Sei sulla pagina 1di 16

Psychotherapy © 2013 American Psychological Association

2013, Vol. 50, No. 1, 52– 67 0033-3204/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0030898

Psychotherapy Outcome: An Issue Worth Re-Revisiting 50 Years Later


Louis G. Castonguay
Penn State University

Responding to an invitation to celebrate the 50th anniversary of the journal Psychotherapy, the goal of
this article is to describe three general ways by which the impact of psychotherapy might be improved:
(a) clinically, by encouraging the assimilation of empirically based principles of change and psychopa-
thology research into day-to-day practice; (b) empirically, by fostering process and outcome research
focused on a wide range of common factors and basic findings; as well as (c) clinically and empirically,
by facilitating active collaboration of practitioners and researchers in various types of practice-oriented
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

research. Reflected in these three potential avenues of growth are the assumptions that (a) we can
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

improve our understanding and impact of psychotherapy by building on convergences and complemen-
tarities across different theoretical orientations, domains of research, and professional expertise, and that
(b) most of the clinical and research suggestions derived by such convergence and plurality may not
require drastic changes in the practice of many psychotherapists.

Keywords: psychotherapy process and outcome, evidence-based practice, practice-oriented research

With its first articles by Hans Strupp (1963, 1964) and Hans psychotherapy works, I would argue that its effect can be im-
Eysenck (1964) the journal Psychotherapy was born with a bang— proved—we can do more to retain some clients, help others further
and plenty of acrimonious fireworks (how exiting, and sad at the benefit from it, and perhaps more importantly, decrease negative
same time, it is to read two giants denounce the other’s view as effects. I would also suggest that one strategy to further enhance
“erroneous and misleading” or “irrelevant, incompetent and im- (as well as better understand) psychotherapy is to foster rapproche-
material”)! Fifty years later, the journal is still thriving, but it is ment (not only between theoretical orientations but also between
witnessing and contributing to a very different field. Much has clinicians and researchers) and to encourage methodological and
changed in half a century. Then, at least in comparison with what epistemological plurality in our quest to reduce psychological
we now know, there was no overwhelming empirical evidence that suffering. Answering an invitation to celebrate the 50th anniver-
psychotherapy (any kind of psychotherapy) works. As the first sary of Psychotherapy, the aim of this article is to address the
issue of Psychotherapy offers us a glimpse of these, debates were current outcome problem by providing clinical and empirical sug-
also raging between scholars of different orientations about the gestions derived, in large part, on my work on process and out-
conceptual and clinical merits of their preferred approaches (see come studies (in controlled and naturalistic settings), psychother-
Castonguay & Goldfried, 1994). apy integration, psychopathology, practice-research networks, and
Today, the efficacy and effectiveness of psychotherapy (at least training.
for the majority of clients who complete it) is no longer in question
(Lambert & Ogles, 2004), and the hostile debates between leaders How Can We Improve the Outcome of Therapy
of divergent schools have substantially decreased. Yet, to use the Clinically?
words of Strupp, I believe that we still have an “outcome prob-
lem.” As noted by Lambert and Ogles (2004), although “positive One way to improve our effectiveness as practitioners is to use
statements about psychotherapy can be made with more confi- and build upon the knowledge that ⬎60 years of psychotherapy
dence than ever before, it is still important to point out that average research has brought us. There are, in my view, different lines of
positive effects mask considerable variability in outcomes” (p. empirical evidence that can provide useful guidelines, if used
181). In addition, it is clear that many clients terminate therapy competently and flexibly, in our assessment, case formulation, as
prematurely (Clarkin & Levy, 2004) and that a non-negligible well as treatment implementation.
number of others deteriorate during treatment (Lambert & Ogles,
2004; Lilienfeld, 2007). Thus, although there is no more doubt that Effective and Harmful Treatments
To begin with, let me make a suggestion that many will consider
“de rigeur” or imperative, whereas others will perceive it as
clinically naïve, if not misguided. If one is to discuss psychother-
This article is based in part on a keynote address presented at the Annual
apy outcome, however, it seems inescapable for him/her to take a
meeting of L’Ordre des Psychologues du Québec, Québec (QC), October,
November 2010. The author is grateful for the feedback provided by James
position about empirically supported treatments (ESTs). My posi-
Boswell and Mark Hilsenroth on an earlier version of this article. tion is based on the assumption that it would be ill advised not to
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Louis G. pay attention to a large number of randomized clinical trials (RCT)
Castonguay, PhD, Department of Psychology, Penn State University, Uni- that have investigated the work of clinicians (most of them expe-
versity Park, PA 16802, USA. E-mail: lgc3@psu.edu rienced, highly trained, and carefully supervised) treating real

52
PSYCHOTHERAPY OUTCOME 53

clients suffering from debilitating conditions. I believe that, at the social interventions is not likely to be restricted to cognitive
minimum, these trials and the ESTs that were identified based on therapy. In the context of current pressures for accountability,
them have provided us with first lines of attack (FLA) for the psychodynamic, interpersonal, behavioral, and humanistic thera-
treatment of many clients with specific clinical problems. pists should push for the conduct of more RCTs comparing the
For some disorders (e.g., obsessive– compulsive disorder), the long-term impact of their preferred orientation to antidepressants,
EST outcome literature currently offers powerful, but limited, FLA as a politically and socially wise strategy to protect and enhance
recommendations, whereas for others, especially depression, the their practice.
list of these recommendations is wider, including behavior ther- In line with the proposed strategy of building our efforts to
apy, cognitive therapy, interpersonal therapy, brief dynamic and improve outcomes based on accumulated research, I would also
process-experiential therapies (Follette & Greenberg, 2005). Con- suggest that clinicians pay attention to non-EST empirical evi-
sidering the fact that depression is one of the most frequent dence to (a) possibly improve the implementation and impact of
problems encountered in clinical practice, the list of ESTs is a clear ESTs, (b) adapt ESTs for particular clients with specific disorders,
example of how outcome research can be viewed as an ally for and/or to (c) find potentially useful intervention strategies in the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

clinicians. It demonstrates that if they identify themselves with one absence of ESTs for particular clients. Briefly described here are
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of three major theoretical orientations (cognitive– behavioral, psy- clinical guidelines from two lines of non-EST research: Principles
chodynamic, and humanistic), practitioners will find in several of change and psychopathology.
treatment manuals specific interventions that (a) are consistent
with their preferred view of human functioning and change and
Principles of Change
that (b) can address problems experienced by a large percentage
of their clients (Castonguay, Schut, Constantino, & Halperin, As an effort to move beyond false dichotomies that frequently
1999). The list of ESTs for depression also indicates that if they emerge in discourses about the process of change (i.e., the impact
chose to, the same therapists could expand their FLA repertoire for of therapy is primarily due to techniques OR to relationship
depressed clients by receiving training in interpersonal therapy, a variables OR to client factors), Larry Beutler and I created a task
well-supported treatment that does not fit neatly (and may, thus, force aimed at consolidating our current empirical findings about
require additional training) into some of the most traditional par- what makes therapy work (Castonguay & Beutler, 2005b). Build-
adigms of contemporary psychotherapy. As will be described ing on two previous task forces (Chambless & Ollendick, 2001;
below, principles of change can be derived from such treatment Norcross, 2002), our initiative involved the pairing of influential
manuals and assimilated by therapists of different orientations. scholars from different theoretical orientations to review the evi-
The outcome literature has also provided us with a list of dence of three specific domains (participant characteristics, rela-
potentially harmful treatments (PHT, Lilienfeld, 2007) for a num- tionship variables, and technical factors), within four types of
ber of clinical problems. Paying close and open-minded attention disorders frequently encountered by clinicians (dysphoric, anxiety,
to such a list can provide practitioners with indispensable guide- personality, and substance use disorders). In addition, we asked
lines regarding interventions that they should avoid using (or use these scholars to derive from their review some principles of
very carefully, see Castonguay, Boswell, Constantino, Hill, & change that could inform therapist assessment, case formulation,
Goldfried, 2010). and treatment plans.
In addition to the current benefits that clinicians can derive from From this collaborative effort emerged 61 empirically based
the outcome literature (in terms of interventions to consider using principles of change (common or unique to specific clinical prob-
and avoiding), I would like to suggest that some studies that have lems) that could serve as guidelines of intervention for therapists
been conducted within the EST movement could in the future of most theoretical orientations. For example, examining the liter-
(assuming the correct political and social conditions) lead to one of ature on client variables in the treatment for anxiety disorders,
the most drastic changes in mental health practice— one that Newman, Crits-Christoph, Connolly Gibbons, and Erickson
would clearly benefit psychotherapists. Although we know that a (2005) concluded that clients with higher levels of impairment (in
large number of depressed clients respond well to several type of terms of severity, distress, Axis I comorbidity, interpersonal prob-
psychosocial and pharmacological treatments, we also know that lems, and social support perceived as being critical), as well as
the relapse rates of successfully treated depressed individuals is clients who perceived their relationship with their parents as neg-
very substantial (see LeMoult, Castonguay, McAleavey, & Joor- ative, are less likely to benefit from psychotherapy. Among the
man, in press). Some RCTs have demonstrated that cognitive possible implications of these findings are: (a) despite the fact that
therapy is not only equivalent in efficacy to medication for de- many cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) protocols have been
pression, but that it is also associated with less relapse (see Hollon empirically validated for anxiety disorders, therapists should be
& Dimidjian, 2009). Based on this empirical evidence, psycho- aware that some individuals suffering from these problems may
therapy scholars, researchers, and clinicians could make a strong require a longer treatment than the number of sessions that are
case (to insurance companies, governmental agencies, and clients) typically prescribed in current EST manuals, and that (b) some of
that the FLA for one of the most frequently treated clinical prob- anxious clients may benefit from an exploration and/or attempt to
lems should not be medication but a well-established form of resolve attachment issues (which are not a typical focus of tradi-
psychotherapy. Sadly, although the percentage of depressed out- tional CBT for anxiety disorders).
patients treated with medication remained above 70% from 1998 As an example of a principle of change related to the treatment
to 2007, the percentage who received psychotherapy decreased of depression, Castonguay et al. (2005) concluded that therapist
from 53.6% to 43.1% during the same period (Marcus & Olfson, self-disclosure is likely to be helpful in the treatment of depression,
2010). I would also argue that the long-term advantage of psycho- but also suggest that therapists may consider using supportive and
54 CASTONGUAY

reassuring self-disclosures rather than challenging ones. Another are common and unique to different approaches), Strupp stated:
relationship-based principle derived for the treatment of depres- “These are staggering research problems, and the available re-
sion, as well as for the other three clinical problems targeted by the search evidence by and large is insufficient” (p. 2). As described
Task Force, was that therapists should strive to establish and below, the current gap in empirical knowledge about therapist
maintain a good working alliance. Complementing this general characteristics reflects what could be viewed as a most deplorable
principle, it should be noted that a substantial empirical literature paradox in the current status of psychotherapy research—what this
has led to complementary knowledge about the alliance, including also reflects, needless to say, is how ahead of his time Strupp was
the findings that it can be predicted by both client and therapist when he wrote the first article ever published in Psychotherapy.
characteristics and behaviors. Although the clinical implications of
these and other findings about the alliance have been described
Psychopathology Research
elsewhere (Castonguay, Constantino, & Grosse-Holtforth, 2006),
it suffices to say that there is no evidence to support therapists Basic research is another source of empirical evidence that can
being disimissive or hostile toward their clients, or that they should be used by clinicians to potentially improve treatment effective-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ignore alliance ruptures as they emerge, as well as the variables ness. Based on the belief that one way of increasing our under-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

that could foster, prevent, or repair them. standing of complex phenomena is to foster connections between
A number of principles of change related to techniques were different domains of knowledge, Tom Oltmanns and I have edited
also derived by the Task Force. Using the treatment of depression a book aimed at weaving one type of basic research, that is,
as an example, these include challenging clients’ cognitions, in- psychopathology, into clinical practice (Castonguay & Oltmanns,
creasing positive reinforcement, improving interpersonal function- in press). In addition to providing the field with expert reviews of
ing, improving marital, family, and social environments, and im- research on the nature and etiology of psychological problems, we
proving emotional awareness, acceptance, and regulation (Follette set out to tackle the difficult, but exciting, challenge of deriving
& Greenberg, 2005). These empirically based principles are suf- clinical implications from research on typical symptoms, clinical
ficiently specific to suggest a clear focus of interventions, yet they features, prevalence, course, comorbidity, onset, and risk factors
are general enough to avoid restricting clinicians to a limited for the most commonly treated disorders in clinical practice. To-
number of interventions to achieve their intended tasks or goals. ward these goals, we built teams of visible scholars, some of them
For example, several techniques can be used to facilitate change in recognized for their expertise in psychopathology, others for their
clients’ beliefs, including interpretation, reformulation, and cogni- research in psychotherapy, and many for their contributions to both
tive restructuring. In line with a complementary (and theoretically of these areas of knowledge. As an attempt to enrich and expand
driven) list of principles of change described below, each of these upon current efforts toward evidence-based practice, each chapter
interventions can be used to foster the acquisition of a new per- relies on rigorous and distinctive sources of knowledge to offer
spective of self and others (Goldfried, 1980). Therapists, in other assessment and treatment guidelines that should not be restricted to
words, do not necessarily have to abandon their preferred orien- any one theoretical orientation.
tations in order to be guided by empirically based principles of Again using depression as an example, here are some of the
change. In addition, these principles of change can help clinicians clinical implications that have been derived from the basic re-
to refine or expand their technical interventions to potentially search on psychopathology (see LeMoult et al., in press, for more
improve their effectiveness in treating clients with depression. For comprehensive and detailed descriptions):
instance, therapists who typically focus on intrapersonal change (a) Despite not being always emphasized in the psychological
(but focusing on insight or cognitive reappraisal) might consider literature, vegetative and somatic symptoms should be carefully
increasing their attention on interpersonal and social changes in and systematically assessed; not only because of their relatively
their clients’ life, not only to improve outcome at the end of low base rate, but also because they may be indicative of depres-
treatment, but also possibly reduce the risk of relapse after the sion severity.
completion of therapy. (b) Although not part of the diagnostic criteria, anger is fre-
It should also be mentioned that many of the principles of quently associated with depression. Therapists should, thus, expect
change related to client characteristics, relationship variables, and the emergence of hostility in the therapeutic relationship and be
technical interventions that have been identified by the Task Force able to discern whether such emotion is a manifestation of depres-
have also been presented as a source of knowledge to potentially sion and/or the consequence of alliance ruptures. Having meta-
reduce harmful effects. Based in large part on these principles, communication skills (Safran & Muran, 2000) as part of their
negative factors and toxic processes have indeed been delineated technical repertoire may also help therapists to increase clients’
to complement the PHT outcome literature in helping graduate awareness (and decrease the negative impact) of their hostile
programs and supervisors to prevent or reduce deterioration that reactions to others—in the therapy room, as well as in their life.
has been observed in a non-negligible percentage of psychotherapy (c) In light of the serious risk of suicide, basic research provides
clients (Castonguay, Boswell, Constantino, et al., 2010). invaluable knowledge to clinicians about the factors that should be
However, what has largely failed to emerge from the same Task assessed when working with depressed clients (including panic
Force are principles of change related to therapist characteristics attacks, alcohol use, concentration, and insomnia).
(see Castonguay & Beutler, 2005a). This is particularly interesting (d) At the beginning of, during, and before terminating therapy,
considering the fact that therapist characteristics were highlighted clinicians would do well to assess clients’ work performance and
by Strupp (1963) 50 years ago as factors to be investigated to satisfaction, sexual functioning, health problems, and health care
better address the outcome problem. Referring to therapists’ level behaviors. Frequently associated with depression, these issues
of expertise, personality, and attitude (as well as techniques that should be addressed (by extensive treatment, specific interven-
PSYCHOTHERAPY OUTCOME 55

tions, and/or referral to other services) to increase the clients’ exercising. Promising findings suggest that such strategies can be
quality of life and possibly reduce the risk of relapse. added to traditional CBT for depression (Hayes, Beevers, Feldman,
(e) When treating depression, therapists should expect that the Laurenceau, & Perlman, 2005), and it is likely that therapists from
number of sessions typically prescribed in RCTs may not be other orientations might also be able to integrate them, flexibly and
sufficient for clients with early onset, high levels of comorbidity, appropriately, into their practice.
and numerous previous episodes. (i) Psychopathology research also shows that parental neglect and
(f) The sharp increase in the prevalence of depression within the abuse, as well as insecure parent– child attachment, are risk factors for
Westernized contemporary culture suggests that an overinvolve- depression. In line with the argument stated above that findings on the
ment in individualist pursuits (for personal accomplishment and/or alliance do not provide any evidence for therapists to be dismissive or
wealth) may reduce opportunities for social support during stress- hostile toward their clients, basic research suggests that the therapeu-
ful times. Clinically, this suggests that when working with depres- tic impact of an attentive, engaged, accepting, and affirming therapist
sion, therapists should not only correct maladaptive thoughts, might be due in part to the corrective experiences that such relational
foster awareness of unfinished business, and/or explore the past, attitudes offer, in contrast with the inadequate forms of parenting
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

but they should also help clients establish and deepen interpersonal many depressed clients experienced. The influence of adverse inter-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and social relationships. personal experiences from the past also suggests that therapists, irre-
Interestingly, this suggestion perfectly fits with some of the spective of their orientation, should be willing to ask about and
principles of change related to technical factors that, as mentioned explore early relationship events and their potential effect on later
above, were derived from psychotherapy research on the treatment relationships in clients’ lives.
of depression (i.e., improving interpersonal functioning, improving (j) Therapists should also be alerted that their clients’ depression
marital, family, and social environment). As noted elsewhere, may be maintained by two tendencies observed in psychopathol-
“such convergence of findings across different communities of ogy research: Depressed individuals excessively seek reassurance
knowledge may not be surprising considering the core aspects of and prefer to interact with others who confirm their negative view
human functioning to which they appear to be linked. As argued by of self. The use of strategies aimed at decreasing these specific
MacLean (1985), communication and play are two of the evolu- relational patterns may help depressed clients to create and main-
tionary developments that differentiate not only humans but all tain more meaningful and healthy interpersonal relationships. As
mammals from reptiles. Denying or not attending to such ways of already mentioned, pursuing this therapeutic goal has been sug-
being forces our clients and us to fight a losing evolutionary gested by different domains of research.
battle!” (Castonguay, 2011, p. 132). In concluding the first section of this article, I would like to
(g) Basic research on cognitive bias (attention, interpretation, suggest that the clinical implications related to empirically based
memory) and emotional regulation demonstrate that depressed principles of change and psychopathology research may not re-
individuals have difficulties disengaging from negative cognitions quire drastic changes in the practice of many clinicians in order to
and sad affect. These findings suggest that interventions that can potentially improve their outcomes. Specifically, most therapists
help clients to become unstuck from maladaptive patterns of would not have to change theoretical orientations in order to use
automatic reaction may be helpful in the treatment of depression. interventions that are consistent with the therapeutic guidelines
Interestingly, the results of basic research investigating the impact derived from these domains of research. I would also argue,
of some strategies to foster adaptive emotional regulation (reap- however, that empirical evidence, even when combining these two
praisal of negative events as opposed to rumination; emotional lines of research with the findings of the EST and PHT outcome
disclosure as opposed to emotional suppression) are consistent literature, is not sufficient, at this stage in our field, to fully
with two of the principles of change derived from psychotherapy improve our effectiveness. As argued elsewhere (Castonguay, Bo-
research (i.e., challenging clients’ cognitions, and improving emo- swell, Constantino, et al., 2010), this might especially be the case
tional awareness, acceptance, and regulation). However, another with regard to the urgent need to address what may be our most
strategy found in experimental labs to be helpful in emotional important outcome problem: Deterioration. To attend to our ethical
regulation, that is, distraction, does not appear to be featured duty of “first, do no harm,” it may be wise to pay attention to
predominantly in most ESTs for depression. Though it may well be different sources of knowledge: Empirical (recognizing the rigor
inappropriate at times and in certain situations, the use of distrac- and validity of diverse quantitative and qualitative methods), clin-
tion can complement our traditional interventions. As I’ve self- ical (recognizing the value of practitioner observations and inno-
disclosed elsewhere, “one of the authors of this chapter has learned vations as opposed to “letting the knowledge from practice drip
that blasting Beethoven’s 9th symphony on his iPod in response to through the holes of a colander,” Kazdin, 2008), and conceptual
pervasive and recurrent ruminations late at night, is not incompat- (recognizing that ideas and practices developed by major scholars
ible with (and may actually be a powerful adjunct to) verbally of theoretical traditions are not all captured in current textbooks
oriented forms of therapy. There are times to explore emotions and and manuals). Rather than being incompatible, these sources of
examine maladaptive cognitions, but there are also times when we knowledge frequently complement each other and can provide us
should get our mind away from them” (in LeMoult et al., in press, with a plurality of information and perspectives to better under-
p. 47). stand the complexity of psychotherapy (Castonguay, 2011). As
(h) Because basic biological and psychological research has dem- also previously argued (Castonguay, Boswell, Constantino, et al.,
onstrated the impact of stressful events on the onset and maintenance 2010), convergence among sources of knowledge (including be-
of depression, psychotherapists would be well-advised to include in tween research domains highlighted several times above) can be a
their clinical repertoire a variety of stress-regulating strategies aimed safe way to build our confidence about what is assumed to facil-
at, for example, increasing healthy patterns of sleeping, eating, and itate or interfere with therapeutic change.
56 CASTONGUAY

How Can We Improve the Outcome of Therapy of emotion) have been associated with positive outcome in these
Empirically? approaches (see Elliott, Greenberg, & Lietaer [2004] and Green-
berg & Pascual-Leone [2006] for reviews).
Let me begin this section, like the previous one, with a statement Interestingly, however, a recent meta-analysis failed to find a
that will be perceived by most as either imperative or naïve: Whereas significant relationship between therapists’ adherence and compe-
different lines of research have already provided guidelines for day- tence to theoretically prescribed interventions in different forms of
to-day practice, further studies on the process and outcome of psy- treatment (Webb, DeRubeis, & Barber, 2010). Moreover, some of
chotherapy are likely to help us improve the effectiveness of psycho- studies have found core components of both CBT and psychody-
social interventions. Although this may well sound like a truism, we namic therapies to be negatively related to improvement (e.g.,
would be ill advised to take for granted that in the current hegemony Castonguay, Goldfried, Wiser, Raue, & Hayes, 1996; Hayes, Cas-
of neurological and biological sciences, large numbers of psychother- tonguay, & Goldfried, 1996; Schut et al., 2005). Rather than
apy studies will be readably supported. It might be wise for psycho- indicating that central interventions of particular orientations are
therapy researchers, clinicians, and mental health administrators to powerless or detrimental, these findings are more likely suggesting
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

collaborate in advocating for the increase of public and private fund- that prescribed techniques, as well as the theoretical models that
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ing toward improving what have been shown to be powerful tools to they are built upon, may not be sufficient to explain change.
attenuate debilitating problems. I would also suggest that a particu- Current and future research could be particularly helpful here by
larly fruitful way to advance current research (let alone convince providing us with findings that could expand our understanding of
governmental and other agencies that it deserves to be further sup- change, which, in turn, could lead to improvements in outcome.
ported) is to build on our strong foundations. We already have Building on our conceptual and empirical foundations, we need
empirical evidence supporting major treatments (in terms of some of to look at the relationship between different variables (related to
their mechanisms of change and their impact for a majority of clients). technique, relationship, and participants’ characteristics) in facili-
And although we should refine these traditional models, we should go tating and/or interfering with the process of change. Fortunately,
beyond theoretical boundaries to further improve what psychotherapy some studies have begun to do this. For example, both descriptive
has to offer to mental health. This could be done at both process and (e.g., Castonguay et al., 1996; Piper et al., 1999) and quantitative
outcome levels. studies (e.g., Owen & Hilsenroth, 2011; Owen, Hilsenroth, &
Rodolfa, in press; Ryum, Stiles, Svartberg, & McCullough, 2010)
have suggested that a consideration of the therapeutic relationship
Process Research
(such as the quality of the alliance), within which prescribed
In order for us to improve outcomes, it seems important to better interventions are used, is critical in order to understand their
understand how therapy works, as well as what accounts for when potential impact on outcome. Furthermore, other studies have
it fails to work. In line with the plea made by Strupp (1963) half suggested that the frequent use of specific interventions (such as
a century ago, this requires research to examine the process of interpretation) may be particularly effective for some clients and
therapy. An important line of process research involves the inves- potentially detrimental for others (Crits-Christoph & Connolly
tigation of interventions and mechanisms of change that are at the Gibbons, 2002). Going one step further, a recent study investigat-
core of specific forms of therapy. At this point in time, empirical ing the interaction between treatment, client, and relationship
support has been found for several components assumed to be variables revealed unexpected findings. Specifically, Høglend et
responsible for change in particular approaches. For example, al. (2011) found that the “impact of transference interpretation on
consistent with a CBT model, the use of specific techniques such psychodynamic functioning was more positive within the context
as self-monitoring (DeRubeis & Feeley, 1990; Feeley, DeRubeis, of a weak therapeutic alliance for patients with low quality of
& Gelfand, 1999), homework (Burns & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991), object relations. For patients with more mature object relations and
and exposure (Foa, Huppert, & Cahill, 2006) have been positively high alliance, the authors observed a negative effect of transfer-
linked with outcome in this orientation. In addition, clients’ cog- ence work.” As further noted by the authors, “[t] he specific effects
nitive change and acquisition of compensatory skills, both hypoth- of transference work was influenced by the interaction of object
esized to mediate change in CBT treatments, have been shown to relations and alliance, but in the direct opposite direction of what
be predictive of improvement in this form of therapy (Barber & is generally maintained in mainstream clinical theory.” (p. 697).
DeRubeis, 2001; Garratt, Ingram, Rand, & Sawalani, 2007; Hof- Although this study does not provide definite answers about what
mann et al., 2007; Strunk, DeRubeis, Chiu, & Alvarez, 2007). facilitate or interfere with change in psychodynamic treatment
Several studies investigating the process of change in psychody- (Ryum et al. [2010], e.g., found that higher focus on transference
namic therapy have also found a significant relationship between work within the context of weaker alliance was associated with
the use of theoretically consistent techniques (such as interventions less therapeutic change), it is noteworthy in its attempt to capture
aimed at fostering emotional experience, addressing defenses, ex- the complexity of psychotherapy. Interestingly it also fits the final
ploring the past, or drawing connections between different rela- recommendation suggested by the Task Force described above. As
tionships in client’s life, including with his or her therapist) and we argued, “[p]articipant, relationship, and technique principles do
therapeutic change (e.g., Ablon & Jones, 1998; Connolly Gibbons not operate in isolation . . .. Perhaps the most difficult and exciting
et al., 2012; Gaston, Thompson, Gallagher, Cournoyer, & Gagon, challenge in psychotherapy research resides in the fact that prin-
1998; Hillsenroth, Ackerman, Blagys, Baity, & Mooney, 2003; ciples that are related to these three domains are in a constant flux
Ulvenes et al., 2012). Similarly, some of the core processes em- of interaction and interdependence. While some research has be-
phasized in humanistic and experiential treatments (such the ther- gun to delineate such complex relationships, much more needs to
apist’s empathy, as well as the client’s deepening and processing be done before we achieve an adequate understanding of how these
PSYCHOTHERAPY OUTCOME 57

factors work with, against, and within one another to enhance Holtforth & Flückiger, 2012; Ladany et al., 2012). In addition,
change” (Castonguay & Beutler, 2005b, p. 368). Ultimately, such each of the two series of conferences has led to a list of recom-
studies are likely to offer a new lens to observe and participate in mendations for future research that has begun to stimulate new
the therapeutic process, as well as innovative and discriminative studies. For example, McAleavey and Castonguay (2012) have
(what to use, when, and with whom) guidelines to improve our recently explored the important question of what interventions
effectiveness. (common or unique to specific orientations) might facilitate in-
Another way to go beyond our current theoretical models of change sight. Surprisingly, they found that the use of psychodynamic
(without repudiating them as inadequate foundations) is to fully techniques was negatively related to the acquisition of new per-
recognize the role and extent of factors that are common to many spectives about self and others. This is yet another example of how
approaches (as noted early in this article, Strupp [1963] emphasized research can challenge the field to modify and/or expand both our
the need for more research on unique and common factors in psy- models and practice to better understand change—and ultimately
chotherapy). A considerable amount of evidence has been collected to improve our outcomes.
support relationship factors (such as the alliance, empathy) that are In a similarly surprising and challenging way, another set of com-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

present in different forms of therapy (Norcross, 2002). What has also mon factors that reflects the complexity of the process of change has
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

been argued elsewhere (Castonguay, 1993), however, is that common been referred to elsewhere as “faux unique” (Castonguay, 2011). As
factors are not restricted to relationship variables. For example, Gold- for the general strategies aimed at facilitating new perspectives and
fried (1980; Goldfried & Padawer, 1982) identified five principles of corrective experiences, these “faux unique” components are not re-
change that cut across different orientations. Theoretically derived, stricted to relationship variables— challenging yet another false di-
these principles of change are complementary to the empirically chotomy that technical interventions are unique to specific approaches
derived principles described above, and whereas some of these prin- and that common (or what is sometimes called, misleadingly in my
ciples focus on relationship variables (expectancies and alliance), view, “nonspecific”) factors are interpersonal variables that are aux-
others focus on strategies of intervention. iliary to techniques (see Castonguay, 1993).
Interestingly, two of the principles of change identified by As reviewed by Blagys and Hilsenroth (2000), and consistent
Goldfried, the acquisition of a new perspective of self (or insight) with its underlying model, process studies have demonstrated that
and the facilitation of corrective experiences have been the focus in CBT there is less focus on affect, past experience, interpersonal
of long-standing collaborations between highly influential scholars relationships (including with the therapist), and avoidance or hin-
and researchers from different theoretical orientations and research dering of treatment progress than in psychodynamic therapy. In-
expertise (quantitative, qualitative, randomized clinical trials, nat- terestingly, however, process-outcome research also suggests that
uralistic investigations, and cases studies). This collaboration has CBT therapists do focus on these issues (even in RCTs) and that
taken place within the context of the Penn State Conferences on they should emphasize them more than they already do. This is
the process of change that my colleague Clara Hill and I have been because what has been linked with outcome in CBT are “heretic”
organizing since 2001. interventions such as: fostering emotional experiencing and ex-
These collaborative efforts have led to a conceptual clarification pression (Castonguay et al., 1996; Castonguay et al., 1998; Watson
of these principles of change from various traditions (CBT, psy- & Bedard, 2006), exploring attachment relationships with early
chodynamic, humanistic, systemic), as well as to consensus about significant others (Hayes et al., 1996), and a set of psychodynamic
their respective natures, the factors that facilitate them, and their techniques that include the exploration of the therapeutic relation-
consequences. Equally important, one of the missions of the Penn ship and defensive maneuvers (Ablon & Jones, 1998; Jones &
State conferences is to stimulate research by having several par- Pulos, 1993). Although not all interventions consistent with psy-
ticipants conduct quantitative and qualitative studies. In one of chodynamic theory have been found to predict outcome in CBT
these investigations, Hayes, Feldman, and Goldfried (2005) found (e.g, Ulvenes et al., 2012), these findings can shed new light on
that insight (exploration of issues leading to a new connection, how CBT works.
meaning, or shift in perspective) was associated with affective The surprising and challenging input that faux unique variables
arousal and hope, the expression of positive and negative views of can provide toward expanding our conceptualization of the process
self, as well as improvement. Reflecting convergence across do- of change is not restricted to CBT. As mentioned above, for
mains of research, this study also found that rumination was example, the acquisition of compensatory skills has been hypoth-
related to worse outcome—such a result is indeed consistent with esized as a specific mechanism to mediate change in CBT and, as
one of the basic findings related to strategies of emotional regu- predicted, has been related to outcome in this treatment. However,
lation described above. a recent study has also found it to be predictive of change in
Other studies presented in the two books that have emerged psychodynamic therapy (Connolly Gibbons et al., 2009).
from the first two series of conferences (Castonguay & Hill, 2005, Basic research can also enrich our understanding of the therapy
2012) have investigated how client narrative expression facilitates process, in part by providing unexpected (at least within the
(and is fostered by) clients’ new perspective of self (Angus & context of our current theories of psychotherapies) directions for
Hardtke, 2005), how such a new perspective (or insight) can improving treatment process and outcome. For example, consistent
develop when working with dreams (Hill et al., 2005), how clients with one of the clinical guidelines derived from psychopathology
view corrective experiences (Heatherington, Constantino, Fried- research described above, therapists may need to pay keen atten-
lander, Angus, & Messer, 2012; Knox, Hess, Hill, Crook-Lyon, & tion to clients’ self-needs. In one study, greater similarity between
Burkhard, 2012), and how these experiences take place in various client-perceived therapist behavior and client self-directed behav-
forms of therapy and in supervision (Anderson, Ogles, Heckman, ior (even when negative in nature) was associated with better
& MacFarlane, 2012; Berman et al., 2012; Castonguay et al., 2012; client-rated alliance early in CBT (Constantino et al., 2005). This
58 CASTONGUAY

finding has important conceptual and clinical implications, as it that are identical, with the exception of one aspect (which is predicted
alerts therapists that a premature emphasis on challenging negative to cause the expected difference between the two comparison condi-
self-conceptions may counteract an optimal balance of the client’s tions). Borkovec has argued that these specific comparative trials are
self-verification and self-enhancement needs (Constantino & Wes- unable to meet this scientific requirement because, fundamentally,
tra, 2012). As yet another fruitful contribution of the Penn State they are not comparing treatments that are equivalent in all-but-one
Conferences, suggestions for better understanding and improving factor (i.e., the specific techniques prescribed by the treatment man-
the process of change have also been derived from developmental uals). At the core of this argument is the fact that it is impossible to
(Bowman & Safran, 2005; Constantino & Westra, 2012) and social determine whether therapists have provided the prescribed treatments
psychology (Haverkamp & Tashior, 2005), as well as cognitive with the same level of competence and attunement to the clients’
and neural sciences (Caspar & Berger, 2005, 2012). Furthermore, needs, simply because the skills that are involved to achieve these
as a unique example of an insightful integration of scientific levels of competence and attunement are different in each treatment.
domains, Adele Hayes and her colleagues used dynamic systems Even if a researcher was to find that, for example, CBT and
and chaos theory to successfully predict that emotional and cog- psychodynamic-interpersonal (PI) therapists evidence the same aver-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

nitive destabilization would be linked with improvement in cog- age on adherence and competence scales, it would be misleading to
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

nitive therapy for depression (Hayes & Strauss, 1998). assume that these therapists were equally “pure” and competent in
To reiterate, these findings (as well as new directions from a implementing the treatment investigated. Because these scales would
diversity of scientific domains discussed above) do not suggest that by definition be assessing different interventions, there is no reason to
our current theories about psychotherapy are fundamentally flawed. believe that a score of say “4 out of 5” on a measure of CBT
What they suggest is that our current models of change are not competence is equivalent to the same score on a measure assessing PI.
sufficient and that process research that is not tied to the constructs of These scales are measuring apples and oranges.
a single psychotherapy model (or to psychotherapy at all) can help us Nevertheless, there are a number of situations where compara-
to expand and build upon such models. Although it is crucial to tive RCTs are relevant tools to address important issues related to
recognize the importance of, and continue to pursue, studies on factors psychotherapy outcome. One of these issues is scientific and
that are assumed to be at the core of particular forms of therapy, we political credibility. In the current state of our field, for example,
also need to think outside the theoretical boxes that have traditionally few will have more than tentative faith in a psychotherapy ap-
guided our treatments. As illustrated next, the knowledge that we can proach for obsessive– compulsive disorder until it is compared
derive from such an “outside” perspective may also fuel our efforts to with an established CBT protocol. In my opinion, a recent RCT
improve treatment outcome. has increased the credibility of psychodynamic therapy for depres-
sion by showing its equivalence to medication (as well as the
humbling lack of difference between these two forms of ESTs to
Outcome Research
a placebo condition; Barber, Barrett, Gallop, Rynn, & Rickels,
In his article for the first issue of Psychotherapy, Eysenck 2012). Furthermore, RCTs can be informative, research and
(1964) revealed some of the earliest roots of RCTs. In his own clinical-wise, by showing that a treatment similar to what is
words, it “is to be hoped that in the near future American psychol- frequently used in day-to-day practice can be as impactful, if not
ogists and psychiatrists will follow the example of their British and superior, to theoretically and technically sophisticated therapies
Commonwealth brethren and set up clinical trials to evaluate the designed by expert researchers. For example, Crits-Christoph et al.
adequacy of these two methods of therapy [psychotherapy and (2009) found that carefully conducted drug counseling was more
behavior therapy] against each other.” (p. 99). Although it seems effective than cognitive and dynamic therapies in the treatment of
that the field has, in large part, ignored what in hindsight appears cocaine addiction. (For other merits of RCTs, see Barber, 2009).
to be have been a crucial recommendation of Strupp (conducting However, I believe that to maximize the clinical impact of
more research examining the contribution of therapist characteris- outcome trials, including RCTs, the field should invest more
tics on outcome), a large segment of psychotherapy research has energy into increasing the efficacy and effectiveness of treatments
followed Eysenck’s wishes for more RCTs (despite having differ- that we know to be effective, rather than comparing them (and, as
ent impacts with respect to their specific recommendations, both described below, some of the studies aimed at this goal can
men were clearly ahead of their time!). actually get closer to establishing cause– effect relationships). This
There is no doubt in my mind that RCTs have given scientific strategy is line with the main theme of this article: building on our
credence to some forms of psychotherapy in the field of mental foundations. Also in line with this theme and consistent with the
health—a field that may constantly be at risk of being overshadowed research evidence described above, I would suggest that one of the
by the medical model and medications. I am also convinced that ways (by no means the only way) to improve our effective treat-
RCTs (in controlled contexts and, as described below, in naturalistic ments is to modify and/or expand on them based on process
settings) should continue to be conducted. However, what I think (common and faux-unique) and basic research.
should receive less attention (and, thus, less funding) are RCTs One example of such an attempt is a research program aimed at
comparing two forms of well-established therapies, especially for expanding cognitive therapy for the treatment of depression. As
disorders for which we already have ESTs. As argued by Borkovec mentioned earlier, my colleagues and I found that therapists’
(Borkovec, 1994; Borkovec & Castonguay, 1998), this is because emphasis on the CT model and techniques was negatively related
these specific comparative trials cannot provide a valid answer to the to outcome (Castonguay et al., 1996). Descriptive analyses sug-
ultimate scientific task that they are aimed at addressing, that is, gested, however, that what was problematic was not the prescribed
demonstrating a cause and effect relationship. Scientific studies in- interventions per se, but the context within which they were used.
vestigate cause and effect relationships by comparing two conditions Our analyses revealed that when confronted with alliance ruptures,
PSYCHOTHERAPY OUTCOME 59

therapists tended to increase their focus on aspects of (or interven- time-consuming research effort that my colleagues and I at Penn
tions prescribed by) the CT model that the clients were reluctant to State University have conducted, with the goal of improving the
accept or engage in. This strategy (also observed in psychody- efficacy of CBT for GAD (see Newman, Castonguay, Borkovec, &
namic therapy to address a similar problem; see Piper et al., 1999) Molnar, 2004). Although CBT was (and is still) the only EST for
did not appear to repair the alliance ruptures, and may even have GAD, an analysis of research trials (including a number of studies
exacerbated them. Based on these findings, we developed an conducted by Borkovec at Penn State) suggested that only 50% of
integrative cognitive therapy (ICT) where therapists use the tradi- the clients fully responded to this treatment (Borkovec & Wish-
tional CT model and interventions, except when confronted with man, 1996). Findings also collected at Penn State further indicated
an alliance rupture. In these circumstances, therapists follow that clients who experienced interpersonal problems at the end of
guidelines specified by both Burns (see Burns & Auerbach, 1996) CBT showed a higher probability of relapse (Borkovec, Newman,
and Safran (see Safran & Segal, 1990), and use interventions Pincus, & Lytle, 2002). Rather than creating a new treatment for
consistent with humanistic, psychodynamic, and interpersonal GAD, my colleagues and I embarked on the task of improving the
models (e.g., exploration of the therapist’s contribution to the efficacy and therapeutic reach of CBT by building on two distinct,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

relationship difficulties). Once the rupture is repaired, the therapist but complementary, scientific traditions: basic research (e.g., psy-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

then returns to the CT protocol, using the same techniques in a way chopathology findings that individuals with GAD have had past
that is experienced less negatively by the client or by using another and current interpersonal problems, as well as difficulty with
intervention prescribed by CT that is more attuned to the client’s experiencing emotions; see Newman et al., 2004) and process
needs. Although preliminary, two studies have been conducted so research (e.g., that the exploration of interpersonal, developmental,
far indicating that the integration of non-CBT interventions to deal and emotional issues have been found to be infrequent, yet pre-
with alliance ruptures in CT for depression is not only possible, but dictive of outcome in CBT).
may lead to incremental efficacy (Castonguay et al., 2004; Con- Influenced by the work of Safran and Segal (1990), we created
stantino et al., 2008). There is, of course, no reason to believe that an integrative treatment where a number of interpersonal (e.g.,
strategies to repair alliance ruptures would be irreconcilable with exploration of the therapeutic relationship), psychodynamic (e.g.,
or irrelevant to the practice of other forms of therapy. exploration of attachment issues with early significant others), and
Additional efforts aimed at expanding CBT have also led to humanistic (e.g., experiential techniques to facilitate acceptance
promising results. These include the integration of techniques to and deepening of emotion) interventions were added to CBT. In
increase clients’ outcome expectations (a process variable that has our first study, we found that the addition of these techniques
received empirical support and has been identified by Goldfried (within a treatment manual called interpersonal/emotional process-
[1980] as general principle of change) in the treatment of depres- ing [I/EP]) to a CBT protocol led to higher effect sizes than the
sion (Constantino, Klein, Smith-Hansen, & Greenberg, 2009); the average found in previous trials on traditional CBT (Newman,
addition of motivational interviewing interventions (which are Castonguay, Borkovec, Fisher, & Nordberg, 2008). Based on these
based substantially on client-centered constructs that have also preliminary, but promising, results, we conducted an RCT com-
received empirical support) in the treatment of generalized anxiety paring (via an additive design) two treatments: our integrative
disorder (GAD) (Westra, Arkowitz, & Dozois, 2009); as well as therapy (involving 14 2-hr sessions sequentially combining a 50-
the integration of interventions aimed at fostering functional sta- min segment of CBT and a 50-min segment of I/EP) and a control
bility (guided, in part, by basic research on the role of healthy sleep condition (involving a 50-min segment of CBT and a 50-min
and exercise) and emotional processing (based on the process segment of supportive listening, aimed at controlling for common
research mentioned above on affective and cognitive destabiliza- factors such as time and therapist attention).1
tion) in the treatment of depression (Grosse Holtforth et al., 2011; Contrary to our prediction, however, the two conditions failed to
Hayes et al., 2007). Such efforts to expand effective treatments be statistically different (Newman et al., 2011). Although these
should by no means be restricted to CBT. Although preliminary, disappointing results could be accounted for by a lack of power to
Nelson and Castonguay (2012) conducted a study that showed that detect real differences, I believe that they likely also reflect the
it is both possible (in terms of process) and promising (in terms of impact of moderators. Put in a larger context, the most important
outcome) for PI therapists to systematically integrate homework (a question, scientifically and clinically speaking, is not whether
therapeutic activity that, as mentioned above, has been associated integrative treatments (this one, as well as the innovative therapies
with and found to be predictive of change in CBT) into their described earlier in this section) are more effective than traditional
treatment of depression. ones, but for whom these integrative approaches are a better
As with the evidence related to principles of change and psy- option. Considering the fact that previous studies indicate that 50%
chopathology mentioned above, the use of these new treatments
would not require drastic change in the practice of many therapists.
1
In line with facets of the current integration movement in psycho- In line with the arguments stated above, we used this additive design
therapy, the above findings suggest that one way of improving our for scientific purposes—i.e., to investigate a cause and effect relationship.
In contrast with traditional comparative-outcome studies, we included the two
outcome is not to force a choice between different treatments (or conditions tested in our second study with the aim of having them be equiv-
to invent new ones) but to assimilate (Messer, 2001) within exist- alent in all respects, with the exception of the addition of specific non-CBT
ing therapies constructs and techniques associated with other ori- techniques. Accordingly, if we were to find the predicted difference between
entations (and/or scientific domains). However, such assimilative these two conditions, this finding would have suggested not only that CBT can
be improved but, more importantly from a scientific perspective, that the cause
treatments are unlikely to be a panacea and may turn out to be only of this improvement (above and beyond common factors, such as time and
a step in our continual quest to increase our therapeutic impact. therapist attention) was the addition of interpersonal, psychodynamic, and
This is a sobering conclusion that I have derived from a long and humanistic interventions to the CBT protocol.
60 CASTONGUAY

of GAD clients benefit from CBT, for example, outcome research Although they address a wide array of topics and involve a
would be most helpful if it was to identify the clients who are most diversity of research methods, POR studies share two important
likely to benefit from an already established EST, and those who features: They are conducted in naturalistic settings and they are
are most likely to need more than our current FLA to fully recover. based on standardized measurement systems used as part of rou-
Based on findings that negatively perceived parenting has been tine clinical practice. In addition, POR studies, as a whole, are
associated with negative outcome in the treatment of anxiety guided by the unifying goals of providing opportunities for clini-
disorders, it may be that clients with attachment problems could cians to (a) be active participants in research, (b) use data collected
further benefit from the exploration of past and current relation- in their setting to inform their own practice, (c) examine questions
ships than those without such difficulties. In line with this hypoth- that they see as clinically relevant, and (d) contribute to advance-
esis, a recent qualitative analysis conducted on the treatment of a ment of scientific knowledge. Recently reviewed under three ma-
client who responded positively in this RCT suggested that while jor approaches (practice-based research, patient-focused research,
CBT brought significant change in handling stressful situations and practice-research networks, Castonguay et al., in press), a few
and reducing somatic symptoms, I/EP facilitated complementary examples of POR are described here.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(and apparently synergistic) improvement (Castonguay et al., Perhaps the most well-known patient-focused, if not POR, stud-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

2012). The client’s exploration of emotional conflicts (in terms of ies have focused on feedback. Propelled by the seminal work of
wishes and fears) toward others (including therapist, spouse, and Michael Lambert, this research is particularly noteworthy, as it can
children), some of the traumatic and developmental sources of help us meet what I referred to earlier as our most important
these conflicts, as well as his maladaptive patterns of relationship clinical responsibility of “first, do no harm.” Although the field has
(as manifested in repeated alliance ruptures) appear to have helped long been alerted that a significant number of clients deteriorate
him better identify his interpersonal needs and change his way of during therapy (Bergin, 1966), Lambert’s research has demon-
relating with others (and himself). But although this specific client strated that clinicians tend to underestimate the number of their
might have benefitted from I/EP, the exploration of emotions and own clients who will deteriorate during treatment and have diffi-
interpersonal issues might not be necessary, or may even interfere culty predicting, at the beginning of therapy, the clients who will
with the impact of CBT for other clients with GAD. In fact, an do so. Research has also shown, however, that the use of feedback
accurate assessment of the complexity of therapy may require us to informing therapists about who is not progressing as expected can
recognize that “pure” and straightforward treatments are some- help reduce deterioration. Moreover, by providing clinical tools,
times as good, if not better, than combined and more complicated including information based on research on alliance rupture and
ones. repair, therapists can further decrease negative effects (see Lam-
bert, 2010 for review). An obvious clinical implication of such a
combination of process and outcome findings is that, irrespective
How Can We Improve the Outcome of Therapy of their theoretical orientation, therapists should seriously consider
Clinically and Empirically? using alliance (and outcome) measures and attempt improve the
alliance when indicated and/or adjust their interventions based on
For the most part, many of the clinical and empirical suggestions the quality and solidity of the therapeutic relationship. Regarding
presented above reflect an integration of different sources of the issue of the therapeutic relationship, it is interesting how
knowledge, and several of them emerge from active collaborations empirical evidence and clinical observations can provide conver-
between researchers of different worldviews (such as the Task gent and complementary knowledge that can help us reduce harm-
Force on principles of change, the book on psychopathology, and ful effects, and, thereby, improve our outcomes. In 1913, way
the Penn State conferences). To a large extent, however, these before any research was ever conducted on the alliance, Freud
integrative efforts reflect what has been described elsewhere as addressed the important question of when therapists should use
empirical imperialism (Castonguay, Boswell, Zack, et al., 2010). interpretation in psychoanalysis. He argued that this should not be
Like most current avenues of evidence-based practice, they repre- done before a good rapport has been established with the client. He
sent unidirectional attempts in building bridges between science then stated that this is especially the case for accurate interpreta-
and clinical work, with individuals (like me) who see few clients tions, adding that “usually the therapeutic effect at the moment is
informing individuals who see many more clients about what nothing; the resulting horror of analysis, however, is ineradicable.”
should be investigated (and how it should be investigated) in order (p. 187).
for the field to better understand and improve psychotherapy. In terms of practice-based research, what appears to have
These efforts are no doubt worthy of consideration, but I would retained most attention in the field are studies on the therapist
like to suggest that they should be complemented by another form effect. Reflecting, in no small way, Bruce Wampold’s major
of collaboration, one between clinicians and researchers. Referred contribution (e.g., Wampold, 2001), we have clear evidence sup-
to as “Practice-Oriented Research” (POR, Castonguay, Barkham, porting what most trainers and supervisors (let alone practitioners)
Lutz, & McAleavey, in press), this type of collaborative partner- believe: Some therapists are better than others. As noted by Lam-
ship has already led to a very large number of studies that have the bert and Ogles (2004), the “therapist factor, as a contributor to
potential to provide innovative contributions toward the current outcome, looms large in the assessment of outcome. Some thera-
outcome problem. Rather than being irreconcilable, practice- pists appear to be unusually effective, while others may not even
oriented research and evidence-based research can be viewed as help the majority of patient who seek their services” (p. 181).
equipoise and complementary strategies in building a robust Interestingly, the findings of a recent study suggest that the ther-
knowledge base and improving clinical practice (Barkham & Mar- apist effect may be quite specific. Based on a sample of 696
gison, 2007; Barkham, Stiles, Lambert, & Mellor-Clark, 2010). therapists and 6,960 clients, Kraus, Castonguay, Boswell, and
PSYCHOTHERAPY OUTCOME 61

Nordberg (2011) found that when their clients’ outcomes were university counseling centers, another study was conducted to
assessed with an instrument that measured several clinical prob- determine whether clients’ utilization of previous mental health
lems and dimensions of functioning, only one therapist was found services (and its link with symptom change) could provide useful
to be effective across all areas (interestingly, very few therapists information to clinicians in terms of their case formulation and
were found to be ineffective on all of problem areas measured). treatment planning (Boswell, McAleavey, Castonguay, Hayes, &
This study suggested that, on average, therapists are effective at Locke, 2012).
treating five or six clinical problem areas. The findings also revealed A number of studies have also been conducted at a PRN that has
that therapist effectiveness did not correlate highly between the dif- been developed in the training clinic at Penn State University.
ferent problems measured. In contrast with the notion that a few Conducted by and for students, such studies have the potential to
therapists are better than most for all clients, these findings suggest optimize the scientific-practitioner model by combining (actually
that the majority of therapists are effective (96% of the therapists were confounding) its three main facets: research, clinical, and training
shown to be effective in treating at least one clinical domain), but the (as argued elsewhere, “simultaneous, seamless, and repeated inte-
competencies of individual practitioners may be specific to particular gration of science and practice activities as early as possible in a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

problem areas. Based on clinicians’ problem with predicting deterio- psychotherapist’s career might create an intellectual and emotional
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ration, I would venture to guess that most therapists would have [hopefully secure] attachment to principles and merits of the
difficulty identifying all, or even most of the clinical problems that Boulder model” [Castonguay, 2011, p. 135]). In one of these
they are particularly good (or bad) at treating. Especially if these studies, Boswell, Castonguay, and Wasserman (2010) showed that
results are replicated, the use of such feedback would not require when used by particular therapists and with particular clients,
drastic changes from most clinicians but could provide them specific cognitive– behavioral interventions can be associated with worse
feedback (positive and constructive) that could confirm, as well as impact. This is yet another example of how POR research could
challenge, their perception of their expertise, which, in turn, could help us address the crucial issue of harmful effects.
lead them to seek referrals, provide supervision, and/or receive addi- In addition to reviewing past and current studies conducted
tional specific training.2 within the POR paradigm, my colleagues and I have offered
POR conducted within the context of practice-research net-
recommendations to foster future clinician–researcher collabora-
works (PRNs) may well represent the optimal antidote for empir-
tions aimed at improving our understanding and the impact of
ical imperialism. When conducted under ideal conditions, such
therapy (Castonguay et al., in press). Among them is to “ask and
research involves the active collaboration of practitioners and
tell.” This can be done by inviting clinicians to present what they
researchers in all aspects of the investigation—from the choice of
consider, based on their practice, to be the issues about process and
the topic to study, the design and implementation of the research
outcome that have been largely ignored by researchers (e.g., Cas-
protocol, and the analysis and dissemination of the results. As
tonguay, Adam-Term, Cavanagh, et al., 2010) and by surveying
noted in Castonguay et al. (in press), this “collaboration aims to
practitioners about the factors that limit the effectiveness of ESTs,
foster a sense of equality, shared ownership, and mutual respect
as they have implemented them in their day-to-day work (Gold-
between researchers and clinicians, and promoting diversity of
fried, et al., 2012).
scholarship (i.e., different ways of understanding and investigating
complex phenomena). It also capitalizes on the complementary Another recommendation is for the field to move beyond efforts
expertise, knowledge, and experiences of each stakeholder to pro- to create “bridges” between science and practice. For many, the
vide unique opportunities for two-way learning to conduct studies growth pathway of the scientific-practitioner model typically in-
that are both clinically relevant and scientifically rigorous.” volves separate, and more or less distinct steps: Scholars (clini-
A relatively large number of PRNs have been developed within cians and/or researchers) formulate ideas and interventions, re-
the context of several professional organizations, to address spe- searchers test these constructs and procedures (frequently in
cific clinical problems, or to conduct scientifically rigorous and controlled situations), and efforts are then made to “translate” the
clinically meaningful research in a variety of treatment settings. scientific results in naturalistic settings. Although this traditional
For example, as part of a series of studies conducted within the pathway has provided, at least in my view, multiple contributions
Pennsylvannia Psychological Association Practice Research Net- to field, it may also explain in part the slow and limited “up-take”
work (PPA PRN), experienced therapists designed and imple- of published research by clinicians. One way to facilitate more
mented, with their own private clients, a study aimed at identifying efficient and widespread use of research findings in day-to-day
what clients found most helpful and hindering during every session psychotherapy is to melt down the distinction between scientific
of their therapy (Castonguay, Boswell, Zack, et al., 2010). As and practice activities. This can be achieved, to a lesser or greater
described in Castonguay, Nelson, Boutselis, et al., (2010), extent, by designing “clinically syntonic” studies; studies involv-
therapist-investigators used the study protocol to collect data that ing tasks that are intrinsically and immediately relevant to clinical
seamlessly and simultaneously served empirical and clinical pur-
poses, thereby allowing them to confound science and practice. In 2
Interestingly, being able to identify therapists who are particularly
another study focused on the treatment of disruptive behavioral effective at treating particular clinical problems does not tell us why they
problems (DBP) in children, clinicians and researchers built a are effective. This still leaves us confronted with what could be viewed as
research program aimed at assessing the use of principles of one of the most intriguing conceptual, clinical, and research paradoxes in
change underlying ESTs for DBP in clinical practice, as well as the psychotherapy today: We know that there is a therapist effect, but we do
not know what makes some therapists more effective than others, in
link between these evidence-based (and practitioner-based) inter- general and for specific problems. Following the exploration of insight and
ventions and treatment outcome (Garland, Hurlburt, & Hawley, corrective experiences, the goal of the current series of Penn State Con-
2006). Within a different PRN infrastructure involving ⬎150 ferences on the process of change is to address this knowledge gap.
62 CASTONGUAY

work (Castonguay, Nelson, Boutselis, et al., 2010). I believe that the clinical repertoire of therapists of different theoretical orienta-
clinicians will be particularly interested in participating and using tions, and could help them address problems and impasses that
research if they are not able to differentiate between the scientific they are confronted with when treating a wide varieties of clinical
and clinical aspects of the study protocol. This is what happened, problems; and (d) that would plan for both quantitative (including
for example, in the PPA PRN study described above, when clini- the investigation of moderators and mediators) and qualitative
cians were reading their clients’ descriptions of helpful and hin- analyses to examine the role of client, therapist, technical, and
dering events that took place during the session that just ended—at relationship variables (as well as their interaction) in facilitating
that precise moment, clinicians were unable to say whether they and interfering with immediate, short-term, and long-term impact
were collecting data or preparing themselves for the next session. of the targeted interventions. However, I also hope to conduct a
They were, of course, doing both. As stated from a more global similar study within a controlled environment by investigating the
perspective, “rather than trying to connect science and practice, as addition of specific interventions to improve treatments already
if they stand on different river banks, we should strive to confound shown to be effective (EST). In my view, both types of studies are
the two activities in order to create a new, unified landscape of important because they reflect distinct research paradigms
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

knowledge and action” (Castonguay et al., in press). (practice-oriented and evidence-based) that have their respective
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

strengths and limitations (in terms of internal and external validity)


and, as mentioned above, they should be viewed as equivalent and
Conclusion
complementary in building an empirically based knowledge to
Even though 50 years have passed since the first articles were guide our practice (Barkham et al., 2010; Castonguay et al., in
published in Psychotherapy, most therapists and clinicians would press). Examining the same interventions in naturalistic and con-
agree that we still have an outcome problem—and that it is worth trolled environments would not only inform our level of confi-
revisiting it. Clinically and empirically, we know that a substantial dence in their impact and mechanisms, but would also shed greater
number of clients do not begin, complete, or benefit (fully or at all) light on their complexity, for example, under which conditions or
from the psychosocial treatments they are offered. Few would also context, with whom, and used in what ways might particular
disagree that considerable work can be done to increase our interventions improve psychotherapy outcome. Replication of re-
understanding of the process of change, as well as to improve, sults within a specific paradigm is also crucial. Each study has its
match, and deliver current treatments in ways that are more attuned own idiosyncrasies (in terms of participants, design, treatment
to our clients’ needs. Considering the complexity and importance protocols, etc.) and imperfections. When similar results are ob-
of these challenges, I strongly believe that we should build on our tained despite variations of methodologies and experimental mis-
empirical, conceptual, and clinical foundations, and that we can takes we can have more confidence in the reliability and validity of
and should do so by establishing connections between different the effects obtained. As an insightful researcher and clinician once
domains of knowledge and practice. As highlighted above, efforts said, “we cannot put much faith into one single study!” (Slodz,
toward such integration and collaboration can be based on several personal communication, November 2003).
convergences of findings and observations, as well as on comple- Although I can easily live with the fact that no one study is
mentary sources of expertise and knowledge. perfect, it is much more unsettling for me to accept that research
The assumption that building on plurality and convergences can is frequently perceived as irrelevant and intrusive by clinicians
improve psychotherapy outcome is based on two obvious prem- (Castonguay, Hayes, & Locke, 2011). With the hope of raising
ises: That this type of rapprochement can widen the scope of our a contrasting picture, I have attempted to describe how research
understanding and the repertoire of our practice, as well as increase can actually serve many important functions. It can provide us
our confidence in interventions or constructs that are supported by with a list of treatments that could be viewed as FLA, a list of
different perspectives of knowledge. The same assumption, how- treatments to avoid (or to be used very carefully), and a large
ever, comes with an unpleasant cost, at least for me: The loss of a number of principles of change that can complement these two
secret (and no doubt grandiose) dream that I (and the incalculable lists. Research can also provide us with lessons of humility by
number of more qualified researchers and practitioners) could, revealing, for example, that a serious percentage of our clients
with all the resources and time in the world, eventually design and deteriorate and that we are largely unable to predict who will be
conduct the “perfect” psychotherapy study—a conceptually ap- harmed during or as a result of therapy. On this basis alone, we
pealing, methodologically flawless, statistically impeccable, and can use research as an ally to improve ourselves—the same way
clinically relevant investigation that might shape the thinking, that, once we have established a good alliance, we tend to
practice, training, and research in the field. Of course, I do envi- encourage our clients to replace maladaptive patterns of living
sion (and hope to conduct) a few studies that I would consider with more healthy ones.
optimal (“best that I can do”) within my own research philosophy Perhaps parallel to the delicate balance of challenge and
and program. As one that could be deduced from the pages above, acceptance (as eloquently stated by Linehan, 1993) that we
at the top of this list would be an RCT that would be (a) designed, strive to achieve with our clients, research also provides us with
implemented, and disseminated in collaboration with clinicians, as many sources of support. For instance, it has increased our
well as researchers with a wide range of methodological/episte- confidence in the assertions of several theoreticians (including
mological and conceptual expertise; (b) based on an additive Freud, and, of course, Rogers) and the observations of many
design with the goal of testing the causal impact of training practitioners that the relationship matters. Furthermore, re-
therapists to use specific interventions in their day-to-day practice; search can surprise us by suggesting, for example, that the
(c) focused on interventions that would be derived from previous exploration of the past and emotion might be beneficial in CBT,
research (psychotherapy and/or basic), could be assimilated within which could, in turn, offer us potential directions for how to
PSYCHOTHERAPY OUTCOME 63

expand our clinical repertoire—at least with some clients. References


Moreover, research provides us with much needed political and
Ablon, J., & Jones, E. (1998). How expert clinician’s prototypes of an ideal
social “munitions” to survive and potentially strive in the heart
treatment correlate with outcome in psychodynamic and cognitive-
of the current pressures for accountability; not only by demon- behavioral therapy. Psychotherapy Research, 8, 71– 83.
strating that psychotherapy is effective for a large number of Anderson, T., Ogles, B. M., Heckman, B. D., & MacFarlane, P. (2012).
clinical problems but it may also offer the first line of attack for Varieties of corrective experiencing in context: A study of contrasts. In
the leading cause of disability in the world— depression (see L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Transformation in psychotherapy:
LeMoult, Castonguay, McAleavey, & Joorman, in press). As Corrective experiences across cognitive behavioral, humanistic, and
potential tools to address our current outcome problem, I have psychodynamic approaches (pp. 281–316). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
also argued that most of the research-based guidelines high-
Angus, L., & Hardtke, K. (2005). Margaret’s story: An intensive case
lighted in this article could be implemented without necessarily analysis of insight and narrative process change in client-centered
imposing drastic changes on the practice of clinicians, irrespec- psychotherapy. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Insight in
tive of their theoretical orientations. This can also be the case psychotherapy (pp. 187–205). Washington, DC: American Psychologi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

for numerous findings that have emerged from other psycho- cal Press.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

therapy (e.g., Castonguay, Muran, Angus, Hayes, Ladany, & Barber, J. P. (2009). Toward a working through of some conflicts in
Anderson, 2010; Straus, Baber, & Castonguay, in press) and psychotherapy research. Psychotherapy Research, 19, 1–12.
Barber, J. P., Barrett, M. S., Gallop, R., Rynn, M., & Rickels, K. (2012).
basic research programs (e.g., Constantino & Castonguay,
Short-term dynamic therapy vs. pharmacotherapy for major depressive
2003). disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 73, 66 –73. doi:10.4088/JCP
Despite what research can offer to day-to-day practice, it is .11m0683
clear that it has yet to substantially guide the work of many Barber, J. P., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2001). Change in compensatory skills in
clinicians. Interestingly, however, a survey of both clinicians cognitive therapy for depression. Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and
and researchers found that practitioners give more importance Research, 10, 8 –13.
to research than researchers give to clinical literature (Beutler, Barkham, M., & Margison, F. (2007). Practice-based evidence as a com-
plement to evidence-based practice: From dichotomy to chiasmus. In C.
Williams, Wakefield, & Entwistle, 1995). It is perhaps, there-
Freeman & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of evidence-based psychother-
fore, not surprising that some clinicians “feel disenfranchised apies: A guide for research and practice (pp. 443– 476). Chichester,
by researchers, believing that research often disregards their United Kingdom: Wiley.
realities and invalidates their experience as professionals” (Gar- Barkham, M., Stiles, W. B., Lambert, M. J., & Mellor-Clark, J. (2010).
land et al., 2006, p. 32). Similar to Kazdin’s (2008) denounce- Building a rigorous and relevant knowledge-base for the psychological
ment of the costly loss of clinical knowledge (through a drip- therapies. In M. Barkham, G. E. Hardy, & J. Mellor-Clark (Eds.),
ping colander), Beutler et al. (1995) warned that “scientists may Developing and delivering practice-based evidence: A guide for the psy-
chological therapies (pp. 21– 61). Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley.
be missing important avenues for identifying critical areas of
Bergin, A. E. (1966). Some implications of psychotherapy research for
research. They may do a better scientific job if they were more
therapeutic practice. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 71, 235–246.
attentive to the writings and ideas of their clinical colleagues” Berman, M., Hill, C. E., Liu, J., Jackson, J., Sim, W., & Spangler, P.
(pp. 989 –990). I am convinced that I and many of my col- (2012). Corrective relational events in the treatment of three cases of
leagues have become better researchers by asking clinicians anorexia nervosa. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Transfor-
what they need to know to avoid and better resolve the impasses mation in psychotherapy: Corrective experiences across cognitive
they struggle with, what they think might be ways to improve behavioral, humanistic, and psychodynamic approaches (pp. 215–
our treatments (including ESTs), and by collaborating with 244). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Beutler, L. E., Williams, R. E., Wakefield, P. J., & Entwistle, S. R. (1995).
them in the design, implementation, and dissemination of clin-
Bridging scientist and practitioner perspectives in clinical psychology.
ically relevant and scientifically rigorous research. American Psychologist, 50, 984 –994.
Many books attest that the collaboration between researchers Blagys, M. D., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2000). Distinctive features of short-term
from different theoretical orientations and scientific domains psychodynamic-interpersonal psychotherapy: A review of the comparative
can lead to fresh and innovative contributions of knowledge, psychotherapy process literature. Clinical Psychology: Science & Prac-
but so can a full partnership between researchers and clinicians. tice, 7, 167–188.
Needless to say, such an alliance might be more difficult to Borkovec, T. D. (1994). Between-group therapy outcome design: Design
and methodology. In L. S. Onken & J. D. Blaine (Eds.), NIDA research
build because the potential partners live in different
monograph no. 137 (pp. 249 –289). Rockville, MD: National Institute on
worlds—we face different pressures, are engaged in different Drug Abuse
tasks, and are motivated, to a large extent, by different incen- Borkovec, T. D., & Castonguay, L.G. (1998). What is the scientific
tives. However, as many beautiful songs, great novels and, if we meaning of empirically supported therapy? Journal of Consulting and
are lucky enough, personal experiences remind us, improbable Clinical Psychology, 66, 136 –142.
unions can also lead to beautiful and deeply gratifying relation- Borkovec, T. D., Newman, M. G., Pincus, A. L., & Lytle, R. (2002). A
ships. Such unions are worth establishing, enhancing, and, like component analysis of cognitive-behavioral therapy for generalized anx-
iety disorder and the role of interpersonal problems. Journal of Consult-
the outcome problem, revisiting. It is with this in mind that I
ing and Clinical Psychology, 70, 288 –298.
would like to reconnect with my own ancestral French culture, Borkovec, T. D., & Wishman, M. A. (1996). Psychological treatment for
paraphrase Jacques Brel, and end this article by telling clini- generalized anxiety disorder. In M. R., Mavissakalian & R. F. Prien
cians and researchers “ne nous quittons pas”—let’s stay to- (Eds.), Long-term treatment of anxiety disorders. Washington, DC:
gether. American Psychiatric Association.
64 CASTONGUAY

Boswell, J. F., Castonguay, L. G., & Wasserman, R. H. (2010). Effects of parison of unique and common factors. Journal of Consulting and
psychotherapy training and intervention use on session outcome. Jour- Clinical Psychology, 64, 497–504.
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 717–723. Castonguay, L. G., Grosse Holtforth, M., Coombs, M. M., Beberman,
Boswell, J. F., McAleavey, A. A., Castonguay, L. G., Hayes, J. A., & R. A., Kakouros, A. A., Boswell, J. F., . . . Jones, E. E. (2005).
Locke, B. D. (2012). The effect of previous mental health service Relationship factors in treating dysphoric disorders. In L. G. Castonguay
utilization on change in counseling clients’ depressive symptoms. Jour- & L. E. Beutler (Eds.), Principles of therapeutic change that work (pp.
nal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 368 –378. 65– 81). New York: Oxford University Press.
Bowman, E. A., & Safran, J. D. (2005). An integrated developmental Castonguay, L. G., & Hill, C. E. (Eds.) (2005). Insight in psychotherapy.
perspective on insight. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Insight Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.
in psychotherapy (pp. 401– 421). Washington, DC: American Psycho- Castonguay, L. G., & Hill, C. E. (Eds.). (2012). Transformation in psy-
logical Press. chotherapy: Corrective experiences across cognitive-behavioral, hu-
Burns, D. D., & Auerbach, A. (1996). Therapeutic empathy in cognitive- manistic, and psychodynamic approaches. Washington, DC: American
behavioral therapy: Does it really make a difference? In P. M. Salkovs- Psychological Association Press.
kis (Ed.), Frontiers of cognitive therapy (pp. 135–164). New York: Castonguay, L. G., Locke, B. D., & Hayes, J. A. (2011). The center for
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Guilford Press. collegiate mental health: An example of a practice-research network in


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Burns, D. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Copying styles, homework university counseling centers. Journal of College Student Psychother-
assignments and the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral therapy. Jour- apy, 25, 105–119.
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 305–311. Castonguay, L. G., Muran, J. C., Angus, L., Hayes, J. H., Ladany, N., &
Caspar, F., & Berger, T. (2005). Insight and cognitive psychology. In L. G. Anderson, T. (Eds.) (2010). Bringing psychotherapy research to life:
Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Insight in psychotherapy (pp. 375–399). Understanding change through the work of leading clinical researchers.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Press. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.
Caspar, F., & Berger, T. (2012). Corrective experiences: What can we learn Castonguay, L. G., Nelson, D. L., Boswell, J. F., Nordberg, S., McAleavey,
from different models and research in basic psychology? In L. G. A., Newman, M., & Borkovec, T. (2012). Corrective experiences in
Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Transformation in psychotherapy: cognitive behavioral and interpersonal/emotional processing therapies:
Corrective experiences across cognitive behavioral, humanistic, and A qualitative analysis of a single case. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill
psychodynamic approaches (pp. 141–157). Washington, DC: American (Eds.), Transformation in psychotherapy: Corrective experiences across
Psychological Association. cognitive behavioral, humanistic, and psychodynamic approaches (pp.
Castonguay, L. G. (1993). “Common factors” and “nonspecific variables”: 245–279). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Clarification of the two concepts and recommendations for research. Castonguay, L., Nelson, D., Boutselis, M,. Chiswick, N., Damer, D.,
Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 3, 267–286. Hemmelstein, N., . . . Borkovec, T. (2010). Clinicians and/or research-
Castonguay, L. G. (2011). Psychotherapy, psychopathology, research and ers? A qualitative analysis of therapists’ experiences in a Practice
practice: Pathways of connections and integration. Psychotherapy Re- Research Network. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, and
search, 21, 125–140. Training, 47, 345–354.
Castonguay, L. G., Adam-Term, R., Cavanagh, T., Hill, B., Johnson, B., Castonguay, L. G., & Oltmanns, T. F. (Eds.) (In press). Psychopathology:
Magnavita, J., . . . Spayd, C. (2010, June). What do clinicians think about Bridging the gap between basic empirical findings and clinical practice.
research? Structured discussion presented at the meeting of the Society New York: Guilford Press.
for Psychotherapy Research, Asilomar, CA. Castonguay, L. G., Pincus, A. L., Agras, W. S., & Hines, C. E. (1998). The
Castonguay, L. G., Barkham, M., Lutz, W., & McAleavey, A. A. (In press). role of emotion in group cognitive-behavioral therapy for binge eating
Practice-oriented research: Approaches and application. In M. J. Lam- disorder: When things have to feel worst before they get better. Psycho-
bert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and be- therapy Research, 8, 225–238.
havior change (6th ed.). New York: Wiley. Castonguay, L. G., Schut, A. J., Aikins, D., Constantino, M. J., Lau-
Castonguay, L. G., & Beutler, L. E. (2005a). Common and unique princi- renceau, J. P., Bologh, L., & Burns, D. D. (2004). Integrative cognitive
ples of therapeutic change: What do we know and what do we need to therapy: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Psychotherapy Integra-
know? In L. G. Castonguay & L. E. Beutler (Eds.), Principles of tion, 14, 4 –20.
therapeutic change that work (pp. 353–369). New York: Oxford Uni- Castonguay, L. G., Schut, A. J., Constantino, M. J., & Halperin, G. S.
versity Press. (1999). Assessing the role of treatment manuals: Have they become
Castonguay, L. G., & Beutler, L. E. (Eds.) (2005b). Principles of thera- necessary but non-sufficient ingredients of change? Clinical Psychol-
peutic change that work. New York: Oxford University Press. ogy: Science and Practice, 6, 449 – 455.
Castonguay, L. G., Boswell, J. F., Constantino, M. J., Goldfried, M. R., & Chambless, D. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically supported
Hill, C. E. (2010). Training implications of harmful effects of psycho- psychological interventions: Controversies and evidence. Annual Review
logical treatments. American Psychologist, 65, 34 – 49. of Psychology, 52, 685–716.
Castonguay, L. G., Boswell, J. F., Zack, S., Baker, S., Boutselis, M., Clarkin, J. F., & Levy, K. N. (2004). The influence of client variables on
Chiswick, N., . . . Grosse Holtforth, M. (2010). Helpful and hindering psychotherapy. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook
events in psychotherapy: A practice research network study. Psycho- of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed., pp. 194 –226). New
therapy: Theory, Research, Practice, and Training, 47, 327–344. York: Wiley.
Castonguay, L. G., Constantino, M. J., & Holtforth, M. G. (2006). The Connolly Gibbons, M. B., Crits-Christoph, P., Barber, J. P., Wiltsey
working alliance: Where we are and where should we go? Psychother- Stirman, S., Gallop, R., Goldstein, L. A., . . . Ring-Kurtz, S. (2009).
apy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 43, 271–279. Unique and common mechanisms of change across cognitive and dy-
Castonguay, L. G., & Goldfried, M. R. (1994). Psychotherapy integration: namic psychotherapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
An idea whose time has come. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 3, 77, 801– 813.
159 –172. Connolly Gibbons, M. B., Thompson, S. M., Scott, K., Schauble, L. A.,
Castonguay, L. G., Goldfried, M. R., Wiser, S., Raue, P. J., & Hayes, A. H. Monney, T., Thompson, D., . . . Crits-Christoph, P. (2012). Supportive-
(1996). Predicting outcome in cognitive therapy for depression: A com- expressive dynamic psychotherapy in the community mental health
PSYCHOTHERAPY OUTCOME 65

system: A pilot effectiveness trial for the treatment of depression. in the treatment of depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Prac-
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 49, 303–316. tice, 14, 224 –239.
Constantino, M. J., & Castonguay, L. G. (2003). Learning from the basics: Gaston, L., Thompson, L., Gallagher, D., Cournoyer, L. G., & Gagnon, R.
Clinical implications of social, developmental, and cross-cultural study (1998). Alliance, technique, and their interactions in predicting outcome
of the self. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 13, 3– 8. of behavioral, cognitive, and brief dynamic therapy. Psychotherapy
Constantino, M. J., Castonguay, L. G., Angtuaco, L. A., Pincus, A. L., Research, 8, 190 –209.
Newman, M. G., & Borkovec, T. D. (2005, June). The impact of Goldfried, M. R. (1980). Toward the delineation of therapeutic change
interpersonal-intrapsychic complementarity on the development and principles. American Psychologist, 35, 991–999.
course of the therapeutic alliance. Paper presented at the 36th annual Goldfried, M. R., Newman, M. G., Castonguay, L. G., Fuertes, J. N.,
meeting of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, Montreal, Canada. Magnavita, J. J., Sobel, L., & Wolfe, A. W. (2012). On the dissemination
Constantino, M. J., Klein, R., Smith-Hansen, L., & Greenberg, R. P. (2009, of clinical experiences in using empirically supported treatments. Man-
October). Augmenting cognitive therapy for depression with an Expec- uscript in preparation.
tancy Enhancement module: Preliminary efficacy. Paper presented at the Goldfried, M. R., & Padawer, W. (1982). Current status and future direc-
meeting of the Canadian chapter of the Society for Psychotherapy tions in psychotherapy. In M. R. Goldfried (Ed.), Converging themes in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Research, Montreal, Canada. psychotherapy (pp. 3– 49). New York: Springer.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Constantino, M. J., Marnell, M., Haile, A. J., Kanther-Sista, S. N., Wol- Greenberg, L. S., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). Emotion in psychotherapy:
man, K., Zappert, L., & Arnow, B. A. (2008). Integrative cognitive A practice-friendly research review. Journal of Clinical Psychology: In
therapy for depression: A randomized pilot comparison. Psychotherapy: Session, 62, 611– 630.
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 45, 122–134. Grosse Holtforth, M., Hayes, A. M., Sutter, M., Wilm, K., Schmied, E.,
Constantino, M. J., & Westra, H. A. (2012). An expectancy-based Laurenceau, J. P., & Caspar, F. (2102). Fostering cognitive-emotional
approach to facilitating corrective experiences in psychotherapy. In processing in the treatment of depression: A preliminary investigation in
L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Transformation in psychotherapy: exposure-based cognitive therapy. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics,
Corrective experiences across cognitive behavioral, humanistic, and 81, 259 –260.
psychodynamic approaches (pp. 121–139). Washington, DC: American Haverkamp, B. E., & Tashiro, T. D. (2005). Cognitive structures and
Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/13747-008 motives as barriers to insight: Contributions from social cognition re-
Crits-Christoph, P., & Connolly Gibbons, M. B. (2002). Relational inter- search. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Insight in psychotherapy
pretations. In J. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: (pp. 355–374). Washington, DC: American Psychological Press.
Therapist contributions and responsiveness to patients (pp. 285–300). Hayes, A. M., Beevers, C. G., Feldman, G. C., Laurenceau, J.-P., Perlman,
New York: Oxford University Press. C. (2005). Avoidance and processing as predictors of symptom change
Crits-Christoph, P., Siqueland, L., Blaine, J., Frank, A., Luborsky, L., and positive growth in an integrative therapy for depression. Interna-
Onken, L. S., . . . Beck, A. T. (1999). Psychosocial treatments for tional Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, 111–122.
cocaine dependence: National Institute on Drug Abuse Collaborative Hayes, A. H., Castonguay, L. G., & Goldfried, M. R. (1996). The effec-
Cocaine Treatment Study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 493– tiveness of targeting the vulnerability factors of depression in cognitive
502. therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 623– 627.
DeRubeis, R. J., & Feeley, M. (1990). Determinants of change in cognitive Hayes, A. M., Feldman, G. C., & Goldfried, M. R. (2005). The change and
therapy for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 469 – 482. growth experiences scale: A measure of insight and emotional process-
Elliott, R., Greenberg, L. S., & Lietaer, G. (2004). Research on experiential ing. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Insight in psychotherapy
psychotherapy. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook (pp. 231–253). Washington, DC: American Psychological Press.
of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed., pp. 493–539). New Hayes, A. M., & Strauss, J. L. (1998). Dynamic systems theory as a
York: Wiley. paradigm for the study of change in psychotherapy: An application to
Eysenck, H. J. (1964). The outcome problem in psychotherapy: A reply. cognitive therapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 97–100. Psychology, 66, 939 –947.
Feeley, M., DeRubeis, R. J., & Gelfand, L. A. (1999). The temporal Heatherington, L., Constntino, M. J., Friedlander, M., Angus, L., &
relation of adherence and alliance to symptom change in cognitive Messer, S. B. (2012). Clients’ perspectives on corrective experiences in
therapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, psychotherapy. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Transformation
67, 578 –582. in psychotherapy: Corrective experiences across cognitive behavioral,
Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., & Cahill, S. P. (2006). Update on emotional humanistic, and psychodynamic approaches (pp. 161–190). Washing-
processing theory. In B. O. Rothbaum (Ed.), The nature and treatment ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
of pathological anxiety (pp. 3–24). New York: Guilford Press. Hill, C. E., Knox, S., Hess, S. A., Crook-Lyon, R. E., Goates-Jones,
Follette, W. C., & Greenberg, L. S. (2005). Techinque factors in treating M. K., & Sim, W. (2005). The attainment of insight in the Hill dream
dysphoric disorders. In L. G. Castonguay & L. E. Beutler (Eds.), model: A case study. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Insight
Principles of therapeutic change that work (pp. 83–109). New York: in psychotherapy (pp. 207–230). Washington, DC: American Psycho-
Oxford University Press. logical Press.
Freud, S. (1913). Further recommendations in the technique of psycho- Hillsenroth, M. J., Ackerman, S. J., Blagys, M. D., Baity, M. R., &
analysis: On beginning the treatment: The questions of the first com- Mooney, M. A. (2003). Short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy for
munications: The dynamics of the cure. In S. J. Ellam (Ed. & Trans.), depression: An examination of statistical, clinical significant, and
Freud’s technique papers: A contemporary perspective (pp. 165–191). technique-specific change. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191,
Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson (Original work published 1913). 349 –357.
Garland, A. F., Hurlburt, M. S., & Hawley, K. M. (2006). Examining Hofmann, S. G., Suvak, M. K., Barlow, D. H., Shear, M. K., Meuret, A. E.,
psychotherapy processes in a services research context. Clinical Psy- Rosenfeld, D., . . . Woods, S. W. (2007). Preliminary evidence for
chology: Science and Practice, 13, 30 – 46. cognitive mediation during cognitive behavioral therapy of panic disor-
Garratt, G., Ingram, R. E., Rand, K. L., & Sawalani, G. (2007). Cognitive der. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 374 –379.
processes in cognitive therapy: Evaluation of the mechanisms of change doi:10.1037/0022-006X.75.3.374
66 CASTONGUAY

Høglend, P., Hersoug, A. G., Bøgwald, K.-P., Amlo, S., Marble, A., Søbye, sion. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Psycho-
Ø., . . . Crits-Christoph, P. (2010). Effect of transference work in the therapy Research. Virginia Beach.
context of therapeutic alliance and quality of object relations. Journal of Newman, M. G., Castonguay, L. G., Borkovec, T. D., Fisher, A. J.,
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 697–706. Boswell, J. F., Szkodny, L., & Nordberg, S. (2011). A randomized
Hollon, S. D., & Dimidjian, S. (2009). Cognitive and behavioral treatment controlled trial of cognitive-behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety
of depression. In I. H. Gotlib & C. L. Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of disorder with integrated techniques from emotion-focused and interper-
depression (2nd ed., pp. 586 – 603). New York: Guilford Press. sonal therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79,
Holtforth, M. G., & Fluckiger, C. (2012). A consistency-theory perspective 171–181.
on corrective experiences: Two kinds of corrective experiences in action. Newman, M. G., Castonguay, L. G., Borkovec, T. D., Fisher, A. J., &
In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Transformation in psychother- Nordberg, S. (2008). An open trial of integrative therapy for generalized
apy: Corrective experiences across cognitive behavioral, humanistic, anxiety disorder. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, and
and psychodynamic approaches (pp. 317–333). Washington, DC: Amer- Training, 45, 135–147.
ican Psychological Association. Newman, M. G., Castonguay, L. G., Borkovec, T. D., & Molnar, C. (2004).
Jones, E. E., & Pulos, S. M. (1993). Comparing the process in psychody- Integrative therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

namic and cognitive-behavioral therapies. Journal of Consulting and C. L. Turk, & D. S. Mennin (Eds.), Generalized anxiety disorder:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Clinical Psychology, 61, 306 –316. Advances in research and practice (pp. 320 –350). New York: Guilford
Kazdin, A. E. (2008). Evidence-based treatment and practice: New oppor- Press.
tunities to bridge clinical research and practice, enhance the knowledge Newman, M. G., Crits-Christoph, P., Connolly Gibbons, M. B., & Erick-
base, and improve patient care. American Psychologist, 63, 146 –159. son, T. M. (2005). Participant factors in treating anxiety disorders. In
Knox, S., Hess, S., Hill, C. E., Crook-Lyon, R. E., & Burkhard, A. W. L. G. Castonguay & L. E. Beutler (Eds.), Principles of therapeutic
(2012). Corrective relational experiences of therapists or therapists-in- change that work (pp. 121–153). New York: Oxford University Press.
training. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Transformation in Norcross, J. C. (Ed.) (2002). Psychotherapy relationships that work: Ther-
psychotherapy: Corrective experiences across cognitive behavioral, hu- apist contributions and responsiveness to patient needs. New York:
manistic, and psychodynamic approaches (pp. 191–213). Washington, Oxford University Press.
Owen, J., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2011). Interaction between alliance and
DC: American Psychological Association.
technique predicting patient outcome during psychodynamic psycho-
Kraus, D. R., Castonguay, L. G., Boswell, J. F., Nordberg, S. S., & Hayes,
therapy. Journal of Nervous Mental Disease, 199, 384 –389.
J. A. (2011). Therapist effectiveness: Implications for accountability and
Owen, J., Hilsenroth, M. J., & Rodolfa, E. (In press). Interaction among
patient care. Psychotherapy Research, 21, 267–276.
alliance, psychodynamic-interpersonal and cognitive behavioural tech-
Ladany, N., Inman, A. G., Hill, C. E., Knox, S., Crook-Lyon, R., Thomp-
niques in the prediction of post-session change. Clinical Psychology and
son, B. J., . . . Walker, J. A. (2012). Corrective relational experiences in
Psychotherapy.
supervision. In L. G. Castonguay & C. E. Hill (Eds.), Transformation in
Piper, W. E., Joyce, A. S., Rosie, J. S., Ogrodniczuk, J. S., McCallum, M.,
psychotherapy: Corrective experiences across cognitive behavioral, hu-
O’Kelly, J. G., & Steinberg, P. I. (1999). Prediction of dropping out in
manistic, and psychodynamic approaches (pp. 335–352). Washington,
time-limited interpretive individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 36,
DC: American Psychological Association.
114 –122.
Lambert, M. J. (2010). Prevention of treatment failure: The use of mea-
Ryum, T., Stiles, T. D., Svartberg, M., & McCullough, L. (2010). The role
suring, monitoring, and feedback in clinical practice. Washington, DC:
of transference work, the therapeutic alliance, and their interaction in
American Psychological Association.
reducing interpersonal problems among psychotherapy patients with
Lambert, M. J., & Ogles, B. M. (2004). The efficacy and effectiveness of
cluster C personality disorders. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Prac-
psychotherapy. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Bergin and Garfield’s handbook tice, and Training, 47, 442– 453.
of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed., pp. 139 –193). New Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (2000). Negotiating the therapeutic alliance:
York: Wiley. A relational treatment guide. New York: Guilford Press.
LeMoult, J., Castonguay, L. G., McAleavey, A. A. & Joorman, J. (In Safran, J. D., & Segal, Z. V. (1990). Interpersonal process in cognitive
press). Depression: Basic research and clinical implications. In L. G. therapy. New York: Basic Books.
Castonguay & T. G. Oltmanns (Eds.), Psychopathology: Bridging the Schut, A. J., Castonguay, L. G., Bedics, J. D., Smith, T. L., Barber, J. P.,
gap between basic empirical findings and clinical practice. New York: Flanagan, K. M., & Yamasaki, A. S. (2005). Therapist interpretation,
Guilford Press. patient-therapist interpersonal process, and outcome in psychodynamic
Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Psychological treatments that cause harm. Per- psychotherapy for avoidant personality disorder. Psychotherapy: The-
spectives on Psychological Science, 2, 53–70. ory, Research, Practice, and Training, 42, 494 –511.
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline Strauss, B., Barber, J. P., & Castonguay, L. G. (In press). Visions in
personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press. psychotherapy research and practice: Reflections from the presidents of
MacLean, P. D. (1985). Brain evolution, relating to family, play, and the the society for psychotherapy research. New York: Routledge.
separation call. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 405– 417. Strunk, D. R., DeRubeis, R. J., Chiu, A. W., & Alvarez, J. (2007). Patient’s
Marcus, S. C., & Olfson, M. (2010). National trends in the treatment of competence in and performance of cognitive therapy skills: Relation to
depression from 1998 to 2007. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, the reduction of relapse risk following treatment for depression. Journal
1265–1273. of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 523–530.
McAleavey, A. A., & Castonguay, L. G. (2012, June). The how and why of Strupp, H. H. (1963). The outcome problem in psychotherapy revisited.
insight: How do we help clients achieve insight, and why do we bother? Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 1–13.
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Psychotherapy Strupp, H. H. (1964). The outcome problem in psychotherapy: A rejoinder.
Research, Virginia Beach. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 101.
Messer, S. B. (2001). Introduction to the special issue on assimilative Ulvenes, P. G., Berggraf, L., Hoffart, A., Stiles, T. C., Svartberg, M.,
integration. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 11, 1– 4. McCullough, L., & Wampold, B. (2012). Different processes for differ-
Nelson, D. L., & Castonguay, L. G. (2012, June). The systematic use of ent therapies: Therapist actions, therapeutic bond, and outcome. Psycho-
homework in psychodynamic–interpersonal psychotherapy for depres- therapy, 49, 291–302.
PSYCHOTHERAPY OUTCOME 67

Wampold, B. (2001). The great psychotherapy debate. New York: Erlbaum. Westra, H. A., Arkowitz, H., & Dozois, D. A. (2009). Adding a motiva-
Watson, J. C., & Bedard, D. (2006). Clients’ emotional processing in tional interviewing pretreatment to cognitive behavioral therapy for
psychotherapy: A comparison between cognitive-behavioral and generalized anxiety disorder: A preliminary randomized controlled trial.
process-experiential psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 1106 –1117. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis
Psychology, 74, 152–159. .2009.07.014
Webb, C. A., DeRubeis, R. J., & Baber, J. P. (2010). Therapist adherence/
competence and treatment outcome: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Received October 13, 2012
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 200 –211. Accepted October 15, 2012 䡲
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Members of Underrepresented Groups:


Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted
If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and
Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript reviewers are vital to the
publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C
Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate
more in this process.

If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write APA Journals at Reviewers@apa.org.
Please note the following important points:

• To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The
experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective
review.

• To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical journals that are most
central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently
published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission
within the context of existing research.

• To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information.
Please include with your letter your vita. In the letter, please identify which APA journal(s) you
are interested in, and describe your area of expertise. Be as specific as possible. For example,
“social psychology” is not sufficient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude
change” as well.

• Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1– 4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to
review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript
thoroughly.

APA now has an online video course that provides guidance in reviewing manuscripts. To learn
more about the course and to access the video, visit http://www.apa.org/pubs/authors/review-
manuscript-ce-video.aspx.

Potrebbero piacerti anche