Sei sulla pagina 1di 7

Seizure (2006) 15, 235—241

www.elsevier.com/locate/yseiz

REVIEW

Cognitive side effects of anti-epileptic drugs


The relevance in childhood epilepsy
Lieven Lagae *

University Hospitals KULeuven, Department Paediatric Neurology, Herestraat 49, 3000 Leuven, Belgium

Received 14 December 2005; accepted 15 February 2006

KEYWORDS Summary In recent years several new anti-epileptic drugs have been introduced,
Cognition; also for the treatment of childhood epilepsy. A major concern is their effect on
Anti-epileptic drugs; learning and cognitive development. Testing the genuine effects on cognition of the
Childhood epilepsy; anti-epileptic drugs is methodologically not easy. At this moment there are very few
Neuropsychology; controlled trials that systematically examine the cognitive side effects of anti-
IQ; epileptic drugs in childhood epilepsy. The available data indicate that the newer
Development anti-epileptic drugs have a safe cognitive profile when prescribed correctly at the
right dose and in monotherapy. Possible negative effects are mainly found for speed of
processing and attention processes. As these processes are important instruments in
every day learning and cognition, it is necessary to test these newer anti-epileptic
drugs in well designed studies and in specific childhood epilepsy syndromes.
# 2006 British Epilepsy Association. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction with refractory epilepsy, a normal IQ was found.1


This is in contrast with the data of a pivotal and
In clinical practice, it is well known that many rather reassuring study by Ellenberg et al. in a group
children with epilepsy have cognitive problems, of normal children with easier to treat epilepsy. At
ranging from mild learning and school problems to the age of 7 years, there was no IQ difference
mental retardation or even mental decline. between epileptic children and their siblings, but
Although these problems are also encountered in only if there was no pre-existing mental retardation
adults with epilepsy, the impact of epilepsy on before the start of the epilepsy.2
developing cognitive processes is likely to be more Several non-independent factors and each to a
prominent. The proportion of children with cogni- variable extent contribute to the possible cogni-
tive problems is higher in refractory epilepsy than tive problems in epilepsy but are very difficult to
in well controlled epilepsy. This already points to study separately.3 Probably the most important
the possible deleterious effect of the epileptic determinants are the epileptic process itself and
process itself on cognitive development. In a recent the underlying brain dysfunction/pathology. Symp-
paper, it was shown that only in 16% of the children tomatic epilepsy has a worse outcome than idio-
pathic epilepsy.4 The unique contributions of the
* Tel.: +32 16343845; fax: +32 16343842. epilepsy-related factors, such as the age of onset,5
E-mail address: Lieven.Lagae@uz.kuleuven.ac.be. type of seizures and epilepsy syndrome, frequency

1059-1311/$ — see front matter # 2006 British Epilepsy Association. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.seizure.2006.02.013
236 L. Lagae

of seizures and epileptic abnormalities on the Methodological issues


EEG are more difficult to disentangle.6 It is the
combination of the underlying brain dysfunction Already in 1995, Vermeulen and Aldenkamp system-
with an epileptic syndrome at a certain age that atically reviewed the potential methodological
explains the cognitive profile. For example, tuber- problems when studying cognitive processes in epi-
ous sclerosis is highly associated with infantile leptic patients. These principles are still valid and
spasms in infancy and if these seizures are not can now be focused more on childhood epilepsy.11
well controlled, many of these children become If one wants to study the cognitive effects of an
mentally retarded and autistic.7 Children with TS anti-epileptic drug, it seems logical that this would
but without infantile spasms at the critical age be done first in normal non-epileptic subjects. How-
have a better prognosis, as well as children with ever, only short term exposure is feasible in non-
idiopathic infantile spasms. Another nice example epileptics and this will therefore not reflect the
illustrating these complex interactions is shown in cognitive side effects after prolonged administra-
the paper of Helmstaedter et al. They showed tion of the drug. Long term and preferentially
(in adults) that long lasting (and especially left) monotherapy trials are ideal. More importantly, a
temporal lobe epilepsy is clearly associated with drug should theoretically be tested in well defined
memory decline, and that this decline could epilepsy syndromes with ‘as homogeneous as possi-
be stopped after resection of the dysfunctional ble’ patient populations. An anti-epileptic drug
hippocampal structures, but only if there was theoretically can induce different cognitive effects
complete seizure control after the resection.8 in different epilepsy syndromes. For instance, it is
Another factor to consider is the psychosocial known that frontal epilepsy in a child is more prone
environment and educational/school system. It to attention problems12; if this epilepsy would
should be realized that even in 2006, in some be treated with a drug with known effects on atten-
countries, epilepsy is a reason not to send the tion processes, the attention problems in this child
children to school, of course increasing the risk for could become worse, and will indeed over-empha-
cognitive problems. size the negative attention side effects of the drug.
This short review deals with the effects of the In addition, children of different ages are suscep-
newer anti-epileptic drugs on cognitive function- tible to different effects of the anti-epileptic drugs.
ing. It is actually surprising that only recently this A similar dysfunction of the attention system at the
problem has been fully recognized and perhaps age of 4 years can cause other problems in a child of
already over-emphasized. A major historical step 8 years, even if both children are known with the
in this respect was the setting-the-scene papers same epilepsy syndrome.
of phenobarbital and its effect on IQ in febrile Another major pitfall is the seizure frequency.
seizures.9,10 Long term use of phenobarbital as Studying cognitive effects of an AED after a convul-
preventive medication for febrile seizures was sive seizure will certainly influence the cognitive
shown to decrease the IQ-levels, even after dis- profile.13 It has been shown that several days after
continuation of the drug. This paper was also influ- a seizure, subjects were still not at their cognitive
ential because it introduced the measurement of baseline. Seizure-free patients are therefore the best
IQ as a major determinant in cognitive studies. candidates for cognitive studies. Along the same line,
Although IQ remains an interesting and global mea- and this has received considerable attention in the
sure, it does not allow precise studies of specific last few years, is the effect of ‘subclinical’ epileptic
cognitive processes; it only gives a rough estimate EEG discharges, which can cause substantial perfor-
of academic potential. mance differences during cognitive testing.14,15 One
With the introduction of several new anti-epilep- can therefore argue that optimal cognitive testing
tic drugs, with more or less similar efficacy, the should always be performed under EEG guidance.16
side effect profile and especially the cognitive An underestimated problem is drug dosages and
and behavioural safety are becoming discriminating more specifically drug blood levels. Clinicians do
factors when prescribing these drugs. Unfortu- realize that for optimal seizure control, a drug
nately, cognitive safety is not yet a major issue in blood level is often a bad guide, especially since
the classic randomized controlled trials. In addition, we do not understand the exact relationship
and this will also be illustrated in this review, it is between blood and brain levels of the drug. How-
very difficult to design a powerful study that unam- ever, for cognitive issues, drug dosages are perhaps
biguously will show the negative or positive cogni- much more important.17,18
tive effects of anti-epileptic drugs. Here again, the In a way, it seems that two study designs are
non-independency of AEDs, epilepsy syndrome and optimal, but both are difficult to perform at large
brain dysfunction are at play. scale:
Cognitive side effects of anti-epileptic drugs 237

- Long term monotherapy in seizure-free patients Table 2 Leuven cognitive battery


with well defined epilepsy: comparison between
Intelligence WPPSI-R, WISC-R
pre-treatment and treatment period. In some
Attention and TEA-Ch, Tower
(benign) epilepsies, double blind cross-over with executive functions
placebo is possible.
- Monotherapy withdrawal study in seizure-free Memory
Verbal AVLT; numbers, stories,
patients.19
word pairs (CMS)
Non-verbal Pictures, dot location,
family scenes, faces (CMS);
Cognitive testing complex figure (ROCF DSS)
Language Boston Naming Test, Token
Several neuropsychological ‘batteries’ can be used
Test, Word Fluency
to study cognitive functions and the influence of
Visual/spatial Developmental Test of Visual
anti-epileptic drugs. Because testing is time con- Motor Integration (VMI)
suming and not every cognitive process can be Motor Purdue Pegboard Test
tested in detail, it is very important to select the
Checklists Child behaviour checklist
most appropriate tests and this should be a hypoth-
(parents), youth self report
esis-driven process. Most batteries therefore are a
(YSR), everyday memory
compromise. However, one should be aware of some questionnaire
basic principles in neuropsychology. One can of
AVLT: Auditory Verbal Learning Test; CMS: Children’s Memory
course define the most important cognitive pro- Scale; ROCF DSS: Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure Developmen-
cesses (Table 1, left column), but here also these tal Scoring System.
categories are not independent. Actually, one can
make a distinction between basic cognitive ‘instru-
ments’ (attention processes, working memory, cog- when you compare their results with age-matched
nitive speed, etc.) and more specific processes controls. To overcome this problem, one can com-
(visuospatial abilities, language, etc.) (Table 1, pare the test results of the patient with the ‘mental
right column). The underlying idea is that one needs age’ instead of the ‘chronological age’. Mental age
the ‘instruments’ to perform normally on the more can be deduced from the non-verbal or verbal IQ.
specific tasks. Therefore, it seems logical that a For instance, a 10-year-old child with a non-verbal
cognitive test battery should at least contain testing IQ of 75 approximately performs at a mental age of
of processing speed and attention. Another metho- 7.5 years and his test results in the specialized
dological remark has to do with the scoring of the neuropsychological batteries should therefore be
tests. Normally, scores are compared with age- compared with 7.5 years controls.20,21 This metho-
appropriate norms and expressed as a z-score or dology allows finding of specific neuropsychological
percentile. However, in mentally retarded children, dysfunctions in one or more domains even in globally
it will be difficult to find specific neuropsychological retarded patients.
dysfunctions: they will perform badly on most tests In Table 2, an example is given of a rather exten-
sive neuropsychological battery that is now being
Table 1 Neuropsychological processes used in our centre for different cognitive studies.
To gain time, we ask the ‘Centre of Student Gui-
 Sensory processing/

}
dance (CLB)’ which is present in every school to
perception
perform an age appropriate IQ test; the more spe-
 Attention/concentration
Cognitive instruments cific tests are done in our centre.
 Speed of processing
 Working memory
 Psychomotor speed Working mechanism of the anti-epileptic
drugs and cognitive profile
 Visuospatial abilities

}
 Problem-solving
 Non-verbal memory In a recent paper by Sankar and Holmes, animal
 Verbal memory studies are reviewed that addressed the cognitive
 Motor control/ Specific processes side effects of anti-epileptic drugs.22 Theoretically,
performance knowing the exact working mechanism and the site
 Verbal reasoning of action of anti-epileptic drugs, it should indeed be
 Imagery possible to predict the type of cognitive side effects
 General intelligence IQ Academic potential of an anti-epileptic drug. Two problems however
arise. First of all, we do not understand the exact
238 L. Lagae

working mechanism of all anti-epileptic drugs. It is Typically, phenobarbital, phenytoin, carbamaze-


not because we know that a drug is working on a pine and valproate are considered as ‘older’ anti-
sodium channel, that we automatically understand epileptic drugs. As already mentioned in the Intro-
why this drug is working in a particular epilepsy duction, long term use of phenobarbital in children
syndrome. Regional and temporal differences of is associated with a decrease in IQ.9,10,28 Although
the expression of this sodium channel in specific the decrease is usually lower than 10 IQ points,
excitatory and inhibitory neurons, for instance, are for some children ‘on the edge’, this decrease
a possible reason for the discrepancies we see in might be substantial. Subsequent use of phenobar-
clinical practice. Second, testing cognitive pro- bital has therefore dramatically decreased in
cesses in rodents, although valuable, remains at a recent years, no studies have examined the neu-
more rudimentary level with difficult extrapolations ropsychological nature of this IQ decrease. In adults
to human cognitive processing. too, phenobarbital has been shown to induce
Nevertheless, some interesting general conclu- more cognitive side effects than carbamazepine
sions can be drawn from this sort of studies. It and phenytoin.29,30
appears that sodium channel acting drugs are asso- In general, phenobarbital is worse than valproic
ciated with the least amount of cognitive side acid and carbamazepine.31,32 However, it should
effects, while the risk is greatest in drugs with also be mentioned that in an open label study in
gabergic action. Gabergic drugs especially influence children with well controlled seizures, no IQ differ-
vigilance and attentional processes. Anti-glutama- ences were found between the phenobarbital, car-
tergic drugs are more associated with effects on bamazepine and valproate group.33 In another
learning and memory, especially those drugs that study comparing phenytoin, carbamazepine and
work on the NMDA receptor. Recently, a totally new valproate in childhood epilepsy, it was shown that
type of working mechanism was found in levetira- only minimal changes were seen in the phenytoin
cetam. This drug binds to a synaptic vesicle SV2A group. In this study, carbamazepine treated
and influences the release of several neurotransmit- children performed worse than valproate treated
tors.23 At this moment, we do not know which children, especially on memory tasks.34 Although
cognitive processes are possibly influenced by this the methodology was certainly not always compar-
‘synaptic modulator’, but it underlines the com- able, different studies have shown that carbama-
plexity and the inherent risk for too fast and too zepine was better than or equal to phenobarbital
simple conclusions in this field. and phenytoin. Comparison of phenobarbital
and phenytoin with valproate yielded variable
Comparing older and newer anti-epileptic results.33—35
drugs Actually, for all these older drugs some adverse
cognitive side effects have been described in child-
Several excellent reviews are available that hood epilepsy and phenobarbital seems to be the
describe the cognitive side effects of the older worst drug in this respect.
and newer anti-epileptic drugs.3,18,24—27. Although In the following paragraphs, the most commonly
several methodological problems were not dealt used newer anti-epileptic drugs are discussed in
with in the studies on the older anti-epileptic drugs, more detail: topiramate, lamotrigine, oxcarbaze-
some robust findings were reproduced in most stu- pine and levetiracetam.
dies, so that some general conclusions are valid.
Also, they provide the background against which the
effects of the newer anti-epileptic drugs should be Topiramate
compared.
It is clear that all the older drugs can induce Topiramate is an effective broad spectrum anti-
psychomotor slowing, a basic cognitive instrument epileptic drug that is being used frequently in sev-
(see Table 1), and this to a variable extent. eral childhood epilepsy syndromes, including some
This was, for instance, nicely illustrated in the of the more catastrophic epilepsies, such as the
Holmfrid withdrawal study.19 Psychomotor slowing Lennox Gastaut syndrome. Studies in healthy adult
is generally measured in reaction time studies and volunteers have shown that topiramate induces
anti-epileptic drugs typically induce a 100—200 ms general mental slowing, psychomotor speed reduc-
increase of reaction time. Although most authors tion and concentration problems, illustrating a gen-
stress that these effects are minimal, this eral effect on the cognitive instruments.36 These
reaction time increase can be very critical in some effects were also dosage dependent.37 It should be
natural situations and especially during learning noted that dosage for topiramate in the first regis-
situations. tration studies was substantially higher than that
Cognitive side effects of anti-epileptic drugs 239

obtained nowadays in clinical practice. This might background EEG epileptic activity and therefore
have over-emphasized these negative cognitive improves basic cognitive possibilities. Overall,
side effects. A postmarketing study confirmed that there are no studies in adults that show a negative
cognitive complaints (especially psychomotor slow- cognitive effect of lamotrigine. Therefore, the
ing) were the primary reason for discontinuation of conclusion of a recent review by Aldenkamp and
the drug but also that, overall, most patients con- Baker was that lamotrigine was a safe drug at the
tinued the medication because of its efficacy. In this cognitive level.53
study, no specific dosage escalation or final dosage
was associated with a greater risk for cognitive side
effects.38 Oxcarbazepine
In epilepsy patients, topiramate also had an
effect on memory and on word fluency, verbal Oxcarbazepine is also a sodium channel blocker but
processing and verbal IQ.39,40 Especially, this latter has a narrower spectrum than lamotrigine. It is
effect is unique and still mis-understood. Verbal especially suited for partial epilepsy syndromes
memory, verbal fluency and verbal learning were and is related to carbamazepine but tolerability
all shown to be influenced by topiramate. This side and side effect profile are said to be better than
effect is not seen with other anti-epileptic drugs carbamazepine. Again, it is surprising that no spe-
and could not be predicted directly from animal cific cognitive studies in children have been pub-
studies. In one study, it was estimated that up to lished. Curran and Java already in 1993 showed that
one third of the patients showed word finding oxcarbazepine induced a positive effect on atten-
problems.41 At this point, there is no satisfactory tion and motor speed in healthy volunteers.54 In
explanation for this effect. As verbal functions adult epilepsy patients, the drug did not show any
involve the fastest circuits in the brain, one could specific cognitive side effects.55—57 Sabers et al.
hypothesize that topiramate preferentially affects showed a positive effect on learning and psychomo-
these circuits. tor speed. In children, the study of Serdaroglu
In children, no systematic studies on cognition et al.58 showed that only ‘drowsiness’ was reported
and topiramate have been performed yet. Moreland as a possible ‘cognitive’ side effect. Overall, and as
et al. showed that about 20% of epileptic children expected, oxcarbazepine seems to be similar in its
displayed decreased cognition, however without cognitive safety as lamotrigine.
any further explanation.42

Levetiracetam
Lamotrigine
Levetiracetam is the newest anti-epileptic drug in
Also, for this anti-epileptic drug, no systematic most countries and is already widely used in child-
studies on cognition in children are available. hood epilepsy. It has a broad spectrum profile and is
As this drug is a rather specific sodium channel unique in its working mechanism: levetiracetam
blocker, animal studies predict few cognitive side binds to a pre-synaptical SV2A protein and modu-
effects.22 Indeed, in adult volunteers, no differ- lates neurotransmitter release.23 Few studies on the
ences with placebo were found and Aldenkamp cognitive side effects are available yet. Cramer
et al.43 and Meador et al.44 even showed positive et al.59 showed in a large study in adults with
effects on some cognitive functions. This was con- epilepsy that levetiracetam did not differ from
firmed in adult epilepsy patients.36,45—47 For other AEDs in the reported frequency of anxiety,
instance, Smith et al. showed that add-on lamotri- emotional lability and nervousness. However, no
gine was not associated with adverse cognitive specific cognitive processes were measured. The
effects in 81 patients with refractory epilepsy.45 study of Neyens et al.60 showed a positive effect in
In some studies it was shown that lamotrogine cognitive tests in levetiracetam responders and no
was able to improve cognitive functioning in refrac- difference from placebo in non-responders. Here
tory epilepsy in children, sometimes irrespective of again, it is difficult to disentangle seizure reduction
seizure control. This was also shown in mentally effects on cognition from drug dependent effects.
retarded children and adults, although effects In this small study, levetiracetam treatment
were not always measured quantitatively.48—52 induced improvement in reaction time, tapping
However, one should be careful with this sort of rate of the non-dominant hand and memory for
findings and not be tempted to call this a genuine simultaneously presented words. In children, acute
psychotropic drug too early. An obvious explanation psychosis has been described in one,61 but not in
could be that this drug decreases the amount of other publications on childhood epilepsy.62
240 L. Lagae

Conclusion 15. Aarts JH, Binnie CD, Smit AM, Wilkins AJ. Selective cognitive
impairment during focal and generalized epileptiform EEG
activity. Brain 1984;107:293—308.
It should be clear from this overview that we need 16. Aldenkamp AP, Arends J. Effects of epileptiform EEG dis-
well designed studies in childhood epilepsy that charges on cognitive function: is the concept of ‘transient
address the potential cognitive side effects of cognitive impairment’ still valid? Epilepsy Behav 2004;5:
S25—34.
anti-epileptic drugs in children. We should be care-
17. Tonnby B, Nilsson HL, Aldenkamp AP, Alpherts WC, Blennow
ful to extrapolate the existing data in adults to G, Elmqvist D, et al. Withdrawal of antiepileptic medication
children. Minor cognitive side effects in adults in children. Correlation of cognitive function and plasma
could cause significant learning and cognitive concentration–—the multicentre ‘Holmfrid’ study. Epilepsy
effects in children. In addition, the existing studies Res 1994;19:141—52.
in adults still suffer from different methodological 18. Ortinski P, Meador KJ. Cognitive side effects of antiepileptic
drugs. Epilepsy Behav 2004;5(Suppl. 1):S60—5.
flaws, and definite conclusions are therefore diffi- 19. Aldenkamp AP, Alpherts WC, Blennow G, Elmqvist D, Heijbel
cult to draw. In general however, the available J, Nilsson HL, et al. Withdrawal of antiepileptic medication in
data indicate that the newer anti-epileptic drugs children-effects on cognitive function: the Multicenter Holm-
are safer at the cognitive level than the older ones, frid Study. Neurology 1993;43:41—50.
and this could become a discriminating factor in 20. Stiers P, Swillen A, De Smedt B, Lagae L, Devriendt K,
D’Agostino E, et al. Atypical neuropsychological profile in
prescription habits. a boy with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Neuropsychol Dev
Cogn C Child Neuropsychol 2005;11:87—108.
21. Wouters H, Fonteyne A, Lagae L, Stiers P, Assessment of
specific memory impairment in a multiple disabled boy with
References fragile-X syndrome and temporal lobe epilepsy. Dev Med
Child Neurol, in press.
1. Boel M. Behavioural and neuropsychological problems in 22. Sankar R, Holmes GL. Mechanisms of action for the commonly
refractory paediatric epilepsies. Eur J Paediatr Neurol used antiepileptic drugs: relevance to antiepileptic drug-
2005;8:291—7. associated neurobehavioral adverse effects. J Child Neurol
2. Ellenberg JH, Hirtz DG, Nelson KB. Do seizures in children 2004;19(Suppl. 1):S6—14.
cause intellectual deterioration? N Engl J Med 1986;314: 23. Lynch BA, Lambeng N, Nocka K, Kensel-Hammes P, Bajjalieh
1085—8. SM, Matagne A, et al. The synaptic vesicle protein SV2A is the
3. Bourgeois B. Determining the effects of antiepileptic drugs on binding site for the antiepileptic drug levetiracetam. Proc
cognitive function in paediatric patients with epilepsy. J Natl Acad Sci USA 2004;101:9861—6.
Child Neurol 2004;19(Suppl. 1):S15—24. 24. Noeker M, Haverkamp-Krois A, Haverkamp F. Development of
4. Bourgeois B, Prensky AL, Palkes HS, et al. Intelligence in mental health dysfunction in childhood epilepsy. Brain Dev
epilepsy: a prospective study in children. Ann Neurol 1983; 2005;27:5—16.
14:438—44. 25. Aldenkamp AP, De Krom M, Reijs R. Newer antiepileptic drugs
5. Vanderlinden L, Lagae LG. Clinical predictors for outcome in and cognitive issues. Epilepsia 2003;44(Suppl. 4):21—9.
infants with epilepsy. Pediatr Neurol 2004;31:52—5. 26. Devinsky O. Cognitive and behavioural effects of antiepilep-
6. Tromp SC, Weber JW, et al. Relative influence of epileptic tic drugs. Epilepsia 1995;36(Suppl. 2):S46—65.
seizures and of epilepsy syndrome on cognitive function. J 27. Brunbech L, Sabers A. Effect of antiepileptic drugs on cog-
Child Neurol 2003;18:407—12. nitive function in individuals with epilepsy. Drugs 2002;62:
7. Zaroff CM, Devinsky O, Miles D, Barr WB. Cognitive and 593—604.
behavioural correlates of tuberous sclerosis complex. J Child 28. Riva. et al. Discontinuation of phenobarbital in children:
Neurol 2004;19:847—52. effects on neurocognitive behavior. Pediatr Neurol 1996;
8. Helmstaedter C, Kurthen M, Lux S, Reuber M, Elger CE. 14:36—40.
Chronic epilepsy and cognition: a longitudinal study in tem- 29. Smith DB, Mattson RH, Cramer JA, et al. Results of a nation-
poral lobe epilepsy. Ann Neurol 2003;54:425—32. wide Veterans Administration Cooperative Study comparing
9. Farwell JR, et al. Phenobarbital for febrile seizures-effects the efficacy and toxicity of carbamazepine, phenobarbital,
on intelligence and on seizure recurrence. N Engl J Med phenytoin, and primidone. Epilepsia 1987;28:S50—8.
1990;322:364—9. 30. Meador KJ, Loring DW, Huh K, et al. Comparative cognitive
10. Camfield CS, Chaplin S, Doyle AB, Shapiro SH, Cummings C, effects of anticonvulsants. Neurology 1990;23:129—37.
Camfield PR. Side effects of phenobarbital in toddlers; beha- 31. Vining EPG, Mellits ED, Dorsen MM, et al. Psychologic and
vioural and cognitive aspects. J Pediatr 1979;95:361—5. behavioural effects of antiepileptic drugs in children: a
11. Vermeulen J, Aldenkamp AP. Cognitive side-effects of chronic double-blind comparison between phenobarbital and val-
antiepileptic drug treatment: a review of 25 years of proic acid. Pediatrics 1987;80:165—74.
research. Epilepsy Res 1995;22:65—95. 32. Calandre EP, Dominguez-Granados R, Gomez-Rubio M,
12. Auclair L, Isabelle J, Olivier D, David L, Eric S. Deficit of Molina-Font JA. Cognitive effects of long-term treatment
preparatory attention in children with frontal lobe epilepsy. with Phenobarbital and valproic acid in school children. Acta
Neuropsychologia 2005;43:1701—12. Neurol Scand 1990;81:504—6.
13. Boylan LS. Peri-Ictal behavioral and cognitive changes. Epi- 33. Chen YJ, Chow JC, Lee IC. Comparison of the cognitive effect
lepsy Behav 2002;3:16—26. of anti-epileptic drugs in seizure-free children with epilepsy
14. Aldenkamp AP, Overweg J, Gutter T, Beun AM, Diepman L, before and after drug withdrawal. Epilepsy Res 2001;44:
Mulder OG. Effect of epilepsy, seizures and epileptiform EEG 65—70.
discharges on cognitive function. Acta Neurol Scand 1996; 34. Forsythe I, Butler R, Berg I, McGuire R. Cognitive impairment in
93:253—9. new cases of epilepsy randomly assigned to carbamazepine,
Cognitive side effects of anti-epileptic drugs 241

phenytoin and sodium valproate. Dev Med Child Neurol 49. Leary PM, Allie S. A new drug suitable for children and young
1991;33:524—34. people with epilepsy and intellectual impairments. Dev Med
35. Mandelbaum DE, Burack GD. The effect of seizure type and Child Neurol 1995;37:371.
medication on cognitive and behavioral functioning in chil- 50. Beran RG, Gibson RJ. Aggressive behaviour in intellectually
dren with idiopathic epilepsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 1997; challenged patients with epilepsy treated with lamotrigine.
39:731—5. Epilepsia 1998;39:280—2.
36. Martin R, Kuzniecky R, Ho S, et al. Cognitive effects of 51. Ettinger AB, Weisbrot DM, Saracco J, et al. Positive and
topiramate, gabapentin and lamotrigine in healthy young negative psychotropic effects of lamotrigine in patients with
adults. Neurology 1999;52:321—7. epilepsy and mental retardation. Epilepsia 1998;39:874—7.
37. Aldenkamp AP, Baker G, Mulder OG, et al. A multicenter, 52. Eriksson AS, Knutsson E, Nergardh A. The effect of lamotri-
randomized clinical study to evaluate the effect on cognitive gine on epileptiform discharges in young patients with drug-
function of topiramate compared with valproate as add-on resistant epilepsy. Epilepsia 2001;42:230—6.
therapy to carbamazepine in patients with partial-onset 53. Aldenkamp AP, Baker G. A systematic review of the effects of
seizures. Epilepsia 2000;41:1167—78. lamotrigine on cognitive function and quality of life. Epilepsy
38. Tatum IV WO, French JA, et al. Postmarketing experience with Behav 2001;2:85—91.
topiramate and cognition. Epilepsia 2001;42: 1134—40. 54. Curran HV, Java R. Memory and psychomotor effects of
39. Thompson PJ, Baxendale SA, Duncan JS, Sander JWAS. Effects oxcarbazepine in healthy human volunteers. Eur J Clin Phar-
of topiramate on cognitive function. J Neurol Neurosurg macol 1993;44:529—33.
Psychiatry 2000;69:636—41. 55. Sabers A, Moller A, Dam M, et al. Cognitive function and
40. Lee S, Sziklas V, Andermann F, et al. The effects of adjunctive anticonvulsant therapy: effect of monotherapy in epilepsy.
topiramate on cognitive function in patients with epilepsy. Acta Neurol Scand 1995;92:19—27.
Epilepsia 2003;44:339—47. 56. Äikiä M, Kälviänen R, Sivenius J, Halonen T, Riekkinen RJ.
41. Shorvon SD. Safety of topiramate: adverse events and rela- Cognitive effects of oxcarbazepine and phenytoin monother-
tionships to dosing. Epilepsia 1996;37(Suppl. 2):18—22. apy in newly diagnosed epilepsy: one year follow-up. Epi-
42. Moreland EC, Griesemer DA, Holden KR. Topiramate for lepsy Res 1992;11:199—203.
intractable childhood epilepsy. Seizure 1999;8:38—40. 57. McKnee PJ, Blacklaw J, Forrest G, et al. A double-blind,
43. Aldenkamp AP, Arends J, Bootsma HP, et al. Randomized, placebo-controlled interaction study between oxcarbaze-
double-blind parallel-group study comparing cognitive pine and carbamazepine, sodium valproate and phenytoin
effects of a low-dose lamotrigine with valproate and placebo in epileptic patients. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1994;37:27—32.
in healthy volunteers. Epilepsia 2002;43:19—26. 58. Serdaroglu G, Kurul S, Tutuncuoglu S, Dirik E, Sarioglu B.
44. Meador KJ, Loring DW, Ray PG, et al. Differential cognitive Oxcarbazepine in the treatment of childhood epilepsy.
and behavioural effects of carbamazepine and lamotrigine. Pediatr Neurol 2003;28:37—41.
Neurology 2001;8:1177—82. 59. Cramer JA, De Rue K, Devinsky O, et al. A systematic review
45. Smith D, Baker G, Davies G, et al. Outcomes of add-on of the behavioral effects of levetiracetam in adults with
treatment with lamotrigine in partial epilepsy. Epilepsia epilepsy, cognitive disorders, or an anxiety disorder during
1993;34:312—22. clinical trials. Epilepsy Behav 2003;4:124—32.
46. Banks GK, Beran RG. Neuropsychological assessment in lamo- 60. Neyens LGJ, Alpherts WCJ, Aldenkamp AP. Cognitive effects
trigine treated epileptic patients. Clin Exp Neurol 1991;28: of a new pyrrolidine derivative (levetiracetam) in patients
230—7. with epilepsy. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry
47. Placidi F, Marciani MG, Diomedi M, et al. Effects of lamo- 1995;19:411—9.
trigine on nocturnal sleep, daytime somnolence and cogni- 61. Kossoff EH, Bergey GK, Freeman JM, Vining EP. Levetirace-
tive functions in focal epilepsy. Acta Neurol Scand 2000; tam psychosis in children with epilepsy. Epilepsia 2001;42:
102:81—6. 1611—3.
48. Buchanan N. The efficacy of lamotrigine on seizure control in 62. Lagae L, Buyse G, Ceulemans B. Clinical experience with
34 children, adolescents and young adults with intellectual levetiracetam in childhood epilepsy: an add-on and mono-
and physical disability. Seizure 1995;4:233—6. therapy trial. Seizure 2005;14:66—71.

Potrebbero piacerti anche