Sei sulla pagina 1di 5

Asian Journal of Psychiatry 5 (2012) 43–47

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Asian Journal of Psychiatry


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ajp

An examination of relationship between neurological soft signs


and neurocognition
Ram Kumar Solanki a, Mukesh Kumar Swami b,*, Paramjeet Singh a
a
Department of Psychiatry, SMS Medical College, Jaipur, India
b
Department of Psychiatry, RNT Medical College, Udaipur, India

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: Objective: Neurological soft signs (NSS) and cognitive function had been examined in schizophrenia, but
Received 30 August 2011 their relationship has remained elusive for several years. We examined the relationship between NSS and
Received in revised form 7 December 2011 cognitive functions in the present study.
Accepted 18 December 2011
Method: A cross sectional study was carried out. Subjects were drawn from first degree relatives of
schizophrenia patients, admitted as inpatient or attending as an outpatient. Controls were recruited by
Keywords: word of mouth from hospital staff and visitors of hospitalized patients. Those subjects who satisfied the
Neurocognition
screening process were subjected to Cambridge Neurological Inventory for soft sign assessment and digit
Neurological soft signs
span test, paired associate learning test (PALT) and visuo-spatial working memory matrix (VSWMM) for
Motor coordination
Visuo-spatial working memory cognitive function assessment. Correlation analysis and structural equation modeling (SEM) was used for
Structural equation modeling analysis.
Result: Significant negative correlation of primitive reflexes with PALT; of motor coordination with
VSWMM, working memory (WM) and cognitive index; of total NSS with WM and cognitive index among
first degree relatives. SEM showed that motor soft signs have important negative influence over WM.
Conclusion: The current findings indicate that NSS have significant negative effect on cognitive
functioning.
ß 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Background family members of subjects with schizophrenia (Hyde et al., 2007;


Sanders et al., 2006). Few studies suggested that NSS and
Neurological soft signs (NSS) have been known to be non- neurocognitive functions capture similar underlying deficits in
localizing (Quitkin et al., 1976) and indicative of generalized schizophrenia (Flashman et al., 1996; Wong et al., 1997). A recent
disruption in neural circuitry between cortical and subcortical study found significant correlation between these two and had
areas (Buchanan and Heinrichs, 1989). They may reflect a argued for common underlying substrate (Chan et al., 2009). A
phenotypic expression of neural dysmaturation and have been review (Bombin et al., 2005) suggested that neurological abnor-
proposed as an endophenotype for schizophrenia (Chan and malities may be the result of early (pre or perinatal) disturbances
Gottesman, 2008). Various neuroanatomical regions have been in brain function. They may remain silent for years, reappearing
correlated with NSS in neuroimaging studies indicating that they during adolescence in the form of neurological signs, predating the
might represent localized deficit and have relation with cognitive appearance of psychotic symptoms and possibly coinciding with
functioning (Hui et al., 2009). the occurrence of negative symptoms and cognitive impairment.
On the other hand, cognitive dysfunction is well known to be an Relation between NSS and cognitive functions is important
integral part of schizophrenia and to have prognostic significance since it may shed light on patho-physiology of schizophrenia.
as an endophenotype (Snitz et al., 2006). Further, if NSS is found to have relation with cognitive functions, it
NSS and cognitive function had been examined in schizophre- may prove useful as a clinical indicator of cognitive status.
nia, but their relationship has remained elusive for several years. Findings presented and discussed in this article are a byproduct
Studies had found that cognitive deficits are related to NSS in of our study of assessment of multiple risk markers of schizophre-
schizophrenia (Arango et al., 1999; Sanders et al., 2004) and among nia. In the current paper we examined the relationship between
NSS and cognitive functions, especially working memory, which is
a core cognitive feature of schizophrenia, has been implicated in
* Corresponding author at: 53/104, V T Road, Mansarovar, Jaipur 302020, India.
the pathogenesis of the schizophrenia and is a particularly strong
E-mail address: mukesh.swami@gmail.com (M.K. Swami). candidate of endophenotype (Gur et al., 2007). Among NSS, motor

1876-2018/$ – see front matter ß 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ajp.2011.12.006
44 R.K. Solanki et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 5 (2012) 43–47

signs have been shown to be more closely related to illness another one is red on a given card. In the two dimensional space of
(Bombin et al., 2005). Therefore, we have focused on these domains a card, these colored squares are situated at a different location in
in the present evaluation. each card. Stimulus is given to the subject according to standard
key for each card and the subject is asked to move RED square’s
2. Materials and methods position in his mind and correct responses are noted. Scores on DST
and VSWMM were summed to get score of working memory (WM)
A cross sectional study was carried out. Subjects were drawn and sum of all test scores were taken as cognitive index.
from first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients, admitted as
inpatient or as outpatients at our institution. The study included 50 2.3. Statistical analysis
consecutive first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients (the
diagnosis of all patients was reviewed and confirmed by two Basic statistical analysis was done with the help of SPSS 17
psychiatrists independently based on ICD-10 criteria) and 30 software. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted with
healthy controls. Controls were recruited by word of mouth from the LISREL 8.80 (student version) for Windows (Jöreskog and
hospital staff and visitors of hospitalized patients. Controls were Sörbom, 2006). For estimation, ‘unweighted least square’ (ULS)
assessed by clinical interview prior to inclusion in study to rule out method was used because of small sample size and the fact that it
any psychopathology. The study was approved by a research does not assume multivariate normality. The validity of the model
review board and ethical committee of the institution. An informed was tested with fit indices including normed fit index (NFI), non-
consent was obtained from the subject prior to participation in the normed fit index (NNFI), comparative fit index (CFI), incremental
study. To be included in study the subjects were screened with a fit index (IFI), and root mean square error of approximation
specially designed screening pro forma (consists of selection (RMSEA) and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR).
criteria). Those subjects who satisfied the screening process were These indices represent the fit between the assumed model and the
recruited into the study. Subjects of either sex, of age 18–65 years baseline model of uncorrelatedness between the observed vari-
and literate enough to read and understand the questionnaires ables. The first four fit indices values of .90 or above, and RMSEA
were included. Subjects with a lifetime history of any psychiatric and SRMR value of .08 or less indicated the model adequately fits
disorder, substance abuse, history of head injury with any the data (Hu and Bentler, 1999).
documented cognitive sequel or with loss of consciousness,
neurological disease or damage, mental retardation, or any medical 3. Results
illnesses that may significantly impair neurocognitive function
were excluded from the study. Finally, subjects having a first- Subjects in both groups were predominantly married, Hindu
degree relative with a psychotic disorder were excluded from the males, having income <Rs 15,000/- per month, and were living in
control group. joint families. The majority of subjects in both groups were
The subject’s sociodemographic data was recorded. After that educated above 8th standard, though controls had higher
each participant in the study was subjected to selected measures education. First degree relative subjects were farmer or workers
for assessment. living in rural areas while the majority of control subjects were
urban dwelling professionals.
2.1. The Cambridge Neurological Inventory: (Chen et al., 1995) First degree relatives (DST, Mean = 7.44, SD = 1.013; PALT,
Mean = 13.64, SD = 4.169; VSWMM, Mean = 2.18, SD = 2.283;
It is a schedule for standardized neurological assessment of primitive reflexes, Mean = .06, SD = .24; motor coordination,
psychiatric patients. Part 2 of the inventory is for soft sign Mean = 2.26, SD = 1.805; motor sequencing, Mean = 5.70,
examinations. There are mainly three groups of testing: (1) assess SD = 2.573; sensory integration, Mean = 3.68, SD = 2.299) scored
‘‘primitive reflexes’’; (2) assess motor coordination (MC) and significantly different from the control group (DST, Mean = 8.83,
repetitive sequential motor execution (MS); (3) assess integration SD = 1.783; PALT, Mean = 16.80, SD = 2.51; VSWMM, Mean = 4.83,
of sensory information (SI). Primitive reflexes consists of Grasp SD = 2.71; primitive reflexes, Mean = .00; motor coordination,
reflex, Snout reflex and Palmomental reflex. MC and MS is assessed Mean = .53, SD = 1.0; motor sequencing, Mean = 1.90, SD = 2.21;
with finger-nose test, finger-thumb tapping, finger-thumb oppo- sensory integration, Mean = 2.13, SD = 1.81) on all the measures.
sition, Diadochokinesis, fist-edge-palm test, oseretsky test, We used partial correlation to control for age and education
rhythm-tapping test and go/no-go test. Integration of SI is assessed which showed significant negative correlation of primitive reflexes
via extinction, finger agnosia, stereognosis, graphesthesia and left– with PALT; of motor coordination with VSSM, WM and cognitive
right orientation. index; of total NSS with WM and cognitive index among first
degree relatives (Table 1). No significant correlation was observed
2.2. Neurocognitive tests in the control group.
Based on the findings of partial correlation we hypothesized a
Digit span test (DST) (Wechsler, 1981) was used as a measure of model of relation between motor NSS and WM and tested the
verbal working memory. It has two parts - digit forward test and model with SEM. The SEM consists of two models, namely the
digit backward test. Paired associate learning test (PALT) (Pershad measurement model and the structure model (Anderson and
and Wig, 1984) is a sub-test of Post Graduate Institute (PGI) Gerbing, 1988). The measurement model shows the relations
memory scale used to assess auditory-verbal memory on indices of between the latent variables and their indicators, whereas the
immediate recall and recognition. This is a cued recall test of verbal structure model shows the potential causal dependencies between
memory. The test consists of two series of associative pairs (5 pair variables. In the current study, the measurement model was based
each). One is for the similar pair and another for dissimilar pair. on four factors consisting of two latent variables, namely WM, and
Visuo-spatial working memory matrix (VSWMM) (Vecchi et al., NSS. DST and VSWMM were ascribed to ‘‘working memory’’. On the
1995; Roopesh, 2000) was used to measure the capacity of a other hand, the MC and motor sequencing were ascribed to
subject’s visuo-spatial working memory. It assesses two different ‘‘neurological soft sign’’. The structural model shows the relation-
components: passive store and active imagery operation. The test ships between the WM and the latent variable of NSS. Analysis was
consists of ten 4  4 matrices each printed on a different card in conducted separately for the first degree relatives and healthy
bright homogenous color. Two consecutive squares are yellow and volunteers. Raw data were used as input data for analysis (Table 4).
R.K. Solanki et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 5 (2012) 43–47 45

Table 1
Result of partial correlation between neurological soft signs and cognitive tests.

Control variable DST PALT VSWMM WM Cognitive index

Age and education First degree relatives (n = 50)


Primitive reflexes r .181 .376** .108 .021 .269
Motor coordination r .172 .119 .319* .337* .288*
Motor sequencing r .188 .192 .159 .208 .286*
Sensory integration r .055 .016 .158 .155 .098
Total NSS r .176 .168 .274 .301* .310*
Controls (n = 30)
Motor coordination r .163 .072 .082 .022 .041
Motor sequencing r .063 .080 .201 .107 .095
Sensory integration r .307 .246 .236 .306 .306
Total NSS r .241 .194 .027 .096 .109

r, correlation coefficient; DST, digit span test; NSS, neurological soft signs; PALT, paired associative learning test; VSWMM, visuo-spatial working memory matrix; WM,
working memory.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.

3.1. Testing the measurement model Table 2


Factor loadings for the measurement model in first degree relatives and control
group (LISREL output).
The results (Table 2 and Figs. 1 and 2) showed that the
measurement model fit the data relatively well. All of the loadings Observed variable Estimate SE Wald R2
statistics
of the observed variables on corresponding latent variables were
statistically significant. Thus, all of the latent variables appeared to Motor coordination R 1.86 0.17 10.78 1.06
have been adequately measured by their respective observed C 0.81 0.13 5.90 0.35
Motor sequencing R 1.53 0.14 10.81 0.41
variables.
C 1.43 0.25 5.74 0.65
Digit span test R 0.46 0.10 4.76 0.20
3.2. Testing the structure model C 2.22 0.46 4.85 1.55
Visuo-spatial working R 1.66 0.10 4.76 0.53
memory matrix
The results (Table 3 and Figs. 1 and 2) showed a good fit of the
C 1.43 0.46 4.85 0.28
structure model to the data in both samples. The structural paths
from NSS to WM (first degree relatives = 0.40, SE-0.06; con- R, first degree relative; C, control; SE, standard error; R2, squared multiple
correlations.
trol = 0.31, SE-0.12) was statistically significant (p < 0.05).
Squared multiple correlations for structural equations for first
degree relatives and the control group were 0.16 (medium effect
size) and 0.094 (small effect size), respectively. These results
suggest that NSS have important negative influence on WM i.e.
[(Fig._1)TD$IG]higher NSS is associated with more severe impairment of WM.

Fig. 1. The structure model of the influence of soft signs on working memory in first degree relatives. MC, motor coordination; MS, motor sequencing; DST, digit span test;
[(Fig._2)TD$IG]
VSWM, visuo-spatial working memory; NSS, neurological soft sign; WM, working memory; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation.

Fig. 2. The structure model of the influence of soft signs on working memory in controls. MC, motor coordination; MS, motor sequencing; DST, digit span test; VSWM, visuo-
spatial working memory; NSS, neurological soft sign; WM, working memory; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation.
46 R.K. Solanki et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 5 (2012) 43–47

Table 3 frontal lobe function). Correlations were highest for the sequenc-
Goodness of fit statistics of both the groups.
ing of complex motor acts subscale of the NES. They suggested that
Index First degree relatives Controls this relationship was indicative of prefrontal cortical dysfunction.
RMSEA 0.0 0.0 Correlation between WM and NSS indicates the possibility of
NFI 1.00 1.00 common neurobiological substrate. In a review, Bombin et al.
NNFI 1.24 1.30 (2005) suggested frontal lobes as putative neuroanatomical
CFI 1.00 1.00 localization for motor signs. Several studies (Janssen et al.,
IFI 1.00 1.00
2009; Keshavan et al., 2003) have also found correlation of motor
SRMR 0.027 0.006
signs with caudate. Janssen et al. (2009) found correlation of motor
CFI, Comparative fit index; IFI, Incremental fit index; NFI, Normed Fit Index; NNFI,
sequencing with smaller right caudate volume while another study
Non-normed fit index; RMSEA, Root mean square error of approximation; SRMR,
Standardized root mean square residual. by Keshavan et al. (2003) found that scores for the repetitive motor
neurological examination abnormalities correlated significantly
with left caudate and cerebellum volumes. Few studies (Schroder
4. Discussion et al., 1991; Dazzan et al., 2002) had found smaller basal ganglia
volumes to be associated with motor deficits.
The current findings indicate that NSS have significant negative Some studies suggest that both frontal lobe and caudate may be
effect on cognitive functioning and indicate the probability of related to motor soft signs.
being a precursor of poor cognitive functioning. Findings are in line On the other hand, several studies (Gur et al., 2007) have shown
with Arango et al. (1999), who suggested neurological signs as a that the prefrontal cortex, particularly the dorso-lateral prefrontal
marker of global cognitive impairment and that some subsets of cortex (DLPFC), is critical for intact WM. Recently, studies have
these signs are specifically related to different neuropsychological found the role of the caudate nucleus to be important in the
tests thought to represent function in different brain areas. process of WM. In one study Postle and D’Esposito (2003) found
Our study showed significant negative correlation of primitive that the caudate nucleus mediates participation of the motor
reflexes with PALT (assessing episodic verbal memory). Previously system in spatial WM performance. Caudate activity has been
Hyde et al. (2007) had shown that primitive reflexes may be observed during the encoding, maintenance, and retrieval phases
indicative of cognitive impairment. Gabalda et al. (2008) explored of WM tasks (Gazzaley et al., 2004; Narayanan et al., 2005;
frontal release signs (FRS) and their associations with verbal Manoach et al., 2003). In an fMRI study Melrose et al. (2007)
memory among patients (n = 63), first-degree relatives (n = 33) concluded that the caudate body is critical for WM. Ashby et al.
and non-psychiatric controls (n = 51). In the entire sample, greater (2005) suggested FROST (short for FROntal-Striatal-Thalamic) as a
FRS were associated with poorer verbal memory. Studies have model of WM maintenance. The model assigns a unique role to the
shown that primitive reflexes, aka FRS, are associated with frontal basal ganglia. It assumes that the head of the caudate and the
lobe function (van Boxtel et al., 2006). Relation of episodic memory globus pallidus contribute to WM maintenance via sustained
with frontal cortex has also been demonstrated in earlier studies activation.
(Wagner, 2002; Tuulio-Henriksson et al., 2002). In a review of In light of all these studies it can be assumed that caudate
functional neuroimaging studies, Fletcher et al. (1997) found that nucleus and frontal cortex are the common neurological structures
encoding and retrieval of episodic memory is associated with underlying motor soft signs and WM. In tune with this, a review by
prefrontal cortex. So the possibility of similar localization may Bombin et al. (2005) found that several considerations support the
explain correlation found in our study. conceptualization of soft signs and cognitive functioning as
We also found significant negative correlation of MC with WM. partially independent phenomena, though they may be ultimately
Cognitive index also showed significant negative correlation with linked. A recent study by Chan et al. (2009) also examined the
MC and sequencing and total NSS. These finding indicates that underlying relationships between NSS and neurocognitive func-
motor soft signs may have a specific relationship with cognitive tions with SEM among patients with schizophrenia and controls.
functions, specially WM. In a recent review of NSS in schizophrenia They found that there were modest to moderate associations
Hui et al. (2009) emphasized that motor NSS are specifically among NSS, executive attention, verbal memory, and visual
associated with some cognitive impairments in schizophrenia. memory and concluded that NSS and neurocognitive tests are
In their study Mohr et al. (1996) also found correlation between two ways of capturing the same construct.
motor signs and cognitive functions. They studied neurological There is still no consensus on whether NSS is specific to
signs in patients with schizophrenia using the neurological schizophrenia or not but even if they are not specific there is
evaluation scale (NES) and found that total score and subscale consensus that they have prognostic significance (Arango et al.,
scores of NES correlated with scores on the Wisconsin Card Sorting 1999; Bombin et al., 2005). Keshavan et al. (2003) suggested that
Test and the Weigl Sorting Test (measures thought to represent neurological signs may serve as expedient bedside measures that

Table 4
Fitted covariance matrix of both groups.

Observed variable Motor coordination Motor sequencing Digit span test Visuo-spatial working
memory matrix

Motor coordination R 3.26


C 1.02
Motor sequencing R 2.83 6.62
C 1.16 4.92
Digit span test R 0.33 0.28 1.03
C 0.55 0.97 3.17
Visuo-spatial working memory matrix R 1.22 1.00 0.76 5.21
C 0.35 0.62 3.17 7.32

R, first degree relative; C, control.


R.K. Solanki et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 5 (2012) 43–47 47

are potentially useful in the assessment of idiopathic psychoses. tomical correlates of neurological soft signs: the AESOP first-onset psychosis
study (abstract). Schizophr. Res. 53 (suppl), 100.
Since atypical antipsychotics have been shown to have better effect Flashman, L.A., Flaum, M., Gupta, S., Andreasen, N.C., 1996. Soft signs and neuro-
on cognitive functions, NSS may also prove useful in choice of psychological performance in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 153, 526–532.
antipsychotic and thus probably improving long term prognosis. Fletcher, P.C., Frith, C.D., Rugg, M.D., 1997. The functional neuroanatomy of episodic
memory. Trends Neurosci. 20 (5), 213–218.
Since the administration of conventional neurocognitive function Gabalda, M.K., Weiss, P.S., Compton, M.T., 2008. Frontal release signs among
tests is time-consuming, the assessment of NSS may save time for patients with schizophrenia, their first-degree biological relatives, and non-
clinical practitioners, as a clinical screening measure for early psychiatric controls. Schizophr. Res. 106 (2–3), 275–280.
Gazzaley, A., Rissman, J., Desposito, M., 2004. Functional connectivity during
cognitive remediation/ rehabilitation. working memory maintenance. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4 (4), 580–599.
Though encouraging, findings of the current study must be Gur, R.E., Calkins, M.E., Gur, R.C., Horan, W.P., Nuechterlein, K.H., Seidman, L.J.,
viewed in light of its limitation like small sample size, cross Stone, W.S., 2007. The consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia: neurocog-
nitive endophenotypes. Schizophr. Bull. 33 (1), 49–68.
sectional design and representativeness of sample (multivariate
Hu, L., Bentler, P.M., 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure
non-normal). Further, small sample size does not allowed for analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equat. Model. 6
inclusion of all the variables in SEM. Due to its limitations, the (1), 1–55.
current study could not conclude definitely that soft signs can Hui, C.L., Wong, G.H., Chiu, C.P., Lam, M.M., Chen, E.Y., 2009. Potential endophe-
notype for schizophrenia: neurological soft signs. Ann. Acad. Med. Singapore 38,
predict cognitive status of a patient. But still it provides important 408–413.
insight to a possible relationship between these variables. Further Hyde, T.M., Goldberg, T.E., Egan, M.F., Lener, M.C., Weinberger, D.R., 2007. Frontal
larger studies are required, with more stringent criteria and using release signs and cognition in people with schizophrenia, their siblings and
healthy controls. Br. J. Psychiatry 191, 120–125.
multiple tests for a cognitive domain, to confirm their relationship. Janssen, J., Diaz-Caneja, A., Reig, S., Bombin, I., Mayoral, M., Parellada, M., Graell, M.,
Moreno, D., Zabala, A., Garcia Vazquez, G., Desco, M., Arango, C., 2009. Brain
morphology and neurological soft signs in adolescents with first-episode
Role of funding source
psychosis. Br. J. Psychiatry 195, 227–233.
Jöreskog, K.G., Sörbom, D., 2006. LISREL 8.8 for Windows [Computer software]
None. Scientific Software International, Inc., Lincolnwood, IL.
Keshavan, M.S., Sanders, R.D., Sweeney, J.A., Diwadkar, V.A., Goldstein, G., Pette-
grew, J.W., Schooler, N.R., 2003. Diagnostic specificity and neuroanatomical
Contributors validity of neurological abnormalities in first-episode psychoses. Am. J. Psychi-
atry 160 (7), 1298–1304.
Manoach, D.S., Greve, D.N., Lindgren, K.A., Dale, A.M., 2003. Identifying regional
All the three authors contributed in the design of the study and,
activity associated with temporally separated components of working memory
literature searches. The second author analyzed the data, and using event-related functional MRI. NeuroImage 20 (3), 1670–1684.
wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to Melrose, R.J., Poulin, R.M., Stern, C.E., 2007. An fMRI investigation of the role of the
and have approved the final manuscript. basal ganglia in reasoning. Brain Res. 1142, 146–158.
Mohr, F., Hubmann, W., Cohen, R., Bender, W., Haslacher, C., Honicke, S., Schlenker,
R., Wahlheim, C., Werther, P., 1996. Neurological soft signs in schizophrenia:
Conflict of interest assessment and correlates. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 246, 240–248.
Narayanan, N.S., Prabhakaran, V., Bunge, S.A., Christoff, K., Fine, E.M., Gabrieli, J.D.,
2005. The role of the prefrontal cortex in the maintenance of verbal working
None. memory: an event-related FMRI analysis. Neuropsychology 19 (2), 223–232.
Pershad, D., Wig, N., 1984. PGI memory scale. Natl. Psychol. Corp..
Postle, B.R., D’Esposito, M., 2003. Spatial working memory activity of the caudate
Acknowledgements nucleus is sensitive to frame of reference. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3 (2),
133–144.
None. Quitkin, F., Rifkin, A., Klein, D.F., 1976. Neurological soft signs in schizophrenia and
character disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 33, 845–853.
Roopesh, B.N., 2000. The role of central executive in encoding of organizational
References strategy and in working memory. M Phil Dissertation, NIMHANS, Banglore.
Sanders, R.D., Schuepbach, D., Goldstein, G., Haas, G.L., Sweeney, J.A., Keshavan, M.S.,
Anderson, J.C., Gerbing, D.W., 1988. Structural equation modeling in practice: a 2004. Relationships between cognitive and neurological performance in neu-
review and recommended two-step approach. Psychol. Bull. 103, 411–423. roleptic-naive psychosis. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 16 (4), 480–487.
Arango, C., Bartko, J.J., Gold, J.M., Buchanan, R.W., 1999. Prediction of neuropsycho- Sanders, R.D., Joo, Y.H., Almasy, L., Wood, J., Keshavan, M.S., Pogue-Geile, M.F., Gur,
logical performance by neurological signs in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry R.C., Gur, R.E., Nimgaonkar, V.L., 2006. Are neurologic examination abnormali-
156 (9), 1349–1357. ties heritable? A preliminary study. Schizophr. Res. 86 (1–3), 172–180.
Ashby, F.G., Ell, S.W., Valentin, V.V., Casale, M.B., 2005. FROST: a distributed Schroder, J., Niethammer, R., Geider, F.J., Reitz, C., Binkert, M., Jauss, M., Sauer, H.,
neurocomputational model of working memory maintenance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1991. Neurological soft signs in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 6, 25–30.
17 (11), 1728–1743. Snitz, B.E., Macdonald III, A.W., Carter, C.S., 2006. Cognitive deficits in unaffected
Bombin, I., Arango, C., Buchanan, R.W., 2005. Significance and meaning of neuro- first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of
logical signs in schizophrenia: two decades later. Schizophr. Bull. 31 (4), 962– putative endophenotypes. Schizophr. Bull. 32, 179–194.
977. Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Haukka, J., Partonen, T., Varilo, T., Paunio, T., Ekelund, J.,
Buchanan, R.W., Heinrichs, D.W., 1989. The neurological evaluation (NES): a struc- Cannon, T.D., Meyer, J.M., Lönnqvist, J., 2002. Heritability and number of
tured instrument for the assessment of neurological signs in schizophrenia. quantitative trait loci of neurocognitive functions in families with schizophre-
Psychiatry Res. 27 (3), 335–350. nia. Am. J. Med. Genet. 114, 483–490.
Chan, R.C., Gottesman, I.I., 2008. Neurological soft signs as candidate endopheno- van Boxtel, M.P., Bosma, H., Jolles, J., Vreeling, F.W., 2006. Prevalence of primitive
types for schizophrenia: a shooting star or a Northern star? Neurosci. Biobehav. reflexes and the relationship with cognitive change in healthy adults: a report
Rev. 32 (5), 957–971. from the Maastricht Aging Study. J. Neurol. 253 (7), 935–941.
Chan, R.C., Wang, Y., Wang, L., Chen, E.Y., Manschreck, T.C., Li, Z.J., Yu, X., Gong, Q.Y., Vecchi, T., Montecellai, M.L., Comoldi, C., 1995. Visuo-spatial working memory:
2009. Neurological soft signs and their relationships to neurocognitive func- structures and variables affecting a capacity measure. Neurology 33 (1), 1549–
tions: a re-visit with the structural equation modeling design. PLoS One 4 (12), 1564.
e8469. Wagner, A.D., 2002. Cognitive control and episodic memory: contributions from
Chen, E.Y., Shapleske, J., Luque, R., McKenna, P.J., Hodges, J.R., Calloway, S.P., Hymas, prefrontal cortex. In: Squire, L.R., Schacter, D.L. (Eds.), Neuropsychology of
N.F., Dening, T.R., Berrios, G.E., 1995. The Cambridge Neurological Inventory: a Memory. The Guilford Press, New York, NY, pp. 174–192.
clinical instrument for assessment of soft neurological signs in psychiatric Wechsler, D., 1981. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. The Psychological Corpora-
patients. Psychiatry Res. 56, 183–204. tion, St. Antonio, TX.
Dazzan, P., Morgan, K.D., Suckling, J., Chitnis, X., Orr, K.G.D., Fearon, P., Hutchison, G., Wong, A.H.C., Voruganti, L.N.P., Heslegrave, R.J., 1997. Neurocognitive deficits and
Salvo, J., Chapple, B., Jones, P., Mallett, R., Leff, J., Murray, R.M., 2002. Neuroana- neurological signs in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 23, 139–146.

Potrebbero piacerti anche