Sei sulla pagina 1di 12

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

Review

Sexual selection and the evolution of behavior, morphology,


neuroanatomy and genes in humans and other primates
Roscoe Stanyon ∗ , Francesca Bigoni
Department of Biology, University of Florence, Anthropology laboratories, via del Proconsolo 12, 50122 Florence, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Explaining human evolution means developing hypotheses about the occurrence of sex differences in
Received 27 January 2014 the brain. Neuroanatomy is significantly influenced by sexual selection, involving the cognitive domain
Received in revised form through competition for mates and mate choice. Male neuroanatomy emphasizes subcortical brain areas
20 September 2014
and visual-spatial skills whereas that of females emphasizes the neocortex and social cognitive areas. In
Accepted 1 October 2014
Available online 14 October 2014
primate species with high degrees of male competition, areas of the brain dealing with aggression are
emphasized. Females have higher mirror neuron activity scores than males. Hundreds of genes differ
in expression profiles between males and females. Sexually selected differences in gene expression can
Keywords:
Sexual dimorphism produce neuroanatomical sex differences. A feedback system links genes, gene expression, hormones,
Social complexity morphology, social structure and behavior. Sex differences, often through female choice, can be rapidly
Female competition modulated by socialization. Human evolution is a dramatic case of how a trend toward pair bonding and
Non-conceptive mating monogamy lowered male competition and increased female choice as a necessary step in releasing the
Brain size cognitive potential of our species.
Epigenetics © 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Gene expression

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 580
2. Social structure and mating systems affect body sex-dimorphism . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 580
2.1. Female promiscuity and cryptic mate choice . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 580
2.2. Sperm competition and genitalia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 580
3. Increasing focus on female–female competition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 581
3.1. Female secondary sexual characteristic: armaments or ornaments? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 582
4. The impact of sexual selection on primate brains and neuroanatomy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 582
4.1. Gross neuroanatomical differences between sexes: from male competition to female empathy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 583
4.2. Sexually selected sex differences in gene expression can produce neuroanatomical differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 585
4.3. Epigenetics, genomic imprinting and hormones can cause sexual dimorphism in neuroanatomy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 585
4.4. Social environment influences differences in neuroanatomy between males and females . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 585
5. A scenario of human evolution linking sexual selection, behavior, social structure, gene expression and neuroanatomy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 586
5.1. Uniqueness of human reproductive anatomy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 586
5.2. Sexual selection drove a clear trend toward pair bonding and increased paternal investment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 586
5.3. Decrease of sexual dimorphism in the genus Homo is evidence of increased female choice and decreased male competition . . . . . . . . . . 587
5.4. The neuroanatomy of pair-bonding and stable social system in the human lineage . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 587
6. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 588
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 588
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 588

∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +39 055 2757738.


E-mail address: roscoe.stanyon@unifi.it (R. Stanyon).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.001
0149-7634/© 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
580 R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590

1. Introduction sexual skin in various polygamous species of primates in relation to


both male fights for access to a mate and female “taste for the beau-
The outlandish size of the human brain has always fascinated tiful”; nevertheless, Darwin only vaguely connected these traits to
evolutionary biologists. Explaining human evolution has in large the mating types recognized today. The degree of male competition
part meant developing hypotheses about why humans have brains depends on both the number of females in groups and female repro-
three times the size of their nearest primate relatives (Sherwood ductive synchrony (Nunn, 1999). Polygynous species are the most
et al., 2008) and to what extent the brain may differ in a sex sexually dimorphic in body and canine size while monogamous
specific manner (Becker et al., 2008). Darwin (1871) developed species have almost no sexual dimorphism for these armaments as
the theory of sexual selection to explain sexual dimorphism, the well as for aesthetic traits.
differences between males and females of the same species. He Male ornaments are rare among mammals in comparison to
proposed that behavioral differences between males and females birds where male ornaments were considered as products of
were at the root of sexual selection. Males competed for access male–male competition. Nevertheless, male skin coloration was
to females whereas the most notable female characteristic was to interpreted as an attractive sexual signal in rhesus macaques (Waitt
choose the best mate among male competitors. Darwin proposed et al., 2003; Dubuc et al., 2014) and in mandrills (Setchell and Jean
that the large brain of humans was driven by sexual selection to Wickings, 2005). Such coloration is status-dependent, suggesting a
its absurd and sex-dependent proportion. As with the peacock’s dual utility of armaments/ornaments in sexual selection (Berglund
resplendent tail, males were the sex most influenced by sexual et al., 1996).
selection, so that “the average standard of mental power in man
must be above that of women. . .thus man has ultimately become 2.1. Female promiscuity and cryptic mate choice
superior to woman”. However, whereas Darwin’s simplified and
ridiculous view of women is typical of the Victorian age (Birkhead, Females instead of males are now the focus of many sexual selec-
2010), his idea that sexual selection could have a profound influ- tion studies, due to the key role of female mate choice. Rather than
ence on the brain is now a lively field of inquiry also in humans being a cooperative venture between the sexes, sexual reproduc-
(Cahill, 2006; Miller, 2011). How sexual selection – which implies tion is now viewed in terms of conflicts of interests among rivals
behavioral flexibility, cognitive abilities and different roles between of the same sex but also between males and females (Birkhead,
sexes – has modulated primate morphology as well as complex 2010). Females may increase reproductive success through promis-
neuroanatomy and gene expression is just beginning to be appre- cuity, especially in multimale–multifemale groups. Darwin did not
ciated (Dunbar, 2007, 2009; Lindenfors et al., 2007; Montgomery account for female promiscuity; he apparently thought that female
and Mundy, 2013). Studies now show that sexual dimorphism in choice was keyed to select one male, not to mate with multiple
neuroanatomy of primates is significantly influenced by sex-biased males. It was only in the last 50 years that the implication of female
behavioral traits that enhance fitness in different mating systems promiscuity became clear (Birkhead, 2010). Through promiscu-
and social structures. Sex differences in neuroanatomy are part of ity females can confound paternity, avoid infanticide and acquire
a feedback system linking social and mating structures, behavior, genetically compatible sperm.
and gene expression to sexual selection. New light is being shed Females also increase their mate choice toward high quality
on the evolution of humans by our improved understanding of the males for critical copulation and post-copulatory mate choice.
processes and mechanisms of sexual selection during primate evo- “Cryptic female choice” (sensu Eberhard 1996) can operate to max-
lution (Cachel, 2006, 2009; Sherwood et al., 2008; Chapais, 2013; imize female choice by disguising or concealing ovulation to relax
Fleagle, 2013). male coercion (Muller et al., 2007; Stumpf and Boesch, 2010). Even
the pH of the primate vagina can determine a drastic selection of
sperm (Dixson and Anderson, 2004) and primate seminal fluids
2. Social structure and mating systems affect body must adapt to neutralize low pH. In primates these female strate-
sex-dimorphism gies are facilitated by lengthy periods of sexual activity around
ovulation.
It is widely acknowledged that sexual selection is modulated
by variations in social structure and the reverse, but it was not 2.2. Sperm competition and genitalia
an easy task to dissect out the many contributing components.
Primate social organization is highly dependent on ecological vari- For males the quantity and quality (vigor and speed) of sperm
ables that influence the spatiotemporal distribution of females. It ejaculated into the female reproductive tract can be a crucial
is no banal conclusion that males follow females (Altmann, 1990). counter strategy to female promiscuity (Anderson et al., 2007).
Female distribution then is a major driving factor in determin- Sperm competition helps explain the variability found in primate
ing variation in social and mating systems (Carnes et al., 2011; genitals. There is an association between relative testis size and pri-
Lindenfors et al., 2004). Primates have a wide range of mating mate mating systems (Dixson and Mundy, 1994a,b; Harcourt and
systems including unusual multimale–multifemale groups (Ostner Gardiner, 1994; Harcourt et al., 1981; Hosken and Stockley, 2004;
et al., 2013). The mating systems of living primates with simpli- Shamloul et al., 2010; Verrell, 1992). Sperm competition is low in
fication are classified as monogamous (pairs), polygynous (one one male mating systems and here the testes are generally smaller.
male with multiple females), polyandrous (one female with multi- Sperm competition should be highest in multimale–multifemale
ple males) and polygynandrous (multimale–multifemale) (Martin, systems due to promiscuous mating and indeed testes size is much
2007). Mating systems are apparently not strongly influenced by larger. Relative testes size is now taken as a good indicator of the
primate phylogeny (Fig. 1), but on the other hand sexual dimor- strength of sperm competition. Sperm vigor follows the same cor-
phism is strongly correlated with the mating system (Fig. 2). relation. Faster sperm are found in groups of multiple adults of
The peak of sex-dimorphism occurs in case of the harem (one both sexes while the slowest sperm are found in one male mating
male–multifemale, i.e. gorilla) or when a male is associated with systems (Nascimento et al., 2008).
a female just for mating, as in orangutan (Fleagle, 2013). The morphology of the penis (head, length and width) may
Social and mating systems determine an incredible range of both evolve to both safely deposit sperm and to facilitate the removal
morphological and behavioral traits. Darwin (1871) discussed sex of competitors’ sperm. It has been hypothesized that one func-
differences in size, canine teeth as well as color, length of pelage and tion of larger penises would be both to remove rivals’ sperm
R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590 581

Fig. 1. A phylogenetic scheme of Simiiformes at the genus level. The various colors correspond to the predominant social mating system: red = multimale–multifemale
(polygynandrous), green = single male–multifemale, blue = monogamous pairs, pink = polyandrous, yellow = fusion–fission, orange = solitary males. Modified from Springer
et al. (2012) (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.5/); data on social structure are from Fleagle (2013). (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend,
the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

and deposit sperm out of reach; therefore they should be Campbell, 2013). Traditionally, it was thought that competition
larger in multimale–multifemale societies. However, the relation- between females was mostly over food. Differences in female rela-
ship of social structure with penis size is not clear-cut. Both tionships and female group size can be dependent on ecological
competition and female choice should influence penile mor- variables that may be conducted back to food resources (Lindenfors
phologies. It does appear that a longer penis is associated with and Tullberg, 2011). It is uncertain how much of this competition
multimale–multifemale species and dispersed mating systems falls under the umbrella of sexual selection. Recently there has
where sperm competition should be high (Dixson, 2002). Part of been some attempt to propose that sexual selection is just a subset
this relationship is certainly due to the fact that sexual swelling, of social selection (Rubenstein, 2012). Whatever the relationship
i.e. the tumescence of perineal skin in some female primates dur- between social selection and sexual selection, we can safely con-
ing the periovulatory period (see Fig. 3, makes the entrance to the clude that mating competition intensity –among males as well as
vagina longer. females – and other parameters of sexual selection – can influ-
ence the group size and the genetic relatedness between group
members.
3. Increasing focus on female–female competition Female–female competition may consist in a fine social manip-
ulation. Pregnant gorilla females are known to copulate only when
Beyond female distribution and female mate choice, there can other females are receptive. The hypothesis is that these matings
be no doubt that female-female competition for dominance rank are a strategy by which pregnant females attempt to minimize
influences reproductive success (Cant and Young, 2013). Darwin male interest in other females, while reinforcing their own sta-
did not consider female competition as a part of sexual selection tus and potentially delaying conception in others (Doran-Sheehy
and it has historically received much less attention than male sex- et al., 2009). Thus female mating competition may be a factor in
ual competition (Rubenstein, 2012). The importance of competition the evolution of non-conceptive mating (Furuichi et al., 2014). A
between females has garnered increased attention (Clutton-Brock, similar interpretation can be given to the finding that female bear
2007; Clutton-Brock and Huchard, 2013; Hrdy, 2013; Stockley and macaques synchronize copulations, instead of cycles (Furtbauer
582 R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590

3.1. Female secondary sexual characteristic: armaments or


ornaments?

Female facial coloration has been traditionally viewed as a sex-


ual signal (Dixson, 1983; Fink et al., 2006). For example in mandrills
it is more vivid in dominant females and during fertile periods
(Setchell et al., 2006). It is now better appreciated that primate
females may have secondary sexual characters that are as striking
as those of males (i.e. swellings Fig. 3), but it is difficult to separate
the effects of female choice from female competition (Rubenstein,
2012). The selective forces behind swellings and other female sec-
ondary sexual characteristics are likely to be multifactorial. One
hypothesis is that exaggerated sexual swellings are ornaments
indicative of female competition for males and are not just limited
to promoting paternity confusion among males. Swellings may be
honest indicators of quality (Zahavi, 1975) and females can use them
to obtain male attention and bonding although this may often be
difficult to establish empirically (Zinner et al., 2002). In several
species differences in the relative size of the swelling are positively
correlated with the female’s body condition and reproductive suc-
cess (Huchard et al., 2009). If they are honest indicators then males
can increase fitness by mating with these females and maintaining
relationships with them. In this manner females can tap into male
aid, support and paternal care for a more prolonged period of time
than simple copulation. Therefore female competition in this case
could be over male contribution to parental care. Recent work in
baboons, where exaggerated swelling are present, shows that lac-
tating females maintain close associations with adult males during
the most vulnerable period of an infant’s life (Ostner et al., 2013).
The hypothesis is that a male’s mating success is enhanced if he
Fig. 2. Sexual dimorphism in body size in Simiiformes primates in relationship to
social structure (see color code in Fig. 1): mmmf = multimale–multifemale (red), bonds with a specific female. This strategy is expected to be more
smmf = single male–mutlifemale (green), sol m = solitary males (orange) consort common in subordinate males who select to invest in a particular
with females only for mating, fufi = fusion–fission (yellow), mo = monogamy (blue), female as opposed to the riskier business of sneaking copulations
pa = polyandry (pink), HSA = Homo sapiens (black). Platyrrhine (New World) Pri-
with multiple females (Ostner et al., 2013). Importantly, stability
mates = P and Catarrhine (Old World) Primates = C. Graph (a) shows the average adult
male weight divided by average adult female weight. Graph (b) shows the percent
in female–male partners may develop in multimale–multifemale
difference of males to females. Data are from Fleagle (2013). (For interpretation of groups. In rhesus macaques, males are known to have better pater-
the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version nity success with females that they have bonded to (Kulik et al.,
of this article.) 2012).
Overall, however, female competition must differ from male
competition due to the greater investment usually made by females
in producing and rearing offspring (Stockley and Campbell, 2013)
et al., 2011). Copulatory calls in chimpanzees are used for social and conflicts among females are most often resolved without overt
manipulation of both males and other females (Townsend et al., physical contests (Young and Bennett, 2013). An example of behav-
2008). Yet female competition is not limited to these subtle maneu- ior regulating social tension could be the sexual rubbing between
vers and may escalate into outright physical aggression. In the female bonobos (De Waal, 2006; Clay and de Waal, 2014; Ryu
golden monkey more than 75% of the harassers were adult or et al., 2014, Fig. 3). Current neuropsychological evidence shows that
sub-adult females (Qi et al., 2011). Sexual interference by females females have been selected to avoid direct conflict. Human females
and infanticide can be considered more direct expressions of have heightened amygdala reactivity to threatening stimuli, giving
female competition. The importance of female infanticide is prob- them superior prefrontal cortical control over emotional behavior
ably underestimated. In chimpanzees, intragroup infanticide is (Campbell, 2013). We know that girls use strategies that minimize
often perpetrated by females (Pusey and Schroepfer-Walker, 2013; direct conflict and disguise competition (Benenson, 2013). Repro-
Stockley and Bro-Jørgensen, 2011; Townsend et al., 2007; Williams ductive conflict among human females is most frequently resolved
et al., 2008). through social castigation, exclusion and punishment (Cant and
Overall, the ultimate effects of female competition are harder Young, 2013).
to measure. Primate females are undoubtedly capable of compet-
ing and fighting over limiting resources when necessary. In some
species females may compete over males. There is apparently some 4. The impact of sexual selection on primate brains and
correlation between increasing group size and decreasing female neuroanatomy
reproductive success. In yellow baboons the direct aggression
of dominant females and matrilineage kin toward other females The fact that primates living in larger groups have larger brains
effectively lower the victims’ reproductive success (Stockley and is a simple but elegant demonstration that neuroanatomy is related
Bro-Jørgensen, 2011; Stockley and Campbell, 2013; Young and to and driven by sociality (Dunbar and Shultz, 2007). However,
Bennett, 2013). In chimpanzees, as in many primates species, high- the underlying relationship is with social complexity (“the social
ranking infant survival, faster maturing daughters, and more rapid brain hypothesis”, Dunbar, 2009) and with mating systems in pri-
production of young are phenomena linked to female competition mates. We have already seen that there are strong morphological
(Pusey et al., 1997). correlates between mating systems, body and canine dimorphism,
R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590 583

Fig. 3. Two bonobo (Pan paniscus) females engaged in genital rubbing. It is thought that genital rubbing enforces bonds between females. The exaggerated, genital swelling
of both females is evident. Human females never show swellings in the anal/genital region while bonobo adult females are never without swellings even if they may follow a
temporal phase. Both promote the same result: concealed ovulation. Swellings in human probably due to the evolution of bipedalism have moved to the front as permanently
swollen breast. Genital rubbing between adult bonobo females also consolidates vital female relationships, but recent research shows that the female in top position is usually
superior in rank (unpublished data). Photograph courtesy of Elisabetta Palagi.

ornaments, as well as sperm competition and testes size. Addi- microcephaly genes and human brain size. They concluded that
tionally, there is a significant negative evolutionary relationship these genes have contributed to the evolution of sexual dimor-
between relative brain size and the level of male competition for phism in the brain and neuroanatomy of primates.
mates. In short, larger brains are associated with lowered male com- Reinius et al. (2008) reported on a conserved sexual genomic sig-
petition and monogamous mating systems in both primates (Fig. 4; nature in primate brains. They identified genes with sex differences
Schillaci, 2006) and birds (West, 2014). In line with Dunbar’s social in brain expression levels in the cortex regions of a New World
brain hypothesis we would conclude that monogamy and pair monkey, an Old World primate and humans. They discovered liter-
bonding is cognitively more demanding. Deception and detecting ally hundreds of genes with different expression profiles between
cheating would have pushed brain size in a sort of arms race. males and females. Many of these genes had higher expression pro-
Sexual dimorphism in brain size can also be explored by files in females. Many of these profiles were conserved between
genomic analysis. Human microcephaly is a heterogeneous group species. However, they could only speculate that these gene profiles
of neurodevelopmental disorders that cause infants to have smaller may underlie important functional differences between the sexes,
heads than normal. Congenital microcephaly is frequently inher- with possible importance during primate evolution. Later Pointer
ited. Over the last decade genomic methods have identified genetic et al. (2013) concluded that exaggerated male sexual dimorphism
loci responsible for microcephaly. Genes that cause congenital in turkeys was associated with the masculinization of gene expres-
microcephaly control important aspects of neural development. sion. According to these authors, sex-biased gene expression is at
These genes are apparently involved in the evolutionary explosion the root of the majority of sexually dimorphic phenotypes in these
of cortical size that characterizes primates and especially humans. birds. It seems likely that gene expression is also at work in primates
Due to their acceleration in the human line, genes linked to human in a similar fashion.
primary microcephaly have also received much attention from
evolutionary biologists and anthropologists. Two examples are 4.1. Gross neuroanatomical differences between sexes: from male
microcephalin (MCPH1) and abnormal spindle-like microcephaly competition to female empathy
(ASPM). In clinical cases brain size is reduced in volume to a size
comparable with that of early hominids. Studies showed that both Evidence now shows that certainly there is a detectable sexual
genes have undergone positive selection during great ape evolu- selection effect on primate neuroanatomy (Dunbar, 2007). Gross
tion (Ponting and Jackson, 2005). Montgomery et al. (2011) studied anatomical differences exist between male and female primate
these genes in 21 primate species and surprisingly found that neuroanatomy that must be related to sexual selection. Male social-
humans were not the only species, which had experienced accel- ity is more related to subcortical brain areas whereas that of females
erated evolution in the genes they considered. is more related to the neocortex (Lindenfors et al., 2007). These find-
Later Montgomery and Mundy (2013) reported on the role of ings would convincingly argue for a mosaic view of neuroanatomy
four microcephaly genes in the evolution of sexual dimorphism both between and within species (male vs. female). A recent
in primate brain size. One aim of the study was to understand study confirmed these results by comparing prefrontal volumes
the phenotypic relevance of selection acting on these genes. Sex- of white/grey matter ratios in males and females of 10 primate
dependent associations were found between variation in three species. Human females evolve less and males more white/grey
584 R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590

Fig. 4. Error-bar plots of residuals from the least-squares regressions of brain (a and b) and testis (c and d) weights on body weight, and sexual mass dimorphism values (e
and f) by mating system and female promiscuity determinations. Variables were loge—transformed prior to regression analysis. Error bars represent one standard error of
the mean. Mating system: MMMF, multi-male/multi-female; PA, polyandrous; Mon, monogamous; SM single male. An increase in body size dimorphism (E-F) is associated
with a decrease in relative brain size (A-B), whereas sperm competition does not effect relative brain size (C-D). From Schillaci (2006). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000062
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.5/).

matter volume than predicted for a non-human anthropoid female fires both when an animal acts and when the animal observes the
(Smaers et al., 2012). Females had more grey matter vs. white mat- identical action performed by another. Thus the neuron appears
ter than male especially in the left hemisphere (Fig. 5). The left to mirror the actions of the other just as if it was performing the
hemisphere is involved in affiliation, social bonding and empa- act. Mirror neurons were discovered in macaques and at first were
thy. These results show that sexual dimorphism and laterality are thought to be restricted to Old World primates (Ferrari et al., 2009;
significant factors in the evolution of the human brain. Cook et al., 2014). We now know that mirror neurons are found
Almost all neuroanatomical data support the conclusion that outside primates and even in birds (Keysers and Gazzola, 2009),
the degree of male competition is positively correlated with neu- although they have been most extensively studied in primates.
roanatomy that controls autonomic functions and sensory-motor Mirror neurons are believed to be important in social animals
skills as well as those relating to aggression including mesen- as they provide a neurological basis for connecting individuals
cephalon, diencephalon and amygdala size and a reduction of the and for understanding the actions of others, through a perception-
septum (Lindenfors et al., 2007). The hypothesis is that in species action mechanism. Mirror neurons are thought to be important in
with high degrees of male competition, areas of the brain dealing imitation, learning, empathy and self-awareness. Long standing cir-
with aggression are emphasized (Lindenfors and Tullberg, 2011). cumstantial evidence suggests that there were sex differences in
Recently MR brain imaging was used to compare male and female traits linked to mirror neurons, most notable empathy. Females
human brains. Overall 121 subjects including 67 females were generally score higher in tests of empathy, sensitivity and emo-
studied. The results confirmed morphometric findings and discrim- tional recognition. Therefore the hypothesis is that females should
inated differences in gray-matter and microstructures in cortical be more richly endowed with mirror neurons. Sex differences in
and subcortical areas. Sex differences were found among others to mirror neurons were documented in humans (Cheng et al., 2008)
favor females in social cognitive areas and males in visual-spatial using neurophysiological methods. Females had higher values
skills (Feis et al., 2013). than males. These few data support the view that the sex differ-
Mirror neurons are another area of the neuroanatomy where ences in mirror neurons were generated by sexual selection in the
we find differences between males and females. A mirror neuron brain.
R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590 585

The rapid evolution of reproductive genes is well known across


many species even if the selective forces pushing these changes
are not often obvious. Recently, 285 genes encoding ejaculate in
humans, chimpanzees, bonobos and gorillas were analyzed (Good
et al., 2013). The hypothesis was that evolutionary rates should
be correlated to the intensity of sperm competition. However, this
hypothesis was not well supported. Instead they found that the
strongest correlation was with effective population size.

4.3. Epigenetics, genomic imprinting and hormones can cause


sexual dimorphism in neuroanatomy

Epigenetic phenomena apparently influence morphology, brain


and behavior (Jašarević et al., 2012). An emerging field of research
is the epigenetic of sexually selected traits. In this case Shi et al.
(2014) recently compared humans, apes and Old World monkeys
and showed that epigenetic regulation through methylation was
significantly reduced in humans for four microcephaly genes. The
larger neural progenitor pool probably contributed to the dramat-
Fig. 5. Weighting distribution in cortical as well as subcortical regions found in
T1 -weighted gray matter segments (of male and female human brains) included in
ically enlarged human brain and cognitive skills associated with
the multimodal classifier. Left (LH) and right (RH) hemispheres were distinguished human evolution.
and are represented in darker or brighter coloring, respectively. Females had more Genomic imprinting also appears to be important in sexual
grey matter vs white matter than male especially in the left hemisphere. The left selection (Wilkins and Haig, 2003). Genomic imprinting leads to
hemisphere is involved in affiliation, social bonding and empathy. These results
parental specific gene expression and is a special case of epigenet-
show that sexual dimorphism and laterality are important in the evolution of the
human brain. From Feis et al. (2013). ics which involves DNA methylation and histone modification. It is
generally thought that females can manipulate genomic imprint-
ing during gestation at the detriment of the father’s genome.
Imprinting would seem to be the natural outcome of conflicting
4.2. Sexually selected sex differences in gene expression can reproductive strategies as predicted by sexual selection theory. In
produce neuroanatomical differences this case the phenomena would appear to favor females as it can be
interpreted to represent a special case of female cryptic selection
The major histocompatibility complex (MHC) is the one area favoring female choice. However, the full implications of imprinting
that has received the most attention for studying the relationship for sexual selection are a field of research waiting to be explored.
of sexual selection with genomics. The main hypothesis is that The best known mechanisms of epigenetic regulation are those
through olfactory communication, choice should be for partners triggered by exposure to steroid hormones. The hypothesis is
with the most dissimilar genes, as this would enhance immune that these hormones influence expression pathways through DNA
response in offspring (Edwards and Hedrick, 1998). The supposed methylation, histone protein alterations, and even changes of non-
effects are wide ranging, including avoiding consanguineous breed- coding RNA. Rimol et al. (2010) showed that common variants of
ing through dispersal. These hypotheses have been tested in both MCPH genes accounted for variation in both normal and diseased
human and non-human primates (Grammer et al., 2005). Setchell human brains. Significantly, they found that there were sex-specific
and Huchard (2010) concluded that MHC dependent mate choice associations with brain volume and cortical surface areas. Only men
occurs across the primate order. But in reality the record is spotty. were negatively affected and they hypothesized that this differ-
For example some support has been found in humans, lemurs ence was due to hormonal differences between sexes during early
and mandrills, but no correlations were found in baboons and embryonic development. Paus (2010) found a clear difference in
macaques (Huchard et al., 2013; Huchard et al., 2010; Huchard and an adolescent sample (12–18 years of age); white matter density
Pechouskova, 2014; Schwensow et al., 2008; Setchell et al., 2013). was greater in females than in males. Apparently, rising levels of
Due to technical difficulties, the interplay between sexual selec- testosterone contributed to a decrease in white matter in male ado-
tion and reproduction in maintaining MHC polymorphisms is not lescents. Treatment of female and male transexuals with hormones
entirely clear (Huchard and Pechouskova, 2014). was shown to provoke changes in neuroanatomy (Rametti et al.,
Homeobox genes are genes that are involved in the regulation 2012; Zubiaurre-Elorza et al., 2014).
of morphogenesis; they mostly act as transcription factors that
switch on cascades of other genes. Homeobox genes are normally 4.4. Social environment influences differences in neuroanatomy
highly conserved, but there are a few cases of rapid evolution and between males and females
copy number changes. RHOXF2 (Rhox homeobox family, member
2), an X-linked gene highly expressed in testes and brains, is one There is additional supporting evidence for the influence of
such case in primates. Recently Niu et al. (2011) compared humans gene expression and epigenetic effects on neuroanatomy and func-
with 16 non-human primate species for this gene as well as the tion from non-primate animal models. Data from animal models
expression patterns. Significant variation was found in copy num- should be informative because, if these processes have meaning-
ber. Most (11) primate species have one copy, while humans and ful effects on the brain in these animals, they should be important
Old World monkeys have two copies and the chimpanzee has six for primates including humans. Bailey and Moore (2012) recently
copies. Humans were unique in that they have homogenized their showed how female choice is often flexible according to the social
two RHOXF2 copies through recombination. Phylogenomic analysis environment in which they are expressed. The genetic effects are
indicates strong positive selection especially in humans and chim- indirect, but can influence the rate and direction of evolutionary
panzees. Sexual selection acting on the male reproductive system change. Sexual selection models often have difficulty capturing
probably drove rapid evolution in RHOXF2 (Niu et al., 2011), but the dynamics, but indirect genetic effects can cause rapid sex-
this hypothesis needs further testing. ual trait divergence, including runaway exaggerated traits through
586 R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590

social feedback (Bailey and Moore, 2012). Female choice can be this hypothesis is valid it demonstrates an astonishing correlation
manifested through sexual imprinting, mate choice, copying and between the evolution of human neuroanatomy and human gen-
learning. They conclude that the social environment is one of the itals. Human testicles on the other hand are less than half that of
most important sources of environmental variation for organisms. chimpanzees (Dixson, 2012). Chimpanzee sperm is faster and more
These variables were tested in insects but are probably even more vital than human sperm. This is because P. troglodytes sperm show
important in social mammals such as primates. significantly higher mitochondrial membrane potential. Mitochon-
Cummings (2012) recently reviewed these mechanisms in dria provide a substantial part of the energy required for sperm
poeciliid fishes. Candidate genes were associated with female motility (Anderson et al., 2007).
preferences and genomic pathways underlying female social inter- Importantly, the genes that are responsible for the unique mor-
actions with males. Importantly, the networks were positively phology of the human penis are beginning to be found. Recent
correlated with female choice, but inhibited in coercive species. research (McLean et al., 2011) shows that in the lineage leading to
Female preferences exhibit a learned component because synap- modern human, males once had small spines on their genitalia such
tic connections are modulated and connections are reinforced as as those found in chimpanzees and other mammals. A comparison
females assess males. These genes are highly conserved across of the genomes of humans, chimpanzees and macaques indicates
vertebrates. This mechanism probably functions in primates since that a DNA sequence thought to play a role in the production of
there is behavioral evidence of experience-dependent mate choice these spines, has been deleted in humans.
across taxa from fish to birds and mammals (Cummings, 2012). So Rapid evolution has been identified for many reproductive genes
we have come full circle in a feedback system that links genes, gene and recent studies have combined phylogenetic tests and infor-
expression, hormones, morphology, social structure and behavior. mation on species mating systems to test for the effects of sexual
Research shows that human sex differences in neuroanatomy selection on these genes. The molecular evolution of the ADAM
may be influenced by socialization. For instance, the white matter (membrane proteins containing A Disintegrin And Metalloprotease
microstructure in female-to-male transsexuals differed from those domain) gene family is one example (Finn and Civetta, 2010). These
in females, but not from that in males (Rametti et al., 2012). It may proteins act in a highly diverse set of biological processes, including
prove difficult to isolate the effects of sexual selection from other fertilization (sperm–egg adhesion) and neurogenesis. It is certain
influences on neuroanatomy. But it may be noted that testosterone that the genomic changes associated with other aspects of human
levels are often associated with male competition, aggression and male reproductive anatomy will soon be forthcoming.
coercion (Book et al., 2001; Mehta and Beer, 2010). Perhaps an addi- Adult human females also have unique reproductive charac-
tional hypothesis is warranted. It is likely that coercive males do not teristics: they lack estrus and have permanently swollen breasts.
provide a social environment that facilitates the evolution of intelli- Concelaed estrus in humans – and also in bonobos – is linked to
gence even when the potential exists. Lower male competition and more equilibrated male–female relationships. The lack of estrus is
increasing female choice may be a necessary initial step in releasing often explained as a strategy to maximize female choice by avoiding
neuroanatomical potential for the entire species. Human evolution male coercion to mate.
is the most dramatic case. If the hypothesis is correct then probably The swollen breasts of human females may be a substitute of the
the expansion of human cognitive capacity was accompanied by a estrus swellings found in many Old World monkeys and apes. It is
lowering of male competition and an increase in female choice. thought that sexual signaling moved to the front as our hominid
ancestor assumed an erect posture and bipedalism (Gallup, 1982;
Mascia-Lees et al., 1986).
5. A scenario of human evolution linking sexual selection,
An analogous transition occurred in the Hamadryas baboon.
behavior, social structure, gene expression and
These monkeys spend most of their time sitting down as they
neuroanatomy
browse grasses on the high Ethiopian plains. To make more visible,
their sexual signaling has moved from the anal/genital area to the
We can now appreciate that a feedback system links behavior,
chest where bright red sexual skin is found today. The most impor-
social organization, mating patterns, and gene expression to sexual
tant hypothesis is that the permanently swollen human breasts
dimorphism: from the form and function of the genitalia, to neu-
function as ornaments.
roanatomy and the genome. It is possible to develop hypotheses
about the interaction of these factors in the evolution of our own
5.2. Sexual selection drove a clear trend toward pair bonding and
species.
increased paternal investment

5.1. Uniqueness of human reproductive anatomy Ornamental breasts must have evolved through increased com-
petition among females for males. Ornaments are evidence that the
Human male reproductive anatomy is strikingly different from investment of the other sex is a limiting resource, important for the
that of other primates. Human males lack the os penis (baculum) survival of offspring. It is hard to escape the conclusion that breasts
found in all other Old World monkeys and apes. The human penis are sexually selected and are used by males to choose mates. Body
also has a distinctive coronal ridge and a unique foreskin. The flac- fat deposition on the breasts and hips also appear to have been
cid pars libera length of the gorilla is only 65 mm and that of the shaped at least in part by male mate choice (Puts, 2010). This is an
orangutan is 85 mm while the length of humans (165 mm), chim- example of how sexual selection theory can aid in explaining the
panzees (144 mm) and bonobos (170 mm) is similar (cf Dixson, unique features of human reproductive anatomy. The differences
2012). However, the penis of both the chimpanzee and bonobo is can best be explained by a trend toward pair bonding and a more
filiform, ranging from 2 to 4 cm at the base (Dixson and Mundy, monogamous mating system. In this case female competition over
1994a). The circumference of the human penis (average 13.5 cm; mates will be stronger under pair bonding so females needed to
Richters et al., 1995) is much larger in comparison with the apes evolve ornaments as extravagant as those more commonly seen in
and Old World monkeys making it both relatively and absolutely males (Clutton-Brock and Huchard, 2013).
larger than that found in any great apes species (Dixson, 2012). This trend also explains the loss of estrus in humans. In many
The larger penis circumference is hypothesized to result from a Old World primates the size of swellings is consistent with cycles:
coevolution with the larger vaginal canal of women, which evolved making swellings a good indicator of fertility. Given that chim-
to permit the passage of larger headed infants (Bowman, 2008). If panzee males are likely to be physically stronger and dominant over
R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590 587

females, this common situation can lead to male coercion negating


female choice. Concealed estrus in humans – and also in bonobos
– is linked to more balanced male–female relationships. The lack
of concealed ovulation is often explained as a strategy to maximize
female choice by avoiding male coercion to mate, although bonobos
and humans resort to different strategies. Humans have eliminated
the outward signs of estrus and ovulation, whereas bonobos main-
tain a prolonged turgid swelling that equally masks female cycles
(Fig. 3). In both species female choice has become the winning
strategy. A collateral effect is to drastically lower male competition
in both species. In bonobos, females forge tight bonds with other
females and are dominant over males, a highly unusual situation
in Old World primates. Male dominance may be the official norm
in the majority of modern human cultures, but clearly female roles
are highly influenced by cultural variables making it difficult to
trace the evolution of dominance structure in our species. However,
Fig. 6. This bar graph shows the percent difference of male to female body size in
we can still conclude that concealed ovulation has lowered human a series of fossil hominids, from the most ancient to modern humans. From left to
male–male competition (Marlowe and Berbesque, 2012) allowing right Australopithecus anamensis, A. afarensis, A. africanus, Homo habilis, H. rudolfensis,
increased female choice. H. erectus, H. neanderthalensis and H. sapiens. Note that sexual dimorphism drops
A trend toward monogamy is characterized by an increase in considerably with the evolution of the genus Homo. Data are from Fleagle (2013).
paternal investment and prolonged pair bonding. Under this sce-
nario hominid females need to attract, choose and retain males 5.3. Decrease of sexual dimorphism in the genus Homo is evidence
for provisioning. When both parents provide considerable care or of increased female choice and decreased male competition
investment in offspring both sexes must optimize their reproduc-
tive success by choosing the best possible mate. Human males are The reduction of sexual dimorphism during the evolution of
therefore expected to be increasingly more selective about their the human line is good material evidence in support of the trend
partners as the trend toward pair bonding intensifies. Develop- toward pair bonding and monogamy. Paleoanthropologists have
mental differences in the hominid face of males and females, not long noted a reduction in sexual dimorphism in the human line
explained by size difference, were attributed to sexual selection. (McHenry, 1992) and the decreasing degree of sexual dimorphism
The relative shortening in men and lengthening in women of the has been used as a criterion of hominization through time. It seems
anterior upper face at puberty is the mechanistic consequence of very likely that this trend is the result of a change in mating pat-
extreme maxillary rotation during ontogeny. terns, a tendency to less polygynous relationships in the human
Other traits are apparently under mate choice control (Verweij line (Plavcan, 2003). Our knowledge from comparative studies in
et al., 2014). McLean et al. (2011) proposed that the loss of spines in primates strongly supports this claim. Exactly when the decrease
humans, might be related to human courtship and pair bonding. The in sexual dimorphism began and reached modern levels of vari-
loss of spines, they say, would result in less sensitivity and longer ability is uncertain, but the general view is that dimorphism was
copulation, and may be associated with stronger pair bonding in more extreme in pre-Homo species and that it is lower in the
humans resulting in greater paternal care for human offspring. The genus Homo (Larsen, 2003). A recent report showing that early
relatively large length of the human penis may also be subject to members of the genus Homo still maintained high levels of sex-
female choice. Surprisingly, larger penis size and greater height had ual dimorphism even after their exit from Africa (Lordkipanidze
almost equivalent positive effects on male attractiveness. Female et al., 2013) supports the view that the trend toward lower dimor-
mate choice could have driven the evolution of larger penises in phism continued at least until the appearance of modern humans
humans (Mautz et al., 2013). (Fig. 6).
Orgasm in human females may have evolved to consolidate
pair bonding, but there is a long-standing discussion even about 5.4. The neuroanatomy of pair-bonding and stable social system
the existence of female orgasm in non-human primates. However, in the human lineage
many physiological components of orgasm are found in non-human
primates including macaques, gorillas, chimpanzees and bonobos. One of the most dramatic tissue expansions in human evolution
The existence of orgasm in these species seems likely and may also is the increased size of the cerebral cortex. Many genomic changes
promote conception (Puts et al., 2012). In humans contrasting pos- have been discovered over the last few years, which promote the
itions view female orgasm as a by-product of male orgasm or as an increase of brain size in our lineage (McLean et al., 2011; Sherwood
adaptation, a directly selected characteristic through mate-choice. et al., 2008). Lindenfors et al. (2007) have shown that brain struc-
Further, in non-human primates female orgasm is more frequent tures have developed differently in primate males and females due
with high ranking than low ranking males (Troisi and Carosi, 1998). to divergent pressures on the sexes and to keep up with social
The high variability in female orgasm frequency and clitoral length cooperation or competitive aggression. It is possible to conclude
suggests that selection for female orgasm is relatively weak. How- that, in the transition from promiscuity to pair bonding (Gavrilets,
ever, this variability may also motivate women to copulate longer 2012), the high investment required from both parents in offspring
to active orgasm and repeatedly with males, which are “good” at requires greater social acuity and abilities to detect cheating. Sexual
sex or have high social value (Puts et al., 2012). This suite of charac- selection may help explain the expansion of the prefrontal cortex,
teristics would promote pleasure, bonding with males who should which mediates important components of complex social behav-
then invest more in eventual offspring. Selection pressure may then ior such as planning, working memory and language. This scenario
influence both male and female traits, which promote orgasm dur- would be in compliance with the social brain hypothesis (Dunbar,
ing copulation. The subsequent pair bonding could then operate in 2009).
humans to reduce sexual dimorphism and canine size, sperm com- The relationship between brain size and the complex neu-
petition and testes size and in general male competition for mates roanatomy needed to support pair bonding is part of a feedback
(Puts et al., 2012). mechanism. As the brain of Homo increased, brain development
588 R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590

after gestation became ever more important leaving the young continuity from non-human and human primates (pp 105–106),
increasingly dependent on provisioning with highly nutritious, still represents a current and controversial topic:
protein-rich foods. The demands of children cannot be adequately “(. . .) the difference in mind between man and the higher ani-
met solely by the mother. These needs could best be guaranteed mals, great as it is, is certainly one of degree and not of kind. (. . .)
by more stable pair bonding. An increasing importance of provi- I will make some few remarks on the probable steps and means
sioning for altricial young would selectively reward provisioning by which the several mental and moral faculties of man have been
males. Thus pair bonding and increasing brain size are certainly an gradually evolved”.
adaptive complex. It is now becoming a general conclusion that Darwin restricted the effects of sexual selection primarily to
transition to long-term pair bonding was a crucial tendency in males, especially male ornaments and armaments, and used it to
human evolution (Walum et al., 2012). Stable pair bonding would account for racial differences such as skin color and intelligence. It
be a necessary first step in the social structure recently proposed is now appreciated that environmental variables may account for
for hunter-gatherers (Hill et al., 2011). skin color and that sex-dimorphic traits in behavior, genital mor-
Compared to other primates, hunter-gatherers have a unique phology, reproductive proteins and gene expression can be molded
social structure. It is often assumed that human social structure by sexual selection. Both females and males compete and can both
must have evolved from the fusion-fission system found today in increase fitness through the choice of superior partners. Similar
chimpanzees and bonobos. However, we cannot be certain if the selection drives operate in both sexes and differences may be quan-
social system of the bonobo or chimpanzee or even something titative rather than qualitative (Clutton-Brock and Huchard, 2013).
intermediate represents the basal condition of the human lineage. A good argument can be made that strong pair bonding accom-
It may be that the fusion–fission system evolved in a common panied by relaxed male competition was an essential step in human
ancestor of these apes after the divergence of humans and is a evolution (Bribiescas et al., 2012). It permitted an increased invest-
derived behavior trait linking these two Pan species. Human evo- ment in offspring and many of the defining life history traits of our
lution from a multimale–multifemale system such as that found in species including the development of human society. It is clear that
many Old World monkeys may be just as plausible and perhaps a the characters shaped by sexual selection are an intimate part of what
better model. it means to be human and so they constitute a legitimate, integral
Chapais (2013) has recently outlined why the distinctive char- part of anthropological investigation. It is important to understand
acteristics of human social groups more likely derived from the the history of sexual selection theory, because with its eclipse in the
multimale–multifemale system. Human societies can be consid- first half of the 20th century virtually all anthropological, social and
ered federations of multifamily groups. These groups resemble psychological sciences developed without recognizing that sexual
multimale–multifemale groups found in other primates with the selection influenced the evolution of the human body, mind and
addition of stable male–female bonding. Hunter-gatherers bands culture. It is equally clear that the mechanisms underlying sex-
may have individuals, which are not connected by either kinship ual selection and the traits associated with them are both more
or marriage ties, yet include males with a vested interest in the off- diverse and more complex than initially realized and may be a part
spring of daughters, sisters and wives. Hill et al. (2011) summarized of a feedback mechanism. Fortunately, methods have also greatly
other particular features of human societies: (i) either sex may dis- improved. Of particular interest are molecular tools, which now
perse or remain in their natal group, (ii) adult brothers and sisters allow the phenotype subject to sexual and social selection to be
often co-reside, and (iii) many individuals in residential groups are linked to the genome.
genetically unrelated.
The differences between human societies is probably based both
on historical and ecological variables, but all can be considered Acknowledgements
as cooperative breeding units. This view is supported by a recent
survey of literature from 45 human societies, which shows that Funding was provided by a grant from the University of Florence
often individuals in the maternal line provide crucial aid especially to RS. The authors thank E. Palagi for the photograph of bonobo
when the contribution of the father is low (Sear and Mace, 2008). swellings and M. Svartman for comments. The authors also thank
All human societies have multilayer complexity, high coordination the reviewers and the editors in particular L. Beani for essential
and an extensive domain of formal kinship recognition, which often points in revision of this paper.
operates as a collective form of mate selection.
Clearly, human social structure facilitates the interactions and
References
connections with a much higher number of individuals than any
other primate species. Hunter-gatherer groups are noted for hav- Altmann, J., 1990. Primate males go where the females are. Anim. Behav. 39 (1),
ing 400 times lower lethal aggressive interactions compared to 193–195.
chimpanzees (Wrangham et al., 2006). Lower aggression would Anderson, M.J., Chapman, S.J., Videan, E.N., Evans, E., Fritz, J., Stoinski, T.S., Dixson,
A.F., Gagneux, P., 2007. Functional evidence for differences in sperm competition
facilitate interaction and the development of large social networks. in humans and chimpanzees. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 134, 274–280.
Therefore the lowered male competition that evolved under pair Bailey, N.W., Moore, A.J., 2012. Runaway sexual selection without genetic correla-
bonding was a fundamental change necessary for the development tions: social environments and flexible mate choice initiate and enhance the
Fisher process. Evolution 66 (9), 2674–2684.
of human society. These larger social networks may help to explain Becker, J.B., Berkley, K.J., Geary, N., Hampson, E., Herman, J.P., Young, E.A., 2008.
why humans evolved exponential capacities for social learning Sex Differences in the Brain. From Genes to Behavior. Oxford University Press,
that resulted in cumulative culture. This quantum leap permitted a Oxford.
Berglund, A., Bisazza, A., Pilastro, A., 1996. Armaments and ornaments: an evolu-
much higher demographic potential compared to our closest rela-
tionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 58, 385–399.
tives, the great apes, and probably lies at the basis of our capacity Benenson, J.F., 2013. The development of human female competition: allies and
to expand over the face of the globe. adversaries. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20130079.
Birkhead, T.R., 2010. How stupid not to have thought of that: post-copulatory sexual
selection. J. Zool. 281, 78–93.
Book, A.S., Starzyk, K.B., Quinsey, V.L., 2001. The relationship between testosterone
6. Conclusions and aggression: a meta-analysis. Aggression Violent Behav. 6 (6), 579–599.
Bowman, E.A., 2008. Why the human penis is larger than in the great apes. Arch.
Sex. Behav. 37 (3), 361.
Without doubt Darwin’s theory of sexual selection (1871) Bribiescas, R.G., Ellison, P.T., Gray, P.B., 2012. Male life history, reproductive effort,
reflected common Victorian biases. Nevertheless, the hypothesized and the evolution of the genus Homo. Curr. Anthropol. 53, S424–S435.
R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590 589

Cachel, S., 2006. Primate and human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Hill, K.R., Walker, R.S., Bozicevic, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., Hurtado,
Cambridge. A.M., Marlowe, F., Wiessner, P., Wood, B., 2011. Co-residence patterns in
Cachel, S., 2009. Using sexual dimorphism and development to reconstruct mating hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science 331,
strategies in ancient primates. In: Potocki, E., Krasinski, J. (Eds.), Primatology: 1286–1289.
Theories, Methods and Research. Nova Science Publishers, New York. Hosken, D.J., Stockley, P., 2004. Sexual selection and genital evolution. Trends Ecol.
Cahill, L., 2006. Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nature reviews. Neuroscience 7, Evol. 19, 87–93.
477–484. Hrdy, S.B., 2013. The ‘one animal in all creation about which man knows the least’.
Campbell, A., 2013. The evolutionary psychology of women’s aggression. Philos. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20130072.
Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20130078. Huchard, E., Courtiol, A., Benavides, J.A., Knapp, L.A., Raymond, M., Cowlishaw,
Cant, M.A., Young, A.J., 2013. Resolving social conflict among females without overt G., 2009. Can fertility signals lead to quality signals? Insights from the evo-
aggression. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20130076. lution of primate sexual swellings. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 276 (1663),
Carnes, L.M., Nunn, C.L., Lewis, R.J., 2011. Effects of the distribution of female pri- 1889–1897.
mates on the number of males. PLoS One 6, e19853. Huchard, E., Knapp, L.A., Wang, J., Raymond, M., Cowlishaw, G., 2010. MHC, mate
Chapais, B., 2013. Monogamy, strongly bonded groups, and the evolution of human choice and heterozygote advantage in a wild social primate. Mol. Ecol. 19,
social structure. Evol. Anthropol. 22, 52–65. 2545–2561.
Cheng, Y., Lee, P.L., Yang, C.Y., Lin, C.P., Hung, D., Decety, J., 2008. Gender differences Huchard, E., Baniel, A., Schliehe-Diecks, S., Kappeler, P.M., 2013. MHC-disassortative
in the mu rhythm of the human mirror-neuron system. PLoS One 3, e2113. mate choice and inbreeding avoidance in a solitary primate. Mol. Ecol. 22,
Clay, Z., de Waal, F.B., 2014. Sex and strife: post-conflict sexual contacts in bonobos. 4071–4086.
Behavior, http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003155. Huchard, E., Pechouskova, E., 2014. The major histocompatibility complex and pri-
Clutton-Brock, T., 2007. Sexual selection in males and females. Science 318, mate behavioral ecology: new tools and future questions. Int. J. Primatol. 35,
1882–1885. 11–31.
Clutton-Brock, T., Huchard, E., 2013. Social competition and its consequences in Jašarević, E., Geary, D.C., Rosenfeld, C.S., 2012. Sexually selected traits: a fundamental
female mammals. J. Zool. 289, 151–171. framework for studies on behavioral epigenetics. ILAR J. 53, 253–269.
Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C., Heyes, C., 2014. Mirror neurons: from origin Keysers, C., Gazzola, V., 2009. Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions,
to function. Behav. Brain. Sci. 37 (02), 177–192. emotions, and sensations. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19 (6), 666–671.
Cummings, M.E., 2012. Looking for sexual selection in the female brain. Philos. Trans. Kulik, L., Muniz, L., Mundry, R., Widdig, A., 2012. Patterns of interventions and the
R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367 (1600), 2348–2356. effect of coalitions and sociality on male fitness. Mol. Ecol. 21, 699–714.
Darwin, C., 1871. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. John Murray, Larsen, C.S., 2003. Equality for the sexes in human evolution? Early hominid sexual
London. dimorphism and implications for mating systems and social behavior. Proc. Natl.
De Waal, F.B., 2006. Bonobo sex and society. Sci. Am. 16 (2), 14–21. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 9103–9104.
Dixson, A.F., 1983. Observations on the evolution and behavioral significance of Lindenfors, P., Fröberg, L., Nunn, C.L., 2004. Females drive primate social evolution.
sexual skin in female primates. Advan. Study Behav. 13, 63–106. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 271, S101–S103.
Dixson, A., 2002. Sexual selection by cryptic female choice and the evolution of Lindenfors, P., Nunn, C.L., Barton, R.A., 2007. Primate brain architecture and selection
primate sexuality. Evol. Anthropol.: Issues. News. Rev. 11, 195–199. in relation to sex. BMC Biol. 5, 20.
Dixson, A.F., 2012. Primate Sexuality: Comparative Studies of the Prosimians, Lindenfors, P., Tullberg, B.S., 2011. Evolutionary aspects of aggression the importance
Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Oxford University Press, Oxford. of sexual selection. Adv. Genet. 75, 7–22.
Dixson, A.F., Anderson, M.J., 2004. Sexual behavior, reproductive physiology and Lordkipanidze, D., Ponce de Leon, M.S., Margvelashvili, A., Rak, Y., Rightmire, G.P.,
sperm competition in male mammals. Physiol.Behav. 83, 361–371. Vekua, A., Zollikofer, C.P., 2013. A complete skull from Dmanisi, Georgia, and the
Dixson, A.F., Mundy, N.I., 1994a. Sexual behavior, sexual swelling, and penile evolu- evolutionary biology of early Homo. Science 342, 326–331.
tion in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Arch. Sex. Behav. 23 (3), 267–280. Marlowe, F.W., Berbesque, J.C., 2012. The human operational sex ratio: effects of
Dixson, A.F., Mundy, N.I., 1994b. Sexual behavior, sexual swelling, and penile evo- marriage, concealed ovulation, and menopause on mate competition. J. Hum.
lution in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Arch. Sex Behav. 23, 267–280. Evol. 63, 834–842.
Doran-Sheehy, D.M., Fernandez, D., Borries, C., 2009. The strategic use of sex in wild Martin, R.D., 2007. The evolution of human reproduction: a primatological perspec-
female western gorillas. Am. J. Primatol. 71, 1011–1020. tive. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 45 (Suppl), 59–84.
Dubuc, C., Allen, W.L., Maestripieri, D., Higham, J.P., 2014. Is male rhesus macaque Mascia-Lees, F.E., Relethford, J.H., Sorger, T., 1986. Evolutionary perspectives on per-
red color ornamentation attractive to females? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 1–10. manent breast enlargement in human females. Am. Anthropol. 88 (2), 423–428.
Dunbar, R.I., 2007. Male and female brain evolution is subject to contrasting selection Mautz, B.S., Wong, B.B.M., Peters, R.A., Jennions, M.D., 2013. Penis size interacts with
pressures in primates. BMC Biol. 5, 21. body shape and height to influence male attractiveness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
Dunbar, R.I., 2009. The social brain hypothesis and its implications for social evolu- U.S.A. 110, 6925–6930.
tion. Ann. Hum. Biol. 36, 562–572. McHenry, H.M., 1992. Body size and proportions in early hominids. Am. J. Phys.
Dunbar, R.I., Shultz, S., 2007. Evolution in the social brain. Science 317, 1344–1347. Anthropol. 87, 407–431.
Eberhard, W.G., 1996. Female control: sexual selection by cryptic female choice. McLean, C.Y., Reno, P.L., Pollen, A.A., Bassan, A.I., Capellini, T.D., Guenther, C., Ind-
Princeton University Press. jeian, V.B., Lim, X., Menke, D.B., Schaar, B.T., Wenger, A.M., Bejerano, G., Kingsley,
Edwards, S.V., Hedrick, P.W., 1998. Evolution and ecology of MHC molecules: from D.M., 2011. Human-specific loss of regulatory DNA and the evolution of human-
genomics to sexual selection. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13 (8), 305–311. specific traits. Nature 471, 216–219.
Feis, D.L., Brodersen, K.H., von Cramon, D.Y., Luders, E., Tittgemeyer, M., 2013. Decod- Mehta, P.H., Beer, J., 2010. Neural mechanisms of the testosterone–aggression rela-
ing gender dimorphism of the human brain using multimodal anatomical and tion: the role of orbitofrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22 (10), 2357–2368.
diffusion MRI data. NeuroImage 70, 250–257. Miller, G., 2011. The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of
Ferrari, P.F., Bonini, L., Fogassi, L., 2009. From monkey mirror neurons to primate Human Nature. Knofp Doubleday Pubishing Group, New York.
behaviours: possible ‘direct’ and ‘indirect’ pathways. Philos. T. Roy. Soc. B. 364, Montgomery, S.H., Capellini, I., Venditti, C., Barton, R.A., Mundy, N.I., 2011. Adap-
2311–2323. tive evolution of four microcephaly genes and the evolution of brain size in
Fink, B., Grammer, K., Matts, P.J., 2006. Visible skin color distribution plays a role anthropoid primates. Mol. Biol. Evol. 28, 625–638.
in the perception of age, attractiveness, and health in female faces. Evol. Hum. Montgomery, S.H., Mundy, N.I., 2013. Microcephaly genes and the evolution of sex-
Behav. 27, 433–442. ual dimorphism in primate brain size. J. Evol. Biol. 26, 906–911.
Finn, S., Civetta, A., 2010. Sexual selection and the molecular evolution of ADAM Muller, M.N., Kahlenberg, S.M., Emery Thompson, M., Wrangham, R.W., 2007. Male
proteins. J. Mol. Evol. 71, 231–240. coercion and the costs of promiscuous mating for female chimpanzees. Proc.
Fleagle, J.G., 2013. Primate Adaptation and Evolution, third ed. Academic Press, Lon- Biol. Sci.: R. Soc. 274, 1009–1014.
don. Nascimento, J.M., Shi, L.Z., Meyers, S., Gagneux, P., Loskutoff, N.M., Botvinick, E.L.,
Furtbauer, I., Mundry, R., Heistermann, M., Schulke, O., Ostner, J., 2011. You mate, I Berns, M.W., 2008. The use of optical tweezers to study sperm competition and
mate: macaque females synchronize sex not cycles. PLoS One 6, e26144. motility in primates. J. R. Soc. Interface 5 (20), 297–302.
Furuichi, T., Connor, R., Hashimoto, C., 2014. Non-Conceptive Conceptive Sexual Niu, A.L., Wang, Y.Q., Zhang, H., Liao, C.H., Wang, J.K., Zhang, R., Che, J., Su, B., 2011.
Interactions in Monkeys, Apes, and Dolphins, Primates and Cetaceans. Springer, Rapid evolution and copy number variation of primate RHOXF2, an X-linked
Japan, pp. 385–408. homeobox gene involved in male reproduction and possibly brain function. BMC
Gallup Jr., G.G., 1982. Permanent breast enlargement in human females: a sociobio- Evol. Biol. 11, 298.
logical analysis. J. Hum. Evol. 11 (7), 597–601. Nunn, C.L., 1999. The number of males in primate social groups: a comparative test
Gavrilets, S., 2012. Human origins and the transition from promiscuity to pair- of the socioecological model. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 46 (1), 1–13.
bonding. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109 (25), 9923–9928. Ostner, J., Vigilant, L., Bhagavatula, J., Franz, M., Schülke, O., 2013. Stable heterosexual
Good, J.M., Wiebe, V., Albert, F.W., Burbano, H.A., Kircher, M., Green, R.E., Halbwax, associations in a promiscuous primate. Anim. Behav. 86, 623–631.
M., André, C., Atencia, R., Fischer, A., 2013. Comparative population genomics of Paus, T., 2010. Growth of white matter in the adolescent brain: myelin or axon?
the ejaculate in humans and the great apes. Mol. Biol. Evol. 30, 964–976. Brain. Cogn. 72 (1), 26–35.
Grammer, K., Fink, B., Neave, N., 2005. Human pheromones and sexual attraction. Plavcan, J.M., 2003. Scaling relationships between craniofacial sexual dimorphism
Eur. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Reprod. Biol. 118, 135–142. and body mass dimorphism in primates: implications for the fossil record. Am.
Harcourt, A.H., Harvey, P.H., Larson, S.G., Short, R.V., 1981. Testis weight, body weight J. Phys. Anthropol. 120, 38–60.
and breeding system in primates. Nature 293, 55–57. Pointer, M.A., Harrison, P.W., Wright, A.E., Mank, J.E., 2013. Masculinization of gene
Harcourt, A.H., Gardiner, J., 1994. Sexual selection and genital anatomy of male expression is associated with exaggeration of male sexual dimorphism. PLoS
primates. Proc. Biol. Sci.: R. Soc. 255, 47–53. Genet. 9, e1003697.
590 R. Stanyon, F. Bigoni / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 46 (2014) 579–590

Ponting, C., Jackson, A.P., 2005. Evolution of primary microcephaly genes and the Smaers, J.B., Mulvaney, P.I., Soligo, C., Zilles, K., Amunts, K., 2012. Sexual dimorphism
enlargement of primate brains. Curr. Opin. Genet. Dev. 15 (3), 241–248. and laterality in the evolution of the primate prefrontal cortex. Brain Behav. Evol.
Pusey, A., Williams, J., Goodall, J., 1997. The influence of dominance rank on the 79, 205–212.
reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science 277, 828–831. Springer, M.S., Meredith, R.W., Gatesy, J., Emerling, C.A., Park, J., Rabosky, D.L.,
Pusey, A.E., Schroepfer-Walker, K., 2013. Female competition in chimpanzees. Philos. Stadler, T., Steiner, C., Ryder, O.A., Janečka, J.E., 2012. Macroevolutionary dynam-
Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20130077. ics and historical biogeography of primate diversification inferred from a species
Puts, D.A., 2010. Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. supermatrix. PLoS One 7, e49521.
Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 157–175. Stockley, P., Bro-Jørgensen, J., 2011. Female competition and its evolutionary con-
Puts, D.A., Dawood, K., Welling, L.L., 2012. Why women have orgasms: an evolution- sequences in mammals. Biol. Rev. 86, 341–366.
ary analysis. Arch. Sex Behav. 41, 1127–1143. Stockley, P., Campbell, A., 2013. Female competition and aggression: interdisci-
Qi, X.G., Yang, B., Garber, P.A., Ji, W., Watanabe, K., Li, B.G., 2011. Sexual interference plinary perspectives. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 368, 20130073.
in the Golden snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana): a test of the sexual Stumpf, R.M., Boesch, C., 2010. Male aggression and sexual coercion in wild West
competition hypothesis in a polygynous species. Am. J. Primatol. 73, 366–377. African chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus. Anim. Behav. 79, 333–342.
Rametti, G., Carrillo, B., Gómez-Gil, E., Junque, C., Zubiaurre-Elorza, L., Segovia, S., Townsend, S.W., Slocombe, K.E., Thompson, M.E., Zuberbühler, K., 2007. Female-led
et al., 2012. Effects of androgenization on the white matter microstructure of infanticide in wild chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 17 (10), R355–R356.
female-to-male transsexuals. A diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychoneuroen- Townsend, S.W., Deschner, T., Zuberbuhler, K., 2008. Female chimpanzees use cop-
docrinology 37 (8), 1261–1269. ulation calls flexibly to prevent social competition. PLoS One 3, e2431.
Reinius, B., Saetre, P., Leonard, J.A., Blekhman, R., Merino-Martinez, R., Gilad, Y., Jazin, Troisi, A., Carosi, M., 1998. Female orgasm rate increases with male dominance in
E., 2008. An evolutionarily conserved sexual signature in the primate brain. PLoS Japanese macaques. Anim. Behav. 56, 1261–1266.
Genet. 4, e1000100. Verrell, P.A., 1992. Primate penile morphologies and social systems: further evidence
Richters, J., Gerofi, J., Donovan, B., 1995. Are condoms the right size (s)? A method for an association. Folia. Primatol. (Basel) 59, 114–120.
for self-measurement of the erect penis. Venereology 8 (2), 77–81. Verweij, K.J., Burri, A.V., Zietsch, B.P., 2014. Testing the prediction from sexual
Rimol, L.M., Panizzon, M.S., Fennema-Notestine, C., Eyler, L.T., Fischl, B., Franz, C.E., selection of a positive genetic correlation between human mate prefer-
Dale, A.M., 2010. Cortical thickness is influenced by regionally specific genetic ences and corresponding traits. Evol. Hum. Behav., http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
factors. Biol. Psychiatry 67 (5), 493–499. evolhumbehav.2014.06.009.
Rubenstein, D.R., 2012. Sexual and social competition: broadening perspectives by Walum, H., Lichtenstein, P., Neiderhiser, J.M., Reiss, D., Ganiban, J.M., Spotts,
defining female roles. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 2248–2252. E.L., Westberg, L., 2012. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is asso-
Ryu, H., Hill, D.A., Furuichi, T., 2014. Prolonged maximal sexual swelling in ciated with pair-bonding and social behavior. Biol. Psychiatry 71 (5),
wild bonobos facilitates affiliative interactions between females. Behaviour, 419–426.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003212. Waitt, C., Little, A.C., Wolfensohn, S., Honess, P., Brown, A.P., Buchanan-Smith, H.M.,
Schillaci, M.A., 2006. Sexual selection and the evolution of brain size in primates. Perrett, D.I., 2003. Evidence from rhesus macaques suggests that male col-
PLoS One 1 (1), e62, http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000062 . oration plays a role in female primate mate choice. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B. Bio.
Schwensow, N., Eberle, M., Sommer, S., 2008. Compatibility counts: MHC-associated 270, S144–S146.
mate choice in a wild promiscuous primate. Proc. Biol. Sci.: R. Soc. 275, 555–564. West, R.J.D., 2014. The evolution of large brain size in birds is related to social, not
Sear, R., Mace, R., 2008. Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on genetic, monogamy. Biol. J. Linn. Soc., n/a-n/a.
child survival. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29 (1), 1–18. Wilkins, J.F., Haig, D., 2003. What good is genomic imprinting: the function of parent-
Setchell, J.M., Jean Wickings, E., 2005. Dominance, status signals and coloration in specific gene expression. Nat. Rev. Genet. 4, 359–368.
male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Ethology 111, 25–50. Williams, J.M., Lonsdorf, E.V., Wilson, M.L., Schumacher-Stankey, J., Goodall, J., Pusey,
Setchell, J.M., Wickings, E.J., Knapp, L.A., 2006. Signal content of red facial col- A.E., 2008. Causes of death in the Kasekela chimpanzees of Gombe National Park,
oration in female mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B. Bio. 273, Tanzania. Am. J. Primatol. 70, 766–777.
2395–2400. Wrangham, R.W., Wilson, M.L., Muller, M.N., 2006. Comparative rates of violence in
Setchell, J.M., Abbott, K.M., Gonzalez, J.P., Knapp, L.A., 2013. Testing for post- chimpanzees and humans. Primates 47 (1), 14–26.
copulatory selection for major histocompatibility complex genotype in a Young, A.J., Bennett, N.C., 2013. Intra-sexual selection in cooperative mammals and
semi-free-ranging primate population. Am. J. Primatol. 75, 1021–1031. birds: why are females not bigger and better armed? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond.
Setchell, J.M., Huchard, E., 2010. The hidden benefits of sex: evidence for MHC- B Biol. Sci. 368, 20130075.
associated mate choice in primate societies. Bioessays 32 (11), 940–948. Zahavi, A., 1975. Mate selection–a selection for a handicap. J. Theor. Biol. 53 (1),
Shamloul, R., el-Sakka, A., Bella, A.J., 2010. Sexual selection and genital evolution: an 205–214.
overview. J. Sex Med. 7, 1734–1740. Zinner, D., Alberts, S.C., Nunn, C.L., Altmann, J., 2002. Evolutionary biology: signifi-
Sherwood, C.C., Subiaul, F., Zawidzki, T.W., 2008. A natural history of the human cance of primate sexual swellings. Nature 420, 142–143, discussion 143.
mind: tracing evolutionary changes in brain and cognition. J. Anat. 212, 426–454. Zubiaurre-Elorza, L., Junque, C., Gómez-Gil, E., Guillamon, A., 2014. Effects of cross-
Shi, L., Lin, Q., Su, B., 2014. Human-specific hypomethylation of CENPJ, a key brain sex hormone treatment on cortical thickness in transsexual individuals. J. Sex
size regulator. Mol. Biol. Evol. 31 (3), 594–604. Med. 11 (5), 1248–1261.

Potrebbero piacerti anche