Sei sulla pagina 1di 15

Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Ageing Research Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/arr

Review

Effects of combined cognitive and exercise interventions on cognition


in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: A systematic
review
Lawla L.F. Law a,∗ , Fiona Barnett b , Matthew K. Yau a , Marion A. Gray c
a
Occupational Therapy Discipline, School of Public Health, Tropical Medicine & Rehabilitation Sciences, James Cook University, Queensland, Australia
b
Institute of Sport & Exercise Science, School of Public Health, Tropical Medicine & Rehabilitation Sciences, James Cook University, Queensland, Australia
c
Cluster for Health Improvement, School of Health and Sport Sciences, University of the Sunshine Coast, Maroochydore DC, Queensland, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Global concern on the potential impact of dementia is mounting. There are emerging calls for studies in
Received 5 November 2013 older populations to investigate the potential benefits of combining cognitive and exercise interventions
Received in revised form 25 February 2014 for cognitive functions. The purpose of this systematic review is to examine the efficacy of combined
Accepted 28 February 2014
cognitive and exercise training in older adults with or without cognitive impairment and evaluate the
Available online 12 March 2014
methodological quality of the intervention studies. A systematic search of Cinahl, Medline, PsycINFO,
ProQuest, EMBASE databases and the Cochrane Library was conducted. Manual searches of the reference
Keywords:
list from the included papers and additional internet searches were also done. Eight studies were identi-
Systematic review
Cognitive impairment
fied in this review, five of which included a cognitively impaired population and three studies included
Healthy older adults a cognitively healthy population. The results showed that combined cognitive and exercise training can
Combined intervention be effective for improving the cognitive functions and functional status of older adults with and without
Dual-task exercise cognitive impairment. However, limited evidence can be found in populations with cognitive impairment
when the evaluation included an active control group comparison. Further well-designed studies are still
needed to explore the potential benefits of this new intervention paradigm.
© 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62
2. Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
2.1. Search strategies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
2.2. Inclusion/exclusion criteria and selection process . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
2.3. Data extraction and analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
2.4. Quality assessment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
3. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
3.1. Study selection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
3.2. Study characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 63
3.3. Participants and settings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64
3.3.1. Studies with cognitively healthy participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64
3.3.2. Studies with cognitively impaired participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64
3.4. Intervention duration and group comparison . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64
3.4.1. Studies with cognitively healthy participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64
3.4.2. Studies with cognitively impaired participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64

∗ Corresponding author at: Occupational Therapy Discipline, James Cook Drive, Douglas; School of Public Health, Tropical Medicine and Rehabilitation Sciences, Rehabili-
tation and Exercise Sciences Building, James Cook University, Townsville, QLD 4811, Australia. Tel.: +61 7 47816678.
E-mail addresses: lan.law@my.jcu.edu.au (L.L.F. Law), fiona.barnett@jcu.edu.au (F. Barnett), matthew.yau@jcu.edu.au (M.K. Yau), marion.gray@usc.edu.au (M.A. Gray).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2014.02.008
1568-1637/© 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
62 L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75

3.5. Intervention programs . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64


3.5.1. Studies with cognitively healthy participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64
3.5.2. Studies with cognitively impaired participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64
3.6. Outcome measures and intervention effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65
3.6.1. Studies with cognitively healthy participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65
3.6.2. Studies with cognitively impaired participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67
3.7. Methodological quality of included studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67
4. Discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70
4.1. Comparison and control group design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70
4.2. Study population screening . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70
4.3. Training sequence in combined intervention . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
4.4. Dual-task component . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
4.5. Intervention period . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
4.6. Outcome measure selection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
4.7. Visual-spatial component . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
4.8. Generalization of training effects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
4.9. Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
5. Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
Conflicts of interest . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
Source of funding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73
Appendix A. Supplementary data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73

1. Introduction peripheral risk factors (e.g. hypertension) for cognitive decline


(Cotman et al., 2007; Pereira et al., 2007).
The increasing prevalence of cognitive impairment with age Whilst the uniqueness of exercise-induced effects to enhance
intensifies the potential impact upon global health and health care. brain health and cognitive functions through multiple routes is
It has been projected that there will be 7.7 million new cases evident, results of intervention studies on single exercise interven-
of dementia each year, implying a new case of dementia every tion are disappointing (Busse et al., 2009; Etnier et al., 2006). A
4 s (WHO and ADI, 2012). The World Health Organization (WHO) recent systematic review showed that strong evidence to support
has urged global efforts to take a serious focus on the potential the effects of exercise interventions on cognitive functions of older
impacts of dementia on the societal and health care systems world- adults is still lacking (Snowden et al., 2011).
wide. Individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) are at high It has been proposed that exercise has to occur in the context
risk of progressing to Alzheimer’s diseases and other dementias, of a cognitively challenged environment in order to be effective for
with reported conversion rate of 50% in 2–3 year (Amieva et al., inducing neural and cognitive benefits rather than exercise alone
2004) and even up to 60–100% in 5–10 years (Morris et al., 2001; (Fabel and Kempermann, 2008; Fabel et al., 2009). Studies also
Petersen, 2004). Addressing the lack of pharmacological treat- have found that a combination of exercise and an enriched envi-
ment for individuals with MCI (Alzheimer’s Association, 2013), it ronment induces more new neurons and benefits the brain greater
is critical to explore the potential effects of non-pharmacological than either exercise or an enriched environment alone (Fabel and
interventions. Kempermann, 2008; Fabel et al., 2009; Olson et al., 2006). An ani-
The benefits of exercise on cognition are widely recognized mal study reported significant improvements in cognitive ability
(Cotman and Berchtold, 2007; Kramer and Erickson, 2007; van when combined physical and cognitive training was undertaken
Praag, 2009). Animal studies have consistently shown exer- (Langdon and Corbett, 2012). A combination of exercise and cogni-
cise increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the dentate tive interventions, either sequentially or simultaneously, appears
gyrus of the hippocampus (Fabel et al., 2003; Kronenberg et to have the potential effect to maintain or improve cognitive func-
al., 2006), an important brain area for learning and memory. tions (Langdon and Corbett, 2012; Schaefer and Schumacher, 2011).
Exercise regulates a number of growth factors such as brain- Indeed, there are emerging calls for studies in older populations
derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), which plays a crucial role to investigate the potential benefits of combined cognitive and
in neuroprotection and synaptic plasticity (Adlard et al., 2005; physical interventions to accomplish an optimal outcome (Amoyal
Vaynman et al., 2004); insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF-1), which and Fallon, 2012; Kraft, 2012; Rebok, 2008; Thom and Clare,
promotes neuronal growth and improves cognitive performance 2011).
(Cotman and Berchtold, 2002; Liorens-Martin et al., 2010); vascular The aims of this review were: (1) to assess the efficacy of
endothelial growth factor (VEGF), which stimulates angiogene- combined cognitive and exercise training to improve cognitive
sis and vasculogenesis (Tang et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2013) functions in older adults with and without cognitive impairment;
and promotes brain ischemic tolerance (Zhang et al., 2011). Fur- (2) to examine the methodological quality of the included studies;
thermore, exercise reduces inflammatory cytokines and oxidative and (3) to summarize the latest results on combined cognitive and
stress, which suggests anti-inflammatory and antioxidant effects exercise training in older adults with or without cognitive impair-
on the brain (Pervaiz and Hoffman-Goetz, 2011; Santin et al., ment.
2011). For the purpose of this review, combined cognitive and exercise
Colcombe et al. (2006) also found that exercise correlates with interventions are structured interventions that combine cognitive
an increase in brain volume over the frontal, parietal, and tem- training and exercise, either conducted in sequence or simultane-
poral cortices in humans. Studies also have shown that exercise ously under dual-tasking paradigms. Cognitive training are defined
can promote cerebral blood flow which enhances neurogenesis as structure repeated practice on tasks with an inherent problem,
and improves learning (Farmer et al., 2004; van Praag et al., 2005), using standardized tasks targeting specific cognitive domains
as well as promotes cardiovascular fitness and therefore reduces and/or teaching strategies and skills in order to optimize cognition

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75 63

and functioning (Clare and Woods, 2003; Martin et al., 2006). derived to compare groups before and after the intervention.
Exercise is defined as a form of physical activity that is planned, (Practical effect size calculator retrieved March 9, 2013 from
structured and repetitive over an extended period of time, with http://gunston.gmu.edu/cebcp/EffectSizeCalculator/d/means-and-
the purpose to improve fitness, performance or health (Casperson standard-deviations.html). Effect sizes were interpreted as small,
et al., 1985). d = 0.20; medium, d = 0.50; or large, d = 0.80 (Cohen, 1988).

2. Methods 2.4. Quality assessment

2.1. Search strategies Methodological quality of the included studies was indepen-
dently assessed by 2 reviewers (LL and FB) using a 13-item checklist
A systematic computer-based search of Cinahl, Medline, modified from the Delphi list (Verhagen et al., 1998), the Physio-
PsycINFO, ProQuest, EMBASE databases and the Cochrane Library therapy Evidence Database (PEDro) scale (Maher et al., 2003) and
was conducted for the time period between January 1995 and the “Design-specific criteria to assess for risk of bias” by the Agency
June 2013. The following search terms were used: (combine* for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) (Viswanathan et al.,
interventions OR rehabilitation OR dual-task OR multi-modal) 2012). Any disagreements identified between the two reviewers
AND (exercise OR physical activity OR resistance training OR were resolved by a third reviewer (MY) as a final decision. Details
endurance training) AND (cognitive training OR cognitive-motor of the quality checklist and the rating criteria are presented in
OR mental) AND (cognitive impairment OR cognit* OR dement* OR Appendix 1.
Alzheimer*). The search was limited to publications in English. All All items on the list applied to both RCT and non-RCT. A qual-
reference lists in selected journal articles were further screened and ity score ranging from 0 to 13 was calculated for each study.
additional internet searches using the same search terms were con- The scale is only used to provide a quantitative reference of the
ducted on Google Scholar to identify additional potentially relevant degree/likelihood that the reported methodology and results of the
articles. included studies are approaching free of bias. As a summary on
quality, a study was defined as: (1) High quality when presenting a
2.2. Inclusion/exclusion criteria and selection process positive score on 10 or more items; (2) Medium quality when pre-
senting a positive score on 7–9 items and (3) Low quality when
As recommended in the Cochrane Handbook of Systematic presenting a positive score on <7 items (<50% of the maximum
Review of Interventions, two reviewers (LL and FB) independently attainable score) (Verhagen et al., 2007; Viswanathan et al., 2012).
screened the titles and abstracts to identify relevant articles and
3. Results
potentially relevant studies. Disagreements between the review-
ers about inclusion were resolved through discussion (Higgins and 3.1. Study selection
Green, 2011). Studies were included in this review if they met the
following criteria: (1) design: randomized controlled trial (RCT) A summary of the selection process is illustrated in Fig. 1. The
or non-randomized controlled trial (NRCT); (2) sample popula- initial database search identified a total of 572 potentially relevant
tion: older adults (aged 60 and older) with or without cognitive citations with 73 from Cinahl, 41 from Medline, 230 from PsycINFO,
impairment/decline or dementia but no mental or neurological dis- 35 from ProQuest, 89 from EMBASE and 104 from Cochrane Library.
orders other than dementia, such as stroke or major depression; After excluding duplicated articles, 474 citations were left. All titles
(3) intervention: combined cognitive and exercise training; and (4) and abstracts were screened according to the inclusion/exclusion
outcome: cognitive functions assessed using neuropsychological criteria described and 461 articles were discarded.
tests as primary or secondary outcomes. Articles were excluded The common reasons for exclusion of articles were the follow-
if they were: (1) non-intervention studies; (2) theoretical articles ing: the interventions were not combined exercise and cognitive
or descriptions of treatment approaches; (3) review articles; (4) intervention (80), they were not intervention studies or were
unpublished studies, abstracts or dissertations; (5) articles with- reviews/theoretical articles (158), they did not include the target
out adequate specification of interventions; (6) non-peer reviewed population under this review (213), and did not include cognitive
articles and book chapters; and (7) non-English language articles. outcomes (10). Full texts of 13 articles were retrieved from the
Multicomponent intervention studies which did not distinguish the initial database search and carefully examined. Cross-referencing
contribution of combined cognitive and physical exercise compo- led to three more articles and the internet search using the same
nent on the effects were also excluded. search terms identified three more articles. A total of 19 articles
were finally retrieved for full text screening.
2.3. Data extraction and analysis After further screening with the inclusion/exclusion criteria, 11
articles were discarded as four articles used exercise only or did not
Data were extracted using a data extraction form for the use combined exercise and cognitive interventions (Burgener et al.,
description of methodology and important trial characteristics 2008; Heyn, 2003; Komai and Yamada, 2012; Onor et al., 2007);
including study populations, intervention type, training delivery, three were on-going studies (Gates et al., 2011; Gillette-Guyonnet
volume of training, outcome measures, and follow-up. This review et al., 2009; O’Dwyer et al., 2007); one did not include cognitive
focused on a description of the studies and their results, and on outcomes (Doi et al., 2013); one was a second paper reporting a data
qualitative synthesis of the findings. However, Cohen’s d effect set from the same study (Fabre et al., 1999); one was a repeat of the
sizes (Cohen, 1988) for cognitive and functional outcomes at online first view (Coelho et al., 2012); and one did not distinguish
pre- and post-intervention were derived on the basis of reported the effects of the combined cognitive and physical training in the
statistics. The standard effect sizes were calculated by dividing study (Shatil, 2013). A total of eight studies were therefore included
the mean score differences of the combined intervention and for this review.
control groups in each study by the pooled estimate standard
deviation for the two groups. When means and standard devi- 3.2. Study characteristics
ations were not available, effect sizes were computed from the
reported P-value or F-values reported in the study (Thalheimer Of the eight studies reviewed, three studies (1 RCT and 2 non-
and Cook, 2002). The 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were also RCTs) included a population of cognitively healthy older adults

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
64 L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75

3.3.2. Studies with cognitively impaired participants


The number of people with cognitive impairment (total 322)
varied from 27 (Coelho et al., 2012) to 126 (Barnes et al., 2013).
The mean age of the study populations ranged from 67.8 (Kounti
et al., 2011) to 82.3 (Schwenk et al., 2010) years. Four studies used
the Mini-mental Status Examination (MMSE) score as a baseline
measure of cognitive function, with an average score of 22.5 ranging
from 15.4 (Coelho et al., 2012) to 29.6 (Kounti et al., 2011). One
study used the modified MMSE score with a mean score of 94.4
(Barnes et al., 2013). One study was conducted in Germany, one in
Brazil, one in Greece, one in United States and one in Japan.

3.4. Intervention duration and group comparison

3.4.1. Studies with cognitively healthy participants


Table 1 presents the characteristics of interventions for cogni-
tively healthy older adults. The total time of intervention varied
from 38 (Fabre et al., 2002) to 67.5 (Oswald et al., 2006) hours,
while the intervention duration ranged from two months (Fabre
et al., 2002) to one year (Oswald et al., 2006).
Two studies were four-arm controlled trials comparing the com-
bined cognitive and exercise training, single cognitive training,
single exercise training and control groups (Fabre et al., 2002;
Legault et al., 2011). Fabre et al. (2002) used no treatment control
and Legault et al. (2011) used health education control. One study
was a six-arm controlled trial comparing combined cognitive and
physical training, combined psycho-educational and physical train-
ing, single cognitive training, single psycho-educational training,
single physical training and no treatment control groups (Oswald
et al., 2006).

Fig. 1. Selection process of the systemic review. 3.4.2. Studies with cognitively impaired participants
Table 2 presents the intervention characteristics for older adults
(Fabre et al., 2002; Legault et al., 2011; Oswald et al., 2006). Four with cognitive impairment. The total time of intervention varied
studies (2 RCT and 2 non-RCTs) included a population of people from 30 (Kounti et al., 2011) to 120 (Suzuki et al., 2012) hours, and
with cognitive impairment including dementia (Schwenk et al., the intervention duration ranged from three months (Barnes et al.,
2010), Alzheimer’s disease (AD; Coelho et al., 2012) and mild cog- 2013; Schwenk et al., 2010) to one year (Suzuki et al., 2012).
nitive impairment (MCI; Kounti et al., 2011; Suzuki et al., 2012). One study (Barnes et al., 2013) was a four-arm controlled trial
One RCT included a population of people with cognitive complaints comparing the combined mental activity and exercise training;
(Barnes et al., 2013). mental activity and exercise control; mental activity control and
Cognitive complaints have been found to be associated with exercise; and mental activity control and exercise control. Four
decreased performance in objective memory and executive func- studies were two-arm controlled trials comparing a single dual task
tion tests (Rouch et al., 2008). Neuroimaging studies have also intervention group with a control group, including motor placebo
shown people with subjective cognitive complaints have structural (Schwenk et al., 2010), no treatment (Coelho et al., 2012), waitlist
brain changes similar to persons with mild cognitive impairment (Kounti et al., 2011) and health education (Suzuki et al., 2012).
and different from those of healthy people (Saykin et al., 2006;
van der Flier et al., 2004). Therefore, for this review, the identi- 3.5. Intervention programs
fied study which included a population with cognitive complaints
(Barnes et al., 2013) was reported under the studies with cogni- 3.5.1. Studies with cognitively healthy participants
tively impaired population. Tables 1 and 2 All three reviewed studies used combined cognitive and
illustrate the intervention characteristics of the reviewed stud- physical training sessions in sequence. All interventions were
ies in cognitively healthy populations and older adults with center-based, with one study also including a home-based walk-
cognitive impairment respectively. Tables 3 and 4 ing program (Legault et al., 2011). The content of cognitive training
illustrate the study characteristics as reported by outcome included computer-based memory training (Legault et al., 2011);
measures and effect sizes of the reviewed studies. processing speed, attention and memory training (Oswald et al.,
3.3. Participants and settings 2006) or structured multicomponent cognitive training (Fabre
et al., 2002). In regards to the exercise sessions, two studies used
3.3.1. Studies with cognitively healthy participants aerobic exercise (Fabre et al., 2002; Legault et al., 2011); and one
The number of cognitively healthy people (total 480) in the stud- study used balance and flexibility training (Oswald et al., 2006).
ies varied from 32 (Fabre et al., 2002) to 375 (Oswald et al., 2006).
The mean age of the study populations ranged from 61.9 (Fabre 3.5.2. Studies with cognitively impaired participants
et al., 2002) to 79.5 (Oswald et al., 2006) years. Only one study per- Of the five reviewed studies, four studies used dual-task training
formed cognitive screening using the modified Mini-mental Status within the exercise sessions (Coelho et al., 2012; Kounti et al., 2011;
Examination (modified MMSE) at baseline (Legault et al., 2011). Schwenk et al., 2010; Suzuki et al., 2012). One study used combined
One study was conducted in France, one in Germany and one in cognitive and physical training by including an independent home-
United States. based computer cognitive training plus a supervised center-based

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75 65

Table 1
Intervention characteristics of studies in cognitively healthy populations.

Study Design Participants Interventions Total intervention Control/comparison (total


hours (h) hours)

Fabre et al. (2002) Non-RCT N = 32; France Supervised group 38 h CG: No training, but meeting
IG/CG/CG*1/CG*2: 8/8/8/8 Combined Aerobic (AT) & Mental for leisure activities
training (MT) CG*1: Aerobic training (AT)
? Sequence CG*2: Mental training (MT)
Mean age (years):
IG/CG1/CG*1/CG*2: AT: brisk walking and/or jogging 16 h
61.9/65.7/65.4/67.5 (intensity determined by
ventilation threshold
2×/week for 60 min/session for 2
months
MMSE score: not conducted
MT: eight themes: perceptive 12 h
activities, attention, intellectual
structuration, association and
imagination, language, spatial
marks, temporal marks and
associated recruiting.
1×/week; 90 min/session for 2
months

Legault et al. (2011) RCT N = 73; United States Supervised group CG: Health education;
IG/CG/CG*1/CG*2: 19/18/18/18 Combined Cognitive (CT) & 62 h 1×/week
Physical training (PT); CG*1: Cognitive training (CT)
PT followed CT on the same day.
Mean age (years): CT: Computer-based memory 20 h
training;
IG/CG/CG*1/CG*2: 24 sessions (2×/week, for 2
76.9/75.4/76.0/77.5 months + 1×/week for 2 months);
40–50 min/session
Modified MMSE: PT: aerobic & flexibility training 42 h
(walking/stationary cycling)
IG/CG/CG*1/CG*2: 32 center-based sessions (2×/week
94.6/94.3/95.6/94.6 for 4 months); 60 min/session; plus
150 min/week home-base walking
sessions for the first month

Oswald et al. Non-RCT N = 375; Germany Supervised group CG: No treatment


(2006) IG/CG/CG*1/CG*2/CG*3/CG*4: Combined Cognitive (CT) & 67.5 h CG*1: Cognitive training (CT)
Physical training (PT) CG*2: Physical training (PT)
32/103/57/32/115/36 PT was carried out before CT or Psy CG*3: Psychoeducational
T in a part of the training groups training (Psy T; 45 h)
and following CT or Psy T in psychoeducation, group
another part of the groups discussion, role playing
Mean age (years): 79.5 CT: Visual search tasks; 45 h CG*4: Combined Psy T + PT
verbal/visual memory tasks; (67.5 h)
memory strategies
30 sessions (1×/week over 1 year);
90 min/session
MMSE score: Not conducted
PT: Balance, perceptual, motor 22.5 h
coordination flexibility; gymnastic
exercises and games
30 sessions (1×/week over 1 year);
45 min/session

IG: intervention group; CG: control group; CG*: comparison group.


MMSE: Mini-mental State Examination

exercise training (Barnes et al., 2013). Regarding the exercise com- 3.6. Outcome measures and intervention effects
ponents, three studies included aerobic and strength training which
targeted exercise intensity (Barnes et al., 2013; Coelho et al., 2012; 3.6.1. Studies with cognitively healthy participants
Suzuki et al., 2012); one study included resistance and balance Table 3 presents the outcomes, key findings and effect sizes of
training (Schwenk et al., 2010) and one study included walking and the reviewed studies in cognitively healthy persons. Two studies
movement-based exercise (Kounti et al., 2011). In regards to the measured general cognitive functions, with one study reporting
dual-task components, three studies (Coelho et al., 2012; Schwenk significant improvement in the cognitive function composite score,
et al., 2010; Suzuki et al., 2012) involved performing motor tasks and sustained at five-year follow-up (Oswald et al., 2006). Two
(e.g. walking, throwing/bouncing ball) and concurrent cognitive studies assessed memory performance, with one study showing
tasks (e.g. calculation, naming or reacting to verbal command). a significant effect on the Wechsler Memory Scale (Fabre et al.,
One study (Kounti et al., 2011) conducted dual-task training using 2002). One study also included subjective measures of cognitive
cognitive-based kinetic exercise with visual and verbal cues (e.g. impairment, functional status and everyday competence (Oswald
wreath, boards with letters, balls and rings). et al., 2006) and found significant improvements in cognitive

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.

66
Table 2
Intervention characteristics of studies in cognitively impaired populations.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Study Design Participants Interventions Total intervention hours (h) Control/comparison (total hours)

Barnes et al. (2013) RCT (Rater-blind) N = 126; older adults with memory Independent Home-based + center-based CG: Mental activity (MA-I) &
complaints; USA supervised class Exercise control (EX-C;
IG/CG/CG*1/CG*2: 32/31/31/32 Combined Mental Activity (MA-I) & Aerobic 72 h stretching, toning) (72 h)
Mean age: 73.4 years Exercise (Ex-I) CG*1: Aerobic exercise (EX-I) &
Modified MMSE: MA-I: independent computer-based training at 36 h Mental activity control (MA-C;
IG/CG/CG*1/CG*2: 94.0/94.4/94.6/94.8 home for visual and auditory processing DVD educational lecture) (72 h)
12 weeks; 60 min/day; 3 days/week CG*2: Exercise control (EX-C) &
EX-I: aerobic & strength training 36 h Mental activity control (MA-C)
12 weeks; 60 min/day; 3 days/week (72 h)

Coelho et al. (2012) Non-RCT N = 27; Alzheimer’s disease (AD); Brazil Supervised group CG: No intervention

L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75


IG/CG: n = 14/13 Multimodal Dual-task exercise: 48 h
Mean age (years): Strength/resistance training, moderate
IG/CG: 78.0/77.1 intensity aerobic capacity/balance training,
MMSE score: motor activities (bouncing ball, walking,
IG/CG: 19.5 ± 4.1/19.0 ± 2.9 exercise with weights); plus simultaneous
cognitive tasks (naming/words generation or
reacting to verbal command)
16 weeks; 3×/week; 60 min/session

Kounti et al. (2011) Non-RCT N = 58; mild cognitive impairment; Greece Supervised group CG: Wait-list control
(Rater-blinded) IG/CG: 29/29 Dual-task kinetic exercise: 30 h
Mean age (years): visual-active movement, visual-walking,
IG/CG: 70.48/67.83 visual-balance, visual-fine motor,
MMSE score: auditory-free movement
IG/CG: 28.03 ± 1.61/27.34 ± 1.83 20 sessions, 1×/week; 90 min/session

Schwenk et al. (2010) RCT N = 61; dementia; Germany Supervised group CG: Motor placebo group: low
Double-blinded IG/CG: 26/35 Dual-task exercise 48 h intensity exercise including
(Rater & subject Mean age (years): Progressive resistance and functional balance flexibility exercise,
blinded) IG/CG: 80.4/82.3 training; then progress to specific dual-task calisthenics, and ball games
MMSE score: training (e.g. walking while throwing while sitting (24 h)
IG/CG: 21.0 ± 2.9/21.7 ± 2.9 ball/calculation; 10–15 min individual)
12 weeks; 2×/week; 120 min/session

Suzuki et al. (2012) RCT (Rater-blind) N = 50; amnestic mild cognitive Supervised group Health education; 3 sessions
impairment; Japan Multicomponent dual-task exercise: 120 h over 12 months
IG/CG: 25/25 Moderate intensity aerobic exercise, muscle
Mean age: 76 ± 7.1 years; strength, postural balance & dual task training;
MMSE score: plus self-monitoring daily home-based
IG/CG: 26.8 ± 1.8/26.6 ± 1.6 exercise (outdoor walking)
80 sessions over 12 months; 2×/week;
90 min/session

IG: intervention group; CG: control group; CG*: comparison group.


MMSE: Mini-mental State Examination.
L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75 67

impairment (Sandoz clinical assessment geriatrics) at follow-up as

d = 1.29 (0.21–2.36)
well as in functional status (Independent living composite score)

Data not available


Post-intervention
at post-intervention and at five-year follow-up. Only one study

for calculation
assessed executive functions but found no effects (Legault et al.,
2011).

3.6.2. Studies with cognitively impaired participants

Effect sizes (95% CI)


Table 4 presents the outcomes, key findings and effect sizes of

d = −0.66 (−1.67 to

Data not available


Pre-intervention
the reviewed studies in people with cognitive impairment. Two

for calculation
outcomes, executive functions (in 4 out of 5 studies) and atten-
tion/processing speed (in 4 out of 5 studies), were commonly used
in the reviewed studies. Two studies reported significant improve-

0.34)
ments in executive functions from Frontal Assessment Battery and
Clock Drawing Test (Coelho et al., 2012) in AD and Rey-Osterrieth

Significant between-group effects


Complex Figure Test (Kounti et al., 2011) in MCI. These two stud-
ies also found significant effects on attention/processing speed
from Symbol Search Subtest of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III

(compared to CG & CG*)


(Coelho et al., 2012) and Test of Everyday Attention (Kounti et al.,
2011).

(compared to CG)
Three studies included measures of Verbal Fluency for language
performance, with two studies of MCI finding significant improve-
ments (Kounti et al., 2011; Suzuki et al., 2012). Kounti et al. (2011)

0/+ at FU
+/+ at FU

+/+ at FU

+/+ at FU
and Suzuki et al. (2012) also examined general cognitive func-
tions and both found significant improvements on Mini-mental

0
0

0
+
state examination (MMSE) scores. Three studies assessed memory
performance, however only one study of MCI showed significant

Executive functions & Memory Composite Score


effects on immediate recall of Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised

Sandoz clinical assessment geriatrics SCAG


(Suzuki et al., 2012). One study of MCI also used subjective mea-
sure of functional status (Functional Rating Scale of Symptoms of

Executive functions Composite Score

Independent Living Composite Score


Cognitive Function Composite Score
Dementia) which showed significant improvement (Kounti et al.,

Episodic Memory Composite Score


2011). One study measured dual-task cost (DTC) in participants
Wechsler memory scale (WMS)
Outcome Measures of interest

with dementia and observed significant effect on DTC combined


motor-cognitive performance (Schwenk et al., 2010).

Everyday competence
BEC 96 questionnaire

Emotional Status
3.7. Methodological quality of included studies

The results of the methodological quality assessment are pre-


sented in Table 5. The reviewed studies fulfilled 7–11 quality
criteria out of the maximum of 13 items. Only one RCT (Schwenk
et al., 2010) performed a concealed randomization and was the only
Post-intervention 2

Post-intervention 4

study that had both rater and subject blinded. However, this was
Post-intervention
12 months; FU 5

the only one study that did not use intention-to-treat in data analy-
Study Characteristics and results of studies in cognitively healthy populations.

sis. Three RCT (Barnes et al., 2013; Legault et al., 2011; Suzuki et al.,
Follow-up

2012) did not state whether the random allocation was concealed,

+: positive effect for intervention group; 0: no difference between groups.


months

months

with two (Legault et al., 2011; Suzuki et al., 2012) did not report the
years

method of random sequence generation. The RCT by Legault et al.


(2011) was stated as single-blinded but did not report the party
(CT) & Physical training

(CT) & Physical training


Combined Aerobic (AT)
& Mental training (MT)

which was blinded. Two RCT (Barnes et al., 2013; Suzuki et al., 2012)
Combined Cognitive

Combined Cognitive

and one other non-RCT (Kounti et al., 2011) reported rater-blinded.


One study (Oswald et al., 2006) recruited a random sample but the
group allocation was not random.
Intervention

All the included studies reported eligibility criteria, baseline


similarity, same follow-up length, pre-specified outcomes as well
CG: control group; CG*: comparison group.
(PT)

(PT)

as point measures and measures of variability for the primary


d: standard between-group effect size.
FU: follow-up; N: number of subjects.

outcome measures. Seven of the eight studies achieved outcome


Subject

N = 375

measures from more than 85% of the participants initially allo-


N = 32

N = 73

cated to groups at least at one time point of measures. Main


methodological shortcomings or bias identified included no or
CI: confidence interval.

inadequate randomization, non-concealed allocation, lack of blind-


Oswald et al. (2006)
Legault et al. (2011)
Fabre et al. (2002)

ing and unclear reliability of outcomes measures in some studies.


All studies fulfilled the criteria for 7 or more quality items,
with one (Schwenk et al., 2010) meeting 11 out of 13 quality
criteria. Therefore, seven of the reviewed studies were consid-
Study
Table 3

ered to be of medium quality and one was assessed as high


quality.

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.

68
Table 4
Study characteristics and results of studies in cognitively impaired populations.

Study Subject Intervention Follow-up Outcome Measures of Interest Significant Effect sizes (95% CI)
between-group effects

Pre-intervention Post-intervention

Barnes et al. (2013) N = 126; older Combined Mental Post-intervention Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test 0
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

adults with Activity & Aerobic 12 weeks (RAVLT)


memory Exercise Verbal Fluency (letter & category) 0
complaints Digit Symbol Substitution Test 0
(DSST)
Trail Making Test (TMT) 0
Eriksen Flanker Test (EFT) 0
Useful Field of View (UFOV) 0

Coelho et al. (2012) N = 27; Alzheimer’s Multimodal Post-intervention Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) + d = −0.35 (−1.11 to d = 1.18 (0.37–2.01)
disease (AD) Dual-task exercise 16 weeks 0.41)
Clock Drawing Test (CDT), + d = −0.14 (−0.89 to d = 0.96 (0.16–1.76)
0.62)
Symbol Search Subtest of WAIS-III + d = −0.04 (−0.79 to d = 1.57 (0.71–2.44)

L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75


(Symbol) 0.72)

Kounti et al. (2011) N = 58; mild Dual-task kinetic Post-intervention Mini-mental State Examination + d = 0.40 (−0.12 to 0.92) d = 0.63 (0.11–1.16)
cognitive exercise 20 weeks (MMSE)
impairment Test of Everyday Attention (TEA) + d = 0.08 (−0.44 to 0.59) d = 0.24 (−0.28 to 0.76)
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test 0
(RAVLT)
Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure + d = 0.01 (−0.50 to 0.53) d = 0.52 (0.001–1.05)
Test (ROCFT)
Verbal Fluency Test (FAS) + d = 0.38 (−0.14 to 0.90) d = 0.62 (0.09–1.14)
Functional Rating Scale of + d = 0.17 (−0.34 to 0.69) d = 0.59 (0.06–1.11)
Symptoms of Dementia (FRSSD)
Functional Cognitive Assessment 0
Scale (FUCAS)
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test 0
(WCST)
Wechsler Adult Intelligence 0
Scale-Revised (WAIS-R)
Rivermead Behavioral Memory 0
Test (RBMT)
Boston Naming Test (BNT) 0

Schwenk et al. N = 61; dementia Dual-task exercise Post-intervention DTC Cognitive performance 0 Data not available for d = 0.99 (0.39–1.59)
(2010) 12 weeks calculation
DTC Combined motor-cognitive +
performance

Suzuki et al. (2012) N = 50; amnestic Multicomponent Post-intervention 6 Mini-mental State Examination + at 6 m & 12 m d = 0.44 (−0.44–0.67) d = 0.11 (−0.45–0.66)
mild cognitive dual-task exercise months, 12 months (MMSE)
impairment Immediate recall WMS-R + at 6 m & 12 m d = 0.09 (−0.46–0.65) d = 0.16 (−0.39–0.72)
Delayed recall WMS-R 0
Digit symbol-coding (DSC) subset 0
WAIS-III
Letter verbal fluency test (LVFT) + at 12 m d = −0.16 (−0.71–0.40) d = 0.22 (−0.34–0.77)
Category verbal fluency test (CVFT) 0
Stroop Color and Word Test 0
(SCWT).

N: number of subjects; DTC: dual-task cost.


d: standard between-group effect size; CI: confidence interval.
+: positive effect for intervention group; 0: no difference between groups.
Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Table 5
Overview of quality rating of reviewed studies.
Study Randomization Concealed Eligibility Baseline similarity Assessor Care provider Subject Valid reliable Same follow-up Outcome from Pre-specified Data Intention-to-treat Sum score
allocation criteria blinded blinded blinded measures length >85% subjects outcomes presentation
Barnes et al. Yes Unclear Yes Yes Yes Unclear Unclear Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes 9
(2013) (computer (not stated) (baseline (stated) (not stated) (not stated) (reference (21% drop-off (mean (stated)
generated characteristics stated) reported) standardized
sequence) presented) changes &
confidence

L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75


intervals)
Coelho et al. No No Yes Yes Unclear No Unclear Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 8
(2012) (assigned) (baseline (not stated) (not stated) (reference (number (mean & SD) (completer
characteristics stated) presented) comparison)
presented
Fabre et al. No No Yes Yes Unclear Unclear No Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 8
(2002) (randomly (baseline (not stated for (not stated) (reference (stated) (mean & (explicitly reported
assigned) characteristics outcome stated) standard error) same number of
presented) measures) subjects at pre and
post)
Kounti et al. No No Yes Yes Yes No No Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 9
(2011) (assigned) (completer baseline (stated) (data & (completer (mean & SD) (completer
comparison) reference comparison) comparison)
stated)
Legault et al. Unclear Unclear Yes Yes Unclear Unclear Unclear Unclear Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 7
(2011) (method of random (not stated) (baseline (not stated) (not stated) (not stated) (stated (data (mean & SD; (stated)
sequence characteristics reference reported) mean change &
generation was not presented) without SE)
stated) information)
Oswald et al. No No Yes Yes No Unclear Unclear Unclear Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 7
(2006) (stated) (stated) (baseline (stated) (not stated) (not stated) (not all stated) (completer (z-score mean & (stated)
characteristics comparison at SD; effect size d)
presented & with post-
adjustment in intervention)
analysis)
Schwenk et al. Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes No 11
(2010) (numbered (by independent (stated baseline and (stated) (stated) (reference (data (mean & SD) (completer
container) person) completer baseline stated) reported) analysis)
comparison)
Suzuki et al. Unclear Unclear Yes Yes Yes No No Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 9
(2012) (method of random (not stated) (baseline (stated) (reference (data (group mean (stated)
sequence characteristics stated) reported) difference &
generation was not presented) confidence
stated) intervals)

69
70 L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75

Overall, the results of the reviewed studies in both populations or exercise training) in the comparison groups as well as the non-
with and without cognitive impairment were conflicting. Among specific components (staff exposure or social contact) in the control
the three studies with cognitively healthy participants, one study group. This design can greatly strengthen the validity and credi-
found no effects while two studies revealed significant effects. bility of the findings in the studies. Nevertheless, the exposure to
Fabre et al. (2002) reported a significant effect on memory perfor- interventions varied between the combined and single interven-
mance (d = 1.29). Oswald et al. (2006) found significant effects, with tions.
within-group effect sizes reported, on general cognitive functions In a population with cognitive complaint, Barnes et al. (2013)
(d+ = 1.14), subjective rating of cognitive impairment (d+ = 0.59) also used a multi-arm comparison group design and the inter-
and functional status (d+ = 0.27), which were sustained at 5-years vention time were the same among all the intervention and
follow-up. comparison groups. However, no significant differences were
Among the five studies with cognitively impaired participants, observed between the intervention and the comparison groups.
one study found no effect while four studies reported significant The authors believed that both the active-control activities and the
improvements in outcomes including cognitive-motor dual-task placebo-control activities may be beneficial to the participants and
cost (d = 0.99) in dementia; executive functions (d = 0.52–1.18) and have potential impact on the outcomes. Therefore, a larger sam-
attention (d = 0.24–1.57) in MCI and AD; general cognitive func- ple size would be required to have adequate power for detecting
tions (d = 0.11–0.63), language (d = 0.22–0.62), memory (d = 0.16) significant group differences (Baskin et al., 2003; Hart et al., 2008).
and subjective rating of functional status (d = 0.59) in MCI. All the other four studies with cognitively impaired popula-
tions that used a dual-task exercise group compared with a control
group reported significant findings. It can be challenged that the
4. Discussion
improvements observed in the studies may not be attributed to
the interventions or, more precisely, the specific component of
This systematic review examined the efficacy of combined cog-
the intervention. The use of a no-treatment group by Coelho et al.
nitive and exercise training to improve cognitive functions in older
(2012) may control for the effects of time and the effects of repeated
adults with and without cognitive impairment. Eight studies were
testing on outcome measures. The use of a waitlist group by Kounti
identified in this review, five of which included a population with
et al. (2011) may also control for the effects from an expectancy of
cognitive impairment and three studies included a population
improvement. The use of health education by Suzuki et al. (2012)
without cognitive impairment. The paucity of research in this area
may further control for other non-specific treatment effects (e.g.
was obvious, particularly among the wide progressively degenerat-
social contact) but the three sessions’ contact time was far less than
ing cognitive continuum in populations with cognitive impairment
the 120 h intervention time, which may lead to ineffective control
(Petersen, 2004). The studies were published within the past 10
of the non-specific moderators by the control group (Hart et al.,
years (2002–2013). This suggests that research to assess the impact
2008; Safer and Hugo, 2006). Schwenk et al. (2010) compared the
of combined cognitive and physical exercise on cognitive functions
intervention group with a low intensity exercise group. This may
in older adults is still in its fledgling stage. The identified studies
allow for a further comparison on the effects from the difference
were from a variety of mainly developed countries. The potential
in exercise intensity. Studies have found that moderate intensity
benefits of a combined intervention appears to be attracting the
aerobic exercise is more effective for improving cognitive function
interest of researchers throughout the world however more stud-
than low intensity exercise (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003), whereas
ies are needed before generalization of reliable evidence can be
combined aerobic and strength training has a greater effect com-
reached, especially in developing countries.
pared to single mode exercise training (Erickson and Kramer, 2009).
The results of this review showed that combined cognitive
A good comparison control should omit the unique intervention
and exercise training can be effective for improving the cognitive
component under investigation, while possessing the common
functions and functional status of older adults with and without
ingredients in equal measure (Safer and Hugo, 2006). Although pos-
cognitive impairment. Significant improvements were found in two
itive findings were reported in all the four studies used dual-task
out of the three studies with cognitively healthy populations and
training as intervention, it can still be questionable whether the
in four out the five studies with cognitively impaired populations.
improvements were all attributed to the dual-task components. An
The limited number and heterogeneity of the reviewed studies do
appropriate comparison with the potential beneficial components
not enable any firm conclusion to be drawn. Nevertheless, specific
or moderators is important to validate the differential effects under
study characteristics can be identified in this review that may help
the investigation. As a result, appropriate activity selection in com-
to promote intervention fidelity in future studies.
parison control and a well-designed comparison group is crucial in
future studies on the efficacy of combined interventions.
4.1. Comparison and control group design
4.2. Study population screening
Appropriate design of the comparison and control groups is
important to allow critical investigation on the comparative effec- Cognitive impairment/dementia may be caused by multiple
tiveness between the specific components of an intervention and intertwined pathological factors (Stewart, 2002). Neurodegen-
other components or common conditions being studied (Hart et al., erative conditions such as AD and cerebrovascular disease,
2008; Kinser and Robins, 2013). including subclinical brain injury, silent brain infarction, and clin-
In cognitively healthy populations, Fabre et al. (2002) found ically overt stroke, are the most common causes of cognitive
the combined training demonstrated significant improvement in impairment/dementias in older individuals (Alzheimer’s Disease
memory compared to either single memory or aerobic training International 2009; Gorelick et al., 2011). Vascular dementia (VaD)
alone. Oswald et al. (2006) showed the combined cognitive and is the second most common form of dementia after AD, accounting
physical training improved general cognitive performance and sub- for 20–30% of the diseased population (Stewart, 2002). Importantly,
jective measures of functional status compared to a no treatment there is increasing evidence that vascular pathology frequently
control, and proved more promising than the single training groups. co-exists with neurodegenerative pathology and most demen-
The multi-arm comparison group design in these studies allow tias have both vascular and neurodegenerative features (Dichgans
a clear comparison between the unique component in the com- and Zietemann, 2012; Neuropathology Group, 2001). More than
bined intervention and the common components (single cognitive 30% of AD population has been found to have cerebrovascular

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75 71

pathology (Roman, 1999). The pathogenic importance of vascular Praag et al., 2005). Neuroimaging studies in populations with cog-
contributions in cognitive impairment and dementia merits partic- nitive impairment reported that compensatory recruitment of new
ular attention in diagnosis and screening for study populations. brain areas were observed during the performance of a demanding
Among the reviewed studies in populations with cognitive task (Reuter-Lorenz and Lustig, 2005; Belleville et al., 2011). Fabel
impairment, Suzuki et al. (2012) used the criteria by Petersen et al. (2009) proposed that the exercise in combined training pre-
et al. (2001) for diagnosis of aMCI population. Kounti et al. (2011) pares the potential of the brain for increased neurogenesis upon
used the criteria of Petersen et al. (2001) and Artero et al. (2006) additional exposure to cognitive enrichment. In a recent animal
for multi-domain MCI and the National Institute of Neurologi- study on the effects of combined training with positive findings,
cal and Communicative Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer’s Disease the physical activity was set to precede the cognitive training or
and Related Disorder Association (NINCDS-ADRDA) for dementia the participants were re-exposed to cognitive training after physi-
(McKanhnn et al., 1984). Both of these studies performed neu- cal training (Langdon and Corbett, 2012). It appears more favorable
roimaging examination, in addition to neuropsychological tests and to have the exercise sessions delivered before the cognitive training
medical history, to confirm a diagnosis and reported exclusion of session, in order to better prepare the brain for the compensatory
persons with stroke. No information was reported on the patho- recruitment process in the subsequent cognitive training sessions.
logical nature of cognitive impairment in the population selected. Nevertheless, more intervention studies are needed to systemati-
Coelho et al. (2012) did not report on the diagnostic details for the cally reveal the potential impacts of training sequence on training
population selection, but used the Diagnostic and Statistical Man- outcomes.
ual of Mental Disorders 4th edition (DSM-IV) criteria (American
Psychiaric Association, 2000) for diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease, 4.4. Dual-task component
which also excluded those with cognitive deficits due to cere-
brovascular disease. Barnes et al. (2013) also excluded those with The high quality RCT by Schwenk et al. (2010) found significant
other neurological conditions in their study with a cognitive com- improvement in dual-task performance only under complex dual
plaint populations. The diagnosis was based on interview, physician task conditions. The patients needed to be adequately challenged
diagnosis and neuropsychological assessment. Indeed, cerebrovas- to reach the effects. It was reported that higher basic DTC were
cular pathologies, such as stroke, have been found to be typically associated with a higher percentage of improvement in DTC. This
associated with VaD (De la Torre, 2002). Studies have shown that highlights the importance of adjusting the level of the training tasks
cerebral infarcts (covert or overt) are additive with Alzheimer’s dis- to meet individual capacity in intervention design, thus making
ease pathology in that cerebral ischemia worsens the effects of AD optimal use of an individual’s latent potential (Hertzog et al., 2009).
on cognitions (Esiri et al., 1999; Zekry et al., 2002). Although the findings from using dual-task intervention are
Only one study (Schwenk et al., 2010) considered the vascular encouraging, the results need to be interpreted with caution. It has
nature of dementia by including the criteria for the diagnosis of been challenged that the similarity of the dual-tasks assigned in
dementia and VaD, based on both the NINCDS-ADRDA (McKanhnn the interventions and the assessment tasks may produce a learn-
et al., 1984) and the National Institute of Neurological Disor- ing effect ultimately affecting the reliability of the outcome results
ders and Stroke-Association International pour la Recherche en (Pichierri et al., 2011). Therefore, selection of alternative dual-tasks
l’ Enseignement en Neurosciences (NINCDS-AIREN; Roman et al., should be considered in future studies using dual-task exercise.
1993). However, the authors did not report on any neuropatho-
logy evaluations or methods for operationalization of the screening 4.5. Intervention period
criteria. Silent infarct also has been found to influence cognition
even in normal aging (Vermeer et al., 2003). Nevertheless, in the Suzuki et al. (2012) reported significant group effects on gen-
reviewed studies with cognitively healthy population, no studies eral cognitive function and memory at 6 months which were
addressed or investigated the potential impact of silent vascular not demonstrated in the MCI population at 12 months treatment
contribution to cognition in normal persons. end. The intervention period in the study of Suzuki et al. (2012)
Studies have shown that the presence of vascular lesions con- was the longest compared to that in other studies (3–5 months)
tributes to the severity of cognitive impairment (Snowden et al., with cognitively impaired participants. A previous meta-analysis
1997; Riekse et al., 2004; Petrovitch et al., 2005). Different sub- has found negative association between the training duration and
groups may have different cognitive deficits and responses to the cognitive intervention effect on persons with MCI and sug-
treatments associated with the differences in etiologies (Gorelick gested that studies with longer sessions and longer duration of
et al., 2011; Moorhouse and Rockwood, 2008). Identifying the total sessions would produce smaller effect sizes (Li et al., 2011).
causes of cognitive impairment/dementia is of particular impor- As revealed by the study results in this review, an intervention
tance in the investigation for effective preventative and treatment period of three to six months has shown to be more favorable
strategies (Stephen et al., 2009). Clear identification and report on than a longer intervention period in populations with cognitive
the specific nature of the study population selected (neurodegen- impairment. Nevertheless, the moderating effect of program dura-
erative, vascular, mixed pathology) will be crucial to facilitate cross tion for cognitively impaired populations may need to be examined
study comparison and generalization of study findings. in future studies.

4.3. Training sequence in combined intervention 4.6. Outcome measure selection

The sequence in delivering the cognitive training and exer- The studies by Suzuki et al. (2012) in amnestic MCI and Barnes
cise training sessions on combined training may have a potential et al. (2013) in participants with memory complaints did not find
impact on the intervention outcomes. Legault et al. (2011) found any effects on executive functions. In contrast, the studies by Coelho
no intervention effects when the physical training was delivered et al. (2012) in AD participants and Kounti et al. (2011) in MCI par-
after the cognitive training. Oswald et al. (2006) however found ticipants found significant improvements in executive functions
positive intervention effects with the physical training sessions and attention. Impairments in executive functions have been found
delivered before the cognitive training for half of the intervention in people with cognitive complaint (Rouch et al., 2008), amnestic
period. Animal studies found that exercise promotes neurogene- and non-amnestic MCI (Reinvang et al., 2012) and AD (McGuinness
sis in the brain and improves learning (Farmer et al., 2004; van et al., 2010). Executive function deficits have consistently been

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
72 L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75

found to predict AD conversion (Ritchie et al., 2001; Tabert et al., context where the demands may be different to that in a test
2006) although it remains unclear how executive functions are environment (Law et al., 2012; Marsiske and Willis, 1995). More
impaired in amnestic MCI (Bisiacchi et al., 2008; Marshall et al., ecologically validated tests should be included to examine the prac-
2011). Therefore, it is important to include executive functions tical translation of intervention effects.
as one of the intervention outcomes in studies with cognitively The ability to maintain independent living in the community
impaired populations. Nevertheless, the changes in executive func- is of particular importance for most elderly adults (Mack et al.,
tions could be so subtle that some of the widely used executive 1997). Studies have shown that persons with cognitive impairment
function tests may not be sensitive enough to detect the mini- have difficulties in daily functioning, especially in complex every-
mal changes (Espinosa et al., 2009; Pickens et al., 2010). Moreover, day tasks that rely heavily on memory and reasoning (Aretouli
the diversity of components under the umbrella of executive func- and Brandt, 2010; Pérés et al., 2006). This imposes a potential
tions (Miyake et al., 2000) may further complicate the difficulty in impact on the safety and quality of life of the person with cogni-
assessing executive functions. The approach to use multiple out- tive impairment (Gauthier et al., 2006; World Health Organization
come measures, as that in the study by Kounti et al. (2011), may be and Alzheimer’s Disease International, 2012). It will be of utmost
helpful but care must be taken to balance the assessment burden importance for future intervention studies to include ecological
on the participants (Pickens et al., 2010). The selection of outcome tests and investigate the generalizability of training effects to real
measures with appropriate sensitivity and specificity to the study world functional outcomes.
population should be of primary concern in future studies (Karrasch Overall, more well-designed studies, with special attention to
et al., 2005; Parra et al., 2012). comparison control and population screening, outcome selection,
training sequence, intervention period and generalizability of out-
4.7. Visual-spatial component comes, are still needed to reveal the differential effects of combined
cognitive and exercise training especially in older population with
Kounti et al. (2011) was the only study that used dual tasks with cognitive impairments. In the absence of a cure for cognitive
visual-spatial stimuli. Studies have found that both MCI and AD impairment or dementia, further research efforts are needed to
patients show deficits in visual spatial function (Iachini et al., 2009; explore the potential benefits of this new intervention paradigm
Possin, 2010) which is also one of the predictors of conversion to to help delay, and possibly reverse the progression of cognitive
dementia of persons with MCI (Amieva et al., 2004; Griffith et al., impairment.
2006). Indeed, visual spatial function is a very important cognition
in everyday functioning, for instance, navigating a car safely in driv- 4.9. Limitations
ing or searching the routes in a new environment. Subtle declines
in this ability would impose a great impact on everyday livings in Limitations of this review should be noted. The limited num-
elderly populations (Alescio-Lautier et al., 2007; Iachini et al., 2009; ber and heterogeneity of the reviewed studies did not allow critical
Possin, 2010). Studies have found that visual spatial ability can be analysis of the results for different groups of populations with cog-
trained through practice (Jaeggi et al., 2008; Wright et al., 2008). nitive impairment. Further, the search terms were limited to title,
Therefore, including visual spatial components in interventions as abstract and keywords to constrain the magnitude of the search
well as in outcome measures are highly recommended in future yield to a manageable size and only citations published in English
studies. were included in this review. These restrictions may limit cover-
age of all potential studies under this topic. Moreover, initial study
4.8. Generalization of training effects screening with title and abstract may lead to further limited cover
of possible intervention identification although full text would be
Training effects can reflect real benefits and practical values of drawn for examination whenever a decision could not be judged
an intervention only if the results of the intervention can be gen- by screening with title and abstract.
eralized to other non-trained tasks, settings and sustained over
time (Boelen et al., 2011; Glasgow et al., 2003). Previous cogni-
tive training studies have shown improvements in training-related 5. Conclusion
outcomes but limited generalization to non-trained tasks (Noack
et al., 2009; Simon et al., 2012). The generalizability of the inter- Results of the present review showed that studies with
vention results to everyday functioning is regarded as an important cognitively healthy populations revealed significant benefits of
implication of success (Glasgow et al., 2003; Lambert et al., 2010). combined cognitive and exercise interventions on general cogni-
In the reviewed studies with a cognitively healthy populations, tive functions, memory and functional status compared to active
Legault et al. (2011) assessed the impact of combined training control groups. Studies with cognitively impaired populations also
on non-trained cognitive domain (executive function) but found showed significant improvements in general cognitive functions,
no significant effects. Oswald et al. (2006) reported positive gen- memory, executive functions, attention and functional status in
eralization of the intervention effects in functional status and persons with MCI and AD or dementia, but lack comparison with
emotional status, which was also sustained over time at 5-year active control groups.
follow up. However, the assessment tools involved were non- In conclusion, combined cognitive and exercise training can
standardized subjective ratings. be effective for improving the cognitive functions and functional
In populations with cognitive impairment, all of the five status of older adults with and without cognitive impairment. How-
reviewed studies used neuropsychological assessments to assess ever, limited evidence can be found in populations with cognitive
training-related cognitive domains including attention, language impairment when the evaluation includes an active control group
and executive functions. Only Kounti et al. (2011) also included comparison. More well-designed studies are required before one
the Functional Rating Scale of Symptoms of Dementia (FRSSD) can draw any firm conclusion on the efficacy of the combined cog-
and the Functional Cognitive Assessment Scale (FUCAS) to assess nitive and exercise intervention in older adults.
functional status, and found positive post-intervention effects for
FRSSD, which is an informant-report rating. Nevertheless, tradi- Conflicts of interest
tional psychometric tests have been challenged for not being able
to adequately reflect older adults’ functioning in a real everyday We declare that there are no conflicts of interest.

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75 73

Source of funding De la Torre, J.C., 2002. Alzheimer disease as a vascular disorder. Stroke 33,
1152–1162, http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.STR.0000014421.15948.67.
Dichgans, M., Zietemann, V., 2012. Prevention of vascular cognitive impairment.
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency Stroke 43, 3137–3146, http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/STROKEAHA.112.651778.
in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. Doi, T., et al., 2013. Effects of multicomponent exercise on spatial–temporal gait
parameters among the elderly with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI):
preliminary results from a randomized controlled trial (RCT). Arch. Gerontol.
Geriatr. 56 (1), 104–108, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2012.09.003.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Erickson, K.I., Kramer, A.F., 2009. Aerobic exercise effects on cognitive and
neural plasticity in older adults. Br. J. Sports Med. 43 (1), 22–24,
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2008.052498.
the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2014.02.008. Esiri, M.M., et al., 1999. Cerebrovascular disease and threshold for dementia in the
early stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 354, 919–920.
Espinosa, A., et al., 2009. Ecological assessment of executive functions in mild cogni-
tive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15 (5),
References 751–757, http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S135561770999035X.
Etnier, J.L., et al., 2006. A meta-regression to examine the relationship between
Adlard, P.A., Perreau, V.M., Cotman, C.W., 2005. The exercise-induced expression of aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Res. Rev. 52 (1), 119–130.
BDNF within the hippocampus varies across life-span. Neurobiol. Aging 26 (4), Fabel, K., et al., 2003. VEGF is necessary for exercise-induced adult hippocampal
511–520. neurogenesis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18 (10), 2803–2812.
Alescio-Lautier, B., et al., 2007. Visual and visuospatial short-term memory in mild Fabel, K., et al., 2009. Additive effects of physical exercise and environmental enrich-
cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease: role of attention. Neuropsycholo- ment on adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice. Front. Neurosci. 3 (50),
gia 45 (8), 1948–1960. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/neuro.22.002.2009.
Alzheimer’s Association, 2013. Mild Cognitive Impairment. Alzheimer’s Association, Fabel, K., Kempermann, G., 2008. Physical activity and the regulation of neuro-
Retrieved from http://www.alz.org/dementia/mild-cognitive-impairment-mci. genesis in the adult and aging brain. Neuromolecular Med. 10 (2), 59–66,
asp (accessed 05.05.13). http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12017-008-8031-4.
Alzheimer’s International, 2009. Alzheimer’s Disease International. Fabre, C., et al., 1999. Evaluation of quality of life in elderly healthy subjects after
World Alzheimer’s Report 2009, http://www.alz.co.uk/research/files/ aerobic and/or mental training. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 28 (1), 9–22.
WorldAlzheimerReport.pdf (accessed 25.09.13). Fabre, C., et al., 2002. Improvement of cognitive function by mental and/or indi-
American Psychiatric Association, 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental vidualized aerobic training in healthy elderly subjects. Int. J. Sports Med. 23,
Disorders. APA, Washington, DC. 415–421.
Amieva, H., et al., 2004. Annual rate and predictors of conversion to dementia in Farmer, J., et al., 2004. Effects of voluntary exercise on synaptic plasticity and gene
subjects presenting Mild Cognitive Impairment criteria defined according to a expression in the dentate gyrus of adult male Sprague-Dawley rats in vivo.
population-based study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 18, 87–93. Neuroscience 124 (1), 71–79.
Amoyal, N., Fallon, E., 2012. Physical exercise and cognitive training clinical interven- Gates, N.J., et al., 2011. Study of Mental Activity and Regular Training (SMART) in at
tions used in slowing degeneration associated with mild cognitive impairment: risk individuals: a randomised double blind, sham controlled, longitudinal trial.
a review of the recent literature. Top. Geriatr. Rehabil. 28 (3), 208–216. BMC Geriatrics 11, 19, http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2318-11-19.
Aretouli, E., Brandt, J., 2010. Everyday functioning in mild cognitive impairment Gauthier, et al., 2006. Mild Cognitive Impairment. Lancet 367 (9518), 1262–1270,
and its relationship with executive cognition. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 25 (3), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68542-5.
224–233, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/gps.2325. Gillette-Guyonnet, S., et al., 2009. Commentary on A roadmap for the preven-
Artero, S., et al., 2006. Revised criteria for mild cognitive impairment: validation tion of dementia II. Leon Thal Symposium 2008. The Multidomain Alzheimer
within a longitudinal population study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 22 (5–6), Preventive Trial (MAPT): a new approach to the prevention of Alzheimer’s
465–470. disease. Alzheimers Dement. 5 (2), 114–121, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.
Barnes, D.E., et al., 2013. The Mental Activity and eXercise (MAX) Trial: a randomized 2009.01.008.
controlled trial to enhance cognitive function in older adults. JAMA Internal Med. Glasgow, R.E., Lichtenstein, E., Marcus, A.C., 2003. Why don’t we see more
173 (9), 797–804, http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.189. translation of health promotion research to practice? Rethinking the efficacy-
Baskin, T.W., et al., 2003. Establishing specificity in psychotherapy: a meta-analysis to-effectiveness transition. Am. J. Public Health 93 (8), 1261–1267.
of structural equivalence of placebo controls. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 71 (6), Gorelick, P.B., et al., 2011. Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment
973–979. and dementia: a statement for healthcare professionals from the American
Belleville, S., et al., 2011. Training-related brain plasticity in subjects at risk of Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke 42 (9), 2672–2713,
developing Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 134 (6), 1623–1634, http://dx.doi.org/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/STR.0b013e3182299496.
10.1093/brain/awr037. Griffith, H.R., et al., 2006. Amnestic mild cognitive impairment: diagnostic outcomes
Bisiacchi, P.S., et al., 2008. Interplay between memory and executive functions in and clinical prediction over a two-year time period. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 12
normal and pathological aging. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 30 (6), 723–733, (2), 166–175.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803390701689587. Hart, T., Fann, J.R., Novack, T.A., 2008. The dilemma of the control condition in
Boelen, D.H., Spikman, J.M., Fasotti, L., 2011. Rehabilitation of executive disorders experience-based cognitive and behavioural treatment research. Neuropsychol.
after brain injury: are interventions effective? J. Neuropsychol. 5 (Pt. 1), 73–113, Rehabil. Int. J. 18 (1), 1–21, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602010601082359.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/174866410X516434. Hertzog, C., et al., 2009. Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development. Psychol.
Burgener, S.C., et al., 2008. The effects of a multimodal intervention on outcomes of Sci. Public Interest 9 (1), 1–65.
persons with early-stage dementia. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 23 (4), Heyn, P., 2003. The effect of a multisensory exercise program on engagement,
382–394, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1533317508317527. behavior, and selected physiological indexes in persons with dementia. Am. J.
Busse, A.L., et al., 2009. Physical activity and cognition in the elderly a review. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement. 18 (4), 247–251.
Dement. Neuropsychol. 3 (3), 204–208. Higgins, J.P.T., Green, S., 2011. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of
Casperson, C.J., Powell, K.E., Christensen, G.M., 1985. Physical activity, exercise, and Interventions. Version 5.1.0. The Cochrane Collaboration, Retrieved from
physical fitness: definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public www.cochrane-handbook.org
Health Rep. 100 (2), 126–131. Iachini, I., et al., 2009. Visuospatial memory in healthy elderly, AD and MCI: a review.
Clare, L., Woods, B., 2003. Cognitive rehabilitation and cognitive training for early- Curr. Aging Sci. 2 (1), 43–59.
stage Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. CDS Rev. 4, CD003260. Jaeggi, S.M., et al., 2008. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working mem-
Coelho, F.G., et al., 2012. Multimodal exercise intervention improves frontal cogni- ory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 6829–6833.
tive functions and gait in Alzheimer’s disease: a controlled trial. Geriatr. Geron- Karrasch, M., et al., 2005. CERAD test performances in amnestic mild cog-
tol. Int. 13 (1), 198–203, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1447-0594.2012.00887.x. nitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol. Scand. 111 (3),
Cohen, J., 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioural Sciences, 2nd ed. 172–179.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. Kinser, P., Robins, J., 2013. Control group design: enhancing rigor in research on
Colcombe, S., Kramer, A.F., 2003. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older mind-body therapies for depression. Evidence-based Complementary and Alter-
adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 14 (2), 125–130, http://dx.doi.org/ native Medicine (Article ID 140467) http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/140467
10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430. Komai, K., Yamada, M., 2012. Effect of cognitive and aerobic training interven-
Colcombe, S.J., et al., 2006. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging tion on older adults with mild or no cognitive impairment: a derivative
humans. J. Gerontol. A: Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 61 (11), 1166–1170. study of the nakajima project. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn Dis. Extra 2 (1), 69–80,
Cotman, C.W., Berchtod, N.C., Christie, L.A., 2007. Exercise builds brain health: key http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000337224.
roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends Neurosci. 30 (9), Kounti, F., et al., 2011. RHEA: a nonpharmacological cognitive training intervention
464–472. in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a pilot study. Top. Geriatr. Rehabil.
Cotman, C.W., Berchtold, N.C., 2002. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance 27 (4), 289–300.
brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 25 (6), 295–301. Kraft, E., 2012. Cognitive function, physical activity, and aging: possible biological
Cotman, C.W., Berchtold, N.C., 2007. Physical activity and the maintenance of cogni- links and implications for multimodal interventions. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn.
tion: Learning from animal models. Alzheimers Dement. 3 (2 Suppl.), S30–S37, B: Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 19 (1–2), 248–263, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2007.01.013. 13825585.2011.645010.

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
74 L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75

Kramer, A.F., Erickson, K.I., 2007. Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of phys- Pichierri, G., et al., 2011. Cognitive and cognitive-motor interventions affect-
ical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11 (8), 342–348. ing physical functioning: a systematic review. BMC Geriatrics 11, 29,
Kronenberg, G., et al., 2006. Physical exercise prevents age-related decline in pre- http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2318-11-29.
cursor cell activity in the mouse dentate gyrus. Neurobiol. Aging 27 (10), Pickens, S., et al., 2010. Systematic review of current executive function measures
1505–1513. in adults with and without cognitive impairments. Int. J. Evid. Based Healthcare
Lambert, K.F., Evans, J.J., Spikman, J.M., 2010. A real-life, ecologically valid test of 8 (3), 110–125, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-1609.2010.00170.x.
executive functioning: the executive secretarial task. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. Possin, K.L., 2010. Visual spatial cognition in neurodegenerative disease. Neurocase
32 (1), 56–65, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803390902806550. 16 (6), 466–487.
Langdon, K.D., Corbett, D., 2012. Improved working memory following novel com- Rebok, G.W., 2008. Mental Capital and Wellbeing Project. State-of-Science
binations of physical and cognitive activity. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 26 (5), Review: SR-E22 Cognitive Training: Influence on Neuropsychological and
523–532, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1545968311425919. Brain Function in Later Life, Retrieved from http://www.bis.gov.uk/assets/
Law, L.L., et al., 2012. Measures of everyday competence in older adults foresight/docs/mental-capital/sr-e22 mcw.pdf
with cognitive impairment: a systematic review. Age Ageing 41, 9–16, Reinvang, I., Grambaite, R., Espeseth, T., 2012. Executive dysfunction in
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afr104. MCI: subtype or early symptom. Int. J. Alzheimers Dis., http://dx.doi.org/
Legault, C., et al., 2011. Designing clinical trials for assessing the effects of cognitive 10.1155/2012/936272 (Article ID: 936272, 8 pp.).
training and physical activity interventions on cognitive outcomes: The Seniors Reuter-Lorenz, P.A., Lustig, C., 2005. Brain aging: reorganizing discoveries about the
Health and Activity Research Program Pilot (SHARP-P) Study, a randomized con- aging mind. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 15 (2), 245–251.
trolled trial. BMC Geriatrics 11, 27, http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2318-11-27. Riekse, et al., 2004. Effects of vascular lesions on cognition in AD: a community-based
Li, H., et al., 2011. Cognitive intervention for persons with mild cognitive impair- study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 52, 1442–1448.
ment: a meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 10 (2), 285–296. Ritchie, K., Artero, S., Touchon, J., 2001. Classification criteria for mild cognitive
Liorens-Martin, M., Torres-Alemán, l., Trejo, J.L., 2010. Exercise modulates impairment: a population-based validation study. Neurology 56 (1), 37–42.
insulin-like growth factor 1-dependent and -independent effects on adult hip- Roman, G.C., 1999. A historical review of the concept of vascular dementia: lessons
pocampal neurogenesis and behavior. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 44 (2), 109–117, from the past for the future. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 13 (Suppl. 3), S4–S8.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mcn.2010.02.006. Roman, G.C., et al., 1993. Vascular dementia: diagnostic criteria for research
Mack, R., et al., 1997. Perceived risks to independent living: the views of older, studies: report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 43,
Community-Dwelling adults. Gerontologist 37 (6), 729–736. 250–260.
Maher, C., Sherrington, C., Herbert, R., Moseley, A., Elkins, M., 2003. Reliability of the Rouch, I., et al., 2008. Cognitive complaints, neuropsychological performance and
PEDro scale for rating quality of randomized controlled trials. Phys. Ther. 3 (83), affective disorders in elderly community residents. Disabil. Rehabil. 30 (23),
713–721. 794–802, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09638280701667825.
Marshall, G.A., et al., 2011. Executive function and instrumental activities of daily liv- Safer, D.L., Hugo, E.M., 2006. Designing a control for a behavioral group therapy.
ing in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. Behav. Ther. 37 (2), 120–130.
7 (3), 300–308, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2010.04.005. Santin, K., et al., 2011. Moderate exercise training and chronic caloric restric-
Marsiske, M., Willis, S.L., 1995. Dimensionality of everyday problem solving in older tion modulate redox status in rat hippocampus. Brain Res. 1421, 1–10,
adults. Psychol. Aging 10 (2), 269–283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.08.003.
Martin, M., et al., 2006. Cognition-based interventions for older people and people Saykin, A.J., et al., 2006. Older adults with cognitive complaints show brain atrophy
with mild cognitive impairment. CDS Rev. 4, 006220. similar to that of amnestic MCI. Neurology 67 (5), 834–842.
McGuinness, B., et al., 2010. Executive functioning in Alzheimer’s dis- Schaefer, S., Schumacher, V., 2011. The interplay between cognitive and motor
ease and vascular dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 25 (6), 562–568, functioning in healthy older adults: findings from dual-task studies and
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/gps.2375. suggestions for intervention. Gerontology 57, 239–246, http://dx.doi.org/
McKanhnn, G., et al., 1984. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the 10.1159/000322197.
NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Schwenk, M., et al., 2010. Dual-task performances can be improved in patients with
Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 34, 939–944. dementia: a randomized controlled trial. Neurology 74, 1961–1968.
Miyake, A., et al., 2000. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their Shatil, E., 2013. Does combined cognitive training and physical activity training
contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit. enhance cognitive abilities more than either alone? A four-condition random-
Psychol. 41 (1), 49–100. ized controlled trial among healthy older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5, 8,
Moorhouse, P., Rockwood, K., 2008. Vascular cognitive impairment: cur- http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2013.00008.
rent concepts and clinical developments. Lancet Neurol. 7 (3), 246–255, Simon, S.S., Yokomizo, J.E., Bottino, C.M., 2012. Cognitive intervention in amnestic
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70040-1. Mild Cognitive Impairment: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36
Morris, J.C., et al., 2001. Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer (4), 1163–1178, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.01.007.
disease. Arch. Neurol. 58 (3), 397–405. Snowden, D.A., et al., 1997. Brain infarction and the clinical expression of AD: the
Neuropathology Group, 2001. Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and nun study. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 277, 813–817.
Aging Study: Pathological correlate of late-onset dementia in a multicenter, Snowden, M., et al., 2011. Effect of exercise on cognitive performance in community-
community-based population in England and Wales. Lancet 357, 169–175. dwelling older adults: review of intervention trials and recommendations
Noack, H., et al., 2009. Cognitive plasticity in adulthood and old age: gauging the for public health practice and research. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 59 (4), 704–716,
generality of cognitive intervention effects. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 27 (5), http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03323.x.
435–453, http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/RNN-2009-0496. Stephen, B.C.M., et al., 2009. Beyond mild cognitive impairment: vascular cog-
O’Dwyer, S.T., et al., 2007. Protocol for Fit Bodies, Fine Minds: a randomized nitive impairment, no dementia (VCIND). Alzheimers Res. Ther. 1 (1), 4,
controlled trial on the affect of exercise and cognitive training on cognitive http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/alzrt4.
functioning in older adults. BMC Geriatrics 7, 23, http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/ Stewart, R., 2002. Vascular dementia: a diagnosis running out of time. Br. J. Psychiatry
1471-2318-7-23. 180, 152–156, http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.180.2.152.
Olson, A.K., et al., 2006. Environmental enrichment and voluntary exercise mas- Suzuki, T., et al., 2012. Effects of multicomponent exercise on cognitive function in
sively increase neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus via dissociable pathways. older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled
Hippocampus 16 (3), 250–260. trial. BMC Neurol. 12, 128.
Onor, M.L., et al., 2007. Impact of a multimodal rehabilitative intervention on Tabert, M.H., et al., 2006. Neuropsychological prediction of conversion to Alzheimer
demented patients and their caregivers. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement. disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63
22 (4), 261–272. (8), 916–924.
Oswald, W.D., et al., 2006. Differential effects of single versus combined cognitive Tang, K., et al., 2010. Exercise-induced VEGF transcriptional activation in brain, lung
and physical training with older adults: the SimA study in a 5-year perspective. and skeletal muscle. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 170 (1), 16–22.
Eur. J. Ageing 3, 179–192. Thalheimer, W., Cook, S., 2002. How to calculate effect sizes from published research:
Parra, M.A., et al., 2012. P300 and neuropsychological assessment in mild a simplified methodology. Work-Learning Research Publication, Retrieved from
cognitive impairment and Alzheimer dementia. Front. Neurol. 3, 172, http://education.gsu.edu/coshima/EPRS8530/Effect Sizes pdf4.pdf
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2012.00172. Thom, J.M., Clare, L., 2011. Bridging the gap between clinical and behavioural geron-
Pereira, A.C., et al., 2007. An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced neurogenesis in tology Part I: promoting late-life mobility and independence: rationale for
the adult dentate gyrus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104 (13), 5638–5643. combined exercise and cognition-focused interventions to improve functional
Pérés, K., et al., 2006. Restriction in complex activities of daily living in MCI: impact independence in people with dementia. Gerontology 57, 265–275.
on outcome. Neurology 67 (3), 461–466. van der Flier, W.M., et al., 2004. Memory complaints in patients with normal cog-
Pervaiz, N., Hoffman-Goetz, L., 2011. Freewheel training alters mouse hip- nition are associated with smaller hippocampal volumes. J. Neurol. 251 (6),
pocampal cytokines. Int. J. Sports Med. 32 (11), 889–895, http://dx.doi.org/ 671–675.
10.1055/s-0031-1279780. van Praag, H., 2009. Exercise and the brain: something to chew on. Trends Neurosci.
Petersen, R.C., 2004. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern. Med. 32 (5), 283–290.
256 (3), 183–194. van Praag, H., et al., 2005. Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis
Petersen, R.C., et al., 2001. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch. in aged mice. J. Neurosci. 25 (38), 8680–8685.
Neurol. 58 (12), 1985–1992. Vaynman, S., Ying, Z., Gomez-Pinilla, F., 2004. Hippocampal BDNF mediates the effi-
Petrovitch, et al., 2005. AD lesions and infarcts in demented and non-demented cacy of exercise on synaptic plasticity and cognition. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20 (10),
Japanese-American men. Ann. Neurol. 57 (1), 98–103. 2580–2590.

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L.L.F. Law et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 15 (2014) 61–75 75

Verhagen, A.P., et al., 1998. The Delphi list: a criteria list for assessment of ran- http://www.who.int/mental health/publications/dementia report 2012/en/
domised clinical trials for conducting systematic reviews developed by Delphi index.html (accessed 08.05.13).
consensus. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 51, 1235–1241. Wright, R., et al., 2008. Training generalized spatial skills. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 15 (4),
Verhagen, A.P., et al., 2007. Exercise proves effective in a systematic review of 763–771.
work-related complaints of the arm, neck, or shoulder. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 60 Zekry, D., et al., 2002. Degenerative and vascular lesions of the brain have synergistic
(2), 110–117. effects in dementia of the elderly. Acta Neuropathol. (Berl.) 103 (5), 481–487.
Vermeer, S.E., et al., 2003. Silent brain infarcts and the risk of dementia and cognitive Zhang, F., Wu, Y., Jia, J., 2011. Exercise preconditioning and brain ischemic tol-
decline. N. Engl. J. Med. 348 (13), 1215–1222. erance. Neuroscience 177, 170–176, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.
Viswanathan, M., et al., 2012. Assessing the Risk of Bias of Individual Studies 2011.01.018.
in Systematic Reviews of Health Care Interventions. Agency for Health- Zhang, P., et al., 2013. Early exercise improves cerebral blood flow through increased
care Research and Quality Methods Guide for Comparative Effectiveness angiogenesis in experimental stroke rat model. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 10, 43,
Reviews. March 2012. AHRQ Publication No. 12-EHC047-EF, Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-10-43.
www.effectivehealthcare.ahrq.gov
World Health Organization, Alzheimer’s Disease International, 2012. Demen-
tia: a public health priority. World Health Organization, Retrieved from

Downloaded for Mahasiswa FK UKDW 01 (mhsfkdw01@gmail.com) at Universitas Kristen Duta Wacana from ClinicalKey.com by Elsevier on February 27, 2018.
For personal use only. No other uses without permission. Copyright ©2018. Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Potrebbero piacerti anche