Sei sulla pagina 1di 18

The Effects of Extensive Reading on

Reading Comprehension, Reading


Rate, and Vocabulary Acquisition

Namhee Suk ABSTRACT


Pukyong National University, Several empirical studies and syntheses of extensive reading have con-cluded
that extensive reading has positive impacts on language learning in second- and
Busan, South Korea
foreign-language settings. However, many of the studies con-tained
methodological or curricular limitations, raising questions about the asserted
positive effects of extensive reading. The present research adds to the limited
set of well-controlled studies by investigating the impact of an extensive reading
approach over a 15-week semester on the reading compre-hension, reading rate,
and vocabulary acquisition of Korean university stu-dents studying English as a
foreign language. A quasi- experimental research design was employed using
four intact classes: two control (n = 88) and two experimental (n = 83). The
control classes received 100-minute intensive reading instruction per week,
whereas the experimental classes received equivalent 70-minute intensive
reading instruction and 30-minute extensive reading instruction per week. A
repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance revealed that the
experimental classes significantly outperformed the control classes on the
combination of the three dependent variables (i.e., reading comprehension,
reading rate, vocabulary acquisition). Subsequent analyses of variance confirmed
the positive impacts of extensive reading on all three areas. Instructional
implications for extensive reading in second-language curricula are discussed.

R eading ability is an important second-language (L2) skill in academic


settings, where L2 learners are required to read to learn and
complete related tasks (Anderson, 2015; Grabe, 2009;
Rosenfeld, Leung, & Oltman, 2001). Given the importance of
reading in academic contexts, a key issue is how L2 reading ability
can best be developed. Based on research to date, reading ability is
only likely to develop gradually when L2 learners are continually
exposed to abun-dant meaningful input, or extensive reading.
Extensive reading has been proposed as one of the most effective
methods for developing reading abilities because it can expose L2
learners to ample amounts of meaningful input, motivate L2 learners
to read, and lead to the de-velopment of skilled reading abilities (Day
& Bamford, 1998; Grabe, 2009; Krashen, 2004, 2011).
One context in which L2 reading is an important goal, but achieve-
ment is somewhat limited, is in the Korean context of English as a
foreign language (EFL). Students in Korean EFL reading classes are
usually taught to analyze short challenging texts and required to
memorize difficult vocabulary in the texts. This traditional intensive
Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1)
pp. 73–89 | doi:10.1002/rrq.152
reading instruction usually requires students to demonstrate their
© 2016 International Literacy Association. thorough understanding of texts by translating them line by line.

73
Although intensive reading with explicit instruction is words automatically, parsing syntactic phrases and
necessary to develop reading skills (e.g., Anderson, clauses, and making meaningful propositions for read-
1999; Paran, 2003), intensive reading-only instruction ing comprehension, which in turn help develop reading
limits exposure to the target language and leads to slow fluency (Grabe, 2009).
reading. In addition, it is likely that students build With respect to the role of implicit learning in L2
nega-tive attitudes toward target-language reading reading fluency development, extensive reading pro-
because of constant exposure to linguistically vides L2 learners with the appropriate conditions
challenging texts, which are not interesting to students. through consistent reading practice with extended
To cope with the problems that intensive reading-only texts. L2 learners can obtain the ability to read longer
instruction cre-ates in Korea and elsewhere, over the texts comfortably at a reasonable rate, a unique ability
last two decades, other L2 settings have turned to an that can be developed only through extensive reading.
extensive reading approach. Additionally, extensive reading provides an ideal set-
Studies of extensive reading have identified ting (i.e., consistent exposure to easy and multiple
positive impacts of extensive reading on L2 reading read-ing materials for long periods of time) for L2
abilities and vocabulary (e.g., Al-Homoud & Schmitt, learners to be constantly exposed to implicit learning
2009; Beglar, Hunt, & Kite, 2012; Horst, 2005; Mason processes. Through extensive reading, L2 learners can
& Krashen, 1997; Pigada & Schmitt, 2006; Webb & incidentally and incrementally reencounter language
Chang, 2015). However, positive results from these input over time and eventually become automatic and
and other studies on extensive reading seem to be fluent read-ers (Grabe, 2009; Nation, 2009).
mitigated due to meth-odological issues, which raise
some questions about the effectiveness of extensive
reading on L2 learning. Therefore, the present study
aims to further investigate the impact of extensive
Extensive Reading in L2
reading on L2 learning through a carefully controlled An understanding of the concept of extensive reading is
semester-long intervention study, incorporating an important because the way it is perceived can greatly
extensive reading component into an already existing af-fect how it is practiced. Grabe and Stoller (2011)
Korean EFL university reading curriculum. defined extensive reading as an approach “in which
learners read large quantities of material that are within
their linguistic competence” (p. 286). According to
Bamford and Day (2004), “extensive reading is an
Implicit Learning in L2 Reading approach to language teaching in which learners read a
The theoretical motivation behind L2 extensive reading lot of easy material in the new language” (p. 1). These
draws from implicit learning. Implicit learning, accord- two defini-tions share two important concepts: (a) In
ing to Kintsch (1998, as cited in Grabe, 2009), involves extensive reading, learners read large amounts of text;
the incremental growth of habitual associative knowl- and (b) in order for learners to read large amounts of
edge, the tacit learning of co-occurrence patterns in the text, reading materials should be within the learners’
input we receive. Implicit learning is “acquisition of reading- proficiency levels (a somewhat more
knowledge about the underlying structure of a complex challenging issue for L2 readers than L1 readers).
stimulus environment by a process which takes place Bamford and Day ex-panded the scope of their
naturally, simply, and without conscious operations” definition by elaborating on additional features of
(Ellis, 2008, p. 121). As defined, implicit learning takes extensive reading, such as self- selection of reading
place without awareness of what is learned from gradual materials, independent reading, reading for general
and continual multiple exposures to the input. Implicit meaning, and reading for informa-tion and enjoyment.
learning plays a central role in learning to read, contrib- Three important issues arise from these discus-
uting to the development of lower level processes in sions, which contribute to a more fine-tuned definition
reading and resulting in automatized processing and of extensive reading. One important feature of exten-
fluency (Grabe, 2009). Nation (2009) has also argued that sive reading, specifically, reading materials that are
L2 reading fluency develops by “making the best use of within learners’ linguistic ability, stresses that reading
what is already known” (p. 2); this argument in-vokes materials should be easy enough to (a) facilitate effort-
implicit learning as a way to strengthen and stabi-lize less comprehension without imposing any significant
lexical entries and lexical networks. To have an impact on learning burden and (b) keep learners on the task inde-
reading development, implicit learning re-quires pendently. Day and Bamford (1998) stressed that read-
considerable amounts of repeated input that de-velop ing within the reader’s linguistic competence can
habitual processing of that input. These conditions contribute to developing a sight vocabulary and thus
develop important L2 reading skills, such as recognizing lead to reading fluency. A second key element of

74 | Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1)


extensive reading emphasizes the large amount of can help promote extensive reading in L2 settings.
meaningful exposure provided in the target language, Furthermore, systematically and successfully imple-
which plays an important role in language learning. By mented extensive reading research with effective in-
being exposed to large quantities of meaningful reading structional techniques is needed to identify ways in
materials for an extended period of time, reading flu- which extensive reading can be integrated with more
ency and reading comprehension can develop incre- traditional L2 reading instruction.
mentally (Grabe, 2009; Nation, 2009). A third key
feature is that learners in an extensive reading program
usually read self-selected reading materials. This fea- Research on Extensive Reading
ture is unique compared with that of other L2 learning
approaches because by selecting their own reading ma- in L2 Settings
terials, L2 learners can satisfy their individual reading A recent meta-analysis of extensive reading research by
needs, such as reading to obtain information or for en- Nakanishi (2015) reported that extensive reading has a
joyment. Furthermore, individual learners’ linguistic large effect on reading rates (Cohen’s d = 0.98) and a
needs can be satisfied because learners can choose medium effect on reading comprehension (Cohen’s d =
level- appropriate reading materials. Satisfying the 0.63) for group contrasts using pre-and posttests. With
needs of individual readers can also facilitate L2 respect to vocabulary, the effect size for pre–post
learners’ moti-vation to read. These features of contrasts is small (Cohen’s d = 0.18). However, this re-
extensive reading re-quire teachers to engage students sult needs to be interpreted with caution. First, the link
in reading large amounts of interesting materials within between vocabulary acquisition and extensive reading
their linguistic competence for long periods of time and is questionable because the confidence interval in the
consistently motivate students to read (Day & Bamford, results included zero. Second, vocabulary improvement
1998; Grabe, 2009; Grabe & Stoller, 2011). outcomes may be impacted by the type of vocabulary
Studies on extensive reading in L2 settings have gain measure used. In his meta-analysis, Nakanishi re-
shown evidence that it leads to gains in reading ported that the most frequently used type of vocabulary
abilities (e.g., Beglar et al., 2012; Elley & Mangubhai, test was standardized L2 vocabulary tests (e.g., the
1983; Mason & Krashen, 1997; Robb & Susser, 1989; Vocabulary Levels Test). Because the Vocabulary
Tanaka & Stapleton, 2007) and vocabulary (e.g., Elley Levels Test, for example, only minimally samples
& Mangubhai, 1983; Horst, 2005; Webb & Chang, high- frequency vocabulary bands, the test may not
2015). Scholars have argued that extensive reading include vocabulary items from materials that students
should be integrated into a language learning program have read during extensive reading, and no significant
because it can yield various kinds of language learning dif-ference between groups would seem to be an
outcomes (Day & Bamford, 1998; Grabe, 2009; expected result. More densely sampled vocabulary
Macalister, 2010; Nation, 2009). measures are needed, especially ones with many high--
Despite the growing interest and belief among L2 frequency vo-cabulary items to generate a useful and
researchers and teachers in the effectiveness of extensive relevant mea-sure of gains made through reading input.
reading, it seems that extensive reading has not been widely Third, among 34 studies measuring rate,
implemented in various L2 settings, especially in EFL comprehension, and vocabulary gains that Nakanishi
settings (Grabe, 2009). L2 teachers seem to be re-luctant to included in his meta- analysis, 16 did not include
implement extensive reading in their classes (Macalister, control groups. Without control groups, it is hard to
2010). L2 scholars (e.g., Grabe, 2009; Nation, 2009) have assert whether extensive reading treatment itself had
suggested a number of reasons for why ex-tensive reading is any effect.
not widely accepted in L2 settings. One reason is that fluent Krashen (2007) also carried out a meta-analysis on the
reading is often not viewed as an important reading goal in effectiveness of extensive reading among adolescents and
an L2 reading curriculum. Although fluency is a crucial part young adults in EFL settings, focusing on cloze and
of advanced reading comprehension, fluency is often reading comprehension tests. The results of the meta-
overlooked in L2 read-ing courses. Another reason is that the analysis for the two outcomes (Cohen’s d = 0.74 and 0.88,
power of implicit learning opportunities is often not respectively) suggested that extensive reading has a strong
appreciated. Teachers and students do not feel that students impact on reading comprehension. However, Krashen’s
are learning when they are reading quietly in a classroom meta-analysis did not provide information on whether the
(Grabe 2009; Nation, 2009). A third reason is that having a effects were based on controlled studies, so it is hard to
strong ex-tensive reading library of L2 graded readers can be determine whether the effects reported were solely
seen as an expensive investment in supplementary resources. because of extensive reading.
Acknowledging and responding to these challenges For future extensive reading research, Nakanishi (2015)
provided several recommendations, which are generally
accepted practices in many other meta-analyses:

The Effects of Extensive Reading on Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Vocabulary Acquisition | 75
Extensive reading studies should include detailed de- passages. The extensive reading group read graded
scriptive statistics, control groups, longer treatment dura- readers and engaged in several extensive reading activi-
tion, younger participants, and larger sample sizes. One ties. The results revealed that the extensive reading group
important and more specific point that is not included in showed significant improvements in reading
his recommendations is that studies need to include the comprehension and reading rate. Despite the positive
amount of reading done by students. Reports on how impact of extensive reading on reading comprehension
much students actually read during treatment can pro-vide and reading rate, the study has clear limitations. First, the
strong evidence for whether students actually en-gaged in amount of reading done by the participants is not
extensive reading during a longer treatment period. Apart reported. In addition, it is not clear whether the partici-
from meta-analyses of extensive reading research pants actually comprehended the texts while reading at
outcomes, a number of key individual L2 studies asserting their normal speed, because the reading rate tests did not
the effectiveness of extensive reading need to be given include reading comprehension questions.
careful consideration. In another EFL study, Al-Homoud and Schmitt
One major assertion from extensive reading is the (2009) investigated the effectiveness of extensive read-
improvement of reading abilities, that is, reading rate ing on L2 reading abilities with 70 male EFL university
and/or reading comprehension (Al-Homoud & Schmitt, students in Saudi Arabia over a 10-week course. The
2009; Beglar et al., 2012; Bell, 2001; Chang & Millett, students in the intensive reading group (n = 23) were
2015; Elley & Mangubhai, 1983; Huffman, 2014; given quizzes and taught new words and reading strate-
Iwahori, 2008; Mason & Krashen, 1997; Robb & Susser, gies, whereas the students in the extensive reading
1989). In an early study in an EFL setting, Robb and group (n = 47) were given time for silent reading of a
Susser carried out a study to examine the effects of ex- self-selected graded reader and were taught reading
tensive reading on 125 Japanese EFL university stu-dents’ and vocabulary learning strategies. The researchers
reading ability over a semester. The students in the found that the extensive reading group showed
experimental group read SRA cards in class and an significantly larger improvements in their reading rate
average of 641 pages in books written for U.S. teenagers (a mean gain of 33.49 words per minute [WPM]) than
outside the class. The control group, in contrast, used a the intensive reading group students did (a mean gain
textbook focused on reading skills. The findings showed of 26.13 WPM). Both groups showed improvements in
that the experimental group made significantly higher reading comprehension; however, there was no
gains on two reading comprehension measures (i.e., significant dif-ference between the groups. Although
“understanding the important facts,” “guessing vocabu- the findings sup-port the claim that reading fluency can
lary from context”; p. 244). The results of reading rate be developed with extensive reading, comprehension
tests showed that the experimental group read signifi- questions should have followed the reading rate
cantly faster (a mean of 336.39 seconds for the total pas- measures to vali-date whether the students actually
sages) than the control group (a mean of 411.90 seconds read the passages at their normal speed with good
for the total passages). However, there are some major comprehension. In addi-tion, the authors did not report
methodological issues to be considered. As the authors data on the amount of reading done by the students in
indicated, the pre- and posttests were not equivalent; the extensive reading group, although they tried to
therefore, the students’ improvements in reading com- estimate the reading amount.
prehension and reading rate could involve a testing ef-fect In a particularly well-controlled study, Beglar et al.
in addition to the extensive reading treatment. In addition, (2012) investigated how pleasure reading affected the
as the authors mentioned, the extensive read-ing students reading rates of 97 Japanese university students over
spent more time reading than did the con-trol group, two semesters (i.e., 28 weeks). Students were divided
which might have contributed to significantly different into four groups: one intensive reading group and three
results. Finally, it is unclear how well the Japanese EFL pleasure reading groups. The students in the intensive
students were able to understand the un-simplified texts, reading group (n = 17) read a collection of fairy tales in
written for U.S. teenagers, for homework. and outside of class. Pleasure reading group 1 (n = 23)
was also engaged in intensive reading of fairy tales in
Bell (2001) conducted a two-semester study with class, but they read self-selected graded readers or un-
26 young adult government employees in the Yemen simplified books outside of class. Pleasure reading
Arab Republic. The study measured both reading rate groups 2 (n = 22) and 3 (n = 35) read six graded readers
and comprehension in two groups: an intensive reading selected by the teacher and self-selected graded readers
group (n = 12) and an extensive reading group (n = or unsimplified books in class; they also read self-
14). The participants in the intensive reading group selected graded readers or unsimplified books outside
read short passages and completed activities focused of class. The researchers used standard words, defined
on grammar, vocabulary, and rhetorical patterns in the as six character spaces each, to calculate the amount of

76 | Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1)


reading done by the students. The students in the inten- graded readers (approximately 30,000 words) over a
sive reading group read about 40,000 standard words, month. The study assessed 133 French words (70
whereas the students in the three pleasure reading nouns and 63 verbs) from the graded readers that the
groups read 136,029, 158,993, and 200,170 standard partici-pant read, using the same pre- and posttests.
words, respectively. Results showed that the pleasure The re-searchers investigated the acquisition of three
reading groups made greater gains in reading rates than types of vocabulary knowledge (i.e., word meaning,
did the intensive reading group. Furthermore, even spelling, grammatical knowledge) in terms of the
among the pleasure reading groups, the two groups that number of word occurrences in the texts. Findings
read more made greater gains in reading rates than did indicated that the participant gained vocabulary
the group that read the least. The pleasure reading knowledge of 65% of the target words as a result of
groups were also able to maintain their reading com- extensive reading, with spelling benefiting the most for
prehension as their reading rates increased. One fairly both nouns and verbs. As the researchers mentioned,
obvious limitation with this study, however, is that the this study may be limited because it is a case study
treatment groups were given the opportunity for much with a highly motivated stu-dent. Another possible
more reading input and were expected to read exten- limitation of the study is the short interval between the
sively outside of class. pre- and posttests (i.e., one month); the short interval
With respect to vocabulary development, it has might have had an impact on test results.
been argued that extensive reading promotes L2 learn- Despite the generally positive reports of extensive
ers’ vocabulary acquisition. Because extensive reading reading on L2 learning, much of the research on exten-
exposes learners to abundant input, vocabulary acquisi- sive reading has had methodological constraints, sug-
tion can occur incidentally and incrementally with re- gesting that additional well-controlled studies are needed.
peated exposures over time. Studies have identified Many studies failed to report the amount of reading done
positive effects of extensive reading on vocabulary ac- by participants. In addition, several stud-ies had
quisition (e.g., Cho & Krashen, 1994; Horst, 2005; methodological design limitations (e.g., lack of a control
Pigada & Schmitt, 2006; Webb & Chang, 2015). group, lack of comprehension questions in read-ing rate
However, the findings from the limited number of stud- tests, different pre- and posttests, short exten-sive reading
ies do not seem to fully support the effectiveness of ex- periods, a small sample size). Several studies also did not
tensive reading on vocabulary acquisition because of report the actual instructional
methodological issues. procedures used during the treatment phase. These -
In an English as a second language (ESL) setting, issues limit the asserted positive effects of extensive
Horst (2005) investigated 21 adult immigrant ESL stu- reading on reading rates, comprehension, and vocabu-
dents’ vocabulary growth in a six-week extensive read- lary acquisition.
ing program, using an innovative method to assess
students’ vocabulary growth. To examine the students’
vocabulary growth attributable to the graded readers
that the students read, the researcher used three proce- The Present Study
dures: electronic scanning of a part of the graded read- Taking into consideration the methodological issues in-
ers, the use of lexical frequency profiling, and the volved in past research on L2 extensive reading, the
creation of a 100-item vocabulary checklist test. purpose of this study is to investigate the impact of ex-
Results of pre-and posttests showed that the tensive reading as an integrated curricular component
participants made a significant mean gain of 17 words in an L2 reading development course on different areas
from the measure of the 1,001–2,000 frequency band of L2 learning (vocabulary, reading rate, and reading
words and off-list words. Although the findings seem comprehension improvement in the L2) over a 15--
to confirm gains in vocabulary through extensive week semester in a Korean EFL university context. To
reading, the participants’ vocabulary growth cannot be date, few studies have investigated the effect of
entirely attributed to the effects of extensive reading extensive reading on multiple areas of L2 learning by
because the pre- and post-tests were not identical. In including extensive reading as an integrated component
addition, the absence of a control group also weakens within an existing curriculum. Most studies
the findings of the study. This study, like many others, investigated the effect of extensive reading by focusing
also does not report the amount of reading done by on one or two areas of L2 learning and used the entire
participants. Finally, check-list ratings for a vocabulary class time for extensive reading treatments. In contrast,
test have been criticized for lacking validity. this study measures the effects of extensive reading
Pigada and Schmitt’s (2006) case study of a learner while still maintaining a commonly used L2 learning
of French also looked at the effectiveness of extensive curriculum, thus adding a measure of ecological
reading on lexical knowledge. The participant read four validity to the study.

The Effects of Extensive Reading on Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Vocabulary Acquisition | 77
One research question was investigated for the cur- intensive English reading instruction, centered on an ESL
rent study: Is there a difference in Korean EFL univer- course book, Real Reading 3: Creating an Authentic
sity students’ vocabulary, reading rate, and reading Reading Experience (Bonesteel, 2011), which contains 12
comprehension improvement in English between inte- units with high-interest topics, including supersti-tions,
grated extensive reading classes and solely intensive neuroscience, sports, magic, and technology. Each unit
reading classes after a 15-week semester? contains two chapters; each chapter has one reading
passage with various activities centered around
previewing the topic, introducing target words, check-ing
reading comprehension, consolidating target-word
Methods knowledge, and practicing strategies for learning vo-
Participants cabulary and reading. The first 30 minutes of each class
Participants of the study were 171 Korean EFL univer- that were different from the experimental classes con-
sity students enrolled in four 2-credit elective English sisted of reviewing vocabulary covered in the previous
reading classes at a national university in southern class, taking a vocabulary quiz, and analyzing and
Korea. There were 88 students in two control classes translating challenging sentence structures that ap-peared
and 83 students in two experimental classes. The par- in the text covered. The remaining 70 minutes of the class
ticipants’ ages ranged from 18 to 26 years old, with a were composed of several activities, including prereading,
mean age of 20 years old (standard deviation [SD] = during-reading, and postreading activities focused on
1.64 years) in the control classes and a mean age of reading strategies (e.g., making predictions, previewing
21.25 years old (SD = 2.06 years) in the experimental texts, finding main ideas and details, mak-ing inferences)
classes. The participants were 87 males and 84 females that were introduced in each chapter. Other activities
(42 males and 46 females in the control classes and 45 centered around teaching new words identified in the
males and 38 females in the experimental classes) textbook chapter, translating passages into Korean,
across all four year levels of the university, majoring in analyzing grammatical structures, and an-swering
various subjects, such as business administration, comprehension and vocabulary questions.
chemistry, engineering, English literature and As out-of-class assignments, the participants were
linguistics, interna-tional commerce, law, marine encouraged to study for vocabulary quizzes on the new
biology, nursing, and physics. vocabulary for the next class and were required to re-read
Approximately 40% of the participants in the four the reading passages covered in class and answer
classes were taking other English classes at the time of the questions that I provided. The reading assignment
study. More specifically, 38 students (43%) in the control questions included a few critical reading questions and/ or
classes and 30 students (36%) in the experimen-tal classes a summary writing activity based on the passages
were taking a total of 168 course hours (mean [M] = 4.42 covered. I collected students’ homework assignments at
course hours, SD = 2.77 course hours) and 135 course each class session and returned the graded homework in
hours (M = 4.50 course hours, SD = 2.74 course hours) of the next class. The participants received 25% of their
other English classes per week, respec-tively, during the course grade from the vocabulary quizzes and the read-
semester, showing relatively equal ac-cess to other English ing assignments. Homework assigned in the course was
classes during the treatment (although the total number of expected to require two to three hours of time outside of
additional course hours favored the control classes). class each week, matching the amount of homework time
According to the results of the pretests measuring reading per week expected from the experimental classes.
comprehension, reading rate, and vocabulary, the The participants in the two experimental classes had
participants’ English reading- proficiency levels seemed to the same amount of in-class time (i.e., 100 minutes a
range from high-beginning to advanced, but the majority week) as the control classes had, using the same ESL
seemed to be at high- beginning to intermediate levels. I course book. The participants received 70 minutes of
was the instructor of the four reading classes. intensive reading instruction that was similar to that re-
ceived by the control classes to satisfy existing univer-sity
curricular expectations. The remaining 30 minutes of
class time that distinguished the experimental classes
Study Design from the control classes were dedicated to extensive
I employed a quasi-experimental research design using reading activities at the beginning of each class session.
four intact English reading classes: two control and two That is, the participants in the experimental classes did
experimental. The two control classes were designated as not engage in a few activities that the students in the
intensive reading classes; the two experimental classes control classes completed. Five extensive reading activi-
were designated as extensive reading classes. The ties were chosen and implemented to facilitate the par-
participants in the two control classes received ticipants’ reading in and out of class: scaffolded silent

78 | Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1)


reading, writing a paper in three minutes, examining levels. Table 1 shows in- and out-of-class reading in-
book blurbs, listening to a story and writing a response, struction and reading requirements for the control and
and MReader (mreader.org; see Robb, 2015), which is experimental classes.
a database of online quizzes for graded readers. See In this study, I taught all four classes to add treat-ment
Appendix A (available as supporting information for control and minimize inaccurate delivery of treat-ment.
the online version of this article) for details on these To maintain validity of the extensive and intensive
five types of activities. reading treatments and ensure that they were implemented
To provide L2 students with easy and enjoyable ex- in the four classes objectively and fairly as planned over
tensive reading experiences, approximately 350 graded time, several fidelity checks were imple-mented. As one
readers, with a total of 155 titles, were available for the treatment fidelity check, I tried to ad-here to treatment
two extensive reading classes. Various genres among the protocols as much as possible by following carefully
graded readers were available, including thrillers, written lesson plans. I also made a checklist of activities
romances, murder mysteries, human interest stories, and that were supposed to be done in each class, and used the
informational books. The levels of the readers ranged checklist to verify that a treat-ment was implemented as
from 800 to 3,500 most frequent headwords (based on intended. In addition, I kept a notebook of my
publishers’ word frequency lists). The partici-pants read observations of each class after each class session,
self-selected graded readers for 15 minutes during the reflecting on adherence to the lesson plans and absence of
scaffolded silent reading at the beginning of each class. I specific teaching techniques because of unexpected
monitored the participants’ reading and provided situations. I used these fidelity checks to deliver equal and
assistance if they encountered difficulties in fair instructional treatments to the groups over time to
understanding the books. The remaining 10–15 min-utes minimize researcher bias.
after the scaffolded silent reading was used for other
extensive reading activities (i.e., writing a paper in three Data Collection
minutes, examining book blurbs, listening to a story and
writing response) to check the participants’ reading Instruments
comprehension and engagement in class and/or to I developed two tests to investigate the effect of exten-sive
increase motivation to read by introducing interest-ing reading on participants’ reading comprehension, reading
books. In order for the participants to be account-able for rate, and vocabulary improvement after the 15- week
their out-of-class extensive reading assignment, they were semester. To measure participants’ reading com-
required to take a quiz on the book that they finished prehension and rate, I used the same passages for the
reading on MReader. The participants were encouraged to reading comprehension test and the reading rate test. I
reach the goal of reading 200,000 words by the end of the chose four reading passages from two different ESL
semester to receive 25% of their course grade from the reading textbooks (Mikulecky & Jeffries, 2003, 2005) by
extensive reading assignment. No other out-of-class considering topic familiarity, text type, and text diffi-culty.
assignments apart from the extensive read-ing assignment First, I considered participants’ familiarity with the topics
(reading 200,000 words) were given to the participants of the texts because overly unfamiliar topics might affect
throughout the semester. It was ex-pected that students in participants’ reading comprehension and/ or reading rate.
both groups thus spent equal amounts of time (about two Second, I considered text type carefully. Because most of
to three hours per week) for their out-of-class the graded readers that the participants would be reading
assignments, which was verified through personal were either stories or informational texts, I selected two
communications with several stu-dents from each group. narrative and two expository pas-sages. Last, I considered
It is important to note that the time spent for the text difficulty as an important factor for text selection.
assignments may have varied de-pending on individual Difficulty of texts was deter-mined by considering the
students’ reading-proficiency levels of the graded readers

TABLE 1
In-and Out-of-Class Reading Instruction and Requirements
Classes In-class Out-of-class
Intensive reading • Intensive reading instruction using a textbook • Intensive reading of a textbook and vocabulary
(100 minutes) review (two to three hours)

Extensive reading • Intensive reading instruction using a textbook • Extensive reading of graded readers (two to three
(70 minutes) hours)
• Extensive reading instruction using graded readers
(30 minutes)

The Effects of Extensive Reading on Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Vocabulary Acquisition | 79
that the participants would be reading during the 15-week First, I scanned, proofread, and edited each graded
semester. I assumed that most participants in the reader for wrong characters that the optical character
experimental classes would be reading graded read-ers recognition software could not recognize while scan-ning.
with a Lexile measure of 600L–700L by the middle of the In total, I built a corpus comprising the texts of the 155
semester. Therefore, I selected two reading pas-sages with graded readers (with a total of 2,673,449 words). After
Lexile measures of 680L and 710L, respec-tively, for the building the entire graded reader text corpus, I entered the
reading comprehension and reading rate tests. For the text file for each graded reader into the Compleat Lexical
remaining two reading passages, I selected two texts with Tutor website to identify words in different frequency
Lexile measures of 920L and 930L, re-spectively, because ranges in the text. Then, I exam-ined each output file per
of the anticipated mixed-level par-ticipants in the control graded reader to select word candidates for a word list to
and experimental classes. I also checked the lexical be used when creating the vocabulary test. Selection
coverage of the selected passages by using criteria for the words to be placed on the lists are as
VocabProfile/BNC-20 on the Compleat Lexical Tutor follows. Fifteen words were selected from the 2,000-word
website (www.lextutor.ca) to confirm that the dif-ficulty band (word frequency from 1,001 to 2,000) per graded
of the vocabulary did not exceed the level of the graded reader as candidates for a word list to be used; both the
readers that the target students might read. frequency (i.e., the number of occurrences) and the part of
Table 2 provides the results of the readability statis- speech of a word were considered for word selection. In
tics and lexical coverage of the four texts. The resulting terms of frequency, a word (including a member of the
measure was a 32-item reading comprehension and rate family) with more than five occurrences in the 1,001–
test (eight multiple-choice questions per passage; see 2,000 words (2,000-word lexical coverage) was included
Appendix B, which is available as supporting informa- in the word lists. For example, if a word (e.g., repeat) oc-
tion for the online version of this article). The test was curred more than five times in a text in different forms,
piloted at three different times: with two native speak- such as repeat (one instance), repeated (three instances),
ers of English, with 12 intermediate ESL students, and and repeats (two instances), I included the word in the
with 10 advanced Korean EFL university students. I word lists. The parts of speech that qualified for inclu-
made minor revisions based on the results of the pilot sion on the word lists were limited to nouns, verbs, ad-
tests. jectives, and adverbs because they are the most frequently
To test words that were likely to appear in the read- distributed lexical word classes across regis-ters (Biber,
ing materials that each participant read, I adapted the Johansson, Leech, Conrad, & Finegan, 1999). I repeated
vocabulary test for the present study from the approach the same procedure to select word candidates for the
used by Horst (2005). The vocabulary test covered frequency ranges of 2,001–3,000 words (3,000-word
words from all 155 graded readers available for the lexical coverage) and 3,001–20,000 words (4,000+-word
par-ticipants. The development of the vocabulary test lexical coverage). Words that ap-peared more than three
was carried out in three steps: scanning the 155 graded times, with some exceptions, in the 3,000-and 4,000+--
readers and building a graded reader text corpus, creat- word ranges qualified for in-clusion. Fifteen word
ing word lists for each graded reader through lexical candidates were selected from the 2,000-and 3,000-word
frequency profiling, and developing a vocabulary test lexical coverage results and 10 words from the 4,000+--
based on the combined word lists of the 155 graded word lexical coverage results.
readers.

TABLE 2
Readability Statistics and Lexical Coverage of the Reading Texts From Which the Passages Were Selected
Readability: Lexile Lexical coverage
Passage length: measure (Flesch–
Text Text type Number of words Kincaid grade level) 2,000 words 3,000 words 4,000+ words
“Becoming a Successful Narrative 403 680L (5.7) 93.93 94.54 95.86
Writer”

“Animals and Language” Expository 400 710L (5.9) 95.54 97.27 99.51

“Going Her Own Way” Narrative 508 920L (8.7) 92.89 95.46 97.05

“The Iceman” Expository 514 930L (7.3) 94.01 97.10 98.64


Note. The first and second texts are from Reading Power: Reading for Pleasure, Comprehension Skills, Thinking Skills, Reading Faster (3rd ed.), by
B.S. Mikulecky and L. Jeffries (2005), White Plains, NY: Pearson. The third and fourth texts are from More Reading Power: Reading for Pleasure,
Comprehension Skills, Thinking Skills, Reading Faster (2nd ed.), by B.S. Mikulecky and L. Jeffries (2003), White Plains, NY: Pearson.

80 | Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1)


In total, I selected 40 word candidates per graded The reliability estimate was .65 for the reading com-
reader for the word lists. prehension test (k = 32). In terms of the vocabulary test,
To develop a 120-item vocabulary test, first, I en- before the administration of the pre- and posttests, I read
tered the entire set of words from the word lists—that is, the directions aloud to the participants to ensure their
(40 word candidates per graded reader) × (155 graded understanding of the directions. The reliability es-timate
reader titles) = 6,200 words—into the Compleat Lexical was .96 for the vocabulary test (k = 120). To en-sure that I
Tutor website. Then, I selected a total of 120 words (i.e., scored the vocabulary test (i.e., writing definitions)
40 word items per range) with high frequency in the three consistently and fairly, I rescored it two more times at
ranges (i.e., 2,000, 3,000, 4,000+) from the output file for approximately two-month intervals; little difference in the
the vocabulary test. Finally, the vocabulary test required results from the three scoring times was found. The lower
participants to write definitions for the 120 se-lected reliability coefficient of the read-ing comprehension test,
words in their native language (see Appendix C, which is when compared with the reli-ability of the vocabulary
available as supporting information for the on-line version test, was due to both the lower number of data points and
of this article). the more complex array of factors influencing
comprehension than discrete vocab-ulary items. A
Procedures vocabulary test with 120 discrete items is almost always
The reading comprehension and rate test and the vo- going to be highly reliable, assuming that the vocabulary
cabulary test were administered as pre-and posttests. assessed is within an appropriate range of student
Prior to administering the reading comprehension and knowledge. Table 3 describes the variables included in the
rate test, I instructed the participants on the test proce- study.
dures. Afterward, the participants did a practice test
with a short text (about 100 words) as an example to Data Analysis
ensure that they understood the test procedures. To test the effects of extensive reading on various areas
Following the practice test, the participants took the of L2 learning after the 15-week semester, I ran a
actual test. To begin, I distributed the first passage to repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance
the participants. I directed them to open their packets to (MANOVA) with group (i.e., extensive reading, inten-
the passage and start reading at the same time. sive reading) and time (i.e., pretest, posttest) as the in-
Simultaneously, I turned on the online stopwatch pro- dependent variables and the three test scores as
jected on the whiteboard. After reading the first pas- dependent variables (i.e., reading comprehension, read-
sage, the participants each recorded their finishing time ing rate, vocabulary). I conducted further analyses on
indicated by the online stopwatch on the test. Then, the three dependent variables using analyses of vari-
they turned the page over, answered the compre- ance (ANOVAs). I checked assumptions of the
hension questions, and recorded their finishing time for MANOVA and found them satisfactory.
the comprehension questions. After the partici-pants
were done with the first passage, I told them to place
their packets with the cover page facing up on their Preliminary Data Analysis
desks. Then, I distributed the next passage. The same It was essential to investigate how much the partici-pants
testing procedures were repeated three more times, in the two extensive reading classes read over the 15--
once per passage. week semester to validate the effects of the extensive

TABLE 3
Description of Variables in the Study
Scale of
Type of variables Variable Operationalization Range of scores measurement
Dependent Reading rate Time spent on reading four passages Number of words read Continuous
per minute

Reading Scores on the reading comprehension and 0–32 Continuous


comprehension rate test

Vocabulary Scores on the vocabulary test 0–120 Continuous

Independent Group Experimental and control 1/2 Categorical

Time Pre-and posttreatment 1/2 Categorical

The Effects of Extensive Reading on Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Vocabulary Acquisition | 81
TABLE 4
Descriptive Statistics for Average Amount Read by the Two Extensive Reading Classes
Extensive reading class 1 (n = 45) Extensive reading class 2 (n = 38) Total (n = 83)
Number of words read

Mean 136,667.16 171,718.16 152,714.60

Standard deviation 74,354.17 81,003.25 78,967.72

Median 147,962.00 201,552.50 190,014.00

Minimum 8,691.00 9,614.00 8,691.00

Maximum 238,997.00 304,651.00 304,651.00

Skewness −0.40 −0.54 −.38

Kurtosis −1.44 −0.86 −1.12

Number of books read

Mean 9.22 11.03 10.05

Standard deviation 4.40 4.62 4.56

Median 10.00 12.00 11.00

Minimum 1.00 1.00 1.00

Maximum 16.00 18.00 18.00

Skewness −0.45 −0.56 −0.44

Kurtosis −1.01 −0.83 −0.93

reading treatment implemented in this study. The num- intensive reading (M = 25.91, SD = 3.43) and extensive
ber of words (books) read is reported based on the re- reading (M = 25.87, SD = 2.85) groups. However, in the
ports from MReader and individual participants’ prereading rate test, there was a larger difference be-
reading record charts kept during the semester. As tween the two groups. The students in the extensive
shown in Table 4, the participants in the two extensive reading classes read a mean of 133.29 WPM (SD = 29.83
reading classes read a mean of 152,714 words (SD = WPM), whereas those in the intensive reading classes
78,967 words) during the semester. Regarding the read a mean of 147.76 WPM (SD = 28.47 WPM), indi-
number of books read, these two classes read a mean of cating that the students in the intensive reading classes
10 books (SD = 4.56 books) during the semester. In read 14.47 more WPM. In the vocabulary test, the mean
terms of the number of different levels of graded score of the extensive reading classes (M = 51.63, SD =
readers read by these two classes, in general, most of 18.90) was also lower than that of the intensive reading
the partici-pants (90%) read graded readers with 800– classes (M = 54.22, SD = 18.79).
2,000 head-word levels and read fairly similar levels of In the posttests, however, the extensive reading
graded readers without much variation. classes produced higher mean scores for all three tests
than the intensive reading classes did. In the reading
comprehension test, the difference in the mean score
between the two groups was relatively small, with a mean
Results of 27.14 (SD = 2.54) for the extensive reading classes and
The research question asked whether there is a differ-ence 25.83 (SD = 3.24) for the intensive reading classes. In
in reading comprehension, reading rate, and vo-cabulary terms of reading rate, the participants in both groups
improvement between the extensive reading classes and improved their reading rates. However, the participants in
the intensive reading classes after a 15-week semester. the extensive reading classes read more WPM (M =
Table 5 presents descriptive statistics for the pre- and 168.42 WPM, SD = 38.72 WPM) than those in the
posttest scores for reading comprehension, reading rate, intensive reading classes read (M = 163.29 WPM, SD =
and vocabulary acquisition between the two groups. In the 33.87 WPM), showing that the participants in the
pretests, for reading comprehension, there was very little extensive reading classes (a mean gain of 35.13 WPM)
difference in scores between the improved more than those in the intensive reading

82 | Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1)


TABLE 5
Descriptive Statistics for Pre-and Posttest Scores by the Extensive Reading (n = 83) and Intensive Reading
(n = 88) Classes (N = 171)
Reading comprehension (k = 32) Reading ratea Vocabulary (k = 120)
Extensive Intensive Extensive Intensive Extensive Intensive
reading reading reading reading reading reading

Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest

Mean 25.87 27.14 25.91 25.83 133.29 168.42 147.76 163.29 51.63 64.70 54.22 57.63

Standard 0.31 0.28 0.37 0.35 3.28 4.25 3.04 3.61 2.07 2.02 2.03 2.06
error

Standard 2.85 2.54 3.43 3.24 29.83 38.72 28.47 33.87 18.90 18.41 18.79 19.30
deviation

Median 26.00 27.00 26.00 26.00 127.77 164.17 147.18 161.50 52.00 67.00 55.00 58.00

Minimum 17.00 20.00 15.00 17.00 65.00 74.00 94.00 98.00 12.00 22.00 8.00 6.00

Maximum 31.00 31.00 32.00 32.00 225.00 268.00 238.00 253.00 98.00 105.00 105.00 108.00

Skewness −0.61 −0.44 −0.72 −0.66 0.75 0.50 0.49 0.45 0.09 −0.35 −0.08 −0.06

Kurtosis 0.37 −0.29 0.71 −0.02 1.14 0.19 0.38 0.01 −0.58 −0.50 0.70 0.40
aFour reading passages were used to assess words read per minute.

classes (a mean gain of 15.53 WPM). For the vocabulary ANOVA tests, I tested the homogeneity of variance
test, the extensive reading classes also gained more words assumption for all three dependent variables at pre- and
(M = 64.70 words, SD = 18.41 words, a mean gain of posttests. The results showed that all of the Levene’s
13.07 words) than the intensive reading classes gained (M tests were not statistically significant (p > .05);
= 57.63 words, SD = 19.30 words, a mean gain of 3.41 therefore, the homogeneity of variance as-sumption was
words). Additionally, when I analyzed the data to find the satisfied. Table 7 presents univariate ANOVA results for
effect of taking other English classes during the study, within-groups contrasts. There was a significant
results showed that those who did not take other English interaction between group and time for reading
classes outperformed those did, indicating that taking comprehension, F(1, 169) = 15.32, p = .000, partial ƞ2 =
other English classes did not have any influence on the .08; the effect size is relatively small (Cohen’s d =
outcome of the study. 0.30). There was also a significant inter-action between
To determine the impact of extensive reading on group and time for reading rate, F(1,
reading comprehension, reading rates, and vocabulary, 169) = 26.27, p = .000, partial ƞ2 = 0.14; the effect size
I used a repeated-measures MANOVA. As shown in is small to medium (Cohen’s d = 0.39). The last sig-
Table 6, the analysis confirmed a significant multivari- nificant interaction effect was found in the vocabu-lary
ate interaction effect between group and time, Wilks’s test, F(1, 169) = 82.29, p = .000, partial ƞ2 = .33,
Ʌ = .61, F(3, 167) = 35.76, p = .000, partial ƞ2 = .39, showing a large effect on vocabulary acquisition
Cohen’s d = 0.80. There was also a significant (Cohen’s d = 0.70).
multivari-ate effect for time, pre-and posttests, Wilks’s As shown in Figure 1, the results indicate that for
Ʌ = .31, F(3, 167) = 123.85, p = .000, partial ƞ2 = .69, reading comprehension, the two groups did not per-
Cohen’s d = 1.49. form differently at the outset. However, the extensive
As follow-up tests to the repeated-measures reading classes showed a significant improvement in
MANOVA, I conducted ANOVA tests on the three reading comprehension at the posttest, whereas results
dependent variables. Prior to performing follow-up do not show any real improvement for the intensive

TABLE 6
Multivariate Analysis of Variance Results of Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Vocabulary (N = 171)
Source Wilks’s Λ F Hypothetical df Error df p η2
Within time .31 123.85 3 167 .000 .69

Interaction (Group × Time) .61 35.76 3 167 .000 .39

The Effects of Extensive Reading on Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Vocabulary Acquisition | 83
TABLE 7
Univariate Analysis of Variance Results (Within-Groups Contrasts)

Source Variable Sum of squares df Mean square F p η2


Interaction Reading 39.31 1 39.31 15.32 .000 .08
(Group × Time) comprehension

Reading rate 8,201.24 1 8,201.24 26.27 .000 .14

Vocabulary 1,991.90 1 1,991.90 82.29 .000 .33

Error (time) Reading 433.54 169 2.57


comprehension

Reading rate 52,757.05 169 312.17

Vocabulary 4,090.84 169 24.21

FIGURE 1 FIGURE 3
Reading Comprehension Mean Scores Across Group Vocabulary Mean Scores Across Group and Time
and Time

more dramatic improvement than shown by the inten-


FIGURE 2 sive reading classes.
Reading Rate Mean Scores Across Group and Time

Discussion
Results from the repeated-measures MANOVA con-
firmed the effectiveness of extensive reading as an inte-
grated part of the existing reading curriculum on various
areas of language learning (reading comprehen-sion,
reading rate, and vocabulary acquisition). Unlike the
results of Nakanishi’s (2015) meta-analysis of exten-sive
reading, which showed no significant difference in
vocabulary improvement, the most noticeable gain from
extensive reading was in vocabulary acquisition. Two
possible reasons for the experimental groups’ im-
provement in vocabulary acquisition can be suggested.
reading classes. Regarding reading rate and vocabulary, as Consistent exposure to graded readers over time seemed
indicated in Figures 2 and 3, although the mean scores of to play a crucial role in facilitating students’ incidental
the extensive reading classes were lower than those of the vocabulary acquisition (Horst, 2005; Nation, 2013; Webb
intensive reading classes at the outset, the extensive & Chang, 2015). In the process of reading multi-ple
reading classes significantly outperformed the intensive graded readers, the participants were exposed to many of
reading classes at the posttests, showing the same/similar high-frequency English

84 | Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1)


words repeatedly; therefore, these multiple exposures component to the reading course can provide high-
may have contributed to incidental acquisition of new frequency vocabulary input more effectively.
English vocabulary and reinforcement of partially With respect to reading rate, constant exposure to
known vocabulary knowledge. large amounts of level-appropriate reading input, which
A second possible reason for the large effect of ex- are within the participants’ reading-proficiency level,
tensive reading on vocabulary may be the fairly robust throughout the semester also likely enabled rate im-
and reliable vocabulary measure drawing on 155 graded provement among extensive reading participants, con-
readers and the broad sampling of high- frequency firming Beglar et al.’s (2012) findings. Grabe (2009)
vocabulary. The measure developed for this study pointed out that “the ability to read extended texts for
assessed more directly the words that the par-ticipants long periods of time is a hallmark of fluent reading” (p.
might have encountered from reading graded readers. 311); the participants in the extensive reading classes in
Few studies have investigated vocabulary learning the present study seemed to have developed their
through extensive reading by focusing on words that ability to read long texts by reading easy reading
participants most likely encountered while reading materials ex-tensively over time, which could lead to
self-selected graded readers. Studies that have improved read-ing rates.
investigated the effects of extensive reading on vocab- In this study, the use of graded readers as reading
ulary acquisition used vocabulary tests, such as the materials likely contributed to the significant gain dif-
Vocabulary Levels Test, to assess the effects of exten- ference in reading rate between the extensive and inten-
sive reading on vocabulary growth (e.g., Al-Homoud sive reading classes. Graded readers, specifically
& Schmitt, 2009; Lee, 2007; Yamamoto, 2011). written for English learners, can play a crucial role in
However, these types of vocabulary tests are not sensi- improve-ment in L2 learners’ reading rates (see also
tive measures of vocabulary growth from extensive Beglar & Hunt, 2014; Beglar et al., 2012). Reading
reading and may fail to represent the words learned level- appropriate graded readers consistently over a
from extensive reading. Those tests probably were not semester made it possible for the participants to read
able to measure the effect of extensive reading accu- without stopping and provided the participants with
rately because they only sparsely sample word knowl- regular reading fluency practice over time. As a result
edge at different frequency bands and include of con-stant reading fluency practice with level--
numerous words that participants who read exten- appropriate graded readers, the participants seemed to
sively might not have encountered through extensive have built English reading confidence, resulting in
reading. As a result, the researchers’ findings did not improved reading rates.
show the effect of extensive reading on vocabulary ac- With respect to reading comprehension results, there
quisition. The vocabulary test designed for this study, was a significant difference in reading compre-hension
which incorporated words that appeared consistently in between the two groups. Although the impact of extensive
multiple vocabulary bands across 155 graded read-ers, reading on reading comprehension turned out to be
seems to be a more effective way to assess students’ relatively small (Cohen’s d = 0.30), it is essen-tial to note
incidental vocabulary learning through extensive that the extensive reading participants, with 30% less
reading. direct intensive reading comprehension instruction,
One possible criticism of these results is that stu-dents significantly outperformed the intensive reading
in the extensive reading group gained more in vo-cabulary participants. Previous studies that used the
knowledge than the control groups did because the words entire class time for extensive reading (e.g., Beglar et
assessed were the same as those to which the extensive al., 2012; Huffman, 2014) failed to show a significant
reading students were exposed. This is true to a certain difference between experimental and control groups in
extent. However, any individual student read, on average, reading comprehension. The present study, which
only 10 of the 155 books used to generate the higher incorporated less than one third of the class time in -
frequency words used for assessment. Perhaps more existing classes for extensive reading, demonstrated a
importantly, the large majority of words used for significant difference in the participants’ reading com-
assessment purposes were very frequent words in the prehension. A possible reason for why this study
English language (being among the 3,000 most frequent showed a significant gain compared with previous
lexical words in English). A goal for any academic L2 studies may be the overall amount of reading done by
reading curriculum should be to expose students to as participants in a given period of time. The extensive
many of these words as possible and have students learn reading participants in the current study read a mean of
as many of them as possible. It is likely that the control 152,714 words in a 15-week semester, whereas the
groups were also exposed to many of these very high- three pleasure reading group participants in Beglar et
frequency words but not as often. The extensive reading al.’s study read a mean of 165,064 standard words in a
groups demonstrated that an added extensive reading 28-week academic year. It seems that more reading in

The Effects of Extensive Reading on Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Vocabulary Acquisition | 85
a shorter period of time may have contributed to the as a course requirement. The small addition of an ex-
significant effect in the present study. tensive reading component to the existing reading cur-
One other possible reason for the significant effect riculum argues for the effectiveness of extensive reading
on reading comprehension is the nature of longitudinal in L2 settings. Five important instructional implica-tions
reading comprehension development itself. Reading for the successful implementation of extensive reading in
comprehension abilities comprise “(a) syntactic knowl- L2 reading classes can be suggested from this study for
edge and processing skills; (b) reading strategies that teachers to help students read consistently.
support comprehension; (c) the integration of reading
strategies and higher-level processing to develop the Setting a Clear Reading Goal
strategic reader; (d) the role of discourse knowledge; When implementing an extensive reading component
and (e) the centrality of vocabulary knowledge” in L2 reading classes, a specific reading goal needs to
(Grabe, 2009, p. 195). Grabe pointed out that a be given to students to facilitate more reading in and
combination of these components influences reading out-side of class. In this study, a 200,000-word reading
comprehension abilities and the development of goal was set, assuming that students would read one
strategic readers. The present 15-week extensive graded reader per week, as the literature suggests (Day
reading study may have been long enough in duration & Bamford, 1998; Nation & Wang, 1999), and that
and reading intensity to pro-duce a significant effect. they would read longer (and more advanced) graded
In sum, the results from this repeated-measures readers as the semester progressed.
MANOVA study help address a real gap in the litera- Setting a specific reading goal for a class requires
ture, which is that impacts of extensive reading on deciding what an appropriate amount of reading for a
various areas of language learning have not generally semester is. Oftentimes, L2 teachers are not sure about
been sufficiently demonstrated in ecologically valid how much students should read in order for extensive
contexts. The findings reported here demonstrate that a reading to be effective. In addition, L2 teachers typi-cally
15-week, semester-long extensive reading routine had a do not think about how many words must be cov-ered.
positive impact on a combination of vocabulary Although one graded reader per week is a good
acquisition, reading rate, and reading comprehension. recommendation, it may not be concrete enough when the
This study provides persuasive empirical evidence that number of words in a graded reader can vary depending
a systematically designed extensive reading ap-proach, on the level of graded readers and the pub-lishers;
as part of a regular reading course, played a crucial role reluctant L2 readers might want to read very short books.
in improving Korean EFL university stu-dents’ Setting a reading goal based on the num-ber of words read
vocabulary acquisition, reading rate, and read-ing can provide a more accurate way to determine the level of
comprehension. effort needed to engage in exten-sive reading. Table 8
suggests possible reading goals based on the number of
words to be read across differ-ent levels of graded readers
Implications for a 15-week extensive reading program. The specified
Although extensive reading has gained in popularity in reading goal will vary depending on students’ reading
several ESL/EFL settings, it has not been widely prac- proficiency levels. For example, for students at an
ticed in L2 contexts, especially in EFL settings, for sev- elementary reading level who start to read a graded reader
eral reasons: a limited emphasis on fluent reading, the with 4,000 running words, setting a goal of reading at
cost of providing many reading materials, and the lack of least 60,000 words over a 15-week semester would be
teacher and administrator awareness about the im- plausible. Students at more advanced English levels need
portance of extensive reading (Grabe, 2009). In this study, higher course goals. In general, it might be necessary for
most extensive reading was done outside of class teachers to set a

TABLE 8
Suggestions for Reading Goals, Based on One Graded Reader per Week for a 15-Week Extensive Reading Program
Book level (number of headwords) a Approximate number of words in a graded reader Suggested reading goals
Elementary (401–800) 4,000–8,000 60,000–120,000

Intermediate (801–1,500) 8,000–20,000 120,000–300,000

Upper intermediate (1,501–2,400) 15,000–25,000 225,000–375,000

Advanced (2,401–3,600) 20,000–35,000 300,000–525,000


aBook levels (headwords) are based on The Extensive Reading Foundation Graded Readers Scale (Extensive Reading Foundation, 2009 b). bExtensive Reading
Foundation. (2009). The Extensive Reading Foundation Graded Readers Scale. Retrieved from http://www.erfoundation.org/scale/ERF_Scale.pdf.

86 | Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1)


reading goal reasonably high, considering their stu- reading scholars have suggested, teachers should pro-
dents’ reading levels, to encourage more reading. vide ongoing class guidance throughout the program
and engage in formal or informal teacher–student con-
Including Extensive Reading Activities ferences on extensive reading practices (Day &
Specific extensive reading activities should be incorpo- Bamford, 1998; Grabe, 2009). Through teacher–
student interac-tions, teachers can learn about students’
rated into an extensive reading program to sustain and
extensive read-ing practices and use individual
increase students’ motivation to read (e.g., Bamford &
students’ input to guide other students.
Day, 2004; Jacobs & Farrell, 2012; Suk, 2016). This
Aside from instructional activities relevant for ex-
study included five extensive reading activities, and
tensive reading success, a final implication of this study
partici-pants, based on interview information,
involves the importance of an integrated extensive/
perceived the activities as helpful in terms of
intensive L2 reading curriculum. In the opening section of
motivating them to read more graded readers. Teachers
this paper, I briefly noted that a key goal of this study was
can incorporate exten-sive reading activities that
not only to contrast intensive reading and extensive
introduce interesting reading materials to students,
reading curricula but also to integrate the two instruc-
provide students with time to read in class, and hold
tional options in a single course format to determine
students accountable for their reading to facilitate their
whether the addition of extensive reading would lead to
reading engagement in and outside of class.
better reading development outcomes. The study has
provided evidence that such an integrated curriculum can
Reading and Becoming Familiar make a difference in EFL university settings. One could
With the Books in the Class say, as a result, that an integrated curriculum is better than
Library only an intensive reading curriculum. This study says
Extensive reading allows students to self-select books; nothing about whether an integrated approach would be
choosing books based on individual students’ reading better than only an extensive read-ing curriculum.
levels and interests can greatly increase their motiva- However, in most academic L2 con-texts, intensive
tion to read (Day & Bamford, 1998). However, reading is the only curricular option allowed, so proposing
teachers might often observe that students struggle in that extensive reading should be
choosing books, especially in settings where extensive the dominant instructional approach may not be widely
reading is a new experience, because they do not know accepted. This study provides a strong argument that a
how to find books that might complement their combined approach, with both extensive and intensive
interests. To recommend books that appropriately reading as part of every week’s instruction, can lead to
match individual students’ interests, it is necessary for better learning outcomes than only an intensive reading
teachers to be fa-miliar with and have knowledge about curriculum can provide.
the books in the class library. Teachers can gain
knowledge about the books available to students by
reading them, collecting information about books that Limitations and Conclusions
are popular among their students, and chatting with
students about the books that they have read. Despite employing a careful methodology, a few limita-
tions are worth pointing out. One limitation of this study is
the quasi-experimental design. The lack of ran-dom
Monitoring Students’
sampling limits the study’s generalizability. Future
Reading Progress research including random sampling would provide more
Because extensive reading can be done both in and out- concrete evidence for the effects of extensive read-ing.
side of class, it is important for teachers to consistently Another limitation can involve some vocabulary items
monitor individual students’ reading progress. Such selected for the vocabulary test based on 155 graded
monitoring can include the use of MReader, student readers available for the experimental groups. It was
reading logs, student book recommendations, and brief assumed that students in both groups might have
discussions with students. The use of MReader to mon- encountered most of the high-frequency words in the
itor student progress turned out to be very successful in 2,000-and 3,000-word levels. It was also assumed that the
this study (see also Robb, 2015). majority of students in the experimental groups might
have encountered only a very small set of non- high--
Providing Ongoing Assistance frequency words in their actual reading because they only
To motivate students to read consistently, it is impor- read 10 of the 155 graded readers on average. Thus,
tant for teachers to know what individual students are students in both groups were expected to be in a
reading and what they think about the books that they reasonably similar position with respect to the overall
are reading in terms of difficulty and interest. As

The Effects of Extensive Reading on Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Vocabulary Acquisition | 87
range of vocabulary exposure (if not the same level of to engage in reading, and lead to successful implemen-
intensity and engagement). Nevertheless, one may tation of extensive reading. The extensive reading ac-
won-der why the vocabulary development of the tivities implemented in this study might work with
control groups was measured through the test on some many L2 student groups in EFL settings, although
words from the graded readers that were less frequent separate research would be needed in U.S. English
and to which they may not have been exposed. Further learner K−12 settings, as they are quite different in na-
studies in the future, testing only the vocabulary that is ture from most EFL settings. Regardless of setting,
equally available for both control and experimental more research is needed to demonstrate the effective-
groups, would provide stronger evidence for the effect ness of specific sets of extensive reading instructional
of exten-sive reading on vocabulary development. A activities.
third possi-ble limitation is the absence of a delayed
posttest to assess vocabulary gains; the positive NOTES
findings of the vo-cabulary test in this study cannot This article is derived from my PhD dissertation. I would like to
confirm that gains through extensive reading would thank my dissertation committee members: William Grabe,
have longer retention. It was not feasible to administer Fredricka Stoller, Joan Jamieson, and Yuly Asencion Delaney at
Northern Arizona University.
a delayed posttest in the specific research setting
because the participants were from different
departments and had a winter break. Future studies REFERENCES
examining vocabulary gains should incorporate a Al-Homoud, F., & Schmitt, N. (2009). Extensive reading in a chal-
delayed posttest into the research design three to four lenging environment: A comparison of extensive and intensive
weeks after the end of the experiment. reading approaches in Saudi Arabia. Language Teaching
Research, 13(4), 383–401. doi:10.1177/1362168809341508
Overall, this study strengthened evidence for the ef- Anderson, N.J. (1999). Exploring second language reading: Issues
fectiveness of extensive reading on various areas of L2 and strategies. Boston, MA: Heinle & Heinle.
learning. The results showed that the extensive reading Anderson, N.J. (2015). Academic reading expectations and chal-
students developed L2 reading abilities and especially lenges. In N. Evans, N. Anderson, & W. Eggington (Eds.), ESL
increased L2 vocabulary acquisition, even though only readers and writers in higher education (pp. 95–109). New York,
NY: Routledge.
30% of the existing class time was used for extensive Bamford, J., & Day, R.R. (Eds.). (2004). Extensive reading
reading instruction. The findings suggest that extensive activities for teaching language. New York, NY: Cambridge
reading can be readily incorporated into existing L2 University Press.
curricula without major changes to the curricula. Beglar, D., & Hunt, A. (2014). Pleasure reading and reading rate
Although the participants in the current study were gains. Reading in a Foreign Language, 26(1), 29–48.
Beglar, D., Hunt, A., & Kite, Y. (2012). The effect of pleasure reading on
Korean EFL university students at a mostly intermedi- Japanese university EFL learners’ reading rates. Language Learning,
ate level, both beginning-and more-advanced-level L2 62(3), 665–703. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00651.x
learners would also benefit from extensive reading. Bell, T. (2001). Extensive reading: Speed and comprehension. The
Related research findings lend support for the benefits Reading Matrix, 1(1), 1–13.
of extensive reading for both beginning and advanced- Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S., & Finegan, E.
(1999). Longman grammar of spoken and written English. Essex,
level learners (e.g., Kweon & Kim, 2008; Pigada & England: Pearson.
Schmitt, 2006; Webb & Chang, 2015). Because Bonesteel, L. (2011). Real reading 3: Creating an authentic reading
extensive reading can contribute to developing lower experience. White Plains, NY: Pearson.
level pro-cessing, beginning and intermediate L2 Chang, A.C.-S., & Millett, S. (2015). Improving reading rates and
comprehension through audio-assisted extensive reading for
learners can build word recognition and syntactic
beginner learners. System, 52, 91–102. doi:10.1016/
parsing skills and thus develop reading fluency through j.system.2015.05.003
extensive reading. Cho, K.S., & Krashen, S. (1994). Acquisition of vocabulary from
This study sheds further light on the implementa- the Sweet Valley Kids series: Adult ESL acquisition. Journal of
tion of an extensive reading program in EFL settings, Reading, 37(8), 662–667.
Day, R., & Bamford, J. (1998). Extensive reading in the second lan-
with implications for university ESL settings. guage classroom. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Instructional techniques used in the study can also be Elley, W., & Mangubhai, F. (1983). The impact of reading on second
fairly easily incorporated into other ESL/EFL reading language learning. Reading Research Quarterly, 19(1), 53–67.
curricula. In an ESL university setting, as a pilot study doi:10.2307/747337
Ellis, N.C. (2008). Implicit and explicit knowledge about language.
of the current study, Suk (2016) explored the effective- In J. Cenoz & N.H. Hornberger (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language
ness of extensive reading activities through student and and education: Vol. 6. Knowledge about language (2nd ed., pp.
teacher perceptions. The results showed that the 119–131). New York, NY: Springer.
activities were perceived as effective in terms of moti- Extensive Reading Foundation. (2009). The Extensive Reading
vating students to read more. The study suggests that Foundation Graded Readers Scale. Retrieved from erfoundation.
org/scale/ERF_Scale.pdf
effective instructional practices can motivate students

88 | Reading Research Quarterly, 52(1)


Grabe, W. (2009). Reading in a second language: Moving from theory Pigada, M., & Schmitt, N. (2006). Vocabulary acquisition from ex-
to practice. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. tensive reading: A case study. Reading in a Foreign Language,
Grabe, W., & Stoller, F.L. (2011). Teaching and researching reading 18(1), 1–28.
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. Robb, T. (2015). Quizzes—a sin against the sixth commandment? In
Horst, M. (2005). Learning L2 vocabulary through extensive read- defense of MReader. Reading in a Foreign Language, 27(1),
ing: A measurement study. Canadian Modern Language Review, 146–151.
61(3), 355–382. doi:10.3138/cmlr.61.3.355 Robb, T.N., & Susser, B. (1989). Extensive reading vs. skills
Huffman, J. (2014). Reading rate gains during a one-semester ex- building in an EFL context. Reading in a Foreign Language, 5(2),
tensive reading course. Reading in a Foreign Language, 26(2), 239– 251.
17–33. Rosenfeld, M., Leung, S., & Oltman, P.K. (2001). Identifying the reading,
Iwahori, Y. (2008). Developing reading fluency: A study of extensive writing, speaking, and listening tasks important for academic success at
reading in EFL. Reading in a Foreign Language, 20(1), 70–91. the undergraduate and graduate levels [TOEFL Monograph Series MS-
Jacobs, G., & Farrell, T.S.C. (2012). Teachers sourcebook for 21]. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
extensive reading. Charlotte, NC: Information Age. Suk, N. (2016). Teacher and student perceptions of extensive
Krashen, S. (2004). The power of reading. Portsmouth, NH: reading activities. Modern English Education, 17(1), 69–88.
Heinemann. Tanaka, H., & Stapleton, P. (2007). Increasing reading input in
Krashen, S. (2007). Extensive reading in English as a foreign lan-guage Japanese high school EFL classrooms: An empirical study explor-
by adolescents and young adults: A meta-analysis. International ing the efficacy of extensive reading. The Reading Matrix, 7(1),
Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 3(2), 23–29. 115–130.
Krashen, S. (2011). Free voluntary reading. Portsmouth, NH: Webb, S., & Chang, A.C.-S. (2015). How does prior word
Heinemann. knowledge affect vocabulary learning progress in an extensive
Kweon, S.O., & Kim, H.R. (2008). Beyond raw frequency: reading pro-gram? Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
Incidental vocabulary acquisition in extensive reading. Reading 37(4), 651–675. doi:10.1017/S0272263114000606
in a Foreign Language, 20(2), 191–215. Yamamoto, Y. (2011). Bridging the gap between receptive and pro-
Lee, S.-Y. (2007). Revelations from three consecutive studies on ductive vocabulary size through extensive reading. The Reading
extensive reading. RELC Journal, 38(2), 150–170. doi:10.1177/ Matrix, 11(3), 226–242.
0033688207079730
Macalister, J. (2010). Investigating teacher attitudes to extensive Submitted December 4, 2015 Final
reading practices in higher education: Why isn’t everyone doing revision received April 28, 2016
it? RELC Journal, 41(1), 59–75. doi:10.1177/0033688210362609 Accepted May 3, 2016
Mason, B., & Krashen, S. (1997). Extensive reading in English as
a foreign language. System, 25(1), 91–102. doi:10.1016/S0346- NAMHEE SUK is an instructor in the Department of English
251X(96)00063-2 Language and Literature at Pukyong National University, Busan,
Mikulecky, B.S., & Jeffries, L. (2003). More reading power: South Korea; e-mail ns76@nau.edu. Her research interests
Reading for pleasure, comprehension skills, thinking skills, include second-language literacy development, literacy
reading faster (2nd ed.). White Plains, NY: Pearson. instruction, and motivation.
Mikulecky, B.S., & Jeffries, L. (2005). Reading power: Reading for
pleasure, comprehension skills, thinking skills, reading faster (3rd
ed.). White Plains, NY: Pearson.
Nakanishi, T. (2015). A meta-analysis of extensive reading research. Supporting Information
TESOL Quarterly, 49(1), 6–37. doi:10.1002/tesq.157
Nation, I.S.P. (2009). Teaching ESL/EFL reading and writing. New Additional supporting information may be found in the
York, NY: Routledge. online version of this article:
Nation, I.S.P. (2013). Learning vocabulary in another language. • Appendix A: The Five Extensive Reading
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Activities Implemented in the Study
Nation, I.S.P., & Wang, K. (1999). Graded readers and vocabulary.
Reading in a Foreign Language, 12(2), 355–380. • Appendix B: Reading Comprehension and Rate
Paran, A. (2003). English teaching essentials: Intensive reading. Test
English Teaching Professional, 28, 40. • Appendix C: Vocabulary Knowledge Test

The Effects of Extensive Reading on Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Vocabulary Acquisition | 89
Copyright of Reading Research Quarterly is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content
may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright
holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

Potrebbero piacerti anche