Sei sulla pagina 1di 27

Journal of Organizational Behavior

J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)


Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/job.344

Linking the Big Five-Factors of personality


to charismatic and transactional
leadership; perceived dynamic work
environment as a moderator
ANNEBEL H. B. DE HOOGH1*, DEANNE N. DEN HARTOG2
AND PAUL L. KOOPMAN1
1
Department of Work and Organizational Psychology, Free University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
2
Department of Management, University of Amsterdam Business School, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Summary In this multi-source study we investigated the relationships between the Big Five personality
traits and both charismatic and transactional leadership behavior, and whether dynamism (the
degree that the work environment is deemed dynamic) moderates these relationships. We also
tested whether dynamism moderates the relationship between leadership behavior and effec-
tiveness. Personality was measured through self ratings using the NEO-PI-R. Subordinates
rated their leaders’ behavior, and peers and superiors provided ratings of effectiveness. Con-
sistent with trait activation theory, results showed that perceived dynamic work environment
moderated the relationships of four of the Big Five-Factors with both charismatic and transac-
tional leadership. Also, charismatic leadership was positively related to perceived effective-
ness, but only in dynamic contexts. Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Introduction

In recent years there has been a resurgence of interest in research on personality traits of effective lea-
ders. This is partly due to the progress that has been made in the field of charismatic or transforma-
tional leadership theory, which attributes importance to personality in predicting leader behavior and
effectiveness (Bryman, 1992; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001). In addition, this interest can be linked to
the emerging consensus regarding the five-factor view of personality that provided a new framework to
integrate empirical findings (e.g., Digman, 1990; Goldberg, 1990).
Recent work demonstrates that stable individual differences in leadership do exist (e.g., Judge,
Bono, Ilies, & Gerhardt, 2002). However, unambiguous links between the five factors and leader

* Correspondence to: Annebel H. B. De Hoogh, Department of Work and Organizational Psychology, Free University, Van der
Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands. E-mail: ahb.de.hoogh@psy.vu.nl

Received 14 June 2004


Revised 15 December 2004
Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 26 April 2005
840 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

behavior have been difficult to establish (e.g., Bono & Judge, 2004; Crant & Bateman, 2000; Judge &
Bono, 2000; Ployhart, Lim, & Chan, 2001). To date there has been little attention in the leadership
literature for the principle of trait activation, which holds that personality traits require trait-relevant
situations for their expression. In other words, an individual will behave in trait-like ways only in those
situations that are relevant to the given trait (Tett & Burnett, 2003). Drawing on trait activation theory,
the relationship between personality and charismatic and transactional leadership may differ depend-
ing on the context and such relationships may only be present in situations in which these leadership
styles encompass viable, trait-relevant responses.
The present study examines the extent to which the work environment is dynamic (i.e., characterized
by challenge and opportunities for change) as a possible moderator of the personality-leadership style
relationship. First, we examine the five factors of personality in relation to charismatic and transac-
tional leadership and test whether the extent of dynamism in the work environment moderates these
relationships. Second, we also address the relationship between leadership and effectiveness and test
for the potential moderating effects of the context in this relationship.

Charismatic and Transactional Leadership

Current research and theory on leadership strongly emphasizes charismatic or transformational


models of leadership as opposed to what Burns (1978) and Bass (1985) have called transactional
models of leadership. Transactional leaders aim to maintain the status quo by rewarding subordi-
nates’ efforts and commitment. Charismatic or transformational leaders, on the other hand,
are believed to change the status quo by infusing work with meaning so that followers’ energies
are mobilized to respond quickly and effectively to demands of the environment (e.g.,
Bass, 1985; Burns, 1978; House, 1977; Pawar & Eastman, 1997; Waldman, Ramirez, House, &
Puranam, 2001). Although transformational and charismatic leadership models differ
somewhat in the way the components are conceptualized, they are often treated as equivalent
(Yulk, 1999). In the present study, we will use the terms charismatic and transformational leader-
ship interchangeably.
Aside from the articulation of an attractive vision for the organization and behaving in ways that
reinforce the values inherent in that vision, some authors hold that charismatic or transformational
leadership should also include empowering behaviors such as, delegation of responsibilities to fol-
lowers, enhancing followers’ capacity to think on their own and encourage them to come up with
new and creative ideas (Yukl, 1999; cf. Dvir, Eden, Avolio, & Shamir, 2002; Kark, Shamir, & Chen,
2003). Transactional leaders influence followers through task-focused behaviors; they clarify expec-
tancies, rules and procedures, emphasizing a fair deal with subordinates (Bass, 1985; Burns, 1978;
House, 1996). Charismatic leadership is believed to build on and augment the impact of transactional
leadership and is expected to motivate followers perform beyond expectations (Bass, 1985; Burns,
1978; House, 1977).
Charismatic leadership theories have made considerable progress in addressing effective leadership
(Yukl, 1999). Many empirical studies and a number of meta-analyses have found that charismatic or
transformational leadership styles are positively related to perceptual and sometimes even financial
performance measures, and that these relationships tend to be stronger than those of transactional
leadership (e.g., Bycio, Hackett, & Allen, 1995; Fuller, Patterson, Hester, & Stringer, 1996; Howell
& Avolio, 1993; Judge & Piccolo, 2004; Lowe, Kroeck, & Sivasubramaniam, 1996; Waldman et al.,
2001).

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 841

Most researchers concerned with charismatic leadership attribute importance to personality in pre-
dicting leader behavior and effectiveness (Bryman, 1992; Den Hartog & Koopman, 2001; Jacobsen &
House, 2001; Judge & Bono, 2000). Over the last decade or so, numerous theoretical models and sev-
eral empirical studies have sought to identify personality traits of charismatic and transactional leaders
(e.g., Bono & Judge, 2004; Church & Waclawski, 1998; Crant & Bateman, 2000; De Hoogh et al.,
2005; Hetland & Sandal, 2003; House & Howell, 1992; Howell & Avolio, 1993; Howell & Higgins,
1990; Judge & Bono, 2000; Kuhnert & Lewis, 1987; Ployhart et al., 2001; Ross & Offermann, 1997;
Sosik, Avolio, & Jung, 2002; Sosik & Dworakivsky, 1998). Together these conceptual models and
empirical studies suggest that various personality traits are possible antecedents of charismatic and
transactional leader behavior.

Five-Factor Model of Personality and Leadership

In personality theory, evidence supporting a five-factor view of personality has been accumulating,
which has led to an emerging consensus on the taxonomy (e.g., Digman, 1990; Goldberg, 1990). Cur-
rent personality theory holds that the basic structure of personality may consist of five factors, often
labeled: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness to experience
(see e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1992b). All five factors of personality are thought to encompass several
correlated but distinct lower level dimensions or traits. The communality of the specific traits defines
each of the five broad factors. The five factors are found consistently through different research meth-
ods across time, contexts, and cultures (e.g., Digman, 1990; Digman & Shmelyov, 1996; McCrae,
Costa, del Pilar, Rolland, & Parker, 1998) and show evidence of heritage (Jang, McCrae, Angleiter,
Riemann, & Livesley, 1998).
In the early days of personality and leadership research there was no taxonomic structure of person-
ality to aid theory development and testing. As a result, numerous different personality traits were
investigated, making integration of results difficult. Nowadays, the Five-Factor model provides a uni-
fied, comprehensive theoretical framework for comparing and accumulating empirical findings.
Accordingly, Judge and colleagues (2002) used the five-factor model as an organizing framework in
their meta-analysis on personality and leadership. The five-factor model explained 16 per cent of var-
iance in leader effectiveness, indicating that leader effectiveness can, to some extent, be predicted from
personality traits when these are organized according to the five-factor model.
To date, little is known about how personality affects leadership. Several studies have linked the Big
Five traits to charismatic leadership and transactional leadership. However, results have differed from
study to study (e.g., Crant & Bateman, 2000; Judge & Bono, 2000; Ployhart et al., 2001). For example,
Judge and Bono (2000) studied a sample of participants of a community service leadership program
who had jobs in a variety of industries and found that self-reports of agreeableness and to a lesser
extent extraversion and openness to experience were related to charismatic leadership. Yet, Crant
and Bateman (2000) studied managers of a financial service organization and reported significant
effects for extraversion but not for the other dimensions of the Big Five. Recently, Bono and Judge
(2004) used the five-factor model as an organizing framework in their meta-analysis on personality
and transformational and transactional leadership. Considerable variability in relationships (strength
and direction) was found across studies included in the meta-analysis, which resulted in generally
weak mean validities for the Big Five factors.
These inconsistent findings suggest that the context in which behavior is assessed may play an
important role. A growing stream of research in the personality field suggests that personality

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
842 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

expression in behavior varies by situation type (e.g., Chatman, Caldwell, & O’Reilly, 1999; Tett &
Burnett, 2003; Tett & Guterman, 2000). However, this premise has received little attention in leader-
ship research to date.

Trait Activation and Charismatic and Transactional Leadership

According to trait activation theory, personality traits require trait-relevant situations for their expres-
sion. In other words, an individual behaves in trait-like ways only in those situations that are relevant to
the given trait. Thus, if one wishes to assess nurturance, one must observe people in situations where
nurturance is a viable response (Tett & Burnett, 2003). Similar points were raised long ago by
researchers such as Murray (1938), Allport (1966), Bem and Funder (1978) and more recently by
Chatman et al. (1999). The deliberate provision of cues for expressing targeted traits can also be recog-
nized in previous research by McClelland, Atkinson, Clark, and Lowell (1958) using the Thematic
Apperception Test (TAT). Trait activation theory has received direct support in a study by Tett and
Guterman (2000), who showed that correlations between self-report trait measures and trait-relevant
behavioral intentions are stronger in situations providing appropriate cues for trait expression.
Drawing on trait activation theory, the relationship of personality with charismatic and transactional
leadership may differ depending on the context in which these leadership styles can be conceived as
viable, trait-relevant responses.
Several authors have argued that charismatic leadership is more likely to emerge in environments
characterized by a high degree of challenge and great opportunities for change, i.e., dynamic environ-
ments (e.g., Bass, 1985; Conger, 1993; Shamir & Howell, 1999). Shamir and Howell (1999, p. 264),
for example state: ‘Charismatic leaders are more likely to emerge ( . . . ) in environments characterized
by a high degree of change and by great opportunities for change than in stable environments that offer
few inducements for change or opportunities for change.’ Likewise, Bass, and Avolio (1993), suggest
that transformational leaders are more likely to come up in organizations facing rapidly changing tech-
nologies and markets than in organizations operating under routine and stable conditions. Shamir and
Howell (1999, p. 264) note with respect to charismatic leaders that: ‘While perceptions of the envir-
onment as calling for change and the identification of opportunities for change are not fully determined
by environmental conditions, and potentially exist in all circumstances, they are more likely to emerge
in dynamic environments.’ Such dynamic environments offer challenge and opportunities for change,
require new interpretations, novel responses, and different levels of effort and investment. In dynamic
environments subordinates’ self-concepts, values and identities can more readily appeal to and
engaged, because of strong orientation needs (Shamir & Howell, 1999). In contrast, in stable environ-
ments, leaders have less room to take dramatic and ‘charismatic’ actions, as these environments offer
fewer inducements for change or opportunities for change. Thus, charismatic leadership may be more
likely to emerge in a dynamic than in a stable work environment.
There is some empirical evidence providing indirect support for the importance of a dynamic work
environment to the emergence of charismatic leadership. Previous research has found evidence of a
link between charismatic leadership and crisis (House, Spangler, & Woycke, 1991) as well as innova-
tion (Howell & Higgins, 1990). Furthermore, Pillai and Meindl (1998) showed that having an organic
structure was positively associated with the emergence of charismatic leadership in work units of a
large organization. Such organic structures tend to be flexible and innovative and face turbulent envir-
onments. Thus, environments characterized by a high degree of challenge and great opportunities for
change (i.e., dynamic work environments) may facilitate the emergence of charismatic leadership

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 843

more than environments that are more structured, stable, and orderly. Therefore, the extent to which an
environment is perceived to be dynamic may be especially important for studying the antecedents of
charismatic leadership.
Alternatively, a more stable environment may be more important for studying the antecedents of
transactional leadership. According to Bass (1985) transactional leadership is most likely to appear
in more routine, stable environments where goals and structures are clear and/or where members work
under formal contracts. Such leadership aims to keep the organization running smoothly and efficiently
by focusing on control by compliance to rules and maintaining stability within the organization rather
than promoting change (Fry, 2003). Transactional leaders may be less likely to emerge in dynamic
circumstances because they tend to be less engaging, choosing to monitor exchange relationships with
employees and maintaining the status quo rather than focusing on change (Ployhart et al., 2001).
Therefore, the degree to which an environment is perceived to be stable may especially be important
for studying the antecedents of transactional leadership.
The results of a study done by Ployhart and colleagues (2001) among participants of a military
training support the idea that the strength of the link between personality and leader behavior may
depend on the context. They found that the effect sizes of the five factor constructs in their relation
to charismatic leadership were stronger for ratings during a two-day assessment exercise than for
ratings done at the end of a three months basic training. The two-day assessment exercise was
designed to challenge candidates’ leadership skills through placing them in dynamic and uncertain
situations. Ratings were collected to evaluate leadership skills over a series of challenging military
tasks. Compared to the three-month basic training, which formed a somewhat more stable, forma-
lized situation that was more predictable to candidates, the circumstances during the two-day
assessment were much more dynamic; offering recruits a great extent of challenge. Ployhart and
colleagues concluded from their results that the link between personality and charismatic leader-
ship might be strongest under conditions that call for charismatic leadership, namely more challen-
ging, dynamic conditions. This proposition has not yet been tested outside the military context. The
present study builds on and extends the research by Ployhart and colleagues (2001). We examine
the relationship between self-ratings of the Big Five-Factors of personality and subordinate ratings
of both charismatic and transactional leadership and we assess dynamic work environment as a
potential moderator of these relationships. Below we present our hypotheses for each of the Big
Five traits.

Big Five traits, dynamic work environment and leadership: hypotheses

Extraversion. Extraverts are social, assertive, active, bold, energetic, and adventurous. Individuals
high on extraversion are dominant in their behavior and expressive when interacting with others
(McCrae & Costa, 1987). Such characteristics play an important role in influencing, persuading and
mobilizing others and are argued to be important for charismatic leaders (Bass, 1985; House, 1977). In
line with these suggestions, in their meta-analysis, Bono and Judge (2004) found extraversion to be the
strongest and most consistent personality correlate of charismatic leadership.
In a dynamic work environment, extraversion may be especially important for charismatic leader-
ship, since in times of turbulence people tend to long for someone communicating clear sense of direc-
tion (Shamir & Howell, 1999). Turbulence may also provide opportunities for leaders to take forceful
actions, which would be far harder or even unacceptable under steady state circumstances (House
et al., 1991). In line with this, Ployhart and colleagues (2001) found that the effect size for extraversion
in explaining charismatic leadership was stronger under conditions of challenge than in a more stable

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
844 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

environment. Thus, we hypothesize that extraversion will be positively related to charismatic leader-
ship, especially so in a dynamic work environment.
Hypothesis 1: Extraversion will be positively related to charismatic leadership and this relationship
will be stronger in dynamic than in stable work environments.

Openness to experience. Individuals scoring high on openness to experience are characterized by


traits such as imagination, unconventionality, autonomy, creativity, and divergent thinking (McCrae
& Costa, 1987). Their prowess in esoteric thinking and fantasy as well as deliberation of social values
(McCrae, 1996) may play a role in the articulation of an attractive vision, a key behavior for charis-
matic leaders. Also, they are creative and divergent thinkers, open to change and new experiences
(McCrae & Costa, 1987) and display independence of judgment and autonomy (Woodman, Sawyer,
& Griffin, 1993). This may make them more likely to find new opportunities and to use unconventional
methods to reach organizational goals, behavior that is often seen as relevant to charismatic leaders
(e.g., Conger & Kanungo, 1994; Conger, Kanungo, Menon, & Mathur, 1997). Empirical evidence
indeed links openness to experience to charismatic leadership (Bono & Judge, 2004; Judge & Bono,
2000).
In a dynamic work environment, openness may especially be important for charismatic leadership,
since these environments offer a high degree of challenge, opportunities for change and they require
new interpretations and novel responses. Accordingly, Ployhart and colleagues (2001) found that
openness to experience explained variance in charismatic leadership, but only in more challenging
conditions. Therefore, we hypothesize that openness to experience will be positively related to charis-
matic leadership, especially so in a dynamic work environment.
Hypothesis 2: Openness to experience will be positively related to charismatic leadership and this
relationship will be stronger in dynamic than in stable work environments.

Agreeableness. Agreeable individuals are altruistic, warm, generous, trusting, and cooperative
(McCrae & Costa, 1987; Costa & McCrae, 1992a). The pro-social aspect of agreeableness may be
an asset to charismatic leaders as agreeable individuals are friendly and sympathetic and arouse liking
in other people (Costa & McCrae, 1988). Also, agreeable people are concerned with others’ interests.
This may be a prerequisite to be able to understand subordinates’ perspective and infuse their work
with meaning, a central characteristic of charismatic leadership (Bass, 1985; Burns, 1978; House,
1977). Being concerned with others may also help charismatic leaders to attend to individual needs
of followers. This willingness and ability to attend to the individual needs of followers is the core
of individualized consideration, a leader behavior which many authors see as an important part of
transformational or charismatic leadership (e.g., Bass, 1985; Conger & Kanungo, 1994; Shamir,
Zakay, Breinin, & Popper, 1998). In line with this, Judge and Bono (2000) found a positive relationship
between agreeableness and charismatic leadership. The measure of charismatic leadership used in the
present study combines the articulation of an attractive vision, providing meaning to follower’s work,
and role modeling of desired behavior with empowering leader behaviors. These include aspects of
individualized consideration, intellectual stimulation and participation in decision-making. Research
has found agreeableness to be positively related to a preference for participative styles of management
(Stevens & Ash, 2001). To be able to delegate and share sensitive information one needs to be trusting
and straightforward, which both are facets of agreeableness. Taking these results together with the
positive relationship found by Judge and Bono (2000), this suggests a positive relationship between
agreeableness and our measure charismatic leadership might be found.
However, we feel that agreeableness may also be a hindrance to charismatic leaders, because highly
agreeable individuals tend to be submissive and conforming (see e.g., Graziano & Eisenberg, 1997). In

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 845

line with this and in contrast with the results reported by Judge and Bono (2000), Lim and Ployhart
(2004) found a negative relationship between agreeableness and charismatic leadership in their study.
Judge and Bono (2000) studied a sample of participants of a community service leadership program
who had jobs in a variety of industries, whereas Lim and Ployhart conducted their study among mili-
tary personnel. Lim and Ployhart suggested that in critical, risky situations (such as the ones military
personnel are often confronted with) agreeableness may not contribute to perceptions of charismatic
leadership, since in times of turbulence people long for a strong leader, communicating a clear sense of
direction (Shamir & Howell, 1999). Highly agreeable individuals may tend to be overly compliant
and try to accommodate everyone, even when they have differing viewpoints or interests (see e.g.,
Graziano & Eisenberg, 1997). This may make them seem less decisive and less confident of their
own vision, especially in times of turbulence when such differences are likely to be more salient.
Agreeable people may therefore be viewed as less charismatic under these circumstances. Whereas,
in a more stable context, agreeableness may be an asset to charismatic leaders, as it may help increase
the quality of their interaction with followers. Thus, we hypothesize that agreeableness will be
negatively related to charismatic leadership in dynamic work environments, and positively in stable
environments.
Hypothesis 3: Agreeableness will be negatively related to charismatic leadership in dynamic work
environments and positively in stable work environments.
Transactional leaders engage in a relationship of mutual dependence with their followers, in which
the contributions of both sides are acknowledged and rewarded. As such, transactional leaders must
meet and respond to the reactions and changing expectations of their followers (Kuhnert & Lewis,
1987). As cooperation and empathy are both hallmarks of agreeableness (Costa & McCrae, 1992a),
agreeableness may be an asset for transactional leaders. Agreeable individuals also tend to be trusting
and straightforward which may help in the process of offering a fair deal to subordinates, behavior
relevant to transactional leadership (Bass, 1985; Burns, 1978; De Hoogh, Den Hartog, & Koopman,
2004; House, 1996). In line with this, the meta-analysis by Bono and Judge (2004) reported a positive
relationship between agreeableness and transactional leadership. However, there was considerable
variability in the size of correlations across the studies included in the meta-analysis, including
some studies reporting negative relationships. Thus, there is sufficient room to test for potential
moderators.
Stable work environments provide a high degree of structure with little ambiguity in pursuing the
objectives of the job and such lack of ambiguity may help ensuring fair and equal treatment and setting
clear and stable guidelines. Agreeableness may be especially important for transactional leadership in
such an environment. In contrast, under more dynamic conditions, leaders who seem more compliant
and less decisive may be viewed as too passive and rated as less transactional. Therefore, we hypothe-
size that agreeableness will be positively related to transactional leadership in stable work environ-
ments and negatively in dynamic work environments.
Hypothesis 4: Agreeableness will be negatively related to transactional leadership in dynamic work
environments and positively in stable work environments.

Conscientiousness. Conscientiousness encompasses dependability, responsibility, dutifulness,


deliberation, achievement orientation, and a concern for following established rules (McCrae & Costa,
1987). Conscientiousness could be seen as part of charismatic leadership, as highly conscientious lea-
ders may inspire followers to perform beyond expectation through setting high standards and acting
dutifully themselves (e.g., Bass, 1985). However, as indicated, conscientiousness also reflects the ten-
dency to be cautious, thoughtful and a strict adherence to standards of conduct (Costa, McCrae, & Dye,

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
846 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

1991). Moreover, Diener, Larsen, and Emmons (1984) found that need for order was negatively related
to choice of novel situations. Therefore, although conscientiousness may be an asset to charismatic
leaders, helping them to set challenging goals, conscientiousness may also be a liability, especially
in a dynamic work environment, as highly conscientious leaders may stick to agreed upon regulations
rather than grasp opportunities at hand. Thus, in a dynamic work environment, conscientiousness may
be negatively related to charismatic leadership. Bono and Judge (2004) found a positive relationship
between conscientiousness and charismatic leadership in their meta-analysis, but again the variability
across studies included in their analysis was considerable, including some studies reporting negative
relationships. Thus, there is sufficient room to test for potential moderators. Considering the above, we
hypothesize that conscientiousness will be negatively related to charismatic leadership in dynamic and
positively in stable work environments.
Hypothesis 5: Conscientiousness will be negatively related to charismatic leadership in dynamic
work environments and positively in stable work environments.
Transactional leaders are characterized by task-focused behaviors; they clarify expectancies, rules
and procedures, emphasizing a fair deal with subordinates (Bass, 1985; Burns, 1978; De Hoogh, Den
Hartog, & Koopman, 2004a; House, 1996). Since individuals high on conscientiousness are precise
and systematic and make detailed and thoughtful plans (Costa & McCrae, 1992a; McCrae & Costa,
1987), conscientiousness is likely to be positively related to transactional leadership. Furthermore, to
monitor successfully, one needs to strictly adhere to standards of conduct, also an element of conscien-
tiousness. Bono and Judge (2004) however do not find this hypothesized positive relationship between
conscientiousness and transactional leadership. The context may play a role here. We expect that con-
scientiousness is especially important for transactional leadership in a stable work environment, as this
environment offers more prescribed, formalized and defined goals and structures. Under more dynamic
conditions, leaders who tend to stick strictly to the preset exchange relationships with employees and
stress maintaining the status quo even when conditions have changed and the former rules and proce-
dures may no longer apply or be fair, may be viewed as too passive or rigid and thus rated as less
transactional. Thus, we hypothesize that conscientiousness will be positively related to transactional
leadership in stable and negatively in dynamic work environments.
Hypothesis 6: Conscientiousness will be negatively related to transactional leadership in dynamic
work environments and positively in stable work environments.

Neuroticism. Neuroticism reflects the tendency to be anxious, defensive, insecure, and emotional
(McCrae & Costa, 1987). It is associated with a lack of self-confidence (McCrae & Costa, 1991).
In contrast, self-confidence is argued to be a central characteristic of charismatic leaders (Bass,
1990; House, 1977). Thus, neuroticism is likely to hinder charismatic leaders, since neurotic leaders
may be unable to picture a competence and trustworthy leader. Indeed, Lim, and Ployhart (2004) found
a negative relationship between neuroticism and charismatic leadership in their military sample. Judge
and Bono (2000) as well as Crant and Bateman (2000), however, failed to find the hypothesized nega-
tive relationship between neuroticism and charismatic leadership. Lim and Ployhart (2004) suggested
that the existence of moderators on the relationship between neuroticism and transformational leader-
ship may explain these contradictive results. Compared with business leaders of the Judge and Bono
(2000) and Crant and Bateman (2000) sample, military personnel often have to work under hazardous
and life-threatening situations, hence the ability to remain calm, secure and non-anxious is critical.
Thus, in risky, tumultuous situations neuroticism may especially be a hindrance to charismatic leaders,
since in times of turbulence people long for a strong command structure and leadership. This may also
extend to dynamic situations. Therefore, we hypothesize:

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 847

Hypothesis 7: Neuroticism will be negatively related to charismatic leadership, and this relationship
will be stronger in dynamic than in stable work environments.

Charismatic leadership, transactional leadership and effectiveness

The first aim of the present study was to address the relationship between personality and charismatic
and transactional leadership, and to test the potential moderating role of a perceived dynamic work
environment. The second aim was to address the effectiveness of these two styles and assess whether
context also moderates this relationship. Several authors have argued that whereas transactional lea-
ders motivate subordinates to perform as expected, the transformational or charismatic leader inspires
followers to go beyond expectations (e.g., Bass, 1985; Burns, 1978; House, 1977). Transactional lea-
ders offer their subordinates a fair deal. Subordinates of such leaders know that their effort will result
in goal attainment and that they will be justly rewarded by their leader for doing what is expected from
them (e.g., House, 1996). Charismatic leaders are believed to infuse followers’ work with values by
articulating an attractive vision, and to behave in ways that reinforce the values inherent in that vision,
so that followers become highly committed to the goal of the collective and achieve more then they are
expected to do (e.g., Shamir, House, & Arthur, 1993). Research shows that transformational leadership
tends to be more effective than transactional leadership (e.g., Fuller et al., 1996; Judge & Piccolo,
2004; Lowe et al., 1996). Thus, we hypothesize that charismatic will be strongly related to perceived
leader effectiveness, and transactional behaviors less so.
Hypothesis 8: Charismatic leader behavior is positively related to perceived effectiveness and more
strongly so than transactional leadership.
Above, we propose that the extent to which a work environment is perceived as dynamic may affect
the emergence or antecedents of charismatic and transactional leadership styles. There is also some
research that suggests that charismatic leaders are more effective in changeable, dynamic environ-
ments than in more certain or stable ones. For example, Waldman et al. (2001) found that the relation-
ship between CEO charismatic leadership and firm financial performance was positive, but this only
held when environmental conditions were uncertain and changeable. Also, De Hoogh et al. (2004b)
found that the relationship between charismatic leadership and subordinates’ positive work attitude to
be stronger in such uncertain, unpredictable environments. As stated, we see a dynamic environment as
a relatively uncertain situation, characterized by a high degree of challenge and great opportunities for
change. In such an environment, charismatic leaders can more easily generate appeal for their vision
(Waldman et al., 2001; Trice & Beyer, 1986). Charismatic leaders can direct follower’s attention to the
existence of opportunities for change, increase their optimism regarding that change, and mobilize
their energies to devote themselves to the attainment of the vision. Such commitment and effort on
the part of the members is expected to enable organizations to respond more quickly and effectively
to environmental shifts and changes (Howell & Avolio, 1993; Shamir & Howell, 1999). As a result,
charismatic leaders are likely perceived as more effective in dynamic work environments than in more
stable ones. Therefore, we hypothesize:
Hypothesis 9: The relationship between charismatic leader behavior and perceived effectiveness
will be stronger in dynamic work environments than in more stable work environments.
Thus the present study has two aims. First, we examine the relationship between self-ratings of the
Big Five-Factors of personality and subordinate ratings of charismatic and transactional leadership and
assess the role of a dynamic work environment as a potential moderator of these relationships. Second
we look at the relationship between leadership and effectiveness and again test the role of dynamism as

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
848 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

a potential moderator. Most previous studies use a cross-sectional design to investigate links between
personality and leadership. Here, we use a longitudinal design. Since the results of studies linking per-
sonality to leadership may have potential benefits for the selection and development of leaders, the Big
Five-Factors were assessed during an evaluation of managerial potential (time 1 measurement, see also
method). At time 2, subordinates rated their focal manager’s leader behavior and peers and/or super-
iors provided ratings of effectiveness. The focal managers themselves provided a measure of perceived
dynamic work environment (time 2). Managers’ own perceptions of the environment are most relevant
for explaining trait relevant behavior as compared to for example perceptions of subordinates or peers,
which is the primary aim of our study. Moreover, in this way, each of the variables measured in this
study was assessed by different raters or at different time points, minimizing common source and rater
bias. The method is outlined in more detail below.

Organizational Context

The data for this article were collected in 2002/2003. At the time of the study the economy in the
Netherlands was in a period of a mild recession. Data collection was done in collaboration with
consultancy firm LTP, one of the leading management consulting firms in the Netherlands, espe-
cially in the area of assessment. Besides assessment LTP operates in the areas of organization diag-
nostics, organization development, culture change programs, management development, coaching
and HR-internet tools. The company was founded in 1927 as an offspring of the Free University of
Amsterdam and works with many different Dutch and international client companies as KLM, ING,
Ernst & Young, ABN AMRO, Solvay, Nissan, as well as several state departments and the judiciary
(for example, all candidates for the position of judge in the Netherlands are assessed by LTP). LTP
still values its scientific background and regularly initiates and participates in research projects.
The sample in this study consists of 83 managers from many different backgrounds who under-
went an assessment for evaluating their managerial potential either for selection (35 per cent) or
developmental purposes (65 per cent) at LTP, and nine months later participated in our multi-source
feedback study. As an incentive, the managers were offered the opportunity to receive a multi-
source feedback report on their leadership styles following participation in the follow-up study.
Most managers who had taken part in the management assessment for personnel selection purposes
and participated in the follow up study were given a positive advice regarding the job for which they
had applied (89.66 per cent). The managers were employed in a diverse cross-section of areas (e.g.,
production, sales, engineering, finance, human resource management), and organizational levels
(28 per cent higher-level managers, 57 per cent middle-level managers, 13 per cent lower-level man-
agers). One third of the managers were women.

Method

Procedure
Participants of a 1-day assessment for evaluating managerial potential at a psychological consulting
firm (n ¼ 341) completed a personality inventory during this assessment (time 1). At the end of the

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 849

assessment day, all participants received a letter with an invitation to take part in a 360-degree feed-
back survey administration to be conducted nine months later. As an incentive, they were offered the
opportunity to receive a multi-source feedback report on their leadership styles following participation
in the follow-up study. In total, 73.04 per cent indicated to be willing to participate by filling out the
relevant form. The final sample size consists of 83 managers who completed a personality inventory
during an assessment (time 1) and nine months later (time 2) participated in a multi-source feedback
study.
Nine months after they had participated in the assessment, survey packets were sent to the homes of
these candidates (time 2). Survey packets contained a questionnaire to be completed by the participant
him-or herself, and six other questionnaires to be completed by the subordinates, superiors, and peers
of the participant. A letter from the researchers assuring confidentiality was sent with each question-
naire. The questionnaires filled out by others were completed anonymously and returned directly to the
researchers in pre-addressed envelopes. Code numbers were included on surveys so that respondents
could be correctly matched for subsequent data analyses.
For 84 candidates we received subordinates surveys containing ratings of leadership style
(24.63 per cent response rate). For 61 of these participants, superiors and/or peers provided ratings
of perceived leader effectiveness. Thus, we used self ratings to measure personality and dynamism,
subordinate ratings to measure leadership styles and peer and/or superior ratings to measure perceived
leader effectiveness, ensuring separate data sources.
Twenty-nine of the participants at time 2 had taken part in the management assessment at time 1 for
personnel selection purposes and 89.66 per cent of them were given a positive advice regarding the job
for which they had applied. The other 55 participants underwent the assessment for developmental
purposes. The average age of the participants was approximately 40 years. Twenty-eight of these par-
ticipants were female.
In the self-report questionnaire at time 2 (n ¼ 82), 83 per cent of the participants indicated that they
had been in their current jobs for 6 months or more (99 per cent at least 3 months). Most participants
(82 per cent) had 6 or more subordinates reporting to them directly and 10 or more subordinates report-
ing to them indirectly. The participants had jobs in a wide range of areas, including production, sales,
engineering, finance, human resource management, and so on. One third of these participants worked
in small to medium sized organizations, the other two thirds worked for organizations with more than
500 employees. Twenty-three participants indicated that they were higher-level managers, 47 were
middle-level managers and 11 considerate themselves lower-level managers. One respondent indicated
he currently did not have a management position; therefore his data was left out of further analyses.
Summarizing, the sample in this study consists of 83 managers who completed a personality inven-
tory during an assessment for evaluating managerial potential (time 1) and nine months later (time 2)
participated in a multi-source feedback study. At time 2, subordinates (n ¼ 256, mean ¼ 3 per manager,
for 75 managers more than one subordinate survey available) rated these managers’ leadership style. In
addition, superiors (n ¼ 39) and/or peers (n ¼ 69) provided ratings of perceived leader effectiveness
(n ¼ 61 managers, mean ¼ 1.77 peer and/or superior rating per manager). At time 2, the focal managers
also filled out a questionnaire in which they indicated the degree to which they perceived their work
environment to be dynamic (n ¼ 80). Figure 1 depicts the research design.
To check for selective non-response, the managers who participated in the multi-source feedback
study at time 2 were compared with those who did not. A multivariate analysis of variance including
gender, age and the Big Five-Factors suggested no significant differences, F(28, 87365) ¼ 0.94,
p > 0.05. Furthermore, we compared the managers in our study for which ratings of perceived leader
effectiveness were available with the group for which none were available. A multivariate analysis of
variance including gender, age, and leadership styles again suggested no significant differences,
F(10, 8248) ¼ 0.92, p > 0.05.

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
850 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

Figure 1. Research design

Measures

Big Five personality traits


The Big Five personality traits were measured with the 240-item revised NEO personality inventory
(Costa & McCrae, 1992b; Authorized Dutch translation by Hoekstra, Ormel, & De Fruyt, 1996; time
1), probably the most extensively validated self-report measure of the Five Factor model (Judge &
Bono, 2000). In the NEO-PI-R the broad five-factor constructs each represent six more specific traits,
called facets. Neuroticism is composed of the facets anxiety, angry hostility, depression, self-con-
sciousness, impulsiveness, and vulnerability. Extraversion consists of the facets warmth, gregarious-
ness, assertiveness, activity, excitement seeking, and positive emotions. Openness to experience
encompasses fantasy, aesthetics, feelings, actions, ideas, and values. Agreeableness includes trust,
straightforwardness, altruism, compliance, modesty, and tender-mindedness. Finally, conscientious-
ness is composed of competence, order, dutifulness, achievement striving, self-discipline, and delib-
eration. Each facet is measured with eight items, and thus each construct is measured with 48 items
(see for sample items Costa & McCrae, 1992b; Hoekstra et al., 1996). Sample reliabilities for the five
factors were 0.86 for neuroticism, 0.79 for extraversion, 0.64 for openness to experience, 0.69 for
agreeableness and, finally, 0.82 for conscientiousness. The reliabilities of two of these five factors
are somewhat low; however, they are in line with past research (Costa & McCrae, 1992b; see also
Hoekstra et al., 1996), where the average is 0.77 across traits. We decided to retain the official scales
to ensure comparability of the results to other studies using this measure. The items have a 5-point
response scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

Leadership styles
Charismatic and transactional leader behaviors were rated by subordinates (at time 2) using two scales
from the Charismatic Leadership in Organizations questionnaire (CLIO; De Hoogh et al., 2004a). The
charismatic leadership scale contains 11 items, which reflect the articulation of an attractive vision,
providing meaning to follower’s work, role modeling of desired behavior, power sharing, intellectual
stimulation, and consideration. Examples of items measuring charismatic leadership are, ‘Has a vision
and imagination of the future,’ ‘Encourages subordinates to develop their potential,’ and ‘Displays
conviction in his/her ideals, beliefs, and values.’ The items have a seven-point response scale, ranging
from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). This scale had an alpha coefficient of 0.88 (n ¼ 256).

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 851

The transactional leadership scale contains 6 items emphasizing fairness of the deal leaders make with
their subordinates. Examples of items are, ‘Ensures that agreements are being kept,’ ‘Can be relied on
to meet obligations,’ and ‘Does not criticize subordinates without good reason.’ The items have a 7-
point response scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The transactional scale
had an alpha coefficient of 0.81 (n ¼ 256).

Perceived effectiveness
Superiors and/or peers provided a measure of perceived leader effectiveness based on three items
(time 2): ‘To what extent is the overall functioning of the person you evaluate satisfactory?’ ‘How cap-
able is the person you are evaluating as a leader?’ and ‘How effective is the person you are evaluating
as a leader?’ Responses were given on a 7-point response scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very
much so). The perceived effectiveness measures had an alpha coefficient of 0.86 (n ¼ 108).

Dynamic work environment


The focal managers provided a measure of perceived dynamic work environment based on three items
(time 2): ‘What is the extent of challenge in your work environment?’ ‘To which degree is your work
environment dynamic?’ And: ‘To what extent does your work environment offer great opportunities
for change?’ The items were rated on a 7-point response scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very
much so). The dynamic work environment scale had an alpha coefficient of 0.68 (n ¼ 80).

Results

Rater agreement

To examine the justification for aggregating individual responses to characterize the leadership style of
focal managers we performed one way-analyses of variance with focal managers as the independent
variable and the mean scores of subordinate raters for both leadership styles as the dependent variables.
Results showed that, for all variables, the between-group variance was significantly different from
zero. Next, two kinds of intra-class correlation coefficients were calculated ICC(1) and ICC(2) (see
Shrout & Fleiss, 1979). The ICC(1) coefficient is an estimate of the degree to which subordinates
of the same focal manager respond similarly. The ICC(1)’s for subordinate ratings of charismatic
and transactional leadership were 0.15 and 0.16, respectively. These values are in line with the median
value of ICC(1) reported in the organizational literature, which equals 0.12 (James, 1982). The ICC(2)
coefficient is an indicator of inter-rater reliability. It marks the degree to which the group means can be
reliably differentiated from each other. The ICC(2)’s for subordinate ratings of charismatic and trans-
actional leadership were 0.55 and 0.57, respectively. These values of ICC(2) are marginal, though can
be considered acceptable given that a mean of only three subordinates rated their managers and the
ICC(2) index is dependent on the number of raters per group (Bliese, 2000). Taken together, these
results provide support for combining subordinates’ responses to provide averaged, aggregated scores
for charismatic and transactional leadership. The leadership dimensions seem sufficiently valid at the
group level to be aggregated and reported at the group level.

Personality and leadership


Table 1 shows the means, standard deviations and correlations for each of the variables. As it can be
seen, the inter-correlations among the five factors of personality are rather high. Neuroticism correlates

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
852 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlations among variables


Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. Extraversion 3.66 0.30


2. Openness to experience 3.52 0.30 0.40**
3. Conscientiousness 3.82 0.31 0.52** 0.03
4. Agreeableness 3.57 0.27 0.21 0.03 0.31**
5. Neuroticism 2.14 0.35 0.45** 0.08 0.66** 0.28*
6. Charismatic 5.31 0.54 0.05 0.08 0.03 0.07 0.18
7. Transactional 5.41 0.58 0.06 0.13 0.04 0.07 0.00 0.49**
8. Effectiveness 5.64 0.62 0.05 0.06 0.05 0.24 0.11 0.07 0.14
9. Dynamic 5.76 0.79 0.09 0.18 0.04 0.25* 0.18 0.14 0.04 0.19a
Note: n ¼ 83, except for dynamic work environment n ¼ 80 and for effectiveness n ¼ 61.
a
n ¼ 60.
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

significantly negative with the other factors (ranging from 0.28 to 0.66), except for agreeableness
(0.08). Furthermore, extraversion correlates significantly positive with openness to experience (0.40)
and conscientiousness (0.52). Conscientiousness correlates significantly positive with agreeableness
(0.31). These correlations are similar to correlations found in previous research when this version
of the NEO-PI-R was administrated to job applicants (see Hoekstra et al., 1996).
The correlations between the five factors of personality and the other variables used in this study are
low, except for the correlation between agreeableness and perceived dynamic work environment
(0.25). Thus, no significant correlations were found between the Big Five-Factors of personality
and charismatic or transactional leadership.
Besides correlation coefficients, we also report standardized regression coefficients for these rela-
tionships in which the effect of one trait is adjusted for the influence of the other traits (see also Judge
& Bono, 2000). For this we regressed charismatic and transactional leadership on the Big Five-Factors
in two separate analyses, as reported in Table 2. Neuroticism had a positive relationship with charis-
matic leadership when the effects of the other Big Five traits were controlled for, suggesting that it
explains a unique portion of variance that is not captured by any of the others variables,  ¼ 0.30,
p ¼ 0.05. However, this model did not reach significance, R2 ¼ 0.06, F ¼ 1.01, p ¼ 0.42. As can be seen
from Table 2, charismatic and transactional leadership were not significantly explained by the Big Five
personality traits. A separate regression analysis also showed that the five factors were not related to
perceived effectiveness (also reported in Table 2).
To examine the effect of perceived dynamic work environment on the relationship between the
Big Five constructs and charismatic leadership, we conducted separate moderated multiple regression
analyses. Although ideally one would include all interaction effects in a single analysis, our relatively

Table 2. Results of hierarchical regression analyses for five factor variables explaining
charismatic and transformational leadership, and perceived effectiveness
Variable Charismatic Transactional Effectiveness

1. Extraversion 0.02 0.07 0.01


2. Openness to experience 0.06 0.10 0.07
3. Conscientiousness 0.13 0.09 0.12
4. Agreeableness 0.11 0.07 0.24
5. Neuroticism 0.30* 0.04 0.13
R2 0.06 0.03 0.07
Note: n ¼ 83, except for effectiveness n ¼ 61. Standardized regression coefficients are shown.
*p ¼ 0.05.

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 853

small sample size (and related low power) and the high correlations between some of the personality
factors required us to be sparse with the specification of the variables in the interaction analyses. Thus,
we follow the data analysis strategy used by Barrick and Mount (1993) and used separate analyses for
each of the Big Five constructs. First, we regressed charismatic leadership on the perceived dynamic
work variable and on one of the Big Five-Factors. In the second step, the interaction predictor was
added to the regression. Table 3 presents the results of these analyses.
Contrary to Hypothesis 1, no significant interaction effect was found for extraversion and dynamic
work environment in explaining charismatic leadership,  ¼ 0.00, p > 0.05. Although the interaction
of openness to experience and dynamic work environment had a significant positive effect on charis-
matic leadership and explained 5 per cent of the variance,  ¼ 0.25, p < 0.05, the form of the interac-
tion (see Figure 2) shows a positive relationship under high dynamic work conditions and a negative
relationship under low dynamic conditions. Thus, the form of the interaction does not (or only par-
tially) support Hypothesis 2, which suggested a greater effect under conditions of a more dynamic
work environment. Furthermore, in support of Hypothesis 3, the interaction between agreeableness
and dynamic work environment had a significant negative effect on charismatic leadership, explaining
10 per cent of variance,  ¼ 0.34, p < 0.01. Similar results were found for conscientiousness in com-
bination with perceived dynamic work environment for explaining charismatic leadership,  ¼ 0.35,

Table 3. Results of moderated regression analyses for five factor variables explaining charismatic and
transactional leadership
Variable Charismatic Transactional

R2 R2  R2 R2 

Step 1 0.03
Extraversion 0.09
Dynamic 0.14
Step 2 0.03 0.00
Extraversion  Dynamic 0.00
Step 1 0.05
Openness to experience 0.17
Dynamic 0.13
Step 2 0.10* 0.05*
Openness  Dynamic 0.25*
Step 1 0.02 0.01
Agreeableness 0.00 0.02
Dynamic 0.08 0.00
Step 2 0.13** 0.10** 0.06 0.05*
Agreeableness  Dynamic 0.34** 0.25*
Step 1 0.02 0.00
Conscientiousness 0.03 0.05
Dynamic 0.14 0.05
Step 2 0.11* 0.09** 0.07 0.07*
Conscientiousness  Dynamic 0.35** 0.30*
Step 1 0.05
Neuroticism 0.24*
Dynamic 0.15
Step 2 0.15** 0.10**
Neuroticism  Dynamic 0.34**
Note: n ¼ 80. Standardized regression coefficients are shown based on the last step in regression procedure.
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
854 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

Figure 2. Regression lines for four personality variables explaining charismatic leadership for high and low levels
of perceived dynamic work environment (þ1, þ2 and 1, 2 standard deviations from the mean)

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 855

Figure 3. Regression lines for two personality variables explaining transactional leadership for high and low
levels of perceived dynamic work environment (þ1, þ2 and 1, 2 standard deviations from the mean)

p < 0.01, supporting Hypothesis 5. Contrary to Hypothesis 7, however, we found a significant positive
interaction for neuroticism and dynamic work environment in explaining charismatic leadership, in the
opposite direction to the one predicted,  ¼ 0.34, p < 0.01. The nature of these interactions for high
and low perceived dynamic work environment is depicted in Figure 2.
In sum, leaders scoring high on openness to experience as well as neuroticism were rated more
charismatic by subordinates in work situations that were perceived as dynamic and less charismatic
in work situations that were rated more stable. The reverse was true for leaders high on agreeableness
and conscientiousness. These leaders were rated less charismatic in dynamic work situations and more
charismatic in stable environments.
Following the same procedures, two separate moderated multiple regression analyses were con-
ducted to examine the effect of perceived dynamic work environment on the relationship of agree-
ableness and conscientiousness with transactional leadership (reported in Table 3). Consistent with
Hypothesis 4 and 6, we found a significant negative beta-weight (i.e., environment is less dynamic)
of the interaction term for both agreeableness and conscientiousness in combination with perceived
dynamic environment,  ¼ 0.25, p < 0.05,  ¼ 0.30, p < 0.05, explaining 5, respectively
7 per cent of variance. The nature of these interactions is depicted in Figure 3. In sum, leaders
high on agreeableness as well as conscientiousness were rated more transactional in work situations
that were perceived as stable and less transactional in work situations that were rated more
dynamic.

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
856 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

Table 4. Results of moderated regression analysis for charismatic leadership explaining


perceived effectiveness
Variable Effectiveness

R2 R2 

Step 1 0.04
Charismatic 0.06
Dynamic 0.03
Step 2 0.10 0.07*
Charismatic  Dynamic 0.14*
Note: n ¼ 60. Standardized regression coefficients are shown based on the last step in regression
procedure.
*p < 0.05.

Leadership and effectiveness

Table 1 reveals that the inter-correlation between charismatic and transactional leadership (0.49) is
somewhat lower than correlations typically found in studies in this area using other measures such
as the MLQ (see e.g., Den Hartog, Van Muijen, & Koopman, 1997; Lowe et al., 1996). The correla-
tions between charismatic leadership as well as transactional leadership and perceived effectiveness
are low and not significant (0.07, 0.14, respectively). Thus contrary to expectations, both charismatic
and transactional leadership were found to be unrelated to perceived effectiveness. This means that
hypothesis 8 is not supported.
To test the impact of perceived dynamic work environment on the relationship between charismatic
leadership and perceived effectiveness, a moderated hierarchical regression analysis was conducted.
First, we regressed the effectiveness variable on charismatic leadership and perceived dynamic work
environment. In the second step, the interaction predictor was added to the regression. Table 4 presents
the results of this analysis. Both charismatic leadership and dynamic work environment had no signif-
icant main effect on effectiveness. However, the interaction between charismatic leadership and per-
ceived dynamic work environment contributed significantly in explaining perceived effectiveness,
 ¼ 0.14, p < 0.05. The nature of this interaction is depicted in Figure 4. Thus, the form of the inter-
action does not (or only partially) support Hypothesis 9; the relationship between charismatic leader

Figure 4. Regression lines for charismatic leadership explaining perceived effectiveness for high and low levels of
perceived dynamic work environment (þ1, þ2 and 1, 2 standard deviations from the mean)

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 857

behavior and perceived effectiveness was more positive in dynamic work environments and more
negative in stable work environments. A separate regression analysis (not reported) showed that trans-
actional leadership and perceived dynamic work environment did not interact in explaining perceived
effectiveness.

Discussion

The main purpose of this study was to assess links between leadership and personality and investigate
whether perceived dynamic work environment moderates the relationship between personality and
charismatic as well as transactional leadership. Our secondary purpose was to address the relationship
between leadership styles and effectiveness and test for the potential moderating effect of dynamism.

Personality and leadership

Trait activation theory posits that personality traits require trait-relevant situations for their expression.
In line with this, the results of our study indicate that the relationships between personality and char-
ismatic and transactional leadership differ depending on the context. Specifically, we found that four of
the Big Five-Factors are differently relevant to charismatic and transactional leadership depending on
the degree to which the work environment is perceived as dynamic.
As expected, we found that agreeableness and conscientiousness positively were related to perceptions
of both charismatic and transactional leadership styles in a stable work environment. In a more dynamic
environment, leaders with these characteristics were rated less charismatic and less transactional. Thus,
the personality traits that help increase the quality of interaction with followers and inspire followers to
perform beyond expectation through setting high standards and acting dutifully themselves (i.e., agree-
ableness and conscientiousness) are found to be especially important for charismatic leadership in a stable
context. In contrast, in a dynamic environment agreeableness and conscientiousness seem to impede the
attribution of charisma as highly agreeable and conscientious leaders may be overly compliant and stick to
agreed upon regulations rather than grasp opportunities at hand.
Furthermore, the personality traits involved in being able to meet and respond to the reactions and
changing expectations of followers and monitor successfully (i.e., agreeableness and conscientious-
ness) are also found to be especially important for transactional leadership in a stable context. In con-
trast, under more dynamic conditions, such agreeable and conscientious leaders who choose to monitor
exchange relationships with employees and stress maintaining the status quo seemed to be viewed as
too rigid and were perceived as less transactional than in stable conditions. Previous research reports
positive as well as negative and non-existent relationships between agreeableness, conscientiousness,
and charismatic leadership and transactional leadership (Crant & Bateman, 2000; Judge & Bono,
2000; Ployhart et al., 2001 see also Bono & Judge, 2004). Our results suggest that distinguishing
between stable and dynamic environments may be needed in developing further understanding regard-
ing the relationships between these variables.
Contrary to our expectations, no direct relationship was found between openness to experience and
charismatic leadership. We found the expected positive relationship between openness to experience
and charismatic leadership only in a work environment perceived as dynamic. Leaders scoring high
on openness to experience were rated more charismatic by subordinates in work situations that were
perceived as dynamic and less charismatic in work situations that were rated more stable. In line with

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
858 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

trait activation theory, the personality trait required for leaders to question the status quo and to find
new opportunities to reach organizational goals (i.e., openness to experience) is likely to be especially
relevant in a dynamic context. Our finding that leaders scoring high on openness to experience were
rated more charismatic by subordinates in work situations that were perceived as dynamic supports this
proposition. In contrast, in more stable contexts, leaders who question the status quo and continually
seek improvements in ways to perform the job may be viewed as too unsettling (e.g., Howell & Avolio,
1993). This may explain why we found leaders with high scores on openness to experience were rated
less charismatic in these contexts. Our findings are in line with those by Ployhart and colleagues
(2001), who found that openness to experience predicted charismatic leadership for participants in
a military training, but that this only held under relatively challenging and not under more stable con-
ditions. Our findings are also in line with meta-analytic results showing great variability in strength and
direction for the relationship between openness to experience and charismatic leadership. Results of
the present study and the one by Ployhart and colleagues (2001) suggest that openness to experience is
more relevant to charismatic leadership in a dynamic than in a stable environment.
Contrary to expectations, we found that leaders who were relatively high on neuroticism were rated
more charismatic by subordinates in work situations that were perceived as dynamic and less charis-
matic in work situations that were rated more stable. Perhaps the negative emotionality of individuals
high on neuroticism may induce them to take an emotionally charged position in their attempts to
change the states quo in a dynamic context, which subordinates may find inspiring (cf. Cable & Judge,
2003). In contrast, in a more stable context, taking an emotionally charged position may run counter to
the stability and continuity of the existing structure, which may lead them to be perceived less charis-
matic. Previous research by Ployhart and colleagues (2001) also found a negative relationship between
neuroticism and charismatic leadership in a more stable environment (i.e. three month basic training)
and no relationship in a more dynamic environment (i.e., two day assessment exercise). Other previous
research that did not take context into account did not find any relationships between neuroticism and
charismatic leadership (Crant & Bateman, 2000; Judge & Bono, 2000) or report negative ones (Lim &
Ployhart, 2004; Bono & Judge, 2004). Our findings suggest that taking differences in context (e.g.
dynamic vs. stable environments) into account in future studies may gain further understanding in
the relationship between neuroticism and charismatic leadership.
We also expected that the personality trait required for leaders to be able to influence, persuade and
mobilize a critical mass of followers (i.e., extraversion) would be especially relevant in a dynamic con-
text. No evidence, however, was found for a relationship between extraversion and charismatic leader-
ship regardless whether we took differences in context (e.g., dynamic versus stable environments) into
account. Our finding runs counter to previous studies that link extraversion with charismatic leadership
(Crant & Bateman, 2000; Judge & Bono, 2000; Bono & Judge, 2004) and more strongly so in a chal-
lenging than in a more stable environment (Ployhart et al., 2001). Perhaps managers in our study were
more likely than in previous studies done in other contexts to rate themselves as extraverts as the rat-
ings were obtained during an evaluation of managerial potential. To them extraversion might seem
consistent with the profile of an effective leader. Thus, this implicit profile might have biased man-
agers’ self-reports of extraversion. The limitations of our study, including the possibility of social
desirability bias, are discussed further below. Future research is needed to further explore the moder-
ating role of the context in the relationship between extraversion and leader behavior.

Leadership and effectiveness


As a secondary aim, we also addressed the relationship between leader behavior and performance.
Contrary to our expectations, no direct relationship was found between charismatic leadership and

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 859

perceived effectiveness. We did however find an interaction effect. Results show that subordinates
evaluations of charismatic leader behavior were positively related to perceived effectiveness as rated
by superiors and peers, but only under dynamic work conditions. Our findings are consistent with the-
ories of charismatic leadership that have suggested that charismatic leadership is likely to be more
effective under conditions of challenge and change (e.g., Howell & Avolio, 1993; Shamir & Howell,
1999). Moreover, our findings are in line with previous research by Waldman et al. (2001). They found
that charismatic leadership of Chief Executive Officers positively affected organizational performance,
but only under conditions of high environmental uncertainty. Taken together, these findings suggest
that charismatic leaders need a certain amount of decision discretion and opportunity for change to
have an impact on performance outcomes.

Limitations and future research


Although our study yields some interesting findings, it also has several limitations that need to be con-
sidered when interpreting the results. First of all, managers’ self-reports of their personality may be
guided by self-presentation of an image that the respondent wishes to convey to the tester, especially
when the questionnaire is used for selection purposes (Hogan, Hogan, & Roberts, 1996), which was
the case here for part of the respondents. The high inter-correlations found between the Big Five-
Factors in this study may be the result of this and this implies that we may have a conservative test
of our hypotheses.
Second, it is possible that managers who did not participate in the present study were more likely to
have been evaluated negatively regarding their managerial potential. Indicating this, only 3 of the 29
participants at time 2 that had taken part in the management assessment at time 1 for personnel selec-
tion purposes were given a negative advice regarding the job for which they had applied. Consequently,
the database available for analyses may suffer from restriction of range. Our check for selective non-
response suggested, however, no significant differences regarding gender, age and scores on the Big
Five-Factors. Also, the majority of managers in the sample underwent the assessment for developmen-
tal rather than selection purposes. Thus, this possible bias is likely to be limited.
Third, as in most multi-source feedback systems, managers in this study selected their raters.
Moreover, we were able to obtain effectiveness ratings for only part of the focal managers (61 out
of 83). Therefore, one might suggest that selection of raters has resulted in a positive bias of raters
toward the focal manager. We, however, checked for selective non-response regarding gender, age,
and leadership styles and did not find any differences. Therefore, we expect this possible positive bias
to be limited.
Due to the managers’ freedom of choice of selecting their raters, only 16 of the managers were rated
by peers as well as superiors. As such, correlation coefficients could only be calculated on a sample of
16 managers ( p ¼ 0.12), and this small sample does not provide us with accurate estimates of agree-
ment or inter-rater reliability. We decided, however, to combine and aggregate peers and superior rat-
ings, since previous meta-analytic studies demonstrate a relatively high correlation between peer and
superior ratings (e.g., Conway & Huffcutt, 1997; Harris & Schaubroeck, 1988). Moreover, in this way
common source and rater bias was minimized; each of the variables measured in this study was
assessed by different raters or at different time points.
Furthermore, reliability estimates for openness to experience, agreeableness and for the dynamic
work environment scale could have been higher. This seems especially pressing for the two Big Five
factors when one considers that these measures contain 48 items per trait. However, the reliabilities
were sufficiently in line with previous studies and the Dutch norm group (see Costa & McCrae, 1992b;
Hoekstra et al., 1996). Thus, we retained the original scales, which makes direct comparisons with

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
860 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

other research possible. Consequently, relationships may have been underestimated due to low relia-
bility of the measurement instruments (i.e., the error in our measures obscured the true relationship).
Future research on the broad Big Five factors and their reliabilities seems, however, necessary.
Attention should also be devoted to expanding and refining the measure of a dynamic work envir-
onment chosen in this study. While the three items that we used have precedents in the literature, they
may provide a somewhat broad measure of environmental dynamism. The somewhat low reliability of
the dynamic work environment scale (0.68) may be the result of this. Perhaps more fine-grained or
focused measures could show us that under the heading of dynamic environment there are certain
forms or sub-contexts that are more conducive to charismatic and transformational leadership while
others may not be at all. Other researchers have investigated slightly different concepts in relation to
charismatic leadership, for example environmental uncertainty (cf. De Hoogh et al., 2004b; Flynn &
Staw, 2004; Waldman et al., 2001) crisis (cf. Pillai, 1996; Pillai & Meindl, 1991) and technological
change (cf. De Hoogh et al., 2004b). Here we have focused on a dynamic environment. The results of
this study show that a dynamic work environment is an important moderator of the relationship
between personality and leadership. It may therefore, be a fruitful road of research to try and map
the differences between the various concepts in their link with charismatic and transactional
leadership.
The focal managers themselves provided a measure of perceived dynamic work environment. Their
own perceptions of the environment are most relevant for explaining trait relevant behavior as com-
pared to for example perceptions of subordinates or peers. But the unavailability of subordinates’ or
superiors’/peers’ ratings represents a limitation with regard to the investigation of dynamic environ-
ment as a moderator of the relationship between charismatic leadership and effectiveness. Their per-
ception of the environment to be dynamic must be most relevant for leaders to more easily generate
appeal for the vision and be rated as more effective. Future research is needed to test this.
Furthermore, we recognize that the sample size for analyses was relatively small. Given the low
statistical power of moderated regression analysis (e.g., Aguinis, 1995; Villa, Howell, Dorfman, &
Daniel, 2003) more interaction effects may have been significant had the sample size been larger.
At the same time, it also means that the moderator effects we did find need to be replicated across
a larger database of firms in future research to test their robustness. Our sample size and the high
inter-correlations found between some of the Big Five-Factors in this study (the highest reaching
up to 0.66) required us to be sparse with the specification of the variables in the interaction analyses.
The most desirable (and more conservative) approach to the interaction analyses would have been to
include all predictors and interaction terms in the same equation. In this way, the effect of one trait and
the interaction is adjusted for the influence of the other traits and possible interactions. This would
mean that in a statistical sense the variables are made independent of one another, even though they
correlate in the real world. This allows for examination of the unique contributions of each variable.
However, including highly correlated variables in the same analysis can inflate the size of the error
terms and thus weaken an analysis. In regression, this implies that error terms may get so large that
none of the coefficients are significant any longer even if effects do exist (Berry, 1993). For example,
when r ¼ 0.9 the precision of estimation of weighting coefficients is already halved (Fox, 1991).
Therefore, we tested for the moderating effect of dynamic work environment on leadership styles
one trait at a time. This way to analyze the data was also used in previous research as we followed
the strategy used by Barrick and Mount (1993) in their study on the Big Five factors and work perfor-
mance, investigating work autonomy as a moderator. In our case, due to inter-correlations and sample
size, this was also the best approach. As we could not investigate all effects simultaneously, some of
our findings may be confounded in the sense that effect sizes might be different after controlling for the
other Big Five traits and potential interactions. Our findings need replicating in future research that is
carried out in situations that allow for the investigation of these effects simultaneously. Such studies

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 861

require larger samples and would ideally also need to be done in situations where correlations among
the Big Five factors are lower (for example, our data on Big Five traits was gathered in a selection
context in which such correlations might be somewhat higher than in a non-selection context). Under
those circumstances, such an approach will permit more powerful tests of hypotheses and control for
the main effects of the Big Five traits and potential interactions.
Finally, the longitudinal design of our study may also introduce a conservative bias in our study
where the results regarding leadership and personality are concerned. Most previous research uses a
cross-sectional design to investigate the link between personality and leadership (e.g., Crant &
Bateman, 2000; Judge & Bono, 2000). Previous research that did use a more longitudinal design
had different measurement points mostly only a few weeks apart from each other (e.g., Ployhart et
al., 2001). In our study the measurement of personality and leader behavior did not only involve dif-
ferent raters (self and subordinates), but measurements were also done nine months apart. Again this is
likely to result in conservative rather than inflated estimates of relationships. Further, we should note
that our outcome measures were only obtained at time 2 and not obtained at time 1, thus we could not
analyze actual change as it is possible in panel designs.
Despite the aforementioned limitations and the longitudinal design of our study, it is notable that we
were able to account for such a significant amount of variance with regard to our interaction hypoth-
eses. Future research using longitudinal designs should test the robustness of our findings and can also
rule out potential alternative explanations. Future work could also aim to identify and test other poten-
tial moderators.
To conclude, in line with trait activation theory our study shows the importance of perceived
dynamic work environments as a moderator of the relationships between four of the Big Five-Factors
and both charismatic leadership and transactional leadership. Further, our study indicates that per-
ceived dynamic work environment acts as an important moderator of the relationship between charis-
matic leadership and perceived effectiveness. More generally, our research suggests that more
attention for the moderating role of the context is needed in this area.

Acknowledgements

We thank management consultancy firm LTP for helping us collect the data. We would also like to
thank Renout E. De Vries and Jan A. Feij and three anonymous reviewers for comments on an earlier
version of this article.

Author biographies

Annebel H. B. De Hoogh is postdoc researcher at the department of Work and Organizational Psy-
chology of the Free University Amsterdam, The Netherlands. She received her PhD from the same
university in 2004. Her research interests are in the areas of personality and leadership.
Deanne N. Den Hartog is full professor of Organizational Behavior at the Management department of
the University of Amsterdam Business School. She holds a PhD from the Free University in

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
862 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Her research interests include leadership and especially cross-cultural
and inspirational leadership processes and personality and leadership as well as (team) performance
management.
Paul L. Koopman (1946) is professor of the Psychology of Management and Organization at the Free
University Amsterdam, The Netherlands. He is interested and actively involved in cross-cultural
research, in particular in relation to issues of HRM, leadership and organizational culture.

References

Aguinis, H. (1995). Statistical power problems with moderated multiple regression in management research.
Journal of Management, 21, 1141–1158.
Allport, G. W. (1966). Traits revisited. American Psychologist, 21, 1–10.
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1993). Autonomy as a moderator of the relationships between the big five
personality dimensions and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 111–118.
Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New York: Free Press.
Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass and Stogdill’s Handbook of leadership: Theory, research and managerial applications
(3rd ed.). New York: Free Press.
Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1993). Transformational leadership: a response to critiques. In M. M. Chemers, & R.
Ayman (Eds.), Leadership: Theory and research perspectives and directions. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bem, D. J., & Funder, D. C. (1978). Predicting more of the people more of the time: assessing the personality of
situations. Psychological Review, 85, 354–364.
Berry, W. D. (1993). Understanding regression assumptions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage
Bliese, P. D. (2000). Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability: implications for data
aggregation and analysis. In K. J. Klein, & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, and
methods in organizations. San Fransisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2004). Personality and transformational and transactional leadership: a meta-analysis.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 901–910.
Bryman, A. (1992). Charisma and leadership in organizations. London: Sage.
Burns, J. M. (1978). Leadership. New York: Harper & Row.
Bycio, P., Hackett, R. D., & Allen, J. S. (1995). Further assessments of Bass’s (1985) conceptualization of
transactional and transformational leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 468–478.
Cable, D. M., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Managers’ upward influence tactic strategies: the role of manager personality
and supervisor leadership style. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24, 197–214.
Chatman, J. A., Caldwell, D. F., & O’Reilly, C. A. (1999). Managerial personality and performance: a semi-
idiographic approach. Journal of Research in Personality, 33, 514–545.
Church, A. H., & Waclawski, J. (1998). The relationship between individual personality orientation and executive
leader behavior. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 71, 99–125.
Conger, J. A. (1993). Max Weber’s conceptualization of charismatic authority: its influence on organizational
research. The Leadership Quarterly, 4, 277–288.
Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. (1994). Charismatic leadership in organizations: perceived behavioral attributes
and their measurement. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15, 439–452.
Conger, J. A., Kanungo, R. N., Menon, S. T., & Mathur, P. (1997). Measuring charisma: dimensionality and
validity of the Conger-Kanungo Scale of charismatic leadership. Canadian Journal of Administrative Sciences,
14, 290–302.
Conway, J. M., & Huffcutt, A. I. (1997). Psychometric properties of multisource performance ratings: a meta-
analysis of subordinate, supervisor, peer and self-ratings. Human Performance, 10, 331–360.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). From catalog to classification: Murray’s needs and the Five-Factor
Model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 258–265.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992a). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO
Personality Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 4, 5–13.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992b). Revised NEO Personality Inventroy (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor
Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 863

Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Dye, D. A. (1991). Facet scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness: a
revision of the NEO Personality Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 887–898.
Crant, J. M., & Bateman, T. S. (2000). Charismatic leadership viewed from above: the impact of proactive
personality. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 63–75.
De Hoogh, A. H. B., Den Hartog, D. N., & Koopman, P. L. (2004a). De ontwikkeling van de CLIO: Een
vragenlijst voor het meten van charismatisch leiderschap in organisaties [The development of the CLIO: a
questionnaire for measuring charismatic leadership in organizations]. Gedrag en Organisatie, 17, 354–382.
De Hoogh, A. H. B., Den Hartog, D. N., Koopman, P. L., Thierry, H., Van den Berg, P. T., Van der Weide, J. G., &
Wilderom, C. P. M. (2004b). Charismatic leadership, environmental dynamism, and performance. European
Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 13, 447–471.
De Hoogh, A. H. B., Den Hartog, D. N., Koopman, P. L., Thierry, H., Van den Berg, P. T., Van der Weide, J. G., &
Wilderom, C. P. M. (2005). Leader motives, charismatic leadership and subordinates’ work attitude in the profit
and voluntary sector. The Leadership Quarterly, 16, 17–38.
Den Hartog, D. N., & Koopman, P. L. (2001). Leadership in organizations. In N. Anderson, D. S. Ones,
H. Kepir-Sinangil, & C. Viswesvaran (Eds.), International handbook of industrial, work & organizational
psychology (Vol. 2). London: Sage.
Den Hartog, D. N., Van Muijen, J. J., & Koopman, P. L. (1997). Transactional versus transformational leadership:
an analysis of the MLQ. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 70, 19–34.
Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., & Emmons, R. A. (1984). Person  situation interactions: choice of situations and
congruence response models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 580–592.
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: the emergence of the Five-Factor Model. Annual Review of
Psychology, 41, 417–440.
Digman, J. M., & Shmelyov, A. C. (1996). The structure of temperament and personality in Russian children.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 341–351.
Dvir, T., Eden, D., Avolio, B. J., & Shamir, B. (2002). Impact of transformational leadership on follower
development and performance: a field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 735–744.
Flynn, F. J., & Staw, B. M. (2004). Lend me your wallets: the effects of charismatic leadership on external support
for an organization. Strategic Management Journal, 25, 309–330.
Fox, J. (1991). Regression diagnostics. Newbury Park, CA: Sage
Fry, L. W. (2003). Toward a theory of spiritual leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 14, 693–727.
Fuller, J. B., Patterson, C. E. P., Hester, K., & Stringer, D. Y. (1996). A quantitative review of research on
charismatic leadership. Psychological Reports, 78, 271–287.
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative ‘description of personality’: the Big Five factor structure. Personality
Process and Individual Differences, 59, 1216–1229.
Graziano, W. G., & Eisenberg, N. (1997). Agreeableness: a dimension of personality. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, &
S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 795–824). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Harris, M. M., & Schaubroeck, J. (1988). A meta-analysis of self-supervisor, self-peer, and peer-supervisor
ratings. Personnel Psychology, 41, 43–62.
Hetland, H., & Sandal, G. M. (2003). Transformational leadership in Norway: outcomes and personality
correlates. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 12, 147–170.
Hoekstra, H. A., Ormel, J., & De Fruyt, F. (Eds.). (1996). NEO PI-R Handleiding. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets
& Zeitlinger.
Hogan, R., Hogan, J., & Roberts, B. W. (1996). Personality measurement and employment decisions: questions
and answers. American Psychologist, 51, 469–477.
House, R. J. (1977). A 1976 theory of charismatic leadership. In J. G. Hunt, & L. L. Larson (Eds.), Leadership:
The cutting edge (pp. 189–207). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.
House, R. J. (1996). Path-Goal theory of leadership: lessons, legacy and a reformulated theory. The Leadership
Quarterly, 7, 323–352.
House, R. J., & Howell, J. M. (1992). Personality and charismatic leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 3, 81–
108.
House, R. J., Spangler, W. D., & Woycke, J. (1991). Personality and charisma in the U.S. presidency: a
psychological theory of leader effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 364–396.
Howell, J. M., & Avolio, B. J. (1993). Transformational leadership, transactional leadership, locus of control, and
support for innovation: key predictors of consolidated-business-unit performance. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 78, 891–902.
Howell, J. M., & Higgins, C. A. (1990). Leadership behaviors, influence tactics, and career experiences of
champions of technological innovation. The Leadership Quarterly, 1, 249–264.

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
864 A. H. B. DE HOOGH ET AL.

Jacobsen, C., & House, R. J. (2001). Dynamics of charismatic leadership: a process theory, simulation model, and
tests. The Leadership Quarterly, 12, 75–112.
James, L. R. (1982). Aggregation bias in estimates of perceptual agreement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67,
219–229.
Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleiter, A., Riemann, R., & Livesley, W. J. (1998). Heritability of facet-level traits in
a cross-cultural twin sample: support for a hierarchical model of personality. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 1556–1565.
Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2000). Five-Factor Model of personality and transformational leadership. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 85, 751–765.
Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Ilies, R., & Gerhardt, M. W. (2002). Personality and leadership: a qualitative and
quantitative review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 765–780.
Judge, T. A., & Piccolo, R. F. (2004). Transformational and transactional leadership: a meta-analytic test of their
relative validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 755–768.
Kark, R., Shamir, B., & Chen, G. (2003). The two faces of transformational leadership: empowerment and
dependency. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 246–255.
Kuhnert, K. W., & Lewis, P. (1987). Transactional and transformational leadership: a constructive/developmental
analysis. Academy of Management Journal, 12, 648–657.
Lim, B. C., & Ployhart, R. E. (2004). Transformational leadership: relations to the Five-Factor Model and team
performance in typical and maximum contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 610–621.
Lowe, K. B., Kroeck, K. G., & Sivasubramaniam, N. (1996). Effectiveness correlates of transformational and
transactional leadership: a meta-analytic review of the MLQ literature. The Leadership Quarterly, 7, 385–425.
McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1958). A scoring manual for the achievement
motive. In J. W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action and society (pp. 179–204). Princeton, NJ: Van
Nostrand.
McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential openness. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 323–337.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Validation of the Five-Factor Model of personality across instruments
and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1991). Adding liebe und arbeid: the full Five-Factor Model and well-being.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 227–232.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., del Pilar, G. H., Rolland, J. P., & Parker, W. D. (1998). Cross-cultural assessment of
the Five-Factor Model: the revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 171–
188.
Murray, H. A. (Ed.). (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University Press.
Pawar, B. S., & Eastman, K. K. (1997). The nature and implications of contextual influences on transformational
leadership: a conceptual examination. Academy of Management Review, 22, 80–109.
Pillai, R. (1996). Crisis and the emergence of charismatic leadership in groups: an experimental investigation.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, 543–562.
Pillai, R., & Meindl, J. R. (1991) . The effects of a crisis on the emergence of charismatic leadership: a laboratory
study. Best Paper Proceedings of the 1991 Academy of Management National Meetings, Miami, FL.
Pillai, R., & Meindl, J. R. (1998). Context and charisma: A ‘meso’ level examination of the relationship of organic
structure, collectivism, and crisis to charismatic leadership. Journal of Management, 24, 643–671.
Ployhart, R. E., Lim, B., & Chan, K. (2001). Exploring relations between typical and maximum performance
ratings and the Five-Factor Model of personality. Personnel Psychology, 54, 809–843.
Ross, S. M., & Offermann, L. R. (1997). Transformational leaders: measurement of personality attributes and
work group performance. Society for Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 1078–1086.
Shamir, B., House, R. J., & Arthur, M. B. (1993). The motivational aspects of charismatic leadership: a self-
concept theory. Organization Science, 4, 1–17.
Shamir, B., & Howell, J. M. (1999). Organizational and contextual influences on the emergence and effectiveness
of charismatic leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 10, 257–283.
Shamir, B., Zakay, E., Breinin, E., & Popper, M. (1998). Correlates of charismatic leader behavior in military
units: subordinates’ attitudes, unit characteristics, and superiors appraisals of leader performance. Academy of
Management Journal, 41, 387–409.
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological
Bulletin, 86, 420–428.
Sosik, J. J., Avolio, B. J., & Jung, D. I. (2002). Beneath the mask: examining the relationship of self-presentation
attributes and impression management to charismatic leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 13, 217–242.

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)
BIG FIVE-FACTORS 865

Sosik, J. J., & Dworakivsky, A. C. (1998). Self-concept based aspects of the charismatic leader: more than meets
the eye. The Leadership Quarterly, 9, 503–526.
Stevens, C. D., & Ash, R. A. (2001). Selecting employees for fit: personality and preferred managerial style.
Journal of Managerial Issues, 4, 500–517.
Tett, R. P., & Burnett, D. D. (2003). A personality trait-based interactionist model of job performance. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 88, 500–517.
Tett, R. P., & Guterman, H. A. (2000). Situation trait relevance, trait expression, and cross-situational consistency:
testing a principle of trait activation. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 397–423.
Trice, H. M., & Beyer, J. M. (1986). Charisma and its routinization in two social movement organizations.
Research in Organizational Behavior, 8, 113–164.
Villa, J. R., Howell, J. P., Dorfman, P. W., & Daniel, D. L. (2003). Problems with detecting moderators in
leadership research using moderated multiple regression. The Leadership Quarterly, 14, 3–23.
Waldman, D. A., Ramirez, G. G., House, R. J., & Puranam, P. (2001). Does leadership matter? CEO leader
attributes and profitability under conditions of perceived environmental uncertainty. Academy of Management
Journal, 44, 134–143.
Woodman, R. W., Sawyer, J. E., & Griffin, R. W. (1993). Toward a theory of organizational creativity. Academy of
Management Review, 18, 293–321.
Yukl, G. (1999). An evaluation of conceptual weaknesses in transformational and charismatic leadership theories.
The Leadership Quarterly, 10, 285–305.

Copyright # 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 26, 839–865 (2005)

Potrebbero piacerti anche