Sei sulla pagina 1di 17

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

Review

Neuroscience of learning arithmetic—Evidence from brain imaging studies


L. Zamarian a,*, A. Ischebeck b, M. Delazer a
a
Clinical Department of Neurology, Medical University Innsbruck, Anichstrasse, 35, A-6020 Innsbruck, Austria
b
Institute for Psychology, Section of Applied Neuropsychology, University of Graz, Austria

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: It is widely accepted that the human brain is remarkably adaptive not only in child development, but also
Received 1 December 2008 during adulthood. Aim of this work is to offer an overview and a systematic analysis of neuroimaging
Received in revised form 5 March 2009 studies on the acquisition of arithmetic expertise. In normally developing children and adults, the gain of
Accepted 11 March 2009
arithmetic competence is reflected by a shift of activation from frontal brain areas to parietal areas
relevant for arithmetic processing. A shift of activation is also observed within the parietal lobe from the
Keywords: intraparietal sulci to the left angular gyrus. Increases in angular gyrus activation with gaining of
Learning
expertise have also been documented in other cognitive domains. It appears that the left angular gyrus
Training
activation is modulated by inter-individual differences in arithmetic performance. The comparison of
Arithmetic
Brain plasticity normal individuals with exceptionally performing individuals (e.g., calculating prodigies) suggests that
Brain activation changes the experts’ arithmetic proficiency relies on a more extended activation network than the network found
Gray matter changes in non-experts. In expert individuals with long-lasting, extensive mathematical training, specific
Dyscalculia structural brain modifications are also evident.
ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 909
2. Calculation in adults . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 910
3. Imaging arithmetic learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 911
3.1. Learning simple arithmetic . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 911
3.2. Learning complex arithmetic . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 912
3.3. Learning effects associated with different learning methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 912
3.4. Learning effects specific to the learning content . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 912
3.5. The trajectory of learning effects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 913
3.6. Transfer between operations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 913
3.7. Specificity of training-related brain activation changes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 914
4. The development of arithmetic . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 914
5. The neural basis of expertise in arithmetic . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 915
6. Inter-individual differences in arithmetic. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 916
7. Structural brain changes due to learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 916
8. Rehabilitation of arithmetic skills in patients. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 917
9. Methodological considerations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 917
10. Summary and discussion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 918
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 918
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 923

1. Introduction

Recent studies using neuroimaging methods have shown that the


human brain is remarkably adaptive not only in child development,
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +43 0512 504 23661. but also later on throughout adulthood (e.g., Kelly et al., 2006;
E-mail address: laura.zamarian@uki.at (L. Zamarian). Poldrack, 2000). It is well recognised that genetic predisposition, but

0149-7634/$ – see front matter ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.03.005
910 L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925

also experience and learning may alter the human brain (for a new perspective and to pose and answer new and otherwise
review, e.g., Ansari and Coch, 2006). Recent studies have specifically unforeseeable questions about the learning brain.
assessed the effect of targeted training on cerebral structures and Over the past decades, rich evidence has been accumulated on
brain activation patterns. For example, Draganski et al. (2006) used the ontogenetic development of arithmetic abilities (for a review,
voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to detect gray matter changes Buttherworth, 2005a), on learning disabilities and developmental
while (and after) medical students learned for their medical exam. dyscalculia (e.g., Buttherworth, 2005b; Geary and Hoard, 2005;
Increase in gray matter during learning was found in the posterior Landerl et al., 2004; Noël et al., 2005), on the decline of arithmetic
and inferior parietal cortex bilaterally, as well as in the posterior knowledge in advanced age (e.g., Duverne and Lemaire, 2005), as
hippocampus. Moreover, gray matter in the posterior hippocampus well as on the breakdown of arithmetic processing after acquired
continued to increase in the 3 months after the learning period. brain damage (e.g., Grafman et al., 1989; Hecaen et al., 1961;
Results of this study suggest that learning a great amount of highly McCloskey et al., 1991; Sokol et al., 1991; Warrington, 1982; for
abstract information leads to a specific pattern of structural gray reviews, e.g., Domahs and Delazer, 2005; Zamarian et al., 2007). The
matter modifications. Learning-induced changes of cerebral activa- development of brain imaging techniques has allowed the specific
tion patterns have also been demonstrated. Recent functional investigation of the cerebral networks supporting quantity proces-
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) investigations have, for sing and calculation (for a review, Ansari, 2008). So far, only a small
example, found that specific training may influence the brain number of studies have been concerned with the cerebral networks
activation patterns in individuals with dyslexia (e.g., Eden et al., supporting arithmetic learning and the acquisition of arithmetic
2004; Gaab et al., 2007; Richards et al., 2007). In a study by Eden et al. expertise. In the present review, we will first summarize neurop-
(2004), adults with developmental dyslexia underwent a phonolo- sychological findings on the cognitive architecture of arithmetic
gically targeted training programme. Behavioural effects of training abilities and on the cerebral networks supporting calculation. Then,
were also reflected by brain activation patterns as measured by fMRI. we will discuss brain imaging studies on the gain of arithmetic skills
Compared with dyslexics who performed no training, trained by healthy adults and on the acquisition of arithmetic abilities
dyslexics showed increased activation in left parietal regions during childhood. Finally, we will summarize evidence on the
suggesting increased reliance on neuronal circuits that are used relation between arithmetic expertise, cerebral activation patterns
by normal readers, as well as increased activation in the right and brain structure.
perisylvian cortex pointing to the engagement of compensatory
mechanisms. Functional plasticity induced by learning has also been 2. Calculation in adults
investigated by imaging studies in other domains, several of them
concerning the effects of musical training (e.g., Bosnyak et al., 2004; Cognitive neuropsychological studies with adults have pro-
Gaab et al., 2006, 2007; for reviews, e.g., Pantev et al., 2001, 2003; vided compelling evidence that arithmetic expertise requires
Trainor et al., 2003). Gaab et al. (2006) compared the activation the interplay of different types of knowledge and that number
patterns of two groups of healthy individuals, one group having had processing (e.g., reading number words or writing Arabic
strong learning effects in a pitch memory task, the second group numerals) and calculation are subserved by a number of different
with weaker training effects. A random-effects analysis comparing modules in the adult brain (e.g., Cappelletti et al., 2001, 2005;
successful learners with less successful learners yielded increased Cipolotti and de Lacy Costello, 1995; Dagenbach and McCloskey,
parietal activation in the left supramarginal gyrus. As suggested by 1992; Dehaene and Cohen, 1997; Delazer et al., 2004, 2006;
the authors, activation of the left supramarginal gyrus was due to Delazer and Benke, 1997; Lampl et al., 1994; McCloskey, 1992;
short-term memory of auditory material. McCloskey et al., 1985; McNeil and Warrington, 1994; Pesenti
Few imaging investigations have been concerned with the effect et al., 1994; Sokol et al., 1991; Van Harskamp and Cipolotti, 2001;
of arithmetic training, though this topic is of eminent importance. Warrington, 1982). In calculation, declarative knowledge of
The acquisition of arithmetic competence is a precondition for simple, overlearned arithmetic facts (knowing that 2 + 2 gives 4)
successful participation in social and professional life as well as for can be distinguished from procedural knowledge (knowing how to
effective exercise of citizenship in a numerate society (Butther- multiply 34  67) and from the recognition of arithmetic signs
worth, 2005a). Individuals with deficits in numerical and arithmetic (e.g., McCloskey, 1992; McCloskey et al., 1985). It has also been
processing face limitations in their autonomy and have poor chances shown that conceptual knowledge, which entails a basic under-
to qualify for adequate professional occupation. Given the high standing of operations and arithmetic principles, is an essential
incidence of dyscalculia (approximately 6% across various countries; and independent component of meaningful and efficient arith-
Shalev, 2004), better understanding of the learning processes and metic (e.g., Hittmair-Delazer et al., 1994, 1995). A number of
possible disturbances is highly needed. Improved knowledge on the influential neuropsychological studies in the last three decades
acquisition of arithmetic skills and concepts is not only essential in focused on the cognitive architecture of calculation and number
the case of disturbances, but also for efficient teaching to normally processing (e.g., McCloskey, 1992). Other studies investigated the
achieving students. Cross-country comparisons have yielded sig- relation between lesion localisation and affected cognitive
nificant differences, possibly linked to the teaching style and components (for reviews, Dehaene et al., 2003; Domahs and
training methods (e.g., OECD, 2007a). Though investigating struc- Delazer, 2005). Several papers confirmed the role of the left
tural and functional effects of arithmetic training may contribute to parietal structures in simple calculation (e.g., Delazer et al., 2006;
the development of targeted teaching and intervention in the future, Lee, 2000; Van Harskamp and Cipolotti, 2001; Warrington, 1982),
the primary aim of this review is not to propose direct classroom although frontal areas seem also to be crucially involved in more
applications (which are regarded by some as being all too often complex calculation processes (e.g., Lucchelli and De Renzi, 1993;
simple and unsatisfying; Goswami, 2006), but to inform researchers Semenza et al., 1997). Moreover, not only cortical lesions but also
from different disciplines as well as practitioners about the sub-cortical lesions, such as those affecting the basal ganglia, have
neuroscience of arithmetic learning. Aim of the present review is shown to cause impairment in calculation (e.g., Corbett et al., 1986;
to contribute to the dialogue between neuroscience and education Dehaene and Cohen, 1997; Delazer et al., 2004; Whitaker et al.,
(for a discussion, e.g., Ansari and Coch, 2006) and to summarize our 1985; Zamarian et al., in press).
current knowledge on the processes related to arithmetic learning. The rapid development of brain imaging techniques in the
As proposed by Ansari and Coch (2006), bridges between education last 15 years has helped the precise identification of the
and neuroscience will enable educators and neuroscientists to gain a neuro-anatomical brain areas supporting arithmetic functions.
L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925 911

In a pioneering study, Roland and Friberg (1985) used single abstract numerical representations mediated by specific neuronal
photon emission tomography to measure regional changes in populations in the parietal lobes.
cerebral blood flow induced by different cognitive tasks. The
arithmetic task (serial subtraction) specifically activated the 3. Imaging arithmetic learning
angular gyrus (AG), bilaterally. This AG activation was explained
to be due to the retrieval of numerical information from memory. A A crucial aspect in the acquisition of arithmetic expertise
number of later studies have shown that number processing and consists in the shift from slow and effortful processing to skilled
calculation are mediated by a distributed network within the and fast retrieval. Such a shift has been documented in several
parietal lobes (e.g., Fias et al., 2003; Gruber et al., 2001; Kong et al., developmental studies (e.g., Ashcraft, 1992; Barrouillet and Fayol,
2005; Menon et al., 2000a; Rickard et al., 2000; Venkatraman et al., 1998; Fuson, 1982, 1988; Lemaire and Siegler, 1995; Siegler, 1988)
2005). According to a highly influential model (e.g., Dehaene et al., as well as in learning studies with adults (Anderson et al., 1999;
2003; Dehaene and Cohen, 1997), three parietal circuits are Logan, 1988; Logan and Klapp, 1991; Rickard, 2004; Wenger,
involved in number processing. One of these circuits (bilateral 1999). Typically, developmental studies describe the acquisition of
horizontal intraparietal sulci) is thought to be domain-specific for arithmetic fact knowledge (such as 3  5 = 15), whereas studies
numerical processing; the other two (posterior parietal attention with adults also employ more complex paradigms. Recent
system and left AG verbal system) are most probably shared with publications have documented how short-term, although inten-
other cognitive domains. The assumption that the intraparietal sive training on arithmetic problems induces increases in accuracy
sulcus (IPS) bilaterally is the crucial area for the representation of and speed that are accompanied by significant changes in brain
quantities has been supported by a large number of neuroimaging activation patterns (Delazer et al., 2003, 2005; Grabner et al., in
studies. Different paradigms have been used to assess the press; Ischebeck et al., 2006, 2007, 2009; Pauli et al., 1994). Fig. 1
representation of quantity, including number comparison tasks depicts an example of improvements of response times and
(e.g., Chochon et al., 1999; Fias et al., 2003; Pesenti et al., 2000; accuracy over 5 days of intensive training on complex arithmetic
Pinel et al., 2001), discrete and analogue comparison tasks (Castelli problems (e.g., 16  7 = ?; 45 27 = ?). As indicated by the trend
et al., 2006), or adaptation to quantities tasks (Cohen Kadosh et al., lines over the five sessions, the longer the training, the faster and
2007; Piazza et al., 2007). As regards calculation, the IPS is more the more accurate the participants’ performance. Functionally,
strongly activated in approximate calculation than in exact acquisition of arithmetic proficiency is reflected by modifications
calculation (e.g., Stanescu-Cosson et al., 2000), and more strongly of cerebral activation patterns. Here below, we report the main
activated in subtraction than in multiplication (e.g., Chochon et al., findings of these imaging studies on brain activation modifications
1999; Kazui et al., 2000; Lee, 2000). The parietal circuit which relies after arithmetic training.
on the left AG is thought to mediate, among other functions, exact
and highly automated calculation such as the retrieval of multi- 3.1. Learning simple arithmetic
plication facts from memory (e.g., 3  4 = ?; Chochon et al., 1999;
Duffau et al., 2002; Lee, 2000; but see Rickard et al., 2000). While Pauli et al. (1994) focused their attention on the effects of
the AG is assumed to subserve skilled arithmetic fact retrieval, it training on one-digit arithmetic problems and monitored the
does not mediate quantity-based operations such as approxima- evoked-related potentials (ERPs) responses of fourteen young
tion, estimation or number comparison. The third parietal region adults over four sessions of intensive practice. Results indicated a
includes the superior parietal lobule and supports visuo-spatial decrease in response times with practice which was greater for
processes, attention and spatial working memory related to the problems with the larger operands (e.g., 8  7) than for the
numerical processing (Dehaene et al., 2003; Simon et al., 2002). problems with the smaller operands (e.g., 3  4). Changes in ERPs
Evidence has been accumulated on the involvement of the IPS, AG were mainly characterized by reduction in fronto-central
and superior parietal lobule in different aspects of numerical positivity, while positivity in parietal sites remained relatively
processing. However, some assumptions of the triple-code model stable over time for both problem sizes. Arithmetic processing
have been controversially discussed. For example, not all authors therefore correlated with a stable level of parietal activity all
agree that the retrieval of arithmetic facts is mediated by verbal along the training course; frontal activity diminished as a
processes (e.g., Zago et al., 2001). A recent topic of debate is function of increasing practice. Possibly, with repetition of the
whether numbers or magnitudes are represented in the brain in problems, participants strengthened retrieval of the correct
abstract form, i.e. independent from symbolic (Arabic numerals, solution from memory, while demands on processes such as
number words or other) or non-symbolic notation (e.g., dot- working memory, monitoring and attention sustained by frontal
patterns), as suggested by Dehaene et al. (1998). In a recent review, areas diminished. To our knowledge, this was the first study
Cohen Kadosh and Walsh (in press) challenge the view of abstract providing evidence that intensive practice of arithmetic leads to
numerical representations and discuss in detail evidence for non- significant modifications of brain activity.

Fig. 1. Latency (a) and accuracy (b) of 17 young participants as a function of operation (multiplication, subtraction) and training session (days 1–5) (related functional results
are reported in Ischebeck et al., 2006). The contrast day 1 versus day 5 is significant for both operations at both latency and accuracy level, t-tests, all p < 0.0001.
912 L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925

3.2. Learning complex arithmetic Several studies have assessed the characteristics of fact acquisi-
tion such as processing speed, accuracy and strategy use in children
Later studies used fMRI to trace training-related modifications (e.g., Ashcraft, 1992; Lemaire and Siegler, 1995; Siegler, 1988),
of brain activation (Delazer et al., 2003, 2005; Grabner et al., in healthy adults (e.g., Anderson et al., 1999; Logan, 1988; Logan and
press; Ischebeck et al., 2006, 2007, 2009). In order to make sure Klapp, 1991; Rickard, 2004; Wenger, 1999) and dyscalculic
that the problems being trained are not known by heart, more individuals (e.g., Domahs et al., 2003, 2008; Girelli et al., 2002;
complex problems were used. The solution of a problem such as Hittmair-Delazer et al., 1994; for a review, Domahs and Delazer,
23  8 is not commonly part of our declarative memory and thus 2005). Some of these studies have also directly compared the effects
has to be calculated. This necessitates the application of sometimes of different learning methods (e.g., Logan and Klapp, 1991; Domahs
effortful, resource demanding and error-prone procedures, and et al., 2003). Brain imaging techniques allow the assessment of the
training should show marked effects at the behavioural level that brain activation patterns associated with fact acquisition as well as
might be correlated with changes in brain activation patterns. the comparison between the neural circuits recruited by different
In the first study of this series (Delazer et al., 2003), young adults learning methods. In Delazer et al.’s (2005) study, young adults were
were trained over 5 days on a set of complex multiplication intensively trained on two new arithmetic operations. One
problems (e.g., 23  8 = ?). After training (day 5), participants operation – strategy condition – had to be learned by applying a
showed proficiency in solving the trained problems, which were new algorithm (e.g., 3 § 12 = {[(12 3) + 1] + 12} = 22), the other
answered almost perfectly and more quickly than novel, untrained operation – drill condition – by memorising the operands-result
problems of equal difficulty. Learning effects were also evident in the association (e.g., 4 # 15 = 24). Note that participants can only solve
imaging results. Untrained problems relative to trained problems new problems in the strategy condition where they are familiar with
significantly activated the IPS, the inferior parietal lobule, and the the underlying algorithm. Hence, three conditions were compared in
inferior frontal gyrus (all left lateralised). The reverse contrast the fMRI experiment, i.e. the two trained conditions (strategy and
(trained versus untrained) indicated relatively stronger activation drill) and a condition of new (strategy) problems. After training,
within the left AG. These results were interpreted as suggestive of a trained problems in both conditions were solved equally fast, and
shift from quantity-based processing and complex procedures (e.g., new (strategy) problems were solved more slowly (Fig. 3). In the
23  8 = (20  8) + (3  8) = 160 + 24 = 184), relying on working fMRI results, stronger AG activation was observed for trained
memory and executive control, to direct memory retrieval of the strategy problems compared with new problems, replicating and
correct solution (e.g., 23  8 = 184). Findings of this study agree with extending previous observations for training effects in multi-
the observation by Poldrack (2000) that learning is reflected by a plication (Delazer et al., 2003). This means that participants made
shift from general purpose processes to task-specific processes, in use of result retrieval in the strategy condition for the problems they
this case to stronger reliance on arithmetic fact retrieval. In Fig. 2, we were familiar with through training. However, when both trained
propose a representation of this shift from frontal and intraparietal conditions were compared with each other, greater left AG
areas to the left AG following intensive training and automated activation was observed for the drill condition than for the strategy
retrieval of arithmetic memory associations. condition, indicating that solution by result retrieval was more
dominant in the drill condition. The lesser activation of the left AG in
3.3. Learning effects associated with different learning methods the strategy condition may be possibly related to the fact that
participants sometimes or simultaneously applied the algorithm
Discussions on the effect of different learning methods have a even in the case of trained problems they had learned by heart.
long tradition in educational psychology and cognitive psychology Results suggest that the way problems are trained makes a great
(Brownell, 1935; Thorndike, 1922). Following a drill approach, difference for the active involvement of brain areas necessary for
instructions focus on the rote memorization of computational problem processing, although both types of training resulted in
skills, i.e. arithmetic fact knowledge and arithmetic procedures problems being solved equally fast.
(Cowan, 2003). Children are taught bonds or associations between
unrelated stimuli and understanding is not necessary for the 3.4. Learning effects specific to the learning content
formation of such bonds (Baroody, 2003). In this view, direct
instruction and drill are seen as efficient methods to ensure the To investigate whether training effects are comparable across
formation of arithmetic expertise. Other approaches put emphasis arithmetic operations, participants of a third study were provided
on the understanding of basic principles and arithmetic relations, with the same training procedure, but had to learn two different
and suggest that arithmetic fact knowledge should be acquired operations (complex multiplication: e.g., 15  7; complex sub-
gradually by procedures and strategies till the students build up a traction: e.g., 43 26; Ischebeck et al., 2006). The triple-code
set of memorized facts. model (e.g., Dehaene et al., 2003; Dehaene and Cohen, 1997)

Fig. 2. Schematic illustration of the brain activations for the contrast untrained versus trained problems and the contrast trained versus untrained problems. Extended
activations in frontal and intraparietal areas are typically found in the contrast untrained versus trained problems. A relative activation of the left angular gyrus is found in the
contrast trained versus untrained problems.
L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925 913

Fig. 3. Latency (a) and accuracy (b) of 16 young participants at post-training (day 5) as a function of learning method (by strategy, by drill) and problems’ type (trained,
untrained) (related functional data are reported in Delazer et al., 2005). In the behavioural post-training assessment (day 5) of the strategy condition, participants were asked
to solve the trained strategy problems (e.g., 3 § 12 = ? (= 22), known algorithm {[(b a) + 1] + b}) as well as new, untrained problems (e.g., 4 § 18 = ? (= 33)). In the assessment
of the drill condition, they were presented with the trained drill problems (e.g., 4 # 15 = ? (=24), unknown algorithm), but also with new problems (e.g., 5 # 12 = ? (=19)).
Obviously, participants could solve the new strategy problems by applying the learned algorithm, whereas with new drill problems they could only guess the answer. Overall,
untrained problems were answered more slowly and less accurately than trained problems, t-tests, all p < 0.05.

assumes that simple multiplication (e.g., 3  4) and simple 3.5. The trajectory of learning effects
subtraction (e.g., 9 4) rely on different cognitive processes
supported by partially different cortical substrates. According to In Ischebeck et al. (2007), the development of the learning
this model, multiplication relies on skilled retrieval of arithmetic process itself was explored. Differently from previous studies, no
facts from verbal long-term memory, whereas subtraction is training was provided prior to the fMRI session. Participants were
solved directly by manipulating the numerical representations of presented in the scanner with two sets of complex multiplication
the operands involved. Consequently, multiplication more strongly problems: one set had high frequency of repetition (‘repeated’), the
activates the left AG, whereas subtraction relies more strongly on other set had low frequency of repetition (‘novel’). With passing
the IPS bilaterally. Differences between operations may be due to time in the scanner, problems’ repetition led to decreases in
the method how simple multiplications and subtractions are activation within fronto-parietal areas and the caudate nucleus,
taught at school. However, they may also be due to differences while relative activation increases were observed in temporo-
with regard to efficiency. Procedures and back-up strategies might parietal regions including the left AG. These effects became
be the best and most simple way for addition and subtraction, significant after approximately eight repetitions of a problem and
whereas they become very effortful for multiplication. In multi- were, as a pattern, similar to previously reported experiments
plication, a greater reliance on memorized results might be the where participants had to train much more intensively (Delazer
most efficient strategy. It is therefore a question of interest et al., 2003, 2005; Ischebeck et al., 2006). Findings of this study
whether comparable training on two different operations, multi- suggest that learning-related brain activation modifications
plication and subtraction, would lead to similar or different appear very early during training and do not differ in principle
training effects. In Ischebeck et al.’s (2006) study, identical and from brain activation changes observed after intensive training.
intensive training was provided for complex multiplication and
complex subtraction problems of similar difficulty. Post-training 3.6. Transfer between operations
results demonstrated a similar learning curve for both operations
(Fig. 1). The analysis of fMRI data, however, revealed systematic Successful transfer between related arithmetic operations is a
differences in brain activation patterns between the trained key feature of efficient arithmetic. The investigation of transfer
operations. While the left AG was significantly activated for effects is important because only the flexible use of acquired
trained multiplication relative to untrained multiplication, no AG knowledge enables people to efficiently answer to new problems.
activation was evident for trained subtraction relative to untrained Also, rehabilitation cannot be considered to be successful if a
subtraction (Fig. 4). These operation-specific brain activation patient learns, for example, to retrieve arithmetic facts from
changes after identical training are possibly associated with memory, but is unable to adapt this knowledge to new situations
differences between operations. Well-known complex multiplica- (for discussions, Delazer, 2003; Lochy et al., 2005). While transfer
tion problems are possibly more efficiently solved by memory between operations has been assessed in experimental psychology
retrieval. By contrast, fast and effortless computation may still play (e.g., Rickard, 2004, 2005) and in neuropsychological case studies
a role in the solution of well-known subtraction problems. (Cipolotti and de Lacy Costello, 1995; Delazer et al., 2004;

Fig. 4. Contrasts between calculation conditions (training effect). The contrasts untrained versus trained multiplication/subtraction are shown in red, trained versus
untrained multiplication in green. The contrast trained versus untrained subtraction did not yield any significant activation. This illustration is part of a figure published in
Ischebeck et al. (2006), here reproduced with permission from Elsevierß.
914 L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925

Hittmair-Delazer et al., 1994), only one study so far has mapping between abstract numerical symbols and the magnitudes
investigated this topic by brain imaging methods. Ischebeck they represent. Callan et al. (2005), for example, trained Japanese
et al. (2009) were interested in the modifications of brain participants on unknown phonograms and found that the
activation patterns associated with transfer effects from newly acquisition of new orthographic-phonological mappings was
acquired multiplication knowledge (e.g., 46  3 = 138) to related related to activation increases within the left AG. Possibly, the
division problems (e.g., 138:3 = 46). Participants were trained on AG supports general fact retrieval processes and mapping
complex multiplication problems and were presented in the processes to stored information in memory, although its function
scanner with trained and new, untrained multiplication problems, may slightly differ between the hemispheres (verbal: left AG;
as well as with division problems that were either related or figural: right AG; Grabner et al., in press). Damage to the left AG has
unrelated to the trained multiplication problems. Replicating often been found in association with calculation deficits, finger
previous findings (Delazer et al., 2003, 2005; Ischebeck et al., 2006, agnosia, left–right disorientation, writing or reading difficulties,
2007), significant training effects appeared within the left AG for and impairment to this AG mapping function might underlie these
multiplication. With regard to transfer, overall effects were disturbances (Ansari, 2008).
relatively weak, possibly due to strong inter-individual differences.
However, relatively greater activation within the left AG was 4. The development of arithmetic
observed for related division problems than for unrelated pro-
blems. Also, there was a significant positive correlation between The most basic foundations of arithmetic emerge early in
activation in the left AG and the size of transfer effects in childhood and preschool children can already present a remark-
behavioural performance at post-training. This study showed that able knowledge of arithmetic. This knowledge is grounded on
transfer, i.e. flexible access to stored knowledge, is reflected by biological roots, experience and cultural influences. There is broad
changes in brain activation patterns, but that individuals may consensus that ‘‘numbers’’ are represented as mental magnitudes
differ with regard to the presence or absence of transfer. by a developmentally and evolutionarily universal system (Cantlon
et al., 2009). This assumption is based on evidence from cognitive
3.7. Specificity of training-related brain activation changes development, comparative cognition, cross-cultural cognition and
neurobiology (for a recent review, Cantlon et al., 2009). However, it
Studies on short-term, intensive arithmetic training consis- is under debate whether non-numerical magnitudes such as time,
tently reported activation increases within the left AG in size, ordinal position and brightness recruit the same neural
association with acquisition of arithmetic knowledge (Delazer mechanisms as numerical magnitudes. It has been proposed that
et al., 2003, 2005; Ischebeck et al., 2006, 2007, 2009). These time, space and quantity are processed by a common generalized
activation increases were found to be more pronounced when the magnitude system (Walsh, 2003). These three dimensions would
type of arithmetic operation (Ischebeck et al., 2006) or the type of be tightly linked since they represent critical magnitudes in the
training (Delazer et al., 2005) encouraged memory retrieval. It is, external world that are used for action. In extension to this view,
however, unclear whether these effects are specific to the learning Cantlon et al. (2009) emphasize that not only time, space and
of arithmetic facts or whether they also emerge in case of learning quantity, but also other continuous dimensions, including size and
of non-arithmetic facts. To address this issue, Grabner et al. (in brightness, may share the same psychophysical signatures,
press) asked their participants to train on two types of problems, comparison processes and neural substrates.
complex multiplication problems (e.g., 14  7 = ?; from Ischebeck Recent research converges on the view that the development
et al., 2006) and complex figural-spatial problems (this study is of arithmetic abilities is based on universal cerebral circuits
also described below because it also addressed whether compe- devoted to the processing of magnitudes or quantities (whether
tence-related activation differences may be attenuated by domain specific or not). Remarkable capacities have been
intensive training). In the figural-spatial condition, drawings of demonstrated for young infants. It has been, for example, shown
three-dimensional objects were presented and participants had to that infants discriminate between small sets of objects that differ
determine the number of object faces. Similar training was in numerosity (e.g., Antell and Keating, 1983; Starkey et al., 1990;
provided and identical improvements in speed and accuracy were Starkey and Cooper, 1980; Wynn et al., 2002; for a different view,
found for the arithmetic and figural-spatial conditions. Functional e.g., Mix et al., 2002a,b). They are able to represent and attend to
data replicated results of previous studies for the arithmetic the numerosity of small sets of objects and seem able to carry out
condition (e.g., Delazer et al., 2003; Ischebeck et al., 2006), as the basic mental manipulations on them. In a highly influential
contrast untrained versus trained multiplication problems showed study, Wynn (1992) reported that infants as young as 4–5
stronger activation in fronto-parietal regions including the inferior months have arithmetical expectations and detect manipulations
frontal cortex and the IPS. The contrast trained versus untrained which violate arithmetic rules when a small number of items is
multiplication problems indicated stronger activation within the used (for an alternative view, e.g., Xu and Spelke, 2000). Through
AG, bilaterally, with a peak of activation in the left AG. Results for education, children gradually acquire a better understanding of
the figural-spatial condition considerably overlapped with the numerosity and learn to understand and use counting principles
network of brain activations found for multiplication. The contrast and counting sequences (Fuson, 1988, 1992; Gelman and Meck,
untrained versus trained figural-spatial problems yielded bilateral 1983). Progressively, children acquire skills and expertise in
fronto-parietal activations; the contrast trained versus untrained applying their counting skills to arithmetic (Fuson and Kwon,
figural-spatial problems demonstrated an increase in activation 1992). As in other domains, the acquisition of arithmetic
within the AG, bilaterally, with a peak of activation in the right AG. expertise is reflected by a shift from slow and effortful processing
This finding of similar training effects in both the arithmetic and to skilled and fast retrieval. While young children use time-
the non-arithmetic condition suggests that the training-related consuming step-by-step procedures (Ashcraft, 1992; Fuson,
activation increases in the AG are not specific to arithmetic fact 1982, 1988), school age children show an increasing degree of
learning. Differently, it points to the involvement of the AG in more optimization (Barrouillet and Fayol, 1998; Lemaire and Siegler,
general processes of fact learning. Recently, it has been proposed 1995) and finally retrieve a set of arithmetic problems from
(Ansari, 2008) that the AG is involved in general symbol-referent memory (note, however, that adults still often use strategies for
mapping processes, such as mapping between arithmetic pro- solving arithmetic problems; Campbell et al., 2004; Campbell
blems and their solutions, grapheme–phoneme mappings or and Timm, 2000; Campbell and Xue, 2001; Geary et al., 1993;
L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925 915

Geary and Wiley, 1991; Kirk and Ashcraft, 2001; LeFevre et al., processing. Tang et al. (2006) compared native English and Chinese
1996a,b; LeFevre and Morris, 1999). speakers on a series of visual and numerical tasks, and found
Although rich evidence has been accumulated on the beha- significant group differences in terms of brain activation and
vioural characteristics of arithmetic development (e.g., Barth et al., functional connectivity in addition and number comparison tasks
2005; Canobi and Bethune, 2008; Gilmore et al., 2007; for reviews, with Arabic digits. In the case of the Chinese speakers, numerical
e.g., Bisanz et al., 2005; Buttherworth, 2005a), less is known about processing seemed to rely more on visuo-motor association
the involvement of the neural structures supporting calculation in networks. English speakers recruited left perisylvian areas to a
different age groups. Developmental neuroimaging studies on greater extent. Language and culture might have affected the
number processing in general have shown that children rely more neural substrates underlying mental arithmetic and numerical-
on prefrontal regions than adults, at least in tasks involving magnitude processing. As Tang et al. (2006) proposed, however,
numerical symbols such as digits (e.g., Kaufmann et al., 2006; these results must not only have been caused by language, but also
Kucian et al., 2008). With increasing age, however, distinct parietal by reading experience and cultural differences in learning
circuits, the bilateral IPS and the left temporo-parietal cortex, seem strategies and in the education system. Cultural effects on
to become more specialized for numerical-magnitude processing functional brain organization are also not specific to the number
and calculation (for a review, Ansari, 2008). Development of domain and have been documented for other cognitive domains as
numerical cognition is thus observed to be associated with well such as, for example, object processing (e.g., Goh et al., 2007;
attenuation of activation in non-critical areas and more focal Kobayashi et al., 2007; Paulesu et al., 2000).
activation in number-relevant areas. This is very similar to
observations of development in other domains (e.g., cognitive 5. The neural basis of expertise in arithmetic
control; e.g., Durston et al., 2006; Schlaggar et al., 2002; for a
discussion, e.g., Casey et al., 2005) and may indicate that While psychological research aims at revealing the modifica-
development is achieved through a fine-tuning of relevant neural tions of cognitive processes related to extensive training and
systems (for a discussion see, e.g., Durston and Casey, 2006). expertise (Anderson et al., 1999; Logan, 1988; Logan and Klapp,
To the best of our knowledge, only two studies so far have 1991; Rickard, 2004; Wenger, 1999), neuroimaging studies focus
investigated developmental changes in mental arithmetic with on the cerebral correlates of expert processing such as, for
fMRI (Kawashima et al., 2004; Rivera et al., 2005). The first study example, Braille readers (e.g., Pascual-Leone and Torres, 1993),
assessed age-related differences in activation patterns related to musicians (e.g., Elbert et al., 1995; Pantev et al., 1998), or
mental arithmetic in a group of seventeen participants (8–19 sommeliers (Castriota-Scanderberg et al., 2005). In a positron
years; Rivera et al., 2005). Participants were asked to verify simple emission tomography (PET) study by Pesenti et al. (2001), a
addition and subtraction equations of the type ‘‘4 + 2 = 6?’’ and calculation expert (R. Gamm) was scanned while doing complex
‘‘7 3 = 4?’’, a task where children and adolescents performed mental arithmetic. Gamm had memorized arithmetic facts and
with high accuracy. An increase in brain activation in correlation calculation algorithms for several hours per day over several years.
with age was found in the left supramarginal gyrus and anterior Using his impressive knowledge of arithmetic problems and the
IPS, as well as in the left lateral temporo-occipital cortex. A episodic encoding-retrieval strategies he has developed on his
decrease in activation was observed in frontal brain areas, in sub- own, Gamm was, for example, able to retrieve from long-term
cortical structures (basal ganglia and thalamus), in the brainstem memory the square of two-digit numbers (e.g., 732) or to quickly
and the left medial temporal lobe. Findings were similar to those of and accurately compute complex multiplication problems (e.g.,
young adults where trained arithmetic problems were compared 76  82; for a description of the behavioural investigation see
with untrained problems (Delazer et al., 2003, 2005; Grabner et al., Pesenti et al., 1999). The comparison of Gamm’s activation pattern
in press; Ischebeck et al., 2006, 2007, 2009; see also Pauli et al., with that of non-expert controls revealed some similarities, but
1994). Both lines of research show that growing expertise or age is also activation foci specific for the expert. In the conjunction
related to a greater involvement of specific parietal areas and to a analysis of computation- versus memory-based problems, Gamm
decrease of reliance on general-purpose (frontal) areas. Of course, and non-expert controls showed bilateral cerebral activation, with
both types of studies, namely, learning studies with adults and a left-sided predominance. Activations were, for example,
studies assessing developmental changes in children, are not fully observed in the supramarginal gyrus, the IPS, the inferior occipital
comparable although they indicate similar results. In children, and middle occipital gyri, the occipito-temporal junction, as well as
learning and brain maturation simultaneously contribute to the in frontal areas (for details, Pesenti et al., 2001, or Appendix of the
activation pattern changes observed (e.g., Durston and Casey, present review). Differently from non-expert controls, activations
2006). specific for Gamm were observed in the medial frontal gyrus, the
Results of a second fMRI study (Kawashima et al., 2004) are also parahippocampal gyrus, the anterior cingulate gyrus and the
suggestive of a functional specialization within the parietal cortex middle occipito-temporal junction (all right sided), as well as in the
with age. Kawashima et al. (2004) compared a group of eight left paracentral lobule. Thus, the expert’s and the non-experts’
children with a group of eight middle-aged adults (age range 9–14 calculation performance relied on brain areas which are typically
and 40–49, respectively). Participants were instructed to mentally involved in visuo-spatial working memory, visual imagery and
solve single-digit addition, subtraction and multiplication pro- numerical processing. However, Gamm seemed to also employ
blems while in the scanner (e.g., ‘‘3 + 5 = ?’’, ‘‘7 2 = ?’’, and brain areas associated with episodic memory processes that might
‘‘4  8 = ?’’). A pattern of bilaterally distributed activations in be related to his use of specific encoding-retrieval strategies during
frontal and intraparietal areas, as well as in temporo-occipital calculation. High-level cognitive expertise in calculation appeared
areas was revealed for the adults. Children showed similar results, thus not to be limited to the improvement of existing processing
although adults showed stronger activation within the IPS, networks, but to involve the recruitment of new processes and
bilaterally, for most mental arithmetic tasks. Increased involve- cerebral areas (Pesenti et al., 2001).
ment of parietal areas in mental calculation with age further A further example that calculation expertise might be bound to
stresses the relevance of these brain areas for numerical processing the use of specific strategies relying on distinct cerebral circuits is
and the acquisition of calculation skills. reported by Hanakawa et al. (2003) in an fMRI study on skilled
Beside development, language and culture might also have an abacus users. Abacus experts are not only extremely efficient in
influence on the involvement of the brain areas underlying number using the tool in its physical form, but also gain the ability to
916 L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925

mentally calculate extraordinarily large numbers with unusual calculation sub-results, then enhanced activation within the left
speed and accuracy (Stigler, 1984). Qualified Japanese abacus AG should be found for non-perfect performers relative to perfect
masters with the highest degree for both mental calculation and performers. Differences between perfect and non-perfect perfor-
abacus operations were compared with non-experts in a series of mers in Menon et al.’s (2000b) study may thus be associated, to
mental-operation tasks requiring the manipulation of numerical, some extent, with differences in the participants’ reliance on
spatial or verbal material (Hanakawa et al., 2003). In the numerical calculation and memory retrieval: the higher the reliance on these
task, experts perfectly performed one-digit and three-digit number processes, the stronger the activation within the left AG.
additions (100% accuracy), and scored with six-digit number Similarly to Menon et al. (2000b), Grabner et al. (2007) aimed at
additions as accurately as non-experts in the one-digit number exploring the relationship between mathematical competence and
condition. No relevant group differences were detected in spatial brain activation in normally performing individuals. In order to
or verbal tasks, whereas the numerical condition yielded ensure that results could be generalized beyond the specific
significant activation differences. Both groups showed activation demands of the experimental tasks, the participants’ groups were
in the superior precentral sulcus, the posterior parietal cortex formed on the basis of performance on standardised psychometric
including the IPS, the fusiform gyrus and the cerebellum. However, tests of intelligence and arithmetic competence. Groups of
this activation pattern was bilaterally symmetrical in the abacus participants with high and low mathematical competence differed
experts, while it was strongly left lateralised in the non-experts. in both mathematical-numerical intelligence tests and experimen-
Additional significant activation differences included stronger tal multiplication tasks (simple condition: one-digit times one-digit;
activations within the posterior superior parietal cortex/precuneus complex condition: two-digit times one-digit), but not with respect
for the experts, and stronger activations in the prefrontal cortex to verbal and figural-spatial intelligence. Individuals with low
and lateral parietal cortex for the non-experts. In sum, abacus mathematical competence performed the experimental multi-
masters showed a greater involvement of brain areas mediating plication tasks less accurately and slower than individuals with
visuo-spatial and visuo-motor processing, a finding that possibly high competence. This was particularly evident in the complex
reflects their main strategy use during complex mental calculation condition (e.g., 13  7 = 91) relative to the simple condition (e.g.,
(Hanakawa et al., 2003). Similarly to Pesenti et al. (2001), experts’ 4  6 = 24). For high-achieving individuals compared with low-
and non-experts’ mental calculation partially relied on common achieving individuals, higher activation was observed within the AG,
neural correlates, but the experts also showed evidence of the middle temporal gyrus, the supplementary motor area and the
employing additional cerebral areas related to the specific strategy medial superior frontal gyrus (all left lateralised). The reverse
being used. Differently from the short-term training studies that contrast (low versus high) did not reveal any significant foci of
mainly found a modulation of activation in the cerebral networks activation. Also, activation within the left AG was positively
commonly involved in mental arithmetic, the studies with high- correlated with the individual level of mathematical competence.
performing individuals and extensive training over several years In sum, performance of the mathematically more competent
indicate activation of different areas, pointing to the recruitment of individuals was supported by a network involving the left AG to a
strategies that are very different from those used by non-experts. stronger extent than the less competent individuals’.
Type of adopted strategy and extent of training may thus play a These findings were replicated and extended in a second study
crucial role in the modification of brain activations. by Grabner et al. (in press), where more competent individuals and
less competent individuals intensively practiced on complex
6. Inter-individual differences in arithmetic multiplication problems following the same training procedure
as in Ischebeck et al. (2006). Behaviourally, the more competent
Inter-individual differences in brain activation have not only individuals outperformed the less competent individuals during
been observed between non-experts and exceptional experts, but the entire training as well as with the untrained multiplication
have also been reported for less-exceptional or normal users of problems in the fMRI test session. In the functional results, a
arithmetic. Menon et al. (2000b) compared a group of perfect stronger activation within the left AG was found for the more
performers (accuracy of 100%) with a group of non-perfect competent participants relative to the less competent participants
performers (mean accuracy of 92%, S.D. 6.2%) on an arithmetic with the untrained multiplication problems. Also, this activation
verification task with simple addition and subtraction (e.g., significantly and positively correlated with the individual level of
‘‘4 + 2 1 = 4?’’). Perfect performers were also faster than non- mathematical competence, replicating the results of Grabner et al.
perfect performers. With regard to brain activation, proportionally (2007). No group difference, however, emerged for the trained
fewer activated voxels were observed within the left AG for perfect multiplication problems. As the authors suggested, these findings
performers compared with non-perfect performers. The authors may reflect, on the one side, that the more competent individuals
concluded that greater arithmetic proficiency leads to greater more strongly relied on fact retrieval processes with untrained
functional optimization within the left AG and thus to less multiplication problems and, on the other side, that gaining of
activation. However, it should be noted that this result is partly mathematical expertise led to attenuation of the competence-
inconsistent with findings from other studies on mathematical related activation difference within the left AG. This attenuation
competence and brain activation (see below Grabner et al., 2007, in resulted from stronger increase of the left AG activation for the less
press), as well as from learning studies that observed an increase of competent individuals following intensive training.
activation within the left AG due to practice, rather than a decrease.
The assumption of a U-shaped curve of activation change within 7. Structural brain changes due to learning
the left AG has thus been so far not well supported. It is also
possible that group effects in Menon et al.’s (2000b) study mirror It has been observed that structural lesions to parietal areas may
the use of different solution strategies. A problem such as lead to severe arithmetic deficits (e.g., Berger, 1926; Henschen, 1919,
‘‘4 + 2 1 = 4?’’ can be quickly and accurately answered by 1920; Lewandowsky and Stadelman, 1908) and that decrease in
applying plausibility strategies, which have been often described parietal gray matter density in premature born children is associated
in verification paradigms (e.g., Lemaire and Reder, 1999; Lochy with lower maths performance at the age of 15 years (Isaacs et al.,
et al., 2000). If verification strategies were efficiently used by 2001). While there is ample evidence that damage to the parietal
perfect performers, while non-perfect performers predominantly areas leads to arithmetic deficits, little is known whether training of
relied on calculation procedures and memory retrieval of arithmetic processing leads to modifications of the brain structure.
L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925 917

Several morphometric studies have shown that experience can alter intelligence (e.g., Kelly et al., 2006) may have an influence on the
the anatomical structure of the human brain, such as, for example, effects of practice on the brain.
the gray matter density. Broad evidence has come in particular from
sensorimotor research (e.g., Draganski et al., 2004, 2006; Münte 9. Methodological considerations
et al., 2002; Pantev et al., 1998; Pascual-Leone and Torres, 1993), but
recent investigations have also observed structural changes of the We would like to briefly address some methodological aspects
human brain after cognitive training (e.g., Aydin et al., 2007; Maguire of functional brain imaging results that may be particularly
et al., 2000, 2003; Mechelli et al., 2004). For example, learning a important for the interpretation of arithmetic learning studies. One
second language was found to modify the gray matter density in the problem with fMRI results is their sensitivity to task difficulty.
left inferior parietal cortex (Mechelli et al., 2004). It was also shown Stronger fronto-parietal activation in more difficult arithmetic
that the amount of this change is directly proportional to the conditions might be related to general difficulty effects rather than
individual’s performance improvement and to the age of acquisition. to specific demands on the brain areas essential for numerical
Aydin et al. (2007) addressed the issue of experience-related processing. In general, more difficult tasks activate fronto-parietal
structural brain changes in a group of academic mathematicians, areas more strongly than easier tasks, which are associated with
hypothesizing that the extensive and persistent training in faster response times and higher accuracy. This is important for the
mathematical thinking the mathematicians undergo for several interpretation of results of learning studies, as trained problems or
years should result in specific regional changes in their brains. This tasks are usually solved faster and more accurately than untrained
is, to our knowledge, the only study on structural brain changes after and unfamiliar problems or tasks. A specific interpretation of
long-lasting and extensive training in mathematics. Significantly stronger fronto-parietal activation with regard to numerical
higher gray matter densities were observed in mathematicians than processing has therefore to be motivated by concurring evidence
in non-mathematicians within the inferior parietal lobule, bilat- or by direct contrasts with other non-numerical tasks. In
erally, and in the left inferior frontal gyrus. Also, the period of time neuroimaging research and from patient studies, much indepen-
spent as a mathematician significantly correlated with relative gray dent evidence has been accumulated in recent years for a specific
matter density increase in the right parietal lobule. Long-term and role of the IPS in numerical and arithmetic processing. Contrasts
intense practice in mathematics seems thus to lead to anatomical between numerical and non-numerical tasks also support this
changes in those brain areas that have been previously shown to be interpretation (e.g., Eger et al., 2003), but in most experiments a
related to numerical processing, arithmetic calculation and visuo- suitable non-numerical control condition cannot be constructed,
spatial processing. due to either a lack of comparability or to time limitations.
A second problem with fMRI research is the absence of an
8. Rehabilitation of arithmetic skills in patients absolute baseline. In general, activation foci shown and reported in
fMRI studies always have to originate from comparisons between
A brain lesion that disrupts number processing and calculation conditions. These are, for example, two active conditions of
abilities can clearly compromise a person’s autonomy. Impair- interest that differ with regard to their demands on cognitive
ments of arithmetic fact retrieval are frequently observed after left resources, or an active and a resting condition (baseline). The
hemispheric brain damage, often in association with aphasic problem in the result interpretation is that there is usually no
disorders (e.g., Delazer et al., 1999; Delazer and Bartha, 2001; direct information about the activation pattern of each single
Domahs et al., 2008; Domahs and Delazer, 2005; Grafman et al., condition. It should be noted that a comparison to baseline does
1982; McCloskey et al., 1991). Nonetheless, acquired arithmetic not solve this problem, as the resting brain is not well understood.
deficits are often overlooked and targeted rehabilitation pro- There are brain areas that are more active during the resting state
grammes are rare (for overviews, Girelli and Seron, 2001; Lochy than in conditions where the participants have to perform a
et al., 2005). Most studies on the rehabilitation of simple demanding task (e.g., McKiernan et al., 2003; Raichle and Snyder,
calculation skills have used a drill-based approach, i.e. extensive 2007; Stark and Squire, 2001; Tomasi et al., 2006). It has been
repetition of problems accompanied by immediate feedback (for a argued that, as the participants are idle during resting, the
discussion, e.g., Lochy et al., 2005), but conceptual approaches have activations observed in the precuneus and in left hemisphere
been described as well (Domahs et al., 2003; Girelli et al., 2002). So regions are due to conceptual as well as semantic and linguistic
far, only one study explored how the remediation of simple processing (Binder et al., 1999). During easier tasks, the individuals
arithmetic skills through rehabilitation may change brain activa- might have more time to indulge in these baseline activities than in
tion patterns. Zaunmüller et al. (in press) scanned a patient with a more difficult tasks (Poldrack, 2000).
left-hemispheric brain lesion using fMRI before and after When two active conditions are compared, it is usually
rehabilitation. Rehabilitation consisted of training on multiplica- assumed that the activations resulting from this subtraction are
tion facts. At post-training, the contrast trained versus untrained positive relative to a resting baseline. However, due to the baseline
problems evidenced an increase in activation within the right AG, problem outlined above, stronger activations observed for a low
that means in the hemisphere contralateral to the lesion and difficulty task relative to a high difficulty task might also reflect a
contralateral to the typical activation reported for healthy lesser degree of deactivation for the easier task. This consideration
individuals (e.g., Delazer et al., 2003). A homologue area in the is important in the case of arithmetic learning studies, where the
right hemisphere, contralateral to the lesion, was thus increasingly appearance of a new activation within the left AG is usually
recruited in arithmetic fact retrieval after rehabilitation. This interpreted as evidence for a new cognitive process and for
interesting finding can only be interpreted with caution. Results of functional reorganization (Kelly et al., 2006; Poldrack, 2000). As
single-case fMRI studies may be difficult to replicate because of the the AG is one of the regions within the brain that are often observed
large variability between single patients with regard to a number to be deactivated during task performance, the observed activation
of variables, such as lesion location, lesion extension, aetiology, could be unspecifically related to a decrease of task difficulty due to
associated deficits, spontaneous recovery and neuro-functional learning. A specific interpretation of this activation difference with
reorganization. Furthermore, there is considerable inter-individual regard to arithmetic processing has therefore to be additionally
variability in the healthy adults’ arithmetic performance (e.g., motivated, either by showing that this area is more active during
Grabner et al., 2007; Menon et al., 2000b), and differences in arithmetic processing than during baseline, or by demonstrating a
performance as well as in the level of education or of general specific task-related modulation of the activation difference. In the
918 L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925

training studies on arithmetic, such a specificity has been observed 2007). Also, it has not been investigated yet whether changes in
even when controlling for task difficulty. For example, the brain activation patterns following arithmetic training are stable
activation within the left AG was stronger the more the type of over time and whether they are modulated by the frequency of
task (Ischebeck et al., 2006) or the type of training (Delazer et al., practice. The issue as to whether arithmetic training effects on the
2005) encouraged fact retrieval from long-term memory. human brain are similar or different across the lifespan may be
explored, for example, by comparing individuals from different age
10. Summary and discussion ranges (e.g., children vs. young adults vs. old adults) in brain
activation patterns that are found to be related to the solution of
The present paper reviewed evidence on the functional and trained and untrained problems. Investigating thoroughly how, for
structural changes in the human brain that have been found in example, the level of competence, the use of specific strategies or
relation to experience and practice with arithmetic processing. One the type of learning method specifically affects brain activation
of the key findings is that, in children and in adults, the gain of patterns may help highlighting ways for improving teaching and
arithmetic expertise is characterized by a shift in activation from training programmes, as well as rehabilitation.
fronto-parietal networks to specific parietal areas. This shift Over the past decade, the progress of neuroimaging methods
indicates a change in the cognitive processes involved in task has greatly increased our knowledge about the functional and
performance following practice, although in the case of children structural changes in the learning brain. This has led to a significant
some of the changes observed might also be due to brain maturation. increase in the public interest in neuroscience (OECD, 2007b) as it
Practice leads to a decrease in activation in frontal brain areas. These has raised hopes for significant improvements in the current
areas sustain general-purpose processes such as working memory education systems and teaching methods. Teachers are highly
and attention control, and are likely to be involved in non- interested in exploiting the insights of neuroscience for the benefit
automatised and complex calculation. Practice also leads to a of their students. However, high expectations and lack of under-
relative increase in activation within the left AG. The AG is thought to standing have also led to neuroimaging findings to be overly
mediate, among other functions, memory retrieval of fact knowl- simplified and misunderstood (Goswami, 2006). As Goswami
edge, possibly unspecific with regard to content or knowledge (2006) points out, most advertised brain-based programmes are
domain. Activation changes within the left AG seem to appear even unlikely to work in the long term so that many may naturally
already very early in the course of practice. A further finding is that fall out of use. At present, researchers agree that simple one-to-one
people with average calculation skills build expertise upon a pre- applications of neuroscience findings to the class room cannot
existing cerebral network involved in arithmetic processing, as work. Neuroscience has only entered the first stage of research and
inter-individual arithmetic performance differences are observed to is yet far from yielding solid evidence on how education can be
modulate activation within this network such as, for example, in the made more efficient.
left AG. The proficiency of expert individuals is, however, a different Neuroscience research uses paradigms, concepts and constructs
matter. Calculation prodigies and abacus masters seem to recruit which have been developed in cognitive psychology, neuropsy-
other cognitive processes and cerebral areas. Although only few such chology, or developmental and educational psychology, adding
individuals have been investigated on a case-by-case basis, the new information by directly investigating changes in the brain due
differences observed might be related to the very specific strategies to learning. With this, neuroscience is firmly grounded in the solid
used by the experts. theoretical and empirical basis created over decades of research in
Most of the studies reviewed here show a consistent pattern of the behavioural sciences. For example, the transition from
brain areas that are employed in mental calculation. These areas calculation strategies to arithmetic fact retrieval has been
are the IPS, bilaterally, for manipulation of quantity representa- investigated intensively in developmental psychology (Ashcraft,
tions, the left AG for the retrieval of memorized facts from long- 1992; Barrouillet and Fayol, 1998; Lemaire and Siegler, 1995;
term memory, and frontal brain areas for the demands on Siegler, 1988) and in cognitive psychology (Anderson et al., 1999;
performance monitoring and working memory, especially in Logan, 1988; Logan and Klapp, 1991; Rickard, 2004; Wenger,
complex calculation. Findings of the developmental and training 1999). For centuries (e.g., Thorndike, 1922), there has been a
studies fit well with this interpretation, as development and debate whether more emphasis should be put on learning by drill
training modify activation in these brain areas consistently with or on learning by strategies in the acquisition of new arithmetic
their assumed specific function in calculation. As calculation facts. Neuroscience may contribute to the debate by showing that
becomes more automatic in children, monitoring and working different neural circuits are involved in the retrieval of arithmetic
memory demands decrease, which is reflected in a decrease of facts as a result of the previous training strategy. Neuroscience also
activation within frontal brain areas. Similarly, in the training shows that additional brain areas may be recruited depending on
studies with healthy young adults, the activation decrease within expertise and strategy choice.
fronto-intraparietal areas and the relative activation increase Neuroscience research alone, however, cannot develop pro-
within the left AG reflect a shift from calculation to result retrieval grammes for successful teaching and cannot create learning
from memory. Also, studies investigating inter-individual differ- environments to students. Only as one part of an inter-disciplinary
ences observe that in particular activation within the left AG work-force can neuroscience play an important role (Stern, 2005).
correlates positively with mathematical competence and not with In the present review, we have highlighted how neuroscience
general intelligence. These studies all contribute to provide a enhances our understanding of the learning brain. With other
consistent picture of the brain areas involved in learning researchers (Ansari and Coch, 2006; Goswami, 2006), we agree that
arithmetic in people with average calculation skills. a close collaboration of neuroscience and education may lead in the
The neuroimaging studies reviewed here offer first insights into future to improved teaching and intervention.
the functional and structural brain correlates of the acquisition of
arithmetic skills, and further issues remain to be addressed. Acknowledgements
Training studies may, for example, investigate through morpho-
metric analysis whether intensive, but short-term practice results The first author has been supported by Austrian Science Fund
in specific structural modifications of the brain areas involved in (FWF) Grant P18896-B05 and by Medizinische Forschungsförder-
numeric processing as was found for expert individuals with ung Innsbruck (MFI) Grant PDO-Nr. 12880 (2007-419). The authors
extensive mathematical experience over several years (Aydin et al., thank Ž. Stević for providing the figures and illustrations.
L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925 919

Appendix A

Main results of the revised papers with regard to brain activation/structural changes in relation to number processing are listed here below
(for more details, see the original paper works). Abbreviations: AG = angular gyrus; IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; IPS = intraparietal sulcus;
IOG = inferior occipital gyrus; IPG = inferior parietal gyrus; ITG = inferior temporal gyrus; MFG = middle frontal gyrus; MOG = middle occipital
gyrus; MTG = middle temporal gyrus; SFG = superior frontal gyrus; SMA = supplemental motor area; SMG = supramarginal gyrus;
SOG = superior occipital gyrus; SPG = superior parietal gyrus; STG = superior temporal gyrus.
Region Left hemisphere Right hemisphere

x y z k x y z k

Aydin et al. (2007) (VBM) (p < 0.0001 uncorrected; extent of cluster size I100)
Mathematicians vs. controls (increase in gray matter)
IFG S42 S34 12
Inferior parietal lobule S38 S50 57
Inferior parietal lobule 32 S46 60
Inferior parietal lobule 54 S58 38
Inferior parietal lobule 57 S60 34

Correlation between gray matter density and time spent as a mathematician (r = 0.84, p < 0.01)
Inferior parietal lobule 57 S60 34

Delazer et al. (2003) (fMRI) (block-related analysis; random effect analysis; one-sample t-test; p < 0.001 uncorrected; extent of cluster size >15)
Contrast complex multiplication untrained vs. trained
IFG S50 6 20 32
Inferior parietal lobule S64 S24 24 21
Sylvian fissure S62 S36 20 32
IPS S20 S56 56 43
Lingual gyrus S8 S84 0 35

Contrast complex multiplication trained vs. untrained


ITG S46 S12 S32 20
Paracentral lobule S24 S20 48 17 14 S12 44 81
Cingulate gyrus S12 S44 24 39
Cingulate gyrus S22 S44 32 43
Cerebellum S22 S58 S32 111 14 S52 S36 26
AG S46 S60 24 47

Delazer et al. (2005) (fMRI) (event-related analysis; fixed effect analysis; p < 0.001 uncorrected) (coordinates transformed in Talairach and Tournoux (1988) space)a
Contrast strategy untrained vs. trained
IFG S32 19 S5 377
MFG S28 6 48 110
MFG, IFG 40 2 37 957
Parietal lobe, IPS (bilateral) 24 S64 40 727
Cerebellum S4 S79 S22 91 44 S52 S23 68

Contrast strategy trained vs. untrained


Medial aspect of frontal lobe (bilateral) 12 54 S6 144
Hippocampal gyrus 24 S9 S18 34
MTG 59 S12 S10 53
STG extending to AG S51 S15 8 184
Precuneus 8 S49 25 104
AG extending to STG 59 S50 17 75

Contrast strategy vs. drill


IFG 36 35 S9 54
Cingulate gyrus (bilateral) S12 35 6 127
Precuneus extending to cuneus (bilateral) 4 S65 18 425

Contrast drill vs. strategy


SFG, medial aspect of frontal lobe (bilateral) S16 56 27 853
IFG S55 32 13 78
Precuneus extending laterally to left AG (bilateral) S8 S68 40 593

Grabner et al. (2007) (fMRI) (block design; random effect analysis)


Contrast multi-digit vs. single digit (whole participant group) (p < 0.01 corrected)
IFG S48 36 28 27
Insula S28 26 S4 494 30 30 S2 437
Middle cingulate gyrus S10 16 44 114 10 26 36 116
IFG, Precentral gyrus S46 8 30 183
MFG S26 S2 54 161
Thalamus S10 S22 12 37 24 S28 6 92
IPG S36 S42 44 727 38 S46 42 85
Cerebellum S42 S66 S30 154 36 S68 S26 280
Calcarine cortex 0 S70 10 69
Vermis, left cerebellum 0 S74 S24 394

Contrast single-digit vs. multi-digit (whole participant group) (p < 0.01 corrected)
AG S58 S64 32 24
920 L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925

Appendix A (Continued )
Region Left hemisphere Right hemisphere

x y z k x y z k

Contrast higher vs. lower mathematical competence (p < 0.00001 uncorrected)


Medial SFG S6 54 34 27
SMA S10 18 56 24
MTG S50 S54 16 56
AG S52 S64 28 45

Contrast lower vs. higher mathematical competence (p < 0.00001 uncorrected)


n.s.

Correlation between activation and mathematical–numerical intelligence (r = 0.63, p < 0.001)


AG S50 S64 28 82

Grabner et al. (in press) (fMRI) (block design; random effect analysis; p < 0.05 corrected)
Contrast complex multiplication untrained vs. trained
MFG 45 33 21 80
SMA S6 12 48 165
IFG S45 6 27 207 51 9 27 94
SFG 30 3 63 47
Insula S27 24 3 89 30 24 3 139
Pons, left thalamus 9 S21 S18 74
SPG, SMG, IPG 27 S69 54 219
SPG, IPG S27 S57 48 292
ITG S54 S60 S12 62
Cerebellum S27 S57 S36 30
Cerebellum S36 S69 S27 32
Cerebellum (bilateral) S6 S75 S30 249
MOG S21 S87 12 32

Contrast complex multiplication trained vs. untrained


Middle cingulum S3 S42 36 36
AG S60 S48 39 126 57 S57 24 75

Contrast complex figural-spatial untrained vs. trained


IFG, precentral gyrus S51 30 27 319
IFG 48 33 30 189
Insula S27 21 6 41 30 21 3 84
SMA S9 12 48 72
Precentral gyrus 51 6 27 168
MFG, precentral gyrus S27 0 57 109
SFG 24 S3 54 137
Right SMG, right SPG, left SPG, left IPG 42 S36 42 4100

Contrast complex figural-spatial trained vs. untrained


Precuneus S3 S63 24 129
AG S54 S66 33 64 54 S57 24 99

Hanakawa et al. (2003) (fMRI) (conjunction analysis; p < 0.05 corrected)


Contrast numerical mental-operation task vs. visual fixation task (abacus experts)
Frontal operculum S48 6 28 73 50 10 36 13
Superior precentral sulcus S32 S6 52 34 24 S6 54 103
IPS 42 S52 56 363
Fusiform gyrus S52 S62 S16 8
Precuneus extending to the IPS S18 S66 60 1311
Precuneus 14 S66 64 60
Cerebellar hemisphere S36 S74 S28 14 40 S74 S24 10
Cerebellar vermis 6 S76 S30 11

Contrast numerical mental-operation task vs. visual fixation task (non-experts)


Prefrontal cortex S52 34 16 97
Anterior cingulate cortex extending to medial frontal gyrus 4 14 45 191
Superior precentral sulcus extending to BA 44 S32 0 54 704
Superior precentral sulcus 34 0 54 86
Fusiform gyrus S48 S58 S12 19
Precuneus extending to lateral parietal area and IPS S24 S62 56 810
Cerebellar hemisphere S38 S62 S30 211 30 S63 S32 677

Contrast experts vs. non-experts in the numerical mental-operation task


Posterior superior parietal cortex S22 S62 58 12

Number-size effect for abacus experts


Frontal operculum S48 6 28
Superior precentral sulcus S28 S4 46 24 S4 50
IPS S34 S52 56 40 S58 56
Fusiform gyrus S46 S62 S18
Posterior superior parietal cortex/precuneus S18 S66 60 14 S66 64

Ischebeck et al. (2006) (fMRI) (event-related analysis; random effect analysis; initial threshold: p < 0.001 uncorrected, p < 0.05 corrected on cluster level)
Contrast multiplication untrained vs. trained
L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925 921

Appendix A (Continued )
Region Left hemisphere Right hemisphere

x y z k x y z k

SMA S3 9 51 79
Precentral gyrus, IFG S51 6 S27 164
Thalamus, Caudate S18 S21 15 39
IPS, AG 42 S45 45 104
IPS S33 S66 39 72

Contrast multiplication trained vs. untrained


AG S48 S66 39 51

Contrast subtraction untrained vs. trained


Insula, IFG S33 21 S3 183
Precentral gyrus, IFG 48 6 33 83
IPS S42 S39 39 154
IPS, AG 24 S63 51 35

Contrast subtraction trained vs. untrained


n.s.

Contrast multiplication trained vs. subtraction trained


n.s.
Contrast subtraction trained vs. multiplication trained
IOG, Fusiform gyrus 42 S69 S12 27
Precuneus, SOG 12 S69 45 96
Calcarine gyrus, Cuneus S12 S75 15 31

Contrast multiplication untrained vs. subtraction untrained


n.s.

Contrast subtraction untrained vs. multiplication untrained


MOG, SOG S33 S78 24 127
Lingual gyrus, SOG, Calcarine gyrus S18 S81 S9 287

Ischebeck et al. (2007) (fMRI) (event-related analysis; random effect analysis; initial threshold: p < 0.001 uncorrected, p < 0.05 corrected on cluster level)
Contrast novel vs. repeated (1st third of the experiment)
Anterior cingulate, SMA S6 15 39 60
Caudate 15 6 12 57

Contrast repeated vs. novel (1st third of the experiment)


n.s.
Contrast novel vs. repeated (2nd third of the experiment)
Precentral gyrus, IFG S48 3 30 65
MOG 30 S87 15 80

Contrast repeated vs. novel (2nd third of the experiment)


SMG, AG 51 S45 33 34
AG S39 S66 45 62

Contrast novel vs. repeated (3rd third of the experiment)


IFG, Precentral gyrus S45 9 24 82
Putamen S18 6 0 76

Contrast repeated vs. novel (3rd third of the experiment)


Postcentral gyrus 21 S39 63 48
SMG, STG, AG 63 S48 30 36
AG S45 S60 33 142

Ischebeck et al. (2009) (fMRI) (block-related analysis; random effect analysis; initial threshold: p < 0.001 uncorrected, p < 0.05 corrected on cluster level)
Contrast multiplication untrained vs. trained
Insula S24 18 3 1615b 30 30 6 163
IFG S42 9 24 1615
MFG S30 6 57 1615 45 39 15 58
MFG, SFG 30 9 57 110
b
SMA S6 S15 51 1615
b
Caudate S18 S3 18 110 18 3 21 110
Thalamus 24 S30 6 42
Postcentral gyrus, Inferior parietal lobule 48 S33 51 60
Superior parietal lobule, Inferior parietal lobule S24 S57 51 2557
Superior parietal lobule, SOG 24 S63 63 671
Superior parietal lobule 18 S69 51 671b
b
IOG S33 S69 S6 2557
b
MOG S30 S69 30 2557
MOG S21 S87 3 2557b

Contrast multiplication trained vs. untrained


Posterior cingulate gyrus, Precuneus S9 S48 33 274
AG S45 S69 45 120

Contrast division unrelated vs. related


n.s.
922 L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925

Appendix A (Continued )
Region Left hemisphere Right hemisphere

x y z k x y z k

Contrast division related vs. unrelated


n.s.

Correlation between activation and the behavioral transfer index for trained multiplication (r = 0.60, p < 0.05) and related division (5 = 0.50, p < 0.05)
AG S45 S69 45

Kawashima et al. (2004) (fMRI) (block design; random effect analysis; p < 0.001 uncorrected; p < 0.05 corrected)
Adult group: addition vs. fixation point
Middle frontal S38 6 26 54 10 42
Inferior frontal S38 0 S2
Supplementary motor 4 S2 66
Premotor S22 S6 52
Inferior temporal S42 S62 S22 40 S54 S26
Intraparietal S32 S60 52
Lateral occipital S20 S94 0 28 S94 0

Adult group: subtraction vs. fixation point


Middle frontal S42 6 28
Supplementary motor S6 S4 72
Intraparietal S30 S58 48 34 S46 40
Inferior temporal S40 S78 S8 46 S62 S12
Lateral occipital S34 S92 2 30 S90 S4

Adult group: multiplication vs. fixation point


Middle frontal 52 44 12
Middle frontal S54 16 24 48 8 30
Inferior frontal S22 22 S2
Intraparietal S32 S50 44 22 S64 50
Inferior temporal S46 S76 S10 36 S58 S24
Lateral occipital S18 S92 S8 32 S86 S4

Children group: addition vs. fixation point


Middle frontal S42 16 22
Supplementary motor 0 12 46
Inferior temporal S48 S60 S18 46 S54 S36
Lateral occipital S20 S102 8 24 S100 6

Children group: subtraction vs. fixation point


Middle frontal S46 12 30
Inferior temporal S52 S52 S12 36 S50 34
Intraparietal 38 S56 52
Lateral occipital S18 S102 6 24 S94 6

Children group: multiplication vs. fixation point


Inferior frontal S36 32 4
Middle frontal S50 8 32
Inferior temporal S38 S86 8 46 S58 S24
Lateral occipital S34 S88 8 24 S98 4

Menon et al. (2000b) (short communication; no coordinates are reported)

Pesenti et al. (2001) (PET) (conjunction analysis; p = 0.001 uncorrected)


Contrast calculation vs. memory retrieval (in both prodigy and non-experts)
Inferior frontal sulcus S46 30 22 42 36 26
MFG S32 8 54
Precentral sulcus, Inferior frontal sulcus S38 2 26
SMG S52 S40 46 42 S40 50
Inferior occipito-temporal junction S52 S52 S24
IOG, MOG S50 S60 S14 54 S60 S10
IPS S22 S68 48 22 S70 54
IPS, intraoccipital sulcus S28 S84 22

Contrast calculation vs. memory retrieval (prodigy vs. non-experts)


Anterior cingulate gyrus 4 46 30
MFG 14 26 56
Parahippocampal gyrus 18 8 S44
Paracentral lobule S12 S34 68
Middle occipito-temporal junction 60 S52 S4

Rivera et al. (2005) (fMRI) (block design; random effect analysis; p < 0.01 uncorrected; p < 0.05 corrected)
Positive correlation between activation
and age
SMG, IPS S51 S37 46 292
Lateral occipital-temporal cortex, including S53 S68 7 431
IOG and MOG, ITG and MTG
L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925 923

Appendix A (Continued )
Region Left hemisphere Right hemisphere

x y z k x y z k

Negative correlation between activation and age


SFG and MFG (bilateral), left IFG, left SMA, S16 59 19 2818
left anterior cingulate cortex
Insula, frontal operculum S32 1 S13 246
Basal ganglia including thalamus S10 S4 S5 1481
(bilateral)
Brainstem (bilateral) S10 S20 S17 256
Medial temporal lobe including hippocampus, S32 S33 S2 344
parahippocampal gyrus, fusiform gyrus
k = cluster size (voxel); n.s. = not significant
a
Brett et al. (2001).
b
Activation is part of a bigger cluster.

References Cappelletti, M., Kopelman, M.D., Morton, J., Butterwoth, B., 2005. Dissociations in
numerical abilities revealed by progressive cognitive decline in a patient with
Anderson, J.R., Fincham, J.M., Douglass, S., 1999. Practice and retention: a unifying semantic dementia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 22, 771–793.
analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 25, 1120–1136. Casey, B.J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., Durston, S., 2005. Imaging the developing brain:
Ansari, D., 2008. Effects of development and enculturation on number representa- what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 104–
tion in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 278–291 (Review). 110.
Ansari, D., Coch, D., 2006. Bridges over troubled waters: education and cognitive Castelli, F., Glaser, D.E., Butterworth, B., 2006. Discrete and analogue quantity
neuroscience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 146–151 (Review). processing in the parietal lobe: a function MRI study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
Antell, S.E., Keating, D.P., 1983. Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. USA 103, 4693–4698.
Child Dev. 54, 695–701. Castriota-Scanderberg, A., Hagberg, G.E., Cerasa, A., Committeri, G., Galati, G., Patria,
Ashcraft, M.H., 1992. Cognitive arithmetic: a review of data and theory. Cognition F., Pitzalis, S., Caltagirone, C., Franckowiak, R., 2005. The appreciation of wine by
44, 75–106. sommeliers: a functional magnetic resonance study of sensory integration.
Aydin, K., Ucar, A., Oguz, K.K., Okur, O.O., Agayev, A., Unal, Z., Yilmaz, S., Ozturk, C., NeuroImage 25, 570–578.
2007. Increased gray matter density in the parietal cortex of mathematicians: a Chochon, F., Cohen, L., van De Moortele, P.F., Dehaene, S., 1999. Differential con-
voxel-based morphometry study. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 28, 1859–1864. tributions of the left and right inferior parietal lobules to number processing. J.
Baroody, A.J., 2003. The development of adaptive expertise and flexibility: the Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 617–630.
integration of conceptual and procedural knowledge. In: Baroody, A.J., Dowker, Cipolotti, L., de Lacy Costello, A., 1995. Selective impairment for simple division.
A. (Eds.), The Development of Arithmetic Concepts and Skills: Cons- Cortex 31, 433–449.
tructing Adaptive Expertise. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. Cohen Kadosh, R., Cohen Kadosh, K., Kaas, A., Henik, A., Goebel, R., 2007. Notation-
1–34. dependent and notation-independent representations of numbers in the par-
Barrouillet, P., Fayol, M., 1998. From algorithmic computing to direct retrieval: ietal lobes. Neuron 53, 307–314.
evidence from number and alphabetic arithmetic in children and adults. Mem. Cohen Kadosh, R., Walsh, V., in press. Numerical representation in the parietal
Cogn. 26, 355–368. lobes: abstract or not abstract? Behav. Brain Sci.
Barth, H., La Mont, K., Lipton, J., Spelke, E.S., 2005. Abstract number and arithmetic in Corbett, A.J., MCCusker, E.A., Davidson, O.R., 1986. Acalculia following a dominant-
preschool children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 14116–14121. hemisphere subcortical infarct. Arch. Neurol. 43, 964–966.
Berger, H., 1926. Über Rechenstörungen bei Herderkrankungen des Großhirns. Cowan, R., 2003. Does it all add up? Changes in children’s knowledge of addition
Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 78, 238–263. combinations, strategies, and principles. In: Baroody, A.J., Dowker, A. (Eds.), The
Binder, J.R., Frost, J.A., Hammeke, T.A., Bellgowan, P.S., Rao, S.M., Cox, R.W., 1999. Development of Arithmetic Concepts and Skills: Constructing Adaptive Expertise.
Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state. A functional MRI Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 35–74.
study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 80–95. Dagenbach, D., McCloskey, M., 1992. The organization of arithmetic facts in mem-
Bisanz, J., Sherman, J.L., Rasmussen, C., Ho, E., 2005. Development of arithmetic skills ory: evidence from a brain-damaged patient. Brain Cogn. 20, 345–366.
and knowledge in preschool children. In: Campbell, J.I.D. (Ed.), Handbook of Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., 1997. Cerebral pathways for calculation: double dissociation
Mathematical Cognition. Psychology Press, New York, pp. 143–162. between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex 33, 219–
Bosnyak, D.J., Eaton, R.A., Roberts, L.E., 2004. Distributed auditory cortical repre- 250.
sentations are modified when non-musicians are trained at pitch discrimina- Dehaene, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Cohen, L., 1998. Abstract representations of
tion with 40 Hz amplitude modulated tones. Cereb. Cortex. 14, 1088–1099. numbers in the animal and human brain. Trends Neurosci. 21, 355–361.
Brett, M., Christoff, K., Cusack, R., Lancaster, J., 2001. Using the Talairach atlas with Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Cohen, L., 2003. Three parietal circuits for number
the MNI template. NeuroImage 13, S85. processing. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 20, 487–506.
Brownell, W.A., 1935. Psychological Considerations in the Learning and the Teach- Delazer, M., 2003. Neuropsychological findings on conceptual knowledge of arith-
ing of Arithmetic. The Teaching of Arithmetic. The Tenth Yearbook of the metic. In: Baroody, A.J., Dowker, A. (Eds.), The Development of Arithmetic
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Teachers College, Columbia Concepts and Skills: Constructing Adaptive Expertise. Lawrence Erlbaum
University, New York. Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 385–407.
Buttherworth, B., 2005a. The development of arithmetical abilities. J. Child Psychol. Delazer, M., Bartha, L., 2001. Transcoding and calculation in aphasia. Aphasiology
Psychiatry 46, 3–18 (Review). 15, 649–681.
Buttherworth, B., 2005b. Developmental dyscalculia. In: Campbell, J.I.D. (Ed.), Delazer, M., Benke, T., 1997. Arithmetic facts without meaning. Cortex 33, 697–710.
Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. Psychology Press, New York, pp. 455– Delazer, M., Domahs, F., Bartha, L., Brenneis, C., Lochy, A., Trieb, T., Benke, T., 2003.
467. Learning complex arithmetic—an fMRI study. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 18, 76–88.
Campbell, J.I.D., Parker, H.R., Doetzel, N.L., 2004. Interactive effects of numerical Delazer, M., Domahs, F., Lochy, A., Karner, E., Benke, T., Poewe, W., 2004. Number
surface form and operand parity in cognitive arithmetic. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. processing and basal ganglia dysfunction: a single case study. Neuropsycho-
Mem. Cogn. 30, 51–64. logia 42, 1050–1062.
Campbell, J.I., Timm, J.C., 2000. Adults’ strategy choices for simple addition: effects Delazer, M., Girelli, L., Semenza, C., Denes, G., 1999. Numerical skills and aphasia. J.
of retrieval interference. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 7, 692–699. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 5, 213–221.
Campbell, J.I., Xue, Q., 2001. Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. J. Exp. Psychol. Delazer, M., Ischebeck, A., Domahs, F., Zamarian, L., Koppelstaetter, F., Siedentopf,
Gen. 130, 299–315. C.M., Kaufmann, L., Benke, T., Felber, S., 2005. Learning by strategies and
Callan, A.M., Callan, D.E., Masaki, S., 2005. When meaningless symbols become learning by drill—evidence from an fMRI study. NeuroImage 25, 838–849.
letters: neural activity change in learning new phonograms. NeuroImage 28, Delazer, M., Karner, E., Zamarian, L., Donnemiller, E., Benke, T., 2006. Number
553–562. processing in posterior cortical atrophy—a neuropsychological case study.
Canobi, K.H., Bethune, N.E., 2008. Number words in young children’s conceptual and Neuropsychologia 44, 36–51.
procedural knowledge of addition, subtraction and inversion. Cognition 108, Domahs, F., Bartha, L., Delazer, M., 2003. Rehabilitation of arithmetic abilities:
675–686. different intervention strategies for multiplication. Brain Lang. 87, 165–166.
Cantlon, J.F., Platt, M.L., Brannon, E.M., 2009. Beyond the number domain. Trends Domahs, F., Delazer, M., 2005. Some assumptions and facts about arithmetic facts.
Cogn. Sci. 13, 83–91. Psychol. Sci. 47, 96–111.
Cappelletti, M., Butterworth, B., Kopelman, M.D., 2001. Spared numerical abilities in Domahs, F., Zamarian, L., Delazer, M., 2008. Sound arithmetic: auditory cues in the
a case of semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia 39, 1224–1239. rehabilitation of impaired fact retrieval. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 18, 160–181.
924 L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925

Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., May, A., 2004. Hanakawa, T., Honda, M., Okada, T., Fukuyama, H., Shibasaki, H., 2003. Neural
Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature 427, correlates underlying mental calculation in abacus experts: a functional mag-
311–312. netic resonance imaging study. NeuroImage 19, 296–307.
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H.G., Winkler, J., Büchel, C., May, A., Hecaen, H., Angelergues, T., Houiller, S., 1961. Les varietes cliniques des acalculies
2006. temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during exten- au cours des lesions retroronlandiques. Revue Neurologique 105, 85–103.
sive practice. J. Neurosci. 26, 6314–6317. Henschen, S.E., 1919. Über Sprach-, Musik-, und Rechenmechanismen und ihre
Duffau, H., Denvil, D., Lopes, M., Gasparini, F., Cohen, L., Capelle, L., Van Effenterre, R., Lokalisationen im Großhirn. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psy-
2002. Intraoperative mapping of the cortical areas involved in multiplication chiatrie 52, 273–298.
and subtraction: an electrostimulation study in a patient with a left parietal Henschen, S.E., 1920. Klinische und anatomische Beiträge zur Pathologie des
glioma. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 73, 733–738. Gehirns, vol. 5. Nordiska Bokhandeln, Stockholm.
Durston, S., Casey, B.J., 2006. What have we learned about cognitive development Hittmair-Delazer, M., Sailer, U., Benke, T., 1995. Impaired arithmetic facts but intact
from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia 44, 2149–2157 (Review). conceptual knowledge—single-case study of dyscalculia. Cortex 31, 139–147.
Durston, S., Davidson, M.C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J.A., Casey, Hittmair-Delazer, M., Semenza, C., Denes, G., 1994. Concepts and facts in calcula-
B.J., 2006. A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Dev. tion. Brain 117, 715–728.
Sci. 9, 1–8. Isaacs, E.B., Edmonds, C.J., Lucas, A., Gadian, D.G., 2001. Calculation difficulties in
Duverne, S., Lemaire, P., 2005. Aging and mental arithmetic. In: Campbell, J.I.D. children of very low birthweight: a neural correlate. Brain 124, 1701–
(Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. Psychology Press, New York, pp. 1707.
397–411. Ischebeck, A., Zamarian, L., Egger, K., Schocke, M., Delazer, M., 2007. Imaging early
Eden, G.F., Jones, K.M., Cappell, K., Gareau, L., Wood, F.B., Zeffiro, T.A., Dietz, N.A., practice effects in arithmetic. NeuroImage 36, 993–1003.
Agnew, J.A., Flowers, D.L., 2004. Neural changes following remediation in adult Ischebeck, A., Zamarian, L., Schocke, M., Delazer, M., 2009. Flexible transfer
developmental dyslexia. Neuron 44, 411–422. of knowledge in mental arithmetic—an fMRI study. NeuroImage 44, 1103–
Eger, E., Sterzer, P., Russ, M.O., Giraud, A.L., Kleinschmidt, A., 2003. A supramodal 1112.
number representation in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron 37, 719– Ischebeck, A., Zamarian, L., Siedentopf, C., Koppelstätter, F., Benke, T., Felber, S.,
725. Delazer, M., 2006. How specifically do we learn? Imaging the learning of
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., Taub, E., 1995. Increased cortical multiplication and subtraction. NeuroImage 30, 1365–1375.
representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science 270, 305– Kaufmann, L., Koppelstaetter, F., Siedentopf, C., Haala, I., Haberlandt, E., Zimmer-
307. hackl, L.B., Felber, S., Ischebeck, A., 2006. Neural correlates of the number-size
Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., Orban, G.A., 2003. Parietal interference task in children. Neuroreport 17, 587–591.
representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. J. Cogn. Neurosci. Kawashima, R., Taira, M., Okita, K., Inoue, K., Tajima, N., Yoshida, H., Sasaki, T.,
15, 47–56. Sugiura, M., Watanabe, J., Fukuda, H., 2004. A functional MRI study of simple
Fuson, K.C., 1982. An analysis of the counting-on procedure in addition. In: arithmetic—a comparison between children and adults. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain
Carpenter, T.H., Moser, J.M., Romberg, T.H. (Eds.), Addition and Subtraction: Res. 18, 227–233.
A Cognitive Perspective. Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 67–78. Kazui, H., Kitagaki, H., Mori, E., 2000. Cortical activation during retrieval of arith-
Fuson, K.C., 1988. Children’s Counting and Concepts of Number. Springer, metical facts and actual calculation: a functional magnetic resonance imaging
New York. study. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 54, 479–485.
Fuson, K.C., 1992. Relationships between counting and cardinality from age 2–8. In: Kelly, C., Foxe, J.J., Garavan, H., 2006. Patterns of normal human brain plasticity after
Bideaud, J., Meljac, C., Fisher, J.P. (Eds.), Pathways to Number, Children’s practice and their implications for neurorehabilitation. Arch. Phys. Med. Reha-
Developing Numerical Abilities. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 127– bil. 87, S20–S29 Review.
149. Kirk, E.P., Ashcraft, M.H., 2001. Telling stories: the perils and promise of using verbal
Fuson, K.C., Kwon, Y., 1992. Learning addition and subtraction: effects of number reports to study math strategies. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 27, 157–
words and other cultural tools. In: Bideaud, J., Meljac, C., Fisher, J.P. (Eds.), 175.
Pathways to Number, Children’s Developing Numerical Abilities. Lawrence Kobayashi, C., Glover, G.H., Temple, E., 2007. Cultural and linguistic effects on neural
Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 351–374. bases of ‘Theory of Mind’ in American and Japanese children. Brain Res. 1164,
Gaab, N., Gabrieli, J.D., Deutsch, G.K., Tallal, P., Temple, E., 2007. Neural correlates of 95–107.
rapid auditory processing are disrupted in children with developmental dys- Kong, J., Wang, C., Kwong, K., Vangel, M., Chua, E., Gollub, R., 2005. The neural
lexia and ameliorated with training: an fMRI study. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 25, substrate of arithmetic operations and procedure complexity. Brain Res. Cogn.
295–310. Brain Res. 22, 397–405.
Gaab, N., Gaser, C., Schlaug, G., 2006. Improvement-related functional plasticity Kucian, K., von Aster, M., Loenneker, T., Dietrich, T., Martin, E., 2008. Development of
following pitch memory training. NeuroImage 31, 255–263. neural networks for exact and approximate calculation: a FMRI study. Dev.
Geary, D.C., Frensch, P.A., Wiley, J.G., 1993. Simple and complex mental subtraction: Neuropsychol. 33, 447–473.
strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in younger and older Lampl, Y., Eshel, Y., Gilad, R., Sarova-Pinhas, I., 1994. Selective acalculia with sparing
adults. Psychol. Aging 8, 242–256. of the subtraction process in a patient with left parietotemporal haemorrhage.
Geary, D.C., Hoard, M.K., 2005. Learning disabilities in arithmetic and mathematics: Neurology 44, 1759–1761.
theoretical and empirical perspectives. In: Campbell, J.I.D. (Ed.), Handbook of Landerl, K., Bevan, A., Butterworth, B., 2004. Developmental dyscalculia and basic
Mathematical Cognition. Psychology Press, New York, pp. 253–267. numerical capacities: a study of 8–9-year-old students. Cognition 93, 99–125.
Geary, D.C., Wiley, J.G., 1991. Cognitive addition: strategy choice and speed-of- Lee, K.M., 2000. Cortical areas differentially involved in multiplication and sub-
processing differences in young and elderly adults. Psychol. Aging 6, 474–483. traction: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study and correlation with a
Gelman, R., Meck, E., 1983. Preschoolers counting: principles before skill. Cognition case of selective acalculia. Ann. Neurol. 48, 657–661.
13, 343–359. LeFevre, J.A., Bisanz, J., Daley, K., Buffone, L., Greenham, St., Sadesky, G., 1996a.
Gilmore, C.G., McCarthy, S.E., Spelke, E.S., 2007. Symbolic arithmetic knowledge Multiple routes to solution of single-digit multiplication problems. J. Exp.
without instruction. Nature 447, 589–591. Psychol. Gen. 125, 284–306.
Girelli, L., Bartha, L., Delazer, M., 2002. Strategic learning in the rehabilitation of LeFevre, J.A., Morris, J., 1999. More on the relation between division and multi-
semantic knowledge. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 12, 41–61. plication in simple arithmetic: evidence for mediation of division solutions via
Girelli, L., Seron, X., 2001. Rehabilitation of number processing and calculation multiplication. Mem. Cogn. 27, 803–812.
skills. Aphasiology 15, 695–712. LeFevre, J.A., Sadesky, G., Bisanz, J., 1996b. Selection of procedures in mental
Goh, J.O., Chee, M.W., Tan, J.C., Venkatraman, V., Hebrank, A., Leshikar, E.D., Jenkins, addition: reassessing the problem-size effect in adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn.
L., Sutton, B.P., Gutchess, A.H., Park, D.C., 2007. Age and culture modulate object Mem. Cogn. 22, 216–230.
processing and object-scene binding in the ventral visual area. Cogn. Affect. Lemaire, P., Reder, L., 1999. What affects strategy selection in arithmetic? An
Behav. Neurosci. 7, 44–52. example of parity and five effects on product verification. Mem. Cogn. 27,
Goswami, U., 2006. Neuroscience and education: from research to practice? Nat. 364–382.
Rev. Neurosci. 7, 406–411 Review. Lemaire, P., Siegler, R.S., 1995. Four aspects of strategic change: contributions to
Grabner, R.H., Ansari, D., Reishofer, G., Stern, E., Ebner, F., Neuper, C., 2007. children’s learning of multiplication. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 124, 83–97.
Individual differences in mathematical competence predict parietal brain acti- Lewandowsky, M., Stadelman, E., 1908. Über einen bemerkenswerten Fall von
vation during mental calculation. NeuroImage 38, 346–356. Hirnblutung und über Rechenstörungen bei Herderkrankung des Gehirns.
Grabner, R.H., Ischebeck, A., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K., Delazer, M., Ebner, F., Journal für Psychologie und Neurologie 11, 249–265.
Neuper, C., in press. Fact learning in complex arithmetic and figural-spatial Lochy, A., Domahs, F., Delazer, M., 2005. Assessment and rehabilitation of acquired
tasks: the role of the angular gyrus and its relation to mathematical compe- calculation and number processing disorders. In: Campbell, J.I.D. (Ed.), Hand-
tence. Hum. Brain Mapp. book of Mathematical Cognition. Psychology Press, NY, pp. 469–485.
Grafman, J., Kampen, D., Rosemberg, J., Salazar, A.M., Boller, F., 1989. The progressive Lochy, A., Seron, X., Delazer, M., Butterworth, B., 2000. The odd-even effect in
breakdown of number processing and calculation ability: a case study. Cortex 25, multiplication: parity rule or familiarity with even numbers? Mem. Cogn. 28,
121–133. 358–365.
Grafman, J., Passafiume, D., Faglioni, P., Boller, F., 1982. Calculation disturbances in Logan, G.D., 1988. Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychol. Rev. 95,
adults with focal hemispheric damage. Cortex 18, 37–50. 492–527.
Gruber, O., Indefrey, P., Steinmetz, P., Kleinschmidt, A., 2001. Dissociating neural Logan, G.D., Klapp, S.T., 1991. Automatizing alphabet arithmetic: I. Is extended
correlates of cognitive components in mental calculation. Cereb. Cortex 11, practice necessary to produce automaticity? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn.
350–359. 17, 179–195.
L. Zamarian et al. / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33 (2009) 909–925 925

Lucchelli, F., De Renzi, E., 1993. Primary dyscalculia after a medial frontal lesion of pseudo-word aural repeat and visual decode: before and after treatment.
the left hemisphere. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 56, 304–307. Neuropsychology 21, 732–741.
Maguire, E.A., Gadian, D.G., Johnsrude, I.S., Good, C.D., Ashburner, J., Franckowiak, Rickard, T.C., 2004. Strategy execution in cognitive skill learning: an item-level test
R.S.J., Frith, C.D., 2000. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of candidate models. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 30, 65–82.
of taxi drivers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 97, 4398–4403. Rickard, T.C., 2005. A revised Identical Elements Model of arithmetic fact repre-
Maguire, E.A., Spiers, H.J., Good, C.D., Hartley, T., Franckowiak, R.S.J., Burgess, N., sentation. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 31, 250–257.
2003. Navigation expertise and the human hippocampus: a structural brain Rickard, T.C., Romero, S.G., Basso, G., Wharton, C., Flitman, S., Grafman, J., 2000. The
imaging analysis. Hippocampus 13, 250–259. calculating brain: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 38, 325–335.
McCloskey, M., 1992. Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: evidence Rivera, S.M., Reiss, A.L., Eckert, M.A., Menon, V., 2005. Developmental changes in
from acquired dyscalculia. Cognition 44, 107–157. mental arithmetic: evidence for increased functional specialization in the left
McCloskey, M., Aliminosa, D., Sokol, S.M., 1991. Facts, rules, and procedures in inferior parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1779–1790.
normal calculation: evidence from multiple single-patient studies of impaired Roland, P.E., Friberg, L., 1985. Localisation of cortical areas activated by thinking. J.
arithmetic fact retrieval. Brain Cogn. 17, 154–203. Neurophysiol. 53, 1219–1243.
McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A., Basili, A., 1985. Cognitive mechanisms in number Schlaggar, B.L., Brown, T.T., Lugar, H.M., Visscher, K.M., Miezin, F.M., Petersen, S.E.,
processing and calculation: evidence from dyscalculia. Brain Cogn. 4, 171–196. 2002. Functional neuroanatomical differences between adults and school-age
McKiernan, K.A., Kaufman, J.N., Kucera-Thompson, J., Binder, J.R., 2003. http:// children in the processing of single words. Science 296, 1476–1479.
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12729491?ordinalpos=1&itool=EntrezSystem2 Semenza, C., Miceli, L., Girelli, L., 1997. A deficit for arithmetical procedures: lack of
.PEntrez.Pubmed.Pubmed_ResultsPanel.Pubmed_DefaultReportPanel.Pubmed_ knowledge or lack of monitoring? Cortex 33, 483–498.
RVDocSum A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deacti- Shalev, R.S., 2004. Developmental dyscalculia. J. Child Neurol. 19, 765–771.
vation in functional neuroimaging. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 394–408. Siegler, R.S., 1988. Strategy choice procedures and the development of multiplica-
McNeil, J.E., Warrington, E.K., 1994. A dissociation between addition and subtrac- tion skill. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 117, 258–275.
tion with written calculation. Neuropsychologia 32, 717–728. Simon, O., Mangin, J.F., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., Dehaene, S., 2002. Topographical
Mechelli, A., Crinion, J.T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Franckowiak, layout of hand, eye, calculation, and language-related areas in the human
R.S., Price, C.J., 2004. Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual parietal lobe. Neuron 33, 475–487.
brain. Nature 431, 757. Sokol, S.M., McCloskey, M., Cohen, N.J., Aliminosa, D., 1991. Cognitive representa-
Menon, V., Rivera, S.M., White, C.D., Glover, G.H., Reiss, A.L., 2000a. Dissociating tions and processes in arithmetic: inferences from the performance of brain-
prefrontal and parietal cortex activation during arithmetic processing. Neuro- damaged subjects. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 17, 355–376.
Image 12, 357–365. Stanescu-Cosson, R., Pinel, P., van De Moortele, P.F., Le Bihan, D., Cohen, L., Dehaene,
Menon, V., Rivera, S.M., White, C.D., Eliez, S., Glover, G.H., Reiss, A.L., 2000b. S., 2000. Understanding dissociations in dyscalculia: a brain imaging study of
Functional optimization of arithmetic processing in perfect performers. Brain the impact of number size on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate
Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 9, 343–345. calculation. Brain 123, 2240–2255.
Mix, K.S., Huttenlocher, J., Levine, S., 2002a. Quantitative Development in Infancy Stark, C.E., Squire, L.R., 2001. When zero is not zero: the problem of ambiguous
and Early Childhood. Oxford University Press, New York. baseline conditions in fMRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 12760–12766.
Mix, K.S., Huttenlocher, J., Levine, S.C., 2002b. Multiple cues for quantification in Starkey, P., Cooper, R.G., 1980. Perception of numbers by human infants. Science
infancy: is number one of them? Psychol. Bull. 128, 278–294. 210, 1033–1035.
Münte, T.F., Altenmüller, E., Jäncke, L., 2002. The musician’s brain as a model of Starkey, P., Spelke, E.S., Gelman, R., 1990. Numerical abstraction by human infants.
neuroplasticity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 473–478 (Review). Cognition 36, 97–128.
Noël, M.P., Rousselle, L., Mussolin, C., 2005. Magnitude representation in children: Stern, E., 2005. Pedagogy meets neuroscience. Science 310, 745.
its development and dysfunction. In: Campbell, J.I.D. (Ed.), Handbook of Math- Stigler, J.W., 1984. Mental abacus’’: the effects of abacus training on Chinese
ematical Cognition. Psychology Press, New York, pp. 179–195. children’s mental calculation. Cogn. Psychol. 16, 145–176.
OECD, 2007a. PISA 2006: Science Competencies for Tomorrow’s World 3. Organisa- Talairach, J., Tournoux, P., 1988. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain.
tion for Economic Cooperation and Development. Thieme Medical, New York.
OECD, 2007b. Understanding the Brain: The Birth of a Learning Science. Organisa- Tang, Y., Zhang, W., Chen, K., Feng, S., Ji, Y., Shen, J., Reiman, E.M., Liu, Y., 2006.
tion for Economic Cooperation and Development. Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by cultures. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
Pantev, C., Engelien, A., Candia, V., Elbert, T., 2001. Representational cortex in 103, 10775–10780.
musicians. Plastic alterations in response to musical practice. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Thorndike, E.L., 1922. The Psychology of Arithmetic. The Macmillan Co., New York.
Sci. 930, 300–314. Tomasi, D., Ernst, T., Caparelli, E.C., Chang, L., 2006. Common deactivation patterns
Pantev, C., Ross, B., Fujioka, T., Trainor, L.J., Schulte, M., Schulz, M., 2003. Music and during working memory and visual attention tasks: an intra-subject fMRI study
learning-induced cortical plasticity. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 438–450. at 4 Tesla. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 694–705.
Pantev, C., Oostenveld, R., Engelien, A., Ross, B., Roberts, L.E., Hoke, M., 1998. Trainor, L.J., Shahin, A., Roberts, L.E., 2003. Effects of musical training on the auditory
Increased auditory cortical representation in musicians. Nature 392, 811–814. cortex in children. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 506–513.
Pascual-Leone, A., Torres, F., 1993. Plasticity of the sensorimotor cortex representa- Van Harskamp, N.J., Cipolotti, L., 2001. Selective impairments for addition, sub-
tion of the reading finger in Braille readers. Brain 116, 39–52. traction and multiplication: implications for the organisation of arithmetical
Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F., Menoncello, L., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S.F., Cotelli, facts. Cortex 37, 363–388.
M., Cossu, G., Corte, F., Lorusso, M., Pesenti, S., Gallagher, A., Perani, D., Price, C., Venkatraman, V., Ansari, D., Chee, M.W.L., 2005. Neural correlates of symbolic and
Frith, C.D., Frith, U., 2000. A cultural effect on brain function. Nat. Neurosci. 3, non-symbolic arithmetic. Neuropsychologia 43, 744–753.
91–96. Walsh, V., 2003. A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and
Pauli, P., Lutzenberger, W., Rau, H., Birbaumer, N., Rickard, T.C., Yaroush, R.A., quantity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 483–488.
Bourne Jr., L.E., 1994. Brain potentials during mental arithmetic: effects of Warrington, E.K., 1982. The fractionation of arithmetical skills: a single case study.
extensive practice and problem difficulty. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2, 21–29. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 34, 31–51.
Pesenti, M., Seron, X., Samson, D., Duroux, B., 1999. Basic and exceptional calcula- Wenger, M.J., 1999. On the whats and hows of retrieval in the acquisition of a simple
tion abilities in a calculating prodigy: a case study. Math. Cogn. 5, 97–148. skill. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 25, 1137–1160.
Pesenti, M., Seron, X., van der Linden, M., 1994. Selective impairment as evidence for Whitaker, H.A., Habiger, T., Ivers, R., 1985. Acalculia from a lenticular-caudate
mental organisation of arithmetical facts: BB, a case of preserved subtraction? infarction. Neurology 35, 161.
Cortex 30, 661–671. Wynn, K., 1992. Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature 358, 749–751.
Pesenti, M., Thioux, M., Seron, X., De Volder, A., 2000. Neuroanatomical substrates of Wynn, K., Bloom, P., Chiang, W.C., 2002. Enumeration of collective entities by 5-
Arabic number processing, numerical comparison, and simple addition: a PET month-old infants. Cognition 83, B55–B62.
study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 461–479. Xu, F., Spelke, E., 2000. Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants.
Pesenti, M., Zago, L., Crivello, F., Mellet, E., Samson, D., Duroux, B., Seron, X., Mazoyer, Cognition 74, B1–B11.
B., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., 2001. Mental calculation in a prodigy is sustained by Zago, L., Pesenti, M., Mellet, E., Crivello, F., Mazoyer, B., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., 2001.
right prefrontal and medial temporal areas. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 103–107. Neural correlates of simple and complex mental calculation. NeuroImage 13,
Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., Dehaene, S., 2007. A magnitude code common to 314–327.
numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron 53, Zamarian, L., Bodner, T., Revkin, S.K., Benke, T., Boesch, S.M., Donnemiller, E.,
293–305. Delazer, M., in press. Numerical deficits in a single case of basal ganglia
Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Riviere, D., LeBihan, D., 2001. Modulation of parietal activation by dysfunction. Neurocase.
semantic distance in a number comparison task. NeuroImage 14, 1013–1026. Zamarian, L., López Rolón, A., Delazer, M., 2007. Neuropsychological case studies on
Poldrack, R.A., 2000. Imaging brain plasticity: conceptual and methodological arithmetic processing. In: Berch, D.B., Mazzocco, M.M.M. (Eds.), Why Is Math So
issues—a theoretical review. NeuroImage 12, 1–13. Hard for Some Children? The Nature and Origins of Mathematical Learning
Raichle, M.E., Snyder, A.Z., 2007. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of Difficulties and Disabilities. Brookes Publishing Co. Inc., pp. 245–263.
an evolving idea. NeuroImage 37, 1083–1090. Zaunmüller, L., Domahs, F., Dressel, K., Lonnemann, J., Klein, E., Ischebeck, A.,
Richards, T., Berninger, V., Winn, W., Stock, P., Wagner, R., Muse, A., Maravilla, K., Willmes, K., in press. Rehabilitation of arithmetic fact retrieval via extensive
2007. Functional MRI activation in children with and without dyslexia during practice: a combined fMRI and behavioural case-study. Neuropsychol. Rehabil.

Potrebbero piacerti anche