Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Food Quality and Preference


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/foodqual

Rapid descriptive sensory methods – Comparison of Free Multiple Sorting,


Partial Napping, Napping, Flash Profiling and conventional profiling
Christian Dehlholm a, Per B. Brockhoff b, Lene Meinert c, Margit D. Aaslyng c, Wender L.P. Bredie a,⇑
a
Department of Food Science, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Copenhagen, Rolighedsvej 30, DK-1958 Frederiksberg C, Denmark
b
Informatics and Mathematical Modelling, Technical University of Denmark, Richard Petersens Plads, Build. 321, DK-2800 Kongens Lyngby, Denmark
c
Danish Meat Research Institute, Maglegårdsvej 2, DK-4000 Roskilde, Denmark

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Two new rapid descriptive sensory evaluation methods are introduced to the field of food sensory eval-
Received 24 August 2011 uation. The first method, free multiple sorting, allows subjects to perform ad libitum free sortings, until
Received in revised form 22 February 2012 they feel that no more relevant dissimilarities among products remain. The second method is a modal
Accepted 23 February 2012
restriction of Napping to specific sensory modalities, directing sensation and still allowing a holistic
Available online 3 March 2012
approach to products. The new methods are compared to Flash Profiling, Napping and conventional
descriptive sensory profiling. Evaluations are performed by several panels of expert assessors originating
Keywords:
from two distinct research environments. Evaluations are performed on the same nine pâté products and
Sensory analysis
Descriptive profiling
within the same period of time. Results are analysed configurationally (graphically) as well as with RV
Napping coefficients, semantically and practically. Parametric bootstrapped confidence ellipses are applied for
Free sorting the graphical validation and comparisons. This allows similar comparisons and is applicable to single-
Flash profile block evaluation designs such as Napping. The partial Napping allows repetitions on multiple sensory
Meat quality modalities, e.g. appearance, taste and mouthfeel, and shows the average of these repetitions to be signif-
icantly more closely related to the conventional profile than other methods. Semantic comparison shows
large differences, with closest relations found between the two conventional profiles. This suggests that
semantic results from an assessor in an evaluation type with no training sessions are dependent on the
assessors’ personal semantic skills. Comparisons of the methods’ practical differences highlight the time
advantage of the rapid approaches and their individual differences in the number of attributes generated.
Crown Copyright Ó 2012 Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction sions by Rosenberg et al. (1968). In this sorting procedure, the


assessors were asked to sort samples once into groups in a way
Since the introduction of the flavour profile method (Cairncross that made sense to the individual assessor only. However, a sorting
& Sjôstrom, 1950) a number of different sensory profiling methods procedure known as the Free Sorting (Steinberg, 1967) had previ-
have been developed. Some methods focus on the training of sub- ously been introduced, with an important difference in the meth-
jects on a sensory vocabulary, whereas other methods let subject odological approach. While Steinberg asked subjects to perform
more freely decide how to indicate differences between samples. multiple sortings, provided it made sense to the subject, Rosenberg
Beside conventional descriptive profiling techniques such as et al. only asked for one sorting per subject. When performing mul-
Quantitative Descriptive Analysis and the Spectrum method (Ci- tiple sortings, the assessor was allowed to make additional sortings
ville & Lyon, 1996; Stone, Sidel, Oliver, Woolsey, & Singleto, of the same sample set, as long as it made sense to the individual.
1974), faster alternatives have gained some popularity for instance After the evaluation, data analysis was performed on the merged
Projective Mapping (Risvik, McEwan, Colwill, Rogers, & Lyon, 1994) data. Despite including only two sortings per subject from the mul-
and its subsequent variants e.g. Napping (Pagès, 2003, 2005), the tiple sorting in the analysis, Rosenberg later concluded that multi-
Flash Profile (FP) (Dairou & Sieffermann, 2002) and Sorting (Rosen- ple sorting would often be superior when compared with single
berg, Nelson, & Vivekana, 1968). sorting (Rosenberg & Kim, 1975). In the present study, the Free
Sorting was introduced to the field of food sensory evaluation in Multiple Sorting (FMS) approach including all individual sortings
a cheese-based study by Lawless, Sheng, and Knoops (1995) with is introduced to the field of food science, representing the branch
references to earlier free sorting of non-food personality impres- of sorting methodologies. The free single sorting evaluation has
been evaluated several times with various results (Faye et al.,
2004; Lelievre, Chollet, Abdi, & Valentin, 2008), whereas the free
⇑ Corresponding author. Tel.: +45 35 33 32 42; fax: +45 35 33 35 09.
multiple sorting as applied here has not.
E-mail address: wb@life.ku.dk (W.L.P. Bredie).

0950-3293/$ - see front matter Crown Copyright Ó 2012 Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.02.012
268 C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277

When Free-choice profiling was introduced by Williams and laboratories, its performance has not previously been documented.
Langron (1984), development and training of the ideosyncratic Hence, we introduced this approach as the ‘Partial Napping’ (PN) as
vocabulary was the method’s cardinal point. The use or not of opposite to the non-restricted ‘Global Napping’ (GN). Here, we per-
the free-choice vocabulary made up a substantial methodological form PNs on appearance, taste and mouthfeel and a GN.
difference compared to, how the ‘conventional descriptive profile’ The aim of this paper is to compare different rapid descriptive
(CP) was and still is, applied in laboratories. Flash Profiling was methods with each other and with a consensus profiling method
introduced later by Dairou and Sieffermann (2002) as a rapid ap- based on the Quantitative Descriptive Analysis (Stone et al.,
proach based on Free-choice profiling (Williams & Langron, 1974). Different features are compared: their configurational out-
1984), leaving out the training sessions and implementing rank put, their semantic output, their reliability and their practical dif-
ordering of samples. Instead, assessors received an introduction ferences. Furthermore, applicability is studied among several
to the samples and were told to generate their own vocabulary sensory panels. A new approach that applies parametric bootstrap-
free-of-choice, based on their own sensory perception and to cover ping allows ellipses of confidence to be displayed for single block
the sensory variations in the samples. After generating relevant evaluation designs, e.g. Napping data.
attributes, they were allowed to see other assessors’ vocabularies
and to add or substitute attributes in their own list.
2. Materials and methods
Dairou and Sieffermann (2002) compared a single sorting with a
conventional descriptive profile (CP) and found configurational
2.1. Samples
relations between the methods and, to a lesser extent, between
vocabularies but suggested a study with more similar products.
Nine different types of commercially available liver pâté were
This was done on two product sets by Delarue and Sieffermann
bought in local Danish supermarkets. Products and brands were se-
(2004), who drew similar conclusions. Later, Blancher et al.
lected in cooperation with one manufacturer so that major varie-
(2007) compared a CP with the FP and FMS and concluded that
ties within the Danish product range were covered. Samples
the FP was configurationally closer to the CP than FMS. However,
were different variations of liver pâté produced by three different
the sorting was a single sorting.
manufacturers and were all from a similar production date. Pack-
Projective Mapping was introduced to the field of food sensory
aging sizes varied between 200 and 500 g. Table 1 shows the prod-
evaluation by Risvik et al. (1994). In Projective Mapping, assessors
uct information available on the package. The pâtés span the
were introduced to the method, but had no further training. They
overall branded differences in texture (coarsely chopped or not),
were supplied with a A4 paper sheet and the sample set and were
fat content (5–25 g/100 g) and bacon content (with or without).
instructed to place samples perceived as similar close to each other
The product numbers will be used throughout for identification
and samples perceived to be more different further apart. Risvik
of the pâtés.
et al., (1994) introduced Projective Mapping, recording the sample
For sensory evaluation, samples were prepared by cutting the
space using A4 paper sheets with unstructured line scales with
surfaces and crust so that each sample would be of similar size
trained assessors, but performed it later on A3 paper sheets with
(5 cm  3 cm  1 cm). The samples were stored at 2 °C and were
structured line scales on consumers, coupling the evaluation to a
served temperate to 15 °C in transparent PET containers, blind-la-
CP on trained assessors (Risvik, McEwan, & Rodbotten, 1997). King,
belled and with three-digit codes.
Cliff, and Hall (1998) experimented with Projective Mapping using
60 cm  60 cm paper sheets (also used later by Kennedy and Hey-
mann (2009) and Nestrud and Lawless (2010)) on untrained asses- 2.2. Methodology
sors and using both unstructured and structured hedonic line
scales. 2.2.1. Experiment overview
Similar to Projective Mapping, Pagès later introduced Napping. This study compared a number of different descriptive sensory
The basic ideas of Napping and Projective Mapping were similar evaluation methods both within and between sensory panels. The
as they were both having similar assessor instructions and that experimental set-up consisted of two different professional sen-
the raw data were based on Euclidean product distances. Although, sory panels, each of which performed a CP, FMS, a GN followed
important methodological differences were found in the Napping by a UFP and three PNs followed by UFPs. Furthermore, Panel A
concept, e.g. the framework of data collection had to be rectangu- performed an FP. Panel B did not consist of enough assessors to
lar, data were not to be scaled and data analysis had to be done perform an independent FP. Fig. 1 shows the overall experimental
using Multiple Factor Analysis (Escofier & Pagès, 1994; Pagès, Ass- set-up. All evaluations were performed on the same products and
elin, Morlat, & Robichet, 1987). The framework consisted of a within the same period of time. Evaluations took place in a stand-
60 cm  40 cm paper sheet, which is approximately A2 size. ardised sensory environment (ISO 8589, 2007) and followed good
Although stated otherwise in other publications, Napping and Pro- sensory practice (Lawless & Heymann, 2010). Randomised com-
jective Mapping are not the same, and Napping should be seen as a plete block designs were assigned to the CPs and the FP, while ran-
special restricted and defined case of Projective Mapping. Napping domised complete designs were assigned to the FMSs and the
was normally coupled with the Ultra Flash Profiling (UFP) (Pagès, nappings.
2003) to collect subjects’ semantic responses, so called in order
to draw conceptual parallels to the free choice and rapid features 2.2.2. Panels
of vocabulary used in the FP. Perrin et al. (2008) showed that the Assessors from two professional sensory panels participated in
UFP is a good descriptive supplement to Napping, though it pro- this study. Panel A consisted of expert assessors, while Panel B con-
vides less descriptive information than that derived from a CP. As sisted of specialised expert assessors (ISO 5492, 2008). Panel A was
Napping is in continuous development, one of the latest variations a panel attached to a university environment (University of Copen-
is the Sorted Napping (Pagès, Cadoret, & Lê, 2010) in which Nap- hagen), and assessors were mainly university students. It was a
ping is combined with a sorting procedure instead of the UFP. This broad panel in the sense that the assessors were used to perform-
study will examine the original Napping attached to the UFP. Pagès ing sensory analysis on a diverse product range. Panel B was a pa-
mentioned in his original paper that it might be a good idea to per- nel at the Danish Meat Research Institute, which supports the meat
form a Napping restricted to one sensory modality. Even though industry. For this reason, their daily work on sensory analysis was
this Napping on modalities has been implemented in some sensory focused on meat products, and they possessed prior specific
C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277 269

Table 1 Assessors performing one type of methodology could not auto-


Product information. matically perform other types of methodologies. However, the pa-
Product Brand information Pork Nutritional information – nel pools of employed expert assessors were limited. For this
liver protein/carbohydrate/fat/NaCl reason, the assessors performing the CP (nine for Panel A and eight
content (approx. g per 100 g) for Panel B) were allowed to perform the FMS as a part of their CP
(%)
training. Similarly, assessors performing the restricted PNs had in a
P1 Brand 1, liver pâté, 30 10/10/15/2 previous session performed the more general GN.
coarsely chopped,
less fat
The fact that the panel pools were limited was the reason that
P2 Brand 1, liver pâté, 30 10/10/15/2 Panel B did not perform an FP.
added bacon, less fat
P3 Brand 2, liver pâté, 35 10/5/20/1.5 2.2.3. Conventional Descriptive Profile
coarsely chopped
P4 Brand 2, liver pâté, 35 10/5/20/-
The CP applied in this study was a consensus descriptive sen-
added bacon sory profile. It consisted of a period of training before the collection
P5 Brand 3, liver pâté, 35 10/15/5/- of repeated measurements. In the analysis, the individual panels
less fat proceeded as normal independent panels and also developed their
P6 Brand 4, liver pâté, 35 10/5/20/1.5
vocabularies independently from one another.
coarsely chopped
P7 Brand 4, liver pâté, 30 10/5/25/1.5 In each panel, the assessors generated a number of attributes
coarsely chopped, which they agreed would cover the sensory variations in the nine
added bacon samples. The panel was subsequently trained to score the intensi-
P8 Brand 4, liver pâté, 35 10/5/15/1 ties of the attributes on 15 cm unstructured line scales anchored
coarsely chopped,
less fat
from ‘absent’ to ‘extreme’. In their training sessions, Panel A used
P9 Brand 5, liver pâté 35 10/5/25/1.5 references that covered all developed attributes, while Panel B
did not use references as they were familiar with the product type.
The vocabulary development and training for Panel A lasted 6 h
knowledge of the products used in this study. The research insti- and was divided into three sessions, while for Panel B it lasted
tute was not located in a university environment and the assessors 4½ h and was divided into two sessions. In the following sessions,
were associated with the labour market. On average, they were the products were evaluated in sensory booths using the balanced
older than those in Panel A. sample presentation design. Each product was evaluated four
Nine Panel A assessors performed the CP. Eight Panel B asses- times by each assessor. Data were collected using the Fizz software
sors performed the CP, but one assessor was only able to carry from Biosystemes. The entire profiling period, including training,
out two out of four repetitions, so his data were not included in was performed over 5 days for Panel A and 4 days for Panel B.
the analysis. Nine Panel A assessors performed the FP. Each Nap-
ping procedure, followed by the UFP, was performed by nine Panel 2.2.4. Flash profiling
A assessors and nine Panel B assessors. FMS was performed by 13 The FP was performed according to Dairou and Sieffermann
Panel A assessors and 10 Panel B assessors. (2002).

Fig. 1. Overview of experimental set-up. Expert assessors from Panels A and B performed the listed methodologies on the same products within the same period of time.
Subpanel sizes are shown for individual methodologies. Subpanels with the same subscript notation performed the same methodology; assessors performing the
conventional profile had performed a free multiple sorting as a part of their training and assessors performing the partial nappings had performed a global napping as a
previous session. Comparisons of methodologies were done both within and between panels.
270 C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277

Assessors received an introduction to the evaluation. After a 2.3. Data analysis


short sample presentation, assessors generated their own free-
choice vocabularies covering the sensory variations in the nine To maintain a similar and comparable approach, results ob-
samples. Assessors were allowed to see other assessors’ generated tained from the different methodologies were analysed in similar
vocabularies to add or substitute attributes in their own list. The ways. The CP raw data set consisted of a matrix with one variable
vocabulary development lasted 1 h and was divided into two per attribute, each variable being numeric and between 0 and 15.
sessions. Panel A had developed a vocabulary consisting of 29 attributes
For each attribute, samples were ranked according to their and Panel B one consisting of 20 attributes. CP data sets were ana-
intensity on an ordinal scale anchored from ‘lower’ to ‘higher’. lysed with Principal Component Analysis (PCA) using averaged
The assessors were allowed to apply the same rank to two or more data. It was chosen to work with unscaled data in this study.
samples if no difference was perceived. Each product was evalu- Other data sets were analysed with Multiple Factor Analysis
ated three times by each assessor in sensory booths, and data were (MFA). MFA combines PCA and Multiple Correspondence Analysis
collected using the Fizz software from Biosystemes. The entire pro- (MCA) according to the nature of the variables (Escofier & Pagès,
filing period, including vocabulary development, lasted 4 h and 1994) and allows grouping of variables.
was divided into 2 days. Only Panel A performed the FP. The FP data consisted of a number of variables, one for each
attribute belonging to a single assessor. The total number of attri-
butes was 189 with each assessor having between 16 and 20 attri-
butes. As the evaluation was performed as rank ordering, each
2.2.5. Napping
variable consisted of the rank number, 1–9, given to that sample
Napping was carried out according to Pagès’ (2003) original
(same ranks of samples were allowed). The FP data set was aver-
idea but was separated into two different approaches, GN and
aged over individual repetitions and grouped by assessor for the
PN. Assessors were introduced to the method, but had no further
MFA.
training. They were provided with a 60 cm  40 cm paper sheet
The FMS data set consisted of one variable for each sorting per-
and were instructed to place samples perceived as similar close
formed. In this way, the total number of variables was 104 for Pa-
to each other and samples perceived to be more different further
nel A and 96 for Panel B. The average of sortings per assessor were
apart. While the assessors gradually tasted the samples, they took
8 for Panel A and 10 for Panel B, ranging between 3 and 12 per
notes on their sensory impressions. When all the samples had been
assessor for Panel A and 6 and 14 per assessor for Panel B. Each var-
placed on the paper, the assessors substituted them with an X and
iable were categorical and consisted of the name of the group that
the samples’ codes. The UFP consisted of the noted sample charac-
each sample belonged to. The variables were grouped for the MFA
teristics and impressions written next to each X.
with all sortings performed per assessor as constituting one group.
When performing a GN, there were no restrictions in the asses-
The Napping data consisted of two variables per subject, one
sor’s separation process, and the assessor was free to give any form
being the X coordinate and one being the Y coordinate of each sam-
of semantic expression. In PN, the word ‘partial’ referred to a
ple’s placement on the evaluation sheet. These two numeric vari-
restriction in sensory modalities. In this study, assessors perform-
ables were grouped for the MFA. Data were not scaled. All
ing Nappings started evaluating samples with a GN where they had
Napping evaluations were performed with nine assessors resulting
no prior experience of evaluating the products. In subsequent ses-
in a data set of 18 variables. These variables were in each data set
sions, they continued to evaluate samples with three PNs, one re-
followed by a contingency table with the UFP results. In the contin-
stricted to separating samples on appearance, one on taste and
gency tables, each variable name were a sample attribute given by
the last one on mouthfeel. All four Nappings were completed both
any subject and the value for each sample was the frequency of
by Panel A and Panel B in 2 h during 1 day.
that attribute applied for each sample by all assessors. This re-
In the following sections of this paper, the GN is treated as one
sulted in contingency tables for GNs with Panel A applying 115
methodology, and the three PNs as one methodology, which then
attributes and Panel B applying 49 attributes. For PNs on appear-
consists of three different kinds of measurements on the same
ance, taste and mouthfeel, Panel A applied 71, 71 and 51 attributed
subjects.
and Panel B applied 40, 65 and 43 attributes, respectively.
Each sample’s data consisted of the X’s coordinates on the paper
Comparisons of overall sample configurations were made using
sheet plus the characteristics that count as attributes.
the RV coefficient (Robert & Escoufier, 1976) to see how closely re-
lated they were according to use of different MFA model dimen-
sions. The significance of the RV coefficient was tested using the
2.2.6. Free Multiple Sorting standardised RV coefficient according to Josse, Pagès, and Husson
The FMS is based on the original idea of multiple sorting by (2008).
Steinberg (1967) adapted to food samples. To graphically display uncertainty within individual methodo-
The assessors were asked to sort the samples in front of them logical approaches and to compare differences in uncertainty be-
into groups in a way that made sense to the individual assessor tween methods, 95% confidence ellipses were applied around the
only. After one sorting of all samples, they were pooled again sample mean points in a multidimensional space. The construction
and the assessor was allowed to make additional sortings of the of the confidence ellipses followed the parametric bootstrap proce-
same sample set. In each sorting, they were allowed to generate dure as outlined by Dehlholm, Brockhoff, and Bredie (2012), which
between two and eight groups containing one to eight samples. bootstraps groups of coordinates based on the MFA groups of vari-
They were instructed to continue in this way until they felt that ables. This procedure makes it possible to construct confidence
all sorting possibilities had been covered. They did not need to ellipses for one-repetition methodologies like Napping. To apply
be able to explain why they sorted as they did, provided it made the same approach for all methodological approaches, results with
sense to the assessors themselves. If they had a clear idea of why more than one repetition were averaged over repetitions.
they sorted as they did, they were encouraged to write down the The sensory attributes used in the different methods were ana-
reason. For each sorting task, data collected for a sample consisted lysed for their significance of discriminating the samples. For CP re-
of the description of the group that the sample belonged to. There sults, significant attributes were selected at p < 0.05 using the
was no preliminary training, and the FMS was carried out both by following analysis of variance model:
Panel A and Panel B during a 1 h session.
C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277 271

Attributei ¼ aðProducti Þ þ bðAssessori Þ þ dðProducti ; Assessori Þ The relation between the methodologies can be evaluated by
þ ei comparing the RV coefficients. Fig. 3 shows the RV coefficients ob-
tained from comparisons of the individually obtained MFA config-
The ANOVA model used to select significant attributes for the FP urations of the applied methods. RV coefficients were calculated
results was: for all possible combinations of methodologies and dimensionality,
but only dimensions that are typically compared are illustrated in
Attributei ¼ aðProducti Þ þ bðRepetitioni Þ þ ei Fig. 3. For Panel A, the values of all demonstrated comparisons
show corresponding levels, and they are all significantly close to
Only those attributes reported by a minimum of three assessors the CP. For two model dimensions, the RV values lie between
were included for interpretation. 0.86 for the PN and 0.74 for the FMS. Panel B shows larger differ-
For the FMS, GN and PS results, the reported descriptions of ences especially for the GN with an RV value of 0.48 for two model
products were based on frequency analysis. Beforehand, lemmati- dimensions and is not configurationally similar to the CP. FMS is
sation was performed in obvious cases. For FMS, characterisations more similar, with RV coefficients between 0.60 and 0.75 according
were reported, if the term did positively describe the product to choice of dimensions, while PN for two model dimensions is
(p < 0.25) and was used by at least two assessors. For GN and PN, 0.74. Panel A and Panel B show the same rank in the methodolo-
attributes were reported, if the term did positively describe the gies’ relation to the CP in two-dimensional model comparisons.
product (p < 0.1) and was applied by at least two assessors. After In that way, PN is the most highly related, followed by FMS and
independent semantic examinations per methodology, results then GN. However, comparing higher model dimensions increases
were separated and listed in tables of terms describing product similarity significantly for some methods, more than would be ex-
appearance, taste or mouthfeel. Before reporting, all semantic re- pected as an artefact. Especially higher model dimensions of the FP
sults were translated from Danish to English. of Panel A and the FMS of Panel B could obtain information similar
All analyses were performed in R version 2.12.1 (Ihaka & Gen- to the CP.
tleman, 1996; R Development Core Team, 2010) applying function- When comparing RV coefficients between panels, the CP results
ality from the add-on packages FactoMineR (Husson, Josse, Le, & show highest similarity (0.94) as the two panels performed an
Mazet, 2010; Lê, Josse, & Husson, 2008) and SensoMineR (Husson overall product configuration that was approx. 95% similar. The
& Lê, 2010; Lê & Husson, 2008). two PN configurations show approx. 80% similarity (0.82), and
FMS configurations show around 70% similarity (0.71) but rising
when including a higher number of dimensions. The GN results
3. Results show lower configurational similarities (0.48) between the panels.
All possible combinations of dimensions, e.g. comparisons of an
3.1. Configuration comparisons uneven number of dimensions between methods, have been calcu-
lated and examined (not shown here). These show RV coefficients
The distributions of samples according to the applied sensory to be similar to the levels presented in Fig. 3. Other combinations
methodology are compared in Fig. 2 for both panels. All configura- are not higher or lower than the corresponding variations for an-
tions are obtained by individual models and are shown with differ- other method comparison. Method comparisons showing a high
ent colours for each methodological approach and panel. Fig. 2 variation within the RV coefficient include the FP and some FMSs.
gives the overall picture that products are separated in similar Individual model configurations for each panel and applied
ways, since all results for an individual product are placed in the methodology are shown in Fig. 4. The resample-based 95% graph-
same area. Products 4 and 9 show a low variability among results, ical confidence intervals are superimposed as ellipses for all sam-
while Products 1 and 2 seem to show the largest variability. For in- ples and show a graphical overview of uncertainty within each
stance, Product 2 shows contradictory results obtained for the GN methodology. The area covered by confidence ellipses is observed
by Panel A and Panel B, for the FMS by Panel A and for the CP by to be larger for the lower plots in the figure and smaller for the
Panel B. upper plots. GN showed the largest ellipses, while the CPs, the FP
and the PNs showed smaller ellipses. FMS is observed to have
smaller ellipses than the ones of GN. In methods showing larger
ellipses, the GN and the FMS, one repetition of the evaluation
was performed, while the CP, the FP and the PNs included several
repetitions. Although, large differences existed between the CP, FP
and PNs in the time spend by the assessors to evaluate the samples
(Table 5). The plots shown in Fig. 4 have been arranged according
to the amount of time spent on evaluation from most time (top) to
least time (bottom). When comparing the two panels in Fig. 4 sim-
ilar structures of uncertainty were observed.

3.2. Semantic comparisons

The results of attribute analysis for all methodological ap-


proaches are shown for each product in the following tables (Ta-
bles 2–4) according to appearance, taste and mouthfeel
attributes, respectively. The sensory descriptions given by the sen-
sory panels were also compared to the sensory information given
on the product labels. The number of attributes varied greatly be-
tween the different methodologies, ranging from 20 terms in the
Fig. 2. Comparative MFA on individual configurations of a conventional descriptive
profile (CP), the flash profile (FP), the free multiple sorting (FMS), the global napping
CP by Panel B to 189 terms in the FP by Panel A (Table 5). The three
(GN) and the partial nappings’ (PN) mean, showing Product 1 (P1) to Product 9 (P9). PNs produced on average 171 terms by each panel. That is an aver-
Suffix-A: Panel A; suffix-B: Panel B. age of 57 terms for each sensory modality.
272 C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277

Fig. 3. RV coefficients for method configuration comparisons. Left: Comparisons between CP and other applied methods for Panel A. Middle: Comparisons between CP and
other applied methods for Panel B. Right: Comparisons between panels within the same applied method. The abscissa shows the number of model dimensions chosen for
comparison (2, 3 or 4 from each MFA result, respectively). The ordinate shows the RV coefficient. For each point, the level of significance for overall configuration similarity is
shown (–p > 0.05; ⁄p < 0.05; ⁄⁄p < 0.01; ⁄⁄⁄p < 0.001).

Regarding appearance attributes (Table 2), the red/reddish col- 3.3. Overall comparisons
our for Product 2 and the light grey colour for Product 9 were usu-
ally mentioned throughout all methods applied. When looking at Except for the CP, all applied methodologies have one thing in
characteristics for specific methodologies, it is worth mentioning common. They can be performed relatively rapidly. Similarities
that in PN both panels described Product 2 as soft, Product 3 as and differences between working with the different methods are
hard/compact (as did the FP) and Product 7 as coarse/crumbly collected in Table 5, separated into a ‘training and introduction’
(as did the FP). Furthermore, both panels in the CP describe Product part, a ‘measuring samples’ part and a ‘results’ part. The overall
7 as red/pink and Products 6, 7 and 8 as coarse/lumpy/structural. time needed to perform a rapid descriptive profile is up to 3 h,
This matches the products’ label information (Table 1), which while the CP is performed within a minimum of 9 h over several
states that Products 1, 6, 7 and 8 are coarsely chopped, and also days and is not rapid. Even though the actual sample measure-
from the same brand. Panel B found in both FMS, GN and PN that ments in the FP are not considered to be rapid, the overall approach
Products 3 and 9 have a light colour. For both products, the panel is regarded as rapid, and, according to the experimenterś choice of
did not find these differences during the CP, which may indicate repetitions, the method has the option of being performed within
that new differences might not come into play when using a stan- half a day.
dard training procedure.
Regarding taste attributes (Table 3), bacon taste for Product 2,
peppery/spicy taste for Product 5 and bacon/smoked/salami taste 4. Discussions
for Product 7 were mentioned throughout all applied methods. In
addition, the CP described Product 1 as spicy (as did the FP) and 4.1. Configuration comparisons
Product 2 as smoked and salty (as did the FP). In FMS, Product 7
was also described as having pork/stale taste and Product 8 as hav- When comparing different methodologies, the CP is chosen as a
ing liver taste. To match the product label information (Table 1), reference because of its assumed sound and valid results and its
the added bacon in Products 2 and 7 was found by all applied widespread use in academia and industry. The fact that GN is
methodologies. Nothing was found for Product 4, even though shown to have the fewest similarities to the CP should not be inter-
the label text indicated added bacon. preted in any way hedonically. The configurational differences
Regarding mouthfeel attributes (Table 4), the terms coarse/ shown between the GN and the CP are a product of applied meth-
lumpy/incoherent were generally mentioned for Product 7. Fur- odologies following different concepts. To choose the right rapid
thermore, the CP was used by both panels to describe Product approach for any given task is not a matter of choosing the one
3 as burning and lumpy, Product 4 as lumpy, Product 5 as burn- most closely related to a CP, but rather choosing the one best sui-
ing and hard/structural, Product 6 as lumpy/coarse (as did the ted to answer the problem in question.
FMS) and Product 7 as burning and gritty/grainy (as did the It is chosen to compare all performed methods in the same MFA
FP). Applying PN, the panels agreed that Product 2 had a fast plot (Fig. 2). This is done to examine the relative configuration dif-
meltdown/greasy mouthfeel. In FMS, GN and PN, only Panel B ferences. In a normal work situation, the analyst would only exam-
indicated that Product 3 was compact. When comparing with ine one data set’s plot with regard to the product configuration.
the product label information (Table 1), there was a general rela- When applying MFA on multiple results, a more optimistic picture
tion between results and Product 7 being labelled as coarsely of different configurations’ closeness is obtained, as the groups of
chopped, while only some of the applied methods showed Prod- results are rotated. In that case, one should remember to look at
ucts 3, 6 and 8 to be coarsely chopped. No methods showed the relative differences. Hence, the figure shows a good single plot
Product 1 as being coarsely chopped, indicating that it is not for comparing the differences between methods rather than the
generally perceived that way. exact placement of the individual sample.
C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277 273

Fig. 4. Individual configurations for all performed methodologies showing each sample’s 95% confidence interval. The higher the plot, the more time is spent on sample
evaluation. CP: conventional descriptive profile, FP: flash profile, FMS: free multiple sorting, GN: global napping, PN: partial nappings’ mean.

Although the RV coefficient is a good single measure for overall overall comparison and result in a lower RV coefficient (Fig. 3),
similarity when comparing multidimensional results, it does not even though all other samples are perceived similar to other meth-
alone provide more than an overall picture. For instance, if a sam- odological approaches. Also, the more rapid approach as GN with
ple in one applied method is placed as an outlier, e.g. Panel B’s PN more variation in results and between panels might result in less
measurement of Product 1 in Fig. 2, it will have an effect on the similarity and larger differences in RV coefficients.
274 C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277

Table 2
Appearance attributes significantly applied for each methodology and product.

Method Panel Product Product 2 Product 3 Product Product 5 Product 6 Product 7 Product 8 Product 9
1 4
Conventional A Moist Moist, red Grey Coarse, lumps of Coarse, lumps of fat, Coarse, lumps Grey
profilea fat, moist, red red of fat, moist
B Reddish Brown Brown, lumpy, Brown, lumpy, pink, Lumpy,
structural structural structural
Flash profileb A Red Compact Compact Coarse Coarse, grainy Coarse Compact,
firm
Free multiple A Reddish Brown Light
sortingc B Reddish Light Grey brown Light
brown brown,
lumpy
Global nappingd A Light Dry Smooth
B Reddish Light Grey,
brown light
Partial napping on A Soft Hard Jelly Crumbly, gritty, Firm,
appearanced incoherent, lumpy light
B Half- Reddish Compact, Compact, Coarse Light
darkened brown, soft light dark
a
Attribute selection based on ANOVA model: Attributei = a(Producti) + b(Assessori) + d(Producti, Assessori) + ei showing terms positively describing (p < 0.05) products.
b
Attribute selection based on ANOVA model: Attributei = a(Producti) + b(Repetitioni) + ei showing terms positively describing (p < 0.05) products and applied by at least
three assessors.
c
Attribute selection based on chi-square statistics showing terms positively describing (p < 0.25) products and applied by at least two assessors.
d
Attribute selection based on chi-square statistics showing terms positively describing (p < 0.1) products and applied by at least two assessors.

The product range examined in this study consists of liver pâtés way. As vocabularies are generated through different concepts,
from different manufactures and with variations in their main the concept itself defines the output. The CP and FP are clearly
ingredients. The sampling is by the authors thought to cover both reductionistic in their vocabulary generating phases, as it is often
larger and smaller product differences. Results, configurationally attempted to break down the product into easier recognisable con-
and semantically, of cause represents the sample set. To generalise cepts with common understandings. The actual measurement is al-
results on studies with other sample sets, one must assess the nat- ways on a reference term, and whatever the form of reference, the
ure of the differences among products. reference will have to be thought of or defined in some way. Hence,
this leads to a more reductionistic approach than those of methods
4.2. Semantic comparisons being more holistic. This is the case, even when profiling more
complex terms. In free-choice vocabulary examples like the FP,
Only the CPs involved vocabulary training. As each panel was vocabularies are potentially idiosyncratic and tailor-made for the
encouraged to develop the optimal vocabulary for the panel con- individual assessor. Nevertheless, the attributes must be definable.
sensus, each panel’s vocabulary was not completely similar. It is In FMS and Napping evaluations, the assessor instructions allow
difficult to compare all acquired vocabularies in a completely fair samples to be separated without being able to express the differ-

Table 3
Taste attributes significantly applied for each methodology and product.

Method Panel Product Product 2 Product 3 Product 4 Product 5 Product 6 Product 7 Product 8 Product 9
1
Conventional A Onion, Bacon, salty, Peppery, Spicy, sweet creamy Peppery, Rancid Bacon, salty, Rancid Liver
profilea spicy smoked sweet spicy smoked
creamy
B Liver, Salty, smoked Salty, sourish Liver, spicy Smoked bacon,
spicy bacon, sweet sweet
Flash profileb A Onion, Bacon, onion, Liver, sweet Herbs (rosemary, Liver, Bacon, smoked,
spicy salty, smoked creamy thyme), liver, spicy, peppery, spicy
sweet spicy
Free multiple A Soap, Bacon, chicken Creamy, Liver, sourish (different) Strong, Nutty Bacon, pork, Liver Liver
sortingc spicy liver liver peppery (coconut) smoked (canned
food)
B Bacon Sourish, spicy, sweet Liver, spicy Bacon, smoked, Liver Abnormal
stale (fishy) (meat smell
taste)
Global A Bouillon Bacon, butcher Peppery Intense,
nappingd salami,
smoked
B Liver pâté Spicy
Partial A Bacon Mild Sweet Spicy Salami
napping B Spicy Bacon, mealy Onion Peppery Bacon, smoked, Cardboard,
on tasted spicy old
a
Attribute selection based on ANOVA model: Attributei = a(Producti) + b(Assessori) + d(Producti, Assessori) + ei showing terms positively describing (p < 0.05) products.
b
Attribute selection based on ANOVA model: Attributei = a(Producti) + b(Repetitioni) + ei showing terms positively describing (p < 0.05) products and applied by at least
three assessors.
c
Attribute selection based on chi-square statistics showing terms positively describing (p < 0.25) products and applied by at least two assessors.
d
Attribute selection based on chi-square statistics showing terms positively describing (p < 0.1) products and applied by at least two assessors.
C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277 275

Table 4
Mouthfeel attributes significantly applied for each methodology and product.

Method Panel Product Product 2 Product 3 Product 4 Product 5 Product 6 Product 7 Product 8 Product 9
1
Conventional A Burning aftertaste, Coherent, hand, Burning aftertaste, Lumpy Burning aftertaste, Coherent,
profilea coherent, hard, lumpy, sticky gritty, hard gritty, lumpy hardness,
lumpy, sticky sticky
B Soft Soft Burning, lumpy Lumpy Burning, structural Lumpy, soft, Burning, gritty, Gritty,
structural lumpy, soft, lumpy, soft
structural
Flash profileb A Compact, greasy Coarse Grainy, lumpy Coarse, Compact,
lumpy firm
Free multiple A Fine, Dry, lumpy Coarse Coarse Coarse Fine, hard
sortingc smooth
B Compact Greasy Compact, gritty Coarse, lumpy, old
Global nappingd A Mealy
B Loose Compact Compact Coarse Fine, firm
Partial napping A Fast Greasy, smooth, Incoherent Coherent,
on mouthfeeld meltdown sticky dry, tough
B Soft Greasy Compact Dry, loose Gritty Lumpy Coarse
a
Attribute selection based on ANOVA model: Attributei = a(Producti) + b(Assessori) + d(Producti, Assessori) + ei showing terms positively describing (p < 0.05) products.
b
Attribute selection based on ANOVA model: Attributei = a(Producti) + b(Repetitioni) + ei showing terms positively describing (p < 0.05) products and applied by at least
three assessors.
c
Attribute selection based on chi-square statistics showing terms positively describing (p < 0.25) products and applied by at least two assessors.
d
Attribute selection based on chi-square statistics showing terms positively describing (p < 0.1) products and applied by at least two assessors.

ence in words. In this way, the approach to the sample can be said Regarding the GN evaluation, the attached UFP makes it possi-
to be more holistic. This is thought to be the case for both GN and ble to use unlimited semantic terms. As with the FMS and the FP,
PN. Where PN is restricted to one sensory modality, GN would be an excessively large vocabulary can be an interpretational draw-
the most holistic approach. In many cases, the assessors were able back. As for GN, acquired terms are fewer and more diverse. In this
to explain, after the evaluation, why samples had been separated paper, it was chosen to compare method semantics by calculating a
as they were. This indicated that, even though the assessor was al- significant relationship to samples based on frequencies for
lowed to be multidimensional in the response, it was often chosen methods with only one repetition. As a result of the sparse and di-
to separate samples using ‘unidimensional’ terms. So, in GN, the verse GN vocabulary, this frequency-based approach resulted in
assessor is given the possibility to respond in a holistic way, but few significant descriptive attributes. Another approach to looking
may choose not to. And on the other hand, unidimensional idiosyn- at Napping data and vocabularies could be to calculate the terms
cratic terms might have been given as an explanation for a significantly describing model dimensions. But then it is only
perceived difference that was not fully understandable. possible to describe an overall model configuration and not the

Table 5
Comparison and differences in applied sensory methodologies.

Conventional profile Flash profile Free multiple sorting Global napping + ultra Partial nappingb + ultra
flash profiling flash profiling
Training and introduction
Training sessions Yes No No No No
Vocabulary introduction Yes Yes No No No
Vocabulary style Consensus Individual Individual Individual Individual
Tasting samples before evaluation Yes Yes No No No
Time used on traininga / 5–6 h 1h 10 min 10 min 10 min
introduction
Measuring samples
Fixed / predetermined vocabulary Yes Yes No No No
Measurements 15 cm unstructured Ranks Contingencies Euclidean distances Euclidean distances
VAS
Repetitions 4 3 1 1 1/1/1
Time used on measurementsc 4h 120 min 50 min 30 min 70 min
Overall time 9–10 h 3h 60 min 40 min 80 min
Holistic sample approach No No Yes Yes Yes
Rapid measurements No No Yes Yes Yes
Results
Individual assessor data sets (k- No Yes Yes Yes Yes
sets)
Output (No. of attributes) Panel A: 29Panel B: 20 Panel A: Panel A: 104Panel B: Panel A: 115Panel B: Panel A: 71/71/51Panel B: 40/65/
189 96 49 43
Statistics applied PCA MFA MFA MFA MFA
Rapid configurational results Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Rapid descriptive results Yes No No Yes Yes
a
For the conventional profile, sessions were spread over several days.
b
The partial nappings (PN) in the experiment consisted of one PN at appearance, one PN at taste and one PN at mouthfeel.
c
For the conventional profile and flash profile, sessions were spread over several days.
276 C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277

individual samples. We should consider whether applied basic sta- number of terms is known. But assessors might apply different
tistics have the properties to justify rapid methodologies at all. attributes and some attributes will surely not be significant. If
Normally, when applying GN and UFP, interpretation would prob- the analyst wants a degree of control over which attributes come
ably be less conservative than here and more interpretational into play, these approaches can be followed. It could be a common
based. situation that the orderer of the evaluation expects a differentia-
In this study, it was chosen to apply all methodologies to expert tion among products on predecided attributes. It would then make
panelists in order to maintain a consistent comparative strategy. sense to train on them. In other cases, the desire is to examine mar-
Some methods may be applicable to naive subjects, consumers kets including unknown products or to promote the development
or also experts. Several studies have shown that the sensory pro- of new and creative vocabularies. Here, a rapid evaluation with a
files are very similar when comparing trained and untrained asses- holistic approach, e.g. GN or PN, would be suitable. In cases were
sors (Husson & Pagès, 2003) or experts and consumers (Worch, Lê, a holistic approach is favoured but focus need to be directed, the
& Punter, 2010). If the methods had been performed on naive PNs performed by modality is an advantage. It is then easy to make
assessors, it might have been necessary to recruit more subjects three distinct models based on Euclidean distances and with
for each evaluation in order to maintain the same separation of superimposed vocabulary for just a single sensory modality. After
samples. examining individual modalities, they can be combined to a model
presenting an overall picture, and, as we see in this study, shows
4.3. Overall comparisons configurations similar to that of the CP. Another adaption would
be, when the analyst is interested in a specific type of information
The obvious difference between methods is found in the time it only, e.g. textural differences. Then it is only necessary to perform a
takes to complete the evaluation. Except for the CP, all approaches PN on texture. But bear in mind, that the amount of recorded data
can potentially be performed in less than half a day, morning or is little per assessor. In this study, the three PNs were performed
afternoon. It is important to take that into consideration in any within a total of 80 min, which is twice as much as the 40 min
application where the focus is on the use of resources. Provided spent in the GN, but still rapid, indeed, and providing three times
the results are valid, it is optimal to minimise resources. Table 5 more data to model then GN. In cases where the product set con-
shows the time spend on measurements to be 30, 50, 70, sists of products with minor differences, e.g. small changes of
120 min and 4 h for the GN, FMS, PNs, FP and CP, respectively. ingredient content, an un-guided approach like GN would not be
The increasing amount of time, spend on sample measurements, expected to show significant descriptions between products. In
seems to comply with the increasing size of the confidence ellipses that case, a CP might show a few significant attributes. But as a ra-
shown in Fig. 4. Notice, that spending 50–70 min on measurements pid approach, it could be meaningful to try out a restricted PN
rather than 30 min seems to lower sample confidence close to beforehand to examine the eventual discriminability.
methods, where 2–4 h are spend on measurements. It seems Evaluations with holistic sample approaches seem to promote
unnecessary to spend 4 h on measurements to show significant dif- any kind of discriminator, explainable or not. A vocabulary devel-
ferences among the selected products. Nevertheless, the choice of opment session can have different purposes and can be directed to-
method will have to rely more on the project aim. wards, for example, product development usage or marketing
An important difference can be seen in the way vocabularies are usage. For marketing purposes, it could be an advantage to apply
applied to samples. Both the CP and FP apply a perceptually ‘reduc- a holistic approach on a group of creative people. Other examples
tionistic’ approach, meaning that they have to break down a whole of industrial applications of sensory evaluations are found in new
sensory perception into individual attributes. In the CP training, product development (NPD). It would be natural and straightfor-
assessors have to adapt the common vocabulary. As a group, or ward to implement rapid approaches, e.g. Napping, in the NPD idea
machine, they are calibrated to perform in the same way. A more phase and a more reliable fixed vocabulary type evaluation, e.g. CP,
individual approach takes place in the FP, where both the calibra- in the later NPD documentation phase.
tion part and the training are left out (as opposed to Free-choice
profiling). With the CP and FP, a predetermined amount of measur- 5. Conclusions
ing points is applied to the sample set. Hence, the focus during
measuring is placed on the chosen semantics as well as on the A CP was compared with the FP, FMS, GN and PNs performed
sample. This is different for FMS and Napping methods, which by several panels. All methods examined show similar configura-
try to follow another concept with the entire focus kept on the tions, though CP and PN are significantly more highly related than
sample. Here, the approaches are individual and based on the dif- others within and between panels. Regarding two-dimensional
ferences the subjects experience between samples. It is not until model configurations, the highest RV coefficients and similarities
after a separation has taken place that subjects are asked to de- between panels are found for the CP (0.94, p < 0.001) followed by
scribe their action, and only if they can. Approaching the samples PN (0.82, p < 0.001), then FMS (0.71, p = 0.005) and finally GN
this way is more holistic and allows the use of intermodal, more (0.48, p = 0.044). Regarding panels individually, Panel A shows
complex and idiosyncratic semantics. high RV coefficients for all methodologies compared with the
When choosing the method for a specific problem, one must CP, while Panel B shows a larger variation. When comparing dif-
consider the method-specific differences rather than choosing a ferent methods to the CP, both panels show the PN to have higher
method just because it can be performed rapidly. It would be a RV coefficients and other methods to have lower RV coefficients.
shame to ‘waste’ resources on panel training and four repetitions Also, both panels show the same declining rank of methods.
if a rapid approach would be sufficient to cover ones needs. Based on resampling, confidence ellipses for each sample show
Although it is clear that faster approaches are sufficient to reveal larger ellipses, especially for FMS and GN, illustrating noisier data.
configurational main dimensions, the largest difference lies in the Although the configurations are similar, the applied semantics are
semantics. different. Applying conservative semantic analysis, four out of
When precise definitions of sensory product differences are nine products (Products 2, 5, 7 and 9) have some descriptors, ap-
needed, the CP may be the method of choice. The CP may also give plied through all methodologies, in common. Comparing seman-
the possibility to describe smaller sensory nuances in the products tics between panels, most common descriptors are found when
due to the longer training period. In FP, the analyst partly lets go of using the CP, though common descriptors are also found among
a certain degree of control. A vocabulary is still present and the the other methods.
C. Dehlholm et al. / Food Quality and Preference 26 (2012) 267–277 277

PN is introduced, and the result is found to be very comparable ISO 8589 (2007). International Standard 8589. Sensory analysis - General guidance
for the design of test rooms. Ref. No. ISO 8589:2007 (E). International
to the CP, even though PN can be performed within a total of
Organization for Standardization. Genève.
80 min. It is recommended to allow the same subjects to perform Josse, J., Pagès, J., & Husson, F. (2008). Testing the significance of the RV coefficient.
PNs on multiple sensory modalities, making it possible to obtain Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 53, 82–91.
both a modality-based and an overall sensory picture of the prod- Kennedy, J., & Heymann, H. (2009). Projective mapping and descriptive analysis of
milk and dark chocolates. Journal of Sensory Studies, 24, 220–233.
uct in question. Compared to GN, multiple PNs provide a larger King, M. C., Cliff, M. A., & Hall, J. W. (1998). Comparison of projective mapping and
data set but may limit the holistic perception by restricting the sorting data collection and multivariate methodologies for identification of
ability to apply cross-modal discriminators. similarity-of-use of snack bars. Journal of Sensory Studies, 13, 347–358.
Lawless, H. T., & Heymann, H. (2010). Principles of good practice. In Sensory
Choosing the right method for an evaluation depends on the evaluation of food – principles and practices, pp. 57–77). New York: Springer.
aim and expected results of the evaluation, but, indeed, rapid Lawless, H. T., Sheng, N., & Knoops, S. S. C. P. (1995). Multidimensional-scaling of
methodologies are suitable to describe differences among sorting data applied to cheese perception. Food Quality and Preference, 6, 91–98.
Lê, S., & Husson, F. (2008). Sensominer: A package for sensory data analysis. Journal
products. of Sensory Studies, 23, 14–25.
Lê, S., Josse, J., & Husson, F. (2008). FactoMineR: An R package for multivariate
Acknowledgements analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 25, 1–18.
Lelievre, M., Chollet, S., Abdi, H., & Valentin, D. (2008). What is the validity of the
sorting task for describing beers? A study using trained and untrained assessors.
This work was financially supported by the Danish Food Indus- Food Quality and Preference, 19, 697–703.
try Agency (Ministry of Food, Agriculture and Fisheries) and the Nestrud, M. A., & Lawless, H. T. (2010). Perceptual mapping of apples and cheeses
using projective mapping and sorting. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 390–405.
Danish Pig Levy Fund.
Pagès, J. (2003). Direct collection of sensory distances: application to the evaluation
of ten white wines of the Loire Valley. Sciences des Aliments, 23, 679–688.
References Pagès, J. (2005). Collection and analysis of perceived product inter-distances using
multiple factor analysis: Application to the study of 10 white wines from the
Blancher, G., Chollet, S., Kesteloot, R., Hoang, D. N., Cuvelier, G., & Sieffermann, J. M. Loire Valley. Food Quality and Preference, 16, 642–649.
(2007). French and Vietnamese: How do they describe texture characteristics of Pagès, J., Asselin, C., Morlat, R., & Robichet, J. (1987). Multiple factor-analysis and its
the same food? A case study with jellies. Food Quality and Preference, 18, use in the sensory data-processing - application to red wines from the Loire
560–575. Valley. Sciences des Aliments, 7, 549–571.
Cairncross, S. E., & Sjôstrom, L. B. (1950). Flavor profiles: A new approach to flavor Pagès, J., Cadoret, M., & Lê, S. (2010). The sorted napping: A new holistic approach in
problems. Food Technology, 4, 308–311. sensory evaluation. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 637–658.
Civille, G. V., & Lyon, B. (1996). ASTM lexicon vocabulary for descriptive analysis. Perrin, L., Symoneaux, R., Maitre, I., Asselin, C., Jourjon, F., & Pages, J. (2008).
Philadelphia: American Society for Testing and Materials. Comparison of three sensory napping (R) procedure: Case of methods for use
Dairou, V., & Sieffermann, J. M. (2002). A comparison of 14 jams characterized by with the ten wines from Loire valley. Food Quality and Preference, 19, 1–11.
conventional profile and a quick original method, the flash profile. Journal of R Development Core Team (2010). R: A language and environment for statistical
Food Science, 67, 826–834. computing. 3-900051-07-0. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical
Dehlholm, C., Brockhoff, P. B., & Bredie, W. L. P. (2012). Confidence ellipses: A Computing.
variation based on parametric bootstrapping applicable on Multiple Factor Risvik, E., McEwan, J. A., Colwill, J. S., Rogers, R., & Lyon, D. H. (1994). Projective
Analysis results for rapid graphical evaluation. Food Quality and Preference, 26, mapping: A tool for sensory analysis and consumer research. Food Quality and
278–280. Preference, 5, 263–269.
Delarue, J., & Sieffermann, J. M. (2004). Sensory mapping using flash profile. Risvik, E., McEwan, J. A., & Rodbotten, M. (1997). Evaluation of sensory profiling and
comparison with a conventional descriptive method for the evaluation of the projective mapping data. Food Quality and Preference, 8, 63–71.
flavour of fruit dairy products. Food Quality and Preference, 15, 383–392. Robert, P., & Escoufier, Y. (1976). Unifying tool for linear multivariate statistical-
Escofier, B., & Pagès, J. (1994). Multiple factor-analysis (Afmult package). methods - rv-coefficient. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series C – Applied
Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 18, 121–140. Statistics, 25, 257–265.
Faye, P., Bremaud, D., Daubin, M. D., Courcoux, P., Giboreau, A., & Nicod, H. (2004). Rosenberg, S., & Kim, M. P. (1975). Method of sorting as a data-gathering procedure
Perceptive free sorting and verbalization tasks with naive subjects: an in multivariate research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 10, 489–502.
alternative to descriptive mappings. Food Quality and Preference, 15, 781–791. Rosenberg, S., Nelson, C., & Vivekana, P. S. (1968). A multidimensional approach to
Husson, F., Josse, J., Le, S., & Mazet, J. (2010). FactoMineR: Multivariate exploratory structure of personality impressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
data analysis and data mining with R. R package version 1.14. Available at 9, 283.
http://www.CRAN.R-project.org/package=FactoMineR. Steinberg, D. D. (1967). The Word Sort: An instrument for semantic analysis.
Husson, F., & Lê, S. (2010). SensoMineR: Sensory data analysis with R. R package Psychonomic Science, 8, 541–542.
version 1.11. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=SensoMineR. Stone, H., Sidel, J., Oliver, S., Woolsey, A., & Singleto, R. C. (1974). Sensory evaluation
Husson, F., & Pagès, J. (2003). Comparison of sensory profiles done by trained and by quantitative descriptive analysis. Food Technology, 28, 24–34.
untrained juries: Methodology and results. Journal of Sensory Studies, 18, Williams, A. A., & Langron, S. P. (1984). The use of free-choice profiling for the
453–464. evaluation of commercial ports. Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture, 35,
Ihaka, R., & Gentleman, R. (1996). R: A language for data analysis and graphic. 558–568.
Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics, 5, 299–314. Worch, T., Lê, S., & Punter, P. (2010). How reliable are the consumers? Comparison
ISO 5492 (2008). International Standard 5492. Sensory analysis – Vocabulary. Ref. of sensory profiles from consumers and experts. Food Quality and Preference, 21,
No. ISO 5492:2008 (E). International Organization for Standardization. Genève. 309–318.

Potrebbero piacerti anche