Sei sulla pagina 1di 28

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2016, 49, 548–575 NUMBER 3 (FALL)

CONTINGENCIES PROMOTE DELAY TOLERANCE


MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI, GREGORY P. HANLEY, AND JOSHUA JESSEL
WESTERN NEW ENGLAND UNIVERSITY

The effectiveness of functional communication training as treatment for problem behavior


depends on the extent to which treatment can be extended to typical environments that include
unavoidable and unpredictable reinforcement delays. Time-based progressive delay (TBPD)
often results in the loss of acquired communication responses and the resurgence of problem
behavior, whereas contingency-based progressive delay (CBPD) appears to be effective for
increasing tolerance for delayed reinforcement. No direct comparison of TBPD and CBPD has,
however, been conducted. We used single-subject designs to compare the relative efficacy of
TBPD and CBPD. Four individuals who engaged in problem behavior (e.g., aggression, vocal
and motor disruptions, self-injury) participated. Results were consistent across all participants,
and showed lower rates of problem behavior and collateral responses during CBPD than during
TBPD. The generality of CBPD treatment effects, including optimal rates of communication
and compliance with demands, was demonstrated across a small but heterogeneous group of
participants, reinforcement contingencies, and contexts.
Key words: contingency-based delay, delayed reinforcement, functional communication
training, generality, schedule thinning, severe problem behavior

Functional communication training (FCT; resurgence of problem behavior (Hanley,


e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985), a form of Iwata, & Thompson, 2001).
function-based differential reinforcement, has To increase the generality of effects, thinning
been shown to reduce problem behavior by the schedule of reinforcement for the functional
teaching the individual an appropriate alterna- communication response (FCR) is often listed
tive behavior that serves the same function as as an essential component of FCT when treat-
problem behavior. In fact, FCT combined with ment is extended to the typical environment
extinction has been shown to be an efficacious (e.g., Durand & Moskowitz, 2015; Kurtz
treatment for a variety of problem behaviors et al., 2011). Various procedures for increasing
that differ both functionally and topographi- tolerance for delays to reinforcement (here
cally (Kurtz, Boelter, Jarmolowicz, Chin, & defined as near-zero levels of problem behavior
Hagopian, 2011; Tiger, Hanley, & Bruzek, and manding during extensive nonreinforce-
2008). Problems arise, however, because care- ment periods and the resumption of manding
givers cannot always reinforce requests immedi- when appropriate) have been evaluated (see
ately, and these periods of nonreinforcement Hagopian, Boelter, & Jarmolowicz, 2011, for a
for appropriate communication can lead to the review). One common procedure involves pro-
graming gradually increasing delays between
the FCR and the delivery of the reinforcer,
This study was conducted in partial fulfillment of the often indicated with a brief signal such as
requirements for a PhD in Behavior Analysis from “wait” (Vollmer, Borrero, Lalli, & Daniel,
Western New England University by the first author. We
thank William H. Ahearn, Chata Dickson, and Rachel 1999). This procedure has been referred to as a
Thompson for their feedback on earlier versions of this delay schedule (Hagopian et al., 2011). Delay
manuscript. schedules have an intuitive appeal because the
Address correspondence to Gregory P. Hanley, Western arrangement best emulates the typical situations
New England University, 1215 Wilbraham Road, Spring-
field, Massachusetts 01119 (e-mail: ghanley@wne.edu). experienced in the natural environment
doi: 10.1002/jaba.333 (i.e., when parents cannot provide requested

548
DELAY TOLERANCE 549

items or interactions, they tell the child to wait were not achieved). In most of their cases, the
and then provide that which was requested addition of punishment was necessary to attain
when it is possible to do so). This procedure, a 90% reduction in problem behavior. Wacker
however, frequently results in the loss of the et al. (2011) also showed that long periods of
newly acquired FCR and a resurgence of prob- FCT treatment (an average of 14 months) were
lem behavior, usually within the first 16 s of required before treatment effects would persist
delay (Fisher, Thompson, Hagopian, Bow- during 5-min periods of extinction, and even
man, & Krug, 2000; Hagopian et al., 2011; longer periods of treatment were required when
Hanley et al., 2001). Delayed reinforcement a 15-min extinction period was used. In addi-
could also elicit negative emotional responses as tion, although problem behavior was reduced
well as evoke an excessive amount of manding during repeated extinction exposures, after
(Fisher et al., 2000) before the resurgence of nearly 2 years of treatment, problem behavior
problem behavior, and these collateral responses was not eliminated for half the children.
may be as disruptive as the original problem The negative side effects observed with
behavior. delayed schedules may also be attributed to a
The apparent obstacle to achieving general contingency-weakening effect that occurs under
effects of FCT may be partly due to the this arrangement (Hanley et al., 2001). Positive
extinction-like periods created by the long contingency strength may be defined as the
delays that could result in the resurgence of the probability of obtaining reinforcement given a
previously reinforced problem behavior response being greater than the probability
(Lieving & Lattal, 2003) and agitated or emo- given no response (Hammond, 1980). Luc-
tional responding (Lerman & Iwata, 1996). zynski and Hanley (2014) found that delivering
Lieving and Lattal (2003) showed that as sche- reinforcement after a delay resulted in a contin-
dules of intermittent reinforcement are gency strength of −1 (the weakest possible con-
thinned, longer periods of nonreinforcement tingency) because no reinforcers were ever
are created that are functionally equivalent to delivered in close temporal proximity to the
conventional extinction and can lead to the communication response. Thus, the delay
resurgence of the previously reinforced schedule created a context that was probably
response. Volkert, Lerman, Call, and Trosclair- aversive to the participating children, in that
Lasserre (2009) demonstrated that problem they preferred a context with no reinforcement
behavior may resurge when the newly acquired at all to one in which delayed reinforcement
FCR is placed on an intermittent schedule of was programmed.
reinforcement during generalization attempts To mitigate the contingency-weakening
with FCT. In fact, deterioration of FCT treat- effects associated with delay, multiple schedules
ment effects during implementation in more (e.g., Fisher, Kuhn, & Thompson, 1998; Han-
typical environments has often been reported ley et al., 2001), chained schedules (e.g., Fisher
(Fisher et al., 2000; Hagopian, Fisher, Sullivan, et al., 1993; Lalli, Casey, & Kates, 1995), or a
Acquisto, & LeBlanc, 1998; Hagopian et al., combination of the two (e.g., Falcomata, Mue-
2011; Hanley et al., 2001; Rooker, Jessel, thing, Gainey, Hoffman, & Fragale, 2013)
Kurtz, & Hagopian, 2013). For instance, have been adopted and have successfully main-
Hagopian et al. (1998) found that when delays tained zero or near-zero rates of problem behav-
to reinforcement and demand fading were ior during long periods of nonreinforcement.
introduced, the efficacy of FCT with extinction Multiple schedules involve a time-based alter-
was maintained in less than one half of the nation between reinforcement and extinction
applications (i.e., clinically acceptable outcomes components, both of which are correlated with
550 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

distinct stimuli (e.g., colored cards). Chained might need to scan the environment and find
schedules incorporate a response requirement, alternative activities while he or she waits for
either a specific number of demands or a spe- the preferred items and others’ attention. Delay
cific duration of time engaged in work activity, schedules are structurally ideal for teaching
to be completed, after which the first instance behavioral expectations in these situations;
of FCR results in reinforcement. Neither multi- however, strategies for mitigating the extensive
ple nor chained schedules, however, precisely negative side effects associated with their appli-
emulate the unplanned and therefore unpre- cation have not yet been articulated.
dictable delays that are often experienced in One change to typical delay procedures that
homes and schools. may reduce the commonly reported negative
Chained and multiple schedules require par- side effects is the addition of probabilistic
ents and teachers to plan periods of nonreinfor- immediate reinforcement of the communica-
cement or demand time, during which an tion response. The addition of immediate rein-
individual’s FCRs are ignored. After these peri- forcement of some FCRs would increase the
ods are over (either when a time criterion has FCR-reinforcer positive contingency strength.
been met or through completion of required This change may also increase the ecological
demands), an interval of reinforcement then validity of this procedure because requests in
sets in, and parents are advised to reinforce the typical environment are also immediately
requests immediately. Delays in the typical granted sometimes. Another change that may
environment, however, do not emulate this improve the effectiveness of delay schedules
arrangement, and are often sudden, unex- involves the addition of a response requirement
pected, and unplanned. Individuals can request during the delay. In other words, the negative
a variety of items at a given time, and in these side effects may be mitigated by changing from
cases, caregivers may not know whether the a time-based delay to a contingency-based delay
reinforcer is available until the specific request in which a chain of responses after the FCR
has been made, making it difficult to plan for will result in the delivery of reinforcement. A
immediate reinforcement. One must also be contingency-based delay increases the FCR
able to tolerate periods in which their reinfor- reinforcer positive contingency strength by
cing activities are suddenly interrupted and building a chain of responses that ultimately
their requests are not granted in the absence of contacts reinforcement, thereby minimizing the
clear stimuli that signal the unavailability of creation of long delays that emulate conven-
reinforcement and even under stimulus condi- tional extinction.
tions that would normally signal immediate These procedural changes to the delay sched-
reinforcement (e.g., a toy is available but the ule were described in a study by Hanley, Jin,
battery runs out). In such cases, the only natu- Vanselow, and Hanratty (2014) in which
rally occurring stimuli may be brief verbal probabilistic immediate reinforcement and
responses of “wait,” “not right now,” or “in a contingency-based delay were used to treat
minute” to the request. The individual is, then, problem behaviors maintained by positive rein-
expected to wait for the request to be granted forcement for one child and a synthesis of posi-
without engaging in repeated manding, prob- tive and negative reinforcement for other
lem behavior, and negative emotional children. During contingency-based delay, fol-
responses. In addition, the individual will often lowing the cue to wait, the children were ini-
be required to comply with an adult’s requests tially required to engage in a tolerance response
or acquiesce to someone else’s preferences dur- (e.g., saying “okay”); progressively more diffi-
ing the delay. At other times, the individual cult response chains were then prompted before
DELAY TOLERANCE 551

reinforcers would be delivered. In this way, the activities in both time-based and contingency-
experimenters were able to extend the delay to based delay conditions. The second purpose of
practical levels that included completion of age- this study was to evaluate the direct effects of
appropriate demands and engagement with contingency-based delay on collateral responses
appropriate leisure items during the delay with- (e.g., excessive manding, negative emotional
out the resurgence of problem behavior. responding) and compliance with adult instruc-
Given that Hanley et al. (2014) implemen- tions, so multiple measures were collected
ted multiple changes to the way in which across all participants. The third purpose of this
delays are traditionally scheduled, the extent to study was to assess the generality of delay toler-
which each of the changes is necessary for the ance training. In addition to the systematic rep-
success of this treatment remains unclear. For lication of the comparison across a wide range
example, the addition of probabilistic reinforce- of participant characteristics and different rein-
ment to increase the contingency strength of forcement contingencies, we also evaluated the
FCRs may be sufficient to produce the same extent to which behavior changes that occurred
results with time-based delay. Also, the mere as a function of experience with either delay
presence of and redirection to an alternative procedure would generalize to a second context
activity may be sufficient to maintain zero in which problem behavior during the delay
levels of problem behavior during the delay. would be reinforced (i.e., a context that emu-
For instance, Fisher et al. (2000) showed that lates typical environments with no extinction
in one case the addition of an alternative work during delays).
activity, without a contingency, was enough to
reduce positively reinforced problem behavior
GENERAL METHOD
and collateral responses during nonreinforce-
ment intervals. These authors, however, did Participants and Settings
not report on the rate of excessive manding or Four individuals, ranging in age from
compliance with demands during these inter- 21 months to 30 years, who had been referred
vals. The extent to which the mere presence of to our university-based outpatient services, par-
an alternative activity during delays to rein- ticipated in this study. Nico was a 23-month-
forcement, without a response contingency, will old typically developing boy who reportedly
be sufficient to eliminate severe problem behav- had difficulty waiting for preferred items and
ior without the emergence of other collateral activities. Nico’s parents reported that he would
responses remains to be investigated. often repeat his requests multiple times, say
The main purpose of this study was to evalu- “no” when told to wait, and would sometimes
ate the direct effects of a response contingency have a tantrum that included crying and flop-
during delayed reinforcement. Although ping if his requests were not granted. Nico
contingency-based delay has been used as the could follow multistep vocal instructions, had a
main treatment (Hanley et al., 2014) or a com- typically developing imitation repertoire and
ponent of treatment (e.g., Carr & Carlson, fine and gross motor skills, and communicated
1993), the effects of a response contingency using gestures, single words, and partially
alone have not been evaluated. We therefore framed sentences. He had an age-appropriate
conducted a comparative analysis of time-based play repertoire including imaginative play. Nico
(TBCD) versus contingency-based (CBPD) attended a center-based day-care.
delay tolerance training. To isolate the effects Will was a 30-year-old man with a diagnosis
of a response contingency alone, we included of pervasive developmental disorder, comorbid
both probabilistic reinforcement and alternative intellectual disability, attention deficit
552 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

hyperactivity disorder, and episodic mood physical disruptions and aggression toward
disorder. He had a long history of severe adults and peers. Alex reportedly became highly
self-injurious behavior (SIB), which consisted emotional and aggressive at home, school, and
primarily of hand-to-head hitting that often led other community outings when his preferred
to open wounds on his forehead, as well as activities were interrupted or not available and
finger biting. Will reportedly engaged in SIB when asked to comply with demands. These
throughout the day at his rehabilitation center, episodes usually led to adults complying with
his group home, and during transport to and Alex’s requests and providing access to pre-
from the center. Staff reported that they often ferred items to calm him down as well as physi-
gave him food and drinks to calm him down. cal and verbal attention (e.g., hugs, squeezes,
He was nonvocal and had no formal communi- reprimands). He could follow multistep vocal
cation system. He could follow some simple instructions, had a typically developing imita-
gestural prompts, had no echoic or motor imi- tion repertoire, fine and gross motor skills, and
tation repertoire, and had limited gross motor spoke in full sentences. He had a developmen-
and fine motor skills. He could walk independ- tally appropriate play repertoire including imag-
ently and feed himself but was not toilet inative play. He had some difficulty
trained and had no independent play or leisure pronouncing certain sounds and was receiving
skills. He attended the day habilitation center speech services. He attended a public school in
5 days per week and spent the majority of his which he spent the majority of his time in a
time eating, taking walks, or sleeping. resource room that included the support of
Jack was a 21-month-old boy with a diagno- paraprofessionals and had an individualized
sis of an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) who educational plan.
engaged in severe problem behavior multiple All sessions for Nico, Jack, and Alex were
times each day. His problem behavior included conducted in small treatment rooms (4 m by
motor disruptions and aggression toward peers 3 m) at a university setting equipped with a
and adults (usually toward his mother) when one-way observation panel, audio-video equip-
preferred activities were interrupted and when ment, child-sized tables, two chairs, and aca-
an item he requested was not available. These demic and play materials as needed. All sessions
episodes usually led to the family leaving social for Will were conducted in an open area in the
settings or providing Jack with the requested day rehabilitation center that contained
item or other preferred items as well as physical cafeteria-style tables and adult-sized chairs. Ses-
and verbal attention (e.g., hugs, squeezes, repri- sions were conducted 2 to 4 days per week,
mands). He could follow simple vocal instruc- two to eight times each day. Sessions lasted
tions, had good fine motor skills, could imitate 3 to 5 min throughout the functional analyses
simple motor responses and partially echo two- and mand analysis. Sessions lasted 3 to 5 min
word phrases, and communicated using ges- (Nico and Jack) or five evocative trials (Will
tures and word approximations. He had no and Alex) during FCT. An evocative trial
independent play or leisure skills. He received involved the presentation of the evocative situa-
early intervention services at home that tion and was as long as necessary to allow the
included early intensive behavioral intervention, target FCR to occur and for any scheduled
speech language services, and occupational ther- delays and reinforcement periods to be pre-
apy. He also attended a home-based day-care. sented. Sessions lasted for five evocative trials
Alex was a 6-year old boy with a diagnosis of throughout the comparative analyses unless the
ASD who engaged in daily episodes of severe time-based session termination criteria were
problem behavior that included vocal and met (20-min session for Nico and 10 min of
DELAY TOLERANCE 553

crying for Jack). A minimum of three evocative items included varied for each child but
trials was required for a session to be included remained the same throughout all assessment
in the analyses. and treatment analyses for each child. During
the assessment, the analyst directed the child
to the tables and reviewed the items that were
Data Collection and Interobserver Agreement
available by touching and naming each activ-
Trained observers recorded data using pencil
ity. She then allowed the child to walk around
and paper during the functional and preference
the table and manipulate the items briefly
analyses for Will. Otherwise, trained observers
before prompting the child to choose three to
collected data via computers that provided a
five preferred items to bring into the session
second-by-second account of participants’
room. The analyst then selected one to three
responses and relevant contextual features. Spe-
items that the child had not chosen during
cific response definitions and data collection
the previous two selection opportunities and
and conversion details are provided in the rele-
used those as the neutral items for the alterna-
vant sections.
tive activity or demands. Access to the tables
Interobserver agreement was assessed by hav-
was typically provided after two to four
ing a second observer collect data on all target
sessions.
behaviors simultaneously but independently
during at least 20% (range, 20% to 60%) of
the sessions in each condition for each partici- PART 1: FUNCTIONAL ASSESSMENT
pant. Each session’s data were divided into 10-s Data Collection and Response Definitions
intervals and compared on an interval-by- Target problem behavior for Jack and Alex
interval basis. Agreement percentages were cal- included aggression (defined as hitting, biting,
culated by dividing the smaller number of kicking, hair pulling, head butting, and push-
responses or duration (in seconds) in each ing) and disruptions (defined as both physical
interval by the larger number, averaging the disruptions such as throwing, ripping, swiping,
fractions, and converting the result to a per- and pushing items, banging items together, and
centage. Interobserver agreement averaged 95% vocal disruptions such as a high-pitched
(range, 76% to 100%) for Nico, 96% (range, scream). Will’s problem behavior was self-
80% to 100%) for Will, 93% (range, 76% to injurious behavior in the form of hand-to-head
100%) for Jack, and 92% (range, 74% to hits and knuckle biting. Target behavior for
100%) for Alex. Nico included problem behavior (i.e., aggres-
sion), minor problem behavior (defined as cry-
Preference Assessment ing, whining, throwing, ripping, and swiping),
We conducted a preference assessment that gestures (defined as reaching and pointing), sin-
involved presenting an array of items simulta- gle words, or framed mands. Counts of partici-
neously, similar to a multiple-stimulus- pants’ problem behavior were collected and
without-replacement preference assessment converted to a rate for all analyses.
(e.g., DeLeon & Iwata, 1996). For Jack, Alex,
and Nico, highly preferred and neutral items Procedure
and activities nominated by caregivers during Open-ended functional assessment interview
an open-ended interview along with other age- and interactive observation. An open-ended
appropriate toys and academic activities (10 to interview, as described by Hanley (2012), was
20 items) were arbitrarily arranged on two conducted with the participants’ caregivers pri-
semicircle-shaped tables before sessions. The marily to discover potential reinforcers that
554 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

might influence the individual’s problem (e.g., presenting writing tasks, taking away toys
behavior and contexts in which problem behav- or tablet, removing attention) in test sessions
ior was most likely. The interview lasted 45 to and allowing 30-s access to the reported conse-
60 min and was followed by a 20-min informal quences immediately after problem behavior,
observation of the participant interacting with and (b) withholding the same evocative situa-
parents (Nico, Jack, and Alex) or staff (Will) in tions in control sessions by presenting the puta-
which play preferences, language skills, topogra- tive reinforcers continuously. Events that were
phies of problem behavior, fine and motor not suspected of maintaining problem behavior
skills, and other unique characteristics described (e.g., escape from demands for Jack, analyst’s
by caregivers during the interview were directly attention and escape from demands for Will)
observed to individualize and prepare for were freely available in both the test and con-
analyses. trol conditions, ensuring that the only differ-
Mand analysis. The open-ended interview ence between test and control conditions was
with Nico’s parents revealed that concerns cen- the programmed reinforcement contingency.
tered exclusively on situations when an item or Will. Staff reported that whenever Will
activity or their attention was not immediately appeared agitated or started to engage in self-
available and Nico was asked to wait, during injury, they gave him snack items. Based on
which time he would mostly engage in exces- the results of this interview and the brief obser-
sive manding and minor problem behavior vation, an analysis of a social-positive reinforce-
such as whining, crying, and throwing items. ment contingency was conducted using the
Given that these behaviors often followed one- typically available food items (e.g., raisins,
word or framed mands for preferred items and crackers, peanuts, cheese, cookies). Two to
the parent reported that Nico seldom engaged three of these snack items were visible but
in any severe problem behavior, a mand analy- slightly out of reach in both test and control
sis (Hernandez, Hanley, Ingvarsson, & Tiger, conditions. During the control condition, very
2007) was determined to be more suitable for small bites of each snack were placed on a
identifying the predominant response form that plate, and the plate was presented to Will
functioned as a mand for tangible items. The approximately every 10 s independent of his
analysis involved rapidly alternating between behavior. Following his selection, the plate was
two conditions. The test consisted of differen- removed. By contrast, the plate with the snack
tial reinforcement of target responses (DRA), items was presented during the test sessions
whereas the control consisted of continuous only after instances of head hitting or finger
noncontingent reinforcement (NCR). During biting; each instance resulted in the plate being
NCR, the preferred toys, DVDs, and activities presented and a snack bite obtained.
were made available freely and continuously. Jack. Based on the interview results and
During DRA, the preferred items were placed observation with Jack’s mother, a synthesized
on a table but access was blocked by the ana- contingency of attention and tangible items
lyst. Access to items was provided for 30 s con- was analyzed in one context conducted by his
tingent on any target response. mother (Context 1) and in another by the ana-
Functional analyses. Following open-ended lyst (Context 2). Two analyses were conducted
interviews, functional analyses were designed to create two baselines from which the direct
for Will’s, Jack’s, and Alex’s problem behavior. and general effects of the delay procedures
The analyses involved rapidly alternating could be evaluated. Preferred items (e.g., a hair
between test and control sessions by brush, broom and dust pan, DVDs) identified
(a) presenting the reported evocative situation by Jack’s mother during the interview and
DELAY TOLERANCE 555

some additional age-appropriate toys and activ- evoked when adults stopped complying with
ities were placed in the preference assessment. his requests (see Bowman, Fisher, Thomp-
Given the mother’s report that to calm Jack son, & Piazza, 1997, for a similar functional
down she would often attend to him and pro- relation) and interrupted his preferred activities
vide access to the preferred items, a synthesized to place demands to engage in other tasks.
attention and tangible reinforcement contin- Given that problem behavior often resulted in
gency was tested in the analysis. During the the simultaneous delivery of attention, removal
control condition, Jack had continuous and of demands, and adult compliance with mands,
noncontingent access to his mother’s (or the a synthesized contingency of positive and nega-
analyst’s) attention (e.g., sitting in her lap, pre- tive reinforcement was arranged in two analyses
tend cooking with her) and access to preferred conducted by the same analyst but in two dif-
items. During the test condition, the adult pre- ferent contexts. Context 1 contained materials
tended to be busy with one of the items and selected from the preference assessment that
also blocked access to all other preferred items. did not include any dinosaur-related items.
Contingent on any instance of problem behav- Instead, items in the preference assessment
ior, the adult immediately attended to Jack included other activities reported as highly pre-
(e.g., comforted and played with him) and gave ferred by Alex’s mother (e.g., drawing activity,
him access to the preferred items for 30 s. Legos, tablet), other age-appropriate toys and
Alex. During the interview, Alex’s mother activities, and demand materials. Context
reported that dinosaurs were his favorite topic. 2 included only Alex’s most preferred activity,
He often engaged in imaginative play and con- which was dinosaur figure sets along with
structed elaborate dinosaur theme sets that dinosaur-themed books and stickers. During
“had to remain untouched” in the family the control condition, Alex was given uninter-
home. He would demand that his parents and rupted access to his preferred activity and the
younger brother play along with the very spe- analyst complied with all of his reasonable
cific roles he would assign to them. Any move- requests (i.e., those that could be granted in the
ment of these items by others, interruption of session room safely) and presented no demands.
play, or failure to assume the assigned role During the test condition, the analyst inter-
resulted in severe tantrums that included rupted play, denied his requests, and presented
aggression and could last up to 30 min. Alex’s a demand (e.g., the therapist deviated from the
specific requests extended to other activities in play as instructed by Alex and told him to do
the home and school. For example, he often something else). Three-step prompting was
demanded that his peers play by his rules in used to ensure compliance with demands. Con-
the gym, and he insisted on doing academic tingent on any instance of problem behavior,
tasks in a specific manner regardless of the tea- the analyst removed demands, allowed Alex to
cher’s instructions. Most interruptions or redir- resume his activity in his preferred manner,
ections of preferred activities resulted in severe and honored his requests for 30 s.
tantrums. Alex’s mother reported that when
these tantrums occurred, she helped Alex calm
down by removing her demands and encourag- Results
ing him to take a breath and tell her what he Nico. Although all of Nico’s target responses,
wants, which then resulted in the resumption including aggression, would have produced
of his preferred activity and compliance with reinforcement during the DRA condition of
his requests. The results of the open-ended the mand analysis, only minor problem behav-
interview suggested that problem behavior was ior (e.g., whining and throwing), gestures, and
556 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

single-word and partially framed mands were behavior yielded access to preferred items and
emitted. Single-word mands, however, emerged adult attention, irrespective of whether his
as Nico’s predominant response (Figure 1). mother or the analyst implemented the contin-
The rate of single-word responses was consist- gency (Figure 1). The result of the functional
ently higher in the DRA sessions, and Nico assessment process suggested that problem
engaged exclusively in single-word mands dur- behavior was maintained by a combination of
ing the last test–control dyad. The results sug- social-positive reinforcers. Although the exact
gested that his predominant response for role of each reinforcer included in the synthe-
preferred tangible items and adult attention was sized contingency was not isolated and the
a single-word mand. extent to which main effects, interactions, or
Will. Problem behavior was observed exclu- both were maintaining problem behavior were
sively in test sessions in which Will’s problem not determined, the analysis did emulate the
behavior resulted in snack items (Figure 1). typical conditions Jack experienced and identi-
The result of the functional assessment process fied a context that demonstrated control over
showed that problem behavior was maintained his problem behavior. The inclusion of all pos-
by access to food. sible contingencies of reinforcement resulted in
Jack. Problem behavior was observed exclu- a reliable baseline from which to evaluate the
sively in test sessions in which Jack’s problem effects of FCT and a highly challenging context

Figure 1. Results of the mand form analysis for Nico and the interview-informed synthesized contingency analyses
for Will, Jack, and Alex.
DELAY TOLERANCE 557

to evaluate reinforcement delay (see Ghaemma- chest or a vocal response of “my way.” Given
ghami, Hanley, Jin, & Vanselow, 2016; Han- Jack’s limited vocal imitation repertoire, a novel
ley et al., 2014; and Jessel, Hanley, & hand gesture was added to supplement the
Ghaemmaghami, 2016, for more detailed dis- vocal response and allow immediate prompting.
cussions of the interview-informed synthesized Alex’s initial FCR was “my way, please.” His
contingency analysis). target FCR consisted of saying “excuse me” and
Alex. Problem behavior was observed exclu- then waiting for acknowledgment before saying
sively in the test conditions when Alex’s prob- “May I have my way please?” FCRs were con-
lem behavior terminated adult instruction and sidered prompted if the analyst prompted any
allowed him access to preferred activities, adult part of the FCR within 10 s of the participant
attention, and having his requests granted emitting the response. Only independent FCRs
(Figure 1). The result of the functional assess- are reported. Counts of participants’ communi-
ment process suggested that problem behavior cation responses and problem behavior were
was sensitive to a combination of social-positive collected and converted to a rate for all
and negative reinforcers. Isolating the suspected analyses.
contingencies of reinforcement was also not
desirable or possible in Alex’s case, because
most events that evoked problem behavior Procedure
involved a simultaneous provision of both neg- When reinforcers were identified for prob-
ative and positive reinforcement. For example, lem behavior or predominant mand forms, we
removing interruptions of activity meant that attempted to replace problem behavior and
he simultaneously escaped the adult instruction simple mand forms with more socially accepta-
and resumed uninterrupted access to his pre- ble and developmentally appropriate mand
ferred activity. That is, both positive and nega- forms via FCT plus extinction. The effects of
tive reinforcement operated in tandem. Similar FCT plus extinction were demonstrated in a
to Jack, however, the inclusion of all possible concurrent operants AB design for Nico and
reinforcers in the contingency also provided us Will and a concurrent operants within a multi-
with a challenging and reliable baseline from ple baseline design across contexts for Jack and
which to evaluate communication and toler- Alex. The test sessions of the mand or func-
ance skills. tional analysis served as the baselines for all
FCT evaluations.
During FCT, access to reinforcers was pro-
PART 2: FUNCTIONAL
vided for approximately 1 min before each ses-
COMMUNICATION TRAINING
sion; the session started by the removal of all
Data Collection and Response Definitions reinforcers and the presentation of an evocative
In addition to problem behavior and situation for each participant (e.g., the analyst
responses defined above, the following paused the DVD player and turned away from
responses were also measured during this phase. Jack, or the analyst placed a bite of food on a
Nico’s initial FCR was a single-word mand plate visible to Will but out of his reach). A tar-
(e.g., “music,” “dance”) identified via the mand get FCR was selected and reinforced on a fixed-
analysis. His target FCR was a fully framed ratio (FR) 1 schedule in which each instance of
mand (e.g., “I want [item] please,” “More the FCR resulted in 30 s of reinforcement. All
[activity] please”). Will’s target FCR consisted problem behavior was placed on extinction.
of handing a food icon to the analyst. Jack’s A small number of presession training trials
target FCR consisted of a hand gesture to his (up to five) were conducted before introduction
558 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

of FCT. These trials included a brief instruc- Results


tional statement, modeling of the FCR, role- Figure 2 depicts the results of FCT. There
play of emitting the FCR, and accessing was a reduction in initial FCRs and variable
reinforcement and praise or correction of the rates of the target FCR observed with Nico,
FCR. During sessions, a most-to-least prompt- but after a period of variability, the target FCR
ing hierarchy was used to teach the target FCR was emitted exclusively and at an optimal rate.
until 80% of FCRs were independent, after FCT resulted in an immediate elimination of
which prompts were faded to a vocal prompt problem behavior for Will and the acquisition
every 60 to 90 s as needed. For Alex, when of the target FCR. Despite some variability in
problem behavior was eliminated in both con- problem behavior, FCT resulted in an eventual
texts and initial FCRs were emitted independ- elimination of problem behavior and acquisi-
ently for two consecutive sessions, the analyst tion of the FCR with Jack in both contexts.
increased the complexity of the response FCT resulted in an immediate reduction of
required via prompting and differential problem behavior for Alex and the acquisition
reinforcement. of the initial FCR in both contexts.

Figure 2. Results of the functional communication training plus extinction (FCT + EXT) for Nico, Will, Jack,
and Alex.
DELAY TOLERANCE 559

Independent and target FCRs eventually qualitative features of the task or the amount,
occurred at a higher rate than simple FCRs and vocally refusing or completing the task in a
to the exclusion of problem behavior in both manner different than what the analyst indi-
contexts. By the end of FCT, all participants cated, and making additional requests).
emitted the target FCR at an efficient rate, Alternative activity engagement was defined as
maximizing reinforcement to near-continuous actively manipulating, responding to
access, in the absence of problem behavior (e.g., dancing to music), or orienting towards
(Will, Jack, and Alex) or initial FCRs (Nico). materials (e.g., neutral toys, beads) as instructed
by the analyst without problem behavior or col-
lateral responses for Nico, Will, and Jack.
PART 3: COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS
Engagement was recorded using a 3-s onset–
OF TOLERANCE TRAINING
offset delay. For Alex, counts were collected on
Data Collection and Response Definitions each verbal and gestural instruction issued by
In addition to response rates defined above the adult and his compliance with each instruc-
(e.g., target problem behavior and FCRs), the tion. Compliance was defined as orienting
following responses and rates were also meas- towards the materials within 5 s of the instruc-
ured during the comparative analysis. The tol- tion and completing the task correctly without
erance response (TR) for Nico, Jack, and Alex any collateral responses or problem behavior
consisted of saying “okay” in an appropriate and without any need for a physical prompt
tone and volume within 5 s in response to the from the adult.
delay cue. TRs were considered prompted if Counts of participants’ communication and
the analyst prompted any aspect of the response tolerance responses, problem behavior, and dis-
within 10 s of the participant emitting the crete collateral responses (i.e., excessive man-
response. Only independent TRs are reported. ding, grabbing and motor disruptions, and
For all participants, an optimal rate of FCR attempts to control) were collected and con-
was calculated by dividing the number of evoc- verted to a rate for all analyses. Duration data
ative trials presented by the total duration of were collected on other collateral responses
that session. For Nico, an optimal rate of TR (e.g., crying), scheduled and experienced delays
was also calculated for each session by dividing (the interval of time between the delivery of
the total number of opportunities (i.e., the total the delay cue, e.g., “wait,” and the delivery of
number of delay trials) by the total duration of reinforcement), and engagement in the alterna-
that session. The optimal rates are depicted in tive activity during delays. The percentage of
each figure as a dotted data path. session engaged in negative emotional respond-
Nico’s collateral responses were in the form ing was calculated by dividing the duration of
of excessive mands (defined as any additional negative emotional responding by the session
requests during the delay that were different duration. The percentage of delay interval
than the target FCR that initiated the delay engaged in alternative activity was calculated by
interval). Will’s collateral responses included dividing the duration of alternative activity
motor disruptions (defined as throwing, swiping, engagement by the delay duration.
and pushing items) and grabbing others. Jack’s
collateral responses were in the form of negative
emotional responding (defined as crying, pout- General Procedure
ing, and saying “no”). Alex’s collateral After 1-min access to all reinforcers, every ses-
responses included attempts to control during sion started with the removal of all reinforcers
the delay (defined as negotiating to change the and the presentation of the participant-specific
560 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

evocative situation (e.g., taking away toys or terminal delay in an efficient manner and per-
tablet, removing attention, presenting writing haps to allow differences in the procedures to be
tasks). Sessions were as long as necessary to revealed more readily than with a less rapid pro-
allow five presentations of the evocative situa- gression. This geometric progression was used as
tions and all the scheduled delays and rein- a guide for increasing the scheduled delay. The
forcement periods, henceforth referred to as geometric progression was used until the target
trials. This resulted in session durations that terminal delay was reached, at which point the
ranged from 2.5 to 40 min depending on the scheduled delay was capped at that level. The
delay. During all delay conditions, FCRs were target terminal delay for each participant was
reinforced immediately on two of five ran- guided by caregiver and setting requirements.
domly selected trials. On the remaining three The participant-specific criteria to increase
trials, the FCR resulted in one of several brief delays are described in the specific procedures
verbal delay signals (e.g., “wait,” “not yet,” “in below. The experienced duration of each delay
a minute”), that were rotated within partici- in a CBPD session was determined based on the
pants, and reinforcement was provided after speed with which the participant completed the
either the scheduled amount of time (TBPD) response requirement and refrained from engag-
or the scheduled response requirement ing in problem behavior or collateral responses.
(CBPD). The specific response contingencies The experienced duration of each delay in a
for each participant during CBPD are sum- TBPD session, however, did not always pre-
marized in Table 1 and explained in detail cisely match this programmed delay due to
under specific procedures. The contingency (a) being yoked to the experienced duration of
and prompting procedures chosen in CBPD the delay in the CBPD session (Nico only),
were dictated by the parental goals and expec- (b) slight variations in the time required to reset
tations during the delay. the reinforcing materials (Will and Context 1 of
A geometric progression starting at 1 s Jack and Alex), or (c) termination of the delay
(i.e., 1, 2, 4, 8, …) was used to reach the due to problem behavior (Context 2 of Jack and

Table 1
Participant-Specific Prompts, Response Contingency, and Consequences During the Delays in the Comparative Analysis

Time-based
Contingency-based delay
delay
Consequences for
Participant Prompts Prompts Contingency PB and CR
Nico None Gestural and physical DRA-DRO: TR and continuous engagement in alternative Restart delay interval
every 30 to 45 s activity without PB or CR (say “okay” then play in area
with low-preference toys)
Will Single vocal Three-step every 15 s DRA: Cumulative number of compliance responses (place Block PB
all the provided beads on a string)
Single vocal DRO: Cumulative amount of time without PB or CR Block PB, pause,
(engage in any or no activity) and hold up timer
Jack Single vocal Single vocal DRA-DRO: TR and cumulative amount of time without Pause timer
PB or CR (say “okay” and engage in any or no activity)
Alex Three-step as Three-step as needed DRA-DRO: TR and cumulative number of compliances Continue demands
needed without PB or CR (say “okay” and independently comply
with a mixture of adult-directed academic and toy-based
demands)

Note. PB = problem behavior, CR = collateral responses, DRA = DRA-based contingency delay, DRO = DRO-based
contingency delay, TR = tolerance response, Three-step = vocal, model, physical prompting.
DELAY TOLERANCE 561

Alex). Reinforcement intervals were increased delay signal and either a single prompt (Jack,
from 30 s to 120 s (30 s for delays of 0 to 32 s, Will in DRO) or multiple prompts (Nico, Will
60 s for delays of 64 to 128 s, and 120 s for in DRA, and Alex) to engage in the alternative
delays of 256 to 600 s). activity or comply with demands. The partici-
No-delay baseline. These sessions were identi- pant was required to emit the TR or either
cal to the final sessions of FCT. Reinforcement engage in additional specific responses or
was withheld until the target communication refrain from engaging in problem behavior or
response was emitted. In all trials, the FCR was collateral responses to terminate the delay. In
immediately reinforced and neither problem other words, reinforcement was withheld until
behavior nor collateral responses resulted in the participant completed the response
programmed consequences. requirements.
Time-based progressive delay (TBPD). On the
three delay trials, the FCR resulted in a delay
signal and either no additional prompts (Nico), Specific Procedures
a single prompt (Will and Jack), or multiple Nico. The relative efficacy of CBPD and
prompts (Alex) to engage in the alternative TBPD was evaluated with Nico in a multiele-
activity or comply with demands. Although the ment design. TR training was conducted before
alternative activity or instructional materials the start of the comparative analysis. Through-
were present and freely available during these out the comparative analysis, the two condi-
sessions, there was no requirement for the par- tions were presented as a dyad in which the
ticipant to engage these materials or independ- first condition to be presented in each dyad
ently comply with demands (i.e., the delay was randomly selected. During both delay con-
ended based on time alone). At the end of the ditions, the highly preferred toys were placed
scheduled delay, the reinforcers were delivered on a table where Nico and the analyst sat, and
with a verbal statement (e.g., “Now you can the neutral toys used as the alternative activity
have —,” “Here you go”). Problem and collat- were placed on foam mats. Green (TBPD) and
eral behavior resulted in no programmed conse- red (CBPD) plastic sheets on the wall and the
quences throughout the session. table of the session room were correlated with
Tolerance response (TR) training. TR training each condition. In addition, the positions of
was conducted before the start of CBPD for the table and foam mats were flipped during
Nico, Jack, and Alex. Training sessions of TBPD and CBPD.
10 trials, 60% of which were delay trials, were During TBPD, FCRs were reinforced imme-
used to teach a specific TR (“okay”) to the diately on two of five randomly selected trials
adult’s delay cues. A minimum of two sessions by providing access to requested toys and atten-
with 80% independent FCRs and TRs were tion. On the remaining three trials, the FCR
conducted before the start of CBPD. The train- resulted in a brief verbal delay signal
ing sessions started with a brief instructional (e.g., “wait”), and access to the highly preferred
statement, modeling of the FCR followed by toys and attention was withheld until the
the delay cue and the TR, role-play of emitting scheduled amount of time had elapsed. Prob-
both the FCR and the TR to access reinforce- lem behavior resulted in no programmed conse-
ment, and ended with praise or any necessary quences throughout the session. CBPD was
corrections. A most-to-least prompting proce- similar to TBPD except that DRA-DRO was
dure was used during each trial. used to terminate the delay. After the delay sig-
Contingency-based progressive delay (CBPD). nal, Nico was required to say “okay” and then
On the three delay trials, the FCR resulted in a play with the less preferred or neutral toys on
562 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

the foam mats for a target amount of time. freely available throughout all sessions. TBPD
Access to the highly preferred toys and atten- was introduced first. During TBPD, FCRs
tion was provided only after continuous were reinforced immediately on two of five ran-
engagement with the alternative activity (neu- domly selected trials by providing access to a
tral toys) for the target amount of time. The bite of food. On the remaining three trials, the
delay was restarted if Nico stopped engaging in FCR resulted in a brief verbal delay signal
the alternative activity or if he engaged in prob- (e.g., “in a minute”) and a single vocal prompt
lem behavior or collateral responses. Gestural (“you can play or bead if you want”) to high-
and physical prompts to engage in the alterna- light the option of engaging with the neutral
tive activity were provided every 30 to 45 s if activities (i.e., there was no requirement to
he was not doing so. engage with these items). Access to food was
Both conditions progressed through the withheld until the scheduled amount of time
delay levels according to the following schedule: had elapsed. Problem behavior was blocked but
one session at each of the first three delays resulted in no other programmed consequences
(approximately 1, 2, and 4 s), two sessions at throughout the session.
each of the next three delays (approximately After a return to the no-delay baseline,
8, 16, and 32 s), and one session at a delay of CBPD was introduced. During CBPD, two of
64 s, irrespective of problem behavior or collat- five randomly selected trials included immedi-
eral responses. The exact duration of each delay ate reinforcement of FCRs, and the remaining
within a session in TBPD was yoked to the three trials included a delay. In this condition,
experienced duration of the delay in the CBPD however, after the delay signal, Will was
session. The comparative analysis was con- prompted to engage in a beading task (“first
cluded at the delay of 64 s, but CBPD was put the beads on”), and access to food was pro-
used to increase the delay to 128 s, after which vided after completion of a predetermined
the treatment was extended to Nico’s father number of beads that corresponded to the tar-
and then his mother at the terminal delay get delay. Vocal and model prompts to engage
of 256 s. in the beading task were provided every 15 s if
Will. We systematically replicated the com- he was not doing so. Attempts at self-injury
parison between CBPD and TBPD using a were blocked but resulted in no other pro-
slightly modified contingency in an ABAC grammed consequences. In both conditions,
design to allow a more independent evaluation delays were increased after one session with no
of the presence of a response contingency dur- problem behavior and collateral responses or
ing delay. For Will, the delay response contin- after two sessions if there were any instances of
gency during CBPD was modified to require these behaviors. The first comparative analysis
completion of a cumulative, rather than a con- was concluded at a delay of 180 s.
secutive, number of beading tasks (i.e., the con- Among other factors, the pace of the pro-
tingency was not reset if Will stopped engaging gression may have contributed to the persist-
in the activity). The terminal delay duration ence of Will’s problem behavior and collateral
was set at 180 s or the placement of roughly responses during the first comparative analysis.
10 beads on the string. A second analysis that used a multielement
During both delay conditions, the edible design was conducted to evaluate the effects of
items were placed on a table where Will and two versions of CBPD, DRA only and DRO
the analyst sat, and various toys and beading only, simultaneously against TBPD with a
materials were also placed on the table in front slower programmed progression of the delay.
of Will. The toys and beading materials were The three conditions were presented in a
DELAY TOLERANCE 563

random and counterbalanced order. The condi- the corner of the room for Jack or the instruc-
tions were signaled using color-correlated tional materials were placed on the table for
stimuli (tablecloths, plates, and beads). The Alex. The direct effects of TBPD and CBPD
time-based condition was signaled with yellow were evaluated in Context 1, and general effects
stimuli and was identical to the previous TBPD were evaluated in Context 2 using terminal
condition. The DRA condition was signaled delay probes (described below). TBPD was
with red stimuli and was identical to the previ- introduced first followed by the no-delay base-
ous CBPD condition. The DRO condition was line, TR training, and finally CBPD. During
signaled with blue stimuli and was similar to both TBPD and CBPD conditions in Context
the CBPD condition described above, except 1, terminal delay baseline probes were con-
that the response contingency was further mod- ducted on every fifth session in Context
ified to consist of the absence of problem 2. Finally, CBPD was implemented in Con-
behavior and collateral responses for a cumula- text 2.
tive amount of time. A single vocal prompt Terminal delay probes (generality test). This
(“you can play or bead if you want”) to high- condition was arranged with Jack and Alex to
light the option of engaging with the neutral evaluate the extent to which treatment effects
activities (i.e., there was no requirement to would generalize to a context in which prob-
engage with these items) was provided after the lem behavior during the delay was reinforced
delay cue. No additional vocal prompts were (i.e., no extinction during the delay). Context
used to direct Will to play or bead, but in addi- 2 (the analyst context for Jack and the dino-
tion to blocking self-injury, the analyst held up saur context for Alex) was designated as the
the timer and paused if Will engaged in any generalization context. The terminal delays of
problem behavior or collateral responding. This approximately 5 min for Jack and 10 min for
comparison was started at a 64-s delay, which Alex were used during these probes. All prob-
was the point at which problem behavior and lem behavior before the emission of FCR was
collateral responses emerged during the previ- placed on extinction. However, any instances
ous analysis. When stable and desirable trends of problem behavior after the delivery of the
were observed in one condition, the delay was denial cue terminated the delay and resulted
increased to 90 s, 120 s, and finally 180 s. in the immediate delivery of the reinforcers.
Jack and Alex. Although the multielement The alternative activity or demands were
designs provided a clear demonstration of the available throughout this condition; there
relative efficacy of each condition, there was was, however, no engagement or compliance
some apparent carryover across conditions requirement. If no problem behavior was
(e.g., the TR generalized to the TBPD context emitted during the delay, the reinforcers were
with Nico). Therefore, to isolate the effects of delivered at the end of the scheduled terminal
each condition better, a multiple baseline delay. This condition served as a rigorous test
design across participants was used with Jack of the generality of delay treatments, given
and Alex to evaluate the direct effects of a that direct reinforcement of problem behavior
response contingency during the delay, while was programmed.
the general effects were demonstrated in the Jack. The general TBPD and CBPD proce-
secondary context with each participant. dures described above were implemented with
During both CBPD and TBPD, highly pre- Jack. The terminal delay was set at 256 s. The
ferred toys were placed on a table where the delay response contingency used during CBPD
children and the adult sat, and the neutral toys included emitting the tolerance response and
used as the alternative activity were placed in engagement in any activity without engaging in
564 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

problem behavior or collateral responses facing the therapist or the demand materials.
(i.e., both a DRA and a DRO contingency) for Starting at the 4-s delay, a demand was added
a cumulative amount of time. to this response chain. The number of demands
During both conditions, after the delay cue, was then increased using a geometric progres-
a single vocal prompt to engage with the alter- sion (1, 2, 4, …). During the delay, demands
native activity (i.e., “play with [activity] if you continued until Alex complied with the target
want”) was issued. No additional prompts were number of demands. In both conditions, delay
used throughout the delay. During CBPD, Jack levels were increased after one session with no
was required to say “okay” in response to the problem behavior or collateral responses, or
delay signal and access to the high-preference after two sessions if there were any instances of
toys and attention was withheld until he met these behaviors. The comparative analysis was
the target cumulative amount of time not concluded at the delay of 64 s or 16 demands.
engaging in any problem behavior or collateral We then merged both contexts into one and
responses. During both TBPD and CBPD, further extended the delay and demand require-
delay levels were increased after one session if ments to 32 demands. The response contin-
no problem behavior or collateral responses gency during the delay was then changed from
occurred, and after two to four sessions if any compliance with a cumulative number of
of these responses occurred. The comparative demands to a consecutive number. To signal
analysis was concluded at the delay of 256 s, this change in the contingency, tokens (check-
after which CBPD was also implemented in marks) were introduced. Alex initially earned a
Context 2 and also with a new adult (Jack’s checkmark for each demand he completed and
father). lost all checkmarks earned in each section if he
Alex. The general TBPD and CBPD proce- engaged in any problem behavior or collateral
dures described above were implemented with responses. Finally, the demand requirement
Alex. The terminal delay was set at 600 s or was changed to a variable ratio of 50 demands.
roughly 50 age-appropriate demands. Given Checkmarks were earned for an average of
that problem behavior was at least partly main- three demands completed, and a total of
tained by escape from demands, the delay 16 checkmarks were required to earn 120 s of
response contingency used during CBPD reinforcement (i.e., an average of 24 demands
included emitting the tolerance response and needed to be completed in a row without any
compliance with a fixed number of adult problem behavior or collateral responses to earn
instructions without engaging in problem reinforcement time).
behavior or attempts to control (i.e., both a
DRA and a DRO contingency).
During both conditions, following the delay Results
cue (“not right now” or “wait”), demands Nico. The no-delay baseline showed that tar-
(e.g., “write J,” “Color the bird blue”) were pre- get FCRs occurred at an optimal rate, problem
sented and three-step prompting (vocal, model, behavior was at zero, and no TRs occurred
full physical) was used to ensure compliance (Figure 3). After TR training and with the
with demands. During CBPD, after the delay introduction of progressive delay, target FCRs
signal, Alex was required to say “okay” and slowly decreased but remained near the optimal
comply with a cumulative number of adult rate in both conditions. The TR was observed
instructions without engaging in any problem in both conditions; however, they occurred at
behavior or collateral responses. At the begin- an optimal rate in CBPD whereas excessive
ning, he was required to say “okay” and sit amounts were emitted during TBPD. After a
DELAY TOLERANCE 565

Will. During the no-delay baseline in Will’s


initial analysis (Figure 4), target FCRs occurred
at an optimal rate, and problem behavior
(i.e., SIB) and collateral responses were at zero
or near-zero levels. FCRs decreased but main-
tained at an optimal rate, and no engagement
in the alternative activity was observed during
TBPD. Problem behavior remained low ini-
tially; however, as the delays increased, collat-
eral responses such as grabbing others and
swiping materials emerged and maintained and
SIB resurged. The return to the no-delay base-
line resulted in an immediate reduction of SIB
and collateral responses, and FCRs persisted.
The introduction of CBPD resulted in a grad-
ual reduction of FCRs toward an optimal rate,
high and variable engagement in the alternative
activity, and zero levels of SIB and collateral
responses, but these latter behaviors resurged as
the demand requirements were increased. Due
primarily to the resurgence of SIB and collateral
responses as the delays were increased, neither
CBPD nor TBPD was effective in developing
tolerance for delayed reinforcement with Will.
Will’s limited fine-motor repertoire and
independent play skills may have contributed
Figure 3. Results of the time-based versus to the moderate level of engagement in the
contingency-based comparative analysis for Nico. alternative activity, which in turn may have
contributed to the resurgence of problem
few sessions of CBPD, Nico spent approxi- behavior. He also may have required a slower
mately 80% of the delay engaging in the alter- progression of the response requirement during
native activity (third panel). Engagement in the the delay to allow him to acquire the beading
alternative activity did not occur for several ses- skills relevant to the alternative activity. These
sions in TBPD and then never exceeded 75%. considerations informed the second analysis,
Collateral responses and problem behavior were the results of which are depicted in Figure 5.
highly variable but occurred almost exclusively The optimal number of FCRs per reinforcer in
in TBPD, despite the fact that the experienced each condition was one; this was obtained dur-
delays were similar across CBPD and TBPD. ing most DRA sessions. By contrast, Will
Overall, it appeared that CBPD was more emitted twice that many FCRs during TBPD
effective than TBPD at increasing Nico’s toler- and DRO. Alternative activity engagement was
ance for delayed reinforcement. CBPD treat- exclusively observed during DRA. Both SIB
ment effects maintained as the delay was and collateral responses occurred at higher rates
extended to an average of 5 min and treatment during TBPD and DRO than during DRA.
was implemented by Nico’s parents (data avail- These patterns persisted as the delay increased
able from the second author). to 180 s. Overall, CBPD using a DRA
566 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

Figure 4. Results of the time-based versus contingency-based comparative analysis for Will.

contingency was more effective for increasing observed as the delay was increased to 256 s,
Will’s tolerance for delayed reinforcement than target FCRs were emitted at a higher than opti-
a time-based or DRO-based contingency. mal rate during the majority of TBPD sessions.
Jack. During the no-delay baseline in both Problem behavior occurred at high and variable
contexts, FCRs occurred at an optimal rate, rates throughout TBPD, and collateral
and problem behavior and collateral responses responses (e.g., crying) increased as the delay
were at zero (Figure 6). With TBPD in Con- was increased. Problem behavior also remained
text 1, there was an increase in the rate of at strength in Context 2. Overall, TBPD did
FCRs with a spike at the 16-s delay. Although not produce tolerance for delayed reinforce-
some engagement in the alternative activity was ment. The return to the no-delay baseline in
DELAY TOLERANCE 567

Figure 5. Results of the comparison between time-based versus DRO-based versus DRA-based delay for Will. The
scheduled delay increased at each phase line.

both contexts resulted in the elimination of consistently experienced a shorter delay than
problem behavior and collateral responses, and that scheduled in Context 2 because he termi-
optimal rates of FCRs. nated the delay through problem behavior.
After TR training, the introduction of Despite the presence of the same “inappropri-
CBPD in Context 1 resulted in near-optimal ate” contingency in Context 2, Jack tolerated
rates of FCRs and TRs, low but persistent the scheduled delay when CBPD was pro-
engagement in the alternative activity, and con- grammed in Context 1, even though the delays
tinued zero rates of problem behavior and col- in Context 2 could have been terminated at
lateral responses as the delay was increased to any point by engaging in problem behavior.
5 min. The DRA-DRO contingency during The TR of “okay” as well as other appropriate
delay for Jack required him to emit the TR and play responses generalized, and presumably as a
then refrain from engaging in any problem result, lower rates of problem behavior and
behavior or collateral responses for the required lower rates of FCR were observed in this sec-
amount of time (i.e., there was no requirement ond context. There were, however, some resid-
to engage with the alternative activity). In addi- ual collateral responses; these were eliminated
tion, the data from Context 2 provide evidence after implementation of CBPD in Context
of the generality of CBPD training. While Jack 2. In summary, CBPD was an effective treat-
experienced TBPD in Context 1, he ment for increasing Jack’s tolerance for delayed
568 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

Figure 6. Results of the time-based versus contingency-based comparative analysis for Jack.

reinforcement. These treatment effects main- Alex. During the no-delay baseline in both
tained when treatment was extended and contexts, FCRs occurred at an optimal rate,
implemented by Jack’s father (data available and problem behavior and collateral responses
from the second author). were at zero (Figure 7). With the introduction
DELAY TOLERANCE 569

of TBPD in Context 1, there was a gradual CBPD increased alternative activity engage-
decrease in the rate of FCRs corresponding to ment and compliance while it maintained zero
the optimal rate, but compliance with demands or near-zero rates of problem behavior and col-
remained at zero throughout this condition, lateral responses and optimal rates of communi-
and there was an immediate increase in the rate cation. Our finding that TBPD is an ineffective
of problem behavior with a spike at the delay method for increasing the generality of FCT
of 16 s, at which point TBPD was terminated. treatment is consistent with previous research
Collateral responses also increased starting at (Fisher et al., 2000; Hagopian et al., 1998;
the delay of 4 s. Overall TBPD was not an Hanley et al., 2001). In fact, despite the various
effective treatment for increasing tolerance for procedural improvements to the manner in
delayed reinforcement with Alex. The return to which TBPD is usually programmed, TBPD
the no-delay baseline in both contexts resulted was still found to be ineffective in our study.
in the elimination of problem behavior, and For example, although, the addition of probabi-
FCRs persisted. After TR training, the intro- listic reinforcement appeared to result in the
duction of CBPD in Context 1 resulted in maintenance of the communication response
occurrences of the TR, a gradual reduction of during TBPD, problem behavior resurged in all
FCRs toward an optimal rate, high but variable cases and within the first 16 s of delay for three
levels of compliance during delays, and near- of four cases. Although the recovery of problem
zero rates of problem behavior throughout. In behavior during this delayed reinforcement pro-
addition, the data from Context 2 provide evi- cedure is likely due to resurgence, as suggested
dence of the generality of CBPD training. by Lieving and Lattal (2003) and Volkert
While Alex experienced TBPD in Context et al. (2009), we did not arrange for the neces-
1, he complied with no demands in Context sary controls to label this recovery as resurgence
2 and terminated the delay through problem with confidence instead of other extinction-
behavior. By contrast, while Alex experienced related phenomena (Bruzek, Thompson, &
CBPD in Context 1 and despite the presence Peters, 2009). Furthermore, the mere presence
of the same “inappropriate” contingency in of an alternative activity and prompts to engage
Context 2, he emitted the TR and complied in these activities or comply with demands
with almost half of the demands presented in were not sufficient to mitigate the negative side
Context 2 before he engaged in problem behav- effects of TBPD. Therefore, it appears that the
ior to terminate the delay. When CBPD was response contingency during the delay is the
introduced in Context 2, high and stable levels necessary component for the effectiveness of
of compliance were achieved, FCRs and TRs progressive delay-tolerance training. Our find-
persisted, and problem behavior and collateral ing, that the presence of a contingency in addi-
responses occurred at zero or near-zero levels. tion to the alternative activity during the delay
By the end of treatment, Alex engaged in is important for achieving delay tolerance, is
approximately 50 demands and experienced consistent with the findings from translational
delays to reinforcement of approximately research on self-control by Mischel, Ebbesen,
10 min with CBPD. and Raskoff Zeiss (1972), Dixon and Cum-
mings (2001), and Dixon, Rehfeldt, and Ran-
dich (2003). For example, Dixon and
GENERAL DISCUSSION Cummings have shown that requiring partici-
Contingency-based delays were more effec- pants to engage in an alternative response dur-
tive than time-based delays in developing parti- ing delay aids in shifting preference from the
cipants’ tolerance for delayed reinforcement. smaller immediate reinforcer to the larger
570 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

Figure 7. Results of the time-based versus contingency-based comparative analysis for Alex.
DELAY TOLERANCE 571

delayed reinforcer while lower levels of problem treatment. Another variable that may play a
behavior are maintained. role in the persistence of treatment effects is
The effects of CBPD were systematically the manner in which antecedents and conse-
replicated across participants aged 21 months quences are arranged during treatment. Our
to 30 years old, with and without developmen- findings suggest that alterations to the design of
tal disabilities or autism, and across both treatment such as those included in CBPD
social-positive and social-negative reinforcement may be an efficient means of obtaining similar
contexts, replicating the results of Hanley resistance in light of changing contexts.
et al. (2014). In addition to the elimination of Although FCT, by the nature of its design,
problem behavior during treatment, CBPD exposes the individual to the natural maintain-
resulted in the acquisition of an appropriate ing contingencies of the response, exposing the
response to a delay signal (i.e., the TR of individual to sufficient exemplars of antecedent
“okay”) and a set of developmentally appropri- conditions may also be important to ensure
ate responses (e.g., compliance with academic generalized responding (Tiger et al., 2008).
demands, functional play skills) that also gener- Rather than relying on the use of a single,
alized to a context in which extinction was not tightly controlled context with a specific task
fully in place. In other words, with CBPD we and clear discriminative stimuli for the delay,
were able to shape a repertoire of “waiting” that we used multiple exemplars of delay cues to sig-
generalized to contexts with an inappropriate nal the onset of delay, a variable array of activ-
contingency (i.e., availability of reinforcement ities and tasks based on child-selected items
for problem behavior during delay). This shap- from the preference assessment, and adult-
ing of a waiting repertoire was systematically selected demand items that changed every two
replicated across participants with varying to four sessions. Within 8 hr of treatment, for
degrees of baseline language, adaptive, and lei- example, Alex was able to engage in appropriate
sure skills. Desirable patterns of behavior dur- communication and tolerance responses in the
ing extended delays were produced for all presence of a variety of evocative situations
participants without the need for positive pun- (e.g., interruption of drawing activity, removal
ishment (Fisher et al., 1993; Hagopian et al., of toys, denial of a request) and to tolerate
1998) or additional noncontingent or differen- delays of approximately 10 to 15 min and
tial reinforcement procedures (Hagopian, Con- complete roughly 50 demands that involved
trucci Kuhn, Long, & Rush, 2005; Rooker various academic and toy-based activities.
et al., 2013) during the delay. The specific response requirements during
The relative speed with which these treat- the delay were also closely matched to the
ment effects were obtained (2 to 8 hr distribu- behavioral expectations regularly experienced
ted across 4 to 24 weeks) suggests that CBPD by the participants. The selection of the alter-
may be a desirable alternative to long-term native activities and the most appropriate
FCT treatment necessary for persistence of prompting procedures, in addition to the spe-
effects during extinction (Wacker et al., 2011). cific evocative contexts and reinforcers, were
Wacker et al. (2011) noted that the antecedents guided by the results of the open-ended inter-
and consequences surrounding the response in views with caregivers. For example, Jack’s
the natural environment often vary from the mother reported that a common situation
specific conditions used during treatment. They involved her preparing dinner and requiring
suggest that variables that enhance treatment Jack to stay away from the stove and do “some-
persistence such as extensive experience with thing else,” with very little supervision or atten-
FCT (nearly 16 months) should be included in tion. Given this context and Jack’s lack of an
572 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

independent play repertoire, a DRO contin- (Hagopian et al., 2011; Hanley et al., 2001).
gency seemed the most suitable. It allowed Jack In addition, obtaining stimulus control over
to engage in a variety of other responses with- the occurrence of FCR can be difficult, some-
out any prompting from his mother and still times resulting in high rates of FCR during the
satisfied the mother’s request that he stay away extinction component and some recovery of
from her cooking area for a few minutes. These problem behavior as the extinction component
considerations increased the ecological validity is increased (see Hanley et al., 2001, for exam-
of treatment, which may further enhance the ples). Given the current state of evidence, a
maintenance of treatment effects. We also used direct comparison of multiple schedules and
delay cues that were commonly presented in CBPD is warranted. In particular, it is impor-
the natural environment (i.e., “wait,” “in a tant to evaluate the extent to which caregivers
minute,” “not right now”) to increase the simi- are able to maintain treatment integrity with
larity of the training context and the context each procedure and whether they prefer one
typically experienced by the participant. over the other. The recipient’s preference for
Finally, for two participants, treatment was these procedures should also be directly evalu-
sequentially implemented in a second context, ated and considered.
and for all participants, treatment was extended Chained schedules have historically been
to the relevant context and training was pro- referred to as demand fading and have been
vided to the caregivers who would be responsi- used to treat negatively reinforced problem
ble for treatment maintenance. behavior (Hagopian et al., 2011; Lalli et al.,
Although other procedures have been shown 1995). Although supplemental strategies
to maintain low levels of problem behavior dur- (including punishment) have often been neces-
ing planned delays of practical duration, these sary to achieve the desired outcomes with
effective procedures also rely on strong contin- demand fading (Hagopian et al., 1998), more
gencies. For example, Luczynski and Hanley recent evaluations by Falcomata et al. (2013)
(2014) showed that the strong positive contin- and Falcomata, Roane, Muething, Stephenson,
gencies within multiple schedules were respon- and Ing (2012) have been conducted in which
sible for their efficacy. Multiple schedules are various elements of both multiple schedules
often used in the treatment of positively rein- (the discriminative stimuli) and chained sche-
forced problem behavior (Hagopian et al., dules (the contingency-based alternation of the
2011), whereas chained schedules are often component change) were used to treat problem
used to treat escape-motivated problem behav- behavior maintained by a synthesis of both pos-
ior (e.g., Hagopian et al., 1998). Multiple sche- itive and negative reinforcement. The contin-
dules have been shown to retain zero or low gency arranged in the traditional chained
levels of problem behavior and sufficient levels schedules is somewhat different than the
of communication even when nonreinforce- arrangement used in CBPD. Chained schedules
ment periods are scheduled for up to 80% of used by Hagopian et al. (1998), Lalli
the observation period (Betz, Fisher, Roane, et al. (1995), and Falcomata et al. (2013) can
Mintz, & Owen, 2013; Fisher et al., 1998; be represented as an FR x FR 1 schedule, in
Hagopian et al., 2011; Hanley et al., 2001). which a certain number of demands are com-
Multiple schedules, however, have often been pleted, after which the communication
programmed using somewhat artificial stimuli response is reinforced immediately. CBPD, by
(but see Kuhn, Chirighin, & Zelenka, 2010) contrast, can be represented as an FR 1 FR
such as different-colored cards that must be x schedule, in which the communication
programmed in the natural environment response is followed by a chain of responses
DELAY TOLERANCE 573

that are progressively increased to accommodate during delays. Finally, the efficacy of a DRA-
the length of delay necessary, including based contingency using momentary and spo-
unplanned delays that may naturally occur. radic monitoring remains to be assessed.
This arrangement also allows the recursive Some additional questions arise from the
implementation of treatment. This is impor- manner in which CBPD was programmed in
tant, given the continuous nature of interac- this study. One question concerns the predicta-
tions between individuals and their caregivers. bility of the delay. Predictability can be defined
For example, a child may request a break from in various ways. One aspect is related to the rel-
work and be told to wait and finish his home- ative proportion of delayed and immediate
work first. After he is done, the child is pro- reinforcement for FCRs. A second aspect relates
vided with a break with his toys. During this to the extent to which the duration of each
break, however, the child may ask that his delay requirement is fixed or variable. Predicta-
mother play along with him. At this point, the bility may also involve cues that inform the
mother can again repeat the treatment proce- participant of the ensuing delay requirement
dure and ask the child to wait and play alone (e.g., contingency-specifying statements or vis-
while she finishes her cooking. When the ual cues such as token boards). Future research
mother joins the play, however, the child may should examine the impact of predictable ver-
make another request for a drink, which the sus unpredictable delay termination
mother may immediately reinforce. In this way, requirements.
CBPD can be practiced continuously because it The main advantage of CBPD lies in its abil-
has a natural fit with common situations. ity to create desirable patterns of behavior while
The CBPD procedure, as described in this it emulates situations that involve unplanned
study, is not without its limitations. Some par- delays and in its ability to yield generalizable
ticipants’ performance, in particular when patterns of behavior that appear to protect indi-
demands were presented, required monitoring viduals from mismanaged contingencies (see
during the delay. For example, Will required also Luczynski & Hanley, 2013). Future inves-
intermittent prompting to continue beading. tigations into the procedural variations that
Alex’s treatment included discrete presentation may enhance the efficacy of this treatment, its
of demands and three-step prompting (instruc- generality, and its social validity are still
tion, model, physical). The need for continu- warranted.
ous monitoring may present a barrier to
implementation when caregivers are busy with
other tasks or other individuals. One possible REFERENCES
extension of this research could involve evaluat-
Betz, A. M., Fisher, W. W., Roane, H. S.,
ing the use of product monitoring as the crite- Mintz, J. C., & Owen, T. M. (2013). A component
rion for the contingent delivery of analysis of schedule thinning during functional com-
reinforcement. Another strategy that could munication training. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 46, 219–241. doi: 10.1002/jaba.23
improve the practicality of CBPD involves the
Bowman, L. G., Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., &
addition of self-monitoring of performance Piazza, C. C. (1997). On the relation of mands and
(Connell, Carta, & Baer, 1993). Individuals the function of destructive behavior. Journal of
could be taught to self-assess and to recruit Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 251–265. doi:
reinforcement when a task is complete. This 10.1901/jaba.1997.30-251
strategy could reduce the amount of monitor- Bruzek, J. L., Thompson, R. H., & Peters, L. C. (2009).
Resurgence of infant caregiving responses. Journal of
ing that caregivers must provide and increase the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 92, 327–343.
an individual’s independent task engagement doi: 10.1901/jeab.2009-92-327
574 MAHSHID GHAEMMAGHAMI et al.

Carr, E. G., & Carlson, J. I. (1993). Reduction of severe punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26,
behavior problems in the community using a multi- 23–36. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1993.26-23
component treatment approach. Journal of Applied Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., Hagopian, L. P.,
Behavior Analysis, 26, 157–172. doi: 10.1901/ Bowman, L. G., & Krug, A. (2000). Facilitating tol-
jaba.1993.26-157 erance of delayed reinforcement during functional
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior communication training. Behavior Modification, 24,
problems through functional communication train- 3–29. doi: 10.1177/0145445500241001
ing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, Ghaemmaghami, M., Hanley, G. P., Jin, S. C., &
111–126. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1985.18-111 Vanselow, N. R. (2016). Affirming control by multi-
Connell, M. C., Carta, J. J., & Baer, D. M. (1993). Pro- ple reinforcers via progressive treatment analysis.
gramming generalization of in-class transition skills: Behavioral Interventions, 31, 70–86. doi: 10.1002/
Teaching preschoolers with developmental delays to bin.1425
self-assess and recruit contingent teacher praise. Jour- Hagopian, L. P., Boelter, E. W., & Jarmolowicz, D. P.
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 345–352. doi: (2011). Reinforcement schedule thinning following
10.1901/jaba.1993.26-345 functional communication training: Review and
DeLeon, I. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Evaluation of a recommendations. Behavior Analysis in Practice,
multiple-stimulus presentation format for assessing 4, 4–16.
reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Hagopian, L. P., Contrucci Kuhn, S. A., Long, E. S., &
Analysis, 29, 519–533. doi: 10.1901/jaba. Rush, K. S. (2005). Schedule thinning following
1996.29-519 communication training: Using competing stimuli to
Dixon, M. R., & Cummings, A. (2001). Self-control in enhance tolerance to decrements in reinforcer density.
children with autism: Response allocation during Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 177–193.
delays to reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior doi: 10.1901/jaba.2005.43-04
Analysis, 34, 491–495. doi: 10.1901/ Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T.,
jaba.2001.34-491 Acquisto, J., & LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness
Dixon, M. R., Rehfeldt, R. A., & Randich, L. (2003). of functional communication training with and with-
Enhancing tolerance to delayed reinforcers: The role out extinction and punishment: A summary of
of intervening activities. Journal of Applied Behavior 21 inpatient cases. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 36, 263–266. doi: 10.1901/ Analysis, 31, 211–235. doi: 10.1901/
jaba.2003.36-263 jaba.1998.31-211
Durand, V. M., & Moskowitz, L. (2015). Functional Hammond, L. J. (1980). The effect of contingency upon
communication training: Thirty years of treating the appetitive conditioning of free-operant behavior.
challenging behavior. Topics in Early Childhood Spe- Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34,
cial Education, 35, 116–126. doi: 10.1177/ 297–304. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1980.34-297
0271121415569509 Hanley, G. P. (2012). Functional assessment of problem
Falcomata, T. S., Muething, C. S., Gainey, S., behavior: Dispelling myths, overcoming implementa-
Hoffman, K., & Fragale, C. (2013). Further evalua- tion obstacles, and developing new lore. Behavior
tions of functional communication training and Analysis in Practice, 5, 54–72.
chained schedules of reinforcement to treat multiple Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & Thompson, R. H. (2001).
functions of challenging behavior. Behavior Reinforcement schedule thinning following treatment
Modification, 37, 723–746. doi: 10.1177/ with functional communication training. Journal of
0145445513500785 Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 17–38. doi: 10.1901/
Falcomata, T. S., Roane, H. S., Muething, C. S., jaba.2001.34-17
Stephenson, K. M., & Ing, A. D. (2012). Functional Hanley, G. P., Jin, C. S., Vanselow, N. R., &
communication training and chained schedules of Hanratty, L. A. (2014). Producing meaningful
reinforcement to treat challenging behavior main- improvements in problem behavior of children with
tained by terminations of activity interruptions. autism via synthesized analyses and treatments. Jour-
Behavior Modification, 36, 630–649. doi: 10.1177/ nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, 16–36. doi:
0145445511433821 10.1002/jaba.106
Fisher, W. W., Kuhn, D. E., & Thompson, R. H. Hernandez, E., Hanley, G. P., Ingvarsson, E. T., &
(1998). Establishing discriminative control of Tiger, J. H. (2007). A preliminary evaluation of the
responding using functional and alternative reinfor- emergence of novel mand forms. Journal of Applied
cers during functional communication training. Jour- Behavior Analysis, 40, 137–156. doi:10.1901/
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 543–560. doi: jaba.2007.96-05
10.1901/jaba.1998.31-543 Jessel, J., Hanley, G. P., & Ghaemmaghami, M. (2016).
Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., Defining and improving the efficiency and control of
Jefferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional com- a functional analysis of a problem behavior. Journal of
munication training with and without extinction and Applied Behavior Analysis.
DELAY TOLERANCE 575

Kuhn, D. E., Chirighin, A. E., & Zelenka, K. (2010). reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Discriminated functional communication: A proce- 47, 500–522. doi: 10.1002/jaba.140
dural extension of functional communication Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Raskoff Zeiss, A. (1972).
training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of
249–264. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2010.43-249 gratification. Journal of Personality and Social
Kurtz, P. F., Boelter, E. W., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Psychology, 21, 204–218. doi: 10.1037/h0032198
Chin, M. D., & Hagopian, L. P. (2011). An analysis Rooker, G. W., Jessel, J., Kurtz, P. F., & Hagopian, L. P.
of functional communication training as an empiri- (2013). Functional communication training with and
cally supported treatment for problem behavior dis- without alternative reinforcement and punishment:
played by individuals with intellectual disabilities. An analysis of 58 applications. Journal of Applied
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, Behavior Analysis, 46, 708–722. doi: 10.1002/jaba.76
2935–2942. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.009 Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Func-
Lalli, J. S., Casey, S., & Kates, K. (1995). Reducing tional communication training: A review and practi-
escape behavior and increasing task completion with cal guide. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 16–23.
functional communication training, extinction, and
Volkert, V. M., Lerman, D. C., Call, N. A., & Trosclair-
response chaining. Journal of Applied Behavior
Lasserre, N. (2009). An evaluation of resurgence
Analysis, 28, 261–268. doi: 10.1901/
during treatment with functional communication
jaba.1995.28-261
training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42,
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Developing a
145–160. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-145
technology for the use of operant extinction in clini-
cal settings: An examination of basic and applied Vollmer, T. R., Borrero, J. C., Lalli, J. S., & Daniel, D.
research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, (1999). Evaluating self-control and impulsivity in
345–382. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1996.29-345 children with severe behavior disorders. Journal of
Lieving, G. A., & Lattal, K. A. (2003). Recency, repeata- Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 451–466. doi:
bility, and reinforcer retrenchment: An experimental 10.1901/jaba.1999.32-451
analysis of resurgence. Journal of the Experimental Wacker, D. P., Harding, J. W., Berg, W. K., Lee, J. F.,
Analysis of Behavior, 80, 217–233. doi: 10.1901/ Schieltz, K. M., Padilla, Y. C., … Shahan, T. A.
jeab.2003.80-217 (2011). An evaluation of persistence of treatment
Luczynski, K. C., & Hanley, G. P. (2013). Prevention of effects during long-term treatment of destructive
problem behavior by teaching functional communica- behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
tion and self-control skills to preschoolers. Journal of Behavior, 96, 261–282. doi: 10.1901/
Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 355–368. doi: jeab.2011.96-261
10.1002/jaba.44
Luczynski, K. C., & Hanley, G. P. (2014). How should Received May 14, 2015
periods without social interaction be scheduled? Chil- Final acceptance December 3, 2015
dren’s preference for practical schedules of positive Action Editor, John Borrero

Potrebbero piacerti anche