Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

MENTAL RETARDATION AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES

RESEARCH REVIEWS 3: 313–322 (1997)

LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATION


IN FRAGILE X SYNDROME

Leonard Abbeduto1,2* and Randi Jenssen Hagerman3,4


1Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin
2Waisman Center on Mental Retardation and Human Development, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin
3Department of Pediatrics, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver, Colorado
4Child Development Unit, Children’s Hospital, Denver, Colorado

In this article, we describe the language and communication associated with FXS. In fact, this profile is frequently what first
problems of individuals with fragile X syndrome (FXS). FXS is a alerts clinicians to the possibility of an FXS diagnosis. In this
common genetic disorder resulting from a single-gene mutation on
the X chromosome. It is associated with a wide spectrum of physical, article, we describe what is known about the language problems
behavioral, cognitive, and language problems. Males are typically of individuals with FXS. We focus on both the nature and
more severely affected than females, with the vast majority of males severity of their problems and, where possible, distinguish
having mental retardation. Language and communication are nega- between those problems that may be unique to FXS and those
tively affected by problems in oral-motor structure and function and shared with other syndromes (e.g., Down syndrome, autism).
by conductive hearing loss associated with recurrent otitis media.
Speech problems of males with FXS include variability in rate and We also consider problems in language within the broader
stuttering-like repetition of sounds. The pattern of speech problems context of the behavioral and neurological development of
displayed by males is unique to FXS and may reflect a form of affected individuals. We begin, however, with a brief review of
developmental dyspraxia. Lexical development is serious delayed in the genetic mechanisms underlying the syndrome and of the
males with FXS. It is less clear, however, whether lexical development
keeps pace with achievements in cognitive development and whether
nonlinguistic dimensions of the behavioral phenotype.
receptive and expressive vocabularies are equally impaired. Morpho-
syntactic development is delayed in males with FXS, with receptive BACKGROUND
morphosyntax being mental-age-appropriate. It is less clear whether FXS is the most common (known) inherited cause of
expressive morphosyntactic keeps pace with mental age in affected mental retardation. Early studies demonstrated a prevalence of 1
males. Communication problems are characteristic of both males
and females and include features that are syndrome-specific. Most
per 1,200 in males and 1 per 2,500 in females [Webb et al.,
notable among the features displayed by males with FXS is persevera- 1986]. More recent molecular studies have suggested a
tion on a word, phrase, or topic in conversation. Several hypotheses prevalence in males closer to 1 per 4,000 [Turner et al., 1996].
have been advanced to explain this perseveration, but the most FXS results from a mutation on the X chromosome. Cyto-
promising focus is on hyperarousal and frontal-lobe-executive func- genetically, this mutation is manifested as a break, or fragile site,
tion deficits. Females with FXS display a run-on, disorganized, and
tangential style of conversation that may result from their well- at the bottom of the X chromosome. In 1991, the Fragile X
documented frontal-lobe-executive function deficits. Language and Mental Retardation 1 Gene (FMR1) was discovered and a
communication intervention for affected individuals requires coordi- trinucleotide repetitive sequence (CGG)n was found at the
nation of medical and behavioral approaches, with the involvement beginning of the gene [Verkerk et al., 1991]. In most non-FXS
of professionals from several disciplines. Future research must focus
on females, on language problems suggested by clinical experience,
individuals, there are approximately 5 to 50 CGG repeats
and on connections between language and communication prob- [Imbert and Mandel, 1995]. In FXS carriers, the repeat number
lems and problems at the neurological and molecular genetic is 53 to 200, which is termed the premutation. The premutation
levels. r 1997 Wiley-Liss, Inc. does not cause intellectual disability but there is a high risk of
MRDD Research Reviews 1997;3:313–322.
expansion when the premutation is passed to the next generation
through a female [Imbert and Mandel, 1995]. Individuals
Key Words: fragile X syndrome; speech; language; communication affected by FXS have more than 230 CGG repeats, and this is
termed a full mutation [Imbert and Mandel, 1995]. In the full

F
ragile X syndrome (FXS) is a common genetic disorder
associated with a broad spectrum of problems [Dykens et Grant sponsor: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development; Grant
numbers: R01 HD24356, P30 HD03352; Grant sponsor: Maternal and Child Health;
al., 1994; Hagerman, 1996a]. Some affected individuals Grant number: MCJ089413; Grant sponsor: March of Dimes; Grant number:
may present with only mild language, cognitive, or behavioral 12-FY96-0492.
problems, whereas others may display mental retardation or even *Correspondence to: Leonard Abbeduto, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Wais-
man Center, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, WI 53705-2280. E-mail:
fail to develop any spoken language. Despite this heterogeneity, abbeduto@waisman.wisc.edu
there is a characteristic pattern of language development and use

r 1997 Wiley-Liss, Inc.


mutation, the FMR1 gene typically is with a mosaic pattern, and 88 for males ated with FXS, the production of FMRP,
methylated, a process that shuts off the with an unmethylated or partially unmeth- and the neural functioning of individuals
gene and prevents the normal production ylated full mutation [Merenstein et al., with FXS. Such analyses, however, are
of the FMR1 protein (FMRP), which (as 1996]. It is important in studies of FXS, just beginning.
discussed below) is thought to be impor- therefore, that language problems are
tant for brain development [Hagerman, studied in relation to cognitive impair- LANGUAGE,
1996a; Oostra, 1996]. ments and that correlations are examined COMMUNICATION, AND
The phenotype of males with FXS between variations at the molecular- RELATED DOMAINS
is comprised of both physical and behav- genetic level and in the language profile. Language is not a unitary ability. It
ioral features. Physical features include a Such data, however, are virtually nonex- consists of several components (e.g., the
long face, prominent ears, and large istent. lexical and the morphosyntactic) that
testicles in adolescent or adult males Females with FXS are usually less emerge from different types of experi-
[Hagerman, 1996a]. Prepubertal males affected than males [Lachiewicz, 1995]. ences and that are tied more or less closely
typically do not have a long face, but the This is the case because females typically to achievements in other domains of
ears are usually prominent. There is have the full mutation on only one of psychological and behavioral functioning
evidence of a connective tissue dysplasia their two X chromosomes and, thus, the [Rondal, 1995]. In this section, we
or loose connective tissue in FXS leading unaffected chromosome moderates the consider the problems that individuals
to hyperextensible finger joints, soft and effects of the mutation [Imbert and with FXS have in acquiring and using
smooth skin, flat feet, and double-jointed Mandel, 1995]. Approximately 50 to 70% skills in the various components of
thumbs. Although these physical features of girls with the full mutation have IQs in language.
are seen in the majority of individuals the borderline or mentally retarded range
with FXS, young children may not have [de Vries et al., 1996]. Females with the Physical Structures Related to
obvious physical features and so clinicians full mutation but without mental retarda- Language
must rely on the behavioral manifesta- tion typically have learning problems, Expressing one’s thoughts through
tions of FXS when considering the including executive function problems spoken language presupposes a functional
diagnosis. that can lead to a limited attention span oral-motor system. Individuals with FXS,
In terms of behavioral features, and impulsivity [Mazzocco et al., 1993; particularly males, suffer from a number
males with FXS often display limited Hagerman et al., 1992; Sobesky et al., of oral-motor problems that could impact
attention spans and hyperactivity [Baum- 1996]. Social anxiety or avoidant disorder speech, including dramatic oral tactile
gartner et al., 1995]. They also may be and shyness are also frequent in females defensiveness [Scharfenaker et al., 1996],
oversensitive to tactile, auditory, olfac- with FXS [Freund et al., 1993]. This drooling because of a failure to swallow
tory, or visual stimuli. Many males with anxiety in combination with language reflexively as saliva builds up [Scharfena-
FXS also avoid eye contact [Cohen et al., impairments can lead to selective mutism ker et al., 1996], truncal hypotonia
1991] and may display autistic-like stereo- in females with the full mutation [Hager- involving the oral-facial muscles [Hager-
typies (e.g., hand flapping, hand biting) man, 1996a]. Because of the phenotypic man, 1996a], and a narrow and high
beginning in the second or third year of differences in males and females with arched palate with dental crowding
life [Hagerman, 1996a]. In fact, approxi- FXS, it is important to distinguish [Hagerman, 1996a]. In addition, cleft
mately 6% of boys with autism also have between them in language studies. palate is seen in 5% of children with FXS
FXS [Brown et al., 1986; Cohen et al., Recent research on the role of the [Partington, 1984], and prognathism oc-
1991]. It is important in studies of language FMRP in brain development promises to curs but typically only in adults with FXS
and FXS, therefore, to examine the relation greatly increase our understanding of the [Fryns et al., 1987; Loesch et al., 1995].
between language problems and the mechanisms involved in the development Although there is little doubt that these
social-emotional features that define FXS of the FXS behavioral phenotype. In functional and structural problems can
and to discriminate between affected particular, absence of the FMRP in interfere with speech, their full impact on
individuals with and without autism. individuals with FXS is hypothesized to the development of language and commu-
Cognitive deficits in males can be interfere with the pruning of neuronal nication in FXS has not been fully
severe. Mental retardation occurs in connections that typically occurs early in explored.
approximately 85% of males with the full development, with the result being an Hearing problems are also com-
mutation [Hagerman et al., 1994b]. enhanced number of neuronal connec- mon in FXS and may complicate the task
Young boys with FXS may have an IQ tions [Reiss et al., 1995a]. In fact, of learning language. In a study by
above 75 [Freund et al., 1995] only to Connery et al. [1997] have reported long, Hagerman et al. [1987], for example, 63%
show declines in IQ by late childhood or thin, and tortuous dendritic spines in of the 30 boys with FXS studied had
adolescence [Wright-Talamante et al., FMR1 knockout mice compared to recurrent bouts of otitis media in early
1996]. Most males with FXS but without unaffected littermates, which suggests childhood, compared to 15% of their
mental retardation have a variant DNA impaired synapse stabilization and prun- unaffected siblings and 38% of develop-
pattern, such as a full mutation without ing without FMRP. Neuroimaging stud- mentally disabled children without FXS.
complete methylation or a mosaic pattern ies in humans have demonstrated in- More recent studies show that 85% of
(i.e., the premutation in some cells with creased size of the hippocampus, caudate, boys with FXS have recurrent otitis
the full mutation and complete or partial and thalamus in individuals with FXS media [Hagerman, 1996a]. These prob-
methylation in others). These males may compared to controls, which is also lems may result from the combination of
be higher functioning because their cells consistent with a lack of normal neuronal changes in facial structure and the loose
contain a limited amount of FMRP pruning in FXS [Reiss et al., 1995a,b; connective tissue associated with FXS
[Tassone et al., unpublished data]. Mean Schapiro et al., 1995]. It is important, interfering with drainage through the
IQ is 41 for males with a completely therefore, to determine the relation eustachian tube. It is important to note,
methylated full mutation, 60 for males between the language problems associ- however, that systematic investigations of
314 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS • FRAGILE X SYNDROME • ABBEDUTO & HAGERMAN
the contributions of recurrent otitis me- [Paul et al., 1984]. These errors reflect Abbeduto, 1993]. Lexical learning de-
dia to the language development of simplification processes similar to those of pends on expertise in other components
individuals with FXS have not been younger, typically developing children of language (e.g., morphosyntax [Gleit-
conducted. rather than an atypical pattern of develop- man and Gillette, 1995] and on various
ment [Palmer et al., 1988]. nonlinguistic, cognitive capacities and
Speech Production Affected males also have been processes (e.g., categorization [Barrett,
The ability to produce speech observed to repeat sounds and syllables at 1995]). The social, cognitive, and mor-
sounds and sequence them appropriately high rates [Newell et al., 1983; Vilkman phosyntactic (described below) limita-
is critical for communication. Speech et al., 1988], although less than in tions associated with FXS suggest that
problems can lead to reduced intelligibil- stuttering [Paul et al., 1984]. They also lexical learning will be seriously dis-
ity (i.e., speech that is difficult for others have difficulty repeating nonreduplicated rupted. It is surprising, therefore, how
to understand) and can affect how a multisyllabic sequences, such as ‘‘pa-ta- little is known about lexical development
speaker is evaluated by others [Kent, ka’’ [Paul et al., 1984]. In contrast, they in FXS.
1993]. Speech production is problematic do reasonably well when repeating iso- Existing data demonstrate that males
for many individuals with FXS, and some lated sounds and words [Niemi et al., with FXS perform well below chrono-
speech problems are claimed to be 1985; Paul et al., 1984]. logical age expectations on both recep-
specific to the syndrome [Schopmeyer tive and expressive measures of vocabu-
and Lowe, 1992]. lary [Madison et al., 1986; Paul et al.,
Most notable among the speech
problems observed are those involving
The social, cognitive, and 1987; Sudhalter et al., 1992]. It is less
clear, however, whether lexical develop-
speaking rate, which have been well morphosyntactic ment keeps pace with achievements in
documented in males with FXS [Dykens limitations associated other domains (e.g., cognition). Madison
et al., 1994]. Affected males display an et al. [1986] reported that the adult males
unusual variability is speaking rate that with FXS suggest that they studied, all of whom were members
consists of unpredictable shifts from rapid of a single family, scored substantially
to slower rates [Hanson et al., 1986].
lexical learning will be higher on standardized tests of lexical
Variability in rate is seen across all levels seriously disrupted. It is knowledge than expected from their
of IQ [Hanson et al., 1986], but may be nonverbal mental ages. In contrast, Paul
more prevalent in adolescents and adults surprising, therefore, how et al. [1987] found that institutionalized
than in boys [Borghgraef, 1987]. Such little is known about adult males had vocabulary scores similar
variability in speaking rate is not seen in to those of age- and cognitively-matched
age- and cognitively-matched persons lexical development in males with nonspecific forms of mental
with Down syndrome [Wolf-Schein et FXS. retardation and males with autism. And
al., 1987], which leads to the hypothesis finally, Sudhalter et al. [1992] found that
that this variability in rate is unique to males with FXS supplied more semanti-
FXS. Verifying this hypothesis, however, cally incorrect words in a sentence
will require comparisons with a greater It has been argued that the profile completion task than did typically devel-
number of other mental retardation of speech problems described for affected oping children matched to them on
syndromes. males, which Hanson et al. [1986] adaptive behavior. Differences in mea-
Males with FXS also produce referred to as cluttering, reflects a develop- sures or participant characteristics (e.g.,
speech of low intelligibility relative to mental dyspraxia; that is, problems in developmental level) may be responsible
chronological age expectations [Paul et planning and controlling the complex for these inconsistent results.
al., 1984], although intelligibility prob- motor sequences involved in speaking. In It is also not clear whether recep-
lems may not be any more severe than fact, it has been suggested that this tive and expressive vocabularies are im-
those seen in other syndromes. Paul et al. dyspraxia is a manifestation of a more paired to the same degree in affected
[1987], for example, found that ratings of general inability to deal with sequential males. On the one hand, Madison et al.
speech intelligibility in conversation did information during both input and out- [1986] reported that their adult males
not differ between adult males with FXS put [Dykens et al., 1994]. It remains to be achieved higher scores in expression than
and males with Down syndrome and determined whether this problem is in comprehension of vocabulary. On the
males with autism matched to them on common to other syndromes. other hand, Paul et al. [1987] reported no
age, IQ, and length of institutionalization. It In contrast to the abundant re- differences between the receptive and
should be noted, however, that the partici- search on males, there are virtually no expressive vocabularies of the institution-
pants in Paul et al. [1987] had been data on females. In addition, little is alized adult males with FXS they studied.
institutionalized and had limited expressive known about the developmental course Again, the differences may be due to
skills. It remains to be determined whether of speech production in males or females. participant or measurement differences.
the intelligibility problems of higher There are also dimensions of speech, such We know less about the lexical
functioning, noninstitutionalized males as volume, pitch, and harshness, that development of females with FXS than
can be similarly characterized. clinical experience suggests are unusual in about that of males. The females in the
Males with FXS also omit, distort, FXS, but which have yet to be docu- family studied by Madison et al. [1986]
and substitute consonants and vowels in mented by systematic research. (only some of whom had mental retarda-
their conversational speech [Hanson et tion) showed little difference between
al., 1986; Newell et al., 1983; Vilkman et Lexical Development their receptive and expressive vocabular-
al., 1988], although there is considerable Lexical development is a life-long ies on average. In addition, we lack data
variability in the rates of such errors even process that is central to many concep- on the strategies that individuals with
among similarly aged males with FXS tions of intelligence [Rosenberg and FXS—whether male or female—use to
MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS • FRAGILE X SYNDROME • ABBEDUTO & HAGERMAN 315
learn new words. Typically developing from several other diagnostic groups. language impairments already described—
preschoolers, for example, make a variety Differences in the results of these studies individuals with FXS have fewer, less
of assumptions about how words relate to may be attributable to variations in well-developed linguistic tools available
referents (e.g., no word can refer to more participant characteristics and the small for communication than do similarly
than one category), and these assump- sample sizes. Moreover, interpretation of aged, typically developing individuals.
tions limit the range of potential mean- the studies is complicated by the lack of Communication problems also are to be
ings the children need to consider [Clark, appropriately matched typically develop- expected from the cognitive and social-
1995]. It is not known whether persons ing comparison groups. emotional limitations associated with
with FXS make similar assumptions. We Research on the morphosyntactic FXS because communication draws
also lack data on the developmental development of females with FXS is heavily on such supporting competencies
course of lexical learning in FXS. virtually nonexistent [Dykens et al., [Abbeduto and Rosenberg, 1987]. Com-
1994]. Madison et al. [1986], however, munication, however, also requires prag-
Morphosyntactic Development did study the members of one extended matic skills and knowledge, which are
Morphosyntax refers to the rules family that included six females whose specific to the task of communication
and elements that govern the combina- IQs fell in the average to moderate range [Abbeduto and Rosenberg, 1987]. Prag-
tion of words into phrases and sentences, of mental retardation. Although the matics include, for example, knowledge
mark grammatical functions such as MLUs of most of the adult females of the procedures for taking turns at
subject and object, and express notions tended to be quite high, the one young talking [Sack et al., 1974] and the ability
such as plurality and tense. Morphosyntax girl studied achieved an MLU far lower to solicit the information needed to
is the linguistic vehicle for expressing than expected from either her nonverbal resolve comprehension failures [Clark,
meaning and ‘‘the major source of the mental age or receptive language age. 1996]. It appears that persons with FXS
infinite combinatorial capability of lan- In closing this section, it is impor- have special difficulty in acquiring and
guage users’’ [Rosenberg and Abbeduto, tant to note that the methods of the using pragmatic skills. Simply put, their
1993, p. 82]. Included in the morphosyn- studies reviewed have been limited in communication is often less adequate
tactic competence of most adult speakers two respects. First, the data collected than expected on the basis of their
of English would be the knowledge that typically have been summarized by only a speech, language, cognitive, and social-
variations in word order signal differences few rather gross indices that collapse emotional impairments.
in meaning and knowledge of the ways in across performance on many different This conclusion is supported by
which plural and singular nouns are morphosyntactic elements and rules (e.g., studies of adaptive behavior. Adaptive
distinguished linguistically. MLU). This has made it impossible to behavior refers to the ability to meet
Affected males have been found to determine whether some aspects of mor- age-appropriate demands imposed by the
perform below chronological age expec- phosyntax are more impaired than others daily life tasks typical for the culture
tations in both receptive and expressive in males with FXS. More importantly, [Dykens, 1995]. Many such tasks require
morphosyntax [Dykens et al., 1994; such measures tell us little about the communication and, thus, communica-
Schopmeyer and Lowe, 1992]. More strategies used by individuals with FXS to tive behaviors are included in measures of
interesting are the findings concerning learn morphosyntax. Identification of adaptive behavior [Dykens, 1995]. In
morphosyntactic functioning in relation these strategies requires analysis of the fact, the Vineland Adaptive Behavior
to nonlinguistic cognitive functioning. sequence of acquisition of specific forms Scales (VABS) [Sparrow et al., 1984],
Such findings address the issue of whether or of errors in the use of specific forms. which is a commonly used informant
morphosyntax poses special problems for Second, expressive language samples have measure [Dykens, 1995], yields scores in
males with FXS (i.e., over and above been collected almost exclusively in four domains, one of which is Communi-
those expected based on their intellectual conversational contexts that are poorly cation. Interestingly, the only longitudi-
retardation). The results on comprehen- standardized in terms of materials, activi- nal data available on communication in
sion are clear: Receptive morphosyntax is ties, and the behavior of the conversa- FXS come from studies of adaptive
commensurate with nonverbal mental tional partner. Without some degree of behavior.
age in affected boys [Paul et al., 1984] and standardization, it is difficult to interpret The scores of males with FXS are
men [Paul et al., 1987], although there is comparisons across diagnostic groups or significantly below chronological age
a need to address this issue in larger, more individuals [Abbeduto et al., 1995]. The expectations for all domains of the VABS,
diverse samples (e.g., in terms of develop- heavy reliance on conversational samples including communication [Dykens, 1995].
mental levels). also has limited the generalizability of In fact, the VABS scores of affected
Results for expression are less clear. results. In fact, there are other naturally males are closer to their mental ages than
Paul et al. [1984] studied three 10- to occurring language tasks (e.g., story to their chronological ages [Dykens et al.,
13-year-olds with FXS, all of whom had telling) that require expressive language, 1988]. Although problems in communi-
mental retardation, and found delays in and morphosyntactic aspects of expres- cation characterize males with FXS
the morphosyntax of their conversational sive language vary considerably across throughout development, studies of adap-
language relative to their nonverbal tasks for many diagnostic groups [Ab- tive behavior suggest that these problems
mental ages. In contrast, the males studied beduto et al., 1995; Dollaghan et al., may become more severe in adolescence.
by Madison et al. [1986] displayed MLUs 1990]. In adolescence, the scores of affected
(i.e., mean length of utterances) equal to males on the Communication and
or in advance of mental-age expectations Communication Socialization domains of the VABS
on average, and neither Ferrier et al. In this section, we consider the begin to lag behind those on Daily Living
[1991] nor Paul et al. [1987] found ability of individuals with FXS to commu- Skills [Dykens et al., 1989, 1994, 1996;
differences in expressive morphosyntax nicate with others through language. Weigers et al., 1993]. The cause of this
between males with FXS and age- and Problems in communication are to be change during adolescence is not known
cognitive-ability matched groups of males expected in light of the speech and [Dykens, 1995]. Perhaps adolescence
316 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS • FRAGILE X SYNDROME • ABBEDUTO & HAGERMAN
brings communication (and socialization) research has been conducted on the esis, perseveration is a strategy emerging
tasks that increasingly involve unfamiliar communication problems of females with from the need to talk in the face of an
people and settings. Males with FXS find FXS, although it has been suggested that inability to find the words needed to
unfamiliar social situations to be stressful they display a run-on, disorganized, and express intended meanings. Sudhalter et
[Cohen, 1995] and their communication tangential style of talking that is related to al. [1992] tested this hypothesis by
may suffer as a result (see below). their executive function deficits [Benetto comparing the sentence-completion per-
In an attempt to better understand and Pennington, 1996; Mazzocco et al., formance of males with FXS to typically
the nature of these communication prob- 1993; Sobesky et al., 1996]. Second, developing boys and girls who were
lems, researchers have turned to direct communication has been assessed almost matched to them in terms of their
observation of the behavior of affected exclusively within a single context— communication ages as determined by
males in either naturally occurring or conversation—and with a limited range the VABS. The sentences presented
semistructured social interactions. In these of partners—almost always a familiar could be completed with a single word
studies, males with FXS consistently caregiver or an experimenter. But com- and differed as to whether they imposed
perform below chronological age expec- munication occurs in many other con- high or low constraints on the word to be
tations on virtually all dimensions of texts (e.g., story telling, entering the selected. Sudhalter et al. found that the
communication examined. In fact, there ongoing activities of others) and with a males with FXS were more likely than
is evidence that they perform more variety of partners (e.g., peers, teachers), the typically developing children to
poorly than do developmental level- each of which requires somewhat differ- produce semantically incorrect lexical
matched individuals with other diagnoses ent skills and knowledge [Rosenberg and responses, especially for sentences with
(e.g., autism, Down syndrome, nonspe- Abbeduto, 1993]. The ability of individu- low constraints. They interpreted this
cific mental retardation) on some dimen- als with FXS to communicate in these finding as supporting the word-retrieval
sions of communication, including the other contexts and with other important hypothesis. Note, however, that these
overuse of highly routinized phrases [e.g., partners remains to be determined. Third, findings demonstrate only that males with
Madison et al., 1996; Paul et al., 1987; there has been no serious attempt to FXS have difficulties with both persevera-
Sudhalter et al., 1990] and the production characterize the ability of individuals with tion and word-retrieval, not that these
of utterances that are only loosely related FXS to fulfill the requirements of the two problems are causally related. Testing
(i.e., tangential) to the conversational listener’s role despite the fact that their the word-retrieval hypothesis would re-
topic [Sudhalter, 1996]. attentional deficits make comprehension quire demonstrating that the rate of
Most notable among the findings problems especially likely. And, finally, perseveration is correlated with the rate
to date, however, are those concerning there have been few attempts to trace the of word-retrieval difficulties.
perseveration (i.e., excessive self-repeti- emergence of the communicative prob- The third hypothesis is that perse-
tion of a word, phrase, sentence, or lems of affected individuals over the veration is a reflection of hyperarousal.
topic). Perseveration is frequent in the course of development [Dykens et al., Both Cohen [1995] and Belser and
language production of many males with 1994]. Sudhalter [1995] have argued that males
FXS [Borghgraef et al., 1987; Hanson et with FXS are excessively aroused by
al., 1986; Sudhalter et al., 1990; but see Causes of Perseveration various classes of stimuli, especially those
Paul et al., 1987]. In fact, males with FXS Because of the pervasive, disruptive that include an interpersonal component,
have been found to engage in more effects of perseveration on the conversa- and that this heightened arousal causes
self-repetition than do age- and IQ- tions of males with FXS, researchers in the high rates of perseveration. In support
matched males with Down syndrome or this area have devoted considerable atten- of this hypothesis, they point out that
with nonspecific forms of mental retarda- tion to its explanation. In this section, we perseveration co-occurs with a variety of
tion [Ferrier et al., 1991; Reiss and briefly consider the four explanations that nonverbal behaviors reflective of anxiety,
Freund, 1992; Wolf-Schein et al., 1987]. have been offered. The first can be including self-stimulation (e.g., hand flap-
Moreover, males with FXS who do not termed the deficient expressive morphosyntax ping) and social avoidance (e.g., actively
meet diagnostic criteria for autism are hypothesis [Sudhalter et al., 1991]. Ac- avoiding eye contact with others). These
more likely to produce self-repetitions cording to this hypothesis, perseveration anxiety-related behaviors also occur at
than are non-FXS males with autism represents a strategy for participating in higher rates in males with FXS than in
[Ferrier et al., 1991]. Such findings conversation when a failure to master age- and IQ-matched males with Down
suggest that perseveration may be unique morphosyntax makes meaningful contri- syndrome [Belser and Sudhalter, 1995]
to FXS, although comparisons with a butions all but impossible. The problem and increase in socially stressful situations
wider range of mental retardation syn- with this hypothesis is that it is not clear [Cohen, 1995]. In a direct test of this
dromes than studied to date are needed to why perseveration rather than echolalia hypothesis, Belser and Sudhalter found
decide the issue. Interestingly, males with or some other pattern of suboptimal that the skin conductance levels of two
FXS—at least those without autism—do language performance should arise as a males with FXS were higher when their
not engage in frequent echolalia (i.e., strategy for the individual with FXS. Nor conversational partner initiated eye con-
repetition of the linguistic contributions is the hypothesis supported by the data: tact than when eye contact was not
of other people [Ferrier et al., 1991]), Sudhalter et al. [1991] did not find a initiated. Belser and Sudhalter also found
which suggests that the perseveration correlation between measures of expres- no variation in skin conductance across
observed in FXS does not reflect a sive syntactic maturity and the rate of the eye contact conditions for two
general tendency to repeat any previous perseveration in a group of 19 males with control participants, one of whom had
behavior. In a later section we consider FXS. Down syndrome and one of whom had
research on the cause(s) of perseveration. The word-retrieval deficit hypothesis ADHD. Because of the small sample size,
In concluding this section, it is of Sudhalter et al. [1992] is the second however, the generalizability of the
important to note several limitations of explanation offered for the perseveration relationships observed by Belser and
the research on pragmatics. First, little of FXS males. According to this hypoth- Sudhalter and their specificity to FXS
MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS • FRAGILE X SYNDROME • ABBEDUTO & HAGERMAN 317
require further study. It is also necessary INTERVENTION using blow toys and whistles or foods of
to specify the psychological mechanisms Because of the broad spectrum of variable textures. Blow toys such as
by which hyperarousal exerts its effects involvement in FXS, it is important to harmonicas, kazoos, and various other
and, thereby, why this particular pattern individualize each therapy program to the ‘‘action’’ whistles need differing amounts
of linguistic performance occurs rather child’s strengths and weaknesses. Vastly of tongue and lip strength to activate.
than some other pattern of suboptimal different approaches must be utilized Additionally, the graded respiration
performance [see Cohen, 1995, for some with the nonverbal child who has moder- needed to work these whistles can help
interesting suggestions in this regard]. ate retardation compared to a child who regulate arousal state [Oetter et al., 1995].
The final hypothesis relates perse- presents with only mild auditory process- Snacking on crunchy and chewy
veration to executive function deficits ing problems and a mild deficit in foods such as Bazooka bubble gum, fruit
and frontal lobe dysfunction. Executive comprehension. Nevertheless, the consis- leather, bagels, and pretzels, or chewing
function deficits have been well docu- tent problems described for males and on rubber tubing can help children with
mented for females with FXS [Mazzocco females who are significantly affected by FXS achieve increased jaw stability and
et al., 1993; Sobesky et al., 1996] and are FXS warrant specific approaches in may decrease oral defensiveness and gag
suspected in males with FXS, although therapy. Suggestions about intervention, reflexes. Direct deep pressure input
their deficits have been more difficult to however, must be based only on clinical through massage to the face, lips, and
measure because of their lower levels of experience because data on treatment gums under the guidance of a speech-
intellectual functioning. Executive func- effectiveness are virtually nonexistent. language pathologist or OT also may help
tion deficits are thought to reflect frontal Medical intervention must be orga- reduce oral defensiveness.
lobe dysfunction and deficits in inhibition nized for the problems that impact speech A significant concern in the devel-
[Bennetto and Pennington, 1996]. A lack and language development, including opment of children with FXS is the IQ
of appropriate inhibition of a high- conductive hearing loss, significant atten- decrease over time in approximately 30%
strength response and repetition of that tion problems or ADHD, and anxiety. of cases [Wright-Talamante et al., 1996].
response could lead to perseveration. In Aggressive use of PE tubes for a conduc- Because cognitive capabilities are essen-
fact, neuroimaging studies have demon- tive hearing loss is essential in early tial for acquiring new language skills and
strated that the caudate, which is involved childhood [Hagerman et al., 1987]. using them in communication, abstract
in the frontal circuits necessary for ADHD can be significantly improved reasoning skills should be a focus in
effective functioning, is enlarged in males with medication, including stimulants language therapy. For preschool and
(and females) with FXS [Abrams and and/or clonidine [Hagerman, 1996b; kindergarten children, early reasoning
Reiss, 1995]. It is thought that enlarge- Hagerman et al., 1988, 1995]. These concepts, including sorting and categoriz-
ment of the caudate may reflect a failure medications can make a dramatic differ- ing, can be topics for therapy, with more
of the processes involved in pruning ence in the ability of a child with FXS to complex reasoning tasks addressed as the
neuronal connections [Comery et al., sit and focus on the language interven- individual with FXS matures and in
1997; Reiss et al., 1994, 1995a,b]. It is tion. Stimulants also improve auditory high-functioning individuals.
possible that these problems in neuronal processing and motor coordination, which For the most severely involved
connectedness facilitate activation of high- can impact oral motor coordination nonverbal individual with FXS, the use
strength linguistic responses and without [Barkley, 1990]. Because anxiety can of combined speech and language therapy
appropriate frontal inhibition, persevera- worsen perseveration and interfere with and occupational therapy is helpful
tion could result. Problems in excessive speech production [Sudhalter, 1992], [Scharfenaker et al., 1996]. The utiliza-
neuronal connectedness also may be the aggressive treatment of anxiety is essen- tion of movement, rhythm, and music
basis of the hyperarousal associated with tial. The use of selective serotonin with sensory integration techniques pro-
FXS. reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) can dramati- vided by an OT and singing may help to
In summary, the cause of persevera- cally decrease anxiety and improve mood stimulate language. A total communica-
tion in males with FXS is not clear. [Hagerman, 1996a]. The first SSRI on tion approach in which signing is used as
Although deficits in expressive morpho- the market, Prozac, has a significant a bridge to speech also can facilitate
syntax and word-retrieval are not the activation effect which may improve language development. Augmentative
basis of perseveration, deficits in other social interactions and in some cases social communication devices, ranging from a
aspects of language may well be involved. aspects of language [Hagerman et al., communication picture board to comput-
The hyperarousal hypothesis is promising 1994a; Kramer, 1993]. Prozac also is the ers, are also helpful. Computer software
and offers the additional advantages of treatment of choice for selective mutism helpful for children with FXS [Scharfena-
explaining several dimensions of the [Black and Uhde, 1994]. ker et al., 1996b] and computer technol-
social behavior of affected males (e.g., Children with FXS demonstrate ogy resource centers in the United States
self-stimulation and body orientation) low tone in the trunk as well as the oral can be found in Scharfenaker et al.
and of connecting the verbal (i.e., per- area. Poor trunk support can sometimes [1996c].
severation) and nonverbal (e.g., gaze lead to ‘‘fixing’’ oral structures in an Young children with FXS and
avoidance) dimensions of their communi- attempt to gain needed stability. Poor moderate to severe retardation often
cative behavior. The frontal-executive separation of tongue, lip, and jaw move- benefit from training programs designed
function hypothesis promises to explain ments then occurs, affecting sound pro- for children with severe autism. The
several dimensions of the FXS behavioral duction skills. Working with an occupa- TEAACH program from North Carolina
phenotype (e.g., the inability to inhibit tional therapist (OT) and physical therapist and other programs for autistic children
responses to external as well as internal (PT) on strengthening stability in the that emphasize language development
stimuli) and offers the advantage of tying trunk may benefit speech production. and social interaction [Rogers et al.,
together work on FMRP expression, Increased oral tone can be achieved 1986, 1991] are often helpful for children
brain development, and communicative through hands-on oral resistance exer- with FXS. In the case of more mildly
behavior. cises or through more adaptive activities affected children, milieu approaches,
318 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS • FRAGILE X SYNDROME • ABBEDUTO & HAGERMAN
which introduce new language forms in nate in light of clear evidence lenges of the varied language
the context of naturally occurring at- that cognitive and social func- tasks that they encounter in
tempts to communicate [Rosenberg and tioning decline during the ado- school, at home, and in the
Abbeduto, 1993], should be used over lescent and early adult years. workplace. Research to date
more didactic approaches whenever pos- These declines, together with has focused almost exclusively
sible. a demand for more abstract on conversation with familiar
Utilization of the imitation strengths forms of language in school caregivers or with an experi-
in FXS can help to improve articulation and in peer relationships dur- enced researcher or clinician.
and social skills by modeling appropriate ing adolescence, suggest that Future research should exam-
speech in social settings. Individuals with the language problems of indi- ine tasks such as narration,
FXS may also have intense interest areas viduals with FXS may worsen entering ongoing play groups,
that can be pulled into therapy to over the course of develop- academically oriented prob-
improve their attention and willingness ment. Both cross-sectional and lem-solving tasks, and non-
to verbalize. Themes of interest can be longitudinal data are needed, face-to-face interactions such
the basis of language stimulation. Word especially those focused on the as talking on the telephone.
processors that utilize visual and auditory transition into adolescence. Partners should be more varied
feedback are particularly motivating for as well, and include teachers,
expanding written language. peers, and adults who vary in
Pragmatics must be an important Recent neuroimaging their familiarity with the par-
therapy focus and may be best treated in a ticipant. Such variations in task
group therapy situation. Again, milieu research, however, has and partner are associated with
rather than didactic approaches should be
used whenever possible. Intervention can
uncovered changes in differing language and commu-
nication demands [Rosenberg
also include field trips to restaurants or many brain structures and Abbeduto, 1993] and may
stores to develop the language skills uncover heretofore hidden
needed in these highly scripted events. As with ties to many communication impairments
the individual with FXS moves into psychological functions. or strengths in individuals with
adolescence and adulthood, the prag- FXS.
matic aspects of various vocational set- Establishing the 5. Several language and commu-
tings should be addressed in therapy and connections between these nication problems have been
tied to job skills training. Intervention hypothesized to be unique to
techniques developed for other popula- and other atypical neural FXS (e.g., perseveration).
tions of adults with mental retardation
[e.g., Calculator and Bedrosian, 1988]
patterns and the language These hypotheses about syn-
drome uniqueness have been
might prove useful for adults with FXS as and connection problems based largely on comparisons
well. of individuals with FXS with autism, Down syndrome,
or nonspecific forms of mental
FUTURE RESEARCH is an important challenge retardation. Additional data in-
Despite the considerable research
conducted on the language and related for future research. volving comparisons with a
wider variety of mental retarda-
problems associated with FXS, more tion syndromes are needed to
work is needed. Here are our suggestions confirm these hypotheses.
for future research. 3. Much of the research to date 6. Explaining the within-group
1. There is a desperate need for has focused on determining variability in language and
more research on the language how far individuals with FXS communication problems seen
and communication problems have progressed in language in FXS will require that we
of females with FXS. Many of and communication relative to look to variations at the mo-
the language problems de- some standard (e.g., develop- lecular genetic level. Varia-
scribed for males with FXS are mental level-matched peers). tions in FMRP expression
apt be shared with females What is lacking, however, are have already been tied to some
(albeit in less severe forms, on data on the strategies that these features of the behavioral phe-
average). Nevertheless, differ- individuals use to acquire new notype [Hagerman, 1996a;
ences in the cognitive and language skills. Ultimately, ef- Tassone et al., 1997]. It seems
social-emotional development fective intervention for indi- likely that ties to language and
of affected males and females, viduals with FXS will require communication problems also
as well as the different life data on learning strategies be- exist.
histories normally experienced cause it is those strategies that 7. There is a need to examine the
by males and females in our must be improved if we are to relations between atypical pat-
culture, make it likely that prevent some of the language terns in brain structure and
there will be gender-specific and communication problems function and the development
language and communication that characterize these indi- and use of language. We have
problems as well. viduals. already noted the possibility
2. We know little about the 4. There is a need for data on the that executive function deficits
developmental course of lan- ability of persons with FXS to and frontal lobe dysfunction
guage in FXS. This is unfortu- meet the communicative chal- may play a role in the per-
MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS • FRAGILE X SYNDROME • ABBEDUTO & HAGERMAN 319
severation typical of males with 10. Many families and researchers Barkley RA. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disor-
FXS. Recent neuroimaging are looking to the future for der: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment.
New York: Guilford Press, 1990.
research [Abrams and Reiss, the benefits of protein therapy Barrett M. Early lexical development. In: Fletcher
1995], however, has uncov- or gene therapy in FXS. How- P, MacWhinney B, eds. The handbook of
ered changes in many brain ever, the hurdles to be over- child language. Oxford: Blackwell, 1995:362–
structures (e.g., the caudate come before protein or gene 392.
therapy becomes a reality are Baumgardner T, Reiss AL, Freund LS, et al.
nucleus, hippocampus, and Specifications of the neurobehavioral associa-
thalamus) with ties to many monumental [Rattazzi and Io- tions in males with fragile X syndrome.
psychological functions (e.g., annou, 1996]. Until that time, Pediatrics 1995;95:744–752.
executive function, attention, new pharmacological interven- Belser RC, Sudhalter V. Arousal difficulties in males
emotional control, motor pro- tions will no doubt be more with fragile X syndrome: A preliminary
report. Dev Brain Dysfunct 1995;8:270–279.
gramming). Establishing the helpful for enhancing cogni- Bennetto L, Pennington BF. The neuropsychology
connections between these and tive function. The nootropics of fragile X syndrome. In: Hagerman RJ,
other atypical neural patterns that may improve memory Cronister AC eds. Fragile X syndrome:
and the language and commu- and learning through enhance- diagnosis, treatment, and research. 2nd edi-
ment of cholinergic systems tion. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Univer-
nication problems of individu- sity Press, 1996:210–248.
als with FXS is an important are presently undergoing ani- Black B, Uhde TW. Treatment of elective mutism
challenge for future research. mal studies. These may prove with fluoxetine: A double blind placebo
8. There are several language- to be useful in FXS along with controlled study. J Am Acad Child Adoles
related problems that have other medication innovations. Psychiatry 1994;33:1000–1006.
11. And finally, our focus here Borghgraef M, Fryns JP, Dielkins A, et al. Fragile X
been observed clinically, but syndrome: A study of the psychological
that have yet to be investigated has been on those individuals profile in 23 prepubertal patients. Clin Genet
systematically. Two examples with FXS who develop some 1987;32:179–186.
are selective mutism (i.e., the language. In fact, some se- Brown WT, Jenkins EC, Friedman E, et al. Fragile
verely affected individuals, par- X and autism: A multicenter survey. Am J
failure to speak in selected Med Genet 1986;23:341–352.
contexts) and self-talk (i.e., ticularly males, fail to develop Calculator SN, Bedrosian JL, eds. Communication
talking aloud but not for pur- spoken language at all. Clinical assessment and intervention for adults with
poses of communicating with experience suggests that this mental retardation. Boston: Little, Brown,
others). Selective mutism has failure to speak is not solely a 1988.
function of the degree of Clark EV. Later lexical development and word
been observed in some girls formation. In Fletcher P, MacWhinney B
with FXS and may reflect the mental retardation charac- (eds): The handbook of child language.
debilitating effects of anxiety terizing the individual. It is Oxford: Blackwell, 1995:393–412.
on the use of existing language important to identify the pre- Clark HH. Using language. New York: Cambridge
dictors of the transition into University Press, 1996.
and communication skills. Re-
spoken communication so that Cohen IL. A theoretical analysis of the role of
search verifying this relation- hyperarousal in the learning and behavior of
ship is needed, as is research on we can identify individuals fragile X males. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res
the role of SSRIs in alleviating most at risk as well as develop Rev 1995;1:286–291.
this condition. Self-talk has interventions to promote their Cohen IL, Sudhalter V, Pfadt A, et al. Why are
acquisition of spoken lan- autism and the fragile X syndrome associated?
been noted in many individu-
als with FXS, especially males. guage. j Conceptual and methodological issues. Am J
Hum Genet 1991;48:195–202.
It has many interesting charac- Comery TA, Harris JB, Willems PJ, et al. Abnormal
teristics, including shifts in ACKNOWLEDGMENTS dendritic spines in fragile X knockout mice:
register and voice. It also often Maturation and pruning deficits. Proc Natl
Preparation of this article was Acad Sci USA 1997;94:5401–5404.
takes the form of ‘‘mum- supported by National Institute of Child Cromer R. A case study of dissociations between
bling.’’ There is a need to Health and Human Development grants language and cognition. In: Tager-Flusberg
document the frequency of R01 HD24356 to L. Abbeduto and P30 H, ed. Constraints on language acquisition:
self-talk as well as to determine HD03352 to the Waisman Center and by Studies of atypical children. Hillsdale, NJ:
its functions for the individual Erlbaum, 1994:141–153.
grants MCJ089413 from Maternal and deVries BBA, Wiegers AM, Smits APT, et al.
and the conditions that elicit it. Child Health and 12-FY96-0492 from Mental status of females with an FMR1 gene
Such research is needed before the March of Dimes awarded to Dr. full mutation. Am J Hum Genet 1996;58:
decisions can be made about Hagerman. 1025–1032.
whether and how to modify Dollaghan CA, Campbell TF, Tomlin R. Video
narration as a language sampling context. J
the self-talk of affected indi- Speech Hear Res 1990;55:582–590.
viduals. REFERENCES
Dykens EM. Adaptive behavior in males with
9. There is an urgent need for Abbeduto L, Rosenberg S. Linguistic communica- fragile X syndrome. Ment Retard Dev Disabil
tion and mental retardation. In: Rosenberg S,
controlled outcome studies re- ed. Advances in applied psycholinguistics.
Res Rev 1995;1:281–285.
garding specific therapy ap- Dykens EM, Leekman J, Paul R, et al. Cognitive,
Vol. 1: Disorders of first language develop- behavioral, and adaptive functioning in fragile
proaches in FXS. Clinically, ment. Cambridge: Cambridge University X and non-fragile X retarded men. J Autism
we have seen the combined Press, 1987:76–125. Dev Disord 1988;18:41–53.
approaches of medication, oc- Abbeduto L, Benson G, Short K, et al. Effects of Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Leckman JF. Adaptive
cupational therapy, and speech sampling context on the expressive language and maladaptive functioning of institutional-
of children and adolescents with mental ized and noninstitutionalized males with
and language therapy to work retardation. Ment Retard 1995;33:279–288. fragile X syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
well synergistically, although, Abrams MT, Reiss AL. The neurobiology of fragile Psychiatry 1989;28:427–430.
again, data are lacking for X syndrome. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Ort SI, et al. Trajectory
outcome research. Rev 1995;1:269–275. of adaptive behavior in males with fragile X

320 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS • FRAGILE X SYNDROME • ABBEDUTO & HAGERMAN


syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 1993;23:135– tion: Research foundations for intervention. Reiss AL, Freund L. Behavioral phenotype of
145. Baltimore, MD: Brooks, 1993:223–239. fragile X syndrome: DSM-III-R autistic
Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Leckman JF. Behavior Kramer PD. Listening to Prozac. New York: behavior in male children. Am J Med Genet
and development in fragile X syndrome. Viking/Penguin, 1993. 1992;43:35–46.
Thousands Oak, CA: Sage, 1994. Lachiewicz AM. Females with fragile X syndrome: Reiss AL, Lee J, Freund L. Neuroanatomy of fragile
Dykens EM, Ort S, Cohen I, et al. Trajectories and A review of the effects of an abnormal gene. X syndrome: The temporal lobe. Neurology
profiles of adaptive behavior in males with Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 1995;1: 1994;44:1317–1324.
fragile X syndrome: Multicenter studies. J 292–297. Reiss A, Abrams M, Greenlaw R, et al. Neuro
Autism Dev Disord 1996;26:287–301. Lachiewicz AM, Hoegerman SF, Holmgren G, et developmental effects of the FMR-1 full
Ferrier LJ, Bashir AS, Meryash DL, et al. Conversa- al. Association of the Robin sequence with mutation in humans. Nat Med 1995a;1:159–
tional skills of individuals with fragile-x the fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet 167.
syndrome: A comparison with autism and 1991;41:275–278. Reiss AL, Freund LS, Baumgardner TL, et al.
Down syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol Loesch DZ, Huggins RM, Hoang NH. Growth in Contribution of the FMR1 gene mutation to
1991;33:776–788. stature in fragile X families: A mixed longitu- human intellectual dysfunction. Nat Genet
Freund LS, Reiss AL, Abrams M. Psychiatric dinal study. Am J Med Genet 1995;58:249– 1995b;11:331–334.
disorders associated with fragile X in the 256. Rogers S, DiLalla. A comparative study of the effect
young female. Pediatrics 1993;91:321–329. Lowe F, Schopmeyer B, Kaye S. Combined of a development based preschool curriculum
Freund LS, Peebles CD, Aylward E, et al. treatment. In Schopmeyer BB, Lowe F (eds): on young children with autism and young
Preliminary report on cognitive and adaptive The fragile X child. San Diego: Singular children with other disorders of behavior and
behaviors of preschool aged males with fragile Publishing, 1992:173–195. development. TECSE 1991;11:29–47.
X. Dev Brain Dys 1995;8:242–251. Madison LS, George C, Moeschler JB. Cognitive Rogers SJ, Herbision JM, Lewis HC, et al. An
Fryns JP, Haspeslagh M, Dereymaeker AM, et al. A functioning in the fragile-X syndrome: A approach for enhancing symbolic communica-
peculiar subphenotype in the fra(X) syn- study of intellectual, memory, and communi- tive interpersonal functioning of young chil-
drome: Extreme obesity, short stature, stubby cation skills. J Ment Defic Res 1986;30:129– dren with autism or severe emotional handi-
hands and feet, diffuse hyperpigmentation. 148. caps. J Dis Early Child 1986;10:135–148.
Further evidence of disturbed hypothalamic Mazzocco M, Pennington B, Hagerman R. The Rondal JA. Exceptional language development in
function in the fra(X) syndrome? Clin Genet neurocognitive phenotype of female carriers Down syndrome: implications for the cogni-
1989;32:388–392. of fragile X: Further evidence for specificity. J tion-language relationship. Cambridge, UK:
Gleitman LR, Gillette J. The role of syntax in verb Dev Behav Pediatr 1993;14:328–335. Cambridge University Press, 1995.
learning. In Fletcher P, MacWhinney B (eds): Merenstein SA, Sobesky WE, Taylor AK, et al. Rosenberg S, Abbeduto L. Language and commu-
The handbook of child language. Oxford: Molecular-clinical correlations in males with nication in mental retardation: Development,
Blackwell, 1995:413–428. an expanded FMR1 mutation. Am J Med processes, and intervention. Hillsdale, NJ:
Goldson E, Hagerman RJ. Fragile X syndrome and Genet 1996;64:389–394. Erlbaum, 1993.
failure to thrive. AJDC 1993;147:605–607. Newell K, Sanborn B, Hagerman R. Speech and
Rousseau F, Rouillard P, Morel M-L, et al.
Hagerman RJ. Molecular and clinical correlations language dysfunction in the fragile X syn-
Prevalence of carriers of premutation-sized
in fragile X syndrome. Ment Retard Dev drome. In Hagerman RJ, McBogg PM (eds):
alleles of the FMR1 gene and implications for
Disabil 1995;1:276–280. The fragile X syndrome: diagnosis, biochem-
the population genetics of the fragile X
Hagerman RJ. Physical and behavioral phenotype. istry, and intervention. Dillon, CO: Spectra,
syndrome. Am J Hum Genet 1995;57:1006–
In: Hagerman RJ, Cronister A, eds. Fragile X 1983:175–200.
1018.
syndrome: diagnosis, treatment, and research. Neimi J, Vilkman E, Ikonen U. Fragile X speech in
2nd edition. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Finnish: Phonological observation. In: Niemi Rousseau F, Morel M-L, Rouillard P, et al.
University Press, 1996a:3–87. J, Koiveselka-Sallienene, eds. Neurolinguistic Surprisingly low prevalence of FMR1 premu-
papers: proceedings of the 2nd Finnish Confer- tation among males from the general popula-
Hagerman RJ. Medical follow-up and psychophar-
ence of Neurolinguistics, 1985. tion. Am J Hum Genet 1996;59(suppl A188):
macology. In: Hagerman RJ, Cronister A,
1069.
eds. Fragile X syndrome: diagnosis, treatment, Oetter P, Richter E, Frick S. M.O.R.E.—
and research. 2nd edition. Baltimore, MD: Integrating the mouth with sensory and Sacks H, Schegloff E, Jefferson G. A simple
Johns Hopkins University Press, 1996b:283– postural functions, 2nd edition. Hugo, MN: systematics for the organization of turn-taking
331. PDP Press, 1995. in conversation. Language 1974;50:696–735.
Hagerman RJ, Altshul-Stark D, McBogg P. Recur- Oostra BA. FMR1 protein studies and animal Scarborough HS, Rescorla L, Tager-Flusberg H, et
rent otitis media in boys with the fragile X model for fragile X syndrome. In: Hagerman al. The relation of utterance length to
syndrome. Am J Dis Child 1987;141:184– RJ, Cronister AC, eds. Fragile X syndrome: grammatical complexity in normal and lan-
187. diagnosis, treatment, and research. 2nd edi- guage-disordered groups. APS 1991;12:
Hagerman RJ, Murphy MA, Wittenberger M. A tion. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Univer- 23–45.
controlled trial of stimulant medication in sity Press, 1996:193–209. Schapiro MB, Murphy DGM, Hagerman RJ, et al.
children with fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Palmer KK, Gordon JS, Coston GN, et al. Fragile X Adult fragile X syndrome: Neuropsychology,
Genet 1988;30:377–392. syndrome: Its relation to speech and language brain anatomy and metabolism. Am J Med
Hagerman RJ, Jackson C, Amiri K, et al. Fragile X disorders. Proceedings, Greenwood Genetic Genet 1995;60:480–493.
girls: Physical and neurocognitive status and Center 1988;7:93–97. Scharfenaker S, O’Connor R, Stackhouse T, et al.
outcome. Pediatrics 1992;89:395–400. Partington MW. The fragile X syndrome. II. An integrated approach to intervention. In
Hagerman RJ, Fulton MJ, Leaman A, et al. Preliminary data on growth and development Hagerman RJ, Cronister A (eds): Fragile X
Fluoxetine therapy in fragile X syndrome. in males. Am J Med Genet 1984;17:175–194. syndrome: diagnosis, treatment, and research,
Dev Brain Dysfunt 1994a;7:155–164. Paul R, Cohen DJ, Breg WR, et al. Fragile X 2nd edition. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
Hagerman RJ, Hull CE, Safanda JF, et al. High syndrome: Its relation to speech and language University Press, 1996a:349–411.
functioning fragile X males: Demonstration of disorders. J Speech Hear Disord 1984;49:328– Scharfenaker S, O’Connor R, Stackhouse T, et al.
an unmethylated, fully expanded FMR-1 332. In: Hagerman RJ, Cronister A, eds. Fragile X
mutation associated with protein expression. Paul R, Dykens E, Watson M, et al. A comparison syndrome: diagnosis, treatment, and research,
Am J Med Genet 1994b;51:298–308. of language characteristics of mentally re- 2nd edition. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
Hagerman RJ, Riddle J, Roberts LS, et al. A survey tarded adults with fragile X syndrome and University Press, 1996b:453–462.
of the efficacy of clonidine in fragile X those with non-specific mental retardation. J Scharfenaker S, O’Connor R, Stackhouse T, et al.
syndrome. Dev Brain Dys 1995;8:336–344. Autism Dev Disord 1987;17:457–468. Appendix 2: Computer Technology Re-
Hanson DM, Jackson A, Hagerman R. Speech Pieretti M, Zhang F, Fu Y-H, et al. Absence of source Centers. In: Hagerman RJ, Cronister
disturbances (cluttering) in mildly impaired expression of the FMR-1 gene in fragile X A, eds. Fragile X syndrome: diagnosis, treat-
males with Martin-Bell/Fragile X syndrome. syndrome. Cell 1991;66:817–822. ment, and research, 2nd edition. Baltimore,
Am J Med Genet 1986;23:195–206. Rattazzi MC, Ioannou YA. Molecular approaches MD: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1996c:
Imbert G, Mandel J. The fragile X mutation. Ment to therapy. In Hagerman RJ, Cronister A 463–466.
Retard Dev Disabil 1995;1:251–262. (eds): Fragile X syndrome: diagnosis, treat- Schopmeyer BB. Speech and language characteris-
Kent RD. Speech intelligibility and communicative ment, and research. 2nd edition. Baltimore, tics in fragile X syndrome. In: Schopmeyer
competence in children. In: Kaiser AP, Gray MD: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1996: BB, Lowe F, eds. The fragile X child. San
DB, eds. Enhancing children’s communica- 412–452. Diego: Singular Publishing, 1992:71–89.

MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS • FRAGILE X SYNDROME • ABBEDUTO & HAGERMAN 321


Sobesky WE, Taylor AK, Pennington BF, et al. of the emergence of deviant language. Am J Vilkman E, Niemi J, Ikonen U. Fragile X speech
Molecular/clinical correlations in females with Ment Retard 1990;94:431–441. phonology in Finnish. Brain Lang 1988;34:
fragile X. Am J Med Genet 1996;64:340–345. Sudhalter V, Scarborough H, Cohen IL. Syntactic 203–222.
Sparrow SS, Balla D, Cichetti D. Vineland Adaptive delay and pragmatic deviance in the language Webb TP, Bundey S, Thake A, et al. The frequency
Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American of fragile X males. Am J Med Genet of the fragile X chromosome among school
Guidance Service, 1984. 1991;38:493–497. children in Coventry. J Med Genet 1986;23:
Sudhalter V. The language system of males with 396–399.
Sudhalter V, Maranion M, Brooks P. Expressive
fragile X syndrome. In Hagerman RJ, McKen- Weigers AM, Curfs LM, Vermeer EH, et al.
semantic deficit in the productive language of
zie P (eds): 1992 international fragile X Adaptive behaviors in the fragile X syndrome:
males with fragile X syndrome. Am J Med
conference proceedings. Dillon, CO: Spectra Profile and development. Am J Med Genet
Genet 1992;43:65–71.
Publishing, 1992:165–166. 1993;47:216–220.
Sudhalter V. The emergence of tangential language Turner G, Webb T, Wake S, Robinson H. Wolf-Schein EG, Sudhalter V, Cohen IL, et al.
in fragile X. Paper presented at the 29th annual Prevalence of the fragile X syndrome. Am J Speech-language and the fragile X syndrome:
Gatlinburg Conference on Mental Retarda- Med Genet 1996;64:196–197. Initial findings. J Am Speech Hear Assoc
tion and Developmental Disabilities, Gatlin- Verkerk AJ, Pierettl M, Sutcliffe JS, et al. Identifica- 1987;29:35–38.
burg, TN, March 1996. tion of a gene (FMR-1) containing a CGG Wright-Talamante C, Cheema A, Riddle JE, et al.
Sudhalter V, Cohen IL, Silverman W, et al. repeat coincident with a breakpoint cluster A controlled study of longitudinal IQ changes
Conversational analyses of males with fragile region exhibiting length variation in fragile X in females and males with fragile X syndrome.
X, Down syndrome, and autism: Comparison syndrome. Cell 1991;65:905–914. Am J Med Genet 1996;64:350–355.

322 MRDD RESEARCH REVIEWS • FRAGILE X SYNDROME • ABBEDUTO & HAGERMAN

Potrebbero piacerti anche