Sei sulla pagina 1di 9

Sex Differences: A Study of the Eye

of the Beholder

John Condry and Sandra Condry


Corrwll University

CONDRY, JOHN, and CONDRY, SANDRA. Sex Differences: A Study of the Eye of the Beholder. CHILD
DEVELOPMENT, 1976, 47, 812-819. In an attempt to assess the effects of labeling on socially mediated sex
differences in infancy, 204 male and female subjects rated the same infent's emotional responses to 4
different arousing stimuli: half of the subjects were told they were observing a "boy" and the other half, a
"girl" The same infant in a particular situation was seen as displaying different emotions and significantly
different levels of emotional arousal depending on the sex attributed to the infant, the sex of the rater, and
the rater's experience with young children. The results suggest a healthy caution be exercised in interpret-
ing studies of sex differences obtained by observers who know the sex of the child being rated.

The fact that we often see that which we ex- that girl infants show a fear of strangers at a
pect to see is sufficiently well known and accepted younger age than do boy infants (Robson, Peder-
to be accorded the status of a cliche. But follow the son, & Moss 1969) and at 2 years and later girls
logic one step further: we usually act on what we show more intense fear than do boys to fear-
think we see, and when those actions are directed provoking stimuli (Jersild & Holmes 1935). Tod-
toward another person, they affect the other per- dler girls display a higher language competence
son in a variety of ways. When our actions are than do toddler boys (Clarke-Stewart 1973); the
directed toward children, the picture is compli- same is true at 2y2 years of age (Bell, Weller, &
cated even more. Children often search for an an- Waldrop 1971). One-year-old boys play more vig-
swer to how they should behave by watching the orously than do girls (Goldberg & Lewis 1969) and
ways adults act toward them. Thus the actions of from 2 to 4 years they engage in more rough and
adults, directed toward children, acquire a re- tumble play (Smith & Connolly 1972). Boys at 2
ality-defining quality. A parent who expects his years of age and older score higher than girls in
child to dislike mushrooms can act toward the physical aggression (Mclntyre 1972) and are also
child in such a way as to bring about the very more passively nonconforming (Pederson & Bell
dislike he expects. A mother who expects her 1970).
daughter to fear mice can interpret the child's first
startled reaction to mice as fear and act accord- A most common question in the study of sex
ingly, thus defining the emotion and the subse- differences is how these differences arise. Mac-
quent appropriate action for the child (Schachter coby and Jacklin (1974) cite three factors that aflFect
& Singer 1962). If the child responds to this defini- the development of sex differences. These are: (1)
tion and it has social support, this socially trans- genetic factors; (2) "shaping" of boylike and girl-
mitted characteristic may become part of that like behaviors by parents and other socializing
child's common behavior repertoire. One area agents; and (3) the child's spontaneous learning of
where a "label" could have significant effects is behavior appropriate for his sex through imitation.
that of sex differences (Maccoby & Jacklin 1974), The study reported here bears on the second of
and so we have chosen to apply our analysis to this these factors.
topic. Could adults be encouraging sex differences Until it is satisfactorily answered, a question
in just such a manner? of equal importance is how many of the sex differ-
ences reported in the literature are actually pres-
Sex diflPerences among infants and young ent in little children. The results of the studies
children are found by many researchers. We know cited above are all based on observations by re-
This study was supported by a grant from the New York State College of Human Ecology, Cornell
University, Ithaca, New York 14853. We are grateful for the assistance of a number of fine students who
helped with this experiment and with several unreported pilot studies of the same topic. In particular we
are indebted to Kathleen Gilbert, Susan Koff, Virginia Vanderslice, and James Garbarino for serving as
experimenters, and Susan Gratz for her careful reading of an earlier manuscript. Address reprint requests
to John C Condry, Department of Human Development and Family Relations, Cornell University,
Ithaca, New York 14853.
[Child Development, 1976, 47. 812-819. © 1976 by the Society for Research in ChiW Devekjpmeirt, Inc. All rights reserved]
Condry and Ck>ndry 813
searchers or teachers. In most observational emotionally arousing stimuli. In the tape in this
studies where sex differences are found (either in- study, the 9-month-old infant is seated in an infant
cidentally or centrally), the sex of the child is seat facing a one-way mirror (with the video cam-
known to the observer. The children are person- era mounted behind the mirror). The infant is
ally knov^Ti by the rater or have sex-appropriate shown the stimuli in sequence, given approxi-
names, clothes, and/or hair styles. How important mately the same amount of time of exposure
is it that the rater knows the sex of the child being (about ^ sec) to each stimulus. Four stimuli are
rated? To determine this we require a situation in shown: a teddy bear, a jack-in-the-box, a doll, and
which raters see identical children performing a buzzer. The entire tape runs about 10 min.
identical behaviors in identical situations, the only
difference being that one child is a boy and one is a Subjects were recruited for participation in
girl. Happily, with videotape technology, we need the experiment in classes, and they were tested in
only one child in a situation and we can tell some classrooms with 60-70 subjects, both male and
raters the child is a boy and other raters that she's female, for each of the three testing sessions. Once
a girl. With this setup, we can answer the ques- seated in the classroom, the experimental materi-
tion: Do observers see differences in a child's be- als were distributed (a packet of instructions, rat-
havior as a function of the sex-type label alone? If ing scales, and demographic information scales)
they do, it would be most useful to know {{what a and subjects were asked not to turn the pages until
rater sees can be predicted from some of his own told to do so. Subjects were told they would be
characteristics. Two likely and easily measured rating the emotional behavior of an infant as part of
characteristics are the rater's sex and experience an ongoing project to study emotional develop-
uith children. ment in the first 2 years of life.
The complete questionnaire contained a self-
The paradigm for this research is suggested in
rating scale for "experience with infants," a scale
two earlier studies by Meyer and Sobieszek (1972)
for recording the emotional reaction of the infant
and by Rothbart and Maccoby (1966). In both
to the various situations or stimuli, a semantic dif-
cases, adults were asked to respond to a stimulus
ferential scale for describing the infant in an over-
which was cross-labeled as to sex of the child.
all way after the emotional labeling data were col-
Rothbart and Maccoby had parents listen to and
lected, and a follow-up questionnaire to complete
rate statements made by a 4-year-old child.
the ruse that we were concerned mainly with the
Fathers allowed both more aggression and depen-
use of the rating scale.
dency from a girl than from a boy, and the reverse
was found of mothers. Meyer and Sobieszek Experience with infants.—The experience
showed videotapes (cross-labeled) to adults who with infants scale asked subjects to describe their
rated attributes of 17-month-old children. Overall, familiarity with infants and children up to 3 years
subjects showed no tendency to attribute more of age. Subjects were asked to describe their ex-
masculine qualities to a child described as male, or perience in the last 5 years only. The 10-point
vice versa, although subjects did show a tendency scale ran from "1: No contact with young children"
to describe a child of their own sex as having more and "2: Have seldom been with young children" to
qualities than a child of the opposite sex. "9: For at least a year had extensive contact with a
young child (e.g., baby brother or sister) and was
Method frequently responsible for his/her care" and "10:
Have had extensive contact with a young child and
Subjects.—A total of 204 subjects, 45 males extensive responsibility for his/her care (e.g., rais-
and 159 females, participated in the experiment. ing own child, working in a day care center)."
The subjects, who ranged in age from 18 to 25
(most were white and all were middle-class), were Emotion rating scale.—Once the subjects
college students attending one of two universities completed the experience scale, the instructions
in upstate New York. All of the subjects were as to the use of the rating scale were given:
tested in a classroom situation. In all cases, both On a videotape you are about to see, you will observe a
male and female experimenters were present, al- child being presented with 4 different stimuli. Each
though the instructions were always read by the stimulus is presented to the child 5 times (for example,
male experimenter. the teddy bear will be pushed toward the child and
pulled back 5 times). The child will respond each time,
Procedure.—An ongoing project to study and your task is to give an overall intensity rating of these
emotional development in infancy provided a con- 5 responses along 3 separate emotional dimensions.
venient source of experimental materials. In these These are the emotions of pleasure, anger and fear.
studies (Ricciuti & Poresky 1972), infants of differ-
ent ages are videotaped responding to several It is possible that you will see the child display only
S14
one emotion during the 5 presentations of the same tively (turned away, stared, cried) to the jack-in-
stimulus. If so, rate the intensity of tfiat emotion and the-box and buzzer.
give the other two emotions a zero rating. But it is also
likely that you see the child show mainly one emotion Setnantic differential scale.—When the vid-
with a lesser amount of one or both of the other emo- eotape rating was completed, subjects were asked
tions. If so, rate the intensity of each emotion shown and to describe the infant they had just seen using a
a zero to any emotion you judge is not present. semantic differential scale (Osgood, Suci, & Tan-
nenbaum 1957) of bipolar items made of three
The 10-point emotion rating scale contained items with high loadings on each of three sub-
places for rating both the type and intensity of the scales. iTiese scales and the items were: activity:
observed emotion. A zero rating indicated the ab- quiet-loud, moving-still, fast-slow; potency: ag-
sence of that particular emotion, a 5 meant "mod- gressive-passive, little-big, strong-weak; and
erately strong," and a 10-point rating indicated the evaluation: good-bad, ugly-pretty, friendly-
strongest possible intensity of that particular emo- unfriendly.
tion. The videotape was stopped after each "stimu-
lus situation," that is, at the end of a sequence Once the semantic differential scale was com-
involving a single stimulus. Each stimulus was plete, the subjects completed a short scale osten-
presented five times, except for the buzzer which sibly designed to measure the quality of the scale
was presented only three times due to the degree they had just used. When this last measure was
of distress displayed by the infant. complete the subjects were told the purpose of the
experiment including the deception about the sex
Manipulation of attributed sex.—On the of the infant they were observing. The reasons for
same page as the infant rating scale was a space the deception were discussed and the subjects
for "infant name, age, and sex." This blank was were thanked and dismissed.
penned in for all subjects. Half of the protocols
gave the name as "David" and the sex as "male, " Results
the other half had the name "Dana" and the sex
"female" written in. These questionnaires were The basic question, of course, is whether the
distributed at random to the subjects seated subjects who think they are watching a girl rate
around the room, so some subjects who were the infant differently than subjects who think they
"told" they were about to watch "David" were sit- are watching a boy and, if so, whether these dif-
ting next to other subjects who were "told" they ferences are related to characteristics of the sub-
were about to watch "Dana. " The age was given as ject (sex, experience with infants) and the charac-
9 months on all protocols. teristics of the stimulus situation and emotion
judged. The variable of experience with infants
Once the response scale was explained and was dichotomized by dividing the experience with
there were no questions as to its use, the experi- infants scale described earlier at the median for
menter continued: "The name and age of the in- each sex of subject. Table 1 presents the mean
fant you will be watching are already written in the intensity ratings for every emotion in all situations
upper right hand comer of the page. You will have by all characteristics of the subjects. An analysis of
to make note of them for later reference.' The variance for unequal cell frequencies (Winer 1962,
semantic differential scale on the next page had a p. 224) was done on these data.
blank for "Infant name, age, sex" which was not
filled in, and the subjects were asked tofillit in "at The only main effects to reach significance are
this time. " The purpose of this roundabout proce- those due to emotion, F(3,2352) = 9.26, p < .001,
dure was, of course, to make the subjects' aware of and situation, F(2,2352) = 88.86, p < .001 and
the attributed sex of the infant without saying the these are of little consequence with respect to our
name or sex out loud. original interests. They suggest only that there are
significant differences to be found in the different
As soon as the instructions were complete for emotions observed and that the emotional re-
the emotion rating scale and the subjects had sponses to the various situations were indeed dif-
completed writing the name and sex of the infant ferent. It is the interactions that are of interest to
they were about to watch on the next page, the the hypotheses under examination.
videotape was showTi. All stimuli were presented
in the same order: teddy bear, jack-in-the-box, A variety of lower-order interactions are
doll, and buzzer. The tape was stopped at the end statistically significant and point to the fact that sex
of each stimulus to allow time for the subjects to labeling significantly affects the ratings of the in-
make their ratings. In general, the infant on the fant. These lower-order interactions are refiective
videotape responded positively (smiled, laughed, of even stronger high-order interactions, so let us
reached out) to the teddy bear and doll, and nega- turn to them. One of the strongest interactions
Ol
Ol ie
0 00

d
n OJ VO o»

s
I
I t o Op ro
t>> 00 so
O c«» »H
0 00 0
to PO 01 Ov to
0 o« 1-1 *-<

00 00 ^ 00 to
<*> 00 • ^ •-< 01 00
3 *J p^ o 01 0
ov
01
*^ 1-1
0^
OO O ov to
o lO p p 00 00
!
0. <S Ov 00
01 o« OJ OJ
CO

PO PO VO
p P »o «> fvJ *^ PO 0
n C> O »^ »*> "^ 0 OJ to PO

PO to \O
o* OJ
o 0 PO 1-1 OJ OJ

e;« O OOO
^^
PO 0 00
00 PO r-)
lO »O V) »O S »o 0 PO
*~ »^ PO O

O V) O
P ^ i-<
ft OJ
g Ui P vO
P O P«J o
00 1-1
OJ OJ OJ PO OJ

Ov vO PO *>.
^ to p wj ^ O) VO 0
oi 0 PO OJ to OJ
Q ^

11 s
03

to
pq •<*• Ov

< 1-i CO O P>J "^ 0 O) OJ

00 cs 1-1 00 OJ to
H PO
'H ^ O *<» 1-1 PO

&; %
PO vp >-• VO VO PO 00 1—1
a w 1-t Ov vq oq •*

PO ^ PO

s ss »o I
I 13 pg 00 PO PO
I-H 00 1^ vO
to
PO

I O PO ro *^ 0 Ol O4 OJ

o
o
s 00
03

to to

I 0 PO
9>
0 Ol
VO

O vO O
s tr VO

I
00
lo PNj to
rH •^ O PO 0 PO


1
11
C/3C/J H 1
S16 Child Development
found in the study is of the relationship between with some small amount of anger thrown in. No
sex of the subject, attributed sex of the infant, and diflFerences appear between the attributed boy and
experience with infants, F(l,2352) = 11.48; p < the attributed girl.
.001. The means for this interaction are presented
in table 2. As can be seen, males with high experi- In contrast, the infant's response to situation
ence with infants see more of a difference due to 2, the jack-in-the-box, is the most ambiguous of
the sex label than males with little experience with all. At first the infant stares at the box and shows a
infants. If anything, females show the opposite ef- slight startled reaction when it is first opened.
fect. Females see about the same difference be- Upon successive presentations the infant be-
tween boys and girls, but the direction of the ef- comes more and more agitated and after the third
fect is the opposite of that for males. That is, presentation the infant cries when the box is
females with high experience with infants give a pushed forward (even before it is opened) and
higher rating (e.g., see a greater intensity of emo- screams when the jack-in-the-box jumps up. The
tional response) in girls than in boys, and the op- means for this rating are given in table 3, exclud-
posite is true of the females with little experience ing the rating for pleasure which is virtually the
with infants. These findings suggest that the eflFect same for the "boy" and the "girl" (1.48 vs. 1.46).
of having experience with children may well be The striking diflFerences manifested in this particu-
diflFerent for men than for women. lar situation, F(6,2352) = 2.46, p < .02, suggest
thatgiucn a certain degree of ambiguity, if you see
Attributed sex also interacted with emotion a boy crying you may well be more willing to at-
and situation, F(6,2352) = 2.31, p < .03. This tribute this emotion to 'anger"—a more "mas-
interaction suggests that when the infant is labeled culine" response and if you think you are watching
as a boy it is seen as showing more pleasure, across a girl, to "fear"—a more "feminine" response.
all situations, than when the infant is labeled as a
girl, although, as the data in table 2 indicated, this Semantic differential. —There were no differ-
tendency is more pronounced for male subjects ences due to the sex of the observer in the seman-
than for female subjects. In fact, it may be the tic differential scales, and so the responses of all
reverse for women who see slightly more inten- observers were combined and analyzed by the
sity" of emotion in the attributed girl than do three subscales of the semantic differential. The
males. In general, there is a good deal of variation results of this analysis are given in table 4.
by situation. Although we have no a priori mea- These data show that the infant as a boy was
sure, subjectively at least, it appears to us that the seen as being slightly, but significantly, more "ac-
more "ambiguous" the situation, the more of a tive" and "potent" than the infant as a girl, al-
difference subjects report seeing between the though the "two" children were seen as equally
sexes. Thus, for example, the teddy bear elicits a "good."
good deal of positive emotion and little else. The
situation is relatively unambiguous and few differ-
ences between the attributed sexes are recorded. Discussion
The same thing appears to be true of the buzzer. The findings of this study bear on the social-
This stimulus is obviously noxious (so much so that mediational approach to the development of sex
the stimulus situation is terminated after only diflFerences. They do not suggest that this is the
three as opposed to the usual five presentations), only way that sex diflFerences come about, but they
and the child cries intensely throughout. The rat- do oflFer a way of assessing the parameters for this
ings refiect this lack of ambiguity, with most sub- manner of producing sex differences. Our findings
jects rating this display as an intense fear response suggest that, when people observe a child behav-

TABLE 2
MEANS ASSOCIATED WITH THE T R I P L E INTERACTION
OF SEX (OF SUBJECT) X ATTRIBUTED SEX
(OF INFANT) X EXPERIENCE WITH INFANTS,
SUMMED OVER A L L OTHER GATEGORIES

SEX OF S: M A L E SEX OF S: FEMALE

ATTRIBUTED S E X High Exp. Low Exp. High Exp. Low Exp.

"Boy" 3 61 2. 66 2.41 2. 72
"Girl" . 1 83 2. 56 2.83 2. 46
Condry and Condry SI 7
TABLE 3 perception. These findings suggest only directions
MEANS ASSOCIATED WITH THE ANGER-FEAR in which to look for differences. Thus males appear
RESPONSE TO SITUATION 2 to observe larger diflFerences between attributed
(JACK-IN-THE-BOX) , boys and girls than do females. This could mean
ARRANGED BY that males are more inclined to perceive differ-
ATTRIBUTED SEX ences (where there are none) and possibly to act
upon these perceptions. This finding gains in im-
ATTRIBUTED SEX portance because much of the work in the area of
parental treatment effects on children is done by
EMOTION "Boy" "Girl" observing mother-child interactions (Nash 1965;
Bronfenbrenner, Note 1). Our findings do not
Anger 3.01 2.74 imply that this is wrong, but in terms of potential
Fear.. 2.82 3.26
sociall^^ mediated treatment differences our find-
ings do suggest that it may be the father who ex-
pects boys and girls to be particularly diflFerent
ing where it is possible to control all aspects of the and, if he believes that, perhaps treats them dif-
situation and to vary only the label of "boy" or ferently as well.
"girl," this manupulation alone is enough to lead
to observed diflFerences in perceived emotional re- Apparently having experience with infants
sponsiveness, and that these diflFerences vary with makes some diflFerence in what one sees, also, in
respect to characteristics of the observer as well as line with the findings of Meyer and Sobieszek
the situation and emotion being judged. These (1972). Indeed, experience with infants also inter-
findings suggest that caution be taken in interpret- acts with actual sex of the observer to produce the
ing observational studies where the sex of the sub- interesting pattem of results described in table 2.
ject is known by the observer and where actions to We found that high-experienced males saw a big-
be observed bear upom some sex stereotypical ger difference between the "boy" and the "girl"
behavior. It would be interesting to know, for than did low-experienced males, while no such
example, in light of these findings, if diflFerences in effect of experience was evident for females. In
observed aggressiveness in male and female nur- addition, the direction of the eflFect was diflFerent
sery school children, reported in an abundance of for males and females. All of this suggest that ex-
studies (Bandura, Ross, & Ross 1963; Durrett perience with children may be represented diflFer-
1959; Hattwick 1937; Oetzel 1966), would hold up ently for males and females, and it may have dif-
when the subjects being observed were cross- ferent effects for each. Once again, the direction
labeled as to their sex. Given the current trend of and substance of these effects must await further
long hair on boys it would undoubtedly be possi- elaboration, but our findings suggest that this is an
ble to videotape "androgynous" children playing important area for exploration.
in a nursery school and have observers rate the The kinds of differences seen between the at-
tapes. tributed boy and the attributed girl are a function
not only of the beholder but also of the "stimulus
In addition to suggesting a method for study-
situation" beheld. Across all situations the attrib-
ing the effects of attributions regarding sex diflFer-
uted boy was seen as displaying more pleasure and
ences, the present study also suggests several
less fear than the attributed girl. When each situa-
areas to explore in the hopes of finding characteris-
tion was viewed alone, by far the most interesting
tics of the observers that lead to diflFerences in
interaction is that reported in table 3. In this par-
ticular segment the emotional response was
somewhat ambiguous, and we found that the
TABLE 4
"negative" emotion displayed was labeled "anger"
MEANS AND SIGNIFICANCE VALUES FOR t TEST if the infant was thought to be a boy, and "fear" if
FOR ANALYSIS OF SEMANTIC DIFFERENTIAL the infant was thought to be a girl. Since the sub-
RESULTS ARRANGED BY SUBSCALE jects were observing the same emotional re-
sponse, this finding serves well to illustrate several
ATTRIBUTED important components of the social mediational
SEMANTIC SEX
DIFFERENTIAL
approach. It is not possible to attribute these dif-
SUBSCALE "Boy" "Girl" t ferences to "eliciting" by the child. The direction
of the effect suggests what might happen next, to
Activity 11.11 10.53 2.40 .05 wit: If you think a child is angry do you treat "him"
Potency 10.31 9.77 2.29 .0.'^ diflFerently than if you think "she" is afraid? Our
Evaluation 11.76 11.94 0.55 N.S. study was not designed to reveal treatment diflFer-
818 Oiild Development
ences but it seems reasonable to assume that a References
child who is thou^t to be afraid is held and cud-
dled more than a child who is thought to be angry. Bandura, A.; Eoss, D.; & Ross, S. A. Transmission d"
Regardless of the direction of the diflFerence, if aggression through imitation of aggressive modek.
future research shows treatment diflFerences, these Jourrud of Abnomud and Social Psychology, 1963,
could highlight an important causal sequence in 66, 3-11.
the development of sex diflFerences. Bell, R. Q.; Weller, G. M.; & Waldrop, M. F. Newborn
and preschooler: organization of behavior and rela-
Afinalpoint made by this interaction of anger tions between periods. Monographs of the Society
and fear with attributed sex is that social in- for Research in ChUd Development, 1971, 36(1-2,
fluences are most likely to occur in situations Series No. 142).
which are somewhat ambiguous. Iliis is particu-
Broverman, I. K.; Vogel, S. R.; Broverman, D. M.;
larly clear in the labeling of emotions (Schachter
Glarkson, F. E.; & Rosenkrantz, P. S. Sex role ste-
1964), the behavior under study in this experi-
reotypes and clinical judgments of mental health.
ment. If a child falls and scrapes a knee, or laughs
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
joyfully at a birthdaiy present, the "situational" at-
1970, 34, 1-7.
tributions are clear and few eflFects due to the sex
Glarke-Stewart, K. A. Interactions between mothere and
label would be expected—or, as our data indicate,
their young children. Monographs of the Society for
are likely to be found. On the other hand, a large
Research in Child Development, 1973, 38(6-7, Se-
number of situations encountered while children
rial No. 153).
^ e growing up are ambiguous, and so lend them-
selves to the kinds of interpretation revealed in Durrett, M. E. The relationship of early infant regula-
this study. In short, stereotypes take over when tion and later behavior in play interviews. Child
other explanations are less viable. The directions Development, 1959, 30, 211-216.
of those stereotypes have been studied by others Goldberg, S., & Lewis, M. Play behavior in the year-old
(cf. Brovennan, Vogel, Broverman, Clarkson, & in&nt: early sex diflferences. Child Development,
Rosenkrantz 1970) and are reflected in our study in 1969, 40, 21-31.
the anger-fear interaction described above and in Hattwick, L. A. Sex differences in hehavior of nursery
the results of the semantic diflFerential analysis. school children. Child Development, 1937, 8, 343-
The semantic diflFerential data are interesting and 355..
they raise some important questions. Is it the case Jersild, A. T., & Holmes, F. B. Ghildren's fears. Child
that the sex label is seldom used "evaluatively" Development, 1935, 20.
although it does have activity and potency mean- Maccoby, E. M., & Jacklin, G. N. The psychology of sex
ings? Or, alternatively, is this result simply the differences. Stanford, Galif.: Stanford University
outcome of the particular situations sampled in Press, 1974.
this research? Since the situations sampled in this Mclntyre, A. Sex differences in children's aggression.
study were reasonably broad and the emotional Proceedings of the 80th Annual Convention of the
attributions quite Variable, we are inclined to be- American Psychological Association, 1972, 7,
lieve that the sex label is seldom used evalua- 93-94.
tively, at least by the population studied. Meyer, J. W , & Sobieszek, B. J. The effect of a child's
sex on adult interpretation of its behavior. De-
All our results lend credence to the possibility velopmental Psychology, 1972, 6, 42-48.
suggested by the social mediational approach de- Nash, J. The fether in contemporary culture and current
scribed in the begirining of tjiis paper. They sug- psychological literature. Child Development, 1965,
gest that when all else is held constant, people still 36, 261-297.
see differences due to the label of "boy" and/or Oetzel, R. M. Glassified summary of research in sex dif-
"girl," and that these (iijBFerences tend to be most ferences. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The developtnent
obvious in ambiguous situations and to follow the of sex differences. Stanford, Galif.: Stanford Univer-
lines of socially accepted sex role Stereotypes. It sity Press, 1966.
would appear, in short, that a lot more than Osgood, G. E.; Suci, G. J.; & Tannenhaum, P. H. The
"beauty" resides in the eye of the beholder. measure of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois,
1957.
Pederson, F. A., & Bell, R. Q. Sex differences in pre-
school children without histories of complications of
Reference Note
pregnancy and delivery. Developtnental Psychol-
1. Bronfenbrenner, U. An emerging theoretical per- ogy, 1970, 3, 10-15.
spective for research in human development. Ricciuti, H. N., & Poresky, R. H. Emotional behavior
Speech presented at president's symposium at an- and development in the first year of life: an analysis
nual meeting of the Society for Research in Ghild of arousal, approach-withdrawal, and affective re-
Development, Philadelphia, March 1973. sponses. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium
Condry and C<Hidry 819
on ChUd Psychology (\'ol. 6). Minneapolis: Univer- kowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental sodal psy-
sity of Minnesota Press, 1^2. chology. New York: Academic Press, 1964.
Robson, K. S.; Federson, F. A.; & Moss, H. A. De- Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. Gognitive, social, and
velopmental observations of dyadic gazing in rela- physiological determinants of emotional state.
tion to the fear of strangers and social approach be- Psychological Review, 1962, 69, 379-399.
havior. Child Development, 1969, -W, 619-^27. Smith, P. K., & GonnoUy, K. Pattems of play and social
Rothbart, M. K.; & Maccoby, E. E. Parents' differential interaction in pre-school children. In N. B. Jones
reactions to sons and daughters. Joumal of Personal- (Ed.), Ethological studies of child behavior. Lon-
ity and Social Psychology, 1966, 4, 237-243. don: Gambridge University Press, 1972.
Schachter, S. The interaction of cognitive and physiolog- Winer, B. J. Statistical principles in experimental design.
ical detenninants of emotional state. In L. Ber- New York: McGraw-HiU, 1962.

Potrebbero piacerti anche