Sei sulla pagina 1di 7

Early Childhood Education Journal, Vol. 35, No.

1, August 2007 (Ó 2007)


DOI: 10.1007/s10643-007-0156-9

Mystery Boxes: Helping Children Improve their Reasoning

Audrey C. Rule1

This guest editorial describes ways teachers can use guessing games about an unknown item in
a ‘‘mystery box’’ to help children improve their abilities to listen to others, recall information,
ask purposeful questions, classify items by class, make inferences, synthesize information, and
draw conclusions. The author presents information from a qualitative analysis of 90 preservice
teachersÕ reflections on using mystery boxes with preschool through elementary students in
urban public school settings in central New York State. Children were enthusiastic about the
game, used a variety of strategies to generate questions, experienced difficulties in paying
attention, remembering and synthesizing information, and had difficulty focusing on more
than one characteristic at a time. Errors included repetition of questions and class inclusion/
exclusion errors. Modeling, thinking out loud, and giving hints helped children become suc-
cessful. Creating oneÕs own mystery box, exercises in identifying attributes and classifying
items, and practice in questioning techniques are among suggested follow-on activities.

KEY WORDS: thinking skills; reasoning; inference-making; questioning; classification.

IMPORTANCE OF THINKING SKILLS programs increase their emphasis on development of


underpinning process skills. Educators of young
While I was waiting for the movie to begin at a
children can lay a strong foundation for the devel-
theatre recently, I watched a slide show of adver-
opment of these important competencies.
tisements for local restaurants and services. One
slide sponsored by a childrenÕs advocacy group an-
MYSTERY BOXES
nounced, ‘‘I spy something green,’’ and encouraged
parents to spend time playing simple thinking games When I was a first grade teacher at an urban
with their children. Thinking skills are important for school in Alabama, I noticed the difficulties my stu-
all of us to successfully maneuver through life and dents had in formulating questions and processing
make good decisions. In fact, a review of the research information. Although our weekly show-and-tell
on important competencies in the workplace (Moy, activities were helping children with verbal expres-
1999) concluded that generic thinking skills such as sion, it wasnÕt enough. So I decided that the ‘‘Star of
collecting, analyzing, and organizing information the Week,’’ in addition to other duties and privileges,
are key skills for workplace success. The author of would bring a secret item to be put in a ‘‘mystery
that study recommended that vocational education box’’ for us to guess. The ‘‘mystery box,’’ was a
cracker box I had covered with colorful wrapping
paper and question marks. We sat in a circle on the
carpet and each student was allowed to ask one
1
Curriculum and Instruction, State University of New York at question having a ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ answer about the
Oswego, 250-G Wilber Hall, Takamine St., Oswego, NY 13126, mystery object to determine its identity. The star sat
USA. next to me, holding the mystery box, and answered
2
Correspondence should be directed to Audrey C. Rule, Curricu-
lum and Instruction, State University of New York at Oswego,
the questions. I transcribed these ‘‘Twenty-Ques-
Swetman Hall, Centennial St., Oswego, NY 13126, USA; e-mail: tions’’ sessions and added them to our weekly news-
arule@oswego.edu letter sent home to parents. Students enjoyed reading

13
1082-3301/07/0800-0013/0 Ó 2007 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC
14 Rule

what everyone had said. Because these records of logic. They werenÕt always able to identify the mys-
mystery object guesses were in studentsÕ own words, tery object, but they asked questions that incorpo-
they formed age-appropriate authentic reading rated previously learned information and made
material related to our class activities. Such content- intelligent guesses.
based reading materials have been proven effective
throughout a wide range of instructional contexts PRESERVICE TEACHERS USE MYSTERY
(Grabe & Stoller, 1997). BOXES WITH CHILDREN
Children found this game exciting but difficult in In my current position as an education professor
the beginning. It took a while for them to think of teaching science methods for childhood education
broad categories for questions (e.g. place where item majors, I had the opportunity to revive my mystery
is found, color, shape of item, purpose of item). They box as a model for preservice teachers to use during
also had initial difficulty in formulating a question so their practicum field experiences. I was looking for an
that it would receive a ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no’’ response. I activity my college education majors could easily
modeled possible queries and helped them rephrase implement at their school placements with preschool,
their questions. For example, if a student asked, kindergarten, and elementary students that would
‘‘What color is it?’’ I suggested they choose a likely practice the science process skills of inference-making
color and ask, ‘‘Is it blue?’’ and logical reasoning. I sought a motivating project
I was somewhat dismayed at the logic errors that would involve my students in making an inex-
children made in trying to determine the mystery pensive curriculum material they could keep for their
objects at first. An example transcript of questions future classrooms–something interesting they could
and answers follows. It is interesting that a child personalize, but a project flexible enough to be used
wondered if the item might be pink after learning that with several grade-levels of students from pre-K to
it was red. Perhaps the child wasnÕt listening or was elementary. I also wanted the activity to be effective
thinking that it was red and pink, but usually, I whether used with one child, a small group, or an
would interject the word ‘‘partly’’ if the item showed entire class, as I could not be sure of how many
several colors. Notice that one child asks if the object students my preservice teachers might be able to
is a car, although it has been established that the involve in an activity. Finally, the project had to
object does not have wheels. Also note that after provide the opportunity for preservice teachers to
children determined the item was a person or figure, a analyze student thinking and reflect on the experi-
child persisted in asking if it had feet. Perhaps the ence. I decided that making mystery boxes would
student wasnÕt paying attention or just wanted to ask satisfy all these requirements.
a question that would receive an affirmative response. My college students were excited to create their
Is it blue? No. mystery boxes. Many covered shoe, cracker, or cereal
Is it red? Yes. boxes with interesting fabric and added flashy trims
Is it pink? No. or jeweled words to decorate these hiding places.
Is it something with wheels? No.
Is it a car? No.
I asked them to gather at least five items from their
Is it a puppet? No. choice of several different categories to prepare for
Does it have wings? No. the activity. Students gathered toys, household items
Can you eat it? No. (e.g., clothespin, bar of soap, fork, picture frame),
Is it a rag? No. articles of clothing (e.g. scarf, bib), tools (e.g., hole-
Can you play with it? Yes.
Is it a block? No.
punch) artificial food (e.g., plastic apple, ice cream
Does it have hair? Yes, painted on. cone ornament), school supplies (e.g., marker, scis-
Is it people or a person or figure? Yes. sors) personal items (e.g., keys, jewelry, ID-card), and
Does it have feet? Yes. recycled items (e.g. soda bottle, yogurt cup).
Is it a man? Yes. My preservice teachers were required to tran-
Is it Santa Claus? Yes, it is a red bendable toy Santa Claus.
scribe two question-and-answer sessions with the
Eventually, after many weeks of practice, my children from practicum placements at urban schools
first graders became quite proficient at formulating with diverse populations of students in central New
general questions and narrowing the scope to deter- York State and to analyze these young learnersÕ
mine the mystery object. By the end of January, thinking. Then they wrote one-page essays discussing
children had dramatically reduced their errors in the experience, analyzing childrenÕs reasoning, and
Mystery Boxes: Helping Children Improve their Reasoning 15

providing suggestions for follow-on activities to help that this strategy of students was not very effective
students improve their thinking processes. With their because their mystery objects could not be found in a
permission, I compiled the insights of my ninety classroom. However, the general categories generated
college students on a spreadsheet, extracting two to by this approach were often helpful in discerning
four statements from each essay. I qualitatively ana- characteristics of the mystery object.
lyzed these ideas to determine major outcomes of the Another strategy used especially by younger
activity. I used a grounded theory approach and the learners was to play off a previous question. If a
inductive ‘‘constant comparison’’ method (Creswell, question about color had just been asked, then a
2005, p. 406) to uncover the categories of insights and student continued in this line of questioning.
outcomes in using this activity with children. This Children were most successful at determining the
approach provided six categories, which I will eluci- identity of the mystery object when general questions
date in the following sections: general observations, about a variety of characteristics were asked and
strategies children use, difficulties children have, children synthesized previous information to formu-
errors they make, ways to help children and sugges- late questions and guesses. Preservice teachers often
tions for follow-on activities. noted that once a couple of characteristics were
determined, the questioning improved markedly.
GENERAL OBSERVATIONS OF CHILDREN They also noticed that some students were able to use
PLAYING THE MYSTERY OBJECT GAME a ‘‘no’’ response to eliminate a class of objects and
make better progress.
Younger children (preschool, kindergarten, first-
graders) found the game very challenging at first, but
soon caught on and wanted to play it again and DIFFICULTIES
again. Many kindergarten children had played ‘‘IÕm As noted above, younger children had some
thinking of’’ before or rhyming ‘‘I Spy’’ while waiting difficulty understanding the game at first, but those
for buses and so were familiar with the idea of with experience with other guessing games caught on
guessing an object. Many preservice teachers more quickly. In two instances, children misunder-
observed that children improved remarkably at the stood and began by guessing what type of box was
second round of the game, learning the types of covered with fabric rather than the object the box
questions to ask by observing others. Because the contained. A few children did not know what a
mystery boxes were colorful and the question-asking ‘‘question’’ was but understood once several exam-
game was an intriguing change from typical activities ples were given.
in the classroom, students seemed to thoroughly Although the suspense of determining the mys-
enjoy playing. tery object held the attention of many students, oth-
ers had difficulty attending to the questions of others.
STRATEGIES CHILDREN USE TO ASK QUES- Some children, who appeared to be listening, forgot
TIONS AND DETERMINE MYSTERY OBJECTS information. Younger childrenÕs memories function
Some children tried to immediately guess what somewhat differently than those of older elementary
was in the box without determining any of its char- children, with younger students focusing on verbatim
acteristics. They named specific objects that seemed information (specific surface forms and properties)
to just pop into their heads as random choices. This rather than relational and semantic information or
strategy was in general not found to be useful, but the ‘‘gist’’ of the experience (Courage & Howe, 2004).
occasionally resulted in a ‘‘lucky’’ correct guess. This means that although younger children may
Other children thought of items that might pos- remember what was said, they may not be interpret-
sibly be in the box and used these ideas to generate ing and applying it to possible objects or question
questions. Sometimes the items (such as toothbrushes choices. The teacher can help by thinking out loud
or toys) were from their personal lives. Often, chil- and modeling the interpretation of information for
dren looked around the room for objects small them.
enough to fit in the box, then asked questions based Frustration and disappointment at ‘‘no’’
on characteristics of these objects. Some preservice responses was a hindrance for a few who seemed to
teachers keyed into this idea and made the game think the entire point of the game was to receive
easier for students by choosing items from the room ‘‘yes’’ responses. The teacher can help these children
for them to guess. Other college students reflected by reminding them that a ‘‘no’’ response provides
16 Rule

important information by eliminating a group of thought the box might contain snow, which could not
possible choices. Many young children did not remain frozen in the warm classroom. When learning
understand that listening to other childrenÕs questions that the shape was not that of a circle, one child
was important, while others seemed unable to syn- assumed it must be square.
thesize information. Reviewing what was known Most of these errors were evidence of faulty
about the object every few questions assisted students inference-making. ‘‘...[I]nferences are mental con-
with poor recall or listening skills. Verbalizing ideas nections between something that we already believe
about the implications of different bits of information is true and something that we believe connects to
helped children put ideas together. Talking about the it in some way’’ (Chiasson, 2005, p. 215). Infer-
process (Short, Schroeder, Kauffman, & Kaser, 2004) ences are based on observations, but use mental
of gleaning information from questions and fitting it processing to go beyond the information to clas-
together is crucial in supporting childrenÕs thinking. sify, explain, generalize, or predict. ‘‘The ability to
Younger children seemed to focus on one char- make good inferences at appropriate stages during
acteristic at a time to the exclusion of others. This is a an inquiry is synonymous with the ability to rea-
developmental characteristic of children in the pre- son well’’ (Chiasson, 2005, p. 215). Inferences
operational stage (‘‘Jean Piaget,’’ 2001; Miller & depend upon consideration of characteristics and
Church, 2003); For example, once a question about qualities, analysis of information to find relation-
color was asked, all questions focused on this aspect, ships, and representation of the results. How can
even after the specific color of the object had been we help children to improve their ability to make
determined. Although several characteristics of the solid inferences?
object might be known, children guessed objects
having only one of these characteristics. Additionally, WAYS TO HELP CHILDREN
some children persisted in asking questions that
Preservice teachers observed that modeling
related to a previous mystery object, apparently not
possible questions, summarizing information,
realizing the current mystery object was something
and thinking out loud helped students. Modeling
different.
question-asking and praising children who asked
questions was shown in an early study with Mexican-
ERRORS American second graders from disadvantaged
Children exhibited a variety of errors as they backgrounds (Zimmerman & Pike, 1972) to be more
attempted to uncover the mystery objects. A common effective than praise alone. When the teacher talks
error was asking a question already asked because of about how he/she thinks about the problem, students
inattention or a lapse in memory. Younger children have a mental model for different ways to approach
often suggested items that were too large for the box the problem (Fredericks, 1992).
(for example, a winter coat, a microwave oven, a When children became frustrated with the game,
giant). However, when the preservice teacher asked if preservice teachers found giving hints and clues
the item could fit in the box, most children realized revived spirits. Several mentioned that they allowed
their mistakes. students to gently shake the box or blindly touch the
By far, the most common errors related to object to obtain information about the size, shape,
class inclusion. For example, after the item had weight, and composition of the item. Those working
been established as some sort of toy animal, a with preschoolers often began by announcing the
student erroneously asked if it might be a ball. general category of the item, such as toy animal, food
Similarly, a student asked if the object could run item, or object related to artwork, to simplify the
after learning the object could not move. Another game.
student asked if the item might be an orange
(fruit) after the previous student learned its color SUGGESTIONS FOR FOLLOW-ON ACTIVITIES
was not orange. Suggestions for follow-on activities to help stu-
Preservice teachers also reported other types of dents improve their reasoning focused on five aspects:
errors. For instance, one child thought the mystery motivating children through ownership, building on
object the preservice teacher brought might be other knowledge, assisting children in considering
something that belonged to him and another thought different properties of objects, helping children
it might be something his mother had used. A child formulate questions, and facilitating childrenÕs
Mystery Boxes: Helping Children Improve their Reasoning 17

understanding of classification and class inclusion/ the process of deductive reasoning, can help
exclusion. determine an unknown. Children might practice
Having children decorate their own mystery asking questions about a known item in the
boxes and bring in items from home can motivate classroom. Additionally, the teacher can record
children. A child who assumes the teacherÕs role in question sets for different mystery objects and later
responding to questions has a different perspective ask children to examine the questions asked and
and may gain important insights as he/she considers determine those that were most effective or any
the questions posed by classmates. Ownership of a that were illogical.
mystery box may prompt a child to initiate informal Finally, assisting students in understanding
practice in asking questions and making inferences class inclusion and exclusion principles, the source
during free time games with peers or relatives. One of the most errors in childrenÕs logic during the
preservice teacher suggested that after children mystery box activities, is important. Practice in
worked in small groups with their own mystery object grouping and categorizing items provides a strong
boxes, items from those sets could be used in the foundation. Students might search and cut from
teacherÕs mystery box for the entire class. Prior magazines items that fit given categories such as
practice and a known set of choices may boost stu- ‘‘animals,’’ ‘‘things found inside a home,’’ things
dent success. that make noise,’’ and ‘‘things found in the
Teachers can combine mystery objects with kitchen.’’ Alternatively, they may be provided with
other more familiar classroom activities to help a large set of items and be asked to define cate-
children build their thinking skills. When studying a gories for grouping. After items have been
particular plant or animal, a plastic model or grouped, the teacher might suggest that he/she is
specimen could be put in the box and the teacher thinking about a food item and ask if it could be
could direct students to ask questions using science the wagon (grouped with ‘‘things with wheels’’) or
vocabulary they have learned. An example of this is another item from different group. Having children
the question, ‘‘Is it an herbivore?’’ asked by a sec- explain why not will help them extract class
ond grader during this investigation. Mystery items exclusion principles.
related to a unit of study might be used to help How can you use a mystery box activity in your
children review concepts. classroom? You might have children bring in secret
Micklo (1995) suggested five key experiences for objects for guessing. Your students could decorate
helping children to develop their logical thinking and their own mystery boxes, taking them home to play
classification skills: describing the attributes of things; the game with family members. Perhaps you will have
telling how things are alike and different; finding a ‘‘Mystery Object of the Week’’ related to a thematic
characteristics an item does not possess; considering unit, the current season, a special event or an
more than one attribute at a time; and distinguishing upcoming holiday. I wish you the best in your
between ‘‘some’’ and ‘‘all.’’ These concepts might be explorations.
practiced with attribute blocks and also applied to
sets of toys or other objects.
The preservice teachers I worked with suggested
that children might practice identifying various REFERENCES
properties of real objects or items in photographs cut Chiasson, P. (2005). PeirceÕs design for thinking: An embedded
from magazines to hone their skills. As questions philosophy of education. Educational Philosophy and Theory,
37(2), 207–226.
about a mystery object are asked, the teacher might Courage, M. K., & Howe, M. L. (2004). Advances in early memory
list known properties on the board. After a mystery development research: Insights about the dark side of the
object has been determined, additional properties can moon. Developmental Review, 24, 6–32.
Creswell, J. W. (2005). Educational research: Planning, conducting,
be added as children consider other questions they and evaluating quantitative and qualitative research. Upper
might have asked. Alternatively, a child might tell Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall.
single characteristics of his/her mystery object while Fredericks, A. D. (1992). Helping your child grow in thinking.
Teaching PreK-8, 23(3), 29.
others guess. Grabe, W., & Stoller, F. L. (1997). Content-based instruction:
Questioning skills can be enhanced by helping Research foundations. In M. A. Snow, & D. M. Brinton
children understand the difference between general (Eds.), The content-based classroom: Perspectives on integrating
language and content. (pp. 5–21). New York: Longman.
and specific questions and how starting with broad Jean Piaget: Champion of childrenÕs ideas (2001). Early Childhood
questions and progressing to narrower categories, Today, 15(5), 43.
18 Rule

Micklo, S. J. (1995). Developing young childrenÕs classification and Short, K. G., Schroeder, J., Kauffman, G., & Kaser, S. (2004).
logical thinking skills. Childhood Education, 72(1), 24–28. Thoughts from the editors. Language Arts, 81(4), 271.
Miller, S. A., & Church, E. B. (2003). Helping children develop Zimmerman, B. J., & Pike, E. O. (1972). Effects of modeling and
logic and reasoning skills. Early Childhood Today, 17(4), 32. reinforcement on the acquisition and generalization of ques-
Moy, J. (1999). The impact of generic competencies on workplace tion-asking behavior. Child Development, 43, 892–907.
performance. Review of research. National Centre for Voca-
tional Education Research, Leabrook, South Australia. (ERIC
Document Reproduction Service No). ED 433 422.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Potrebbero piacerti anche