Sei sulla pagina 1di 20

Iron Supplementation in Pregnancy or Infancy

and Motor Development: A Randomized


Controlled Trial
a,b c b,d c
Rosa M. Angulo-Barroso, PhD, Ming Li, MD, Denise C.C. Santos, PhD, Yang Bian, BSN, Julie Sturza,
b e b b b,f
MPH, Yaping Jiang, PhD, Niko Kaciroti, PhD, Blair Richards, MPH, Betsy Lozoff, MD

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: Insufficient iron levels for optimal fetal and infant development is abstract

a concern during pregnancy and infancy. The goal of this study was to assess the effects of

iron supplementation in pregnancy and/or infancy on motor development at 9 months.

METHODS: The study was a randomized controlled trial (RCT) of infancy iron supplementation

linked to an RCT of pregnancy iron supplementation, conducted in Hebei, China. A total of

1482 infants were randomly assigned to receive placebo (n = 730) or supplemental iron (n =

752) from 6 weeks to 9 months. Gross motor development (assessed by using the Peabody

Developmental Motor Scale, Second Edition, instrument) was the primary outcome.

Neurologic integrity and motor quality were secondary outcomes.

RESULTS: Motor outcome was available for 1196 infants, divided into 4 supplementation

period groups: (1) placebo in pregnancy/placebo in infancy (n = 288); (2) placebo in

pregnancy/iron in infancy (n = 305); (3) iron in pregnancy/placebo in infancy (n = 298);

and (4) iron in pregnancy/iron in infancy (n = 305). Using the Peabody Developmental

Motor Scale, instrument, iron supplementation in infancy but not pregnancy improved

gross motor scores: overall, P < .001; reflexes, P = .03; stationary, P < .001; and locomotion,

P < .001. Iron supplementation in infancy improved motor scores by 0.3 SD compared

with no supplementation or supplementation during pregnancy alone. Effects of iron

supplementation in infancy alone were similar to effects with iron in both pregnancy and

infancy.

CONCLUSIONS: The RCT design supports the causal inference that iron supplementation in

infancy, with or without iron supplementation in pregnancy, improved gross motor test
scores at 9 months.

NIH

a
Department of Kinesiology, California State University, Northridge, Northridge,
b f
California; Center for Human Growth and Development, and Department of Pediatrics
and Communicable Diseases, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan;
c e
Department of Pediatrics, and Clinical Laboratory, Peking University First Hospital,
d
Beijing, China; and Human Movement Sciences Graduate Program, Methodist
University of Piracicaba, Piracicaba, SP, Brazil

Dr Angulo-Barroso conceptualized and designed the study, acquired data, interpreted


data, and drafted and revised the manuscript; Dr Li conceptualized and designed the
study, acquired data, interpreted data, and critically reviewed the manuscript; Dr Santos
interpreted the data and drafted and revised the manuscript; Ms Bian coordinated and
supervised data collection and critically revised the manuscript; Ms Sturza and Mr
Richards analyzed data and revised the statistical analysis sections critically for
important intellectual content; Dr Jiang acquired data and critically reviewed the
laboratory assessment of iron status sections; Dr Kaciroti conceptualized and designed
the study, analyzed data, and revised the statistical analysis sections critically for
important intellectual content; and Dr Lozoff conceptualized and designed
the study, interpreted data, revised the manuscript, organized funding of the study, and
provided

WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Iron defi ciency in infancy is associated


with poorer motor development. Some randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of iron
supplementation in infancy show positive effects

on motor behavior, but others do not. Few RCTs of iron


supplementation in pregnancy reported motor outcomes.

WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: The study linked an infancy RCT to a pregnancy
RCT of iron supplementation to support causal inferences about developmental
impacts of timing and duration. Iron supplementation in infancy, regardless of
supplementation in

pregnancy, improved gross motor development at 9 months.

To cite: Angulo-Barroso RM, Li M, Santos DC, et al. Iron Supplementation in Pregnancy or Infancy and
Motor Development: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Pediatrics. 2016;137(4):e20153547
Downloaded from by guest on January 18, 2017
ARTICLE

PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 4, April 2016:e20153547

successful motor skills in early 1


such as locomotion, affects the
The acquisition of varied and childhood, especially gross motor skills 2
development of cognitive and socio-
3
emotional capabilities. Movement is deficiency anemia in mothers but had the University of Michigan and Peking
considered to be a little impact on fetal neonatal iron University First Hospital. The RCTs are
17
status ; infancy iron supplementation briefly described here; full details were
17,18
reduced ID at 9 months, with no added reported previously.
vehicle for improving knowledge of self
4–6 benefit of pregnancy iron
and environment such that being 18
7 supplementation. There were no Participants
active assists in learning how to learn. adverse effects of iron supplementation
Many factors, including nutrition, on infant health or growth overall or
8 Participants were infants born to
contribute to motor development. among infants who were iron-sufficient
women in the pregnancy RCT.
Lack of sufficient iron, which is
common during pregnancy and infancy, at birth. We report here the effects on
may have adverse effects through iron’s The pregnancy RCT enrolled 2371
gross motor development, neurologic
9–12
integrity, and quality of motor behavior women with uncomplicated singleton
role in muscle and brain function.
pregnancies between June 2009
Establishing a causal connection at 9 months. This assessment was more
between lack of iron and lower comprehensive than previous studies,
developmental test scores in humans reflecting and December 2011 who were
largely depends on randomized randomized to receive iron/folate or
controlled trials (RCTs) of iron that motor development requires placebo/folate. Most attrition was due
supplementation. In a 2010 expert to mothers giving birth in a
behavioral and motor control as well as
review that organized RCTs of iron skill development. We nonparticipating hospital. In the
supplementation in infancy according infancy RCT, 1482 infants were enrolled
to duration and child age, 6 of 8
13 between December 2009 and June 2012
hypothesized that the greatest impact and were randomly assigned to receive
pertinent RCTs reported benefits on would be when iron supplementation placebo (n =
motor development. coincided with the period of rapid
change in motor development
730) or supplemental iron (n = 752)
The investigators considered the
from 6 weeks to 9 months. Infants with
evidence sufficient to conclude that
(ie, during infancy). We also cord ferritin concentrations suggesting
long-term (>2 months) iron
supplementation during infancy
predicted greater benefits with iron brain ID (<35 μg/L) were excluded. At 9
supplementation during pregnancy months, 1276 infants provided outcome
improves motor development. There is 18
little research on motor outcomes in and infancy than supplementation data (September 2010–March 2013).
infancy with iron supplementation in only 1 period.
during pregnancy. Enrollment and Informed Consent

14
A recent summary included 4 RCTs METHODS Mothers were informed of the infant
and found only 1 that assessed motor development study at prenatal visits.
15
development in infancy. Maternal After delivery, project staff provided
Study Setting and Design further information and obtained
iron/folate supplementation (14
weeks’ gestation to delivery) did signed informed consent.
The study (an RCT of infancy iron
supplementation connected to an RCT Randomization and Masking
not improve infant motor scores in the of pregnancy iron
15,16
first or second year. Together with
the RCTs of iron supplementation in Infants were randomly assigned 1:1 to
supplementation) was designed to
infancy, these the iron or placebo group by a
support causal inferences regarding the
University of Michigan biostatistician
developmental effects of reducing ID in
(N.K.) using PROC SURVEYSELECT in SAS
findings suggest motor development the fetus, young infant, or during both
(SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC). The code
benefits from iron supplementation periods. The design resulted in 4
was broken only after study completion.
during infancy but not pregnancy. groups based on period of
Supplements were in identical dark-
supplementation in pregnancy and/or
colored bottles, and participants and
The present study focused on infancy: (1) placebo in
personnel were unaware of group
developmental impacts of timing and pregnancy/placebo in infancy
assignment.
duration of iron supplementation by (placebo/placebo); (2) placebo in
linking an RCT of iron supplementation pregnancy/iron in infancy (placebo/
in infancy to an RCT of iron iron); (3) iron in pregnancy/placebo in Interventions
supplementation in pregnancy. Iron infancy (iron/placebo); and (4) iron in
status and growth pregnancy/iron in infancy (iron/iron). All participating pregnant women
The study, conducted received daily folate (0.40 mg) and
in rural Hebei Province, China, was either iron (300 mg ferrous sulfate) or
outcomes were reported previously. 17
Pregnancy iron supplementation placebo from enrollment to birth.
reduced iron deficiency (ID) and iron- approved by the ethics committees of Infants received a single daily
Downloaded from by guest on  January 18, 2017
2 BARROSO et al
ANGULO-
dose of ∼1 mg/kg of elemental iron as Madrid, Spain) or carrier (placebo) from 6
an iron protein succinylate oral weeks to 9 months.
18
solution (Ferplex,Italfarmico, S.A.,
Study Outcomes
assessment, the PDMS-2 outcomes are
The primary motor outcome was presented as raw scores, controlling for Statistical Methods
gross motor development, assessed age in days.
by using the Peabody Developmental The primary analytic approach was
Motor Scale, Second Edition (PDMS- The INFANIB assesses infant neurologic 2
19 integrity. The total score of overall based on intention to treat. A χ test and
2), instrument. Secondary analysis of variance
neurologic integrity is a composite derived
outcomes were neurologic integrity, from 20 items within 5 factors
evaluated by using the Infant (spasticity/muscle tone, head and trunk model were used to test for overall
Neurologic International Battery control, vestibular function, legs/lower limb
20
group differences in demographic and
(INFANIB), and motor quality, function, and French angles [shoulder and biologic data. An analysis of covariance
assessed by using the Behavior hip angles]). Items are scored 1 to 5 model was used to test for group
Rating Scale (BRS) of the Bayley (abnormal to normal). Results are expressed differences for primary and secondary
Scales of Infant Development, Second as raw subscale and total scores, controlling outcomes, controlling for age at testing.
20
21 for age. SAS PROC GLMSELECT with stepwise
Edition. inclusion was used to determine if
additional background variables should
The PDMS-2 gross motor dimension at
The BRS motor quality factor is based on be included in
9 months provides an overall motor examiner ratings of infant motor
quotient derived from 3 subscales performance. The factor is generated from
8 items related to muscle tone and the final models. Planned pairwise
(reflexes, stationary, comparisons were conducted if the
movement control and quality. Items are
overall statistical test results were
rated 1 to 5, with higher values indicating
and locomotion). Reflexes reflect 21
significant. For effects of
automatic reactions to environmental more consistently appropriate behavior. supplementation timing, key contrasts
events (eg, righting reflex, parachute Results are expressed as the BRS motor were: (1) iron/placebo versus
reflex). Stationary assesses postural quality factor total raw score, controlling placebo/iron, followed by
control within the center of gravity and for age.
equilibrium (eg, sitting while (2) iron/placebo versus placebo/
manipulating a toy, transitioning placebo and (3) placebo/iron versus
Developmental testing occurred in dedicated
rooms at the Maternity and Child Health Care placebo/placebo to confirm a
to sit from prone). Locomotion covers Center. supplementation effect. For duration,
moving from 1 place to another (eg, key contrasts were: (1) iron/iron versus
crawling, sitting to crawling or iron/placebo and (2) iron/ iron versus
Infants were accompanied by a
19
standing). A Chinese version of the placebo/iron, followed by (3) iron/iron
parent/guardian and given time for
PDMS-2 instrument is routinely used at versus placebo/ placebo. Two different
adjusting to the setting, frequent breaks,
Peking University First Hospital to track types of effect size measures were used:
naps, and/or feeding. US and Chinese 2
motor development and intervention investigators trained Chinese supervisory partial η squared (η ) to express the
effects in the rehabilitation clinic. The personnel, who then jointly trained magnitude of the overall association
clinic follows the standard definition of coders/testers and provided ongoing between group and dependent variable
ceiling but also elicits each child’s supervision. Reliability was assessed before in the analysis of variance model (effect
optimal performance by administering and during testing; reliability levels were sizes were low [0.01], medium [0.06],
a preset maximum number of items in 23
and large [0.14]) and Cohen’s d to
≥90%.
each subscale based on age. Passed
indicate the difference between 2 group
items above ceiling for each subscale means in pooled SD units (small, <0.2;
22 Sample Size
are included. In our study, only a 24
medium, 0.5; and large, 0.8). Based on
primary findings, logistic regression
few infants (31 of 1195) passed items Gross motor outcomes were available for was used to estimate the relative risk
above ceiling, solely in the locomotion 1196 infants. This sample size was (95% confidence interval) of scoring in
subscale. Using scores with passes sufficient to detect small effect size the lowest quartile for gross motor
above ceiling did not affect PDMS-2 development based on iron
differences of ≥0.16 SD between the 2
outcome in the RCT. Therefore, scoring supplementation in infancy. In addition,
was preserved as groups in the pregnancy RCT and ≥0.23 SD multiple/logistic regression was used
for any pairwise comparison among the 4 to model relations between bottles of
customary at Peking University First pregnancy/ infancy groups. iron received and outcomes.
Significance was set at P
Hospital. Because almost all infants
were similar in age at the 9-month Downloaded from by guest on January 18, 2017
< .05.
PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 4, April 2016 3
RESULTS and iron/iron, n = 305. Of the 80 infants
1). There was no differential attrition who were assessed at 9 months but did
according to RCT group. Of the 1276 not provide gross
Attrition in the pregnancy RCT was
largely due to women who gave birth at infants assessed for iron status
17
a nonparticipating hospital. The main motor development data, the
reason for the 14% overall attrition in or growth at 9 months, 1196 had data PDMS-2 tool was not administered
the infancy RCT was refusal or on gross motor development: for 67 and was not scorable for 13.
withdrawal (Fig placebo/placebo, n = 288; placebo/
iron, n = 305; iron/placebo, n = 298;
Participant Characteristics Nonetheless, ID remained common: quartile according to PDMS-2 norms.
19
59% in groups receiving iron in infancy For reflexes and stationary, <2% of
(placebo/iron and iron/iron) versus infants had such low scores. However,
The groups were similar in background
69% in infancy placebo groups 22.1% of locomotion scores were below
characteristics at birth (Table 1). Most
(iron/placebo and placebo/placebo) (P the 25th percentile cutoff. The
infants were first-born. Both genders
< .001). proportions were significantly lower in
were included and approximately
equally represented. Almost all were groups that received supplemental iron
born at term (>37 weeks’ gestation) Study Outcomes in infancy, compared with groups that
and weighed 3.36 kg on average. At 9 did not (P < .001): placebo/iron, 50
months, there was a suggestive overall (16%) of 305; iron/iron, 57 (19%) of
Groups that received iron in infancy 305; placebo/placebo, 70 (24%) of 288;
difference in age at gross motor
(placebo/iron and iron/iron) and iron/placebo, 87 (29%) of 298. The
developmental testing. The
placebo/iron and iron/iron groups
reported significantly better PDMS-2 risk of being in the lowest quartile was
averaged 1.7 days younger than the scores than those that did not reduced by 36% in placebo/iron and
iron/placebo and placebo/ placebo iron/iron groups, compared with the
groups (P = .02). Mean infant weight- (iron only during pregnancy [iron/ iron/placebo and placebo/placebo
for-age z score was 0.89. More than placebo] or in neither time period groups (relative risk, 0.64 [95%
80% of the infants were breastfeeding [placebo/placebo]). The pattern was confidence interval, 0.52–0.80]).
at the time of the 9-month assessment. similar for overall gross motor score (P
Mothers averaged ∼25 years of age, and 2
< .001; p-η = 0.02) and for the There were no group differences in
2 overall neurologic integrity (INFANIB
subscales: reflexes (P = .03; p-η total score, P = .43). However, the
most completed middle school. Most
families were stressed financially; 84% groups differed in the head and trunk
had incomes below the local county 2 factor, which is most related to gross
0.01), stationary (P < .001; p-η = 0.03),
threshold for public housing motor development (P < .001). Scores
2
25 and locomotion (P < .001; p-η were better in groups receiving
assistance. Family support of child
development was similar across
groups. 0.02) (Table 2). iron supplementation in infancy
compared with groups that did not.
Motor quality (examiner BRS ratings)
The groups differed in iron status, as The timing analysis highlighted the
did not show group differences (P
expected by the RCT designs (Table 1). benefits of iron supplementation on
In keeping with the findings gross motor development in infancy.
The placebo/iron group had higher = .93). There were no statistically
PDMS-2 scores than the iron/placebo significant relations between the
of improved maternal iron status with
group (iron only during pregnancy), number of bottles of iron received and
iron supplementation in the
and placebo/iron group scores were motor outcomes.
also higher than the placebo/placebo
17
pregnancy RCT, there was more group scores. The duration analysis
maternal ID in the placebo/iron and indicated no added benefit of iron
placebo/placebo groups than in the supplementation in either pregnancy DISCUSSION
iron/placebo and iron/iron groups (P < or infancy; the placebo/iron and
.001). However, there were no group iron/iron groups did not differ from The uniqueness of our study design (an
differences in fetal-neonatal iron status each other, and both were higher than infancy RCT built upon a pregnancy
at birth. Neonatal iron status was the placebo/placebo group. RCT) addresses specific questions
generally poor, as indicated by cord regarding timing and duration of iron
ferritin levels <75 μg/L or zinc To further characterize the beneficial supplementation and supports causal
protoporphyrin/heme ratio >118 effect of infancy iron supplementation inferences. We found that iron
μmol/mol in >40%. In keeping with on gross motor supplementation from 6 weeks to 9
hematology findings in the infancy RCT, months had a positive effect on overall
18 gross motor development at 9 months.
iron status was worse in the groups performance, we analyzed the
that did not receive iron The effect was similar whether
proportion of infants in each group supplementation was provided only
supplementation in infancy. with subscale scores in the lowest during infancy or also to mothers
Downloaded from by guest on  January 18, 2017
4 ANGULO-BARROSO et al
FIGURE 1
Infancy RCT: fl owchart of participants.

Downloaded from by guest on January 18, 2017
PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 4, April 2016 5
TABLE 1 Infant and Family Characteristics
Characteristic
PP (n = 288)
PI (n = 305)
IP (n = 298)
II (n = 305)
a
P
Infant characteristics at birth

Male sex
155/288 (54)
141/305 (46)
149/298 (50)
158/305 (52)
.29
Birth weight, g
3373.3 ± 373.3
3368.2 ± 375.3
3329.5 ± 370.1
3379.8 ± 350.2
.39
Gestational age, wk
39.7 ± 1.1
39.7 ± 1.1
39.8 ± 1.1
39.7 ± 1.1
.69
First/only child
217/283 (77)
235/298 (79)
240/293 (82)
231/300 (77)
.18
ID: serum ferritin <75 μg/L or zinc protoporphyrin/heme
118/288 (41)
127/305 (42)
131/298 (44)
127/305 (42)
.89
>118 μmol/mol
Infant characteristics at 9 mo

Age at testing, mo
9.31 ± 0.42
9.29 ± 0.41
9.34 ± 0.49
9.25 ± 0.40
.06
9-mo weight-for-age z score
0.88 ± 1.02
0.79 ± 1.09
0.92 ± 0.98
0.97 ± 0.98
.15
b
Milk feeding pattern

.50
Only breast milk
108/217 (50)
115/238 (48)
114/230 (50)
134/237 (57)

Breast milk and formula


73/217 (34)
78/238 (33)
71/230 (31)
70/237 (29)

Only formula
36/217 (17)
45/238 (19)
45/230 (20)
33/237 (14)

c
ID
d,e
195/286 (68)
d,f
179/298 (60)
e
204/296 (69)
f
175/300 (58)

.01
Anemia, hemoglobin <110 g/L
129/286 (45)d
101/298 (34)e
118/296 (40)d,e
119/300 (40)d,e
.05
ID anemiag
108/265 (41)d
81/278 (29)e
99/277 (36)d,e
101/282 (36)d,e
.04
Maternal and family characteristics
Maternal age, y
24.6 ± 4.0)
24.8 ± 3.5)
24.6 ± 3.8)
25.1 ± 4.0
.44
Maternal education, high school or higher
89/287 (31)
115/299 (38)
98/297 (33)
96/302 (32)
.21
Net family income, ≤50 000 yuan/y
243/284 (86)
245/300(82)
236/288 (82)
255/295 (86)
.27
Maternal mood total score at 9 mo (maximum = 30,
6.09 ± 4.54
6.06 ± 4.48)
5.60 ± 4.37
6.38 ± 4.35
.21
h
possible depression ≥10)

Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment


31.5 ± 4.0
31.8 ± 4.0
31.3 ± 4.0
31.5 ± 3.9
.51
total score at 9 mo (maximum = 45)

i
Maternal ID: body iron <0 mg/kg
d
174/284 (61.3)
d
204/304 (67.1)
e
119/294 (40.5)
e
128/302 (42.4)
<.001

Values are n/total (%) for categorical values and mean ± SD for continuous variables. The n values vary due to missing data. II, iron in pregnancy/iron in infancy; IP, iron in pregnancy/ placebo in infancy; PI, placebo in pregnancy/iron in infancy; PP, placebo in
pregnancy/placebo in infancy.
a 2
Analysis of variance for continuous variables, x test for categorical variables.
Feeding solid foods was generally initiated between 4 and 6 months of age. Solids were typically not iron fortifi ed at the time.
b

c d
ID was defi ned as ≥2 abnormal iron measurements (mean corpuscular volume <74 fl , zinc protoporphyrin/heme >69 μmol/mol heme, serum ferritin <12 μg/L). Groups with same superscript do not differ; different letters indicate statistically signifi cant difference (P < .
05).
e f
Groups with same superscript do not differ; different letters indicate statistically signifi cant difference (P < .05). Groups with same superscript do not differ; different letters indicate statistically signifi cant difference (P < .05).
g
Anemia by cutoff defi ned as hemoglobin <110 g/L, and ID was defi ned as ≥2 abnormal iron measurements (mean corpuscular volume <74 fl , zinc protoporphyrin/heme >69 μmol/mol heme, serum ferritin <12 μg/L).
h 26
Maternal mood evaluated by using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale.
i 27
Body iron was calculated by using serum ferritin and soluble transferring receptor (sTfR), according to the formula of Cook et al : body iron (mg/kg) = – [log10(sTfR*1000/ferritin) – 2.8229]/0.1207.

TABLE 2 Primary Outcome: Gross Motor Development Assessed According to the PDMS-2 at 9 Months
Subscale
Mean (95% CI)
a
P
Effect Size d

Timing
Duration

PP (n =288) PI (n =305) IP (n =298) II (n =305) II Versus PP

II Versus
IP Versus PI PP
versus
IP
PP
PI Versus
IP
II Versus
PI

Reflexes Locomotion 87.5 (86.5–

14.4 (14.3– Stationary 39.5 (38.7– 90.0 (89.0–90.9)


33.5 (33.4– 41.4 (40.7–42.2) 87.4 (86.4–
14.6 (14.5–14.7)
34.0 (33.8–34.1) 39.5 (38.8– 89.9 (89.0–
14.4 (14.3–
33.4 (33.3– 41.3 (40.5– <.001
14.7 (14.5– <.001 0.31
b
34.0 (33.8–
.03 <.001 0.30
b
0.01
b
0.16 b 0.01 0.30
0.35
b
0.30
b 0.30
0.00 0.08
b 0.27
b
0.01
0.28
0.16 b 0.03 0.29
b
0.37
b
0.18 0.01 0.28
b gross
88.4)
0.03 0.29
b

b 40.2)
0.19
88.3)
33.7)
90.8)
40.3)
14.6)
42.0)
33.6)
34.1)
14.6)
14.8)

motor

Overall
iron in pregnancy/iron in infancy; IP, iron in pregnancy/placebo in infancy; PI, placebo
in pregnancy/iron in infancy; PP, placebo in pregnancy/placebo in infancy. Subscale n
values vary slightly due to missing data.

a
Analysis of covariance model covarying age at testing. Pairwise comparisons
b
expressed as effect size d (difference between means divided by pooled SD). Signifi
cant difference between groups, P < .05.

CI, confi dence interval; II,


Downloaded from by guest on January 18, 
2017
6 ANGULO-BARROSO et al

during pregnancy. The benefits were There are several possible Gross motor development was assessed
mostly related to stationary and explanations for motor benefits of iron by using 3 different measures to
locomotor skills. Furthermore, iron supplementation in infancy but not include aspects of neuromotor
supplementation during infancy pregnancy. Brain areas mature at development (INFANIB) and motor
reduced the proportion of children in different times and need iron at behavior (BRS motor quality factor) as
the lowest quartile for the locomotor 11,12
different rates. Various motor well as global development
subscale, regardless of whether their domains (eg, reflexes, sensory
mothers received iron integration, postural control, motor (PDMS-2 gross motor). Although there
supplementation during pregnancy. activity, motor coordination and were no group differences in the total
INFANIB score, closer examination of
These results do not seem attributable planning) are subserved by different the factor most
to other factors. The groups were brain areas and networks. Based on
similar with respect to background current understanding, the complex related to gross motor development
characteristics at birth; group brain areas and pathways involved in (head and trunk control) demonstrated
differences in maternal ID were as gross motor development mature most a pattern similar
expected based on results of the rapidly in the first year of
pregnancy RCT. The groups were also
similar in family and infant to PDMS-2 results. We found no
characteristics at 9 months. It is life, thus requiring more iron and differences in BRS motor quality
unlikely that the small difference in increasing vulnerability to lack of
28
iron. Iron is specifically required for factor, in contrast to our previous
testing age accounted for the findings. oligodendrocyte function and myelin results in a small observational
29–32 38
Age was a covariate in all analyses and formation. Consequently, neural study. The BRS is not a direct
did not remain significant in pathways that are involved in motor assessment of motor skills
skill acquisition, such as the
any model. Furthermore, the corticospinal and corticostriatal tracts,
and may thus be less sensitive to the
may be more vulnerable to effects of ID
groups exhibiting more advanced in infancy than during
development of specific motor skills
motor development (placebo/iron and more influenced by tester
and iron/ iron) were the youngest, expertise and experience.
gestation because these pathways are
albeit only by a few days. 33,
not completely myelinated at birth. Several gross motor skills developing
34
The results confirm our hypothesis Iron supplementation in infancy at ∼9 months seemed sensitive to iron
that the greatest impact would might also improve gross motor supplementation in infancy. Our PDMS-
development indirectly by reducing 2 locomotor findings denote better
concurrent behaviors associated with crawling in infants who receive iron
be when iron supplementation 35 supplementation during infancy. Onset
coincided with the period of rapid ID, such as withdrawal and lower
36 of standing with lateral progression (ie,
change in motor development; that is, spontaneous motor activity. Better cruising) also occurs at ∼9 months, as
infancy. Our findings of better motor motor scores in the placebo/ iron and does the ability to pull from sitting to
outcome with iron supplementation in iron/iron groups did not seem to result 39–41
infancy are in agreement with standing. Our stationary subscale
from the potential effects on maternal
behavior of findings imply better performance in
transitioning
a 2010 expert review of previous
13
RCTs. However, the results did not iron supplementation in
from sitting to standing with iron
confirm our prediction of greater pregnancy. Although mothers in supplementation in infancy. A benefit
benefits with iron supplementation the iron/ placebo group had better of iron supplementation in infancy on
during both pregnancy and infancy. iron status than those in the earlier onset of specific motor
The lack of benefit of iron placebo /iron group and might milestones has been reported in some
supplementation during pregnancy on
have been more proactive about previous RCTs.
42–44
Similarly, an
motor development is consistent
their children’s development, as association between better
37
with the sole relevant RCT in a suggested by Perez et al, their
14 infants did not exhibit better iron status in infancy and earlier
recent summary. motor development at 9 months. onset of walking was reported in 2
observational studies.
45,46
These comparable to previous RCTs of iron iron supplementation was less than
supplementation due to differences in typically observed in other infant
locomotor-related benefits of iron
several respects: we used a different 48
RCTs. The likely explanation for the
supplementation may enhance infant motor assessment (PDMS-2 vs Bayley
development in other domains (eg, limited reduction in prevalence of ID
or motor milestones in other studies),
2,3,40,47
cognitive, social-emotional). our population was growing well and
mainly breast-fed, and the prevalence was a combination of poor iron status
of ID was higher than in some other at birth, high iron needs for growth,
Our results may not be directly and insufficient supplemental iron
studies. The hematologic response to
Downloaded from by guest on  January 18, 2017
PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 4, April 2016 7
CONCLUSIONS infancy RCTs and overall supervision of
18 the infancy RCT; and Dr Guobin Xu for
intake. Furthermore, the magnitude supervision of laboratory measures of
of the effects we observed might not The RCT design of this study supports iron status. We also appreciate the
generalize to other populations. For the causal inference that iron dedicated efforts of the project
instance, effects might be stronger in supplementation in infancy, with or physicians and nurses at Sanhe Maternal
samples with a greater reduction in ID without iron supplementation in and Child Health Center.
with iron supplementation. Because pregnancy, improved gross motor test
infant complementary foods were not scores at 9 months. The study confirms
generally iron fortified at the time of developmental benefits
our study, and delayed cord clamping
was not routine, the results do not of routine iron supplementation in ABBREVIATIONS
contribute to the discussion regarding infancy, perhaps especially in settings
alternatives in which iron deficiency is common. BRS:  Behavior Rating Scale ID: iron
deficiency INFANIB: Infant Neurologic
to iron supplementation. In any case, ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
our results regarding timing and
duration require replication, and International Battery PDMS-2: 
We thank Drs Gengli Zhao and Min Peabody Developmental
further research is needed on the
mechanisms whereby iron Zhou for overall direction of the preg-
nancy RCT; Drs Zhixiang Zhang, Twila Motor Scale, Second Edition
Tardif, and Xing Li for help
supplementation can improve coordinating the pregnancy and
infant motor development. RCT:  randomized controlled trial
views of funding 21, 2015 Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
sources. The
authors had full
control of primary Address FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: Dr Lozoff was an unpaid speaker at 2 seminars
overall supervision of supported by Lee’s Pharmaceutical Holdings Limited. The topic was iron defi
the research group. data and did not correspondence to
Rosa M. Angulo- ciency and child development (Shanghai, April 11, 2010, and Beijing, May 15,
All authors approved have an agreement 2011). The company covered hotel accommodations and, for the 2011 seminar,
the fi nal manuscript with the funders that Barroso, PhD, internal airfare between Hangzhou and Beijing. The other authors have indicated
as submitted. All limited their ability to Department of they have no fi nancial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
authors agree to be complete the Kinesiology,
accountable for all research as California State
planned. FUNDING: A grant from the National Institutes of Health (R01 HD052069), which
aspects of the work University, included funding from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health
in ensuring that Redwood Hall 250, and Human Development and the Offi ce of Dietary Supplements, provided support for
questions related to 18111 Nordhoff St, the infancy study and laboratory measures of iron status
This trial has been
the accuracy or Northridge, CA for mothers and infants (Dr Lozoff, Principal Investigator). Vifor
registered at
integrity of any part 91330-8287. E- Pharma, Ltd provided fi nancial support for the pregnancy study. The
of the work are www.clinicaltrials.g
ov (identifi er mail: São Paulo Research Foundation–FAPESP/Brazil (2014/00018-0) and
appropriately rosa.angulobarroso Methodist University of Piracicaba–UNIMEP/Brazil provided fi nancial
investigated and NCT00613717).
@csun.edu support for Dr Santos. Funded by the National Institutes of Health
resolved.
(NIH).
DOI:
10.1542/peds.201 PEDIATRICS (ISSN
The content is solely
5-3547 Numbers: Print, POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated
the responsibility of
the authors and does 0031-4005; Online, they have no potential confl icts of interest to disclose.
not necessarily 1098-4275).
represent the offi cial
Accepted for
publication Dec REFERENCES
Clearfi eld :214–241 Dev. 2008;17 ment in 9-
MW. The 2000;24( (3):213– month-old
Anderson DI, Gibson JJ. The Ecological Approach to Visual 218 infants in
Campos JJ, role of Perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin; 1979 4):385–
Clearfi eld relation to
crawling
Witherington MW. 397 cultural
DC, et al. and Learning to Angulo-
walking Piaget J. The Construction of Reality in differences
The role of walk Barroso and iron
locomotion in experience changes the Child. New York, NY: Basic Books; Adolph RM,
KE. status.
psychological in infant infants’ social 1954 Schapir
Learning Dev
development. spatial interactions. o L, Psychobiol
Front memory. J Infant Behav to move. Liang
Thelen E. Motor development as foundation and Curr Dir .
Psychol. Exp Child Dev. W, et al. 2011;53(2)
2013;4:440 Psychol. 2011;34(1):1 future of Psychol Motor :196–210
2004;89(3) 5–25 developmental psychology. Int J Behav Sci. develop
Wang J, s K. cellular iron metabolism. Biochem J.
Pantopoulo Regulation of 2011;434(3):365–381
Downloaded from by guest on January 18, 2017
ANGULO- et al
8 BARROSO
TX: PRO-ED, Inc; 2000 1996;17(2):83–93

Beard JL. Iron biology in immune function,


muscle metabolism and neuronal Roncagliolo M, Garrido M, Walter T, Peirano P,
Ellison PH, Horn JL, Browning CA.
functioning. J Nutr. Lozoff B. Evidence of altered central nervous
Construction of an Infant Neurological system development in infants with iron defi
2001;131(2S-2):568S–579S, International Battery (INFANIB) for the ciency anemia at 6 mo: delayed maturation of
discussion 580S assessment of neurological integrity in auditory brainstem responses. Am J Clin Nutr.
infancy. Phys Ther. 1985;65(9):1326– 1998;68(3):683–690
Lozoff B, Georgieff MK. Iron defi ciency and 1331
brain development. Semin Pediatr Neurol. Cai J, Zhang YP, Shields LB, et al. Correlation
2006;13(3):158–165 between
Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development.
San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
Georgieff MK, Brunette KE, Tran PV. Early Corporation; 1993 electrophysiological properties,
life nutrition and neural plasticity. Dev morphological maturation, and olig gene
Psychopathol. 2015;27(2):411–423 Zhao G, Bian Y, Li M. Impact of passing changes during postnatal motor tract
items above the ceiling on the assessment development. Dev Neurobiol.
Grantham-McGregor S, Baker-Henningham results of Peabody developmental motor 2013;73(9):713–722
H. Iron defi ciency in childhood: causes and scales [in Chinese]. Beijing Da Xue Xue Bao.
consequences for childhood development. 2013;45(6):928–932
Annales Nestle. 2010;68:105–119 Rothwell J. Control of Human
Murphy KR, Myors B. Statistical Power Voluntary Movement. London, UK:
Saint SE, Frick JE. Prenatal supplementation Analysis: A Simple and General Model for Chapman & Hall; 1994
and its effects on early childhood cognitive Traditional and Modern Hypothesis Tests.
outcome. In: Wallace TC. Dietary Supplements Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2004 Lozoff B, Klein NK, Nelson EC, McClish DK,
in Health Promotion. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor Manuel M, Chacon ME. Behavior of infants with
and Francis Group; 2015:75-104 iron-defi ciency anemia.
Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the
Child Dev. 1998;69(1):24–36
Li Q, Yan H, Zeng L, et al. Effects of maternal
Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY:
multimicronutrient supplementation on the Academic Press; 1977
mental development of infants in rural Angulo-Kinzler RM, Peirano P, Lin E,
Sanhe People’s Government. Sanhe public
Garrido M, Lozoff B. Spontaneous motor
housing benefi ts guidelines. 2013. Available at: activity in human infants with iron-defi
western China: follow-up evaluation of a ciency anemia. Early Hum Dev.
double-blind, randomized, controlled trial. www.he.xinhuanet.
com/zfwq/sanhe/zhengwu/zhengwu/ 2013- 2002;66(2):67–79
Pediatrics. 2009;123(4). Available at:
10/21/c_117803056.htm. Accessed on June 15,
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/ 2014
full/123/4/e685 Perez EM, Hendricks MK, Beard JL, et al.
Mother-infant interactions and infant
Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection development are altered by maternal iron defi
Chang S, Zeng L, Brouwer ID, Kok FJ, Yan of postnatal depression. Development of the ciency anemia. J Nutr. 2005;135(4):850–855
H. Effect of iron defi ciency anemia in 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression
pregnancy on child mental Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782–786
development in rural China. Pediatrics. Shafi r T, Angulo-Barroso R, Jing Y,
2013;131(3). Available at: www. Angelilli ML, Jacobson SW, Lozoff B. Iron
pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/131/3/ e755 Cook JD, Flowers CH, Skikne BS. The defi ciency and infant motor development.
quantitative assessment of body iron. Blood. Early Hum Dev. 2008;84(7):479–485
2003;101(9):3359–3364
Zhao G, Xu G, Zhou M, et al. Prenatal iron
supplementation reduces maternal anemia, WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study
iron defi ciency, and iron defi ciency anemia Rice D, Barone S Jr. Critical periods of Group. WHO Motor Development Study:
in a randomized clinical trial in rural China, vulnerability for the developing nervous windows of achievement for six gross motor
but iron defi ciency remains widespread in system: evidence from humans and development milestones.
mothers and neonates. J Nutr. animal models. Environ Health Perspect.
2015;145(8):1916–1923 2000;108(suppl 3):511–533
Acta Paediatr Suppl. 2006;450(suppl
450):86–95
Lozoff B, Jiang Y, Li X, et al. Low-dose Algarín C, Peirano P, Garrido M, Pizarro F,
iron supplementation in infancy Lozoff B. Iron defi ciency anemia
modestly increases infant iron status at in infancy: long-lasting effects on auditory Bertenthal BI, Campos JJ, Barrett KC. Self-
and visual system functioning. Pediatr Res. produced locomotion: an organizer of
9 months without decreasing growth or
2003;53(2):217–223 emotional cognitive, and social development
increasing illness in a randomized in infancy. In:
clinical trial in rural China [published Emde R, Harmon R, eds. Continuity and
online ahead of print January 20, 2016]. Beard JL, Connor JR. Iron status and Discontinuities in Development. New York,
J Nutr. doi:10.3945/ jn.115.223917 neural functioning. Annu Rev Nutr. NY: Plenum; 1984
2003;23:41–58
Folio MK, Fewell R. Peabody Brill B. Motor development and cultural
Developmental Motor Scales: Connor JR, Menzies SL. Relationship of iron to attitudes. Themes in Motor Development.
Examiner’s Manual. 2nd ed. Austin, oligodendrocytes and myelination. Glia. 1986;35:297–313
Downloaded from by guest on  January 18, 2017
PEDIATRICS Volume 137, number 4, April 2016 9
JB, Walter T, Pino P. Behavioral and ciency anemia in healthy full-term infants.
developmental effects of preventing iron-defi Pediatrics. 2003;112(4):846–854
Lozoff B, De Andraca I, Castillo M, Smith
controlled study. BMJ.
2001;323(7326):1389–1393
Olney DK, Pollitt E, Kariger PK, et al. Biringen Z, Emde RN, Campos JJ,
Combined iron and folic acid Appelbaum MI. Affective reorganization in
Kariger PK, Stoltzfus RJ, Olney D, et al. Iron the infant, the mother, and the dyad: the role
defi ciency and physical growth predict of upright locomotion and its timing. Child
supplementation with or without zinc
attainment of walking but not crawling in Dev. 1995;66(2):499–514
reduces time to walking unassisted among
poorly nourished Zanzibari infants. J Nutr.
Zanzibari infants 5- to 11-mo old. J Nutr.
2005;135(4):814–819
2006;136(9):2427–2434 Pasricha SR, Hayes E, Kalumba K, Biggs BA.
Effect of daily iron
Stoltzfus RJ, Kvalsvig JD, Chwaya HM, et al. Siegel EH, Stoltzfus RJ, Kariger PK, et al.
Effects of iron supplementation Growth indices, anemia, and diet
supplementation on health in children
independently predict motor milestone
aged 4-23 months: a systematic review
and anthelmintic treatment on motor and and meta-analysis of randomised
language development of preschool acquisition of infants in south central Nepal. J controlled trials. Lancet Glob Health.
Nutr. 2005;135(12):2840–2844 2013;1(2):e77–e86
children in Zanzibar: double blind, placebo

Downloaded from by guest on 
January 18, 2017
10 ANGULO-BARROSO et al
Iron Supplementation in Pregnancy or Infancy and Motor Development: A 
Randomized Controlled Trial

Rosa M. Angulo­Barroso, Ming Li, Denise C.C. Santos, Yang Bian, Julie Sturza, 
Yaping Jiang, Niko Kaciroti, Blair Richards and Betsy Lozoff

Pediatrics; originally published online March 2, 2016; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015­
3547

Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services /content/early/2016/02/29/peds.2015­3547.full.html

References This article cites 36 articles, 16 of which can be accessed free
at:
/content/early/2016/02/29/peds.2015­3547.full.html#ref­list­1

Citations This article has been cited by 2 HighWire­hosted articles:
/content/early/2016/02/29/peds.2015­3547.full.html#related­u
rls

Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in
the following collection(s):
Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
/cgi/collection/development:behavioral_issues_sub
Growth/Development Milestones
/cgi/collection/growth:development_milestones_sub
Nutrition
/cgi/collection/nutrition_sub

Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml

Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly 
publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, 
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk 
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All 
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031­4005. Online ISSN: 1098­4275.

Downloaded from by guest on January 18, 2017
Iron Supplementation in Pregnancy or Infancy and Motor Development: 
A Randomized Controlled Trial

Rosa M. Angulo­Barroso, Ming Li, Denise C.C. Santos, Yang Bian, Julie 
Sturza, Yaping Jiang, Niko Kaciroti, Blair Richards and Betsy Lozoff

Pediatrics; originally published online March 2, 2016; DOI: 
10.1542/peds.2015­3547

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, 
is located on the World Wide Web at:
/content/early/2016/02/29/peds.2015­3547.full.html

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication,
it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, and trademarked
by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, 
Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print
ISSN: 0031­4005. Online ISSN: 1098­4275.

Downloaded from by guest on January 18, 2017

Potrebbero piacerti anche