
the expert neuropsychologist should attempt to opinions on the issues at hand. At a minimum,
obtain language in the order protecting the test this would require them to have knowledge of the
protocols containing actual test stimuli, such relevant literature and sufficient supervised and/or
that these are used only for the specific case in recent clinical experience with the type of patient/
litigation, and prevented from other disclosure, problem being referred. Neuropsychologists
Ethical Practice of Forensic Neuropsychology 107

typically have a doctorate in clinical psychology, SUGGESTIONS OF BIAS,


and as such usually have had experiences in other COLLUSION (ES 2.04 BASES
areas besides neuropsychology. This might allow FOR SCIENTIFIC AND
them to serve as experts in cases involving chronic PROFES SIONAL
pain, psychopathology, child custody, or a host of JUDGMENTS)
other issues, but they should make sure of their
ongoing competence before agreeing to be “Will you be able to hit a home run on this case?”
retained. Psychologists and nonpsychologists
alike might refer to the “definition of a neuro- It is the rare attorney who is so psychopathic or
psychologist” papers promulgated either by aggressive as to make an up-front suggestion to a
Division 40 (Clinical Neuropsychology) of the potential expert that only a certain opinion is
American Psychological Association (1989) or by being sought, but it happens. It is more common
the National Academy of Neuropsychology for an attorney or insurance company representa-
(2001) to help determine who might have the tive to inquire about a professional’s training and
necessary credentials to offer a neuropsycholo- experience. Questions about board certification,
gical opinion. Both definitions seem to agree that work setting and experience, and familiarity with
board certification may be or is the best evidence a particular clinical issue are entirely appropriate
of specialized training and knowledge in neuro- and should be welcomed. However, in some cases
psychology. the attorney may drop subtle or not-so-subtle
hints that certain opinions are being sought.
M E T H O D O F R E TA I N E R It may be the case that an attorney is naive and
(ES 3.06 CONFLICT OF inexperienced and that they can be quickly dis-
INTEREST; 6.04 FEES abused of your willingness to go along for the ride.
AND FINANCIAL Of course, some attorneys are not naive about such
ARRANGEMENTS; 6.07 issues, and these might be refused one’s services.
REFERRALS AND FEES) Attorneys are not the only possible source of
bias in a forensic neuropsychological examina-
1) “Will you work on lien?” 2) “Will you work tion. As reviewed in a chapter by Sweet, Grote,
for a reduced fee?” and van Gorp (2002), both the neuropsycholo-
gical expert and the patient may also introduce
Since plaintiff lawyers typically don’t collect a fee elements of unfairness or preconceived notions
from a client until a settlement or jury verdict has of outcome of an evaluation. The interested
been awarded, they sometimes will ask experts to reader might review Sweet and Moulthrop’s (1999)
wait to get paid. Besides not knowing when this article on ways one might attempt to identify bias
will occur, the psychologist may not even know if in one’s own work. The subsequent commentary
they will get paid. It is difficult to imagine how an and critique of this article by Lees-Haley (1999),
expert can maintain their neutrality, say in front as well as Sweet and Moulthrop’s response to
of a jury, if they know that none of their fees will Lees-Haley, provide a range of complementary
be paid unless the plaintiff receives a favorable and contradictory views.
verdict. Similarly, disability insurance companies
or defense litigators may sometimes ask an expert REVIEW OF RECORDS/
to “cap” their bill for a less-than-usual amount. E X A M I N AT I O N O F PAT I E N T
Although this may not be as problematic as (ES 9.01 BASES FOR
working on lien, it is not without its own set of AS SES SMENT)
problems. Working for less than your usual fee
may tempt one to cut corners and not do as “Don’t worry Doctor, we’ll tell you what you
thorough a job as is necessary or typical. On the need to know about this case.”
other hand, psychologists should also consider
doing pro bono work for impoverished clients. Neuropsychologists unfamiliar with their role
One should carefully think such things through may not fully appreciate their duty to exhaust
and try to ensure that their work will remain of all reasonable means to understand a case
the highest quality and not be tainted by the before they reach their opinion. They need to be
manner of retainer before agreeing to one of these pro-active in asking for and receiving appropriate
alternative financial arraignments. background information. These might include
108 forensic neuropsychology

school and work records to estimate premorbid At times, attorneys will want the input of neuro-
functioning, or deposition transcripts of the psychologists even though they know there will
patient’s collaterals to determine their view of not be an opportunity for an independent medi-
how a person’s daily life has been affected by an cal evaluation. The reasons for this “denial” of
accident or illness. Medical records can be volu- evaluation can vary, but may include legal strategy
minous in these cases, but the forensic neuro- on the part of the attorney, an unwillingness to
psychologist should routinely attempt to review pay fees associated with a clinical evaluation, the
things such as ambulance and police records, the passage of a court-imposed deadline of the time
statements of witnesses, as well as emergency by which the evaluation could have been con-
room, hospital, and imaging records. Psycholo- ducted, or that the person in question simply isn’t
gists should not passively rely on the judgment of required to present themselves for an evaluation.
the retaining party to determine which records Since the number of reason for nonevaluation
will be sent. Attorneys or insurance companies can vary so widely, so does an appropriate response
might send only a subset of a voluminous group on the part of the consulting neuropsychologist.
of records to a psychologist for review, and it may Obviously, we don’t have to be co-opted or other-
be the case that these are sufficient for the psycho- wise participate in schemes initiated by attorneys
logist to understand the situation. If they are not based on their procrastination, cheapness, or lack
sufficient, it is the psychologist’s duty to ask for of attention to detail. To do so without disclaimer
additional records, which may or may not be would put a neuropsychologist at risk of issuing
available. If they are not available or not sent incomplete or incorrect opinions and accordingly
for some other reason, the psychologist should put one at risk of complaints of incompetent
document this in their records and report, and practice from the examined client. However, at
note the effect that this lack of information had on times there may be legitimate or unavoidable
their opinion. circumstances in which a client is indeed unavail-
In most cases, a decision needs to be made as able for examination. While it might be preferen-
to whether the psychologist is going to request to tial for some to simply “just say no” to any
do their own examination of a patient. This consultation request that doesn’t allow for per-
request for examination will often be agreed to by sonal examination, such automatic denial may
a retaining party, but in some cases this request preclude the court from benefiting from at least a
may not be granted. This could be because certain partial or preliminary analysis by a competent
deadlines have passed or the patient’s attorney is neuropsychologist. It is not difficult to imagine
somehow successful in refusing to accede to scenarios wherein a neuropsychologist’s review of
such a request. It should be noted that failure to records could make a difference in the court’s
personally examine a patient does not mean that a understanding of a client’s mental or cognitive
psychologist cannot produce opinions. Ethical health. As long as the neuropsychologist had
Standard 9.01c addresses this in acknowledging wanted to (but couldn’t) examine a client, had
that in some situations an individual examination some reasonable method of determining their
is not warranted or necessary for opinions. opinion, and expressed the limitations of their
Obviously, psychologists will typically have to opinion because of the lack of ability to conduct
offer a more limited range of opinions in cases an examination, it would seem that they acted
where they only review records and have not seen within the bounds of ethical conduct.
a patient, but again, they are not precluded from
offering any opinions. The primary point is that S A F E G UA R D I N G
psychologists need to determine what must be PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTS (9.04
reviewed or done in order for them to arrive at R E L E A S E O F R AW D ATA )
their opinions. They should not rely on the
opinions of retaining or opposing parties to deter- “We’ll let you test our patient only if we tape your
mine this. Obstacles should be fought against, evaluation.” “Send your complete file to us.”
and if the result is unsuccessful (records or patient
not examined) the effect of this needs to be Attorneys have a duty to represent their client’s
incorporated into the report. interests, and they do not have to be objective or
even “friendly” in how they do this. They often
“Doctor, please provide your opinion about this will voice active suspicion of opposing experts
person’s mental health even though you will not and imply that only a biased incompetent expert
have the opportunity to examine them.” could have arrived at certain opinions. Sometimes
Ethical Practice of Forensic Neuropsychology 109

they may feel this way even before the patient is further use of these recordings. Additional details
evaluated and go to great lengths to insure that an on ways to handle requests for release of raw data
evaluation will be done “fairly.” Therefore, they or test materials can be found in another position
may attempt to obtain court orders that allow the paper put out by the National Academy of Neuro-
examination to be videotaped or audiotaped. psychology (www.nanonline.org).
Demands might be made to allow an attorney or
their representative (such as another psychologist R A N G E A N D D E TA I L O F
or a court reporter) to sit in on the evaluation. I N T E RV I E W ( E S 9 . 0 1 B A S E S
At the end of the evaluation, attorneys might FOR AS SES SMENT)
insist that copies of all test materials be sent to
them. Any of these scenarios should cause the “I’m only answering the interview questions that
psychologist to invoke Ethical Standard 9.04. I think are relevant.”
Test data should not be released to nonpsycholo-
gists unless there’s good reason to (see Figure 4.1 It may not be unusual for patients to come to an
and earlier discussion). In the case of a post- independent medical evaluation with the feeling
evaluation request for raw data, it is nearly inevi- that the evaluating psychologist is there to help
table now that requested raw data will have to be them no matter what (if referred through their
copied and sent to someone else. However, this attorney) or there to hurt them no matter what (if
does not mean that psychologists need to agree to referred by their insurer or an opposing attorney).
infringements on the evaluation itself. While it Attempts to convince them of the evaluator’s neu-
may be that attorneys are sincerely interested in trality might be attempted, but still might be
ensuring that only appropriate interviewing and answered by the patient with scorn or disbelief. In
testing is being conducted, it seems equally if not any event, the patient’s attitude should not deter-
more likely that such requests are meant to intimi- mine the number, range, or type of questions asked
date the evaluating psychologist, perhaps even to of them. Despite attempts to build some rapport,
the point of withdrawing from the case for ethical some patients may go on to refuse to answer inter-
concerns or because of the unpleasantness of being view questions. This could range from an isolated
monitored by an adversarial party. Psychologists question or two to a steadfast refusal to answer any
should attempt to educate others about the nega- all-forthcoming questions. In such situations the
tive impact that this third-party observation may neuropsychologist must determine if the informa-
have on the obtained results. Position papers from tion is needed for them to arrive at opinions being
the National Academy of Neuropsychology asked of them. If the information indeed is needed
(Axelrod et al., 2000) and the American Academy (as will probably be the case, since the question
of Clinical Neuropsychology (2001), as well as was asked in the first place), the clinician then has
other sources (Constantinou, Ashendorf, & the dilemma of determining how to proceed. It
McCaffrey, 2002) can be cited in such instances, might be possible to persuade the patient into con-
and previously have caused judges to disallow an tinuing with the interview by pointing out the rel-
invasion of the evaluation process. However, since evance of the questions. Alternatively, the question
some states may allow an attorney to be present might be asked either later in the interview or
during any independent medical evaluation, the rephrased in another way that the patient does not
psychologist must be prepared for this and have find objectionable. However, the neuropsycholo-
ready a series of responses for different scenarios. gist should not refrain from terminating an evalu-
It may be that, if forced, a neuropsychologist would ation if it appears that the client’s resistance is so
agree to allow an attorney to quietly sit behind a high as to keep one from obtaining the needed
client during the interview or testing. Such a stance background information.
may be the best that can be hoped for in certain
cases. However, it is difficult to imagine how a psy- R E L AT I O N S H I P S W I T H
chologist would allow this observer to interfere LITIGANTS AND CLAIMANTS
with the evaluation process, such as by interrupt- (ES 9.03 INFORMED CONSENT
ing with questions, comments, or derisive sneers IN AS SES SMENTS)
and snorts. It is also difficult to imagine situations
in which a psychologist might allow recording “Doctor, will you send me a copy of my report?”
equipment during the administration of psycho-
logical tests, unless there was some sort of agree- It might seem self-evident that litigants and
ment or protective order that prohibited release or claimants understand the reason they’re being
110 forensic neuropsychology

referred for a forensic neuropsychological assess- occur if a “one-size-fits-all” solution had been
ment, as one could presume they’ve been informed attempted on what will obviously be a compli-
by their attorney or insurance company about cated and diverse range of situations.
this. Since one can’t make such an assumption, it One possible solution has been proposed by
is incumbent upon a neuropsychologist to explain the Board of Directors of the National Academy
the circumstances of the assessment. This debrief- of Neuropsychology. Their official statement on
ing minimally should include an explanation that informed consent in clinical neuropsychology
the patient is being referred for an independent practice (National Academy of Neuropsychology,
medical evaluation (IME) and not for treatment. 2003a), approved in October 2003, includes both
Patients need to understand that there are limits a flowchart indicating for which situations either
to confidentiality and that a report detailing the assent or consent should be obtained, as well as a
results will be written. They also need to under- sample template for informed consent. The last
stand that feedback won’t be offered to them document explains some of what the patient
unless the neuropsychologist is authorized to do should expect during the evaluation (e.g., inter-
so. Litigants should have an opportunity to ask view, memory testing), foreseeable risks such as
questions. The patient’s understanding of the frustration or fatigue, estimated fees and length of
evaluation might be memorialized by having evaluation (including that patients are ultimately
them sign some sort of IME waiver or disclaimer. responsible for the fee), and limits of confidenti-
However, since some litigants have been fore- ality. Presenting such a document to a patient,
warned “to not sign anything,” it is also acceptable and the patient eventually signing it, has some
to have a verbal explanation and consent, which is obvious merits, particularly in that it provides a
then documented by the psychologist. In some strong counterclaim to any later possible patient
states such as Florida, written informed consent is claims that he or she did not know what to expect.
required for any psychological service. In such A subsequent position paper on independent and
states, the retaining attorney should be made court-ordered neuropsychological evaluations
aware of the need for informed consent prior to includes an example of an informed consent form
the date of evaluation, so that the informed con- for this particular type of evaluation (National
sent form can be forwarded to opposing counsel Academy of Neuropsychology, 2003b). This docu-
for review prior to the examination. ment forewarns the examinee regarding tests of
The ethics code requires psychologists to make symptom validity in such a manner as to minimize
a determination of whether a patient is competent potential for detection of these procedures;
prior to obtaining informed consent. If there is specifically, the examinee is advised to answer
some reason to doubt this (and the basis for this questions as accurately as they can; for example,
doubt will probably have to be documented some- when discussing their problems they are not to
where), then the patient may need to only give minimize major problems, nor are they to exag-
assent. This seems to be a less-stringent criterion gerate lesser concerns. Moreover, they are informed
than consent. The former may be generally inter- that they are expected to give their best effort
preted as primarily presenting information to a during testing, clarifying that they are not expected
patient and allowing the patient to ask questions to get every answer or problem correct, for no one
or have the opportunity to make objections ever does, but must give their best effort. Last, they
known. However, it may not always be required are advised that part of the examination will
that the patient make some sort of indication that address the accuracy of their responses as well as
he or she understands and agrees to proceed with the degree of effort that they exert on the tests.
the assessment if it appears that the patient lacks
the ability to do so. In contrast, patients who ini- INFORMED CONSENT
tially are presumed or known to be competent F O R S E RV I C E S O F
would be required to give consent, meaning that AN INTERPRETER (ES 9.03,
there is some indication both that they understand SECTION (C))
what is proposed and they have agreed to this.
How much detail should neuropsychologists “Doctor, this claimant does not speak English.
provide in explaining what will happen if a patient Will you please evaluate this patient for me?”
is assessed, and how should this be documented?
This is not spelled out by the ethics code and could Non-English-speaking persons continue to
not be, given the inevitable problems that would constitute a sizable percentage of the population
Ethical Practice of Forensic Neuropsychology 111

of many American cities and regions. This is due equally inappropriate. That is, proceeding as if the
in part to new immigrants coming to America, patient is fluent in English and is familiar with
but also because some persons do not become “majority” American culture and not describing
fluent in the English language even after having the limits of the evaluation in one’s report is just as
been in the United States for years or decades. likely to lead to an inappropriate outcome. Instead,
These factors make it important that psycholo- each referral of this type will dictate various
gists realize that the last revision of the APA Ethics ways of proceeding, which might range from
Code mandates that informed consent is also referral to a colleague, to judicious and limited
required for services of an interpreter. This has evaluation, to refusal to accept the referral even in
been separated out here because this seems to be the absence of referring elsewhere.
an issue different from what is usually considered If a clinician elects to proceed with the use of
a routine part of obtaining informed consent for an interpreter, the ethical standard does serve
assessment. Ethical Standard 9.03 states that as a warning that patients must agree to this, and
“Psychologists using the services of an interpreter the interpreter must be cautioned against the
obtain informed consent from the client/patient inappropriate release of information to non-
to use that interpreter, ensure that confidentiality authorized others concerning either information
of test results and test security are maintained, about the patient or the specifics of a test’s items
and include in their recommendations, reports, or content. Clinicians should also be cautious
and diagnostic or evaluative statements, including about asking friends or relatives of the friend to
forensic testimony, discussion of any limitations stand in as interpreters because their lack of
on the data obtained.” Also relevant is Section objectivity may interfere with accuracy. Patients
9.02(c): “Psychologists use assessment methods might also be reluctant to disclose personal
that are appropriate to an individual’s language information fully in interviews if a friend or
preference and competence, unless the use of an relative is serving as an interpreter.
alternative language is relevant to the assessment
issues.” “Doctor, you are ordered by this court to have
These standards bring to mind issues raised by your evaluation videotaped through a one-way
Artiola and Mullaney (1998) and others (LaCalle, mirror, but you are not to notify the examined
1987) that discuss the evaluation of patients client about such taping.”
who come from a different culture or who speak
a different language than the evaluator. There is While attempting to preclude the presence of a
no doubt that such patients should, when possi- third party observing or videotaping the exami-
ble, be referred to clinicians who speak the same nation of a child because of the potential biasing
first language as the patient. Also, the clinician effects on the examination, a judge proposed (and
should attempt to use assessment instruments immediately ordered) what he thought to be a
developed and normed for the appropriate ethnic sensible compromise: The videotaping would take
group. Ponton and Leon-Carrion (2001), for place, but behind a one way mirror. This would
example, have written extensively on how to ostensibly kill two birds with one stone: The plain-
evaluate Hispanic patients appropriately. tiff ’s attorney is granted his/her wish of monitor-
It is less clear, however, what to do if a clini- ing the examination and the potential of scaring,
cian cannot find an appropriate colleague for alarming, or biasing the child with videotaping
referral. This problem is especially acute in large equipment and technicians is removed by means
cities such as Chicago or New York, where the of a secret recording. At first glance, this might
clinician is likely to encounter patients who speak seem like an acceptable and preferred solution,
Urdu, Mandarin, or myriad other languages. but upon further analysis, it is not. As described
It is unlikely that these clinicians will have access in a recent NAN position paper (Bush et al., 2009),
to colleagues who either speak these languages the problem is that of deception. By not inform-
or have appropriately normed testing materials ing the client of an essential component of the
available. evaluation (the videotaping) one is engaged in
What should be done in such circumstances? an activity undermining the client’s (or the
Although it may be tempting simply to tell a refer- client’s guardian’s) ability to be informed of the
ent that “It can’t be done,” this runs the risk of context of the evaluation and how it is being
denying clients appropriate services. Obviously, conducted. Beside the primary problems involved
responses at the other end of the spectrum may be in deceiving a client, it is not difficult to further
112 forensic neuropsychology

imagine other complications that could arise Clinicians have a responsibility to keep up
during a secret videotaping. These could include with developments in their field. Failure to make
the client making a “confession” meant only reasonable efforts to do so may well justify charges
for the ears of the neuropsychologist but instead of failure to adhere to the standards of one’s field
being recorded for all eternity and anyone and to maintain one’s competence.
authorized to review the tape. Also, the client
might well detect the presence of the supposedly A C C U R AT E A N D F U L L
undetected videographer by hearing them through REPORTING OF FINDINGS
the wall or wandering into the taping room. Of (ES 9.01 BASES FOR
course, the angry and bewildered reaction that AS SES SMENT; 9.10
might understandably result from such detection EXPLAINING AS SES SMENT
would certainly and negatively affect any chances R E S U LT S )
of a valid examination being conducted post-
discovery. “Doctor, please don’t mention that my client
was once arrested. It has nothing to do with
PERFORMING A this case.”
C O M P E T E N T E VA L UAT I O N “Doctor, don’t include my wife’s IQ scores
(ES 9.01 BASES FOR in your report. Her employer has no need to
A S S E S S M E N T, 2 . 0 3 know them.”
M A I N TA I N I N G
COMPETENCE) A neuropsychological interview is just as, if not
more than, important than any test that might be
“You don’t need to give my client a malingering administered (Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004).
test. I can tell you he’s honest.” These interviews need to assess a broad range of
factors that will assist the neuropsychologist
As earlier reviewed, neuropsychologists should in placing the test data in proper context. Both
not enter into cases in which they do not have clinical and forensic patients should routinely be
proper expertise. This would include awareness of asked questions about their families of origin;
recent developments in the field and knowing educational attainment and achievement; previ-
some of the seminal papers about issues being ous medical, psychiatric and substance abuse
discussed. For instance, the papers by Dikmen histories; and so on. It is not unusual for such
et al. (1995) and Binder (Binder, 1997; Binder, interviews to reveal potentially embarrassing
Rohling, & Larrabee, 1997) and the book by details that a client, or their attorney, would prefer
McCrea (2007) are extremely important contribu- not go into a report. Of course, this possibility
tions in how to diagnose and understand mild should have been discussed beforehand during
traumatic brain injury, but it is evident that many the informed consent for assessment, and these
neuropsychologists are unfamiliar with these details need not be reported if they don’t contrib-
papers, and seem to have no greater understand- ute to an understanding of the patient’s neuropsy-
ing of this topic than what they might have learned chological status. However, the neuropsychologist
in graduate school years or decades ago (also see should remember that they are ultimately respon-
chapter 9, this volume). Similarly, some clinicians sible for providing a full and correct neuro-
have argued that tests of effort, or “malingering psychological opinion, and they may not be able
tests,” don’t need to be given since they can assess to arrive at such opinions if they agree to omit
this by virtue of their observation of the patient, relevant biographical details.
or because that area of research isn’t well devel-
oped in their opinion. Such comments, of course, ACCURACY IN TESTIMONY
run counter to empirical research (Grote et al., (ES 9.06 INTERPRETING
2000; Sweet, 1999; Youngjohn, Spector, & Mapou, A S S E S S M E N T R E S U LT S )
1998; chapter 5 this volume). Some of these same
clinicians also have argued that it is appropriate to “Doctor, please don’t mention that you found scor-
give tests that have not been re-normed in over 50 ing errors that would help my opponent’s case.”
years. The basis for such claims seems quite thin.
The point is that our clinical work is based on As entire texts (Brodsky, 1999) have been written
scientific investigations, and these are ongoing. to provide extensive detail on how to prepare for
Ethical Practice of Forensic Neuropsychology 113

and conduct oneself in giving expert testimony, immediate reporting is required, more typically a
this review will give detail for just one component complaint filed in the midst of ongoing litigation
of testimony—that of giving full and honest opin- could be perceived as a tactic of intimidation or
ions about one’s findings. Testifying at deposition harassment. A 2003 position paper from the
or in court can be very stressful and unnerving, American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology,
especially if the retaining attorney attempts to published in The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
coach or even bias your testimony beforehand. detailed this issue.
In most cases, attorneys will ask to meet with their
retained expert for 15 to 60 minutes prior to a S U M M A RY
deposition. This typically will involve some dis- This chapter has reviewed the increasing use of
cussion of their expert’s findings and opinions, neuropsychologists by courts, the vastly increased
including what the expert might say in response availability of position papers and other publica-
to likely questions from the deposing attorney. tions on the ethical practice of forensic neuro-
However, the occasional attorney might attempt psychology, the context in which these ethics are
to convince the expert to withhold certain opin- grounded, and the continued importance of the
ions if possible. This could include the discovery protection of raw data/test materials.
of scoring errors made by another psychologist, It is hoped that this review will contribute to
and the realization that accurate scoring could the ethical practice of this burgeoning subfield,
be seen as “helping the opposition.” Of course, a as it is gratifying to see the inroads made by our
forensic neuropsychologist cannot ignore this profession both in the application of our craft and
data, and must inform the retaining attorney of in the vastly increased attention being made by its
their opinions, even if the opinions are based on members to provide the public well-validated and
errors not yet discovered by others in the case. ethical services.

P ROT E C T I N G T H E P U B L I C REFERENCES
(ES 1.05 REPORTING American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology
E T H I C A L V I O L AT I O N S ) (2001). Policy statement on the presence of third
Psychologists will sometimes encounter reports or party observers in neuropsychological assessments.
opinions from colleagues whose work seems to be The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 433–39.
so riddled with error, or without any reasonable American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology
scientific foundation, that it raises a question as to (2003). Official position of the American Academy
whether that colleague is acting in a competent of Clinical Neuropsychology on Ethical Complaints
and ethical manner. Reaching a decision about Made Against Clinical Neuropsychologists During
whether to contact the colleague, or perhaps even- Adversarial Procedures. The Clinical Neuro-
tually reporting the colleague to a regulatory or psychologist, 17, 443–45.
professional organization, can be difficult. The American Psychological Association (1992). Ethical
psychologist should determine whether the per- principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
ceived differences in opinions or work products American Psychologist, 47, 1597–1611).
simply reflect an honest difference in opinion or American Psychological Association (2002). Ethical
whether a colleague’s work can be viewed by others principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
American Psychologist, 57, 1060–73.
as representing a significant threat to patients or
American Psychological Association (2003). 1992–
the public good. Previous reviews (Deidan &
2002 Ethics codes comparison. www.apa.org/
Bush, 2002; Grote, Lewin, Sweet, & van Gorp,
ethics/codecomparison.html
2000) have detailed when and how a psychologist Artiola, L. & Mullaney, H. (1998). Assessing patients
might proceed in such situations. Obviously, friv- whose language you do not know. Can the absurd be
olous or harassing complaints should never be ethical? The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 113–26.
made, but it is recommended that psychologists Attix, D., Donders, J., Johnson-Greene, D., Grote, C.,
take appropriate steps when they perceive that a Harris, J., & Bauer, R. (2007). Disclosure of neuro-
colleague has acted in a grossly incompetent or psychological test data: Official position of Division
unethical manner. 40 (Clinical Neuropsychology) of the American
It is also recommended that one not contact Psychological Association, Association of Postdoc-
another psychologist, or file a complaint, toral Programs in Clinical Neuropsychology, and
until incident litigation has fully resolved. American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology.
Although there may be emergent situations where The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21, 232–38.
114 forensic neuropsychology

Axelrod, B., Barth, J., Faust, D., Fisher, J., Heilbronner, study. Journal of Clinical & Experimental
R., Larrabee, G., et al. (2000). Presence of third Neuropsychology, 22, 709–719.
party observers during neuropsychological testing: Grote, C., Lewin, J., Sweet, J., & van Gorp, W. (2000).
Official statement of the National Academy of Responses to perceived unethical practices in
Neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical Neuro- clinical neuropsychology: Ethical and legal consid-
psychology, 15, 379–80. erations. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 14,
Binder, L. (1997). A review of mild head trauma: II. 119–34.
Clinical implications. Journal of Clinical & Heilbronner, R., Sweet, J., Morgan, J., Larrabee, G.,
Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 432–57. Millis, S., & the Conference Participants, 2009
Binder, L., Rohling, M., & Larrabee, G. (1997). American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology
A review of mild head trauma: I. Meta-analytic Consensus Conference Statement on the neuro-
review of neuropsychological studies. Journal psychological assessment of effort, response bias,
of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, and malingering. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
421–31. 23, 1093–129.
Brodsky, S. (1999). The expert expert witness: More Kaufmann, P. (2009). Protecting raw data and psycho-
maxims and guidelines for testifying in court. logical tests from wrongful disclosure: A primer on
Washington, D.C: American Psychological the law and other persuasive strategies. The Clinical
Association. Neuropsychologist, 23, 1130–59.
Bush, S. (2007). Ethical decision making in clinical LaCalle, J. (1987). Forensic psychological evaluations
neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University through an interpreter: Legal and ethical
Press. issues. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 5,
Bush, S. & Drexler, M. (2002) (Eds.). Ethical issues in 29–43.
clinical neuropsychology. Lisse, The Netherlands: Lees-Haley, P. (1999). Commentary on Sweet and
Swets & Zeitlinger Moulthrop’s debiasing procedures. Journal of
Bush, S., Pimental, P., Ruff, R., Iverson, G., Barth, J., & Forensic Neuropsychology, 1, 43–57.
Broshek, D. (2009). Secretive recording of neuro- Lezak, M., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004).
psychological testing and interviewing: Official Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York:
position of the National Academy of Neuro- Oxford University Press.
psychology. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, McCrea, M. (2007). Mild traumatic brain injury and
24, 1–2. postconcussion syndrome. New York: Oxford
Constantinou, M., Ashendorf, L., & McCaffrey, R. University Press.
(2002). When the third party of a neuropsycho- National Academy of Neuropsychology (2000). Test
logical evaluation is an audio-recorder. The Clinical security. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15,
Neuropsychologist, 16, 407–12. 383–6.
Daw Holloway, J. (2003, January). What takes prece- National Academy of Neuropsychology (2001).
dence: HIPAA or state law? Monitor on Psychology, Definition of a neuropsychologist. Official state-
34, 28. ment of the National Academy of Neuropsycho-
Deidan, C. & Bush. S. (2002). Addressing perceived logy approved by the Board of Directors 5/5/2001.
ethical violations in clinical neuropsychology. www.nanonline.org
In S. Bush & M. Drexler (Eds.), Ethical issues in National Academy of Neuropsychology (2003a).
clinical neuropsychology. Lisse, The Netherlands: Informed consent in clinical neuropsychology
Swets & Zeitlinger. practice. Official statement of the National Academy
Dikmen, S., Machamer, J., Winn, H., & Temkin, R. of Neuropsychology approved by the Board of
(1995). Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year post Directors 10/13/2003. www.nanonline.org
head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 80–90. National Academy of Neuropsychology (2003b). Inde-
Division 40 of the American Psychological Association. pendent and court-ordered forensic neuropsycho-
(1989). Definition of a clinical neuropsychologist. logical examinations. Official statement of the
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 3, 22. National Academy of Neuropsychology approved
Fisher, C. (2003, January/February). Test data standard by the Board of Directors 10/14/2003. www.nano-
most notable change in new APA ethics code. nline.org
The National Psychologist. 12. Ponton, M. & Leon-Carrion, J. (Eds.). (2001).
Grote, C., Kooker, E., Garron, D., Nyenhuis, D., Smith, Neuropsychology and the Hispanic patient. Mahwah,
C., & Mattingly, M. (2000). Performance of com- NJ: Erlbaum.
pensation seeking and non-compensation-seeking Rabin, L. A., Barr, W. B., & Burton, L. A. (2005).
samples on the Victoria Symptom Validity Test: Assessment practices of clinical neuropsycholo-
Cross validation and extension of a standardization gists in the United States and Canada: A survey of
Ethical Practice of Forensic Neuropsychology 115

INS, NAN, and APA Division 40 members. Archives Sweet, J. & Moulthrop, M. (1999). Response to Lees-
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(1), 33–65. Haley’s commentary: Debiasing techniques cannot
Sweet, J. (1999). (Ed.). Forensic neuropsychology. Lisse, be completely curative. Journal of Forensic Neuro-
The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. psychology, 1, 49–57.
Sweet, J., Grote, C., & van Gorp, W. (2002). Ethical Turner, S. M., DeMers, S. T., Fox, H. R., & Reed, G. M.
issues in forensic neuropsychology. In S. Bush & (2001). APA’s guidelines in tests user qualifications.
M. Drexler (Eds.), Ethical issues in clinical neuro- American Psychologist, 56, 1099–1113.
psychology. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Youngjohn, J., Spector, J., & Mapou, R. (1998). Failure
Zeitlinger. to assess motivation, need to consider psychiatric
Sweet, J. & Moulthrop, M. (1999). Self-examination disturbance, and absence of objectively verified
questions as a means of identifying bias in adver- physical pathology: Some common pitfalls in the
sarial assessment. Journal of Forensic Neuropsy- practice of forensic neuropsychology. The Clinical
chology, 1, 73–88. Neuropsychologist, 12, 233–36.
5
Assessment of Malingering
G L E N N J. L A R R A B E E

This chapter reviews definitions of malingering, normal memory); inconsistencies between


research designs for investigation of malingering, neuropsychological test scores and the suspected
and discusses various symptom validity tests and etiology of brain dysfunction (e.g., impaired
response bias procedures developed to identify IQ with normal memory in alleged hypoxic brain
the presence of malingering. The primary purpose injury); inconsistencies between neuropsy-
is to provide a conceptual understanding of the chological test scores and medically documented
construct of malingering rather than offer a severity of injury (e.g., performance at levels char-
detailed review of the procedures developed for acteristic of those associated with prolonged coma
detection of malingering. Detailed reviews of in a patient who had a blow to the head without
these procedures are provided by Boone (2007a) loss of consciousness); and inconsistencies between
and Larrabee (2007), and the edited case book by neuropsychological test scores and behavioral
Morgan and Sweet (2009) provides an extensive presentation (e.g., failure on measures of recent
bibliography of various measures of symptom and remote memory contrasted with the ability,
validity and response bias. Last, the American during interview, to report an accurate clinical
Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology (AACN) history). Analysis of inconsistencies, as well as
has published a consensus statement on the consistencies in performance helps define a second
neuropsychological assessment of effort, response type of validity: performance validity.
bias, and malingering (Heilbronner, Sweet, Inconsistencies in test scores often are the
Morgan, Larrabee, & Millis, 2009). result of variable effort and motivation. Variable
motivation can be secondary to factors outside
DEFINITION OF the patient’s conscious intent or control, such as
MALINGERING depression, anxiety, or conversion disorder, or
Accurate neuropsychological evaluation is based may result from conscious, intentional response
on test procedures that are reliable, yielding con- distortion such as occurs in factitious disorder or
sistent and stable scores, and valid, producing true malingering (Iverson & Binder, 2000; Larrabee,
measures of the abilities and traits that we assume 1990). Factitious disorder and malingering
we are measuring; thus, tests of attention should share intentional, volitional distortion or misrep-
yield reliable and valid assessments of attention, resentation of symptoms, but differ in that the
and tests of memory should yield reliable and motivation for factitious disorder is the psycho-
valid measurement of memory function. Test pro- logical need to assume a sick or disabled role
cedures that have proven reliability and validity, (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
however, may yield scores that are not reliable or Disorders, fourth edition [DSM-IV], American
valid measures for the individual examined; in Psychiatric Association, 1994). In contrast, malin-
other words, these test performances are not an gering is the intentional production of false or
accurate measure of the examinee’s actual level of grossly exaggerated physical and psychological
ability. In these instances, questions about the symptoms for external incentives such as obtain-
accuracy of patient performance arise because ing monetary compensation or avoiding criminal
of significant inconsistencies in test data prosecution (DSM-IV, American Psychiatric
(Iverson & Binder, 2000; Larrabee, 1990). These Association, 1994; Rogers, 2008).
may include inconsistencies between neuropsy- Rogers (1997a; 2008) discussed various
chological domains (e.g., impaired attention with explanatory models of malingering, including
Assessment of Malingering 117

(a) mentally disordered (pathological), (b) antiso- Drawing on the work of Rogers (1990a,
cial/criminological, and (c) adaptational. The 1990b), Pankratz and Binder (1997), Greiffenstein
adaptational model, first proposed by Rogers and colleagues (Greiffenstein, Baker, & Gola,
in 1990 (1990a, 1990b), considers would-be 1994; Greiffenstein, Gola, & Baker, 1995), and
malingerers as engaging in a cost–benefit analysis Nies and Sweet (1994), Slick, Sherman, and
when confronted with an assessment perceived Iverson (1999) have proposed diagnostic criteria
as indifferent or in opposition to their needs. for malingered neurocognitive dysfunction
In this model, malingering is more likely to occur (MND). This is defined by Slick et al. (1999) as the
when the context is adversarial, the personal volitional exaggeration or fabrication of cognitive
stakes are very high, and there are no other dysfunction for the purpose of obtaining substan-
perceived viable alternatives. tial material gain (e.g., compensation for injury)
Rogers (1997a) noted that descriptive data or avoiding or escaping legally obligated formal
generally support the adaptational model, with duty (e.g., prison, military) or responsibility
higher prevalence of malingering in adversarial (e.g., competency to stand trial).
settings (forensic vs. nonforensic) or when per- The Slick et al. (1999) criteria require consid-
sonal stakes are particularly high (e.g., avoiding eration of separate criteria including (A) presence
military combat or succeeding in personal injury of a substantial external incentive, (B) consider-
litigation). Rogers considered the adaptational ation of evidence from neuropsychological
model as providing “the broadest and least testing, (C) evidence from self-report, and
pejorative explanation of malingering” (Rogers, (D) behaviors meeting necessary criteria from B
1997a, p. 8). As a consequence, clinicians may be and C are not fully accounted for by psychiatric,
less likely to ignore evidence of malingering developmental, or neurological factors. Evidence
because a person does not fit more judgmental from neuropsychological testing (B) includes defi-
criteria (Iverson & Binder, 2000). The cost–benefit nite negative response bias, defined as below
analysis underlying the adaptational model also chance performance (p < .05) on one or more
allows gradations of malingering (mild, moder- forced-choice measures of cognitive function,
ate, severe) and better fits the definition of whereas probable response bias involves perfor-
malingering as involving either fabricated or exag- mance on one or more well-validated psychomet-
gerated deficits. ric tests or indices consistent with feigning.
Malingering can occur in one of three patterns Evidence from neuropsychological testing also
in neuropsychological settings (Iverson & includes discrepancies between test data and pat-
Binder, 2000; Larrabee, 2000): (a) false or exag- terns of brain functioning, discrepancies between
gerated reporting of symptoms (Larrabee, 1998; test data and observed behavior, discrepancies
Nelson, Sweet, & Demakis, 2006; Wygant et al., between test data and reliable collateral reports,
2007); (b) intentionally poor performance on and discrepancies between test data and docu-
neuropsychological tests (Binder & Willis, 1991; mented background history.
Mittenberg, Rotholc, Russell, & Heilbronner, The Slick et al. (1999) indicators of evidence
1996); or (c) a combination of symptom exaggera- from self-report (C) include self-reported history
tion and intentional performance deficit (Heaton, discrepant with documented history, self-reported
Smith, Lehman, & Vogt, 1978; Larrabee, 2003a). symptoms discrepant with known patterns of
Extensive research exists on specialized proce- brain functioning, self-reported symptoms dis-
dures to detect malingering, such as the Portland crepant with behavioral observations, and self-
Digit Recognition Test (PDRT; Binder & Willis, reported symptoms discrepant with information
1991) the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM; obtained from collateral informants. Self-reported
Tombaugh, 1996), and Word Memory Test (WMT; evidence of malingering also includes evidence of
Green, Lees-Haley, & Allen, 2002), as well as exaggerated or fabricated psychological dysfunc-
research identifying patterns of malingering on tion based on well-validated validity scales or
standard clinical tests (Mittenberg, Aguila- indices from measures such as the MMPI-2.
Puentes, Patton, Canyock, & Heilbronner, The Slick et al. (1999) criteria for definite
2002) and fabrication and exaggeration of symp- MND include (A) presence of a substantial
toms on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality external incentive, (B1) definite negative response
Inventory-2 (MMPI-2; Butcher et al., 2001; bias (e.g., worse-than-chance performance on
Lees-Haley, 1992; Lees-Haley, English, & Glenn, forced-choice testing), and (D) behaviors that
1991). meet necessary criteria for definite negative
118 forensic neuropsychology

response bias that are not fully accounted for by et al. (1999), the MPRD criteria allow a gradation
psychiatric, neurological, or developmental of certainty regarding diagnosis, ranging from
factors. The criteria for probable MND include definite to probable to possible MPRD. Definite
(A) presence of a substantial external incentive; MPRD is defined either by presence of signifi-
(B) two or more types of evidence from neuro- cantly worse-than-chance performance on two-
psychological testing, excluding definite negative alternative forced choice testing, or presence of
response bias or one type of evidence from one or more compelling inconsistencies, in the
neuropsychological testing, excluding definite presence of an external incentive, and absence of
negative response bias, and (C) one or more types alternative neurologic, psychiatric, or develop-
of evidence from self-report; and (D) behaviors mental explanations. Probable MPRD is defined
that meet necessary neuropsychological testing by presence of external incentive, and two or more
criteria and self-report that are not fully accounted types of “probable” evidence from criteria B, C, or
for by psychiatric, neurological, or developmental D, in the absence of alternative clinical explana-
factors. The criteria for possible MND include tions. Possible MPRD is defined by external incen-
(A) presence of a substantial external incentive; tive, only one type of evidence (exclusive of
(C) evidence from self-report; and (D) evidence worse-than-chance or compelling inconsistency)
from self-report is not fully accountable by psy- from criteria B, C, or D and absence of alternative
chiatric, neurological, or developmental factors, explanations, or by the patient who meets a diag-
or criteria for either definite or probable MND are nosis sufficient for MPRD, with the exception that
met, but the D criteria (psychiatric, neurological the E criteria for absence of significant neurologic,
or developmental factors) cannot be ruled out. psychiatric, or developmental conditions cannot
Slick et al. (1999) urged thorough consider- be met. These criteria are also discussed in the
ation of differential diagnoses before concluding chronic pain chapter by Greve, Bianchini, and
that a person is malingering. They recommended Ord in the present volume.
that a “reasonable doubt” strategy be applied to Larrabee, Greiffenstein, Greve, and Bianchini
decisions about the probability that a patient is (2007) reviewed the diagnostic criteria for MND
malingering, keeping in mind the limitations of and MPRD, noting several common features of
assessment methodology and the cost of false- evidence supporting symptom or performance
positive errors. invalidity. First, symptom report and test perfor-
Bianchini, Greve, and Glynn (2005) have mance are atypical in pattern or degree of deficit
extended the Slick et al. (1999) diagnostic criteria for expected findings in bona fide neurologic,
for MND to malingered pain-related disability psychiatric, or developmental disorders. Second,
(MPRD). Similar to MND, the criteria for MPRD the diagnostic criteria for both MND and MPRD
require an A criterion for presence of a substantial require presence of multiple indicators of symp-
external incentive, and E criteria for ruling out tom exaggeration or performance invalidity, and
presence of a viable legitimate neurologic, psychi- that there are no viable alternative explanations
atric, or developmental explanation for pain for these atypical symptom endorsement or exam-
symptom portrayal. Bianchini et al. (2005) also ination performance patterns. Larrabee et al.
make the important point that malingerers have (2007) observe that it is the combined improba-
an end goal of demonstrating disability, a goal bility of findings, in the context of external incen-
that can be met not only by demonstration of tive, without any viable alternative explanation
impairment of functional capacity on physical that establishes the intent of the examinee to
examination (B criteria) or pain scale endorse- malinger.
ment (D criteria), but a goal that can also be met
by intentional underperformance on measures of RESEARCH DESIGNS FOR
cognitive ability (C criteria; see Myers & Diep, I N V E S T I G AT I O N O F
2000, for an example). Bianchini et al. introduce MALINGERING
the new criterion of “compelling inconsistency,” Rogers (1997b; 2008) reviewed basic research
which occurs when there is a discrepancy in the designs for evaluation of malingering, including
way a patient presents when knowingly being the case study approach, simulation studies,
evaluated in contrast with when they are unaware known-group designs, and differential prevalence
of being evaluated, such that it is reasonable to designs. The case study approach was used by
believe the patient is purposefully controlling the Pankratz, Fausti, and Peed (1975) in the evalua-
difference. Similar to the MND criteria of Slick tion of hysterical deafness and by Hiscock and
Assessment of Malingering 119

Hiscock (1989) in their initial demonstration of the Binder and Rohling (1996) meta-analysis of
the validity of their Digit Memory Test (DMT). litigating and nonlitigating closed injury. As
Denney (1996) also employed a case study design Rogers (1997b) pointed out, very little is learned
in the evaluation of claimed remote memory loss from differential prevalence designs because we
in criminal defendants professing amnesia for the do not know who is dissimulating in each group
events contemporaneous with the alleged crime. and how many are dissimulating in each group.
Although these authors employed a single-case or The fourth type of research design is the
multiple-single-case approach, all three investiga- known-groups design, also known as the criterion
tions relied on the normal approximation to the groups design (Heilbronner et al., 2009). This
binomial—applied to two-alternative, forced- requires a two-part approach: establishing the
choice testing—that could be used to generate criterion groups (bona fide patients and malin-
probabilities of malingering (this approach is gerers) and conducting a systematic analysis of
discussed in greater detail in the section on similarities and differences between the criterion
forced-choice symptom validity testing). groups. This research design has benefited signifi-
Simulation studies typically involve use of cantly from the specification of diagnostic criteria
noninjured persons who are provided with for malingering by Slick et al. (1999), which
instructions to attempt to feign deficit success- provide stronger confidence in creating the
fully in an imaginary litigation scenario. Rogers criterion groups. These criteria for malingering
(1997b) noted the main problem with these types have been employed by Greve, Bianchini, Mathias,
of studies is generalizability. That is, will the find- Houston, and Crouch (2002) to contrast the
ings in normal individuals simulating malinger- performance of subjects who had probable MND
ing generalize to the real-world setting, in which with that of persons who had moderate-to-severe
the financial stakes are considerably larger and TBI. I (Larrabee, 2003b) employed the Slick et al.
involve real potential for financial gain? Rogers (1999) criteria to compare and contrast perfor-
(1997b) recommended a design strategy by which mance on the MMPI-2 as well as on a select
four groups are studied: (a) simulating nonclini- number of standard neuropsychological test
cal subjects, (b) honestly responding nonclinical procedures in subjects with definite or probable
subjects, (c) honestly responding clinical subjects, MND and subjects who had suffered moderate
and (d) clinical subjects simulating greater impair- and severe TBI (Larrabee, 2003a). As Rogers
ment than they really experience. Note that this (1997b) noted:
strategy should increase generalizability of the
results over studies that merely employ simulating Employment of known-groups comparisons
and nonsimulating nonclinical subject groups. addresses fully the clinical relevance of dis-
A number of neuropsychological investiga- simulation research. First, the research
tions have contrasted simulators with groups of typically is conducted in clinical or other pro-
subjects who have genuine clinical disorders, fre- fessional settings where dissimulation is
quently suffering from moderate-to-severe trau- expected to occur. Second and more impor-
matic brain injury (TBI). Heaton et al. (1978) used tant, the persons engaging in dissimulation are
this approach, which has also been employed by doing so for real-world reasons. (p. 416)
Mittenberg and his group in their investigations
of profiles of malingering on the Wechsler Adult When using a clinical comparison group to
Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, contrast performance either with a normal sub-
1981), Halstead-Reitan Battery (HRB; Reitan & ject group simulating malingering or with a
Wolfson, 1993), and Wechsler Memory Scale– known group of malingerers, it is important that
Revised (WMS-R; Mittenberg, Azrin, Millsaps, & the clinical group not include malingering sub-
Heilbronner, 1993; Mittenberg et al., 1996; jects. This is particularly problematic when the
Mittenberg, Theroux, Zielinski, & Heilbronner, clinical group includes persons who themselves
1995; Wechsler, 1987). are in litigation. One cannot merely assume that a
The differential prevalence design is rarely group of subjects with a bona fide condition such
used and poses several problems of interpretation. as moderate or severe TBI are performing at their
In this design, a group known to have a higher best. Rohling, Binder, and Langhinrischen-
base rate of malingering is investigated and con- Rohling (1995) found that patients in chronic
trasted with a group not suspected of having an pain in litigation averaged 0.48 pooled standard
elevated rate of malingering. An example of this is deviations higher on pain scales than those not in
120 forensic neuropsychology

litigation, a value quite close to the .47 effect size impairment secondary to amnestic disorder,
found by Binder and Rohling (1996) in contrast- dementia, or severe psychiatric disorder;
ing the neuropsychological performance of Schretlen, Brandt, Krafft, & Van Gorp, 1991).
TBI patients in litigation versus the performance Because of these problems, it has been recom-
of nonlitigating TBI patients. Thus, when using mended that the test not be the sole measure of
a compensation-seeking clinical control group, effort and motivation (Iverson & Binder, 2000),
this group must itself be carefully screened for and that the test should not be used in truly
malingering. amnestic populations or with patients suffering
severe psychopathology (Schretlen et al., 1991).
SPECIALIZED TESTS A meta-analysis of the 15-Item Test (Vickery,
OF RESPONSE BIAS FOR Berry, Inman, Harris, & Orey, 2001) reported an
DETECTION OF average specificity (i.e., correct identification of
MALINGERED clinical patients as not malingering) of 92.5%
NEUROCOGNITIVE (based on eight studies), average sensitivity (i.e.,
DEFICITS correct detection of malingerers as malingering)
Several specialized tests of response bias for detec- of 43.3% (based on seven studies), and average hit
tion of malingered cognitive deficit have been rate of 70.5% (based on six studies).
devised, dating to Rey’s 15-item and dot-counting Two modifications of the 15-Item Test have
procedures developed over 40 years ago (Lezak, appeared (Boone, Salazar, Lu, Warner-Chacon, &
Howieson, & Loring, 2004; Rey, 1964). In modern Razani, 2002; Griffin, Glassmire, Henderson, &
times, these procedures fall in two categories: McCann, 1997). Griffin et al. eliminated the
(a) tasks that appear to measure a cognitive sequence of three geometric patterns and three
ability, but are so simple that even persons with lowercase letters on the original Rey 15-Item Test
significant neuropsychological deficits can per- and replaced them with two numerical sequence
form perfectly or near perfectly (e.g., Rey 15-Item stimuli redundant with the numerical sequences
Test); and (b) tests based on forced-choice stimu- on the original 15-Item Test. The sensitivity and
lus presentation, which can be evaluated for specificity of the revised “Rey-II” were evaluated
worse-than-chance performance using the normal in honest and dissimulating college students and
approximation to the binomial theorem, usually honest and dissimulating nonlitigating clinical
referred to as symptom validity testing (SVT; groups of board-and-care residents (clinical
e.g., PDRT, Binder & Willis, 1991). groups included those with schizophrenia and
developmental disability and elderly persons
Simplistic Tests Performed needing residential care). Performance on the
Normally by Brain-Injured Persons Rey-II was contrasted with performance on
The Rey 15-Item Test (Rey, 1964) is probably the original 15-Item Test (with the exception of
the best-known task typically performed normally not having a condition of a dissimulating clinical
by brain-injured persons, so that poor perfor- group on the original 15-Item Test). Griffin et al.
mance may be considered a result of reduced found that both quantitative and qualitative scores
motivation. The 15 redundant stimuli (e.g., quan- on the Rey-II were superior in discriminating
titatively equivalent Roman and Arabic numerals; genuine from poor effort in the college students
upper- and lowercase identical letters of the (qualitative scores had 100% specificity and 69%
alphabet; see Lezak et al., 2004) are presented for sensitivity; quantitative scores had 97% specificity
10 seconds, then withdrawn, with the subject and 73% sensitivity). These values were lower in
asked to draw as many of the items as he or she the college students for the original 15-Item Test
can recall. Various cutoffs have been recom- for qualitative scores (100% specificity, 57% sensi-
mended, but performance is typically considered tivity) and quantitative scores (98% sensitivity,
motivationally suspect at 67% or less (Greiffenstein 40% specificity). In the clinical population admin-
et al., 1994; Lee, Loring, & Martin, 1992; Lezak istered the Rey-II, qualitative score specificity was
et al., 2004). The Rey 15-Item Test has been 75%, with sensitivity of 71%.
criticized as having poor sensitivity to the The modification of the 15-Item Test devel-
presence of malingering (Iverson & Binder, 2000; oped by Boone, Salazar, et al. (2002) maintains the
Sweet, 1999). Others have questioned both original stimuli and format of the 15-Item Test,
sensitivity and specificity (e.g., elevated level of but adds a recognition trial administered follow-
false-positive errors in patients with genuine ing the standard drawing-from-recall condition.
Assessment of Malingering 121

The recognition trial presents Rey’s 15 original study to 55.6% on cross-validation. Boone and Lu
stimuli with 15 numeric, geometric, and alpha- speculated that the decline in sensitivity may be
betic foil stimuli on one page. The subject is then the product of increased sophistication of proba-
instructed to look at the page, which includes “the ble malingerers regarding effort tests, and/or rep-
15 things that I showed you as well as 15 items resent the effects of compromised test security.
that were not on the page,” and circle the things Additional procedures for evaluation of effort
they remember from the page they previously that are sufficiently simple so that nonlitigating
viewed. Scoring includes (a) recall correct, clinical patients can perform adequately include
(b) recognition correct, (c) false-positive recogni- Rey’s Dot-Counting Test (Boone et al., 2002;
tions, and (d) a combination score computed as Lezak et al., 2004; Rey, 1964; Youngjohn, Burrows,
recall correct plus recognition correct minus & Erdal, 1995), and the b Test (Boone et al., 2000).
false-positive recognitions (combined score). The Dot-Counting Test requires the subject to
Boone, Salazar, et al. (2002) evaluated the count dots presented on index cards as quickly as
sensitivity and specificity of the Rey 15-Item possible. Some of the dots are grouped, whereas
Plus Recognition trial in four groups of subjects: others are not grouped. A person taking as long or
(a) a litigating group with independent evidence longer to count grouped dots as they did to count
of suspect effort, (b) a clinic patient group (non- ungrouped dots is displaying motivationally
demented mixed neurological and psychiatric suspect performance (Lezak et al., 2004).
patients), (c) learning-disabled college students, Vickery et al. (2001) included the Dot-Counting
and (d) normal control subjects (mean age 61.3 Test as one of the tasks in their meta-analysis of
years). These authors found the best combination measures of effort. These authors found that the
of sensitivity and specificity for the combined Dot-Counting Test was equivalent in mean effect
(recall plus recognition minus false-positive) size to the 15-Item Test in separating subjects
score, yielding a sensitivity of 71.4% and specific- showing poor effort from those demonstrating
ity of 91.7–93.9%. In addition, Boone et al. identi- adequate effort. Both the Dot-Counting Test and
fied four false-positive errors never made by clinic Rey 15-Item Test yielded smaller effect sizes than
or learning-disabled patients and rarely (by one the DMT (Hiscock & Hiscock, 1989), the PDRT
subject) by the normal controls. (Binder & Willis, 1991), and the 21-Item Test
Of suspect-effort cases, 14% made at least one (Iverson, Franzen, & McCracken, 1991). Sensitivity
of the false-positive recognition errors compared and specificity values could not be computed for
with 0.02% of the three groups of normal-effort the Dot-Counting Test by Vickery et al. because of
subjects. Boone, Salazar, et al. (2002) noted that, inconsistencies in scoring procedures.
although these four errors were not frequent in Subsequent to the Vickery et al. (2001) meta-
the suspect-effort group, they may serve as virtual analysis, Boone, Lu, et al. (2002) published Dot-
pathognomonic signs of noncredible performance Counting Test data on large samples of persons
when present (i.e., the presence of these signs with suspect effort, as well as on a large sample of
would have nearly 100% positive predictive value clinical subjects. Using an E score based on
for noncredible performance). ungrouped counting time plus grouped counting
The Boone, Salazar, et al. (2002) investigation time and number of errors, sensitivity was 100%
showed that addition of a recognition trial to the in a criminal forensic sample and 75% in a civil
15-Item Test improved both sensitivity and speci- litigation sample, with specificity of at least 90% in
ficity compared to the traditional version of the the clinical groups. Boone and Lu (2007) con-
test as well as to the other recent modification of ducted a cross-validation of the Dot Counting
the test stimuli (Griffin et al., 1997). Indeed, Test E score on 91 litigation/compensation-seek-
Boone, Salazar, et al. noted that the combined ing subjects failing other symptom validity test
score raised sensitivity by 50% relative to recall procedures, compared to 111 clinical patients and
alone and maintained high specificity. found little change in specificity (validation
Boone and Lu (2007) have cross-validated the sample = >90%; cross-validation sample = 89.2%)
Rey 15-Item Test with recognition, using 90 addi- or sensitivity (validation sample = 75%, cross-
tional noncredible patients and 105 comparison validation sample = 72.5%).
patients. Using an E score cutoff of <20 yielded a The b Test (Boone, Lu, et al., 2000; Boone,
specificity of 85.7% (comparing favorably with the Lu, & Herzberg, 2002) requires subjects to
original derivation sample specificity of >91%) circle all the lowercase b’s in a 15-page booklet,
but sensitivity declined from 71% in the original discriminating the b’s from the q’s, p’s, d’s, and
122 forensic neuropsychology

those lowercase letters with diagonal stems. Boone proportions of right and wrong answers, respec-
et al. (2000; 2002) compared the b Test perfor- tively. In a two-alternative task, P = Q =.5; in
mance of litigating subjects suspected of malin- a four-alternative task, P = .25, Q = .75. A one-tail
gering to the performance of nonlitigating test is the most appropriate, given the suspected
learning-disabled subjects; older depressed sub- intent of producing suppressed scores (Larrabee,
jects; nonlitigating individuals with moderate and 1992). Since the binomial distribution involves a
severe closed head injury (CHI), left or right discrete variable and the normal distribution is
hemisphere cerebrovascular accident, or schizo- for a continuous variable, a correction factor of .5
phrenia; and normal elderly. Boone et al. (2002) is recommended to maximize correspondence
have also developed an E score for the b Test based with the normal curve (for x falling below NP, the
on weighted combinations of omission errors, 0.5 is added to x; for x falling above NP, the 0.5 is
commission errors, and average time per stimulus subtracted from x; Siegel, 1956).
page. The b Test E score cutoffs are provided for Note that because the formula for the normal
the various clinical groups above. Patients with approximation to the binomial is based on the
bona fide severe traumatic brain injury perform standard normal (z) distribution, forced-choice
the best, whereas patients with stroke or schizo- tests can be constructed “on the spot” for a par-
phrenia perform the worst, necessitating higher ticular patient. Pankratz et al. (1975) constructed
E score cutoffs to maintain a reasonable specific- a subject-specific forced-choice test for evaluating
ity. Comparing a group of litigants with suspect feigned deafness, and Pankratz, Binder, and
effort/probable malingering to all of the clinical Wilcox (1987) did the same for evaluation of
cases combined yields a sensitivity of 73.6% exaggerated somatosensory deficits. Denney
and specificity of 85.1%, at an E score cutting (1996) demonstrated that three criminal defen-
score of <120 (for valid test performance). dants suspected of malingering remote memory
impairment in the context of evaluation for
Measures of Response Bias and competency to proceed to trial performed at
Symptom Validity Based on significantly worse-than-chance level on correct
Forced-Choice Testing identification of facts relevant to the alleged
The application of forced-choice methodology offenses, presented in two-alternative, forced-
and the normal approximation to the binomial to choice format.
evaluate validity of test performance represents a A variety of SVTs utilizing the forced-choice
major advance in the evaluation of malingering format have been published. Many used prese-
(Binder, 1990; Binder & Pankratz, 1987; Hiscock ntation of individual sequences of digits to the
& Hiscock, 1989). In a forced-choice task (e.g., subject, who after some period of time must
identifying whether one has been touched once or then “recognize” the multidigit number in a two-
twice; identifying which of two 5-digit numbers alternative, forced-choice format: DMT (Guilmette,
was presented previously), it is conceivable that Hart, & Giuliano, 1993; Hiscock & Hiscock, 1989);
someone could perform at chance level, consis- PDRT (Binder, 1993; Binder & Kelly, 1996; Binder
tent with zero ability. If, however, someone per- & Willis, 1991); Victoria Symptom Validity Test
forms significantly worse than chance, based on (Grote et al., 2000; Slick, Hopp, Strauss, & Spellacy,
application of the normal approximation to the 1996); or Computerized Assessment of Response
binomial theorem, the assumption can be made Bias (CARB; Conder, Allen, & Cox, 1992).
that they had to know the correct answer to per- Other procedures use words as test stimuli,
form at such an improbably poor level. At extreme including the 21-Item Test, in which the subject is
levels of probability (e.g., .05 or .01), it can be presented with a list of 21 items they must freely
argued that such an improbable performance is recall, followed by two-alternative, forced-choice
tantamount to admission of malingering testing (Iverson, Franzen, & Mc-Cracken, 1991,
(Larrabee, 2000). 1994). The Word Memory Test (WMT) requires
The formula for the uncorrected z approxima- the subject to learn a list of several word pairs
tion to the binomial is presented over two trials, followed by immediate
and then delayed forced-choice recognition of
z = ( X − NP )/ NPQ one word from each word pair, with additional
yes/no recognition and paired associate, followed
where X is the patient’s score correct, N is the by free-recall trials (Green, Iverson, & Allen, 1999;
number of test items, and P and Q are the expected Green, Rohling, Lees-Haley, & Allen, 2001;
Assessment of Malingering 123

Iverson, Green, & Gervais, 1999; Green, Lees- item difficulty is taken into account so that
Haley, & Allen, 2002). persons are expected to do less well as the task
There is also a visual memory malingering becomes more difficult. Departures from this
measure based on two-alternative, forced-choice expectation, particularly reversal of errors with
recognition of easily encoded visual stimuli, the better performance on more difficult than on
Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM; Rees, easier items, are associated with a motivated
Tombaugh, Gansler, & Moczynski, 1998; performance deficit.
Tombaugh, 1996). Berry and colleagues (Inman Studies of the comparative sensitivity of SVTs
et al., 1998) have developed the Letter Memory are beginning to appear. Vickery et al. (2001) con-
Test, a procedure for evaluating effort based on ducted a meta-analysis of the DMT, PDRT,
recognition of arrays of three, four, and five con- 21-Item Test, 15-Item Test, and Dot-Counting
sonants presented in forced-choice format involv- Test. The DMT was the most sensitive, separating
ing two, three, or four choices (note that each honest and dissimulating responders by approxi-
stimulus length is tested in two-, three-, or four- mately 2 SD, whereas the 21-Item Test and PDRT
alternative format so that stimulus length is fully had effect sizes of approximately 1.5 SD and 1.25
crossed with number of alternatives). Last, SD, respectively. In contrast, effect sizes were
Frederick (1997) developed a forced-choice mea- equivalent, and the smallest, for the 15-Item and
sure of nonverbal and verbal abilities, the Validity Dot-Counting Tests, approximately 0.75 SD.
Indicator Profile (VIP; also see Frederick, Crosby, Vickery et al. (2000) also conducted sensitivity
& Wynkoop, 2000; Frederick, Sarfaty, Johnston, & and specificity analyses. Based on four studies, the
Powel, 1994). DMT had a specificity of 95.1% and a sensitivity
The two-alternative, forced-choice memory of 83.4%; the PDRT had a specificity of 97.3%,
analog tests (CARB, DMT, PDRT, 21-Item Test, sensitivity of 44%, and combined hit rate of 71.2%
TOMM, Victoria Symptom Validity Test, and based on two studies. The 15-Item Test had a
WMT) can be scored for worse-than-chance specificity of 92.5%, sensitivity of 43.3%, and
performance using the normal approximation to combined hit rate of 70.5% based on a range of six
the binomial. In addition, these procedures can be to eight studies. The 21-Item Test had a specificity
scored on the basis of empirically derived cutoff of 100%, sensitivity of 22%, and combined hit rate
scores that minimize false-positive identification of 60.7% based on four to six studies.
in nonlitigating groups of clinical patients because Vickery et al. (2000) noted that the sensitivity
it has been found that many litigants suspected of of the 21-Item Test could be improved by using a
malingering do not perform at levels that are sig- more liberal cutoff score than that originally
nificantly worse than chance; for example, Greve, proposed by the 21-Item Test authors. Vickery
Binder, and Bianchini (2009) found that below et al.’s study was weighted more heavily with
chance performance (p <0.05, one-tailed) on any noninjured dissimulators than studies using a
of the various trials of the PDRT, WMT, or TOMM known-group design employing actual clinical
occurred at frequencies of 6.2%, 7.1% and 2.6%, malingerers, which may limit the generalizability
respectively. Typically, forced choice tasks are of their findings. These authors recommended
sufficiently easy that the majority of nonlitigating against the use of any one measure, in isolation, as
neurological patients can perform extremely a malingering screening device because of the
well (e.g., for the Letter Memory Test and DMT, less-than-perfect sensitivities of the tests they
the average correct scores for nonlitigating neuro- reviewed.
logical patients were 99.5% and 99.3% correct, Subsequent to the review by Vickery et al.
respectively, contrasted with 54.8% and 64.4% (2001), Bianchini, Mathias, Greve, Houston, and
correct, respectively, for normal community Crouch (2001) reported a higher sensitivity (77%)
volunteers faking head injury; see Inman et al., for the PDRT, with a specificity of 100% in a
1998; for the TOMM, the majority of nonlitigat- known-group design discriminating clinical
ing neurological and psychiatric patients scored malingerers from subjects with moderate and
above the cutoff of 90% correct, see Rees et al., severe CHI. Inman and Berry (2002) found that
1998; Tombaugh, 1996). the Letter Memory Test and DMT attained the
For the VIP, Frederick developed a more highest hit rates for identification of malingering
elaborate performance curve analysis based on in college students with and without history of
item difficulty. On the VIP, items are randomly mild head injury who took the tests following
presented as to item difficulty level. In scoring, instructions for good effort versus instructions to
124 forensic neuropsychology

deliberately perform poorly. The DMT had a (Larrabee, 1990; 2000; Slick et al., 1999). Test
specificity of 100%, sensitivity of 64%, and overall patterns should make “neuropsychological sense”
hit rate of 82%; the Letter Memory Test had a (Larrabee, 1990); hence, a patient with impaired
specificity of 100%, sensitivity of 73%, and overall attention should not have above average memory
hit rate of 87%, compared to the 21-Item Test and (Mittenberg et al., 1993); a patient with impaired
15-Item Test, which had sensitivities of 2% and Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition
5%, respectively, and a specificity of 100% each. (WAIS-III; Wechsler, 1997a) Digit Span at a level
The classification results for the DMT and Letter characteristic of dementia should not have
Memory Test were also superior to the use of above average WAIS-III Vocabulary (Mittenberg
malingering cutoff scores or formulas for Reliable et al., 1995, 2001); a patient with very poor Grip
Digit Span (RDS; cf. Greiffenstein et al., 1994; Strength (Reitan & Wolfson, 1993) should not
100% specificity, 27% sensitivity, 65% combined perform above average on the Grooved Pegboard
hit rate); Seashore Rhythm Test (Reitan & Wolfson, (Lafayette Instrument, P.O. Box 5729, Lafayette,
1993) errors greater than 8 (Trueblood & Schmidt, IN; Greiffenstein, Baker, & Gola, 1996).
1993; 98% specificity, 27% sensitivity, 64% com- Mittenberg, Aguila-Puentes, et al. (2002)
bined hit rate); Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test referred to multiple variable pattern analysis
(AVLT; Rey, 1964) recognition less than 8 as neuropsychological profiling of symptom
(Greiffenstein et al., 1994; 100% specificity, 16% exaggeration and malingering. The authors
sensitivity, 60% combined hit rate); Digit Symbol credited Heaton et al. (1978) with the first demon-
(Wechsler, 1981) scale score less than 5 (cf. stration that patterns of neuropsychological test
Trueblood, 1994; 100% specificity, 2% sensitivity, performance could discriminate 16 non-injured
53% combined hit rate); and the Bernard, dissimulators from 16 nonlitigating patients
McGrath, and Houston (1996) Wisconsin Card with moderate-to-severe CHI. Using the WAIS
Sorting Test (WCST; Heaton, Chelune, Talley, Kay, (Wechsler, 1955) and HRB, discriminant function
& Curtiss, 1993) discriminant function (100% analysis correctly classified 100% of the dissimu-
specificity, 9% sensitivity, 58% overall hit rate). lators and patients with CHI. Based on the
Greve, Ord, Curtis, Bianchini, and Brennan MMPI (Hathaway & McKinley, 1983) alone, only
(2008) compared the diagnostic accuracy of the one subject in each group was misidentified.
PDRT, TOMM, and WMT for discrimination of Significant univariate differences were found on
probable TBI malingerers and probable pain the WAIS Digit Span, and for HRB tests including
malingerers from nonmalingering patients with Category Test errors, Tactual Performance
bona fide TBI, and bona fide chronic pain. Test Total Time, Memory and Location, Finger
Employing receiver operating characteristic Tapping, Finger Agnosia, suppressions, and the
(ROC) analyses, Greve et al. found that the PDRT, Hand Dynamometer.
TOMM, and WMT had equivalent area under Mittenberg et al. (1996) replicated the Heaton
curve (AUC) maximum values for discriminating et al. results with an HRB discriminant function
the TBI malingerers from TBI patients (PDRT contrasting noninjured dissimulating subjects
Easy AUC = 0.89; TOMM Trial 1 AUC = 0.85; from nonlitigating subjects with CHI (mild,
WMT IR AUC = 0.86), and for discriminating moderate, and severe TBI). Significant univariate
chronic pain malingerers from chronic pain differences were found on Category Test errors,
patients (PDRT Total AUC = 0.78; TOMM Trial 1 Tactual Performance Test Total Time, Speech
AUC = 0.76; WMT Consistency AUC = 0.83). Sounds Perception Errors and Seashore Rhythm
Correct, Trails B, Finger Tapping, Sensory Suppre-
AS SES SMENT OF ssions, Finger Agnosia, and Finger Tip Number
MALINGERING USING Writing.
AT Y P I C A L P E R F O R M A N C E Patterns of performance indicative of poor
PAT T E R N S O N S TA N D A R D effort have also been reported on individual neu-
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ropsychological tests, such as poor recognition
TESTS memory on the AVLT (Binder, Villanueva,
Detection of malingering through the presence Howieson, & Moore, 1993; Boone, Lu, & Wen,
of atypical performance patterns on standard 2005); and poor recognition memory on the
neuropsychological tests is the basis of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; Delis,
performance inconsistency approach discussed in Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1987; Millis, Putnam,
the section on the definition of malingering Adams, & Ricker, 1995). More complex patterns
Assessment of Malingering 125

characteristic of intentional underperformance


TABLE 5.1 PERFORMANCE OF
including atypical recall and recognition have
LITIGANTS WITH DEFINITE OR
been developed for the second edition of the
CVLT, the CVLT-2 (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & PROBABLE MALINGERED
Ober, 2000; Wolfe et al., 2010), and for the AVLT NEUROCOGNITIVE DYSFUNCTION AND
(Barrash, Suhr, & Manzel, 2004). Atypical error CLINICAL PATIENTS ON
patterns on problem-solving tests such as the NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TESTS
WCST (Bernard et al., 1996; Suhr & Boyer, 1999) SENSITIVE TO MALINGERING
and Category Test (Tenhula & Sweet, 1996) have
Test MNDa Clinical p Effect
also been identified. Patientsb Sizec
I (Larrabee, 2003a) found that scores on Benton
Visual Form Discrimination (VFD; Benton, Sivan, VFDd
Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, 1994), Finger Tapping M 26.39 29.89 .0005 1.02
(FT), RDS, Wisconsin Card Sorting Failure-to- SD (4.58) (2.25)
Maintain Set (FMS), and the Lees-Haley Fake Bad FTe
Scale (FBS; Lees-Haley, English, & Glenn, 1991) M 69.27 83.85 .005 .70
on the MMPI-2 were useful in discriminating liti- SD (27.95) (13.78)
gants with definite or probable MND from patients RDSf
with moderate/severe TBI and various neurologi- M 7.15 9.78 .0005 1.33
cal and psychiatric disorders. Table 5.1 presents SD (1.82) (2.11)
the test data for litigants with MND and for Clinical FMSg
Patients (TBI, depression, and various neurologi- M 1.29 .56 .005 .67
cal conditions). These data, the aforementioned SD (1.36) (0.84)
studies, and the discriminant function analyses of FBSh
Mittenberg and colleagues (Mittenberg et al., M 26.95 16.48 .0005 1.99
1993, 1995, 1996) suggested that standard test pro- SD (5.36) (5.22)
cedures particularly sensitive to poor effort are
a
MND = 24 litigants with definite and 17 probable litigants with
tests of sensorimotor function, attention, recogni- neurocognitive dysfunction
tion memory, and problem solving. b
Clinical patients = 27 moderate/severe TBI, 14 psychiatric and 13
A set of symptom validity scores has been mixed neurologic diagnosis
c
Effect size: in pooled SD units.
developed for the WAIS-IV (Wechsler, 2008) and d
VFD = Benton Visual Form Discrimination
WMS-IV (Wechsler, 2009) as part of The e
FT = Finger Tapping combined right and left hand raw scores
f
RDS = Reliable Digit Span
Advanced Clinical Solutions (ACS; Holdnack and g
FMS = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Failure to Maintain Set
Drozdick (2009). The ACS includes a chapter on h
FBS = MMPI-2 Symptom Validity Scale
assessment of suboptimal effort based on five
scores: Reliable Digit Span from WAIS-IV, the
recognition trials of Logical Memory, Verbal can choose test performance cutoffs identifying 2,
Paired Associates, and Visual Reproduction, and 5, 10, 15, and 25% of the combined clinical sample
the score from a newly developed free-standing (i.e., false alarm rates of 2% to 25%), then check
SVT, the Word Choice Test (a two alternative, the corresponding percent of the simulators (i.e.,
forced choice word recognition procedure similar sensitivity values for detection of feigning), and
to the verbal part of the Recognition Memory Test the percentage of the “no stimulus” group who
of Warrington). Data are provided on the perfor- achieve these scores. Data are also provided as to
mance of the WAIS-IV and WMS-IV clinical the percentage of clinical subjects who are mis-
groups (e.g., TBI, temporal lobectomy, etc.) on identified as showing poor effort, and percentage
these measures, as well as the performance of a of simulators correctly identified, based on mul-
group of 50 healthy simulators, and 50 healthy tiple positive test indicators (e.g., 2, 3, 4, or 5
subjects taking the Word Choice Test, Logical scores exceeding a particular cutoff ).
Memory, Verbal Paired Associates, and Visual Miller et al. (2011) evaluated the diagnostic
Reproduction recognition tests without having sensitivity of the five ACS scores to feigned
been exposed to the stimuli during the learning impairment, using 39 healthy individuals simulat-
trials for these tests (i.e., recognition memory is ing TBI, and 45 nonlitigating subjects with actual
tested without the subject having been presented moderate and severe TBI. Logistic regression
with the to-be-remembered stimuli). The examiner including the five ACS variables yielded an area
126 forensic neuropsychology

under curve (AUC) of .95, showing outstanding Rapport, Farchione, Coleman, and Axelrod
discrimination (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 2000). (1998) attempted a replication of the Greiffenstein
A second logistic regression that removed the et al. (1996) motor function results with Grip
Word Choice Test, showed a statistically signi- Strength, Finger Tapping, and the Grooved
ficant change compared to the full model includ- Pegboard. These authors, using naive and coached
ing Word Choice, providing strong support for noninjured college student dissimulators and
including Word Choice in the full model. Word noninjured college student controls, did not
Choice, by itself, showed excellent discrimination, replicate the Greiffenstein et al. results. Although
with an AUC of .84. Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were the dissimulators were provided with descriptions
1.03 for Reliable Digit Span, .21 for Logical Memory of effects of spinal cord injury and mild head
Recognition, .67 for Verbal Paired Associates injury, they were not specifically advised to
Recognition, 1.81 for Visual Reproduction Reco- perform poorly on the motor function tests. This
gnition, and 1.38 for Word Choice. may explain the differences between the Rapport
et al. (1998) results and those of Greiffenstein
Motivational Impairment on et al., who used actual litigating patients chosen
Motor Function Tests for presence of reduced motor function scores.
Binder and Willis (1991) contrasted the neurop- Finger Tapping scores are also available from
sychological test performance of ten subjects with the Heaton et al. (1978) and Mittenberg et al.
mild CHI with low motivation on the PDRT (15 (1996) studies contrasting noninjured dissimula-
or less of 36 on the “hard” items or 30 or less of 72 tors with nonlitigating patients with CHI. Despite
total items) with that of 19 subjects with mild CHI 18 years of difference between these two investi-
scoring 23 or higher on the “hard” PDRT. The gations, conducted in Denver, Colorado (Heaton
poorly motivated subjects with mild CHI per- et al., 1978), and Ft. Lauderdale, Florida
formed significantly lower than the normally (Mittenberg et al., 1996), there is a striking simi-
motivated subjects with mild CHI on the Finger larity in dominant plus nondominant raw score:
Tapping and Grooved Pegboard Tests. The Heaton et al. dissimulators averaged 63.1 taps,
Greiffenstein et al. (1996) compared the Grip and the Mittenberg et al. dissimulators averaged
Strength, Finger Tapping Test, and Grooved 63.0 taps.
Pegboard performance of 54 subjects with mod- Arnold et al. (2005) reported separate score
erate-to-severe CHI who also had unambiguous distributions on Finger Tapping for males and
motor abnormalities on standard neurological females, and determined that the best discrimina-
examination (note that dense hemiplegics were tor of probable malingerers from bona fide clini-
excluded) with the performance of 131 litigating cal groups was the dominant hand performance.
patients with post-concussion syndrome (PCS) For females, a dominant hand score of <29 yielded
who performed on at least one motor function a sensitivity of 61% and specificity of 92%; while
score poorer than T40 using the normative data for males, a dominant hand score of <36 had a
of Heaton, Grant, and Matthews (1991). The sub- sensitivity of 50% with a specificity of 90%.
jects with moderate-to-severe CHI showed the Interestingly, when the male and female data are
expected pattern of performing best on Grip combined for the dominant- plus nondominant-
Strength and worst on the Grooved Pegboard hand raw scores, the optimal cutting score for dis-
Test, whereas the PCS subjects performed poorest criminating probable malingerers from clinical
on Grip Strength and better on Finger Tapping patients was 63 for a sensitivity of 49% and speci-
and the Grooved Pegboard. In particular, the most ficity of 90%. These values are nearly identical to
significant PCS/CHI differences were on Grip those reported by me (Larrabee, 2003a), support-
Strength. Although the PCS subjects had higher ing the generalizability of findings from both
scores on MMPI Scales 1, 2, 3, and 7 than the sub- investigations.
jects with moderate-to-severe CHI, consistent Table 5.2 displays average Finger Tapping data
with heightened reports of depression, anxiety, for dissimulating and head-injured subjects in the
and physical symptoms in the PCS group, these Heaton et al. (1978) and Mittenberg et al. (1996)
MMPI scales did not correlate with motor perfor- studies; the Rapport et al. (1998) dissimulators;
mance in the PCS group. The authors suggested a group of dissimulators from Orey, Cragar, and
that the low Grip Strength scores of their PCS Berry’s (2000) study; a group of subjects with
litigants were consistent with malingering, but MND from Binder and Willis’s (1991) study;
not necessarily proof of malingering. my (2003a) subjects with definite MND and
Assessment of Malingering 127

TABLE 5.2 COMBINED DOMINANT PLUS


NONDOMINANT FINGER TAPPING SPEED FOR
HEADINJURED, SIMULATING, AND MALINGERING
SUBJECTS

Subject Group

Study Traumatic Simulatorsa MNDb


Brain Injury

Heaton et al. (1978)c


M 80.2 63.1 —
SD (21.4) (17.1) —
Mittenberg et al. 1996)d
M 75.6 63.0 —
SD (20.4) (32.7) —
Binder & Willis (1991)e
M — — 71.89
SD — — (16.5)
Rapport et al. (1998)f
M — 60.0 —
SD — (19.2) —
Orey et al. (2000)g
M — 64.19 —
SD — (18.67) —
Larrabee (2003b)h
M 78.37 — 70.97
SD (13.64) — (26.72)
Arnold et al. (2005)i
M 85.24 — 68.37
SD (16.06) — (17.88)
a
Simulators = Noninjured persons simulating impairment
b
MND = Malingered Neurocognitive Deficit
c
Heaton et al. = 16 simulators, 16 Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI)
d
Mittenberg et al. = 80 simulators, 80 TBI
e
Binder & Willis = 10 MND
f
Rapport et al. = 31 simulators
g
Orey et al. = 26 simulators
h
Larrabee = 27 TBI, 24 MND
i
Arnold et al. = 24 TBI, 77 MND

moderate-to-severe TBI; and the Arnold et al. hand) Finger Tapping scores are seen in patients
(2005) probable malingerers and moderate/severe with Huntington’s disease (48), multiple sclerosis
TBI patients. Greiffenstein (2007) has reported (60), and cortical dementia (62), from a series
similar data for combined (right plus left hand) reported by Butters, Goldstein, Allen, and
Finger Tapping data in samples of simulators, Shemansky (1998). Last, Arnold and Boone
known groups, and clinical patients, including (2007) provide a helpful appendix summarizing
many of the studies reported in Table 5.2, as well data from a variety of studies contrasting either
as additional clinical bench mark data for clinical dissimulating subjects with clinical cases, or
patients with known neuromotor dysfunction known-groups of probable malingerers contrasted
secondary to such conditions as multiple sclero- with relevant clinical groups (e.g., moderate/
sis, Huntington’s disease, and cerebrovascular severe TBI) for several motor function tasks
disease. In the data reviewed by Greiffenstein including grip strength, the Grooved Pegboard,
(2007), the lowest mean combined (right plus left and Finger Tapping.
128 forensic neuropsychology

Digit Span and Malingering probable malingerers from patients with TBI
Mittenberg et al. (1995) found that the simple and a sensitivity of 68% and specificity of 89% for
difference score between Vocabulary and Digit an RDS of 7 or less in discriminating probable
Span age-scaled scores worked almost as well as a malingerers from subjects with persistent PCS.
discriminant function analysis employing seven Meyers and Volbrecht (1998) replicated
WAIS-R subtests in discriminating noninjured the RDS findings of Greiffenstein et al. (1994).
dissimulators from nonlitigating patients with In a comparison of 47 litigants with mild brain
CHI. Mittenberg et al. (2001) replicated this result injury and 49 nonlitigants with mild brain injury,
using the WAIS-III, subjects with CHI, nonin- only 4.1% of nonlitigants had an RDS of 7 or less,
jured dissimulators, and a group of litigants iden- contrasted with 48.9% of litigants. In my (Larrabee,
tified as probable malingerers by virtue of current 2003a) investigation, an RDS score of 7 or less
functioning IQ more than 15 points below esti- correctly identified 50% of litigants with definite
mated premorbid level of function. Trueblood MND and 93.5% of persons with moderate and
and Schmidt (1993) recommended consideration severe TBI.
of malingering when the age-scaled score for Digit In a unique investigation, Etherton, Bianchini,
Span was less than 7. Ciota, and Greve (2005) administered the Digit
Suhr and Barrash (2007) reviewed research Span subtest to three groups of undergraduates:
contrasting performance of simulators or proba- 1) a group undergoing a cold-pressor test,
ble malingerers to clinical comparison samples 2) a group following standard instructions, and
including chronic pain and TBI. A Digit Span 3) a group simulating pain-related memory deficit
age-corrected scaled score of <5 was associated in pursuit of personal injury litigation. There were
with sensitivities ranging from 25% to 47% no differences in RDS performance between the
(median = 30%), and specificities ranging from group undergoing cold-pressor testing and the
94% to 100% (median = 98%). The review by group tested on standard Digit Span instructions,
Babikian and Boone (2007) also supports a cut- and no subject in either group had an RDS of 7 or
ting score of <5 on Digit Span age-scaled score as less. By contrast, 65% of the simulator group had
associated with >90% specificity, but sensitivity RDS of 7 or less.
was low, ranging from 19% to 32%. Babikian and Suhr and Barrash (2007) and Babikian and
Boone (2007) observed that raising the Digit Span Boone (2007) have also reviewed published
age-scaled score cutoff to <6 increased sensitivity research on RDS. Suhr and Barrash reported
from 36% to 47%, while limiting false positives to sensitivities ranging from 19.6% to 71%, and
10%, with the exception of stroke patients, who specificities ranging from 71.6% to 100% for an
had a 14% false positive rate (per Heinly, Greve, RDS score of <8, concluding that an RDS cutoff of
Bianchini, Love, & Brennan, 2005). In a study sub- <7 was a solid recommendation, but that a cutoff
sequent to these two reviews, Greve et al. (2007) of <8 might also indicate malingering, particu-
reported sensitivity of 46% and specificity of larly in the presence of other malingering indica-
97% for a WAIS-III Digit Span age-scaled score of tors to help minimize false positives and with
<6 for discriminating between probable malinger- consideration of the nature and severity of the
ers with neurotoxic claims and nonmalingering neurological injury (see Larrabee, 2003a).
patients with toxic exposure. Babikian and Boone (2007) computed a weighted
Greiffenstein et al. (1994) developed a proce- average sensitivity of 68% for probable malinger-
dure for evaluating effort based on the WAIS/ ers in the studies they reviewed, but weighted
WAIS-R/WAIS-III Digit Span Subtest: Reliable average specificity of 81% for moderate/severe
Digit Span (RDS). This is computed by summing TBI, noting a further drop in specificity for stroke
the longest string of digits repeated without error and for a general neurological sample, using an
over two trials under both forward and backward RDS cutoff of <8. Babikian and Boone (2007)
conditions. As an example, Greiffenstein et al. observed that lowering the RDS cutoff to <7
noted that a patient who passed both trials of caused a drop in sensitivity to probable malinger-
three digits forward and both trials of two digits ing, ranging from 38% to 57% in the studies they
backward, but failed one trial at four digits reviewed, but that specificity was 88% or higher
forward and one trial at three digits reversed, using this cutoff, with the exception of stroke
would have an RDS of 5. Greiffenstein et al. patients (Heinly et al., 2005).
reported a sensitivity of 70% and specificity of Contemporaneous with the Suhr and Barrash
73% for an RDS of 7 or less in discriminating (2007) and Babikian and Boone (2007) reviews,
Assessment of Malingering 129

Greve et al. (2007) investigated RDS in neurotoxic specificity (no false positives) in discriminating
claimants classified as probable malingerers, forensic examinees with valid data from
nonmalingering neurotoxically exposed patients, those with invalid data consistent with the
and patients with history of TBI or chronic pain. presence of probable malingering (on the basis
RDS <8 was associated with a sensitivity of 54% of failure of TOMM and/or VIP). Wolfe et al.
and specificity of 89%. Lowering the RDS cutoff (2010) found that a logistic regression based on
to <7 reduced sensitivity to 46% but improved CVLT-II Long-Delay Free Recall, Total Recog-
specificity to 97%. nition Discriminability (d′) and Total Recall
Discriminability, discriminated well between
Measures of Recognition Memory litigants/claimants with alleged mTBI failing at
Several investigators have reported dispropor- least two SVTs in a manner consistent with
tionate impairment in recognition memory in probable malingering, and nonlitigating patients
probable malingerers. Binder et al. (1993) found with moderate or severe TBI. The receiver operat-
significantly lower Rey AVLT recognition scores ing characteristic area under curve was .83, and
in litigants with mild TBI failing the PDRT than sensitivity was .49 at a specificity of .90.
in nonlitigating head-injured patients. Suhr, Donders and Strong (2010) attempted to
Tranel, Wefel, and Barrash (1997) also reported cross-validate the Wolfe et al. (2010) logistic
poorer AVLT recognition scores for probable regression, employing passing or failing the WMT
malingerers in comparison to patients with head as the “gold standard” for presence of invalid effort
injury, depression, or somatization disorder. using a sample of mild, moderate, and severe TBI.
Binder, Kelly, Villanueva, and Winslow (2003) Although there was a significant association
found that litigating subjects with mild TBI with between pass/fail on both the WMT and CVLT-II
good motivation outperformed litigating subjects logistic regression, the CVLT-II false positive rates
with mild TBI who had poor motivation on the were much higher than originally reported by
recognition trial of the AVLT. The poorly moti- Wolfe et al. (2010). To achieve .90 specificity,
vated litigants with mild TBI were also outper- Donders and Strong had to raise the CVLT-II
formed on AVLT recognition by a group of logistic regression cutoff to .67, resulting in
normally motivated nonlitigating patients with a sensitivity of .375. This contrasts with specificity
moderate-to-severe TBI. Atypical patterns of rec- of .90 and sensitivity of .49 associated with a
ognition play a prominent role in the Exaggeration cutoff score of .6245 in the Wolfe et al. (2010)
Index (EI) developed for the AVLT by Barrash investigation. Donders and Strong reported
et al. (2004). Four of the seven atypical perfor- significant correlations between all three CVLT-II
mance patterns comprising the EI are based on validity indicators and duration of coma, con-
clinically atypical patterns of recognition memory trasted with no significant correlations between
performance, including worsening recognition, the WMT scores and coma or between CVLT-II
learned words not recognized, recalled words not forced choice scores and coma. The authors noted
recognized, and exceedingly poor recognition. this demonstrated that the CVLT-II scores com-
Millis et al. (1995) found that litigants with prising the invalid effort equation measure factors
mild TBI with poor motivation produced lower related to cognitive ability rather than serving
scores on CVLT Recognition Hits and only as measures of invalid effort. They concluded
Discriminability than did patients who had mod- that investigators use great caution in applying
erate or severe TBI. Coleman, Rapport, Millis, the Wolfe et al. (2010) formula, prior to further
Ricker, and Farchione (1998) replicated these independent replication, and recommended the
findings with a normal subject simulation design, more conservative cutoff of .67, along with admin-
although the effect was attenuated when the sub- istration of other SVTs in addition to the CVLT-II
jects were provided with cues on how to elude logistic regression for invalid effort.
detection as a malingerer. Poor performance on recognition memory in
The CVLT-II (Delis et al., 2000) contains an association with poor motivation has also been
optional forced choice recognition task as a mea- reported for the Rey Complex Figure Test (Corwin
sure of symptom validity. Root, Robins, Chang, & Bylsma, 1993; Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941);
and Van Gorp (2006) found that a total correct of Warrington Recognition Memory Test
14 or less out of 16 on forced choice recognition (Warrington, 1984), and recognition tasks/scores
had a sensitivity of .44 and specificity of .93, and on the Wechsler Memory Scale III (WMS-III;
that scores of 11 or less were associated with 100% Wechsler, 1997b). Meyers and Volbrecht (1999),
130 forensic neuropsychology

using a differential prevalence design, found substantially (35% for CVMT, 52.9% for CRM) in
that litigants (primarily those with mild TBI) litigants failing at least two SVTs but not at values
produced memory error patterns characterized that were significantly worse-than-chance. These
by poor recognition performance on the Rey data show that both the CVMT and CRM SVT
Complex Figure Test, in contrast to nonlitigating scales were more sensitive to detection of definite
individuals with mild TBI, who did not show evi- than probable malingering.
dence of poor recognition. Lu, Boone, Cozolino,
and Mitchell (2003) found that a combination Measures of Problem-Solving Ability
score based on copy plus true positive minus Persons identified as malingering also performed
atypical recognition errors discriminated suspect poorly and in neurologically atypical patterns of
effort cases from a variety of clinical cases on the performance on measures of problem solving
Rey Complex Figure Test. Millis (1992, 1994) and such as the WCST (Heaton et al., 1993) and the
Millis and Putnam (1994) have shown that litigat- Category Test of the HRB. Bernard et al. (1996)
ing subjects with mild TBI performed more poorly found that noninjured simulators produced
on the Recognition Memory Test, more so on poorer performance ratios on Categories relative
Words than on Faces, in comparison to nonlitigat- to Perseverative Errors on the WCST in compari-
ing subjects with moderate and severe TBI. son to patients with TBI or other central nervous
Subsequently, Kim et al. (2010) found both the system disorders.
total correct score for Warrington Words (sensi- Suhr and Boyer (1999) found that student
tivity of 88.9%, specificity of 91.9%), and the total simulators as well as probable malingerers could
time to complete the recognition memory task be discriminated from nonlitigating subjects
(sensitivity of 65.5%, specificity of 90.7%) discrim- with mild and moderate TBI on the basis of
inated well between subjects with external incen- WCST Categories and FMS, with a lower ratio
tive who had failed at least two independent SVTs of Categories to FMS in the malingering groups.
from subjects who had failed at most one SVT. I (Larrabee, 2003a) found that FMS alone worked
Other recognition memory procedures are more effectively than the Suhr and Boyer discrim-
also sensitive to detection of poor effort. Glassmire inant score based on Categories and FMS in dis-
et al. (2003) found that poor performance on criminating litigants with definite MND from
WMS-III Faces I was characteristic of noninjured patients with moderate and severe TBI.
persons trying to simulate impairment, as well as Greve and Bianchini (2002) found an unac-
characteristic of participants with moderate and ceptably high false-positive rate for both the
severe TBI attempting to dissimulate impairment. Bernard et al. (1996) and Suhr and Boyer (1999)
Finally, Langeluddecke and Lucas (2003) found WCST discriminant function equations in dis-
that WMS-III delayed auditory recognition criminating simulating normal college students
memory tasks successfully discriminated a group from patients with TBI, various neurological con-
of litigating subjects with mild TBI with probable ditions, and substance abuse. In another investi-
MND from a group of litigating individuals with gation, Greve et al. (2002) improved the sensitivity
mild TBI with good motivation as well as from a and specificity of WCST malingering indicators
group of litigating individuals with severe TBI. I by combining multiple indicators (e.g., Bernard
(Larrabee, 2008a) developed embedded SVT et al., 1996, criteria and Suhr & Boyer, 1999, crite-
scales for the Continuous Visual Memory Test ria), including “unique” errors on the WCST as
(CVMT; Trahan & Larrabee, (1988) and the another variable sensitive to malingering. These
Continuous Recognition Memory Test (CRM; authors also provided different cutoffs for the
Hannay, Levin & Grossman, 1979), identifying Bernard et al. (1996) and Suhr and Boyer (1999)
those CVMT or CRM items typically failed by functions that minimized false-positive errors in
litigants performing significantly less-than-chance discriminating probable malingerers from patients
on the PDRT, which were typically passed by with TBI with adequate motivation.
patients with moderate or severe TBI. Sensitivity King, Sweet, Sherer, Curtiss, and Vanderploeg
for the CVMT SVT was 83% with specificity at (2002) found varying degrees of classification
88.9%, with CRM SVT scores showing a sensitiv- accuracy for the Bernard et al. (1996) and Suhr
ity of 87.5% at a specificity of 93%. Although spec- and Boyer (1999) WCST classification formulas,
ificity remained high on cross validation with as well as for a logistic regression formula devel-
neurologic and psychiatric patients (93% for oped by King et al. that employed FMS, Categories
CVMT, 96% for CRM), sensitivity dropped Achieved, and Percent Conceptual Responses.
Assessment of Malingering 131

Overall, these formulas worked well; however, that the number of errors on Subtests I and II
King et al. found classification limitations in was consistently the most accurate malingering
association with chronicity and severity of the indicator regardless of degree of coaching or
reported TBI and advised against use of any single presence of TBI. Sweet and King (2002) provided
WCST insufficient effort criterion in isolation. further review of Category Test validity indicators
Subsequently, Greve, Heinly, Bianchini, and Love with case examples and recommendations for
(2009), using samples of mTBI and moderate/ use.
severe TBI plus a large clinical sample, found that Greve, Bianchini, and Roberson (2007) evalu-
most WCST scores were not effective in discrimi- ated the sensitivity and specificity of a variety of
nating litigants with invalid SVT performance Booklet Category Test scores in discriminating
from those with valid SVT performance, with the litigants with invalid SVT performance consistent
exception of FMS, and the Suhr and Boyer (1999) with probable malingering from those TBI
and King et al. (2002) logistic regressions. At patients with valid test performance, and from a
100% specificity (0% false positives), FMS had a clinical group with various neurological disorders
sensitivity of 16%, which was clearly superior to but who did not have any external incentives.
that of the Suhr and Boyer (1999) formula, 8%, They found that the total error score was the most
and King et al. (2002) formula, 5%. Greve et al. accurate indicator overall, with the effectiveness
offered three possibilities for the limited sensitiv- of the other indicators related to injury severity.
ity of WCST scores to malingering: 1) there are Greve et al. also compared their results to those of
large individual differences in all WCST scores in Tenhula and Sweet (1996), using the cutoff scores
the normative population; 2) measures of execu- from this earlier investigation, and found that in
tive dysfunction, like the WCST, are very sensitive their sample, the false positive rates were higher
to acquired brain impairment, so that persons than those reported by Tenhula and Sweet. When
with legitimate impairment may perform in the Greve et al. adjusted their cutoffs to yield false
same range as those who show evidence of malin- positive rates equivalent to those reported by
gering; and 3) persons who are malingering do Tenhula and Sweet, the sensitivity was substan-
not perform atypically on the WCST as they do tially lower than that reported by Tenhula and
not perceive the test as relevant or reflecting the Sweet. Moreover, total errors on Category Subtests
type of impairment they think is a consequence of I and II, found by both Tenhula and Sweet (1996)
traumatic brain injury. and Dicarlo et al. (2000) to be the most effective in
Indicators of insufficient effort suggestive discriminating malingering from TBI patients
of malingering have also been developed for was a poor discriminator in Greve et al. (2007),
the Category Test. Tenhula and Sweet (1996) with a sensitivity of 15% at a false positive rate of
performed a double cross-validation study con- 4% using a cutoff of 2 or more errors.
trasting normal subjects simulating impairment
on the Category Test with performance on the D E T E C T I O N O F S Y M P TO M
Category Test of normal subjects performing E X A G G E R AT I O N
optimally and with nonlitigating patients with Forensic neuropsychological evaluation entails
severe TBI. A number of Category Test indicators both direct examination of the litigant/defendant,
showed good discrimination of the simulating including interview, testing, and behavioral
malingerers from the normal and severe TBI observation, as well as record review. These
groups, including Total Number of Errors, Items sources of information are then integrated by the
from Subtests I and II, “Bolter et al.” items (Bolter, forensic neuropsychologist. The direct part of the
Picano, & Zych, 1985; items rarely missed by evaluation includes interviewing, testing of neu-
patients with bona fide brain damage), Number of ropsychological abilities, and quantification of
Errors on Subtest VII, and Number of “Easy” symptom report through interview (including
items (items rarely missed by the group with structured interview); use of symptom checklists;
severe TBI, similar to the Bolter et al. items). The and use of formal self-report instruments such as
best sensitivity (75.6%) and specificity (98.1%) the MMPI-2 (Butcher, Graham, Ben-Porath,
were associated with number of errors on Tellegen, Dahlstrom, & Kaemmer, 2001) or pain
Subtests I and II. scales such as the McGill Pain Questionnaire
In a replication study that also investigated the (MPQ; Melzack, 1975). Obviously, a litigant/
effects of coaching on feigned performance, defendant can control both the degree of effort
DiCarlo, Gfeller, and Oliveri (2000) also found exerted on testing and the number and magnitude
132 forensic neuropsychology

of reported symptoms. Consequently, the forensic indiscriminant manner. The MMPI/MMPI-2


examiner must be able to discriminate legitimate also include measures such as the F-minus-K
from exaggerated symptom report. index, Obvious–Subtle difference scores, and
The present review of measures of symptom Dissimulation-Revised scales, which are sensitive
report will focus selectively on the MMPI-2. to malingering of psychiatric disorder (Greene,
Although there are many other measures of symp- 2000). Of all the scales, F is one of the most effec-
tom report, the MMPI-2 has the greatest research tive in discriminating valid from invalid psychiat-
database of any omnibus personality scale. Berry ric profiles (see Berry et al., 1991).
and Schipper (2007) reviewed a number of scales Following the publication of the MMPI-2
for detection of feigned psychiatric symptoms (Butcher et al., 1989), Arbisi and Ben-Porath (1995)
during forensic neuropsychological examinations developed the Infrequency-Psychopathology
on a variety of what they termed “quality control Scale, F(p), by selecting MMPI-2 items endorsed
indicators,” including adequate number of by 20% or less of hospitalized psychiatric patients
publications, operating characteristics from as well as infrequently endorsed by 20% or less of
appropriate samples, convergence in operating MMPI-2 normal subjects. In a subsequent inves-
characteristics, appropriate predictive power, tigation, Arbisi and Ben-Porath (1998) found that
independent cross-validation, and neurologic F(p) outperformed F in distinguishing between
control groups. Only the FBS from the MMPI-2 psychiatric patients performing honestly and
and the Structured Interview of Reported those attempting to fake bad.
Symptoms (SIRS; Rogers et al., 1992) met all seven Gass and Luis (2001) demonstrated that the
criteria (note though, that the SIRS neurologic sensitivity of F(p) to symptom exaggeration was
group was a group with mental retardation rather reduced by the presence of four items from the L
than acquired neurologic deficit). Two scales met scale of the MMPI-2. These authors showed that
all criteria with the exception of not having a neu- these four items actually measured defensiveness,
rologic control group: the Negative Impression not exaggeration, and recommended removal of
scale and Malingering Index from the Personality the items from the F(p). A shortened version of
Assessment Inventory (Morey, 1991), and the F(p) that omitted the four L scale items was
Total Score from the Miller Forensic Assessment found to be superior to the original F(p) as a
of Symptoms Test (Miller, 2001). measure of symptom exaggeration. In a subse-
The most widely used symptom report scale in quent study, Arbisi, Ben-Porath, and McNulty
forensic neuropsychology is the MMPI/MMPI-2 (2003) found that the original F(p) scale contain-
(Lees-Haley, Smith, Williams, & Dunn, 1996). The ing the four L scale items was more effective in
MMPI and MMPI-2 have long played a role in identifying malingering than was the revised
assessment of malingered symptom report (Berry, version suggested by Gass and Luis (2001). The
Baer, & Harris, 1991; Graham, 2006; Greene, 2000; F(p) scale was included as a standard validity scale
Rogers, Sewell, & Salekin, 1994). The prototypic in the revised edition of the MMPI-2 (Butcher
MMPI scale for detection of malingering is the et al., 2001).
Infrequency or F scale, which was originally Despite demonstration of the sensitivity of F,
derived by selecting items endorsed by 10% or less Fb, F(p), F minus K, and other MMPI-2 scores
of the normal adult sample (Graham, 2006). The such as the sum of Obvious-Subtle difference, and
MMPI-2 (Butcher et al., 1989) includes the F scale, Dissimulation-Revised scores to detection of exag-
as well as the Fb scale, developed for items occur- gerated psychiatric symptomatology (Rogers et al.,
ring on the second half of the MMPI-2, again 1994), these MMPI-2 validity scales may not be
endorsed by 10% or less of the normal adult sensitive to exaggeration of symptoms in personal
sample, using the same methodology as that used injury settings for litigants pursuing neuropsycho-
to develop F. In addition, the MMPI-2 includes logical claims. Indeed, Greiffenstein et al. (1995)
measures of response inconsistency, including the found that F, total sum of Obvious-Subtle differ-
Variable Response Inconsistency scale (VRIN), a ence, and F minus K did not discriminate probable
measure designed to detect response inconsis- malingerers from subjects with TBI and from
tency as the reason for significant elevations at a T those with persistent PCS despite the presence of
score of 100 or more on F or 90 or more on Fb. A significant group differences on several measures
second consistency score, True Response of neuropsychological malingering, including the
Inconsistency scale (TRIN), measures the ten- Rey 15-Item Test, Recognition Word List, AVLT
dency to respond either “True” or “False” in an Recognition score, RDS, and PDRT-27.
Assessment of Malingering 133

Lees-Haley and colleagues (Lees-Haley et al., Butcher, 1991). In addition, the sample of probable
1991; Lees-Haley, 1992) recognized a different malingerers produced scores on Scale 3 that excee-
pattern of symptom reporting in personal-injury ded those produced by noninjured persons simulat-
litigants and developed a scale that would be sen- ing somatoform disorder on the MMPI-2 (Sivec,
sitive to personal-injury exaggeration, the FBS. Lynn, & Garske, 1994). These data were used to pro-
The FBS was constructed on a rational content pose a somatic malingering profile on the MMPI-2,
basis, taking into consideration unpublished fre- characterized by FBS scores of ≥24 for males and
quency counts of malingerer’s MMPI-2 test data ≥26 for females, with T scores of ≥80 on Scales Hs
and responses that fit a model of goal-directed and Hy (conservative) or ≥80 on Hy (liberal).
behavior with a focus of (a) appearing honest; In a subsequent investigation, I (Larrabee,
(b) appearing psychologically normal except 2003c) found that the FBS was significantly more
for the influence of the alleged cause of injury; sensitive to detection of symptom exaggeration
(c) avoiding admitting preexisting psychopathol- than F, Fb, or F(p) in 33 litigants with definite or
ogy; (d) attempting to minimize the impact of probable MND. Moreover, the MMPI-2 profiles
previously disclosed preexisting complaints; of the definite/probable malingerers were charac-
(e) minimizing or hiding pre-injury antisocial or terized by significantly higher elevations on Scales
illegal behavior; and (f) presenting a degree of 1, 3, and 7 than scores produced by a variety of
injury or disability within perceived limits of clinical groups, including those with nonlitigating
plausibility. The FBS contains 18 items scored in severe CHI, multiple sclerosis, spinal cord injury,
the “True” direction and 25 items scored in the chronic pain, and depression. The definite and
“False” direction. probable malingerers also produced significantly
Greene (1997) presented data to show that the higher elevations on Scales 2 and 8 than were pro-
FBS did not correlate strongly with F (0.14), Fb duced by all clinical groups, with the exception of
(0.26), or F(p) (0.08), in contrast to significant the depressed clinical patient group. I (Larrabee,
correlations between F, Fb, and F(p) that averaged 2003c) contrasted the neuropsychological malin-
.75 for normal subjects and patients with mental gering profile obtained, with elevations on the
disorders. Most of the FBS items occurred on FBS and Scales 1, 2, 3, 7, and 8, with the Graham,
MMPI-2 Scales 1 and 3, with six on Scale 1, seven on Watts, and Timbrook (1991) profile for simulated
Scale 3, and seven appearing on Scales 1 and 3. One psychiatric disturbance, which was characterized
FBS item appeared on L, one on K, four on F, four on by significantly elevated F and significant eleva-
Scale 2, two on Scale 4, four on Scale 6, three on tions on Scales 6 and 8. These data were inter-
Scale 7, six on Scale 8, and three on Scale 0 (note that preted as showing at least two patterns of
there is some item overlap on more than one scale). malingering on the MMPI-2: malingered injury
Lees-Haley et al. (1991), using an FBS cutoff and malingered severe psychiatric disturbance.
of 20 or higher, found that 24 of 25 (96%) of per- Miller and Donders (2001) found that litigat-
sonal injury claimants assessed as malingering ing patients with mild TBI produced higher FBS
emotional distress were correctly classified, with scores than nonlitigating patients with mild TBI
18 of 20 (90%) claimants assessed as presenting and were twice as likely to produce FBS scores
genuine injuries correctly classified. Subsequently, that fell beyond 23 for males and 25 for females.
Lees-Haley (1992) found sensitivity and specific- The finding that both the litigating and nonlitigat-
ity values of 75% and 96%, respectively, using an ing individuals with mild TBI produced more sig-
FBS cutoff of 24 or higher for males and 74% nificant elevations on the FBS than nonlitigating
and 92%, respectively, for a cutoff of 26 or higher patients with moderate-to-severe TBI led Miller
for females, for discriminating pseudo-post- and Donders to recommend caution in using the
traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) claimants from FBS solely as an indicator of symptom exaggera-
claimants with legitimate emotional distress. tion. The authors recommended that elevated FBS
I (Larrabee, 1998) found that 11 of 12 litigants scores be interpreted as suggesting the likelihood
with independent objective evidence of malinger- that something other than acquired cerebral
ing on tasks such as the Rey 15-Item Test and dysfunction was accounting for maintaining
PDRT produced elevated scores (≥24 for males, the patient’s symptomatology. In this vein, they
≥26 for females), whereas only 3 had elevated recommended that elevated FBS scores be supple-
F scales (T > 69 for the MMPI, T > 64 for the mented with other data demonstrating invalid
MMPI-2), despite T scores on Scales 1 and 3 that performance before concluding that a person is
exceeded scores for chronic pain samples (Keller & malingering.
134 forensic neuropsychology

Martens, Donders, and Millis (2001) repli- authors were able to discriminate litigating
cated the Miller and Donders (2001) results, dem- patients with chronic pain with cognitive com-
onstrating higher FBS scores for litigating subjects plaints from nonlitigating patients with chronic
with mild TBI in comparison to subjects with pain with cognitive complaints using their weig-
moderate or severe TBI. They also showed a sig- hted index. Moreover, the empirically derived
nificant association between the FBS and mea- cutoff score that had 100% specificity (i.e., identi-
sures of invalid effort derived from the CVLT. fied none of the nonlitigating patients with
Prior psychiatric history was associated with ele- chronic pain) identified 36% of the litigating
vations on the FBS in subjects with mild TBI, patients with chronic pain and 86% of noninjured
including those who were not litigating, indicat- persons attempting to simulate chronic pain
ing that invalid response set alone was not the sole impairment. FBS scores greater than 24 identified
explanation of elevated FBS scores. Not a single 42% of the litigating patients with chronic pain,
patient with moderate or severe TBI produced whereas only 16% of the nonlitigants scored in
invalid scores on both the FBS and CVLT, rein- this range. No nonlitigating patient with chronic
forcing the suggestion that use of multiple inde- pain scored over 29 on the FBS.
pendent criteria to determine the presence of Aguerrevere, Greve, Bianchini, and Meyers
invalid response set improved diagnostic accu- (2008) developed a revised version of the Meyers
racy for malingering (Slick et al., 1999; Sweet, Index for the MMPI-2 that omitted both the
1999). Obvious minus Subtle score and the Dissimula-
Greiffenstein, Baker, Gola, Donders, and tion Scale-Revised, since these two scales are no
Miller (2002) compared groups of litigating longer provided in the commercially available
patients with mild TBI with atypical symptom scoring program from Pearson. Litigants claiming
history/outcome, litigating individuals with mod- TBI classified with probable malingered neu-
erate-to-severe TBI, and nonlitigating individuals rocognitive dysfunction were discriminated from
with moderate-to-severe TBI. Setting specificity nonmalingering TBI subjects equally well by the
at 80% for the litigating individuals with moder- original Meyers Index (receiver operating charac-
ate-to-severe TBI resulted in an FBS cutting score teristic [ROC] area under curve [AUC] of .780)
above 23 and identified 57% of atypical litigants and by the abbreviated Meyers Index (ROC AUC
with mild TBI, but only 4% of nonlitigating indi- = 0.781). Similarly, litigants with chronic pain
viduals with moderate-to-severe TBI. Although classified with malingered pain-related disability
Greiffenstein et al. (2002) could not exclude litiga- were discriminated from nonmalingering chronic
tion status as a potential factor in FBS elevation in pain subjects equally well by the original Meyers
their litigating group with moderate-to-severe Index (AUC = 0.923) and the abbreviated Meyers
TBI, there was some evidence that FBS scores Index (AUC = 0.923). The reader should note that
were related to neurological abnormalities in the AUC values of .9 or greater provide outstanding
litigating patients with moderate-to-severe TBI discrimination (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 2000).
(e.g., the FBS correlated positively with the pres- I (Larrabee, 2003b) found that the FBS was
ence of anosmia and residual motor impairment). superior to F, Fb, F(p), Meyers’s Weighted Validity
Of interest, the FBS correlated significantly with Index, F minus K, Dissimulation Scale–Revised,
three measures of invalid neuropsychological test Obvious–Subtle difference, and Es in discriminat-
performance, including the PDRT-27, Rey 15-Item ing 26 litigants with definite MND (worse than
Test, and Rey’s Word Recognition List, in the liti- chance on the PDRT) from 29 patients with mod-
gating subjects with atypical mild TBI, but the erate or severe TBI. In a combined sample of defi-
FBS did not correlate with these measures in nite and probable MND plus the TBI subjects, the
the litigating group with moderate-to-severe TBI. FBS was the only MMPI-2 validity scale that cor-
The FBS also correlated significantly with a related significantly with the PDRT. The FBS also
Symptom Improbability Rating Scale score within correlated significantly with all other MMPI-2
the litigating sample with atypical mild TBI. validity scales except for F(p) and F minus K, but
Meyers, Millis, and Volkert (2002) developed correlated most strongly with Es and Meyers’s
a composite, weighted validity index for the Index. The FBS correlated significantly with
MMPI-2 based on the T score for F, the raw MMPI-2 clinical scales 1, 2, 3, 6, 7, 8, and 0, with
F-minus-K difference, F(p), Dissimulation Scale– the strongest correlations occurring with 1, 2, 3,
Revised, Es (Ego Strength), sum difference of and 7. An FBS cutoff score above 20 or 21 pro-
Obvious and Subtle scores, and the FBS. These vided optimal classification of the malingering
Assessment of Malingering 135

and head-injured groups, with a sensitivity of .808 was set at 95%. At a malingering base rate of .40,
and specificity of .862. None of the subjects with positive predictive powers (i.e., probability of
moderate-to-severe TBI scored higher than 30 on malingering) were over .80 for F (0.83), Fb (.89),
the FBS, with only 3% (one subject) scoring higher FBS (0.88), DS-r (0.90), Meyers Index (0.84), K
than 25. I concluded that the data were consistent (0.80), and ES (0.93). The reader should note that
with the presence of different dimensions of lower T score cutoffs (>80) were used for F and Fb
exaggeration on the MMPI-2, extending the find- based on malingering versus nonmalingering
ings in my (Larrabee, 2003c) investigation. These comparisons in this study, compared to what is
data were seen as consistent with the demonstra- typically recommended in the test manual (e.g.,
tion by Lanyon (2001) of MMPI-2 dimensions 100 for F, 110 for Fb; Butcher et al., 2001).
of (a) exaggerated psychiatric symptoms and (b) Bianchini, Etherton, Greve, Heinly, and Meyers
exaggerated health concerns in a mixed forensic (2008) also found that a variety of MMPI-2 valid-
sample (child custody evaluees, personal injury ity scales differentiated litigants with malingered
litigants, and criminal defendants; note that a third pain-related disability from chronic pain patients
dimension not relevant to the present discussion without evidence of malingering, as did the Hs
was also obtained: exaggeration of virtue). and Hy scales of the MMPI-2. FBS demonstrated
Ross, Millis, Krukowski, Putnam, and Adams the largest effect size for discriminating between
(2004) reported a sensitivity of .90 and specificity nonmalingering chronic pain and those with defi-
of .90 using an FBS cutoff of 21 or higher or 22 or nite malingered pain-related disability, d = 2.4,
higher to discriminate 59 probable malingerers followed by the Meyers Index, d = 2.2, and
from 59 nonlitigating patients with moderate and Fb, d = 2.1. Consistent with the findings of other
severe TBI. None of their nonlitigating subjects investigators, FBS raw scores ≥30 were associated
with moderate-to-severe TBI scored higher than with essentially zero false positives.
26 on the FBS (100% specificity). The sensitivity and specificity of the FBS
In an investigation that included the subjects decline when investigated in psychiatric settings.
from my studies (Larrabee, 1998, 2003b, 2003c) as Rogers, Sewell, and Ustad (1995) found that F, Fb,
well as additional clinical and malingering sub- F minus K, and F(p) were superior to the FBS in
jects, the FBS was the single most sensitive mea- correct identification of psychiatric outpatients
sure for discriminating definite MND from taking the MMPI-2 under honest or simulated
moderate and severe TBI compared with other malingering conditions. In the honest condition,
measures of motivational impairment derived the FBS had a 21.1% false-positive rate, with only
from Benton VFD, FT, RDS, and Wisconsin Card a 48.5% sensitivity in the malingered condition.
Sorting FMS (Larrabee, 2003a). The FBS remained Rogers et al. (1995) commented that their results
the most frequently failed validity indicator in a did not necessarily demonstrate the invalidity of
cross-validation discriminating litigants with prob- the FBS, which they observed may be more sensi-
able MND from groups of nonlitigating psychiat- tive in context-specific (e.g., personal injury) or
ric and neurological patients (also see Table 5.1). diagnosis-specific (e.g., PTSD) circumstances.
The FBS has also been shown by others to be Correlations of the FBS with other measures
sensitive to the presence of symptom exaggera- of symptom validity support the construct valid-
tion in mixed personal injury samples. Posthuma ity of the FBS. Slick et al. (1996) reported more
and Harper (1998) found the FBS was elevated in significant correlations of the FBS with the
a sample of personal injury litigants contrasted Victoria Symptom Validity Scale than were found
with FBS scores produced by a child custody liti- for the F scale. Martens et al. (2001) reported a
gant sample. Tshushima and Tshushima (2001) significant association between the FBS and per-
found that only the FBS significantly discrimi- formance invalidity measures developed from the
nated a sample of personal injury litigants from a California Verbal Learning Test. Greiffenstein
sample of clinical patients, and the FBS also had et al. (2002) found significant associations between
the largest effect size contrasting the personal the FBS and a Symptom Improbability Rating
injury group with a group of job applicants under- Scale, Grip Strength, Finger Tapping Speed,
going employment screening. Greve, Bianchini, PDRT-27, Rey 15-item Test, and Rey Word List in
Love, Brennan, and Heinly (2006) found that a a sample of probable malingerers showing atypi-
variety of MMPI-2 validity scales worked well at cal outcome for mild TBI. I reported a significant
detecting malingering in TBI, including FBS, Fb, association between the FBS and the PDRT
DS-r, the Meyers Index, and ES, when specificity (Larrabee, 2003b), and the FBS was frequently
136 forensic neuropsychology

associated with other performance invalidity The Butcher et al. (2003) article actually con-
measures derived from standard clinical tests tained data supporting the construct validity of
including Benton Visual Form Discrimination, the FBS. These authors performed a content anal-
Finger Tapping, Reliable Digit Span, and the ysis identifying five groups of items: (a) somatic
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test FMS score in sam- symptoms, (b) sleep disturbance, (c) tension or
ples of both definite and probable malingerers stress, (d) low energy/anhedonia, and (e) denial of
(Larrabee, 2003a). The FBS was also associated deviant attitudes or behaviors. Although Butcher
with Reliable Digit Span performance in a sample et al. criticized the FBS as having poor internal
of probable pain malingerers (Larrabee, 2003d). consistency, the median Cronbach α of .62 for all
Two studies have appeared that are critical of six subject samples is quite similar to the Cronbach
the FBS as a measure of symptom exaggeration. α of .64 for males and .63 for females for the F
Butcher, Arbisi, Atlis, and McNulty (2003) con- scale reported in the MMPI-2 manual (Table D-7,
cluded that the FBS had an unacceptably high rate p. 97; Butcher et al., 1989). Of particular interest,
of false-positive identification in clinical groups, the Cronbach α was .85 for the personal injury
overidentified malingering in a personal injury sample, showing that the five content areas of the
sample, and showed poor internal consistency. FBS identified by Butcher et al. are highly interre-
Butcher et al. studied six subject samples; four lated in personal injury litigants compared to
samples were obtained from the National lower values in patients with chronic pain (0.47)
Computer Systems (NCS) database (profiles sent and general medical patients (0.58). Last, Butcher
in by clinicians for NCS scoring/interpretation), et al., in their Table 4, showed that the FBS corre-
including psychiatric inpatients, individuals in a lated most strongly with MMPI-2 Scales 1, 2, 3, 7,
correctional facility, general medical patients, and and 8, which are the MMPI-2 scales most likely to
those with chronic pain. Another sample was be elevated in personal injury probable malinger-
from a large tertiary care Veterans Affairs medical ers (Boone & Lu, 1999; Larrabee, 1998, 2003b,
center, with a sixth sample that included personal 2003c; Ross et al., 2004).
injury litigants. Bury and Bagby (2002) compared the MMPI-2
Butcher et al. (2003) did not report the per- validity scales of a sample of patients diagnosed
centage of subjects involved in compensation or with PTSD to samples of university students
litigation actions in the psychiatric, chronic pain, completing the MMPI-2 under standard instruc-
general medical, or Veterans Affairs samples and tions and under four conditions of exaggeration:
did not report the context in which the MMPI-2s (a) faking PTSD; (b) coached on PTSD symptoms
were conducted in the correctional facility (e.g., only; (c) coached on MMPI-2 validity scales only;
competency to proceed to trial, criminal respon- and (d) coached on both PTSD symptoms as well
sibility, consideration for early release). Moreover, as MMPI-2 validity scales. These authors found
Butcher et al. did not report results on measures that the family of F scales, particularly Fb and
of exaggeration and symptom validity that were F(p), consistently produced the highest overall
independent of the MMPI-2 for their correctional classification rates and relatively stable estimates
facility or personal injury samples. Hence, speci- of positive and negative predictive power (NPP)
ficity values based on true false positives cannot across the different malingering conditions. In
be computed; sensitivity values, absent indepen- contrast, the authors found that the FBS was
dent assessment of malingering in the correctional ineffective and failed to produce significant group
facility and personal injury samples, also cannot differences between the PTSD claimants and
be computed. At best, elevated scores in the per- the research participants in each of the fake PTSD
sonal injury sample can serve as base rate indica- conditions.
tions of the frequency of malingering, assuming The Bury and Bagby (2002) article suffers,
the FBS is a valid indicator of symptom exaggera- however, from a “fatal” research design error:
tion. Considered in this light, the rates of exag- 100% of their clinical PTSD sample was seeking
geration of 24.1% for males (FBS > 23) and 37.9% continuation or reinstatement of compensation
for females (FBS > 25) reported by Butcher et al. from the Workplace Safety and Insurance Board
(2003) are well within previously reported base of Toronto, Ontario, Canada. In addition, Bury
rates of malingering in neuropsychological per- and Bagby did not assess their clinical sample for
sonal injury evaluations (59%, Greiffenstein et al., evidence of symptom exaggeration/test perfor-
1994; 42%, Grote et al., 2000; 49%, Meyers & mance invalidity on measures that were indepen-
Volbrecht, 1998). dent of the MMPI-2. This is a critical design error
Assessment of Malingering 137

that invalidates their conclusions. Indeed, Bury Greiffenstein, Baker, Peck, Axelrod, and
and Bagby stated: “In the context of these incen- Gervais (2004) found that the FBS had good sensi-
tives, symptom exaggeration is expected, and the tivity, specificity, and positive predictive power
comparatively low classification rates may be a (PPP) in discriminating 48 nonlitigating patients
result of the presence of individuals in the work- suffering psychological trauma following events
place PTSD comparison sample who were actu- such as completed rape or serious injury (mutila-
ally exaggerating or malingering their condition” tion/traumatic amputation) from 57 litigants with
(pp. 482–483). atypical symptom report seeking compensation for
The authors (Bury & Bagby, 2002) went on to psychological damages following relatively minor
state that there was evidence in their clinical PTSD events (e.g., minor frights not meeting DSM-IV
sample that some of the claimants were likely “gatekeeper” criteria for major trauma). In con-
exaggerating their symptoms. Hence, the FBS may trast, the F family (F, Fp, and F minus K) showed
well have been a poor discriminator of the clinical poor discriminant utility. Logistic regression
PTSD sample because a sizable proportion of the yielded optimal FBS cutting scores of 21 for males
sample was malingering. This interpretation is and 26 for females. FBS scores greater than 30 for
supported by review of Table 1 on page 476 of the females and greater than 29 for males were associ-
Bury and Bagby article, which shows a mean FBS ated with 100% positive predictive power for detec-
of 26.31 for their clinical PTSD sample, a value tion of implausible psychological trauma claims.
that is well within the range of mean FBS scores There have been two meta-analyses published
produced by analogue malingerers and suspected on the use of the FBS in civil forensic practice
malingerers (Lees-Haley, Iverson, Lange, Fox, & (Nelson, Hoelzle, Sweet, Arbisi, & Demakis, 2010;
Allen, 2002; see Table 5.3). Nelson, Sweet, & Demakis, 2006). Nelson et al.

TABLE 5.3 FBS SYMPTOM VALIDIT Y SCALE


ENDORSEMENT BY CLINICAL PATIENTS,
SIMULATORS, AND MALINGERERS

Subject Group

Study Clinical Simulators Malingerers


Patients

Lees-Haley et al. (1991)a


M 15.7 25.0 27.6
SD (4.11) (8.5) (4.65)
Lees-Haley (1992)b
M 18.2 — 27.2
SD (5.3) — (5.2)
Larrabee (2003)e
M 15.67 — 26.15
SD (6.02) — (5.41)
Ross et al. (2004)d
M 14.61 — 28.61
SD (4.65) — (5.12)
a
Lees-Haley et al.: 25 Clinical Ss with emotional distress following personal injury;
67 noninjured Ss simulating emotional reaction to injury; 25 Ss malingering after
personal injury
b
Lees-Haley: 64 clinical Ss with emotional distress following injury; 55 Ss malingering
PTSD.
c
Larrabee: 29 clinical patients with moderate/Severe TBI; 26 litigants with definite
malingered neurocognitive dysfunction
d
Ross et al.: 59 Ss with moderate/severe TBI, and 59 litigants with probable malingered
neurocognitive dysfunction.
138 forensic neuropsychology

(2006) reported the largest grand effect size for diseases, and epilepsy. An FBS of ≤22 occurred in
the FBS (0.96) followed by Obvious-Subtle (0.88), 90.3% of these clinical subjects, 95% had FBS of
Dissimulation-Revised-2 (DS-r-2; .79), F–K ≤25, and 99.7% scored ≤29. Greiffenstein et al.
(0.69), and F Scale (0.63). In a subsequent meta- offer detailed, empirically based recommenda-
analysis, Nelson et al. (2010) included an addi- tions for use and interpretation of the FBS, includ-
tional 13 investigations, which when added to the ing joint use with other MMPI-2 validity scales,
original set of 19 analyses from Nelson et al. consideration of a general threshold score of ≥23
(2006), yielded a total of 32 studies, representing (90% specificity), and consideration of a variety
2,218 over-reporting subjects, and 3,123 compari- of moderating variables, including gender and
son subjects. Effect size remained large for the pre-existing psychiatric history, injury severity,
FBS, d = 0.95, exceeded only by the DS-r-2 (d = and medical history. They advise never to use FBS
1.03) and Obvious-Subtle score (d = 1.00). The alone; rather, it should be combined with other
largest FBS effect sizes were in investigations data. Scores of 30 and above were noted to have a
including malingered TBI (d = 1.28), and gender 99–100% posterior probability of indicating pro-
was found to be a significant moderating variable, motion of suffering across all settings, and the
leading the authors to suggest that differential FBS was particularly useful in neuropsychological
cutoff scores be considered for men versus women. settings wherein complaints of somatic dysfunc-
Nelson et al. (2010) concluded that the updated tion, cognitive symptoms, and nonpsychotic emo-
meta-analysis continued to support use of the FBS tional symptoms are common.
as a validity measure, noting that FBS was partic- The FBS was added to the formal scoring
ularly effective when effort was known to be insuf- program for the MMPI-2 in September, 2006,
ficient and assessment occurred in the context of following a review conducted by the University of
traumatic brain injury. Minnesota Press (Ben-Porath, Graham, & Tellegen,
Wygant et al. (2007) have published a unique 2009). Ben-Porath et al. (2009) have prepared a
investigation of the relationship between context monograph detailing the development and
of evaluation, symptom validity testing, and research results for the FBS, now known as the
MMPI-2 validity and clinical scales. These authors Symptom Validity Scale (but still referred to by the
found that the FBS was associated with symptom letters FBS). Ben-Porath et al. also offer interpre-
validity test failure (TOMM and/or WMT) in tive recommendations for the use of the FBS.
both civil and criminal forensic settings. The only Other MMPI-2 invalidity scales have been deve-
significant association of symptom validity test loped subsequent to the FBS, including the Resp-
failure with the F family of MMPI-2 validity scales onse Bias Scale (RBS; Gervais, Ben-Porath, Wygant
was in the criminal forensic setting, in association & Green, 2007), and the Henry-Heilbronner Index
with elevation on Fp. Both the civil litigants and (Henry, Heilbronner, Mittenberg, & Enders, 2006).
criminal defendants elevated RC1 and FBS, but Additionally, a revised version of the MMPI-2, the
only the criminal defendants elevated Fp and RC8. MMPI-2-RF has been published (Ben-Porath &
Wygant et al. concluded that criminal defendants Tellegen, 2008) which contains a new validity
demonstrated a more global pattern of exaggera- scale, Fs, designed to capture somatic complaints
tion and feigned cognitive impairment, whereas that are rarely endorsed in a medical population.
civil litigants restrict exaggeration to somatic The MMPI-2-RF represents a major advance in the
complaints and feigned cognitive impairment. evolution of the MMPI, and has been totally
The extensive publications that have appeared restructured with the goal of creating purer clinical
supporting the validity of the FBS correlate well scales with reduced item overlap, resulting in a
with the results of a survey conducted by Sharland 338-item version of the test with the core clinical
and Gfeller (2007) who found that the FBS was scales comprised of the Restructured Clinical
the third most widely used measure of symptom Scales (also available on the standard MMPI-2).
validity in a poll of NAN professional members Validity scales include revised versions of VRIN
and fellows. Greiffenstein, Fox, and Lees-Haley and TRIN, F, Fb, and Fp, with a new frequency
(2007) provide a comprehensive review of the FBS of endorsement analysis for the revision of F-r.
research, and a summary table showing the fre- The four L scale items that previously occurred on
quency distribution of FBS scores in 1,052 clinical Fp have been dropped. FBS was reduced to those
subjects, including those with moderate and 30 items still remaining on the MMPI-2 RF.
severe TBI, psychiatric patients, medically ill The RBS was developed by using multiple
patients, substance abusers, nontraumatic brain regression analysis to identify 28 MMPI-2 items
Assessment of Malingering 139

that discriminated between non-head-injury dis- Employing logistic regression analysis to predict
ability claimants who passed or failed the WMT, pass/fail SVT performance, RBS did not add to
CARB, TOMM (for over half the claimants), and the discrimination provided by the MMPI-2 over-
Medical Symptom Validity Test (MSVT; Green, reporting scales in the criminal forensic sample,
2004), for a minority of claimants (Gervais et al., but did account for a significant increase in
2007). The RBS correlated significantly with prediction over and above the MMPI-2 over-
the FBS, r = 0.65, F and Fb, r = 0.67, and with Fp, reporting scales in the civil forensic sample.
r = 0.36. The RBS added significant incremental Wygant et al. concluded that the RBS was associ-
variance when entered following the F-family and ated with SVT failure in both criminal and civil
FBS scores, but none of the other response exag- forensic samples.
geration scales added significant incremental vari- Smart et al. (2008) utilized classification tree
ance when the RBS was entered first in regression analysis with Optimal Data Analysis (CTA with
equations that predicted failing performance on ODA), to determine which MMPI-2 validity and
the WMT, TOMM, CARB, or MSVT. Gervais et al. clinical scales discriminated between examinees
found a larger Cohen’s d for the RBS (0.92) than failing SVTs and those passing SVTs, with a
for the FBS (0.61). Cohen’s d was .63 for F, 1.00 sample including subjects evaluated in secondary
for Fb, and .57 for Fp. At an RBS cutoff score of gain contexts (disability claim, personal injury
16 or more, sensitivity was .48 at a specificity of litigation) and those examined in clinical con-
.89, defining the invalid performance group on texts. RBS and Hy were the only MMPI-2 vari-
the basis of failing both the WMT and MSVT. At ables selected by the CTA with ODA analysis,
the score of 17 or greater recommended by Gervais with cutoff scores of <16.5 RBS (raw score) identi-
et al., sensitivity dropped to .41 at a specificity of .95 fying valid SVT performance, as well as RBS
in those failing both the WMT and MSVT. scores > 16.5 but with Hy <79.5. Invalid SVT per-
Nelson, Sweet, and Heilbronner (2007) con- formance was associated with RBS >16.5, and Hy
trasted samples with external incentive versus no > 79.5. When RBS was excluded from the MMPI-2
incentive (differential prevalence design) on sev- scales that were subjected to analysis, Hy replaced
eral MMPI-2 validity scales, including the FBS RBS as the primary node in the hierarchical tree
and an older version of the RBS. The RBS yielded (i.e., most salient variable for discrimination of
the largest effect size (d = 0.65), followed closely pass/fail SVT status), with additional discrimina-
by the FBS (d = .60). The RBS correlated highly tion provided by Pa, Fb, K, and Fp. FBS did not
with the FBS (r = 0.74). By contrast, the effect enter into the final classification model, nor did it
sizes were .22 for F, .01 for Fb, and .05 for Fp. enter into the model based on RBS and Hy. These
Nelson et al. suggested that the RBS and FBS may data support the validity of the RBS, at a cutoff
represent a similar construct of symptom validity score identical to the RBS raw score of ≥17 pro-
and may outperform other MMPI-2 validity scales posed by Gervais et al. (2007) and demonstrate
in discriminating patient groups with and without that Hy also has a major contribution to detection
secondary gain. of invalid SVT performance, consistent with the
Wygant et al. (2010) evaluated the association role Hy played in the somatic malingering profile
of RBS scores with pass/fail performance on the proposed by me (Larrabee, 1998).
WMT and/or TOMM in both criminal and civil Henry et al. (2006) derived the 15-item Henry-
forensic samples. RBS was correlated with most of Heilbronner Index (HHI) from both the 43-item
the validity scales and clinical scales in both FBS and the 17-item Pseudoneurologic Scale
samples, but the correlations were higher with (PNS) of Shaw and Matthews (1965). Using logis-
the F family and RC8 in the criminal compared to tic regression, the authors compared the ability of
the civil forensic sample. Although RBS had a the FBS, PNS, and a combined FBS-PNS scale to
large effect size in discriminating criminal foren- discriminate a group of 45 litigation/compensa-
sic subjects who failed SVTs from those who did tion-seeking examinees, defined as meeting Slick
not, with a d = 1.48, F family scales yielded higher et al. (1999) criteria for either definite or probable
values of d (Fp = 1.65; F = 1.61), with MMPI-2-RF malingered neurocognitive dysfunction from a
F-r and Fp-r yielding effect sizes of 1.48 and 1.46, group of 74 nonlitigation, non-compensation-
respectively. In the civil forensic sample, RBS seeking TBI patients, the majority with mTBI. All
yielded the largest effect size, d = 1.24, though three scales discriminated between the head-
not meaningfully different than the values of FBS injured and probable malingering groups, with
(d = 1.18), Fs (d = 1.14), and FBS-r (d = 1.13). the FBS and the combined FBS-PNS performing
140 forensic neuropsychology

better than the PNS. Further item analysis yielded FBS-r (0.363), HHI (0.270), Fs (0.270), and FBS
15 items that best discriminated the two groups, (0.258). Logistic regression showed that both
with the HHI comprised of nine FBS items, four RBS and FBS (or FBS-r) added significantly to
PNS items, and two items shared by FBS and PNS. discrimination of the malingering and clinical
A score of 8 or higher had a sensitivity of .80 and samples, with no other validity scale adding to
a specificity of .89. A principal components analy- the discrimination provided by RBS and FBS (or
sis of the HHI showed that it comprised one dom- RBS and FBS-r). Overall, these data support the
inant factor, including mostly physical items, validity of the RBS, FBS, FBS-r, and HHI, although
leading Henry et al. to characterize the scale as a the HHI does not appear to add additional useful
“pseudosomatic index,” which they contended information to that provided by RBS and FBS/
may be a purer measure of somatic malingering FBS-r. These results differ from those of Whitney
than the FBS and MMPI-2 scales Hs and Hy, as et al. (2008), likely due to larger sample size in the
identified by me (Larrabee, 1998). Larrabee (2008, February) study, the fact that the
Whitney, Davis, Shepard, & Herman (2008) Larrabee study used a civilian rather than a vet-
evaluated the prediction of failure on the TOMM eran population, and the Larrabee investigation
by the RBS, FBS, F family scales, HHI, and Fptsd compared litigants/clamainants who met Slick
scale (Elhai, Ruggiero, Frueh, Beckham, Gold, & et al. (1999) criteria for either definite or probable
Feldman, 2002). In a VA sample wherein 24 passed malingered neurocognitive dysfunction, whereas
the TOMM and 22 failed, only the RBS, Fb, and the Whitney et al. study discriminated between
HHI significantly discriminated the groups, with subjects who either passed or failed the TOMM.
the RBS and HHI showing large effect sizes of .98 In other words, the Larrabee investigation con-
and .90, respectively. Scores on TOMM trials 1, 2, trasted malingering versus clinical patient samples
and Retention all correlated with the RBS and whereas the Whitney et al. study evaluated pass/
HHI, but not at all with FBS. Multiple regression fail performance on a single SVT.
predicting TOMM pass/failure showed that In summary, there appear to be at least two
no scale added to the prediction of TOMM types of malingering detected by the MMPI-2. In
performance beyond the prediction based upon the first, scores are elevated on F and the F scale
RBS alone. Last, the RBS had an ROC area under derivatives, including Fb, F(p), and F minus K,
curve of .75. Whitney et al. found that an RBS with extreme elevations on MMPI-2 clinical
cutoff score of 19 or higher had a sensitivity Scales 6, 8, and RC8 (Graham, 2006; Greene, 2000;
of .23 and a specificity of .96. The best likelihood Wygant et al., 2007; Wygant et al., 2010). This pat-
ratio, however, was associated with an RBS cutoff tern is consistent with exaggeration of severe psy-
score of 17 or higher, at which sensitivity was chopathology and likely occurs with greater
.50 with specificity of .92. It is noteworthy that frequency in settings in which suffering psychosis
this cut score of 17 matches the optimal cutoff may mitigate external consequences such as con-
score originally published by Gervais et al. (2007), viction of crimes or compulsory military service.
and also reported and recommended by Smart The second type of malingering detected by the
et al. (2008). MMPI-2 is exaggeration of health and physical
I (Larrabee, 2008, February) compared the injury symptoms in the context of personal injury
diagnostic classification of the RBS, FBS, FBS-r, litigation in general, and litigation for neuropsy-
HHI, F, Fp, Fptsd, and Fs in the discrimination of chological claims specifically. This is character-
a group of definite and probable malingerers from ized by elevations on the FBS, RBS, and HHI, as
a group of clinical patients with moderate and well as by elevations on Scales 1, 2, 3, 7, and 8.
severe TBI and various neurologic and psychiatric Last, as with any measure of exaggeration or
diagnoses. FBS (d = 1.99), RBS (d = 1.91, FBS-r (d performance invalidity, diagnostic certainty
= 1.85), and the HHI (d = 1.77) yielded very large increases with the presence of abnormal scores on
effect sizes, as well as large ROC area under curve other independent indicators (Larrabee, 2003b;
values (0.917, .901, .900, and .892, respectively). Martens et al., 2001; Slick et al., 1999; Sweet,
When the various validity scales were predicted 1999). Per the work of Donders and colleagues
by four embedded/derived SVT scores (Benton (Martens et al., 2001; Miller & Donders, 2001),
Visual Form Discrimination, Combined Finger preexisting psychiatric history can be a mitigating
Tapping, Reliable Digit Span, and Wisconsin Card factor in nonlitigating individuals with mild TBI,
Sorting Test Failure-to-Maintain-Set), RBS and per Greiffenstein et al. (2002), presence of
showed the highest R2 value, .436, followed by anosmia or residual motor impairment can be a
Assessment of Malingering 141

mitigating factor in evaluating the FBS scores of or chronic fatigue, 33.51% for pain or somatoform
litigating patients with moderate and severe TBI. disorders, 29.49% for neurotoxic disorders, and
25.63% for electrical injury.
ADDITIONAL DIAGNO STIC The Mittenberg, Patton et al. (2002) and my
C O N S I D E R AT I O N S (Larrabee, 2003a) base rates for malingering of
The diagnosis of malingering in the individual 40% for litigants with mild TBI and Mittenberg,
case, using the Slick et al. (1999) criteria, involves Patton, et al.’s malingering base rate of 33.51% for
relying on psychometric measures of response pain or somatoform disorders are quite consistent
bias. These measures are associated with various with the results of the Carroll, Abrahamse, and
degrees of sensitivity (to the true presence of Vaiana (1995) investigation of the costs of excess
response bias) and specificity (accurate detection medical claims for automobile personal injuries.
of the absence of response bias). The diagnostic Carroll et al. analyzed “soft” (i.e., sprain/strain)
accuracy of response bias procedures in the indi- and “hard” (i.e., fractures) injury claims as a func-
vidual case also depends on the base rate or tion of compensation system per state (e.g., tort
frequency of malingering in the forensic popula- system, dollar no-fault, and verbal no-fault) and
tion. Given information on sensitivity, specificity, found that 35–42% of all medical costs submitted
and base rate, one can compute for individual in support of auto injury claims were excessive.
measures of response bias the PPP, or probability Other studies have provided further support for
that someone with a positive score is truly a base rate of malingering of 40% or more in set-
malingering, and NPP, or probability that some- tings with external incentive. Ardolf, Denney,
one with a negative score truly is not malingering and Houston (2007) found that of those persons
(Baldessarini, Finklestein, & Arana, 1983; also see referred for pre-trial pre-sentencing neuropsy-
chapter 1, this volume). chological evaluation, 21.9% met Slick et al. (1999)
Two articles provide converging support for a criteria for definite malingering, and 32.4% met
40% prevalence of malingering in litigating claim- criteria for probable malingering, for a combined
ants with mild TBI. Mittenberg, Patton, Canyock, definite/probable rate of 54.3%. Van Hout,
and Condit (2002) surveyed members of the Schmand, Wekking, and Deelman (2006) found
American Board of Clinical Neuropsychology that 57.2% of a sample with suspected neurotoxic
who did forensic work and in personal injury liti- injury failed at least one of three SVTs. Greve et al.
gants with mild TBI found base rates of malinger- (2006) reported a malingering prevalence of 40%
ing of 38.5% unadjusted for referral source, (33.3% probable, 6.7% definite) in persons claiming
increasing slightly to 41.24% when adjusted for exposure to occupational and environmental sub-
referral source. Additional information obtained stances. All of these investigations were of subjects
in this survey showed that the board-certified who had known external incentives. The consis-
respondents typically employed several sources of tency of these base rates led my colleagues and I to
information similar to the Slick et al. (1999) crite- propose a new “magical number”: 40% plus or
ria for probable MND, strengthening the likely minus 10, as representative of the base rate of invalid
accuracy of the base rate obtained by Mittenberg, neuropsychological testing in settings with external
Patton, et al. The base rate figures obtained by incentive (Larrabee, Millis, & Meyers, 2009).
Mittenberg, Patton, et al. for litigants with mild The review of SVTs published by Vickery et al.
TBI are quite close to the base rate of malingering (2001), covering data obtained on the DMT,
in mild TBI as I reported (Larrabee, 2003a) based PDRT, Rey 15-Item Test, 21-Item Test, and Dot-
on my review of 11 studies (Binder & Kelly, 1996; Counting Test, showed an average sensitivity of
Frederick et al., 1994; Greiffenstein et al., 1994; 56.0%, average specificity of 95.7%, and average
Grote et al., 2000; Heaton et al., 1978; Meyers & hit rate of 76.8%. These data showed findings
Volbrecht, 1998; Millis, 1992; Millis et al., 1995; characteristic of most SVTs or measures of
Rohling, Green, Allen, & Iverson, 2002; Trueblood response bias: The specificity is set at a high value
& Schmidt, 1993; Youngjohn et al., 1995), which to minimize the occurrence of false-positive
identified 548/1,363 persons (40%) who showed errors, that is, misidentifying someone as a malin-
motivated performance deficit suggestive of gerer who is not truly malingering. The conse-
malingering. Of additional interest, Mittenberg, quence of setting specificity high is that sensitivity
Patton, et al. (2002) also provided adjusted base is low; that is, using the Vickery et al. (2001) data,
rates of malingering for several other alleged con- on average 44% of persons who truly are malin-
ditions, including 38.61% for fibromyalgia (FM) gering go undetected.
142 forensic neuropsychology

Computation of PPP and NPP, using the aver- whereas specificity increased as a function of the
age sensitivity and specificity values reported by number of motivationally impaired scores. Perfect
Vickery et al. (2001) and malingering base rate of specificity was achieved at three or more test
40%, yields a PPP of .897 (proportion of true pos- failures, the same result I reported (Larrabee,
itives to true positives plus false positives) and an 2003a). Subjects with three or more test failures
NPP of .765 (proportion of true negatives to true had 100% PPP for malingering regardless of the
negatives plus false negatives) for a single failed base rate. Persons with two or more test failures
SVT. These data show that the probability of truly had PPP of .952 and NPP of .808 at a malingering
identifying malingering when test scores exceed base rate of 40% in the Vickery et al. investigation.
the cutoff is higher than the probability of These values, obtained with a simulation design,
correctly detecting the absence of malingering. can be compared to the PPP of .913 and NPP of
Consequently, negative findings on SVTs are poor .921 I reported (Larrabee, 2003a) in a known-group
at ruling out the presence of malingering; in design for discriminating definite and probable
contrast, greater confidence can be afforded MND from moderate-to-severe TBI, psychiatric,
positive findings on SVTs (Bianchini, Mathias, & and neurological patients, employing two or more
Greve, 2001; Faust & Ackley, 1998; Slick et al., test failures at a malingering base rate of 40%.
1999). Thus, a clinician doing a forensic assess- Victor, Boone, Serpa, Buehler, and Ziegler
ment does not merely tally the number of passes (2009) reported results that were quite similar to
and failures on SVTs and conclude malingering or my findings (Larrabee, 2003a) and the results of
absence of malingering based on which outcome Vickery et al. (2004). Employing four embedded/
has the greatest number of tallies. derived indicators (derived from the Rey-
Actually, using multiple criteria for assessment Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, Auditory Verbal
of malingering is supported on a statistical basis, Learning Test, Finger Tapping, and Reliable Digit
both for improving sensitivity and for keeping Span) to discriminate probable malingerers from
constant or increasing specificity (Iverson & a mixed neurologic and psychiatric sample, Victor
Franzen, 1996; Larrabee, 2003a; Martens et al., et al. obtained a sensitivity of 83.8% and specificity
2001; Orey, Crager, & Berry, 2000; Vickery et al., of 93.9% for failure of any two indicators, and a
2004). An example of this, using a known-group sensitivity of .514 with a specificity of .985 for
design, is one of my studies (Larrabee, 2003a). failure of any three indicators. These values for
This investigation derived cutoff scores on five failure of any two or failure of any three matched
tests for discriminating litigants with definite quite closely the data from my earlier investiga-
MND from patients who had moderate or severe tion (Larrabee, 2003a) and the results of Vickery
TBI: Benton VFD, FT, RDS, WCST FMS, and the et al. (2004); specifically, the best overall hit rates
Lees-Haley FBS from the MMPI-2. The sensitivity were with two failed indicators, and failure of
and specificity, respectively, of the individual tests three indicators was associated with essentially no
at specific cutoff scores were .48 and .931 for VFD, false positives (no cases in my investigation or that
.40 and .935 for FT, .50 and .935 for RDS, .48 and of Vickery et al., 2004; one case in Victor et al.,
.871 for FMS, and .808 and .862 for FBS. Evaluating 2009). Moreover, Victor et al. also found that the
all possible pairwise combinations of scores hit rates for two failed indicators matched the hit
exceeding cutoff (e.g., FT and RDS; FMS and FBS; rates when all four indicators were entered as con-
etc.) resulted in a sensitivity of .875 and specificity tinuous variables in a logistic regression, the same
of .889. Evaluating all possible three-way combi- as I reported for failure of any two indicators com-
nations reduced sensitivity to .542 (still higher pared to a logistic regression based on the five
than the individual sensitivities for VFD, FT, RDS, indicators in my investigation (Larrabee, 2003a).
and FMS), but resulted in 100% specificity (i.e., no My finding of improved sensitivity and con-
false-positive scores), a value better than that stant or improved specificity (Larrabee, 2003a) by
achieved by any individual test. aggregating scores across multiple measures of
Vickery et al. (2004) reported similar results in malingering is understood as a function of the
a simulation design in which noninjured persons rarity of test scores exceeding malingering cutoffs
and persons with moderate-to-severe TBI com- for truly impaired patients in combination with
pleted testing under conditions of standard as well regression to the mean. My (Larrabee, 2003a)
as malingering instructions. Sensitivity dropped investigation included 27 subjects with moderate
as a function of the number of scores falling in or severe TBI, 13 nonlitigating patients with vari-
the range of motivational impairment increased, ous disorders affecting the central nervous system,
Assessment of Malingering 143

and 14 patients with psychiatric disorders, pri- particular cutoff score by the false positive rate
marily major depressive disorder, yielding a total that is associated with that same cutoff score. For
of 54 clinical patients. The performance of these example, to use values typical of most SVTs which
54 patients on VFD, FT, RDS, FMS, and FBS was show sensitivities of .50 at specificities of .90
analyzed for this chapter to determine mean per- (Vickery, et al. 2001), a sensitivity of .50 is divided
formance and standard deviations for this mixed by the false alarm rate of .10 (1–specificity, or
clinical sample. Then, z scores were determined, 1–.90) to obtain a likelihood ratio of 5.0. One can
relative to the clinical sample, for scores on the then premultiply this likelihood ratio by the base
five malingering indicators beyond the empirical rate odds of malingering, to obtain the posttest
cutoffs for malingering. The average z score for odds of malingering. Per data cited in this chapter,
these cutoffs, collapsed across all five malingering a value of .40 is a widely replicated base rate of
indicators, was 1.57 SD beyond the clinical group malingering. This is converted to base rate odds
mean (showing an average false-positive rate per by the formula odds ÷ 1–odds, or .40 ÷ 1–.40 or
test of 5.8%). The average intercorrelation (using .67. Premultiplying the likelihood ratio of 5.0 by
Fisher’s z transformation) of the five malingering the base rate odds of .67 yields a posttest odds of
indicators was .175. Thus, based on regression to 3.35. These posttest odds can then be used as the
the mean and the average z score associated with new odds of malingering, which can be used to
the malingering cutoffs, the predicted value of premultiply the likelihood ratio based on a second
performance on any given test given a perfor- independent SVT. Assuming that the second SVT
mance at a z score of 1.57 on another malingering has the same operating characteristics as the first
indicator was (0.175)(1.57) or a z score of .274, a one (i.e., sensitivity of .50 at specificity of .90), the
value substantially below the average z score of new posttest odds after applying this second, inde-
1.57 associated with performance invalidity. Thus, pendent SVT are 3.35 × 5.0 or 16.75. Applying a
although a clinical patient may on rare occasion third, independent SVT with the same operating
(5.8%) obtain a score beyond the cutoff for malin- characteristics gives posttest odds of 16.75 × 5.0 or
gering on any one of the five malingering indica- 83.75. At any point in this chain of likelihood
tors I identified (Larrabee, 2003a), it is not likely ratios, the posttest odds can be converted back to
that two scores will exceed the cutoff. probabilities of malingering by the formula odds
Boone and Lu (2003) made a similar point ÷ odds + 1; for example, after applying one SVT,
related to the probability of multiple scores exceed- the probability of malingering is 3.35 ÷ 4.35 or .77;
ing cutoff for malingering in nonmalingering clini- after two failed SVTs, the probability of malin-
cal patients. They observed that specificity is gering is 16.75 ÷ 17.75 or .94; after three failed
typically set at .90 to keep false-positive identifica- SVTs, the probability of malingering is 83.75 ÷
tion at a minimum. Assuming the tests are not 84.75 or .99. The reader will notice that the values
strongly correlated (see the above discussion), for failure of two and failure of three SVTs closely
Boone and Lu noted that the overall false-positive approximate the values reported by me (Larrabee,
rate for six “failed” effort measures could be as low 2003a), Vickery et al. (2004), and Victor et al.
as 1 in 1 million (0.1 × 0.1 × 0.1 × 0.1 × 0.1 × 0.1). (2009), based on the actual empirical data.
Applying this reasoning to the Slick et al. (1999) Moreover, chaining of likelihood ratios shows
criteria for probable response bias, the probability that even at low base rates of malingering, for
of a false-positive score for two tests exceeding example, .10, multiple SVT failure leads to poste-
cutoff for malingering, each with a specificity of .90 rior probabilities over .90. In my paper (Larrabee,
with no correlation between the two tests in a non- 2008b), failure of one SVT with a sensitivity of
litigating clinical sample, is (0.1 × 0.1) or .01. Boone .50 and specificity of .90 yields a posterior proba-
and Lu (2003) conclude that rather than inflating bility of malingering of .36 at a base rate of malin-
(false positive) error, administering several inde- gering of .10, as might be expected in a low
pendent and well-validated effort techniques serves base-rate condition, even using an indicator with
to substantially increase diagnostic accuracy. a low false-positive rate of .10. Failure of a second
The finding of increased probability of malin- independent SVT with the same operating char-
gering as a function of multiple failed SVTs is acteristics (sensitivity of .50, specificity of .90) at a
supported by the methodology of chaining of like- base rate of .10 now yields a posterior probability
lihood ratios (Grimes & Schulz, 2005; also see of malingering of .74. Failure of a third indepen-
Chapter 1, this volume). The positive likelihood dent SVT at a base rate of .10 now yields a poste-
ratio is obtained by dividing the sensitivity at a rior probability of .93.
144 forensic neuropsychology

It is important to note that all of the computa- Heinly, Greve, Bianchini, Love, & Brennan, 2005;
tions discussed in the preceding two paragraphs Larrabee, 2003a) yielded a mean RDS of 7.11
assume independent SVTs, producing cutoff and standard deviation of 2.47, which was not
scores that are each associated with sensitivities of significantly different from the pooled simulator
.50 and specificities of .90. These values will performance. Data are also available regarding
change as a function of differing likelihood ratios exaggerated symptom report on the MMPI-2 FBS.
and base rates, and the probabilities generated by Pooled FBS data on 72 simulators (Bianchini,
chaining of likelihood ratios will be inflated if the Etherton, Greve, Heinly, & Meyers, 2008; Dearth,
SVTs are correlated rather than independent Berry, Vickery, Vagnini, Baser, et al., 2005) yielded
(Grimes & Schulz, 2005). Inflated probabilities as an FBS mean of 25.7, with a standard deviation
a function of correlated SVTs did not occur when of 6.4. This was actually significantly lower
I analyzed my own data by direct empirical meth- (p <0.01) than pooled data on 72 definite malin-
ods (which would allow for SVT intercorrelation) gerers (Bianchini et al., 2008; Greve, Bianchini,
versus using chaining of likelihood ratios Love et al., 2006; Larrabee, 2003b), yielding a
(Larrabee, 2008b). This was because the SVT mean of 28.5 and standard deviation of 4.66.
intercorrelation averaged .175, as reported earlier Hence, the definite malingerers showed more
in this section of the current chapter. exaggeration than a noninjured group of persons
The accumulating empirical evidence subse- intentionally producing injury-associated symp-
quent to the seminal article by Slick et al. (1999) toms on the MMPI-2.
provides statistical support for these criteria, in My colleagues and I (Larrabee et al., 2007)
particular, the criteria for definite and probable next explored the similarity of performance of
malingering. My colleagues and I (Larrabee, litigants/claimants meeting criteria for probable
Greiffenstein, Greve, & Bianchini, 2007), demon- malingering to that of litigants/claimants meeting
strated that significantly worse-than-chance per- criteria for definite malingering. Heinly et al.
formance showed definite intent to perform (2005) found no differences on Digit Span-based
poorly, and that groups defined by the Slick et al. scores, including Reliable Digit Span and the
criteria for probable malingering did not differ Digit Span scaled score in comparisons of the
from those defined by criteria for definite malin- performance of probable versus definite malin-
gering (other than by the presence of worse-than- gerers. Similarly, Greve, Bianchini, Love, et al.
chance performance in the definite malingering (2006) found no differences in a number of differ-
samples). To show that intent underlies worse- ent MMPI-2 validity scales, including the FBS
than-chance performance, we compared the (e.g., mean FBS was 26.9, SD = 6.4 for 31 probable
performance of groups of litigants/claimants malingerers, vs. mean FBS of 29.8, SD = 3.9 for 14
with worse-than-chance performance to that of definite malingerers). I (Larrabee, 2003a) found
noninjured persons simulating impairment. In no differences in test scores contrasting the
making this comparison, the simulators are performance of 17 probable malingerers and
known to be intentionally manipulating their test 24 definite malingerers on Benton VFD, com-
performance because they have been instructed bined (dominant and nondominant hand) Finger
to do so. Consequently, showing equivalent or Tapping, Reliable Digit Span, Wisconsin Card
worse SVT performance (independent of the Sorting Test Failure-to-Maintain set, and FBS.
original forced choice SVT chosen for group Moreover, I found no significant differences
formation) by litigants with worse-than-chance between the performances of the probable and
performance compared to simulators would definite malingerers on four other sensitive neu-
support intent on the part of the definite malin- ropsychological measures including Controlled
gerers. For example, performance on Reliable Oral Word Association, Trail Making B, Verbal
Digit Span (RDS; one of the best studied SVT Selective Reminding, and the CVMT.
indicators) yielded a pooled mean of 6.89 and In a very unique investigation, Bianchini,
standard deviation of 3.03 for 138 simulators Curtis, and Greve (2006) demonstrated a dose-
(Inman & Berry, 2002; Etherton, Bianchini, Ciota, response relationship between the magnitude of
& Greve, 2005; Strauss, Slick, Levy-Bencheton, potential compensation and failure on tests sensi-
Hunter, MacDonald, et al., 2002). Corresponding tive to malingering. They compared three groups
data for a pooled sample of 77 definite malinger- of patients varying in level of potential compensa-
ers (Etherton, Bianchini, Ciota, Heinly, & Greve, tion: a) a no-incentive group, b) limited incentive
2006; Etherton, Bianchini, Greve, & Heinly 2005; as provided by Louisiana Workers’ Compensation
Assessment of Malingering 145

law, and c) high incentive as provided by federal base rate of malingering at .40 has a posterior
law. Severity of brain injury was also analyzed as a probability of malingering of .94. Additionally,
factor, comparing patients with mild TBI to those per earlier discussion in this chapter of classifica-
with moderate/severe TBS. For every SVT indica- tion statistics, specificities of 1.00 are associated
tor examined, mTBI patients covered by federal with a positive predictive power of 1.00 (true pos-
law showed considerably higher rates of positive itives/true positives + 0 false positives). Per the
findings and diagnosable malingering than results of Vickery et al. (2004), Victor et al. (2009),
patients covered by state law. By contrast, patients and my own work (Larrabee, 2003a), failure of
with moderate/severe TBI showed a general three independent SVTs is essentially associated
incentive effect (i.e., those with external incentive with zero false positives and a 1.00 probability of
failed SVTs at a higher rate than in the no- malingering. Boone (2007b) makes the same con-
incentive group, but this rate did not differ in fed- clusion relating the presence of three failed SVTs
eral vs. state disability settings). Similar findings to the diagnosis of definite noncredible neurocog-
of more pronounced compensation effect in mild nitive performance (note that an important excep-
versus severe TBI have been reported by Binder tion to this rule, as pointed out by Meyers &
and Rohling (1996). Vollbrecht, 2003, is the patient who requires a
In summary, we (Larrabee et al., 2007) 24-hour supervised living setting). Consequently,
contended that the lack of difference between failure of three independent SVTs in the context
definite malingerers and simulators (who are of external incentive and unexplained by neuro-
known to be producing false deficits and exagger- logic, psychiatric, or developmental factors is
ated symptoms) further demonstrates “intent” as diagnostically equivalent to the worse-than-
the motivation for performing significantly chance performance characteristic of definite
worse-than-chance. Second, the lack of differ- malingering. In this regard, the reader should
ences in performance and symptom report when note that failed SVT performance at 100% speci-
comparing probable to definite malingerers (with ficity is not conceptually equivalent to the active
the exception of performance on two-alternative avoidance of the correct response that is associ-
forced choice testing) establishes the validity of ated with significantly worse-than-chance perfor-
the Slick et al. (1999) probable malingering crite- mance on two-alternative forced choice testing.
ria. Last, the dose-response existing between SVT In our review of diagnostic criteria, we also
failure and malingering diagnosis demonstrated recommended that C criteria (subjective com-
by Bianchini et al. (2006), and Binder and col- plaint) be given equal weight (Larrabee et al.,
leagues meta-analytic work on compensation 2007). In the original Slick et al. criteria, evidence
effect sizes in TBI (0.47, Binder & Rohling, 1996) of exaggeration of symptoms or inconsistent or
and chronic pain (0.48, Rohling, Binder, & atypical symptom report alone, on multiple inde-
Langhinrischen-Rohling, 1995) establishes the pendent indicators, could only result in a diagno-
role that external incentive plays in motivating sis of possible malingering. Our recommended
malingering behavior in settings with external change to allow multiple, independent measures
incentive. of symptom exaggeration to define probable
We concluded our review of the criteria for malingering corrects the original asymmetry
malingering by offering several empirically based between criteria for feigned neuropsychological
recommendations (Larrabee et al., 2007). First, test performance and exaggerated symptoms.
given the data showing the effect on posterior Whereas it is the current author’s experience that
probability of diagnosis by aggregating multiple most persons defined as malingering both feign
positive indicators, we suggested that presence of deficit and exaggerate symptoms, there are smaller
any two positive indicators was sufficient for a subgroups who either feign performance or exag-
diagnosis of probable malingering, provided there gerate symptoms, but do not do both. Again, the
was a setting with external incentive and no pri- requirement for symptom report is that there be
mary explanation of poor performance as a func- multiple independent indicators (i.e., multiple
tion of neurologic, psychiatric, or developmental elevated validity indicators on the MMPI-2 would
factors. As shown in my paper on aggregating only count as one independent indicator; evi-
likelihood ratios (Larrabee, 2008b), failure of two dence from independent personality tests or
independent SVTs associated with cut scores symptom rating scales must be evident to count
having sensitivity to malingering of .50 and speci- towards the criteria for multiple independent
ficity of .90, in a setting of external incentive with indicators of symptom exaggeration).
146 forensic neuropsychology

As we observed in our review of the criteria a way as to drive home the point of how atypical
for malingering (Larrabee et al., 2007), the clini- the performance is; for example, when discussing
cian does not need to make an inference of intent; a derived or embedded procedure for which there
rather, intent is demonstrated by the presence of are data obtained on a noninjured simulator
multiple, highly improbable events. The research group, performance can be described as “similar
reviewed in the present chapter, as well as in recent to the performance of noninjured persons
reviews of malingering (Boone, 2007; Larrabee, attempting to feign impairment, who themselves
2007), shows that individual SVT measures, perform differently than patients with severe trau-
whether they be free-standing or embedded/ matic brain injury.” The same can be done with
derived, have cutoff scores that are rare in patients symptom report; for example, in Larrabee (2003c)
with bona fide clinical disorder, either in terms of I present MMPI-2 data for a probable malinger-
extreme levels of performance abnormality, or ing sample, as well as for samples of patients with
atypical nature of performance (e.g., recognition chronic pain, major depressive disorder, severe
worse than recall, or attention worse than TBI not in litigation, multiple sclerosis, and spinal
memory). The additional requirement for the cord injury (the majority of whom were quadri-
presence of multiple positive scores on indepen- plegic). It is compelling when the data for the case
dent SVTs thus results in multiple rare/atypical on which you are testifying produce MMPI-2
performances and/or symptom endorsements. primary clinical scale scores falling above the
Known group/criterion group designs evaluating mean scores for the probable malingering sample,
the effects on diagnostic probability of positive which themselves are higher than those produced
scores on multiple independent SVTs (Larrabee, for the bona fide clinical samples presented in the
2003a, 2008b; Vickery et al. 2004; Victor et al. same article.
2009) statistically support this interpretation of On occasion, a judge may prohibit you
intent; that is, the probability of malingering from any testimony about malingering. In such a
increases from one to two to three positive SVT circumstance, one can still comment on the
findings, such that diagnostic probability is in the validity of the examinee’s neuropsychological
.90 range for two SVT failures and essentially 1.00 examination results. What I have done (also see
for the presence of three indicators. Kaufmann, chapter 3) is testify that neuropsy-
chological evaluation involves interviewing and
TESTIFYING ABOUT testing (including both symptom report measures
MALINGERING such as the MMPI-2, and performance on various
As discussed by Kaufmann (Chapter 3, this tests of abilities, such as memory and intelligence).
volume), testifying about malingering can be The tests we use are valid measures of the abilities
challenging. A key issue to keep in mind is that we are assessing, showing decrements in patients
one cannot comment upon the truthfulness or who have lesions in their brains, and correlation
credibility of the examinee. This includes describ- with external criteria such as the ability to safely
ing symptom exaggeration as lying. How can this drive a motor vehicle. In addition to the validity
be handled? I have found that keeping to the Slick of the test, the validity of a particular patient’s per-
et al. (1999) MND or Bianchini et al. (2005) formance must be evaluated, so that we know we
MPRD definitions is very helpful, for they objec- are obtaining an accurate measure of their actual
tify the definition of malingering and the criteria abilities. I then describe how we determine perfor-
for determining its presence. Describing malin- mance validity, including studies wherein a group
gering as exaggeration and/or fabrication of defi- of simulators (noninjured persons trying to per-
cits in the context of external incentives is form poorly in an attempt to mimic brain injury
preferable to the DSM-IV definition of malinger- or illness) is compared to groups of patients with
ing as “. . .the intentional production of false or objectively documented severe brain injury, or
grossly exaggerated. . .” (p. 739; italics added). severe psychiatric or developmental problems.
When being cross examined on a diagnosis of The simulators end up performing atypically for
malingering, opposing counsel may try to have common neurologic, psychiatric, and develop-
you use the terms “lying,” “false,” or “not telling mental problems, and it is this differing pattern of
the truth.” Avoid these terms, referring back to the performance or symptom endorsement that allows
original definition. determination of the validity of test performance.
When discussing symptom validity results, Examples can be offered such as grossly over-
certain data can be placed into perspective in such reported symptoms far in excess of what is seen in
Assessment of Malingering 147

actual clinical disorders, or patterns of perfor- Bias Scale (RBS, Gervais et al., 2007) used a sample
mance not seen in brain-injured patients, such as that was comprised of over 50% chronic pain and
poor attention with normal memory. These proce- orthopedic injury, with over 50% either referred
dures developed to address validity of perfor- by the Workers Compensation Board or attor-
mance are called symptom validity tests. Additional neys, selecting RBS MMPI-2 items that distin-
studies are done in which litigants with multiple guished examinees passing SVTs from those
SVT failures are compared to nonlitigating clini- failing SVTs. These data provide evidence for SVT
cal groups with bona fide neurological and/or psy- failure in chronic pain cases seen in the context of
chiatric disorders, further establishing valid versus external incentive. Similar data have been pub-
invalid patterns of performance. Testimony can lished by Bianchini, Greve, and colleagues for
then convey that when a particular examinee both symptom exaggeration (Bianchini et al.,
shows multiple symptom validity failures, it is a 2008) and SVT failure (Greve et al., 2008).
strong indication that test performance is invalid. I (Larrabee, 2003d) compared the pain scale
In such circumstances, poor performances are endorsement of 29 litigants with definite or proba-
more likely the result of invalid performance, ble MND who also complained of pain and had
whereas normal-range scores are themselves an FBS scores of at least 22 to published data on patients
underestimate of actual level of ability. with chronic pain for the MPQ (Melzack, 1975;
Mikail, Dubreuil, & D’Eon, 1993); Pain Disability
FUTURE DIRECTIONS Index (Tait, Chibnall, & Krause, 1990); and Modified
The 1990s characterized a veritable explosion in Somatic Perception Questionnaire (MSPQ; Main,
research on malingering, culminating with the 1983). At a specificity of .90 (i.e., a score only
publication of the Slick et al. (1999) criteria for achieved by fewer than 10% of patients with chronic
malingered neurocognitive dysfunction. The area pain, from samples that were not characterized by
of identification of poor effort on specialized tests compensation-seeking versus non-compensation-
of response bias (e.g., PDRT, CARB, WMT, seeking status or screened for malingering), the
TOMM) and identification of atypical perfor- sensitivities were .21 for the MPQ, .59 for the Pain
mance patterns on standard clinical tests (e.g., Disability Index, and .90 for the MSPQ. In particu-
Mittenberg’s work on the WAIS-R/III, WMS-R, lar, the MSPQ showed high sensitivity at high spec-
HRB) is better defined than the area of assessment ificities (sensitivity values were .86 and .69,
of exaggerated symptom report, particularly in respectively, at specificities of .95 and .99).
the area of personal injury litigation. Subsequently, Brasseux, Greve, Gianoli,
It is particularly noteworthy that there is very Soileau, and Bianchini (2008) found a significant
little research on malingering of chronic pain, association between performance on computer-
despite the near total reliance on self-report instru- ized dynamic posturography (CDP) and degree of
ments for assessment of chronic pain. Indeed, symptom endorsement on the MSPQ, further
Turk and Melzack (2001) did not review malinger- supporting the validity of the MSPQ as a measure
ing in the second edition of their Handbook of of somatic symptom exaggeration. Patients who
Pain Assessment. This is particularly important had aphysiologic sway patterns on CDP were
given Mittenberg et al.’s (2002) results showing a more likely to have higher MSPQ scores. Brasseux
base rate of malingering of 38.61% for FM/chronic et al. concluded that patients with both aphysio-
fatigue and 33.51% for pain/somatoform disor- logic CDP and high MSPQ scores were more
ders, values consistent with Meyers et al.’s (2002) likely to be exaggerating their balance-related
data that yielded a base rate for malingering of complaints and deficits.
36% determined from scores produced on their Another area that has seen limited research is
MMPI-2 weighted validity index by a large sample the area of subtypes of malingering. The majority
of litigating chronic pain patients. of specialized tests of response bias follow memory
Gervais et al. (2001) found that 30% of patients paradigms (e.g., PDRT, CARB, WMT, TOMM),
with FM receiving disability failed either the with the exception of the VIP, Dot Counting, and
CARB or WMT, with the failure rate increasing to the b Test. Yet, research investigations contrasting
44% in patients with FM seeking disability. These simulating or known groups of malingerers to
values were contrasted with CARB/WMT failure patients with moderate or severe TBI showed
rates of 0% in rheumatoid arthritis and 4% in atypical performance on measures of perception,
patients with FM not receiving disability. A later motor function, attention, and problem solving
paper reporting on the derivation of the Response (Greiffenstein et al., 1996; Heaton et al., 1978;
148 forensic neuropsychology

Larrabee, 2003a; Mittenberg et al., 1993, 1995, Three investigations employing taxometric
1996). I have identified subtypes of malingering analysis of both symptom report and SVT perfor-
through cluster analysis that showed specific mance have failed to find evidence of a malinger-
impairment on (a) memory, perception, and ing taxon, rather, demonstrating that symptom
motor function; (b) symptom exaggeration; (c) endorsement/SVT performance showed evidence
motor function and symptom exaggeration; (d) of dimensional latent structure. Walters et al.
problem solving; and (e) perception and problem (2008) evaluated the latent structure of the six
solving (Larrabee, 2004a). These data highlight nonoverlapping primary scales of the SIRS, as well
the need for additional measures of malingering as the MMPI-2 F, Fp and Dissimulation (Ds)
of motor dysfunction, impaired perception, and scales. In both criminal and civil forensic samples,
abnormal problem solving. I also identified, the SIRS showed evidence of dimensional rather
through cluster analysis, two basic types of than taxonic structure. Similarly, F, Fp and Ds
MMPI-2 profiles present in a large sample of per- showed evidence of dimensional structure.
sonal injury litigants with MND: subtype varia- Walters, Berry, Lanyon, and Murphy (2009) per-
tions on a somatoform exaggeration profile formed a taxometric analysis of three factor
(characterizing the majority, 88%, of profiles) and scales extracted from the Health Problem Over-
a subtype profile showing exaggeration of severe statement scale in a large (n = 1,050) outpatient
psychopathology (Larrabee, 2004b). mental health sample, and a criminal and civil
Related to the issue of subtypes of malingering (child custody or disability) sample (n = 194). The
is whether or not malingering occurs along a con- taxometric analyses supported a dimensional
tinuum of symptom report and performance on rather than taxonic latent structure. Last, Walters,
measures of ability, as opposed to representing the Berry, Rogers, Payne, and Granacher (2009) inves-
presence of a distinct category of persons who are tigated the latent structure of a civil forensic sample
not just quantitatively different but also qualita- that was administered the TOMM, Letter Memory
tively different from nonmalingering clinical Test, and VSVT. Again, evidence was found for a
examinees. This question has been addressed with dimensional rather than a taxonic latent structure.
taxometric analysis, which allows determination Referring to their earlier work (Walters et al.,
as to whether the data being analyzed varies only 2008), Walters, Berry, Rogers, et al. (2009) noted
along a continuum, referred to as dimensional that both feigned mental disorder and feigned
latent structure, or varies due to the presence of a cognitive disorder appeared to be supported by
true dichotomy between valid and invalid perfor- dimensional constructs, but this did not mean
mance, referred to as taxonic latent structure that these two categories of feigning were sup-
(Meehl & Yonce, 1994; 1996; Ruscio, Haslam, & ported by the same dimension or dimensions.
Ruscio, 2006). Three papers have found evidence They also noted the need for evaluation of a third
for a discrete category or taxon of malingering. domain, that of feigned medical complaints
Strong, Greene, and Schinka (2000) found evi- (a topic addressed by Walters, Berry, Lanyon &
dence of a taxon for the F and Fp scales of the Murphy, 2009, reviewed above). Ascertaining
MMPI-2 in two samples, one from an inpatient whether these three domains fall along a common
psychiatric hospital, and the other from a VA hos- dimension or dimensions requires test data on all
pital that included a wide range of patients, nearly three domains from a large sample of subjects
50% of whom were being evaluated for disability with the incentive to feign in all three domains.
claims. In a follow-up investigation with a crimi- Walters recommended consideration of explor-
nal forensic sample, Strong, Glassmire, Frederick, atory or confirmatory factor analysis to determine
and Greene (2006) found evidence for a taxon the number of dimensions underlying the con-
based on MMPI-2 Fp endorsement. The unique structs and whether different domains of feigning
feature of this investigation was that they utilized and their respective detection strategies share
a sample suspected to contain subgroups of severe common dimensions. Walters, Berry, Rogers et al.
psychopathology resulting in lack of competency (2009) concluded that there is no evidence yet for
in the criminal setting, as well as a subgroup feign- or against a general feigning construct that encom-
ing incompetency. Last, Frazier, Youngstrom, passes all domains and detection strategies.
Naugle, Haggerty, and Busch (2007) found evi- This last observation regarding domains of
dence for a taxon based on a taxometric analysis malingering and detection strategies is particu-
of VSVT performance, in a large, general clinical larly pertinent to the research reviewed in the
sample, without any external incentive. present chapter. There are at least two areas of
Assessment of Malingering 149

symptom over-reporting: over-reporting of severe noted that one of the most pressing questions is
psychiatric symptoms (faking crazy) and over- whether it is possible to tease apart legitimate
reporting of health, cognitive, and injury symp- from exaggerated impairment when both may be
toms (faking hurt), with multiple domains of present.
invalid neuropsychological test performance, I have encountered a case, referred by the
including motor function, attention, memory, and attorneys for the defense, in which the plaintiff
problem-solving skills. The multivariate nature of suffered a severe CHI (admission Glasgow Coma
both symptom over-reporting and invalid test Scale of 3, with 1 month to follow commands and
performance may be why three studies found tax- 3 months of post-traumatic amnesia). On direct
onicity (Frazier et al., 2007; Strong et al., 2000; evaluation, the plaintiff failed the Rey 15-Item
Strong et al., 2006) and three did not (Walters Test with a score of 5, PDRT-27 correct total of
et al., 2008; Walters, Berry, Lanyon, et al., 2009; 48%, and a Lees-Haley FBS of 28. On earlier test-
Walters, Berry, Rogers, et al., 2009): none of these ing with another examiner, the plaintiff performed
investigations evaluated more than one test or one poorly on the “Easy” items of the Category Test
test domain in the taxometric analyses that they and had an RDS score of 7. These poor scores on
conducted. Measures included either scales from measures of effort and malingering precluded
the MMPI-2, SIRS, single SVTs such as the VSVT, confident interpretation of the plaintiff ’s very
or three SVTs from the same test domain: memory poor performance across a variety of tasks. He did
paradigm forced choice procedures (VSVT, appear to have a potentially valid pattern of motor
TOMM, and Letter Memory Test). impairment, with performance declining from
Moreover, patterns of test performance may Grip Strength to FT, to the Purdue and Grooved
vary by the context in which malingering can Pegboards (cf. Greiffenstein et al., 1996). Because
occur. As shown by Wygant et al. (2007), SVT fail- of his poor effort, other conclusions had to be
ure was associated with the FBS and RC1 (mea- based on base rate expectations. Per the results
sures sensitive to exaggerated physical, cognitive, of Dikmen et al. (1994) regarding employment
and nonpsychotic emotional distress) and with Fp following TBI, it was pointed out that only 8% of
and RC8 (measures of exaggerated psychosis) in a patients with TBI who had time-to-follow-
criminal forensic setting, whereas only the FBS commands of 29 days or more had returned to work
and RC1 were associated with SVT failure in a within 2 years, making it unlikely that the plaintiff
civil forensic setting. Collecting a large civil and was going to return to gainful employment.
criminal forensic sample, employing the full range Thus, when poor effort and brain dysfunction
of SVT and self-report validity scales, with multi- coexist, it is possible to reach conclusions based
ple indicators of each domain, would appear to be on published outcome data in patient groups
necessary to truly answer the question of whether sharing similar injury severity characteristics (see
these measures of symptom exaggeration and Dikmen, Machamer, Winn, & Temkin, 1995;
response bias show dimensionality versus taxo- Dikmen et al., 1994; Rohling, Meyers, & Millis,
nicity, a design also recommended by Walters, 2003). Similarly, in the presence of evidence for
Berry, Rogers, et al. (2009). Certainly, the cluster poor effort, scores in the low end of the normal
analyses referenced above suggesting the presence range should be given greater weight because
of subgroups of malingerers (Larrabee, 2004a, b, these low normal scores themselves may well rep-
February), and the different contingencies of pass/ resent an underestimate of actual ability. Perhaps
fail on five different SVTs reported in my earlier another approach to evaluating the presence of
investigation (Larrabee, 2003a), suggest that there legitimate deficit co-occurring with poor motiva-
may well be discrete subgroups of malingerers; if tion can be derived from future research on sub-
discrete subgroups exist, it stands to reason that a types of malingering, such that patterns of various
single primary or multiple primary taxons of cognitive performances that are valid can be dis-
malingering can exist, and these can be demon- criminated from invalid patterns of performance
strated by taxometric analysis. (i.e., legitimate memory impairment accompa-
Since Slick et al.’s (1999) observation that nied by malingered motor function). Bianchini
almost no research exists on the coexistence of et al. (2003) provide additional discussion of these
legitimate neuropsychological dysfunction with issues.
poor effort or malingering, cases have started to Finally, future research on malingering needs
appear in the literature (Bianchini, Greve, & Love, to further analyze clinical and individual differ-
2003; Boone & Lu, 2003; Paniak, 2009). Slick et al. ence variables related to the occurrence of false
150 forensic neuropsychology

positive scores on SVTs; in other words, we need of feigned cognitive impairment. A neuropsychologi-
to determine the features of the Slick et al. (1999) cal perspective (pp. 103–27). New York: Guilford.
D criteria. Boone’s (2007) edited volume contains Baldessarini, R. J., Finklestein, S., & Arana, G. W.
pertinent information in this regard. Features of (1983). The predictive power of diagnostic tests
clinical patients with bona fide impairment who and the effects of prevalence of disease. Archives of
produce positive scores on SVTs typically include General Psychiatry, 40, 569–73.
documented severe brain injury (prolonged coma; Barrash, J., Suhr, J., & Manzel, K. (2004). Detecting
structural lesions in the brain), and/or need poor effort and malingering with an expanded
version of the Auditory Verbal Learning Test
for 24-hour supervised care (Larrabee, 2003a;
(AVLTX): Validation with clinical samples. Journal
Larrabee et al., 2007; Meyers & Volbrecht, 2003).
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26,
125–40.
AC K N OW L E D G M E N T Ben-Porath, Y. S., Graham, J. R., & Tellegen, A.
I acknowledge the assistance of Matthew Miliano (2009). The MMPI-2. Minnesota Multiphasic
in the preparation of this chapter. Personality Inventory-2 Symptom Validity (FBS)
Scale. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
REFERENCES Ben-Porath, Y. S. & Tellegen, A. (2008). MMPI-2-RF
Aguerrevere, L. E., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., & (Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2-RF).
Meyers, J. E. (2008). Detecting malingering in Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
traumatic brain injury and chronic pain with an Benton, A. L., Sivan, A. B., Hamsher, K. deS.,
abbreviated version of the Meyers Index for the Varney, N. R., & Spreen, O. (1994). Contributions
MMPI-2. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, to neuropsychologic assessment: A clinical manual
831–38. (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic Bernard, L. C., McGrath, M. J., & Houston, W. (1996).
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). The differential effects of simulating malingering,
Washington, DC: Author. closed head injury, and other CNS pathology on
Arbisi, P. A. & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (1995). An MMPI-2 infre- the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Support for the
quent response scale for use with psychopathological “pattern of performance” hypothesis. Archives of
populations: The Infrequency Psychopathology Scale, Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 231–45.
F(p). Psychological Assessment, 7, 424–31. Berry, D. T. R., Baer, R., & Harris, M. (1991). Detection
Arbisi, P. A. & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (1998). The ability of of malingering on the MMPI: A meta-analysis.
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 19–45.
validity scales to detect fake-bad responses in Berry, D. T. R. & Schipper, L. J. (2007). Detection of
psychiatric inpatients. Psychological Assessment, feigned psychiatric symptoms during forensic neu-
10, 221–28. ropsychological examinations. In G. J. Larrabee
Arbisi, P. A., Ben-Porath, Y. S., & McNulty, J. (2003). (Ed.). Assessment of malingered neuropsychological
Refinement of the MMPI-2 F(p) scale is not neces- deficits (pp. 226–63). New York: Oxford.
sary: A response to Gass and Luis. Assessment, 10, Berry, D. T. R., Wetter, M. W., Baer, R. A., Youngjohn,
123–28. J. R., Gass, C. S., Lamb, D. G., et al. (1995). Over-
Ardolf, B. R., Denney, R. L., & Houston, C. M. (2007). reporting of closed head injury symptoms on the
Base rates of negative response bias and malingered MMPI-2. Psychological Assessment, 7, 517–23.
neurocognitive dysfunction among criminal defen- Bianchini, K. J., Curtis, K. L., & Greve, K. W. (2006).
dants referred for neuropsychological evaluation. Compensation and malingering in traumatic brain
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 145–59. injury: A dose-response relationship? The Clinical
Arnold, G. & Boone, K. B. (2007). Use of motor and Neuropsychologist, 20, 831–47.
sensory tests as measures of effort. In K. B. Boone Bianchini, K. J., Etherton, J. L., Greve, K. W., Heinly,
(Ed.). Assessment of feigned cognitive impairment. M. T., & Meyers, J. E. (2008). Classification accu-
A neuropsychological approach (pp. 178–209). racy of MMPI-2 validity scales in the detection of
New York: Guilford. pain-related malingering. A known groups study.
Arnold, G., Boone, K. B., Lu, P., Dean, A., Wen, J., Assessment, 15, 435–49.
Nitch, S., et al. (2005). Sensitivity and specificity Bianchini, K. J., Greve, K. W., & Glynn, G. (2005). On
of Finger Tapping Test scores for the detection of the diagnosis of malingered pain-related disability:
suspect effort. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19, Lessons from cognitive malingering research. Spine
105–20. Journal, 5, 404–417.
Babikian, T. & Boone, K. B. (2007). Intelligence tests as Bianchini, K. J., Greve, K. W., & Love, J. M. (2003).
measures of effort. In K. B. Boone (Ed.). Assessment Definite malingered neurocognitive dysfunction in
Assessment of Malingering 151

moderate/severe traumatic brain injury. The Boone, K. B. & Lu, P. H. (1999). Impact of somatoform
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 574–80. symptomatology on credibility of cognitive perfor-
Bianchini, K. J., Mathias, C. W., & Greve, K. W. (2001). mance. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13, 414–419.
Symptom validity testing: A critical review. The Boone, K. B. & Lu, P. H. (2003). Noncredible cognitive
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 19–45. performance in the context of severe brain injury.
Bianchini, K. J., Mathias, C. W., Greve, K. W., Houston, The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 244–54.
R. J., & Crouch, J. A. (2001). Classification accu- Boone, K. B. & Lu, P. H. (2007). Non-forced choice
racy of the Portland Digit Recognition Test in trau- effort measures. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.). Forensic
matic brain injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, neuropsychology: A scientific approach (pp. 27–43).
15, 461–70. New York: Oxford.
Binder, L. M. (1990). Malingering following minor Boone, K. B., Lu, P. H., Back, C., King, C., Lee, A.,
head trauma. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 4, Philpott, L., et al. (2002). Sensitivity and specificity
25–36. of the Rey Dot Counting Test in patients with sus-
Binder, L. M. (1993). Assessment of malingering after pect effort and various clinical samples. Archives of
mild head trauma with the Portland Digit Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, 625–42.
Recognition Test. Journal of Clinical and Experi- Boone, K. B., Lu, P. H., & Herzberg, D. (2002). The b Test
mental Neuropsychology, 15, 170–82. manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Binder, L. M. & Kelly, M. P. (1996). Portland Digit Boone, K. B., Lu, P., Sherman, D., Palmer, B., Back, C.,
Recognition Test performance by brain dysfunc- Shamieh, E., et al. (2000). Validation of a new
tion patients without financial incentives. Assess- technique to detect malingering of cognitive
ment, 3, 403–409. symptoms: The b Test. Archives of Clinical Neuro-
Binder, L. M., Kelly, M. P., Villanueva, M. R., & psychology, 15, 227–41.
Winslow, M. M. (2003). Motivation and neuropsy- Boone, K. B., Lu, P., & Wen, J. (2005). Comparisons of
chological test performance following mild head various RAVLT scores in the detection of noncred-
injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro- ible memory performance. Archives of Clinical
psychology, 25, 420–30. Neuropsychology, 20, 301–19.
Binder, L. M. & Pankratz, L. (1987). Neuropsychological Boone, K. B., Salazar, X., Lu, P., Warner-Chacon, K., &
evidence of a factitious memory complaint. Journal Razani, J. (2002). The Rey 15-Item Recognition
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, Trial: A technique to enhance sensitivity of the Rey
167–71. 15-Item Memorization Test. Journal of Clinical and
Binder, L. M. & Rohling, M. L. (1996). Money matters: Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 561–73.
A meta-analytic review of the effects of financial Brasseux, R., Greve, K. W., Gianoli, G. J., Soileau, J. S.,
incentives on recovery after closed-head injury. & Bianchini, K. J. (2008). The relationship between
American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 7–10. the Modified Somatic Perception Questionnaire
Binder, L. M., Villanueva, M. R., Howieson, D., & and dynamic platform posturography. Otology and
Moore, R. T. (1993). The Rey AVLT recognition Neurotology, 29, 359–62.
memory task measures motivational impairment Bury, A. S. & Bagby, R. M. (2002). The detection of feigned
after mild head trauma. Archives of Clinical uncoached and coached posttraumatic stress disor-
Neuropsychology, 8, 137–47. der with the MMPI-2 in a sample of workplace acci-
Binder, L. M. & Willis, S. C. (1991). Assessment of dent victims. Psychological Assessment, 14, 472–84.
motivation after financially compensable minor Butcher, J. N., Arbisi, P. A., Atlis, M. M., & McNulty,
head trauma. Psychological Assessment: A Journal J. L. (2003). The construct validity of the Lees-Haley
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 3, 175–81. Fake Bad Scale. Does this scale measure somatic
Bolter, J. F., Picano, J. J., & Zych, K. (1985, October). malingering and feigned emotional distress?
Item error frequencies on the Halstead Category Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, 473–85.
Test: An index of performance validity. Paper pre- Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R.,
sented at the annual meeting of the National Tellegen, A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989). Minnesota
Academy of Neuropsychology, Philadelphia, PA. Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI-2).
Boone, K. B. (2007a) (Ed.). Assessment of feigned cogni- Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
tive impairment. A neuropsychological approach. Butcher, J. N., Graham, J. R., Ben-Porath, Y. S., Tellegen,
New York: Guilford. A., & Kaemmer, B. (2001). Minnesota Multiphasic
Boone, K. B. (2007b). A reconsideration of the Slick Personality Inventory (MMPI-2. Revised Ed.).
et al. (1999) criteria for malingered neurocognitive Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
dysfunction (pp. 301–319). In K. B. Boone (Ed.). Butters, M. A., Goldstein, G., Allen, D. N., & Shemansky,
Assessment of feigned cognitive impairment. A neu- W. J. (1998). Neuropsychological similarities and
ropsychological approach. New York: Guilford. differences among Huntington’s disease, multiple
152 forensic neuropsychology

sclerosis, and cortical dementia. Archives of Clinical veterans presenting with combat-related PTSD.
Neuropsychology, 13, 721–30. Journal of Personality Assessment, 79, 531–49.
Carroll, S., Abrahamse, A., & Vaiana, M. (1995). The Etherton, J. L., Bianchini, K. J., Ciota, M. A., & Greve,
costs of excess medical claims for automobile per- K. W. (2005). Reliable Digit Span is unaffected by
sonal injuries. Santa Monica, CA: RAND. laboratory-induced pain. Implications for clinical
Coleman, R. D, Rapport, L. J., Millis, S. R., Ricker, J. H., use. Assessment, 12, 101–106.
& Farchione, T. J. (1998). Effects of coaching on Etherton, J. L., Bianchini, K. J., Ciota, M. A., Heinly,
detection of malingering on the California Verbal M. T., & Greve, K. W. (2006). Pain, malingering
Learning Test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental and the WAIS-III Working Memory Index. Spine
Neuropsychology, 20, 201–210. Journal, 6, 61–71.
Conder, R., Allen, L., and Cox, D. (1992). Computerized Etherton, J. L., Bianchini, K. J., Greve, K. W., & Heinly,
Assessment of Response Bias Test manual. Durham, M. T. (2005). Sensitivity and specificity of reliable
NC: Cognisyst. digit span in malingered pain-related disability.
Corwin, J. & Bylsma, F. W. (1993). Translations and Assessment, 12, 130–36.
commentary on excerpts from Andre’ Rey’s Faust, D. & Ackley, M. A. (1998). Did you think it was
Psychological examination of traumatic encephal- going to be easy? Some methodological suggestions
opathy and P. A. Osterrieth’s The Complex Figure for the investigation and development of malinger-
Copy Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 3–21. ing detection techniques. In C. R. Reynolds (Ed.),
Dearth, C. S., Berry, D. T. R., Vickery, C. D., Vagnini, V. Detection of malingering during head injury litiga-
L., Basser, R. E., Orey, S. A., et al. (2005). Detection tion (pp. 1–54). New York: Plenum Press.
of feigned head-injury symptoms on the MMPI-2 Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Naugle, R. I.,
in head-injured patients and community con- Haggerty, K. A., & Busch, R. M. (2007). The latent
trols. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, structure of cognitive symptom exaggeration on
95–110. the Victoria Symptom Validity Test. Archives of
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 197–211.
(1987). California Verbal Learning Test: Adult Frederick, R. I. (1997). Validity Indicator Profile
version. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp. manual. Minnetonka, MN: NCS Assessments.
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. Frederick, R. I., Crosby, R. D., & Wynkoop, T. F. (2000).
(2000). CVLT-II. California Verbal Learning Test Performance curve classification of invalid respon-
(2nd ed., adult version). San Antonio, TX: Psycho- ding on the Validity Indicator Profile. Archives of
logical Corp. Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 281–300.
Denney, R. L. (1996). Symptom validity testing of Frederick, R. I., Sarfaty, S. D., Johnston, J. D., & Powel,
remote memory in a criminal forensic setting. J. (1994). Validation of a detector of response bias
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 589–603. on a forced-choice test of nonverbal ability. Neuro-
DiCarlo, M. A., Gfeller, J. D., & Oliveri, M. V. (2000). psychology, 8, 118–25.
Effects of coaching on detecting feigned cognitive Gass, C. S. & Luis, C. A. (2001). MMPI-2 scale F(p) and
impairment with the Category Test. Archives of symptom feigning: Scale refinement. Assessment, 8,
Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 399–413. 425–29.
Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., Winn, H. R., & Temkin, Gervais, R. O., Russell, A. S., Green, P., Allen, L. M.,
N. R. (1995). Neuropsychological outcome at Ferrari, R., & Pieschl, S. D. (2001). Effort testing in
1-year post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, patients with fibromyalgia and disability incen-
80–90. tives. The Journal of Rheumatology, 28, 1892–99.
Dikmen, S. S., Temkin, N. R., Machamer, J. E., Gervais, R. O., Ben-Porath, Y. S., Wygant, D. B., &
Holubkov, A. L., Fraser, R. T., & Winn, H. R. (1994). Green, P. (2007). Development and validation of a
Employment following traumatic head injuries. Response Bias scale (RBS) for the MMPI-2.
Archives of Neurology, 51, 177–86. Assessment, 14, 196–208.
Donders, J. & Strong, C-A. (2010, December 21). Glassmire, D. M., Bierley, R. A., Wisniewski, A. M.,
Embedded effort indicators on the California Greene, R. L., Kennedy, J. E., & Date, E. (2003).
Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition (CVLT-II): Using the WMS-III Faces subtest to detect malin-
An attempted cross-validation. The Clinical gered memory impairment. Journal of Clinical and
Neuropsychologist. Retrieved January 5, 2011 from Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 465–81.
ttp://dx.doi.org/1080/13854046.2010.536781 Graham, J. R. (2006). MMPI-2. Assessing personality
Elhai, J. D., Ruggiero, K. J., Frueh, B. C., Beckham, J. C., and psychopathology (4th ed.). New York: Oxford
Gold, P. B., & Feldman, M. E. (2002). The Infrequency- University Press.
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Scale (Fptsd) for the Graham, J. R., Watts, D., & Timbrook, R. E.
MMPI-2: Development and initial validation with (1991). Detecting fake-good and fake-bad MMPI-2
Assessment of Malingering 153

profiles. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, An analysis of the specificity of two methods in
264–77. nonmalingering normal and patient samples.
Green, P. (2004). Green’s Medical Symptom Validity Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro-
Test (MSVT): User’s manual. Edmonton: Green’s psychology, 24, 48–54.
Publishing. Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Black, F. W., Heinly, M.
Green, P., Iverson, G. L., & Allen, L. M. (1999). Detecting T., Love, J. M., Swift, D. A. et al. (2006). The preva-
malingering in head injury litigation with the Word lence of cognitive malingering in persons report-
Memory Test. Brain Injury, 13, 813–819. ing exposure to occupational and environmental
Green, P., Lees-Haley, P. R., & Allen, L. M. (2002). The substances. NeuroToxicology, 27, 940–50.
Word Memory Test and the validity of neuropsy- Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Love, J. M., Brennan, A., &
chological test scores. Journal of Forensic Neuro- Heinly, M. T. (2006). Sensitivity and specificity of
psychology, 2, 97–124. MMPI-2 validity scales and indicators to malingered
Green, P., Rohling, M. L., Lees-Haley, P. R., & Allen, neurocognitive dysfunction in traumatic brain
L. M. (2001). Effort has a greater effect on test injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 491–512.
scores than severe brain injury in compensation Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Mathias, C. W., Houston,
claimants. Brain Injury, 15, 1045–60. R. J., & Crouch, J. A. (2002). Detecting malingered
Greene, R. L. (1997). Assessment of malingering performance with the Wisconsin Card Sorting
and defensiveness by multiscale inventories. In Test: A preliminary investigation in traumatic brain
R. Rogers (Ed.), Clinical assessment of malingering injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16, 179–91.
and deception (2nd ed.) (pp. 169–207). New York: Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., & Roberson, T. (2007).
Guilford Press. The Booklet Category Test and malingering in trau-
Greene, R. L. (2000). The MMPI-2. An interpretive matic brain injury: Classification accuracy in known
manual (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. groups. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21, 318–37.
Greiffenstein, M. F. (2007). Motor, sensory, and Greve, K. W., Binder, L. M., & Bianchini, K. J. (2009).
perceptual-motor pseudoabnormalities. In G. J. Rates of below-chance performance in forced-
Larrabee (Ed.). Assessment of malingered neuropsy- choice symptom validity tests. The Clinical
chological deficits (pp. 100–30). New York: Oxford. Neuropsychologist, 23, 534–44.
Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Gola, T. (1994). Greve, K. W., Heinly, M. T., Bianchini, K. J., & Love, J. M.
Validation of malingered amnesia measures with a (2009). Malingering detection with the Wisconsin
large clinical sample. Psychological Assessment, 6, Card Sorting Test in mild traumatic brain injury.
218–24. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23, 343–62.
Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Gola, T. (1996). Greve, K. W., Ord, J., Curtis, K. L., Bianchini, K. J., &
Motor dysfunction profiles in traumatic brain injury Brennan, A. (2008). Detecting malingering in trau-
and postconcussion syndrome. Journal of the matic brain injury and chronic pain: A comparison
International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 477–85. of three forced choice symptom validity tests. The
Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., Gola, T., Donders, J., Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22, 896–918.
& Miller, L. (2002). The Fake Bad Scale in atypical Greve, K. W., Springer, S., Bianchini, K. J., Black, F. W.,
and severe closed head injury. Journal of Clinical Heinly, M. T., Love, J. M., et al. (2007). Malingering
Psychology, 58, 1591–1600. in toxic exposure. Classification accuracy of
Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., Peck, E., Axelrod, B., Reliable Digit Span and WAIS-III Digit Span scaled
& Gervais, R. (2004). The Fake Bad Scale and scores. Assessment, 14, 12–21.
MMPI-2 F-Family in detection of implausible Griffin, G. A. E., Glassmire, D. M., Henderson, E. A., &
trauma claims. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 18, McCann, C. (1997). Rey II: Redesigning the Rey
573–90. Screening Test of Malingering. Journal of Clinical
Greiffenstein, M. F., Fox, D., & Lees-Haley, P. R. (2007). Psychology, 53, 757–66.
The MMPI-2 Fake Bad Scale in detection of Grimes, D. A. & Schulz, K. F. (2005). Epidemiology 3.
noncredible brain injury claims. In K. B. Boone Refining clinical diagnosis with likelihood ratios.
(Ed.). Assessment of feigned cognitive impairment. Lancet, 365, 1500–1505.
A neuropsychological perspective (pp. 210–35). Grote, C. L., Kooker, E. K., Garron, D. C., Nyenhuis, D.
New York: Guilford. L., Smith, C. A., & Mattingly, M. L. (2000).
Greiffenstein, M. F., Gola, T., & Baker, W. J. (1995). Performance of compensation-seeking and non-
MMPI-2 validity scales versus domain specific compensation-seeking samples on the Victoria
measures in detection of factitious traumatic brain Symptom Validity Test: Cross-validation and
injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 230–40. extension of a standardization study. Journal of
Greve, K. W. & Bianchini, K. J. (2002). Using the Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23,
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to detect malingering: 709–719.
154 forensic neuropsychology

Guilmette, T., Hart, K., & Giuliano, A. (1993). Inman, T. H. & Berry, D. T. R. (2002). Cross-validation
Malingering detection: The use of a forced-choice of indicators of malingering. A comparison of nine
method in identifying organic versus simulated neuropsychological tests, four tests of malingering,
memory impairment. The Clinical Neuropsycholo- and behavioral observations. Archives of Clinical
gist, 7, 59–69. Neuropsychology, 17, 1–23.
Hannay, H. J., Levin, H. S., & Grossman, R. G. (1979). Inman, T. H., Vickery, C. D., Berry, D. T. R., Lamb, D.
Impaired recognition memory after head injury. G., Edwards, C. L., & Smith, G. T. (1998).
Cortex, 15, 269–283. Development and initial validation of a new proce-
Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1983). Minnesota dure for evaluating adequacy of effort given during
Multiphasic Personality Inventory manual. New neuropsychological testing: The Letter Memory
York: Psychological Corp. Test. Psychological Assessment, 10, 128–39.
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & Iverson, G. L. & Binder, L. M. (2000). Detecting exag-
Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test geration and malingering in neuropsychological
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment assessment. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation,
Resources. 15, 829–58.
Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1991). Iverson, G. L. & Franzen, M. D. (1996). Using multiple
Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead- objective memory procedures to detect simulated
Reitan Battery: Demographic corrections, research malingering. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
findings, and clinical applications. Odessa, FL: Neuropsychology, 18, 38–51.
Psychological Assessment Resources. Iverson, G. L., Franzen, M. D., & McCracken, L. M.
Heaton, R. K., Smith, H. H., Jr., Lehman, R. A., & Vogt, (1991). Evaluation of an objective assessment tech-
A. J. (1978). Prospects for faking believable deficits nique for the detection of malingered memory
on neuropsychological testing. Journal of Consult- deficits. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 667–76.
ing and Clinical Psychology, 46, 892–900. Iverson, G. L., Franzen, M. D., & McCracken, L. M.
Heilbronner, R. L., Sweet, J. J., Morgan, J. E., Larrabee, (1994). Application of a forced-choice memory
G. J., Millis, S. R., and conference participants procedure designed to detect experimental malin-
(2009). American Academy of Clinical Neuro- gering. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 9,
psychology consensus conference statement on the 437–50.
neuropsychological assessment of effort, response Iverson, G. L., Green, P., & Gervais, R. (1999, March/
bias, and malingering. The Clinical Neuropsy- April). Using the Word Memory Test to detect
chologist, 23, 1093–129. biased responding in head injury litigation. The
Heinly, M. T., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Love, J. M., Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation, 2–6.
& Brennan, A. (2005). WAIS Digit Span-based Keller, L. S. & Butcher, J. N. (1991). Assessment of
indicators of malingered neurocognitive dysfunc- chronic pain patients with the MMPI-2. Minneapolis:
tion. Assessment, 12, 429–44. University of Minnesota Press.
Henry, G. K., Heilbronner, R. L., Mittenberg, W., & Kim, M. S., Boone, K. B., Victor, T., Marion, S. D.,
Enders, C. (2006). The Henry-Heilbronner Index: Amano, S., Cottingham, M. E., et al. (2010). The
A 15 item empirically derived MMPI-2 subscale Warrington Recognition Memory Test for Words
for identifying probable malingering in personal- as a measure of response bias: Total score and
injury litigants and disability claimants. The Clini- response time cutoffs developed on “real world”
cal Neuropsychologist, 20, 786–97. credible and noncredible subjects. Archives of
Hiscock, M. & Hiscock, C. K. (1989). Refining the Clinical Neuropsychology, 25, 60–70.
forced-choice method for the detection of malin- King, J. H., Sweet, J. J., Sherer, M., Curtiss, G., &
gering. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Vanderploeg, R. D. (2002). Validity indicators
Neuropsychology, 11, 967–74. within the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Application
Holdnack, J. A. & Drozdick, L. W. (2009). Advanced of new and previously researched multivariate pro-
Clinical Solutions for WAIS-IV and WMS-IV. Clinical cedures in multiple traumatic brain injury samples.
and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16, 506–23.
Hom, J. & Denney, R. L. (Eds.). (2002a). Detection of Langeluddecke, P. M. & Lucas, S. K. (2003). Quantitative
response bias in forensic neuropsychology. Part I. measures of memory malingering on the Wechsler
Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 2, 1–166. Memory Scale–Third Edition in mild head injury
Hom, J. & Denney, R. L. (Eds.). (2002b). Detection of litigants. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18,
response bias in forensic neuropsychology. Part II. 181–97.
Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 3, 167–314. Lanyon, R. I. (2001). Dimensions of self-serving mis-
Hosmer, D. & Lemeshow, S. (2000). Applied logistic representation in forensic assessment. Journal of
regression, 2nd ed. New York: Wiley Interscience. Personality Assessment, 76, 169–79.
Assessment of Malingering 155

Larrabee, G. J. (1990). Cautions in the use of neuropsy- of malingered neuropsychological deficits (pp. 334–
chological evaluation in legal settings. Neuro- 71). New York: Oxford.
psychology, 4, 239–47. Larrabee, G. J., Millis, S. R., & Meyers, J. E. (2009).
Larrabee, G. J. (1992). On modifying recognition 40 plus or minus 10, a new magical number: Reply
memory tests for detection of malingering. to Russell. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23,
Neuropsychology, 6, 23–27. 746–53.
Larrabee, G. J. (1998). Somatic malingering on the Lee, G., Loring, D., & Martin, R. (1992). Rey’s 15-Item
MMPI and MMPI-2 in litigating subjects. The Visual Memory Test for the detection of malingering:
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 179–88. Normative observations on patients with neurologi-
Larrabee, G. J. (2000). Forensic neuropsychological cal disorders. Psychological Assessment, 4, 43–46.
assessment. In R. D. Vanderploeg (Ed.), Clinician’s Lees-Haley, P. R. (1992). Efficacy of MMPI-2 validity
guide to neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed., scales and MCMI-II modifier scales for detecting
pp. 301–35). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. spurious PTSD claims: F, F-K, Fake Bad Scale, Ego
Larrabee, G. J. (2003a). Detection of malingering using Strength, Subtle-Obvious Subscales, Dis, and DEB.
atypical performance patterns on standard neu- Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 681–88.
ropsychological tests. The Clinical Neuropsycho- Lees-Haley, P. R., English, L. T., & Glenn, W. J. (1991).
logist, 17, 410–25. A Fake Bad Scale for the MMPI-2 for personal injury
Larrabee, G. J. (2003b). Detection of symptom exag- claimants. Psychological Reports, 68, 203–210.
geration with the MMPI-2 in litigants with malin- Lees-Haley, P. R., Iverson, G. L., Lange, R. T., Fox, D.
gered neurocognitive dysfunction. The Clinical D., and Allen, L. M. (2002). Malingering in foren-
Neuropsychologist, 17, 54–68. sic neuropsychology: Daubert and the MMPI-2.
Larrabee, G. J. (2003c). Exaggerated MMPI-2 symp- Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 3, 167–203.
tom report in personal injury litigants with malin- Lees-Haley, P. R., Smith, H. H., Williams, C. W., &
gered neurocognitive deficit. Archives of Clinical Dunn, J. T. (1996). Forensic neuropsychological
Neuropsychology, 18, 673–86. test usage: An empirical survey. Archives of Clinical
Larrabee, G. J. (2003d). Exaggerated pain report in liti- Neuropsychology, 11, 45–51.
gants with malingered neurocognitive dysfunction. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004).
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 395–401. Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York:
Larrabee, G. J. (2004a, February). Identification of sub- Oxford University Press.
types of malingered neurocognitive dysfunction. Paper Lu, P. H., Boone, K. B., Cozolino, L., & Mitchell, C.
presented at the annual meeting of the International (2003). Effectiveness of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex
Neuropsychological Society, Baltimore, MD. Figure Test and the Meyers and Meyers Recognition
Larrabee, G. J. (2004b, February). Subtypes of MMPI-2 Memory Trial in the detection of suspect effort. The
profiles in litigants with malingered neurocognitive Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 426–40.
dysfunction. Paper presented at the annual meeting Main, C. J. (1983). The Modified Somatic Perception
of the International Neuropsychological Society, Questionnaire (MSPQ). Journal of Psychosomatic
Baltimore, MD. Research, 27, 503–514.
Larrabee, G. J. (2007, Ed.). Assessment of malingered Martens, M., Donders, J., & Millis, S. R. (2001). Evaluation
neuropsychological deficits. New York: Oxford. of invalid response sets after traumatic head injury.
Larrabee, G. J. (2008a). Malingering scales for the Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 2, 1–18.
Continuous Recognition Memory Test and Conti- Meehl, P. E., & Yonce, L. J. (1994). Taxometric analysis:
nuous Visual Memory Test. The Clinical Neuro- I. Detecting taxonicity with two quantitative
psychologist, 23, 167–80. indicators using means above and below a sliding
Larrabee, G. J. (2008b). Aggregation across multiple cut (MAMBAC procedure). Psychological Reports,
indicators improves the detection of malingering: 74, 1059–274.
Relationship to likelihood ratios. The Clinical Meehl, P. E. & Yonce, L. J. (1996). Taxometric analysis
Neuropsychologist, 22, 666–79. II. Detecting taxonicity using covariance of two
Larrabee, G.J. (2008, February). Evaluation of new quantitative indicators in successive intervals of a
MMPI-2 validity scales in malingering civil liti- third indicator (MAXCOV procedure). Psycho-
gants and nonmalingering clinical patients. logical Reports, 78, 1091–227.
Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Melzack, R. (1975). The McGill Pain Questionnaire:
International Neuropsychological Society, Major properties and scoring methods. Pain, 30,
Waikoloa, Hawaii. 191–97.
Larrabee, G. J., Greiffenstein, M. F., Greve, K. W., & Meyers, J. E. & Diep, A. (2000). Assessment of malin-
Bianchini, K. J. (2007). Refining diagnostic criteria gering in chronic pain patients using neuropsycho-
for malingering. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.). Assessment logical tests. Applied Neuropsychology, 7, 133–39.
156 forensic neuropsychology

Meyers, J. E., Millis, S. R., & Volkert, K. (2002). A valid- symptom exaggeration. Journal of Clinical and
ity index for the MMPI-2. Archives of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 1094–1102.
Neuropsychology, 17, 157–69. Mittenberg, W., Rotholc, A., Russell, E., & Heilbronner,
Meyers, J. E. & Volbrecht, M. E. (1998). Validation of R. (1996). Identification of malingered head injury
reliable digits for detection of malingering. on the Halstead-Reitan Battery. Archives of Clinical
Assessment, 5, 303–307. Neuropsychology, 11, 271–81.
Meyers, J. E. & Volbrecht, M. E. (1999). Detection of Mittenberg, W., Theroux, S., Aguila-Puentes, G.,
malingerers using the Rey Complex Figure and rec- Bianchini, K., Greve, K., & Rayls, K. (2001).
ognition trial. Applied Neuropsychology, 6, 201–207. Identification of malingered head injury on the
Meyers, J. E., & Volbrecht, M. E. (2003). A validation of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–3rd Edition. The
multiple malingering detection methods in a large Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 440–45.
clinical sample. Archives of Clinical Neuropsycho- Mittenberg, W., Theroux, S., Zielinski, R. E., &
logy, 18, 261–76. Heilbronner, R. L. (1995). Identification of malin-
Mikail, S. F., Dubreuil, S., & D’Eon, J. L. (1993). A com- gered head injury on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
parative analysis of measures used in the assess- Scale–Revised. Professional Psychology: Research
ment of chronic pain patients. Psychological and Practice, 26, 491–98.
Assessment, 5, 117–20. Morey, L. C. (1991). Personality Assessment Inventory
Miller, H. A. (2001). Miller-Forensic Assessment of professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Symptoms Test (M-FAST): Professional manual. Assessment Resources.
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Morgan, J. E. & Sweet, J. J. (2009, Eds.). Neuropsychology of
Miller, J. B., Millis, S. R., Rapport, L. J., Bashem, J. R., malingering casebook. New York: Psychology Press.
Hanks, R. A., & Axelrod, B. N. (2011. Detection of Nelson, N. W., Hoelzle, J. B., Sweet, J. J., Arbisi, P. A., &
insufficient effort using the Advanced Clinical Demakis, G. J. (2010). Updated meta-analysis of
Solutions for the Wechsler Memory Scale, Fourth the MMPI-2 Symptom Validity Scale (FBS):
Edition. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25, Verified utility in forensic practice. The Clinical
160–172. Neuropsychologist, 24, 701–24.
Miller, L. J. & Donders, J. (2001). Subjective symptom- Nelson, N. W., Sweet, J. J., & Demakis, G. J. (2006).
atology after traumatic head injury. Brain Injury, Meta-analysis of the MMPI-2 Fake Bad Scale:
15, 297–304. Utility in forensic practice. The Clinical Neuro-
Millis, S. R. (1992). The Recognition Memory Test in the psychologist, 20, 39–58.
detection of malingered and exaggerated memory Nelson, N. W., Sweet, J. J., & Heilbronner, R. L. (2007).
deficits. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 406–414. Examination of the new MMPI-2 Response Bias
Millis, S. R. (1994). Assessment of motivation and scale (Gervais): Relationship with MMPI-2 valid-
memory with the Recognition Memory Test after ity scales. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
financially compensable mild head injury. Journal Neuropsychology, 29, 67–72.
of Clinical Psychology, 50, 601–605. Nies, K. J. & Sweet, J. J. (1994). Neuropsychological
Millis, S. R. & Putnam, S. H. (1994). The Recognition assessment and malingering: A critical review of
Memory Test in the assessment of memory impair- past and present studies. Archives of Clinical Neuro-
ment after financially compensable mild head psychology, 9, 501–52.
injury: A replication. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Orey, S. A., Cragar, D. E., & Berry, D. T. R. (2000). The
70, 384–86. effects of two motivational manipulations on the
Millis, S. R., Putnam, S. H., Adams, K. M., & Ricker, J. H. neuropsychological performance of mildly head-
(1995). The California Verbal Learning Test in the injured college students. Archives of Clinical
detection of incomplete effort in neuropsychological Neuropsychology, 15, 335–48.
evaluation. Psychological Assessment, 7, 463–71. Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure
Mittenberg, W., Aguila-Puentes, G., Patton, C., complexe. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 206–356.
Canyock, E. M., & Heilbronner, R. L. (2002). Paniak, C. (2009). Moderate-to-severe traumatic brain
Neuropsychological profiling of symptom exag- injury: Probable malingering. . .and then not. In J.
geration and malingering. Journal of Forensic E. Morgan and J. J. Sweet (Eds.). Neuropsychology
Neuropsychology, 3, 227–40. of malingering casebook (pp. 101–20). New York:
Mittenberg, W., Azrin, R., Millsaps, C., & Heilbronner, Psychology Press.
R. (1993). Identification of malingered head injury Pankratz, L. & Binder, L. M. (1997). Malingering
on the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Psycho- on intellectual and neuropsychological measures. In
logical Assessment, 5, 34–40. R. Rogers (Ed.) Clinical assessment of malingering
Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit, and deception (2nd ed., pp. 223–36). New York:
D. C. (2002). Base rates of malingering and Guilford Press.
Assessment of Malingering 157

Pankratz, L., Binder, L. M., & Wilcox, L. (1987). Evaluation MMPI-2: A systematic analysis of fake-bad indica-
of an exaggerated somato-sensory deficit with symp- tors. Assessment, 2, 81–89.
tom validity testing. Archives of Neurology, 44, 798. Rohling, M. L., Binder, L. M., & Langhinrischen-
Pankratz, L., Fausti, S. A., & Peed, S. A. (1975). A forced Rohling, J. (1995). Money matters: A meta-analytic
choice technique to evaluate deafness in a hysteri- review of the association between financial com-
cal or malingering patient. Journal of Consulting pensation and the experience and treatment of
and Clinical Psychology, 43, 421–22. chronic pain. Health Psychology, 14, 537–47.
Posthuma, A. B. & Harper, J. F. (1998). Comparison of Rohling, M. L., Green, P., Allen, L. M., & Iverson, G. L.
MMPI-2 responses of child custody and personal (2002). Depressive symptoms and neurocognitive
injury litigants. Professional Psychology: Research test scores in patients passing symptom validity
and Practice, 29, 417–43. tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17,
Rapport, L. J., Farchione, T. J., Coleman, R. D., & 205–22.
Axelrod, B. N. (1998). Effects of coaching on malin- Rohling, M. L., Meyers, J. E., & Millis, S. R. (2003).
gered motor function profiles. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychological impairment following trau-
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 89–97. matic brain injury: A dose–response analysis. The
Rees, L. M., Tombaugh, T. N., Gansler, D., & Moczynski, Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 289–302.
N. (1998). Five validation experiments of the Test Root, J. C., Robbins, R. N., Chang, L., & Van Gorp, W. G.
of Memory Malingering (TOMM). Psychological (2006). Detection of inadequate effort on the
Assessment, 10, 10–20. California Verbal Learning Test-Second edition:
Reitan, R. M. & Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead- Forced choice recognition and critical item analy-
Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and sis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
clinical interpretation (2nd ed.). South Tucson, AZ: Society, 12, 688–96.
Neuropsychology Press. Ross, S. R., Millis, S. R., Krukowski, R. A., Putnam, S. H.,
Rey, A. (1941). L’examen psychologique dans le cas & Adams, K. M. (2004). Detecting probable malin-
d’encephalopathie traumatique. Archives de gering on the MMPI-2: An examination of the Fake-
Psychologie, 28, 286–340. Bad Scale in mild head injury. Journal of Clinical and
Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie [The Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 115–24.
clinical examination in psychology]. Paris: Presses Ruscio, J., Haslam, N., & Ruscio, A. M. (2006). Introduction
Universitaires de France. to the taxometric method: A practical guide. Mahwah,
Rogers, R. (1990a). Development of a new classifica- N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
tory model of malingering. Bulletin of the American Schretlen, D., Brandt, J., Krafft, L., & Van Gorp, W.
Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 18, 323–33. (1991). Some caveats in using the Rey 15-Item
Rogers, R. (1990b). Models of feigned mental illness. Memory Test to detect malingered amnesia.
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 21, Psychological Assessment, 3, 667–72.
182–88. Sharland, M. J. & Gfeller, J. D. (2007). A survey of neu-
Rogers, R. (1997a). Introduction. In R. Rogers (Ed.), ropsychologists’ beliefs and practices with respect
Clinical assessment of malingering and deception to the assessment of effort. Archives of Clinical
(2nd ed., pp. 1–19). New York: Guilford Press. Neuropsychology, 22, 213–23.
Rogers, R. (1997b). Researching dissimulation. In R. Shaw, D. J. & Matthews, C. G. (1965). Differential
Rogers (Ed.), Clinical assessment of malingering MMPI performance of brain damaged versus
and deception (2nd ed., pp. 398–426). New York: pseudoneurologic groups. Journal of Clinical
Guilford Press. Psychology, 21, 405–408.
Rogers, R. (2008). An introduction to response styles. Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behav-
In R. Rogers (Ed), Clinical assessment of malinger- ioral sciences. New York: Mc-Graw-Hill.
ing and deception (3rd ed., pp. 3–13). New York: Sivec, H. J., Lynn, S. J., & Garske, J. P. (1994). The effect
Guilford Press. of somatoform disorder and paranoid psychotic
Rogers, R., Bagby, R. M., & Dickens, S. E. (1992). role-related dissimulations as a response set on the
Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms (SIRS) MMPI-2. Assessment, 1, 69–81.
and professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Slick, D. J., Hopp, G., Strauss, E., & Spellacy, F. (1996).
Assessment Resources. Victoria Symptom Validity Test: Efficiency for
Rogers, R., Sewell, K. W., & Salekin, R. T. (1994). A detecting feigned memory impairment and rela-
meta-analysis of malingering on the MMPI-2. A tionship to neuropsychological tests and MMPI-2
systematic examination of fake-bad indicators. validity scales. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Assessment, 1, 227–37. Neuropsychology, 18, 911–22.
Rogers, R., Sewell, K. W., & Ustad, K. L. (1995). Slick, D. J., Sherman, E. M. S., & Iverson, G. L. (1999).
Feigning among chronic outpatients on the Diagnostic criteria for malingered neurocognitive
158 forensic neuropsychology

dysfunction: Proposed standards for clinical prac- Trahan, D. E. & Larrabee, G. J. (1988). Continuous
tice and research. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, Visual Memory Test. Lutz, FL: Psychological
13, 545–61. Assessment Resources.
Smart, C. M., Nelson, N. W., Sweet, J. J., Bryant, F. B., Trueblood, W. (1994). Qualitative and quantitative
Berry, D. T. R., Granacher, R. P., et al. (2008). Use of characteristics of malingered and other invalid
MMPI-2 to predict cognitive effort: A hierarchi- WAIS-R and clinical memory data. Journal of
cally optimal classification tree analysis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16,
the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 597–607.
842–52. Trueblood, W. & Schmidt, M. (1993). Malingering and
Strauss, E., Slick, D. J., Levy-Bencheton, J., Hunter, M., other validity considerations in the neuropsycho-
MacDonald, S. W., & Hultsch, D., F. (2002). logical evaluation of mild head injury. Journal of
Intraindividual variability as an indicator of malin- Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15,
gering in head injury. Archives of Clinical 578–90.
Neuropsychology, 17, 423–44. Tsushima, W. T. & Tsushima, V. G. (2001). Comparison
Strong, D. R., Glassmire, D. M., Frederick, R. I., & of the Fake Bad Scale and other MMPI-2 validity
Greene, R. L. (2006). Evaluating the latent struc- scales with personal injury litigants. Assessment, 8,
ture of the MMPI-2 F(p) scale in a forensic sample: 205–212.
A taxometric analysis. Psychological Assessment, Turk, D. C., & Melzack, R. (Eds.). (2001). Handbook of
18, 250–61. pain assessment (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford
Strong, D. R., Greene, R. L., & Schinka, J. A. (2000). A Press.
taxometric analysis of MMPI-2 infrequency scales Van Hout, M. S. E., Schmand, B., Wekking, E. M., &
[F and F(p)] in clinical settings. Psychological Deelman, B. G. (2006). Cognitive functioning in
Assessment, 12, 166–73. patients with suspected chronic toxic encephalopa-
Suhr, J. A. & Barrash, J. (2007). Performance on stan- thy: Evidence for neuropsychological disturbances
dard attention, memory, and psychomotor speed after controlling for insufficient effort. Journal of
tasks as indicators of malingering. In G. J. Larrabee Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 77, 296–
(Ed.). Assessment of malingered neuropsychological 303.
deficits (pp. 131–70). New York: Oxford. Vickery, C. D., Berry, D. T. R., Dearth, C. S., Vagnini, V.
Suhr, J. A. & Boyer, D. (1999). Use of the Wisconsin L., Baser, R. E., Cragar, D. E., et al. (2004). Head
Card Sorting Test in the detection of malingering injury and the ability to feign neuropsychological
in student simulator and patient samples. Journal deficits. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19,
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 37–48.
701–708. Vickery, C. D., Berry, D. T. R., Inman, T. H., Harris, M.
Suhr, J., Tranel, D., Wefel, J., & Barrash, J. (1997). J., & Orey, S. A. (2001). Detection of inadequate
Memory performance after head injury: Contri- effort on neuropsychological testing: A meta-
butions of malingering, litigation status, psycho- analytic review of selected procedures. Archives of
logical factors, and medication use. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 45–73.
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, Victor, T. L., Boone, K. B., Serpa, J. G., Buehler, J., &
500–514. Ziegler, E. A. (2009). Interpreting the meaning of
Sweet, J. J. (1999). Malingering: Differential diagnosis. In multiple symptom validity test failure. The Clinical
J. J. Sweet (Ed.), Forensic neuropsychology (pp. 255– Neuropsychologist, 23, 297–313.
85). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger. Walters, G. D., Berry, D. T. R., Lanyon, R. I., & Murphy,
Sweet, J. J. & King, J. H. (2002). Category Test M. P. (2009). Are exaggerated health complaints
validity indicators: Overview and practice recom- continuous or categorical? A taxometric analysis of
mendations. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, the Health Problem Overstatement Scale.
3, 241–74. Psychological Assessment, 21, 219–26.
Tait, R. C., Chibnall, J. T., & Krause, S. (1990). The Pain Walters, G. D., Berry, D. T. R., Rogers, R., Payne, J. W.,
Disability Index: Psychometric properties. Pain, & Granacher, R. P. (2009). Feigned neurocognitive
40, 171–82. deficit: Taxon or dimension? Journal of Clinical and
Tenhula, W. N. & Sweet, J. J. (1996). Double cross-vali- Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 584–93.
dation of the Booklet Category Test in detecting Walters, G. D., Rogers, R., Berry, D. T. R., Miller, H. A.,
malingered traumatic brain injury. The Clinical Duncan, S. A., McCusker, P. J., et al. (2008).
Neuropsychologist, 10, 104–116. Malingering as a categorical or dimensional
Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). TOMM. Test of memory construct: The latent structure of feigned psycho-
malingering. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health pathology as measured by the SIRS and MMPI-2.
Systems. Psychological Assessment, 20, 238–47.
Assessment of Malingering 159

Warrington, E. K. (1984). Recognition Memory Test. Wolfe, P. L., Millis, S. R., Hanks, R., Fichtenberg, N.,
Windsor, U.K.: NFER-Nelson. Larrabee, G. J., & Sweet, J. J. (2010). Effort indica-
Wechsler, D. (1955). WAIS manual. New York: tors within the California Verbal Learning Test-II
Psychological Corp. (CVLT-II). The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 24,
Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R manual. New York: 153–68.
Psychological Corp. Wygant, D. B., Sellbom, M., Ben-Porath, Y. S., Stafford,
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised K. P., Freeman, D. B., & Heilbronner, R. H. (2007).
manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp. The relation between symptom validity testing and
Wechsler, D. (1997a). WAIS-III. Administration and scor- MMPI-2 scores as a function of forensic evaluation
ing manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp. context. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22,
Wechsler, D. (1997b). WMS-III. Administration and 489–99.
scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace. Wygant, D. B., Sellbom, M., Gervais, R. O., Ben-Porath,
Wechsler, D. (2008). WAIS-IV. Administration and Y. S., Stafford, K. P., Freeman, D. B., et al. (2010).
scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. Further Validation of the MMPI-2 and MMPI-
Wechsler, D. (2009). WMS-IV. Administration and 2-RF Response Bias Scale: Findings from disability
scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. and criminal forensic settings. Psychological
Whitney, K. A., Davis, J. J., Shepard, P. H., & Herman, Assessment, 22, 745–56.
S. M. (2008). Utility of the Response Bias Youngjohn, J. R., Burrows, L., & Erdal, K. (1995). Brain
Scale (RBS) and other MMPI-2 validity scales in damage or compensation neurosis? The controver-
predicting TOMM performance. Archives of sial postconcussion syndrome. The Clinical Neuro-
Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 777–86. psychologist, 9, 112–23.
6
Functional Neuroimaging in
Forensic Neuropsychology
JOSEPH RICKER

INTRODUCTION AND tests are quite sensitive to a variety of situations


C H A P T E R O V E RV I E W that statistically depart from “average.” In other
Technological advances in neuroimaging have words, many tests will differentiate between a
had a significant and still growing impact on clinical group as compared to a nonclinical con-
how the functions of the human brain are studied trol group. What is of much greater importance,
and understood. In addition to providing a useful however, and what must be established before a
adjunct to the psychometric approaches to test has clinical validity, is whether the test has
understanding human brain-behavior function specificity: Is this test result specific or unique to a
(i.e., neuropsychology and cognitive neurosci- particular condition or clinical population but not
ence), the techniques developed by physicists, to others. Of particular concern in both forensic
engineers, and radiologists have provided ways of and clinical settings is the application of func-
assessing brain-behavior relationships that often tional neuroimaging procedures to cases of ques-
cannot be realized through any other approaches. tioned or questionable injury (e.g., mild head
In fact, the growth of studies of human cognition trauma, exposure to known or putative toxins).
that utilize functional brain imaging techniques The present chapter provides an overview of
has been exponential (Müller, 2007). This enthu- several functional neuroimaging procedures and
siasm for exciting uses for technology, however, their applications in the context of populations
must be tempered by an appreciation for scientific that are routinely encountered in the practice
rigor and the need to establish diagnostic utility of of forensic neuropsychology. This chapter will
these techniques. Clinical neuropsychologists and also address many clinical limitations of these
others are increasingly encountering functional procedures that have implications for exercising
neuroimaging and electrophysiological tech- caution when they are utilized in any evaluation,
niques (e.g., SPECT, PET, QEEG, MEG, fMRI) in forensic or otherwise.
individual clinical and forensic cases. The clinical The clinical responsibility for interpreting the
application of these techniques remains, more content of neuroimaging data rests with radiolo-
often than not, still investigational in the popula- gists, not neuropsychologists. It is important, how-
tions encountered in forensic neuropsychological ever, for neuropsychologists, other clinicians, and
evaluations (Ricker & Arenth, 2008). All tests, triers of fact to consider the multitude of factors
however, whether based in MRI physics, radioiso- and conditions that may result in positive neu-
tope emission, electrophysiology, or paper-and- roimaging findings. It is not advisable to simply
pencil administration, must demonstrate adequate accept the findings from investigative techniques
sensitivity, specificity, and acceptability within as evidence of neuropathology and subsequently,
their relevant disciplines before being applied in whether intentionally or unintentionally, draw
clinical or forensic cases in a reliable and valid forceful conclusions of causality based on other
manner. findings or the patient’s self-report. This seems like
As is the case in all forms of clinical tests and it should be obvious, but stepping back from the
procedures, neuroimaging technologies have crit- striking visual impact that pictures generated from
ical limitations. Many psychological and medical functional imaging often have can be difficult.
Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic Neuropsychology 161

REVIEW OF CLAS SES OF RESTING VERSUS


EVIDENCE FOR A C T I VAT E D I M A G I N G
NEURODIAGNO STIC In functional neuroimaging, one can conceptual-
PROCEDURES ize techniques into two broad categories: “resting”
The U.S. Department of Health and Human and “activated” (Ricker, 2005). Resting paradigms
Services holds medical tests and procedures to are those that acquire images during nondynamic
standards of evidence. The fact that a medical test (i.e., “static” or baseline) conditions. There have
or procedure exists, is available, or has validity been numerous studies using resting functional
with certain populations under certain conditions neuroimaging in various populations, most of
does not constitute evidence of its reliability or which have examined chronic glucose uptake
validity under other circumstances. Instead, (exclusively with PET), resting cerebral blood
experts in appropriate fields evaluate new and flow (using PET or SPECT) or resting electro-
existing tests and technologies with reference to physiological activity (e.g., EEG, QEEG, or MEG).
their technical reliability and validity, and, most Essentially, resting studies are those that occur
importantly, their utility in specific situations and when a participant is ostensibly not engaged in
with specific populations. any specific task. Resting studies, by design, have
Technologies are, of course, evaluated for no explicit or systematic requirements of the par-
safety, but they are also rated in terms of their ticipant other than those required to successfully
utility (i.e., “established,” “promising,” “investiga- acquire a technically valid image, such as having
tional,” “doubtful,” or “unacceptable”). In addition, the participant lie still, minimizing head move-
there are classes of evidence ratings. These include, ment, eliminating extraneous light and noise, and
in descending order of support, the following: so forth (Raichle, 2001).
Class I—Supporting evidence is provided by one Unlike resting studies, activation studies
or more well-designed, randomized, controlled require participants to systematically receive sen-
clinical trials; Class II—Supporting evidence pro- sory input (e.g., a visual array) or engage in an
vided by one or more well-designed clinical stud- activity (either cognitive or motor) in order to
ies, for example, case control, cohort studies; Class examine changes in some time- or stimulus-
III—Supporting evidence is provided by expert linked aspect of brain physiology (Roland, 1993).
opinion, nonrandomized historic controls, or at These tasks are usually administered in adherence
least one case report. It is notable that the types of to a necessarily strict protocol, typically with some
functional neuroimaging that are encountered in form of overt response required in order to pro-
forensic cases—SPECT, PET, fMRI, QEEG—are vide verification that the participant is actively
all rated as investigational in terms of their engaged in the requested task. Because of the
application in potential forensic populations, technologies related to, and physical properties of,
for example, mild brain injury, (Therapeutics and the dependent variables examined, activation
Technology Subcommittee of the American studies have much briefer time windows than
Academy of Neurology, 1996; American College resting studies. Some technologies are able to
of Radiology 2008; Nuwer, 1994). Nonetheless, as image changes across time in a relatively continu-
empirical support is gradually established, their ous manner (e.g., MEG and event-related fMRI).
valid use is likely to become a future reality. Given this degree of control, the experimenter is
The American College of Radiology rates all able to make more reliable inferences about
neuroimaging and radiographic assessment and cerebral activity underlying the cognitive process
interventions in detail, using various criteria for in question (Small, 2006).
clinically appropriate use (i.e., the technique or
device has sufficient evidence to support its non- RADIOLIGAND-BASED
investigational use for a specific clinical syndrome IMAGING
[e.g., brain injury] or procedure [e.g., mammog-
raphy]). Under current nomenclature, neuroim- Single Photon Emission Computed
aging technologies are rated on a scale of 1 to 9, Tomography (SPECT)
with 1 representing “least appropriate,” and SPECT is an alternative approach to functional
9 representing “most appropriate.” Detailed imaging with an emphasis on regional cerebral
descriptions and downloadable full committee blood flow (rCBF) imaging, but specific neurore-
reports are available from the American College ceptor imaging studies are also possible (Masdeu
of Radiology at www.acr.org/ac. & Arbizu, 2008). SPECT technology is based on
162 forensic neuropsychology

the concept that regional changes in brain activity fluctuating changes. SPECT is also not as diverse
or chemistry can be indirectly measured via as other imaging techniques, such as PET, but
externally placed gamma radiation detectors novel ligands are under continuous development
(“cameras”), which detect the regional accumula- (Ogawa et al., 2009). Recent technologic advances
tions of tracer flow or receptor-binding isotopes. have provided for multiple-headed scanners,
The dependent variable in SPECT derives which offers improved resolution. The compound
from the well-established principle of increased 123-I–para iodoamphetamine (IAMP) provides a
cerebral activity correlating with increased blood unique look at immediate and delayed perfusion.
flow. That is, when neural activity in a region of In addition, IAMP has amphetamine-like quali-
the brain increases, related glucose and oxygen ties, which allow for distinct presynaptic and post-
requirements also increase. Since the blood supply synaptic imaging. Although IAMP is no longer
carries glucose and oxygen to the brain, the flow available in the United States, a newer compound
of blood to the active area increases (Ingvar & (99m-Technetium–HMPAO) is being used for the
Risberg, 1965). The radioisotopes themselves are same purposes. Technetium is a primary blood
absorbed into the glial cells, but are not readily perfusion agent with the property of rapid cerebral
excreted. Thus, the absorbed radioisotopes remain uptake. It is retained in brain structures for several
in greater concentration in the more active areas. hours, allowing for image capture at various time
Through normal radioactive decay, the isotope points following ligand administration.
emits annihilated radioactive particles (i.e., pho- Recent technologic advances have provided
tons), which are then detected by the external for multiple-headed scanners, which have
cameras. Computer-based reconstruction then improved resolution. SPECT does have several
permits representations to be made of differences sources of potential measurement error, however
in blood flow. (Zhang et al., 1994; Sohlberg, Watabe, & Iida,
Relative to other functional imaging tech- 2008). Unlike PET, SPECT imaging requires that
niques, there are several advantages of SPECT regional counts be normalized to an area that is
(Wintermark et al., 2005). It is more widely avail- presumably free of injury, and its resolution does
able than PET or fMRI. Unlike with PET, the not yet approach that of PET imaging, although
radioisotopes that are typically used with SPECT resolution has improved dramatically with the
can be ordered and delivered in advance, thus increased availability of combined SPECT/CT
precluding the necessity of an onsite cyclotron technology (Seo, Mari, & Hasagawa, 2008). Color
and chemist, as required for PET. SPECT imaging software can produce visually
SPECT has limited use in imaging a change in striking images, but reliable and valid interpreta-
blood flow from one point in time to another, for tion is best accomplished through quantitative
example from pre-ictal to ictal state (Habert & pixel counts (Loutfi & Singh, 1995; Habert et al.,
Huberfeld, 2008). It is not appropriate, however, 2011). It must also be noted that although SPECT
for use in mapping blood flow changes that change can be used quantitatively, this is not the case in
rapidly over time within a single scanning session, most settings. Visual inspection of SPECT maps is
such as those encountered in most states of cogni- a qualitative process, and interpretation may vary
tive activity, or for making direct inferences about from clinician to clinician. In addition, image
brain metabolism (Roland 1993; Small, 2006). reconstruction is typically based on presumptions
SPECT has some potential for studying discrete about which brain regions are “normal.” Relative
cognitive processes if multiple scanning sessions flow values in SPECT are often based upon a
are used and explicit experimental task conditions region such as the thalamus or cerebellum. While
and complex subtraction analyses are utilized such assumptions might be valid for some popu-
(Ludwig et al., 2008). Such experimental and sta- lations with focal lesions (e.g., stroke), they might
tistical rigor is quite beyond most nonacademic not be valid for populations whose involvement
clinical settings, however. is more diffuse (e.g., TBI). Although SPECT is
The most commonly used radioactive isotopes very sensitive to detecting regional differences in
for SPECT are absorbed by the brain within 2 min- resting blood flow, there is little specificity to the
utes, but may have half-lives of several hours. The patterns that are obtained and the results depicted
effect of this is that once the tracer has been admin- in series of SPECT images can be affected by
istered, the resulting images that are acquired will many factors including mood disturbances, medi-
remain the same for the next several hours, thus cations, or current substance use (Ricker &
making SPECT a poor tool for measuring rapid or Zafonte 2000; Granacher, 2008).
Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic Neuropsychology 163

Positron Emission Tomography (PET) hydrogen atoms in the magnetic field emit a
Positron emission tomography (PET) is another response, but this results in enough signal to
radioisotope-based approach to imaging. The permit the reconstruction of images. Since most
variable of interest is usually glucose absorption of the atoms that are excited in this process are
(measured by uptake of fluorodeoxyglucose), found within water molecules, water content and
although oxygen-15 PET scanning is also used for tissue density dictate the signal that is detected by
activated imaging studies (Buckner & Logan, the scanner and subsequently digitally recon-
2001). In spite of the exponential growth in inves- structed into an image (Springer et al., 2000;
tigational fMRI publications (overwhelmingly Huettel, Song, & McCarthy, 2009)
conducted with healthy individuals), PET remains Functional magnetic resonance imaging
the “gold standard” for functional neuroimaging (fMRI) is a variant of conventional MRI. The crit-
(Chen et al., 2008). It is also remains quite expen- ical difference, however, is that the dependent
sive, however, (approximately US $3,000 for a variable of interest in fMRI is alteration in signal
series of resting PET images) and requires a cyclo- intensity related to increases in blood flow result-
tron to be present onsite for most radioisotopes. ing from changes in brain activity. Thus, fMRI is
PET has been widely used as a research tool since more concerned with changes in brain activity
the 1970s, but its application for persons with than anatomic structure per se. While the primary
traumatic brain injury has been minimally inves- goal of structural MRI is to generate high-resolu-
tigated. PET has the capability of demonstrating tion anatomic images of underlying brain struc-
specific biochemical or physiologic processes ture, the goal of functional MRI is to allow the
associated with cerebral blood flow and metabo- investigator to make inferences about regional
lism (Gulyas & Sjoholm, 2007). The resulting PET changes in brain activity (Aguirre, 2006). As has
image represents the spatial distribution of radio- been discussed with other blood-flow-dependent
isotopes, which is usually portrayed on an actual techniques, in fMRI, specific tasks or stimuli are
or standardized anatomic (MRI) template. introduced to the individual in the scanner in
PET studies typically utilize tracers such as order to elicit increased cerebral activity. When
[18F]-fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) for the quantifi- neural activity increases in a region, there is an
cation of “resting” (i.e., nonactivated) regional increase in blood flow to that region. When the
brain metabolism. Studies of blood flow changes brain is at rest, there is a tight correlation between
associated with motor or cognitive activity can be regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF), regional
accomplished through the use of tracers such as cerebral metabolic rate for glucose (rCMRglc)
oxygen-15 labeled water. Radiolabeled ligands and regional cerebral metabolic rate for oxygen
that target dopaminergic, serotonergic, and other (rCMRO2). With activity, however, rCBF can
receptor systems have been developed, as are increase by over 50%, which is well beyond meta-
genetically mediated transport markers and bolic demands. The physiologic basis for this is
pathology-specific ligands (Herholz, Carter, & not clear (Weisskoff 2000). With the excess of
Jones, 2007). blood flow to the region, particularly in light of
only minimal increase in oxygen extraction, there
Functional Magnetic is a resulting localized abundance of oxyhemoglo-
Resonance-Based Imaging (fMRI) bin relative to deoxyhemoglobin in the venous
All magnetic resonance-based imaging techniques and capillary beds that perfuse the active regions
capitalize on the presence of hydrogen in all of the of cortex. Oxyhemoglobin is naturally diamag-
body’s tissues. When the nuclei of hydrogen atoms netic, while deoxyhemoglobin is paramagnetic
are placed in a strong magnetic field, they align in (i.e., becomes readily magnetized within a mag-
parallel to the field’s direction. In MRI, radiofre- netic field). With increased neural activity and
quency (RF) pulses are presented at a 90° angle concomitant increased blood flow, there is a net
relative to the magnetic field. When this occurs, increase in diamagnetic material (oxyhemoglo-
the hydrogen nuclei realign and begin spinning in bin), and a net decrease in paramagnetic material
a different direction (“excitation”). The RF pulse is (deoxyhemoglobin). This results in an increase in
then stopped and the nuclei return to their origi- signal intensity that can be detected externally,
nal alignment and spin. This process of resuming and is represented as higher signal intensity on
previous nuclei states results in the emission of an T2* (“T2-star”)-weighted scans. This change in
electronic signal that can be detected by the scan- signal intensity is referred to as the blood oxygen
ner. It should be noted that only about 1% of the level dependent (or “BOLD”) effect (Chen &
164 forensic neuropsychology

Ogawa, 2000). In magnets of “average” strength computer hardware requirements must be con-
(e.g., 1.5 Tesla), the signal changes appear to ema- sidered, along with data storage and data security.
nate from veins and large venules. In high-field Finally, fMRI protocols do not and cannot “auto-
magnets (e.g., 3, 4, or even greater Tesla, an index matically” or “objectively” yield brain maps, nor
of magnitude in comparison to the magnetic field are there at present normative values for fMRI
strength of the earth’s gravity), signal is more scans or activity levels (Aguirre, 2006). The result-
likely obtained from microvessels, small venules, ing images must be carefully processed and skill-
and capillaries. The signal changes obtained are fully reconstructed, and this reconstruction
very small, on the order of 1% to 6%, and occur process should be considered to be as much art as
over approximately a 2- to 6-second timeframe science. The approach that one takes in recon-
(depending on brain region, age of the partici- structing and displaying the data in the form of
pant, and the task being performed). brain images data will impact the portrayal, and
Because fMRI utilizes the body’s natural physi- potentially the interpretation, of the end product.
cal responses to high-strength magnetism, no
exogenous tracers, radioisotopes, or contrast ELECTROPHYSIOLOGICAL
agents are necessary. The anatomic resolution of TECHNIQUES
fMRI is superior to that of SPECT or PET. There
are numerous activation paradigms that can be Quantitative
carried out in fMRI, and it allows for greater flex- Electroencephalography (QEEG)
ibility in paradigm with reference to repeatability Electroencephalography (EEG) is a neuro-
and brevity of overall session (Huettel et al., 2009) physiological index of cerebral function that has
As with any imaging procedure, however, widespread use. In medical rehabilitation popula-
fMRI can be impacted by numerous variables. tions, it is used to assess seizure activity, coma,
Although publications using fMRI in healthy and gross outcome. Data from traditional qualita-
individuals have appeared since 1990, few FMRI tive EEG analysis does not, however, allow for
studies exist in clinical populations, and compara- identification and quantification of the spectra of
tively fewer in populations of primary interest in wave frequencies that occur in the human brain.
forensic neuropsychology practice. Movement Thus, conventional EEG is useful clinically only as
can disrupt head alignment, and may also perturb a very gross monitor, and has little utility in
the magnetic field itself. Thus, head movement advance prediction of outcome. When Fourier
must be eliminated, and other extraneous move- transform analysis is applied to EEG, however, it
ments must be reduced. Overt responses to tasks allows for continuous monitoring and quantifica-
must be minimal at most. Movements of the jaw tion across all cerebral wave frequencies. This
required for talking are often considered too approach is more commonly referred to as quan-
excessive during fMRI, but event-related para- titative electroencephalography
digms do allow for some degree of verbal respond- Quantitative electroencephalography (QEEG)
ing (Ishikawa, 2002). is the term applied to a group of inter-related
The normal high frequency noise within the technologies that are centered on the mathemati-
scanner must be considered when evaluating cal concept of spectral analysis (Wallace et al.,
individuals with possible acquired neuropathol- 2001). In essence, the EEG signal is digitally pro-
ogy or neuropsychiatric disturbance. The exam- cessed and the relative contributions of each fre-
iner or technician must also monitor for quency are identified and quantified. When
idiosyncratic responses such as claustrophobia, digitized, the individual component frequencies
anxiety, boredom, disengagement, or actual onset of a complex waveform (i.e., amount of alpha,
of sleep while in the scanner. Although virtually beta, delta, and theta contained within the signal)
any contemporary MRI scanner can be adapted to can be discerned in a manner superior to that of
functional imaging, fMRI is still investigational in traditional visual analysis of EEG printed output
virtually all populations, including brain trauma, (Nuwer et al., 2005)
and is not considered appropriate for routine clin- There are several approaches to displaying
ical use (American College of Radiology, 2008), QEEG spectral data. In the compressed spectral
and is thus primarily a research tool at this time. array (CSA) format, the frequency components
This also limits its availability to primarily aca- from a series of epochs (e.g., 30 sec. blocks of
demic medical centers. Even a single fMRI session time) are quantified. The output is then repre-
will generate a very large volume of data. Thus, sented sequentially either in print or graphically,
Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic Neuropsychology 165

permitting interpretation of changes in the EEG presence of an asymmetrically oriented neuron,


signal over time (Luccas et al., 1999). Another the sources and sinks create dipolar electromag-
approach is topographical mapping (Skrandies, netic fields that cancel one another out. The intra-
1995). In this format, each electrode in the EEG cellular current between the region of synaptic
montage is assigned a color value (or grayscale activation and the point at which the current
shading level) for each frequency range. The color returns to extracellular space does not cancel out
or shading represents the frequency level underly- (Tang et al., 2002). This magnetic field can be
ing the electrode. The color map (or shading recorded.
gradient) is then superimposed on an oval (repre- Although intuitively similar to EEG, MEG has
senting the head). The resulting brain map resem- some advantages. First, MEG frequencies are
bles what might be obtained from a resting SPECT technically easier to record than those from EEG,
or PET. It is imperative, however, that one appre- given that the detectors are located in a helmet for
ciates the fact that the topographical map is placement adjacent to the scalp, and thus do not
derived from a minimal number of data points, have to be individually applied (or intercon-
reflects only cortical surface activity, and has nected). Second, magnetic fields are not affected
many interpolated color values (e.g., the colors or by the variability in skull thickness over different
shadings between electrodes are interpolated). A regions of cortex. Third, in general, the compo-
third approach to data representation is that of nent structure of the MEG response is actually
probability mapping (Nuwer et al., 2005). This simpler than that derived through EEG. There are,
approach utilizes topographic mapping as a basis, of course, disadvantages to MEG. MEG is very
but compares the map to a normative database expensive and thus not widely available. MEG
(i.e., a composite map). An individual’s map may also does not detect deep (e.g., subcortical)
then be statistically compared to the normative sources of activity. MEG, along with EEG, lacks
map, and inferences based on the normal distri- the anatomic precision of other neuroimaging
bution are made in interpretation. techniques (Stern & Silbersweig, 2001).

Magnetoencephalography (MEG) DIAGNO STIC GROUPS


In addition to electrical activity, neurons also gen- ENCOUNTERED IN
erate minute magnetic fields that can be measured FORENSIC CONTEXTS
using an approach known as magnetoencepha-
lography (MEG). MEG involves the use of liquid Brain Injury
helium that cools conducting coils to almost abso- SPECT Studies of Brain Injury
lute zero. When cooled to such low temperatures, In chronic moderate and severe TBI (i.e., at least
the electrical resistance of the conductor is greatly several months following acute injury), resting
reduced, and very small changes in magnetic field PET and SPECT studies generally consistently
can be detected (King et al., 2000). The miniscule demonstrated either hypometabolism or decrea-
nature of these fields requires significant ampli- sed resting blood flow, predominantly within
fication in order to be useful. Using devices known prefrontal cortex (Bergsneider et al., 1997, 2001;
as superconducting quantum-interference devices Fontaine et al., 1999; Gross et al., 1996; Jansen
(SQUIDs), the changes in magnetic fields pro- et al 1996; Langfitt et al., 1986; Rao et al., 1984;
duced by neuronal activity can be detected. An Ricker et al., 2001; Tenjin et al., 1990). In most of
array of conductors is situated around the head of these studies, decreased blood flow and metabo-
the participant, which allows for the placement of lism were generally in excess of what might be
multiple detectors (Bagic, 2007). expected based solely on findings from structural
The physiological basis of MEG is that of scans (i.e., CT and MRI). The presence of
normal neuronal membrane signal conduction. decreased resting blood flow or metabolism is
The flow of electrical current within an active not, however, in and of itself evidence of compro-
neuron generates a magnetic field. When a syn- mised or nonfunctional brain tissue (e.g., Duara
apse becomes active, there is a current flow across et al., 1992).
the neuronal membrane. This current diffuses Numerous investigations have demonstrated
intracellularly, and then emerges extracelluarly at that SPECT is better than structural imaging (i.e.,
a fixed distance from where it began (i.e., from CT and MRI) in the detection of the presence
dendrite to synapse). This results in the opportu- and extent of trauma-related lesions. SPECT
nity for “sources” and “sinks” extracelluarly. In the has been applied to acute brain injury and has
166 forensic neuropsychology

demonstrated regionally decreased blood flow in fMRI) in differential diagnosis, prognosis, and
the presence of normal acute CT scans, but there intervention.
is tremendous variability across individuals (i.e., SPECT has been shown to be of use in research
no pathognomonic profile emerges) (Abdel- studies following brain injury, but there is no
Dayem et al., 1987). Positive SPECT findings have particular SPECT profile that is pathognomonic
also sometimes demonstrated in cases of below- or reliable for brain injury (Herscovitch, 1996;
average neuropsychological test scores in cases of Granacher, 2008). Clinically, the literature does
mild brain trauma, but it should be noted that not support the routine use of SPECT for the
SPECT findings are usually not predictive of test evaluation of post-concussion syndrome in
performance (Umile et al., 1998). specific, or actually for brain injury in general.
In spite of advances in technology and The Therapeutics and Technology Subcommittee
data analysis, the utility of SPECT’s characterizing of the American Academy of Neurology (1996)
specific illness and injury states or predicting has rated SPECT as an investigational procedure
outcome remains controversial (Granacher, for the study of brain trauma. More recently, the
2008). SPECT has shown particular utility American College of Radiology (2008) continues
when correlating neuropsychological parameters to rate SPECT as inappropriate (a rating of “1” on
with the chronic effects of severe brain injury a 1 to 9 scale, with “1” indicating “least appropri-
(Ichise et al., 1994), but caution must be exercised ate”) in the evaluation of mild head trauma, with
given that systemic metabolic abnormalities, slightly increased appropriateness (a rating of “4”
substance use, and emotional disorders can on the same scale) “for selected cases” of closed
impact SPECT results and interpretations head trauma (across levels of injury severity) with
(Wortzel et al., 2008). For example, the results of associated neurologic or cognitive signs. In spite
SPECT (and PET) studies have been shown to be of the professional cautions and the present con-
abnormal in disorders such as depression clusions regarding the lack of scientific support
(Sackheim et al., 1990), manic-depressive disor- for the routine use of SPECT in brain injury,
der (Iidaka et al., 1995), obsessive-compulsive SPECT appears to be used frequently in clinical
disorders (Adams et al., 1993), generalized anxi- and forensic contexts as a means of supporting a
ety disorders (Uchiyama et al., 1997), and schizo- diagnosis of brain injury.
phrenia (Paulman et al., 1990). SPECT frequently
demonstrates abnormalities in dementia (Read PET Studies of Brain Injury
et al., 1995) and cerebrovascular disease (Masdeu Experimental studies of traumatic brain injury
& Brass, 1995), and also suggests decreased tracer have shown that cerebral hyperglycolysis is a
uptake in normal aging (Mozley et al., 1996). pathophysiological response that occurs in
SPECT also demonstrates positive functional response to injury-induced neurochemical cas-
neuroimaging findings in domains that frequently cades. Bergsneider and colleagues (1997; 2001)
covary with (or are attributed to) TBI, such as have shown, via FDG-PET, that hyperglycolysis
learning disabilities (Wood et al., 1991) and som- occurs in both regional and global settings after
atization disorder (Lazarus et al., 1989). Finally, severe brain injury in humans. Such abnormali-
SPECT often demonstrates abnormalities in ties may also exist transiently after milder injury.
aggression (Amen et al., 1996), alcohol depen- Several studies have demonstrated PET’s abil-
dence (Modell & Mountz, 1995), alcohol with- ity to detect abnormalities not seen on static imag-
drawal (Mampunza et al., 1995), opiate use ing in cases of moderate and severe brain injury
(Krystal et al., 1995), hallucinogenic drug use (see review in Ricker, 2005). In addition, data exist
(Hertzman et al., 1990), and cocaine abuse (Miller to suggest that functional imaging demonstrates
et al., 1992). In addition, at least 6% of noninjured, areas of physiological dysfunction beyond the
medically healthy individuals may demonstrate boundaries of static lesions seen on structural
focal abnormalities on SPECT imaging (Umile imaging. Langfitt and colleagues (1986) presented
et al., 1998). Some investigators have noted that some of the earliest functional imaging data from
when used in a prospective design, a negative individuals who had sustained severe TBI. FDG-
SPECT scan is a good predictor of a favorable out- PET was also compared with CT, MRI, and SPECT
come after brain injury, and that SPECT overall studies. They were able to demonstrate with PET
correlates well with the severity of the initial that cerebral hypometabolism extended beyond
trauma (Jacobs et al., 1994). Still lacking are pro- the morphometric boundaries of lesion as imaged
spective studies of SPECT (as well as PET and with the structural methods of CT and MRI.
Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic Neuropsychology 167

Cobalt-55 PET can also demonstrate regional subjects during both free and cued recall. The
abnormalities following severe TBI beyond those change in allocation of neural resources during
shown by CT and MRI (Jansen et al., 1996). It has tasks with greater cognitive load may suggest
been demonstrated that the intermediate meta- greater frontal lobe involvement resulting from
bolic reduction phase begins to resolve approxi- increased cognitive effort. Of additional note is
mately one month following injury, regardless of the finding that during recognition tasks, both the
injury severity (Bergsneider et al., 2001). The cor- controls and the individuals with TBI performed
relation between the extent of change in neuro- at comparable behavioral levels (and within
logic disability and the change in CMRglc from normal limits), yet the individuals with TBI still
the early to late period is modest, however. demonstrated increased change in regional cere-
In spite of the application of functional imag- bral blood flow relative to the controls. This sug-
ing studies in forensic situations when there is a gests that after brain injury, individuals must exert
question about the occurrence of, or disability more cognitive effort than controls to attain the
resulting from, traumatic brain injury, there are same level of overt behavior. A subsequent O-15
surprisingly few studies that have actually PET study in TBI by a different group of investiga-
attempted to correlate functional imaging find- tors has also demonstrated similar findings on a
ings with cognition. In most of the studies that verbal list-learning task (Levine et al., 2002).
have attempted to correlate functional imaging
with testing, the findings from psychometric FMRI Studies of Brain Injury
assessment have been examined with those from In the first fMRI studies of individuals with TBI
functional imaging conducted either prior to or (McAllister et al., 1999; 2001), the investigators
following memory evaluation. For example, Ruff examined individuals with a very recent history of
and colleagues described FDG-PET and neurop- mild brain injury (i.e., within the previous 30
sychological test findings among a selected series days). As in the Ricker et al. (2001) PET study of
of nine individuals who had sustained mild brain chronic severe TBI, the individuals with mild TBI
trauma whose static imagings (CT or MRI) were in the McAllister studies demonstrated intact
negative (Ruff et al., 1994), but the findings have behavioral performance on a verbal working
minimal generalizability given that subjects were memory task, but they did show right hemisphere
specifically selected for inclusion based upon out- lateralized fMRI activation in response to
come rather that a priori criteria. In addition, increased working memory load. In an fMRI
scanning and the neuropsychological evaluation investigation of working memory following mod-
were separated in time by an average of 11 months. erate and severe TBI (Christodoulou et al., 2001),
In a study of 20 persons with mild traumatic brain increased blood flow and more widespread dis-
injury it was noted that local abnormal cerebral persion of cortical activation was noted during
metabolic rates correlated with complaints and working memory tasks. This again suggests that
neuropsychological test results obtained during increased cognitive effort is reflected in increased
the chronic phase of recovery (Gross et al., 1996). brain activation on fMRI. A study by Perlstein
In moderate and severe TBI, resting PET studies and colleagues (2004) represented a significant
have demonstrated frontal hypometabolism, with advance in the study of TBI-related problems in
correlated poor performance on neuropsycho- working memory. In the fMRI component of their
logical tests that are thought to be mediated by study, they used an event-related design, employed
frontal lobe functioning (Fontaine et al., 1999) as parametric manipulation of task difficulty, and
well as widespread white matter disconnection examined not only the central executive but also
(Wu et al., 2004). the phonological loop within verbal working
PET activation studies are likely to be far more memory. As part of a larger behavioral study,
sensitive to the functional effects of brain injury seven persons with TBI and seven controls were
or disease, as such paradigms introduce in vivo examined using fMRI during the verbal N-Back.
cognitive challenges (Baron, 1995). The first PET They provided data that demonstrated a disrup-
study to apply a cognitive activation paradigm tion in the dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal
with individuals who sustained severe TBI (Ricker network known to mediate verbal working
et al., 2001) demonstrated left frontal lobe rCBF memory, with greater disruption occurring at
changes in individuals with TBI during free recall increasing levels of cognitive load. Although this
when compared to controls, but rCBF increases study was certainly a needed contribution to the
were noted in more posterior brain regions in TBI literature, there were several methodological
168 forensic neuropsychology

limitations. There was no examination of spatial in the TBI group was associated with reduced
working memory, thus any generalizations must be performance on memory tasks after rehabilita-
strictly limited to verbal working memory. The tion, creating an inverted-U quadratic relation-
authors point out that they did not have informa- ship between performance and activation, which
tion on acute injury status for most of the persons was therefore predictive of outcome. The authors
with TBI, nor did they have prior neuroradiologic opined that underactivation may represent struc-
data, loss of consciousness duration, or length of tural injury to the gray or white matter within the
post-traumatic amnesia for many patients. Finally, region of interest or possibly injury to areas pro-
of the seven patients examined, one had sustained a jecting to that region, while overactivation may
severe initial TBI, and the remaining six were char- indicate intact and engaged cortical areas, which,
acterized as having sustained moderate injuries. despite “effortful utilization,” failed to produce
In a recent study (Newsome et al., 2007), ten improvement in functional memory as tested.
persons with severe TBI were compared to six Strangman and colleagues (2009) subsequently
persons with orthopedic injuries using an N-Back conducted an additional study of 20 participants
task in a 1.5 Tesla MR scanner. The stimuli were with moderate or severe TBI and 20 healthy con-
photographs of faces. The findings of this study trols using the same behavioral and fMRI para-
were generally comparable to those of previous digm from their 2008 study. This study evaluated
studies, with most group differences noted in the whether individuals with TBI and controls acti-
1-Back condition. This study only examined per- vated the same networks during verbal learning,
sons with severe TBI, however, and only up to a and whether TBI affected neural activity during
2-Back level of cognitive load, thus application of encoding when participants were instructed to
the results to other levels of TBI must be made use semantic clustering. Despite baseline testing
with caution. In addition, although the stimuli indicating significant differences in behavioral
used in this study were visual, faces are processed performance between groups, direct comparison
differently by the brain than purely spatial infor- of group fMRI activation across all tasks, indicated
mation. As such, this paradigm did not address no significant differences, suggesting that indi-
the specific cognitive or neural architecture of the viduals in the TBI group activated the same gen-
visuospatial sketchpad component of working eral networks as healthy controls. Further
memory. Each of the published fMRI studies of functional connectivity analyses suggested a func-
working memory after TBI has addressed rather tional (but not anatomical) breakdown in connec-
limited aspects of working memory processing tivity between the left DLPFC and other areas
and examined rather limited ranges of injury normally associated with strategic control.
severity (e.g., mild only, severe only, or small Most recently, Russell, Arenth, Scanlon,
numbers of moderate and severe only). Kessler, and Ricker (2011) examined both encod-
Only very recently has episodic memory, ing and recognition of verbal and visual stimuli
which is arguably the memory domain that is using fMRI. Twelve adults with chronic severe,
most frequently assessed clinically (e.g., through moderate, and complicated mild injuries were
list-learning tests), been formally studied with compared with a matched group of 12 controls.
fMRI in persons with TBI. The first of these Behavioral task performance did not differentiate
studies (Strangman et al., 2008) involved the the groups. During neuroimaging, however, the
teaching of a verbal learning strategy to a group of group of individuals with TBI exhibited increased
54 individuals with documented memory impair- activation, as well as increased bilaterality and
ment after moderate or severe TBI and a matched dispersion as compared to controls. These results
group of controls. FMRI scanning was conducted were generally consistent with previous imaging
only during the encoding phase of a verbal learn- and behavioral findings in TBI, but there was an
ing paradigm. Free recall and recognition were additional novel finding. Persons with TBI dem-
assessed outside of the scanner. The aim of the onstrated more subcortical activation, (i.e., cau-
study was to assess the ability of fMRI results to date and thalamus), while activations for the
predict outcome after a subsequent 12-session control group was mostly cortical. While the exact
rehabilitation program focused on teaching of nature of this finding is not known, there is evi-
internal strategies for memory improvement. The dence that these subcortical structures are
findings suggested that left prefrontal areas were involved in cognitive processes, and, even if not
related to strategic verbal learning. They also typically injured after TBI, are highly intercon-
observed that BOTH under- and over-activation nected with frontal cortex through white matter
Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic Neuropsychology 169

tracts that are known to be susceptible to compro- Academy of Neurology, along with the American
mise after moderate and severe TBI. Clinical Neurophysiology Society (formerly the
Although PET and FMRI clearly represent American Electroencephalographic Society), have
more advanced approaches to brain imaging when published a practice guideline report on the use of
compared to SPECT, neither has reached a suffi- digital EEG which discusses the insufficiency of
cient threshold of evidence for routine use in mild evidence for the use of digital EEG in the diagno-
brain trauma. PET is currently classified at the sis and assessment of post-concussion syndrome,
same level of clinical appropriateness (or lack or minor or moderate brain injury (Nuwer, 1994).
thereof) as SPECT by the American College of Although certainly not everyone aggress with the
Radiology (2008). Although there are now more conclusions of the AAN (e.g., Hoffmann et al.,
fMRI studies of TBI, and many are currently per- 1999), additional cross-validation with indepen-
forming large-scale studies, the ACR has not fully dent groups and appropriate experimental design
evaluated fMRI. will be needed to establish the utility of QEEG in
clinical and forensic differential diagnosis. A more
QEEG Studies after TBI recent comprehensive review by Nuwer et al.
The literature regarding the utility of QEEG in (2005) summarizes the state of the science quite
brain injury is growing. Much of the investiga- succinctly: “QEEG may be useful as a research
tional work in the area of QEEG and brain injury technique in the hands of highly expert users. But
has been in the area of coma (Wallace, Wagner, & its high rate of false positive findings and the lack
Wagner, 2001). For example, previous work has of diagnostic specificity preclude routine clinical
shown poor outcome among a population of per- usefulness” (p. 2017).
sons with severe TBI who continued to show slow
pattern responses (Bricolo et al., 1978). A poor TOX I C E X P O S U R E
prognosis is more likely among individuals with Neuroanatomic imaging techniques such as CT
severe TBI that have silent or slow QEEG patterns, and MRI are often utilized in cases of known or
while those that have active sleep-wake cycles and suspected toxic exposure. Given the nature of
variation of pattern tend to have much better out- possible neurotoxin effects, however, functional
comes (Sironi, 1983). The amount of QEEG slow- neuroimaging procedures are likely to be of
ing corresponds to reaction level scale scores in greater validity in the detecting of neuro-
comatose individuals (Matousek et al., 1996). The physiological dysfunction.
prognostic efficacy of QEEG spectral analysis has Measures of neurotoxins in blood, urine, or
been compared to that of the Glasgow Coma Scale occasionally hair may be more useful in confirm-
(GCS; Teasdale & Jennett, 1974), brainstem audi- ing exposure if available within a period of recent
tory evoked potentials, and CT in individuals in exposure such that they would still be present.
coma within 3 weeks of TBI (Thatcher et al., 1991), Examples of these are carboxyhemoglobin levels,
although generalizability is limited (Ricker & blood lead levels, and 24-hour urinary mercury
Zafonte, 2000). and arsenic (Armstrong, 2004). Advanced func-
There has been less systematic evaluation of tional brain imaging tests are generally non-
the efficacy of QEEG in discriminating individu- specific and are more useful in ruling out other
als with mild TBI from controls, and there remains causes of cognitive dysfunction, for example, mass
some question as to the clinical utility of this lesions or vascular changes.
brain-mapping approach in this type of differen- Some toxic exposures (e.g., those from sol-
tial diagnosis (Hagglund & Persson, 1990; vents) are associated with a nonspecific finding of
Thatcher et al., 1989). In fact, although QEEG can cortical atrophy on CT. CT alone may not be con-
demonstrate impressive statistical discriminant sidered sensitive to early subclinical signs (Triebig
functions in mild brain injury, it has been demon- & Lang, 1993) and may be most useful to rule
strated that it may actually have an inferior hit out mass lesions. Structural MRI (i.e., T1- and
rate relative to traditional clinical diagnosis T2-weighted) may be useful to identify white-
(Ricker & Zafonte, 2000; Nuwer et al., 2005). matter changes, especially after solvent exposure.
While still certainly holding promise, it can be MRI has been shown to be more sensitive to chronic
concluded that, at present, QEEG remains more cortical effects of solvent exposure than CT (Filley,
of a research tool than a diagnostic instrument in 1999; Myint, 1995; Rosenberg et al., 1988).
brain trauma. The Therapeutics and Technology Regional cerebral blood flow studies using
Assessment Subcommittee of the American SPECT or PET have been used to demonstrate
170 forensic neuropsychology

functional correlates of abnormal neuropsy- (Batts, 2009; Brower & Price, 2001). The nature of
chological test results in occupational exposure these findings is not specific, however. Such find-
cases with various etiologies. These approaches ings may be neurodevelopmental, or they may be
may not be very helpful in distinguishing neuro- acquired through brain injury or substance
toxic from other abnormalities at present, however abuse.
(Armstrong, 2004). PET or SPECT studies of recep- There have been several studies that have used
tor binding may be of greater utility in demonstrat- SPECT to examine aggressive and antisocial
ing receptor system-specific effects of toxins (Edling behavior (e.g., Hirono et al., 2000; Kuruoglu et al.,
et al., 1997). Cognitive activation paradigms have 1996; Amen et al., 1996). Each study has demon-
been applied to persons with solvent exposure. strated some level of decreased blood flow in the
Using O-15 PET, Haut and colleagues (2000) dem- frontal and temporal lobes (although there is
onstrated regional activation differences during a variability of blood flow differences within lobes
working memory task in persons exposed to across studies). As with TBI, however, such
solvents. Activation differences were suggestive of a finding is not, in and of itself, pathognomonic
impaired frontal lobe activity with increased for a propensity toward violence, and may be the
recruitment of other brain regions during task result of a variety of other factors. Of course, one
performance, a finding consistent with other must also consider the possibility that genuine
clinical populations during working memory tasks, frontal lobe dysfunction may result in disinhibi-
but not specific to solvent exposure. tion, emotional disruption, and deficient judg-
There is a growing utilization of event-related ment, which may contribute to some aspects of
potentials, especially in the experimental litera- violent or criminal behavior.
ture (Reinhardt et al., 1997). P300 and N250 laten- PET studies have contrasted forensic psychiat-
cies and P200 amplitude indicated significant ric patients with controls. Some studies have dem-
differences between solvent-exposed painters and onstrated decreased frontal blood flow and
controls (Morrow et al., 1992). Visual evoked metabolism associated with violent behavior
potentials have been suggested being susceptible (Volkow & Tancredi, 1987; Volkow et al., 1995).
to alteration by exposure to solvents, metals, pes- A PET investigation of “impulsive aggression”
ticides, and other neurotoxins such as carbon with patients with personality disorders (chiefly
monoxide (Urban & Lukas, 1990). antisocial, borderline, and narcissistic) showed
decreased anterior medial and left anterior orbito-
CRIMINAL FORENSICS frontal metabolism, which correlated with higher
In the same manner that neuropsychological test- self-ratings of aggression (Goyer et al., 1994).
ing is used in the determination of mitigating Raine and colleagues (1997) presented PET
brain-based factors in violence or even murder, data from 41 individuals charged with murder or
functional neuroimaging is also increasingly used manslaughter. Compared with controls matched
to defend individuals that have been charged with for age, gender, and previous psychiatric diagno-
such criminal activity. Functional neuroimaging sis where applicable, individuals charged with
is also used in cases where a defendant has been murder or manslaughter demonstrated (as a
convicted but mitigating factors might result in a group) statistically significant bifrontal hypome-
reduction in penalty (e.g., avoidance of the death tabolism while performing a cognitive task known
penalty). As has been pointed out by others to activate prefrontal cortex. A later examination
(Intrator et al., 1997; Mayberg, 1996; Raine et al., of these data (Raine et al., 1998b) involved sepa-
1998; Raine & Yang, 2006), and should be quite rating this sample into either a “predatory” (oper-
obvious to any reader of this chapter, one must ationalized as controlled, purposeful aggression)
use great caution before inferring positive brain or “affective” (operationalized as impulsive, emo-
imaging findings as mitigating criminal behavior. tionally charged aggression) category. “Affective”
The overwhelming majority of individuals that murderers demonstrated lower prefrontal metab-
demonstrate a positive finding in a neuroimaging olism compared to controls. Predatory murderers
study will have no history or crime or violence, actually resembled controls in terms of frontal
nor will they be at any increased probability of metabolism.
ever engaging in such behaviors. Most imaging studies of violent or criminal
Structural and functional neuroimaging stud- behavior seem to demonstrate an association
ies of aggressive and violent individuals have gen- between aggression and prefrontal hypoactivity
erally demonstrated frontal lobe abnormalities (Bufkin & Luttrell, 2005). Most studies in this
Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic Neuropsychology 171

arena focus on bifrontal (specifically prefrontal) potentials studies by Rosenfeld, e.g., Rosenfeld,
decreases in activity, but there are often other Ellwanger, & Sweet, 1995), this line of inquiry has
regions of hypoactivity as well. Inconsistencies in seen rapid growth in the cognitive and social
findings are likely attributable to variability in neuroscience literatures with advances in func-
experimental design and selection of participants. tional neuroimaging, and has received a great deal
The finding of prefrontal hypoactivity cannot be of attention in forensic circles (Moriarty, 2008)
interpreted as a chronic substrate for uncontrol- and the popular media (Adler, 2008). In fact, at
lable violent behavior, however, as such a finding least two private companies in the US offer com-
can be experimentally induced. For example, in a mercial “lie detection” services through the use of
PET study of healthy volunteers, Pietrini and fMRI (Greeley & Iles, 2007).
colleagues asked participants to imagine a sce- The first fMRI studies to experimentally
nario involving their own aggressive behavior. examine deception utilized tasks that required
Such imagery was associated with significant focal “simple” deceptive responses (e.g., lying about
reductions in ventromedial frontal blood flow, whether one has a particular playing card in one’s
compared to those elicited by neutral scenarios. possession). These studies have demonstrated
Thus, frontal lobe hypoactivity may be more increased prefrontal and parietal activity when
associated with affective changes that may be participants are lying (Ganis et al., 2003; Kozel
associated with aggression, but these do not et al., 2004; Langleben et al., 2002; Lee et al., 2002;
necessarily represent an inability to avoid com- Nunez et al., 2005; Spence et al., 2001). This has
mitting an act of violence (Mayberg, 1996). been interpreted as an “inhibition of truth” medi-
With reference to competency, the contribu- ated by prefrontal cortex, whereas true responses
tion of functional imaging is less clear (in com- are seen as being mediated by posterior brain
parison to the examination of possible mitigating structures (Spence et al., 2004).
factors), and might in fact be inappropriate. More recently, the focus of this literature has
Competency to stand trial, as one example, is expanded from simply looking at the neural
based upon a multitude of factors related more to correlates of deception in group data toward using
cognitive capacity and behavior rather than a yes/ fMRI to attempt to differentiate lies from truth in
no decision as to the presence or absence of a individuals. Langleben and colleagues (2005) used
change in the brain. A defendant might certainly fast event-related fMRI in a forced-choice
have a previous brain injury or chronic neurologic paradigm and examined individual changes in
illness (e.g., seizure disorder), but could still be brain activation using receiver operator charac-
quite competent to participate in trial proceed- teristic (ROC) curves. They reported being able to
ings and to ultimately be held accountable for his discriminate deception from true responding for
or her actions. A very detailed and thoughtful single events at an accuracy of 78%, while the
examination of these factors is presented in this predictive ability expressed as the area under the
volume in the chapters by Denney. Finally, Patel, ROC curve was 85%. Critics of the use of fMRI as
Meltzer, Mayberg, and Levine (2007) have a “lie detection” technology note that experimen-
discussed many methodological problems in this tal tasks do not mimic real-world scenarios in
literature, including an absence of prospective which persons might be motivated to lie, for
data, small sample sizes, or inadequate controls. example, for financial gain or to avoid a negative
In particular, there are factors than can certainly consequence (Greely & Illes, 2007).
impact functional imaging that are not taken Although not asked to determine if a state-
into account in many studies, including poverty, ment by a patient is an outright lie, forensic
abuse, violence, emotional disruption (either pre- neuropsychologists are routinely asked to use psy-
existing or in reaction to involvement with the chometric tests to evaluate effort and the potential
criminal justice system), and substance misuse. for embellishment of cognitive impairment. Thus,
a potential topic of interest would be to examine
Using Functional Neuroimaging the neural correlates of performance on symptom
to Study Deception validity paradigms that are used to measure
In recent years, there has been increased emphasis effort. Allen et al. (2007) recently administered
on the use of functional brain imaging to the delayed recognition component of the Word
detect the cerebral correlates of deception. Memory Test (Green, 2003) and demonstrated
Although the search for a neurobiological “sub- that performance on the WMT was associated
strate” of deception is not new (see event-related with changes in brain activation in regions of
172 forensic neuropsychology

dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and parietal lobes, ready for routine deployment in forensic
regions which are well known to be active during evaluations (Batts, 2009). This should not be mis-
even simple cognitive task performance. While interpreted as a negative statement, however.
the authors conclude that their findings cast In fact, it remains the opinion and expectation of
doubt on the WMT as a symptom validity test, this the author that functional neuroimaging will
conclusion was premature. The study examined eventually achieve the same level of applicability
four healthy men in their 20s. No patient popula- in forensic contexts that neuropsychological
tion was examined and, more importantly, there assessment has already achieved. As with neuro-
was no dissimulation condition presented against psychological assessment, this will not occur
which one could contrast all participants’ error- overnight, nor will it occur solely from the publi-
free performances. Such a dissimulation condition cation of a handful of case studies or small inves-
is critical, as more recent fMRI studies have dem- tigations drawn only from pre-selected personal
onstrated that cerebral activity elicited by honest injury or criminal forensic cases. It will occur after
recall errors is different from activity associated years of additional systematic research, first
with intentionally feigned memory impairment. with many independent samples of clinical popu-
For example, Lee et al. (2009) used fMRI to char- lations, and then in studies that directly compare
acterize cerebral activity in ten men who were clinical populations with litigating populations.
examined using a forced-choice word list recogni- This is the standard that has been successfully
tion paradigm. Participants were examined in a followed in forensic neuropsychology, and there is
truthful recognition condition (i.e., answer as no reason to hold functional neuroimaging
accurately as possible) and also in a condition in techniques to a lower standard. In addition, this
which they were instructed: “You are required to standard is not solely restricted to forensic appli-
feign a memory problem and deliberately do badly cations, as well-designed and controlled studies of
on the test. Imagine a scenario which envisages functional imaging can only serve to improve
that a poor result will lead to an attractive sum of diagnosis and treatment for all clinical popula-
money as compensation for your memory prob- tions, regardless of the context or setting in which
lem. You should fake skillfully to avoid detection” such studies are requested.
(Lee et al., 2009; p. 407). The findings demon-
strated that intentionally faked responses were REFERENCES
associated with significant activation in left vent- Abdel-Dayem, H., Sade, K., Kouris, K., Bahar, R. H.,
rolateral prefrontal cortex, right posterior cingu- Higazi, I., Eriksson, S., et al. (1987). Changes in
late, and left precuneus. No significant activations cerebral perfusion after acute head injury: Comp-
were observed for errors during the truthful arison of CT with Tc-99 HMPAO SPECT. Radiology,
responding condition. The authors conclude that 165, 221–26.
faking memory impairment is actually a more Adams, B. L., Warneke, L. B., McEwan, A. J. B., &
cognitively demanding process than responding Fraser, B. A. (1993). Single photon emission com-
truthfully, and that it requires different cerebral puterized tomography in obsessive compulsive dis-
substrates, namely those associated with inhibited order: A preliminary study. Journal of Psychiatry
a correct response. While compelling and well and Clinical Neuroscience, 18(3), 109–112.
Adler, M. (2008, January 14). Neuro Law: The New
designed, these findings will need replication by
Frontier. National Public Radio.
other groups, and application with clinical popu-
Aguirre, G. K. (2006). In M. D’Esposito (Ed.),
lations with bona fide acquired brain injury.
Functional MRI: Applications in clinical neurology
and psychiatry (pp. 9–23). Boca Raton, FL: Taylor
FUTURE RESEARCH & Francis.
The past decade has yielded tremendous advances Alster, J., Pratt, H., & Feinsod, M. (1993). Density
in functional neuroimaging technologies and spectral array, evoked potentials, and temperature
approaches to data analysis. In addition, there rhythms in the evaluation and prognosis of
have been exploratory studies in several clinical the comatose patient. Brain Injury, 7(3),
populations, including populations that might 191–208.
be encountered in the context of forensic neuro- Allen, M. D., Bigler, E. D., Larsen, J., Goodrich-
psychological practice. In spite of these techno- Hunsaker, N. J., & Hopkins, R. O. (2007). Functional
logical advances, these techniques remain neuroimaging evidence for high cognitive effort on
investigational in most clinical populations, the Word Memory Test in the absence of external
and do not, from a scientific perspective, appear incentives. Brain Injury, 21(13–14), 1425–28.
Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic Neuropsychology 173

Amen, D. G., Stubblefield, M., & Carmichael, B. (1996). (Eds.), Functional MRI (pp. 103–112). Berlin:
Brain SPECT findings and aggressiveness. Annals Springer-Verlag.
of Clinical Psychiatry, 8, 129–37. Chen, J. J., Wieckowska, M., Meyer, E., & Pike, G. B.
American College of Radiology. (2008). American (2008). Cerebral blood flow measurement using
College of Radiology Appropriateness Criteria: fMRI and PET: A cross-validation study. International
Head Trauma. Available at www.acr.org/ac. Journal of Biomedical Imaging, 2008: 516359.
Armstrong, A. (2004). Neuropsychological differential Christodoulou, C., DeLisa, J., Ricker, J. H., Madigan,
diagnosis of toxic exposure. In J. H. Ricker (Ed.), N., Bly, B. M., Lange, G., et al. (2001). Functional
Differential diagnosis in adult neuropsychological magnetic resonance imaging of working memory
assessment. New York: Springer. impairment following traumatic brain injury.
Bagic, A. & Sato, S. (2007). Principles of electroenceph- Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
alography and magnetoencephalography. In F. G. 71, 161–68.
Hillary & J. DeLuca (Eds.), Functional neuroimag- Duara, R., Barker, W. W., Chang, J., Yoshii, F., Lowenstein,
ing in clinical populations (Chapter 3; pp. 71–96). D. A., & Pascal, S. (1992). Viability of neocortical
New York: Guilford Press. function shown in behavioral activation state PET
Baron, J. C. (1995). Étude de la neuroanatomie functio- studies in Alzheimer disease. Journal of Cerebral
nelle de la perception par la tomographie à posi- Blood Flow and Metabolism, 12(6), 927–34.
tons. Revue Neurologique, 151, 511–517. Edling, C., Hellman, B., Arvidson, B., Andersson, J.,
Batts, S. (2009). Brain lesions and their implications in Hartvig, P., Lilja, A., et al. (1997). Do organic sol-
criminal responsibility. Behavioral Science and vents induce changes in the dopaminergic system?
Law, 27(2), 261–72. Positron emission tomography studies of occupa-
Bergsneider, M., Hovda, D., & Shalmon, E. (1997). tionally exposed subjects. International Archives of
Cerebral hyperglycolysis following severe trau- Occupational and Environmental Health, 70(3),
matic brain injury in humans: A positron emission 180–86.
tomography. Journal of Neurosurgery, 86, 241–51. Filley, C. M. (1999). Toxic leukoencephalopathy.
Bergsneider M, Hovda DA, McArthur DL, Etchpare Clinical Neuropharmacology, 22(5), 249–60.
M., Huang S.C, Sehati N., et al. (2001). Metabolic Fontaine, A., Azouvi, P., Remy, P., Bussel, B., & Samson,
recovery following human traumatic brain injury Y. (1999). Functional anatomy of neuropsychologi-
based on FDG-PET: Time course and relationship cal deficits after severe traumatic brain injury.
to neurological disability. Journal of Head Trauma Neurology, 53, 1963–68.
Rehabilitation, 16(2), 135–48. Ganis, G., Kosslyn, S. M., Stose, S., Thompson, W. L., &
Bricolo, A., Turazzi, S., & Faccioli, F. (1978). Clinical Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2003): Neural correlates of
application of compressed spectral array in long- different types of deception: An fMRI investiga-
term EEG monitoring of comatose patients. tion. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 830–36.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neuro- Goldstein, A. S. (1967). The insanity defense. New
physiology, 45, 211–25. Haven: Yale University Press.
Brower, M.C. & Price, B.H. (2001). Neuropsychiatry of Goyer, P. F., Andreason, P. J., & Semple, W. E. (1994).
frontal lobe dysfunction in violent and criminal Positron emission tomography and personality
behaviour: A critical review. Journal of Neurology, disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 10, 21–28.
Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 71, 720–26. Granacher, R. P. (2008). Applications of functional
Buckner, R. L. & Logan, J. M. (2001). Functional neu- neuroimaging to civil litigation of mild traumatic
roimaging methods: PET and FMRI. In R. Cabeza brain injury. Journal of the American Academy of
& A. Kingstone (Eds.), Handbook of functional Psychiatry and Law, 36, 323–28.
neuroimaging of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Greely, H. T. & Illes, J. (2007). Neuroscience based lie
Press. detection: The urgent need for regulation. American
Bufkin, J. L. & Luttrell, V. R. (2005). Neuroimaging Journal of Law & Medicine, 33, 377–431.
studies of aggressive and violent behavior: current Green, P. (2003). Manual for the Word Memory Test.
findings and implications for criminology and Edmonton: Green Publishing.
criminal justice. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 6(2), Gross, H., Kling, A., Henry, G., Herndon, C., &
176–91. Lavretsky, H. (1996). Local cerebral glucose metab-
Cantor, D. S. (1999). An overview of quantitative EEG. olism in patients with long-term behavioral and
In J. R. Evans & A. Abarbanel (Eds.), Introduction cognitive deficits following mild head injury.
to quantitative EEG and neurofeedback (pp. 3–27). Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuro-
San Diego: Academic Press. science, 8(3), 324–34.
Chen, W. & Ogawa, S. (2000). Principles of BOLD Gulyas, B. & Sjoholm, N. (2007). Principles of positron
functional MRI. In C. T. Moonen & P. A. Bandettini emission tomography. In F. G. Hillary & J. DeLuca
174 forensic neuropsychology

(Eds.), Functional neuroimaging in clinical popula- blood flow and the Hamilton Rating Scale for
tions (Chapter 1; pp. 3–30). New York: Guilford. Depression in mood disorders: A study using
Habert, M. O., Horn, J. F., Sarazin, M., Lotterie, J. A., 123-iodoamphetamine single photon emission
Puel, M., Onen, F., et al. (2011). Brain perfusion computerized tomography. Clinical Psychiatry,
SPECT with an automated quantitative tool can 37(9), 951–58.
identify prodromal Alzheimer’s disease among Ingvar, G. H. & Risberg, J. (1965). Influence of mental
patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neuro- activity upon regional blood flow in man. Acta
biology of Aging, 32(1), 15–23. Neurologica Scandinavica, 14, 183–86.
Habert, M. O. & Huberfeld, G. (2008). Ictal single Intrator, J., Hare, R., Stritzke, P., Brichtswein, K.,
photon computed tomography and SISCOM: Dorfman, D., Harpur, T., et al. (1997). A brain imag-
Methods and utility. Neurochirurgie, 54(3), 226–30. ing (single photon emission computerized tomog-
Hagglund, Y. & Persson, H. (1990). Does Swedish ama- raphy) study of semantic and affective processing in
teur boxing lead to chronic brain damage? Acta psychopaths. Biological Psychiatry, 42(2), 96–103
Neurologica Scandinavica, 82, 353–60. Ishikawa, T. (2002). Brain regions activated during the
Haut, M. W., Leach, S., Kuwabara, H., Whyte, S., mandibular movement tasks in functional mag-
Callahan, T., Ducatman, A., et al. (2000). Verbal netic resonance imaging. Kokubyo Gakkai Zasshi,
working memory and solvent exposure: A positron 69, 39–48.
emission tomography study. Neuropsychology, 14, Jacobs, A., Put, E., Ingels, M., & Bossuyt, A.
551–58. (1994). Prospective evaluation of Technetium 99m
Herholz, K., Carter, S. F., & Jones, M. (2007). Positron HMPAO-SPECT in mild and moderate traumatic
emission tomography imaging in dementia. British brain injury. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 35, 942–47.
Journal of Radiology, 80(2), 160–67. Jansen, H. M. L., van der Naalt, J., & van Zomeren,
Herscovitch, P. (1996). Functional brain imaging: Basic A. H. (1996). Cobalt-55 positron emission tomog-
principles and application to head trauma. In M. raphy in traumatic brain injury: A pilot study.
Rizzo & D. Tranel (Eds.), Head injury and the post- Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry,
concussive syndrome (pp. 89–118). New York: 60(2), 221–24.
Churchill Livingstone. Kado, H., Komuro, T., Shimogawara, M., & Uehara, G.
Hertzman, M., Reba, R. C., & Kotlyarov, E. V. (1990). (1996). Development of a multi-channel SQUID
Single photon emission computed tomography in system for magnetoencephalogram applications.
phencyclidine and related drug abuse. American Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophys-
Journal of Psychiatry, 147(2), 255–56. iology, 47, 405–15.
Hirono, N., Mega, M., & Dinov, I. (2000). Left fronto- King, D. W., Park, Y. D., Smith, J. R., & Wheless, J. W.
temporal hypoperfusion is associated with aggres- (2000). Magnetoencephalography in neocortical
sion in patients with dementia. Archives of epilepsy. Advances in Neurology, 84, 415–23.
Neurology, 57, 861–66. Kozel, F. A., Padgett, T. M., & George, M. S. (2004).
Hoffman, D. A., Lubar, J. F., Thatcher, R. W., Sterman, A replication study of the neural correlates of
M. B., Rosenfeld, P. J., Striefel, S., et al. (1999). deception. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 852–56.
Limitations of the American Academy of Neuro- Krystal, J. H., Woods, S.W., Kosten, T. R, & Rosen, M.
logy and American Clinical Neurophysiology I. (1995). Opiate dependence and withdrawal:
Society paper on QEEG. Journal of Neuropsychiatry Preliminary assessment using single photon
and Clinical Neuroscience, 11(3), 401–407. emission computerized tomography (SPECT).
Holman, B. L. & Devous, M. D. (1992). Functional American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 21(1),
brain SPECT: The emergence of a powerful clinical 47–63.
method. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 33(10), Kuruoglu, A. C., Arikan, Z., Vural, G., Karatas, M.,
1888–1904. Arac, M., & Isik, E. (1996). Single photon emission
Huettel, S. A., Song, A. W., & McCarthy, G. (2009). computerized tomography in chronic alcoholism.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (2nd ed). British Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 348–54.
Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. Langfitt, T. W., Obrist, W. D., Alavia, A., Grossman, R.
Ichise, M., Chung, D. G., Wang, P., Wortzman, G., Gray, I., Zimmerman, R., Jaggi, J., et al. (1986).
B. G., & Franks, W. (1994). Technetium-99m- Computerized tomography, magnetic resonance
HMPAO SPECT, CT, and MRI in the evaluation imaging, and positron emission tomography in the
of patients with chronic traumatic brain injury: study of brain trauma. Journal of Neurosurgery, 64,
A correlation with neuropsychological perfor- 760–67.
mance. Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 35, 1217–26. Langleben, D. D., Schroeder, L., Maldjian, J. A., Gur, R.
Iidaka, T., Nakajima, T., Ogikubo, T., & Fukuda, H. C., McDonald, S., Ragland, J. D., et al. (2002).
(1995). Correlations between regional cerebral Brain activity during simulated deception: An
Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic Neuropsychology 175

event-related functional magnetic resonance study. Mayberg, H. (1996). Medical-legal inferences from
Neuroimage, 15(3), 727–32. functional neuroimaging evidence. Seminars in
Langleben, D. D., Loughead, J. W., Bilker, W. B., Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 1, 195–201.
Ruparel, K., Childress, A. R., Busch, S. I., et al. McAllister, T. W., Saykin, A. J., Flashman, L. A.,
(2005). Telling truth from lie in individual subjects Sparling, M. B., Johnson, S. C., Guerin, S. J., et al.
with fast event-related fMRI. Human Brain (1999). Brain activation during working memory 1
Mapping, 26(4), 262–72. month after mild traumatic brain injury: A func-
Lazarus, A. & Cotterell, K. P. (1989). SPECT scan reveals tional MRI study. Neurology, 53, 1300–1308.
abnormality in somatization disorder patient. McAllister, T. W., Sparling, M. B., Flashman, L. A.,
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 50(12), 75–476. Guerin, S. J., Mamourian, A. C., & Saykin, A. J.
Lee, T. M., Liu, H. L., Tan, L. H., Chan, C. C., Mahankali, (2001). Differential working memory load effects
S., Feng, C. M., et al. (2002). Lie detection by func- after mild traumatic brain injury. NeuroImage, 14,
tional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain 1004–1012.
Mapping, 15, 157–64. Miller, B. L., Mina, I., Giombetti, R., & Villanueva-
Lee, T. M., Au, R. K. C., Liu, H-L., Ting, K. H., Huang, Meyer, J. (1992). Neuropsychiatric effects of
C. M., & Chan, C. C. H (2009). Are errors differen- cocaine: SPECT measurements. Journal of Addictive
tiable from deceptive responses when feigning Disorders, 11(4), 47–58.
memory impairment? An fMRI study. Brain and Modell, J. G. & Mountz, J. M. (1995). Focal cerebral
Cognition, 69, 406–412. blood flow change during craving for alcohol mea-
Levine, B., Cabeza, R., McIntosh, A. R., Black, S. E., sured by SPECT. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
Grady, C. L., & Stuss, D. T. (2002). Functional reor- Clinical Neurosciences, 7(1), 15–22.
ganisation of memory after traumatic brain injury: Moriarty, J. C. (2008). Flickering admissibility:
A study with H2–150 positron emission tomogra- Neuroimaging evidence in the U.S. courts
phy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Behavioral Science and the Law, 26, 29–49.
Psychiatry, 73(2), 173–81. Morrow, L. A., Steinhauer, S. R., & Hodgson, M. J.
Loutfi, I. & Singh, A. (1995). A comparison of quanti- (1992). Delay in P300 latency in patients with
tative methods for brain single photon emission organic solvent exposure. Archives of Neurology,
computed tomography analysis in head trauma 49, 315–20.
and stroke. Investigative Radiology, 30, 588–94. Mozley, P. D., Kim, H. J., & Gur, R. C. (1996). Iodine-
Luccas, F. J., Anghinah, R., Braga, N. I., Fonseca, L. C., 123-IPT SPECT imaging of CNS dopamine trans-
Frochtengarten, M. L., Jorge, M. S., et al. (1999). porters: Nonlinear effects of normal aging on
Guidelines for recording/analyzing quantitative striatal uptake values. Journal of Nuclear Medicine,
EEG and evoked potentials. Arquivas Neuro- 37(12), 1965–70.
psiquiatras, 57(1), 132–46. Müller, R-A. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of
Ludwig, C., Chicherio, C., Terraneo, L., Magistretti, P., developmental disorders. In F. G. Hillary & J.
de Ribaupierre, A., & Slosman, D. (2008). DeLuca (Eds.), Functional neuroimaging in clinical
Functional imaging studies of cognition using populations (Chapter 6; pp 145–84). New York:
99mTc-HMPAO SPECT: Empirical validation using Guilford.
the n-back working memory paradigm. European Myint, R. T. (1995). Occurrence of abnormal magnetic
Journal of Nuclear Medicine and Molecular Imaging, resonance imaging of brain among employees
35(4), 695–703. exposed to mixed solvents, heavy metals and pesti-
Mampunza, S., Verbanck, P., Verhas, M., & Martin, P. cides. In D. E. Hartman. (Ed.), Neuropsychological
(1995). Cerebral blood flow in just detoxified alco- toxicology, (Second Edition). New York: Plenum
hol dependent patients: A 99mTc-HMPAO-SPECT Press.
study. Acta Neurologica Belgica, 95(3), 164–69. Newsome, M. R., Scheibel, R. S., Steinberg, J. L.,
Masdeu, J. C. & Brass, L. M. (1995). SPECT imaging Troyanskaya, M., Sharma, R. G., Rauch, R. A., et al.
of stroke. Journal of Neuroimaging, 5(Suppl. 1), (2007). Working memory brain activation follow-
14–22. ing severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex, 43(1),
Masdeu, J. C. & Arbizu, J. (2008). Brain single photon 95–111.
emission computed tomography: Technological Nunez, J. M., Casey, B. J., Egner, T., Hare, T., & Hirsch,
aspects and clinical applications. Seminars in J. (2005): Intentional false responding shares neural
Neurology, 28(4), 423–34. substrates with response conflict and cognitive
Matousek, M., Takuchi, I., Starmark, J., & Stalhammer, control. Neuroimage, 25, 267–77.
D. (1996). Quantitative EEG analysis as a supple- Nuwer, M. R. (1990). On the controversies about clini-
ment to the clinical coma scale. Acta Anaesthe- cal use of EEG brain mapping. Brain Tomography,
siologica Scandinavica, 40, 824–31. 3(1), 103–111.
176 forensic neuropsychology

Nuwer, M. (1994). Assessment of digital EEG, quanti- deprivation. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and
tative EEG, and EEG brain mapping. Report of the Behavioral Neurology, 11, 1–7.
American Academy of Neurology and the Raine, A. & Yang, Y. (2006). Neural foundations
American Clinical Neurophysiology Society. to moral reasoning and antisocial behavior.
Neurology, 49, 277–92. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(3),
Nuwer, M. R., Hovda, D. A., Schrader, L. M., & Vespa, 203–13.
P. M. (2005). Routine and quantitative EEG in mild Rao, N., Turski, P. A., Polcyn, R. E., Nickels, R. J.,
traumatic brain injury. Clinical Neurophysiology, Matthews, C. G., & Flynn, M. M. (1984). 18F posi-
116(9), 2001–25. tron emission computed tomography in closed
Ogawa, M., Tsukada, H., Hatano, K., Ouchi, Y., Saji, H., head injury. Archives of Physical Medicine &
& Magata, Y. (2009). Central in vivo nicotinic ace- Rehabilitation, 65, 780–85.
tylcholine receptor imaging agents for positron Read, S. L., Miler, B. L., Mena, I., & Kim, R. (1995).
emission tomography (PET) and single photon SPECT in dementia: Clinical and pathological cor-
emission computed tomography (SPECT). relation. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society,
Biological Pharmacology Bulletin, 32(3), 337–40. 42(11), 1243–47.
Patel, P., Meltzer, C. C., Mayberg, H. S., & Levine, K. Reinhardt, F., Drexler, H., Bickel, A., Claus, D., Ulm,
(2007). The role of imaging in United States court- K., Angerer, J., et al. (1997). Electrophysiological
rooms. Neuroimaging Clinics of North America, investigation of central, peripheral and autonomic
17(4), 557–67. nerve function in workers with long-term low-
Paulman, R. G., Devous, M. D., & Gregory, R. R. (1990). level exposure to carbon disulphide in the viscose
Hypofrontality and cognitive impairment in industry. International Archives of Occupational
schizophrenia: Dynamic single photon tomogra- and Environmental Health, 70(4), 249–56.
phy and neuropsychological assessment of schizo- Ricker, J. H., Müller, R-A., Zafonte, R. D., Black, K.,
phrenic brain function. Biological Psychiatry, 27, Millis, S. R., & Chugani, H. (2001). Verbal recall
377–99. and recognition following traumatic brain injury:
Perlstein, W. M., Cole, M. A., Demery, J. A., Seignourel, A [15O]-water positron emission tomography
P. J., Dixit, N. K., Larson, M. J., et al. (2004). study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro-
Parametric manipulation of working memory load psychology, 23(2), 196–206.
in traumatic brain injury: Behavioral and neural Ricker, J. H. & Arenth, P. M. (2008). Functional
correlates. Journal of the International neuroimaging in clinical neuropsychology. In J. E.
Neuropsychological Society, 10(5), 724–41. Morgan & J. H. Ricker (Eds.), Textbook of clinical
Pietrini, P., Guazzelli, M., & Basso, G. (2000). Neural neuropsychology (Chapter 41; pp. 840–47).
correlates of imaginal aggressive behavior assessed New York: Taylor & Francis.
by positron emission tomography in healthy Ricker, J. H. (2005). Functional neuroimaging in
subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, medical rehabilitation populations. In J. DeLisa &
1772–81. B. Gans (Eds.), Rehabilitation medicine (4th ed.).
Ponton, M. O. & Ardila, A. (1999). The future of neu- Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven.
ropsychology with Hispanic populations in the Ricker, J. H. & Zafonte, R. D. (2000). Functional
United States. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, neuroimaging in traumatic head injury: Clinical
14(7), 565–80. applications and interpretive cautions. Journal of
Raichle, M. E. (2001). Functional neuroimaging: A his- Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 15(2), 859–68.
torical and physiological perspective. In R. Cabeza Roland, P. (1993). Brain activation. New York:
& A. Kingstone (Eds.), Handbook of functional Wiley-Liss. 1993.
neuroimaging of cognition (pp. 3–26). Cambridge, Rosenberg, N. L., Spitz, M. C., Filley, C. M., Davis, K.
MA: MIT Press. A., & Schaumberg, H. H. (1988). Central nervous
Raine, A., Buchsbaum, M., & LaCasse, L. (1997). Brain system effects of chronic toluene abuse: Clinical,
abnormalities in murderers indicated by positron brainstem evoked response and magnetic reso-
emission tomography. Biological Psychiatry, 42, nance imaging studies. Neurotoxicology and
495–508. Teratology, 10, 489–95.
Raine, A., Meloy, J. R., & Bihrle, S. (1998). Reduced Rosenfeld, J. P., Ellwanger, J., & Sweet, J. (1995).
prefrontal and increased subcortical brain func- Detecting simulated amnesia with event-related
tioning assessed using positron emission tomogra- brain potentials. International Journal of Psycho-
phy in predatory and affective murderers. physiology, 19(1), 1–11.
Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 16, 319–32. Ruff, R., Crouch, J. A., Tröster, A. I., Marshall, L. F.,
Raine, A., Stoddard, J., & Bihrle, S. (1998). Prefrontal Buchsbaum, M. S., Lottenberg, S. et al. (1994).
glucose deficits in murderers lacking psychosocial Selected cases of poor outcome following a minor
Functional Neuroimaging in Forensic Neuropsychology 177

brain trauma: Comparing neuropsychological and Strangman, G. E., O’Neil-Pirozzi, T. M., Goldstein, R.,
PET assessment. Brain Injury, 8(4), 297–308. Kelkar, K., Katz, D. I., Burke, D., et al. (2008).
Russell, K. C., Arenth, P. M., Scanlon, J. M., Kessler, L. Prediction of memory rehabilitation outcomes in
J., & Ricker, J. H. (2011). An FMRI investigation of traumatic brain injury by using functional mag-
episodic memory after TBI. Journal of Clinical & netic resonance imaging. Archives of Physical
Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(5), 538–47. Medicine and Rehabilitation, 89, 974–81.
Sackheim, H. A., Prohovnik, I. & Moeller, J. Tang, A. C., Pearlmutter, B. A., Malaszenko, N. A.,
(1990). Regional cerebral blood flow in mood dis- Phung, D. B., & Reeb, B. C. (2002). Independent
orders: Comparison of major depressives and components of magnetoencephalography: Locali-
normal controls at rest. Archives of General zation. Neural Computation, 14(8), 1827–58.
Psychiatry, 47, 60–70. Teasdale, G. & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma
Seo, Y., Mari, C., & Hasegawa, B. (2008). Technological and impaired consciousness. Lancet, 2, 81–84.
development and advances in single-photon emis- Tenjin, H., Ueda, S., & Mizukawa, N. (1990). Positron
sion computed tomography/computed tomogra- emission tomographic studies on cerebral hemo-
phy. Seminars in Nuclear Medicine, 38(3), 177–98. dynamics in patients with cerebral contusion.
Sironi, V. (1983). Diagnostic and prognostic value of Neurosurgery, 26, 971–79.
spectral analysis in posttraumatic coma. In R. Thatcher, R. W., Walker, R. A., Gerson, I., & Geisler,
Vilans, (Ed.), Advances in Neurotraumatology (pp. F. H. (1989). EEG discriminant analysis of mild
329–30). Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica. head trauma. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Skrandies, W. (1995). Visual information processing: Neurophysiology, 73, 94–106.
Topography of brain electrical activity. Biological Thatcher, R. W., Cantor, D., & McAlster, R. (1991).
Psychology, 40(1–2), 1–15. Comprehensive predictions of outcome in closed
Small, S. A. (2006). Neurobiological correlates of imag- head injured patients. Annals of the New York
ing. In M. D’Esposito (Ed.), Functional MRI: Academy of Science, 620, 82–101.
Applications in clinical neurology and psychiatry Therapeutics and Technology Subcommittee of the
(pp. 1–8). Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis. American Academy of Neurology. (1996). Assess-
Sohlberg, A., Watabe, H., & Iida, H. (2008). Three- ment of brain SPECT. Neurology, 46, 278–85.
dimensional SPECT reconstruction with transmis- Triebig, G. & Lang, C. (1993). Brain imaging tech-
sion-dependent scatter correction. Annals of niques applied to chronically solvent-exposed
Nuclear Medicine, 22(7), 549–56. workers: Current results and clinical evaluation.
Spence, S. A., Farrow, T. F., Herford, A. E., Wilkinson, I. Environmental Research, 61, 239–50.
D., Zheng, Y., & Woodruff, P. W. (2001). Behavioural Uchiyama, M., Sue, H., Fukumitsu, N., Mori, Y., &
and functional anatomical correlates of deception Kawakami, K. (1997). Assessment of cerebral ben-
in humans. Neuroreport, 12, 2849–53. zodiazepine receptor distribution in anxiety disor-
Spence, S. A., Hunter, M. D., Farrow, T. F., Green, R. D., ders by 123-I iomazenil SPECT. Nippon Acta
Leung, D. H., Hughes, C. J., et al. (2004). A cogni- Radiologica, 57(1), 41–46.
tive neurobiological account of deception: evidence Umile, E. M., Plotkin, R. C., & Sandel, M. E. (1998).
from functional neuroimaging. Philosophical Functional assessment of mild traumatic brain
Transactions of the Royal Society of London– injury using SPECT and neuropsychological
Biological Sciences, 359, 1755–62. assessment. Brain Injury, 12, 577–94.
Springer, C. S., Patlak, C. S., Playka, I., & Huang, W. Urban, P. & Lukas, E. (1990). Visual evoked potentials
(2000). Principles of susceptibility contrast-based in rotogravure printers exposed to toluene. British
functional MRI: The sign of the functional response. Journal of Industrial Medicine, 47, 819–23.
In C. T. Moonen & P. A. Bandettini (Eds.), Functional Volkow, N. D. & Tancredi, L. (1987). Neural substrates
MRI (pp. 91–102). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. of violent behavior: A preliminary study with posi-
Stern, E. & Silbersweig, D. A. (2001). Advances in tron emission tomography. British Journal of
functional neuroimaging methodology for the Psychiatry, 151, 668–73.
study of brain systems underlying human neurop- Volkow, N. D., Tancredi, L. R., & Grant, C. (1995).
sychological function and dysfunction. Journal of Brain glucose metabolism in violent psychiatric
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(1), patients: A preliminary study. Psychiatry Research,
3–18. 61, 243–53.
Strangman, G. E., Goldstein, R., O’Neil-Pirozzi, T. M., Wallace, B. E., Wagner, A. K., Wagner, E. P., &
Kelkar, K., Supelana, C., Burke, D., et al. (2009). McDeavitt, J. T. (2001). A history and review of
Neurophysiological alterations during strategy- quantitative electroencephalography in traumatic
based verbal learning in traumatic brain injury. brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation,
Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 23, 226–36. 16(2), 165–90.
178 forensic neuropsychology

Weisskoff, R. M. (2000). Basic theoretical models of influences on reading disability. Reading and
BOLD signal change. In C. T. Moonen & P. A. Writing, 3(3–4), 379–93.
Bandettini (Eds.), Functional MRI (pp. 115–23). Wortzel, H. S., Filley, C. M., Anderson, C. A., Oster, T.,
Berlin: Springer-Verlag. & Arciniegas, D. B. (2008). Forensic applications
Whitebread, C. H. & Slobogin, C. (1993). Criminal of cerebral single photon emission computed
procedure: An analysis of cases and concepts (third tomography in mild traumatic brain injury. Journal
edition). New York, NY: Foundation Press. of the American Academy of Psychiatry and Law,
Wintermark, M., Sesay, M., Barbier, E., Borbély, K., 36, 310–22.
Dillon, W. P., Eastwood, J. D., et al. (2005). Wu, H. M., Huang, S. C., Hattori, N., Glenn, T. C.,
Comparative overview of brain perfusion imaging Vespa, P. M., Hovda, D. A., et al. (2004). Subcortical
techniques. Journal of Neuroradiology, 32(5), white matter metabolic changes remote from focal
294–314. hemorrhagic lesions suggest diffuse injury after
Wood, F., Flowers, L., Buchsbaum, M., & Tallal, P. human traumatic brain injury. Neurosurgery,
(1991). Investigation of abnormal left temporal 55(6), 1306–15.
functioning in dyslexia through rCBF, auditory Zhang, J. J., Park, C. H., & Kim, S. M. (1994). Brain
evoked potentials, and positron emission tomo- SPECT artifact in multidetector SPECT systems.
graphy. Special issue: Genetic and neurological Clinical Nuclear Medicine, 19(9), 789–91.
7
Perinatal Brain Injury
H . G E R RY TAY L O R

INTRODUCTION brain insults, as well as some of the challenges


The human brain is undergoing rapid matura- involved in conducting these evaluations. The
tional changes during the last trimester of fetal concluding section provides an overview of cur-
development, beginning at 28 weeks’ gestational rent knowledge, highlights important themes, and
age (GA). Studies examining brain growth reveal emphasizes the need for further research.
a four-fold increase in cortical gray matter (GM)
from 29–42 weeks’ GA and a five-fold increase in COMMON FORMS OF
myelinated white matter (WM) from 35–41 weeks’ P E R I N ATA L B R A I N I N S U LT
GA (Ajayi-Obe, Saeed, Cowan, Rutherford, &
Edwards, 2000; Huppi & Inder, 2001; Huppi Very Low Birth Weight/Very Preterm
et al., 1996). This period of development is also Birth (VLBW/VPTB)
characterized by an increase in GM-WM differ- Definition and Epidemiology
entiation, cellular metabolism, and cortical Improvements in neonatal intensive care made
infolding. Birth substantially prior to term GA over the last few decades have resulted in increased
and at a very low birth weight interferes with this survival of children with low birth weight (<2500
growth process, as children born preterm show g, 5 lbs. 8 oz.), comprising 8.2% of the population
reduced GM and WM volumes and less cortical of live births (Hamilton, Martin, & Ventura,
surface area and complexity at term equivalent 2006). Most of these increases have occurred in
age than term-born children (Inder, Warfield, the subset of children with very low birth weight
Wang, Huppi, & Volpe, 2005). Other major causes (VLBW, <1500 g, 3 lbs. 5 oz., 1.5% of births), with
of disruption in early brain development are even more dramatic changes in survival observed
perinatal hypoxic ischemic encephalopathy (HIE) in the smaller subset of children with extremely
and ischemic or hemorrhagic strokes in term or low birth weight (<1000 g, 2 lbs. 3 oz., 0.5% of
near-term children. The present chapter focuses births). Many studies use GA rather than birth
on these conditions as the three most common weight to index the extent of prematurity. Preterm
causes of acquired brain insults during the perin- birth is defined as GA <37 weeks, very preterm
atal period, which extends from 20 weeks’ GA and birth (VPTB) as GA <32 weeks, and extremely
28 days postnatal age (Raju et al., 2007). Other preterm birth as GA <29 weeks. Although either
forms of perinatal brain insult not considered birth weight or GA criteria can be used to define
in this chapter include those caused by brain the degree of prematurity, birth weight may fall
infections (e.g., meningitis, herpes simplex below the level that is expected for GA (e.g., birth
encephalitis), malnutrition, prenatal toxic expo- weight <10% for GA). In these cases, the child is
sures, and genetic diseases or malformations said to be small for GA (SGA) or to have intra-
(e.g., fetal alcohol exposure, spina bifida, neurofi- uterine growth retardation. Because developmen-
bromatosis). tal problems have been widely documented in the
In the first section of this chapter, I summarize children with VLBW or VPTB (Taylor, 2009),
the neuropathological features of these condi- referred to here collectively as children with
tions, their developmental consequences, and VLBW/VPTB, this subset of the broader popula-
factors that place children at risk for poorer out- tion of children born preterm or at a low birth
comes. The second section describes procedures weight is the focus of the present section.
and principles of forensic neuropsychological Most preterm births are spontaneous and of
assessments of children with potential perinatal unknown etiology, though risk factors include
180 forensic neuropsychology

previous preterm deliveries, fertility treatments hypoxic ischemia, with associated deprivation of
and multiple gestations (twins or other multiple oxygen and glucose (Volpe, 2008). Secondary
births), placental or uterine abnormalities, and sources of insult include excitotoxicity related to
pre-eclampsia (Alexander & Slay, 2002; the release of glutamate, the presence of pro-
Goldenberg & Rouse, 1998). In a minority of inflammatory cytokines associated with maternal
cases, delivery is induced because of fetal growth or fetal infection, and free radical production in
restriction or maternal risk factors. The vast response to injury. Because of the presence of
majority of infants born with birth weight <750 g arterial end zones in the periventricular and sub-
now survive with higher mortality among cortical WM and the special sensitivity of WM to
those with birth weight <500 g or with GA secondary damage, this brain region is selectively
<25 weeks (Fanaroff et al., 2007; Hintz, Kendrick, affected. Damage to subplate neurons in this
Vohr, Poole, & Higgins, 2005b; Wilson-Costello, region also adversely affects early neurogenesis.
Friedman, Minich, Fanaroff, & Hack, 2005). WM is more involved than GM, though early
development of GM is also affected, with cortical
Neuropathology and Neonatal atrophy and ischemic/infarctive insults to GM in
Medical Complications the hippocampus, thalamus, basal ganglia, brain
The four major types of neuropathology stem, and cerebellum (Inder et al., 1999; Srinivasan
associated with VLBW/VPTB are germinal et al., 2007; Volpe, 2009). MRIs conducted later in
matrix-intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH), per- childhood and beyond show residual forms of
iventricular hemorrhagic infarction (PHI), neuropathology as evident in reduced volumes of
periventricular leukomalacia (PVL), and post- WM and GM, ventricular dilatation, and intra-
hemorrhagic ventricular dilatation (Volpe, 2008, parenchymal cysts and porencephaly (Allin et al.,
2009). Germinal matrix-IVH is graded from I-III. 2004; Nosarti et al., 2002, 2004). Selective reduc-
Grade I IVH remains within the germinal matrix, tions in volume relative to total brain volume have
grade II IVH extends into the ventricular space, also been documented in the temporal, parietal,
and grade III IVH involves both hemorrhage into and occipital lobes, basal ganglia, corpus callo-
and expansion of this space. PHI is often associ- sum, amygdala, hippocampus, thalamus, and
ated with germinal matrix-IVH but is a larger and cerebellum (Nosarti et al., 2008; Srinivasan et al.,
more asymmetric type of IVH frequently localized 2007). Neonatal medical factors associated with
in the caudothalamic grove (Inder & Volpe, 2000). volume reductions include more extreme VLBW/
Formerly considered a grade IV germinal matrix VPTB and evidence of brain abnormality as man-
IVH, PHI is now recognized as a distinct form of ifest by ventriculomegaly, IVH, PVL, or WM
brain insult that results in extensive damage to injury (Inder, Warfield, Wang, Huppi, & Volpe,
periventricular WM. PVL involves more diffuse 2005; Kessler et al., 2004; Peterson et al., 2000,
and bilateral damage to WM that can entail either 2003). Other medical complications of VLBW/
WM necrosis, referred to as cystic PVL, or damage VPTB that either contribute directly to neuropa-
to oligodendroglial precursors of myelin manifest thology or compromise subsequent brain devel-
as noncystic changes to WM on T2-weighted opment include chronic lung disease (otherwise
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). While only known as bronchopulmonary dysplasia, or BPD),
5–15% of neonates with VLBW show evidence of lack of respiration (apnea), a collapsed lung
PVL on neonatal ultrasounds, this procedure is (pneumothorax), failure of normal closure of a
less sensitive to smaller lesions than MRI and rates pathway between the aorta and pulmonary artery
of noncystic PVL are much higher (Inder, Wells, (patent ductus arteriosis), jaundice (hyperbiliru-
Mogridge, Spencer, & Volpe, 2003). Counsell et al. binemia), infection (sepsis), inflammation/infec-
(2003), for example, found evidence for noncystic tion of the intestines (necrotizing enterocolitis),
WM abnormalities in 68% of a sample of neonates abnormal formation of retinal blood vessels
with a mean birth weight 1086g and mean GA of (retinopathy), and nutritional problems.
29 weeks. Finally, post-hemorrhagic ventricular
dilatation is a rarer form of insult involving hydro- Outcomes
cephalus secondary to necrotic tissue surrounding Medical consequences of VLBW/VPTB include
the ventricles or to malabsorption of cerebral neurological and sensory disorders, such as
spinal fluid. cerebral palsy (CP), vision and hearing problems,
One important determinant of these brain and shunt-dependent hypdrocephalus. CP most
insults is the susceptibility of the preterm brain to typically entails spastic hemiplegia or diplegia,
Perinatal Brain Injury 181

with greater involvement of lower extremities. attention and psychomotor abilities in formal
These impairments are most common in children testing.
with extremely low birth weight, with estimates in By early school age (ages 5–6 years), children
this subset of the VLBW/VPTB population of with VLBW/VPBT begin to display problems in
8–17% for CP, 1–2% for hearing impairment academic learning and are more likely than NBW
requiring hearing aids, and 1–5% for vision children to receive special education services
impairment as defined by a corrected acuity of (Resnick et al., 1999; Saigal, Szatmari, &
worse than 20/200 (Msall, 2006). Other medical Rosenbaum, 1992; Schneider, Wolke, Schlagmuller,
consequences for some children are respiratory & Meyer, 2004). Cognitive deficits and behavior
problems such as asthma, rehospitalizations for problems are also well documented in this age
surgeries or treatments for related medical condi- group (Kilbride, Thorstad, & Daily, 2004; Marlow
tions such as lung disease, and reduced height, et al., 2007; Mikkola et al., 2005; Taylor et al.,
weight, and head circumference relative to age 2000). Adverse effects of VLBW/VPTB on cogni-
standards (Wilson-Costello, 2007). tion, behavior, and learning extend throughout
Administration of neonatal and early child- the school-age years and into adulthood (Hack,
hood behavior scales reveals more abnormalities 2005; Saigal, 2000; Taylor, Minich, Klein, & Hack,
in state control, orientation, and attention in 2004b). A small minority of children with VLBW/
VLBW/VPTB survivors compared with term- VPTB are globally mentally deficient, while the
born normal birth weight (NBW, ≥37 weeks GA, majority has scores on IQ or other tests of general
≤2500g birth weight) controls (Brown et al., cognitive ability that range from borderline to
2009; Rose, Feldman, Rose, Wallace, & McCarton, average levels (Taylor, 2009). Even children with
1992; Wolf et al., 2002). Other early develop- more extreme degrees of VLBW/VPTB have
mental deficits in children with VLBW/VPTB mean scores on these tests in the low average to
compared with NBW controls include less-well- average range (Anderson, Doyle et al., 2003;
developed social and communicative interactions Taylor et al., 2000), demonstrating considerable
with parents and lower scores on tests of executive variability in outcome.
function and motor and language skills Cognitive deficits are most pronounced in the
(Woodward, Edgin, Thompson, & Inder, 2005; domains of executive function, visuomotor skills,
Edgin et al., 2008; Grunau, Kearney, & Whitfield, and memory (Taylor, 2008, 2009). Simple lan-
1990; Landry, Smith, Miller-Loncar, & Swank, guage functions such as naming vocabulary are
1997; Rose, Feldman, & Jankowski, 2005; Rose, relatively intact, though language processing skills
Feldman, Jankowski, & Van Rossem, 2005). are more clearly affected and global language
Children with VLBW/VPTB also obtain lower delays may be more evident in younger children
scores than their NBW peers on the original or (Luu et al., 2009; Luoma, Herrgard, Martikainen,
revised versions of the Bayley Scales of Infant & Ahonen, 1998). Academic problems in VLBW/
Development (BSID) (Hack et al., 2000); rates of VPTB samples relative to NBW control groups are
impairment as defined by scores <70 ranging documented by lower scores on tests of reading,
from 22–37% in children with extremely low birth spelling, math, and writing, and higher rates of
weight (Msall, 2006). Cognitive impairments in grade repetition and special education assistance
areas such as executive function are not fully (Taylor, in press). Learning disabilities are also
accounted for by global developmental delays and more common in children with VLBW/VPTB
can be found even in children with less extreme than in NBW controls, and mathematics skills are
low birth weight (Edgin et al., 2008; Espy et al., more negatively affected than reading or global
2002). Developmental delays in early childhood cognitive abilities (Litt et al., 2005; Taylor, Espy, &
are of questionable value in predicting future cog- Anderson, 2009). Children with VLBW/VPTB
nitive competence. Although severe impairment have more attention difficulties, social problems,
in early childhood is a valid predictor of future internalizing and externalizing symptoms, and
deficits, many children with mild-to-moderate atypical behavior than NBW controls (Anderson
delays function within the average range of intel- et al., 2003; Breslau et al., 1996a; Hoff, Hansen,
ligence in later school-age testing (Hack et al., Munck, & Mortensen, 2004; Indredavik et al.,
2005a). The poor predictive validity of mid-to- 2005; Saigal, Pinelli, Hoult, Kim, & Boyle, 2003).
moderate impairments may reflect neural plastic- Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
ity, limitations in testing methods, or difficulties is especially prevalent relative to base rates, with
in engaging young children with weakness in one study reporting this disorder in 23% of a
182 forensic neuropsychology

VLBW/VLTB sample compared with 6% of NBW that affected work capacity in the individuals with
controls (Botting, Powls, & Cooke, 1997). preterm birth. However, rates of unemployment
Cognitive, achievement, and behavior deficits in a U.S. sample did not differ between young
in VLBW/VPTB cohorts remain relatively con- adults with VLBW and NBW controls (Hack et al.,
stant throughout the school-age years relative to 2004) and adult outcomes of VLBW/VPTB have
similarly aged NBW children (Breslau, Paneth, & in some ways proved to be more positive than
Lucia, 2004; Breslau et al., 2001; Saigal, 2000; anticipated based on the high rates of disabilities
Taylor, 2009, in press). In a longitudinal study that present in this population during the school-age
compared the cognitive development of children years (Saigal & Rosenbaum, 2007; Wilson-
with birth weights <750 g and 750–1499g with Costello, 2007). Differences in educational attain-
NBW controls, our research team found increas- ment and health status are relatively small;
ingly pronounced deficits from mean ages 7–14 most adults with VLBW complete high school,
years in the <750 g group on measures of execu- live independently, and exhibit less risk-taking
tive function and psychomotor skills (Taylor et al., behaviors than their NBW peers.
2004b). We found increasing deficits in one or
both of the VLBW groups on other cognitive Predictors
measures only at higher levels of environmental Several risk factors account for variability in
advantage. This pattern of results suggests either outcomes of VLBW/VPTB. Children with brain
that the negative effects of social disadvantage on abnormalities on neonatal cranial ultrasounds are
children with VLBW/VPTB wane over time or more likely than other survivors of VLBW/VPTB
that children with VLBW/VPTB are less respon- to have neurodevelopmental impairments (Ansel
sive to environmental advantage than NBW chil- et al., 2006; Luu et al., 2009; Sherlock et al., 2005;
dren. For other measures, we found that the gap Vohr et al., 2003; Whitaker et al., 1996, 1997;
between the 750–1499 g group and NBW group Woodward, Anderson, Austin, Howard, & Inder,
narrowed with age, but only for children from 2006). The risk for these impairments is highest in
more disadvantaged families. This second pattern children with grade III germinal matrix-IVH or
of findings suggest some recovery or compensa- PHI (Sherlock, Anderson & Doyle, 2005), whereas
tion over time in the latter subset of children, children with grade I and II germinal matrix-IVH
perhaps because they were able to become more are at some, though lesser, risk (Patra, Wilson-
independent learners or were less constrained by Costello, Taylor, Minich, & Hack, 2006). Accom-
their family environments with increasing age. panying shunt-dependent hydrocephalus is an
Changes over time were nevertheless evident for additional risk factor (Adams-Chapman, Hansen,
only select measures, with the bulk of the assess- Stoll, & Higgins, 2008). Early brain insults as
ments suggesting relatively stable deficits across revealed on MRIs conducted at term equivalent
the school-age years in the <750 g group and with age or later in childhood also predict poorer devel-
the 750–1499 g group exhibiting persisting opmental outcomes (Abernethy, Cooke, & Foulder-
deficits only in motor skills and executive func- Hughes, 2004; Edgin et al., 2008; Inder et al., 2005;
tion. Similar deficits continued to be present at Indredavik et al., 2005; Nosarti et al., 2008; Peterson
mean age 17 years (Taylor, Minich, Bangert, et al., 2000). MRI findings related to poorer out-
Filipek, & Hack, 2004a). comes are structural abnormalities such as PVL,
Compared with same-aged NBW controls, IVH, ventricular enlargement, and reduced WM
young adults with VLBW/VPTB have persisting and GM volumes. However, severe deficits are not
weaknesses in IQ and executive function, lower inevitable, even in children with the most severe
educational attainment, and more health and neonatal brain insults. In their follow-up assess-
behavior problems (Allin et al., 2006; Cooke, ments of 21 preterm children with PHI at 12 years
2004; Hack, 2005; Saigal et al., 2007). Behavior of age, Roze et al. (2009) found 5 children (24%)
problems in the young adults with VLBW include without CP, 8 with IQ scores within the average
internalizing symptoms in women, inattention range (42%), and 9 (53%) with behavior ratings
problems in men, and thought problems in both within the normal range. Similarly, Bassan et al.
sexes (Hack, 2005). Moster, Lie, and Markestad (2007) found that one-third of their sample of
(2008) found that proportionally more adults preterm children with PHI had no detectable
born at 23–27 weeks’ GA in Norway had disability motor or cognitive deficits at 30 months of age.
pensions compared with term-born adults. They The degree of VLBW/VPTB is another
also found a higher rate of medical disabilities important risk factor. Children with more extreme
Perinatal Brain Injury 183

low birth weight or prematurity are more vulner- disadvantaged families (Breslau et al., 1996a;
able to neonatal brain insults and have more pro- Taylor et al., 1998; Landry, Smith, Miller-Loncar,
nounced deficits in cognition, achievement, and & Swank, 1998). Other studies have observed the
behavior (Anderson et al., 2003; Klebanov et al., opposite pattern, with the effects of VLBW/VPTB
1994; Sherlock et al., 2005). To illustrate, our more evident in children from more advantaged
research group found that 63% of children with backgrounds (Taylor et al., 2006). Differences in
<750 g birth weight had a developmental impair- this regard may reflect variations in the type of
ment at middle school age, compared with 38% of outcome assessed (e.g., behavioral adjustment vs.
the 750–1499 g birth weight group and 18% of the cognitive abilities) or in sample characteristics
NBW group (Taylor et al., 2000). Impairment was (Taylor et al., 1998).
defined broadly as a neurological or sensory dis-
order, low IQ or adaptive functioning, an aca- HIE in Term Infants
demic problem, or behavior ratings suggestive of Definition and Epidemiology
ADHD. Adverse effects of VLBW/VPTB on devel- Hypoxic ischemia is the combination of dimin-
opment are also observed in children with less ished oxygen in the blood (hypoxia) with dimin-
extreme VLBW/VPTB, such as the participants in ished blood perfusion (ischemia). These
our study with 750–1499 g birth weight (Taylor conditions deplete glucose to the brain, adversely
et al., 2004b), though deficits in these children affect metabolism, and result in accumulation of
tend to be milder and more selective than those the neurotransmitter glutamate and firing of glu-
observed in cases of more extreme VLBW/VPTB. tamate receptors (Perlman, 1997, 2006; Volpe,
This gradient of sequelae, with milder deficits in 2001, 2008). Causes of hypoxic ischemia include
children with higher birth weight and GA, extends impaired exchange of oxygen and carbon dioxide
throughout the range low birth weight and pre- (asphyxia), lack of respiration, heart failure/mal-
maturity (Breslau, Chilcoat, DelDotto, Andreski, function, and loss of cerebrovascular autoregula-
& Brown, 1996b; Linnet et al., 2006; van Baar, tion. Hypoxic ischemia can occur in the
Vermaas, Knots, de Kleine, & Soons, 2009). antepartum, intrapartum, or postpartum periods
Neonatal medical risks other than identifiable (i.e., before, during, and after labor and delivery).
brain injury are chronic lung disease, treatment Antepartum factors contributing to hypoxic isch-
with postnatal steroids, necrotizing enterocolitis, emia include conditions such as maternal
jaundice, sepsis, apnea, hypothyroxinemia, patent hypotension, maternal diabetes, placental abnor-
ductus arteriosis, and retinopathy of prematurity malities, coagulation disorders during the fetal
(Cooke, 2005; Hack et al., 2000; Hintz et al., 2005a; period, intrauterine growth retardation, uterine
Ishaik, Mirabella, Asztalos, Perlma, & Rovet, hemorrhage or infection, hydrops fetalis (fetal
2000). Longer neonatal hospital stays and greater edema), and congenital malformations (Rolland
numbers of neonatal risks also predict poorer & Hill, 1995). Intrapartum factors are labor events
outcomes (Laptook et al., 2005; Taylor, Klein, such as placental abruption, cord prolapse, meco-
Drotar, Schluchter, & Hack, 2006). Associations nium, persistent late or variable heart rate decel-
of placental abnormalities with later cognitive erations, and mechanical injury. Postpartum
functioning suggest that risks include antenatal as sources are complications such as respiratory dif-
well as neonatal factors (Redline, Minich, Taylor, ficulties and heart or lung malfunction. Infants at
& Hack, 2007). risk for hypoxic ischemia due to events occurring
Outcomes for children with VLBW/VPTB are during labor and delivery frequently have histo-
also associated with characteristics of the home ries of antepartum risks, making it difficult in
and family environment. Lower socioeconomic many cases to sort out the origins of perinatal
status and nonoptimal parenting characteristics brain insult.
are associated with poorer outcomes indepen- The term “HIE” has been inconsistently applied
dently of VLBW/VPTB (Aylward, 2005; Taylor and is at times referred to in the literature as “birth
et al., 2004b; Vohr et al., 2003). While associations asphyxia” or “neonatal encephalopathy.” The latter
of environmental factors with developmental out- term is sometimes used to refer to a wider range of
comes are generally similar in VLBW/VPTB and brain insults at term birth and to avoid the assump-
NBW samples, these factors can also moderate tion of asphyxia as the causal factor in all cases.
the effects of VLBW/VPTB. For example, some Nevertheless, there is reasonable consensus that
studies have found more pronounced effects of asphyxia is a meaningful clinical entity that can
VLBW/VPTB on children from more socially cause encephalopathy in term-born infants and
184 forensic neuropsychology

that it can be identified by the occurrence of three (apoptosis) has a delayed time course and reflects
consecutive events (Dilenge, Majnemer, & Shevell, responses to injury that lead to further brain insult
2001; Perlman, 2006; Shaywitz & Fletcher, 1993; due to the production of glutamate and the related
Robertson & Finer, 1988; Volpe, 2008). The first build-up of calcium, free radicals such as nitric
event is an indication of placental asphyxia as evi- oxide, and inflammatory cytokines, as well as
denced by placental abruption, a prolapsed cord, alternations in cerebral blood flow (Volpe, 2008).
in utero passage of meconium, or abnormal fetal In contrast to the greater effects of prematurity on
heart rate decelerations (as defined relative to end WM, HIE results in more damage to GM. Term-
of uterine contractions). The second event is phys- born infants with HIE may have PVL, IVH, or
iological stress immediately at or shortly after birth intraparenchymal hemorrhage (Cowan et al.,
as evidenced by low Apgar scores, acidosis (e.g., 2003; Ramaswamy, Miller, Barkovich, Partridge,
cord umbilical arterial pH < 7), lack of respiration, & Ferriero, 2004; Volpe, 2008). However, the two
and/or multi-organ failure (Shaywitz & Fletcher, major forms of neuropathology associated with
1993). The Apgar score is a clinical rating of the this condition are selective neuronal necrosis and
infant’s appearance or skin color, pulse rate, parasagittal cerebral injury (Chau et al., 2009;
grimace or other signs of irritability, activity as an Ferriero, 2004; Huang & Castillo, 2008; Lawrence
indication of muscle tone, and respiration. Each & Inder, 2008). Selective neuronal necrosis can be
of the five items is rated from poor to good on a diffuse or fall into one of three regional patterns
0–2 scale, for a total of up to 10 points. The Apgar involving insult to the subcortical GM and paras-
rating is typically done at 1, 5, and 10 minutes after agittal region of the perirolandic cortex, subcorti-
birth, with persistently low scores (e.g., <6 at 1 cal GM and brainstem, or ventral pons and
and 5 minutes) indicating significant concern. The hippocampus. The cerebellum and spinal cord are
risk for asphyxia-related brain insult is increased other vulnerable brain regions. Figure 7.1 illus-
with multiple signs of physiological stress (Dilenge trates that pattern of brain insult in an infant with
et al., 2001; Perlman, 2006). The third event is a the combination of subcortical GM and parasag-
persistent clinical/behavioral symptom of brain ittal insult. Parasagittal cerebral injury is more
insult (i.e., encephalopathy), such as hypotonia or confined to cortical GM, most commonly in the
reflex abnormalities, abnormal levels of arousal, parietal occipital region, but includes damage to
feeding or breathing difficulties, or seizures. Most underlying subcortical WM. The latter form of
studies of children with HIE grade the severity of insult is often referred to as a “watershed injury”
encephalopathy from mild to severe based on a due to its location in border zones between the
system originally developed by Sarnat and Sarnat anterior, middle, and posterior cerebral circula-
(1976). Mild or Stage 1 HIE is typically defined by tion. In addition to revealing lesions in these
a hyper-alert or hyper-excitable state in the absence areas, MRIs taken with the first several days of
of other clinical signs; moderate or Stage 2 HIE by birth indicate lost of GM-WM differentiation,
lethargy, hypotonia, or diminished reflexes, with with decreased diffusion evident in diffusion-
or without seizures; and severe or Stage 3 HIE by weighted MRI.
stupor, motor flaccidity, or absent reflexes. A population-based study conducted in
The incidence of HIE is estimated at 2–4 per Western Australia found that several antenatal
1000 lives births, with estimates of neonatal mor- risk factors were associated with HIE in term
tality rates of 15–20% (Huang & Castillo, 2008). infants, including maternal thyroid disease,
Although neonatal encephalopathy can be present maternal viral illness, relatively short or long GA,
without evidence of perinatal stress, this form of infertility treatment, severe pre-eclampsia, mod-
encephalopathy is more likely to be of antenatal erate to severe vaginal bleeding, placental abnor-
origin (Perlman, 2006). Indications of fetal distress mality, intrauterine growth retardation, a family
without accompanying encephalopathy are much history of seizures or other neurological disorder,
more common than HIE and usually associated and socioeconomic status (Badawi et al., 1998a).
with normal outcomes (Dilenge et al., 2001). Intrapartum risk factors were also identified but
were present in only a minority of cases (Badawi
Neuropathology and Risk Factors et al., 1998b). Although the high prevalence of
Hypoxic-ischemia results in immediate cell antenatal risk factors is well established (Ferriero,
death (necrosis) due to swelling and rupturing of 2004; Perlman, 2006; Redline, 2008; Rolland &
cells, as well as secondary or reperfusion damage Hill, 1995; Volpe, 2008), there is substantial evi-
in response to injury. This secondary damage dence that most brain insults associated with term
Perinatal Brain Injury 185

FIGURE 7.1: Deep nuclear gray matter with cortical injury in a term infant who had a history of hypoxic
ischemic encephalopathy (HIE). (A) T1-weighted axial view from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)
shows hyperintense injury throughout deep nuclear gray matter. (B) T1-weighted coronal view shows exten-
sion of hyperintense injury from the deep nuclear gray matter into the parasagittal area. (C) T2-weighted
axial image demonstrates low intensity extending from subcortical to parasagittal cortex bilaterally. Reprinted
from Clinics in Perinatology, Vol. 35, R. K. Lawrence & T. E. Inder, Anatomic changes and imaging in assess-
ing brain injury in the term infant, pp. 679–693, 2008, with permission from Elsevier.

HIE occur around the time of birth. Cowan et al. greater involvement in the upper than lower
(2003) found evidence on MRI of acute brain extremities (Badawi et al., 2005; Himmelmann,
insult in 80% of term infants with HIE and in 69% Hagberg, Wiklund, Eek, & Uvebrant, 2007;
of term infants presenting only with neonatal sei- Perlman, 2006; Rennie, Hagmann, & Robertson,
zures. The determination that brain insults 2007; Volpe, 2008). In some cases, children exhibit
occurred prior to labor and delivery, rather than extrapyramidal signs (i.e., dystonia and chore-
acutely, was made on the basis of signs of gliosis, oathetosis) 6 months to several years later. This
mineralization, or cysts. The investigators sug- “delayed onset” syndrome is considered second-
gested that antenatal events may predispose some ary to damage to the thalamus and putamen.
infants to perinatal brain insults but that these Studies that have examined developmental
insults often occur in the intrapartum period. outcomes in children with term HIE have docu-
Consistent with these findings and notwithstand- mented lower IQ and other cognitive weaknesses
ing the complexities of sorting out causation in relative to same-aged peers without HIE. In one of
cases of term HIE, authorities on perinatal brain the first formal investigations of developmental
insults regard events occurring in the intrapartum outcomes in a well-defined cohort, Robertson and
period as playing a major role in at least a minor- Finer (1985) compared term-born children to
ity of cases of term HIE (Ferriero, 2004; Perlman, groups with mild, moderate, or severe HIE at 3½
1997; 2006; Volpe, 2008). years of age. The children with mild HIE had no
neurodevelopmental handicaps as defined by CP,
Outcomes visual or hearing impairment, mental retardation,
A population-based study by Badawi et al. (2005) or severe seizure disorder. Moreover, mean scores
revealed a 13% prevalence of CP in children with on measures of IQ, language, and perceptual-
term newborn encephalopathy, whereas Vannuci motor skills fell within the average range for this
and Perlman (1997) found that about 25% of chil- group. In contrast, all of the children with severe
dren with term HIE had a permanent neurologic HIE and 21% of the moderate HIE group had at
deficit. Available estimates suggest that about least one handicap. The mild HIE group scored
half of infants with severe HIE die and almost better in testing than both other groups with HIE,
all survivors have neurologic deficits, whereas and the moderate group did better than the severe
about 10–20% of those with moderate HIE die group. In a subsequent follow-up at 5½ years of
and 30–40% have neurologic deficits (Bhat et al., age of children without major neurologic or
2009). CP is most typically spastic quadriplegia, mental disability, Robertson and Finer (1988)
often with a dyskinetic component and with found that the group with moderate HIE had
186 forensic neuropsychology

lower scores on IQ perceptual-motor tests com- ability, performed more poorly on a wide range of
pared with the mild HIE group, other term chil- specific neuropsychological skills, and had more
dren treated in a tertiary neonatal care center symptoms of hyperactivity and emotional prob-
without encephalopathy, and a group of term chil- lems and fewer pro-social behaviors on parent or
dren cared for in well-child nurseries. This teacher ratings. The children with moderate HIE
research team also documented delayed school without CP did not differ from their classmates in
readiness skills in 40% of children with moderate overall cognitive ability but had weaknesses in
HIE without CP, sensory deficits, seizures, or language, sensorimotor skills, narrative memory,
mental retardation (Robertson & Grace, 1992). In and sentence repetition. Teacher ratings indicated
a third follow-up of this sample at 8 years of age, that both HIE groups made less progress toward
Robertson, Finer, and Grace (1989) again docu- educational goals than their classmates. Although
mented poorer performance by the moderate and mean test scores for the severe HIE group that
severe HIE groups relative to a representative fell within the average range indicate that some
sample of peers on tests of IQ and perceptual- of these children survived without generalized
motor ability, as well as on measures of vocabu- cognitive impairment, the results also docu-
lary, reading, spelling, and arithmetic. Even mented lower skill levels in this group compared
children with moderate HIE who did not have with peers.
severe cognitive deficits or neurologic impair- Studies of older survivors of term HIE
ment obtained lower IQs and had more learning reveal persisting developmental impairments.
problems than the peer controls, while the mild Lindstrom, Lagerroos, Gillberg, and Fernell
HIE group performed similarly to the controls. (2006) examined 28 adolescents and young adults
More recent studies substantiate a high rate of aged 15–19 years with moderate HIE selected
mental deficiency in children with severe HIE, from a larger population with 5-minute Apgar
with evidence for less severe developmental scores <7. Eight of the participants (29%) had a
impairments in a number of skill areas in children neurological diagnosis. Seven of the 11 persons
with moderate HIE (Gonzalez & Miller, 2006). with intelligence testing scored in the borderline
Shankaran, Woldt, Koepke, Bedard, and Nandyal range of IQ or below. Parent data revealed more
(1991) found that 15 of 24 term children with problems in short-term memory, time perception,
moderate or severe HIE had either a neurologic making friends, attention, and a measure of atypi-
deficit or low IQ at 5 years of age, and that there cal behaviors in the HIE sample compared with
was a high level concordance between these out- siblings. Maneru, Junque, Botet, Tallada, and
comes and outcomes measures at 1 year. In Guardia (2001) compared another small sample
another study of early childhood outcomes, Dixon of adolescents with mild or moderate HIE with a
et al. (2002) found that scores on the Griffiths matched group of healthy adolescents on a neu-
Mental Development Scales at 16 months were ropsychological test battery. Adolescents with IQ
lower in their population-based cohort of term <85, CP, sensory deficits, or mental retardation
children with neonatal encephalopathy than in a were excluded from the sample. Results revealed
community control group. These differences that the adolescents with moderate HIE had lower
remained even when they excluded children with- scores on tests of memory, perceptual-motor
out CP. Low total scores on this measure, defined skills, and executive function, while scores for the
as scores more than two standard deviations group with mild HIE did not differ from controls.
below the means for controls, were found in 36% Other case studies of children meeting criteria for
of the severe HIE group and 19% of the moderate HIE demonstrate that effects on adolescents can
HIE group. be specific to episodic memory, with relative pres-
Rates of CP in a sample followed to 7 years of ervation of immediate recall and acquired seman-
age by Marlow, Rose, Rands, and Draper (2005) tic knowledge (de Haan et al., 2006; Gadian et al.,
were 6% in children with moderate HIE and 42% 2000). Asphyxia-related damage to the hippocam-
in those with severe HIE, though the criteria for pus has been implicated in these cases.
severe HIE were less restrictive than in many
other studies. The major contribution of this study Predictors
is the evidence it offers for deficits in children As evident from these findings, more severe HIE
without CP. Compared with classroom controls, is a major predictor of developmental outcomes
the children with severe HIE but without CP and outcomes are worst for children with severe
obtained lower scores on tests of global mental HIE. Less pervasive deficits are found in children
Perinatal Brain Injury 187

with moderate HIE, though as a group these chil- with scores <7 for more than 5 minutes. Persons
dren are a higher risk for cognitive, learning, and with histories of encephalopathy, asphyxia, or
behavior impairment compared with children congenital disorders were excluded and the two
with mild HIE or control groups without HIE. low-Apgar groups were compared with a refer-
Additional clinical predictors of poorer outcome ence group of persons who met these same crite-
include more persistent neonatal neurological ria but had 1-minute Apgar scores >6. Adjusting
symptoms, CP or sensory disorders, seizures, and for antenatal, neonatal, and social factors, both
placements on multiple anticonvulsant medica- low-Apgar groups had slightly higher rates of low
tions (Dixon et al., 2002; Himmelmann et al., IQ than the group with Apgar scores >6. The
2007; Rennie et al., 2007; Robertson & Finer, 1988; group with prolonged low scores also had lower
Shankaran et al., 1991; Volpe, 2008). Children school grades at ages 15–16 years. In a subsequent
with delayed-onset extrapyramidal syndrome study, Odd et al. (2009) assessed the effect of
may do well relative to others with CP. resuscitation with or without encephalopathy on
Neonatal encephalopathy appears to be criti- the IQs of 8-year-old term-born children partici-
cal component of risk for neurodevelopmental pating in a longitudinal study. Findings revealed
impairment in infants with HIE (Amiel-Tison & higher rates of low IQ (<80) in both resuscitation
Ellison, 1986; Badawi et al., 2005; Dilenge et al., groups compared with controls, though the
2001; Perlman, 2006; Volpe, 2008). To illustrate, increased risk associated with low Apgar scores
Moster, Lie, and Markestad (2002) assessed out- was low and rates were higher among the children
comes in a sample of children 8 to 13 years of age with encephalopathy. The results of both studies
with 5-minute Apgar scores <4 and in a control were interpreted as evidence that even mild, tran-
group without neonatal problems. Compared sient hypoxia has the potential to lead to neuronal
with the controls, children in the low-Apgar damage with associated decrements in IQ.
sample with neonatal encephalopathy had more The type of abnormalities found on MRI is
motor impairments; more developmental, learn- one of the best predictors of neurodevelopmental
ing, attention, and other behavior problems by outcome. CP, visual impairment, and generalized
parent report; and higher rates of epilepsy, whereas cognitive deficits or developmental delays are
children in the low-Apgar sample without enceph- most common in children with insult to the basal
alopathy were at much lower risk for negative out- ganglia, thalamus, posterior limb of the internal
comes. Consistent with these results, Thomson, capsule, and perirolandic cortex (Barnette et al.,
Searle, and Russell (1977) found that a sample of 2002; Mercuri et al., 2004; Miller et al., 2005;
5- to 10-year-old children with low Apgar scores, Rutherford et al., 1998). More widespread cortical
most of whom did not show signs of neonatal involvement accompanies more severe insults to
encephalopathy, performed similarly to children these regions. A second pattern of brain insult
with normal Apgar scores on a measure of psy- detected on MRI involves damage to cortical WM
cholinguistic abilities. Other studies have also in the arterial borderline or watershed zones that
found that low Apgar scores lack utility in pre- extends in more severe cases to cortical GM. In
dicting outcomes when considered in isolation examining neonatal MRIs in 173 term infants
(Blackman, 1988; Viggedal, Lundalv, Carlsson, & with neonatal encephalopathy, Miller et al. (2005)
Kjellmer, 2002), and one investigation demon- found that 30% of their sample had normal scans,
strated normal development in a majority of chil- 25% displayed the basal ganglia/thalamus pattern
dren requiring resuscitation at birth so long as of insult, and 45% had insult primarily to water-
Apgar scores rebounded within the first several shed regions. Infants with the basal ganglia/thala-
minutes of birth (Jain, Ferre, Vidyasagar, Nath, & mus pattern had more severe degrees of
Sheftel, 1991). encephalopathy and more seizures and they
Recent evidence, however, suggests that even received more intensive resuscitation compared
brief depressions in Apgar scores may have some with infants with the watershed pattern. None of
effect on long-term cognitive development (Odd, the infants with normal MRIs had CP or impair-
Lewis, Whitelaw, & Gunnell, 2009; Odd, ment on developmental testing at 30 months,
Rasmussen, Gunnell, Lewis, & Whitelaw, 2008). compared with rates of CP and developmental
Odd et al. (2008) examined IQ in 18-year-old impairment, respectively, of 50% and 56% for
term-born individuals from the Swedish Birth infants with the basal ganglia/thalamus pattern
Registry in two low-Apgar groups, one with scores and 11% and 18% for infants with the watershed
<7 for only 0–5 minutes after birth and the other pattern. Findings from Rutherford, Pennock,
188 forensic neuropsychology

Schwieso, Cowan, and Dubowitz (1996) further thrombosis (Barnette & Inder, 2009; Perlman,
reinforce associations of more severe insults in 2004; Raju et al., 2007; Volpe, 2008). Existing
the basal ganglia or more extensive WM abnor- research literature related to these conditions,
malities with poorer neurodevelopmental out- their developmental consequences, and predic-
comes at 2 years of age. The effects of these insults tors of outcome is summarized below.
likely vary with the extent of injury, as one study
reports good cognitive outcomes in some children Perinatal Arterial Ischemic Stroke,
with moderate encephalopathy for whom MRIs Referred to Commonly as Ischemic
indicated more isolated insults to the basal gan- Perinatal Stroke (IPS)
glia (Pasternak & Gorey, 1998). If IPS occurs in conjunction with neonatal enceph-
Abnormalities on MRI are found even in chil- alopathy, clinical signs are similar to those seen
dren without CP and are related to impairments with HIE (e.g., seizures, hypotonia, lethargy, poor
on tests of IQ and perceptual-motor ability and feeding), though motor tone or movements may be
minor neurological dysfunction. Barnette et al. asymmetrically abnormal (Barnette & Inder, 2009;
(2002) found WM abnormalities or mild to mod- Volpe, 2008). Often, however, IPS is not accompa-
erate lesions in the basal ganglia in 80% of a nied by neonatal encephalopathy and is identified
sample of 5- to 6-year-old children without CP by isolated focal seizures that appear either in the
who had neonatal encephalopathy. In another neonatal or postnatal period or by post-natal
study of young children born at term who met presentation of CP or motor asymmetries (Nelson
criteria for HIE but did not have CP, Steinman & Lynch, 2004). The majority of children with
et al. (2009) found that the severity of brain insults CP related to perinatal stroke are not encephalo-
conforming to the watershed-type pattern pre- pathic at birth (Wu et al., 2004). Cases of delayed
dicted Verbal IQ scores at 4 years of age. The diagnosis of IPS are referred to as presumed peri-
extent of watershed injury was related to Verbal natal ischemic stroke and are defined as neuro-
IQ only (i.e., not to Performance IQ) and was logical deficits or seizures in infants with normal
evident even in analysis that controlled for the neonatal neurological histories that become mani-
severity of insult in the basal ganglia distribution. fest after the perinatal period and are accompanied
Further support for the utility of MRI in predict- by neuroimaging findings consistent with perina-
ing outcomes is provided by van Kooij et al. tal stroke (Kirton et al., 2008; Raju et al., 2007).
(2007), who examined the cross-sectional area of Such evidence, however, is presumptive and in
the corpus callosum at school age in term-born some cases it may be difficult to be certain as to
children with mild and moderate HIE and in whether insult occurred prior to the end of the
age-matched schoolmate controls. They found perinatal period. The incidence of IPS is estimated
that this structure was smaller in the group with at between 1 in 2,300–5,000 births.
moderate HIE than in controls and that its size The most common neuromotor disorder in
correlated with performance on a test of motor children with IPS is hemiplegic CP, although some
ability. These relationships remained even after children have subtle motor asymmetries or
excluding children with CP. WM abnormalities impaired motor abilities without CP (Cioni et al.,
on MRI have been detected even in children with 1999; Mercuri et al., 2004; Volpe, 2008). Lesions
mild HIE who are developing normally at 2 years can also be bilateral and a small minority of chil-
of age, though the implications of these abnor- dren present with quadriparesis or monoparesis
malities for longer-term outcomes are uncertain (Lee et al., 2005b). Infarction is most often found
(Rutherford et al., 1996). These several findings in the distribution of the left middle cerebral
suggest that the extent of watershed insults and artery, with greater involvement of the arms than
WM injury are related to cognitive deficits in chil- the legs. The reasons for greater vulnerability of
dren without CP or generalized mental deficiency the left hemisphere are not well understood, but
(Gonzalez & Miller, 2006). may reflect the more direct pathway for clots
from the heart through the left carotid artery or to
Stroke in Term Infants differences in blood flow in the left versus right
A stroke is a neurological dysfunction due to a carotids related to early patent ductus arteriosus
disruption in the brain’s blood supply. The pri- (Trauner, Chase, Walker, & Wulfeck, 1993).
mary sources of this disruption in the term infant Hemiplegia is more likely when insults are large
without HIE are perinatal arterial ischemic stroke, and affect major portions of the middle cerebral
intracranial hemorrhage, and cerebral sinovenous artery distribution, including the cerebral cortex,
Perinatal Brain Injury 189

WM, basal ganglia, posterior limb of the internal scores on measures of IQ, immediate verbal
capsule, and cerebral peduncles (De Vries, Van der memory, receptive vocabulary, and verbal fluency
Grond, Van Haastert, & Groenendaal, 2005; Lee were lower in term-born 5- to 8-year-old children
et al., 2005b; Raju et al., 2007; Trauner et al., 1993). with IPS from twin gestations than in the co-twin
Insult to the non-stroke hemisphere also increases without IPS. Other studies have found that,
the likelihood of hemiplegic CP (Volpe, 2008). while children with IPS score below expectations
Fortunately, many young children function well on developmental, IQ, and neuropsychological
motorically despite having hemiplegic CP, and measures, mean scores generally fall within the
hemiplegia may resolve during early childhood average range (Hogan, Kirkham, & Isaacs, 2000;
(Wulfeck Trauner, & Tallal, 1991). Possibly related Jan & Camfield, 1998). Variations in outcome are
to the manner in which cases without neonatal nevertheless evident in the literature, with some
encephalopathy are ascertained (i.e., based on studies indicating significant deficits in children
asymmetric motor impairment), children diag- with IPS (Koelfen et al., 1993; Raju et al., 2007).
nosed with presumed perinatal ischemic stroke These inconsistencies may relate to different crite-
have higher rates of hemiplegia than those with ria for sample selection. Some studies select only
neonatal diagnoses of IPS (Lee et al., 2005b). children with strictly focal or unilateral lesions
Distinctive features of presumed perinatal ischemic and exclude children with symptoms of HIE,
stroke include a tendency for greater impairment while other studies recruit children without regard
in the legs than arms and evidence of PHI in some to these features or fail to specify ascertainment
children (Kirton et al., 2008; Rennie et al., 2007). methods.
Numerous maternal, fetal, and neonatal Trauner and colleagues at the University of
factors place infants at increased risk for IPS California, San Diego followed well-specified
(Barnette & Inder, 2009; Nelson & Lynch, 2004; cohorts of children with unilateral perinatal focal
Raju et al., 2007). Risks for IPS include both intra- stroke and compared these children with controls
partum factors and maternal-fetal factors, such as (Ballantyne, Scarvie, & Trauner, 1994; Ballantyne,
thrombocytopenia or infection, maternal autoim- Spilkin, Hesselink, & Trauner, 2008; Ballantyne,
mune disorder, preeclampsia, congenital heart Spilkin & Trauner, 2007; Bates & Roe, 2001; Stiles,
disease, polycythemia (excess red blood cells), Nass, Levine, Moses, & Reilly, 2009; Trauner et al.,
persistent pulmonary hypertension, and twin-to- 1993; Trauner, Nass, & Ballantyne, 2001; Wulfeck
twin transfusion syndrome. Lee et al. (2005a) et al., 1991). The samples recruited by this research
examined risk factors associated with IPS in group were restricted to children with either IPS
infants enrolled in a large health care system. or hemorrhagic strokes within the perinatal
Eighty-five percent of the 40 cases they identified period and excluded cases with bilateral, progres-
with IPS were term-born. Results revealed that sive, or multifocal lesions. Wulfeck et al. (1991)
several intrapartum factors were more common found seizures in about half of a sample of 14 chil-
among the cases than among matched controls, dren selected in this manner and followed to 6–24
including chorioamnionitis (a vaginal infection), months of age. Delays in language and motor
prolonged rupture of membranes, prolonged development were also observed in about half of
second stage of labor, fetal heart rate abnormality, this cohort. However, global cognitive function-
cord abnormality, vacuum-assisted delivery, and ing on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development
emergency cesarean delivery. Maternal factors was within the normal range for most of the
associated with IPS were preeclampsia, oligohy- children. Trauner et al. (1993) documented
dramnios (excessive amniotic fluid), and decreased normal IQ scores in all 21 children followed to
fetal movement. The children with IPS were also between 2 and 9 years of age. A later study revealed
more likely to be diagnosed with asphyxia or HIE. higher parent ratings of behavior problems on
Adverse consequences of IPS other than CP several scales of the Child Behavior Checklist
and motor impairments include epilepsy in in 4- to 15-year-old children with unilateral
25–50% of children, deficits in vision and sensory perinatal strokes compared with a control group
perception, and problems relative to normative (Trauner et al., 2001). However, none of the group
standards or control groups in cognition, lan- differences was significant when IQ was con-
guage, learning, and behavior (Barnette & Inder, trolled and mean behavior problem ratings for the
2009; Golomb et al., 2001; Golomb, Saha, Garg, stroke group fell within the average range.
Azzouz, & Williams, 2007; Mercuri et al., 2003; For the most part, studies by the San Diego
Trauner, 2003). Talib et al. (2008) found that group have failed to reveal differential effects of
190 forensic neuropsychology

left-versus right-sided lesions on language and electroencephalographic (EEG) abnormalities,


visual-spatial skills, as is typical of later-occurring large or bilateral lesions, multiple lesion types,
strokes (Ballantyne et al., 2008; Bates & Roe, 2001; and coagulation disorders (Ballantyne et al., 2007;
Bates et al., 2001; Stiles et al., 2009). These investi- Ballantye et al., 2008; Bava, Archibald, & Trauner,
gators have observed early language delays with 2007; Bava et al., 2005; Golomb et al., 2007;
lesions to either hemisphere, with the type of Koelfen et al., 1993; Mercuri et al., 2003; Stiles
delay varying somewhat according to the side of et al., 2009). CP accompanied by neonatal enceph-
lesion (left-hemisphere lesions more strongly alopathy is associated with worse developmental
associated with delays in speech production and outcomes than either condition in isolation
right-hemisphere lesions with poor comprehen- (Badawi et al., 2005; Golomb et al., 2007; Jan &
sion). Although the early language delays resolved Camfield, 1998; Ramaswamy et al., 2004). Poor
with advancing age, residual deficits in language- outcomes are also more common with presumed
processing skills were observed at older ages perinatal ischemic stroke than with non-delayed
(Ballatyne et al., 2007). Unilateral lesions to either (i.e., neonatal or early postnatal) presentations of
hemisphere also resulted in impairments in IPS (Kirton et al., 2008). Golomb et al. (2001)
visual-spatial abilities. Like language impair- found that all 22 of the children they followed
ments, these deficits changed with age but per- with this diagnosis into early childhood had
sisted into childhood and were to some extent persistent hemiparesis and that nearly half of
lesion-specific. Children with left-hemisphere the sample had speech, behavior, or learning
insult had more problems in processing the problems. In examining a sample of 59 children
component parts of figures than the overall with presumed perinatal ischemic stroke to a
spatial configuration, whereas those with right- median age of 4 years, Kirton et al. (2008) found
hemisphere stroke showed the reverse pattern cognitive or behavioral problems in 29%,
(Stiles et al., 2008). Face and affect processing may language disorders in 22%, visual deficits in 18%,
also be compromised in children with unilateral and epilepsy in 42%. Cortical insults were associ-
perinatal stroke, at least early in development ated with cognitive-behavioral problems, visual
(Stiles et al., 2009). impairment, or epilepsy, while involvement of the
Similarly, Ballantyne et al. (1994) found that basal ganglia predicted motor deficits.
perinatal strokes in either hemisphere had adverse
effects on IQ. In this study, a post-stroke group of Intracranial Hemorrhage
persons tested at 4–20 years of age was compared Intracranial hemorrhage in term newborns can
with controls on Wechsler Verbal, Performance, be classified as epidural, subdural, subarachnoid,
and Full-Scale IQs. Although all three IQ scores intracerebral (intraparenchymal or intraventricu-
for the stroke group were within the broadly aver- lar), or intracerebellar (Volpe, 2008). Estimates of
age range, they were lower than the IQ scores of the annual incidence of all types of intracranial
the controls. Verbal and Performance IQs were hemorrhages are about 1 per 4,000 births (Gupta,
comparable for the left-hemisphere lesion group, Kechli, & Kanamalla, 2009; Perlman, 2004).
while Verbal IQ was higher than Performance IQ Subarachnoid and subdural hemorrhages are the
for the group with right-hemisphere lesions. most frequent types among term neonates,
Replication of this finding is needed but the whereas intracerebral and intracerebellar hemor-
pattern of results is consistent with the right- rhages are more common in preterm children.
hemisphere “crowding hypothesis,” wherein reor- Intracranial hemorrhage in term neonates often
ganization of a damaged right hemisphere in occurs in association with birth trauma and
support of language functions occurs at the asphyxia, though some hemorrhages occur with-
expense of nonlanguage functions (Lidzba, Staudt, out a recognizable precipitating event and smaller
Wilke, & Krageloh-Mann, 2006). A more general hemorrhages are found even in term infants with
theme that emerges from review of this literature normal outcomes (Bergman et al., 1985; Gupta
is the capacity for neural reorganization following et al., 2009; Jhawar, Ranger, Steven, and Del
early focal stroke (Bates & Roe, 2001; Bava, Maestro, 2005; Volpe, 2008). Some neonates with
Ballantye, & Trauner, 2005; Staudt, Krageloh- significant intracranial hemorrhage present
Mann, Holthausen, Gerloff, & Grodd, 2004; Staudt with seizures, generalized hypotonia, or signs of
et al., 2002; Stiles et al., 2009; Talib et al., 2008). increased intracranial pressure such as a decreased
Risks for worse developmental and cognitive level of consciousness, but others are asymptom-
outcomes following IPS include CP, seizures and atic. Risk factors include maternal-placental
Perinatal Brain Injury 191

problems, such as pregnancy-induced hyper- study of full-term children with more diverse
tension and placental abruption, and perinatal types of intracranial hemorrhage also suggested
complications, such as birth trauma, asphyxia, an association of neonatal asphyxia with poorer
thrombocytopenia, inherited coagulopathy (pre- developmental outcomes (Hanigan, Powell,
disposition to hemorrhage), and sinovenous Miller, & Wright, 1995). More recently, Jhawar
thrombosis. More rarely, intracranial hemorrhage et al. (2005) contacted parents by phone or during
can stem from a ruptured aneurysm or arterio- clinic visits to obtain outcome information in a
venous malformation (Perlman, 2004). sample of 1- to 10-year-old full-term children
Jhawar, Ranger, Steven, and Del Maestro with different types of intracranial hemorrhage.
(2003) found that rates of forceps use, thrombo- Of the 49 children assessed, about one quarter
cytopenia, and signs of hypoxia (low Apgar scores had motor problems and one quarter had either
and need for resuscitation) were higher in a cohort sensory impairments or indications of learning
of 66 full-term infants with intracranial hemor- difficulties. Factors related to worse outcomes
rhage than in a group of 104 controls. They also were thrombocytopenia and a frontal location of
found that forceps use was more closely associ- hemorrhage. Poorer outcomes are also associated
ated with subarachnoid or subdural hemorrhage, with IVH and early and recurrent seizures,
whereas thrombocytopenia was more closely whereas outcomes are frequently good for chil-
related to intraparenchymal hemorrhage. This dren with uncomplicated subdural hemorrhage
finding is consistent with other reports indicating or subarachnoid hemorrhage over the cerebral
an association of intrapartum trauma with sub- convexities (Gupta et al., 2009; Perlman, 2004;
dural and subarachnoid hemorrhages (Perlman, Volpe, 2008).
2004; Volpe, 2008). Jhawar et al. (2003) emphasize
that their data do not imply a causal relationship Cerebral Sinovenous Thrombosis
of forceps use or hypoxia with intracranial Cerebral sinovenous thrombosis involves block-
hemorrhage, as pregnancy complications may age of the system of veins outside the parenchyma
result in both forceps use and intracranial hemor- that drain blood from the brain. With an esti-
rhage, and intracranial hemorrhage may itself mated annual incidence of <1 per 100,000 chil-
precipitate respiratory distress. dren (deVerber et al., 2001), this form of stroke is
Although the above-cited studies by the rare and occurs most frequently in neonates. The
San Diego group included participants with veins most often involved are the superior sagittal
focal intraparenchymal hemorrhage, few studies sinus and lateral sinuses, but thrombosis may also
have examined the developmental consequences occur in the cortical veins, straight sinus, vein of
of more diverse types of perinatal intracranial Galen, internal cerebral vein, or jugular vein.
hemorrhage. One exception is an investigation Brain edema is most commonly observed on neu-
that followed 18 full-term survivors with neonatal roimaging, though IVH or intraparenchymal
supratentorial (i.e., intraparenchymal or intra- hemorrhagic infarction may also be present
ventricular) hemorrhage at 1–7 years of age (Fitzgerald, Williams, Garg, Carvalho, & Golomb,
(Bergman et al., 1985). Outcomes included 2006; Volpe, 2008). Presenting symptoms fre-
nine children with no evidence of neurodevelop- quently include seizures and a decreased level of
mental abnormality, three with severe cognitive consciousness. Risks factors include maternal-
deficits, spastic quadriplegia, and frequent placental problems, such as infection, maternal
seizures, two with IQ <50 and hemiplegia, three hypertension/preeclampsia, premature rupture of
without CP who had cognitive deficits, and one membranes, placental abruption, and gestational
child with a history of neurological disorder diabetes (deVerber et al., 2001; Fitzgerald et al.,
who died in infancy. Discovery of abnormal 2006). Neonatal illnesses or complications, such
outcomes in about half of this small sample is as hypoxia, infection, dehydration, and heart
consistent with results from studies that followed disease, are additional risk factors. Although
children with neonatal intracranial hemorrhage long-term outcome studies are lacking, 23 of the
over only the first year of life (Bergman 29 neonates (79%) with cerebral sinovenous
et al., 1985). The children in this study whose thrombosis followed by Fitzgerald et al. (2006) to
histories were suggestive of hypoxic ischemia or a median of 2 years had seizures, developmental
traumatic injury at birth had a much higher rate problems, or CP. Worse outcomes are associated
of disability than children for whom the hemor- with infarction (Fitzgerald et al., 2006; deVerber
rhage was of unknown etiology. Findings from a et al., 2001).
192 forensic neuropsychology

FORENSIC AS SES SMENTS OF and follow the guidelines as set forth by the Board
C H I L D R E N W I T H P E R I N ATA L of Directors, American Academy of Clinical
B R A I N I N S U LT S Neuropsychology (2007). One of the differences is
the need for families to understand that a forensic
Procedures for Conducting Forensic assessment is done for purposes of litigation and
Child Neuropsychological Assessment on referral from an attorney, and that this implies
Forensic neuropsychological assessments of that the family will not be billed for the evaluation
children with perinatal brain insults are typically and will not receive direct feedback regarding
conducted because of an alleged personal injury findings and recommendations or be followed for
resulting from the medical care received during clinical care. As in other assessments, my personal
the perinatal period. As in other personal injury preference is to begin by asking about current
cases, the purposes of these assessments are to concerns about the child. I then review the child’s
identify impairment, determine the likely cause of medical, developmental, educational, and family
impairment, and make projections regarding history with the parent. I typically review medical,
the child’s future outcomes and needs (Dennis, therapy, and educational records prior to seeing
1989; Donders, 2005; Goldberg, 2006; Wills & the child and family and thus already have infor-
Sweet, 2006). As in all forensic neuropsycho- mation about the child’s medical history. This
logical assessments, the overarching goal is to information is critical in interpreting findings and
provide triers of fact, including the judge and jury, drawing conclusions as it is more objective than
with assessment findings and research informa- parent report and may provide information not
tion that pertain to legal questions about a case obtained from parent interview. However, asking
(Giuliano, Barth, Hawk, & Ryan, 1997). Neuro- parents to review the child’s birth and other med-
psychologists are unable to address questions ical history can sometimes provide information
about standards of care of other professionals or that may not be included in the medical record,
render opinions that require training and exper- such as the sequence of interventions undertaken
tise outside their own (e.g., medicine, neurophysi- following neonatal hospital discharge. One of the
ology, neurochemistry). Their role is to review objectives of parent interviews and record reviews
medical records to obtain information about the is to obtain information about the child’s past
child’s neurological, developmental, family, and and current medical status and abilities. Perinatal
educational history of potential relevance to any events are of special interest in cases of alleged
current problems in cognition, learning, or behav- perinatal brain insult, though subsequent medical
ior. They then assess the child and interpret find- history is relevant in evaluating the outcomes of
ings in light of this information. these events and in exploring other potential
A unique feature of forensic assessments, as influences on development, such as postnatal
opposed to nonforensic ones, is the need for the illnesses or injuries. Additional aims are to con-
neuropsychologist to be able to explicitly defend sider other factors that may contribute to neu-
methods and conclusions and make judgments robehavioral or learning problems, such as
regarding cause and prognosis based on “reason- disadvantages related to schooling or the family
able certainty.” These evaluations also involve environment or a family history of learning, atten-
more limited relationships with the child and tion, or psychiatric disorder.
family and a willingness to subject one’s methods, My practice is to employ the same basic
findings, and interpretations to scrutiny from approach to child assessment that I would in non-
attorneys and other neuropsychologists working forensic cases, including comprehensive assess-
with them. Familiarity with the disorder or condi- ments of a wide range of cognitive abilities and
tion for which the child has been referred, a com- academic skills, a child interview, and parent
prehensive and organized approach to assessment, and teacher behavior checklists. To enhance the
circumspection in drawing conclusions, and a interpretability of findings, I am careful to restrict
willingness to communicate with attorneys and assessment as much as is feasible to measures
work within the time constraints often imposed that are in reasonably common use by child neu-
by these cases are also important in undertaking ropsychologists and that have well-established
evidence-based forensic assessments. norms and reliability. When possible, I also avoid
The assessment procedures I employ in these tests that have been administered recently
cases are in most ways identical to procedures I and that may be susceptible to practice effects.
follow in conducting nonforensic assessments For example, if one intelligence test has been
Perinatal Brain Injury 193

administered within the previous year, preference key aspect of a comprehensive neuropsychological
is given to an alternative test. Finally, I give special evaluation. Well standardized behavior rating
consideration to issues of effort and potential scales provide measures of a range of behavior
malingering. I have seldom encountered what problems and are useful in determining if the
I would consider intentional distortion or mis- child meets criteria for a behavior disorder.
representation of test performance by a child or Administration of parent and teacher question-
adolescent involved in a forensic evaluation. naires and of child self-report ratings provides
But the possibility of malingering exists even in multiple perspectives on the nature and severity of
young children (Henry, 2005; Lu & Boone, 2002; behavior problems across different contexts. The
Seidel, 1998). Documentation of effort is also validity scales contained in several of the currently
justified by the fact that children and adolescents available behavior questionnaires permit formal
vary substantially in motivation level and that assessment of response bias. Measures of adaptive
poor effort can limit the interpretability of test behavior skills are likewise important in determin-
results in the absence of outright malingering. ing the child’s level of functioning, or disability, in
I have found the Test of Memory Malingering everyday settings in areas such as communication,
(TOMM, Constantinou & McCaffrey, 2003) useful self care, and socialization. Results suggestive of
for this purpose. Alternative effort testing is problems in behavior, adjustment, or adaptive
available for children (Courtney, Dinkins, Allen, functioning also help justify recommendations for
& Kuroski, 2003; Green & Flaro, 2003), but the interventions in these areas.
TOMM is applicable to a wide age range and
involves only picture stimuli. More comprehen- Considerations in Rendering
sive evaluation of effort requires awareness of Judgments in Forensic Cases
worse-than-expected performances, unexpected In formulating impressions regarding children
or inconsistent patterns of findings, and lack with alleged perinatal brain insults, the neuro-
of congruity of test results with performance of psychologist summarizes the child’s strengths and
activities of daily living, as well as monitoring weaknesses and the nature of any developmental
of the child’s affect, verbalizations, and attempts to deficits, describes factors that are likely to have
maintain motivation (Heilbronner et al., 2009). contributed to deficits, and utilizes information
Child testing typically entails administration from the assessment to recommend appropriate
of a broad range of standardized measures of cog- clinical, educational, or family interventions.
nitive and academic competencies. Cognitive What most clearly distinguishes forensic neurop-
assessments include developmental or intelligence sychological assessments of children from nonfo-
test batteries combined with measures of language rensic evaluations is the need to render formal
abilities, visual-spatial and perceptual-motor judgments with regard to the cause of the child’s
skills, memory, and attention and executive func- impairments and the child’s future problems
tion. Tests of academic readiness skills or of read- and service needs. As in forensic assessments of
ing, mathematics, and other academic skills serve adults, these judgments are made with “reason-
a dual purpose. Results help both in evaluating the able neuropsychological certainty” and are based
impact of a potential perinatal brain insult on an on several considerations (Arkes, 1989; Hartman,
important area of functioning and in establishing 1999; Lewin, 1998).
needs for special interventions. To illustrate, dis- One consideration in rendering a judgment
covery of disorders in reading or mathematics that about causality is the plausibility of perinatal brain
are not accompanied by the types of cognitive insult as a potential explanation for the child’s
deficits typically associated with these disorders impairment. Past or present abnormalities on
raise the possibility that poor motivation or sub- neuroimaging or neurological examinations help
standard instruction may be in part responsible establish brain insult as a credible cause of impair-
for the child’s poor academic performance. Con- ment. The severity of neonatal encephalopathy is
versely, significant cognitive impairments not another consideration, even when neuroimaging
accompanied by academic problems suggest either is unavailable or fails to reveal abnormality.
poor effort on the cognitive tasks or provision of A developmental history consistent with perinatal
special academic assistance that has helped the injury is also relevant in evaluating the causal
child to compensate for cognitive weaknesses. basis for impairment. To illustrate, a neurological
Assessment of emotional adjustment, behavior basis for impaired attention and executive func-
problems, and adaptive behavior skills is another tion in a school-age child with VLBW/VPTB
194 forensic neuropsychology

would be implausible in the face of evidence for developmental impairments that are characteris-
fully normal development during the first year of tic of perinatal brain insults. Other possible
life. Similarly, with the exception of the postnatal origins of impairment are suggested by medical
emergence of CP in presumed perinatal ischemic histories of significant illnesses or trauma and by
stroke and in some cases of HIE involving dela- family histories of learning, attention, develop-
yed-onset extrapyramidal syndrome, children mental, or behavior disorders. Postnatal illnesses
diagnosed with HIE or perinatal strokes typically or injuries are relevant to the extent that they could
display developmental problems early in life. plausibly account for any impairment identified by
Deficits first detected during the preschool or the assessment. Family histories of neurodevelop-
school-age years and preceded by normal early mental or psychiatric conditions raise the possibil-
postnatal development suggest that perinatal ity that the child’s impairments would have been
brain insult, if it occurred at all, may have been present even in the absence of an alleged perinatal
too minimal to yield enduring deficits. brain insult. The case for perinatal brain insult as a
Another consideration in evaluating causality primary cause is weakened if there is a clear family
is the consistency of assessment findings. Parent history of developmental disorder and the child’s
and teacher ratings can differ in relation to the deficits and developmental history conform to
different demands placed on children at home that disorder, especially in the absence of evidence
and at school and the different expectations of for an earlier neurological or developmental
parents and teachers. Nevertheless, gross discrep- impairment linked to perinatal brain insult.
ancies between parent and teacher ratings or The neuropsychologist can render one of three
between questionnaire data and descriptions of opinions with regard to the causation of impair-
the child contained in medical or educational ments identified in the assessment. The first
records or child observations during the assess- option is to conclude, with reasonable neuropsy-
ment suggest caution in interpreting findings. The chological certainty, that the perinatal event
consistency of results across different measures of alleged to have resulted in brain insult is a proxi-
child outcome is also relevant. For example, mal cause of the deficits, indicating a judgment
evidence for a level of daily functioning that is that impairment would not have occurred had it
higher than expected based on assessment not been for the event. The second option is to
findings suggests that the child did not perform determine that the alleged event is not, to a rea-
optimally in testing, whereas lower-than-expected sonable degree of certainty, the proximal cause of
daily functioning raises the possibility that adverse the impairments, suggesting that the impairments
environmental circumstances may be interfering were likely caused by factors other than perinatal
with the child’s development. event. The third option is to indicate that the
A further consideration is the possibility of perinatal event may have contributed to the
other explanations for the child’s impairment. impairments but that it is not possible with
Children from families with lower socioeconomic reasonable certainty to conclude that the event
status, fewer resources, and greater stress or insta- was a proximal cause.
bility are at increased risk for lower cognitive abil- Because opinions about prognosis and service
ities and more behavior problems (Bradley & needs, like those regarding causation, are made
Corwyn, 2002; Burchinal, Roberts, Hooper, & with reasonable neuropsychological certainty, the
Zeisel, 2000; McLoyd, 1998). For this reason, neuropsychologist may also be called upon to
environmental factors need to be taken into defend these judgments. While calling up one’s
account in interpreting assessment findings. One own clinical experience is relevant in justifying
way to do so is to revise developmental expecta- these opinions, reference to relevant research find-
tions for children growing up under disadvan- ings is essential in formulating evidence-based
taged circumstances. In weighing the influence of opinions. As summarized above, research on the
environmental factors, it is also important to long-term effects of VLBW/VPTB documents
entertain the possibility that the child’s disability residual problems in cognition, achievement, and
may itself have contributed to family adversity behavior. Similarly, longer-term follow-up studies
and to examine the child’s history of develop- of children with perinatal HIE or stroke, as well as
mental problems. Children whose problems are with other pediatric neurological conditions affect-
primarily due to environmental disadvantage ing infants and young children, offers convincing
may have less severe or selective deficits and his- evidence that deficits persist throughout develop-
tories that are relatively free of the early postnatal ment and into adulthood (Ballantyne et al., 2008;
Perinatal Brain Injury 195

Grimwood, Anderson, Anderson, Tan, & Nolan, On the other hand, the tendency for deficits to
2000; Hack, 2005; Haupt et al., 1994; Kerns, Don, persist over time suggests some ongoing impact
Mateer, & Streissguth, 1997; Klapper & Birch, on functioning even for children with less perva-
1966; Klonoff, Clark, & Klonoff, 1993; Lindstrom sive impairments.
et al., 2006; Maneru et al., 2001; Muter, Taylor, & Few controlled studies have been conducted to
Vargha-Khadem, 1997; Taylor & Alden, 1997; test the efficacy of rehabilitation programs, special
Taylor et al., 2000; Taylor et al., 2004a). Exceptions education services, or behavioral treatments
to this generalization include a tendency toward designed for children with perinatal brain insults
less pronounced language impairments and devel- (Taylor, 2009). Recommended interventions are
opmental delays across the first few years of life in thus based on clinical needs and typically include
children with VLBW/VPTB (Hack et al., 2005a; occupational, physical, and speech-language thera-
Luoma et al., 1998) and improvements relative to pies, psychological counseling, and educational
age standards in early language delays and in some programs. Other options include full-time care
later-developing language abilities following focal facilities for children or adults with the most severe
unilateral stroke (Ballantyne et al., 2008; Stiles et al., disabilities and assisted living or group home
2009). Other research suggests that some of the arrangements for adults requiring daily supervi-
cognitive consequences of VLBW/VPTB become sion. Each of these services can be justified by evi-
more pronounced with age relative to normative dence that the individual would be unable to
expectations, but only among children at highest function adequately without assistance and by
neonatal risk (Ment et al., 2003; Taylor et al., 2004b). research support for the effectiveness of rehabi-
The possibility the deficits become more pro- litation, education, and behavioral programs for
nounced with age is reasonable in view of the individuals with brain injuries (Beaulieu, 2002;
increasing cognitive and social demands imposed Cicerone et al., 2005; Ylvisaker et al., 2001; Ylvisaker
by higher levels of education, independent living, et al., 2005; Ylvisaker et al., 2007). Based on clinical
and entry into the work force. However, few studies experience with patients with neurodevelopmental
have followed children with perinatal brain insults disorders and the advice of educational experts
longitudinally and there is little basis for projecting (e.g., Goldstein, Sprafkin, Gershaw, & Klein, 1980;
overall declines in functioning relative to age Shapiro, 1996; Thompson, 1999), I typically
expectations in a majority of this population. recommend the types of strategies outlined in
Based on developmental research and studies Table 7.1. Considerations in making recommenda-
of children with various types of early brain insult, tions are the correspondence of treatment strate-
predictors of worse long-term outcomes include gies and regimens with the individual’s present or
educational or behavioral problems during the projected strengths and weaknesses, their consis-
school-age years, low IQ, and physical handicaps tency with current standards of practice, and their
(Cooke, 2004; Hack, 2005; Hack et al., 2005a; viability for the individual or family.
Hack et al., 2005b; Haupt et al., 1994; Klapper &
Birch, 1966; Klebanov et al., 1994). One or more Communicating Findings and
of these factors has been associated with more Providing Expert Testimony
limited educational attainments, more behavior Discussions of the assessment findings with attor-
problems, and lower rates of employment and neys and preparation of the written report take
independent living during adulthood. Risks for place after the neuropsychologist has drawn con-
unemployment and lack of independence may be clusions about the child’s strengths and weak-
greatest in children with mental retardation, nesses and made judgments about causation,
CP, or sensory disorders (Hack, 2005; Klapper future outcomes, and treatment needs. Professio-
& Birch, 1966; Richardson & Koller, 1996). I am nal integrity and the utility of the findings for
nevertheless circumspect in rendering judgments triers of fact demand that the neuropsychologist
about long-term outcomes based on findings from maintain objectivity in interpreting results and
evaluations conducted in childhood, as outcomes forming opinions. Steps that the neuropsycho-
will depend on subsequent experiences and inter- logists can take to protect against bias are to:
ventions. While limitations in independent func- (1) accept referrals with the explicit understand-
tioning during adulthood are likely for children ing that the evaluation will be objective and
with severe neurodevelopmental disabilities, long- impartial; (2) include multiple sources of
term outcomes may be less predictable for chil- information in drawing conclusions (e.g., test
dren with less profound or pervasive impairments. results, observations, background information,
TABLE 7.1. RECOMMENDATIONS FOR CHILDREN WITH PERINATAL BRAIN INSULTS

1. A predictable environment. Provide a predictable and consistent environment with clear and specific rules
and daily routines. Target particular behaviors for reward or correction, determine specific consequences
beforehand, and carry through on consequences as consistently as possible. Consequences should be
reasonable and appropriate (e.g., not letting the child go on to a second activity before cleaning up after the
first activity) and should not prevent the child from taking advantage of opportunities for positive interactions
with others.
2. Positive feedback. Provide frequent positive attention consisting of praise, or demonstrations of interest in how
the child is doing on an assigned task or about an area of special interest. Verbalize and then model behaviors
the child is expected to use when upset or frustrated. Avoid lecturing or threatening.
3. Structure. Structure tasks as much as possible, breaking complex assignments into simple steps and offering
guidance through each step to insure successful completion. If the child confuses multistep instructions,
present directions one step at a time, and limit the amount of new information given at one time. Set work
priorities so that the child does not become overwhelmed with too many demands at once. Allow for sufficient
practice to master the information or skill. Monitor learning to determine when supplemental instruction is
needed, and consider providing outside tutoring to reinforce classroom learning. Shorten periods of direct
instruction and intersperse these with high interest activities to aid sustained attention. Teach study and test
taking skills by incorporating these activities into the child’s daily lesson plans.
4. Engagement. Use “advanced organizers” (e.g., pre-exposure to vocabulary words in a new reading assignment,
or questions to answer in reading a textbook passage), focus on specific and explicit learning goals, and use
concrete methods and examples for tying concepts together or organizing information. Directly engage the
child in learning through active recitation, retelling, demonstrations, visual aids, and manipulatives. Provide
assistance with perceptual-motor and spatial tasks (e.g., charts, graphs, maps) through step-by-step approach,
modeling, and verbal guidance. Encourage the family to reinforce concepts at home by taking advantage
of activities of daily living. For example, teach number concepts by counting and grouping (adding and
subtracting) silverware when helping to set or clear the table or doing laundry.
5. Problem-solving. Use problem-solving approach in tackling larger projects. This approach includes
(1) identifying the goals, (2) brainstorming to consider possible plans of action and to select the best one,
(3) specifying steps to accomplish the plan, (4) getting together required materials, (5) carrying out the plan,
and (6) monitoring the product for accuracy and revising until goals reached. If original goals are not realistic,
reset goals and repeat above steps.
6. Social skills. Make rules for social interactions and communication with peers explicit and practice these skills.
To foster pragmatic language and social skills, place the child in smaller groups of children who are accepting,
who model positive social interactions, and who are responsive and patient (e.g., study buddies or project team
members).
7. Classroom routines. Have the child follow the same routine from the beginning to the end of class. For
example, at the beginning of each class have the child copy the assignment from the board or begin work
on a prearranged assignment. Consider seating the child next to a student who can help the child prepare
for classwork. Assemble a work folder that follows the child from class to class to encourage completion
of assignments during “down times” when the teacher cannot work with the child individually, or when
the regular class material may be inappropriate for the child. Also follow a set routine in doing homework.
Structure and anticipatory guidance may be especially important in transitioning to new settings or procedures
and in peer social interactions.
8. Appropriate goals. Assess the appropriateness of the level of work to be completed and the child’s
understanding of the assignment. Expectations should be within the child’s capabilities. Allow extra time to
complete assignments or for more direct engagement with materials. Encourage self correction of errors on
homework and tests. Go over these expectations with the child and provide cues regarding differing classroom
demands (e.g., log book of rules and assignments, list of rules posted to desk). Explain to the child how to ask
for help, when it is allowed to leave the room and for what reasons, what to do when there are interruptions in
the classroom, and when talking is permitted. Allow the child extra time to respond to requests or directions
and, whenever possible, help the child prepare ahead for question sessions or discussions.

196
Perinatal Brain Injury 197

TABLE 7.1. Continued


9. Test modifications. Modify classroom tests by: (1) arranging for individual review of test materials prior to test,
(2) telling the child how many answers will be true and how many answers will be false in a true/false test,
(3) reducing the number of foils in multiple-choice tests, (4) prearranging before the test, but unannounced to
the rest of the class, to have the child only attempt every other problem with double credit for each problem, or
(5) allowing the child to use class notes along with a study guide or any other essential aids (e.g., dictionary).
10. Support services. Make use of available resources to treat identified developmental, learning, and behavior
disorders, including special education, rehabilitation therapies (occupation, physical, speech-language),
accommodation plans, medical management as appropriate for the child’s condition, and counseling services.
11. Monitoring of progress and needs. Periodically reevaluate progress and school performance as the child
advances through the grades so that interventions can be adjusted as needs change. Assist in developing
learning skills, independence, and interests. Work toward higher educational and vocational attainments of
persons with special needs by suggesting enrollment in colleges or universities that have programs to meet
these needs or by recommending involvement in vocational rehabilitation programs. Re-evaluate levels of
functioning in young adulthood to determine capabilities for independent living and to arrange for appropriate
rehabilitation and support services. Some individuals may not need ongoing support or may require only
periodic counseling to assist in decision-making, coping with interpersonal problems, or management of
finances, while others may have to remain in the care of their family or be placed in group homes or assisted
living settings.

interviews, ratings); (3) consider multiple factors professionals or hospitals for care alleged to have
that may affect test findings and alternative expla- caused the condition. These suits may also claim
nations for the results; and (4) render only those that improper acute medical management led to a
opinions that can be defended based on the facts worsening of the condition and more adverse
of the case and relevant research literature (Borum, outcomes. In my experience, the most common
Otto, & Golding, 1993; Meyer et al., 2001; Sweet, claim is that the perinatal condition would
1999). The utility of written reports is enhanced not have occurred had pregnancy, fetal, or labor
by statements explaining the basis for the opin- status been properly monitored and actions
ions offered and by inclusion of relevant historical been taken to prevent its occurrence. Examples
information. In appearing at depositions and in of such actions include earlier treatments to
court to present and defend assessment methods prevent or forestall preterm birth or, more
and findings, it is useful to be well acquainted commonly, efforts to deliver full-term children
with the details of the case and to discuss the logic sooner or via a cesarean section rather than vagi-
of assessment and the basis of test interpretations nally. Another type of claim is that skull fractures
and other judgments in layman’s terms. The neu- or intracranial hemorrhages occurring at the time
ropsychologist is also well advised to ask for rep- of delivery were caused by delivery procedures,
etition and clarification to insure full understanding such as inappropriate or improper use of forceps
of questions posed, resist making unsupported or vacuum extraction. Medical malpractice suits
speculations, qualify responses to avoid overgen- involving a child with a perinatal condition fur-
eralizations, and confine testimony to one’s area of ther allege that the condition resulted in brain
competence (Adams & Rankin, 1996; Barsky & damage and that this damage is the primary
Gould, 2002; Greiffenstein & Kaufmann, present reason for the child’s subsequent cognitive, learn-
volume; Guilmette & Giuliano, 1991; Hess, 1999; ing, or behavior problems. My experience sug-
Larrabee, 2005). gests that personal injury claims are rarely made
in cases of preterm birth, perhaps because most
Challenges in Conducting Forensic of these births are spontaneous, induced deliver-
Assessments of Children with ies are performed out of medical necessity, and
Perinatal Conditions care is typically provided in neonatal intensive
A child with a perinatal condition is typically care units. Neuropsychological assessments are
referred for neuropsychological assessment most often requested in cases involving HIE or
after suit has been brought against medical stroke in full-term or near-term infants and entail
198 forensic neuropsychology

alleged intrapartum mismanagement of labor and vocabulary or word knowledge. Problems in


delivery. mathematics are also common. Questions about
Offering evidence-based opinion in these etiology would thus be raised if a school-age child
cases places several demands on the neuropsy- presented with a specific language-based learning
chologist. One of these demands is to be aware of disability with intact nonverbal and mathematics
evidence for and against the claim that the perina- skills. As evident from the above review, less is
tal condition was related to an intrapartum event. known about cognitive profiles associated with
The neuropsychologist is not qualified to assess HIE, although problems have been identified in
standards of care or make medical judgments. both language and nonverbal skills. The existing
However, knowledge with regard to both intra- literature suggests more age-dependent and subtle
partum and prepartum risk factors may be useful cognitive deficits in children with focal unilateral
in reviewing the medical record and in under- strokes. A related issue is the extent to which the
standing the basis of a suit. If the medical record severity of a perinatal brain insult corresponds
suggests that prepartum risks were present with the severity of the child’s impairment.
(e.g., pregnancy complications, thrombocytope- A child with less extreme VLBW/VPTB who did
nia), the neuropsychologist may be able to form not have significant neonatal complications is
an opinion as to whether HIE or stroke was the not likely to be diagnosed with moderate to
cause of a neuropsychological impairment, but severe mental retardation, while global develop-
may wish to avoid reference to intrapartum events mental deficits are common in survivors with
as the ultimate source of HIE or stroke. In these PHI (Sherlock et al., 2005). Likewise, global
instances, opinion with regard to the causal mech- mental deficiency would be more consistent with
anism responsible for HIE or stroke may be best severe HIE or large bilateral strokes than with
to deferred to the appropriate medical specialist. moderate HIE or unilateral strokes.
It is also important that the neuropsychologist Additional challenges relate to the young age
critically review evidence for the presence and at which children with perinatal conditions are
severity of perinatal complications and search often referred and the significant numbers of these
for information in the medical record that may children with neurological disorders. Tests appro-
help predict developmental outcomes. Although priate for infants and young children (birth
neuropsychologists rely on medical experts for through the preschool years) provide useful mea-
their opinions in this regard, definitions of HIE sures of cognitive skills and allow for some dif-
and asphyxia may vary across practitioners. ferentiation of abilities. Measures of early motor
Knowledge of the broader professional literature skills, language, adaptive functioning, and behav-
and standard definitions of this condition helps ior problems shed further light on early develop-
to maintain an evidence-based orientation. Depe- mental progress across multiple domains.
nding on their backgrounds and areas of speci- However, owing to the early stage of the child’s
alization, medical experts also vary in their development and to difficulties in testing young
knowledge of the factors that predict worse out- children, these assessments may provide limited
comes in children with the perinatal conditions information about learning and memory, execu-
discussed in this chapter. Awareness of medical tive functions, and higher-order reasoning. Except
risk factors, such as the degree of low birth weight/ in cases of more severe neurodevelopmental dis-
preterm birth and accompanying medical prob- ability, the eventual effects of brain insult on child
lems, the presence and severity of seizures and behavior, socialization, and academic competence
neurological disturbance, and abnormalities on may also be difficult to gauge from these evalua-
neonatal ultrasounds or postnatal neuroimaging, tions. Although assessments of early development
is useful in making and defending judgments are useful in determining needs for interventions,
about causation and prognosis. more definitive judgments with respect to the
A further challenge is to consider the possibil- effects of a perinatal brain insult on development
ity of alternative explanations for children’s may need to be deferred until the child is older
impairments. While no uniform or “signature” (Baron, 2008).
deficits are associated with perinatal brain insults, Determining the nature and extent of impair-
impairment in children with VLBW/VPTB is ment can also be problematic in children who
more likely on tests of perceptual-motor skill, have epilepsy, CP, or a sensory disorder. In these
memory, and executive function than on tests of cases, the consequences of perinatal brain insult
more “crystallized” abilities such as naming on test performance or behavior are confounded
Perinatal Brain Injury 199

by problems that the child has in manipulating or primarily in white matter and in subcortical and
seeing test materials, hearing instructions, or periventricular structures but vary with the degree
speaking intelligibly; or by active seizure activity of neonatal risk (Inder et al., 2005; Kesler et al.,
or side effects of anticonvulsant medications on 2004; Volpe, 2008, 2009). The sequelae of HIE
alertness or cognition. Tests that reduce depen- differ according to the severity of neonatal ence-
dence on motor skills or speech may be useful in phalopathy and the pattern of associated brain
assessing children with CP or sensory disorders. lesions, with generally worse outcomes for chil-
Examples include forced-choice tests of memory, dren with the basal ganglia/thalamus pattern of
spatial skills, or problem solving that minimize selective neuronal necrosis than for those with
demands on manipulative or oral-motor skills. watershed-type lesions. Strokes may result in
Efforts to become familiar with a child’s speech more focal damage and less developmental
patterns and testing limits by allowing extra time impairment than either VLBW/VPTB or HIE, but
for motor or oral responses can also be helpful in poorer outcomes are found in children with larger
discerning the nature of the child’s abilities. or bilateral lesions, neonatal encephalopathy, CP,
Testing of children with poorly controlled seizures and seizures. The three perinatal conditions also
may need to be put off until seizures are under have distinct neuromotor sequelae. CP in children
better control or spread across multiple sessions with VLBW/VPTB is more likely to involve the
to allow for assessment during seizure-free lower limbs than the upper limbs, while children
periods. The potential effects of anticonvulsant with HIE may manifest dyskinetic CP and
medications on test performance and behavior hemiplegia is more characteristic of children with
also need to be taken into account in interpreting perinatal stroke.
assessment findings. A third conclusion is that the developmental
consequences of perinatal brain insults differ from
CONCLUSIONS the effects of lesions sustained at later ages and
Survey of the existing literature reveals a good from common heritable conditions, such as learn-
deal of information that is relevant in conducting ing disabilities, speech-language impairment, and
neuropsychological assessments of children with ADHD. A recent comparison by Anderson et al.
perinatal brain insults, whether for forensic or (2009) of children with brain damage originating
nonforensic purposes. One conclusion from this at different stages of development revealed poorer
review is that the conditions that lead to most of cognitive outcomes in children with congenital
these insults—VLBW/VPTB, HIE, and stroke— and perinatal insults than in those with later-
cannot be considered unitary disorders with occurring lesions. Their results are consistent with
singular causes and outcomes. Each condition is other findings indicating more adverse develop-
highly variable with respect to severity and acute mental consequences of perinatal or early postna-
complications, precipitating factors, and deve- tal injuries (Max et al., 2004; Pavlovic et al., 2006).
lopmental outcomes. For example, while some At the same time, the research reviewed in this
children with VLBW/VPTB fail to exhibit demon- chapter suggests considerable potential for plastic-
strable developmental problems, other children ity of function and neural reorganization following
have selective neuropsychological, academic, or perinatal brain insults. Studies of focal perinatal
behavior problems despite normal IQ, and still stroke suggest that early-appearing deficits in lan-
others have severe and pervasive impairments. guage and visual-spatial abilities can even resolve
Children with HIE and stroke also have variable over time, albeit with subtle residual deficits
outcomes ranging from normal development to (Akshoomoff, Feroleto, Doyle, & Stiles, 2002; Stiles
selective neurobehavioral problems and more et al., 2009). A limited form of neural plasticity is
profound neurodevelopmental disorders. also implied by findings indicating that, despite
A second conclusion is that outcomes are persisting deficits, children with VLBW/VPTB
shaped in part by perinatal disease factors that are acquire many skills at rates similar to those of their
unique to each condition. For children with term-born peers (Taylor et al., 2004b).
VLBW/VPTB, these factors include the degree of In contrast to heritable disabilities, perinatal
prematurity, brain abnormalities evident in the brain insults are frequently associated with early
perinatal period or later in development, and postnatal neurological or developmental deficits.
neonatal medical complications, such as necrotiz- Other distinctions are specific to the type of
ing enterocolitis and chronic lung disease. perinatal insult. Children with focal perinatal
Brain insults in infants with VLBW/VPTB occur stroke have less extensive language impairments
200 forensic neuropsychology

and exhibit a different course of language devel- Few studies, for example, have examined develop-
opment than children with specific language mental outcomes in children with uncomplicated
impairment (Reilly & Wulfeck, 2004). Children forms of extraparenchymal hemorrhage. Finally,
with VLBW/VPTB are more prone to generalized to better appraise the “real life” consequences of
impairments in academic skills and pronounced perinatal brain injury we need to know more
problems in mathematics than children with con- about effects of perinatal brain insults on chil-
genital (nonacquired) forms of learning disabili- dren’s behavior and learning, to examine how
ties; and ADHD in this population may be these effects change with advancing age, and to
characterized more by inattentiveness than by evaluate adult outcomes. A related need is to iden-
hyperactivity (Litt et al., 2005; Taylor, Espy, & tify environmental and genetic factors that buffer
Anderson, 2009; Taylor, 2009; Taylor, in press). or exacerbate injury consequences, examine
The learning and behavior problems associated favorable outcomes in high risk children to
with HIE have not been well specified but include explore compensatory processes, and test treat-
deficits in executive function and new learning. ments to promote children’s development
Discovery of prominent problems in these areas (Aylward, 2005; de Haan et al., 2006; Luciana,
may thus be useful in distinguishing the develop- 2003; Taylor, 2009). Neural reorganization is pos-
mental “footprint” of these conditions from pat- sible but may be optimized by early and continu-
terns of impairment that are more typical of ing efforts to foster new learning (Nelson, 2000).
children with heritable disabilities. The utility of neuropsychological assessment
Further research is needed to enhance the stems from the sensitivity of tests of specific cog-
value of neuropsychological assessments of chil- nitive abilities to different types of insult and the
dren with perinatal brain insults. One important potential of these tests to further knowledge of
research direction will be to follow children with brain-behavior relationships. Results from these
these insults from an early age using measures evaluations are also useful in determining
that can evaluate specific cognitive abilities. the cognitive basis of behavior and learning prob-
Evaluations of outcomes have often been limited lems and in recommending interventions. An
to neurological diagnoses or global measures of evidence-based approach requires that findings
development, especially in assessing the effects of be interpreted in light of existing research on test
HIE (de Haan et al., 2006). As demonstrated by validity, disease manifestations, and predictors of
the extensive research literature on neuropsycho- outcome, as well as an appreciation of the multiple
logical outcomes of VLBW/VPTB, measures of influences on children’s development.
more subtle or selective deficits in language, per-
ceptual-motor and spatial skills, memory, and AC K N OW L E D G M E N T S
executive function are fundamental to compre- Past studies by our research team sited in the
hensive assessment of the sequelae of perinatal chapter were funded by NIH grants HD39756,
brain insults (Aylward, 2002). These measures HD26554, and HD34177. The author is indebted
also provide more precise endpoints for assessing to Maureen Hack, M.B.Ch.B. for her collaboration
the effects of variations in disease severity, perina- and leadership in the area of neonatal follow-up.
tal complications, and findings from brain imag-
ing. Research involving longer-term follow-up REFERENCES
and comprehensive neuropsychological testing Abernethy, L. J., Cooke, R. W. I., & Foulder-Hughes, L.
may uncover previously undetected consequences. (2004). Caudate and hippocampal volumes, intel-
To illustrate, this approach may reveal subtle ligence, and motor impairment in 7-year-old chil-
effects of mild HIE or of symptoms of asphyxia dren who were born preterm. Pediatric Research,
even when not accompanied by neonatal enceph- 55, 884–93.
Abernethy, L. J., Palaniappan, M., & Cooke, R. W. I.
alopathy. More objective and uniformly applied
(2002). Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging
measures of asphyxia, encephalopathy, and stroke
of the brain in survivors of very low birth weight.
will further advance research on risks for poor
Archives of Disease in Childhood, 87, 279–83.
outcomes of HIE (Ferriero, 2004; Raju et al., 2007; Adams, R. L., & Rankin, E. J. (1996). A practical guide to
Rolland & Hill, 1995). Forensic evaluations of forensic neuropsychological evaluations and
children with HIE will also be better informed by testimony. In R. L. Adams, O. A. Parsons, J. L.
research on the specific contributions of intrapar- Culbertson, & S. J. Nixon (Eds.), Neuropsychology
tum events to HIE and by studies of outcomes fol- for clinical practice: Etiology, assessment, and treat-
lowing different types of intracranial hemorrhage. ment of common neurological disorders (pp. 455–87).
Perinatal Brain Injury 201

Washington, DC: American Psychological Badawi, N., Felix, J. F., Kurinczuk, J. J., Dixon, G.,
Association. Watson, L., Keogh, J. M., et al. (2005). Cerebral
Adams-Chapman, I., Hansen, M. I., Stoll, B. J., Higgins, palsy following term newborn encephalopathy: A
R. (2008). Neurodevelopmental outcome of population-based study. Developmental Medicine
extremely low birth weight infants with posthem- & Child Neurology, 47, 293–98.
orrhagic hydrocephalus requiring shunt insertion. Badawi, N., Kurinczuk, J. J., Keogh, J. M., Alessandri, L.
Pediatrics, 121, e1167–77. M., O’Sullivan, F., Burton, P. R., et al. (1998a).
Akshoomoff, N. A., Feroleto, C. C., Doyle, R. E., & Antepartum risk factors for newborn encephalop-
Stiles, J. (2002). The impact of unilateral brain athy: The Western Australian case-control study.
injury on perceptual organization and visual British Medical Journal, 317, 1549–53.
memory. Neuropsychologia, 40, 539–61. Badawi, N., Kurinczuk, J. J., Keogh, J. M., Alessandri, L.
Ajayi-Obe, M., Saeed, N., Cowan, F. M., Rutherford, M., O’Sullivan, F., Burton, P. R., et al. (1998b).
M. A., & Edwards, A. D. (2000). Reduced develop- Intrapartum risk factors for newborn encephalop-
ment of cerebral cortex in extremely preterm athy: The Western Australian case-control study.
infants. The Lancet, 356, 1162–63. British Medical Journal, 317, 1554–58.
Alexander, G. R. & Slay, M. (2002). Prematurity at Ballantyne, A. O., Scarvie, K. M., & Trauner, D. A.
birth: Trends, racial disparities, and epidemiology. (1994). Verbal and Performance IQ patterns in
Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities children with perinatal stroke. Developmental
Research Reviews, 8, 215–20. Neuropsychology, 10, 39–50.
Allin, M., Henderson M., Suckling, J., Nosarti, C., Ballantyne, A. O., Spilkin, A. M., Hesselink, J., &
Rushe, T., Fearon, P., et al. (2004). Effects of very Trauner, D. A. (2008). Plasticity in the developing
low birth weight on brain structure in adulthood. brain: Intellectual, language and academic func-
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 46, tions in children with ischaemic perinatal stroke.
46–53. Brain, 131, 2975–85.
Allin, M., Rooney, M., Cuddy, M., Wyatt, J., Walshe, M., Ballantyne, A. O., Spilkin, A. M., & Trauner, D.A. (2007).
Rifkin, L., et al. (2006). Personality in young adults Language outcome after prenatal stroke: Does side
who are born preterm. Pediatrics, 117, 309–316. matter? Child Neuropsychology, 13, 494–509.
Ambalavanan, N., Carlo, W. A., Shankaran, S., Bann, Barnette, A. R. & Inder, T. E. (2009). Evaluation and
C. M., Emich, S. L., Higgins, R. D., et al. (2006). management of stroke in the neonate. Clinics in
Predicting outcomes of neonates diagnosed with Perinatology, 36, 125–36.
hypoxemic-ischemic encephalopathy. Pediatrics, Barnette, A., Mercuri, E., Rutherford, M., Haataja, L.,
118, 2084–93. Frisone, M. F., Henderson, S., et al. (2002).
Amiel-Tison, C., & Allison, P. (1986). Birth asphyxia in Neurological and perceptual-motor outcome at
the full-term newborn: Early assessment and out- 5–6 years of age in children with neonatal enceph-
come. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, alopathy: Relationship with neonatal brain MRI.
28, 671–82. Neuropediatrics, 33, 242–48.
Anderson, P., Doyle, L. W., and the Victorian Infant Baron, I. S. (2008). Maturation into impairment: The
Collaborative Study Group. (2003). Neurobehavioral merit of delayed settlement in pediatric forensic
outcomes of school-age children born extremely low neuropsychology cases. In R. L. Heilbronner (Ed.),
birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s. Journal of Neuropsychology in the courtroom: Expert analysis
the American Medical Association, 289, 3264–72. of reports and testimony (pp. 66–78). New York:
Anderson, V., Spencer-Smith, M., Leventer, R., Guilford.
Coleman, L., Anderson, P., Williams, J., et al. Barsky, A. & Gould, J. (2002). Clinicians in court. NY:
(2009). Childhood brain insult: Can age at insult Guilford.
help us predict outcome? Brain, 132, 45–56. Bassan, J., Limperopoulos, C., Visconti, K., Mayer, D.
Ansel, P-Y., Livinec, F., Larroque, B., Marret, S., Arnaud, L., Feldman, H. A., Avery, L., et al. (2007).
C., Pierrat, V., et al. (2006). Cerebral palsy among Neurodevelopmental outcome in survivors of
very preterm children in relation to gestational age periventricular hemorrhagic infarction. Pediatrics,
and neonatal ultrasound abnormalities: The 120, 785–92.
IPIPAGE Cohort Study. Pediatrics, 117, 828–35. Bates, E., Reilly, J., Wulfeck, B., Dronkers, N., Opie, M.,
Arkes, H.R. (1989). Principles in judgment/decision Fenson, J., et al. (2001). Differential effects of uni-
making research pertinent to legal proceedings. lateral lesions on language production in children
Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 7, 429–56. and adults. Brain and Language, 79, 223–65.
Aylward, G.P. (2005). Neurodevelopmental outcomes of Bates, E. & Roe, K. (2001). Language development in
infants born prematurely. Journal of Developmental children with unilateral brain injury. In C.A.
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 427–40. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Handbook of
202 forensic neuropsychology

developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 281– Brown, N. C., Inder, T. E., Bear, M. J., Hunt, R. W.,
307). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Anderson, P. J., & Doyle, L. W. (2009). Neuro-
Bava, S., Archibald, S. L., & Trauner, D.A. (2007). Brain behavior at term and white and gray matter abnor-
structure in prenatal stroke: Quantitative magnetic malities in very preterm infants. The Journal of
resonance imaging (MRI) analysis. Journal of Pediatrics, 155, 32–38.
Child Neurology, 22, 841–47. Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Hooper, S., & Zeisel,
Bava, S., Ballantyne, A. O., & Trauner, D.A. (2005). S. A. (2000). Cumulative risk and early cognitive
Disparity of verbal and performance IQ following development: A comparison of statistical risk
early bilateral brain damage. Cognitive and Beha- models. Developmental Psychology, 36, 793–807.
vioral Neurology, 18, 163–70. Chau, V., Poskitt, K. J., Sargent, M. A., Lupton, B. A.,
Beaulieu, C. (2002). Rehabilitation and outcome Hill, A., Roland, E., et al. (2009). Comparison of
following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Surgical computer tomography and magnetic resonance
Clinics of North America, 82, 393–408. imaging scans on the third day of life in term new-
Bergman, I., Bauer, R. E., Barmada, M. A., Latchaw, borns with neonatal encephalopathy. Pediatrics,
R. E., Taylor, H. G., David, R., et al. (1985). 123, 319–26.
Intracranial hemorrhage in the full-term neonatal Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Malec, J. F., Langenbahn,
infant. Pediatrics, 75, 488–96. D. M., Felicetti, T., Kneipp, S., et al. (2005).
Bhat, M. A., Charoo, B. A., Bhat, J. I., Ahmad, S. M., Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: Updated
Ali, S. W., & Mufti, M. (2009). Magnesium sulfate review of the literature from 1998 through 2002.
in severe prenatal asphyxia: A randomized, pla- Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
cebo-controlled trial. Pediatrics, 123, e1–e6. 86, 1681–92.
Blackman, J. A. (1988). The value of Apgar scores in Cioni, G., Sales, B., Paolicelli, P. B., Petacchi, E., Scusa,
predicting developmental outcome at age five. M. F., & Canapicchi, R. (1999). MRI and clinical
Journal of Perinatology, 8, 206–210. characteristics of children with hemiplegic cerebral
Board of Directors (2007). American Academy of palsy. Neuropediatrics, 30, 249–55.
Clinical Neuropsychology (AACN) practice guide- Constantinou, M. & McCaffrey, R.J. (2003). Using the
lines for neuropsychological assessment and consul- TOMM for evaluating children’s efforts to perform
tation. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21, 209–31. optimally on neuropsychological measures. Child
Borum, R., Otto, R., & Golding, S. (1993). Improving Neuropsychology, 9, 81–90.
clinical judgment and decision making in Cooke, R. W. I. (2004). Health, lifestyle, and quality of
forensic evaluation. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, life for young adults born very preterm. Archives of
21, 35–76. Disease in Childhood, 89, 201–206.
Botting, N., Powls, A., & Cooke, R. W. I. (1997). Cooke, R. W. I. (2005). Perinatal and postnatal factors
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorders and other in very preterm infants and subsequent cognitive
psychiatric outcomes in very low birth weight and motor abilities. Archives of Disease in Childhood.
children at 12 years. Journal of Child Psychology Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 90, F60–63.
and Psychiatry, 8, 931–41. Counsell, S. J. Allsop, J. M., Harrison, M. C., Larkman,
Bradley, R. H. & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic D. J., Kennia, J. L., Kapellou. O., et al. (2003).
status and child development. Annual Review of Diffusion-weighted imaging of the brain in pre-
Psychology, 53, 371–99. term infants with focal and diffuse white matter
Breslau, N., Brown, G. G., DelDotto, J. E., Kumar, S., abnormality. Pediatrics, 112, 1–7.
Ezhuthachan, S., Andreski, P., et al. (1996a). Courtney, J. C., Dinkins, J. P., Allen, L. M., III, &
Psychiatric sequelae of low birth weight at six years Kuroski, K. (2003). Age-related effects in children
of age. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 24, 385–400. taking the Computerized Assessment of Response
Breslau, N., Chilcoat, J. D., DelDotto, J. E., Andreski, P., Bias and Word Memory Test. Child Neuropsy-
& Brown, G. G. (1996b). Low birth weight and chology, 9, 109–116.
neurocognitive status at six years of age. Biological Cowan, F., Rutherford, M., Groenendaal, F., Eken, P.,
Psychiatry, 40, 389–97. Mercuri, E., Bydder, G. M., et al. (2003). Origin
Breslau, N., Chilcoat, H. D., Susser, E. S., Matte, T., Liang, and timing of brain lesions in term infants with
K., & Peterson, E. L. (2001). Stability and change in neonatal encephalopathy. Lancet, 361, 736–42.
children’s intelligence quotient scores: A comparison de Haan, M., Wyatt, J. S., Roth, S., Vargha-Khadem, F.,
of two socioeconomically disparate communities. Gadian, D., & Mishkin, M. (2006). Brain and cog-
American Journal of Epidemiology, 154, 711–717. nitive-behavioural development after asphyxia at
Breslau, N., Paneth, N. S., & Lucia, V. C. (2004). The term birth. Developmental Science, 9, 350–58.
lingering academic deficits of low birth weight Dennis, M. (1989). Assessing the neuropsychological
children. Pediatrics, 114, 1035–40. abilities of children and adolescents for personal
Perinatal Brain Injury 203

injury litigation. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 3, roles, and problems. In R. J. McCaffrey, A. Williams,
203–29. J. Fisher, & L. Laing (Eds.), The practice of forensic
deVeber, G., Andrew, M., Adams, C., Bjornson, B., neuropsychology: Meeting challenges in the court-
Booth, F., Buckley, D. J., et al. (2001). Cerebral room (pp. 1–35). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
sinovenous thrombosis in children. The New Goldberg, M. A. (2006). Examining children in per-
England Journal of Medicine. 345, 417–23. sonal injury claims. In S. Sparta & G. P. Koocher
De Vries, L. S., Van der Grond, J. Van Haastert, I. C., & (Eds.), Forensic mental health assessment of children
Groenendaal, F. (2005). Prediction of outcome in and adolescents (pp. 245–59). NY: Oxford.
newborn infants with arterial ischaemic stroke Goldenberg, R. L. & Rouse, D.J. (1998). Prevention of
using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance premature birth. The New England Journal of
imaging. Neuropediatrics, 36, 12–20. Medicine, 229, 313–20.
Dilenge, M.-E., Majnemer, A., & Shevell, M. I. (2001). Goldstein, A. P., Sprafkin, R. P., Gershaw, N. J., & Klein,
Long-term developmental outcome of asphyxiated P. (1980). Skill streaming the adolescent: A struc-
term neonates. Journal of Child Neurology, 16, tured learning approach to teaching prosocial skills.
781–92. Champaign IL: Research Press.
Dixon, G., Badawi, N., Kurinczuk, J. J., Keogh, J. M., Golomb, M. R., MacGregor, D. L., Domi, T., Armstong,
Silburn, S. R., Zubrick, S. R., et al. (2002). Early D. C., McCrindle, B. W., Mayank, S., et al. (2001).
developmental outcomes after newborn encephal- Presumed pre- or perinatal arterial ischemic stroke:
opathy. Pediatrics, 109, 26–33. Risk factors and outcomes. Annals of Neurology,
Donders, J. (2005). Forensic aspects of pediatric trau- 50, 163–68.
matic brain injury. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.), Forensic Golomb, M. R., Saha, C., Garg, B., Azzouz, F., & Williams,
neuropsychology: A scientific approach (pp. 182– L. S. (2007). Association of cerebral palsy with other
208). NY: Oxford. disabilities in children with perinatal arterial isch-
D’Souza, S. W., McCartney, E., Nolan, M., & Taylor, I. emic stroke. Pediatric Neurology, 37, 245–49.
G. (1981). Hearing, speech, and language in survi- Gonzalez, F. F. & Miller, S. P. (2006). Does perinatal
vors of severe perinatal asphyxia. Archives of asphycia impair cognitive function without cere-
Disease in Childhood, 56, 245–52. bral palsy? Archives of Disease in Childhood Fetal
Edgin, J. O., Inder, T. E., Anderson, P. J., Hood, K. M., and Neonatal Edition, 91, F454–59.
Clark, C. A. C., & Woodward, L. J. (2008). Executive Green, P. & Flaro, L. (2003). Word Memory Test per-
functioning in preschool children born very pre- formance in children. Child Neuropsychology, 9,
term: Relationship with early white matter pathol- 189–207.
ogy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Grimwood, K., Anderson, P., Anderson, V., & Nolan, T.
Society, 14, 90–101. (2000). Twelve-year outcomes following bacterial
Espy, K. A., Stalets, M. M., McDiarmid, M. M., Senn, T. meningitis: Further evidence for persisting effects.
E. Cwik, M. F., & Hamby, A. (2002). Executive Archives of Diseases of Childhood, 83, 111–116.
functions in preschool children born preterm: Grunau, R. V. E., Kearney, S. M., & Whitfield, M. F.
Application of cognitive neuroscience paradigms. (1990). Language development at 3 years in preterm
Child Neuropsychology, 8, 83–92. children of birth weight below 1000 g. British
Fanaroff, A. A., Stoll, B. J., Wright, L. L., Carlo, W. A., Journal of Disorders of Communication, 25, 173–82.
Ehrenkranz, R. A., Stark, A. R., et al., for the Guilmette, T. J. & Giuliano, A. J. (1991). Taking the
NICHD Neonatal Research Network. (2007). stand: Issues and strategies in forensic neuro-
Trends in neonatal morbidity and mortality for psychology. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5,
very low birthweight infants. American Journal of 197–219.
Obstetrics & Gynecology, 196, 147.e1–47.e8. Gupta, S. N., Kechli, A. M., & Kanamalla, U. S. (2009).
Ferriero, D. M. (2004). Neonatal brain injury. New Intracranial hemorrhage in term newborns:
England Journal of Medicine, 351, 1985–95. Management and outcomes. Pediatric Neurology,
Fitzgerald, K. C., Williams, L. S., Garg, B., Carvalho, K. 40, 1–12.
S., & Golomb, M. R. (2006). Cerebral sinovenous Hack, M. (2005). Young adult outcomes of very-
thrombosis in the neonate. Archives of Neurology, low-birth-weight children. Seminar in Fetal &
63, 405–409. Neonatal Medicine, 11, 127–37.
Gadian, D. G., Aicardi, J., Watkins, K. E., Porter, D. A., Hack, M., Flannery, D. J., Schluchter, M., Carter, L.,
Mishkin, M., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2000). Borawski, E., & Klein, N. (2002). Outcomes in
Developmental amnesia associated with early young adulthood for very-low-birthweight infants.
hypoxic-ischaemic injury. Brain, 123, 449–507. The New England Journal of Medicine, 346, 149–57.
Giuliano, A. J., Barth, J. T., Hawk, G. L., & Ryan, T. V. Hack, M., Taylor, H. G., Drotar, D., Schluchter, M.,
(1997). The forensic neuropsychologist: Precedents, Cartar, L., Wilson-Costello, D., et al. (2005a).
204 forensic neuropsychology

Poor predictive validity of the Bayley Scales of Hintz, S. R., Kendrick, D. E., Stoll. B. J., Vohr, B. R.,
Infant Development for cognitive function of Fanaroff, A. A., Donovan, E. F., et al., for the
extremely low birth weight children at school age. National Institute of Child Health, Human
Pediatrics, 10, 333–41. Developmental Neonatal Research Network
Hack, M., Wilson-Costello, D., Friedman, H., Taylor, (2005a). Neurodevelopmental and growth out-
H. G., Schluchter, M., & Fanaroff, A. A. (2000). comes of extremely low birth weight infants after
Neurodevelopment and predictors of outcomes of necrotizing enterocolitis. Pediatrics, 115, 696–703.
children with birth weights of less than 1000g. Hintz, S. R., Kendrick, D. E., Vohr, B. R., Poole, W. K.,
Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 154, & Higgins, R. D., for the National Institute of Child
725–31. Health, Human Developmental Neonatal Research
Hack, M., Youngstrom, E., Cartar, L., Schluchter, M., Network (2005b). Changes in neurodevelopmental
Taylor, H. G., Flannery, D. J., et al. (2004). outcomes at 18 and 22 months’ corrected age
Behavioral outcomes and evidence of psychopa- among infants of less than 25 weeks’ gestational age
thology among very low birth weight infants at age born in 1993–1999. Pediatrics, 115, 1645–51.
20 years. Pediatrics, 114, 932–40. Hoff, B., Hansen, B. M., Munck, H., & Mortensen, E. L.
Hack, M., Youngstrom, E. A, Cartar, L, Schluchter, M., (2004). Behavioral and social development of chil-
Taylor, H. G., Flannery, D., et al. (2005b). Predictors dren born extremely premature: 5-year follow-up.
of internalizing symptoms among very low birth Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 285–92.
weight young women. Journal of Developmental Hogan, A. M., Kirkham, F. J., & Isaacs, E. B. (2000).
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 93–104. Intelligence after stroke in childhood: Review of
Hamilton, B. E., Martin, J. A., & Ventura, S. J. (2006). the literature and suggestions for future research.
Birth: Preliminary data for 2005. National vital sta- Journal of Child Neurology, 15, 325–32.
tistics report, National Center for Health Statistics, Huang, B. & Castillo, M. (2008). Hypoxic-ischemic
55, Hyattsville, MD. brain injury: Imaging findings from birth to adult-
Hanigan, W. C., Powell, F. C., Miller, T. C., & Wright, R. hood. RadioGraphics, 28, 417–39.
M. (1995). Symptomatic intracranial hemorrhage Huppi, P. S. & Inder, T. E. (2001). Magnetic resonance
in full-term infants. Child’s Nervous System, 11, techniques in the evaluation of the perinatal brain:
698–707. Recent advances and future directions. Seminars in
Hartman, D. E. (1999). Neuropsychology and the Neonatology, 6, 195–210.
(neuro)-toxic tort. In J. J. Sweet (Ed.), Forensic neu- Huppi, P. S., Schuknecht, B., Boesch, C., Bossi, E.,
ropsychology: Fundamentals and practice (pp. 339– Felblinger, J., Fusch, C., et al. (1996). Structural and
67). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. neurobehavioral delay in postnatal brain develop-
Haupt, R., Fears, T. R., Robison, L. L., Mills, J. L., ment of preterm infants. Pediatric Research, 39,
Nicholson, H. S., Zeltzer, L. K., et al. (1994). 895–901.
Educational attainment in long-term survivors of Inder, T. E., Huppi, P. S., Warfield, S., Kikinis, R., Zientara,
childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Journal G. P., Barnes, P. D., et al. (1999). Periventricular
of the American Medical Association, 272, 1427–32. white matter injury in the premature infant is fol-
Heilbronner, R. L., Sweet, J. J., Morgan, J. E., Larrabee, lowed by reduced cerebral cortical gray matter
G. J., Millis, S. R., and Conference Participants. volume at term. Annals of Neurology, 46, 755–60.
(2009). American Academy of Clinical Neuro- Inder, T. E., Wells, S. J., Mogridge, N. B., Spencer, C., &
psychology consensus conference statement on the Volpe, J. J. (2003). Defining the nature of the cere-
neuropsychological assessment of effort, response bral abnormalities in the premature infant: A qual-
bias, and malingering. The Clinical Neuro- itative magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal
psychologist, 23, 1093–129. of Pediatrics, 143, 171–79.
Henry, G. K. (2005). Childhood malingering: Faking Inder, T. E. & Volpe, J. J. (2000). Mechanisms of perina-
neuropsychological impairment in an 8-year-old. tal brain injury. Seminars in Neonatology, 5, 3–16.
In R. L. Heilbronner (Ed.), Forensic neuropsycho- Inder, T. E., Warfield, S. K., Wang, H., Huppi, P. S., &
logical casebook (pp. 205–217). NY: Guilford. Volpe, J. J. (2005). Abnormal cerebral structure is
Hess, A. K. (1999). Serving as an expert witness. In A. present at term in premature infants. Pediatrics,
K. Hess & I. B. Weiner (Eds.), The handbook of 115, 286–94.
forensic psychology—Second Edition (pp. 521–55). Indredavik, M. S., Skranes, J. S., Vik, T., Heyerdahl, S.,
New York: John Wiley & Sons. Romundstad, P., Myhr, G. E., et al. (2005). Low-
Himmelmann, K., Hagberg, G., Wiklund, L. M., Eek, birth-weight adolescents: Psychiatric symptoms
M. N., & Uvebrant, P. (2007). Dyskinetic cerebral and cerebral MRI abnormalities. Pediatric
palsy: A population-based study of children born Neurology, 33, 259–66.
between 1991 and 1998. Developmental Medicine Ishaik, G., Mirabella, G., Asztalos, E., Perlma, K., &
and Child Neurology, 49, 246–51. Rovet, J. (2000). Hypothyroxinemia of prematurity
Perinatal Brain Injury 205

and the development of attention and memory behaviors in children at varying degrees of biologi-
in infants with low risk prematurity: A pilot cal risk. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1040–53.
study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C. L., &
Pediatrics, 21, 172–79. Swank, P. R. (1998). The relation of change in
Jain, L., Ferre, C., Vidyasagar, K., Nath, S., & Sheftel, D. maternal interactive styles to the developing social
(1991). Cardiopulmonary resuscitation of appar- competence of full-term and preterm children.
ently stillborn infants: Survival and long-term out- Child Development, 69, 105–23.
come. Journal of Pediatrics, 118, 778–82. Laptook, A. R., O’Shea, M., Shankaran, S., Bhaskar, B., &
Jan, M. M. S. & Camfield, P. R. (1998). Outcome of the NICHD Neonatal Network. (2005). Adverse neu-
neonatal stroke in full-term infants without signifi- rodevelopmental outcomes among extremely low
cant birth asphyxia. European Journal of Pediatrics, birth weight infants with a normal head ultrasound:
157, 846–48. Prevalence and antecedents. Pediatrics, 115, 673–80.
Jhawar, B. S., Ranger, A., Steven, D., & Del Maestro, R. Larrabee, G. J. (2005). Forensic neuropsychology:
F. (2003). Risk factors for intracranial hemorrhage A scientific approach. New York: Oxford.
among full-term infants: A case-control study. Larrabee, G. J. (2000). Forensic neuropsychological
Neurosurgery Online, 52, 581–90. assessment. In R. D. Vanderploeg (Ed.), Clinician’s
Jhawar, B. S., Ranger, A., Steven, D. A., & Del Maestro, guide to neuropsychological assessment (pp. 301–
R. F. (2005). A follow-up study of infants with 35). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
intracranial hemorrhage at full-term. The Canadian Lawrence, R. K. & Inder, T. E. (2008). Anatomic
Journal of Neurological Sciences, 32, 332–39. changes and imaging in the term infant. Clinics in
Kerns, K. A., Don, A., Mateer, C. A., & Streissguth, A. Perinatology, 35, 679–93.
P. (1997). Cognitive deficits in nonretarded adults Lee, J., Croen, L. A., Backstrand, K. H., Yoshida, C. K.,
with fetal alcohol syndrome. Journal of Learning Henning, L. H., Lindan, C., et al. (2005a). Maternal
Disabilities, 30, 685–93. and infant characteristics associated with perinatal
Kesler, S. R., Ment, L. R., Vohr, B., Pajot, S. K., Schneider, arterial strike in the infant. Journal of the American
K. C., Katz, K. H., et al. (2004). Volumetric analysis Medical Association, 293, 723–29.
of regional cerebral development in preterm chil- Lee, J., Croen, L. A., Lindan, C., Nash, K. B., Yoshida,
dren. Pediatric Neurology, 31, 318–25. C. K., Ferriero, D. M., et al. (2005b). Predictors of
Kilbride, H. W., Thorstad, K., & Daily, D. K. (2004). outcome in perinatal arterial stroke: A population-
Preschool outcome of less than 801-gram preterm based study. Annals of Neurology, 58, 303–308.
infants compared with full-term siblings. Pediatrics, Lewin, J. (1998). The genesis and evolution of legal
113, 742–47. uncertainty about “reasonable medical certainty.”
Kirton, A., deVeber, G., Pontigon, A-M., MacGregor, Maryland Law Review, 57, 380–504.
D., & Shroff, M. (2008). Presumed perinatal isch- Lidzba, K., Staudt, M., Wilke, M., & Krageloh-Mann, I.
emic stroke: Vascular classification predicts out- (2006). Visuospatial deficits in patients with early
comes. Annuals of Neurology, 63, 436–43. left-hemispheric lesions and functional reorgani-
Klapper, Z. S. & Birch, H. G. (1966). The relation of zation of language: Consequence of lesion or reor-
childhood characteristics to outcome in young ganization? Neuropsychologia, 44, 1088–94.
adults with cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicine Lindstrom, K., Lagerroos, P., Gillberg, C., & Fernell, E.
and Child Neurology, 8, 645–56. (2006). Teenage outcome after being born at term
Klebanov, P. K., Brooks-Gunn, J., & McCormick, M.C. with moderate neonatal encephalopathy. Pediatric
(1994). School achievement and failure in very low Neurology, 35, 268–74.
birthweight children. Developmental and Behavioral Linnet, K. M., Wisborg, K., Agerbo, E., Secher, N. J.,
Pediatrics, 15, 248–56. Thomsen, P. H., & Henriksen, T.B. (2006).
Klonoff, H., Clark, C., & Klonoff, P. S. (1993). Long-term Gestational age, birth weight, and the risk of hyper-
outcome of head injuries: A 23-year follow-up study kinetic disorder. Archives of Disease in Childhood,
of children with head injuries. Journal of Neurology, 91, 655–60.
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 56, 410–415. Litt, J., Taylor, H. G., Klein, N., & Hack, M. (2005).
Koelfen, W., Freund, M., Konig, S., Varnholt, V., Rohr, Learning disabilities in children with very low
H., & Schultze, C. (1993). Results of parenchymal birth weight: Prevalence, neuropsychological cor-
and angiographic magnetic resonance imaging and relates, and educational interventions. Journal of
neuropsychological testing of children after stroke Learning Disabilities, 38, 130–41.
as neonates. European Journal of Pediatrics, 152, Lu, P. H. & Boone, K. B. (2002). Suspect cognitive
1030–35. symptoms in a 9-year-old child: Malingering by
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C.L., & proxy? The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 1, 90–96.
Swank, P. R. (1997). Predicting cognitive-language Luciana, M. (2003). Cognitive development in children
and social growth curves from early maternal born preterm: Implications for theories of brain
206 forensic neuropsychology

plasticity following early injury. Development and Haynes (Eds.), (2000). Evidence-based medicine:
Psychopathology, 15, 1017–47. How to practice and teach EBM. Edinburgh,
Luoma, L., Herrgard, E., Martikainen, A., & Ahonen, Scotland: Churchill Livingstone.
T. (1998). Speech and language development of Mikkola, K., Ritari, N., Tommiska, V., Salokorpi, T.,
children born at <32 weeks gestation: A 5-year Lehtonen, L., Tammela, O., et al. (2005). Neuro-
prospective follow-up study. Developmental developmental outcome at 5 years of age of a national
Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 380–87. cohort of extremely low birth weight infants who
Luu, T. M., Ment, L. R., Schneider, K. C., Katz, D. H., were born in 1996–1997. Pediatrics, 116, 1391–
Allan, W. C., & Vohr, B. R. (2009). Lasting effects of 1400.
preterm birth and neonatal brain hemorrhage at 12 Miller, S. P., Ramaswamy, V., Michelson, D., Barkovich,
years of age. Pediatrics, 123, 1037–44. A. J., Holshouser, B., Wycliffe, N., et al. (2005).
Maneru, C., Junque, C., Botet, F., Tallada, M., & Patterns of brain injury in term neonatal encephal-
Guardia, J. (2001). Neuropsychological long-term opathy. Journal of Pediatrics, 146, 453–60.
sequelae of perinatal asphyxia. Brain Injury, 15, Moster, D., Lie, R. T., & Markestad, T. (2002). Joint
1029–39. association of Apgar scores and early neonatal
Marlow, N., Hennessy, E. M., Bracewell, M.A., Wolke, symptoms with minor disabilities at school age.
D., for the EPICure Study Group. (2007). Motor Archives of Disease in Childhood Fetal Neonatal
and executive function at 6 years of age after Edition, 86, F16–21.
extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics, 120, 793–804. Moster, D., Lie, R. T., & Markestad, T. (2008). Long-
Marlow, N., Rose, A. S., Rands, C. E., & Draper, E. S. term medical and social consequences of preterm
(2005). Neuropsychological and educational prob- birth. The New England Journal of Medicine, 359,
lems at school age associated with neonatal enceph- 262–73.
alopathy. Archives of Disease in Childhood Fetal & Msall, M. E. (2006). Neurodevelopmental surveillance
Neonatal Edition, 90, F380–87. in the first 2 years after extremely preterm birth:
Max, J. E., Robin, D. A., Taylor, H. G., Yeates, K. O., Fox, Evidence, challenges, and guidelines. Early Human
P. T., Lancaster, J. L., et al. (2004). Attention function Development, 82, 157–66.
after childhood stroke. Journal of the International Muter, V., Taylor, S., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (1997).
Neuropsychological Society, 10, 976–86. A longitudinal study of early intellectual develop-
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage ment in hemiplegic children. Neuropsychologia, 35,
and child development. American Psychologist, 53, 289–98.
185–204. Nelson, C. A. (2000). Neural plasticity and human
Ment, L. R., Vohr, B., Allan, W., Katz, K. H., Schneider, development: The role of early experience in
K. C., Westerveld, M., et al. (2003). Change in sculpting memory systems. Developmental Science,
cognitive function over time in very low-birth- 3, 115–36.
weight infants. Journal of the American Medical Nelson, K. B. & Lynch, J. K. (2004). Stroke in newborn
Association, 289, 705–11. infants. The Lancet Neurology, 3, 150–58.
Mercuri, E., Anker, S., Guzzetta, A., Barnette, A., Nosarti, C., AlAsady, M. H. S., Frangou, S., Stewart, A.
Haataja, L., Rutherford, M., et al. (2003). Neonatal L., Rifkin, L., & Murray, R. M. (2002). Adolescents
cerebral infarction and visual function at school who were born very preterm have decreased brain
age. Archives of Disease in Childhood Fetal and volumes. Brain, 125, 1616–23.
Neonatal Edition, 88, F487–91. Nosarti, C., Giouroukou, E., Healy, E., Rifkin, L.,
Mercuri, E., Anker, S., Guzzetta, A., Barnette, A. L., Walshe, M., Reichenberg, A., et al. (2008). Gray
Haataja, L., Rutherford, M., et al. (2004). Visual and white matter distribution in very preterm ado-
function at school age in children with neonatal lescents mediates neurodevelopmental outcome.
encephalopathy and low Apgar scores. Archives of Brain, 131, 205–217.
Disease in Childhood Fetal and Neonatal Edition, Nosarti, C., Rushe, T. M., Woodruff, P. W. R., Stewart,
89, F258–62. A. L., Rifkin, L., & Murray, R. M. (2004).
Mercuri, E., Barnette, A., Rutherford, M., Guzzetta, A., Corpus callosum size and very preterm birth:
Haataja, L., Cioni, G., et al. (2004). Neonatal cere- Relationship to neuropsychological outcome.
bral infarction and neuromotor outcome at school Brain, 127, 2080–89.
age. Pediatrics, 113, 95–100. Odd, D. E., Lewis, G., Whitelaw, A., & Gunnell, D.
Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., (2009). Resuscitation at birth and cognition
Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R., et al. (2001). at 8 years of age: A cohort study. Lancet, 373,
Psychological testing and psychological assess- 1615–22.
ment: A review of evidence. In D. L. Sackett, S. E. Odd, D. E., Rasmussen, F., Gunnell, D., Lewis, G., &
Straus, W. S. Richardson, W. Rosenberg, & R. B. Whitelaw, A. (2008). A cohort study of low Apgar
Perinatal Brain Injury 207

scores and cognitive outcomes. Archives of Reilly, J. S. & Wulfeck, B. B. (2004). Issues in plasticity
Disease in Childhood Fetal & Neonatal Edition, 93, and development: Language in atypical children.
F115–20. Brain and Language, 88, 163–66.
Pasternak, J. F. & Gorey, M. T. (1998).The syndrome of Rennie, J. M., Hagmann, C. F., & Robertson, N. J.
acute near-total intrauterine asphyxia in the term (2007). Outcome after intrapartum hypoxic ischae-
infant. Pediatric Neurology, 18, 391–98. mia at term. Seminars in Fetal & Neonatal Medicine,
Patra, K., Wilson-Costello, D., Taylor, H. G., Minich, 12, 398–407.
N., & Hack, M. (2006). Grades I-II intraventricular Resnick, M. B., Gueorguieva, R. V., Carter, R. L., Ariet,
hemorrhage in extremely low birth weight infants: M., Sun, Y., Roth, J., et al. (1999). The impact of low
Effects on neurodevelopment. Journal of Pediatrics, birth weight, prenatal conditions, and sociodemo-
149, 169–73. graphic factors on educational outcome in kinder-
Pavlovic, J., Kaufmann, F., Boltshauser, E., Mori, A. C., garten. Pediatrics, 104, e74.
Mercati, D. G., Haenggeli, C.-A., et al. (2006). Richardson, S. A. & Koller, H. (1996). Twenty-two
Neuropsychological problems after paediatric years: Causes and consequences of mental retarda-
stroke: Two year follow-up of Swiss children. tion. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Neuropediatrics, 37, 13–19. Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2005).
Perlman, J. M. (1997). Intrapartum hypoxic-ischemic Recall memory in the first three years of life: A lon-
cerebral injury and subsequent cerebral palsy: gitudinal study of preterm and term children.
Medicolegal issues. Pediatrics 99, 851–59. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47,
Perlman, J. M., (2004). Brain injury in the term infant. 653–59.
Seminars in Perinatology, 28, 515–424. Robertson, C. & Finer, N. (1985). Term infants with
Perlman, J. M. (2006). Intrapartum asphyxia and cere- hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy: Outcome at 3.5
bral palsy: Is there a link? Clinics in Perinatology, years. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology,
33, 335–53. 27, 473–84.
Peterson, B. S., Anderson, A. W., Ehrenkranz, R., Staib, Robertson, C. M. T., & Finer, N. N. (1988). Educational
L. H., Tageldin, M., Colson, E., et al. (2003). readiness of survivors of neonatal encephalopathy
Regional brain volumes and their later neurodevel- associated with birth asphyxia at term. Journal
opmental correlates in term and preterm infants. of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 9,
Pediatrics, 111, 939–48. 298–306.
Peterson, B. S., Vohr, B., Staib, L. H., Cannistraci, C. J., Robertson, C. M. T., Finer, N. N., & Grace, M. G. A.
Dolberg, A., Schneider, K. C., et al. (2000). Regional (1989). School performance of survivors of neona-
brain volume abnormalities and long-term cogni- tal encephalopathy associated with birth asphyxia
tive outcome in preterm infants. Journal of the at term. Journal of Pediatrics, 114, 753–60.
American Medical Association, 284, 1939–47. Robertson, C. M. T. & Grace, M. G. A. (1992).
Raju, T. N. K., Nelson, K. B., Ferriero, D., Lynch, Validation of prediction of kindergarten-age
J.K., and the NICHD-NINDS Perinatal Stroke school-readiness scores of nondisabled survivors
Workshop Participants. (2007). Ischemic perinatal of moderate neonatal encephalopathy in term
stroke: Summary of a workshop sponsored by the infants. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 83
National Institute of Child Health and Human (Supplement 2), S51–57.
Development and the National Institute of Rolland, E. H. & Hill, A. (1995). Clinical aspects of
Neurological Disorders and Stroke. Pediatrics, 120, perinatal hypoxic-ischemic brain injury. Seminars
609–616. in Pediatric Neurology, 2, 57–71.
Ramaswamy, V., Miller, S. P., Barkovich, A. J., Partridge, Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., Jankowski, J. J., & Van
J. C., & Ferriero, D. M. (2004). Perinatal stroke in Rossem, R. (2005). Pathways from prematurity and
term infants with neonatal encephalopathy. infant abilities to later cognition. Child Development,
Neurology, 62, 2088–91. 76, 1172–84.
Redline, R. W. (2008). Cerebral palsy in term infants: Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., Rose, S. L., Wallace, I. F., &
A clinicopathologic analysis of 158 mediocolegal McCarton, C. (1992). Behavior problems at 3 and 6
case reviews. Pediatric and Developmental years: Prevalence and continuity in full-terms and
Pathology, 11, 456–64. preterms. Development and Psychopathology, 4,
Redline, R. W., Minich, N., Taylor, H. G., & Hack, M. 361–74.
(2007). Placental lesions as predictors of cerebral Roze, E., Van Braeckel, K. N. J. A., van der Veere, C. N.,
palsy and abnormal neurocognitive function at 8 Maathuis, C. G. B., Martijn, A., & Bos, A. F. (2009).
years of age in extremely low birth weight infants Functional outcome at school age of preterm
(<1 Kg). Pediatric and Developmental Pathology, infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarc-
10, 282–92. tion. Pediatrics, 123, 1493–1500.
208 forensic neuropsychology

Rutherford, M. A., Pennock, J. M., Counsell, S. J., haemorrhage at 8 years of age in a regional cohort
Mercuri, E., Cowan, F. M., Dubowitz, L. M. S., et al. of ELBW/very preterm infants., Early Human
(1998). Abnormal magnetic resonance signal in Development, 81, 909–916.
the internal capsule predicts poor neurodevelop- Srinivasan, L., Dutta, R., Counsell, S. J., Allsop, J. M.,
mental outcome in infants with hypoxic-ischemic Baordman, J. P., Rutherford, M. A., et al. (2007).
encephalopathy. Pediatrics, 102, 323–28. Quantification of deep gray matter in preterm
Rutherford, M., Pennock, J., Schwieso, J., Cowan, F., & infants at term-equivalent age using manual
Dubowitz, L. (1996). Hypoxic-ischaemic enceph- volumetry of 3-Tesla magnetic resonance images.
alopathy: Early and late magnetic resonance imag- Pediatrics, 119, 759–65.
ing findings in relation to outcome. Archives of Staudt, M., Krageloh-Mann, I., Holthausen, J., Gerloff,
Disease in Childhood, 75, F145–51. C., & Grodd, W. (2004). Searching for motor func-
Saigal, S. (2000). Follow-up of very low birthweight tions in dysgenic cortex: A clinical transcranial
babies to adolescence. Seminars in Neonatology, 5, magnetic stimulation and functional magnetic
107–118. resonance imaging study. Journal of Neurosurgery
Saigal, S., Pinelli, J., Hoult, L., Kim, M. M., & Boyle, M. Pediatrics 101, 69–77.
(2003). Psychopathology and social competencies Staudt, M., Lidzba, K., Grodd, W., Wildgruber, D., Drb,
of adolescents who were extremely low birth M., & Drageloh-Mann, I. (2002). Right-hemispheric
weight. Pediatrics, 111, 969–75. organization of language following early left-
Saigal, S. & Rosenbaum, P. (2007). What matters in the sided brain lesions: Functional MRI topography.
long term: Reflections on the context of adult out- NeuroImage, 16, 954–67.
comes versus detailed measures in childhood. Steinman, K. J., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Glidden, D. V.,
Seminars in Fetal & Neonatal Medicine, 12, 415–22. Kramer, J. H., Miller, S. P., Barkovich, A. J., et al.
Saigal, S., Stoskopf, B., Boyle, M., Paneth, N., Pinelli, J., (2009). Neonatal watershed brain injury on
Streiner, D., et al. (2007). Comparison of current magnetic resonance imaging correlates with verbal
health, functional limitations, and health care of IQ at 4 years. Pediatrics, 123, 1025–30.
young adults who were born with extremely low Stiles, J., Nass, R. D., Levine, S. C., Moses, P., & Reilly, J.
birth weight and normal birth weight. Pediatrics, S. (2009). Perinatal stroke: Effects and outcomes.
119, E562–e573. In K. O. Yeates, M. D. Ris, H. G. Taylor, &
Saigal, S., Szatmari, P., & Rosenbaum, P. (1992). Can B. Pennington (Eds.), Pediatric neuropsychology:
learning disabilities in children who were extremely Research, theory, and practice (2nd Edition, pp.
low birth weight be identified at school entry? 181–210), New York, Guilford.
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Stiles, J., Stern, C., Appelbaum, M., Nass, R., Trauner,
13, 356–62. D., & Hesselink, J. (2008). Effects of early focal
Sarnat, H. B. & Sarnat, M. S. (1976). Neonatal enceph- brain injury on memory for visuospatial patterns:
alopathy following fetal distress. Archives of Selective deficits of global-local processing.
Neurology, 33, 696–705. Neuropsychology, 22, 61–73.
Schneider, W., Wolke, D., Schlagmuller, M., & Meyer, Stiles, J. & Thal, D. (1993). Linguistic and spatial cogni-
R. (2004). Pathways to school achievement in very tive development following early focal brain injury:
preterm and full-term children. European Journal Patterns of deficit and recovery. In M. H. Johnson
of Psychology of Education, 19, 385–406. (Ed.), Brain development and cognition (pp. 643–
Seidel, W. T. (1998). An apparent case of malingering 64). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
in a 9-year-old. Archives of Clinical Neuropsycho- Sweet, J.J. (1999). Forensic neuropsychology: Funda-
logy, 13, 82–83. mentals and practice. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets
Shankaran, S., Woldt, E., Koepke, T., Bedard, M. P., & & Zeitlinger.
Nandyal, R. (1991). Acute neonatal morbidity and Talib, T. L., Pongonis, S. J., Williams, L. S., Garg, B. P.,
long-term central nervous system sequelae of peri- Sokol, D. K., Saha, C., et al. (2008). Neuropsycho-
natal asphyxia in term infants. Early Human logic outcomes in a case series of twins discordant
Development, 25, 135–48. for perinatal stroke. Pediatric Neurology, 38, 118–25.
Shapiro, E. S. (1996). Academic skills problems: Direct Taylor, H. G. (2008). Low birth weight. In J. E. Morgan
assessment and intervention. New York: Guilford. & J. H. Ricker (Eds.), Textbook of clinical neuropsy-
Shaywitz, B. A. & Fletcher, J. M. (1993). Neurological, chology. New York: Taylor & Francis.
cognitive, and behavioral sequlae of hypoxic- Taylor, H. G. (2009). Children with very low birth
ischemic encephalopathy. Seminars in Perinatology, weight or very preterm birth. In K. O. Yeates, M. D.
17, 357–66. Ris, H. G. Taylor, & B. Pennington (Eds.), Pediatric
Sherlock, R. L., Anderson, P. J., & Doyle, L. W. (2005). neuropsychology: Research, theory, and practice
Neurodevelopmental sequelae of intraventricular (2nd Edition, pp. 26–70). New York: Guilford.
Perinatal Brain Injury 209

Taylor, H. G. (in press). Academic performance and van Kooij, B. J. M., Van Handel, M., Uiterwaal, C. S. P.
learning disabilities. In C. Nosarti, R. Murray, & M., Groenendaal, F., Neivelstein, R. A. J.,
M. Hack (Eds.), Preterm birth: Long-term effects on Rademaker, K. J., et al. (2007). Corpus callosum
brain & behavior. Cambridge University Press. size in relation to motor performance in 9- to
Taylor, H. G. & Alden, J. (1997). Age-related differences 10-year-old children with neonatal encephalopa-
in outcomes following childhood brain insults: An thy. Pediatric Research, 63, 103–108.
introduction and overview. Journal of the Inter- Vannucci, R. C. & Perlman, J. M. (1997). Interventions
national Neuropsychological Society, 3, 1–13. for perinatal hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy.
Taylor, H. G., Espy, K. A., & Anderson, P. J. (2009). Pediatrics, 100, 1004–1014.
Mathematics deficiencies in children with very low Vargha-Khadem, F., Salmond, C. H., Watkins, K. E.,
birth weight or very preterm birth. Developmental Friston, K. J., Gadian, D. G., & Mishkin, M. (2003).
Disabilities Research Reviews, 15, 52–59. Developmental amnesia: Effect of age at injury.
Taylor, H. G., Klein, N., Drotar, D., Schluchter, M., & Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
Hack, M. (2006). Consequences and risks for USA, 100, 10055–10060.
<1000-g birth weight for neuropsychological skills, Viggedal, G., Lundalv, E., Carlsson, G., & Kjellmer, I.
achievement, and adaptive functioning. Develop- (2002). Follow-up into young adulthood after
mental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 459–69. cardiopulmonary resuscitation in term and near-
Taylor, H. G., Klein, N., Minich, N., & Hack, M. (2000). term newborn infants. II. Neuropsychological con-
Middle school-age outcomes in children with very sequences. Acta Paediatrica, 91, 1218–26.
low birthweight. Child Development, 71, 1495– Vohr, B., Allan, W. C., Westerveld, M., Schneider, K. C.,
1511. Katz, K. H., Makuch, R. W., et al. (2003). School-
Taylor, H. G., Klein, N., Schatschneider, C., & Hack, M. age outcomes of very low birth weight infants in
(1998). Predictors of early school age outcomes in the Indomethacin Intraventricular Hemorrhage
very low birthweight children. Developmental and Prevention Trial. Pediatrics, 111, e340–46.
Behavioral Pediatrics, 19, 235–43. Volpe, J .J. (2001). Perinatal brain injury: From patho-
Taylor, H. G., Minich, N., Bangert, B., Filipek, P. A., & genesis to neuroprotection. Mental Retardation
Hack, M. (2004a). Long-term neuropsychological and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 7,
outcomes of very low birth weight: Associations 56–64.
with early risks for periventricular brain insults. Volpe, J. J. (2008). Neurology of the newborn (5th ed.),
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier.
Society, 10. Volpe, J. (2009). Brain injury in premature infants:
Taylor, H. G., Minich, N. M., Klein, N., & Hack, M. A complex amalgam of destructive and develop-
(2004b). Longitudinal outcomes of very low birth mental disturbances. Lancet Neurology, 8, 110–24.
weight: Neuropsychological findings. Journal of the Whitaker, A. H., Feldman, J. F., Van Rossem, R.,
International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 149–63. Schonfeld, I. S., Pinto-Martin, J. A., Torre, C.,
Thomson, A. J., Searle, M., & Russell, G. (1977). Quality Blumenthal, S. R., et al. (1996). Neonatal cranial
of survival after severe birth asphyxia. Archives of ultrasound abnormalities in low birth weight
Disease in Childhood, 52, 620–26. infants: Relation to cognitive outcomes at six years
Thompson, S. (1999). The source for nonverbal learning of age. Pediatrics, 98, 719–29.
disabilities. East Moline, IL: LinguiSystems. Whitaker, A. H., Van Rossem, R., Feldman, J. F.,
Trauner, D. A. (2003). Hemispatial neglect in young Schonfeld, I. S., Pinto-Martin, J. A., Torre, C., et al.
children with early unilateral brain damage. (1997). Psychiatric outcomes in low-birth-weight
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, children at age 6 years: Relation to neonatal cranial
160–66. ultrasound abnormalities. Archives of General
Trauner, D. A., Chase, C., Walker, P, & Wulfeck, B. Psychiatry, 54, 847–56.
(1993). Neurologic profiles of infants and Wills, K. E. & Sweet, J. (2006). Neuropsychological
children after perinatal stroke. Pediatric Neurology, considerations in forensic child assessment. In S.
9, 383–86. Sparta (Ed.), Forensic mental health assessment of
Trauner, D. A., Nass, R., & Ballantyne, A. (2001). children and adolescents (pp. 260–84). NY: Oxford.
Behavioural profiles of children and adolescents Wilson-Costello, D. (2007). Is there evidence that long-
after pre- or perinatal unilateral brain damage. term outcomes have improved with intensive care?
Brain, 124, 995–1002. Seminars in Fetal & Neonatal Medicine, 12, 344–
van Baar, A. L., Vermaas, J., Knots, E., de Kleine, M. J. 54.
K., & Soons, P. (2009). Functioning at school age of Wilson-Costello, D., Friedman, H., Minich, N.,
moderately preterm children born at 32 to 36 Fanaroff, A. A., & Hack, M. (2005). Improved
weeks’ gestational age. Pediatrics, 124, 251–57. survival rates with increased neurodevelopmental
210 forensic neuropsychology

disability for extremely low birth weight infants in Wulfeck, B. B., Trauner, D. A., & Tallal, P. A. (1991).
the 1990s. Pediatrics, 115, 997–1003. Neurologic, cognitive, and linguistic features of
Wolf, M., Koldewijn, K., Beelen, A., Smit, B., Hedlund, infants after early stroke. Pediatric Neurology, 7,
R., & de Groot, I. J. M. (2002). Neurobehavioral 266–69.
and developmental profile of very low birth weight Ylvisaker, M., Adelson, D., Braga, L. W., Burnett, S. M.,
preterm infants in early infancy. Acta Paediatrica, Glang, A., Feeney, T., et al. (2005). Rehabilitation
91, 930–38. and ongoing support after pediatric TBI: Twenty
Woodward, L. J., Anderson, P. J., Austin, N. C., Howard, years of progress. Journal of Head Trauma
K., & Inder, T. E. (2006). Neonatal MRI to predict Rehabilitation, 20, 90–104.
neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm infants. Ylvisaker, M., Todis, B., Glang, A., Urbanczyk, B.,
New England Journal of Medicine, 355, 685–94. Franklin, C., DePompei, R., et al. (2001). Educating
Woodward, L. J., Edgin, J. O., Thompson, D., & Inder, students with TBI: Themes and recommendations.
T. E. (2005). Object working memory deficits pre- Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16, 76–93.
dicted by early brain injury and development in Ylvisaker, M., Turkstra, L., Coehlo, C., Yorkston, K.,
the preterm infant. Brain, 128, 2578–87. Kennedy, M., Sohlberg, M. M., et al.
Wu, Y., March, W. M., Croen, L.A., Grether, J. K., (2007). Behavioural interventions for children and
Escobar, G. J., & Newman, T. B. (2004). Perinatal adults with behaviour disorders after TBI: A sys-
stroke in children with motor impairment: A pop- tematic review of the evidence. Brain Injury, 21,
ulation-based study. Pediatrics, 114, 612–619. 769–805.
8
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic
Brain Injury
J AC O B U S D O N D E R S

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is an acquired increasing age, motor vehicle accidents account
condition where the head is subjected to acute for the majority of the more severe injuries
external mechanical forces that cause at least tem- (DiScala, Osberg, Gans, Chin, & Grant, 1991).
porary alteration in the physiological functioning It must be appreciated that many cases of
of the brain. This is a significant public health childhood TBI are preventable. For example,
problem because about 30% of all childhood alcohol abuse and perception of injury risk by
injury deaths result from TBI and there is consid- parents are much more meaningful predictors
erable neurobehavioral morbidity in the survivors of accidental injury in children than a develop-
of pediatric TBI. The purpose of this chapter is to mental personality characteristic of being “acci-
provide an update of the most recent neurobehav- dent-prone” (Rivara, 1995). Furthermore, the
ioral research and evidence-based practice guide- presence of pre-injury risk-taking behaviors
lines regarding TBI in children and adolescents, is not necessarily associated with risk for greater
with a specific focus on legal and forensic aspects. severity of TBI (Olsson, Le Brocque, Kenardy,
Anderson, & Spence, 2008). Unfortunately, avail-
able protective measures such as infant car seats,
EPIDEMIOLOGY
belt restraints, and bicycle helmets are not always
Incidence statistics vary considerably across
used consistently or most effectively (for a review,
studies because of differences in definition and
see Gregg & Appleton, 2006). Improvements are
inclusion criteria. In a comprehensive review,
still needed with regard to the ability of health
Kraus (1995) estimated an average incidence of
care professionals to recognize risks of serious
180 per 100,000 children per year, but a recent
child abuse TBI before it takes place (Hicks &
prospective birth cohort study that also included
Gaughan, 1995; Miller, 1999). There is also evi-
nonhospitalized cases yielded considerably higher
dence that a very brief hospital-based education
estimates, ranging from 1,100 to 1,850 per 100,000
program, aimed at parents and provided at the
across the 0–15-year age span (McKinlay et al.,
time of all infants’ births, can be effective in reduc-
2008). It should be realized that, in the latter
ing the incidence of child abuse-related TBI (Dias,
investigation, about 90% of the injuries were fairly
Smith, DeGuehery, Mazur, & Shaffer, 2005).
mild in nature and that only 30% warranted
hospital admission. In the US, rates of hospitaliza-
tion for TBI range from 80 per 100,000 in toddlers I N J U RY S E V E R I T Y
to 129 per 100,000 in older adolescents, whereas The vast majority of cases of pediatric TBI can
death rates range from 6 to 24 per 100,000, respec- likely be considered “mild” in nature in the sense
tively (Keenan & Bratton, 2006). that they are not associated with prolonged coma
Rates of TBI are about 1.3 to 2.0 times greater or intracranial lesions on neuroimaging. However,
for boys than those for girls (Rivara, 1994), most of the classifications of severity of TBI were
and there are indications for higher death and initially developed for use with adults, so caution
hospitalization rates among African-American is necessary when applying them to children. For
compared to Caucasian children (Langlois, example, estimates of posttraumatic amnesia may
Rutland-Brown, & Thomas, 2005). Falls are a be difficult to obtain with young children who
common cause in young children but with have limited verbal skills, and such estimates are
212 forensic neuropsychology

notoriously unreliable when they are obtained linear displacement that causes focal lesions such
retrospectively. The Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS, as cortical contusions and subdural hemorrhage,
Teasdale & Jennett, 1974) is an ordinal rating scale and intracranial rotation that may result in diffuse
that classifies TBI as mild (scores 13–15), moder- lesions such as axonal shearing.
ate (9–12), or severe (3–8). However, reliability of Focal involvement of the prefrontal areas
this scale may be compromised when children are tends to be associated with an increased risk for
sedated or when they have compromise of other long-term neurobehavioral sequelae of TBI
parts of the body such as intubation or spinal cord (Levin, Zhang et al., 2004; Max et al., 2006)
injury. Furthermore, it is customary to classify an although an increasing number of studies have
injury as “complicated mild” when the GCS com- suggested that it may be more the total number
posite score exceeds 12 but is accompanied by an and volume rather than the location of the cere-
intracranial lesion on neuroimaging (Williams, bral lesions that is predictive of such outcome
Levin, & Eisenberg, 1990). In addition, focal neu- (Power, Catroppa, Coleman, Ditchfield, &
rological signs such as pupil reflex abnormalities Anderson, 2007; Salorio et al., 2005; Slomine
may also signify more serious injury (Capruso & et al., 2002). In addition, diffuse white matter
Levin, 1992; Prasad, Ewing-Cobbs, Swank, & involvement and thinning of the corpus callosum,
Kramer, 2002). For all of these reasons, exclusive as well as reduction of the cortical gray mantle,
reliance on the total score of the GCS is not have been associated with detrimental neurobe-
advised with pediatric TBI. However, the time to havioral effects (Catroppa, Anderson, Ditchfield,
which children follow verbal commands (equiva- & Coleman, 2008; Fearing et al., 2008; Merkley
lent to a score of 6 on the Motor subscale on the et al., 2008; Serra-Grabulosa et al., 2005; Verger
GCS) appears to be a fairly reliable and commonly et al., 2001). Recent research has also highlighted
used indicator of length of coma, with the latter the vulnerability of subcortical structures such as
being negatively correlated with neurobehavioral the hippocampus and cerebellum (Braga, Souza,
outcomes (Massagli, Michaud, & Rivara, 1996). Najjar, & Dellatolas, 2007; Wilde et al., 2007).
Posttraumatic epilepsy, defined as repeated sei- Secondary injuries are those that arise indi-
zures that persist for more than 1 week post injury, rectly from trauma, and may continue to develop
can also occur in about 10–20% of (especially for several days after injury. In children, they are
young) children with severe TBI, and is also asso- most often the result of disrupted cerebral circula-
ciated with worse outcomes (Statler, 2006). tion and an associated cascade of neurochemical
CT scan is still the most appropriate technique events, leading to hypoxic-ischemic injury, diffuse
to evaluate acute lesions on the day of impact but edema, and neuronal excitotoxicity. There is grow-
some chronic neurodegenerative changes may not ing evidence that prevention of such secondary
become fully stable until evaluated with MRI insults, aimed at adequate management of cere-
months or years later (Adelson et al., 2003). There bral homeostasis through limiting intracranial
have been considerable developments in recent hypertension and optimizing cerebral perfusion
years in the area of more advanced MRI tech- pressure, is critical with regard to survival after
niques, including diffusion weighted and diffu- pediatric TBI (Adelson et al., 2003, Kochanek,
sion tensor imaging with associated fractional 2005; Udomphorn, Armstead, & Vavilala, 2008).
anisotropy and tractography, that show consider- There is some controversy about the potential
able promise in the evaluation of children with benefit of other specific acute care interventions,
TBI (Ashwal, Holshouser, & Tong, 2006; Ewing- such as hypothermia (Giza, Mink, & Madikians,
Cobbs et al., 2008; Galloway, Tong, Ashwal, 2007; Hutchison et al., 2008). There are promising
Oyoyo, & Obenaus, 2008; Hanten et al., 2008, developments in the area of utilization of serum
Wozniak et al., 2007). biomarkers in the prediction of outcome of pedi-
atric TBI, but much of this research is in the early
PAT H O P H Y S I O L O G Y stages and will require further replication (Berger,
The mechanisms by which TBI causes brain 2006; Berger, Beers, Richichi, Wiesman, &
impairment are multifactorial, and involve both Adelson, 2007). In addition, there is some recent
primary and secondary injuries (for a review, see: research identifying the presence of the apolipo-
Bauer & Fritz, 2004). Most often, there is an accel- protein E epsilon 4 genotype as being associated
erating or decelerating force to the skull that can with an increased risk for unfavorable outcomes
cause primary disruption of the underlying brain after pediatric TBI (Brichtová & Kozák, 2008), but
matter at the time of trauma. These forces include this has not yet seen extensive replication.
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury 213

COMMON interactive effects of multiple potential predictor


N E U R O B E H AV I O R A L variables. Results from these studies indicated
SEQUELAE that children with severe TBI continue to demon-
There is a growing consensus that the vast strate considerable behavioral adjustment diffi-
majority of children who sustain uncomplicated culties as well as cognitive deficits for several years
mild TBI have an essentially unremarkable long- after injury; the latter most notably in speed of
term recovery, especially when compared to chil- information processing and delayed recall of new
dren with orthopedic injuries, even though they information. Those cognitive skills have been
may be symptomatic within the first few weeks to identified in various independent studies as
months after injury (for reviews, see: Carroll et al., vulnerable to severe pediatric TBI (Calhoun &
2004; Kirkwood et al., 2008; Satz et al., 1997). Dickerson-Mayes, 2005; Hanten et al., 2004;
Persistent symptomatology may be more common Prigatano, Gray, & Gale, 2008; Van Heugten et al.,
in those children with uncomplicated mild TBI 2006). Other comprehensive investigations have
who have premorbid complications such as spe- also identified deficits in complex attention as well
cial education histories or family dysfunction as various aspects of executive functioning, such
(Ponsford et al., 1999). However, these symptoms as working memory, prospective memory, and
do not appear to be specific in any way to the novel problem solving, as fairly common after
experience of a concussion (Nacajauskaite, severe pediatric TBI (Catroppa, Anderson, Morse,
Endziniene, Jureniene, & Schrader, 2006). The Haritou, & Rosenfeld, 2007; Slomine et al., 2002;
inclusion of a control group with orthopedic inju- Mandalis, Kinsella, Ong, & Anderson, 2007;
ries has proven to be of crucial importance in Nadebaum, Anderson, & Catroppa, 2007; Ward,
evaluating the sequelae of mild TBI. Otherwise, Shum, McKinlay, Baker, & Wallace, 2007).
such children may appear to have neurobehav- Severe TBI is a direct risk factor for novel
ioral impairments when compared to healthy psychiatric disorders in children, ranging from
children, whereas they tend to be indistinguish- affective instability to impaired social judgment
able from children with injuries that were (Max, Robertson & Lansing, 2001), although
restricted to other parts of the body (Bijur & worsening of premorbid aggressive behaviors is
Haslum, 1995; Bijur, Haslum, & Golding, 1996). also possible (Cole et al., 2008). Furthermore,
It has been well established that more severe children with severe TBI tend to have significant
forms of pediatric TBI (i.e., when there are objec- difficulties with the pragmatic and interpretive
tive intracranial lesions on neuroimaging and/or aspects of language and social cognition/problem
prolonged coma) are associated with an increased solving (Chapman et al., 2004; Dennis & Barnes,
likelihood of persistent neurobehavioral sequelae 2001; Ganesalingam, Yeates, Sanson, & Anderson,
(for detailed reviews, see Donders, 2007, 2008). It 2007; Yeates et al., 2004, 2007), all of which can
is also abundantly clear that there is no such thing affect behavioral and interpersonal adjustment.
as a unitary profile of cognitive and psychosocial It is important to keep in mind that some of
strengths and weaknesses after pediatric TBI. In the symptoms that are common after severe pedi-
fact, various distinct subtypes of recovery have atric TBI are not specific to that condition. For
been identified in both domains, and have been example, problems with complex attention are
related to injury severity characteristics (Butler, also commonly seen in children with attention
Rourke, Fuerst, & Fisk, 1997; Campbell, Kuehn, deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in the
Richards, Ventureyra, & Hutchison, 2004; absence of any history of TBI (Barkley, 2003,
Donders & Warschausky, 1997; Mottram & 2006). There is at this time no “signature” profile
Donders, 2006). With those reservations in mind, of neuropsychological or other test results that
a few general trends can be identified. can be used unequivocally in isolation to distin-
Taylor and Yeates and their colleagues (Taylor guish TBI from other common pediatric condi-
et al., 2002; Yeates et al., 2002a, 2004) have com- tions.
pleted a series of studies of children with pediatric As much as the research has advanced in
TBI that included baseline measures of premor- recent years in describing the impact of pediatric
bid status as well as a control group with orthope- TBI, there have been relatively fewer studies that
dic injuries. Moreover, the children received have provided evidence-based support for specific
comprehensive, multimodal follow-up for several interventions. In order to prevent the potentially
years and the authors employed multivariate cumulative effect of repetitive mild injuries within
statistical procedures to consider the additive and a short time period, the general consensus about
214 forensic neuropsychology

sports-related concussion in children and adoles- Longitudinal research has also demonstrated
cents is that return to play should be based on that a considerable minority of children with
resolution of symptoms and that “when in doubt, residual neurobehavioral sequelae of TBI who
sit them out” (Kirkwood, Yeates, & Wilson, 2006; could profit from special education support are
Lovell & Fazio, 2008) but there is not yet a firm not receiving adequate services (Taylor et al.,
scientific base for determining the exact duration 2003). Thus, even when neuropsychological
of suspension of sports activities in these cases. assessment is helpful in establishing risks and
There is modest support for the effect of stimulant characterizing the problems that contribute to
medication for the management of attention academic difficulties in children with TBI, this
problems in children with TBI (Bates, 2006; Jin & does not automatically guarantee efficient or ade-
Schachar, 2004), but ADHD symptoms tend to quate service delivery. For this reason, knowledge
get treated more frequently when there is a pre- of federal and state guidelines regarding eligibility
injury history of that diagnosis (Levin et al., 2007). for special education services is important.
Despite the evidence for an increased risk for Under U.S. public law enacted as part of the
sleep problems for several years after severe pedi- Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, TBI
atric TBI, there are essentially no clinical guide- was defined as a separate category of special educa-
lines or efficacy data for treatment of these tion in 1991, which was reauthorized as the
problems (Beebe et al., 2007). The empirical sup- Individuals with Disabilities Education Improve-
port for specific forms of cognitive and behavioral ment Act in 2004. The following definition of TBI
rehabilitation interventions after pediatric TBI is was included in this federal legislation:
mixed, with one review paper endorsing recom-
mendations for family involvement and cognitive Traumatic brain injury means an acquired
rehabilitation of attention and memory (Laatsch injury to the brain caused by an external physi-
et al., 2007) and another one concluding that there cal force, resulting in total or partial functional
was insufficient evidence for specific recommen- disability or psychosocial impairment, or both,
dations for interventions for executive dysfunc- that adversely affects a child’s educational per-
tion (Kennedy et al., 2008). formance. The term applies to open or closed
head injuries resulting in impairments in one
S P E C I A L E D U C AT I O N or more areas, such as cognition; language;
S E RV I C E S memory; attention; reasoning; abstract think-
In light of the significant neurobehavioral seque- ing; judgment; problem solving; sensory, per-
lae that have been described with severe pediatric ceptual, and motor abilities; psychosocial
TBI, it is no surprise that children who incur behavior; physical functions; information pro-
such injuries at an early age are at risk for under- cessing; and speech. The term does not apply to
developed school readiness skills (Taylor et al., brain injuries that are congenital or degenera-
2008). Furthermore, considerable proportions tive or brain injuries induced by birth.
of these children need special education services
for extended periods of time (Clark, Russman, It is important to realize that states differ
& Orme, 1999; Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, widely in their level of embracement or expansion
Iovino, & Miner, 1998; Kinsella et al. 1997). of this federal definition. States cannot use a defi-
Neuropsychological assessment can be helpful in nition of TBI that is more restrictive than the fed-
identifying children who need such services. eral one, but they are at liberty to adopt a more
Miller and Donders (2003) demonstrated that inclusive definition. For example, some states have
scores on the California Verbal Learning Test— added provisions to include children under the
Children’s Version (CVLT–C; Delis, Kramer, definition of TBI who acquired their brain injury
Kaplan, & Ober, 1994), obtained at an average of as the result of vascular (e.g., stroke) or hypoxic
three months after injury, were more accurate in (e.g., near-drowning) conditions (Katsiyannis &
the prediction of special education placement two Conderman, 1994). Therefore, it is crucial that
years later than a host of demographic (age, both families and providers be aware of their local
gender, parental occupational status) and injury state laws in this regard. This kind of information
(length of coma and lesions on neuroimaging) can typically be obtained from the state’s
variables. This reinforces the criterion validity and Department of Education.
incremental value of neuropsychological assess- Regardless of the state or local jurisdiction,
ment in children with TBI. there are federal guidelines and requirements
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury 215

under the Individuals with Disabilities Education It is important to realize that if parents do not agree
Improvement Act with regard to the special educa- with the IEPC findings and do not want to go
tion process (for detailed reviews, see Hibbard, through mediation, they have the right to argue
Martin, Cantor, & Moran, 2006; Madigan, Hall, & their case directly through the courts, and they can
Glang, 1997). These principles govern that any seek a second opinion from an independent
student with TBI should be able to receive a free expert. However, they are required to pay for all
and appropriate public education, based on a non- the associated legal and professional fees privately.
discriminatory evaluation, and that this education Especially with children with TBI, who tend to
take place in the environment that is the least change in their performance and needs over time,
restrictive for the child. Parents should be active IEPC plans may need to be reviewed more fre-
participants in this process and there should be a quently than the yearly (or sometimes once every
set of procedures regarding accountability, gener- three years) customary standard. More detailed
ally known as due process. It is important to under- reviews about special education procedures that
stand that simply providing a diagnosis of TBI have been written in language that is intelligible to
does not automatically qualify the child for special parents include Schoenbrodt (2001) and Semrud-
education services, and that there are often differ- Clikeman (2001).
ences between what may be medically desirable A final important thing to consider for neu-
and what can is truly educationally necessary. ropsychologists in interaction with the education
In order to be considered for any kind of spe- system is that school professionals are typically
cial education placement, the child must be working under rules regarding privacy of infor-
referred to the school system’s special education mation that are different from those that apply in
services committee. For many children with TBI health care. Although neuropsychologists are
who receive hospital-based rehabilitation services, used to medical records that are governed by the
such a referral can often be made (with parental Health Insurance Portability and Accountability
consent) by treating professionals. In other Act, educational records fall instead under the
instances, the parents may need to request an relatively less restrictive Family Educational
evaluation or meeting on their own. Schools Rights and Privacy Act. Ernst, Pelletier, and
cannot ignore such a request and must provide a Simpson (2008) provide a helpful discussion of
meaningful response. administrative, legislative, and practical aspects of
The most appropriate services for a child with providing neuropsychological consultation to
TBI will be determined by an Individualized schools.
Educational Planning Committee (IEPC), which
should include an individual with some special- M O D E R AT I N G A N D
ized training in the area of the suspected disability M E D I AT I N G VA R I A B L E S
(i.e., sequelae of TBI). Parents have the right to be There is no doubt that the likelihood of long-
members of this team and they are also allowed to term neurobehavioral sequelae of pediatric TBI
have assistance in this regard. For example, many increases with greater injury severity. However,
states have “parent advocacy” programs. even among children with severe TBI, outcomes
The IEPC should result in a statement regard- tend to vary considerably. There are several demo-
ing eligibility for services, specific goals, a delinea- graphic variables that may play a moderating or
tion of the accommodations and adaptations for mediating role in this regard (see Holmbeck, 2002
the child, as well as a recommendation for specific for a general review of moderating and mediating
placement. If parents do not agree with the deci- effects). A moderator is a variable that specifies
sion of the IEPC, they have the right to request an the circumstances under which a condition results
arbitration or mediation hearing. State laws differ in a particular outcome. In this case, the nature of
widely on timeframes in this regard. However, the the relationship between severity of TBI and neu-
mediation process cannot be used to deny or robehavioral outcome may vary as a function of
delay a due process hearing, and the results of the one or more characteristics of the child and family,
mediation are not binding (Lorber & Yurk, 1999). such as demographic background or premorbid
A due process hearing is administered by an developmental history. A mediator, on the other
impartial hearing officer of the court, and pediat- hand, is a variable that explains how a predictor
ric neuropsychologists may be called upon as variable results in a particular outcome. In this
expert witnesses in this context. Rulings from due case, the effect of pediatric TBI on a specific neu-
process hearings are binding to all parties involved. robehavioral outcome is essentially transmitted
216 forensic neuropsychology

via another variable, such that TBI is associated Another moderator variable that has been
with the mediator, which in turn affects the studied extensively is pre-injury family back-
outcome measure. ground. Taylor and colleagues (2002) found that
One of the most significant moderating this tends to have a greater effect on behavioral
influences on recovery after TBI is age. There and psychosocial outcomes than on cognitive
are three distinct but inter-related dimensions outcomes, with the effects of severe TBI being
of age that need to be considered in this particularly detrimental in cases where there was
regard (Taylor & Alden, 1997): age at injury, time greater family stress and/or low socioeconomic
since injury, and age at assessment of outcome. status. However, even with cognitive outcomes
Until the early 1990s, there was a common such as calculation skills, children with severe
lore that the earlier in life the TBI occurred, TBI caught up with orthopedic controls over a
the better, because of a presumed great degree 12-month period only when family stressors were
of plasticity in children’s brains. This myth has low. Anderson and colleagues (2006) also sug-
clearly been debunked. There is strong evidence gested the possibility of a “double hazard” in that
that TBI that is acquired early in life interferes children with severe injuries from socially disad-
with skills that are still in a phase of rapid devel- vantaged families are at greatest risk for poor
opment, which puts young children at increased functional outcome at 30 months post injury.
risk for long-term deficits (Anderson, Catroppa, Findings with regard to other potential
Haritou, Morse, & Rosenfeld, 2005; Catroppa, moderators of the impact of pediatric TBI have
Anderson, Morse, Haritou, & Rosenfeld, 2008; been mixed, with some but not widely replicated
Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Landry, Kramer, & DeLeon, evidence for relatively greater risk for complicated
2004). Longitudinal research has suggested that outcome with ethnic minority status (Yeates et al.,
children with severe TBI tend to show partial 2002b). However, these findings need to be inter-
recovery during the first year after injury but that preted in the context of the fact that attrition rates
this recovery reaches a plateau after that time and tend to be higher among ethnic minority groups
that there remain considerable residual neurobe- in longitudinal studies. In addition, findings have
havioral deficits at extended follow-up (Yeates been somewhat inconsistent with regard to
et al., 2002a). It is important to realize that this a possible moderating effect of gender, with some
recovery trend is typically much more apparent studies suggesting worse memory outcomes in
for cognitive than for behavioral adjustment char- boys (Donders & Woodward, 2003) and others
acteristics (Taylor et al., 2002). Other researchers identifying more frequent obsessive-compulsive
have demonstrated that some neurobehavioral symptoms in girls (Grados et al., 2008). Thus,
deficits may not become fully manifest until the the possible moderating impact of factors
child gets older (Anderson, Catroppa, Rosenfeld, other than age and psychosocial adversity remains
Haritou, & Morse, 2000; Fay et al., 1994; Levin, unresolved.
Hanten et al., 2004). This may reflect an interac- There is also increasing research with regard
tion of delayed maturation of specific (especially to factors that mediate the effect of pediatric
prefrontal) brain regions with an increase in the TBI on neurobehavioral outcome variables. In the
complexity of environmental demands on the cognitive domain, there is evidence that the
child. effect of severity of TBI on verbal learning is
Related to the issue of age is the adult outcome mediated by speed of information processing
of children with TBI. Several studies have reported (Donders & Minnema, 2004; Donders & Nesbit-
poor vocational outcomes after severe pediatric Greene, 2004). Specifically, length of coma nega-
TBI (Kieslich, Marquardt, Galow, Lorenz, & tively impacted processing speed, which was in
Jacobi, 2001; Nybo, Sainio, & Müller, 2004). When turn associated with worse performance on the
such injuries are sustained early in childhood, CVLT–C, a task where successive stimuli are pre-
they are associated with worse cognitive and psy- sented to the child at a fairly rapid pace. Another
chosocial outcomes in early adulthood than when example of a mediator is the recent finding that
sustained during adolescence (Donders & deficits in self-regulation mediated the effect of
Warschausky, 2007). However, it needs to be pediatric TBI on some social and behavioral out-
understood that these are group data, and that comes (Ganesalingam, Sanson, Anderson, &
prediction of adult outcome in individual cases is Yeates, 2007). Specifically, the presence of severe
fraught with many uncertainties and a high risk of TBI predicted poorer scores on measures of
misclassification. behavioral inhibition, which were in turn related
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury 217

to worse parental ratings of assertion and worse C O M P L I C AT I N G P R E M O R B I D


teacher ratings of self-control. The exploration of H I S TO R I E S
mediating effects typically requires large samples Most studies of pediatric TBI have excluded
and advanced statistical analyses, but this is an children with premorbid complicating histories in
area of potential growth in the understanding of order to avoid potential confounding of results.
the consequences of pediatric TBI. However, neuropsychologists are often asked
to determine whether persistent postinjury
FA M I LY F U N C T I O N I N G neurobehavioral complaints or symptoms are due
When children sustain severe TBI, family life gets to TBI or to pre-existing conditions such as
interrupted and yet parents often need to be the ADHD, learning disability, or both.
most important long-term advocates for their Several studies have documented that lower
children, particularly within the educational and levels of pre-injury functioning increase the risks
rehabilitation systems. As was mentioned above, for less favorable outcome after severe pediatric
family factors account for variability in children’s TBI, both for the child and the family (Anderson
neurobehavioral outcomes, over and above that et al., 2005; Rivara et al., 1996; Woodward et al.,
explained by injury-related variables, and a more 1999). However, such group studies do not
adaptive pre-injury family environment may act allow easy determination of which children with
as a buffer regarding the impact of TBI (Anderson prior complicating histories do or do not have
et al., 2005; Schwartz et al., 2003). Examples of additional problems after severe TBI. Thus, the
more adaptive family coping include open com- presence of premorbid complicating factors is a
munication, lack of rigidity, equitable and efficient relative but not an absolute risk factor for poor
role distributions, and acceptance. Chronic life outcome.
stressors and lack of interpersonal resources may Another interpretive problem is due to the
exacerbate the degree of TBI-related burden on fact that some of the neurobehavioral symptoms
families (Stancin, Wade, Walz, Yeates, & Taylor, that children may demonstrate after TBI (e.g.,
2008). There are also indications that psychoso- inattention) are not specific to that condition.
cial adversity, such as the presence of family Thus, one cannot assume on the basis of the pres-
stressors, tends to remain more predictive of ence of symptoms that are known to be common
cognitive outcomes than injury severity or lesion after TBI that the associated cause must be TBI.
location (Max et al., 2005). This kind of reverse reasoning would amount to a
Longitudinal research has confirmed that logical fallacy with a high likelihood of misattri-
substantial proportions of families of children bution, particularly when dealing with children
who have experienced a severe TBI continue to with uncomplicated mild TBI, where the prepon-
report high levels of injury-related stress associ- derance of the studies does not suggest persistent
ated with some aspect of the child’s recovery or injury-related sequelae (Satz et al., 1997).
the family’s reactions many years later (Wade Although things may appear to be less compli-
et al., 2002). This may actually worsen over time if cated with moderate to severe TBI, diagnostic
parents do not receive adequate social support inferences are not a panacea with increasing
(Wade et al., 2006). Behavioral changes in the injury severity either. Specifically, it has been sug-
child as the result of TBI may negatively affect gested that moderate to severe TBI with subcorti-
reciprocal interactions between child and parent cal involvement may result in “secondary” ADHD
(Wade et al., 2008). It has also been shown that the (Gerring et al., 1998; Max et al., 2004; Slomine
influences between child behavioral problems and et al., 2005). At the same time, level of pre-existing
family adjustment are bidirectional in nature problems with attention has been shown to
(Taylor et al., 2001). This suggests that attempts at moderate post-injury attention deficits in chil-
intervention should target both children and par- dren with TBI (Yeates et al., 2005). For all of these
ents. A recent randomized clinical trial has sug- reasons, it is imperative that clinicians obtain
gested that online cognitive-behavioral family premorbid academic and health records on
problem solving therapy can improve both paren- children with TBI.
tal level of distress and child behavioral outcomes The fact that both injury severity and premor-
(Wade, Carey, & Wolfe, 2006a, 2006b). Although bid history affect outcome after pediatric TBI may
effect sizes were somewhat small, this is an exam- be illustrated by the findings from a consecutive
ple of empirical support for a specific clinical sample of 100 children with TBI (66% from
intervention. motor vehicle accidents) between the ages of
218 forensic neuropsychology

6 and 16½ years (median = 12½) who were T score. As can be seen in Table 8.1, variables
referred to a regional rehabilitation hospital. There associated with injury severity as well as variables
were no a priori exclusion criteria, other than reflecting premorbid history were statistically
that the child should have English as the primary significant predictors in this model. There were
language and not have any kind of hearing no problems with collinearity (e.g., all variance
problem. All children were evaluated with the inflation factors <1.72). The composite model
CVLT–C within 1 year after injury (median = 3 was statistically significant, F (10, 87) = 4.66,
months) as part of a more comprehensive neuro- p < 0.0001, R2 = 0.35, with longer length of
psychological evaluation. Data from two children coma explaining approximately 24% of unique
were excluded because they failed validity criteria variance and presence of a special education
on the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM; history about 6%.
Tombaugh, 1996–see below). Characteristics of The independent contributions of injury
the remaining sample were as follows: 79% was severity and premorbid history can also be dem-
Caucasian, 60% was male, 53% had an acute onstrated by considering the respective CVLT–C
intracranial lesion on neuroimaging, 34% had T scores of the groups with (M = 41. 26, SD =
a length of coma ≥24 hrs (range = 0–47), and 11.25) and without (M = 49.33, SD = 10.88) prior
median parental education was 12 years (range = special education placement. First of all, children
8–20). Children with prior complicating histories in the former group were no more likely than chil-
were not excluded; most notably, 23% had received dren in the latter group to have coma ≥24 hrs, χ2
prior treatment for ADHD, 19% had received spe- (N = 98) = 0.05, p > 0.80. Second, in both groups,
cial education services (most often because of performance on the CVLT–C correlated strongly,
learning disability), and 19% had received some negatively with length of coma, ranging from
kind of psychopharmacological and/or psycho- –0.58 (p < 0.009) in the group with prior special
therapeutic treatment (note: categories were not education history to –0.46 (p < 0.0001) in the
mutually exclusive, as some children had multiple group without it. These findings confirm that
prior risk factors). This sample is completely inde- prior characteristics of the child as well as severity
pendent of those reported previously in Donders of injury are, largely independent of each other,
(2005) or Donders & Nesbit-Greene (2004). predictive of cognitive outcome after pediatric
The mean composite CVLT–C T score of the TBI. This is consistent with the results from other
final sample was 47.65 (SD = 11.36). Table 8.1 recent investigations pertaining to academic
presents the result from a linear multiple regres- (Catroppa & Anderson, 2007) as well as adaptive
sion analysis, considering the impact of various behavioral outcomes (Catroppa et al., 2008).
demographic and injury-related variables on this
CHILD ABUSE TBI
Child abuse TBI, also known as inflicted or
nonaccidental TBI, affects primarily children
TABLE 8.1 PREDICTORS OF CVLTC
under three years of age (Brown & Minns, 1993).
PERFORMANCE IN CHILDREN WITH In fact, 25% of all TBI and up to 90% of severe
TBI N = 98 cases of TBI in this age group are the result of
Variable SRC t p abuse (Carty & Pierce, 2002; King, MacKay, &
Sirnick, 2003). It has been suggested that the less
Prior psychiatric history –0.14 –1.24 .22 favorable neurobehavioral outcome after infant or
Prior special education 0.25 2.65 .01 preschool TBI compared to other age groups in
Prior treatment of ADHD 0.19 1.66 .10 epidemiological studies may be due at least in part
Age at assessment –0.04 –0.38 .71 to the high rate of inflicted injury in the youngest
Ethnicity 0.08 0.87 .39 children (Ewing-Cobbs, Duhaime, & Fletcher,
Gender –0.01 –0.04 .97 1995). Of these, the shaken-baby condition, in
Parental education –0.05 –0.60 .55 which the child is subjected to rapid angular
Diffuse neuroimaging lesion 0.11 1.00 .32 acceleration-deceleration forces that are often
Focal neuroimaging lesion 0.10 1.03 .31 compounded by actual impact, has the greatest
Length of coma –0.42 –3.73 .004 mortality and morbidity (Duhaime, Christian,
Moss, & Seidl, 1996). However, the extent and
Note. CVLT–C = California Verbal Learning Test—Children’s
Version. TBI = traumatic brain injury. SRC = standardized regres- implications of child abuse TBI are uncertain, for
sion coefficient. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. various reasons.
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury 219

First of all, it is difficult to get exact estimates include subdural hemorrhages, seizures, and reti-
of the prevalence of the problem. Some available nal hemorrhages (Ewing-Cobbs, Kramer, et al.,
statistics suggest that child abuse occurs in at least 1998; Ewing-Cobbs et al., 2000). In a separate
13–15 per 1,000 children, but this may be a recent investigation, children with inflicted TBI
gross underestimation due to inconsistencies in were almost 7 times more likely than children with
definition and underreporting by perpetrators accidental TBI to have subdural hemorrhage in
(Ammerman & Galvin, 1998; Knutson & DeVet, the absence of a skull fracture, and more than 9
1995). A second problem is that family instability times more likely to experience posttraumatic sei-
and confidentiality restrictions make it difficult to zures (Myhre, Grogaard, Dyb, Sandvik, & Nordhov,
follow many abused children longitudinally 2007). All these factors are associated with greater
(Duhaime et al., 1996). Furthermore, nonabusive overall injury severity, resulting poorer outcome
experiences such as parental neglect, poverty, in motor, cognitive, and behavioral domains
and a highly stressed family environment are (Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Kramer, & Landry, 1999).
common comorbid factors that can confound mea- Several other studies have reported that, after con-
surement of the incremental significance of the trolling for demographic variables and severity of
physical trauma (Brown, 2002). Child abuse TBI injury, children with accidental TBI had better
often involves repetitive trauma, for which care is neurobehavioral outcomes than children who sus-
often not sought for hours or days after injury, tained their TBI as the result of child abuse (Beers,
leading to worse outcomes (Keenan, Runyan, & Berger, & Adelson, 2007; Keenan et al., 2006).
Nocera, 2006; Makaroff & Putnam, 2003). Specific deficits in working memory, inhibition,
Despite all of these difficulties, there have been and other forms of executive functions have also
important new developments in the field of child been reported after inflicted TBI in children, com-
abuse brain injury over the past several years. pared to demographically matched controls
Several studies have identified hypoxic-ischemic (Stipanicic, Nolin, Fortin, & Gobeil, 2008).
injury as one of the most damaging factors affect- The role for the neuropsychologist in the
ing mortality and neurobehavioral morbidity after evaluation of child abuse TBI may be twofold.
inflicted childhood TBI, with a possible secondary First, whenever child abuse is suspected, medical
augmenting effect of neurotoxicity due to a sharp and health care professionals in virtually all states
increase in excitatory amino acids (Geddes et al., are subject to mandatory reporting laws
2001; Ruppel et al., 2001; Stoodley, 2002). Recent (Kalichman, 1999). Second, a long-term develop-
research suggests that specific serum biomarker mental perspective is needed regarding these
concentrations show a distinctly different tempo- children (Finkelhor & Dziuba-Leatherman, 1994;
ral development after child abuse as compared to Miller, 1999). There is currently no evidence for a
accidental TBI, contributing to more prolonged unique neuropsychological profile that is selec-
neuronal, astroglial, and axonal compromise tively associated with child abuse TBI. However,
(Berger, Adelson, Richichi, & Kochanek, 2006). since these children typically get injured at an
The specific anatomical and radiological find- early age (when many skills are still emerging or
ings associated with inflicted TBI in childhood in a stage of rapid development) and are also more
have also become clearer. It is important to realize likely to grow up in a stressful or unstable family
that children who have been physically and/or environment, they are at increased risk for long-
sexually abused have been reported to have term neurobehavioral deficits. Consequently,
smaller cerebellar volume, reduced mid-sagittal many of these children may need to be followed
callosal size, and increased lateral ventricles, com- for a considerable number of years post injury.
pared to demographically matched controls, even
in the absence of known trauma to the head (De NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL
Bellis et al., 1999; De Bellis & Kuchibhatia, 2006). E VA L UAT I O N
This may be due to stress-induced hormonal There is no single battery or assessment style
influences that adversely affect brain structure that is uniquely appropriate for the evaluation of
and function (Bremmer, 2002). However, the children with TBI but a few general guidelines
work by Ewing-Cobbs and her colleagues has can be provided. First of all, a careful review of
clarified neuroimaging and other physical find- medical and school records is important. For
ings that appear to be unique to inflicted brain example, parental retrospective estimates of their
injury. Most specifically, in addition to nonspe- child’s duration of coma or premorbid academic
cific brain atrophy, the hallmarks of inflicted TBI achievement are not always accurate, and datings
220 forensic neuropsychology

of onset of psychopathological symptoms are interpretation has been specifically validated in


particularly unreliable (Angold, Erkanli, Costello, children with TBI (Mottram & Donders, 2005).
& Rutter, 1996). Several studies have indicated Clinicians should also be cognizant of the psy-
that the post-injury neuropsychological test chometric strengths and weaknesses of the instru-
scores of children with learning disabilities or ments that they use. It has been well established
psychiatric disorders and who subsequently that age is of considerable importance when con-
sustained a moderate to severe TBI were virtually sidering neuropsychological test scores (Forster &
undistinguishable from those of demographically Leckliter, 1994). Yet, norms for a variety of com-
matched children who sustained TBI of similar monly used instruments are often based on sam-
severity but who did not have complicating ples that are either too small or insufficiently
premorbid histories (Donders & Strom, 1997, stratified to adequately capture these age effects
2000). It was only through formal comparison (Baron, 2004; Kizilbash, Warschausky, & Donders,
with premorbid test results that a further deterio- 2001). In addition, clinicians should be knowledge-
ration in functioning as the result of TBI could be able about the clinical validation of psychometric
demonstrated in the children with pre-existing tests for the specific populations with which they
conditions. intend to use them (American Educational
A careful interview and history are also essen- Research Association, American Psychological
tial. This should address in particular any factors Association, & National Council on Measurement
that might augment or interact with the direct in Education, 1999). For example, the fact that a
organic influence of TBI, such as pre-existing certain test has a specific factor structure in the
problems or the post-injury family environment. standardization sample is no guarantee that it is
In addition, other stressors should be reviewed able to differentiate the same constructs in children
that may have occurred at the time of injury (e.g., with TBI (Woodward & Donders, 1998). Even
witnessing the accidental fatal injuries to a family when two tests are normed on the same, compre-
member) or that are unrelated but still potentially hensive sample it does not necessarily mean that
relevant for the outcome of the child (e.g., range they are equally sensitive to injury severity (Donders
of resources of the local school district). For & Giroux, 2005; Donders & Nesbit-Greene, 2004).
example, subjective anxiety may contribute to the Awareness of the differences in versions of
maintenance of nonspecific somatic, cognitive, specific tests is also important. For example, sev-
and behavioral symptoms after mild TBI eral studies have reported sensitivity of the Tower
(Mittenberg, Wittner, & Miller, 1997). of London, a measure of executive functioning, to
As to specific tests, it is important to include in severity and lesion characteristics in children with
the evaluation measures that tap into the abilities TBI (Levin et al., 1997; Shum et al., 2000).
most commonly affected by severe TBI. For exam- However, very different versions of the task and
ple, an evaluation that includes no or few tasks different scoring methods were used across inves-
with a speeded component may miss the difficul- tigations, which is potentially problematic (Baker,
ties with processing speed that many of these Segalowitz, & Ferlisi, 2001; Berg & Byrd, 2002).
children have, and the associated potentially There are currently available at least two very dif-
helpful adaptations and accommodations that ferent versions of this task that have been stan-
could be considered in the classroom (Ylvisaker dardized and normed for use with children as
et al., 2001). In addition, it is crucial to avoid over- young as 7 years (Culbertson & Zillmer, 2001)
reliance on measures of academic achievement and 8 years (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001) of
and psychometric intelligence because those tests age. Research is still needed to determine the
rely heavily on overlearned skills that are often degree to which they are interchangeable, or
relatively preserved after TBI. Rather, the evalua- which version has better criterion validity with
tion should incorporate reliable measures that tap regard to the sequelae of pediatric TBI.
into new learning and that have established valid- Neuropsychologists also need to be cognizant
ity. A good example is the CVLT–C, a task that is of what happens to widely used tests when these
not only sensitive to severity of TBI (Levin et al., get revised, and how that impacts the sensitivity
2000; Roman et al., 1998) but also has consider- of those tests to pediatric TBI. For example, the
able predictive accuracy with regard to long-term recent revision of a commonly used test of psy-
educational placement (Miller & Donders, chometric intelligence, the Wechsler Intelligence
2003). In addition, this is one of the few tests Scale for Children—Fourth Edition (Wechsler,
for which the multifactorial construct basis for 2003), appears to have resulted in an instrument
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury 221

that is less sensitive to severity of TBI than its the performance of adults with TBI, and formal
predecessor; likely because of the reduced assessment of the validity of neuropsychological
emphasis on speed of performance (Donders & symptoms and test results has pretty much
Janke, 2008). become the standard of care in that field (for
Particularly with regard to higher-level execu- reviews, see Boone, 2007; Larrabee, 2007). Until
tive abilities, it is also important to avoid exclusive recently, relatively little attention was paid to this
reliance on psychometric instruments that have issue in the evaluation of children with TBI.
been administered in the type of structured, dis- However, even young children have the capability
tractor-free environment that is typical of most to use deception (Polak & Harris, 1999) and they
neuropsychological evaluations. In fact, the eco- are not always willing to do their best on neuropsy-
logical validity of that practice has been chal- chological tests (Mäntynen, Poikkeus, Ahonen,
lenged, specifically in reference to the evaluation Aro, & Korkman, 2001).
of children with TBI (Silver, 2000). In this context, There are several recent studies that have
a potentially useful addition to the assessment explored the feasibility of using forced-choice
arsenal for the pediatric neuropsychologist may measures of effort and motivation in the clinical
be the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive assessment of children. Constantinou and
Function (BRIEF; Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & McCaffrey (2003) were the first to evaluate this
Kenworthy, 2000). This rating scale, which can be formally by examining the performance of 5- to
completed by parents or teachers and for which a 12-year old children on the Test of Memory
self-report form is also available for older chil- Malingering (TOMM), a task that had originally
dren, appears to offer incremental information been developed for use with adults (Tombaugh,
about the daily functioning of children with TBI 1996; Rees, Tombaugh, Gansler, & Moczynski,
that cannot be accounted for by laboratory tests 1998). The results indicated that almost all
alone (Mangeot, Armstrong, Colvin, Yeates, & children were more than 90% accurate on the
Taylor, 2002; Nadebaum et al., 2007; Vriezen & second trial of this instrument; clearly “passing”
Pigott, 2002). In addition, it includes formal valid- the cut-off point for invalid performance. Similar
ity checks for possible inconsistent or overly nega- findings were obtained in a mixed pediatric
tive responding. Recent research has also suggested sample (Donders, 2005) as well as in various
that this instrument can be used effectively for studies with healthy children (Blaskewitz, Merten,
prospective and longitudinal tracking of executive & Kathmann, 2008; Nagle, Everhart, Durham,
functioning over the first year after pediatric TBI McCammon, & Walker, 2006). Others have cau-
(Sesma, Slomine, & McCarthy, 2008). tioned, however, that some measures of effort that
Finally, it is important to include formal mea- were designed for adults need to be interpreted
sures of emotional and psychosocial adjustment with caution when used with children under the
in the evaluation, instead of focusing on cognitive age of 11 years (Courtney, Dinkins, Allen, &
variables only. In light of the research on behav- Kuroski, 2003) or with children who do not have
ioral and interpersonal sequelae of pediatric TBI at least a third-grade reading level (Green & Flaro,
(Bloom et al., 2001; Max et al., 2006; Taylor et al., 2003).
2002), it is essential to address these areas of func- To illustrate the fact that the TOMM really
tioning. Several rating scales offer global screen- measures effort and is not affected by the acquired
ing of adjustment from the perspective of the cognitive impairment that is so common after
child, the parent, or the teacher (Achenbach & pediatric TBI, it may be helpful to revisit the data
Rescorla, 2001; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). from the above-described consecutive sample of
More in-depth measures of coping style and per- 100 6–16½ year-old children who were all evalu-
sonality that offer both parent and child versions ated with the TOMM as well as the CVLT–C
are available in the Personality Inventory for within 1 year after injury. Only two of these chil-
Children—Second Edition (Lachar & Gruber, dren failed conventional “adult” validity criteria
1999) and the Personality Inventory for Youth on the TOMM (i.e., 98% pass rate). In fact, slightly
(Lachar & Gruber, 1995). more than half (n = 57) of the children had a score
of 100% correct on the first trial of the TOMM,
S Y M P T O M VA L I D I T Y and the vast majority of them (n = 85) obtained a
TESTING perfect score on the second trial.
There has been an explosion in recent years of One of the two children who failed validity
investigations of effort and motivation affecting criteria on the TOMM was a six-year-old girl
222 forensic neuropsychology

with an unremarkable premorbid history who eligibility for special education services to evalua-
had sustained a right frontal skull fracture tion of possible sequelae of child abuse. Clinical
with underlying contusion, with <30 min loss of practitioners need to have a working knowledge
consciousness, when she was struck as a pedes- of federal and state guidelines and regulations
trian by a motor vehicle. The other was a 13-year- about special education criteria as well as manda-
old boy who had sustained a mild concussion, tory reporting guidelines. It is clear that prior
with negative neuroimaging findings, during a child history, family characteristics, and psycho-
sports activity, but his case was complicated by a social adversity factors need to be considered
premorbid history of pharmacological treatment in the evaluation of the sequelae of pediatric TBI.
for depression. Both children obtained scores on In addition, potential moderating (e.g., age) and
the second trial of the TOMM that were not mediating (e.g., processing speed) variables must
statistically significantly different from chance. be considered when evaluating outcomes.
Importantly, both children readily admitted to Pediatric neuropsychologists must have a basic
having provided poor effort when they were appreciation of the pathophysiology of pediatric
gently confronted about their behavior, and both TBI, especially the various risk factors contribut-
were able to modify this behavior. For example, ing to hypoxic-ischemic injury, as well as a rea-
they obtained scores that were well within normal sonable degree of familiarity with recent advances
limits on several other tests that were adminis- in neuroimaging, such as diffusion tensor MRI.
tered after this conversation. In addition, they need to have a more advanced
To formally evaluate the respective relation- understanding of the construct, criterion, and
ships between injury severity and performances ecological validities of the psychometric tests that
on the psychometric tests, product-moment cor- they employ with these children. Development of
relations were computed in the complete sample solid and properly stratified norms for more
between length of coma, and respective perfor- pediatric neuropsychological tests is still needed.
mances on the TOMM and CVLT–C. These data Incorporation of formal measures of symptom
are presented in Table 8.2. Inspection of this table validity in the assessment of children with TBI in
suggests that neither of the TOMM variables a forensic context is desirable.
covaried meaningfully with injury severity, Mild, uncomplicated pediatric TBI is typically
whereas performance on the CVLT–C was not associated with significant permanent neu-
strongly inversely related to duration of coma. robehavioral sequelae. At the same time, there is
Thus, it is clear that poor performance on a symp- no doubt that a long-term developmental per-
tom validity test like the TOMM cannot be spective is necessary with regard to the evaluation
explained away by acquired cerebral or cognitive of moderate to severe TBI in children. Particularly
dysfunction in children with TBI. with regard to TBI that is sustained early in life,
additional prospective studies are needed to delin-
CONCLUSION eate recovery into adolescence and adulthood, as
Children with TBI can present in a variety of well as the various influences on variability in
forensic contexts, ranging from consideration of outcome. Future research should explore novel
opportunities for primary, secondary, and tertiary
prevention. Finally, additional randomized con-
trolled clinical trials are needed to provide an evi-
TABLE 8.2 CORRELATIONS BET WEEN dence base for the best practices in the care of
INJURY AND PSYCHOMETRIC children with TBI, all the way from acute care
VARIABLES IN CHILDREN WITH TBI medical management through rehabilitation to
N = 100 long-term educational/vocational training and
independent community integration.
1. 2. 3. 4.
1. TOMM Trial 1 — 0.82* 0.18 0.01 REFERENCES
2. TOMM Trial 2 — 0.04 0.02 Achenbach, T. M. & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for
3. CVLT–C Total — –0.49* ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington,
4. Length of coma — VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for
Children, Youth, & Families.
Note. TOMM = Test of Memory Malingering. CVLT–C =
California Verbal Learning Test—Children’s Version. Adelson, P. D., Bratton, S. L., Carney, N. A., Chesnut,
* p < 0.0001 R. M., Du Coudray, H. E. M., Goldstein, B., et al.
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury 223

(2003). Guidelines for the acute medical manage- Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 48,
ment of severe traumatic brain injury in infants, 697–701.
children, and adolescents. Critical Care Medicine, Bauer, R. & Fritz, H. (2004). Pathophysiology of trau-
31, 419–91. matic injury in the developing brain: An introduc-
American Educational Research Association, American tion and short update. Experimental Toxicology
Psychological Association & National Council on and Pathology, 56, 65–73.
Measurement in Education American Educational Beebe, D. W., Krivitzky, L., Wells, C. T., Wade, S. L.,
Research Association, American Psychological Taylor, H. G., & Yeates, K. O. (2007). Parental
Association, & National Council on Measurement report of sleep behaviors following moderate or
in Education (1999). Standards for educational and severe pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of
psychological testing. Washington, DC: Author. Pediatric Psychology, 32, 845–50.
Ammerman, R. T. & Galvin, M.R. (1998). Child mal- Beers, S., Berger, R., & Adelson, P. (2007). Neurocogni-
treatment. In R. T. Ammerman & J. V. Campo tive outcome and serum markers in inflicted versus
(Eds.), Handbook of pediatric psychology and non-inflicted traumatic brain injury in young chil-
psychiatry, vol. II: Disease, injury, and illness dren. Journal of Neurotrauma, 24, 97–105.
(pp. 31–69). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Berg, W. K. & Byrd, D. L. (2002). The Tower of London
Anderson, V. A., Catroppa, C., Dudgeon, P., Morse, S. spatial problem-solving task: Enhancing clinical
A., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J. V. (2006). Under- and research implementation. Journal of Clinical
standing predictors of functional recovery and and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 586–604.
outcome 30 months following early childhood Berger, R. P. (2006). The use of serum biomarkers to
head injury. Neuropsychology, 20, 42–57. predict outcome after traumatic brain injury in
Anderson, V. A., Catroppa, C., Haritou, F., Morse, S., & adults and children. Journal of Head Trauma
Rosenfeld, J. V. (2005). Identifying factors contri- Rehabilitation, 21, 315–33.
buting to child and family outcome 30 months after Berger, R. P., Adelson, P. D., Richichi, R., & Kochanek,
traumatic brain injury in children. Journal of Neuro- K. M. (2006). Serum biomarkers after traumatic and
logy, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 76, 401–408. hypoxemic brain injuries: Insight into the biochem-
Anderson, V. A., Catroppa, C., Rosenfeld, J., Haritou, ical response of the pediatric brain to inflicted brain
F., & Morse, S. A. (2000). Recovery of memory injury. Developmental Neuroscience, 28, 327–35.
function following traumatic brain injury in pre- Berger, R. P., Beers, S. R., Richichi, R., Wiesman, D., &
school children. Brain Injury, 14, 679–92. Adelson, P. D. (2007). Serum biomarker concentra-
Angold, A., Erkanli, A., Costello, E. J., & Rutter, M. tions and outcome after pediatric traumatic brain
(1996). Precision, reliability and accuracy in the injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 24, 1793–1801.
dating of symptom onsets in child and adolescent Bijur, P. E. & Haslum, M. (1995). Cognitive, behavioral,
psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and and motoric sequelae of mild head injury in a
Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 657–64. national birth cohort. In S. H. Brown & M. E.
Ashwal, S., Holshouser, B. A., & Tong, K. A. (2006). Michel (Eds.), Traumatic head injury in children
Use of advanced neuroimaging techniques in the (pp. 147–64). New York, NY: Oxford.
evaluation of pediatric traumatic brain injury. Bijur, P. E., Haslum, M., & Golding, J. (1996). Cognitive
Developmental Neuroscience, 28, 309–26. outcomes of multiple head injuries in children.
Baker, K., Segalowitz, S. J., & Ferlisi, M. C. (2001). The Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
effect of differing scoring methods for the Tower of 17, 143–48.
London task on developmental patterns of perfor- Blaskewitz, N., Merten, T., & Kathmann, N. (2008).
mance. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 309–313. Performance of children on symptom validity tests:
Barkley, R. A. (2003). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity TOMM, MSVT, and FIT. Archives of Clinical
disorder. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Child Neuropsychology, 23, 379–91.
psychopathology, 2nd ed. (pp. 75–143). New York: Bloom, D. R., Levin, H. S., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Saunders,
Guilford. A. E., Song, J., & Kowatch, R. A. (2001). Lifetime
Barkley, R. A. (2006). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity and novel psychiatric disorders after pediatric trau-
disorder. In D. A. Wolfe & E. J. Mash (Eds.), matic brain injury. Journal of the American Academy
Behavioral and emotional disorders in adolescents: of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 572–279.
Nature, assessment, and treatment (pp. 91–152). Boone, K. B. (Ed., 2007). Assessment of feigned cogni-
New York: Guilford. tive impairment: A neuropsychological perspective.
Baron, I. S. (2004). Neuropsychological evaluation of the New York: Guilford.
child. New York: Oxford. Braga, L. W., Souza, L. N., Najjar, Y. J., & Dellatolas, G.
Bates, G. (2006). Medication in the treatment of the (2007). Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) find-
behavioral sequelae of traumatic brain injury. ings and neuropsychological sequelae in children
224 forensic neuropsychology

after severe traumatic brain injury: The role of predictors of functional recovery 5 years following
cerebellar lesion. Journal of Child Neurology, 22, pediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI). Journal of
1084–89. Pediatric Psychology, 33, 707–718.
Bremmer, J. D. (2002). Neuroimaging of childhood Chapman, S. B., Sparks, G., Levin, H. S., Dennis, M.,
trauma. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 7, Roncadin, C., Zhang, L., et al. (2004). Discourse
104–112. macrolevel processing after severe pediatric trau-
Brichtová, E., & Kozák, L. (2008). Apolipoprotein matic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsycho-
E genotype and traumatic brain injury in children: logy, 25, 37–60.
Association with neurological outcome. Child’s Cole, W. R., Gerring, J. P., Gray, R. M., Vasa, R. M.,
Nervous System, 24, 349–56. Salorio, C. F., Grados, M., et al. (2008). Prevalence
Brown, G. W. (2002). Measurement and the epidemiol- of aggressive behavior after severe pediatric trau-
ogy of childhood trauma. Seminars in Clinical matic brain injury. Brain Injury, 22, 932–39.
Neuropsychiatry, 7, 66–79. Clark, E., Russman, S., & Orme, S. (1999). Traumatic
Brown, J. K. & Minns, R. A. (1993). Non-accidental head brain injury: Effects on school functioning and
injury, with particular reference to whiplash shaking intervention strategies. School Psychology Review,
injury and medicolegal aspects. Developmental 28, 242–50.
Medicine and Child Neurology, 35, 849–69. Constantinou, M. & McCaffrey, R. J. (2003). Using the
Butler, K., Rourke, B. P., Fuerst, D. R., & Fisk, J. L. TOMM for evaluating children’s effort to perform
(1997). A typology of psychosocial functioning in optimally on neuropsychological measures. Child
pediatric closed-head injury. Child Neuropsycho- Neuropsychology, 9, 81–90.
logy, 3, 98–133. Courtney, J. C., Dinkins, J. P., Allen, L. M., & Kuroski,
Calhoun, S. L. & Dickerson-Mayes, S. (2005). Proce- K. (2003). Age-related effects in children taking the
ssing speed in children with clinical disorders. Computerized Assessment of Response Bias and
Psychology in the Schools, 42, 333–43. Word Memory Test. Child Neuropsychology, 9,
Campbell, C. G. N., Kuehn, S. M., Richards, P. M. P., 109–116.
Ventureyra, E., & Hutchison, J. S. (2004). Medical Culbertson, W. C. & Zillmer, E. A. (2001). Tower of
and cognitive outcome in children with traumatic London—Drexel University (TOLDX). Toronto:
brain injury. Canadian Journal of Neurological Multi-Health Systems.
Sciences, 31, 213–219. De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Clark, D. B., Casey, B.
Capruso, D. X.,& Levin, H. S. (1992). Cognitive impair- J., Giedd, J. N., Boring, A. M., et al. (1999).
ment following closed head injury. Neurologic Developmental traumatology part II: Brain devel-
Clinics, 10, 879–93. opment. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1271–84.
Carroll, L. J., Cassidy, J. D., Peloso, P. M., Borg, J., Von De Bellis, M. D. & Kuchibhatia, M. (2006). Cerebellar
Holst, H., Holm, L., et al. (2004). Prognosis for volumes in pediatric maltreatment-related post-
mild traumatic brain injury: Results of the WHO traumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 60,
collaborative centre task force on mild traumatic 697–703.
brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-
43, 84–105. Kaplan Executive Function System. Austin: The
Carty, H. & Pierce, A. (2002). Non-accidental injury: Psychological Corporation.
A retrospective analysis of a large cohort. European Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A.
Radiology, 12, 2919–25. (1994). California Verbal Learning Test—Children’s
Catroppa, C. & Anderson, V. (2007). Recovery in Version. Austin: The Psychological Corporation.
memory function and its relationship to academic Dennis, M. & Barnes, M. A. (2001). Comparison of lit-
success at 24 months following pediatric TBI. Child eral, inferential, and intentional text comprehension
Neuropsychology, 13, 240–61. in children with mild or severe closed head injury.
Catroppa, C., Anderson, V., Ditchfield, M., & Coleman, Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16, 456–68.
L. (2008). Using magnetic resonance imaging to Dias, M. S., Smith, K., DeGuehery, K., Mazur, P., Li, V.,
predict new learning outcome at 5 years after & Shaffer, M. L. (2005). Preventing abusive head
childhood traumatic brain injury. Journal of Child trauma among infants and young children: A hos-
Neurology, 23, 486–96. pital-based, parent education program. Pediatrics,
Catroppa, C., Anderson, V. A., Morse, S. A., Haritou, 115, 470–77.
F., & Rosenfeld, J. V. (2007). Children’s attentional DiScala, J. A., Osberg, J. S., Gans, B., Chin, L. J., &
skills 5 years post-TBI. Journal of Pediatric Grant, C. C. (1991). Children with traumatic head
Psychology, 32, 354–69. injury: Morbidity and postacute management.
Catroppa, C., Anderson, V. A., Morse, S. A., Haritou, Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
F., & Rosenfeld, J. V. (2008). Outcome and 72, 662–66.
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury 225

Donders, J. (2005). Performance on the Test of Memory Ewing-Cobbs, L., Duhaime, A. C., & Fletcher, J. M.
Malingering in a mixed pediatric sample. Child (1995). Inflicted and noninflicted traumatic brain
Neuropsychology, 11, 221–27. injury in infants and preschoolers. Journal of Head
Donders, J. (2007). Traumatic brain injury. In S. J. Trauma Rehabilitation, 10, 13–24.
Hunter & J. Donders (Eds.), Pediatric neuropsy- Ewing-Cobbs, L., Fletcher, J. M., Levin, H. S., Iovino, I.,
chological intervention (pp. 91–111). New York: & Milner, M. E. (1998). Academic achievement
Cambridge. and academic placement following traumatic brain
Donders, J. (2008). Traumatic brain injury of child- injury in children and adolescents: A two-year lon-
hood. In J. E. Morgan & J. H. Ricker (Eds.), Textbook gitudinal study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 158–70). New York: Neuropsychology, 20, 769–81.
Taylor and Francis. Ewing-Cobbs, L., Kramer, L., Prasad, K., Canales, D.
Donders, J. & Giroux, A. (2005). Discrepancies between N., Louis, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., et al. (1998).
the California Verbal Learning Test–Children’s Neuroimaging, physical, and developmental
Version and the Children’s Category Test after pedi- findings after inflicted and noninflicted traumatic
atric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the Inter- brain injury in young children. Pediatrics, 102,
national Neuropsychological Society, 11, 386–91. 300–307.
Donders, J. & Janke, K. (2008). Criterion validity of the Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M., Kramer, L., & Landry, S.
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth (1999). Inflicted traumatic brain injury:
Edition after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Relationship of developmental outcome to injury
Journal of the International Neuropsychological severity. Pediatric Neurosurgery, 31, 251–58.
Society, 14, 651–55. Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M., Kramer, L., Louis, P. T.,
Donders, J. & Minnema, M.T. (2004). Performance Baumgartner, J., Fletcher, J. M., et al. (2000). Acute
discrepancies on the California Verbal Learning neuroradiologic findings in young children with
Test–Children’s Version (CVLT-C) in children with inflicted or noninflicted traumatic brain injury.
traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Child’s Nervous System, 16, 25–33.
Neuropsychological Society, 10, 482–88. Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M. R., Landry, S. H., Kramer,
Donders, J. & Nesbit-Greene, K. (2004). Predictors L., & DeLeon, R. (2004). Executive functions fol-
of neuropsychological test performance after lowing traumatic brain injury: A preliminary anal-
pediatric traumatic brain injury. Assessment, 11, ysis. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 487–512.
275–84. Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M. R., Swank, P., Kramer, L.,
Donders, J. & Strom, D. (1997). The effect of traumatic Cox, C. S., Fletcher, J. M., et al. (2008). Arrested
brain injury on children with learning disability. development and disrupted callosal microstruc-
Pediatric Rehabilitation, 1, 179–84. ture following pediatric traumatic brain injury:
Donders, J. & Strom, D. (2000). Neurobehavioral Relation to neurobehavioral outcomes. Neuroimage,
recovery after pediatric head trauma: Injury, pre- 42, 1305–1315.
injury, and post-injury issues. Journal of Head Fay, G. C., Jaffe, K. M., Polissar, N. L., Liao, S., Rivara, J.
Trauma Rehabilitation, 15, 792–803. B., & Martin, K. M. (1994). Outcome of pediatric
Donders, J. & Warschausky, S. (1997). WISC-III factor traumatic brain injury at three years: A cohort
index score patterns after traumatic head injury in study. Archives of Physical Medicine and
children. Child Neuropsychology, 3, 71–78. Rehabilitation, 75, 733–41.
Donders, J. & Warschausky, S. (2007). Neurobehavioral Fearing, M. A., Bigler, E. D., Wilde, E. A., Johnson, J. L.,
outcomes after early versus late childhood trau- Hunter, J. V., Li, X., et al. (2008). Morphometric
matic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma MRI findings in the thalamus and brainstem after
Rehabilitation, 22, 296–302. moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Journal
Donders, J. & Woodward, H. (2003). Gender as a mod- of Child Neurology, 23, 729–37.
erator of memory after traumatic brain injury in Finkelhor, D. & Dziuba-Leatherman, J. (1994).
children. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, Victimization of children. American Psychologist,
18, 106–115. 49, 173–83.
Duhaime, A. C., Christian, C., Moss, E., & Seidl, T. Forster, A. A. & Leckliter, I.N. (1994). The Halstead-
(1996). Long-term outcome in infants with the Reitan neuropsychological test battery for older
shaken-impact syndrome. Pediatric Neurosurgery, children: The effects of age versus clinical status on
24, 292–98. test performance. Developmental Neuropsychology,
Ernst, W. J., Pelletier, S. L. F., & Simpson, G. (2008). 10, 299–312.
Neuropsychological consultation with school per- Galloway, N. R., Tong, K. A., Ashwal, S., Oyoyo, U.,
sonnel: What clinical neuropsychologists need to & Obenaus, A. (2008). Diffusion-weighted
know. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22, 953–76. imaging improves outcome prediction in pediatric
226 forensic neuropsychology

traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 25, disabilities: Clinical research and practice (pp. 208–
1153–62. 33). New York: Guilford.
Ganesalingam, K., Sanson, A., Anderson, V., & Yeates, K. Hicks, R. A. & Gaughan, D. C. (1995). Understanding
O. (2007). Self-regulation as a mediator of the effects fatal child abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 19,
of childhood traumatic brain injury on social and 855–63.
behavioral functioning. Journal of the International Holmbeck, G. N. (2002). Post-hoc probing of signifi-
Neuropsychological Society, 13, 298–311. cant moderational and mediational effects in
Ganesalingam, K., Yeates, K. O., Sanson, A., & studies of pediatric populations. Journal of Pediatric
Anderson, V. (2007). Social problem solving skills Psychology, 27, 87–96.
following childhood traumatic brain injury and its Hutchison, J. S., Ward, R. E., Lacroix, J., Hébert, P. C.,
association with self-regulation and social and Barnes, M. A., Bohn, D. J., et al. (2008). Hypothermia
behavioral functioning. Journal of Neuropsychology, therapy after traumatic brain injury in children.
1, 149–70. New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 2447–56.
Geddes, J. F., Vowles, G. H., Hackshaw, A. K., Nickols, Jin, C. & Schachar, R. (2004). Methylphenidate treat-
C. D., Scott, I. S., & Whitwell, H. L. (2001). ment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
Neuropathology of inflicted head injury in chil- secondary to traumatic brain injury: A critical
dren. II. Microscopic brain injury in infants. Brain, appraisal of treatment studies. CNS Spectrums, 9,
124, 1299–1306. 217–26.
Gerring, J. P., Brady, K. D., Chen, A., Vasa, R., Grados, Kalichman, S.C. (1999). Mandated reporting of suspected
M., Bandeen-Roche, K. J., et al. (1998). Premorbid child abuse: Ethics, law, and policy. Washington,
prevalence of ADHD and development of second- DC: American Psychological Association.
ary ADHD after closed head injury. Journal of the Katsiyannis, A. & Conderman, G. (1994). Serving
American Academy of Child and Adolescent individuals with traumatic brain injury: A national
Psychiatry, 37, 647–54. survey. Remedial and Special Education, 15,
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. 319–25.
(2000). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Keenan, H. T. & Bratton, S.L. (2006). Epidemiology
Function. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment and outcomes of pediatric traumatic brain injury.
Resources. Developmental Neuroscience, 28, 256–63.
Giza, C. C., Mink, R. B., & Madikians, A. (2007). Keenan, H. T., Runyan, D. K., & Nocera, M. (2006).
Pediatric brain injury: Not just little adults. Current Child outcomes and family characteristics 1 year
Opinions in Critical Care, 13, 143–52. after severe inflicted or noninflicted traumatic
Grados, M. A., Vasa, R. A., Riddle, M. A., Slomine, B. brain injury. Pediatrics, 117, 317–24.
S., Salorio, C., Christensen, J., et al. (2008). New Kennedy, M. R. T., Coelho, C., Turkstra, L., Ylvisaker,
onset obsessive-compulsive symptoms in children M., Sohlberg, M. M., Yorkston, K., et al. (2008).
and adolescents with severe traumatic brain injury. Intervention for executive functions after trau-
Depression and Anxiety, 25, 298–407. matic brain injury: A systematic review, meta-
Green, P. & Flaro, L. (2003). Word Memory Test per- analysis and clinical recommendations.
formance in children. Child Neuropsychology, 9, Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 18, 257–99.
189–207. Kieslich, M., Marquardt, G., Galow, G., Lorenz, R., &
Gregg, J. & Appleton, R. (2006). Prevention of brain Jacobi, G. (2001). Neurological and mental out-
injury. In R. Appleton & T. Baldwin (Eds.), come after severe head injury in childhood:
Management of brain injured children, 2nd ed. (pp. A long-term follow-up of 318 children. Disability
313–28). New York: Oxford. and Rehabilitation, 23, 665–69.
Hanten, G., Chapman, S. B., Gamino, J. F., Zhang, L., Kinsella, G. J., Prior, M., Sawyer, M., Ong, B., Murtagh,
Benton, S. B., Stallings-Roberson, G., et al. (2004). D., Eisenjaher, R., et al. (1997). Predictors and indi-
Verbal selective learning after traumatic brain cators of academic outcome in children 2 years fol-
injury in children. Annals of Neurology, 56, 847–53. lowing traumatic brain injury. Journal of the
Hanten, G., Wilde, E. A., Menefee, D. S., Li, X., Lane, S., International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 608–616.
Vasquez, C., et al. (2008). Correlates of social prob- Kirkwood, M. W., Yeates, K. O., Taylor, H. G., Randolph,
lem solving during the first year after traumatic brain C., McCrea, M., & Anderson, V. A. (2008).
injury in children. Neuropsychology, 22, 357–70. Management of pediatric mild traumatic brain
Hibbard, M. R., Martin, T., Cantor, J., & Moran, A. I. injury: A neuropsychological review from injury
(2006). Students with acquired brain injury: through recovery. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
Identification, accommodations, and transitions in 22, 769–800.
the schools. In J. E. Farmer, J. Donders, & S. Kirkwood, M. W., Yeates, K. O., & Wilson, P. E. (2006).
Warschausky (Eds.), Treating neurodevelopmental Pediatric sport-related concussion: A review of the
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury 227

clinical management of an oft-neglected popula- Levin, H. S., Song, J., Scheibel, R. S., Fletcher, J. M.,
tion. Pediatrics, 117, 1359–71. Harward, H., Lilly, M., et al. (1997). Concept
King, W. J., MacKay, M., & Sirnick, A. (2003). Shaken formation and problem-solving following closed
baby syndrome in Canada: Clinical characteristics head injury in children. Journal of the International
and outcomes of hospital cases. Canadian Medical Neuropsychological Society, 3, 598–607.
Association Journal, 168, 155–59. Levin, H. S., Zhang, L., Dennis, M., Ewing-Cobbs, L.,
Kizilbash, A., Warschausky, S., & Donders, J. (2001). Schachar, R., Max, J., et al. (2004). Psychosocial
Assessment of speed of processing after pediatric outcome of TBI in children with unilateral frontal
head trauma: Need for better norms. Pediatric lesions. Journal of the International Neuro-
Rehabilitation, 4, 71–74. psychological Society, 10, 305–316.
Knutson, J. F. & DeVet, K. A. (1995). Physical abuse, Lorber, R. & Yurk, H. (1999). Special pediatric issues:
sexual abuse, and neglect. In M. C. Roberts Neuropsychological applications and consultations
(Ed.), Handbook of pediatric psychology, 2nd ed. in schools. In J. Sweet (Ed.), Forensic neuropsychol-
(pp. 589–616). New York: Guilford. ogy: Fundamentals and practice (pp. 369–418).
Kochanek, P. M. (2005). Pediatric traumatic brain Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.
injury: Beyond the guidelines. Current Treatment Lovell, M. R. & Fazio, V. (2008). Concussion manage-
Options in Neurology, 7, 441–50. ment in the child and adolescent athlete. Current
Kraus, J. F. (1995). Epidemiological features of brain injury Sports Medicine, 7, 12–15.
in children: Occurrence, children at risk, causes, and Madigan, K. A., Hall, T. E., & Glang, A. (1997). Effective
manner of injury, severity, and outcomes. In S. H. assessment and instructional practices for students
Broman & M. E. Michel (Eds.), Traumatic head injury with ABI. In A. Glang, G. H. S. Singer, & B. Todis
in children (pp. 22–39). New York: Oxford. (Eds.), Students with acquired brain injury: The
Laatsch, L., Harrington, D., Hotz, G., Marcantuono, J., school’s response (pp. 123–83). Baltimore: Brookes.
Mozzoni, M. P., Walsh, V., et al. (2007). An evi- Makaroff, K. I. & Putnam, F. W. (2003). Outcomes of
dence-based review of cognitive and behavioral infants and children with inflicted traumatic brain
rehabilitation treatment studies in children with injury. Developmental Medicine and Child
acquired brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Neurology, 45, 497–502.
Rehabilitation, 22, 248–56. Mandalis, A., Kinsella, G., Ong, B., & Anderson, V.
Lachar, D. & Gruber, C. P. (1995). Personality Inventory (2007). Working memory and new learning fol-
for Youth (PIY). Los Angeles: Western Psychological lowing pediatric traumatic brain injury.
Services. Developmental Neuropsychology, 32, 683–701.
Lachar, D. & Gruber, C.P. (1999). Personality Inventory Mangeot, S., Armstrong, K., Colvin, A. N., Yeates, K.
for Children—Second Edition (PIC-2). Los Angeles: O., & Taylor, H. G. (2002). Long-term executive
Western Psychological Services. function deficits in children with traumatic brain
Langlois, J. A., Rutland-Brown, W., & Thomas, K. E. injuries: Assessment using the Behavior Rating
(2005). The incidence of traumatic brain injury Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF). Child
among children in the United States: Differences Neuropsychology, 8, 271–84.
by race. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 20, Mäntynen, H., Poikkeus, A. M., Ahonen, T., Aro, T., &
229–38. Korkman, M. (2001). Clinical significance of test
Larrabee, G. J. (Ed., 2007). Assessment of malingered refusal among young children. Child
neuropsychological deficits. New York: Oxford. Neuropsychology, 7, 241–50.
Levin, H. S., Hanten, G., Max, J., Li, X., Swank, P., Ewing- Massagli, T. L., Michaud, L. J., & Rivara, F. P. (1996).
Cobbs, L., et al. (2007). Symptoms of attention- Association between injury indices and outcome
deficit/hyperactivity disorder following traumatic after severe traumatic brain injury in children.
brain injury in children. Journal of Developmental Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 28, 108–118. 77, 125–32.
Levin, H. S., Hanten, G., Zhang, L., Swank, P. R., Ewing- Max, J. E., Lansing, A. E., Koele, S. L., Castillo, C. S.,
Cobbs, L., Dennis, M., et al. (2004). Changes in Bokura, H., Schachar, R., et al. (2004). Attention
working memory after traumatic brain injury in deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and ado-
children. Neuropsychology, 18, 240–47. lescents following traumatic brain injury.
Levin, H. S., Song, J., Scheibel, R. S., Fletcher, J. M., Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 159–71.
Harward, H. N., & Chapman, S. B. (2000). Max, J. E., Schachar, R. J., Levin, H. S., Ewing-Cobbs,
Dissociation of frequency and recency processing L., Chapman, S. B., Dennis, M., et al. (2005).
from list recall after severe closed head injury in Predictors of secondary attention-deficit/hyperac-
children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical and tivity disorder in children and adolescents 6 to 24
Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 1–15. months after traumatic brain injury. Journal of the
228 forensic neuropsychology

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Nybo, T., Sainio, M., & Müller, K. (2004). Stability of
Psychiatry, 44, 1041–49. vocational outcome in adulthood after moderate to
Max, J. E., Levin, H. S., Schachar, R. J., Landis, J., Saunders, severe preschool brain injury. Journal of the
A. E., Ewing-Cobbs, L., et al. (2006). Predictors of International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 719–23.
personality change due to traumatic brain injury in Olsson, K. A., Le Brocque, R. M., Kenardy, J. A.,
children and adolescents six to twenty-four months Anderson, V., & Spence, S. H. (2008). The influ-
after injury. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical ence of pre-injury behavior on children’s type of
Neurosciences, 18, 21–32. accident, type of injury and severity of injury. Brain
Max, J. E., Robertson, B. A. M., & Lansing, A. E. (2001). Injury, 22, 595–602.
The phenomenology of personality change due to Polak, A. & Harris, P. L. (1999). Deception by young
traumatic brain injury in children and adolescents. children following noncompliance. Developmental
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuro- Psychology, 35, 561–68.
sciences, 13, 161–70. Ponsford, J., Wilmott, C., Cameron, P., Ayton, G.,
McKinlay, A., Grace, R. C., Horwood, L. J., Fergusson, Nelms, R., Curran, C., et al. (1999). Cognitive and
D. M., Ridder, E. M., & MacFarlane, M. R. (2008). behavioral outcome following mild traumatic brain
Brain Injury, 22, 175–181. injury in children. Journal of Head Trauma
Merkley, T. L., Bigler, E. D., Wilde, E. A., McCauley, S. Rehabilitation, 14, 360–72.
R., Hunter, J. V., & Levin, H. S. (2008). Diffuse Power, T., Catroppa, C., Coleman, L., Ditchfield, M., &
changes in cortical thickness in pediatric moder- Anderson, V. (2007). Do lesion site and severity
ate-severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of predict deficits in attentional control after pre-
Neurotrauma, 25, 1343–45. school traumatic brain injury (TBI)? Brain Injury,
Miller, L. (1999). Child abuse brain injury: Clinical, 21, 279–92.
neuropsychological, and forensic considerations. Prasad, M. R., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Swank, P. R., & Kramer,
Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation, 17, 10–19. L. (2002). Predictors of outcome following trau-
Miller, L. J. & Donders, J. (2003). Prediction of educa- matic brain injury in young children. Pediatric
tional outcome after pediatric traumatic brain Neurosurgery, 36, 64–74.
injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 48, 237–41. Prigatano, G. P., Gray, J. A., & Gale, S. D. (2008).
Mittenberg, W., Wittner, M. S., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Individual case analysis of processing speed diffi-
Post-concussion syndrome occurs in children. culties in children with and without traumatic
Neuropsychology, 11, 447–52. brain injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22,
Mottram, L. & Donders, J. (2005). Construct validity of 603–619.
the California Verbal Learning Test–Children’s Rees, L. M., Tombaugh, T. N., Gansler, D. A., &
Version (CVLT–C) after pediatric traumatic brain Moczynski, N. (1998). Five validation experiments
injury. Psychological Assessment, 17, 212–217. of the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM).
Mottram, L. & Donders, J. (2006). Cluster subtypes on Psychological Assessment, 10, 10–20.
the California Verbal Learning Test–Children’s Reynolds, C. R. & Kamphaus, C. W. (2004). Behavior
Version (CVLT–C) after pediatric traumatic Assessment System for Children–Second Edition
brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30, (BASC-2). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
865–83. Service.
Myhre, M. C., Grogaard, J. B., Dyb, G. A., Sandvik, L., Rivara, F. P. (1994). Epidemiology and prevention of
& Nordhov, M. (2007). Traumatic head injury in pediatric traumatic brain injury. Pediatric Annals,
infants and toddlers. Acta Pediatrica, 96, 1159–63. 23, 12–17.
Nacajauskaite, O., Endziniene, J., Jureniene, K., & Rivara, F. P. (1995). Developmental and behavioral
Schrader, H. (2006). The validity of post-concus- issues in childhood injury prevention. Journal
sion syndrome in children: A controlled historical of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 16,
cohort study. Brain and Development, 28, 507–514. 362–70.
Nadebaum, C., Anderson, V., & Catroppa, V. (2007). Rivara, J. B., Jaffe, K. M., Polissar, N. L., Fay, G. C., Liao,
Executive function outcomes following traumatic S., & Martin, K. M. (1996). Predictors of family
brain injury in young children: A five year follow- functioning and change 3 years after traumatic
up. Developmental Neuropsychology, 32, 703–28. brain injury in children. Archives of Physical
Nagle, A. M., Everhart, D. E., Durham, T. W., Medicine and Rehabilitation, 77, 754–64.
McCammon, S. L., & Walker, M. (2006). Deception Roman, M. J., Delis, D. C., Willerman, L., Margulac,
strategies in children: Examination of forced choice M., Demadura, T. L., De La Pena, J. J., et al. (1998).
recognition and verbal learning and memory tech- Impact of pediatric traumatic brain injury on com-
niques. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, ponents of verbal memory. Journal of Clinical and
777–85. Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 245–58.
Forensic Aspects of Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury 229

Ruppel, R. A., Kochanek, P. M., Adelson, P. D., Rose, International Neuropsychological Society, 11,
M. E., Wisniewski, S. R., Bell, M. J., et al. (2001). 65–653.
Excitatory amino acid concentrations in ventricu- Stancin, T., Wade, S. L., Walz, N. C., Yeates, K. O., &
lar cerebrospinal fluid after severe traumatic brain Taylor, H. G. (2008). Traumatic brain injury in
injury in infants and children: The role of child early childhood: Initial impact on the family.
abuse. Journal of Pediatrics, 138, 1–3. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
Salorio, C. F., Slomine, B. S., Grados, M. A., Vasa, R. A., 29, 253–61.
Christensen, J. R., & Gerring, J. P. (2005). Statler, K. D. (2006). Pediatric posttraumatic seizures:
Neuroanatomical correlates of CVLT-C perfor- epidemiology, putative mechanisms of epilepto-
mance following pediatric traumatic brain injury. genesis and promising investigational progress.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Developmental Neuroscience, 28, 354–63.
Society, 11, 686–96. Stipanicic, A., Nolin, P., Fortin, G., & Gobeil, M. F.
Satz, P., Zaucha, K., McCleary, C., Light, R., Asaranow, (2008). Comparative study of the cognitive seque-
R., & Becker, D. (1997). Mild head injury in chil- lae of school-aged victims of shaken baby syn-
dren and adolescents: A review of studies (1970– drome. Child Abuse and Neglect, 32, 415–28.
1995). Psychological Bulletin, 122, 107–31. Stoodley, N. (2002). Non-accidental head injury in
Schoenbrodt, L. (2001; Ed.). Children with traumatic children: Gathering the evidence. Lancet, 360,
brain injury: A parents’ guide. Bethesda, MD: 271–72.
Woodbine house. Taylor, H. G. & Alden, J. (1997). Age-related differences
Schwartz, L., Taylor, H. G., Drotar, D., Yeates, K. O., in outcomes following childhood brain insults: An
Wade, S. L., & Stancin, T. (2003). Long-term behav- introduction and overview. Journal of the
ior problems following pediatric traumatic brain International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 555–67.
injury: Prevalence, predictors, and correlates. Taylor, H. G., Swartwout, M. D., Yeates, K. O., Walz, N.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 28, 251–63. C., Stancin, T., & Wade, S. L. (2008). Traumatic brain
Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2001). Traumatic brain injury injury in young children: Post-acute effects on cog-
in children and adolescents: Assessment and inter- nitive and school readiness skills. Journal of the
vention. New York: Guilford. International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 734–45.
Sesma, H. W., Slomine, B. S., Ding, R., & McCarthy, M. Taylor, H. G., Yeates, K. O., Wade, S. L., Drotar, D.,
L. (2008). Executive functioning in the first year Stancin, T., & Burant, C. (2001). Bidirectional
after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Pediatrics, child-family influences on outcomes of traumatic
121, e1686–95. brain injury in children. Journal of the International
Serra-Grabulosa, J. M., Junqué, C., Verger, K., Neuropsychological Society, 7, 755–67.
Salgado-Pineda, P., Mañeru, C., & Mercader, J. M. Taylor, H. G., Yeates, K. O., Wade, S. L., Drotar, D.,
(2005). Cerebral correlates of declarative memory Stancin, T., & Minich, N. (2002). A prospective
dysfunction in early traumatic brain injury. Journal study of short- and long-term outcomes after trau-
of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 76, matic brain injury in children: Behavior and
129–131. achievement. Neuropsychology, 16, 15–27.
Shum, D., Short, L., Tunstall, J., O’Gorman, J. G., Taylor, H. G., Yeates, K. O., Wade, S. L., Drotar, D.,
Wallace, G., Shepard, K., et al. (2000). Performance Stancin, T., & Montpetite, M. (2003). Long-term
of children with traumatic brain injury on a 4-disk educational interventions after traumatic brain
version of the Tower of London and the Porteus injury in children. Rehabilitation Psychology, 48,
Maze. Brain and Cognition, 44, 59–62. 227–36.
Silver, C. H. (2000). Ecological validity of neuropsy- Teasdale, G. & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma
chological assessment in childhood traumatic and impaired consciousness: A practical scale.
brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, Lancet, 2, 81–84.
15, 973–88. Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). Test of Memory Malingering.
Slomine, B. S., Gerring, J. P., Grados, M. A., Vasa, R., Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
Brady, K. D., Christensen, J. R., et al. (2002). Udomphorn, Y., Armstead, W. M., & Vavilala, M. S.
Performance on measures of executive function (2008). Cerebral blood flow and autoregulation
following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Brain after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Pediatric
Injury, 16, 759–72. Neurology, 38, 225–34.
Slomine, B. S., Salorio, C. F., Grados, M. A., Vasa, R. A., Van Heugten, C. M., Hendriksen, J., Rasquin, S., Dijcks,
Christensen, J. R., & Gerring, J. P. (2005). B., Jaeken, D., & Vles, J. H. S. (2006). Long-term
Differences in attention, executive functioning, neuropsychological performance in a cohort of
and memory in children with and without ADHD children and adolescents after severe pediatric
after severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of the traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 20, 895–903.
230 forensic neuropsychology

Verger, K., Junqué, C., Levin, H. S., Jurado, M. A., Pérez- Williams, D. H., Levin, H. S., & Eisenberg, H. M.
Gómez, M., Bartrés-Faz, D., et al. (2001). Correlation (1990). Mild head injury classification.
of atrophy measures on MRI with neuropsychologi- Neurosurgery, 27, 422–28.
cal sequelae in children and adolescents with trau- Woodward, H. & Donders, J. (1998). The performance
matic brain injury. Brain Injury, 15, 211–21. of children with traumatic head injury on the Wide
Vriezen, E. R. & Pigott, S. E. (2002). The relationship Range Assessment of Memory and Learning—
between parental report on the BRIEF and perfor- Screening. Applied Neuropsychology, 5, 113–119.
mance-based measures of executive function in Woodward, H., Winterhalter, K., Donders, J., Hackbarth,
children with moderate to severe traumatic brain R., Kuldanek, A., & Sanfilippo, D. (1999). Prediction
injury. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 296–303. of neurobehavioral outcome 1–5 years post pediat-
Wade, S. L., Carey, J., & Wolfe, C. R. (2006a). The effi- ric traumatic head injury. Journal of Head Trauma
cacy of an online cognitive-behavioral family inter- Rehabilitation, 14, 351–59.
vention in improving child behavior and social Wozniak, J. R., Krach, L., Ward, E., Mueller, B. A.,
competence following pediatric brain injury. Muetzel, R., Schnoebelen, S., et al. (2007).
Rehabilitation Psychology, 51, 179–89. Neurocognitive and neuroimaging correlates of
Wade, S. L., Carey, J., & Wolfe, C. R. (2006b). The effi- pediatric traumatic brain injury: A diffusion tensor
cacy of an online family intervention to reduce imaging (DTI) study. Archives of Clinical
parental distress following pediatric brain injury. Neuropsychology, 22, 555–68.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, Yeates, K. O., Armstrong, K., Janusz, J., Taylor, H. G.,
445–54. Wade, S., Stancin, T., et al. (2005). Long-term atten-
Wade, S. L., Taylor, H. G., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., Yeates, tion problems in children with traumatic brain
K. O., & Minich, N. M. (2002). A prospective study injury. Journal of the American Academy of Child
of long-term caregiver and family adaptation fol- and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 574–84.
lowing brain injury in children. Journal of Head Yeates, K. O., Bigler, E. D., Dennis, M., Gerhardt, C. A.,
Trauma Rehabilitation, 17, 96–111. Rubin, K. H., Stancin, T., et al. (2007). Social out-
Wade, S. L., Taylor, H. G., Yeates, K. O., Drotar, D., comes in childhood brain disorder: A heuristic
Stancin, T., Minich, N. M., et al. (2006). Long-term integration of social neuroscience and develop-
parental and family adaptation following pediatric mental psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 133,
brain injury. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 535–56.
1072–83. Yeates, K. O., Swift, E., Taylor, H. G., Wade, S. L.,
Wade, S. L., Taylor, H. G., Walz, N. C., Salisbury, S., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., et al. (2004). Short- and
Stancin, T, Bernard, L. A., et al. (2008). Parent- long-term social outcomes following pediatric
child interactions during the initial weeks follow- traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International
ing brain injury in young children. Rehabilitation Neuropsychological Society, 10, 412–26.
Psychology, 53, 180–90. Yeates, K. O., Taylor, H. G., Wade, S. L., Drotar, D.,
Ward, H., Shum, D., McKinlay, L., Baker, S., & Wallace, Stancin, T., & Minich, N. (2002a). A prospective
G. (2007). Prospective memory and pediatric trau- study of short- and long-term neuropsychological
matic brain injury: Effects of cognitive demand. outcomes after traumatic brain injury in children.
Child Neuropsychology, 13, 219–39. Neuropsychology, 16, 514–23.
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Yeates, K. O., Taylor, H. G., Woodrome, S. E., Wade, S.
Children—Fourth Edition. San Antonio: Psycho- L., Stancin, T., & Drotar, D. (2002b). Race as a
logical Corporation. moderator of parent and family outcomes follow-
Wilde, E. A., Bigler, E. D., Hunter, J. V., Fearing, M. A., ing pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of
Scheibel, R. S., Newsome, M. R., et al. (2007). Pediatric Psychology, 27, 393–403.
Hippocampus, amygdale, and basal ganglia mor- Ylvisaker, M., Todis, B., Glang, A., Urbanczyk, B.,
phometrics in children after moderate-to-severe Franklin, C., DePompei, R., et al. (2001). Educating
traumatic brain injury. Developmental Medicine students with TBI: Themes and recommendations.
and Child Neurology, 49, 294–99. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16, 76–93.
9
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury
G L E N N J. L A R R A B E E

Mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI) accounts outcome of MTBI, the role of symptomatic
for 85% of all traumatic brain injury, with an complaint in MTBI, and last, the workup of the
average incidence of 503 per 100,000, placing the individual MTBI case.
annual incidence of MTBI at over 1 million cases
in the United States (Bazarian et al., 2005; National DIAGNO STIC CRITERIA
Center for Injury Prevention and Control, 2003). Diagnostic criteria for MTBI have evolved from
Not surprisingly, MTBI is the most frequent research criteria, as well as from the recommen-
type of case seen by neuropsychologists doing dations of committees of learned clinicians.
forensic work in personal injury settings (Ruff & Mittenberg and Strauman (2000) observed that
Richardson, 1999). inconsistencies in the definition of MTBI compli-
Despite the frequency with which MTBI cate comparison and generalization across stud-
occurs, and despite the significant research that ies. These authors did observe that there is a
has occurred on sequelae of MTBI over the past general consensus across studies that MTBI is
25 years, the issue of the incidence, cause, and defined by an admission Glasgow Coma Scale
persistence of deficits following MTBI remains (GCS) of 13 to 15. Yet, one of the largest outcome
controversial (Bigler, 2008; Mittenberg & studies of the full range of TBI severity identified
Strauman, 2000; Ruff & Jamora, 2009). Zasler the MTBI group by the measure of less than
(2000) wrote of two “extreme camps”: one that 1 hour for time to follow commands (Dikmen,
generally believes that mild brain injury or post- Machamer, Winn, & Temkin, 1995). Levin, Mattis,
concussive disorders do not occur or do not cause et al. (1987) specified MTBI by criteria that
long-term impairment or disability, and the other included loss of consciousness for less than 20
that tends to overdiagnose or “overtreat” MTBI minutes, post-resuscitation GCS of 13–15, absence
and post-concussive disorders, with inadequate of focal neurological abnormalities, and no evi-
consideration of alternative causes. There has dence of intracranial mass lesion. In the Levin,
been a spirited exchange between Bigler and Mattis, et al. (1987) investigation, there was no
Lees-Haley and colleagues regarding the issue of evidence of persistent neuropsychological deficit
persistent deficits and persistent neurological on 3-month follow-up for patients meeting their
damage in MTBI (Bigler, 2001, 2003; Fox & Allen, MTBI diagnostic criteria. In a subsequent investi-
2003; Green, 2003; Lees-Haley, Green, Rohling, gation, Williams, Levin, and Eisenberg (1990)
Fox, & Allen, 2003). found that those TBI patients with admission
The continuing debate over the incidence, GCS values of 13–15 who had evidence of intrac-
cause, and persistence of deficits in MTBI likely ranial lesion, characterized as “complicated”
results from several factors. One factor involves MTBI, had poorer 6-month outcomes than did
the criteria used to diagnose MTBI. Another those subjects who had GCS values of 13–15
factor is the sophistication of research design for without evidence of intracranial pathology. The
evaluation of the consequences of MTBI. A third Williams et al. complicated MTBI patients had
factor is the confusion of symptomatic complaint outcomes more similar to the moderate TBI (GCS
with neurological dysfunction. A fourth factor, 9–12) than the MTBI patients. In contrast,
related to the first factor, is the lack of proper Dikmen et al.’s (1995) MTBI group was classified
differential diagnosis. In the following sections of by time to follow commands less than 1 hour,
this chapter, I review the diagnosis of MTBI, the including patients with potential structural lesions
232 forensic neuropsychology

or secondary complications, and showed no evi- report to a physician that they were “dazed” and
dence of persistent neuropsychological deficits at because they endorsed symptoms of headache,
1-year post-trauma in comparison to an orthope- irritability, and forgetfulness, typically on a history/
dic trauma control group. These data suggest that record form completed in the physician’s office.
restricting of loss of consciousness to less than 20 This goes counter to the recommended practice of
minutes as a criterion for MTBI is too conserva- defining MTBI by acute injury characteristics
tive; rather, the criterion for loss of consciousness rather than by severity of symptoms at random
could be extended to just under 1 hour on the points after trauma (see Alexander, 1995).
basis of Dikmen et al.’s (1995) results. Indeed, Lees-Haley, Fox, and Courtney
The Mild Traumatic Brain Injury Committee (2001), studying groups of litigating individuals
of the Head Injury Interdisciplinary Special with MTBI and other injury (nonbrain injury)
Interest Group of the American Congress of trauma, found, during retrospective questioning,
Rehabilitation has proposed a definition of MTBI that although 67% of those with MTBI reported
(American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine confusion and 71% reported feeling dazed imme-
[ACRM], 1993). These criteria define MTBI by diately after trauma, high proportions of the non-
traumatically induced physiological disruption of TBI group also reported being dazed (51%) and
brain function, as manifested by at least one of the confused (65%). These data highlight the limited
following: (a) any period of loss of consciousness; utility of self-reported feelings of being dazed and
(b) any loss of memory for events immediately confused as a marker of the occurrence of MTBI.
before or after the accident; (c) any alteration in By contrast, EMS or ER records documenting
mental state at the time of the accident (e.g., feel- independently observed confusion or disorienta-
ing dazed, disoriented, or confused); (d) focal tion can provide evidence consistent with MTBI.
neurological deficit(s) that may or may not be Influenced by the ACRM criteria and by the
transient, but for which the severity of the injury report of the National Center for Injury Prevention
does not exceed the following: (e) loss of con- and Control to the Centers for Disease Control
sciousness of approximately 30 minutes or less; (CDC; 2003), the World Health Organization
(f) after 30 minutes, an initial GCS of 13–15; and (WHO; Carroll, Cassidy, Holm, Kraus, &
(g) post-traumatic amnesia (PTA) not greater Coronado, 2004) also proposed a definition of
than 24 hours. MTBI. MTBI is defined by WHO as:
The ACRM definition includes the head being
struck, the head striking an object, and the brain an acute brain injury resulting from mechani-
undergoing an acceleration–deceleration move- cal energy to the head from external physical
ment (i.e., whiplash) without direct external forces. Operational criteria for clinical identifi-
trauma to the head. The ACRM does not include cation include: (i) one or more of the following:
specific symptoms as part of the primary criteria confusion or disorientation, loss of conscious-
for MTBI, but does state that physical symptoms ness for 30 minutes or less, post-traumatic
(e.g., dizziness, headache), cognitive deficits (e.g., amnesia for less than 24 hours, and/or other
attention, memory), and behavioral changes or transient neurological abnormalities such as
alterations in degree of emotional responsivity focal signs, seizure, and intracranial lesion not
(e.g., irritability, emotional lability) are additional requiring surgery; (ii) Glasgow Coma Scale
evidence that an MTBI has occurred. score of 13–15 after 30 minutes post-injury or
A potential problem with the ACRM criteria later upon presentation for healthcare. These
is that they allow for determination of criteria manifestations of MTBI must not be due to
beyond the acute (i.e., day of injury) status of drugs, alcohol, medications, caused by other
the patient. For example, I have seen several cases injuries or treatment for other injuries (e.g.
of alleged MTBI with no evidence of loss of systemic injuries, facial injuries or intubation),
consciousness, disorientation, or PTA based on caused by other problems (e.g. psychological
emergency medical service (EMS) or emergency trauma, language barrier or coexisting medical
room (ER) records or based on detailed interview conditions) or caused by penetrating cranio-
(e.g., the patient accurately recalls the events of cerebral injury (p. 115).
the accident and post-accident treatment, verified
against Department of Motor Vehicles, EMS, and Ruff et al. (2009) observed that there were
ER records), but on examination 1 to 2 months more similarities than differences between the
later are diagnosed as having MTBI based on their ACRM and WHO criteria for MTBI. Importantly,
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 233

the WHO criteria for alteration of mental state at severity of MTBI (American Academy of
time of injury removed the term “dazed” from the Neurology, 1997). Concussion is defined as a
diagnosis, reducing the chance of false positive trauma-induced alteration in mental status that
diagnosis in non-TBI groups endorsing this may or may not involve loss of consciousness,
symptomatic complaint (see above earlier discus- with confusion and amnesia identified as the hall-
sion of problems caused by use of the term “dazed,” marks of concussion. The American Academy of
and Lees-Haley et al., 2001). Second, the WHO Neurology (1997) proposed three different grades
criteria emphasize that focal neurologic signs of concussion: Grade I, characterized by transient
must be transient, contrasted with the ACRM cri- confusion, no loss of consciousness, and concus-
teria which allow focal signs to be persisting. This sion symptoms or mental status abnormalities on
modification may result in a lesser chance of examination that resolve in less than 15 minutes;
including cases of “complicated MTBI” (Williams Grade II, characterized by transient confusion, no
et al., 1990) in the MTBI diagnostic category. On loss of consciousness, and concussion symptoms
the other hand, allowing cases to be identified as or mental status abnormalities on examination
MTBI who have intracranial lesions not requiring that last more than 15 minutes; and Grade III,
surgery could include cases considered to be characterized by any loss of consciousness, either
“complicated” by other investigators. brief (seconds) or prolonged (minutes).
Another problem posed by various diagnostic Hinton-Bayre and Geffen (2002) investigated
criteria for MTBI is that the criteria themselves do concussion severity grades in 21 professional
not provide much information for assessing gra- rugby league players who suffered concussion.
dations in severity of MTBI. This is particularly Testing concussed athletes at 2 and 10 days post-
important when attempting to assess or conduct trauma, Hinton-Bayre and Geffen found no rela-
research on the group of patients with MTBI who tionship between concussion severity, using the
do not show the characteristic pattern of full American Academy of Neurology (AAN) criteria,
recovery, a group characterized by Ruff and and two other sets of criteria (Colorado Medical
Richardson (1999) as the “miserable minority.” Society, 1990; Cantu, 1986) and performance on
Subdividing MTBI by severity was supported Speed of Comprehension, Digit Symbol, and the
by Culotta, Sementelli, Gerold, and Watts (1996), Symbol Digit Test. Similarly, Lovell, Iverson,
who found greater need for neurosurgical inter- Collins, McKeag, and Maroon (1999) found no
vention, more computed tomography (CT) scan differences in neuropsychological test perfor-
abnormalities, and increased incidence of skull mance as a function of positive loss of conscious-
fractures in patients with GCS of 13 compared to ness, no loss of consciousness, or uncertain loss of
GCS of 14. Patients with GCS of 14 had greater consciousness in persons admitted to the ER for
morbidity than patients with GCS of 15. Culotta evaluation of uncomplicated MTBI.
et al. recommended segregating MTBI patients In contrast, McCrea, Kelly, Randolph, Cisler,
with GCS of 15 from those with scores of 14 and and Berger (2002), in an investigation of immedi-
13. Although these data showed the benefits of ate neurocognitive effects of sports concussion,
grading severity of MTBI relative to acute injury found that subjects with loss of consciousness
characteristics, Culotta et al. did not study long- were most severely impaired immediately after
term neuropsychological outcome for these dif- injury, whereas those without loss of conscious-
ferent levels of GCS. ness or PTA were least impaired. All of the McCrea
Ruff and Richardson (1999) proposed subdi- et al. subjects returned to preinjury baseline levels
viding MTBI by three categories of severity, pro- of performance within 48 hours of injury.
ceeding from less to more severe: Type I, altered The investigations of Culotta et al. (1996),
state or transient loss of consciousness, PTA of 1 Hinton-Bayre and Geffen (2002), and Lovell et al.
to 60 seconds, and one or more neurological (1999) did not provide consistent evidence either
symptom; Type II, definite loss of consciousness, supporting or contradicting the value of grading
with time either unknown or less than 5 minutes, severity of MTBI. In contrast, the study by McCrea
PTA of 60 seconds to 12 hours, and one or more et al. (2002) demonstrated differences between
neurological symptom; and Type III, loss of con- MTBI subjects as a function of presence/absence
sciousness of 5 to 30 minutes, PTA greater than 12 of loss of consciousness both immediately follow-
hours, and one or more neurological symptom. ing and 15 minutes after trauma. The McCrea
Published standards for gradation of concus- et al. (2002) data clearly support the value of grad-
sion in sports also allow for assessment of the ing severity of MTBI in the first hour post-trauma,
234 forensic neuropsychology

providing support for the utility of these criteria experience of being injured sufficiently to be
regarding return-to-play decisions (American transported to the hospital, and the potentially
Academy of Neurology, 1997). Sports concussion confounding effects of pain. The trauma control
guidelines are in general agreement that athletes and TBI patients did not differ on age, education,
should be symptom-free before returning to play or gender. Also, by using a trauma control group,
(Moser et al., 2007; Randolph, McCrea, & Barr, Dikmen et al. indirectly controlled for effects
2005). Additional research is needed to establish related to litigation. Although Dikmen et al. did
the value of grading severity of MTBI relative to not report percentage involved in litigation for the
long-term outcome of MTBI, although current data trauma control group, there is no reason to expect
showing acute, day-of-injury differences as a func- a significant difference in litigation for the trauma
tion of MTBI severity do not support persisting dif- control and the more mildly injured patients with
ferences beyond two days (McCrea et al., 2002). TBI, particularly those who followed commands
within less than 1 hour. Both the TBI and trauma
A P P R O P R I AT E control subjects were examined at 1 month and
RESEARCH DESIGNS 1 year post-trauma. At 1 year post-trauma, the
One significant factor that has contributed to the MTBI group (those taking less than 1 hour to
apparent contradictory findings in the MTBI lit- follow commands) did not differ significantly
erature is the use of samples of convenience as from the trauma control group in performance on
opposed to prospective outcome research or a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery.
sports concussion research utilizing pre-injury The importance of using orthopedic trauma
baseline testing followed by repeated measures controls is underscored by the work of Bijur,
examination subsequent to MTBI (McCrea, 2008). Haslam, and Golding (1996), who found that an
Certainly, subject-as-own-control designs, includ- apparent cumulative effect of one versus two
ing pre-injury testing, with repeated measures versus three MTBIs in children disappeared when
studies of nonconcussed persons to control for these children were compared with children who
practice effects, are the strongest designs for inves- had suffered one, two, or three orthopedic inju-
tigation of the outcome of MTBI. The weakest ries. When Dikmen et al.’s (1995) orthopedic con-
designs are those employing clinical samples of trol group data mean data are scored using the
convenience, such as might be aggregated from a Heaton, Grant, and Matthews (1991) demograph-
clinic specializing in treatment of persons with ically corrected normative data, the T scores for
persisting symptoms 1 to 2 years post-MTBI. the two most brain-injury-sensitive measures on
Differences in study outcome as a function of the Halstead-Reitan, the Category Test and the
research design can be very significant. Dikmen Impairment Index, are 43 and 45, respectively.
et al. (1995) conducted what is arguably the best The orthopedic control Impairment Index itself
prospective outcome study of the complete range matches precisely the Impairment Index for
of TBI severity. These investigators compared Dikmen et al.’s MTBI subjects, as well as closely
patients with TBI with six different levels of sever- matches the T score for the Overall Test Battery
ity, defined by time to follow commands (the Mean, T = 44.54, for the MTBI sample reported
highest level of function on the motor component by Meyers and Rohling (2004). These data strongly
of the GCS; this score is not affected by the pres- support the practice of using orthopedic trauma
ence of intubation, which can affect the verbal controls to account for possible pre-injury neu-
component of the GCS and consequently have an ropsychological differences as well as for general
impact on the total GCS score). The mildest level injury effects (i.e., effects due to injury but not
of injury was represented by the group that took specifically related to brain injury).
less than 1 hour to follow commands, whereas the Results from studies using samples of conve-
most severely injured group took 29 days or more nience that did not control for effects of nonhead
to follow commands. traumatic injury can be quite different. For exam-
In addition, Dikmen et al. (1995) collected a ple, Leininger, Gramling, Farrell, Kreutzer, and
large trauma control sample that had been Peck (1990) found significant neuropsychological
admitted to the hospital because of traumatic performance differences comparing 31 patients
injuries not involving the head. By using such a with MTBI and brief loss of consciousness and
control group, Dikmen et al. controlled for socio- 22 MTBI patients experiencing “dazing” injuries
economic status as well as for psychosocial factors without loss of consciousness to the performance
associated with suffering traumatic injury, the of a group of 23 control subjects who were friends
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 235

and family of inpatients with head injury at the reported by Leininger et al. (1990) and Guilmette
Medical College of Virginia. The MTBI patients and Rasile (1995) cannot be accepted as represen-
were tested, on average, 7 months post-trauma. tative of outcome from MTBI when considered in
In another study, Guilmette and Rasile (1995) terms of a dose–response analysis of TBI (Dikmen
compared the verbal memory performance of et al., 1995; Rohling, Meyers, & Millis, 2003).
16 patients with MTBI to the performance of The above differences in study outcome may
16 controls who were community volunteers be partly a consequence of motivational factors
matched on age, education, and gender to the secondary to litigation. In the Leininger et al.
MTBI patients. On testing done a mean of 16.4 (1990) study, 39 patients were pursuing litigation,
months post-trauma, the MTBI patients per- and 14 were not pursuing litigation. Leininger
formed significantly less well than the control et al. did compare the two groups and found that
subjects on all three memory tests. the litigating group performed significantly less
The differences in outcome between the pro- well on the copy portion of the Complex Figure
spective investigation using trauma controls con- Test, but this difference did not remain significant
ducted by Dikmen et al. (1995) and the when the Bonferroni correction procedure was
nonprospective investigations of Leininger et al. applied. However, the litigating/nonlitigating
(1990) and Guilmette and Rasile (1995) that did contrasts suffered from both an imbalance of
not use trauma control subjects, are more obvious subjects (over twice as many litigant as nonlitigant
when the study results are examined for effect subjects) and low power, given the sample size of
sizes. These effect sizes, represented in terms of d, the nonlitigating group. Leininger et al. did
the difference between the MTBI and control not report the mean performances and standard
group mean, in pooled standard deviation units deviations for the litigating and nonlitigating
(i.e., the pooled MTBI/control group standard subjects, so the reader is unable to see whether
deviation) are shown in Table 9.1. there is a consistent pattern of lower mean
As can be seen in Table 9.1, the effect size for scores for the litigants. Guilmette and Rasile
the Dikmen et al. MTBI group, tested 1 year post- (1995) did not specify the proportion of their
trauma, is essentially zero, consistent with com- MTBI sample who had a potentially compensable
plete overlap of the MTBI and trauma control injury, but did note that they screened subjects
group score distributions. In contrast, the 0.57 who had ongoing worker’s compensation or
effect size for the Leininger et al. (1990) MTBI personal injury litigation with forced-choice
group is most similar to the effect size for the symptom validity testing. They did not, however,
Dikmen et al. (1995) group who took between 6 match their MTBI and control group subjects on
and 13 days to follow commands. Even more sig- symptom validity test performance or use analysis
nificant is the 1.10 effect size for the Guilmette and of covariance with symptom validity performance
Rasile (1995) MTBI group, which is most similar as the covariate.
to the effect size for the Dikmen et al. (1995) Litigation effects are significant. Binder
group who took between 14 and 28 days to follow and Rohling (1996) found an effect size of 0.47
commands. Clearly, the neuropsychological data between litigating and nonlitigating TBI patients,

TABLE 9.1 EFFECT SIZE AS A FUNCTION OF SEVERIT Y


OF BRAIN INJURY

Severity of Injurya

Study Minor Brain 1–24 2–5 days 6–13 days 14–28 days 29+ days
Injury hours
Dikmen et al., 1995 .02 .23 .45 .69 1.33 2.30
Leininger et al., 1990 .57 — — — — —
Guilmette & Rasile, 1995 1.10 — — — — —
a
severity of injury is GCS of 13–15 and/or time–to–follow commands less than one hour for Minor Brain Injury; other values are
time–to–follow commands.
236 forensic neuropsychology

with the effect particularly strong for those with the norm is full recovery, with no long-term
MTBI. Moreover, Binder and Kelly (1996) found residual deficits (Belanger, Curtiss, Demery,
that, on the Portland Digit Recognition Test (a Lebowitz, & Vanderploeg, 2005; Belanger &
measure of symptom validity and malingering), up Vanderploeg, 2005; Binder, 1997; Binder et al.,
to 47% of a series of individuals with MTBI per- 1997; Dikmen et al., 1995; Frencham, Fox, &
formed at the level of the bottom 2% of a sample of Mayberry, 2005; Levin, Mattis, et al., 1987; Ruff
nonlitigating patients with severe TBI. Green, et al., 1989; Schretlen & Shapiro, 2003). Binder
Rohling, Lees-Haley, and Allen (2001) found that et al. (1997) conducted a meta-analysis of eight
measures of effort sensitive to malingering published studies of adult MTBI, with 11 total
accounted for 50% of the variance in neuropsycho- samples studied at least 3 months post-trauma and
logical test performance, in contrast to 10% selected on the basis of a history of MTBI rather
accounted for by TBI severity. Last, Mittenberg, than because they were symptomatic; the study
Patton, Canyock, and Condit (2002), in a survey of had a subject attrition rate on follow-up of less
American Board of Clinical Neuropsychology than 50%. The total aggregated sample included
(ABCN) board-certified neuropsychologists who 314 patients with MTBI and 308 control subjects.
did forensic work, found a base rate of malingering Using the g statistic (MTBI and control pooled
of approximately 40% in litigating MTBI subjects. standard deviation), the overall effect size of
Thus, future outcome research on MTBI 0.07 was nonsignificant, but the d statistic (control
optimally should be based on prospective investi- group standard deviation) of 0.12 was significant
gations using trauma control subjects (Dikmen at p < .03. Measures of attention had the
et al., 1995; Satz et al., 1999), and symptom valid- largest effect sizes, with g = 0.17, p < .02, and d =
ity testing (Green et al., 2001). Satz et al. also 0.20, p < .006. Binder et al. (1997) observed that
advocated using a noninjury reference group as a these small effect sizes were equivalent to 2
third group, so that MTBI versus other injury and points for the WAIS-R (Wechsler Adult Intelligence
MTBI versus no injury comparisons can be made. Scale–Revised) IQ or WMS-R (Wechsler Memory
Per their Table 1, Satz et al.’s design with three Scale–Revised) General Memory score (for d =
subject groups allows evaluation of four separate 0.12) and 3 points on the Attention/Concentration
conclusions: (a) no effect, head injury or other Index of the WMS-R (for d = 0.20). These values
injury; (b) general injury effect, body and head; are smaller than the measurement errors for
(c) specific head injury effect; and (d) other injury, these WAIS-R and WMS-R scores (Wechsler,
not head injury, must be the causal factor. 1981, 1987).
Studies involving MTBI clinical samples of Binder et al. (1997) computed an estimated
convenience can still yield potentially useful prevalence of persistent neuropsychological
information if they employ an orthopedic trauma impairment of 5% based on their effect-size anal-
control group matched on age, education, and ysis. At such a low base rate of impairment, the
gender and screened and matched for perfor- positive predictive value of neuropsychological
mance on sensitive symptom validity tests such as test scores (true positives divided by true positives
the Word Memory Test (Green, Iverson, & Allen, plus false positives) was 0.32 at a sensitivity of
1999) or the Portland Digit Recognition Test 0.90 and specificity of 0.90 (note that lower sensi-
(Binder & Kelly, 1996). Alternatively, studies that tivity of 0.80 and specificity of 0.88, values based
have a primary focus other than MTBI, such as on the Heaton, Grant, and Matthews, 1991, data,
Bornstein et al.’s (1993) study of HIV-positive yield a positive predictive value of only 0.26). In
patients with and without history of MTBI or contrast, negative predictive values (true nega-
Alterman, Goldstein, Shelly, and Bober’s (1985) tives divided by true negatives plus false nega-
investigation of alcoholic subjects with and with- tives) were 0.98 to 0.99. Binder et al. (1997)
out history of MTBI, can be considered “pseudo- concluded that at these low positive but high neg-
prospective” (cf. Binder, Rohling, & Larrabee, ative predictive values, the clinician would more
1997) because the primary focus is the clinical likely be correct when not diagnosing rather than
condition (HIV status or alcohol abuse) with the diagnosing brain injury in patients reporting
presence or absence of MTBI on a historic basis. chronic disability after MTBI.
The likelihood of full recovery from single,
THE TYPICAL OUTCOME uncomplicated MTBI is further supported by data
Cumulative research on the outcome of a single, from sports concussion research. Macciocchi,
uncomplicated MTBI shows that neuropsycho- Barth, Alves, Rimel, and Jane (1996) followed
logical deficits may persist for up to 3 months, but collegiate athletes who were tested preseason and
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 237

then within 1, 5, and 10 days post-MTBI; these McCrea (2008), in a comprehensive review of
individuals were contrasted with control athletes MTBI and post-concussion syndrome, provides a
tested preseason and at 1, 5, and 10 days post- summary figure of recovery from MTBI. This is
trauma. Inspection of group mean performance reproduced as Figure 9.1, and shows how symp-
showed no significant performance difference tomatic recovery, cognitive recovery, recovery of
compared to baseline at 24 hours post-trauma; balance, and recovery from the reversible meta-
however, the MTBI group did differ when their bolic effects of MTBI converge, with normaliza-
change scores (pre- to post-injury) were com- tion at 7 days post-trauma. McCrea et al. (2009),
pared to the change scores of the noninjured in a subsequent review, note that fMRI studies
control group. At 10 days, there were essentially suggest a hypothesis that MTBI physiologically
no differences in the MTBI group versus the manifests in a pattern of decreased cerebral acti-
control group. vation in select attention-related neural circuits
Collins et al. (1999) found that college football during the earliest acute period, 12 to 24 hours
players who were concussed showed significantly post-injury, likely relating to the acute physiologi-
poorer Hopkins Verbal Learning Test perfor- cal mechanisms of injury. Increased activation
mance compared to noninjured players at 24 then becomes evident during the subacute phase,
hours after injury. Moderate differences in perfor- up to 1 month post-injury, apparently in relation
mance persisted until at least 5 days post-trauma. to compensatory mechanisms, with return to
Echemendia, Putukian, Mackin, Julian, and normal activation patterns beyond one month
Shoss (2001) found significant differences in neu- post-trauma. Similar data have been subsequently
ropsychological test performance for athletes sus- published for symptom report, cognitive func-
taining MTBI in comparison to nonconcussed tion, balance, and quantitative EEG by McCrea,
control athletes at 2 hours and 48 hours post- Prichep, Powell, Chabot, and Barr (2010).
trauma. There were no multivariate group differ- Compared to control subjects, symptom report,
ences at 1 week post-trauma, although univariate cognitive function, and balance differences,
differences were seen on a few measures. At 1 apparent on day of injury, normalized by eight
month post-injury, a statistically significant dif- days post-trauma and did not differ from values
ference was found on one measure, with the reported for noninjured controls. Quantitative
injured athletes marginally outperforming the EEG differences were significant in comparison to
noninjured athlete control subjects. controls on day of injury, 3 days, and 8 days post-
McCrea et al. (2003) found that concussed trauma, but were not significantly different at
collegiate football players showed recovery over 45 days post-trauma.
the first week post-trauma. Symptoms of concus- The good long-term outcome of a single,
sion (e.g., headache, dizziness) gradually resolved uncomplicated MTBI appears to extend across
by Day 7. Directly measured cognitive function the age range. Full recovery from MTBI is gener-
improved to baseline (preinjury) levels within 5 to ally expected for children (Bijur, et al., 1996;
7 days following concussion, and balance deficits Satz et al., 1997). Very young children, aged 3 to 7
dissipated within 3 to 5 days post-trauma. The years, also make good recovery for intellectual
mild impairments in cognitive processing and skills, receptive language, and everyday and spa-
verbal memory that were apparent on neuropsy- tial memory, although they may have persistent
chological testing 2 days post-trauma resolved by problems in higher linguistic skills (Anderson,
Day 7. McCrea et al. (2003) found no significant Catroppa, Morse, Haritou, & Rosenfeld, 2001).
differences in symptoms or functional impair- Older adults also showed good outcome from
ments in the concussed and nonconcussed athlete a single, uncomplicated MTBI. Goldstein, Levin,
controls 90 days following concussion. Goldman, Clark, and Altonen (2001) found that
In a meta-analysis of sports-related concus- older adults (average age 62.3 years) suffering
sion based on 21 studies involving 790 cases of MTBI performed at similar levels to older adults
concussion and 2014 control cases, Belanger and not suffering MTBI; this was examined on mea-
Vanderploeg (2005) found acute effects of con- sures of attention, memory, and executive func-
cussion within the first 24 hours post-injury of tion when tested, on average, 25 days post-trauma.
approximately one standard deviation or more on The only difference between the older adults with
measures of delayed memory, memory acquisi- MTBI and control subjects was seen on the
tion, and global cognitive function. No residual Controlled Oral Word Association test. Both the
deficits were seen on testing conducted 10 days or individuals with MTBI and noninjured controls
more post-trauma. outperformed a group of older adults suffering
Symptom Recovery Cognitive Recovery Postural Stability Recovery
30 30 24
Concussion 22 Concussion
25 Control 29 Control
20

BESS Total Score


GSC Total Score

SAC Total Score


20 28 18
16
15 27
14
Concussion
10 26 Control 12

5 25 10
8
0 24 6
Baseline CC PG 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 90 Baseline CC PG 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 90 Baseline CC PG 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 90
Day Day Day
Assessment Point Assessment Point Assessment Point

500

Ca2+
400
K+
% Control

300

CMRgluc
200
lactate
100

glutamate CBF
0
0 1 2 4 6 8 12 16 20 25 30 1 6 12 24 2 3 4 6 8 10
Time (minutes) (hours) (days)

FIGURE 9.1: Recovery curves for symptoms, cognitive functioning, and postural stability following MTBI. Higher scores on the GSC indicate more severe symp-
toms. Lower scores on the SAC indicate more severe cognitive dysfunction. Higher scores on the Balance Error Scoring System (BESS) indicate more severe balance
problems. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals. Baseline is preinjury. CC, time of concussion; PG, postgame/postpractice. From McCrea (2008), McCrea
et al. (2003), and Giza and Hovda (2001), with permission.
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 239

moderate TBI on all measures of language, atten- findings on measures known to be sensitive to
tion, memory, and executive dysfunction. acute effects of MTBI as well as to residual deficits
In a subsequent article, Goldstein and Levin following moderate to severe TBI, including Trail
(2001) reviewed extensive literature on cognitive Making B (d = 0.05) and List Learning and
outcome following mild and moderate TBI in Memory (d = 0.00; note that this category included
adults over 50 years of age. These authors the Dikmen et al., 1995 study in which Verbal
concluded that patients suffering MTBI, unlike Selective Reminding had an effect size of 0.11).
those suffering moderate TBI, exhibited cognitive The Pertab et al. results are clearly discrepant
functioning similar to noninjured controls by 1 to from other meta-analytic results (Belanger &
2 months post-trauma. Despite evidence of full Vanderploeg, 2005; Belanger et al., 2005; Binder
neuropsychological recovery, MTBI patients et al., 1997; Frencham et al., 2005; and Schretlen &
continued to report significant anxiety, depres- Shapiro, 2003). Their data are also discrepant
sion, and somatic preoccupation. Goldstein and from expectation relative to the known sensitivity
Levin found that the lowest post-resuscitation of specific neuropsychological tests to acute effects
GCS and the presence of intracranial pathology of MTBI as well as sensitive to long-term residual
were more strongly associated with outcome than effects of moderate and severe TBI.
durations of PTA and impaired consciousness. Further underscoring the atypical findings of
Schretlen and Shapiro (2003) included sports Pertab et al. (2009) are two studies also published
concussion studies with traditional prospective in 2009: Ettenhofer and Abels (2009), and Ivins,
clinical studies in their meta-analysis and found Kane, and Schwab (2009). Ettenhofer and Abels
that persons with MTBI essentially returned to a (2009) compared samples of college students
baseline level of performance within 1 to 3 months with a) history of MTBI, n = 63, and b) orthope-
post-trauma. Indeed, there was a 97% overlap of dic injury, n = 63, studied an average of 36.75
control and MTBI test performance by 1 month months post-trauma. The only significant group
post-trauma, and the 95% confidence intervals difference on neuropsychological testing was on
of effect size estimates at both 30–89 days and Trial 2 of the Delis Kaplan Trail Making Test, on
more than 89 days post-trauma included zero. which the MTBI sample performed better than
Schretlen and Shapiro interpreted these data as the orthopedic trauma sample. There were no
showing that the overall cognitive test perfor- group differences on a Post-concussion Symptom
mance of those with MTBI was essentially Checklist (PCSC; Gouvier, Cubic, Jones, Grantley,
indistinguishable from that of matched controls & Cutlip (1992), nor were there group differences
by 1 month post-trauma. in psychiatric symptoms on the Brief Symptom
Similar findings demonstrating full recovery Inventory (BSI; Derogotis & Spencer, 1982).
within 1 to 3 months post-MTBI have been Interestingly, the total scores on the PSC and BSI
reported in meta-analyses conducted by were strongly correlated with one another, at a
Frencham et al. (2005), and Belanger et al. (2005). similar level of magnitude within both the MTBI
Frencham et al. reported an effect size of 0.11 in and orthopedic trauma samples.
persons assessed over three months post-MTBI, Ivins, et al. (2009) compared computerized
with time-since-injury a significant moderator test scores (ANAM measures of working memory,
variable, and effect sizes tending to zero with processing speed, and reaction time) and post-
increasing time post-injury. Belanger et al. found concussion symptoms of soldiers with self-
no residual neuropsychological impairment by reported MTBI occurring up to two years prior to
3 months post-injury (d = 0.04) in unselected or testing (n = 124; median time since injury of 19
prospective samples of MTBI cases. months; 7 months = 25th percentile; 44 months =
Pertab, James, and Bigler (2009) reported a 75th percentile), to those of noninjured service
re-analysis of the Binder et al. (1997) and persons (n = 703), and service persons who
Frencham et al. (2005) meta-analyses that they sustained injury not involving TBI (n = 129), all of
interpreted as showing a clinically lasting effect, 3 whom had served in Iraq or Afghanistan. Also
months post-injury, on select subsets of neuro- evaluated was history of prior MTBIs. Data were
psychological measures, including Digit Span screened using predetermined cutoffs for invalid
(d = 0.31), Coding (d = 0.33), and Verbal Paired performance (i.e., screened for “bad data”). There
Memory (d = 0.52). However, they omitted 7 of were no differences among the groups in terms of
the 25 studies (28%) analyzed by Binder et al. their endorsement of post-concussion symptoms,
and Frencham et al., and reported other negative with the exception of the noninjured group
240 forensic neuropsychology

showing a higher rate of endorsement of one studies, yielded effect sizes ranging from 0.26
problematic post-concussion symptom compared (depression) to −.28 (anxiety), with an overall
to the MTBI or injured without TBI. There was effect size of 0.05. None of the inverse variance-
no effect of one or two or more prior TBIs on weighted effect sizes were significantly different
post-concussion symptom endorsement, and no from zero. Panayiotou et al. concluded that their
evidence of poorer ANAM performance by those results lent additional support to the argument
with MTBI versus those with injury but no TBI that symptoms of PCS are not unique to MTBI
and those without injury. Interestingly, those patients, occurring commonly in the general
who sustained TBI in Iraq or Afghanistan were population, non-brain-injured trauma patients,
less likely to have “bad data” on the ANAM math- personal injury claimants, chronic pain patients,
ematical processing test than those who were and psychiatric patients. They observed that
uninjured, and those with a lifetime history of premorbid psychological health, expectation of
multiple TBI were less likely to have bad data on symptoms, emotional reactions to the experience
the matching to sample test than those with no of a traumatic event, expression of a predisposed
lifetime history of TBI. disorder as a result of a traumatic stressor, ineffec-
Ivins et al. (2009) also reported no differences tive coping strategies, and poor motivation may
on ANAM performance for moderate or severe better explain the enduring subjective complaints
TBI; however, they used a criterion of loss of of MTBI patients, than any specific effects of
consciousness of greater than 20 minutes and/or MTBI.
post-traumatic amnesia of greater than 24 hours The above findings showing essentially full
to define moderate injury or worse; they did not neuropsychological and psychological recovery
base this on typical GCS criteria of 9–12 for mod- following a single, uncomplicated MTBI for most
erate, and 8 or below for severe. Moreover, 98% of patients also appear to extend to return to work
their sample qualified for a self-reported diagno- following MTBI. Dikmen et al. (1994) found that
sis of MTBI. Given Dikmen et al.’s findings of no 82% of TBI patients who took 5 or fewer hours
differences in comparison to orthopedic controls to follow commands had returned to work by
at one year post-trauma in subjects who took up 12 months, with 84% back to work by 24 months,
to one hour to follow commands, it is likely that compared to 87% of orthopedic trauma controls
some proportion of the small percentage of who had returned to work in the first year post-
cases considered “moderate” or “severe” by Ivins trauma.
et al. actually would have fallen in the range of Other summaries support the typical outcome
Dikmen et al.’s “mild” category. This inference is of good recovery following a single, uncompli-
supported by lack of report by Ivins et al. of medi- cated MTBI. The Institute of Medicine (2008)
cal services provided to any of their TBI sample. convened a group of expert scientists to review
Although persons with MTBI may escape medical literature concerning the long-term consequences
surveillance, it is unlikely that those with moder- of traumatic brain injury of all severities, includ-
ate or severe TBI would escape such surveillance. ing both closed head trauma and penetrating
Thus, the Ivins et al. investigation also highlights brain injuries. In the summary of findings regard-
some of the problems associated with research on ing evidence for causal association, the following
self-reported history of TBI. levels of evidence were identified: 1) sufficient
A meta-analysis focusing specifically on emo- evidence of a causal relationship, 2) sufficient evi-
tional symptoms after MTBI has also demon- dence of an association, 3) limited/suggestive
strated absence of significant persisting problems evidence of an association, 4) inadequate/insuffi-
(Panayiotou, Jackson, & Crowe, 2010). Eleven cient evidence to determine whether an associa-
studies were analyzed, including a total of 352 tion exists, and 5) limited/suggestive evidence of
MTBI and 765 control participants, examined an no association. Most of the conclusions regarding
average of 12.1 months post-injury, considering MTBI fell in category 4 (inadequate/insufficient
scores from measures of depression, anxiety, evidence of association), including MTBI and
coping, and psychosocial disability. Data were neurocognitive deficits, MTBI without loss of
analyzed such that positive values of d were asso- consciousness and dementia of the Alzheimer-
ciated with greater symptom report in MTBI rela- type, MTBI and PTSD, MTBI and long-term
tive to control subjects. Analysis of Cohen’s d with adverse social functioning (e.g., unemployment,
effect sizes weighted by the inverse variance to diminished social relationships, decrease in the
correct for sample size differences across different ability to live independently), and all levels of
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 241

TBI in reference to mania or bipolar disorder, (GCS 13–15 without CT scan abnormalities). The
attempted suicide, multiple sclerosis, amyotrophic GCS 13–15 group with CT-identified lesions was
lateral sclerosis, as well as TBI survived for identified as mild/complicated by Williams et al.
6 or more months and premature death. Carroll (1990).
et al. (2004), in a review conducted under the Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) is more
auspices of the World Health Organization sensitive than CT scan in detecting lesions follow-
(WHO), concluded that for adults, cognitive ing mild-to-moderate TBI (Levin, Amparo, et al.,
deficits and symptoms are common in the acute 1987; Levin, Williams, Eisenberg, High, & Guinto,
stages of MTBI, but the majority of studies 1992). Levin et al. (1992) found that although
report recovery for most persons within 3–12 MRI-identified lesions were more prevalent than
months. those identified by CT in mild-to-moderate head
McCrae et al. (2009) presented an integrated injury and more prominent in moderate (GCS
model of recovery after uncomplicated MTBI 9–12) relative to mild (GCS 13–15) TBI, these
encompassing three time periods post-trauma: lesions resolved over 1 to 3 months follow-up,
acute (immediately post-trauma to approximately paralleling the recovery seen in neuropsycho-
5 days), subacute (approximately 5 days to 30 logical test scores. For some of the subjects with
days), and chronic period (greater than 30 days). acute MRI-identified lesions, however, neuro-
For the chronic period, a relatively small percent- psychological impairment persisted despite
age of patients were observed to report persistent resolution of the lesion on MRI.
symptoms and cognitive or other complaints Dikmen, Machamer, and Temkin (2001)
which may be influenced by injury (e.g. cases of further addressed factors related to presence and
complicated MTBI with abnormal structural persistence of deficits following MTBI, including
imaging findings), or non-injury-related factors patients with complicated MTBI. Four groups of
(e.g., depression, chronic pain, PTSD, life stress or subjects were formed, characterized by decreasing
secondary gain); the brain returned to a normal sample size as a function of stringency of defini-
state of cerebral function (e.g., normal activation tion of the groups. The most inclusive group was
on functional neuroimaging); persistent post- identified by GCS of 13–15, followed by a group
concussion symptoms may be observed in a small defined by GCS and time-to-follow commands of
percentage (<5%) of MTBI cases, significantly less than 1 hour, followed by a group who met the
influenced by noninjury factors. prior conditions with the added condition of
normal CT scan, followed by the most restrictive
FA C T O R S P O T E N T I A L LY classification that met all preceding criteria plus
R E L AT E D T O P E R S I S T E N T had PTA less than or equal to 24 hours. At 1
P RO B L E M S F O L L OW I N G month post-trauma, the group defined by GCS
M I L D T R A U M AT I C 13–15 alone differed on the Verbal Selective
B R A I N I N J U RY Reminding Test from all other MTBI groups,
Wilson, Teasdale, Hadley, Wiedman, and Lang which did not differ from one another or from a
(1994) demonstrated the need to consider GCS group of control patients with non-TBI trauma.
and PTA in evaluating MTBI. These investigators There were no 1-month group differences on
found a close association between GCS and PTA measures of attention, psychomotor speed, or
for those patients who experienced 6 hours or Performance IQ. At 1 year post-trauma, there
more of coma following TBI (note that 6 hours of were no group differences on any measures.
coma is beyond what would be considered In a subsequent investigation, Dikmen,
MTBI). Eight patients had coma less than 6 hours Machamer, and Temkin (2003) found that TBI
with PTA greater than 7 days, and three of these patients with GCS of 13–15 who also had abnor-
eight were only briefly unconscious, if at all. These mal CT scans differed from TBI patients with
data demonstrated the importance of basing GCS of 13–15 without abnormal CT scans for
diagnosis of MTBI on both circumscribed loss of both level of consciousness and neuropsycholo-
consciousness and circumscribed PTA. gical outcome. The individuals with GCS 13–15
Williams et al. (1990) found that patients with with positive CT scans had fewer cases with
GCS of 13–15 and CT evidence of structural GCS of 15, with a corresponding greater number
lesion were more similar in neuropsychological of cases with GCS 13 and 14; had longer time
outcome to patients with moderate TBI (GCS to follow commands; and had longer PTA than
9–12) than they were similar to those with MTBI the group with GCS 13–15 without CT scan
242 forensic neuropsychology

abnormality. Similarly, the GCS 13–15 CT-positive symptomatic complaints and not be fully recov-
group had lower verbal memory (Selective ered (Sadowski-Cron et al., 2006). Subjects meet-
Reminding), Performance IQ, Trail Making B, ing criteria for MTBI who had evidence for
and Halstead Impairment Index at testing 1 month traumatic lesion on MRI performed less well
post-trauma than the group of GCS 13–15 within 96 hours of injury on post-admission test-
with normal CT, which itself did not differ from a ing than those who had MTBI without MRI evi-
control group with non-TBI trauma. At 1 year dence of traumatic lesion (Kurca, Sivak & Kucera,
post-trauma follow-up, the CT-positive group 2006). Iverson (2006) found that those with com-
had lower Selective Reminding than the trauma plicated MTBI performed less well on some tests
control group, with no differences between the of a neuropsychological battery than those with
CT-negative and trauma control group. uncomplicated MTBI when evaluated within 2
Based on the research of Levin and colleagues weeks of injury, but the effect sizes were small to
(Levin et al., 1992; Williams et al., 1990) and medium, and the two groups could not be differ-
Dikmen and colleagues (Dikmen et al., 2001, entiated using logistic regression analysis. Lange,
2003), TBI patients with GCS of 13–15 who also Iverson, and Franzen (2009) found that patients
have CT scan or MRI abnormalities may not have with complicated MTBI performed less well on 3
a truly MTBI. Moreover, the Dikmen et al. (2001, of 13 cognitive measures, but had no significant
2003) and Wilson et al. (1994) investigations differences in the proportion of impaired scores,
showed the importance of considering duration with the exception of delayed verbal recall, on test-
of PTA in differentiating true MTBI from those ing conducted an average of 3.5 days post-trauma.
with apparent cases of MTBI who actually have Returning to the original conceptualization of
more severe injuries. complicated MTBI as equivalent to moderate TBI
Radiologic abnormalities in cases with GCS of suggested by Williams et al. (1990), Kashluba,
13 to 15 have not always been associated with Hanks, Casey, and Millis (2008) found few
poorer outcome. Hanlon, Demery, Martinovich, neuropsychological differences between these
and Kelly (1999) found no difference with respect groups either at rehabilitation discharge, or at
to neuropsychological status or vocational out- 1-year follow-up. The only exception was that the
come between patients who had positive findings complicated MTBI group showed less severely
on CT scan versus those who were CT negative at impaired information-processing speed and
testing conducted approximately 6 months post- verbal learning than did the moderate TBI group.
trauma. Hughes et al. (2004) found neuropsycho- Both groups exhibited evidence of incomplete
logical differences at 72 hours post-trauma recovery at 1-year follow-up.
between those MTBI with MRI abnormalities and To summarize, MTBI with radiologic evidence
those without such findings; however, neither of space-occupying lesion may or may not make
group differed at 6 month follow-up on either full neuropsychological recovery, similar to find-
return to work or on post-concussion symptom ings characterizing moderate TBI. This contrasts
endorsement. In a study with small sample sizes with the typical finding of full recovery within
(n = 14 for uncomplicated MTBI, complicated 3 months that characterizes patients suffering
MTBI and normal controls), the complicated uncomplicated MTBI.
MTBI group differed in cognitive function from Some investigators have presented data
the uncomplicated MTBI group and control suggesting that MTBI results in a reduction in
groups, which did not differ from one another on cerebral reserve (see Satz, 1993, for a general
testing done within 40 days post-trauma (Borgaro, discussion of brain reserve capacity following
Prigatano, Kwasnica, & Rexer, 2003). Both the brain injury). Ewing, McCarthy, Gronwall, and
complicated and uncomplicated MTBI differed Wrightson (1980) found evidence for persisting
from the control group on affective measures, but cognitive deficits in apparently recovered MTBI
did not differ from one another. On admission to patients who were subjected to hypoxic stress.
hospital, persons with GCS 14–15 and intracranial This finding appears consistent with evidence
abnormalities on CT scan did not perform differ- from the same laboratory demonstrating a longer
ently on neuropsychological tests at time of admis- recovery time for persons following MTBI if there
sion or show GCS differences in comparison to was a history of prior concussion (Gronwall, 1989;
those with GCS 14–15 and no CT abnormalities, Gronwall & Wrightson, 1975).
although at one year follow-up those with CT Reduced cerebral reserve was also suggested
abnormalities were more likely to have persistent by the Collins et al. (1999) sports concussion
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 243

investigation, which found that collegiate football damage to the head. Cullum and Thompson
players with one or more prior concussions who (1999) referenced an ongoing investigation that
also had preexisting learning disability had poorer found that MTBI patients with and without
baseline (preseason) test results on the Trail history of prior MTBI did not differ from one
Making Test Part B and on the Symbol Digit another or from a group of non-TBI trauma
Modalities Test than did football players without control subjects when tested within 1 week and
history of prior concussion. Guskiewicz et al. within 2 months of the accident. Iverson, Brooks,
(2003) found that football players with a history Lovell, & Collins (2005) found no measurable
of three or more concussions were three times effect of one or two previous self-reported
more likely to experience a subsequent concus- concussions on high school or college athletes’
sion than football players without history of preseason neuropsychological test performance
prior concussion. There was a dose effect of prior or symptom reporting. Collie, McCrory, and
concussion with length of recovery: 30% of play- Makdissi (2006) found no association between
ers with history of three or more prior concus- self-reported history of up to four or more prior
sions had symptoms lasting greater than 1 week concussions and performance on a computerized
compared to 14.6% of players with one previous cognitive assessment battery for a large sample of
concussion. Subsequently, Guskiewicz et al. Australian rules football players. Bruce and
(2005), employing a questionnaire methodology Echemendia (2008) found no differences in
(but no direct neuropsychological evaluation), performance on a computerized cognitive battery
found a significant association between recurrent or in symptom report for a large sample of
concussion (3 or more) and clinically diagnosed collegiate male athletes who had no, one, two, or
mild cognitive impairment, and self-reported more prior self-reported concussions. Ivins et al.
memory impairments in retired professional (2009) found no differences in neuropsychologi-
football players. Although there was no assoc- cal performance or post-concussive symptom
iation between recurrent concussion and report for persons with no, one, or two, or more
Alzheimer’s disease, there was an earlier onset of prior TBIs in military veterans with no injury,
Alzheimer’s disease in the retired football players injury but no MTBI, and MTBI tested upon return
relative to the general American male population. from deployment in Afghanistan or Iraq.
Data also exist that challenge the hypothesis of Belanger, Spiegel, and Vanderploeg (2010)
reduced cerebral reserve. Alterman et al. (1985) conducted a meta-analysis of the sports concus-
found no significant neuropsychological perfor- sion literature based on eight studies, involving
mance differences between alcoholic patients 614 cases of multiple MTBI and 926 control cases
with and without history of a prior MTBI. suffering a single, self-reported MTBI. The overall
Dikmen, Donovan, Loberg, Machamer, and effect of multiple self-reported MTBIs was mini-
Temkin (1993) found no significant interactions mal and not significant (d = 0.06). Belanger et al.
between alcohol use and head trauma severity in found significant heterogeneity of results using
relation to neuropsychological outcome in their the Q statistic, and therefore conducted subse-
study of the full range of head trauma severity quent meta-analysis by test domain. Significant
(i.e., MTBI, moderate and severe TBI). Bornstein heterogeneity remained for six of eight ability and
et al. (1993) found no significant neuropsy- symptom report domains. Per subtest domain,
chological differences between those HIV-positive executive function (d = 0.24) and delayed memory
patients with a history of MTBI and those without (d = 0.16) showed small but significant effect sizes.
history of MTBI. Although Belanger et al noted little association
Others have not found convincing evidence of between two or more MTBIs and persistent
poorer outcome in persons sustaining multiple problems, they did hypothesize that there may
MTBIs. Bijur et al. (1996) did not find evidence still be a “threshold effect” yet to be determined
for cumulative effects of MTBI in comparing chil- that could lead to persistent sequelae as a function
dren with a history of one, two, or three MTBIs of multiple prior concussion, calling to mind
with non-head-injured orthopedic controls dementia pugilistica and a prior meta-analysis
with history of one, two, or three orthopedic (Belanger & Vanderploeg, 2005) that showed a
injuries. Bijur et al. suggested that cognitive defi- significant effect (d = 0.71) using number of
cits associated with multiple MTBI were second- boxing bouts, length of boxing career, and/or
ary to social and personal factors related to frequency of heading in soccer as the measure of
multiple injuries rather than resulting from exposure.
244 forensic neuropsychology

To summarize, there is no consistent evidence S Y M P T O M AT I C C O M P L A I N T


at the present time showing an association of I N M I L D T R A U M AT I C
multiple MTBI with increased neuropsychologi- B R A I N I N J U RY : T H E
cal impairment in comparison to cases with a POSTCONCUS SION
single MTBI. It is important to consider that SYNDROME
some persons suffering multiple MTBI may differ Post-concussion syndrome (PCS) refers to a con-
neuropsychologically on a premorbid basis, as stellation of somatic (e.g., headache), cognitive
suggested by Bijur et al. (1996). Additional (e.g., memory), and emotional (e.g., irritability)
research using orthopedic trauma control subjects symptoms following MTBI (Alexander, 1995;
with histories of one, two, or three prior injuries Axelrod et al., 1996). There has been a longstand-
may provide further clarification of this issue. ing controversy over whether PCS results from
The use of improvised explosive devices (IEDs) neurological or physiogenic factors versus non-
in the wars in Afghanistan and Iraq have led neurological or psychogenic factors (Gasquoine,
to blast injury as the hallmark injury of these 1997; Lishman, 1988). Physiogenic factors are likely
conflicts (Belanger, Kretzmer, Yoash-Gantz, operative in the first month post-trauma, during
Pickett, & Tupler, 2009). Belanger et al. observed the time when ongoing neuropsychological
that the primary mechanism of brain injury from deficits are resolving, as per Figure 9.1, whereas
explosion is over-pressurization or blast wave psychogenic factors are more likely related to
effect, contrasted with the acceleration/decelera- chronic PCS, also known as persistent post-concus-
tion effects characteristic of typical closed head sion syndrome (PPCS), persisting several months
injury, raising the possibility that blast injury after MTBI (Alexander, 1995; Binder, 1997).
results in a different type of brain trauma. To For example, Dikmen, McLean, and Temkin
examine these issues further, Belanger et al. (2009) (1986) found that, 1 month after trauma, 3 of 12
compared the neuropsychological test results of PCS symptoms (bothered by noise, insomnia, and
patients suffering mild or moderate/severe TBI memory difficulties) discriminated MTBI from
from blast injury to groups of patients suffering control subjects, but at 1 year, there were no dif-
mild or moderate TBI from nonblast trauma ferences in symptom endorsement. Binder (1997)
effects. Overall, there were no neuropsychological reported that 7–8% of MTBI patients remained
differences as a function of blast versus non-blast symptomatic on a chronic basis, a value slightly
injury. Effects were observed for injury severity higher than the 5% rate of persistent neuropsy-
(mild vs. moderate/severe), but these were mod- chological deficit reported by Binder et al. (1997).
erated also by presence of PTSD symptomatology. Noninjury factors have been reported in asso-
Only the CVLT-II showed a continuing signifi- ciation with either severity of symptom report
cant difference between mild and moderate/ and/or persistence of symptom report. Kashluba,
severe TBI following covariation of time-since- Paniak, and Casey (2008) compared high and low
injury and PTSD symptom report. Belanger et al. symptom severity MTBI groups and found that
(2009) concluded that cognitive sequelae follo- the groups were not differentiated by loss of con-
wing TBI were determined more by severity of sciousness, length of loss of consciousness, dura-
injury than mechanism of injury on verbal learn- tion of PTA or injury severity score, but the groups
ing and memory measures. did differ on the percentage seeking or receiving
In a study of blast-related MTBI alone, compensation (60.0% for high severity, 23.8% for
Brenner et al. (2010) studied a total of 45 soldiers, low severity). The high severity group also showed
all of whom reported a blast-caused MTBI, 28 of a greater number of pre-injury life stressors,
whom had persisting symptoms, and 17 of whom pre-injury treatment for psychological problems,
presented with symptoms of PTSD. The group and pre-injury use of analgesic, psychotropic,
with persisting symptoms of MTBI did not differ or neurological medication. Stulemeijer, Vos,
on neuropsychological test performance from the Bleijenberg, and van der Werf (2007) found that
subjects denying persisting problems. Similarly, PCS cognitive complaints were strongly related
the subjects with PTSD symptoms did not differ to lower educational levels, emotional distress,
on neuropsychological test performance from personality, and poorer physical functioning, but
those without PTSD. To date, the results of not to injury characteristics, concluding that
Brenner et al. (2010) and Belanger et al. (2009) do self-reported cognitive complaints were more
not provide any support for an atypical outcome strongly related to premorbid traits and physical
following blast-related MTBI. and emotional state factors than to actual
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 245

cognitive impairments on formal neuropsycho- endorsement in healthy university students and


logical testing. Kirsch et al. (2010) compared an healthy community volunteers, for example, 81%
outlier subgroup of MTBI with extreme persisting endorsed headache, 52% endorsed dizziness, 78%
distress on the Rivermead Post-concussion endorsed irritability, 73% endorsed concentration
Symptoms Questionnaire to a group without per- problems, and 56% endorsed memory problems.
sisting complaints and found that the outlier In a subsample of depressed participants, endorse-
group had a history of prior head injury, preinjury ment of PCS symptoms increased, with 96%
disability, history of substance use, unemploy- reporting headaches, 75% reporting dizziness,
ment, and elevated somatic symptoms when ini- 62% reporting nausea, 83% reporting poor con-
tially seen in the emergency department. At 12 centration, and 75% reporting memory problems
months, the outlier group had a higher use of (Garden & Sullivan, 2010). Lees-Haley and Brown
health services and presence of litigation in the (1993) found very high endorsement of PCS
post-injury period than the non-outlier group. symptoms in persons in litigation for emotional
It is noteworthy that although prior head injury distress or industrial stress, without neurological
was a characteristic of the outlier group, 39 of 45 claims or injuries, with 89% reporting headaches,
non-outliers had prior history of head injury 53% reporting memory problems, and 77%
and did not convert to outlier status, with report reporting irritability. In this vein, litigating MTBI
of good recovery at 12 months. Last, Lange, subjects endorsed more symptoms than nonliti-
Iverson, Brooks, and Rennison (2010) found that gants, despite not differing on demographic char-
MTBI subjects receiving workers’ compensation acteristics, TBI severity ratings, or premorbid risk
who failed the Test of Memory Malingering factors for poor outcome (Feinstein, Ouchterlony,
(TOMM; Tombaugh, 1996), endorsed more post- Somerville, & Jardine, 2001). PCS-type symptoms
concussive complaints, reported poorer cognitive increase in relation to stress, independent of pres-
performance, and performed less well on tests of ence or absence of history of MTBI (Gouvier
attention, memory, and executive functioning et al., 1992). As noted in an earlier section of this
than did MTBI patients receiving workers’ com- chapter, Ettenhofer and Abels (2009) found no
pensation who passed the TOMM, demonstrating differences in PCS symptom endorsement on the
a clear association between poor effort (failed Gouvier et al. (1992) PCSC for MTBI compared
TOMM), elevated symptom report and reduced to an orthopedic injury sample, on testing done
cognitive performance. approximately 3 years post-trauma, but that PCSC
There is increasing evidence that questions endorsement was related to psychiatric symptom
the validity of the PCS symptom constellation. endorsement on the Derogatis and Spencer (1982)
To qualify as a syndrome, a condition must BSI for both groups of subjects. Moreover, BSI
demonstrate a set of specifically associated symp- Somatization correlated with PCSC Dizziness and
toms broadly present in persons who have the Fatigue; BSI Hostility correlated with PCSC
condition and absent in those who do not have it Irritibility; BSI Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms
(note the relevance to sensitivity and specificity). correlated with PCSC Memory Problems and
Symptoms thought to be representative of PCS, Difficulty Concentrating, and BSI Total correlated
such as headache, memory difficulty, and irrita- with PCSC Anxiety.
bility, are also seen in persons with suspected neu- Table 9.2 displays PCS-type complaints for the
rotoxic exposure (Larrabee, 1990; Lees-Haley & Lees-Haley and Brown (1993) outpatient medical
Brown, 1993) and in persons who have chronic patients, nonneurological litigants, and a sample
pain but no history of TBI (Iverson & McCracken, of individuals with MTBI reported by Mittenberg,
1997). Level of depression is more strongly associ- DiGuilio, Perrin, and Bass (1992). These data
ated with PCS report than is head injury status clearly demonstrate the poor specificity for pur-
(Brulot, Strauss, & Spellacy, 1997; Gunstad & ported PCS symptomatic complaints, which can
Suhr, 2001; Trahan, Ross, & Trahan, 2001). These occur with high frequency in nonconcussed
symptoms occur frequently in the non-brain- persons, such that relying on these symptoms as
injured population; for example, Lees-Haley indicative of PCS would result in abnormally
and Brown (1993) found that 62% of outpatient high false-positive diagnoses. In this vein, it is
family practice patients endorsed headache, 20% noteworthy that of the eight symptoms proposed
endorsed memory problems, and 38% endorsed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
irritability. Subsequently, Garden and Sullivan Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV; American
(2010) reported high rates of PCS symptom Psychiatric Association, 1994) as part of the
246 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 9.2 COMPLAINT BASE RATES

Percent Endorsing Complaint


Symptom Mild Medical Non-neurologic
Traumatic Outpatientsb Litigantsb
Brain Injurya

Headache 59.1 62 88
Anxiety 58.3 54 93
Depression 63.2 32 89
Poor 70.5 26 78
Concentration
Dizziness 52.0 26 44
Visual Problems 45.4 22 32
Irritability 65.9 38 77
Fatigue 63.9 58 79
Trouble Thinking 57.6 16 59c
Poor Memory 50.6d 20 53
a
Mittenberg et al., 1992; 100 persons with MTBI
b
Lees-Haley & Brown, 1993; 50 nonlitigating medical outpatients and 170 per-
sons in litigation for psychological stress or distress, without neurological claims
or injuries
c
“Confusion” in Lees-Haley & Brown
d
“Forgets why they entered a room,” the most frequent of 20 memory complaints
in Mittenberg et al.

research criteria for PCS, only one—disordered minimal justification for preferring either set of
sleep—discriminated individuals with MTBI diagnostic criteria. In a subsequent investigation,
from control subjects at 1 month post-trauma in McCauley et al. (2008) found that persistent
the Dikmen et al. (1986) study. This same symp- post-concussional syndrome (PCS) criteria of the
tom of disturbed sleep was endorsed by 59% of ICD-10 were met 3.1 times more frequently than
the Iverson and McCracken (1997) non-TBI the persistent post-concussional disorder (PCD)
chronic pain patients. All eight DSM-IV PCS criteria of DSM-IV. Potential compensation was
symptoms also had high rates of occurrence in the an equally significant factor in both criteria sets,
Lees-Haley and Brown (1993) non-head-injured and there was a higher frequency of major depres-
litigants, ranging from 44% (dizziness) to over sive episode and PTSD in subjects meeting PCS
80% (disordered sleep, 92%; headache, 88%; anxi- and PCD criteria, lending support to the hypoth-
ety, 93%). Gunstad and Suhr (2004) reported esis that psychological factors play a significant
higher PCS endorsement rates in depressed role in persistent PCS/PCD.
subjects and headache sufferers than in control Post-concussion syndrome and other “neuro-
subjects and subjects with history of MTBI, who logical” symptoms are poorly correlated with
did not differ from one another. actual neuropsychological test performance for a
In addition, different diagnostic criteria result variety of clinical samples. Patients with severe
in differing numbers of MTBI subjects classified TBI, Alzheimer-type dementia, and nontraumatic
as showing persistent post-concussional disorder. focal hemispheric brain disease frequently dem-
Boake et al. (2004) found that fewer MTBI sub- onstrate anosognosia or denial or minimization
jects met DSM-IV criteria of both cognitive defi- of deficit (Feher, Larrabee, Sudilovsky, & Crook,
cit and symptoms interfering with social role 1994; Prigatano, 1996; Prigatano & Altman, 1990).
functioning than met the ICD-10 criteria, which Conversely, persons without neuropsychological
were limited to history of TBI plus persistent deficit may misreport memory dysfunction
symptom report. Boake et al. opined that in the secondary to depression (Larrabee & Levin, 1986;
absence of evidence about the relative advantages Larrabee, West, & Crook, 1991; Williams, Little,
of the DSM-IV and ICD-10 diagnostic criteria Scates, & Blockman, 1987) or secondary to soma-
for post-concussional syndrome, there was tization (Hanninen et al., 1994). Brulot et al.
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 247

(1997) found no correlation between Minnesota Failure to appreciate the nonspecificity of


Multiphasic Personality Inventory 2 (MMPI-2) PCS symptoms can lead to misdiagnosis and
neurocorrection items (i.e., those items purport- iatrogenesis of persistent complaints (Larrabee,
edly related to neurological dysfunction) and 1997; Newcombe, Rabbitt, & Briggs, 1994). If a
initial GCS, duration of PTA, or neuropsychologi- patient is prone to attend selectively to common
cal test performance in a large sample of TBI phenomenon and incorrectly attribute these to
patients comprised primarily of subjects with “brain injury,” then “diagnosing” these symptoms
MTBI. Rather, the purported neurocorrection as PCS can be quite damaging. Such a patient,
items were significantly associated with elevations who on a premorbid basis manifests tendencies
on the MMPI-2 Depression Content Scale. toward excessive health concerns, may focus
Given these findings, the presence of an asso- excessively on common daily symptoms such as
ciation between cognitive symptom complaint irritability and forgetfulness and become more
and actual test performance should raise suspi- anxious regarding these “symptoms,” fearing that
cions regarding the accuracy of both symptom these are the result of their recent MTBI. The
report and test performance. Indeed, Gervais, problem is compounded if these individuals are
Ben-Porath, Wygant and Green (2008) found that informed by a variety of health care professionals
a significant association between scores on a that they have suffered TBI.
Memory Complaint Inventory and performance Consistent with the above conceptualization of
on the California Verbal Learning Test actually PPCS, Mittenberg, Tremont, Zielinski, Fichera, and
disappeared when subjects who failed symptom Rayls (1996) found that providing a patient with
validity testing were excluded from the analysis. MTBI with a printed cognitive behavioral manual
Mittenberg et al. (1992) found that head and having the patient meet with a therapist prior
trauma-naıve normal subjects, when asked to to hospital discharge resulted in shorter duration of
imagine they had sustained an MTBI, endorsed symptoms and lower symptom endorsement rates
the same set of PCS symptoms as did persons with at 6-month follow-up in comparison to a nontreat-
actual MTBI. Of particular interest, the subjects ment control group who received routine hospital
with MTBI underestimated the frequency at treatment and discharge recommendations. The
which these symptoms occurred prior to their cognitive behavioral manual reviewed the nature
trauma (note that this “good old days” bias and incidence of expected symptoms, the cognitive
reported by Mittenberg et al. has been replicated behavioral model of symptom maintenance and
by Gunstad & Suhr, 2001; 2004, and Iverson, treatment, as well as techniques for reducing symp-
Lange, Brooks, & Rennison, 2010). Mittenberg toms and instructions for gradual resumption of
et al. (1992) related their results to a process of premorbid activities (see Mittenberg, Zielinski, &
selective attentional bias to common internal Fichera, 1993).
states, stress, and arousal. Putnam and Millis Negative expectations about outcome of MTBI
(1994) provided a similar explanation, charac- can also affect neuropsychological test perfor-
terized PPCS as a somatoform disorder, and mance in addition to maintaining or increasing
drew parallels between selective attentional PCS symptom report. Suhr and Gunstad (2002)
bias in persons with MTBI and the self-directed found that those MTBI subjects who had their
selective attention to bodily sensations in persons attention called to their prior head injury history
with elevated health concerns (see Watson & and who were advised of the potential effects of
Pennebaker, 1989; also see Mittenberg & head injury on cognition (“diagnosis threat”) per-
Strauman, 2000). Subsequently, Delis and Wetter formed less well on measures of general intellect
(2007), influenced by the work of Mittenberg and memory than did a group of MTBI subjects
et al. (1992), Putnam and Millis (1994), and provided with neutral test instructions. Moreover,
Watson and Pennebaker (1989), proposed criteria the diagnosis threat group rated themselves as
for cogniform disorder and cogniform condition, putting forth less effort on the tests, and their self-
which delineate a type of somatoform disorder rated degree of effort correlated with their perfor-
characterized by excessive or atypical symptom mance on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
report in the absence of otherwise demonstrated and with their performance on the Complex
neurological abnormality or evidence for malin- Figure Test. Although Suhr and Gundstad did not
gering. PPCS would fit nicely into the cogniform administer symptom validity testing, the scores
criteria proposed by Delis and Wetter (Larrabee, reported in their Table 2 are not consistent with
2007). the presence of malingering in the diagnosis threat
248 forensic neuropsychology

group (e.g., their mean age-scaled score on litigation to be associated with persistent com-
Digit Span was 11.1). Hence, the motivational plaints and poorer neuropsychological test per-
factors affecting the diagnosis threat group’s formance in MTBI, and by the best-evidence
performance appear to be unintentional and out- synthesis conducted by the World Health
side their conscious control. In a subsequent Organization, which concluded that where symp-
investigation, Suhr and Gunstad (2005) replicated toms of MTBI persisted, compensation/litigation
the finding of poorer performance in their “diag- was the only consistent factor, with little evidence
nosis threat” group in comparison to the group for other predictors (Carroll et al., 2004).
receiving neutral test instructions. Contrary to
their earlier findings, the “diagnosis threat” group DIFFERENTIAL DIAGNO SIS
did not rate themselves as putting forth less effort O F T H E PAT I E N T W I T H M I L D
on testing than the neutral instruction group. T R A U M AT I C B R A I N I N J U RY
Moreover, they did not perform differently from As noted by Binder et al. (1997), the low base rate
the neutral group on an objective measure of per- (5%) of persistent impairment following MTBI,
formance validity, the Word Memory Test (Green results in positive predictive power of less than
et al., 1996), nor was their performance related to 50%, indicating a clinician is more likely to be
increased anxiety or depression. In summary, the incorrect than correct in diagnosing brain damage
“diagnosis threat” research of Suhr and Gunstad as the cause of persistent impairment. These data
(2002; 2005) is a compelling demonstration of do not indicate that persistent impairment cannot
iatrogenesis in MTBI. occur, but do strongly underscore the need for the
Last, persistent symptomatic complaint of clinician to perform a careful differential diagno-
persons having sustained MTBI has been used to sis of the individual MTBI patient because the risk
characterize a group of subjects thought to repre- of false-positive diagnosis is high (Binder, 1997).
sent a “miserable minority,” who are suffering The necessity of careful differential diagnosis is
from continuing effects of their MTBI (Bigler, underscored by the data in Figure 9.2 from Iverson
2008; Ruff, Camenzuli, & Mueller, 1996; Ruff & (2005), which displays effect sizes (mean of MTBI
Richardson, 1999). The estimate typically associ- group minus the mean of the comparison group,
ated with the prevalence of this “miserable minor- divided by the pooled standard deviation) for
ity” is 15% (Alexander, 1995). As Greiffenstein MTBI at various points of recovery (0–6 days,
(2009) has observed, this prevalence rate is inac- 7–30 days, 1–3 months), in comparison to effect
curate, as it was based on a material misunder- sizes for acute and chronic moderate/severe TBI,
standing of two studies: McClean, Temkin, cannabis use, dysthymia, benzodiazepine with-
Dikmen, and Wyler (1983), and Rutherford, drawal, depression, litigation, ADHD, bipolar
Merret, and McDonald, (1979). Greiffenstein disorder, chronic benzodiazepine use, and exag-
clarified that the McClean et al. investigation fol- geration/malingering. Figure 9.2 clearly shows
lowed only 20 patients for 1 month; there was no how a patient’s clinical history and disorders other
1-year follow-up (i.e., no chronic follow-up data). than MTBI provide an important framework for
For the Rutherford et al. cases, 19 (14.5%) still interpretation of neuropsychological test data.
reported complaints at 1-year follow-up, but eight Risk factors for persistent problems following
were involved in lawsuits, six showed malingering MTBI were reviewed in the section, “Factors
signs at 6 months, and most complained of new Potentially Related to Persistent Problems Follo-
symptoms at 1 year that were not reported at 6 wing Mild Traumatic Brain Injury.” These factors
weeks post-injury. Moreover, Greiffenstein noted include the possible effects of prior MTBIs on
that there was no control group, which made it increased symptoms or prolonged recovery time
difficult to draw conclusions about population following a subsequent MTBI. If such effects do
base rates for symptoms which, as already dis- occur, they should be manifested early on,
cussed in this section and demonstrated in with demonstration of prolonged confusion/
Table 9.2, are nonspecific for MTBI. Interestingly, disorientation or other PCS symptoms in the
Rutherford (1989), in a follow-up to his earlier acute and subacute period following an MTBI
work, concluded that the presence of litigation (cf. Guskiewicz et al., 2003). Conversely, a very
was associated with more than doubling the rapid return to orientation and processing of
long-term symptom rate. Rutherford’s conclusion, ongoing memory following an MTBI would
over 20 years ago, has been echoed by the meta- appear to contradict any potential additive effects
analysis of Belanger et al. (2005) which found of a prior MTBI.
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 249

1.1
1
0.9
0.8
0.7
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0

AD tion
C rate ver

Bi

C rd

Ex ic b er
M

M at

D bis ere I 0

Be ym

Li ssi ine
M

M
M

an

hr
ys

ep
TB –3 ys

tig on

po
od

od e-
TB 6 d

TB 30
TB

ag en
H
nz ia

on sor
na sev TB

th

re ep

la
er

e se
I
I 7 ay

I 1 da

ge zo
I

od
0–

ra
ia

i
z

tio iaz
-
da

n/ ep
d
ys

m
s

al ne
e

in
TB –6

ith

ge
I> mo

dr

rin
24 nt

aw

g
al
m hs
on
th
s

FIGURE 9.2: Neuropsychological effect sizes for MTBI at different points of recovery, in comparison to
moderate-severe TBI, and various other disorders, drug use/withdrawal, litigation, and exaggeration/malin-
gering. Effect sizes less than 0.3 are very small and difficult to detect in individual patients because the patient
and control groups largely overlap (overlap for an effect size of 0.3 is 78.7%). From Iverson (2005) and
McCrea (2008), with permission.

As noted, individuals with apparent MTBI “oriented times three” actually describe the
who may have admission GCS values in the MTBI highest level of verbal response on the GCS.
range of 13 to 15, who also have CT or MRI abnor- Moreover, orientation to time is typically the last
malities, focal neurological findings on day of component of orientation to recover during the
injury, or PTA lasting longer than 24 hours are resolution of PTA (High, Levin, & Gary, 1990).
not actually individuals with MTBI. Rather, these Hence, someone who is oriented times three
are cases of complicated MTBI. Such cases may (time, place, and person) is most likely no longer
have recovery patterns more similar to moderate in PTA. The ER records will also contain informa-
TBI as opposed to MTBI; that is, these cases may tion on presence/absence of focal neurological
have persistent neuropsychological deficits on a signs and whether a CT scan was done, as well as
chronic basis. the results of any CT scan.
Thus, the careful differential diagnosis of the The section on PCS, which reviewed the
patient with MTBI must begin with the acute nonspecificity of symptoms for MTBI, demon-
injury characteristics (Alexander, 1995). Records strated the relationship of these symptoms to
from EMS rescue runs and hospital ER records factors of depression, stress, and somatization.
are important for characterizing day-of-injury Consequently, any careful differential diagnosis of
TBI severity. These records will provide informa- the individual MTBI case should also consider the
tion on level of orientation, GCS rating, time of possibility of symptom expression and mainte-
accident, and time spent in the hospital, as well as nance because of a diagnosable psychiatric disor-
procedures conducted in the hospital. The der. Binder (1997) cited Symonds (1937), who
memory and orientation of the patient with MTBI stated that, “It is not only the kind of injury that
can be evaluated during clinical interview and matters, but the kind of head” (p. 1092). As Binder
later cross-checked against the EMS and ER observed, the kind of head is virtually as broad as
records. Statements characterizing a patient as the DSM-IV.
250 forensic neuropsychology

In addition to being sensitive to neurological heightened daily stress. Cogniform disorder, as


dysfunction, performance on neuropsychological proposed by Delis and Wetter (2007) and dis-
tests is also affected by a variety of psychiatric cussed earlier in this chapter, may explain PPCS
disorders, particularly disorders such as depres- as a specific type of somatoform disorder charac-
sion that affect attention and effortful processing terized by atypical and/or excessive cognitive
(Cohen, Weingartner, Smallberg, Pickar, & complaints.
Murphy, 1982; Cohen, 1993). Several DSM-IV Developmental disorders (Spreen, Risser, &
disorders, including schizophrenia, mood disor- Edgell, 1995; Yeates, Ris, Taylor, & Pennington,
ders, anxiety disorders, and particularly posttrau- 2010) can also affect symptomatic complaints
matic stress disorder (PTSD), are characterized by and neuropsychological test performance. Deve-
accompanying cognitive symptoms (American lopmental learning disorders can present with
Psychiatric Association, 1994; also see Figure 9.2). specific reduction in verbal cognitive, calcula-
Levin et al. (2001) found no neuropsychologi- tional, and memory skills (Fletcher, Lyon, Fuchs,
cal differences at 3 months post-trauma between & Barnes, 2007; Rourke, 1991). Developmental
trauma controls and a group of patients with nonverbal learning disability can result in signifi-
mild-to-moderate TBI. In contrast, when the cant social interaction difficulties in addition to
trauma control and MTBI groups were combined, specific reductions in nonverbal visual perceptual
then divided on the basis of presence/absence of skills (Rourke, 1995). Persons with preexisting
either PTSD or major depressive disorder (MDD), diagnoses of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
differences emerged on neuropsychological test- der can show neuropsychological deficits in atten-
ing. Patients with PTSD performed significantly tion, as well as problems in impulse control
less well on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test than (Barkley, 2006; also see Figure 9.2).
did patients without PTSD. Patients with MDD Chronic pain and pharmacological treatment
performed significantly less well on the Symbol of pain are also related to symptomatic complaints
Digit Modality Test, Rey Complex Figure, Verbal similar to those in PCS (Iverson & McCracken,
Selective Reminding Test, and Wisconsin Card 1997) as well as to changes in neuropsychological
Sorting Test than did patients without MDD. In test performance (Taylor, Cox, & Mailis, 1996;
particular, Levin et al. found that MDD was Uomoto & Esselman, 1993). Chronic pain com-
comorbid with PTSD, in addition to the afore- plaints, particularly headache and neck/shoulder
mentioned neuropsychological abnormalities, and back pain, are significantly more frequent in
and associated with disability measured by the those with MTBI relative to moderate or severe
Glasgow Outcome Scale and Community TBI; for instance, 95% of those with MTBI in one
Integration Questionnaire. series had pain complaints, in contrast to only 22%
Chapter 13 in this volume reviews somato- of individuals with moderate-to-severe TBI with
form stress disorders, which must also be consid- pain complaints (Uomoto & Esselman, 1993).
ered in any differential diagnosis of suspected Taylor et al. (1996) compared the information-
MTBI. Mittenberg and Strauman (2000) consid- processing performance of three groups of
ered PPCS following MTBI as a unique somato- patients, those with (a) whiplash, (b) chronic pain
form disorder specific to concussion. Alternatively, without history of whiplash or head injury, and
significant overlap of PCS symptoms with other (c) moderate-to-severe TBI. The whiplash and
disorders not characterized by history of concus- chronic pain groups were carefully matched on
sion (e.g., chronic pain, Iverson & McCracken, IQ, WMS-R Digit and Visual Span, pain severity
1997; litigants claiming psychological injuries, ratings, and MMPI-2 profiles. Taylor et al. found
Lees-Haley & Brown, 1993) suggests that PPCS no group differences on either the Paced Auditory
may be one of many exemplars of a more general Serial Addition Test (PASAT) or Auditory
category of somatoform stress disorder rather Consonant Trigrams procedures. Both the chronic
than specific to MTBI. This conclusion was sup- pain and whiplash groups had higher pain ratings
ported in particular by Gouvier et al. (1992), who and MMPI-2 Depression scales than the moder-
found no difference in rates of PCS symptom ate-to-severe TBI group, but did not differ from
endorsement in college students who had history one another. These data demonstrate the effects of
of MTBI compared with students who did not chronic pain on symptom report and neuropsy-
have history of MTBI. Moreover, both the MTBI chological test performance. Moreover, these data
and non-MTBI students experienced increased contradict the association between whiplash and
PCS symptom endorsement under periods of brain dysfunction secondary to MTBI claimed by
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 251

some investigators (Sweeney, 1992). Indeed, FUTURE DIRECTIONS


Taylor et al. (1996) concluded that their results There are two main areas for which additional
demonstrated poor specificity for detecting subtle research will be of interest: (a) developing a better
effects of brain trauma and did not support the understanding of the persons falling in the miser-
theory of neuronal degeneration as the etiology of able minority who have less than the expected
whiplash-related cognitive complaints. full recovery following MTBI and (b) additional
Substance abuse, particularly alcohol abuse, is research into the consequences of multiple
a significant differential diagnostic consideration, MTBIs. Do persons in the miserable minority
particularly given the known association of alco- have preexisting problems that predispose to
hol use with motor vehicle accidents. As reviewed, experiencing less than full recovery from MTBI?
alcohol use does not appear to affect outcome Following how many MTBIs does someone need
adversely in MTBI (Alterman et al., 1985; Dikmen to become concerned about potential permanent
et al., 1993). Alcohol abuse, in and of itself, how- neuropsychological deficits?
ever, is certainly associated with neuropsychologi- Research on identification of persons who are
cal deficits (Parsons, Butters, & Nathan, 1987). going to fall in the miserable minority needs to be
Data also exist demonstrating that nonalcoholic conducted on a prospective basis from the time of
progeny of alcoholic parents perform more poorly injury forward, rather than on the basis of samples
on neuropsychological testing than do nonalco- of convenience collected from persons presenting
holic progeny of nonalcoholic parents (see Parsons, for clinical services 1 or 2 years post-trauma.
1987, for a review of these findings; also see As discussed in this chapter, following patients
Montserrat, Rodriguez-Holguin, & Cadaveira, prospectively, from the time they enter the emer-
1999). Bolla reviews the neuropsychological effects gency room, ensures a study of a representative
of drugs of abuse in chapter 11 of this volume. sample of MTBI.
A major consideration in any differential diag- Given the strong evidence emerging from the
nosis of MTBI is the possibility that deficits and sports concussion research of full recovery from a
complaints are the result of malingering. single MTBI (Belanger et al., 2009; Collins et al.,
Malingering, the exaggeration or fabrication of 1999; Echemendia et al., 2001; Macciocci et al.,
symptoms or deficits for external incentives in 1996; McCrea, 2008; McCrea et al., 2003) and the
adversarial circumstances, is reviewed by Larrabee strong evidence, in prospective research, of full
in chapter 5 of this volume. Malingering is a fre- recovery (Belanger et al., 2005; Binder et al., 1997;
quent occurrence in litigated MTBI cases. Dikmen et al., 1995; Frencham et al., 2005;
Larrabee (2003) and Mittenberg et al. (2002) Schretlen & Shapiro, 2003), it may develop that
found, using different methodologies, an approxi- persons falling in the miserable minority have
mate 40% base rate of malingering in litigated persisting complaints and problems on a psycho-
MTBI cases, suggesting that this is a reliable esti- logical basis as part of a somatoform equivalent
mate of malingering base rate on average. disorder (Delis & Wetter, 2007; Mittenberg et al.,
Larrabee, Millis, and Meyers (2009) reviewed 1992, 1996; Putnam & Millis, 1994; Suhr &
additional research that further supports an aver- Gundstad, 2002; 2005), as a consequence of
age base rate of malingering of 40% in settings post-traumatic stress disorder (Levin et al., 2001),
with external incentive, including MTBI as well as or as a consequence of preexisting psychiatric or
other alleged conditions. The base rate of 40% is neurological conditions (Dikmen et al., 2001;
eight times the 5% frequency of persistent neu- Luis, Vanderploeg, & Curtiss, 2003).
ropsychological deficits in individuals with MTBI Alternatively, it may develop that, as a func-
who were followed prospectively since the time of tion of greater neurological insult, persons with
their injury and reported by Binder et al. (1997). GCS of 13 are at greater risk for less-than-full
Indeed, Mittenberg et al. (2002) argued that, given recovery than are persons who have GCS of 15
Binder et al.’s 5% base rate of persistent deficit (Culotta et al., 1996). Consequently, efforts
following MTBI and their survey data indicating a directed at grading the severity of MTBI, such as
malingering base rate of 40%, the actual base have been developed for sports concussion
rate of malingering in litigants showing neuro- (American Academy of Neurology, 1997) or
psychological deficits following MTBI may be as criteria developed by Ruff and Richardson (1999)
high as 88%. Refer to chapter 5 in this volume for that emphasize duration of PTA and persistence
further discussion of the evaluation and diagnosis of neurological symptoms in the first 24 hours
of malingering. post-trauma, may yield information useful in
252 forensic neuropsychology

predicting who will become part of the miserable appropriate research designs, neither investiga-
minority. Of interest, Vanderploeg, Curtiss, and tion employed an orthopedic trauma control
Belanger (2005) reported data showing no signifi- group. Given evidence that orthopedic trauma
cant differences between veterans reporting controls may differ in neuropsychological test
a history of MTBI compared to those reporting performance relative to a noninjured healthy
history of injury in a motor vehicle accident and population (Dikmen et al., 1995; Bijur et al. 1996),
those reporting no injury history on 15 separate it would appear prudent to incorporate such
neuropsychological tests (mean effect size = −.03, subjects in DTI studies of MTBI, to control for
consistent with meta-analytic outcome data on possible premorbid differences in white matter
chronic effects of MTBI); rather, there was density that could underlie these apparent non-
evidence for a subgroup characterized by prob- TBI differences in neuropsychological abilities.
lems with left-sided inattention and gait distur- Importantly, the role of psychological factors
bance. In particular, subtle attention problems (Delis & Kaplan, 2007; Mittenberg et al., 1992;
(defined as examinee request to discontinue the Suhr & Gunstad, 2002; 2005) and litigation
PASAT) were associated with left-sided visual (Belanger et al., 2005; Binder & Rohling, 1996;
imperceptions, and excessive proactive inhibition Carroll et al., 2004) in persistence of MTBI symp-
on the CVLT (Trial 1 minus Trial B difference tomatology must be explored further. Per the
of 3 or more) was associated with impaired information reviewed in the present chapter, as
tandem gait. These data suggest greater neuro- well as in other reviews of MTBI (Carroll et al.,
logic insult in those MTBI subjects manifesting 2004; McCrea, 2008), psychological and litigation
these difficulties which, since subjects were factors are most strongly associated with persis-
identified based on a self-reported rather than tence of symptomatic complaints in cases of
medically documented history of MTBI, may uncomplicated MTBI.
have characterized individuals who actually had Additional research is needed on the relation-
sustained a complicated MTBI. ship of multiple MTBIs to the likelihood of
MRI diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) holds persistent neuropsychological deficits. Gronwall’s
promise as a means for identifying those MTBI laboratory (Gronwall, 1989; Gronwall &
subjects who may have sustained relatively greater Wrightson, 1975) showed that persons with
initial trauma. Rutgers et al. (2008) found differ- a history of prior MTBI took longer to recover
ences in corpus callosum DTI measures of than persons with a single MTBI. The issue of per-
fractional anisotropy (FA) and apparent diffusion sistence of deficit was not addressed. Collins et al.
coefficient (ADC) in subjects with MTBI in com- (2002) reported that concussed high school
parison to healthy control subjects, when scanned athletes with three or more prior concussions
less than 3 months post-trauma. For those MTBI were 9.3 times more likely than concussed
subjects who were scanned 3 months or greater athletes with no prior history of concussion to
post-trauma, there were no FA or ADC differ- demonstrate three to four abnormal on-field
ences relative to healthy controls. Mayer et al. markers of concussion severity. McCrea et al.
(2010) found that DTI FA differences in the left (2003) found greater risk of sustaining a subse-
and right hemisphere discriminated between an quent concussion in football players with history
MTBI group scanned an average of 12 days post- of preexisting concussion and longer time for
trauma and a healthy control group, following symptom resolution in patients with history of
correction for differences in premorbid intellec- three as opposed to one prior concussion. As
tual functioning. Interestingly, neuropsycholo- noted earlier, cumulative effects of multiple MTBI
gical test scores did not discriminate these two have not been consistently demonstrated in the
groups, after controlling for premorbid intellec- literature (cf. Belanger et al., 2010).
tual function. A subset of the subjects were seen at Of particular interest in relation to evaluating
3–5 month follow-up, and showed a trend towards the effects of multiple MTBIs is the work of Bijur
normalization of DTI FA. The Rutgers et al. (2008) et al. (1996), who found that an apparent cumula-
and Mayer et al. (2010) studies show a heightened tive effect of multiple MTBIs in children did not
sensitivity of DTI to subacute effects of MTBI, reflect cumulative neuropsychological deficit;
with follow-up data suggesting a parallel with rather, the lower performance of children with
neuropsychological outcome, given an apparent multiple MTBI paralleled the performance of
normalization of DTI values at 3 months or more children with multiple orthopedic injuries. In
post-trauma. Of note per earlier discussion of other words, children at risk for multiple injuries
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 253

(head or orthopedic) had lower neuropsychologi- Belanger, H. G., Curtiss, G., Demery, J. A., Lebowitz, B.
cal scores than those children with a single injury K., & Vanderploeg, R. D. (2005). Factors moderat-
(head or orthopedic), showing Satz et al.’s (1999) ing neuropsychological outcomes following mild
“general injury effect, head and body,” but not traumatic brain injury: A meta-analysis. Journal of
showing evidence for head injury effect. the International Neuropsychological Society, 11,
The optimal “laboratories” for evaluation of 215–27.
the effects of multiple MTBIs are the sports Belanger, H. G., Kretzmer, T., Yoash-Gantz, R., Pickett,
concussion programs currently in place in T., & Tupler, L. A. (2009). Cognitive sequelae of
blast-related versus other mechanisms of brain
professional and collegiate sports programs.
trauma. Journal of the International
Preseason baseline neuropsychological testing
Neuropsychological Society, 15, 1–8.
allows more accurate measurement of post-MTBI
Belanger, H. G., Spiegel, E., & Vanderploeg, R. D.
cognitive change. Per Bijur et al.’s (1996) research, (2010). Neuropsychological performance follow-
a control group of athletes suffering one or ing a history of multiple self-reported concussions:
more significant orthopedic injuries would be of A meta-analysis. Journal of the International
interest as a control for general injury (head Neuropsychological Society, 16, 262–67.
and nonhead) effects on neuropsychological test Belanger, H. G. & Vanderploeg, R. D. (2005). The neu-
performance. ropsychological impact of sports-related concus-
sion: A meta-analysis. Journal of the International
AC K N OW L E D G M E N T Neuropsychological Society, 11, 345–57.
I acknowledge the assistance of Matthew Miliano Bigler, E. D. (2001). The lesion(s) in traumatic brain
in the preparation of this chapter. injury: Implications for clinical neuropsychology.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 95–131.
REFERENCES Bigler, E. D. (2003). Neurobiology and neuropathology
Alexander, M. P. (1995). Mild traumatic brain injury: underlie the neuropsychological deficits associated
Pathophysiology, natural history, and clinical man- with traumatic brain injury. Archives of Clinical
agement. Neurology, 45, 1253–60. Neuropsychology, 18, 595–621.
Alterman, A. I., Goldstein, G., Shelly, C., & Bober, B. Bigler, E. D. (2008). Neuropsychology and clinical
(1985). The impact of mild head injury on neurop- neuroscience of persistent post-concussive syn-
sychological capacity in chronic alcoholics. Inter- drome. Journal of the International Neuro-
national Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 155–62. psychological Society, 14, 1–22.
American Academy of Neurology. (1997). Practice Bijur, P. E., Haslum, M., & Golding, J. (1996). Cognitive
parameter: The management of concussion in sports outcomes of multiple mild head injuries in chil-
(summary statement). Neurology, 48, 581–85. dren. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 17,
American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine. (1993). 143–48.
Definition of mild traumatic brain injury. Journal Binder, L. M. (1997). A review of mild head trauma.
of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 8, 86–87. Part II: Clinical implications. Journal of Clinical
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 432–57.
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Binder, L. M. & Kelly, M. P. (1996). Portland Digit
Washington, DC: Author. Recognition Test performance by brain dysfunc-
Anderson, V., Catroppa, C., Morse, S., Haritou, F., & tion patients without financial incentives.
Rosenfeld, J. (2001). Outcome from mild head Assessment, 3, 403–409.
injury in young children: A prospective study. Binder, L. M. & Rohling, M. L. (1996). Money matters:
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro- A meta-analytic review of the effects of financial
psychology, 23, 705–717. incentives on recovery after closed head injury.
Axelrod, B. N., Fox, D. D., Lees-Haley, P. R., Earnes, K., American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 5–8.
Dolezal-Wood, S., & Goldman, R. S. (1996). Latent Binder, L. M., Rohling, M. L., & Larrabee, G. J. (1997).
structure of the Postconcussion Syndrome Questi- A review of mild head trauma. Part I: Meta-analytic
onnaire. Psychological Assessment, 8, 422–27. review of neuropsychological studies. Journal of
Barkley, R. A. (Ed.) (2006). Attention-deficit hyperac- Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19,
tivity disorder. A handbook for diagnosis and treat- 421–31.
ment (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Boake, C., McCauley, S. R., Levin, H. S., Contant, C. F.,
Bazarian, J. J., McClung, J., Shah, M. N., Cheng, Y. T., Song, J. X., Brown, S. A., et al. (2004). Limited agree-
Flesher, W., & Kraus, J. (2005). Mild traumatic ment between criteria-based diagnoses of postcon-
brain injury in the United States, 1998–2000. Brain cussional syndrome. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
Injury, 19, 85–91. Clinical Neurosciences, 16, 493–99.
254 forensic neuropsychology

Borgaro, S. R., Prigatano, G. P., Kwasnica, C. A., & Colorado Medical Society. (1990). Report of the sports
Rexer, J. L. (2003). Cognitive and affective sequelae medicine committee. Guidelines for the management
in complicated and uncomplicated mild traumatic of concussion in sports. Denver, CO: Author.
brain injury. Brain Injury, 17, 189–98. Cullum, C. M. & Thompson, L. L. (1999). Evaluation of
Bornstein, R. A., Podraza, A. M., Para, M. F., Whitacre, neuropsychological status following mild trau-
C. C., Fass, R. J., Rice, R. R., et al. (1993). Effect of matic brain injury. In M. J. Raymond, T. L. Bennett,
minor head injury on neuropsychological perfor- L. C. Hartlage, & C. M. Cullum (Eds.), Mild
mance in asymptomatic HIV-1 infection. Neuro- traumatic brain injury (pp. 31–47). Austin, TX:
psychology, 7, 228–34. Pro-Ed.
Brenner, L. A., Terrio, H., Homaifar, B. Y., Gutierrez, P. Culotta, V. P., Sementilli, M. E., Gerold, K., & Watts,
M., Staves, P. J., Harwood, J. E. F., et al. (2010). C. C. (1996). Clinicopathological heterogeneity in
Neuropsychological test performance in soldiers the classification of mild head injury. Neurosurgery,
with blast-related Mild TBI. Neuropsychology, 24, 38, 245–50.
160–67. Delis, D. C. & Wetter, S. R. (2007). Cogniform disorder
Bruce, J. M. & Echemendia, R. J. (2009). History of and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for
multiple self-reported concussions is not associ- excessive cognitive symptoms. Archives of Clinical
ated with reduced cognitive abilities. Neurosurgery, Neuropsychology, 22, 683–87.
64, 100–106. Derogatis, L. R. & Spencer, P. M. (1982). The Brief
Brulot, M. M., Strauss, E., & Spellacy, F. (1997). Validity Symptom Inventory (BSI) administration, scoring
of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality and procedures manual-I. Baltimore, MD: Johns
Inventory-2 correction factors for use with patients Hopkins University School of Medicine.
with suspected head injury. The Clinical Neuro- Dikmen, S. S., Donovan, D. M., Loberg, T., Machamer,
psychologist, 11, 391–401. J. E., & Temkin, N. R. (1993). Alcohol use and its
Cantu, R. C. (1986). Guidelines for return to contact effects on neuropsychological outcome in head
sports after a cerebral concussion. Physical Sports injury. Neuropsychology, 7, 296–305.
Medicine, 14, 10. Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., & Temkin, N. R. (2001).
Carroll, L. J., Cassidy, J. D., Holm, L., Kraus, J., & Mild head injury: Facts and artifacts. Journal of
Coronado, V. G. (2004). Methodological issues and Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23,
research recommendations for mild traumatic 729–38.
brain injury: The WHO collaborating centre task Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., & Temkin, N. R. (2003,
force on mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of February). Mild traumatic brain injury with and
Rehabilitation Medicine, Suppl. 43, 113–25. without CT abnormalities. Paper presented at the
Carroll, L. J., Cassidy, J. D., Peloso, P. M., Borg, J., von annual meeting of the International
Holst, H., Holm, L., et al. (2004). Prognosis for Neuropsychological Society, Honolulu, HI.
mild traumatic brain injury: Results of the WHO Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., Winn, H. R., & Temkin,
collaborating centre task force on mild traumatic N. R. (1995). Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year
brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 80–90.
Suppl. 43, 84–105. Dikmen, S., McLean, A., & Temkin, N. (1986).
Cohen, R. A. (1993). The neuropsychology of attention. Neuropsychological and psychosocial conse-
New York: Plenum Press. quences of minor head injury. Journal of Neurology,
Cohen, R. M., Weingartner, H., Smallberg, S. A., Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 49, 1227–32.
Pickar, D., & Murphy, D. L. (1982). Effort and Dikmen, S. S., Temkin, N. R., Machamer, J. E.,
cognition in depression. Archives of General Holubkov, A. L., Fraser, R. T., & Winn, H. R. (1994).
Psychiatry, 39, 593–97. Employment following traumatic head injuries.
Collie, A., McCrory, P., & Makdissi, M. (2006). Does Archives of Neurology, 51, 177–86.
history of concussion affect current cognitive status? Echemendia, R. J., Putukian, M., Mackin, R. S., Julian,
British Journal of Sports Medicine, 40, 550–51. L., & Shoss, N. (2001). Neuropsychological test
Collins, M. W., Grindel, S. H., Lovell, M. R., Dede, D. performance prior to and following sports-related
E., Moser, D. J., Phalin, B. R., et al. (1999). mild traumatic brain injury. Clinical Journal of
Relationship between concussion and neuropsy- Sports Medicine, 11, 23–31.
chological performance in college football players. Ettenhofer, M. L. & Abels, N. (2009). The significance
Journal of the American Medical Association, 282, of mild traumatic brain injury to cognition and
964–70. self-reported symptoms in long-term recovery
Collins, M. W., Lovell, M. R., Iverson, G. L., Cantu, R. from injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
C., Maroon, J. C., & Field, M. (2002). Cumulative Neuropsychology, 31, 363–72.
effects of concussion in high school athletes. Ewing, R., McCarthy, C., Gronwall, D., & Wrightson, P.
Neurosurgery, 51, 1175–81. (1980). Persisting effects of minor head injury
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 255

observable during hypoxic stress. Journal of Clinical Green, P., Allen, III, L. M., & Astner, K. (1996). The
Neuropsychology, 2, 147–55. Word Memory Test. Durham, NC: CogniSyst, Inc.
Feher, E. P., Larrabee, G. J., Sudilovsky, A., & Crook, T. Green, P., Iverson, G. L., & Allen, L. (1999). Detecting
H. (1994). Memory self-report in Alzheimer’s dis- malingering in head injury litigation with the Word
ease and in age-associated memory impairment. Memory Test. Brain Injury, 13, 813–19.
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 7, Green, P., Rohling, M. L., Lees-Haley, P. R., & Allen, L.
58–65. M. (2001). Effort has a greater effect on test scores
Feinstein, A., Ouchterlony, D., Somerville, J., & Jardine, than severe brain injury in compensation claim-
A. (2001). The effects of litigation on symptom ants. Brain Injury, 15, 1045–60.
expression: A prospective study following mild Greiffenstein, M. F. (2009). Clinical myths of forensic
traumatic brain injury. Medical Science and the neuropsychology. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
Law, 41, 116–21. 23, 286–96.
Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. Gronwall, D. (1989). Cumulative and persisting effects
A. (2007). Learning disabilities. From identification of concussion on attention and cognition. In H. S.
to intervention. New York: Guilford. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, & A. L. Benton (Eds.),
Fox, D. D. & Allen, L. M. (2003). Discussion. Rebuttal Mild head injury (pp. 153–62). New York: Oxford
to Bigler’s response to our commentary on “The University Press.
lesion(s) in traumatic brain injury”: Let’s stick to Gronwall, D. & Wrightson, P. (1975). Cumulative effect
science. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18, of concussion. Lancet, 2, 995–97.
623–24. Guilmette, T. J. & Rasile, D. (1995). Sensitivity, speci-
Frencham, K. A. R., Fox, A. M., & Maybery, M. T. ficity, and diagnostic accuracy of three verbal
(2005). Neuropsychological studies of mild trau- memory measures in the assessment of mild brain
matic brain injury: A meta-analytic review of injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 338–44.
research since 1995. Journal of Clinical and Experi- Gunstad, J. & Suhr, J. A. (2001). “Expectation as etiol-
mental Neuropsychology, 27, 334–51. ogy” versus “the good old days”: Postconcussion
Garden, N. & Sullivan, K. A. (2010). An examination syndrome symptom reporting in athletes, head-
of the base rates of post-concussion symptoms: ache sufferers, and depressed individuals. Journal
The influence of demographics and depression. of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7,
Applied Neuropsychology, 17, 1–7. 323–33.
Gasquoine, P. G. (1997). Post-concussion symptoms. Gunstad, J. & Suhr, J. A. (2004). Cognitive factors
Neuropsychology Review, 7, 77–85. in postconcussion syndrome symptom report.
Gervais, R. O., Ben-Porath, Y. S., Wygant, D. B., & Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 391–405.
Green, P. (2008). Differential sensitivity of the Guskiewicz, K. M., McCrea, M., Marshall, S. W., Cantu,
Response Bias Scale (RBS) and MMPI-2 validity R. C., Randolph, C., Barr, W., et al. (2003).
scales to memory complaints. The Clinical Neuro- Cumulative effects associated with recurrent con-
psychologist, 22, 1061–79. cussion in collegiate football players. The NCAA
Giza, C. C. & Hovda, D. A. (2001). The neurometabolic concussion study. Journal of the American Medical
cascade of concussion. Journal of Athletic Training, Association, 290, 2549–55.
36, 228–35. Guskiewicz, K. M., Marshall, S. W., Bailes, J.,
Goldstein, F. C., & Levin, H. S. (2001). Cognitive out- McCrea, M., Cantu, R. C., Randolph, C., et al.
come after mild and moderate traumatic brain (2005). Association between recurrent concussion
injury in older adults. Journal of Clinical and and late-life cognitive impairment in retired
Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 739–53. professional football players. Neurosurgery, 57,
Goldstein, F. C., Levin, H. S., Goldman, W. P., Clark, 719–26.
B. A., & Altonen, T. K. (2001). Cognitive and Hanninen, T., Reinikainen, K. J., Helkala, E.-L.,
neurobehavioral function after mild versus moder- Koivisto, K., Mykkanen, L., Laakso, M., et al.
ate traumatic brain injury in older adults. Journal (1994). Subjective memory complaints and per-
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, sonality traits in normal elderly subjects. Journal of
373–83. the American Geriatric Society, 42, 1–4.
Gouvier, W. D., Cubic, B., Jones, G., Brantley, P., & Hanlon, R. E., Demery, J. A., Martinovich, Z., & Kelly,
Cutlip, Q. (1992). Postconcussion symptoms and J. P. (1999). Effects of acute injury characteristics
daily stress in normal and head-injured college on neuropsychological status and vocational out-
populations. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, come following mild traumatic brain injury. Brain
7, 193–211. Injury, 13, 873–87.
Green, P. (2003). Discussion. Welcoming a paradigm Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1991).
shift in neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-
Neuropsychology, 18, 625–27. Reitan battery. Demographic corrections, research
256 forensic neuropsychology

findings, and clinical applications. Odessa, FL: Postconcussion Symptoms Questionnaire. Archives
Psychological Assessment Resources. of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 91, 35–42.
High, W. M., Levin, H. S., & Gary, H. E. (1990). Kurca, E., Sivak, S., & Kucera, P. (2006). Impaired cog-
Recovery of orientation following closed head nitive functions in mild traumatic brain injury
injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental patients with normal and pathologic magnetic
Neuropsychology, 12, 703–14. resonance imaging. Neuroradiology, 48, 661–69.
Hinton-Bayre, A. D. & Geffen, G. (2002). Severity of Lange, R. T., Iverson, G. L., Brooks, B. L., & Rennison,
sports-related concussion and neuropsychological V. L. A. (2010). Influence of poor effort on self-
test performance. Neurology, 59, 1068–70. reported symptoms and neurocognitive test per-
Hughes, D. G., Jackson, A., Mason, D. L., Berry, E., formance following mild traumatic brain injury.
Hollis, S., & Yates, D. W. (2004). Abnormalities on The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 32, 961–72.
magnetic resonance imaging seen acutely follow- Lange, R. T., Iverson, G. L., & Franzen, M. D. (2009).
ing mild traumatic brain injury: Correlation with Neuropsychological functioning following compli-
neuropsychological tests and delayed recovery. cated vs. uncomplicated mild traumatic brain
Neuroradiology, 46: 550–58. injury. Brain Injury, 23, 83–91.
Institute of Medicine of the National Academies Larrabee, G. J. (1990). Cautions in the use of neuropsy-
(2008). Gulf War and Health. Vol. 7. Long-term chological evaluation in legal settings.
consequences of traumatic brain injury. Washington, Neuropsychology, 4, 239–47.
D. C.: The National Academies Press. Larrabee, G. J. (1997). Neuropsychological outcome,
Iverson, G. L. (2005). Outcome from mild traumatic postconcussion symptoms, and forensic consider-
brain injury. Current Opinions in Psychiatry, 18, ations in mild closed head trauma. Seminars in
301–317. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 2, 196–206.
Iverson, G. L. (2006). Complicated vs. uncomplicated Larrabee, G. J. (2003). Detection of malingering using
mild traumatic brain injury: Acute neuropsycho- atypical performance patterns on standard neu-
logical outcome. Brain Injury, 20, 1335–44. ropsychological tests. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
Iverson, G. L., Brooks, B. L., Lovell, M. R., & Collins, 17, 410–25.
M. W. (2005). No cumulative effects for one or two Larrabee, G. J. (2007). Commentary on Delis and
previous concussions. British Journal of Sports Wetter, “Cogniform disorder and cogniform con-
Medicine, 40, 72–75. dition: Proposed diagnoses for excessive cognitive
Iverson, G. L., Lange, R. T., Brooks, B. L., & Rennison, symptoms.” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
V. L. A. (2010). “Good old days” bias following 22, 683–87.
mild traumatic brain injury. The Clinical Neuro- Larrabee, G. J. & Levin, H. S. (1986). Memory self-
psychologist, 24, 17–37. ratings and objective test performance in a normal
Iverson, G. L. & McCracken, L. M. (1997). elderly sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
“Postconcussive” symptoms in persons with Neuropsychology, 8, 275–84.
chronic pain. Brain Injury, 11, 783–90. Larrabee, G. J., Millis, S. R., & Meyers, J. E. (2009).
Ivins, B. J., Kane, R., & Schwab, K. A. (2009). 40 plus or minus 10, a new magical number: Reply
Performance on the Automated Neuropsycholo- to Russell. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22,
gical Assessment Metrics in a nonclinical sample 1061–79.
of soldiers screened for mild TBI after returning Larrabee, G. J., West, R. L., & Crook, T. H. (1991).
from Iraq and Afghanistan: A descriptive The association of memory complaint with com-
analysis. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, puter-simulated everyday memory performance.
24, 24–31. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro-
Kashluba, S., Hanks, R. A., Casey, J. E., & Millis, S. R. psychology, 13, 466–78.
(2008). Neuropsychologic and functional outcome Lees-Haley, P. R. & Brown, R. S. (1993).
after complicated mild traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychological complaint base rates of 170
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, personal injury claimants. Archives of Clinical
89, 904–11. Neuropsychology, 8, 203–209.
Kashluba, S., Paniak, C., & Casey, J. E. (2008). Persistent Lees-Haley, P. R., Fox, D. D., & Courtney, J. C. (2001).
symptoms associated with factors identified by the A comparison of complaints by mild brain injury
WHO task force on mild traumatic brain injury. claimants and other claimants describing subjective
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22, 195–208. experiences immediately following their injury.
Kirsch, N. L., de Leon, M. B., Maio, R. F., Millis, S. R., Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 689–95.
Tan-Scrhiner, C. U., & Frederiksen, S. (2010). Lees-Haley, P. R., Green, P., Rohling, M. L., Fox, D. D.,
Characteristics of a mild head injury subgroup & Allen, L. M. (2003). Commentary. The lesion(s)
with extreme, persisting distress on the Rivermead in traumatic brain injury: Implications for clinical
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 257

neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical Neuro- McCrea, M. A. (2008). Mild traumatic brain injury and
psychology, 18, 585–94. post-concussion syndrome. The new evidence base
Leininger, B. E., Gramling, S. E., Farrell, A. D., Kreutzer, for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Oxford
J. S., & Peck, E. A. (1990). Neuropsychological University Press.
deficits in symptomatic minor head injury patients McCrea, M., Guskiewicz, K. M., Marshall, S. W., Barr,
after concussion and mild concussion. Journal W., Randolph, C., Cantu, R. C., et al. (2003). Acute
of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 53, effects and recovery time following concussion in
293–96. collegiate football players. The NCAA concussion
Levin, H. S., Amparo, E., Eisenberg, H. M., Williams, D. study. Journal of the American Medical Association,
H., High, W. M., McCardle, C., et al. (1987). 290, 2556–63.
Magnetic resonance imaging and computerized McCrea, M., Iverson, G. L., McAllister, T. W., Hammeke,
neurobehavioral sequelae of mild and moderate T. A., Powell, M. R., Barr, W. B., et al. (2009).
head injuries. Journal of Neurosurgery, 66, 706–713. An integrated review of recovery after mild trau-
Levin, H. S., Brown, S. A., Song, J. X., McCauley, S. R., matic brain injury (MTBI): Implications for clini-
Boake, C., Contant, C. F., et al. (2001). Depression cal management. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
and posttraumatic stress disorder at 3 months after 23, 1368–90.
mild to moderate traumatic brain injury. Journal of McCrea, M., Kelly, J. P., Randolph, C., Cisler, R., &
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, Berger, L. (2002). Immediate neurocognitive effects
754–69. of concussion. Neurosurgery, 50, 1032–42.
Levin, H. S., Mattis, S., Ruff, R. M., Eisenberg, H. M., McCrea, M., Prichep, L., Powell, M. R., Chabot, R., &
Marshall, L. F., Tabaddor, K., et al. (1987). Barr, W. B. (2010). Acute effects and recovery after
Neurobehavioral outcome following minor head sport-related concussion: A neurocognitive and
injury: A three-center study. Journal of Neuro- quantitative brain electrical activity study. Journal
surgery, 66, 234–43. of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 25, 283–92.
Levin, H. S., Williams, D. H., Eisenberg, H. M., High, McLean, A., Temkin, N. R., Dikmen, S., & Wyler, A. R.
W. M., & Guinto, F. C. (1992). Serial MRI and neu- (1983). The behavioral sequelae of head injury.
robehavioral findings after mild to moderate closed Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 361–76.
head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, Meyers, J. E. & Rohling, M. L. (2004). Validation of the
and Psychiatry, 55, 255–62. Meyers Short Battery on mild TBI patients. Archives
Lishman, W. A. (1988). Physiogenesis and psychogen- of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 637–51.
esis in the “post-concussional syndrome.” British Mittenberg, W., DiGiulio, D. V., Perrin, S., & Bass, A. E.
Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 460–69. (1992). Symptoms following mild head injury:
Lovell, M. R., Iverson, G. L., Collins, M. W., McKeag, Expectation as aetiology. Journal of Neurology,
D., & Maroon, J. C. (1999). Does loss of conscious- Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 55, 200–204.
ness predict neuropsychological decrements after Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit,
concussion? Clinical Journal of Sports Medicine, 9, D. C. (2002). Base rates of malingering and symp-
193–98. tom exaggeration. Journal of Clinical and
Luis, C. A., Vanderploeg, R. D., & Curtiss, G. (2003). Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 1094–1102.
Predictors of post-concussion symptom complex Mittenberg, W. & Strauman, S. (2000). Diagnosis of
in community-dwelling male veterans. Journal of mild head injury and the post-concussion syn-
the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, drome. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 15,
1001–15. 783–91.
Macciocchi, S. N., Barth, J. T., Alves, W., Rimel, R. W., Mittenberg, W., Tremont, G., Zielinski, R. F., Fichera,
& Jane, J. A. (1996). Neuropsychological function- S., & Rayls, K. R. (1996). Cognitive behavioral pre-
ing and recovery after mild head injury in colle- vention of post-concussion syndrome. Archives of
giate athletes. Neurosurgery, 39, 510–14. Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 139–45.
Mayer, A. R., Ling, J., Mannell, M. V., Gasparovic, C., Mittenberg. W., Zielinski, R., & Fichera, R. (1993).
Phillips, J. P., Doezema, D., et al. (2010). A pro- Recovery from mild head injury: A treatment
spective diffusion tensor imaging study in mild manual for patients. Psychotherapy in Private
traumatic brain injury. Neurology, 74, 643–50. Practice, 12, 37–52.
McCauley, S. R., Boake, C., Pedroza, C., Brown, S. A., Montserrat, C. M., Rodriguez-Holquin, S., & Cadaveira,
Levin, H. S., Goodman, H. S., et al. (2008). F. (1999). Neuropsychological characteristics in
Correlates of persistent postconcussional children of alcoholics: Familial density. Journal of
disorder: DSM-IV criteria versus ICD-10. Journal Studies on Alcohol, 60, 509–13.
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, Moser, R. S., Iverson, G. L., Echemendia, R. J., Lovell,
360–79. M. R., Schatz, P., Webbe, F. M., et al. (2007).
258 forensic neuropsychology

Neuropsychological evaluation in the diagnosis Ruff, R. M., Camenzuli, L., & Mueller, J. (1996).
and management of sports-related concussion. Miserable minority: Emotional risk factors that
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 909–16. influence the outcome of a mild traumatic brain
National Center for Injury Prevention & Control injury. Brain Injury, 10, 551–65.
(2003). Report to Congress on mild traumatic Ruff, R. M., Iverson, G. L., Barth, J. T., Bush, S. S.,
brain injury in the United States: Steps to prevent a Broshek, D. K., the NAN Policy and Planning
serious public health problem. Atlanta, GA: Centers Committee (2009). Recommendations for diag-
for Disease Control and Prevention. nosing a mild traumatic brain injury: A National
Newcombe, F., Rabbitt, P., & Briggs, M. (1994). Minor Academy of Neuropsychology education paper.
head injury: Pathophysiological or iatrogenic Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 3–10.
sequelae? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Ruff, R. M. & Jamora, C. W. (2009). Myths and mild
Psychiatry, 57, 709–16. traumatic brain injury. Psychological Injury and the
Panayiotou, A., Jackson, M., & Crowe, S. F. (2010). Law, 2, 34–42.
A meta-analytic review of the emotional symptoms Ruff, R. M., Levin, H. S., Mattis, S., High, W. M.,
associated with mild traumatic brain injury. Journal Marshall, L. F., Eisenberg, H. M., et al. (1989).
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32, Recovery of memory after mild head injury: A
463–73. three-center study. In H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg,
Parsons, O. A. (1987). Neuropsychological conse- & A. L. Benton (Eds.), Mild head injury (pp. 176–
quences of alcohol abuse: Many questions—some 88). New York: Oxford University Press.
answers. In O. A. Parsons, N. Butters, & P. E. Ruff, R. M. & Richardson, A. M. (1999). Mild traumatic
Nathan (Eds.), Neuropsychology of alcoholism: brain injury. In J. J. Sweet (Ed.), Forensic neuropsy-
Implications for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 153– chology. Fundamentals and practice (pp. 313–38).
75). New York: Guilford. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger.
Parsons, O. A., Butters, N., & Nathan, P. E. (Eds.). (1987). Rutgers, D. R., Fillard, P., Paradot, G., Tadie, M.,
Neuropsychology of alcoholism: Implications for diag- Lasjaunias, P. K., & Ducreux, D. (2008). Diffusion
nosis and treatment. New York: Guilford Press. tensor imaging characteristics of the corpus callosum
Pertab, J. L., James, K. M., & Bigler, E. D. (2009). in mild, moderate, and severe traumatic brain injury.
Limitations of mild traumatic brain injury meta- American Journal of Neuroradiology, 29, 1730–35.
analyses. Brain Injury, 23, 498–508. Rutherford, W. H. (1989). Postconcussion symptoms:
Prigatano, G. P. (1996). Behavioral limitations TBI Relationship to acute neurological indices, indi-
patients tend to underestimate: A replication vidual differences, and circumstances of injury. In
and extension to patients with lateralized cerebral H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg, & A. L. Benton (Eds.).
dysfunction. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, Mild head injury (pp. 217–28). New York: Oxford
191–201. University Press.
Prigatano, G. P. & Altman, I. M. (1990). Impaired Rutherford, W. H., Merrett, J., & McDonald, J. (1979).
awareness of behavioral limitations after traumatic Symptoms at one year following concussion from
brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and minor head injuries. Injury, 10, 225–30.
Rehabilitation, 71, 1058–64. Sadowski-Cron, C., Schneider, J., Senn, P., Radanov, B.
Putnam, S. H. & Millis, S. R. (1994). Psychosocial fac- P., Ballinari, P., & Zimmerman, H. (2006). Patients
tors in the development and maintenance of with mild traumatic brain injury: Immediate and
chronic somatic and functional symptoms follow- long-term outcome compared to intra-cranial
ing mild traumatic brain injury. Advances in injuries on CT scan. Brain Injury, 20, 1131–37.
Medical Psychotherapy, 7, 1–22. Satz, P. (1993). Brain reserve capacity on symptom
Randolph, C., McCrea, M., & Barr, W. B. (2005). Is onset after brain injury. A formulation and review
neuropsychological testing useful in the manage- of evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology,
ment of sport-related concussion? Journal of 7, 273–95.
Athletic Training, 40, 139–54. Satz, P., Alfano, M. S., Light, R., Morgenstern, H.,
Rohling, M. L., Meyers, J. E., & Millis, S. R. (2003). Zaucha, K., Asarnow, R. F., et al. (1999). Persistent
Neuropsychological impairment following trau- post-concussive syndrome: A proposed methodol-
matic brain injury: A dose–response analysis. The ogy and literature review to determine the effects,
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 17, 289–302. if any, of mild head and other bodily injury. Journal
Rourke, B P. (Ed.). (1991). Neuropsychological valida- of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21,
tion of learning disability subtypes. New York: 620–28.
Guilford Press. Satz, P., Zaucha, K., McCleary, C., Light, R., Asarnow,
Rourke, B. P. (Ed.). (1995). Syndrome of nonverbal R., & Becker, D. (1997). Mild head injury in
learning disabilities. Neurodevelopmental manifes- children and adolescents: A review of studies
tations. New York: Guilford Press. (1970–1995). Psychological Bulletin, 122, 107–31.
Mild Traumatic Brain Injury 259

Schretlen, D. J. & Shapiro, A. M. (2003). A quantitative brain injury. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
review of the effects of traumatic brain injury 16, 435–45.
on cognitive functioning. International Review of Uomoto, J. M. & Esselman, P. C. (1993). Traumatic
Psychiatry, 15, 341–49. brain injury and chronic pain: Differential types
Spreen, O., Risser, A. T., & Edgell, D. (1995). and rates by head injury severity. Archives of
Developmental neuropsychology. New York: Oxford Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 74, 61–64.
University Press. Vanderploeg, R. D., Curtiss, G., & Belanger, H. G.
Stulemeijer, M., Vos, P. E., Bleijenberg, G., & van der (2005). Long-term neuropsychological outcomes
Werf, S. P. (2007). Cognitive complaints after mild following mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of
traumatic brain injury: Things are not always what the International Neuropsychological Society, 11,
they seem. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 63, 228–36.
637–45. Watson, D. & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health com-
Suhr, J. A. & Gunstad, J. (2002). “Diagnosis threat”: The plaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central
effect of negative expectations on cognitive perfor- role of negative affectivity. Psychological Review,
mance in head injury. Journal of Clinical and 96, 234–54.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 448–57. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–
Suhr, J. A. & Gunstad, J. (2005). Further exploration of Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
the effect of “diagnosis threat” on cognitive perfor- Corporation.
mance in individuals with mild head injury. Journal Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised.
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
23–29. Williams, D. H., Levin, H. S., & Eisenberg, H. M.
Sweeney, J. E. (1992). Nonimpact brain injury: Grounds (1990). Mild head injury classification.
for clinical study of the neuropsychological effects Neurosurgery, 27, 422–28.
of acceleration forces. The Clinical Neuropsycho- Williams, J. M., Little, M. M., Scates, S., & Blockman,
logist, 6, 443–57. N. (1987). Memory complaints and abilities among
Symonds, C. P. (1937). Mental disorder following head depressed older adults. Journal of Consulting and
injury. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, Clinical Psychology, 55, 595–98.
30, 1081–92. Wilson, J. T., Teasdale, G. M., Hadley, D. M., Wiedman,
Taylor, A. E., Cox, C. A., & Mailis, A. (1996). Persistent K. D., & Lang, D. (1994). Post-traumatic amnesia:
neuropsychological deficits following whiplash: Still a valuable yardstick. Journal of Neurology,
Evidence for chronic mild traumatic brain injury? Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 198–201.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Yeates, K. O., Ris, M. D., Taylor, H. G., & Pennington,
77, 529–35. B. F. (Eds.) (2010). Pediatric neuropsychology.
Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). The Test of Memory Research, theory, and practice (2nd ed.). New York:
Malingering. North Tonowanda, NY: Multi-Health Guilford.
Systems. Zasler, N. (2000). Medical aspects. In S. A. Raskin & C.
Trahan, D. E., Ross, C. E., & Trahan, S. L. (2001). A. Mateer (Eds.), Neuropsychological management
Relationships among post-concussional-type of mild traumatic brain injury (pp. 23–38). New
symptoms, depression, and anxiety in neurologi- York: Oxford University Press.
cally normal young adults and victims of mild
10
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury
TRESA ROEBUCK-SPENCER AND MARK SHERER

Mortality and morbidity due to trauma are major (Binder, 1997; Binder & Willis, 1991). As a group,
public health problems in the United States. Data patients with moderate and severe TBI have
from the National Center for Health Statistics poorer outcome both in terms of mortality and
(Kung, Hoyert, Xu, & Murphy, 2008) indicate that morbidity than patients with mild TBI (Dikmen,
accidents are the fifth leading cause of death for all Machamer, Winn, & Temkin, 1995; Dikmen
Americans with motor vehicle accidents and falls et al., 1994; Levin, 1993; Levin, 1995; Zhang, Jiang,
accounting for a large proportion of these deaths. Zhong, Yu, & Zhu, 2001) and may require
The most recent Centers for Disease Control longer-term outpatient evaluation and treatment
(CDC) data indicate that there are over 1.5 million by neuropsychologists to address these issues. In
new cases of traumatic brain injury (TBI) a year, contrast, the long-term effects of mild TBI remain
and that as a result of TBI, 1,224,000 Americans controversial. Most consecutive series of trauma
were treated in hospital emergency departments, center patients with uncomplicated mild TBI
290,000 were hospitalized, and 51,000 died show that cognitive symptoms and deficits are
(Rutland-Brown, Langlois, Thomas, & Xi, 2006). resolved by 3 months post-injury (Dikmen,
It is estimated that 80,000 to 90,000 persons have McLean, & Temkin, 1986; Institute of Medicine,
new onset of disability each year due to TBI 2009; Levin et al., 1987; Williams, Levin, &
(Thurman, Alverson, Dunn, Guerrero, & Sniezek, Eisenberg, 1990). However, other investigators
1999), and that at least 5.3 million Americans live have found significant self-report of neurobehav-
with disability due to TBI (Langlois, Rutland- ioral symptoms in samples of patients with mild
Brown, & Wald, 2006). When considering medi- TBI who are referred for services in the post-acute
cal costs and lost productivity, the lifetime cost of period (Alexander, 1992; Cicerone & Kalmar,
TBI in the United States is an estimated 60 billion 1995). These persistent complaints have been
dollars annually (Finkelstein, Corso, & Miller, referred to as post-concussion syndrome, which
2006). This estimate does not take into account is viewed by some as maintained by psychological
the emotional cost of TBI or the indirect impact rather than neurological factors (McCrea, 2008;
on families, caregivers, and the community. Mittenberg & Strauman, 2000). Recent reviews
Given this high incidence and the likelihood confirm that incomplete recovery from mild TBI
of persistent impairments, neuropsychologists is often associated with preexisting psychiatric or
often play an important role in the evaluation and substance abuse problems, poor general health,
treatment of persons with TBI. Patients with concurrent orthopedic injuries, or comorbid
moderate and severe TBI are much more likely problems such as chronic pain, life stress, unem-
than patients with mild TBI to require inpatient ployment, and protracted litigation (Iverson,
(Harrison-Felix, Newton, Hall, & Kreutzer, 1996) 2005; McCrea, 2008).
or post-acute rehabilitation (Malec & Moessner, Forensic issues in mild TBI are addressed in
2000; Sherer, Bergloff, High, & Nick, 1999) and chapter 9 by Larrabee. The present chapter will
are often first seen by neuropsychologists in focus on moderate to severe TBI and will review:
these settings. Patients with mild TBI may be (1) classification of TBI; (2) incidence of moder-
more likely to be seen in forensic or other com- ate and severe TBI; (3) course of recovery after
pensation-related settings where determination of moderate and severe TBI; (4) neuropsychological
whether the patient has any brain impairment or assessment of patients with moderate and severe
persisting sequelae is the focus of the evaluation TBI; (5) outcome after moderate and severe TBI
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 261

including mortality as well as physical, cognitive, Return to a conscious or responsive state


neurobehavioral, and functional status outcomes; after TBI is indicated by ability to follow simple
and (6) prediction of outcome after TBI. commands, indicate yes/no reliably through
words or gestures, give intelligible verbalizations,
C L A S S I F I C AT I O N O F or other purposeful behaviors (Giacino, et al.,
TBI SEVERITY 2002). Most studies of recovery from TBI have
The CDC define TBI as “an occurrence of injury to used ability to follow commands as the primary
the head that is documented in a medical record indication of return to a conscious state, but some
with one of the following conditions attributed to have used purposeful withdrawal from a painful
head injury: 1) observed or self-reported decreased stimulus (Levin, 1995; Whyte, Cifu, Dikmen, &
level of consciousness, 2) amnesia, 3) skull fracture, Temkin, 2001). Whyte and colleagues (2001)
or 4) objective neurological or neuropsychological found that ability to follow commands was the
abnormality or diagnosed intracranial lesion” more useful of the two indices. The interval from
(Marr & Coronado, 2004). Notably, some authors onset of injury to recovery of ability to follow
do not find that skull fracture is always associated commands has proven to be a useful index in
with brain impairment (Williams et al., 1990). TBI determining injury severity. While “time to follow
severity is generally determined based on the depth commands” is the most accurate term for this
of impaired consciousness or duration of impaired interval, some authors use the term “duration of
consciousness. The most commonly used measure unconsciousness.” This term may be inaccurate,
of depth of impaired consciousness is the Glasgow as some aphasic patients may clearly be “con-
Coma Scale (GCS) (Teasdale & Jennett, 1974). The scious,” but fail to demonstrate command follow-
GCS measures levels of responsiveness in eye open- ing (Levin, 1995). Poorer outcomes are associated
ing, motor movement, and verbal communication. with longer intervals from time of injury to time
Scores range from 3 to 15 with higher scores indi- of recovery of ability to follow commands. Time
cating more intact functioning. Patients with scores to follow commands has been shown to be predic-
of 7 and below are definitely in coma and over 50% tive of global outcome, neuropsychological func-
of those with scores of 8 are in coma (Jennett & tioning, personal independence, and employment
Teasdale, 1977). GCS scores used for classification outcome after TBI (Dikmen & Machamer, 1995;
of TBI severity should be obtained immediately Dikmen, McLean, Temkin, & Wyler, 1986;
post-resuscitation or at admission to the emer- Dikmen et al., 1994). The usefulness of this index
gency department. Patients with post-resuscitation of TBI severity is limited by the lack of a com-
GCS scores of 3 to 8 are classified as having had monly agreed upon classification scheme. Also, it
severe TBI and those with scores from 9 to 12 are may not be possible to determine time to follow
classified as having had moderate injuries (Clifton, command intervals that map neatly onto severity
Hayes, Levin, Michel, & Choi, 1992; Hannay & categories as determined by GCS ratings. While
Sherer, 1996; Levin & Eisenberg, 1991). Some duration of time to follow commands shows
researchers further divide the severe group into very the expected dose-response relationship to GCS
severe (GCS 3 to 5) and severe (GCS 6 to 8) (Zhang, severity categories with more severe injuries
et al., 2001). Patients with GCS scores between 13 having longer time to follow commands intervals,
and 15 are classified as having had mild injuries, but there is substantial overlap in time to follow com-
outcomes of these patients depend on the presence mands durations among extremely severe, severe,
or absence of depressed skull fracture or intracra- moderate, and mild GCS categories (Sherer,
nial pathology as shown on the initial CT scan. Struchen, Yablon, Wang, & Nick, 2008).
Patients with GCS scores between 13 and 15 with no Another index of TBI severity is duration of
depressed skull fracture or pathology on initial CT post-traumatic amnesia (PTA). PTA refers to the
scan have uncomplicated mild TBI and have very phase of recovery from TBI during which the
favorable outcomes (Dikmen, Machamer, & patient is responsive, but is acutely confused and
Temkin, 2003; Williams et al., 1990). Patients with disoriented and is unable to form and retain new
initial GCS scores between 13 and 15 who do have memories (Russell, 1932; Symonds, 1937). While
depressed skull fractures or CT scan abnormalities virtually all authors define PTA duration as the
have complicated mild TBI and their outcomes are interval from injury to recovery of orientation, a
more similar to patients with moderate TBI few (e.g., Ellenberg, Levin, & Saydjari, 1996; Sherer,
(Dikmen, Machamer, & Temkin, 2003; Williams Hart, Whyte, Nick, & Yablon, 2005) define PTA
et al., 1990). duration as the interval from recovery of ability
262 forensic neuropsychology

to follow commands to recovery of orientation. concussion, patients with PTA of one to seven
While disorientation and memory disturbance days are classified as having had severe concus-
are hallmarks of this phase of recovery, recent sion, and patients with PTA greater than seven
researchers have noted the similarity of this state days are classified as having had very severe con-
to delirium and have recommended use of the cussion. Unfortunately, TBI severity classification
term “post-traumatic confusional state” (Sherer, based on the Russell and Smith schema is mark-
Nakase-Thompson, Yablon, & Gontkovsky, 2005; edly different than classification based on initial
Sherer, Yablon, Nakase-Richardson, & Nick, 2008; GCS scores. As with time to follow commands,
Stuss et al., 1999). Numerous studies have shown there is a clear dose-response effect for PTA dura-
that duration of PTA is predictive of various tion between GCS severity categories, but there is
aspects of outcome after TBI including neuro- marked overlap in PTA duration among GCS
psychological outcome, independent living status, severity categories. Thus, use of the Russell and
and return to work (Dikmen et al., 1994; Ellenberg Smith criteria will result in misclassification of
et al., 1996; Sherer, Sander, et al., 2002). many persons with mild or moderate injuries
Duration of PTA can be assessed retrospec- based on initial GCS scores as severe (Sherer,
tively by waiting until the patient is no longer con- Struchen, et al., 2008). Thus, clinicians should be
fused and asking him/her to report the first wary of classifying patient injury severity based
memory that he/she can recall following the brain on PTA duration. It is strongly recommended that
injury (Symonds & Russell, 1943). Additionally, investigators not determine injury severity for
based on review of medical records, some some subjects based on GCS scores and for others
researchers have used documentation of consis- based on PTA duration. This practice will likely
tent orientation in all spheres as evidence that an result in misleading findings.
individual has cleared PTA (Traumatic Brain There are only a few investigations comparing
Injury Model Systems National Data Center, PTA duration and time to follow commands as
1999). This practice is consistent with research indices of TBI severity. Dikmen and colleagues
showing that orientation improves in a predict- (1986) found that time to follow commands and
able manner with orientation to time being a GCS were more predictive of cognitive status at 1
potential marker for clearing PTA (High, Levin, & month post-TBI than was PTA duration. However,
Gary, 1990). More commonly, duration of PTA is Katz and Alexander (1994) found that PTA dura-
determined prospectively by serial assessment of tion accounted for significantly more of the vari-
the patient’s degree of disorientation. The most ability in Glasgow Outcome Scale scores at 6
commonly used measure for this purpose is the months and at 12 months post-injury than did
Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Scale time to follow commands. In multivariable regres-
(GOAT) (Levin, O’Donnell, & Grossman, 1979). sion models predicting outcome at 6 and 12
Other similar scales include the Orientation Log months, PTA duration entered the model while
(Jackson, Novack, & Dowler, 1998), the Oxford time to follow commands did not. Based on these
Scale (Fortuny, Briggs, Newcombe, Ratcliff, & limited data, it is not possible to determine
Thomas, 1980), and the Westmead Scale (Shores, whether PTA duration or time to follow com-
Marosszeky, Sandanam, & Batchelor, 1986). mands is the better predictor of outcome after
Although the reliability of retrospective assess- TBI. This likely depends on the time frame of the
ment of PTA duration has been criticized, esti- prediction and the outcome being predicted.
mates of PTA duration have been shown to be
similar across these two assessment methods I N C I D E N C E O F M O D E R AT E
(McMillan, Jongen, & Greenwood, 1996). AND SEVERE TBI
As with time to follow commands, the useful- The overall incidence rate of TBI is difficult to
ness of duration of PTA as an index of TBI sever- determine. This is because many individuals with
ity is limited by the lack of commonly agreed mild TBI may not seek medical care and if they do
upon criteria for intervals indicating severe, mod- seek care they may not be admitted to a hospital.
erate, and mild injuries. Perhaps the most com- A CDC study covering the year 2003 estimated
monly used criteria were developed by Russell the total incidence of TBI at 1.6 million and that
and Smith (1961). In this scheme, patients with 1,224,000 persons sought medical care following
PTA less than one hour are classified as having TBI (Rutland-Brown, et al., 2006). TBI rates were
had slight concussion, patients with PTA of one to the highest among young children (ages 0–4) and
24 hours are classified as having had moderate then peaked again in adolescence. Visits to the
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 263

emergency department were highest among have coma (Ommaya & Gennarelli, 1974). Coma
children between the ages of 0 and 14, and hospi- is a temporary nonresponsive state in which the
talization and death rates were the highest among patient has closed eyes, follows no instructions,
older adults (age 65 and older). Consistent with gives no communication, and shows no purpose-
past data, rates for men were higher than rates for ful movements (Teasdale & Jennett, 1974). Recent
women, with approximately 1.5 times as many large consecutive series of patients admitted to
TBIs in males as females. Falls and motor vehicle emergency departments with severe TBI indicate
accidents were found to be the leading causes of that 23% to 49% of patients do not recover from
TBI. Falls account for the largest number of coma (Murray et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 2001).
TBIs in the very young and very old, while motor Those patients who do survive almost always
vehicle accidents account for most of the TBIs recover to a more responsive state. A small per-
in adolescence and young adulthood (Langlois, centage of surviving patients, 1% to 5%, may
Rutland-Brown, & Thomas, 2006; Rutland-Brown remain in a nonresponsive vegetative state
et al., 2006). These numbers are likely to underes- (Murray, et al., 1999), but even these patients
timate the true incidence of TBI, as they do not show recovery of some brainstem functioning
account for persons with TBIs treated in private and have return of sleep/wake cycles with periods
doctor’s offices or outpatient care settings, or of eye opening.
persons that did not seek medical care. Surviving, nonvegetative patients recover to
Incidence varies widely due to methodological some degree of responsiveness to the environ-
differences across studies. A review by Kraus and ment. This level of responsiveness may be mark-
Chu (2005) demonstrated that the incidence of edly reduced at first as in the minimally conscious
brain injury ranged from 92 cases per 100,000 state (Giacino, et al., 2002). Patients in this state
persons to 618 per 100,000 with an average rate show minimal, but definite evidence of awareness
of fatal plus nonfatal hospitalized brain injuries of self or the environment. Examples include
estimated at 150 per 100,000 persons per year. localized motor responses to noxious stimuli or
Emergency-department-based studies report sounds, sustained visual fixation, vocalization in
higher rates of TBI. For instance, Jager and col- response to a stimulus, smiling or crying in
leagues (2000) indicated that 444 persons per response to a stimulus, and inconsistent com-
100,000 seek medical care for TBI each year. The mand following. Resolution of the minimally
vast majority of these injuries would have been conscious state is indicated by consistent com-
mild and would not have required hospitalization. mand following, verbal or gestural yes/no
With changes in health care provision, there has responding, intelligible verbalization, or some
been a trend for fewer patients with mild TBI to other evidence of consistent purposeful behavior
be admitted to hospitals (Thurman & Guerrero, such as functional use of objects. This state is
1999). However, the hospitalization rates for generally temporary but may be permanent in a
patients with moderate and severe TBI remain small subset of patients.
largely unchanged from earlier reports. CDC esti- Most commonly, resolution of coma is
mates from the year 2000 documented that of followed by a responsive, but markedly confused
patients hospitalized due to TBI, over 50% had state. Patients with moderate TBI may have loss
mild injuries, while 21% had moderate injuries, of consciousness at the time of injury, but (by def-
and 19% had severe injuries, resulting in about inition) are responsive but confused at presenta-
102,500 moderate and severe TBIs per year. These tion to the emergency department. The period of
rates do not include patients who expired prior to acute confusion following TBI is a temporary
hospitalization (Thurman & Guerrero, 1999). phase of recovery (Levin, 1993). Most patients
CDC estimates that approximately 56,000 persons recover from the confused state, at least to some
die from TBI each year and 80,000 to 90,000 are degree, though they may be left with persistent
left with persistent disability due to TBI each year cognitive and behavioral impairments (Levin,
(Thurman, et al., 1999). 1992). The confused phase of recovery after TBI is
a complex state that manifests in a variety of neu-
C O U R S E O F R E C O V E RY robehavioral impairments. Early writers (Russell,
FROM TBI 1932; Symonds, 1937) described deficits in
Significant TBI results in some degree of impaired arousal, memory, orientation, attention, language,
consciousness (Levin, 1992; Ommaya & behavior, mood, and perception. The period of
Gennarelli, 1974). Patients with severe injuries confusion is now commonly called post-traumatic
264 forensic neuropsychology

amnesia (PTA), but early writers used many terms primarily characterized by memory impairment
including acute traumatic psychosis, after effects as suggested by the term PTA. Using their assess-
of concussion, traumatic confusion, and delirium ment procedure for confusion, the Confusion
Many previous investigations of the period of Assessment Protocol (CAP), Sherer and col-
confusion after TBI have primarily focused on leagues (2005) found that seven key symptoms
disorientation and memory impairment. High characterize the confused state after TBI. These
and colleagues (1990) found that orientation after are: (1) disorientation, (2) impaired cognition,
TBI recovers sequentially with initial recovery of (3) restlessness, (4) fluctuation of symptom
orientation to person followed by orientation to presentation, (5) sleep disturbance, (6) decreased
place and time. Memory disturbance after head daytime level of arousal, and (7) psychotic-type
trauma is characterized by some loss of ability symptoms. Diagnosis of PTCS using the CAP
to recall events immediately preceding injury showed 87% agreement with a clinical diagnosis
(retrograde amnesia) as well as a period of inabi- on delirium. Presence of confusion at admission
lity to encode and later recall new memories to inpatient rehabilitation made a unique contri-
(anterograde amnesia) (Levin, 1992). Memory bution to functional status at discharge after
impairment during post-traumatic confusion is adjustment for other known predictors of TBI
greatest for explicit, episodic memory with some outcome.
sparing of implicit and procedural memory More recently, Sherer and colleagues (2008)
(Ewert, Levin, Watson, & Kalisky, 1989). showed that severity of confusion at a fixed time
Attentional impairments during early recov- (21 days) post-injury made a contribution to
ery from TBI include difficulties focusing prediction of early and late outcome after TBI.
attention on the examiner, sustaining attention, In addition, presence or absence of each of seven
processing information, and excessive distracti- symptoms of confusion 21 days post-injury was
bility (Levin, 1993). In their investigation of predictive of early outcome and, in most cases,
attentional functions in confused patients, Stuss late outcome. Of particular importance, this
and colleagues (1999) demonstrated that atten- investigation showed that psychotic-type symp-
tional abilities recover in an orderly manner after toms occur in approximately 40% of patients in
TBI. Performance on attentional tasks improved early recovery from TBI. Even though these symp-
prior to obtaining a GOAT score in target range or toms resolve in almost all patients, patients with
ability to recall three words at a 24-hour delay. early psychotic-type symptoms were at markedly
Stuss and colleagues argued that attentional increased risk for poor long-term outcome. These
disturbance is a key aspect of impaired conscious- findings support the position of Stuss and
ness after TBI. They noted the similarity of this colleagues (1999) that this early phase of recovery
state to delirium and proposed the term “post- after TBI is better characterized as PTCS as
traumatic confusional state” (PTCS) to replace opposed to PTA.
the more commonly used PTA. After resolution of PTA (or PTCS), patients
Other investigators have studied motor continue to show progressive resolution of physi-
restlessness (agitation) in confused patients after cal, cognitive, and behavioral impairments. There
TBI. These investigations have found an associa- is general agreement that recovery continues for
tion of restlessness with cognitive impairment. up to 18 months after moderate or severe TBI
Agitation is common in patients with low levels (Dikmen, Reitan, & Temkin, 1983; Levin, 1995;
of cognitive function, but rare in patients with Tabaddor, Mattis, & Zazula, 1984) and there is
higher levels of cognitive function (Corrigan & some evidence that some cognitive functions
Mysiw, 1988). Patients with intermediate levels of may continue to recover after this 18 months’
cognitive function are equally likely to be agitated timeframe (Millis et al., 2001; van Zomeren &
or nonagitated. Patients experience cognitive Deelman, 1978).
improvement prior to resolution of agitation as In a few patients, recovery may be compromi-
opposed to resolution of agitation followed by sed by late complications such as post-traumatic
cognitive recovery. epilepsy or post-traumatic hydrocephalus. Late
Work by Sherer and colleagues (2005; Sherer, seizures (greater than two weeks post-injury)
Yablon, et al., 2008) has provided additional occur in only about 4% to 7% of survivors of non-
support for considering the period of confusion penetrating TBI, but the incidence may be as high
after TBI to be a complex neurobehavioral as 50% for patients with penetrating TBI
state similar to delirium as opposed to a state (Annegers, Hauser, Coan, & Rocca, 1998; Yablon,
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 265

1996). The incidence of post-traumatic hydro- decreased initiation, and poor awareness of these
cephalus is not well known due to wide variation and other changes (for review see Roebuck-
in the degree of monitoring for this condition. Spencer, Banos, Sherer, & Novack, 2010).
One prospective series in which all patients with Behavioral manifestations of disinhibition may
moderate or severe TBI admitted for inpatient include aggression/agitation, social disinhibition,
rehabilitation received head CT scans found an impulsivity, risk-taking behavior, and affective
incidence of 13% (Yu, Yablon, Ivanhoe, & Boake, lability (Grafman et al., 1996; Kim, 2002).
1995). Impaired self-awareness is common after moder-
TBI patients with large focal hemispheric ate and severe TBI both in the acute (Sherer, Hart,
lesions or certain subcortical, brain stem, or cere- et al., 2003) and the post-acute periods (Sherer,
bellar lesions may have persistent motor impair- et al., 1998). This tendency for some TBI survi-
ments (Bontke, Zasler, & Boake, 1996; Horn & vors to be partially or totally unaware of problems
Sherer, 1999). Such impairments may include can be a primary source of family member stress
spasticity, dysphagia (impaired swallowing), and contributes to poor functional outcome and
dysarthria, balance disturbances, or hemiparesis. return to previous activities, including work
Most patients with moderate or severe TBI show (Prigatano, Altman, & O’Brien, 1990; Sherer et al.,
good resolution of motor impairments. 1998; Sherer, Hart, et al., 2003). Patients with poor
The prognosis for recovery of cognitive self-awareness have poor motivation to change as
abilities after moderate and severe TBI is less they do not perceive the need to change (Malec &
favorable than for motor functions. Risk for per- Moessner, 2000). The most common emotional
sistent cognitive impairment is related to initial changes following moderate to severe TBI include
injury severity as indicated by post-resuscitation depression with prevalence rates ranging from
GCS score or time to follow commands (Dikmen, 15% to 60% (Kim, et al., 2007; Rogers & Read,
Machamer, et al., 1995; Tabaddor et al., 1984). 2007) and anxiety with prevalence rates ranging
Even at one year post-injury, all patients with very from 2% to 44% (Rogers & Read, 2007). Rates of
severe TBI (time to follow commands ≥ 14 days) psychotic disorders after TBI are less clear due to
have residual cognitive impairments while more inconsistent definitions and methodology, but are
than one-half of those with time to follow com- generally not considered to be common following
mands between one hour and 13 days have resid- TBI (Kim et al., 2007).
ual deficits (Dikmen, Machamer, et al., 1995). Patients and family members are more likely
While the general course of recovery of cognitive to report these neurobehavioral and emotional
abilities is for continuing improvements for about impairments than either cognitive or physical
18 months post-injury, a subgroup of patients impairments in the post-acute period (Brooks,
shows improvement beyond this period (Millis Campsie, Symington, Beattie, & McKinlay, 1987;
et al., 2001). There is limited evidence that another Lezak, 1987, 1989). Family members report that
subgroup of patients may show late decline (Millis these neurobehavioral symptoms, particularly
et al., 2001; Ruff et al., 1991). Age at time of injury personality change and threats of violence, cause
appears to be a risk factor for late decline with more stress than cognitive or physical impair-
older age at time of injury indicating greater risk ments (Brooks, Campsie, Symington, Beattie, &
for late decline. McKinlay, 1986; Riley, 2007). There is some evi-
The typical pattern of impairments after blunt dence that family member report of these symp-
head trauma includes slowed fine motor move- toms may actually increase with the passage of
ments, decreased attention, decreased cognitive time (Brooks et al., 1986). It is unclear whether
speed, memory impairment, impaired complex this is due to an actual increase in the frequency or
language skills and discourse, and impaired exec- severity of behavioral problems as opposed to a
utive functions (Levin, 1993). Severe persistent greater sensitivity to the effects of these problems.
aphasia or visual perceptual impairment are
uncommon after diffuse injuries but may occur in NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL
patients with focal injuries (Levin, 1993). A S S E S S M E N T O F PAT I E N T S
Patients with moderate and severe TBI may W I T H M O D E R AT E A N D
also show persistent neurobehavioral impair- SEVERE TBI
ments and emotional changes (Oddy, Humphrey, There are a number of contributions that neuro-
& Uttley, 1978; Satz et al., 1998). Neurobehavioral psychological assessment can make to the care of
changes include problems with disinhibition, persons with moderate and severe TBI. Such
266 forensic neuropsychology

evaluations provide documentation of cognitive, simple in terms of test directions, processing


behavioral, and emotional status and may also demands, and response requirements in order to
assist with determination of patients’ ability to enhance chances of test completion. Neuro-
function independently (Sherer & Novack, 2003). psychological tests measuring language compre-
Documentation of status is useful to provide hension, attention, short-term memory, visual
feedback to family members, improve patient acuity, and tracking may be useful. Examples of
self-awareness, guide treatment efforts, and assess specific neuropsychological tests used in acute
the effectiveness of medication trials. Areas of batteries for individuals in later stages of PTA
functional ability that may be assessed include (e.g., GOAT scores > 40) include Complex
decision-making capacity, capacity for safe and Ideational Material from the Boston Diagnostic
independent home functioning, driving capacity, Aphasia Exam, the Mini Token Test, Auditory
and ability to return to work. Number Search Test, Visual Number Search Test,
The focus of neuropsychological assessment is Digit Span, and the Trailmaking Test (Hannay &
determined both by the goals of the assessment Sherer, 1996; Pastorek et al., 2004).
and the stage of recovery of the patient. Early neu- While a range of neuropsychological batteries
ropsychological assessment may focus on deter- have been used in the literature to assess the
mining level of responsiveness and documenting cognitive effects of TBI, no specific battery of tests
changes in level of responsiveness in minimally for TBI has been proposed and widely accepted.
conscious patients. Measures such as the Coma Initial and follow-up comprehensive neuro-
Recovery Scale (Giacino, Kezmarsky, DeLuca, & psychological evaluations of persons with moder-
Cicerone, 1991) provide a structured repeatable ate or severe TBI should nonetheless assess a wide
protocol for assessing low-level patients. Areas range of abilities including orientation, fine motor
assessed include arousal and attention, auditory skills, divided and sustained attention, cognitive
perception, visual perception, motor function, speed, memory, language skills, visual-perceptual
oromotor ability, communication, and initiation. skills, and executive functions (Clifton et al., 1992;
With responsive but confused patients, assess- Dikmen, Machamer, et al., 1995; Hannay & Sherer,
ment focuses on orientation, attentional skills, 1996; Traumatic Brain Injury Model Systems
ability to form new memories, and level of agita- National Data Center, 1999). Key impairments
tion. Measures such as the GOAT (Levin, et al., shown by almost all patients with moderate
1979) or Orientation Log (Jackson, et al., 1998) to severe TBI include attentional difficulties,
can be used to assess orientation. The Toronto slowed information processing speed, verbal
Test of Acute Recovery After TBI (Stuss, et al., learning and memory, and executive functioning.
1999) includes simple measures of attentional Thus, tests assessing these domains are particu-
skills and ability to form and retain new memo- larly important to include in any battery assessing
ries. The Agitated Behavior Scale (Corrigan, 1989) these patients and may be the most predictive of
is the most commonly used measure of agitation outcome (see prediction of outcome section
after TBI. The Confusion Assessment Protocol included later in this chapter). Table 10.1 provides
(Sherer, Nakase-Thompson, et al., 2005) includes a list of neuropsychological tests organized by cog-
elements of all these areas and preliminary find- nitive domain that have been used frequently in
ings indicate that it may be useful in assessing a the research literature on moderate to severe TBI.
wide range of symptoms of confusion after TBI. Neurobehavioral problems such as mental
Some writers recommend delaying adminis- flexibility, planning, unusual thought content,
tration of formal neuropsychological tests until agitation, disinhibition, emotional withdrawal,
the patient has emerged from PTA (Clifton, et al., hostility, depression, anxiety, and motor slowing
1992). This recommendation is based on the should also be assessed (McCauley et al., 2001).
assumption that confused, disoriented patients Measures such as the Neurobehavioral Rating
will perform poorly on all tests and, thus, little Scale (Levin et al., 1987) and the Neurobehavioral
additional information will be obtained. Functioning Inventory (Kreutzer, Marwitz, Seel,
Nonetheless, there is some evidence that adminis- & Serio, 1996) are helpful with assessment of
tration of selected neuropsychological measures neurobehavioral impairments. Because of fre-
to patients still in PTA can result in useful data quent problems with poor self-awareness follow-
that are predictive of later functional status ing moderate to severe TBI, it is helpful to collect
(Hannay & Sherer, 1996; Pastorek, Hannay, & this information from a family member or
Contant, 2004). Such tests should be relatively caregiver, in addition to the patient.
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 267

TABLE 10.1 EX AMPLES OF COMMONLY USED TESTS


BY COGNITIVE DOMAIN FOR NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL
ASSESSMENT OF TBI
Cognitive Domain Tests Utilized
Orientation: Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test2
Attention/Processing Speed/Flexibility: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test1,4
Digit Span2
Seashore Rhythm Test3
Stroop Color Word Test3
Trail Making Test, Parts A2 and B1,2
Symbol Digit Modalities Test2
Digit Symbol1
Memory: Rey Complex Figure Test1
Selective Reminding Test1,3
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test2
WMS-Logical Memory Test2,3
WMS-Visual Reproduction3
California Verbal Learning Test4
Language: Controlled Oral Word Association Test1,2
Token Test2
WAIS-VIQ3
Visuospatial Skills: Benton Visual Form Discrimination Test2
Block Design Test2
WAIS-PIQ3
Tactual Performance Test3
Problem Solving/Reasoning: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test1,2
Category Test3
Motor: Grooved Peg Board Test1,2
Finger Tapping Test3
Namewriting3
Behavior: Neurobehavioral Rating Scale1
Neurobehavioral Functioning Inventory2
1
(Clifton, et al., 1992)
2
(Traumatic Brain Injury Model Systems National Data Center, 1999)
3
(Dikmen, Machamer, et al., 1995)
4
(Dikmen, Machamer, Powell, & Temkin, 2003)
Descriptions of these tests can be found in reference volumes on neuropsychological assessment such as
(Lezak, Howieson, Loring, Hannay, & Fischer, 2004) and (Spreen & Strauss, 1998).

The relationship between cognitive function- these areas. Three scales that have been included
ing and personal independence and employment in the TBI Model Systems protocol (Traumatic
is complex. While neuropsychological findings Brain Injury Model Systems National Data Center,
are related to functional outcomes such as 1999) are described below.
personal independence (Hart et al., 2003) and The Disability Rating Scale (DRS) (Rappaport,
employment (Dikmen et al., 1994; Sherer, Novack, Hall, Hopkins, Belleza, & Cope, 1982) was devel-
et al., 2002; Sherer, Sander, et al., 2002), these oped to track patient progress after TBI from
functional outcomes are also influenced by coma to return to community activities. The DRS
a variety of other factors such as premorbid func- is a 30-point scale which rates eight areas of
tioning, demographic variables, environmental functioning: eye opening; verbalization; motor
supports, and family support (Sherer, Nick, et al., response; level of cognitive ability for daily activi-
2003). There are a number of instruments that can ties of feeding, toileting, and grooming; overall
be used to directly rate patient functioning in level of dependence; and employability. Higher
268 forensic neuropsychology

scores indicate greater disability. Inter-rater respondents reported a median of 20 years of clin-
reliability has been shown to range from 0.97 to ical experience with patients with TBI (range = 7
0.98 (Gouvier, Blanton, LaPorte, & Nepomuceno, to 35 years). Guidelines for timing of assessments
1987; Rappaport et al., 1982). The DRS has been based on this survey are summarized in Table 10.2.
shown to be sensitive to improvements in func- These guidelines provide general suggestions.
tioning between two and six months post-injury, Timing of testing for any specific patient should be
as well as between six months and one year (Hall, determined by clinician judgment based on evalu-
Cope, & Rappaport, 1985). ation of factors unique to that patient.
The Supervision Rating Scale (SRS) (Boake, In cases of moderate or severe TBI, there
1996) can be used to quantify the level of personal will generally be clear-cut medical evidence indi-
independence. The level of supervision received is cating that the patient sustained brain injury.
rated on a 13-point ordinal scale ranging from This may be in the form of radiological findings,
“independent” to “full-time direct supervision medical documentation of loss of consciousness,
(with patient in physical restraints).” The SRS has coma, or sustained confusion, or operative reports.
been shown to have satisfactory inter-rater reli- Nonetheless, such patients may occasionally
ability (Boake, 1996). SRS scores are related to exaggerate symptoms when seen for follow-up
patient living arrangement and to skills in activi- evaluations, particularly if they are engaged in
ties of daily living. litigation. Consequently, the symptom validity
The Community Integration Questionnaire measures described by Larrabee in chapter 5
(CIQ) (Willer, Rosenthal, Kreutzer, Gordon, & should also be administered for those moderate
Rempel, 1993) was used in the TBI Model Systems or severe TBI patients involved in litigation or
battery in the past and was developed to assess compensation actions.
degree of community integration after TBI.
Community functioning is rated in three areas: OUTCOME AFTER
home integration, social integration, and produc- M O D E R AT E A N D S E V E R E T B I
tive activity. Studies of inter-rater reliability have Outcome after moderate and severe TBI can be
found moderate to strong reliability (Sander et al., assessed in many ways. Neurosurgical studies may
1997; Willer, Ottenbacher, & Coad, 1994). Validity focus on early survival while rehabilitation studies
studies have shown that CIQ scores are correlated may focus on return to work or independent
with other measures of functional status such as living. The Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS)
the DRS (Sander, et al., 1999). (Jennett & Bond, 1975) is the most commonly
In an attempt to provide some guidance regard- used measure of overall outcome after TBI. With
ing timing of neuropsychological evaluations for the GOS, outcomes are rated in five categories:
patients with TBI of different severities, Sherer (1) Death, (2) Vegetative State (unable to
and Novack (2003) conducted a survey of 41 neu- follow commands or communicate), (3) Severe
ropsychologists. Survey participants were selected Disability (conscious but requiring assistance to
based on board certification, published research meet basic physical and cognitive needs such
on TBI, and current participation in TBI research. feeding, toileting, grooming, or personal safety),
Thirty-three of those surveyed responded. These (4) Moderate Disability (able to meet basic

TABLE 10.2 SCHEDULE FOR NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL


EVALUATIONS AFTER TBI
Time of Evaluation Severe TBI Moderate TBI Mild TBI
At resolution of PTA X X X
1 week to 1 month post-injury X
3 months post-injury X X X
6 months post-injury X X
1 year post-injury X X X
2 years post-injury X X
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 269

physical and cognitive needs and use public trans- disability by 6 months. By one year post-injury,
portation and work in a sheltered workshop, but 17% to 22% of patients with severe TBI remain
unable to return to nonsheltered work or resume with moderate disability (Choi et al., 1994; Zhang
other major societal roles), and (5) Good Recovery et al., 2001).
(able to return to nonsheltered work though Good recovery is achieved by only 22% of
perhaps in a decreased capacity and resume social patients with severe TBI by 3 months post-injury
roles though some neurologic or psychologic (Choi & Barnes, 1996; Choi et al., 1994). By 6
impairments may remain). months post-injury, this has increased to over
Death is a common outcome after severe TBI. 35% (Choi et al., 1994; Zhang et al., 2001). At one
Death rates for severe TBI patients who have been year after severe TBI, 46% to 54% of patients with
hospitalized range from 23% to 50% (Braakman, severe TBI have reached good recovery (Choi
Gelpke, Habbema, Maas, & Minderhoud, 1980; et al., 1994; Zhang et al., 2001). Thus, for patients
Jiang, Gao, Li, Yu, & Zhu, 2002; Marion, 1996; who survive severe TBI, good recovery is the most
Murray et al., 1999; Zhang et al., 2001). The most likely GOS outcome by one year post-injury. Note
typical death rate is about 40%. Causes of early that these patients may remain with significant
death after TBI include brain swelling, diffuse cognitive or neurobehavioral problems even
axonal injury, increased intracranial pressure, and though they have recovered well enough to return
intracranial hematomas (Graham, Adams, & to work.
Gennarelli, 1993; Marion, 1996). Death is a rare outcome after moderate TBI,
Only a few patients remain in a vegetative state occurring in fewer than 10% of cases (Murray
after severe TBI. At 3 months post-injury, less et al., 1999; Stein, 1996). When death does occur,
than 10% of patients are in a vegetative state (Choi it is likely to be due to associated trauma or medi-
& Barnes, 1996; Choi et al., 1994) and by 6 months, cal complications (Signorini, Andrews, Jones,
only 4% remain vegetative (Murray, et al., 1999). Wardlaw, & Miller, 1999). Vegetative state is even
Of those who are vegetative at 3 months, 50% a rarer outcome after moderate TBI with some
improve, 25% expire, and 25% remain in a vegeta- large trauma series reporting no cases (Murray
tive state so that, by one year post-injury, the inci- et al., 1999).
dence of vegetative state ranges from less than 1% Severe disability does occur after moderate
(Jiang et al., 2002) to 2% or 3% (Choi & Barnes, TBI, though it is uncommon. Some reports
1996; Choi et al., 1994) of severe TBI survivors. indicate that no moderately injured patients
Outcome proportions for patients with severe remain with severe disability at 6 months post-
disability, moderate disability, and good recovery injury (Williams et al., 1990) while others find
below are for patients with severe TBI who sur- that 6% (Stein, 1996) to 14% (Murray et al., 1999)
vived their acute hospitalizations excluding those have severe disability. Moderate disability is a
who expired. At 3 months post-injury, approxi- more common outcome and is seen in about 25%
mately 31% to 32% of surviving patients initially of patients at 6 months post-injury (Jain, Layton,
hospitalized with severe TBI remain with severe & Murray, 2000; Stein, 1996; Williams et al., 1990).
disability (Choi & Barnes, 1996; Choi et al., 1994). Good recovery is by far the most common out-
By 6 months post-injury, only about 22% remain come after moderate TBI with 53% (Murray et al.,
with severe disability (Choi & Barnes, 1996; Choi 1999) to 73% (Williams et al., 1990) of cases
et al., 1994; Murray et al., 1999). By one year post- showing good recovery by 6 months post-injury.
injury, many patients who experienced severe dis- As with severe TBI patients, good recovery cannot
ability at 3 months post-injury have recovered to be taken to mean complete recovery.
moderate disability or good recovery. Only about While data on GOS outcome categories are
17% of surviving patients remain with severe dis- informative, clinicians, family, and patients are
ability (Choi & Barnes, 1996; Choi et al., 1994). more likely to be concerned about the likelihood
Approximately 30% of severe TBI patients that a patient will return to work and/or to inde-
have moderate disability at 3 and 6 months post- pendent living. Employment outcomes have been
injury (Choi & Barnes, 1996; Choi et al., 1994; studied more intensively than independent living
Murray et al., 1999). However, some of the patients outcomes. Perhaps this is because of the greater
who had moderate disability at 3 months have ease of characterizing employment status as
achieved good recovery by 6 months while some opposed to degree of personal independence.
patients who were vegetative or had severe dis- Reported return to work rates following TBI
ability at 3 months have recovered to moderate range from 22% to 66% (Sander, Kreutzer,
270 forensic neuropsychology

Rosenthal, Delmonico, & Young, 1996). The wide than those lost to follow-up (68%). This suggests
range of rates is contributed to by inter-study that the one-year post-injury employment rate for
differences in injury classification, populations the 1083 patients is likely to overestimate the
sampled, time from injury to follow-up, and overall one-year post-injury employment rate.
differing definitions of employment. Previous For the 1083 patients with available one-year
investigations have focused on two populations of post-injury employment data, the employment
patients with TBI, consecutive cases seen at rate was 35%.
trauma centers and patients seen for inpatient Moderate and severe TBI results in decreased
rehabilitation. Both populations are important, personal independence. Persons with moderate
but findings based on one population should not and severe TBI have a markedly increased risk for
be generalized to the other. The subset of TBI institutional (usually nursing home) placement as
patients admitted for inpatient rehabilitation compared to noninjured controls (Dikmen,
excludes those with very poor outcomes such as Machamer, & Temkin, 1993; Kersel, Marsh, Havill,
vegetative patients and those with very good out- & Sleigh, 2001). Even so, only about 5% require
comes such as those who are oriented and inde- institutional care at one year post-injury (Dikmen
pendent with activities of daily living prior to et al., 1993; Kersel et al., 2001). More commonly,
discharge from the acute care hospital. persons who were living independently prior to
Brooks and colleagues (1987) reported on a injury reside with a family member or significant
series of 134 patients with severe TBI seen on an other post-injury. In one series, almost half of
acute neurosurgical service. Most patients (75%) persons with moderate and severe TBI who were
were employed at time of injury but only 25% living independently prior to injury are living
were employed at follow-up, assessed from two to with parents at one year post-injury (Dikmen,
seven years post-injury. Dikmen and colleagues et al., 1993).
(1994) reported on a series of 366 patients with In a study of caregiver supervision, Hart and
TBI who were admitted to a trauma center. colleagues (2003) reported on a series of 563
Patients who were not employed at time of injury patients with TBI who were seen for inpatient
were excluded from study. At one year post-injury, rehabilitation. Patients who were could not com-
26% of patients with severe injuries had returned plete a neuropsychological evaluation during
to work and 56% of patients with moderate inpatient rehabilitation were excluded from the
injuries had returned to work. By two years post- study meaning that patients with more severe
injury, 37% of patients with severe injuries were injuries may be under-represented in this study
working while 64% of patients with moderate sample. Sixty-nine percent of this sample were
injuries were working. Age, education, pre-injury rated as receiving no supervision at one year fol-
work history, injury severity, and neuropsycho- low-up. Twenty-four percent received varying
logical test performance, including ability to degrees of part-time supervision and 7% received
undergo testing at one month were all strongly full-time supervision. Amount of supervision
related to the amount of time it took patients to received at follow-up was generally related to ini-
return to work after sustaining a TBI. Doctor and tial injury severity as determined by GCS rating
colleagues (2005) examined unemployment rates with those having more impaired initial GCS rat-
at one year post-injury after adjusting for unem- ings receiving more supervision at follow-up.
ployment rates in the general population and However, initial GCS scores for those who were
found that 42% of individuals with TBI were independent at follow-up ranged from 3 to 15, as
unemployed compared with a 9% expected unem- did initial GCS scores for those receiving the
ployment rate. highest levels of supervision at follow-up.
Sherer and colleagues (2003) reported on 1615 The literature on very long-term outcome fol-
patients with TBI who were admitted to 17 TBI lowing history of moderate to severe TBI is grow-
Model Systems sites for inpatient rehabilitation. ing due to evidence that some individuals with
Of this population, 72% were employed at the TBI show a decline in cognitive functioning later
time of injury. One year employment outcome in life. History of moderate to severe TBI has now
data were available on 1083 patients. Of these been accepted as a risk factor for degenerative
1083 patients, 63% had severe injuries, 16% had neurological diseases (Institute of Medicine,
moderate injuries, and 20% had complicated mild 2009). A study by Plassman and colleagues (2000)
injuries. Patients who were available at follow-up found greater risk for dementia and specifically
had a higher pre-injury employment rate (76%) for Alzheimer’s disease in WWII veterans who
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 271

had a history of medically documented moderate 2001; Vespa et al., 2002), (6) elevated intracranial
to severe TBI. This relationship was not found for pressure (Eisenberg & Weiner, 1987; Jiang et al.,
mild TBI. Other studies have also found an 2002), and (7) hypoxia (Andrews et al., 2002;
increased risk for Alzheimer’s type dementia fol- Eisenberg & Weiner, 1987; Jiang et al., 2002). Of
lowing history of TBI (French et al., 1985; Guo demographic variables, age is most predictive of
et al., 2000; Heyman et al., 1984; Schofield et al., death with older age being associated with greater
1997). Higher rates of past TBI also have been risk of death (Jiang et al., 2002; Mosenthal et al.,
found in studies of patients with Parkinson’s dis- 2002; Susman et al., 2002). Early neurosurgical
ease or probable Parkinson’s disease (Bower et al., management also affects death rates. Centers that
2003; Goldman et al., 2006; Taylor et al., 1999). managed patients aggressively, as indicated by
The relationship between TBI and later neurode- intracranial monitor placement, had 40% lower
generative disease may be driven by the accumu- mortality rates than centers with less aggressive
lation of multiple proteins in the brains of management (Bulger et al., 2002).
individuals with TBI that is presumed to be trig- Late functional status (return to work, return
gered by the injury (Uryu et al., 2007). Although to school, and personal independence) after TBI
manifestation of neurodegenerative disease does is affected by even more factors than early out-
not occur until later in life, this accumulation of come. As time from injury to outcome becomes
proteins appears to start early in the course of greater, injury characteristics become less impor-
injury. Other studies propose a genetic vulnera- tant and other factors such as premorbid func-
bility that increases the association between TBI tioning and environmental supports become
and neurodegenerative disease (Guo et al., 2000; more important. Factors predictive of functional
Mayeux et al., 1995). However, this relationship outcome can be categorized into pre-injury fac-
remains inconclusive (Millar, Nicoll, Thornhill, tors (including demographic variables), injury
Murray, & Teasdale, 2003). severity variables, neuroanatomical variables,
physical impairments, cognitive and neurobehav-
PREDICTION OF OUTCOME ioral impairments, and environmental supports.
There is a very large literature on prediction of See Table 10.3 for a review of studies using these
outcome after moderate and severe TBI. variables to predict functional outcome. These
Interpretation of this literature is complicated by factors are generally studied by examining predic-
the wide variety of populations sampled, time tion of outcome for groups of patients using
frames of outcomes, and outcomes studied. multivariable regression models. While these
Factors predictive of a given outcome in a partic- models account for substantial variability in func-
ular sample over a specified timeframe may not be tional outcomes after TBI, substantial additional
at all predictive of apparently related outcomes in variability remains unexplained. Further, results
a different sample over a different timeframe. This may be specific to the sample or particular data
is particularly the case for subsamples that are set utilized. In each data set there are individual
highly selected (e.g., patients admitted for post- outcomes that are better or worse than predicted
acute rehabilitation services). We will briefly by the regression models. Consequently, clini-
review predictors of death after TBI and func- cians must be cautious in applying findings from
tional status at follow-up. these studies to the prediction of a specific out-
Factors most predictive of death after TBI are come for a specific patient.
those that directly indicate neurologic and physi- Compared to the number of studies on
ologic status early after injury. Such factors predicting return to school and personal indepen-
include: (1) level of responsiveness as indicated by dence after TBI, the literature on return to work
admission GCS score (Eisenberg & Weiner, 1987; after TBI is quite extensive. It should be noted that
Mosenthal et al., 2002), (2) pupillary responses many studies define work broadly and may include
(Andrews et al., 2002; Jiang et al., 2002; Wardlaw, return to school as indicating a return to produc-
Easton, & Statham, 2002), (3) initial CT scan find- tivity after TBI. Unfortunately, these studies do
ings (particularly presence of subarachnoid blood not provide separate analyses for return to school.
or mass lesion such as subdural hematoma) Injury severity as indicated by initial GCS score,
(Eisenberg & Weiner, 1987; Mataro et al., 2001; time to follow commands, or duration of PTA is
Wardlaw et al., 2002), (4) elevated temperature predictive of employment outcome with those
(Andrews et al., 2002; Jiang et al., 2002), (5) elec- with more severe injuries having poorer outcomes
trophysiologic findings (Claassen & Hansen, (Dikmen et al., 1994; Doctor et al., 2005). Time to
272 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 10.3 FACTORS PREDICTIVE OF FUNCTIONAL OUTCOME AFTER TBI


Predictors References
Pre-injury factors
Age (Brown, et al., 2005; Dikmen, et al., 1994; Donders &
Warschausky, 2007; Keyser-Marcus, et al., 2002; Poon,
Zhu, Ng, & Wong, 2005; Ruff, et al., 1993; Sherer, Nick,
et al., 2003; Testa, Malec, Moessner, & Brown, 2005)
Years of education (Hart, et al., 2003; Sherer, et al., 1999; Sherer, Sander, et al.,
2002)
Pre-injury employment (Dikmen, et al., 1994; Keyser-Marcus, et al., 2002; Sherer,
Nick, et al., 2003; Sherer, Sander, et al., 2002)
Substance use (MacMillan, Hart, Martelli, & Zasler, 2002; Sherer, et al.,
1999)
Injury severity variables
Initial GCS (Dikmen, et al., 1994; Levin, et al., 1990; Poon, et al., 2005)
Time to follow commands (Dikmen, Ross, et al., 1995; Dikmen, et al., 1994; Hart, et al.,
2003; Ruff, et al., 1993)
Duration of PTA (Boake, et al., 2001; Brown, et al., 2005; Sherer, Sander, et al.,
2002; Sherer, Yablon, et al., 2008)
Neuroanatomical variables (Hanlon, Demery, Kuczen, & Kelly, 2005; T. W. Teasdale &
Engberg, 2005; Wedekind & Lippert-Gruner, 2005)
Physical impairments (Brown, et al., 2005)
Cognitive and neurobehavioral impairments
Early cognitive status (Boake, et al., 2001; Dikmen, et al., 1994; Sherer, Novack,
et al., 2002; Sherer, Sander, et al., 2002)
Neuropsychological testing (Boake, et al., 2001; Doctor, et al., 2005; Hanks, et al., 2008;
Hart, et al., 2003; Kinsella, et al., 1997)
Post-injury depression (Ruff, et al., 1993; Seel, et al., 2003)
Impaired self-awareness (Sherer, et al., 1998; Trudel, Tryon, & Purdum, 1998)
Early functional status (Gollaher, et al., 1998; Ponsford, Olver, Curran, & Ng, 1995)
Environmental supports
Family functioning (Catroppa, et al., 2008; Ewing-Cobbs, et al., 2006; Prigatano,
et al., 1994)
Post-acute brain injury rehabilitation (High, Roebuck-Spencer, Sander, Struchen, & Sherer, 2006;
Malec & Basford, 1996; Sander, Roebuck, Struchen, Sherer,
& High, 2001)

follow commands and PTA duration are generally employment status, also has substantial impact on
more predictive of outcome than initial GCS employment outcomes (Dikmen et al., 1994;
score, particularly for patients who undergo inpa- Sherer, Nick, et al., 2003).
tient brain injury rehabilitation (Sherer, Sander, Neuropsychological assessment makes a
et al., 2002; Sherer, Yablon, et al., 2008). Of demo- unique contribution to prediction of employment
graphic characteristics, age and years of education beyond that made by injury and demographic
have the strongest associations with employment variables with more intact early cognitive status
outcome with age at injury greater than 50 years predicting more favorable long-term employment
predicting poorer outcome (Dikmen et al., 1994) outcome (Sherer, Novack, et al., 2002). A recent
and higher years of education predicting more prospective study found that a brief battery of
favorable outcome (Sherer, Sander, et al., 2002). neuropsychological tests administered early in the
Pre-injury adjustment, particularly pre-injury recovery course was predictive of employability at
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 273

one year (Hanks et al., 2008). Neuropsychological placement at one year post-injury (Dikmen, Ross,
tests were able to predict functional outcome Machamer, & Temkin, 1995). Institutional place-
above and beyond functional and injury severity ment in the post-acute period is a less frequent
variables providing support for the incremental outcome than dependence on others for supervi-
validity of neuropsychological testing in moderate sion and assistance with household management.
to severe TBI. Tasks requiring rapid cognitive This dependence results in many persons who
processing and cognitive flexibility appear to be lived independently prior to TBI living with par-
particularly useful in making these predictions ents or others at one and two year post-injury
(Boake et al., 2001). For instance, Doctor and follow-up. As with institutional placement, initial
colleagues (2005) found that greater risk of unem- injury severity predicts likelihood of living with
ployment at one year post-injury was associated others after injury (Dikmen, Ross, et al., 1995).
with poor performance on the Trailmaking Test Degree of cognitive impairment as assessed by
Part B and the Digit Symbol subtest of the Wechsler neuropsychological tests makes a unique contri-
Adult Intelligence Scale administered at one bution to prediction of supervision requirements
month post-injury. after TBI (Hanks et al., 2008; Hart et al., 2003).
Significant TBI has a number of effects on As with other functional outcomes, personal
school performance. TBI survivors are more independence after TBI is affected by a number
likely to fail grades, are more likely to receive of noninjury factors such as sex, marital status,
special education services, have more behavioral and ethnicity (Chang, Ostir, Kuo, Granger, &
problems, have poorer academic performance, Ottenbacher, 2008).
and score more poorly on cognitive tests than In conclusion, moderate to severe TBI is dis-
children with no TBI (Ewing-Cobbs et al., 2006; tinct in its initial presentation, course of symp-
Hawley, 2004). Contrary to some early thoughts, toms, and outcome as compared to milder forms
younger age at injury is associated with poorer of TBI. These individuals sustain a wide range of
outcome due to interference with skills acquisi- neuropsychological and neurobehavioral impair-
tion during key developmental phases (Donders ments that can be long lasting and affect their
& Warschausky, 2007). For older (college-aged) ability to return to previous levels of personal,
patients, return to school is often considered a social, and occupational functioning. Factors pre-
productive outcome and combined with return to dicting short and long-term outcome are varied
work in outcome studies (e.g., Ip, Dornan, & and complex in nature. This chapter was intended
Schentag, 1995) so that there is little or no unique to assist neuropsychologists in understanding the
investigation of factors that predict return to expected course of recovery, the expected out-
school for this age group. Prediction of return to comes, and the factors that help to predict out-
school for younger patients may be a moot point come following moderate to severe TBI. By
as these children have a legal right to an appropri- understanding these factors, the neuropsycholo-
ate educational program regardless of degree of gist will be better suited to evaluate these patients,
impairment. Nonetheless, school performance for provide psychoeducation and treatment, and help
children is predicted by injury severity (Catroppa, patients and families plan for the future. Finally,
Anderson, Morse, Haritou, & Rosenfeld, 2008) knowledge of these factors is essential for the neu-
and neuropsychological assessment (Kinsella ropsychologist working with these patients in
et al., 1997). Even children with mild TBI have forensic settings by allowing for identification of
poorer adaptive skills though academic perfor- expected behavioral and performance patterns
mance may be less affected (Catroppa et al., 2008). and using this knowledge to make informed
Effects of brain injury are moderated by pre-injury decisions and predictions about the future.
functioning, socio-economic status, and family
functioning (Catroppa et al., 2008; Ewing-Cobbs AC K N OW L E D G M E N T
et al., 2006). Completion of this chapter was partially sup-
TBI also has a marked effect on personal ported by the National Institute on Disability and
independence. Greater injury severity, as indi- Rehabilitation Research Grant #H133A070043,
cated by longer time to follow commands interval, the Texas TBI Model System of TIRR
is associated with decreased personal indepen-
dence after TBI. Residential placement may be REFERENCES
considered a proxy for personal independence. Alexander, M. (1992). Neuropsychiatric correlates of
Over 40% of those with time to follow commands persistent postconcussive syndrome. Journal of
greater than 28 days require institutional Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 7, 60–69.
274 forensic neuropsychology

Andrews, P. J., Sleeman, D. H., Statham, P. F., McQuatt, elements that predict outcome after traumatic
A., Corruble, V., Jones, P. A., et al. (2002). Predicting brain injury: A prospective multicenter recursive
recovery in patients suffering from traumatic brain partitioning (decision-tree) analysis. Journal of
injury by using admission variables and physio- Neurotrauma, 22(10), 1040–51.
logical data: a comparison between decision tree Bulger, E. M., Nathens, A. B., Rivara, F. P., Moore, M.,
analysis and logistic regression. Journal of Neuro- MacKenzie, E. J., & Jurkovich, G. J. (2002).
surgery, 97(2), 326–36. Management of severe head injury: Institutional
Annegers, J. F., Hauser, W. A., Coan, S. P., & Rocca, variations in care and effect on outcome. Critical
W. A. (1998). A population-based study of seizures Care Medicine, 30(8), 1870–76.
after traumatic brain injuries. New England Journal Catroppa, C., Anderson, V. A., Morse, S. A., Haritou,
of Medicine, 338(1), 20–24. F., & Rosenfeld, J. V. (2008). Outcome and predic-
Binder, L. M. (1997). A review of mild head trauma. tors of functional recovery 5 years following pedi-
Part II: Clinical implications. Journal of Clinical atric traumatic brain injury (TBI). Journal of
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19(3), 432–57. Pediatric Psychology, 33(7), 707–18.
Binder, L. M. & Willis, S. C. (1991). Assessment of Chang, P. F., Ostir, G. V., Kuo, Y. F., Granger, C. V., &
motivation after financially compensable minor Ottenbacher, K. J. (2008). Ethnic differences in
head trauma. Psychological Assessment, 3, 175–81. discharge destination among older patients with
Boake, C. (1996). Supervision rating scale: A measure traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine
of functional outcome from brain injury. Archives and Rehabilitation, 89(2), 231–36.
of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 77(8), Choi, S. C. & Barnes, T. Y. (1996). Predicting Outcome
765–72. in the head-injured patient. In R. K. Naravan, J. E.
Boake, C., Millis, S. R., High, W. M., Jr., Delmonico, R. Wilberger & J. T. Povlishock (Eds.), Neurotrauma
L., Kreutzer, J. S., Rosenthal, M., et al. (2001). Using (pp. 779–92). New York: McGraw-Hill.
early neuropsychologic testing to predict long- Choi, S. C., Barnes, T. Y., Bullock, R., Germanson,
term productivity outcome from traumatic brain T. A., Marmarou, A., & Young, H. F. (1994).
injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Reha- Temporal profile of outcomes in severe head injury.
bilitation, 82(6), 761–68. Journal of Neurosurgery, 81(2), 169–73.
Bontke, C. F., Zasler, N. D., & Boake, C. (1996). Cicerone, K. D. & Kalmar, K. (1995). Persistent post-
Rehabilitation of the head-injured patient. In R. K. concussion syndrome: The structure of subjective
Naravan, J. E. Wilberger & J. T. Povlishock (Eds.), complaints after mild traumatic brain injury.
Neurotrauma (pp. 841–58). New York: McGraw- Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 10, 1–17.
Hill. Claassen, J. & Hansen, H. C. (2001). Early recovery
Bower, J. H., Maraganore, D. M., Peterson, B. J., after closed traumatic head injury: somatosensory
McDonnell, S. K., Ahlskog, J. E., & Rocca, W. A. evoked potentials and clinical findings. Critical
(2003). Head trauma preceding PD: A case-control Care Medicine, 29(3), 494–502.
study. Neurology, 60(10), 1610–1615. Clifton, G. L., Hayes, R. L., Levin, H. S., Michel, M. E.,
Braakman, R., Gelpke, G. J., Habbema, J. D., Maas, A. & Choi, S. C. (1992). Outcome measures for
I., & Minderhoud, J. M. (1980). Systematic selec- clinical trials involving traumatically brain-injured
tion of prognostic features in patients with severe patients: report of a conference. Neurosurgery,
head injury. Neurosurgery, 6(4), 362–70. 31(5), 975–78.
Brooks, D. N., Campsie, L., Symington, C., Beattie, A., Corrigan, J. D. (1989). Development of a scale for
& McKinlay, W. (1987). The effects of severe head assessment of agitation following traumatic brain
injury on patient and relative within seven years of injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 2, Neuropsychology, 11(2), 261–77.
1–13. Corrigan, J. D. & Mysiw, W. J. (1988). Agitation follow-
Brooks, N., Campsie, L., Symington, C., Beattie, A., & ing traumatic head injury: Equivocal evidence
McKinlay, W. (1986). The five year outcome of for a discrete stage of cognitive recovery. Archives
severe blunt head injury: a relative’s view. Journal of of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 69(7),
Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 49(7), 487–92.
764–70. Dikmen, S. & Machamer, J. (1995). Neurobehavioral
Brooks, N., McKinlay, W., Symington, C., Beattie, A., & outcomes and their determinants. Journal of Head
Campsie, L. (1987). Return to work within the first Trauma Rehabilitation, 10, 74–86.
seven years of severe head injury. Brain Injury, Dikmen, S., Machamer, J., & Temkin, N. (1993).
1(1), 5–19. Psychosocial outcome in patients with moderate to
Brown, A. W., Malec, J. F., McClelland, R. L., Diehl, severe head injury: 2-year follow-up. Brain Injury,
N. N., Englander, J., & Cifu, D. X. (2005). Clinical 7(2), 113–24.
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 275

Dikmen, S., Machamer, J., & Temkin, N. (2003). Late intellectual and academic outcomes following
Mild TBI with or without CT abnormalities. traumatic brain injury sustained during early
Journal of the International Neuropsychological childhood. Journal of Neurosurgery, 105(4 Suppl),
Society 9(2), 172–73. 287–96.
Dikmen, S., Machamer, J., Winn, R., & Temkin, N. R. Finkelstein, E., Corso, P., & Miller, T. (2006). The inci-
(1995). Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year post dence and economic burden of injuries in the United
head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 80–90. States. New York: Oxford University Press.
Dikmen, S., Machamer, J. E., Powell, J. M., & Temkin, Fortuny, L. A., Briggs, M., Newcombe, F., Ratcliff, G., &
N. R. (2003). Outcome 3 to 5 years after moderate Thomas, C. (1980). Measuring the duration of post
to severe traumatic brain injury. Archives of Physical traumatic amnesia. Journal of Neurology,
Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84(10), 1449–57. Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 43(5), 377–79.
Dikmen, S., McLean, A., Jr., Temkin, N. R., & Wyler, French, L. R., Schuman, L. M., Mortimer, J. A., Hutton,
A. R. (1986). Neuropsychologic outcome at one- J. T., Boatman, R. A., & Christians, B. (1985).
month postinjury. Archives of Physical Medicine A case-control study of dementia of the Alzheimer
and Rehabilitation, 67(8), 507–513. type. American Journal of Epidemiology, 121(3),
Dikmen, S., McLean, A., & Temkin, N. (1986). 414–21.
Neuropsychological and psychosocial conse- Giacino, J. T., Ashwal, S., Childs, N., Cranford, R.,
quences of minor head injury. Journal of Neurology, Jennett, B., Katz, D. I., et al. (2002). The minimally
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 49(11), 1227–32. conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria.
Dikmen, S., Reitan, R. M., & Temkin, N. R. (1983). Neurology, 58(3), 349–53.
Neuropsychological recovery in head injury. Giacino, J. T., Kezmarsky, M. A., DeLuca, J., &
Archives of Neurology, 40(6), 333–38. Cicerone, K. D. (1991). Monitoring rate of
Dikmen, S., Ross, B. L., Machamer, J. E., & Temkin, N. recovery to predict outcome in minimally respon-
R. (1995). One year psychosocial outcome in head sive patients. Archives of Physical Medicine and
injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 72(11), 897–901.
Society, 1(1), 67–77. Goldman, S. M., Tanner, C. M., Oakes, D., Bhudhikanok,
Dikmen, S., Temkin, N. R., Machamer, J. E., Holubkov, G. S., Gupta, A., & Langston, J. W. (2006). Head
A. L., Fraser, R. T., & Winn, H. R. (1994). injury and Parkinson’s disease risk in twins. Annals
Employment following traumatic head injuries. of Neurology, 60(1), 65–72.
Archives of Neurology, 51(2), 177–86. Gollaher, K., High, W., Sherer, M., Bergloff, P.,
Doctor, J. N., Castro, J., Temkin, N. R., Fraser, R. T., Boake, C., Young, M. E., et al. (1998). Prediction of
Machamer, J. E., & Dikmen, S. S. (2005). Workers’ employment outcome one to three years following
risk of unemployment after traumatic brain injury: traumatic brain injury (TBI). Brain Injury, 12(4),
A normed comparison. Journal of the International 255–63.
Neuropsychological Society, 11(6), 747–52. Gouvier, W. D., Blanton, P. D., LaPorte, K. K., &
Donders, J. & Warschausky, S. (2007). Neurobehavioral Nepomuceno, C. (1987). Reliability and validity of
outcomes after early versus late childhood trau- the Disability Rating Scale and the Levels of
matic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Cognitive Functioning Scale in monitoring recov-
Rehabilitation, 22(5), 296–302. ery from severe head injury. Archives of Physical
Eisenberg, H. M. & Weiner, R. L. (1987). Input vari- Medicine and Rehabilitation, 68(2), 94–97.
ables: How information from the acute injury can Grafman, J., Schwab, K., Warden, D., Pridgen, A.,
be used to characterize groups of patients for stud- Brown, H. R., & Salazar, A. M. (1996). Frontal lobe
ies of outcome. In H. S. Levin, J. Grafman & H. M. injuries, violence, and aggression: a report of the
Eisenberg (Eds.), Neurobehavioral recovery from Vietnam Head Injury Study. Neurology, 46(5),
head injury. New York: Oxford University Press. 1231–38.
Ellenberg, J. H., Levin, H. S., & Saydjari, C. (1996). Graham, D. I., Adams, J. H., & Gennarelli, T. A. (1993).
Posttraumatic amnesia as a predictor of outcome Pathology of brain damage in head injury. In P. R.
after severe closed head injury. Prospective assess- Cooper (Ed.), Head Injury. Baltimore: Williams &
ment. Archives of Neurology, 53(8), 782–91. Wilkins.
Ewert, J., Levin, H. S., Watson, M. G., & Kalisky, Z. Guo, Z., Cupples, L. A., Kurz, A., Auerbach, S. H.,
(1989). Procedural memory during posttraumatic Volicer, L., Chui, H., et al. (2000). Head injury and
amnesia in survivors of severe closed head injury. the risk of AD in the MIRAGE study. Neurology,
Implications for rehabilitation. Archives of 54(6), 1316–23.
Neurology, 46(8), 911–16. Hall, K., Cope, D. N., & Rappaport, M. (1985). Glasgow
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M. R., Kramer, L., Cox, C. S., Outcome Scale and Disability Rating Scale:
Jr., Baumgartner, J., Fletcher, S., et al. (2006). Comparative usefulness in following recovery in
276 forensic neuropsychology

traumatic head injury. Archives of Physical Medicine Iverson, G. L. (2005). Outcome from mild traumatic
and Rehabilitation, 66(1), 35–37. brain injury. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 18(3),
Hanks, R. A., Millis, S. R., Ricker, J. H., Giacino, J. T., 301–317.
Nakase-Richardson, R., Frol, A. B., et al. (2008). Jackson, W. T., Novack, T. A., & Dowler, R. N.
The predictive validity of a brief inpatient neurop- (1998). Effective serial measurement of cognitive
sychologic battery for persons with traumatic brain orientation in rehabilitation: The Orientation Log.
injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
Rehabilitation, 89(5), 950–57. 79(6), 718–20.
Hanlon, R. E., Demery, J. A., Kuczen, C., & Kelly, J. P. Jager, T. E., Weiss, H. B., Coben, J. H., & Pepe, P. E.
(2005). Effect of traumatic subarachnoid haemor- (2000). Traumatic brain injuries evaluated in U.S.
rhage on neuropsychological profiles and voca- emergency departments, 1992–1994. Academy of
tional outcome following moderate or severe Emergency Medicine, 7(2), 134–40.
traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 19(4), 257–62. Jain, N. S., Layton, B. S., & Murray, P. K. (2000).
Hannay, H. J. & Sherer, M. (1996). Assessment of out- Are aphasic patients who fail the GOAT in PTA?
come from head injury. In R. K. Narayan, J. E. A modified Galveston Orientation and Amnesia
Wilberger & J. T. Povlishock (Eds.), Neurotrauma Test for persons with aphasia. Clinical Neuro-
(pp. 723–47). New York: McGraw-Hill. psychologist, 14(1), 13–17.
Harrison-Felix, C., Newton, C. N., Hall, K. M., & Jennett, B. & Bond, M. (1975). Assessment of
Kreutzer, J. S. (1996). Descriptive findings from the outcome after severe brain damage. Lancet,
traumatic brain injury model systems national data 1(7905), 480–84.
base. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 11(5), Jennett, B. & Teasdale, G. (1977). Aspects of coma after
1–14. severe head injury. Lancet, 1(8017), 878–81.
Hart, T., Millis, S., Novack, T., Englander, J., Fidler- Jiang, J. Y., Gao, G. Y., Li, W. P., Yu, M. K., & Zhu, C.
Sheppard, R., & Bell, K. R. (2003). The relationship (2002). Early indicators of prognosis in 846 cases
between neuropsychologic function and level of of severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of
caregiver supervision at 1 year after traumatic Neurotrauma, 19(7), 869–74.
brain injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Katz, D. I. & Alexander, M. P. (1994). Traumatic brain
Rehabilitation, 84(2), 221–30. injury. Predicting course of recovery and outcome
Hawley, C. A. (2004). Behaviour and school performance for patients admitted to rehabilitation. Archives of
after brain injury. Brain Injury, 18(7), 645–59. Neurology, 51(7), 661–70.
Heyman, A., Wilkinson, W. E., Stafford, J. A., Helms, Kersel, D. A., Marsh, N. V., Havill, J. H., & Sleigh, J. W.
M. J., Sigmon, A. H., & Weinberg, T. (1984). (2001). Psychosocial functioning during the year
Alzheimer’s disease: A study of epidemiological following severe traumatic brain injury. Brain
aspects. Annals of Neurology, 15(4), 335–41. Injury, 15(8), 683–96.
High, W. M., Jr, Levin, H. S., & Gary, H. E., Jr. (1990). Keyser-Marcus, L. A., Bricout, J. C., Wehman, P.,
Recovery of orientation following closed-head Campbell, L. R., Cifu, D. X., Englander, J., et al.
injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental (2002). Acute predictors of return to employment
Neuropsychology, 12(5), 703–14. after traumatic brain injury: A longitudinal follow-
High, W. M., Jr., Roebuck-Spencer, T., Sander, A. M., up. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabili-
Struchen, M. A., & Sherer, M. (2006). Early versus tation, 83(5), 635–41.
later admission to postacute rehabilitation: Impact Kim, E. (2002). Agitation, aggression, and disinhibi-
on functional outcome after traumatic brain injury. tion syndromes after traumatic brain injury. Neuro
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Rehabilitation, 17(4), 297–310.
87(3), 334–42. Kim, E., Lauterbach, E. C., Reeve, A., Arciniegas, D. B.,
Horn, L. J. & Sherer, M. (1999). Rehabilitation of trau- Coburn, K. L., Mendez, M. F., et al. (2007).
matic brain injury. In M. Grabois, S. J. Garrison, K. Neuropsychiatric complications of traumatic brain
A. Hart, & L. D. Lehmkuhl (Eds.), Physical medi- injury: A critical review of the literature (a report
cine and rehabilitation: The complete approach (pp. by the ANPA Committee on Research). Journal
1281–1304). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Science. Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 19(2),
Institute of Medicine (2009). Gulf War and health, 106–27.
Volume 7: Long-term consequences of traumatic Kinsella, G. J., Prior, M., Sawyer, M., Ong, B., Murtagh,
brain injury. Washington, DC: The National D., Eisenmajer, R., et al. (1997). Predictors and
Academies Press. indicators of academic outcome in children 2 years
Ip, R. Y., Dornan, J., & Schentag, C. (1995). Traumatic following traumatic brain injury. Journal of the
brain injury: Factors predicting return to work or International Neuropsychological Society, 3(6),
school. Brain Injury, 9(5), 517–32. 608–16.
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 277

Kraus, J. F., & Chu, L. D. (2005). Epidemiology. disability following traumatic brain injury. Journal
In J. M. Silver, T. W. McAllister & S. C. Yudofsky of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 2, 57–69.
(Eds.), Textbook of traumatic brain injury Lezak, M. D. (1989). Assessment of psychosocial
(pp. 3–26). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric dysfunctions resulting dysfunctions resulting
Publishing, Inc. from head trauma. In M. D. Lezak (Ed.), Assessment
Kreutzer, J. S., Marwitz, J. H., Seel, R., & Serio, C. D. of behavioral consequences of head trauma
(1996). Validation of a neurobehavioral function- (pp. 113–43). New York: Alan R. Liss, Inc.
ing inventory for adults with traumatic brain Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Hannay,
injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabi- H. J., & Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological
litation, 77(2), 116–24. assessment (Fourth ed.). New York: Oxford
Kung, H., Hoyert, D. L., Xu, J., & Murphy, S. L. (2008). University Press.
Deaths: Final data for 2005. National Vital Statistics MacMillan, P. J., Hart, R. P., Martelli, M. F., & Zasler, N.
Reports, 56(10). D. (2002). Pre-injury status and adaptation
Langlois, J. A., Rutland-Brown, W., & Thomas, K. E. following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury,
(2006). Traumatic brain injury in the United States: 16(1), 41–49.
Emergency department visits, hospitalizations, and Malec, J. F., & Basford, J. S. (1996). Postacute brain
deaths. Atlanta (GA): Centers for Disease Control injury rehabilitation. Archives of Physical Medicine
and Prevention, National Center for Injury and Rehabilitation, 77, 198–207.
Prevention and Control. Malec, J. F. & Moessner, A. M. (2000). Self-awareness,
Langlois, J. A., Rutland-Brown, W., & Wald, M. M. distress, and postacute rehabilitation outcome.
(2006). The epidemiology and impact of traumatic Rehabilitation Psychology, 45, 227–41.
brain injury: A brief overview. Journal of Head Marion, D. W. (1996). Outcome from severe head
Trauma Rehabilitation, 21(5), 375–78. injury. In R. K. Narayan, J. E. Wilberger, & J. T.
Levin, H. S. (1992). Neurobehavioral recovery. Journal Povlishock (Eds.), Neurotrauma (pp. 767–77). New
of Neurotrauma, 9 Suppl 1, S359–373. York: McGraw-Hill.
Levin, H. S. (1993). Neurobehavioral sequelae of closed Marr, A. & Coronado, V. (Eds.). (2004). Central Nervous
head injury. In P. R. Cooper (Ed.), Head injury (pp. System Injury Surveillance Data Submission
525–51). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Standards-2002. Atlanta (GA): Centers for Disease
Levin, H. S. (1995). Prediction of recovery from trau- Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury
matic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 12(5), Prevention and Control.
913–22. Mataro, M., Poca, M. A., Sahuquillo, J., Pedraza, S.,
Levin, H. S. & Eisenberg, H. M. (1991). Management of Ariza, M., Amoros, S., et al. (2001). Neuro-
head injury. Neurobehavioral outcome. Neuro- psychological outcome in relation to the traumatic
surgery Clinics of North America, 2(2), 457–72. coma data bank classification of computed tomo-
Levin, H. S., Gary, H. E., Jr, Eisenberg, H. M., Ruff, R. graphy imaging. Journal of Neurotrauma, 18(9),
M., Barth, J. T., Kreutzer, J., et al. (1990). 869–79.
Neurobehavioral outcome 1 year after severe head Mayeux, R., Ottman, R., Maestre, G., Ngai, C., Tang,
injury. Experience of the Traumatic Coma Data M. X., Ginsberg, H., et al. (1995). Synergistic effects
Bank. Journal of Neurosurgery, 73(5), 699–709. of traumatic head injury and apolipoprotein-
Levin, H. S., High, W. M., Goethe, K. E., Sisson, R. A., epsilon 4 in patients with Alzheimer’s disease.
Overall, J. E., Rhoades, H. M., et al. (1987). The Neurology, 45(3 Pt 1), 555–57.
neurobehavioural rating scale: Assessment of the McCauley, S. R., Levin, H. S., Vanier, M., Mazaux, J. M.,
behavioural sequelae of head injury by the clini- Boake, C., Goldfader, P. R., et al. (2001). The
cian. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Neurobehavioural Rating Scale–Revised:
Psychiatry, 50(2), 183–93. Sensitivity and validity in closed head injury assess-
Levin, H. S., Mattis, S., Ruff, R. M., Marshall, L. F., ment. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
Tabaddor, K., & al., e. (1987). Neurobehavioral Psychiatry, 71(5), 643–51.
outcome following minor head injury: A three- McCrea, M. (2008). Mild traumatic brain injury and
center study. Journal of Neurosurgery, 66, 234–43. post-concussion syndrome: The new evidence base
Levin, H. S., O’Donnell, V. M., & Grossman, R. G. for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Oxford
(1979). The Galveston Orientation and Amnesia University Press.
Test: A practical scale to assess cognition after head McMillan, T. M., Jongen, E. L., & Greenwood, R. J.
injury. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, (1996). Assessment of post-traumatic amnesia
167(11), 675–84. after severe closed head injury: Retrospective or
Lezak, M. D. (1987). Relationships between personal- prospective? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery
ity disorder, social disturbances, and physical and Psychiatry, 60(4), 422–27.
278 forensic neuropsychology

Millar, K., Nicoll, J. A., Thornhill, S., Murray, G. D., & Productivity after neuropsychologically oriented
Teasdale, G. M. (2003). Long-term neuropsycho- milieu rehabilitation. Journal of Head Trauma
logical outcome after head injury: Relation to Rehabilitation, 9(1), 91–102.
APOE genotype. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery Rappaport, M., Hall, K. M., Hopkins, K., Belleza, T., &
& Psychiatry, 74(8), 1047–52. Cope, D. N. (1982). Disability Rating Scale for
Millis, S. R., Rosenthal, M., Novack, T. A., Sherer, M., severe head trauma: Coma to community. Archives
Nick, T. G., Kreutzer, J. S., et al. (2001). Long-term of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 63, 118–23.
neuropsychological outcome after traumatic brain Riley, G. A. (2007). Stress and depression in family
injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, carers following traumatic brain injury: The influ-
16(4), 343–55. ence of beliefs about difficult behaviours. Clinical
Mittenberg, W. & Strauman, S. (2000). Diagnosis of Rehabilitation, 21(1), 82–88.
mild head injury and the postconcussion syn- Roebuck-Spencer, T. M., Banos, J., Sherer, M., &
drome. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, Novack, T. (2010). Neurobehavioral aspects of trau-
15(2), 783–91. matic brain injury sustained in adulthood. In
Mosenthal, A. C., Lavery, R. F., Addis, M., Kaul, S., S. Hunter & J. Donders (Eds.), Principles and prac-
Ross, S., Marburger, R., et al. (2002). Isolated trau- tice of lifespan developmental neuropsychology (pp.
matic brain injury: Age is an independent predic- 329–344). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
tor of mortality and early outcome. Journal of Rogers, J. M. & Read, C. A. (2007). Psychiatric comor-
Trauma, 52(5), 907–911. bidity following traumatic brain injury. Brain
Murray, G. D., Teasdale, G. M., Braakman, R., Cohadon, Injury, 21(13–14), 1321–33.
F., Dearden, M., Iannotti, F., et al. (1999). The Ruff, R. M., Marshall, L. F., Crouch, J., Klauber, M. R.,
European Brain Injury Consortium survey of head Levin, H. S., Barth, J., et al. (1993). Predictors of
injuries. Acta Neurochirurgica Supplementum, outcome following severe head trauma: Follow-up
141(3), 223–36. data from the Traumatic Coma Data Bank. Brain
Oddy, M., Humphrey, M., & Uttley, D. (1978). Injury, 7(2), 101–111.
Subjective impairment and social recovery after Ruff, R. M., Young, D., Gautille, T., Marshall, L. F.,
closed head injury. Journal of Neurology, Barth, J., Jane, J. A., et al. (1991). Verbal learning
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 41(7), 611–616. deficits following severe head injury: Heterogeneity
Ommaya, A. K. & Gennarelli, T. A. (1974). Cerebral in recovery over 1 year. Journal of Neurosurgery, 75,
concussion and traumatic unconsciousness. S50–58.
Correlation of experimental and clinical observa- Russell, W. R. (1932). Cerebral involvement in head
tions of blunt head injuries. Brain, 97(4), 633–54. injury. A study on the examination of two hundred
Pastorek, N. J., Hannay, H. J., & Contant, C. S. (2004). cases. Brain, 55, 549–603.
Prediction of global outcome with acute neuropsy- Russell, W. R. & Smith, A. (1961). Post-traumatic
chological testing following closed-head injury. amnesia in closed head injury. Archives of
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Neurology, 5, 4–17.
Society, 10(6), 807–817. Rutland-Brown, W., Langlois, J. A., Thomas, K. E., &
Plassman, B. L., Havlik, R. J., Steffens, D. C., Helms, M. Xi, Y. L. (2006). Incidence of traumatic brain injury
J., Newman, T. N., Drosdick, D., et al. (2000). in the United States, 2003. Journal of Head Trauma
Documented head injury in early adulthood and Rehabilitation, 21(6), 544–48.
risk of Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Sander, A. M., Fuchs, K. L., High, W. M., Jr, Hall,
Neurology, 55(8), 1158–66. K. M., Kreutzer, J. S., & Rosenthal, M. (1999). The
Ponsford, J. L., Olver, J. H., Curran, C., & Ng, K. (1995). Community Integration Questionnaire revisited:
Prediction of employment status 2 years after trau- An assessment of factor structure and validity.
matic brain injury. Brain Injury, 9(1), 11–20. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
Poon, W. S., Zhu, X. L., Ng, S. C., & Wong, G. K. (2005). 80(10), 1303–1308.
Predicting one year clinical outcome in traumatic Sander, A. M., Kreutzer, J., Rosenthal, M., Delmonico,
brain injury (TBI) at the beginning of rehabilita- R., & Young, M. E. (1996). A multicenter longitudi-
tion. Acta Neurochirurgica Supplementum, 93, nal investigation of return to work and community
207–208. integration following traumatic brain injury. Jour-
Prigatano, G. P., Altman, I. M., & O’Brien, K. P. (1990). nal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 11(5), 70–84.
Behavioral limitations that traumatic-brain-injured Sander, A. M., Roebuck, T. M., Struchen, M. A., Sherer,
patients tend to underestimate. The Clinical M., & High, W. M., Jr. (2001). Long-term mainte-
Neuropsychologist, 4(2), 163–76. nance of gains obtained in post-acute rehabilita-
Prigatano, G. P., Klonoff, P. S., O’Brien, K. P., Altman, I. tion by persons with traumatic brain injury. Journal
M., Amin, K., Chiapello, D., et al. (1994). of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16(4), 1–19.
Moderate and Severe Traumatic Brain Injury 279

Sander, A. M., Seel, R. T., Kreutzer, J. S., Hall, K. M., Sherer, M. & Novack, T. (2003). Neuropsychological
High, W. M., Jr., & Rosenthal, M. (1997). Agreement assessment after traumatic brain injury in adults.
between persons with traumatic brain injury and In G. P. Prigatano & N. H. Pliskin (Eds.), Clinical
their relatives regarding psychosocial outcome neuropsychology and cost outcome research
using the Community Integration Questionnaire. (pp. 39–60). New York: Psychology Press, Inc.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Sherer, M., Novack, T. A., Sander, A. M., Struchen, M.
78(4), 353–57. A., Alderson, A., & Thompson, R. N. (2002).
Satz, P., Zaucha, K., Forney, D. L., McCleary, C., Neuropsychological assessment and employment
Asarnow, R. F., Light, R., et al. (1998). outcome after traumatic brain injury: A review.
Neuropsychological, psychosocial and vocational The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16(2), 157–78.
correlates of the Glasgow Outcome Scale at 6 Sherer, M., Sander, A. M., Nick, T. G., High, W. M., Jr.,
months post-injury: A study of moderate to severe Malec, J. F., & Rosenthal, M. (2002). Early cogni-
traumatic brain injury patients. Brain Injury, 12(7), tive status and productivity outcome after trau-
555–67. matic brain injury: Findings from the TBI model
Schofield, P. W., Tang, M., Marder, K., Bell, K., Dooneief, systems. Archives of Physical Medicine and
G., Chun, M., et al. (1997). Alzheimer’s disease Rehabilitation, 83(2), 183–92.
after remote head injury: An incidence study. Sherer, M., Struchen, M. A., Yablon, S. A., Wang, Y., &
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, Nick, T. G. (2008). Comparison of indices of
62(2), 119–24. traumatic brain injury severity: Glasgow Coma
Seel, R. T., Kreutzer, J. S., Rosenthal, M., Hammond, F. Scale, length of coma and post-traumatic amnesia.
M., Corrigan, J. D., & Black, K. (2003). Depression Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
after traumatic brain injury: A National Institute 79(6), 678–85.
on Disability and Rehabilitation Research Model Sherer, M., Yablon, S. A., Nakase-Richardson, R., &
Systems multicenter investigation. Archives of Nick, T. G. (2008). Effect of severity of post-
Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84(2), traumatic confusion and its constituent symptoms
177–84. on outcome after traumatic brain injury.
Sherer, M., Bergloff, P., High, W., Jr., & Nick, T. G. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
(1999). Contribution of functional ratings to 89(1), 42–47.
prediction of long-term employment outcome Shores, E. A., Marosszeky, J. E., Sandanam, J., &
after traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 13(12), Batchelor, J. (1986). Preliminary validation of a
973–81. clinical scale for measuring the duration of post-
Sherer, M., Bergloff, P., Levin, E., High, W. M., Jr, Oden, traumatic amnesia. Medical Journal of Australia,
K. E., & Nick, T. G. (1998). Impaired awareness 144(11), 569–72.
and employment outcome after traumatic brain Signorini, D. F., Andrews, P. J., Jones, P. A., Wardlaw, J.
injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, M., & Miller, J. D. (1999). Predicting survival using
13(5), 52–61. simple clinical variables: A case study in traumatic
Sherer, M., Hart, T., Nick, T. G., Whyte, J., Thompson, brain injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery
R. N., & Yablon, S. A. (2003). Early impaired and Psychiatry, 66(1), 20–25.
self-awareness after traumatic brain injury. Archives Spreen, O. & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neu-
of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84(2), ropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and
168–76. commentary (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford
Sherer, M., Hart, T., Whyte, J., Nick, T. G., & Yablon, S. University Press.
A. (2005). Neuroanatomic basis of impaired self- Stein, S. C. (1996). Outcome from moderate head
awareness after traumatic brain injury: Findings injury. In R. K. Narayan, J. E. Wilberger & J. T.
from early computed tomography. Journal of Head Povlishock (Eds.), Neurotrauma (pp. 755–65). New
Trauma Rehabilitation, 20(4), 287–300. York: McGraw-Hill.
Sherer, M., Nakase-Thompson, R., Yablon, S. A., & Stuss, D. T., Binns, M. A., Carruth, F. G., Levine, B.,
Gontkovsky, S. T. (2005). Multidimensional assess- Brandys, C. E., Moulton, R. J., et al. (1999). The
ment of acute confusion after traumatic brain acute period of recovery from traumatic brain
injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and injury: Posttraumatic amnesia or posttraumatic
Rehabilitation, 86(5), 896–904. confusional state? Journal of Neurosurgery, 90(4),
Sherer, M., Nick, T. G., Sander, A. M., Hart, T., Hanks, 635–43.
R., Rosenthal, M., et al. (2003). Race and produc- Susman, M., DiRusso, S. M., Sullivan, T., Risucci, D.,
tivity outcome after traumatic brain injury: Nealon, P., Cuff, S., et al. (2002). Traumatic brain
Influence of confounding factors. Journal of Head injury in the elderly: Increased mortality and worse
Trauma Rehabilitation, 18(5), 408–24. functional outcome at discharge despite lower
280 forensic neuropsychology

injury severity. Journal of Trauma, 53(2), 219–23; van Zomeren, A. H. & Deelman, B. G. (1978). Long-
discussion 223–14. term recovery of visual reaction time after closed
Symonds, C. P. (1937). Mental disorder following head head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery
injury. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, and Psychiatry, 41(5), 452–57.
30, 1081–94. Vespa, P. M., Boscardin, W. J., Hovda, D. A., McArthur,
Symonds, C. P. & Russell, W. R. (1943). Accidental D. L., Nuwer, M. R., Martin, N. A., et al. (2002).
head injuries: Prognosis in service patients. Lancet, Early and persistent impaired percent alpha vari-
7–10. ability on continuous electroencephalography
Tabaddor, K., Mattis, S., & Zazula, T. (1984). Cognitive monitoring as predictive of poor outcome after
sequelae and recovery course after moderate and traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurosurgery,
severe head injury. Neurosurgery, 14(6), 701–708. 97(1), 84–92.
Taylor, C. A., Saint-Hilaire, M. H., Cupples, L. A., Wardlaw, J. M., Easton, V. J., & Statham, P. (2002).
Thomas, C. A., Burchard, A. E., Feldman, R. G., Which CT features help predict outcome after head
et al. (1999). Environmental, medical, and family injury? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and
history risk factors for Parkinson’s disease: A New Psychiatry, 72(2), 188–92; discussion 151.
England-based case control study. American Wedekind, C. & Lippert-Gruner, M. (2005). Long-term
Journal of Medical Genetics, 88(6), 742–49. outcome in severe traumatic brain injury is signifi-
Teasdale, G. & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma cantly influenced by brainstem involvement. Brain
and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Injury, 19(9), 681–84.
Lancet, 2(7872), 81–84. Whyte, J., Cifu, D., Dikmen, S., & Temkin, N. (2001).
Teasdale, T. W. & Engberg, A. W. (2005). Subjective Prediction of functional outcomes after traumatic
well-being and quality of life following traumatic brain injury: A comparison of two measures of
brain injury in adults: A long-term population- duration of unconsciousness. Archives of Physical
based follow-up. Brain Injury, 19(12), 1041–48. Medicine and Rehabilitation, 82(10), 1355–59.
Testa, J. A., Malec, J. F., Moessner, A. M., & Brown, A. Willer, B. S., Ottenbacher, K. J., & Coad, M. L. (1994).
W. (2005). Outcome after traumatic brain injury: The community integration questionnaire. A com-
Effects of aging on recovery. Archives of Physical parative examination. American Journal of Physical
Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86(9), 1815–23. Medicine and Rehabilitation, 73(2), 103–111.
Thurman, D. J., Alverson, C., Dunn, K. A., Guerrero, J., Willer, B. S., Rosenthal, M., Kreutzer, J. S., Gordon, W.
& Sniezek, J. E. (1999). Traumatic brain injury in A., & Rempel, R. (1993). Assessment of commu-
the United States: A public health perspective. nity integration following rehabilitation for trau-
Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 14(6), 602– matic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma
615. Rehabilitation, 8(2), 75–87.
Thurman, D. J. & Guerrero, J. (1999). Trends in hospi- Williams, D. H., Levin, H. S., & Eisenberg, H. M.
talization associated with traumatic brain injury. (1990). Mild head injury classification. Neuro-
Journal of the American Medical Association, surgery, 27(3), 422–28.
282(10), 954–57. Yablon, S. (1996). Posttraumatic seizures. In L. J. Horn
Traumatic Brain Injury Model Systems National Data & N. D. Zasler (Eds.), Medical rehabilitation of
Center (1999). Traumatic Brain Injury Model traumatic brain injury (pp. 363–94). Philadelphia:
Systems National Database Syllabus. West Orange, Hanley & Belfus, Inc.
New Jersey : TBI Model Systems National Data Yu, E. J., Yablon, S., Ivanhoe, C., & Boake, C. (1995).
Center. Posttraumatic hydrocephalus: Incidence and
Trudel, T. M., Tryon, W. W., & Purdum, C. M. (1998). outcome following screening of consecutive
Awareness of disability and long-term outcome admissions. Archives of Physical Medicine and
after traumatic brain injury. Rehabilitation Psycho- Rehabilitation, 76, 1041.
logy, 53, 267–81. Zhang, J., Jiang, J. Y., Zhong, T., Yu, M., & Zhu, C.
Uryu, K., Chen, X. H., Martinez, D., Browne, K. D., (2001). Outcome of 2,284 cases with acute trau-
Johnson, V. E., Graham, D. I., et al. (2007). Multiple matic brain injury. Chinese Journal of Traumato-
proteins implicated in neurodegenerative diseases logy, 4, 152–55.
accumulate in axons after brain trauma in humans.
Experimental Neurology 208(2), 185–92.
11
Neurotoxic Injury
KAREN I. BOLLA

INTRODUCTION group on the chronic effects of organic solvents


This chapter focuses on the determination of on the central nervous system categorized the
neurotoxic injury subsequent to possible expo- neurobehavioral effects associated with solvent
sure to specific neurotoxicants. A neurotoxin is a exposure. These categories were based on the
chemical or substance that is harmful or fatal to severity of symptoms, and categories are pre-
the central nervous system of biological organ- sented in Table 11.1).
isms when introduced in certain quantities or Painter’s syndrome originated in 1979 when
doses (Loomis & Hayes, 1996). It is important to Arlein-Soborg et al. (1979) from Denmark
remember that even the most innocuous of reported a brain syndrome that they termed
substances can have harmful effects if introduced chronic painter’s syndrome. This report was based
into the body in high enough doses. For example, on 70 house painters who had an average of 24
even water can be toxic if ingested in large enough years of job-related exposure to solvents and
quantities. Also, a chemical can be harmful or who were referred for medical evaluation for sus-
have adverse effects that are species specific. That pected organic solvent intoxication or dementia.
is, a chemical that is lethal to an insect may be Following the release of this report, governments
harmless to a human. in the Scandinavian countries began to compen-
The associations between neurologic symp- sate workers for cognitive and psychiatric disabil-
toms and exposure to some specific chemicals are ities that were determined to be work related.
still controversial. In fact, much of the research on Although its impact was enormous, this initial
the neurological sequelae of neurotoxicants has description of chronic solvent encephalopathy did
been litigation-fueled (Lees-Haley & Williams, not include a control group, made no attempt to
1997). While the literature continues to debate control for background variables such as level of
whether low-level chronic exposure results in education or intelligence, and based its conclu-
neurotoxic insult, it is generally accepted that sions solely on clinical judgments about the
higher levels of exposure to certain chemicals can subjective complaints of patients seeking medical
result in neurocognitive sequelae. Even with treatment. For the most part, reports of neuro-
higher levels of exposure, the association between behavioral difficulties in workers exposed to
exposure and central nervous system effects may solvents are of questionable validity because many
be reported by some studies, while other studies of the studies published in the scientific literature
fail to find a similar relationship. A prime example contain methodological limitations. Inconsist-
of this is the relationship between solvent expo- encies in the literature can be attributed to differ-
sure and toxic encephalopathy. ences in the populations studied (control and
Both the scientific and lay communities exposed groups) with respect to pre-morbid level
accept that exposure to solvents causes chronic of cognitive and psychiatric functioning, age of
painter’s syndrome or solvent encephalopathy. the sample, and severity of the exposure (i.e.,
This well-known syndrome is characterized by intensity and duration). Furthermore, many stud-
headache, fatigue, difficulty concentrating, poor ies make no attempt to control for alcohol use,
memory, depressed mood, sleep disturbance, associated medical disorders (e.g., diabetes,
decreased libido, and irritability. In fact, in 1985, hypertension), educational level, or level of intel-
a World Health Organization (WHO) working ligence. For example, when Gade et al., attempted
282 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 11.1. CATEGORIES OF THE T YPES OF SOLVENT ENCEPHALOPATHY

Type I Subjective nonspecific encephalopathy symptoms only


Type IIa Sustained personality and mood change. Negative neurobehavioral findings. Unclear if symptoms
are reversible.
Type IIb Impairment of intellectual function documented by objective neurobehavioral test results and
possible mild neurologic signs. After removal from exposure, symptoms should remain stable or
improve, not become worse.
Type III Dementia with neurologic signs, neurobehavioral deficits and possible neuroradiologic findings
(e.g., frontal lobe atrophy). Related to repeated severe exposure (i.e., paint huffers). May be
irreversible but generally does not progress.

Cranmer and Goldberg, 1987.

to replicate the data of Arlien-Soborg et al. (Gade found statistically significant dose-related associ-
et al., 1988; Arlien-Soborg et al., 1982), they added ations between exposure to solvents and decre-
a control group that was matched to the workers ments in neurobehavioral test scores although the
in terms of sex, age, education, and intelligence. magnitude of the effects suggests that they are
When they re-analyzed the test data of 20 patients probably clinically insignificant (Cherry et al.,
reported on by Arlien-Soborg et al. (1982) using 1985; Maizlish et al., 1987; Gade et al., 1988;
this control group, the neurometric test scores no Triebig et al., 1988; Bolla et al., 1990 ; Bleecker
longer showed any significant difference between et al., 1991; Bolla et al., 1995). It therefore appears
the exposed group and the controls. A similar that, while 10 years or more of exposure may
result was obtained by Cherry et al. in a group of increase an individual’s risk of developing neu-
workers exposed to methyl-N-butyl ketone robehavioral symptoms, few significant adverse
(MBK) and toluene for an average of 11.7 years effects may ever actually develop. This finding
(Cherry et al., 1985). When these authors matched emphasizes that if an individual was exposed to a
a control group of workers to their exposed work- neurotoxicant, the exposure would have had to be
ers on the basis of a verbal intelligence test of significant intensity and duration to cause
(NAART), the association between poor neuro- symptoms. If there is no evidence of exposure, no
behavioral test performance and exposure to relationship can be established between symp-
solvents disappeared. Also, many studies use toms and exposure to toxicants. Also, if an indi-
biased sampling by allowing subjects to self-select vidual was exposed to sufficiently elevated levels
into their study groups; for example, they may of neurotoxicants, they should have acute physical
report only on workers who present to a medical symptoms, such as headaches, dizziness, and
clinic with symptoms or complaints, or only on fatigue, prior to developing chronic symptoms,
workers involved in litigation (Morrow et al., such as neurocognitive deficits. Recent evidence
1989; Morrow et al., 1990). Similar methodologi- suggests a caveat to this pattern. As discussed later
cal mistakes are also seen in investigations of in this chapter, neurotoxicant exposure may result
potential neurotoxins other than solvents, such as in “accelerated aging” of neurological function
the heavy metal, manganese (Lees-Haley et al., and structure.
2004). Studies that use such nonrandom samples As with any diagnostic process, the ability to
are fatally flawed in their methodology and their make a differential diagnosis between neurotoxi-
findings cannot be viewed as scientifically sound. cant exposure, neurological disease, psychiatric
Nevertheless, these faulty studies can be used by disturbance, or malingering (see Dr. Larrabee’s
attorneys during litigation to influence judges, chapter) is based on the combined evidence taken
juries, and the media. from the industrial hygiene data, occupational,
Two critical reviews of the literature (World medical, social, and academic histories, as well as
Health Organization, 1985; Cranmer & Goldberg, the physical and neurological examinations,
1986) concluded that at least 10 years of exposure biological monitoring, nerve conduction studies,
are necessary to produce an increased risk of EEG, CT/MRI, and the neuropsychological evalu-
neurobehavioral sequelae from solvent exposure. ation. It is therefore essential that patients with
A number of methodologically sound studies potential neurotoxic exposure are evaluated by an
Neurotoxic Injury 283

interdisciplinary team of health care professionals associated with Hurricane Katrina (Riggs et al.,
with expertise in neurotoxicology. Ideally, this 2008). As with other neurotoxicants, the link
“dream team” should be comprised of a neuro- between mold and health effects is still controver-
psychologist, a neurologist, an occupational sial. A statement published by the American
medicine physician/internist, an industrial College of Occupational and Environmental
hygienist, and other medical specialties as neces- Health Medicine (2002) concludes that there is no
sary (i.e., pulmonologist). scientific evidence linking serious human health
Whereas some health effects such as pulmo- effects, which include neurocognitive effects, to
nary distress and gastrointestinal symptoms are inhaled mycotoxins (a secondary metabolite of
noticed immediately by the affected individual, mold) in the home, school, or office environment.
neurocognitive effects may go unrecognized while Moreover, in 2004, the Institute of Medicine
the individual is acutely ill. Acute, high-level (IOM) concluded that mold exposure is associ-
exposure to a toxicant often results in clearly iden- ated with respiratory illness and that immuno-
tifiable signs (e.g., delirium, seizures, flu-like compromised persons are at risk for fungal-related
symptoms, or unconsciousness), but the residual infections (IOM Committee on Damp Indoor
effects, involving alterations in cognition, mood, Spaces and Health, 2004). Of note, research on the
and personality, are usually quite subtle. The most possible neurocognitive sequelae of mold expo-
common clinical presentation is one of poor con- sure is still needed as there are currently no avail-
centration, short-term memory loss, depressed able scientifically rigorous studies (see Dr. Binder’s
mood, anxiety, restlessness, loss of interest in chapter for further discussion of toxic mold).
work and hobbies, decreased libido, irritability, An in-depth discussion of the physical, cogni-
headaches, weakness, sleep disturbances ranging tive, and neuropsychiatric symptoms associated
from insomnia to somnambulism, and symptoms with significant exposure to specific neurotoxicants
consistent with peripheral neuropathy. Since this and drugs of abuse is beyond the scope of this
constellation of symptoms is nonspecific and chapter but several reviews are available (Bolla &
could be related to a host of neurological as well as Roca, 1994; Rosenstock et al., 2004; Hartman DE,
psychiatric etiologies, determining if these symp- 1995; Bolla & Cadet, 2007). However, Tables 11.2–
toms are a result of neurotoxic injury can be 11.4 summarize the main categories of identified
extraordinarily challenging. neurotoxicants and the physical and cognitive dif-
ficulties generally associated with significant expo-
CATEGORIES OF TOXINS LEADING sure to these substances (Bolla & Roca, 1994; Bolla
TO REFERRAL FOR EVALUATION & Cadet, 2007). Table 11.5 presents symptoms
The main categories of chemicals that have been associated with drugs of abuse (Bolla & Cadet,
associated with CNS injury are the heavy metals 2007). Neurotoxicant-symptom relationships sum-
(e.g., lead, mercury), solvents (e.g., mixed solvents, marized in the tables were extracted from a review
toluene), pesticides (organophosphates), and of numerous epidemiological studies of effects of
gases (e.g., carbon monoxide). In addition, recent various neurotoxicants on the nervous system. The
work has shown that very heavy doses of drugs of reader is cautioned that, as with any brain injury,
abuse (e.g., alcohol, cocaine, MDMA, marijuana, there is large individual variability in the develop-
methamphetamine) are also associated with ment of symptoms and therefore, not all symptoms
lower scores on neuropsychological measures will be present in a single individual. Also readily
(Gonzalez, 2007; Bolla et al., 1998; Bolla et al., apparent in these tables is that the symptoms result-
1999; Bolla et al., 2002) and abnormalities in brain ing from significant exposure to a specific neuro-
activity (Bolla et al., 2003; Eldreth et al., 2004; toxicant are quite generic. For example, several
Nestor et al., 2008) and brain density (Matochik neurotoxicants can produce memory, perceptuo-
et al., 2003; Matochik et al., 2005) and neural motor, and emotional symptoms.
integrity (Cowan et al., 2008) when measured In summary, individuals with known signifi-
using neuroimaging methods. cant neurotoxicant exposures have reported
Most recently, so-called “toxic mold” in homes, cognitive, physical, and affective changes. With
schools, and office buildings has been a promi- acute, high-level exposure, the physical symptoms
nent topic in the news and is increasingly becom- appear to be immediate, prominent, and
ing the basis for litigation. Notably, mold growth primary. Other primary symptoms may include
has become a major public health concern in confusion and mood alterations. Conversely,
the greater New Orleans area after the flooding with low-level exposure, mood and cognitive
TABLE 11.2. SYMPTOMS ASSOCIATED WITH HEAVY METAL EXPOSURE

Metal Associated Symptoms Neurologic Findings Neurocognitive Findings

Aluminum Respiratory dysfunction Cognitive decline; halting speech; ataxia Personality change; fatigue; memory;
attention/executive function

Arsenic
Acute GI distress; respiratory distress; cardiac Headache; nervousness; vertigo; paralysis; seizures; Verbal memory; drowsiness; confusion; stupor;
distress; elevated temperature; Mee’s lines myelopathy; hyperreflexia; neuropathy organic psychosis resembling paranoid
schizophrenia; delirium; agitation; emotional
lability
Chronic Abdominal pain; dermatitis; increased risk Headaches; fatigue; restlessness; vertigo; cognitive Verbal memory; drowsiness; confusion; stupor;
of cancer decline; visual changes or optic neuropathy; organic psychosis resembling paranoid
seizures; painful sensorimotor peripheral schizophrenia; delirium; agitation; emotional
neuropathy lability

Lead
Acute
Children Respiratory distress; flu-like symptoms Lethargy; cognitive decline; gait disorder; ataxia; Lethargy; hyperactivity
seizures
Adults GI distress; miscarriages; joint pain; flu-like Fatigue; delirium; seizures Delirium
symptoms

Lead (continued)
Chronic
Children Changes in auditory threshold; behavioral Learning disorders Intelligence; reaction time; perceptual motor
problems; cognitive decline; learning performance; memory; reading; spelling;
disabilities; attention deficit hyperactivity auditory processing; attention;
disorder (ADHD)
Adults Miscarriage/stillbirth; arthralgia; anemia; Scoptic visual effects; depression; irritability; sleep Depression; apathy; confusion; fatigue; tension;
hypertension; gout; renal effects; disturbance; decline in libido; cognition (learning restlessness; anger; visual intelligence;
decreased sperm count and memory); fasciculations; paresthesias; visuomotor; general intelligence; memory;
sensorimotor polyneuropathy; changes in auditory psychomotor speed; rate of learning; attention;
threshold visuoconstruction; manual dexterity
Manganese Anorexia; manganese pneumonia Headaches; apathy; fatigue; depression; Sleepiness; asthenia; anorexia; impaired speech;
hyperexcitability; dysarthria; psychotic behavior; insomnia; hallucinations; mental excitement;
tremor; gait disorders; microphagia; parkinsonism aggression; mania; dementia; frontal lobe
dysfunction; emotional lability; parkinsonism;
judgment; memory

Mercury
Inorganic
Acute Bronchial irritation; chills; gingivitis; GI Weakness; irritability; delirium; psychosis Confusion
distress; bloody diarrhea; brownish
mouth lesions; metallic breath;
respiratory distress; renal failures
Chronic Salivary gland swelling; excessive salivation; Shyness; fatigue; weakness; personality changes; Irritability; avoidant behavior; overly sensitive
gingivitis; renal dysfunction hyper-irritability; insomnia; depression; cognitive interpersonal behavior; shyness; depression;
decline; visual disturbance; intentional tremor; lassitude; fatigue; agitation; visuospatial; visual
parkinsonism; seizures; painful paresthesias; memory; reaction time; learning
peripheral polyneuropathy (sensorimotor
axonopathy)
Organic Primarily affects the nervous system Cognitive decline; neurasthenia; paresthesias;
ataxia; restricted visual fields; cortical blindness;
peripheral polyneuropathy; intention tremor;
motor neuron disease (ALS-like)

Bolla and Roca, 1994; Bolla and Cadet, 2003.


TABLE 11.3. SYMPTOMS ASSOCIATED WITH ORGANIC SOLVENT EXPOSURE
Organic Solvents Associated Symptoms Neurologic Findings Neurocognitive Findings
Mixtures Irritant effects; contact dermatitis Headaches; fatigue; irritability; depression; Executive functions; eye-hand coordination/
sleep difficulties; cognitive decline; decreased manual dexterity; visuoconstruction; odor
olfaction; peripheral neuropathy; myopathy identification

Carbon Disulfide Pulmonary and dermal irritants; cardiac Headaches; irritability; cognitive decline; Psychosis; depression; personality change;
effects; toxic threshold lowered in psychosis; delirium; hearing loss; loss of insomnia; eye-hand coordination; motor
alcoholism; diabetes mellitus; renal and corneal reflex; parkinsonism; peripheral speed, emotionality; energy level; psychomotor
hepatic diseases polyneuropathy performance; reaction time; vigilance;
visuomotor functions, construction; retarded
speech

Carbon Tetrachloride GI distress; hiccups; liver and kidney Intoxication; headaches; vertigo; delirium; Lethargy; confusion
damage; toxic threshold lowered in seizures; parkinsonism; optic atrophy; visual
alcoholism; obesity; diabetes; liver and difficulties
kidney disease

Ethylene Glycol Renal effects; cardiopulmonary effects Restlessness; agitation; seizures; absent corneal Fatigue; personality changes; depression
reflexes; coma

Methyl Alcohol (methanol) GI distress Headache; weakness; incoordination; delirium;


hallucinations; visual loss; stupor; seizures;
parkinsonism; death

Methyl-N-Butyl Euphoria Weight loss; sensorimotor polyneuropathy Personality changes; depression


Ketone (MBK)

N-Hexane Euphoria Headaches; poor appetite; mild euphoria; Headaches; appetite; depression; euphoria
peripheral polyneuropathy
Toluene (Methyl Benzene) Pulmonary effects; cardiac effects Euphoria; fatigue; ataxia; dizziness; tremor; Acute exposure: Excitation at lower or shorter
cognitive decline; seizures; delirium concentrations; depression at higher
concentrations; fatigue; confusion; anxiety;
reaction time; concentration

Olfaction; optic atrophy; hearing loss; peripheral Chronic exposure: Huffing exhileration; euphoria;
neuropathy (in cases w/associated N-hexane disinhibition; performance IQ; memory; motor
exposure); alcohol intolerance control; dementia; attention; visuospatial
function; apathy; flattened affect

Trichlorethylene (TCE) Cardiopulmonary effects; toxic threshold Headaches; insomnia; fatigue; anxiety; trigeminal Headaches; dizziness; fatigue; diplopia; alcohol
reduced with alcohol nerve damage; neuro-ophthalmological intolerance; neurasthenia; anxiety; lability;
findings; alcohol intolerance; cognitive insomnia; concentration; manual dexterity;
decline; hearing loss; peripheral neuropathy visuospatial accuracy; reaction time; memory

Bolla and Roca, 1994; Bolla and Cadet, 2003.


288 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 11.4. SYMPTOMS ASSOCIATED WITH EXPOSURE TO


MISCELLANEOUS NEUROTOXINS

Neurotoxin Associated Symptoms Neurologic Findings Neurocognitive Findings

Carbon Monoxide None Headache; irritability; “Apathetic mask-like facial


dizziness; cognitive expression”; dementia;
decline; impaired vision; amnesia; disorientation;
blindness; deafness; irritability; cognitive
seizures; parkinsonism; efficiency and flexibility;
coma verbal and visual memory,
spatial deficits

Organophosphates GI distress; excessive Headache; fatigue; dizziness; Confusion; fatigue;


sweating and salivation; decreased consciousness; headache; vigilance;
hypothermia; liver sleep disturbance; concentration; information
dysfunction cognitive decline; blurred processing; depression;
vision; absent papillary anxiety; irritability;
response; muscular memory and learning;
fasciculations; tremor; visuoconstruction; tension;
delayed polyneuropathy restlessness; anxiety;
(OPIDP) apprehension

Bolla and Roca, 1994; Bolla and Cadet, 2003.

TABLE 11.5. SYMPTOMS ASSOCIATED WITH DRUGS OF ABUSE


Drug Neurological Symptoms Neurocognitive Findings
Alcohol Peripheral neuropathy Memory; executive function; visuospatial
function
Amphetamine Meth-induced psychosis; hyperactivity; Verbal memory; executive function; attention
euphoria; headaches; confusion;
vasculitis; strokes
Cocaine Vasoconstriction; stroke Executive function; visuoperception;
psychomotor speed; manual dexterity
MDMA Psychiatric disturbance; sleep disturbance; Visual and verbal memory;
Ectasy pupillary dilation; tremors; possible executive function
seizures; strokes
Marijuana Vasoconstriction Memory; executive function; psychomotor
speed; manual dexterity
Opiates Peripheral neuropathy Memory; executive function

Bolla and Cadet, 2003.

changes appear to be the primary symptoms. Once health effects have been detected, it can
Individuals exposed to neurotoxicants have be problematic to relate these in a causal fashion
reported difficulties in the areas of concentration to a specific neurotoxicant exposure. The diagno-
and short-term memory. These individuals also sis of neurotoxicant-related damage is generally
complain of disorientation, depression, irritabil- one of exclusion. Therefore, other causes of cen-
ity, fatigue and sleep disturbances, decreased tral and peripheral nervous system dysfunction
libido, headaches, and weakness (Bolla & Cadet, must be ruled out, and a history of significant
2007). exposure must be substantiated.
Neurotoxic Injury 289

PRINCIPLES OF on vacations. Biological plausibility is absent if the


N E U ROTOX I C O L O G Y patient claims that his headaches are related to
In behavioral neurotoxicology, the ability to make solvent exposure and reports that these headaches
accurate causal-inferences about a possible expo- last all day and night, seven days a week, and never
sure and the presence of adverse neurologic symp- remit.
toms relies on how well the available data complies It has generally been accepted that, except for
with the principles of neurotoxicology. These carbon monoxide (CO) exposure and organophos-
include the presence of a dose-response relation- phate (i.e., pesticide and nerve-agent exposure)
ship, the biological plausibility of symptoms, and exposures, neurological and other physical symp-
the occurrence of symptoms in close proximity to toms will occur acutely at the time of high-level
an exposure. Of paramount importance is the pres- exposure. Once the source of the exposure is elimi-
ence of a dose-response relationship. This encom- nated, these symptoms will begin to remit or at the
passes the idea that as the degree of exposure least remain constant over time. The proximity to
increases (intensity/concentration or duration) the exposure principle posits that symptoms will not
symptoms (physical, cognitive, and psychological) increase after removal from exposure and do not
should also become more severe. That is, there develop after a delayed time interval. As mentioned
should be a direct relationship between the dose of previously, the only two chemicals where symp-
a chemical agent and the response obtained. toms may worsen following significant exposure
Therefore, if a patient reports exposure to a chemi- are in cases of CO and organophosphate exposure
cal in a well-ventilated area, for a short duration of (Parkinson et al., 2002; Senanayake et al., 1982).
time, and claims subsequent severe neurocognitive This viewpoint may need to be modified in
difficulties, then the symptoms are out of propor- light of the availability of new information. In
tion to the level of exposure. recent years, a number of important studies have
Another principle of neurotoxicology relates documented that lead exposure in adults may
to biological plausibility. For example, if all of the hasten cognitive decline with advancing age. In
animal studies show that an extremely high level three longitudinal studies of occupational or envi-
of exposure to a specific chemical does not pro- ronmental lead exposure in 3,052 adults, higher
duce any health effects in animals, then there tibia lead level was consistently associated with
would be little reason to suspect that health effects persistent and possibly progressive changes in cog-
would be produced in humans at a lower level. nition and brain structure long after exposure had
Another prime example is the waxing and waning ended and lead levels had declined (Schwartz et al.,
of specific symptoms with solvent exposure. Acute 2000; Schwartz et al., 2001; Schwartz et al., 2005).
significant exposure to solvents almost always Older adults with higher tibia lead levels showed
results in headaches which then resolve once greater longitudinal declines in the domains of
removed from the source of exposure. Therefore, verbal memory and learning, visual memory, and
the characteristic pattern of solvent-induced executive function. This line of evidence provides
headaches is that they develop after the individual convincing evidence that adult lead exposure could
arrives at work in the morning and resolve after modify the rate and slope of cognitive decline that
they leave work, on the weekends, and when away occurs naturally with aging (Figure 11.1).

Adult Exposure
Cognitive Function

Neurotoxicant
A D
Exposure
B C

Dementia Threshold

0 15 25 35 45 55 65 75 85 95 105
Age (years)

FIGURE 11.1: A decline in cognitive function may begin earlier (B); occur at a steeper slope (C),
or both (A), normal cognitive decline dotted line (D).
290 forensic neuropsychology

New state-of-the art neuroimaging techniques water, soil, and environmental samples in homes,
are also increasing our understanding of the schools, office buildings, and industrial sites. These
effects of neurotoxicants on the brain. For instance, samples are analyzed by sophisticated analytical
higher tibia lead was associated with a decrease in chemistry methods (e.g., mass spectroscopy) to
volume of total brain, parietal white and gray ascertain the composition of chemicals in the sam-
matter, temporal white matter, and the cingulate ples and if the concentrations of the identified
gyrus, and insula (Stewart et al., 2006). These chemicals are above the acceptable threshold limit
changes in brain volume may mediate those cog- values (TLV) set by the American Conference of
nitive changes that have been well documented Governmental Industrial Hygienists (American
(Caffo et al., 2008). Furthermore, although not Conference of Governmental Industrial Hygienists
widely used, magnetic resonance spectroscopy (ACGIH), 2008). The TLVs refer to airborne con-
(MRS) can be utilized to examine neuronal loss, centrations of substances, in conditions that nearly
possibly contributing to deficits in cognitive all workers may be exposed on a daily basis. TLVs
functioning. In a case example, one individual are intended to be used only as guidelines in the
with significant past lead exposure had a lower practice of industrial hygiene in the control of
N-acetylaspartate: creatine ratio, a marker of potential workplace health hazards. TLVs can not
neuronal density (Weisskopf et al., 2004). These be used as an absolute value above which someone
neuroimaging results highlight the potential will develop adverse health effects because of the
future usefulness of these state-of-the-art tech- wide variation in individual susceptibility.
niques for examining long-term, residual neuro- In addition to industrial hygiene sampling,
logical consequences of exposure to lead as well as other sources documenting exposure level infor-
to other neurotoxicants. However, it is imperative mation can be utilized. For example, in cases of
to acknowledge that these techniques can be used carbon monoxide (CO) poisoning, the fire depart-
presently only in research settings and it would be ment will often have records of CO levels in the
a serious error to prematurely use these investiga- dwelling where the person was exposed. In addi-
tional techniques in clinical or medical-legal tion, emergency room records will contain labo-
arenas. ratory data on carboxyhemoglobin levels, which if
elevated, will provide evidence that a significant
EXPOSURE AS SES SMENT exposure has occurred. Since carboxyhemoglobin
Are the patient’s symptom complaints caused by levels are elevated in smokers, smoking status
exposure to a neurotoxic substance? must be considered when interpreting this infor-
mation. In a nonsmoker, carboxyhemoglobin
Establishing Exposure levels higher than 8–10% are considered elevated;
This is the primary referral question addressed to greater than 50% life-threatening; and greater
the neuropsychologist in the context of toxic tort than 70–75% usually fatal (Bolla & Cadet, 2007).
litigation. To answer this question with any degree However, for CO exposure, carboxyhemoglobin
of certainty, it must first be established that an levels will drop rapidly once the person is removed
individual has been exposed to a neurotoxicant from the source of exposure. Therefore, it is
and second, if the exposure to the neurotoxicant important to gain knowledge about the time
was sufficient enough to cause CNS injury. period that has elapsed between the suspected
Without empirical evidence documenting an exposure and the person’s treatment in the ER,
exposure, it is difficult to address the question of including the time of the blood draw for carboxy-
causality. In the context of forensic neuropsychol- hemoglobin levels. This explains why documenta-
ogy, the inability to establish a strong association tion of a significant exposure is of utmost
between an exposure and neurologic effects will importance and is the first step in determining if
not be useful to a plaintiff. On the other hand, the CNS symptoms are related to the exposure.
defense can use this absence of empirical exposure Furthermore, if air samples of CO are higher
data to its advantage. Therefore, it is paramount to than the TLV of 50 ppm, then a case for a CO
establish if an exposure can be documented early exposure can be made. If, however, the dwelling
in the evaluation process, preferably prior to the has been ventilated and the carboxyhemoglobin
initial interview with the patient. levels are taken a few hours or days after the max-
In general, both plaintiff and defense attorneys imum exposure, then there will be little industrial
will already possess or have access to industrial hygiene or biological data to support significant
hygiene data. Industrial hygienists collect air, CO exposure even if significant exposure has
Neurotoxic Injury 291

occurred. Without data to verify an exposure, it is data linking neurological injury to an adverse
difficult to determine the intensity of current and event.
past exposure, or even if an individual has been
exposed to a toxic chemical. However, knowledge H I S T O RY
of the toxokinetics of the neurotoxicant is required Since there is often no definitive evidence (i.e.,
to interpret this type of information. If there is no biomarkers, industrial hygiene data) to make
empirical evidence of an exposure, it will gener- causal inferences about negative neurologic effects
ally be difficult to relate neurologic symptoms to a of a neurotoxic exposure, in-depth histories must
neurotoxic origin. be obtained. These histories should include infor-
mation within the medical, occupational, envi-
E S TA B L I S H I N G S U F F I C I E N T ronmental, social, academic, and psychiatric
EXPOSURE domains, to allow ruling out factors accounting
“The most important tenet of establishing for symptoms and test performance other than
a neurotoxic cause of any neurologic problem is the alleged exposure.
that the opportunity for exposure does not prove
that the symptoms were caused by the exposure” M E D I C A L H I S T O RY
(London & Albers, 2007). Often patients will A detailed medical history is essential to deter-
complain of residual neurocognitive effects mine if there are alternative explanations for the
following exposure to an unknown chemical, patient’s symptom complaints. Patients should be
usually one with a strong, unpleasant odor. One asked specific questions about previous neuro-
such substance is tar used in roofing and highway logic problems including head injury, the pres-
repair. Many individuals report becoming ill when ence of migraines, sleep disorders, substance use
exposed to tar and claim neurocognitive sequelae (especially alcohol use), previous learning dis-
as a result of this exposure. However, there is no abilities (i.e., were they ever held back a grade in
scientific evidence showing that tar is a neuro- school), symptoms of attention deficit disorder
toxicant. These odorous chemical irritants may with and without hyperactivity, and symptoms
produce allergy-type symptoms (e.g., runny eyes compatible with carpal tunnel syndrome, which
and nose, congestion) but do not cause perma- can be confused with symptoms of neurotoxicant-
nent brain injury. There are an unlimited number associated polyneuropathy. It is particularly
of chemical substances that we encounter in important to review the patient’s medical records.
everyday life where there is no evidence of neuro- It is informative if the patient reports a long series
toxicity. of severe medical complaints that they believe are
Once it has been established that an exposure caused by neurotoxicant exposure but there is no
to a potentially neurotoxic chemical has occurred, history that the patient ever sought medical atten-
it then needs to be determined if the exposure tion for these symptoms, or if they saw physicians
was of sufficient duration or intensity to produce for other concerns and did not mention the
neurologic injury. To accomplish this task, a bio- alleged symptoms.
logical measurement (a biomarker) of exposure
is required. Unfortunately, with the exception O C C U PAT I O N A L A N D
of the heavy metals, biomarkers for many neuro- E N V I R O N M E N TA L H I S T O RY
toxicants are either difficult to obtain or non- We must rely on the occupational and environ-
existent. For example, solvents are difficult to mental history to determine if there was sufficient
measure because of their rapid metabolism and exposure to a neurotoxicant to produce neuro-
clearance. N-hexane, an aliphatic hydrocarbon, logic effects. An in-depth history relating to a per-
has a half-life in fat tissue of about 64 hours. son’s occupational setting and specifics of the
Therefore, N-hexane and other organic solvents duties required by the job must be obtained not
are not cumulative chemicals, which means that only from the patient but from other sources.
only recent exposure (i.e., end of work shift) can Questions that must be addressed are aimed at
be evaluated by a biological marker. Even if a assessing the work environment where the poten-
patient had elevated levels of N-hexane at one tial exposure occurred. The reader is referred to
time, the levels would not be elevated after only a Kilbourne and Weiner (1990) for an occupational
few days with no exposure. Cases of solvent expo- history questionnaire. Does the person work in an
sure can be akin to cases of mild traumatic brain industry with potential exposure to a neurotoxi-
injury with respect to the paucity of empirical cant? Does the person have a job which places
292 forensic neuropsychology

him in close proximity to an exposure? Since the and childhood traumas. Therefore, it is important
routes of absorption of neurotoxicants are inhala- to obtain information about the number of mar-
tion, ingestion, and dermal absorption, does the riages, past and current treatments for psychiatric
person have a significant exposure that is biologi- illness, and if there were any instances of physical
cally plausible? For example, although a person or sexual childhood trauma. In particular, history
lives in a house with lead paint, if the paint is in of childhood physical or sexual trauma is associ-
good shape (i.e., not peeling) and is not disturbed, ated with increased risk for somatization disorder
then there is no potential source of significant in adulthood (see Binder chapter).
exposure. Did the person come in contact with a It is also important to ascertain whether
neurotoxicant? Does the employer enforce safety there has been a change in neurological status.
standards? Is there medical monitoring (i.e., rou- School records, if available, can provide critical
tine testing for elevated lead levels)? If the com- information about premorbid level of functioning
pany does not medically monitor or enforce safety (especially National Standardized Test results).
standards, it may not be because of negligence, it For example, it is not uncommon for a patient to
may be because the work environment has been claim that they are having difficulty in mathemat-
determined to be safe. Has the patient experi- ics or reading as a result of their potential expo-
enced symptoms while at work? Have co-workers sure. However, very poor grades in these subjects
complained of similar symptoms? as well as poor scores on a national standardized
It is also important to determine if individuals test compared to a national sample would indicate
claiming neurotoxic exposure are disgruntled that the individual has always had trouble in
workers who have unstable work histories. How these areas.
many jobs has the patient had? What kind of work
performance evaluations has the individual T H E E VA L UAT I O N
received? Does the patient show unexplained Historically, neuropsychological techniques have
poor job performance evaluations and excessive consisted of oral and written tests that are admin-
absenteeism from work long before a claim for istered by a trained examiner on a one-to-one
exposure is made? basis. A number of computerized test batteries
Environmental sources of exposure must also have also been developed (e.g., the World Health
be explored since these may be responsible for the Organization Neurobehavioral Test Battery and
exposure, rather than the work site. What is the the Neurobehavioral Evaluation System-NES-2;
person’s living situation? Do they live in an old or Letz & Baker, 1985) specifically for the evaluation
relatively new home? Do they live in the country of neurocognitive sequelae of neurotoxicant
or the city? What is the source of water? If water exposure.
comes from a well, the well may be contaminated. Advantages and disadvantages exist for both
Do they live in old housing in the city which could interviewer-administered and computer-admin-
be contaminated with lead paint, is the paint in istered tests (Kane & Kay, 1992). For interviewer-
poor repair, and if so, have they tried to remove administered tests, the advantages include human
the lead paint? Does the patient garden? Gardening interaction and encouragement by the examiner,
may be associated with exposure to excessive the ability to determine problem-solving strate-
amounts of pesticides. Has their home been gies by actually observing the individual perform
treated with pesticides lately? What are the indi- the tests, and the ability to administer tasks requir-
vidual’s hobbies? Patients can become ill from ing verbal presentation and verbal responses. For
high exposure in poorly ventilated areas to paint example, verbal memory cannot be adequately
thinners used in refinishing antique furniture, assessed by a computer without sophisticated
become lead exposed from lead solder used in computer hardware. The disadvantages of inter-
making stained glass, and may be exposed to viewer-administered tests include standardization
heavy metals from glazes if they make ceramics. of administration between different testers and
between testing sessions. In epidemiological
SOCIAL, ACADEMIC, AND investigations, interviewer-administered tests are
P S Y C H I AT R I C H I S T O RY more labor intensive and require a large study
The collection of social history is important to team to administer the tests.
gain a feel for psychological stability. Individuals Computerized testing offers excellent stan-
claiming exposure to neurotoxicants may have a dardization in administering and scoring. Further-
long history of antisocial personality disorder, more, in epidemiological studies, multiple work
somatoform disorder, major depressive disorder, stations and computers can be set up to test groups
Neurotoxic Injury 293

of workers simultaneously. However, extensive cognitive domain thoroughly with procedures


normative populations may not be available such as the California Verbal Learning Test-II
for computer-administered tests, which is not (CVLT-II) (Delis et al., 2000) and the Rey Auditory
the case for interviewer-administered tests. The Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (Rey, 1964). Close
normative values for interviewer-administered inspection of performance patterns on the RAVLT
tests cannot be used to compare the results of or the CVLT-II in individuals with neurotoxic
written tests adapted for the computer because exposure show a learning effect over the trials, but
the performance demands of the tasks change at a lower rate than expected. Performance on the
even though the tests appear to be similar (Kane RAVLT (trial 1–5) also declines as years of expo-
& Kay, 1992). sure to lead increases (Bolla et al., 1995) and peak
A thorough assessment includes tests that tibia lead levels increase (Schwartz et al., 2000;
assess the cognitive domains of Intelligence, Lan- Schwartz et al., 2005). These tests also allow exam-
guage, Verbal and Visual Learning and Memory, ination for signs of malingering. For example, the
Perception, Motor/Tactile skills, Attention, Psycho- person may be embellishing symptoms if they
motor Speed, Executive Function and Personality recall a word consistently on the free recall learn-
(Lezak et al., 2004; Spreen & Strauss, 1998). The ing trials but then fail to recognize it on the
cognitive areas most often reported to be adversely Recognition Subtest (Binder et al., 2003; Wiggins
affected by neurotoxicants are manual dexterity, & Brandt, 1988; Bernard, 1990).
psychomotor speed, verbal and visual memory Visual memory can also be assessed with tests
and learning, attention, and executive functions such as the Symbol Digit Paired Associate
(refer to Tables 11.2–11.5). Learning Task (Kapur & Butters, 1977a; Kapur &
Verbal intelligence can be assessed using the Butters, 1977b) or the Rey Complex Figure
Verbal Subtests from the Wechsler Adult (Osterrieth, 1944).
Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV) (Wechsler, 2008). Sustained attention and executive/motor skills
For brevity of testing, the Vocabulary Subtest can have been reported to be affected by neurotoxic
be used alone to obtain a good estimate of verbal exposure, as well. Two tests that are sensitive in
intelligence because it correlates highly with the detecting not only neurotoxic exposure, but any
Full-Scale IQ score. type of CNS damage, are the Digit-Symbol
Although there are many standardized tests to Substitution Test from the WAIS-III (now the
evaluate language and aphasia, such as the Western Coding subtest from the WAIS-IV) and the Trail
Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, 2006) and the Boston Making Test (Trails A and B) from the Halstead
Diagnostic Aphasia Battery (Goodglass et al., Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery (Reitan &
2000), extensive evaluation of language function- Davison, 1974). The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
ing in suspected cases of neurotoxic exposure is and the Category Test are also sensitive to deficits
unnecessary because most neurotoxins do not in executive function and reasoning (Lezak et al.,
selectively impair language. Therefore, deficits in 2004). The Stroop Test and Consonant Trigrams
language (e.g., paraphasias) suggest an alternative are measures of divided attention as well as execu-
etiology for symptoms. The significant aspects of tive functions (Lezak et al., 2004).
language can be quickly assessed by confronta- Simple Visual Reaction Time is another
tional naming, repetition of words and phrases, test that has been shown to be affected by neuro-
spontaneous writing of a sentence, writing a sen- toxins (Balbus et al., 1997). Reaction time can be
tence to dictation, and rating verbal expression. measured by using either a reaction-time device
At low levels, neurotoxins affect new learning or a computer. Stimuli are randomly presented
and recent memory, and they do not affect remote so that the presentation of the next stimulus
memory. If gaps exist in the individual’s early cannot be anticipated. When this task is given
memories, then a neurotoxic etiology is unlikely. over 44 or more trials, an index of vigilance, sus-
Remote memory can be assessed by asking about tained attention, and response variability can be
significant early life events (wedding or occupa- determined. In addition to examining the mean
tional details, or historical events). or median reaction, the standard deviation has
Difficulties with anterograde memory (ability been shown to be especially sensitive for measur-
to learn new information) is one of the character- ing variability in rate of response which appears to
istics of neurotoxic exposure (Bleecker et al., 1991; be strongly affected by lead exposure (Balbus
Schwartz et al., 1993; Schwartz et al., 2000; Bolla et al., 1997).
et al., 1995; Stewart et al., 1999; Schwartz et al., Manual dexterity and coordination have been
2005). Therefore, it is important to evaluate this shown to deteriorate with exposure to various
294 forensic neuropsychology

neurotoxins, especially to a combination of caution. To determine if a score falls outside the


organic and inorganic lead (Bolla et al., 1995; normal range, adequate norms must be used for
Stewart et al., 1999; Schwartz et al., 1993; Schwartz comparison. For example, intellectual ability can
et al., 2000; Schwartz et al., 2001a; Schwartz et al., predict test performance for verbal, perceptual,
2001b). The Purdue Pegboard (Purdue Research executive function, and psychomotor speed tasks
Foundation, 1948; Agnew et al., 1988) and the (Larrabee, 2000), but few norms are currently
Grooved Pegboard (Klove, 1963; Klove, 1963) are available for individuals with low levels or high
measures of fine motor speed and dexterity. The levels of intellectual functioning. Comparing the
Finger Oscillation Task (Finger Tapping) (Reitan test results of an individual with lower intellectual
& Davison, 1974) is a measure of dexterity/simple ability with normative values based on a more
motor speed. Brain damage from most neurotox- intelligent group could lead to the erroneous con-
ins is diffuse, and significantly faster scores on one clusion of CNS injury where none exists. This is
hand compared to the other (greater than 10% especially problematic in behavioral neurotoxi-
difference) may suggest a lateralized dysfunction cology because there are few norms available for
and would therefore be incongruent with a diag- unskilled laborers (i.e., blue-collar workers), many
nosis of a neurotoxic exposure. Although, epide- of whom never finished high school. Conversely,
miological studies find that nondominant hand performance decrements in highly intelligent
performance may be more sensitive to lead expo- people may be missed, since even their diminished
sure, the effect is small (Bolla et al., 1995; Stewart performance may still meet or exceed the upper
et al., 1999; Schwartz et al., 2002). limits of a normative group representing a more
Not only is it important to use tests that have average level of intellectual functioning. In addi-
been shown to be sensitive to detecting subtle tion, cognitive performance is also influenced
neurocognitive effects from exposures, but tests by age and sex. Therefore, to ensure an accurate
sensitive enough to show a dose-response asso- diagnosis, the clinician must use norms that
ciation are favorable. Significant dose-related have been adjusted to include the effects of
associations (increased exposure, poorer perfor- modifying variables such as education, intelli-
mance) between quantitative measures of expo- gence, age, and sex.
sure to a mixture of organic and inorganic lead or
to only inorganic lead (duration of exposure, peak Base Rates
tibia bone lead, current blood lead) have been When attempting to determine if a patient’s
reported for the RAVLT, Serial Digit Learning cognitive complaints are consistent with those
test, Rey Complex Figure Delayed, Trails A and B, that have been reported with exposure to neuro-
Stroop, Block Design, Purdue Pegboard, and toxicants, one must consider the base rate of the
Finger Tapping (Bolla et al., 1995; Stewart et al., complaint in the general population. For example,
1999; Schwartz et al., 2000; Schwartz et al., 2005). in a study of 40–90-year-old community-dwelling
Likewise, increased lifetime weighted average healthy individuals, 83% complained of difficulty
exposure to a mixture of solvents was associate remembering names, 60% complained that they
with decreased performance on the Digit Symbol misplaced items, and 53% complained of word-
substitution, Serial Digit learning, Reaction time, finding problems (Bolla et al., 1991). These base
Trails A and B, and bilateral finger tapping rates increase moderately when associated with
(Bleecker et al., 1991). Establishing evidence of head injury of toxic exposure, but increase dra-
dose-related associations on the neuropsycholo- matically once someone enters the legal arena to
gical assessment helps rule out non-neurologic become a personal injury claimant (Lees-Haley &
psychiatric factors and malingering. Systematically Brown, 1993; Dunn et al., 1995). Table 11.6 is
evaluating for malingering must be included in taken from the work of Dunn et al. (1995) and
every evaluation of a potential case of neurotoxic shows the frequency of symptom complaints in
exposure. The reader is referenced to Dr. Larrabee’s samples of controls, head-injured and/or toxic-
chapter on malingering for guidelines. injured individuals, and personal-injury claim-
ants. Therefore, it is paramount to remember that
I N T R P R E TAT I O N just because a patient complains of a specific
OF FINDINGS symptom and that symptom has been reported
with exposure to a specific neurotoxicant, this
Adequate Normative Values for Reference does not mean that the individual was exposed to
Speculation about the cause of poor performance a chemical that permanently injured her/his
on a neurobehavioral test must be made with nervous system.
TABLE 11.6. BASE RATES OF COMPLAINTS
SYMPTOM FREQUENCY
Symptom Medical Controls Head Injured/ Personal Injury
(n=113) Toxic (n=68) (n=156)
Anxiety 41 56 87
Trouble sleeping 30 40 81
Headaches 50 57 77
Depression 27 41 76
Tension 24 40 74
Concentration problems 21 34 71
Fatigue 37 56 71
Difficulty concentrating 18 32 69
Impatience 33 41 64
Irritability 27 31 63
Restlessness 16 37 63
Confusion 5 18 58
Feeling disorganized 20 32 58
Thinking clearly 11 27 57
Neck pain* 31 40 56
Loss of interest 13 15 51
Easily distracted 15 24 49
Loss of efficiency 10 13 49
Loss of temper 11 19 49
Attention problems 10 25 48
Word finding 12 24 46
Feeling partially disabled 4 16 44
Weakness 6 15 44
Dizziness 21 28 41
Nausea 20 32 39
Sexual problems 8 18 39
Shoulder pain* 14 24 38
Slowed thinking 5 16 38
Blurred vision 18 32 37
Rapid heartbeat 15 19 37
Poor judgment 4 16 35
Recent memory problems 5 18 35
Chest pressure 13 13 34
Trouble hearing 12 28 34
Numbness 5 21 34
Painful tingling 5 16 34
Visual problems 7 21 34
Trouble reading 1 6 33
Fear of non-cancer illness 6 19 33
Trouble walking 4 6 32
Trembling 5 15 31
Feeling totally disabled 3 3 30
Bumping into things 14 13 30
Diarrhea* 26 47 28
Perspiring for no reason 5 13 26
Loss of common sense 4 6 24
Marital problems 9 12 24
Fine motor coordination 2 4 20
(Continued)

296 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 11.6. Continued


Long-term memory problems 0 7 18
Speech problems 2 6 17
Slurred speech 1 6 15
Elbow pain* 8 22 14
Impotence 4 2 14
Not knowing where I am 0 4 12
= Mean No. Symptoms 9 14 25

Dunn et al., 1995;


*indicates that the symptom is a distractor.

It is important to note that neurobehavioral decrements in neuropsychological test perfor-


tests can have high sensitivity, but low specificity, mance. These decrements are usually observed in
meaning that they can be very effective in detect- the areas of attention, learning and memory,
ing deficits, but less effective in identifying the processing speed, and psychomotor speed. Since
causes of those deficits (see Larrabee chapter on similar cognitive domains are affected by both
scientific approach). It is important to determine neurotoxicants and alterations in mood, it can be
if cognitive decrements are related to neuro- difficult to determine the relative contribution of
toxicant exposure or to another CNS, medical, each to decrements in neuropsychological test
or neuropsychiatric disorder. Alterations in per- performance. More specifically, such symptoma-
formance can be related to age, sex, educational tology could be related to neurotoxic exposure, to
level, native intelligence, cultural differences, the patient’s emotional state, to the patient’s per-
cultural deprivation, motivation, involvement in sonality characteristics, or to some combination
litigation, frustration, fatigue, emotional prob- of the three.
lems like anxiety and depression, and personality In order to determine the etiology of com-
characteristics. plaints, patterns of performance and inconsisten-
Consequently, while some individual tests are cies during testing (such as superior performance
sensitive, they are of little value when attempting on harder tasks with poor performance on easier
to delineate the precise etiology of a person’s defi- ones, or lower than chance performance on all of
cits. The neuropsychological evaluation cannot be the tasks) must be examined carefully. Repeat
used in isolation to make a diagnosis, but rather testing will also assist in this area, since test
must be used in combination with medical, social, performance should remain relatively static over
school, and occupational histories. Although the course of several days. If significant alterations
patients may report a recent onset of cognitive in test scores are indicated, an affective or motiva-
difficulties, review of school records and prior tional disturbance is the likely culprit. If test
employment evaluations might indicate long- performance declines without re-exposure, then a
standing problems (i.e., subnormal intelligence or progressive disease or secondary psychological
learning disability). reaction to the exposure is suspect (Bolla &
Furthermore, long-term or heavy alcohol use Rignani, 1997).
(Bondi et al., 1998; Parsons, 1998), heavy cocaine Emotional reactions to a perceived exposure
use (Bolla et al., 1999), heavy MDMA (Ecstasy) may be as important as, or even more important
use (Bolla et al., 1998), or even very heavy than, the direct physiological effects of the chemi-
marijuana use (Bolla et al., 2003; Gonzalez, 2007) cals, especially when considering etiology and
may produce a number of cognitive deficits. persistence of symptoms. Figures 11.2 and 11.3
Medical conditions such as diabetes can cause illustrate the complex relationships between
symptoms of peripheral neuropathy as well as exposure and neurologic sequelae (Bolla, 1996b;
cognitive deficits (Bruce et al., 2003). Other Bolla, 1996a; Bolla & Rignani, 1997). The fear
medical conditions to consider in the differential associated with suspected exposures can be
diagnosis are past history of head injury, hyper- stressful enough to cause significant mental disor-
tension, thyroid disease, and renal or hepatic ders such as somatoform disorders, anxiety disor-
disorders. Depression or anxiety can also produce ders, adjustment disorders, typical and atypical
Neurotoxic Injury 297

EXPOSURE TO NEUROTOXICANTS

ACUTE, HIGH INTENSITY

Cognitive Symptoms Physical Symptoms


(acute confusional (peripheral neuropathy)
state) (nausea)

Hypersensitivity to Anxiety/Depression
Endogenous Stimuli

Primary symptoms Secondary symptoms

FIGURE 11.2: Schematic representation of the development and persistence of physical, cognitive, and
affective symptoms following acute, high-intensity chemical exposure. Solid lines are primary symptoms, and
dotted lines are secondary symptoms.

post-traumatic stress disorders, and idiopathic hypersensitive to endogenous stimuli. Expecta-


environmental intolerance (IEI). In addition, spe- tions by susceptible individuals that exposure to
cific inherent personality characteristics may pre- chemicals has adversely affected their health may
dispose an individual to develop physical, result in enhanced awareness of normal bodily
cognitive, and psychological symptoms (Bolla- sensations. This normal physiological activity may
Wilson et al., 1988; Kellner, 1985; Mechanic, then be erroneously attributed to neurotoxic-
1972). Clinical observation suggests that more related abnormal physical, cognitive, and affective
perfectionist and anxious individuals may be symptoms (Bolla-Wilson et al., 1988). The degree

EXPOSURE TO NEUROTOXICANTS

CHRONIC, LOW INTENSITY

Cognitive Symptoms Physical Symptoms


(memory, psychomotor, (headaches, fatigue)
speed) (attention deficits)

Hypersensitivity to Anxiety/Depression
Endogenous Stimuli

Primary symptoms Secondary symptoms

FIGURE 11.3: Schematic representation of the development and persistence of physical, cognitive, and
affective symptoms following chronic, low-intensity chemical exposure. Solid lines are primary symptoms
and dotted lines are secondary symptoms.
298 forensic neuropsychology

to which an individual is hypersensitive to endog- neuropathy were ever reported to any doctor by
enous stimuli (normal bodily sensations) may the patient, and the patient is only claiming
determine the duration and intensity of symp- cognitive problems. If there are no acute effects, it
toms. Those psychological mechanisms have been would be highly unlikely to have chronic effects.
observed in toxic exposure, as well as in persistent After discontinuation of exposure, both CNS and
post-concussion syndrome (Bolla-Wilson et al., PNS effects recover.
1988; Mittenberg et al., 1992) and in idiopathic When evaluating patients who present with
environmental intolerance (a. k. a. multiple chem- symptoms of a possible neurotoxicant exposure,
ical sensitivity [MCS]; see Dr. Binder’s chapter for it is irresponsible to routinely attribute their
further discussion). symptoms to the presence of a neuropsychiatric
The neuropsychological evaluation provides disorder. It is equally irresponsible to attribute
unique information on the functional integrity of neurotoxic-like symptoms to neurotoxicant
the CNS. As with any diagnostic process, the exposure without rigorously exploring other
ability to make a differential diagnosis between etiologies. If other causes for the symptoms are
neurotoxicant exposure, neurologic disease, med- not considered, then many treatable conditions
ical illness, or neuropsychiatric disturbance is will remain unidentified and untreated.
based on the combined evidence taken from the
medical, occupational, social, and academic his-
REFERENCES
tories, physical and neurologic exams, biological
Agnew, J., Bolla-Wilson, K., Kawas, C., & Bleecker,
monitoring, EEG, CT/MRI, and the neuropsycho-
M. L. (1988). Purdue Pegboard age and sex norms
logical evaluation. for ages forty and older. Developmental Neuro-
Central nervous system effects are best psychology, 4, 29–35.
discovered with neuropsychological assessment. American Conference of Governmental Industrial
However, interpretation of decrements in Hygienists (ACGIH) (2008). Threshold limit values
performance on these tests is often erroneous if for chemical substances and physical agents and bio-
made by individuals who lack specific expertise logical exposure indices. Cincinnati, OH: ACGIH.
in neurobehavioral toxicology. This is illustrated Arlien-Soborg, P., Bruhn, P., Gyldensted, C., et al.
nicely by the N-hexane example presented (1979). Chronic painter’s syndrome. Chronic toxic
earlier in this chapter. If the clinician is not encephalopathy in house painters. Acta Neurologica
knowledgeable about the specific symptoms asso- Scandinavica, 60, 149–56.
ciated with specific neurotoxic agents, they may Arlien-Soborg, P., Henriksen, L., Gade, A., Gyldensted,
conclude that a mild deficit in verbal memory C., & Paulson, O. B. (1982). Cerebral blood flow in
(without having the patient examined for periph- chronic toxic encephalopathy in house painters
eral neuropathy) was due to exposure to N-hexane exposed to organic solvents. Acta Neurol Scand, 66,
when this conclusion would not be biologically 34–41.
plausible. Balbus, J. M., Stewart, W., Bolla, K. I., & Schwartz, B. S.
The neurological examination, electroenceph- (1997). Simple visual reaction time in organolead
alogram (EEG), computed tomography (CT) scan, manufacturing workers: A comparison of different
methods of modeling lead exposure and reaction
and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans are
time. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 32,
generally not helpful in making a specific diagno-
544–49.
sis of toxic encephalopathy but are helpful in
Bernard, L. C. (1990). Prospects for faking believable
ruling out other causes of the patient’s symptoms. memory deficits on neuropsychological tests and
In contrast, nerve conduction studies are useful in the use of incentives in simulation research. Journal
detecting peripheral neuropathies that are often of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12,
associated with exposure to specific chemicals. 715–28.
For example, the predominant neurologic feature Binder, L. M., Kelly, M. P., Villanueva, M. R., &
of N-hexane exposure is peripheral neuropathy, Winslow, M. M. (2003). Motivation and neuropsy-
not cognitive disturbance. Symmetrical sensory chological test performance following mild head
dysfunction in the hands and feet is the usual pre- injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
senting complaint. Weakness in the lower extrem- Neuropsychology, 25, 420–30.
ities is also a common symptom. Decreased Bleecker, M. L., Bolla, K. I., Agnew, J., et al. (1991). Dose-
response to pin, vibration, and thermal stimula- related subclinical neurobehavioral effects of chronic
tion is found on examination. The clinician should exposure to low levels of organic solvents. American
become suspicious if no symptoms of peripheral Journal of Industrial Medicine, 19, 715–28.
Neurotoxic Injury 299

Bolla, K., Brown, K., Eldreth, D., Tate, K., & Cadet, J.-L. polysubstance abusers with concurrent alcohol
(2002). Dose-related neurocognitive effects of abuse. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
marijuana use. Neurology, 59, 1337–43. Society, 4, 319–28.
Bolla, K., Eldreth, D., London, E. D., Kiehl, K. A., Bruce, D. G., Casey, G. P., Grange, V., Clarnette, R. C.,
Mouratidis, M., Contoreggi, C., et al. (2003). Almeida, O. P., Foster, J. K., et al. (2003). Cognitive
Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in abstinent impairment, physical disability, and depressive
cocaine abusers performing a decision-making symptoms in older diabetic patients: the Fremantle
task. NeuroImage, 19, 1085–94. cognition in diabetes study. Diabetes Research and
Bolla, K. & Roca, R. (1994). The neuropsychiatric Clinical Practice, 61, 59–67.
sequella of exposure to neurotoxic compounds. In Caffo, B., Chen, S., Stewart, W., Bolla, K., Yousem, D.,
M. L. Bleecker (Ed.), Occupational neurology and Davatzikos, C., et al. (2008). Are brain volumes
clinical neurotoxicology (Baltimore, MD: Williams based on magnetic resonance imaging mediators
and Wilkins. of the associations of cumulative lead dose
Bolla, K. I. (1996a). Neurobehavioral performance in with cognitive function? Am J Epidemiol, 167,
multiple chemical sensitivities. Regulatory Toxico- 429–37.
logy and Pharmacology, 24, S52–54. Cherry, N., Hutchins, H., Pace, T., et al. (1985).
Bolla, K. I. (1996b). Neuropsychological evaluation for Neurobehavioral effects of repeated occupational
detecting alterations in the central nervous system exposure to toluene and paint solvents. British
after chemical exposure. Regulatory Toxicology and Journal of Industrial Medicine, 41, 291–300.
Pharmacology, 24, S48–51. Cowan, R. L., Joers, J. M., & Dietrich, M. S. (2008).
Bolla, K. I. & Cadet, J.-L. (2007). Exogenous acquired N-acetylaspartate (NAA) correlates inversely with
metabolic disorders of the nervous system: Toxins cannabis use in a frontal language processing
and illicit drugs. In C. Goetz & E. J. Pappert (Eds.), region of neocortex in MDMA (Ecstasy) polydrug
Textbook of clinical neurology (3rd ed., pp. 865–96). users: A 3 T magnetic resonance spectroscopy
Chicago, Illinois: W.B. Saunders Co. study. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 92(1), 105–110.
Bolla, K. I., Lindgren, K. N., Bonaccorsy, C., & Cranmer, J. M. & Goldberg, L. (1986). Human aspects of
Bleecker, M. L. (1991). Memory complaints in the solvent neurobehavioral effects. Report of the work-
elderly—fact or fiction? Archives of Neurology, 48, shop session on clinical and epidemiological topics.
61–64. Proceedings of the Workshop on Neurobehavioral
Bolla, K. I., McCann, U. D., & Ricaurte, G. (1998). Effects of Solvents. Neurotoxicology, 7, 45–56.
Memory Impairment in abstinent MDMA Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A.
(“Ecstasy”) users. Neurology, 51, 1532–37. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test – 2nd
Bolla, K. I. & Rignani, J. (1997). Clinical course of neu- Edition. San Antonio: The Psychological
ropsychological functioning after chronic exposure Corporation.
to organic and inorganic lead. Archives of Clinical Dunn, J. T., Lees-Haley, P. R., Brown, R. S., Williams, C.
Neuropsychology, 12, 123–31. W., & English, L. T. (1995). Neurotoxic complaint
Bolla, K. I., Rothman, R. B., & Cadet, J.-L. (1999). base rates of personal injury claimants: Implications
Dose-related neurobehavioral effects of chronic for neuropsychological assessment. Journal of
cocaine use. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Clinical Psychology, 51, 577–84.
Neurosciences, 11, 361–69. Eldreth, D. A., Matochik, J. A., Cadet, J. L., & Bolla,
Bolla, K. I., Schwartz, B. S., Agnew, J., Ford, P., & K. I. (2004). Abnormal brain activity in prefrontal
Bleecker, M. L. (1990). Subclinical neuropsychiat- brain regions in abstinent marijuana users.
ric effects of chronic low level solvent exposure in Neuroimage, 23, 914–20.
US paint manufacturers. Journal of Occupational Gade, A., Mortensen, E. L., & Bruhn, P. (1988). Chronic
Medicine, 32, 671–77. painters “syndrome.” A reanalysis of psychological
Bolla, K. I., Schwartz, B. S., Stewart, W., et al. (1995). A test data in a group of diagnosed cases, based on
comparison of neurobehavioral function in work- comparisons with matched controls. Acta
ers exposed to a mixture of organic and inorganic Neurologica Scandinavica, 77, 293–306.
lead and in workers exposed to solvents. American Gonzalez, R. (2007). Acute and non-acute effects of can-
Journal of Industrial Medicine, 27, 231–46. nabis on brain functioning and neuropsychological
Bolla-Wilson, K., Wilson, R. J., & Bleecker, M. L. performance. Neuropsychol Rev, 17, 347–61.
(1988). Conditioning of physical symptoms after Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., & Barresi, B. (2000). The
neurotoxic exposure. Journal of Occupational Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination. San
Medicine, 30, 684–86. Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
Bondi, M. W., Drake, A. I., & Grant, I. (1998). Verbal Hartman, D.E. (1995). Neuropsychological Toxicology:
learning and memory in alcohol abusers and Identificaiton and assessment of human neurotoxic
300 forensic neuropsychology

syndromes. (2nd ed.) New York: Kluwer Academic Maizlish, N. A., Fine, L. J., Albers, J. W., Whitehead, L.,
Plenum. & Langolf, G. D. (1987). A neurological evaluation
IOM (Institute of Medicine) Committee on Damp of workers exposed to mixtures of organic
Indoor Spaces and Health (2004). Damp indoor solvents. British Journal of Industrial Medicine, 44,
spaces and health. The Academies Press. 14–25.
Kane, R. L. & Kay, G. G. (1992). Computerized assess- Matochik, J., London, E. D., Eldreth, D., Cadet, J.-L., &
ment in neuropsychology: A review of tests and Bolla, K. (2003). Frontal cortical tissue composi-
test batteries. Neuropsychology Review, 3, 1–117. tion in abstinent cocaine abusers: A magnetic reso-
Kapur, N. & Butters, N. (1977a). An analysis of the nance imaging study. NeuroImage, 19, 1095–1102.
visuoperceptual deficits in alcoholics. Journal of the Matochik, J. A., Eldreth, D. A., Cadet, J. L., & Bolla, K.
Study of Alcohol, 38, 2025–55. I. (2005). Altered brain tissue composition in heavy
Kapur, N. & Butters, N. (1977b). Visuoperceptive defi- marijuana users. Drug Alcohol Depend., 77, 23–30.
cits in long-term alcoholics and alcoholics with Mechanic, D. (1972). Social psychologic factors affect-
Korsakoff ’s psychosis. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, ing the presentation of bodily complaints. New
38, 2025–35. England Journal of Medicine, 286, 1132–39.
Kellner, R. (1985). Functional somatic symptoms and Mittenberg, W., DiGuilio, D. V., Perrin, S., & Bass, A. E.
hypochondriasis: A survey of empirical studies. (1992). Symptoms following mild head injury:
Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 821–33. Expectation as etiology. Journal of Neurology,
Kertesz, A. (2006). Western Aphasia Battery - Revised Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 55, 200–204.
(WAB-R). London, Ontario: University of Ontario. Morrow, L. A., Ryan, C. M., Goldstein, G., & Hodgson,
Kilbourne, E. W. & Weiner, J. (1990). Occupational and M. J. (1989). A distinct pattern of personality dis-
environmental medicine: The internist’s role. turbance following exposure to mixtures of organic
Annals of Internal Medicine, 113, 974–82. solvents. J Occup Med, 31, 743–46.
Klove, H. (1963b). Clinical neuropsychology. In F. M. Morrow, L. A., Ryan, C. M., Hodgson, M. J., & Robin,
Forster (Ed.), The medical clinics of North America N. (1990). Alterations in cognitive and psychologi-
(pp. 1647–58). New York: WB Saunders. cal functioning after organic solvent exposure. J
Larrabee, G. J. (2000). Association between IQ and Occup Med, 32, 444–50.
neuropsychological test performance: Commen- Nestor, L., Roberts, G., Garavan, H., & Hester, R.
tary on Tremont, Hoffman, Scott, and Adams (2008). Deficits in learning and memory:
(1998). Clinical Neuropsychol, 14, 139–45. Parahippocampal hyperactivity and frontocortical
Lees-Haley, P. R. & Brown, R. S. (1993). Neuro- hypoactivity in cannabis users. Neuroimage, 40,
psychological complaint base rates of 170 personal 1328–39.
injury claimants. Archives of Clinical Neuro- Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure
psychology, 8, 203–209. complex: Contribution a l’etude de la perception et
Lees-Haley, P. R., Greiffenstein, M. F., Larrabee, G. J., & de la memoire. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 286–356.
Manning, E. L. (2004). Methodological problems Parkinson, R. B., Hopkins, R. O., Cleavinger, H. B.,
in the neuropsychological assessment of effects of Weaver, L. K., Victoroff, J., Foley, J. F. et al. (2002).
exposure to welding fumes and manganese. Clin White matter hyperintensities and neuropsycho-
Neuropsychol, 18, 449–64. logical outcome following carbon monoxide
Lees-Haley, P. R. & Williams, C. W. (1997). poisoning. Neurology, 58, 1525–32.
Neurotoxicity of chronic low-dose exposure to Parsons, O. A. (1998). Neurocognitive deficits in alco-
organic solvents: a skeptical review. J Clin Psychol, holics and social drinkers: A continuum?
53, 699–712. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,
Letz, R. & Baker, E. L. (1985). Computer-administered 22, 954–61.
neurobehavioral testing: On defining its limitations. Purdue Research Foundation (1948). Examiner’s
In Environmental health document 3 (pp. 158–62). Manual for the Purdue Pegboard. Chicago, IL:
Copenhagen: World Health Organization. Science Research Associates.
Lezak, M. D., Howieson D.B, & Loring D.W. (2004). Reitan, R. M. & Davison, L. A. (1974). Clinical neurop-
Neuropsychological assessment. (4 ed.) New York: sychology: Current status and applications. New
Oxford University Press. York, NY: Hemisphere Publishing Corp.
London, Z. & Albers, J. W. (2007). Toxic neuropathies Rey, A. (1964). L’examen clinique en psychologie. Paris:
associated with pharmaceutic and industrial Presses Universitaires de France.
agents. Neurol Clin, 25, 257–76. Riggs, M. A., Rao, C. Y., Brown, C. M., Van, S. D.,
Loomis, T. A. & Hayes, A. W. (1996). Loomis’s essentials of Cummings, K. J., Dunn, K. H., et al. (2008).
toxicology. (4th ed.) San Diego, CA: Academic Resident cleanup activities, characteristics of
Press. flood-damaged homes and airborne microbial
Neurotoxic Injury 301

concentrations in New Orleans, Louisiana, October Stewart, W., Schwartz, B. S., Simon, D., Bolla, K. I.,
2005. Environ. Res., 106, 401–409. Todd, A. C., & Links, J. (1999). The relation between
Rosenstock, L., Cullen, M., Brodkin, C., & Redlich, C. neurobehavioral function and tibial and chelatable
(2004). Textbook of clinical occupational and envi- lead levels in 543 former organolead manufactur-
ronmental medicine. (Second ed.) Philadelphia: ing workers. Neurology, 52, 610–17.
Saunders. Stewart, W. F., Schwartz, B. S., Davatzikos, C., Shen, D.,
Schwartz, B. S., Bolla, K. I., Stewart, W., Ford, D. P., Liu, D., Wu, X., et al. (2006). Past adult lead expo-
Agnew, J., & Frumkin, H. (1993). Decrements in sure is linked to neurodegeneration measured by
neurobehavioral performance associated with brain MRI. Neurology, 66, 1476–84.
mixed exposure to organic and inorganic lead. Triebig, G., Claus, D., Csuzda, I., Druschky, K.-F., Holler,
American Journal of Epidemiology, 137, 1006–21. P., Kinzel, W., et al. (1988). Cross-sectional epide-
Schwartz, B. S., Lee, B. K., Bandeen-Roche, K., Stewart, miologic study on neurotoxicity of solvents in paints
W., Bolla, K., Links, J., et al. (2005). Occupational and lacquers. International Archives of Occupational
lead exposure and longitudinal decline in neurobe- and Environmental Health, 60, 233–41.
havioral test scores. Epidemiology, 16, 106–113. Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Schwartz, B. S., Lee, B. K., Lee, G. S., Stewart, W. F., Lee, Scale Fourth Edition: Administration and Scoring
S. S., Hwang, K. Y., et al. (2001). Associations of Manual. (4th ed.) San Antonio: The Psychological
blood lead, dimercaptosuccinic acid-chelatable Corporation.
lead, and tibia lead with neurobehavioral test scores Weisskopf, M. G., Hu, H., Mulkern, R. V., White, R.,
in South Korean lead workers. Am J Epidemiol, Aro, A., Oliveira, S., et al. (2004). Cognitive deficits
153, 453–64. and magnetic resonance spectroscopy in adult
Schwartz, B. S., Stewart, W., Bolla, K. I., Simon, D., monozygotic twins with lead poisoning. Environ
Banden-Roche, K., Gordon, B., et al. (2000). Past Health Perspect, 112, 620–25.
adult lead exposure is associated with longitudinal Wiggins, E. C. & Brandt, J. (1988). The detection of
decline in neurobehavioral function. Neurology, simulated amnesia. Law and Human Behavior, 12,
55, 1144–50. 57–78.
Senanayake, N. et al. (1982). Acute polyneuropathy World Health Organization (WHO) (1985). Chronic
after poisoning by a new organophosphate insecti- effects of organic solvents on the central nervous
cide. New England Journal of Medicine, 306, system and diagnostic criteria. Environmental
155–57. Health Services: Document 5. Copenhagen: WHO.
Spreen, O. & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neu-
ropsychological tests. (2nd ed.) New York: Oxford.
12
The Psychological Assessment of
Persons with Chronic Pain
K E V I N W. G R E V E , K E V I N J . B I A N C H I N I ,
A N D J O N AT H A N S . O R D

Pain is no evil unless it conquers us.


— george eliot
He that is uneasy at every little pain is never without some ache.
—proverb

INTRODUCTION Frymoyer, 1988; Rubin, 2007) and approximately


The International Association for the Study of one-third experiencing chronic spinal pain in
Pain (IASP) defines pain as “an unpleasant their lifetime (Von Korff et al., 2005).
sensory and emotional experience associated Financially, the direct and indirect costs of
with actual or potential tissue damage, or chronic pain are substantial. Pain complaints
described in terms of such damage” (Merskey & result in millions of physician office visits per year
Bogduk, 1994, p. 212). Importantly, the IASP’s (Woodwell, 2004) and Americans spend an esti-
definition considers pain a subjective experience mated $70 billion per year in health care costs
influenced by both psychological and contextual related to chronic pain (American Academy of
factors that is not necessarily dependent on tissue Pain Management, 2003). Pain has a large impact
damage or specific nociceptive activation. Pain is on health-related quality of life (Morken et al.,
usually termed chronic when symptoms do not 2002) and back pain alone is thought to contrib-
follow the natural course of healing after injury or ute to over 100 million lost workdays yearly (Guo,
persist for longer than three months without Tanaka, Halperin, & Cameron, 1999). The total
biological value (Merskey & Bogduk, 1994). As economic burden of pain-related health care and
described by Pappagallo and Werner (2008), disability has been estimated by some to exceed
“Acute pain ordinarily has a useful purpose, such $150 billion a year (Gatchel & Okifuji, 2006;
as signaling damage or that something is wrong. Mayer, Gatchel, & Polatin, 2000).
By contrast, chronic pain has no such value, but is Neuropsychologists will almost inevitably be
a disease in its own right, causing widespread confronted with issues of pain and pain-related
suffering, distress, and disability” (p. 17). disability in their practice. Pain is a feature of a
variety of conditions commonly encountered by
Scope of the Problem neuropsychologists such as multiple sclerosis,
Chronic pain is a widespread problem estimated Parkinson’s disease, and traumatic brain injury
to affect between 10% and 20% of the general (TBI). Chronic pain after TBI, for example, occurs
population (Verhaak, Kerssens, Dekker, Sorbi, & in 75% of mild TBI patients and 32% of moderate-
Bensing, 1998) and 20% to 25% of primary care severe TBI patients (Nampiaparampil, 2008). Pain
patients (Gureje, Von Korff, Simon, & Gater, is an important prognostic indicator for injury
1998). Spine-related pain is a particularly common recovery (Alexander, 1995; Mooney, Speed, &
problem with over half of adults experiencing an Sheppard, 2005) and should be addressed in
episode of spine-related pain (Andersson, 1998; order to reduce residual disability and maximize
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 303

functional outcomes. Therefore, an understanding pathology without frequent symptoms at baseline


of pain and the factors that influence pain-related did not have a significant relationship with the
disability is essential for the neuropsychologist; development of knee symptomology 15 months
particularly when evaluating cases in the context later (Englund et al., 2007). Similarly, among
of workers’ compensation claims or personal persons with carpal tunnel syndrome there was
injury litigation, where pain can directly influence no association between electrodiagnostic findings
the assessment of disability. and symptoms/disability (Chan et al., 2007).
Conversely, many people present to physicians
Physical Factors Alone Are Inadequate with a range of physical symptoms for which no
to Explain Outcome medical explanation can be found. Seventy-two
A strict physical reductionist school of thought percent of the Englund et al. (2008) sample with-
holds that all pain is entirely physically based and out meniscal pathology or radiographic evidence
that all treatment should follow from this assump- of osteoarthritis were symptomatic. Nearly 20% of
tion (see Thompson, 2005). This reductionist patients seen in primary care clinics present with
thought runs counter to current mainstream sci- medically unexplained symptoms, a finding not
ence which views symptoms and disability result- explained by comorbid depression or anxiety
ing from pain-related conditions (see appendix (Barsky, Orav, & Bates, 2005). Among first-time
A) as a product of biological, psychological, and referrals to a neurology service, 61% (59% of
social factors (Gatchel, Peng, Peters, Fuchs, & females, 63% of males) had at least one medically
Turk, 2007). Research has increasingly demon- unexplained symptom (Fink, Steen Hansen, &
strated that pathological findings from commonly Sondergaard, 2005). Thirty-four percent (41.3%
used medical diagnostic techniques (see appendix of females, 27.7% of males; 20.5% of inpatients,
B) are not sufficient to explain current or future 43.2% of outpatients) of the Fink et al. (2005)
pain-related symptoms or disability. cases met the diagnostic criteria an ICD-10 soma-
For example, pathology of the spine is associ- toform disorder. In this context, neuropsycholo-
ated with pain generation (Schwarzer, Aprill, gists are particularly familiar with psychogenic
Derby, Fortin, Kine, & Bogduk, 1995). However, nonepileptic seizures which are diagnosed in
objective evidence of physical pathology does not approximately 30% of patients evaluated using
always correlate with pain symptoms or disability, video-EEG monitoring (Benbadis, O’Neill, Tatum,
nor does it reliably predict who will become symp- & Heriaud, 2004; Gates, Ramani, Whalen, &
tomatic in the future. Cervical and lumbar disc Loewenson, 1985; Martin, Burneo, Prasad, et al.,
abnormalities including some that would be con- 2003). Please refer to the chapter by Binder in
sidered surgical lesions have been found in the the present volume for further discussion of
cervical and lumbar spines of asymptomatic unexplained medical symptoms.
patients (Boden, Davis, Dina, Patronas, & Wiesel,
1990; Boden, McCowin, Davis, Dina, Mark, & Summary
Wiesel, 1990; Jarvik, Hollingworth, Heagerty, In short, objective physical findings do not fully
Haynor, & Deyo, 2001). Additionally, the presence explain the symptom presentation of a large
of an identifiable abnormality of the disc or spinal proportion of patients seen by physicians for
canal in the lumbar spine of asymptomatic patients complaints of pain. In fact, psychosocial factors
does not predict subsequent low back pain seven may predict important outcomes (e.g., return to
years later (Borenstein et al., 2001). What did pre- work) better than physical variables such as MRI-
dict the development of low back pain after three identified morphologic abnormalities (Schade,
years in cases asymptomatic at baseline was self- Semmer, Main, Hora, & Boos, 1999). Linton
reported depression at baseline (Jarvik, (2000) has shown that multiple psychological
Hollingworth, Heagerty, Haynor, Boyko, & Deyo, factors are important in the transition from acute
2005). Those cases with depression at baseline to chronic pain. Pain is a complex biopsychoso-
consistently reported greater experienced pain. cial phenomenon and consideration of psychoso-
This applies not just to spine pain but to other cial factors is essential for a comprehensive
types of pain as well. Incidental meniscal findings understanding and appropriate treatment of pain
on MRI of the knee are common in the general and patients with pain (Gatchel & Okifuji, 2006).
population (Englund et al., 2008) and more than These findings emphasize the point that the
60% of persons with one or more meniscal tears pathophysiology and psychosocial factors
are asymptomatic. Further, a finding of meniscal underlying pain-related disability are complex
304 forensic neuropsychology

individually and in combination and that an dystonia. A higher degree of family conflict was
understanding of both is important in the clinical present in the somatization group; however, there
management of the pain patient (Gatchel & were no group differences for neglect, sexual
Okifuji, 2006). The remainder of this chapter will abuse, or witnessing violence. Exposure to emo-
review these psychosocial factors along with tional abuse accounted for 50% of the variance in
relevant contextual factors and the evidence for unexplained symptoms. These effects are not
their association with chronic pain and their simply explained by psychiatric comorbidity, as
relevance for the clinical management of the Spitzer, Barnow, Gau, Freyberger, and Grabe
chronic pain patient. Issues pertaining to assess- (2008) found that the odds of having been sexu-
ment and diagnosis will then be discussed. Finally, ally abused in childhood were nine times higher
we will discuss the treatment of patients with pain in persons who met DSM criteria for somatiza-
and the role of the psychological evaluation in tion disorder than in those meeting criteria for
those interventions. major depressive disorder.
Childhood adversity is also an important
P S Y C H O S O C I A L FA C T O R S predictor of pain symptoms (Davis, Luecken, &
I N PA I N Zautra, 2005). For example, Walker, Gelfand,
There has now developed a voluminous empirical Gelfand, Green, and Katon (1996) found that
literature demonstrating that consideration of women with both irritable bowel syndrome and
psychosocial factors is essential for a comprehen- chronic pelvic pain were significantly more likely
sive understanding and appropriate treatment of to have a history of childhood sexual abuse as well
patients with pain (see the following for detailed as a variety of mood and anxiety disorders, soma-
reviews: Gatchel, 2004; Keefe, Rumble, Scipio, tization disorder, and hysterectomy. Childhood
Giordano, & Perri, 2004; Linton, 2000). The rela- adversities are more common in somatoform
tionship between psychosocial factors and pain- pain disorder and fibroymalgia is particularly
related disability is not simple cause-and-effect associated with severe maltreatment in childhood
but is instead reciprocal and complex. Linton (Imbierowicz & Egle, 2003; Walker, Keegan,
(2000) concluded that these factors are related to Gardner, Sullivan, Bernstein, & Katon, 1997;
every aspect and phase of neck and back pain and Wolfe & Hawley, 1998). Brown, Berenson,
particularly important in the transition between and Cohen (2005) surveyed a community sample
acute and chronic pain. (n = 649) and found that adult chronic pain was
The following section will review several of associated with a history of sexual abuse and that
these factors and their relationship to outcome in this effect was not attributable to the presence of
pain-related conditions. The subsequent section depression at the time the survey was completed.
will review the influence of socioeconomic factors Walsh, Jamieson, Macmillan, and Boyle (2007)
in pain. While this chapter presents these factors found that a history of physical and sexual abuse
in discrete sections, this should not imply inde- differentiated adult chronic pain patients with
pendence. Many of the factors are closely linked disability in some aspect of their life from those
conceptually and functionally, and recognition of with pain but without disability.
their interrelatedness is important. A history of childhood adversity is also associ-
ated with outcomes in the treatment of pain
Childhood Adversity (Linton, 2000). Two studies are particularly
Childhood adversity in the form of abuse, neglect, revealing. In the first study, Schofferman,
and abandonment has proven an important Anderson, Hines, Smith, and White (1992) retro-
predictor of current and future mental health spectively examined 86 patients who underwent
(Arnow, 2004; Taylor & Jason, 2002; Widom, lumbar spine surgery. Five types of childhood
DuMont, & Czaja, 2007). For example, childhood trauma were considered: 1) physical abuse;
adversity is associated with the presence of medi- 2) sexual abuse; 3) emotional neglect or abuse;
cally unexplained symptoms. Brown, Schrag, and 4) abandonment; and, 5) chemically dependent
Trimble (2005) found that physical/emotional caregiver. Of patients who had experienced three
abuse was more common and more extreme in or more of these types of trauma, 85% had unsuc-
patients with unexplained neurological symptoms cessful surgical outcomes. In contrast, only 5% of
who met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of the patients who had experienced none of these
Mental Disorders (DSM) criteria for somatization traumas had unsuccessful surgery. A similar study
disorder than in those with a neurologically based of patients who had undergone multidisciplinary
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 305

evaluation for refractory back pain also found a patients use their physical symptoms as a way of
high incidence of childhood traumas, especially dealing with, and communicating about, their
in patients with minimal signs of pathology emotional lives . . . in this type of symptom mag-
(Schofferman, Anderson, Hines, Smith, & Keane, nification, physical symptoms may be easier to
1993). accept as causing current unhappiness and
Not surprisingly, health care utilization is discontent than admitting that some psychologi-
greater in persons with a history of abuse, neglect, cal reason is contributing to it” (p. 204; Gatchel,
or serious family dysfunction in childhood 2004). In short, somatization reflects the expres-
(Arnow, 2004; Arnow, Hart, Hayward, Dea, & sion of psychological problems manifested in
Barr-Taylor, 2000). Even disability retirement is physical symptoms and complaints, a tendency to
associated with childhood adversity. Of nearly complain of or develop physical symptoms and
9,000 community survey respondents ranging in illness when under emotional stress, and be exces-
age from 40 to 54, those with multiple childhood sively focused on one’s physical functioning.
adversities were more than three times as likely to Somatization may be best viewed as a potentially
take disability retirement compared to those with maladaptive personality and/or coping style. That
no history of such adversity (Harkonmaki et al., is, as used here, somatization refers not to a diag-
2007). Spitzer et al. (2008) concluded “childhood nostic entity but to a mode of thinking about one’s
sexual abuse is an important factor in the multi- self and world that contributes to medically unex-
factorial aetiopathogenesis of somatization disor- plained illness and excess disability.
der” (p. 335) and Arnow (2004) stated that “the The MMPI (Hathaway & McKinley, 1943) and
more severe the abuse, the stronger the associa- its revision, the MMPI-2 (Butcher et al., 1989), are
tion with poor outcomes in adulthood” (p. 10). the most widely used psychological assessment
The mechanisms underlying the translation of instruments (Lubin, Larsen, & Matarazzo, 1984)
adverse childhood experiences into health-related and are among the most commonly used in evalu-
phenomena are complex and multifactorial ating chronic pain patients (e.g., Robinson,
(Spitzer et al., 2008). Physiologically, it is known Swimmer, & Rallof, 1989; Keller & Butcher, 1991;
that childhood adversity causes acute and chronic Deardorff, Chino, & Scott, 1993; Slesinger, Archer,
disruption of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal & Duane, 2002). The MMPI has been used in the
(HPA) axis (Bremner & Vermetten, 2001; Tarullo, evaluation of chronic pain patients to predict
& Gunnar, 2006; Teicher, Andersen, Polcari, response to treatment (Love & Peck, 1987), likeli-
Anderson, Navalta, & Kim, 2003) and is associ- hood of return to work (e.g., Bigos et al., 1991;
ated with the development of psychopathology Vendrig, Derken, & de Mey 1999), and outcome
and certain pain syndromes (i.e., chronic pelvic from spinal surgery (Block, Gatchel, Deardorff,
pain; Heim, Ehlert, Hanker, & Hellhammer, 1998). & Guyer, 2003). Classically, elevations on scales
Behaviorally, Waldinger et al. (2006) found that in 1 (Hypochondriasis) and 3 (Hysteria) of the
women, fearful attachment mediated the link MMPI have been ascribed to factors such as som-
between childhood trauma and somatization. In atization (Block et al, 2003; Blumetti & Modesti,
men, attachment style and trauma each contrib- 1976; Friedman, Gleser, Smeltzer, Wakefield, &
uted independently to the development of soma- Schwartz, 1983; Marks & Seeman, 1963).
tization. The authors concluded that “childhood Studies by Bigos et al. (1991) and Applegate
trauma shapes patients’ styles of relating to others et al. (2005) showed that early (pre-injury) ten-
in times of need, and these styles, in turn, influ- dencies or predispositions for somatization, as
ence the somatization process” (p. 129). The pro- measured by the MMPI, are linked to subsequent
cess of somatization may be one link between development of physical symptoms including
childhood psychological trauma and problematic back pain. Bigos et al. (1991) conducted a longitu-
pain outcomes (Roelofs & Spinhoven, 2007). dinal study of 3,020 aircraft employees to identify
risk factors for reporting acute back pain at work,
Somatization about 16% of whom developed back problems
Somatization and related terms (e.g., somatoform over the four-year follow-up period. During
disorder) have a complex and sometimes contro- slightly more than 4 years of follow-up, 279
versial history (Lamberty, 2008). Nonetheless, as a subjects reported back problems. The 20% of
narrow concept, somatization is a central factor in participants with the highest Scale 3 scores were
understanding disability attributed to chronic twice as likely to report back problems as those
pain. Somatization refers to the way “certain with lower scores. Applegate et al. (2005) found
306 forensic neuropsychology

that the MMPI at college admission (n = 2332) Edwards, Bingham, Bathon, and Haythornthwaite
predicted pain-related conditions at 30 year (2006) reviewed four mechanisms by which pain
follow-up. Among men, Scales 1, 3, and 5 catastrophizing may act: 1) interfering with pain
(Masculinity/Femininity) were most predictive of coping and beneficial health behaviors; 2) increas-
the number of chronic pain conditions. Among ing attention to pain; 3) amplifying pain process-
women, Scales 1, 3, and 6 (Paranoia) were most ing in the central nervous system; and, 4)
predictive. Thus, for both men and women, Scales maladaptive impacts on the social environment.
1 and 3 predict chronic pain 30 years later. Pain catastrophizing predicts the development
In patients with chronic pain, somatization is of chronic pain complaints in the general popula-
associated with more medically unexplained tion (Severeijns, Vlaeyen, van den Hout, & Picavet,
symptoms, poorer response to treatment, and 2005). Pain catastrophizing is associated with
future development of disability. For example, greater pain vigilance and preoccupation with pain
Block, Vanharanta, Ohnmeiss, and Guyer (1996) and physical problems (Goubert, Crombez, & Van
found a higher incidence of nonorganic symptom Damme, 2004) and may mediate the reduced activ-
responses on discograms in patients with eleva- ity level seen in some clinical patients (Sullivan,
tion on Scales 1 and 3, suggesting that these scales Stanish, Sullivan, & Tripp, 2002). Pain catastroph-
are related to psychological mechanisms of pain izing is also related to variety of important func-
complaints, including somatization, rather than tional and outcome variables, pain intensity,
purely physiological processes. Block et al. also psychological distress, and level of disability
found that these Scale elevations were associated (Severeijns, Vlaeyen, van den Hout, & Weber, 2001;
with poorer response to both surgery and conser- Turner, Jensen, Warms, & Cardenas, 2002; Woby,
vative care. Gatchel, Polatin, Mayer, and Garcy Watson, Roach, & Urmston (2004). This relation-
(1994) examined acute back pain patients (n = ship is independent of level of physical injury or
152) and found that elevations on Scale 3 pre- impairment (Severeijns et al., 2001). For reviews of
dicted higher levels of disability one year later. pain catastrophizing, see Sullivan et al. (2001) and
Edwards et al. (2006). There is evidence that pain
Catastrophizing catastrophizing is a precursor to the development
Over the past 25 years “pain catastrophizing” has of pain-related fear (Leeuw et al., 2007).
emerged as an important predictor of pain experi-
ence and pain-related disability (Sullivan et al., Personality Disorder
2001). Pain catastrophizing has been defined as a The prevalence of personality disorder in the
tendency to fear pain, have a fear-inducing under- chronic pain population ranges from 31% to 60%
standing of the meaning of pain (e.g., the presence (Gatchel et al., 1996; Fishbain et al., 1986; Reich,
of pain is an indication of harm), and/or a ten- Tupin, & Abramowitz, 1983; Large, 1986; Monti
dency to allow pain to be a dominant focus of et al., 1998; Polatin et al., 1993). There is consider-
one’s life (Proctor, Gatchel, & Robinson, 2000). able variability in rates of specific disorders, prob-
More simply, it is the interpretation of pain as ably due to issues of diagnostic and assessment
being extremely threatening (Crombez, Eccleston, reliability (Adams, Luscher, & Bernat, 2001). The
Baeyens, & Eelen, 1998). Psychometrically, pain most common specific disorders are paranoid,
catastrophizing has proven to be a stable construct histrionic, borderline, avoidant, and dependent.
that is related to but not redundant with depres- Others have identified a sense of entitlement
sion and other forms of psychological distress (Bellamy, 1997; Cook & Degood, 2006) and the
(Sullivan et al., 2001). presence of embitterment (Swirtun & Renstrom,
Turner and Aaron (2001) suggest that cata- 2008) as important personality factors in patients
strophizing reflects a relatively stable personality with pain. Personality disorders are considered to
disposition whose manifestation may be influ- be long-standing patterns of interaction and thus
enced by situational variables such as changes in not a consequence of a specific event (DSM-
physical condition or implementation of specific IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000).
cognitive interventions. Theoretical formulations However, there is evidence that the report of life-
to explain pain catastrophizing and hypotheses time symptomology may be influenced by state
regarding the mechanisms by which pain cata- factors (Fishbain et al., 2006; Weisberg, 2000a)
strophizing influence reports of pain and pain- and contextual factors such as financial incentive.
related disability have only begun to emerge We have examined the effect of malingering in
(Sullivan et al., 2001; Turner & Aaron, 2001). pain patients on Axis II scale scores from the
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 307

Millon Multiaxial Clinical Inventory-III (MCMI- seeking treatment for pain-related conditions
III; Millon, Davis, & Millon, 1997). Chronic pain are particularly likely to report symptoms of
patients who met published criteria for either depression, as the prevalence of major depression
Definite or Probable Malingered Pain-Related in this population has been reported to be over
Disability (MPRD; Bianchini, Greve & Glynn, 50% (Dersh et al., 2006; Mayer, Towns, Neblett,
2005) scored significantly higher than nonmalin- Theodore, & Gatchel, 2008).
gering pain patients on the Avoidant, Depressive, Depression is a particularly important consid-
Dependent, Negativistic, Masochistic, Schizoty- eration in patients with chronic pain, as studies
pal, and Borderline scales (Aguerrevere, Greve, have shown a near linear association between
Bianchini, & Meyers, 2009). They also tended to self-reported pain intensity and depressive symp-
score higher on the Schizoid and Paranoid scales. toms (Carroll, Cassidy, & Cote, 2000; Currie &
In contrast, those who showed no evidence of Wang, 2004). However, depression and chronic
malingering scored higher on the scales associ- pain have a complex, reciprocal, relationship as:
ated with narcissistic, histrionic, and compulsive (a) there is some overlap between symptoms (e.g.,
personality disorders. Neither group tended to sleep disturbances or reduced activity levels);
elevate the Antisocial and Sadistic scales. These (b) they may share physiological mechanisms,
data show that malingering is associated with self- specifically norepinephrine (NE) and 5-HT
report that emphasizes distress and disability. serotonin dysregulation (Bair, Robinson, Katon,
When personality issues are present in nonmalin- & Kroenke, 2003); (c) the presence of either pre-
gering pain patients the tendency is toward their dicts future development of the other (Gureje,
having a sense of specialness (narcissistic), dra- Simon, & Von Korff, 2001); and (d) comorbidity
matic over-reaction and need for attention (histri- complicates treatment for both conditions
onic), and emotional/behavioral over-control (Moultry & Poon, 2009). Depression may be a
(compulsive). particularly important predictor of pain-related
Consideration of personality disorders in pain disability (Alshuler, Theisen-Goodvich, Haig, &
is important because “the treatment success Geisser, 2008), with studies suggesting that it may
(defined by decreased pain perception, increased serve as a moderator for the relationships between
function, and decreased health care use) with other psychological vulnerabilities discussed in
patients with personality disorders is low, and any this section and self-perceived disability (Boersma
way to maximize compliance, independence, & Linton, 2005, 2006).
decreased emotionality, and other features of per- Anxiety can also influence perceptions of pain
sonality disorders are [sic] likely to result in and response to treatment. Anxious expectations
improved outcomes” (Weisberg, 2000b; p. 235). have been found to significantly increase the per-
Weisberg (2000b) goes on to outline several rea- ceived intensity of painful stimuli by directly
sons for assessing for personality disorder in facilitating nociceptive transmission (Colloca &
patients with pain. First, identification of long- Benedetti, 2007). Anxiety also impacts outcome
standing personality features helps determine following surgery, as higher pre-surgical anxiety
changes in interaction style that are attributable to is associated with slower recovery and more com-
the painful condition and thus provides a clearer plications post-surgery (Kiecolt-Glaser, Page,
target for treatment. Second, identification of Marucha, MacCallum, & Glaser, 1998). Similarly,
maladaptive interaction styles can help the Trief, Grant, and Fredrickson (2000) found that
clinician adjust their reactions to the patient higher levels of pre-surgical anxiety significantly
so as to facilitate the development of a therapeutic predicted poorer functional outcome one year
relationship. Finally, the treatment itself can be after receiving lumbar spine surgery to relieve
modified to better fit the patient’s character. pain. The negative effects of anxiety on patient
outcome may be explained in part by the relation-
Mood and Anxiety Disorders ship between anxiety sensitivity and fear of pain
Mood and anxiety disorders are common in (Martin, McGrath, Brown, & Katz, 2007).
patients with chronic pain. In a large survey of the
general population, approximately 18% of persons Fear-Avoidance Model of
reporting chronic spinal pain were diagnosed Pain-Related Disability
with a comorbid mood disorder, while approxi- Fear is a reaction that occurs in the presence of a
mately 27% were diagnosed with some form of specific, identifiable, immediate threat; often lead-
anxiety disorder (Von Korff et al., 2005). Patients ing to escape or avoidance behaviors (Rachman,
308 forensic neuropsychology

1998). Fear-avoidance models of chronic pain- pain-related fear and anxiety (Vlaeyen et al.,
related disability have been proposed whereby the 2004). It is the emotional and coping response to
emotional, cognitive, and behavioral factors dis- the injury which determines whether recovery
cussed above interact with the pain experience to will be complicated. Coping strategies account for
contribute to a reinforcing cycle of fear and anxi- adjustment to chronic pain above and beyond
ety towards pain-related stimuli (see Asmundson, what is predicted by pain-related history and
Vlaeyen, & Crombez, 2004; Vlaeyen, Kole- tendency to somaticize (Rosenstiel & Keefe, 1983).
Snijders, Boeren, & van Eek, 1995; Vlaeyen & Regardless of the specific trigger, fear-avoidance is
Linton, 2000). Fears can be directed towards pain an obstacle to functional improvement in chronic
itself, reinjury, or specific activities such as move- pain patients (Smeets, van Geel, & Verbunt, 2009;
ment (i.e., kinesiophobia). Avoidance of activities, Turner et al., 2006; Vlaeyen et al., 1995; Vlaeyen &
in turn, contributes to the development and main- Linton, 2000).
tenance of functional disability (Leeuw et al.,
2007; Woby, Watson, Roach, & Urmston, 2004). Summary
Fear-avoidance models of pain-related disability Psychological factors serve as important prog-
are similar to models of the development and nostic indicators of cases that transition from
maintenance of panic disorder and agoraphobia acute to chronic pain (Carragee, Alamin, Miller,
(Klein & Gorman, 1987). & Carragee, 2005; Dersh, Gatchel, & Polatin, 2001;
Figure 12.1 illustrates Leeuw et al.’s (2007) Keefe et al., 2004; Linton, 2000; Pincus, Burton,
version of this model. Importantly, this model Vogel, & Field, 2002). Psychopathology can
explicitly includes physical injury and pain sever- increase perceived pain intensity, hamper reha-
ity. The maladaptive element of the Fear-Avoidance bilitation efforts, and magnify perceived disabili-
model is reciprocal which allows for increasing ties, all of which serve to reinforce and perpetuate
disability and the development of comorbid pain-related dysfunction (Gatchel & Dersh, 2002;
depression. Pain catastrophizing appears to be a Holzberg, Robinson, Geisser, & Gremillion, 1996;
critical element, or trigger, in the development of Leeuw et al., 2007).

INJURY
DISUSE
DISABILITY
DEPRESSION
RECOVERY

AVOIDANCE
ESCAPE
PR TIV
MO
L

EV ATI
SA

EN ON
OU

CONFRONTATION
TA
AR

PAIN PAIN EXPERIENCE


TIV

DE TIV

ANXIETY
MO
E

FE ATI
L
SA

HYPERVIGILANCE
NS ON
OU

IVE

FEAR
AR

OF PAIN
THREAT PERCEPTION

CATASTROPHIZING LOW FEAR

NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY
THREATENING ILLNESS INFORMATION

FIGURE 12.1: Fear-avoidance model of pain-related disability. Science+Business Media: Leeuw, M.,
Goossens, M. E., Linton, S. J., Crombez, G., Boersma, K., & Vlaeyen, J. W. (2007). The fear-avoidance
model of musculoskeletal pain: Current state of scientific evidence. Reproduced from Journal of Behavioral
Medicine, 30, 77–94, with kind permission from Springer.
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 309

C O N T E X T UA L R I S K et al., 2002; Hoogendoorn et al., 2002; Shaw et al.,


FA C T O R S 2005). A study by Hagen et al. (2000) found that
In the past twenty years a sizeable literature has unskilled workers are two to three times more
developed examining noninjury characteristics/ likely to retire due to disability than professionals
risk factors, of patient or context, and how these and concluded that this relationship may be
affect symptomatic and functional outcomes. For partly due to a social class effect, rather than just
example, injury recovery is affected by physical physical job demands, as the relationship between
characteristics such as age and weight, as well as professional level and disability retirement
demographic factors such as education and remained consistent at higher levels of levels of
income (Rubin, 2007). It is also important to keep the socioeconomic scale. Volinn, Van Koevering,
in mind that recovery from injury often occurs in and Loeser (1991) also found that lower pay is
the context of a complex social network, a consid- associated with longer back-pain chronicity.
eration that is particularly essential when it comes Similarly, Tate (1992) found that younger workers
to compensable injuries, where complications can with higher preinjury wages, greater seniority,
arise from a number of sources including the and less severe injuries were more likely to return
patient’s attitudes about their work or company, to work post injury.
the relationship with the insurance and claims Elements of the relationship between the
adjusters, treatment delays, attorney involvement, worker and the company, including job satisfac-
and litigation. This effect is not limited to injuries tion and availability of accommodations, can also
that are clearly altered or contaminated by psy- impact outcome and even the initiation of symp-
chological factors. Rather, contextual factors are tom reports. For example, in a study of 3,020
known to impact injury results even in patients aircraft industry employees, those who reported
with well-described and documented physical that they “hardly ever” enjoyed their work were
injuries. The effect has been shown in patients 2.5 times more likely to report a back injury than
with more significant and documented physical those who reported more positive feelings about
injuries and in patients with less clearly docu- their job/work (Bigos et al., 1991). Shaw, Pransky,
mented physical injury, including whiplash (Atlas Patterson, and Winters (2005) examined psycho-
et al., 2000, 2006; Carroll et al., 2008; Cassidy, social factors in patients referred to an occupa-
Carroll, Cote, Berglund, & Nygren, 2003; Cassidy tional medicine clinic for back pain and found
et al., 2000). that some elements of the patient’s job character-
istics, including job tenure, physical work
Education demands, availability of modified duty, and earlier
Lower education has been identified as a prognos- reporting to employer, were more predictive of
tic indicator of work-related disability (Breslin outcome than physical examination. Similarly,
et al., 2008; Hagen, Holte, Tambs, & Bjerkedal, Turner, Franklin, Fulton-Kehoe, Sheppard,
2000). A review by Dionne et al. (2001) found that Wickizer, and Wu (2007) found that baseline
lower education is associated with longer pain demographic variables, symptom severity, func-
duration following back injury and a higher rate of tional limitations, lack of job accommodation,
pain recurrence. Even after controlling for age, job physical demands, job psychosocial condi-
pain duration, sex, and incentive status, lower tions, and psychosocial characteristics predicted
education was significant associated with higher chronic disability following a claim for carpal
self-perceived disability (Roth & Geisser, 2002). tunnel syndrome. Each domain of variables
This relationship may be explained by the finding (sociodemographic, clinical, psychosocial, work-
that lower education is associated with more mis- related) added significantly to the prediction of
conceptions about back pain (Goubert, Crombez, chronic disability.
& De Boudeauhuij, 2004), as Roth and Geisser Aside from the complexities of these interac-
found that the relationship between education tions, the injured worker who is attempting to
and disability was mediated by maladaptive pain return to work may encounter obstacles. For the
beliefs and coping strategies (e.g., catastrophizing). subset of patients that have performed heavy
physical labor, returning to their previous level of
Occupation and Work-Related Factors work may no longer be possible and a return to
Physical work load and job satisfaction both are even modified work can pose considerable chal-
prognostic indicators of back-pain-related work lenges. The sometimes daunting obstacles for a
absences and disability (Bigos et al., 1991; Hagen worker to return to work include: insufficient
310 forensic neuropsychology

training or education to do work other than heavy treatment and patient dissatisfaction with care
work, advanced age, a feeling of uncertainty about that can arise in some treatment systems.
other job tasks, poor information or misinforma-
tion about what would be required for a work Delays in Treatment
return, unavailability of vocational rehabilitation, When an injury occurs within a compensatory
and financial disincentives for returning to context there are a number of different sources for
work; all superimposed on the other risk factors delays in the ultimate rehabilitation of the patient.
discussed in this chapter. For example, in many state worker’s compensa-
tion systems second opinions can be required
Financial Incentive Effects before surgeries or rehabilitation can be funded.
It is not uncommon for chronic pain cases to be On the other side of things, Bernacki and Tao
seen in a context involving financial compensa- (2008) found that the presence of attorney
tion such as personal injury litigation, workers representation is associated with longer claim
compensation, or disability determinations. Over duration, suggesting some mechanism of delay.
the years, the presence of financial incentive has Thus, either “side” can contribute to delays. It
been shown to be a robust factor related to out- should be mentioned that these delays are not
come in patients with pain. Patients seen in a minor or inconsequential: Following the switch
compensatory context report significantly more from a tort-compensation system to a no-fault
pain, depression, and disability than patients not system in Canada, Cassidy et al. (2000) observed
involved in compensation (Chibnall & Tait, 1994; that the average time between injury and claim
Rainville, Sobel, Hartigan, & Wright, 1997; resolution for whiplash was more than halved.
Rohling, Binder, & Langhinrichsen-Rohling, Crook and Moldofsky (1994) conducted a
1995). Further, compensation status is associated prospective, longitudinal, cohort study of patients
with overall decreased treatment efficacy (Gatchel, with musculoskeletal soft tissue impairment fol-
Polatin, & Mayer, 1995; Rainville et al., 1997; lowing a work-related injury. They looked at prog-
Rohling et al., 1995) including worse surgical out- nostic factors for the likelihood that the injured
comes (Harris, Mulford, Solomon, van Gelder, & worker would return to work or remain on work
Young, 2005), even for clearly defined spinal disability at a given point in time. Factors included
pathology (Atlas et al., 2000; Atlas et al., 2006). As gender, age, return to work attempts, and site of
an example of the systemic effects of compensa- injury. They showed a strong relationship between
tory context, recent changes to a “no fault” com- the passage of time and persistent disability.
pensation system in Canada were found to result Efforts to return early to work contributed to a
in a lower incidence of lower-back pain and whip- decrease in overall work disability.
lash injuries following accidents and better prog- McIntosh, Frank, Hogg-Johnson, Bombardier,
nosis for recovery (Cameron et al., 2008; Cassidy, & Hall (2000) examined compensation claimants
Carroll, Cote, Berglund, & Nygren, 2003). seen for rehabilitation to determine prognostic
There is evidence that at least some of the rela- factors of chronicity. They found several factors
tionship between compensation and injury recov- predicted work disability: 1) working in the con-
ery for some patients may be due to alterations in struction industry; 2) older age; 3) lag time from
their clinical presentations. For example, a longi- injury to treatment; 4) pain referred into the leg;
tudinal study by Overland et al. (2008) found that and 5) three or more positive Waddell’s nonor-
reports of pain, anxiety, depression, sleep distur- ganic signs. They identified protective factors,
bances, and somatic symptoms steadily increased including a higher score on a modified version of
as a financial disability determination neared, the low back outcome score, intermittent pain,
only to steadily decrease after the determination and prior history of back problems. They listed
was made. In fact, a study by our group found that several possibilities to explain the apparent coun-
a sizeable minority of chronic pain patients seen terintuitive problem of a prior history of back
in a compensatory context were exaggerating problems, including a prior history of learning
symptoms and disability during psychological through physical therapy leading to increased
evaluation (Greve et al., 2009). However, the coping skills abilities, and also noted that, although
relationship between compensable injury and this group may have additional future problems,
recovery is complex and cannot be fully explained once a patient has had back problems and returned
by patient presentation. The following discussion to work this may predict a shorter duration of
will focus on the contributions of delays in future problems.
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 311

Olney, Quenzer, and Makowsky (1999) exam- controlling for demographics, satisfaction with
ined patients who underwent carpal tunnel release medical care prior to injury, job satisfaction, type
in three different contexts: 1) no worker’s com- of injury, and provider type. They found the
pensation claim; 2) uncontested worker’s com- injured workers who reported less favorable treat-
pensation claim; and 3) a contested worker’s ment experiences were 3.54 times as likely (95%
compensation claim that required the interven- confidence interval, 1.20–10.95, P = .021) to be
tion of an attorney. There were no statistically sig- receiving time lost compensation for inability to
nificant differences in postoperative return of grip work due to injury 6 or 12 months after filing
strength and in postoperative return to work the claim compared to patients whose treatment
intervals in comparing the non-work-related experience was more positive. This is important
patients with the uncontested worker’s compensa- because it shows not only the relationship with the
tion patients. However, the contested worker’s company but also that the relationship with treat-
compensation patients were much less likely and ment providers can be important for understand-
slower to return to light-duty and to return to full ing and predicting outcome in injured workers.
duty. Return of grip strength was slower and less The same group (Wickizer et al., 2004) also
in the contested worker’s compensation patients. examined the relationship between patient
These examiners concluded that within worker’s satisfaction and retention of an attorney. They
compensation, contested claims were associated found that patient satisfaction was a significant
with a higher risk of poor outcome; while patients predictor of attorney retention by an injured
with uncontested worker’s compensation claims worker. However, since the mean date from claim
had outcomes nearly as good as noncompensa- initiation to attorney retention was so long (368
tion patients. days) they conclude that attorney retention was a
Similar results were reported by Stover, correlate rather than a predictor of disability.
Wickizer, Zimmerman, Fulton-Kehoe, and
Franklin (2007) in a larger prospective cohort Summary
sample of workers’ compensation claimants with An understanding of the context of the care of
more diverse injuries. These examiners studied patients with pain is of demonstrable importance
population-based retrospective inception cohort given the relationship, described in this section,
of 81,077 workers who had four or more days of between some of these variables and measures of
work disability resulting from workplace injuries symptoms and outcomes. This may be particu-
over six years’ time. They found similar predictors larly important in patients who are treated in a
of long-term disability in this different and larger context where financial incentive is present, as
population including: delay between injury and these situations are commonly accompanied by
first medical treatment, older age, construction complex interrelationships between various mem-
industry, logging occupation, longer time for bers of the claims environment (e.g., companies,
medical treatment to claim filing, back injury, insurance adjusters, attorneys). Thus, these rela-
smaller firm size, female gender, higher unem- tionships can be the source of good or bad effects
ployment rate, and having dependents. on outcome and some patients may have increased
vulnerability to the potential negative effects.
Satisfaction with Care
Wickizer, Franklin, Fulton-Kehoe, Turner, Mootz, DIAGNO STIC AND
and Smith-Weller (2004) examined injured AS SES SMENT IS SUES
worker satisfaction with their care and the impact
of these feelings on important outcome variables. Diagnostic Considerations
Their sample included 681 workers who had From a psychological perspective there are a
ongoing follow-up care after initial treatment number of different clinical/diagnostic phenom-
and examined their satisfaction with interper- ena/entities that are relevant in the assessment
sonal and technical aspects of care, finding that and treatment of the chronic pain patient. Many
satisfaction with interpersonal technical aspects of those have been discussed in the preceding
of care was strongly and positively associated with sections. The following section addresses the
their ratings of their overall treatment experience. three major conditions which would be associated
As a group they found that the satisfaction mea- with nonorganic or nonphysiological presen-
sures explain 3% of the variance in the patient’s tations by persons claiming chronic pain:
reports of the overall treatment experience after somatoform disorders, factitious disorder, and
312 forensic neuropsychology

malingering. All of these are particularly impor- other (Barsky & Borus, 1999). Importantly,
tant, not just because they complicate treatment patients with somatoform disorders are not con-
and recovery, but also because they can lead to sciously feigning or exaggerating symptoms, as is
invasive treatment efforts (e.g., surgery) which seen with factitious disorders and malingering.
themselves can be physically damaging and cause
disability. These conditions may also contribute to Factitious Disorder
over-prescribing and over-use of narcotic pain Factitious disorder is the consistent intentional
medications and other medications with addic- production of signs or symptoms of illness in the
tion potential (e.g., benzodiazepines). absence of external incentives. Symptom presen-
tation is deliberate and motivation comes from
Somatoform Disorders internal sources such as a need to assume the sick
The somatoform disorders category of the role. Presentation may include symptom fabrica-
DSM-IV includes somatization disorder, undif- tion (e.g., subjective complaints that do not exist),
ferentiated somatoform disorder, conversion dis- symptom production (e.g., intentional self-harm),
order, pain disorder, hypochondriasis, body symptom exaggeration, or manipulation of objec-
dysmorphic disorder, and somatoform disorder tive medical signs (e.g., warming a thermometer).
NOS. The common feature of these disorders is Pain complaints are the most commonly reported
that they are characterized by persistent com- symptom in patients with factitious disorder
plaints of physical symptoms that suggest a medi- (Krahn, Li, & O’Conner, 2003).
cal condition, but for which there is either no Establishing the prevalence of factitious
identifiable physiological cause or, in the case of disorder is made difficult by the deceptive
pain disorder, where medical findings do not nature of the condition, reluctance to assign the
explain all of the symptoms or degree of disability. “factitious disorder” label, and the possibility that
As such, psychological factors are thought to play some factitious patients may be counted multiple
a role in the onset, severity, and maintenance of times. Estimates have placed the prevalence of
physical symptoms. factitious disorder at around 1% for patients seen
Nearly half (45%) of medical inpatients have in a general hospital setting (Sutherland & Rodin,
at least one medically unexplained symptom and 1990). An extreme form of factitious disorder
approximately 20% meet DSM-IV criteria for a known as Munchausen syndrome is thought to
somatoform disorder (Fink, Hansen, & Oxhoj, account for approximately 10% of these cases.
2004). Prevalence is generally higher in women Overall, factitious disorder is thought to be about
than in men and approximately half of somaticiz- twice as prevalent in females as in males (Krahn,
ing patients have comorbid anxiety or depression Li, & O’Conner, 2003).
(Barsky, Orav, & Bates, 2005). However, while the Factitious disorder is differentiated from
construct of somatization is important for under- somatoform disorders, where symptom produc-
standing chronic pain, somatoform diagnoses tion is unconscious; and malingering, where
based on DSM-IV criteria can be problematic external incentives are present. However, it should
considering that many patients seeking treatment be noted that it is not always possible to defini-
for chronic pain meet criteria for somatoform tively differentiate factitious disorder from malin-
pain disorder while almost none meet criteria for gering. For example, a feigned illness could be
somatization disorder (Polatin et al., 1993; Dersh motivated by a need for attention (internal
et al., 2006). incentive), a desire to avoid household or work
Somatoform tendencies (the tendencies duties (external incentive), or both. In these cases
common to somatoform patients) include com- the DSM-IV allows for reasonable inferences to
ponents that are cognitive, perceptual, and behav- be made regarding the source of individual’s
ioral in nature. Elements include excessive worry motivation (i.e., internal versus external) as direct
about serious illness (i.e., hypochondriasis), evidence is not always available.
hypervigilence towards bodily sensations, nega- Patients with factitious disorder have been
tive interpretations or disease attributions for observed to intentionally engage in self-injurious
benign sensations, sensitization to averse somatic behavior and seek out unnecessary medical treat-
sensations, and excessive care-seeking or need for ment (Eastwood & Bisson, 2008; Lande, 1996;
illness validation. These factors are often viewed Masterton, 1995; Paar, 1994; Wise & Ford, 1999).
as a cycle in which cognitions, perceptions, and However, factitious disorder and malingering are
behaviors mutually reinforce and strengthen each not necessarily as distinct as the DSM–IV seems
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 313

to suggest. Eisendrath (1996) presented several the factitious patient. Of course these patients
cases in which the self-injurious behavior of often actively try to avoid detection.
patients with factitious disorder served as the
basis of civil litigation. In these particular cases, Malingering
the symptom production not only met the intra- Malingering is a potential problem in contexts
psychic needs of the factitious patient but was also where there are financial incentives to appear
reinforced by the potential secondary gain associ- disabled. Malingering is “the intentional produc-
ated with litigation. Similarly, Greve, Bianchini, tion of false or grossly exaggerated physical or
and Ameduri (2003) reported a patient with a psychological symptoms, motivated by external
psychological profile consistent with factitious incentives such as avoiding military duty, avoid-
disorder who intentionally feigned sensory loss ing work, obtaining financial compensation,
that she attributed to a work injury. evading criminal prosecution, or obtaining drugs”
Eisendrath and McNeil (2004) discuss the (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association,
complex cases of four factitious disorder patients 2000; p. 739). Pain often occurs in the context of a
who also had financial incentive in the form of legally compensable event such as a work-related
civil litigation and who died as a result of their injury or incident in which some other party is
factitious behavior. Since both factitious disorder potentially liable. In fact, back pain is the most
and malingering involve intentional exaggeration common reason for filing a workers compensa-
of symptoms these cases illustrate that the line tion claim (Guo, Tanaka, Halperin, & Cameron,
between malingering and factitious disorder can 1999).
sometimes be blurred. More specifically, the The prevalence of malingering in patients with
source of motivation for intentional exaggeration pain is between 20% and 50% (Mittenberg, Patton,
can come from both the desire to assume the sick Canyock, & Condit, 2002; Greve, Ord, Bianchini,
role and the desire for external gain, potentially in & Curtis, 2009; Kay & Morris-Jones, 1998),
the same patient. depending on the method of assessment and case-
It is reasonable to assume that factitious specific factors. These rates are consistent with
disorder reflects greater psychopathology than those reported in other compensable conditions
that seen in most persons who choose to malin- and contexts such as criminal forensic settings
ger. However, there is no reason to believe that (Ardolf, Denney, & Houston, 2007), social secu-
patients with factitious tendencies are any less rity disability evaluations (Chafetz, 2008), toxic
subject to the influences of the potential second- exposure (Greve, Bianchini, Black et al., 2006),
ary gain available in the medico-legal context than traumatic brain injury (Larrabee, 2003), and
persons without the intrapsychic needs character- among Vietnam-era veterans receiving services
istic of factitious disorder. Moreover, the level of within the Veterans Administration system
premorbid psychopathology in some patients (Larrabee, Millis, & Meyers, 2008). These studies
who do malinger may approach that more charac- demonstrate that overall malingering, including
teristic of factitious patients, yet their behavior in patients with pain, is not a rare event or trivial
still reflects malingering. Put simply, some malin- phenomenon despite claims to the contrary
gerers will engage in potentially self-injurious (e.g., Fishbain, Cutler, Rosomoff, & Rosomoff,
behavior and malingering should not be ruled out 1999; Sears, Wickizer, & Franklin, 2008). In
simply on the basis that a person is seeking medi- patients with pain, malingering potentially has a
cally unnecessary treatment including surgery very large economic impact given the prevalence
(Bianchini, Heinly, & Greve 2004; Bianchini & of pain-related complaints.
Greve, 2009). DSM-IV-TR does not provide criteria for the
Factitious disorder presents an unusual but diagnosis of malingering; instead it describes
important form of psychological complication of circumstances under which “malingering should
physical symptom complaints. Of particular be strongly suspected” (p. 739). There are,
interest is the characteristic willingness of some however, published criteria for the diagnosis of
factitious patients to engage in self-harm. The malingering. The Slick, Sherman, and Iverson
Eisendrath and McNeil (2004) paper cited above (1999) criteria for Malingered Neurocognitive
includes patients whose factitious tendencies Dysfunction (MND) can be used with patients
ultimately resulted in their death. Thus detection with pain though this system is narrowly focused
of this form of psychological complication is on malingering of cognitive deficits and is thus
important for the life and sometimes survival of limited in application to pain patients (Bianchini,
314 forensic neuropsychology

Etherton, & Greve, 2004; Greve, Bianchini, Black who have been diagnosed as malingering using
et al., 2006; Greve, Bianchini, Love, Brennan, & explicit criteria (usually based on the Slick et al.,
Heinly, 2006). Bianchini, Greve, and Glynn (2005) 1999, or Bianchini et al., 2005, systems) to
modified the Slick et al. system to address the etiologically similar patients, often ones with
multidimensional (e.g., cognitive, emotional, greater objective pathology than the malingering
physical) clinical presentation of patients with group. This method is referred to as “known-
pain. Their approach differed conceptually from groups” (Greve & Bianchini, 2004; Rogers, 1997)
earlier diagnostic systems by focusing on disabil- or “criterion-groups” (Frederick, 2000; Dawes &
ity attributed to the compensable injury or illness Meehl, 1966) validation. Using this approach one
rather than the injury or illness itself. In this view, can identify score levels beyond which few or
the question of interest in addressing malingering no nonmalingering patients perform. Thus, the
in pain is not just about the veracity of the pain degree of “improbability” is expressed as the false
complaint itself, but of the degree of disability that positive error rate at a given score level. Over the
is attributed to the pain. last decade and a half numerous techniques for
Thus, the magnitude of disability becomes the the detection of poor effort, response bias, under-
central forensic issue because how disabled a performance, and malingering have been devel-
person is or claims to be dramatically affects the oped and empirically validated. A review of this
monetary value of his or her claim. Malingered research is beyond the scope of this chapter and
pain-related disability (MPRD) refers to the inten- the reader is referred to two recently published
tional exaggeration or fabrication of cognitive, texts on this topic for more information (Boone,
emotional, behavioral, or physical dysfunction 2007; Larrabee, 2007), as well as the chapter by
attributed to pain for the purposes of obtaining Larrabee in the present volume.
financial gain, to avoid work, or to obtain drugs While the data on cognitive malingering in
(incentive). As with any malingering diagnosis, a traumatic brain injury patients can be generalized
diagnosis of MPRD requires evidence of external to patients with pain these results may result in an
incentive and intentional symptom production. increased false negative error rate in pain patients
Typically, it is not difficult to identify the presence who lack objective brain pathology (Bianchini,
and nature of external incentives in a given case. Etherton, & Greve, 2004). Moreover, the numerous
Determination of intent is a more difficult task. psychosocial factors that can complicate a chronic
At the heart of the issue of malingering is the pain presentation may result in high false
question “is the patient intentionally performing positive error rates on self-report measures sensi-
below his or her true capacity or manifesting more tive to somatization (Aguerrevere, Guise, Greve,
disability/symptoms than is actually the case?” Bianchini, Meyers, 2008; Bianchini, Etherton,
Importantly, neither the Slick et al. (1999) Greve, Heinly, & Meyers, 2008). Therefore, a
criteria for MND nor the Bianchini et al. (2005) number of indicators of malingered cognitive,
criteria for MPRD require that the clinician deter- emotional, and physical disability have now been
mine whether or not a specific/single behavior is validated in patients with chronic pain (some of
diagnostic of malingering. Instead, the ultimate these methods are discussed in the assessment sec-
determination of intent is dealt with in a more tion below). Validation of physical capacity mea-
comprehensive manner by considering multiple, sures for the detection of malingering is currently
highly improbable events as indicative of intent. under way in our laboratory and in Europe (J. Kool,
Thus, it is not necessary to rely on single events personal communication, January 28, 2009).
that unequivocally demonstrate intent because A striking feature of some of these data is that
the MND and MPRD criteria are based on behav- the rate of positive findings (i.e., test failure) is
iors and symptom report that are atypical for and associated in a dose-response fashion with
not representative of expected clinical findings in increasing objective evidence of malingering and
legitimate, unequivocal neurological, psychiatric, not with injury characteristics (Ben-Porath,
or developmental disorders. Thus, intent is Greve, Bianchini, & Kaufmann, 2009; Greve,
inferred as a result of the combined improbability Bianchini, Etherton, Meyers, Curtis, & Ord, 2009;
of events rather than necessarily relying on a Greve, Bianchini, Etherton, Ord, & Curtis, 2009;
single definitive indication of intent (Larrabee, Greve, Etherton, Ord, Bianchini, & Curtis, 2009).
Greiffenstein, Greve, & Bianchini, 2007). This finding in pain patients is complemented
Generally speaking, “improbability” is deter- by the finding in TBI patients that the magnitude
mined empirically by comparing clinical patients of potential compensation is associated with
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 315

malingering test failures (Bianchini, Curtis, & After the clinical interview the patient is
Greve, 2006). Interestingly, in Canada, a change in administered standardized psychological and neu-
personal injury liability law was followed by a nearly ropsychological tests and procedures. Pain patients
50% decline in disability claims related to back pain may report disabling problems in multiple domains
(Cassidy, Carroll, Cote, Berglund, & Nygren, 2003). (e.g., physical, emotional, and cognitive; Schnurr
Taken together, these findings support the conclu- & MacDonald, 1995; Myers & Diep, 2000; Ericsson
sion that empirically defined “improbable” test per- et al., 2002; Wilson, Eriksson, D’Eon, Mikail &
formance is motivated by financial incentive. Emery, 2002), all of which may have important
implications for the decision-making of various
Assessment clinicians (Gatchel, 2004). Therefore, the psycho-
This section is intended to provide an overview of logical pain evaluation should be designed to
the conceptual approach to the assessment of the objectively assess the common cognitive, psycho-
chronic pain patient and not to discuss in detail logical, emotional, and physical complaints of
the many psychological tests which might be patients with pain and pain-related disability
included in such an evaluation. The psychological including attention and concentration, processing
evaluation must be capable of separating symp- speed, learning and memory, general knowledge,
toms/disabilities involving psychological reac- intelligence and problem solving skills, physical
tions to physical symptoms, including pain, from complaints, and physical capacity, current emo-
somatization and from intentional exaggeration tional state, and personality style. It is helpful to
(e.g., malingering, factitious disorder) at a level use broad measures of intelligence such as the
consistent with currently available techniques and WAIS-III/IV or WASI as well as well-validated
research. Given the importance of psychological neuropsychological tests of memory and related
factors in various forms in cases with chronic cognitive functions, omnibus personality tests and
pain, when properly conducted, the psychological behavioral health measures. The inclusion of
evaluation has the potential to shed important narrow measures of constructs not tapped by the
light on the clinical circumstances of a given broader personality tests (e.g. pain catastrophiz-
patient and provide important guidance to treat- ing, coping skills) is important as well.
ment efforts. However, in order to do this prop- Moreover, because the results of psychological
erly, a comprehensive approach is required, one tests can be invalidated by biased responding and/
that actively considers all of the differential diag- or poor effort, the psychological pain evaluation
nostic possibilities. should contain indicators over-reporting of sub-
As with any psychological evaluation, the psy- jective symptoms (e.g. MMPI-2 validity scales)
chological pain evaluation requires a careful and under-performance on ability tests (both
review of all available medical records and, when stand-alone SVTs and embedded indicators of
possible, any other records which may have bear- performance validity). The comprehensive assess-
ing on subsequent conclusions (e.g., accident ment of response and performance validity is
reports, personnel records, depositions). Like any important even in cases not seen in a compensa-
psychological evaluation, the pain psychological tion-seeking context because exaggeration of
evaluation will typically begin with a clinical inter- pain-related disability may also be motivated by
view. This will generally address the nature of the the desire to obtain pain medication. Unlike many
accident, efforts at treatment, and current status psychological assessments of patients with pain
including symptoms and complaints. However, that tend to focus on emotional function alone,
the interview should also assess the variety of this multidimensional battery approach allows a
complicating factors such as recent and ongoing comprehensive assessment of an individual’s
psychosocial stressors, past psychiatric history, functional status that can lead to detailed recom-
childhood psychological trauma, and job factors mendations regarding the management of
that might predict poor outcome from treatments. their pain and pain-related disability and supply
Ideally, patients should not be given information valuable information to vocational experts and
regarding pain-related symptoms, including life-care planners. This approach to the assess-
symptom checklists and direct questioning about ment of the chronic pain patient that incorporates
symptoms, until after they have been asked to SVTs and neuropsychological tests of memory,
spontaneously report all of their symptoms in all working memory, processing speed, and intellec-
domains so as to reduce the risk of suggesting tual and problem solving skills, is similar to that
symptoms that may bias subjective report. of Meyers and Diep (2000).
316 forensic neuropsychology

I N T E RV E N T I O N A L I S S U E S and pain catastrophizing as well as compliance


problems and deception.
Pre-procedure Psychological Screening These risk factors have been summarized and
This chapter summarizes the literature showing integrated into a presurgical screen risk factor
that psychosocial factors influence pain complaints check list by Block et al. (2001). A similar medical
and pain-related disability. The importance of con- risk factor checklist is part of their comprehensive
sidering such factors in the context of invasive presurgical screen. See Table 12.1 for both. The
medical efforts to relieve pain, which do involve presurgical screening approach developed by
damaging tissue and thus have the potential to do Block et al. (2001) has been thoroughly explicated
harm, is essential. in Block et al. (2003) and the ultimate process is
laid out in the form of a flowchart (see Figure 12.2;
“Spine surgery’s ultimate effectiveness . . . flowchart from Block et al., 2003, pg. 108). Similar
depends on much more than the surgeons’ approaches have been reported for screening of
diagnostic acumen and technological skill. patients being considered for morphine pump
Psychological factors exert very strong influ- and dorsal column stimulator implantation
ences–ones that can improve, or inhibit, the (Bruns & Disorbio, 2009) though these have not
patient’s ultimate recovery . . . surgical results yet been subjected to the same level of research. It
can be greatly augmented by the inclusion of is likely that similar evaluation of all pain patients
psychological components in the assessment will identify those who would be expected to have
and preparation of patients for spine surgery, complicated course and delayed outcomes regard-
as well as in post-operative rehabilitation” (p. 4; less of the specific interventions.
Block et al., 2003). Block et al. (2003) have argued that pre-
procedure psychological screening is an essential
The psychological assessment of surgical risk component in the medical diagnostic process
is not simply a matter of determining if a patient of spine surgery candidates, especially when the
is psychotic, has a pathological body image, has major goal is symptomatic pain reduction. Fur-
somatic delusions, or is depressed. While the pres- ther emphasizing this point, Lebovits (2000) has
ence of significant psychopathology which might stated that
lead to post-surgical psychological instability or
problems with medical compliance is an issue of “although the treatment of a patient with
importance, this type of problem may be a minor- chronic pain mandates a comprehensive evalu-
ity in presurgical pain cases, particularly those ation of the medical as well as psychological
seen in a medicolegal context. There is now strong contributions to the etiology, maintenance,
empirical research regarding what psychological and exacerbation of pain, evaluating and
factors predict outcome from surgery (e.g., Block treating patients with chronic pain with a uni-
et al., 2003; Bruns & Disorbio, 2009; Voorhies, modal, strictly medical approach still occurs.
Jiang, & Thomas, 2007). This, unfortunately, often leads to iatrogenic
Nelson, Novy, Averill, and Berry (1996) have effects, such as failed surgical interventions
argued that in addition to active psychosis, sui- and activity restriction” (p. 126).
cidality, untreated major depression, and sub-
stance dependence, factors such as somatization, Psychological factors and presurgical screen-
lack of social support, and cognitive deficits ing may provide a very specific benefit for those
should exclude patients from receiving a spinal managing a pain case. Psychological factors are
cord stimulator. They also noted that disincentive important to surgery outcome even in the context
to recover related to compensation or litigation of clear and objective indications for surgery
issues was a factor that would require exclusion. (Voorhies et al., 2007). In cases where there is
Similarly, Block and colleagues (Block et al., 2001; disagreement about the physical indications for
Epker & Block, 2001) have demonstrated that the surgery, the results of a presurgical psychological
presence of certain psychosocial factors predicts screen may provide even more valuable guidance.
poor outcome from lumbar disc surgery. Among Specifically, a psychological evaluation that
these factors are financial incentive, history of identifies factors in a patient that would predict
abuse or abandonment, job dissatisfaction, prob- higher risk of poor outcome from surgical inter-
lems with social support, substance abuse, pre- vention may encourage continued conservative
existing psychopathology, depression/anxiety, interventions and even a shift to functional
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 317

TABLE 12.1 CHECKLIST OF MEDICAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL


RISK FACTORS FROM BLOCK ET AL., 2001 

Medical Risk Factors Score Psychological Risk Factors Score

Chronicity Litigation
< 6 months 0 Pending litigation 2
6–12 months 1 Workers’ compensation
12 + months 2 Currently receiving 2
Job dissatisfaction
Previous spine surgery Moderate 1
0 0 Extreme 2
1 1 Heavy job demands
2+ 2 > 50-pound frequent lift 2
Substance abuse
Surgery type Pre-injury 1
Laminectomy/discectomy 1 Current 2
Fusion 2 Family reinforcement of pain
Moderate 1
Nonorganic signs Extreme 2
Present 2 Marital dissatisfaction
Moderate to extreme 1
Nonspine medical treatment Physical or sexual abuse
Some treatment 1 Pre-injury 1
Multiple hospitalizations 2 Current 2
Pre-injury psychological treatment
Smoking Outpatient 1
< 1 pack per day 1 Inpatient 2
> 1 pack per day 2 MMPI Elevations >70 (maximum score 4)
Hypochondriasis 2
Obesity Hysteria 2
< 50% overweight 1 Depression (pre-existing) 2
Depression (reactive) 2
Psychopathic deviate 2
Psychasthenia 1
Coping Strategies Questionnaire (max score 2)
Low self-reliance 2
Poor pain control 2

Medical total score: Psychological total score:


High risk threshold = 8 High risk threshold = 10
Total maximum = 13 Total maximum = 23

restoration that incorporates active management Towns, Neblett, Theodore, & Gatchel, 2008). This
of psychological risk factors and comorbidities recognition has led to the utilization of a variety
(see discussion below). of psychiatric and psychological therapies for the
treatment of pain, pain related disability, and
Behavioral Pain Treatments psychiatric comorbidities to pain conditions.
Patients with pain conditions, particularly A full review of the role of psychopharmacology
chronic conditions, have a higher incidence of management strategies for pain and pain-related
affective disorders than the general public (Dersh, affective disorders is beyond the scope of this
Gatchel, Mayer, Polatin, & Temple, 2006; Mayer, chapter. The reader is referred to Polatin and
318 forensic neuropsychology

8+ Present
6+ Poor Prognosis
Total Discharge
Total
psychosocial Adverse recommended
medical
4 –7 clinical
risk risk
features

Absent Poor Prognosis


0–5 Noninvasive
treatment
0–3 recommended

Interview Risk Factors Total 6+


Factor Pts. medical Present Fair Prognosis
Job dissatisfaction 2 risk Compliance and
Worker’s comp. 2 motivation
Litigation 2 Adverse measures
Spousal solicit. 1 clinical recommended
No spouse support 1 features
Abuse & abandon. 1
Substance abuse 2 0–5 Absent
Good Prognosis
Psych. history 2
Postoperative
psychological
treatment
Testing Risk Factorsa recommended
Factor Pts. Medical Risk Factors
Pain sensitivity 2 Factor Pts.
Depression-chronic 2 Pain 6–12 mos. 1
Depression-reactive 1 Pain > 12 mos. 2 Adverse Clinical Features
Highly destructive surgery 2 Good Prognosis
Anger 2
Nonorganic signs 2 Inconsistency No psych.
Anxiety 2
Abnormal pain drawing 2 Medication seeking treatment
Depressed-pathol. 4
Prior spine surgeries Staff splitting needed
Catastrophizing 2 2 or more 2 Compliance issues
1 1 Threatening
aMaximum of 4 points for MMPI Prior medical probs. 2 Resignation
items + 2 points for Smoking 2 Deception
catastrophizing Obesity 1 Personality disorders

FIGURE 12.2: Algorithm for determining presurgical psychological screening prognosis. MMPI =
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Reproduced from Block, Gatchel, Deardorff, & Guyer, 2003,
with permission from the APA.

Dersh (2004) for a review of relevant issues and Guzman et al. (2002), working through
applications of these interventions. the Cochrane Database System, recently con-
Psychological treatments, including cognitive cluded that multiple reviewed studies provide
behavior therapy (CBT) and behavior therapy “strong” evidence that intensive multidisciplinary
(BT), have been studied for their efficacy in biopsychosocial rehabilitation with a functional
dealing with pain, pain disability, and affective restoration approach improves pain and function.
comorbidities, including depression. Applied in They also indicate that less intensive interventions
isolation these treatments have a weak effect on did not show improvements in clinically relevant
pain and disability, although they may be of some outcomes. They also indicate there was contra-
help in altering mood outcomes (Eccleston, dictory evidence regarding vocational outcomes
Williams, & Morley, 2009). In contrast, combin- of intensive intervention, some trials reported
ing therapies in a multidisciplinary rehabilitation improvements in work readiness, but others
format has shown promise, particularly for treat- showed no significant reduction in sickness
ing patients at higher risk for poor outcome. The leaves. They note that particularly the less inten-
central features of these approaches include mul- sive outpatient psychophysical treatments did not
tidisciplinary symptom treatment, including improve pain function or vocational outcomes
physical therapy treatment and psychological when compared with non-multidisciplinary out-
assessment and treatment for psychological com- patient therapy or usual care.
orbidities, an assessment of psychological and Recently, Howard, Mayer, and Gatchel (2009)
other risk factors for poor outcome, the system- studied one important component of this
atic management of the risk factors, and an orga- approach: the emphasis on a work return. Injured
nized and systematic approach to work return workers who stayed at work (“presentees”)
(Westman et al., 2006). were more likely than “absentees” to complete a
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 319

functional restoration treatment program, to Arnow, B. A. (2004). Relationships between childhood


return to work, to retain work 1-year post treat- maltreatment, adult health and psychiatric out-
ment, and to not have a decreasing job demand comes, and medical utilization. Journal of Clinical
from preinjury to post-treatment time periods. Psychiatry, 65 Suppl 12, 10–15.
Thus, the emphasis on early work return may be a Arnow, B. A., Hart, S., Hayward, C., Dea, R., & Barr
particular critical factor in the rehabilitation of Taylor, C. (2000). Severity of child maltreatment, pain
patients with pain. Ultimately, consideration of complaints and medical utilization among women.
psychosocial and socioeconomic complexities of Journal of Psychiatric Research, 34(6), 413–21.
Ash, L. M., Modic, M. T., Obuchowski, N. A., Ross, J.
patients with pain in a comprehensive pain man-
S., Brant-Zawadzki, M. N., & Grooff, P. N. (2008).
agement program with a functional restoration
Effects of diagnostic information, per se, on patient
focus can lead to a reduction in the need for inva-
outcomes in acute radiculopathy and low back
sive treatment procedures and overall improved pain. AJNR: American Journal of Neuroradiology,
outcomes (Gatchel & Okifuji, 2006). 29(6), 1098–1103.
Alschuler, K. N., Theisen-Goodvich, M. E., Haig, A. J.,
REFERENCES & Geisser, M. E. (2008). A comparison of the rela-
Adams, H. E., Luscher, K. A., & Bernat, J. A. (2001). tionship between depression, perceived disability,
The classification of abnormal behavior: An over- and physical performance in persons with chronic
view. In H. E. Adams & P. B. Sutker (Eds.), pain. European Journal of Pain, 12, 757–764.
Comprehensive handbook of psychopathology (3rd Asmundson, G. J., Vlaeyen, J. W., & Crombez, G.
ed., pp. 3–28). New York: Kluwer Academic/ (Eds.). (2004). Understanding and treating fear of
Plenum Publishers. pain. Oxford: Oxford Press.
Aguerrevere, L. E., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., & Meyers, Atlas, S. J., Chang, Y., Kammann, E., Keller, R. B., Deyo,
J. E. (2009). Validation of the Bianchini et al. (2005) R. A., & Singer, D. E. (2000). Long-term disability
Criterion for Malingered Pain-Related Disability. A and return to work among patients who have a her-
poster presented at the NAN’s 26th Annual niated lumbar disc: the effect of disability compen-
Conference & Workshops. New Orleans, LA. sation. Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery. American
Aguerrevere, L. E., Guise, B. J., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, Volume, 82(1), 4–15.
K. J., & Meyers, J. E. (2008). Detecting Malingered Atlas, S. J., Chang, Y., Keller, R. B., Singer, D. E., Wu, Y.
Pain-Related Disability with the Modified Somatic A., & Deyo, R. A. (2006). The impact of disability
Perception Questionnaire and the Pain Disability compensation on long-term treatment outcomes
Inventory. A poster presented at the AACN’s 6th of patients with sciatica due to a lumbar disc her-
Annual Conference & Workshops. Boston, MA. niation. Spine, 31(26), 3061–69.
Alexander, M. P. (1995). Mild traumatic brain injury: Bair, M. J., Robinson, R. L., Katon, W., & Kroenke, K.
Pathophysiology, natural history, and clinical man- (2003). Depression and pain comorbidity: a litera-
agement. Neurology, 45(7), 1253–60. ture review. Archives of Internal Medicine, 163,
American Academy of Pain Management. (2003). 2433–45.
Proceedings of the 2003 annual meeting of the Barsky, A. J. & Borus, J. F. (1999). Functional somatic
American Chronic Pain Association. Denver, CO. syndromes. Annals of Internal Medicine, 130(11),
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic 910–21.
and statistical manual, Fourth Edition, Text revision. Barsky, A. J., Orav, E. J., & Bates, D. W. (2005).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Somatization increases medical utilization and costs
Andersson, G. B. (1998). Epidemiology of low back independent of psychiatric and medical comorbid-
pain. Acta Orthopaedica Scandinavica. Supple- ity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(8), 903–910.
mentum, 281, 28–31. Bellamy, R. (1997). Compensation neurosis: Financial
Applegate, K. L., Keefe, F. J., Siegler, I. C., Bradley, L. A., reward for illness as nocebo. Clinical Orthopaedics
McKee, D. C., Cooper, K. S., et al. (2005). Does and Related Research, (336), 94–106.
personality at college entry predict number of Benbadis, S. R., O’Neill, E., Tatum, W. O., & Heriaud, L.
reported pain conditions at mid-life? A longitudi- (2004). Outcome of prolonged video-EEG moni-
nal study. Journal of Pain, 6(2), 92–97. toring at a typical referral epilepsy center. Epilepsia,
Ardolf, B. R., Denney, R. L., & Houston, C. M. 45(9), 1150–53.
(2007). Base rates of negative response bias and Bennett, R. (2007). Myofascial pain syndromes and
malingered neurocognitive dysfunction among their evaluation. Best Practice and Research. Clinical
criminal defendants referred for neuropsychologi- Rheumatology, 21(3), 427–45.
cal evaluation. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, Ben-Porath, Y. S., Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., &
21(6), 899–916. Kaufmann, P. M. (2009). The MMPI-2 Symptom
320 forensic neuropsychology

Validity Scale (FBS) is an empirically validated scans of the lumbar spine in asymptomatic
measure of over-reporting in personal injury liti- subjects. A prospective investigation. Journal of
gants and claimants: Reply to Butcher et al. (2008). Bone and Joint Surgery. American Volume, 72(3),
Psychological Injury and the Law, 1, 62–85. 403–408.
Bernacki, E. J. & Tao, X. G. (2008). The relationship Boden, S. D., McCowin, P. R., Davis, D. O., Dina, T. S.,
between attorney involvement, claim duration, and Mark, A. S., & Wiesel, S. (1990). Abnormal mag-
workers’ compensation costs. Journal of Occupati- netic-resonance scans of the cervical spine in
onal and Environmental Medicine, 50(9), 1013– asymptomatic subjects. A prospective investiga-
1018. tion. Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery. American
Bianchini, K. J., Curtis, K. L., & Greve, K. W. (2006). Volume, 72(8), 1178–84.
Compensation and malingering in traumatic brain Boersma, K. & Linton, S. J. (2005). Screening to iden-
injury: A dose-response relationship? The Clinical tify patients at risk: Profiles of psychological risk
Neuropsychologist, 20(4), 831–47. factors for early intervention. Clinical Journal of
Bianchini, K. J., Etherton, J. L., & Greve, K. W. (2004). Pain, 21(1), 38–43; discussion 69–72.
Diagnosing cognitive malingering in patients with Boersma, K. & Linton, S. J. (2006). Psychological
work-related pain: Four cases. Journal of Forensic processes underlying the development of a chronic
Neuropsychology, 4, 65–85 pain problem: A prospective study of the relation-
Bianchini, K. J., Etherton, J. L., Greve, K. W., Heinly, ship between profiles of psychological variables in
M. T., & Meyers, J. E. (2008). Classification accu- the fear-avoidance model and disability. Clinical
racy of MMPI-2 validity scales in the detection of Journal of Pain, 22(2), 160–66.
pain-related malingering: A known-groups study. Bohr, T. W. (1995). Fibromyalgia syndrome and myo-
Assessment, 15(4), 435–49. fascial pain syndrome. Do they exist? Neurologic
Bianchini, K. J. & Greve, K. W. (2009). Chronic pain as Clinics, 13(2), 365–84.
a context for malingering. In J. E. Morgan & J. J. Boone, K. B. (Ed.). (2007). Assessment of feigned cogni-
Sweet (Eds.), Neuropsychology of malingering case- tive impairment: A neuropsychological perspective.
book (pp. 307–32). New York: Psychology Press. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Bianchini, K. J., Greve, K. W., & Glynn, G. (2005). On Boos, N., Rieder, R., Schade, V., Spratt, K. F., Semmer,
the diagnosis of malingered pain-related disability: N., & Aebi, M. (1995). 1995 Volvo Award in clini-
Lessons from cognitive malingering research. The cal sciences. The diagnostic accuracy of magnetic
Spine Journal, 5(4), 404–417. resonance imaging, work perception, and psycho-
Bianchini, K. J., Heinly, M. T., & Greve, K. W. (2004). social factors in identifying symptomatic disc
Unnecessary cervical disc surgery for a malinger- herniations. Spine, 20(24), 2613–25.
ing patient. Spine Journal, 4(6), 718–719. Borenstein, D. G., O’Mara, J. W., Jr., Boden, S. D.,
Bigos, S. J., Battie, M. C., Spengler, D. M., Fisher, L. D., Lauerman, W. C., Jacobson, A., Platenberg, C.,
Fordyce, W. E., Hansson, T. H., et al. (1991). A pro- et al. (2001). The value of magnetic resonance
spective study of work perceptions and psychoso- imaging of the lumbar spine to predict low-back
cial factors affecting the report of back injury. pain in asymptomatic subjects: A seven-year
Spine, 16(1), 1–6. follow-up study. Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery.
Block, A. R., Gatchel, R. J., Deardorff, W. W., & American Volume, 83-A(9), 1306–1311.
Guyer, R. D. (2003). The psychology of spine Bremner, J. D. & Vermetten, E. (2001). Stress and
surgery. Washington, DC: American Psychological development: Behavioral and biological conse-
Association. quences. Development and Psychopathology, 13,
Block, A. R., Ohnmeiss, D. D., Guyer, R. D., Rashbaum, 473–89.
R. F., & Hochschuler, S. H. (2001). The use of Breslin, F. C., Tompa, E., Zhao, R., Pole, J. D., Amick Iii,
presurgical psychological screening to predict B. C., Smith, P. M., et al. (2008). The relationship
the outcome of spine surgery. Spine Journal, 1(4), between job tenure and work disability absence
274–82. among adults: A prospective study. Accident Analy-
Block, A. R., Vanharanta, H., Ohnmeiss, D. D., & Guyer, sis and Prevention, 40(1), 368–75.
R. D. (1996). Discographic pain report. Influence Brown, J., Berenson, K., & Cohen, P. (2005).
of psychological factors. Spine, 21(3), 334–38. Documented and self-reported child abuse and
Blumetti, A. & Modesti, L. (1976). Psychological pre- adult pain in a community sample. Clinical Journal
dictors of success or failure of surgical intervention of Pain, 21(5), 374–77.
for intractable back pain. In J. J. Bonica & D. Albe- Brown, R. J., Schrag, A., & Trimble, M. R. (2005).
Fessard (Eds.), Advances in pain research and ther- Dissociation, childhood interpersonal trauma, and
apy. New York: Raven Press. family functioning in patients with somatization
Boden, S. D., Davis, D. O., Dina, T. S., Patronas, N. J., & disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(5),
Wiesel, S. W. (1990). Abnormal magnetic-resonance 899–905.
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 321

Bruehl, S. & Chung, O. Y. (2006). Psychological Chan, L., Turner, J. A., Comstock, B. A., Levenson, L.
and behavioral aspects of complex regional pain M., Hollingworth, W., Heagerty, P. J., et al. (2007).
syndrome management. Clinical Journal of Pain, The relationship between electrodiagnostic find-
22(5), 430–37. ings and patient symptoms and function in carpal
Bruns, D. & Disorbio, J. M. (2009). Assessment of tunnel syndrome. Archives of Physical Medicine
biopsychosocial risk factors for medical treatment: and Rehabilitation, 88(1), 19–24.
A collaborative approach. J Clin Psychol Med Chapman, C. R. (1978). Pain: The perception of nox-
Settings, 16(2), 127–47. ious events. In R. A. Sternbach (Ed.), The psychol-
Buitenhuis, J., de Jong, P. J., Jaspers, J. P., & Groothoff, J. ogy of pain (pp. 169–202). New York: Raven Press.
W. (2008). Catastrophizing and causal beliefs in Chibnall, J. T. & Tait, R. C. (1994). The Pain Disability
whiplash. Spine, 33(22), 2427–33; discussion 34. Index: Factor structure and normative data.
Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W.G., Graham, J.R., Tellegen, Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989). MMPI-2: Manual for 75(10), 1082–86.
administration and scoring. Minneapolis, MN: Clauw, D. J. (2007). Fibromyalgia: Update on mecha-
University of Minnesota Press. nisms and management. Journal of Clinical Rheu-
Butler, S. (2001). Disuse and complex regional pain matology, 13(2), 102–109.
syndrome. In R. N. Harden, R. Baron, & W. Janig Colloca, L. & Benedetti, F. (2007). Nocebo hyperalge-
(Eds.), Complex regional pain syndrome. Seattle: sia: how anxiety is turned into pain. Current
IASP Press. Opinions in Anaesthesiology, 20, 435–39.
Cameron, I. D., Rebbeck, T., Sindhusake, D., Rubin, G., Cook, A. J. & Degood, D. E. (2006). The cognitive risk
Feyer, A. M., Walsh, J., et al. (2008). Legislative profile for pain: Development of a self-report
change is associated with improved health status in inventory for identifying beliefs and attitudes that
people with whiplash. Spine, 3, 250–54. interfere with pain management. Clinical Journal of
Cameron, T. (2004). Safety and efficacy of spinal cord Pain, 22(4), 332–45.
stimulation for the treatment of chronic pain: Couch, J. R., Lipton, R. B., Stewart, W. F., & Scher, A. I.
A 20-year literature review. Journal of Neuro- (2007). Head or neck injury increases the risk of
surgery, 100(3 Suppl Spine), 254–67. chronic daily headache: A population-based study.
Carragee, E. J., Alamin, T. F., Miller, J. L., & Carragee, J. Neurology, 69(11), 1169–77.
M. (2005). Discographic, MRI, and psychosocial Croft, P., Rigby, A. S., Boswell, R., Schollum, J., &
determinants of low back pain disability and Silman, A. (1993). The prevalence of chronic wide-
remission: A prospective study in subjects with spread pain in the general population. Journal of
benign persistent back pain. Spine Journal, 5(1), Rheumatology, 20(4), 710–713.
24–35. Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Baeyens, F., & Eelen, P.
Carroll, L. J., Holm, L. W., Ferrari, R., Ozegovic, D., & (1998). When somatic information threatens, cata-
Cassidy, J. D. (2009). Recovery in whiplash-associ- strophic thinking enhances attentional interfer-
ated disorders: Do you get what you expect? Journal ence. Pain, 75(2–3), 187–98.
of Rheumatology, 5, 1063–1070. Crook, J. & Moldofsky, H. (1994). The probability of
Carroll, L. J., Holm, L. W., Hogg-Johnson, S., Cote, P., recovery and return to work from work disability
Cassidy, J. D., Haldeman, S., et al. (2008). Course as a function of time. Quality of Life Research, 3
and prognostic factors for neck pain in whiplash- Suppl 1, S97–109.
associated disorders (WAD): Results of the Bone Currie, S. R. & Wang, J. (2004). Chronic back pain and
and Joint Decade 2000–2010 Task Force on Neck major depression in the general Canadian popula-
Pain and Its Associated Disorders. Spine, 33(4 tion. Pain, 107, 54–60.
Suppl), S83–92. D’Ambrosia, R. D. (2005). Epidemiology of osteoar-
Cassidy, J. D., Carroll, L., Cote, P., Berglund, A., & thritis. Orthopedics, 28(2 Suppl), s201–205.
Nygren, A. (2003). Low back pain after traffic Davis, D. A., Luecken, L. J., & Zautra, A. J. (2005). Are
collisions: A population-based cohort study. Spine reports of childhood abuse related to the experi-
(Phila Pa 1976), 28(10), 1002–1009. ence of chronic pain in adulthood? A meta-analytic
Cassidy, J. D., Carroll, L. J., Cote, P., Lemstra, M., review of the literature. Clinical Journal of Pain,
Berglund, A., & Nygren, A. (2000). Effect of elimi- 21(5), 398–405.
nating compensation for pain and suffering on the Dawes, R. M. & Meehl, P. E. (1966). Mixed group vali-
outcome of insurance claims for whiplash injury. dation: A method for determining the validity of
New England Journal of Medicine, 342(16), 1179–86. diagnostic signs without using criterion groups.
Chafetz, M. D. (2008). Malingering on the social Psychological Bulletin, 66(2), 63–67.
security disability consultative exam: Predictors Deardorff, W. W., Chino, A. F., & Scott, D. W. (1993).
and base rates. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, Characteristics of chronic pain patients: Factor
22(3), 529–46. analysis of the MMPI-2. Pain, 54(2), 153–58.
322 forensic neuropsychology

de Mos, M., de Bruijn, A. G., Huygen, F. J., Dieleman, J. predicts disability in long-term chronic pain patients.
P., Stricker, B. H., & Sturkenboom, M. C. (2007). Disability and Rehabilitation, 24(6), 334–40.
The incidence of complex regional pain syndrome: Ferrari, R. & Schrader, H. (2001). The late whiplash
a population-based study. Pain, 129, 12–20. syndrome: A biopsychosocial approach. Journal
Dersh, J., Gatchel, R. J., Mayer, T., Polatin, P., & Temple, of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 70(6),
O. R. (2006). Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in 722–26.
patients with chronic disabling occupational spinal Filippi, M. & Rocca, M. A. (2008). Headache
disorders. Spine, 31(10), 1156–62. and migraine. Neurological Sciences, 29 Suppl 3,
Dersh, J., Gatchel, R. J., & Polatin, P. (2001). Chronic 336–38.
spinal disorders and psychopathology. Research Fink, P., Hansen, M. S., & Oxhoj, M. L. (2004). The
findings and theoretical considerations. Spine prevalence of somatoform disorders among inter-
Journal, 1(2), 88–94. nal medical inpatients. Journal of Psychosomatic
Dersh, J., Polatin, P. B., & Gatchel, R. J. (2002). Chronic Research, 56(4), 413–18.
pain and psychopathology: Research findings and Fink, P., Steen Hansen, M., & Sondergaard, L. (2005).
theoretical considerations. Psychosomatic Medicine, Somatoform disorders among first-time referrals to
64(5), 773–86. a neurology service. Psychosomatics, 46(6), 540–48.
Dionne, C. E., Von Korff, M., Koepsell, T. D., Deyo, R. A., Fishbain, D. A., Cole, B., Cutler, R. B., Lewis, J.,
Barlow, W. E., & Checkoway, H. (2001). Formal Rosomoff, H. L., & Rosomoff, R. S. (2006). Chronic
education and back pain: A review. Journal of Epide- pain and the measurement of personality: Do states
miology and Community Health, 55(7), 455–68. influence traits? Pain Medicine, 7(6), 509–29.
Eastwood, S. & Bisson, J. I. (2008). Management of fac- Fishbain, D. A., Cutler, R., Rosomoff, H. L., & Rosomoff,
titious disorders: A systematic review. Psychotherapy R. S. (1999). Chronic pain disability exaggeration/
and Psychosomatics, 77(4), 209–18. malingering and submaximal effort research.
Eccleston, C., Williams, A. C., & Morley, S. (2009). Clinical Journal of Pain, 15(4), 244–74.
Psychological therapies for the management of Fishbain, D. A., Goldberg, M., Meagher, B. R., Steele,
chronic pain (excluding headache) in adults. R., & Rosomoff, H. (1986). Male and female chronic
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, (2), pain patients categorized by DSM-III psychiatric
CD007407. diagnostic criteria. Pain, 26(2), 181–97.
Edwards, R. R., Smith, M. T., Stonerock, G., & Frederick, R. I. (2000). Mixed group validation: A
Haythornthwaite, J. A. (2006). Pain-related cata- method to address the limitations of criterion group
strophizing in healthy women is associated with validation in research on malingering detection.
greater temporal summation of and reduced habit- Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 18(6), 693–718.
uation to thermal pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, Friedman, A. F., Gleser, G. C., Smeltzer, D. J., Wakefield,
22(8), 730–37. J. A., Jr., & Schwartz, M. S. (1983). MMPI overlap
Eisendrath, S. J. (1996). When Munchausen becomes item scales for differentiating psychotics, neuro-
malingering: Factitious disorders that penetrate tics, and nonpsychiatric groups. Journal of Cons-
the legal system. Bulletin of the American Academy ulting and Clinical Psychology, 51(4), 629–31.
of Psychiatry and the Law, 24(4), 471–81. Frymoyer, J. W. (1988). Back pain and sciatica. New
Eisendrath, S. J. & McNiel, D. E. (2004). Factitious England Journal of Medicine, 318(5), 291–300.
physical disorders, litigation, and mortality. Gabriel, S. E., Crowson, C. S., & O’Fallon, W. M. (1999).
Psychosomatics, 45(4), 350–53. The epidemiology of rheumatoid arthritis in
Englund, M., Guermazi, A., Gale, D., Hunter, D. J., Rochester, Minnesota, 1955–1985. Arthritis and
Aliabadi, P., Clancy, M., et al. (2008). Incidental Rheumatism, 42(3), 415–20.
meniscal findings on knee MRI in middle-aged Gatchel, R. J. (2004). Psychosocial factors that can
and elderly persons. New England Journal of influence the self-assessment of function. Journal
Medicine, 359(11), 1108–1115. of Occupational Rehabilitation, 14(3), 197–206.
Englund, M., Niu, J., Guermazi, A., Roemer, F. W., Gatchel, R. J. & Dersh, J. (2002). Psychological disor-
Hunter, D. J., Lynch, J. A., et al. (2007). Effect of ders and chronic pain: Are there cause and effect
meniscal damage on the development of frequent relationships? In D. C. Turk & R. J. Gatchel (Eds.),
knee pain, aching, or stiffness. Arthritis and Psychological approaches to pain management:
Rheumatism, 56(12), 4048–54. A practitioners handbook (2nd ed., pp. 30–51).
Epker, J. & Block, A. R. (2001). Presurgical psychologi- New York: Guilford Press.
cal screening in back pain patients: A review. Gatchel, R. J., Garofalo, J. P., Ellis, E., & Holt, C. (1996).
Clinical Journal of Pain, 17(3), 200–205. Major psychological disorders in acute and chronic
Ericsson, M., Poston, W. S., Linder, J., Taylor, J. E., TMD: An initial examination. Journal of the American
Haddock, C. K., & Foreyt, J. P. (2002). Depression Dental Association, 127(9), 1365–70, 72, 74.
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 323

Gatchel, R. J. & Okifuji, A. (2006). Evidence-based Digit Span Test in chronic pain: Classification
scientific data documenting the treatment and accuracy in detecting malingered pain-related
cost-effectiveness of comprehensive pain programs disability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 1–16.
for chronic nonmalignant pain. Journal of Pain, Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Etherton, J. L., Ord, J. S.,
7(11), 779–93. & Curtis, K. L. (2009). Detecting Malingered
Gatchel, R. J., Peng, Y. B., Peters, M. L., Fuchs, P. N., Pain-Related Disability: Classification Accuracy of
& Turk, D. C. (2007). The biopsychosocial the Portland Digit Recognition Test. The Clinical
approach to chronic pain: Scientific advances and Neuropsychologist, 23, 850–69.
future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Love, J. M., Brennan, A.,
581–624. & Heinly, M. T. (2006). Sensitivity and specificity
Gatchel, R. J., Polatin, P. B., & Mayer, T. G. (1995). The of MMPI-2 validity scales and indicators to malin-
dominant role of psychosocial risk factors in the gered neurocognitive dysfunction in traumatic
development of chronic low back pain disability. brain injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20(3),
Spine, 20(24), 2702–709. 491–512.
Gatchel, R. J., Polatin, P. B., Mayer, T. G., & Garcy, P. D. Greve, K. W., Etherton, J. L., Ord, J., Bianchini, K. J., &
(1994). Psychopathology and the rehabilitation of Curtis, K. L. (2009). Detecting malingered pain-
patients with chronic low back pain disability. related disability: Classification accuracy of the test
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, of memory malingering. The Clinical Neuropsycho-
75(6), 666–70. logist, 23(7), 1250–71.
Gates, J. R., Ramani, V., Whalen, S., & Loewenson, R. Greve, K. W., Ord, J. S., Bianchini, K. J., & Curtis, K. L.
(1985). Ictal characteristics of pseudoseizures. (2009). Prevalence of malingering in patients with
Archives of Neurology, 42(12), 1183–87. chronic pain referred for psychologic evaluation in
Geisser, M. & Colwell, M. (1999). Chronic back pain: a medico-legal context. Archives of Physical
Conservative approaches. In A. R. Block, E. F. Medicine and Rehabilitation, 90(7), 1117–26.
Kremer, & E. Fernandez (Eds.), Handbook of pain Guo, H. R., Tanaka, S., Halperin, W. E., & Cameron,
syndromes (pp. 169–90). Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum. L. L. (1999). Back pain prevalence in U.S. industry
Gelfman, R., Melton, L. J., 3rd, Yawn, B. P., Wollan, and estimates of lost workdays. American Journal
P. C., Amadio, P. C., & Stevens, J. C. (2009). Long- of Public Health, 89(7), 1029–35.
term trends in carpal tunnel syndrome. Neurology, Gupta, A., Silman, A. J., Ray, D., Morriss, R., Dickens,
72(1), 33–41. C., MacFarlane, G. J., et al. (2007). The role of psy-
Goubert, L., Crombez, G., & De Bourdeaudhuij, I. chosocial factors in predicting the onset of chronic
(2004). Low back pain, disability, and back pain widespread pain: Results from a prospective popu-
myths in a community sample: Prevalence and lation-based study. Rheumatology, 46(4), 666–71.
interrelationships. European Journal of Pain, 8(4), Gureje, O., Von Korff, M., Simon, G. E., & Gater, R.
385–94. (1998). Persistent pain and well-being: A World
Goubert, L., Crombez, G., & Van Damme, S. (2004). Health Organization Study in Primary Care. JAMA,
The role of neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and 280(2), 147–51.
pain-related fear in vigilance to pain: A structural Guzman, J., Esmail, R., Karjalainen, K., Malmivaara,
equations approach. Pain, 107(3), 234–41. A., Irvin, E., & Bombardier, C. (2002).
Greve, K. W. & Bianchini, K. J. (2004). Setting empiri- Multidisciplinary bio-psycho-social rehabilitation
cal cut-offs on psychometric indicators of negative for chronic low back pain. Cochrane Database of
response bias: A methodological commentary Systematic Reviews(1), CD000963.
with recommendations. Archives of Clinical Neuro- Hagen, K. B., Holte, H. H., Tambs, K., & Bjerkedal, T.
psychology, 19(4), 533–41. (2000). Socioeconomic factors and disability retire-
Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., & Ameduri, C. J. (2003). ment from back pain: A 1983–1993 population-
Use of a forced-choice test of tactile discrimination based prospective study in Norway. Spine (Phila Pa
in the evaluation of functional sensory loss: 1976), 25(19), 2480–87.
A report of three cases. Archives of Physical Hagen, K. B., Tambs, K., & Bjerkedal, T. (2002). A pro-
Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84(8), 1233–36. spective cohort study of risk factors for disability
Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Black, F. W., Heinly, M. retirement because of back pain in the general
T., Love, J. M., Swift, D. A., et al. (2006). The preva- working population. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 27(16),
lence of cognitive malingering in persons report- 1790–96.
ing exposure to occupational and environmental Harden, R. N. & Bruehl, S. P. (2006). Diagnosis of com-
substances. Neurotoxicology, 27(6), 940–50. plex regional pain syndrome: Signs, symptoms,
Greve, K. W., Bianchini, K. J., Etherton, J. L., Meyers, J. and new empirically derived diagnostic criteria.
E., Curtis, K. L., & Ord, J. S. (2009). The Reliable Clinical Journal of Pain, 22(5), 415–19.
324 forensic neuropsychology

Harkonmaki, K., Korkeila, K., Vahtera, J., Kivimaki, Kay, N. R. & Morris-Jones, H. (1998). Pain clinic man-
M., Suominen, S., Sillanmaki, L., et al. (2007). agement of medicolegal litigants. Injury, 29(4),
Childhood adversities as a predictor of disability 305–308.
retirement. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Keefe, F. J., Rumble, M. E., Scipio, C. D., Giordano,
Health, 61(6), 479–84. L. A., & Perri, L. M. (2004). Psychological aspects
Harris, I., Mulford, J., Solomon, M., van Gelder, J. M., of persistent pain: Current state of the science.
& Young, J. (2005). Association between compen- Journal of Pain, 5(4), 195–211.
sation status and outcome after surgery: A meta- Keller, L. S. & Butcher, J. N. (1991). Assessment of
analysis. JAMA, 293(13), 1644–52. chronic pain patients with the MMPI-2. Minnea-
Hathaway, S. R. & McKinley, J. C. (1943). MMPI polis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
manual. New York: Psychological Corporation. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Page, G. G., Marucha, P. T.,
Heim, C., Ehlert, U., Hanker, J. P., & Hellhammer, D. H. MacCallum, R. C., & Glaser, R. (1998). Psychological
(1998). Abuse-related posttraumatic stress disor- influences on surgical recovery. Perspectives from
der and alterations of the hypothalamic-pituitary- psychoneuroimmunology. American Psychologist,
adrenal axis in women with chronic pelvic pain. 53(11), 1209–18.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 60(3), 309–18. Klein, D. F. & Gorman, J. M. (1987). A model of panic
Holzberg, A. D., Robinson, M. E., Geisser, M. E., & and agoraphobic development. Acta Psychiatrica
Gremillion, H. A. (1996). The effects of depression Scandinavica. Supplementum, 335, 87–95.
and chronic pain on psychosocial and physical Konstantinou, K. & Dunn, K. M. (2008). Sciatica:
functioning. Clinical Journal of Pain, 12(2), 118–25. Review of epidemiological studies and prevalence
Hoogendoorn, W. E., Bongers, P. M., de Vet, H. C., estimates. Spine, 33(22), 2464–72.
Ariens, G. A., van Mechelen, W., & Bouter, L. M. Krahn, L. E., Li, H., & O’Connor, M. K. (2003). Patients
(2002). High physical work load and low job satis- who strive to be ill: Factitious disorder with physi-
faction increase the risk of sickness absence due to cal symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry,
low back pain: Results of a prospective cohort 160(6), 1163–68.
study. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, Lamberty, G. J. (2008). Understanding somatization in
59(5), 323–28. the practice of clinical neuropsychology. New York:
Howard, K. J., Mayer, T. G., & Gatchel, R. J. (2009). Oxford University Press.
Effects of presenteeism in chronic occupational Lande, R. G. (1996). Factitious disorders and the
musculoskeletal disorders: Stay at work is vali- “professional patient.” Journal of the American
dated. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Osteopathic Association, 96, 468–72.
Medicine, 51(6), 724–31. Large, R. G. (1986). DSM-III diagnoses in chronic
Imbierowicz, K. & Egle, U. T. (2003). Childhood adver- pain. Confusion or clarity? Journal of Nervous and
sities in patients with fibromyalgia and somato- Mental Disease, 174(5), 295–303.
form pain disorder. European Journal of Pain, 7(2), Larrabee, G. J. (2003). Detection of malingering using
113–119. atypical performance patterns on standard neuro-
Jarvik, J. G. & Deyo, R. A. (2002). Diagnostic evalua- psychological tests. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
tion of low back pain with emphasis on imaging. 17(3), 410–25.
Annals of Internal Medicine, 137(7), 586–97. Larrabee, G. J. (Ed.). (2007). Assessment of malingered
Jarvik, J. G., Hollingworth, W., Heagerty, P. J., Haynor, neuropsychological deficits. New York: Oxford
D. R., Boyko, E. J., & Deyo, R. A. (2005). Three- University Press.
year incidence of low back pain in an initially Larrabee, G. J., Greiffenstein, M. F., Greve, K. W., &
asymptomatic cohort: Clinical and imaging risk Bianchini, K. J. (2007). Refining diagnostic criteria
factors. Spine, 30(13), 1541–48; discussion 49. for malingering. In G. J. Larrabee (Ed.), Evaluation
Jarvik, J. J., Hollingworth, W., Heagerty, P., Haynor, D. of malingering in the neuropsychological examina-
R., & Deyo, R. A. (2001). The Longitudinal tion (pp. 334–71). New York: Oxford University
Assessment of Imaging and Disability of the Back Press.
(LAIDBack) Study: Baseline data. Spine (Phila Pa Larrabee, G. J., Millis, S. R., & Meyers, J. E. (2008).
1976), 26(10), 1158–66. Sensitivity to brain dysfunction of the Halstead-
Jensen, M. C., Brant-Zawadzki, M. N., Obuchowski, Reitan vs an ability-focused neuropsychological
N., Modic, M. T., Malkasian, D., & Ross, J. S. (1994). battery. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22(5),
Magnetic resonance imaging of the lumbar spine 813–25.
in people without back pain. New England Journal Lebovits, A. H. (2000). The psychological assessment
of Medicine, 331(2), 69–73. of patients with chronic pain. Current Review of
Jensen, R. & Stovner, L. J. (2008). Epidemiology and Pain, 4(2), 122–26.
comorbidity of headache. Lancet Neurology, 7(4), Lee, M. J., Cassinelli, E. H., & Riew, K. D. (2007).
354–61. Prevalence of cervical spine stenosis. Anatomic
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 325

study in cadavers. Journal of Bone and Joint Surgery. McLean, S. A., Williams, D. A., & Clauw, D. J. (2005).
American Volume, 89(2), 376–80. Fibromyalgia after motor vehicle collision:
Leeuw, M., Goossens, M. E., Linton, S. J., Crombez, G., Evidence and implications. Traffic Injury Preven-
Boersma, K., & Vlaeyen, J. W. (2007). The tion, 6(2), 97–104.
fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: Merskey, H. (1965). The effect of chronic pain upon the
Current state of scientific evidence. Journal of response to noxious stimuli by psychiatric patients.
Behavioral Medicine, 30(1), 77–94. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 8, 405–419.
Linton, S. J. (2000). A review of psychological risk Merskey, H. M. & Bogduk, N. (1994). Classification of
factors in back and neck pain. Spine, 25(9), chronic pain (2nd ed.). Seattle: IASP Press.
1148–56. Meyers, J. E. & Diep, A. (2000). Assessment of mali-
Love, A. W. & Peck, C. L. (1987). The MMPI and ngering in chronic pain patients using neuro-
psychological factors in chronic low back pain: psychological tests. Applied Neuropsychology, 7(3),
A review. Pain, 28(1), 1–12. 133–39.
Lubin, B., Larsen, R. M., & Matarazzo, J. D. (1984). Midgette, L. A. & Scher, A. I. (2009). The epidemiology
Patterns of psychological test usage in the of chronic daily headache. Current Pain and
United States: 1935–1982. American Psychologist, Headache Reports, 13(1), 59–63.
39, 451–55. Millon, T., Davis, R., & Millon, C. (1997). Millon
Mailis-Gagnon, A., Nicholson, K., Blumberger, D., & Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-III (2nd ed.).
Zurowski, M. (2008). Characteristics and period Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems.
prevalence of self-induced disorder in patients Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit,
referred to a pain clinic with the diagnosis of com- D. C. (2002). Base rates of malingering and symp-
plex regional pain syndrome. Clinical Journal of tom exaggeration. Journal of Clinical and Experi-
Pain, 24(2), 176–85. mental Neuropsychology, 24(8), 1094–1102.
Marks, P. A. & Seeman, W. (1963). The actuarial Monti, D. A., Herring, C. L., Schwartzman, R. J., &
description of abnormal personality. Baltimore: Marchese, M. (1998). Personality assessment of
Williams & Wilkins. patients with complex regional pain syndrome
Martin, R., Burneo, J. G., Prasad, A., Powell, T., Faught, type I. Clinical Journal of Pain, 14(4), 295–302.
E., Knowlton, R., et al. (2003). Frequency of epi- Mooney, G., Speed, J., & Sheppard, S. (2005). Factors
lepsy in patients with psychogenic seizures moni- related to recovery after mild traumatic brain
tored by video-EEG. Neurology, 61(12), 1791–92. injury. Brain Injury, 19(12), 975–87.
Martin, A. L., McGrath, P. A., Brown, S. C., & Katz, J. Morken, T., Riise, T., Moen, B., Bergum, O., Hauge,
(2007). Anxiety sensitivity, fear of pain and pain- S. H., Holien, S., et al. (2002). Frequent musculo-
related disability in children and adolescents with skeletal symptoms and reduced health-related
chronic pain. Pain Research and Management, 12, quality of life among industrial workers. Occup
267–72. Med (Lond), 52(2), 91–98.
Masterton, G. (1995). Factitious disorders and the Moultry, A. M. & Poon, I. O. (2009). The Use of
surgeon. British Journal of Surgery, 82, 1588–89. Antidepressants for Chronic Pain. US Pharmacist,
Mayer, T. G., Gatchel, R. J., & Polatin, P. B. (Eds.). 34, 26–34.
(2000). Occupational musculoskeletal disorders: Nampiaparampil, D. E. (2008). Prevalence of chronic
Function, outcomes and evidence. Philadelphia, PA: pain after traumatic brain injury: A systematic
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. review. JAMA, 300(6), 711–19.
Mayer, T. G., Towns, B. L., Neblett, R., Theodore, B. R., Nelson, D. V., Kennington, M., Novy, D. M., & Squitieri,
& Gatchel, R. J. (2008). Chronic widespread pain in P. (1996). Psychological selection criteria for
patients with occupational spinal disorders: implantable spinal cord stimulators. Pain Forum, 5,
Prevalence, psychiatric comorbidity, and associa- 93–103.
tion with outcomes. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 33(17), Nnoaham, K. E. & Kumbang, J. (2008). Transcutaneous
1889–97. electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) for chronic
McCracken, L. M., Gross, R. T., Aikens, J., & Carnrike, pain. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews(3),
C. L., Jr. (1996). The assessment of anxiety and fear CD003222.
in persons with chronic pain: A comparison of Olney, J. R., Quenzer, D. E., & Makowsky, M. (1999).
instruments. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Contested claims in carpal tunnel surgery:
34(11–12), 927–33. Outcome study of worker’s compensation factors.
McIntosh, G., Frank, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., Bombardier, Iowa Orthopaedic Journal, 19, 111–21.
C., & Hall, H. (2000). Prognostic factors for time Overland, S., Glozier, N., Henderson, M., Maeland,
receiving workers’ compensation benefits in a J. G., Hotopf, M., & Mykletun, A. (2008). Health
cohort of patients with low back pain. Spine, 25(2), status before, during and after disability pension
147–57. award: The Hordaland Health Study (HUSK).
326 forensic neuropsychology

Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 65(11), review of the association between financial com-
769–73. pensation and the experience and treatment of
Paar, G. H. (1994). Factitious disorders in the field chronic pain. Health Psychology, 14(6), 537–47.
of surgery. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Rosenstiel, A. K. & Keefe, F. J. (1983). The use of coping
62(1–2), 41–47. strategies in chronic low back pain patients:
Pappagallo, M., & Werner, M. (2008). Chronic pain: Relationship to patient characteristics and current
A primer for physicians. London: Remedica. adjustment. Pain, 17(1), 33–44.
Pearce, J. M. (1999). The myth of chronic whiplash Roth, R. S. & Geisser, M. E. (2002). Educational
syndrome. Spinal Cord, 37(11), 741–48. achievement and chronic pain disability: Mediating
Pincus, T., Burton, A. K., Vogel, S., & Field, A. P. (2002). role of pain-related cognitions. Clinical Journal of
A systematic review of psychological factors as Pain, 18(5), 286–96.
predictors of chronicity/disability in prospective Rubin, D. I. (2007). Epidemiology and risk factors for
cohorts of low back pain. Spine, 27(5), E109–120. spine pain. Neurologic Clinics, 25(2), 353–71.
Polatin, P. B. & Dersh, J. (2004). Psychotropic medica- Sandroni, P., Benrud-Larson, L. M., McClelland, R. L.,
tion in chronic spinal disorders. Spine Journal, & Low, P. A. (2003). Complex regional pain syn-
4(4), 436–50. drome type I: Incidence and prevalence in Olmsted
Polatin, P. B., Kinney, R. K., Gatchel, R. J., Lillo, E., & county; a population-based study. Pain, 103(1–2),
Mayer, T. G. (1993). Psychiatric illness and chronic 199–207.
low-back pain. The mind and the spine—which Schade, V., Semmer, N., Main, C. J., Hora, J., & Boos, N.
goes first? Spine, 18(1), 66–71. (1999). The impact of clinical, morphological, psy-
Poveda Roda, R., Bagan, J. V., Diaz Fernandez, J. M., chosocial, and work-related factors on the outcome
Hernandez Bazan, S., & Jimenez Soriano, Y. (2007). of lumbar discectomy. Pain, 80(1–2), 239–49.
Review of temporomandibular joint pathology. Schnurr, R. F. & MacDonald, M. R. (1995). Memory
Part I: Classification, epidemiology and risk fac- complaints in chronic pain. Clinical Journal of
tors. Medicina Oral, Patología Oral y Cirugía Bucal, Pain, 11(2), 103–111.
12(4), E292–298. Schofferman, J., Anderson, D., Hines, R., Smith, G., &
Proctor, T., Gatchel, R. J., & Robinson, R. C. (2000). Keane, G. (1993). Childhood psychological trauma
Psychosocial factors and risk of pain and disability. and chronic refractory low-back pain. Clinical
Occupational Medicine, 15(4), 803–12, v. Journal of Pain, 9(4), 260–65.
Rachman, S. (1998). Anxiety. Hove, UK: Psychological Schofferman, J., Anderson, D., Hines, R., Smith, G., &
Press. White, A. (1992). Childhood psychological trauma
Rainville, J., Sobel, J. B., Hartigan, C., & Wright, A. correlates with unsuccessful lumbar spine surgery.
(1997). The effect of compensation involvement Spine, 17(6 Suppl), S138–144.
on the reporting of pain and disability by patients Schwarzer, A. C., Aprill, C. N., Derby, R., Fortin, J.,
referred for rehabilitation of chronic low back pain. Kine, G., & Bogduk, N. (1995). The prevalence and
Spine, 22(17), 2016–24. clinical features of internal disc disruption in
Reich, J., Tupin, J. P., & Abramowitz, S. I. (1983). patients with chronic low back pain. Spine (Phila
Psychiatric diagnosis of chronic pain patients. Pa 1976), 20(17), 1878–83.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 140(11), 1495–98. Scott, N. A., Guo, B., Barton, P. M., & Gerwin, R. D.
Richter, M., Ferrari, R., Otte, D., Kuensebeck, H. W., (2009). Trigger point injections for chronic non-
Blauth, M., & Krettek, C. (2004). Correlation of malignant musculoskeletal pain: A systematic
clinical findings, collision parameters, and psy- review. Pain Medicine, 1, 54–69.
chological factors in the outcome of whiplash- Sears, J. M., Wickizer, T. M., & Franklin, G. M. (2008).
associated disorders. Journal of Neurology, Overstating the prevalence of symptom exaggera-
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 75(5), 758–64. tion in workers’ compensation cases. Journal of
Robinson, M. E., Swimmer, G. I., & Rallof, D. (1989). Hand Surgery. American Volume, 33(6), 1014–1015;
The P-A-I-N MMPI classification system: A critical author reply 1015–1017.
review. Pain, 37(2), 211–14. Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression,
Roelofs, K. & Spinhoven, P. (2007). Trauma and medi- development, and death. San Francisco: W. H.
cally unexplained symptoms towards an integra- Freeman.
tion of cognitive and neurobiological accounts. Severeijns, R., Vlaeyen, J. W., van den Hout, M. A., &
Clinical Psychology Review, 27(7), 798–820. Picavet, H. S. (2005). Pain catastrophizing and con-
Rogers, R. (1997). Introduction. In R. Rogers (Ed.), sequences of musculoskeletal pain: A prospective
Clinical assessment of malingering and deception study in the Dutch community. Journal of Pain,
(2nd ed., pp. 1–19). New York: The Guilford Press. 6(2), 125–32.
Rohling, M. L., Binder, L. M., & Langhinrichsen- Severeijns, R., Vlaeyen, J. W., van den Hout, M. A., &
Rohling, J. (1995). Money matters: A meta-analytic Weber, W. E. (2001). Pain catastrophizing predicts
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 327

pain intensity, disability, and psychological distress Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in
independent of the level of physical impairment. Sports, 18(3), 318–24.
Clinical Journal of Pain, 17(2), 165–72. Tarullo, A. R. & Gunnar, M. R. (2006). Child maltreat-
Shaw, W. S., Pransky, G., Patterson, W., & Winters, T. ment and the developing HPA axis. Hormones and
(2005). Early disability risk factors for low back Behavior, 50(4), 632–39.
pain assessed at outpatient occupational health Tate, D. G. (1992). Workers’ disability and return to
clinics. Spine, 30(5), 572–80. work. American Journal of Physical Medicine and
Skootsky, S. A., Jaeger, B., & Oye, R. K. (1989). Rehabilitation, 71(2), 92–96.
Prevalence of myofascial pain in general internal Taylor, R. R. & Jason, L. A. (2002). Chronic fatigue,
medicine practice. Western Journal of Medicine, abuse-related traumatization, and psychiatric
151(2), 157–60. disorders in a community-based sample. Social
Slesinger, D., Archer, R. P., & Duane, W. (2002). Science and Medicine, 55(2), 247–56.
MMPI-2 characteristics in a chronic pain popula- Teicher, M. H., Andersen, S. L., Polcari, A., Anderson,
tion. Assessment, 9(4), 406–14. C. M., Navalta, C. P., & Kim, D. M. (2003). The
Slick, D. J., Sherman, E. M., & Iverson, G. L. (1999). neurobiological consequences of early stress and
Diagnostic criteria for malingered neurocognitive childhood maltreatment. Neuroscience and Biobeh-
dysfunction: Proposed standards for clinical prac- avioral Reviews, 27(1–2), 33–44.
tice and research. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, Tempelhof, S., Rupp, S., & Seil, R. (1999). Age-related
13(4), 545–61. prevalence of rotator cuff tears in asymptomatic
Smeets, R. J., van Geel, K. D., & Verbunt, J. A. (2009). Is shoulders. Journal of Shoulder and Elbow Surgery,
the fear avoidance model associated with the 8(4), 296–99.
reduced level of aerobic fitness in patients with Thompson, E. N. (2005). Letter to the editor of PAIN.
chronic low back pain? Archives of Physical Pain, 113, 244–47.
Medicine and Rehabilitation, 90(1), 109–117. Thomsen, J. F., Gerr, F., & Atroshi, I. (2008). Carpal
Smith, T. R. (2004). Epidemiology and impact of tunnel syndrome and the use of computer mouse
headache: An overview. Primary Care, 31(2), and keyboard: A systematic review. BMC Musculos-
237–41, v. keletal Disorders, 9, 134.
Spitzer, C., Barnow, S., Gau, K., Freyberger, H. J., & Trief, P. M., Grant, W., & Fredrickson, B. (2000).
Grabe, H. J. (2008). Childhood maltreatment in A prospective study of psychological predictors
patients with somatization disorder. Australian and of lumbar surgery outcome. Spine, 25, 2616–21.
New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 42(4), 335–41. Turner, J. A. & Aaron, L. A. (2001). Pain-related cata-
Spitzer, W. O., Skovron, M. L., Salmi, L. R., Cassidy, strophizing: What is it? Clinical Journal of Pain,
J. D., Duranceau, J., Suissa, S., et al. (1995). Scientific 17(1), 65–71.
monograph of the Quebec Task Force on Whiplash- Turner, J. A., Franklin, G., Fulton-Kehoe, D., Sheppard,
Associated Disorders: Redefining “whiplash” and L., Wickizer, T. M., Wu, R., et al. (2006). Worker
its management. Spine, 20(8 Suppl), 1S–73S. recovery expectations and fear-avoidance
Stover, B., Wickizer, T. M., Zimmerman, F., Fulton- predict work disability in a population-based
Kehoe, D., & Franklin, G. (2007). Prognostic workers’ compensation back pain sample. Spine,
factors of long-term disability in a workers’ com- 31(6), 682–89.
pensation system. Journal of Occupational and Turner, J. A., Franklin, G., Fulton-Kehoe, D., Sheppard,
Environmental Medicine, 49(1), 31–40. L., Wickizer, T. M., Wu, R., et al. (2007). Early pre-
Sullivan, M. J., Stanish, W., Sullivan, M. E., & Tripp, D. dictors of chronic work disability associated with
(2002). Differential predictors of pain and disabil- carpal tunnel syndrome: A longitudinal workers’
ity in patients with whiplash injuries. Pain Research compensation cohort study. American Journal of
and Management, 7(2), 68–74. Industrial Medicine, 50(7), 489–500.
Sullivan, M. J., Thorn, B., Haythornthwaite, J. A., Keefe, Turner, J. A., Jensen, M. P., Warms, C. A., & Cardenas,
F., Martin, M., Bradley, L. A., et al. (2001). D. D. (2002). Catastrophizing is associated
Theoretical perspectives on the relation between with pain intensity, psychological distress, and
catastrophizing and pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, pain-related disability among individuals with
17(1), 52–64. chronic pain after spinal cord injury. Pain, 98(1–2),
Sutherland, A. J. & Rodin, G. M. (1990). Factitious 127–34.
disorders in a general hospital setting: Clinical Vendrig, A. A., Derksen, J. L., & de Mey, H. R. (1999).
features and a review of the literature. Psycho- Utility of selected MMPI-2 scales in the outcome
somatics, 31(4), 392–99. prediction for patients with chronic back pain.
Swirtun, L. R. & Renstrom, P. (2008). Factors affecting Psychological Assessment, 11, 381–85.
outcome after anterior cruciate ligament injury: Verhaak, P. F., Kerssens, J. J., Dekker, J., Sorbi, M. J., &
A prospective study with a six-year follow-up. Bensing, J. M. (1998). Prevalence of chronic benign
328 forensic neuropsychology

pain disorder among adults: A review of the litera- Weisberg, J. N. (2000b). Studies investigating the prev-
ture. Pain, 77(3), 231–39. alence of personality disorders in patients with
Vlaeyen, J. W., Kole-Snijders, A. M., Boeren, R. G., & chronic pain. In R. J. Gatchel & J. N. Weisberg
van Eek, H. (1995). Fear of movement/(re)injury (Eds.), Personality characteristics of patients with
in chronic low back pain and its relation to behav- pain (pp. 221–39). Washington, DC: American
ioral performance. Pain, 62(3), 363–72. Psychological Association.
Vlaeyen, J. W. & Linton, S. J. (2000). Fear-avoidance Westman, A., Linton, S. J., Theorell, T., Ohrvik, J.,
and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal Wahlen, P., & Leppert, J. (2006). Quality of life and
pain: A state of the art. Pain, 85(3), 317–32. maintenance of improvements after early multi-
Vlaeyen, J. W., Timmermans, C., Rodriguez, L. M., modal rehabilitation: A 5-year follow-up. Disability
Crombez, G., van Horne, W., Ayers, G. M., et al. and Rehabilitation, 28(7), 437–46.
(2004). Catastrophic thinking about pain increases Wickizer, T. M., Franklin, G., Fulton-Kehoe, D., Turner,
discomfort during internal atrial cardioversion. J. A., Mootz, R., & Smith-Weller, T. (2004). Patient
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 56(1), 139–44. satisfaction, treatment experience, and disability
Volinn, E., Van Koevering, D., & Loeser, J. D. (1991). outcomes in a population-based cohort of injured
Back sprain in industry. The role of socioeconomic workers in Washington State: Implications for
factors in chronicity. Spine, 16(5), 542–48. quality improvement. Health Services Research,
Von Korff, M., Crane, P., Lane, M., Miglioretti, D. L., 39(4 Pt 1), 727–48.
Simon, G., Saunders, K., et al. (2005). Chronic Wickizer, T. M., Franklin, G., Turner, J., Fulton-Kehoe,
spinal pain and physical-mental comorbidity in D., Mootz, R., & Smith-Weller, T. (2004). Use of
the United States: Results from the national comor- attorneys and appeal filing in the Washington State
bidity survey replication. Pain, 113(3), 331–39. workers’ compensation program: Does patient sat-
Voorhies, R. M., Jiang, X., & Thomas, N. (2007). isfaction matter? Journal of Occupational and
Predicting outcome in the surgical treatment of Environmental Medicine, 46(4), 331–39.
lumbar radiculopathy using the Pain Drawing Widom, C. S., DuMont, K., & Czaja, S. J. (2007). A pro-
Score, McGill Short Form Pain Questionnaire, and spective investigation of major depressive disorder
risk factors including psychosocial issues and axial and comorbidity in abused and neglected children
joint pain. Spine Journal, 7(5), 516–24. grown up. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(1),
Waddell, G., McCulloch, J. A., Kummel, E., & Venner, 49–56.
R. M. (1980). Nonorganic physical signs in low- Williams, G. R., Jr., Rockwood, C. A., Jr., Bigliani, L. U.,
back pain. Spine, 5(2), 117–25. Iannotti, J. P., & Stanwood, W. (2004). Rotator cuff
Wade, J. B., Dougherty, L. M., Hart, R. P., Rafii, A., & tears: Why do we repair them? Journal of Bone and
Price, D. D. (1992). A canonical correlation analy- Joint Surgery. American Volume, 86-A(12), 2764–76.
sis of the influence of neuroticism and extraversion Wilson, K. G., Eriksson, M. Y., D’Eon, J. L., Mikail, S.
on chronic pain, suffering, and pain behavior. Pain, F., & Emery, P. C. (2002). Major depression and
51(1), 67–73. insomnia in chronic pain. Clinical Journal of Pain,
Waldinger, R. J., Schulz, M. S., Barsky, A. J., & Ahern, 18(2), 77–83.
D. K. (2006). Mapping the road from childhood Winfield, J. B. (2000). Psychological determinants of
trauma to adult somatization: The role of attach- fibromyalgia and related syndromes. Current
ment. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(1), 129–35. Review of Pain, 4(4), 276–86.
Walker, E. A., Gelfand, A. N., Gelfand, M. D., Green, Wise, M. G. & Ford, C. V. (1999). Factitious disorders.
C., & Katon, W. J. (1996). Chronic pelvic pain and Primary Care, 26(2), 315–26.
gynecological symptoms in women with irritable Woby, S. R., Watson, P. J., Roach, N. K., & Urmston, M.
bowel syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic (2004). Adjustment to chronic low back pain—the
Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 17(1), 39–46. relative influence of fear-avoidance beliefs, cata-
Walker, E. A., Keegan, D., Gardner, G., Sullivan, M., strophizing, and appraisals of control. Behaviour
Bernstein, D., & Katon, W. J. (1997). Psychosocial Research and Therapy, 42(7), 761–74.
factors in fibromyalgia compared with rheumatoid Wolfe, F. & Hawley, D. J. (1998). Psychosocial factors
arthritis: II. Sexual, physical, and emotional abuse and the fibromyalgia syndrome. Zeitschrift fur
and neglect. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59(6), 572–77. Rheumatologie, 57 Suppl 2, 88–91.
Walsh, C. A., Jamieson, E., Macmillan, H., & Boyle, M. Wolfe, F., Ross, K., Anderson, J., Russell, I. J., & Hebert,
(2007). Child abuse and chronic pain in a commu- L. (1995). The prevalence and characteristics of
nity survey of women. Journal of Interpersonal fibromyalgia in the general population. Arthritis
Violence, 22(12), 1536–54. and Rheumatism, 38(1), 19–28.
Weisberg, J. N. (2000a). Personality and personality Woodwell, D. A. & Cherry, D. K. (2004). National
disorders in chronic pain. Current Review of Pain, Ambulatory Medical Care Survey: 2002 summary.
4(1), 60–70. Advance Data, 26, 1–44.
Appendix

The following sections will provide a brief review Carpal Tunnel


of chronic pain conditions along with relevant Carpal tunnel syndrome is caused by compres-
medical diagnostic procedures and treatments. sion of the median nerve which runs from the
This review is not meant to be exhaustive but forearm to the hand. Symptoms may include tin-
rather is intended as a brief introduction to some gling, numbness, and pain in the hand and wrist
of the more common conditions and procedures which may also radiate up the arm. Many factors
that a psychologist is likely to encounter in the can contribute to the development of carpal tunnel
assessment of chronic pain patients. The focus syndrome including congenital predisposition,
will be on chronic nonmalignant conditions wrist trauma, or any number of conditions that
rather than acute pain or pain related to cancer or result in narrowing of the carpal tunnel passage-
terminal illness. way in the wrist. The estimated incidence of
carpal tunnel syndrome in the U.S. population is
APPENDIX A: REVIEW approximately 400 per 100,000 person-years
O F PA I N - R E L AT E D (Gelfman et al., 2009). While hand-arm vibra-
CONDITIONS tions and some types of repetitive motions are
thought to be causal agents, contrary to common
Arthritis perception no evidence has been found to
Arthritis refers to a large group of joint disorders support a connection between computer use and
with symptoms that often include swelling, the development of carpal tunnel syndrome
stiffness, and pain. The two most common types (Thomsen, Gerr, & Atroshi, 2008).
are osteoarthritis and rheumatoid arthritis.
Osteoarthritis (OA) is a degenerative condition Chronic Widespread Pain/
involving the breakdown of cartilage in the joints Fibromyalgia
as well as pathophysiologic changes in the under- Chronic widespread pain is typically defined as
lying bone and joint capsule. OA is primarily pain in three or more body segments that persists
found in load-bearing joints and the phalanges. for at least three months. Chronic widespread
Onset of symptoms is typically gradual and pain is the core feature of fibromyalgia, a condi-
worsens with activity. The incidence of OA tion that commonly also presents with fatigue,
increases with age, affecting over half of men and sleep disturbances, and stiffness. Clinical diagno-
women over age 65 and nearly all over age 80 sis of fibromyalgia relies on pain elicited from
(D’Ambrosia, 2005). pressure on discrete anatomical areas referred to
Rheumatoid arthritis (RA) is a systemic as tender points, although the diagnostic useful-
inflammatory disease that causes a number of ness of tender points has been criticized (Clauw,
problems including joint degeneration and pain, 2007). Some have questioned the clinical useful-
primarily in peripheral regions. Other common ness of fibromyalgia as a diagnosis (e.g., Bohr,
symptoms include fatigue, malaise, and stiffness. 1995), and the condition is now often viewed as
Symptom onset is typically gradual but in a minor- one end of a spectrum of chronic pain conditions
ity of cases onset may be abrupt. The prevalence rather than a distinct clinical entity. The self-
rate of RA in the US is approximately 1% (Gabriel, reported point prevalence of chronic widespread
Crowson, & O’Fallon, 1999). While the exact pain is approximately 11% (Croft, Rigby, Boswell,
etiology is unknown, a genetic link has been Schollum, & Silman, 1993) while an estimated 2%
identified and infectious agents have been specu- of the population meets criteria for fibromyalgia
lated to contribute. (Wolfe, Ross, Anderson, Russell, & Herbert,
330 forensic neuropsychology

1995). Both conditions are more prevalent in have been documented (Mailis-Gagnon,
females. Nicholson, Blumberger, & Zurowski, 2008). It is
While the exact pathophysiological mecha- now understood that behavioral factors can play a
nisms underlying chronic widespread pain and role in the development and maintenance of CRPS
fibromyalgia are not known, abnormalities in symptoms and psychological evaluation and
CNS pain processing are thought to play a role in intervention is recommended for chronic CRPS
sensitization to pain. Psychosocial factors includ- cases (Bruehl & Chung, 2006).
ing adverse childhood experiences, vulnerability
to stress, mood and anxiety disorders, somatiza- Disc Bulge and Herniation
tion, and catastrophizing are thought to underlie The spine is composed of vertebrae segments that
the development of these conditions and are pre- are connected to each other by a complex consist-
dictive of patient prognosis (Gupta et al., 2007; ing of two facet joints and an intervertebral disc.
Winfield, 2000). While trauma is sometimes The disc is composed of a nucleus surrounded by
reported as a trigger for fibromyalgia, current layers of fibrous cartilage called the annulus and
conceptions do not support a direct biomechani- interfaces with the verterbral bodies at the disc
cal link. Instead, trauma is thought to act as a gen- endplate. Over time, vertebral discs may naturally
eral stressor which can trigger fibromyalgia only if begin to bulge outward beyond the vertebral body
the necessary underlying factors are present margins as they lose elasticity and expand hori-
(McLean, Williams, & Clauw, 2005). zontally. If the disc continues to degenerate or is
exposed to traumatic mechanical stress, a hernia-
Complex Regional Pain Syndrome tion may occur as the annular fibers tear and allow
Complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) involves the disc nucleus to protrude against or through
a heterogeneous group of symptoms that typically the annulus wall. In these conditions, pain may
occur in a limb following trauma. Symptoms are result directly from the annulus tears, from irrita-
disproportionate to the initial injury and may tion caused by the release of chemicals from the
include radiating pain, swelling, altered skin color nucleus, or by compression of the nerve root.
or temperature, reduced range of motion, and However, disc bulges and herniations are rela-
sensory or motor disturbances. CRPS I (reflex tively common, especially in older populations,
sympathetic dystrophy) is diagnosed in the and are unlikely to be symptomatic unless neural
absence of major nerve damage, and CRPS II compromise is involved (Boos et al., 1995; Jensen
(causalgia) is diagnosed if nerve damage is pres- et al., 1994).
ent. The estimated incidence rate of CRPS is 5 to
26 per 100,000 person years, with females out- Facet Joint Syndrome
numbering males 4:1 (de Mos et al., 2007; The facet, or zygapophyseal, joints are also thought
Sandroni, Benrud-Larson, McClelland, & Low, to be a common contributor to back pain. This
2003). Diagnosis of CRPS is difficult, however, joint pain may be caused directly through arthritic
due to the lack of objective diagnostic signs. processes, or indirectly by impinging on nearby
Current clinical criteria proposed by the structures such as the nerve root. The processes
International Association for the Study of Pain responsible for the development of facet joint pain
allow for a diagnosis of CRPS based solely on often co-occur with disc degeneration and thus
symptom report. These criteria have been criti- distinguishing the specific etiology of pain symp-
cized for having low specificity (. 36) and over- toms is often difficult.
diagnosis may be a problem (Harden & Bruehl,
2006). Meniscal Tears
The pathophysiology underlying CRPS is not Meniscal tears may occur due to knee trauma or
well understood. A number of physiological alter- due to age-related degenerative processes. Each
ations have been observed in the central and knee contains two menisci which are tough carti-
peripheral nervous system, immune system, and lage structures located between the femur and
vascular system, but the cause of these changes, tibia. The menisci serve to absorb shock and dis-
particularly in the absence of neuropathy, in not tribute mechanical stress evenly across the knee
clear. Prolonged disuse of a limb can reproduce joint. Meniscal tears are common in asymptom-
many of the features of CRPS in healthy individu- atic adults and evidence suggests that meniscal
als (Butler, 2001) and cases of self inflicted CRPS tears do not directly cause pain symptoms, but
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 331

they may contribute to the development of criteria for the identification of trigger points or the
osteoarthritis (Englund et al., 2007). diagnosis of myofascial pain syndrome (Bennett,
2007).
Migraine
Migraines are characterized by recurrent severe Radiculopathy/Sciatica
head pain that may be accompanied by neuro- Radiculopathy refers to a disruption of (or near)
logical disturbances. Pain may be unilateral or the nerve root that can result in pain as well as
bilateral, is often described as throbbing, and may sensory or motor disturbances. An important fea-
be accompanied by sensitivity to stimuli and/or ture of radiculopathy is that symptoms are often
nausea. In a minority of patients, onset may be referred to the limb associated with the disrupted
preceded by prodromes (premonitory symptoms) nerve. Radiculopathy involving the sciatic nerve,
such as mood changes or aura (focal neurologic often referred to as sciatica, is relatively common
symptoms) such as visual disturbances. Migraines with a lifetime prevalence of around 25%
are classified according to whether aura is (Konstantinou & Dunn, 2008). Sciatic symptoms
present (classic migraine) or absent (common typically manifest unilaterally in the lower back
migraine). Approximately 10% of the general and legs.
population is thought to be affected (Jensen &
Stovner, 2008). While the mechanisms underlying Rotator Cuff Tears
migraine are not completely understood, evidence The rotator cuff is composed of a group of four
has suggested neurotransmitter and neuromor- muscles that provide mobility and stabilization to
phological abnormalities play a role (Filippi & the shoulder area. Tears in the rotator cuff tendons
Rocca, 2008). usually occur at the point at which they attach to
the humeral head (the ball of the shoulder joint).
Muscle Ligament Injuries Rotator cuff tears can be classified according to
Muscle sprains and strains are relatively common severity as partial thickness tears, full thickness
consequences of strenuous physical activity. tears, or full thickness tears with complete detach-
Sprains are injuries involving ligaments, which ment. Rotator cuff tears are relatively common in
are bands of cartilage that connect bones and hold asymptomatic patients (Tempelhof, Rupp, & Seil,
them in alignment. Sprains are usually caused by 1999) and it is not completely understood what
trauma that displaces a joint resulting in stretch- factors contribute to the development and mainte-
ing or tearing of the associated ligament(s). Strains nance of symptoms (Williams, Rockwood, Bigliani,
are injuries involving muscles or the tendons that Iannotti, & Stanwood, 2004).
attach muscles to bone. Strains are usually caused
by a quick movement that over-stretches or over- Spinal Stenosis
contracts a muscle resulting in damage or tearing Spinal stenosis refers to a narrowing of the spinal
to the muscle or tendon. Treatment and recovery canal, nerve root canal, or foraminal openings
depend on the severity of injury. Mild to moder- from which nerve roots exit the canal. Symptoms
ate injuries will typically heal with self-care and typically occur when these nerve fibers become
rest while severe cases may require immobiliza- impinged. An estimated 5% of the adult popula-
tion or surgery. tion shows signs of spinal stenosis, with the preva-
lence increasing with age (Lee, Cassinelli, & Riew,
Myofascial Pain Syndrome 2007). The condition can be congenital, but is
Myofascial pain syndrome refers to musculoskele- more commonly acquired through degenerative
tal pain arising from localized trigger points in a processes. Cervical stenosis is associated with
taut band of muscle. These focal trigger points are radiating arm pain, numbness, and paresthesia.
tender to palpation and may cause muscle weak- Lumbar stenosis is associated with lower back pain
ness or reduced range of motion. If the trigger and radiating bilateral or unilateral leg pain. More
point is active, as opposed to latent, application of severe cases may present with other symptoms
pressure is said to produce referred pain in a pre- including myelopathy (spinal cord dysfunction).
dictable pattern. Myofascial trigger points are
reportedly found in 30% of patients seeking treat- Spondylolysis & Spondylolisthesis
ment for pain (Skootsky, Jaeger, & Oye, 1989). Spondylolysis refers to a stress fracture of the pars
However, there are currently no well-validated interarticularis, the narrow bridge between the
332 forensic neuropsychology

upper and lower facet joint of a vertebrae. A con- more than 15 episodes per month. The incidence
dition known as spondylolisthesis can occur if the of chronic headaches is reported to be 3 per 100
fracture is bilateral and the vertebrae slip out of person-years (Midgette & Scher, 2009). Muscular
alignment. Most consider spondylolysis to repre- contractions as well as psychogenic factors (e.g.,
sent a fatigue fracture resulting from chronic stress or anxiety) are thought to have an etiological
mechanical stress rather than a single traumatic role. Head and neck injuries have been shown to
event, though trauma may worsen a condition. increase the odds of developing chronic daily head-
Spondylolysis is relatively common, particularly aches (Couch, Lipton, Stewart, & Scher, 2007).
in athletes, and most cases are asymptomatic.
When pain is present it is thought to be caused by Whiplash-associated Disorder (WAD)
nerve root compression, intervetebral disc pain, Whiplash-associated disorders (WAD) refer to a
or facet joint pain. collection of symptoms resulting from rapid
hyperextension/flexion of the neck, often associ-
Spondylosis ated with motor vehicle accidents. The Quebec
Spondylosis is a condition caused by age-related Task Force classifies WAD into grades (I-III)
disc degeneration that causes a number of patho- based on symptom report and clinical findings
logical processes that can ultimately result in a (Spitzer et al., 1995). Grade I includes complaints
narrowing of the spinal canal. One mechanism is of neck pain or stiffness with no physical
the formation of osteophytic bars along the signs; Grade II includes neck complaints as well as
ventral spinal canal caused by increased mechani- musculoskeletal signs such as decreased range
cal stress. Pain may result from compression or of motion or point tenderness; and Grade III
irritation of the cauda equine or nerve root. includes neck complaints along with neurological
signs such as motor or sensory disturbances. An
Temporomandibular Disorder injury involving a fracture or dislocation may be
Temporomandibular disorder (TMD), also referred to as a Grade 4; however, the terms “whip-
referred to as temporomandibular joint (TMJ) lash” and “WAD” are generally reserved for cases
syndrome, involves pain originating from the in which observable pathology is not present.
TMJ, a complex joint that connects the mandible Clinical manifestations of WAD may include a
to the skull at points just in front of the ear. Most number of diverse symptoms including deafness,
people will show signs and symptoms of TMD at dizziness, tinnitus, and headache. Acute neck
some point in their life and approximately 5% will pain, the primary symptom of WAD, is thought to
seek treatment (Poveda Roda et al., 2007). TMD result from soft tissue injury and there is little evi-
can be broadly categorized into two potentially dence that whiplash injuries differ from any other
overlapping syndromes according to the etiology musculoskeletal sprain or strain (Pearce, 1999).
of symptoms: myogenous (muscle-related) TMD WAD typically resolves quickly and chronic and/
and arthrogenous (joint-related) TMD. The cause or late onset symptoms in the absence of pathol-
of myogenous TMD is not completely under- ogy are not expected. Psychosocial risk factors
stood; however, it is commonly comorbid with such as compensation status, injury perceptions,
bruxism and jaw clenching and has been linked to and coping styles are a stronger predictor of out-
stress and anxiety (Poveda Roda et al., 2007). come than collision severity (Buitenhuis, de Jong,
Arthrogenous TMD can result from a number of Jaspers, & Groothoff, 2008; Carroll et al., 2008;
joint conditions including disc displacement, Carroll, Holm, Ferrari, Ozegovic, & Cassidy, 2009;
degenerative joint disorders, recurrent disloca- Ferrari & Schrader, 2001; Richter et al., 2004).
tions, and arthritis.
APPENDIX B: DIAGNO STIC
Tension-Type Headaches PROCEDURES
Tension-type headaches involve mild to moderate
bilateral frontal-occipital head pain that is often Diagnostic Injections
described as pressure or tightening. Episodic Injection of local anesthetics, steroids, neural
tension headaches are very common, with 90% of blockades, or even irritants can be useful for
the general population experiencing them at some determining the source of pain symptoms. A typi-
point in life (Smith, 2004). The condition is typi- cal procedure involves the injection of the agent
cally considered chronic if a patient experiences into a target location, after which changes in the
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 333

patient’s pain symptoms are noted. These tech- Imaging


niques can help identify the source of pain symp- Radiography and magnetic resonance imaging
toms and allow for differentiation between local (MRI) are the most common forms of imaging.
versus referred pain, somatic versus visceral pain, Radiography involves the use of X-ray to view
and peripheral versus central etiologies. While internal tissue and is particularly useful for exam-
diagnostic injections can offer the advantage of ining bony structures in the body. The three main
pinpointing a specific cause of symptoms, it categories of radiography are: 1) static images, 2)
should be noted that these procedures rely on the fluoroscopy, and 3) computed tomography (CT).
patient’s accurate report of symptoms and some Static images are classic X-ray snapshots on film.
have been criticized for having poor specificity in Fluoroscopy is the use of X-rays to provide real-
patient populations with external incentives. time dynamic internal imaging—a technique
often used to guide the placement of instruments
Discography, Myelography, & during surgical procedures. CT scanning uses
Arthrography X-rays to collect numerous image slices which are
Injections can also be used to introduce contrast then assembled into a detailed three-dimensional
materials to enhance standard imaging tech- structural view. MRI uses strong magnetic fields
niques. Arthograms involve the injection contrast to provide what is essentially an image of water
agents into a joint to better image interior soft tis- distribution in the body. MRI is particularly suited
sues. In a myelogram, contrast agent is introduced to examining soft tissue structures and the
into the dura surrounding the spinal cord and high-definition images allow for very accurate
nerves, which allows for a detailed view of nerve identification of spinal disc herniation and nerve
arrangement and impingements. Discograms root compression.
involve the injection of contrast material into the While imaging is undoubtedly a useful medi-
nucleus of an intervertebral disc to highlight any cal tool, there are some reasons to interpret imag-
defects in the disc’s structural integrity. Often dis- ing findings with caution. Imaging “abnormalities”
cograms also serve as a diagnostic injection due to are common in asymptomatic individuals (Jensen
the mildly irritating nature of the contrast mate- et al., 1994) and may not necessarily predict future
rial. If the injection elicits symptoms that are sim- symptoms. For example, Jarvik et al. (2005) found
ilar to those normally experienced, it is considered that MRI findings are not significant prognostic
an indication that the targeted disc is responsible. indicators for the development of low back pain.
However, as with all diagnostic injections, reli- A review of the diagnostic accuracy of imaging
ance on patient report can call the accuracy of the for patients with low back pain determined that
procedure into question when used in patient these procedures are not called for unless a patient
populations with external incentive. is over the age of 50 or signs of systemic disease
are present (Jarvik & Deyo, 2002). In fact, early
Electrodiagnosis imaging for acute back pain was found to have no
Electrodiagnostics involves the study of human benefit to outcome and knowledge of the imaging
physiology using devices that produce and mea- results was actually detrimental to patients’ sense
sure electrical current in the body. An electro- of well-being (Ash et al., 2008).
myogram (EMG) uses a needle to directly measure
the electrical activity of a muscle during different Physical Examination/
stages of activity. Abnormal electrical activity Functional Capacity Exams (FCEs)
can indicate nerve and muscle pathologies. Physical examination can aid in diagnosis as well
A nerve conduction study (NCS) delivers an as provide information about the type and degree
electrical charge to a peripheral nerve while a of functional limitations. Musculoskeletal aspects
recording electrode is placed in the innervated examined may include gait, posture, sensitivity to
muscle. This arrangement allows for the determi- palpation, range of motion, and strength.
nation of the nerve conduction velocity which is a Neurological aspects examined may include focal
sensitive indicator of nerve damage. An NCS also CNS signs, motor disturbances, reflexes, and
has the advantage of being able to isolate the spe- muscle tone. In patients without clear pathology,
cific site of nerve damage or impingement by physical examination can also provide evidence of
stimulating the nerve at various locations along nonorganic illness behaviors. Waddell signs are
its path. often used as an indication of psychosocial
334 forensic neuropsychology

involvement in the clinical presentation of patients anticonvulsants, anxiolytics, muscle relaxants, or


with back pain (Waddell, McCulloch, Kummel, & corticosteroids have also proved effective for some
Venner, 1980). Most formal functional capacity types of chronic pain. An implantable intrathecal
evaluations (FCEs) include a variety of indicators pump may be used to deliver analgesic medica-
that allow the determination of whether the results tion directly into the spinal fluid in cases of
are valid. However, their reliability in detecting chronic intractable pain. The targeted delivery
malingering has not yet been established. system allows for smaller doses to be delivered at
regular intervals and can avoid some complica-
APPENDIX C: MEDICAL tions associated with systemic routes of adminis-
T R E AT M E N T S tration.

Electrical Stimulation Nerve Decompression


Electrical stimulation is thought to provide anal- There is a variety of procedures used to relieve
gesia through presynaptic dorsal horn inhibition, nerve impingement depending on the pathology
activation of endogenous pain-control pathways, involved. Discectomy is a relatively common sur-
modulation of nerve activity, or some combina- gical intervention used to treat disc herniations.
tion of these processes. Transcutaneous electrical The procedure involves the removal or ablation of
nerve stimulation (TENS) is used to treat a variety a portion of disc nucleus in an effort to relieve
of pain conditions by the application of a mild pressure on the nerve root. Laminectomy involves
electric current to the surface of the skin. A TENS the removal of a small amount of bone from the
unit is attached to the targeted location via lamina, which is commonly used to help decom-
electrodes and is generally worn throughout the press the nerve root in treating spinal stenosis.
day. Patients typically have control over the acti-
vation of the unit and the amplitude, duration, Neurolytic Blockade/Neurotomy
and frequency of the electric pulse. There is wide Neurolytic blockades work by interrupting the
variation among patients regarding optimal TENS transmission of nociceptive signals to the brain
settings as well as the onset, degree, and duration by damaging specific nerve fibers. Methods
of analgesia provided. Evidence for the overall include chemical neurolysis, cryocoagulation,
effectiveness of TENS treatments has been mixed radiofrequency coagulation, and neuroablation.
(Nnoaham & Kumbang, 2008). The most significant challenge to these procedures
Surgically implantable stimulation devices is to disable nociceptive transmission while spar-
may also be used to directly deliver electrical ing motor and sensory nerves. Pain relief after
impulses to the dorsal column, peripheral successful treatment is often long lasting but not
nerves, or the spinal nerve root. Since not all necessarily permanent, as targeted nerves may
patients respond to spinal cord stimulation, a trial regenerate.
phase is usually implemented before internaliza-
tion of the device. In patients who achieve analge- Spinal Fusion
sia during the trial period and have been properly Spinal fusion is used to treat spinal instability by
screened for psychological risk factors, implant- eliminating motion in a segment of two or more
able stimulation devices have generally proved a adjacent vertebral bodies. Methods of fusion vary
safe and effective form of pain control (Cameron, depending on the number and location of joints
2004). to be fused. In a lumbar fusion, the most common
method of fixation is to attach rods or plates to the
Medication vertebrae by inserting screws into the pedicle. In
Analgesic medication for the treatment of chronic an inter-body fusion, the disc is removed and
pain is typically divided into three progressive replaced by an implant. Fusion of two or three
steps depending on the severity of symptoms: vertebrae can often be achieved with little or no
1) nonsteriodal anti-inflammatories (NSAIDs); impact on overall flexibility or mobility.
2) weak opiates such as codeine or hydrocodone;
and 3) strong opiates such as oxycodone or mor- Therapeutic Injections
phine. Opiates are often combined with NSAIDs Corticosteroid injections are the most frequently
to increase effectiveness. In addition to analgesics, used type of therapeutic injection and provide
adjuvants such as tricyclic antidepressants, SSRIs, pain relief primarily by reducing inflammation.
The Psychological Assessment of Persons with Chronic Pain 335

Epidural steroid injections can relieve local pain exist ranging from the injection of local anesthet-
and reduce nerve impingement. In some cases, ics or corticosteroids directly into trigger points
these treatments can halt the cycle of irritation to superficial “dry” injections similar to acupunc-
and inflammation and provide relatively long- ture. The variety of methods and lack of appropri-
lasting relief. ately controlled studies makes it difficult to draw
Trigger point injections involve injections into any conclusions about the effectiveness of trigger
specific trigger points for the relief of musculosk- point injections (Scott, Guo, Barton, & Gerwin,
eletal pain. Many different types and methods 2008).
13
Forensic Assessment of Medically
Unexplained Symptoms
L AU R E N C E M . B I N D E R

Disorders of medically unexplained symptoms Forensic neuropsychologists encounter these


without clearly demonstrated pathophysiological disorders in two ways. First, persons seeking
origin are characterized more by disability, disability from private insurers or from govern-
symptoms, and suffering than by objective medi- ment entitlement programs often claim one or
cal findings and pathology (Deary, Chalder, & more of these conditions as an explanation of
Sharpe, 2007; Labarge & McCaffrey, 2000; Rief & their inability to work. Second, some of these dis-
Broadbent, 2007). Illnesses including fibromyal- orders are common and may be seen concurrently
gia (Bohr, 1996; Grace, Nielson, Hopkins, & Berg, with other potentially compensable problems
1999), chronic fatigue syndrome (Abbey & such as alleged toxic encephalopathy (Bolla, this
Garfinkel, 1991; Buchwald & Garrity, 1994; volume) or traumatic brain injury (Larrabee, this
DeLuca, Johnson, Ellis & Natelson, 1997; DiPino volume; Roebuck-Spencer & Sherer, this volume).
& Kane, 1996; Fukuda et al., 1994), multiple In addition, experience suggests that medically
chemical sensitivities (Black, 2000; Bolla, 2000; unexplained symptoms are common in many
McCaffrey & Yantz, 2007; Sparks, 2000), symp- clinical neuropsychological settings.
toms following war and terrorism incidents
(Clauw, Engel, Aronowitz et al., 2003), and toxic A LT E R N AT I V E
mold syndrome and sick building syndrome E X P L A N AT I O N F O R
(Burge, 2001; Hardin, Kelman, & Saxon, 2003; S Y M P TO M S
Hodgson, 2000; Khali & Bardana, 2005; Kuhn & Cognitive symptoms are not signs of cognitive
Ghannoum, 2003; Lees-Haley, 2003; 2004; impairment. Negative emotions were more
McCaffrey & Yantz, 2007; Reijula & Tuomi, 2003; strongly predictive of subjective cognitive symp-
Robbins et al., 2000) have been referred to as toms than were objective cognitive problems in
functional somatic syndromes (Barsky & Borus, some study populations (Dux, Woodard, Calamari
1999) and as collections of medically unexplained et al., 2008; Hoppe, Elger, & Helmstaedter, 2007;
symptoms (Binder & Campbell, 2004; Deary et al., Larrabee & Levin, 1986; Rohling, Green, Allen, &
2007; Rief & Broadbent, 2007). This review dis- Iverson, 2002; Seidenberg, Taylor, & Haltiner,
cusses neuropsychological assessment of medi- 1994; Williams, Little, Scates, & Blockman, 1987).
cally unexplained symptoms. I am not attempting For example, subjective cognitive impairment was
to provide comprehensive reviews of the above more strongly related to scores on the Beck
disorders, but I have focused on certain largely Depression and Anxiety Inventories than to
medically unexplained disorders that either are objective cognitive performance in veterans with
commonly encountered in the practice of forensic Persian Gulf War-related illnesses (Binder,
clinical neuropsychology in adults, or that are Storzbach, Anger et al., 1999). Fibromyalgia and
particular exemplars of functional somatic chronic fatigue syndrome patients also have
syndromes in the neuropsychological setting. cognitive complaints more severe than their
Portions of this chapter were based on previous objective deficits (Grace, Nielson, Hopkins, &
work (Binder & Campbell, 2004). This chapter is Berg, 1999; Tiersky, Johnson, Lange et al., 1997).
organized in terms of the common assessment Cognitive complaints in some persons are
issues associated with these disorders. an index of and surrogate for emotional
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 337

distress—emotional distress communicated in about 50% of the cases and met liberal criteria
different language than the complaints typically for post-concussive syndrome in about 90% of the
associated with depression and anxiety. Neuro- cases (Iverson, 2006).
cognitive symptoms warrant investigation that Litigation, like depression, is associated with
may show objective abnormalities and disease neurological complaints despite the absence of
(Babiloni, Visser, Frisoni et al., 2008; Kearney- any neurological history. Patients in litigation
Schwartz, Rossignol, Bracard et al., 2009; Mitchell, claiming psychological and physical damage who
2008), but symptoms are not signs of disease. did not have neuropsychological histories or
Rather, symptoms are an expression of “dis-ease.” claims reported the following symptoms on a
Symptoms, whether they are cognitive or somatic, questionnaire: concentration problems, 78%; con-
are expressions of unease about one’s health. The fusion, 59%; memory loss, 53%; dizziness, 44%;
term “cogniform” has been proposed to describe and word finding problems, 34% (Lees-Haley &
patients with a pervasive belief that they have Brown, 1993). Persons with pain complaints that
cognitive impairment despite no neuropsycho- they attributed to work-related injuries had more
logical or neurological evidence of cognitive cognitive complaints than pain patients with
impairment (Delis & Wetter, 2007). non-work-related problems and as many cogni-
Reports of headaches, memory loss, irritabi- tive complaints as head-injured persons not
lity, dizziness, and other physical, cognitive, and involved in litigation (Iverson, King, Scott, &
emotional complaints are nonspecific. These Adams, 2001).
symptoms are not diagnostic of mild traumatic The hypothesis that post-concussive syndrome
brain injury, toxic encephalopathy, or other forms complaints were unrelated to suspected mild TBI
of brain dysfunction (Fox, Lees-Haley, Earnest, & was indirectly tested by comparing mild TBI and
Dolezal-Wood, 1995). Symptoms that often are more severely head-injured patients (Novack,
associated with brain dysfunction are not diag- Daniel, & Long, 1984). Mild TBI patients were
nostic of brain dysfunction, an issue often of inter- nearly twice as likely as the more severely injured
est to researchers on mild traumatic brain injury. to have elevations on the MMPI HS and HY
Normal control subjects frequently complained scales, which assess physical complaints. The
of symptoms of traumatic brain injury (TBI) such number of post-concussive symptoms was more
as memory loss, irritability, headaches, and strongly associated with elevations of these two
dizziness (Gouvier, Uddo-Crane, & Brown, 1988). MMPI scales than with neuropsychological data.
Post-concussive syndrome (PCS) symptoms were Novack et al. suggested that the number of post-
at least partly attributable to stress and did not concussive symptoms was an index of emotional
distinguish between TBI and control subjects adjustment. This inverse relationship between
(Gouvier, Cubic, Jones, Brantly, & Cutlip, 1992). acute severity of head injury and MMPI/MMPI-2
Among college student controls, many symptoms abnormality has been replicated—the less severely
were as common among controls as among TBI head-injured patients had greater MMPI/MMPI-2
patients (Gouvier et al., 1988). In the Gouvier elevations, especially on scales HS and HY
et al. study, normal controls had these symptom (Leininger, Kreutzer, & Hill, 1991; Scott, Emick, &
frequencies: memory loss, 20%; loss of interest, Adams, 1999; Youngjohn, Davis, & Wolf, 1997).
36%; temper problems, 37%; fatigue, 28%. The Pseudoneurological symptoms are conceiv-
relatives of the same college student participants ably neurological complaints, such as limb weak-
also observed high rates of “PCS symptoms” in ness, dizziness, memory loss, or weakness of a
the control group, an observation that suggests limb, that are not correlated with any objective
that reports of significant others may provide mis- neurological findings. Pseudoneurological com-
leading information after suspected mild TBI plaints are increased by stressors and can continue
(Gouvier et al., 1988). chronically (Cardena & Spiegel, 1993; Escobar,
The symptoms of depression overlap with Canino, Rubio-Stipec, & Bravo, 1992). One week
symptoms thought to be associated with brain after the San Francisco Bay area earthquake
dysfunction. In college students without history of 1989, 30% of exposed persons complained of
of head injury, symptoms considered typical of dizziness and 71% of concentration difficulty
post-concussive syndrome had a correlation with (Cardena & Spiegel, 1993). One year after a flood
depressive symptoms of 0.76 (Iverson & Lange, there were more pseudoneurological symptoms
2003). Clinically depressed patients met conser- in persons exposed to the flood than in nonex-
vative criteria for post-concussive syndrome in posed persons (Escobar et al., 1992). Neurological
338 forensic neuropsychology

symptoms were more common in firefighters with spray project than those participants with a more
post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) than in benign view of these technologies.
those without this disorder (McFarlane, Atchison, Negative expectations can have an effect on
Rafalowicz, & Papay, 1994). Dizziness was seven cognitive test performance. Negative expectations
times more common and headaches three times and stereotyping regarding associations between
more common in returned Iraqi veterans with a one’s demographic group and performance
diagnosis of PTSD than in veterans without PTSD appears to explain the inferior performance of
(Hoge, Terkahopian, Castro et al., 2007). Non- females compared with males on tests of mathe-
neurological somatic symptoms such as pseudo- matical abilities (Spencer, Steele, & Quinn, 1999;
cardiac problems also were far more common in Steele, 1997). Suhr and Gunstad (2002; 2005)
veterans with PTSD (Hoge et al. 2007). demonstrated that college students with a history
Expectations and attributions have an impor- of mild TBI performed worse on neuropsy-
tant role in the generation of medically unex- chological measures when their injury and its
plained symptoms (Deary et al., 2007; Rief & possible effect on test performance was empha-
Broadbent, 2007). There is evidence that symp- sized prior to testing than students with similar
toms simply are misattributed to certain events injury history who received instructions that did
when the symptoms actually are explained by not include information linking mild TBI to
other events or are preexisting. Mittenberg and cognitive problems. Suhr and Gunstad termed
colleagues introduced the notion of “expectation this phenomenon “diagnosis threat.” Just as a
as etiology” in connection with symptoms after demographic group stereotype was associated
mild TBI, showing that nonpatient controls were with lower performance (Spencer et al., 1999;
able to predict symptoms reported by patients Steele, 1997), beliefs about the effects of medical
(Mittenberg, DiGuilio, Perrin, et al., 1992) and history and diagnosis had a neuropsychological
later showing that simple reassurance and educa- effect in these studies of students with prior mild
tion significantly reduced symptoms (Mittenberg, TBIs. Negative expectations also influenced neu-
Tremont, Zielinski, et al., 1996). In another study, ropsychological performance in persons with
prolonged symptoms after mild TBI were pre- multiple chemical sensitivities (Smith & Sullivan,
dicted by patients’ expectations about prognosis 2003). This research on the effect of negative
(Whittaker, Kemp, & House, 2007). expectations on performance is consistent with
Expectations of illness also affect symptoms the work of Bandura and others on self-efficacy.
after exposure to manmade devices. In a study of Bandura and Locke (2003) summarized several
Vietnam War veterans, self-reported herbicide meta-analytic studies showing that beliefs in one’s
exposure to Agent Orange was related to greater competence had salutary effects on academic, cog-
psychological and medical problems, but objec- nitive, athletic, and other types of tasks and wrote:
tive history of exposure was unrelated (Korgeski
& Leon, 1983). This study of Vietnam veterans The evidence from these meta-analyses is
may have been weakened by imprecise objective consistent in showing that efficacy beliefs con-
measurement of Agent Orange exposure, how- tribute significantly to the level of motivation
ever. In an experimental study, the belief of and performance. Efficacy beliefs predict not
Norwegian patients who suffered from chronic only the behavioral functioning between indi-
headaches that their laboratory-induced head- viduals at different levels of perceived self-
aches were caused by radio frequency waves ema- efficacy but also changes in functioning in
nating from mobile phones was associated with individuals at different levels of efficacy over
both actual and sham mobile phone usage time and even variation within the same indi-
(Oftedal, Straume, Johnsson, & Stovner, 2007). A vidual in the tasks performed and those
New Zealand study assessed the effect of beliefs shunned or attempted but failed (p. 87).
about the effects on human health of technologies
such as microwave radiation, hormone additives A LT E R N AT I V E
in food, and fluoridated water (Petrie, Broadbent, E X P L A N AT I O N S F O R
Kley, et al., 2005). Belief that such technologies COGNITIVE DEFICITS
were harmful was associated with more symp- Like cognitive complaints, cognitive deficits also
toms. Furthermore, participants who believed are not specific to neurological disease or injury.
that such technologies were harmful reported Objective neuropsychological deficits are common
more adverse health effects from a local pesticide in a host of psychiatric disorders. Cognitive
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 339

deficits occur in affective disorders (reviewed in Patients with medically unexplained symp-
Burt, Zembar, & Niederehe, 1995; Cassens, Wolfe, toms obtain knowledge and beliefs about their
& Zola, 1990; Johnson & Magaro, 1987; conditions from a variety of sources. Often, these
Langenecker, Lee, & Bieliauskas, 2009; Rogers, patients express skepticism with mainstream
Kasai, Koji, et al., 2004). Patients with depression, medicine (Staudenmayer, 2001). This rejection of
particularly inpatients, may have difficulties with mainstream medical thinking is unsurprising,
attention, abstract thinking, processing speed, because many recognized experts believe that
memory, manual dexterity, visuospatial skills these conditions are either of disputed origin or
(Burt et al., 1995; Cassens et al., 1990), executive surrogates for psychological disorders (Abbey &
dysfunction (Langenecker et al., 2009; Rogers Garfinkel, 1991; Albers & Berent, 2000; Barsky &
et al., 2004), and intellectual decline (Sackeim Borus, 1999; Binder & Campbell, 2004; Black,
et al., 1992). Patients with depression or history of 2000; Bohr, 1996; Ferguson, 1997; Gabriel et al.,
depression are neuropsychologically heteroge- 1994; Hadler, 1997; Hennekens et al., 1996;
neous (Langenecker et al., 2009). Patients with Hyams, Wignall, & Roswell, 1996; Katon &
pseudoneurological symptoms perform poorly on Walker, 1993; Kroenke & Price, 1993; Rief &
neuropsychological tests. These findings will be Broadbent, 2007; Sanchez-Guerrero, Schur,
summarized in detail later in this chapter. Sergent, & Liang, 1994; Staudenmayer, 2001;
Walker, Keegan, Gardner, Sullivan, Katon, &
GENERAL Bernstein, 1997; Youngjohn, Spector, & Mapou,
CHARACTERISTICS 1997). Patients with these disorders often reject
OF DISORDERS WITH psychological explanations for their symptoms,
M E D I C A L LY U N E X P L A I N E D preferring biomedical and somatic explanations
S Y M P TO M S (Butler, Chalder & Wessely, 2001; Ford, 1997;
Barsky and Borus (1999) describe characteristics Nimnuan, Hotopf, & Wessely, 2001). The doctor-
of these conditions. The amount of disability patient relationship becomes more problematic
contrasts markedly with the degree of measured when there is disagreement about causation of
physical limitations and examination and labora- symptoms, as often occurs in medicolegal cases
tory abnormalities. Some patients with these (Engel, Adkins, & Cowan, 2002). Therefore,
conditions tend to view themselves as severely patients seek out health care professionals who
disabled (Hadler, 1999; Moss-Morris, Petrie, & share their belief system and who are likely to rec-
Weinman, 1995; Wolfe et al., 1997), and they often ommend alternative medical treatment or expla-
apply for disability payments (Van der Werf, nations (Black, 1996; Nimnuan, et al., 2001). The
Prins, Jongen, van der Meer, & Bleijenberg, 2000; general tendency of physicians to provide medical
Wolfe et al., 1997). The degree of disability often explanations (Nimnuan, Hotopf, & Wessely, 2000)
contrasts with the degree of disability reported by and the proliferation of alternative medicine
patients with demonstrable medical pathology, explanations for symptoms help patients avoid
for example, patients with heart disease, amputa- treatment by physicians who interpret somatic
tions, cancer, or rheumatoid arthritis. According symptoms as stress related in origin. Patients often
to Barsky and Borus, the fact that subjective dis- have educated and diagnosed themselves through
tress so often exceeds objective medical findings self-help groups, books and pamphlets, and
suggests that symptom magnification is a primary the Internet (Wessely, 1997), and unexplained
feature of these disorders. illnesses are more likely in adults whose parents
Ford (1997) described similar characteristics suffered from serious illnesses (Hotopf, Mayou,
in what he labeled as “fashionable illnesses.” Wadsworth, & Wessely, 1999).
Fashionable illnesses are characterized by vague, Many of these disorders have overlapping
subjective multi-system complaints, a lack of symptoms, reducing the reliability and validity of
objective laboratory findings, quasi-scientific their diagnoses (Deary, 1999; Wessely, Nimuan, &
explanations, overlap from one fashionable diag- Sharpe, 1999), and they may not be distinct enti-
nosis to another, symptoms consistent with ties. The same patient could receive a diagnosis of
depression or anxiety or both, and denial of psy- fibromyalgia from a rheumatologist, a diagnosis
chosocial distress or attribution of it to the illness. of chronic fatigue syndrome from an internist,
According to Ford, fashionable diagnoses repre- and a diagnosis of multiple chemical sensitivity
sent a heterogeneous collection of physical dis- from a clinician interested in that disorder
eases, somatization, and anxiety or depression. (Buchwald & Garrity, 1994). The complaints of
340 forensic neuropsychology

fatigue and joint pain are common to chronic stories, promotes some illnesses. Rather than
fatigue syndrome, fibromyalgia, toxic mold depending solely on medical authority for infor-
illness, silicone breast-implant-related illness, and mation, support groups also are viewed as sources
Gulf War-related illnesses. of information. Shorter noted that advocacy
Not only do the symptoms of these illnesses groups were responsible for the name change
overlap, but many of the symptoms are common from chronic fatigue syndrome to chronic
in healthy samples. Clearly, purportedly “neuro- immune dysfunction syndrome, despite the lack
logical” symptoms such as headaches, fatigue, of evidence of immune dysfunction in this illness
memory loss, and dizziness occur commonly in (Wessely, 1997). Shorter’s work predated the wide-
the general population (Ferguson, Mittenberg, spread availability of medical information and
Barone, & Schneider, 1999; Gouvier, Uddo-Crane, opinion on the Internet, which perhaps has con-
& Brown, 1988; Kroenke & Price, 1993; Sawchyn, tributed to the contemporary media trend that
Brulot, & Strauss, 2000). Healthy control partici- Shorter described.
pants reported the following frequencies of According to Shorter, the increasing degree of
neurological symptoms: fatigue, 33%; headaches, social isolation may play a role in culturally
58%; forgetfulness, 58%; poor concentration, 35% induced illnesses. He noted that people were less
(Paniak et al., 2002). Pain also is commonly expe- likely to receive consensual validation and feed-
rienced. In one large survey, joint pains (37%), back regarding the unimportance of common
back pain (32%), headaches (25%), chest pain symptoms such as fatigue and headache from, for
(25%), arm or leg pain (24%), and abdominal pain example, a wise aunt or friend. In the absence of
(24%) were reported by community dwellers sensible social input about normal symptoms,
(Kroenke & Price, 1993). The oft-replicated find- people may worry excessively about their signifi-
ing that these symptoms are common in various cance. Medical care providers with scientifically
control samples implies that people who view unsupported explanations also are sources of
themselves as able-bodied usually do not find beliefs about illnesses, and they may be iatrogenic
these symptoms to be disabling or pay undue causes of symptom production and maintenance
attention to them. Although people with disor- (Black, 2000).
ders that are medically unexplained may experi- In contrast to many of these arguments,
ence some of these symptoms to a more extreme Ferguson and Cassaday (2001-2002) hypothesized
degree than people who consider themselves that conditions labeled in this chapter as unex-
healthy, clinical observation suggests that some of plained are caused by a complex interaction of
them also seem to view common symptoms as biological, psychological, and environmental
signs of a serious illness, and some believe their influences. They emphasized the role of the
outcome will be bad (Sharpe, Chalder, Palmer, & immune system and its responsiveness to psycho-
Wessely, 1997). logical factors and stressors. Ferguson and
Shorter (1992) provided a historical view of Cassaday suggested that some of these illnesses
illnesses of mysterious origin. He maintained that could by caused by what they call a “bio-associa-
historical eras and cultures shaped the symptoms tive” mechanism or what psychologists refer to as
of illnesses. The individual is pressured to produce Pavlovian or classical conditioning. An illness ini-
symptoms that are considered legitimate. Different tially caused by a pathogen could be associated
symptoms are considered socially acceptable in with a trigger such as an odor. Later, the illness
different eras. As symptom legitimacy changes could be triggered by another odor. In this expla-
over time, people respond by producing different nation, the unconditioned stimulus is the patho-
symptoms. Paralysis, for example, no longer is gen and the conditioned stimulus is the odor. This
accepted in the mainstream North American cul- theory has received empirical support in the case
ture. In recent years, pain and fatigue have been of multiple chemical sensitivities (Sorg, Tschirgi,
accepted by our culture. Shorter posited that Swindell, et al., 2001; Van den Bergh, et al., 2001;
chronic fatigue syndrome and fibromyalgia repre- Winters, Devriese, Van Diest, et al., 2003). Others
sent contemporary prototypes of culturally have rejected a dualistic mind-body distinction in
induced illnesses. attempting to understand medically unexplained
Shorter also argued that medical authority has symptoms (Rief & Broadbent, 2007).
declined and the influence of the media on popu- Illnesses with medically unexplained symp-
lar opinions regarding illness has increased. The toms may be multiply determined (Ford,
media, always searching for new and sensational 1997). For example, a small percentage of veterans
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 341

with Gulf War illnesses suffered from leshma- PSEUDONEUROLOGICAL


niasis, a parasitic disease. Some Gulf War ILLNES S
veterans have diagnoses of chronic fatigue Pseudoneurological symptoms are complaints
syndrome or fibromyalgia. There may be multiple such as dizziness, numbness, weakness, and
etiologies for chronic fatigue syndrome, both memory loss that are not associated with objec-
pathophysiological and psychiatric (Afari & tive evidence of neurological disease. Research in
Buchwald, 2003). this area is useful in the understanding of all med-
Rief and Broadbent (2007), while not viewing ically unexplained symptoms.
illnesses such as fibromyalgia and chronic fatigue Many years ago, evidence of authentic neuro-
syndrome as having pathophysiological causes logical disease sometimes could not be found
as do diseases such as cancers and pneumonias, using the existing primitive diagnostic tools,
also believe that an explanation of medically potentially leading to incorrect diagnosis of
unexplained symptoms as purely psychogenic is pseudoneurological disease. In longitudinal
overly simplistic. They believe that some symp- research that began not long after World War II,
toms, at least initially, may have pathophysio- Slater (1965) showed that many patients in his era
logical (e.g., influenza) or psychophysiological diagnosed with hysteria ultimately were proven to
(e.g., stress of wartime deployment or battle have genuine neurological disease. After the
trauma) causes. However, patients with chronic advent of more advanced neurodiagnostic tools,
medically unexplained symptoms overemphasize there were similar studies. In one investigation, 56
the role of organic causes, an explanation that patients with diagnoses of conversion disorder
is unhelpful and countertherapeutic. Rief and were followed an average of 4.5 years later. Only
Broadbent reviewed many psychological contri- two patients later developed an organic deficit
butions to physical complaints, including tenden- that might have been related to the original epi-
cies to adapt catastrophic interpretations of bodily sode of illness (Couprie, Wijdicks, Rooijmans, &
sensations, to overestimate the probabilities of van Gijn, 1995). Others have reported similar
negative outcomes, to distort the probabilities of findings in longitudinal research suggesting that
organic causes of their symptoms provided by incorrect diagnoses of somatoform problems
their physicians, and to exhibit anxious attach- prove uncommon (Kent, Tomasson, & Coryell,
ment behaviors in their interactions with health 1995; Mace & Trimble, 1996; Crimlisk, Bhatia,
care personnel in a way that leads to overutiliza- Cope, David, Marsden, & Ron, 1998). In contrast
tion of health care. They also note the importance to the primitive workups of Slater’s era, these more
of rewards from the social system, including social recent studies confirmed the correctness of the
reinforcement from family and health care pro- diagnoses in the vast majority of patients diag-
viders and financial incentives for illness. nosed by neurological evaluation as suffering
Excessive anxiety about health often underlies from pseudoneurological illness.
medically unexplained symptoms (Taylor & Despite the absence of neurological disease,
Asmundson, 2004). The cognitive behavioral patients with pseudoneurological complaints
approach to health anxiety assumes that health often have measurable neuropsychological
anxiety is developed and maintained by cata- deficits. In the first of these studies (Matthews,
strophic interpretations of relatively normal or Shaw, & Klove, 1966), neurological and pseudo-
minor bodily signs and symptoms. People with neurological patients were compared on the
medically unexplained symptoms attempt to Halstead-Reitan battery and the WAIS. Statistical
cope by excessively checking their bodies, over- comparisons yielded significant differences on 17
attending to bodily sensations, and seeking reas- of 26 variables. However, the authors stated, “In
surance. Autonomic arousal, the physiological spite of the relatively high levels of statistical sig-
component of anxiety, often plays a role, both in nificance obtained by many of the comparison
creating more bodily sensations and in arousing variables, the use of any single one of them to clas-
fear about them. Excessive and medically unnec- sify individuals remains a doubtful procedure.
essary pursuit of health care often ensues. Useful cutoff points for the comparison variables
Unnecessary diagnostic tests and medications could not be established . . .” (Matthews et al.,1966,
may lead to additional, iatrogenic symptoms. p. 250). In a mixed group of pseudoneurological
Focus on disability may lead to inactivity, with cases, 54% were misclassified as brain damaged
numerous negative health consequences for mus- based on the Generalized Neuropsychological
culoskeletal and other systems of the body. Deficit Scale of the Halstead Reitan Battery, 34%
342 forensic neuropsychology

misclassified based on the Halstead Impairment epileptic seizure patients on the Portland Digit
Index, and 22% misclassified using the Average Recognition Test (PDRT), a measure of motiva-
Impairment Rating (Sherer & Adams, 1993). tion to remember (Binder, Salinksy & Smith,
Pseudoneurological patients also were impaired 1994), but they generally did not perform in the
on a verbal learning task (Sherer, Nixon, Parsons, range typically associated with malingering on
& Adams, 1992). this measure. The PDRT findings in the non-
More recent neuropsychological investiga- epileptic group (Binder et al., 1994) were consis-
tions of pseudoneurological patients often have tent with earlier qualitative observations judged
studied patients with nonepileptic seizures and to imply inconsistent effort (Brown et al., 1991)
consistently have documented poor performance. and with data showing frequent failures in patients
In these studies, patients were diagnosed with with nonepileptic events on another measure of
either epileptic or nonepileptic seizures after motivation, the Word Memory Test (Williamson,
intensive EEG video telemetry monitoring. Drane, & Stroup, 2007). One study, however,
Criteria for classification were described in detail failed to find differences between patients with
in these studies. The first of these studies (Wilkus, epileptic and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures
Dodrill, & Thompson, 1984) used a structured on measures of effort (Cragar, Berry, Fakhoury,
test battery with cutoff scores previously estab- et al., 2006). The influence of emotional factors on
lished in the same population. The test battery did neuropsychological performance was supported
not show any significant differences between the in a study showing that patients with psychogenic
groups with epileptic and nonepileptic seizures. seizures and emotional problems as measured by
The epileptic group performed abnormally on an the MMPI-2 performed worse on cognitive
average of 46% of the tests, and the nonepileptic testing than patients with psychogenic seizures
seizure patients produced abnormal scores on an with either relatively normal or purely somato-
average of 51% of the tests, a nonsignificant differ- form MMPI-2 profiles (Cragar, Berry, Fakhoury,
ence. A similar study found no significant differ- et al., 2002).
ences between patients with epileptic and In addition to measurable neuropsychological
nonepileptic seizures on 16 neuropsychological impairment, there is significant disability associ-
measures (Brown, Levin, Ramsay, Katz, & ated with pseudoneurological conditions. Many
Duchowny, 1991). Based on a qualitative analysis nonepileptic seizure patients never work again
of test data, Brown et al. concluded that the non- (Barry et al., 1998). The cause of nonepileptic
epileptic patients had inconsistent abnormalities seizures usually is psychiatric. Hence, the cause of
characteristic of non-neurological conditions. disability associated with nonepileptic seizures
In another investigation, Binder, Kindermann, also is psychiatric.
Heaton, and Salinsky (1998) compared control Investigators have studied the relationships
participants with epileptic and nonepileptic between mild head injury, seizure type, and the
seizure patients. The two patient groups with opportunity for financial compensation. Epileptic
seizures performed similarly and demonstrated seizures were not commonly found after mild
significant impairment compared with the con- head injury in an epidemiological study, and they
trol group; for example, the nonepileptic seizure were more common after moderate and especially
patients had a mean WAIS-R Full Scale IQ of severe head injury (Annegers, Hauser, Coan, &
92.0, a mean score 9.9 points lower than the con- Rocca, 1998). The prevailing opinion is that trau-
trol group. The difference between nonepileptic matic seizures of epileptic origin can be caused by
seizure patients and healthy controls on Trail cortical contusions. These contusions can be con-
Making Part B was 31.0 seconds. The mean for the sistent with a head injury defined as mild solely by
nonepileptic seizure group on Logical Memory I Glasgow Coma Scale criteria, however, a head
of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised was at injury with a Glasgow score of 13–15 and a contu-
the 31st percentile with no data available on this sion is often classified as a complicated mild head
measure from the control group. injury. If there is no evidence of contusions from
Factors other than neurological disease acute neuroimaging, and if the head injury was
explain neuropsychological abnormalities in trivial with little or no loss of consciousness and
patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. little or no post-traumatic amnesia, post-injury
There are studies suggesting the influence of seizures in a forensic setting usually will be of
motivational and emotional factors. Nonepileptic nonepileptic origin. Nonepileptic seizures, in con-
seizure patients performed more poorly than trast with epileptic seizures, commonly follow
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 343

mild head injuries that offer the opportunity for neuroendocrine abnormalities associated with
financial compensation (Westbrook, Devinsky, & fibromyalgia and that the illness is caused by
Geocadin, 1998). abnormal sensory processing (Bennett, 1999).
Diagnosis of seizures is within the domain of However, emotional problems also are associated
neurology, particularly the subspecialty of epilep- with neuroendocrine disorders (Barlow, 2000),
tology. Intensive EEG monitoring remains the and it is not clear that there are neuroendocrine
gold standard method of diagnosis of seizure type. abnormalities in fibromyalgia specific to that
Neuropsychological assessment can aid in the condition. Some investigators have reported
evaluation of post-injury seizures by identifying reduced cerebral blood flow in the subcortical
which patients fit the psychological profile of non- and brainstem regions in patients with fibromyal-
epileptic seizure patients or are likely to be diag- gia (Kwiatek, Barnden, Tedman, et al., 2000), but
nosed as nonepileptic with appropriate diagnostic similar findings are nonspecific and occur in psy-
tools. Persons with neuropsychological correlates chiatric patients (Hakala, Karlsson, Ruotsalainen,
of nonepileptic seizures are candidates for inten- et al., 2002; Lange, Wang, DeLuca, & Natelson,
sive EEG monitoring where definitive diagnosis is 1998). Posterior fossa decompression has been
possible. The MMPI-2 is the best single predictor used as neurosurgical treatment of fibromyalgia
for this purpose. In settings with high base rates of based solely on the rationale of overlapping
nonepileptic seizures, the MMPI-2 is about 70% symptoms with Chiari type I malformation, but
accurate in predicting the differential of epileptic this practice is controversial (Nash, Cheng, Meyer,
or nonepileptic seizures as diagnosed by neurol- & Remler, 2002).
ogy through intensive EEG monitoring, accord- There are some well-established facts regard-
ing to a review of studies (Dodrill, Wilkus, & ing fibromyalgia. It is far more frequent in females
Batzel, 1993). Conversion V profiles and scores on than in males (Wolfe et al., 1995) and often is the
HS or HY above 79 are associated with nonepilep- basis for claims of disability (Wolfe et al., 1997).
tic seizures. Epileptic seizure groups score about The condition sometimes is associated with mild
10 points lower on HS and HY than nonepileptic neuropsychological deficits (Hart, Martelli, &
groups (Dodrill et al., 1993). In adult settings, sei- Zasler, 2000), but the reason for this association,
zure onset generally is more recent in nonepilep- when it exists, is unclear. In one study the fibro-
tic than epileptic seizures. Chronicity of seizures myalgia group performed within the normal
and routine EEGs were combined with the range on all cognitive measures although there
MMPI-2 to yield 86% accuracy in predicting sei- were some differences between fibromyalgia
zure diagnosis with approximately equal accuracy patients and controls (Grace et al., 1999). Other
for both types of seizures (Storzbach, Binder, investigators found that more than a third of the
Salinsky, Campbell, & Mueller, 2000). fibromyalgia patients who either were seeking
disability payments or who already were receiving
F I B R O M YA L G I A them scored below the cutoffs on measures of
Fibromyalgia is characterized by widespread joint motivation to remember (Gervais, Russell, Green,
pain, insomnia (Bohr, 1996), and nonrestorative et al., 2001). When participants with evidence of
sleep. The diagnosis of fibromyalgia is based upon poor effort were eliminated, there were no differ-
the trigger point exam. In response to four kilo- ences between fibromyalgia patients and controls
grams of finger pressure applied by the examiner (Suhr, 2003). Subjective cognitive complaints out-
(pressure sufficient to blanch the fingernail of the stripped objective abnormalities (Grace et al.,
examiner), the patient must report pain in at least 1999) when only patients who passed a measure
11 of 18 trigger points, including sites both above of effort were included (Suhr, 2003). In a study
and below the waist. These criteria have proved that requires replication, fibromyalgia patients,
problematic (Bohr, 1996; Hadler, 1997) for two compared with controls with memory complaints,
reasons. First, it is difficult for a clinician to were slower on measures of naming speed but
consistently apply four kilograms of pressure and were better than controls on some measures of
second, it is not likely that patients will reliably visuomotor speed (Leavitt & Katz, 2008).
report the experience of pain, an inherently Fibromyalgia patients were more likely than
subjective event. The illness can be viewed as control subjects to have a history of psychiatric
entirely subjective (Hadler, 1997). problems including mood and anxiety disorders,
The etiology of fibromyalgia is unknown. according to a meta-analytic review (Henningsen,
It is believed by some investigators that there are Zimmerman, & Sattel, 2003), and more medically
344 forensic neuropsychology

unexplained physical symptoms across several heterogeneity of the illness was amplified by the
organ systems (Walker, Keegan, Gardner, Sullivan, finding that CFS of sudden onset was associated
Katon, & Bernstein, 1997). Fibromyalgia patients with slower information processing speed (Clay-
had more evidence of abuse and trauma in both poole, Noonan, Mahurin, et al., 2007). Exercise
childhood and adulthood, compared with patients caused a more severe decline in cognition in
with rheumatoid arthritis (Walker, Keegan, people with CFS than controls (LaManca et al.,
Gardner, Sullivan, Bernstein, & Katon, 1997). 1998). Subsequently, an investigation using mono-
zygotic twins discordant for the illness found little
C H R O N I C FAT I G U E evidence of vulnerability to cognitive problems
SYNDROME soon after exercise in people with CFS (Claypoole,
Diagnostic criteria for chronic fatigue syndrome Mahurin, Fischer, et al., 2001).
(CFS) in the United States include chronic Factors independent of the symptoms of CFS
disabling fatigue and at least four of eight other may be responsible for the association found in
features including muscle ache, joint pain, subjec- many studies between neuropsychological deficits
tive cognitive problems, sore throat, new head- and the illness. Monozygotic twins discordant for
ache, nonrestorative sleep, post-exertion malaise, CFS showed no cognitive differences (Mahurin,
and swollen lymph glands (Fukuda et al., 1994). Claypoole, Goldberg, et al., 2004). In the Mahurin
These symptoms are similar to those associated et al. study, however, both healthy twins and
with fibromyalgia. Although some symptoms of twins with CFS performed worse than controls on
chronic fatigue are similar to the symptoms of measures of information processing speed, and
depression, the emotional aspects of depression their findings suggested that the cognitive deficits
such as sadness, anhedonia, and low self-esteem preexisted the onset of illness. Although premor-
often seem to be absent in CFS (Jason et al., 1997). bid mental aptitude differences were found as
Afari and Buchwald (2003) reviewed CFS. Like measured by the Armed Forces Qualification Test
fibromyalgia and multiple chemical sensitivities, between CFS and control participants after the
it is more prevalent in females than males (Jason Persian Gulf War, these premorbid differences
et al., 1999). did not explain the impaired performance of the
Neuropsychological impairment in CFS has CFS group compared with and controls on some
been reviewed (DiPino & Kane, 1996; Tiersky measures of information processing speed and
et al., 1997). Intellect and complex problem- attention (Binder et al., 2001).
solving abilities are preserved, but CFS patients Several investigators have studied regional
suffer from a deficit in complex information pro- cerebral blood flow in CFS, typically with single
cessing (Tiersky et al., 1997). Clearly, subjective photon emitted computerized tomography
ratings of cognitive impairment are more pro- (SPECT). Early studies suggested that the illness
nounced than objective neuropsychological find- was associated with reductions in cerebral blood
ings (Tiersky et al., 1997). There is mixed evidence flow (Ichise et al., 1992; Schwartz et al., 1994).
of motivational problems on neuropsychological Subsequent studies parallel results previously
testing in patients with CFS. A deficit was found reviewed for fibromyalgia: Either there are no
on a forced choice measure of motivation to abnormalities in CFS (Fischler et al., 1996; Lewis
remember in a clinical sample (Van der Werf et al., et al., 2001) or the abnormalities are nonspecific
2000), but not on a similar measure in a research and similar to those found in psychiatric groups
sample composed largely of people who did not (Lange et al., 1998). A fluorine-deoxyglucose
consider themselves disabled (Binder, Storzbach, positron emission tomography (PET) study
Campbell, Rohlman, & Anger, 2001). A meta-ana- suggested that hypometabolism in the brainstem
lytic review showed that chronic fatigue syndrome was found only in CFS and not in depression
often is associated with a history of anxiety and (Tirelli et al., 1998), but a study using the same
depression (Henningsen et al., 2003), but cogni- technique found no differences between a group
tive deficits were found in another study despite with CFS and a group with somatization disorder
controlling for history of psychiatric illness (Hakala et al., 2002).
(DeLuca, Johnson, Ellis, & Natelson, 1997). The Controversies regarding the etiology of CFS
same study (DeLuca et al., 1997) found that CFS have been reviewed (Afari & Buchwald, 2003;
of gradual onset was associated with more psychi- Tiersky et al., 1997). Various hypotheses have
atric illness but less severe memory problems than been advanced including viral, limbic system, and
CFS of sudden onset. The need to consider the immune system explanations, all lacking basic
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 345

scientific support. There is no evidence of a spe- allergy syndrome. MCS was reviewed compre-
cific viral etiologic agent. In some quarters, CFS is hensively in an edited volume (Sparks, 2000), and
viewed as the equivalent of a psychiatric condi- Labarge and McCaffrey (2000), Staudenmeyer
tion. According to this view, patients with CFS are (2001), and McCaffrey and Yantz (2007) have
engaging in illness behavior and have normal reviewed the topic from neuropsychological and
complaints (Barsky & Borus, 1999). In the view of psychological perspectives. The illness is charac-
others (Henningsen et al., 2003), the lack of unity terized by symptoms in multiple organ systems in
between diagnoses of CFS and depression and/or reaction to a variety of low levels of chemically
anxiety suggests that CFS should not be consid- unrelated common odors at doses that do not
ered a psychiatric entity. cause symptoms in the general population. The
Wessely and colleagues (Sharpe et al., 1997) substances causing discomfort in people with
have adopted a complex view of the etiology of MCS are irritants rather than neurotoxicants
CFS, emphasizing the distinction among factors (Bolla, 2000). Symptoms of MCS include fatigue,
that may have predisposed patients to develop the confusion, dizziness, and gastrointestinal, muscu-
illness such as lifestyle, work stress, and personal- loskeletal, and respiratory problems. These symp-
ity; factors that may have triggered the illness such toms may last for days or weeks. Patients with
as viral infection or life events; and factors that MCS believe they are sensitive to certain odors
may have perpetuated the illness such as cerebral and they avoid common odors such as perfume,
dysfunction, sleep disorder, depression, inconsis- gasoline and other petroleum products, new
tent activity, and misunderstanding of the illness carpets, and ordinary household cleansers. Some
and fear of making it worse. people with MCS attempt to avoid close contact
It also is hypothesized that the low levels of with printed matter because of perceived sensi-
natural killer cells in the immune system explain tivity to ink. Food allergies are common in this
the illness (Whiteside & Friberg, 1998), although population. The avoidance of so many common
the relationship between low-level immune substances often leads to disability. Many patients
system activation and CFS symptoms is unclear with MCS also are diagnosed with CFS and
(Afari & Buchwald, 2003; Wessely, 1997). More fibromyalgia (Bell, Baldwin, & Schwartz, 1998;
controversial is the posterior fossa decompression Buchwald & Garrity, 1994; Sparks, 2000), because
surgery that also has been performed on fibromy- the symptoms overlap among these disorders.
algia patients (Nash et al., 2002). Just as neuropsy- MCS may follow the misperception of exposure to
chological findings differ in subgroups (Claypoole toxins, such as occurred in the Persian Gulf War
et al., 2007; DeLuca et al., 1997), the etiology of of 1991 (Reid, Hotopf, Hull, et al., 2001) and
CFS may be heterogenous (Afari & Buchwald, during construction of a chemical weapons incin-
2003). erator in Umatilla, Oregon (Binder, Storzbach, &
Salinsky, 2006). Expectations that a particular
M U LT I P L E C H E M I C A L odor is harmful clearly play a role in the deve-
SENSITIVITIES lopment of symptoms (Das-Munshi, Rubin, &
The term “multiple chemical sensitivities” (MCS) Wessely, 2006; Winters et al., 2003).
has been discarded by some authorities (Sparks, This clinical entity is controversial, indeed.
2000) for several reasons, including the lack of The clinical ecological approach views the illness
conclusive pathophysiological evidence that as organic and caused by chemical exposure and
people with the diagnosis are abnormally sensi- recommends costly lifestyle changes and other
tive to specific chemicals (Dalton & Hummel, interventions. Mainstream medicine has been
2000), because the relationship between symp- particularly critical of the clinical ecological
toms and exposures is unproven, and because approach (American Academy of Allergy and
MCS is not a clinically defined disease with gener- Immunology, 1986; American College of Phys-
ally accepted underlying pathophysiological icians, 1989). Laboratory provocation in patients
mechanisms or validated criteria for diagnosis. As diagnosed with MCS with the chemicals identi-
discussed by Sparks (2000), the term “idiopathic fied as incitants yielded extremely low sensitivity
environmental intolerance” has been proposed as and specificity values, data strongly suggesting
a replacement. Although MCS is the term com- that the diagnosis of MCS has no validity
monly employed by many clinicians, the condi- (Staudenmayer, Selner, & Buhr, 1993). Consistent
tion has other labels, including chemical with these results, objective medical findings
intolerance, environmental illness, and total usually do not exist (Labarge & McCaffery, 2000).
346 forensic neuropsychology

MCS often is associated with various psychiatric to a solvent-like alcohol scent (butanol), a winter-
illnesses (Black, 2000; Staudenmayer, 2001), green scent, and a balsam scent reported more
especially depression (Caccappolo-van Vliet, symptoms when they were warned of solvent
Kelly-McNeil, Natelson, et al., 2002) and anxiety. exposure than when they were told they would be
Experimental data from some laboratories pro- exposed to a natural extract with a relaxing effect.
vided no evidence that people with MCS are any The odors and self-reported chemical intolerance
more sensitive to odors than control subjects also had significant effects on symptoms. There
(Dalton & Hummel, 2000; Doty et al., 1988), while are a variety of cognitive influences related to
other laboratory challenge studies suggested both sensory and somatic responses to chemical stimu-
a physiological hypersensitivity and a psychologi- lation (Dalton & Hummel, 2000). In one classical
cal component to the illness (Fiedler, Giardino, conditioning study there was no effect on somatic
Natelson, et al., 2004; Osterberg, Orbaek, Karlson, symptoms without a warning about harmful
et al., 2003). consequences (Winters et al., 2003).
A model of MCS involving neurohormonal The few available neuropsychological studies
sensitization of olfactory-limbic circuits has been of MCS show little or no evidence of neurocogni-
proposed (Bell, Baldwin, & Schwartz, 1998). tive impairment in controlled investigations
According to this model, some individuals can be (Bolla, 2000; Caccappolo-van Vliet et al., 2002;
sensitized by low doses of chemicals that may not Labarge & McCaffrey, 2000; Simon, Daniell,
have been troublesome to them in the past. In this Stockbridge, et al., 1993). Bolla emphasized the
model, amplification of responses is hypothesized lack of specificity of neuropsychological testing
to occur in the limbic system, including the in persons with symptoms of MCS because of the
mesolimbic dopaminergic (reward) pathway. lack of a biological marker of MCS and the
Subsequent activation of sensitized pathways is absence of evidence of neurocognitive problems
purported to lead to impaired function of behav- among people with MCS symptoms, including
ioral, immune, and other systems. cognitive tests thought to be especially sensitive to
MCS can be caused by classical conditioning. limbic system dysfunction (Brown-DeGagne &
Limited pairings of a conditioned stimulus of a McGlone, 1999). Expectancy appears to play a
noxious odor and an unconditioned stimulus of role in neuropsychological problems thought to
air rich in carbon dioxide led to the conditioning be associated with MCS. In a placebo-controlled
of both somatic symptoms and altered respiratory study, people with MCS had lowered neuro-
responses in humans in reaction to just the condi- psychological performance when they perceived
tioned stimulus of the odor (Van den Bergh et al., that they were exposed to substances that they
2001). Importantly, conscious awareness of the considered “chemical,” but there were no perfor-
relationship between the odor and the symptoms mance decrements associated with the actual
induced by carbon dioxide was not necessary for substances to which they were exposed (Smith &
the effects to occur. Symptomatic reactions gener- Sullivan, 2003). In summary, there is no credible
alized not only to new, noxious odors but also evidence of neuropsychological impairment
to situations in which participants imagined caused by MCS.
situations where the symptoms had occurred.
There were individual differences in the effect. TOX I C M O L D A N D S I C K
Participants who had higher scores on a measure BUILDING SYNDROME
of negative affectivity (neuroticism) or who suf- A guideline of the American College of
fered from nonorganic illnesses had more learned Occupational and Environmental Medicine
symptoms and more easily generalized the illness (Hardin et al., 2003) summarized the state of
response to new odors. Extinction was demon- knowledge regarding toxic mold and human
strated with sufficient exposure to just the condi- health. Some fungi produce beneficial metabolites
tioned stimulus. This work has been replicated by known as mycotoxins that can be converted into
the same group and by another lab (Sorg, Tschirgi, antibiotics such as penicillin and cyclosporine.
Swindell, et al., 2001; Van den Bergh et al., 2001; “Mold” is the commonplace term for multicellular
Winters, Devriese, Van Diest, et al., 2003). fungi that grow in a mat. The growth of molds
Experimental work on the interaction of in indoor environments has been cited as the
health expectancy and the pleasantness of odors explanation for a variety of nonspecific symp-
also shows a psychological influence on symp- toms. However, the reality is that fungi and
tomatology (Dalton, 1999). Participants exposed molds are ubiquitous and exposure is unavoidable
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 347

unless draconian isolation measures are utilized. and cerebral infarctions. Subarachnoid hemor-
Essential for the decomposition of organic mate- rhage is a rare complication (Erly, Labadie,
rial, many species live on the surface of the Williams, et al., 1999).
human body. Toxic-mold-related illnesses resulting from
Fungi can harm human health through allergy, breathing air in buildings allegedly contaminated
infection, and toxicity. Allergic responses occur in by mold are a different matter than well-defined,
roughly 5% of the population and usually are serious diseases such as ergotism, which is caused
experienced as allergic asthma or rhinitis, the by eating mold-infected grain, or disseminated
latter commonly known as “hay fever.” Uncommon coccidioidomycosis. To be sure, mold can be
allergy syndromes include allergic bronchopul- found in buildings. However, mycotoxins are not
monary aspergillosis and allergic fungal sinusitis. significantly volatile; they do not off-gas into the
Ingestion of foods with sufficient quantity of cer- environment or pass through solid substances
tain molds can cause illnesses known as myco- such as walls. The musty odor associated with
toxicoses. Superficial fungal infections such as mold is not caused by mycotoxins.
tinea pedia, commonly known as “athletes foot” Sick building syndrome is a poorly defined set
and tinea cruris, commonly known as “jock itch” of symptoms attributed to occupancy of an alleg-
are common in humans. Of much greater concern edly contaminated building. Usually, no specific
are infections in persons who are immunocom- cause is found for the complaints. In some cases a
promised, including patients on immunosuppres- mycotoxin-producing mold, Stachybotrys char-
sant medications and persons with AIDS and tarum, is identified in the building, but generally,
severe diabetes. Previously healthy persons are there is no scientific evidence of a causal relation-
subject to rare, serious fungal infections caused by ship between indoor air exposure to Stachybotrys
fungi including Cryptococcus and Histoplasma or chartarum and human illnesses. Hardin et al.
to hypersensitivity pneumonitis, an exaggerated (2003) concluded that fungi rarely are significant
immune system response after inhalation of large human pathogens, except through ingestion of
quantities of protein such as fungal proteins. contaminated foods and that current scientific
“Organic dust toxic syndrome” has been described evidence does not support the proposition that
after inhalation exposure of agricultural workers human health has been adversely affected by
to spoiled grain products. These illnesses are asso- inhaled mycotoxins. Similar views have been
ciated with thick clouds of dust, and not with expressed by others (Burge, 2001; Kuhn &
musty odor from wood with dry rot. The concen- Ghannoum, 2003; Robbins et al., 2000).
tration of fungi in grain silos is far in excess of the Hodgson (2000) suggested that sick building
concentration in normal indoor environments syndrome could be caused by problems associ-
(Lees-Haley, 2004). ated with humidification and ventilation systems.
Ergotism, in one form, is a neurological dis- He also noted that individuals varied in their sus-
ease resulting from consumption of the alkaloids ceptibility to the two most common building-
derived from the Claviceps purpurea fungus, related problems, namely, mucosal problems in
found in rye and other grains. A neurovascular either the nasal passages or eyes.
gangrenous form of the disease also occurs. Some relevant neuropsychological data on
Disease outbreaks throughout history probably sick building syndrome have been published. In
were more commonly associated with rye wheat one study, there were no differences between
consumption because the dark fungus is more dif- exposed persons and controls (Hodgson et al.,
ficult to see in dark rye flour than in lighter col- 1998). An experimental acute exposure study of
ored flours. Outbreaks of this disease were respiratory irritants, including volatile organic
common until the 19th century in Europe. Out- compounds and ozone, showed no neuropsycho-
breaks continue, particularly in less-developed logical effect (Fiedler et al., 2005).
countries. Neurological symptoms include par- Some authors have claimed that their data
esthesias, muscle contractions, seizures, psycho- supported a link between inhalation of toxic mold
sis, and signs of peripheral neuropathy (Pryse- in buildings and neuropsychological deficits, but
Phillips, 2003; Walton, 1985). these studies, as reviewed in more detail elsewhere
Coccidioides immitis, a mold found in the soil (McCaffrey & Yantz, 2007), are badly flawed. For
in the southwestern United States, can be inhaled example, Gordon, Johanning, and Haddad (1999),
and lead to a pulmonary infection, followed some- in a clinical study that was not peer reviewed,
times by disseminated diseases such as vasculitis reported neurocognitive impairment, but there
348 forensic neuropsychology

were no control subjects and the participants were depression is associated with large changes in
self-selected after claiming cognitive impairment immune system functioning, for example, lower
caused by inhalation of toxic mold. When patients natural killer-cell activity and change in white
are self-selected there could be many reasons for blood cell counts (Herbert & Cohen, 1993).
their symptoms other than the cause that they Suppression of immune system function persists
allege. Other authors have claimed that there were in persons with PTSD (Boscarino, 2008) and in
neuropsychological effects from exposure (Baldo, persons with past history of PTSD but with no
Ahmad, & Ruff, 2002). Critiques of the Baldo current psychiatric disorder (Kawamura, Kim, &
et al. study (Lees-Haley, 2004; McCaffrey & Yantz, Asukai, 2001). Cytokine-induced immune system
2005) pointed out several methodological flaws. activation temporarily produces memory impair-
Among the errors were lack of any reasonable ment (Reichenberg, Yirmiya, Schuld, et al., 2001).
control group, lack of information about the type PTSD among Vietnam veterans was associated
and intensity of exposure, and the self-selected with self-reported diagnoses of circulatory, diges-
nature of the patient/litigants in the study. tive, musculoskeletal, nervous system, respiratory,
Neuropsychological abnormalities in this popula- and infectious diseases (Boscarino, 1997) and
tion sometimes can be explained by suboptimal more than two-fold increase in mortality
motivation rather than by actual cognitive impair- (Boscarino, 2008).
ment (Stone, Boone, Back-Madruga, et al., 2006). A neuroanatomical model of panic (Gorman,
At the present time, there is no credible scien- Kent, Sullivan, & Coplan, 2000) was based on
tific evidence that inhalation of toxic mold such as animal studies of conditioned fear due to the
stachybotrys in the concentrations typically found behavioral and physiologic similarities between
in residences and workplaces causes encephalopa- panic and conditioned fear. Gorman et al.
thy (Lees-Haley, 2003; 2004; McCaffrey & Yantz, described a fear network with the amygdala inter-
2007; Terr, 2005). Respiratory, systemic, and cog- acting with the hippocampus and prefrontal
nitive complaints are common in patients alleging cortex. The amygdala receives the sensation
injury from toxic molds, but transient irritation, of a conditioned stimulus and coordinates auto-
unrelated and preexisting diseases, and psycho- nomic, sympathetic, and behavioral responses
genic problems explain the complaints (Khalili & to the conditioned stimulus. Projections from
Bardana, 2005). There is no specific pathophy- the amygdala to the periaquadactal gray causes
siological mechanism to explain the complaints of postural freezing. The fear network could be
patients alleging building-related inhalational inappropriately activated if bodily cues were pro-
mold toxicity (Khalili & Bardana, 2005). The con- cessed defectively. As noted above, early stressors
dition has been characterized as litigation-induced also cause changes in neuroendocrine function-
(Lees-Haley, 2003; 2004; Terr, 2005) and as a ing in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis.
pseudoillness (Terr, 2005). Davidson (2000) reviewed further relation-
ships between emotions, psychopathology, and
PSYCHOLOGICAL STRES S neurophysiology. People have baseline electro-
AND DISEASE physiological differences that are stable and that
Human and infrahuman studies indicate that predict individual differences in reactivity to emo-
stressors can cause pathological changes in func- tionally eliciting events. Davidson characterized
tioning in the immune and endocrine systems these differences as forming a neurophysiological
and disease (McEwen, 2002). Individuals differ vulnerability to the elicitation of both positive and
in their biological susceptibility to stressors, pos- negative emotions. The intensity of negative affect
sibly because of psychological traits (Kirschbaum is related to these neurophysiological differences
et al., 1995). Animal models have shown that as well as to the immune system functioning and
early stressors cause chronic alterations in hypo- activity of the autonomic nervous system. In
thalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function, chroni- infrahuman subjects, early environmental manip-
cally negative emotions, and easily elicited alarm ulations lead to differences in reactivity and brain
reactions in juvenile monkeys, changes that circuitry (Barlow, 2000; Davidson, 2000).
persist into adulthood (Barlow, 2000). In veterans PTSD and severe stressors have been asso-
of World War II and Vietnam the stress of combat ciated with cognitive abnormalities and structural
experience was associated with an increased brain changes in humans. Excess levels of
risk of physical decline and death (Boscarino, glucocorticoids secreted during stress may con-
2008; Elder, Shanahan, & Clipp, 1997). In humans, tribute to neuronal death and atrophy in the
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 349

hippocampus in PTSD and major depression interpretation of assessment results as well as the
(Lee, Ogle, & Sapolsky, 2002; Sapolsky, 2000). possible impact on health of some forms of
Hippocampal size was associated with diagnoses inaccurate self-disclosure.
of PTSD and depression (Bremner et al., 1995).
Neuroimaging findings associated with PTSD ACCURACY OF
have been reviewed (Grossman, Buchsman, & SELF-DISCLOSURE AND
Yehuda, 2002). The chapter by Andrikopoulos and I M PA C T O N H E A LT H
Greiffenstein on PTSD in the current volume fur- In this section, literature will be reviewed showing
ther reviews this literature and studies of possible that the level of accuracy of people who are pro-
cognitive abnormalities associated with PTSD. viding their medical and psychiatric histories or
Exposure to trauma is associated with the in reporting current psychological distress is less
development of medically unexplained physical than may generally be assumed by health care
symptoms, PTSD, depression, and substance professionals. People often are inaccurate in
abuse (Beckham et al., 1998; Schnurr & Spiro, reporting easily defined medical events (Harlow
1999; Sledjeski, Speisman, & Dierker, 2008; Stein, & Linet, 1989). For example, acute medical ill-
Walker, Hazen, & Forde, 1997). As noted above, nesses and the use of sick leave were not accurately
trauma exposure in those who survive natural reported a relatively short time later (Rogler,
disasters is associated with pseudoneurological Malgady, & Tryon, 1992). Recall of acute illnesses
symptoms (Cardena & Spiegel, 1993; Escobar occurred in only 42% of cases only a month later,
et al., 1992). Somatization symptoms are associ- and recall of medical events was 70% accurate
ated with both PTSD (Andreski, Chilcoat, & after one month but only 26% accurate 9–12
Breslau, 1998) and panic disorder (Simon, Katon, months later (Rogler et al., 1992). Patients with
& Sparks, 1990). Somatization also is associated medically unexplained neurological symptoms
with a history of childhood sexual abuse tend to overpathologize their medical histories
(Morrison, 1989). Physical and sexual abuse and (Schrag, Brown, & Trimble, 2004).
other types of trauma also are associated with People also often are inaccurate reporters of
nonepileptic seizures (Alper, Devinsky, Perrine, childhood events of potential psychological
Vazquez, & Luciano, 1993; Rosenberg, Rosenberg, significance. For example, a longitudinal study
Williamson, & Wolford, 2000) and diagnoses of found that participants at age 18 often had recol-
fibromyalgia (Walker, Keegan, Gardner, Sullivan, lections discrepant from more contemporaneous
Bernstein, & Katon, 1997). data, including reports by their mothers of family
conflict at various ages, earlier self-reports of
T H R E AT S T O T H E hyperactivity, and earlier self-reports of depres-
A C C U R A C Y O F T H E H I S T O RY sion (Henry, Moffit, Caspi, Langley, & Silva, 1994).
P ROV I D E D B Y T H E A 30-year follow-up of ex-child guidance clinic
EXAMINEE patients, using methodology similar to Henry
Neuropsychological assessment requires differen- et al., examined interview agreement with
tial diagnosis (Binder, 1997). The patient may be events previously recorded (Robins et al., 1985).
symptomatic because of depression or other psy- Agreement ranged from low to high, depending
chiatric problems, pseudoneurological problems, on the event. For example, after 30 years 91%
developmental learning disabilities, medical ill- correctly recalled not always living with their
ness unrelated to the injury, malingering parents, but only 47% were cognizant of their
(Larrabee, this volume), or chronic pain disorder family receiving welfare assistance and only 56%
(Greve, Bianchini, & Ord, this volume). It is erro- remembered living with relatives.
neous to automatically attribute symptoms to a Traumatic events sometimes are underre-
traumatic brain injury (Roebuck-Spencer & ported or otherwise distorted. Adults who were
Sherer, this volume; Larrabee, this volume), sexually abused or otherwise traumatized during
alleged neurotoxic exposure (Bolla, this volume), childhood or early in adulthood, according to
or other events without performing a differential childhood records contemporaneous with the
diagnosis. In order to make an accurate differen- reported abuse, often failed to report this history
tial diagnosis it is essential to obtain accurate when directly asked many years later (Widom &
historical information. The purpose of this sec- Morris, 1997). A longitudinal study of Persian
tion is to discuss the barriers to obtaining an Gulf War veterans found that participants were, in
accurate history and some other confounds in the general, more likely to report specific traumatic
350 forensic neuropsychology

events two years after their return from the preexisting problems occurred in patients who
Persian Gulf than one month after their return, judged the MVA to either be their fault or no one’s
and that the increase in traumatic memories was fault, the rate of denial of preexisting pain was
associated with increased symptoms of PTSD twice as high in patients who blamed the MVA on
(Southwick, Morgan, Nicolaou, & Charney, 1997). another party. In this study, substance abuse,
These authors felt that exaggeration was the most psychological problems, and drug and alcohol
likely reason for the increase in self-reported abuse were considered relevant comorbid condi-
exposure to traumatic events. A competing hypo- tions; about 75% of the participants had comorbid
thesis not ruled out by the authors was that the conditions documented in preexisting records
initial reports minimized meaningful events that that they denied when providing past medical
later, after reflection, were more fully recalled. histories after the MVAs. In summary, patients,
Another longitudinal study of Gulf War veterans litigants, and people in a position to become
showed that participants who reported high litigants are poor historians and minimize the
combat stress were more likely to believe that they reports of premorbid problems.
had been exposed to chemical or biological weap- Not only is past mental health and medical
ons (Stuart, Ursano, Fullerton, & Wessely, 2008). history incorrectly reported in, on the average, a
Although exposure to stressors may be exag- minimizing direction, but current mental health
gerated, mental health history generally is mini- problems sometimes are denied. In one investiga-
mized (Simon & VonKorff, 1995). In one study, tion, volunteer participants completed self-report
depression was studied by structured interview, ratings of psychiatric symptoms and were rated by
and a year later in follow-up the subjects com- an expert on mental health through use of the
pleted a self-report instrument measuring depres- Early Memory Test (Shedler, Mayman, & Manis,
sion (Coyne, Thompson, & Racioppo, 2001). 1993). This test was blindly scored by the expert
There was no significant overlap between the ini- based on qualitative factors such as self-represen-
tial structured interview and self-report after a tation, affective tone, coherence of narrative, and
year, a finding the authors found “disheartening” presence of internal contradictions. Some partici-
(Coyne et al., 2001, p. 167). Premorbid somatic pants were classified as reporting illusory mental
and mental symptoms often associated with post- health because they were abnormal on the Early
concussive syndrome also were minimized in a Memory Test and normal on self-report (Shedler
longitudinal study of athletes, some of whom sus- et al., 1993).
tained concussions while participating in inter- Cognitive behavioral theorists have provided
collegiate athletics (Ferguson et al., 1999). partial explanation for the phenomenon of illu-
Ferguson et al., who characterized this observa- sory mental health. Anxiety is conceptualized as
tion as the “good old days” phenomenon, repli- comprising three loosely coupled response sys-
cated some of the findings of other researchers tems of overt behavior, verbal report, and physio-
(Hilsabeck, Gouvier & Bolter, 1998; Mittenberg logical activation (Lang & Cuthbert, 1984). People
et al., 1992). Symptomatic patients after mild TBI often experience stressors physiologically or
reported fewer premorbid symptoms than did respond behaviorally without reporting or experi-
normal controls (Gunstad & Suhr, 2001; encing a cognitive component of the anxiety; they
Mittenberg et al., 1992). The same observation feel anxious but are unable to verbalize what is
was made of people in litigation claiming head- making them anxious (Barlow, 2000). Many
injury symptoms (Lees-Haley, Williams, & patients with medically unexplained symptoms
English, 1996). Denial of well-documented preex- deny psychopathology, but often the medically
isting problems has been documented in striking unexplained somatic or neurological symptoms
fashion in patients after motor vehicle accidents coexist with emotional complaints (Cragar et al.,
(MVAs) with axial pain complaints (Carragee, 2002; Henningsen et al., 2003; Rief & Broadbent,
2008). The accuracy of past medical, psychologi- 2007).
cal, and substance abuse histories provided by
335 patients from five spine specialist clinics LY I N G A N D E X A G G E R AT I O N
was compared with pre-MVA records (Don & The preceding section discussed inaccurate
Carragee, 2009). About 50% of the patients over- reporting of mental health and medical history
all who denied pre-MVA axial pain problems without respect to the intentionality of the inac-
actually had preexisting axial pain documented in curate reporting. This section considers lying,
medical records. Although denial of documented the intentional distortion of information, and
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 351

exaggeration of history. Mental health experts compensable incident such as an accident, or their
generally are not permitted to testify in court past medical history.
regarding the credibility of people whom they Level of education is related to premorbid and
have examined, an issue that is determined only post-injury neuropsychological performance.
by the trier of fact. Despite this evidentiary restric- Whenever possible, self-reported educational
tion, experts may choose how much weight to attainment should be checked in forensic cases.
give to reports by examinees regarding pre-injury If transcripts are not available, they should be
and post-injury history, including educational requested. If one cannot obtain a transcript, some
and occupational attainments, medical history, colleges (Johnson-Greene & Binder, 1995) and
symptoms, and limitations. An expert should give high schools may verify by telephone attendance
less weight to improbable statements by an exam- and attainment of degrees and diplomas. One
inee, and statements that cannot be verified are nonforensic patient reported that he had received
less probable if they are given by a person who is a degree from the Illinois Institute of Technology.
known to have been inaccurate or deceptive about A phone call to this university revealed no record
other aspects of her/his history. Therefore, of his attendance. When asked why he made a
attention should be paid to inconsistencies and false report, he replied, “Because it’s a good
misstatements, some of which could be lies. school.” The same patient with cerebral palsy who
Some people, usually males, exaggerate ath- reported that he was a football star also claimed
letic or military exploits. Some well-publicized that he had graduated from college. The univer-
incidents of resume padding by public figures sity in question had no record of his attendance.
provide examples. An Oregon Congressman, Wes Grades obtained in high school sometimes are
Cooley, lost his seat in the U.S. House of Repre- exaggerated in forensic cases (Greiffenstein, Baker,
sentatives in the 1990s after it was discovered that & Johnson-Greene, 2002), underscoring the
his claim of military service in the Korean War need to review school records rather than rely on
was false. Cooley also made false claims about self-report.
motorcycle racing success. A manager of the Medical history sometimes is underreported
Toronto Blue Jays baseball team, Tim Johnson, to such an extreme that one must question if
was fired after falsely claiming combat experience the underreporting is deliberate. Whether mini-
in Vietnam; and a University of Notre Dame foot- mization of relevant prior problems was inten-
ball head coach, George O’Leary, was terminated tional or not was not directly assessed in the axial
when it was discovered that he exaggerated his pain study summarized above (Don & Carragee,
football playing experience and education. 2009), but I would infer that many of the partici-
Patients also occasionally exaggerate past pants were lying. Patients who felt the MVA was
experiences. A woman undergoing intensive EEG another party’s fault often did not report history
monitoring for diagnosis of medically intractable of axial pain problems even when they had
seizures reported onset of the seizures while serv- received spinal imaging, pain injections, and, in a
ing in an intelligence unit in Vietnam when her few cases, invasive procedures for pain. As an
jeep struck a land mine, overturning her jeep and example from my practice, a woman with chronic
causing a cerebral concussion. She was applying pain had not received any treatment for an alleged
for VA benefits for an alleged disability. Her injury at work until 20 days had elapsed, when she
service record indicated that she had not served made a single emergency department visit and
in Vietnam, and the EEG monitoring led to a then received no additional treatment for another
diagnosis of nonepileptic seizures. A book has five months. Her work-related disability began
been written about exaggeration of Vietnam War seven months after her alleged injury. Her degree
combat exploits (Burkett & Whitley, 1998). One of disability was disproportionate to the degree of
examinee made grandiose claims about his base- injury and the medical findings. She failed to
ball playing prowess despite an obvious physical provide details to multiple examiners about two
deformity to his throwing arm that he reported separate, similar, financially compensable injuries
occurred in an accident during adolescence. within the past ten years.
Another examinee with cerebral palsy reported Psychotherapy notes of treatment prior to a
that he had been a high school football star. People mild head injury, when they are available, some-
who are untruthful about their past may not be times reveal many more sessions of treatment and
truthful about their symptoms, time of onset, or much more serious psychopathology than
symptoms in relation to a potentially financially reported by the examinee. Such distortions may
352 forensic neuropsychology

be deliberate attempts to maximize the financial Undergraduates who had the opportunity to
recovery from litigation, but they also could result write about past emotional trauma experienced a
from a general tendency to distort past episodes significant drop in visits to the student health
of emotional problems (Coyne et al., 2001). center, even when compared with students who
wrote about the facts of the trauma without
I M PA C T O F D I S T O R T I O N writing about the associated emotions (Smyth,
O F M E D I C A L H I S T O RY 1998). In a similar study of the effect of trauma
Data suggest that people who deny psychological writing, sexual offenders incarcerated for treat-
problems, that is, people whose psychological ment decreased health care visits after a trauma
defenses do not allow them to recognize their writing exercise (Richards, Beal, Seagal, &
problems, are at risk for medical illnesses because Pennebaker, 2000). Writing about emotions led to
their defenses are associated with increased auto- a beneficial effect in short-term measures of
nomic reactivity. Shedler et al. (1993) found that immune system functioning, including circulat-
participants classified in the illusory mental health ing total lymphocytes and CD4 (helper) T lym-
group showed greater coronary maximal and phocyte levels. Thought suppression was harmful
mean reactivity than participants in either the for short-term immune system functioning as
subjectively distressed or genuinely healthy measured by CD3 T lymphocyte levels (Petrie,
groups. Furthermore, in the group with illusory Booth, & Pennebaker, 1998). Difficulty expressing
mental health, the lower the self-rating of psy- feelings was associated with increased somatic
chological problems, the greater the coronary complaints (Kirmayer & Robbins, 1993).
reactivity. Studies summarized in the preceding sections
There is limited experimental evidence that demonstrate that people tend to underreport past
people with medically unexplained disorders also events including stressors, episodes of medical
can be classified as experiencing illusory mental and mental problems, and treatment for those
health. In one study, fibromyalgia patients were problems. Traumatic events, whether encountered
experimentally presented with emotionally pro- during childhood or during adulthood, can cause
vocative images. These patients reacted more serious medical and mental health problems.
physiologically while acknowledging less affective These events also can mediate effects which lead
change than control subjects (Brosschot & Aarsse, to social disruptions and somatic or psychological
2001). In this study the fibromyalgia patients were problems. The medical and mental effects of
both highly anxious and highly defensive, and trauma can be reduced through disclosure and
tended to attribute their physiological responses emotional processing, but disclosure and emo-
to somatic rather than psychological causes. tional processing frequently do not occur. In the
In a series of studies, Pennebaker and col- next section the implications of these findings for
leagues showed that inhibition of thoughts and the assessment of medically unexplained disor-
emotions was associated with increased auto- ders is discussed.
nomic nervous system activity in the short term
and that inhibition of emotions in the long term AS SES SMENT OF
served as a cumulative stressor that increased the M E D I C A L LY U N E X P L A I N E D
probability of psychosomatic disease. Their data S Y M P TO M S
also indicated that not discussing an emotional Conversion disorder in the form of nonepileptic
trauma was a harmful form of inhibition. Adults seizures is an excellent model of a pseudoneuro-
who reported in a questionnaire that they had logical condition often associated with inaccurate
been subjected to childhood trauma or who self-report of psychiatric symptoms and history.
suffered recent trauma were more likely to have Intensive EEG monitoring and neuroimaging
suffered health problems within the past year data rule out or make unlikely a diagnosis of
(Pennebaker & Susman, 1988). Another survey epilepsy or other neurological explanations for
examined the health effects of a recent, sudden the seizures. Data reviewed above indicate that
death of a spouse. The more respondents talked patients with nonepileptic seizures generally have
about the death of a spouse, the fewer health prob- neuropsychological abnormalities.
lems they suffered in the following year. However, There are two observations about patients
the more they simply ruminated about the death with nonepileptic seizures relevant to the present
without talking about it, the more health prob- discussion. First, even after careful evaluation,
lems they suffered (Pennebaker & Susman, 1988). the majority of these patients do not receive any
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 353

psychiatric diagnosis other than conversion traumas during childhood or adulthood


disorder (Alper, Devinsky, Perrine, Vazquez, & (Staudenmayer, 1996). There also is evidence that
Luciano, 1995). This observation is extraordinary. some people with these disorders both overreact
Despite high levels of disability, despite frequently to stressors and deny their anxiety (Brosschot &
having a poor prognosis (Ettinger, Devinsky, Aarsse, 2001), that is, they lack insight into their
Weisbrot, Ramakrishna, & Goyal, 1999), and current psychological experience.
despite the truly bizarre nature of their symptoms, It is hypothesized that denial and lack of
many of these patients neither demonstrate nor insight about past and present psychological
report any psychopathology other than their stressors and current problems occurs commonly
seizures. The extreme nature of the symptom of in people with medically unexplained disorders
nonepileptic seizures surely indicates underlying (Staudenmayer, 1996). What many people
psychopathology, but this psychopathology often with medically unexplained symptoms do not
cannot be detected because it is not acknowl- know, or what they do not report to health care
edged. Anecdotally, some of these patients deny professionals, are the very things that might be
existing, clear-cut psychiatric history, including hurting them. This hypothesis is consistent with
hospitalizations and suicide attempts. the evidence reviewed above and is testable
Second, consistent with the fact that some of through large scale, longitudinal investigations.
these patients receive a psychiatric diagnosis only For example, people with documented history of
of conversion disorder, the mean MMPI-2 profiles childhood abuse (Widom & Morris, 1997) or
of this population show a Conversion V (Binder other social upheavals (Henry et al., 1994) could
et al., 1994; Brown et al., 1991; Wilkus et al., 1984). be assessed for relatively common conditions such
The highest MMPI-2 elevations in the mean pro- as fibromyalgia and chronic fatigue. It is hypoth-
file, by a wide margin, are on scales HS and HY. esized that participants subjected to childhood
The mean profile is less severely elevated on scales traumas will suffer a higher lifetime prevalence of
measuring symptoms of depression and anxiety. medically unexplained disorders, especially if
The conventional interpretation of a MMPI-2 they do not disclose or have not emotionally
Conversion V profile (Greene, 2000) is that the processed the childhood traumatic events that
person uses physical symptoms as a form of psy- were reported earlier. The psychological or psy-
chological defense against the experience and chophysiological pathogen may be less severe
source of psychological distress. Graham (1990, p. than a history of childhood sexual or physical
90) described this profile as indicating persons abuse, for example, a neurophysiological predis-
who “present themselves as normal, responsible, position towards intolerance of ordinary stressors,
and without fault. . . . They prefer medical expla- a trait that can be measured (Barlow, 2000;
nations for their symptoms, and they lack insight Davidson, 2000). People with this trait could be
into psychological factors underlying their prob- followed longitudinally and their prevalence of
lems . . . they do not show appropriate concern medically unexplained symptoms could be com-
about their symptoms and problems.” pared with the prevalence in people without a
It is likely that the lessons of nonepileptic sei- high degree of neurophysiological reactivity to
zure patients with conversion disorders apply, at ordinary stressors.
least in some cases, to other types of medically Patients with medically unexplained disorders
unexplained disorders. Some studies of people also are heterogeneous with respect to reports of
with fibromyalgia and CFS and other similar con- emotional problems and often report depression
ditions show increased rate of psychiatric prob- or anxiety (Rief & Broadbent, 2007), with similar
lems, especially anxiety and mood disorders, prior findings among patients with psychogenic
to the onset of their illness (Black, 2000; Walker, seizures (Cragar, Berry, Fakhoury, et al., 2002).
Keegan, Gardner, Sullivan, Katon, & Bernstein, Patients with emotional complaints can be
1997). Furthermore, many people with medically expected to be more likely to seek mental health
unexplained somatic symptoms concurrently treatment.
have psychiatric complaints (Rief & Broadbent,
2007). However, many people with these condi- I M P L I C AT I O N S F O R
tions do not report either mental health treatment FORENSIC PRACTICE
or significant episodes of psychiatric disorder Mental health professionals, including neuro-
prior to the onset of their illnesses, and they do psychologists, can perform the most accurate
not necessarily report unusual psychological assessment when they have access to mental
354 forensic neuropsychology

health and medical records from the past performance (Binder, Iverson, & Brooks, 2009).
(Coyne et al., 2001). Denial of past mental illness In a neuropsychological test battery, the average
may not be accurate. Moreover, the life events that normal person will have a few abnormal scores.
may have been most damaging to the mental Low scores also are caused by developmental learn-
health of the patient being examined may be the ing disabilities (O’Donnell, Kurtz, & Ramanaiah,
very events not reported to the clinician, despite 1983; Selz & Reitan, 1979), or by attention deficit
careful inquiry. Records sometimes reveal that the disorder (Fischer, Barkley, Edelbrock, & Smallish,
same stressful life events that were denied to 1990). Neuropsychological impairments are asso-
the current examiner were acknowledged to pre- ciated with certain personality disorders, depres-
vious examiners. Anecdotal evidence suggests sion, substance abuse (Gonzalez, Vassileva, &
that childhood traumatic events sometimes are Scott, 2009; Rourke & Grant, 2009), and other psy-
initially denied and later acknowledged to the chiatric disorders (Johnson & Magaro, 1987; Judd
same clinician. Asking about a history of psycho- & Ruff, 1993; O’Leary, Brouwers, Gardner, &
logical traumas in multiple ways or waiting Cowdry, 1991). As reviewed elsewhere in this
until the examinee is more at ease may increase volume, neuropsychological testing must include
the probability that the patient will provide accu- assessment of effort because suboptimal motiva-
rate information. People with medically unex- tion in some samples of patients with medically
plained symptoms should be asked about history unexplained symptoms is common (Gervais et al.,
of possible childhood and adulthood stressors 2001; Suhr, 2003), including some cases with no
and traumas, according to the International identified external incentives (Kemp, Coughlan,
Consensus Group on Depression and Anxiety Rowbottom, et al., 2008).
(Ballenger, Davidson, Lecrubier, et al., 2004). Assuming that cognitive abnormalities
Although actual childhood abuse may be denied resulted from a valid examination of mental abili-
in adulthood (Widom & Morris, 1997), such his- ties, the neuropsychologist can assist the patient
tory may not be obtained unless the clinician asks. in various ways, such as documenting the find-
History of childhood abuse is not inevitably asso- ings, measuring the extent of the cognitive prob-
ciated with medically unexplained symptoms, lem, suggesting means of coping with the deficits,
however (Raphael, Widom, & Lange, 2001). The and recommending and implementing therapy.
effect of childhood trauma on adult mental and However, when evaluating patients with medi-
physical health is variable. Stressors in adulthood cally unexplained disorders such as fibromyalgia,
may be poorly tolerated in people with history of MCS, toxic mold syndrome, or chronic fatigue
adverse childhood experiences only if they also syndrome, it should not be assumed that the
have a history of more recent adult stressors presence of cognitive abnormalities signifies the
(Stewart, Petrie, Balfour, et al., 2004). presence of a traditionally defined neurological
Studies reviewed here also have implications disease affecting the brain.
specifically for neuropsychological practice. Many A history of medically unexplained symptoms
people with these disorders have mild neuropsy- or illness provides an explanation for mild
chological abnormalities (Binder et al., 2001; cognitive deficits that may be more compelling
DiPino & Kane, 1996; Grace et al., 1999, Hart than a potentially compensable condition such
et al., 2000; Tiersky et al., 1997) that do not neces- as mild head injury or alleged toxic exposure.
sarily signify the presence of neurological disease, The cognitive deficits in information processing
at least not the way neurological disease currently speed, attention, and memory associated with
is defined. Evidence reviewed above clearly indi- illnesses such as chronic fatigue and fibromyalgia
cates that brain chemistry can be altered by psy- are strikingly similar to the deficits that some
chological traumas. There also is some support for authors ascribe to late post-concussive syndrome
the hypothesis that brain structures may be altered (Larrabee, this volume) and neurotoxicity (Bolla,
by psychological traumas. These findings blur the this volume).
distinctions between “organic” and “psychiatric” Sensible treatment recommendations can
illnesses. be made despite some uncertainty about the
Cognitive abnormalities are nonspecific. possible multifactorial etiology of these condi-
Their presence does not necessarily signify the tions. Cognitive behavioral therapy, sometimes in
presence of neurological disease or injury (Bolla, combination with exercise, has been effective for
2000). Cognitive abnormalities may be caused fibromyalgia (Burckhardt, 2002), chronic fatigue
by the considerable natural variability in human syndrome (Afari & Buchwald, 2003; Friedberg &
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 355

Jason, 2001; Raine, Haines, Sensky, et al., 2002), Alper, K., Devinsky, O., Perrine, K., Vazquez, B., &
multiple chemical sensitivities (Staudenmayer, Luciano, D. (1995). Psychiatric classification of
1996), Gulf War illnesses (Donta, Clauw, Engel, nonconversion nonepileptic seizures. Archives of
et al., 2003), and health anxiety (Taylor & Neurology, 52, 199–201.
Asmundson, 2004). Clinicians should recom- American Academy of Allergy and Immunology
mend exercise in some form. Clearly, negative (1986). Position statement: Clinical ecology.
expectancies play a role for many people with Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 78,
these ill-defined conditions (Dalton, 1999; Deary 269–71.
American College of Physicians (1989). Clinical
et al., 2007; Moss-Morris et al., 1995; Taylor &
ecology. Annals of Internal Medicine, 111, 168–78.
Asmundson, 2004). The classically conditioned
Andreski, P., Chilcoat, H., & Breslau, N. (1998). Post-
responses of multiple chemical sensitivities may
traumatic stress disorder and somatization symp-
be treated with extinction, that is, repeated toms: A prospective study. Psychiatry Research, 79,
presentation of the odor without illness (Van den 131–38.
Bergh et al., 2001). This behavior therapy proba- Annegers, J. F., Hauser, W. A., Coan, S. P., Rocca, W. A.
bly requires medical input. (1998). A population-based study of seizures after
The presence of a medically unexplained traumatic brain injuries. New England Journal of
disorder, coupled with subjective cognitive Medicine, 338, 20–24.
complaints, provides a rationale for a neuro- Babiloni, C., Visser, P. J., Frisoni, G., De Deyn, P. P.,
psychological evaluation. The evaluation should Bresciani, L., Jelic, V., et al. (2008). Cortical sources
include an in-depth psychological assessment. of resting EEG rhythms in mild cognitive impair-
The not-always-attainable goal of such an ment and subjective memory complaint. Neurobiology
assessment is to provide a psychological explana- of Aging. 2008 Nov 20. [Epub ahead of print]
tion for the physical symptoms, and to determine Baldo, J. V., Ahmad, L., & Ruff, R. (2002). Neuro-
what treatment might be helpful. Among the psychological performance of patients following
psychological assessment questions that should mold exposure. Applied Neuropsychology, 9, 193–
be answered, whenever possible, are the follow- 202.
ing: Is a mood disorder present? Is an anxiety Ballenger, J. C., Davidson, J. R. T., Lecrubier, Y., Nutt,
disorder present? Has there been sufficient D. J., Marshall, R. D., Nemeroff, C. B., et al. (2004).
emotional trauma to cause PTSD or post- Consensus statement update on posttraumatic
traumatic stress symptoms? Is post-traumatic stress disorder from the International Consensus
Group on Depression and Anxiety. Journal of
stress present? Is there secondary gain or illness
Clinical Psychiatry, 65, suppl. 1, 55–62.
behavior in the form of financial incentives or
Bandura, A. & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-
social/familial attention? If one of these condi-
efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of
tions is identified, how should it be treated? Applied Psychology, 88, 87–99.
Psychological problems in the absence of clear Barlow, D. H. (2000). Unraveling the mysteries of anxi-
physical disease or injury are often the most par- ety and its disorders from the perspective of emo-
simonious explanations for neuropsychological tion theory. American Psychologist, 55, 1247–63.
symptoms. Barry, E., Krumholz, A., Bergey, G. K., Chatha, H.,
Alemayehu, S., & Grattan, L. (1998). Nonepileptic
posttraumatic seizures. Epilepsia, 39, 427–31.
REFERENCES Barsky, A. J. & Borus, J. F. (1999). Functional somatic syn-
Abbey, S. E. & Garfinkel, P. E. (1991). Neurasthenia dromes. Annals of Internal Medicine, 130, 910–21.
and chronic fatigue syndrome: The role of culture Beckham, J., Moore, S., Feldman, M., Hertzberg, M.,
in the making of a diagnosis. American Journal of Kirby, A., & Fairbank, J. A., (1998). Health status,
Psychiatry, 148, 1638–46. somatization and severity of posttraumatic stress
Afari, N. & Buchwald, D. (2003). Chronic fatigue syn- disorder in Vietnam combat veterans with post-
drome: A review. American Journal of Psychiatry, traumatic stress disorder. American Journal of
160, 221–36. Psychiatry, 155, 1565–69.
Albers, J. W. & Berent, S. (2000). Controversies in neu- Bell, I. R., Baldwin, C. M., & Schwartz, G. E. (1998).
rotoxicology. Current status. Neurological Clinics, Illness from low-level environmental chemicals:
18, 741–63. Relevance to chronic fatigue syndrome and fibro-
Alper, K., Devinsky, O., Perrine, K., Vazquez, B., & myalgia. American Journal of Medicine, 105
Luciano, D. (1993). Nonepileptic seizures and (Supp. 3A), 74S–82S.
childhood sexual and physical abuse. Neurology, Bennett, R. M. (1999). Emerging concepts in the neu-
43, 1950–53. robiology of chronic pain: Evidence of abnormal
356 forensic neuropsychology

sensory processing in fibromyalgia. Mayo Clinical Implications for research and clinical surveillance.
Proceedings, 74, 385–98. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 196,
Binder, L. M. (1997). A review of mild head trauma. 100–107.
Part II: Clinical implications. Journal of Clinical Bremner, J. D., Randall, P., Scott, T. M., Bronen, R. A.,
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 432–57. Seibyl, J. P., Southwick, S. M., et al. (1995). MRI-based
Binder, L. M. & Campbell, K. A. (2004). Medically measurement of hippocampal volume in patients
unexplained symptoms and neuropsychological with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.
assessment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 973–81.
Neuropsychology, 26, 369–92. Brosschot, J. F. & Aarsse, H. R. (2001). Restricted emo-
Binder, L. M., Iverson, G. L., & Brooks, B. L. (2009). To tional processing and somatic attribution in fibro-
err is human: “Abnormal” neuropsychological myalgia. International Journal of Psychiatric
scores and variability are common in healthy adults. Medicine, 31, 127–46.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 31–46. Brown, M. C., Levin, B. E., Ramsay, R. E., Katz, D. A.,
Binder, L. M., Kindermann, S. S., Heaton, R. K., & & Duchowny, M. S. (1991). Characteristics of
Salinsky, M. C. (1998). Neuropsychologic impair- patients with nonepileptic seizures. Journal of
ment in patients with nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy, 4, 225–29.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, 513–22. Brown-DeGagne, A. & McGlone, J. (1999). Multiple
Binder, L. M., Salinsky, M. C., & Smith, S. P. (1994). chemical sensitivity: A test of the olfactory-limbic
Psychological correlates of psychogenic seizures. model. Journal of Occupational and Environmental
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro- Medicine, 41, 366–77.
psychology, 16, 524–30. Buchwald, D. & Garrity, D. (1994). Comparison of
Binder, L. M., Storzbach, D., Anger, W. K., Campbell, patients with chronic fatigue syndrome, fibromyal-
K. A., Rohlman, D. S., & members of the Portland gia, and multiple chemical sensitivities. Archives of
Environmental Hazards Research Center. (1999). Internal Medicine, 154, 2049–53.
Subjective cognitive complaints, affective distress, Burckhardt, C. S. (2002). Nonpharmacologic manage-
and objective cognitive performance in Persian ment strategies in fibromyalgia. Rheumatologic
Gulf War veterans. Archives of Clinical Neuro- Diseases Clinics of North America, 28, 291–304.
psychology, 14, 531–36. Burge, H. A. (2001). Fungi: toxic killers or unavoidable
Binder, L. M., Storzbach, D., Campbell, K. A., Rohlman, nuisances? Annals of Allergy, Asthma, and
D. S., Anger, W. K., & members of the Portland Immunology, 87, (6 Supplement 3), 52–56.
Environmental Hazards Research Center. (2001). Burkett, B. G. & Whitley, G. (1998). Stolen valor: How
Neurobehavioral deficits associated with chronic the Vietnam generation was robbed of its heroes and
fatigue syndrome in veterans with Gulf War unex- its history. Dallas: Verity Press.
plained illnesses. Journal of the International Burt, D. B., Zembar, M. J., & Niederehe, G. (1995).
Neuropsychological Society, 7, 835–39. Depression and memory impairment: A meta-
Binder, L. M., Storzbach, D., & Salinsky, M. (2006). analysis of the association, its pattern, and specific-
MMPI-2 profiles of persons with multiple chemi- ity. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 285–305.
cal sensitivities. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, Butler, J. A., Chalder, T., & Wessely, S. (2001). Causal
848–57. attributions for somatic sensations in patients with
Black, D. W. (1996). Iatrogenic (physician-induced) chronic fatigue syndrome and their partners.
hypochondriasis: Four patient examples of Psychological Medicine, 31, 97–105.
“chemical sensitivity.” Psychosomatics, 37, 390–93. Caccappolo-van Vliet, E., Kelly-McNeil, K., Natelson,
Black, D. W. (2000). The relationship of mental disor- B., Kipen, H., & Fiedler, N. (2002). Anxiety sensi-
ders and idiopathic environmental intolerance. tivity and depression in multiple chemical sensi-
Occupational Medicine, 15, 557–70. tivities and asthma. Journal of Occupational and
Bohr, T. (1996). Problems with myofascial pain syn- Environmental Medicine, 44, 890–901.
drome and fibromyalgia syndrome. Neurology, 45, Cardena, E. & Spiegel, D. (1993). Dissociative reactions
593–97. to the San Francisco bay area earthquake of 1989.
Bolla, K. I. (2000). Use of neuropsychological testing in American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 474–78.
idiopathic environmental testing. Occupational Carragee, E. J. (2008). Validity of self-reported history
Medicine State of the Art Reviews, 15, 617–24. in patients with acute back or neck pain after
Boscarino, J. A. (1997). Diseases among men 20 years motor vehicle accidents. Spine Journal, 8, 311–319.
after exposure to severe stress: Implications for Cassens, G., Wolfe, L., & Zola, M. (1990). The neuro-
clinical research and medical care. Psychosomatic psychology of depressions. Journal of Neuro-
Medicine, 59, 605–614. psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 2, 202–13.
Boscarino, J. A. (2008). Psychobiologic predictors Clauw, D. J., Engel, C. C. Jr., Aronowitz, R., Jones, E.,
of disease mortality after psychological trauma. Kipen, H. M., Kroenke, K., et al. (2003).
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 357

Unexplained symptoms after terrorism and war: symptoms: A theoretical and empirical review.
An expert consensus statement. Journal of Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 781–97.
Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 45, Delis, D. C. & Wetter, S. R. (2007). Cogniform disorder
1040–48. and cogniform condition: Proposed diagnoses for
Claypoole, K., Mahurin, R., Fischer, M. E., Goldberg, excessive cognitive symptoms. Archives of Clinical
J., Schmaling, K. B., Schoene, R. B., et al. (2001). Neuropsychology, 22, 589–604.
Cognitive compromise following exercise in DeLuca, J., Johnson, S. K., Ellis, S. P., & Natelson, B. H.
monozygotic twins discordant for chronic fatigue (1997). Cognitive functioning is impaired in
syndrome: Fact or artifact? Applied Neuropsy- patients with chronic fatigue syndrome devoid of
chology, 8, 31–40. psychiatric disease. Journal of Neurology, Neuro-
Claypoole, K. H., Noonan, C., Mahurin, R. K., surgery, and Psychiatry, 62, 151–55.
Goldberg, J., Erickson, T., & Buchwald D. (2007). A DiPino, R. K. & Kane, R. L. (1996). Neurocognitive
twin study of cognitive function in chronic fatigue functioning in chronic fatigue syndrome.
syndrome: The effects of sudden illness onset. Neuropsychology Review, 6, 47–60.
Neuropsychology, 21, 507–13. Dodrill, C. B., Wilkus, R. J., & Batzel, L. W. (1993). The
Couprie, W., Wijdicks, E. F., Rooijmans, H. G., & van MMPI as a diagnostic tool in nonepileptic seizures.
Gijn, J. (1995). Outcome in conversion disorder: A In A. J. Rowan & J. R. Gates (Eds.), Nonepileptic
follow-up study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, seizures (pp. 211–219). Boston: Butterworth-
and Psychiatry, 58, 750–52. Heinemann.
Coyne, J. C., Thompson, R., & Racioppo, M. W. (2001). Don, A. S. & Carragee, E. J. (2009). Is the self-reported
Validity and efficiency of screening for history of history accurate in patients with persistent axial
depression by self-report. Psychological Assessment, pain after a motor vehicle accident? Spine Journal,
13, 163–70. 9, 4–12.
Cragar, D. E., Berry, D. T. R., Fakhoury, T. A., Cibula, J. Donta, S. T., Clauw, D. J., Engel, C. C., et al. (2003).
E., & Schmitt, F. A. (2002). A review of diagnostic Cognitive behavioral therapy and aerobic exercise
techniques in the differential diagnosis of epileptic for Gulf War veterans’ illnesses: A randomized
and nonepileptic seizures. Neuropsychology Review, controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical
12, 31–64. Association, 289, 1396–1404.
Cragar, D. E., Berry, D. T. R., Fakhoury, T. A., Cibula, J. Doty R. L., Deems, D. A., Frye, R. E., Pelberg, R., &
E., & Schmitt, F. A. (2006). Performance of patients Shapiro, A. (1988). Olfactory sensitivity, nasal
with epileptic or psychogenic nonepileptic seizures resistance, and autonomic function in patients
on four measures of effort. The Clinical Neuro- with multiple chemical sensitivities. Archives of
psychologist, 20, 552–66. Otolaryngology and Head and Neck Surgery, 114,
Crimlisk, H. L., Bhatia, K., Cope, H., David, A., 1422–27.
Marsden, C. D., & Ron, M. A. (1998). Slater revis- Dux, M. C., Woodard, J. L., Calamari, J. E., Messina, M.,
ited: 6-year follow-up study of patients with medi- Arora, S., Chik, H., et al. (2008). The moderating
cally unexplained motor symptoms. British Journal role of negative affect on objective verbal memory
of Medicine, 316, 582–86. performance and subjective memory complaints in
Dalton, P. (1999). Cognitive influences on health symp- healthy older adults. Journal of the International
toms from acute chemical exposure. Health Neuropsychological Society, 14, 327–36.
Psychology, 18, 579–90. Elder, G. H., Shanahan, M. J., & Clipp, E. C. (1997).
Dalton, P. & Hummel, T. (2000). Chemosensory func- Linking combat and physical health: The legacy of
tion and response in idiopathic environmental World War II in men’s lives. American Journal of
intolerance. Occupational Medicine State of the Art Psychiatry, 154, 330–36.
Reviews, 15, 539–56. Engel, C. C. Jr., Adkins, J. A., & Cowan, D. N. (2002).
Das-Munshi, J., Rubin, G. J., & Wessely, S. (2006). Caring for medically unexplained physical symp-
Multiple chemical sensitivities: A systematic review toms after toxic environmental exposures: Effects
of provocation studies. Journal of Allergy and of contested causation. Environmental Health
Clinical Immunology, 118, 1257–64. Perspectives, 110, Suppl. 4, 641–47.
Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathol- Erly, W. K., Labadie, E., Williams, P. L., Lee, D.M.,
ogy, and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plastic- Carmody, R. F., & Seeger, J. F. (1999). Disseminated
ity. American Psychologist, 55, 1196–1214. coccidioidomycosis complicated by vasculitis:
Deary, I. J. (1999). A taxonomy of medically unex- A cause of fatal subarachnoid hemorrhage in two
plained symptoms. Journal of Psychosomatic cases. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 20,
Research, 47, 51–59. 605–608.
Deary, V., Chalder, T., & Sharpe, M. (2007). The cogni- Escobar, J. I., Canino, G., Rubio-Stipec, M., &
tive behavioural model of medically unexplained Bravo, M. (1992). Somatic symptoms after a
358 forensic neuropsychology

natural disaster: A prospective study. American Fukuda, K., Straus, S. E., Hickie, I., Sharpe, M. C.,
Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 965–67. Dobbins, J. G., Komaroff, A., & the International
Ettinger, A. B., Devinsky, O., Weisbrot, D. M., Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group (1994).
Ramakrishna, R. K., & Goyal, A. (1999). A com- The chronic fatigue syndrome: A comprehensive
prehensive profile of clinical, psychiatric, and psy- approach to its definition and study. Annals of
chosocial characteristics of patients with Internal Medicine, 121, 953–59.
psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia, 40, Gabriel, S. E., O’Fallon, W. M., Kurland, L. T., Beard, C.
1292–98. M., Woods, J. E., & Melton, L. J. (1994). Risk of
Ferguson, E. & Cassaday, H. J. (2001–2002). Theoretical connective-tissue diseases and other disorders
accounts of Gulf War syndrome: From environmen- after breast implantation. The New England Journal
tal toxins to psychoneuroimmunology and neuro- of Medicine, 330, 1697–749.
degeneration. Behavioral Neurology, 13, 133–47. Gervais, R. O., Russell, A. S., Green, P., Allen L. M.,
Ferguson, J. H. (1997). Silicone breast implants and Ferrari, R., & Pieschl, S. D. (2001). Effort testing in
neurological disorders. Report of the Practice patients with fibromyalgia and disability incen-
Committee of the American Academy of Neuro- tives. Journal of Rheumatology, 28, 1892–99.
logy. Neurology, 48, 1504–1507. Gonzalez, R., Vassileva, J., & Scott, J. C. (2009).
Ferguson, R. J., Mittenberg, W., Barone, D. F., & Neuropsychological consequences of drug abuse.
Schneider, B. (1999). Post-concussion syndrome Neuropsychology of depression and related mood
following sports-related head injury: Expectation disorders. In I. Grant & K. M. Adams (Eds.).
as etiology. Neuropsychology, 13, 582–89. Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric
Fiedler, N., Giardino, N., Natelson, B., Ottenweller, J. and neuromedical disorders. 3rd edition. (pp. 455–
E., Weisel, C., Lioy, P., et al. (2004). Responses to 79). New York: Oxford University Press.
controlled diesel vapor exposure among chemi- Gordon, W. A., Johanning, E., & Haddad. L. (1999).
cally sensitive Gulf War veterans. Psychosomatic Cognitive impairment associated with exposure to
Medicine, 66, 588–98. toxigenic fungi. In E. Johanning (Ed.) Bioaerosols,
Fiedler, N., Laumbach, R., Kelly-McNeil, K., Lioy, P., fungi, and mycotoxins: Health effects, assessment,
Fan, Z. H., Zhang, J., et al. (2005). Health effects of prevention, and control (pp. 94–98). New York:
a mixture of indoor air volatile organics, their Eastern New York Occupational and Environ-
ozone oxidation products, and stress. Environ- mental Health.
mental Health Perspectives, 113, 1542–48. Gorman, J. M., Kent, J. M., Sullivan, G. M., & Coplan,
Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Edelbrock, C. S., & Smallish, J. D. (2000). Neuroanatomical hypothesis of panic
L. (1990). The adolescent outcome of hyperactive disorder, revised. American Journal of Psychiatry,
children diagnosed by research criteria: II. 157, 493–505.
Academic, attentional, and neuropsychological Gouvier, W. D., Cubic, B., Jones, G., Brantley, P., &
status. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psycho- Cutlip, Q. (1992). Post-concussion symptoms and
logy, 58, 580–88. daily stress in normal and head-injured college
Fischler, B., D’Haenen, H., Cluydts, R., Michiels, V., populations. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
Demets, K., Bossuyt, A., et al. (1996). Comparison 7, 193–211.
of 99m Tc HMPAO SPECT scan between chronic Gouvier, W. D., Uddo-Crane, M., & Brown, L. M.
fatigue syndrome, major depression and healthy (1988). Base rates of post-concussional symptoms.
controls: An exploratory study of clinical correlates Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 273–78.
of regional cerebral blood flow. Neuropsycho- Grace, G. M., Nielson, W. R., Hopkins, M., & Berg, M.
biology, 34, 175–83. A. (1999). Concentration and memory deficits in
Ford, C. V. (1997). Somatization and fashionable patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Journal of
diagnoses: Illness as a way of life. Scandinavian Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21,
Journal of Work and Environmental Health, 23, 477–87.
Supp 3, 7–16. Graham, J. R. (1990). MMPI-2. Assessing personality
Fox, D. D., Lees-Haley, P. R., Earnest, K., & Dolezal- and psychopathology. New York: Oxford University
Wood, S. (1995). Base rates of post-concussive Press.
symptoms in health maintenance organization Greene, R. L. (2000). The MMPI-2. An interpretive
patients and controls. Neuropsychology, 9, manual (2nd Ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
606–611. Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Johnson-Greene, D.
Friedberg, F. & Jason, L. A. (2001). Chronic fatigue (2002). Actual versus self-reported scholastic
syndrome and fibromyalgia: Clinical assessment achievement of litigating post-concussion and
and treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57, severe closed head injury claimants. Psychological
433–55. Assessment, 14, 202–208.
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 359

Grossman, R., Buchsbaum, M. S., & Yehuda, R. (2002). neuropsychological symptomatology. Journal of
Neuroimaging studies in post-traumatic stress Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20,
disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 25, 328–38.
317–40. Hodgson, M. (2000). Sick building syndrome.
Gunstad, J. & Suhr, J. A. (2001). “Expectation as etiol- Occupational Medicine State of the Art Reviews, 15,
ogy” versus “the good old days”: Post-concussion 617–24.
syndrome symptom reporting in athletes, head- Hodgson, M. F., Morey, P., Leung, W., Morrow, L.,
ache sufferers, and depressed individuals. Journal Miler, D., Jarvis, B. B., et al. (1998). Building-
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, associated pulmonary disease from exposure to
323–33. stachybotrys chartarum and aspergillus versicolor.
Hadler, N. M. (1997). Fibromyalgia: La maladie est Journal of Occupational and Environmental
morte. Vive le malade! Journal of Rheumatology, Medicine, 40, 241–48.
24, 1250–51. Hoge, C. W., Terkahopian, A., Castro, C. A., Messer, S.
Hadler, N. M. (1999). Occupational musculoskeletal C., & Engel, C. C. (2007). Association of posttrau-
disorders, 2nd edition. Philadelphia: Lippincott, matic stress disorder with somatic symptoms,
Williams, & Wilkins. health care visits, and absenteeism among Iraqi
Hakala, M., Karlsson, H., Ruotsalainen, U., Koponen, War veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164,
S., Bergman, J., Stenman, H., et al. (2002). Severe 150–53.
somatization in women is associated with altered Hoppe, C., Elger, C. E., & Helmstaedter, C. (2007).
cerebral glucose metabolism. Psychological Long-term memory impairment in patients with
Medicine, 32, 1379–85. focal epilepsy. Epilepsia, 48 Suppl. 9, 26–29.
Hardin, B. D., Kelman, B. J., & Saxon, A. (2003). Hotopf, M., Mayou, R., Wadsworth, M., & Wessely, S.
Adverse human health effects associated with (1999). Childhood risk factors for adults with
molds in the indoor environment. Journal of medically unexplained symptoms: Results from a
Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 45, national birth cohort study. American Journal of
470–78. Psychiatry, 156, 1796–1800.
Harlow, S. D. & Linet, M. S. (1989). Agreement between Hyams, K. C., Wignall, F. S., & Roswell, R. (1996). War
questionnaire data and medical records. The evi- syndromes and their evaluation: From the U.S.
dence for accuracy of recall. American Journal of Civil War to the Persian Gulf War. Annals of
Epidemiology, 129, 233–48. Internal Medicine, 125, 398–405.
Hart, R. P., Martelli, M. F., & Zasler, N. D. (2000). Ichise, M., Salit, I. E., Abbey, S. E., Chung, D. G., Gray,
Chronic pain and neuropsychological functioning. B., Kirsh, J. C., et al. (1992). Assessment of regional
Neuropsychology Review, 20, 131–49. cerebral perfusion by 99Tcm-HMPAO SPECT in
Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., Matthews, C. G. (1991). chronic fatigue syndrome. Nuclear Medicine
Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead- Communications, 13, 767–72.
Reitan Battery. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Iverson, G. L., King, R. J., Scott, J. G., & Adams, R. L.
Resources, Inc. (2001). Cognitive complaints in litigating patients
Hennekens, C. H., Lee, I. M., Cook, N. R., Hebert, P. R., with head injuries or chronic pain. Journal of
Karlson, E. W., LaMotte, F., et al. (1996). Self- Forensic Neuropsychology, 2, 19–30.
reported breast implants and connective-tissue Iverson, G. L. (2006). Misdiagnosis of the persistent
diseases in female health professionals: A retro- postconcussion syndrome in patients with depres-
spective cohort study. Journal of the American sion. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21,
Medical Association, 275, 616–21. 303–10.
Henningsen, P., Zimmermann, T., & Sattel, H. (2003). Iverson, G. L. & Lange, R. T. (2003). Examination of
Medically unexplained physical symptoms, anxi- “postconcussion-like” symptoms in a healthy
ety, and depression: A meta-analytic review. sample. Applied Neuropsychology, 10, 137–44.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 528–33. Jason, L. A., Richman, J. A., Friedberg, F., Wagner, L.,
Henry, B., Moffit, T. E., Caspi, A., Langley, J., & Silva, P. Taylor, R., & Jordan, K. M. (1997). Politics, science,
A. (1994). On the “Remembrance of Things Past”; and the emergence of a new disease: The case of
a longitudinal evaluation of the retrospective chronic fatigue syndrome. American Psychologist,
method. Psychological Assessment, 6, 92–101. 52, 973–83.
Herbert, T. B. & Cohen, S. (1993). Depression and Jason, L. A., Richman, J. A., Rademaker, A.W.,
immunity: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Jordan, K. M., Plioplys, A.V., Taylor, R.R., et al.
Bulletin, 113, 472–86. (1999). A community-based study of chronic
Hilsabeck, R. C., Gouvier, W. D., & Bolter, J. F. fatigue syndrome. Archives of Internal Medicine,
(1998). Reconstructive memory bias in recall of 159, 2129–37.
360 forensic neuropsychology

Johnson, M. H. & Magaro, P. A. (1987). Effects of mood Kwiatek, R., Barnden, L., Tedman, R., Jarrett, R., Chew,
and severity on memory processes in depression J., Rowe, C., et al. (2000). Regional cerebral blood
and mania. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 28–40. flow in fibromyalgia: Single-photon-emission
Johnson-Greene, D. & Binder, L. M. (1995). Evaluation computed tomography evidence of reduction in
of an efficient method for verifying higher educa- the pontine tegmentum and thalami. Arthritis and
tion credentials. Archives of Clinical Neuro- Rheumatology, 43, 2823–33.
psychology, 10, 251–53. Kuhn, D. M. & Ghannoum, M. A. (2003). Indoor mold,
Judd, P. H. & Ruff, R. M. (1993). Neuropsychological toxigenic fungi, and Stachybotrys chartarum:
dysfunction in borderline personality disorder. Infectious disease perspective. Clinical Microbiology
Journal of Personality Disorders, 7, 275–84. Review, 16, 144–72.
Katon, W. & Walker, E. A. (1993). The relationship Labarge, A. S. & McCaffrey, R. J. (2000). Multiple
of chronic fatigue to psychiatric illness in chemical sensitivity: A review of the theoretical
community, primary care and tertiary care and research literature. Neuropsychology Review,
samples. In G. R. Bock & J. Whelan (Eds.), Chronic 10, 183–211.
fatigue syndrome (pp. 193–211). Chichester: John LaManca, J. J., Sisto, S. A., DeLuca, J., Johnson, S. K.,
Wiley & Sons. Lange, G., Pareja, J., et al. (1998). Influence of
Kawamura, N., Kim, Y., & Asukai, N. (2001). exhaustive treadmill exercise on cognitive func-
Suppression of cellular immunity in men with a tioning in chronic fatigue syndrome. American
past history of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Medicine, 105, 59S–65S.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 484–86. Lang. P. J. & Cuthbert, B. N. (1984). Affective informa-
Kearney-Schwartz, A., Rossignol, P., Bracard, S., tion processing and the assessment of anxiety.
Felblinger, J., Fay, R., Boivin, J. M., et al. (2009). Journal of Behavior Assessment, 6, 369–95.
Vascular structure and function is correlated to Lange, G., Wang, S., DeLuca, J., & Natelson, B. H.
cognitive performance and white matter hyperin- (1998). Neuroimaging in chronic fatigue syn-
tensities in older hypertensive patients with subjec- drome. American Journal of Medicine, 105 (supp.
tive memory complaints. Stroke. 2009 Feb 26. 3A), 50S–3S.
[Epub ahead of print] Langenecker, S.A., Lee, H.J., & Bieliauskas, L.A. (2009).
Kemp, S., Coughlan, A. K., Rowbottom, C., Wilkinson, Neuropsychology of depression and related mood
K., Teggart, V., & Baker, G. (2008). The base rate disorders. In I. Grant & K.M. Adams (Eds.).
of effort test failure in patients with medically Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiat-
unexplained symptoms. Journal of Psychosomatic ric and neuromedical disorders. (3rd edition). New
Research, 65, 319–25. York: Oxford University Press (pp. 523–59).
Kent, D. A., Tomasson, K., & Coryell, W. (1995). Larrabee, G. J. & Levin, H. S. (1986). Memory self-
Course and outcome of conversion and somati- ratings and objective test performance in a normal
zation disorders: A four-year follow-up. Psycho- elderly sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
somatics, 36, 138–44. Neuropsychology, 8, 275–84.
Khalili, B. & Bardana, E. J. (2005). Inhalational mold Leavitt, F. & Katz, R. S. (2008). Speed of mental opera-
toxicity: Fact or fiction? A clinical review of 50 tions in fibromyalgia: A selective naming speed
cases. Annals of Allergy, Asthma, and Immunology, deficit. Journal of Clinical Rheumatology, 14, 214–
95, 239–46. 218.
Kirmayer, L. J. & Robbins, J. M. (1993). Cognitive and Lee, A. L., Ogle, W. O., & Sapolsky, R. M. (2002). Stress
social correlates of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale. and depression: Possible links to neuron death in
Psychosomatics, 34,41–52. the hippocampus. Bipolar Disorders, 4, 117–28.
Kirschbaum, C., Prussner, J. C., Stone, A. A., Federenko, Lees-Haley, P. R. (2003). Toxic mold and mycotoxins
I., Gaab, J., Lintz, D., et al.(1995). Persistent high in neurotoxicity cases: Stachybotrys, Fusarium,
cortisol responses to repeated psychological stress Trichoderma, Aspergillus, Penicillium, Cladospo-
in a subpopulation of healthy men. Psychosomatic rium, Alternaria, Trichothecenes. Psychological
Medicine, 57, 468–74. Reports, 93, 561–84.
Korgeski, G. P. & Leon, G. R. (1983). Correlates of self- Lees-Haley, P. R. (2004). Commentary on
reported and objectively determined exposure to “Neuropsychological performance of patients
Agent Orange. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, following mold exposure.” The Scientific Review
1443–49. of Mental Health Practice, 3, 60–66.
Kroenke, K. & Price, R. K. (1993). Symptoms in the Lees-Haley, P. R. & Brown, R. S. (1993). Neuro-
community. Prevalence, classification, and psychi- psychological complaint base rates of 170 personal
atric comorbidity. Archives of Internal Medicine, injury claimants. Archives of Clinical Neuropsy-
153, 2474–80. chology, 8, 203–209.
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 361

Lees-Haley, P. R., Williams, C. W., & English, L. T. Moss-Morris, R., Petrie, K. J., & Weinman, J. (1995).
(1996). Response bias in in self-reported history of The impact of catastrophic beliefs on functioning
plaintiffs compared with nonlitigating patients. in chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Psycho-
Psychological Reports, 79, 811–18. somatic Research, 39, 31–37.
Leininger, B. E., Kreutzer, J. S., & Hill, M. R. (1991). Nash, F., Cheng, J. S., Meyer, G. A., & Remler, B. F.
Comparison of minor and severe head injury (2002). Chiari type I malformation: Overview of
emotional sequelae using the MMPI. Brain Injury, diagnosis and treatment. Western Medical Journal,
5, 199–205. 101, 35–40.
Lewis, D. H., Mayberg, H. S., Fischer, M. E., Goldberg, Nimnuan, C., Hotopf, M., & Wessely, S. (2000).
J., Ashton, S., Graham, M. M., et al. (2001). Medically unexplained symptoms: How often and
Monozygotic twins discordant for chronic fatigue why are they missed? Quarterly Journal of Medicine,
syndrome: Regional cerebral blood flow SPECT. 93, 21–28
Radiology, 219, 766–73. Nimnuan, C., Hotopf, M., & Wessely, S. (2001).
Mace, C. J. & Trimble, M. R. (1996). Ten-year progno- Medically unexplained symptoms: An epide-
sis of conversion disorder. British Journal of miological study in seven specialties. Journal of
Psychiatry, 169, 282–88. Psychosomatic Research, 51, 361–67.
Mahurin, R. K., Claypoole, K. H., Goldberg, J. H., Novack, T. A., Daniel, M. S., & Long, C. J. (1984).
Arguelles, L., Ashton, S., & Buchwald D. (2004). Factors related to emotional adjustment following
Cognitive processing in monozygotic twins head injury. International Journal of Clinical
discordant for chronic fatigue syndrome. Neuro- Neuropsychology, 6, 139–42
psychology, 18, 232–39. O’Donnell, J. P., Kurtz, J., & Ramanaiah, N. V. (1983).
Matthews, C. G., Shaw, D. J., & Klove, H. (1966). Neuropsychological test findings for normal,
Psychological test performance in neurological and learning-disabled, and brain-damaged young
“pseudo-neurological” subjects. Cortex, 2, 244–53. adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psycho-
McCaffrey, R. J. & Yantz, C. L. (2005). “Cognitive logy, 51, 726–29.
impairment associated with toxigenic fungal Oftedal, G., Straume, A., Johnsson, A., & Stovner, L. J.
exposure:” A critique and critical analysis. Applied (2007). Mobile phone headache: A double blind,
Neuropsychology, 12, 134–37. sham-controlled, provocation study. Cephalgia, 27,
McCaffrey, R. J. & Yantz, C. L. (2007). Cognitive com- 447–55.
plaints in multiple chemical sensitivity and toxic O’Leary, K. M., Brouwers, P., Gardner, D. L., & Cowdry,
mold syndrome. In K. B. Boone (Ed.) Assessment of R. W. (1991). Neuropsychological testing of
feigned cognitive impairment. A neuropsychological patients with borderline personality disorder.
perspective (pp. 384–404). New York: Guilford American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 106–111.
Press. Osterberg, K., Orbaek, P., Karlson, B., Akesson, B., &
McEwen, B. (2002). The end of stress as we know it. Bergendorf, U. (2003). Annoyance and perfor-
Washington, DC: Joseph Henry Press. mance during the experimental chemical challenge
McFarlane, A. C., Atchison, M., Rafalowicz, E., & of subjects with multiple chemical sensitivity.
Papay, P. (1994). Physical symptoms in post- Scandinavian Journal of Work and Environmental
traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychosomatic Health, 29, 40–50.
Research, 38, 715–26. Paniak, C., Reynolds, S., Phillips, K., Toller-Lobe G.,
Mitchell, A. J. (2008). The clinical significance of Melnyk, A., & Nagy, J. (2002). Patient complaints
subjective memory complaints in the diagnosis within 1 month of mild traumatic brain injury:
of mild cognitive impairment and dementia: A A controlled study. Archives of Clinical Neuro-
meta-analysis. International Journal of Geriatric psychology, 17, 319–34.
Psychiatry, 23, 1191–1202. Pennebaker, J. W. & Susman, J. R. (1988). Disclosure of
Mittenberg, W., DiGuilio, D. V., Perrin, S., & Bass, A. E. traumas and psychosomatic processes. Social
(1992). Symptoms following mild head injury: Science in Medicine, 26, 327–32.
Expectation as aetiology. Journal of Neurology, Petrie, K. J., Booth, R. J., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1998).
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 55, 200–204. The immunological effects of thought suppression.
Mittenberg, W., Tremont, G., Zielinski, R. E., Fichera, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
S., & Rayls, K. R. (1996). Cognitive-behavioral 1264–72.
prevention of postconcussion syndrome. Archives Petrie, K. J., Broadbent, E. A., Kley, N., Moss-Morris,
of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, 139–45. R., Horne, R., & Rief, W. (2005). Worries about
Morrison, J. (1989). Childhood sexual histories of modernity predict symptoms complaint after
women with somatization disorder. American environmental pesticide spraying. Psychosomatic
Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 239–41. Medicine, 67, 778–82.
362 forensic neuropsychology

Pryse-Phillips, W. (2003). Companion to clinical Schedule. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
neurology (2nd Ed.). New York: Oxford University 180, 215–22.
Press. Rohling, M. L., Green, P., Allen, L. M., & Iverson, G. L.
Raine, R., Haines, A., Sensky, T., Hutchings, A., (2002). Depressive symptoms and neurocognitive
Larkin, K., & Black, N. (2002). Systematic test scores in patients passing symptom validity
review of mental health interventions for patients tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17,
with chronic somatic symptoms: Can research 205–22.
evidence from secondary care be generalized to Rosenberg, H. J., Rosenberg, S. D., Williamson, P. D., &
primary care? British Medical Journal, 325, (7372), Wolford, G. L. (2000). A comparative study of
1082. trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder preva-
Raphael, K. G., Natelson, B. H., Janal, M. N., & lence in epilepsy patients and psychogenic nonepi-
Nayak, S. (2002). A community-based survey of leptic seizure patients. Epilepsia, 47, 447–52.
fibromyalgia-like pain complaints following the Rourke, S. B. & Grant, I. (2009). The neurobehavioral
World Trade Center terrorist attacks. Pain, 100, correlates of alcoholism. In I. Grant & K. M. Adams
131–39. (Eds.). Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsy-
Raphael, K. G., Widom, C. S., & Lange, G. (2001). chiatric and neuromedical disorders. (3rd edition).
Childhood victimization and pain in adulthood: New York: Oxford University Press (pp. 398–454).
A prospective investigation. Pain, 92, 283–93. Sackeim, H. A., Freeman, J., McElhiney, M., Coleman,
Reichenberg, A., Yirmiya, R., Schuld, A., Kraus, T., E., Prudic, J., & Devanand, D. P. (1992). Effects of
Haack, M., Morag, A., et al. (2001). Cytokine- major depression on estimates of intelligence.
associated emotional and cognitive disturbances Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro-
in humans. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, psychology, 14, 268–88.
445–52. Sanchez-Guerrero, J., Schur, P. H., Sergent, J. S., &
Reid, S., Hotopf, M., Hull, L., Ismail, K., Unwin, C, & Liang, M. H. (1994). Silicone breast implants
Wessely, S. (2001). Multiple chemical sensitivity and rheumatic disease: Clinical, immunologic,
and chronic fatigue syndrome in British Gulf War and epidemiologic studies. Arthritis & Rheumatism,
veterans. American Journal of Epidemiology, 153, 37, 158–68.
604–609. Sapolsky, R. M. (2000). Glucocorticoids and hip-
Reijula, K. & Tuomi, T. (2003). Mycotoxins of asper- pocampal atrophy in neuropsychiatric disorders.
gilli: Exposure and health effects. Frontiers of Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 925–35.
Bioscience, 8, 3232–35. Sawchyn, J. M., Brulot, M. M., & Strauss, E. (2000).
Richards, J. M., Beal, W. E., Seagal, J. D., & Pennebaker, Note on the use of the Post-concussion Syndrome
J. W. (2000). Effects of disclosure of traumatic Checklist. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15,
events on illness behavior among psychiatric 1–8.
prison inmates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Schnurr, P. P. & Spiro, A. A. III (1999). Combat expo-
109, 156–60. sure, PTSD symptoms and health behaviors as pre-
Rief, W. & Broadbent, E. A. (2007). Explaining medi- dictors of physical health in older veterans. Journal
cally unexplained symptoms–models and mecha- of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 353–59.
nisms. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 821–41. Schrag, A., Brown, R. J., & Trimble, M. R. (2004).
Robbins, C. A., Swenson, L. J., Neally, M. L., Gots, R. E., Reliability of self-reported diagnoses in patients
& Kelman, B. J. (2000). Health effects of mycotoxins with neurologically unexplained symptoms.
in indoor air: A critical review. Applied Occupational Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
and Environmental Hygiene, 15, 773–84. 75, 608–11.
Robins, L. N., Schoenberg, S. P., Holmes, S. J., Ratcliff, Schwartz, R. B., Garada, B. M., Komaroff, A. L., Tice,
K. S., Benham, A., & Works, J. (1985). Early home H. M., Gleit, M., Jolexa, F. A., et al. (1994). Detection
environment and retrospective recall: Test for of intracranial abnormalities in patients with
concordance between siblings with and without chronic fatigue syndrome: Comparison of MR
psychiatric disorders. American Journal of Ortho- imaging and SPECT. American Journal of Roent-
psychiatry, 55, 27–41. genology, 162, 935–41.
Rogers, M. A., Kasai, K., Koji, M., Fukuda, R., Iwnami, Scott, J. G., Emick, M. A., & Adams, R. L. (1999).
A., Nakagome, K., et al. (2004). Executive and The MMPI-2 and closed-head injury: Effects of
prefrontal dysfunction in unipolar depression: litigation and head injury severity. Journal of
A review of neuropsychological and imaging Forensic Neuropsychology, 1, 3–13.
evidence. Neuroscience Research, 50, 1–11. Seidenberg, M., Haltiner, A., Taylor, M. A., Hermann,
Rogler, L. H., Malgady, R. G., & Tryon, W. W. (1992). B. B., & Wyler, A. (1994). Development and
Issues of memory in the Diagnostic Interview validation of a Multiple Ability Self-Report
Forensic Assessment of Medically Unexplained Symptoms 363

Questionnaire. Journal of Clinical and Experi- Sorg, B. A., Tschirgi, M. L., Swindell, S., Chen, L., &
mental Neuropsychology, 16, 93–104. Fang, J. (2001). Repeated formaldehyde effects in
Selz, M. & Reitan, R. M. (1979). Neuropsychological an animal model for multiple chemical sensitivity.
test performance of normal, learning disabled, and Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 933,
brain-damaged older children. Journal of Nervous 57–67.
and Mental Disease, 167, 298–302. Southwick, S. M., Morgan, C. A., Nicolaou, A. L., &
Sharpe, M., Chalder, T., Palmer, I., & Wessely, S. (1997). Charney, D. S. (1997). Consistency of memory for
Chronic fatigue syndrome. A practical guide to combat-related traumatic events in veterans of
assessment and management. General Hospital Operation Desert Storm. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 19, 185–99. Psychiatry, 154, 173–77.
Shedler, J., Mayman, M., & Manis, M. (1993). The illu- Sparks, P. J. (2000). Idiopathic environmental intoler-
sion of mental health. American Psychologist, 48, ances: Overview. Occupational Medicine: State of
1117–31. the Art Reviews, 15, 497–510.
Sherer, M. & Adams, R. L. (1993). Cross-validation of Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999).
Reitan and Wolfson’s Neuropsychological Deficit Stereotype threat and women’s math performance.
Scales. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28.
429–36. Staudenmayer, H. (1996). Clinical consequences of the
Sherer, M., Nixon, S. J., Parsons, O. A., & Adams, R. L. EI/MCS “diagnosis”: Two paths. Regulatory
(1992). Performance of alcoholic and brain- Toxicology and Pharmacology, (1 Part 2), S96–110.
damaged subjects on the Luria Memory Words Staudenmayer, H. (2001). Idiopathic environmental
Test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 7, intolerances (IEI): Myth and reality. Toxicology
499–504. Letters, 120, 333–42.
Shorter, E. (1992). From paralysis to fatigue. A history of Staudenmayer, H., Selner, J. C., & Buhr, M. P. (1993).
psychosomatic illness in the modern era. New York: Double-blind provocation chamber challenges in
The Free Press. 20 patients presenting with “multiple chemical
Simon, G. E., Daniell, W., Stockbridge, H., Claypoole, sensitivity.” Regulatory Toxicology and
K., & Rosenstock, L. (1993). Immunologic, psy- Pharmacology, 18, 44–53.
chological, and neuropsychological factors in Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air. How stereo-
multiple chemical sensitivity. A controlled study. types shape intellectual identity and performance.
Annals of Internal Medicine, 119, 97–103. American Psychologist, 52, 613–29.
Simon, G. E., Katon, W. J., & Sparks, P. J. (1990). Stein, M., Walker, J., Hazen, A., & Forde, D. (1997).
Allergic to life: Psychological factors in environ- Full and partial posttraumatic stress disorder:
mental illness. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, Findings for a community survey. American
901–906. Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1114–19.
Simon, G. E., & VonKorff, M. (1995). Recall of psychi- Stewart, C. A., Petrie, R. X. A., Balfour, D. J. K.,
atric history in cross-sectional surveys: Implications Matthews, K., & Reid, I. C. (2004). Enhanced
for epidemiologic research. Epidemiologic Reviews, evoked responses after early adversity and repeated
17, 221–27. platform exposure: The neurobiology of vulnera-
Slater, E. (1965). Diagnosis of “hysteria.” British Medical bility. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 868–70.
Journal, 1, 1395–99. Stone, D., Boone, K. B., Back-Madruga, C., & Lesser, I.
Sledjeski, E. M., Speisman, B., & Dierker, L. C. (2008). (2006). Has the rolling uterus finally gathered
Does number of lifetime traumas explain the rela- moss? Somatization and malingering of cognitive
tionship between PTSD and chronic medical con- deficit in six cases of “toxic mold” exposure. The
ditions? Answers from the National Comorbidity Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 766–85.
Survey-Replication (NCS-R). Journal of Behavioral Storzbach, D., Binder, L. M., Salinsky, M. C., Campbell,
Medicine, 31, 341–49. B. R., & Mueller, R. M. (2000). Improved predic-
Smith, S. & Sullivan, K. (2003). Examining the influ- tion of nonepileptic seizures using combined
ence of biological and psychological factors on MMPI and EEG measures. Epilepsia, 41, 332–37.
cognitive performance in chronic fatigue syn- Stuart, J. A., Ursano, R. J., Fullerton, C. S., & Wessely, S.
drome: A randomized double-blind, placebo- (2008). Belief in exposure to chemical and biologi-
controlled, crossover study. International Journal cal agents in Persian Gulf War soldiers. Journal of
of Behavioral Medicine, 10, 162–73. Nervous and Mental Disease, 196, 122–27.
Smyth, J. M. (1998). Written emotional expression: Suhr, J. A. (2003). Neuropsychological impairment in
Effect sizes, outcome, types, and moderating vari- fibromyalgia: Relation to depression, fatigue, and
ables. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, chronic pain. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
66, 174–84. 55, 321–29.
364 forensic neuropsychology

Suhr, J. A. & Gunstad, J. (2002). “Diagnosis threat”: The Wessely, S. (1997). Chronic fatigue syndrome: A 20th
effect of negative expectations on cognitive perfor- century illness? Scandinavian Journal of Work and
mance in head injury. Journal of Clinical and Environmental Health, 23 (Suppl. 3), 17–34.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 448–57. Wessely S., Nimnuan C., & Sharpe, M. (1999).
Suhr, J. A. & Gunstad, J. (2005). Further exploration of Functional somatic syndromes: One or many?.
the effect of “diagnosis threat” on cognitive perfor- Lancet, 354, 936–39.
mance in individuals with mild head injury. Journal Westbrook, L. E., Devinsky, O., & Geocadin, R. (1998).
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, Nonepileptic seizures after head injury. Epilepsia,
23–29. 39, 978–82.
Suhr, J. & Spickard, B. (2007). Including measures of Whiteside, T. L. & Friberg, D. (1998). Natural killer
effort in neuropsychological assessment of pain cells and natural killer cell activity in chronic
and fatigue-related medical disorders. In K. B. fatigue syndrome. American Journal of Medicine,
Boone (Ed.) Assessment of feigned cognitive impair- 105 (Supp. 3A), 27S–34S.
ment. A neuropsychological perspective (pp. 259– Whittaker, R., Kemp, S., & House, A. (2007). Illness
80). New York: Guilford Press. perceptions and outcome in mild head injury: A
Taylor, S. & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2004). Treating health longitudinal study. Journal of Neurology,
anxiety. New York: Guilford Press. Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 78, 544–646.
Terr, A. I. (2005). Toxic mold disease: A diagnosis of Widom, C. S. & Morris, S. (1997). Accuracy of adult
litigation. Annals of Allergy, Asthma, and recollections of childhood victimization: Part 2.
Immunology, 95, 215–16. Childhood sexual abuse. Psychological Assessment,
Tiersky, L. A., Johnson, S. K., Lange, G., Natelson, B. 9, 34–46.
H., & DeLuca, J. (1997). Neuropsychology of Wilkus, R. J., Dodrill, C. B., & Thompson, P. M. (l984).
chronic fatigue syndrome: A critical review. Journal Intensive EEG monitoring and psychological stud-
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, ies of patients with pseudoepileptic seizures.
560–86. Epilepsia, 25, 100–107.
Tirelli, U., Chierichetti, G., Tavio, M., Simonelli, C., Williams, J. M., Little, M. M., Scates, S., & Blockman,
Bianchin, G., Zanco, P., et al. (1998). Brain positron N. (1987). Memory complaints and abilities among
emission tomography (PET) in chronic fatigue depressed older adults. Journal of Consulting and
syndrome: Preliminary data. American Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 595–98.
Medicine, 105 (supp. 3A), 54S–8S. Williamson, D. J., Drane, D. L., & Stroup, E. S. (2007).
Van den Bergh, O., Devriese, S., Winters, W., Symptom validity tests in the epilepsy clinic. In K.
Veulemans, H., Nemery, B., Eelen, P., et al. (2001). Boone (Ed.) Assessment of feigned cognitive impair-
Acquiring symptoms in response to odors: A learn- ment (pp. 346–65). New York: Guilford Press.
ing perspective on multiple chemical sensitivity. Winters, W., Devriese, S., Van Diest, I., Nemery, B.,
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 933, Veulemans, H., Eelen, P., et al. (2003). Media warn-
278–90. ings about environmental pollution facilitate the
Van der Werf, S. P., Prins, J. B., Jongen, P. H. J., van der acquisition of symptoms in response to chemical
Meer, J. W. M., & Bleijenberg, G. (2000). Abnormal substances. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 332–38.
neuropsychological findings are not necessarily a Wolfe, F., Anderson, J., Harkness, D., Bennett, R. M.,
sign of cerebral impairment: A matched compari- Caro, X. J., Goldenberg, D. L., et al. (1997). Work
son between chronic fatigue syndrome and multi- and disability status of persons with fibromyalgia.
ple sclerosis. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, Journal of Rheumatology, 24, 1171–78.
and Behavioral Neurology, 13, 199–203. Wolfe, F., Ross, K., Anderson, J., Russell, I. J., & Hebert,
Walker, E. A., Keegan, D., Gardner, G., Sullivan, M., L. (1995). The prevalence and characteristics of
Katon, W. J., & Bernstein, D. (1997). Psychosocial fibromyalgia in the general population. Arthritis &
factors in fibromyalgia compared with rheumatoid Rheumatism, 38, 19–28.
arthritis: I. Psychiatric diagnoses and functional Youngjohn, J. R., Davis, D., & Wolf, I. (1997). Head
disability. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 565–71. injury and the MMPI-2: Paradoxical severity
Walker, E. A., Keegan, D., Gardner, G., Sullivan, M., effects and the influence of litigation. Psychological
Bernstein, D., & Katon, W. J. (1997). Psychosocial Assessment, 9, 177–84.
factors in fibromyalgia compared with rheumatoid Youngjohn, J. R., Spector, J., & Mapou, R. L. (1997).
arthritis: II. Sexual, physical, and emotional abuse Neuropsychological findings in silicone breast-
and neglect. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 572–77. implant complainants: Brain damage, somatiza-
Walton, J. (1985). Brain’s diseases of the nervous System. tion, or compensation neuroses? The Clinical
(9th Ed.) New York: Oxford University Press. Neuropsychologist, 11, 132–41.
14
Something to Talk About?
The Status of Post-traumatic Stress Disorder
in Clinical Neuropsychology

J I M A N D R I K O P O U L O S A N D M A N F R E D F. G R E I F F E N S T E I N

“No diagnosis in the history of American research in a manner that justifies a specific
psychiatry has had a more dramatic and perva- neuropsychology referral? Or does neuropsy-
sive impact on law and social justice than post- chological assessment only inform cognate issues
traumatic stress disorder” (Stone, 1993, p. 23). and not central ones? These questions give prece-
dence to the title for this chapter: “Something to
Clinical and forensic neuropsychologists may Talk About?”
react with puzzlement to a new but more frequent Neuropsychology delving into PTSD might
referral question: “Patient/claimant states they presumably contribute something to the dialogue
cannot work because of post-traumatic stress of what has been a controversial clinical entity
disorder. Please conduct neuropsychological from the start. The PTSD controversy has had a
testing to verify diagnosis.” The perplexed response greater impact upon the mental health field than
is understandable, for the question contains tacit the mild head injury (MHI) controversy has had
assumptions at odds with the traditional role of in clinical neuropsychology.
neuropsychology. First, neuropsychologists typi-
cally evaluate prospects for cognitive and func- G E N E R A L C O N S I D E R AT I O N S
tional residuals of confirmed brain dysfunction There are some self-imposed limitations to the
(e.g., definite stroke) or strongly suspected scope of this chapter. First, the discussion will be
dysfunction (e.g., dementia). The referral ques- limited to adults. Second, the focus is on civilian
tion, however, implies a neurological basis, either PTSD, with a cursory discussion of combat
structural or physiological, that is reliably associ- PTSD to help illustrate some points and explain
ated with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). the history of the disorder. Because a large por-
Second, neuropsychologists evaluate patients tion of the PTSD literature comes from the
whose leading features are cognitive in nature, veteran population, it deserves more space then
even if there is no prior evidence to suspect available in this chapter, but a distinction must be
clinical brain damage. PTSD’s cardinal signs, made to illustrate the poor generalization from
however, involve lingering and irrational fear military to civilian patients. In their meta-analysis
reactivity, not core cognitive complaints. Third, of PTSD risk factors, Brewin, Andrews, and
the question assumes neuropsychological testing Valentine (2000, p. 729) summarize the literature:
has incremental validity, that it has attained “This distinction represents the single largest and
greater accuracy in diagnosis of PTSD than tradi- most important division in the literature on the
tional psychometric and clinical methods. prediction of PTSD, and it accounted [sic] for
However, most of the literature on PTSD diagno- the greatest number of significant moderator
sis and research relies on history, interview, obser- variables.”
vation, and personality tests. There are many factors that severely limit
The begging of multiple questions leads back generalization from a military sample to civilian
to the unanswered one: Does professional and allegations of PTSD (Horner & Hamner, 2002).
scientific clinical neuropsychology have a basis to First, female gender is a risk for civilian PTSD
inform PTSD diagnosis, treatment, theory, and (Brewin et al., 2000), but the veteran PTSD
366 forensic neuropsychology

population is overwhelmingly male. Second, the PTSD samples, which are important for
medicolegal aspects of PTSD within the developing classification studies of legitimate
Department of Veteran Affairs (VA) compensa- versus invalid presentations of PTSD. Instead,
tion system also differ in that the burden of simulation groups are used. In the present chap-
proof is lower than in civil litigation. Per a recent ter, we only review a few simulation studies in any
directive, the VA is “amending its adjudication detail, as the results of these types of investiga-
regulations regarding service connection for tions may not be generalizable to clinical practice.
PTSD by eliminating the requirement of evidence For example, in a “Cry for Help” MMPI-2 analog
corroborating occurrence of the claimed in- investigation, all ten basic clinical scales were
service stressor in claims in which PTSD is diag- elevated when the psychiatric outpatient sample
nosed in service” (Department of Veterans Affairs, was asked to take the test a second time under
2008, p. 64208). Third, medical and psychiatric Cry for Help instructions (Berry et al., 1996). It is
conditions that arise out of military service are uncommon for a patient exaggerating psychiatric
compensable for the remainder of the veteran’s symptoms to have elevations on all ten scales.
life; consequently a noncompensation PTSD To remedy the absence of known-groups
sample cannot be formed within the VA, which studies, we offer as an alternative the application
makes it difficult to validate symptom validity of a concept borrowed from the forensic neuro-
tests (SVTs) to use in civilian studies. psychology literature. One procedure for the
There are other methodological issues the detection of malingered cognitive dysfunction is
reader should be aware of in examining the neuro- the level of impairment method. This is also
psychology and PTSD literature. Studies in which referred to as the floor effect strategy (Backhaus,
patients are recruited via newspapers, advertise- Fichtenberg, & Hanks, 2004), or the dose response
ments, or similar means (e.g., undergraduate relationship method (Bianchini, Curtis, & Greve,
students reporting “traumas”) and diagnosed with 2006). This procedure involves a comparison of a
PTSD are not easily generalizable. As an example, person’s score on a cognitive test to the average
Stein, Kennedy, and Twamley (2002) adminis- score of individuals who are markedly impaired
tered a broad range of common neuropsycholo- as established by unequivocal medical evidence
gical tests in addition to a well-validated structured of a brain injury (i.e., coma, abnormal imaging
interview for PTSD. They reported no cognitive studies, or neurological examination), or those
group effects between trauma-exposed patients who have a more severe injury as reflected in a
with PTSD, without PTSD, and normal controls worse Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS). This level of
on tests of verbal memory, but significant differ- impairment method can also be applied to psy-
ences in nonverbal cognitive tasks. Stein et al.’s chological tests. In our proposed operationaliza-
(2002) findings are the reverse of what most tion the severity of PTSD as measured by
researchers report in military studies. Bottom standardized tests or interviews arising from a
line: The study differs because the sample was minor motor vehicle accident (MVA) should not
recruited via posted advertisements and personal be at the same level or exceed that of a person
contacts among treatment centers specializing who might be the victim of rape. Until more
in domestic violence. It was not a consecutive civilian malingering studies using a known-
clinical sample of treatment-seekers. groups design appear in the PTSD literature, the
Our discussion recognizes there is a near level of impairment method remains one of
absence of PTSD known-groups studies (com- the easiest, most intuitive (especially to a jury),
pensable versus noncompensable patient groups and straightforward methods for assessing malin-
in which malingering is operationally defined), gered PTSD. Most neuropsychologists that are
unlike the MHI literature in which MHI litigants assessment focused are more likely to be exposed
are compared to more severely head-injured to PTSD patients within an independent medical
nonlitigants. While there is no shortage of epide- evaluation (IME) context. Although not ideal,
miological and clinical studies of civilian PTSD the level of impairment method can be applied
arising from rape, terrorism, and other traumas, within one’s own practice-specific sample of
finding a large sample of civilians claiming PTSD litigants. Those with operationally defined severe
with detailed psychological and SVT assessment stressors or traumas (e.g., traumatic amputation,
as well as known litigation status is rare. Most psy- demonstrable bodily injury or witnessing a death,
chologists, let alone neuropsychologists, do not etc.) can be compared to those with everyday
have access to both litigating and nonlitigating stressors.
Something to Talk About? 367

PTSD DEFINED were introduced in previous editions of the DSM.


The first was the inclusion in DSM-III of “delayed”
“Saving PTSD from itself in DSM-V” (Spitzer, PTSD, whereby symptoms can appear six months
First, & Wakefield, 2007, p. 233). or more after the trauma (American Psychiatric
Association, 1980). This is relevant because neu-
Posttraumatic stress disorder is widely viewed ropsychologists first see such patients long after
as a persistent anxiety disorder temporally linked traumas. The concept of delayed PTSD is a nullifi-
with a life-threatening event. It is also widely cation of the principle that trauma, physical or
agreed that the core features are re-experiencing psychological, should have a statute of limitations;
the trauma (in nightmares, flashbacks, or exces- it should appear within a reasonable time proxi-
sive rumination), and numbing (emotional under- mate to the event. The second controversy con-
reactivity). Depending on what definition is used, cerned expanding the definition of trauma. In
other symptoms used in PTSD diagnosis are DSM-III events outside the experience of the
nonspecific, meaning they overlap with other majority of people such as combat, rape, torture,
anxiety disorders or conditions. These nonspecific and similar traumas were required. In DSM-III-R
features include avoidance (also seen in phobia, this gatekeeper threshold was loosened to include
generalized anxiety disorder, and personality dis- more commonplace events (American Psychiatric
orders), insomnia, poor concentration, restricted Association, 1987). Both of these controversies
range of affect (depression, normal reactivity will be discussed later.
style, and medications), proto-emotions such as
irritability, and hypervigilance (paranoid features H I S T O R I C A L C O N T E X T,
and general anxiety, antisocial and narcissistic THE DSM, AND THE
personality disorders). C O N T ROV E R S I E S
Table 14.1 summarizes the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual-IV-Text Revision (DSM- “The concept of PTSD took off like a rocket,
IV-TR) criteria for PTSD (American Psychiatric and in ways that had not initially been antici-
Association, 2000). There are three important pated” (Andreasen, 2004, p. 1322).
points about the DSM-IV-TR criteria. First, the
status of Criterion A as a “gatekeeper” is critical: An older and experienced clinician may dis-
Both prongs (A-1 and A-2) must be met confi- cover that they have to contend with disorders
dently before even applying the symptom criteria that were infrequent at the time of their training.
(B-D). A person involved in a potentially life- The appearance of new psychological disorders
threatening event (e.g., high speed car accident) can indicate an advance in clinical science, new
who acts calmly (by self-report and/or ER records) adaptive challenges from an altered environment,
does not meet Criterion A-2 and hence cannot be or an environmental sociopolitical change. If,
diagnosed with PTSD. Conversely, a person who however, a disorder exists at time A, disappears
reacts with horror and panic to a single harsh crit- from the nomenclature at time B, only to reappear
icism at work (possibly meets A-2 by self-report) under a different name at time C, one would at the
cannot meet A-1 (life-threatening event). Second, very least have to consider the validity of the diag-
a common mistake is to make a “presumptive nosis. If the same societal circumstance dictated
PTSD diagnosis” based on the mere fact of an its emergence and re-emergence at times A and C
accident, assault, crime, or other event (meets respectively, it is reasonable to weigh the role of
only A-1). The base rates for PTSD (to be dis- political factors in diagnostic validity.
cussed in depth below) make clear that posttrau- In 1952, the first Diagnostic & Statistical
matic resiliency is the rule and PTSD is the Manual (DSM) posited the diagnostic category of
exception in traumas where the only incentive is “Gross Stress Reaction” in which the stressor was
primary gain (reducing distress). Third, individual to be coded as either due to combat or a civilian
symptoms must be linked to the trauma and not catastrophe (e.g., fire, earthquake, explosion)
merely present. Complaining of hypervigilance is (American Psychiatric Association, 1952).
insufficient to meet D-4. Hypervigilance for a spe- Of particular note is the statement, “If the reac-
cific situation naturally resembling the trauma tion persists, this term is to be regarded as a tem-
must be present. porary diagnosis to be used only until a more
The current criteria contain two controversies, definitive diagnosis is established,” (p. 40). There
relevant to the clinical neuropsychologist, that are two implications to this statement that came
368 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 14.1 * DSMIVTR CRITERIA FOR POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER

A. The person has been exposed to a traumatic event in which both of the following were present:
1) the person experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with an event or events that involved actual or
threatened death or serious injury, or a threat to the physical integrity of self or others
2) the person’s response involved intense fear, helplessness, or horror
B. The traumatic event is persistently reexperienced in one (or more) of the following ways:
1) recurrent and intrusive distressing recollections of the event, including images, thoughts, or perceptions
2) recurrent distressing dreams of the event
3) acting or feeling as if the traumatic event were recurring (includes a sense of reliving the experience,
illusions, hallucinations, and dissociative flashback episodes, including those that occur on awakening or
when intoxicated)
4) intense psychological distress at exposure to internal or external cues that symbolize or resemble an
aspect of the traumatic event
5) physiological reactivity on exposure to internal or external cues that symbolize or resemble an aspect of
the traumatic event
C. Persistent avoidance of stimuli associated with the trauma and numbing of general responsiveness (not present
before the trauma), as indicated by three (or more) of the following:
1) efforts to avoid thoughts, feelings, or conversations associated with the trauma
2) efforts to avoid activities, places, or people that arouse recollections of the trauma
3) inability to recall an important aspect of the trauma
4) markedly diminished interest or participation in significant activities
5) feeling of detachment or estrangement from others
6) restricted range of affect (e.g., unable to have loving feelings)
7) sense of a foreshortened future (e.g., does not expect to have a career, marriage, children, or a normal
life span)
D. Persistent symptoms of increased arousal (not present before the trauma), as indicated by two (or more) of the
following:
1) difficulty falling or staying asleep
2) irritability or outbursts of anger
3) difficulty concentrating
4) hypervigilance
5) exaggerated startle response
E. Duration of the disturbance (symptoms in Criteria B, C, and D) is more than 1 month.
F. The disturbance causes clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important
areas of functioning.
Specify if:
Acute: if duration of symptoms is less than 3 months
Chronic: if duration of symptoms is 3 months or more
With Delayed Onset: if onset of symptoms is at least 6 months after the stressor

* modified from the DSM-IV-TR

to dominate the future debate on PTSD: First, a capture their nature. There was no basis for creat-
chronic form of this condition was not reliably ing a new diagnostic category. Nancy Andreasen,
observed in the years after World War II combat the “midwife at the birth of PTSD” recounts the
operations ended, and second, in the event history (Andreasen, 2004). For unknown reasons,
that symptoms persisted, alternative diagnostic gross stress reaction was dropped in DSM-II
categories within DSM were more likely to (American Psychiatric Association, 1968).
Something to Talk About? 369

Andreasen, a psychiatrist studying the psychiatric 1990s (Verger et al., 2004), 34.3% of the victims of
consequences of severe burns in the early 1970s the Oklahoma City bombing (North et al., 1999),
was called upon by Robert Spitzer, the Chairperson and 24.8% of survivors of the Rwanda genocide
of the DSM Task Force for the upcoming DSM-III (Pham, Weinstein, & Longman, 2004). These are
to assist in addressing the psychological conse- individuals with direct experience of the trau-
quences of the returning Vietnam veteran matic event. For individuals that might circu-
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980). At the itously be affected by an act, the prevalence of
time, an active group of Vietnam veterans and PTSD is cut by a third. The prevalence of probable
academicians vigorously lobbied to have the diag- PTSD following the attacks of September 11 in
nosis of “post-Vietnam syndrome” included in the the New York metropolitan area was 11.2% in the
DSM-III. Realizing this would exclude noncom- two months following the attacks (Schlenger
bat trauma, they instead coined the term “post- et al., 2002). This could be an overestimate as the
traumatic stress disorder” for which Andreasen method used was a self-report measure of PTSD
wrote the entire text description based on her versus a clinical interview. This includes persons
work with burn victims. claiming PTSD from having watched the attacks
Forensic neuropsychologists should not only on television. Some remain understandably skep-
be aware of the medicolegal DSM-related implica- tical about the possibility of developing PTSD
tions of PTSD, but also the controversies outside from watching television: “virtual PTSD”
the legal context. The reader is referred to two (McNally & Breslau, 2008). Using a DSM-based
texts. Young (1995) in his book The Harmony of interview module, the rate is closer to 7.5% one
Illusions: Inventing Post-traumatic Stress Disorder month after the attacks and 0.6% at six months
outlines this controversial history. While PTSD is (Galea et al., 2003). Using the level of impairment
real, the facts that surround its invention are the method, the psychological consequences of the
topic of his book. It was the anti-war mental health above traumatic events can now be compared
professionals, the most prominent of which were against more commonplace events such as a
Robert Jay Lifton and Sarah Haley, who helped to minor MVA that the patient reports resulted in
push the newly coined construct into the psychi- PTSD.
atric nomenclature. In a second, more clinically A second group of epidemiological studies
oriented text, PTSD researcher Brewin (2003) that need to be reviewed are the population-based
addresses the “malady or myth” of PTSD. The studies along with the conceptual arguments for
arguments of the “skeptics” are that PTSD need- modification of the A criteria. These arguments
lessly medicalizes normal human reaction to provided the rationale and background for remov-
trauma, is not clearly distinct from existing ing the DSM-III requirement that the stressor or
psychiatric disorders, and its invention has no event be outside the realm of normal human
historical parallel. experience. This criterion was weakened but still
present in DSM-III-R, and omitted completely in
EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDIES DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association,
1994). This has served to broaden the types of
“The full syndrome as defined by the DSM-III events that could potentially lead to a diagnosis of
was common only in veterans wounded in PTSD. Not unexpectedly, it is the view of some
Vietnam” (Helzer, Robins, & McEvoy, 1987, that the decision to broaden Criterion A needs to
p. 1630). be reconsidered (Andreasen, 2004). We share this
view.
Here we consider the prevalence of PTSD via Shortly before the “outside the realm of usual
a review of the best clinical studies using popula- human experience” criterion was removed from
tions that have been through horrific events. Four the DSM-IV, two prominent PTSD researchers
well-done epidemiological studies about truly made some observations that arguably make a
traumatic events are remarkably consistent in case for the weakening of the stressor criterion
their conclusions on the prevalence of PTSD. including a) defining “unusualness” in regard to
Approximately 30% of individuals experiencing the event is difficult as it varies based on the sub-
events rarely seen by most people will develop jective perspective of not only the patient, but the
PTSD: 34.9% of Ugandan and Congolese child clinician; b) events still within the experience of
soldiers (Bayer, Klasen, & Adam, 2007), 31% of most persons can be distressing and cause the
victims in the Paris terrorist bombings of the mid syndrome (citing divorce and job loss); c) since
370 forensic neuropsychology

even the most extreme levels of stress may not PTSD. With this loosening of the criteria, “almost
always lead to PTSD, it must be something in or everyone has experienced a PTSD-level event”
about the person’s perception of the stressor that (p. 703).
may be relevant (Davidson & Foa, 1991). The We conclude with a discussion of demographic
step-wise process of expanding PTSD beyond the risk factors associated with the development of
Vietnam veteran (for whom it was intended) was PTSD. Two meta-analytic studies have identified
termed “conceptual bracket creep” by Harvard these factors. In the first, Brewin et al. (2000)
psychologist Richard McNally (2003). This means identified 14 risk factors. Trauma severity, lack of
that Criterion A, requiring that the event be out- social support, and life stress conveyed the largest
side the realm of everyday experience, became risk of developing PTSD. This is followed by:
increasingly diluted and captured more patients adverse childhood events, low intelligence, low
with each edition of DSM. SES, childhood abuse, female gender, family and
An early paper by PTSD epidemiologist Naomi personal psychiatric history, a previous trauma,
Breslau convincingly argues that there was very lack of education, younger age, and minority
little empirical evidence for the validity of PTSD status. When excluding military studies, low intel-
as a distinct clinical entity in DSM-III (Breslau & ligence and younger age at onset were no longer
Davis, 1987). There is no literature to support the risk factors. What merits mention, and perhaps is
view that a discrete class of stressors increases the not coincidental, is that many of these variables
probability of developing a psychiatric disorder. can be viewed as predictors of litigation. In a later
They concluded: meta-analysis, a different approach was used
(Ozer, Best, Lipsey, & Weiss, 2008). Trauma sever-
Extraordinary stressors are like more ordinary ity was not studied as a predictor because, by
stressful events with respect to their complex definition, a stressor is required for the diagnosis.
differential effects upon individuals. Personal They found that peritraumatic dissociation and
characteristics and the nature of the social emotional responses, perceived support, and
environment modify the likelihood and form perceived life threat were the highest predictors
of the response of individuals to all types of while prior trauma, prior adjustment, and family
stressors. (p. 255). psychiatric history were all equal predictors.

Breslau followed this with an epidemiological THE NEUROBIOLOGY


study of young adults from the Detroit, Michigan OF PTSD
area. The lifetime prevalence of exposure to trau-
matic events was 39.1%. The rate of PTSD in those “Now we know much about the neurobiology
who were exposed was 23.6%, yielding lifetime of PTSD: So what?” (Figley, 2010).
prevalence in the sample of 9.2% (Breslau, Davis,
Andreski, & Peterson, 1991). This set the stage for A fundamental question is “Does stress cause
the argument that “traumatic events” are not brain damage?” A brief review of structural imag-
uncommon in the general population and can ing literature may provide answers. The integrity
produce PTSD. This viewpoint was codified in the of the human hippocampus plays a major role in
most often cited PTSD epidemiological study, the clinical neuropsychology and in recent PTSD
National Comorbidity Survey, which was the first scholarship as well. The role of the hippocampus
nationally representative mental health survey in in recent memory is so well established that it
the United States to use a fully structured DSM- does not need review in this chapter, but its role
III-R diagnostic interview (Kessler, Somnega, in stress physiology does require review. It is
Bromet, Hughes, & Nelson, 1995). They con- generally accepted that stress hormones, such as
cluded that PTSD is more prevalent than previ- corticosteroids, are transiently suppressive of
ously believed, and that “virtually all qualifying hippocampal function (McEwen, 1999). These
events for PTSD are quite common, and that none facts lead naturally to the question whether PTSD
is such that virtually everyone exposed to it devel- is associated with structural changes in the hip-
ops PTSD” (p. 1057). Breslau and Kessler (2001) pocampus, and much literature arose around the
were to later investigate the stressor criterion. question.
Nineteen events were used to operationalize the Bremner et al. (1995) conducted the first study
A-1 criterion resulting in a 59% increase in the to analyze hippocampal volume in persons with
number of events that can be used to diagnose PTSD. Twenty-six Vietnam PTSD-disordered and
Something to Talk About? 371

22 nondisordered veterans, matched for demo- exposure. Gilberston et al. (2002) examined sev-
graphic characteristics, underwent MRI, and right enteen monozygotic twins discordant for
hippocampal volume was significantly smaller by Vietnam-era combat exposure. Hippocampal
8% relative to controls. There was no difference in MRI data showed smaller hippocampal volume in
the volume of other brain regions. Surprisingly, combat-experienced twins with more severe
Wechsler Memory Scale verbal subtest scores PTSD symptoms, but the noncombat probands
were correlated with smaller right hippocampal also exhibited smaller hippocampal volume. The
volume in PTSD patients only. This study trig- data supported the notion that smaller hip-
gered many follow-up studies, and fed a prema- pocampi in PTSD represented a preexisting con-
ture belief that PTSD was an organic brain dition.
syndrome. Pitman et al. (2006) also studied twins; the
Subsequent neuroimaging studies showed index cases were PTSD and the probands unex-
widely varied and contradictory results, and none posed to combat. Both index cases and probands
replicated Bremner et al.’s (1995) findings. Gurvits, showed increased neurological soft signs, dimin-
Shenton, Hokama, and Ohta (1996) used MRI ished hippocampal volume, and presence of
with a small PTSD sample (seven with PTSD and abnormal cavum septum pellucidum.
eight without PTSD), and reported both left and A more exhaustive review of the neurobiology
right hippocampi were significantly smaller in the of PTSD is beyond the scope of this chapter. Three
combat PTSD group, and hippocampal volume chapters in the edited text by Vasterling and
was directly correlated with self-reported combat Brewin (2005), devoted to the biology of PTSD,
exposure. Bonne et al. (2001) conducted a pro- are recommended. The neurobiology of PTSD is
spective study of hippocampus volume in recent discussed throughout the current chapter when
trauma survivors who developed PTSD (N = 37). clinically applicable, and more specifically as it
The patients with PTSD did not differ from trauma might apply to the forensic arena. The work of
survivors without PTSD in hippocampal volume, McNally (2006) is especially insightful.
right or left, at one-week and half-year follow-ups, Part of the reluctance in avoiding an extensive
and there was no volume reduction over time. basic science discussion has to do with how this
Villareal et al. (2002) assessed both hippocampal might be translated into clinical practice. The
and whole brain volume in civilian PTSD, finding impasse facing the field of translational research is
lower left hippocampal volume and lower white that much of what is learned in the laboratory
matter/intracranial volume ratios, consistent with does not always transfer to the clinic in a way that
generalized white matter atrophy. Hedges et al. is clinically meaningful (Figley, 2010). For exam-
(2003), in a pilot study with only four examinees, ple, in a PTSD study of MVA patients with and
showed bilateral hippocampal volumes were sig- without PTSD, EEG asymmetry was demon-
nificantly smaller in post-traumatic stress disor- strated. Activation of the right anterior and poste-
der subjects. Vermetten, Vythilingam, Southwick, rior regions during exposure to trauma-related
Charney, and Bremner (2003) found smaller hip- pictures was seen. After treatment, there was a
pocampi that normalized after treatment with reduction of right-sided anterior activation (Rabe,
paroxetine. Jatzko et al. (2006) looked for long Beauducel, Zoolner, Maercker, & Karl, 2006). The
term changes fifteen years post-trauma in chronic common method for measuring psychophysio-
PTSD patients: Neither conventional volumetry logical reactivity related to PTSD has been the
nor morphometry detected any differences in script-imagery paradigm (McNally, 2006). The
hippocampus volume or structure. Weniger, physiologic reactivity of the patient being rem-
Lange, Sachsse, and Irle (2008) examined the inded of the event is said to provide indirect proof
caudality of hippocampal volume. They found of PTSD. Keane et al. (1998) conducted the largest
significantly smaller posterior hippocampi in study on the physiological concomitants of PTSD.
PTSD, with no difference in the volumes of Vietnam veterans were exposed to audiovisual
anterior hippocampus or subiculum. There were presentations of combat. The results of heart rate,
also no volume differences between PTSD result- skin conductance, and EMG findings identified
ing from prolonged childhood abuse compared to 75% of veterans with and without PTSD. Abnormal
single adult-trauma exposure. physiological reactivity has been shown in patients
Correlation does not equal causation, and with PTSD following MVA in the Albany Motor
some investigators examined prospects that hip- Vehicle Project (Blanchard et al., 1996a). Such
pocampal volume differences predated trauma a differential response between the PTSD and
372 forensic neuropsychology

non-PTSD patients would indirectly suggest the (Wrenger, Lange, Langer, Heuft, & Burgmer,
presence of the disorder independent of patient 2008). Presumably, the likelihood of PTSD would
self-report. be higher if a person were to witness an injury of
Does this biological response support the a family member in that same accident. In a study
diagnosis of PTSD? In an imaginative experiment, addressing PTSD in 68 children, no father had
skin conductance, heart rate, and EMG response PTSD one year following the accident and 5.7% of
of the left lateral frontalis were measured in the mothers did (Landolt, Vollrath, Timm,
persons claiming to be abducted by aliens. Those Gnhem, & Sennhouser, 2005).
reporting the most negative alien experiences Deviation from sound study design can result
(versus those with neutral or positive experiences) in gross overestimates of PTSD and affect the
had the greatest reactivity on all three physio- validity of clinical beliefs. The earliest and largest
logical measures (McNally et al., 2004). The reac- body of work on the psychological consequences
tivity reactions of these subjects to the audio of MVA is the Albany Motor Vehicle Accident
recordings of their abduction were at least as great Project, summarized in the book After the Crash,
as the findings in the Keane study. According by Blanchard and Hickling (2004). The problem
to McNally (2006), “Reactivity, however, is not in their approach was the use of a self-selected
specific for PTSD; rather, it is specific for emo- sample. Participants comprised a combination
tionally evocative memories, whether true or of paid volunteers recruited via newspapers
false” (p. 273). ads and treatment seekers; over half were in
litigation. The subjects (N=161) were seen an
T H E M OTO R V E H I C L E average of 11 months following their MVA.
ACCIDENT & PTSD Of these, 68% had PTSD and a mean Clinician
Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) (Weathers
“Giving the same diagnosis to death camp et al., 2004) in the severe range (mean score of
survivors and someone who has been in a 72). Given the obvious external incentive of their
motor vehicle accident diminishes the magni- patient sample, the long post-accident duration,
tude of the stressor and the significance of and the high severity of symptom report in a
PTSD” (Andreasen, 2004, 1321). majority, one would think that symptom validity
should require some minimum level of scrutiny,
Can a MVA in which no serious injury is sus- which is a basic tenet of forensic psychology.
tained cause PTSD? Unless “severe motor auto- Blanchard and Hickling, however, dismissed any
mobile accidents” are experienced, as noted in the prospect that malingering could corrupt the data.
DSM-IV-TR, developing PTSD is improbable. They state, a) “We made no effort to check the
Furthermore, if the DSM-III requirement that the veracity of participants’ reports but found no
event be outside the realm of everyday human obvious reason to doubt them…We had no
experience is considered, many MVAs would not instances for which we felt we had been misled”
qualify. The best literature employs a prospective (p. 197); b) “In our larger sample of more than 400
cohort methodology. Using this methodology, participants, only two participants had been iden-
PTSD is rare following a MVA. The studies below tified as having faked or exaggerated their symp-
are chosen because they represent the best epide- toms” (p. 243); c) “We do not routinely use the
miological investigations; they are prospective, MMPI-2 or other tests that might add validity
assess consecutively admitted hospital patients, measures or tests of malingering to the assess-
and use structured clinical interviews. ment” (p. 246); d) “We did not collect medical
In one early study, 107 patients were examined records on our patients” (p. 247); e) “Initially,
an average of 28 months after an accident. Only participants were asked if they contacted a
one patient had PTSD (Malt, 1988). In another lawyer; if they answered affirmatively, they were
series (N=106) comprised exclusively of patients scored as involved in litigation” (p.186). This last
sustaining severe life-threatening injuries (60% point requires elaboration, as it is an overlooked
from MVA), 1.9% had PTSD at one year (Schnyder, issue in malingering research.
Moergeli, Trentz, Klaghofer, & Buddeberg, 2001). When forming compensation- and noncom-
Of 100 consecutive MVA admissions to an inten- pensation-seeking samples, the statement of the
sive care unit, 8% had PTSD four to six weeks patient that they have not retained an attorney
post-injury (Matsuoka et al., 2008). At one year, does not define compensation status or potential
only 2.5% (N=208) had PTSD in another study compensability. If, for example, a patient waiting
Something to Talk About? 373

at a stop sign is rear-ended, their claim of not prevalence rate of 16.5% at one year. In this study
being involved in litigation at the time of evalua- they did find that PTSD was three times more
tion is no guarantee that this will not occur in the common in litigants (24.6%) versus nonlitigants
future. It is naïve to accept at face value the (8.1%).
patient’s denial of litigation if the circumstances of
the injury clearly indicate that it is compensable. H E A D I N J U RY A N D P T S D
Litigation status should be defined by whether the
injury is compensable (someone can be held “Late-appearing dual diagnosis is a litigation
accountable) versus noncompensable (e.g., phenomenon so intertwined with secondary
occurred in one’s home, single car accidents, car gain as to be a byproduct of it” (Greiffenstein &
accidents where the patient is at fault, or sports Baker, 2008, p. 565).
injuries), and not by the involvement of an attor-
ney. Consequently, when a study describes a clini- The medicolegal implications of MHI and
cally referred, symptomatic, noncompensable PTSD are well known. It goes without saying
MHI sample, one has to question how that group that the synergistic effect of combining both dis-
was formed. In the experience of the authors, such orders in one patient can appear, at least on the
clinical referrals are very rare (Andrikopoulos, surface, a challenging assessment to perform. The
2001; Greiffenstein, Baker, & Gola, 1994). task has been made more seemingly onerous of
A second large series of studies comes from late by the emergence of two developments. The
Bryant and colleagues from the University of New first of these is the concern over the cognitive
South Wales in Australia. While their methodol- effects of blast injury, the so called “signature
ogy is sounder than the Albany studies, the PTSD wound” of the Iraq and Afghanistan conflicts
prevalence rates in their two earliest prospective (Bhattacharjee, 2008). The MHI and cognitive
studies are unexpectedly high and remarkably impairment that is said to result from the blast is
consistent with approximately 25% of patients coupled with the traumatic events of combat that
suffering from PTSD (Bryant & Harvey, 1998; can also lead to PTSD (Hoge et al., 2008). A dis-
Harvey & Bryant, 1998). One of these studies cussion of blast injury is beyond the scope of this
involved an MHI sample suffering from PTSD chapter. The reader is referred to the mild head
(a topic that will be discussed more in depth injury chapter by Larrabee in this text.
below). Notable by its absence in these two studies Suffice it to say that if blast injury alone
is any discussion of the possibility that litigation (without consideration of more traditional indi-
may play some role. Even within the Bryant and cators such as the GCS and post-traumatic amne-
Albany MVA Project studies, in the infrequent sia) were accepted as a mechanism of action for
instances where litigation was a variable of inter- head injury, our conceptualization of head injury
est, it predicted PTSD status (Blanchard et al., and how it has been studied for decades would
1996b; Blanchard et al., 1998; Bryant & Harvey, have to change. We believe that there is no evi-
1995; Bryant & Harvey, 2003). Finally, using the dence that this mechanism of injury provides any
level of impairment method outlined above, the incremental clinical information above and
PTSD prevalence rate in the Bryant studies beyond the traditional measures that predict
exceeds that of patients with more serious MVA head injury outcome (primarily the GCS). This
(Schnyder et al., 2001; Matsuoka et al, 2008), and development has served to popularize an impres-
equals or approaches the PTSD rates of the child sion that untangling the effects of a head injury
soldier and Rwanda studies (Bayer et al., 2007; from the symptoms of PTSD can be difficult
Pham et al., 2004). (Bryant, 2008). This is a misrepresentation. A
A third group of studies comes from the post-concussive syndrome and PTSD are two
University of Oxford and provides lower base wholly separate clinical entities. The core features
rates. The earliest and best of these studies assessed that define each can and should be partitioned.
188 consecutive patients and found that 11% met While one can argue that head-injured and PTSD
the DSM-III criteria for PTSD one-year post- patients both have memory, attention, and emo-
injury (Mayou, Bryant, & Duthie, 1993). Litigation tional problems, one can also point to schizo-
status was not addressed. Ehlers, Mayou, and phrenics who also have such problems. A clinician
Bryant (1998) assessed a larger consecutive series would not confuse the core symptoms of schi-
(N=781) using a PTSD questionnaire (versus a zophrenia with the post-concussive symptoms
semi-structured interview) and found a higher of MHI.
374 forensic neuropsychology

In tandem with these developments is the PTA appears to be protective against selected
concern over the effects of sports concussion, with re-experiencing symptoms.
some suggesting that repeated traumas can A prospective study by Dikmen and colleagues
result in an encephalopathy (Omalu et al., 2005). of head injuries of varying severity yielded
The best evidence from the sport concussion a similar prevalence rate of as many as 11%
literature shows that following a single concus- (N=125) meeting symptom criteria at any
sion, cognitive function returns to baseline in a point during the six months following the
week (McCrea et al., 2003). The evidence that injury, with only 5.6% meeting the full criteria at
repeated concussions can produce an encephal- six months (Bombardier et al., 2006). Another
opathy came initially from a handful of neuro- prospective study (N=69) revealed 13% prevalence
pathological case studies in which there is an at three months post injury (Levin et al., 2001). A
absence of clinical data to corroborate the neuro- similar prevalence rate of 14% (N=122) was
pathology (Omalu et al., 2005, 2006). The con- observed at six months (Gil, Caspi, Ben-Ari,
cepts of chronic traumatic encephalopathy and Koren, & Klein, 2005). None of the above studies
blast injury have served to promulgate and extend relied on SVT or reported on litigation status.
a debate in the absence of any data. These issues In the one study that reported litigation status,
are further discussed in the chapter on MHI in Koren, Arnon, and Klein (1999) noted that 100%
the current volume. of the PTSD group was in litigation and 97% of
Applying the same methodology (prospective, the non-PTSD group, because all Israeli citizens
consecutively examined hospital admissions, are entitled to compensation. The implication is
knowledge of litigation status, and a standardized that even when the trauma is compensable, the
DSM PTSD diagnosis) that we applied to the co-occurrence of mild brain injury and PTSD is
examination of the prevalence of PTSD following uncommon.
MVA to the co-occurrence of PTSD and MHI, we Notwithstanding the well-done epidemiologi-
find that the two conditions infrequently occur cal study of Bryant et al. (2009), the debate regard-
simultaneously. The largest MHI/PTSD data set ing the relationship between MHI and PTSD
produced to date comes from a recent publication arose primarily from earlier studies by Bryant and
of Bryant et al. (2009). Using the American colleagues that addressed both conditions in the
Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine (1993) defi- same patient. Again, similar to the Bryant studies
nition of MHI, they examined 425 patients with which addressed only PTSD, the prevalence of
MHI and 532 without MHI at three months post- PTSD in head-injury patients was similarly high
injury, using the CAPS. At follow-up, 11.8% of the (about 25%) and litigation status is not mentioned
MHI group had PTSD, and 7.5% of the non-MHI (Bryant & Harvey, 1999; Bryant, Marosszeky,
group had PTSD. Once the severity of the injury Crooks, & Gurka, 2000), including in literature
was controlled for, they concluded that PTSD is reviews (Bryant, 2001).
more likely to occur in those with MHI. The MHI In the studies mentioned above, for those that
group also had a higher CAPS severity score at address the presence of a comorbid MHI in the
follow-up (19.83 for MHI and 16.9 for the non- context of PTSD, none take into account the base
MHI group). The advantage of the study is that it rate of malingering following MHI (Mittenberg,
provides a reasonable prevalence rate and CAPS Patton, Canyock, & Condit, 2002), which averages
data on a group of moderately physically injured around 40% (see chapter 5 this volume). The topic
patients (mean of 12 days in hospital) upon which of malingering, which occupies the largest share
the clinician can rely to apply the level of impair- of current neuropsychological research, is over-
ment method. The CAPS score of the whole looked (Sweet, King, Malina, Bergman, &
sample falls into the lowest CAPS severity range Simmons, 2002). While some of the well-designed
of minimal symptoms. No neuropsychological studies above answer important epidemiological
testing was conducted to confirm that those iden- questions with regard to PTSD and head injury,
tified as having had a MHI actually had one that only in clinical samples can the issue of litigation
resulted in any impairment, although impairment and malingering be addressed, provided that the
at three months post uncomplicated MHI is question is asked.
unlikely (see Larrabee chapter in the present A final issue regarding the relationship
volume). Longer post-traumatic amnesia (PTA) between head injury and PTSD merits mention. It
was associated with less severe intrusive memo- has been debated whether one can develop PTSD
ries at the acute assessment, suggesting that in the context of a head injury that results in a loss
Something to Talk About? 375

of consciousness and/or post-traumatic amnesia, “ D E L AY E D ” P T S D


since there is no “witnessing” of the traumatic
event. The earliest study to address this issue “…PTSD—which advocates say can manifest
within a medicolegal context convincingly itself twenty years after the triggering inci-
argues that the two disorders do not coexist dents—is political, not scientific” (Burkett &
(Sbordone & Liter, 1995). The method employed Whitley, 1998, p. 232).
to detect PTSD may not be acceptable from
a clinical/research standpoint, but within a medi- According to the DSM-IV-TR, delayed PTSD
colegal context it represents sound clinical refers to the presentation of PTSD after six months
reasoning. Sbordone and Liter asked MHI and have elapsed since the time of the traumatic event.
PTSD litigants to volunteer their symptoms rather To understand the inherently illogical premise of
than giving a PTSD interview or self-report delayed PTSD, one needs only to draw a parallel
measure. The MHI group, 85% of whom reported with the delayed symptom presentation often seen
a loss of consciousness, did not volunteer any in MHI litigants, even those seen clinically. The
of the symptoms that are specific to PTSD (e.g., first 100 MHI patients referred by 11 different
nightmares, hypervigilance, avoidance, intrusive neurologists to the first author were seen approxi-
recollections, etc.). This mostly theoretical mately one year after the injury. These presumably
issue of the relationship between PTSD and neurologically mediated symptoms may appear
loss of consciousness is partially unnecessary closer to the trial or hearing date than to the actual
since the real issues are if cognitive impairment accident based on a medical record review. This
can arise from a single uncomplicated head time lag is consistent with the MHI litigation lit-
injury, and if PTSD can be seen following a minor erature. The decision to sue may occur to some-
MVA. In the event that a patient reports amnesia one at some point after the accident. They are now
from a MHI and post-traumatic symptoms, left with the difficult circumstance of having to
what is the likelihood that malingering will be complain of symptoms that they had not reported
present? before. As the symptoms they are now complain-
Greiffenstein and Baker (2008) examined the ing of may be severe and presumably due to a
coexistence of PTSD symptoms and MHI in sudden neurological event, one is hard pressed to
litigants. They formed a dual diagnosis group explain why they have never reported these symp-
(N = 95) composed of patients claiming both toms before. It would be analogous to seeing a
amnesia secondary to the head injury and re- neurologist for the first time to report loss of sen-
experiencing symptoms of PTSD, and a sub- sation for a stroke that occurred months or years
syndromal group (N=228) consisting of those before. Such a sequence of events would make
with avoidance and hyperarousal without the re- little “neurological sense.” In the case of a MHI,
experiencing symptoms. The remaining patients when the patient is eventually sent to the neurop-
(N=476) had no PTSD symptoms and were desig- sychologist, they understand that this person
nated as the late post-concussive syndrome assesses cognitive abilities and not physical
group. The validity measures employed included symptoms, which may be the only thing they
finger tapping, grip strength, the Test of Memory complained about since the injury. When the neu-
Malingering (TOMM), MMPI-2 Symptom Validity ropsychologist inquires about cognitive symp-
Scale (FBS) raw score and F T-score, and Reliable toms, the patient is put in a situation of deciding
Digit Span. Using two separate cutoffs based on whether to report such symptoms. Delayed PTSD
previous research to designate probable invalidity should be characterized in the same way.
(bottom tenth percentile) and definite invalidity The delayed onset subtype was introduced
(≤1 percentile) they found a very high rate of non- as a by-product of the Vietnam War. The main
credible performance. The dual diagnosis group argument for the introduction of the term in
failure rate ranged from 88% for the MMPI-2 FBS DSM-III was that the syndrome could emerge
to 25% for grip strength using the probable inva- long after the trauma. The initial intent of the
lidity rule. The definite rule failure rate was 49.5% delayed onset type, at least as conceptualized by
for the FBS to 1.1 percentile, respectively for grip Andreasen (2004), was that PTSD may not mani-
strength. The major finding is that those claiming fest until the veteran returned home, since a stress
re-experiencing symptoms with amnesia have reaction in the course of combat is not adaptive.
incrementally higher failure rates on validity What was not anticipated is that veterans may
indicators. file a PTSD claim at any time, often years after the
376 forensic neuropsychology

precipitating event. An early population-based Experience Study indicated 2.2% of Vietnam


epidemiological study, done shortly after the veterans had PTSD in the month preceding the
DSM-III was published, revealed that the delayed examination (Centers for Disease Control, 1988).
PTSD was found only among Vietnam veterans The cultural climate of the times dictated this
(Helzer et al., 1987). The preponderance of the evi- PTSD prevalence rate to be too low and the study
dence since then indicates that PTSD symptoms was disregarded. A partial vindication came from
develop within hours or days of the trauma. For a more accurate (but still high) reanalysis of the
those individual cases that are reported to have NVVRS data presented in the journal Science,
had a delayed onset, it usually involves delayed putting the lifetime prevalence at 18.7% and cur-
help-seeking or subsyndromic symptoms. A sys- rent PTSD at 9.1% (Dohrenwend et al., 2006). The
tematic review of the evidence has suggested that authors used military records to construct a
the delayed onset of PTSD in the absence of previ- combat exposure measure and to cross-check that
ously unrecognized symptoms is rare (Andrews, with the diagnoses assigned to 260 veterans.
Brewin, Philpott, & Stewart, 2007). Not withstand- Burkett and Whitley (1998), in their often
ing any flawed literature that might support the cited book Stolen Valor, argue against such a high
contention of delayed PTSD, it only stands to prevalence of PTSD in the NVVRS. They estimate
reason that there would be no rational explanation that 75% of those receiving compensation for
for why a person experiencing a truly horrific PTSD do not have the disorder. The limitation of
event would be completely asymptomatic follow- the book, in scientific terms, is that Burkett and
ing the event but symptomatic six months later. Whitley selected cases already suspicious for
symptom over-statement; theirs is not a represen-
MALINGERED PTSD tative sample. To cure this, Frueh et al. (2005)
used the Freedom of Information Act to draw a
“It is rare to find a psychiatric diagnosis that random sample of 100 files for veterans receiving
anyone wants to have, but PTSD seems to be benefits for combat PTSD. Only 41% had clearly
one of them” (Andreasen, 1995, p. 963). been in combat. Assuming not all combat veter-
ans develop PTSD, a malingering base rate
Concerns about the ease with which PTSD of >41% is implied. Frueh et al. further reported
could be malingered began at the conclusion of no statistical difference in the CAPS scores of the
the Vietnam War. The earliest published report on combat (76) and no combat (79) groups, but the
malingered combat PTSD in Vietnam veterans unclear combat group had a statistically higher
came from Sparr and Pankratz (1983). Loren score (94) than the other two groups. Scores in the
Pankratz, a VA psychologist, arguably introduced 70s suggest severe PTSD and the 90s extreme
the forced choice method for the detection of PTSD. The absence of combat exposure in concert
malingering to clinical psychology (Pankratz, with the difficult-to-reconcile CAPS scores sug-
Fausti, & Peed, 1975). More has been written gests malingering. Test-specific methods are
about malingered combat PTSD than the civilian needed to diagnose malingered PTSD. For now,
variety. The compensable nature of the disorder the methods that do exist are the same as those
within the VA makes it a desirable diagnosis. found in the forensic neuropsychology literature.
PTSD-specific SVT is in its infancy.
Prevalence of PTSD Malingering Two observations regarding malingered PTSD
According to the influential National Vietnam merit mention. First, the brief review above indi-
Veterans Readjustment Study (NVVRS), 30.9% of cates the literature on malingered PTSD is limited.
men (over 960,000) and 26.9% of women (over Second, the opening sentence of one malingering
1,900) have had PTSD at some point in their lives review declares, “PTSD is easy to fake” (Knoll &
(Kulka et al., 1990). When you include the Resnick, 2006). There are few forensic reviews in
lifetime prevalence of partial PTSD, an additional which this statement is not repeated. The cumula-
22.5% of male veterans and 21.2% of female tive effect of these two observations might cause
veterans have some symptoms of PTSD. This one to conclude that the detection of malingered
would mean that 53.4% of the men and 48.1% of PTSD is difficult. This is purportedly illustrated
the women have had clinically significant stress by a simulation study in which 94% of naïve sub-
reaction symptoms, out of 1.7 million veterans. jects correctly identified PTSD symptoms on a
A more accurate but less influential study pre- self-report measure (Burges & McMillan, 2001).
dated the publication of the NVVRS. The Vietnam This nihilistic conclusion from the abstract does
Something to Talk About? 377

not tell the whole story. Only one subject out of tions. The test relies solely on the forced choice
134 was able to correctly volunteer enough method with no clear rationale of why PTSD
symptoms to meet the DSM criteria for PTSD. patients should fail to recognize emotional
When a second group of subjects were asked to expressions; psychic numbing refers to an internal
complete the same self-report measure, 16 non- poverty of emotions, not a failure to recognize
PTSD symptoms were added to the 17 PTSD emotions in others. The MENT may be effective
symptoms, and 5 of the 16 non-PTSD symptoms only because of the response set induced in
were identified as PTSD symptoms by over 50% of the patient, that the test is sensitive to PTSD.
simulators. The purpose of the next section is to A further difficulty is methodological: A different
dissuade the reader from the notion that PTSD is set of instructions was given to the PTSD patient
easy to feign. group (Morel, 1998). As for the Q-PTSD, no
information or other studies are available other
The MENT than the study cited above. There are equally lim-
Morel (1998) developed the only stand-alone ited data on other PTSD specific SVT measures.
forced-choice test to assess malingered PTSD: the
Morel Emotional Numbing Test (MENT). This DA P S
test consists of three sets of 60 items in which the The Detailed Assessment of Posttraumatic
patient is asked to match a facial expression (e.g., Stress (DAPS) is a 104-item self-report PTSD
sad, happy, etc.) with the word that best describes instrument (Briere, 2001). An apparent advantage
the face. The subject is told that the test measures of the DAPS, compared to other self-report mea-
the ability to identify facial expressions and that in sures, is the presence of a validity scale, the
some individuals with PTSD this ability is Negative Bias Scale (NBS), to capture feigned
impaired. In this initial validation study, Morel PTSD. The face validity of the items suggests
reports an overall malingering hit rate of 95.6% in insensitivity to malingering. For example, it seems
a veteran population. In a second small veteran improbable that a patient feigning PTSD would
study the MENT had a hit rate of 75.7% (Morel, endorse wanting to take their clothes off in public.
2008a). It appears, however, that these were neu- Demakis, Gervais, and Rohling (2008) studied
ropsychological and not PTSD referrals. One 301 consecutively referred litigants seen for psy-
MENT validation study (Morel, 2008b) reported chological assessment that included the DAPS,
on an eight-question response bias test, the Quick and both cognitive and psychological SVTs:
Test for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (Q-PTSD). Computerized Assessment of Response Bias
The Q-PTSD classified 81% of a veteran disability (Allen, Conder, Green, & Cox, 1997), the WMT,
claimant group as being credible or not credible and trial 2 of the TOMM (Tombaugh, 1996). The
based on their MENT performance. failure rates for the many SVTs ranged from 35%
The first civilian study using the MENT in (MMPI-FBS) to 4% (MMPI-F), with the DAPS
conjunction with the Structured Inventory of NBS failed by 25%. In all, 29% failed at least one
Malingered Symptomatology (Widows & Smith, cognitive validity measure, and 49% failed at least
2005) and Word Memory Test (WMT) (Green, one psychological validity measure. A concern is
2003) in 61 civilian PTSD claimants revealed that the heterogeneous sample of persons claiming
40% failed the MENT, 51% the SIMS and WMT, different forms of psychological disability, not just
with 25% failing all three (Merten, Thies, PTSD. Further, the significance of different failure
Schneider, & Stevens, 2009). In one other study rates for different SVTs is unclear, because the
that included patients claiming recovered memo- litigating group was so mixed as to etiology.
ries of childhood abuse, patients did no worse on No conclusions can be drawn about comparative
the MENT than those without such memories. All sensitivity of these measures.
participants scored beyond the cutoff for malin-
gering on the MENT. While all patients denied TSI
litigation, it is not known who had PTSD, as this One additional self-report instrument, the Trauma
was not assessed (Geraerts, Jelicic, & Mereckelbach, Symptom Inventory (TSI) merits brief mention
2006). for two reasons (Briere, 1995). First, it is the most
The MENT requires further validation by popular self-report instrument among self-desig-
other researchers in civilians, using a known- nated “traumatologists” (Elhai, Gray, Kashdan, &
groups design. Two additional concerns are the Franklin, 2005a) and has a response bias measure,
rationale behind the MENT and the test instruc- the Atypical Response (ATR) scale. The first ATR
378 forensic neuropsychology

validation study compared an outpatient sample summarized their views on the insensitivity of
believed to show genuine PTSD (N=47) with 63 the ATR: It is a general validity scale that is not
undergraduates simulators. The ATR classifica- designed to detect malingered PTSD, and the TSI
tion rate of 59% (Elhai et al., 2005b), was barely is not tied to the DSM criteria.
above the sample base rate of 55% malingering
(only 4% incremental hits). Another study used Other Scales
just simulation groups and found an overall hit The Miller-Forensic Assessment of Symptoms
rate of 83% (Edens, Otto, Dwyer, 1998). A subse- (Miller, 2001) and the Structured Interview of
quent simulation study suggested that the ATR Reported Symptoms (SIRS) (Rogers, Bagby, &
cut score produced a high false positive rate Dickens, 1992) are two structured interviews
(Rosen et al., 2006). A PTSD combat study identi- developed for the detection of malingering. There
fied 19% of the TSI profiles as invalid based on the are no known-groups studies using these instru-
recommended ATR cutoff. Only one profile was ments in civilian PTSD litigants. However, one
identified based on the Inconsistent Response PTSD veteran study suggested a 53% malingering
scale of the TSI (Nye, Qualls, & Katzman, 2006). incidence rate, with a mean SIRS score of 89 and a
Given, however, that the base rate for malingered CAPS score of 87 (Freeman, Powell, & Kimbrell,
combat-PTSD may be over 50% (see earlier dis- 2008). In a recent simulation study, the SIRS was
cussion of Frueh et al., 2005), the TSI appears administered to an inpatient “severe trauma”
insufficiently sensitive when used in isolation. sample (not necessarily with PTSD) under stan-
In a final study an outpatient PTSD sample, a dard conditions and a volunteer group of feigners
remitted-trauma simulation group that was (Rogers, Payne, Correa, Gillard, & Ross, 2009).
coached to feign PTSD and an uncoached remit- The SIRS false positive rate was unacceptably
ted-trauma simulation group were compared high, but Rogers et al. developed a post hoc
(Efendov, Sellbom, & Bagby 2008). Although the Trauma Index from three SIRS scales, and this
ATR and FBS discriminated the uncoached simu- reduced false positives.
lators from the outpatient PTSD sample, there
was no difference on the ATR between the coached T H E F O R E N S I C E VA L UAT I O N
simulators and patient PTSD group. This implies
the ATR is better able to detect naïve fakers, but “If mental illnesses were rated on the New York
not ones with better education in PTSD symp- Stock Exchange, PTSD would be a growth
toms. The Infrequency scales of the MMPI-2 stock to watch” (Lees-Haley, 1986, p. 17).
(F, FB and FP) were better than the FBS and ATR in
distinguishing coached from PTSD patients under Unlike most other psychiatric diagnoses,
treatment. The limitation of the study was that the PTSD is “incident specific.” Valid diagnosis of
PTSD patient group was workers’ compensation PTSD requires a concrete incident that presum-
claimants, some of whom may have been exagger- ably precipitated the disorder. This makes PTSD a
ating despite best efforts to exclude malingerers, a “litigation friendly” diagnosis, because other
shortcoming acknowledged by the authors. The mental illnesses have multifactorial etiologies.
reliance on financially motivated ex-workers as a Because of the simplistic post hoc ergo propter hoc
“genuine” PTSD group is a recurrent problem in nature of the diagnosis, the forensic examiner
this literature (see also Bury & Bagby, 2002), espe- must reach to other factors to better distinguish
cially given the vague nature of the industrial between legitimate versus feigned PTSD, and
traumas and the unchanged symptoms reported between PTSD and other non-PTSD psychiatric
years later. On the basis of these mixed findings disorders.
being derived from various samples and method- The Practice Guidelines from the International
ologies, and in the absence of known-groups stud- Society for Traumatic Stress Studies (ISTSS)
ies, the TSI cannot be recommended. At most, the provide some assessment recommendations that
above studies tell us the mean ATR scores of a go beyond the simple “after, therefore because
clinical sample (61.60 in Elhai et al. 2005b) and a of ” reasoning (Foa, Keane, Friedman, & Cohen,
litigant sample (64.85 in Efendov et al. 2008) 2008). The recommendations that appear most
“screened” for malingering are very similar, indi- practical in the forensic context are excerpted.
cating either the insensitivity of the ATR to magni- First and foremost, multiple measures and meth-
fication, or the influence of secondary gain in a ods should be employed in assessing the PTSD
putative “genuine” PTSD group. Elhai et al. (2005b) patient. A semi-structured interview should be
Something to Talk About? 379

used. With generally unlimited time constraints “Questioning the link between PTSD and cogni-
for an IME, the use of the CAPS is recommended. tive dysfunction,” Danckwerts and Leathem
DSM-IV-TR linked PTSD self-report measures (2003) review the PTSD literature. Of the 15
should complement the personality testing. The studies they cite, only five are from civilian
MMPI-2 and Personality Assessment Inventory populations, and two of those are from the same
(PAI) are offered as possibilities, and can be help- sample. The authors warn about the generalizabil-
ful in the differential diagnosis of non-PTSD psy- ity of the veteran data to the general population,
chiatric disorders. Finally, malingering should be and conclude by arguing that a more sophisticated
assessed routinely in all clinical and research con- neuropsychological approach that includes
texts and where the potential for malingering is imaging might answer the question. Although we
high, malingering-specific instruments should be agree on the limitations of generalizability from
considered. While this may seem self-evident, the military samples and the utility of some neurop-
assessment of malingering was not addressed in sychological measures, we are doubtful that
the earlier ISTSS guideline (Foa, Keane, & neuroimaging will add incremental validity.
Friedman, 2000). The description of the forensic Also, there is no reason to believe that neuropsy-
evaluation is presented in the order of suggested chological testing in PTSD (or any psychiatric
administration: cognitive and SVT, personality disorder) would serve the purpose of lesion local-
testing, patient interview, self-report-instruments, ization, because there is no “lesion” in PTSD.
and the informant interview if the opportunity Neuropsychological testing serves no other pur-
presents itself. pose than to document the presence and severity
of functional cognitive impairment associated
Cognitive Function in PTSD with either an emotional disturbance or baseline
Issues regarding the neuropsychological etiology (pre-trauma) limitations.
or consequences of PTSD aside, PTSD, like any Misguided attempts to “neurologize” psychia-
other major psychiatric disorder, is frequently try have been encouraged by the advent of
accompanied by cognitive complaints. The typical advanced neuroimaging techniques. Early in the
examination can be short (approximately 2½ era of functional imaging an interesting paper
hours) and include some neuropsychological titled “Frontal Lobology: Psychiatry’s New
tests. This can be comprised of two tests per cog- Pseudoscience” illustrates this point (David,
nitive domain, abbreviated intelligence testing, 1992). Many psychiatric disorders have been sub-
and three or four SVT procedures that include the jected to the scanner. The modal conclusion for
embedded indicators and the preferred personal- most psychiatric disorders is that there is either
ity test. Some cognitive testing is justified for three hypo- or hypermetabolism in the frontal lobes, or
reasons. First, patients with PTSD may present in the cortical and subcortical connections with
with subjective cognitive impairment, especially occasional abnormality in the hippocampus.
concentration and attention problems associated There is virtually no psychiatric population (or
with putative heightened arousal. Second, objec- neurobehavioral disorder) in which the frontal
tive cognitive measures are necessary to substan- lobes have somehow not been implicated, and
tiate these complaints, or assess any functional PTSD is no exception (Francati, Vermetten, &
impact of emotionality. Finally, cognitive testing Bremner, 2007). In neuropsychology the measur-
provides an opportunity to administer SVTs and ing of frontal lobe function can be viewed as a
embedded measures of response bias (covered in second “g factor.” It is a ubiquitous phenomenon
chapter 5 of this text). One might not expect applicable to all patients, the clinical implications
malingered cognitive impairment in PTSD claim- of which are sometimes unknown.
ants, but persons who feign PTSD must also feign Next follows a brief review on the cognitive
functional disability. As an example, between 37% correlates of PTSD diagnoses. Studies included
and 60% of disability-seeking pain patients will are those addressing the cognitive performance
fail the TOMM, even though cognitive dysfunc- of adults with civilian PTSD who are seeking
tion is not at the core of pain complaints (Greve, treatment, or who are prospectively enrolled. For
Etherton, Ord, Bianchini, & Curtis, 2009). the review to be clinically meaningful, and in
The literature on the cognitive correlates of order to apply the level of impairment method,
PTSD shares the same limitation as the MMPI-2 only studies employing commonly used neuro-
literature: The overwhelming majority of the psychological tests with means and standard devi-
research is on veterans. In an article entitled ations reported are included. Some of the studies
380 forensic neuropsychology

cited below contain a normal control group. Two Winn, & Temkin, 1995). In other words, why
additional caveats are that some studies include would a psychological trauma cause more severe
non-English-speaking patients, and that not all impairment than seen in patients with prolonged
studies used a reliable structured interview to coma and radiologically demonstrated structural
identify PTSD. abnormalities in the brain?
When a strict level of impairment analysis is We conclude this section with a discussion of
applied, the results of neuropsychologically based the most interesting cognitive finding that is of
studies support broad conclusions of nonspecific heuristic and clinical value: premorbid cognitive
differences between civilian groups with and function as a predictor of PTSD. As clearly stated
without PTSD. Findings include: PTSD patients by Vasterling and Brailey (2005), there is no good
do worse than trauma patients without PTSD reason to jump to the conclusion that psycholo-
(Jenkins, Langlais, Delis, & Cohen, 2000; Kivling- gical trauma is the cause of mild cognitive deficits.
boden & Sundbom, 2003; Yehuda, Golier, It is reasonable to consider that baseline cognitive
Halligan, & Harvey, 2004); PTSD patients do weaknesses are a risk factor for emotional distur-
worse than trauma patients without PTSD on a bance. Some well-designed studies examined this
plurality to half of the tests (Johnsen, issue. A Vietnam Twin Registry study (2386 twins)
Kanagaratnam, & Asbjørsen, 2008; LaGarde, found a significant dose-response relationship
Doyon, & Brunet, 2010; Matsuo et al., 2003); between precombat cognitive ability and risk for
trauma patients without PTSD do not differ from PTSD (Kremen et al., 2007). After controlling for
normal controls (Jenkins et al., 2000; Yehuda confounders, veterans at the 75th percentile in
et al., 2004); PTSD patients do worse than normal terms of cognitive ability had almost a 50% lower
controls (Bremner et al., 1995; Gil, Calev, risk of developing PTSD than those at the 25th
Greenberg, Kugelmass, & Lerer, 1990; LaGarde percentile. Twins without PTSD had the highest
et al., 2010; Yehuda et al., 2004); those with more cognitive scores, those twins where both had
PTSD symptoms do worse than those with fewer PTSD had the lowest cognitive scores, and PTSD-
symptoms (Brandes et., 2002); and PTSD patients discordant pairs had intermediate scores.
do not differ from non-PTSD psychiatric patients Differences in precombat cognitive ability were
(Gil et al., 1990). Further, this general impairment only evident in the dizygotic pairs discordant for
is relatively mild with low-average mean group PTSD; the monozygotic pairs discordant for
performances at worst. PTSD did not differ on precombat cognitive abil-
Pattern analysis (comparison of cognitive ity. The authors concluded that pre-exposure cog-
domains) shows the most commonly reported nitive ability is a risk or a protective factor for
relative weakness is on verbal memory and timed PTSD, and that the variance in PTSD explained
attention measures (Vasterling & Brailey, 2005). by pre-exposure cognitive ability is accounted for
Other studies showed mixed results for visual entirely by common genetic factors.
memory and visuocognitive function, and lower Another informative Vietnam twin study
IQ. But there has been no demonstration of speci- questions the conclusion that cognitive deficits
ficity on neuropsychological measures in common are attributable to PTSD (Gilbertson et al., 2006).
use: Despite the evidence suggesting greater cog- The study included monozygotic twin pairs in
nitive impairment in PTSD patients, overall gen- which one brother served in combat (exposed)
eral impairment appears to be no greater than and their twin brother did not (unexposed). Four
other patients with mood and anxiety disorders. groups were formed: 19 veterans exposed to
The difference between PTSD and non-PTSD combat with PTSD and their noncombat twin
groups is a statistical difference versus a clinically without PTSD; 24 veterans exposed to combat
meaningful difference. The implication is that who did not develop PTSD and their noncombat
outright defective scores (greater than would be twin without PTSD. The main finding was no
seen by chance alone or falling two standard devi- difference in IQ between twin pairs regardless of
ations below the mean) should raise suspicion of combat exposure, and that PTSD-diagnosed
malingered impairment. While the suggested combat veterans had lower IQ and cognitive
level of impairment method is to compare the scores than non-PTSD veterans on verbal memory
PTSD claimant’s scores with those of the samples and attention, but did not differ on visual memory
above, another approach would be to use a well- or spatial perception measures. The two implica-
defined moderately to severely head-injured tions are: (1) Cognitive capacity, as a familial trait,
group as the comparator (Dikmen, Machamer, influences risk for developing PTSD, and (2) none
Something to Talk About? 381

of the cognitive impairments within the groups few MMPI-2 studies on civilian PTSD, and much
rose to a level that might be considered clinically less on PTSD litigants. In a literature review of the
significant. The only exception was perseverative original MMPI in PTSD patients, out of
errors on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in 21 studies, only three involved civilian popula-
combat veterans with PTSD, but they did not tions; two involved patients in motor vehicle
differ from their unexposed twins. accidents, some of who may have been litigants.
The Gilbertson et al. (2006) findings are The 8–2 profile was the most common two-point
supported by a large epidemiological civilian code (62%), but only in half of the studies did this
study following disastrous brush fires (Parslow & code-type differentiate the PTSD sample from
Jorm, 2007). Through a fortunate coincidence, the the non-PTSD psychiatric sample (Wise, 1996).
baseline cognitive abilities of all subjects had A second limitation is that the very robust somatic
already been assessed twice pre-disaster, as part of malingering profile was meant to identify
a longitudinal study on cognition and mental ill- pseudoneurological and not pseudopsychiatric
ness. Those with higher PTSD symptoms showed symptoms. In line with this argument, the FBS
lower baseline scores on immediate and delayed cut-off for identifying neurological malingering is
memory from the California Verbal Learning much lower than for identifying psychiatric
Test, digit backwards, Symbol Digit Modalities, malingering (see Greiffenstein, Fox, & Lees-Haley,
and an estimate of verbal intelligence (Spot-the- 2007, for further discussion of the moderating
Word-Test). Additionally, while subjects showed effects of psychiatric disturbance on the FBS).
the expected improvement in cognitive function Lees-Haley (1992) compared a “pseudo-PTSD”
from baseline to the second testing as a result of sample (N=55) to a control group (N=64) who
practice effects, those reporting the most PTSD were also litigants claiming to be psychologically
symptoms showed significantly less improvement disabled. The main finding was that original FBS
on the California Verbal Learning Test during the cutoff proposed by the author of 20 was raised to
third wave of testing which occurred after the 24 for men and 26 for women in the case of psy-
fires. There is an inverse relationship between chological injury claims (Lees-Haley, English, &
premorbid cognitive function as measured three Glenn, 1991). While the absence of a nonlitiga-
years before the fires and the presence of PTSD tion PTSD sample is problematic, the study illus-
symptoms: The lower the premorbid cognitive trates the importance of adjusting the FBS to fit
ability, the higher the risk for PTSD symptom the claimed injury. For example, an FBS score as
reporting. The finding of lower attention, process- low as 21 may differentiate MHI litigants from
ing speed and verbal memory as a predictors severely head-injured non-litigants (Larrabee,
of PTSD is in line with the Brewin et al. (2000) 2003; Ross, Millis, Krukowski, Putnam, & Adams,
meta-analysis. 2004). In contrast, the mean FBS score for 61
DSM diagnosed PTSD work injury claimants
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality all of whom were seeking or receiving workers’
Inventory-2 compensation benefits was 26.31 (Bury & Bagby,
The gold standard for personality assessment in 2002).
forensic matters is the MMPI-2. It has the most A third limitation is that many PTSD MMPI-2
research, can show distinctive litigant profiles, studies are simulation studies. The limitations of
and has validity scales. Among litigants exagger- studies that employ this method (e.g., Bury &
ating neurological symptoms, the “somatic malin- Bagby, 2002) have been addressed (Greiffenstein
gering” profile, consisting of marked elevations in et al., 2004). Greiffenstein et al. (2004) formed a
the Hypocondriasis and Hysteria scales, has been probable post-traumatic stress group (Prob-PTS)
the most robust finding among the clinical scales that was clinically referred, and an improbable
(Larrabee, 1998), while the FBS has been the best post-traumatic stress group (Imp-PTS) of liti-
measure of malingering among the validity scales gants. For the Prob-PTS group, their event was
(Nelson, Hoelzle, Sweet, Arbisi, & Demakis, outside the realm of usual human experience.
2010). We will focus only on the MMPI-2. At the They were seen within five months of the event
time of this writing there are no published PTSD rather than the “delayed PTSD” of the Imp-PTS
studies on the MMPI-2-RF (Ben-Porath & group seen two years post-event. This contrast
Tellegen, 2008). between groups is the strength of the study.
The MMPI-2 PTSD literature contains a A third group consisted of litigants with old but
number of limitations. The first is that there are major traumas (Lit-MPTS). The mean FBS scores
382 forensic neuropsychology

of the Prob-PTS, Imp-PTS, and the Lit-MPTS identifying chronic PTSD (42%), and identified
groups are 20, 28, and 24, respectively. A score of 83% of panic disorder patients as true negatives
30 or greater suggests a high probability of malin- (Gaston, Brunet, Koszycki, & Bradwejn, 1996). Of
gering regardless of the base-rate level. The note is that the authors contend that no patient
F-family of scales was not sensitive (F, F(p), and was involved in litigation. The authors conclude
F-K) in differentiating the groups. A limitation that clinical and forensic evaluations of PTSD
acknowledged by the authors is that the majority patients should not employ assessment proce-
of the Prob-PTS group sustained their injury in dures that have been validated on Vietnam veter-
the workplace, making it compensable despite the ans.
denial of litigation. The absence of a structured In a follow-up to the latter study, the same
PTSD interview or self-report instrument is also a group using the same sample subjected all the
limitation. It is not known which patients actually MMPI items for analysis in an effort to create a
met criteria for PTSD. new scale that might identify acute and chronic
Aside from FBS, a general validity measure, a PTSD (Gaston, Brunet, Koszycki, & Bradwejn,
PTSD-specific malingering scale has been identi- 1998). A 32-item MMPI acute PTSD scale and a
fied. The Infrequency-Posttraumatic Stress 41-item chronic PTSD scale were developed that
Disorder (Fptsd) scale was developed to identify had a hit rate of 83% and 75% respectively in com-
malingered PTSD (Elhai et al., 2002). The disad- parison to the panic disorder sample. The authors
vantage of the scale is that it was initially devel- listed the corresponding MMPI-2 items that make
oped on Vietnam veterans. The results are mixed up both scales. Of note was that only seven of the
as to their applicability in civilian PTSD. In the items from the PK scale overlapped with the
Elhai et al. (2004) study the Fptsd possessed incre- chronic PTSD scale, and four items for the acute
mental validity beyond the traditional F-related PTSD scale, clearly supporting the earlier conten-
validity scales in discriminating malingered from tion of the authors that combat PTSD is not gen-
genuine PTSD. In a simulation study using college eralizable to civilian PTSD (Gaston et al., 1996).
students simulating PTSD and sexual abuse vic- There appears to have been no attempt to replicate
tims, the Fptsd demonstrated some incremental these promising findings. In another civilian study
validity over F but not over Fp. The authors con- the PK scale failed to detect those with and with-
cluded that the Fptsd might be more appropriate out PTSD. Unfortunately, the sample was seen in
for combat PTSD the context of work-related psychological injuries
In addition to identifying malingered PTSD, (Scheibe, Bagby, Miller, & Dorian, 2001). A review
the MMPI-2 has been employed in identifying by Lyons and Wheeler-Cox (1999), however, sug-
PTSD. The 49-item MMPI PK scale was devel- gests that while the MMPI-2 may be helpful in
oped with Vietnam era veterans to identify PTSD distinguishing PTSD from those without a psy-
(Keane, Malloy, & Fairbank, 1984). The scale was chiatric disorder, it does not do well in distin-
modified in the MMPI-2 by the elimination of guishing PTSD from other psychiatric disorders.
three items that were repeated in the original scale The above mixed results would suggest that the
but are now presented only once (Lyons & Keane, PK scale has limited value in ruling in PTSD due
1992). This resulted in a new recommended cutoff to poor specificity; on the other hand, nonelevated
for combat veterans of 28 instead of 30. The first scores may be useful as one indicator of the
attempt to validate the PK scale in a civilian absence of PTSD.
sample compared two small PTSD samples: the In addition to the MMPI-2, the PAI has also
validation (N=18) and a cross-validation sample been recommended by the ISTSS guidelines. No
(N=15) with a control sample of psychiatric known-group designs using the PAI or the Millon
patients representative of a typical outpatient Clinical Multiaxial Personality Inventory-III
practice (N=35). A cut score of 19 resulted in an exist.
87% and 88% hit rate in the validation and cross- In summary, the FBS has most promise in
validation samples, respectively (Koretzky & Peck, identifying malingered PTSD symptoms. Whether
1990). The scale correctly classified 80% of bat- the PK scale has any ability in identifying civilian
tered women with and without PTSD using a PTSD from non-PTSD conditions remains an
cutoff of 22 (Perrin, Van Hasselt, & Hersen, 1997). open question. Since the accepted standard pro-
In a large study of outpatient treatment seeking cedure for identifying PTSD remains the inter-
civilians, the PK scale did not identify acute PTSD view, the use of any MMPI-2 scale in the
(hit rate 18%), was only moderately successful in identification of PTSD may very well be a moot
Something to Talk About? 383

point, although absence of evidence for signifi- objective testing, especially the cognitive testing
cant problems with anxiety and depression on the in the case of PTSD. Should they decide not to
MMPI-2 could provide data contradicting the feign the testing, they are now put in a catch-22
presence of PTSD. situation whereby they must somehow communi-
cate disability and/or impairment. These patients
T H E PAT I E N T I N T E RV I E W convey their impairment via the interview only.
The semi-structured interview is the cornerstone It is a rare situation that a patient feigns impair-
of the PTSD evaluation. Before discussing the ment on formal testing and does not communi-
mechanics of the interview, two prerequisite cate this impairment via the interview. Symptoms
points need to be emphasized. First, there is the that suggest disability (and malingering when
patently obvious caution regarding self-report: present in an exaggerated or atypical nature) are
“Self-report is problematic because it communi- virtually always communicated in the interview,
cates a personal experience that is not falsifiable” but not always on the testing. Once the patient
(Greiffenstein & Baker, 2008, p. 566). Falsifiability conveys their symptoms, a good interviewer
is the essence of science and is also a requirement takes control of the interview and examines each
in deciding what constitutes admissible expert symptom to determine its origin. For example,
testimony in some legal venues (i.e., Daubert). the most common movement disorder is tremor.
Subjective evidence under the control of the No competent neurologist will simply record
respondent does not provide sufficient evidence the tremor as absent or present without charac-
to declare a person impaired or disabled. It is a terizing it and reaching a diagnosis. A final
tenuous contention at best to say “with a reason- caveat about the recommended interview format
able degree of medical certainty” that a patient described below is that unlike objective testing
has a psychiatric disorder when the sole basis is there is, by definition, some variability among cli-
the patient’s self-report. The prerequisite second nicians in how to approach the interview. It is
point concerns how the subjectivity of the patient hoped that the reader will find the suggestions
interview can be overcome. As in medicine, herein useful.
the most critical part of any clinical evaluation While there may be advantages to doing the
(including neuropsychological) is the clinical patient interview first for purposes of building
interview and history of the illness. While test rapport, particularly if the interview begins with
administration requires only literacy, arriving at a a discussion of the traumatic event, four reasons
diagnosis via the interview implies knowledge of are offered for conducting the PTSD interview at
syndromes. It is exceedingly common to find the completion of the testing. If the intent is to
recorded in the majority of neuropsychological malinger, the more information the examiner has
evaluations a list of symptoms the patient reported ahead of the interview (both medical records
without any effort by the interviewer to char- and testing) works to the disadvantage of the
acterize these symptoms in a manner that would claimant. Second, if the testing is malingered
result in a diagnosis independent of any other outright, the clinical interview can be shortened if
information, namely the actual testing. This time is an issue, unless there is independent
phenomenon has been referred to as the “bean medical record evidence supporting presence of
counting” of symptoms (Bielauskas, 1999). PTSD (e.g., ER evidence of significant post-
In order for patients involved in litigation to accident emotional distress requiring sedation in
be compensated for an injury they must report a litigant who then malingers, but also may have
symptoms that produce impairment. In the neu- PTSD). Third, the IME context provides for the
ropsychological evaluation, the patient may man- collection of extensive records where the basic
ifest this impairment via cognitive and personality medical, social, psychiatric, and injury history is
testing. Should a patient decide to feign testing, already available. Finally, at the conclusion of a
knowing what to feign is difficult. It requires well-done interview, the patient who malingers
knowledge of what the tests measure, and what the interview may sense that they did not provide
pattern and level of performance one might see a believable constellation of symptoms. If the
depending on the disorder under consideration. interview were done at the onset, and with the
Additionally, an examinee may be aware that patient feeling they may not have provided a
SVTs are administered. Consequently, from the reasonable narrative, they might take a more
perspective of the examinee, a cautious approach conservative approach to the upcoming testing.
to the evaluation would be to not malinger the This may result in less sensitivity in detecting
384 forensic neuropsychology

malingering, when present, on the objective psy- conclusion of the neuropsychological interview
chological testing. the patient is asked to describe, in detail, the
The examiner should begin with a neurop- PTSD-causing event without any prompting or
sychological interview similar to the one typi- questioning from the examiner.
cally done when assessing for the presence of a Before the CAPS interview begins the exam-
neurobehavioral disorder. Since a portion of the iner has some idea from the neuropsychological
PTSD evaluation will involve cognitive testing, interview as to the presence of PTSD. Did the
the results of the testing need to be corroborated patient volunteer symptoms of PTSD when asked
by a neuropsychological interview. One suggested to list their symptoms? Were there any signs of
format is for the interview to begin by asking the distress as they were describing the PTSD-causing
patient to volunteer the symptoms they are expe- event? Did the patient endorse symptoms of major
riencing or in what ways they act or behave differ- depression or general anxiety? A lifetime history
ently, without necessarily mentioning the trauma. of at least one other psychiatric disorder is found
Here it is important for the patient to volunteer an in roughly 80% of PTSD patients (Kessler et al.,
exhaustive list. At this point it is not necessary for 1995). Were symptoms consistent with panic
them to give examples of the symptoms or elabo- attack or generalized anxiety disorder endorsed?
rate on them; this should be reserved for the direct Was the neuropsychological interview malin-
symptom inquiry phase of the interview. The vol- gered?
unteering of these symptoms results in a bi-modal Some patients prepared for a PTSD interview
distribution. There are a few patients who come are perplexed by questions regarding cognitive,
prepared with an exhaustive list. In contrast, there motor, and sensory symptoms they had not
are those that volunteer few symptoms, yet the planned on answering or rehearsed. As an exam-
symptoms volunteered initially can mushroom ple, a patient who experienced a significant trau-
during the direct symptom inquiry. Volunteering matic event asked two or three times during the
the symptoms of a disorder one does not have is neuropsychological interview why he was not
difficult. It is made easy for the patient by intro- being asked about his accident; not something
ducing the purpose of the interview (as instructed one suffering from PTSD would normally insist
by the CAPS interview module), and volunteering on discussing. They must now decide on the spot
the symptoms for the patient so they could admit if these are symptoms they should endorse. Since
or deny them. Volunteering symptoms, direct it is not likely that they have endorsed non-PTSD
symptom inquiry, and self-report instruments symptoms before, providing the onset and course
(administered in that order) can result in an of these symptoms, much less examples, becomes
increasing number of reported symptoms with problematic, resulting in a stilted and awkward
each successive method in litigants with question- narrative. There is some evidence that unexpected
ably valid PTSD claims. questions increase the ability of the interviewer to
Following the volunteering of all symptoms, detect malingering (Vrij et al., 2009).
the structured symptom inquiry portion of the Elhai et al. (2005a) surveyed members of the
interview begins with the neuropsychological ISTSS on the instruments they most commonly
interview. This involves posing direct questions use to assess traumatic events. The “gold standard”
regarding cognitive, motor/sensory, and non- for interview-based PTSD assessment is the
PTSD psychiatric symptoms. The patient is asked CAPS. This is by far the most commonly used
if they have the symptom, the onset, and if the instrument by both researchers and clinicians.
symptom has gotten better, worse, or the same The CAPS is the only PTSD interview that assesses
since the onset. Randomly mixed in with the both frequency and severity of symptoms. The
motor and sensory symptoms, the examiner Structured Clinical Interview for DSM- IV (SCID)
should inquire about all of the symptoms that is the second most common, though three times
make up panic disorder. Mixing in physical symp- the number of traumatologists preferred the
toms of an anxiety disorder in the midst of basic CAPS to the SCID (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, &
neurological symptoms disguises the intent. The Williams, 1997). However, one disadvantage to
physiological concomitants of PTSD symptoms the CAPS is that it takes longer to give (as long as
are addressed in the CAPS interview where the an hour) when you include the initial prompts
intent of the interview is more obvious. There is and follow-up questions. Aside from the time it
no reason why the examiner cannot inquire takes to give, some amount of practice and
about the same symptom more than once. At the training to become comfortable with the test is
Something to Talk About? 385

required. The extensive research on the CAPS has that of well-defined PTSD groups from published
been summarized (Weathers, Kean, & Davidson, studies.
2001). We recommend the CAPS as the preferred With regard to PTSD symptoms it is critical
structured interview in forensic evaluations. It is that the patient be asked to provide concrete exam-
beyond the scope of this chapter to cover the ples of their symptoms. For the patient who is
CAPS in detail. We will focus only on how the malingering, the examiner will appreciate the pro-
administration should be tailored for the IME. longed pauses, vague examples, no examples, lack
The interview portion of the PTSD evaluation of detail, the contradictory answers, “I don’t know”
needs to be carefully tailored and planned to the answers, or even nonresponsive answers that might
circumstances of the claimant’s history (namely suggest that the patient does not have PTSD. The
medical records since the trauma), is labor inten- patient attempting to feign PTSD may have some
sive, detailed, and can possibly last well over an awareness in the second or third hour of the inter-
hour. It goes without saying that in an IME each view that their narrative may not be believable. It is
test is not introduced to the patient in a manner then not uncommon for revealing statements such
that makes it clear what the test is measuring. as “to tell you the truth, doc,” “to be honest, doc,”
Similarly, one does not preface a PTSD interview “this is difficult to explain,” or “I know this is not
in such a fashion so as to make its purpose obvi- making sense” may be volunteered.
ous. Per the CAPS manual, the interview is intro- Two PTSD symptoms merit special attention:
duced by telling the patient that they are going to flashbacks and nightmares. Both of these symp-
be asked about potentially traumatic events, and toms should be treated with scrutiny as they are
to identify the one that is the most stressful, which portrayed in the popular media as symptoms of
then becomes the focus of the CAPS interview. In PTSD. The onset, course, frequency, and nature of
the IME context, this introduction is not neces- these symptoms should be fully explored includ-
sary since the PTSD causing event is known. ing detailed examples. In a recent PTSD IME, the
When asked to describe the PTSD-causing event whole of the patient’s PTSD consisted of flash-
at the conclusion of the neuropsychological inter- backs and nightmares. The PTSD criteria were
view, did the patient volunteer symptoms of fear, not even met on the PTSD self-report instrument.
helplessness, or horror? If yes, Criterion A-2 has The patient reported the dream as always being an
been met. If they do not volunteer these symp- exact replication of the event. This is improbable.
toms, per the CAPS instructions, they are asked As reported by McNally (2004), the majority of
how they responded emotionally to the event, and patients asked to prospectively record their night-
finally asked directly about A-2 symptoms. As to mares dream about events related to the trauma
Criterion A-1, whether the trauma is of sufficient instead of “instant replays” of the trauma (p.7). As
severity to cause PTSD, the examiner should for flashbacks, it is commonly overlooked that
accept it as meeting A-1 criteria no matter how these were described as rare in DSM-III when
trivial. While we are of the belief that the event they were introduced, and have continued to be
should be outside the realm of everyday human considered rare in the current DSM-IV-TR. PTSD
experience, in the discussion section of the IME historians have offered a plausible explanation for
report the reader should be made aware of the this rarity. They have convincingly argued that
controversy surrounding the stressor criterion. flashbacks were rarely described in wars preced-
The trier of fact can decide if the event is of suffi- ing the Vietnam War (Jones et al. 2003). The flash-
cient severity to produce PTSD. The examiner back was actually a metaphor borrowed from the
should then go through the CAPS interview film industry and blended into the newly coined
making sure to cover all the questions. Item 22 of PTSD construct (Frankel, 1994; Lembcke, 1998).
the CAPS requires the examiner to estimate the Brewin (2003) summarized the argument of the
overall validity of the interview, but provides no skeptics (versus “saviors”) that inventing the term
direction on how to do this in some objective PTSD was not necessary since the symptoms of
reproducible manner. CAPS items that were the PTSD patient can be found in already existing
denied in the previous interviews are skipped and diagnostic categories; flashbacks are the only
those previously reported are elaborated on via symptom unique to PTSD.
the prompts outlined in the CAPS booklet. The There are additional observations that may
CAPS is then scored in order to obtain a severity suggest malingered PTSD. As currently defined,
score, and the level of impairment method is almost all persons have had at least one event that
applied by comparing the patient’s CAPS score to can meet A-1 criteria as being life threatening.
386 forensic neuropsychology

The CAPS and some self-report instruments (he had a prior psychiatric history). Both men
provide an opportunity to identify stressful events were examined two years and one year after their
aside from the one that is the focus of the IME. traumas. Neither met the criteria for PTSD by
There may be instances where past events that are their own report. In the third IME, a women who
normally outside the realm of usual human expe- sustained a laceration of her finger at work did not
rience did not produce PTSD, but the trivial one, report any PTSD symptoms until six months after
which is the subject of the IME, did. In one the injury, and, after aggressive treatment, had a
instance, a patient developed delayed PTSD fol- CAPS score of 63 (severe PTSD) 17 months after
lowing an electrical injury, but suffered no such the injury. The only similarity is that all three
symptoms when during his teenage years he wit- patients claimed to have experienced significant
nessed the traumatic amputation of his father’s peritraumatic stress at the time of the trauma
leg, which was followed by a 20-minute attempt to based on a self-report PTSD instrument, with the
rescue him. That noncompensable event produced two male patients understandably reporting
no PTSD symptoms. In one case that went to trial, higher elevations. Of note, medical records con-
a Vietnam veteran denied any symptoms of PTSD temporaneous with the accidents documented
from his combat experience, none following a typical PTSD symptoms in the two men, but did
MVA that was litigated in which he sustained not support the presence of these symptoms in the
some injuries, but did report PTSD following a woman until six months post-trauma. Moreover,
second MVA in which he sustained no demon- only in the female patient did the symptoms per-
strable injury. sist. Any nonimprovement and/or worsening of
It stands to reason that any person experienc- symptoms should be viewed with skepticism.
ing a truly horrific event has a goal of forgetting Nonimprovement in symptoms is sometimes seen
that event and moving on with their lives, while in the face of treatment that has gone way beyond
for the litigating patient the maintenance of symp- the proven time-limited therapy that is recom-
toms is necessary. It is important to remember mended for PTSD. In the case of rape, prospective
that the natural course of any sudden traumatic treatment studies show that after 6 weeks of treat-
event, be it neurological (e.g., stroke or head ment there is a marked improvement in CAPS
injury) or psychological is recovery. Even in the scores (Resick, Nishith, Weaver, Astin & Feuer,
most common of civilian traumatic events such as 2002). In addition, one may find that the CAPS
rape, recovery is the rule. While over 90% may be score of the litigant receiving longer treatment
diagnosed with PTSD within two weeks after the exceeds the mean CAPS score of rape victims who
rape, by week 12 less than half (47%) have PTSD have received one course of time-limited treat-
(Rothbaum, Foa, Riggs, Murdoch, & Walsh, ment, followed by recovery.
1992). Similarly, DSM-IV-TR reports that half of The interview of a family member or an infor-
the cases of PTSD show full recovery within three mant may or may not be needed, depending on
months (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). the situation, or may not be possible. In some
As already noted, PTSD in its original form, gross instances the informant might refuse the inter-
stress reaction, was viewed as a temporary condi- view or the attorney representing the patient may
tion. Even for litigants who experience terrible deny access to a family member. In an IME con-
events, the need to forget them may outweigh the text the informant interview serves three pur-
need to be compensated. poses. First, failure to conduct such an interview
By chance, in the course of a single week, three is sometimes raised in deposition, or trial testi-
separate PTSD IMEs were conducted by J. A. in mony as reflecting a shortcoming of the IME.
three cities within one state. In the first instance, a Family members are frequently included in the
driver of a compact car, in a successful suicide legal process by the plaintiff ’s side to convey how
attempt, rammed his car head-on into the semi- the patient has been affected by the injury or
truck of the patient. The driver of the car died, and event. A defense expert might want to avail him
the patient and his wife barely escaped from the or herself of the same opportunity so as to avoid
cab of the semi-truck before it was engulfed in the accusation that they have ignored important
flames. In the second incident, a driver employed collateral information. For some examiners the
by the city accidentally hit and killed a pedestrian. informant interview may serve this latter purpose
Both men had immediate PTSD symptoms, sought versus gathering any information that would add
immediate help, including two inpatient treat- to the evaluation from a diagnostic and treatment
ments for the patient that killed the pedestrian standpoint. If it is determined that the patient is
Something to Talk About? 387

malingering, the interview of the informant would (i.e., suggesting symptoms that are later presented
add little as it does not negate the fact that the during the interview). For this reason, these are
patient is malingering. The exception would be a the only tests that are given at the completion of
case in which malingering is present, but there is the interview. The incidence of PTSD can vary by
also evidence of legitimate PTSD. Second, the two-fold depending on whether a self-report
informant can provide useful information about checklist is used versus the standard interview,
the psychological reaction of the patient to a gen- especially if litigation is present (Sumpter &
uine trauma. Informant interviews, whether clini- McMillan, 2005). Completion of these instruments
cal or within a medicolegal context, should always allows for the evaluation of discordant informa-
be done in the absence of the patient. tion between personality testing, the patient, and
In conclusion, there are some points regarding informant interviews. In one example, a patient
the PTSD interview that need to be emphasized. was asked in an open-ended format to describe her
First, regardless of the fact that some forensic emotional response to the accident; intense fear,
neuropsychologists might not rank the clinical helplessness, and horror were not mentioned
interview as a high priority in the IME process, it (including in the medical records). Even when
is nevertheless the method by which PTSD is asked directly in the CAPS interview she denied
diagnosed. The interview needs to be planned and them. It was only after the interview when this
carefully executed proceeding from open-ended question was asked on a self-report instrument
to more structured questioning. Second, when that she admitted to experiencing these emotions.
malingering is present, the results of the interview Immediately recognizing the contradiction, she
need to be conveyed in the report in a way that stated, “This is going to contradict some of the
communicates the high likelihood of malinger- things we talked about now that I read them.”
ing, regardless of the formal testing. The interview Choosing among PTSD self-report instruments
of the patient and informant should stand on their is not as straightforward as the use of the CAPS.
own. Points arguing the case for a malingered Excluding measures designed for combat PTSD,
interview should be numbered and lettered for there are two instruments from the top ten that
the purposes of allowing the reader to easily iden- are preferred by ISTSS members (Elhai et al.,
tify the exact reasons that argue for the opinions 2005a): the PTSD Checklist (PCL) (Weathers, Litz,
expressed in the report. An interview is easier to Herman, Huska, & Keane (1993), and the Posttrau-
explain to a referral source or a jury than is a test. matic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS) (Foa, 1995).
Regrettably, there is more to say about the clinical Both are used by 16% of society members. The PCL
interview that cannot be conveyed in this chapter takes less time to give. It has 17 items (versus 49 for
due to space limitations. the PDS), is free, and has a Spanish version (Miles,
Marshall, & Schell, 2008). The PDS has the advan-
PTSD Self-Report Measures tage of assessing all symptoms of PTSD, not just the
While easily malingered, a self-report instrument 17 core DSM symptoms, and can be computer or
has the advantage of quantifying what the patient hand scored. The DAPS ranks third (9% of mem-
reports, as opposed to the interview, which may bers). The DAPS, like the PDS, assesses both DSM
be argued is more subjectively interpreted. The and non-DSM symptoms. A possible advantage of
self-report instrument should not be viewed by the DAPS over the PDS is the presence of a response
the examiner with skepticism, but instead as pro- bias scale. As the DAPS is a relatively new scale,
viding the patient with an opportunity to either there is little published research, which makes it a
accurately report or over-report symptoms in major drawback. This precludes the use of the level
the easiest and clearest way. This then allows for of impairment method. One seemingly attractive
the implementation of the level of impairment feature of the DAPS and PDS is the normative data
method, provided that there has been sufficient provided in the test manuals.
research on the instrument in well-defined PTSD However, as Rosen (2004) has tirelessly
clinical samples. PTSD self-report measures that emphasized, one has to assume that every PTSD
are tied to the DSM criteria are recommended by database is at least partially contaminated by
the ISTSS treatment guidelines (Foa et al., 2008). malingering. As an example, the normative data
The need to use a DSM-linked measure effectively for the PDS is comprised of 242 subjects with
rules out the most popular trauma scale, the TSI. mixed traumas, 27.8% of whom are combat veter-
Giving these self-report instruments after ans. The employment status of the normative
the interview prevents “indirect coaching” sample suggests the possibility of secondary gain:
388 forensic neuropsychology

36.8% were unemployed and 15.4% disabled. physician. When the patient is inaccurately diag-
When the PDS was published 15 years ago, trau- nosed with PTSD this may perpetuate a sequence
matologists rarely addressed malingered PTSD in of iatrogenic consequences that are set in motion
published studies. Even less is known about the not necessarily by the PTSD claimant seeking
DAPS normative data, and it appears to be less compensation, but by a doctor only trying to help.
generalizable than the PDS norms. The PDS The pitfalls in the diagnosis of PTSD are ubiqui-
normative sample was recruited through clinical tous errors made by presumably knowledgeable
and research centers while the DAPS sample mental health care professionals. It goes without
was recruited through mailed advertisements, saying that a clinician who does not specialize
after which the questionnaires were mailed in psychiatric disorders cannot begin to consider
out. The sample included 70 university students. the complexities involved in making a diagnosis
Additionally, the PDS has two other advantages of PTSD. The near constant coverage by the
over the DAPS: it measures impairment in func- media of the psychological effects of the Middle
tioning and has more research behind it. This East conflicts serves to make health care profes-
arguably makes the PDS the preferable self-report sionals “PTSD hypervigilant” so as to not miss the
instrument. diagnosis.
Self-report instruments of depression and Two cases illustrate this iatrogenic hypervigi-
anxiety should also be administered. The Patient lance. In the first instance the patient was involved
Health Questionniare-9 (Kroenke, Spitzer, & in a MVA resulting in no injury, except for feeling
Williams, 2001) and Generalized Anxiety somewhat “skittish” when he drives. He relayed
Disorder-7 (Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams, & Löwe, this skittishness to his internist, an army reserve
2006) are recommended. These brief measures are officer just returning from the Middle East who
publicly available, and assess not only the severity indicated, “We see a lot of this over in Iraq,” and
of these conditions, but also whether the DSM- diagnosed him with PTSD. Once the diagnosis
IV-TR criteria for either condition are met. This found its way into the medical record, the insur-
will also assess the presence of comorbid condi- ance company balked at offering treatment. The
tions that may or may not have been reported in patient was not sure what to do, and the case
the neuropsychological interview. dragged on for two years until the IME was com-
pleted. When the patient was told he did not have
I AT R O G E N I C PTSD, he was relieved. In another example, a
P T S D , T R E AT M E N T patient with a preexisting chronic and severe his-
R E C O M M E N D AT I O N S , tory of depression was involved in a MVA in
A N D C A S E C O N S U LTAT I O N which she sustained no injury. The case took a
sudden turn when her family doctor, who had
“It was later during unrelated activity that seen her many times before and since her acci-
I bumped into a description of posttraumatic dent, saw that she was not getting better. In the
stress disorder and thought to myself ‘Whoa, second year post injury he added PTSD to her
this guy has a lot of these symptoms’” (Stein, depression diagnosis. Unlike depression, the
2002, p. 1514). PTSD incident-specific diagnosis changed the
direction of her litigation in the mind of her attor-
Competent consultation may go a long way in ney. This prompted an IME at the request of the
correcting any misconceptions about the case and defense, which resulted in a diagnosis of malin-
directing the referral source to the proper health gering, and in the trial the jury found for the
care professional or resource, recommending defense. The malingering unnecessarily arose
treatment, or an IME if necessary. The recom- when the patient was indirectly called upon by the
mendations below derive from evidence-based diagnosis of her physician to now present with
approaches to the treatment of PTSD and our symptoms of PTSD, which she never had, and
suggestions where no guidelines exist. which she could not successfully malinger.
It is in the ethics code, and hopefully in the These two instances of well-intentioned
nature of every health care professional to want to doctors may appear to be exceptions, but in fact,
help their patient. The first step in that process is a physician gatekeepers are forever called upon to
correct diagnosis from which proper treatment be cognizant of every possible malady that might
follows. The frontline health care professional, ail the patient, including PTSD. It is one thing to
making most diagnoses, is the primary care recognize and refer, but mere recognition is not
Something to Talk About? 389

the sole precondition for competency. The above (FDA) as they are the gatekeeper for drug approval
quote from Stein (2002) comes from a Journal of in the United States. In order for the FDA to
the American Medical Association article. The approve a medication they require at least two
Clinical Crossroads feature of the journal offers randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled
teaching points via clinical vignettes. In this arti- trials. The only FDA-approved medications for
cle a primary care physician made a diagnosis of the treatment of PTSD are Paxil (paroxetine) and
PTSD after “stumbling upon” the diagnosis in the Zoloft (sertraline). A Cochrane meta-analysis has
course of some “unrelated activity.” The recom- demonstrated the efficacy of the selective sero-
mendation of the article is for the primary care tonin reuptake inhibitors, the most often studied
physician to actively tackle this diagnosis. As for of the medications used to treat PTSD (Stein,
litigation, the article acknowledged, “. . . misuse of Ipser, & Sedat, 2006). Per the NICE (2005) guide-
the diagnosis for monetary gain may occur on lines, pharmacotherapy should not be offered as
occasion . . .” (Stein, 2002, p. 1517). The case dis- first-line treatment over psychotherapy unless,
cussant predicted a good outcome for the patient, per the Australian guidelines a) psychotherapy
Mr. M., with no treatment recommended (neither has proved ineffective; b) the patient refuses ther-
pharmacological or psychotherapy). In an update apy; c) it is used to stabilize the patient before
to this case, two years later, we find Mr. M. par- therapy is begun in severe cases; d) it is used to
tially symptomatic and seeing a therapist who was lessen the symptoms that might be exacerbated by
initially consulted because of the accident (Ship, psychotherapy (Forbes, et al., 2009). Concomitant
2004). Could this case have been managed differ- short-term pharmacological treatment should
ently? We will first begin with the evidenced also be considered for any sleep problems, and
based-treatment recommendations followed by a should be continued for 12 months before gradual
discussion on how to assist a referral source in withdrawal. Before it is determined whether
managing the PTSD claim. Unlike the treatment psychotherapy or medication is needed, “watchful
recommendations, our forensic case consultation waiting” is recommended in the first month
suggestions are based primarily on experience. during the acute stress disorder phase. A follow-
In making treatment recommendations, we up at one month is suggested at which time treat-
decided to draw from as many different official ment is considered. The Cochrane review by
guidelines and sources as possible in an effort to Bisson and Andrew (2007) recommends trauma-
provide cumulative evidence for a certain recom- focused cognitive behavioral therapy (TFCBT),
mendation. While treatment recommendations eye movement desensitization and reprocessing
typically are not sought in forensic evaluations of (EMDR), and stress management. There is some
civil litigants or private disability claimants, such indication that TFCBT and EMDR are superior to
recommendations are not uncommon in workers’ stress management.
compensation cases. Additionally, not all recom- While EMDR is recognized as a treatment for
mendations are uniform and we find some recom- PTSD, it is not without controversy. The contro-
mendations more useful and user friendly than versy is partially due to some early, arguably
others. Our recommendations have to be tem- unfounded claims regarding its efficacy, atheoreti-
pered, and thus modified by the fact that they are cal nature, and current lack of empirical support
being made within a medicolegal and not a clinical for treating certain populations (children and
context. The recommendations we include come combat PTSD) and multitrauma PTSD (Rubin,
from the ISTSS (Foa et al., 2008), the practice 2003). As TFBCT has more data and is grounded
guidelines of the American Psychiatric Association in cognitive behavior therapy, it might be a prefer-
(Benedek, Friedman, Zatzick, & Ursano, 2009), able treatment modality. Clinicians with experi-
the Cochrane Reviews (Bisson & Andrew, 2007; ence in the treatment of PTSD, and who have
Stein, Ipser, & Seedat, 2006) the Australian Center been trained in these techniques should conduct
for Posttraumatic Mental Health (Forbes, these therapies. TFCBT should be 8 to 12 sessions
Wolfgang, & Cooper, 2009), and the guidelines in length, conducted once a week. Consideration
published by the National Institute for Health and can be given to extending the 12 sessions if there
Clinical Excellence (NICE) in Britain, which we are comorbid or other psychosocial problems that
highly recommend (National Institute for Health are contributing. The focus of therapy should be
and Clinical Excellence, 2005). primarily on the trauma.
The simplest of recommendations to make Since symptoms of PTSD generally improve
comes from the Federal Drug Administration with time, when a patient does not respond to
390 forensic neuropsychology

treatment it generally means one of three things: a non-doctoral-level therapist cannot benefit the
the diagnosis is not accurate and the patient is patient, but all things being equal, it is hard to
being treated for the wrong thing, the patient’s argue against having a person with a terminal
symptoms are refractory to treatment and/or the degree treat the patient. While one cannot predict
patient is not getting better because they are not how long treatment might take, requesting a time-
truly having the symptoms, or they are not as line from the therapist does two things. First, a
severe as the patient reports. therapist agreeing to at least a tentative timeline is
The above recommendations presume a clini- a tacit understanding on the part of the therapist
cal context in which the goal of the patient is to and patient that the treatment of choice for PTSD
get better. The absence of improvement, the wors- is time limited and trauma focused. Second, a
ening of symptoms, having not returned to work, timeline creates an expectation that the patient
or consulting with an attorney usually invites con- will improve, and puts the burden on both the
sultation and recommendations that are more patient and therapist to work toward that end. The
fitting with what is now a medicolegal context. patient should have only one psychotherapist in
Table 14.2 offers an approach to managing the addition to the physician providing the medica-
PTSD claim within a medicolegal context that is tion. The patient’s attorney may insist on another
based on nothing more than personal experience. or second therapist but this should be resisted
Frequently, it is the absence of improvement in unless it is the patient’s wish. It is possible that a
symptoms that call for case consultation or an new therapist is justified if the patient has not
IME. If the patient has not improved on medica- reached a level of comfort with the therapist that
tion, treatment should be transferred to a psychia- will allow therapy to progress in a successful
trist for more aggressive management. While the manner. A second therapist request should signal
insurer, employer, or workers’ compensation the need for an IME before treatment with the
nurse might be accustomed to choosing a certain new therapist begins.
doctor in the case of surgery or other common- It is not uncommon, in the case of a physical
place medical procedure, the patient should be injury or after surgery, for the insurer to ask for an
allowed to choose a psychotherapist, preferably a impairment rating from the treating physician.
doctoral-level psychologist familiar with the treat- This should never be asked of the therapist as this
ment of PTSD and TFCBT. This is not to say that creates an irreconcilable conflict for them, which

TABLE 14.2 PTSD CASE CONSULTATION

What to recommend regarding treatment:


If no improvement medication management should be transferred to a psychiatrist
The patient should choose mental health treater, preferably a psychologist
Request from provider approximate timeline for treatment
Evaluate the necessity, reasons and need to switch therapists at the patient’s request
Multiple providers should not treat the PTSD
Do not ask the treater to do an impairment rating
Do not ask the treater if the patient is malingering
Recommend additional cycle of TFCBT when malingering is present following a truly
traumatic event
When to recommend an IME:
The trauma is trivial or commonplace event
The PTSD evaluation of the treater is incomplete
A forensic psychologist should be recommended over a psychiatrist
The PTSD symptoms appear in medical records after a delay (> 1 month)
The patient is not compliant with treatment
One course of TCCBT has failed
The patient is not improving with TFCBT or pharmacotherapy
Treater is unwilling to commit to a treatment timeline
Treater assuming the role of expert witness
Something to Talk About? 391

will be discussed in more detail below. By the d) assuming an advocacy versus a scientific role.
same token, asking the therapist if the patient is The consultant should make the referral source
malingering puts them in an awkward position. aware of any limitation of the evaluation of the
First, it is rare for a therapist to ever suggest, much treater. This might require an IME or some
less document, malingering. Second, the lack of obligation on the part of the treater to account for
improvement that will eventually require an IME these diagnostic lapses.
will address the issue of malingering. The prob- With psychological testing at our disposal,
lematic moment comes when there is malingering and a vast malingering literature to rely on, a psy-
present in the face of a clearly serious injury (e.g., chologist (versus psychiatrist) should do the IME.
traumatic amputation). In this circumstance, all The advantage psychologists hold is indirectly
parties are informed of the malingering diagnosis, acknowledged by psychiatry. It is not uncommon
and a brief eight-session course of additional for psychiatrists to call upon psychologists to do
TFCBT can be recommended after which point personality testing on their behalf in order to
treatment should end. The purpose of this is to supplement their interview. On the other hand, as
demonstrate a good faith attempt to try to help already argued above, this recommendation is
the patient, given the severity of the injury, despite negated by the suggestion that a good interview
the presence of malingering. Here the case may be may be all that is needed. The medical records
complicated by the possibility that the patient may should establish the reporting of emotional symp-
have PTSD despite the malingering diagnosis. toms within weeks of the trauma. Delayed symp-
Additionally, they may have had PTSD at one tom onset should be considered the paradoxical
point, recovered, yet still report symptoms that unequivocal Babinski sign indicating the absence
they no longer have, making the detection of of PTSD. While delayed PTSD signals the need
malingering more challenging since they do not for an IME in the mind of the forensic psycholo-
have to feign symptoms but maintain them. gist, it is usually the lack of improvement, failed
Table 14.2 outlines when an IME should be treatment and/or failure to return to work that
recommended. Here we start at the beginning: the result in the referral source initiating an IME.
event that presumably produced the PTSD. More Based on a review of the last half dozen PTSD
in line with DSM-III, the event should be outside evaluations conducted by the first author, in which
the realm of everyday human experience. If the malingering was diagnosed, the average amount
event is trivial, including any MVA that does not of time between the trauma and the IME is
result in serious injury, one should consider an approximately 22 months. With the exception of
IME before treatment is instituted. It is the rule one case in which medical records were equivocal
rather than the exception that the PTSD evalua- as to the onset of symptoms, all presented with
tion of the treater is often incomplete. In all fair- delayed PTSD. The last circumstance that might
ness to the treater, the scrutiny and thoroughness call for the intervention of an expert is when the
with which one approaches a clinical evaluation is treating clinician assumes, often unknowingly,
not at the level of a forensic evaluation. For exam- the role of an expert witness. It should be the duty
ple, in routine clinical care, depression is diag- of every consultant to educate all parties on the
nosed without any clear evidence that the patient critical distinction between the role of a treater
was asked about all the symptoms that make up versus an expert (see chapter 2, Greiffenstein
the disorder. Keane, Buckley, and Miller (2003) and Kaufmann, in the present volume, for further
outline the common errors made in the forensic discussion of this issue).
assessment of PTSD. While these errors should Invariably, the patient’s attorney will often call
not occur in an IME context, they are ubiquitous upon the patient’s mental health treaters to make
in the routine clinical evaluation of PTSD. They some statement about the patient’s condition and/
include a) relying solely on patient self-report for or rebut the statements of an expert witness.
the diagnosis to the exclusion of corroborative A treater consenting to provide medicolegal opin-
information; b) the clinical criteria for PTSD ions regarding their patient is problematic. First,
are not fulfilled either because the clinician did the type and amount of data gathered as part of a
not ask, or failed to record the symptoms; c) fail- clinical referral are not adequate to form a proper
ure to provide examples or characterize the symp- foundation to determine causation for medicole-
toms (here we sometimes find the criteria are gal purposes. Secondly, treaters who provide testi-
listed, sometimes verbatim from the DSM, but no mony as expert witnesses create an irreconcilable
examples of the symptoms are provided); and ethical conflict between their therapeutic role as a
392 forensic neuropsychology

treater (or fact witness, which is what treaters are select measures of attention, processing speed
considered) and the role of an expert witness, and verbal memory in diagnosis and prognosis.
which is the role the treater would be enacting Empirical literature supports the notion that
when making a causal inference between the intelligence level is a risk factor for development
symptoms the patient reports, and the most prob- of PTSD: Lower intelligence is associated with
able cause. Treaters should limit their activity to greater reporting of PTSD symptoms, and higher
treating the patient, providing testimony as a fact intelligence supports resilience or better progno-
witness if necessary, and testifying only to the sis. Measures of attention, processing speed, and
opinions contained in their original report only. verbal memory play a similar role, with evidence
They should not act as post-hoc experts. Two arti- suggesting utility of certain measures of executive
cles that address this issue are highly recom- dysfunction such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting
mended (Greenberg & Shuman, 1997; Strasburger, Test, and measures of visual memory function.
Gutheil, & Brodsky, 1997). This advice runs con- The practice implication is there is no need for a
trary to the recommendation provided by the lengthy neuropsychological test battery beyond
JAMA article in regard to Mr. M. as discussed an acceptable measure of IQ, and well-validated
above: “Although it can be a burden, physicians measures of attention, processing speed and
who care for traumatically injured patients should verbal and visual memory, in addition to standard
be willing to complete necessary paperwork and/ history-gathering and personality testing and
or testify on the status of their patients when administration of SVTs, if the only claim is PTSD
required, or risk further imperiling the patients’ (note that in many cases, a PTSD claim may be
recovery” (Stein, 2002, p. 1517). accompanied by a brain injury claim, in which
case a comprehensive neuropsychological battery
FINAL THOUGHTS would be justified).
Second, it follows that neuropsychologists
“I don’t know about this PTSD, but they did should not market their services as superior in
bomb symptom validity testing.” (Anonymous quality or diagnostic accuracy to that of psycholo-
Forensic Neuropsychologist) gists who are not neuropsychologists, regarding
claims of PTSD alone. This assumes equality of
We would like to end by revisiting the ratio- clinical psychology training and experience. But it
nale for the title of this chapter: “Something to does not mean we should refuse such referrals.
Talk About?” The title is meant to convey the Third, our Ethics Code reminds us to be aware
humility with which clinical neuropsychology of the boundaries of our competence (American
should approach a topic that some could right- Psychological Association, 2002). The forte of the
fully argue does not fall under its purview. It is clinical neuropsychologist is that the forensic lit-
because this is a forensic neuropsychology text erature at their disposal dwarfs the literature of all
that PTSD is included. While clinical neuro- other disciplines. The “traumatologists” lack this
psychology is identified as a specialty requiring literature, and have only recently formally
a certain skill set, assessing PTSD requires no recognized the importance of assessing response
prerequisite training per se. bias in clinical and research assessments of PTSD
Our answer is that we have nothing to talk (Foa et al., 2008).
about if the issue is lesion localization. The totality Fourth, our greatest strength is the assessment
of the neuropsychological, neurophysiologic and of response bias. Although non-neuropsycholo-
cognitive literature, applied both clinically and gists receive some training in this important area,
empirically, has shown there is no definitive neu- neuropsychologists have been on the forefront of
rological basis to PTSD that is widely accepted deception and malingering research for at least
(Vasterling & Brewin, 2005). The hot streak of two decades. A problem one finds in the PTSD
hippocampal size research ended with futility: civilian literature is the dissociation between what
The right, left, or both hippocampuses showed a forensic neuropsychologist may be interested in
association with PTSD, but hippocampal differ- and what those who study and treat the disorder
ences were also shown to predate trauma. focus on. Traumatologists assess PTSD and not
Importantly, hippocampal size varies within time malingering, and neuropsychologists interested
within individuals. in forensic assessment of PTSD assess malinger-
First, the main contribution of neuropsy- ing, but have little to say about the PTSD. While
chologists is to address the role of intelligence and our PTSD colleagues might benefit from our
Something to Talk About? 393

malingering research, if all we have to talk about American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic
is the binomial theorem we are not likely to and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.).
engender confidence. There appears to be an Washington, DC: Author.
inverse relationship between the enthusiasm an American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic
individual has for SVT and their knowledge of the and statistical manual of mental disorders (Rev. 3rd
syndrome they are trying to refute with the SVT. ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
When there is diagnostic uncertainty about a case, American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic
the rapidity with which the neuropsychologist and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
Washington, DC: Author.
asks the obligatory “What were their SVT scores?”
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic
can be embarrassing at times. One has to question
and statistical manual of mental disorders (Rev. 4th
if SVT has led to the “dumbing down” of clinical
ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
neuropsychology. In medicine, advances in American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical
technology (e.g., MRI) put fewer intellectual principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
burdens on the physician to identify the syndrome Washington, DC: Author.
based solely on the history and physical. If foren- Andreasen, N. C. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disor-
sic neuropsychology is to make a contribution in der: Psychology, biology, and the Manichaean war-
the assessment of PTSD, they have an obligation fare between false dichotomies. American Journal
to acquire clinical competency in PTSD. The of Psychiatry, 152, 963–965.
accusation once leveled against the informally Andreasen, N. C. (2004). Acute and delayed posttrau-
trained weekend workshop neuropsychologist, matic stress disorders: A history and some issues.
“the weekend wonder,” is a condemnation that The American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1321–1323.
may be rightfully leveled at us by our PTSD Andrews, B., Brewin, C. R., Philpott, R., & Stewart, L.
colleagues who devote more than just weekends (2007). Delayed-onset posttraumatic stress disor-
to a serious and complex disorder. der: A systematic review of the evidence. American
In closing, we revisit the question posed in the Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1319–1326.
introduction to this chapter regarding the incre- Andrikopoulos, J. (2001). Malingering disorientation
mental validity of neuropsychological testing in to time, personal information, and place in mild head
the assessment of PTSD. The neuropsychologist is injured litigants. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15,
in a good position to address the medicolegal impli- 393–396.
cations of the PTSD diagnosis, but not in a unique Backhaus, S. L., Fichtenberg, N. L., & Hanks, R. A.
(2004). Detection of sub-optimal performance
position. Our future success hinges on acquiring a
using a floor effect strategy in patients with trau-
comparable clinical and research PTSD knowledge
matic brain injury. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
base on the accurate diagnosis of PTSD when it is
18, 591–603.
present, and being able to generate malingering Bayer, C. P., Klasen, F., & Adam, H. (2007). Association
PTSD literature using a known-groups design for of trauma and PTSD symptoms with openness to
detection of malingering when it is present. reconciliation and feelings of revenge among
former Ugandan and Congolese child soldiers. The
AC K N OW L E D G M E N T Journal of the American Medical Association, 298,
The authors wish to thank Ryanne Skalberg for 555–59.
her editorial assistance. Benedek, D. M., Friedman, M. J., Zatzick, D., & Ursano,
R. J. (2009). Guideline watch (March 2009):
REFERENCES Practice guideline for the treatment of patients
Allen, L. M., Conder, R. L., Green, P., & Cox, D. R. with acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress
(1997). CARB’97 manual for the computerized assess- disorder. Focus, 7, 3–31.
ment of response bias. Durham, NC: CogniSyst. Ben-Porath, Y. S. & Tellegen, A. (2008). MMPI-2-RF
American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine. (1993). (Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2-
Definition of mild traumatic brain injury. Journal Restructrured Form) manual for administration,
of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 8, 86–87. scoring and interpretation. Minneapolis, MN: The
American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Diagnostic University of Minnesota Press.
and statistical manual of mental disorders. Berry, D. T., Adams, J. J., Clark, C. D., Thacker, S. R.,
Washington, DC: Author. Burger, T. L., Wetter, M. W., et al. (1996). Detection
American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic of a cry for help on the MMPI-2: An analog
and statistical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). investigation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67,
Washington, DC: Author. 26–36.
394 forensic neuropsychology

Bhattacharjee, Y. (2008). Shell shock revisited: Solving stressors. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 144,
the puzzle of blast trauma. Science, 319, 406–408. 578–583.
Bianchini, K. J., Curtis, K. L., & Greve, K. W. (2006). Breslau, N., Davis, G. C., Andreski, P., & Peterson, E.
Compensation and malingering in traumatic brain (1991). Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress
injury: A dose-response relationship? The Clinical disorder in an urban population of young adults.
Neuropsychologist, 20, 831–847. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 216–222.
Bieliauskas, L. A. (1999). Personality and emotional Breslau, N. & Kessler, R. C. (2001). The stressor crite-
functioning. In J. Sweet (Ed.), Forensic neuropsy- rion in DSM-IV posttraumatic stress disorder: An
chology: Fundamentals and practice (pp. 121–144). empirical investigation. Biological Psychiatry, 50,
Amsterdam/Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger. 699–704.
Bisson, J. & Andrew, M. (2007). Psychological treat- Brewin, C. R. (2003). Post-traumatic stress disorder:
ment of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Malady or myth? New Haven, CT: Yale University
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, Press.
3(CD003388). doi: 10.1002/14651858 Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. (2000).
Blanchard, E. B., & Hickling, E. J. (2004). After the Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic
crash: Assessment and treatment of motor vehicle stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. Journal
accident survivors (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 748–766.
American Psychological Association. Briere, J. (2001). Detailed assessment of posttraumatic
Blanchard, E. B., Hickling, E. J., Buckley, T. C., Taylor, stress (DAPS). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assess-
A. E., Vollmer, A., & Loos, W. R. (1996a). ment Resources.
Psychophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder Briere, J. (1995). Trauma Symptoms Inventory profes-
related to motor vehicle accidents: Replication and sional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
extension. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Assessment Resources.
Psychology, 64, 742–751. Bryant, R. A. (2001). Posttraumatic stress disorder and
Blanchard, E. B., Hickling, E. J., Taylor, A. E., Buckley, mild brain injury: Controversies, causes and con-
T. C., Loos, W. R., & Walsh, J. (1998). Effects of liti- sequences. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
gation settlements on posttraumatic stress symp- Neuropsychology, 23, 718–728.
toms in motor vehicle accident victims. Journal of Bryant, R. A. (2008). Disentangling mild traumatic
Traumatic Stress, 11, 337–354. brain injury and stress reactions. New England
Blanchard, E. B., Hickling, E. J., Taylor, A. E., Loos, W. Journal of Medicine, 358, 525–527.
R., Forneris, C. A., & Jaccard, J. (1996b). Who Bryant, R. A., Creamer, M., O’Donnell, M., Silove, D.,
develops PTSD from motor vehicle accidents? Clark, C. R., & McFarlane, A. C. (2009). Post-
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 1–10. traumatic amnesia and the nature of post-traumatic
Bombardier, C. H., Fann, J. R., Temkin, N., Esselman, stress disorder after mild traumatic brain injury.
P. C., Pelzer, E., Keough, M., et al. (2006). Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms during the Society, 15, 862–867.
first six months after traumatic brain injury. Journal Bryant, R. A. & Harvey, A. G. (1995). Psychological
of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 18, impairment following motor vehicle accidents.
501–508. Australian Journal of Public Health, 19, 185–188.
Bonne, O., Brandes, D., Gilboa, A., Gomori, J. M., Bryant, R. A. & Harvey, A. G. (1998). Relationship
Shenton, M. E., Pitman, R. K., et al. (2001). between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic
Longitudinal MRI study of hippocampal volume in stress disorder following mild traumatic brain
trauma survivors with PTSD. The American Journal injury. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 625–
of Psychiatry, 158, 1248–1251. 629.
Brandes, D., Ben-Schachar, G., Gilboa, A., Bonne, O., Bryant, R. A. & Harvey, A. G. (1999). Postconcussive
Freedman, S., & Shalev, A. (2002). PTSD symp- symptoms and posttraumatic stress disorder after
toms and cognitive performance in recent trauma mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Nervous and
survivors. Psychiatry Research, 110, 231–238. Mental Disease, 187, 302–305.
Bremner, J. D., Randall, P., Scott, T. M., Bronen, R. A., Bryant, R. A. & Harvey, A. G. (2003). The influence of
Seibyl, J. P., Southwick, S. M., et al. (1995). MRI- litigation on maintenance of posttraumatic stress
based measurement of hippocampal volume in disorder. The Journal of Nervous and Mental
patients with combat-related posttraumatic Disease, 191, 191–193.
stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, Bryant, R. A., Marosszeky, J. E., Crooks, J., & Gurka, J.
152, 973–981. A. (2000). Posttraumatic stress disorder after severe
Breslau, N. & Davis, G. C. (1987). Posttraumatic traumatic brain injury. American Journal of Psych-
stress disorder: The etiologic specificity of wartime iatry, 157, 629–631.
Something to Talk About? 395

Burges, C. & McMillan, T. M. (2001). The ability of Elhai, J. D., Gray, M. J., Kashdan, T. B., & Franklin,
naive participants to report symptoms of post- C. L. (2005a). Which instruments are most
traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Clincal commonly used to assess traumatic event exposure
Psychology, 40, 209–214. and posttraumatic effects?: A survey of traumatic
Burkett, B. G. & Whitley, G. (1998). Stolen valor: How stress professionals. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18,
the Vietnam generation was robbed of its heroes and 541–545.
its history. Dallas, TX: Verity Press. Elhai, J. D., Gray, M. J., Naifeh, J. A., Butcher, J. J., Davis,
Bury, A. S. & Bagby, R. M. (2002). The detection of J. L., Falsetti, S. A., et al. (2005b). Utility of the
feigned uncoached and coached posttraumatic Trauma Symptom Inventory’s atypical response
stress disorder with the MMPI-2 in a sample of scale in detecting malingered post-traumatic stress
workplace accident victims. Psychological disorder. Assessment, 12, 210–219.
Assessment, 14, 472–484. Elhai, J. D., Naifeh, J. A., Zucker, I. S., Gold, S. N.,
Centers for Disease Control (1988). Health status of Deitsch, S. E., & Frueh, B. C. (2004). Discriminating
Vietnam veterans. I. Psychosocial characteristics. malingered from genuine civilian posttraumatic
The Centers for Disease Control Vietnam experi- stress disorder: A validation of three MMPI-2
ence study. Journal of the American Medical Infrequency scales (F, Fp, and Fptsd). Assessment,
Association, 259, 2701–2707. 11, 139–144.
Danckwerts, A. & Leathem, J. (2003). Questioning the Elhai, J. D., Ruggiero, K. J., Frueh, B. C., Beckham, J. C.,
link between PTSD and cognitive dysfunction. Gold, P. B., & Feldman, M. E. (2002). The
Neuropsychology Review, 13, 221–235. Infrequency-Posttraumatic Stress Disorder scale
David, A. S. (1992). Frontal lobology: Psychiatry’s new (Fptsd) for the MMPI-2: Development and initial
pseudoscience. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, validation with veterans presenting with combat-
244–248. related PTSD. Journal of Personality Assessment,
Davidson, J. R. & Foa, E. B. (1991). Diagnostic issues in 79, 531–549.
posttraumatic stress disorder: Considerations for Figley, C. R. (2010). Now we know much about the
the DSM-IV. The Journal of Abnormal Psychology, neurobiology of PTSD. So what? [Review of the
100, 346–355. book Post-traumatic stress disorder: Basic science
Department of Veterans Affairs. (2008). Posttruamatic and clinical practice]. PsycCRITQUES, 55.
stress disorder. Federal Register, 73, 64208 (Oct. 29, First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B.
2008, to be codified at 38 C.F.R., pt. 3). W. (1997). User’s guide for the structured clinical
Demakis, G. J., Gervais, R. O., & Rohling, M. L. (2008). interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders (SCID-I-:
The effect of failure on cognitive and psychological Clinician. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
symptom validity tests in litigants with symptoms Press.
of post-traumatic stress disorder. The Clinical Foa, E. B. (1995). Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale
Neuropsychologist, 22, 879–895. manual. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.
Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., Winn, H. R., & Temkin, Foa, E. B, Keane, T. M., & Friedman, M. J. (2000).
N. R. (1995). Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year Effective treatments for PTSD: Practice guidelines
post head injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 80–90. from the International Society for Traumatic Stress
Dohrenwend, B. P., Turner, J. B., Turse, N. A., Adams, studies. New York: Guilford Press.
B. G., Koenen, K. C., & Marshall, R. (2006). The Foa, E. B, Keane, T. M., Friedman, M. J., & Cohen, J. A.
psychological risks of Vietnam for U.S. veterans: A (2008). Effective treatments for PTSD: Practice
revisit with new data and methods. Science, 313, guidelines from the International Society for
979–982. Traumatic Stress studies (2nd ed.). New York:
Edens, J. F., Otto, R. K., & Dwyer, T. J. (1998). Guilford Press.
Susceptibility of the trauma symptom inventory to Forbes, D., Wolfgang, B., & Cooper, J. (2009) Post-
malingering. Journal of Personality Assessment, 71, traumatic stress disorder: Best practice GP guide-
379–392. lines. Australian Family Physician, 38, 106–111.
Efendov, A. A., Sellbom, M., & Bagby, R. M. (2008). Francati, V., Vermetten, E., & Bremner, J. D. (2007).
The utility and comparative incremental validity of Functional neuroimaging studies in posttraumatic
the MMPI-2 and Trauma Symptom Inventory stress disorder: Review of current methods and
validity scales in the detection of feigned PTSD. findings. Depression and Anxiety, 24, 202–218.
Psychological Assessment, 20, 317–326. Frankel, F. H. (1994). The concept of flashbacks in
Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A., & Bryant, B. (1998). historical perspective. International Journal of
Psychological predictors of chronic posttraumatic Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 321–336.
stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. Freeman, T., Powell, M., & Kimbrell, T. (2008).
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 508–519. Measuring symptom exaggeration in veterans with
396 forensic neuropsychology

chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., Axelrod, B., Peck,
Psychiatric Research, 158, 374–380. E. A., & Gervais, R. (2004). The Fake Bad Scale and
Frueh, B. C., Elhai, J. D., Grubaugh, A. L., Monnier, J., MMPI-2 F- Family in detection of implausible
Kashdan, T. B., Sauvageot, J. A., et al. (2005). psychological trauma claims. The Clinical Neuro-
Documented combat exposure of US veterans psychologist, 18, 573–590.
seeking treatment for combat-related post- Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Gola, T. (1994).
traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Validation of malingered amnesia measures with a
Psychiatry, 186, 467–472. large clinical sample. Psychological Assessment, 6,
Galea, S., Vlahov, D., Resnick, H., Ahern, J., Susser, E., 218–224.
Gold, J., et al. (2003). Trends of probable post- Greiffenstein, M. F., Fox, D., & Lees-Haley, P. R. (2007).
traumatic stress disorder in New York City after The MMPI-2 Fake Bad Scale in detection of non-
the September 11 terrorist attacks. The American credible brain injury claims. In K. B. Boone (Ed.).
Journal of Epidemiology, 158, 514–524. Assessment of feigned cognitive impairment. A neu-
Gaston, L., Brunet, A., Koszycki, D., & Bradwejn, J. ropsychological perspective (210–235). New York:
(1996). MMPI profiles of acute and chronic PTSD Guilford Press.
in a civilian sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9, Greve, K. W., Etherton, J. L., Ord, J., Bianchini, K. J., &
817–832. Curtis, K. L. (2009). Detecting malingered pain-
Gaston, L., Brunet, A., Koszycki, D., & Bradwejn, J. related disability: Classification accuracy of the test
(1998). MMPI scales for diagnosing acute and of memory malingering. The Clinical Neuro-
chronic PTSD in civilians. Minnesota Multiphasic psychologist, 23, 1250–1271.
Personality Inventory. Journal of Traumatic Stress, Gurvits, T. V., Shenton, M. E., Hokama, H., & Ohta, H.
11, 355–365. (1996). Magnetic resonance imaging study of hip-
Geraerts, E., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2006). pocampal volume in chronic, combat-related post-
Symptom over-reporting and recovered memories traumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 40,
of childhood sexual abuse. Law and Human 1091–1099.
Behavior, 30, 621–630. Harvey, A. G. & Bryant, R. A. (1998). The relationship
Gil, T., Calev, A., Greenberg, D., Kugelmass, S., & Lerer, between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic
B. (1990). Cognitive functioning in post-traumatic stress disorder: A prospective evaluation of motor
stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 3, vehicle accident survivors. Journal of Consulting
29–45. and Clinical Psychology, 66, 507–512.
Gil, S., Caspi, Y., Ben-Ari, I. Z., Koren, D., & Klein, E. Hedges, D. W., Allen, S., Tate, D. F., Thatcher, G. W.,
(2005). Does memory of a traumatic event increase Miller, M. J., Rice, S. A., et al. (2003). Reduced
the risk for posttraumatic stress disorder in patients hippocampal volume in alcohol and substance-
with traumatic brain injury? A prospective study. naïve Vietnam combat veterans with posttraumatic
American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 963–969. stress disorder. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology,
Gilbertson, M. W., Shenton, M. E., Ciszewski, A., Kasai, 16, 219–224.
K., Lasko, N. B., Orr, S. P., & et al. (2002). Smaller Helzer, J. E., Robins, L. N., & McEvoy, L. (1987). Post-
hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnera- traumatic stress disorder in the general population:
bility to psychological trauma. Nature Neuroscience, Findings of the epidemiologic catchment area
5, 1242–1247. survey. New England Journal of Medicine, 317,
Gilbertson, M. W., Paulus, L. A., Williston, S. K., 1630–1634.
Gurvits, T. V., Lasko, N. B., Pitman, R. K., et al. Hoge, C. W., McGurk, D., Thomas, J. L., Cox, A. L.,
(2006). Neurocognitive function in monozygotic Engel, C. C., & Castro, C. A. (2008). Mild traumatic
twins discordant for combat exposure: Relation- brain injury in U.S. soldiers returning from Iraq.
ship to posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of New England Journal of Medicine, 358, 453–463.
Abnormal Psychology, 115, 484–495. Horner, M. D. & Hamner, M. B. (2002). Neurocognitive
Green, P. (2003). Green’s Word Memory Test for Windows: functioning in posttraumatic stress disorder.
User’s manual. Edmonton: Green’s Publishing. Neuropsychology Review, 12, 15–30.
Greenberg, S. A. & Shuman, D. W. (1997). Irreconcilable Jatzko, A., Rothenhöfer, S., Schmitt, A., Gaser, C.,
conflict between therapeutic and forensic roles. Demirakca, T., Weber-Fahr, W., et al. (2006).
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 28, Hippocampal volume in chronic posttraumatic
50–57. stress disorder (PTSD): MRI study using two dif-
Greiffenstein, M. F. & Baker, W. J. (2008). Validity ferent evaluation methods. Journal of Affective
testing in dually diagnosed post-traumatic stress Disorders, 94, 121–126.
disorder and mild closed head injury. The Clinical Jenkins, M. A., Langlais, P. J., Delis, D., & Cohen, R. A.
Neuropsychologist, 22, 565–582. (2000). Attentional dysfunction associated with
Something to Talk About? 397

posttraumatic stress disorder among rape survi- Kulka, R. A., Schlenger, W. E., Fairbank, J. A., Hough,
vors. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 14, 7–12. R. L., Jordan, B. K., Marmar, C. R., et al. (1990).
Johnsen, G. E., Kanagaratnam, P., & Asbjørnsen, A. E. Trauma and the Vietnam War generation. New
(2008). Memory impairments in posttraumatic York: Brunner/Mazel Inc.
stress disorder are related to depression. Journal of LaGarde, G., Doyon, J., & Brunet, A. (2010). Memory
Anxiety Disorders, 22, 464–474. and executive dysfunctions associated with acute
Jones, E., Vermaas, R. H., McCartney, H., Beech, C., posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research,
Palmer, I., Hyams, K., et al. (2003). Flashbacks and 177, 144–149.
post-traumatic stress disorder: The genesis of a Landolt, M. A., Vollrath, M., Timm, K., Gnehm, H. E.,
20th-century diagnosis. British Journal of & Sennhauser, F. H. (2005). Predicting posttrau-
Psychiatry, 182, 158–163. matic stress symptoms in children after road
Keane, T. M., Buckley, T. C., & Miller, M. W. (2003). traffic accidents. Journal of the American
Forensic Psychological Assessment in PTSD. In R. Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44,
I. Simon (Ed.), Posttraumatic stress disorder in 1276–1283.
litigation: Guidelines for forensic assessment (2nd Larrabee, G. J. (1998). Somatic malingering on the
ed., pp. 119–140). Washington, DC: American MMPI and MMPI-2 in personal injury litigants.
Psychiatric Publishing. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 179–188.
Keane, T. M., Kolb, L. C., Kaloupek, D. G., Orr, S. P., Larrabee, G. J. (2003). Detection of symptom exaggera-
Blanchard, E. B., Thomas, R. G., et al. (1998). Utility tion with the MMPI-2 in litigants with malingered
of psychophysiological measurement in the diag- neurocognitive dysfunction. The Clinical Neuro-
nosis of posttraumatic stress disorder: Results from psychologist, 17, 54–68.
a Department of Veterans Affairs cooperative Lees-Haley, P. (1986). Pseudo post-traumatic stress
study. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology, disorder. Trial Diplomacy, 9, 17–20.
66, 914–923. Lees-Haley, P. R. (1992). Efficacy of MMPI-2 validity
Keane, T. M., Malloy, P. F., & Fairbank, J. A. (1984). scales and MCMI-II modifier scales for detecting
Empirical development of an MMPI subscale for spurious PTSD claims: F, F-K, fake bad scale, ego
the assessment of combat-related posttraumatic strength, subtle-obvious subscales, DIS, and DEB.
stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 681–689.
Psychology, 52, 888–891. Lees-Haley, P. R., English, L. T., & Glenn, W. J. (1991).
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M., & A fake bad scale on the MMPI-2 for personal injury
Nelson, C. B. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder claimants. Psychological Reports, 68, 203–210.
in the national comorbidity survey. Archives of Lembcke, J. (1998). The “right stuff ” gone wrong:
General Psychiatry, 52, 1048–1060. Vietnam veterans and the social construction of
Kivling-bodèn, G. & Sundbom, E. (2003). Cognitive post-traumatic stress disorder. Critical Sociology,
abilities related to post-traumatic symptoms among 24, 37–64.
refugees from the former Yugoslavia in psychiatric Levin, H. S., Brown, S. A., Song, J. X., McCauley, S. R.,
treatment. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 57, 191– Boake, C., Contant, C. F., et al. (2001). Depression
198. and posttraumatic stress disorder at three months
Knoll, J. & Resnick, P. J. (2006). The detection of malin- after mild to moderate traumatic brain injury.
gered post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatric Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro-
Clinics of North America, 29, 629–647. psychology, 23, 754–769.
Koren, D., Arnon, I., & Klein, E. (1999). Acute stress Lyons, J. A., & Keane, T. M. (1992). Keane PTSD scale:
response and posttraumatic stress disorder in traffic MMPI and MMPI-2 update. Journal of Traumatic
accident victims: A one-year prospective, follow-up Stress, 5, 111–117.
study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 367–373. Lyons, J. A. & Wheeler-Cox, T. (1999). MMPI, MMPI-2
Koretzky, M. B. & Peck, A. H. (1990). Validation and and PTSD: Overview of scores, scales, and profiles.
cross-validation of the PTSD subscale of the MMPI Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12, 175–183.
with civilian trauma victims. Journal of Clinical Malt, U. (1988). The long-term psychiatric conse-
Psychology, 46, 296–300. quences of accidental injury: A longitudinal study
Kremen, W. S., Koenen, K. C., Boake, C., Purcell, S., of 107 adults. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The
Eisen, S., Franz, C. E., et al. (2007). Pretrauma cog- Journal of Mental Science, 153, 810–818.
nitive ability and risk for posttraumatic stress disor- Matsuo, K., Taneichi, K., Matsumoto, A., Ohtani, T.,
der. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 361–368. Yamasue, H., Sakano, Y., et al. (2003). Hypoactivation
Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (2001). The of the prefrontal cortex during verbal fluency test
PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity mea- in PTSD: A near-infrared spectroscopy study.
sure. Journal of Internal Medicine, 16, 606–613. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 124, 1–10.
398 forensic neuropsychology

Matsuoka, Y., Nishi, D., Nakajima, S., Kim, Y., Homma, for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of
M., & Otomo, Y. (2008). Incidence and prediction of Personality Assessment, 70, 299–314.
psychiatric morbidity after a motor vehicle accident Morel, K. R. (2008a). Comparison of the Morel
in Japan: The Tachikawa cohort of motor vehicle Emotional Numbing test for posttraumatic stress
accident study. Critical Care Medicine, 36, 74–80. disorder to the Word Memory test in neuropsy-
Mayou, R., Bryant, B., & Duthie, R. (1993). Psychiatric chological evaluations. The Clinical Neuro-
consequences of road traffic accidents. British psychologist, 22, 350–362.
Medical Journal, 307, 647–651. Morel, K. R. (2008b). Development of a validity scale
McCrea, M., Guskiewicz, K. M., Marshall, S. W., Barr, for combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder:
W., Randolph, C., Cantu, R. C., et al. (2003). Acute Evidence from simulated malingerers and actual
effects and recovery time following concussion in disability claimants. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry
collegiate football players: The NCAA concussion & Psychology, 19, 52–63.
study. The Journal of the American Medical National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence
Association, 290, 2556–2563. (2005). The management of PTSD in adults and
McEwen, B. S. (1999). Stress and hippocampal plastic- children in primary and secondary care. Post-
ity. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 105–122. traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), 26. London:
McNally, R. J. (2003). Progress and controversy in the National Institute for Health and Clinical
study of posttraumatic stress disorder. Annual Excellence. Retrieved from http://nice.org.uk
Review of Psychology, 54, 229–252. Nelson, N. W., Hoelzle, J. B., Sweet, J. J., Arbisi, P. A., &
McNally, R. J. (2004). Conceptual problems with the Demakis, G. J. (2010). Updated meta-analysis of
DSM-IV criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder. the MMPI-2 Symptom Validity scale (FBS):
In G.M. Rosen (Ed.), Post-traumatic stress disorder: Verified utility in forensic practice. The Clinical
Issues and Controversies (pp. 1–14). New York: Neuropsychologist, 24, 701–724.
John Wiley and Sons. North, C. S., Nixon, S. J., Shariat, S., Mallonee, S.,
McNally, R. J. (2006). Applying biological data in the McMillen, J. C., Spitznagel, E. L., et al. (1999).
forensic and policy areas. Annals of New York Psychiatric disorders among survivors of the
Academy of Sciences, 1071, 267–276. Oklahoma City bombing. The Journal of the
McNally, R. J. & Breslau, N. (2008). Does virtual trauma American Medical Association, 282, 755–762.
cause posttraumatic stress disorder? American Nye, E. C., Qualls, C., & Katzman, J. (2006). The trauma
Psychologist, 63, 282–283. symptom inventory: Factors associated with invalid
McNally, R. J., Lasko, N. B., Clancy, S. A., Macklin, M. profiles in a sample of combat veterans with post-
L., Pitman, R. K., & Orr, S. P. (2004). Psycho- traumatic stress disorder. Military Medicine, 171,
physiological responding during script-driven 857–860.
imagery in people reporting abduction by space Omalu, B. I., DeKosky, S. T., Hamilton, R. L., Minster,
aliens. Psychological Science, 15, 493–497. R. L., Kamboh, M. I., Shakir, A. M., et al. (2006).
Merten, T., Thies, E., Schneider, K., & Stevens, A. Chronic traumatic encephalopathy in a National
(2009). Symptom validity testing in claimants with Football League player: Part II. Neurosurgery, 59,
alleged posttraumatic stress disorder: Comparing 1086–1093.
the Morel Emotional Numbing test, the Structured Omalu, B. I., DeKosky, S. T., Minster, R. L., Kamboh,
Inventory of Malingered Symptomatology, and the M. I., Hamilton, R. L., & Wecht, C. H. (2005).
Word Memory test. Psychological Injury and Law, Chronic traumatic encephalopathy in a National
2, 284–293. Football League player. Neurosurgery, 57, 128–134.
Miles, J. N., Marshall, G. N., & Schell, T. L. (2008). Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S.
Spanish and English versions of the PTSD check- (2008). Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder
list-civilian version (PCL-C): Testing for differen- and symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psycho-
tial item functioning. Journal of Traumatic Stress, logical Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice and
21, 369–376. Policy, (Suppl, S), 3–36.
Miller, H. A. (2001). Miller Forensic Symptoms Test Pankratz, L., Fausti, A., & Peed, S. (1975). A forced-
professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological choice technique to evaluate deafness in the hys-
Assessment Resources. terical or malingering patient. Journal of Consulting
Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit, and Clinical Psychology, 43, 421–422.
D. C. (2002). Base rates of malingering and symp- Parslow, R. A. & Jorm, A. F. (2007). Pretrauma
tom exaggeration. Journal of Clinical and Experi- and posttrauma neurocognitive functioning and
mental Neuropsychology, 24, 1094–1102. PTSD symptoms in a community sample of young
Morel, K. R. (1998). Development and preliminary adults. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164,
validation of a forced-choice test of response bias 509–515.
Something to Talk About? 399

Perrin, S., Van Hasselt, V. B., & Hersen, M. (1997). Sbordone, R. J. & Liter, J. C. (1995). Mild traumatic
Validation of the Keane MMPI-PTSD scale against brain injury does not produce post-traumatic stress
DSM-III-R criteria in a sample of battered women. disorder. Brain Injury, 9, 405–412.
Violence and Victims, 12, 99–104. Scheibe, S., Bagby, R. M., Miller, L. S., & Dorian, B. J.
Pham, P. N., Weinstein, H. M., & Longman, T. (2004). (2001). Assessing posttraumatic stress disorder
Trauma and PTSD symptoms in Rwanda: with the MMPI-2 in a sample of workplace accident
Implications for attitudes toward justice and rec- victims. Psychological Assessment, 13, 369–374.
onciliation. The Journal of the American Medical Schlenger, W. E., Caddell, J. M., Ebert, L., Jordan, B. K.,
Association, 292, 602–612. Rourke, K. M., Wilson, D., et al. (2002).
Pitman, R. K., Gilbertson, M. W., Gurvits, T. V., May, Psychological reactions to terrorist attacks:
F. S., Lasko, N. B., Metzger, L. J., et al. (2006). Findings from the national study of Americans’
Clarifying the origin of biological abnormalities in reactions to September 11. The Journal of the
PTSD through the study of identical twins discor- American Medical Association, 288, 581–588.
dant for combat exposure. Annals of the New York Schnyder, U., Moergeli, H., Trentz, O., Klaghofer, R., &
Academy of Sciences, 1071, 242–254. Buddeberg, C. (2001). Prediction of psychiatric
Rabe, S., Beaducel, A., Zollner, T., Maercker, A., & Karl, morbidity in severely injured accident victims at
A. (2006). Regional brain electrical activity in one-year follow-up. American Journal of Respiratory
posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle and Critical Care Medicine, 164, 653–656.
accident. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, Ship, A. N. (2004). A 46-year-old man with anxiety and
687–698. nightmares after a motor vehicle collision, 2 years
Resick P. A., Nishith P., Weaver, T. L., Astin M.C., & later. The Journal of the American Medical
Feuer, C. A. (2002). A comparison of cognitive- Association, 292, 2262.
processing therapy with prolonged exposure and a Sparr, L. & Pankratz, L. D. (1983). Factitious posttrau-
waiting condition for the treatment of chronic matic stress disorder. American Journal of
posttraumatic stress disorder in female rape vic- Psychiatry, 140, 1016–1019.
tims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Spitzer, R. L., First, M. B., & Wakefield, J. C. (2007).
70, 867–879. Saving PTSD from itself in DSM-V. Journal of
Rogers, R., Bagby, M. R., & Dickens, S. E. (1992). Anxiety Disorders, 21, 233–241.
Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms profes- Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B., & Löwe, B.
sional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment (2006). A brief measure for assessing generalized
Resources. anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Archives of Internal
Rogers, R., Payne, J. W., Correa, A. A., Gillard, N. D., Medicine, 166, 1092–1097.
Ross, C. A. (2009). A study of the SIRS with severely Stein, M. B. (2002). A 46-year-old man with anxiety
traumatized patients. Journal of Personality and nightmares after a motor vehicle collision. The
Assessment, 91, 429–438. Journal of the American Medical Association, 288,
Rosen, G. (2004). Malingering and the PTSD database. 1513–1522.
In Gerald Rosen (Ed.), Post-traumatic stress disor- Stein, D. J., Ipser, J. C., & Seedat, S. (2006).
der: Issues and Controversies (pp. 85–99). New Pharmacotherapy for post-traumatic stress disor-
York: John Wiley and Sons. der (PTSD). Cochrane Database of Systematic
Rosen, G. M., Sawchuk, C. N., Atkins, D. C., Brown, Reviews, 1(CD0027595). doi: 10.1002/14651858
M., Price, J. R., & Lees-Haley, P. R. (2006). Risk of Stein, M. B., Kennedy, C. M., & Twamley, E. W. (2002).
false positives when identifying malingered pro- Neuropsychological function in female victims of
files using the Trauma Symptom Inventory. Journal intimate partner violence with and without post-
of Personality Assessment, 86, 329–333. traumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 52,
Ross, S. R., Millis, S. R., Krukowski, R. A., Putnam, S. 1079–1088.
H., & Adams, K. M. (2004). Detecting incomplete Stone, A. A. (1993). Post-traumatic stress disorder and
effort on the MMPI-2: An examination of the Fake- the law: Critical review of the new frontier. The
Bad scale in mild head injury. Journal of Clinical Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 115–124. the Law, 21, 23–36.
Rothbaum, B. O., Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Murdoch, T., Strasburger, L. H., Gutheil, T. G., & Brodsky, A. (1997).
& Walsh, W. (1992). A prospective examination of On wearing two hats: Role conflict in serving as
post-traumatic stress disorder in rape victims. both psychotherapist and expert witness. American
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 5, 455–475. Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 448–456.
Rubin, A. (2003). Unanswered questions about the Sumpter, R. E. & McMillan, T. M. (2005). Misdiagnosis
empirical support for EMDR in the treatment of of post-traumatic stress disorder following severe
PTSD: A review of research. Traumatology, 9, 4–30. traumatic brain injury. The British Journal of
400 forensic neuropsychology

Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 186, Weathers, F., Litz, B., Herman, D., Huska, J., & Keane,
423–426. T. (1993). The PTSD Checklist (PCL): Reliability,
Sweet, J. J., King, J. H., Malina, A. C., Bergman, M. A., validity, and diagnostic utility. Paper presented at
& Simmons, A. (2002). Documenting the promi- the annual meeting of the International Society for
nence of forensic neuropsychology at national Traumatic Stress Studies, San Antonio, TX.
meetings and in relevant professional journals Weathers, F. W., Newman, E., Blake, D. D., Nagy, L. M.,
from 1990 to 2000. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, Schnurr, P. P., Kloupek, D. G., et al. (2004). Clinician
16, 481–494. administered PTSD scale (CAPS). Los Angeles, CA:
Tombaugh, T. (1996). Test of Memory Malingering Western Psychological Services.
manual. New York: Multi-Health Systems. Weathers, F. W., Keane, T. M., & Davidson, J. R. (2001).
Vasterling, J. J. & Brailey, K. (2005). Neuropsychological Clinician-administered PTSD scale: A review of
findings in adults with PTSD. In J. J. Vasterling and the first ten years of research. Depression and
C. R. Brewin (Eds.), Neuropsychology of PTSD: Anxiety, 13, 132–156.
Biological, cognitive and clinical perspectives (178– Weniger, G., Lange, C., Sachsse, U., & Irle, E. (2008).
207). New York: Guilford Press. Amygdala and hippocampal volumes and cogni-
Vasterling, J. J. & Brewin, C. R. (2005). Neuropsychology tion in adult survivors of childhood abuse with
of PTSD: Biological, cognitive, and clinical perspec- dissociative disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandi-
tives. New York: Guilford Press. navica, 118, 281–290.
Verger, P., Dab, W., Lamping, D. L., Loze, J. Y., Widows, M. R. & Smith, G. P. (2005). Structured inven-
Deschaseaux-Voinet, C., Abenhaim, L., et al. tory of malingered symptomatology. Professional
(2004). The psychological impact of terrorism: An manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment
epidemiologic study of posttraumatic stress disor- Resources.
der and associated factors in victims of the 1995- Wise, E. A. (1996). Comparative validity of MMPI-2
1996 bombings in France. The American Journal of and MCMI-II personality disorder classifications.
Psychiatry, 161, 1384–1389. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 569–582.
Vermetten, E., Vythilingam, M., Southwick, S. M., Wrenger, M., Lange, C., Langer, M., Heuft, G., &
Charney, D. S., & Bremner, J. D. (2003). Long-term Burgmer, M. (2008). Psychiatric disorders after an
treatment with paroxetine increases verbal declara- accident: Predictors and the influence of the psy-
tive memory and hippocampal volume in post- chiatric condition prior to an accident. European
traumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 54, Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of
693–702. European Psychiatrists, 23, 434–440.
Villarreal, G., Petropoulos, H., Hamilton, D. A., Yehuda, R., Golier, J. A., Halligan, S. L., & Harvey, P. D.
Rowland, L. M., Horan, W. P., Griego, J. A., et al. (2004). Learning and memory in Holocaust survi-
(2002). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy vors with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological
of the hippocampus and occipital white matter in Psychiatry, 55, 291–295.
PTSD: Preliminary results. Canadian Journal of Young, A. (1995). The harmony of illusions: Inventing
Psychiatry, 47, 666–670. post-traumatic stress disorder. Princeton, NJ:
Vrij, A., Leal, S., Granhag, P. A., Mann, S., Fisher, R. P., Princeton University Press.
Hillman, J., et al. (2009). Outsmarting the liars: The
benefit of asking unanticipated questions. Law and
Human Behavior, 33, 159–166.
15
Assessing Civil Competencies in
Older Adults with Dementia1
Consent Capacity, Financial Capacity, and Testamentary Capacity

D A N I E L C . M A R S O N , K AT I N A H E B E R T , A N D
ANDREA C. SOLOMON

INTRODUCTION 16 million by 2050 (Alzheimer’s Association,


In the five years that have passed since the first 2008). Put differently, currently every 72 seconds
publication of Forensic Neuropsychology, issues of a person develops Alzheimer’s disease in our
competency assessment in the elderly have country; by 2050, without effective treatments,
received increasing attention both in terms of that rate will increase to a new case every 33
scholarship and public recognition (American seconds (Alzheimer’s Association, 2008). In the
Bar Association Commission on Law and Aging context of these figures, the growing societal
& American Psychological Association, 2005; impact of dementia on decisional capacities in
American Bar Association/American Psycho- the elderly is alarmingly apparent.1
logical Association Assessment of Capacity in Impairment of competency, or decision-
Older Adults Project Working Group, 2006, 2008; making capacity (Appelbaum & Gutheil, 1991), is
Fackelmann, 2008). A major reason for this is the an inevitable consequence of neurodegenerative
demographic dynamics of our aging society. dementias like Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and
Unfortunately, with the graying of America has Parkinson’s disease (PD) (Dymek, Atchison,
also come the dementing of America (Marson & Harrell, & Marson, 2001a; Marson, Ingram, Cody,
Zebley, 2001). Prevalent neurodegenerative dis- & Harrell, 1995c; Marson, Schmitt, Ingram, &
eases in later life, such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD) Harrell, 1994a). As capacities for memory, judg-
and Parkinson’s disease (PD), cause dementias ment, reasoning, and planning erode, AD patients
that progressively erode cognition and everyday eventually lose decision-making capacity in every
functioning (Griffith et al., 2003; McKhann et al., sphere of life. Specific competencies that are lost
1984). As the first Baby Boomers approach retire- include the capacity to make medical decisions
ment age, the prevalence of dementia in the US (Marson, Hawkins, McInturff, & Harrell, 1997a;
is expected to rise dramatically (Alzheimer’s Marson et al., 1995c), to consent to research (Kim,
Association, 2008). Some estimates project that Caine, Currier, Leibovici, & Ryan, 2001), to
by 2030 there will be approximately 72 million manage financial affairs (Marson, 2001a; Marson
adults over the age of 65 in the US, representing et al., 2000), to execute a will (Spar & Garb, 1992;
an increase from 12% (in 2003) to 20% of the U.S. Walsh, Brown, Kaye, & Grigsby, 1997), to drive
population (Wan, Sengupta, Velkoff, & Debarros, (Drachman, Swearer, & Group, 1993; Hunt et al.,
2005). Moreover, the oldest-old (persons aged 85 1997), to manage medications (Barberger-Gateau,
and older) population in the US is expected to Dartigues, & Letenneur, 1993), to live indepen-
reach 9.6 million by 2030 and 20.9 million by 2050 dently, and ultimately to handle even the most
(Wan et al., 2005). Although the majority of older basic activities of daily life. Patients with
adults do not have dementia, advancing age is a Parkinson’s disease and dementia have demon-
major risk factor for cognitive impairment, and strated deficits in medical decision-making
the prevalence of AD in persons aged 65 and older capacity (Dymek et al., 2001a; Griffith, Dymek,
is estimated to reach 7.7 million by 2030 and 11 to Atchison, Harrell, & Marson, 2005; Martin et al.,
402 forensic neuropsychology

2008) and are likely to be impaired in other higher patients with cognitive impairment and demen-
order capacities such as financial management. tia. At the same time, we seek to incorporate new
Loss of competency in dementia has important studies and findings within the different chapter
consequences for patients and their families, for sections. We begin by outlining basic theoretical
health care and legal professionals, and for society concepts that provide a general context for under-
as a whole (Marson et al., 1994a). standing competency assessment and research.
As noted above, since the first edition of this We then focus on conceptual and empirical aspects
book, there have been important developments in of three specific civil competencies: medical deci-
the field of civil competency assessment in the sion-making capacity (treatment consent capac-
elderly. Befitting the continuing growth of this ity), financial capacity, and testamentary capacity
area, there has been a notable increase in peer- (capacity to execute a will). In the area of treat-
reviewed publications, particularly in the areas of ment consent, we consider a cognitive model of
consent to treatment and research, as reflected by consent capacity, and then review psychometric
several recent review articles (Dunn, Nowrangi, and neuropsychological studies of this compe-
Palmer, Jeste, & Saks, 2006; Karlawish, 2008; Lai tency in patients with AD, PD with dementia, and
& Karlawish, 2007; Palmer & Savla, 2007). There MCI. In the area of financial capacity, we present a
has been new attention paid to the competency of clinically based conceptual model, and then
patients in the prodromal stages of dementia, review psychometric and other studies of this
commonly referred to as mild cognitive impair- competency in patients with AD and MCI. In the
ment (MCI) (Jefferson et al., 2008; Okonkwo area of testamentary capacity, we present the legal
et al., 2007; Okonkwo et al., 2008a). Perhaps of elements of this capacity, and emphasize the con-
greatest significance, however, is a series of three tinuing need for conceptual and empirical research
handbooks on competency assessment in older regarding this important civil competency. We
adults that have been jointly published by the describe two approaches to clinical assessment of
American Bar Association (ABA) and American testamentary capacity: contemporaneous and ret-
Psychological Association (APA) (American Bar rospective, and note recent contributions by the
Association Commission on Law and Aging & ABA-APA working group to this area.
American Psychological Association, 2005; Finally, in a chapter epilogue, we note strengths
American Bar Association/American Psycho- and limitations of competency research in older
logical Association Assessment of Capacity in adults with MCI and dementia, and suggest direc-
Older Adults Project Working Group, 2006, 2008). tions for future research in the field.
These three handbooks, which respectively
address assessment of diminished capacity by C O N C E P T UA L I S S U E S I N
attorneys, by probate court judges in guardian- COMPETENCY AS SES SMENT
ship proceedings, and by psychologists, are the Competency is an elusive and frequently misun-
products of an interdisciplinary group of attor- derstood medical-legal construct (Marson,
neys and psychologists called the ABA-APA 2001b). Sound clinical assessment and empirical
Working Group for Assessment of Competency research in the area of competency requires iden-
in Older Adults. The handbooks represent a suc- tification and clarification of terminology and
cessful bridging of science and practice in the field basic concepts (Marson, Schmitt, Ingram, &
of competency assessment; each volume incorpo- Harrell, 1994b). In this section, we highlight a
rates a guiding conceptual framework along with number of key terms and principles related to
pragmatic content areas and appendices that pro- competency.
vide clear direction to professionals within the
discipline. Taken together, the body of new work Key Terms and Principles
since the first edition of Forensic Neuropsychology What is “Competency”?
underscores the clear emergence of civil compe- Competency refers to an individual’s legal
tency assessment as a distinct and expanding field capacity to make certain decisions and to perform
of clinical, ethical, and medical-legal research certain acts (Appelbaum & Gutheil, 1991). In the
(Karlawish & Schmitt, 2000; Marson & Ingram, U.S. legal system, adults are presumed to have the
1996; Moye & Marson, 2007). capacity to exercise choices and make decisions
In keeping with the prior edition, this chapter for themselves until proven otherwise (Appelbaum
presents conceptual and clinical approaches to & Roth, 1981). However, neurological or psychi-
assessing different civil competencies in older atric illness may impair one’s ability to make
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 403

rational decisions. In addition, some individuals or competency will refer to their clinical meaning,
with developmental or acquired disabilities in unless otherwise indicated.
childhood will never, even as adults, possess the
competency to make certain decisions. Under Multiple Competencies: Competency
these circumstances, the state, through exercise of to do What?
its protective parens patriae power, may deem The term competency is commonly used in an
these persons incompetent and appoint substitute undifferentiated way to describe a variety of
decision makers (Kapp, 1992). Accordingly, com- capacities (Marson et al., 1994b). Competency,
petency may be usefully defined as “a threshold however, is not a unitary construct; rather, there
requirement for an individual to retain the power are multiple specific competencies. The typical
to make decisions for themselves” (Appelbaum & adult possesses a number of distinct competen-
Gutheil, 1991) (p. 218). Since a legal declaration cies, including the capacity to make a will; to
of incompetency entails deprivation of rights drive; to consent to medical treatment; to manage
and autonomy, competency evaluations and financial affairs; and ultimately, to manage all of
determinations are serious matters (Appelbaum his or her personal affairs. Each competency
& Gutheil, 1991; Marson et al., 1994b). involves a distinct combination of functional
abilities and skills that distinguish it from other
Capacity versus Competency competencies (Grisso, 1986). For instance, the
The terms capacity and competency are often used cognitive and physical abilities requisite for
interchangeably (Marson, 2001b). This can be driving (knowledge of road signs, visual scanning
misleading, as the terms, although related, repre- abilities, reaction time, gross motor skills) are
sent distinct concepts (Kapp, 1992). Capacity arguably quite distinct from those involved in
denotes a clinical status as judged by a health care making a medical decision (e.g., understanding
professional, whereas competency denotes a legal medical concepts, short-term memory for medi-
status as determined by a legal professional (i.e., a cal symptoms and treatment options, rational
judge). A capacity evaluation involves a clinical weighing of risks and benefits) (Marson, 2001b).
assessment and judgment based on a patient’s his- In addition, each competency tends to operate
tory, presentation, and test performance. A judge within a specific and somewhat individualized
may consider such clinical capacity findings as context (Grisso, 1986). For example, issues related
part of his/her competency decision-making pro- to competency to consent to treatment almost
cess, but will also consider other sources of invariably arise in a hospital or medical setting.
authority, such as statutes, case law precedent, and The reality of multiple competencies suggests that
principles of equity and justice. It is important rather than asking “Is the individual competent?”
and useful to be mindful of the capacity/compe- one should ask “Is the individual competent to do
tency distinction, although for reasons of conve- X in Y context?” (Marson et al., 1994b).
nience the terms are often used interchangeably
(Marson, 2001b). Specific versus General Competency
It should be noted that recently judges and One useful distinction for analyzing different
other legal professionals have begun to favor the competencies is that of “specific” versus “general”
use of the term capacity in referencing matters of competency (Appelbaum & Gutheil, 1991).
legal competency, in response to concerns that General competence is defined as the capacity to
the term competency carries with it historically manage “all one’s affairs in an adequate manner”
pejorative and archaic connotations. This change (Appelbaum & Gutheil, 1991) (p. 219), and is the
of usage can present some difficulties in maintain- focus of most state statutes in the U.S. governing
ing the capacity-competency distinction discussed guardianship. Specific competency, in contrast,
above. Perhaps the best current solution is simply concerns the capacity to perform a specific act or
to distinguish between matters of “clinical set of specific actions (Appelbaum & Gutheil,
capacity” and “legal capacity” (American Bar 1991). As suggested above, there are many specific
Association Commission on Law and Aging & competencies recognized by the law (Marson,
American Psychological Association, 2005; 2001b), including the capacity to manage finan-
American Bar Association/American Psycholo- cial affairs, make a will, be a parent (adoption and
gical Association Assessment of Capacity in Older custody), stand trial, and consent to medical treat-
Adults Project Working Group, 2006, 2008). For ment. In the authors’ experience, each specific
purposes of this chapter, use of the terms capacity competency must be approached and analyzed
404 forensic neuropsychology

discretely, as each taps distinct underlying func- and Communicative Disorders and Stroke–
tional abilities. Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Asso-
ciation (NINCDS-ADRDA) criteria for probable
Limited Competency AD (McKhann et al., 1984) may nonetheless be
Because competency determination by its nature competent to consent to medical treatment or
results in a categorical assignment (i.e., competent research or to perform other activities such as
vs. incompetent), competency outcomes have driving or managing financial affairs. A key point
historically been treated as dichotomous proposi- here is that in making determinations of compe-
tions (Appelbaum & Gutheil, 1991). Limited tency, the examiner must perform a functional
competency refers to the fact that, within a gen- analysis; that is, the examiner must consider
eral or specific competency, an individual may whether the individual possesses the skills and
have the ability to perform some actions but not abilities integral to performing a specific act in its
others. For instance, a patient with mild dementia context. Certainly, diagnosis is a relevant factor in
of the AD type may be unable to engage in evaluating competency. However, because diag-
complex investment and financial decisions, but nosis conveys no specific functional information,
may retain the ability to write checks and manage it cannot by itself be dispositive of the competency
small daily sums of money (American Bar question (Marson et al., 1994b).
Association/American Psychological Association
Assessment of Capacity in Older Adults Project Mental Status Impairment Does Not
Working Group, 2008; Marson, 2001b). As such, Constitute Incompetence
this individual could be characterized as having For similar reasons, mental status and neurop-
limited competency to manage his or her financial sychological impairment cannot, in isolation,
affairs. The legal system has recognized the impor- decide issues of competency (Marson, 2001b).
tance of limited competency through its use of Such evaluations are important for diagnosing
limited guardianships and conservatorships dementia and for measuring level of cognitive
(American Bar Association/American Psycholo- impairment, and they are clearly highly relevant
gical Association Assessment of Capacity in Older to a competency evaluation. However, like diag-
Adults Project Working Group, 2006). nosis, they cannot by themselves be dispositive of
the competency issue (Grisso, 1986; High, 1992).
Intermittent Competency and As noted by Grisso, decision makers must go fur-
Restoration of Competency ther and “present the logic that links these clinical
It is important to recognize that competency observations [i.e., test results] to the capacities
status may change over time (Marson, 2001b). For with which the law is concerned” (Grisso, 1986,
instance, fluctuations in chronic psychiatric p. 8). For example, neuropsychological impair-
illness may periodically compromise an individu- ments in attention, auditory verbal comprehen-
al’s ability to give consent to receive medication or sion, and abstractive capacity become relevant to
to manage his or her personal affairs. In situations a competency determination only when they are
of intermittent competency, periodic re-evalua- meaningfully related to competency-specific
tions are indicated. In some cases, the underlying functional impairments (e.g., the inability to
neurological or psychiatric condition compromis- express a treatment preference, or to rationally
ing an individual’s competency may resolve, result- explain the treatment choice) (Marson, Chatterjee,
ing in the individual regaining decision-making Ingram, & Harrell, 1996; Marson, Cody, Ingram,
abilities. In such cases, legal competency may be & Harrell, 1995a; Marson et al., 1997a).
restorable through a formal court hearing and
decision (American Bar Association/American Individual Values and Preferences
Psychological Association Assessment of Capacity An individual’s lifelong personal values and
in Older Adults Project Working Group, 2006). preferences, and historical patterns of decision
making, are important facts to consider in
Diagnosis Does Not Constitute conducting a competency evaluation. The consis-
Incompetence tency of an older adult’s current choices with his/
A diagnosis of dementia or other neurological or her historical values and preferences is often a
psychiatric disorder is not synonymous with critical factor in a clinician deciding between
incompetence (Marson, 2001b). A patient who competency and incompetency. Knowledge of
meets the National Institute of Neurological an individual’s core values can play a particularly
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 405

significant role in guardianship cases, as it can of whether a dementia patient is competent to


guide a court’s candidate selection and assign- consent to treatment is of considerable interest to
ment of a guardian, and can (and should) shape health care providers (Farnsworth, 1990; Marson
the guardian’s own activities on behalf of the et al., 1994b; Sherlock, 1984).
protected individual (American Bar Association/
American Psychological Association Assessment Cognitive Model for Consent Capacity
of Capacity in Older Adults Project Working Consent capacity may be conceptualized as con-
Group, 2006). sisting of three core cognitive tasks: comprehen-
sion and encoding of treatment information;
C A PA C I T Y T O C O N S E N T T O information processing and internally arriving at
M E D I C A L T R E AT M E N T a treatment decision; and communication of the
The capacity to consent to medical treatment treatment decision to a clinical professional
(treatment consent capacity) is a fundamental (Alexander, 1988; Marson, 2001b; Marson &
aspect of personal autonomy, as it concerns inti- Harrell, 1999). These core cognitive tasks occur in
mate decisions regarding the care of a person’s a specific context: a patient’s dialogue with a phy-
body and mind (Marson, 2001b). Consent capac- sician, psychologist, or other health care profes-
ity refers to a patient’s cognitive and emotional sional about a medical condition and potential
capacity to accept a proposed treatment, to refuse treatments (Marson et al., 1995a). The compre-
treatment, or to select among treatment alterna- hension/encoding task involves oral and written
tives (Grisso, 1986; Tepper & Elwork, 1984). In comprehension and encoding of novel and often
the US, consent capacity is the cornerstone of the complex medical information presented verbally
medical-legal doctrine of informed consent, to the patient by the treating clinician. The infor-
which requires that a valid consent to treatment mation processing/decision-making task involves
be informed, voluntary, and competent (Kapp, the patient processing (at different levels depend-
1992; Marson, Ingram, Cody, & Harrell, 1995b). ing on the complexity of the information and
From a functional standpoint, consent capacity treatment options) the consent and other infor-
may be viewed as an “advanced activity of daily mation presented, integrating this information
life” (ADL) (Wolinsky & Johnson, 1991) and with established declarative and episodic knowl-
an important aspect of functional health and edge (including personal values and risk prefer-
independent living skills in both younger and ences), rationally weighing this information, and
older adults. arriving internally at a treatment decision. The
As a competency, consent capacity is distinc- decision communication task involves the patient
tive for several reasons: (1) it arises in a medical communicating his/her treatment decision to the
rather than legal setting; (2) it generally involves clinician in some understandable form (e.g., oral,
a physician, psychologist, or other health care written, and/or gestural expression of consent/
professional, rather than a legal professional, as nonconsent).
decision maker; and (3) these judgments are This model affords a basis for understanding
rarely subject to judicial review (Grisso, 1986). As the cognitive structure of consent capacity deci-
discussed above, clinicians are not deciding sions, and also loss of competency in neurodegen-
competency in a formal legal sense. However, erative disorders (Marson, 2001b). For example,
their decisions can have the same effect as a short-term memory ability is relevant to consent
courtroom determination insofar as the patient capacity because impaired learning and short-
will often yield decision-making power to others term recall will limit the amount of encoded
(Appelbaum & Gutheil, 1991). medical information available for further proce-
As discussed below, in dementia and more ssing. Similarly, receptive language skills are
specifically in Alzheimer’s disease, consent capac- relevant to consent capacity because they afford
ity becomes increasingly impaired as the disease comprehension of treatment-related information.
progresses (Marson et al., 1995a). As cognitive Conceptualization and executive functions are
skills such as memory, reasoning, judgment, and important to consent capacity because they are
planning decline, patients lose the ability to necessary for organized processing of treatment
encode and process medical information and to information. Judgment and reasoning abilities
make coherent treatment decisions. Because it is are equally important as they make possible a
often unclear at what point cognitive decline patient’s rational weighing of all this information
translates into loss of competency, the question and his/her internal determination of a treatment
406 forensic neuropsychology

choice. Expressive language skills, in turn, may be Administration involves the individual
important because of their relevance to effective simultaneously reading and listening to an oral
communication of the treatment choice in the presentation of the vignette information.
patient-clinician dialogue. In this regard, it should The individual then answers questions designed
be noted that treatment consent capacity is a to test consent capacity under five different
highly verbally mediated competency (the only legal thresholds or standards. These standards
pragmatic arguably is the signature on the form) or thresholds have been drawn from case law
and thus verbal measures are likely to load highly and the psychiatric literature (Appelbaum &
on it (Marson & Harrell, 1999). Grisso, 1988; Roth, Meisel, & Lidz, 1977). They
are set forth below in order of increasing difficulty
Psychometric Assessment of for patients with AD type dementia (Marson et al.,
Consent Capacity 1995c):
In recent years a number of investigators have
developed instruments for assessment of consent S1: capacity simply to “evidence” a treatment
capacity in different patient populations (Cea & choice;
Fisher, 2003; Dunn et al., 2006; Edelstein, Nygren, S3: capacity to “appreciate” the consequences
Northrop, Staats, & Pool, 1993; Grisso & of a treatment choice;
Appelbaum, 1991, 1995; Grisso, Appelbaum,
Mulvey, & Fletcher, 1995; Janofsky, McCarthy, & S4: capacity to reason about treatment/
Folstein, 1992; Marson, Earnst, Jamil, Bartolucci, provide “rational reasons” for a treatment
& Harrell, 2000; Marson et al., 1995c; Marson, choice; and
McInturff, Hawkins, Bartolucci, & Harrell, 1997b; S5: capacity to “understand” the treatment
Vellinga, Smit, van Leeuwen, van Tilburg, & situation and treatment choices.
Jonker, 2004). These instruments represent a fun-
damental advance in the competency field, as they The above standards represent different
have brought psychometric rigor, in the form of thresholds for evaluating capacity to consent
standardized content and administration, and (Marson et al., 1994b). For example, S1 (evidenc-
quantitative scoring and norming, to an area in ing a choice) requires nothing more for compe-
which previously subjective clinical interviews tency than an individual’s communication of a
were the standard. treatment choice. S3 (appreciating consequences)
Our group has developed an instrument for is of moderate difficulty and requires patients to
empirically assessing the capacity of dementia appreciate how a treatment choice will affect
patients to consent to medical treatment under them personally. S4 (rational reasons) bases
different legal standards (Capacity to Consent to competency upon an individual’s ability to supply
Treatment Instrument, CCTI; Marson et al., rational reasons for the treatment choice. S5 is a
1995c). Specifically, we developed two clinical comprehension standard and requires a patient
vignettes (i.e., neoplasm vignette and cardiac to demonstrate conceptual and factual knowledge
vignette) designed to test competency under concerning the medical condition, symptoms,
five distinct standards of consent capacity. Each and treatment choices and their respective
vignette presents a hypothetical medical problem risks/benefits. It should be noted that these
and symptoms, and two treatment alternatives four standards can be readily applied to other
with associated risks and benefits. The medical competencies to consent, such as capacity to
content of each vignette was reviewed by a neu- consent to research, and to decisional capacity
rologist with expertise with the elderly and generally.
dementia. The vignettes, which are presented In addition to these four standards, there is an
orally and in writing in an uninterrupted disclo- additional consent-related ability described as
sure format (Grisso & Appelbaum, 1991), were making the “reasonable” treatment choice (when
written at a fifth- to sixth-grade reading level the alternative is manifestly unreasonable) (Roth
(Flesch, 1974) with low syntactic complexity et al., 1977). This ability, which we reference as
and a moderate information load. The adminis- [S2], emphasizes outcome rather than the mere
tration format for each vignette approximates fact of a decision or how it has been reached. The
an informed consent dialogue and requires the patient who fails to make a decision that is roughly
subject to consider two different treatment congruent with the decision that a “reasonable”
options with associated risks and benefits. person in like circumstances would make is
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 407

viewed as incompetent. [S2] is not an accepted Consent Capacity in Patients with


legal standard for judging consent capacity Alzheimer’s Disease
because of concerns about arbitrariness in deter- Capacity Performance and Impairment
mining what constitutes a “reasonable choice” Outcomes in AD
(Tepper & Elwork, 1984). Accordingly, [S2] is In an initial study, we used the CCTI to investi-
referenced in brackets to distinguish it from the gate loss of competency in patients with mild
other four established Ss. However, [S2] remains Alzheimer’s disease (n = 15), patients with mod-
useful as a means of understanding treatment erate AD (n = 14), and older controls (n = 15).
preferences of patients with neurocognitive Performance on the five CCTI standards was
disorders (Dymek, Atchison, Harrell, & Marson, compared across groups. As shown below in
2001b). Table 15.1, the CCTI discriminated the perfor-
The CCTI has a detailed and well-operational- mance of the normal controls, the mild AD group,
ized scoring system for each vignette according to and the moderate AD group on three of the five
the specific legal standards outlined above. In standards. While the three groups performed
prior work with AD patients, three trained raters equivalently on minimal standards requiring
achieved high inter-rater reliability for standards merely a treatment choice (S1) or the reasonable
with both interval (r > . 83, p < .0001) (LS3-LS5) treatment choice [S2], mild AD patients per-
and categorical scales (>96% agreement) (S1, formed significantly below controls on more
[S2]) (Marson et al., 1995c). The CCTI scoring difficult standards requiring rational reasons (S4)
system permits evaluation of both an individual’s and understanding treatment information (S5).
competency performance and competency Moderate AD patients performed significantly
impairment status on each standard. By “compe- below controls on appreciation of consequences
tency performance,” we refer to the quantitative (S3), rational reasons (S4), and understanding
score that an individual achieves on a particular treatment (S5), and significantly below mild AD
standard as determined by the CCTI scoring patients on S4 and S5 (Marson et al., 1995c).
system. By “competency impairment status,” we In addition to capacity performance, the
refer to the level of impairment and assigned cat- impairment outcome of AD patients on the
egorical outcome (competent, marginally compe- standards was classified as capable, marginally
tent, or incompetent) of an individual on a capable, or incapable using psychometric cutoff
standard, based on use of psychometric cutoff scores referenced to control group performance
scores derived from normal control performance on each S. As shown in Table 15.2, assignment of
(Marson et al., 1995c). It should be emphasized capacity status resulted in a consistent pattern of
that these impairment outcomes are useful compromise (marginally capable and incapable
research classifications, but do not represent outcomes) among AD patients that related
actual clinical or legal judgments of capacity or both to dementia stage and stringency of stan-
competency. dard. Mild AD patients demonstrated substantial

TABLE 15.1 PERFORMANCE ON CCTI CAPACIT Y STANDARDS BY


GROUP 2
N LS1 [LS2]* LS3 LS4 LS5
0–4 0–1 0–10 0–12 0–70
Older Controls 15 4.0 (0.0) .93 8.7a (1.2) 10.3b, c (3.8) 58.3b, d (6.6)
Mild AD 15 3.9 (0.4) 1.00 7.1 (2.0) 6.1e (3.4) 27.3e (9.6)
Moderate AD 14 3.6 (0.9) .79 5.9 (2.7) 2.3 (2.4) 17.9 (10.6)

* No group differences emerged on [LS2] (X2 = 4.2, p = .12)


a
Normal mean differs significantly from moderate AD mean (p < .001)
b
(p < .0001)
c
Normal mean differs significantly from mild AD mean (p < .01)
d
(p < .0001)
e
Mild AD mean differs significantly from moderate AD mean (p < .01)
408 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 15.2 EXPERIMENTAL OUTCOMES BY CCTI


CAPACIT Y STANDARD AND GROUP 3
Standard/Group Capable Marginally Capable Incapable
S1 Evidencing Choice
Controls 15 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
Mild AD 13 (87%) 2 (13%) 0 (0%)
Moderate AD 11 (79%) 1 (7%) 2 (14%)
[S2] Reasonable Choice
Controls 14 (93%) 1 (7%)
Mild AD 15 (100%) 0 (0%)
Moderate AD 11 (79%) 3 (21%)
S3 Appreciate Consequences
Controls 14 (93%) 1 (7%) 0 (0%)
Mild AD 10 (67%) 2 (14%) 3 (20%)
Moderate AD 5 (36%) 2 (14%) 7 (50%)
S4 Reasoning
Controls 14 (93%) 1 (7%) 0 (0%)
Mild AD 7 (47%) 5 (33%) 3 (20%)
Moderate AD 1 (7%) 3 (22%) 10 (71%)
S5 Understand Treatment
Controls 15 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
Mild AD 0 (0%) 1 (7%) 14 (93%)
Moderate AD 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 14 (100%)

*Control (N=15); Mild AD (N=15); Moderate AD (N=14)

compromise on S4 (53%) and S5 (100%), the two choice (S1) and making the reasonable choice
most stringent and clinically relevant CCTI stan- ([S2]), but significantly below controls on com-
dards. Moderate AD patients demonstrated sig- plex standards of appreciation, reasoning, and
nificant compromise on the three more complex understanding (S3, S4, S5) (p < .02). Control
and clinically relevant standards S3 (64%), S4 performance was stable over time on all capacity
(93%), and S5 (100%). The results raised the con- standards. At one-year follow-up, the mild AD
cern that, depending on circumstances such as group did not show significant decline from
the complexity of intervention, the level of treat- baseline on any capacity standard. However, at
ment risk, and the standard to be applied, a major- two-year follow-up, the mild AD group showed
ity of moderate AD patients and many patients significant declines from baseline on the three
with mild AD may lack consent capacity (Marson, complex standards (S3, S4, S5) (p < .02), and a
2001b; Marson et al., 1995c). trend on one of the simple standards (S1). The
findings replicated the prior finding that mild
Longitudinal Decline in Consent AD patients have impaired consent capacity
Capacity in Mild AD Patients at baseline and extended prior work by showing
More recently, our group has investigated consent that individuals with mild AD experience
capacity longitudinally in patients with mild AD significant decline on complex consent abilities of
(Huthwaite et al., 2006). We conducted a two-year appreciation, reasoning, and understanding over a
study comparing healthy older adults (n=15) and two-year period. The findings highlight the value
mild AD patients (n=20) using the CCTI. At base- of early assessment and regular monitoring at
line, mild AD patients performed equivalently two-year intervals of decisional capacity in
with controls on simple standards of evidencing a patients with mild AD (Huthwaite et al., 2006).
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 409

Cognitive Predictors of Consent and the progressive cognitive changes character-


Capacity in AD Patients istic of AD, and represent an initial step toward a
In addition to providing a standardized basis for neurologic model of competency (Marson et al.,
evaluating competency performance and out- 1996; Marson et al., 1995a).
come, instruments like the CCTI also provide a Since these initial studies, a number of other
psychometric criterion for investigating neuro- investigators have begun to examine the relation-
cognitive changes associated with loss of consent ship of specific neuropsychological abilities to
capacity in neurodegenerative disorders such as decisional capacity (Moye, Karel, Gurrera, & Azar,
AD. We used the CCTI and a neuropsychological 2006; Palmer & Jeste, 2006). An excellent review
test battery sensitive to dementia in the above of work in this area has recently been published
sample to identify cognitive predictors of declin- by Palmer and colleagues (Palmer & Savla, 2007),
ing competency performance in AD patients who note that “this line of [neuropsychological]
under the four standards (Marson et al., 1996; research may foster the development of better
Marson et al., 1995a). Table 15.3 presents stepwise consent procedures, as well as aid in refining the
multiple regression results for the combined AD construct of decisional capacity toward a form
group for these standards. that better reflects the underlying neurocognitive
Findings from these psychometric studies processes” (Palmer & Savla, 2007) (p. 1047). In
suggest that multiple cognitive functions are asso- the sections that follow, we reference some of
ciated with loss of consent capacity in patients these new studies that seek to identify the cogni-
with Alzheimer’s disease, as measured by the tive underpinnings of consent capacity deficits in
CCTI standards (Marson et al., 1996; Marson different neurocognitive disorders.
et al., 1995a). Deficits in conceptualization,
semantic memory, and probably verbal recall Consent Capacity in Individuals with
appear to be associated with impaired capacity in Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI)
both mild and moderate AD patients to under- Treatment Consent Capacity in
stand a treatment situation and choices (S5). Patients with MCI
Deficits in simple executive function (word flu- Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a transitional
ency) appear linked to the impaired capacity of phase between normal cognitive aging and AD
both mild and moderate AD patients to provide and other dementias (Morris et al., 2001; Petersen
rational reasons for a treatment choice (S4), and et al., 2001). Individuals with MCI may present
to the impaired capacity of moderate AD patients with memory or other cognitive deficits, and the
to identify the consequences of a treatment choice amnestic form of MCI (aMCI) represents the sub-
(S3). Finally, receptive aphasia and semantic type most likely to progress to AD (Palmer,
memory loss (severe dysnomia) may be associ- Backman, Winblad, & Fratiglioni, 2008; Tabert
ated with the impaired ability of advanced AD et al., 2006; Yaffe, Petersen, Lindquist, Kramer, &
patients to evidence a simple treatment choice Miller, 2006). Although intact functional status
(S1). The results offer insight into the relationship was originally considered a key diagnostic feature
between different legal thresholds of competency distinguishing MCI from AD (Petersen et al.,

TABLE 15.3 MULTIVARIATE COGNITIVE PREDICTORS * OF CAPACIT Y


PERFORMANCE IN THE AD GROUP N=29 4
S1 S3 S4 S5
(Evidencing Choice) (Appreciating Consequences) (Reasoning) (Understanding
Treatment)
R2 p R2 p R2 p R2 p
SAC .44 .0001 CFL 58 .0001 DRS IP .36 .0008 DRS CON .70 .0001
BNT .11 .001

* No measures achieved univariate or multivariate significance for the control group. CFL=Controlled Oral Word Association Test (Benton &
Hamsher, 1978); BNT= Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983); DRS CON= Dementia Rating Scale Conceptualization
subscale (Mattis, 1976); DRS IP= Dementia Rating Scale Initiation/Perseveration subscale (Mattis, 1976); SAC= Simple Auditory Comprehension
Screen (Eisenson, 1954)
410 forensic neuropsychology

1999), it is increasingly recognized that MCI we found that verbal memory was the primary
involves subtle declines in higher order func- predictor of medical decision-making capacity for
tional abilities (Artero, Touchon, & Ritchie, 2001; three of the four CCTI standards (understanding,
Griffith et al., 2003; Okonkwo et al., 2007; reasoning, appreciation), and executive function
Okonkwo et al., 2008a; Ritchie, Artero, & Touchon, was a secondary predictor. The cognitive models
2001; Touchon & Ritchie, 1999), including medi- were generally robust in the aMCI group, with
cal decision-making capacity (Okonkwo et al., overall R2 values of .53 and .30 for understanding
2007; Okonkwo et al., 2008a; Okonkwo et al., and reasoning, respectively. The overall R2 value
2008b). As discussed below, several recent studies for the appreciation model was comparatively
have shown that declines in treatment consent small at .16; this finding was consistent with prior
capacity occur in individuals with MCI prior to data suggesting that appreciation is a dimension of
the emergence of a frank dementia. consent capacity more difficult to cognitively
Our group recently examined medical deci- model (Dymek et al., 2001a; Gurrera, Moye, Karel,
sion-making capacity in 56 older controls, 60 indi- Azar, & Armesto, 2006). Overall, the findings
viduals with aMCI, and 31 patients with mild AD clearly indicate that medical decision-making
(Okonkwo et al., 2007). Results from this study capacity is mediated primarily by short-term
showed that individuals diagnosed with aMCI verbal memory in MCI, a finding highly consistent
exhibit impaired medical decision-making capac- with the amnestic deficits characteristic of MCI.
ity relative to older controls. The MCI participants
in our study performed more poorly than controls Research Consent Capacity in
on cognitively complex consent standards of Patients with MCI
understanding treatment, reasoning about treat- Consent capacity is relevant not only in a medical
ment, and appreciating consequences of treat- treatment decision-making context, but also in
ment. Interestingly, the individuals with aMCI the context of decisional capacity to participate in
performed better than patients with mild AD on research (Jefferson et al., 2008; Karlawish et al.,
standards of reasoning and understanding. These 2008). Jefferson and colleagues (2008) recently
findings suggest that consent capacity is also com- examined cognitive correlates of decisional capac-
promised in MCI but to a lesser degree than in a ity in a sample of individuals with MCI (n=40)
frank AD-type dementia. and 40 cognitively intact older adults. The authors
We have also investigated longitudinal decline assessed capacity to give informed consent for a
in medical decision-making capacity in a group of hypothetical clinical trial, as well as examined
healthy older controls (n = 88) and individuals neurocognitive functioning across a variety of
with aMCI (n = 116) over a period of one to three cognitive domains. Results from this study indi-
years (mean = 1.7 years). Results from this study cate that individuals with MCI perform more
(Okonkwo et al., 2008a) showed that declines in poorly than controls on measures of decisional
medical decision-making capacity are detectable capacity, particularly understanding and reason-
in individuals with aMCI over a three-year period. ing. The data also indicate that executive dysfunc-
Specifically, individuals with aMCI exhibited pro- tion is associated with impaired decisional
gressive decline in their ability to understand con- capacity in MCI. This finding contrasted with our
sent information and a similar trend emerged for group’s prior observation that verbal memory is
decline in ability to reason about treatment. The preeminent as the predictor in MCI. Overall, this
data also indicated that individuals with aMCI study’s findings indicate that decisional capacity
exhibit a marked drop in their ability to under- declines in MCI occur in both research and
stand consent information at the time of conver- clinical settings, and suggest that consent capacity
sion to AD, and that the rate of decline on this be carefully evaluated in potential research
ability accelerates after conversion. participants with MCI.

Cognitive Predictors of Consent Consent Capacity in Patients with


Capacity in MCI Parkinson’s Disease
We have also developed cognitive models of med- Over the past decade, most medical decision-
ical decision-making capacity in healthy and making capacity research involving older adult
cognitively impaired older adults (Okonkwo et al., dementia populations has focused on AD
2008b). In a study of 56 older controls, 60 indi- (Karlawish et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2001; Marson,
viduals with aMCI, and 31 patients with mild AD, Earnst, Jamil, Bartolucci, & Harrell, 2000; Marson
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 411

et al., 1997b; Marson et al., 1994b), with relatively the PD patients were demented, as reported by
little attention paid to consent capacity in patients the study neurologist and as reflected by the mean
with Parkinson’s disease. PD is one of the most DRS Total Score of 117 (sd=14.5) (Dymek et al.,
prevalent and disabling of neurologic disorders 2001b). Thus, the PD patient group in this study
(Jacobs, Stern, & Mayeux, 1997); over time, patients probably closely approximated how a formally
become extremely incapacitated by their progres- diagnosed group of patients with Parkinson’s dis-
sive motor, cognitive, and neuropsychiatric impair- ease dementia syndrome (PDDS) would perform.
ments (Marson & Dymek, 2004). Impairment of Accordingly, we reference the PD group as PDDS
medical decision-making capacity in PD and its to reflect its probable dementia status.
dementia syndrome is likely to differ in important Results from this study revealed that the PDDS
ways from that in AD, as these two dementias have group performed worse than the control group on
differing neuropathology and neurologic sub- the four core CCTI consent abilities: evidencing a
strates, and cognitive, psychiatric, and motor fea- choice, appreciating consequences, reasoning about
tures (Farlow & Cummings, 2008; Griffith et al., treatment (rational reasons), and understanding
2008; Heindel, Salmon, Shults, Walicke, & Butters, treatment (S1, S3-S5). In contrast, PDDS patients
1989; Mahler & Cummings, 1990; Stern, Richards, performed equivalently with controls on the
Sano, & Mayeux, 1993). The prominence of motor experimental consent ability of reasonable choice
dysfunction in PD has tended to obscure the con- ([S2]) (Dymek et al., 2001b) (Table 15.4).
tributions of cognitive impairment and dementia Impairment outcome status of PDDS patients
to patients’ functional decline. In this regard, con- on the standards was classified as capable, mar-
sent capacity is a competency that is particularly ginally capable, or incapable using psychometric
suitable for study in PD, as it is primarily a verbally cutoff scores referenced to control group perfor-
based construct (Marson & Dymek, 2004). mance (Marson et al., 1995c) (Table 15.5). Similar
to AD patients (see above), PD patients demon-
Capacity Performance and Impairment strated a pattern of capacity compromise (defined
Outcomes in Patients with PD as the combination of marginally capable and
and Dementia incapable outcomes) that related to stringency of
Our research group has conducted several studies the standard: [S2] (5%), S1 (30%), S3 (45%), S4
of consent capacity and cognitive performance in (55%), and S5 (80%).
individuals with PD. In 2001, we published find- These data suggest that medical decision-
ings from a study of medical decision-making making capacity is impaired in individuals with
capacity in 20 older adults with idiopathic PD and PDDS. In addition, findings from this study
cognitive impairment (Dymek et al., 2001b). indicate that patients with PDDS may have more
Although a formal dementia diagnosis using difficulty with complex and clinically challenging
DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, aspects of medical decision making (i.e., reason-
1994) was not used in the study, most if not all of ing and understanding). Similar to findings in

TABLE 15.4 GROUP COMPARISONS ON DEMOGRAPHIC, DEMENTIA SCREEN, AND


CCTI CONSENT STANDARD VARIABLES 5
Older Controls PDDS Patients
Variable (n = 20) (n = 20) F p
Mean sd Range Mean sd Range
Age 68.1 (5.8) [60–79] 75.0 (7.5) [63–86] 10.4 .003
Education 14.8 (2.3) [9–18] 14.3 (3.1) [9–20] 0.34 .56
DRS Total Score 141.2 (3.1) [138–144] 117.3 (14.5) [72–129] 51.8 .0001
S1-Evidencing Choice 4.0 (0.0) [4] 3.6 (.8) [1–4] 5.9 .020
[S2]-Reasonable Choice 0.95 (0.2) [0–1] 0.95 (0.2) [0–1] — —
S3-Appreciate Consequences 8.9 (1.2) [6–10] 7.5 (2.5) [1–10] 5.7 .022
S4-Reasoning about Treatment 10.9 (3.7) [6–21] 5.1 (3.2) [1–11] 27.9 .000
S5-Understanding Treatment 59.0 (6.0) [51–68] 36.8 (13.0) [1–60] 47.8 .000
412 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 15.5 EXPERIMENTAL CAPACIT Y OUTCOMES BY


CCTI STANDARD AND PATIENT GROUP 6
Capable Marginally Incapable
Capable
S1 Evidencing Choice
Controls 20 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
PDDS Patients 14 (70%) 5 (25%) 1 (5%)
[S2] Reasonable Choice
Controls 19 (95%) — 1 (5%)
PDDS Patients 19 (95%) — 1 (5%)
S3 Appreciate Consequences
Controls 17 (85%) 3 (15%) 0 (0%)
PDDS Patients 11 (55%) 5 (25%) 4 (20%)
S4 Reasoning about Treatment
Controls 20 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
PDDS Patients 9 (45%) 10 (50%) 1 (5%)
S5 Understanding Treatment
Controls 20 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
PDDS Patients 4 (20%) 3 (15%) 13 (65%)

Capable: For S3-S5, scores falling at or above 1½ SDs below the control group mean on the S.
For S1, a score of 4. For [S2], a score of 1. Marginally Capable: For S3-S5, scores falling between
1½ and 2½ SDs below the control group mean on the S. For S1, a score of 3. No marginally capable
outcomes are possible on [S2]. Incapable: For S3-S5, scores falling below 2½ or more SDs below
the control group mean on the S. For S1, a score of 0–2. For [S2], a score of 0.

studies with AD patients (Marson et al., 1995c), it and no predictor models are reflected in the two
appears that the four CCTI standards may be tables (Dymek et al., 2001b).
hierarchical in difficulty for PDDS patients, with These findings suggest that declines in execu-
standards tapping reasoning (S4) and medical tive functions, and to a lesser extent memory, are
treatment information comprehension (S5) as key neurocognitive changes associated with com-
being the most difficult and requiring a level of petency loss in patients with PD (Dymek et al.,
information retention and processing that may be 2001b). Executive dysfunction was closely associ-
beyond the capacity of many PDDS patients. ated with PDDS patient performance on S5
(understanding treatment) and also S4 (reasoning
Cognitive Predictors of Consent about treatment). Specifically, simple measures of
Capacity in PD patients executive function (i.e., EXIT 25; Royall, Mahurin,
with Dementia & Gray, 1992) and memory (i.e., DRS Memory
We also examined cognitive predictors of PDDS subscale; Mattis, 1976) predicted S5 performance,
patient performance and outcome on the CCTI together accounting for 68% of score variance.
capacity standards. Tables 15.6 and 15.7 present Using the EXIT 25, a nonparametric discriminant
the univariate correlates and also the multivariate function analysis (DFA), showed a very high
predictors of PDDS patient performance and classification rate of S5 impairment outcomes
capacity outcome on three of the standards. No (90%), which increased to 95% when both the
cognitive measures correlated significantly with EXIT 25 and DRS Memory were used. The EXIT
S3 or [S2] on the univariate level (Dymek et al., 25 is a bedside test of simple executive abilities,
2001b), and thus these two standards were including verbal and spatial fluency, inhibition,
excluded from subsequent multivariate analyses primitive reflexes, and set flexibility (Royall et al.,
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 413

TABLE 15.6 COGNITIVE PREDICTORS OF CAPACIT Y STANDARD


PERFORMANCE FOR PDDS PATIENTS N=20 7

Standard Variable(s) Predictor Univariate Correlation Stepwise Regression

r p cumR2 p

S1: Evidencing Choice DRS Memory .73 .000 .55 .000 STEP 1
WAIS-R .67 .002
Comprehension
DRS Attention .58 .008
EXIT 25 −.53 .02
S4: Rational Reasons EXIT 25 −.67 .002 .45 .002 STEP 1
Trails B −.60 .005
DRS Attention .58 .008
WMS-LM II .48 .04
S5: Understanding Treatment EXIT 25 −.75 .000 .56 .000 STEP 1
DRS Memory .71 .000 .68 .000 STEP 2
WAIS-R .70 .001
Comprehension
WMS-R LM II .65 .002

1992). DRS Memory is a composite measure of as recall of factually complex material (Dymek
memory, consisting of short-term verbal recall et al., 2001b; Dymek, Marson, & Harrell, 1999).
items, orientation items, and verbal and visual Executive dysfunction was also associated
recognition items (Mattis, 1976). Thus, basic exec- with PDDS patient performance on S4 (reason-
utive, and to a lesser extent memory, functions ing). The EXIT 25 was the key predictor of S4
appear to mediate PDDS patients’ capacity to com- performance, accounting for 45% of score vari-
prehend a treatment situation and choices. This ance. Using the EXIT 25, nonparametric DFA
finding was consistent with the task demands of showed a very high classification rate of S4 impair-
S5, which require conceptual organization as well ment outcomes (90%), which increased to 95%

TABLE 15.7 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PREDICTORS OF


EXPERIMENTAL CAPACIT Y STANDARD OUTCOMES IN THE
PDDS GROUP 8
Standard Predictor Variable (s) Classification
Accuracy Rate*
S1: Evidencing Choice DRS Memory 70%
DRS Memory/WAIS-R Comprehension 100%
S4: Rational Reasons EXIT 25 90%
EXIT 25/Trails B 95%
S5: Understanding Treatment EXIT 25 90%
EXIT 25/DRS Memory 95%

* Percentage of capacity outcomes on each standard that were accurately classified by the respective neuro-
psychological predictor variable(s). As there are three outcomes (capable, marginally capable, incapable), a chance
classification rate would be 33%.
414 forensic neuropsychology

when using both EXIT 25 and Part B of the Trail suggest a link between fronto-striatal dysfunc-
Making Test (Trails B) (Reitan, 1958). Trails B is a tion, cognitive impairment (particularly executive
measure of visuomotor sequencing and set flexi- dysfunction), and competency loss in PD (Dymek
bility that is strongly associated with executive et al., 2001a; Marson & Dymek, 2004).
ability (Reitan, 1958). Thus, basic executive func-
tions also appear to mediate PDDS patients’ Consent Capacity in Patients with
capacity to provide rational reasons for a choice of PD Without Dementia
medical treatment. This finding was consistent Consent capacity is also compromised in
with the task demands of S4, which require an individuals with PD who exhibit cognitive impair-
individual to integrate information regarding two ment but do not meet criteria for dementia. In a
treatment choices and their risk/benefit profiles, recent study (Martin et al., 2008b) of 16 patients
and to provide logical reasons (pro and con) for with PD and cognitive impairment without
his/her treatment choice. dementia (PD-CIND), 16 patients with PD
Simple memory and comprehension/ dementia (PDD), and 22 healthy older adults, we
judgment abilities were associated with PDDS examined medical decision-making capacity with
patient performance on S1. As discussed above, the CCTI. We assessed capacity performance on
S1 is a minimal standard requiring only commu- the five different consent standards and assigned
nication of a treatment choice. DRS Memory impairment outcomes (capable, marginally capa-
emerged as the only multivariate predictor of ble, or incapable) on the standards for the two
PDDS patients’ S1 scores, accounting for 55% of patient groups.
the variance and correctly classifying 70% The findings from this study revealed that,
of patient impairment outcomes. When DRS relative to controls, PD-CIND patients demon-
Memory was coupled with WAIS-R Compre- strated significant impairment on the understand-
hension, they together correctly classified 100% of ing treatment consent standard, clinically the
S1 competency outcomes. These findings suggest most stringent CCTI standard. Relative to con-
that simple memory and comprehension/ trols and PD-CIND patients, PDD patients were
judgment deficits underlie PD patients’ declining impaired on the three clinical standards of under-
capacity for simply communicating a treatment standing, reasoning, and appreciation. These
choice (S1). results suggest that impairment in decisional
The findings from this study are consistent capacity is already present in cognitively impaired
with our current understanding of PD as a disor- patients with PD without dementia and worsens
der of fronto-striatal circuitry with associated as these patients progress to the point of demen-
executive dysfunction (Lichter, 2001; Mahurin, tia. As such, these finding indicate that clinicians
Feher, Nance, Levy, & Pirozzolo, 1993; White, Au, and researchers should carefully assess decisional
Durso, & Moss, 1992). The most problematic capacity in all patients with PD with cognitive
CCTI standards for the PD patients were the rea- impairment.
soning and comprehension standards (S4 and S5)
that are the most cognitively complex (Dymek Consent Capacity in Patients
et al., 1999; Marson et al., 1995c). Previous with PD Dementia versus
research has shown that PD patients perform well Patients with AD
on simple cognitive tasks; however, as task com- An important area of future competency research
plexity increases, performance deteriorates, likely involves comparisons of capacity performance
a result of impaired higher order executive across neurocognitive disorders. Such studies can
control of cognitive processes (Filoteo, Maddox, reveal how different neuropathologies can give
Ing, & Song, 2007; Girotti et al., 1986; Gotham, rise to differing patterns of cognitive impairment
Brown, & Marsden, 1988; Lichter, 2001; Troster, that in turn can result in different forms of capac-
2008). The findings are also consistent with ity impairment. We conducted an initial study of
research on patients with frontal lobe dementia this type by comparing the CCTI performance of
and significant executive dysfunction who show older controls (n=18), patients with PD and
impaired decision making capacity despite intact dementia (PDDS) (n=17), and patients with mild
language, memory, perception, and the absence of AD (n=22) (Griffith et al., 2005). The PDDS
apraxia and agnosia (Schindler, Rachmandi, group and mild AD group had similar levels of
Matthews, & Podell, 1995). As such, these data cognitive impairment as measured by Dementia
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 415

Rating Scale Total Score (Mattis, 1988). While and mobility, financial capacity is a core aspect of
both the PDDS and AD groups performed below individual autonomy in our society.
controls on the cognitively complex understand-
ing, reasoning, and appreciation standards, we Definitions of Financial Capacity
found important dissociations between the two From a legal standpoint, financial capacity repre-
dementia groups on the understanding (S5) and sents the financial skills sufficient for handling
evidencing choice (S1) standards. The PDDS one’s estate and financial affairs, and is the basis
group performed better than the AD group on the for determinations of conservatorship of the estate
understanding standard, a finding consistent with (or guardianship of the estate, depending on the
the more intact episodic memory found in PD state legal jurisdiction) (American Bar
versus AD. In contrast, PDDS patients performed Association/American Psychological Association
below AD patients on the basic evidencing choice Assessment of Capacity in Older Adults Project
standard, a finding that may reflect the inatten- Working Group, 2008). Broadly construed, finan-
tiveness, distractability, and bradyphrenia charac- cial capacity also conceptually encompasses more
teristic of PD (Griffith et al., 2005). We believe specific legal capacities such as contractual capac-
that this small study shows the future promise of ity, donative capacity, and testamentary capacity.
cross-dementia and cross-disorder studies of Thus, financial capacity is an important area of
competency. assessment in the civil legal system (Marson &
Hebert, 2008a).
C A PA C I T Y T O M A N A G E Historically, the legal standard for financial
F I N A N C I A L A F FA I R S capacity in conservatorship statutes was generally
(and vaguely) cast as the capacity to manage “in a
Background reasonable manner all of one’s financial affairs”
The capacity to manage financial affairs (i.e., (American Bar Association/American Psycho-
financial capacity) comprises a broad range of logical Association Assessment of Capacity in
conceptual, pragmatic, and judgment abilities Older Adults Project Working Group, 2008). A
(utilized across a range of everyday settings) that more modern and specific standard is set forth in
are critical to the independent functioning of Section 410(2) of the Uniform Guardianship and
adults in our society (Marson, 2001a; Marson Protective Proceedings Act (UGPPA), which
et al., 2000). As such, it differs in many respects states that a court may appoint a conservator if
from medical decision-making capacity, which is the court determines that “the individual is unable
almost exclusively a verbally mediated capacity to manage property and business affairs because
arising in a medical setting. of an impairment in the ability to receive and
Epidemiological studies in the elderly have evaluate information or make decisions, even
suggested that financial capacity is an “advanced” with the use of appropriate technological assis-
or instrumental activity of daily living (IADL) tance”; and the individual has property that will
(Marson et al., 2000; Wolinsky & Johnson, 1991). be wasted or dissipated unless management is
The advanced ADLs are mediated by higher cog- provided, or funds are needed for the support of
nitive functions and can be distinguished from the individual or of others entitled to the individ-
“household” ADLs (e.g., meal preparation, shop- ual’s support (American Bar Association/
ping, housekeeping) and “basic” ADLs (e.g., bath- American Psychological Association Assessment
ing, dressing, walking) (Wolinsky & Johnson, of Capacity in Older Adults Project Working
1991). Financial capacity entails a complex set of Group, 2008).
abilities, ranging from basic skills of identifying There is not yet a widely accepted clinical defi-
and counting coins/currency, to conducting cash nition for financial capacity. However, Marson
transactions, to managing a checkbook and bank has recently proposed the following: “the capacity
statement, to higher level abilities of making to manage money and financial assets in ways that
investment decisions. As might be expected, such meet a person’s needs and which are consistent with
abilities can vary enormously across individuals, his/her values and self-interest.” (see Widera,
depending on a person’s socioeconomic status, Steenpaas, Marson, and Sudore, 2011) (p. 698).
occupational attainment, and overall financial This working definition seeks to incorporate
experience (Marson, 2001a; Marson et al., 2000). the performance and judgment aspects character-
Along with medical decision-making, driving, istic of financial capacity (described in more
416 forensic neuropsychology

detail below), as well as consideration of a person’s loss of control over one’s own funds and
longstanding values. checkbook implicates a core aspect of personal
independence in our society and can lead to
Performance and Judgment Aspects depression and other significant psychological
of Financial Capacity consequences (Moye, 1996). Third, loss of FC has
Financial capacity can be understood to have clinical significance to health care professionals.
both a performance aspect and a judgment aspect. Impairments in higher order financial skills
In order to have financial capacity, a person must and judgment are often early functional changes
be able to perform a variety of financial tasks and demonstrated by dementia patients (Marson et al.,
skills in order to meet his/her needs. Such tasks 2000) and by some patients with mild cognitive
and skills include understanding basic financial impairment (Griffith et al., 2003; Triebel, Martin,
concepts, possessing basic monetary skills, carry- Griffith et al., 2009). As discussed below, research
ing out cash transactions in a grocery store, and has shown that patients suffering from mild
paying bills. However, in order to have financial Alzheimer’s disease demonstrate significant
capacity, an individual must also be able to impairments in most financial activities and in
exercise judgment and act in ways that protect and many specific financial abilities (Marson et al.,
enhance financial self-interest. Thus, in addition 2000). Fourth, declining financial capacity is
to performance skills, the individual must be closely linked to legal issues of elder abuse
able to carry out financial activities in ways that (Marson et al., 2000). Financial exploitation is an
promote and protect his/her self-interest. all-too-common form of elder abuse commonly
These performance and judgment dimensions associated with victims’ diminished or impaired
of financial capacity are distinct. By way of mental capacities (Nerenberg, 1996). There are
example, a patient with schizophrenia may have a daily media accounts of older adults victimized in
number of intact financial performance skills consumer fraud and other scams (Walton, 2002;
(count coins/currency, carry out cash transac- “Woman out $5,300 in two cons,” 1996). Older
tions, use a money order or checkbook), but due adults can also be more covert victims of undue
to concurrent mental illness and substance abuse influence exercised by family members, profes-
problems, is unable to exercise good judgment sionals, and third parties (Marson, Huthwaite, &
and utilize the skills in ways that meet basic Hebert, 2004; Spar & Garb, 1992).
needs and protect his/her self-interests. Such a Finally, loss of FC can trigger important legal
patient may impulsively spend his/her disability issues of guardianship and conservatorship
entitlement check within a few days of receipt, (Grisso, 1986; Marson et al., 2000). Dispropor-
resulting in disruption of his/her medication tionately high numbers of older adults are subjects
regimen and nutrition, and threatening continu- each year of conservatorship proceedings (pro-
ity of housing such that he/she may become bate court proceedings concerning control and
homeless. management of an individual’s assets and estate),
due to the high incidence of dementias and other
Consequences of Loss of mental and medical illnesses affecting financial
Financial Capacity competency in this age group (Grisso, 1986).
Loss of financial capacity (FC) has a number of In addition, an individual’s financial skills are
important consequences for dementia patients also considered as part of a guardianship proceed-
and families, and implications for health care and ing (American Bar Association/American Psycho-
legal professionals (Marson, 2001a; Marson et al., logical Association Assessment of Capacity in
2000). First, loss of FC can have economic and Older Adults Project Working Group, 2006).
household consequences. People suffering from These two legal proceedings involve significant
dementia such as Alzheimer’s disease often have time and expense for families (Marson, 2001a).
difficulty paying their bills and carrying out basic
financial tasks (Overman & Stoudemire, 1988). Clinical Conceptual Model of
They are continually at risk for making decisions Financial Capacity
that endanger assets needed for their own long- Despite its clear relationship to everyday living
term care or intended for testamentary distribu- and independence, there has been a surprising
tion to family members. Second, there are also lack of conceptual and empirical study of finan-
important psychological consequences to finan- cial capacity. Early work in the area of IADLs
cial capacity loss. Much like loss of the car keys, offered elementary and unsatisfactory schema
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 417

such as ‘financial management skills,’ without (Griffith et al., 2003): (1) specific financial abilities
providing needed conceptual structure or detail. or tasks, each of which is relevant to a particular
We present below a clinically based conceptual domain of financial activity; (2) general domains
model of financial capacity in older adults of financial activity, which each have clinical rele-
(Marson & Zebley, 2001; Marson et al., 2000; vance to the independent functioning of commu-
Martin et al., 2008a). This model has been the nity dwelling older adults; and (3) overall financial
basis for instrument development and for ongo- capacity, which reflects a global measure of capac-
ing studies of financial capacity in MCI, AD, and ity based on the summation of domain- and task-
other clinical populations. level performance. Our conceptual model of FC
Because financial capacity represents a broad currently comprises 9 domains, 18 tasks, and 2
continuum of activities and specific skills, it may global levels (Griffith et al., 2003; Martin et al.,
be best conceptualized as a series of domains of 2008a). It is presented below in Table 15.8.
activity that each have specific clinical relevance
(Griffith et al., 2003; Marson, 2001a; Marson et al., Psychometric Studies of Financial
2000). Examples of these domains include: basic Capacity in Alzheimer’s Disease
monetary skills, carrying out cash transactions, Financial Capacity in Mild and
managing a checkbook, managing a bank state- Moderate AD
ment, and exercising financial judgment. This Using this conceptual model, our laboratory
domain-based approach is clinically oriented and has conducted studies seeking to investigate FC
is consistent with the presumed multidimension- in cognitively impaired populations such as
ality of FC and its variability across individuals. Alzheimer’s disease (Marson, 2001a; Marson et al.,
It is also consistent with the previously discussed 2000). The Financial Capacity Instrument (FCI) is
legal doctrine of limited financial competency a psychometric instrument designed by our group
adopted within most state legal jurisdictions, to assess performance of older adults at the task,
which recognizes that an individual may be domain, and global levels of the conceptual model.
competent to carry out some financial activities The original FCI (FCI-6) assessed six domains
and not others (Grisso, 1986; Marson, 2001a; and 14 tasks (Marson et al., 2000) (the global level
Marson et al., 2000). was introduced in later versions of the FCI—see
In addition to domains of activity, our model below). In an initial study, a sample of 23 older
identifies specific financial abilities, or tasks controls and 53 AD patients (30 with mild demen-
(Marson, 2001a; Marson et al., 2000). Tasks reflect tia, and 23 with moderate dementia), were admin-
more basic financial skills that comprise domain- istered the FCI-6 (Marson et al., 2000). As shown
level capacities. For example, the domain of in Table 15.9, we found that mild AD patients per-
‘financial conceptual knowledge’ might draw formed equivalently with control participants on
upon specific abilities, such as understanding Domain 1 (basic monetary skills), but significantly
simple concepts (e.g., a loan or savings) and prag- below controls on the other five domains.
matically applying such concepts in everyday life Moderate AD patients performed significantly
(e.g., selecting interest rates, identifying a medical below controls and mild AD patients on all
deductible, and making simple tax computations). domains. On the FCI tasks, mild AD patients per-
The domain of financial judgment might consist formed equivalently with controls on simple tasks
of tasks related to detection/awareness of financial such as naming coins/currency, counting coins/
fraud, or of making informed investment choices. currency, understanding parts of a checkbook,
Therefore, tasks represent basic abilities that when and detecting risk of mail fraud. Mild AD patients
combined constitute broader, clinically relevant performed significantly below controls on more
domains of financial activity. We have defined complex tasks such as defining and applying
tasks as being simple or complex, depending on financial concepts, obtaining change for vending
the level of cognitive resources they appear to machine use, using a checkbook, understanding
require (Marson, 2001a; Marson et al., 2000). and using a bank statement, and making an invest-
Our model also considers FC at the global ment decision. Moderate AD patients performed
level (Griffith et al., 2003; Marson & Zebley, 2001). significantly below controls and mild AD patients
Competency is ultimately an overall categorical on all tasks (Marson, 2001a; Marson et al., 2000).
judgment or classification made by a clinician or Using a cut-score method derived from the
legal professional. Thus, the conceptual model of performance of the control group (Marson,
financial capacity currently has three levels 2001a; Marson et al., 2000), we translated the
418 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 15.8 REVISED CONCEPTUAL MODEL OF FINANCIAL CAPACIT Y * :


18 TASKS, 9 DOMAINS, 2 GLOBAL SCORES 9
Task Description Task
Difficulty
Domain 1 Basic Monetary Skills
Task 1a Naming coins/currency Identify specific coins and currency Simple
Task 1b Coin/currency relationships Indicate relative monetary values of coins/currency Simple
Task 1c Counting coins/currency Accurately count groups of coins and currency Simple
Domain 2 Financial Conceptual Knowledge
Task 2a Define financial concepts Define a variety of simple financial concepts Complex
Task 2b Apply financial concepts Practical application/computation using concepts Complex
Domain 3 Cash Transactions
Task 3a 1 item grocery purchase Enter into simulated 1 item transaction; verify change Simple
Task 3b 3 item grocery purchase Enter into simulated 3 item transaction; verify change Complex
Task 3c Change/vending machine Obtain change for vending machine use; verify change Complex
Task 3d Tipping Understand tipping convention; calculate/identify tips Complex
Domain 4 Checkbook Management
Task 4a Understand checkbook Identify and explain parts of check and check register Simple
Task 4b Use checkbook/register Enter into simulated transaction; pay by check Complex
Domain 5 Bank Statement Management
Task 5a Understand bank statement Identify and explain parts of a bank statement Complex
Task 5b Use bank statement Identify specific transactions on bank statement Complex
Domain 6 Financial Judgment
Task 6a Detect mail fraud risk Detect and explain risks in mail fraud solicitation Simple
Task 6c Detect telephone fraud risk Detect and explain risks in telephone fraud solicitation Simple
Domain 7 Bill Payment
Task 7a Understand bills Explain meaning and purpose of bills Simple
Task 7b Prioritize bills Identify overdue utility bill Simple
Task 7c Prepare bills for mailing Prepare simulated bills, checks, envelopes for mailing Complex
Domain 8 Knowledge of Assets/Estate Indicate/verify asset ownership, estate arrangements Simple
Domain 9 Investment Decision-Making Understand options; determine returns; make decision Complex
Global 1 Sum of Domains 1–7 Overall functioning across tasks and domains Complex
Global 2 Sum of Domains 1–8 Overall functioning across tasks and domains Complex

quantitative performance of the AD patients into found capable on Domains 4 and 5 (checkbook
categorical impairment outcomes (capable, management, bank statement management), and
marginally capable, incapable) on each domain. less than 15% were found capable on Domain 6
Table 15.10 presents capacity outcomes for the (financial judgment). These findings suggest that
mild and moderate AD subgroups on the FCI-6 the FCI domains may form a general hierarchy of
domains. In the context of a prototype instrument difficulty for patients with mild AD. Moderate AD
and small control sample, these outcomes should patients, in turn, demonstrated very high rates of
be interpreted cautiously. However, mild AD incapable outcomes on all FCI domains (range
patients demonstrated an interesting pattern of 90–100%). The relationship of the AD patients’
capacity loss across the domains. While approxi- dementia level to their capacity outcomes was sta-
mately 50% of mild AD patients were found capa- tistically robust for all domains (Table 15.10)
ble on Domains 1, 2, and 3, less than 30% were (Marson et al., 2000).
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 419

TABLE 15.9 FCI6 DOMAIN AND TASK PERFORMANCE BY GROUP 10

Score Controls Mild AD Moderate AD


Range [n = 23] [n = 30] [n = 20]
Domain 1 Basic Monetary Skills 0–79 77.9a (1.9) 75.5c (3.5) 57.9 (16.3)
Task 1a Naming Coins/Currency 0–30 30.0a (0.0) 30.0c (0.0) 26.7 (4.7)
Task 1b Coin/Currency Relationships 0–37 36.0a (1.8) 34.0c (3.0) 22.7 (9.2)
Task 1c Counting Coins/Currency 0–12 11.9a (0.3) 11.5c (0.8) 8.6 (3.8)
Domain 2 Financial Concepts 0–41 35.5a,b (2.7) 29.6c (5.4) 19.1 (6.3)
Task 2a Defining Concepts 0–16 13.0a,b (1.9) 9.7c (2.9) 7.1 (2.7)
Task 2b Applying Concepts 0–25 22.5a,b (1.4) 19.9c (3.6) 12.0 (4.6)
Domain 3 Cash Transactions 0–48 46.2a,b (2.7) 38.6c (8.5) 22.2 (10.1)
Task 3a 1 Item Purchase 0–16 15.3a (2.5) 14.4c (3.2) 8.6 (4.9)
Task 3b 3 Item Purchase 0–16 15.2a,b (1.3) 10.7c (5.0) 4.6 (3.3)
Task 3c Change/Vending Machine 0–16 15.7a,b (0.6) 13.6c (2.8) 9.0 (4.1)
Domain 4 Checkbook/Register 0–62 60.2a,b (2.1) 50.7c (8.0) 33.3 (16.1)
Task 4a Understanding Checkbook 0–32 30.7a (1.5) 27.9c (3.1) 20.6 (7.6)
Task 4b Using Checkbook 0–30 29.5a,b (1.5) 22.8c (6.1) 12.2 (9.1)
Domain 5 Bank Statement 0–40 37.4a,b (2.2) 28.6c (7.6) 14.9 (7.2)
Task 5a Understanding Bank 0–22 19.7a,b (2.1) 15.0c (4.1) 8.0 (3.6)
Statement
Task 5b Using Bank Statement 0–18 17.7a, b (0.9) 13.6c (4.3) 6.9 (4.1)
a,b c
Domain 6 Financial Judgment 0–37 30.0 (3.0) 20.8 (5.4) 10.7 (5.1)
Task 6a Detecting Fraud Risk 0–10 8.6a (2.0) 7.8c (2.2) 6.9 (2.8)
Task 6b Investment Decision 0–27 21.4a,b (2.1) 13.0c (4.4) 5.3 (3.5)
a
Normal control mean differs from moderate AD mean using LSD post hoc test (p < .01)
b
Normal control mean differs from mild AD mean (p <.01)
c
Mild AD mean differs from moderate AD mean (p < .01)

This study represented the first empirical effort preserved autonomous financial activity should
to investigate loss of financial capacity in patients be carefully evaluated and monitored.
with AD (Marson, 2001a). The findings suggest (2) Moderate AD patients are at great risk
that significant impairment of financial capacity for loss of all financial activities. Although each
occurs in AD, even in the early stage of the disease. AD patient must be considered individually,
Mild AD patients appear to experience deficits in it is likely that most moderate AD patients
complex financial abilities (tasks), and some level will be unable to manage their financial affairs
of impairment in almost all financial activities (p. 883).
(domains). Moderate AD patients appear to expe-
rience loss of both simple and complex financial Declining Financial Capacity
abilities, and severe impairment across all finan- in Patients with Mild AD
cial activities. Based on these initial findings, we In another study in our laboratory, we showed
proposed two preliminary clinical guidelines for that financial capacity declines in mild AD over
assessment of financial capacity in patients with the course of just one year (Martin et al., 2008a).
mild and moderate AD (Marson et al., 2000): In a sample of 55 individuals with mild AD and 63
healthy older adults, we examined financial capac-
(1) Mild AD patients are at significant risk for ity on two occasions with the FCI-9. Results from
impairment in most financial activities, in this study replicated our prior cross-sectional
particular complex activities like checkbook findings and indicated that patients with mild AD
and bank statement management. Areas of exhibit global financial capacity deficits relative to
420 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 15.10 CAPACIT Y OUTCOMES ON FCI6 DOMAINS ACROSS AD


PATIENT SUBGROUPS 11
Capable Marginally Incapable p*
Capable
Domain 1 Basic Monetary Skills .0002
Mild AD Patients 53% (16/30) 17% (5/30) 30% (9/30)
Moderate AD Patients 10% (2/20) 0% (0/20) 90% (18/20)
Domain 2 Financial Concepts .002
Mild AD Patients 47% (14/30) 13% (4/30) 40% (12/30)
Moderate AD Patients 5% (1/20) 5% (1/20) 90% (18/20)
Domain 3 Cash Transactions .0002
Mild AD Patients 47% (14/30) 10% (3/30) 43% (13/30)
Moderate AD Patients 0% (0/20) 0% (0/20) 100% (20/20)
Domain 4 Checkbook/Register .02
Mild AD Patients 27% (8/30) 13% (4/30) 60% (18/30)
Moderate AD Patients 0% (0/20) 5% (1/20) 95% (19/20)
Domain 5 Bank Statement .003
Mild AD Patients 27% (8/30) 16% (5/30) 57% (17/30)
Moderate AD Patients 0% (0/20) 0% (0/20) 100% (20/20)
Domain 6 Financial Judgment .007
Mild AD Patients 13% (4/30) 37% (11/30) 50% (15/30)
Moderate AD Patients 0% (0/18) 6% (1/18) 94% (17/18)

* Significance of difference in capacity outcomes across dementia stage (mild v. moderate AD) using chi square

same-aged cognitively intact peers at baseline. instrumental activities of daily living (Daly
At one-year follow up, participants in the mild et al., 2000; Morris, 2002; Ritchie et al., 2001;
AD group exhibited marked declines in overall Griffith et. al 2003).
financial capacity and on the majority of financial In an initial study using an expanded FCI
capacity domains. In contrast, the healthy older (FCI-9), we examined financial capacity in a
adults exhibited stable financial capacity over the cross-sectional sample of 21 older controls, 21
course of the study. Overall, the AD group exhib- patients with amnestic MCI, and 22 patients with
ited relatively rapid change, with an overall 10% mild AD (Griffith et al., 2003). We found that, at
decline over the course of one year—from approx- the domain level, controls performed significantly
imately 80% of the control group’s performance better than mild AD subjects on all domains with
level at the baseline assessment to 70% of the the exception of Domain 8 (Knowledge of Assets/
control group’s performance level at follow-up. Estate). Controls performed significantly better
than the MCI group on Domains 2 (Financial
Psychometric Study of Financial Concepts), 4 (Checkbook Management), 5 (Bank
Capacity in Mild Cognitive Impairment Statement Management), 6 (Financial Judgment),
Financial Capacity in Patients with MCI and 7 (Bill Payment). There were no domains on
Our group has also examined financial capacity in which the MCI group performed better than con-
patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). trols. In turn, the MCI group performed signifi-
As noted above, MCI represents a transitional cantly better than mild AD patients on all domains
phase between normal cognitive aging and except Financial Judgment and Knowledge of
dementia (Morris et al., 2001; Petersen et al., Assets/Estate.
2001), with less cognitive and in particular func- At the task level, controls performed signifi-
tional impairment than is characteristic of demen- cantly better than the mild AD group on most
tia but nevertheless with emerging declines in abilities, with the exception of simple tasks of
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 421

basic monetary skills, cash transactions, and tele- models of FC across the continuum from normal
phone fraud. Controls performed significantly aging to dementia. As reflected below in
better than the MCI group on tasks of applying Table 15.12, robust cognitive models of FC
financial concepts, understanding and using a emerged for each group. Written arithmetic skill
bank statement, understanding bills, and prepar- (WRAT-3 Arithmetic) was the primary predictor
ing bills for mailing. The MCI group, in turn, of FC across all three models, accounting for
demonstrated significantly higher scores than the 27% (control model), 46% (mild AD model),
AD group on tasks of understanding and applying and 55% (aMCI model) of variance. Visuomotor
financial concepts, using a vending machine, tracking/executive function was a secondary
understanding and using a checkbook, under- cognitive predictor of FC across the two patient
standing and using a bank statement, prioritizing models. Short-term verbal memory and visuomo-
bills, and preparing bills for mailing. There were tor tracking/executive function were secondary
no tasks on which the MCI group performed predictors for the normal aging group. The find-
better than controls. ings strongly implicate written arithmetic skills
For overall financial capacity (Domains 1–7), as a critical cognitive function supporting FC
control participants performed significantly in both cognitive aging and neurodegenerative
better than MCI and AD participants, and MCI disease.
participants performed significantly better than
AD participants. On an experimental measure of Summary of Financial
overall financial capacity that included knowledge Capacity Research
of assets and estate arrangements (Domains 1–8), In summary, this line of research supports our
smaller samples of control and MCI subjects conceptual model of FC and suggests the utility of
performed significantly better than AD patients psychometric instruments for direct assessment
but did not differ significantly from each other. of financial capacity in patients with neurodegen-
This study represents one of the first published erative disease. The FCI represents a potential
reports of psychometric evidence for higher order advance in functional assessment in dementia
functional decline and capacity loss in MCI (Moye, 2003). It is specific to the construct of
(Griffith et al., 2003). Using a direct assessment financial capacity, and is based on a model con-
approach, we found that patients with amnestic ceptualizing financial capacity as a series of
MCI demonstrated significant, albeit mild, defi- discrete spheres of activity (domains) linked to
cits on some (but not all) financial abilities com- independent community functioning. The FCI
pared to age, education, gender, and racially operationalizes these domains with tests of
matched healthy controls. MCI patients showed a specific financial abilities (tasks), which are objec-
decline in overall financial capacity (Domains tive and behaviorally anchored. It also provides
1–7) of 1.74 SD units compared to control partici- global estimates of financial capacity useful to cli-
pants (Table 15.11). These results suggest that nicians. In addition, the FCI has demonstrated
initial declines in more complex financial abilities construct validity by discriminating the financial
begin in MCI prior to the development of a frank performance and capacity outcomes of controls,
dementia. mild AD patients, and moderate AD patients
(Marson et al., 2000), and the performance of
Cognitive Predictors of Financial controls, amnestic MCI patients, and mild AD
Capacity in MCI and Mild AD patients (Griffith et al., 2003). Finally, neurocog-
We recently identified cognitive predictors of nitive modeling of financial capacity has yielded
FC in a sample of healthy older controls (n=85), promising results, with findings suggesting that
aMCI patients (n=113), and mild AD patients written arithmetic skills are strongly associated
(n=43) (Sherod et al., 2009). Our goal was to with FC performance in both cognitive aging and
develop and compare multivariate cognitive prodromal and clinical AD (Sherod et al., 2009).
TABLE 15.11 COMPARISONS OF GROUP PERFORMANCE ON THE FCI9 12
Score Controls MCI AD Patients P Post-hoc
Range (n = 21) Patients (n =22) (2-tailed) p <.05
X (SD) (n = 21) X (SD)
X (SD)
Domain 1/Basic Monetary Skills 0–48 45.2 (3.5) 44.3 (3.7) 41.0 (6.3) .011 C M>A
Naming Coins/Currency 0–8 7.9 (0.3) 7.8 (0.4) 7.6 (0.7) .050 C>A
Coins/Currency Relationships 0–28 24.9 (3.4) 24.8 (3.5) 22.0 (5.5) .044 —
Counting Coins/Money 0–12 12.0 (0.2) 11.7 (0.5) 11.5 (1.2) .119 —
Domain 2/Financial Concepts 0–40 36.9 (3.2) 33.1 (4.7) 27.6 (7.5) .001 C> M > A
Understanding Concepts 0–15 13.8 (1.2) 12.9 (1.8) 11.2 (2.6) .001 C M> A
Applying Concepts 0–25 23.1 (2.4) 20.2 (3.6) 16.4 (5.3) .001 C >M > A
Domain 3/Cash Transactions 0–30 27.0 (3.4) 24.8 (3.6) 20.6 (5.9) .001 C M>A
One Item Transaction 0–6 6.0 (0.0) 5.8 (0.6) 5.5 (1.1) .044 C>A
Multi-Item Transaction 0–7 6.1 (1.9) 5.7 (1.8) 4.5 (2.4) .037 C>A
Vending Machine 0–9 8.6 (0.9) 8.0 (1.4) 5.6 (1.9) .001 C M> A
Tipping 0–8 6.3 (1.7) 5.4 (1.5) 5.1 (2.1) .068 —
Domain 4/Checkbook 0–54 53.2 (1.5) 50.6 (2.9) 42.9 (8.0) .001 C M >A
Management
Understanding Checkbook 0–24 23.5 (0.8) 22.8 (1.5) 21.2 (2.5) .001 C M >A
Using Checkbook 0–30 29.6 (1.4) 27.9 (2.2) 21.7 (6.0) .001 C M >A
Domain 5/Bank Statement 0–38 35.2 (2.7) 29.9 (5.6) 23.3 (8.3) .001 C> M> A
Management
Understanding Bank Statement 0–18 16.2 (1.6) 13.4 (2.7) 10.6 (3.9) .001 C> M> A
Using Bank Statement 0–20 19.1 (1.5) 16.5 (4.0) 12.7 (4.9) .001 C> M> A
Domain 6/Financial Judgment 0–26 25.6 (1.2) 23.2 (3.7) 23.5 (3.5) .029 C> M A
Mail Fraud 0–8 8.0 (0.0) 7.3 (1.3) 7.1 (1.6) .045 C> A
Telephone Fraud 0–18 17.6 (1.2) 15.9 (3.0) 16.4 (2.3) .051 —
Domain 7/Bill Payment 0–46 43.7 (3.3) 38.4 (6.2) 28. 3 (9.6) .001 C> M> A
Understanding Bills 0–6 5.9 (0.4) 5.1 (1.2) 4. 7 (1.6) .006 C> M A
Identifying/Prioritizing Bills 0–13 12.6 (0.6) 12.3 (1.1) 10. 9 (1.7) .001 C M> A
Preparing Bills for Mailing 0–27 25.1 (3.3) 20.4 (6.1) 12. 7 (8.4) .001 C> M> A
Domain 8/Assets & Estate 0–20 18.1 (1.6) 17.4 (2.6) 16. 2 (2.8) .068 —
Arrangementsa
Domain 9/Investment Decision 0–17 13.9 (2.9) 12.4 (2.3) 9. 2 (3.5) .001 C M> A
Makingb
FCI Total Score (Domains 1–7) 0–282 266.8 (13.2) 243.8 (21.7) 207.2 (38.0) .001 C >M >A
FCI Total Score (Domains 1–8)a 0–302 282.1 (14.1) 264.0 (17.8) 223.8 (39.9) .001 C M >A
a
control = 15, MCI = 13, AD = 21
b
control = 21, MCI = 19, AD = 18
C>A = control mean is greater than AD mean C>M>A = control mean is greater than MCI mean and AD mean, and MCI mean is greater
than AD mean
C>MA = control mean is greater than MCI and AD means CM>A = control and MCI means are greater than AD mean

422
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 423

TABLE 15.12 MULTIVARIATE COGNITIVE PREDICTOR MODELS a OF FCI TOTAL


SCORE DOMAINS 17 ACROSS GROUPS 13

Control Predictor Model MCI Predictor Model Mild AD Predictor Model


(n = 85) (n = 113) (n = 43)
Measure r Cum R2 Measure r Cum R2 Measure r Cum R2

Step 1 Arithmetic .54 .27 ‡ Arithmetic .74 .55‡ Arithmetic .67 .46‡
Step 2 Log Mem II .35 .35 † Trails B −.73 .66‡ Trails A .60 −.0.56†
Step 3 Trails A −.29 .38* Race −.55 .69‡ Log Mem I .63 65†
Digits Forward .34 Dig Symbol .59
Trails B −.31 Vis Rep I .54

* p < .05
† p < .01
‡ p < .001
a
Tasks in bold entered into the multivariate predictor models, while non-bolded tasks did not enter into the model.
Arithmetic = Wide Range Achievement Test-Third Edition (Arithmetic subtest)
Log Mem I = Logical Memory I (immediate recall) subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised
Log Mem II = Logical Memory II (delayed recall) subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised
Digits Forward = Digit Span subtest (digits forward portion) from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition
Dig Symbol = Digit Symbol from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition
Spat Back Raw = Spatial Span Backwards subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition
Vis Rep I = Visual Reproduction I (immediate recall) subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition

T E S TA M E N TA RY fact appear to be increasing in number (Nedd,


C A PA C I T Y 1998). This increase in will contest litigation reflects
a number of factors, in particular our aging society
Background and increasing numbers of older adults with neu-
In this section we discuss conceptual and clinical rologic, psychiatric, and medical impairments that
aspects of a third civil competency: capacity to impair mental capacity (Marson et al., 2004). Other
make a will (testamentary capacity). The freedom factors include the breakdown of the nuclear family
to choose how one’s property and other posses- and increase in blended families with conflicting
sions will be disposed of following death—known agendas, and the enormous transfer of wealth
as the right of testation—is a fundamental right currently ongoing between the World War II and
under Anglo-American law (Frolik, 2001; Marson baby boomer generations (Nedd, 1998).
et al., 2004). A key requirement of the law of The legal concept of testamentary capacity
testation is that a testator (person making the will) should be distinguished from that of undue
have testamentary capacity or competency (TC): influence. In order to make a valid will, the law
“that measure of mental ability recognized in law also requires, in addition to testamentary
as sufficient for the making of a will” (Black, 1968). capacity, that the testator be free from undue
If testamentary capacity is lacking at the time of influence by another individual who may profit
execution of the will, the will is invalid and void in from a new will or a legal amendment of an
effect (Perr, 1991). The legal requirement of testa- existing will (codicil) (Spar, Hankin, & Stodden,
mentary capacity exists across all state jurisdic- 1995). Thus, even when a testator has sufficient
tions, and Anglo-American law has strongly testamentary capacity, the will may be voided
supported testation over intestacy (Frolik, 2001; by the court if the court or jury deems that the
Marson et al., 2004). Public policy and legal prec- volition of the testator was supplanted by an indi-
edent have clearly favored allowing individuals to vidual exercising undue influence over him/
choose how their property will be distributed after her. The doctrine of undue influence, which also
death rather than leaving such decisions to state exists in various forms across state jurisdictions, is
laws governing intestacy. However, despite the thus analytically distinct from testamentary
legal system’s tendency to favor the rights of the capacity—it applies in cases in which the testator
testator, cases challenging the validity of wills and possesses some level of testamentary capacity
specifically the testamentary capacity and/or inde- that is subverted through a relationship with the
pendent volition of testators are common and in influencer (American Bar Association/American
424 forensic neuropsychology

Psychological Association Assessment of Capacity this area by Spar and Garb focused on developing
in Older Adults Project Working Group, 2008; practical approaches to clinical assessment as
Frolik, 2001). As discussed further below, these opposed to model building (Spar & Garb, 1992).
two legal issues very frequently co-occur and One useful approach to conceptual model building
intertwine in will contests. involves direct analysis of the component legal ele-
ments of testamentary capacity. Using the model for
Legal Elements of Testamentary Capacity analyzing legal capacities developed by Grisso
The current legal requirements for testamentary (Grisso, 2003), this approach begins by identifying
capacity in the United States vary to some degree the specific legal standard for testamentary capacity
from state to state. In many states (although (the legal elements of testamentary capacity) and
not all) four specific criteria or elements are next involves identification of the ‘functional’
recognized (Marson et al., 2004; Spar & Garb, requirements of these legal elements. What cogni-
1992). A testator must: tive, emotional, and pragmatic abilities (functional
requirements) are required to understand the nature
(1) understand the nature of the testa- of a will, know the nature and extent of one’s prop-
mentary act (i.e., know what a will is); erty, know the natural objects of one’s bounty
(2) understand and recollect the nature and (potential heirs), and know general plan of property
situation of his or her property; disposition? Systematic conceptualization of the
functional requirements will allow clinical evalua-
(3) have knowledge of the persons who are the
tors to better determine in individual cases whether
natural objects of his or her bounty; and
or not the testator has testamentary capacity, which
(4) know the manner in which the in turn will assist judges who must make the ulti-
disposition of the property is to occur. mate legal determination of testamentary capacity.
This approach to the issue naturally leads to
The way in which these elements are weighed questions regarding (1) the neuropsychological
by courts in determining the validity of a will abilities that underlie each of the component legal
varies across states (Frolik, 2001; Spar et al., 1995). elements, and whether they can be conceptual-
Some states require that the testator meet only ized and measured, and (2) more generally, how
one or two of the criteria for a will to be valid. to operationalize the legal elements of testamen-
Other states may require additional criteria; tary capacity for purposes of assessment instru-
for example, that the testator understand a will, ment development.
demonstrate memory of all property and poten-
tial heirs, and ‘hold this information in mind’ Preliminary Neuropsychological
while developing a plan for disposition of assets Model of Testamentary Capacity
(Spar & Garb, 1992; Walsh et al., 1997). Some initial ideas for development of a neuropsy-
In addition to the elements mentioned above, chological model of testamentary capacity can be
many states also require that the testator at the found in the legal and psychological literature.
time the will is executed not exhibit delusions Walsh and colleagues, in conjunction with the
and/or hallucinations that may result in a will that American Bar Association, identified several fac-
excludes or favors potential heirs based on false tors required for the determination of testamen-
beliefs and/or is uncharacteristic of the testator’s tary capacity as defined in medical terms (Walsh
preferences in the absence of delusions and et al., 1997). These are functional autonomy, work-
hallucinations (Spar & Garb, 1992; Walsh et al., ing memory, orientation, attention, and calcula-
1997). However, a will may be ruled valid if tion (Walsh et al., 1997). Likewise, research on
delusions and hallucinations are discrete, unasso- medical decision-making and financial capacity
ciated with the testator’s property and potential has found correlations between these civil compe-
heirs, and/or have seemingly little or no impact tencies and performance on neuropsychological
on testator’s plan for the disposition of assets measures of conceptualization, calculation, seman-
(Marson et al., 2004; Walsh et al., 1997). tic memory, verbal recall, and executive function,
particularly word fluency (Marson, Sawrie, Stalvey,
Conceptual Models of McInturff, & Harrell, 1998; Marson et al., 1996;
Testamentary Capacity Marson et al., 1995a). A very recent study has
There continues to be a need for useful conceptual identified written arithmetic ability as the primary
models of testamentary capacity that can inform predictor of financial capacity in cognitively
clinical and forensic assessment. Initial studies in normal controls, patients with amnestic MCI, and
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 425

patients with mild AD (Sherod et al., 2009). It is comprehension abilities, and sufficient
likely that arithmetic ability may also play a role in language abilities to express the testator’s
the element of testamentary capacity relating to understanding (borne out by the recent
understanding the nature and extent of property, language domain findings referenced above
and possibly also to disposition of assets. in the recent paper by Roked and Patel;
What is currently needed, however, are empir- Roked & Patel, 2008). Recognition items
ical studies that specifically correlate performance may assist a testator with expressive
on neuropsychological test measures with mea- language problems. A reply of ‘yes’ or ‘no’ to
sures of testamentary capacity. The literature is an attorney’s queries regarding the nature of
silent here with the exception of one recent brief a will is unlikely to be satisfactory in this
report (Roked & Patel, 2008). This study from regard, as such responses do not clearly
England found that two screening measures (the support the testator’s independent
MMSE and CAMDEX-R) were each able to pre- understanding of the element. Similarly, a
dict 87% of independent rater judgments of testa- testator’s signature on a legal document by
mentary capacity in a sample of 74 patients with itself does not demonstrate understanding,
mild to moderate AD. The language domain of as a signature is an automatic procedural
the CAMDEX-R was the strongest cognitive behavior not dependent upon higher level
domain predictor, while short-term memory and cognition (Greiffenstein, 1996).
concentration domains were not predictors.
2. Cognitive Functions Related to Knowing
Testamentary capacity was also associated with
the Nature and Extent of Property: The
dementia severity, with 62.5% of mild AD patients
second legal element of testamentary
rated as capable, versus 35% of moderate AD
capacity requires that the testator
patients and 2.5% of severe AD patients. Insofar as
remember the nature and extent of his or
its major conclusion is that global mental status is
her property to be disposed. As reported
associated with testamentary capacity in AD,
earlier, some states differ in their
knowledge provided by this study is limited. In
interpretation of this (§2.04 Variation in
addition, the study did not provide needed infor-
Requirements, pp. 2–13) (Walsh et al.,
mation regarding the relationship of cognitive per-
1997). Possible cognitive functions
formance to the respective legal elements of
involved here would include semantic
testamentary capacity. Nonetheless, the study rep-
memory concerning assets and ownership,
resents a first step in what hopefully will be a grow-
historical memory and short-term memory
ing focus of forensic neuropsychological research.
enabling recall of both long-term and more
Although empirical data are presently lacking,
recently acquired assets and property, and
a theoretical neuropsychological model of testa-
knowledge and comprehension of the value
mentary capacity continues to have heuristic value.
tied to different assets and property. If the
Our capacity research group has previously
testator has recently purchased new
described the following hypothesized cognitive
possessions prior to his or her execution of
constructs for the four legal elements of testamen-
a will, then impairment in short-term
tary capacity described above (Marson et al., 2004).
memory (the hallmark sign of early AD)
A discussion of the material below can also be
can significantly impact his or her recall of
found in the ABA/APA capacity handbook for psy-
these items. Testators also must be able to
chologists (American Bar Association/American
form working estimates of value for key
Psychological Association Assessment of Capacity
pieces of property that reasonably
in Older Adults Project Working Group, 2008).
approximate their true value; it is likely that
executive function abilities play a role here.
1. Cognitive Functions Related to
(Marson et al., 2004; American Bar
Understanding the Nature of a Will: This
Association/American Psychological
element requires a testator to understand
Association Assessment of Capacity in
the nature, purposes and consequences of a
Older Adults Project Working Group, 2008)
will, and to express these verbally or in
some other adequate form to an attorney or 3. Cognitive Functions Related to Knowing the
judge. Possible cognitive functions involved Objects of One’s Bounty: This legal element
may include semantic memory regarding requires that the testator be cognizant of
terms such as death, property, and those individuals who represent his natural
inheritance, verbal abstraction and heirs, or other heirs who can place a
426 forensic neuropsychology

reasonable claim on the estate. Historical civil competency practice and research, one can
and also short term episodic personal anticipate the emergence of conceptually based
memory of these individuals, and of the assessment instruments.
nature of their relationships with the
testator, would appear to be prominent Empirical Studies of
cognitive abilities associated with this Testamentary Capacity
element. As dementias like AD progress, There is currently little published empirical
testators may be increasingly unable to research on testamentary capacity (Marson et al.,
recall family members and acquaintances, 2004). In part this reflects the still early develop-
leading ultimately to failure to recognize mental stage of the field of capacity assessment
these individuals in photographs or even generally. With the exception of treatment con-
when presenting in person (Marson et al., sent capacity, for which there is now a reasonable
2004; American Bar Association/ body of research (Appelbaum & Grisso, 1995;
American Psychological Association Dunn et al., 2006; Grisso & Appelbaum, 1995;
Assessment of Capacity in Older Adults Grisso et al., 1995; Karlawish, 2008; Kim et al.,
Project Working Group, 2008). 2001; Marson et al., 1995c; Palmer & Savla, 2007),
4. Cognitive Functions Related to a Plan for relatively little conceptual and empirical research
Distribution of Assets: This final legal element has been conducted thus far regarding other
of testamentary capacity requires that the important civil competencies such as financial
testator be able to express a basic plan for capacity (Marson, 2001a; Marson et al., 2000;
distributing his or her assets to the intended Martin et al., 2008a; Sherod et al. 2009). However,
heirs. Insofar as this element integrates the this point notwithstanding, the area of testamen-
first three elements in a supraordinate tary capacity seems to have been particularly
fashion, the proposed cognitive basis for this neglected. Although there is a growing literature
element arguably represents an integration providing general clinical guidelines for assess-
of the cognitive abilities underlying the other ment of testamentary capacity (American Bar
three elements. Accordingly, higher order Association/American Psychological Association
executive function abilities are implied as the Assessment of Capacity in Older Adults Project
testator must demonstrate a projective Working Group, 2008; Marson et al., 2004; Spar &
understanding of how future dispositions of Garb, 1992) there is currently no body of empiri-
specific property to specific heirs will occur, cal research that can inform and advance the field.
and the rationale for these dispositions Given the prevalence and societal importance of
(Marson et al., 2004; American Bar inheritance by will, this remains a key knowledge
Association/American Psychological gap in neuropsychological forensic science as it
Association Assessment of Capacity in Older relates to civil competencies and the elderly.
Adults Project Working Group, 2008).
Prototype Psychometric Instrument for
The preliminary theoretical neuropsychologi- Assessing Testamentary Capacity
cal model of testamentary capacity proposed As discussed above, no standardized psychologi-
above represents a step towards conceptual model cal measures of testamentary capacity currently
building in this area. The model currently focuses exist in everyday practice. However, a prototype
on proposed cognitive demands of the legal psychometric instrument, called the Testamentary
elements, and does not address the emotional/ Capacity Instrument (TCI), has recently been
psychiatric aspects of the capacity. Such a model developed and field tested by the lead author and
would require empirical verification in an older a co-author (K.H.) (Hebert, Scogin, & Marson, in
adult sample through use of a relevant testamen- preparation). The TCI is a structured, psychomet-
tary capacity instrument and neuropsychological ric measure for assessing and differentiating the
test measures. As discussed below, there is cur- testamentary capacity of cognitively-intact versus
rently a lack of psychological assessment instru- cognitively-impaired older adults.
ments and associated empirical research specific The TCI measures capacity using the four legal
to this domain of forensic practice (but in the legal elements of testamentary capacity discussed above.
sphere, refer to The Legal Capacity Questionnaire Performance on each element is based on the indi-
[§ 1.10, pp. 1–17 to 1–22]; Walsh et al., 1997). vidual’s ability to recall or recollect information
As testamentary capacity matures as an area of pertinent to the execution of a will. The degree to
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 427

which memory for relevant information is required Initial Empirical Study of Testamentary
by law varies (Walsh et al., 1997). For this reason, Capacity in Dementia
the four elements are measured using free recall Using the TCI, a member of our group (K.H.,
and recognition items (multiple-choice and also co-author) has conducted an initial empirical
forced-choice). An individual who may not be study of testamentary capacity in a small sample
able to freely recall information pertinent to a legal of cognitively healthy older controls and patients
element may still be able to accurately identify this with mild to moderate AD (Hebert et al., in prep-
information in a recognition or forced-choice aration). This paper is currently in preparation for
(Yes-No/True-False) format. All items are admin- publication and thus formal specific findings will
istered orally or in writing and are quantitatively not be reported here. However, it appears that
scored. Within a legal element, item scores are impairment in testamentary capacity is already
summed, giving rise to an overall performance evident in the early stages of AD, with all four ele-
score. These performance scores, in turn, can sup- ments of testamentary capacity showing compro-
port judgment outcomes (capable, marginally mise. In addition, distinct forms of cognitive
capable, or incapable), for the individual element, impairment are associated with the elements of
and also for overall testamentary capacity. testamentary capacity, and with overall testamen-
Although if necessary the TCI is designed to be tary capacity, in AD (Hebert et al., in preparation).
a stand alone assessment, its administration to an The results appear very promising and support
older adult testator should ideally co-occur with a the feasibility of psychometrically measuring
comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation. The testamentary capacity constructs and of pursuing
standardized and objective cognitive and emotional empirical studies of testamentary capacity in
test data will provide an important overall context cognitively impaired older adults.
for the evaluation, and can help inform findings
regarding the specific legal elements, as well as Clinical Assessment of
guide the clinician’s overall judgment of capacity. Testamentary Capacity
Standardized assessment of testamentary capac- Current clinical practice in cases of testamentary
ity involves certain methodological challenges that capacity can be divided into two major areas:
require attention. Unlike knowledge of a will (1) prospective clinical assessments of testamen-
(Element 1), information concerning a testator’s tary capacity involving living testators and family
assets/property, his/her natural heirs, and his/her members, usually completed contemporaneously
plan of distribution (Elements 2–4) is individual with a will execution (American Bar Association/
specific and not as readily amenable to standardized American Psychological Association Assessment
inquiry across patients/clients. Accordingly, it is of Capacity in Older Adults Project Working
very important to obtain accurate information Group, 2008; Marson et al., 2004; Spar & Garb,
regarding the testator’s property and heirs from reli- 1992); and (2) retrospective analyses of testamen-
able collateral sources, in order to evaluate and verify tary capacity and undue influence in cases involv-
the testator’s own responses to questions tapping ing a now deceased or incompetent testator
these three legal elements (Spar & Garb, 1992). Thus, (American Bar Association/American Psycholo-
the TCI explicitly seeks collateral information for all gical Association Assessment of Capacity in Older
four legal elements. However, collateral sources Adults Project Working Group, 2008; Greiffenstein,
sometimes may have limited or inaccurate informa- 1996; Marson et al., 2004; Spar & Garb, 1992). In
tion regarding the testator’s assets and/or relation- each of these areas, current practice patterns vary
ships with potential heirs (Marson et al., 2004). In quite widely in approach and quality, in large part
addition, collateral sources may have potential con- due to uneven conceptual understanding among
flicts of interest insofar they are often also prospec- many practitioners of capacity assessment gener-
tive heirs of the testator. Such conflicts of interest ally (Marson & Ingram, 1996; Marson, 2001b),
may thus bias responses of collateral sources to and of the legal requirements of testamentary
inquiries regarding the testator’s assets and heirs, capacity and undue influence specifically.
as well as regarding the testator’s general cognitive
function, psychiatric health, and quality of rela- Contemporaneous Assessment of
tionships with other prospective heirs. These Testamentary Capacity
issues obviously require application of clinical In certain circumstances, an attorney, judge, or
judgment by the examiner in selecting collateral family member may request that a mental health
sources and using the TCI and related instruments. professional assess the capacity of a living testator
428 forensic neuropsychology

prior to or contemporaneously with his/her execu- interview with the testator may prove beneficial
tion of a will. Two common scenarios underlie for illustrating both testamentary capacity and the
such a referral. The attorney or judge may have lack of outside influences; however, this should
concerns about the testamentary capacity of the first be discussed and cleared with the testator’s
proposed testator, and therefore will seek clinical attorneys.
expertise and input on the issue before proceeding As part of the evaluation, an assessment of the
further. Alternatively, in cases of ongoing or antic- testator’s lifelong values about money, personal
ipated family conflict, the foresighted attorney property, and finances can be very useful. As dis-
may seek to preempt a future will contest by having cussed above, for reasons of public policy, courts
his client undergo a capacity assessment prior to invoke a low legal threshold for upholding wills
or contemporaneous with will execution (Marson and permitting legal transfer of property after
et al., 2004; American Bar Association/American death. Thus, the personal values and interests of
Psychological Association Assessment of Capacity the testator may be given considerable weight by
in Older Adults Project Working Group, 2008). courts (American Bar Association/American
Spar and colleagues have written cogently on Psychological Association Assessment of Capacity
the topic of contemporaneous clinical assessment in Older Adults Project Working Group, 2008). In
of testamentary capacity and undue influence this regard, important information can be ascer-
(Spar & Garb, 1992; Spar et al., 1995). Their clini- tained by reviewing prior wills of a testator, which
cal interview guidelines for testamentary capacity will presumably reflect the prior application of
published in 1992 continue to represent a key con- his/her values to the assignment of property to
tribution to forensic practice in this area. The key designated heirs. A testator’s radical departure
aspects of the interview are to ‘assess the legal ele- from prior testamentary value patterns in a new
ments of testamentary capacity, identify any fea- will, known legally as an ‘unnatural will,’ may lead
tures of the testator’s personality and mental status a court to consider whether a testator is suffering
that could affect susceptibility to undue influence, from diminished capacity or from coercion
and determine the nature, extent, and general through the effects of undue influence (American
functional consequences of mental illness, if any’ Bar Association/American Psychological
(pp. 171–172) (Spar & Garb, 1992). The authors Association Assessment of Capacity in Older
highlight the importance of conducting the clini- Adults Project Working Group, 2008). Issues of
cal interview in close proximity to the moment the continuity of a testator’s values are also important
testamentary document is executed (Marson & in both contemporaneous and retrospective
Hebert, 2008b). Interviews conducted in close assessments of testamentary capacity (see below).
proximity to the time of testamentary document
execution are more likely to be influential in court Retrospective Assessment of
than those conducted at more distant time peri- Testamentary Capacity
ods. This consideration is important as courts gen- Although contemporaneous evaluations of
erally place great emphasis on the testator’s mental testamentary capacity are highly desirable and
functioning at the time in question and recognize useful, they probably do not represent the major-
that individuals’ mental functioning can vary at ity of forensic evaluations in this area. More fre-
different time points (Spar et al., 1995). quently, neuropsychologists and other mental
A second and perhaps more difficult challenge health professionals are called upon by attorneys,
for the clinical examiner is to obtain as much by the probate court, or by interested family mem-
information as possible about the testator’s bers, to render retrospective opinions regarding
property (Element 2) and names and relationships the existence of testamentary capacity (and some-
of potential heirs (Element 3). As noted above, times undue influence) at a previous point in time.
this can be a difficult task when the testator’s Retrospective evaluations of testamentary capac-
informants are limited to family members who ity usually arise after the death (or sometimes the
may also serve to profit from the examiner’s testi- incompetency) of a testator, when heirs and/or
mony. An objective independent source of infor- other interested parties contest a will on grounds
mation regarding a testator’s potential heirs and that the decedent lacked testamentary capacity at
possessions is strongly recommended but may the earlier time of will execution. Although recog-
not always be practical. A private interview nized and used by the courts, no clear rules for
with only the testator is recommended to limit conducting such evaluations have been estab-
outside influences. A videotaped recording of the lished (Spar & Garb, 1992). However, there is
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 429

increasing academic attention to the issue 3. Where possible and as appropriate,


(American Bar Association/American Psycholo- contact and speak with individuals who
gical Association Assessment of Capacity in Older knew the decedent testator and can offer
Adults Project Working Group, 2008; Marson informed lay and professional judgments
et al., 2004; Marson & Hebert, 2008b; Shulman, about mental status and capacity at the
Cohen, & Hull, 2005). time of will execution.
The process of retrospective evaluation has
4. Obtain information about the attorneys
sometimes been described as a ‘neuropsycholo-
involved in the prior will execution. Who
gical autopsy’ (Greiffenstein, 1996), and neurop-
was the attorney and what history did he
sychological methods and knowledge can be
or she have with the client? What was the
particularly useful for these purposes (Marson,
series of professional interactions leading
2002). Greiffenstein proposed several steps for
up to the will execution?
determining testamentary capacity retrospec-
tively (Greiffenstein, 1996). First, the clinician 5. Assess for the presence and severity of a
should consider whether the legal issue at hand mental disorder at the time of will
pertains to testamentary capacity or undue influ- execution. With older adults, the most
ence, or both. Next, the date of the legal transac- often disputed wills are often those that
tion should be identified, as this date will help were made or modified when an
determine the relevance of contemporaneous individual had a memory disorder or a
mental status, medical, and lay testimony evi- diagnosed dementia. Is there evidence,
dence. This is typically the date in which the will through medical or other records, of a
was signed. The clinician must also identify the mental disorder that might affect cognitive
type of neurologic or psychiatric disorder that the and emotional abilities related to the
testator had and determine which, if any, cogni- elements of testamentary capacity? In
tive abilities were impacted. This is done by some cases, there may be specific cognitive
gathering evidence of normal and abnormal cog- or neuropsychological test information
nitive and emotional behavior occurring as close that will shed light on mental abilities
as possible to the date of will execution. relevant to testamentary capacity.
A more comprehensive and detailed approach
6. In cases of dementia, if possible seek to
to retrospective assessment of testamentary
determine the stage of dementia at the
capacity is presented in the recent ABA–APA
time of will execution, as it can
capacity assessment handbook for psychologists
significantly inform the clinical judgment
(American Bar Association/American Psycholo-
of testamentary capacity. The Clinical
gical Association Assessment of Capacity in Older
Dementia Rating (CDR) and the Global
Adults Project Working Group, 2008). As part of
Deterioration Scale (GDS) (Morris, 1993;
the handbook, a set of recommended steps are
Reisberg, Ferris, deLeon, & Crook, 1982)
presented for the evaluator. A slightly modified
represent dementia staging tools for cases
and expanded version of these steps is set forth
of Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia stage
below (reprinted by permission of the ABA-APA)
when properly determined can be an
(pp. 86–87).
evidentiary source that clinicians and
Recommended Steps in Conducting probate courts can both use in making
a Retrospective Evaluation of retrospective capacity determinations.
Testamentary Capacity 7. Keep in mind that the presence of a
1. Identify the operative legal standard for dementia or other mental disorder is not
testamentary capacity in your state sufficient in itself to decide the
jurisdiction. retrospective capacity issue. Because the
2. Organize medical, legal, and other legal threshold for testamentary capacity is
records relevant to the capacity issue. low, some individuals with mild dementia
Creating a chronological timeline may still be capable of making a new will,
reflecting important medical and lay whereas patients with more advanced
events and contacts, and relevant legal dementia increasingly will not be.
transactions, is essential to organizing However, every capacity matter is
information for the assessment. individual specific and irrespective of
430 forensic neuropsychology

diagnosis, cognitive impairment, and/or the presumption of competency must stand, inso-
dementia stage requires an analysis of the far as there would be insufficient evidence to rebut
individual’s mental status and condition in the presumption or place it in legal question.
relation to the particular jurisdictional
elements for testamentary capacity. Undue Influence and
Testamentary Capacity
8. Assess testamentary capacity by
As mentioned above, undue influence is a
determining whether there is clinical and
separate legal ground for voiding a will which is
other evidence in the record supporting
related to but also distinct from testamentary
the critical legal elements of this capacity.
capacity. Undue influence has been defined as ‘any
In some cases it may not be possible to
improper or wrongful constraint, machination, or
render such a judgment, if there is
urgency of persuasion whereby the will of a person
insufficient evidence of the testator’s
is overpowered and he is induced to do or forbear
cognitive, emotional, and everyday
an act which he would not do or would do if left to
functional abilities contemporaneous with
act freely’ and also as ‘influence which deprives
the prior will execution.
person influenced of free agency or destroys free-
9. In addition to offering a capacity dom of his will and renders it more the will of
judgment, a neuropsychological expert another than his own’ (Black, 1968, pp. 1697–1698).
may in some instances be well-positioned In situations of undue influence, the testator retains
to offer a retrospective opinion regarding some level of capacity but is subjected to direct or
the possible role of undue influence in will indirect coercion that subverts his/her volition and
procurement. Many will contest cases thus the validity of the will. The resulting will thus
involve an associated, alternative legal reflects the preferences of the coercing party rather
claim of undue influence, with the than the testator, benefits the coercing party over
contention that even if the testator other potential heirs, and is inconsistent with what
possessed residual testamentary capacity, the testator’s wishes would be in the absence of this
it was supplanted by the actions of a influence (Haldipur & Ward, 1996). Indicators of
third-party influencer. undue influence include the active participation of
the coercing party in attaining a will and/or con-
There are a number of information sources that trolling the testamentary act, the role of the coerc-
can assist a clinician in making a retrospective ing party as an advisor or confidant to the testator
assessment of testamentary capacity (Spar & Garb, and his or her use of this relationship to influence
1992). These include the testator’s business records, the way in which the testator disposes of his or her
checkbook and other financial documents, and assets, and provisions within the will that are incon-
personal documents such as family films, videos, sistent with prior and/or subsequent expressions of
notebooks, and diaries. Medical records yield par- the testator’s intent in executing a will (Frolik, 2001;
ticularly useful information including mental status Marson et al., 2004).
and neuropsychological testing, diagnosis, level of Older persons with dementia or significant
impairment, and behavioral observations. As noted cognitive decline are usually highly vulnerable to
above, clinicians will find it beneficial to interview undue influence. Chronic physical and mental ill-
the testator’s family, friends, business associates, ness as well as memory loss and cognitive dys-
and other involved professionals (i.e., physician, function associated with dementia increase the
attorney, accountant, notary public, etc.) regarding dependency of older adults on others, thereby
the testator’s cognitive and functional abilities increasing their susceptibility to undue influence
during the time that the will was executed. (Haldipur & Ward, 1996). However, susceptibility
Ultimately, the clinician must assemble all of to undue influence is not restricted to individuals
the information described above, and make a who have cognitive impairment and/or those that
judgment as to whether or not the testator had require assistance in their general care and deci-
testamentary capacity at the prior relevant time sion making. As such, a will may also be ruled
points. In some cases it may not be possible to invalid by the courts even in the absence of medi-
render such a judgment, if there is insufficient evi- cal and/or mental illness if other indicators of
dence of the testator’s cognitive, emotional, and undue influence are present (Walsh et al., 1997).
functional abilities contemporaneous with the The topic of undue influence receives
prior will execution. In such cases, it is likely that considerable attention in the recent ABA–APA
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 431

capacity assessment handbook for psychologists degenerative diseases, such as Huntington’s disease,
(American Bar Association/American Psycholo- ALS or MS, and in acquired disorders such as trau-
gical Association Assessment of Capacity in Older matic brain injury or cerebrovascular accident, may
Adults Project Working Group, 2008). A number also affect different competencies (Dymek et al.,
of useful psychological conceptual frameworks 2001a; Marson et al., 2001; Marson et al., 2005). For
for understanding and analyzing undue influence example, recent studies have examined medical
are presented there (pp. 114–116). All of these are decision-making capacity in patients with trau-
relevant to situations of testamentary capacity. matic brain injury (Dreer, De Vivo, Novack,
For example, the Brandle/Heisler/Stiegel model Krzywanski, & Marson, 2008; Marson et al., 2005),
focuses on perpetrator behavior in cases of undue an acquired neurocognitive disorder with a very
influence. While the perpetrator’s ultimate goal is different trajectory of impairment—and recovery—
financial exploitation of the testator, this can be than neurodegenerative disease.
accomplished by a variety of means, including In addition, normal age-related cognitive
isolation from others and from information changes may affect higher order functional capac-
sources, the creation of fear and feelings of ities like consent capacity and financial capacity
vulnerability, fostering dependency and a sense of (Diehl, Willis, & Schaie, 1995; Park, 1992; Willis,
inadequacy, inducing negative emotions such as 1996b). Little is known about whether and to what
shame, and also rendering intermittent acts of extent such normative age-related changes may
kindness (American Bar Association/American affect the competency of nondemented older
Psychological Association Assessment of Capacity adults. Thus, studies using different age cohorts of
in Older Adults Project Working Group, 2008). healthy adults, as well as patient groups with neu-
Another useful clinical framework is the ‘SCAM’ rodegenerative diseases and dementias other than
model proposed by Bernatz which views undue AD and PD, are necessary to expand our under-
influence as an ‘inter-relational concept between standing of competency in dementia and in
victim and perpetrator’ (p. 114), and which incor- normal aging. The neurocognitive predictor
porates core factors of susceptibility of the victim, model for financial capacity in cognitively normal
a confidential relationship, active procurement of older adults (reported above) is instructive in this
financial transactions by the perpetrator, and regard (Sherod et al., 2009).
resulting financial loss or exploitation (American Finally, in an exciting development occurring
Bar Association/American Psychological Asso- as the current volume was finalized, the field is
ciation Assessment of Capacity in Older Adults now beginning to use neuroimaging techniques as
Project Working Group, 2008). a means to better understand the specific changes
in the brain that underlie capacity impairment and
EPILOGUE: CONCLUSION loss in dementia. Using structural MRI, our group
A N D S U M M A RY recently found that atrophy in the angular gyrus is
In this chapter we have examined conceptual, significantly associated with overall financial
empirical, and clinical aspects of competency loss capacity (as measured by the FCI) in patients with
in older adults with dementia by focusing on three amnestic MCI (Griffith et al., 2010). We also found
specific civil competencies: treatment consent that written arithmetic was the primary mediator
capacity, financial capacity, and testamentary capac- of this relationship between angular gyrus volume
ity. It should be apparent that our group has used and financial skill, replicating prior neuropsycho-
MCI and Alzheimer’s disease, and also Parkinson’s logical predictor findings discussed above (Sherod
disease with dementia, as the clinical context for et al., 2009). Thus, using advanced neuroimaging
understanding loss of competency. By virtue of its techniques, investigators are now poised to develop
relentless progressive nature, AD and its prodrome a true ‘neuroscience of capacity’ that will advance
MCI continue to be the most useful prism with both practice and research.
which to begin to understand relationships between In conclusion, as an investigator who first
abnormal cognition and loss of decisional capacity began studying competency in older adults more
in aging. At the same time, the reported study than 20 years ago, it is gratifying to see the increas-
results may be quite specific to the MCI/AD or PD ing clinical and academic interest in this topic.
context and may not always necessarily generalize Although much work still needs to be done, the
well to other dementias (Dymek et al., 2001a) or to field of civil competency assessment in the elderly
normal aging. For this reason, it is important to has indisputably emerged as an authentic area of
understand how cognitive changes in other neuro- clinical, academic, and forensic practice, and shows
432 forensic neuropsychology

great future promise as a field of study in our aging Archives of Neurology, 57, p. 881, with permission of
society. It will be exciting to see how this still young the American Medical Association.
field continues to evolve over the next decade. 12. Adapted from ‘Impaired financial abilities in
mild cognitive impairment: A direct assessment
N OT E S approach’ by Griffith et al. 2003, Neurology, 60 (3), p. 453,
1. Supported by grants from the National Institute with permission of American Academy of Neurology.
on Aging (1P50 AG16582 (Alzheimer’s Disease 13. Adapted from ‘Neurocognitive predictors of
Research Center) and R01 AG21927), the National financial capacity across the dementia spectrum: Normal
Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH55427), and the aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s
National Institute of Child Health and Human disease’ by Sherod et al. 2009, Journal of the Internatio-
Development (R01 HD053074). nal Neuropsychological Society, with permission of
2. Adapted from ‘Assessing the competency of Cambridge University Press.
Alzheimer’s disease patients under different legal stan-
dards: A prototype instrument’ by Marson et al. 1995, REFERENCES
Archives of Neurology, 52, p. 952, with permission of Alexander, M. (1988). Clinical determination of mental
the American Medical Association. competence. Archives of Neurology, 45, 23–26.
3. Adapted from ‘Assessing the competency of Alzheimer’s Association. (2008). Alzheimer’s disease
patients with Alzheimer’s disease under different legal facts and figures: 10 million U.S. baby boomers will
standards’ by Marson et al. 1995, Archives of Neurology, develop Alzheimer’s disease. Retrieved December 1,
52, p. 952, with permission of the American Medical 2008, from http://www.alz.org/national/documents/
Association. report_alzfactsfigures2008.pdf
4 Adapted from ‘Toward a neurologic model of American Bar Association Commission on Law, Aging
competency: Cognitive predictors of capacity to con- & American Psychological Association. (2005).
sent to treatment in Alzheimer’s disease using three Assessment of older adults with diminished
legal standards’ by Marson et al. 1996, Neurology, 46, capacity: A handbook for lawyers. Washington, DC:
p. 669, with permission of the American Academy of American Bar Association and American Psych-
Neurology. ological Association.
5. Adapted from ‘Competency to consent to medi- American Bar Association/American Psychological
cal treatment in cognitively impaired patients with Association Assessment of Capacity in Older Adults
Parkinson’s disease’ by Dymek et al. 2001. Neurology, Project Working Group. (2006). Judicial determina-
56, p. 19, with permission of AAN Enterprises, Inc. tion of capacity of older adults in guardianship pro-
6. ‘Competency to consent to medical treatment ceedings. Washington, DC: American Bar
in cognitively impaired patients with Parkinson’s Association & American Psychological Association.
disease’ by Dymek et al. 2001. Neurology, 56, p. 20, with American Bar Association/American Psychological
permission of AAN Enterprises, Inc. Association Assessment of Capacity in Older
7. Preprinted from ‘Competency to consent to Adults Project Working Group. (2008). Assessment
medical treatment in cognitively impaired patients of older adults with diminished capacity: A hand-
with Parkinson’s disease’ by Dymek et al. 2001. book for psychologists. Washington, DC: American
Neurology, 56, p. 21, with permission from AAN. Bar Association and American Psychological
8. Reprinted from ‘Competency to consent to Association.
medical treatment in cognitively impaired patients American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic
with Parkinson’s disease’ by Dymek et al. 2001. and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
Neurology, 56, pp. 17–24 (Addendum: Neurology web Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
site), with permission of AAN. Appelbaum, P. & Grisso, T. (1988). Assessing patients’
9. Reprinted from ‘Impaired financial abilities in capacities to consent to treatment. N Engl J Med,
mild cognitive impairment: A direct assessment 319, 1635–38.
approach’ by Griffith et al. 2003, Neurology, 60 (3), Appelbaum, P. & Grisso, T. (1995). The MacArthur
p. 450, with permission of AAN. Treatment Competence Study. I: Mental illness and
10. Reprinted from ‘Assessing financial capacity in competence to consent to treatment. Law and
patients with Alzheimer’s disease: A conceptual model Human Behavior, 19, 105–26.
and prototype instrument’ by Marson et al. 2000, Appelbaum, P. & Gutheil, T. (1991). Clinical handbook
Archives of Neurology, 57, p. 880, with permission of of psychiatry and the law (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD:
the American Medical Association. Williams & Wilkins.
11. Reprinted from ‘Assessing financial capacity in Appelbaum, P. & Roth, L. (1981). Clinical issues in the
patients with Alzheimer’s disease: A conceptual model assessment of competence. American Journal of
and prototype instrument’ by Marson et al. 2000, Psychiatry, 138, 1462–67.
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 433

Artero, S., Touchon, J., & Ritchie, K. (2001). Disability Farlow, M. R. & Cummings, J. (2008). A modern hypoth-
and mild cognitive impairment: A longitudinal esis: The distinct pathologies of dementia associated
population-based study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, with Parkinson’s disease versus Alzheimer’s disease.
16(11), 1092–97. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 25(4), 301–308.
Barberger-Gateau, P., Dartigues, J. F., & Letenneur, L. Farnsworth, M. (1990). Competency evaluations in a
(1993). Four Instrumental Activities of Daily general hospital. Psychosomatics, 31, 60–66.
Living Score as a predictor of one-year incident Filoteo, J. V., Maddox, W. T., Ing, A. D., & Song, D. D.
dementia. Age Ageing, 22(6), 457–63. (2007). Characterizing rule-based category learn-
Benton, A. & Hamsher, K. (1978). Multilingual Aphasia ing deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease.
Examination. Iowa City : The University of Iowa. Neuropsychologia, 45(2), 305–20.
Black, H. (1968). Black’s law dictionary (4th ed.). St. Flesch, R. (1974). The art of readable writing. New York:
Paul, Minnesota: West Publishing. Harper & Row.
Cea, C. D. & Fisher, C. B. (2003). Health care decision- Frolik, L. A. (2001). The strange interplay of testamen-
making by adults with mental retardation. Ment tary capacity and the doctrine of undue influence.
Retard, 41(2), 78–87. Are we protecting older testators or overriding
Daly, E., Zaitchik, D., Copeland, M., Schmahmann, J., individual preferences? Int J Law Psychiatry, 24(2–
Gunther, J., & Albert, M. (2000). Predicting con- 3), 253–66.
version to Alzheimer disease using standardized Girotti, G., Grassi, M., Carella, F., Soliveri, P., Musicco,
clinical information. Arch Neurol, 57(5), 675–80. M., Lamperti, E., et al. (1986). Possible involve-
Diehl, M., Willis, S., & Schaie, K. (1995). Everyday ment in attention processes in Parkinson’s disease.
problem solving in older adults: Observational In M. Yahr & K. Bergmann (Eds.), Advances in
assessment and cognitive correlates. Psychology neurology (pp. 425–29). New York: Raven Press.
and Aging, 10, 478–91. Gotham, A., Brown, R., & Marsden, C. (1988). ‘Frontal’
Drachman, D., Swearer, J., & Group, C. S. (1993). cognitive functions in patients with Parkinson’s dis-
Driving and Alzheimer’s disease: The risk of ease ‘on’ and ‘off ’ levodopa. Brain, 111, 199–231.
crashes. Neurology, 43, 2448–56. Greiffenstein, M. (1996). The neuropsychological
Dreer, L. E., De Vivo, M. J., Novack, T. A., Krzywanski, S., autopsy. Michigan Bar Journal, 75(5), 424–25.
& Marson, D. C. (2008). Cognitive predictors of Griffith, H. R., Belue, K., Sicola, A., Krzywanski, S.,
medical decision-making capacity in traumatic brain Zamrini, E., Harrell, L., et al. (2003). Impaired finan-
injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 53(4), 486–97. cial abilities in mild cognitive impairment: A direct
Dunn, L. B., Nowrangi, M. A., Palmer, B. W., Jeste, D. assessment approach. Neurology, 60(3), 449–57.
V., & Saks, E. R. (2006). Assessing decisional capac- Griffith, H. R., den Hollander, J. A., Okonkwo, O. C.,
ity for clinical research or treatment: A review of O’Brien, T., Watts, R. L., & Marson, D. C. (2008).
instruments. Am J Psychiatry, 163(8), 1323–34. Brain N-acetylaspartate is reduced in Parkinson
Dymek, M., Atchison, P., Harrell, L., & Marson, D. Disease with dementia. Alzheimer’s Disease and
(2001a). Competency to consent to treatment in Associate Dementia. 22(1): 54–60.
cognitively impaired patients with Parkinson’s dis- Griffith, H. R., Dymek, M. P., Atchison, P., Harrell, L., &
ease. Neurology, 56, 17–24. Marson, D. C. (2005). Medical decision-making in
Dymek, M. P., Atchison, P., Harrell, L., & Marson, D. C. neurodegenerative disease: mild AD and PD with
(2001b). Competency to consent to medical treat- cognitive impairment. Neurology, 65(3), 483–85.
ment in cognitively impaired patients with Griffith, H. R., Stewart, C. C., Stoeckel, L. E., Okonkwo,
Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 56(1), 17–24. O. C., den Hollander, J. A., Martin, R. C., et al.
Dymek, M. P., Marson, D. C., & Harrell, L. (1999). (2010). MRI volume of the angular gyri predicts
Factor structure of capacity to consent to medical financial skill deficits in people with amnestic mild
treatment in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: An cognitive impairment. Journal of the American
exploratory study. Journal of Forensic Neuro- Geriatrics Society. 58: 265–74.
psychology, 1, 27–48. Grisso, T. (1986). Evaluating competencies: Forensic
Edelstein, B., Nygren, M., Northrop, L., Staats, N., & assessments and instruments. New York: Plenum
Pool, D. (1993). Assessment of capacity to make Press.
medical and financial decisions. Paper presented at Grisso, T. (2003). Evaluating competencies: Forensic
the 101st Annual Convention of the American assessments and instruments (Second ed. Vol. 16).
Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. New York: Kluwer Academic.
Eisenson, J. (1954). Examining for aphasia. New York: Grisso, T. & Appelbaum, P. (1991). Mentally ill and
The Psychological Corporation. non-mentally ill patients’ abilities to understand
Fackelmann, K. (2008, Tuesday, March 11). Memory informed consent disclosure for medication. Law
loss, money loss. USA Today, p. 6D. and Human Behavior, 15, 377–88.
434 forensic neuropsychology

Grisso, T. & Appelbaum, P. (1995). The MacArthur Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983).
Treatment Competence Study. III: Abilities of Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
patients to consent to psychiatric and medical Kapp, M. (1992). Geriatrics and the law: Patient
treatments. Law and Human Behavior, 19, 149–69. rights and professional responsibilities. New York:
Grisso, T., Appelbaum, P., Mulvey, E., & Fletcher, K. Springer.
(1995). The MacArthur Treatment Competence Karlawish, J. (2008). Measuring decision-making
Study. II: Measures of abilities related to compe- capacity in cognitively impaired individuals.
tence to consent to treatment. Law and Human Neurosignals, 16(1), 91–98.
Behavior, 19, 127–48. Karlawish, J., Kim, S. Y., Knopman, D., van Dyck, C.
Gurrera, R. J., Moye, J., Karel, M. J., Azar, A. R., & H., James, B. D., & Marson, D. (2008). The views of
Armesto, J. C. (2006). Cognitive performance pre- Alzheimer disease patients and their study part-
dicts treatment decisional abilities in mild to mod- ners on proxy consent for clinical trial enrollment.
erate dementia. Neurology, 66(9), 1367–72. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry, 16(3), 240–47.
Haldipur, C. & Ward, M. (1996). Competence Karlawish, J. & Schmitt, F. (2000). Why physicians
and other legal issues. In M. Hersen & V. Van need to become more proficient in assessing their
Hasslet (Eds.), Psychological treatment of older patients’ competency and how they can achieve
adults: An introductory text (pp. 103–25). New this. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 48,
York: Plenum. 1014–1016.
Hebert, K., Scogin, F., & Marson, D. C. (in prepara- Kim, S., Caine, E., Currier, G., Leibovici, A., & Ryan,
tion). Impaired testamentary capacity in patients M. (2001). Assessing the competence of persons
with mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease: A new with Alzheimer’s disease in providing informed
avenue of civil competency research. consent for participation in research. American
Heindel, W. C., Salmon, D. P., Shults, C. W., Walicke, Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 712–717.
P. A., & Butters, N. (1989). Neuropsychological evi- Lai, J. M. & Karlawish, J. (2007). Assessing the capacity
dence for multiple implicit memory systems: A com- to make everyday decisions: A guide for clinicians
parison of Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s and an agenda for future research. Am J Geriatr
disease patients. J Neurosci, 9(2), 582–87. Psychiatry, 15(2), 101–11.
High, D. (1992). Research with Alzheimer’s disease Lichter, D. G. (2001). Movement disorders and frontal-
subjects: Informed consent and proxy decision- subcortical circuits. In D. G. Lichter & J. L.
making. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, Cummings (Eds.), Frontal-subcortical circuits in
40, 950–57. psychiatric and neurological disorders (pp. 260–
Hunt, L., Murphy, C., Carr, D., Duchek, J., Buckles, V., 313). New York: Guilford.
& Morris, J. (1997). Reliability of the Washington Mahler, M. & Cummings, J. (1990). Alzheimer disease
University Road Test: A performance-based assess- and the dementia of Parkinson disease: Comparative
ment for drivers with dementia of the Alzheimer investigations. Alzheimer Disease and Associated
type. Archives of Neurology, 54, 707–712. Disorders, 4(3), 133–49.
Huthwaite, J. S., Martin, R. C., Griffith, H. R., Anderson, Mahurin, R., Feher, E., Nance, M., Levy, J., & Pirozzolo,
B., Harrell, L. E., & Marson, D. C. (2006). Declining F. (1993). Cognition in Parkinson’s disease and
medical decision-making capacity in mild AD: related disorders. In R. Parks, R. Zec & R. Wilson
A two-year longitudinal study. Behav Sci Law, (Eds.), Neuropsychology of Alzheimer’s disease and
24(4), 453–63. other dementias (pp. 247–68). New York: Oxford
Jacobs, D., Stern, Y., & Mayeux, R. (1997). Dementia in University Press.
Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, and Marson, D. (2001a). Loss of financial capacity in
other degenerative conditions. In T. Feinberg & M. dementia: Conceptual and empirical approaches.
Farah (Eds.), Behavioral neurology and neuropsy- Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 8, 164–81.
chology (pp. 579–87). New York: McGraw-Hill. Marson, D. (2002). Cognitive models of financial capac-
Janofsky, J., McCarthy, R., & Folstein, M. (1992). The ity in MCI. Paper presented at the Gerontological
Hopkins Competency Assessment Test: A brief Society of America, Boston, MA.
method for evaluating patients’ capacity to give Marson, D. & Dymek, M. (2004). Competency in
informed consent. Hospital and Community Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Ethics, Law and
Psychiatry, 43, 132–36. Aging, 10, 63–82.
Jefferson, A. L., Lambe, S., Moser, D. J., Byerly, L. K., Marson, D., Earnst, K., Jamil, F., Bartolucci, A., &
Ozonoff, A., & Karlawish, J. H. (2008). Decisional Harrell, L. (2000). Consistency of physicians’ legal
capacity for research participation in individuals standard and personal judgments of competency
with mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc, in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the
56(7), 1236–43. American Geriatrics Society, 48, 911–18.
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 435

Marson, D., Huthwaite, J., & Hebert, K. (2004). Marson, D. C. & Hebert, K. (2008a). Financial capacity.
Testamentary capacity and undue influence in the In B. L. Cutler (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology
elderly: A jurisprudent therapy perspective. Law and the law (Vol. 1, pp. 313–16): Sage.
and Psychology Review, 28, 71–96. Marson, D. C. & Hebert, K. (2008b). Testamentary
Marson, D. & Ingram, K. (1996). Competency to con- capacity. In B. L. Cutler (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psy-
sent to treatment: A growing field of research. chology and the law (Vol. 2, pp. 798–801): Sage.
Journal of Ethics, Law and Aging, 2, 59–63. Marson, D. C., Ingram, K. K., Cody, H. A., & Harrell, L.
Marson, D., Sawrie, S., Stalvey, T., McInturff, B., & E. (1995b). Assessing the competency of patients
Harrell, L. (1998). Neuropsychological correlates with Alzheimer’s disease under different legal stan-
of declining financial capacity in patients with dards. Archives of Neurology, 52, 949–54.
Alzheimer’s disease. Paper presented at the 26th Marson, D. C., Ingram, K. K., Cody, H. A., & Harrell, L.
Annual Meeting of the International Neuro- E. (1995c). Assessing the competency of patients
psychological Society, Honolulu, HA. with Alzheimer’s disease under different legal
Marson, D., Strickland, A., Hethcox, A., DeVivo, M., standards. A prototype instrument. Arch Neurol,
Taylor, S., Krzywanski, S., et al. (2001). Assessing 52(10), 949–54.
competency to consent to treatment in traumatic Marson, D. C., McInturff, B., Hawkins, L., Bartolucci,
brain injury. Paper presented at the 21st Annual A., & Harrell, L. E. (1997b). Consistency of
Conference of the National Academy of physician judgments of capacity to consent in
Neuropsychology, San Francisco, California. mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc, 45(4),
Marson, D. & Zebley, L. (2001). The other side of the 453–57.
retirement years: Cognitive decline, dementia, and Marson, D. C., Sawrie, S. M., Snyder, S., McInturff, B.,
loss of financial capacity. Journal of Retirement Stalvey, T., Boothe, A., et al. (2000). Assessing
Planning, 4(1), 30–39. financial capacity in patients with Alzheimer dis-
Marson, D. C. (2001b). Loss of competency in ease: A conceptual model and prototype instru-
Alzheimer’s disease: Conceptual and psychometric ment. Arch Neurol, 57(6), 877–84.
approaches. Int J Law Psychiatry, 24(2–3), 267–83. Marson, D. C., Schmitt, F., Ingram, K. K., & Harrell, L.
Marson, D. C., Chatterjee, A., Ingram, K. K., & Harrell, E. (1994a). Determining the competency of
L. E. (1996). Toward a neurologic model of compe- Alzheimer’s patients to consent to treatment and
tency: Cognitive predictors of capacity to consent research. Alzheimer’s Disease and Associated
in Alzheimer’s disease using three different legal Disorders, 8(Suppl: 4), 5–18.
standards. Neurology, 46(3), 666–72. Martin, R., Griffith, H. R., Belue, K., Harrell, L.,
Marson, D. C., Cody, H. A., Ingram, K. K., & Harrell, L. Zamrini, E., Anderson, B., et al. (2008a). Declining
E. (1995a). Neuropsychologic predictors of com- financial capacity in patients with mild Alzheimer
petency in Alzheimer’s disease using a rational rea- disease: A one-year longitudinal study. Am J Geriatr
sons legal standard. Arch Neurol, 52(10), 955–59. Psychiatry, 16(3), 209–19.
Marson, D. C., Dreer, L. E., Krzywanski, S., Huthwaite, Martin, R. C., Okonkwo, O., Hill, J., Griffith, H. R.,
J. S., Devivo, M. J., & Novack, T. A. (2005). Triebel, K., Bartolucci, A., et al. (2008). Medical
Impairment and partial recovery of medical deci- decision-making capacity in cognitively impaired
sion-making capacity in traumatic brain injury: Parkinson’s disease patients without dementia.
A 6-month longitudinal study. Arch Phys Med Movement Disorders. 23 (13): 1867–74.
Rehabil, 86(5), 889–95. Mattis, S. (1976). Dementia rating scale. In R. Bellack
Marson, D. C., Earnst, K. S., Jamil, F., Bartolucci, A., & & B. Karasu (Eds.), Geriatric psychiatry. New York:
Harrell, L. E. (2000). Consistency of physicians’ Grune & Stratton.
legal standard and personal judgments of compe- Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia rating scale (DRS). Odessa,
tency in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. J Am FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Geriatr Soc, 48(8), 911–18. McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R.,
Marson, D. C. & Harrell, L. E. (1999). Neurocognitive Price, D., & Stadlan, E. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of
models that predict physician judgments of capac- Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA
ity to consent in patients with mild Alzheimer’s work group under the auspices of the Department of
disease. In D. Park, R. Morrell & K. Shifrin (Eds.), Health and Human Services Task Force on
Medical information processing and aging. New Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34, 939–44.
York: Lawrence Erlbaum. Morris, J. (1993). The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR):
Marson, D. C., Hawkins, L., McInturff, B., & Harrell, L. Current version and scoring rules. Neurology, 43,
E. (1997a). Cognitive models that predict physician 2412–14.
judgments of capacity to consent in mild Alzheimer’s Morris, J. (2002). Challenging assumptions about
disease. J Am Geriatr Soc, 45(4), 458–64. Alzheimer’s disease: Mild cognitive impairment
436 forensic neuropsychology

and the cholinergic hypothesis. Annals of Neurology, Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry, 16(7),
51(2), 143–44. 603–11.
Morris, J. C., Storandt, M., Miller, J. P., McKeel, D. W., Park, D. (1992). Applied cognitive aging research.
Price, J. L., Rubin, E. H., et al. (2001). Mild cogni- In F. Craik & T. Salthouse (Eds.), Handbook of
tive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer aging and cognition (pp. 449–93). Hillsdale, NJ:
disease. Arch Neurol, 58(3), 397–405. Erlbaum.
Moye, J. (1996). Theoretical frameworks for compe- Perr, I. N. (1991). Alleged brain damage, diminished
tency in cognitively impaired elderly adults. Journal capacity, mens rea, and misuse of medical
of Aging Studies, 10, 27–42. concepts. J Forensic Sci, 36(3), 722–27.
Moye, J. (2003). Guardianship and conservatorship. In Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C.,
T. Grisso (Ed.), Evaluating competencies: Forensic Morris, J. C., Rabins, P. V., et al. (2001). Current
assessments and instruments (2nd ed., pp. 309–90). concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch
New York, NY: Plenum Press. Neurol, 58(12), 1985–92.
Moye, J., Karel, M. J., Gurrera, R. J., & Azar, A. R. Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J.,
(2006). Neuropsychological predictors of decision- Tangalos, E. G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cogni-
making capacity over 9 months in mild-to-moder- tive impairment: Clinical characterization and out-
ate dementia. J Gen Intern Med, 21(1), 78–83. come. Arch Neurol, 56(3), 303–308.
Moye, J. & Marson, D. C. (2007). Assessment of deci- Reisberg, B., Ferris, S., deLeon, M., & Crook, T. (1982).
sion-making capacity in older adults: An emerging The Global Deterioration Scale for assessment of
area of practice and research. J Gerontol B Psychol primary degenerative dementia. American Journal
Sci Soc Sci, 62(1), P3–P11. of Psychiatry, 139, 1136–39.
Nedd, H. (1998, July 30, 1998). Fighting over the care Reitan, R. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as
of aging parents: More siblings clashing over an indication of organic brain damage. Perceptual
money and control. USA Today, p. Page 1A. and Motor Skills, 8, 271–76.
Nerenberg, L. (1996). Financial abuse of the elderly. Ritchie, K., Artero, S., & Touchon, J. (2001).
Washington, DC: National Center on Elder Abuse. Classification criteria for mild cognitive impair-
Okonkwo, O., Griffith, H. R., Belue, K., Lanza, S., ment: A population based validation study. Neuro-
Zamrini, E. Y., Harrell, L. E., et al. (2007). Medical logy, 56, 37–42.
decision-making capacity in patients with mild Roked, F. & Patel, A. (2008). Which aspects of cogni-
cognitive impairment. Neurology, 69(15), 1528–35. tive function are best associated with testamentary
Okonkwo, O., Griffith, H. R., Copeland, J. N., Belue, K., capacity in patients with Alzheimer’s disease? Int J
Lanza, S., Zamrini, E., et al. (2008a). Medical deci- Geriatr Psychiatry, 23(5), 552–53.
sion-making capacity in mild cognitive impair- Roth, L., Meisel, A., & Lidz, C. (1977). Tests of compe-
ment: A three-year longitudinal study. Neurology, tency to consent to treatment. American Journal of
71(19), 1474–80. Psychiatry, 134, 279–84.
Okonkwo, O. C., Griffith, H. R., Belue, K., Lanza, S., Royall, D., Mahurin, R., & Gray, K. (1992). Bedside
Zamrini, E. Y., Harrell, L. E., et al. (2008b). assessment of executive cognitive impairment: The
Cognitive models of medical decision-making Executive Interview. Journal of the American
capacity in patients with mild cognitive impair- Geriatrics Society, 40, 1221–26.
ment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc, 14(2), 297–308. Schindler, B., Rachmandi, D., Matthews, M., & Podell,
Overman, W. & Stoudemire, A. (1988). Guidelines for K. (1995). Competency and the frontal lobe: The
legal and financial counseling of Alzheimer’s dis- impace of executive dysfunction on decisional
ease patients and their families. American Journal capacity. Psychosomatics, 36, 400–404.
of Psychiatry, 145, 1495–1500. Sherlock, R. (1984). Competency to consent to medical
Palmer, B. W. & Jeste, D. V. (2006). Relationship of care: Toward a general view. General Hospital
individual cognitive abilities to specific compo- Psychiatry, 6, 71–76.
nents of decisional capacity among middle-aged Sherod, M. G., Griffith, H. R., Copeland, J., Belue K.,
and older patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Krzywanski, S., Zamrini E. Y., et al. (2009).
Bull, 32(1), 98–106. Neurocognitive predictors of financial capacity
Palmer, B. W. & Savla, G. N. (2007). The association of across the dementia spectrum: Normal aging, MCI,
specific neuropsychological deficits with capacity and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International
to consent to research or treatment. J Int Neuropsychological Society. 15(2): 258–67.
Neuropsychol Soc, 13(6), 1047–59. Shulman, K. I., Cohen, C. A., & Hull, I. (2005).
Palmer, K., Backman, L., Winblad, B., & Fratiglioni, L. Psychiatric issues in retrospective challenges of tes-
(2008). Mild cognitive impairment in the tamentary capacity. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 20(1),
general population: Occurrence and progression to 63–69.
Assessing Civil Competencies in Older Adults with Dementia 437

Spar, J. & Garb, A. (1992). Assessing competency to decision-making capacity: An overview. Int Psycho-
make a will. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, geriatr, 16(4), 397–419.
169–74. Walsh, A. C., Brown, B. B., Kaye, K., & Grigsby, J.
Spar, J. E., Hankin, M., & Stodden, A. (1995). Assessing (1997). Mental capacity: Legal and medical aspects
mental capacity and susceptibility to undue influ- of assessment and treatment (2nd ed.). Deerfield,
ence. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 13, 391– Illinois: Clark, Boardman, & Callaghan.
403. Walton, V. (2002, February 2, 2002). Con man
Stern, Y., Richards, M., Sano, M., & Mayeux, R. (1993). sentenced to 20 years. Birmingham News, pp.
Comparison of cognitive changes in patients with 11A–2A.
Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. Arch Neurol, Wan, H., Sengupta, M., Velkoff, V. A., & Debarros,
50(10), 1040–45. K. A. (2005). U.S. Census Bureau, current publica-
Tabert, M. H., Manly, J. J., Liu, X., Pelton, G. H., tion reports, 65+ in the United States: 2005.
Rosenblum, S., Jacobs, M., et al. (2006). Neuro- Retrieved December 1, 2008, from http://www.
psychological prediction of conversion to census.gov/prod/2006pubs/p23–209.pdf
Alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive White, R., Au, R., Durso, R., & Moss, M. (1992).
impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 63(8), 916–24. Neuropsychological function in Parkinson’s dis-
Tepper, A. & Elwork, A. (1984). Competency to con- ease. In R. White (Ed.), Clinical syndromes in adult
sent to treatment as a psychological construct. Law neuropsychology: The practitioner’s handbook.
and Human Behavior, 8, 205–23. New York: Elsevier Science.
Touchon, J. & Ritchie, K. (1999). Prodromal cognitive Widera, E., Steenpaas, V., Marson, D., & Sudore, R.
disorder in Alzheimer’s disease. Intenational (2011). Finances in the older patient with cognitive
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 556–63. impairment. Journal of the American Medical
Triebel, K., Martin, R., Griffith, H. R., Marceaux, M. A., Association, 305(7), 698–706.
Okonkwo, O. C., Harrell, L., Clark, D., Brockington, Willis, S. (1996b). Everyday cognitive competence in
J., Powers, R., Bartolucci, A. & Marson, D. (2009). elderly persons: Conceptual issues and empirical
Declining financial capacity in patients with mild findings. The Gerontologist, 36, 595–601.
cognitive impairment: A one-year longitudinal Wolinsky, F. & Johnson, R. (1991). The use of health
study. Neurology. 73, 928–34. services by older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc
Troster, A. I. (2008). Neuropsychological characteristics Sci, 46, 345–57.
of dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s dis- ‘Woman out $5,300 in two cons.’ (1996, March 3, 1996).
ease with dementia: Differentiation, early detection, Birmingham News.
and implications for ‘mild cognitive impairment’ Yaffe, K., Petersen, R. C., Lindquist, K., Kramer, J., &
and biomarkers. Neuropsychol Rev, 18(1), 103–119. Miller, B. (2006). Subtype of mild cognitive impair-
Vellinga, A., Smit, J. H., van Leeuwen, E., van Tilburg, ment and progression to dementia and death.
W., & Jonker, C. (2004). Instruments to assess Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord, 22(4), 312–19.
16
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology
and Assessment of Competency
ROBERT L. DENNEY

Clinical neuropsychologists provide unique neuropsychology practitioners; nevertheless, the


services in a wide variety of settings. Although the information reveals the reality that neuropsy-
idea of forensic neuropsychological practice may chology has something to contribute to criminal
bring to mind involvement in personal injury and forensic matters (Denney & Sullivan, 2008;
other civil tort cases, there are a growing number Denney & Wynkoop, 2000; Martell, 1992b).
of clinical neuropsychologists providing services Clinical neuropsychology’s involvement in the
to participants in criminal forensic proceedings criminal judicial system is understandable because
as well (Denney & Wynkoop, 2000; Giuliano, the field has much to contribute when issues of
Barth, Hawk, & Ryan, 1997; Heilbronner, 2004). cognition compromising central nervous system
Kaufmann (2008) reported results of a LEXUS pathologies arise. Neuropsychologists can bring
search using the term “neuropsycholo.” This to the judicial system their understanding of
search reveals the term was used in 3,294 court neuroanatomy, neuropathology, and most impor-
cases over the prior 70 years. Approximately 1,775 tant, how neuropathological conditions affect
of those cases occurred in criminal court, with thinking skills and decision-making capacity
over two-thirds falling within the last decade. This (Bigler & Clement, 1997; Lezak, Howieson,
trend is understandable given the apparent higher Loring, Hannay, & Fischer, 2004; Ricker, 2003).
rates of brain injury among criminal populations No less important is neuropsychology’s ability
(Barr, 2008; Magaletta, Diamond, McLearen, & to identify when unusual behaviors are not caused
Denney, 2010; Martell, 1992a), but it also reflects by neuropathological conditions. The obvious
the relevance of neuropsychological expertise to example is feigning of deficits, but nonneuro-
the criminal judicial system in general. pathological conditions come to play as well, such
Borum and Grisso (1995) surveyed test use in as psychopathy, other personality disturbances,
criminal forensic evaluations and found 46–50% and general psychiatric concerns. Ruling out neu-
of forensic psychologists indicated they used rocognitive deficits in non-neurological condi-
some type of neuropsychological assessment in tions is just as important as identifying the
their pretrial evaluations. Mittenberg, Patton, presence of potentially disabling neurocognitive
Canyock, and Condit (2002) presented results of a concerns. Further, such evaluations can delineate
national survey of board-certified neuropsycho- neurocognitive functioning when diagnostic
logists regarding the estimated base rates of issues other than neuropathology exist, such as
symptom exaggeration and malingering. The 131 developmental and psychiatric conditions.
survey respondents indicated they completed a Criminal courts need clear understanding
total of 33,531 annual evaluations. Of these evalu- regarding a defendant’s cognitive functioning
ations, 34% were considered forensic in nature when there is concern that it may be compromised.
(19% personal injury, 11% disability/workers’ It has been long held that the U.S. Constitution
compensation, and 4% criminal litigation). This requires defendants to have adequate understand-
4% constitutes 1,341 criminally related forensic ing and the ability to aid in their own behalf
evaluations per year for just 131 practitioners. when facing criminal proceedings affecting their
One could argue this sample is a unique subset of “liberty or life” (Youtsey v. United States, 1899).
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 439

Under the U.S. Constitution, society can CRIMINAL FORENSICS


deprive people of their liberty only under two doc- A S A S U B S P E C I A LT Y
trines: police power and parens patriae. Parens OF FORENSIC
patriae refers to the government looking after the NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
citizen’s welfare in a parental role. An example In the “Petition for the Recognition of Forensic
includes civil commitment procedures for mental Psychology as a Specialty in Professional Psycho-
health treatment. Police power is, of course, the logy,” the American Board of Forensic Psychology
criminal justice system. The U.S. Constitution and American Psychology–Law Society jointly
outlines minimal acceptable rights for citizens of defined forensic psychology in this manner
the United States under federal and state laws. States (Forensic Specialty Council, 2000):
can always provide more personal rights than
dictated by the U.S. Constitution, but not less. [Forensic psychology is] the professional prac-
The aspects of the Constitution most relevant tice by psychologists within the areas of clinical
for practitioners interfacing with the criminal psychology, counseling psychology, neuropsy-
justice system are the 5th, 6th, and 14th chology, and school psychology, when they are
Amendments. The 5th Amendment includes the engaged regularly as experts and represent
right to be free from self-incrimination. The 6th themselves as such, in an activity primarily
Amendment guarantees the right to counsel and intended to provide professional psychological
representation. The 14th Amendment establishes expertise to the judicial system. (p. 6)
that everyone will have equal protection under
the law. Last, both the 5th and 14th Amendments Under this definition, forensic neuropsychology
declare that no one will lose “life, liberty, or prop- could be viewed as a specialty area within clinical
erty” without due process of the law (Denney & neuropsychology. Neuropsychologists can pro-
Sullivan, 2008). vide expertise to the judicial system in civil as well
Constitutional guidelines have tremendous as criminal areas. In this regard, the application of
implication for the practice of neuropsychology neuropsychological expertise to criminal forensic
in criminal areas. For example, due process matters could be considered a subspecialty of
requires a mentally competent defendant. Aside forensic neuropsychology. Although good clinical
from mental competency, there are other times neuropsychological skill and expertise form the
during criminal proceedings when courts can basis of sound practice, forensic neuropsychology,
benefit from neuropsychological expertise, such particularly criminal forensic matters, requires
as mental state of the defendant at the time of the understanding of a unique knowledge domain
crime, mitigating issues and treatment needs to beyond that of sound clinical practice (Denney &
consider at the time of sentencing, and prerelease Wynkoop, 2000; Martell, 1992b; Sullivan &
assessment of potential increased dangerousness. Denney, 2008).
Participants in the criminal judicial system can
benefit from the input of neuropsychology, but FORENSIC
neuropsychologists need to understand the NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
unique aspects of forensic work in the criminal AS A UNIQUE PRACTICE
setting and have specialized knowledge and tech- SETTING
niques for competent practice (Melton, Petrila, There are striking differences between neuro-
Poythress, & Slobogin, 2007; Denney & Sullivan, psychology practice in the general clinical setting
2008). and that of the forensic setting, particularly the
The goal of this chapter is to provide neurop- criminal realm. Goals of the two specialties, by
sychological practitioners with an introductory definition, differ greatly. For example, the goal of
understanding of issues involved in practicing in clinical evaluation is most often alleviation
the criminal forensic arena in general and the of human suffering and improvement of levels of
assessment of competency to stand trial in par- functioning through evaluation and development
ticular. Chapter 17 continues the introduction by of efficient intervention. With the exception of
focusing on assessment of criminal responsibility, treatment recommendations, the goal of forensic
diminished capacity and responsibility, danger- evaluation is most often to determine whether a
ousness assessment, death penalty, and ethical defendant’s psychological problems meet a spe-
and professional development issues. cific legal standard. These disparate goals create
440 forensic neuropsychology

different assumptions, roles, alliances, and tremendous motivation to manipulate the evalua-
methods (Denney & Wynkoop, 2000; Goldstein, tor and judicial system (Rogers, 1997). It is coun-
2003; Greenberg & Shuman, 1997; Heilbrun, terproductive to assume defendants have
2001). neurocognitive deficits, want help for deficits, or
will present themselves in an honest manner
Assumptions within such a harsh and potentially punitive
In clinical practice, neuropsychologists assume, setting. It is no surprise, then, that these differ-
for the most part, that patients voluntarily ences in assumptions result in different roles for
seek help because they want relief from bother- neuropsychologists.
some symptoms. There is often a diagnosable
condition that occasions the service, whether Roles
the service is assessment or intervention. Certainly, There are striking differences between the roles
within the neurorehabilitation setting, an alliance of clinical provider and forensic examiner. Heilbrun
is built. The overriding theme is one of colla- (2001) outlined differences between the roles of
boration, mutual goals, and belief. Criminal forensic examiner and treatment clinician. As can
defendants are often not self-referred, or even be seen from Table 16.1, the differences between
voluntary, recipients of services. In many the two roles reveal themselves in a variety of
instances, they may not have a psychological or attitudes and behaviors. The clinical provider
neuropsychological complaint. In addition, the maintains a role consistent with helping the
possibility of harsh punishment can create patient. Rather than patient–helper, however, the

TABLE 16.1 DIFFERENCES BET WEEN TREATMENT AND FORENSIC ROLES FOR
MENTAL HEALTH PROFESSIONALS

Dimension Therapeutic Forensic


Purpose Diagnose and treat symptoms of illness Assist decision-maker or attorney

Examiner-examinee Helping role Objective or quasi-objective stance


relationship
Notification of purpose Implicit assumptions about purpose Formal and explicit notification
shared by doctor and patient
Who is being served Individual patient Variable; may be court, attorney, and
patient
Nature of standard being Medical, psychiatric, Medical, psychiatric, neuropsychological,
considered neuropsychological and legal
Data sources Self-report, behavioral observations, Self-report, behavioral observations,
medical diagnostic procedures, medical diagnostic procedures,
and neuropsychological testing. and neuropsychological testing.
Occasional corroborative Nearly always incorporate
information. corroborative and surreptitious
observation by
Response style of Assumed to be predominantly reliable others.
examine Not assumed to be reliable
Clarification of reasoning Optional
and limits of knowledge Very important
Written report Brief, conclusory statement common
Lengthy and detailed, documents findings,
Court testimony Not expected reasoning, and conclusions
Expected

Adapted from Kirk Heilbrun, Principles of Forensic Mental Health Assessment. New York: Springer/Kluwer Academic/Plenum, 2001, p. 9,
Table 1.2. With permission of the author.
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 441

forensic evaluator maintains a more neutral role, a in the Ethical and Professional Issues section of
role consistent with being a “seeker of truth” and chapter 17, this volume.
judicial educator (Greenberg & Shuman, 1997;
Saks, 1990). It can often be a difficult role to main- Methodology
tain. The evaluator should realize his or her opin- Given the different assumptions, roles, and
ion has the potential to do significant harm, alliances, it is no surprise competent and ethical
particularly from a standard psychotherapeutic forensic evaluation requires somewhat different
mindset. Potential consequences can be great. methodology from that of routine clinical prac-
Probably the most serious example includes capital tice. Clinical practice typically incorporates an
cases, for which the evaluator must provide an interview with the patient, and perhaps an infor-
opinion on competence to be executed. A neuro- mant familiar with the patient, and neuropsy-
psychologist who is uncomfortable with the task of chological testing to characterize the patient’s
being an unbiased seeker of truth should avoid difficulties or to arrive at a diagnosis and make
forensic practice. treatment recommendations. The entire process is
designed to provide assistance to the patient, his
Alliance or her caregivers, and medical managers in a
Developing therapeutic alliances with neuro- timely fashion. Forensic assessment requires a
rehabilitation patients is required for successful broader base of information sources than is
rehabilitation outcomes. Sohlberg and Mateer typical of clinical practice.
(2001) went so far as to include it as a basic The evaluator must also place more weight on
principle of cognitive rehabilitation. They noted, objective test results than subjective complaints,
“Cognitive rehabilitation requires a sound self-report checklists, and behavior during clini-
therapeutic alliance among the therapist, client, cal interviews. Systematic assessment of negative
and family members or care givers” (p. 21). response bias and malingering is a necessity in
A therapeutic alliance allows the therapist the criminal forensic setting. Surreptitious obser-
to foster motivation and hopefulness on behalf vation can be invaluable, particularly when signs
of the patient (Parenté & Herrmann, 1996). of poor motivation or symptom exaggeration
Forensic evaluations are not therapeutic endeav- exist (Denney & Wynkoop, 2000; Wynkoop &
ors. As such, the forensic examiner’s allegiance is Denney, 1999). The evaluator must carry out the
with finding the truth in a thorough, ethical evaluation much like a detective would attempt to
manner. sleuth out the truth. It can take time to locate
It must be remembered who is the recipient of and review past medical and educational records
these services. In general clinical work, the patient and interview others familiar with the defendant.
is clearly the recipient. In this manner, the patient Nonetheless, this “search for truth” requires
is the client. The client role is less clear in forensic the forensic psychologist to gather information
endeavors. Forensic examiners must realize the from a wide variety of sources aside from the
patient is generally not the client. More typically, defendant and to consider more critically the
the recipient of services, particularly evaluative defendant’s self-report. Along with specialized
services, is the court (and by extension, the jury) knowledge domains, the contrast in methodology
or attorney. Other distinctions between the two between clinical and forensic practice is a major
roles are presented in Table 16.1. difference between specialties. This difference in
Lack of therapeutic alliance in forensic evalu- theoretical basis necessitates a broader model of
ation does not, however, eliminate the need to practice.
develop rapport with the defendant or to treat Denney and Wynkoop (2000) adapted Mrad’s
him or her with dignity and respect. Rapport (1996) multiple data source model (MDSM) to
fosters self-disclosure and motivation to perform the practice of criminal forensic neuropsychol-
during neuropsychological testing. It is possible ogy. The model represents a synthesis of various
to maintain a professional and ethical relationship authors in forensic psychology (Grisso, 1988;
while maintaining the strict boundaries of the Melton et al., 2007; Shapiro, 1984, 1991, 1999),
forensic evaluation process. The difference in particularly related to the assessment of sanity
alliance between clinical and forensic evaluations (and other past mental states). The model is
is exemplified in the limited confidentiality in represented in Figure 16.1. Although designed to
criminal forensic practice. Issues of confidential- guide the evaluator in tapping all relevant infor-
ity within the criminal evaluation are addressed mation sources for identifying a defendant’s
442 forensic neuropsychology

Self-report Other Data Sources Opinion


Psychological tests
Self-reported symptoms Neuropsychological tests
and behavior when Present mental
Present Mental status exam
aware of being observed status and
Behavioral observations
diagnosis
especially unobtrusive
Medical/neurological exam

Defendant’s explanation Arrest reports


of his thoughts, feelings, Investigative records
Time of Mental state,
and behavior prior to, Witness statements
Offense motivation, and Ultimate
during, and following the Video/audio tapes
diagnosis at Issue
offense Physical evidence
time of offense
Confessions
Family/friend reports

Self-reported social history: Hospital/psych. records


Childhood NCIC/FBI record Historical
Education Employment records symptoms,
History Employment/military School records diagnosis, and
Relationships Military records patterns of
Substance abuse Probation officer behavior
Criminal record accounts
Psychiatric treatment Family/friend reports

FIGURE 16.1: Multiple data source model. Reproduced with kind permission of David Mrad, Ph.D.,
ABPP.

mental state at the time of the offense, it is also competency to be sentenced, but it quickly evolved
helpful as a general model of forensic assessment. into an evaluation of past competency to stand
The model ensures the evaluator will acquire trial and sanity.
corroborative information rather than relying on
the defendant’s self-report and presentation “Where’s the Video?” A Case of
during interviews and testing. Vital Corroborative Data
The first two columns represent sources of A 70-year-old man with diabetes was convicted
information (self-report and corroborative) which by a jury of gambling and money laundering.
when combined can lead to an understanding of After his conviction, defense acquired a mental
mental state. Each row represents a different point health evaluation by a neuropsychiatrist in prepa-
in time (currently, historically, and a specific time ration for diminished capacity arguments during
in the past). There should be reasonable consis- sentencing. The neuropsychiatrist found signs of
tency between each of the columns and rows. The periventricular white matter changes on magnetic
model is discussed more thoroughly as it relates resonance imaging and concluded the man was
to sanity in chapter 17, this volume. It can be quite not competent to be sentenced. The court referred
helpful in the assessment of competency as well him for inpatient mental health evaluation to
because it facilitates information acquisition from address competency to proceed with sentencing.
objective information sources, particularly when During this evaluation, more than one of his
dealing with retrospective competency. recent attorneys and the defendant’s wife (a code-
The following case exemplifies the need to fendant also convicted) reported him having
acquire corroborative information about the terrible debilitation, particularly at the time of the
defendant’s past behavior. It also demonstrates recent trial. The case became quite complicated
how evaluations for competency can lead to because new charges were brought against the
complicated additional judicial inquiries. The case man for jury tampering and obstruction of justice
started out as a reasonably simple evaluation for secondary to allegations he contacted and
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 443

attempted to manipulate a juror during the recent carrying out real estate transactions, and the other
trial. The judge in charge of the jury tampering/ businessperson with whom he was interacting
obstruction case requested a mental health evalu- described his negotiation and financial analysis
ation focused on criminal responsibility. Both skills as “good as always.”
issues were addressed during the same inpatient More striking yet was the fact he was going
evaluation, with reports going to separate courts. through divorce proceedings in a local court
Neuropsychological testing revealed only during the same week as the criminal trial. When
minor bilateral motor slowing. All other results, he was not in federal court, he was in divorce court.
including measures of attention, concentration, His divorce attorney was one of the attorneys
learning, memory, and abstract reasoning, were in claiming how impaired he was during the criminal
the range of average to slightly above average given trial. The best part was the fact that this particular
his age and educational background. Regarding divorce court videotaped all its proceedings.
his current functioning, I considered him compe- On several videotapes, dated the same week and
tent to proceed, but the issue was much more interspersed with court appearances in the crimi-
complicated regarding his past mental state. nal trial, the defendant was vigorously questioned
Regarding the time of the trial, I received a by his attorney regarding real estate property lines
great deal of information from the defense attor- and property values while pointing out boundaries
ney and very little information from prosecution on a map. He was able to follow rapid-fire ques-
beyond basic investigative material, including the tioning and respond quickly. There were no signs
contacted juror statements and the defendant’s of cognitive compromise on these tapes.
private investigator’s statements. This man assisted Last, and even more astounding, was the
the defendant in acquiring personal information acquisition of court transcripts of a postconvic-
about the juror. Available information suggested a tion detention hearing at which the defendant’s
combination of uncontrolled insulin-dependent attorney (the very one claiming how incompetent
diabetes mellitus, recent subcortical strokes, stress he was during the trial) was arguing before
of the trial, and depression resulted in some level the court that the court should release the defen-
of cognitive compromise at the time of the trial. dant on bond because his assistance was needed
I provided this opinion regarding his appreciation in preparation for sentencing. The attorney
of the wrongfulness of contacting a juror (sanity): actually made statements to the effect that he
expected the defendant to continue working on
There is evidence of mental illness or defect at the case as well as he did during the trial and that
the time [of the alleged offense]. . . . Although his his help had been invaluable; he was like having a
judgment was impacted to a degree, he was not “paralegal.”
so incapacitated by illness as to cause him to not All of this new information flew in the face of
understand what he was doing or what was going my previous opinion that he demonstrated some
on around him. On the contrary, his behavior as cognitive deficits during the trial. My opinion
outlined in the investigative reports suggested he changed, and I explained in the report how the
made comments and acted in a manner consis- new information had an impact on my reasoning.
tent with someone who knew their actions were The defendant was found by the court to have
wrong and/or potentially illegal. been competent during the trial. This case dem-
onstrates how easy it is to come to a determina-
As a result of my opining that the defendant tion and only later find out vital information had
had “some amount of incapacity at the time,” not been received. Solid corroborative informa-
defense now claimed he was incompetent during tion regarding defendant behavior during exami-
the past trial, and counsel was ineffective by not nation of mental state at the time of the offense is
noticing this fact. The first court then requested a vital in nearly any circumstance, but particularly
retrospective competency evaluation to deter- when there is documented presence of potentially
mine if he was, in fact, competent to stand trial debilitating disease.
during the jury trial in which he was convicted on This case demonstrates the complexity of
gambling and money laundering. neuropsychological assessment in the criminal
During this evaluation, striking new informa- forensic setting. Using the multiple data source
tion came to light. During the lengthy trial model as a template to ensure information from
process, the defendant was out of jail on bond all relevant areas should increase diagnostic
and living at his home. In his free time, he was clarity and provide the needed information to
444 forensic neuropsychology

address forensic questions competently. Even overview of these points, with brief descriptions
having the appropriate information, however, will (Grisso, 1988).
not facilitate the evaluator making a correct for-
mulation about forensic questions if he or she Threshold to Seek
does not understand the correct legal standards Competency Evaluation
relevant to that forensic question. I now turn to Competency evaluations are the most commonly
one of the most commonly encountered legal requested mental health studies in criminal
issues, criminal competency. forensics. Defense attorneys typically raise the
issue of a defendant’s competency to proceed;
CRIMINAL COMPETENCIES however, prosecutors and judges (termed sua
Competency is broadly considered the capacity to sponte) can raise the issue as well. When defense
decide or perform certain functions. From a legal counsel raises the issue, not uncommonly, it is
perspective, a large portion of the concept of over the objection of the defendant, particularly
competency includes the cognitive construct of when psychosis is involved. This event places the
“knowledge.” It implies a person’s understanding defense attorney in a unique ethical position
of issues pertaining to participation in a specific between fulfilling the wishes of the client and also
legal proceeding (Reisner & Slobogin, 1990). This protecting his or her constitutional right to a fair
understanding includes some sense of apprecia- trial. It also strains the attorney–client relation-
tion for such issues as nature of the procedure, ship (Melton et al., 2007).
risks, likelihood of success, available alternative Steadman, Monahan, Hartstone, Davis, and
options and strategies, and relative advantages/ Robbins (1982) found that 6,500 defendants were
disadvantages of potential courses of action. adjudicated not competent to stand trial and com-
The issue of competency can arise during any mitted to public mental institutions for treatment
phase of the criminal judicial process, from the in 1978. They estimated this result came from
first contact a suspect has with law enforcement to 25,000 requested competency evaluations. Hoge
the time of sentencing and even to the point of and colleagues (1997) cited a personal communi-
execution in capital cases. Table 16.2 presents an cation from Thomas Grisso regarding information

TABLE 16.2 SPECIFIC COMPETENCIES IN THE CRIMINAL JUSTICE PROCESS

Competency To: General Issue in Question

Confess (or to Waive Understanding and appreciation of rights to silence and legal counsel when the rights
Rights at Pretrial may be waived at the request of law enforcement investigators seeking a self-
Investigations) incriminating statement
Plead Guilty Understanding and appreciation of above, and of the right to a jury trial, the right to
confront one’s accusers, and the consequences of a conviction
Waive Right to Counsel Understanding and appreciation of the dangers of self-representation at trial
Stand Trial Ability to assist an attorney in developing and presenting a defense, and to understand
the nature of the trial and its potential consequences
Be Sentenced Understanding and appreciation of nature of the sentence to be imposed (after trial has
resulted in conviction)
Waive Further Appeal Understanding and appreciation of right for additional appeal and potential
(when facing an consequences of waiving it
execution)
Be Executed Understanding and appreciation of nature and purpose of the punishment, and ability
to assist counsel in any available appeal

Note: The wording of these definitions does not conform to prevailing legal terminology. They are intended only to convey the general issues
raised in each specific competency. Reproduced from Competency to Stand Trial Evaluations, by T. Grisso, 1988, p. 3. with permission of
Professional Resource Press.
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 445

derived from a 50-state survey (Grisso, Cocozza, The judge denied a request for mistrial and
Steadman, Fisher, & Greer, 1994) in which state continued the trial even without the defendant’s
forensic directors were asked to estimate the presence, citing his absence as voluntary. The jury
number evaluations of competency to stand trial found him guilty, and he was sentenced to life
performed. The total estimate was from 24,000 to in prison. The case eventually came before the
39,000 studies. It is not surprising that so many U.S. Supreme Court to determine whether
competency evaluations are done in the United the trial court erred in not addressing the compe-
States, as the threshold to raise concern over tency issue. The Supreme Court concluded there
competency is quite low, as revealed by the U.S. was certainly enough evidence to meet the bona
Supreme Court in Pate v. Robinson (1966) and fide doubt standard presented in Pate. Trial courts
Drope v. Missouri (1975). should consider any evidence coming from
Theodore Robinson was found guilty of irrational behavior, demeanor at trial, or any prior
murder and sentenced to life in prison by the medical opinions. Disconcerting information
Illinois state court. Although his attorney con- from even one of these sources may trigger
tended throughout the court proceedings that an inquiry into the defendant’s competency to
Robinson was not competent to proceed and was proceed.
insane at the time of the offense and Robinson’s In the time since these two cases were decided,
relatives and family friend testified during the it has become clear that courts will rarely refuse a
trial that he was insane, the trial court never request for competency evaluation. This very low
stopped the trial to have him examined for com- threshold for referral results in the fact that most
petence. Robinson’s history included significant defendants referred for competency evaluation
childhood traumatic brain injury, subsequent are competent to proceed. Consistent with this
erratic behavior, and state hospitalization as an belief is the finding that only an estimated 26% of
adult for psychotic behavior. It was later argued those competency evaluations mentioned in the
that Robinson “deliberately waived the defense of Steadman et al. (1982) survey were considered
his competence to stand trial by failing to demand not competent by the court.
a [competency] hearing as provided by Illinois More recent research from various states
law” (Pate v. Robinson, 1966, p. 821). The U.S. across the United States reveals estimates from
Supreme Court responded, “It was contradictory 13% to 46% of evaluees were considered not com-
to argue that a defendant may be incompetent, petent to proceed by mental health evaluators
and yet knowingly or intelligently ‘waive’ his right (Cox & Zapf, 2004; Riley, 1998; Rosenfeld &
to have the court determine his capacity to Wall, 1998; Warren, Rosenfeld, Fitch, & Hawk,
stand trial” (p. 821). The High Court concluded 1997). Cochrane, Grisso, and Frederick (2001)
the trial court’s failure to address the competency reviewed data for 1,710 criminal defendants
issue deprived him of his right to a fair trial. referred by federal courts throughout the US
The Court went on to conclude that a hearing on and found an overall 18% rate of recommended
competency should occur whenever the evidence incompetence in the federal jurisdiction. While
raises a “bona fide doubt” as to the defendant’s some of these estimates appear high, Melton
competency. and colleagues (2007) suggested “when more
The U.S. Supreme Court considered this rigorous (i.e., more valid) evaluation standards
issue further in the interesting case of Drope and procedures are applied, the percentage found
v. Missouri (1975). James Drope was charged incompetent is likely to be lower” (p. 142). This
with rape. Prior to trial, the defense had a psychia- low rate of incompetency occurs for at least
trist evaluate him; the psychiatrist concluded two other reasons beyond a low threshold to
he needed mental health treatment. The defense request evaluation. As it will become apparent,
requested a delay in the proceedings for the the threshold for competency to stand trial is also
defendant to receive psychiatric treatment, but not very high.
the trial judge denied the request and proceeded Last, many competency referrals occur for
with trial. reasons unrelated to mental health concern, such
During the trial, the defendant’s wife (the as attorney ignorance, confusion between sanity
victim) testified and confirmed his history of and competency, as an information-seeking ruse,
“strange behavior.” On the second day of the as a tactical delay, and for strategic planning of
trial, he shot himself in the stomach in an the case (Melton et al., 2007; Roesch & Golding,
apparent suicide attempt and was hospitalized. 1978; Rosenberg & McGarry, 1972).
446 forensic neuropsychology

Competency as a U.S. under the new standard. The following has


Constitutional Right been now termed the “Dusky standard” (Dusky v.
The concept of not allowing a mentally “defective” United States, 1960):
defendant to progress through the criminal
judicial system can be traced to at least the middle [The] test must be whether he has sufficient
1600s in British Common Law (Melton et al., present ability to consult with his lawyer with a
2007). In 1899, the U.S. Court of Appeals for the reasonable degree of rational understanding—
6th Circuit determined it was a violation of the and whether he has a rational as well as factual
U.S. Constitution’s 14th Amendment right to due understanding of the proceedings against him.
process to allow a mentally incompetent person (p. 402)
to proceed through the criminal judicial process
(Youtsey v. United States, 1899). The circuit court The Dusky standard spells out the minimal
wrote that it was: level of competency necessary under the U.S.
Constitution for all criminal jurisdictions in the
Fundamental that an insane person can neither United States. As such, it has been written into
plead to an arraignment, be subjected to a trial, statute in one form or other in most jurisdictions
or, after trial, receive judgment, or after judg- in the United States (Grisso, Borum, Edens, Moye,
ment, undergo punishment. . . . It is not “due & Otto, 2003; Marcopulos, Morgan, & Denney,
process of law” to subject an insane person to 2008). Although wording varies, each jurisdiction
trial upon an indictment involving liberty or addresses these two prongs, factual and rational,
life. (pp. 940–941) at the minimum.
Dusky also made several other key points.
In 1899, the term insane was more broadly defined Competency is an issue of current ability as
and often referred to lack of competency, as in opposed to mental state at some time in the past
this passage. Youtsey established the constitution- (the exception, of course, is an evaluation of retro-
ality of adjudicative competency, but it was not spective competency, as briefly touched in the
until 1960 that the U.S. Supreme Court established above case example). Implied is the need to remain
a holding on what constituted the difference competent for the foreseeable near future, at least
between competency and incompetency (Dusky as long as the upcoming proceeding.
v. United States, 1960). Occasionally, a defendant’s competency will
In Dusky, the U.S. Supreme Court identified the vary from week to week in what amounts to a
requirement that a person needed a rational as well “moving target.” Under these circumstances,
as factual understanding for competency to stand providing treatment, reevaluation, and guarantee-
trial. Milton Dusky was arrested in August 1958 for ing the defendant a fair trial process can be
kidnapping a 15-year-old girl, transporting her from difficult work for the judge.
Kansas to Missouri, and raping her. He was referred The term ability to consult with his or her
for mental health evaluation, and a psychiatrist tes- lawyer implies capacity to do so rather than desire
tified Dusky was “unable to properly understand to do so. It is not unusual for criminal defendants
the proceedings against him and unable to ade- not to want to cooperate with counsel for reasons
quately assist counsel in his defense” (p. 402) because not rooted in mental illness. The ability to identify
of severe mental illness. He was found competent, the motivation for this lack of cooperation is the
nonetheless, because he was oriented and had some task of the forensic evaluator. Further, it is impor-
recollection of the events in question. He was then tant to keep in mind “ability to assist” counsel
convicted of kidnapping. The U.S. Court of Appeals does not imply a constitutional right to a “mean-
for the 8th Circuit affirmed the conviction. ingful attorney–client relationship” (Morris v.
The U.S. Supreme Court reviewed the Slappy, 1983).
case, overturned the conviction, and remanded Last, the standard also includes the phrase
it back to the trial court for new competency reasonable level of understanding, rather than a
assessment, indicating the level of competency perfect level of understanding; a criminal defen-
was not adequate. They wrote, “It is not enough dant is not expected to have perfect understand-
for the district judge to find that the defendant ing. Although these small components of the
is oriented to time and place and has some Dusky standard are important, the core issue of
recollection of events” (p. 402). Dusky was to concern remains the nature of “rational as well as
receive a new trial if he was found competent factual understanding.”
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 447

According to Reisner and Slobogin (1990), Competency as a Contextual Issue


factual understanding comprises a person’s Grisso (1988) presented a conceptual framework
strict understanding. Examples include a defen- that emphasized the contextual nature of criminal
dant’s ability to repeat information provided, competency. From this perspective, the likely
paraphrasing that information in his or her own demands placed on a defendant must be consid-
words, and displaying an ability to put the infor- ered before determining whether the defendant
mation to use. It can be evaluated by observing can rise to the level of performance required.
how information is used in decision making and Although subsequent U.S. Supreme Court
can include such abilities as judgment, compre- case law (Godinez v. Moran, 1993) implied
hension, good reality testing, rational weighing of otherwise, it is still a valuable exercise in under-
risks and benefits, and relevance of facts to the standing the nature of the interaction between
immediate situation. Although there are various the defendant’s capabilities and the likely situa-
descriptions of specific points within this concept tional demands. As I will address later in the
of competency for various activities, the general chapter, the concern is particularly important for
understanding of competency as outlined in instances in which otherwise competent
Dusky is the core aspect of competency for any defendants may not be competent to represent
point in the criminal judicial process. themselves (Indiana v. Edwards, 2008). Viewing
One issue Dusky does not explicitly address is competency as an adaptive function in this
the presence of mental abnormality beyond lack manner makes the process of determining what
of knowledge. Milton Dusky was considered to neurocognitive deficits likely play a role in the
have a severe mental illness, so the issue was in defendant’s lack of competence and provides
the record before the U.S. Supreme Court guidance in determining prognosis and treatment
(Frederick, DeMier, & Towers, 2004). It was needs. Grisso presented five areas of analysis
simply not listed in the final Court decision.1 relevant for neuropsychologists performing
Subsequent case law makes it clear the compe- competency evaluations: functional description
tency deficiency must be caused by mental abnor- of specific abilities, causal explanations for deficits
mality as opposed simply to ignorance. Individuals in competency abilities, interactive significance
can be unable to assist in their defense for purely of deficits in competency ability, conclusory
physical reasons, but as it relates to cognitive opinions about legal competency and incompe-
functioning, some type of diagnosable mental tency, and prescriptive remediation for deficits in
condition is required. Most jurisdictions make competency abilities.
this requirement much more clear. An example is
the current federal statute (Title 18, U.S.C., Section Functional Description of Specific Abilities
4241a). The statute is very reminiscent of Dusky, The primary objective of a competency evaluation
with added emphasis on mental abnormality: is not foreign to clinical neuropsychology because
“The defendant, as a result of mental disease or it deals with describing functional strengths and
defect, is unable to understand the nature and weaknesses. The difference is in knowing specific
consequences of the proceedings against him or legal standards sufficiently to appreciate the rele-
assist properly in his defense.” vance of those deficits to the determination of
The current federal standard also highlights competency.
another aspect of competency by replacing
“consult” with “assist” regarding working with Causal Explanations for Deficits in
counsel. There is no definitive rule as to how much Competency Abilities
ability one needs to assist “properly.” The amount The logical next step is determining the cause of
of assistance necessary to give direction to those deficiencies, another task well known by
counsel or rally a defense on one’s own behalf neuropsychologists. The key here is, again, know-
likely varies depending on the complexity of the ing the appropriate legal standard and how that
case. In this regard, the standard for competency standard is applied. Neuropsychologists are
to proceed implicates the need to evaluate the equipped to communicate neuroanatomical and
context and situational demands into which the neuropathological bases for deficits presented and
defendant will likely need to go. Grisso (1986, to rule out other potential causes of performance
1988) provided insight into the need to consider (ignorance, situational influences, cultural
contextual issues in determining competence to influences, and malingering). Lack of knowledge
proceed. pertaining to the law (ignorance) is not grounds
448 forensic neuropsychology

for incompetency. The knowledge and reasoning conclusions, particularly in those areas most
skill deficits must be rooted in diagnosable relevant to competency, and allow the trier of fact
pathology. to make the final legal decision. The final legal
decision before the court is considered the
Interactive Significance of Deficits in “ultimate issue.” In competency hearings, the
Competency Ability ultimate issue is whether the defendant is compe-
The neuropsychologist should attempt to place tent to proceed. Providing an opinion on the
the defendant’s strengths and weaknesses into ultimate issue has been debated, particularly
context, that is, how the neurocognitive function when the expert is providing an opinion before a
interacts with the ecological demands required of jury. There is relatively pervasive case law and
him or her given the specific legal situation. statute limiting mental health experts from
Although case law suggests the general standard providing ultimate issue opinions on sanity, and
for competency may not change based on the in some jurisdictions, there are formal limits on
complexity of the case, the level of rational and providing ultimate issue opinions on competency
factual understanding required is not necessarily as well.
interpreted in the same manner for every situa- Grisso (1988) pointed out three reasons to
tion, particularly when dealing with the issue of a refrain from providing ultimate issue opinion tes-
defendant’s self-representation. timony in the area of competence. First, psycholo-
The demands required of the defendant gists are experts in mental health issues, not law.
will vary given the complexity of the case. In He argued they are not in a position to know
this regard, two defendants with the exact which demands will be placed on a defendant in
same neurocognitive deficits could vary in their the course of his or her legal actions. In this regard,
competence based on the complexity of the case. clinicians should leave the opinion up to those
More is required from a defendant in a long, who are experts in the judicial process. Second, an
multiple-count bank fraud trial than for a argument has been made that mental health
single charge of illegal reentry after deportation. experts can have too great an influence on the
Likewise, pleading guilty will press cognitive minds of legal decision makers. Although such
capacity less than a lengthy trial. Before conclud- may be the case in jury trials, it is difficult to
ing a defendant’s competency, there must fathom that a judge will be unduly swayed to an
be a sense of which demands will be placed opinion of even the best-qualified neuropsycholo-
on him or her through the particular legal gist. Third, Grisso suggested no new information
proceedings. This gray aspect of competency is provided to the judge by mental health profes-
determination is an excellent example of why it is sionals giving their opinions on the ultimate issue
important to remember the judge makes the final after a description of the neuro/psychopathology
decision regarding a defendant’s competency, not with its functional strengths and weaknesses is
the expert witness. made. In some jurisdictions, however, the expert
is required to provide an opinion on the ultimate
Conclusory Opinions About issue regarding competency (Title 18 U.S. Code,
Legal Competency and Sections 4241 and 4247). The debate on providing
Incompetency ultimate issue opinions on competency will likely
Evaluators provide opinions regarding the defen- continue in academic settings and scholarly writ-
dant’s competency. The trier of fact (in this ings whether or not it makes any real difference in
instance, the judge) will make the actual legal the courtroom.
finding regarding competence. The forensic
neuropsychologist’s role is simply to provide an Prescriptive Remediation for
expert opinion for the court’s consideration. Deficits in Competency Abilities
Judges consider other salient facts beyond that If the forensic neuropsychologist believes the
provided by the forensic neuropsychologist before defendant is not competent, it is his or her respon-
making a legal ruling. sibility to provide prognostic considerations and
Grisso (1986, 1988) and colleagues (2003) outline remedial options from a clinical perspec-
made the argument that mental health profes- tive, truly the neuropsychologist’s element. The
sionals should refrain from actually providing a clinician can educate the court regarding the
definitive opinion on competency. They suggested nature of the condition, which treatment options
instead that they limit their opinions to clinical are available, and the likely success potential for
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 449

each. Grisso (1988) pointed out these issues to consider the factors related to the issue of
consider: whether the defendant meets the criteria
for competence to proceed; that is,
• Whether the defendant’s deficits are whether the defendant has sufficient
remediable; present ability to consult with counsel with
a reasonable degree of rational
• If so, the treatment that is required for
understanding and whether the defendant
remediation;
has a rational, as well as factual,
• How long the remediation is likely to
understanding of the pending proceedings.
require;
• The local facilities or programs in which (3) In considering the issue of competence to
the treatment is available; and proceed, the examining experts shall first
• The conditions of restriction represented consider and specifically include in their
by each of these facilities. (p. 21) report the defendant’s capacity to:
1. Appreciate the charges or allegations
Depending on the nature of the legal case, against the defendant;
courts may have the option to place defendants in 2. Appreciate the range and nature of
community treatment or rehabilitation programs. possible penalties, if applicable, that
In many instances, the court has little option but may be imposed in the proceedings
to refer the defendant to forensic hospitals run by against the defendant;
state or federal correctional agencies. For exam-
3. Understand the adversarial nature of
ple, the federal law dictates that U.S. district judges
the legal process;
are to commit incompetent defendants to the
custody of the U.S. attorney general for inpatient 4. Disclose to counsel facts pertinent to
mental health treatment focused on competency the proceedings at issue;
restoration (Title 18, U.S.C., Section 4241d). 5. Manifest appropriate courtroom
A definite aspect of this commitment is an addi- behavior; and
tional assessment of competency to proceed. 6. Testify relevantly; and include in their
Customarily, mental health treatment facili- report any other factor deemed
ties under the department of corrections have relevant by the experts.
little in the way of neurocognitive remediation
(4) If the experts should find that the
capability. It is important to remember the goal of
defendant is incompetent to proceed, the
treatment is remediation of deficits sufficient to
experts shall report on any recommended
restore competency. This level of therapeutic
treatment for the defendant to attain
outcome is likely lower than that espoused in
competence to proceed. In considering the
general clinical rehabilitation. The ultimate goal is
issues relating to treatment, the examining
the ability to advance successfully through legal
experts shall specifically report on:
proceedings rather than successful independent
living and community reentry. 1. The mental illness causing the
incompetence;
More Detailed Considerations 2. The treatment or treatments
of Competency appropriate for the mental illness of
Others have attempted to define further the the defendant and an explanation of
functional capacities that go into competency as each of the possible treatment
formulated in Dusky. A previously cited (Grisso, alternatives in order of choices;
1986; Melton et al., 2007) example is Florida’s 3. The availability of acceptable
2002 Title XLVII (Criminal Procedure and treatment and, if treatment is available
Corrections), Chapter 916.12 (Mentally Deficient in the community, the expert shall so
and Mentally Ill Defendants), which spells out the state in the report; and
standard of incompetency in this manner, starting 4. The likelihood of the defendant’s
with paragraph (2): attaining competence under the
treatment recommended, an
(2) The experts shall first determine whether assessment of the probable duration of
the person is mentally ill and, if so, the treatment required to restore
450 forensic neuropsychology

competence, and the probability that elements of competency presented by judges in


the defendant will attain competence case law. Wieter v. Settle was an interesting case in
to proceed in the foreseeable future. the U.S. District Court for the Western District of
Missouri in 1961. Although Wieter v. Settle is not
There have been clinician/researchers involved a jurisdictionally authoritative decision, meaning
in attempting to operationalize Dusky and pro- it does not have much power to control other
vide evaluators with specific functional abilities. judicial decisions, it has been presented widely to
The Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry better enable mental health professionals to
(1974) developed a 21-point list of abilities from understand the very basic aspects of “reasonable
competency assessment instruments available at understanding” as it relates to mental health com-
the time (Table 16.3). These abilities can assist the petency (Denney & Wynkoop, 2000; Grisso, 1988;
evaluator by providing an outline and road map Marcopulos et al., 2008; Stafford, 2003).
for clinical interviewing. Failure in any one or Wieter was arrested and charged with a fed-
more points does not necessarily constitute eral misdemeanor offense after he claimed to
incompetency, but the points help ensure evalua- have placed a bomb on a commercial airliner trav-
tors identify and consider all relevant issues eling from Seattle, Washington, to Los Angeles,
before coming to conclusions. California. He was found not competent to stand
In contrast to the detail that came out of crim- trial and committed for inpatient mental health
inal competency research at the hands of behav- treatment. After 18 months, he filed a habeas
ioral scientists, is the emphasis on the most basic corpus motion seeking relief from mental health

TABLE 16.3 GROUP FOR THE ADVANCEMENT OF PSYCHIATRY 21ITEM


LIST TO ASSIST IN THE EVALUATION OF COMPETENCY

1. Understand current legal situation.


2. Understand current charges.
3. Understand facts relevant to the case.
4. Understand the legal issues and procedures in the case.
5. Understand legal defenses available in the defendant’s behalf.
6. Understand the dispositions, pleas, and penalties possible.
7. Appraise the likely outcomes.
8. Appraise the roles of defense counsel, prosecuting attorney, judge, jury, witnesses, and defendant.
9. Identify and locate witnesses.
10. Relate to defense counsel.
11. Trust and communicate relevantly with counsel.
12. Comprehend instructions and advice.
13. Make decisions after receiving advice.
14. Maintain a collaborative relationship with counsel and help plan legal strategy.
15. Follow testimony for contradictions or errors.
16. Testify relevantly and be cross-examined if necessary.
17. Challenge prosecution witnesses.
18. Tolerate stress at the trial and while awaiting trial.
19. Refrain from irrational and unmanageable behavior during trial.
20. Disclose pertinent facts surrounding the alleged offense.
21. Protect himself by using available legal safeguards.

Reproduced from Misuse of Psychiatry in the Criminal Courts: Competency to Stand Trial (Group for the Advancement of
Psychiatry, Report 89, 1974), Formulated by the Committee on Psychiatry and Law. Reproduced with permission.
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 451

commitment and release because he already Competency to Confess


spent more time in custody than he would have The 5th Amendment of the U.S. Constitution
if convicted of the misdemeanor. guarantees the right to be free from self-incrimi-
A psychiatrist filed a report indicating Wieter nation, and the 6th Amendment guarantees the
was still not competent because of mental illness. right to counsel. The U.S. Supreme Court affirmed
The U.S. District Court reheard the case and dis- these rights in Miranda v. Arizona (1966). As a
agreed with the psychiatric opinion. In considering result of this famous case, the concept has become
the man competent, the court outlined eight mini- known as the “right to remain silent.” If the sus-
mal abilities required for competency to proceed pect waives the right to silence and input from
(Table 16.4). Subsequent case law substantially counsel, any statements given can be used against
softened the requirement for memory of the events him or her. The Court spelled out the need for any
such that a competent defendant does not necessar- statements given by the suspect to be voluntary,
ily need to recall details of the alleged offense intelligent, and knowing for them to be admissible
(Wilson v. United States, 1968). This issue is in later criminal proceedings.
addressed further under competency and amnesia. Although Miranda appeared to limit law
enforcement deception, later court cases have
Competency as Decisional Capacity outlined that:
Much of the discussion around Dusky has to do
not only with knowledge, but also with the ability As long as the person subjected to interroga-
to go through a process. Implied within the tion appears to understand the right to remain
concept of competency to proceed is decisional silent and the right to counsel subsequent
capacity, that is, the idea of capacity to make waiver of those rights will usually be “knowing
important legal decisions. Contrasted with deci- and intelligent”; other types of misunderstand-
sional capacity is procedural capacity, by which a ings or misimpressions are not relevant to the
person can understand the nature and conse- admissibility issue. (Melton et al., 2007, p. 169)
quences of proceedings. Criminal defendants
make many decisions in the process of assisting in Greater emphasis has been placed on the term
their defense, but there are some decisions that “voluntary” in subsequent cases as much of the
have unique standing because they involve waiv- concern has revolved around what constitutes
ing rights guaranteed under the U.S. Constitution. “coercion.”
Three examples are the right to remain silent, right Coercion was addressed in light of mental
to counsel, and right to trial. I address each of health concerns by the U.S. Supreme Court in
these in turn. Colorado v. Connelly (1986). Barry Francis

TABLE 16.4 MINIMAL ABILIT Y REQUIREMENTS FOR CRIMINAL COMPETENCY


AS OUTLINED BY THE U.S. DISTRICT COURT IN WIETER V. SETTLE  1961 .
ADAPTED FOR CLARIT Y.

1. Mental capacity to appreciate his/her presence in relation to time, place, and things.
2. Elementary mental processes such that he/she apprehends (i.e., seizes and grasps with what mind he/she has)
that he/she is in a Court of Justice, charged with a criminal offense.
3. Apprehends that there is a judge on the bench.
4. Apprehends that there is a prosecutor present who will try to convict him/her of a criminal charge.
5. Apprehends that he/she has a lawyer (self-employed or court-appointed) who will undertake to defend him/her
against that charge.
6. Apprehends that he/she will be expected to tell his/her lawyer the circumstances, to the best of his/her mental
ability, (whether colored or not by mental aberration) the facts surrounding him/her at the time and place where
the law violation allegedly occurred.
7. Apprehends that there is, or will be, a jury present to pass upon evidence adduced as to his/her guilt or
innocence of such charge.
8. He/she has memory sufficient to relate those things in his/her own personal manner.
452 forensic neuropsychology

Connelly flew from Boston to Denver to confess Court concluded that competency to waive the
to the Denver police about a murder committed right to an attorney is not based on a determina-
several months earlier in Colorado. Despite tion of the defendant’s level of legal knowledge.
repeated Miranda warnings that he need not talk, Even though it may be bad judgment, competent
he insisted on giving self-incriminating details of defendants can act as their own attorneys
the murder. Police observed no overt signs of regardless of how ignorant they are in the law and
mental illness. Later testimony presented that the criminal procedure.
defendant was “compelled” to confess by com- A discussion on the issue of waiving one’s right
mand hallucinations from God. to trial by pleading guilty needs to start with a
The U.S. Supreme Court ultimately opined non–mental health case. In 1970, the U.S. Supreme
that the “respondent’s perception of coercion Court decided North Carolina v. Alford. Alford
flowing from the ‘voice of God,’ however impor- chose to plead guilty in a capital murder case
tant or significant such a perception may be in simply to avoid the possibility of the death pen-
other disciplines, is a matter to which the United alty. In his statement pleading guilty, he asserted
States Constitution does not speak” (p. 524). There he did not commit the crime, but was pleading
needed to be police activity amounting to “over- guilty to avoid the death penalty. The U.S. Supreme
reaching” to find his confession not voluntary. Court reviewed the issue based on the question of
The Court confirmed that the waiver of Miranda whether Alford was coerced by the gravity of the
rights must be knowing, intelligent, and volun- situation into waiving his right to a trial. The
tary. Regardless of the Colorado v. Connelly Court summarized the issue that defendants must
decision’s emphasis on police overreaching, both voluntarily, knowingly, and understandingly waive
Miranda and Connelly suggest mental health a right to trial. They found Alford had made a
professionals have a place in evaluating the com- “voluntary and intelligent choice among alterna-
petency of criminal defendants to confess. tives;” his plea was not compelled merely by the
risk of execution.
Competency to Plead Guilty and The Alford decision set the stage for the 1973
Waive Counsel U.S. Court of Appeals for the 6th Circuit decision
Post-Dusky courts also struggled with the level of in Sieling v. Eyman. In this case, the 9th Circuit
competency required to waive the constitutional established what appeared to be a higher standard
right to counsel and trial. Both of these rights to waive rights to trial and plead guilty. The court
were addressed and the concepts brought together seemed to propose a standard of a “reasoned
in Godinez v. Moran (1993). However, in very choice among alternatives” in addition to the
recent U.S. Supreme Court law, the issue of com- general Dusky criteria. The U.S. Supreme Court
petency to waive counsel was revisited (Indiana v. encompassed each of these concerns and appeared
Edwards, 2008). I must first lay the groundwork to resolve the inconsistencies, for a time, with
for these two important cases and then spell out their reasoning in Godinez v. Moran (1993).
the current standard in these areas. Richard Moran was charged with murdering a
In 1966, the U.S. Supreme Court decided bartender and bar patron in August 1984. A few
Westbrook v. Arizona, which questioned whether days later, he allegedly murdered his wife and
a general competency hearing addressing Dusky attempted suicide by shooting himself in the
language was adequate when the defendant chose abdomen. He survived and was handed over to
to waive right to counsel. Westbrook was initially police in the hospital recovery room. He was
found competent to stand trial. He then dismissed found competent to stand trial and initially
his attorney and chose to act as his own attorney pleaded not guilty. Two and one half months later,
(termed acting pro se). The High Court eventually he requested that his defense attorney be dis-
reviewed the case and sent it back to the trial charged, and that he be allowed to plead guilty.
court, suggesting the need to determine whether The trial court accepted his waiver of counsel and
the defendant made an “intelligent and compe- trial once assured that he was not pleading guilty
tent” waiver of his constitutional right to counsel. in response to threats or promises, and that he
This finding suggested a higher standard of understood the charges and potential conse-
competency to act as one’s own attorney than that quences facing him.
outlined in Dusky. In 1985, Moran was sentenced to death. In
In a non–mental health, but related, case, 1987, he filed a petition for post-conviction relief
Faretta v. California (1975), the U.S. Supreme claiming he had not been mentally competent to
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 453

represent himself. The trial court, Nevada Supreme Ahmad Edwards is a man with schizophrenia
Court, and U.S. District Court rejected his appeal, who was caught in the act of trying to steal a pair
but the U.S. Court of Appeals for the 9th Circuit of shoes from a department store. While in the
reversed the prior decisions, holding the due store, he withdrew a handgun and fired it as
process clause of the 14th Amendment required security personnel were attempting to detain him,
the court to hold a competency inquiry before wounding a bystander. He was charged with
accepting his decision to discharge counsel and attempted murder, battery with a deadly weapon,
enter a guilty plea. Similar to the reasoning in criminal recklessness, and theft. His mental state
Sieling v. Eyman (1973), the Court required was in question during three competency hear-
a reasoned-choice-among-alternatives inquiry. ings and two self-representation requests. He was
The U.S. Supreme Court took up the question of initially found not competent and sent to a foren-
whether the competency standard for pleading sic hospital for competency restoration treatment.
guilty or waiving right to counsel was higher than After 7 months of treatment, the trial court found
the competency standard for standing trial. They him competent to assist his attorneys and stand
rejected the 9th Circuit’s use of reasoned choice trial. Seven months later, defense counsel sought
and concluded: another competency hearing and presented
additional testimony from a psychiatrist and
We can conceive of no basis for demanding a neuropsychologist indicating he was not able to
higher level of competence for those defen- cooperate with his attorney because of delusions.
dants who choose to plead guilty . . . nor do we The court again found him not competent and
think that a defendant who waives his right to returned him to the state hospital. After 8 more
the assistance of counsel must be more compe- months of treatment, he was found competent
tent than a defendant who does not. (p. 332) again. Just before the trial, however, he asked the
court for permission represent himself (i.e., waive
Although the Court specifically indicated the his right to counsel) and for an additional delay to
Dusky standard was not increased, they made this the trial so he could prepare. The court refused
slight enhancement: both requests, and the jury convicted him of
criminal recklessness and theft. They could not
A finding that a defendant is competent to reach a verdict on the attempted murder and
stand trial, however, is not all that is necessary battery charges. Approximately 6 months later,
before he may be permitted to plead guilty or the state retried him on the murder and battery
waive his right to counsel. In addition to deter- charges. Again, he requested to represent himself.
mining that a defendant who seeks to plead Trial court cited his lengthy history of schizophre-
guilty or waive counsel is competent, a trial nia, concluded he was competent to stand trial,
court must satisfy itself that the waiver of his but did not find him competent to represent him-
constitutional rights is knowing and voluntary. self. Proceeding with assistance of counsel, he was
(p. 333) convicted on both counts.
Edwards took his case to the Indiana appeals
Godinez trumped the reasoning presented court arguing that the trial court deprived him of
in Westbrook and Sieling by establishing that com- his constitutional right to self–representation
petency to plead guilty or waive counsel requires based on Faretta. The appeals court overturned
no higher standard than competency to stand his conviction and ordered a new trial. The gov-
trial. However, the trial court must now specifi- ernment appealed it to the Indiana Supreme
cally place in the record an inquiry into the defen- Court, who agreed with the trial court in many
dant’s thought process to verify it is a “knowing” aspects but, because of Faretta and Godinez, chose
and “voluntary” decision. Godinez recapitulated to affirm the appeals court in over-turning the
Dusky and spelled out the minimum required conviction and requiring the trial court to let him
standard for decisional capacity; however, it left represent himself. At Indiana’s request, the U.S.
open the unusual possibility that a person could Supreme Court heard the case to consider the
be competent to stand trial while at the same time constitutionality of not allowing an otherwise
not competent to represent him or herself. This competent defendant to represent himself.
unusual consideration is exactly the case in the In considering the case, the U.S. Supreme
very recent U.S. Supreme Court case of Indiana v. Court pointed out that Faretta did not take into
Edwards (2008). consideration the presence of mental illness
454 forensic neuropsychology

because Faretta was not a mental-health-related remotely analogous to hypothesis testing. As with
case. Additionally, the Court noted that even the science, the court starts out with a default assump-
Faretta decision, as well as later cases, indicated tion. In the example of criminal conviction, defen-
the right to self-representation was not absolute. dants are assumed innocent until shown otherwise.
The court noted subtle, but important, distinc- Enough evidence must be demonstrated on the
tions between the Edwards case and Godinez. record to move the mind of the trier of fact (judge
Godinez wanted to represent himself in order to or jury) to a level “beyond reasonable doubt” to find
change his not guilty plea to a guilty plea. He did the defendant guilty of the crime. In this example,
not seek to represent himself in a trial. In that case the prosecution carries the burden of proof, and the
the U.S. Supreme Court only had to answer the standard of proof is beyond reasonable doubt.
question of his decisional competency, not his There are three major standards of proof for
procedural competency to represent himself. issues most relevant to neuropsychologists:
Additionally, in Godinez, the state sought to allow beyond reasonable doubt, clear and convincing,
Moran the right to represent himself. The issue is and preponderance of the evidence. Although
confusing because Moran had successfully quite artificial, behavioral scientists can use
obtained the right to waive counsel and plead percentages to communicate better the concept to
guilty. He only filed the appeal after he received a such legal terms. In this regard, beyond reason-
death sentence, claiming he had actually not been able doubt is most similar to 90% to 95% (e.g.,
competent to waive counsel and plead guilty in an p < .05), clear and convincing approximates 75%,
attempt to overturn his conviction. In Edwards, and preponderance amounts to just slightly better
the issue was whether the state could disallow than half (51%; Melton et al., 2007, p. 217). Each
the defendant to represent himself during trial of these standards is used during different inqui-
proceedings. ries in criminal procedure, and there are other
The U.S. Supreme Court concluded that standards used by appellate courts when review-
it could not maintain a single standard of compe- ing trial court records. I turn to two criminal
tency as it pertained to a defendant who is compe- competency cases to demonstrate the standard of
tent to proceed to trial with aid of counsel proof and who holds the burden of proof in com-
compared to proceeding without aid of counsel. petency determinations.
The court noted the fact that a mentally ill defen- In 1984, Teofilo Medina stole a gun, held up
dant can be competent to proceed with counsel four business establishments, murdered three
but not competent to proceed without counsel. employees, attempted to rob a fourth, and shot at
The court also concluded that allowing self-repre- two passersby. He was apprehended, tried, and
sentation under such unique circumstances does convicted of three counts of first-degree murder.
not affirm the dignity of the defendant. Last, the Before the trial, defense moved for a hearing on
court noted the importance that courts must not Medina’s competency to stand trial. He was found
only ensure a fair trial, but they must also ensure competent, although there was conflicting
that the trial appears fair to those observing the expert testimony. After the guilty verdict, Medina
proceedings. It is not a long stretch to imagine reinstated a previously withdrawn plea of not
the justices reviewing in their minds the debacle guilty by reason of insanity. The jury found him
presented when Colin Ferguson represented sane, and he was sentenced to death.
himself during the televised New York trial On appeal, it was argued his constitutional
(Bardwell & Arrigo, 2002). In Edwards, the U.S. right to due process was violated by placing the
Supreme Court separated the issue of competency burden of proof on him to establish his incompe-
to represent oneself during trial proceedings from tence to stand trial. In 1992, the U.S. Supreme
the issues of competency to waive counsel and Court heard the case and held (a) the California
plead guilty as outlined in Godinez. Evaluators statute requiring that the party asserting incom-
must now consider this specific question when petency of the defendant to stand trial had the
formulating their opinions regarding a criminal burden of proving incompetence did not violate
defendant’s competence to stand trial procedural due process rights of the defendant,
and (b) the statute providing for the presumption
Burden and Standard of Proof of competency did not violate procedural due
It is helpful for neuropsychologists to understand process.
how courts make decisions. Evidence is compared Medina v. California (1992) not only revealed
to sometimes vague legal standards in a manner it is constitutional to place the incompetency
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 455

burden on the defendant, but also revealed that and convincing standard, the state runs too great
the burden is on whoever asserts that the defen- a risk of finding an incompetent defendant
dant is not competent because defendants are competent to proceed and thereby violating his or
presumed competent to stand trial. The table her due process rights under the 14th Amendment.
turns after the court has found a defendant not Although Medina provided that it is constitution-
competent and commits him or her to mental ally sound to place the burden on defendants,
health treatment for restoration of competency. Cooper dictated that the standard of proof in
At the end of treatment, the burden is now on determining incompetency can be no more strict
whoever asserts the defendant is competent to than preponderance of the evidence, that is, just
proceed because it is assumed the defendant enough to tip the scales.
remains not competent. The issue of who
carries the burden of proof in competency deter- Competency to Refuse an
minations is not particularly critical given the Insanity Defense
particular standard of proof involved. The impor- As demonstrated in Faretta v. California (1975),
tance of this standard of proof was addressed competent defendants have the right to make
by the U.S. Supreme Court in Cooper v. Oklahoma decisions that may not be in their best interests or
(1996). are otherwise demonstrative of poor judgment.
In Oklahoma at the time Byron Cooper was Occasionally, it is difficult to tell whether such
charged with murdering an 86-year-old man decisions reflect poor judgment. Such is the case
during the course of a burglary, defendants in when a mentally ill (but competent) defendant
criminal prosecutions were assumed competent has a very good case for a successful insanity
to stand trial until shown otherwise by clear and defense, but he or she chooses to forgo that
convincing evidence. Questions regarding defense. Judge Bazelon of the Court of Appeals for
Cooper’s competence were raised on five separate the District of Columbia wrote these words about
occasions before and after the trial. Initially, society’s responsibility in this regard (Whalem v.
a pretrial judge relied on a psychologist’s opinion United States, 1965):
that Cooper was not competent and committed
him to a state hospital for treatment. After 3 In the courtroom confrontations between the
months of treatment, however, the trial court individual and society the trial judge must
found him competent to proceed. At this point, he uphold this structural foundation by refusing
was presumed competent until shown by clear to allow the conviction of an obviously men-
and convincing evidence not competent. One tally irresponsible defendant, and when there is
week before trial, the lead defense attorney sufficient question as to a defendant’s mental
raised the issue of competence, but the judge responsibility at the time of the crime, that issue
held to his earlier determination that Cooper was must become part of the case. (pp. 818–819)
competent.
On the first day of trial, his bizarre behavior In Whalem (1965), the appeals court held that
led to a further competency hearing. Additional the trial court could impose an insanity defense on
expert testimony was heard suggesting Cooper a competent defendant even against the defendant’s
was incompetent, but there was concern about desires. That reasoning was reviewed after the
malingering. The judge held to his earlier deter- North Carolina v. Alford (1970) and Faretta (1975)
mination and pushed on with the trial. Cooper decisions in another District of Columbia Circuit
was found guilty. Before sentencing, defense Court case, Frendak v. United States (1979).
moved for a mistrial and another competency Paula Frendak’s coworker, Willard Titlow, was
hearing, but the motions were denied. Cooper fatally shot on the first floor of their office build-
was sentenced to death. The Oklahoma Court of ing. Frendak traveled out of Washington, D.C., on
Criminal Appeals affirmed the decision. a multistate, multicountry trip before she was
The Court of Appeals ruling was overturned apprehended in Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates,
by a unanimous decision of the U.S. Supreme for not surrendering her passport. She was found
Court. It was concluded Oklahoma’s clear and to possess a pistol and 45 rounds of ammunition.
convincing evidence rule regarding proof of She was extradited back to the United States and
incompetence “offends a principle of justice that is charged with first-degree murder. She underwent
deeply rooted in the traditions and conscience of multiple competency evaluations and was ulti-
our people” (p. 362). By holding to a clear mately found competent. Frendak was convicted.
456 forensic neuropsychology

The trial court then overrode her conviction evaluations, such tools aid in clinical determina-
and found her not guilty by reason of insanity. tion, but do not replace a thorough integration
She appealed partly to reject the insanity plea. The of all relevant information as part of a multiple
District of Columbia Court of Appeals held that data source assessment. Several competency
the trial judge may not force an insanity defense instruments have limited utility at this point and
on a defendant found competent to stand trial if receive only a brief mention here. I then review
the defendant intelligently and voluntarily decided the Competency Assessment Instrument (CAI,
to reject the insanity defense. There were several and its revised version R-CAI), Fitness Interview
potentially intelligent rationales for rejecting an Test–Revised (FIT-R), Georgia Court Compete-
insanity defense (Frendak v. United States, 1979): ncy Test (GCCT, and its revised version GCCT-
MSH), Competency Assessment for Standing
1) A defendant may fear that an insanity Trial for Defendants With Mental Retardation
acquittal will result in the institution of (CAST*MR), MacArthur Competency Assess-
commitment proceedings which lead to ment Tool–Criminal Adjudication (MacCAT-CA),
confinement in a mental institution for a and the Evaluation of Competency to Stand
period longer than the potential jail Trial–Revised (ECST-R).
sentence. The Competency Screening Test (CST;
2) The defendant may object to quality of Laboratory of Community Psychiatry, 1973;
treatment or the type of confinement to Lipsitt, Lelos, & McGarry, 1971) was developed as
which he or she may be subject in an a brief screening test to aid in deciding who
institution for the mentally ill. needed more thorough assessment regarding
competency to proceed. It was devised by a group
3) A defendant, with good reason, may
of psychiatrists, psychologists, and lawyers with a
choose to avoid the stigma of insanity.
National Institute of Mental Health grant from
4) Other collateral consequences of an the Center for Studies of Crime and Delinquency.
insanity acquittal can also follow the It is a 22-item sentence-completion test. Each of
defendant throughout life (e.g., the right the items is scored 0, 1, or 2 and summed for a
to vote, serve on a federal jury, or even total score. Total scores below 21 suggest further
restrict his or her ability to obtain a competency assessment is warranted. There has
drivers license). been criticism of the CST on conceptual, psycho-
5) A defendant also may oppose the metric, and predictive utility grounds. See Grisso
imposition of an insanity defense because and colleagues (2003) for a more thorough review.
he or she views the crime as a political or The CST may have value as a general screener for
religious protest which a finding of agencies, but it appears to have limited utility
insanity would denigrate (pp. 376–378). for neuropsychologists performing competency
evaluations.
It is important for a forensic examiner to be The Interdisciplinary Fitness Interview
aware of these potential reasons for wanting to (Golding, Roesch, & Schreiber, 1984) was devel-
avoid an insanity defense because the rationale for oped as a research tool for Canadian jurisdictions.
choosing a defense goes to competency by way of The semistructured interview format includes
“assisting properly” in one’s defense. The evalua- items on psychopathology as well as legal issues.
tor will need to assess the impact of any mental The unique aspect of the procedure includes a
illness on the defendant’s ability to make an intel- joint interview of an attorney with a mental health
ligent and voluntary judgment in this regard. professional. No additional research has come out
on the Interdisciplinary Fitness Interview since
Competency Assessment Tools Grisso’s detailed review of it in 1986.
There are a number of competency assessment The Computer Assisted Determination of
tools available to assist in the assessment of com- Competency to Proceed (Barnard et al., 1991,
petency to proceed. Some of these tools are simply 1992) is a computer-administered, 272-item,
interview outlines; others are structured inter- 18-scale test that addresses social history, psycho-
views and more formalized tests. They all have logical functioning, and legal knowledge. Items
one characteristic in common, however: They do include yes/no, true/false, and multiple choice
not determine who is or is not competent to pro- formats. It takes over an hour to administer,
ceed. As with all competent neuropsychological and the computer produces a narrative report
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 457

summary. It was designed to provide organized The instrument is more commonly used as an
and relevant information directly from the defen- interview outline and guide to guarantee impor-
dant for the clinician to review prior to a face-to- tant areas are not overlooked during the compe-
face interview. Preliminary research of the tency interview process (Denney & Wynkoop,
instrument seemed positive, but there were sig- 2000; Stafford, 2003).
nificant concerns about its initial validation. There
has been no published research of the instrument The Fitness Interview
since 1992, and there is no published manual Test–Revised
(Grisso et al., 2003). Melton and colleagues (2007) The Fitness Interview Test (FIT; Roesch, Webster,
discuss the test only briefly and recommend & Eaves, 1984) was a Canadian adaptation of the
against its use clinically. CAI. It included additional items regarding trial
procedures and a separate section addressing gen-
Competency Assessment Instrument eral defendant mental status. An extensive revi-
The CAI (Laboratory of Community Psychiatry, sion occurred secondary to Canada’s 1992 revision
1973; McGarry, Lelos, & Lipsitt, 1973) was devel- of the Criminal Code (Roesch, Zapf, Eaves, &
oped alongside the CST. It is a semistructured Webster, 1998). Changes in the FIT were substan-
interview that takes about 45 minutes to adminis- tial (Grisso et al., 2003). The FIT-R retained the
ter. It was originally developed with a scoring structured interview format but became more
system based on 5-point Likert ratings of 13 consistent with United States law. It has now been
competency-related functions. The functional published in the United States by Professional
domains were derived from appellate cases, legal Resource Press as a manual with a CD-ROM
literature, and experience of the multidisciplinary which includes the FIT-R interview for unlimited
development team (Stafford, 2003). There are no printing as well as a number of case law references
standardized administration rules. Although and research citations (Roesch, Zapf, & Eaves,
clinicians are free to formulate their own ques- 2006). It starts with four introductory questions
tions to address each knowledge domain, example (such as “Do you have a lawyer at this point?”)
questions are provided for each. then moves on to 70 questions grouped in 16
The original CAI manual did not specify the headings under the following three major sections
order in which the functions should be addressed, (which correspond to the Criminal Code of
and many of the questions required only yes or no Canada, Section 2):
answers. It was revised by forensic practitioners
working in the Trial Competency Program at Section I: Understanding the Nature or
Atascadero State Hospital in California on three Object of the Proceedings: Factual
different occasions over the course of 20 years Knowledge of Criminal Procedure
(Riley, 1998). The order of questions was changed
to flow more logically during a competency inter- 1. Understanding of Arrest Process
view. Questions were also added and reworded in 2. Understanding of the Nature and
a more open-ended format. An additional domain Severity of Current Charges
was added to deal with capacity to cope with stress 3. Understanding of the Role of Key
of incarceration prior to trial. Riley stressed Participants
“probing of an area should continue until the
4. Understanding of the Legal Process
examiner is satisfied that as much as is reasonably
possible is known about [the domain]” (p. 14). He 5. Understanding of Pleas
further pointed out it is not always necessary to 6. Understanding of Court Procedure
ask every question under each dimension as some
questions are not appropriate given the charge Section II: Understanding the Possible
(e.g., misdemeanor) or current stage of legal Consequences of the Proceedings:
proceedings (e.g., questions about trial when the Appreciation of Personal Involvement in and
defendant is facing sentencing). Importance of the Proceedings
Administration of the CAI remains flexible.
7. Appreciation of the Range and Nature
A portion of the Revised CAI is shown in
of Possible Penalties
Table 16.5.2 Although the CAI and R-CAI use a
scoring system with cutoffs for incompetency, the 8. Appraisal of Available Legal Defenses
scoring system is generally used solely for research. 9. Appraisal of Likely Outcome
458 forensic neuropsychology

TABLE 16.5 PORTIONS OF THE REVISED CAI.

1. Understanding of Charge(s):
What are you charged with?
Is this a felony or misdemeanor?
Which is more serious?
What would anyone have to do to commit the crime of?
2. Appreciation of Penalties:
If you’re found guilty as charged, what are the possible sentences the Judge could give you?
Where would you serve such a sentence?
What does probation mean?
What are some of the conditions a person must follow on probation?
What happens if a person is found Not Guilty?
Where do they send people found Not Guilty by Reason of Insanity?
3. Appraisal of Available Defenses:
What pleas can a person enter in court?
What does Not Guilty mean?
Guilty?
Not Guilty by Reason of Insanity
4. Appraisal of Functions of Courtroom Participants:
In the courtroom during a trial, what is the job of
a. Public Defender?
b. District Attorney?
c. Judge
d. Jury
e. Defendant
f. Witnesses
5. Understanding the Court Procedures:
Do defendants always have to testify in their own cases?
If you do have to testify, will you have to tell everything that happened?
If you testify, who asks you questions first?
Then who can ask you questions?
What is the district attorney trying to do?
What is the difference between a court trial and a jury trial?

Note: Originally published by NIMH, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 77–103. Reproduced with permission of Paul
D. Lipsitt. Revised by John A. Riley, PhD, reproduced with permission

Section III: Communicate with Counsel: As with the CAI, responses to questions are rated
Ability to Participant in Defense 0, 1, or 2 for adequacy. Scores are summed to
achieve performance for each section as well as
10. Capacity to Communicate Facts to
overall total performance. There are no norms
Lawyer
available. Since it is presented as a 30-minute
11. Capacity to Relate to Lawyer screening tool, a poor performance during one of
12. Capacity to Plan Legal Strategy the three sections would suggest the need for
13. Capacity to Engage in Own Defense more in depth assessment.
Viljoen, Roesch, and Zapf (2002) completed a
14. Capacity to Challenge Prosecution
study regarding inter-rater reliability between
Witnesses
psychologists, physicians, nurses, and graduate
15. Capacity to Testify Relevantly students. The overall intraclass correlation
16. Capacity to Manage Courtroom coefficients based on the full sample of raters
Behavior (p. 27) regarding judgment of fitness were quite high,
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 459

falling from 0.94 to 1.00. Additional research Presentation scale. In their subsequent analysis,
demonstrated similarly good inter-rater correla- the Atypical Presentation scale demonstrated a
tions with adolescents (Viljoen, Vincent, & 90% classification rate for individuals simulating
Roesch, 2006). Viljoen and colleagues (2006) also psychosis.
demonstrated a three-factor model that corre- Although the GCCT-MSH has become
sponded roughly to the requirements of Dusky, a popular instrument throughout a variety of
that is, understanding/reasoning about charges jurisdictions (Grisso, 1986), it has been criticized
and legal proceedings, appreciation of the conse- for only representing the factual knowledge prong
quences of their charges and proceedings, and of the Dusky standard (Rogers et al., 2001). The
ability to communicate with counsel. There is no GCCT-MSH is a helpful tool that only takes about
research regarding the measures’ susceptibility to 20 minutes to administer, but clinicians need to
feigned incompetency, nor does it have items remember it remains a screening instrument
designed to detect feigned incompetence. The designed to augment more thorough competency
FIT-R is presented as only a screening tool, but it assessment.
appears to be a valid and useful measure to assist
neuropsychologists in the assessment of trial- Competence Assessment for Standing
related competency. Trial for Defendants with
Mental Retardation
Georgia Court Competency Test The CAST*MR3 was developed by Everington and
The Georgia Court Competency Test (GCCT) Luckasson (1992) to overcome difficulties inher-
was developed as an in-house competency screen- ent in using open-ended questions with mentally
ing tool at Central State Hospital, Milledgeville, retarded defendants. The first 40 items are three
Georgia, to assess competency to proceed in a alternative multiple-choice questions dealing with
relatively rapid manner (Wildman et al., 1980). basic legal concepts and knowledge that would
In its original form, it was comprised of 17 ques- help the defendant assist in his or her defense. The
tions designed to address knowledge of courtroom last ten items are open-ended questions dealing
and legal proceedings, current charges and possi- with the specific charges and situation of the
ble penalties, and relationship with an attorney. defendant. Each of these items is scored as 0, 0.5,
Johnson and Mullett (1988) modified the instru- or 1 point based on accuracy and detail. Results
ment and raised the number of items to 21 (now are totaled for each of the three sections (basic
referred to as the GCCT-MSH). Nicholson (1992, legal concepts, skills to assist defense, and under-
cited in Rogers, Grandjean, Tillbrook, Vitacco, & standing case events) as well as a total score.
Sewell, 2001) reviewed available reliability studies The procedure takes about 30–40 minutes to
of the GCCT and concluded the test has moder- administer. Results are then compared with
ately high test–retest reliability, high α coefficients, performances (mean and standard deviation) of
and excellent interscorer reliability. four groups of criminal defendants: not mentally
Early research suggested the GCCT had a retarded (MR); MR but not referred for
stable three-factor structure, termed “general legal evaluation; MR, referred for pre-trial evaluation
knowledge,” “courtroom layout,” and “specific and considered competent; and MR, referred
legal knowledge” (Bagby, Nicholson, Rogers, & for pretrial evaluation and considered not
Nussbaum, 1992; Nicholson, Briggs, & Robertson, competent.
1988). Rogers, Ustad, Sewell, and Reinhart (1996) Everington (1990) outlined the development
and Ustad, Rogers, Sewell, and Guarnaccia (1996) of the CAST*MR. Cronbach’s α and Kuder-
failed to demonstrate this factor structure with Richardson 20 coefficients ranged from 0.93 to
confirmatory factor analysis, but exploratory 0.85 for three groups of older adolescents and
factor analysis with mentally ill jail and forensic adults with MR from two states. The 2-week,
hospital samples suggested two factors related to test–retest Pearson r’s were 0.90 and 0.89.
“legal knowledge.” Research revealed classification Subsequent studies using criminal defendants
rates of 68% to 78% on the GCCT (Wildman et al., from Wisconsin, Ohio, Maryland, and New
1980) and 81.8% on the GCCT-MSH (Nicholson, Mexico were completed. Ten checks of inter-rater
Robertson, Johnson, & Jensen, 1988). reliability were done, which resulted in 100%
Gothard, Rogers, and Sewell (1995) demon- agreement for the multiple choice items and 80%
strated the instrument’s susceptibility to malin- agreement for open-ended questions. Inter-rater
gering and added an eight-item Atypical agreement improved to 86.6% with a larger
460 forensic neuropsychology

number of comparisons (Everington & Dunn, The other items require the subject to choose
1995). Competent/not-competent classification between two alternative pleading options.
agreement rates with independent forensic Scoring focuses on the reasoning behind the
examiners varied from 62.9% to 71.43%, similar choice. The Appreciation items rely on the test
to hit rates of the GCCT and GCCT-MSH. subject’s actual legal situation rather than the
The CAST*MR appears to be an easy test to hypothetical vignette. The subject must compare
feign, simply by choosing incorrect answers on his or her situation to that of others and decide
items from sections I and II and then claiming whether he or she is “more likely,” “less likely,”
ignorance during section III. Everington, Notario- or “just as likely” to have various outcomes.
Smull, and Horton (2007) demonstrated this fact Summary scores are obtained for each section
in a simulation design study. They incorporated (Understanding, Reasoning, and Appreciation).
mildly mentally retarded (MR) individuals and The MacCAT-CA takes about 45 to 60 minutes to
mentally typical individuals, all of whom had administer.
been involved in the criminal justice system. They Inter-rater reliability was determined with 48
incorporated sections I and II only. Half of each protocols from the norming sample (Otto et al.,
group was randomly assigned to feign ignorance 1998; Poythress et al., 1999). Scale score intraclass
and pretend they were on death row seeking to correlations were 0.90 (Understanding), 0.85
save their lives, and the other half of each group (Reasoning), and 0.75 (Appreciation). Rogers
was asked to perform their best. MR feigners (2001) pointed out these scores were likely inflated
performed significantly worse than genuine because of hierarchical questions that cue raters.
MR performance; however, there was no signifi- Nevertheless, Rogers et al. (2001) reported results
cant difference between the MR feigners and from Rogers and Grandjean (2000) for 14 subjects
mentally typical feigners. The authors suggested that produced inter-rater correlations from 0.92
that performances falling within the random to 0.99. There are no published test–retest correla-
range on either section I or II should raise the tions. Cronbach’s αs were 0.85 (Understanding),
suspicion for feigning. Last, no literature 0.81 (Reasoning), and 0.88 (Appreciation) based
exists addressing how brain-injured individuals on the original studies (Otto et al., 1998).
perform on the CAST*MR. MacCAT-CA norms are based on 283 adjudi-
cated incompetent defendants, 249 mentally ill
MacArthur Competency Assessment defendants presumed competent, and 197
Tool–Criminal Adjudication randomly selected jail inmates also presumed
The MacCAT-CA (Poythress et al., 1999) is a competent. Subjects came from six states and
trimmed down, clinical version of the broader ranged in age from 18 to 65 years. Of the sample,
research instrument, MacArthur Structured 90% were male. Approximately half of the sample
Assessment of Competencies of Criminal was nonHispanic white defendants (Poythress
Defendants (MacSAC-CD; Otto et al., 1998). The et al., 1999). Percentile rank scores are provided
MacCAT-CA is a structured interview consisting for each of the scales in the MacCAT-CA manual.
of 22 items organized under three domains: There are tables in the back of the manual in order
Understanding, Reasoning, and Appreciation. to make comparisons between a defendant’s score
Four questions under Understanding and to the three normative groups on each of the three
Reasoning incorporate a brief vignette about two domains.
individuals fighting during a pool game in a bar. Predictive ability of the MacCAT-CA appears
One individual strikes the other with a pool reasonably strong and as good as the CST and
cue and is charged with a crime. The subject GCCT-MSH (Stafford, 2003). Each of the scale
taking the MacCAT-CA must assume the mindset scores was correlated with the a priori compe-
of the defendant in the vignette to answer tency classifications (Understanding, 0.36; Reas-
questions under Understanding and Reasoning oning, 0.42; Appreciation, 0.49). Performance of
sections. Several questions under Understanding incompetent groups was significantly lower than
incorporate brief training after initially inade- that of the presumed competent groups.
quate responses and follow up with additional Zapf, Skeem, and Golding (2005) completed
questioning. Items under Reasoning are of confirmatory factor analyses on the original
two types. The first requires the test subject to normative sample of 729 individuals. They con-
indicate which of two facts are more important sidered various theoretically driven models of
for the hypothetical defendant to tell his attorney. criminal competency and found a three-factor
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 461

structure best fit the data. The three-factor the instrument rather than an across-the-board
structure did not exactly fit the three aspects of use for all defendants. In my own experience
the MacCAT-CA (Understanding, Reasoning, & evaluating neurologically compromised defen-
Appreciation). Results suggested a hierarchical dants, I have found the MacCAT-CA to perform
structure that closely approximated the theoreti- well; however, I observed one defendant with a
cal underpinnings of the MacCAT-CA. All items significant frontal lobe injury who responded
from Understanding combined with items 14–16 quite concretely to the pool cue assault vignette.
from the Reasoning section comprised the first He kept thinking he was going to be charged
factor. The second factor included items 9–13 with that assault despite my repeated explana-
from Reasoning section, and all items from the tions. He appeared competent to proceed in all
Appreciation section comprised the third factor. other aspects of his performance. This case
They also completed step-wise regression analy- appears to be the exception as other frontal-
ses with MacCAT-CA subscales predicting lobe-injured defendants did not have this specific
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IQ and Brief trouble, in my experience.
Psychiatric Rating Scale total score. As compe-
tency status was a sizeable confound they used a Evaluation of Competency to
series of six regression analyses to yield path coef- Stand Trial–Revised
ficients. Results revealed a significant indepen- The ECST-R (Rogers, Tillbrook, & Sewell, 2004) is
dent positive relation between MacCAT-CA a structured interview composed of four main
scores and IQ and negative relation between sections with an additional set of “background”
MacCAT-CA performance and increased psy- questions (Rogers et al., 2001, p. 510). It was
chopathology. The authors concluded, “the developed to measure the two Dusky prongs,
MacCAT-CA appears to represent a psychome- understanding and ability to assist counsel, from
trically sound normative approach to measuring both factual as well as rational perspectives.
the generalized aspects of understanding, reason- The Consult With Counsel scale includes ten
ing, and appreciation—as well as an overarching questions to assess the attorney–client relation-
component—as applied to adjudicative compe- ship. The scale also incorporates five criteria:
tence” (p. 443). “(i) perceptions of the relationship, (ii) defendant’s
The manual includes scale cutoff scores as well expectations of the attorney, (iii) defendant’s
as tables that address sensitivity, specificity, false- understanding of the attorney’s expectations,
negative rate, and positive and negative predictive (iv) resolving disagreements, and (v) special
value for every score of each MacCAT-CA scale means of communicating with the attorney”
(Poythress et al., 1999). Grisso et al. (2003) high- (p. 510). The Factual Understanding scale includes
lighted the fact that, although the MacCAT-CA 15 questions regarding courtroom participants.
authors provided cutoffs, they discouraged users The Rational Understanding is comprised of ten
from taking these indicators as signs of compe- questions that measure the defendant’s ability to
tency or incompetency. The MacCAT-CA also make rational decisions. In addition, the ECST-R
does not include an index of subject response set, includes an Atypical Presentation Scale (ATP) to
and Rogers, Sewell, Grandjean, and Vitacco assist in identifying feigned incompetency. This
(2002) demonstrated the susceptibility of the scale has four independent subscales (Realistic,
MacCAT-CA to malingering. Psychotic, Nonpsychotic & Impaired) as well as
From a practical perspective, Pinals, Tillbrook, two composite scales, Both (comprised of
and Mumley (2006) discussed the instrument’s Psychotic and Nonpsychotic subscales) and BI
utility within an inpatient forensic hospital and (comprised of Psychotic, Nonpsychotic [Both
provided anecdotal accounts of its general effec- composite], and Impaired subscales). The Realistic
tiveness in assisting in competency assessment; scale serves only as filler items and is not consid-
however, they noted malingerers and those con- ered a malingering screen.
sidered not competent both occasionally provided Rogers et al. (2001) evaluated the internal and
“I don’t know” responses, and “defendants with inter-rater reliabilities of the ECST-R by combin-
neuropsychological impairments (particularly in ing data from previous studies. The α coefficients
frontal lobe functioning) had difficulty retaining for the combined data were 0.72 (Consult With
and manipulating information presented in the Counsel), 0.86 (Rational Understanding), 0.90
more complex items” (p. 186). They recommended (Factual Understanding), and 0.93 (total score).
a defendant-specific approach to choosing to use Inter-rater reliability correlations were 0.97
462 forensic neuropsychology

(Consult With Counsel), 0.99 (Rational weaknesses as they relate to issues of competency.
Understanding), 1.00 (Factual Understanding), It is possible a defendant may perform well on any
and 1.00 (total score). of these competency instruments and yet demon-
In this regard, performance of the ECST-R strate such severe cognitive deficits as to bring
was commensurate with that for the MacCAT-CA their competency into serious question. Likewise,
and somewhat better than for the GCCT-MSH. it may very well be possible for a defendant to
Principle axis factoring of the ECST-R revealed a perform poorly on a competency assessment tool
strong two-factor solution accounting for 43.5% for reasons indirectly related to competency.
of the variance. This solution produced 21 sub- None of these assessment tools have been
stantial and unique loadings with only two cross validated with brain-injured or neurological
loadings. The two factors corresponded to Factual populations. Anecdotally, I have seen brain-
Understanding and Rational Understanding injured defendants who have been found compe-
(Ability to Consult With Counsel fell under tent to proceed have trouble with taking the
Rational Understanding). perspective of the hypothetical defendant during
Vitacco, Rogers, Gabel, and Munizza (2007) the MacCAT-CA, where they have had little trou-
compared the relative effectiveness of the ECST-R ble with the ECST-R. In contrast, Pinals and
Atypical Presentation Scale, Miller Forensic colleagues noted the increased structure of the
Assessment of Symptoms Test (M-FAST; Miller, MacCAT-CA was helpful with certain individuals
2001), and Structured Inventory of Malingered with cognitive impairment. The increased
Symptomatology (SIMS; Widows & Smith, 2005) structure of all these structured interviews would
using the Structured Interview of Reported likely be helpful with most brain-injured defen-
Symptoms (SIRS; Rogers, Tillbrook, & Sewell, dants as long as the evaluator is mindful of
2004) as the criterion measure among 100 men potential confusion that could arise due to the
who were court ordered to undergo mental health MacCAT-CA vignette. One area of potential
assessment. All of the malingering subscales concern regarding the use of the ECST-R with
obtain effect sizes that were large to very large. neurocognitively impaired individuals is the focus
The Psychotic Scale and Both Scale were most of the scoring criteria. It seems to be focused more
effective at identifying positive SIRS results, on whether or not psychotic mentation is present
obtaining a sensitivity rate of 0.95 (missing only rather than lack of understanding or confusion
one presumed malingerer). Nonpsychotic and BI due to cognitive dysfunction. However, the exam-
scales demonstrated excellent sensitivity rates as iner is freer to include expanded queries and even
well (0.90 each). The poorest performer, paraphrasing compared to the MacCAT-CA,
Impairment, still demonstrated an impressive which ameliorates this concern to some degree.
0.81 sensitivity. As these are simply screening Regarding the CAST*MR, Stafford (2003)
measures, their respective specificity rates were noted, “This instrument contributes legally rele-
poor (ranging from 0.77 to 0.52). The respective vant data and norms to the assessment of men-
negative predictive power scores for the scales tally retarded or otherwise cognitively impaired
ranged from 0.94 to 0.98 based upon a base rate of defendants” (p. 369; emphasis added). Empiricism
21%. These results suggest the ATP scales were aside, the CAST*MR does allow comparisons
largely effective at identifying malingerers and between a brain-injured defendant’s performance
reasonably effective at ruling out malingering of and that of competent and incompetent individu-
exaggerated psychiatric presentation. als with mental retardation. This qualitative
comparison can be helpful in evaluating the com-
Applicability of Competency Assessment petence of brain-injured defendants, as results
Tools to Neuropsychological Evaluations can help validate perceptions of the defendant’s
As competency is a contextual issue, each of the basic level of knowledge.
competency assessment tool authors cautioned None of the competency assessment tools
against the use of strict cutoffs and recommended include measures of cognitive ability beyond legal
using their instruments to augment the compe- knowledge and decision-making capacity. The
tency evaluation by obtaining standardized CAST*MR and MacCAT-CA both correlate posi-
information about the defendant’s level of under- tively with IQ (Everington & Dunn, 1995;
standing and reasoning ability related to legal Poythress et al., 1999; Zapf et al., 2005). Little is
issues. It is up to the neuropsychologist to bring known regarding the impact neurocognitive
together each defendant’s cognitive strengths and deficits have on competency to proceed. Clearly,
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 463

there is a role for neurocognitive measures in memory (Logical Memory), and verbal and
establishing a criminal defendant’s cognitive nonverbal social intelligence (Comprehension and
strengths and weaknesses that can help in the Picture Arrangement), but not semantic memory
court’s decision. (Vocabulary and Information) or achievement.
Nestor, Daggett, Haycock, and Price (1999) See Table 16.6 for specific test scores.
retrospectively studied the neuropsychological Grandjean (2006) investigated neurocognitive
functioning of 181 defendants who underwent functioning among 55 defendants who had been
inpatient evaluations at Bridgewater State Hospital adjudicated incompetent and referred for inpatient
in Massachusetts for competency to stand trial. competency restoration treatment. She found com-
The sample consisted of 128 defendants consid- petent and incompetent defendants did not differ
ered competent to proceed and 53 defendants in regard to attention, visuospatial, or nonverbal
considered not competent to proceed. Subjects memory functioning. Competent defendants per-
were predominantly diagnosed with psychotic formed better than incompetent defendants on
and major mood disorders. There were no com- measures of verbal comprehension, verbal memory,
parisons of ethnic/racial differences, which sub- social judgment, and executive functions.
stantially limits the study. Also, sample selection Results from Nestor et al. and Grandjean
bias was a concern. Despite these shortcomings, suggest functional domains of intellect, attention,
results suggested competent defendants score verbal learning/memory and social judgment are
higher in areas of psychometric intelligence, important areas of focus. Logic in terms of
attention, memory (particularly verbal), episodic what appears important to properly assist in one’s

TABLE 16.6 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST SCORES FOR


COMPETENT AND NOT COMPETENT GROUPS.
Measure Competent Not Competent P Effect Size
Intelligence
WAIS-R FIQ 88.25 (15.09) 82.00 (14.44) <0.01 .054
WAIS-R VIQ 88.54 (15.28) 83.06 (15.24) <0.01 .046
WAIS-R PIQ 89.01 (15.50) 82.79 (14.07) <0.01 .043
Memory
WMS-R GMI 88.12 (19.83) 78.25 (18.62) <0.01 .061
WMS-R VerMI 89.26 (17.40) 80.17 (15.87) <0.01 .070
WMS-R VisMI 92.06 (17.69) 84.64 (20.01) <0.05 .039
Attention/Concentration
WMS-R A/CI 87.58 (19.45) 84.00 (21.09) NS
TMT-A sec. 36.70 (17.03) 55.00 (32.19) <0.01 .101
TMT-B sec. 121.29 (92.90) 146.93 (112.74) NS
Executive Function
WCST #Cat. 3.98 (2.06) 3.19 (2.06) NS
Academic Abilities
WRAT-R Read. 81.22 (19.41) 78.88 (23.30) NS
WRAT-R Spell. 77.31 (19.14) 76.71 (22.73) NS
WRAT-R Arith. 79.89 (18.48) 75.05 (22.05) NS

Note: Arith, arithmetic; A/CI, Attention/Concentration Index; Cat., Category Test; FIQ, full-scale IQ; GMI,
General Memory Index; PIQ, performance IQ; Read., reading; Sec., seconds; Pell., spelling; TMT-A, Trail
Making Test-A; TMT-B, Trail Making Test-B; VerMI, Verbal Memory Index; BIQ Verbal IQ; VisMI, Visual
Memory Index; WAIS-R, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test;
WMS-R, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; WRAT-R, Wide-Range Achievement Test-Revised. Adapted from
“Competence to stand trial: A neuropsychological inquiry,” by P.G. Nestor, D. Daggett, J. Haycock, & M. Price,
(1999), Law and Human Behavior, 23, pp. 397–412, with kind permission of Springer.
464 forensic neuropsychology

own defense, particularly when considering the a serious criminal matter that is related to his
potential impact of Edwards v. Indiana (2008) and or her memory disorder. Organic factors may
self-representation, suggest neuropsychological play a role when concussion, alcohol intoxica-
assessment of competency to stand trial should tion, or epileptic seizure occurs during a crime,
thoroughly measure a wide range of functional with subsequent limited amnesia for the crime
abilities. At a minimum, such an assessment itself, but in these cases memory problems typ-
should focus on intelligence, attention and con- ically do not exist prior to the crime. (p. 287)
centration, speed of mental processing, verbal
learning and memory, mental flexibility, social It appears the most common form of organic
reasoning, and decision-making capacity. More amnesia is limited to specific events and is sec-
research needs to occur on the nature and level ondary to alcohol or drug intoxication at the time
of cognitive ability needed for competency to of the alleged crime. Criminal defendants occa-
proceed, and particularly to represent one’s self sionally experience a neurological disease severe
without the assistance of counsel. enough to hinder recall of events around the time
Last, none of these competency instruments of the offense or experience a stroke or other neu-
have indices of cognitive validity. Only the GCCT- rological event after their arrest, but before trial,
MSH and ECST-R have associated validity indica- that will raise concern about their competency in
tors, and these are largely directed toward general and their recollection of allegedly crimi-
psychiatric disturbance. All of the competency nal events in particular (Wynkoop & Denney,
assessment tools have substantial face validity as 1999). It is possible for defendants to experience
they are predominantly structured interviews. the neurological trauma at the time of their arrest
They appear easy to fake by defendants simply (e.g., gunshot wounds, head trauma from motor
claiming ignorance or lack of cognitive ability. vehicle accidents). In these instances, it is not
Competency assessment tools are helpful adjunct unreasonable to suspect some loss of memory for
procedures, but competency evaluations of trau- events directly preceding arrest (retrograde amne-
matically brain-injured defendants require a sia), which can include the crime that occasioned
broader neurocognitive evaluation with a battery the arrest. A defendant’s ability to recall events
of tests that include measures of negative response constituting the alleged offense is an important
bias specific to neurocognitive functioning. issue and one that speaks to their ability to estab-
lish a reasonable defense against criminal prose-
Amnesia and Competency cution.
to Stand Trial Amnesia has the capacity to limit competency
Claimed amnesia for the alleged criminal activity to stand trial substantially. For example, the last
is not unusual (Schacter, 1986). Estimates range criteria put forth by the court in Wieter v. Settle
from 23% to 65% for homicide (Bradford & Smith, (1961, Table 4) includes “memory sufficient to
1979; Guttmacher, 1955; Leitch, 1948; Parwatikar, relate those things in his/her own personal
Holcomb, & Menninger, 1985). Of 100 cases of manner.” In 1968, however, the U.S. Court of
claimed amnesia, 90% pertained to murder or Appeals for the District of Columbia addressed
attempted murder (Hopwood & Snell, 1933). The the issue in an interesting manner in Wilson v.
rates of claimed amnesia appear much lower for United States. Defendant Wilson incurred a trau-
nonhomicide crimes. Taylor and Kopelman matic brain injury when his vehicle hit a tree at
(1984) found 8% claimed amnesia for 120 cases of the conclusion of a high-speed chase. He was
nonhomicide violent crimes, and there were no unconscious at the scene after having ‘fractured
claims of amnesia for 47 nonviolent crimes. his skull and ruptured several blood vessels in his
Relevant to legal issues, amnesia has been clas- brain’ (p. 461). He remained unconscious for 3
sified as organic or functional and chronic or lim- weeks. Subsequently, he denied recollection of his
ited. Schacter (1986) provided this summary: offenses (five counts of assault with a deadly
weapon and robbery). There were no observable
In the large majority of criminal cases that mental difficulties beyond his claimed memory
involve amnesia, the loss of memory either has loss. He was found competent and convicted. The
a functional origin or concerns only a single federal court of appeals concluded that amnesia,
critical event. I have found no cases in the lit- in and of itself, did not necessarily eliminate
erature in which a patient afflicted with chronic competency to stand trial. The court outlined six
organic amnesia has come before the courts on criteria for determining the impact amnesia has
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 465

TABLE 16.7 CRITERIA FOR IMPACT OF AMNESIA ON COMPETENCY

1. The extent to which the amnesia affected the defendant’s ability to consult with and assist his lawyer.
2. The extent to which the amnesia affected the defendant’s ability to testify in his own behalf.
3. The extent to which the evidence could be extrinsically reconstructed in view of the defendant’s amnesia. Such
evidence would include evidence relating to the crime itself as well as any reasonably possible alibi.
4. The extent to which the government assisted the defendant and his counsel in that reconstruction.
5. The strength of the prosecution’s case.
6. Any other facts and circumstances that would indicate whether or not the defendant had a fair trial.

Note: Issues set forth in Wilson v. United States (391 F.2d 460, 1968), pp. 463–464, to aid trial courts in determining whether or not amnesia for
the alleged offense substantially impacts the defendant’s competency to stand trial.

on competency (Table 16.7). Note most of these that Rinchack “display[ed] defects that would
criteria (3–6) relate to strictly investigative and suggest organic brain involvement, and . . . these
prosecutory issues. For example, the court made would undoubtedly interfere with his clarity of
this conclusion regarding the strength of the pros- thought, an would make it difficult for him to
ecution’s case (Wilson v. United States, 1968): properly assist . . . in his own defense when it
would be necessary to recall things, [and] associ-
Most important here will be whether the ate them properly with time and occurrences.”
Government’s case is such as to negate all rea- (p. 1561; quoting from the expert report)
sonable hypotheses of innocence. If there is He was committed to treatment at the U.S.
any substantial possibility that the accused Medical Center for Federal Prisoners for approxi-
could, but for his amnesia, establish an alibi or mately four months and returned to court for an
other defense, it should be presumed that he additional competency hearing. An expert from
would have been able to do so. (p. 464) the U.S. Medical Center concluded that “Rinchack
did not appear to be suffering from ‘a true organic-
While Wilson is a precedent-setting case, it type amnesia’ and that he was ‘able to remember a
has very limited jurisdictional authority. In my good deal more than he . . . claimed he could.’”
experience dealing with the issue of amnesia in He was found competent to proceed by the mag-
federal courts within different federal circuits, istrate judge and ultimately convicted during a
trial courts have often relied upon a more recent jury trial. He then appealed with three arguments,
United States Circuit Court of Appeals case, one of which was that his due process rights were
United States v. Rinchack (1987). violated by the court “forcing him to go to trial
In 1980, investigative information clearly con- while he was suffering from severe head trauma
nected Louis Rinchack to the smuggling of a large and amnesia.” The United States Court of Appeals
quantity of marijuana in to the United States from for the 11th Circuit rejected each of his conten-
Jamaica in a twin engine airplane. The Florida tions and affirmed the conviction.
state case was dismissed because the trial court The appeals court outlined factors for trial
suppressed most of the state’s evidence, but he was judges to consider that were similar to that spelled
indicted in federal court in 1985. During the out in Wilson (1968):
intervening time, Rinchack had been working on
a vessel in the Pacific Ocean until he was struck 1. The defendant’s ability to take the stand
on the head with a steel cable and rendered uncon- and testify and otherwise participate in his
scious for a day and a half. After his federal indict- defense;
ment, he claimed he was not competent to stand 2. Whether the amnesia is temporary or
trial due to the work accident “which left him with permanent;
memory loss, damage to his cerebellum and brain 3. Whether the crime and the defendant’s
stem, vertigo, dizziness, and seizures” (p. 1561). whereabouts can be properly
The trial court found him not competent, based reconstructed without the defendant’s
upon these conclusions: testimony, including any facts giving rise
Rinchack appeared to suffer from memory to a defense;
loss, confusion, blackouts and catatonic episodes 4. Whether access to government files would
as a result of the accident. The report also noted aid in preparing for trial;
466 forensic neuropsychology

5. The strength of the government’s case amnesia, also including the offense (Denney,
against the defendant. (p. 1569) 1996; Frederick, Carter, & Powel, 1995; Frederick
& Denney, 1998).
The appeals court concluded Rinchack’s rights Symptom validity testing for remote memory
had not violated after considering each of these entails creating, from investigative records (includ-
issues. Further, the court addressed the important ing eye witness accounts and prosecutory recon-
issue of potential alibis in this manner: structions) and information regarding defendant’s
Finally, while we can never know for sure what life history from collateral informants for those
Rinchack might be able to remember if he did not defendants claiming total retrograde amnesia,
suffer from amnesia, there does not appear to be two-alternative forced-choice questions based on
any real possibility the amnesia is “locking in” information about the alleged criminal activity.
exculpatory information. (p. 1570). Questions can also be developed to assess recollec-
In 2006, the 7th Circuit Court of Appeals, in a tion of well-known facts about courtroom partici-
case called United States v. Andrews, also addressed pants and the criminal law process as these areas
this issue, stood by the same standards, and noted can be considered a component of remote memory.
that its “sister” circuits, including the 11th Circuit, Questions are developed that include the correct
have agreed similarly in regard to such amnesia answers and foils that are as reasonably plausible
(Davis v. Wyrick, 1985; United States v. Swanson, as the correct alternatives. Developed questions
1978; United States v. Villegas, 1990). Clearly there are presented to the defendant orally. Only those
is no “per se” (meaning across the board) rule that questions for which the defendant claims no recol-
lack of memory for the events constituting the lection are kept in the analysis. Results are applied
offense makes a defendant not competent to stand to the binomial theorem to identify below-chance
trial. Each case must be considered based on its responding. Significantly worse-than-chance per-
unique merits. formance (i.e., more frequent choice of foil vs. cor-
Issues relating to the nature of legal cases, rect alternative) provides evidence the defendant
including the possibility of “exculpatory informa- actually knew the correct material but was inten-
tion,” are explicitly non–mental health. The nature tionally choosing the wrong answer to appear
of amnesia and extent to which it affects the defen- impaired. See Denney (1996) for a thorough
dant’s ability to consult with his or her lawyer and description of the process. Although time con-
testify in his or her own behalf, however, are clini- suming, the procedure has demonstrated consid-
cal issues. Neuropsychologists have the knowl- erable clinical utility (Denney, 1996; Wynkoop &
edge and tools to speak directly to these concerns. Denney, 1999) and a sound statistical basis
Given the issues outlined in the above cases, a (Frederick & Denney, 1998).
trial court may rule a defendant competent to In an interesting simulation design,
proceed even with legitimate amnesia. It should Merckelbach, Hauer, and Rassin (2002) had
be clear to the reader that most courts consider a undergraduate students perform a mock crime
defendant’s claim of amnesia quite seriously in and feign amnesia for the event afterward. The
terms of their potential competency to proceed. It researchers then constructed 15 two-alternative,
is also understandable that courts are concerned forced-choice questions as outlined in Frederick
about claimed amnesia and the possibility of et al. (1995) and Denney (1996). The purpose was
malingering. to determine whether or not the presumed poor
Researchers long presented the perspective sensitivity of forced-choice procedures (Rogers,
that a substantial number of amnestic claims for Harrell, & Liff, 1993) would be true for this spe-
criminal events are feigned (Adatto, 1949; cific task. Forty percent of the students performed
Bradford & Smith, 1979; Hopwood & Snell, 1933; below chance using a test of very few items. Jelicic,
Lynch & Bradford, 1980; O’Connell, 1960; Merckelback, and van Bergen (2004) completed
Parwatikar, Holcomb, & Menninger, 1985; Power, another simulation mock crime design but using
1977; Price & Terhune, 1919). There have been a a forced-choice of 50 items. Twenty-five items
variety of methods presented to evaluate legiti- were related to the mock crime and 25 bogus
macy of claimed amnesia (see Rubinsky & Brandt, items were interspersed between genuine items.
1986; Schacter, 1986). Symptom validity testing Bogus items were peripherally related to the crime
has been adapted for evaluating the veracity of and included two answers that were equally plau-
criminal defendants claiming partial amnesia for sible but unknowable. In this instance, 59% of
the alleged offense as well as total retrograde participants scored below chance on the genuine
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 467

items. Generalizing from this study should be In an earlier decision (Washington v. Harper,
done carefully as it was a simulation study where 1990), the court described a balance between state
87% of the students were female and the crime interests and rights of an incarcerated man by
entailed stealing a pornographic magazine from a citing “overriding state interest” as adequate
bar. Nonetheless, the paradigm of interspersing authority to force medicate. In a similar vein,
peripherally related but unknowable items courts have found the prosecution of dangerous
between genuine items appears promising as a defendants a reasonable state interest such that
method to increase the procedure’s sensitivity. forced medication for trial competency was
justified (United States v. Charters, 1988; Khiem v.
Treatment of the United States, 1992).
Incompetent Defendant This position appears to be tacitly supported
The courts have authority to commit incompetent by the U.S. Supreme Court given the fact they
defendants for mental health treatment involun- refused to review the District of Columbia Circuit
tarily. Jackson v. Indiana (1972), a famous and far- Court’s decision to allow forced medication of
reaching U.S. Supreme Court case, outlined that Russell Weston, a clearly violent man,4 to restore
the length of such commitments must have a his competency to stand trial in a capital case.
“rational relationship” to the nature of the com- The U.S. Supreme Court provided an opinion
mitment. In other words, it is not constitutional to regarding involuntary medication for competency
hold someone strictly for treatment to restore restoration of a nonviolent defendant during June
competency longer than the person would have 2003, and the impact of the Sell v. United States
been held if convicted of the crime. Criminal decision has been alarming.
defendants can be held longer if they are consid- Charles Sell, a St. Louis area dentist, was
ered dangerous to themselves or others as a result originally charged with insurance fraud, Medicare
of mental illness, however. fraud, and money laundering.5 He had a history of
Continued commitment for danger to self psychotic mental illness. Dr. Sell was initially
requires a change from a criminally established found competent to stand trial, but regressed to a
commitment to a civil commitment procedure. more clearly psychotic state. He was then found
Most jurisdictions have a process for continued not competent to stand trial and was committed
criminal commitment for those defendants con- for mental health treatment to restore his compe-
sidered dangerous to others. For example, in the tency to proceed. It was recommended he receive
federal system, 18 U.S.C., § 4246, allows contin- antipsychotic medication, but he would not con-
ued commitment of an incompetent defendant sent. Over the course of the next 6 years, the
who is judicially determined to “presently suffer defense argued that he had a right to refuse treat-
from a mental disease or defect as a result of which ment with antipsychotic medications. The 8th
his release would create a substantial risk of bodily Circuit Court ruled in favor of forcibly medicat-
injury to another person or serious damage to ing him, but stayed the order, pending review by
property of another” but only until a “suitable” the U.S. Supreme Court. The U.S. Supreme Court
(i.e., safe) state placement is found, or the defen- vacated the circuit court order and remanded the
dant is no longer dangerous because of mental case back to the district court for further consid-
illness. Involuntary commitment for treatment eration. The High Court concluded that the forc-
to restore competency to proceed does not neces- ible administration of antipsychotic medication
sarily mean treatment with psychiatric medica- solely to restore competency could be done under
tions, however. certain circumstances.
In Riggins v. Nevada (1992), the U.S. Supreme First, the Court recommended trial courts
Court provided vague guidance regarding consider such cases based on Washington v.
medicating incompetent defendants over their Harper (1990) grounds (i.e., is the person danger-
objections. Although the justices indicated ous to self or others grounds). If so, there is no
“treatment with antipsychotic medication was legal concern. If the defendant is not dangerous to
medically appropriate and, considering less self or others, then these four issues must be
intrusive alternatives, essential for the sake of resolved affirmatively by the trial court in order to
[the defendant’s] own safety or the safety of others” justify forced medication: (a) There are important
(p. 135), they did not spell out whether involun- government interests; (b) those important
tary treatment simply to restore competency was government interests will be significantly furthered
appropriate. by medication; (c) the medication is necessary
468 forensic neuropsychology

(i.e., there are no less-intrusive alternatives); and neuropsychologists incorporate slightly different
(d) such medicine is medically appropriate. methodology by including corroborative infor-
The impact of the Sell decision has been mation sources and systematic assessment of
striking. At this point in the federal jurisdiction, negative response bias and malingering.
criminal defendants committed to inpatient The chapter covered the mental health case
treatment for purposes of restoration who do not law at it relates to criminal competency and how it
consent to take antipsychotic medication after applies to various points in criminal proceedings.
psychiatric recommendation must undergo an Particularly, the Dusky standard and its concep-
additional hearing (Sell hearing) for the referring tual bases were emphasized. Case law related to
district court to consider first the issue of danger- decisional competency, such as pleading guilty,
ousness, then the four points outlined by the U.S. waiving right to counsel, and refusing an insanity
Supreme Court. In this manner, the referring defense, was addressed. The impact of amnesia on
court can authorize forced administration of competency as well as methods to assess its verac-
antipsychotic medication solely for the restora- ity was covered. Last, treatment issues were
tion of competency. The process is generally more addressed as they related to hospitalization and
effective when courts entertain the issue of com- forced treatment for the sole purpose of restoring
petency and the proper treatment at the same competency.
time. The difficulty arises when it is determined Chapter 17 furthers the discussion of neurop-
after the hearing that involuntary medicine is the sychological practice in the criminal forensic
most clinically appropriate method to restore arena by expanding on the multiple data source
competency and the defendant then waits in a model and applying it to criminal responsibility
forensic hospital to have a second hearing with evaluations. The history and current standing of
the court for this specific purpose. Too many the insanity defense are covered. Issues around
times, the legal process has been slow to respond sentencing are presented, such as diminished
to these defendant’s needs even with repeated capacity and responsibility, dangerous prediction,
prompting by forensic evaluators. In other and death penalty. Last, general ethical and pro-
instances, courts have ruled that nonviolent fessional development issues are addressed.
defendants should not be involuntarily medicated Neuropsychologists have an opportunity to make
to seek restoration of competency. In too many of substantial contributions to each of these criminal
these instances the defendant will spend months areas if they have the prerequisite knowledge base
in a mental health treatment facility in the state of and understand important legal and ethical issues
florid psychosis waiting for the time they will inherent in criminal forensics.
spontaneously become competent to proceed
with their case. In most of these instances, antip- N OT E S
sychotic medication could facilitate a return to Opinions expressed in this chapter are those of the
competency and allow defendants to effectively author and do not necessarily represent the position of
defend themselves in the court of law. the Federal Bureau of Prisons or the U.S. Department
of Justice.
S U M M A RY 1. Frederick, DeMier, and Towers obtained pri-
This chapter introduced the practice of neuro- mary source documents related to Dusky.
psychology in the criminal forensic arena as a 2. Those wishing to obtain the CAI may request
unique practice opportunity. Neuropsychologists the Competency to Stand Trial and Mental Illness
are more commonly applying principles of brain– Packet from Paul Lipsitt, LLB, PhD, at Student Mental
behavior relationships and assessment results to Health Clinic, Boston University, 881 Commonwealth
Avenue, West Entrance, Boston, MA 02215. The
the specific questions of criminal courts. Although
Revised CAI may be obtained from Christine
neuropsychology has a great deal to offer the
Mathiesen, PhD, ABPP, Atascadero State Hospital,
courts, practice of neuropsychology in this setting
Centralized Psychological Assessment Services/
requires different assumptions and different CADM, P.O. Box 7001, Atascadero, California, USA,
methods of evaluation from that of typical clinical 93423–7001; telephone: 1–805-468–2000
settings. Neuropsychologists must recognize that, 3. CAST-MR Ordering information: IDS
in forensic settings, the client is typically not the Publishing, P.O. Box 389, Worthington, OH 43085;
patient. The evaluator’s alliance is with the truth telephone 1–614-885–2323.
rather than the patient or referral source. As a 4. Weston was charged with a shooting rampage
result of this different conceptual standing, in the U.S. Capitol building, which resulted in the
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 469

deaths of two Capitol police. The facts of the case Denney, R. L. (1996) Symptom validity testing of
clearly reveal Weston as the perpetrator. remote memory in a criminal forensic
5. He subsequently was charged with attempted setting. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11,
murder of a federal agent, but the U.S. Supreme Court 589–603.
made its decision based solely on the facts as if Sell was Denney, R. L. & Sullivan, J. P. (2008). Constitutional,
considered a nondangerous criminal defendant. judicial, and practice foundations of criminal foren-
sic neuropsychology. In R. L. Denney & J. P. Sullivan
(Eds.) Clinical neuropsychology in the criminal
REFERENCES forensic setting (pp. 1–29). New York: Guilford.
Adatto, C. P. (1949). Observations on criminal patients Denney, R. L. & Wynkoop, T. F. (2000). Clinical neu-
during narcoanalysis. Archives of Neurology and ropsychology in the criminal forensic setting.
Psychiatry, 62, 82–92. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 15, 804–28.
Bagby, R. M., Nicholson, R. A., Rogers, R., & Nussbaum, Drope v. Missouri, 420 U.S. 162 (1975).
D. (1992). Domains of competency to stand trial: Dusky v. United States, 362 U.S. 402 (1960).
A factor analytic study. Law and Human Behavior, Everington, C. T. (1990). The Competence Assessment
16, 491–507. for Standing Trial for Defendants With Mental
Bardwell, M. C. & Arrigo, B. A. (2002). Criminal com- Retardation (CAST-MR): A validation study.
petency on trial: The case of Colin Ferguson. Durham, Criminal Justice and Behavior, 17, 147–68.
North Carolina: Carolina Academic Press. Everington, C. T. & Dunn, C. (1995). A second valida-
Barnard, G. W., Nicholson, R., Hankins, G., Raisani, tion study of the Competence Assessment for
K., Patel, N., Gies, D., et al. (1992). Item metric Standing Trial for Defendants With Mental
and scale analysis of a new computer-assisted Retardation (CAST-MR). Criminal Justice and
competency assessment instrument (CADCOMP). Behavior, 22, 44–59.
Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 10, 419–35. Everington, C. T. & Luckasson, R. (1992). Competence
Barnard, G. W., Thompson, J. W., Freeman, W. C., Assessment for Standing Trial for Defendants With
Robbins, L., Gies, D., & Hankins, G. C. (1991). Mental Retardation (CAST*MR): Test manual.
Competency to stand trial: Description and evalu- Worthington, OH: IDS.
ation of a new computer assisted assessment tool Everington, C. T., Notario-Smull, H., & Horton, M. L.
(CADCOMP). Bulletin of the American Academy (2007). Can defendants with mental retardation
of Psychiatry and Law, 19, 367–81. successfully fake their performance on a test of
Barr, W. B. (2008). Neuropsychological approaches to competence to stand trial? Behavioral Sciences and
criminality and violence. In R. L. Denney & J. P. the Law, 25, 545–60.
Sullivan (Eds.) Clinical neuropsychology in the Faretta v. California, 422 U.S. 806 (1975).
criminal forensic setting (pp. 238–72). New York: Forensic Specialty Council. (2000). Petition for the rec-
Guilford Press. ognition of forensic psychology as a specialty in pro-
Bigler, E. D. & Clement, P. F. (1997). Diagnostic clinical fessional psychology. Submitted to the American
neuropsychology (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: University Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
of Texas Press. Frederick, R. I., Carter, M., & Powel, J. (1995). Adapting
Borum, R. & Grisso, T. (1995). Psychological test use in symptom validity testing to evaluate suspicious
criminal forensic evaluations. Professional complaints of amnesia in medicolegal evaluations.
Psychology: Research and Practice, 26, 465–73. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and
Bradford, J. W. & Smith, S. M. (1979). Amnesia and the Law, 23, 231–37.
homicide: The Padola case and a study of 30 cases. Frederick, R. I., DeMier, R. L., & Towers, K. (2004).
Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and Assessing competency to stand trial: Foundations in
Law, 7, 219–31. case law. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press.
Cochrane, R. E., Grisso, T., & Frederick, R. I. (2001). Frederick, R. I. & Denney, R. L. (1998). Minding your
The relationship between criminal charges, diag- “ps and qs” when using forced-choice recognition
noses, and psycholegal opinions among federal tests. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 193–205.
pretrial defendants. Behavior Sciences and the Law, Frendak v. United States, 408 A.2d 364 (1979).
19, 565–82. Giuliano, A. J., Barth, J. T., Hawk, G. L., & Ryan, T. V.
Colorado v. Connelly, 107 S.Ct. 515 (1986). (1997). The forensic neuropsychologist: Precedents,
Cooper v. Oklahoma, 517 U.S. 348 (1996). roles, and problems. In R. J. McCaffrey, A. D.
Cox, M. L. & Zapf, P. A. (2004). An investigation of Williams, J. M. Fisher, & L. C. Laing (Eds.), The
discrepancies between mental health professionals practice of forensic neuropsychology: Meeting chal-
and the courts in decisions about competency. lenges in the courtroom (pp. 1–36). New York:
Law and Psychology Review, 28, 109. Plenum Press.
Davis v. Wyrick, 766 F.2d 1197 (1987). Godinez v. Moran, 125 L. Ed. 2d, 509 U.S. 389 (1993).
470 forensic neuropsychology

Golding, S., Roesch, R., & Schreiber, J. (1984). Assessment mock crime. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
and conceptualization of competency to stand trial: 19, 525–31.
Preliminary data on the Interdisciplinary Fitness Johnson, W. G. & Mullett, N. (1988). Georgia Court
Interview. Law and Human Behavior, 8, 321–24. Competency Test-R. In M. Hersen & A. S. Bellack
Goldstein, A. M. (2003). Overview of forensic psychol- (Eds.), Dictionary of behavioral assessment tech-
ogy. In I. B. Weiner (Series Ed.) & A. M. Goldstein niques (p. 234). New York: Pergamon.
(Vol. Ed.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. 11: Forensic Kaufmann, P. M. (2008). Admissibility of neuro-
psychology (pp. 3–20). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. psychological evidence in criminal cases.
Gothard, S., Rogers, R., & Sewell, K. W. (1995). Feigning In R. L. Denney & J. P. Sullivan (Eds.) Clinical
incompetency to stand trial: An investigation of neuropsychology in the criminal forensic setting
the GCCT. Law and Human Behavior, 19, 363–73. (pp. 55–90). New York: Guilford.
Grandjean, N. R. (2006). Neuropsychological predic- Khiem v. United States, 612 A.2d 160 (DC Cir., 1992).
tors of incompetency to stand trial in defendants Laboratory of Community Psychiatry, Harvard Medical
referred for competency restoration. Dissertation School. (1973). Competency to stand trial and
Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and mental illness (DHEW Publication No. ADM77–
Engineering, 66(7-B), 3947. Retrieved May 21, 103). Rockville, MD: Department of Health,
2009, from PsycINFO database. Education, and Welfare.
Greenberg, S. A. & Shuman, D. W. (1997). Irreconcilable Leitch, A. (1948). Notes on amnesia in crime for the
conflict between therapeutic and forensic roles. general practitioner. Medical Press, 219, 459–63.
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 28, Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., Hannay,
50–57. H. J., & Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological
Grisso, T. (1986). Evaluating competencies: Forensic assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford.
assessments and instruments. New York: Plenum Lipsitt, P. D., Lelos, D., & McGarry, A. L. (1971).
Press. Competency for trial: A screening instrument.
Grisso, T. (1988). Competency to stand trial evaluations: American Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 105–109.
A manual for practice. Sarasota, FL: Professional Lynch, B. E. & Bradford, J. M. W. (1980). Amnesia: Its
Resource Exchange. detection by psychophysiological measures.
Grisso, T., Borum, R., Edens, J. F., Moye, J., & Otto, Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and
R. K. (2003). Evaluating competencies: Forensic the Law, 8, 288–97.
assessments and instruments (2nd ed.). New York: Magaletta, P. R., Diamond, P. M., McLearen, A. M., &
Kluwer Academic/Plenum. Denney, R. L. (2010). Traumatic brain injuries in
Grisso, T., Cocozza, J., Steadman, H. J., Fisher, W., & correctional populations: Understanding and
Greer, A. (1994). The organization of pretrial foren- responding to an important need. In Managing
sic evaluation services: A national profile. Law and Specific Populations in Jails and Prisons (pp. 6.1–
Human Behavior, 18, 377–94. 6.19). Kingston, NJ: Civic Research Institute.
Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry. (1974). Marcopulos, B. A., Morgan, J. E., & Denney, R. L. (2008).
Misuse of psychiatry in the criminal courts: Neuropsychological evaluation of competency to
Competency to stand trial. New York: Committee proceed. In Denney, R. L., & Sullivan, J. P. (Eds.)
on Psychiatry and the Law. Clinical neuropsychology in the criminal forensic set-
Guttmacher, M. S. (1955). Psychiatry and the law. ting (pp. 176–203). New York: Guilford Press.
New York: Grune and Stratton. Martell, D. A. (1992a). Estimating the prevalence of
Heilbrun, K. (2001). Principles of forensic mental health organic brain dysfunction in maximum-security
assessment. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. forensic psychiatric patients. Journal of Forensic
Heilbronner, R. L. (2004). A status report on the Science, 37, 878–93.
practice of forensic neuropsychology. The Clinical Martell, D. A. (1992b). Forensic neuropsychology and
Neuropsychologist, 18(2), 312–26. the criminal law. Law and Human Behavior, 16,
Hoge, S. K., Bonnie, R. J., Poythress, N., Monahan, J., 313–36.
Eisenberg, M., & Feucht-Haviar, T. (1997). The McGarry, A. L., Lelos, D., & Lipsitt, P. D. (1973).
MacArthur adjudicative competence study: Deve- Competency to stand trial and mental illness.
lopment and validation of a research instrument. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Law and Human Behavior, 21, 141–79. Medina v. California, 505 U.S. 437 (1992).
Hopwood, J. S. & Snell, H. K. (1933). Amnesia in rela- Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., Slobogin, C.,
tion to crime. Journal of Mental Science, 79, 27–41. with Lyons, P. M., & Otto, R. K. (2007). Psychological
Indiana v. Edwards, 544 U.S. 164 (2008). evaluations for the courts (3rd ed.). New York:
Jackson v. Indiana, 406 U.S. 715 (1972). Guilford Press.
Jelicic, M., Merckelbach, H., & van Bergen, S. (2004). Merckelbach, H., Hauer, B., & Rassin, E. (2002).
Symptom validity testing of feigned amnesia for a Symptom validity testing of feigned dissociative
Criminal Forensic Neuropsychology and Assessment of Competency 471

amnesia: A simulation study. Psychology, Crime & Price, G. E. & Terhune, W. B. (1919). Feigned amnesia
Law, 8, 311–318. as a defense reaction. Journal of the American
Miller, H. A. (2001). M-FAST: Miller Forensic Assess- Medical Association, 72, 565–67.
ment of Symptoms Test professional manual. Lutz, Reisner, R. & Slobogin, C. (1990). Law and the mental
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. health system (2nd ed.). St. Paul, MN: West.
Miranda v. Arizona, 384 U.S. 436 (1966). Ricker, J. H. (2003) (Ed.). Differential diagnosis in adult
Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit, neuropsychological assessment. New York: Springer.
D. C. (2002). Base rates of malingering and symp- Riggins v. Nevada, 504 U.S. 127 (1992).
tom exaggeration. Journal of Clinical and Experi- Riley, J. A. (1998, March) Introducing the Revised-CAI
mental Neuropsychology, 24, 1094–1102. and assessment of trial competency—Use of the
Morris v. Slappy, 461 U.S. 1 (1983). Revised-Competency Assessment Instrument hand-
Mrad, D. (1996, September). Criminal responsibility book. Presentation at American Psychology–
evaluations. Paper presented at Issues in Forensic Law Society Biennial Conference, Redondo
Assessment Symposium, Federal Bureau of Prisons, Beach, CA.
Atlanta, GA. Riley, S. E. (1998). Competency to stand trial adjudica-
Nestor, P. G., Daggett, D., Haycock, J., & Price, M. tion: A comparison of female and male defendants.
(1999). Competence to stand trial: A neuropsy- Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and
chological inquiry. Law and Human Behavior, 23, the Law, 26, 223–40.
397–412. Roesch, R. & Golding, S. (1978). Legal and judicial
Nicholson, R. A., Briggs, S. R., & Robertson, H. C. interpretation of competency to stand trial.
(1988). Instruments for assessing competency to Criminology, 16, 420.
stand trial: How do they work? Professional Roesch, R., Webster, C., & Eaves, D. (1984). The Fitness
Psychology: Research and Practice, 19, 383–94. Interview Test: A method for examining fitness to
Nicholson, R. A., Robertson, H. C., Johnson, W. G., & stand trial. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Research
Jensen, G. (1988). A comparison of instruments Report of the Centre of Criminology, University of
for assessing competency to stand trial. Law and Toronto.
Human Behavior, 12, 313–21. Roesch, R., Zapf, P., Eaves, D., & Webster, C. (1998).
North Carolina v. Alford, 400 U.S. 25 (1970). Fitness Interview Test (rev. ed.). Burnaby, British
O’Connell, B. A. (1960). Amnesia and homicide. British Columbia, Canada: Mental Health, Law and Policy
Journal of Delinquency, 10, 262–76. Institute, Simon Fraser University.
Otto, R., Poythress, N., Edens, J., Nicholson, R., Roesch, R., Zapf, P., & Eaves, D. (2006). FIT-R: Fitness
Monahan, J., Bonnie, R., et al. (1998). Psychometric Interview Test—Revised: A structured interview for
properties of the MacArthur Competence assessing competency to stand trial. Sarasota, FL:
Assessment Tool—Criminal Adjudication. Psycho- Professional Resource Press.
logical Assessment, 10, 435–43. Rogers, R. (1997). Introduction. In R. Rogers (Ed.),
Parenté, R. & Herrmann, D. (1996). Retraining cogni- Handbook of diagnostic and structured interviewing
tion: Techniques and applications. Gaithersburg, (pp. 1–19). New York: Guilford Press.
MD: Aspen. Rogers, R. (2001). Focused forensic interviews. In R.
Parwatikar, S. D., Holcomb, W. R., & Menninger, Rogers (Ed.), Handbook of diagnostic and struc-
K. A., II. (1985). The detection of malingered tured interviewing (pp. 296–357). New York:
amnesia in accused murderers. Bulletin of the Guilford Press.
American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 13, Rogers, R. & Grandjean, N. R. (2000, March).
97–103. Competency measures and the Dusky Standard: A
Pate v. Robinson, 383 U.S. 375 (1966). conceptual mismatch? Biennial convention of the
Pinals, D. A., Tillbrook, C. E., & Mumley, D. L. (2006). American Psychology-Law Society, New Orleans,
Practical application of the MacArthur Competence Louisiana.
Assessment Tool-Criminal Adjudication Rogers, R., Grandjean, N. R., Tillbrook, C. E., Vitacco,
(MacCAT-CA) in a public sector forensic setting. M. J., & Sewell, K. W. (2001). Recent interview-
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and based measures of competency to stand trial:
the Law, 34, 179–88. A critical review augmented with research data.
Power, D. J. (1977). Memory, identification and crime. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 19, 503–518.
Medicine, Science, and the Law, 17, 132–39. Rogers, R. Harrell, E. H. & Liff, C. D. (1993). Feigning
Poythress, N., Nicholson, R., Otto, R., Edens, J., neuropsychological impairment: A critical review
Bonnie, R., Monahan, J., et al. (1999). The of methodological and clinical considerations.
MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool—Criminal Clinical Psychological Review, 13, 255–74.
Adjudication: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Rogers, R., Sewell, K. W., Grandjean, N. R., &
Psychological Assessment Resources. Vitacco, M. (2002). The detection of feigned mental
472 forensic neuropsychology

disorders on specific competency measures. United States v. Charters, 863 F.2d 302 (4th Cir., 1988).
Psychological Assessment, 14, 177–83. United States v. Rinchack, 820 F.2d 1557 (11th Cir.,
Rogers, R., Tillbrook, C. E., & Sewell, K. W. (2004). 1987)
Evaluation of Competency to Stand Trial–Revised. United States v. Swanson, 572 F.2d 523 (5th Cir., 1978).
Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. United States v. Villegas, 899 F.2d 1324 (2nd Cir.,
Rogers, R., Ustad, K. L., Sewell, K. W., & Reinhart, V. 1990).
(1996). Dimensions of incompetency: A factor Ustad, K. L., Rogers, R., Sewell, K. W., & Guarnaccia,
analytic study of the Georgia Court Competency C. A. (1996). Restoration of competency to stand
Test. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 14, 323–30. trial: Assessment with the GCCT-MSH and the
Rosenberg, A. H. & McGarry, L. (1972). Competency CST. Law and Human Behavior, 20, 131–46.
for trial: The making of an expert. American Journal Viljoen, J. L., Roesch, R., & Zapf, P.A. (2002). Interrater
of Psychiatry, 128, 1092–96. reliability of the Fitness Interview Test across 4
Rosenfeld, B. & Wall, A. (1998). Psychopathology and professional groups. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry,
competence to stand trial. Criminal Justice and 47, 945–52.
Behavior, 25, 443–62. Viljoen, J. L., Vincent, G. M., & Roesch, R. (2006).
Rubinsky, E. W. & Brandt, J. (1986). Amnesia and Assessing adolescent defendants’ adjudicative
criminal law: A clinical overview. Behavioral competence: Interrater reliability and factor struc-
Sciences and the Law, 4, 27–46. ture of the Fitness Interview Test-Revised. Criminal
Saks, M. J. (1990). Expert witnesses, nonexpert Justice and Behavior, 33, 467–87.
witnesses, and nonwitness experts. Law and Vitacco, M. J., Rogers, R., Gabel, J., & Munizza, J.
Human Behavior, 14, 291–313. (2007). An evaluation of malingered screens with
Schacter, D. L. (1986). Amnesia and crime: How competency to stand trial patients: A known-
much do we really know? American Psychologist, groups comparison.
41, 286–95. Warren, J. I., Rosenfeld, B., Fitch, W. LO., & Hawk, G.
Sell v. United States, 539 U.S. 166 (2003). (1997). Forensic mental health clinical evaluation:
Shapiro, D. L. (1984). Psychological evaluation and expert An analysis of interstate and intersystemic differ-
testimony. New York: van Nostrand Reinhold. ences. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 377–90.
Shapiro, D. L. (1991). Forensic psychological assessment. Washington v. Harper, 494 U.S. 210 (1990).
Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Westbrook v. Arizona, 384 U.S. 150 (1966).
Shapiro, D. L. (1999). Criminal responsibility evalua- Whalem v. United States, 346 F.2d 812 (DC Cir., 1965).
tions: A manual for practice. Sarasota, FL: Widows, M. & Smith, G.P. (2005) Structured inventory
Professional Resource Press. of malingered symptomatology professional manual.
Sieling v. Eyman, 478 F.2d 211 (1973). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Sohlberg, M. M. & Mateer, C. A. (2001). Cognitive Wieter v. Settle, 193 F. Supp. 318 (W.D. Mo., 1961).
rehabilitation. New York: Guilford Press. Wildman, R., Batchelor, E., Thompson, L., Nelson, F.,
Stafford, K. P. (2003). Assessment of competence Moore, J., Patterson, M., et al. (1980). The Georgia
to stand trial. In I. B. Weiner (Series Ed.) & A. M. Court Competency Test: An attempt to develop a
Goldstein (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. rapid, quantitative measure for fitness for trial.
11: Forensic psychology (pp. 359–80). Hoboken, NJ: Unpublished manuscript, Forensic Services
Wiley. Division, Central State Hospital, Milledgeville, GA.
Steadman, H., Monahan, J., Hartstone, E., Davis, S., & Wilson v. United States, 391 F.2d 460 (DC Cir., 1968).
Robbins, P. (1982). Mentally disordered offenders: Wynkoop, T. F. & Denney, R. L. (1999). Exaggeration
A national survey of patients and facilities. Law of neuropsychological deficit in competency to
and Human Behavior, 6, 31–38. stand trial. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 1,
Sullivan, J. P. & Denney, R. L. (2008). A final word on 29–53.
authentic professional competence in criminal Youtsey v. United States, 97 F. 937, 940 (6th Circ.
forensic neuropsychology. In R. L. Denney & J. P. 1899).
Sullivan (Eds.) Clinical neuropsychology in the Zapf, P. A. & Roesch, R. (2001). A comparison of
criminal forensic setting (pp. 391–400). New York: MacCAT-CA and FIT for making determinations
Guilford Press. of competency to stand trial. International Journal
Taylor, P. J. & Kopelman, M. D. (1984). Amnesia of Law and Psychiatry, 24, 81–92.
for criminal offences. Psychological Medicine, 14, Zapf, P. A., Skeem, J. L., & Golding, S. L. (2005). Factor
581–88. structure and validity of the MacArthur Compe-
United States v. Andrews, 469 F.3rd 1113 (7th Cir., tency Assessment Tool—Criminal Adjudication.
2006). Psychological Assessment, 17, 433–45.
17
Criminal Responsibility and Other
Criminal Forensic Issues
ROBERT L. DENNEY

In chapter 16, I presented criminal forensic exemplified, in methodological terms, by the


neuropsychology as a subspecialty of forensic multiple data source model of assessment.
psychology. It is a practice area that requires a
unique knowledge base. The forensic role requires M U LT I P L E D ATA
an appreciation of the significantly different SOURCE MODEL
alliance, goals, and methodology. I introduced the The MDSM is presented in Figure 16.1 (see p.440,
multiple data source model (MDSM) of forensic this volume). It represents a synthesis of the works
assessment. I described mental health case law of various authors in forensic psychology in gen-
regarding competency to proceed and outlined eral (Grisso, 1988; Melton, Petrila, Poythress, &
decisional competency as it relates to confessions, Slobogin, 2007) and Shapiro’s work in criminal
pleading guilty, waiving one’s right to an attorney, responsibility evaluation in particular (1984,
and refusing an insanity defense. The impact of 1991, 1999). The primary aspects of this integrated
amnesia on competency and treatment to restore model were first presented by Mrad (1996),
competency were also addressed. Each of adapted and published for use in criminal forensic
these areas establishes a foundation to cover the neuropsychology (Denney & Wynkoop, 2000),
more advanced areas of criminal forensics and later applied to civil forensic work (McLearen,
presented here. Pietz, & Denney, 2004).
In this chapter, I expand on the forensic assess- Figure 16.1 provides an overall picture of how
ment model, particularly as it pertains to the information sources are classified and integrated
assessment of criminal responsibility. I cover the to form reasonable clinical decisions and adequate
historical antecedents of the insanity defense and support for specific forensic questions, particu-
its current legal conceptualizations. Sentencing larly those that involve an opinion about behavior
issues, such as diminished capacity and responsi- at some point in the past. Most commonly, the
bility, prediction of dangerousness, and death past opinion will deal with mental state at the time
penalty, are covered. The chapter ends with gen- of a specific crime, but occasionally retrospective
eral ethical and professional development issues competency evaluations occur as well. Using this
for neuropsychologists entertaining the idea of model better ensures that the evaluator considers
developing proficiency in criminal forensics. relevant information, identifies inconsistencies
Part of the unique knowledge base in criminal between data sources, and comes to a reasonable
forensics is the realization that forensic evaluators conclusion, whether addressing past mental states
do not share the same goals as general clinical or current diagnostic issues.
providers. The client is typically not the patient, Figure 16.1 represents a cross-tabulation of
and the evaluator’s alliance is with the truth as information sources (self-report and corrobora-
opposed to the patient or referral source. As such, tive information) and points on the timeline (cur-
forensic neuropsychologists utilize a different rently, the time of the offense, and life history).
methodology that includes extensive use of cor- The three rows represent periods of time. The top
roborative information and the systematic assess- row includes information to address current
ment of negative response bias and malingering. mental functioning. The lower row refers to life
The conceptual difference of this approach is best history. The middle row focuses on mental state,
474 forensic neuropsychology

behavior, and motivation at the time of the alleged conversation between the defendant and his or
offense (or the row can represent any event in the her attorney is often helpful. Federal Rule of
past, such as retrospective study of competency to Criminal Procedure 12.2(c)(4) provides this pro-
stand trial, enter a guilty plea, waive right to coun- tection for federal defendants:
sel, understanding Miranda warning, and compe-
tence to confess). The focus on mental state, No statement made by a defendant in the
behavior, and motivation for that time period has course of any examination conducted under
been referred to as MSO for mental state (at the this rule (whether conducted with or without
time of the) offense (Melton et al., 2007). The left the defendant’s consent), no testimony by the
column of self-report covers the defendant’s expert based on the statement, and no other
perception and recollection, and the middle fruits of the statement may be admitted into
column focuses on information derived from evidence against the defendant in any criminal
sources other than the defendant. Self-report and proceeding except on an issue regarding mental
corroborative information cover each of the three condition on which the defendant [has either
time periods. The third column refers to the clini- introduced mental health evidence or notice of
cian’s opinion about mental state and likely diag- intent to rely on a mental health defense].
nosis for each period of time.
There should be a general flow of consistency This protection is provided for subjects of court-
between boxes vertically and horizontally. For ordered evaluations, for which questions of the
example, a defendant’s self-reported history in the defendant’s willingness to cooperate most com-
area of mental health symptoms should be rea- monly arise. Relatedly, 12.2(d) allows the court to
sonably consistent with their current mental state exclude any expert evidence (even defense evi-
and MSO given known understanding of the nat- dence) if the defendant does not cooperate with a
ural course for that particular mental health con- court-ordered examination. Reminding defense
cern. The age of onset and symptom manifestation counsel of such rules and allowing the defendant
should make sense. Likewise, medical records, to consult with counsel often resolves the defen-
family reports, and current assessment techniques dant’s concern and allows the evaluation to pro-
should be reasonably consistent from past to ceed as usual. Occasionally, defendants are quite
present. The major consistency check, however, suspicious of their attorney and will not cooperate
occurs horizontally. Defendant’s report of per- because of paranoid ideation. Typically, these
sonal history should be consistent with educa- instances relate to evaluations for competency to
tional records, medical records, family reports, stand trial and are court ordered. The evaluator
and so on. This consistency acts as a veracity check will need to progress with the evaluation with
and cannot be ignored. The ultimate focus after whatever information becomes available and limit
incorporating current and background informa- his or her opinions appropriately.
tion is the row dealing with MSO. It deserves In the federal system, as in most states, intent
special consideration. to rely on an insanity defense requires notification
to the court by the defense. In this regard, defen-
Mental State at Offense dants are generally, ostensibly, cooperative because
Like every other aspect of this model, there should they have discussed the potential referral for
be reasonable consistency between defendant mental health evaluation with counsel before
self-report regarding thoughts and actions during making the request to the judge. An insanity-
and around the time of the offense and that of cor- referred defendant who indicates no desire to use
roborative sources. It is customary to discuss with a mental health defense raises a unique concern.
the defendant his or her perception of events lead- Because insanity is an “affirmative” defense, mean-
ing up to the alleged offense, behaviors and moti- ing the defense must actively seek it and carries
vations during the events, and what happened the burden of proof, competent defendants have
afterward. Although it is desirable to interview the right to refrain from a mental health defense.
the defendant about such issues, occasionally the Continuing a mental health evaluation on the
defendant’s mental illness interferes. The defen- issue of sanity is problematic if the defendant does
dant may be unwilling to talk about it or may not want a mental health defense.
indicate that his or her attorney directed him or Under this circumstance, the issue goes more
her not to speak of events leading up to the alleged to competence to refuse an insanity defense (see
offense. In such cases, coordinating a telephone chapter 16). These issues are discussed further
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 475

under Ethical and Professional Issues. The point rea. Actus reus refers to the performance of
here is that most of the time defendants are a prohibited act. Mens rea refers to the mental
willing to provide their perspective of their func- state, literally “guilty mind.” Mental health profes-
tioning at MSO if they are seeking a mental health sionals address mens rea when they provide
defense. Occasionally, defendants will be unable opinions on sanity and diminished capacity.
to do so because of acute mental illness or signifi-
cant neurocognitive deficits. Inability to provide Historical Introduction to Insanity
self-report information does not necessarily The concept of evaluating the perpetrator’s mental
eliminate the ability to determine MSO, but it state and culpability is as old as documented
often limits the opinion to some degree. The history. Mosaic law, written in the 13th century
amount of limitation depends on the quality of B.C. (e.g., Numbers 35:22), describes situations in
corroborative information at MSO. which “intention” to murder is translated as
Acquisition of corroborative information at “malice aforethought” (Jewish Publication Society,
MSO is paramount in evaluations of sanity and 1982). Suggestions of exculpatory “madness” were
past mental states. In this regard, the middle box apparent in preclassical Greek writing (Robinson,
under Other Data Sources for MSO is vital under 1996). Greek moral philosophy clearly addressed
any circumstance and should be viewed as a king- inner will. With the sixth century Justinian Code,
pin of the model. Examples of other data sources ecclesiastical law influenced secular law by intro-
dealing with MSO include witness statements, ducing mens rea more formally. It became clear
videotape and audiotape recordings (actual that mental illness influenced a defendant’s mens
observed defendant behavior), family reports, and rea, and courts began wrestling with the defini-
courtroom transcripts. Each of the surrounding tion of insanity.
boxes must be rationally consistent with the infor- Early British legal history recognized the
mation from this domain. Synthesizing informa- unjustness of holding persons responsible for acts
tion from each of the other boxes allows the stemming from mental aberrations. From 1265
evaluator to derive a reasoned opinion about the on, legal scholars used terms such as “wild beast,”
defendant’s MSO. Once this is done, the evaluator “idiots, madmen, [those who] wholly loseth
can consider the ultimate issue, that is, whether memory and understanding,” and “infant, brute
the defendant’s mental health compromised and or wild beast” (Melton et al., 2007). Some British
altered thinking meets the legal definition in ques- courts during the 1700s concentrated more on the
tion (e.g., insanity, retrospective competency). focused issues of an individual’s understanding
It will become apparent under sections deal- between good and evil and right and wrong. In
ing with standards for insanity in this chapter that the 1800s, some British jurisdiction broadened
presence of a mental disease or defect is not insanity by using such volitional concepts as
enough by itself to conclude lack of criminal “controlling disease” and “acting power within
responsibility. Making such a judgment simply on which cannot be resisted.” Finally, in 1843 Daniel
presence of mental abnormality, without analyz- M’Naghten was found not guilty by reason of
ing defendant’s behavior with corroborative infor- insanity for shooting the British prime minister’s
mation, constitutes what has been referred to as personal secretary (Rex v. M’Naghten, 1843).
the “forensic leap of faith” (Denney & Wynkoop,
2000; Mrad, 1996). To the right of the opinion Ye people of England: exult and be glad, for
about MSO box is the ultimate issue box (Figure ye’re now at the will of the merciless mad . . .
16.1, p.440, this volume). The separation of this Thomas Campbell (published in several
box from the opinion box signifies a distinctly English papers in response to M’Naghten’s
separate analysis that requires comparison of the acquittal, 1843)
MSO opinion to the specific legal standard (e.g.,
insanity) in that criminal jurisdiction. As will Daniel M’Naghten1 had a persecutory delusional
become apparent, insanity standards are not uni- belief that incorporated the Tory Party leader,
form across jurisdictions. Prime Minister Robert Peel. He traveled to
London to ambush Prime Minister Peel, but inad-
CRIMINAL RESPONSIBILITY vertently shot Peel’s personal secretary, Edward
A conclusion of criminal responsibility (guilt) for Drummond. He was acquitted as insane, and
a criminal offense requires proof, beyond reason- there was immediate outcry in public and Parl-
able doubt, of two elements, actus reus and mens iament as demonstrated by Thomas Campbell’s
476 forensic neuropsychology

satire (Moran, 1981). After much debate regard- test” (State v. Jones, 1871), in part secondary to
ing the nature of sanity, the House of Lords called criticisms of rigidity and narrowness of previous
the judges of the Supreme Court to answer five standards (Durham v. United States, 1954).
specific questions. The answers to these questions Also, in response to the growing hope in psycho-
became the M’Naghten rules. A portion of analysis as a method of understanding human
those rules outlined a strictly right/wrong test nature, Judge Bazelon opened the doors to psy-
for insanity, which has become known as the chiatry in the criminal courts by defining insanity
M’Naghten test. Here is the key portion of that as any action that was a “product of mental dis-
judicial decision: ease or defect” (Durham v. United States, 1954).
This new Durham standard was exceptionally
To establish a defence on the ground of insan- broad and had occasionally been referred to as the
ity it must be clearly proved that, at the time of “but for” test, meaning but for the mental illness,
committing of the act, the party accused was the crime would have never occurred. The
labouring under such a defect of reason, from Durham “product test” was a hopeful experiment
disease of the mind, as not to know the nature in humanizing the criminal law, but difficulties
and quality of the act he was doing, or, if he did inherent in such a broad and poorly defined test
know it, that he did not know he was doing soon appeared.
what was wrong. (p. 210) Several legal decisions over the next 20 years
attempted to tighten the product test for insanity
Insanity in the United States by more precisely defining mental disease
The M’Naghten right/wrong test for insanity (McDonald v. United States, 1962) and by limiting
became the standard in the United States as well what mental health professionals could say in
as England, although it was criticized for its front of a jury (Washington v. United States, 1967).
rigidity and sole reliance on cognitive under- By 1972, nearly every federal jurisdiction in
standing (Melton et al., 2007). This criticism the United States adopted the American Law
gradually led U.S. jurisdictions to loosen the stan- Institute’s (ALI’s) Model Penal Code criteria
dard for insanity. The “irresistible impulse” rule (1955),2 which included a two-prong test for
was first adopted in Alabama (Parsons v. State, insanity involving cognition and volition (United
1886) and later by a federal jurisdiction (Davis v. States v. Brawner, 1972). Under the ALI standard,
United States, 1895). This rule was a straightfor- a defendant could be found insane if he or she
ward volitional controls standard. As described in lacked substantial capacity, as a result of mental
Parsons, a defendant would be considered not disease or defect, to appreciate the criminality (or
responsible if he or she lost the power to choose wrongfulness) of his or her acts or to conform his
between right and wrong and, at the same time, or her conduct to the requirements of the law
the criminal behavior was a product of mental (Table 17.1). Either one, or both, of these prongs
disease solely. must be “substantially” impaired as a result of
In 1954, the District of Columbia Court of “mental disease or defect.”
Appeals carried on similar language when it In many respects, the ALI standard combined
adopted a little-known New Hampshire “product the M’Naghten right/wrong knowledge test with

TABLE 17.1 KEY ELEMENTS OF THE AMERICAN LAW


INSTITUTE’S MODEL PENAL CODE
STANDARD FOR INSANIT Y.

The ALI proposed a two-prong test, cognition and volition:


A. Lacks substantial capacity as result of mental disease or defect to:
1. appreciate criminality (optionally, wrongfulness), or
2. conform conduct to requirements of the law.
It contained an optional “caveat paragraph”:
B. Repeated criminal behavior does not constitute a mental disease.

Note: Adapted from American Law Institute (1955).


Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 477

the irresistible impulse test. Nevertheless, there The federal statute effectively eliminated the
were distinct differences that made the ALI volitional prong and required the presence of a
criteria a much more attainable standard. The ALI “severe” mental disease or defect. It changed the
replaced M’Naghten’s rigid term, “did not know” burden of proof to keep it on the defense by a clear
with a more flexible “appreciate” standard. In and convincing evidence standard rather than
addition, the M’Naghten standard suggested an automatically switching the burden over to the
“all or-nothing” criteria with “did not know” as prosecution by beyond reasonable doubt when
opposed to the ALI’s “lacked substantial capacity” the sanity issue is raised (which was the case pre-
formulation. Many jurisdictions also chose to viously). This change makes insanity an “affirma-
adopt the word wrongfulness rather than criminal- tive defense.” It retained the ALI standard’s use of
ity. The wrongfulness term references the point “appreciate” rather than the strict M’Naghten
that a person can hold a moral appreciation of knowledge test. The U.S. Congress also added
wrongfulness as opposed to simply appreciating paragraph (b) to Federal Rule of Evidence (FRE)
the act was criminal. On the other side of the coin, 704, which placed a restriction against mental
however, the ALI formulation contained a second health professionals providing an opinion in front
“optional” paragraph, termed the caveat para- of a jury on the ultimate issue of whether the
graph, that effectively eliminated repeated crimi- defendant is insane or not and, more broadly,
nal acts, in and of themselves (i.e., antisocial whether the defendant could appreciate the
personality disorder), as constituting mental dis- wrongfulness of his or her behavior. Although
ease. The ALI standard was adopted by a majority unable to provide that opinion verbally in front of
of jurisdictions in the United States and remains a a jury, professionals are directed to provide their
popular standard today (Melton et al., 2007). opinion in written reports [18, U.S.C., § 4247(c)
(4)(B)]. The IDRA also established that once an
John Hinckley and the Insanity Defense individual is acquitted by reason of insanity, he or
Reform Act she is committed to the U.S. attorney general for
The ALI standard was in place in 1981 when John secure hospitalization and is assumed dangerous
Hinckley shot President Ronald Reagan, Press until evaluated and judicially determined to be
Secretary James Brady, and two law enforcement otherwise (18, USC, § 4243). One can readily see
personnel. Hinckley was believed to have a delu- how IDRA standard represents a synthesis of past
sional disorder by which he believed this act insanity concepts and lessons learned.
would endear him to a particular Hollywood
actress. He was found not guilty by reason of Nature, Quality, and Wrongfulness
insanity under the volitional prong of the ALI The exact meanings of nature, quality, and wrong-
standard. There was an immediate public back- fulness have been debated. Melton and colleagues
lash that resulted in the Insanity Defense Reform (2007) considered these words a two-prong stan-
Act (IDRA) as part of the broader Crime Control dard of (a) nature and quality and (b) wrongful-
Act, which became law in 1984. This standard is ness (p. 198). They suggested that individuals who
currently in place within the federal jurisdiction do not meet the first standard will likely not meet
and sets the basis for insanity in a majority of state the second. Further, nature and quality are often
jurisdictions. Title 18, U.S.C., § 17 contains the ignored altogether as juries focus on wrongful-
current federal definition of insanity (Table 17.2). ness (Goldstein, 1967, cited in Melton et al., 2007).

TABLE 17.2 CURRENT FEDERAL STANDARD TITLE 18, U.S.C., § 17

(a) Affirmative defense. It is an affirmative defense to a prosecution under any Federal statute that, at the time of
the commission of the acts constituting the offense, the defendant, as a result of a severe mental disease or defect,
was unable to appreciate the nature and quality or the wrongfulness of his acts. Mental disease or defect does not
otherwise constitute a defense.
(b) Burden of proof. The defendant has the burden of proving the defense of insanity by clear and convincing
evidence.
478 forensic neuropsychology

Shapiro (1984, 1991, 1999) described the view but also they refer to the ALI standard. In 1988,
that nature and quality have subtle differences. the U.S. Court of Appeals for the 8th Circuit
Nature is quite basic, such that, “When the defen- applied this same clarification and procedure
dant picked up the gun, did he know the object to the post-Hinckley IDRA insanity standard
was a gun?” The concept of quality goes more to (United States v. Dubray, 1988). Dubray also
consequences. A defendant would not appreciate clarified the FRE 704(b) limitation of testimony
the consequences if he beheaded his sleeping on the ultimate issue of sanity by allowing testi-
brother thinking it would be funny to see the mony on motivation, mental state, and diagnosis
brother get up and look for his head in the morn- at the time of the offense. Dubray only governs
ing. Shapiro and colleagues (Goldstein, Morse, & federal jurisdictions in the 8th Circuit (North
Shapiro, 2003) considered the terms one basic Dakota, South Dakota, Minnesota, Nebraska,
concept. In Clark v. Arizona (2006), the U.S. Iowa, Missouri, and Arkansas), but many other
Supreme Court discussed the issue of nature and jurisdiction throughout the country have adopted
quality in contrast to wrongfulness and suggested this definition of wrongfulness as well.
the three terms represent two distinct constructs:
a cognitive capacity (appreciation of nature and A Case Study in Wrongfulness
quality) and a moral capacity (appreciation of Let us assume a mother3 of a very young child
wrongfulness). In this same case, the High Court becomes psychotic with delusions that the only
affirmed Arizona’s elimination of the cognitive way to save her child from the fires of hell is to
capacity construct altogether in favor of a strictly drown him. She realized she would be arrested
right/wrong definition of insanity. Experience has afterward and likely convicted of murder because
additionally shown criminal responsibility deci- she understood it is against the law to kill her
sions most often balance on the defendant’s appre- child. Although realizing this fact, she earnestly
ciation of wrongfulness. believed killing her child was the right thing to do
because, in her psychotic state of understanding,
Is Wrongfulness Moral or Criminal? it was the only possible way for her to truly save
The ALI standard included the optional use of the her child.
word “wrongfulness” in place of “criminality” In this scenario, the woman appreciated the
(Table 17.1). This option reflected the possibility nature of the act; that is, she realized the act would
that a criminal defendant may have appreciated terminate the life of her child on earth. She also
that his or her act was criminal, but because of appreciated the quality of the act in that the con-
mental illness, believed it was morally justified. sequences included her son’s death and even the
This concept was initially presented in 1915 by fact she would be arrested and likely convicted.
Judge Cordozo in New York, who described “mis- The issue of her appreciation of wrongfulness
perceptions” as a result of mental disease that could becomes much more problematic. She clearly
cause a defendant to believe his or her act was mor- appreciated the criminality of her actions as she
ally justified (People v. Schmidt, 1915). In 1970, the even voiced her awareness of her arrest and likely
U.S. Court of Appeals for the 9th Circuit (federal conviction. It is reasonable in this instance, given
jurisdictions in California, Oregon, Washington, a jurisdiction that interprets wrongfulness as
Arizona, Montana, Idaho, Nevada, Alaska, and moral rather than strictly criminal, to argue she
Hawaii) adopted wrongfulness and gave the exam- did not appreciate the moral wrongfulness and
ple of a delusion leading to belief in moral justifica- was, therefore, insane. Although criminally
tion (Wade v. United States, 1970). In a similar case, wrong, she believed it was the morally right thing
the 9th Circuit reiterated the point that the moral to do as it was the only way she could save her
justification must be a result of mental disease child from hell.
(United States v. Sullivan, 1976). The 9th Circuit This scenario highlights the importance of
Court went further in 1977 by declaring juries understanding the law in the jurisdiction in which
should be instructed that wrongfulness can be the case is prosecuted. In a strictly criminal juris-
interpreted as moral rather than criminal only if diction, a mental health expert should opine that
there are facts in the case that raise this unique she was sane. In a jurisdiction in which wrongful-
distinction (United States v. Segna, 1977). ness can mean moral as well as criminal, the
Not only are Wade, Sullivan, and Segna note- expert should raise the issue of her appreciating
worthy for raising the issue of moral wrongfulness the moral wrongfulness and opine that she was
in the federal jurisdiction and outlining its use, insane. Not understanding this difference in the
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 479

law can cause otherwise quite competent and relatively rare, and successful insanity acquittals
respected mental health experts to come to a are even less common (Silver, Cirincione, &
wrong conclusion. The importance in evaluating a Steadman, 1994; Steadman et al., 1993). National
criminal defendant’s sanity is applying our skills data are available that reveal only 2,542 people
in evaluation and diagnosis to the correct stan- were found insane in the entire United States in
dard of law. 1980 (Steadman, Rosenstein, MacAskiel, &
Manderschied, 1988). As said by the legal scholar,
Current Status of the Insanity Defense Michael Perlin, successful insanity acquittals are
The federal jurisdictions are uniformly IDRA “rarer than snake bites in Manhattan” (Perlin,
right or wrong because of statutory authority. As 1992).
of 2006, 28 states had adopted the federal stan- The incidence of brain-injury–related insanity
dard in one form or other, 14 states use the ALI acquittals appears to be even rarer. Steadman and
criteria, three use an irresistible impulse test, New colleagues studied four states regarding insanity
Hampshire continues with the product test, and pleas, acquittals, and diagnostic characteristics
Kansas, Idaho, Montana, North Dakota, and Utah before and after the Hinckley-related reforms
have eliminated the insanity defense but allow (Steadman et al., 1993). They did not specifically
expert testimony on mens rea (e.g., diminished identify brain injury as a diagnostic category, but
capacity) (Melton et al., 2007). they found 69% of those entering insanity pleas
About a dozen states have adopted a “guilty had schizophrenia, another psychosis, or major
but mentally ill” (GBMI) provision in addition to affective disorder. Nestor and Haycock studied
an insanity defense as an alternative verdict for murderers committed to a Massachusetts forensic
juries to consider. This finding would convict a hospital (Nester & Haycock, 1997). Of the 13
defendant of the crime, and thereby guaranteeing insanity acquittees referred for neuropsycholo-
a criminal sentence, but makes it clear he or she is gical evaluation, 12 were considered psychotic at
mentally ill and in need of treatment. The GBMI the time of the crime. Melton his colleagues
provision has been significantly criticized on con- reported the results of six studies that revealed
ceptual grounds and, in most cases, does not 67% to 97% of insanity acquittees had a signifi-
appear to ensure mental health treatment for the cant psychosis, suggesting psychosis is usually
convicted (Melton et al., 2007). required for successful insanity defense (Melton
A surprising finding is that, even with these et al., 2007).
various formulations, including ALI, IDRA, and There are no studies identifying the rates of a
abolition of the insanity defense, research sug- neuropsychological basis for insanity. Available
gests no difference between conviction rates or research would suggest the condition would
rates of hospitalization (Steadman et al., 1993). need to rise to the level of psychosis for success,
Apparently this finding is because every state has particularly in those jurisdictions without the
a method of hospitalizing mentally ill persons volitional prong in the insanity standard. Of 456
convicted of crimes (Favole, 1983), and individu- consecutive referrals for sanity evaluation at the
als acquitted by reason of insanity appear to spend U.S. Medical Center during the early 1990s, only
the same amount of time securely hospitalized as 17 were diagnosed with an organic mental illness,
they would have been incarcerated if convicted and none were considered insane (Denney &
(Steadman et al., 1993). Wynkoop, 2000). Cochrane, Grisso, and Frederick
(2001) combined additional cases (N = 1,170)
Insanity and Traumatic Brain Injury from this database to cover a longer period of
Changing the insanity standard back to a test of time and found little support for insanity opinions
appreciation for right and wrong would appear to based on organic disorders.
make it a much more difficult standard to meet Denney and Wynkoop (2000) provided the
for potentially insane defendants, particularly example of an insanity evaluee diagnosed with
when their “mental disease or defect” is caused by dementia caused by Alzheimer’s disease and
traumatic brain injury (TBI). Contrary to this considered insane for disorderly conduct and
appearance, however, the overall incidence of trespassing on U.S. postal property. The defendant
insanity pleas and their ultimate success rates had delusions that the U.S. Postal Service was
seem to have changed little since the reform stealing his mail. In this instance, psychosis caused
(Steadman et al., 1993). Also contrary to public his insanity, and Alzheimer’s dementia caused his
perception is the finding that insanity defenses are psychosis.
480 forensic neuropsychology

In many instances, it is apparent that cases are history of PTSD and major depression. In seeking
not referred for evaluation because they are not other data regarding his mental state at the time
prosecuted when an organic mental illness was of the offense, investigative records revealed
severe enough at the time to clearly result in crim- that when he went into the bank he carried a
inal actions (called pretrial diversion). Of those handgun. He ushered all the bank employees into
that are prosecuted, many are dealt with at the an office. He acquired access to the walk-in safe.
level of competency to stand trial. Given the often Last, he asked the employees where the videotape
static or degenerative nature of neurological con- recording device was that videotaped bank robber-
ditions, these people are found not competent to ies. He then went to the room and confiscated the
stand trial and remain so. Consequently, they videotape that had recorded his entrance to the
never reach a point at which sanity is at issue. bank. He locked the employees in the office and
It is apparent that evaluators more often face departed. Throughout the robbery, he demanded
insanity evaluations when some form of less- employees not look at his face and to turn away
obvious organic mental disorder is present and from him. Given the entirety of information
may have had an impact on the defendant’s past available using the MDSM, the evaluator provided
behavior. It is unlikely that mild brain injury cases this opinion:
without co-morbid psychosis would support an
insanity defense with the current right/wrong Although he had been diagnosed prior to the
standard (Denney & Wynkoop, 2000; Yates & robbery with PTSD and depression, behavior
Denney, 2008). Under the ALI standard, such described in the offense suggested he well
defenses would appear to have a greater chance of understood what he was doing and the wrong-
success given the volitional component. fulness of his actions. He used a gun to gain
control of the bank employees. He carried with
Brain Injury and Insanity: Importance him a bag for money. He ordered them not to
of the Multiple Data Source Model give him bait money or dye packs. He cau-
and Not Ignoring Data tioned them against pulling any alarm. He
The following case example demonstrates the demanded the video tape from the security
importance of obtaining corroborative informa- camera. He pulled the telephone out of the
tion as part of a neuropsychological evaluation on room in which he left them. He repeatedly told
the issue of criminal responsibility. Although it is them to not look at him. After leaving the bank,
important to use the MDSM (Figure 16.1, p. 440, he changed his outward appearance and
this volume) for acquiring information, it is imper- attempted to flee the area.
ative the information is integrated in a manner
consistent with the known natural course of TBI. The defendant was subsequently evaluated by a
This is the only way to arrive at a reasonable foren- neuropsychologist hired by the defense. The neu-
sic conclusion regarding such complicated issues. ropsychologist provided this opinion:
A 28-year-old man was charged with armed
bank robbery and referred for evaluation of his He suffers cognitive deterioration from his
sanity. Nine years previously, he was in a serious head injury, with his IQ 40–50% below his
motor vehicle accident and experienced a possible memory functions and achievement skills. The
TBI with potential brief loss of consciousness weak score on ability to attend and concentrate
(medical records were not available). There were is consistent with this deterioration.
no known complications. Two years later, he Additionally, in the critical area of thinking
became a police officer and joined the depart- abstractly and solve problems in a hypotheti-
ment’s Special Weapons and Tactics team. He won cal-deductive fashion, as measured by the
awards on the police force for his excellent work. Category Test, he was in the impaired range.
He started experiencing significant stress second- Given these limitations from his 1985 head
ary to a deathly sick child and overwhelming debt. injury, when stressed, he had little or no reserve
He was diagnosed with post-traumatic stress dis- to call upon for coping skills. I do not agree that
order (PTSD) and major depression while in the his reality contact was not substantially impaired
community. Nine years after the putative TBI, he at the time of the offense [emphasis added]. In
robbed a bank. my opinion, he saw no options and had no
The defendant was referred for an evaluation choices. He did not act rationally and did not
regarding his criminal responsibility in light of his appreciate the consequences of his actions.
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 481

This opinion was provided by an expert who The R-CRAS promises increased scientific
had read the previous report and knew of the rigor in the process of sanity evaluations and pro-
defendant’s actions in the bank. He nonetheless vides increased standardization for insanity
ignored this vital corroborative information assessments as well as an estimate of known error
about the man’s behavior and concluded that not rates for predictive classification (Grisso, 2003).
only was his abstract thinking and hypothetical- However, it has been criticized regarding its
deductive problem-solving skills impaired, but method of “quantifying” vague clinician judg-
also his reality contact was impaired. Aside from ments (Melton et al., 2007). See the work of
ignoring important corroborative information Golding and Roesch (1987), Rogers and Ewing
from the time of the offense, the evaluator also (1992), and Golding, Skeem, Roesch, and Zapf
disregarded the atypical pattern of recovery for (1999) for discussion of these criticisms. The
brain injury. The defendant had become a deco- R-CRAS may prove invaluable as a training tool
rated police officer and SWAT team member after for evaluators learning to perform criminal
the claimed head injury. Barring some interven- responsibility evaluations, but available research
ing event to the brain, there is no explanation for suggests the R-CRAS is rarely used in forensic
that course of recovery from brain injury. A com- practice (Borum & Grisso, 1995).
parison of information dealing with past history,
current presentation, and time of the alleged Diminished Capacity and
offense would have revealed these striking incon- Responsibility
sistencies. Diminished capacity is considered a mens rea
defense. In other words, it refers to a decreased
Criminal Responsibility level of culpability because of lesser intent (Clark,
Assessment Tools 1999). In this regard, first-degree murder, second-
Rogers (1984) developed the Rogers Criminal degree murder, and manslaughter differ in their
Responsibility Assessment Scales (R-CRAS) as a level of intent. Without invoking the insanity
tool to assist in the evaluation of criminal respon- defense, defendants occasionally bring mental
sibility. It is designed for use after the evaluator has state into play by claiming a decreased level of
completed all clinical interviews and obtained cor- intent because of such factors as alcohol or drug
roborative information (e.g., ancillary interviews, intoxication, medication use, neurological condi-
medical and investigative records, etc.). It contains tions, or extreme emotional disturbance (Melton
30 items grouped in these topic areas: Patient’s et al., 2007; Yates & Denney, 2008). An extreme
Reliability, Organicity, Psychopathology, Cognitive example is the automatism defense, by which
Control, and Behavioral Control. Each item is defendants claim no conscious awareness of their
assigned a score based upon a 5- or 6-point Likert- acts. Examples have included crimes committed
type scale. Items focus on areas relevant to the ALI while sleepwalking, during a seizure, while
standard, with additional variables related to the unaware secondary to head injury or other
GBMI standard and right/wrong standard encephalopathic conditions, and even during
(M’Naghten and IDRA). The instrument helps cli- dissociative episodes (Barnard, 1998). Although
nicians rate the defendant in several areas: reliabil- courts have generally allowed testimony to this
ity regarding potential malingering; signs indicative issue, they have limited its use when the defen-
of organic mental illness, mental retardation, and dant experienced the disability previously and
mental illness; and whether there were indications should have taken precautions to prevent a poten-
of cognitive impairment or loss of behavioral con- tial criminal event. An example would be a man
trol at the time of the alleged offense. The ratings with a known history of aggression secondary to
culminate in a decision tree that guides the exam- complex-partial seizure disorder who refuses pro-
iner to a forensic conclusion: sane, insane, or no phylactic treatment to help avoid seizures (and
opinion. The R-CRAS manual includes summary thereby aggression and assault).
statistics regarding the scales based on three sam- When considering diminished capacity, it
ples (n = 73, n = 111, n = 76) of individuals consid- must be realized that there are both general and
ered sane or insane by forensic examiners based on specific intent crimes. Felon in possession of a
the ALI model. Inter-rater reliability and concor- weapon is an example of a general intent crime.
dance between opinions based on R-CRAS rating By definition, possessing the weapon carries with
and court verdicts are both respectable (Grisso, it the prerequisite intent as long as the defendant
2003; Melton et al., 2007). understood, or should have understood, that it
482 forensic neuropsychology

was illegal for him or her to possess a weapon. phase of death penalty cases in some jurisdictions.
Bank robbery requires specific intent, that is, Potential for dangerousness is generally always
resolve for a particular act to occur. Intent must be an issue for defendants who are considered not
differentiated from motive. Motive prompts an competent to stand trial and unrestorable. In the
act, whereas intent “refers only to the state of federal jurisdiction, the presiding court must
mind with which the act is done” (West Publishing, address the defendant’s potential dangerousness
1990, p. 810). to others and significant property of others on
The most common basis for diminished release because the charges can be dismissed when
capacity is intoxication (Marlowe, Lambert, & a mentally defective defendant is considered
Thompson, 1999). An example would include unlikely to become competent in the foreseeable
whether a defendant could form the prerequisite future (18, U.S.C., § 4241 and 4241).
intent to first-degree murder. It may very well be Unrestorable incompetent defendants in the
that alcohol intoxication made this level of intent federal jurisdiction can be held in a secure
rather unlikely. Under this circumstance, a jury hospital indefinitely if they are considered dan-
could use a lesser included offense such as second- gerous because of mental defect (18, U.S.C.,
degree murder or even manslaughter, depending § 4246). Nearly the same issue arises after a defen-
on the facts of the case. dant is found insane and hospitalized in a secure
A similar argument can be made for the effects facility (18, U.S.C., § 4243). The issue can come up
of neurological compromise. An example would again when a sentenced inmate who is potentially
include a dentist charged with inappropriate dis- dangerous because of mental disease or defect
pensing of narcotics and whose judgment was reaches the end of a sentence because federal
compromised by early frontal dementia. The com- statute allows potential extended commitment
promise may not be so severe as to eliminate (18, U.S.C., § 4246). In each of these scenarios, the
intent fully (which should result in an acquittal), Federal Bureau of Prisons under authority of
but it can allow the jury to apply the law in a fair the U.S. attorney general has the mandate and
manner. challenge to find suitable state placement—a
A related, and often confused, term is dimin- placement that will further ensure public safety.
ished responsibility. This term actually refers to Most states have similar statutory procedures.
mitigating circumstances of the crime that war- Consequently, it is common for mental health
rant a lesser punishment (Clark, 1999). Such professionals to provide expert opinions on risk of
issues are brought before the court at the time of dangerousness for the deciding court on each of
sentencing. Diminished responsibility is particu- these occasions.
larly relevant in jurisdictions that no longer have Assessment of risk in the prediction of future
the volitional prong in their insanity standard. dangerousness poses certain ethical dilemmas as
Individuals with frontal lobe damage often have it balances the liberty interests of the individual
impulse control problems that potentially impact against the safety needs of the community (Grisso
their ability to refrain from performing certain & Applebaum, 1992). Given the liberty interests
criminal acts. Deficits in cognitive, emotional, involved, it is imperative neuropsychologists per-
and behavioral controls secondary to neurological forming this function conform their work to the
compromise are relevant to a defense against standard of practice as outlined in current litera-
many criminal charges, either at trial or at ture. In Barefoot v. Estelle (1983), James P. Grigson
sentencing. testified regarding defendant Barefoot’s danger-
ousness without ever meeting the man. Justice
Assessing Risk of Dangerousness Blackmun wrote, “Doctor Grigson testified that
It is not uncommon for neuropsychologists whether Barefoot was in society at large or in a
involved in criminal proceedings to be asked by prison society there was a ‘one hundred percent
the court to assess a criminal defendant’s propen- and absolute’ chance that Barefoot would commit
sity for violence if released from custody. The future acts of violence that would constitute a
issue of dangerousness is certainly relevant in continuing threat to society” (emphasis original;
cases of neurological compromise (Volavka, Justice Blackmun, dissent, p. 3408). Although an
Martell, & Convit, 1992; Barr, 2008). Such ques- egregiously overstated conclusion, recent research
tions can arise during detention hearings at any suggests that mental health professionals can pre-
phase of the criminal proceedings. Risk assess- dict violence at a rate significantly better than
ment findings are also used in the sentencing chance when they include relevant factors in the
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 483

decision analysis (Bauer et al., 2003; Monahan, clinical. Dispositional factors included demo-
1992; Monahan & Steadman, 1994; Mossman, graphic (e.g., age, gender, race), personality
1994; Steadman, 2000). (e.g., impulsivity and psychopathy), and cognitive
concerns (IQ and neurological impairment).
Factors Known to Increase Historical factors included social history (family,
Risk of Dangerousness work, and education), mental hospitalization
In this section, I focus on factors relevant for the history (prior hospitalizations and compliance),
assessment of future risk of violence. There is and history of crime and violence. Contextual
quite a large body of research on risk assessment factors included perceived stress, social support
in general and specifically on that pertaining to systems, and physical aspects of the environment
sex offenders. I consider general issues of risk (e.g., presence of weapons). Clinical factors
assessment and that which logically pertains to included Axis I diagnosis, symptoms, Axis II
neuropsychology. For a recent review pertaining diagnosis, level of functioning, and substance
to risk assessment of sexual predators, see the abuse.
work of Conroy (2003). The MacArthur study (Monahan, 2003;
Much of the research regarding violence pre- Monahan et al., 2001) revealed some surprises
diction, and certainly that dealing with sex offend- and confirmations regarding violence. Prior vio-
ers, has dealt with static and dynamic prediction lence and criminality were strongly associated
variables (Bonta, Law, & Hanson, 1998). Static with community violence in this group of psychi-
variables refer to fixed and unchanging facts atric patients. The prevalence rates for men and
regarding an individual’s life (e.g., number and women were actually about the same, but the
nature of past violent offenses, demographic char- nature of the violence was different. Women are
acteristics), and dynamic variables can change. more likely to be violent toward family members,
These variables are subdivided into stable and but violence from men is more likely to result in
acute dynamic variables. Stable variables have the serious injury. History of childhood physical
potential for change (e.g., substance abuse his- abuse, but not sexual abuse, appears associated
tory), and acute variables are clearly situation spe- with postdischarge violence. As in the Swanson
cific (e.g., intoxication). The bulk of actuarial et al. (1990) study, presence of substance abuse or
research on violence prediction focuses on static dependence in conjunction with mental illness
variables (Conroy, 2003). significantly raised potential for violence; how-
The research of Swanson and colleagues sug- ever, a diagnosis of a major mental illness was
gested that being male, young, of lower socioeco- associated with a lower rate of violence compared
nomic status, and an abuser of drugs or alcohol, to personality or adjustment disorders. Also, pres-
having a major mental disorder, and particularly ence of schizophrenia appeared less associated
having a major mental disorder in combination with violence than depression or bipolar disorder.
with substance abuse/dependence are demo- Delusions and hallucinations were associated with
graphic factors that increase risk of violence in the violence less than nondelusional suspiciousness
community (Swanson, Holzer, Ganju, & Jono, and presence of violent thoughts. Those scoring
1990). Meta-analysis suggests that the strongest high in anger during hospitalization were twice as
predictor of future violence is past violence likely to commit violent acts after discharge as
(Mossman, 1994). Monahan and colleagues those with low anger scores. The nature of the
(2001) reported results of the MacArthur Violence environment also played a role in violence deter-
Risk Assessment Study, the largest study of com- mination, with high-crime neighborhoods more
munity violence prediction of its kind (Monahan, associated with postdischarge violence. There is
2003). The study included male and female civilly little indication in the MacArthur study that neu-
committed patients from several facilities, with rological compromise contributed to an increased
follow-up in the community at 20 weeks and 1 likelihood of postdischarge violence. The facts
year after discharge. They focused on variables that only a few of these subjects had neurologi-
believed to relate to potential violence (see Melton cally related concerns and that brain injury did
et al., 2007 [particularly Table 9.4], and Monahan, not appear to be assessed thoroughly clouds the
2003, for a more thorough review). potential contribution that neurological disorder
Rather than just static and dynamic variables, may have on postdischarge violence.
the MacArthur study considered variables in four Neurocognitive contributions to risk have
factors: dispositional, historical, contextual, and been studied less thoroughly. Research with
484 forensic neuropsychology

childhood neuropathology implied that early Conklin & Stanford, 2002; Drake, Hietter, &
cerebral deficits can predispose future dangerous- Pakalnis, 1992; Drake, Pakalnis, Brown, & Hietter,
ness, particularly when combined with the envi- 1988; Evans, 1997; Gerstle, Mathias, & Stanford,
ronmental factor of an abusive family (Golden, 1998; Houston & Stanford, 2001; Kiehl, Hare,
Jackson, Peterson-Rohne, & Gontkovsky, 2002; Liddle, & McDonald, 1999; Mathias & Stanford,
Lewis, Lovely, Yeager, & Della Femina, 1989; 1999; Raine & Venables, 1988; Raine, Venables, &
Lewis et al., 1985). It is logical that neuropsycho- Williams, 1990). There are also lines of research
logical factors can play a role in the production of implicating neurochemical alterations and vio-
violence. lence (Gregg & Siegel, 2001; Linnoila & Charney,
1999; Miczek, 1987; Siegal & Shaikah, 1992), with
Neuropathology and the Potential a particular emphasis on decreased 5-HT
for Violence (serotonin) and its treatment with selective
It is well known that large portions of the brain serotonin reuptake inhibitors (Coccaro, 1992;
are involved in inhibition of behavior. Damage Coccaro & Kavoussi, 1997; Dolan, Deakin,
to the prefrontal cortex and temporal poles or Roberts, & Anderson, 2002; Kavoussi, Liu, &
frontal-subcortical system or diffuse axonal Coccaro, 1994; Kent et al., 1988; Krakowski, 2003;
shearing can cause a behavioral disinhibition Shaikah, De Lanerolle, & Siegel, 1997; Volavka,
syndrome, often termed “pseudopsychopathic,” 2002). The research appears to support the pro-
that can surface as a combination of jocularity, posed two-part classification of violent behavior
impulsivity, behavioral dyscontrol, and sexual as either premeditated or impulsive (Barratt,
disinhibition (Benson & Miller, 1997; Mesulam, Stanford, Dowdy, Liebman, & Kent, 1999; Frick,
2000; Mills, Cassidy, & Katz, 1997). See the work 1995; A. B. Heilbrun, Heilbrun, & Heilbrun, 1978;
of Damasio and Anderson (2003) for a review of Hubbard et al., 2002; Malone et al., 1998;
clinical cases that manifested these personality Pulkkinen, 1996; Scarpa & Raine, 2000;
characteristics. Smithmyer, Hubbard, & Simons, 2000; Vitaro,
A classic example of this syndrome is found in Gendreau, Tremblay, & Oligny, 1998; Weinshenker
the case of Phineas Gage, a 19th century railroad & Siegel, 2002).
foreman who survived an explosion in which an Among the impulsively aggressive, there
iron tamping bar (about 1 m long, 3 cm wide) was appears to be substantial evidence of neurocog-
blasted up under his cheekbone and out through nitive compromise consistent with previous
the top of his head. His physician described the research regarding executive dysfunction among
change in his personality by writing that the “equi- the violent (Blair, 2001; Giancola et al., 1996;
librium . . . between his intellectual faculties and Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000; Mungus, 1988;
animal propensities seems to have been destroyed” Stanford, Greve, & Gerstle, 1997). In addition, the
(Harlow, 1868, reprinted in Macmillan, 2000, impulsively aggressive appear responsive to
p. 414). One—possibly augmented—account of treatment with certain antiseizure medications
Gage described him as “so loudmouthed, boister- (Barratt, Stanford, Felthous, & Kent, 1997;
ous, and profane that eventually ‘the police drove Houston & Stanford, 2006; Sieberer & Emrich,
him from Boston Common’” (Blackington, 1956, 2009; Stanford et al., 2001).
cited in Macmillan, 2000, p. 97). Houston, Stanford, Villemarette-Pittman,
Many researchers are implicating the prefron- Conklin, and Helfritz (2003) suggested “growing
tal and frontal lobes in violence and criminal evidence of neurobiological deficits specific to
behavior (Best, Williams, & Coccaro, 2002; impulsive aggressive behavior (i.e., reduced
Brower & Price, 2001; New et al., 2002; Raine, central serotonergic functioning, executive dys-
2002; Raine et al., 1998; Volkow et al., 1995). function, [and] prefrontal deficits) may serve as
Correspondingly, decreased executive function- markers of an ineffective behavioral control
ing appears to be associated with an increase in system in these individuals” (p. 82). The nature of
some forms of aggression (Barratt, Stanford, Kent, the deficits in those with impulsive, reactive
& Felthous, 1997; Giancola, Moss, Martin, Kirisci, aggression, in concert with apparent treatment
& Tarter, 1996; Paschall & Fishbein, 2002; benefits of antiseizure medication, may even sug-
Villemarette-Pittman, Stanford, & Greve, 2003). gest the possibility of neuroregulatory abnormal-
Electroencephalogram and evoked potential ity. Overall, there appears to be enough research
alterations have been implicated in violence as implicating a relationship between neurocogni-
well (Bars, Heyrend, Simpson, & Munger, 2001; tive functioning and aggression, particularly the
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 485

impulsive type, to indicate neuropsychology Harris, Rice, & Cormier, 2005). The VRAG is a
has a place in the evaluation of violent criminal somewhat time-consuming file review because it
defendants (Brower & Price, 2001; Barr, 2008). includes the PCL-R as one of its variables. This
weakness not withstanding, it has demonstrated
Risk Assessment Tools itself as a useful predictive tool (Quinsey et al,
Given the generally improved prediction accuracy 2005.; Rice & Harris, 1995).
of actuarial information over clinical judgment, The last risk assessment method reviewed here
several successful efforts to develop risk assess- was developed from the MacArthur Violence
ment tools have occurred (Monahan, 2003). Risk Assessment Study data and called the
The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised Classification of Violence Risk (COVR; Monahan,
(PCL-R; Hare, 1991) and the screening version Steadman, Applebaum et al., 2005). The
(PCL-SV; Hart, Cox, & Hare, 1995) have the larg- MacArthur researchers developed an iterative
est research base. There is also a more recently classification tree “approach to violence risk
adapted youth version (Forth, Kosson, & Hare, assessment . . . predicated on an interactive and
2003). The PCL-R is considered a “robust predic- contingent model of violence, one that allows
tor of future violent behavior” (Conroy, 2003), many different combinations of risk factors to
particularly when predicting violence in the com- classify a person as high or low risk” (Monahan,
munity (Walsh & Walsh, 2006). The PCL-R is a 2003, pp. 533–534). The goal was to incorporate
structured interview and file review that produces the type of information reasonably available in
20 scales loaded on two broad factors within clinical records in a manner that does not make
the conceptual domain of psychopathy (Hare, the assessment of risk arduously long. The tree
Harpur, & Hakstian, 1990). The measure requires incorporates a sequence of questions that classify
specialized training and is rather lengthy, but it is subjects as either high or low risk.
considered to have good inter-rater reliability (see Monahan and colleagues (2001) evaluated the
Melton et al., 2007). iterative classification tree in a multiple model
Webster, Douglas, Eaves, and Hart (1995) approach that combined risk assessment meth-
developed the HCR-20, which consists of 20 rat- ods. They classified all the civilly committed
ings of historical, clinical, and risk management patients in to one of five categories of risk and fol-
factors. Douglas and Webster (1999) identified lowed their progress for 20 weeks after discharge.
retrospectively that scores above the median Results of this method were favorable. In
increased the likelihood of past violence and anti- Monahan’s words (2003), “This combination of
social behavior on average by a factor of 4. models produced results not only superior to
Douglas, Ogloff, Nicholls, and Grant (1999) found those of any of its constituent models, but supe-
that scores above median increased the risk of rior to any other actuarial violence risk assess-
violence by factors of 6 to 13 in a group of civilly ment procedure reported in the literature to date”
committed patients during the 2 years after their (p. 534). Subsequently, the authors validated the
hospital discharge. The HCR-20 has also been computerized program (Monahan, Steadman,
shown effective at predicting risk of violence Robbins et al., 2005), and it is now available from
among those with intellectual deficiencies Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. One
(Lindsay et al., 2008). caveat regarding the COVR is that it has only been
The Violence Risk Appraisal Guide (VRAG; validated on psychiatric inpatients preparing for
Harris, Rice, & Quinsey, 1993) was developed in release (McCusker, 2007; Monahan et al., 2006).
Canada on a large number of men with serious Evaluating risk of violence has become increas-
criminal offenses housed in a maximum security ingly sophisticated with the development of spe-
hospital. Of 50 predictor variables, a series of cific assessment methods. Each of these methods
regression equations identified 12 variables that was developed with prisoners or psychiatric
became the VRAG. The 12 final variables are rated patients at risk for violence, and none has been
and totaled to obtain a global score which can be validated with neurological patients. Logic would
compared to a table of probabilities for violence dictate a similar profile of risk-increasing vari-
recidivism over seven- and ten-year periods. ables between the two populations (particularly
Considerable amount of research now exists sug- with the apparent prevalence rate for neurocog-
gesting the VRAG has moderate to high correla- nitive compromise in violent offenders; Martell,
tions to violent recidivism, with Receiver Operator 1992), but ultimately this remains an empirical
Characteristic AUCs of 0.70 to 0.85 (Quinsey, question. Until that research materializes,
486 forensic neuropsychology

neuropsychologists who practice risk assessment Defendant Ford was convicted of a capital offense
in the criminal setting must rely on clinical judg- and sentenced to death. While on death row, the
ment predicated on other, established nonneuro- issue of his mental health and competency arose.
logical risk factors. The Florida state procedure at that time did not
allow an adversarial or judicial determination of
D E AT H P E N A LT Y I S S U E S competency because it was determined by the
Nowhere does the need for competent and ethical governor with the assistance of a panel of mental
practice demonstrate itself greater than in the health professionals. In ruling Florida’s procedure
practice of capital cases. As with any criminal unconstitutional, the U.S. Supreme Court also
case, neuropsychologists can be called on to pro- held that executing a person not competent
vide expertise at any phase of the process; none- because of mental illness is a violation of the 8th
theless, there are unique issues in capital cases Amendment right to be free from cruel and
that go beyond the obvious life-or-death outcome. unusual punishment. The Court concluded that
These issues include a priori ethical consider- doing so (a) has questionable retributive value,
ations, competency to be executed and to waive (b) has no deterrence value, and (c) simply affronts
appeals, age requirements for imposing a death humanity. Fascinating quotations that give insight
sentence, mental retardation, and death penalty to the reasoning behind this decision include the
mitigation. need to protect the “condemned from fear and
pain without the comfort of understanding” and
A Priori Ethical Considerations the need to protect the “dignity of society from
Before accepting the responsibility of practicing barbarity.”
in capital cases, the neuropsychologist must con- In deciding Ford, the U.S. Supreme Court did
sider his or her personal position on the death not address the specific aspects of competency to
penalty in general. Ethical standards dictate that be executed much beyond the basic Dusky require-
psychologists recognize when personal views have ments (Dusky v. United States, 1960; see chapter
the potential to interfere with our professional 16). The Court referenced Florida’s standard that
work. As I reviewed in chapter 16, the roles of the person must have the mental capacity to
forensic evaluators are different from that of clini- understand the nature of the death penalty and
cal providers. There are times when our decisions why it was imposed. Justice Powell, in the concur-
may have untoward implications for criminal ring opinion, pointed out that a person needs to
defendants. Each neuropsychologist must objec- understand the connection between the crime
tively evaluate his or her feelings toward the death and the punishment. Most other jurisdictions that
penalty and decide if he or she is capable of accept- allow execution have more detailed standards. An
ing the possibility of providing an opinion that, in example description is presented in Reisner and
some aspect, might pave the way for an execution Slobogin (1990); the test of competency is whether
of a human being (Denney, 2005). the prisoner lacks, as a result of:
For example, forensic evaluators are occasion-
ally requested to provide an opinion on whether a defects of his faculties, sufficient intelligence to
criminal defendant is competent to be executed. understand the nature of the proceedings
A neuropsychologist should not accept this role if against him, what he was tried for, the purpose
unwilling to provide an affirmative opinion if the of his punishment, the impending fate which
data support such a conclusion. As in any other awaits him, a sufficient understanding to know
forensic endeavor, forensic practitioners need to any fact which might exist which would make
remain objective in their roles and remember the his punishment unjust or unlawful, and the
client in this instance is the court, not the defen- intelligence requisite to convey such informa-
dant. In my opinion, clinicians who are strongly tion to his attorneys or the court (p. 946).
opposed to the death penalty have too great a pos-
sibility of inadvertent bias to participate in capital This more detailed inquiry suggests the prisoner
cases as nonpartisan evaluators. must have capacity to assist in any potential
appeals in addition to an understanding of his
Competence to Be Executed current legal situation (as indicated in Dusky) and
In 1986, the U.S. Supreme Court ruled in a com- an appreciation of impending death. Heilbrun
plicated and lengthy criminal due process and and colleagues (Heilbrun, 1987; Heilbrun &
competency case called Ford v. Wainwright. McClaren, 1988; Heilbrun, Radelet, & Dvoskin,
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 487

1992) suggested the ability to prepare for death International, 2001). This situation becomes
spiritually and psychologically as a needed com- complicated because it is not uncommon for
ponent of competency as well. prisoners to develop mental health concerns
Related to the issue of evaluating competency while on death row (Cunningham & Goldstein,
for execution is the controversial concern over the 2003). While rare, suicide is not unheard of for
ethical appropriateness of providing treatment for inmates living on death row. Three of 3,254 pris-
condemned prisoners for the purpose of restoring oners held on death row in 36 states and the fed-
their competency to be executed. The issue has eral prison system died by suicide in 2005 (U.S.
been debated at length (Ferris, 1997; Leong, Department of Justice, 2005). It is not surprising
Weinstock, Silva, & Eth, 1993). The American that appeals courts request mental health evalua-
Medical Association (1995) put forth a statement tion for prisoners wishing to waive their appeals.
that providing treatment for the restoration of There is no unique standard for waiving death
competence for such reasons is ethically unac- penalty appeals beyond that generally spelled out
ceptable unless it is to relieve extreme suffering. in case law. It can be deduced from Dusky v.
Bonnie (1990) outlined an analysis that revealed United States (1960), Godinez v. Moran (1993),
the issue as a complex one with no easy answers. and North Carolina v. Alford (1970) that individu-
Much of the concern pertains to the greater als must have a factual and rational understand-
harm in facilitating a person’s death versus reliev- ing of their situation and are waiving their appeal
ing suffering. There are instances when individu- in a knowing, intelligent, and voluntary manner
als would rather receive treatment and die a sane (see chapter 16 for a discussion of this aspect of
person than languish away on death row in a state competency). In addition to other possible mental
of mental deterioration (see Singleton v. Norris, health concerns, it is important for the mental
1997, Heaney concurring; Singleton v. Norris, health evaluator to rule out depression as the pre-
2003). The issue could be resolved by adopting the cipitating cause for such a waiver.
recommendation of commuting a death row
inmate’s sentence to life without parole if the Death Penalty and
inmate is found incompetent for execution Mental Retardation
(Wexler & Winick, 1991). Of course, this decision In 2002, the U.S. Supreme Court decided the case
could raise the potential for malingering to new of Atkins v. Virginia, a case spelling out that it is
heights. excessive to execute a mentally retarded individ-
Whether one should provide competency ual. It is rare for the High Court to overturn its
restoration treatment to death row inmates is an own previous decision, but it did just that in
issue that will likely continue in debate for many Atkins. The court previously addressed this issue
years. Until some definitive guidance is provided in Penry v. Lynaugh (1989). In Penry, the court
in this thorny ethical area, it is doubly incumbent decided it was not necessarily “cruel and unusual
for neuropsychologists to be aware of their views punishment” to execute a mentally retarded indi-
and the potential impact these views can have on vidual based on an analysis of societal views at
professional opinions in capital cases. that time as evidenced by state laws. Societal views
dictate the “evolving standards of decency” as it
Competency to Waive Death relates to cruel and unusual. Because very few
Penalty Appeals states had prohibitions against executing the men-
As of December 31, 2009, death row inmates spent tally retarded, the court concluded the prevailing
an average of 169 months on death row (U.S. societal thought suggested it was not so extreme
Department of Justice, 2009). It is not uncommon as to be considered cruel and unusual. The court
under prolonged housing on the unique setting of did spell out, however, that the cruel and unusual
death row for individuals to grow weary of the clause of the 8th Amendment requires a case-by-
ongoing appeals process. In death penalty cases, case analysis. In the case of mental retardation,
prisoners receive legal representation even when the jury must be able to consider the impact of
they do not wish it; consequently, they can find mental retardation evidence in its decision about
themselves in a position to refuse appeals filed on sentencing. The Penry case was a controversial
their behalf. Refusing further appeal amounts to 5-to-4 decision, so it was not surprising to see the
the prisoner “volunteering” for execution. Just issue addressed again in Atkins.
over 12% of those executed between 1977 and During August 1996, Daryl Atkins and an
2001 chose to drop their appeals (Amnesty accomplice abducted Eric Nesbitt, robbed him,
488 forensic neuropsychology

forced him to withdraw an additional $200 from uncertainty as to whether either justification
an ATM, and then drove him to an isolated loca- underpinning the death penalty—retribution and
tion, where he was shot eight times and killed. deterrence of capital crimes—applies to mentally
Atkins was convicted of abduction, armed rob- retarded offenders. With respect to retribution,
bery, and capital murder. During the penalty the severity of the appropriate punishment neces-
phase of his trial, a psychologist for the defense sarily depends on the offender’s culpability. If the
testified that he had a full-scale IQ of 59 and con- culpability of the average murderer is insufficient
cluded he was mildly mentally retarded. A rebut- to justify imposition of the death penalty, the
tal psychiatrist testified he was a psychopath and reduced culpability of the mentally retarded
not mentally retarded. The jury, nonetheless, sen- criminal surely does not merit that form of retri-
tenced Atkins to death. The Supreme Court of bution. Regarding deterrence, the same cognitive
Virginia affirmed the judgment of the trial court, and behavioral deficits that lessen the culpability
and the case was appealed to the U.S. Supreme of mentally retarded defendants also make it less
Court. likely that they can control their conduct based on
In July 2002, the U.S. Supreme Court reversed the possibility of execution. Second, mentally
the judgment of the Virginia Supreme Court and retarded offenders face an increased risk for
remanded the case back to the sentencing court to wrongful execution because they may unwittingly
consider sentencing other than the death penalty. provide false confessions, be less able to provide
In coming to this conclusion, the U.S. Supreme meaningful assistance to their counsel, are typi-
Court reevaluated society’s perspective on the cally poorer witnesses, and may inadvertently
issue in a similar fashion as was done in Penry. foster the perception they lack remorse for their
The court outlined that a punishment is excessive crimes based on their demeanor in court.
and therefore prohibited by the 8th Amendment The Atkins (2002) decision makes the diag-
as cruel and unusual if it is not “graduated and nostic aspect of forensic assessment in capital
proportioned to the offense.” A claim that a judg- cases even more important. Although each state
ment is excessive must be judged by the standards must follow the Atkins decision, the U.S. Supreme
that currently prevail. Society’s evolving standards Court did not spell out the specifics of how mental
of decency must allow for a proportionality review retardation is defined or, for that matter, how
that is informed by “objective factors to the maxi- much mental retardation is needed to obviate the
mum possible extent.” death penalty. The Court left these details up to
As in Penry, the Court viewed legislation the states to decide. In addition to sub-average
enacted by the country’s legislatures to be the intelligence, the diagnosis requires impairments
clearest and most reliable objective evidence of in adaptive functioning and evidence of the con-
contemporary values. At the time of Penry, there dition prior to age 18 years. It is not difficult to
were only two states with statutes against execut- fathom a situation in which an individual is raised
ing the mentally retarded. Although an additional in an extremely rural setting with no access to
16 states enacted statutes prohibiting the execu- special education assessments or classes. In this
tion of mentally retarded offenders since Penry, “It situation, there may be little evidence of sub-
is not so much the number of these states that is average intelligence prior to the age of 18 years.
significant, but the consistency of the direction of For individuals who have been incarcerated since
the change.” The Court further concluded that they were young juveniles, adaptive functioning
mental retardation does not necessarily eliminate may be rather difficult to assess. Common mea-
a person’s ability to appreciate right from wrong, sures of adaptive functioning have not been vali-
but the condition causes diminished capacities to dated in the correctional environment and do not
understand and process information, communi- appear to translate well.
cate, abstract from mistakes and learn from expe- Last, what is the evaluator to do with illegal
rience, engage in logical reasoning, control aliens and those from other countries and
impulses, and understand the reaction of others. cultures? These concerns place an additional
The Court noted “their deficiencies do not warrant burden on the evaluator and the profession’s
an exemption from criminal sanctions, but they measurement tools. It is absolutely imperative
do diminish their personal culpability” (p. 348). that evaluations done in capital cases proceed
The U.S. Supreme Court considered two with utmost care and consideration, particularly
additional reasons to agree with the legislative before empirically derived methods to address
consensus. First, the Court maintained there is these concerns are developed.
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 489

Death Penalty Mitigation (f) The defendant acted under duress or


In 1972, the U.S. Supreme Court considered the under the domination of another person.
death penalty in the United States at that time to (g) At the time of the murder, the capacity of
be cruel and unusual punishment because the the defendant to appreciate the criminality
manner in which capital sentences were decided [wrongfulness] of his conduct or to
in Georgia was capricious (Furman v. Georgia, conform his conduct to the requirements
1972). This decision discontinued death penalty of law was impaired as a result of mental
litigation in the United States at the time because disease or defect or intoxication.
none of the states had a system that was substan-
tially different. The death penalty statutes were The defense is certainly not limited to this partic-
rewritten by 35 states to correct the problem ular list of mitigating facts. In Lockett v. Ohio
(Latzer, 1998). In 2002, the U.S. Supreme Court (1978), the U.S. Supreme Court held the trier of
determined that juries, rather judges, must make fact could “not be precluded from considering as
the decision whether or not to impose a death a mitigating factor, any aspect of the defendant’s
sentence (Ring v. Arizona, 2002). character or record and any circumstances of the
The U.S. Supreme Court accepted as constitu- offense that the defendant proffers as a basis for a
tional Georgia’s rewrite of their state statute (Gregg sentence of less than death” (p. 604). Regarding
v. Georgia, 1976). Georgia’s capital sentencing this decision, Cunningham and Goldstein (2003)
process included the presentation of aggravating noted these possibilities for mitigation:
and mitigating factors. With the exception of trea-
son and aircraft hijacking, it required at least one Any information regarding the defendant’s
of ten specified aggravating circumstances to be background, as a child or as an adult, could be
established beyond reasonable doubt to impose considered relevant. Thus, factors such as a his-
the death penalty. The process also allowed the tory of childhood trauma (e.g., physical or
defense to introduce mitigating circumstances, sexual abuse), verbal abuse, exposure to drugs
but the jury was not bound to cite any particular and alcohol, neglect and abandonment, undi-
mitigating fact to make the binding recommenda- agnosed or misdiagnosed conditions (e.g.,
tion against the death penalty. The Court noted, mental retardation, emotional disturbance,
“No longer can a jury wantonly and freakishly learning disability, attention deficit/hyperac-
impose the death sentence; it is always circum- tivity disorder), gang or cult membership, wit-
scribed by the legislative guidelines” (Gregg v. nessing a death of a family member or friend
Georgia, p. 893). could be considered nonstatutory mitigation.
Regarding mitigating circumstances, the U.S. In addition, any circumstances related to the
Supreme Court referenced the American Law crime could be considered mitigating. Such
Institute’s Model Penal Code (Tent. Draft No. 9, nonstatutory factors as the minor role played
1959, comment 3, p. 71) list of potential facts: by the defendant in the crime, his or her sug-
gestibility quiescence to authority, perceived
(a) The defendant has no significant history coercion, a sense desperation based on real or
of prior criminal activity. imaginary beliefs, or a need for self-perceived
(b) The murder was committed while the moral retribution could be introduced as non-
defendant was under the influence of statutory mitigators. Lockett requires defense
extreme mental or emotional disturbance. attorneys, forensic psychological and psychiat-
ric experts to explore all avenues of mitigation
(c) The victim was a participant in the
because the factors that can be introduced are
defendant’s homicidal conduct or
not limited to those specifically delineated by
consented to the homicidal act.
statute (p. 412, emphasis in the original).
(d) The murder was committed under
circumstances which the defendant The neuropsychologist contemplating death pen-
believed to provide a moral justification alty mitigation work is referred to the work of
or extenuation for his conduct. Cunningham and Goldstein (2003); Reynolds,
(e) The defendant was an accomplice in a Price, and Niland (2003); and Heilbronner
murder committed by another person and Waller (2008) for a thorough review of impor-
and his participation in the homicidal act tant aspects of this work. The time involvement
was relatively minor. in death penalty mitigation is certainly not
490 forensic neuropsychology

consistent with common neuropsychological, or Forensic Psychology,” issues of confidentiality, and


even forensic, work (see the survey of Sweet, 5th Amendment protections.
Peck, Abramowitz, & Etzweiler, 2002, in which
clinical interview and history time in minutes Specialty Guidelines for
was M = 57, SD = 27). Cunningham and Goldstein Forensic Psychology
note, “Obtaining a history in this comprehensive The Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychology
detail routinely requires 8 to 20 hours of interview (“Guidelines” hereafter) are currently in draft
with the defendant, exclusive of any psychological form. The original document had a slightly differ-
testing” (p. 422). Although in many instances ent name “Specialty Guidelines for Forensic
this amount of interviewing can be done by Psychologists” (Committee on Ethical Guidelines,
a mitigation specialist or other forensic specialist 1991). While these 1991 “Guidelines” are still in
and incorporated in to the neuropsychological effect, they have been going through the revision
evaluation (Heilbronner & Waller, 2008). process since 2002. The current draft, dated March
Cunningham and Goldstein also note neuro- 18, 2011, as well as any more current drafts, is
psychological and neurological assessments are available at http://www.ap-ls.org/links/profes-
“indicated” in most cases because, first, such sionalsgfp.html (Committee on the Revision of
findings could have a significant mitigating effect, the Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychology,
and second, there is a growing body of research 2008). The “Guidelines” are jointly developed and
implicating neurocognitive compromise in vio- adopted by Division 41 of the American
lent offenders. Psychological Association (American Psychology–
Neuropsychological assessment in death Law Society) and the American Academy of
penalty litigation could also focus on aggravating Forensic Psychology (members of the forensic
factors, but in this regard it is likely limited to board affiliated with the American Board of
addressing increased risk of dangerousness. Forensic Psychology). The 2011 “Guidelines” are
Other aggravating factors are uniquely not related meant to be consistent with the Ethical Principles
to mental health (e.g., particularly heinous crime, of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as well as
killing in association with another crime, etc.). “improve the quality of forensic psychological
Neuropsychologists can evaluate capital defen- services; enhance the practice and facilitate the
dants to demonstrate the absence of mitigating systematic development of forensic psychology;
factors (i.e., rebut a defense expert). It is important encourage a high level of quality in professional
for evaluators to remain objective regardless of practice; and encourage forensic practitioners to
who requests their services. Death penalty mitiga- acknowledge and respect the rights of those they
tion is truly a unique area of practice and one that serve” (p. 2).
requires the utmost competence, integrity, and The “Guidelines” are aspirational in nature
ethical conduct. As Cunningham and Goldstein and serve to guide the psychologist when dealing
(2003) pointed out, “In no situation is professional with forensic issues. The importance of this cannot
competence more important than in death be overstated. The “Guidelines” are not meant
penalty cases . . . it is imperative to present find- as guidance only for those professionals who
ings in a thorough, objective fashion” (p. 416). consider themselves “forensic psychologists.” The
introduction provides this summary:
ETHICAL AND
PROFES SIONAL IS SUES IN For the purposes of these Guidelines, forensic
CRIMINAL FORENSICS psychology refers to professional practice by
The unique aspect of practicing neuropsychology any psychologist working within any sub-
in the criminal forensic setting carries with it a discipline of psychology (e.g., clinical, develop-
unique set of ethical concerns. As this volume mental, social, cognitive) when applying the
contains a chapter specifically covering ethical scientific, technical, or specialized knowledge
considerations in forensic neuropsychological of psychology to the law to assist in addressing
practice, I focus on only those issues specific to legal, contractual, and administrative matters.
the criminal forensic setting. In addition to famil- Application of the Guidelines does not depend
iarity with the “Ethical Principles of Psychologists on the practitioner’s typical areas of practice or
and Code of Conduct” (American Psychological expertise, but rather on the service provided in
Association, 2002), neuropsychologists need to be the case at hand. . . . Such professional conduct
fluently aware of the “Specialty Guidelines for is considered forensic from the time the
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 491

practitioner reasonably expects to, agrees to, or competency to stand trial, but he said nothing
is legally mandated to, provide expertise on an about any future testimony on other potential
explicitly psycholegal issue. (p. 2) issues. Subsequent to the evaluation, Smith was
found competent to proceed and convicted of
The “Guidelines” were not written to apply to capital murder. Texas maintains a bifurcated
psychologists asked to provide professional capital trial process by which the defendant
psychological services when the psychologist was undergoes trial regarding guilt initially, and then
not aware at the time of the service that it would he or she undergoes another trial by the same jury
become forensic in nature. For these individuals, regarding whether to impose the death penalty.
however, they may be helpful in preparing to For the jury to impose a death penalty, the prose-
communicate professional opinions in the foren- cution must demonstrate potential for future vio-
sic arena. See the work of Denney (2005) for an lence. During the sentencing phase of Smith’s
application of the “Guidelines” to two neuropsy- case, Dr. Grigson testified that the man was
chology cases completed in the criminal forensic a psychopath and likely to kill again. The jury
setting. Bush, Connell, and Denney (2006) also sentenced Smith to death. The ethical concern
provide a model of ethical decision-making when here is that Dr. Grigson did not provide Smith
participating in the forensic arena. It is important with a full disclosure of potential uses for the
for neuropsychologists practicing in the criminal information gained in the competency evaluation.
forensic arena to be familiar with the “Guidelines.” The 1991 “Guidelines” addressed this issue at
The “Guidelines” cover many more aspects of IV.E.3:
forensic practice, but I cover just two critical
areas in the criminal setting, confidentiality and After a psychologist has advised the subject of
protecting criminal defendant rights. a clinical forensic evaluation of the intended
uses of the evaluation and its work product, the
Confidentiality in Criminal Forensics psychologist may not use the evaluation work
A correct understanding of confidentiality is product for other purposes without explicit
imperative in the criminal setting. Some jurisdic- waiver to do so by the client or the client’s legal
tions provide confidentiality between the evalua- representative.
tor and defendant under the “work product rule”
as set out by case law (United States v. Alvarez, The issue is less clearly stated in the 2011
1975). Other jurisdictions do not provide for “Guidelines,” but they are present under section 6
mental health evaluation confidentiality; in other “Informed Consent, Notification and Assent”:
words, the fact of the evaluation and the evalua-
tor’s opinion are discoverable even if there was no Because substantial rights, liberties, and prop-
report written and the requesting attorney does erties are often at risk in forensic matters and
not wish testimony (Edney v. Smith, 1976). When because the methods and procedures of forensic
providing evaluations as a result of a direct court practitioners are complex and may not be accu-
order, confidentiality does not exist. It is impera- rately anticipated by the recipients of forensic
tive for the evaluator to understand the rule in use services, forensic practitioners strive to inform
within that case jurisdiction. The evaluator must service recipients about the nature and param-
describe his or her understanding of the use of the eters of the services to be provided. (p. 9)
information to the defendant. This difficulty arises
when a clinician evaluates competency to stand Additional clarification comes at section 6.03 with
trial, but the clinician’s testimony is requested for the words, “including potential consequences of
issues of rebutting an insanity defense or, worse, participation” (p. 10). Neuropsychologists must
to provide an opinion regarding potential aggra- be cognizant of criminal defendant’s rights regard-
vating issues prior to sentencing. Such an occur- ing limits of confidentiality and informed assent.
rence is not unusual in death penalty cases and Related to this specific event is the duty to watch
has been the issue of U.S. Supreme Court rulings out for the constitutional rights of criminal
(e.g., Estelle v. Smith, 1981). defendants.
Estelle v. Smith (1981) is another case in which
James Grigson, MD, provided testimony. The dif- Fifth Amendment Protections
ficulty in this case was that Dr. Grigson may have In the example in the preceding section, the U.S.
informed Smith that he was evaluating him on his Supreme Court held that Smith’s 5th Amendment
492 forensic neuropsychology

right to be protected from self-incrimination was examiner. Relatedly, it is important for examiners
violated by allowing Dr. Grigson to testify on the to limit their opinions in written reports to strictly
issue of future dangerousness at the penalty phase the referral question and supporting conclusion
of the proceedings. The High Court noted: as well. Additional non-requested opinions in
forensic reports can play havoc on the legal pro-
When Dr. Grigson went beyond simply report- ceedings. Forensic examiners are asked to provide
ing to the court on the issue of competence and expert opinions about very specific issues; well-
testified for the prosecution at the penalty trained and competent forensic psychologists
phase on the crucial issue of respondent’s realize this fact.
future dangerousness, his role changed and
became essentially like that of an agent of the Training and Certification in
state recounting unwarned statements made in Criminal Forensics
a post-arrest custodial setting. It is hoped this chapter and chapter 16 have
made it clear that performing neuropsychological
Even though he warned the defendant about the evaluations in the criminal forensic arena is a
limits of confidentiality, Dr. Grigson provided unique endeavor that requires a unique knowl-
him with no indication the information used in edge domain. The “Ethical Principles” make it
the competency evaluation could be used against clear that psychologists do not perform activities
him at a future penalty proceeding. Actions that for which they are not adequately trained. The
cause an infringement on a defendant’s constitu- amount of specialized forensic training required
tional rights, such as was done here, not only for ethical and competent practice is not clear.
violate the “Ethical Principles” (Principle E as well The issue was preliminarily addressed by Otto and
as Informed Consent), but also are counter to the Heilbrun (2002). Some states have developed
goals aspired to in the “Guidelines.” minimal requirements for criminal forensic prac-
The “Ethical Principles” also point out psy- tice (Farkas, DeLeon, & Newman, 1997). Often,
chologists’ responsibility to avoid harm. It is clear these requirements include a few hours attending
that, in the case of Estelle v. Smith (1981), Dr. state-sponsored educational events with varying
Grigson did not avoid harm. One could argue that amounts of mandated supervision.
providing potentially damaging testimony in any Sullivan and Denney (2003) present basic and
capital case is tantamount to causing harm. By aspirational requirements for competent criminal
that reasoning, any unbiased ethical forensic work forensic neuropsychological practice. In discuss-
could be considered unethical. Clearly, this is not ing “authentic professional competence,” Sullivan
the case. There is a difference between providing and Denney (2008) expand their ideas by incor-
an unfavorable opinion and violating a defen- porating “Entry, Proficiency, and Specialty” levels
dant’s constitutional rights. It is important to keep of competence in forensic matters based upon the
this distinction clear (Denney, 2005; Denney & Villanova Conference (Bersoff et al., 1997). They
Wynkoop, 2000; Heilbrun, 2001). note that not all neuropsychologists will desire to
It is generally important for forensic examin- seek “specialty” level of practice within the sub-
ers to limit the scope of their opinions to the refer- specialty of forensic neuropsychology, but aware-
ral questions and the related and supporting ness of these different levels will protect the
conclusions. In the example, Dr. Grigson did not clinical from practicing beyond their authentic
limit his opinion to competency. When asked to level of professional competence.
testify at the sentencing phase, the safest course of No one really knows the exact point at which
action would have been to refuse to do so based clinical neuropsychological practice ends and
on the fact he did not provide Smith with a warn- criminal forensic practice begins. The “Guidelines”
ing that information derived from the evaluation suggest criminal forensic work begins when one
could be used against him in a sentencing pro- knowingly accepts such a case. In addition to
ceeding (Denney, 2005). In addition, an evalua- Sullivan and Denney (2003, 2008), Heilbronner
tion regarding dangerousness is an entirely and Frumkin (2003) raised the issue of this “divid-
different endeavor with a potentially substantial ing line” and make a recommendation for one
difference in data sources and evaluative mea- potential model of practice. It is good that these
sures. Dr. Grigson should have recommended issues are beginning to be discussed in the profes-
that the attorney requesting such testimony sional literature. Time will reveal how well such
seek another evaluation from an independent suggestions are accepted.
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 493

Neuropsychologists interested in developing Neuropsychologists entering the arena of forensic


criminal forensic practices can begin acquiring practice need to prepare adequately for the
knowledge content from texts and seminars. I heightened scrutiny that comes with the court of
strongly recommend Psychological Evaluations for law (Sullivan & Denney, 2003, 2008). Attempting
the Courts (Melton et al., 2007). It is an astounding to bypass this preparation by presenting oneself as
text that covers nearly the entirety of forensic prac- competent based on a vanity board is misrepre-
tice, both civil and criminal. This endorsement is sentation to the public. Although the public, and
not intended to mean there are no other good many in the judicial system, will not recognize
texts; there are. Denney and Sullivan (2008) is the this ruse, misrepresentation nonetheless remains
first text devoted to the application of clinical neu- ethically inappropriate (American Psychological
ropsychology to the criminal forensic arena. Association, 2002; Committee on Ethical
Seminar training can be obtained from Guidelines, 1991; Committee on the Revision of
national workshops by the AAFP. These work- the Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychology,
shops are uniformly strong in content and 2011).
presented at various times during the year and in
various locations around the country. Supervision S U M M A RY
is also a necessary component of training in This chapter builds on chapter 16 by reviewing
criminal forensics. I recommend receiving major areas of importance to neuropsychological
supervision from an individual who has proven practice in the criminal forensic arena. Covered
competency to provide psychological services were the nature of the forensic mental health eval-
in the criminal forensic setting. Attainment of uation in general, criminal responsibility, sentenc-
board certification in forensic psychology by the ing issues, risk assessment, competency to be
American Board of Forensic Psychology is executed, death penalty mitigation, and impor-
the clearest example of this achievement and tant professional and ethical concerns. The chap-
would represent the “specialty” level of practice ter dealt strictly with adult criminal proceedings.
(Bersoff et al., 1997; Sullivan & Denney, 2008). There is a host of other issues inherent in juvenile
I admonish eager neuropsychologists to be wary work. Although there is a considerable body of
of board-certifying bodies that bestow “diplo- literature dealing with psychological evaluation of
mate” status with the ease of signing a credit individuals in the juvenile justice system (see
card (so-called vanity boards and checkbook Grisso’s work), the first step in application of clin-
credentials). These bodies have attempted to ical neuropsychology and neurodevelopmental
improve their appearance by including multiple issues to that population has only recently begun
choice examinations, but still do not require face- (Grisso et al., 2003; Wynkoop, 2003; Wynkoop,
to-face, oral examinations of knowledge base 2008).
and practice skill at the hands of documented The application of clinical neuropsychology to
experts in the field. Otto and Heilbrun (2002) and assist parties in the criminal justice system is a
Fisher (2008) present clear and even poignant unique practice opportunity. It requires a dis-
discussion on this issue. Last, the National tinctly different knowledge base in addition to
Benchbook on Psychiatric and Psychological solid, empirically based, clinical neuropsycho-
Evidence and Testimony (Parry, 1998), a judge’s logical knowledge and skill. Neuropsychologists
handbook, addresses the issue of board certifica- interested in working in the criminal forensic
tion and vanity boards specifically: setting are encouraged to acquire that knowledge
base and skill in a manner sufficient for compe-
The court should be aware that while specialty tent and ethical practice of an applied empirical
board certification is prestigious within the science.
various professions, witnesses who have not
obtained this level of credentialing are not N OT E S
automatically presumed to be deficient by Opinions expressed in this chapter are those of the
comparison, nor are they unsuited per se to author and do not necessarily represent the position of
provide expert testimony. Judges must also be the Federal Bureau of Prisons or the U.S. Department
wary of witnesses claiming certification by of Justice.
various “mail-order” boards, that do not 1. There are several variants in spelling for
require an oral examination or other indicia of M’Naghten. Moran (1981), in his 18-month period of
a rigorous qualifying process. (p. 55) research, found documents that appeared to indicate
494 forensic neuropsychology

the man actually spelled his name “McNaughtan,” but M. J. Farah (Eds.), Behavioral neurology and neu-
M’Naghten is used here because it was used in the orig- ropsychology (pp. 401–408). New York: McGraw–
inal legal case. Hill.
2. Formally adopted by the ALI on May 24, 1962. Bersoff, D. N., Goodman-Delahunty, J., Grisso, J. T.,
3. Very loosely drawn from Texas v. Andrea Yates Hans, V. P., Poythress, N. G., & Roesch, R. G.
(2003). (1997). Training in law and psychology: Models
from the Villanova Conference. American
REFERENCES Psychologist, 52, 1301–1310.
American Law Institute. (1955). Model Penal Code Best, M., Williams, J. M., & Coccaro, E. F. (2002). Evidence
Section 4.01(1)(2), (Tent. Draft No. 4). Philadelphia: for a dysfunctional prefrontal circuit in patient with
Author. an impulsive aggressive disorder. Proceedings of the
American Law Institute. (1959). Model Penal Code (Tent. National Academy of Science, 99, 8448–53.
Draft No. 9, comment 3). Philadelphia: Author. Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggres-
American Medical Association Council on Ethical and sion, the antisocial, personality disorders, and psy-
Judicial Affairs. (1995). Physician participation in chopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
capital punishment: Evaluation of prisoner compe- Psychiatry, 71, 727–731.
tence to be executed: Treatment to restore compe- Bonnie, R. J. (1990). Dilemmas in administering the
tence to be executed. CEJA Report 1995, Section death penalty: Conscientious abstention, profes-
6-A-95. Chicago: Author sional ethics, and the needs of the legal system.
American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical Law and Human Behavior, 14, 67–90.
principles of psychologists and code of conduct. Bonta, J., Law, M., & Hanson, R. K. (1998). The predic-
American Psychologist, 57, 1060–73. tion of criminal and violent recidivism among
Amnesty International. (2001, April). The illusion of mentally disordered offenders: A meta-analysis.
control. New York: Author. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 123–42.
Atkins v. Virginia, 153 L. Ed 2d 335 (2002). Borum, R. & Grisso, T. (1995). Psychological test use in
Barefoot v. Estelle, 103 S.Ct. 3383 (1983). criminal forensic evaluations. Professional
Barnard, P. G. (1998). Diminished capacity and autom- Psychology: Research and Practice, 26, 465–73.
atism as a defense. American Journal of Forensic Brower, M. C. & Price, B. H. (2001). Neuropsychiatry
Psychology, 16, 27–62. of frontal lobe dysfunction in violent and criminal
Barr, W. B. (2008). Neuropsychological approaches to behaviour: A critical review. Journal of Neurology,
criminality and violence. In R. L. Denney & J. P. Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 71, 720–26.
Sullivan (Eds), Clinical neuropsychology in the Bush, S. S., Connell, M. A., & Denney, R. L. (2006).
criminal forensic setting (pp. 238–72). New York: Ethical Issues in Forensic Psychology: Key Concepts
Guilford Press. and Resources. Washington, DC: American
Barratt, E. S., Stanford, M. S., Dowdy, L., Liebman, M. Psychological Association.
J., & Kent, T. A. (1999). Impulsive and premedi- Clark, C. R. (1999). Specific intent and diminished
tated aggression: A factor analysis of self-reported capacity. In I. B. Weiner & A. K. Hess (Eds.),
acts. Psychiatry Research, 86, 163–73. Handbook of forensic psychology (2nd ed., pp.
Barratt, E. S., Stanford, M. S., Felthous, A., & Kent, T. A. 350–78). New York: Wiley.
(1997). The effects of phenytoin on impulsive and Clark v. Arizona, 548 U.S. 735 (2006).
premeditated aggression: A controlled study. Journal Coccaro, E. F. (1992). Impulsive aggression and
of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17, 341–49. central serotonergic system function in humans:
Barratt, E. S., Stanford, M. S., Kent, T. A., & Felthous, An example of a dimensional brain-behavior rela-
A. (1997). Neuropsychological and cognitive psy- tionship. International Clinical Psychopharma-
chophysiological substrates of impulsive aggres- cology, 7, 3–12.
sion. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 1045–47. Coccaro, E. F. & Kavoussi, R. J. (1997). Fluoxetine and
Bars, D. R., Heyrend, F. L., Simpson, C. D., & Munger, impulsive aggressive behavior in personality-
J. C. (2001). Use of visual evoked-potential studies disordered subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry,
and EEG data to classify aggressive, explosive 54, 1081–88.
behavior of youths. Psychiatric Services, 52, 81–86. Cochrane, R. E., Grisso, T., & Frederick, R. I. (2001).
Bauer, A., Rosca, P., Khawalled, R., Grinshpoon, A., & The relationship between criminal charges, diag-
Gruzniewski, A. (2003). Dangerousness and risk noses, and psycholegal opinions among federal
assessment: The state of the art. Israel Journal of pretrial defendants. Behavioral Sciences and the
Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 40, 182–90. Law, 19, 565–82.
Benson, D. F. & Miller, B. L. (1997). Frontal lobes: Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic
Clinical and anatomic aspects. In T. E. Feinberg & Psychologists. (1991). Specialty guidelines for
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 495

forensic psychologists. Law and Human Behavior, Drake, M. E., Pakalnis, A., Brown, M. E., & Hietter, S.
15, 655–65. Retrieved August 1, 2009, from http:// A. (1988). Auditory event related potentials in vio-
www.aafp.ws/pdfs/foren.pdf lent and nonviolent prisoners. European Archives
Committee on the Revision of the Specialty Guidelines of Psychiatry and Neurological Sciences, 238, 7–10.
for Forensic Psychology. (2011). Specialty guide- Durham v. United States, 214 F.2d 862 (D.C. Cir.
lines for forensic psychology. March 11, 2011 Draft. 1954).
Retrieved July 16, 2011, from http://www.ap-ls. Dusky v. United States, 362 U.S. 402 (1960).
org/aboutpsychlaw/3182011sgfpdraft.pdf Edney v. Smith, 425 F. Supp. 1038 (E.D.N.Y. 1976).
Conklin, S. M. & Stanford, M. S. (2002). Differences in Estelle v. Smith, 451 U.S. 454 (1981).
the late positive potential during an affective pic- Evans, J. R. (1997). Quantitative EEG findings in a
ture task in impulsive aggressive individuals group of death row inmates. Archives of Clinical
[abstract]. Psychophysiology, 39(Suppl.), S27. Neurology, 12, 315–316.
Conroy, M. A. (2003). Evaluation of sexual predators. Farkas, G., DeLeon, P., & Newman, R. (1997). Sanity
In I. B. Weiner (Series Ed.) & A. M. Goldstein (Vol. examiner certification: An evolving national
Ed.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. 11: Forensic agenda. Professional Psychology: Research and
psychology (pp. 463–84). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Practice, 28, 73–76.
Cunningham, M. D. & Goldstein, A. M. (2003). Favole, R. J. (1983) Mental disability in the American
Sentencing determinations in death penalty cases. criminal process: A four issue survey. In J. Monahan
In I. B. Weiner (Series Ed.) & A. M. Goldstein & H. Steadman (Eds.), Mentally disordered offend-
(Vol. Ed.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. 11: Forensic ers: Perspectives from law and social science
psychology (pp. 407–36). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley (pp. 281–95) New York: Plenum Press.
Damasio, A. R. & Anderson, S. W. (2003). The frontal Ferris, R. (1997). Psychiatry and the death penalty.
lobes. In K. M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Psychiatric Bulletin, 21, 746–48.
Clinical neuropsychology (4th ed., pp. 404–46). Ford v. Wainwright, 477 U.S. 399 (1986).
New York: Oxford University Press. Fisher, J. M. (2008) Presenting neuropsychological
Davis v. United States, 160 U.S. 469 (1895). findings, opinions, and testimony to the criminal
Denney, R. L. (2005). Ethical challenges in forensic court. In R. L. Denney & J. P. Sullivan (Eds.),
neuropsychology, section 1: Neuropsychology in Clinical neuropsychology in the criminal forensic
the criminal forensic arena. In S. Bush (Ed.), A setting (pp. 349–90). New York: Guilford Press.
casebook of ethical challenges in neuropsychology Forth, A. E., Kosson, D. S., & Hare, R. D. (2003). The
(pp. 15–22). Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets and Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version. North
Zeitlinger. Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health System.
Denney, R. L. & Sullivan, J. P. (Eds.) (2008). Clinical Frick, P. J. (1995). Callous-unemotional traits and con-
neuropsychology in the criminal forensic setting. duct problems: A two-factor model of psychopathy
New York: Guilford Press. in children. Issues in Criminological and Legal
Denney, R. L. & Wynkoop, T. F. (2000). Clinical neu- Psychology, 24, 47–51.
ropsychology in the criminal forensic setting. Journal Furman v. Georgia, 408 U.S. 238 (1972).
of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 15, 804–28. Gerstle, J. E., Mathias, C. W., & Stanford, M. S. (1998).
Dolan, M., Deakin, W. J. F., Roberts, N., & Anderson, I. Auditory P300 and self-reported impulsive aggres-
(2002). Serotonergic and cognitive impairment in sion. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmoacological
impulsive aggressive personality disordered offend- and Biological Psychiatry, 22, 575–83.
ers: Are there implications for treatment? Psycho- Giancola, P. R., Moss, H. B., Martin, C. S., Kirisci, L., &
logical Medicine, 32, 105–117. Tarter, R. E. (1996). Executive cognitive function-
Douglas, K., Ogloff, J., Nicholls, T., & Grant, I. (1999). ing predicts reactive aggression in boys at high risk
Assessing risk for violence among psychiatric for substance abuse: A prospective study.
patients: The HCR-20 Violence Risk Assessment Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 20,
Scheme and the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening 740–44.
Version. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Godinez v. Moran, 125 L. Ed. 2d, 509 U.S. 389 (1993).
Psychology, 67, 917–30. Golden, C. J., Jackson, M. L., Peterson-Rohne, A., &
Douglas, K. & Webster, C. (1999). The HCR-20 Gontkovsky, S. T. (2002). Neuro-psychological fac-
Violence Risk Assessment Scheme: Concurrent tors in violence and aggression. In V. B. Van Hasselt
validity in a sample of incarcerated offenders. & M. Hersen (Eds.), Aggression and violence: An
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 26, 3–19. introductory text (pp. 40–53). Needham Heights,
Drake, M. E., Hietter, S. A., & Pakalnis, A. (1992). EEG MA: Allyn and Bacon.
and evoked potentials in episodic-dyscontrol syn- Golding, S. & Roesch, R. (1987). The assessment of
drome. Neuropsychobiology, 26, 125–28. criminal responsibility: Approach to a current
496 forensic neuropsychology

controversy. In I. B. Weiner & A. K. Hess (Eds.), of subsequent parole risk of the murderer. Journal of
Handbook of forensic psychology (pp. 395–432). Criminal Law and Criminology, 69, 108–114.
Golding, S. L., Skeem, J. L., Roesch, R., & Zapf, P. A. Heilbrun, K. (1987). The assessment of competency for
(1999). The assessment of criminal responsibility: execution: An overview. Behavioral Sciences and
Current controversies. In A. K. Hess & I. B. Weiner the Law, 5, 383–96.
(Eds.), The handbook of forensic psychology (2nd Heilbrun, K. (2001). Principles of forensic mental health
ed., pp. 379–408). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. assessment. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
Goldstein, A. M., Morse, S. J., & Shapiro, D. L. (2003). Heilbrun, K. & McClaren, H. (1988). Assessment of
Evaluation of criminal responsibility. In I. B. competency to execution? A guide for mental
Weiner (Series Ed.) & A. M. Goldstein (Vol. Ed.), health professionals. Bulletin of the American
Handbook of psychology, Vol. 11: Forensic psychol- Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 16, 205–216.
ogy (pp. 381–406). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Heilbrun, K., Radelet, M., & Dvoskin, J. (1992).
Gregg v. Georgia, 49 L.Ed.2d 859 (1976). The debate of treating individuals incompetent
Gregg, T. R. & Siegel, A. (2001). Brain structures and for execution. American Journal of Psychiatry,
neurotransmitters regulating aggression in cats: 149, 596–604.
Implications for human aggression. Progress in Houston, R. J. & Stanford, M. S. (2001). Mid-latency
Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological evoked potentials in self-reported impulsive
Psychiatry, 25, 91–140. aggression. International Journal of Psycho-
Grisso, T. (1988). Competency to stand trial evaluations: physiology, 40, 1–15.
A manual for practice. Sarasota, FL: Professional Houston, R. J. & Stanford, M. S. (2006). Characterization
Resource Exchange. of aggressive behavior and phenytoin response.
Grisso, T. & Applebaum, P. S. (1992). Is it unethical to Aggressive Behavior, 32, 38–43.
offer predictions of future violence? Law and Houston, R. J., Stanford, M. S., Villemarette-Pittman,
Human Behavior, 16, 621–33. N. R., Conklin, S. M., & Helfritz, L. E. (2003).
Grisso, T., with Borum R., Edens, J. F., Moye, J. & Otto, Neurobiological correlates and clinical implica-
R. K. (2003). Evaluating competencies: Forensic tions of aggressive sub-types. Journal of Forensic
assessments and instruments (2nd ed.). New York: Neuropsychology, 3, 67–87.
Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. Hubbard, J. A., Smithmyer, C. M., Ramsden, S. R.,
Grisso, T., Steinberg, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., Parker, E. H., Flanagan, K. D., Dearing, K. F., et al.
Scott, E., Graham, S., et al. (2003). Juveniles’ com- (2002). Observational, physiological, and self-
petence to stand trial: A comparison of adolescents’ report measures of children’s anger: Relations to
and adults’ capacities as trial defendants. Law and reactive versus proactive aggression. Child
Human Behavior, 27, 333–63. Development, 73, 1101–1118.
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist– Jewish Publication Society. (1982). Tanakh: A new
Revised. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health translation of the holy scriptures according to the
Systems. traditional Hebrew texts. Philadelphia: Author.
Hare, R. D., Harpur, T. J., & Hakstian, A. R. (1990). The Kavoussi, R. J., Liu, J., & Coccaro, E. F. (1994). An open
revised Psychopathy Checklist: Reliability and factor trial of sertraline in personality disordered patients
structure. Psychological Assessment, 2, 338–41. with impulsive aggression. Journal of Clinical
Harris, G., Rice, M., & Quinsey, V. L. (1993). Violent Psychiatry, 55, 137–41.
recidivism of mentally disordered offenders: The Kent, T. A., Brown, C. S., Bryant, S. G., Barratt, E. S.
development of a statistical prediction instrument. Felthous, A. R., & Rose, R. M. (1988). Blood
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 20, 315. platelet uptake of serotonin in episodic aggression:
Hart, S., Cox, D., & Hare, R. (1995). The Hare Correlation with red blood cell proton T1
Psychopathy Checklist: Screening version. Niagara, and impulsivity. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24,
NY: Multi-Health Systems. 454–57.
Heilbronner, R. L. & Frumkin, I. B. (2003). Neuro- Kiehl, K. A., Hare, R. D., Liddle, P. F., & McDonald, J. J.
psychology and forensic psychology: Working col- (1999). Reduced P300 in criminal psychopaths
laboratively in criminal cases. Journal of Forensic during a visual oddball task. Biological Psychiatry,
Neuropsychology, 3, 5–12. 45, 1498–1507.
Heilbronner, R. L. & Waller, D. (2008). Neuro- Krakowski, M. (2003). Violence and serotonin:
psychological consultation in the sentencing phase Influence of impulse control, affect regulation, and
of capital cases. In R. L. Denney & J. P. Sullivan social functioning. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and
(Eds.) Clinical neuropsychology in the criminal foren- Clinical Neurosciences, 15, 294–305.
sic setting (pp. 273–94). New York: Guilford Press. Latzer, B. (1998). Death penalty cases: Leading U.S.
Heilbrun, A. B., Heilbrun, L. C., & Heilbrun, K. L. (1978). Supreme Court cases on capital punishment. Boston:
Impulsive and premeditated homicide: An analysis Butterworth-Heinemann.
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 497

Leong, G. B., Weinstock, R., Silva, J. A., & Eth, S. (1993). Mesulam, M.-M. (2000). Behavioral neuroanatomy:
Psychiatry and the death penalty: The past decade. Large-scale networks, association cortex, frontal
Psychiatric Annals, 23, 41–47. syndromes, the limbic system, and hemispheric
Lewis, D. O., Lovely, R., Yeager, C., & Della Femina, D. specializations. In M.-M. Mesulam (Ed.), Principles
(1989). Toward a theory of the genesis of violence: of behavioral and cognitive neurology (2nd ed., pp.
A follow-up study of delinquents. Journal of the 1–120). New York: Oxford University Press.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Miczek, K. A. (1987). The psychopharmacology of
Psychiatry, 28, 431–36. aggression. In L. L. Iverson, S. D. Iverson, & S. H.
Lewis, D. O., Moy, E., Jackson, L. D., Aaronson, R., Snyder (Eds.). Handbook of psychopharmacology
Restifo, N., Serra, S., et al. (1985). Biopsychological (pp. 183–228). New York: Plenum Press.
characteristics of children who later murder: A Mills, V. M., Cassidy, J. W., & Katz, D. I. (1997).
prospective study. American Journal of Psychiatry, Neurological rehabilitation: A guide to diagnosis,
142, 1161–67. prognosis, and treatment planning. Malden, MA:
Lindsay, W. R., Hogue, T. E., Taylor, J. L., Steptoe, L., Blackwell Science.
Mooney, P., O’Brien, G., et al. (2008). Risk assess- Monahan, J. (1992). Mental disorder and violent
ment in offenders with intellectual disability: A behavior: Perceptions and evidence. American
comparison across three levels of security. Psychologist, 47, 511–21.
International Journal of Offender Therapy and Monahan, J. (2003). Violence risk assessment. In I. B.
Comparative Criminology, 52, 90–111. Weiner (Series Ed.) & A. M. Goldstein (Vol. Ed.),
Linnoila, M. & Charney, D. S. (1999). The neurobiol- Handbook of psychology, Vol. 11: Forensic psychol-
ogy of aggression. In D. S. Charney, E. J. Nestler, & ogy (pp. 527–40). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
B. S. Bunney (Eds.), Neurobiology of mental illness Monahan, J., & Steadman, H. J. (1994). Toward a reju-
(pp. 855–71). New York: Oxford University Press. venation of risk assessment research. In J. Monahan
Lockett v. Ohio, 438 U.S. 586 (1978). & H. J. Steadman (Eds.). Violence and mental disor-
Macmillan, M. (2000). An odd kind of fame: Stories of der: Developments in risk assessment. Chicago:
Phineas Gage. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. University of Chicago Press.
Malone, R. P., Bennett, D. S., Luebbert, J. F., Rowan, A. Monahan, J., Steadman, H. J., Appelbaum, P. S., Grisso,
B., Biesecker, B. A., Blaney, B. L., et al. (1998). T., Mulvey, E. P., Roth, L. H., et al. (2005). The clas-
Aggression classification and treatment response. sification of violence risk. Lutz, FL: Psychological
Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 34, 41–45. Assessment Resources.
Marlowe, D. B., Lambert, J. B., & Thompson, R. G. Monahan, J., Steadman, H. J., Appelbaum, P. S., Grisso,
(1999). Voluntary intoxication and criminal T., Mulvey, E. P., Roth, L. H., et al. (2006). The clas-
responsibility. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 17, sification of violence risk. Behavioral Sciences and
195–217. the Law, 24, 721–30.
Martell, D. A. (1992). Estimating the prevalence of Monahan, J, Steadman, H. J., Robbins, P., Appelbaum,
organic brain dysfunction in maximum-security P., Banks, S., Grisso, T., et al. (2005). An actuarial
forensic psychiatric patients. Journal of Forensic model of violence risk assessment for persons
Science, 37, 878–93. with mental disorders. Psychiatric Services, 56,
Mathias, C. W. & Stanford, M. S. (1999). P300 under stan- 810–815.
dard and surprise condition in self-reported impulsive Monahan, J., Steadman, H. J., Silver, E., Applebaum,
aggression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology P. S., Robbins, P. C., Mulvey, E. P., et al. (2001).
and Biological Psychiatry, 23, 1037–51. Rethinking risk assessment: The MacArthur Study of
McCusker, P. J. (2007). Issues regarding the clinical use Mental Disorder and Violence. New York: Oxford
of the Classification of Violence Risk (COVR) University Press.
assessment instrument. International Journal of Moran, Richard (1981). Knowing right from wrong: The
Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, insanity defense of Daniel McNaughten. New York:
51, 676–85. Free Press.
McDonald v. United States, 312 F.2d 847 (DC Cir. Morgan, A. B. & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-
1962). analytic review of the relation between antisocial
McLearen, A. M., Pietz, C. A., & Denney, R. L. (2004). behavior and neuropsychological measures of
Evaluation of psychological damages. In W. executive function. Clinical Psychology Review, 20,
O’Donohue & E. Levensky (Eds.), Forensic psychol- 113–36.
ogy (pp. 267–99). New York: Academic. Mossman, D. (1994). Assessing predictions of violence:
Melton, G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., Slobogin, C., Being accurate about accuracy. Journal of Consulting
with Lyons, P. M., & Otto, R. K. (2007). Psychological and Clinical Psychology, 62, 783–92.
evaluations for the courts (3rd ed.). New York: Mrad, D. (1996, September). Criminal responsibility
Guilford Press. evaluations. Paper presented at Issues in Forensic
498 forensic neuropsychology

Assessment Symposium, Federal Bureau of Prisons, Raine, A., Venables, P. H., & Williams, M. (1990).
Atlanta, GA. Relationships between N1, P300, and contingent
Mungus, D. (1988). Psychometric correlates of episodic negative variation recorded at age 15 and criminal
violent behavior: A multidimensional neuropsy- behavior at age 24. Psychophysiology, 27, 567–74.
chological approach. British Journal of Psychiatry, Reisner, R. & Slobogin, C. (1990). Law and the mental
152, 180–87. health system (2nd ed.). St. Paul, MN: West.
Nestor, P. G. & Haycock, J. (1997). Not guilty by reason Rex v. M’Naghten, 10 Cl. & F. 200, 8 Eng. Rep. 718 (H.L.
of insanity of murder: Clinical and neuropsycho- 1843).
logical characteristics. Journal of the American Reynolds, C., Price, J. R., & Niland, J. (2003).
Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 25, 161–71. Applications of neuropsychology in capital felony
New, A. S., Hazlett, E. A., Buchsbaum, M. S., Goodman, (death penalty) defense. Journal of Forensic
M., Reynolds, D., Mitropoulou, V., et al. (2002). Neuropsychology, 3, 89–123.
Blunted prefrontal cortical 18fluorodeoxyglucose Rice, M. & Harris, G. (1995). Violent recidivism:
positron emission tomography response to meta- Assessing predictive validity. Journal of Consulting
chlorophenylpiperazine in impulsive aggression. and Clinical Psychology, 63, 737–48
Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 621–29. Ring v. Arizona. 536 U.S. 584 (2002).
North Carolina v. Alford, 400 U.S. 25 (1970). Robinson, D. N. (1996). Wild beasts and idle humours:
Otto, R. K. & Heilbrun, K. (2002). The practice of The insanity defense from antiquity to the present.
forensic psychology: A look toward the future in Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
light of the past. American Psychologist, 57, 5–18. Rogers, R. (1984). Rogers Criminal Responsibility
Parry, J. W. (1998). National benchbook on psychiatric and Assessment Scales (RCRAS) and test manual.
psychological evidence and testimony. Washington, Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
DC: American Bar Association Commission on Rogers, R. & Ewing, C. P. (1992). The measurement of
Mental and Physical Disability Law. insanity: Debating the merits of the R-CRAS and
Parsons v. State. 81 Ala. 577, 2 So. 854 (1886). its alternatives. International Journal of Law and
Paschall, M. J. & Fishbein, D. H. (2002). Executive Psychiatry, 15, 113.
cognitive functioning and aggression: A public Scarpa, A. & Raine, A. (2000). Violence associated with
health perspective. Aggression and Violent Behavior, anger and impulsivity. In J. C. Borod (Ed.), The
7, 215–35. neuropsychology of emotion (pp. 320–39). New
Penry v. Lynaugh, 492 U.S. 1 (1989). York: Oxford University Press.
People v. Schmidt, 216 N.Y. 324, 110 N.E., 945 (1915). Shaikah, M. B., De Lanerolle, N. C., & Siegel, A. (1997).
Perlin, M. L. (1992). The law and persons with mental Serotonin 5-HT1A and 5-HT2/1C receptors in the
disability–Tape 6: The criminal law process. midbrain periaqueductal gray differentially modu-
Springfield, IL: Baxley Media Group. late defensive rage behavior elicited from the
Pulkkinen, L. (1996). Proactive and reactive aggression medial hypothalamus of the cat. Brain Research,
in early adolescence as precursors to anti- and 765, 198–207.
prosocial behavior in young adults. Aggressive Shapiro, D. L. (1984). Psychological evaluation and expert
Behavior, 22, 241–57. testimony. New York: van Nostrand Reinhold.
Quinsey, V. L., Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Cormier, Shapiro, D. L. (1991). Forensic psychological assessment.
C. A. (2005). Violent offenders: Appraising and Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
managing risk (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Shapiro, D. L. (1999). Criminal responsibility evalua-
American Psychological Association. tions: A manual for practice. Sarasota, FL: Profe-
Raine, A. (2002). Annotation: The role of prefrontal ssional Resource Press.
deficits, low autonomic arousal, and early health Sieberer, M. & Emrich, H. M. (2009). Evidenz für den
factors in the development of antisocial and aggres- einsatz von antikonvulsiva bei persönlich-
sive behavior in children. Journal of Child Psychology keitsstörungen [Empirical evidence for the use of
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43, 417–34. anticonvulsants in peraonality disorders]. Forts-
Raine, A., Meloy, J. R., Bihrle, S., Stoddard, J., LaCasse, chritte Der Neurologie-Psychiatrie, 77, 133–38.
L., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1998). Reduced prefrontal Siegel, A. & Shaikah, M. B. (1992). Neurotransmitters
and increased subcortical brain functioning and aggressive behavior: Some new perspectives.
assessed using positron emission tomography in In K. T. Strongman (Ed.). International review of
predatory and affective murderers. Behavioral studies on emotion (Vol. 2, pp. 5–22). Chichester,
Sciences and the Law, 16, 319–32. England: Wiley and Sons.
Raine, A. & Venables, P. H. (1988). Enhanced P3 Silver, E., Cirincione, C., & Steadman, J. H. (1994).
evoked potentials and longer P3 recovery time in Demythologizing inaccurate perceptions of the
psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 25, 30–38. insanity defense: Legal standards and clinical
Criminal Responsibility and Other Criminal Forensic Issues 499

assessment. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 2, United States v. Alvarez, 519 F.2d 1036 (3rd Cir., 1975).
163–78. United States v. Brawner, 471 F.2d 969 (DC Cir., 1972).
Singleton v. Norris, 108 F.3d 872 (8th Cir., 1997). See United States Department of Justice. (2005, December)
Judge Heaney concurring. Office of Justice Programs, Bureau of Justice
Singleton v. Norris, No. 00–1492 (8th Cir., 2003). Statistics, Bulletin Capital Punishment, 2005
Smithmyer, C. M., Hubbard, J. A., & Simons, R. F. December 2006, NCJ 215083, Revised January 30,
(2000). Proactive and reactive aggression in delin- 2007. Retrieved July 16, 2011, from http://bjs.ojp.
quent adolescents: Relations to aggression outcome usdoj.gov/content/pub/pdf/cp05.pdf
expectancies. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, United States Department of Justice. (2009, December).
29, 86–93. Capital punishment 2009. Table 12. Table 12.
Stanford, M. S., Greve, K. W., & Gerstle, J. E. (1997). Average time between sentencing and execution,
Neuropsychological correlates of self-reported by year, 1977-2009. Retrieved July 16, 2011,
impulsive aggression in a college sample. Personality from http://bjs.ojp.usdoj.gov/content/pub/pdf/
and Individual Differences, 23, 961–66. cp09st.pdf
Stanford, M. S., Houston, R. J., Mathias, C. W., Greve, United States v. Dubray, 854 F.2d 1099 (8th Cir., 1988).
K. W., Villemarette-Pittman, N. R., & Adams, D. United States v. Segna, 555 F.2d 226 (9th Cir., 1977).
(2001). A double-blind placebo-controlled cross- United States v. Sullivan, 544 F.2d 1052 (9th Cir.,
over study of phenytoin in individuals with impul- 1976).
sive aggression. Psychiatry Research, 103, 193–203. Villemarette-Pittman, N. R., Stanford, M. S., & Greve,
State v. Jones, 50 N.J. 369 (1871). K. W. (2003). Language and executive function in
Steadman, H. J. (2000). From dangerousness to risk self-reported impulsive aggression. Personality and
assessment of community violence: Taking stock at Individual Differences, 34, 1533.
the turn of the century. Journal of the American Vitaro, F., Gendreau, P. L., Tremblay, R. E., & Oligny, P.
Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 28, 265–71. (1998). Reactive and proactive aggression differen-
Steadman, H. J., McGreevy, M. A., Morrissey, J. P., tially predict later conduct problems. Journal of
Callahan, L. A., Robbins, P. C., & Cirincione, C. Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
(1993). Before and after Hinckley: Evaluating insan- Disciplines, 39, 377–85.
ity defense reform. New York: Guilford Press. Volavka, J. (2002). Neurobiology of violence (2nd ed.).
Steadman, H. J., Rosenstein, M. J., MacAskiel, R. L., & Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Manderschied, R. W. (1988). A profile of mental Volavka, J., Martell, D., & Convit, A. (1992).
disordered offenders admitted to inpatient psychi- Psychobiology of the violent offender. Journal of
atric services in the United States. Law and Human Forensic Sciences, 37, 237–51.
Behavior, 12, 91–99. Volkow, N. D., Tancredi, L. R., Grant, C., Gillespie, H.,
Sullivan, J. P. & Denney, R. L. (2003). Constitutional Valentine, A., Mullani, N., et al. (1995). Brain glu-
and judicial foundations in criminal forensic neu- cose metabolism in violent psychiatric patients: A
ropsychology. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, preliminary study. Psychiatry Research:
3, 13–44. Neuroimaging, 61, 243–53.
Sullivan, J. P. & Denney, R. L. (2008). A final word on Wade v. United States, 426 F.2d (9th Cir., 1970).
authentic professional competence in criminal Walsh, T. & Walsh, Z., (2006). The evidentiary intro-
forensic neuropsychology. In R. L. Denney & J. P. duction of Psychopathy Checklist-Revised assessed
Sullivan (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology in the psychopathy in U.S. Courts: Extent and appropri-
criminal forensic setting (pp. 391–400). New York: ateness. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 493–507.
Guilford Press. Washington v. United States, 390 F.2d 444 (D.C. Cir.,
Swanson, J. W., Holzer, C. E., Ganju, V., & Jono, R. T. 1967).
(1990). Violence and psychiatric disorder in the Webster, C., Douglas, K., Eaves, D., & Hart, S. (1995).
community: Evidence from the epidemiologic HCR-20: Assessing risk for Violence (version 2).
catchment area surveys. Hospital and Community Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: Simon
Psychiatry, 41, 761–70. Fraser University.
Sweet, J. J., Peck, E. A., Abramowitz, C., & Etzweiler, S. Weinshenker, N. J. & Siegel, A. (2002). Bimodal classi-
(2002). National Academy of Neuropsychology/ fication of aggression: Affective defense and preda-
Division 40 of the American Psychological tory attack. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 7,
Association practice survey of clinical neuro- 327–250.
psychology in the United States, part I: Practitioner West Publishing. (1990). Black’s law dictionary
and practice characteristics, professional activities, (6th ed). St. Paul, MN: Author.
and time requirements. The Clinical Neuro- Wexler, D. & Winick, B. (1991). Therapeutic jurispru-
psychologist, 16, 109–27. dence as a new approach to mental health law
500 forensic neuropsychology

policy analysis and research. University of Miami forensic setting (pp. 295–325). New York: Guilford
Law Review, 45, 992–97. Press.
Wynkoop, T. F. (2003). Neuropsychology of juvenile Yates, K. F. & Denney, R. L. (2008). Neuropsychology
adjudicative competence. Journal of Forensic in the assessment of mental state at the time of the
Neuropsychology, 3, 45–65. offense. In R. L. Denney & J. P. Sullivan (Eds.)
Wynkoop, T. F. (2008). Neuropsychology in the juve- Clinical Neuropsychology in the Criminal Forensic
nile justice system. In R. L. Denney & J. P. Sullivan Setting (pp. 204–37). New York: Guilford Press.
(Eds.) Clinical Neuropsychology in the criminal
18
Trends in Forensic Practice and Research
J E R RY J. S W E E T A N D D AW N G I U F F R E M E Y E R

Adversary system. A procedural system, such


as the Anglo-American legal system, involving
active and unhindered parties contesting with
each other to put forth a case before an independent
decision-maker. Black’s Law Dictionary. Seventh Ed.
—garner, 1999 , p. 54

The American civil and criminal legal systems and relevant empirical research, the impact of
rely on experts in a variety of capacities, most pertinent legal rulings, and the growing literature
fundamentally to provide relevant information providing guidance on a range of forensic
that can assist in important decision making. The matters specifically written for clinical neuro-
same is true for other adjudicatory proceedings psychologists.
that are less formal and take place outside of
courtrooms. Though not all forensic contexts in PA R T I C I PAT I O N I N T H E
which clinical neuropsychologists consult regard- LEGAL SYSTEM
ing civil litigants and claimants and criminal Psychologists have been participating in the
defendants are literally adversarial (i.e., involving American legal system for decades. In fact,
antagonistic elements), all have the potential to be Division 41 (American Psychology-Law Society)
so and many actually will be. This omnipresent within the largest psychology professional organi-
possibility represents a salient distinction from zation in the world, the American Psychological
clinical contexts, with the latter characterized by a Association (APA), was formed first in 1981 as a
shared goal between neuropsychologist and forensic division and then in 1984 as the American
patient regarding seeking to understand and Psychology-Law Society because of the meaning-
improve the patient’s identified problems. The ful involvement of psychologists specializing in
various implications of this context for all involved forensic matters. More broadly, APA itself has
parties have been well identified by various participated in the legal system for decades, by
neuropsychology writers and researchers, who filing numerous amicus briefs that are intended to
have over the years created a very large relevant inform the judiciary regarding psychological
literature. The question we will address within the topics of relevance to rulings (American Psycho-
closing chapter of this text fundamentally con- logical Association, 2010). Some of these topics
cerns the future of forensic neuropsychology. concern matters of import to clinical neuro-
More specifically, in interrogatory form and psychology (e.g., confidentiality, misuse of psy-
adopting a common phrase used by attorneys, chological test materials, test security).
do we have an opinion within a reasonable degree We have previously defined forensic neuro-
of certainty as neuropsychologists regarding where psychology and some key concepts as
the practice and scientific study of forensic neuro-
psychology is going from here? The bases of the . . . all neuropsychological practice in which a
opinions to follow will be numerous, including clinician provides evaluation or consultative
historical data from a search of federal and state services to an individual involved in a proceed-
case opinions, trends evident in practice surveys ing that is potentially adversarial in nature.
502 forensic neuropsychology

Adversarial proceedings are those that involve 4000 3786


two or more interested parties who must reach
3500
a resolution of a common concern or disagree-
ment from potentially antagonistic positions. 3000
Adversarial proceedings may be either formal,
often taking place in a courtroom and involv- 2500
ing criminal, civil (including personal injury
2000
and medical malpractice), or family (including
divorce and child custody) law or informal, 1500
often involving administrative matters, such as 1130
disability determination, fitness for duty, and 1000
due process educational hearings. In all these
500
proceedings, the offering of specialty knowl- 222
1 3 23
edge by the clinical neuropsychologist is to 0
inform a “trier-of-fact” or an administrative 1950–59 1960–69 1970–79 1980–89 1990–99 2000–10
process regarding an individual who is desig-
FIGURE 18.1: Cases identified in legal search
nated a litigant, if involved in formal court
engine LexisNexis® Academic by decade that men-
proceedings, or as a claimant, if involved in less
tion “neuropsychologist” or “neuropsychology” or
formal proceedings (e.g., seeking disability
“neuropsychological.”
status or seeking special considerations
in an educational system).” (Sweet, Ecklund-
Johnson, & Malina, 2008, p. 869) neuropsychologists and/or reference to the subject
matter of neuropsychology by name has increased
Related to formal proceedings associated with dramatically over time. In reality, the numbers
forensic neuropsychology, electronic databases shown do not include instances in which neuro-
containing historical documentation of civil and psychologists consulted with attorneys about cases,
criminal proceedings in the United States federal but were not disclosed and did not participate at
and state court systems are available for public testifying experts. And, as a reminder, the Lexis
scrutiny, and afford us the opportunity to gather database would not include informal forensic cases,
information pertaining to clinical neuropsycho- such as disability determination, fitness for duty,
logists. Perhaps the most well known of these and due process educational hearings. Thus, the
databases is LexisNexis® Academic, with searches numbers depicted in Figure 18.1 represent only a
of this database often referred to simply as Lexis subset of actual cases involving neuropsychologists
searches. Previously, Taylor (1999) used such a or our subject matter.
search as the basis for his observation that prior to LexisNexis® Academic also contains an archive
April 1997, the number of cases in a database of of legal materials that are primarily for, and pri-
state and federal court decisions in which the term marily written by, attorneys. A second search, the
“neuropsychologist” occurred was 401. Taylor resulting graphic of which is shown in Figure 18.2,
also reported that 98% of these decisions occurred was conducted to identify the number and type
after 1980. A major implication of these observa- of legal materials containing the terms “neuro-
tions is that formal forensic involvement by psychologist” or “neuropsychology” or “neuro-
neuropsychologists is a relatively recent phenom- psychological.” Again, it is apparent from the
enon. For clinicians who have practiced over the dramatic increases over time that attorneys are
last several decades, there is anecdotal informa- producing and consuming an increasing number
tion that such involvement has increased over the of legal materials that contain information rele-
years. vant to the specialty of clinical neuropsychology.
To capture a more exact depiction of the growth It does not require a leap of logic to conclude that
of forensic neuropsychology, Lexis searches were this is because the cases they handle in litigation
carried out in June 2010. The first search consisted increasingly require input of neuropsychologists
of identifying cases in the state and federal courts in as expert witnesses.
which the terms “neuropsychologist” or “neuropsy-
chology” or “neuropsychological” were found. The TRENDS
resulting graph is shown in Figure 18.1. Categorized Additional evidence of the growth of forensic
into decades, it is apparent that the involvement of neuropsychology can be found within our own
Trends in Forensic Practice and Research 503

600
559 in this volume; Greiffenstein & Kaufmann, earlier
550 in this volume; and Sweet, Ecklund-Johnson, &
500 Malina, 2008). Additionally, there are issues
450 inherent to the forensic context that occur less
400 commonly in general clinical practice, which
350 must be addressed to ensure a complete, sound
300 evaluation (e.g., secondary gain issues). While the
250 American Psychological Association’s (APA)
193
200 Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of
150 Conduct (1992; 2002) provides general guidance
100 on psychologists’ ethical conduct and practice,
50 18 additional assistance, specifically addressing
0 0 2
0 conflicts that occur frequently during forensic
1950–59 1960–69 1970–79 1980–89 1990–99 2000–10 activities, became necessary.
FIGURE 18.2: Law reviews, legal journal articles,
Leading forensic practitioners and researchers
and other publications for attorneys identified by
provided a preponderance of articles, book chap-
legal search engine (LexisNexis® Academic) by
ters, and texts, which clarified practice issues
decade that mention “neuropsychologist” or “neuro-
common to forensic neuropsychologists, and
psychology” or “neuropsychological.”
offered direction on ethical conduct in situations
confronted by practitioners within the field (e.g.,
Note. The above information was obtained on June 11, 2010. The
Bush, 2005; Bush & Drexler, 2002). While benefi-
“Easy” search function was used, searching “Legal” document
cial, practitioners were left with a myriad of infor-
sources and then excluding individual case documents.
mation and opinions to sift through and synthesize
independently. Thus, the National Academy of
literature. Survey data suggests that over time, Neuropsychology (NAN) and the American
neuropsychologists have increased their involve- Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology (AACN)
ment in forensic cases (Sweet, Moberg, & Suchy, have created a number of position papers, intended
2000). More recent data (Sweet, Nelson, & to provide the official stance of these organizations
Moberg, 2006) indicates that for adult neuro- on acceptable practices. Topics addressed have
psychologists, forensic activities account for an included test security (AACN, 2007; NAN, 2000b),
average of 12% of their practice, with a mean third party observers (AACN, 2001; NAN, 2000a),
of 4.0 hours per week (SD = 7.7). For pediatric ethical complaints filed against neuropsycholo-
neuropsychologists the average is somewhat less, gists engaged in forensic activities (AACN, 2003),
accounting for approximately 6% of their activi- and the use of symptom validity tests (Bush et al.,
ties, or 1.7 hours per week (SD = 3.9). For those 2005; Heilbronner, Sweet, Morgan, Larrabee,
who identify as both pediatric and adult neurop- Millis, & Conference Participants, 2009).
sychologists, forensic activities account for an Forensic research involving all types of forensic
average of nearly 21% of their practice (6.9 hours evaluations, including disability/independent
per week; SD = 10.0). Compared to clinical evalu- medical examinations, civil litigation cases, and
ations, forensic evaluations typically require more criminal proceedings, has flourished as the field
time to complete; they also apparently contribute has grown. In 2002, Sweet and colleagues invento-
disproportionately to neuropsychologists’ incomes ried the contents from 1990 to 2000 of the most
and there is a positive correlation between time popular clinical neuropsychology journals, for
spent in forensic activities and income level topics related to forensic practice. They found that
(Sweet et al., 2006). publications focusing upon forensic neuropsychol-
As neuropsychologists increased their involve- ogy had increased substantially within the prior
ment in forensic activities, they have encountered decade, from approximately 4% to 14% (Sweet,
new challenges to their ability to provide objec- King, Malina, Bergman, & Simmons, 2002).
tive, ethical, scientifically informed evaluations, During the same period of time, presentations on
and opinions. Ethical challenges may arise from relevant forensic topics at the annual meetings of
the differences in psychologists’ roles in clinical NAN and Division 40 (which occurs at the annual
and forensic settings, while others occur due to meeting of APA) correspondingly increased.
the differences in psychologists’ and lawyers’ roles While the scientific literature has advanced,
(for elaboration of these issues, see Grote, earlier rulings within the legal world have evolved to
504 forensic neuropsychology

reinforce the need for a scientifically grounded witnesses (Cheng & Yoon, 2005; Groscup, Penrod,
approach to forensic neuropsychology. In 1993, Studebaker, Huss, & O’Neil, 2002); however,
with the Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, judges receive limited training in scientific meth-
Inc. ruling, the United States Supreme Court spec- ods, which may impede their ability to appropri-
ified four criteria by which the admissibility of ately apply Daubert criteria to evidence offered by
evidence should be determined, clarifying that expert witnesses (Dahir et al., 2005; Faigman &
evidence presented by expert witnesses should be Monahan, 2005). When judges fail to exclude
derived by methods that are generally accepted testimony of questionable scientific validity, it is
within the scientific community (the sole crite- left to jurors to evaluate the merit of expert
rion elaborated by the previous relevant ruling, witnesses’ opinions, to consider possibly (likely)
known as the Frye standard from Frye v. United conflicting information from expert witnesses
States, 1923); they are able to have been subjected retained by the opposing sides, and to determine
to empirical testing; they have a known error how to integrate information presented by wit-
rate (which is acceptable); and/or they have been nesses with other evidence to develop a verdict.
subjected to peer review. While judges retain the Research has indicated that jurors lack knowledge
capacity to decide which of the criteria to apply of complex scientific methodology, and that while
and how to apply them, and a number of states having opposing experts may sensitize jurors to
retain the Frye standard rather than Daubert, empirically relevant issues, it may also increase
this Supreme Court ruling clearly demonstrates their uncertainty regarding all expert testimony in
that the forensic psychology community will a case and fail to assist them with forming deci-
be expected to employ scientifically accepted sions that are grounded in science (e.g., Levett &
methods in their work, or risk having expert Kovera, 2008). Psychologists and neuropsycholo-
testimony excluded. gists may serve as educators to the court, for exam-
Following the Daubert ruling, many within ple, as technical advisors to assist federal judges or
the field expressed the concern that courts may as court-appointed experts to federal juries, and,
apply the Daubert standards in a manner that in light of the increased emphasis that courts have
could constrain neuropsychological practice, for placed upon scientifically valid testimony since
example, by limiting admissible evidence to Daubert, medical and legal experts alike have
include data collected via fixed assessment batter- urged courts to increase their use of court-
ies only. There is no evidence suggesting that neu- appointed experts (Faigman & Monahan, 2005).
ropsychologists working with a flexible test battery When Daubert or other evidentiary standards
approach have had their testimony excluded with are applied to neuropsychologists’ testimony, or
greater frequency than those who employ a fixed in some cases misapplied, judicial rulings can set
test battery approach (Greiffenstein, 2009). It is precedent that is adopted in other jurisdictions
the case that since the Daubert ruling, challenges and can have the effect of widely affecting forensic
to evidence presented by neuropsychologists have activities. As an example of a ruling that might
increased, related to a number of different issues have broadly affected neuropsychologists, but
that do not represent a pattern. There is no indica- ultimately did not only because a higher court
tion to suggest that evidence from neuropsychol- ruling turned back the precedent, we can consider
ogists is challenged or deemed inadmissible more a case from New Hampshire involving opposing
frequently than evidence given by other profes- neuropsychology experts. In 2005, a case came
sionals (Sweet, Ecklund-Johnson, & Malina, before the New Hampshire Supreme Court, in
2008). When individual neuropsychologists have which a lower court had excluded the testimony
been excluded by a judge, it was because the prac- of a pediatric neuropsychologist, via a Daubert
titioner did not adhere to scientifically sound challenge initiated by the defendant. In this case,
practice, the outcome of which is reasonable. Baxter v. Temple, the plaintiff ’s neuropsychologist
However, this does not mean that neuropsy- was characterized as having administered a bat-
chologists should simply ignore the goings-on of tery of tests that represented a flexible battery
the legal world and trust that the courts will figure approach. In response to a Daubert challenge by
out the best way to apply the Daubert standard to the defendant, in which the defense-retained neu-
psychologists’ testimony (or the Frye standard, as ropsychologist claimed that only a fixed battery,
the case may be). It appears that Daubert has such as the Halstead-Reitan battery is valid, the
raised awareness of the need to consider the trial court considered excluding the plaintiff ’s
scientific validity of evidence offered by expert neuropsychologist. In fact, after considering
Trends in Forensic Practice and Research 505

relevant evidence the trial court decided that the measurement). Neuropsychologists have scien-
battery of tests administered by the plaintiff ’s tific clinical training that makes them particularly
neuropsychologist had not been validated in a well suited to inform the triers-of-fact in such
forensic context, and because the battery itself matters, as well as others that will arise in the
(i.e., the set of tests together) had not been sub- future. Moreover, the specialty of clinical neuro-
jected to peer review and did not have a known psychology would be well advised to look for
error rate, the neuropsychologist’s testimony was relevant and appropriate opportunities to educate
inadmissible. The New Hampshire Supreme Court the legal system regarding our area of practice, in
noted “distilled to its essence, the defendants’ much the same way as has occurred with the topic
position is that Dr. X [the plaintiff ’s pediatric of ensuring test security (cf. Kaufmann’s 2009
neuropsychologist] should have used a fixed bat- treatise regarding strategies and legal arguments
tery, such as the Halstead-Reitan battery, rather for protection of raw data that are the subject of
than devising her own. . . .” Ultimately, assisted by forensic proceedings).
an amicus brief submitted by AACN (2007), the
New Hampshire Supreme Court disagreed with L O O K I N G F O RWA R D
that position, reversed the trial court’s decision,
and found that the neuropsychologist’s testimony The Future Involvement of Practitioners
based on a flexible battery was admissible. As is evident from the Lexis data in Figure 18.1,
A U. S. Supreme Court ruling provides another involvement of clinical neuropsychologists in the
example of the importance of forensic psycholo- legal system as expert witnesses is a relatively
gists’ utilization of scientifically grounded assess- recent phenomenon. Arguably, this involvement
ment techniques. In Atkins v. Virginia (2002) the did not occur in meaningful numbers until the
Supreme Court ruled that the death penalty 1990s. The curvilinear steep increase of this
should not be applied to individuals who are growth likely cannot continue, and may in the
mentally retarded/intellectually impaired. The next ten to twenty years reach asymptote, with
need for forensic psychologists and neuropsy- subsequent growth yoked proportionately to the
chologists to use thorough, well-validated assess- number of relevant new civil litigants and claim-
ment techniques is never more apparent than in ants and criminal defendants. Importantly, there
legal circumstances that may result in an indivi- is no reason to believe that the initial factors that
dual receiving the death sentence. This ruling also created interest by the legal community in the
illustrates the role that psychologists who abide by expertise of clinical neuropsychologists will cause
a scientist-practitioner model could play in the a waning of future interest. That is, the specialty
evolution of the legal application of significant will continue to be heavily wedded to empiricism,
judicial actions. Duvall and Morris (2006) provide which is foundational to the scientist-practitioner
a discussion of the means by which states have that has in the past been, and will continue in the
enacted the Atkins ruling, each having determined future to be, the prevailing conceptual framework
a definition of mental retardation/intellectual of neuropsychology’s training model and approach
impairment, which in numerous cases apparently to clinical practice. It is this orientation to scien-
has not been well informed by principles of psy- tific inquiry and objective hypothesis testing that
chometric theory. As with the Baxter v. Temple leads to knowledge growth and refinement of
New Hampshire case, Atkins illustrates the oppor- practice procedures, and assures that opinions of
tunities that may be created by the judicial system neuropsychologist expert witnesses have solid
for psychologists to educate and assist triers-of- foundations.
fact within the legal system. In the case of Baxter, Increasingly, health care has invested intellec-
occurring within the smaller venue of one state’s tually and economically in “empirically proven”
court system, the necessary assistance was very assessments and interventions. Though represent-
focused (an amicus brief provided by AACN). ing a foreign and new approach in some areas of
With regard to Atkins-like decisions, the process health care, such as in some forms of psychother-
of influencing the creation or modification of apy, this is a familiar, and one can even character-
extant statutes and related regulations can require ize it as a “tried-and-true,” approach in clinical
broad efforts (e.g., regarding Atkins in particular, neuropsychology. There has been no evidence
defining operationally at what psychometric across decades of lessened interest in research
point mental retardation/intellectual impairment among clinical neuropsychologists, though as
begins and the consideration of standard error of the majority of practitioners transferred from
506 forensic neuropsychology

institutional employment to private practice, a neuropsychology. Related, the inventory of the


smaller proportion are producing practice-related forensic neuropsychology literature in the three
research, which is mirrored by “psychologists in most popular clinical practice journals (Archives
all areas of psychology and for scientists in all of Clinical Neuropsychology, Journal of Clinical
disciplines” (Norcross, Karpiak, & Santoro, 2005, and Experimental Neuropsychology, and The
pp. 1481–1482). In fact, the productivity evident Clinical Neuropsychologist) from 1990 to 2000
within the most popular journals of the field carried out by Sweet, King, Malina, Bergman, and
suggests sustained scientific interest and appre- Simmons (2002) provides evidence that is very
ciable growth of practice-related scientific suggestive regarding the future of research in this
research. Additionally, the clinical patient groups area. This inventory revealed that the journals
with which clinical neuropsychologists work will contained a greater proportion of forensic articles
continue to include a nontrivial percentage who, from 1990 to 2000. Moreover, there were 139 arti-
because of acquired or perceived injury, go on to cles published during this era that were “substan-
file lawsuits or seek disability status. Thus, tially” addressing forensic practice, as opposed to
neuropsychological practices will continue to be “partially” or “not” addressing forensic practice.
relevant to the legal system and administrative The top ten topics of these forensic articles are
processes in which claims of impairment must be shown in Table 18.1.
adjudicated. Will the future resemble the past in terms of
The increasing numbers of neuropsycholo- forensic research topics? There are at least four
gists specializing in pediatric cases reflects an reasons to believe that response bias and malin-
interesting phenomenon, which mirrors the his- gering research will continue to be the most
torical development and identity distinction of frequent topic studied by forensic investigators.
pediatric neurologists. This development has First, psychology is the discipline that historically
forensic implications. To be sure, there are many has addressed whether samples of behavior are
more adult than pediatric forensic cases in crimi- valid. Specifically, since the early 1990s neuropsy-
nal and civil courts. There are also disability cases, chologists have accomplished a great deal related
which are nearly exclusively adult. It seems to devising procedures that can effectively dis-
unlikely that as a group pediatric neuropsycholo- criminate between valid and invalid responses, a
gists will proportionately ever be as involved in subset of which can be determined to be the result
forensic practice as their adult colleagues have of deliberate deception. This research area has
become. However, to the extent that the subspe-
cialty of pediatric neuropsychology expands its
scientific literature regarding differential diagno-
sis, including the influences of emotional states, TABLE 18.1 TOP TEN FORENSIC TOPICS
motivation, and effort on neuropsychological PUBLISHED IN THE THREE MOST
measures, there will be increasing demands for POPULAR CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY
involvement with relevant pediatric civil litigants JOURNALS 1 FROM 1990 TO 2000
and claimants and criminal defendants. In recent
years, relevant pediatric literature has in fact been 1. Malingering
growing (e.g., Donders, 2005; Flaro & Boone, 2. Measures of cognitive abilities
2009; McCaffrey & Lynch, 2009; Wills & Sweet, 3. Decision making
2006). Forensic applications unique to pediatric 4. Psychometrics (including test validity and
practice include issues of due process in the reliability)
educational settings, and medical malpractice 5. Litigation effects
claims including birth trauma (see Taylor chapter, 6. Base rates
this volume). Wynkoop (2008) presents an infor- 7. Ethics
mative review of forensic neuropsychology in the 8. Appropriate tests
juvenile justice system. 9. Expert testimony
10. Use of norms
Future Directions of Forensic Research 1
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Journal of Clinical and
Response Bias and Related Topics Experimental Neuropsychology, and The Clinical Neuropsychologist.
The adage that “past behavior is the best predictor Source: Sweet, J. J., King, J., Malina, A., Bergman, M., & Simmons, A.
(2002). Documenting the prominence of forensic neuropsychology
of future behavior” is likely to apply to those at national meetings and in relevant professional journals from
who conduct research relevant to forensic 1990–2000. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16, 481–494.
Trends in Forensic Practice and Research 507

established clinical neuropsychologists as the (Boone, 2007) and malingered neuropsychologi-


most likely experts in a decision-making arena to cal deficits (Larrabee, 2007). In no preceding year
have an objective methodology on which to base were two such books published. Even more
an opinion that responses are invalid and the recently, the first neuropsychology of malingering
extent to which the invalid responses represent casebook was published (Morgan & Sweet, 2009).
malingering. Second, base-rate studies of response If anything, the amount of interest and the
invalidity and malingering have demonstrated related activity of forensic researchers are increas-
that the context within which an evaluation takes ing on this topic. The fact that the American
place is highly associated with the risk of insuffi- Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology held its
cient effort and symptom over-reporting, which first ever consensus conference on the topic of
can invalidate test results (Ardolf, Denney, & “Neuropsychological Assessment of Effort,
Houston; 2007; Mittenberg, Patton, Canyock, & Response Bias, and Malingering” (Heilbronner,
Condit, 2002). Such research has established Sweet, Morgan, Larrabee, Millis, & Conference
clearly that involvement in a forensic context Participants, 2009) should serve to underscore
raises the risk of invalid responding, which of that this area of investigation will continue to be
course needs to be identified in order that deci- prominent to practitioners and researchers.
sions by judges, juries, disability insurance carri-
ers, and so forth are based on valid information. Continued Investigation of
Societal resources are finite; resources given to Assessment Context
nondeserving individuals diminish unnecessarily There is no question that the influence of context
the remaining resources available for those who on assessment results will continue to be a major
are deserving. Third, the domains of interest in part of forensic neuropsychology research. There
assigning awards or in allowing alternate conse- are likely to be more refined and sophisticated
quences (e.g., psychiatric hospitalization rather approaches used by researchers, especially as
than prison incarceration) to individuals who are archival databases grow in size, allowing for more
injured or ill are frequently the very practice specific analyses of subgroups. As a straightfor-
domains of clinical neuropsychologists (e.g., ward case example, consider that within a forensic
cognitive function, differential diagnosis of context of civil litigation there are circumstances
acquired brain dysfunction versus serious psychi- that can create opposite motivations for litigants.
atric illness). Fourth, there is a constant tension Figure 18.3 demonstrates a comparison of pre-
between the profession of clinical neuropsychol- sumptive negative and positive motivation within
ogy and the legal system with regard to sharing a comparable secondary gain context of individu-
detailed information regarding practice tech- als suing for damages after motor vehicle acci-
niques and decision rules. Though practitioners dents. Litigant A is pursuing a litigation claim of
strive to maintain test security and not share the persistent brain injury years subsequent to motor
intricate details of our response-bias detection vehicle accident involving a documented mild
methods, the legal system and the more general traumatic brain injury. Litigant B is pursuing a
interest of behavioral scientists to publish openly litigation claim of chronic pain and physical
in peer-review journals in order to allow inde- changes years subsequent to a motor vehicle acci-
pendent validation ensures that eventually new dent involving an uncomplicated mild traumatic
methods must be devised, which requires more brain injury, as well as multiple well-documented,
scientific research. For these reasons, it seems non-central nervous system injuries. The head
almost certain that topics related to response bias, injury of Litigant B was thought to be benign and
such as in the reporting of cognitive, somatic, and not to have caused persisting brain injury, which
affective symptoms and response validity in the explains why Litigant B has not claimed altered
performance of tests of ability that depend on the cognitive status as a result of the accident. In fact,
effort of the examinee, will continue to be the Litigant B’s claim of physical injury being caused
most widely researched of forensic topics. As these by the defendant requires that his memory of the
topics inform the general topic, malingering will accident scenario is considered intact because
continue to receive the highest degree of interest Litigant B’s recollection of the events of the acci-
from forensic neuropsychologists. dent does not match that of other witnesses, and
Reinforcing this view, consider that in 2007 has therefore been called into question. The other
two edited textbooks were published that focused witnesses described circumstances, which, if
exclusively on feigned cognitive impairment true, would prove that the defendant was not at
508 forensic neuropsychology

Test of Memory Malingering multiple sclerosis that had been judged in a rou-
100 tine clinical context to have caused memory
80 impairment. Again, as databases of forensic
experts increase in size, more fine-tuned group
60
Litigant A studies involving such contrasting subcontexts
40 will be possible.
Litigant B
20
0 Integrative Assessment Research
Trial 1 Trial 2 Retention Apart from the research topic of malingering and
its numerous subtopics, the primary bases of clin-
Victoria Symptom Validity Test ical practice and forensic practice for neuro-
100 psychologists are our test procedures, which are a
80 constant focus of research investigation. These
60 tests and the manner in which neuropsychologists
Litigant A integrate a wide variety of assessment information
40
Litigant B from multiple sources and ultimately reach
20 diagnostic and treatment decisions are topics that
0 are data based and on which we strive to make
Easy Difficult Total improvements via scientific research. There is an
ongoing need for research to provide guidance to
Green’s Word Memory Test
clinicians who must integrate many different indi-
100
vidual data, representing what in isolation may
80 vary from low reliability to high reliability, into a
60 robust opinion (Larrabee, 2008; Victor, Boone,
40 Litigant A Serpa, Buehler, & Ziegler, 2008). Related, among
Litigant B the extensions of current research approaches
20 that are likely to become more common is the
0 use of statistical analyses and decision rules
Immed. Delayed Consist. that consider multiple points of information to
Recall Recall
strengthen conclusions (e.g., Larrabee, 2008).
FIGURE 18.3: Positive and negative effects on
motivation within civil litigation (percent correct on Development of Diversity-related
three common effort measures). Specific Norms
The federal government and large professional
organizations, such as the American Psychological
fault. Therefore, if Litigant B’s memory is not Association, have in recent years emphasized the
accurate, the liability portion of the case is likely importance of identifying and respecting ethnic
to be dismissed. Moreover, a preexisting condi- and cultural diversity in our society. In a field, like
tion of multiple sclerosis of more than 20 years clinical neuropsychology, in which the fundamen-
and medical documentation by a neurologist tal assessment approach is normative, it is espe-
prior to the accident raised doubt regarding cially important to develop norms that can be
Litigant B’s ability to recall the accident accurately. applied appropriately to examinees. To be clear,
To summarize, whereas Litigant A may receive a this is not a new issue or a new development within
litigation award if memory is impaired by the the specialty. Rather, it is an issue that is receiving
accident at issue, Litigant B’s case may be dis- new emphasis, related to which a greater amount
missed if capable memory is not demonstrated. of energy is being invested. Related neuropsycho-
Within Figure 18.3, it is apparent that even the logical literature is growing, and will need to con-
same forensic context of civil litigation can have tinue to grow in order to keep professionals abreast
opposite effects on motivation on three com- of the rapidly changing language, racial, and ethnic
monly used effort measures that appear on the population demographics that comprise “culture”
surface to be tests of memory ability. Litigant A in the United States (Manly, 2008). Increasingly,
demonstrated obvious insufficient effort on the modal person being evaluated in the office of a
all three effort measures, whereas Litigant B U.S. clinical neuropsychologist will appear less
performed nearly flawlessly, despite longstanding well represented by past normative data, requiring
Trends in Forensic Practice and Research 509

development and application of new normative (cf. practice surveys, such as Sweet, Nelson, &
datasets that are more refined with regard to cul- Moberg, 2006). The same is true with regard to
tural diversity, including influences of low socio- other technologies, such as positron emission
economic status and low levels of literacy. More tomography (PET), single photon emission com-
pertinent to this textbook, increasingly, the indi- puterized tomography (SPECT), functional MRI
viduals involved in civil and criminal proceedings, (fMRI), functional near-infrared spectroscopy
workers’ compensation claims, and disability (fNIRS), and newer magnetic resonance-based
claims will have culturally diverse backgrounds, technologies, such as diffusion tensor imaging
presenting new challenges in understanding their (DTI). In fact, neuropsychologists have frequently
presentations on psychometric instruments. collaborated with physicians in terms of clinical
A recent summit conference focused on research involving sophisticated neuroimaging
“Challenges in the Neuropsychological Assess- technologies, as is evident in the relevant litera-
ment of Ethnic Minorities: A Problem Solving ture (see structural imaging review by Kurth &
Summit” (Romero, Lageman, Kamath, Irani, Sim, Bigler, 2008; and functional imaging review by
Suarez, et al., & the Summit Participants, 2009). Ricker & Arenth, 2008). With regard to functional
Experts on this topic and leaders of key neuropsy- brain imaging, neuropsychologists are so involved
chology organizations met for the purpose of dis- that the Division of Clinical Neuropsychology
cussing scientific advances, challenges, education (Division 40) of the American Psychological
and training, organizational change, and involve- Association published an official position paper
ment related to ensuring the accuracy of neuropsy- on the topic (APA Division 40, 2004). It seems
chological assessment results that could be that with each new sophisticated development of
affected by cultural background and ethnic minor- an imaging technology that can be applied to the
ity status. Using statistics from the Pew Hispanic brain, the opportunities for neuropsychological
Center as an example, the participants noted the research and growth of knowledge related to the
increasing diversity of examinees in the United brain and behavior expands further (see special
States in terms of language, culture, education, issue of The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 2007,
and ethnic minority status, which is not matched Issue 1 on “Neuropsychological Technologies” for
by the diversity of neuropsychological examiners additional information).
in these same variables. As the specialty of clinical What then of the application of neuroimaging
neuropsychology develops procedures and guide- technologies and other new experimental brain
lines for meeting these challenges, there will investigation techniques to forensic practice?
be implications for forensic neuropsychological Some, like the so-called “brain fingerprinting” are
evaluations. Related, forensic examiners will need unique commercial ventures that have received
to keep abreast of best practices and the means of sharp-edged critique by fellow psychophysiology
applying relevant norms. experts (cf. Rosenfeld, 2005). Other technologies,
such as PET, SPECT, and fMRI are more widely
Neuroimaging accepted in the scientific interdisciplinary neuro-
At this point, a perennial question pertaining to science research community, with a growing
neuroimaging might be raised by some readers. number of clinical applications in at least some
That is, will progress in neuroimaging have a neg- medical centers. However, related to these latter,
ative impact on clinical and forensic practice in more widely known technologies, numerous legal
years to come? The answer, borne out by historical authorities over time have provided cautionary
facts, is simply and emphatically “No.” In fact, the tones regarding premature adoption of new imag-
opposite has been true. Consider, for example, ing technologies by the legal system (e.g., Khoshbin
that in the nascent days of American clinical neu- & Khoshbin, 2007, Moriarty, 2008) and courts
ropsychology, there were no computerized tomog- have been understandably conservative in allow-
raphy (CT) and magnetic resonance image (MRI) ing such information into evidence. Petit (2007), a
scans of the brain. Those technologies had not yet law school professor who reviewed the status of
been applied clinically. It was during the era of federal court rulings related to admitting or exclud-
applications of CT and MRI to clinical conditions ing results of functional imaging and “brain fin-
of the brain that the specialty of clinical neuropsy- gerprinting” into evidence, provided an apt quote:
chology has seen its greatest growth in terms of
training programs, job positions, and growth of We have been and should continue to be
the private practice sector, all healthy signs cautious about bringing science into the
510 forensic neuropsychology

courtroom. We should keep the use of science individuals who are functioning normally and are
in judicial inquiries some distance behind the often interpreted using terms that indicate possi-
vanguards of scientific inquiry. But we have ble age-related change in small blood vessels,
been and should continue to be open to what is which is of unknown clinical significance.
helpful in the pursuit of the goals of our legal Ultimately, it is behavior, not images of brain
system, even if the result is a profound transfor- tissue, which is relevant to clinical and forensic
mation of how that system operates. (p. 340) consultations by neuropsychologists. Within neu-
ropsychology, this position has been clearly stated
Given that neuropsychologists are very involved by imaging expert Bigler (2001), in an article
in pushing the envelope of research technologies the title of which addresses the topic directly,
that can expand our understanding of brain- “Neuropsychological testing defines the neuro-
behavior relationships, we can look forward to an behavioral significance of neuro-imaging defined
augmentation, rather than a diminution, of our abnormalities.” Outside of neuropsychology,
current activities, as historical developments in Khobshin and Khobshin (2007) have expounded
technology have already shown. Perhaps as sug- on this point and noted:
gested by Ricker and Arenth (2008), functional
neuroimaging will become “a common adjunct The risks of the misuse of brain imaging in the
to traditional assessment and intervention.” courtroom are undeniable. We have strongly
However, in terms of near term application to recommended that even structural brain images
forensic practice, there are two developments that be used only for the purpose of linking a struc-
are prerequisite for reliance on these technologies. tural abnormality or injury to a specific deficit,
First, extensive experience in clinical settings that and then to be used only as a tool for interpreta-
establishes clear practice guidelines for the disor- tion by the expert witness to assess its clinical
der in question would need to be developed. The significance. Furthermore, we have recom-
American College of Radiology (ACR) develops mended that functional brain images not be
“appropriateness” guidelines, and in fact has done used for the purpose of linking a particular
so with regard to specific clinical conditions, such functional change in a modular fashion in the
as head trauma. At the time of last revision in brain to assess motivation, propensity, or respon-
2008 (ACR, 2008), the appropriateness of such sibility for a complex behavior or an inability to
functional technologies as PET and SPECT for inhibit it (very similar to the inadequacies of
evaluating head trauma of any severity was at the polygraphs currently used for lie-detection).
lowest possible rating. If the technologies under Given the current state of medical and scientific
consideration are deemed inappropriate in a rou- knowledge about brain science, once functional
tine clinical context, it is difficult to imagine a brain images are admitted as evidence for these
forensic context within which these technologies purposes, the adversarial system is an inade-
would be considered probative by a judge. Second, quate forum for determining the evidentiary
if sufficient scientific and clinical knowledge is validity of such evidence. Guidance is needed
eventually accrued for these technologies, such for judges who must make evidentiary determi-
that they are considered clinically appropriate, the nations from the medical profession, in con-
behavioral and real life implications of abnormal junction with relevant scientific societies,
findings will still need to be defined. To state it concerning the proper use of the images and of
simply, an anatomical image in isolation is not of the accompanying testimony in the courtroom.
use in a clinical or forensic context, unless the . . . We would like to emphasize that the image
image has a known relationship to function and alone used as evidence of behavior is at this time
quality of life, or impacts on issues of competence and for many years to come at best wishful
or criminal responsibility. An imaging abnormal- thinking and material for science fiction, and at
ity is not synonymous with a deficiency in the worst, pseudoscience. (pp. 191-192)
ability to function in daily life. This is well known
in mainstream neuroradiology. As a concrete To state an obvious truism about a fundamen-
example, frequently, neuroradiologists find evi- tal aspect of the relationship of brain function to
dence of brain tissue density changes that form a behavior . . . the brain is involved in all behavior,
patchy appearance described as “periventricular whether that behavior is a simple correct response
white matter changes” on CT or MRI structural to a well-researched “effort test,” known to be
scans of the brain. These findings can be found in objectively easy in terms of producing few errors,
Trends in Forensic Practice and Research 511

or more complex behavior that involves pretend- Even when new technologies reveal new
ing to be impaired when one is not actually means of creating images of the brain, this new
impaired. In fact, the nature of some behaviors, information is not automatically relevant to foren-
such as deliberately concealing information that is sic proceedings. With Hill’s criteria of causation in
known by simply disavowing the knowledge actu- mind, what is first needed in order to be relevant
ally involves a greater degree of brain activity and is the determination of whether such new images
different networks of activity within the brain. support causation. As Ricker (this volume) has
This well-known fact has been extensively docu- discussed, imaging technologies such as SPECT,
mented in years of research related to psychophys- PET, and fMRI remain investigational, in part due
iological means of detecting deception (e.g., see to the poor specificity of abnormal findings, a
review by Rosenfeld, 1999). Nevertheless, if one problem that significantly limits determination of
ignores what is already known and applies func- causation. What is also needed, as noted earlier in
tional imaging technology in a naïve manner to this section of our chapter, is the determination
the question of whether responding is effortful of whether the new images offer information
(e.g., Allen, Bigler, Larsen, Goodrich-Hunseker, & pertaining to real-life functioning.
Hopkins, 2007), erroneous logic can produce
unfounded interpretations. Much more reason- Overview of Future Research
ably and consistent with prior relevant research, With no factor identified on the horizon that is
Browndyke et al. (2008) used a common effort likely to alter greatly the fundamental activities of
test in concert with event-related fMRI to show clinical neuropsychologists, or the need for empir-
that neural circuits involved in feigned memory ical research as a means of making additional
vary with response demand and error type. progress, these topics are very likely to continue
Indeed, the truism remains that the brain is to receive a great deal of attention from forensic
involved in all behavior, including withholding of researchers. Their current status in terms of rank-
effort during evaluations. ing second and third, respectively, in Table 18.1 is
Though future developments in technology, unlikely to change in the next ten years.
especially imaging technology, will at times pro-
duce exciting advances, for any context in which Beyond Scientific Practice—The Final
causation is important, such as is true in forensic Frontier: Ethics and Objectivity
applications, new technologies will need to be One might conclude from the above that perhaps
scrutinized in a number of ways. The well-known continued scientific investigation will be the only
criteria of causation proposed by Sir Austin driving force that creates progress in forensic neu-
Bradford Hill (Hill, 1965) are relevant. Van ropsychology. While true that substantial progress
Reekum, Streiner, and Conn (2001) discussed will require continued scientific research, there
Hill’s causation criteria in the context of their are important topics that are relatively difficult,
possible application in the field of neuropsychia- and in some instances impossible, to scrutinize by
try, which is quite relevant to consideration of means of the scientific method. We turn our atten-
neuroimaging technologies. According to Reekum tion now to topics of ethics and objectivity.
et al., Hill’s causation criteria involve (1) demon-
strated strong association between possible Ethics
causative agent and outcome, (2) consistency of A clear signal that the subspecialty practice of
findings across sites and methodologies of forensic neuropsychology has begun to mature is
research, (3) demonstrated specificity of outcomes evidence of greater guidance in the literature
produced by possible causative agent, (4) tempo- regarding relevant professional behavior (within
ral sequence of possible causative agent occurring general practice guidelines: AACN, 2007; within
before the outcome of interest, (5) a dose-response official position papers related to forensic prac-
or “biological gradient” between possible caus- tice: Bush, Barth, et al., 2005; Bush, Ruff, et al.,
ative agent and outcome, (6) a “biologic rationale” 2005), and in particular greater guidance regard-
that plausibly explains the relationship between ing ethics. As readers know by reaching the end of
the possible causative agent and the outcome, this text, a complete chapter (chapter 4) by Grote
(7) “coherence,” such that what is already on ethics is contained in this current volume.
known is in agreement with the causation argu- Increasingly, ethics pertaining to forensic neurop-
ment, (8) evidence from experimental studies, sychology is a topic that is being addressed, in the
and (9) evidence from analogous conditions. form of journal articles (e.g., Grote, Lewin, Sweet,
512 forensic neuropsychology

& Van Gorp, 2000; Johnson-Greene & Bechtold, litigated case. It is well known that expert
2002; Malina, Nelson, & Sweet, 2005) and book witnesses often “collide” when offering opinions to
chapters (e.g., Fisher, Johnson-Greene, & Barth, legal decision-makers. Within the legal commu-
2002; Grote, 2005; Seward & Connor, 2009; Sweet, nity, there has been debate regarding how to resolve
2005; Sweet, Grote, & Van Gorp, 2002). Readers the often-observed “battle of experts” in litigation
can also refer to the general textbook on ethics (cf. Rein, Todd, & Howell, 2006). In fact, the fric-
written specifically for neuropsychologists by tion between experts has at times boiled over inap-
Bush (2007), within which there is a detailed propriately into accusations of ethical violations, to
listing of citations pertaining to forensic activities, the point that AACN created a position paper to
as well as a separate forensic ethics textbook, the offer guidance regarding resolution of concerns
co-authors of which include two neuropsycho- about an expert’s behavior that arise during adver-
logists (Bush, Connell, & Denney, 2006). sarial proceedings, such as civil litigation (AACN,
The trend of increasing attention being paid to 2003). As noted by Sweet (2005), it is axiomatic
ethics related to forensic activities of neuro- that (1) “. . .no one can claim a more ethical posi-
psychologists is likely to continue. Why? There tion simply by virtue of the fact that he or she is
are several fundamental aspects of performance retained more by the plaintiff or more by the
of professional work in forensic contexts that pro- defense in adversarial proceedings.” (p. 59), and (2)
vide a basis for understanding the ongoing need “. . .the rate of actual ethical violations is not pro-
to pay attention to ethics. First, as noted earlier, a portionate to the increased number of ethical con-
forensic context is inherently adversarial. In the cerns raised during forensic proceedings.” (p. 60)
process of providing opinions that are frequently Because of the increasing prominence of
met with contrary opinions and at times extensive forensic neuropsychology within the broader
questioning by attorneys who may favor a set of specialty of clinical neuropsychology, it seems
supporting opinions, interactions between neu- likely that issues related to objectivity versus bias
ropsychology colleagues can become less than will continue to be addressed at professional
“collegial.” Second, ethics address areas of conferences and workshops, and in relevant
professional conduct that are at times complicated peer-reviewed journals. That this occurs is not an
to understand in a routine clinical situation, which indication that neuropsychologists have any
are compounded by the interface of different greater degree of frequency or severity of these
professions (i.e., psychology, law) that may not problems. Rather, it is an indication that as a
share the same viewpoints on key issues. Third, group of specialists whose training and clinical
the change of roles and related practice behaviors practice is firmly rooted in a scientist-practitioner
when one is providing clinical services to a patient perspective, neuropsychologists strive to remain
versus providing forensic consultation to an objectively focused, even when applying our
attorney regarding a plaintiff can be substantial, knowledge base in an adversarial context creates
and at times contradictory. Fourth, as noted previ- pressures to do otherwise.
ously, it appears that clinical neuropsychology as a
specialty is continuing to develop a greater S U M M A RY A N D
involvement in forensic activities. FINAL ARGUMENTS
As noted within this chapter and indeed evident
Objectivity versus Bias: The Roles throughout this textbook, forensic neuropsycho-
and Behavior of Experts logy has become well recognized and well estab-
Related to ethics, the specific topic of the impor- lished within the American civil and criminal
tance of maintaining objectivity while engaged as legal system, as well as in other forensic settings.
a neuropsychologist in forensic tasks has been Though the majority of our analysis has focused
discussed increasingly. Early on in this discussion, on relevant data and scientific investigations, the
Sweet and Moulthrop (1999a, 1999b) published a evidence that forensic neuropsychology has
series of self-examination questions aimed at “arrived” is also easy to find on an anecdotal level.
encouraging individual practitioners to proac- Any experienced forensic neuropsychologist can
tively consider whether their opinions were objec- provide examples of the now common place
tive. Johnson-Greene and Bechtold (2002) offered agreement of other experts in medical specialties
guidance from an ethics perspective regarding to defer to the opinions of clinical neuropsycho-
appropriate conduct when reviewing the work of logists regarding the selection, administration,
a forensic expert retained on the opposite side of a scoring, and interpretation of neuropsychological
Trends in Forensic Practice and Research 513

assessment procedures. This was not always the action could be envisioned to parallel what is
case, as perhaps even 15–20 years ago, physicians currently taking place at present in the form of
were commonly expected to be deferred to by steps being taken within the American Board
psychologists on all matters pertaining to injury of Professional Psychology (ABPP) to create a
and health status, and the reverse scenario of a subspecialization in pediatric neuropsychology,
physician deferring to a psychologist was not at all within ABPP’s specialty board of the American
common. Board of Clinical Neuropsychology.
Whereas popular media have occasionally The career length of a clinical neuropsycholo-
depicted assessment procedures, such as testing gist can easily be thirty years. In the span of a
with inkblots, well-known television shows such single career of that length, the specialty practice
as Law and Order now explicitly refer to specific area of forensic neuropsychology has literally
expertise of a “neuropsychologist.” The Alaska Bar come into existence and become a visible and
Association newsletter known as “The Alaska Bar substantial part of clinical neuropsychology. We
Rag” reported on the disciplinary action of an have no way of knowing what another thirty-year
attorney whose requirement to be reinstated was span will bring, but no factor yet identified sug-
to submit the expert opinion “from a board- gests that the current trajectory and presence of
certified psychologist or neuropsychologist that forensic neuropsychology will diminish.
he is fit to engage in the practice of law” (Alaska
Bar Association, April-June 2008). And, perhaps
REFERENCES
the most interesting example of all is the state
Alaska Bar Association (April-June 2008). Board of
court appeal of an individual convicted of murder Governors action item April 28 & 29, 2008. The
and sentenced to death. The appeal (Clark v. Alaska Bar Rag.
Mitchell, 1992/2005) alleged that the conviction Allen, M., Bigler, E., Larsen, J., Goodrich-Hunsaker,
occurred in large part because the defense lawyer N., & Hopkins, R. (2007). Functional neuroimag-
had not retained a neuropsychologist to present ing evidence for high cognitive effort on the Word
evidence! Memory Test in the absence of external incentives.
There seems little doubt that continued scien- Brain Injury, 21, 1425–28.
tific investigation related to neuropsychological American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology.
assessment procedures will continue to bolster (2001). Policy statement on the presence of third
the credibility of scientifically oriented forensic party observers in neuropsychological assessments.
neuropsychologists who provide evidence that The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 433–39.
informs the triers of fact in adversarial proceed- American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology.
ings, whether in the courtroom or in other (2003). Official position of the American Academy
contexts. As involvement in forensic settings of Clinical Neuropsychology on ethical complaints
continues to increase, there is a strong likelihood made against clinical neuropsychologists during
that relevant graduate coursework and post- adversarial proceedings. The Clinical Neuro-
graduate residency opportunities will increase. psychologist 17, 443–45.
A strong likelihood exists that forensic neuro- American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology.
psychologists will be board certified, which, (2007, July 30). Amicus brief in support of flexible
battery use: State of New Hampshire Supreme Court.
with the exception of the specialty of clinical
Retrieved July 29, 2009, from http://www.theaacn.
neuropsychology, is still not well established in
org/position_papers/Baxter_v_Temple.pdf
other psychology practice specialties. In fact,
American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology.
AACN, the membership organization of American (2007). Practice guidelines for neuropsychological
board-certified clinical neuropsychologists, has, assessment and consultation. The Clinical Neuro-
since its inception of an annual meeting, recog- psychologist, 21, 209–31.
nized the importance of forensic practice by American College of Radiology. (2008, October). ACR
assigning a substantial portion of its meeting appropriateness criteria®: Head trauma. Retrieved
program to workshops by experts in forensic June 24, 2010, from http://www.acr.org/ac
neuropsychology. Related, as more individuals American Psychological Association. (1992). Ethical
within the specialty of clinical neuropsychology principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
take on the identity of a “forensic neuropsycholo- American Psychologist, 47, 1597–1611.
gist,” there is a strong likelihood that a board American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical
certification subspecialty in forensic neuropsy- principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
chology will be created. In many ways, this future American Psychologist, 57, 1060–73.
514 forensic neuropsychology

American Psychological Association Division 40. Cheng, E. K. & Yoon, A. H. (2005). Does Frye or
(2004). Official position of the Division of Clinical Daubert matter? A study of scientific admissibility
Neuropsychology (APA Division 40) on the role of standards. Virginia Law Review, 91, 471–513.
neuropsychologists in clinical use of fMRI: Clark v. Mitchell, 584 N.E.2d 662 (Ohio St. 1992), denial
Approved by the Division 40 Executive Committee of writ affirmed, 425 F.3d 270 (6th Cir. 2005).
July 28, 2004. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 18, Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc., 509 U.S.
349–51. 579 (1993).
American Psychological Association. (2010). Index of Dahir, V. B., Richardson, J. T., Ginsburg, G. P., Gatowski,
APA amicus briefs by issues. Retrieved June 21, S. I., Dobbin, S. A., & Merlino, M. L. (2005). Judicial
2010, from http://www.apa.org/about/offices/ogc/ application of Daubert to psychological syndrome
amicus/index-issues.aspx and profile evidence: A research note. Psychology,
Ardolf, B. R., Denney, R. L., & Houston, C. M. (2007). Public Policy, and Law, 11, 62–82.
Base rates of negative response bias and malingered Donders, J. (2005). Performance on the Test of Memory
neurocognitive dysfunction among criminal defen- Malingering in a mixed pediatric sample. Child
dants referred for neuropsychological evaluation. Neuropsychology, 11, 221–27.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21, 899–916. Duvall, J. C. & Morris, R. J. (2006). Assessing mental
Atkins v. Virginia, 536 U.S. 304 (2002). retardation in death penalty cases: Critical issues
Bauer, R. M. & Sweet, J. J. (Eds.). (2007). Special sec- for psychology and psychological practice.
tion on neuropsychological technologies [Special Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 37
issue]. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 21(1). (6), 658–65.
Baxter v. Temple, 157 N.H. 280, 949 A.2d 167 (2008). Faigman, D. L. & Monahan, J. (2005). Psychological
Bigler, E. (2001). Neuropsychological testing defines evidence at the dawn of the law’s scientific age.
the neurobehavioral significance of neuroimaging- Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 631–59.
identified abnormalities. Archives of Clinical Neuro- Fisher, J. M., Johnson-Greene, D., & Barth, J. T. (2002).
psychology, 16, 227–36. Evaluation, diagnosis, and interventions in clinical
Boone, K. B. (Ed.). (2007). Assessment of feigned cogni- neuropsychology in general and special popula-
tive impairment: A neuropsychological perspective. tions: An overview. In S. Bush & M. Drexler (Eds.)
New York: Guilford Press. Ethical issues in clinical neuropsychology (pp. 3–22).
Browndyke, J., Paskavitz, J., Sweet, L., Cohen, R., New York: Taylor & Francis.
Tucker, K., Welsh-Bohmer, et al. (2008). Flaro, L. & Boone, K. (2009). Using objective effort
Neuroanatomical correlates of malingered memory measures to detect noncredible cognitive test per-
impairment: Event-related fMRI of deception on a formance in children and adolescent. In J. Morgan
recognition memory task. Brain Injury, 22, 481–89 & J. Sweet (Eds.) Neuropsychology of malingering
Bush, S. S. (Ed.). (2005). A casebook of ethical challenges casebook. New York: Psychology Press.
in neuropsychology. New York: Psychology Press. Frye v. United States, 293 F. 1013 (D.C. Cir. 1923).
Bush, S. S. (Ed.). (2007). Ethical decision making in clini- Garner, B. A. (Ed.). (1999). Black’s law dictionary. (7th
cal neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University ed.). St. Paul, MN: West Group.
Press. Greiffenstein, M. F. (2009). Clinical myths of forensic
Bush, S., Barth, J., Pliskin, N., Arffa, S., Axelrod, B., neuropsychology. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
Blackburn, L., et al. (2005). Independent and 23 (2), 286–96.
court-ordered forensic neuropsychological exami- Grote, C. L. (2005). Ethical challenges in forensic neu-
nations: Official statement of the National Academy ropsychology, Part II. In S. Bush (Ed.) A Casebook
of Neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical Neuro- of ethical challenges in neuropsychology (pp. 23–29).
psychology, 20, 997–1007. New York: Taylor & Francis.
Bush, S., Connell, M., & Denney, R. (2006). Ethical Grote, C. L., Lewin, J. L., Sweet, J. J., & Van Gorp, W. G.
practice in forensic psychology: A systematic model (2000). Responses to perceived unethical practices
for decision making. Washington, D.C.: American in clinical neuropsychology: Ethical and legal con-
Psychological Association. siderations. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 14,
Bush, S. S. & Drexler, M. L. (Eds.). (2002). Ethical issues 119–34.
in clinical neuropsychology. Exton, PA: Swets & Groscup, J. L., Penrod, S. D., Studebaker, C. A., Huss,
Zeitlinger. M. T., & O’Neil, K. M. (2002). The effects of Daubert
Bush, S., Ruff, R., Tröster, A., Barth, J., Koffler, S., on the admissibility of expert testimony in state
Pliskin, N., et al. (2005). Symptom validity assess- and federal criminal cases. Psychology, Public
ment: Practice issues and medical necessity. Official Policy, and Law, 8, 339–72.
position of the National Academy of Neuro- Heilbronner, R. L., Sweet, J. J., Morgan, J. E.,
psychology. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, Larrabee, G. J., Millis, S. R., & Conference
20, 419–26. Participants. (2009). American Academy of Clinical
Trends in Forensic Practice and Research 515

Neuropsychology consensus conference statement National Academy of Neuropsychology. (2000a).


on the neuropsychological assessment of effort, Presence of third party observers during neuro-
response bias, and malingering. The Clinical Neuro- psychological testing: Official statement of the
psychologist, 23, 1093–129. National Academy of Neuropsychology. Archives of
Hill, A. B. (1965). The environment and disease: Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 379–80.
Association or causation? Proceedings of the Royal National Academy of Neuropsychology. (2000b). Test
Society of Medicine, 58, 293–300. security: Official statement of the National
Johnson-Greene, D. & Bechtold, K. T. (2002). Ethical Academy of Neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical
considerations for peer review in forensic neuro- Neuropsychology, 15, 383–86.
psychology. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16, News from the bar: Board of Governors action item
97–104. April 28 and 29, 2008. (2008, April-June). The
Kaufmann, P. (2009). Protecting raw data and psycho- Alaska Bar Rag, 32(2).
logical tests from wrongful disclosure: A primer on Norcross, J., Karpiak, C., & Santoro, S. (2005). Clinical
the law and other persuasive strategies. The Clinical psychologists across the years: The Division of
Neuropsychologist, 23, 1130–59. Clinical Psychology from 1960 to 2003. Journal of
Khoshbin, L. S. & Khoshbin, S. (2007). Imaging the Clinical Psychology, 61, 1467–83.
mind, minding the image: An historical introduc- Petit, M. (2007). FMRI and BF meet FRE: Brain imag-
tion to brain imaging and the law. American Journal ing and the Federal Rules of Evidence. American
of Law & Medicine, 33, 171–92. Journal of Law & Medicine, 33 (2007): 319–40.
Kurth, S. & Bigler, E. (2008). Structural neuroimaging Rein, B., Todd, K., & Howell, J. (2006). Collisions
in clinical neuropsychology. In J. Morgan & of expert testimony: Why Rule 56 should be
J. Ricker (Eds.) Textbook of clinical neuropsycho- amended. Publication of the AEI-Brookings Center
logy. New York: Psychology Press. for Regulatory Studies. Retrieved April 6, 2009
Larrabee, G. (Ed.). (2007). Assessment of malingered from http://www.wileyrein.com/docs/publications/
neuropsychological deficits. New York: Oxford 12796.pdf
University Press. Ricker, J. & Arenth, P. (2008). Functional neuroimag-
Larrabee, G. (2008). Aggregation across multiple indi- ing in clinical neuropsychology. In J. Morgan &
cators improves the detection of malingering: J. Ricker (Eds.) Textbook of clinical neuropsychol-
Relationship to likelihood ratios. The Clinical ogy. New York: Psychology Press.
Neuropsychologist, 22, 666–79. Romero, H. R., Lageman, S.K., Kamath, V., Irani, F.,
Levett, L. & Kovera, M. (2008). The effectiveness of Sim, A., Suarez, P., et al., the Summit Participants.
opposing expert witnesses for educating jurors (2009). Proceedings: Challenges in the Neuro-
about unreliable expert evidence. Law and Human psychological Assessment of Ethnic Minorities:
Behavior, 32, 363–74. A Problem-Solving Summit. The Clinical Neuro-
Malina, A., Nelson, N., & Sweet, J. (2005). Framing the psychologist, 23, 761–79.
relationships in forensic neuropsychology: Ethical Rosenfeld, J. P. (1999). Cognitive psychophysiology in
issues. In S. Bush (Guest Ed.) Special Issue: Ethics detection of malingered cognitive deficit. In J. Sweet
in forensic neuropsychology. Journal of Forensic (Ed.). Forensic neuropsychology: Fundamentals and
Neuropsychology, 4, 21–44. practice. New York: Taylor & Francis.
Manly, J. (2008). Critical issues in cultural neuro- Rosenfeld, J. P. (2005). “Brain fingerprinting”: A criti-
psychology: Profit from diversity. Neuropsychology cal analysis. The Scientific Review of Mental Health
Review, 18, 179–83. Practice, 4, 20–37.
McCaffrey, R. & Lynch, J. (2009). Malingering follow- Seward, J. & Connor, D. (2009). Ethical issues in assign-
ing documented brain injury: Neuropsychological ing (or withholding) a diagnosis of malingering.
evaluation of children in a forensic setting. In J. In J. Morgan & J. Sweet (Eds.) Neuropsychology
Morgan & J. Sweet (Eds.) Neuropsychology of malin- of malingering casebook. New York: Psychology
gering casebook. New York: Psychology Press. Press.
Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit, Sweet, J. J. (2005). Ethical challenges in forensic neuro-
D. C. (2002). Baserates of malingering and symp- psychology. Part V. In S. Bush, (Ed.) A casebook of
tom exaggeration. Journal of Clinical and Experi- ethical challenges in neuropsychology. New York:
mental Neuropsychology, 24, 1094–1102. Taylor & Francis.
Morgan, J. E. & Sweet, J. J. (Eds.). (2009). Neuro- Sweet, J. J., Ecklund-Johnson, E., & Malina, A. (2008).
psychology of malingering casebook. New York: Overview of forensic neuropsychology. In J. Morgan
Psychology Press. & J. Ricker (Eds.) Textbook of clinical neuropsy-
Moriarty, J. C. (2008). Flickering admissibility: chology. New York: Taylor & Francis.
Neuroimaging evidence in the U.S. courts. Sweet, J. J., Grote, C., & Van Gorp, W. (2002).
Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 26, 29–49. Ethical issues in forensic neuropsychology.
516 forensic neuropsychology

In S. Bush, & M. Drexler (Eds.) Ethical issues in Taylor, S. (1999). The legal environment pertaining to
clinical neuropsychology (pp. 103–33) New York: clinical neuropsychology. In J. J. Sweet (Ed.). Forensic
Taylor & Francis. neuropsychology: Fundamentals and practice. New
Sweet, J. J., King, J., Malina, A., Bergman, M., & Simmons, York: Psychology Press.
A. (2002). Documenting the prominence of forensic Van Reekum, R., Streiner, D., & Conn, D. (2001).
neuropsychology at national meetings and in relevant Applying Bradford Hill’s criteria for causation to
professional journals from 1990–2000. The Clinical neuropsychiatry: Challenges and opportunities.
Neuropsychologist, 16, 481–94. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuro-
Sweet, J. J., Moberg, P. J., & Suchy, Y. (2000). Ten-year science, 13, 318–25.
follow-up survey of clinical neuropsychologists: Victor, T., L., Boone, K. B., Serpa, J. G., Buehler, J., &
Part I. Practice and beliefs. The Clinical Neuro- Ziegler, E. A. (2008). Interpreting the meaning of
psychologist, 14, 18–37. multiple symptom validity test failure. The Clinical
Sweet, J. J. & Moulthrop, M. (1999a). Self-examination Neuropsychologist, 23, 297–313.
questions as a means of identifying bias in adver- Wills, K. & Sweet, J. J. (2006). Neuropsychological con-
sarial cases. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 1, siderations in forensic child assessment. In J. Sparta
73–88. & G. Koocher (Eds.). Forensic mental health assess-
Sweet, J. J. & Moulthrop, M. (1999b). Response to Lees- ment of children and adolescents (pp. 260–84). New
Haley commentary: Debiasing techniques cannot York: Guilford Press.
be completely curative. Journal of Forensic Neuro- Wynkoop, T. F. (2008). Neuropsychology in the juve-
psychology, 1, 49–57. nile justice system. In R. L. Denney & J. P. Sullivan
Sweet, J. J., Nelson, N., & Moberg, P. (2006). The TCN/ (Eds.). Clinical neuropsychology in the criminal
AACN “Salary Survey”: Professional practices, forensic setting (pp. 295–325). New York: Guilford
beliefs, and incomes of U. S. neuropsychologists. Press.
The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20, 325–64.
INDEX

Page numbers followed by an “f, ” “t,” or “n” denote figures, tables, or notes, respectively.

AACN. See American Academy of Clinical dangerousness and, 484–85


Neuropsychology SPECT and, 166, 170
AAN. See American Academy of Neurology TBI and, 265
ABA. See American Bar Association alcohol, 283
ABA Model Rules of Professional Conduct, 26–27, 27t amnesia from, 464
ABCN. See American Board of Clinical diminished capacity from, 482
Neuropsychology MTBI and, 236, 243, 251
abduction, 60n1 neurotoxic injury from, 291
Ability Focused Battery (AFB), 14–15 neurotoxic symptoms from, 288t
ability to assist, 446 pediatric TBI and, 211
abnormal performance on tests, 17 SPECT for, 166
absolute discovery, 31–32 ALI. See American Law Institute
AC. See Attention Concentration Index aluminum, 284t
N-acetylaspartate: creatine ratio, 290 Alzheimer’s disease (AD), 8, 401
ACRM. See American Congress of Rehabilitation capacity with, 407–9, 409t
Medicine FC with, 417–20
ACS. See Advanced Clinical Solutions informed consent with, 407–9
activated imaging, 161 MTBI and, 240
activities of daily living (ADL), 415 PCS and, 246
actus reus, 475 TBI and, 270–71
AD. See Alzheimer’s disease American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology
ADC. See apparent diffusion coefficient (AACN), 77, 503
ADHD. See attention deficit hyperactivity disorder on ethics, 102, 104
ad hoc hypotheses, 3 on raw data, 104, 105f, 108–9
ADL. See activities of daily living American Academy of Forensic Psychology, 40
administrative law, 38t American Academy of Neurology (AAN), 13
admissibility MTBI and, 233
Daubert and, 88, 90–91 QEEG and, 169SPECT and, 166
expert testimony and, 70–94 American Bar Association (ABA), 402
in trial phase, 50–52 American Board of Clinical Neuropsychology
wrongful disclosure and, 91–93 (ABCN), 25
zealous advocacy and, 79 MTBI and, 236
Advanced Clinical Solutions (ACS), 16, 125–26 American College of Occupational and Environmental
adversarial setting, 26 Health Medicine, 283
advocacy. See also zealous advocacy American College of Radiology, 161
expert testimony and, 60n12 SPECT and, 166
AFB. See Ability Focused Battery American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine
affective lability, 265 (ACRM), 232
affirmative defense, 474, 477t American Law Institute (ALI), 476–77
Afghanistan, 244 American Psychological Association (APA), 102,
age. See also dementia 402, 503
pediatric TBI and, 216 ethics and, 103, 104, 111
aggression. See also dangerousness risk on informed consent, 111
518 index
amicus brief, 24 Atypical Response (ATR), 377–78
in Baxter, 77 AUC. See area under curve
amnesia. See also post-traumatic amnesia Auditory Number Search Test, 266
anterograde, 264 Auditory Verbal Learning Test. See Rey Auditory Verbal
CST and, 464–67, 465t Learning Test
GOAT, 262 authority figures, 43
amphetamine, 288t Average Impairment Rating, 342
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, 241 AVLT. See Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
analogy, 9
ANAM, 239–40 badgering, 55
Andreasen, Nancy, 368–69 Balance Error Scoring System (BESS), 238f
Andrews. See United States v. Andrews Barefoot v. Estelle, 482
anonymity, 39 basal ganglia/thalamus pattern, 187
anterograde amnesia, 264 base rate, 11–12, 507
anxiety, 350 COI, 12
chronic pain and, 307 for malingering, 141, 145
generalized anxiety disorders, 166 for MTBI, 4–5, 246t
medically unexplained symptoms and, 341 for neurotoxic injury, 294–98, 295t–296t
MTBI and, 240 Bates stamped, 60n14
neurotoxic injury and, 296 Baxter v. Temple, 24, 75–77, 504, 505
PTSD and, 388 amicus brief in, 77
TBI and, 265 fixed battery and, 70
APA. See American Psychological Association flexible battery and, 70
Apgar, 184 in limine in, 75
IQ and, 187 Bayes Information Criterion (BIC), 15
apnea, 183 Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID), 181
apolipoprotein E, 212 Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function
apoptosis, 184 (BRIEF), 221
apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC), 252 behavior therapy, 318, 389
area under curve (AUC), 11 Bendectin, 73
ACS and, 125–26 Benton, Arthur, 12
AFB and, 15 Benton Visual Form Discrimination (VFD), 125,
malingering and, 124 125t, 136
TBI and, 134 benzodiazepines, 248, 249f
Arithmetic, WAIS-IV, 7 BESS. See Balance Error Scoring System
arsenic, 169, 284t beyond a reasonable doubt, 28, 475
arterial end zones, 180 bias
arthritis, 329 CARB, 122
arthrography, 333 confirmation, 5, 57
assessment phase, 40–49 Consensus Conference Statement on the
general contours and, 40–45 Neuropsychological Assessment of Effort, Response
report writing in, 45–49 Bias, and Malingering, 77
association, 8 debiasing procedures, 45
asthma, 181 ethics and, 107
Atkins v. Virginia, 487–88, 505 hindsight bias, 5–6
ATP. See Atypical Presentation Scale in neuropsychological evaluation, 4–6
ATR. See Atypical Response vs. objectivity, 512
Attention Concentration Index (AC), 6 RBS, 138–40, 147
MTBI and, 236 response bias, 77, 120–25
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), in testimony, 57
181–82, 183 BIC. See Bayes Information Criterion
MTBI and, 248, 249f, 250 binomial theorem, 17, 122
neurotoxic injury and, 291 bio-associative mechanisms, 340
pediatric TBI and, 213–14, 217 biological gradient, 8
attorney interactions, 24–26 biological plausibility, 289
phases of, 35–57 biological severity indexing, 7
principles of, 59t biomarkers, 212
attributions, 338 bipolar disorder, 241, 248, 249f
Atypical Presentation Scale (ATP), 461 birth defects, 74
index 519

Block Design carpal tunnel syndrome, 329


raw data from, 104 Carpenter v. Yamaha Motor Corp., 92
WAIS-IV, 7 CAST*MR. See Competence Assessment for Standing
blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD), 163–64 Trial for Defendants with Mental Retardation
Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Exam, 266, 293 catastrophizing, 306
Boston Process Approach (BPA), 75–77 Category Test, 16
Boulder Conference, 71 malingering and, 124, 131
BPA. See Boston Process Approach MTBI and, 234
BPD. See bronchopulmonary dysplasia WAIS-IV, 7
Brady, James, 82 causation, 8, 511
brain. See also perinatal brain injury; traumatic brain competency and, 447–48
injury; specific features of brain expert testimony and, 80–81
intrapartum brain damage, 56 perinatal brain injury and, 193–94
pediatric TBI and, 216 CBT. See cognitive behavior therapy
plasticity of, 216 CCTI. See Capacity to Consent to Treatment Instrument
BRIEF. See Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive CDC. See Centers for Disease Control
Function CDR. See Clinical Dementia Rating
Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI), 239 Centers for Disease Control (CDC), 232, 260
bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD), 180 central nervous system (CNS). See also brain; spine
Broyles v. Reilly, 92 injury to, 5
Bruno-Golden, Barbara, 75–7 neurotoxic injury in, 290, 298
BSI. See Brief Symptom Inventory toxins and, 283
BSID. See Bayley Scales of Infant Development central sinovenous thrombosis, 191
b Test, 121–22 cerebellum
burden of proof, 454–55 pediatric TBI and, 212
SPECT and, 162
CAI. See Competency Assessment Instrument cerebral palsy (CP), 180–81
calcium, 184 cerebral sinovenous thrombosis and, 191
California Verbal Learning Test-Children’s Version HIE and, 186, 188
(CVLT-C), 214, 218, 218t, 221–22 IPS and, 188–90
California Verbal Learning Test-Second Edition cerebral reserve, 242–43
(CVLT-II), 6, 13, 15 cerebrovascular accident. See stroke
malingering and, 124–25, 129 cerebrovascular disease, 166
neurotoxic injury and, 293 certiorari, 74
raw data from, 104 cesarean section, 189
Cameron. See United States v. Cameron CFS. See chronic fatigue syndrome
cannabis. See marijuana Chandler Exterminators v. Morris, 51
CAP. See Confusion Assessment Protocol Chappelle v. Ganger, 60n17
capacity. See also diminished capacity; financial capacity; CHI. See closed head injury
testamentary capacity child abuse
with AD, 407–9, 409t chronic pain and, 304–5
vs. competency, 403 pediatric TBI and, 211, 218–19
FC, 415–21 Chiperas v. Rubin, 31
FCEs, 333–34 chorioamnionitis, 189
FCI, 417–421, 418t, 419t, 420t, 422t, 423t chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS), 141, 339–40, 344–45
for informed consent, 405–15 chronic pain, 302–19
with MCI, 409–10 anxiety and, 307
with PD, 410–14 behavior therapy for, 317–19
with PDDS, 413t catastrophizing with, 306
TC, 421–31 child abuse and, 304–5
Capacity to Consent to Treatment Instrument (CCTI), diagnosis and assessment of, 311–15, 332–34
406–9, 407t, 408t, 409t Digit Span and, 128
for PDDS, 411–15, 411t, 412t, 413t, factitious disorder and, 312–13
CAPS. See Clinician Administered PTSD Scale FBS and, 136
CARB. See Computerized Assessment of Response Bias fear-avoidance with, 307–8, 308f
carbon disulfide, 286t interventions for, 316–19
carbon monoxide (CO), 283, 288t, 289, 290–91 malingering and, 147, 306–7, 313–15
carbon tetrachloride, 286t mood disorders and, 307
carboxyhemoglobin, 169, 290 MPQ and, 147
520 index
chronic pain (continued) Community Integration Questionnaire (CIQ), 268
MTBI and, 250 compelling inconsistency, 118
personality disorders and, 306–7 compensation for services
psychological screening for, 316–17 ethics and, 107
psychosocial factors in, 304–8 pre-assessment phase and, 39–40
risk factors for, 309–11, 317t Competence Assessment for Standing Trial for
somatization disorder and, 305–6 Defendants with Mental Retardation (CAST*MR),
somatoform disorders and, 312 459–60
specific conditions, 329–32 competency, 82
treatment for, 334–35 assessment tools for, 456–64
chronic widespread pain, 329–30 vs. capacity, 403
cingulate gyrus, 290 causation and, 447–48
CIQ. See Community Integration Questionnaire as Constitutional right, 445–46
civil law, 38t as contextual issue, 447
TPO and, 35 in criminal law, 444t
Clark v. Arizona, 478 for death penalty, 486–87
Classification of Violence Risk (COVR), 485 as decisional capacity, 451–52
classification statistics, 9–12 definition of, 402–3
clear and convincing evidence, 28 with dementia, 401–32
Cline v. Firestone Tire, 33 evaluation of, 450t
clinical assessment general, 403–4
competency requirements of neuropsychologists for, intermittent, 404
102–3 limited, 403
instruments for, 43 malingering and, 148
Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR), 429 to plead guilty, 452–54
clinical psychology, five noble traditions of, 4 prescriptive remediation for, 448–49
clinical reports, 45–49, 45t to refuse insanity defense, 455–56
Clinician Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS), 372, 374, restoration of, 404
379, 384–87 specific, 403–4
closed head injury (CHI) SVT for, 466
b Test and, 122 test scores for, 463t
malingering and, 124, 149 treatment for, 467–68
SPECT and, 166 to waive counsel, 452–54
CNS. See central nervous system to waive death penalty appeals, 487
CO. See carbon monoxide Competency Assessment Instrument (CAI), 457, 458t
coagulation disorders, 190 competency requirements of neuropsychologists
cocaine, 283 for clinical assessment, 102–3
neurotoxic symptoms from, 288t ethics and, 106–7
SPECT and, 166 competency to stand trial (CST), 40
Coccidioides immitis, 347 amnesia and, 464–67, 465t
coercion, 451–52 in criminal law, 438–69
cognitive abnormalities, 354 diminished capacity and, 442–44
cognitive behavior therapy (CBT), 318, 389 expert testimony and, 82–83
cognitive deficits. See also competency neuroimaging and, 171
alternative explanations for, 338–39 plea entering with, 83
death penalty and, 84, 487–88 punishment and, 84
HIE and, 185–86 trial and, 83–84
with MTBI, 242 videotaping and, 442–44
from neurotoxic injury, 289f Complex Figure Test. See Rey Complex Figure Test
with PTSD, 379–81 complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS), 330
VLBW/VPTB and, 181–82, 195 compressed spectral array (CSA), 164–65
cognitive symptoms, 336–37 computed tomography (CT), 333, 509
Cohen’s d, 240 for MTBI, 233, 241–42
coherence, 9 for neurotoxic injury, 298
COI. See condition of interest for pediatric TBI, 212
collegiality versus opposition, 29 for TBI, 265
collusion, 107 for toxic exposure, 169–70
Colorado v. Connelly, 451–52 Computerized Assessment of Response Bias (CARB), 122
Coma Recovery Scale, 266 computerized dynamic posturography (CPD), 147
index 521

conceptual bracket creep, 370 CST. See competency to stand trial


concussion, 233 CT. See computed tomography
condition of interest (COI), 10 current ability, 446
base rate, 12 insanity defense and, 479–80
confidentiality, in criminal law, 491 cutoff scores, 9–10
confirmation bias, 5, 57 CVLT-C. See California Verbal Learning Test-Children’s
integrity check and, 57 Version
confusion CVLT-II. See California Verbal Learning Test-Second
from expert testimony, 89–90 Edition
PTCS, 262, 264 cytokines, 184
with TBI, 263–64
Confusion Assessment Protocol (CAP), 264, 266 dangerousness risk
Consensus Conference Statement on the assessment of, 482–83
Neuropsychological Assessment of Effort, Response assessment tools for, 485–86
Bias, and Malingering, 77 neuropathology of, 484–85
consistency analysis, 6–8 risk factors for, 483–84
consistency of association, 8 DAPS. See Detailed Assessment of Posttraumatic Stress
Consonant Trigrams, neurotoxic injury and, 293 Daubert v. Merrell Dow, 3, 50–52, 56, 74, 504
consulting admissibility and, 71, 73, 88, 90–91
fixed battery for, 75–77 criminal law and, 88, 90
flexible battery for, 75–77 expert testimony and, 90–91
litigation, 36–38 Frye and, 90
Continuous Recognition Memory Test (CRM), 130 lie detection and, 86
Continuous Visual Memory Test (CVMT), 130 trilogy of, 74–75
Controlled Oral Word Association, 35 wrongful disclosure and, 92
MTBI and, 237 death penalty
Cooley, Wes, 351 cognitive deficits and, 84
Cooper v. Oklahoma, 455 competency for, 486–87
coping, 240 criminal responsibility and, 486–90
corpus callosum, 212 mental retardation and, 487–88
corrective mechanisms, 39 mitigating circumstances with, 489–90
corticosteroids, 334–35 de bene esse deposition, 52
courtroom familiar, 26 debiasing procedures, 45
COVR. See Classification of Violence Risk deception
CP. See cerebral palsy fMRI for, 171–72
CPD. See computerized dynamic posturography neuroimaging for, 171–72
criminal law, 38t test for, 72–73
competency in, 444t Decosta. See Limbaugh-Kirker v. Decosta
confidentiality in, 491 definition by exclusion, 87
CST in, 438–69 delayed onset syndrome, 185, 187
Daubert and, 88 HIE and, 194
ethics in, 490–93 delayed PTSD, 375–76
expert testimony in, 87–90 dementia. See also Alzheimer’s disease
FRE and, 88 competency with, 401–32
IDRA and, 88 SPECT and, 166
neuroimaging in, 170–72 TBI and, 270–71
self-report in, 474 dementia pugilistica, 243
TPO and, 35 denial of evaluation, 108
criminal responsibility, 475–86 Department of Veteran Affairs (VA), 366
death penalty and, 486–90 deposition, 52–56
diminished capacity and, 481–82 depression. See also major depressive disorder;
criterion-groups, 119, 314 manic-depressive disorder
cross examination, 52–53 MTBI and, 240, 248, 249f, 250
crowding hypothesis, 190 neurotoxic injury and, 296
Crown, Barry, 81 PCS and, 246
CRPS. See complex regional pain syndrome PTSD and, 388
cruel and unusual punishment, 84 SPECT and, 166
Cryptococcus spp., 347 symptoms of, 337
CSA. See compressed spectral array TBI and, 265
522 index
detail, 48 Durham v. United States, 476
Detailed Assessment of Posttraumatic Stress (DAPS), 377 Dusky v. United States, 83, 446–47, 449–51, 487
Detroit Edison Co. v. National Labor Relations Board, 31 Dyer v.Trachtman, 37
wrongful disclosure and, 92
DFA. See discriminant function analysis Ecstasy. See MDMA
diabetes ECST-R. See Evaluation of Competency to Stand
maternal, 183 Trial-Revised
neurotoxic injury and, 296 edema, 212
diagnosis threat, 43 educating the jury, 53
PCS and, 247–48 Edwards v. Indiana, 464
diagnostic error management, 44 EEG. See electroencephalography
Diagnostic & Statistical Manual (DSM), 367–69, 368t Eighth Amendment, 84
dicta, 60n16, 93n2 electroencephalography (EEG), 85, 164–65
dictionary for laymen, 47, 47t IPS and, 190
differential prevalence design, 119 for neurotoxic injury, 298
diffuse edema, 212 for pseudoneurological illness, 342–43
diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), 252, 509 electromyography (EMG), 333
Digit Memory Test (DMT), 119, 122–24 elements of expert witness control, 53
Digit Span, 266. See also Reliable Digit Span EMDR. See eye movement desensitization and
malingering and, 124, 128–29, 144 reprocessing
MTBI and, 239 EMG. See electromyography
WAIS-IV, 7 encephalopathy. See also hypoxic ischemic
Digit Symbol encephalopathy
TBI and, 273 solvents and, 281, 282t
WAIS-IV, 7 epilepsy, 189
diminished capacity, 84–85, 88 episodic memory, 168
criminal responsibility and, 481–82 ergo propter hoc, 48
CST and, 442–43 ergotism, 347
diminished responsibility, 84–85, 482 Estelle v. Smith, 491, 492
direct examination, in deposition, 52–53 Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct
Disability Rating Scale (DRS), 267–68 (APA), 30–32, 503
disc bulge and herniation, 330 ethics, 26, 30, 101–13, 511–12
discectomy, 334 accurate and full reporting and, 112
discipline, for testimony, 56 APA and, 103, 104, 111
discography, 333 bias and, 107
disconfirmatory information, 6 collusion and, 107
discovery, 30 compensation for services and, 107
in trial phase, 49 competency requirements of neuropsychologists and,
discovery deposition, 52 106–7
discriminant function analysis (DFA), 412 in criminal law, 490–93
disinhibition, 265 death penalty and, 486
Dissimulation Scale-Revised, 134 informed consent and, 109–12
DMT. See Digit Memory Test interpretation and, 112–13
doctor-patient relationship, 37 interviews and, 109
dose-response relationship, 25 obstacles to, 106–13
inverse, 44–45 patient examination and, 107–8
with malingering, 144–45 protecting the public and, 113
with MTBI, 235 pull of affiliation and, 38
with neurotic injury, 289 raw data and, 103–6, 105f, 108–9
Dot-Counting Test, 121 records and, 107–8
double hazard, 216 in trial phase, 56
Drope v. Missouri, 445 ethylene glycol, 286t
DRS. See Disability Rating Scale Evaluation of Competency to Stand Trial-Revised
DRS Memory, 412–14 (ECST-R), 461–62
DSM. See Diagnostic & Statistical Manual event-related potentials, 170
DTI. See diffusion tensor imaging exaggeration. See symptom exaggeration
Dubray. See United States v. Dubray exceptionalism, 60n6
due process, 82 EXIT 25, 412–14
duration of unconsciousness, 261 expectations, 338
index 523

experiment, 9 malingering and, 124, 125, 127t


expert testimony, 36–38 neurotoxic injury and, 294
admissibility and, 70–94 Finger Tip Number Writing, 124
advocacy and, 60n12 FIQ. See Full Scale IQ
causation and, 80–81 Fitness Interview Test (FIT), 457–59
confusion from, 89–90 five noble traditions, of clinical psychology, 4
in criminal law, 87–90 fixed battery, 24
CST and, 82–83 admissibility of, 51
Daubert and, 90–91 Baxter v. Temple and, 70
discrediting of, 80 for consulting, 75–77
elements of expert witness control, 53 ethics of, 39
MMPI-2 FBS and, 79–80 Fla. DOT v. Piccolo, 31
qualification of, 72–75 FLD. See frontal lobe defense
relevance of, 89 flexible battery, 24
reliability of, 79, 88–89 admissibility of, 51
scope of, 74 Baxter v. Temple and, 70
unqualification for, 81–82 for consulting, 75–77
wrongful disclosure and, 91–93 18-F-fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG), 163
extrapyramidal syndrome, 185, 194 fluoroscopy, 333
eye movement desensitization and reprocessing FM. See fibromyalgia
(EMDR), 389 fMRI. See functional magnetic resonance imaging
FMS. See Wisconsin Failure-to-Maintain Set
FA. See fractional anisotropy fNIRS. See functional near-infrared spectroscopy
facet joint syndrome, 330 football. See sports
factitious disorder, 116, 312–13 forced-choice testing, 122–23
fact witness, 36–38 Ford v. Wainwright, 486–87
Fake Bad Scale (FBS). See Symptom Validity Scale forensically relevant instruments, 43
false alternatives, 54–55 forensically specific instruments, 43
false conceit gambit, 56 Forensic Psychia!, 73f
false negatives, 10, 44 Forensic Psycholo!, 73f
false positive, 44 forensic reports, 45–49, 45t
for malingering, 141, 143 Fourteenth Amendment, 82, 439
family-wise error, 28 fractional anisotropy (FA), 252
Faretta v. California, 452–54 FRE. See Federal Rules of Evidence
fashionable illnesses, 339 free radicals, 184
fatigue. See chronic fatigue syndrome Frendak v. United States, 455–56
Faust, David, 75–76 frontal lobe defense (FLD), 85, 484
FBS. See Symptom Validity Scale neuroimaging and, 170
FC. See financial capacity PET and, 170
FCI. See Financial Capacity Instrument Frye v. United States, 3, 50–52, 60n16, 504
FDG. See 18-F-fluorodeoxyglucose admissibility and, 72–73
fear-avoidance, 307–8, 308f Daubert and, 90
Federal Rules of Evidence (FRE), 27, fMRI and, 86
on admissibility, 50 general acceptance and, 74
criminal law and, 88 SVT and, 70
on expert testimony, 72 FT. See Finger Tapping
on general acceptance, 74 Full Scale IQ (FIQ), 16
fibromyalgia (FM), 141, 329–30, 343–44 neurotoxic injury and, 293
malingering and, 147 functional capacity exams (FCEs), 333–34
15-Item Test. See Rey 15-Item Test functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), 85–86,
Fifth Amendment, 83, 439, 491–92 163–64, 509
financial capacity (FC), 415–21, 418t for deception, 171–72
with AD, 417–20 for TBI, 167–69
with MCI, 420–21 functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), 509
Financial Capacity Instrument (FCI), 417–21, 419t, 420t, fungi. See toxic mold
422t, 423t
Finger Agnosia, 124 GA. See gestational age
Finger Tapping (FT), 6, 7, 15, 125t, 135 Gage, Phineas, 484
CHI and, 126 Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test (GOAT), 262, 264
524 index
gambits, 54–56 MTBI and, 234
Gansler, David, 88, 89 neurotoxic injury and, 293
GBMI. See guilty but mentally ill and pseudoneurological illness, 341–42
GCCT. See Georgia Court Competency Test Hand Dynamometer, 124
GCS. See Glasgow Coma Scale HCR-20, 485
GDS. See Global Deterioration Scale headache, 332
general acceptance, 51 migraine, 331
Frye and, 74 head injury. See also closed head injury; traumatic brain
general competency, 403–4 injury
general contours, 40–45 PTSD and, 373–75
General Electric v. Joiner, 25, 56, 74 Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act
expert testimony and, 88–91 (HIPAA), 104, 105–6
general intent, 88 hearsay rule, 79
generalized anxiety disorders, 166 heavy metals, 282
General Memory Index (GM), 6 symptoms from, 284t–285t
Georgia Court Competency Test (GCCT), 459 Henry-Heilbronner Index (HHI), 138, 139–40
germinal matrix, 182 N-hexane, 286t, 291
gestational age (GA), 179 HIE. See hypoxic ischemic encephalopathy
Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS), 7 Hinckley, John, 82, 477
for MTBI, 231, 233, 241–42, 249 hindsight bias, 5
for PCS, 247 HIPAA. See Health Insurance Portability and
for pediatric TBI, 212 Accountability Act
and pseudoneurological illness, 342 hippocampus
and PTSD, 366 pediatric TBI and, 212
QEEG and, 169 PTSD and, 371
for TBI, 261, 262 Histoplasma spp., 347
Glasgow Outcome Scale (GOS), 268–69 HIV, 236, 243
Global Deterioration Scale (GDS), 429 holdings, 93n2
glutamate, 180 Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, 237
HIE and, 184 Houston Conference, 103
GM. See General Memory Index; gray matter HPA. See hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis
GOAT. See Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test HRB. See Halstead-Reitan Battery
Godinez v. Moran, 452–54, 487 Huntington’s disease, 13
good recovery, 269 H-words, 14–15
GOS. See Glasgow Outcome Scale hydrops fetalis, 183
G-PIPE. See Group Protecting the Integrity of hyperbilirubinemia, 180
Psychological Examinations hypertension
gray matter (GM), 179 maternal, 191
HIE and, 184 neurotoxic injury and, 296
neurotoxic injury and, 290 hypotension, 183
VLBW/VPTB and, 180 hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA), 305
Grenitz v. Tomlian, 56, 70 hypothermia, 212
on expert testimony, 81 hypotheses, 3–4
Griggs et al. v. Duke Power, 28 hypothyroxinemia, 183
Grip Strength, 15 hypoxic ischemic encephalopathy (HIE), 179
PCS and, 126 delayed onset syndrome and, 194
Grisso, Thomas, 444–45 MRI for, 185, 185f, 187–88
Grooved Pegboard, 6, 7, 15 pediatric TBI and, 212
neurotoxic injury and, 294 stroke and, 188
Group for the Advancement of Psychiatry, 450, 450t in term infants, 183–88
Group Protecting the Integrity of Psychological
Examinations (G-PIPE), 92 IADL. See instrumental activity of daily living
guilty but mentally ill (GBMI), 479 IAMP. See 123-I-para iodoamphetamine
Gulf War, 340–41 IASP. See International Association for the Study of Pain
iatrogenic PTSD, 388–89
hallucinogenics, SPECT and, 166 idiopathic environmental intolerance (IEI), 297
Halstead, Ward, 12–13 IDRA. See Insanity Defense Reform Act of 1984
Halstead-Reitan Battery (HRB), 8, 13–14 IEDs. See improvised explosive devices
malingering and, 119, 124 IEI. See idiopathic environmental intolerance
index 525

IEPC. See Individualized Educational Planning interpreters, informed consent for, 110–12
Committee interviews, 6–7
illusory correlations, 5 ethics and, 109
illusory mental health, 350 for perinatal brain injury, 192
IMEs. See independent medical examinations for PTSD, 383–88
Immigration and Nationality Act, 87 intracranial hemorrhage, 190–91
Impairment Index, 234 intrapartum brain damage, 56
improbability, 118, 146, 314 intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH), 180
improvised explosive devices (IEDs), 244 cerebral sinovenous thrombosis and, 191
impulsivity intracranial hemorrhage and, 191
dangerousness and, 484–85 in utero, 56
TBI and, 265 inverse dose-response effect, 44–45
inadmissible character evidence, 78–79 in vitro studies, 73–74
incompetency. See competency IOM. See Institute of Medicine
independent medical examinations (IMEs), 32–35 123-I-para iodoamphetamine (IAMP), 162
doctor-patient relationship and, 37 IPS. See ischemic perinatal stroke
informed consent for, 110 IQ, 16
for PTSD, 366, 390–91, 390t Apgar and, 187
Index Scores, of WAIS-IV, 16–17 FIQ, 16, 293
Indiana v. Edwards, 453–54 HIE and, 185–86
Individualized Educational Planning Committee IPS and, 189–90
(IEPC), 215 Performance IQ, 7, 57
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, 215–16 Verbal IQ, 7, 57
Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement VLBW/VPTB and, 182
Act of 2004, 215 Iraq, 244
informed consent ischemic perinatal stroke (IPS), 188–90
with AD, 407–9 ISTSS. See International Society for Traumatic Stress
capacity for, 405–15 Studies
ethics and, 109–12 IVH. See intraventricular hemorrhage
for interpreters, 110–12
for videotaping, 111–12 Jackson v. Indiana, 467
initial meeting, 35–36 Jenkins v. United States, 50
injections, for chronic pain, 332–33, 334–35 expert witness testimony and, 71
injury and (CVMT). See Continuous Visual Memory Test Joiner. See General Electric v. Joiner
in limine, 52
in Baxter v. Temple, 75 known-groups design, 119
in Santos-Buenos and, 87 for malingering, 314
SVT and, 78 Kumbo Tire Co. v. Carmichael, 56, 74
INS. See International Neuropsychological Society
insanity defense, 446 Larry P. v. Riles, 28
competency to refuse, 455–56 law. See also attorney interactions; civil law;
criminal responsibility and, 475–86 criminal law
expert testimony and, 82–83 administrative, 38t
TBI and, 479–81 conflicts in, 26–29
Insanity Defense Reform Act of 1984 (IDRA), 70, 82, FN and, 23–61
84–87, 477 probate, 38t
criminal law and, 88 lead poisoning, 76, 283, 284t, 292
Institute of Medicine (IOM), 283 learned treatise, 55
instrumental activity of daily living (IADL), 415 learning disabilities, 181
insula, 290 MTBI and, 250
integrity check, 57 pediatric TBI and, 217
intermediate evidentiary standard, 94n11 legal bases, 25
intermittent competency, 404 legal primacy, 60n6
International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP), 302 legal settings, 38t
International Neuropsychological Society (INS), 101 Letter Memory Test, 123–24
International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies LexisNexis Academic, 502, 502f
(ISTSS), 378–79 lie detection, 86, 86f
interpretation, 30 likelihood ratio (LR), 10
ethics and, 112–13 for malingering, 143–44, 145
526 index
Limbaugh-Kirker v. Decosta, 79, 80 MMPI-2 for, 146
limited competency, 404 MND and, 146
List Learning and Memory, 239 MPRD and, 146
litigation consulting, 36–38 MTBI and, 251
LNNB. See Luria Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery patterns of, 117
Logical Memory, 125 PCS and, 247–48
logistic regression, 10–11 perinatal brain injury and, 193
loss of consciousness problem-solving ability measures and, 130–31
with MTBI, 232, 233, 240 PTSD and, 376–77
with PCS, 244 RDS and, 128–29, 144
with PTSD, 374–75 recognition memory measurements and, 129–30
LR. See likelihood ratio research designs for, 118–20
Lumsden v. United States, 31 ROC for, 11f
Luria Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery (LNNB), simulation studies for, 144
13–14 subtypes of, 147–48
lying, 350–52 SVT for, 142–44, 149
lie detection, 86, 86f symptom exaggeration with, 131–41
testimony about, 146–47
MacArthur Competency Assessment Tool-Criminal Malingering Index, 132
Adjudication (MacCAT-CA), 460–61 manganese, 282, 285t
MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study, 485 manic-depressive disorder
magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), 333, 509 MTBI and, 241, 249f
for chronic pain, 303 SPECT and, 166
for HIE, 185, 185f, 187–88 marijuana, 248, 249f, 283, 296
for MTBI, 241–42 neurotoxic symptoms from, 288t
for neurotoxic injury, 298 marketing, 60n2
for pediatric TBI, 212 maternal diabetes, 183
for PVL, 180 maternal hypertension, 191
for toxic exposure, 169 maternal hypotension, 183
for VLBW/VPTB, 182 MBK. See methyl-N-butyl ketone
WM and, 180 McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ), 131–32
magnetoencephalography (MEG), 165 chronic pain and, 147
major depressive disorder (MDD), 13 MCI. See mild cognitive impairment
MTBI and, 249f, 250 MCS. See multiple chemical sensitivity
PCS and, 246 MCs. See medical controls
malingered neurocognitive dysfunction (MND), 117–18, MDD. See major depressive disorder
313–14 MDMA, 283, 296
malingering and, 146 neurotoxic symptoms from, 288t
MTBI and, 248, 249f MDSM. See multiple data source model
response bias and, 120–24 medical controls (MCs), 5
TBI and, 141 medical history
malingered pain-related disability (MPRD), 118, 146, accuracy of, 349
307, 314 distortion of, 352
malingering, 6, 507 medically unexplained symptoms, 336–55
assessment of, 116–50 assessment of, 352–53
base rate of, 141, 145 with CFS, 344–45
CHI and, 149 characteristics of, 339–41
chronic pain and, 147, 306–7, 313–15 with FM, 343–44
criteria for, 117 lying and, 350–52
definition of, 116–18 with MCS, 345–46
diagnosis of, 141–46 medical history accuracy and, 349
Digit Span for, 128–29, 144 medical history distortion and, 352
dose-response relationship with, 144–45 pseudoneurological illness, 341–43
false positive for, 141, 143 psychological stress and, 348–49
FBS and, 137t self-appraisal and, 349–50
FM and, 147 symptom exaggeration and, 350–52
FT for, 127t with toxic mold, 346–48
incompetency and, 148 Medical Symptom Validity Test (MSVT), 139
LR for, 143–44, 145 Medina v. California, 454–55
index 527

MEG. See magnetoencephalography moderate TBI, 260–73


memory. See also specific memory types and tests incidence of, 262–63
LNNB for, 14 neuropsychological assessment of, 265–68
MTBI and, 232, 235, 250 outcomes after, 268–71
neurotoxins and, 283 prediction of outcome for, 271–73
pediatric TBI and, 213 Modified Somatic Perception Questionnaire
TBI and, 264 (MSPQ), 147
Memory and Location, 124 modus tollens, 4
meniscal tears, 330–31 mold, 283
mens rea, 84, 87, 94n14, 475 monoparesis, 188
MENT. See Morel Emotional Numbing Test Monte Carlo method, 17
mental retardation. See cognitive deficits mood disorders
mental state at offense (MSO), 474–75 chronic pain and, 307
mental status examination (MSE), 41 MTBI and, 250
mercury, 169, 283, 285t neurotoxic injury and, 296
mere conclusory comment, 48 Morel Emotional Numbing Test (MENT), 377
methamphetamine, 283 motivation impairments, 126–27
method conflicts, 28 motor function tests, 126–27
methyl alcohol, 286t motor vehicle accidents (MVA), 372–73
methyl-N-butyl ketone (MBK), 282, 286t MPQ. See McGill Pain Questionnaire
Meyers Neuropsychological Battery, 14 MPRD. See malingered pain-related disability
migraine, 331 MRI. See magnetic resonance imaging
mild cognitive impairment (MCI), 402 MSE. See mental status examination
capacity with, 409–10 MSO. See mental state at offense
FC with, 420–21 MSPQ. See Modified Somatic Perception Questionnaire
mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI), 231–53 MSVT. See Medical Symptom Validity Test
complaint base rates for, 4–5, 246t MTBI. See mild traumatic brain injury
diagnostic criteria for, 231–34 muddying the record, 49
differential diagnosis for, 248–51 multi-method convergent means of data gathering, 40
neuropsychological effect sizes for, 249f multiple chemical sensitivity (MCS), 298, 345–46
outcomes with, 236–41 multiple data source model (MDSM), 473–75, 480–81
PCS and, 244–48 multiple measures, 15
persistent problems following, 241–44 multiple sclerosis, 13
recovery curves for, 238f chronic pain with, 302
research designs for, 234–36 MTBI and, 241
severity of, 235t Munchausen by proxy, 85
SVT and, 145 muscle ligament injuries, 331
Miller-Forensic Assessment of Symptoms, 378 MVA. See motor vehicle accidents
Mini Token Test, 266 mycotoxins, 283
Minner v. American Mortg., 24 myelography, 333
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 myofascial pain syndrome, 331
(MMPI-2), 11, 11f, 13, 49
for chronic pain, 305–6, 315 NAART, 282
expert testimony and, 80 NAB. See Neuropsychological Assessment Battery
FBS and, 133–38 National Academy of Neuropsychology (NAN), 101,
for malingering, 117, 124, 125, 146 102, 503
for MTBI, 250 on raw data, 109
for PCS, 126, 247 National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, 232
for pseudoneurological illness, 342–43 National Computer systems (NCS), 136
for PTSD, 136–37, 381–83 National Institute of Neurological and Communicative
symptom exaggeration and, 132–33 Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer’s Disease and
TPO and, 34 Released Disorders Association (NINCDS-
Miranda v. Arizona, 83, 451–52, 474 ADRDA), 404
miserable minority, 233 National Standardized Test, 292
MMPI-2. See Minnesota Multiphasic Personality nature, 477–78
Inventory-2 N-Back, 167–68
M’Naghten, Daniel, 475–77 NBW. See normal birth weight
MND. See malingered neurocognitive dysfunction NCS. See National Computer systems; nerve conduction
Model Penal Code, 476 study
528 index
necrotizing enterocolitis, 180, 183 nonsteroidal anti-inflammatories (NSAIDs), 334
negative predictive power (NPP), 10 nonverbal behaviors, 54
formula for, 11 normal birth weight (NBW), 181
PPP, 11–12 normative basis for tests, 43
sensitivity and, 142 North Carolina v. Alford, 452, 487
specificity and, 142 novel problem solving, 213
Nelson v. Nelson, 71 NPP. See negative predictive power
nerve conduction study (NCS), 333 NSAIDs. See nonsteroidal anti-inflammatories
nerve decompression, 334 null hypothesis, 4
NES. See Neurobehavioral Evaluation System
Neurobehavioral Evaluation System (NES), 292 OA. See osteoarthritis
Neurobehavioral Functioning Inventory, 266 objectivity, 48–49, 57
Neurobehavioral Rating Scale, 266 vs. bias, 512
neuroimaging, 85–86, 160–72, 509–11. See also specific in deposition, 53–54
neuroimaging modalities vs. zealous advocacy, 26–27
activated, 161 obsessive-compulsive disorder, 166
in criminal law, 170–72 ODA. See Optimal Data Analysis
CST and, 171 odds ratio (OR), 10
for deception, 171–72 Oklahoma Premorbid Intelligence Estimate (OPIE), 16
electrophysiological techniques for, 164–65 oligohydramnios, 189
evidence classes and, 161 opiates
FLD and, 170 neurotoxic symptoms from, 288t
for neurotoxic injury, 290 SPECT and, 166
radioligand-based, 161–64 OPIE. See Oklahoma Premorbid Intelligence Estimate
resting, 161 Optimal Data Analysis (ODA), 139
for toxic exposure, 169–70 OR. See odds ratio
neurolytic blockade, 334 organization, 48
Neuropsychia!, 73f organophosphates, 283, 288t
Neuropsycholo!, 73f, 438 Orientation Log, 266
Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB), 17 orthopedic injuries, MTBI and, 234, 252–53
neuropsychological diagnosis. See also specific conditions osteoarthritis (OA), 329
classification statistics in, 9–12 OTBM. See overall test battery mean
neuropsychological evaluation. See also specific conditions overall test battery mean (OTBM), 7, 14
bias in, 4–5 oxyhemoglobin, 163
errors in, 4–5
with science, 4–9 Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT), 7, 250
SVT in, 77–80 PAI. See Personality Assessment Inventory
videotaping of, 33t pain. See also chronic pain
neuropsychological testing, 12–18 CRPS, 330
neurotoxic injury, 281–98 IASP, 302
base rate for, 294–98, 295t–296t MPQ, 131–32, 147
cognitive deficits from, 289f MPRD, 118, 146, 307, 314
exposure assessment for, 290–98, 297f myofascial pain syndrome, 331
interpretation of findings for, 294–98 painter’s syndrome, 281–82
medical history and, 291 paint thinners, 292
occupational and environmental history and, 291–92 parens patriae, 439
social, academic, and psychiatric history and, 292 Parkinson’s disease (PD), 13, 401
sufficient exposure for, 291 consent capacity with, 410–15
toxin categories, 283, 288 chronic pain with, 302
nexus, 48 PD-CIND, 414
NINCDS-ADRDA. See National Institute of Neurological Parkinson’s disease dementia syndrome (PDDS), 411–15
and Communicative Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer’s consent capacity with, 411t, 412t, 413t
Disease and Released Disorders Association paroxetine (Paxil), 389
nitric oxide, 184 PASAT. See Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test
non-CNS litigants, 5 passive answering, 54
nondisclosure privilege, 31–32 patent ductus arteriosis, 180, 183
nonepileptic seizures, 342–43 Pate v. Robinson, 445
nonevaluation, 108 Paxil. See paroxetine
non obstante veredicto, 28 PCBs. See polychlorinated biphenyls
index 529

PCD. See persistent post-concussional disorder polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs), 74


PCL-R. See Psychopathy Checklist-Revised portal to portal billing, 39
PCS. See post-concussion syndrome Portland Digit Recognition Test (PDRT), 44
PCSC. See Post-Concussion Symptom Checklist for malingering, 117, 124, 129
PD. See Parkinson’s disease for MTBI, 236
PD and cognitive impairment without dementia for pseudoneurological illness, 342
(PD-CIND), 414 positive predictive power (PPP), 10
PDDS. See Parkinson’s disease dementia syndrome of FBS, 137
PDRT. See Portland Digit Recognition Test formula for, 11
PDS. See Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale NPP, 11–12
pediatric TBI, 211–22 sensitivity and, 142
child abuse and, 218–19 specificity and, 142
complicating premorbid histories with, 217–18 positron emission tomography (PET), 163, 509
epidemiology of, 211 FLD and, 170
family functioning and, 217 for TBI, 166–67
moderating and mediating variables with, 215–17 for toxic exposure, 169–70
neurobehavioral sequelae from, 213–14 Post-Concussion Symptom Checklist (PCSC), 239, 245
neuropsychological evaluation of, 219–21 post-concussion syndrome (PCS), 126
pathophysiology of, 212 MTBI and, 237, 240, 244–48
severity of, 211–12 QEEG for, 169
special education for, 214–15 SPECT for, 166
SVT for, 221–22 symptoms of, 244, 246t, 337
Penry v. Lynaugh, 487–88 post hoc, 48
People v. Sebastianelli, 24 post-traumatic amnesia (PTA), 232, 233
per diem, 39 MTBI and, 239, 241, 249
Performance IQ, 7 PTSD and, 375
perinatal brain injury, 179–200 TBI and, 261–64
central sinovenous thrombosis, 191 post-traumatic confusional state (PTCS), 262, 264
forensic assessments for, 192–99 post-traumatic hydrocephalus, 265
HIE, 183–88 Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS), 387–88
intracranial hemorrhage, 190–91 post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), 51, 365–93
IPS, 188–90 cognitive deficits with, 379–81
recommendations for, 196t–197t definition of, 367
socioeconomic status and, 194 delayed, 375–76
testimony for, 195–97 DSM and, 367–69, 368t
VLBW, 179–83 epidemiological studies on, 369–70
VPTB, 179–83 forensic evaluation for, 378–88
periventricular hemorrhagic infarction (PHI), 180 head injury and, 373–75
VLBW/VPTB and, 182 iatrogenic, 388–89
periventricular leukomalacia (PVL), 180 interviews for, 383–87
persistent post-concussional disorder (PCD), 246 malingering and, 376–77
personal attacks, 60n3 MMPI-2 for, 136–37
Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI), 132, 379 MTBI and, 240, 244, 250
personality disorders, 306–7 MVA and, 372–73
persuasion, 49 neurobiology of, 370–72
pesticides, 283, 292 neurotoxic injury and, 296–97
PET. See positron emission tomography psychological stress and, 348–49
PHI. See periventricular hemorrhagic infarction self-report of, 387–88
physical examinations, 333–34 symptom exaggeration and, 133
Picture Completion, 16 symptoms of, 338
placenta post-trial phase, 56–57
cerebral sinovenous thrombosis and, 191 post-Vietnam syndrome, 369
HIE and, 183–84 PPP. See positive predictive power
intracranial hemorrhage and, 190–91 practice pattern surveys, 24
VLBW/VPTB and, 183 practicing competent neuropsychology, 25
plausibility, 8–9 pre-assessment phase
pneumothorax, 180 compensation for services and, 39–40
PNS. See Pseudoneurologic Scale initial meeting in, 35–36
police power, 439 role selection in, 36–38
530 index
predictive value statistics, 10 RDS. See Reliable Digit Span
preeclampsia, 189 Reagan, Ronald, 82
cerebral sinovenous thrombosis and, 191 reasonable level of understanding, 446
preexisting conditions, 7 receiver operating characteristic (ROC), 11, 78
pediatric TBI and, 220 for malingering, 11f, 124
premorbid psychopathy, 42t for TBI, 134
preponderance of evidence standard, 28, 48 recognition memory measurements, 129–30
pretrial diversion, 480 recognition trials, 121
prevailing school of thought, 73 Recognition Word List, 132
prevalence/base rate. See base rate records
priorities, 49 ethics and, 107–8
probability mapping, 165 for MTBI, 232
probable assistance, 29 SGFFP and, 37
probable MPRD, 118 testimony from, 37
probate law, 38t regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF), 161
problem-solving ability measures, 130–31 fMRI and, 163
pro bono, 107 PET and, 167
pro se, 452 regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose
protecting the public, 113 (rCMRglc), 163
proximate cause, 46 PET and,163, 167
pseudoneurological illness, 341–43 regional cerebral metabolic rate for oxygen
pseudoneurological symptoms, 337–38 (rCMRO2), 163
Pseudoneurologic Scale (PNS), 139–40 Rehnquist, William, 74
pseudoscience, 3 Reitan, Ralph, 13
psychological stress, 348–49 relevance
Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R), 485 of evidence, 78
psychotic disorders, 265 of expert testimony, 89
PTA. See post-traumatic amnesia reliability, of expert testimony, 79, 88–89
PTCS. See post-traumatic confusional state Reliable Digit Span (RDS), 124, 125t, 126, 135
PTSD. See post-traumatic stress disorder malingering and, 125, 128–29, 144
PTSD Checklist, 387 symptom exaggeration and, 132
pull of affiliation, 35, 38 remittitur, 60n4
Purdue Pegboard, 294 reports
PVL. See periventricular leukomalacia in assessment phase, 45–49
clinical, 45–49, 45t
QEEG. See quantitative electroencephalography forensic, 45–49, 45t
Q-PTSD. See Quick Test for Posttraumatic Stress self-report, 387–88, 473–74
Disorder response bias, 77
quadriparesis, 188 forced-choice testing and, 122–24
quality, 477–78 MND and, 120, 122–24
quantitative electroencephalography (QEEG), 164–65, Response Bias Scale (RBS), 138–40, 147
169, 237 responsibility. See also criminal responsibility
Quick Test for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder diminished, 84–85, 482
(Q-PTSD), 377 responsivity to social and political forces, 28
resting imaging, 161
RA. See rheumatoid arthritis Restructured Clinical Scales, 138
radiculopathy, 331 retinopathy, 180, 183
radiofrequency (RF), 163 pediatric TBI and, 219
radioligand-based imaging, 161–64 retrospective competency determinations, 83
Ragge v. MCA/Universal Studios, 34 Rey 15-Item Test, 120–21, 135
raw data, 29–32 symptom exaggeration and, 132
ethics and, 103–6, 105f, 108–9 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT), 14, 124
wrongful disclosure and, 92 malingering and, 129
RBS. See Response Bias Scale neurotoxic injury and, 293
rCBF. See regional cerebral blood flow PCS and, 247
rCMRglc. See regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose Rey Complex Figure Test, 130
rCMRO2. See regional cerebral metabolic rate for oxygen MTBI and, 235, 250
R-CRAS. See Rogers Criminal Responsibility Assessment neurotoxic injury and, 294
Scales PCS and, 247
index 531

Rey’s Dot-Counting Test, 121 sepsis, 180, 183


Rey Word List, 135 Serial Digit Learning test, 294
RF. See radiofrequency serotonin, 484
rheumatoid arthritis (RA), 329 sertraline (Zoloft), 389
Riggins v. Nevada, 467 serum biomarkers, 212
right/wrong test, 476 severe TBI, 260–73
Rinchack. See United States v. Rinchack incidence of, 262–63
Rivermead Post-concussion Symptoms Questionnaire, 245 neuropsychological assessment of, 265–68
ROC. See receiver operating characteristic outcomes after, 268–71
Rogers Criminal Responsibility Assessment Scales prediction of outcome for, 271–73
(R-CRAS), 481 SGA. See small for GA
roles, 440–41, 440t, 442f SGFFP. See Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychology
conflicts with, 28–29 Shaheen, Sandra, 75
in pre-assessment phase, 36–38 shaken-baby syndrome, 218
rotator cuff tears, 331 Sharrer & Sharrer v. Sunscape Landscape Nursery, Inc.
Rush v. Megee, 71 et al., 79
shunt-dependent hydrocephalus, 182
Santos-Bueno. See United States v. Jose Santos-Bueno sick building syndrome, 346–48
Saylor, F. Dennis, 88 Sieling v. Eyman, 452–53
schizophrenia, 13 Sierra v. Reyes, 80
b Test and, 122 Simmons v. Mullen, 71–72, 75
MTBI and, 250 Simple Visual Reaction Time, 293
SPECT and, 166 simulation studies, for malingering, 119, 144
Schudel v. General Electric, 50 single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT),
sciatica, 331 161–62, 509
science, 3–4 aggression and, 170
neuropsychological evaluation with, 4–9 cerebellum and, 162
neuropsychological testing and, 12–18 for CFS, 344
scope for TBI, 165–66
of expert testimony, 74 for toxic exposure, 169–70
of testimony, 50–51 SIRS. See Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms
Seashore Rhythm Test, 124 Sixth Amendment, 439
Sebastianelli. See People v. Sebastianelli skull fracture, 197, 219, 233
SEE. See standard errors of estimate TBI and, 261
Segna. See United States v. Segna small for GA (SGA), 179
seizures smoking, 290
cerebral sinovenous thrombosis and, 191 social judgment, 213
HIE and, 185 socioeconomic status
IPS and, 190 pediatric TBI and, 216
nonepileptic, 342–43 perinatal brain injury and, 194
pediatric TBI and, 219 TBI and, 273
pseudoneurological illness and, 342 VLBW/VPTB and, 183
self-appraisal, 57 soft psychology, 4
medically unexplained symptoms and, 349–50 solvents
MTBI and, 243 encephalopathy and, 281, 282t
perinatal brain injury and, 193 symptoms with, 286t–287t
self-awareness, 265 somatization disorder
self-incrimination, 439 chronic pain and, 305–6
self-regulation, 216 PCS and, 247
self-report SPECT and, 166
in criminal law, 473–74 somatoform disorders, 141
of PTSD, 387–88 chronic pain and, 312
Sell v. United States, 467–68 PCS and, 247
sensitivity, 5, 11–12 Spatial Span, 104
MCS, 298 special education, 214–15
NPP and, 142 Specialty Guidelines for Forensic Psychology (SGFFP), 32,
PPP and, 142 490–91
of Rey 15-Item Test, 120 on compensation for services, 40
SVT and, 12 records and, 37
532 index
specific competency, 403–4 Symptom Improbability Rating Scale, 135
specific intent, 88, 94n13 symptom validity, 77–80
specificity, 5, 8, 11–12 Symptom Validity Scale (FBS), 11, 11f, 49, 70, 125t
NPP and, 142 for chronic pain, 136
PPP and, 142 expert testimony and, 80
of Rey 15-Item Test, 120 for malingering, 137t
SPECT. See single photon emission computed MMPI-2 and, 138
tomography PPP of, 137
Speech Sounds Perception Errors, 124 symptom exaggeration and, 133–38
spine for TBI, 133–35, 138
chronic pain with, 303 WMT and, 138–39
disc bulge and herniation in, 330 symptom validity test (SVT), 10–11, 44
spinal fusion, 334 for chronic pain, 315
spinal stenosis, 331 for competency, 466
spondylolisthesis, 331–32 Daubert and, 51
spondylolysis, 331–32 forced-choice testing for, 122
spondylosis, 332 Frye and, 70
sports hearsay rule and, 79
MTBI and, 233–34, 236–37, 243, 251 in limine and, 78
pediatric TBI and, 214 for malingering, 142–44, 149
Spot-the-Word-Test, 381 for MTBI, 145
SQUIDs. See superconducting quantum-interference in neuropsychological evaluations, 77–80
devices for pediatric TBI, 221–22
SRS. See Supervision Rating Scale for PTSD, 366
standard errors of estimate (SEE), 16 sensitivity and, 12
standard of proof, 28, 454–55 specificity and, 12
star witness mentality, 57 for TBI, 145
steroids, 183 systolic blood pressure deception test, 72–73
for chronic pain, 334–35
strength of association, 8 Tactual Performance Test Total Time, 124
stress, 348–49. See also post-traumatic stress disorder Tarver v. State, 25
stroke Task Force for Test User Qualifications (TTFUQ), 102
b Test and, 122 taxon based malingering, 148
HIE and, 188 TBI. See traumatic brain injury
IPS, 188–90 TBI Model Systems protocol, 267
Stroop Test, 293 TC. See testamentary capacity
Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms (SIRS), TCE. See trichloroethylene
132, 378 TCI. See Testamentary Capacity Instrument
sua sponte, 444 Technetium, 162
subarachnoid hemorrhage, 190–91 temporality, 8
subdural hemorrhage, 190–91 temporomandibular disorder (TMD), 332
pediatric TBI and, 219 TENS. See transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation
subpoenas, 60n9 tension-type headache, 332
substance abuse. See specific substances tests, 12–18. See also specific tests
sufficient present ability standard, 83 in common use, 43–44
Sullivan. See United States v. Sullivan cutting score, 12
superconducting quantum-interference devices data, 104
(SQUIDs), 165 materials for, 104
Supervision Rating Scale (SRS), 268 security, 31
supporting board certification, 25 validity of, 44
SVT. See symptom validity test wrongful disclosure of, 91–93
Symbol Digit Modalities Test, 243 testamentary capacity (TC), 421–31
MTBI and, 250 undue influence and, 430–31
for PTSD, 381 Testamentary Capacity Instrument (TCI), 426–27
Symbol Digit Paired Associate Learning Task, 293 testimony. See also expert testimony
symptom exaggeration bias in, 57
with malingering, 131–41 discipline for, 56
medically unexplained symptoms and, 350–52 about malingering, 146–47
MTBI and, 248 for perinatal brain injury, 195–97
index 533

scope of, 50–51 MND and, 141


in trial phase, 52–56 neuropsychological assessment of, 267t, 268t
Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM), 117, 123 PET for, 166–67
FBS and, 138 prediction of outcome from, 272t
PCS and, 245 QEEG for, 169
pediatric TBI and, 218, 221–22 ROC and, 134
perinatal brain injury and, 193 SPECT for, 165–66
Test of Premorbid Functioning (TOPF), 16 SVT and, 145
TFCBT. See trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy symptom exaggeration and, 133–34
therapeutic alliances, 441 symptoms and, 337
Therapeutics and Technology Assessment Subcommittee, 13 treater versus expert, 29, 440t, 442f
QEEG and, 169 trial phase, 49–56
third-party observations (TPO), 32–35 admissibility in, 50–52
G-PIPE and, 92 deposition in, 52–56
threshold effect, 243 discovery in, 49
threshold limit values (TLV), 290 ethics in, 56
thrombocytopenia, 189 testimony in, 52–56
intracranial hemorrhage and, 191 trichloroethylene (TCE), 287t
thyroid disease, 296 trilogy of Daubert, 74–75
TLV. See threshold limit values TRIN. See True Response Inconsistency scale
TMD. See temporomandibular disorder tripod, 40–41
toluene, 283, 287t true negatives, 10
Tomlin v. Holecek, 33–34 True Response Inconsistency scale (TRIN), 132
TOMM. See Test of Memory Malingering T-scores, 43
TOPF. See Test of Premorbid Functioning TSI. See Trauma Symptom Inventory
topographical mapping, 165 TTFUQ. See Task Force for Test User Qualifications
Toronto Test of Acute Recovery After TBI, 266 21-Item Test, 122
toxic exposure, neuroimaging for, 169–70 type I error, 28
toxic mold, 283, 346–48
TPO. See third-party observations UGPPA. See Uniform Guardianship and Protective
Trail Making A, 7 Proceedings Act
for competency, 463t ultimate issue, 448
for neurotoxic injury, 293 undue influence, 430–31
Trail Making B, 6, 7, 14 Uniform Guardianship and Protective Proceedings Act
for competency, 463t (UGPPA), 415
for malingering, 124 United States v. Andrews, 466
for MTBI, 239, 243 United States v. Cameron, 84–85
for neurotoxic injury, 293 United States v. Dubray, 478
for PDDS, 414 United States v. Jose Santos-Bueno, 87–90
for TBI, 273 United States v. Rinchack, 465
Trail Making Test, 13, 266 United States v. Segna, 478
transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation United States v. Sullivan, 478
(TENS), 334 upsetting the witness, 55
trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy
(TFCBT), 389 VA. See Department of Veteran Affairs
Trauma Symptom Inventory (TSI), 377–78 Validity Indicator Profile (VIP), 123
traumatic brain injury (TBI). See also mild traumatic brain Validity Quotient (VQ), 47
injury; moderate TBI; pediatric TBI; severe TBI validity threats, 41
AUC and, 134 Variable Response Inconsistency scale (VRIN), 132
chronic pain with, 302 vegetative state, 269
classification of, 261–62 Verbal IQ, 7
course of recovery from, 263–65 Verbal Paired Associates, 125
Digit Span for, 128–29 Verbal Selective Reminding Test, 6, 14
FBS and, 133–35, 137t, 138 MTBI and, 239, 250
fMRI for, 167–69 verbal working memory, 167–68
hindsight bias with, 5–6 very low birth weight (VLBW), 179–83
insanity defense and, 479–81 very preterm birth (VPTB), 179–83
malingering and, 119, 124, 125 VFD. See Benton Visual Form Discrimination
MDSM for, 480 Victoria Symptom Validity Test, 122
534 index
videotaping Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition (WMS-III), 13
informed consent for, 111–12 raw data from, 104
of neuropsychological evaluation, 33t weight, 51–52
Vietnam, 369 Westbrook v. Arizona, 452–53
Violence Risk Appraisal Guide (VRAG), 485 Western Aphasia Battery, 293
VIP. See Validity Indicator Profile Whalem v. United States, 455
Visual Number Search Test, 266 whiplash-associated disorder (WAD), 332
Visual Reproduction, 125 Whitaker v. Parker, 71
VLBW. See very low birth weight white matter (WM), 179
Vocabulary Subtest, 16 HIE and, 184, 188
for malingering, 124 IPS and, 188
for neurotoxic injury, 293 neurotoxic injury and, 290
voir dire, 52–53 pediatric TBI and, 212
VPTB. See very preterm birth VLBW/VPTB and, 180
VQ. See Validity Quotient WHO. See World Health Organization
VRAG. See Violence Risk Appraisal Guide Wide-Range Achievement Test-Revised (WRAT-R), 463t
VRIN. See Variable Response Inconsistency scale Wieter v. Settle, 450, 464
VSVT, 149 Williams v. CSX Transportation Inc., 78
Wilson v. United States, 464–65
WAD. See whiplash-associated disorder Winnans v. N.Y. & Erie Railroad Co., 71
Wade v. United States, 478 Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), 6, 52, 124, 136
WAIS-III. See Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, for malingering, 125, 130–31
Third Edition for MTBI, 250
WAIS-IV. See Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, for neurotoxic injury, 293
Fourth Edition Wisconsin Failure-to-Maintain Set (FMS), 125, 125t
WAIS-R. See Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised WM. See white matter
war, 244, 369 WMS-III. See Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition
MTBI and, 244 WMS-R. See Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised
Washington v. Harper, 467 WMT. See Word Memory Test
watershed pattern, 187 Word Choice Test, 125
WCST. See Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Word Memory Test (WMT), 122
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition for deception, 171–72
(WAIS-III) FBS and, 138
for chronic pain, 315 for malingering, 117, 129
for malingering, 124 for PCS, 248
for neurotoxic injury, 293 Working Memory Index, 7
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition work product rule, 491
(WAIS-IV), 7, 16 World Health Organization (WHO), 232–33, 241
for chronic pain, 315 neurotoxic injury and, 292
for malingering, 125 on solvents, 281
for neurotoxic injury, 293 WRAT-R. See Wide-Range Achievement Test-Revised
for TBI, 273 wrongful disclosure, 91–93
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R), 8, wrongfulness, 477–79
14, 16–17
for competency, 463t X-ray, 333
malingering and, 119
MTBI and, 236 Youtsey v. United States, 446
for PDDS, 414
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth zealous advocacy
Edition, 220–21 admissibility and, 79
Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R), 6, 13, 17 vs. objectivity, 26–27
for competency, 463t zero-order validity study, 28
malingering and, 119 Zoloft. See sertraline
MTBI and, 236 z-scores, 43

Potrebbero piacerti anche