Sei sulla pagina 1di 39

Personality and Social Psychology

Review http://psr.sagepub.com/

The What, How, Why, and Where of Self-Construal


Susan E. Cross, Erin E. Hardin and Berna Gercek-Swing
Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2011 15: 142 originally published online 17 August 2010
DOI: 10.1177/1088868310373752

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://psr.sagepub.com/content/15/2/142

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Society for Personality and Social Psychology

Additional services and information for Personality and Social Psychology Review can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://psr.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://psr.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://psr.sagepub.com/content/15/2/142.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Apr 5, 2011

OnlineFirst Version of Record - Aug 17, 2010

What is This?

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Personality and Social Psychology Review

The What, How, Why, and 15(2) 142­–179


© 2011 by the Society for Personality
and Social Psychology, Inc.
Where of Self-Construal Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1088868310373752
http://pspr.sagepub.com

Susan E. Cross1, Erin E. Hardin2, and Berna Gercek-Swing1

Abstract
Since the publication of Markus and Kitayama’s pivotal article on culture and the self, the concepts of independent, relational,
and interdependent self-construal have become important constructs in cultural psychology and research on the self. The authors
review the history of these constructs, their measurement and manipulation, and their roles in cognition, emotion, motiva-
tion, and social behavior. They make suggestions for future research and point to problems still to be sorted out. Researchers
interested in these constructs have many opportunities to make important contributions to the literature in a variety of fields,
including health psychology, education, counseling, and international relations.

Keywords
culture, ethnicity, self, identity, social cognition

When research on the self burst onto the social psychological in the research record to explore Where to from here? with
scene more than three decades ago, it reflected the cultural the hopes that researchers will continue to further refine the
assumptions and beliefs of the researchers’ backgrounds. methods used to investigate this topic and further explore the
These researchers were largely from European, Canadian, implications of self-construal for human behavior.
and European American backgrounds, and they assumed that
the self was autonomous, independent of the influence of
others, bounded, unique, integrated, and fundamentally prior What Is Self-Construal?
to society and social relationships (Geertz, 1975). In the inter- Psychological researchers have long been interested in the
vening decades, both the researchers studying the self and the existence and implications of multiple representations of the
assumptions about the self have become more culturally self. William James (1890), for example, distinguished the
diverse. This diversity has resulted in the articulation and material, social, and spiritual selves. One of the most produc-
development of the concept of self-construal. First coined by tive distinctions has been made between private and public
Markus and Kitayama (1991), research on self-construal has selves (Baumeister, 1986; Greenwald & Pratkanis, 1984).
come to have a prominent place among influential concepts This distinction between inner, unobservable selves and the
in psychology, especially in regard to research on self-related outer selves we portray to others provided a useful conceptual
concepts and processes, social cognition, close relationships, framework for understanding a range of representations of the
and cultural psychology. self and their implications for cognition, affect, and behavior.
This article reviews the existing research on self-construal Not surprisingly, however, this early work did not explicitly
and its role in cognition, emotion, motivation, and social consider the ways in which culture shapes the development
behavior. Our overarching goal is to provide a guidebook for and expression of these different types of self. In one of the
researchers—an integrated look at where research and theory earliest efforts to do so, Triandis (1989) elaborated on the
on self-construal are after 20 years and where we believe they distinctions among the private, public, and collective selves
are going and need to go. To achieve this goal, we first address (cf. Greenwald & Pratkanis, 1984) and argued that cultural
the What is it? question with a brief review of the history of differences reliably lead to differential expression or sampling
this construct and its development since the original statement
by Markus and Kitayama (1991). Second, we address How is 1
Iowa State University, Ames, IA, USA
it investigated? with a review of the most common measure- 2
Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, USA
ment tools and manipulations. Third, we address the question,
Corresponding Author:
Why is self-construal important? by detailing the accumulated Susan E. Cross, Iowa State University, Department of Psychology,
evidence regarding its role in psychological processes. Through- W117 Lagomarcino Hall, Ames, IA 50011
out, we also seek to point out gaps, problems, or inconsistencies Email: scross@iastate.edu

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 143

of these three selves. Around the same time, Markus and and regulating emotional expression to maintain group har-
Kitayama (1991) similarly reviewed research that showed that mony are considered signs of maturity. Interpersonal relation-
members of European or American and Asian cultures con- ships are of utmost importance to those with an InterSC, with
structed differing self-views. As they illustrated, Western the individual concerned primarily with how he or she benefits
cultures prioritize the individual over the group, and individuals the group. Others become a source of definition for the self
seek independence, autonomy, and separateness from others. (I am a responsible student and an impulsive friend), and social
In East Asian cultures, the group is prioritized over the indi- comparison is used to determine whether one is fulfilling obli-
vidual, and individuals seek to fit into the group and maintain gations within those relationships.
harmony in the group. Although other researchers had broached Markus and Kitayama (1991) and others (e.g., Singelis,
the issue of the self as critical in understanding cultural dif- 1994; Triandis, 1989) have argued that individuals possess
ferences, Triandis (1989) and Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) both IndSC and InterSC but that cultural context typically
articles were the first to make this argument clearly and to promotes the development of one or the other self-construal
make it accessible to a wide audience. more strongly. As noted above, the United States and Japan
Markus and Kitayama (1991) coined the term self-construal are generally considered prototypical cultures that promote
in describing the ways that Americans and Japanese define development of IndSC and InterSC, respectively, with IndSC
and make meaning of the self. To construe means to “show or tending to be more well elaborated and salient in Western
explain the meaning or intention of” (Random House Diction- countries and InterSC tending to be more well elaborated and
ary, 1980); thus, self-construal refers to how individuals define salient in non-Western countries, including parts of Asia,
and make meaning of the self. Markus and Kitayama identified Africa, and Central and South America. Not surprisingly then,
two such self-construals, independent and interdependent. IndSC and InterSC are typically identified as corresponding
Although they noted that these are only two of many possible to individualist and collectivist cultures. However, Markus
self-construals, the term self-construal has become virtually and Kitayama never make this connection explicitly.
synonymous with independence and interdependence; thus, Nonetheless, the connection between IndSC and individual-
self-construal is typically defined as how individuals see the ism and between InterSC and collectivism is clear—so clear,
self in relation to others. in fact, that it can be difficult to distinguish between self-
Markus and Kitayama (1991) proposed that Europeans and construal and individualism–collectivism (IND-COL). Con-
Americans construe the self as fundamentally individual and ceptually, IND-COL is a dimension used to describe cultures,
separate from others, and they labeled this the independent whereas self-construal describes individuals. IND-COL, then,
self-construal (IndSC). For persons with high IndSC, the ques- is not an individual differences variable, whereas self-construal
tion, “Who am I?” is likely to be answered with reference to is. This distinction is complicated by the fact that where a
internal traits that are stable across situations (e.g., outgoing, country falls on the IND-COL dimension is typically deter-
shy, intelligent, ambitious) or that set one apart from others mined by the average responses of individuals on self-report
(e.g., especially creative, a straight-A student). Demonstrating surveys. Indeed, many self-report measures of IND-COL and
one’s uniqueness is an important basis of self-esteem. Being self-construal share items. Despite these measurement prob-
“the same person” across situations and communicating assert- lems, the conceptual distinction remains: IND-COL describes
ively are considered signs of maturity. Interpersonal relation- cultures; self-construal describes individuals. Sports analogies
ships are, of course, important to those with an IndSC, but are helpful in understanding this distinction. We might refer to
significant others are typically important foremost for how a football team as having a good defense, whereas we describe
they benefit the individual (e.g., for the support or esteem they the individual members of the defense as quick, agile, and ath-
provide to the independent individual). Furthermore, others are letic. Without these individual traits of quickness, agility, and
a source of social comparison for confirming one’s uniqueness athleticism, the team could not have a strong defense, just as
(compared to my friends, I am more creative) and internal a culture can not be individualistic if the majority of its mem-
traits (I demonstrate my compassion in my relationships with bers are not independent. Similarly, the individual members
my family). of the football team vary in the degree to which they possess
In contrast, Markus and Kitayama (1991) pointed out that these traits, just as individuals within a culture vary in the
the Japanese tend to construe the self as fundamentally con- degree to which they espouse an independent versus interde-
nected to others and defined by relationships with others, which pendent self-construal.
they labeled the interdependent self-construal (InterSC). For A strong defense in football requires more than just skilled
persons with high InterSC, the question, “Who am I?” is likely players; it also requires strategies and game plans that go
to be answered with reference to important relationships (e.g., beyond the individual players’ abilities. Similarly, self-construal
daughter, friend, coworker) or group memberships (fraternity is only one of several components that distinguish individual-
member, Asian American). Demonstrating one’s ability to fit istic and collectivistic cultures. For example, Triandis (1995)
into the group is an important basis of self-esteem. Changing identified three additional attributes that distinguish individu-
one’s behavior to respond to the demands of different situations alistic and collectivist cultures: (a) variation in structure of goals

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


144 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

(in-group focused vs. individual focused), (b) behavior as a relational–interdependent self-construal (often shortened to
function of social norm versus individual attitudes, and (c) a relational self-construal [RelSC]). They defined RelSC as the
focus on the needs of the in-group versus social exchanges. extent to which people define themselves in terms of close
These attributes may in some ways be related to self-construal relationships and differentiated it from the group-centered
(e.g., variation in goals may derive from variation in self- collective–interdependent self-construal described by Markus
construal), but in this article, we will define self-construal as and Kitayama (1991; often measured by the Singelis, 1994,
one component of IND-COL. Unfortunately, the two terms are Interdependent Self-Construal Scale).
often confused or used interchangeably. Where relevant, we RelSC, therefore, is a general individual difference construct
comment on this confusion and strive to articulate the source and is distinct from other similar-sounding constructs that focus
of the confusion and clarify the important differences. on specific close relationships. For example, the Inclusion of
Other in the Self scale (Aron, Aron, Tudor, & Nelson, 1991) taps
commitment to a specific relationship rather than a general ori-
Tripartite Self-Construal: Relational–Interdependent entation to represent oneself in terms of close relationships (as
and Collective–Interdependent Self-Construals does the Relational-Interdependent Self-construal Scale [RISC]).
The distinctions made by Markus and Kitayama (1991) and Andersen’s (Andersen & Chen, 2002) theory of relational selves
Triandis (1989) began to stimulate investigation into other vari- also focuses on specific relationships and on individuals’ distinc-
eties of self-construal. Triandis described private, public, and tive and persistent ways of interacting in particular relationships.
collective selves, but this tripartite distinction has unfortunately For instance, Leigh’s interactions with her peers may be marked
received relatively little empirical attention. Instead, researchers by confidence and positive emotions, but the particular relational
have focused more attention on self-construal based on close self (in Andersen’s terms) that has been created in her interactions
relationships as a third dimension of self-construal (Brewer & with her overbearing older brother may be uniquely marked by
Gardner, 1996; Y. Kashima et al., 1995). Y. Kashima and his resentment and anger. RelSC, instead, is conceived as a relatively
colleagues (1995) were the first to demonstrate that independent, global dimension that captures the extent to which people con-
collective, and relational views of the self were empirically ceptualize themselves as defined by their close relationships.
separable. In addition, they argued that whereas individualism Several researchers have sought to differentiate RelSC and
and collectivism differentiated Western from East Asian cultural collective–interdependent self-construal. For example, Gabriel
groups, across cultures men and women were best differentiated and Gardner (1999; also see Baumeister & Sommer, 1997)
on the relatedness dimension. investigated the hypothesis that American men and women
In a separate line of research, Cross and Madson (1997; Cross, differ in the extent to which they are characterized by these
Bacon, & Morris, 2000) argued that gender differences in behavior two forms of InterSC. Using a variety of methods, they found
among Western participants could also be explained by differences that American women were more likely than men to character-
in self-construal. Integrating research by gender theorists (e.g., ize themselves in relational terms but men were more likely
Gilligan, 1982; Jordan & Surrey, 1986; Stewart & Lykes, 1985) than women to describe themselves in terms of larger, group
with the newer work on culture and self, they suggested that collectives. Thus, InterSC may be considered to have two com-
women in Western societies were more likely than men to construct ponents: a relational component (RelSC) and a collective or
self-views that incorporated close relationships with other people. group-oriented component (Coll-InterSC).
Men, on the other hand, were more likely to conceive of themselves Unfortunately this distinction is rarely observed by research-
as independent or separate from their close relationships (but for ers; many times, the term interdependent self-construal is used
a different perspective on gender differences in self-construal, see without regard to whether close relationships or group member-
Baumeister & Sommer, 1997; Gabriel & Gardner, 1999). Their ships are the focus of attention. This is understandable, given the
article went on to detail how these differences in self-construals history of the concept of InterSC. With a focus on East–West
could explain many observed gender differences in cognition, differences, Triandis (1995) and Markus and Kitayama (1991)
motivation, emotion, and social behavior. defined collectivism and InterSC in terms of close relationships
Although Cross and Madson (1997) used the term inter- and important in-groups, including families and work groups
dependent self-construal to refer to ways in which women in (thus, they grouped together relational and group-oriented com-
the West may define themselves in terms of close relationships, ponents of self-construal). More recently, Yuki, Maddux, Brewer,
they acknowledged that the form of interdependence experi- and Takemura (2005) showed that East Asian in-groups tend to
enced by Western women could be quite different from the be based on relational ties whereas Western groups are based on
group-oriented interdependence of East Asians. As they wrote categorical distinctions between in-groups and out-groups. Thus,
then, “[F]or U.S. adults, a self-construal based on relationships they suggest that InterSC among East Asians primarily focuses
with others may be more likely to focus on individual relation- on close relationships (and so emphasizes the RelSC) whereas
ships (e.g., with one’s spouse, close friends, siblings) than on InterSC among European Americans primarily focuses on groups
group memberships or social roles” (p. 8). In later work, Cross and collectives (and so emphasizes Coll-InterSC). Building on
and her colleagues (2000), referred to this as the this work and taking the perspective of social identity theory,

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 145

Brewer and Chen (2007) argued that collectivism is best under- Effects on emotion. Arguing that most emotions implicate the
stood as membership in large shared social categories (e.g., eth- self, Markus and Kitayama (1991) hypothesized that “emo-
nicity, nationality) rather than in groups of relationship tional experience should vary systematically with the construal
connections, and they showed that most measures of collectivism of the self” (p. 235). More specifically, emotional experience
or InterSC focus on interpersonal relationships. As this debate should vary in two ways. First, the same emotion may be elic-
illustrates, there is significant confusion over the boundaries ited by different conditions for IndSC and InterSC. Second,
between RelSC and Coll-InterSC. We view InterSC as a super- people with IndSC versus InterSC will express and experience
ordinate category, with RelSC and Coll-InterSC as subcompo- different emotions. Markus and Kitayama (1991) distinguished
nents within this larger category. In many cases researchers have between so-called ego-focused emotions (e.g., anger, pride)
used InterSC to refer to both relational and collective orientations; and other-focused emotions (e.g., shame). They hypothesized
we have chosen to adopt the researchers’ usage in these cases that those with an IndSC are more likely to express and to
except where one or the other subcomponents is made explicit. experience ego-focused emotions, whereas those with an
Despite these distinctions, most research continues to focus on InterSC are more likely to express and to experience other-
the more general consequences of InterSC and IndSC articulated focused emotions. Relatedly, Markus and Kitayama hypoth-
by Markus and Kitayama (1991). In the next section, we briefly esized that people with differing self-construals would express
review their hypotheses. and/or experience the same emotions with differing intensity.
For example, not only are interdependent individuals expected
to experience anger less often than independent individuals,
Hypothesized Consequences of Cultural but they are expected to experience anger less intensely when
Differences in Self-Construal they do experience it.
Markus and Kitayama (1991) offered detailed discussions of
the implications of self-construal for cognition, emotion, and Effects on motivation. Markus and Kitayama (1991) offered
motivation, reviewing cross-cultural literature that supported several hypotheses regarding the effects of self-construal on
their hypotheses. Of course, such literature did not directly the types of goals individuals are motivated to pursue. The
measure self-construal, so Markus and Kitayama relied on the most intuitive of these is that individuals with an InterSC are
assumption that Japanese people were more likely to have an more likely to be motivated by socially oriented goals. Impor-
InterSC, whereas those from Western cultures were more likely tantly, it is not that these individuals have a higher need for
to have an IndSC (cf. Matsumoto, 1999). Markus and Kita- affiliation, per se, but rather that they are more likely to be
yama demonstrated that a broad range of cultural differences motivated to fulfill their roles within important relationships.
in cognition, emotion, and motivation could be explained Second, Markus and Kitayama argued that agency, or a sense
by invoking cultural differences in self-construal. We briefly of personal control and efficacy, is experienced differently,
review their hypotheses concerning the effects of self-construal depending on one’s self-construal. Specifically,
on cognition, emotion, and motivation.
for those with independent selves, agency will be
Effects of self-construal on cognition. Given that those with an experienced as an effort to express one’s internal
InterSC define the self in relation to others and are motivated needs, rights, and capacities and to withstand undue
to fit in to the current social context, Markus and Kitayama social pressure, whereas among those with interde-
(1991) argued that interdependent individuals are more likely pendent selves, agency will be experienced as an effort
to pay attention to others and to social context, resulting in to be receptive to others, to adjust to their needs and
three primary effects on cognition. First, individuals with an demands, and to restrain one’s own inner needs or
InterSC are likely to know more about, and thus to have more desires. (p. 240)
elaborate cognitive representations of, others than are those
with an IndSC. Second, individuals with an InterSC are more In other words, independent and interdependent individuals
likely to have cognitive representations of the self that incor- both experience a sense of being active agents in pursuit of
porate a social context than are independent individuals. their goals, but these specific goals differ depending on one’s
Finally, Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that self-con- self-construal (cf. Kağitçibaşi, 2005).
strual also affects cognitive processes that appear to be non- Third, Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that motives
social, such as counterfactual thinking or reasoning about such as self-enhancement take on different meanings when the
abstract concepts. They argued that interdependent individuals self that is being enhanced is independent versus interdepen-
are more likely to consider their role in the relationship dent. For example, given an InterSC, self-esteem is based on
between themselves and the interviewer and thus to respond one’s ability to fit in, whereas for those with an IndSC, self-
to apparently abstract questions differently than an indepen- esteem is based on one’s ability to be unique. Thus, to enhance
dent individual who is much less, if at all, concerned with how the IndSC, one is motivated to demonstrate uniqueness, result-
his or her answer will be construed. ing in self-serving biases, which are much less common among

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


146 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

those with an InterSC. These differences in the self and bases for behaviors described in short vignettes, supporting the pre-
for self-esteem also mean that different motives will be expe- dictive validity of the SCS. However, the interitem reliabilities
rienced as more or less positive or desirable, depending on of the two scales tend to be adequate at best. Singelis (1994)
self-construal. For example, the so-called need for abasement reported Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities of .73 and .74 for the
may be desirable for those with an InterSC but considered IndSC scale and .69 and .70 for the InterSC scale. Most pub-
almost pathological for those with an IndSC. lished research using these scales report comparable reli-
Finally, given that a sign of maturity for those with an abilities, although they have been found to be even lower
InterSC is the ability to refrain from expressing private attitudes (e.g., α = .58 and .53 on the InterSC and InterSC scales,
and feelings to maintain social harmony, counterattitudinal respectively, among undergraduates in Hong Kong; Singelis,
behavior is not likely to create dissonance-related distress Bond, Sharkey, & Lai, 1999).
among those with an InterSC. In other words, those with an Additional items have been added to the SCS, resulting in
InterSC are much less likely to be motivated to reduce cogni- various versions being used in the literature, including IndSC
tive dissonance than are those with an IndSC. scales with 13 (Singelis et al., 1999), 14 (Trafimow & Finlay,
In the remainder of this article, we examine the ways 1996), 15 (Hardin, Leong, & Osipow, 2001), and 16 items (Kwan,
that researchers have responded to and elaborated on the Bond, & Singelis, 1997) and InterSC scales with 14 (e.g.,
original Markus and Kitayama (1991) theses. Because of Yamada & Singelis, 1999) and 15 items (e.g., Hardin et al.,
space limitations, we focus on research that employs either 2001; Kwan et al., 1997). The 12- and 15-item versions appear
measures or manipulations of self-construal. Unfortunately, to be most common. In addition, some authors have created
therefore, a thorough description of research that uses cul- a unidimensional self-construal score by reverse scoring
ture as a proxy for self-construal must be saved for another the InterSC items (e.g., Aaker, 2000; Pöhlmann & Hannover,
time. We begin first with a review of the most frequently 2006); however, such unidimensional scores are clearly con-
used means of measuring and manipulating self-construal, trary to the intended use of the SCS and theoretical understand-
followed by a review of the research examining the role of ing of the nature of self-construal (Singelis, 1994). Although
self-construal in cognition, emotion, motivation, and social most items have good face validity, several have been ques-
behavior. tioned (e.g., “I value being in good health above everything”;
Hardin, Leong, & Bhagwat, 2004; Levine et al., 2003). The
SCS has been used in hundreds of studies and translated into
How Is Self-Construal Studied? numerous languages, including Chinese (Aaker & Schmitt,
Approaches to Measuring and 2001), Japanese (Ozawa, Crosby, & Crosby, 1996), Greek
Manipulating Self-Construal (Nezlek, Kafetsios, & Smith, 2008), Thai (Polyorat & Alden,
Measuring Independent and Interdependent 2005), Arabic, and Hebrew (Kurman, 2001).
A second self-report measure of self-construal is that devel-
Self-Construal
oped by Gudykunst et al. (1996). As with the SCS (Singelis,
Likert-type scales. The majority of research on self-construal 1994) a large item pool was drawn from existing measures
has relied on self-report measures with Likert-type scales. (including the SCS) and supplemented with original items. Data
Measures of IND-COL, sociotropy–autonomy, allocentrism, from four countries (United States, Japan, Korea, and Australia)
and other related constructs are sometimes used as proxy were submitted to a principal components analysis, after stan-
measures of self-construal. However, several measures have dardizing item responses within culture. On one of the two
been designed specifically to measure IndSC and InterSC as factors, 29 items had acceptably high loadings, resulting in a
individual-differences variables as defined by Markus and 14-item IndSC scale and a 15-item InterSC scale. A total of
Kitayama (1991). The first and most common of these is the 3 (6) items on the IndSC (InterSC) scale also appear on the
Self-Construal Scale (SCS; Singelis, 1994). The SCS provides Singelis scales. Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities were adequate
separate scores for IndSC and InterSC, consistent with theo- to good, ranging from .80 (United States) to .85 (Korea) on
retical predictions that the two self-construals are orthogonal the IndSC scale and from .73 (Korea) to .83 (Australia) on
dimensions rather than opposite ends of a single continuum the InterSC scale. Although the four cultural groups did not
(Singelis, 1994). An initial 45-item pool included original items differ as might be expected in mean scores on the IndSC and
and modified items drawn from existing measures of related InterSC scales, self-construal did predict high and low context
constructs. Exploratory principal components analysis, fol- communication as expected.
lowed by confirmatory factor analyses, in multiethnic Hawaiian Hackman, Ellis, Johnson, and Staley (1999) further tested
samples resulted in two 12-item scales. Scores on the items and refined the Gudykunst et al. (1996) scales. Using confir-
demonstrated the expected between-group differences, with matory factor analysis (CFA) with a large sample of under-
Asian Americans being more interdependent and less indepen- graduates drawn from three countries (the United States,
dent than European Americans. Furthermore, InterSC scores New Zealand, and Kyrgyzstan), Hackman et al. were unable
predicted participants’ tendency to make situational attributions to get the hypothesized two-factor model to adequately fit the

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 147

data, despite numerous data-driven modifications to the model. than two factors during development of their self-construal
However, when the IndSC and InterSC items were factor ana- scales. However, there is considerable evidence that this simple
lyzed separately, two well-fitting one-factor solutions were two-factor structure does not provide a good fit to the data
derived after dropping several items from each scale and allow- from any of the scales (Hardin, 2006; Hardin et al., 2004;
ing pairs of error variances among some items to covary. These Levine et al., 2003). In fact, the results of a CFA led Singelis
analyses resulted in an 11-item IndSC scale and a 12-item (1994) to note that for the SCS, “Though clearly superior to
InterSC scale. Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities were adequate to the one-factor model, the two-factor model seems only ade-
good, ranging from .89 (United States) to .77 (Kyrgyzstan) on quate” (p. 586). The two-factor model was found to be a poor
the IndSC scale and from .86 (New Zealand) to .78 (Kyrgyzstan) fit to responses on (a) the Leung and Kim (1997) scale in
on the InterSC scale. Moreover, the 11-item IndSC scale dem- samples of college students from the United States (mainland
onstrated measurement invariance (i.e., invariant factor load- and Hawaii), Japan, and South Korea (Levine et al., 2003),
ings) across all three cultures; two items on the InterSC scale (b) the Gudykunst et al. (1996) scale among college students
were not invariant in the Kyrgyzstan sample. Collapsing across in Hawaii (Levine et al., 2003), and (c) Singelis’s (1994) SCS
the three samples, both scales demonstrated measurement among college students in South Korea, Japan (Levine et al.,
invariance between genders. 2003), and the mainland United States (Hardin, 2006; Hardin
A third Likert-type measure of self-construal was developed et al., 2004; Levine et al., 2003).
by Leung and Kim in 1997 and described in an unpublished More recent research suggests that despite being designed
manuscript (cited in Levine et al., 2003); not surprisingly, there- to measure self-construal in terms of independence and inter-
fore, it has been used less in the literature than the other two dependence, these measures actually have a multidimensional
measures. It consists of two 15-item scales, with the majority of structure. Content analyses of the TST (Somech, 2000) and
items (11 independent and 9 interdependent) appearing on at factor analyses of the Leung and Kim (1997) scale (Levine
least one of the other two scales (see Levine et al., 2003, for a et al., 2003) and the Singelis (1994) scale (Guo, Schwartz, &
list of items appearing on all three measures). Other less fre- McCabe, 2008; Hardin, 2006; Hardin et al., 2004; Sato &
quently used measures have been developed by Oyserman McCann, 1998) all show that multidimensional structures fit
(1993), Kağitçibaşi (2007), and E. S. Kashima and Hardie (2000). the data better than a simple two-factor structure. For example,
Hardin et al. (2004) identified a higher order factor structure
Twenty Statements Test as a measure of self-construal. The Twenty underlying items on the SCS, with four independence factors
Statements Test (TST; Kuhn & McPartland, 1954) has also (Autonomy/Assertiveness, Individualism, Behavioral Consis-
been used as a self-report measure of self-construal (e.g., tency, and Primacy of Self) and two interdependent factors
Agrawal & Maheswaran, 2005; Somech, 2000). On the TST, (Esteem for Group and Relational Interdependence). Note that
participants are asked to complete 20 sentence stems that the two interdependence factors identified in the SCS items
begin, “I am . . .” Responses to these statements may then be map closely onto the RelSC–Coll-InterSC distinction discussed
coded by the researcher as either independent (e.g., “I am above. Importantly, this higher order multifactor structure rep-
intelligent”; “I am shy”), relational (e.g., “I am Bob’s girl- licated to samples of Asian American (Hardin et al., 2004),
friend”; “I am a mother”), or interdependent (e.g., “I am on European American (Hardin, 2006; Hardin et al., 2004),
the soccer team”; “I am African American”). The number of African American, and Latino/a (Hardin, 2006) students.
independent, relational, and interdependent statements gener- Measuring such specific aspects of IndSC and InterSC does
ated may then be used as self-construal scores. Unfortunately, seem to be useful. For example, Somech (2000) found that
authors rarely provide detailed descriptions of their coding although kibbutz members were generally more interdependent
schemes, and researchers may define RelSC and InterSC quite and less independent than urban residents in Israel, the kibbutzim
differently. As a result, it is sometimes difficult to compare endorsed some aspects of IndSC more than the urban residents,
the findings from different researchers using the TST. Fur- who endorsed some aspects of InterSC more than the kibbutzim.
thermore, the TST does not assess the importance of self-views Cultural differences in in-group favoritism were better explained
to the individual. Members of East Asian societies often belong by the degree to which participants emphasized personal or in-
to fewer groups compared to members of Western societies, group interests than by broader measures of self-construal or
but these groups are arguably more important, stable, and IND-COL (Y.-R. Chen, Brockner, & Katz, 1998). Hardin (2006)
self-defining for East Asians (Triandis, 1989). also found that more than 50% more variance in social anxiety
could be accounted for by aspects of independence and interde-
Self-report measures of self-construal: Two or more factors? pendence than by the broader dimensions.
Given that Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) self-construal Based on this evidence that existing measures of self-
theory posits two dimensions of self-construal, IndSC and construal have a multidimensional structure and that these
InterSC, all three Likert-type measures described above were specific aspects of independence and interdependence are
explicitly designed to measure only these two factors. Neither useful predictors of between-group differences and other psy-
Singelis (1994) nor Gudykunst et al. (1996) considered more chological phenomena, there is a clear need for a psychometrically

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


148 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

sound measure of multidimensional self-construal. Hardin, Clark, Oullette, Powell, & Milberg, 1987, which focuses on
Cross, and Hoang (2009) have begun development of such a a desire for reciprocity and equity in close relationships), trait-
measure. oriented measures of communion or expressivity (Spence,
Helmreich, & Stapp, 1974), and a measure of the emotional
Implicit measures of self-construal. Recently, Kitayama and his consequences of investment in close relationships used by
colleagues have used several different measures of basic psy- Y. Kashima et al. (1995).
chological tendencies as implicit measures of self-construal. Research examining the psychometric properties of the
Kitayama, Mesquita, and Karasawa (2006) were apparently RISC scale among North American college students (N =
the first to suggest that measures of psychological processes 4,228; reported in Cross et al., 2000) revealed that it has good
thought to reliably be related to IndSC and InterSC might internal reliability (Cronbach’s alphas generally range from
be used as implicit measures of self-construal. They suggested .85 to .90) and good stability over a 2-month period (test–retest
that measures of the tendency to experience socially engaging reliability is .76). Among these North American samples,
emotions (e.g., guilt, relatedness) versus socially disengaging women usually score higher than men (ds range from –.17 to
emotions (e.g., anger, pride) might be useful as implicit mea- –.57). This measure correlates moderately positively with
sures of self-construal (see the section below on self-construal other measures of relatedness (e.g., the Communal Orientation
and affect for more on the distinction between socially engag- scale, r = .41, and the Singelis InterSC Scale, r = .41) but does
ing and disengaging emotions). Later, Kitayama, Park, Sevincer, not correlate with measures of independence (e.g., the Singelis
Karasawa, and Uskul (2009) used such a measure as one of IndSC Scale, r = .08). The RISC scale generally is unrelated
five implicit measures of self-construal. The others were ten- to well-being measures such as Satisfaction with Life (r = .07;
dency to make situational versus dispositional attributions, use Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) and the Rosenberg
of holistic versus focused attention, relational versus individual (1965) Self-Esteem Scale (r = .01). Examination of the incre-
correlates of happiness, and symbolic self-inflation. As expected, mental utility of the RISC scale showed that it predicted a
participants from the United States scored significantly more significant proportion of variance in scores on broad measures
independently (i.e., reported more socially disengaging emo- of attitudes toward relationships and group memberships,
tions, made more situational attributions, used more focused controlling for other related measures (Cross et al., 2000).
attention, demonstrated stronger personal correlates of happi- Thus, the RISC scale taps self-definition in a unique fashion
ness, and demonstrated a larger inflated self) than participants that is not tapped by other measures of relatedness, expres-
from Japan; moreover, participants from two Western European sivity, or communalism.
countries (the United Kingdom and Germany) did not differ Although Cross and Madson (1997) argued that women in
from each other and scored between the U.S. and Japanese the United States are more likely to construct a RelSC than are
participants on all five measures. Despite this consistent pattern men, RelSC is characterized by the degree to which one includes
of results, the five implicit measures were uncorrelated with others in the self or defines the self in terms of close relation-
each other across all four groups, a point to which we return ships, not the degree to which individuals endorse gender-
at the end of the article. In addition, scores on an explicit measure stereotyped attributes. In fact, Cross and her colleagues (2000)
of self-construal, Singelis’s (1994) SCS, were also uncorrelated were wary of making cross-cultural claims about gender dif-
with all five implicit measures, across all four groups. This lack ferences in their measure of the RelSC because they recognized
of convergence between the explicit SCS measure and the that cultures may vary widely in their construction of gender-
implicit measures is consistent with other research that shows related norms. Unfortunately, others have at times assumed that
that implicit and explicit measures sometimes do not correlate culturally based stereotypes of men and women are equivalent
(e.g., Bosson, Swann, & Pennebaker, 2000; Greenwald & to IndSC and RelSC, respectively (e.g., Guimond, Chatard,
Farnham, 2000). Martinot, Crisp, & Redersdorff, 2006). For example, Guimond
et al. (2006) define IndSC with items that were found to be
central to the stereotype of men in France: lazy, coarse, boastful,
Measuring Relational–Interdependent Self-Construal and selfish. Although these authors argue that this set of attri-
Soon after making the argument that gender differences among butes is consistent with the masculine gender role of dominance
Western populations could be explained in terms of a RelSC, and power, their argument is based on the mistaken assumption
Cross and her colleagues created a measure of this relational that IndSC is defined by the male gender role and that RelSC
form of self-construal (termed the Relational-Interdependent is defined by the female gender role. This confounding of cul-
Self-construal Scale; Cross et al., 2000). This measure focuses tural gender stereotyping and self-construal impedes progress;
on the degree to which the individual defines the self in terms cultural stereotypes may vary widely, but definitions of IndSC,
of close relationships. A sample item is “My close relationships InterSC, and RelSC are anchored in a theoretical focus on the
are an important reflection of who I am.” Its explicit focus on relation of self to others. This theoretical focus on self–other
the individual’s self-definition distinguishes it from more gen- relations that defines self-construal is independent of a particular
eral measures of communal orientation (e.g., the measure by culture’s understanding of gender.

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 149

Measuring Collective–Interdependent Self-Construal (or group) interdependence, new measures such as this are
likely to be of increasing utility.
Although the RelSC is defined in terms of significant dyadic
relationships, the Coll-InterSC is defined in terms of significant
group memberships. Brewer and Chen (2007) demonstrated Between- and Within-Group Differences in
that although most existing measures of collectivism and of Self-Construal
interdependence assess both types of interdependence, relation- These measures have facilitated the explosion in research on
ally oriented items are more than twice as common. Brewer self-construal by allowing researchers explicitly to measure
and Chen argued that cross-cultural researchers need to clearly IndSC and InterSC and to test their relations to a range of other
delineate between these two types of interdependence, as they cognitive, affective, and behavioral variables (which we review
have very different implications and predict different outcomes. more thoroughly below). The development of these measures
For example, although past research has noted that men tend to has also allowed researchers to explore between- and within-
score lower than women on measures of interdependence (see group differences in self-construal. To the surprise of many,
Cross & Madson, 1997), Gabriel and Gardner (1999) demon- however, the results from such research tend to be inconsistent,
strated that distinguishing between RelSC and Coll-InterSC often showing that individuals from different countries do not
yields more nuanced findings: Men score lower than women in demonstrate the expected differences in self-construal. After
terms of RelSC but higher than women in terms of reviewing the literature, Matsumoto (1999) concluded that
collective–InterSC. North Americans do not have higher IndSCs than East Asians,
The measure used in Gabriel and Gardner (1999) study, who do not have higher InterSCs than North Americans. A
the Collective Interdependent Self-Construal Scale (CISC), later meta-analysis (Levine et al., 2003) also found that East
was, until recently, the only measure specifically designed to Asians do not have a higher InterSC than North Americans,
assess collective or group InterSC. Gabriel and Gardner cre- nor do East Asians have a higher InterSC than IndSC. Both
ated the CISC by modifying the RISC (Cross et al., 2000) to reviews find examples not only of an absence of difference
replace references to relationship partners with references to (e.g., North Americans and Japanese having equally high Ind-
group memberships (e.g., “When I think of myself, I often SCs; Krull et al., 1999; Sato & Cameron, 1999) but of theoreti-
think of groups I belong to as well” instead of “When I think cally incongruent differences, such as North Americans having
of myself, I often think of my close friends of family as well”). a higher InterSC than Japanese (Kleinknecht, Dinnel,
In a sample of college students, the CISC showed excellent Kleinknecht, Hiruma, & Hirada, 1997; Sato & Cameron, 1999;
interitem reliability (α = .90), was uncorrelated with scores for a review, see Oyserman, Coon, & Kemmelmeier, 2002).
on the RISC (r = .07), and was correlated with a measure of More recent research continues to find such anomalies using
masculinity (r = .25). these measures (Kitayama et al., 2009). Matsumoto concludes
More recently, Harb and Smith (2008) created the Six-Fold that the theory of self-construal itself is fundamentally flawed:
Self-Construal Scale that integrates Brewer’s work on the Culture does not reliably predict self-construal in the ways
personal, relational, and group selves (Brewer & Chen, 2007; predicted by theory.
Brewer & Gardner, 1996) with Singelis’s work on vertical Other authors have been less quick to attribute the mixed
and horizontal IND-COL (Singelis, Triandis, Bhawuk, & results in the literature to flawed theory without first considering
Gelfand, 1995). The scale assesses the horizontal and vertical other possibilities. One of these is that scores on self-construal
collective self-construal as well as the horizontal and vertical measures are affected by situational priming, such that uncon-
RelSC, the personal self-construal, and a humanity-bound trolled aspects of the research environment inadvertently acti-
self-construal. Each of the six dimensions comprises five items vate one or the other self-construal (Levine et al., 2003). Across
(e.g., “I feel I have a strong relationship with _______”) that three studies using the Leung and Kim (1997) scales, however,
are rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale. The horizontal collec- there was no evidence that situational priming affected either
tive dimension referred to “students at my university,” whereas InterSC or InterSC scores among North Americans.
the vertical collective dimension refers to “my social group- Levine et al. (2003) interpreted these results as supporting
ing.” In samples of college students from the United Kingdom, another explanation for the mixed results in the literature: that
Lebanon, Syria, and Jordan, reliabilities on all six dimensions measures of self-construal are fundamentally flawed. These
were adequate to good, ranging from .68 on the personal self authors argued that because the InterSC is defined as a situation-
dimension (Jordan) to .92 on the vertical collective dimension ally determined, and therefore variable, sense of self, interde-
(the United Kingdom). Most reliabilities were in the mid-.80s. pendence scale scores should be sensitive to priming. In failing
As might be expected, the Arab samples tended to score higher to find such priming effects, they concluded that the InterSC
than the U.K. sample on the collective self-construals, whereas scale lacks construct validity. Other authors, however, have
the U.K. sample tended to score higher than the other samples argued that although we would expect a high InterSC indi-
on the RelSCs. In light of Brewer and Chen’s (2007) call for vidual’s sense of self and concomitant cognitions, emotions,
researchers to distinguish between the relational and collective or behaviors to vary based on the situation, we would not expect

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


150 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

his or her fundamental level of InterSC to be variable (Gudykunst self-construal, despite demonstrating unexpected patterns on
& Lee, 2003; M.-S. Kim & Raja, 2003). Thus, these authors the explicit measure (Singelis’s, 1994, SCS).
contend, the failure of priming to induce changes in self-construal One other methodological issue also accounts for the con-
scale scores is actually evidence for the construct validity of flicting data on between- and within-group differences in self-
these scales. construal: problems with the content of existing self-report
Other contextual factors may, in fact, explain the failure to measures (Noguchi, 2007). Hardin et al. (2004) noted that
find predicted between-group differences, however. Heine, the items on Singelis’s (1994) SCS do not capture several
Lehman, Peng, and Greenholtz (2002) demonstrated that so- important aspects of IndSC (e.g., the importance of one’s
called reference group effects inherent in the Likert-type scales private thoughts and feelings or the role of others in social
used by common self-report measures of self-construal can comparison) or InterSC (e.g., a preference for communicating
account for much of the data. Just as a 5’9” man would rate indirectly). Noguchi argued more specifically that (a) existing
himself somewhat tall if he lived in a culture where the average measures fail to adequately assess the extent to which indi-
male height was 5’8” but short if in a culture where the average viduals rely on and are sensitive to external versus internal
height was 6’, a moderately interdependent individual might information and (b) this is precisely the domain in which cul-
rate himself or herself quite differently in terms of interde- tural differences should be most likely to emerge. Thus,
pendence depending on his or her reference group. If North researchers have failed to find expected cross-cultural differ-
Americans use other (presumably highly independent, on aver- ences in self-construal because they have failed to measure
age) North Americans as their reference group, they will have specific aspects of self-construal most likely to differ cross-
a lower IndSC score than if they use Japanese as their referent culturally. To test this hypothesis, Noguchi constructed a new
group, who are presumably perceived to be less independent. measure with items specifically designed to tap into the extent
Heine et al. (2002) argued that because measures of self- to which one considers others’ viewpoints (i.e., the extent to
construal do not specify a reference group, participants are which one relies on external versus internal information).
likely to use their own cultural group as their referent. Indeed, Across several studies, Noguchi found that Japanese students
across two studies with bicultural Canadian and Japanese par- did score higher on this other-focused factor whereas American
ticipants, such reference group effects were found in scores on students scored higher on a self-focused factor. Interestingly,
Singelis’s (1994) SCS. When participants were given no explicit Americans consistently scored higher on a helping-others
instructions regarding reference groups (i.e., the standard self- factor, providing further evidence for the importance of both
construal scale instructions were used), expected between-group the specific content of items on self-construal scales and mea-
differences were weak or absent. When participants were explic- suring self-construal multidimensionally.
itly asked to use their own group as a reference, differences
opposite to that predicted by theory were found: Canadians had
significantly higher InterSC scores than the Japanese. However, Manipulations of Self-Construal
when participants were explicitly asked to use the other group The development of priming manipulations took research on
as their reference, expected between-group differences were self-construal into a new era. This development allows research-
found: Bicultural Canadians comparing themselves to “most ers to move from reliance on culture as a proxy for self-con-
Japanese” perceived themselves as significantly more indepen- strual or on explicit self-report measures to experimental
dent and less interdependent than bicultural Japanese who were manipulations of these constructs. As a result, researchers can
comparing themselves to “most North Americans,” which is more confidently examine causal hypotheses and within-
exactly what self-construal theory would predict. culture consequences of activation of the three different com-
Furthermore, because individuals within the same country ponents of self-construal (Oyserman & Lee, 2008). The premise
are likely to share a common cultural standard of reference, in this work is that all persons, no matter their cultural back-
reference-group effects should be attenuated or absent when ground, construct independent, relational, and collective–inter-
self-construal scale scores are compared for different ethnic dependent self-construals. Cultural practices and affordances,
groups within the same country (e.g., Asian Americans com- however, result in variability in the elaboration and accessibil-
pared to European Americans), allowing expected between- ity of independent, relational, or interdependent self-knowl-
group differences more readily to emerge (Heine et al., 2002). edge. Priming techniques can make the self-knowledge
Indeed, Levine et al. (2003) noted that most studies that use associated with one or another of these self-construals tem-
multiethnic samples within the same country yielded results porarily accessible; thus, priming can be used to experimen-
consistent with the predictions of self-construal theory. Finally, tally examine the effects of IndSC, RelSC, and InterSC on
we would expect such reference-group effects to affect explicit, behavior. In this section, we describe the primary ways that
self-report measures of self-construal, but not more implicit self-construal has been manipulated and some of the issues
measures. Indeed, Kitayama et al. (2009) found that North surrounding these manipulations.
Americans, Western Europeans, and Japanese differed in the The first studies to manipulate IndSC and InterSC were
expected directions on all five of their implicit measures of conducted by Trafimow, Triandis, and Goto (1991). They

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 151

examined the effects of two different manipulations with U.S. These differences were mediated by differences in TST responses;
college students. In the first technique, participants are asked participants primed with InterSC tended to describe themselves
to think about what makes them different from their family in terms of their relationships and group memberships more than
and friends (IndSC prime) or what makes them similar to their did the IndSC-primed participants; TST responses in turn pre-
family and friends (InterSC prime; we refer to this as the simi- dicted responses to the values and helping measures.
lar/different prime technique). In the second technique, par- Until recently, it was unclear whether these common
ticipants read a story about a ruler selecting a general to send InterSC priming techniques made RelSC or group-oriented,
to war based on either individualistic concerns (how it would Coll-InterSC accessible (or perhaps a combination of the two;
increase the ruler’s status, prestige) or collective concerns Oyserman & Lee, 2008). Brewer and Gardner (1996) attempted
(because he was a member of the ruler’s family). They exam- to tease apart these two dimensions in studies that contrasted
ined the effects of these manipulations on open-ended responses a large group collective to the small group implied in the trip
to the “Who am I?” test (Kuhn & McPartland, 1954). The to the city task, but they were only partly successful. In their
proportions of two kinds of statements were analyzed: private meta-analysis of IND-COL priming manipulations, Oyserman
or idiocentric self-views (deriving from the IndSC) and col- and Lee (2008) categorize the original Brewer and Gardner
lective self-views (deriving from the InterSC). A third cat- (1996) pronoun circling task as a relational collectivism
egory, which they termed allocentric responses, referred to manipulation, but they categorize the Sumerian ruler story
aspects of “interdependence, friendship, responsiveness to manipulation as a mixture of relational and group-level collectiv-
others, and sensitivity to the viewpoints of others” (p. 650). ism. Unfortunately, they provide little explanation for the basis
Because these relational responses were not relevant to their of these decisions, and little empirical evidence is available to
hypotheses, they were eliminated from the analyses. As guide such decisions. In short, there has been much ambiguity
expected, participants exposed to the IndSC primes described remaining about which domain of the InterSC is primed by these
themselves using more individual, private terms than did those commonly used primes—the RelSC or the Coll-InterSC.
exposed to the InterSC primes, whereas those exposed to the Recently, however, Stapel and van der Zee (2006) modified
InterSC primes reported more collective and group-oriented the Brewer and Gardner (1996) manipulation to make these
responses than did those exposed to the IndSC primes (also see distinctions more clear. For RelSC, they modified the paragraph
Ybarra & Trafimow, 1998, Study 3). Examination of a small about the trip to the city to start with sentences that clearly focus
group of Asian Americans in their sample revealed a very on a dyad (“I am with you. We are together, the two of us.”).
similar pattern of responses to the primes. For the Coll-InterSC, they began the paragraph with sentences
Unfortunately, these studies did not include a neutral or that focus on being part of a group (“We are a group of people.”).
no-prime control condition, which would help disentangle the Manipulation checks documented that the IndSC, RelSC, and
direction of the effects in these studies. Given that one would Coll-InterSC manipulations had the intended effects on the
expect European American participants to describe themselves degree to which participants valued being an independent,
using primarily personal, individual descriptors, it is unclear unique person, being in close relationships, or being part of
from these results whether the proportions of the two types of a group. Using this new method, researchers will be better able
self-statements were increased or decreased by the manipula- to develop and test theories that distinguish the role and func-
tions relative to a control condition. tion of RelSC and Coll-InterSC in behavior.
A few years later, Brewer and Gardner (1996) and Gardner, Several other priming tasks have also been used successfully
Gabriel, and Lee (1999) introduced a third approach to manipu- in other studies. For example, Utz (2004) asked participants to
lating self-construal. In this technique, participants read a story write about a typical day in the life of a student from a first-
about going on a trip to the city and circle either singular pronouns person singular perspective or from a third-person singular
(I, me, mine; IndSC prime) or plural pronouns (we, our, us; perspective. She found this manipulation interacted with chronic
InterSC prime). Control conditions included a similar task in social value orientation to predict cooperative behavior. Stapel
which third-person pronouns (they, them) or impersonal pronouns and Koomen (2001) primed IndSC and InterSC by having par-
(it) were circled. The first series of studies by Brewer and Gardner ticipants write a story about themselves, describing either “Who
(1996) focused on aspects of the interdependent (we) self and I am” using the words I, me, myself, and mine in each sentence
compared the effects of the we circling task to the control tasks. or “Who we are” using the words we, our, ourselves, and ours
They hypothesized that when the InterSC (we-us) is primed, (see Wang and Ross, 2005, for a similar task). Others have used
others are included in the self, resulting in an increased perception a scrambled sentence task with words such as I, me, mine,
of similarity to others. Using a reaction time task, Brewer and unique, different, and independence to prime IndSC and we,
Gardner found that we primed participants made judgments of our, us, cooperative, share, and similar to prime InterSC (Briley
similarity more quickly than those in the they prime condition. & Wyer, 2001; Kühnen & Hannover, 2000; Utz, 2004).
A subsequent study revealed that InterSC-primed participants Several cautions are in order for the researcher interested
endorsed collectivist values and obligations to help more than in priming self-construal. First, the degree to which manipula-
did IndSC-primed participants (Gardner et al., 1999). tions prime the self-construal versus other dimensions of

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


152 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

culture or of individualism and collectivism (IND-COL) is or obvious priming manipulations. When participants are
often unclear. For example, some studies use bilingual par- exposed to a very explicit priming task (Moskowitz & Roman,
ticipants and manipulate the language used in the study (Kem- 1992) or when they are reminded of a previous task meant
melmeier & Cheng, 2004; Ross, Xun, & Wilson, 2002; to prime a construct (Strack, Schwarz, Bless, Kubler, & Wanke,
Trafimow, Silverman, Fan, & Law, 1997) or manipulate cul- 1993), their judgments contrast with the direction of the expected
tural icons (Y.-Y. Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martinez, prime (termed a correction contrast). To examine this effect
2000). These approaches likely activate the associated self- in the self-construal domain, Kühnen and Hannover created
construal (e.g., InterSC when Chinese language or icons are a scrambled sentence task in which the four-word sentences
used) but also likely activate many other dimensions of IND- focused on either IndSC (“I like being unique”) or InterSC
COL that are unrelated to self-construal. For example, Oyser- (“I support my team”). Each scrambled sentence also included
man and Lee (2008) conclude that languages vary in the degree one additional word that was related to the type of prime—
to which they are focused on abstract versus concrete ways of either a word reflecting independence (assertive) or a word
thinking, which could in turn influence a variety of cognitive reflecting interdependence (help). The authors varied the
tasks. In addition, some manipulations (e.g., language or cul- salience of the IndSC or InterSC prime by having participants
tural icons) likely activate other aspects of a specific culture write down either the unscrambled sentence (the “overt” prime)
that are unrelated to self-construal. Because of these concerns, or the unnecessary word (the “subtle” prime). When participants
studies that manipulate the language used by participants as were exposed to the subtle primes, the IndSC prime led to less
a proxy for variation in self-construal have been excluded perceived similarity to a target person than the InterSC prime,
from our review of the literature. More importantly, research- as would be expected. But the opposite occurred when they
ers should be cognizant of the many ways that people from were primed with the overt IndSC prime: Participants viewed
different cultures differ from each other and should not pre- themselves as more similar to the target person than when
sume that priming different self-construals in members of primed with the InterSC prime. The effects in the explicit prime
one cultural group is equivalent to using members of different condition may be a consequence of the correction effect sug-
cultural groups. It is a gross understatement to say that observ- gested by others (Strack et al., 1993), or it may be because of
ing the behavior of an American student who has been exposed a rebalancing of the need for uniqueness and relatedness, as
to one of the InterSC tasks described above is not the same suggested by Brewer’s (1991) optimal distinctiveness theory.
as observing a Japanese or Chinese student. If, however, self- Whatever the explanation, this research provides a note of
construal is the mechanism hypothesized to explain cultural caution to researchers that very strong, explicit manipulations
differences in behavior, this approach would shed light on of self-construal may have unintended consequences.
this causal process in ways that measures do not. Finally, researchers should ask themselves, “How might
Which of the manipulations described above is most effec- chronic individual differences interact with priming manipula-
tive in producing differences in self-construal? A meta-analysis tions to affect outcomes?” Relatively little research has focused
of the consequences of priming manipulations by Oyserman on the interaction of individual differences and priming manipu-
and Lee (2008) helps answer this question. They examined the lations within cultural groups (but see Utz, 2004). Gardner and
effects of priming tasks on self-concept-related outcomes (e.g., her colleagues (1999) found that priming the culturally nondomi-
the TST and responses to the Singelis SCS measure). Their nant dimension of self-construal (e.g., InterSC for European
results show that the strongest effects were found using the Americans and IndSC for East Asians) resulted in greater dif-
similar/different task (d = .44, n = 4), followed by the Sumerian ferences relative to a no-prime condition than priming the
warrior task (d = .37, n = 6). Other manipulations (e.g., the culturally dominant self-construal. The work by Gardner et al.
pronoun circling task) had smaller effects (d < .29) on self- (1999) also points to our final point about priming manipula-
concept-related outcome measures (also see Lalwani & Shavitt, tions: Relatively few studies have used these priming techniques
2009, for a comparison of the effects of different manipulations with non-Western samples. The Oyserman and Lee (2008) meta-
on responses to the TST). Oyserman and Lee’s meta-analysis analysis of IND-COL priming studies compared the effects of
also revealed that the different priming approaches differen- these oft-used priming techniques for participants from Europe
tially affected other types of behavior. For example, the similar/ or European American backgrounds and Asian backgrounds.
different task had the strongest effects on relationality tasks Very few studies have used Asian samples (or samples from
(e.g., liking for another participant), and the pronoun circling Africa or Latin America, for that matter), but Oyserman and
task had the strongest effects on cognitive tasks (e.g., perfor- Lee’s results indicate that the effects of the Sumerian Warrior
mance on the Embedded Figures task). This suggests that the and the similar/different priming tasks are similar for East Asians
different priming techniques may activate somewhat different and European Americans for most tasks (see their Table 6).
cognitive processes and that researchers should choose their More studies are needed to assess the effectiveness of the pronoun
approaches carefully in light of the outcomes in question. circling task for Asian samples (see, e.g., Oyserman, Sorenson,
Priming is sometimes a delicate task, and Kühnen and Reber, & Chen, 2009). As Kitayama, Duffy, and Uchida (2007)
Hannover (2000) caution against the use of very transparent suggest, if the self of East Asians is understood to be

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 153

chronically intertwined with others, priming I may activate (particularly in Western societies). Figure 1 depicts the ways that
we as well, resulting in weaker priming effects among East IndSC, Coll-InterSC, and RelSC are thought to influence cogni-
Asians than has been observed among Westerners. tion, affect, motivation, and, through them, social behavior.
Although we strive to distinguish Coll-InterSC and RelSC in the
figure, many research findings cannot be easily categorized into
Summary and future directions for measures and one or the other of these components of InterSC. In those cases,
manipulations the figure includes consequences of InterSC with Coll-InterSC.
The methods of measuring or manipulating IndSC, RelSC, and In this section, we review research in each of these domains in
Coll-InterSC are continuing to evolve. New measurement which self-construal is measured or manipulated using the most
approaches are focusing on clarifying specific facets of each of common approaches described above.
these self-views (Hardin et al., 2009) and on developing implicit
measures of IndSC and InterSC (Kitayama et al., 2009). Implicit
measures of RelSC are still lacking, however. One starting place Self-Construal Influences on Cognition
for the development of an implicit measure of RelSC is the indi- Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that individuals with high
vidual versus relational version of the IAT used by Cross, Morris, InterSC should be especially likely to pay attention to others and
and Gore (2002, Study 1). Undoubtedly, however, creating an the social context of interaction, resulting in very elaborate cogni-
implicit measure valid for multiple cultures is a daunting task. tive representations of others and self-representations that include
Several new manipulations of self-construal have been social contexts. Since their formulation, researchers have focused
reported in the past decade, but researchers frequently fail to on two primary ways that variation in self-construal affects cogni-
include control conditions. Researchers seeking to use these tion: differences in attention to the context or relationships and
manipulations should attempt to specifically define the dimen- different information processing styles. In the following sections,
sion of self-construal of interest and think carefully about the we review the existing research on each of these processes,
implied focus of the priming manipulation (e.g., relational or describing their influences on self-views, social cognition, and
collective–interdependent self-construal). The newly developed nonsocial cognition.
manipulations that differentiate these two dimensions will
enhance researchers’ ability to untangle their effects on behavior. Context sensitivity versus insensitivity and self-descriptions. Much
Finally, the great majority of these approaches have been devel- of the early research that followed the initial publication of
oped in the West by Western researchers with Western assump- Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) and Triandis’s (1989) articles
tions about the person. Although cultural psychology has begun focused on examining the hypothesis that persons with high
to shine a light on these assumptions and biases, many of these InterSC were more likely to describe themselves in terms of
methods may nonetheless reflect cultural assumptions about their social contexts, including close relationships, group
the person, about ways of thinking and knowing, and about memberships, and social identities. As Markus and Kitayama
relationships. Focused attempts to consider these constructs hypothesized, people from collectivist cultures report more
from non-Western perspectives and to devise methods to assess social, collective, or group-oriented self-descriptions than do
or manipulate them that are free from Western biases are the people from individualistic cultures (Bond & Cheung, 1983;
next steps in exploring how variation in self-construal influences Cousins, 1989; Kanagawa, Cross, & Markus, 2001; Rhee,
behavior. In the next section, we review research that has exam- Uleman, Lee, & Roman, 1995; Shweder & Bourne, 1984). In
ined the role of self-construal in cognition, emotion, motivation, addition, manipulations of IndSC and InterSC generally result
and social interaction. in the corresponding differences in self-descriptions on the
TST (Oyserman & Lee, 2008).
Perhaps more importantly, IndSC and InterSC are hypoth-
How Do Self-Construals esized to influence the variability of self-descriptions across
Shape Behavior? situations. If people with high InterSC are sensitive to situ-
For more than three decades, researchers have demonstrated the ational or relational context, then they should tend to describe
central importance of the self in information processing, affect themselves differently in different situations. Evidence to sup-
regulation, motivation, and social behavior (for reviews, see Bau- port this hypothesis is mixed. Cross-cultural studies that use
meister, 1998; Leary & Tangney, 2003; Markus & Wurf, 1987). country as a proxy for self-construal do find that East Asian
Most of this work, however, was based on Western samples and participants’ self-descriptions depend on situational context
assumed an IndSC. If the self is defined interdependently, many more than did those of American participants (Cousins, 1989;
of these self-related processes may vary. Although Markus and Kanagawa et al., 2001; Rhee et al., 1995; Suh, 2002).
Kitayama (1991) and Triandis (1989) focused on the role of dif- The existing evidence, however, does not clearly support
fering views of the self in explaining cultural differences in behav- the association between self-concept variability across situa-
ior, researchers soon found that variation in self-construal was tions and measures of self-construal. The clearest support comes
also a useful tool for investigating within-culture processes from a study in which self-construal was manipulated using

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


154 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

COGNITIVE CONSEQUENCES

• Low context sensitivity


• Differentiation, separation, and
contrast

AFFECTIVE CONSEQUENCES
INTERPERSONAL BEHAVIOR
• Prefer socially disengaging
emotions • Self promoting
INDEPENDENT • Well-being based on emotional • Prefer direct communication
SELF-CONSTRUAL experiences and high self-esteem • Willing to confront or use dominating
• Self-consistency promotes well- strategies
being

MOTIVATIONAL CONSEQUENCES

• Individualistic values
• Self-enhancement
• Promotion focused
• Value primary control strategies

COGNITIVE CONSEQUENCES

• High context-sensitivity
• Connection and assimilation

AFFECTIVE CONSEQUENCES SOCIAL BEHAVIOR


• Group oriented
• Well-being based on adherence to
COLLECTIVE- • Prefer indirect
social norms and harmony with
INTERDEPENDENT others communication
• Avoid confrontation; use
SELF-CONSTRUAL • Prefer socially engaging emotions
cooperative strategies in
interactions
• Likely to imitate and seek
MOTIVATIONAL CONSEQUENCES proximity to others

• Value group harmony


• Self-criticism
• Prevention focused
• Value secondary control strategies

COGNITIVE CONSEQUENCES

• High relationship sensitivity


• Assimilation to others and focus on
similarity with close others

SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
AFFECTIVE CONSEQUENCES
• Relationship-promoting
RELATIONAL SELF- • Well-being based on quality of close • Adjust to partner
CONSTRUAL relationships • Focus on joint gains for relationship

MOTIVATIONAL CONSEQUENCES

• Value warm relationships


• Other-enhancement
• Pursue relational goals

Figure 1. Components of self-construal and their influence on cognition, affect, motivation, and social behavior
Note: Research findings that cannot easily be categorized as relational–interdependent or collective–interdependent were included with collective–
interdependent self-construal.

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 155

the similar/different manipulation. Participants described them- Given that InterSC is associated with context-sensitive
selves as more context dependent in the InterSC priming condi- cognition, how might it affect memory? In one study, both
tion than did participants in the IndSC priming condition European American and Asian or Asian American participants
(Kühnen, Hannover, & Schubert, 2001). Studies that measure were primed with IndSC or InterSC and asked to recall their
self-construal are less clear, however. For example, a multi- earliest memories (Wang & Ross, 2005). The IndSC-primed
country study revealed that self-concept consistency was posi- participants tended to describe more individual-focused mem-
tively related to IndSC but not to InterSC (Church et al., 2008). ories, whereas the InterSC-primed participants tended to
Another multicountry study discovered that RelSC predicted describe more group- focused memories and memories that
contextualized self-description differently for Japanese and focused on social interaction.
Korean participants and was not associated with contextualized If people with high InterSC are especially likely to attend
self-description for participants with European backgrounds to the context when thinking about themselves and others, then
(Y. Kashima et al., 2004). In three studies reported by Cross, they should also tend to consider situational factors when mak-
Gore, and Morris (2003), RelSC (measured by the Cross et al., ing attributions for others’ behavior. In fact, cultural variation
2000, RISC scale) generally was not associated with self- in situational versus dispositional attributions was the focus
concept variability across important relationships (rs ranged of some of the earliest research by cultural social psychologists
from .04 to .14; also see Locke & Christensen, 2007). (e.g., Miller, 1984; Morris & Peng, 1994). In general, these
Although recent studies have continued to explore cultural studies revealed that East Asian participants are more likely
differences in self-concept stability within and across relation- to attribute others’ behavior to situations than are European
ship contexts (see English & Chen, 2007), there is little evidence Americans. Singelis (1994), in his original publication describ-
of self-construal as a mediator of these cultural differences. In ing the SCS, reported that measures on the InterSC subscale
part, this may be because of the methods used to assess variation were positively associated with situational attributions, control-
in the self-concept across situations. Most often, these methods ling for participants’ cultural background. Surprisingly, how-
request the participants to describe themselves in different situ- ever, other researchers have seldom attempted to link attribution
ations (e.g., at home, at school, or with friends) or in different processes to differences in self-construal. One exception is the
relationships (e.g., with family, with specific friends). Partici- work by Oishi, Wyer, and Colcombe (2000), who found that
pants from Western cultural backgrounds may feel a press to subliminal priming with interdependence-related concepts led
describe themselves consistently across contexts. The Kühnen to lower levels of dispositional attributions for another person’s
et al. (2001) study described above, which manipulated self- negative behaviors compared to priming with independence-
construal, assessed context-sensitive self-concept with three related concepts (but see Krull et al., 1999).
items from the SCS (e.g., “I act the same no matter who I am Finally, if persons with a high InterSC pay attention to and
with”). Studies that use experience sampling methods, in which remember information about social contexts, then they should
participants describe themselves using handheld computers have a rich and easily accessible store of interpersonal knowl-
when alerted, may more accurately tap the degree to which edge (as suggested by Markus & Kitayama, 1991). For exam-
self-construal predicts the extent to which people think about ple, Markus and Kitayama suggest that the asymmetric results
themselves differently in different situations. of self-to-other versus other-to-self comparisons (as originally
shown in Holyoak & Gordon, 1983) are because of IndSC, in
Context-sensitive social cognition. A variety of social-cognitive which unique aspects of the self are privileged over other
tasks have also been linked to variation in self-construal. In information. They provided evidence that this asymmetry was
studies in which self-construals were primed, Haberstroh, absent among Japanese participants, presumably because of
Oyserman, Schwarz, Kühnen, and Ji (2002) showed that the greater knowledge of others accessible in the InterSC.
InterSC-primed Western participants were more likely than Kühnen and Haberstroh (2004) tested this hypothesis in a
IndSC-primed participants to take a target person’s prior knowl- study in which self-construal was manipulated using the pro-
edge into account and avoided providing redundant informa- noun search task (Gardner et al., 1999). They found the typical
tion. Similarly, InterSC-primed participants who prepared to asymmetry among German participants in the IndSC-prime
engage in a debate with an opponent listed more of the oppo- condition but no asymmetry in the InterSC-prime condition.
nent’s possible arguments in the debate and indicated a greater
willingness to listen to the opponent’s position compared to Relational self-construal and relationship sensitivity. In research
IndSC-primed participants (Gardner & Le, 2000, reported in that focused specifically on RelSC, Cross and her colleagues
Gardner & Seeley, 2001). People whose InterSC is chronically (2002) argued that if the RelSC is a central framework for
activated (or primed in an experiment) tend to give more defining the self, then it should influence information process-
weight to others’ views or subjective norms about their goals ing without conscious control. In a series of studies using
and behaviors than do people with high IndSC, whose per- North American participants, they examined the role of RelSC
sonal attitudes are more likely to direct behavior (Torelli, 2006; in a variety of implicit cognitive processes that centered on
Trafimow & Finlay, 1996; Ybarra & Trafimow, 1998). relationship-oriented material. For example, they found that

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


156 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

participants who scored high on the RISC scale remembered prime (i.e., a task that requires one to ignore the context per-
more relationship-related information about a target person formed by a IndSC-primed participant), performance on the
(Study 3) and organized information about others in terms of task was enhanced among both Western and East Asian par-
their relationships (Study 4). In addition, persons with high ticipants (Oyserman et al., 2009).
RelSC were more likely to respond positively to relationship- Finally, considerable research has demonstrated significant
oriented terms in an IAT task (Greenwald, McGee, & Schwartz, cultural differences in attention to the context versus attention
1998) and were more likely to have dense associative networks to the focal objects in a display (Masuda & Nisbett, 2001;
for relationship–oriented terms, compared to lows on their Morris & Peng, 1994; Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan,
RISC scale (Study 2). In short, persons with chronically high 2001). Can these differences be accounted for by differences
RelSC are “tuned” to pay attention to and organize their worlds in self-construal? This question was addressed in an innovative
in terms of relationships. study that examined neural responses to target objects or to
When others are connected to the self and viewed as self- novel contextual stimuli among European American and Asian
defining, a person will tend to consider the other person’s American participants (Lewis, Goto, & Kong, 2008). The
interests and take the other’s perspective in social interaction Event Related Potentials (ERP) data revealed the expected
and decision making. Cross and her colleagues (2000) found cultural differences in attention to the focal object or the con-
that students with high RelSC were more likely to consider text; this, in turn, was mediated by scores on the Singelis
the needs and wishes of friends and family members when (1994) InterSC scale. In another study, priming IndSC and
making decisions about how to spend their summer than were InterSC in Chinese participants resulted in neural activity that
low RelSC students. Likewise, Gore and Cross (2006) found reflected cultural differences in cognition. Z. Lin, Lin, and
that North Americans with high RelSC tended to include other Han (2008) found that priming IndSC resulted in increased
people in their rationale for pursuing important goals. Among focus on local targets in a compound letter task (i.e., the small
new college roommates, those with high RelSC were better letters that make up larger letters), whereas an InterSC prime
able to predict their roommate’s responses to a set of questions resulted in increased focus on the global target (i.e., the larger
about values and beliefs than were low relationals, indicating letter), as evidenced by neural activity in the extrastriate cor-
better memory for the roommate’s revelations about themselves tex (which is related to visual perceptual processing; also see
(Cross & Morris, 2003). Thus, RelSC is associated with atten- Z. Lin & Han, 2009). These and other recent studies of neural
tion to, consideration of, and memory for relational contexts, activity (e.g., Hedden, Ketay, Aron, Markus, & Gabrieli, 2008)
especially important or close relationships. provide provocative evidence that self-construal underlies
important cultural differences in cognition.
Context-sensitive nonsocial cognition. Researchers have paid sub-
stantial attention to the roles of self-construal and context- Connecting and assimilating versus differentiating and contrasting.
sensitivity for nonsocial events. Kühnen et al. (2001) found Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that people with a high
when IndSC was primed, German and American participants IndSC will strive to differentiate themselves from others, whereas
were quicker to find geometric figures that had been drawn people with a high InterSC will seek to connect to others. Since
within more complex geometric designs (the Embedded Figures then, considerable research has focused on this effect for the
Test; Witkin & Berry 1969). In contrast, InterSC-primed par- self and social judgments; research on nonsocial judgments
ticipants performed better than IndSC-primed participants on is just beginning to emerge.
a task that was especially sensitive to context-dependent think-
ing (also see Konrath, Bushman, & Grove, 2009). Others have Connecting versus differentiating in self- and social judgments.
demonstrated similar effects among Western participants using Social comparison processes have been thoroughly explored
a modified Stroop task and a cognitive switching task (Han- in social psychology research, but it entered a new era with
nover, Pohlman, & Springer, 2005). Likewise, InterSC-primed the finding that self-construal moderated social comparisons
North American participants were more sensitive to contextual processes. As Stapel and Koomen (2001) demonstrated, when
information presented in a causal reasoning induction task than the IndSC is activated, contrast or differentiation from the
were IndSC-primed participants (K. Kim, Grimm, & Markman, comparison target occurs. When the InterSC is activated,
2007; also see Haberstroh et al., 2002). people tend to assimilate or view themselves as similar to the
Much of the early work linking self-construal to context- comparison target (also see Kemmelmeier & Oyserman, 2001;
dependent thinking was limited to Western European or North Kühnen & Haberstroh, 2004; Kühnen & Hannover, 2000;
American participants. More recently, Oyserman and her col- Stapel & van der Zee, 2006)
leagues showed that priming the InterSC promotes better Behavioral evidence for assimilation versus contrast effects
memory for the location of objects in an array among both of self-construal on behavior was demonstrated in a study of
European American participants (Kühnen & Oyserman, 2002) the effects of priming a negatively stereotyped group on test
and East Asian participants (Oyserman et al., 2009). Further- performance (Bry, Follenfant, & Meyer, 2008). When exposed
more, when the demands of a task matched the self-construal to the stereotype of dumb blondes, French students performed

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 157

worse on a general knowledge test if the InterSC was salient processes has understandably focused on self and social judg-
than if it was not (and worse than participants in a control ments, but work exploring the effects of self-construal on non-
condition), suggesting that the InterSC-primed participants social cognitive processes is expanding and suggests promising
had connected themselves with the dumb blonde stereotype. avenues for new research. For example, spatial judgments
A contrast effect among IndSC-primed participants was dem- (Krishna, Zhou, & Zhang, 2008) and aesthetic preferences
onstrated in Study 2: Participants exposed to the dumb blonde (Zhang, Feick, & Price, 2006) are influenced by self-construal
stereotype performed better than participants in the control priming. As suggested above, researchers interested in learning
condition when the IndSC was made salient (for related styles, creativity, and other dimensions of education may find
research, see Blanton & Stapel, 2008; Keller & Molix, 2008; consideration of self-construals useful in their work.
Sui & Han, 2007). Some processes linked to the self-construal by Markus and
Cross and her colleagues (2002) hypothesized that persons Kitayama (1991), however, have not received much attention
with high RelSC will also strive for connections with relation- by self-construal researchers. For example, they suggest that
ship partners and so will tend to see similarities between them- differences in self-construal may be the source of observed
selves and close others. North American participants rated the cultural differences in counterfactual thinking and categoriza-
descriptiveness of multiple traits, values, and abilities for them- tion. To our knowledge, researchers have not (successfully)
selves and a same-sex friend. An indirect measure of similarity investigated these hypotheses with measures or manipulations
was calculated by computing intraclass correlations for each of self-construal (but see interesting cross-cultural findings
person’s pair of ratings in each domain (traits, values, and for categorization by Ji, Zhang, & Nisbett, 2004). Furthermore,
abilities). In regression analyses that controlled for self-esteem, although scant research exists on the relations between self-
RISC scale scores significantly predicted each type of simi- construal and attribution, these studies suggest that manipula-
larity. In short, both RelSC and InterSC promote ways of think- tions of self-construal may provide important insights into
ing that enhance connections, similarity or assimilation in social attribution-related processes.
comparison. IndSC promotes differentiation, contrast, and New evidence linking self-construal to variation in neural
cognitive separation from others. activity is exciting and provocative. Although examination of
neural activity cannot replace studies of cognitive behavior,
Connecting versus differentiating in nonsocial cognition. The they provide support for the contention that culture’s effects
tendency to engage in differentiation versus assimilation may on the individual are more than skin deep.
also apply to other domains of cognition. To date, however,
few people have investigated this hypothesis in nonsocial
domains. One study extended this line of reasoning to the Self-Construal Influences Affect
generation of novel ideas. When IndSC was primed, people Despite strong interest in cultural differences in emotion, little
tended to generate more unusual examples of categories (e.g., research has actually measured self-construal to investigate
clothing or furniture) compared to control conditions or directly how independence and interdependence relate to vari-
InterSC-priming conditions (Wiekens & Stapel, 2008). In ous affective consequences. The few studies that have directly
another study, Oyserman and her colleagues (2009) argued measured self-construal tend to find that, in a variety of sam-
that the completion of multiple-choice tests, such as GRE tests ples, IndSC is associated with greater subjective happiness
that require students to select the correct antonym for a word (Elliott & Coker, 2008) as well as decreased depression (e.g.,
or to complete analogies, requires a differentiation mind-set B. T. Lam, 2005; Okazaki, 1997; Sato & McCann, 1998),
(i.e., the ability to separate the correct answer from the rest). unhappiness (Y. Kim, Kasser, & Lee, 2003), general anxiety
They found that InterSC-primed participants performed worse (e.g., Hardin, Varghese, Tran, & Carlson, 2006; Y. Kim, Kasser,
on these tasks than IndSC-primed or control participants. & Lee, 2003; Xie, Leong, & Feng, 2008), and social anxiety
These studies suggest that many of the academic tasks valued (Hardin et al., 2006; J. J. Hong & Woody, 2007; Okazaki,
in Western cultural contexts, such as creativity, finding novel 1997; Xie et al., 2008), whereas InterSC is often associated
solutions to a problem, and completion of multiple-choice-type with increased levels of these negative affects (Hardin et al.,
tasks, are related to IndSC and that exploration of other 2006; Okazaki, 1997; Sato & McCann, 1998).
learning- and education-related topics may profit from con- Such results, of course, beg the question of why self-construal
sideration of the role of self-construal. is related to affect in these ways. Kitayama, Karasawa, and
Mesquita (2004) laid out a dual process model of emotion in
Summary and recommendations for future research. As Markus and which cultural presses to be either independent or interdepen-
Kitayama (1991) predicted, variation in self-construal is related dent result in distal and proximal cultural affordances that
to a variety of cognitive processes. In particular, researchers have encourage the development of culturally consistent affective
focused on the role of self-construal in context-dependent or – experience and expression. More specifically, they argue that
independent cognition and on cognitive tendencies to differentiate IndSC is related to a tendency to experience and express
or to connect information. Most of the research on cognitive socially disengaging emotions such as anger or pride, whereas

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


158 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

InterSC is related to a tendency to experience socially engag- although the correlation between self-construal and unhappi-
ing emotions, such as guilt or relatedness. Although not specifi- ness was statistically significant among the South Korean
cally mentioned by Kitayama et al., social anxiety would be sample (r = –.15) but not in the U.S. sample (r = –.10), the
another socially engaging emotion because it refers to feelings relative magnitude of the correlations was not compared sta-
of nervousness that occur in the context of social relationships. tistically to determine if the relation is actually stronger for
It is not surprising that individuals high in InterSC, for whom the South Koreans than for the Americans.
interpersonal relationships are central to definitions of the In addition to these relations being at least partly artifactual,
self, would express more concern about appropriate behavior it is likely that other variables mediate these relations. For
in social contexts. Interestingly, IndSC is often found to be a example, InterSC is also associated with maladaptive perfec-
better predictor of social anxiety than InterSC; this appears to tionism, probably because of the emphasis on being a good
be particularly true for European Americans as opposed to East group member; however, after controlling for maladaptive
Asians (Xie et al., 2008). perfectionism, InterSC no longer predicted depressive symp-
But what of the relation of self-construal to other types of toms (Yoon & Lau, 2008). Thus, it is not that being interde-
negative affect, such as unhappiness and depression? Kita- pendent is inherently depressing but rather that being
yama, Karasawa, and Mesquita (2004) explicitly identify interdependent is associated with other variables, some of
these emotions as neither engaging nor disengaging, and thus which may in turn be associated with negative affect. Just as
we might expect them to be unrelated to self-construal. It is independence may manifest in some individuals as an unhealthy
likely that the observed relations are largely artifactual, disconnection from others, interdependence may manifest in
explained by the high correlations between social anxiety some individuals as an unhealthy overdependence on others
and other types of negative affect. Okazaki (1997) demon- that leads to maladaptive perfectionism, among other things.
strated that once social anxiety is controlled, neither IndSC Perhaps more likely, existing measures of self-construal may
nor InterSC is related to depression. Furthermore, ethnic be incapable of adequately distinguishing independence from
group differences in depression disappeared when social anxi- social isolation and interdependence from dependence.
ety was controlled. Thus, the common finding that Asian Given the importance of interpersonal relationships to those
Americans self-report higher levels of depression than Euro- with an InterSC and the importance of internal, private self-
pean Americans (e.g., Aldwin & Greenberger, 1987; Kuo, evaluations to those with an IndSC, several researchers have
1984) is likely, at least in part, because of the fact that Asian argued—and demonstrated—that life satisfaction is predicted
Americans self-report higher levels of social anxiety than by different variables, depending on self-construal. Large
European Americans, which in turn is at least in part because multinational studies have found that life satisfaction is pre-
of the greater interdependence and lower independence com- dicted by emotions (an internal, private experience) in individu-
mon among Asian Americans (Okazaki, 1997). alistic cultures but by adherence to social norms in collectivist
Such failure to control for social anxiety may account for cultures (Schimmack, Radhakrishnan, Oishi, Dzokoto, & Ahadi,
other findings that, on the surface, appear to contradict predic- 2002; Suh, Diener, Oishi, & Triandis, 1998). However, because
tions of self-construal theory. For example, given that interde- neither of these studies directly measured self-construal, the
pendence is culturally consistent with collectivist values, we hypothesis that self-construal accounted for this difference
might expect InterSC to be associated with positive outcomes remained speculative until a recent study provided direct sup-
in collectivist cultures. To be sure, some evidence does support port (Suh, Diener, & Updegraff, 2008). Suh et al. showed that
this hypothesis: InterSC was associated with greater life sat- for individuals who were relatively higher in independence
isfaction among college students in Hong Kong but was unre- than interdependence, life satisfaction was predicted only by
lated to life satisfaction among college students in the United emotions (i.e., the balance of positive and negative affect) and
States (Kwan et al., 1997). Surprisingly, then, greater relative not by the extent to which the individuals felt significant others
InterSC predicted greater unhappiness and less happiness in would evaluate the participants’ lives positively. For those
South Koreans but was unrelated to unhappiness among stu- with relatively stronger InterSC, however, both these social
dents in the United States (Y. Kim, Kasset, & Lee, 2003). If appraisals and emotion predicted life satisfaction.
the measures of happiness and unhappiness used in this study Such findings are consistent with those of Kwan et al.
are highly correlated with social anxiety, the apparently incon- (1997), who found that in both Hong Kong and the United
sistent finding that InterSC predicts unhappiness in South Korea States relationship harmony fully mediates the relation between
may simply be an artifact of InterSC predicting social anxiety. interdependence and life satisfaction, whereas global self-
These results, however, must be interpreted with caution. First, esteem fully mediates the relation between independence and
InterSC scores were subtracted from IndSC scores to create a life satisfaction. In a multiethnic U.S. sample, self-esteem also
unidimensional self-construal score that reflected an individ- mediated the relation between independence and life satisfac-
ual’s relative level of interdependence. As discussed previously, tion (measured in terms of positive and negative affect),
such unidimensional scores are inconsistent with both theory although relationship harmony did not mediate the relation
and intended use of most self-construal scales. In addition, between interdependence and life satisfaction (Reid, 2004).

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 159

Similarly, B. T. Lam (2005) found that self-esteem fully medi- InterSC. One study that has done so examined the roles of
ates the relation between independence and depression in a self-construal and family cohesion in predicting mental distress
sample of Vietnamese American adolescents: Those with greater among Asian American high school students (Liu & Goto,
independence tended to have more positive self-appraisals, which 2007). The authors found that increasing interdependence was
resulted in fewer depressive symptoms. Contrary to past research associated more strongly with increasing distress among those
(e.g., Kwan et al., 1997; Okazaki, 1997; Sato & McCann, 1998), adolescents low in independence; although increasing inter-
however, interdependence was also associated with greater global dependence was also associated with increasing distress for
self-esteem and less depression and self-esteem also fully medi- those high in independence, the relationship was weaker. Impor-
ated the relation between interdependence and depression. This tantly, family cohesion mediated this effect, with adolescents
is likely because of the more bicultural nature of Lam’s adoles- high in both independence and interdependence reporting more
cent sample. Unlike the other samples, which used college stu- family cohesion than adolescents high in interdependence but
dents (Kwan et al., 1997; Okazaki, 1997; Sato & McCann, 1998), low in independence. For these latter adolescents low in inde-
Lam’s participants were adolescents in junior high and high pendence, increasing interdependence was actually associated
school who were still living at home with their parents. Given with decreasing family cohesion. Although acculturation of
that a majority of Lam’s participants (87%) reported Vietnamese adolescents and their parents was not measured, these findings
was the primary language spoken at home, it appears these par- were attributed to higher independence representing a more
ticipants largely were living with ethnically enculturated families; bicultural identity on the part of adolescents, which buffered
thus, Lam argued that higher levels of interdependence reflected the negative effects of acculturative conflict between parents
an important cultural consistency with the participants’ home and adolescents. These results highlight the need for researchers
environments, which in turn was associated with greater self- not only to consider IndSC and InterSC as orthogonal constructs
esteem and less depression. but also to consider interactions between them.
Indeed, B. T. Lam (2005) found that different factors pre-
dicted self-esteem for adolescents high in IndSC versus high Relational self-construal and affect. RelSC and well-being are
in InterSC. Based on arguments similar to those of Kwan associated under some conditions. One of these is the type of
et al. (1997), Lam argued that family cohesion should be more well-being considered. Although RelSC is not associated with
important to the self-esteem of high InterSC adolescents. general psychological well-being (defined in terms of life sat-
Given that these adolescents’ peer groups are more likely to isfaction, depressive symptoms, perceived stress, positive and
reflect dominant cultural values, peer support should be more negative affect) among predominantly European American
important to the self-esteem of high IndSC adolescents. As college students (Cross, Gore, & Morris, 2003, Studies 1
predicted, family cohesion fully mediated the relation between and 3), RelSC is associated with greater relational well-being
interdependence and global self-esteem, whereas peer support (defined in terms of positive relations with others and aspects
partially mediated the relation between independence and of collective self-esteem; Cross et al., 2003, Study 2). Sample
self-esteem. also affects whether relations between well-being and RelSC
Together, these studies (Kwan et al., 1997; B. T. Lam, 2005; are found. Berkel and Constantine (2005) argued that a need
Suh et al., 2008) demonstrate that IndSC and, in some samples, for affiliation may be stronger and more beneficial to women
InterSC predict life satisfaction at least in part through their of color in predominantly White environments; therefore, they
relation with self-appraisals such as global self-esteem and hypothesized that RelSC would predict life satisfaction among
affect. Furthermore, interdependence predicts life satisfaction these women of color. Indeed, in their sample of African
through its relation with interpersonal constructs such as rela- American and Asian American women recruited from a pre-
tionship harmony, family cohesion, and social appraisals. dominantly White university, greater RelSC predicted greater
By simply measuring self-construal, however, none of these life satisfaction, even after controlling for relationship har-
studies demonstrates that self-construal causes these differences mony and family conflict.
in the bases of life satisfaction; however, Suh et al. (2008) con- Some researchers are also examining whether different factors
ducted a second study that does. They primed either an IndSC predict well-being for those with a more RelSC. Unfortunately,
or an InterSC and obtained the same results: Life satisfaction much of this research relies on gender as a proxy for RelSC
was predicted by both social appraisal and emotion for those rather than measuring it directly. However, this research does
primed with InterSC, but life satisfaction was predicted only tend to support the hypothesis that positive interpersonal rela-
by emotion for those primed with IndSC. Moreover, the same tionships are more predictive of life satisfaction for those with
pattern emerged whether self-construal was manipulated in the a more relational InterSC. For example, women show greater
United States or in Korea, providing strong evidence that self- decrements in daily life satisfaction than men when they feel
construal causes differences in the bases of life satisfaction. misunderstood in interpersonal interactions (Lun, Kesebir, &
Although most of the studies cited above examined inde- Oishi, 2008), and women’s life satisfaction (measured in terms
pendence and interdependence as orthogonal constructs, none of positive and negative affect) was predicted by relationship
of the research considered interactions between IndSC and harmony, whereas men’s life satisfaction was not (Reid, 2004).

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


160 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Summary and recommendations for future research. As evi- Asian Americans, a specific aspect of InterSC, relational inter-
denced by the relative brevity of the preceding section, sur- dependence (as captured by Singelis’s, 1994, SCS), was associ-
prisingly little research has been conducted that has actually ated with social anxiety. Moreover, some aspects of IndSC
measured self-construal in investigating the relations between (autonomy/assertiveness and behavioral consistency), but not
self-construal and affect. The little research that has been done others (individualism and primacy of self), were associated
has tended to focus on whether one type of self-construal is with social anxiety. For European Americans, only autonomy/
more strongly associated with particular types of affect than assertiveness predicted social anxiety. Thus, Markus and
another type of self-construal and whether different variables Kitayama’s (1991) prediction that the same emotion is elicited
mediate the relation between one type of self-construal and by different conditions depending on self-construal may be
affect as compared to another type of self-construal. Markus more nuanced. Does social appraisal predict life satisfaction
and Kitayama’s (1991) specific predictions about self-construal equally well for those high in the relational interdependence
and affect (i.e., that the same emotion is elicited by different component of InterSC as for those high in the esteem for group
conditions depending on self-construal, that different emotions component? Or do different types of social appraisal predict
are experienced by independent and interdependent people and life satisfaction, with positive appraisals from significant others
to different degrees) have received support. For example, the being more relevant for those high in the relational interdepen-
same construct of life satisfaction is elicited by different con- dence component whereas positive appraisals from one’s group
ditions depending on self-construal: by internal experiences, are more relevant for those high in the collective interdepen-
such as emotion or positive self-esteem, for those with a more dence or esteem for group components? Turning to another of
IndSC, and by relationship variables, such as relationship Markus and Kitayama’s predictions, that high IndSC and high
harmony or social appraisal, for those with a more InterSC InterSC individuals experience different emotions, are certain
(Kwan et al., 1997; B. T. Lam, 2005; Suh et al., 2008). emotions more tied to some aspects of self-construal than oth-
Apart from the research reviewed here, however, most of ers? As noted above, research that examines the consequences
the research relevant to these predictions has relied on either of experimentally manipulating these aspects of self-construal
proxies of self-construal (e.g., culture of origin; Cole, Bruschi, would provide the most compelling evidence for the role of
& Tamang, 2002; Kitayama et al., 2006) or measures of IND- dimensions of self-construal in affective experience.
COL (e.g., Fischer, Manstead, & Rodriguez Mosquera, 1999).
Although generally consistent with predictions, such research
fails to explicitly link observed differences to self-construal. Self-Construal Influences Motivation and
In addition, more research like that of Suh et al. (2008) that Self-Regulation
actually manipulates self-construal is needed. Demonstrating, According to Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) conceptualization,
for example, that priming an IndSC leads participants to expe- people with high InterSC are expected to have a greater number
rience a socially disengaging emotion (e.g., anger) whereas of social motives compared to those with high IndSC. Indeed,
priming an InterSC leads participants to experience a socially these social motives are an integral part of the definition of
engaging emotion (e.g., shame) in response to the same stimu- InterSC. The independent self is motivated to be autonomous
lus would provide compelling evidence that self-construal and agentic. In contrast, being a part of social groups and
causes differences in emotional experience. maintaining harmonious relationships with important others
In addition, work such as that by Liu and Goto (2007) raises are of great importance to the interdependent self. Recently,
important questions about how IndSC and InterSC interact in Wiekens and Stapel (2008) provided relevant evidence. Using
predicting affective experiences. Are such interactions found Brewer and Gardner’s (1996) method, they primed either the
only among bicultural participants, such as Liu and Goto’s IndSC or the InterSC of their participants. Participants whose
Asian American high school students, for whom being high IndSC was primed reported higher levels of motivation to be
in both independence and interdependence reflects an impor- independent, different, and alone compared to participants
tant adaptation to living in two cultures, or are similar interac- whose InterSC was primed. Those who received the InterSC
tions found among monocultural individuals? We know that prime reported higher levels of motivation to be accepted, to
individuals high in independence and relatively low in inter- conform, and to be together than those who received the IndSC
dependence experience life satisfaction as a result of internal prime (also see Verplanken, Trafimow, Khusid, Holland, &
experiences such as affect or self-esteem; is the same true for Steentjes, 2009). Other studies have shown that measures of
individuals high in both independence and interdependence? IndSC are positively related to the importance of individual
Moreover, who is more likely to chronically experience high goals and to status and achievement motivations and are nega-
IndSC and InterSC, and under what conditions might others tively related to affiliation motivation (Brutus & Greguras,
demonstrate both types of self-construal? 2008; van Horen, Pöhlmann, Koeppen, & Hannover, 2008).
Finally, recent work on specific aspects of IndSC and InterSC In contrast, InterSC is positively related to the importance of
raises new questions. For example, Hardin (2006) found that social goals (van Horen et al., 2008). Likewise, when facing
although InterSC was uncorrelated with social anxiety among a serious illness, people with high interdependent self-views

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 161

(as measured by the E. S. Kashima & Hardie, 2000, scale) Markus and Kitayama (1991) speculated on the implications
had more social concerns (e.g., being a burden on people close of different self-systems for self-enhancement and self-
to me), whereas people with high independent self-views consistency self-motives. Although considerable research has
expressed more individual concerns (e.g., not being able to examined differences in consistency motives for Westerners
rely on myself; Uskul & Hynie, 2007). and East Asians (with a focus on cognitive dissonance pro-
Second, agency for IndSC and InterSC results from differ- cesses; see Heine & Lehman, 1997; Hoshino-Browne et al.,
ent motivational sources. For IndSC, personal goals, desires, 2005; Kitayama, Snibbe, Markus, & Suzuki, 2004), to our
and abilities become the fuel for action, whereas for InterSC knowledge no studies have used measures or manipulations
coordination of the goals, desires, and needs of relational oth- of self-construal. Below, we focus on self-enhancement because
ers with those of the self are the primary sources of motivation it has been the focus of significant cross-cultural and self-
(Kitayama & Uchida, 2005; Markus & Kitayama, 2004). In construal research and has prompted a lively debate.
Markus and Kitayama’s (2004) conceptualization, what differs
is not the content of the needs, goals, and motivations but the Self-enhancement. Self-enhancement, which is motivated by
processes that put these into action. For instance, two students the need to see oneself with positive regard and to protect the
who have the goal of achieving in school might have different self from negative information, has long been considered to
motivational processes underlying this goal. One of these be one of the basic tendencies of the self. Biases related to
students, who has high IndSC, might see school success as a self-enhancement are viewed as conducive to having a healthy
way of actualizing himself or herself, and the other student, and well-adjusted self (Taylor & Brown, 1988).
who has high InterSC, might be motivated to be successful to According to Heine, Lehman, Markus, and Kitayama
better realize his or her obligations to her family and to the (1999), however, self-enhancement motivation is mostly a
society. Here, self-actualization can be considered a personal result of the IndSC. From the point of view of IndSC, behavior
reason for goal pursuit, whereas fulfilling the wishes of one’s is directed by the internal characteristics of the unique indi-
family can be considered a relational reason. It is important vidual. Thus, enhancing positive internal attributes and seeing
to note, however, that personal and relational aspects of a goal oneself with positive regard are beneficial. The InterSC, on
are different than its autonomous and controlled aspects (Deci the other hand, is not as separated from the surrounding group
& Ryan, 2000). That is, a person may be intrinsically motivated as the IndSC. Maintaining personal relationships, pursuing
to pursue a relational goal if the goal is autonomous (“I am group harmony, and being willing to sacrifice one’s own needs
pursuing this because it is important to someone who is impor- are expected. Given the InterSC, self-enhancement may elicit
tant to me”) rather than controlled (“I am pursuing this goal jealousy or envy from others and so could be detrimental for
because other people expect me to”). Likewise, someone may basic social needs such as maintaining harmonious relation-
be unmotivated to pursue a personal goal if the goal is con- ships. Thus, persons with a high InterSC would not be highly
trolled (“I am pursuing this because the situation demands it”) motivated to see themselves in a positive light. Much research
rather than autonomous (“I am pursuing this because I really on self-enhancement has relied on group comparisons using
believe it is an important goal to have”). IND-COL of the given culture as a proxy measure for the
In two longitudinal studies conducted with North American prevalent self-construal of the cultural group under investiga-
participants, Gore and Cross (2006) examined the relation tion. This line of research has provided valuable evidence for
between self-construal and relational versus personal reasons this perspective with a wide range self-enhancing behaviors
for pursuing goals. Their results showed that people with (e.g., Chang & Asakawa, 2003; Heine, Kitayama, & Lehman,
higher RelSC had more relational-autonomous reasons for 2001; Heine & Lehman, 1995; Heine, Takata, & Lehman, 2000;
their goals compared to those with lower RelSC (as assessed Kitayama, Markus, Matsumoto, & Norasakkunkit, 1997).
by the RISC scale; Cross et al., 2000). They found that both Others, however, have claimed that self-enhancement is a
personal-autonomous and relational-autonomous reasons for universal motive, and they have argued the self-enhancement
goals affected perceived progress and effort toward achieving will occur in different domains depending on the salient dimen-
the goal. A cross-cultural replication of this study obtained sion of self-construal. For example, Sedikides, Gaertner, and
similar results among American and Japanese samples (Gore, Toguchi (2003, Study 2) conducted a study where they employed
Cross, & Kanagawa, 2009). These studies demonstrate the the better-than-average method to observe group differences.
link between RelSC and forms of interdependent agency, as Participants rated themselves relative to a hypothetical group
proposed by Markus and Kitayama (2004). member on two sets of behaviors and traits that were differen-
A third and very important implication of different self- tially meaningful for IndSC and InterSC. The two groups did
construal concerns self-related motives. Motives that are not differ from each other on overall self-enhancement, but par-
directly related to the self (i.e., self-motives) are expected to ticipants high on IndSC (as measured by Singelis’s, 1994, scale)
be experienced very differently by people with differing self- self-enhanced on the independence-related measure, whereas
construals. These self-motives can be described as directed at participants high on InterSC self-enhanced on the interdepen-
establishing a particular self-representation (see Leary, 2007). dence-related measure. The measure of self-enhancement used

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


162 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

in the Sedikides et al. studies and in similar research by Kurman responses rather than contrasting (i.e., self-enhancing) affec-
(2001), the better-than-average method, has been criticized on tive responses.
multiple grounds (Heine & Hamamura, 2007). In this approach,
participants are asked to compare themselves to an average per- Self-regulation. Self-regulation can be defined broadly as the
son on specific characteristics, traits, and abilities on which most exercise of control over oneself to attain desired end states
people consider themselves better than most others. In a given (Schmeichel & Baumeister, 2004). It is an essential element
population, if most people consider themselves better than most in any goal-directed behavior, and as such it is extremely
others, this would be a statistical impossibility, and so it is con- relevant for the dynamic relations between motivation and
sidered a bias (Alicke, 1985). Any given individual, on the other self-construal. Most self-construal research in this area has
hand, can be objectively better than most other people. Correlat- centered on the topic of regulatory focus. Yet there has been
ing self-construal scores of individual participants with their some research attention to differences of the direction of con-
standing on the better-than-average measure is therefore not trol and self-regulatory strength across the two primary self-
appropriate for assessing self-enhancement bias at the individual construals. In this section, we briefly summarize this line of
level. research.
A second problem with this measure concerns the process According to Higgins (1996, 1997) there are two basic self-
of making comparisons with an average other. It has been regulatory foci: a promotion focus, which is characterized by
argued that when people are asked to compare themselves to a motivation to approach desired end states, and a prevention
an average other, they engage in absolute rather than relative focus, which is characterized by a motivation to avoid undesired
evaluations of themselves (Klar & Giladi, 1999). In fact, when end states. Regulatory focus has important consequences for
people are evaluating any randomly selected target, they tend behavior. For instance, prevention focus is shown to increase
to evaluate it as better than average. Because most people have speed of braking in an accident-prone simulated traffic condi-
a favorable absolute evaluation of themselves, it appears that tion, which points to risk avoidance (Werth & Forster, 2007).
most people have a bias in comparative measures (for further Although these regulatory foci can be situationally induced by
criticism of this method, see Klar & Giladi, 1997). various priming techniques (e.g., Shah, Higgins, & Friedman,
Other studies have approached the role of self-construal in 1998), individuals differ in their chronic levels of prevention
self-enhancement from different theoretical perspectives. For and promotion focus. Chronic self-regulatory foci have roots
example, Gardner, Gabriel, and Hochschild (2002) examined in early socialization practices, where parental emphasis on
this issue from the perspective of Tesser’s (1980) self-evaluation positive outcomes by rewarding desired behavior fosters pro-
maintenance (SEM) model. SEM predicts that people reflect motion focus and parental emphasis on negative outcomes by
on the success of close others when the success is in a self- punishing undesired behavior fosters prevention focus in the
irrelevant domain, and this in turn boosts self-esteem. When the child (Higgins & Silberman, 1998).
success of the close other is in a self-relevant domain, this Parental practices are one antecedent of chronic regulatory
results in comparison and thus a reduction in self-esteem. focus. Another antecedent could be self-construal. Maintaining
These effects are reversed when the social other is not a close relational harmony, fitting into one’s proper roles in the society,
person. Gardner et al. (2002) manipulated the self-construal and living up to the standards of significant others are essential
of their participants to see if the above predictions hold true for people with high InterSC. This requires self-monitoring
when InterSC is activated. Consistent with the predictions of and sensitivity to self-relevant information that could signal
SEM theory, participants in the IndSC prime condition evalu- success or failure in the fulfillment of obligations. Hence,
ated their friend as more successful than a stranger in a self- InterSC may support a prevention self-regulatory focus. People
irrelevant domain and as less successful in a self-relevant with high IndSC, on the other hand, are socialized to be self-
domain. Participants in the InterSC prime condition evaluated actualized individuals who pursue personal aspirations. Unlike
their friend as more successful than a stranger in both the self- people with InterSC, who act with a concern for not harming
relevant and self-irrelevant domains. This shows that for people significant relationships, those with IndSC are motivated to
with an InterSC, comparing oneself to a close other is not a be positively distinct persons with achievements. Hence, IndSC
source of threat but instead is an opportunity to bask in the may support a promotion focus.
reflected glory of the relational other (also see Cheng & Lam, Lee, Aaker, and Gardner (2000) tested these assumptions
2007). Similarly, people exposed to upward and downward using a sample of participants with chronic IndSC versus
social comparisons respond differently depending on their primed InterSC (as measured by the Singelis SCS) as well as a sample
self-construal. When InterSC is primed (compared to IndSC), of participants whose self-construal was experimentally primed.
people experience less positive affect after exposure to a down- They utilized specific scenarios that provided the same informa-
ward comparison target and less negative affect after exposure tion, except for a difference in framing. Half of the scenarios
to an upward comparison target (White, Lehman, & Cohen, were presented in terms of gains (gain-framed information),
2006). In short, priming the InterSC seems to create a sense and the other half were presented in terms of losses (loss-framed
of having a bond with the target, resulting in stronger empathic information). Results showed that participants with high InterSC

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 163

evaluated loss-framed information as more important than the People who construe themselves in relation to others are
gain-framed information, and participants with high IndSC hypothesized to prefer secondary control over primary control
evaluated gain-framed information as more important than the (Morling, Kitayama, & Miyamoto, 2002; Weisz et al., 1984).
loss-framed information (also see Aaker & Lee, 2001). More Therefore, one would expect people with high InterSC to
recently, Hamilton and Biehal (2005) provided additional sup- engage in secondary control when faced with stressors. Indeed,
port for these effects of self-construal on financial risk taking. A. G. Lam and Zane (2004) showed in a cross-cultural study
Participants were primed with either IndSC or InterSC and that this assumption was correct. At the group level, Asian
asked to allocate a hypothetical budget across several funds that participants scored higher than Americans on a self-report
varied in their gain and risk levels. They found that the IndSC- measure of secondary control and American participants in
primed participants allocated the budget so as to maximize gain turn, scored higher than Asians on primary control. At the indi-
and took greater risks, whereas the InterSC-primed participants vidual level, IndSC was positively related to primary control,
allocated the budget so as to minimize losses and risk. Moreover, whereas InterSC was related to secondary control. Moreover,
the relation between self-construal prime and risk taking was culture’s influence on direction of control was mediated by
completely mediated by a measure of promotion and prevention self-construals.
orientation. These results point to the willingness of persons with high
Zhang and Mittal (2007) tested a similar hypothesis about InterSC to adjust themselves to fit into a situation or harmonize
regulatory focus using a different experimental paradigm. They with others. As a result, people with chronically high InterSC
reasoned that people with a promotion focus would find enriched may develop strong self-control strategies over time. This idea
options more attractive but that people with a prevention focus of increasing one’s ability to exert self-control is consistent
would prefer impoverished options. Enriched options have high with recent research on self-regulation. According to Baumeister
attribute scatter with extreme values. Impoverished options have and his colleagues, self-regulatory strength is comparable to
values clustered toward the average. Suppose that you are a home the strength of a muscle: Like the use of a muscle, use of one’s
buyer and the four important criteria are the size of the rooms store of self-regulatory resources will eventual lead to temporary
and the deck and the condition of the kitchen and bathrooms. exhaustion, termed “ego-depletion” (Baumeister & Vohs, 2003;
Suppose further that you have two options from which to choose. Schmeichel & Baumeister, 2004). Although it may be exhausted,
The first house, which represents the enriched option, has huge the strength of this “muscle” can also be improved by chronic
rooms and a fabulous kitchen with state-of-the-art appliances. use. For this reason, one would expect people with high InterSC,
But the deck is very small and the bathrooms are not in good who are accustomed to adjusting to situations, to experience less
shape. The second house, which represents the impoverished ego-depletion after a self-regulatory task, compared to others.
option, is average on all four criteria: average-sized rooms and Seeley and Gardner (2003) tested this hypothesis in a
deck and kitchen and bathrooms in acceptable condition. study in which participants engaged in a self-regulation task
In a series of cross-cultural studies with Chinese and (suppressing any thoughts of a “white bear” for 5 min) followed
American samples, Zhang and Mittal (2007) found that par- by another task that also required considerable self-control
ticipants with high InterSC (as measured by Trafimow et al.’s, (squeezing a handgrip exerciser). In line with the hypothesis,
1991, scale) evaluated impoverished options as more attractive participants with high InterSC persisted on the second task
than enriched options. Participants with high IndSC, however, longer than participants with high IndSC, indicating lower
evaluated enriched options as more attractive than impover- levels of regulatory depletion among the former group. Simi-
ished options. Moreover, self-construal completely mediated larly, regulatory depletion was more pronounced for Americans
the relation between culture and option evaluations. In addi- compared to Asians. This study provides initial behavioral
tion, they employed a questionnaire measure of regulatory evidence that self-construal is related to differences in self-
focus in a separate study and successfully showed that the regulation, but much more research is needed to uncover the
relation of self-construal (as measured by Singelis’s, 1994, particular types of situations and specific self-regulation tasks
scale) with option evaluations was mediated by regulatory that may be influenced by variation in self-construal.
focus. This lends strong support to the argument that different
self-systems afford different regulatory foci. Summary and recommendations for future research. Most of
Regulatory focus is only one aspect of the motivational Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) predictions concerning the
components of self-regulation. Another important aspect is the relations between motivation and self-construal have found
direction of control. Challenged by a stressful situation, people empirical support. First, self-construal research showed that
can cope with it in two different ways. They can either direct social motives are an integral part of the definition for InterSC.
their control efforts to the environment to change it to fit their Evidence for the importance of social motives for people with
personal needs (primary control) or direct control toward them- InterSC comes both from studies that manipulated self-
selves through altering feelings and cognitions to adjust to the construals (e.g., Wiekens & Stapel, 2008) and from studies
objective environment (secondary control; Weisz, Rothbaum, that measured participants’ existing self-construals (e.g., Brutus
& Blackburn, 1984). & Greguras, 2008; van Horen et al., 2008).

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


164 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) second proposal—that (A. G. Lam & Zane, 2004). And finally, research on self-
agency differs for IndSC and InterSC—also has received regulatory strength points out that people with high InterSC
empirical support. Specifically, for people with high IndSC, experience less regulatory depletion compared to people with
personal concerns such as self-actualization are important in high IndSC in some situations (Seeley & Gardner, 2003). This
goal pursuit. For people with high InterSC, however, relational research on the role of self-construal in self-regulation is pre-
concerns are important sources of motivation. For people with liminary but promising, and it may be beneficial in numerous
high RelSC, relational reasons are very influential in goal domains.
pursuits in the long run, and they serve as intrinsic sources of To date, although considerable attention has been paid to
motivation (Gore & Cross, 2006; Gore et al., 2009). These cognitive approaches to the issue of consistency (as described
initial findings point to the importance of considering prevail- above), no one has examined the role of self-construal in dis-
ing theories of motivation and agency from the perspective sonance processes. Cross-cultural research has made great
of variation in self-construal. For example, although self- strides in showing how dissonance processes vary for people
determination theory posits that relatedness is one of three pri- from East Asian versus Western backgrounds (Hoshino-Browne
mary motives for all people (the others being competence and et al., 2005; Kitayama, Snibbe, et al., 2004). To be brief, West-
autonomy), it has received far less attention than the other two. ern participants tend to seek consistency within themselves,
This may in part be because of the assumptions of the self as whereas East Asians tend to seek consistency with others’ views
independent and separate from others that pervade the environ- and expectations. Unfortunately, most research in this area uses
ment of Western researchers and the populations they study. culture as a proxy for self-construal and has not used explicit
Other forms of social motivation, such as motivations that focus measures or manipulations of self-construal (but see Pöhlmann,
on one’s reputation in the community (as are represented in the Carranza, Hannover, & Iyengar, 2007, for a closely related study).
concepts of face or honor), also may be linked to variation in Considering the importance of self-consistency as a self-motive
self-construal. and the range of cross-cultural findings on this topic, examina-
Among all of the proposals of Markus and Kitayama (1991), tion of the hypothesized role of (measured or manipulated)
the one that has prompted the liveliest debate was that self- self-construal in this process will be an informative next step.
motives such as self-enhancement would take on different Because of its influence on motivation and self-regulation,
meanings given different self-systems. In their view, self- the possible applications of self-construal theory are almost
enhancement motivation is mostly a result of the IndSC, which countless. Researchers in applied disciplines have begun to
promotes evaluating oneself positively. For the InterSC, how- examine its importance in a variety of domains. Links have
ever, the need to be a good group member requires self- been made to the fields of leisure studies (Walker, Deng, &
improvement and a self-critical attitude. Although considerable Dieser, 2005), eating disorders (Green, Scott, DeVilder, Zeiger,
cross-cultural research has lent support for these predictions & Darr, 2006), health (Lun et al., 2008; Uskul & Hynie, 2007),
(e.g., Heine et al., 2000; Heine, Kitayama, & Lehman, 2001; counseling (Yeh & Arora, 2003), consumer behavior (Zhang
Heine, Kitayama, Lehman, et al., 2001; Kitayama et al., 1997), & Shrum, 2009), and business management (Gelfand, Major,
some researchers have argued that people with high InterSC Raver, Nishii, & O’Brien, 2006; see next section). We suspect
also engage in self-enhancement, but only in ways mean- that further applications in education, vocational psychology,
ingfully related to their self-construal (e.g., Kurman, 2001; and marital and family therapy—to name just a few—are not
Sedikides et al., 2003). Others, in contrast, have proposed that far behind.
self-enhancement motivation is not related to self-construal
at all. In a series of studies by Thomsen, Sidanius, and Fiske’s
(2007), self-enhancement was not related to self-construal Self-Construal Shapes Interpersonal Behavior
but to the endorsement of interpersonal leveling beliefs. In How does variation in self-construal shape interaction with
sum, although there is controversy, the existing evidence other people? Although Markus and Kitayama (1991) did not
mostly supports the proposal of Markus and Kitayama (1991) address social behavior specifically, there are many obvious
that self-enhancement takes on different meanings for differ- applications of their thesis to social interaction. People with
ent self-systems. high InterSC should seek to maintain connectedness and har-
Self-regulation has been another topic that has attracted mony in relationships, whereas those with high IndSC should
considerable research attention. In general, self-construal stud- seek to maintain individuality and separateness from others
ies concerning regulatory focus, direction of control, and self- (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Pursuit of these goals may be
regulatory strength are generally in line with the predictions relatively automatic or nonconscious when the associated
derived from Markus and Kitayama’s (1991) theorizing. self-construal is chronically or temporarily activated. For
InterSC supports prevention focus, whereas IndSC supports example, people who have chronically high InterSC (or who
promotion focus (Lee et al., 2000; Zhang & Mittal, 2007). are exposed to an InterSC prime) tend to sit closer to another
Concerning direction of control, IndSC is positively related person in a lab situation than do those with a primed IndSC
to primary control and InterSC is related to secondary control or chronically high IndSC (Holland, Roeder, van Baaren,

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 165

Brandt, & Hannover, 2004). Similarly, priming the InterSC provides an excellent opportunity to examine relationship
results in a greater likelihood of imitating the behavior of another processes among previously unacquainted persons who have
person, compared to priming the IndSC or a control condition not self-selected to live together.
(van Baaren, Maddux, Chartrand, de Bouter, & van Knippen- Cross and her colleagues hypothesized that persons with
berg, 2003). Why might imitation be important in the develop- high RelSC will seek to make the most of the situation and will
ment or maintenance of social relationships? Imitation may strive to develop a close relationship with the partner. If a close
create a sense of interpersonal similarity or synchrony, which friendship is not possible with the new roommate, the very
then greases the wheels of interpersonal interaction. In general, relational person will nonetheless seek to have a harmonious,
people who are mimicked by an interaction partner tend to like relatively conflict-free relationship. One way to promote a rela-
that partner, to have greater rapport with the partner, and to tionship with a new partner is to share openly one’s own interests,
engage in more prosocial behavior (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; goals, values, and beliefs and to attend closely to and respond
van Baaren, Holland, Steenaert, & van Knippenberg, 2003; sensitively to the self-disclosures of one’s partner. Gore et al.
see Chartrand, Maddox, & Lakin, 2005, for a review). The (2006, Study 2) found that participants’ degree of chronically
association between self-construal and mimicry is bidirec- activated RelSC had both direct and indirect effects on their
tional: People who are imitated by others also come to own and their roommates’ closeness and commitment to the
describe themselves more interdependently than do people relationship. Both high RelSC individuals and their roommates
who are not mimicked (as measured by responses to the TST; reported enhanced relationship quality after 1 month. This effect
Ashton-James, van Baaren, Chartrand, Decety, & Karremans, was mediated by perceptions of one’s own and one’s partner’s
2007). Furthermore, when participants were unobtrusively emotional disclosure and perceptions that one’s partner responded
imitated by another person, they were more likely to describe sensitively to one’s own disclosures. In short, because high
themselves as interdependent with others (relative to non- RelSC participants disclosed openly and responded sensitively
mimicked participants), which in turn predicted their willing- to their roommates, they created a supportive environment
ness to help a third party (Ashton-James et al., 2007). for the development of a close relationship (for other studies
Self-construal also influences self-presentation; IndSC prim- of self-construal and self-disclosure, see Cross et al., 2000,
ing increases self-enhancing forms of self-presentation, Study 3; Morry, 2005; Terzino & Cross, 2009).
whereas InterSC priming increases self-presentations of When a person pays close attention to his or her relationship
social appropriateness (Lalwani & Shavitt, 2009). partner’s self-revelations, that person should be able to predict
People with chronically high InterSC also behave in a more the partner’s responses to a variety of situations. Cross and
other-oriented fashion, compared to those with a high IndSC. Morris (2003) found that participants with high RelSC were
For example, when exposed to scenarios that pitted the indi- better able to predict their roommate’s responses to a values
vidual’s self-interest against the interest of an important in- measure. This association was strongest in relationships that
group, InterSC-primed European American and Asian American were relatively distant and weaker in relationship in which the
participants were more likely than IndSC-primed participants two roommates were very close. Cross and Morris (2003) sug-
to choose the group’s welfare over their own (Gardner, Gabriel, gested that in very close relationships, the relationship itself
& Dean, 2004). Similarly, when given power to distribute motivates the participants to attend to each others’ cares and
tasks to themselves and to another participant, highly relational concerns and thereby enhances agreement on this task. In more
participants distributed the tasks less selfishly than did lows on distant relationships, the person with high RelSC may pay
this dimension (S. Chen, Lee-Chai, & Bargh, 2001). close attention to the partner as a strategy for avoiding conflict
or in hopes that a deeper relationship is still possible. In their
Self-construal and close relationships. These studies of proxim- second study, Cross and Morris found evidence for the latter
ity, mimicry, and other-oriented behavior utilized laboratory goal: Participants with high RelSC had overly optimistic views
procedures in which participants interacted with strangers. of their roommate’s feelings of closeness in the relationship.
Thus, they provide compelling confirmation that the InterSC Such optimism can foster continued efforts to work at the
promotes positive relational behavior even when there is no relationship in hopes of greater intimacy eventually. Indeed,
prior relationship between the partners. When there is an other studies suggest that persons with high RelSC or InterSC
ongoing relationship or the possibility of continuing interac- report closer relationships and more social support than lows
tion, however, individuals with high RelSC should be espe- (Cross et al., 2000), and they are more likely to include others
cially likely to engage in a variety of behaviors that promote in the self (Cross et al., 2000; Vorauer & Cameron, 2002).
closeness and harmony in the relationship. Cross and her Furthermore, persons with high RelSC are less likely than
colleagues (Cross et al., 2003; Cross & Morris, 2003; Gore, others to view conflict with friends and close others as a zero-
Cross, & Morris, 2006) have investigated how North Ameri- sum game, in which one person wins and the other loses. Gore
cans with varying levels of RelSC (as measured by the RISC and Cross (in press) asked participants to imagine themselves
scale, Cross et al., 2000) interact with strangers who were in a variety of situations in which they were resolving a conflict
assigned to be their roommates. The roommate situation with a friend, roommate, or another close other. The results

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


166 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

revealed zero-sum thinking among those with low RelSC but processes. Perhaps Sinclair and Fehr (2005) said it best when
not among those with high RelSC. For the low RelSC partici- they closed their article with this statement: “[I]t may even be
pants, decisions that benefitted the partner were perceived as the case that self-construal is the fundamental, distal factor that
costly to the self. For the participants with high RelSC, there drives many of the other self-related processes that have been
was typically no relation between evaluations of the outcome examined in the close relationships literature” (p. 303).
of a decision for oneself and for the relationship partner. In a
third study, where participants were asked to recall a conflict Self-construal and communication processes. When people’s self-
situation, high RelSC participants indicated regret over making construals focus them on interdependence versus independence,
a self-serving decision. In other words, persons with high their characteristic ways of communicating will reflect these
RelSC do not perceive a trade-off between acting in their own orientations. As Singelis and Brown (1995) describe, the focus
interests and acting in the interests of close others. Instead, on harmonious relationships and the definition of the self
consistent with the notion that the other is part of the self, they in terms of close relationships and in-groups among those
perceive decisions that benefit the relationship as ultimately with high RelSC or Coll-InterSC should result in a preference
benefitting the self as well. for indirect communication, sensitivity to the context in social
Friendships and roommates are important parts of people’s interaction, attention to others’ thoughts and feelings, and non-
lives, but romantic relationships receive much more attention confrontational conflict resolution styles. In short, the goal of
from researchers. To date, however, researchers have paid little communication, given RelSC or Coll-InterSC, is the mainte-
attention to the role of variation in self-construal in romantic nance of harmonious relationships with close others and in-
relationships. Given the strong association of gender with group members. In contrast, for individuals with high IndSC,
RelSC (Cross et al., 2000), one might expect that self-construal the goal of communication is to express the person’s unique
plays a significant role in many aspects of relationship devel- goals, wishes, thoughts, feelings, and abilities. As a result, IndSC
opment and maintenance. Sinclair and Fehr (2005) examined should be associated with direct communication styles, little
the association between self-construal and responses to attention to contextual aspects of social interaction, attention
dissatisfaction in romantic relationships. When measured to one’s own thoughts and feelings during social interaction,
(Study 1) or primed (Study 2), IndSC was positively associ- and willingness to engage in confrontational dispute resolu-
ated with the preference to use the active, constructive strat- tion styles.
egy of voice when one is dissatisfied with the relationship. Researchers have made several inroads into investigating
Measured (Study 1) and primed (Study 2) InterSC was posi- these theoretical consequences of self-construal for commu-
tively associated with the passive, constructive strategy of nication processes. For example, InterSC is positively related
loyalty, in which the person waits for things to improve (also to concern for a conversation partners’ feelings and possible
see Yum, 2004). These findings are consistent with other negative evaluation of the self (Gudykunst et al., 1996; M.-S.
research that suggests that IndSC is associated with a promo- Kim, Sharkey, & Singelis, 1994; Mandel, 2003), a desire to
tion focus and InterSC is associated with a prevention focus avoid arguments (M. Kim, Aune, Hunter, Kim, & Kim, 2001),
(Lee et al., 2000) and with other studies showing that indi- and the use of cooperative strategies in group discussions
viduals with high InterSC avoid dominating forms of conflict (Oetzel, 1998). IndSC is positively related to a concern for
resolution (see below). clarity or directness in communication (Gudykunst et al., 1996;
In what other ways might self-construal influence relation- M.-S. Kim et al., 1994); open and expressive communication
ship development and maintenance? Persons with high RelSC (Gudykunst et al., 1996); and the use of assertive or dominat-
may be more likely than others to make situational (rather ing strategies in group discussions (Oetzel, 1998; also see
than dispositional) attributions for a partner’s bad behavior, Bresnahan, Ohashi, Liu, Nebashi, & Liao, 1999). In addition,
which are associated with relationship satisfaction (Bradbury IndSC is associated with approval of others’ positive self-
& Fincham, 1990). High RelSC persons tend to perceive them- presentations, but InterSC is associated with approval of oth-
selves as more similar to their partner than low relationals, ers’ negative self-presentations and approval of others when
which can also help maintain relationships (Byrne & Blaylock, they attribute their own success to the help of others (Ellis &
1963; Cross et al., 2002). When a relationship is threatened, Wittenbaum, 2000; J. Kim, Kim, Kam, & Shin, 2003).
highly relational participants may be more likely to engage Differing self-construals should also engender different
in prorelationship behavior rather than withdraw (Lydon, reactions to conflict, especially conflict with meaningful others
Menzies-Toman, Burton, & Bell, 2008). If InterSC is associated (e.g., friends, relatives, or coworkers). One conceptualization
with perceiving the self as malleable (as cross-cultural research of conflict strategies draws on conceptions of face and facework
suggests; Heine, Kitayama, Lehman, et al., 2001), then highly (Ting-Toomey, 1988). Facework is the strategies one uses to
interdependent persons may tend to adjust to their relationship protect, challenge, or defend one’s own or another’s “claimed
partners to maintain harmony in the relationship (Morling et al., sense of positive image in the context of social interaction” (or
2002). In short, a focus on variation in self-construal may help face; Oetzel et al., 2001, p. 235). In a study that included
researchers better understand a wide variety of relationship participants from China, Germany, Japan, and the United

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 167

States, Oetzel and his colleagues (2001) asked participants to organizations. Across three studies, Brockner and his colleagues
recall a conflict and to rate their experience in that conflict found that perceptions of procedural fairness (e.g., the degree
with respect to several facework strategies. IndSC (as measured to which one has a voice in decisions or the fairness of inter-
by the Gudykunst et al., 1996, measure) was strongly associ- personal treatment) were more strongly related to a variety of
ated with facework strategies that defended one’s own face. outcomes (e.g., cooperation, positive affect, and desire to inter-
InterSC was strongly associated with strategies that reflected act with the other party) for people who had high InterSC than
concern for the other’s face and mutual face concerns, such as for lows on this dimension (also see Brockner, Chen, Mannix,
problem solving, apologizing, and giving in to the other person. Leung, & Skarlicki, 2000; Gollwitzer & Bücklein, 2007;
InterSC was negatively related to dominating strategies in Holmvall & Bobocel, 2008). Research focused on equity in
the interaction (also see Oetzel & Ting-Toomey, 2003; Ting- social exchange revealed that responses to inequitable treatment
Toomey, Oetzel, & Yee-Jung, 2001). depended on the participants’ RelSC and their relationship with
Unfortunately, most of these studies of self-construal and the other person: Persons with high RelSC were less likely
communication employ cross-sectional designs and use only than lows to respond negatively when a close other had treated
self-report data. New strides in understanding how variation them unfairly (Y. Chen, Chen, & Portnoy, 2009, Study 2) but
in self-construal relates to communication processes may be were more likely to respond negatively when a person who
made by using experimental paradigms that manipulate self- was not close treated them badly (Holmvall & Sidhu, 2007).
construal and employ behavioral measures of direct or indirect Others have found that the degree to which the three dimen-
communication strategies. A good example of an experimental sions of self-construal (IndSC, RelSC, and InterSC) moderate
approach is found in a recent study by Seeley Howard, Gardner, the association between procedural justice and work-related
and Thompson (2007). Three studies showed that the behavior outcomes differs depending on the specific forms of procedural
of InterSC-primed participants who had high power in a nego- justice under investigation (Johnson, Selenta, & Lord, 2006).
tiation depended on the situational context. When negotiating
with a lower power individual, high-power and InterSC-primed Summary and recommendations for future research. Surprisingly
participants were more generous and more likely to reach a little research has focused on the effects of differing self-
settlement with their lower power opponent than were IndSC- construals for social behavior, close relationships, and other
primed participants (Study 1). In a second study, power and phenomena related to interactions with others or with groups.
self-construal were again manipulated but participants engaged Consistent with expectations, InterSC is associated with a vari-
in either a negotiation with one other person or a team negotia- ety of processes that serve to enhance or maintain relationships,
tion, in which teams of three participants negotiated with each including mimicry, prosocial behavior, and indirect, noncon-
other. Interestingly, when negotiating with a group, high power frontational communication strategies. Cross and her col-
InterSC-primed participants were less generous than the high- leagues have examined the role of RelSC in a variety of
power IndSC-primed participants. Negotiating individually relationship-related processes, but many more processes may
or as a group did not affect the final settlement amounts decided be fruitfully explored from the perspective of variation in self-
by high-power IndSC-primed participants. These studies sug- construal. For example, many gender differences in behavior
gest that persons with high InterSC have a sense of responsi- and relationship processes have been observed, and these dif-
bility or attachment to other persons in the negotiation but ferences may be in part a function of gender differences in
that the attachment depends on the context. When negotiating self-construal (Cross & Madson, 1997; Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton,
individually with a lower status target, the high-power and 2001). When the mechanisms that explain gender differences
high-InterSC negotiator will behave benevolently. But when in behavior are identified (e.g., differences in self-construal),
negotiating as part of a team, their loyalties and attachments researchers are better able to stipulate the conditions under
lie with the other team members. Their commitment is to pursue which those behaviors are most likely to be expressed, ways
the interests of the team, resulting in less generous settlements that the behaviors may be modified, and other factors that
with the opposing party. may maintain or bolster the behaviors. Consider the finding
This work highlights the potential importance of self- that many couples differ in their expectation of closeness or
construal to organizational psychology and leadership (van intimacy in the marriage, with women desiring more close-
Knippenberg, van Knippenberg, De Cremer, & Hogg, 2004). ness and communication of love and affection than men (see
A few researchers have begun to consider the role of self- Eldridge & Christensen, 2002, for a review). If these differ-
construal in topics of organizational justice. For example, ences derive from different self-construals, then both research-
Brockner, De Cremer, van den Bos, and Chen (2005) argued ers and therapists have a theoretically rich starting point for
that because procedural fairness communicates that individu- further investigation into the cognitive, affective, and moti-
als are respected and valued in the organization, it therefore vational foundations of differences in communication, conflict,
reflects the importance of relational values. Consequently, and marital satisfaction.
people who tend to define themselves in terms of their relation- Cross-cultural studies have observed cultural differences
ships may be especially sensitive to procedural fairness in in a variety of social phenomena, such as conformity (H. Kim

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


168 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

& Markus, 1999), trust (Yamagishi & Yamagishi, 1994), and rather than the context in an attention task, or to prefer disen-
social loafing (Earley, 1993). To date, these studies have not gaging to socially engaging emotions. Moreover, individuals
been followed up by additional work aimed at identifying the who tended to engage in one of these IndSC-related tasks were
mechanisms responsible for these differences. For example, not necessarily likely to engage in other IndSC-related tasks.
one would expect that participants with high InterSC would Kitayama and his colleagues (2009), along with Y. Kashima
be more likely to conform to the behavior of in-group members (2009), offer helpful insight into these sorts of apparent anoma-
than would those with high IndSC. lies in cross-cultural data. First, Kitayama et al. remind us
There are also promising avenues for further research into of Simpson’s paradox: the observation that constructs that are
the consequences of different self-construals for social behav- related at the societal level (e.g., InterSC and context-sensitive
ior in other settings, such as the workplace. Many aspects of thinking) may not necessarily be related at the individual level
organizational behavior, such as citizenship behaviors, job (also see Oyserman & Uskul, 2008; van de Vijver, van Hemert,
withdrawal, and leadership styles, may be linked to differences & Poortinga, 2008). They also contend that cultural mandates
in self-construals (e.g., Gahan & Abeysekera, 2009; Gelfand to be independent or interdependent may be enacted through
et al., 2006). Ultimately, whether a person considers himself any of several different cultural tasks. For example, two
or herself to be independent of others or fundamentally inter- Americans, who both strive to be independent, may go about
dependent with others in a situation can importantly affect that by focusing on different dimensions of independence. Ed
other social behaviors, such as conformity, compliance, aggres- demonstrates his independence by focusing on his uniqueness
sion, altruism, cooperation, and persuasion. and differentiating himself from others, whereas Valerie dem-
onstrates her independence by aggressively pursuing her
personal goals. Moreover, each cultural task is associated with
Where to From Here? specific psychological tendencies (e.g., using focused vs. holis-
Self-construal was initially proposed as a means of understand- tic attention or making dispositional vs. situational attribu-
ing cultural differences in behavior (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; tions). Just as different individuals use different cultural tasks
Triandis, 1989). Today, however, researchers also recognize to fulfill their culture’s mandate, different cultural tasks are
the potential role of within-culture variation in self-construal associated with different sets of psychological tendencies. Thus,
for explaining many psychological phenomena (e.g., Cross & within an individual, there may be relatively little association
Madson, 1997). We suspect that these dual goals—understanding among different culture-related processes.
cultural differences and within-culture processes—will continue Second, Kitayama et al. (2009) observe that these cultural
to motivate considerable research in the near term. To further mandates begin to influence children from birth and the vari-
these goals, we next turn to theoretical, conceptual, and meth- ous psychological tendencies and cultural tasks used to comply
odological issues that sometimes thwart this work. with the cultural mandate become habitual, automatic, or rote.
As we mentioned in the section on Between- and Within- Explicit views of the self, as tapped by measures of self-
Group Differences in Self-Construal, there are times when construal, develop in adolescence, and they may not be linked
measures of self-construal fail to demonstrate the expected conceptually to these automatic, habitual, or overlearned tasks.
cultural patterns of differences (Oyserman et al., 2002). There Thus, Americans and East Asians may perform very differently
are also many times measures of self-construal fail to mediate on a variety of culture-linked tasks (as they did in Kitayama
cultural differences in specific behaviors (Kitayama et al., et al.’s, 2009, study), but performance on these tasks may not
2009). We are confident that a great many studies have been require the activation of the associated self-construal. Kashima
relegated to the file drawer because manipulations of self- (2009) also notes that culturally based processes are typically
construal fail to produce the expected effects on behavior. In learned and become automatized in specific situations and
an innovative study, Kitayama and his colleagues presented activities; some behaviors are strongly driven by the norms of
an argument for why explicit measures of self-construal may particular contexts rather than by personal beliefs or self-views
fail to predict cultural differences in behaviors that have been (Zou et al., 2009). Thus, attempts to elicit cultural tasks and
linked theoretically to the self. They observed the expected behaviors (i.e., through priming) may not be effective if the
differences in American, Western European, and Japanese attempt does not tap the appropriate context or if the behavior
participants’ performance on several tasks (e.g., dispositional is strongly driven by situational factors.
attribution, focused vs. holistic attention, and preference for Third, explicit measures of self-construal appear to assess
disengaging or socially engaging emotions), but there was no Kitayama et al.’s cultural tasks (e.g., being unique or self-effacing)
systematic association between performance on these tasks rather than the accompanying psychological tendencies (e.g.,
and Singelis’s (1994) measure of IndSC and InterSC. Moreover, preferring engaged or disengaged emotions). Indeed, many of
there was little to no association of the tasks with each other. the specific dimensions of self-construal identified by Hardin et
In other words, people who described themselves as high in al. (2004) as underlying items on Singelis’s (1994) SCS (e.g.,
IndSC were no more likely than those low on this dimension primacy of self) map directly onto Kitayama et al.’s cultural tasks.
to engage in dispositional attribution, to focus on the target Given that Kitayama et al. (2009) argue explicitly that different

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 169

cultural tasks are associated with different psychological tenden- or ways of thinking (or mind-sets; Oyserman et al., 2009) are
cies, we would expect weak correlations, at best, between broad primed, not cultures. The danger in calling these latter proce-
measures of cultural tasks (e.g., explicit measures such as the dures cultural priming is that it equates culture with self-con-
Singelis SCS scales) and the specific measures of psychological strual or mind-set and ignores the fact that these are just two of
tendencies. Instead, specific psychological tendencies (e.g., cul- the many ways culture shapes individual behavior. Moreover,
tural differences in dispositional attribution) may be better the distinction made by Kitayama et al. (2009) among very
explained by domain- specific measures (e.g., beliefs in attitude– basic cognitive and affective processes (their psychological
behavior consistency; see Y. Kashima, Siegal, Tanaka, & Kashima, tendencies), complex behavioral goals (their cultural tasks),
1992). Likewise, Kashima (2009) argues that the commonly used and the cultural mandates of individualistic and collectivist
priming tasks induce specific ways of thinking or feeling that are societies reminds us that the global concept of culture cannot
associated with particular behaviors but not with others. The be adequately represented by a very specific way of thinking.
reason that the pronoun circling priming task promotes attention In our view, equating culture with self-construal or mind-set
to the target rather than the context (as in the Kühnen et al., 2001, (or equating self-construal with a specific psychological ten-
studies), he contends, is that thinking about oneself as an indi- dency) can lead outsiders to view social or personality psycholo-
vidual prompts analytical processing of information among Eng- gists engaged in cultural psychology as naïve, uninformed, and
lish speakers. Thinking about oneself as “we,” in contrast, may overly narrow in our interests.
hinder analytical thinking. Cultural differences in processes that In addition, cross-cultural researchers should remember that
are unrelated to analytical thinking, however, may not be influ- our understanding of self-construal has developed out of com-
enced by “I” versus “we” primes (see Oyserman & Lee’s, 2008, parisons between Europeans or Americans and East Asians. Con-
review for the effects of different types of primes on cognitive sequently, they may not apply as well to Africans or Latin
and other outcomes). In short, when researchers seek to demon- Americans. Research in these understudied regions will help us
strate that a particular cultural difference in behavior is associated identify the culture-based assumptions that are largely implicit in
with self-construals, they would be wise to focus on the specific this work. For example, we wonder whether InterSC and holistic
dimension of IndSC, RelSC, or InterSC that accounts for the thinking are associated as strongly in Africa as in East Asia. Latin
behavior and to consider the specific contexts in which the behav- Americans are typically more expressive and emotive than East
ior develops. Asians; thus, the associations between InterSC and emotional
These issues raise the question of whether self-construal expression may be very different in Latin American cultures than
is best understood as the cause of a variety of behaviors or in East Asian societies. Similarly, InterSC is positively associated
is better viewed as a way to interpret cultural differences in with the self-criticism and social isolation dimensions of the self-
behavior. Kashima (2009) offers a compelling argument for compassion scale in Taiwan (which has a Confucian heritage) but
self-construal as an interpretive tool rather than a causal con- negatively associated with these dimensions in Thailand (which
struct (and he acknowledges as well that an interpretive tool has a strong Buddhist heritage; Neff, Pisitsungkagarn, & Hsieh,
can be a causal mechanism). In our view, this is similar to 2008). Just as English is spoken and written differently in the
saying that extraversion does not cause talkativeness so much United States and the United Kingdom, so also people in Africa,
as it is a way to interpret or describe that behavior. Yet just Latin America, or Middle Eastern societies may express IndSC,
as measures of extraversion have been productively used to RelSC, and Coll-InterSC with somewhat different “dialects.”
predict a wide variety of behaviors, so also many of the studies Self-construals represent how people view themselves with
reviewed here suggest that measures of self-construal are use- respect to other people—as separate, independent, and autono-
ful tools in the exploration of within-culture patterns of behav- mous; as related to individual friends, family, and coworkers;
ior. Likewise, priming techniques help researchers begin to or as part of larger social groups. This conceptualization has
approximate the ways that self-construal influences thinking generated considerable research and fruitful theories. Unfor-
and feeling. As better measures of self-construal are developed tunately, not all that research could be reviewed here, but we
and as priming techniques become more specific, some of have attempted to provide an overview of those efforts and a
these apparent anomalies may be resolved. snapshot of the current status of these constructs. We hope this
Researchers may best avoid dead ends and detours along serves as a useful tour book for researchers exploring this new
their routes if they are careful about how they describe their and promising terrain of the self.
work and recall how they got where they are now. Authors have
at times referred to the pronoun circling task or other common Acknowledgments
priming tasks as “cultural priming.” This term is most appro- We would like to thank Kari Terzino, Tsui-Feng Wu, and Meng Zhang,
priately applied to the cultural icons task devised by Y.-Y. Hong who offered helpful comments on this article.
and her colleagues (2000) and perhaps to studies that use lan-
guage as a prime with bicultural participants. For other primes, Declaration of Conflicting Interests
however, particular perceptions of the self (as an individual or The authors declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect
as a member of a group, or as similar to or different from others) to the authorship and/or publication of this article.

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


170 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Financial Disclosure/Funding assimilation and contrast. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for chology, 94, 1018-1032. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.1018
the authorship, and/or publication of this article: This article was Bond, M. H., & Cheung, T. (1983). College students’ spontaneous
completed while the first and third authors were supported by self-concept: The effect of culture among respondents in Hong
National Science Foundation Grant BCS0646360. Kong, Japan, and the United States. Jou­rnal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 14, 153-171. doi:10.1177/0022002183014002002
References Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., Jr., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking
Aaker, J. L. (2000). Accessibility or diagnosticity? Disentangling the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the
the influence of culture on persuasion processes and attitudes. elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 340-357. doi:10.1086/209567 79, 631-643. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.631
Aaker, J. L., & Lee, A. Y. (2001). “I” seek pleasures and “we” avoid Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Attributions in mar-
pains: The role of self-regulatory goals in information process- riage: Review and critique. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 3-33.
ing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 33-49. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.1.3
doi:10.1086/321946 Bresnahan, M. J., Levine, T. R., Shearman, S. M., Lee, S. Y.,
Aaker, J., & Schmitt, B. (2001). Culture-dependent assimilation and Park, C., & Kiyomiya, T. (2005). A multimethod multitrait
differentiation of the self—Preferences for consumption symbols validity assessment of self-construal in Japan, Korea, and the
in the United States and China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy- United States. Human Communication Research, 31, 33-59.
chology, 32, 561-576. doi:10.1177/0022022101032005003 doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2005.tb00864.x
Agrawal, N., & Maheswaran, D. (2005). The effects of self-construal Bresnahan, M. J., Ohashi, R., Liu, W. Y., Nebashi, R., & Liao, C.
and commitment on persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, (1999). A comparison of response styles in Singapore and
31, 841-849. doi:10.1086/426620 Taiwan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 342-358.
Aldwin, C., & Greenberger, E. (1987). Cultural differences in the pre- doi:10.1177/0022022199030003004
dictors of depression. American Journal of Community Psychology, Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On being the same and differ-
15, 789-813. doi:10.1007/BF00919803 ent at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by 17, 475-482. doi:10.1177/0146167291175001
the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. Jour- Brewer, M. B., & Chen, Y. (2007). Where (who) are collectives
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1621-1630. in collectivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individu-
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.6.1621 alism and collectivism. Psychological Review, 114, 133-151.
Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. (2002). The relational self: An interper- doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.133
sonal social-cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109, 619-645 Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this “we”?
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.619 Levels of collective identity and self representations.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M., & Nelson, G. (1991). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 83-93.
Close relationships as including other in the self. Jour- doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.1.83
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 241-253. Briley, D. A., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2001). Transitory determinants of
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.241 values and decisions: The utility (or nonutility) of individualism
Ashton-James, C., van Baaren, R. B., Chartrand, T. L., Decety, J., & and collectivism in understanding cultural differences. Social
Karremans, J. (2007). Mimicry and me: The impact of mimicry Cognition, 19, 197-227. doi:10.1521/soco.19.3.197.21474
on self-construal. Social Cognition, 25, 518-535. doi:10.1521/ Brockner, J., Chen, Y., Mannix, E. A., Leung, K., & Skarlicki, D. P.
soco.2007.25.4.518 (2000). Culture and procedural fairness: When the effects of what
Baumeister, R. F. (1986). Public self and private self. New York, you do depend on how you do it. Administrative Science Quarterly,
NY: Springer. 45, 138-159. doi:10.2307/2666982
Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Brockner, J., De Cremer, D., van den Bos, K., & Chen, Y. R. (2005).
Baumeister, R. F., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). What do men want? The influence of interdependent self-construal on procedural
Gender differences and two spheres of belongingness: Comment fairness effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
on Cross and Madson (1997). Psychological Bulletin, 122, 38-44. Processes, 96, 155-167. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.11.001
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.38 Brown, P., & Brown, S. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in lan­
Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Self-regulation and the guage usage. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
executive function of the self. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney Brutus, S., & Greguras, G. J. (2008). Self-construals, motivation,
(Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 197-217). New York, and feedback-seeking behaviors. International Journal of
NY: Guilford. Selection and Assessment, 16, 282-291. doi:10.1111/j.1468-
Berkel, L. A., & Constantine, M. G. (2005). Relational variables and 2389.2008.00434.x
life satisfaction in African American and Asian American college Bry, C., Follenfant, A., & Meyer, T. (2008). Blonde like me: When
women. Journal of College Counseling, 8, 5-13. self-construals moderate stereotype priming effects on intellec-
Blanton, H., & Stapel, D. A. (2008). Unconscious and spontaneous tual performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,
and . . . complex: The three selves model of social comparison 751-757. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.06.005

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 171

Byrne, D., & Blaylock, B. (1963). Similarity and assumed similarity well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,
of attitudes between husbands and wives. Journal of Abnormal 933-944. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.933
and Social Psychology, 67, 636-640. doi:10.1037/h0045531 Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-
Chang, E. C., & Asakawa, K. (2003). Cultural variations on optimis- construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5-37.
tic and pessimistic bias for self versus a sibling: Is there evidence doi:10.1037/0033-2909.122.1.5
for self-enhancement in the West and self-criticism in the East Cross, S. E., & Morris, M. L. (2003). Getting to know you: The
when the referent group is specified? Journal of Personality and relational self-construal, relational cognition, and well-being.
Social Psychology, 84, 569-581. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.569 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 512-523.
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: doi:10.1177/0146167202250920
The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Jour- Cross, S. E., Morris, M. L., & Gore, J. S. (2002). Thinking about one-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893-910. self and others: The relational–interdependent self-construal and
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893 social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social psychology,
Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W. W., & Lakin, J. L. (2005). Beyond the 82, 399-418. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.399
perception-behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pur-
moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & suits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psycho-
J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought 2: The new unconscious logical Inquiry, 11, 227-268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01
(pp. 334-361). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Deiner, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The
Chen, S., Lee-Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). Relationship Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49,
orientation as a moderator of the effects of social power. 71-75. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 173-187. Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Implicit self and identity. In
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.173 M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity
Chen, Y., Chen, X., & Portnoy, R. (2009). To whom do positive (pp. 153-175). New York, NY: Guilford.
norm and negative norm of reciprocity apply? Effects of ineq- Earley, P. C. (1993). East meets West meets Mideast: Further explo-
uitable offer, relationship, and relational-self orientation. Jour- rations of collectivistic and individualistic work groups. Academy
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 24-34. doi:10.1016/j of Management Journal, 36, 319-348. doi:10.2307/256525
.jesp.2008.07.024 Eldridge, K. A., & Christensen, A. (2002). Demand-withdraw commu-
Chen, Y.-R., Brockner, J., & Katz, T. (1998). Toward an explanation nication during couple conflict: A review and analysis. New York,
of cultural differences in in-group favoritism: The role of individ- NY: Cambridge University Press.
ual versus collective primacy. Journal of Personality and Social Elliott, I., & Coker, S. (2008). Independent self-construal, self-
Psychology, 75, 1490-1502. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1490 reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting
Cheng, R. W., & Lam, S. (2007). Self-construal and social com- happiness. Australian Journal of Psychology, 60, 127-134.
parison effects. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, doi:10.1080/00049530701447368
197-211. doi:10.1348/000709905X72795 Ellis, J. B., & Wittenbaum, G. M. (2000). Relationships between
Church, T. A., Anderson-Harumi, C. A., del Prado, A. M., self-construal and verbal promotion. Communication Research,
Curtis, G. J., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., Valdez Medina, J.,  .  .  .  27, 704-722. doi:10.1177/009365000027006002
Katigbak, M. S. (2008). Culture, cross-role consistency, and English, T., & Chen, S. (2007). Culture and self-concept stability:
adjustment: Testing trait and cultural psychology perspectives. Consistency across and within contexts among Asian Americans
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 739-755. and European Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.739 chology, 93, 478-490. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.478
Clark, M. S., Oullette, R., Powell, M. C., & Milberg, S. (1987). Recipi- Fischer, A., Manstead, A. S. R., & Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M.
ent’s mood, relationship type, and helping. Journal of Personality (1999). The role of honor-related vs. individualistic values in
and Social Psychology, 53, 94-103. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.94 conceptualizing pride, shame, and anger: Spanish and Dutch
Cole, P. M., Bruschi, C. J., & Tamang, B. L. (2002). Cultural dif- cultural prototypes. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 149-179.
ferences in children’s emotional reactions to difficult situations. doi:10.1080/026999399379311
Child Development, 73, 983-996. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00451 Gabriel, S., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Are there “his” and “hers”
Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the types of interdependence? The implications of gender differences
United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, in collective versus relational interdependence for affect, behav-
124-131. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.124 ior, and cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational– 77, 642-655. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.642
interdependent self-construal and relationships. Journal Gahan, P., & Abeysekera, L. (2009). What shapes an individual’s
of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 791-808. work values? An integrated model of the relationship between
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.791 work values, national culture, and self-construal. Interna-
Cross, S. E., Gore, J. S., & Morris, M. L. (2003). The relational– tional Journal of Human Resource Management, 20, 126-147.
interdependent self-construal, self-concept consistency, and doi:10.1080/09585190802528524

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


172 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Dean, K. K. (2004). The individual as Gudykunst, W. B., & Lee, C. M. (2003). Assessing the validity of
“melting pot”: The flexibility of bicultural self-construals. Cahiers self-construal scales: A response to Levine et al. Human Com-
de Psychologie Cognitive, 22, 181-201. munication Research, 29, 253-274. doi:10.1093/hcr/29.2.253
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Hochschild, L. (2002). When you and Gudykunst, W. B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., Nishida, T.,
I are “we,” you are not threatening: The role of self-expansion in Kim, K., & Heyman, H. (1996). The influence of cultural indi-
social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vidualism-collectivism, self-construals, and individual values on
82, 239-251. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.2.239 communication styles across cultures. Human Communication
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). “I” value freedom, Research, 22, 510-543. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1996.tb00377.x
but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cul- Guimond, S., Chatard, A., Martinot, D., Crisp, R. J., & Redersdorff, S.
tural differences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10, 321-326. (2006). Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differ-
doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00162 ences in self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Gardner, W. L., & Seeley, E. A. (2001). Confucius, “Jen,” and the chology, 90, 221-242. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.221
benevolent use of power. In A. Y. Lee-Chai & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Guo, X., Schwartz, S. J., & McCabe, B. E. (2008). Aging, gender,
The use and abuse of power (pp. 263-280). Philadelphia, PA: and self: Dimensionality and measurement invariance analysis on
Psychology Press. self-construal. Self and Identity, 7, 1-24.
Geertz, C. (1975). On the nature of anthropological understanding. Haberstroh, S., Oyserman, D., Schwarz, N., Kühnen, U., & Ji, L.
American Scientist, 63, 47-53. (2002). Is the interdependent self more sensitive to question con-
Gelfand, M. J., Major, V. S., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L. H., & O’Brien, K. text than the independent self? Self-construal and the observa-
(2006). Negotiating relationally: The dynamics of the relational tion of conversational norms. Journal of Experimental Social
self in negotiations. Academy of Management Review, 31, 427-451. Psychology, 38, 323-329. doi:10.1006/jesp.2001.1513
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and Hackman, M. Z., Ellis, K., Johnson, C. E., & Staley, C. (1999). Self-
women’s development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. construal orientation: Validation of an instrument and a study of
Gollwitzer, M., & Bücklein, K. (2007). Are “we” more punitive than the relationship to leadership communication style. Communica-
“me”? Self-construal styles, justice-related attitudes, and punitive tion Quarterly, 47, 183-195.
judgments. Social Justice Research, 20, 457-478. doi:10.1007/ Hamaguchi, E. (1985). A contextual model of the Japanese: Toward a
s11211-007-0051-y methodological innovation in Japan studies. Journal of Japanese
Gore, J. S., & Cross, S. E. (2006). Pursuing goals for us: Rela- Studies, 11, 289-321.
tionally autonomous reasons in long-term goal pursuit. Jour- Hamilton, R. W., & Biehal, G. J. (2005). Achieving your goals
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 848-861. or protecting their future? The effects of self-view on goals
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.848 and choices. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 277-283.
Gore, J. S., & Cross, S. E. (in press). Conflicts of interest: Relational doi:10.1086/432237
self-construal and decision-making in interpersonal contexts. Hannover, B., Birkner, N., & Pöhlmann, C. (2006). Ideal selves and
Self and Identity. self-esteem in people with independent or interdependent self-
Gore, J. S., Cross, S. E., & Kanagawa, C. (2009). Acting in our interests: construal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 119-133.
Relational self-construal and goal motivation across cultures. Moti- doi:10.1002/ejsp.289
vation and Emotion, 33, 75-87. doi:10.1007/s11031-008-9113-1 Hannover, B., & Kühnen, U. (2005). Culture, context, and cognition:
Gore, J. S., Cross, S. E., & Morris, M. L. (2006). Let’s be friends: Rela- The semantic procedural interface model of the self. European
tional self-construal and the development of intimacy. Personal Review of Social Psychology, 15, 297-333.
Relationships, 13, 83-102. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2006.00106.x Hannover, B., Pöhlmann, C., Springer, A., & Roeder, U. (2005).
Green, M. A., Scott, N. A., DeVilder, E. L., Zeiger, A., & Darr, S. Implications of independent versus interdependent self-
(2006). Relational–interdependent self-construal as a function knowledge for motivated social cognition: The semantic pro-
of bulimic symptomatology. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, cedural interface model of the self. Self and Identity, 4, 159-175.
943-951. doi:10.1002/jclp.20270 doi:10.1080/13576500444000245
Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Harb, C., & Smith, P. B. (2008). Self-Construals across cultures:
Association Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Beyond independence–interdependence. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022-1038. Psychology, 39, 178-197. doi:10.1177/0022022107313861
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022 Hardin, E. E. (2006). Convergent evidence for the multidimension-
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Mea- ality of self-construal. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37,
suring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit 516-521. doi:10.1177/0022022106290475
association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Hardin, E. E., Cross, S. E., & Hoang, U. (2009). [Development of
74, 1464-1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464 the Multidimensional Self-Construal Scale (MDSCS)]. Unpub-
Greenwald, A. G., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1984). The self. In R. S. lished raw data.
Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, Hardin, E. E., Leong, F. T. L., & Bhagwat, A. A. (2004). Factor
pp. 129-178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. structure of the Self-Construal Scale revisited: Implications for

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 173

the multidimensionality of self-construal. Journal of Cross- Holland, R. W., Roeder, U., van Baaren, R. B., Brandt, A. C., &
Cultural Psychology, 35, 327-345. doi:10.1177/0022022104 Hannover, B. (2004). Don’t stand so close to me: The effects of
264125 self-construal on interpersonal closeness. Psychological Science,
Hardin, E. E., Leong, F. T. L., & Osipow, S. H. (2001). Cultural 15, 237-242. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00658.x
relativity in the conceptualization of career maturity. Journal of Holmvall, C. M., & Bobocel, D. R. (2008). What fair procedures say
Vocational Behavior, 58, 36-52. doi:10.1006/jvbe.2000.1762 about me: Self-construals and reactions to procedural fairness.
Hardin, E. E., Varghese, F. V., Tran, U. V., & Carlson, A. Z. (2006). Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 105,
Anxiety and career exploration: Gender differences in the role 147-168. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.09.001
of self-construal. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 69, 346-358. Holmvall, C. M., & Sidhu, J. (2007). Predicting customer ser-
doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2006.05.002 vice employees’ job satisfaction and turnover intentions: The
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). roles of customer interactional justice and interdependent self-
Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psycho- construal. Social Justice Research, 20, 479-496. doi:10.1007/
logical Science, 19, 12-17. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02038.x s11211-007-0049-5
Heine, S. J., & Hamamura, T. (2007). In search of East Asian self- Holyoak, K. J., & Gordon, P. C. (1983). Social reference points.
enhancement. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 881-887.
4-27. doi:10.1177/1088868306294587 doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.5.881
Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., & Lehman, D. R. (2001). Cultural differ- Hong, J. J., & Woody, S. R. (2007). Cultural mediators of self-
ences in self-evaluation: Japanese readily accept negative self- reported social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45,
relevant information. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 1779-1789. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.01.011
434-443. doi:10.1177/0022022101032004004 Hong, Y.-Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., & Benet-Martinez, V.
Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach
Leung, C., & Matsumoto, H. (2001). Divergent consequences to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709-720.
of success and failure in Japan and North America: An inves- doi:10.1037/0003-066X.55.7.709
tigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves. Hoshino-Browne, E., Zanna, A. S., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P.,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599-615. Kitayama, S., & Lackenbauer, S. (2005). On the cultural guises
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.599 of cognitive dissonance: The case of Easterners and Western-
Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1995). Cultural variation in unre- ers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 294-310.
alistic optimism: Does the West feel more vulnerable than the doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.294
East? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 595-607. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA:
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.4.595 Harvard University Press (reprinted 1983).
Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1997). Culture, dissonance, and Ji, L., Zhang, Z., & Nisbett, R. E. (2004). Is it culture or is it language?
self-affirmation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on
23, 389-400. doi:10.1177/0146167297234005 categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). 87, 57-65. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.57
Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Johnson, R. E., Selenta, C., & Lord, R. G. (2006). When organizational
Review, 106, 766-794. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.766 justice and the self-concept meet: Consequences for the organi-
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K., & Greenholtz, J. (2002). zation and its members. Organizational Behavior and Human
What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Lik- Decision Processes, 99, 175-201. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.07.005
ert scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Jordan, J. V., & Surrey, J. L. (1986). The self-in-relation: Empathy and
Social Psychology, 82, 903-918. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.903 the mother-daughter relationship. In T. Bernay & D. W. Cantor
Heine, S. J., Takata, T., & Lehman, D. R. (2000). Beyond self- (Eds.), The psychology of today’s women (pp. 81-104). Cambridge,
presentation: Evidence for self-criticism among Japanese. MA: Harvard University Press.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 71-78. Kağitçibaşi, C. (2005). Autonomy and relatedness in cultural con-
doi:10.1177/0146167200261007 text: Implications for self and family. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Higgins, E. T. (1996). The “self-digest”: Self-knowledge serving Psychology, 36, 403-422. doi:10.1177/0022022105275959
self-regulatory functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Kağitçibaşi, C. (2007). Family, self, and human development
chology, 71, 1062-1083. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1062 across cultures: Theory and applications (2nd ed.). Mahwah,
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychol- NJ: Erlbaum.
ogist, 52, 1280-1300. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.12.1280 Kanagawa, C., Cross, S. E., & Markus, H. R. (2001). “Who am
Higgins, E. T., & Silberman, I. (1998). Development of regula- I?” The cultural psychology of the conceptual self. Personal-
tory focus: Promotion and prevention as ways of living. In ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 90-103. doi:10.1177/
J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Motivation and self- 0146167201271008
regulation across the life span (pp. 79-113). New York, NY: Kashima, E. S., & Hardie, E. A. (2000). The development and vali-
Cambridge University Press. dation of the Relational, Individual, and Collective Self-Aspects

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


174 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

(RIC) Scale. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 19-48. interactive constraints. International Journal of Intercultural Rela-
doi:10.1111/1467-839X.00053 tions, 18, 117-140. doi:10.1016/0147-1767(94)90008-6
Kashima, Y. (2009). Culture comparison and culture priming: A crit- Kim, Y., Kasser, T., & Lee, H. (2003). Self-concept, aspirations,
ical analysis. In R. S. Wyer, C. Chiu, & Y. Hong (Eds.), Under- and well-being in South Korea and the United States. Journal of
standing culture: Theory, research, and application (pp. 53-77). Social Psychology, 143, 277-290.
New York, NY: Psychology Press. Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., & Uchida, Y. (2007). Self as cultural mode of
Kashima, Y., Kashima, E., Farsides, T., Kim, U., Strack, F., being. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of cultural
Werth, L., & Yuki, M. (2004). Culture and context-sensitive psychology (pp. 136-174). New York, NY: Guilford.
self: The amount and meaning of context-sensitivity of phe- Kitayama, S., Karasawa, M., & Mesquita, B. (2004). Collective and
nomenal self differ across cultures. Self and Identity, 3, 125-141. personal processes in regulating emotions: Emotion and self in
doi:10.1080/13576500342000095 Japan and the U.S. In P. Philippot & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), The
Kashima, Y., Siegal, M., Tanaka, K., & Kashima, E. S. (1992). Do regulation of emotion (pp. 251-273). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
people believe behaviours are consistent with attitudes? Towards Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V.
a cultural psychology of attribution processes. British Journal of (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of
Social Psychology, 31, 111-124. the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism
Kashima, Y., Yamaguchi, S., Kim, U., Choi, S., Gelfand, M. J., in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
& Yuki, M. (1995). Culture, gender, and self: A perspective 1245-1267. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1245
from individualism-collectivism research. Journal of Per- Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2006). Cultural
sonality and Social Psychology, 69, 925-937. doi:10.1037/ affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging
0022-3514.69.5.925 and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States.
Keller, J., & Molix, L. (2008). When women can’t do math: The inter- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 890-903.
play of self-construal, group identification, and stereotypic perfor- doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.890
mance standards. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K.
437-444. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.01.007 (2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Com-
Kemmelmeier, M., & Cheng, B. Y. (2004). Language and self- paring North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal
construal priming: A replication and extension in a Hong Kong of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 236-255. doi:10.1037/
sample. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 705-712. a0015999
doi:10.1177/0022022104270112 Kitayama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R., & Suzuki, T. (2004). Is there
Kemmelmeier, M., & Oyserman, D. (2001). The ups and downs of any “free” choice? Self and dissonance in two cultures. Psychologi-
thinking about a successful other: Self-construals and the con- cal Science, 15, 527-533. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00714.x
sequences of social comparisons. European Journal of Social Kitayama, S., & Uchida, Y. (2005). Interdependent agency: An
Psychology, 31(3), 311-320. doi:10.1002/ejsp.47 alternative system for action. In R. M. Sorrentino, D. Cohen,
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and J. M. Olson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Cultural and social behavior:
health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 472-503. The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 10, pp. 137-164). Hillsdale, NJ:
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472 Lawrence Erlbaum.
Kim, H., & Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony Klar, Y., & Giladi, E. E. (1997). No one in my group can be below
or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and the group’s average: A robust positivity bias in favor of anony-
Social Psychology, 77, 785-800. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.4.785 mous peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,
Kim, J., Kim, M., Kam, K. Y., & Shin, H. (2003). Influence of 885-901. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.885
self-construals on the perception of different self-presentation Klar, Y., & Giladi, E. E. (1999). Are most people happier than they
styles in Korea. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 6, 89-101. peers, or are they just happy? Personality and Social Psychology
doi:10.1111/1467-839X.t01-1-00013 Bulletin, 25, 585-594. doi:10.1177/0146167299025005004
Kim, K., Grimm, L. R., & Markman, A. B. (2007). Self-construal Kleinknecht, R. A., Dinnel, D. L., Kleinknecht, E. E., Hiruma, N.,
and the processing of covariation information in causal reasoning. & Hirada, B. A. (1997). Cultural factors in social anxiety:
Memory & Cognition, 35, 1337-1343. A comparison of social phobia symptoms and Taijin Kyo-
Kim, M., Aune, K. S., Hunter, J. E., Kim, H., & Kim, J. (2001). The fusho. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 11, 157-177. doi:10.1016/
effect of culture and self-construals on predispositions toward S0887-6185(97)00004-2
verbal communication. Human Communication Research, 27, Konrath, S., Bushman, B. J., & Grove, T. (2009). Seeing my world
382-408. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2001.tb00786.x in a million little pieces: Narcissism, self-construal, and cog-
Kim, M.-S., & Raja, N. S. (2003). When validity testing lacks validity: nitive-perceptual style. Journal of Personality, 77, 1197-1228.
Comment on Levine et al. Human Communication Research, 29, doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00579.x
275-290. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2003.tb00839.x Krishna, A., Zhou, R., & Zhang, S. (2008). The effect of self-
Kim, M.-S., Sharkey, W. F., & Singelis, T. M. (1994). The relationships construal on spatial judgments. Journal of Consumer Research,
between individuals’ self-construals and perceived importance of 35, 337-348. doi:10.1086/588686

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 175

Krull, D. S., Loy, M. H., Lin, J., Wang, C., Chen, S., & Zhao, X. Levine, T. R., Bresnahan, M. J., Park, H. S., Lapinski, M. K.,
(1999). The fundamental fundamental attribution error: Cor- Wittenbaum, G. M., Shearman, S. M., . . . Ohashi, R. (2003). Self-
respondence bias in individualist and collectivist cultures. construal scales lack validity. Human Communication Research,
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1208-1219. 29, 210-252. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2003.tb00837.x
doi:10.1177/0146167299258003 Lewis, R. S., Goto, S. G., & Kong, L. L. (2008). Culture and
Kuhn, M. H., & McPartland, T. (1954). An empirical investiga- context: East Asian American and European American differ-
tion of self-attitudes. American Sociological Review, 19, 58-76. ences in P3 event-related potentials and self-construal. Person-
doi:10.2307/2088175 ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 623-634. doi:10.1177/
Kühnen, U., & Haberstroh, S. (2004). Self-construal activation and 0146167207313731
focus of comparison as determinants of assimilation and contrast in Lin, Z., & Han, S. (2009). Self-construal priming modulates the
social comparisons. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 22, 289-310. scope of visual attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Kühnen, U., & Hannover, B. (2000). Assimilation and contrast in social Psychology, 62, 802-813. doi:10.1080/17470210802271650
comparisons as a consequence of self-construal activation. Euro- Lin, Z., Lin, Y., & Han, S. (2008). Self-construal priming modu-
pean Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 799-811. doi:10.1002/1099- lates visual activity underlying global/local perception. Biologi-
0992(200011/12)30:6<799::AID-EJSP16>3.0.CO;2-2 cal Psychology, 77, 93-97. doi:10.1016/j.bio­psycho.2007.08.002
Kühnen, U., Hannover, B., & Schubert, B. (2001). The semantic- Liu, F. F., & Goto, S. G. (2007). Self-construal, mental distress,
procedural interface model of the self: The role of self-knowledge and family relations: A mediated moderation analysis with Asian
for context-dependent versus context-independent modes of think- American adolescents. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority
ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 397-409. Psychology, 13, 134-142. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.13.2.134
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.397 Locke, K. D., & Christensen, L. (2007). Re-construing the
Kühnen, U., & Oyserman, D. (2002). Thinking about the self influ- relational–interdependent self-construal and its relationship
ences thinking in general: Cognitive consequences of salient with self-consistency. Journal of Research in Personality, 41,
self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 389-402. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.04.005
492-499. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00011-2 Lun, J., Kesebir, S., & Oishi, S. (2008). On feeling understood and
Kuo, W. H. (1984). Prevalence of depression among Asian- feeling well: The role of interdependence. Journal of Research in
Americans. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 172, 449-457. Personality, 42, 1623-1628. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.06.009
doi:10.1097/00005053-198408000-00002 Lydon, J. E., Menzies-Toman, D., Burton, K., & Bell, C. (2008). If-
Kurman, J. (2001). Self-enhancement: Is it restricted to individualistic then contingencies and the differential effects of the availability
cultures? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1705-1716. of an attractive alternative on relationship maintenance for men
doi:10.1177/01461672012712013 and women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,
Kwan, V. S. Y., Bond, M. H., & Singelis, T. M. (1997). Pancultural 50-65. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.50
explanations for life satisfaction: Adding relationship harmony to Mandel, N. (2003). Shifting selves and decision making: The effects
self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, of self-construal priming on consumer risk-taking. Journal of
1038-1051. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.1038 Consumer Research, 30, 30-40. doi:10.1086/374700
Lalwani, A. K., & Shavitt, S. (2009). The “me” I claim to be: Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Impli-
Cultural self-construal elicits self-presentational goal pursuit. cations for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological
Unpublished manuscript, University of Texas at San Antonio. Review, 98, 224-253. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
Lam, A. G., & Zane, N. W. S. (2004). Ethnic differences in coping Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2004). Models of agency: Socio-
with interpersonal stressors: A test of self-construals as cultural cultural diversity in the construction of action. In V. Murphy-
mediators. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 446-459. Berman & J. J. Berman (Eds.), Cross-cultural differences in
doi:10.1177/0022022104266108 perspectives on the self: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation
Lam, B. T. (2005). Self-construal and depression among Vietnamese- (Vol. 49, pp. 1-57). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
American adolescents. International Journal of Intercultural Markus, H., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept: A social
Relations, 29, 239-250. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.05.007 psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38,
Leary, M. R. (2007). Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. 299-337. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.001503
Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 317-344. doi:10.1146/annurev Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus
.psych.58.110405.085658 analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and
Leary, M. R., & Tangney, J. P. (Eds.). (2003). Handbook of self and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
identity. New York, NY: Guilford. 922-934. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.922
Lee, A. Y., Aaker, J. L., & Gardner, W. L. (2000). The pleasures and Matsumoto, D. (1999). Culture and self: An empirical assessment
pains of distinct self-construals: The role of interdependence in of Markus and Kitayama’s theory of independent and interde-
regulatory focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, pendent self-construal. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2,
78, 1122-1134. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.6.1122 289-310. doi:10.1111/1467-839X.00042

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


176 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social Okazaki, S. (1997). Sources of ethnic differences between Asian
explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, American and White American college students on measures of
961-978. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.5.961 depression and social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural prac- 106, 52-60. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.52
tices emphasize influence in the United States and adjustment in Oyserman, D. (1993). The lens of personhood: Viewing the self and
Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 311-323. others in a multicultural society. Journal of Personality and Social
doi:10.1177/0146167202286003 Psychology, 65, 993-1009. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.993
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American Oyserman, D. (2006). High power, lower power and equality: Cul-
and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal ture beyond individualism and collectivism. Journal of Consumer
of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949-971. doi:10.1037/ Psychology, 16, 352-356. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1604_6
0022-3514.67.6.949 Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethink-
Morry, M. M. (2005). Allocentrism and friendship satisfaction: ing individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical
The mediating roles of disclosure and closeness. Canadian assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3-72.
Journal of Behavioural Science, 37, 211-222. doi:10.1037/ doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3
h0087258 Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influ-
Moskowitz, G. B., & Roman, R. J. (1992). Spontaneous trait infer- ence what and how we think? Effects of priming individual-
ences as self-generated primes: Implications for conscious social ism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311-342.
judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311
728-738. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.5.728 Oyserman, D., Sorensen, N., Reber, R., & Chen, S. X. (2009). Con-
Neff, K. D., Pisitsungkagarn, K., & Hsieh, Y.-P. (2008). Self- necting and separating mind-sets: Culture as situated cognition.
compassion and self-construal in the United States, Thailand, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 217-235.
and Taiwan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 267-285. doi:10.1037/a0015850
doi:10.1177/0022022108314544 Oyserman, D., & Uskul, A. K. (2008). Individualism and collectiv-
Nezlek, J. B., Kafetsios, K., & Smith, C. V. (2008). Emotions in ism: societal-level processes with implications for individual-
everyday social encounters: Correspondence between culture and level and society-level outcomes. In F. J. R. van de Vijver,
self-construal. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 366-372. D. A. van Hemert, & Y. H. Poortinga (Eds.), Multilevel analysis
doi:10.1177/0022022108318114 of individuals and cultures (pp. 145-174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Ozawa, K., Crosby, M., & Crosby, F. (1996). Individualism and
Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic resistance to affirmative action: A comparison of Japanese and
cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291-310. doi:10.1037/0033- American samples. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26,
295X.108.2.291 1138-1152. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1996.tb02289.x
Noguchi, K. (2007). Examination of the content of individualism/ Pöhlmann, C., Carranza, E., Hannover, B., & Iyengar, S. S. (2007).
collectivism scales in cultural comparisons of the USA and Japan. Repercussions of self-construal for self-relevant and other-
Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 131-144. relevant choice. Social Cognition, 25, 284-305. doi:10.1521/
Oetzel, J. G. (1998). Explaining individual communication pro- soco.2007.25.2.284
cesses in homogeneous and heterogeneous groups through Pöhlmann, C., & Hannover, B. (2006). Who shapes the self of inde-
individualism-collectivism and self-construal. Human Commu- pendents and interdependents? Explicit and implicit measures
nication Research, 25, 202-224. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1998. of the self’s relatedness to family, friends and partner. European
tb00443.x Journal of Personality, 20, 525-547. doi:10.1002/per.599
Oetzel, J. G., & Ting-Toomey, S. (2003). Face concerns in inter- Polyorat, K., & Alden, D. L. (2005). Self-construal and need-for-
personal conflict: A cross-cultural empirical test of face cognition effects on brand attitudes and purchase intentions in
negation theory. Communication Research, 30, 599-624. response to comparative advertising in Thailand and the United
doi:10.1177/0093650203257841 States. Journal of Advertising, 34, 37-48.
Oetzel, J., Ting-Toomey, S., Masumoto, T., Yokochi, Y., Pan, X., Random House Dictionary. (1980). New York: Ballantine Books.
Takai, J., & Wilcox, R. (2001). Face and facework in conflict: Reid, A. (2004). Gender and sources of subjective well-being. Sex
A cross-cultural comparison of China, German, Japan, and Roles, 51, 617-629. doi:10.1007/s11199-004-0714-1
the United States. Communication Monographs, 68, 235-258. Rhee, E., Uleman, J. S., Lee, H. K., & Roman, R. J. (1995). Spon-
doi:10.1080/03637750128061 taneous self-descriptions and ethnic identities in individualistic
Oishi, S., Wyer, R., & Colcombe, S. (2000). Cultural variation and collectivistic cultures. Journal of Personality and Social
in the use of current life satisfaction to predict the future. Psychology, 69, 142-152. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.142
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 434-445. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.434 Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 177

Ross, M., Xun, W. Q. E., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). Language and the Singelis, T. M., Bond, M. H., Sharkey, W. F., & Lai, C. S. Y. (1999).
bicultural self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, Unpackaging culture’s influence on self-esteem and embarrass-
1040-1050. doi:10.1177/01461672022811003 ability: The role of self-construals. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Sato, T., & Cameron, J. E. (1999). The relationship between collec- Psychology, 30, 315-341. doi:10.1177/0022022199030003003
tive self-esteem and self-construal in Japan and Canada. Journal Singelis, T. M., & Brown, W. J. (1995). Culture, self, and col-
of Social Psychology, 139, 426-435. lectivist communication: Linking culture to individual
Sato, T., & McCann, D. (1998). Individual differences in related- behavior. Human Communication Research, 21, 354-389.
ness and individuality: An exploration of two constructs. Per- doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1995.tb00351.x
sonality and Individual Differences, 24, 847-859. doi:10.1016/ Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D., & Gelfand, M. J.
S0191-8869(98)00020-8 (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and
Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-
Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well- Cultural Research: Journal of Comparative Social Science, 29,
being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Jour- 240-275. doi:10.1177/106939719502900302
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 582-593. Somech, A. (2000). The independent and the interdependent
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.4.582 selves: Different meanings in different cultures. International
Schmeichel, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Self-regulatory Journal of Intercultural Relations, 24, 161-172. doi:10.1016/
strength. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of S0147-1767(99)00030-9
self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 84-98). Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The Personal
New York, NY: Guilford. Attributes Questionnaire: A measure of sex-role stereotypes and
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of masculinity-femininity. Journal Supplement Abstract Service
values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 4, 43-44.
In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology Stapel, D. A., & Koomen, W. (2001). I, we, and the effects of oth-
(pp. 1-62). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. ers on me: How self-construal level moderates social comparison
Sedikides, C., & Brewer, M. B. (Eds.). (2001). Individual self, rela- effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 766-781.
tional self, collective self. New York, NY: Psychology Press. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.766
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural self- Stapel, D. A., & van der Zee, K. I. (2006). The self-salience model of
enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, other-to-self effects: Integrating principles of self-enhancement,
60-79. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.60 complementarity, and imitation. Journal of Personality and
Seeley, E. A., & Gardner, W. L. (2003). The “selfless” and self- Social Psychology, 90, 258-271. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.258
regulation: The role of chronic other-orientation in avert- Stewart, A. J., & Lykes, M. B. (1985). Conceptualizing gender in
ing self-regulatory depletion. Self and Identity, 2, 103-117. personality theory and research. In A. J. Stewart & M. B. Lykes
doi:10.1080/15298860309034 (Eds.), Gender and personality: Current perspectives on theory
Seeley Howard, E. A., Gardner, W. L., & Thompson, L. (2007). and research (pp. 2-13). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
The role of the self-concept and the social context in determin- Strack, F., Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Kubler, A., & Wanke, M. (1993).
ing the behavior of power holders: Self-construal in intergroup Awareness of the influence as a determinant of assimilation ver-
versus dyadic dispute resolution negotiations. Journal of Per- sus contrast. European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 53-62.
sonality and Social Psychology, 93, 614-631. doi:10.1037/ doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420230105
0022-3514.93.4.614 Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective
Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., & Friedman, R. S. (1998). Performance well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
incentives and means: How regulatory focus influences goal 1378-1391. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1378
attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Triandis, H. C. (1998). The shifting
285-293. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.285 basis of life satisfaction judgments across cultures: Emotions
Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1984). Does the concept of the versus norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
person vary cross-culturally? In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine 494-512. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.482
(Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion Suh, E. M., Diener, E., & Updegraff, J. A. (2008). From culture
(pp. 158-199). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. to priming conditions: Self-construal influences on life satisfac-
Sinclair, L., & Fehr, B. (2005). Voice vs. loyalty: Self-construals and tion judgments. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 3-15.
responses to dissatisfaction in romantic relationships. Journal doi:10.1177/0022022107311769
of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 298-304. doi:10.1016/j Sui, J., & Han, S. (2007). Self-construal priming modulates neural
.jesp.2004.06.004 substrates of self-awareness. Psychological Science, 18, 861-866.
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and inter- doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01992.x
dependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Takano, Y., & Osaka, E. (1999). An unsupported common view:
Bulletin, 20, 580-591. doi:10.1177/0146167294205014 Comparing Japan and the U.S. on individualism/collectivism.

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


178 Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(2)

Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 311-341. doi:10.1111/ Uskul, A. K., & Hynie, M. (2007). Self-construal and concerns elic-
1467-839X.00043 ited by imagines and real health problems. Journal of Applied
Tangney, J. P., & Leary, M. R. (2003). The next generation of self- Social Psychology, 37, 2156-2189. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816
research. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self .2007.00283.x
and identity (pp. 667-674). New York, NY: Guilford. Utz, S. (2004). Self-construal and cooperation: Is the interdepen-
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A dent self more cooperative than the independent self? Self and
social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Identity, 3, 177-190. doi:10.1080/13576500444000001
Bulletin, 103, 193-210. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193 van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Steenaert, B., & van Knippenberg,
Terzino, K. A., & Cross, S. E. (2009). Predicting commitment in A. (2003). Mimicry for money: Behavioral consequences of imita-
new relationships: Interactive effects of relational self-construal tion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(4), 393-398.
and power. Self and Identity, 8, 321-341. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00014-3
Tesser, A. (1980). Self-esteem maintenance in family dynamics. van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T. L., de
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 77-91. Bouter, C., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). It takes two to
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.1.77 mimic: Behavioral consequences of self-construals. Jour-
Tesser, A., Crepaz, N., Beach, S. R. H., Cornell, D., & Collins, J. C. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1093-1102.
(2000). Confluence of self-esteem regulation mechanisms: On doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1093
integrating the self-zoo. Personality and Social Psychology van de Vijver, F. J. R., van Hemert, D. A., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2008).
Bulletin, 26, 1476-1489. doi:10.1177/01461672002612003 Conceptual issues in multilevel models. In F. J. R. van de Vijver,
Thomsen, L., Sidanius, J., & Fiske, A. P. (2007). Interpersonal level- D. A. van Hemert, & Y. H. Poortinga (Eds.), Multilevel analysis
ing, independence, and self-enhancement: A comparison between of individuals and cultures (pp. 3-26). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Denmark and the US, and a relational practice framework for Erlbaum.
cultural psychology. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, van Horen, F., Pöhlmann, C., Koeppen, K., & Hannover, B. (2008).
445-469. doi:10.1002/ejsp.366 Importance of personal goals in people with independent ver-
Ting-Toomey, S. (1988). Intercultural conflict styles: A face-negotiation sus interdependent selves. Social Psychology, 39, 213-221.
theory. In Y. Y. Kim & W. Gudykunst (Eds.), Theories in intercul- doi:10.1027/1864-9335.39.4.213
tural communication (pp. 213-235). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. van Knippenberg, D., van Knippenberg, B., De Cremer, D., &
Ting-Toomey, S., Oetzel, J. G., & Yee-Jung, K. (2001). Self-construal Hogg, M. A. (2004). Leadership, self, and identity: A review
types and conflict management styles. Communication Reports, and research agenda. Leadership Quarterly, 15, 825-856.
14, 87-104. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2004.09.002
Torelli, C. J. (2006). Individuality or conformity? The effect of inde- Verplanken, B., Trafimow, D., Khusid, I. K., Holland, R. W., &
pendent and interdependent self-concepts on public judgments. Steentjes, G. M. (2009). Different selves, different values: Effects
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 16, 240-248. doi:10.1207/ of self-construals on value activation and use. European Journal
s15327663jcp1603_6 of Social Psychology, 39, 909-919. doi:10.1002/ejsp.587
Trafimow, D., & Finlay, K. A. (1996). The importance of subjective Vorauer, J. D., & Cameron, J. J. (2002). So close, and yet so far:
norms for a minority of people: Between-subjects and within- Does collectivism foster transparency overestimation? Jour-
subjects analyses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1344-1352.
22, 820-828. doi:10.1177/0146167296228005 doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1344
Trafimow, D., Silverman, E. S., Fan, R. M., & Law, J. S. F. (1997). Walker, G. J., Deng, J., & Dieser, R. B. (2005). Culture, self-construal,
The effects of language and priming on the relative accessibility of and leisure theory and practice. Journal of Leisure Research, 37,
the private self and the collective self. Journal of Cross-Cultural 77-99.
Psychology, 28, 107-123. doi:10.1177/0022022197281007 Wang, Q., & Ross, M. (2005). What we remember and what we tell: The
Trafimow, D., & Smith, M. D. (1998). An extension of the “two- effects of culture and self-priming on memory representations and
baskets” theory to Native Americans. European Journal of narratives. Memory, 13, 594-606. doi:10.1080/09658210444000223
Social Psychology, 28, 1015-1019. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099- Weisz, J. R., Rothbaum, F. M., & Blackburn, T. C. (1984).
0992(1998110)28:6<1015::AID-EJSP900>3.0.CO;2-S Standing out and standing in: The psychology of control in
Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of America and Japan. American Psychologist, 39, 955-969.
the distinction between the private self and the collective self. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.39.9.955
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649-655. Werth, L., & Forster, J. (2007). The effects of regulatory focus on brak-
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.649 ing speed. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 2764-2787.
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differ- doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00280.x
ing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506-520. White, K., Lehman, D. R., & Cohen, D. (2006). Culture, self-
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506 construal, and affective reactions to successful and unsuccessful
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO: others. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 582-592.
Westview. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.10.001

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012


Cross et al. 179

Wiekens, C. J., & Stapel, D. A. (2008). I versus we: The effects of Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14, 92-101.
self-construal level on diversity. Social Cognition, 26, 368-377. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.14.2.92
doi:10.1521/soco.2008.26.3.368 Yuki, M., Maddux, W. W., Brewer, M. B., & Takemura, K. (2005).
Witkin, H. A., & Berry, J. W. (1975). Psychological differentiation Cross-cultural differences in relationship- and group-based
in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol- trust. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 48-62.
ogy, 6, 4-87. doi:10.1177/0146167204271305
Xie, D., Leong, F. T. L., & Feng, S. (2008). Culture-specific per- Yum, Y. (2004). Culture and self-construal as predictors of
sonality correlates of anxiety among Chinese and Caucasian col- responses to accommodative dilemmas in dating relationships.
lege students. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 11, 163-174. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 21, 817-835.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2008.00253.x doi:10.1177/0265407504047839
Yamada, A. M., & Singelis, T. M. (1999). Biculturalism and self-con- Zhang, Y., Feick, L., & Price, L. J. (2006). The impact of self-
strual. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23, 697-709. construal on aesthetic preference for angular versus rounded
Yamagishi, T., & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and commitment in the shapes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 794-805.
United States and Japan. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 129-166. doi:10.1177/0146167206286626
doi:10.1007/BF02249397 Zhang, Y., & Mittal, V. (2007). The attractiveness of enriched and
Ybarra, O., & Trafimow, D. (1998). How priming the private self or impoverished options: Culture, self-construal, and regulatory
collective self affects the relative weights of attitudes and sub- focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 588-598.
jective norms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, doi:10.1177/0146167206296954
362-370. doi:10.1177/0146167298244003 Zhang, Y., & Shrum, L. J. (2009). The influence of self-construal
Yeh, C. J., & Arora, A. K. (2003). Multicultural training and on impulsive consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 35,
interdependent and independent self-construal as predictors of 838-850. doi:10.1086/593687
universal-diverse orientation among school counselors. Journal Zou, X., Tam, K., Morris, M. W., Lee, S., Lau, I. Y., & Chiu, C.
of Counseling & Development, 81, 78-83. (2009). Culture as common sense: Perceived consensus versus
Yoon, J., & Lau, A. (2008). Maladaptive perfectionism and personal beliefs as mechanisms of cultural influence. Journal of
depressive symptoms among Asian American college stu- Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4), 579-597. doi:10.1037/
dents: Contributions of interdependence and parental relations. a0016399

Downloaded from psr.sagepub.com at West Uni from Timisoara on December 2, 2012

Potrebbero piacerti anche