Sei sulla pagina 1di 19

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

ScienceDirect
Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70 – 88
www.elsevier.com/locate/intmar

Of “Likes” and “Pins”: The Effects of Consumers' Attachment to


Social Media
Rebecca A. VanMeter a,⁎& Douglas B. Grisaffe b & Lawrence B. Chonko b
a
Department of Marketing, Miller College of Business, Ball State University, Muncie, IN 47306, United States
b
Department of Marketing, College of Business, The University of Texas at Arlington, Arlington, TX 76019, United States

Available online 10 November 2015

Abstract

Marketing researchers and practitioners are showing substantial interest in social media communication, trying to understand the challenges and
opportunities associated with this new cultural and social phenomenon. In this research, the authors examine social media as a new attachment
phenomenon, positing likely predictive links to marketing-related social media behaviors. Researchers have demonstrated useful applicability of
psychological attachment theory to a variety of other marketing contexts, including special possessions, places, brands, and services. Attachment to
such varied focal targets has been shown to influence behaviors of interest to marketers. However, research to date has yet to develop a
conceptualization or operationalization of attachment in the social media context. The authors seek to contribute to the literature in two primary
ways: first, we provide a foundational definition of attachment to social media, and conduct four initial studies to develop a measure that meets
desired reliability and validity standards. Secondly, in a fifth study, we use this validated measure to test its empirical usefulness in predicting social
media behaviors in an applied retail setting. Taken together, the results are particularly valuable in demonstrating that attachment to social media is
a distinct, measurable phenomenon that helps to explain various activities on social media platforms, including C2C advocacy and C2B supportive
communication behaviors. Results reveal practical guidance for marketing managers wrestling with developing effective social media marketing
strategies.
© 2015 Direct Marketing Educational Foundation, Inc., dba Marketing EDGE. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Social media; Attachment theory; Scale development; Attachment behaviors; Social media advocacy; Social media support

Introduction hand, one influencer can drive thousands of potential customers


(or more) to a website or store. On the other hand, that same
Social media has become an important new cultural and influencer can spread his or her dissatisfaction and erode your
social phenomenon, changing the way millions of people and brand equity and profitability” (Gillan 2010). Social media is
businesses connect and communicate. Academic researchers thus radically changing the communication landscape. The
and practitioners in marketing are showing substantial interest average American now spends an average of 3.2 hours a day,
in this new form of communication, trying to understand the or 22.4 hours a week, on social networking sites. This is not
challenges and opportunities associated with social media. limited to young users. Those 35–49 spend an average of
According to Jim Davis, Sr., Vice President and CMO of SAS, 21 hours per week, and those 50–64 spend an average of
“just a few years ago, we were talking about the information 17 hours a week on social media (Ipsos 2013). Even the first
revolution — today, we are witnessing a social media lady of the United States has used social media to engage in
revolution. For business, it's a double-edged sword. On one “hashtag activism” (e.g., #BringBackOurGirls).
Social media has also become a major focus in corporate
marketing strategy. According to the 2014 Social Media
⁎ Corresponding author. Industry Report (Stelzner 2014), 97% of companies are using
E-mail addresses: vanmeter@bsu.edu (R. VanMeter), grisaffe@uta.edu some form of social media to market their business. However,
(D.B. Grisaffe), lchonko@uta.edu (L.B. Chonko). only about a third of them feel like they are doing so efficiently,

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intmar.2015.09.001
1094-9968/© 2015 Direct Marketing Educational Foundation, Inc., dba Marketing EDGE. All rights reserved.
R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88 71

indicating a disconnect between what marketing managers offering that attachment to social media is predictive of
believe they should be doing and knowing exactly what to do to consumer's social media behaviors. As far as we are aware,
leverage social media. This poses a serious problem since ef- this is the first article to examine attachment to social media,
fective marketing strategy depends upon concentrating re- this set of behavioral outcomes in the social media context, and
sources optimally against well-specified activities to increase it is the first to demonstrate these effects in relation to attitude
sales and create sustainable competitive advantage (Aaker toward social media and time spent on social media.
2008). Without a clear course of action to incorporate social
media into marketing strategy, most C-suite officers will be left Theoretical Background
wanting regarding the link between social media marketing
efforts and return on marketing investment. Key Concepts
Academic research also has begun investigating various
social media phenomena including personality traits of those Before presenting our conceptualizations and hypotheses, we
who use social media (Ehrenberg et al. 2008), how use of social define two key components of our research. First, in order to
media impacts the individual (Valkenburg, Peter, and Schouten define attachment to social media (ASM) we leverage the existing
2006), as well as a host of brand-related (Gensler et al. 2013; definitions from prior multi-disciplinary work on attachment
Hollebeek, Glynn, and Brodie 2014; Labrecque 2014; Naylor, (Bowlby 1979; Brocato, Baker, and Voorhees 2014; Park et al.
Lamberton, and West 2012) and organization related issues 2010; Thomson, MacInnis, and Park 2005). The common core
(Blazevic et al. 2014; Rapp et al. 2013; Wang, Yu, and Wei among these definitions is the strength of the bond between the
2012). person and the attachment object. Therefore, we define attachment
The motivation of our research adds to this growing literature to social media (ASM) simply as the strength of a bond between a
and stems from an interest in the core ingredient of social media, person and social media. Secondly, social media in this paper is
the individual actor (Peters et al. 2013). Therefore, in our defined as an interactive platform that allows social actors to create
research, we undertake a new direction in marketing-related and share in multi-way, immediate, and contingent communica-
social media research, seeking a strong theoretical understanding tions (Kietzmann et al. 2011; Peters et al. 2013). The focus is on
of psychological connections consumers may be forming with social media as a holistic phenomenon, rather than on one specific
social media and how this connection subsequently impacts social media platform or another (e.g., Facebook, Instagram).
various consumer behaviors. Are some people forming espe- Other research, such as Hollebeek, Glynn, and Brodie (2014), has
cially strong bonds with social media itself? If so, does that related to the brand in the context of social media. Hollebeek,
stronger affinity manifest in social media behaviors that are Glynn, and Brodie 2014 scale is specific to a “brand” of social
important to marketers? Prior research has shown that people media, whereas, our scale is a psychological individual-level
develop attachments to various “targets” of attachment, includ- difference factor with an unbranded medium.
ing places (e.g., Brocato, Baker, and Voorhees 2014), people Recent research has also taken a holistic approach (e.g.,
(e.g., Bowlby 1979), and brands (e.g., Thomson, MacInnis, and Rapp et al. 2013). Justification for this stance ties to the external
Park 2005). Social media now powerfully enables connection reality that social media is continuously evolving — new
and interaction with these attachment foci (Wilcox and Stephen platforms arise and old platforms become out of vogue. There-
2013). It seems plausible that individuals may develop an fore for our purposes we wish to investigate attachment to
attachment to the conduit itself, since social media facilitates social media rather than attachment to a specific social media
their interaction with other attachment target(s). As has been platform. Our approach could be adapted in future research if
historically demonstrated with other attachment research (e.g., researchers seek to study attachment to a specific platform of
Hazan and Zeifman 1999), attachment manifests itself in various interest (e.g., Pinterest). With definitions of ASM and social
behavioral outcomes or consequences. Behavioral manifestation media in hand, we now present the theoretical logic underlying
of social media attachment could involve elevated activity in our investigation.
social media dialogues, postings, viewings, sharings, etc., many
of which could support important marketing outcomes. Attachment Theory
The current research makes three key contributions in both
attachment theory and social media research. First, we extend Attachment theory has been particularly useful in psychol-
attachment theory's role in marketing to the domain of social ogy and marketing, and we posit that it also offers an excellent
media by demonstrating that individuals exhibit variation in framework to investigate peoples' growing psychological con-
their attachment to social media. Second, we provide an em- nections with social media. Thus in this paper, we use attach-
pirically validated measure that captures quantitative variation ment theory to test consumers' attraction to social media and
in attachment to social media. Third, and most importantly, we examine behavioral consequences of that attachment such as
empirically demonstrate that ASM has distinct behavioral im- being an advocate of a brand or company via social media
plications. Attachment to social media more accurately predicts and interacting with brands and companies on social media.
consumer behaviors, advocacy, and supportive communication Explication of this ASM construct contributes theoretically by
back to the organization above attitude toward social media and extending attachment theory into the domain of social media.
time spent on social media. Beyond their theoretical signifi- Originally, attachment theory described strong “bonds” be-
cance, the results have significant managerial implications, tween mothers and infants attachment that met fundamental
72 R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88

needs for safety and security through maintenance of proximity Marketing researchers in particular are grappling with how social
(Ainsworth and Bell 1970; Bowlby 1969). This theory has been media impacts areas such as brand management (Gensler et al.
generalized to other relationships, such as with friends (Trinke 2013); consumer brand engagement (Hollebeek, Glynn, and
and Bartholomew 1997), romantic partners (Hazan and Shaver Brodie 2014; Labrecque 2014); consumers' purchase intentions
1994), and celebrities (Thomson 2006). In marketing, researchers (e.g., Wang, Yu, and Wei 2012); consumer self-control (Wilcox
have studied the development of attachments to tangible objects and Stephen 2013); brand perceptions (e.g., Naylor, Lamberton,
such as gifts (Mick and DeMoss 1990), collectables (Slater and West 2012); seller, retailer, and consumer interactions
2001), or other types of special or favorite objects (Ball and (Blazevic et al. 2014; Rapp et al. 2013); the appropriateness
Tasaki 1992; Kleine, Kleine, and Allen 1995). More recently, of viral marketing via social media for various product types
research in marketing has focused on emotional attachment to (Schulze, Schöler, and Skiera 2014); as well as company ROI
brands (Park et al. 2010; Thomson, MacInnis, and Park 2005), (e.g., Hoffman and Fodor 2010). According to J. Walker Smith,
retail places (Brocato, Baker, and Voorhees 2014), as well as executive chairman of the London-based marketing consultancy
attachment in intangible service marketing contexts (Mende, The Futures Company and a Marketing News columnist, “we are
Bolton, and Bitner 2013). still in the learning phase of social media. The presumption
In this paper, we extend the trend toward a broader set of across the industry is that social media is the TV of tomorrow, so
potential attachment foci by proposing that consumers also it is risky to put off getting to scale with digital investments, even
become attached to social media. In that social media prominently if there is still a lot to learn.”
facilitates people “connecting” with other people, as well as Marketers sense that social media is a catalyst to capitalize
connections with organizations, causes, companies and brands, it on customer generated word-of-mouth (WOM); however, very
logically could be a new medium by which individuals find little empirical evidence exists to support this intuition. Positive
relationships that offer comfort, safety and security. As in other WOM has a long history of attention as a kind of customer-
domains, we also propose that this psychological attachment then generated marketing effort by which new customer acquisition
results in a variety of behavioral outcomes, several of which are is facilitated (de Matos and Rossi 2008). Social media has the
especially relevant to marketers. potential for exponentially higher degrees of positive WOM
For example, attachments have been shown to manifest in a within and across large numbers of socially networked individ-
particularly salient classical behavioral consequence, separation uals. The cost and scope of reach in facilitating WOM on social
distress (e.g., Bowlby 1980; Hazan and Zeifman 1999; Thomson, media becomes very compelling. If attachment to social media
MacInnis, and Park 2005). This outcome involves the distress is predictive of WOM on social media, there would be great
that attached individuals feel when there is an actual or threatened practical value in this insight as a driver of a critical marketing
separation from the attachment figure (Park et al. 2010). For phenomenon of interest.
example, young children may exhibit emotional distress when Likewise, relationship marketing emphasizes payoffs for
taken to a daycare facility or when a babysitter arrives. The establishing and maintaining relationships with customers. This
threat of, or actual separation from, the attachment figure evokes is no longer just one-way communication from a business to the
distress. In the same way we would expect individuals who consumer (B2C). Social media now offers a powerful way for
are attached to social media to express psychological distress at customers to not only proactively and regularly communicate
the threat of separation from social media. Therefore, our first with companies and brands via social media (C2B), but also
attachment-focused hypothesis regarding social media follows from consumers to other consumers (C2C). If such C2C and
from this classic outcome of separation distress (Park et al. 2010): C2B processes occur in higher degrees among individuals who
are more strongly attached to social media, the study of social
H1. Individuals who are more strongly attached to social media
media attachment phenomenon and its resultant behavioral
will express more distress under threat of separation from social
consequences has very important implications for marketing.
media than those who are less attached to social media.
Thus, we explore ASM in relation to social media: C2C advocacy
and supportive C2B communication with organizations.
In a marketing context, individuals with strong attachments
It has long been recognized that WOM advocacy is important
to social media might experience distress if their lifeline to
in the growth and vibrancy of brands (Brown et al. 2005). This
social reference information about products, services, or organi-
WOM advocacy is affected by emotion (Berger and Milkman
zations was suddenly inaccessible.
2012), is used to gain personal and social benefits (Alexandrov,
Lilly, and Babakus 2013), and when communicated on social
Marketing-related Outcomes of Attachment to Social Media media, may be used by consumers to help make their purchasing
decisions (Pan and Chiou 2011). Therefore, consumer advocacy
Social media marketing is expected to make up 21.4% of is especially relevant and timely in social media research because
marketing budgets by 2019 (Soat 2014). However, 63% of social media has opened new ways for customers to direct com-
marketers are currently unsure of how social media marketing munication with each other (C2C communication) to specifically
is impacting their ROI (Stelzner 2014). Why are marketers discuss a company or brand. Part of the potential power here is
willing to continue investing in social media marketing efforts that social media allows C2C communication to occur in a
if the return is so elusive? Part of the issue may be the sense one-to-many format. One consumer's post can communicate
of unrealized potential present in this new form of media. information and sentiment to hundreds of other consumers
R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88 73

instantaneously. Thus, we consider advocacy via social media to only socially, but also in relation to consumer behaviors.
be a very important phenomenon to marketers and posit that Therefore, we hypothesize the following:
higher degrees of social media based WOM are more likely to
H4a. Individuals who are more strongly attached to social
occur for those who are more strongly attached to social media.
media will engage in notably more social-related activities on
Specifically, we hypothesize:
social media than those who are less attached to social media.
H2. Individuals who are more strongly attached to social media H4b. Individuals who are more strongly attached to social
will engage in more C2C advocacy via social media than those media will engage in notably more consumer-related activities
who are less attached to social media. on social media than those who are less attached to social
media.
In the same way that social media enhances consumers'
ability to communicate with other consumers, it is also in- In the remainder of this paper, we test our four key hy-
creasingly recognized as a way for businesses to listen to con- potheses across a series of studies. Since application of attach-
sumer feedback (Agnihotri et al. 2012). One way companies ment theory to the social media context is new to the literature,
can field consumer feedback is when consumers directly and we first operationalize a measure of attachment to social media
proactively communicate with organizations via social media (ASM). Then, we conduct additional studies to explore ASM as
(C2B). Consumers might do so for the purposes of com- a predictor of marketing-relevant social media behaviors. We
menting, complaining, commending, or otherwise supporting examine ASM in relation to C2C advocacy, C2B supportive
the organization. In this paper we focus on support for the communication behaviors, and amount of social media activity
organization, because it is a more positive and active form of in social- and consumer-related life domains.
behavior meant to help the organization (Bettencourt 1997)
rather than merely a passive and reactive response by cus- Method
tomers. We view consumer support of an organization as a
particularly valuable form of C2B communication enabled by Study 1
social media.
Customer supportive behaviors have been examined in As per classically accepted psychometric procedures (e.g.,
contexts involving service organizations (e.g., Bettencourt Churchill 1979; DeVellis 2003), we first sought to generate and
1997) and concepts from that line of research are transferrable test a broad pool of initial items aimed at tapping into the
to social media research. Social media offers a fast and proposed construct of attachment to social media.
effective means for consumers to communicate to organiza-
tions and has a high level of applied marketing relevance. Methodology
Recent research shows that 83% of marketers are specifically
interested in engaging their audience via social media Participants and Design. We asked colleagues to contact
(Stelzner 2014). We posit here that those who are more friends they felt were “frequent users” of social media, and to
strongly attached to social media are likely to engage in C2B request participation in a short informal survey. We sought to
help and support to a greater degree than those who are less obtain inputs from approximately twenty respondents in this
attached to social media. Therefore, we hypothesize the initial phase, being particularly interested in qualitative descrip-
following: tions about the role of social media in these individuals' lives.
Our aim was to gather elemental descriptions in respondents'
H3. Individuals who are more strongly attached to social own words that would inform item generation.
media will engage in more C2B supportive communication We asked a series of four questions on 7-point Likert-type
behaviors than those who are less attached to social media. scales: “Please indicate the degree to which you consider social
media to be part of your life; Please indicate how important social
Finally, attachment theory indicates that attached individuals media is in your life right now; How much would you agree or
spend significantly more time and effort to be in proximity to disagree with this statement: Social Media brings meaning to my
the targets of their attachment (Hazan and Zeifman 1999). life; and To what extent would you say you are emotionally
Those who are more strongly attached to social media are likely attached to social media?” Each item was followed by an open-
to have more activity on social media than those who are less ended question asking respondents why they gave their particular
attached. For example, in the social media realm, this could chosen rating. Ultimately, we acquired rich qualitative responses
involve more posting, tweeting, reading of others' posts, and from a convenience sample of 21 social media users (79%
other socially-related behaviors than others. From a marketing female, average age 36, age range 27–59).
perspective, those more strongly attached should be more likely
to do things like use social media to talk to others about a Item Generation. After collecting the qualitative responses,
brand, purchase something because of what they read on social thematic analysis was conducted on the textual data to classify
media, or engage in any number of other brand related verbatims into dimensions based on the natural language of
behaviors via social media. Accordingly, strong attachment to respondents. We generated a set of working items by taking the
social media likely results in more activity on social media not verbatims of respondents and transforming them into complete
74 R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88

sentences that could be rated using Likert scales. This served as collection of quantitative data to further explore the structure of the
a starting point for an initial item pool (Richins and Dawson items, and to undertake item purification per accepted measure-
1992). The pool of items was then compared to extant social ment development practices (Churchill 1979; DeVellis 2003).
media literature (e.g., Fennis, Pruyn, and Maasland 2005;
Hollenbeck and Kaikati 2012; Stephen and Galak 2012). Study 2
Additional items were generated for themes that could be
important based on the literature, but which had not been Study 2 addressed three objectives: (1) to understand the
explicitly mentioned in the exploratory research. underlying structure of attachment to social media, (2) to
A preliminary set of 53 items was generated based on this produce a purified set of items to measure those dimensions,
qualitative research and review of literature. Following item and (3) to test the purified subset for replicability and reliability
purification practices (cf. Bearden, Hardesty, and Rose 2001; in a new sample. To accomplish these objectives, we collected
Shimp and Sharma 1987) we ran several pretests of these items, data from a first sample and conducted exploratory factor
examining correlations, descriptive statistics, and item analyses analysis and item purification to arrive at a preliminary inter-
looking for early indications of potentially low communality, pretable set of dimensions. We then collected new data within a
ambiguity in the language that was used, and complexities in non-student validation sample to enact a test of replicability of
the wording of our items that might represent more than one the derived factor structure. Details of our procedures follow.
underlying concept. Based on these early statistical explora-
tions and conceptual item analyses, improvements were made
Methodology
to produce our initial working set of items aimed at accurately
reflecting the domain of interest.
Participants and Design. Two hundred thirty-eight under-
We then utilized the key informant technique (Parasuraman,
graduate business students (47% female, average age 24, age
Grewal, and Krishnan 2006) as a means of further refining the
range 18–49) completed an online survey in exchange for partial
initial item pool. We presented the working set of items to two
course credit; twelve respondents were dropped because they did
separate industry experts, asking for open-ended critique and
not provide complete data. Secondly, we collected a replication
feedback. The aim was to get a preliminary check of face
and validation sample from a distinct non-student population. As
validity and content validity, as well as a check of thoroughness
part of an undergraduate marketing research course, about 900
of the domain covered by the proposed set as a whole. Based on
faculty and staff across a southwestern university were contacted
the key informant expert inputs, the wording of a few items was
to participate in an online survey. Participation was incentivized
modified for clarity, and the proposed structure was expanded
with a drawing for one of seven $25 gift cards to one of five local
to capture an additional theme one expert felt was missing.1 In
restaurants. Of those contacted, 328 began the survey and 226
order to incorporate the missing theme, we developed additional
faculty and staff (67% female, average age 44, age range 18–over
items using the language from the expert's description of the
91) provided usable data.
missing subdimension.

Findings Measures. The questionnaire first asked participants to in-


Completion of this process resulted in a revised, starting dicate the various social media platforms they use. Next,
pool consisting of 45 items. Anticipating a reflective measure- participants responded to the working set of ASM items. Items
ment specification according to classical test theory (DeVellis were randomized in blocks of nine to eleven questions, and
2003), overlapping statements were designed to reflect higher- blocks were randomized in their presentation to respondents to
level themes from the item generation phase. For example, the avoid the threat of order biases. All items were asked on 7-point
following items centered around the idea of nostalgia: “Using Likert scales (1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”).
social media makes me feel nostalgic about things that I have Respondents also provided basic demographic information as
done in the past; Sometimes social media reminds me of warm part of the survey.
memories from my past; I use social media to reconnect with
'the good old days'." Another example of a set of overlapping Results
items focused on social media's role in helping a person to stay
informed: “I use social media to see what other people's Factor Analyses. In the initial analysis sample, the 45 items
opinions are on topics that are important to me; Social media were subjected to exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with oblique
is one of the main ways I get information about others, and rotation2 using SPSS version 22. Several iterations of item
Social media is one of my primary sources of information about purification and re-analysis addressed issues of low loadings,
news.” high cross-loading, low measures of sampling adequacy, a lack
The ultimate output of Study 1 was an elementally developed, of interpretability, or low communalities, to arrive at a solution
key-informant-refined item pool. This was the starting point for

2
Both Varimax and Oblique rotations were run with each sample and
1
Influence via social media was the proposed missing dimension. produced comparable results. The Oblique results are discussed within.
R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88 75

with statistical desiderata according to classical test theory (Hair connected to others, for example, “I use social media because it
et al. 2010).3 The final working purified set contained 27 items makes staying in touch with others convenient.” The second
which factored to eight interpretable dimensions (Table 1). These factor, Nostalgia, taps into respondents' ability to use social
eight dimensions explained 79.6% of the variance in the data with media in order to reconnect with people, places, and events
all communalities exceeding the .60 cut off (Hair et al. 2010). We from the past, as when respondents agree with items such as,
also examined the correlations between the obliquely rotated “Sometimes social media reminds me of warm memories from
dimensions in anticipation of a test of a second-order construct my past.” The third factor, Informed, deals with social media's
specification (see Appendix A). A second-order structure for role in keeping the respondent informed about news and events,
overall construct measurement has been observed in the brand for example, “Social media is one of the main ways I get
attachment literature (e.g., Park et al. 2010; Thomson, MacInnis, information about major events.” The fourth factor, Enjoyment,
and Park 2005). Therefore, we believed that significantly cor- reflects social media's role as a way for the respondent to
related subdimensions would support further testing of a single experience relaxation and enjoyment, illustrated by items such
second-order conceptualization. As expected, all obliquely rotated as, “I use social media as a way for me to de-stress after a long
dimensions correlated positively and significantly. Alpha reliabil- day.” The fifth factor, Advice, reflects respondents' ability to
ity coefficients were computed for each factor and ranged from seek forms of advice from others via social media. An example
.82 to .91, which is well within desired guidelines (Nunnally and item is, “If I'm unsure about an upcoming decision I get input
Bernstein 1994). We also computed an alpha reliability coefficient from friends on social media.” The sixth factor, Affirmation,
across summated scales for the eight first-order factors considered taps into respondents' ability to feel assured and supported
together. This preliminary view of a second-order structure also through their usage of social media, as demonstrated by the
demonstrated adequate reliability (α = .90) (Table 1). following item, “It makes me feel accepted when people
comment on my social media posts.” The seventh factor,
Replication and Verification. The factor structure from the Enhances My Life, demonstrates social media's role in making
initial item purification replicated in the non-student sample respondents' lives better, for example, “Social media enhances
(n = 226). The coefficients, communalities, and internal reliabil- my life.” The eighth factor, Influence, taps into the ability to use
ities of the 27 items with eight subdimensions were thus verified social media to encourage, influence, and help others. An
in a more generalizable replication sample (Table 1). The eight example item for this dimension is, “Sometimes I post things
dimensions explained 88.2% of the variance in the non-student just to have a positive effect on other peoples' moods.5”
replication, with all communalities exceeding the .60 acceptabil-
ity cut off (Hair et al. 2010). The internal consistency of all
Discussion
subdimensions was .80 or higher. A single second-order factor
A refined set of 27 items was thus identified from the
indicated by the first-order dimensions also demonstrated internal
original item pool of Study 2. These items reflect a multifaceted
reliability with Cronbach's alpha of .93.
structure consisting of eight subdimensions, preliminarily
The results from the factor analyses provided empirical
considered as indicative of a second-order construct of attach-
evidence in support of eight first-order factors and collectively
ment to social media (ASM). Using an additional sample,
one second-order factor.4 The eight first-order subdimensions
the initial findings replicated well and the dimensionality of
are named and defined as follows (see Table 1): the first factor,
ASM was shown to be stable and verifiable in a non-student
Connecting, reflects respondents' use of social media to stay
sample.

Study 3
3
We sought “simple structure” in which each variable loaded highly on one
and only one factor. Variables that cross-loaded were deleted one at a time and
the analysis re-run, per Hair et al. (2010). Besides seeking simple structure, we In Study 3, we sought additional psychometric evidence
also sought a solution with a sufficient number of factors to explain a large regarding our operationalization while simultaneously starting
proportion of variance in the item set. In addition to the variance explained to focus on various social media outcomes of relevance to
criterion, we also sought a solution that produced communalities of greater than
marketers. Thus, the objective of Study 3 was threefold:
.50 for each retained item, anticipating a more rigorous future test with CFA. In
addition, we required interpretability for sets of items loading on retained (1) to test the proposed second order structure of ASM with
factors, particularly in light of the knowledge acquired in the item generation Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA), (2) to test the proposed
phase of the research. We also tested the internal consistency reliability of each structure against competing measurement models, and (3) to
face valid item set to ensure the solution produced reliable multi-item sets. The begin to see how ASM relates to marketing relevant outcomes
final 8 factor solution had the clearest simple structure, the most interpretable
set of factors, strong communalities above the desired threshold, a high
percentage of variance explained for the solution as a whole, and internally
reliable multi-item sets. Examinations of solutions with more or less factors
5
were less ideal with respect to combined consideration of these criteria. See The eight example items presented here may act as surrogates of their
Appendix B for the full pattern matrix for the non-student sample. respective first order factors, per Hair et al. (2010). Preliminary analyses show
4
In both samples the Bartlett's test was statistically significant, the KMO was these eight items to demonstrate favorable internal consistency as a set and to
above .9, and the individual measures of sampling adequacy for each item were share substantial variance with the full 27-item scale. As noted in the discussion
greater than .5 (Hair et al. 2010, see Appendix C). Collectively, these indicate section, a focus for future research would be development of a short scale
sufficient intercorrelation to justify EFA. version of ASM.
76 R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88

Table 1
Study 2: Purification & replication sample.
Items Purification sample (n = 238) Replication sample (n = 226)
Mean Standard deviation Pattern coefficients Pattern coefficients
1. I use social media to interact with friends. 5.31 1.51 .82 .81
2. Social media provides a way for me to stay connected to people across 5.71 1.41 .80 .96
distances.
3. I use social media because it makes staying in touch with others 5.32 1.54 .80 .87
convenient.
4. Social media provides a way for me to keep in touch with others that I 5.42 1.46 .70 .77
care about.
Connecting (.92) (.95)
5. Social media allows me to look back at meaningful events, people, and 5.08 1.55 .78 .75
places from my past.
6. Using social media makes me feel nostalgic about things that I have 4.09 1.65 .75 .83
done in the past.
7. Sometimes social media reminds me of warm memories from my past. 4.66 1.58 .74 .71
Nostalgia (.82) (.93)
8. Social media is one of one of the main ways I get information about 3.76 1.49 .88 .85
major events.
9. Social media allows me to stay informed about events and news. 3.95 1.43 .88 .79
10. Social media is one of my primary sources of information about news. 3.40 1.59 .85 .88
Informed (.88) (.93)
11. I use social media as a way for me to de-stress after a long day. 4.29 1.77 .73 .76
12. I use social media to give myself a break when I've been busy. 5.07 1.56 .65 .84
13. Social media is an enjoyable way to spend time. 4.57 1.42 .44 .63
Enjoyment (.84) (.92)
14. I seek advice for upcoming decisions using social media. 3.10 1.63 .83 .77
15. I get advice about medical questions on social media. 2.26 1.39 .81 .89
16. If I'm unsure about an upcoming decision I get input from friends on 3.21 1.75 .81 .84
social media.
Advice (.83) (.91)
17. When others comment on my posts I feel affirmed. 4.22 1.62 .85 .85
18. When people respond to my posts in social media I feel like they care 3.55 1.36 .84 .90
about me.
19. It makes me feel accepted when people comment on my social media posts. 4.16 1.69 .82 .91
Affirmed (.91) (.95)
20. Social media makes my life a little bit better. 4.09 1.50 .75 .92
21. Social media enhances my life. 3.95 1.50 .70 .88
22. My life is a little richer because of social media. 3.78 1.66 .66 .92
Enhances My Life (.89) (.95)
23. Sometimes I post things just to have a positive effect on other peoples' 4.07 1.79 .88 .72
moods.
24. I post on social media to brighten other peoples' day. 3.89 1.75 .88 .70
25. I post things on social media that I think will be helpful to my friends' lives. 3.88 1.70 .85 .68
26. I want to inspire other people with my social media posts. 3.73 1.75 .79 .74
27. I think it is important to share things on social media so those I care 3.84 1.69 .58 .50
about stay informed.
Influence (.91) (.94)
EASM (.90) (.93)
Bolded numbers in parenthesis are the Cronbach's alpha. All items had the full range from 1–7.

by formally testing Hypotheses H1 regarding separation distress dropped because individuals reported not using social media
(Bowlby 1980; Hazan and Zeifman 1999). at all. The final sample contained two hundred and nine
respondents.
Methodology
Measures. The 27 items for ASM and a set of 5 items
Participants and Design. Two hundred forty-six undergrad- measuring separation distress were randomly presented in
uate business students (44% female, average age 24, age range blocks of five to ten items. The 27 ASM items (Table 1) were
19–46) completed an online survey in exchange for partial measured on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “Strongly Disagree”
course credit. Of those, five responses were dropped because to 7 = “Strongly Agree”). Regarding separation distress, we
they did not provide complete data, and thirty-two were adapted existing items from the literature (Fraley and Davis
R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88 77

Influence

Advice

Informed

Connecting
ASM
ASM
Enjoyment

Nostalgia

Affirmed

Enhances
My Life

a) Second-Order Structure b) Competing First-Order Structure

Influence

Influence

Advice
Advice
Cognitive
Informed
Informed

Connecting
Connecting

Enjoyment
Enjoyment

Nostalgia Affective Nostalgia

Affirmed Affirmed

EnhancesLife
Enhances My
Life

c) Competing Second-Order Structure d) Competing 8 Factor First-Order


Structure

Fig. 1. Study 3: Competing measurement model specification for ASM. a. Second-order structure. b. Competing first-order structure. c. Competing second-order
structure. d. Competing 8 factor first-order structure.
78 R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88

1997; Hazan and Zeifman 1994) to appropriately reflect a Table 2


social media context (I would miss social media if I didn't have Study 3: Second-order CFA measurement model results.
it, social media would be hard for me to live without, I would be Loadings CR Ave
sad without social media). Additional items were adapted from Connecting .79 .88 .71
Park et al. (2010) who employed two separation distress items 1. Interact .85
measured on 11-point scales (anchored by 0 = “not at all” 2. Connected across distances .78
3. Convenient staying in touch .93
and 10 = “completely”) (to what extent would you be distressed
4. Keep in touch .81
if the social media you use were discontinued? and to what Nostalgia .88 .83 .70
extent is it difficult to imagine life without the social media you 5. Look back on past .87
use?). 6. Nostalgic .76
7. Warm memories from past .87
Informed .71 .87 .75
Results
8. Main ways get info .88
9. Stay informed .90
Measurement Model. Following previous second-order con- 10. Primary source of info .82
ceptualizations in the brand domain (Park et al. 2010; Thomson, Enjoyment .90 .83 .70
MacInnis, and Park 2005), we applied confirmatory factor 11. De-stress .81
12. Give myself a break .85
analysis (CFA) to test the appropriateness of a second-order 13. Enjoyable .85
ASM conceptualization in the social media domain (Fig. 1a). Advice .66 .78 .65
In this model, the 27 items loaded on 8 factors, which in turn 14. Upcoming decision .82
loaded on a single second-order factor, our ASM superordinate 15. Advice about medical .68
construct.6 All fit statistics for this model meet standard criteria 16. Unsure .90
Affirmed .81 .91 .81
(e.g., Hu and Bentler 1999): degrees of freedom [df] = 316;
17. Feel affirmed .87
chi-squared = 638.91; root mean square error of approximation 18. Feel cared about .91
[RMSEA] = .07; nonnormed fit index [NNFI] = .98; compara- 19. Feel accepted .92
tive fit index [CFI] = .98; and standardized root mean square Enhances My Life .86 .85 .72
residual [SRMR] = .066. Loadings for each of the items on the 20. Little bit better .87
21. Enhances .89
factors were statistically significant (p b .01). As reported in
22. Little richer .79
Table 2, the average variance extracted (AVE) and construct Influence .70 .91 .73
reliabilities (CR) all meet or exceed the minimum accepted 23. Positively effect others' moods .87
standards of .5 and .7 respectively (Fornell and Larcker 1981). 24. Brighten others' day .84
Subsequently, three competing measurement models (Hair 25. Helpful to friends' lives .89
26. Inspire others .89
et al. 2010) were estimated for comparative purposes, similar 27. Those I care about stay informed .79
to the approach taken in the emotional attachment to brands EASM .93 .63
literature by Thomson, MacInnis, and Park (2005) and Park et
Numbers in bold are the loadings to the second-order EASM factor.
al. (2010). The first model specified all the items as loading
on a single first-order latent factor, with none of the eight
subdimensions delineated (Fig. 1b). The second model tested a
competing second-order structure (Fig. 1c) in which dimensions adapted item had a statistically significant loading on the latent
of attachment to social media reflected either more cognitive or construct of separation distress, with loadings ranging between
more affective elements. The third model tested a first-order eight .76 and .94. The measure of separation distress is internally
subdimensions latent factor model (Fig. 1d). As the results show consistent α = .94. The average variance extracted (AVE) and
(Table 3), the proposed ASM second-order factor model fits the construct reliabilities (CR) all meet or exceed the minimum
data better than the three competing specifications. This provides
confirmatory support for the tenability of a second-order ASM
specification, with our eight first-order subdimensions.
Table 3
Study 3: Assessment of competing models.
Convergent and Criterion-related Validity With Separation
χ2 df RMESA NNFI CFI SRMR
Distress. Finally, we turn to the predictive ability of our
attachment measure against the classic attachment outcome of (b) One factor 3,163.93 324 .19 .80 .82 .11
(c) Two factors 1,746.57 323 .17 .90 .91 .01
separation distress. We therefore specified an SEM model to
(d) 8 1st order factors 1,542.78 324 .16 .91 .92 .38
test Hypothesis H1, relating ASM to social media separation (a) 2nd order 574.13 316 .06 .98 .98 .07
distress. Regarding measurement of separation distress, each
Δχ2
(b) vs (a) 2,589.8 (8) ⁎
(c) vs (a) 1,172.44 (7) ⁎
6 (d) vs (a) 968.65 (8) ⁎
Measure of sampling adequacy for this data are KMO = .904 and Bartlett's Test
of Sphericity was Approx. chi-square = 4027.367, df = 351, p-value ≤ .000. ⁎ p-Value b .001.
R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88 79

accepted standards of .5 and .7 respectively (Fornell and social media report higher levels of experiencing distress in the
Larcker 1981). The Fit statistics for the measurement model form of negative emotions at the threat of separation from social
including separation distress also met all the standard criteria (Hu media.
and Bentler 1999): chi-squared = 899.76 (454), RMSEA = .067, With sufficient confidence in the ASM measure, and initial
NNFI = .98, CFI = .98, SRMR = .07. support for predictive links to a classical attachment outcome,
With evidence supporting construct measurements, we then we undertook studies 4 and 5 to examine ASM in relation to
examined the predicted associations of ASM with separation additional social media activities that have specific practical
distress. The path from ASM to this outcome is meaningfully and managerial marketing implications.
large and statistically significant, the standardized value is .70
(t-value = 11.04, p-value ≤ .001, Fig. 2). As expected, the
context-specific measure of social media attachment predicts Study 4
the context-adapted attachment outcome of separation distress
from social media. This provides further supporting psycho- In Study 4, we explore ASM as a predictor of consumer
metric evidence for criterion-related validity (concurrent and social media behaviors of direct relevance to marketing: C2C
predictive), while at the same time providing an initial em- advocacy of a company via social media (Hypothesis H2),
pirical test of ASM's ability to predict marketing relevant and C2B supportive communication behavior via social media
behavior. For marketing purposes, it is likely that those who do (Hypothesis H3). Regarding C2C advocacy, social media has
not like to be disconnected from social media (i.e., separation revolutionized consumers' power to communicate with and
distress), would be more valuable target consumers for things influence others, vastly multiplying the number of people with
like being reached through social media campaigns and spreading whom they can share criticism or commendation of products
positive word-of-mouth via social media. and services in real-time. Regarding C2B supportive commu-
nication behaviors, social media has also opened important
Discussion new channels by which consumers can directly interact with
Study 3 provides support for the second-order representation organizations. The trend toward collaborative marketing and
of ASM with its eight subdimensions. That conceptualization branding enables marketing managers to give consumers the
fits the data better than other model specifications. Further, we opportunity to post both favorable and unfavorable comments,
provide evidence of support for ASM's criterion-related validity, share photos, and provide ideas, feedback, and/or other input to
being strongly related to a key theory-based outcome of attach- the organization via various social media platforms (Malär et al.
ment as represented in a social media context. Hypotheses 2011). In Study 4, we test ASM as a predictor of C2C and C2B
H1 is supported. Those who are more strongly attached to social media behaviors.

Fig. 2. Study 3: Path from ASM to Separation Distress.


80 R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88

We also address a plausible rival explanatory variable as Table 4


part of Study 4. We sought to differentiate ASM's predictive Study 4: (a) Social media advocacy adapted from White and Schneider (2000).
power from a general measure of attitude toward social (a) Social media advocacy adapted from White and Schneider (2000)
media. A test of this difference has been important in previous When people ask me for a recommendation about a restaurant, I use social
media to make a recommendation for this restaurant.
marketing-related applications of attachment theory (e.g., Park When I know someone is looking for a restaurant, I use social media to urge
et al. 2010). This comparison also provides an additional them to check into this restaurant.
test of discriminant validity for ASM while simultaneously Without anyone even mentioning restaurants, I find myself telling others on
testing for the greater predictive power of a theory-driven social media about the positive experiences I have had with this restaurant.
attachment scale as compared to a generic measure of favorable Because of my experiences with this restaurant, I try to convince friends, family, and
coworkers on social media to switch to this restaurant for their dinning needs.
attitude.
(b) Social media supportive behaviors adapted from Bettencourt (1997)
Methodology
I would use social media to let this restaurant know of ways that they can better
serve my needs.
Participants and Design. Data were collected as part of a I would use social media to make constructive suggestions to this restaurant on
larger marketing research study in collaboration with a cluster how to improve their service.
of restaurants that were newly built in a southwestern city that If I have a useful idea on how to improve service, I would let this restaurant
know on social media.
borders a large urban university and an existing commercial
When I experience a problem with this restaurant, I would say something to
district. Members of several groups from the university and the them on social media so they can improve their service.
community were invited to participate in this study. Commu- If I notice a problem, I would inform this restaurant on social media even if it
nity group administrators e-mailed a survey invitation to their does not affect me.
membership rosters on our behalf. We were not given access If this restaurant's price is incorrect to my advantage, I would still use social
media to advise someone at the restaurant.
to these confidential lists, nor their size. However, from this
If I had a good experience with this restaurant I would say something to the
invitation process, 146 community group members initially restaurant on social media.
responded (58% were female, average age 52, ranging from
20–83). Of these, 120 respondents had top-of-mind awareness
of the new restaurant cluster and of those 94 provided complete
data. For our purposes in Study 4, we focused on the non-
student, community member stakeholder group. We infused
several construct measures into the data collection effort with (ATT) served as two independent variables, each also measured
this group. with multiple-item indicators. We allowed ASM and ATT to
simultaneously compete for explanatory power in predicting
Measures. The 27 items for ASM were assessed on 7-point the C2C and C2B outcomes.
Likert scales (1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”). Our relatively small sample size precluded use of tradi-
To contrast ASM from a general measure of attitude toward tional structural equations modeling (SEM) for estimation
social media, participants also indicated on 6-point semantic purposes and led us to select Partial Least Squares (PLS) as
differential scales the extent to which they viewed social media a modeling approach better suited to smaller sample size
as “good” versus “bad,” “positive” versus “negative,” and the applications (Fornell and Bookstein 1982; Hair et al. 2010). We
extent to which they “like it” versus “dislike it” (Batra and used SmartPLS 2.0 (Ringle, Wende, and Will 2005) to conduct
Stayman 1990). the estimation. The SmartPLS program generates traditional
Regarding dependent measures, we measured social media SEM measurement model statistics and also offers the ad-
based C2C advocacy by adapting four items from White and vantage of a built-in empirical bootstrapping algorithm to
Schneider (2000, see Table 4), keeping their 7-point rating scales produce standard errors and statistical tests of the estimated
(1 = “describes me very well” and 7 = “does not describe me model parameters. Our estimated model for Study 4 is shown in
well at all”). Regarding C2B social media supportive communi- Fig. 3.7
cation behaviors, we adapted Bettencourt's (1997, see Table 4)
voluntary participation measures because of their emphasis Results
on active involvement with and support for an organization.
We considered these items to be reflective of some of the Outer Model Results. PLS-generated measurement model
ways consumers could supportively communicate with organi- statistics all met acceptable SEM standards. Regarding the
zations via social media. Per Bettencourt (1997), each was
measured on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = “very likely” and 7 =
“very unlikely”).
7
We also conducted parallel estimation with Multivariate Multiple Linear
Analytical Approach. C2C advocacy and C2B supportive Regression (Finn 1974; Lutz and Eckert 1994). While MMLR produced
equivalent statistical and substantive results we felt PLS offered superior
communication behavior served as two dependent variables estimation given its ability to operationalize latent variables as multi-item
each measured with multiple-item indicators. Attachment to indicator constructs. Application of MMLR required computation of composite
Social Media (ASM) and general attitude toward social media variable indices prior to analysis.
R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88 81

Fig. 3. Study 4: ASM and ATT simultaneously predicting C2C and C2B social media behavioral intentions.

dependent constructs of C2C advocacy and C2B supportive t = 5.09). Further, these ASM-related effects showed dominant
communication, AVEs were .85 and .73 respectively. AVEs for predictive power in contrast to general attitude toward social
the independent constructs of ASM and ATT were respectively media (ATT). ATT was non-significant in predicting both C2C
.52 and .88. Construct reliabilities (CR) were as follows: C2C advocacy (γ = .129, bootstrap t = 1.24) and in predicting
advocacy = .96; C2B supportive communication behaviors = C2B supportive communication behaviors (γ = .086, bootstrap
.95; ASM = .90; ATT = .96. Cronbach's alpha values for C2C t = .710).
advocacy, C2B supportive communication behaviors, ASM,
and ATT were .94, .94, .87, and .94, respectively. The squared
Discussion
correlation among each pair of constructs was always less than
In Study 4 we applied the ASM measure in an externally-valid
the AVE values for the given constructs in the pair, indicating
restaurant/retail context using a non-student sample. We predicted
reasonable support for discriminant validity. Further, the squared
marketing-relevant social media outcomes of C2C advocacy and
correlation of ASM and ATT was .31, indicating sufficient and
C2B supportive communication behaviors using ASM and a
substantial unique variance for a competing comparative test of
competing explanatory variable of general attitude toward social
ASM and ATT in the inner model.8
media. Our theory-driven measure of attachment to social media
showed significant and superior predictive power relative to a
Inner Model Results. Meaningful proportions of variance
generic measure of attitude toward social media. ASM signifi-
were explained in both dependent variables of interest. The
cantly predicts C2C and C2B social media behaviors even
PLS-reported R-square was .41 for C2C advocacy, and .36
after taking into account general attitude toward social media.
for C2B supportive communication behaviors. Regarding the
Additionally, general attitude toward social media does not in-
structural paths of Fig. 3, attachment to social media (ASM)
crementally predict C2C social media advocacy or C2B social
showed sizable and statistically significant effects in predicting
media supportive communication above and beyond the substan-
C2C advocacy (γ = .588, bootstrap t = 7.05) and in predicting
tial variance explained by ASM since results indicated that the
C2B supportive communication behaviors (γ = .509, bootstrap
attitude measure was not statistically significant in predicting
either social media advocacy or supportive communication.
Study 4 further supports ASM's applied usefulness in a real
8
marketing context with a non-student sample. Hypotheses H2
Multicollinearity diagnostics for this level of correlation among ASM and and H3 were fully supported. Those who are more strongly
ATT would be TOL = .643, VIF = 1.56. These are well below thresholds at
which any concerns arise regarding potential redundancy. ASM is sufficiently attached to social media showed greater propensity to express
distinct from general attitude toward social media. See Appendix D for positive C2C word-of-mouth via social media and higher levels
descriptive statistics and correlations among the variables in the model. of C2B supportive communication behaviors via social media.
82 R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88

These effects were not simply attributable to general attitudes and consumer-related domains. For each domain, respondents
toward social media. were asked to check all boxes that applied for any behaviors they
had engaged in during the previous two weeks (see Appendices
Study 5 E and F). The more boxes a respondent checked in a given life
domain, the more behaviorally active in social media they were
Study 4 demonstrated that those who are more attached to in that domain. Summated counts of checked boxes for each
social media are more likely to interact with other consumers respondent in each life domain produced two distinct outcome
via social media about a brand or organization, as well as more measures of social media activity. It should be noted that this
likely to use social media to interact with the brand or or- check-box behavioral self-report approach also serves to measure
ganization itself. However, the dependent constructs in each the two distinct dependent variables in a methodologically
case involved scaled intention-type measures. In Study 5 we different way from ASM. Thus, concerns about common method
seek to move past intention measures to outcomes involving bias in any observed relationships with ASM should be alleviated
respondent self-reports of actual social media behaviors. under this design strategy.
We developed two self-report behavioral checklists, one for a
general social life domain and one for a consumer-related life Analytical Approach. The number of behaviors checked in a
domain. In each domain, a list of several possible social media given life domain for any individual produces a count measure
actions was provided. Participants were asked to respond with a for a dependent variable — a nonnegative integer with a
“check all that you've done in the last two weeks” approach. We restricted range that violates the assumptions of ordinary least
chose two substantially varied life domains to further confirm squares regression. A Poisson regression model is appropriate
the generalizability of ASM, while showing relevance not only for count regression, but assumes equality of variance and
for social media in traditional interpersonal manifestations, but mean in the data. When overdispersion occurs with greater
also in consumer-related manifestations. Thus, we formally test variance than the mean, a negative binomial model is more
H4a and H4b proposing that those who are more strongly appropriate (Long 1997). Unlike the Poisson model, it does not
attached to social media should be notably more behaviorally rely on assumed equivalence between the mean and variance of
active on social media, both in socially- and consumer-related the number of behaviors, making it a more desirable model in
activities. many cases (Long and Freese 2006; Park, Chen, and Gallagher
Additionally, we control for another rival predictor of these 2002). Tests for overdispersion in both of our models revealed
two domains of outcome behaviors: general amount of time that negative binomial regression was the more appropriate
spent on social media. Our reasoning was that more time spent analytical procedure (Long and Freese 2006). Therefore, we
on social media logically could produce more behavioral applied negative binomial models for the self-reported number
manifestations, but that more time spent did not necessarily of behaviors an individual preformed via social media in two
equate to greater affinity or attachment. By statistically including different life domains while testing the rival predictor of time
a self-reported measure of time spent on social media, we spent on social media.
eliminate a rival hypothesis and model how attachment to social In our Hypotheses H4a and H4b, we are particularly interested
media uniquely translates into social- and consumer-related in the behavioral activity of those who are strongly attached to
social media behaviors. social media. We propose that individuals who are more strongly
attached to social media will show especially high levels of social
Methodology media activity in the social- and consumer-related domains.
If these relationships are confirmed, marketers have a defined
Participants and Design. Two hundred fifty-eight undergrad- psychographic group to target — individuals strongly attached to
uate business students (47% female, average age 24, age range social media — who will display significantly more desired
18–49) completed an online survey in exchange for partial social media behaviors, especially of the kind most relevant to
course credit. Thirty-six respondents were dropped because marketers.
they did not provide complete data leaving a working sample We conducted negative binomial regression with the count
size of 222. variable predicted by the continuous variables of ASM and
the rival predictor of time spent on social media. We ran one
Measures. Each respondent was asked which of several social
media platforms they currently use, and for those mentioned, to
report the average amount of time they spend per week on each Table 5
of these social media platforms. The respective amounts of time Study 5: Negative binomial parameter estimates for social media behaviors.
were summed across all reported platforms for a total average Parameter B Std. error Sig. Exp(B)
amount of time spent per week on social media. Additionally, the
Social behaviors (Intercept) .776 .155 .000 2.17
survey contained the items measuring ASM asked with 7-point Time spent .004 .002 .067 1.00
Likert scales (1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”) in ASM .334 .038 .000 1.40
the same manner used in our previous studies. Two distinct Brand behaviors (Intercept) − .240 .273 .379 .79
checklists of social media actions were also presented to Time spent .000 .004 .950 1.00
ASM .321 .066 .000 1.38
respondents asking about their social media behaviors in social-
R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88 83

The results of these models confirm H4a and H4b and are
made visually clear by examining the estimated marginal means
from the negative binomial models. These are the predicted
number of social- and consumer-related behaviors at the mean of
ASM, as well as at plus and minus one standard deviation from
the mean, after controlling for average time spent on social media.
Fig. 4 shows graphs of these estimated marginal means for
(a) social-related behaviors and (b) consumer-related behaviors.
The graphs demonstrate generally increasing patterns of
behavioral activity via social media. As one becomes more
attached to social media, significant gains in social behaviors are
observed. Regarding consumer behaviors, we see similar results.
Stronger attachment relates to a sharp increase in the number of
observed consumer-related behaviors on social media.

Discussion
In Study 5, we extend the findings from Study 4 on C2C
advocacy intentions and C2B supportive communication inten-
tions to focus on self-reported actual social media behaviors in the
social and consumer domains. Results indicate support for H4a
and H4b, in that individuals who are more strongly attached to
social media showed more behavioral activities on social media
and specifically, more consumer related behaviors. These included
items such as: Shared when I was at a company's location, “Liked”
a company or brand's post, Bought something because of what I
read on company or brand's social media page, or Advocated for a
company or brand on my personal social media page. Additionally,
we find that time spent on social media is a marginal predictor of
Fig. 4. Study 5: Estimated marginal means of behavior counts as a function of social behaviors via social media; however, the key finding for
ASM levels. (a) Social-related behaviors. (b) Consumer-related behaviors. marketers is time spent on social media is not predictive of
interaction or engagement with brands via social media. Therefore,
by concentrating on those who are more strongly attached to social
media, a company can determine in advance individuals who are
negative binomial model predicting counts of total self-reported more likely to interact and engage with their brand, company, or
social-related actions on social media in the last two weeks and a organization via social media. The same cannot be said for
second model predicting counts of total self-reported consumer- individuals who merely spend greater amounts of time on social
related actions on social media in the last two weeks in order to media in general.
specifically address Hypotheses H4a and H4b.
General Discussion
Results
The likelihood ratio chi-squares involving tests of the Through a programmatic set of five studies, we conceptu-
overall model compared to a null model are significant for alized a measure of consumers' attachment to social media.
both dependent variables. For the model predicting social Our operationalization includes eight distinct elements: (1)
behaviors the likelihood chi-square was 95.7(2), p-value ≤ .000 connecting — use of social media to stay connected to others,
and for the model predicting consumer-related behaviors the (2) nostalgia — the ability to use social media in order to
likelihood chi-square was 26.1(2), p-value ≤ .000. Additionally, remember things from the past, (3) informed — social media's
the test of model effects shows that attachment to social media is a role in keeping an individual informed, (4) enjoyment — social
significant predictor of social behaviors: Nagelkerke R-square = media's role in helping an individual to experience relaxation
.35, W = 112.9, df = 1, p-value ≤ .000; and consumer behav- and enjoyment, (5) advice — an individual's use of social
iors: Nagelkerke R-square = 0.11, W = 28.6, df = 1, p-value ≤ media to seek advice from others, (6) affirmation — an
.000. Additionally, we see that time spent on social media is a individual feeling assured and supported from social media
marginally significant predictor of social behaviors via social usage, (7) enhances my life — social media's role in enhancing
media but is not a significant predicator of consumer behaviors a person's life, and (8) influence — the ability to use social
via social media. The results indicate that being more strongly media to encourage, influence, and help others. These eight
attached to social media compared to being less attached is a dimensions serve as indicators of a second-order measure of
significant predictor of both social and consumer behaviors (see ASM. We provide preliminary empirical evidence for the
Table 5). This result is consistent across models. reliability and validity of the ASM measure, and test its
84 R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88

usefulness in predicting specific outcomes of interest to social media work together to drive brand-specific social media
marketers. All hypotheses received statistical support in our activities. For example, is someone's attachment to the brand more
modeling efforts. Theoretically, our work contributes to an of what drives his/her participation and advocacy with the brand
expansion of attachment theory into a new substantive domain via social media, or is it attachment to social media, or is it both,
within marketing, namely social media. Managerially, our results possibly even with one moderating the effects of the other? If both
show that ASM predicts C2C advocacy and C2B supportive have additive or interactive effects, simultaneous study of the
communication behaviors via social media, as well as social constructs together might shed further light on what is driving
media behaviors in social- and consumer-related domains. various consumer social media activities.
As marketers seek ways to understand and leverage social Additionally, engagement has emerged as an important mar-
media phenomenon, at least one trend is to focus on aggregate keting objective and performance metric (Mersey, Malthouse,
streams of big data from the social media “fire hose.” Identifiable and Calder 2010; MSI 2013). Recently, formal measures of
individual-level data is often limited to geo-demographic char- online and social media engagement have emerged (Calder,
acteristics from host sites, or through various scraping, ag- Malthouse, and Schaedel 2009; de Valck, van Bruggen, and
gregation and analysis tools. However, psychologically-oriented Wierenga 2009; Hollebeek, Glynn, and Brodie 2014; Van der
variables capturing psychographic characteristics may be more Lans et al. 2010); however, there seems to be two competing
relevant in relation to consumer behavior. Attachment to social definitions of social media engagement. Hollebeek, Glynn, and
media, as operationalized by the ASM measure, offers a dis- Brodie (2014) define engagement as having a behavioral com-
tinct psychologically-based individual difference variable which ponent while the root of all Calder and Malthouse's work
relates meaningfully to important social media phenomena. does not have a behavioral component. The discrepancy in the
Further, the predictive power of ASM is demonstrated to be definition of social media engagement first needs to be addressed
incremental above other rival measures of general attitude toward then, the relationship between ASM and social media engage-
social media and time spent on social media. Marketers seeking ment can be investigated.
to cognize consumer behaviors such as advocacy or supportive Finally, we have shown that those who are more strongly
communication back to the organization via social media can attached to social media are more likely to positively interact
benefit from this quantitative individual-level variable. with companies and/or organizations via social media. To that
Those who are more strongly attached to social media would extent, we also only investigated consumer's positive advocacy
be especially desirable regarding marketing initiatives and and supportive communications via social media. Clearly,
campaigns designed for social media. These individuals are not however, not all communication via social media is positive.
only more likely to appreciate social media based relationships Future research should investigate whether the individual trait
with organizations, but also are more likely to participate with ASM also predicts greater levels of negativity on social media
the company, to offer input to the organization via social media, such as the likelihood to complain about brands, organizations,
and to advocate on behalf of the company to others via social or even other consumers via social media, as well as the
media. Their more extensive social activities on social media propensity to share negative WOM via social networks. This
also offer broader and more intensive reach whenever their would extend the negative word-of-mouth literature (Edison
consumer generated content is used to promote a brand or and Geissler 2011; Mattila and Wirtz 2004; Zhang, Feick, and
organization. Thus, by segmenting and targeting individuals Mittal 2014) directly in the social media context (King,
who are more strongly attached to social media, companies' Racherla, and Bush 2014). The comparison and investigation
social media efforts could become much more efficient and of negative WOM are outside the scope of this particular paper
effective. ASM offers managers a measurable way to segment but are worthy of future investigation. With the introduction of
and subsequently demonstrate the impact of marketing efforts our research to the literature, a measure of ASM now exists
in the social media space. ASM offers a mechanism by which where these and other emerging substantive research questions
to demonstrate how marketing efforts in social media translate can be explored. For now, ASM offers promise in marketing
into desirable customer outcomes that ultimately impact ROI. contexts in helping to identify which customers are most likely
to be listening, engaging, and reacting on social media.
Limitations & Future Research
Acknowledgements
Future psychometric work would also bolster the case for the
attachment to social media measure, and its applicability to other The authors thank Dr. Dawn Iacobucci (Vanderbilt University)
marketing studies. For example, tests of content validity might Dr. Gordon Bruner II (Southern Illinois University), Dr. Traci
have raters/judges sort items under subscale definitions. Also, Freling (University of Texas-Arlington), Dr. Wendy Casper
longitudinal data could provide additional evidence for reliability (University of Texas-Arlington), Dr. Emily Goad (Illinois State
by examining test–retest stability. Finally, it would be helpful to University), Holly Syrdal (University of Texas-Arlington), and
have a short version of the new measure. Certainly, use of ASM in Dr. Marcus Butts (University of Texas-Arlington) for their
applied and academic studies would benefit from a short scale. insightful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. A
Our work on attachment to social media leverages previous version of the article was presented at the Society for Marketing
work on attachment to brands. A logical next step would be to Advances Conference in October 2013. The audience comments
combine these to see how attachment to brands and attachment to are gratefully acknowledged.
R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88 85

Appendix A. Study 2: Factor Intercorrelations and Descriptive Statistics

Connecting Informed Nostalgia Enjoyment Enhances My Life Advice Affirmed Influence


Connecting
Informed .52
Nostalgia .54 .46
Enjoyment .52 .47 .50
Enhances My Life .43 .51 .49 .64
Advice .22 .47 .37 .41 .57
Affirmed .50 .42 .56 .57 .62 .45
Influence .40 .39 .44 .36 .43 .43 .48
Mean 5.44 3.70 4.61 4.64 3.94 2.85 3.98 3.88
Std. deviation 1.30 1.37 1.36 1.34 1.39 1.36 1.42 1.52

Appendix B. Study 2: Full Pattern Matrix, Replication (Non-student) Sample

Component
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
My life is a little richer because of social media. .064 .001 .921 − .009 .010 .043 − .027 − .042
Social media enhances my life. .037 − .036 .876 − .019 .028 − .027 .013 .067
Social media makes my life a little bit better. − .061 .058 .917 − .020 .025 − .033 .008 .018
I use social media to interact with friends. .096 .080 .080 .079 .807 − .058 − .013 − .020
Social media provides a way for me to stay connected to people across distances. − .039 .007 − .005 − .020 .956 .017 .002 .009
I use social media because it makes staying in touch with others convenient. .021 .007 .011 − .086 .872 .068 − .057 .034
Social media provides a way for me to keep in touch with others that I care about. .020 − .062 .062 − .031 .767 − .046 − .091 .079
Sometimes I post things just to have a positive effect on other peoples' moods. − .068 − .020 .106 − .005 .001 − .720 − .209 .137
I post on social media to brighten other peoples' day. − .020 − .024 .155 − .019 − .060 − .704 − .178 .150
I post things on social media that I think will be helpful to my friends' lives. .064 .070 .034 − .226 .128 − .676 .063 − .062
I want to inspire other people with my social media posts. .164 .160 − .031 − .011 .032 − .737 − .067 − .001
I think it is important to share things on social media so those I care about stay informed. .199 .207 .022 − .143 .125 − .504 .162 .105
I seek advice for upcoming decisions using social media. .015 .768 .073 .028 .101 − .132 − .057 .026
If I'm unsure about an upcoming decision I get input from friends on social media. − .019 .838 .019 − .036 .099 − .097 − .034 .005
I get advice about medical questions on social media. .017 .886 .003 − .030 − .094 .087 − .008 .095
Social media allows me to look back at meaningful events, people, and places from my past. .019 .036 − .002 − .045 .194 − .016 − .745 .011
Using social media makes me feel nostalgic about things that I have done in the past. .052 .086 .086 − .069 − .059 − .021 − .828 .006
Sometimes social media reminds me of warm memories from my past. .099 − .025 − .013 − .053 .107 − .113 − .709 .065
When others comment on my posts I feel affirmed. .006 − .057 .071 − .849 .058 − .087 .025 .002
When people respond to my posts in social media I feel like they care about me. − .017 − .006 − .024 − .900 − .015 − .044 − .046 .097
It makes me feel accepted when people comment on my social media posts. .047 .075 .027 − .914 − .012 .081 − .056 − .029
Social media is one of the main ways I get information about major events. − .033 .028 − .005 − .076 .054 − .029 − .038 .853
Social media allows me to stay informed about events and news. .102 − .050 .028 − .037 .093 .002 − .063 .790
Social media is one of my primary sources of information about news. .039 .122 .045 .023 − .052 .000 .051 .881
I use social media as a way for me to de-stress after a long day. .757 .133 .132 − .079 − .049 .073 − .107 − .030
I use social media to give myself a break when I've been busy. .836 − .048 − .047 − .006 .090 − .069 − .045 .096
Social media is an enjoyable way to spend time. .626 − .083 .148 − .049 .091 − .115 − .002 .154
Extraction method: Principal Component Analysis.
Rotation method: Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization.
a. Rotation converged in 14 iterations.

Appendix C. Study 2: KMO and Bartlett's Test

Purification sample Replication sample


KMO Measure of Sampling Adequacy .926 .945
Bartlett's Test of Sphericity Approx. chi-square 4,553.00 7,259.06
df 351 351
Sig. .000 .000
86 R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88

Appendix D. Study 4: Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among Constructs

Correlations among constructs


Mean Standard deviation Composite reliability Average variance extracted ASM ATT C2C advocacy C2B support
ASM 4.62 .95 .90 .52 1.00
ATT 5.71 .93 .96 .88 .56 1.00
C2C advocacy 3.27 1.51 .96 .85 .70 .42 1.00
C2B support 4.05 1.46 .95 .73 .53 .35 .45 1.00

Appendix E. Study 5: Social-Related Checklist None of these in the last 2 weeks.

Below we use the word “post” to refer to the general ability Appendix F. Study 5: Consumer-Related Checklist
to share content and “page” refers to the general social media
platform, with the understanding that different platforms refer to Now we would like you to think about Brands and
the “page” differently (i.e., Twitter feed, Pinterest pinboard, Companies in general. Below are some activities that people
etc.). do when using social media. Please indicate which of these you
Listed below are some activities that people do when using have done in the last 2 weeks associated with brands and
social media. companies.
Please indicate which of these you have done in the last Below we use the word “post” to refer to the general ability
2 weeks associated with your social life. Please check all that to share content and “page” refers to the general social media
apply. platform, with the understanding that different platforms refer
Looked at what my friends posted. to the “page” differently (i.e., Twitter feed, Pinterest pinboard,
Shared new trend in fashion, music, and etc. social media. etc.).
Felt better about myself after reading someone else's post. Please check all that apply.
Liked a friends post. Shared when I was at a company's location.
Sought advice from my friends. Made a positive commented on a company or brand's social
Felt worse about myself after reading someone else's post. media page.
Commented on a friends post. Shared a photo on a company or brand's social media page.
Looked at old pictures from my photos. Looked at a company or brands' social media page.
Became mad or frustrated after reading someone else's Made a negative comment on a company or brand's social
post. media page.
Re-posted/shared a friends post. Changed my mind based on something I saw on a company
Looked at old pictures of my friends. or brand's social media page.
De-friended someone. Learned information from a company or brand's social media
Shared my location. page.
Re-posted a story that touched me. Bashed a company or brand on my personal social media
Shared a photo. page.
Looked at someone else's location. “Unliked” a company or brand's social media page.
Changed my mind based on something I've read on social “Liked” a company or brand's social media page.
media. Advocated for a company or brand on my personal social
Re-posted a photo a friend posted. media page.
Facebook “stalked” a friend. Participated in a company or brand's contest.
Encouraged a friend. Read a company or brand's post.
Invited others to events. Asked a question on a company or brand's social media
Looked at posts of someone I'm not friends with. page.
Reconnected with someone from my past. Bought something because of what I read on company or
RSVP'd to attend an event. brand's social media page.
Became friends with someone new. “Liked” a company or brand's post.
Connected with a celebrity. Looked for new trends in fashion, music, etc. on a company
Looked for new trends in fashion, music, etc. on social or brand's social media page.
media. RSVP'd to attend a company or brand's event.
R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88 87

Shared a company or brand's post. Ehrenberg, Alexandra, Suzanna Juckes, Katherine M. White, and Shari P.
Looked at pictures on a company or brand's social media Walsh (2008), “Personality and Self-esteem as Predictors of Young
People's Technology Use,” CyberPsychology & Behavior, 11, 6, 739–41.
page. Fennis, Bob M., Ad Th. H. Pruyn, and Mascha Maasland (2005), “Revisiting
None of these in the last 2 weeks. the Malleable Self: Brand Effects on Consumer Self-perceptions of
Personality Traits,” Advances in Consumer Research, 32, 1, 371–7.
Finn, Jeremy D. (1974), A General Model for Multivariate Analysis. Orlando:
References Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Fornell, Claes and David F. Larcker (1981), “Evaluating Structural Equation
Aaker, David A. (2008), Strategic Market Management. John Wiley & Sons. Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error,” Journal of
Agnihotri, Raj, Prabakar Kothandaraman, Rajiv Kashyap, and Ramendra Singh Marketing Research, 18, 39–50.
(2012), “Bringing “Social” into Sales: The Impact of Salespeople's Social ——— and Fred L. Bookstein (1982), “Two Structural Equation Models:
Media Use on Service Behaviors and Value Creation,” Journal of Personal LISREL and PLS Applied to Consumer Exit-voice Theory,” Journal of
Selling & Sales Management, 32, 3, 333–48. Marketing Research, 19, 4, 440–52.
Ainsworth, Mary D. Salter and Silvia M. Bell (1970), “Attachment, Fraley, R. Chris and Keith E. Davis (1997), “Attachment Formation and
Exploration, and Separation: Illustrated by the Behavior of One-year-olds Transfer in Young Adults' Close Friendships and Romantic Relationships,”
in a Strange Situation,” Child Development, 41, 1, 49–67. Personal Relationships, 4, 2, 131–44.
Alexandrov, Aliosha, Bryan Lilly, and Emin Babakus (2013), “The Effects of Gensler, Sonja, Franziska Völckner, Yuping Liu-Thompkins, and Caroline
Social- and Self-motives on the Intentions to Share Positive and Negative Wiertz (2013), “Managing Brands in the Social Media Environment,”
Word of Mouth,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 41, 5, Journal of Interactive Marketing, 27, 4, 242–56.
531–46. Gillan, Paul (2010), “The New Conversation: Taking Social Media from Talk to
Ball, A. Dwayne and Lori H. Tasaki (1992), “The Role and Measurement of Action,” Harvard Business Review, 1–24.
Attachment in Consumer Behavior,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1, 2, Hair, Joseph F., William C. Black, Barry J. Babin, Rolph E. Anderson, and
155–72. Ronald L. Tatham (2010), Multivariate Data Analysis, Vol. 7. Upper Saddle
Batra, Rajeev and Douglas M. Stayman (1990), “The Role of Mood in River: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Advertising Effectiveness,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17, 2, 203–14. Hazan, Cindy and Debra Zeifman (1994), “Sex and the Psychological Tether,” in
Bearden, William O., David M. Hardesty, and Randall L. Rose (2001), “Consumer Advances in Personal Relationships, Vol. 5. Kim Bartholomew, Daniel Perlman,
Self‐confidence: Refinements in Conceptualization and Measurement,” Journal editors. London: Jessica Kingsley, 151–78.
of Consumer Research, 28, 1, 121–34. ——— and Phillip R. Shaver (1994), “Attachment as an Organizational
Berger, Jonah and Katherine L. Milkman (2012), “What Makes Online Content Framework for Research on Close Relationships,” Psychological Inquiry, 5,
Viral?” Journal of Marketing Research, 49, 2, 192–205. 1, 1–22.
Bettencourt, Lance A. (1997), “Customer Voluntary Performance: Customers ——— and Debra Zeifman (1999), “Pair Bonds as Attachments,” in Handbook
as Partners in Service Delivery,” Journal of Retailing, 73, 3, 383–406. of Attachment, Jude Cassidy, Phillip R. Shaver, editors. New York:
Blazevic, Vera, Caroline Wiertz, June Cotte, Ko de Ruyter, and Debbie Isobel Guilford, 336–54.
Keeling (2014), “GOSIP in Cyberspace: Conceptualization and Scale Hoffman, Donna L. and Marek Fodor (2010), “Can you Measure the ROI of
Development for General Online Social Interaction Propensity,” Journal of Your Social Media Marketing?” Sloan Management Review, 52, 1, 41–9.
Interactive Marketing, 28, 2, 87–100. Hollebeek, Linda D., Mark S. Glynn, and Roderick J. Brodie (2014),
Bowlby, John (1969), Attachment and Loss: Attachment, Vol. 1. Reading: “Consumer Brand Engagement in Social Media: Conceptualization, Scale
Addison-Wesley. Development and Validation,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 28, 2,
——— (1979), The Making and Breaking of Affectional Bonds. London: 149–65.
Tavistock. Hollenbeck, Candice R. and Andrew M. Kaikati (2012), “Consumers' Use of
——— (1980), Attachment and Loss: Sadness and Depression, Vol. 3. New Brands to Reflect Their Actual and Ideal Selves on Facebook,”
York: Basic Books. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 29, 4, 395–405.
Brocato, E. Deanne, Julie Baker, and Clay M. Voorhees (2014), “Creating Hu, Li-tze T. and Peter M. Bentler (1999), “Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in
Consumer Attachment to Retail Service Firms Through Sense of Place,” Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria Versus New Alterna-
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 1–21. tives,” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1,
Brown, Tom J., Thomas E. Barry, Peter A. Dacin, and Richard F. Gunst (2005), 1–55.
“Spreading the Word: Investigating Antecedents of Consumers' Positive Ipsos (2013), “Social Networking Eats Up 3+ Hours per Day for the Average
Word-of-Mouth Intentions and Behaviors in a Retailing Context,” Journal American User,” Retrieved January 05, 2013 from http://www.marketingcharts.
of the Academy of Marketing Science, 33, 2, 123–38. com/wp/interactive/social-networking-eats-up-3-hours-per-day-for-the-average-
Calder, Bobby J., Edward C. Malthouse, and Ute Schaedel (2009), “An american-user-26049/.
Experimental Study of the Relationship Between Online Engagement Kietzmann, Jan H., Kristopher Hermkens, Ian P. McCarthy, and Bruno S.
and Advertising Effectiveness,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 23, 4, Silvestre (2011), “Social Media? Get Serious! Understanding the Functional
321–31. Building Blocks of Social Media,” Business Horizons, 54, 3, 241–51.
Churchill Jr., Gilbert A. (1979), “A Paradigm for Developing Better Measures King, Robert Allen, Pradeep Racherla, and Victoria D. Bush (2014), “What We
of Marketing Constructs,” Journal of Marketing Research, 16, 1, 64–73. Know and Don't Know About Online Word-of-Mouth: A Review and
de Matos, Celso Augusto and Carlos Alberto Vargas Rossi (2008), “Word-of- Synthesis of the Literature,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 28, 3,
Mouth Communications in Marketing: A Meta-analytic Review of the 167–83.
Antecedents and Moderators,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Kleine, Susan Schultz, Robert E. Kleine III, and Chris T. Allen (1995), “How Is
Science, 36, 4, 578–96. a Possession“ Me” or “ Not Me”? Characterizing Types and an Antecedent
De Valck, Kristine, Gerrit H. Van Bruggen, and Berend Wierenga (2009), of Material Possession Attachment,” Journal of Consumer Research, 22, 3,
“Virtual Communities: A Marketing Perspective,” Decision Support Systems, 327–43.
47, 3, 185–203. Labrecque, Lauren I. (2014), “Fostering Consumer–Brand Relationships in
DeVellis, Robert F. (2003), Scale Development: Theory and Applications. Social Media Environments: The Role of Parasocial Interaction,” Journal of
(Applied Social Research Methods). London: Sage. Interactive Marketing, 28, 2, 134–48.
Edison, Steve W. and Gary L. Geissler (2011), “An Investigation of Negative Long, J. Scott (1997), “Regression Models for Categorical and Limited
Word-of-Mouth Communication Among Market Mavens,” Journal of Dependent Variables,” Advanced Quantitative Techniques in the Social
Behavioral Studies in Business, 3, 1–12. Sciences, 7.
88 R.A. VanMeter et al. / Journal of Interactive Marketing 32 (2015) 70–88

——— and Jeremy Freese (2006), Regression Models for Categorical Ringle, Christian M., Sven Wende, and Alexander Will (2005), SmartPLS
Dependent Variables Using Stata. College Station: Stata Press. Release 2.0. Hamburg, Germany: University of Hamburg.
Lutz, J. Gary and Tanya Eckert (1994), “The Relationship Between Canonical Schulze, Christian, Lisa Schöler, and Bernd Skiera (2014), “Not All Fun and
Correlation Analysis and Multivariate Multiple Regression,” Educational Games: Viral Marketing for Utilitarian Products,” Journal of Marketing, 78,
and Psychological Measurement, 54, 3, 666–75. 1, 1–19.
Malär, Lucia, Harley Krohmer, Wayne D. Hoyer, and Bettina Nyffenegger (2011), Shimp, Terence A. and Subhash Sharma (1987), “Consumer Ethnocentrism:
“Emotional Brand Attachment and Brand Personality: The Relative Importance Construction and Validation of the CETSCALE,” Journal of Marketing
of the Actual and the Ideal Self,” Journal of Marketing, 75, 4, 35–52. Research, 24, 3, 280–9.
Marketing Science Institute (2013), “MSI Call for Research Proposals on Social Slater, Jan S. (2001), “Collecting Brand Loyalty: A Comparative Analysis of
Interactions and Social Media Marketing,” Retrieved from http://www.msi. How Coca-Cola and Hallmark Use Collecting Behavior to Enhance Brand
org/uploads/files/2013-11-07_MSI_Social_Media_Competition.pdf. Loyalty,” Advances in Consumer Research, 28, 1, 362–9.
Mattila, Anna S. and Jochen Wirtz (2004), “Consumer Complaining to Firms: Soat, Molly (2014), Marketers Forge Ahead on Social Media Spending
The Determinants of Channel Choice,” Journal of Services Marketing, 18, Despite Uncertainties, Research Shows. Marketing News Weekly (Sep-
2, 147–55. tember, Retrieved from https://www.ama.org/publications/eNewsletters/
Mende, Martin, Ruth N. Bolton, and Mary Jo Bitner (2013), “Decoding Marketing-News-Weekly/Pages/cmo-survey.aspx).
Customer–Firm Relationships: How Attachment Styles Help Explain Stelzner, Michael A. (2014), How Marketers Are Using Social Media to Grow
Customers' Preferences for Closeness, Repurchase Intentions, and Changes Their Businesses. Research Report. Social Media Examiner (May 19).
in Relationship Breadth,” Journal of Marketing Research, 50, 1, 125–42. Stephen, Andrew T. and Jeff Galak (2012), “The Effects of Traditional and
Mersey, Rachel Davis, Edward C. Malthouse, and Bobby J. Calder (2010), Social Earned Media on Sales: A Study of a Microlending Marketplace,”
“Engagement with Online Media,” Journal of Media Business Studies, 7, 2, Journal of Marketing Research, 49, 5, 624–39.
39–56. Thomson, Matthew, Deborah J. MacInnis, and C. Whan Park (2005), “The Ties
Mick, David G. and Michelle DeMoss (1990), “Self-gifts: Phenomenological That Bind: Measuring the Strength of Consumers' Emotional Attachments
Insights from Four Contexts,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17, 3, 322–32. to Brands,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15, 1, 77–91.
Naylor, Rebecca Walker, Cait Poynor Lamberton, and Patricia M. West (2012), ——— (2006), “Human Brands: Investigating Antecedents to Consumers'
“Beyond the “Like” Button: The Impact of Mere Virtual Presence on Brand Strong Attachments to Celebrities,” Journal of Marketing, 70, 3, 104–19.
Evaluations and Purchase Intentions in Social Media Settings,” Journal of Trinke, Shanna J. and Kim Bartholomew (1997), “Hierarchies of Attachment
Marketing, 76, 6, 105–20. Relationships in Young Adulthood,” Journal of Social and Personal
Nunnally, Jum C. and Ira H. Bernstein (1994), Psychometric Theory. New Relationships, 14, 5, 603–25.
York: McGraw-Hill. Valkenburg, Patti M., Jochen Peter, and Alexander P. Schouten (2006), “Friend
Pan, Lee-Yun and Jyh-Shen Chiou (2011), “How Much Can You Trust Online Networking Sites and Their Relationship to Adolescents' Well-being and
Information? Cues for Perceived Trustworthiness of Consumer-generated Social Self-esteem,” CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9, 5, 584–90.
Online Information,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 25, 2, 67–74. Van der Lans, Gerrit, Ralf Van Bruggen, Jehoshua Eliashberg, and Berend
Parasuraman, Arun, Dhruv Grewal, and Ramu Krishnan (2006), Marketing Wierenga (2010), “A Viral Branching Model for Predicting the Spread of
Research. Boston: Cengage Learning. Electronic Word of Mouth,” Marketing Science, 29, 2, 348–65.
Park, Seung Ho, Roger Rongxin Chen, and Scott Gallagher (2002), “Firm Wang, Xia, Chunling Yu, and Yujie Wei (2012), “Social Media Peer
Resources as Moderators of the Relationship Between Market Growth and Communication and Impacts on Purchase Intentions: A Consumer
Strategic Alliances in Semiconductor Start-ups,” Academy of Management Socialization Framework,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 26, 4,
Journal, 45, 3, 527–45. 198–208.
Park, C. Whan, Deborah J. MacInnis, Joseph Priester, Andreas B. Eisingerich, White, Susan S. and Benjamin Schneider (2000), “Climbing the Commitment
and Dawn Iacobucci (2010), “Brand Attachment and Brand Attitude Ladder the Role of Expectations Disconfirmation on Customers' Behavioral
Strength: Conceptual and Empirical Differentiation of Two Critical Brand Intentions,” Journal of Service Research, 2, 3, 240–53.
Equity Drivers,” Journal of Marketing, 74, 6, 1–17. Wilcox, Keith and Andrew T. Stephen (2013), “Are Close Friends the Enemy?
Peters, Kay, Yubo Chen, Andreas M. Kaplan, Björn Ognibeni, and Koen Online Social Networks, Self-esteem, and Self-control,” Journal of Consumer
Pauwels (2013), “Social Media Metrics—A Framework and Guidelines for Research, 40, 1, 90–103.
Managing Social Media,” Journal of Interactive Marketing, 27, 4, 281–98. Zhang, Yinlong, Lawrence Feick, and Vikas Mittal (2014), “How Males and
Rapp, Adam, Lauren Skinner Beitelspacher, Dhruv Grewal, and Douglas E. Females Differ in Their Likelihood of Transmitting Negative Word of
Hughes (2013), “Understanding Social Media Effects Across Seller, Mouth,” Journal of Consumer Research, 40, 6, 1097–108.
Retailer, and Consumer Interactions,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing
Science, 41, 5, 547–66.
Richins, Marsha L. and Scott Dawson (1992), “A Consumer Values Orientation
for Materialism and Its Measurement: Scale Development and Validation,”
Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 3, 303–19.

Potrebbero piacerti anche