Sei sulla pagina 1di 18

Corporal Punishment in Adolescence and Physical Assaults on Spouses in Later Life: What

Accounts for the Link?


Author(s): Murray A. Straus and Carrie L. Yodanis
Source: Journal of Marriage and Family, Vol. 58, No. 4 (Nov., 1996), pp. 825-841
Published by: National Council on Family RelationsNational Council on Family Relations
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/353973
Accessed: 14/09/2010 20:47

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ncfr.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

National Council on Family Relations is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Journal of Marriage and Family.

http://www.jstor.org
MURRAY A. STRAUS AND CARRIE L. YODANIS
University of New Hampshire

CorporalPunishmentin Adolescence and


Physical Assaults on Spouses in Later Life:
What Accounts for the Link?

There is considerable evidence that corporal pun- Studies of family violence have found consider-
ishment is associated with the subsequent aggres- able evidence that the experience of frequent cor-
sion of children, and there is recent evidence that poral punishment as an adolescent is related to an
later in life this aggression includes physical as- increased rate of assaulting a spouse later in life.
saults on spouses. Yet there has been no direct Gelles (1974) studied 80 families and found that
test of either modeling of cultural norms or other spouses who had experienced corporal punish-
processes that could account for the link between ment frequently (monthly to daily) had a higher
corporal punishment and partner violence. Using rate of assaulting a partnerthan those who had not
data on 4,401 couples who participated in the been hit. Carroll (1977) studied 96 couples and
National Family Violence Survey, this article re- found that "36.6% of those who had experienced a
ports such a test. The theoretical model specified high degree of parental punishment reported as-
three processes: social learning, depression, and saulting a spouse compared to 14.5% of those who
truncated development of nonviolent conflict-res- had not" (p. 176). Other researchers report similar
olution skills. Logistic regression was used to es- results. Johnson's (1984) study of 61 abusive men
timate separate models for men and women. The and 44 nonabusive men found that the experience
findings are consistent with the theoretical model. of corporal punishment is significantly related to
Because corporal punishment of adolescents oc- both minor and severe spouse abuse. Straus' anal-
curs in over half of U.S. families, the findings ysis of a nationally representative sample of 2,143
suggest that elimination of this practice can re- American couples (1990) found that the more cor-
duce some of the psychological and social pro- poral punishment these husbands and wives had
cesses that increase the likelihood offuture mari- experienced early in life, the higher the probability
tal violence and perhaps other violence as well. of assaulting their spouses. Kalmuss' reanalysis of
the same sample (1984), using more adequate sta-
tistical methods, showed that experiencing corpo-
ral punishment as a teenager more than doubled
Family Research Laboratory, 126 Horton Social Science Cen-
ter, University of New Hampshire, Durham, NH 03824 the probability of husband-to-wife and wife-to-
(mas2 @christa.unh.edu). husband assaults. Straus and Kaufman Kantor
(1994) studied a second nationally representative
Key Words: adolescence, corporal punishment, depression, sample (N = 2,149) and found that corporal pun-
family violence, norms, spousal assault.

Journal of Marriage and the Family 58 (November 1996): 825-841 825


826 Journal of Marriage and the Family

ishment was a significant risk factor for assaults chronicity of corporal punishment of adolescents
on wives, even when other potentially influential is important for the research presented here be-
variables, such as socioeconomic status, gender, cause corporal punishment at that age is the main
age, witnessing violence between parents, and al- independent variable.
cohol use, were controlled.
The findings on physical assaults against a Cultural Norms That Support
spouse are consistent with many studies of physi- Corporal Punishment
cal aggression by children against other children.
These studies found that the more corporal pun- Consistent with the high prevalence rates, corpo-
ishment children experienced, the greater their ral punishment is widely believed to be a neces-
physical aggressiveness against other children sary form of discipline. Although approval rates
(Kandel, 1992). One of the most recent studies have been decreasing, as recently as 1968, 94% of
used a longitudinal design that controlled for the a national sample agreed that "a good hard spank-
child's antisocial behavior at Time 1 and also for ing is sometimes necessary" (Straus & Mathur,
other parental behaviors, such as parental support 1994). The belief in the necessity of corporal pun-
(Straus, Sugarman, & Giles-Sims, 1995). The re- ishment is so strong that parents who make it
sults show that use of corporal punishment at known that they would never spank tend to be
Time 1 produced an increase in antisocial behav- perceived as ineffective, and their children tend to
ior 2 years later. Parallel results were found for be viewed as poorly behaved, even though they
children 3-5 years old, 6-9 years old, and 10 are as well-behaved or better behaved then chil-
years old or older. dren whose parents spank (Carson, 1986).
Among social scientists, with the exception of
CORPORAL PUNISHMENT IN AMERICAN FAMILIES
those who study the physical aggressiveness of
children, little attention has been paid to possible
Despite considerable evidence showing a link be- harmful side effects of corporal punishment.
tween corporal punishment and physically aggres- Straus (1994a) examined 10 widely used text-
sive behavior, corporal punishment has not re- books in child development and found that eight
ceived much attention in the family violence liter- of the 10 did not have an entry in the index or
ature. Part of the reason for the inattention might table of contents for "corporal punishment,"
be that corporal punishment is so widely used and "spanking," "discipline," or "physical punish-
the idea that corporal punishment is sometimes ment." A page-by-page content analysis located
necessary and harmless is so embedded in Ameri- material on corporal punishment in nine of the
can culture that researchers tend not to regard it as books, but the space on that topic ranged from
an important issue. This section, therefore, re- one sentence to a maximum of only four pages.
views findings on the prevalence of and the cultur- This is remarkably little attention paid to a mode
al norms surroundingcorporal punishment. of socialization experienced by over 90% of tod-
dlers and that continues for 13 or 14 years for just
Prevalence of Corporal Punishment over half of American children.
The minuscule space devoted to corporal pun-
Corporal punishment-the use of physical force ishment in child development textbooks is also re-
with the intention of causing pain but not injury markable because evidence of possible harmful
for purposes of correction or control-is legal in side effects has been available at least since the
every state of the United States. Over 90% of par- landmark study of Sears, Maccoby, and Levin
ents actually do use corporal punishment on tod- (1957), which first reported that corporal punish-
dlers (Straus, 1991; Straus, 1994a). Moreover, ment of preschool-age children is associated with
parents who use corporal punishment tend to do aggression toward other children and a less well-
so very frequently. In fact, 7.5% of the mothers of developed conscience. Many studies since then
3- to 5-year-old children interviewed for the Na- have found that corporal punishment is associated
tional Longitudinal Study of Youth hit the child with physical aggression by children. A number of
during the interview (Giles-Sims, Straus, & Sug- studies also have found a link between corporal
arman, 1995). Just over half of American parents punishment and physical abuse (reviewed in Straus
continue corporal punishment into their children's & Yodanis, 1994). The study by Kadushin and
adolescence, and they hit them an average of Martin (1981) shows that most cases of physical
eight times in the previous 12 months (Straus & abuse are the end point of a continuum that began
Donnelly, 1993). The high prevalence and with corporal punishment and got out of hand. De-
Corporal Punishment and Assaults on Spouses 827

spite this, a review of over 120 books on child tion actually holds such beliefs than is willing to
abuse found that fewer than half ever mention the express approval in response to survey questions.
possibility that corporal punishment increases the These attitudes reflect a carry-over from an era
risk of physical abuse (Straus & Yodanis, 1994). when husbands did have the legal right to "physi-
The virtual silence on corporal punishment also cally chastise" an "errant"wife (Calvert, 1974).
applies to books for parents. It is often assumed The courts began nullifying this common law
that Benjamin Spock, the author of one of the principle in the 1870s, but it has survived in
most widely read books on baby and child care in American culture and in the culture of the crimi-
the U.S., advises parents not to spank. In fact, the nal justice system. To take just one of thousands
most recent revision of Baby and Child Care of examples, a New Hampshire judge, in accept-
(Spock & Rothenberg, 1992), like previous edi- ing a plea bargain from a man who stabbed his
tions, tells parents to "try"to avoid corporal pun- wife, admonished him by saying that he should
ishment, rather than stating unambiguously that a have slapped her instead ("Darts and Laurels,"
child should never be spanked. Carson (1986) ex- 1993).
amined 31 of the most widely sold advice books More than 20 years ago, Straus (1976) docu-
for parents. She found that 35% said nothing at all mented the multitude of ways in which the ac-
about corporal punishment, 30% encouraged par- tions and inactions of the criminal justice system
ents to use corporal punishment, and only 35% continued to legitimize spousal assault. There has
discouraged using corporal punishment. None un- been remarkable progress since then, largely due
equivocally advised parents to never spank. If to the efforts of the women's movement. Instead
Carson's study were repeated today, there would of advising police officers to avoid interfering in
be at least one widely sold book that does advo- "domestic disturbances," most police departments
cate not spanking (Leach, 1991), and recent trends now require or recommend arrest (Sherman &
(Straus, 1994a, Chapter 11; Straus & Mathur, Cohn, 1989). Nevertheless, recent studies docu-
1994) suggest that in the future more parent ad- ment the continued inattention of the criminal jus-
vice books are likely to advise never using corpo- tice system to domestic assaults (Ferraro, 1989;
ral punishment. Jones, 1994; Kaufman Kantor & Straus, 1990;
Waaland & Keeley, 1985). The reasons for the
THE LINK BETWEENCORPORALPUNISHMENT persistence of norms legitimizing marital violence
AND SPOUSALASSAULT are multiple and complex. We examine the hy-
pothesis that one reason is found in the preva-
Although several studies of the relationship be- lence of corporal punishment by parents. This
tween corporal punishment and physically assault- teaches children that hitting is a morally correct
ing a spouse were reviewed in the introduction, way of dealing with misbehavior.
none of these studies investigated processes that Social learning theory suggests that children
might have produced this relationship. Several learn to use and value violence by observing and
processes are probably involved. This article tests modeling the behavior of their parents (Bandura,
theoretical models that incorporate three process-
1973). We think this is especially likely to happen
es: learning implicit cultural norms justifying vio- if the violence observed is in the form of corporal
lence, truncated development of nonviolent con-
punishment of children because corporal punish-
flict-resolution skills manifested in a high level of ment is a legal and socially approved behavior.
conflict, and depression-based aggression. Thus, when parents use corporal punishment to
correct and teach, it is accompanied by a hidden
Learning the Cultural Norms of Violence curriculum. Two of the hidden lessons are ". . .
that violence can be and should be used to secure
Although physically assaulting a spouse is a crim- good ends-the moral rightness of violence. ..
inal act, American culture legitimizes such acts in
[and] the idea that violence is permissible when
various ways. National surveys show that at least other things don't work" (Straus et al., 1980,
a quarterof the population approves of slapping a
103-104).
spouse under some circumstances (Gelles & Thus, one component of our model, the nor-
Straus, 1988; Greenblat, 1983; Stark & McEvoy, mative approval of violence, is based on the as-
1970; Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980; Straus,
sumption that corporal punishment teaches chil-
Kaufman Kantor, & Moore, 1994). Moreover, we dren that when someone misbehaves and won't
think that a much larger percentage of the popula- listen to reason, it is appropriateto hit them. Par-
828 Journal of Marriage and the Family

ents think that this applies only to their hitting a lence of severe depressive symptoms and suicidal
child who misbehaves. However, studies of chil- ideation among adult women and men.
dren show that children who are spanked tend to A second relevant line of research links de-
apply that principle to other children who misbe- pression with hostile and aggressive behavior to-
have, as they see it. Our study builds on that re- ward others. Although depressed individuals are
search by investigating the possibility that the typically thought of as passive and motivationally
lessons learned may persist into adulthood and deficient, a growing body of research suggests
marital relationships because it is almost in- that depression often may be associated with ag-
evitable that, sooner or later, a spouse will "mis- gression, especially in the form of uncontrolled
behave" and not "listen to reason," as the partner violent outbursts against others (Berkowitz,
sees it. We theorize that when this occurs, hus- 1993). The concomitance of depression and ag-
bands and wives who have been hit as adolescents gression, apparent among children (Garber, Quig-
for misbehavior are more likely as adults to hit a gle, Panak, & Dodge, 1991) as well as adults, led
spouse who misbehaves. Berkowitz (1983, 1993) to speculate that depres-
sive symptomatology represents one of several
Truncated Development of types of aversive stimuli that instigate hostility or
violence. Research by Maiuro, Cahn, Vitaliano,
Conflict-Resolution Skills
Wagner, and Zegree (1988), Julian and McKenry
The second part of the models we estimated starts (1993), and Tolman and Bennett (1990) found
from the assumption that the more parents rely on that this includes aggression directed at spouses.
corporal punishment to deal with misbehavior, the Studies focusing specifically on domestic vio-
less opportunity the child has to observe, partici- lence also have observed elevated levels of de-
pate in, and learn nonviolent modes of influencing pressive symptomatology among individuals who
the behavior of another person or modifying their physically assault their spouses. For example, Ju-
own behavior to adapt to the situation. To the ex- lian and McKenry (1993) reported that more than
tent that this assumption is correct, persons whose twice as many domestically violent males (45%)
parents frequently used corporal punishment will, scored in the clinically depressed range on a self-
as adults, have less skill in managing conflict and, report measure of depressive symptomatology
therefore, have more unresolved conflicts with than nonviolent males (20%) did and that depres-
their spouses. A high level of conflict, in turn, in- sive symptoms discriminated between violent and
creases the risk of violence (Straus et al., 1980, nonviolent men after controlling for race, quality
Chart 13). The probability of using physical force of marital relationship, life stress, and alcohol
is increased further because such persons are also use. Maiuro et al. (1988) found that rates of clini-
more likely to believe that hitting a spouse is cal depression were higher among men who as-
sometimes appropriate.The data available for this saulted their wives (67%) than among men who
research made it possible to test this hypothesis in- were assaultive toward nonfamily members
directly because they included a measure of the (34%), and nonassaultive men (4%).
presumed consequence of lack of such skills: a The link between depression and spouse as-
measure of unresolved marital conflict. sault has not yet been adequately explained and
most likely represents a complex, reciprocal rela-
Depression tionship. However, several researchers (Maiuro et
al., 1988; Tolman & Bennett, 1990) have suggest-
The third component of the models we tested ed that physical violence may reflect a maladap-
identifies depression as an intervening variable. tive strategy to deal with feelings of helplessness
The inclusion of depression is based on two relat- that accompany depression. An individual may
ed lines of research. The first links corporal pun- act aggressively against his or her spouse in an ef-
ishment with depression as an adult. Straus fort to reestablish control over a discordant mari-
(1994b) and Straus and Kaufman Kantor (1994) tal relationship that is in jeopardy of dissolving.
found that, after statistically controlling for six Enduring patterns of low self-esteem and personal
risk factors (e.g., witnessing parents assault each insecurity or fears of abandonment may predis-
other as a teen, socioeconomic status), frequent pose certain individuals to respond to perceived
corporal punishment during adolescence was as- threats of loss with aggression.
sociated with a two-fold increase in the preva- Our research builds on these observations by
examining the degree to which depressive symp-
Corporal Punishment and Assaults on Spouses 829

tomatology mediates the relationship between women respondents (odds ratio = 1.67, p < .01),
adolescent corporal punishment and marital vio- and a significant relationship between approval of
lence in adulthood. Postulating that depression violence and marital conflict, but only for male
serves as a precursor to spousal aggression in no respondents (odds ratio = 2.52, p < = .001). The
way contradicts our earlier findings that we inter- difference between the findings for men and
preted as showing that depression is a conse- women might reflect gender differences in social-
quence of marital violence (Stets & Straus, 1990). ization and conflict management and, specifical-
We believe that both processes are likely to ly, the tendency for men to externalize problems
occur. Because corporal punishment in adoles- in the form of aggression and for women to inter-
cence has been shown to be associated with an in- nalize problems in the form of depression.
creased probability of depression as an adult
(Straus, 1994a; Straus, 1994b), we reason that at METHOD
least part of the cause of depression is not trace-
able to being physically assaulted by a spouse. Models Tested
This assumption permits us to include depression
as an intervening variable in the model we test. We used logistic regression to test the theoretical
model. We estimated separate models for men and
for women in order to allow for the possibility that
Interrelationships of Mediating Variables
the effects of corporal punishment might be differ-
Up to this point, the discussion of the theory to be ent for men and women and because it is widely
tested has treated each of the three components believed that assaults on spouses by women have
separately. One can think of conflict-resolution a different etiology than assaults on spouses by
skills, approval of violence, and depression as al- men (Cascardi & Vivian, 1995; Nazroo, 1995;
ternative explanations for the link between corpo- Straus, 1993). The model for male respondents
ral punishment as an adolescent and marital vio- used the occurrence of an assault by the husband
lence. From that perspective, the study can be on the wife as the dependent variable. The model
viewed as a test of competing theories. Our per- for female respondents used the occurrence of an
spective, however, is that the three hypothesized assault by the wife on the husband as the depen-
intervening processes are linked and complemen- dent variable. Both models specify that corporal
tary and mutually reinforcing. Our perspective is punishment experienced as an adolescent increas-
based on research showing that marital conflict es the probability of approving hitting a partner,
and depression are linked (Julian & McKenry, conflict between spouses, and depression and that
1993; Maiuro et al., 1988; Beach, Sandeen, & each of these three intervening variables increases
O'Leary, 1990). As for links with norms that ap- the probability of physically assaulting a spouse.
prove of violence, although we have not found In addition to corporal punishment, five other ex-
previous research showing a connection between ogenous variables were included to control for
approval of violence and marital conflict and de- possible confounds with corporal punishment (see
pression, we think that such a connection is plau- Table 1). We used logistic regression (logit) be-
sible as the cognitive manifestation of the already cause our interest was in whether or not there had
demonstrated link between marital conflict and been an assault in the previous 12 months, and
depression. The data for this study permitted an logit is designed for estimating models in which
empirical examination of that issue by regressing the dependent variable is a nominal variable
each of the three mediating variables on the other (Aldrich & Nelson, 1984; Hamilton, 1992). All
two, while controlling for the seven exogenous seven exogenous variables and the three interven-
variables. ing variables first were regressed on the partner
Logistic regression analyses (that included assault measure. Then each intervening variable
controls for the exogenous variables described was regressed on the five exogenous variables.
below) revealed a very strong relationship be- The results, in the form of the significant odds ra-
tween depression and marital conflict among both tios, were displayed in the form of a modified path
male and female respondents (odds ratio for model (Hagenaars, 1993, p. 15) in order to facili-
women = 3.42, p < .001; for men odds ratio = tate examining and interpretingthe findings. How-
2.52, p < .001). As for the other two hypothesized ever, as Hagenaars notes, "there are no rules for
links, we found a significant relationship between decomposing the total effect in terms of direct and
depression and approval of violence, but only for indirect effects" (p. 17).
830 Journal of Marriage and the Family

Sample 1979, 1990; Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, &


Sugarman, 1996). The violence items ask if any
The data for this study are from the 1985 National of the following acts were used in anger in the
Family Violence Survey (described in detail in last 12 months: "threw something at the partner;
Straus & Gelles, 1990). This is a nationally repre-
pushed, grabbed, or shoved; slapped; kicked, bit,
sentative sample of American couples selected by or hit with fist; hit or tried to hit with something;
multistage, stratified, random-digit dialing. To be choked; beat up; threatened with a knife or gun;
included in the survey, a respondent had to be 18 used a knife or gun."
years old or older and fall into one of the follow- Husband-to-wife assault was scored as present
ing categories: (a) presently living together as a if the husband carried out one or more of these
male-female couple with or without children, or nine acts in the previous 12 months, and wife-to-
(b) a single parent living with a child under the husband assault was scored if the wife carried out
age of 18, or (c) divorced or separated within the any of these acts during that time. Table 1 shows
previous 2 years. A random process was used to that 12% of the men and 13% of the women as-
select either the male or female partnerfor the ap- saulted their partnerin the previous 12 months.
proximately 35-minute telephone interview. For Numerous psychometric assessments have
brevity of exposition, we will use terms such as been conducted on the CTS to evaluate its relia-
"assault a spouse" and "marital conflict," even
bility and validity as an instrument for measuring
though not all of the couples were married. The spousal violence (see Straus, 1990, for a review
response rate for this survey was 84%. The logis- of these studies). Internal consistency coefficients
tic regression program used for this study
(alpha) range from .69 to .88 for the husband-to-
(STATA 3.0) uses listwise deletion for missing wife assault scale and from .79 to .82 for the
data. This resulted in a sample of 4,401 (2,557 wife-to-husband assault scale. In addition, evi-
women and 1,844 men), considerably fewer than dence from both treatment and community sam-
the 6,002 cases in the full sample. Lost were pri-
ples shows that interspousal agreement in CTS
marily respondents who did not answer one of the scores falls within the moderate to high range.
conflict or violence questions. We, therefore, in- For example, Babcock, Waltz, Jackson, and
vestigated whether the reduced sample differed Gottman (1993) and Browning and Dutton (1986)
from the full sample in respect to the 11 variables
reported significant interspousal correlations on
used in the regression analysis. Comparison of the the husband-to-wife assault scale (rs = .42 and
listwise and full columns of Table 1 shows only a .65, respectively). In addition, Cantos, Neidig,
small difference. and O'Leary (1993) found strong and significant
correlations between couples on both the hus-
Measures of Spousal Assault band-to-wife and wife-to-husband assault scales
(rs = .90 and .84, respectively). Moreover, the in-
Physical assaults by husbands on wives and by terspousal correlations remained significant (rs =
wives on husbands were measured using the vio- .51 for both scales) when only those couples re-
lence scale of the Conflict Tactics Scales (Straus porting violence were included in the analyses.

TABLE 1. DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR KEY VARIABLES

Men Women
Variable Listwise Full Listwise Full
n 1,844 2,337 2,557 3,665
Corporalpunishmentby motheras a teen (%) 44.2 43.8 35.1 36.2
Corporalpunishmentby fatheras a teen (%) 44.1 43.9 25.7 26.1
Approvedslappinga spouse in some situations(%) 28.1 28.0 17.7 18.0
Assaulted spouse in past year (%) 12.2 11.6 13.2 13.2
Witnessedviolence between parentsas a teen (%) 12.9 12.7 12.7 13.9
Depressive SymptomsIndex (% high) 6.6 7.2 10.9 12.9
MaritalConflict Index (% high) 8.4 8.7 9.9 10.6
Age of respondent(mean) 42 42.6 41 40.7
Mean numberof childrenat home 1 1 1 1
Mean Socioeconomic StatusIndex score 50 49.6 48 48
Percentminority 26.1 27.8 28.0 33.8
Corporal Punishment and Assaults on Spouses 831

Somewhat lower levels of agreement were ob- bined, the percentages increase to 58% of men
tained in Szinovacz's (1983) analysis of CTS data and 44% of women. Furthermore, when parents
from a community sample of couples. Couples hit teenagers, it typically is not an isolated event.
agreed on only 40% of their responses to the Among the half of the population that was hit in
wife-to-husband assault index and on 27% of adolescence, the mean number of times they ex-
their responses to the husband-to-wife assault perienced corporal punishment in the previous 12
scale. These discrepancies did not, however, alter months was 7.9, and the median, 4.
substantive relationships between violence and A limitation of this measure is that respon-
various risk factors. For example, violence was dents whose parents discontinued corporal pun-
associated with low income and education, re- ishment by the teen years had to be coded as ex-
gardless of whether the data were based on hus- periencing no corporal punishment, whereas the
band or wife reports (Szinovacz, 1983). 90% prevalence rate among toddlers cited earlier
makes it likely that almost all respondents had
been hit by their parents at younger ages. Thus,
Independent Variables
the "none" category means none as a teenager,
Corporal punishment. Respondents were asked, rather than no corporal punishment at all, and
"Thinking about when you, yourself, were a could be a source of Type II error. Despite this
teenager, about how often would you say your and other limitations, the rates estimated by this
mother or stepmother used physical punishment method colTespond to corporal punishment rates
like slapping or hitting you? Think about the year from two studies using contemporaneous data ob-
in which this happened the most." The response tained by interviewing parents of teenage chil-
categories were never, once, twice, 3-5 times, dren. (For further information, see Straus, 1990;
6-10 times, 11-20 times, more than 20 times. We Straus & Donnelly, 1993; Wauchope & Straus,
asked a parallel question about corporal punish- 1990.)
ment by the respondent's father. It is important to keep in mind that the data
Empirical research and socialization theories refer to corporal punishment in adolescence and
of parent-child relationships have noted that that the findings, therefore, refer to corporal pun-
mothers and fathers spend unequal time, perform ishment at that age. Although this limits the appli-
unique parenting roles, have different interac- cability of the findings to the half of American
tions, and form dissimilar relationships with their adults who were hit by their parents when they
children (Demom, 1992; Peterson & Rollins, were in their teens, that is obviously a large sector
1987). Parental use of corporal punishment is no of the U.S. population.
exception (Straus, 1994). Given the distinct roles
of men and women in childrearing, it is important Approval of marital violence. This variable was
to examine the father's and the mother's use of measured using two questions from the survey
corporal punishment separately. As we will dis- conducted for the National Commission on the
cuss later, our research confirms that there are Causes and Prevention of Violence (as given in
gender differences in the consequences of using Owens & Straus, 1975). Respondents were asked,
corporal punishment. "Are there situations that you can imagine in
The questions on corporal punishment focused which you would approve of a husband slapping
on adolescence for two reasons. First, we be- a wife's face?" Respondents who answered yes
lieved that the data based on asking adults about were coded 1. The question was replicated for a
corporal punishment at earlier ages (preadoles- wife slapping her husband's face (see Table 1).
cence) would be less accurate. Second, based on
theoretical grounds presented elsewhere (Straus Depression. Identifying who is depressed in a
& Donnelly, 1993), we believed that corporal large cross-sectional sample of Americans is a
punishment during adolescence is likely to have difficult and controversial task. The method used
the most serious side effects. in the 1985 National Family Violence Survey is
Use of recall data probably results in an under- based on the Psychiatric Epidemiological Re-
estimation of both prevalence and chronicity. search Instrument or PERI (Dohrenwend et al.,
Nevertheless, the rates in Table 1 are remarkably 1976). The PERI provides data on a number of
high. The first two rows of Table 1 show the per- different psychiatric problems and is too long to
centage of men and women who were hit by their be included in the half-hour interviews that were
father and by their mother during their teen years. conducted. The measure of depression used in
If hitting by the father and the mother are com- this study consists of the following four PERI
832 Journal of Marriage and the Family

items that Newman (1984) found most indicative Socioeconomic status. A socioeconomic index
of depression: "botheredby feelings of sadness or was computed by factoring the following five
depression, felt very bad and worthless, had times items using the SPSS/PC principle components
when you couldn't help wondering if anything analysis: education of the wife and the husband,
was worthwhile anymore, felt completely hope- their occupational prestige scores, and the com-
less about everything." bined income of the couple. This resulted in one
Respondents were asked to indicate how often factor that explained 56% of the variance in these
in the past year each of the above occurred using three indicators and has an alpha reliability coef-
the following categories: never = 0, almost never ficient of .80.
= 1, sometimes = 2, fairly often = 3, very often =
4. These items were factor-analyzed using the Ethnic group. European Americans were coded 0
SPSS Principle Components Program. The analy- and African Americans, Hispanic Americans, or
sis found a single factor that accounted for 66% others were coded 1.
of the variance. The Depressive Symptoms Index
used in this study is the factor-weighted sum of Number of children. This variable is the number
these four items and has an alpha coefficient of of children younger than 18 years old who lived
reliability of .82. For the logistic regressions to be in the household with the respondent at the time
reported, this variable was dichotomized at the of the interview.
90th percentile. The 90th percentile was used be-
cause we wanted a measure that is analogous to FINDINGS
the proportion of the population suffering from
serious depression (Holden, 1991).
Corporal Punishment and Assaults on Partners
Marital conflict. The extent of unresolved conflict That corporal punishment is related to assaults on
in the respondent's marriage was measured by spouses is confirmed for this study by the odds
questions about how often the respondent and the ratios in the last column of the first two rows of
spouse or partner disagreed on five issues: man- parts A and B of Table 2. All are in the hypothe-
aging the money; cooking, cleaning, or repairing sized direction, although one of the four is not
the house; social activities and entertaining; affec- statistically significant. The interpretationof these
tion and sexual relations; and issues about the data can be illustrated by the odds ratio of 1.087
children. The response categories were 0 = never, in the last column of the first row in part A of
1 = sometimes, 2 = usually, 3 = almost always, 4 Table 2. An odds ratio is a ratio of the odds at two
= always. Because all areas of conflict, such as different values of x and often is used compara-
conflict over children, are not applicable to all tively to describe the strength of an effect (Hamil-
couples, the index was computed by taking an av- ton, 1992, p. 230). In this case it indicates that
erage of the completed items. The index has an each increase of one category in the seven-cate-
alpha coefficient of reliability of .87. It was di- gory corporal punishment index increases the
chotomized at the 90th percentile in order to iden- odds of a man physically assaulting his partnerby
tify couples with a high level of continuing unre- 1.087 or 9%.
solved conflict.

Corporal Punishment and the


Exogenous Variables Intervening Variables
Violence between respondent's parents. The ex- The relationship of corporal punishment to the
tent of violence in the respondent's family of ori- first of the processes that might help explain the
gin was measured by asking if and, if so, how link between corporal punishment and assault of a
often their father or stepfather hit or threw some- partner (approval of a husband slapping his wife)
thing at their mother during their teenage years. A is given in the first column of Table 2 and graphed
parallel question was asked about the mother or in Figure 1 for male respondents. Figure 1 is a
stepmother. The response categories were the conditional effect plot (Hamilton, 1992, 1993) that
same as those for the CTS items. controls for witnessing violence between parents.
It shows that the more corporal punishment expe-
Age. The age variable refers to the age of the part- rienced as an adolescent, the greater the probabili-
ner who was interviewed. ty that a man will approve of slapping a wife's
face and that this relationship applies both to men
Corporal Punishment and Assaults on Spouses 833

TABLE 2. REGRESSION MODELS TESTING DIRECT AND INDIRECT PATHS

Odds Ratios for

Independent Approval Marital Spousal


Variable of Violence Depression Conflict Assaults
Men
Corporalpunishmentby mother 1.114*** 1.089* 1.191*** 1.087*
Corporalpunishmentby father 1.084** 1.062 .983 1.034
Husband-to-wifeviolence of parents 1.562*** 1.446* 1.315 2.295***
Age of respondent .985*** .993 .991* .949***
Socioeconomic status 1.001 .982*** .982*** .993
Ethnic group .948 1.092 1.417* 1.468**
Numberof children 1.017 1.016 1.037 .973
Approvalof violence - - 2.045***
Depression -2.935***
Maritalconflict -2.641***
Model X2 75.13*** 29.44*** 48.20*** 214.75***
Pseudo R2 .03 .03 .04 .16
Women
Corporalpunishmentby mother 1.126*** 1.131*** 1.106** 1.093*
Corporalpunishmentby father 1.031 1.097** 1.060 1.083*
Husband-to-wifeviolence of parents 1.558*** 1.703*** 1.559** 2.106***
Age of respondent .987*** .999 .999 .936***
Socioeconomic status .991** .985*** .995* 1.004
Ethnic group .893 1.350** 2.208*** 1.435**
Numberof children .953 .982 .901* .822***
Approvalof violence - -2.089***
Depression - 2.066***
Maritalconflict - - 3.427***
Model X2 78.07*** 106.76*** 71.42*** 362.80***
Pseudo R2 .03 .04 .04 .18
Note: See Hamilton, 1993, for an explanationof the pseudo R2.For men, n = 1,844. For women, n = 2,557.
*p <.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001. (one-tailedtests)

who witnessed violence between their parents (the ers (see Table 1), is not significantly related to ei-
upper line in the graph) and to those who did not ther depression or marital conflict for men. For
(the lower line). Figure 1 was calculated by hold- women, corporal punishment by fathers is signifi-
ing constant at the mean all other exogenous vari- cantly related to an increased odds of depression,
ables shown in Table 2. We found a similar func- but not of marital conflict.
tion (not shown) with slightly lower intercepts for
women when we plotted approval of a wife slap-
Theoretical Model
ping her husband against the extent to which the
women in this sample experienced corporal pun- Figures 2 and 3 display the results in Table 2 in
ishment by their mothers. the form of path diagrams. The diagrams follow
We also computed plots such as Figure 1 for the conventions for path analysis based on OLS
the relation of corporal punishment to the other regression, but because they are based on the lo-
two intervening variables (depression and marital gistic regression results, the numbers on each path
conflict), but those are not shown here. The odds are the odds ratios. Only paths that are statistical-
ratios for these two variables are given in the sec- ly significant at the p < .05 level (one-tailed test)
ond and third columns of Table 2. Column 2 are shown. Although the odds ratios that appear
shows that corporal punishment of an adolescent on some of these paths may seem small, the ef-
by mothers consistently is associated with an in- fects accumulate across each level of the indepen-
creased probability of both men and women being dent variable (as illustrated in Figure 1).
high in depression, and column 3 shows that cor-
poral punishment is associated with an increased Assaults by husbands. Figure 2 diagrams the
probability of high marital conflict. However, model estimated for men, using assaults by hus-
corporal punishment by fathers, even though al- bands as the dependent variable. The upper path
most as frequent as corporal punishment by moth- shows a direct relationship between corporal pun-
834 Journal of Marriage and the Family

FIGURE 1. APPROVAL OF A HUSBAND SLAPPING HIS WIFE BY MOTHER'S USE OF CORPORAL PUNISHMENT

.5 --------------------------------------- __

0 Parents violent
I.
Q,
.CL
.4 ---------
Q

CL
0.
h/ ~~~~~Parents
not violent
'0 .3- - - - -- - - - - - - - - - -- - - - - - - - - -

m,

l5
0
*a

.2-
Never
T5 -1tim 10 tims >0 ti
Never Once
Once Twice 3-5 times 6-10 times 11-20 times >20 times

Corporal Punishment by Mother

ishment by mothers and husband-to-wife assault. cause corporal punishment during adolescence in-
The odds ratio of 1.09 shows that each increase of creases the probability of depression.
one category in the seven-category corporal pun- Moving down Figure 2 to approval of violence
ishment index multiplies the odds ratio by 1.09 or reveals significant indirect paths from corporal
9%. Because the odds ratio is from a model that punishment by each parent to approval of slap-
specified other variables (see Table 2), it is a net ping a partner. The odds ratios on these paths
relationship after partialing out the effect of the show that each increase of 1 unit of corporal pun-
other nine variables in the model. ishment by mothers multiplies the odds of ap-
Our main theoretical interest, however, is in proval of violence by 1.11 or 11%. Similarly, the
the indirect paths, through depression, approval of odds of approving violence in marriage are multi-
violence, and marital conflict. The path in Figure plied by 1.08 for each increase of 1 unit in corpo-
2 from corporal punishment to depression indi- ral punishment by a man's father. In turn, the path
cates that there is a significant relationship be- from approval of violence is associated with more
tween experiencing corporal punishment as a teen than double the odds of an assault by the husband
and being depressed. The odds ratio on the path (odds ratio of 2.04). These findings are consistent
indicates that each increase of 1 unit of corporal with the theory that corporal punishment teaches
the moral legitimacy of hitting someone who mis-
punishment by mothers multiplies the odds of de-
pression by 1.09 or 9%. In turn, the path from de- behaves, and this, in turn, increases the probabili-
pression to assaults by husbands indicates that ty of actually hitting or physically assaulting a
there also is a significant relationship between de- spouse who misbehaves-as the partnersees it.
The third element of the theoretical model to
pression and physical assault. Depression is asso-
ciated with almost triple the odds of an assault by account for the link between corporal punishment
a husband (odds ratio of 2.93). Thus, the findings and assault on a spouse is the possibility that cor-
support the idea that the link between corporal poral punishment of adolescents restricts their op-
punishment and marital violence occurs partly be- portunity to learn nonviolent modes of conflict
resolution and, therefore, increases the probability
Corporal Punishment and Assaults on Spouses 835

FIGURE 2. PATH DIAGRAM FOR HUSBAND-TO-WIFE OVERALL ASSAULT INDEX BASED ON LOGISTIC REGRESSION

Corporal
Punishment
by Mother ,

Assault

Age of
Respon

Socio-

Status

Etniit 2Conflict
____a___
Ethnicity

Number
of
Children

Note: Numbersare odds ratios;n = 1,844 men.

of a high level of marital conflict. The significant ry in corporal punishment by the mothers of the
odds ratio of 1.19 on the path from corporal pun- women in this study multiplies the odds of being
ishment by the mother to marital conflict, coupled high in depression by 1.13 or 13% for each in-
with the odds ratio of 2.64 for the path from mari- crease of 1 unit in the 7-unit corporal punishment
tal conflict to assault by the husband, are consis- measure. Similarly, the odds ratio of 1.10 on the
tent with that aspect of the theory. path from corporal punishment by fathers indi-
cates that each increase of 1 unit in being hit as an
Assaults by wives. Figure 3 diagrams the model adolescent by a woman's father multiplies the
estimated for women, which used assaults by odds of depression by an average of 10%. In turn,
women on their partners as the dependent vari- the odds ratio of 2.07 on the path from depression
able. The findings are similar to those in the shows that a high level of depression more than
model estimated for men and are highly consis- doubles the probability of an assault by the wife.
tent with the theory. Moving down Figure 3 to approval of vio-
The depression component is supported by the lence, it can be seen that corporal punishment by
paths linking corporal punishment to assault a woman's mother multiplies the odds of her ap-
through depression. Each increase of one catego- proval of hitting a partner by 1.13. Approval of
836 Journal of Marriage and the Family

FIGURE 3. PATH DIAGRAM FOR WIFE-TO-HUSBAND OVERALL ASSAULT INDEX BASED ON LOGISTIC REGRESSION ANALYSES

Corporal
Punishment
by Mother

by
Father

Socio-

Status

Ethnicity

Number/
of /
Children

Note: Numbers are odds ratios; n = 2,557 women.

violence, in turn, is associated with doubling the with a greater risk of causing injury than slap-
odds that a wife will assault her husband. Finally, ping, shoving, and throwing things. The severe
corporal punishment of adolescent women by assaults are measured using the last five items on
their mothers multiplies the odds of couple con- the Conflict Tactic Scales. The results for severe
flict by 1.10, and a high level of marital conflict assaults were similar to those just reported. For
multiplies the odds of a woman assaulting her example, the logistic regression in panel A of
partnerby 3.43. Table 2 shows eight statistically significant inde-
pendent variables and a pseudo R2 of .16. The
Replication of the Findings Using Severe Assaults equivalent logistic regression, using the presence
of severe assault as the dependent variable, found
We also investigated the possibility that the find- five significant independent variables (depres-
ings just presented might be different if the de- sion, violence approval, marital conflict, respon-
pendent variable were severe assaults associated
Corporal Punishment and Assaults on Spouses 837

dent's age, and ethnicity) and a pseudo R2 of .19. Socioeconomic status. Contrary to our expecta-
Similarly, the logistic regression in part B of tions, we found that socioeconomic status was in-
Table 2 resulted in nine significant independent directly, rather than directly, related to a lower
variables and a pseudo R2 of .18. The replication probability of spousal assault. For both men and
using severe assault as the dependent variable women, high socioeconomic status was associated
also resulted in eight significant effects (depres- with a lower risk of assaulting a partner because,
sion, violence approval, marital conflict, corporal consistent with many other studies (Dohrenwend
et al., 1992) high socioeconomic status was asso-
punishment by mothers, witnessing violence in
the family of origin, respondent's age, ethnicity, ciated with a reduced probability of depression.
and number of children) and a pseudo R2 of .22. For men, but not for women, there also was an in-
direct relationship between socioeconomic status
Copies of the logistic regression analyses of se-
vere assaults are available from the first author. and marital violence through a reduced probability
of marital conflict. For women, but not for men,
there was an indirect relationship between socio-
Other Direct and Indirect Effects economic status and the probability of assaulting a
This research focused on understanding the pro- husband through a reduced probability of approv-
cess by which corporal punishment of adolescents ing the slapping of a partner.
leads to violence in marriage later in the life
Ethnic minority. Membership in an ethnic group
cycle. In doing that, however, a number other
was associated with a higher probability of marital
findings emerged, and although they do not bear
on the primary research questions, they deserve at conflict and of assaulting a spouse for both men
least brief mention. The interrelations among the and women. In addition, for women, minority
mediating variables were described previously. group membership was linked to spousal assaults
Here we examine the links of the exogenous vari- through an increased probability of depression.
ables to the mediating and dependent variables.
Number of children. This was included in the
Violence between parents. As other researchers models because of the possibility that the number
of children is confounded with the variables of
(Jaffe, Wolfe, & Wilson, 1990; Straus, 1992)
have found, we found that witnessing violence theoretical interest, such as corporal punishment,
between parents was associated with an increased couple conflict, and depression. For men, as
shown by the lack of significant paths in Figure 2,
probability of assault on one's own partner. For
women, witnessing violence also was related to the number of children had no relationship to
an increased probability of depression, which, in marital conflict or spousal assault once other vari-
turn, increased the odds of a marital assault. For ables such as age and socioeconomic status were
both men and women, witnessing violence by controlled. Figure 3 shows that for women, hav-
parents multiplies the odds of approving slapping ing more children was associated with a de-
a partner by 1.56 times. For women, it also in- creased probability of marital conflict and a de-
creases the probability of couple conflict. creased probability of assaults on husbands.

Age. Both Figures 2 and 3 show that age is asso- SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS
ciated with a decrease in the probability of ap-
This research tested a theoretical model that in-
proving hitting a partner and a decrease in the
probability of actually hitting a partner. There corporated three components-normative ap-
were no significant paths from age to any other proval of violence, depression, and marital con-
variable, including marital conflict. Thus, mar- flict (which we used as a proxy for deficits in
conflict-resolution skills)-to explain why corpo-
riages of older couples do not have less conflict,
ral punishment increases the risk later in life of
only less violence. Perhaps as partners age, they
learn that physical coercion is not an effective assaulting a spouse. The results of logistic regres-
sion analyses of data from a nationally represen-
way of resolving the inevitable conflicts that
occur in families, or perhaps they begin to set dif- tative sample of husbands and wives were largely
ferent boundaries for what is tolerable at the consistent with this model. Corporal punishment
hands of their partners. in adolescence was associated with an increased
probability of approving violence against one's
spouse, experiencing depression as an adult, and
838 Journal of Marriage and the Family

elevating levels of marital conflict. In turn, each punishment is associated with subsequent vio-
of these components was associated with an in- lence (Strassberg, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1994)
creased probability of physically assaulting one's and antisocial behavior (Straus et al., 1995).
spouse. Thus, as hypothesized, at least part of the Third, the belief that a good, hard spanking is
association between corporal punishment and sometimes necessary implies that corporal pun-
adult spousal violence is explained by these three ishment is not harmful if it is used only rarely. To
variables. Moreover, the associations remained evaluate this possibility, we examined differences
significant, despite controlling for age, socioeco- among respondents who were never hit, hit only
nomic status, ethnic group, and witnessing vio- once, hit only twice, hit three times, etc. Those
lence between parents. analyses showed that each increase in corporal
punishment of an adolescent, starting with just
one instance, was associated with an increase in
Methodological Considerations
the approval of violence and in actual violence to-
Although the analysis controlled for a number of ward a spouse (Straus, 1994a, Chart 7-2). A
possible sources of spurious findings, several study by Turner and Finkelhor (1996), using data
other methodological issues need to be considered from interviews with children from 10 to 16 years
to properly evaluate the findings and to inform old, also found that even one or two instances of
and guide future research. corporal punishment at that age were associated
First, no matter how many variables are statis- with an increase in stress in children. However, it
tically controlled in nonexperimental research, is possible that infrequent corporal punishment of
there is always the possibility that some unspeci- still younger children is relatively harmless, and
fied variable might be the true source of the ob- that issue needs to be investigated.
served relationships. For example, one potential
confound that we were unable to control for was
Policy Implications
whether parents who used corporal punishment
also engaged in violence that was severe enough To the extent that the findings can be interpreted
to be classified as physical abuse. It is possible as reflecting a causal relationship between the
that the confounding of corporal punishment with corporal punishment of adolescents and assault-
physical abuse accounts for at least part of the ing a spouse later in life, eliminating or reducing
findings reported here. However, some of our corporal punishment can contribute to reducing
previous research included data that made it pos- marital violence because more than half of Amer-
sible to exclude abused children, and, in each ican parents hit adolescent children. The results of
case, we found that corporal punishment contin- this research also suggest that a reduction in cor-
ued to have significant harmful side effects poral punishment would have a beneficial impact
(Straus, 1994a; Vissing, Straus, Gelles, & Harrop, on one of the most pervasive forms of psycholog-
1991; Yodanis, 1992). Nevertheless, confounding ical distress-depression. Close to 7% of the pop-
corporal punishment with physical abuse is a con- ulation will experience an episode of major de-
cern that future research needs to address. pression at some point in their lives (Charney &
Second, the corporal punishment data were Weissman, 1988). Moreover, mood disorders ac-
obtained by asking respondents whether they count for more use of mental health services than
were hit by their parents when they were adoles- any other psychiatric disorder and are responsible
cents. The effects of recall inaccuracies, including for the majority of all attempted and completed
selective memory biases among individuals who suicides (Boyer & Guthrie, 1986; Charney &
hit their spouses, must, therefore, be considered. Weissman, 1988). Thus, in addition to the associ-
Recall of corporal punishment, for example, may ation of corporal punishment with physical vio-
help perpetrators of marital violence justify ac- lence, the severity of the consequences associated
tions that they have taken for other reasons. with depression also underscores the need for a
Given that more than half of the population re- greater understanding of the deleterious effects of
calls being hit at this age, we believe it is unlikely corporal punishment.
that only those who were predisposed to violence Police and prosecutorial policies intended to
recall such events. However, as in the case of reduce partner violence have been weakly imple-
confounding corporal punishment with physical mented. Our findings on the link between corpo-
abuse, studies that do not depend on recall, in- ral punishment and the approval of slapping a
cluding prospective studies, find that corporal spouse suggest that corporal punishment may be
Corporal Punishment and Assaults on Spouses 839

one of the factors underlying this weak imple- data to quantify the differential reporting effect. Jour-
mentation. The police, prosecutors, and judges nal of Marriage and the Family, 48, 375-379.
Calvert,R. (1974). Criminaland civil liability in hus-
are, of course, among the more than half of all band-wifeassaults.In S. K. Steinmetz& M. A. Straus
Americans who have experienced the violence- (Eds.), Violence in the family (pp. 88-91). New York:
justifying effects of corporal punishment, and that Harper& Row.
may be part of the reason the criminal justice sys- Cantos, A. L., Neidig, P. H., & O'Leary, K. D. (1993).
tem so often fails to act against all but the most Men and women's attributions of blame for domestic
violence. Journal of Family Violence, 8, 289-300.
egregious cases of marital assault. If these inter- Carroll, J. C. (1977). The intergenerational transmission
pretations are correct, ending corporal punish- of family violence: Long-term effect of aggressive
ment in childrearing could not only reduce the behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 3, 289-299.
rate of marital violence, but also contribute to Carson, B. (1986). Parents who don't spank: Deviation
in the legitimation of physical force. Unpublished
ending the de facto institutional practices that tol- doctoral dissertation, University of New Hampshire,
erate marital violence. Durham.
Cascardi, M., & Vivian, D. (1995). Context for specific
episodes of marital violence: Gender and severity of
NOTE violence differences. Journal of Family Violence, 10,
An earlier version of this article was presented under a 265-293.
different title at the annual meeting of the American So- Charney, E. A., & Weissman, M. M. (1988). Epidemol-
ciety of Criminology, Phoenix, October 29, 1993. We ogy of depressive and manic episodes. In A. Geor-
are particularly indebted to Nancy Asdigian for revi- goats & R. Cancro (Eds.), Depression and mania.
sions to the statistical analysis and the text that greatly New York: Elsevier Science.
Darts and laurals. (1993, June 8). Portsmouth [NH]
improved this version and to the members of the
1993-1994 Family Research Laboratory Seminar for Herald, Section B.
Demo, D. H. (1992). Parent-child relations: Assessing
many insightful comments and suggestions. Jean Giles- recent changes. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
Sims provided particularly helpful comments. The arti-
cle is part of a research program on corporal punish- 54, 104-117.
ment at the Family Research Laboratory, University of Dohrenwend, B. P., Levav, I., Shrout, P. E.,
New Hampshire, Durham. The work has been support- Schwartz, S., Naven, G., Link, B. G., Skodol, A. E.,
ed by the National Instituteof Mental Health Grants & Stueve, A. (1992). Socioeconomic status and psy-
R01MH40027 and T32MH15161 and the University of chiatric disorders: The causation-selection issue. Sci-
New Hampshire. ence, 255, 946-951.
Dohrenwend,B. S., Kranoff,L., Askenasy, A. R., &
Dohrenwend, B. P. (1976). Exemplification of a
REFERENCES methodfor scalinglife events:The PERIlife events
scale. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19,
Aldrich, J. H., & Nelson, F. D. (1984). Linear probabili- 205-229.
ty, logit, and probit models. ThousandOaks, CA: Ferraro, K. J. (1989). Policing woman battering. Social
Sage. Problems, 36, 61-74.
Ames, L. B. (1979). Child care and development (Rev. Garber,J., Quiggle,N. L., Panak,W., & Dodge, K. A.
ed.). New York:Harper/Lippincott. (1991). Aggressionand depressionin children:Co-
Babcock, J. C., Waltz, J., Jackson, N. S., & Gottman, J. morbidity, specificity, and social cognitive process-
M. (1993). Powerandviolence:The relationbetween ing. In. D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester Sym-
communication patterns, power discrepancies, and posium on Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 2.
domesticviolence.Journalof Consultingand Clini- Internalizing and externalizing expression of dysfunc-
cal Psychology,61, 40-50. tion.Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.
Bandura,A. (1973).Aggression:A social learninganal- Gelles, R. J. (1974). The violent home: A study of physi-
ysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. cal aggression between husbands and wives. Beverly
Beach, S. R. H., Sandeen, E. E., & O'Leary, K. D. Hills,CA: Sage.
(1990).Depressionin marriage.New York:Guilford. Gelles, R. J., & Straus, M. A. (1979). Determinants of
Berkowitz, L. (1983). Aversively stimulated aggression: violence in the family: Toward a theoretical integra-
Some parallels and differences in research with ani- tion. In W. R. Burr, R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & I. L. Reiss
mals and humans. American Psychologist, 388, 1135- (Eds.), Contemporary theories about the family (pp.
1144. 549-581).New York:FreePress.
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, conse- Gelles, R. J., & Straus,M. A. (1988). Intimateviolence.
quences, and control. Philadelphia: Temple Universi- New York: Simon & Schuster.
ty Press. Giles-Sims, J., Straus, M. A., & Sugarman, D. B.
Boyer, J. L., & Guthrie, L. (1985). Assessment and (1995). Child,maternal,andfamilycharacteristics
as-
treatmentof the suicidalpatient.In E. E. Beckham& sociated with corporal punishment. Family Relations,
Leber, W. R. (Eds.), Handbook of Depression: Treat- 44, 170-176.
ment, Assessment, and Research. Homewood, IL: Greenblat, C. S. (1983). A hit is a hit is a hit... Or is it?
Dorsey Press. Approval and tolerance of the use of physical force
Browning,J., & Dutton,D. (1986). Assessmentof wife by spouses. In D. Finkelhor, R. J. Gelles, G. T. Hotal-
assaultwith the ConflictTacticsScale: Using couple
840 Journal of Marriage and the Family

ing, & M. A. Straus (Eds.), The dark side offamilies Spock, B., & Rothenberg, M. B. (1992). Baby and child
(pp. 235-260). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. care. New York: Dutton.
Hagenaars, J. A. (1993). Loglinear models with latent Stark, R., & McEvoy, J. (1970). Middle-class violence.
variables. Quantitative applications in the social sci- Psychology Today, 4, 52-65.
ences. Newbury Park, CA: A Sage University Paper. Stets, J. E., & Straus, M. A. (1990). Gender differences
Hamilton, L. C. (1992). Regression with graphics. Pa- in reporting marital violence and its medical and psy-
cific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. chological consequences. In M. A. Straus & R. J.
Hamilton, L. C. (1993). Statistics with Stata 3. Belmont, Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in American families:
CA: Duxbury/Wordsworth. Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145
Holden, C. (1991). Depression: The news isn't depress- families (pp. 151-166). New Brunswick, NJ: Transac-
ing. Science, 254, 1450-1452. tion.
Jaffe, P. G., Wolfe, D. A., & Wilson, S. K. (1990). Chil- Strassberg, Z., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E.
dren of battered women. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. (1994). Spanking in the home and children's subse-
Johnson, M. E. (1984, August). Correlates of early vio- quent aggression toward kindergartenpeers. Develop-
lence experience among men who are abusive toward ment and Psychopathology, 6, 445-461.
female mates. Paper presented at the second National Straus, M. A. (1976). Sexual inequality, cultural norms,
Conference for Family Violence Researchers, and wife-beating. Victimology, 1, 54-76.
Durham, NH. Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and
Jones, A. (1994). Next time she'll be dead: Battering violence: The Conflict Tactics Scale. Journal of Mar-
and how to stop it. Boston: Beacon Press. riage and the Family, 41, 75-88.
Julian, T. W., & McKenry, P. C. (1993). Mediators of Straus, M. A. (1990). Corporal punishment, child abuse,
male violence toward female intimates. Journal of and wife-beating: What do they have in common? In
Family Violence, 8, 39-56. M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence
Kadushin, A., & Martin, J. A. (1981). Child abuse: An in American families: Riskfactors and adaptations to
interactional event. New York: Columbia University violence in 8,145 families (pp. 403-424). New
Press. Brunswick, NJ: transaction.
Kalmuss, D. S. (1984). The intergenerational transmis- Straus, M. A. (1991). Discipline and deviance: Physical
sion of marital aggression. Journal of Marriage and punishment of children and violence and other crime
the Family, 46, 11-19. in adulthood. Social Problems, 38, 133-152.
Kandel, E. (1992). Physical punishment and the devel- Straus, M. A. (1992). Children as witness to marital vio-
opment of aggressive and violent behavior. Unpub- lence: A risk factor for life-long problems among a
lished manuscript, Family Research Laboratory, Uni- nationally representative sample of American men
versity of New Hampshire, Durham. and women. In D. F. Schwarz (Ed.), Children and vi-
Kaufman Kantor, G., & Straus, M. A. (1990). Response olence: Report of the 23rd Ross Roundtable on Criti-
of victims and the police to assaults on wives. In cal Approaches to Common Pediatric Problems (pp.
M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence 98-104). Columbus, OH: Ross Laboratories.
in American families: Riskfactors and adaptations to Straus, M. A. (1993). Physical assaults by wives a major
violence (pp. 473-488). New Brunswick, NJ: Trans- social problem. In R. J. Gelles & D. R. Loseke (Eds.),
action Publishers. Current controversies on family violence (pp. 67-87).
Leach, P. (1991). Your growing child. New York: Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Knopf. Straus, M. A. (1994a). Beating the devil out of them:
Maiuro, R. D., Cahn, T. S., Vitaliano, P. P., Wagner, Corporal punishment in American families and its ef-
B. C., & Zegree, J. B. (1988). Anger, hostility, and fect on children. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass/Lexing-
depression in domestically violent versus generally ton.
assaultive men and nonviolent control subjects. Jour- Straus, M. A. (1994b). Corporal punishment of children
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 17-23. and depression and suicide in adulthood. In J. Mc-
Navaro, J. (1995). Uncovering gender differences in the Cord (Ed.), Coercion and punishment in long-term
use of marital violence: The effect of methodology. perspective (pp. 59-80). New York: Cambridge Uni-
Sociology, 29(3), 475-494. versity Press.
Newman, J. P. (1984). Sex differences in symptoms of Straus, M. A., & Donnelly, D. (1993). Corporal punish-
depression: Clinical disorder or normal distress. Jour- ment of teenage children in the United States. Youth
nal of Health and Social Behavior, 25, 136-159. and Society, 24, 419-442.
Owens, D. M., & Straus, M. A. (1975). The social struc- Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1990). Physical violence
ture of violence in childhood and approval of vio- in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to
lence as an adult. Aggressive Behavior, 1, 193-211. violence in 8,145 families. New Brunswick, NJ:
Peterson, G. W., & Rollins, B. C. (1987). Parent-child Transaction Books.
socialization. In M. R. Sussman & S. K. Steinmetz Straus, M. A., Gelles, R. J., & Steinmetz, S. K. (1980).
(Eds.), Handbook of marriage and the family (pp. Behind closed doors: Violence in the American fami-
471-508). New York: Plenum Press. ly. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. C., & Levin, H. (1957). Pat- Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S., & Sug-
terns of childrearing. New York: Row, Peterson. arman, D. B. (1996). The revised Conflict Tactics
Sherman, L. W., & Cohn, E. G. (1989). The impact of Scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psy-
research on legal policy: The Minneapolis domestic chometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283-
violence experiment. Law and Society Review, 23, 316.
117-144. Straus, M. A., & Kaufman Kantor, G. K. (1994). Corpo-
ral punishment by parents: A risk factor in the epi-
Corporal Punishment and Assaults on Spouses 841

demiology of depression, suicide, alcohol abuse, Szinovacz, M. E. (1983). Using couple data as a
child abuse and wife beating. Adolescence, 29, 543- methodological tool: The case of marital violence.
561. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 45, 633-644.
Straus, M. A., Kaufman Kantor, G. K., & Moore, D. M. Tolman, R. M., & Bennett, L. W. (1990). A review of
(1994, August). Trends in cultural norms approving quantitative research on men who batter. Journal of
marital violence from 1968 to 1968 in relation to Interpersonal Violence, 5, 87-118.
gender, class, and other social characteristics. Paper Turner, H. A., & Finkelhor, D. (1996). Corporal punish-
presented at the annual meeting of the American So- ment as a stressor among youth. Journal of Marriage
ciological Association, Los Angeles. and the Family, 58, 155-166.
Straus, M. A., & Mathur, A. K. (1994, September). So- Vissing, Y. M., Straus, M. A., Gelles, R. J., & Harrop,
cial change and change in approval of corporal pun- J. W. (1991). Verbal aggression by parents and psy-
ishment by parents from 1968 to 1992. Paper present- chosocial problems of children. Child Abuse and Ne-
ed at the International Symposium on Violence glect, 15, 223-238.
Against Children and Youth, University of Bielefeld, Waaland, P., & Keeley, S. (1985). Police decision mak-
Germany. ing in wife abuse: The impact of legal and extralegal
Straus, M. A., Sugarman, D., & Giles-Sims, J. (1995, factors. Law and Human Behavior, 9, 355-366.
April). Spanking by parents and anti-social behavior Wauchope, B. A., & Straus, M. A. (1990). Physical pun-
of children: A longitudinal analysis. Paper presented ishment and physical abuse of American children: In-
at the Conference on Research on Discipline: The cidence rates by age, gender and occupational class.
State of the Art, Deficits, and Implications. Division In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical vio-
of Community Pediatrics, University of North Caroli- lence in American families: Risk factors and adapta-
na Medical School. tions to violence (pp. 133-148). New Brunswick, NJ:
Straus, M. A., & Yodanis, C. L. (1994). Physical abuse. Transaction.
In M. A. Straus (Ed.), Beating the devil out of them: Yodanis, C. L. (1992). Corporal punishment and the
Corporal punishment in American families and its ef- fusion of love and violence. Unpublished master's
fects on children (pp. 81-98). San Francisco: Jossey- thesis, University of New Hampshire, Durham.
Bass/Lexington.

Families are Our Strength


4,000 professionals from family-related disciplines.
For over half a century, NCFR has linked multidisciplinary family professionals. Our non-partisan,
international membership includes:
* Educators * Researchers ? Therapists * Clergy ?
Psychologists * Social Workers * Health Care Professionals * Policy Makers
Membership Benefits
* Quarterly journals and newsletter:
Journalof Marriageand the Family-A prestigious and scientific forum for the publication of origi-
nal theory, research interpretation, and critical discussion of materials related to marriage and
the family. Recent topics include: work & family, violence & aggression, dating patterns, child-
hood residence, intergenerational and ethnic issues.
Family Relations-An applied scholarly journal designed for family practitioners. Emphasis is
placed on relationships across the life cycle with implications for intervention, education, and
public policy. Recent topics include: parent education, family therapy, family policy, gender and
diversity issues.
NCFR Report-A quarterly newsletter that updates NCFR members on national and internation-
al family issues.
* Special Interest Sections:
Family Policy, Feminism & Family Studies, Family Therapy, International, Family & Health,
Family Science, Education & Enrichment, Research & Theory, Ethnic Minorities, Religion &
Family Life.
* Reduced fees for Annual NCFR Conference.
* Member discount on NCFR books and other materials.
* Member discount on national certification program for Family Life Educators.
Writeor callfor a FREE brochure.
National Council on Family Relations
Att: Membership JMF11-96
3989 Central Avenue N.E., Suite 550
Minneapolis, MN 55421
(612) 781-9331 * FAX (612) 781-9348
Toll Free (888) 781-9331 * E-mail ncfr3989@ncfr.com

Potrebbero piacerti anche