Sei sulla pagina 1di 3

Current Biology

Dispatches
7. Shadmehr, R., Smith, M., and Krakauer, J. 12. Choi, J.T., and Bastian, A.J. (2007). walking in persons poststroke.
(2010). Error correction, sensory prediction, Adaptation reveals independent control Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 23,
and adaptation in motor control. Annu. Rev. networks for human walking. Nat. Neurosci. 735–744.
Neurosci. 33, 89–108. 10, 1055–1062.
17. Mazzoni, P., and Krakauer, J.W. (2006). An
8. Snaterse, M., Ton, R., Kuo, A.D., and Donelan, 13. Reisman, D.S., Bastian, A.J., and Morton, S.M. implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy
J.M. (2011). Distinct fast and slow processes (2010). Neurophysiologic and rehabilitation during visuomotor adaptation. J. Neurosci. 26,
contribute to the selection of preferred step insights from the split-belt and other locomotor 3642–3645.
frequency during human walking. J. Appl. adaptation paradigms. Phys. Ther. 90,
Physiol. 110, 1682–1690. 187–195. 18. Reisman, D.S., Block, H.J., and
Bastian, A.J. (2005). Interlimb coordination
9. Reynolds, R.F., and Bronstein, A.M. (2003). 14. van Ingen Schenau, G.J. (1980). Some during locomotion: what can be adapted
The broken escalator phenomenon. Aftereffect fundamental aspects of the biomechanics of and stored? J. Neurophysiol. 94, 2403–2415.
of walking onto a moving platform. Exp. Brain overground versus treadmill locomotion. Med.
Res. 151, 301–308. Sci. Sports Exerc. 12, 257–261. 19. Finley, J.M., Bastian, A.J., and
Gottschall, J.S. (2013). Learning to be
10. Mussa-Ivaldi, F.A., Hogan, N., and Bizzi, E. 15. Vazquez, A., Statton, M.A., Busgang, S.A., and economical: the energy cost of walking
(1985). Neural, mechanical, and geometric Bastian, A.J. (2015). Split-belt walking tracks motor adaptation. J. Physiol. 591,
factors subserving arm posture in humans. adaptation recalibrates sensorimotor 1081–1095.
J. Neurosci. 5, 2732–2743. estimates of leg speed but not position or
force. J. Neurophysiol. 114, 3255–3267. 20. Selinger, J.C., O’Connor, S.M., Wong, J.D.,
11. Redding, G.M., Rossetti, Y., and Wallace, B. and Donelan, J.M. (2015). Humans can
(2005). Applications of prism adaptation: a 16. Reisman, D.S., Wityk, R., Silver, K., and continuously optimize energetic cost
tutorial in theory and method. Neurosci. Bastian, A.J. (2009). Split-belt treadmill during walking. Curr. Biol. 25,
Biobehav. Rev. 29, 431–444. adaptation transfers to overground 2452–2456.

Zoology: At Last an Exit for Ctenophores


Gonzalo Giribet
Museum of Comparative Zoology and Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, 26 Oxford Street, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA
Correspondence: ggiribet@g.harvard.edu
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.036

Ctenophores, one of the most basal branches in the tree of life, have been found to have a through-gut,
complete with mouth and anus. Basal animals are surprisingly complex and simplification has been
rampant in animal evolution.

It has been generally accepted that life key discovery. These anal pores were 150 and 250 described species, but
in general and animals in particular already reported in the nineteenth due to their gelatinous nature, they are
evolved from simple forms to more Century by Louis Agassiz [2] but had difficult to collect and study, and thus
complex ones. Early evolving animals been thought to be mostly non-functional. many remain to be formally described.
lacked tissues or a basement membrane Now, Presnell and collaborators [1] Ctenophores have been reported to live
isolating their internal milieu from the were able to demonstrate the active use at the water’s surface, where they can
environment. Then with tissue-level of the anal pores in two ctenophore be locally abundant, to depths exceeding
integration, guts and nervous systems species, Mnemiopsis leidyi and 7000 meters.
evolved, and the gut, which started as a Pleurobrachia bachei, kept in captivity Ctenophores are the largest animals
simple blind sac-like structure, became in laboratories on the east and west to swim by ciliary action. For this, they use
more complex, regionalized, and coasts of the U.S. (Figure 1). This behavior their characteristic rows of
acquired a second opening, with a was also observed in free-living iridescent ciliary plates (hence their name
mouth and an anus. But does this specimens of a multitude of ctenophore combs or ctenes) that are present at least
evolutionary sequence reflect what genera. during some part of their life cycle. In
really happened? In an elegant new Comb jellies are mostly macro-pelagic addition to ciliary action, ctenophores can
article in this issue of Current Biology, (from a few centimeters to almost 1.5 also move by muscular contraction of
Presnell and collaborators [1] meters in body length) and exclusively their lobes, or use their musculature to
demonstrate that comb jellies (members marine animals of a gelatinous withdraw the retractable tentacles into
of the animal phylum Ctenophora) consistency, nearly all of which spend tentacular sheaths. They are carnivorous,
possess a through-gut, with a mouth for their lives in the water column. A handful feeding on other small pelagic and
ingesting their prey and two fully- of benthic species, including a sessile planktonic animals, often captured by a
functioning anal pores — the latter is the one, are known. There are between pair of tentacles loaded with a special

R918 Current Biology 26, R913–R936, October 24, 2016 ª 2016 Elsevier Ltd.
Current Biology

Dispatches

Figure 1. Ctenophores with a full belly.


Cultured Mnemiopsis leidyi: adult (left) and 9-day post hatching mid-transition between juvenile cydippid form and adult form (right). The white particles in the
center of the body constitute the gut contents. Photos: William E. Browne.

type of sticky cells called ‘colloblasts’, regarded to form a common clade called unique nervous system that probably
and ingested alive. ‘Coelenterata’ together with cnidarians — evolved in parallel to that of other
Based on all these characteristics, it was assumed that their digestive animals, muscles and a through-gut [6,8].
ctenophores have been considered system, despite having two aborally- This would provide further evidence for
relatively complex animals — they have positioned (at the opposite end to the secondary simplification in sponges
discrete muscles and a diffuse but mouth) anal pores, functioned like those and placozoans, and a convergence in
highly integrative nervous system — at of cnidarians, i.e., the food was ingested body plan with cnidarians. Some of
least when compared to other basal by the mouth and the digested remains these implications would still stand even
offshoots of the animal tree of life, such were also expelled by the same oral if sponges were more basally placed than
as placozoans, sponges and cnidarians opening. ctenophores. Nonetheless, a question
(jelly fishes, anemones, corals, etc.). The new work by Presnell and remains with respect to the origins of the
In fact, some authors considered them colleagues [1] elegantly shows that anus in ctenophores and that of a
to be more closely related to the the ingested food passes through the clade of bilaterian animals called
‘higher animals’ or bilaterians due to different sections of the ctenophore’s ‘Nephrozoa’ — all bilaterally symmetrical
their complex anatomy [3]. However, complex digestive tract, and that after animals, except a small clade called
phylogenetic evidence based on being digested, its remains are actively Xenacoelomorpha [12]. Nephrozoans
molecular data has unambiguously evacuated through the anal pores. were thought to have incorporated two
shown that ctenophores ought to be more Food remains are only expelled through anatomical innovations to their body
basally placed than originally thought, the mouth under specific stressful plan: discrete excretory organs for
either as the sister group to all other conditions similar to those leading to eliminating nitrogenous waste more
extant animals [4–8] or the first offshoot vomiting in many other animals, including efficiently (e.g., different types of
after the divergence of sponges from humans. The normal path followed by nephridial organs, including the kidney),
other animals [9–11]. Irrespective of which the ingested prey of these voracious and the through-gut with mouth and
of these positions is correct, all modern animals is thus unidirectional, as in anus. Whether the anus of ctenophores
studies suggest that placozoans, which most bilaterians. is homologous to that of nephrozoans
are the anatomically simplest animals, The implications of this discovery are remains unanswered.
and cnidarians, which lack a through- numerous. If ctenophores are the sister The specific series of events by which
gut, are more derived phylogenetically group to all other animals, as many principal features of animal bodies
than the more complex ctenophores. phylogenetic studies still indicate, the evolved is still missing key pieces, but it
However, due to their phylogenetic extant representatives of this lineage thus now seems clear that Darwin’s ‘‘from so
position — ctenophores were once constitute fairly complex animals, with a simple a beginning’’ may not necessarily

Current Biology 26, R913–R936, October 24, 2016 R919


Current Biology

Dispatches

apply to the base of the animal tree of 3. Nielsen, C. (1995). Animal Evolution, 9. Pisani, D., Pett, W., Dohrmann, M., Feuda, R.,
Interrelationships of the Living Phyla, 1st Rota-Stabelli, O., Philippe, H., Lartillot, N., and
life. Instead, placozoans and cnidarians Edition (Oxford: Oxford University Press). Wörheide, G. (2015). Genomic data do not
are probably simpler than some of their support comb jellies as the sister group to all
ancestors (i.e., the common ancestor they 4. Dunn, C.W., Hejnol, A., Matus, D.Q., Pang, K., other animals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112,
Browne, W.E., Smith, S.A., Seaver, E., Rouse, 15402–15407.
shared with ctenophores). This should G.W., Obst, M., Edgecombe, G.D., et al.
actually not come as a surprise following (2008). Broad phylogenomic sampling 10. Nosenko, T., Schreiber, F., Adamska, M.,
improves resolution of the animal tree of life. Adamski, M., Eitel, M., Hammel, J.,
the description of many independent Nature 452, 745–749. Maldonado, M., Müller, W.E., Nickel, M.,
cases of extreme simplification in Schierwater, B., et al. (2013). Deep metazoan
parasitic metazoans, as exemplified by 5. Hejnol, A., Obst, M., Stamatakis, A., Ott, M., phylogeny: When different genes tell
Rouse, G.W., Edgecombe, G.D., Martinez, P., different stories. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 67,
orthonectids [13] and myxozoans [14]. Baguñà, J., Bailly, X., Jondelius, U., et al. 223–233.
Ctenophores and their troubled (2009). Assessing the root of bilaterian animals
with scalable phylogenomic methods. Proc. R. 11. Philippe, H., Derelle, R., Lopez, P., Pick, K.,
phylogenetic position may hold the key to Soc. B Biol. Sci. 276, 4261–4270. Borchiellini, C., Boury-Esnault, N., Vacelet, J.,
resolving some of these recalcitrant Renard, E., Houliston, E., Quéinnec, E., et al.
mysteries. 6. Moroz, L.L., Kocot, K.M., Citarella, M.R., (2009). Phylogenomics revives traditional
Dosung, S., Norekian, T.P., Povolotskaya, I.S., views on deep animal relationships. Curr. Biol.
Grigorenko, A.P., Dailey, C., Berezikov, E., 19, 1–17.
REFERENCES Buckley, K.M., et al. (2014). The ctenophore
genome and the evolutionary origins of neural 12. Telford, M.J., and Copley, R.R. (2016).
systems. Nature 510, 109–114. Zoology: War of the worms. Curr. Biol. 26,
1. Presnell, J.S., Vandepas, L.E., Warren, K.J., R335–337.
Swalla, B.J., Amemiya, C.T., and Browne, 7. Whelan, N.V., Kocot, K.M., Moroz, L.L., and
W.E. (2016). The presence of a functionally Halanych, K.M. (2015). Error, signal, and the 13. Mikhailov, Kirill V., Slyusarev, Georgy S.,
tripartite through-gut in ctenophora has placement of Ctenophora sister to all other Nikitin, Mikhail A., Logacheva, Maria D.,
implications for metazoan character trait animals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 5773– Penin, Aleksey A., Aleoshin, Vladimir V., and
evolution. Curr. Biol. 26, 2814–2820. 5778. Panchin, Yuri V. (2016). The genome of
Intoshia linei affirms orthonectids as highly
2. Agassiz, L. (1850). Contributions to the natural 8. Ryan, J.F., Pang, K., Schnitzler, C.E., simplified spiralians. Curr. Biol. 26, 1768–
history of the Acalephæ of North America. Nguyen, A.D., Moreland, R.T., Simmons, 1774.
Part II: On the beroid medusæ of the shores D.K., Koch, B.J., Francis, W.R., Havlak, P.,
of Massachusetts, in their perfect state of Smith, S.A., et al. (2013). The genome of 14. Okamura, B., and Gruhl, A. (2016). Myxozoa +
development. Memoirs of the American the ctenophore Mnemiopsis leidyi and its Polypodium: A common route to
Academy of Arts and Sciences. New Series 4, implications for cell type evolution. Science endoparasitism. Trends Parasitol. 32,
313–374. 342, 1242592. 268–271.

Episodic Memory: Rats Master Multiple Memories


William A. Roberts
Department of Psychology, Western University, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada
Correspondence: roberts@uwo.ca
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.042

A new study in which rats had to discriminate odors according to whether they were novel for a
particular environmental context has found that they can accurately discriminate a large number of odors
and multiple context transitions, suggesting that they are able to form and remember multiple episodic
memories.

People show phenomenal memory for images will seem familiar because they that occurred at different times and in
pictures: when shown a list of over 2000 were first seen earlier in the list, and more different contexts. In this issue of Current
pictures, each briefly exposed, people recently seen faces will seem more Biology, Panoz-Brown et al. [2] report
accurately recognize seen pictures from familiar. The presentation of an historical that rats too can remember multiple
unseen pictures with over 90% accuracy figure, say Abraham Lincoln, will retrieve episodic memories encoded in different
on a subsequent test [1]. Such an ability semantic or reference memories of contexts.
is undoubtedly underlain by a primate general information about slavery and the It is widely acknowledged that
visual system highly developed for the American civil war. Still another picture in animals form semantic memories, as
recording and storage of visual images. the sequence might be that of a close they learn to make Pavlovian responses
If a person is exposed to a sequence of friend: this image retrieves multiple to conditioned stimuli or to make
facial images of other people, some episodic memories of shared experiences instrumental responses of lever pressing

R920 Current Biology 26, R913–R936, October 24, 2016 ª 2016 Elsevier Ltd.

Potrebbero piacerti anche