Sei sulla pagina 1di 12

432 Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior 44 (4) August 2014

© 2013 The American Association of Suicidology


DOI: 10.1111/sltb.12067

Suicide Risk Assessment and Risk Formulation


Part II: Suicide Risk Formulation and the
Determination of Levels of Risk
ALAN L. BERMAN, PHD, AND MORTON M. SILVERMAN, MD

The suicide risk formulation (SRF) is dependent on the data gathered in


the suicide risk assessment. The SRF assigns a level of suicide risk that is intended
to inform decisions about triage, treatment, management, and preventive inter-
ventions. However, there is little published about how to stratify and formulate
suicide risk, what are the criteria for assigning levels of risk, and how triage and
treatment decisions are correlated with levels of risk. The salient clinical issues
that define an SRF are reviewed and modeling is suggested for an SRF that might
guide clinical researchers toward the refinement of an SRF process.

The relationship between a suicide risk inform a clinical judgment about the level
assessment (SRA) and a suicide risk formu- of risk.
lation (SRF) is analogous to the relationship Surprisingly, there is little in the clin-
between a list of ingredients and a recipe. ical literature that trains to the development
A recipe informs the cook of what ingredients of clinical judgment either via a model for
get mixed with what other ingredients and an SRF or an extended guideline for estab-
in what order, at what temperature the mix- lishing levels of risk. Furthermore, the liter-
ture gets cooked, and for how long. Unto ature lacks studies on the reliability and
itself, the list of ingredients fails to inform validity of clinical judgment as it relates to
the individual of how the ingredients in the SRF. What Motto (1989) noted remains
dish are to be combined and to comprise true almost a quarter century later:
the desired final dish. Similarly, the factors
of risk that constitute an SRA (i.e., the pres- to date we have no established and
ence vs. absence of predisposing, precipitat- generally accepted procedure to guide us
ing, and acute risk and protective factors) in [the assessment of suicide risk]. Innu-
represent the essential ingredients to inform merable decisions regarding risk are
an SRF. An SRF must involve some under- made and implemented every day—the
standing of how risk factors interact, exac- job gets done—but how it gets done is
erbate, and otherwise fuel heightened or determined primarily by the skills and
lowered risk of suicide, no less how they philosophy of the individual clinician. (p.
245)
ALAN L. BERMAN, American Association of
Suicidology, Washington, DC, USA, and MORTON The detection of suicide risk, the
M. SILVERMAN, Department of Psychiatry, The determination of the level of risk, and the
University of Colorado Denver School of Medi- consequent triage and treatment decisions
cine, Aurora, CO, USA. that are dependent on that determination
Address correspondence to Alan L. Ber-
man, American Association of Suicidology, 5221 are, perhaps, the most significant judgments
Wisconsin Avenue, N.W., Washington, DC a clinician must make. The failure to rea-
20015; E-mail: berman@suicidology.org sonably accomplish these tasks has the
BERMAN AND SILVERMAN 433

potential for significant negative outcomes direct instruction offered in this two-vol-
for both the patient (a possibly preventable ume tome was that given by Stevenson and
death by suicide) and the clinician (a pre- Steppe, who recommended, “to begin with
ventable loss of a patient). It is time for the the thought content of the depressive mood
field of suicidology to move beyond the and work gradually upward to possible sui-
intuition and philosophy of the clinician cidal thoughts” (p. 228).
(which can be idiosyncratic and/or unreli- Shneidman, writing in Freedman,
able), and to develop and train to models Kaplan, and Sadock’s The Modern Synopsis of
for formulating a patient’s risk of suicide, Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry/II
using standardized criteria and definitions. (1975; pp. 870–874), offered “prodromal
Here, we offer a review of the salient clues to suicide” and a discussion of the
clinical issues that define an SRF, provide a concepts of perturbation, inimicality (hostil-
historical perspective on how clinicians have ity), and lethality, but no direction as to
been trained to make an SRF, and suggest how judgments of level of risk were to be
modeling for an SRF that might guide clini- made.
cal researchers toward refinement and valida- Mackinnon and Michels, in The Psy-
tion as a trainable guide to clinical judgment. chiatric Interview in Clinical Practice (1971,
pp. 204–209), wrote of the need for active
inquiry and discussion of suicide in the psy-
A BRIEF HISTORICAL chiatric interview, noting the latter “[was]
PERSPECTIVE crucial in gauging the severity and danger
of the patient’s depression” (emphasis added).
It is instructive to view the question Their discussion on evaluating suicide risk
of how—and if—SRF has been taught over (pp. 414–415) only in this context of
the years. This can indirectly be carried out depression, however, offers nothing to
through the lens of major and significant guide clinical judgment.
psychiatric textbooks and other writings Early suicidology researchers attemp-
from the last century. ted to build instruments for evaluating “sui-
Two volumes written in the 1940s cide potential.” In the 1960s, clinicians at
(American Psychiatric Association, 1944; the Los Angeles Suicide Prevention Center
Zilboorg & Henry, 1941) outlined the his- (Farberow, Heilig, & Litman, 1968) began
tory of psychiatry and medical psychology constructing a “suicide potential scale” to
to the dates of publication, with scant ref- evaluate a patient or caller who identified
erence to even the word suicide, no less him- or herself as suicidal. Ten categories
mention of how SRFs may have been con- of observations, including more than 60
ducted or taught. Similarly, the American individual weighted items, constituted a
Psychiatric Association’s One Hundred Years summated score and relatively differentiated
of American Psychiatry (1944) makes no risk ratings ranging from low to high. High
mention of suicide risk, an SRA, or an risk was conceptualized as “a prediction of
SRF. A third history of psychiatry volume suicide probability” of 10,000 per 100,000.
(Alexander & Selesnick, 1966) strikingly Contemporaneously, Tuckman and Young-
does not even reference the word suicide in man (1968) developed a scale for differenti-
its index! ating high versus low suicide risk among
Writing in Arieti’s classic American those who had made an attempt (defining
Handbook of Psychiatry (1959), Levine and high risk operationally as death from suicide
Lederer argued that “direct and helpful within 1 year of the attempt). Using a data
information” (p. 1926) such as the recogni- set of more than 1,000 nonstandardized
tion and evaluation of suicide risks should police death investigations, these research-
be given by teachers of psychiatry to stu- ers ultimately presented 14 risk factors
dents in medical training. Yet the only associated with high risk. Neither of these
434 SUICIDE RISK ASSESSMENT AND RISK FORMULATION

early efforts was ever replicated, nor vali- risk of death by suicide, would produce far
dated. more false positives than could be clinically
Cohen, Motto, and Seiden (1966) clas- tolerated. To illustrate this, were we to
sified suicide attempts of 193 inpatients into have a scale that was 95% accurate at pre-
one of four grades of seriousness and fol- dicting imminent risk (defined as death by
lowed these patients over a 5- to 8-year per- suicide within the next 48 hours) and were
iod to identify those who were at high risk; we to apply this scale to 100,000 individu-
that is, made a subsequent attempt or died als, the scale would correctly find almost all
by suicide. Ultimately, they derived a 15- those who, indeed, would die by suicide,
item Suicide Risk Assessment Scale (SRAS) but would simultaneously and falsely iden-
with weighted scores totaling over 800 and a tify almost 5,000 individuals as imminently
“table of risk” ranging from “very low” to at risk!
“very high” (defined as a score > 555, having The need remains for clinical models
an approximated suicide rate of over 10% to inform clinical judgments about levels of
over the next 2 years). The SRAS was risk. Moreover, questions are raised as to
intended to supplement, not substitute for, whether clinical judgments of risk are to be
clinical judgment and “when the Scale is not based reasonably on science, or intuition, or
consistent with clinical judgment, clinical both, and whether good clinical judgment
judgment [was to] be given precedence” regarding a patient’s relative level of suicide
(Motto, Heilbrun, & Juster, 1985). risk can be trained.
Perhaps the most significant contribu-
tion to developing an SRF was offered by
Beck, Resnik, and Lettieri (1974) at the WHAT IS MEANT BY “LEVELS OF
National Institutes of Mental Health RISK”?
(NIMH) in the early 1970s. These research-
ers developed sophisticated models for for- Traditionally, clinical professionals
mulating risk among four demographic evaluate a patient’s risk of suicide on a
groups defined by age and gender (old– quasi-Likert scale ranging from no risk to
young, male–female), and, similar to earlier low risk to moderate risk to high risk, and
scales, ratings of risk with scaled scores above whether high risk might be imminent risk.
7 indicating high risk or the likelihood of However, standardized, operational defini-
being dead by suicide within 2 years. That tions of these levels of risk are difficult to
none of these or other attempts to predict find. As noted by Simon (2011), models and
suicide via research-based scales has survived strategies for making judgments of levels of
to offer a standard for clinicians was presaged risk have been proposed, but there is no
by Diggory (in Beck et al., 1974): research that has specifically examined the
relative validity of any of these schemas, no
The intuitive, common sense combina- less the process and outcome of clinical
tion of plausible suicide predictors into decision making that is intended to result
scales for evaluating risk without system- from an assignment of a level of risk. What
atic, empirical testing or verification has is generally understood by the clinician is
been tried. … But, I believe that at pres- merely that these levels of risk exist on an
ent hardly anyone seriously believes that ordinal scale; that higher versus lower levels
such a procedure is likely to fill the need of judged risk carry more significant
for better prediction. (p. 62) demands for aggressive treatment planning,
triage, and intervention; and that a determi-
Given the low base rate of suicide, it nation of “imminent risk” is a statutory
has been intuitively understood by suicidol- requirement criterion for civil commitment
ogists that predictive scales, no less any and potentially a clinician’s duty to warn
attempting actuarially to derive levels of (Tarasoff v. Regents of the University of
BERMAN AND SILVERMAN 435

California, 17 Cal. 3d 425,551 P.2d334, sory time frame on an unpredictable act” (p.
131 Cal. Rptr. 14). 296).
The National Suicide Prevention
Imminent Risk Lifeline gives guidance to its affiliated crisis
call centers via its imminent risk policy
In the United States, statutes for which appears to align the crisis worker’s
assisted treatment (involuntary hospitaliza- judgment not with intuition, but with gut
tion) vary considerably (Berman, 2011a), feelings and anxiety. A caller is determined
but, typically, employ language to guide the to be at imminent risk of suicide if the cen-
clinician as follows: ter staff responding to the call believe,
based on information gathered during the
1. Real and present threat of substan-
exchange from the person at risk or some-
tial harm to self or others
one calling on his or her behalf, that there
2. Likely to injure if not thwarted
is a close temporal connection between the
3. Unable to care for self
person’s current risk status and actions that
4. … in the reasonable future
could lead to his or her suicide. The risk
must be present in the sense that it creates
As the reader may note, these statu-
an obligation and immediate pressure on
tory provisions offer language that leaves
center staff to take urgent actions to reduce
the determination of imminent risk entirely
the caller’s risk; that is, “if no actions were
up to the clinician’s best judgment applied
taken, the center staff believes that the
to a subjectively determined timeline (i.e.,
caller would be likely to seriously harm or
some undefined number of hours into the
kill him/her self” (www.suicidepreventionli-
future, e.g., 24, 48, or 72 hours).1 Hence,
feline.org/crisiscenters/bestpractices).
imminent risk determinations are clinical
In making a judgment of imminent
and temporally related predictions of behav-
risk, the clinician is asked to reasonably
ior in the near future (with no agreed-upon
anticipate the possibility of a patient’s poten-
operational definition of what is meant by
tially lethal suicidal behavior in the very near
the “near future”).
future; hence, to act to protect the patient.
No less, the demand to make such a
Therefore, this judgment is central to deter-
near-term prediction of a patient’s behavior
mining liability (whether a breach in the
asks clinicians to do considerably better than
standard of care occurred) in the assessment
meteorologists making near-term predictions
and treatment for a patient now dead by sui-
of precipitation. In spite of significantly bet-
cide. In the legal context, this judgment is
ter and more scientific instrumentation (e.g.,
one of “foreseeability.” Again, if the court
Doppler Radar) and computer models for
holds clinicians accountable for formulating
these predictions, meteorologists have been
a patient’s imminent risk of suicide, should
shown to be wrong more often than right in
we not try to base this judgment on some-
their predictions for the next 48 hours (www.
thing more than intuition?
freakonomics.com/2008/04/21/how-valid-are-
tv-weather-forecasts/). Simon (2006) was cor-
Just How Reliable Are Clinicians’
rect when he wrote that this near-term
Judgments of Levels of Risk?
prediction of imminent risk “imposes an illu-
Cahill and Rakow (2012) investigated
1
An oft-quoted reference (Hirschfeld & differences in clinical judgment using 35
Russell, 1997) defines imminent risk as a suicide hypothetical case examples provided to seven
attempt occurring within 48 hours of the time practitioners by regressing 10 case variables
that the patient is seen, short-term risk as an
attempt occurring within days or weeks, and onto their judgments, and found that all but
long-term risk as an attempt occurring within two potential risk factors were related to risk
weeks to years. and priority judgments. Further, they
436 SUICIDE RISK ASSESSMENT AND RISK FORMULATION

observed that “low risk” might be particu- risk patients are inherently complex and their
larly subject to variability in its interpreta- communications are often indirect or pur-
tion. Even the term risk could be ambiguous, posely unclear. Yet, this is the input that a cli-
they noted, “reflecting the probability of nician must use to derive a judgment that
occurrence for some” and “the severity of defines a level of risk. Motto (1992) wrote, “If
occurrence to others.” Perhaps, then, it we cannot predict suicide, what can we do? …
should not surprise us that Appleby and col- We can … predict the risk of suicide rather
leagues (Appleby et al., 2012; Appleby, than the suicide itself. … There is no avoiding
Shaw, & Amos, 1999) reported that, at last subjectivity … what I have referred to … as
contact, the therapist assessed no suicide risk an intuitive judgment.” In the same volume,
in 30% and low risk in 54% of patients who Maltsberger (1992) wrote, “Because no one
then died by suicide. knows how to integrate [a large mass of data]
Clinicians tend to believe that clinical … precisely and with empirical certainty, the
judgment improves with increased educa- clinician is forced to rely on clinical experi-
tion, training, or experience. However, clin- ence and inductive reasoning” (p. 38).
ical experience and “expertise” add little to Shea (1999, 2012) argued that the
the accuracy of clinical judgment (Hilton, clinical formulation of risk is based on a
Harris, & Rice, 2006). As one illustration of cognitive understanding of data gathered
this, Berman (2011a) presented two case about risk, ideation, and protective factors
vignettes (of persons who died by suicide) and an intuitive process that takes into
to clinical suicidologist experts (blinded to account such factors as the clinician’s famil-
case outcome) and reported that only iarity with the patient and the patient’s
between 30% and 40% rated these patients character structure. Although he offers case
at imminent risk. If critical clinical thinking examples of his judgment based on these
is influenced by knowledge and experience databases, he provides no further direction.
(Bittner & Tobin, 1998), we have yet to Fawcett et al. (1990) provided ground-
validate it. Indeed, a meta-analysis of 75 breaking research by prospectively studying
clinical judgment studies involving more 954 mood disordered patients and, based on
than 4,600 clinicians concluded that educa- those who died by suicide over the next 2 to
tion and clinical experience have a very 10 years, differentiated acute (from weeks up
small effect (only 13%) on the accuracy of to 1 year) versus long-term risk factors.
clinical judgments (Spengler et al., 2009). Simon (1992) incorporated these short-term
(although now not limited to their empirical
context within mood disordered patients)
MODELS FOR FORMULATING and long-term variables, rating each on a
SUICIDE RISK scale from low to high, or as a nonfactor, and
judged overall as a clinical mosaic, which he
As regression models and other statis- later termed a “systematic suicide risk assess-
tically based methods of combining, scaling, ment” (Simon, 2012). However, neither
or otherwise actuarially integrating suicide approach clearly outlines how to formulate
risk factors into some sort of formula for levels of risk, especially when an individual
judging level of risk have been shown to have has risk factors which are not all “short
poor specificity (Schiepek et al., 2011), an term,” “acute,” or “long term.”
alternative approach to this standard of care Nevertheless, Maltsberger (1986,
clinical responsibility needs to be found. 1992) suggested roughly grouping personal
An SRF involves integrating informa- factors (p), exterior factors (e), and mental
tion derived from an SRA through an inferen- state factors (ms) and then proposed that
tial cognitive process. The information risk (R) will be a rough function of the sum
gathered through an SRA is potentially com- of these: R = f(p + e + ms)/f(c), with
plex and often ambiguous, in part because at- c = character = disposition to react
BERMAN AND SILVERMAN 437

adaptively versus maladaptively to emo- tion of factors, was strongly associated with
tional injury. suicide in the year after discharge and, fur-
Based on Joiner’s (2005) interper- ther, that risk categorization is of no value
sonal-psychological theory of suicide and in efforts to decrease the numbers of
research (Van Orden, Witte, Gordon, patients who will die by suicide after hospi-
Bender, & Joiner, 2008), a more recent tal discharge. Our interpretation of this
model adopted by the National Suicide Pre- meta-analysis is that the low-risk patients
vention Lifeline asserts that desire, intent, may have been misdiagnosed or poorly
and acquired capability, in a context of low assessed, in part because once they denied
buffers or connectedness, are associated with SI they were deemed to be free of suicide
imminent risk. While the individual compo- risk and ready for discharge (see Silverman
nents of these factors have support for their & Berman, 2013).
association with greater lifetime risk of sui-
cide, no test of the asserted combination of
desire, intent, and capability and imminent A PROPOSED MODEL BASED ON
or even near-term risk has yet been made. EVIDENCE-BASED ACUTE RISK
Moreover, some of the observations that FACTORS
define these components are problematic.
For example, capability is defined by any of The American Association of Suicidol-
four observed behaviors: an attempt in pro- ogy’s Recognizing and Responding to
gress, planning, preparation, and expressed Suicide Risk (RRSR) clinical training pro-
intent to die. As noted in our companion gram’s model for conducting an SRF asserts
article on SRA (Silverman & Berman, 2013), that the presence of chronic risk factors
that a great many persons who die by sui- establishes the patient’s lifetime vulnerability
cide do not display any of these behaviors to be suicidal across his or her lifetime, while
dooms this model to failure in determining the presence of one or more warning signs
a great number of “imminent suicides.” (acute risk factors—see Rudd et al., 2006), in
That said, research attempting to validate that context of lifetime vulnerability, estab-
this model is warranted and desired. lishes a basis for the foreseeability of near-
About 4% of patients hospitalized for term risk (see www.suicidology.org). We
a medically serious suicide attempt can be offer this model for an SRF as it builds on
expected to die by suicide within 18 months Litman’s (1990) conceptualization of a “sui-
after hospital discharge (Beautrais, 2004), cide zone,” in which he proposed that vul-
and more than 1% of psychiatric patients nerable individuals move in and out of
die by suicide within a year of discharge periods of heightened risk when a lot of
(Ho, 2003). A recent systematic meta-analy- things (symptomatic, environmental, etc.)
sis of controlled studies (Large, Sharma, occur at once. The model further assumes
Cannon, Ryan, & Nielssen, 2011) investi- that the greater the number of warning signs,
gated risk factors for suicide within 1 year the greater the risk of death by suicide,
after discharge from a psychiatric hospital. although no actuarial statement is made
Factors weakly associated with postdis- about cutoff scores (e.g., number of acute
charge suicide were reports of suicidal ideas risk factors needed to define imminent risk).
(OR = 2.47) and a diagnosis of major That this may yet be possible, however, is
depression (OR = 1.91). Patients deemed to evidenced by Berman (2011b), who reviewed
be high risk were more likely to die by sui- 55 psychologically autopsied suicides of
cide than other discharged patients, but the Americans who died on railroad rights-of-
strength of this association was not much way and found five or more of the 10 warn-
greater than the association with some of ing signs captured in the acronym IS PATH
the individual risk factors. These authors WARM (www.suicidology.org) in the major-
concluded that no one factor, or combina- ity of cases (55%), and three or more of the
438 SUICIDE RISK ASSESSMENT AND RISK FORMULATION

Threat of loss of position or identity


warning signs in 84% of cases. The RRSR
approach asserts that protective factors do

Contributory risk factors


not buffer when acute risk is present. In our
legal work, we have encountered too many
cases of individuals who had died by suicide
who were married with children, were reli-

Firearm accessibility
gious leaders (see case illustration), were in

Financial strain
treatment with a mental health professional,
or had future plans, to assert that protective

Religiosity
factors actually do protect when factors of

Buffers
acute risk are present in an already vulnera-
ble individual.

Case Illustration

Increased alcohol use


Increased withdrawal

protective influence of his religious attachment, puts Father Bill at


Acute risk factors
Father Bill, a 45-year-old Catholic

Multiple acute risk factors in a context of elevated lifetime risk, in

high suicide risk of the near-term (at the least until the audit is
spite of no stated suicide ideation and the once but now fragile
priest, enters treatment because he has been

Feeling trapped
Hopelessness
feeling mildly depressed and anxious. At his
initial session, he describes an increasing
Insomnia
Anxiety
level of panic in anticipation of an upcom-

Rage
ing audit of his parish finances, because he
had improperly advanced to himself, then

accomplished and his fears are not realized)


repaid most, but not all, of his salary to
help support his younger sister who has
Family history of depression and suicide

severe medical problems. Further, he


describes feeling rage at the Church’s lack
Diabetes, cardiovascular problems

of response to this sister’s claims that she


Chronic risk factors

had been sexually abused by her parish


History depressive episodes

priest years earlier. Father Bill believes the


audit was ordered as retaliation against him
for his support of his sister’s claim. Father
Middle-aged male

Bill appears fatigued and jittery, and com-


plains of symptoms of insomnia, problems
concentrating, hopelessness, and a fear that
Smoker

he would not be able to cope with an


impending demotion. As a result, he has
withdrawn from friends and supporters. He
denies thinking about suicide, but admits to
a family history of depression (his mother)
SRF case formulation: Father Bill

and suicide (a maternal uncle). He has a


chronic history of diabetes and has had car-
diovascular problems that have necessitated
Level of risk formulation

two surgical interventions in the past dec-


ade. Subsequent to these, his primary care
physician put him on antidepressants for
brief depressive episodes. In spite of this
history, he continues a 30-year cigarette
habit. He drinks occasionally but has
recently increased both the frequency and
amount of his drinking. He owns a firearm.
BERMAN AND SILVERMAN 439

As a related example of this model, dispositional decision making about the


not all overweight, smoking, drinking, dia- need for hospitalization.
betic older men are going to have a myo- In this spirit, the SAFE-T pocket
cardial infarction (low-to-moderate risk card was developed by Screening for
level), but those men with these risk factors Mental Health and the Suicide Prevention
who also complain of shortness of breath Resource Center. In giving guidance on both
and chest pain (warning signs) are at high the assessment and formulation of risk,
risk of a myocardial infarction—although it links a judgment of risk level with recom-
not all men presenting with the warning mended disposition. The downside is that it
signs of shortness of breath and chest pain maintains a focus on current SI or suicidal
are currently experiencing a myocardial behavior as a separate and integral category
infarction or are destined to develop one. of observation in informing that judgment
Unfortunately, we yet lack research of level of risk (www.sprc.org/library/
into very near-term risk factors for suicide. safe_t_pcktcrd_
Warning signs (Rudd et al., 2006) were edc.pdf).
identified by their association with suicide
over the subsequent 12 months, not the
next 30 or 7 days or, for that matter, A NOTE ON CLINICAL
48 hours. There is a pressing need for this JUDGMENT AND ITS
research to be conducted. In fact, the TRAINABILITY
Research Task Force of the National Action
Alliance for Suicide Prevention has priori- Clinical errors are both universal and
tized the assessment of who is at risk of sui- inevitable; however, clinical judgments, par-
cide in the near future as one of its 12 ticularly those made under the stress of
aspirational goals (http://actionalliance seeking to be lifesaving, may be especially
forsuicideprevention.org/task-force/research- prone to error. In one study, for example,
prioritization). researchers were unable to predict nearly
The outcome of an SRF is a disposi- two-thirds of the violent crime that ulti-
tion. In an emergency department, for mately occurred and nearly two-thirds of
example, there are at least five dispositional the persons whom they predicted would be
outcomes from an SRF: release without fol- violent were not (Monahan, 1984). As noted
low-up or recommendation for further by Karthikeyan and Pais (2010), “clinical
treatment, release with recommendation for judgment conjures up visions of the arche-
follow-up outpatient care, release with crisis typal clinician endowed with infinite wis-
management and safety planning interven- dom and breathtaking clairvoyance”
tions, release with ongoing interventions (p. 623). We recognize that creating clini-
(psychotherapy and/or medications), or hos- cians in this mold is an unattainable goal.
pitalization (for safety and further assess- As for whether education and/or
ment). In this case, the patient, more than training can improve the accuracy of clinical
likely, is at the emergency department judgments, the jury is still out (Spengler
because of suicidal behavior or suicide idea- et al., 2009). Of course, in the arena of risk
tion; hence, triage decisions would have to formulation, the criterion variable against
include an assessment of the patient’s sup- which judgments of risk would be measured
port system, evidence of controls over SI is that of a suicidal behavior; that is, a
and/or subsequent attempt, ability to coop- patient judged to be at a higher level of risk
erate, and degree of resolution of life crises of a suicidal outcome would, indeed, engage
that prompted the admission, in addition to suicidal behavior more frequently than a
an assessment of current acute risk factors. patient at a lower level of risk. Given the
Similarly, these variables might be consid- low base rate of suicidal behaviors among
ered in an outpatient setting to help guide patients, addressing this question would
440 SUICIDE RISK ASSESSMENT AND RISK FORMULATION

require a very large number of patients to very near-term risk; research validating
be followed over a very long period of time. models for an SRF that tests which of the
For example, in Schulberg et al.’s (2005) many components of the SRA and to what
study of 761 primary care patients with degree does the frequency, duration, inten-
uncomplicated depression and suicide idea- sity, timing, and so forth of each of these
tion who were followed up over 6 months, components determine the appropriate level
there were no deaths by suicide and only of risk in an SRF; and research on how
one patient made a suicide attempt of clinicians should best weight clinical infor-
unknown level of potential lethality; that mation and observations to determine level
patient had been judged to be of intermedi- of risk judgments.
ate (moderate) risk at baseline. Clinicians deserve a better roadmap
Nevertheless, the American Associa- or clinical tool to assist in assigning an indi-
tion of Suicidology’s Task Force on Train- vidual to a level of low risk, medium risk,
ing (Schmitz et al., 2012) has made high risk, acute risk, or imminent risk of
multiple recommendations for preventing suicide. Without such an understanding of
suicide through improved training (in this relative contributions or weights to calibrate
case, to make a level of risk judgment) an individual’s level of suicide risk, the cli-
focused on building essential skills. The nician is left to his or her education, train-
question remains as to whether training can ing, intuition, judgment, and prior
improve these skills and, if so, through what experience to determine how best to inter-
methods of training. vene and manage the patient. We believe it
While it is tempting to seek actuar- is necessary to bring as much science into
ial models for predicting a patient’s risk this process as is possible. Despite the obvi-
of violence (in contrast to relying on ous appeal, it remains dangerous and unpre-
purely clinical judgment), there is evidence dictable to rely on clinical intuition or
to suggest that these models may not be experience in arriving at a judged risk level
superior (Litwak, 2001). Further, the ques- for an individual, especially when their life
tion remains as to whether suicidology or death hangs in the balance.
will ever be able to model statistically As noted by Berman (2012), “Perhaps
those decision rules that would better there is no equally compelling patient and
inform the accuracy of clinical judgments patient problem in the mental health arena
(Dawes, Faust, & Meehl, 1989; Levine & as that of the suicidal patient where our
Levine, 2007; Swets, Dawes, & Monahan, gaps in understanding are so great and our
2000). As Nakash and Alegria (2013) have opportunities for making a difference are so
suggested, the cognitive processes involved profound” (p. 631).
in clinical judgment include an apprecia-
tion of nonverbal cues and affective com-
munications from patients (and hence have CONCLUSIONS
termed these processes “implicit clinical
judgment”); the potential to create a via- Stratifying a patient’s level of risk of
ble actuarial model that fails to capture suicide through an SRF has significant
these inputs is questionable. implications for clinical outcomes and
hence for the patient at risk. For example, a
patient judged to be at imminent risk may
THE NEED FOR EMPIRICAL be hospitalized, perhaps against his or her
RESEARCH will, consequently be enraged at the mental
health system, be turned off to further help-
As noted, there is a need for a great seeking (upon discharge), and become yet
deal of research to inform an SRF—includ- more suicidal as a consequence. Moreover,
ing research on risk factors associated with as clinicians are potentially held accountable
BERMAN AND SILVERMAN 441

through tort actions when, and if, a patient It is remarkable that the science that
in treatment dies by suicide, improving the must underlie the process of conducting an
clinical judgments involved in clinical deci- SRF has lagged so far behind the process
sion making regarding the triage and treat- itself. We argue in this article that the time
ment for these patients has significant has come to significantly change this. Mod-
implications for the clinician. Some (Ryan, els for conducting an SRF exist. There is an
Nielssen, Paton, & Large, 2010) have gone urgent need to test and refine these models
so far to argue that risk formulations should to better inform clinical judgment and deci-
not form the basis for clinical decision mak- sion making. Further, there is an urgent
ing, preferring to focus attention simply on need to better inform the training of both
the treatment needs of each patient, regard- existing and future generations of clinicians
less of risk of adverse events. We agree that in making these potentially lifesaving judg-
a patient’s treatment needs should be fore- ments. The bottom line is that, to the
most in the mindset of the clinician, but, in extent possible, a science-based SRF is infi-
litigious societies, this argument (against nitely more preferable to an intuition-based
conducting an SRF) has serious negative or merely theoretically based SRF.
consequences for the clinician.

REFERENCES

ALEXANDER, F. G., & SELESNICK, S. T. Simon & R. E. Hales (Eds.), The American Psy-
(1966). The history of psychiatry: Practice from prehis- chiatric Publishing textbook of suicide assessment and
toric times to the present. New York: Harper & Row. management (2nd ed., pp. 621–636). Washington,
American Psychiatric Association. (1944). DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
One hundred years of American psychiatry. New BITTNER, N., & TOBIN, E. (1998). Critical
York: Columbia University Press. thinking: Strategies for clinical practice. Journal
APPLEBY, L., KAPUR, N., SHAW, J., HUNT, of Nursing Staff Development, 14, 267–272.
I., FLYNN, S., WHILE, D., ET AL. (2012, July). The CAHILL, S., & RAKOW, T. (2012). Assessing
national confidential inquiry into suicide and homicide risk and prioritizing referral for self-harm: When
by people with mental illness annual report. Man- and why is my judgment different from yours?
chester, UK: The University of Manchester. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 19, 399–410.
APPLEBY, L., SHAW, J., & AMOS, T. (1999). COHEN, E., MOTTO, J. A., & SEIDEN, R.
Suicide within 12 months of contact with mental (1966). An instrument for evaluating suicide
health services: National clinical survey. British potential: A preliminary study. American Journal
Medical Journal, 318, 1235–1239. of Psychiatry, 122, 886–891.
ARIETI, S. (1959). American handbook of DAWES, R. M., FAUST, D., & MEEHL, P.
psychiatry (Vols I–III). New York: Basic Books. E. (1989). Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Sci-
BEAUTRAIS, A. L. (2004). Further suicidal ence, 243, 1668–1674.
behavior among medically serious suicide at- DIGGORY, J. (1974). Predicting suicide:
tempters. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, Will-o-the-wisp or reasonable challenge? In A.
34, 1–11. T. Beck, H. L. P. Resnik, & D. J. Littieri (Eds.),
BECK, A. T., RESNIK, H. L. P., & LETTE- The prediction of suicide (pp. 59–70). Bowie, MD:
IRI, D. J. (Eds.). (1974). The prediction of suicide. Charles Press.
Bowie, MD: Charles Press. FARBEROW, N. L., HEILIG, S. M., & LIT-
BERMAN, A. L. (2011a, July). The assess- MAN, R. E. (1968). Techniques in crisis intervention:
ment of imminent risk for suicide. Paper presented A training manual. Los Angeles, CA: Suicide Pre-
at the 31st International Congress on Law and vention Center Inc.
Mental Health, New York, NY. FAWCETT, J., SCHEFTNER, W. A., FOGG,
BERMAN, A. L. (2011b, November). Coun- L., CLARK, D. C., YOUNG, M. A., HEDEKER, D.,
termeasures to reduce suicides on railway rights-of- ET AL. (1990). Time-related predictors of suicide
way in the United States. Washington, DC: Amer- in major affective disorder: A controlled study.
ican Association of Suicidology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1189–1194.
BERMAN, A. L. (2012). Suicide research: FREEDMAN, A. M., KAPLAN, H. I., &
Knowledge gaps and opportunities. In R. I. SADOCK, B. J. (1975). Comprehensive textbook of
442 SUICIDE RISK ASSESSMENT AND RISK FORMULATION

psychiatry (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & (Eds.), Suicide: Understanding and responding (pp.
Wilkins. 129–142). Madison, CT: International Universi-
HILTON, N. Z., HARRIS, G. T., & RICE, ties Press.
M. E. (2006). Sixty-six years of research on the MOTTO, J. A. (1992). An integrated
clinical versus actuarial prediction of violence. approach to estimating suicide risk. In R. W.
The Counseling Psychologist, 34, 400–409. Maris, A. L. Berman, J. T. Maltsberger, & R. I.
HIRSCHFELD, R. M. A., & RUSSELL, J. M. Yufit (Eds.), Assessment and prediction of suicide
(1997). Assessment and treatment of suicidal (pp. 625–639). New York: Guilford.
patients. New England Journal of Medicine, 337, MOTTO, J. A., HEILBRUN, D., & JUSTER,
910–915. R. (1985). Development of a clinical instrument
HO, T. P. (2003). The suicide risk of dis- to estimate suicide risk. American Journal of Psy-
charged psychiatric patients. Journal of Clinical chiatry, 142, 680–686.
Psychiatry, 64, 702–707. NAKASH, O., & ALEGRIA, M. (2013).
JOINER, T. E. (2005). Why people die by sui- Examination of the role of implicit clinical judg-
cide. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ments during mental health intake. Qualitative
KARTHIKEYAN, G., & PAIS, P. (2010). Clin- Health Research, 23, 645–654.
ical judgment and evidence-based medicine: National Action Alliance for Suicide Pre-
Time for reconciliation. Indian Journal of Medical vention Clinical Care Intervention Task Force
Research, 132, 623–626. Report. (2012). Retrieved May 13, 2013, from
LARGE, M., SHARMA, S., CANNON, E., http://actionallianceforsuicideprevention.org/
RYAN, C., & NIELSSEN, O. (2011). Risk factors sites/actionalliancefor suicideprevention.org/files/
for suicide within a year of discharge from psy- taskforces/ClinicalCareInterventionReport.
chiatric hospital: A systematic meta-analysis. pdf.
Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, RUDD, M. D., BERMAN, A. L., JOINER, T.
45, 619–628. E., NOCK, M. K., SILVERMAN, M. M., MANDRU-
LEVINE, M., & LEDERER, H. D. (1959). SIAK, M., ET AL. (2006). Warning signs for sui-
Teaching of psychiatry in medical schools. In S. cide: Theory, research and clinical applications.
Arieti (Ed.), American handbook of psychiatry (pp. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 36, 255–
1923–1934). New York: Basic Books. 262.
LEVINE, B. J., & LEVINE, D. W. (2007). RYAN, C., NIELSSEN, O., PATON, M., &
How accurately could we screen for individual LARGE, M. (2010). Clinical decisions in psychia-
risk? Using summary data to examine discrimi- try should not be based on risk assessment. Aus-
natory accuracy of a risk marker. Preventive Med- tralasian Psychiatry, 18, 398–403.
icine, 44, 342–347. SCHIEPEK, G., FARTACEK, C., STURM, J.,
LITMAN, R. (1990). Suicides: What do KRALOVEC, K., FARTACEK, R., & PLODERL, M.
they have in mind? In D. Jacobs & H. Brown (2011). Nonlinear dynamics: Theoretical per-
(Eds.), Suicide: Understanding and responding (pp. spectives and application to suicidology. Suicide
143–156). Madison, CT: International Universi- and Life-Threatening Behavior, 41, 661–675.
ties Press. SCHMITZ, W. M., ALLEN, M. H., FELD-
LITWAK, T. R. (2001). Actuarial versus MAN, B. N., GUTIN, N. J., JAGHN, D., KLEESPIES,
clinical assessments of dangerousness. Psychology, P. M., ET AL. (2012). Preventing suicide through
Public Policy, and Law, 7, 404–443. improved training in suicide risk assessment and
MACKINNON, R. A., & MICHELS, R. care: An American Association of Suicidology
(1971). The psychiatric interview in clinical practice. task force report addressing serious gaps in U.S.
Philadelphia, PA: W. B. Saunders. mental health training. Suicide and Life-Threaten-
MALTSBERGER, J. T. (1986). Suicide risk: ing Behavior, 42, 292–304.
The formulation of clinical judgment. New York: SCHULBERG, H. C., LEE, P. W., BRUCE,
New York University Press. M. L., RAUE, P. J., LEFEVER, J. J., WILLIAMS, J.
MALTSBERGER, J. T. (1992). The psychody- W., ET AL. (2005). Suicide ideation and risk lev-
namic formulation: An aid in assessing suicide risk. els among primary care patients with uncompli-
In R. W. Maris, A. L. Berman, J. T. Maltsberger cated depression. Annals of Family Medicine, 3,
& R. I. Yufit (Eds.), Assessment and prediction of sui- 523–528.
cide (pp. 25–49). New York: Guilford. SHEA, S. C. (1999). The practical art of sui-
MONAHAN, J. (1984). The prediction of cide assessment. New York: John Wiley.
violent behavior: Toward a second generation of SHEA, S. C. (2012). The interpersonal art
theory and policy. American Journal of Psychiatry, of suicide assessment: Interviewing techniques
141, 10–15. for uncovering suicidal intent, ideation, and
MOTTO, J. A. (1989). Problems in suicide actions. In R. I. Simon & R. E. Hales (Eds.),
risk assessment. In D. Jacobs & H. N. Brown The American Psychiatric Publishing textbook of sui-
BERMAN AND SILVERMAN 443

cide assessment and management (2nd ed. pp. 29– SPENGLER, P. M., WHITE, M. J., ÆGISD,
56). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric S. A., MAUGHERMAN, A. S., ANDERSON, L. A.,
Publishing. COOK, R. S., ET AL. (2009). The meta-analysis of
SHNEIDMAN, E. S. (1975). Psychiatric clinical judgment project. The Counseling Psycholo-
emergencies: Suicide. In A. M. Freedman, H. I. gist, 37, 350–399.
Kaplan, & B. J Sadock (Eds.), The modern synopsis STEVENSON, I., & STEPPE, W. M. (1959).
of comprehensive textbook of psychiatry II (pp. 870– The psychiatric examination. In S. Arieti (Ed.),
874). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. American handbook of psychiatry (pp. 215–234).
SILVERMAN, M. M., & BERMAN, A. L. New York: Basic Books.
(2013). Suicide risk assessment and risk formula- SWETS, J. A., DAWES, R. M., & MONAHAN,
tion part I: A focus on suicide ideation in asses- J. (2000). Psychological science can improve
sing suicide risk. Suicide and Life-Threatening diagnostic decisions. Psychological Science in the
Behavior, 44, 420–431. doi: 10.1111/sltb.12065 Public Interest, 1, 1–26.
SIMON, R. I. (2006). Imminent suicide: TUCKMAN, J., & YOUNGMAN, W. F.
The illusion of short-term prediction. Suicide and (1968). A scale for assessing suicidal risk in
Life-Threatening Behavior, 36, 296–301. attempted suicide. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
SIMON, R. I. (2002). Suicide risk assess- 24, 17–19.
ment in managed care settings. Primary Psychia- VAN ORDEN, K. A., WITTE, T. K., GOR-
try, 9(4), 42–49. DON, K. H., BENDER, T. W., & JOINER, T. E.,
SIMON, R. I. (2011). Improving suicide JR. (2008). Suicidal desire and the capability for
risk assessment with evidence-based psychiatry. suicide: Tests of the interpersonal-psychological
In M. Pompili & R. Tatarelli (Eds.), Evidence- theory of suicidal behavior among adults. Journal
based practice in suicidology: A sourcebook (pp. 45– of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 72–83.
54). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe. ZILBOORG, G., & HENRY, G. W. (1941). A
SIMON, R. I. (2012). Suicide risk assess- history of medical psychology. New York: W.W.
ment: Gateway to treatment and management. In Norton.
R. I. Simon & R. E. Hales (Eds.), The American
Psychiatric Publishing textbook of suicide assessment Manuscript Received: June 11, 2013
and management (2nd ed., pp. 3–28). Washington, Revision Accepted: October 3, 2013
DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Potrebbero piacerti anche