Sei sulla pagina 1di 18

Introspective research: verbal protocols

Summary
I n this chapter, you have looked at the odd history o f introspective reporting
from various disciplinary inquiries into the operations o f the human mind.
You have seen that i n the twentieth century introspective studies moved
from enthusiastic endorsement to abandonment to enthusiastic resurrection.
This rise/fall/rise history may serve to remind us that choice o f research
paradigms is a matter o f fashion as much as o f objective analysis. Certainly
4 CLASSROOM RESEARCH:
I N T E R A C T I O N ANALYSIS

introspective studies in language-related areas are at the time o f w r i t i n g on


the rising tide of acceptance and enthusiasm.

You have also considered how current studies have tried to respond to some
of the earlier criticisms o f introspective studies, particularly i n regard to
issues o f validity and reliability. A variety o f cautions have been issued, and Introducing classroom research
research procedures aimed at m i n i m i z i n g slack i n such studies have been
Zounds!I never was so bethumpd with words,
prescribed.
Since first I called my brothers father dad.
In sum, i t is necessary to design studies based on a model of mental processes, W I L L I A M SHAKESPEARE, I 5 6 4 - 1 6 1 6
and to incorporate proper warm-up activities, careful task instructions, and on his early elementary school Language Arts experience.
appropriate monitoring of participants' task performances in order to optimize Re-used i n KingJohn Act 2 , scene 1, line 466
opportunities for valid and reliable findings.
Gladly wolde he learn and gladly teche.
GEOFFREY CHAUCER, C I 3 4 3 - I 4 O O
Canterbury Tales, Prologue, line 3 0 8

One of the criticisms frequently leveled by educationalists at second language


acquisition research, and educational research generally, is that research and
researchers are too far removed from teachers' and learners' immediate
concerns. One response to this criticism has been to try to focus research
more directly on issues clearly important to teachers and learners. I n
particular, the ways i n which teachers and learners interact in the classroom
have become a major concern i n the attempt to make educational research
more accessible and practical.

This chapter, then, is about teachers and learners and how they interact i n
the broad set of contexts we have labeled classrooms. The cover term C L A S S R O O M
we take to cover a wide range of learning contexts where learners and teachers
meet i n the context o f second language acquisition—classes i n schools,
multi-media labs, distance learning situations, one-to-one tutoring, on the
job training, computer-based instruction, and so on. Like classroom', the
term I N T E R A C T I O N is used w i t h a variety of different senses; it has been used
w i t h respect to

1 theories of linguistic description,


2 models o f second language acquisition,
3 instructional exchanges between teachers and learners,
Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis

4 task completion conversations between learners and learners, as well as and videos made for research purposes taken i n language classes are hard to
5 the internal conversations between authors and readers. come by and often of poor technical quality.

The study o f classrooms and o f interaction have independent and rich A fourth approach is to have each of you generate some classroom interaction
research histories. data i n the roles of teachers and learners. While this also has its disadvantages
(most o f you already know the language o f this class pretty thoroughly), it
We will be considering the intersection of these traditions—second language seems the best option. I t stays w i t h the plan o f the book to have you, the
acquisition studies involving research into interactions w i t h i n a classroom readers, be the sources o f the data as well as analysts of the data. I f you are i n
environment. We will be looking i n particular at two issues that have been o f a class, everyone can have access to the same data, as necessary. A n d we can
long-term interest to classroom interaction researchers—the nature o f focus on particular aspects o f classroom interaction that we want to highlight
teachers' correction o f learner errors and learner-to-learner communication by careful selection o f interaction tasks.
in task-based group work.
This fourth option, then, is the one we have chosen to take in this chapter.
You may want to pursue one of the other options outlined above i f the local
situation encourages this, or i f you are working alone.
Experiencing classroom research
Although we have defined classrooms and interaction quite broadly, most
classroom interaction research studies are done i n regular school classrooms Teacher interactions with learners
and focus on interactions between teachers and learners or between learners W h a t teachers do i n classrooms has been the major focus o f classroom
and learners. interaction studies. There are over 200 classroom observation instruments
Given our experiential approach to the study of second language acquisition that have been developed, and most o f these focus on how teachers interact
research, our first choice w o u l d be to ask you to go out and gather some w i t h learners. Over twenty observation instruments have been developed
detailed data from language teaching classes. Unfortunately, we cannot just for studying classroom interaction i n second language classes. Again,
require you to go and collect data from live second language classes for a most o f these focus on the teacher (Long and Sato 1983). Some principal
variety o f obvious reasons: (a) i t may be a time when no classes are i n session, topics of interest have been:
(b) you may have difficulty gaining access to classrooms, (c) you may not fit 1 Teacher questions
inconspicuously into a language class, (d) i t may be difficult to make high- 2 Teacher error corrections
quality recordings o f class interactions, (e) the data might be such that you 3 Quantity o f teacher speech
can't analyze them usefully because the class happens to be watching a film, 4 Teacher explanations
and so on. 5 Teacher wait-time' for student responses
A second approach might be to use the daily doings of your own class—the You are going to collect some data about one of these topics for later analysis.
class you are i n using this book-—as the source o f interaction data. Again, I t is always best to tape record any k i n d o f verbal data you are collecting;
there are some problems w i t h this: (a) the class is not a language teaching usually you can't write as fast as people can speak. Also, a taped record allows
class, (b) it is hard to participate i n a class fully and simultaneously do all the you to check your notes and add new comments such as vocal tone, pauses,
jobs a classroom researcher has to do—one role or the other w i l l suffer, and loudness, etc. You may also find i t useful to use video recordings so that non-
(c) the teacher of this class may not be keen to be a research object right now. verbal signals—gestures, facial expressions, body postures, participant spacings,
A third approach would be to get a film or video featuring a lot o f school etc.—can be available for analysis. I f you have a partner to transcribe the data,
classroom footage and use this as the data source. There are problems w i t h some o f these exercises can be done without an audio or video recorder, but
this choice as well: (a) while there are many films featuring classrooms as they are best done w i t h them i f at all possible.
settings—Blackboard Jungle, Up the Down Staircase, The Breakfast Club,
Lord of the Flies, The Dead Poets Society, Stand By Me, for example—these are
movie classrooms and not much like classrooms i n real life; (b) there are not
many feature films focusing on language teaching and learning; and (c) films
Classroom research: interaction analysis
Classroom research: interaction analysis

Teacher error correction Table 41 Error-type profile comparing two teachers-Teacher A-l andTeacher B-2
(For a characterization o f the teachers i n the study referred to here, see
A l l learners make errors i n learning a new language. Some commentators Appendix 4.1 on page 271.)
even feel that errors are necessary stepping-stones to acquiring a second
language. But most learners and most teachers feel that i t is part o f the Errors No. A - l No. B-2 % of total (A-l) % of total (B-2)
teacher's responsibility to let learners know i f they have made an error and to
assist them i n not making a similar error again. Learners and teachers have Phonological 13 50 28.8 37
preferences for what kinds of error correction they feel most useful and least Lexical 16 17 35.6 13
Mo rph/Syn tactic 4 10 8.9
intrusive i n classroom interaction. 6
Discourse 5 11 11.1 8
Dialect 3 28 6.7 21
Content 4 20 8.9
Exercise 4.1 15
Total 45 136 100.0 100
Think about the following error from a beginning oral English class: He not go
home. Now answer the following questions: (Adapted from Nystrom 1983:174, 178)
1 How many different ways can you think of by which a language teacher
might respond to this spoken error? Put a different way, how many oral Nystrom provided further information on her four bilingual teachers' error
error correction types can you think of? Write these down. correction styles. I n Table 4.2, she compares teacher response profiles for her
four teachers and identifies nine different types o f teacher error correction
2 What would be your preference of these types of error correction?
(models correct form, repeats faulty form, etc.).
Rank four of them in terms of your preference.

Table 4.2 Teacher response profiles in percentages


(For a characterization o f the teachers in the study referred to here, see
Nystrom (1983) looked at speech error correction behaviors of four teachers Appendix 4.1 on page 271.)
i n bilingual primary school classrooms i n the U.S. The kinds o f error noted
were classified as: P H O N O L O G I C A L , Lexical, M O R P H O L O G I C A L , S Y N T A C T I C , Teacher corrections to errors A-l A-2 B-l B-2
D I S C O U R S E , Dialect, and Content. Profiles o f the kinds o f errors noted by
Models correct form 10 20
two of the coded teachers, A - l and B-2, are shown i n Table 4 . 1 . 17
Drills correct form 6 10 10
Repeats faulty form 10 16 12
Prompts correct form 13 13
Exercise 4.2 13
Explains correct form 3 4 7
Create an error that you think typical of each of the types of error listed above. (Re)states question/prompt 3 12 8
Don't worry if the linguistic terms are not altogether familiar to you. Create what Tells student what to say 14 10 _ 5
Reduces directions 2 _
you think are likely interpretations of the terms and create error examples to
Expands directions
match. Keep a copy of these error examples for later analysis. 3 4 - 4

Note: This table is abbreviated and omits teacher responses to correct


student productions. Thus, the columns do not add up to 100%.

(Adapted from Nystrom 1983: 175)


Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis

groups, and greater comprehension o f the content and skills they are
Exercise 4.3 studying. (Stahl 1995: 1)
Create an error and an error correction for each of the nine types. Pick errors I n order to get a sense o f how learner-to-learner interaction is studied i n
with 3rd person present tense verbs as your learner error type (for example, *L/m language learning classes, you will next take part i n a cooperative classroom
like to play hockey, or *K/m want to hits him). For each error type, write down the learning activity. You will observe two learners communicating i n a cooperative
error example and the example of a possible teacher correction. Take the roles of picture-sequencing task, and you w i l l code the communications using a
both error maker and error corrector. If working with a partner, take turns in simple coding system. The codes w i t h examples are shown i n Table 4.3.
these roles. Stay with this one type of student error (3rd person singular present Notice that there are nine codes—three codes that could be considered
tense verbs) but use different vocabulary for each example. Keep your list of error positive codes, a parallel group of three negative codes, and five neutral codes
and error correction examples for later data analysis. Be sure to note the error as follows:
correction types you did not include in your list in the previous exercise.
The P O S I T I V E C O D E S are
proposal without reasoning,
proposal w i t h reasoning, and
Learner-to-learner interactions support.
In the previous section you have experienced something similar to classroom T h e N E G A T I V E C O D E S are
interaction research as i t bears on teachers' interactions w i t h learners. T h e counter-proposal without reasoning,
other major type of classroom interaction is between learners and learners. I n counter-proposal w i t h reasoning, and
communicative methodology, increasing emphasis is placed on language non-support.
learning tasks which involve pair work and group work. Given this perspective,
it is obviously critical to know what goes on i n these convocations of learners The N E U T R A L C O D E S are

and how, i f at all, learner-to-learner interactions contribute to language describes a picture,


acquisition. filler,,
doubt,
Since the following exercise involves two learners i n an interaction situation, neutral, and
it would be helpful i f those of you working alone could get a family member operational.
or friend to do the picture sequencing task w i t h you. I f you can't find anyone
you can convince to play this game w i t h you, you can find data for learners One o f the classic activity types found i n second language instructional
doing this task in Appendix 4.2 on pages 272-3. materials is the P I C T U R E - S E Q U E N C I N G task. I n picture sequencing a set o f
four or more pictures following some plot line is presented i n random order.
I n one area o f pedagogy, interaction patterns and learning outcomes have Either following teacher dictation or group discussion, learners organize the
been studied extensively over the last 40 years. This area is labeled broadly pictures i n what they feel to be their correct or logical order. We have
cooperative learning. provided four picture sequences for use i n this activity i n Appendix 4.3 on
Cooperative learning is group-learning activity organized so that learning pages 2 7 4 - 7 . T h e pictures i n each set are labeled alphabetically for
is dependent on the socially structured exchange of information between identification. The alphabetical labeling has nothing to do w i t h the final
learners i n groups and i n which each learner is held accountable for his sequence.
or her own learning and is motivated to increase the learning of others.
(Olsen and Kagan 1992:8)

Research studies of cooperative learning i n very diverse school settings


and across a wide range o f content areas, have revealed that students
completing cooperative learning group tasks tend to have higher
academic test scores, higher self-esteem, greater numbers o f positive
social skills, fewer stereotypes o f individuals o f other races or ethnic
Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis

Table 4.3 Interaction codes for cooperative picture ordering task 3 The picture sequencers will be designated as A and B. The observers will
each observe one picture sequencer, A or B.
Picture Sequencer Moves Code Example 4 The randomly organized picture problem sets are shown in
Describes a Picture D There's a clock on the wall that says Appendix 4.3. The task is for picture sequencers to lay out the pictures
'8:00' and then to organize them, through discussion, into what strikes them as
the most natural sequence of pictures.
Proposal wlo Reasoning P- I think picture C should go first.
5 Using the codes in Table 4.3, observer for A will code and take
Proposal ivl Reasoning P+ I think picture C should go first shorthand notes of what A says; observer for B will code and take
because the sun is coming up shorthand notes of what B says. We recommend a practice round and a
Support S Looks right. I agree. What's next? for-the-record round using codes in Table 4.3.

N Nope. Unh, unh. Don't think so. 6 In the next section, the observers will pool their codes and shorthand
Non-support
Some other. notes into a mini-transcription sketching the task interactions of A and B,
so keep your records.
Counter-proposal wlo Reasoning CP- No, I think B is first.
7 The group of four should discuss the coding and the transcriptions.
Counter-proposal wl Reasoning CP+ But everybody's happy in B. I think
8 What patterns emerged in the interaction? Were there any utterances
it should go first.
that could not be coded?
Filler, Doubt, Neutral F Mmmm. Maybe. Let's see 9 Reverse roles of picture sequencers and observers so that the former
Operational O What do we have to do? Shall I observers now become A and B picture sequencers. (If there are more
start? Are we done? than four in the group, it is better to have extra observers than extra
picture sequencers.)
Photocopiable © Oxford University Press
10 In the second round, picture sequencers are more likely to be self-
conscious about the way their utterances will be coded. Did this happen?
Did the patterns of the first interaction and the second interaction differ
Exercise 4.4 in any way?
It is best to work with an enlarged A4 or letter size photocopy of the picture set
(Appendix 4.3 on pages 274-7) cut into individual pictures so they can be moved
around by the cooperating pair. (As always, a tape recorder is very helpful for this I n this section you have experienced two kinds o f classroom interaction. You
activity. However, if you have a secretary/recorder, the activity is designed to be have created examples of different kinds of teacher error correction feedback
feasible without a tape recorder.) Keep the coding sheets and any discussion notes and have worked to analyze data from a two-person cooperative picture-
for compiling and analysis. sequencing task. We w i l l next t u r n to looking at how data from such
observations can be compiled and analyzed.
Now, do this exercise in the following steps:
1 Form a pair with another student and then get together with another
pair so that there are four of you. (If the class size does not divide up into
exact groups of four, the residual group should be more rather than Compiling classroom research data
fewer than four.) We noted in both this chapter and in our previous discussion o f introspective/
2 One pair will play the role of picture sequencers and the other pair, retrospective studies that data are typically recorded on a tape (or video)
classroom observers. (If you are working with a single partner in this recorder. The classroom interaction researcher w i l l also have notes and/or
exercise, it will be necessary to tape record your interactions as have done partial transcriptions during data collection. Compiling the data
participants doing the task and analyze the taped interactions as for analysis w i l l involve transcribing data from the tape recordings and
observers.) interleaving notes and transcriptions made at the time of data collection.
Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis

We talked i n the previous chapter about the critical task o f segmenting the Discuss any differences and resolve them, by writing some new pairs, if
transcription so that each segment represents a u n i t o f an apparent short necessary.
thought or utterance. These segmented units become the core elements that 5 Now combine the piles of the two groups so that you have nine piles
you will code and analyze. I n one sense, segmentation o f classroom interaction with two examples in each pile.
data is easier than segmenting introspection data. The major segments i n
interaction data usually comprise each speaker's utterances. The beginning
and end of segments are often signaled by change o f speaker.
This is a good opportunity to combine all the piles from all class members.
You w o u l d then have a comprehensive set o f error correction examples as
Teacher error-correction data produced and sorted by language teaching professionals. W h i l e these are,
perhaps, not quite the same as examples taken from actual classroom interaction,
The word compile' comes from the Latin meaning 'Come, let us pile together' they are, in themselves, interesting data. They can, of course, now be used to
(well, almost). Certainly, one o f the tasks o f compiling is to get the data compare w i t h error correction examples taken directly from language
collected into piles of some sort for later analysis. For example, i f you want to classes. Combine the piles of slips for each o f the nine error types so that the
look at characteristics o f certain kinds o f teacher error correction, you w i l l whole class can review all the error correction example types. You may want
need to have all the error corrections o f the same k i n d piled together. to glue or tape them to a single sheet o f paper and post these on the wall o f
However, what seems obvious about sorting items into a pile for one the class or produce photocopies for each member o f the class. I n this way,
researcher may not be so obvious to another researcher. the whole class can review all the error correction example types.

Exercise 4.5 Learner-to-learner interaction data


In the previous section, you generated some learner errors and some examples of
I n the 'Experiencing classroom research' section o f this chapter, y o u
teacher correction types in response to those errors (Exercise 4.3). This is a good
examined the interaction i n a two-person, learner-to-learner cooperative
opportunity to check if the labels you gave your error correction examples would picture-sequencing task. You now need to compile your data from this
be the same labels as someone else would give to these examples. The following exercise.
directions suggest how you may do this.

If you are working on your own, you will find samples of various kinds of teacher
error corrections in Appendix 4.2 on pages 272-3. Your task is to classify your Exercise 4.6
own earlier examples and the examples in Appendix 4.2 as to the type of teacher Draw or photocopy a data table like that shown in Table 4.4. (Sample entries for A
error correction you think each sample represents. and B are shown below.) If you feel there are other details you want to record,
1 Retrieve your teacher error correction examples from Exercise 4.3. construct your own data table. Then follow these steps:
Since there were nine error correction types, there should be nine 1 If you have tape recordings, you should transcribe these into the table
examples. Prepare nine slips of paper, each with a single example on it. now.
Make sure that the error correction type label is not included on the slip.
2 Get your coding notes and any notes made at the time of the
2 If you are working in a class, form pairs and then join with another pair observations.
so that you have a group of four. Give the other pair your nine slips in
3 Note any gaps in your notes and try to fill these.
random order.
4 Note any exchanges that were difficult or impossible to code.
3 Now with your partner, label the other pair's nine examples according
to your judgement as to the error correction type. Write your error 5 Write down any particular problems in the sequencing—first picture,
correction type label on the back of each slip. last picture, sequencing of similar pictures?

4 Rejoin with the other pair and exchange your labels. Did you give the
examples the same labels as the pair that created the examples did?
Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis

Table 4.4 Picture sequencing observation data table (www) 1 What kinds o f learner errors are teachers most likely to correct?
2 What are some o f the different ways i n which teachers correct learners'
A/B Said Code Pic no. Comments errors in second language production?
3 D o teachers and learners have similar or different preferences i n respect to
A This looks like the start. P- D Look at pics, don't say
error correction feedback?
anything for a minute.
4 W h a t are some o f the patterns o f interaction i n learner-to-learner
B Mmmm F D B looking at other pics communication i n carrying out cooperative classroom tasks?
5 H o w do different interaction patterns facilitate or hinder learner-to-
learner communication and the language learning o f individual learners?

Teacher error-correction data


Sinclair and Coulthard (1975) claim that the most frequent kind of classroom
interaction is conducted between teachers (T) and students (S) in the form
of IRF exchanges, where I = Initiating move; R = Responding move; F = Follow-
up move. For example:

I/T: What comes next?


Photocopiable © Oxford University Press
R/S: Boy going.
F/T: D i d you say that the boy is going somewhere?

As the IRF example suggests, I is frequently a question, R is a student response


Analyzing classroom interaction data to the question and F is often some form o f teacher identification or
correction o f student response errors.
What are we looking for in the data from classroom interaction observation?
Some early studies came up w i t h the somewhat startling data that teacher In the previous section, we looked at a number o f different types o f learner
talk took up 89% of classroom verbal interaction time, and that, 9 1 % of the errors and a number of different styles o f teacher error correction. You found
time, teachers already knew the answers to the questions they were asking or made up examples of learner errors for the following seven error categories:
(Nunan 1989: 26). These data were surprising but were they accurate, and
1 phonological 5 discourse
did they indicate something good or bad? T h e initial thought o f most
2 lexical 6 dialect
teachers was, perhaps, ' M Y classes aren't like that—at least, I don't t h i n k they
3 morphological 7 content
are'. So some major questions o f later research studies were:
4 syntactic
1 H o w is class talk time divided between teachers and learners?
You also composed some examples o f nine common types of teacher error
2 What are some o f the different language patterns used i n teacher-learner
correction. The types of teacher error correction you examined were:
interaction?
3 H o w self-aware are teachers of how they use language i n their interactions 1 models correct form 6 (re)states teaching prompt
with their students? 2 drills correct form 7 tells student what to say
4 W h a t kinds o f learning differences do different patterns o f teacher talk 3 repeats faulty form 8 reduces instructions
4 prompts correct form 9 expands directions
make?
5 explains correct form
5 Can and w i l l teachers re-create their teaching practices i f it appears that
better practices are available?
I n this chapter, we are looking specifically to find answers to questions such
as the following:
Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis

etc.). Some errors may be ambiguous or of more than one type. Discuss
Exercise 4.7 why you gave the learner errors the labels you did.
Review all the error correction example types and their classification labels from 3 Consider the teacher's error corrections. How many of the errors did
the previous section (Exercise 4.5). she correct? What percentage of all errors was this? Identify each of the
1 As a teacher, what would your preference be for these different types of teacher's error corrections and label them using the error correction
error correction? Rank order the nine error type from your most categories listed above.
favored (I) to least favored (9). 4 Outline your conclusions based on this brief set of data.
2 Consider the same error correction types from the your own 5 Read and discuss the 'Author's Notes on Transcription' in the second
perspective as a language learner. Rank the correction types from most section of Appendix 4.2 on page 273 (Allwright 1975).
favored (I) to least favored (9).
3 How similar are your two rankings, as teacher and as learner?

If you are using this text in a class group, each member of the class should now Learner-to-learner interaction data
vote for which of the nine types of error correction strategies they, as teachers, In the last section, you developed a data table for picture sequencing observation
personally prefer, using the same ranking system as above. The votes should then using a formatted coding system. Take a look back at that table now.
be tallied and added together to give a class ranking of favored error correction
types. (In this election, the lowest number wins.) Your database of teacher error
correction preferences is in this way enlarged. Exercise 4.9
Take notes as you answer the following questions:
1 What patterns are evident in the table? Who initiates most moves? Who
It would also be interesting to see how much variation i n individual class talks most? Is there any evidence that one or both picture sequencers
member rankings there is. (We will be returning i n Chapter 6 to a consideration felt they did not get their fair say?
of how such ranking data can be handled more formally.)
2 Rate how interactive or individual this activity was. Did partners work
We are now going to look at a longer piece o f teacher-learner interaction together or as two people each acting as if they were doing the task
(Appendix 4.2 on pages 2 7 2 - 3 ) . I n this interaction, there are several alone? What evidence would you offer for the interacting or soloing
different kinds o f teacher IRF sets. These are usually teacher questions —• analysis?
learner errors —* teacher error corrections. 3 Did cooperative interaction seem a good classroom activity for the pairs
observed? Why or why not?
4 Any early suggestions as to how to revise the task to make it more
Exercise 4.8
balanced? More effective?
Singly or in pairs follow these steps:
1 Consider the teacher questions. (The transcript has at least six clear
questions.) Decide if the teacher knew or did not know the answer to This is really the first look we have had at a formatted observation instrument
each question before she asked it. If you think the teacher knew the constructed for the purpose of recording and analyzing some kinds of classroom
answer to the question, label the question as D (standing for display interaction. One o f the major sources o f data has come from classroom
question). If you think the teacher did not know the answer to the observations using a variety o f such observation instruments (also called
question, label the question R (standing for referential question). (This schedules). W i t h over 200 such observation instruments reported i n the
distinction as applied to teacher questions in second language classes is literature, Tt seems that every researcher who wants to investigate classroom
discussed in detail in Longand Sato 1983). Discuss why you gave the interaction has felt bound to devise his/her own scheme' (Nunan 1989: 83).
questions the label you did.
2 Consider the learner errors. Identify each of these and label it as to type
using the error categories listed above (phonological, morphological,
Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis

Chaudron (1988) characterizes 26 o f the instruments designed just for the Most teachers and non-teachers alike have views about what constitutes
analysis o f interaction i n second language classrooms. For each o f these good teaching. Often these views are widely shared and held to be axiomatic.
instruments, Chaudron indicates the following: Many of these axioms are built into teacher preparation programs and might
be phrased as follows:
1 the recording procedure: is there a specific behavior noted on each
appearance (a CATEGORY S Y S T E M ) , or are all behaviors w h i c h occur Axiom 1 Learners learn more effectively when given positive feedback
w i t h i n a specified time period noted (a S I G N SYSTEM)? (praise, approval) than negative feedback (criticism, disapproval).
2 the degree of researcher intuition required: is i t high or low ? Axiom 2 Learners learn from hearing, reading, and interacting w i t h well-
3 the number o f categories i n the system: i n the instruments reviewed i t formed examples of how the L2 is used to communicate. Giving ungrammatical
varied between 7 and 73; or negative examples is not helpful in second language acquisition.
4 multiple coding: can behavior be considered i n more than one category? Axiom 3 Giving learners time to think after a teacher's question (encouraging
5 real-time coding: is coding done live d u r i n g class observation or later a longer teacher-wait time') is a good thing and w i l l produce longer and
using an audio or video recording of the class? more thoughtful learner responses.
6 the unit o f analysis: is the unit an arbitrary time interval or a specific Axiom 4 Real questions to learners w i l l produce more interaction than
analytical unit such as a move, a cycle, an episode, etc.? phony questions to which the teacher already knows the answer.
7 the focus o f observation: is i t verbal, paralinguistic, nonlinguistic, Axiom 5 The more language interaction involving learner participation that
cognitive, affective, pedagogical, content-related, discourse-related? goes on i n class, the better the opportunities for language learning.

Exercise 4.10 Exercise 4. II

Answer the following questions: Consider the five axioms presented above and answer the following questions:
1 If Chaudron were to characterize the coding system you used in coding Do you agree or disagree with these? How strongly?
the picture-sequencing task, how would he characterize this code with
respect to the seven dimensions he proposes? Assign to each axiom a rating using the following scale:
0 = Strongly disagree
2 If you were to look for a better coding system for this kind of
1 = Disagree
observation of learner-learner interaction, what would you look for?
2 = No opinion
3 = Agree
4 = Strongly Agree
1 Compare your ratings with other students if possible. Which items do
Designing your own classroom research most people concur on?
I n this chapter we have so far looked at collecting, compiling, and analyzing 2 On which items do people not concur?
some examples of classroom interaction data. We w i l l now consider some o f
the issues involved in designing such a study.

I n t h i n k i n g about your own research design, you need to simultaneously To many these axioms seem reasonable, and classroom researchers have
consider your personal beliefs or tenets, your research topic 2iA the research looked for classroom evidence that bear them out. Researchers positively
technique by which you might gather data on this topic. inclined towards these principles often find evidence i n their data to support
them, while other researchers, perhaps a bit more skeptical, often suggest
that the evidence is less convincing.
Clarifying your beliefs, picking your tenets The literature is full o f both positive research evidence to support these
Where are you coming from? W h a t are your beliefs about what good axioms and negative research evidence suggesting that there is no reported
teaching is? W h a t are your beliefs about what effective learning looks like? effect on learner achievement. As evidence for the evidence, so to speak, here
is a list of some of the research reports for and against:
Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis 97

For axiom 1 Picking a topic


positive evidence was found by Rosenshine and Furst (1973), and
There is a host o f potential topics on which to conduct your own classroom
negative evidence is cited by Long (1983).
interaction research. We have mentioned a variety of teacher-focused topics:
For axiom 2 teacher questions, teacher error corrections, quantity o f teacher speech,
positive evidence is offered by Schwartz (1986), and teacher explanations, teacher wait-time' for learner responses, etc. A similar
negative evidence is cited by W h i t e (1991). range o f topics on learner-focused themes could be considered.

For axiom 3 While we know o f no checklist o f potential classroom interaction research


positive evidence is given by Holley and King (1971), and topics, there have been several attempts to sketch out the factors affecting
negative evidence is cited by Long (1984). how teachers teach and how learners learn. You might find looking at one o f
these w i l l stimulate your interest i n looking more closely at how one of these
For axiom 4
factors is realized i n the classroom. Shavelson and Stern (1981), for example,
positive evidence is given by Brock (1986), and
sketch the broad range of issues comprising the factors which impact on how
negative evidence is cited by Long (1984).
teachers teach. Look at the adaptation o f their view of this domain shown i n
For axiom 5 Figure 4 . 1 .
positive evidence is reported by Seliger (1977), and
negative evidence is reported by Day (1984).
Exercise 4.12
This is n o t to say that these axioms are bad, or incorrect, or useless as
teaching principles. Nevertheless, i t is true that such principles are often Examine the array of factors in Figure 4.1 which are claimed to influence how
hard to confirm consistently i n studies o f what actually takes place i n the teachers teach.
classroom. As teachers know, there are complex interactions going on 1 As you consider undertaking a classroom interaction research study,
simultaneously i n any classroom; neat answers are hard to find. Recently choose six of the factors that seem to be of most interest to you.
there has been growing reluctance among practitioners to prescribe and
2 Which of the factors do you find confusing?
proscribe teaching practices. Good practice is held to be an individual matter
directed by the specific content to be taught, by the beliefs, experience, and If possible, discuss with a partner why you chose those factors you did. Also
preferences o f the teacher, by the situational context i n w h i c h teaching discuss any factors you found confusing to see if you can come to a better
occurs, and by the needs, interests, and personalities o f the learners. understanding of them.

Johnson quotes Kent, a teacher-in-preparation who got the message' during


his practicum experience: • 4.1 The domain of research on teachers judgements, decisions and behavior
This experience [the practicum placement] has opened m y eyes to Teacher cognitive processes
some very different ways of teaching. I used to think that there must be Teacher characteristics Information selection and integration
one right way to teach and all I had to do was figure out the right way Beliefs Attributions
and then do it. I now see that there are lots o f right ways. What makes Antecedent conditions Conceptions of subject Heuristics
it right depends on what students want and need, and that is always Information about students Matter Availability
different. (Johnson 1995: 33) Ability Cognitive complexity Representativeness
Participation Anchoring
This k i n d o f open-ended view o f teaching makes classroom interaction Behavior problems Saliency
analysis somewhat problematical. W h a t is good teaching and what kinds o f Conflict/stress
interactions are we looking for i n second language classes? I t is important for
Nature of instructional task
researchers to be clear about their own tenets and candid i n their reports Goals Inferences
about how such tenets shaped their research. Subject matter Judgements
Students Expectations
Activities Hypotheses
Decisions
98 Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis

Classroom/school environment 5 The proliferation o f observational instruments 'has resulted i n many


Groupness studies o f similar phenomena that lack true comparability' (Chaudron
Evaluative climate 1988: 180) and this has led to the existence o f many stand-alone studies
Extra-class pressures which do little to advance understanding of classroom processes.
Consequences for teachers planning 6 Observation is usually done over a period o f time i n live classes where the
Instruction personalities i n the class are unknown to the researcher. It is challenging to
Selection of content find such classes and the time to undertake such observations.
Grouping of students
7 As we pointed out in the previous section, classroom observation instruments
Selection of activities
are inevitably biased towards some particular research perspective. Given
Interaction with students
some current lack of consensus on what constitutes good teaching and the
Teaching routines
general lack of success in finding consistent relationship between classroom
Behavior problems
Tutoring behavior and learner achievement, it is hard to recommend any one
observation instrument.
Teacher evaluation Consequences for students
O f judgements A l l of this is not to say that the practice of schedule-based classroom observation
O f decisions is dead, or that useful research reports based on the use o f classroom
O f teaching routines observation instruments are not found i n the literature, or that you should
not consider undertaking a classroom interaction research study using a
(Shavelson and Stern 1981)
classroom observation instrument. (If this is the direction you wish to go in,
you w i l l find useful overviews o f observation instrument types used in
second language classroom research i n A l l w r i g h t 1988; Day 1990; and
Picking a technique Nunan 1989.)

As we have indicated, classroom interaction research is an extremely active There are, however, a variety o f alternative research techniques that have
and important area o f second language research. There is a wide range o f been and are being employed. These include:
books and journals focused in whole or i n part on the subject. D o i n g such
1 diaries 8 simulated classroom data
research requires spending a great deal of time i n classrooms recording and
2 journals 9 protocol analysis
taking notes.
3 ethnographic records 10 action research
We have seen that much classroom interaction research has been based on 4 field notes 11 stimulated recall
the use o f classroom observation instruments; you used an example of one i n 5 interviews 12 case studies
your look at learner-learner interaction i n a cooperative learning task. 6 questionnaires 13 formal experiments
Otherwise, we have not done much w i t h observation instruments i n this 7 checklists
chapter. This is so for several reasons:
I n fact, all o f the research methods discussed i n this book (and a few more)
1 There is a huge range o f such classroom observation instruments available have been employed i n the study o f what goes on i n classrooms. Let us
(200+) and exposure to one or two will not tell much about the range. consider i n slightly more detail three of these alternatives: (a) action research,
2 M o s t o f the instruments take considerable training to use, and such (b) simulated classroom data, and (c) stimulated recall.
training is not within the scope of this book.
A C T I O N R E S E A R C H is 'a form o f self-reflective enquiry undertaken by
3 Transcription and analysis of observational data is extremely time-consuming
participants i n social situations i n order to improve the rationality and justice
(15-20 hours per hour of observation) and usually complicated.
o f their practices . . . ' (Carr and Kemmis 1985: 220). I t is a less stylized, more
4 Classroom observations using these instruments represented the
situational form of classroom research and thus avoids some of the criticisms
dominant method o f research on classrooms i n the 1960s and 70 s. This
leveled at other methods. Kemmis and McTaggart (1988) outline the steps
practice declined considerably following sharp attacks on the use o f such
to be taken i n action research as follows:
observation instruments (for example, Stubbs 1975).
100 Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis 101

Phase I : Develop a plan of action to improve what is already happening.


Phase I I : Act to implement the plan.
Interpreting classroom research
Phase I I I : Observe the effects of action i n context In this chapter you have so far collected and analyzed several different kinds
Phase IV: Reflect o n these effects of classroom interaction data. Each of these lends itself to particular kinds o f
interpretation. We w i l l now first examine some general views and reservations
S I M U L A T E D C L A S S R O O M DATA We have already looked at procedures for
about the interpretation of such data. We w i l l then consider how these might
generating simulated classroom data when we asked you to generate and
shape the interpretation of data collected and analyzed thus far.
rank alternative techniques by which teachers correct learner errors. This is
not as precise a method as classroom observation for examining how teachers Michael Stubbs cites w i t h some concern the following procedures typical o f
actually correct student errors over a longish period of time, but it is considerably classroom interaction research:
less taxing, and i t does provide data that are interesting and useful for
Researchers return from the field w i t h video recordings, audio recordings,
classroom practitioners.
notebooks, or other data. From these recordings or notes, they selection
S T I M U L A T E D R E C A L L is a technique i n which the researcher records and quotation and discussion, short extracts of, say, teacher-pupil dialogue.
transcribes parts o f a lesson and then gets the teacher (and where possible, From these extracts, and probably more generally also w i t h i n the
the learners) to comment on what was happening at the time that the corpus o f the data, they further select particular features o f language
teaching and learning took place. (Nunan 1989: 94) w h i c h they regard as evidence for educational statements. (Stubbs
1981: 63. Author's italics.)

Exercise 4.13 Thus, what the reader sees is a very reduced and researcher-shaped version o f
the original data. Reductions from classroom situation to researcher
We are going to ask you to do a stimulated recall of a class period. You can use an interpretation run something like this:
upcoming class in which you are enrolled (maybe, a class in this course). If you are
not enrolled in a class you may be able to use a movie or television class. The 1 The world of classroom interactions 0
analysis will be similar. Using whatever data you can get, do the following: 2 Interactions the researcher observed 0
3 Interactions the researcher recorded as data 0
1 Pick a period in the day when you have time and energy to do this, maybe
4 Interactions the researcher selected for analysis 0
with the foreknowledge of your teacher. It is useful to have a tape
5 Extracts from interactions selected for reporting 0
recorder running, to remind you later of what went on in class. In
6 Features selected from extracts for focus 0
addition, set aside a piece of paper on which to make notes of what is
7 Features highlighted as evidence for interpretations
going on in class. Try to note distinct sections of the class and some of
the things you noticed that went on in each section. If a textbook was Interpretations, then, are very strongly influenced by the selection o f data
used, you can probably also use this as a guide to how the lesson was made at several points along the research trail. The implication is also that,
organized. having made a different selection of data, we might have been led to a different
2 At the end of the day, replay the tape, go back over your notes, any text interpretation. Some have even questioned i f it is possible for researchers to
material used, and your own memory of the recently transpired events. be altogether objective in selecting data for interpretation when they have
already invested i n a particular hypothesis i n terms o f what they expect to
3 Describe briefly what went on in each segment of the lesson and your
find. One way to check which factors may have influenced your interpretation
reaction to it. Your reactions might be to one of more of the following
is to look at how another researcher might interpret the same or similar data.
questions: Was the segment relevant? Did it relate to what went before
and came after? Was the point of the segment made clear? Who Researchers try to interpret their own research findings i n relationship to
participated in each segment and how? Was this segment of the lesson what is already known, to the research that has preceded their own. We have
interesting, memorable? already seen that the variety of different approaches to such research and the
huge array o f observational, coding, and reporting formats constitutes a
particular problem i n classroom interaction research. Indeed, the comparison
and interpretation o f a current research study w i t h other studies using
102 Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis 103

different observational or analytical models often provides a key challenge to Table 4.6 Yucels report of teacher error-correction behaviors
the researcher.
T 'What d i d you do at the weekend?'
You have already seen one system taken from Nystrom (1983) for classifying S T go to the cinema.'
techniques w h i c h teachers use i n correcting learner errors i n speaking. T ....
Nystrom's table (Table 4.5) contains no examples though you have, we trust,
Corrective behaviors observed o f teachers i n class N = 10
assembled and classified a set o f examples which you feel represent the error
correction types identified there.
Example of teacher error correction Type of correction In-class %
Another system for such classification is presented i n Chaudron (1988). instances
This system has been used by a variety o f researchers working i n this area.
Yucel (2000) used an adaptation of Chaudron's typology to compare learner
1 Don t say go; say went.
y 1 Negation - -
preferenceswith teacher practices. Yucel used Chaudrons classification system 2 / went to the cinema. 2 Repetition with 13 26
and examples to look at teacher error correction practices and learner error change
correction preferences. Tables 4.6 and 4.7 summarize Yucel's findings. 3 Yesterday, I... 3 Prompt 5 10

Table 4.5 Nystrom typology of teacher error correction strategies


4 Go is the present tense. You need
the past tense here.
4 Explanation - -
Teacher response profiles (in percentages) ( N = 4)

(For a characterization o f the teachers i n the study referred to here, see


5 What's the second word? 5 Question -• -
Appendix 4.1 on page 271.) 6 Students? (class gives answer) 6 Transfer 2 4

Teacher Corrections A-l A-2 B-l B-2


7 Mmmmm (disapproval) 7 Disapprove - -
8 Please repeat the sentence. 8 Repeat (explicit) 4 8
Models correct form 10 20 - 17

Drills correct form 6 10 - 10


9 What? 9 Repeat (implicit) - -
Repeats faulty form 10 16 - 12
10 Again. Where did you go? 10 Altered question - -
11 Really? Which film did you see? 11 Ignore 19 38
Prompts correct form 13 13 - 13
12 When you went to the cinema, 12 Provide and expand 7 14
Explains correct form 3 4 - 7 did you have a good time?
(Re)states question/prompt

Tells student what to say


3
14
12

10
-- 8

5
Yucel's observational data on 13 teachers' error-correction behaviors. (Context:
university-level English preparatory classes in Turkey.)
-
Reduces directions

Expands directions
2

3
-
4
-- 4
(Adapted from Yucel 2000: 150-1)

Nystrom's observational data o n four teachers' error-correction behaviors,


already discussed. (Context: primary school bilingual classes i n U.S.)

(Adapted from Nystrom 1983: 175)


104 Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis 105

Table 4.7 Yucel's report of learner error-correction preferences


Exercise 4.14
Error correction preferences of 22-25-year-old students N = 84
Examine the three tables and discuss the following:
Example of teacher error correction Type of correction Number of 1 Which error-correction types appear to be similar in Nystrom's and
Students
Yucel's classification systems? Which types are unique to each system?
1 Don't say go; say went. 1 Negation - Are there any that you find questionable?

2 Repetition with 13 2 Why would there be significant differences in two systems meant to
2 I went to the cinema.
change classify all teacher error-correction types?

10 3 What issues need to be considered in comparing teacher error


3 Yesterday, I... 3 Prompt
correction behavior and learner error-correction preferences from the
4 Go is the present tense. You need 4 Explanation 29 Yiicel study?
the past tense here.
4 Which error classification would you prefer to use if you were to
5 What's the second word? 5 Question 2 conduct a study of teacher error correction? Why?

6 Students? (class gives answer) 6 Transfer - 5 How might you balance these three sets of results in interpreting them?

7 Mmmmm (disapproval) 7 Disapprove -


8 Please repeat the sentence. 8 Repeat (explicit) 8
You have compiled and analyzed some cooperative learning task data using a
9 What? 9 Repeat (implicit) - relatively simple nine-code observational system (Table 4.3). Another more
elaborate interaction coding system was adapted by L i m (2000) i n her study
10 Again. Where did you go? 10 Altered question 9
on cooperative taskwork. The task in Lim's case included having cooperating
11 Really? Which film did you see? 11 Ignore 5 pairs reconstruct a dictated text from memory. Although codes i n the two
When you went to the cinema, 12. Provide and expand 8 cooperative studies reflect task differences, there are several codes that appear
12
did you have a good time? to refer to similar kinds of cooperative or non-cooperative behavior between
the partners. Our adaptation o f Lims system is shown in Table 4.8.
Yucel's questionnaire data on error-correction preferences of22-25-year-old
English language learners. (Context: same as Table 4.6.)
Exercise 4.15
(Adapted from Yucel 2000: 150-1)
Now, review your data table on cooperative interaction (Table 4.4), and answer
There are several interpretation issues that arise i n trying to compare the data the following questions:
presented in Tables 4.5, 4.6, and 4.7. These issues involve comparing: 1 Which of the Lim codes could be used in coding the picture sequencing
data?
1 data from different learner/teacher populations
2 What codes might usefully be added to the picture sequencing code set?
2 averaged and un-averaged teacher behavior counts
3 percentages and raw data 3 Are there any codes in the original set that Lim's codes do not include?
4 data using different systems o f error correction classification
5 data from teacher behavior and learner-expressed preferences

Significance of classroom research


Mid-twentieth-century linguistic studies and the interpretation o f these i n
guides intended for language teachers tended to focus somewhat independently
106 Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis

Table 4.8 Scaffolding functions in two-person student exchanges on learner language production (speaking and writing) and learner language
reception (listening and reading). A similar tendency is found i n some more
POSITIVE CODES
current discussions on language learning w h i c h t u r n on the relative
Recruitment of Interest (RI): importance o f language output' o f the learner versus language 'input' to the
Nominating or initiating topics and identifying of points for discussion. learner.
Modelling (M): A n interactional view sees speaking and listening—language output and
" Offering behaviour for imitation. It may involve completion or even explication of a solution input—as inextricably linked and, o f necessity, considered as an integrated
already partially executed by the partner. system. A n interactional view sees language
Feedback (F):
as a vehicle for the realization o f interpersonal relations and for the
Providing partner with information on a performance as it compares to the required standard.
performance o f social transactions between individuals . . . Language
Partner may also assist by highlighting the features of the task that are relevant.
teaching content, according to this view, may be specified and organized
Direction Maintenance (DM):
by patterns of exchanges and interaction or may be left unspecified, to
Keeping each other in pursuit of the task and working towards its completion. It involves
be shaped by the inclinations of learners i n interactions. (Richards and
deployment of questions and invitation to proceed to the next step.
Rodgers 2001:21)
Group Maintenance (GM)
Maintaining harmony in the group and lowering affective barriers through speech acts. There are three areas i n which classroom interaction studies have had major
Attempts to control frustration level in self and peer in order to complete the task influence on discussions of language pedagogy. These areas involve
Questioning ( Q l ) : a) Teacher-student interaction
Requesting for verbal response that assists by producing a mental operation the partner cannot b) Student-student interaction
or would not produce alone. This interaction assists further by giving the assistor information
c) Student-text interaction, in which the student reader or writer is perceived
about the partners understanding.
as involved i n an interactive dialog comprising author and reader.
Questioning (Q2):
Asking questions for the purpose of clarification as well as requests for help. Requests for help Let us consider each o f these briefly.
here refer to issues being discussed and not new problems.
a) I n a recent meta-analysis o f studies looking at the role o f interaction i n
Propositional Knowledge (PK): second language acquisition, Ellis (1999:8) concludes that there is clear
Contributing new ideas, information or detail recalled that assists in the successful completion evidence that social and intermental interaction are major forces i n the
of the task. acquisition of an L 2 \n classroom situations, Ellis notes the central role
Task Structuring (TS):
of the teacher i n controlling discourse as a means o f learners' obtaining
Helping partner and self to participate in the task by structuring. For example, sequencing,
the quality o f interaction likely to foster L2 acquisition. A number o f
segregating or structuring a task into or from components to modify the task.
Negative Codes techniques such as the teacher's recasting' o f student responses into
Erroneous Feedback (EF): more grammatically correct or more socially appropriate forms have
Giving misleading feedback or inaccurate information offering garbled explanations. Includes been demonstrated to support L2 learning.
pointing out problems and expressing doubt when the solution is already correct.
b) Student-student interaction patterns and their role i n language acquisition
Assertions without Explanations (A):
have been examined in some detail by researchers studying the impact o f
Making assertions or categorically stating a point of view without offering any explanation or
cooperative learning routines on language learning. Students working i n
justification. Repeating a point of view without elaboration.
cooperative classroom groups need to make themselves understood, so
Lack of Frustration Control (FC):
they tend to adjust their input to make i t comprehensible. The student
Expressing overt exasperation with partner or task. This frustration can manifest itself in words
speaker i n a pair or small group has the luxury o f adjusting speech to the
or tone of voice. level appropriate to the listener to negotiate meaning—a luxury n o t
Inauthentic Questions (NQ): available to the teacher speaking to a whole class.
Posing questions that do not genuinely seek clarification or understanding, including questions
asked with no particular purpose beside trying to confirm personal views. Because students are at a somewhat similar learning stage, their utterances
to one another tend to fall w i t h i n Vygotsky s zone o f proximal develop-
(Adapted from L i m 2000: 66-74)
108 Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis 109

merit' (Vygotsky 1978). The developmental level of any student is what 1 As a class, discuss the ways in which the Binet text might be considered
he or she can do alone; the proximal level is what he or she can do with more interactive than the Wirth text.
supportive collaboration. The student-student interaction student 2 Consider the pros and cons of each kind of text from the perspective of
interactions are said to have a higher probability of being 'developmentally a second language learner.
appropriate' than teacher-student interactions may be.
Sample A :
Finally, language acquisition is underwritten by input received repeatedly The first permanent settlement in New England was made by the Pilgrims, or
from a variety o f sources. 'Redundancy' is critical for language learning, Separatists, who established a colony on the coast of Massachusetts in 1620. This
and the cooperative learning group is a natural source o f redundant settlement, like Jamestown, was promoted by a commercial company. These early
communication. I n small group discussions, students each use a variety settlers were interested in improving their economic condition, but there was
of phrases providing the opportunity for the listeners to hone i n on another factor which played a very important part, namely, religion.
meaning as well as gain the repeated input necessary for learning to move (Fremont P. Wirth. (1936). American History. N.Y.: American Book Co.)
from short-term comprehension to long-term acquisition. One
Sample B:
summary of research i n this area concludes that
The Pilgrims landed on November 11,1620, from a ship called the Mayflower.
Language acquisition is determined by a complex interaction o f a Who were the Pilgrims and why did they come to America? Were they
number of critical input, output, and context variables. A n examination adventurers, conquerors, gold seekers?
of these critical variables reveals cooperative learning has a dramatic No, they were not. A few of those on the Mayflower came on the chance of
positive impact on almost all o f the variables critical to language getting land and farms of their own. But most came for another reason. They came
acquisition. (Kagan 1995:1) because they wished to worship God in their own way - a simple and faithful way,
but not the way of the Established Church of the England of their time.
c) A number o f those interested i n first and second language reading (and
They were family men, for the most part. They brought their wives and their
writing) pedagogy have also adapted an interactional stance i n regards to
children with them on a 64-day voyage, in a small tossing ship. One child was born
classroom instruction i n literacy. Grabe (1988) posits a notion o f inter-
on the voyage, two others just after the landfall. The whole company numbered a
activity i n second language reading and makes a distinction between
little over a hundred human beings. It was backed by an English company whose
reading as an interactive process (interaction between top-down and
investors put money into the venture. But the backbone of the venture was this
bottom-up processing) and interactive models o f reading (interaction
group of quiet, family men, bringing their wives and children to a coast at the
between reader and author as //reader and author were engaged i n a text-
world's end.
based conversation). One exercise favored i n this approach to pedagogy
Why did they do such a crazy thing? Why on earth did they take such a chance?
has writers first construct a piece of prose argumentation as a two-person
Nobody ordered them to do it, bribed them to do it. They went to great trouble
dialog before trying to put their argument into expository prose. Similarly,
and pain, uprooted their homes, left everything they had known behind, from the
readers in the interactive mode are encouraged to direct questions to an
memories of childhood to the things in the house that one looks at and cannot
author before beginning their reading, and then to search the text to see i f
take because there will be no room, and yet remembers.
their questions have been answered.
They wanted to worship God in their own way. They were resolved and
determined to worship God in their own way.
Exercise 4.16 (Stephen Vincent Binet (1944). America. NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.)

We have mentioned several senses in which reading and writing have been
considered interactively. However, some texts appear to be more interactive than
others. Linguistic studies of human interaction have been most insightful where the
social relations between interactors are somewhat fixed and agreed upon by
Below are two excerpts from school texts on American history. One was written interactors. Key studies include the Sinclair and Coulthard (1975) analysis
by a history professor and text writer, Fremont P. Wirth (Sample A) and one by a of teacher/student interaction, the study o f therapist/client interaction in
novelist, Stephen Vincent Binet, under temporary hire as a writer of school history psychotherapy by Labov and Fanshel (1977), and the examination o f doctor/
texts (Sample B). Compare the two texts and do the following: patient interaction by Candlin, Leather, and Bruton (1978). Thus, the study
110 Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis 111

o f classroom interaction has been seminal i n the attempts to understand w i t h research i n other subject area classrooms. However, classroom
more generally the patterns of human interaction. As we have noted previously, inter-action research i n second language classrooms is unique i n that
a number o f classroom observational techniques continue to be proposed, language is both the m e d i u m o f instruction and the content o f
the intent o f which is to track interaction patterns more clearly and to link instruction. This overlay o f medium and content of instruction provides
these to successful practice. Thus, classroom interaction studies continue to both special challenges and the opportunity for special insights.
have a central place in current linguistic examination o f human interaction. 5 Further professionalization of teaching There has been a growing interest
i n the further professionalization o f teaching, including involvement o f
classroom teachers i n the process o f research. This move ties in w i t h
other decentralizing trends i n both general and second language education.
Reflecting on classroom research These trends include school-based curriculum development, field-based
You have had a chance to look at several different uses of classroom interaction teacher preparation, and professional self-evaluation projects. The classroom
research, both w i t h respect to teacher-learner interaction and learner- is the teacher's home territory, a place where teachers are experts in their
learner interaction. Classroom interaction research has addressed a variety o f field and masters o f their environment. The classroom seems the ideal
key issues i n classroom instruction using a variety o f observational and setting for more rigorous inquiry by teachers.
reporting techniques. We have seen that there is such a rich array o f these 6 Bridging the theory-practice gap Some of the citations in our introductory
techniques that i t is often difficult to find common ground i n comparing chapter suggest the alienation that some practitioners feel towards
findings and i n getting a sense o f cumulative understanding. We have also 'research'. One o f the goals of classroom interaction research is to narrow
seen that some teaching axioms that seem reasonably commonsensical and the gap between theory and practice, allowing teachers to become
straightforward have been difficult to validate by means o f classroom enthusiastic producers as well as consumers o f educational research.
interaction studies. A n d yet, w i t h these difficulties acknowledged, classroom 7 The durability of classroom patterns Classrooms have looked pretty
interaction research is one o f the most active areas i n contemporary second much the same for the last 1,000 years. Despite changes in content,
language learning studies. technologies, methods, educational priorities, and professionalization o f
teaching, school classrooms and the activities that go on within school
Consider some possible reasons that might justify current enthusiasm for
classrooms have not changed much over the last millennium. The role
classroom interaction research:
and orientation of teachers and learners have been maintained although
1 Universal experience Almost everyone is a veteran observer o f classroom content has varied. So i t appears that analyzing what happens in regular
interaction, having spent many years o f life as a classroom learner. I t is classrooms involving teachers and learners is likely to stay i n fashion and
interesting to know something deeper about a topic i n which you are an relevant to improving education for the foreseeable future. (One thing
expert and an insider. that has changed is the importance placed i n former times on corporal
2 Importance of educational improvement Education occupies the biggest punishment.)
budgets o f most governmental agencies and almost no one, regardless 8 Classrooms as ideal environments for the study of talk The classroom
o f their schooling location or the subject o f instruction, is fully satisfied has features that make i t a particularly attractive environment for the
w i t h the ways that classes work. So there are almost always, almost study o f talk i n general. Ethnographers of communication examine how
everywhere, movements afoot to improve the delivery o f education, to talk is systematically patterned i n ways that reveal, or define, how the
make classroom interaction more efficient, more effective, maybe more speakers perceive their relationships and situation. Classrooms represent
inspiring. a strongly marked local social system in which relationships and situations
3 Unsettling findings Early classroom studies came up with some somewhat are somewhat fixed, allowing researchers intimate looks at the language
startling data. We previously referred to research that suggested that i n which marks these relationships and situations.
many classrooms, teacher talk took up most o f the interactional time 9 Homegrown nature of classroom research Many o f the techniques for
and that most o f that time, teachers asked questions to w h i c h they conducting SLA research and even motivation for particular kinds o f
already knew the answers. Were the findings accurate and how should research typically come from outside the field o f applied linguistics.
they be interpreted? However, with respect to studies o f classroom talk, educational researchers
4 Uniqueness of second language classes Research i n second language acknowledge that the initial impetus behind the investigations o f
classrooms shares many o f the same interests and techniques o f inquiry classroom talk came not from educational researchers but from applied
112 Classroom research: interaction analysis Classroom research: interaction analysis 113

linguists, such as Hymes, Gumperz, Sinclair, and Coulthard. The widely task). T h e n you considered different ways o f grouping and coding
influential model o f communicative competence emerged from classroom interaction data and saw that there were a rich array o f alternative systems
interaction studies by socio-linguist Dell Hymes (1971), whose research that researchers have proposed for carrying out this aspect o f classroom
concerns were largely directed towards the problems encountered by interaction research. You also explored ways to code both linguistic and non-
children from one cultural background who entered classrooms where linguistic data, both of which play important roles i n human interaction.
communicative demands were defined primarily in terms o f another. I n
Researchers make choices about what classroom interactions they observe
a sense, then, critical aspects of the study of classroom interaction can be
and report. Only a small portion of classroom interaction is actually isolated
said to have been home-grown, to have been initiated and developed
for recording, analysis, and interpretation, so both as researcher and reader
within the field o f applied linguistics. Applied linguists writing on the
you found that you need to be critical o f drawing conclusions too facilely
subject of classroom interaction research continue to be widely read and
from tip-of-the-iceberg displays o f data. You examined your own beliefs
cited by researchers from other traditions.
about what effective classrooms comprise and also reviewed candidate topics
10 Context for many current controversies"'It may seem odd to cite
for research and techniques for examining those topics. Choices here
controversy' as grounds for significance o f an area of research. However,
critically shape the kind of classroom interaction study you might do and the
as Hammersley (1986: xii) notes, given its recent history, classroom
results you might obtain.
research has been the site for some major theoretical and methodological
debates'. These debates take place not only across disciplines, as might You then looked at the diffuse range of ways i n which interaction is used in
be expected, but also within disciplines where many key issues find focus contemporary second language discussions and noted a variety o f the other
in how classroom research is best done. Thus, educational psychologists, factors that have contributed to the widespread interest i n classroom interaction
second language specialists, social anthropologists, linguists, sociologists, research. We invited you to consider not only the academic significance o f
and ethnomethodologists all assert a m u l t i p l i c i t y o f views as to how interaction studies but also the very personal benefits that a more focused
classroom interaction research should be carried out both within their look at the classroom can bring i n the way o f insights and ideas to the
own areas of specialization as well as in the wider context of teaching arid practicing teacher.
learning generally.

Exercise 4.17
Consider the above claims regarding the significance of classroom interaction
research. Then do the following:
1 Pick three of these that you find interesting and/or compelling.
2 Jot down your reasons for selecting each of the three.
3 What insights have you had about classroom interaction that may be
useful in your own teaching?

Summary
I n this chapter, you have traveled the path o f classroom research from your
own experience as the participants being studied to your reflections on
designing classroom interaction research studies o f your own. You looked
closely at two key areas o f classroom interaction—the interaction between
teachers and learners (when teachers correct learner errors) and the interaction
between learners (when learners work i n pairs carrying out a cooperative

Potrebbero piacerti anche