Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKIJCInternational Journal of Consumer Studies1470-6423 2006 The Author; Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing

g LtdOctober 200631220229Original Article Gender differences in green


purchase behaviourM.M. Mostafa

International Journal of Consumer Studies ISSN 1470-6431

Gender differences in Egyptian consumers green purchase


behaviour: the effects of environmental knowledge, concern
and attitude
Mohamed M. Mostafa
College of Business Administration, Gulf University for Science and Technology, Hawally, Kuwait

Keywords Abstract
Consumer behaviour, Egypt, environmental
gender differences, green marketing. This study investigates the influence of three cognitive and attitudinal factors on gender
differences in green purchase behaviour. Using a large sample size (n = 1093), a survey has
Correspondence been developed and administered across Egypt. The findings from the multivariate analysis
Mohamed M. Mostafa, College of Business of variance (MANOVA) confirm the influence of consumers ecological knowledge, con-
Administration, Gulf University for Science and cern and attitude on gender differences in green purchase behaviour. Consistent with
Technology, PO Box 7202, Hawally 32093, previous studies, this study found that women appeared to be less aware of environmental
Kuwait. issues compared with men. However, contrary to other studies conducted in the West, men
E-mail: mostafa@usa.com showed more environmental concern and more positive outlook towards green purchase
compared with women. The study discusses how the present findings may help policy
doi: 10.1111/j.1470-6431.2006.00523.x makers and marketers alike to fine-tune their environmental and marketing programmes.

were unsure if they would pay more for green products. On the
Introduction average, those respondents who indicated that they would pay more
Green consumerism is described as being a multifaceted concept, for green products were willing to pay between 15% and 20% more.
which includes preservation of the environment, minimization of As a result of the increasing number of green consumers, mar-
pollution, responsible use of non-renewable resources and animal keters are targeting the green segment of the population. Recycled
welfare and species preservation (McEachern and McClean, paper and plastic goods and dolphin-safe tuna are examples of
2002). Green or environmentally friendly products are broadly products positioned on the basis of environmental appeal (Baner-
defined as products that will not pollute the earth or deplore jee et al., 1995). Marketers are also incorporating the environment
natural resources, and can be recycled or conserved (Shamdasani into many marketing activities, including product and package
et al., 1993). Some examples of these products are household design (Bhat, 1993; Polonsky et al., 1997) and pricing (Kapelianis
items manufactured with post-consumer plastics or paper, recycla- and Strachan, 1996). Marketers have even gone as far as to develop
ble or reusable packaging, energy-efficient light bulbs and deter- specific models for the development of green advertising and
gents containing ingredients that are biodegradable, non-polluting green marketing strategy (McDaniel and Rylander, 1993; Menon
and free of synthetic dyes or perfumes. and Menon, 1997). Nowadays, environmental consciousness is not
Evidence suggests that a growing number of consumers in the only an ideology of activists, but also a matter of market compe-
US and Western Europe are becoming more environmentally tition (McCloskey and Maddock, 1994), which influences con-
responsible in terms of their personal habits and lifestyles (Stone sumer behaviour (Follows and Jobber, 1999). DSouza (2004)
et al., 1995). In a worldwide study, Starch (1996) identified the total claims that the green vision is a reality and needs to be more
number of green consumers as being 15%. In the UK 10% are functionally understood to allow marketers to develop strategies
recognized as being hardcore green consumers (Curlo, 1999). aimed to meet the green consumers needs.
Many US polls since the 1990s indicate that the percentage of Compared with what has been happening in the West, consum-
consumers with a strong degree of environmental awareness ranges ers in Egypt, as well as in the wider context of the Arab world, are
from 37% to 96% (Erickson and Kramer-Leblanc, 1991). Recent just at the stage of green awakening. For example, Egypt has
polls also report that 50% of Americans claim to look for environ- passed recently a sweeping, 103-article law intended to clean up
mental labels and to switch brands based on environment- Egypts air, land and water, as well as to protect the Mediterranean
friendliness (Phillips, 1999). In a study of Australian consumers, and Red Sea coasts. It also affords greater protection for wildlife
Suchard and Polonsky (1991) found that 61.5% of the respondents (Napoli, 1994). This may explain the fact that little is understood
would pay more for environmentally safe products while 22.2% about consumers green purchase behaviour in this part of the

220 International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
M.M. Mostafa Gender differences in green purchase behaviour

world. Indeed, researchers agree that very little research has been ately, but statistically significant positive association, has been
conducted concerning cross-cultural studies on environmental atti- further supported by Grunerts (1993) study on the purchase of
tudes or behaviour of different ethnic, cultural or religious groups green products.
(Klineberg, 1998; Schultz and Zelezny, 1999). The timing is there- Arcury (1990) suggested that an individuals gender may be a
fore right for consumer research that examines gender differences factor in the amount of environmental knowledge he or she pos-
in green purchase behaviours in an Arab non-Western context. sesses as well as the amount of the individual displays for the
Virtually no pertinent research has been undertaken in Egypt or the environment. Gendall and Smith (1995) compared knowledge of
Arab world in the field of green marketing. To remedy this void in environmental facts across six countries. In all six countries men
the literature, this study attempts to look at the influence of various tended to have higher levels of knowledge than women. Tikka
cognitive and attitudinal factors on the gender differences in green et al. (2000, p. 16) found that knowledge of nature and the envi-
purchase behaviour of Egyptian consumers. ronment seemed to be dependent on gender because the mean
The objective of this research is to examine how perceptions of knowledge points scored by men were higher than those scored by
environmental knowledge, concern and attitude relate to gender. women. In a recent study in Upper Egypt, Briggs et al. (2003)
While consumer attitudes towards environmental issues have been found that womens environmental knowledge are becoming more
widely canvassed during recent years, there have been remarkably spatially restricted than those that many men possess. Thus, it is
few gender-specific studies of this phenomenon. For example, hypothesized that:
Wehrmeyer and McNeil (2000) suggest that little information H1: Men are more likely to have more environmental knowl-
exists on gender differences in environmental perception and val- edge compared with women.
ues. The aim of this research is to fill this research gap by examin-
ing gender differences in Egyptian consumers green purchase
Concern
behaviour. More specifically, we aim to analyse gender differences
in Egyptian consumers: Environmental concern has been traditionally viewed as a unidi-
environmental knowledge; mensional construct ranging from unconcerned about the environ-
environmental concern; and ment at the low end to concerned at the high end, as measured by
green purchase attitudes. the new environmental paradigm (NEP) (Milfont and Duckitt,
This paper will first review relevant literature and develop research 2004). On the other hand, Schultz (2000) proposed that environ-
hypotheses. Next, methodology and results will be discussed. mental concerns have three correlated factors: concern for the self
Implications section follows. Finally, research limitations and (egoistic), other people (altruistic) and the biosphere (biospheric).
directions for future research will be presented. In this study, the Academic studies examining environmental concern include
terms green, environmentally friendly and environmentally Fritzsche and Dueher (1982) who examined the effects of concern
conscious are used interchangeably. for the environment on the choice of deodorant container, and
Kinnear and Taylor (1973) who examined the effects of ecological
concern on attitudes towards phosphates in laundry detergents.
Literature review and Other studies include: Prothero and McDonald (1992), examining
hypotheses development green cosmetics and toiletries, and Barr et al. (2003), examining
Drawing on research from North America, Australasia and recycled packaging. Baldassare and Katz (1992) and Seguin et al.
Europe, there is a wealth of evidence that suggest that a wide (1998) have argued that environmental concern can have a signifi-
variety of factors influence gender differences in green purchase cant impact on the degree to which individuals are motivated to
behaviour. These can be characterized as environmental knowl- change behavioural practices so as to attempt to alleviate the
edge, concern and attitudes. problem. In a number of studies, environmental concern has been
found to be a major determinant of buying organic food (e.g.
Grunert, 1993). However, it should be noted that reviews of the
Knowledge
many studies analysing the direct empirical relationship between
Environmental knowledge can be defined as a general knowledge environmental concern and behaviour all agree in the conclusion
of facts, concepts and relationships concerning the natural envi- that this relation is low to moderate (e.g. Hines et al., 1987). Taken
ronment and its major ecosystems (Fryxell and Lo, 2003, p. 45). together, the general attitude environmental concern seems to
Thus, environmental knowledge involves what people know about explain not more than 10% variance of specific environmental
the environment, key relationships leading to environmental behaviours (Bamberg, 2003).
aspects or impacts, an appreciation of whole systems, and col- Mohai (1992) and Stern (1992) found that women are more
lective responsibilities necessary for sustainable development. concerned about environmental issues than are men and that there
Schahn and Holzer (1990) have distinguished two types of knowl- are value and belief differences between men and women regard-
edge when examining environmental action: abstract and concrete. ing the environment. In their exhaustive review of the literature,
The former relates to knowledge concerning environmental issues: Davidson and Freudenberg (1996) conclude that, under certain
problems, causes, solutions and so on. The latter relates to behav- circumstances, women express greater environmental concern
ioural knowledge that can be utilized and acted upon. In their than do men. In a study of 231 pre-college students, Riechard and
analysis of 128 previous studies, Hines et al. (1987) argued that Peterson (1998) found that female students had significantly
abstract knowledge was the most significant type when predicting higher perception of environmental risk scores than male students.
environmental action. They have noted an average correlation of Theoretical explanations for gender differences involve the
0.30 between ecological knowledge and behaviour. This moder- increased knowledge of and tolerance for technological advances,

International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd 221
Gender differences in green purchase behaviour M.M. Mostafa

support for economic growth and lower perception of environmen- the comprehensive survey, three marketing professors were first
tal risks among men (Blocker and Eckberg, 1997). For example, invited to assess the foregoing measurement instrument. After one
the trust in science and technology hypothesis postulates that pre-test with 40 subjects, the final version of the questionnaire
women are more likely than men to lack faith in science and included two sections. The first section consisted of demographic
technology, and are therefore more concerned about environmen- data such as age, gender and education level. The second section
tal problems and less likely to support a technological fix covered the following topics: perceived environmental knowledge,
(Davidson and Freudenburg, 1996). Some eco-feminist writings environmental concern and green purchase attitude.
also suggest that women are potentially more environmentalist Collecting data by mail surveys in the Arab world has been very
than men because a biospheric orientation (Merchant, 1979). difficult (Nasif et al., 1991; Harzing, 1997). In order to ensure an
Thus, it is hypothesized that: acceptable number of responses, a convenience sample was used.
H2: Women are more likely to express higher environmental A network of contacts at universities throughout Egypt cooperated
concern compared with men. in distributing and returning the questionnaire. All of the contacts
were university professors or administrators. Each contact re-
ceived a packet containing between 100 and 200 questionnaires,
Attitudes
depending on the number of students with whom they interacted.
The social psychology literature on behavioural research has The questionnaires were administered to groups of students who
established attitudes as important predictors of behaviour, behav- completed them in the classroom. Students responded voluntarily
ioural intention and explanatory factors of variants in individual and were not compensated for their participation.
behaviour (Kotchen and Reiling, 2000). Many studies have specif- A total of 1500 questionnaires were distributed. Confidentiality
ically focused on the relationship between environmental attitudes of responses was emphasized in the cover letter with the title
and environmentally related behaviours. These studies include Confidential survey and in the text. To reduce social desirability
investigations of environmental attitudes and political participa- artefacts, the cover letter indicated that the survey seeks attitudes
tion (Mohai, 1990, 1992), choice of recreational activities (Luzar towards green purchase and nothing else. In total, 1274 responses
et al., 1995) and willingness to modify behaviour (Walsh and were received by the cut-off date, but 181 questionnaires were
McGuire, 1992). Consumer attitudes have also been used in past discarded because the respondents failed to complete the research
studies to predict energy conservation and ecologically conscious instrument appropriately. The effective sample size, thus, was
purchase and use of products. Kassarjian (1971) studied consum- 1093. Around half of the participants were female (52.8%).
ers reaction towards a gasoline that reduced air pollution and Almost 22% of the students were seniors, 17% were juniors, 25%
found that attitude towards air pollution was the most important were sophomore, 27% were freshmen and 9% were graduate stu-
variable in determining consumers behaviour towards the prod- dents. Average age in the sample was 22.7 years.
uct. In a study conducted in Germany, Balderjahn (1988) found Although the extent to which student subjects represent the
that a positive attitude towards ecologically conscious living general population can be debated (Greenberg, 1987; Gordon
resulted in ecologically responsible buying and using of products, et al., 1987), using university students as subjects has been a
including the use of automobiles. It also prompted consumers to research method practiced worldwide for many years, mainly for
publicly show environmental concern by signing ecologically rel- their accessibility to the researcher and homogeneity as a group
evant petitions and supporting or joining an anti-pollution organi- (Calder et al., 1981). US research found marketing students
zation. The usual findings reveal a strong association between responses to questions relating to environmental consciousness to
environmental attitude and ecological behaviour (e.g. Lynne and be similar to those recorded by the general public as a whole
Rola, 1988). However, a number of studies have found either a (Synodinos, 1991).
moderate relationship between environmental attitude and ecolog-
ical behaviour (e.g. Axelrod and Lehman, 1993; Smith et al.,
Measures
1994) or a weak relationship (e.g. Berger and Corbin, 1992).
Many studies have shown gender differences in environmental All constructs used in this study were measured by various items
attitudes. Brown and Harris (1992) found statistically significant on five-point Likert-type scales (1 = completely disagree to
differences between men and women in environmental attitudes 5 = completely agree). It is widely believed that attitudes are best
and their opinion about forest policy preferences. Tikka et al. measured by way of multiple measures and the general trend in
(2000) found significant differences in environmental attitude measuring environmental issues is via several items instead of
index scores between the two genders with men having more single-item questions (Gill et al., 1986). The items contain an
negative attitudes towards the environment compared with explicit key expression representing the specific construct (see
women. Thus, it is hypothesized that: Appendix). Positive and negative formulations of the items were
H3: Women are more likely to express positive attitudes towards presented for guaranteeing the content balance of the study. All
green products compared with men. items are based on scales that have been previously validated.
Perceived environmental knowledge was measured using the
perceived knowledge of environmental issues scale (Ellen et al.,
Method 1997). The scale was found to be valid and reliable with reported
value of 0.86 (Mohr et al., 1998). This scale has been chosen
Sample
because other efforts are either seriously dated or unreliable or
The questionnaire used in this study was an attractive three-page both. For example, Maloney et al. (1975) proposed a 15-item
booklet with a cover page of brief instructions. Before conducting instrument to measure environmental knowledge. Later, Benton

222 International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
M.M. Mostafa Gender differences in green purchase behaviour

(1994) encountered unacceptable internal consistency in using this the author translated the scales items into Arabic. Then, these
scale ( = 0.38). Recent use of the scale has also given highly Arabic items were back-translated into English by a bilingual
skewed distributions, with a high number of respondents giving expert to make sure that the original content was kept in translation
correct answers to the questions utilized, rendering the data col- to decrease discrepancies between the English and the Arabic
lected unusable (Kristensen and Grunert, 1991). Weaver (2002) measurements. No individual items were problematic in transla-
developed a three-item environmental knowledge scale. These tion. The translated version was cross-checked independently by
items include knowledge about the greenhouse effect, the relation- another group of bilingual researchers and was pre-tested to allow
ship between pesticides and chemicals used in food and human final adjustments before full-scale applications.
health, and the environmental consequences of car pollution. Ini- As the scales that were used to measure the general environ-
tially, these three items were grouped together as an index, but the mental attitudes and knowledge domains had not previously been
index did not meet reliability standards with Cronbachs alpha cross-culturally validated in an Arab non-Western context, the
well below 0.5. calculation of itemtotal correlations for the pooled data was first
Unlike previous research (e.g. Chan, 2001), we measure knowl- used as a basis for detecting poor items. Items with itemtotal
edge of environmental issues using perceived environmental correlation of 0.30 or less were eliminated from the analysis.
knowledge instead of factual environmental knowledge. Rolston Following recommendations by Anderson and Gerbing (1988), the
and Di Benedetto (1994) warned researchers against measuring retained items were subjected to an exploratory principal compo-
factual consumer knowledge about environmental issues because nent analysis, separately for each scale, to further investigate the
even experts cannot agree on a products effect on the environ- unidimensionality of the scales. We chose the oblique rotation
ment. In fact research documents the low level of knowledge because the attitude and behavioural dimensions are expected to
consumers have about environmental issues and the meaning of be correlated among themselves. Advocates of the oblique rotation
environmental terms (e.g. Ellen, 1994). In their review of 24 assert that in the real world important factors are likely to be
surveys on environmental issues, Hastak et al. (1994) report, for correlated; thus searching for unrelated factors is unrealistic
example, that a majority of consumers know the term recyclable (Dixon, 1993). Finally, the retained items were combined into sum
but many do not have a detailed understanding of the term and its scales and reliabilities and means were calculated.
implications for purchasing decisions. Three items were eliminated from the NEP scale to produce an
There have been numerous environmental concern scales that acceptable reliability. This is consistent with previous research
have been developed each with various advantages and disadvan- reporting the use of the NEP scale cross-culturally. For example,
tages; however, owing to its sound psychometric properties and Bechtel et al. (1999) eliminated two items from the NEP scale
relatively small number of items, the new NEP scale was chosen when it was administered in Brazil. The reliabilities, measured
for use in the present study. The new revised NEP scale (Dunlap with Cronbachs alpha, were 0.778, 0.804 and 0.932 for the per-
et al., 2000) is an updated and expanded 15-item version of its ceived environmental knowledge scale, environmental concern
predecessor, which has been used for over two decades (Dunlap scale and green purchase attitude scale respectively. Nunnally
and Van Liere, 1978). While several of the items remain, the new (1978) suggested that an alpha value of 0.7 is acceptable. The
NEP differs in the way items are selected to illicit five facets alpha values found for the scales indicated therefore that they are
of environmental concern: reality of limits to growth; anti- sufficiently reliable measures of environmental knowledge, con-
anthropocentrism; the fragility of natures balance; rejection of the cern and attitudes towards green purchase.
idea that humans are exempt from the constraints of nature; and
the possibility of an eco-crisis or ecological catastrophe. The anal-
ysis of new NEP results has revealed predictive and construct
Results
validity in addition to a marginal increase of internal consistency For the statistical analysis of the questionnaires, SPSS 13.0 was
from the original scale (Dunlap et al., 2000). selected because of its ability to extensively analyse quantitative
In the environmental psychology literature, NEP is commonly data. A one-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)
taken to measure general environmental concern (Poortinga et al., was performed to investigate gender differences in green purchase
2004, p. 72). The scale has been used to examine environmental behaviour. Three dependent variables were used: environmental
concern in the US (e.g. Caron, 1989; Nooney et al., 2003), Canada knowledge, environmental concern and attitudes towards green
(Edgell and Nowell, 1989), Sweden (Widegren, 1998), the Baltic purchase. The independent variable was gender. Preliminary
States (Gooch, 1995), Turkey (Furman, 1998) and Japan (Pierce assumption testing was conducted to check for normality, linear-
et al., 1999). This study represents the first application of the scale ity, univariate and multivariate outliers, homogeneity of variance
in the Arab world. covariance matrices and multi-collinearity, with no serious
Green purchase attitude was measured using a three-item scale violations noted. There was a statistically significant difference
that found to be valid and reliable (Taylor and Todd, 1995). The between men and women on the combined dependent variables:
three statements coded by five-point semantic-differential scales [F (3, 1089) = 243.063, P < 0.001; Wilks Lambda = 0.599;
were used in previous research to operationalize the global mea- partial eta squared = 0.401. Table 1 shows the multivariate tests
sure of attitudes towards green purchase (Chan, 2001). conducted on the combined dependent variables.
When the results for the dependent variables were considered
separately, all the differences reached statistical significance using
Procedures
a Bonferroni adjusted alpha level of 0.017. This adjustment has
The Arabic version of the scales was created through careful been used to reduce the chance of a Type I error finding a
translation and back-translation techniques (McGorr, 2000). First, significant result when in fact there is not one (Tabachnick and

International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd 223
Gender differences in green purchase behaviour M.M. Mostafa

Table 1 Multivariate tests

Effect Value F Hypothesis d.f. Error d.f. Significance Partial eta squared

Pillais trace 0.401 243.063 3 1089 0.000 0.401


Wilks lambda 0.599 243.063 3 1089 0.000 0.401
Hotellings trace 0.670 243.063 3 1089 0.000 0.401
Roys largest root 0.670 243.063 3 1089 0.000 0.401

Table 2 Tests of between-subjects effects

Source Dependent variable Type III sum of squares d.f. Mean square F Significance Partial eta squared
a
Corrected model Totpek 3 054.391 1 3 054.391 426.858 0.000 0.281
Totnep 20 464.387b 1 20 464.387 276.229 0.000 0.202
Totgpa 1 928.553c 1 1 928.553 359.388 0.000 0.248
Intercept Totpek 383 224.766 1 383 224.766 53 556.472 0.000 0.980
Totnep 2 842 826.312 1 2 842 826.312 38 372.548 0.000 0.972
Totgpa 152 699.221 1 152 699.221 28 455.638 0.000 0.963
Gender Totpek 3 054.391 1 3 054.391 426.858 0.000 0.281
Totnep 20 464.387 1 20 464.387 276.229 0.000 0.202
Totgpa 1 928.553 1 1 928.553 359.388 0.000 0.248
Error Totpek 7 806.680 1091 7.156
Totnep 80 826.625 1091 74.085
Totgpa 5 854.546 1091 5.366
Total Totpek 391 462.000 1093
Totnep 2 926 057.000 1093
Totgpa 159 044.000 1093
Corrected total Totpek 10 861.070 1092
Totnep 101 291.012 1092
Totgpa 7 783.100 1092
a
R squared = 0.281 (Adjusted R square = 0.281).
b
R squared = 0.202 (Adjusted R square = 0.201).
c
R squared = 0.248 (Adjusted R square = 0.247).
Totpek, total perceived environmental knowledge; Totnep, total environmental concern as measured by new environmental paradigm; Totgpa, total
green purchase attitude.

Table 3 Estimated marginal means

95% Confidence interval

Dependent variable Gender Mean SD Lower bound Upper bound

Totpek Male 20.428 0.118 20.197 20.659


Female 17.080 0.111 16.861 17.298
Totnep Male 55.413 0.379 54.669 56.156
Female 46.754 0.358 46.042 47.448
Totgpa Male 13.169 0.102 12.969 13.369
Female 10.508 0.096 10.319 10.697

Fidell, 2001). Table 2 shows the separate tests results of between- ronmental knowledge in men (20.428) and women (17.080) is
subjects effects. statistically significant, one-way ANOVA was used. The results
An inspection of the estimated marginal means shown in (d.f. 1, 1091 = 426.858, P < 0.001) suggest that there is a signifi-
Table 3 indicated that men reported higher levels of perceived cant difference between men and men in overall perceived envi-
environmental knowledge, concern and attitudes towards green ronmental knowledge. This result implies that men are generally
purchase than women. Bray and Maxwell (1982) suggest that if a more knowledgeable in environmental issues than women. This
significant MANOVA effect is detected, the follow-up test is to result provides strong support for the first hypothesis. The result
perform univariate ANOVAs for each dependent variable. To test also corroborates the results of previous research conducted in the
whether the difference between the mean score of perceived envi- West. For example, Arcury and Johnson (1987) found that the

224 International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
M.M. Mostafa Gender differences in green purchase behaviour

Table 4 Univariate ANOVA tests for dependent variables

Sum of squares d.f. Mean square F Significance

Totpek Between groups 3 054.391 1 3 054.391 426.858 0.000


Within groups 7 806.680 1091 7.156
Total 10 861.070 1092
Totnep Between groups 20 464.387 1 20 464.387 276.229 0.000
Within groups 80 826.625 1091 74.085
Total 101 291.012 1092
Totgpa Between groups 1 928.553 1 1 928.553 359.388 0.000
Within groups 5 854.546 1091 5.366
Total 7 783.100 1092

degree of environmental knowledge was dependent on gender: cultural values (Chan and Lau, 1998). In this research we contrib-
men gave correct answers to the knowledge questions more often uted to the existing body of green consumer behaviour research by
than women. The authors found that gender had an even greater testing the influence of three cognitive and attitudinal factors on
impact on knowledge than the educational establishment. Table 3 gender differences in green purchase behaviour in an Arab non-
shows also that men reported higher levels of environmental con- Western context.
cern. To test whether the difference between the mean score of The high environmental concern among Egyptian consumers
concern in men (55.413) and women (46.745) is statistically found in this study contradicts previous research conducted in the
significant, one-way ANOVA was used. The results (d.f. 1, West. Some authors have argued that environmental concern is a
1091 = 276.229, P < 0.001) suggest that there is a significant dif- post-material value a set of attitudes that develop among wealthy
ference between men and women in overall environmental con- people once the more basic needs of food and safety have been met
cern. This result implies that men are generally more concerned (e.g. Inglehart, 1990; Buttel, 1992). This line of reasoning leads to
about environmental issues than women. This result fails to sup- the prediction that developing nations like Egypt should be less
port the second hypothesis. This result also contradicts previous concerned about environmental issues than the US or European
research conducted in the West (e.g. Davidson and Freudenburg, countries. Our results show that the traditional wisdom that envi-
1996). However, it should be noted that whether women are, in ronmental concern is a luxury afforded by only the wealthy is
reality, more concerned about the environment than men has not unfounded cross-culturally.
been determined conclusively by empirical studies (Momsen,
2000). It appears that more analyses and explanations are needed
Marketing implications
in this area. Finally, Table 3 indicates that men reported more
positive attitudes towards green purchase. To test whether the The knowledge of the green consumer is important for the whole
difference between the mean score of green purchase attitudes in supply chain and especially for the retailers, because the environ-
men (13.169) and women (10.508) is statistically significant, one- mental issues influence the purchase and nutritional decisions of
way ANOVA was used. The results (d.f. 1, 1091 = 359.388, six out of 10 consumers in the US and the UK (Ottman, 1992).
P < 0.001) suggest that there is a significant difference between Knowing the green profile of consumers in Egypt allows market-
men and women in overall green purchase attitudes. This result ers to develop strategies specifically targeted at this segment.
implies that men have generally more positive attitudes towards Building up beliefs about green products could, perhaps, be
green purchase than women. This result fails to support the third accomplished through green promotional materials and green
hypothesis. It is also at odds with previous research conducted in advertisements that appeal to consumer rationality and emotions.
the West (e.g. Tikka et al., 2000). However, in a Canadian study Schuhwerk and Lefkoff-Hagius (1995) have noted that although
Eagles and Muffitt (1990) found no environmental attitude differ- green appeals are not significantly more influential than non-green
ences between the genders. It seems that this area warrants more appeals for environmentally involved consumers, they are much
future research. Table 4 shows ANOVAs results conducted to test more persuasive than non-green appeals for environmentally unin-
separately gender differences in perceived environmental knowl- volved consumers. By attaching themselves to environmental
edge, concern and attitudes towards green purchase. causes, marketers not only educate consumers, but also may
improve their environmental image.
Implications
Policy implications
Research implications
Egypt faces pressing environmental problems such as air and
Lee and Green (1991) claim that while most of the major con- water pollution (Chukwuma, 1995). Those problems cause
sumer behaviour models have been developed and tested in the increasing support for the local environment. Understanding fac-
West, relatively little attention has been devoted to investigating tors affecting environmental attitudes is necessary before the
the validity of these models under other cultural settings. However, county can move towards more effective environmental policies
this cross-cultural testing is essential as most behavioural theories and institutional actions designed to increase environmentally
are rooted in psychology, which, in turn, is heavily bound by friendly behaviour.

International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd 225
Gender differences in green purchase behaviour M.M. Mostafa

Some authors argue that green policies are profitable through Anderson, J. & Gerbing, D. (1988) Structural equation modeling in prac-
cost reduction (Rugman and Verbeke, 2000). Green firms can tice: a review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bul-
shape future regulations and reap first-mover advantage (Porter letin, 103, 411423.
and Van der Linde, 1995). It can be argued, then, that the ideal Arcury, T. (1990) Environmental attitudes and environmental knowledge.
Human Organization, 49, 300304.
solution to environmental problems in Egypt is for companies to
Arcury, T. & Johnson, T. (1987) Public environmental knowledge: a
include green policies in their long-term strategies. Thus, Egyptian statewide survey. Journal of Environmental Education, 18, 3137.
government should expedite green purchase through legislation. Axelrod, L. & Lehman, D. (1993) Responding to environmental concerns:
Given that the development of green marketing in Egypt, as well what factors guide individual action? Journal of Environmental Psy-
as in the rest of the Arab world, is still in its infancy, it is suggested chology, 13, 149159.
that the Egyptian government should work closely with the busi- Baldassare, M. & Katz, C. (1992) The personal threat of environmental
ness community and form strategic alliances with environmental problems as predictor of environmental practices. Environment and
groups to come up with a comprehensive eco-certification scheme Behavior, 24, 602616.
and a set of guidelines that regulate the practices of green market- Balderjahn, I. (1988) Personality variables and environmental attitudes as
ing. For example, in the United Arab Emirates, the Federal Envi- predictors of ecologically responsible consumption patterns. Journal of
Business Research, 17, 5156.
ronmental Agency and the Emirates Environmental Group are
Bamberg, S. (2003) How does environmental concern influence specific
doing admirable work in spreading awareness about environmen- environmentally related behaviors? A new answer to an old question.
tal issues, including green purchasing, in the country. By working Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 2132.
closely with the public and private sectors, these entities have Banerjee, S., Gulas, C. & Iyer, E. (1995) Shades of green: a multidimen-
succeeded in large measure in preserving the environment (Gulf sional analysis of environmental advertising. Journal of Advertising, 24,
News, 2004). 2131.
Barr, S., Ford, N. & Gilg, A. (2003) Attitudes towards recycling household
waste in Exeter, Devon: quantitative and qualitative approaches. Local
Limitations and future research Environment, 8, 407421.
Bechtel, R., Corral-Verdugo, V. & Pinhiero, J. (1999) Environmental
First, the measures used in the study are based on self-reports of
belief system: United States, Brazil, and Mexico. Journal of Cross-
past behaviours or predictions about future actions. Although such Cultural Psychology, 30, 122128.
self-reports often represents fairly good approximations of actual Benton, R. (1994) Environmental knowledge and attitudes of undergradu-
behaviours (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980), they clearly have limita- ate business students compared to non-business students. Business and
tions. In particular, socially desirable past behaviours are usually Society, 33, 191205.
over-reported and less desirable past behaviours are under- Berger, I. & Corbin, R. (1992) Perceived consumer effectiveness and faith
reported. Future research should examine the potential impact of in others as moderators of environmentally responsible behaviors. Jour-
social desirability bias on consumers responses to questions about nal of Public Policy and Marketing, 11, 7989.
green purchase behaviour. Second, this study has used a cross- Bhat, V. (1993) Green marketing begins with green design. Journal of
Business and Industrial Marketing, 8, 2631.
sectional rather than a longitudinal approach. This implies that
Blocker, T. & Eckberg, D. (1997) Gender and environmentalism: results
much more emphasis has been placed on observing consumers
from the 1993 general social survey. Social Science Quarterly, 78,
behaviours than on observing changes in behaviour. There would 841858.
seem to be hence a need for much more longitudinal research to Bray, J. & Maxwell, S. (1982) Analyzing and interpreting significant
focus on observing changes in consumers behaviour over time. MANOVAs. Review of Educational Research, 52, 340367.
Third, for the purpose of this research, a MANOVA approach was Briggs, J., Sharp, J., Hamed, N. & Yacoub, H. (2003) Changing womens
adopted to identify gender differences in behavioural intention roles, changing environmental knowledge: evidence from Upper Egypt.
regarding green products. The use of quantitative methods alone is The Geographical Journal, 169, 313325.
valuable in establishing relationships between variables, but is Brown, G. & Harris, C. (1992) The US forest service: toward the new
considered weak when attempting to identify the reasons for those resource management paradigm? Society and Natural Resources, 5,
231245.
relationships (Chisnall, 1997). Using qualitative research along
Buttel, F. (1992) Environmentalization: origins, processes, and implica-
quantitative methods in future studies may enable us to further
tions for rural social change. Rural Sociology, 57, 127.
explore relationships among variables. Finally, This study was Calder, B., Phillips, L. & Tybout, A. (1981) Designing research for appli-
conducted in Egypt using college students as subjects so some cations. Journal of Consumer Research, 8, 197207.
caution should be observed in generalizing its results to other Arab Caron, J. (1989) Environmental perspectives of blacks: acceptance of the
countries. However, Souiden (2002) suggests that there is a num- new environmental paradigm. Journal of Environmental Education, 20,
ber of market communalities among Arab countries in terms of 2126.
consumers response to certain marketing stimuli. Although Egypt Chan, R. (2001) Determinants of Chinese consumers green purchasing
may reflect market and consumer attitudes in other Arab countries, behavior. Psychology and Marketing, 18, 389413.
future research might extend the list of countries and use subjects Chan, R. & Lau, L. (1998) A test of the Fishbein-Ajzen behavioral inten-
tion model under Chinese cultural settings: Are there any differences
from other populations to assess more significantly pan-Arab con-
between PRC and Hong Kong consumers? Journal of Marketing Prac-
sumers behaviours regarding green products.
tice: Applied Marketing Science, 3, 85101.
Chisnall, P. (1997) Marketing Research, 5th edn. McGraw-Hill, Berkshire,
UK.
References
Chukwuma, C. (1995) Environmental, developmental and health
Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (1980) Understanding Attitudes and Predicting perspectives in Egypt. Environmental Management and Health, 6,
Behavior. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. 2937.

226 International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
M.M. Mostafa Gender differences in green purchase behaviour

Curlo, E. (1999) Marketing strategy, product safety, and ethical factors in research studies. Proceedings of the 1994 Marketing and Public Policy
consumer choice. Journal of Business Ethics, 21, 3748. Conference. pp. 94108. Arlington, VA.
DSouza, C. (2004) Eco-label programmes: a stakeholder (consumer) per- Hines, J., Hungerford, H. & Tomera, A. (1987) Analysis and synthesis of
spective. Corporate Communications, 9, 179188. research on environmental behavior: a meta-analysis. Journal of Envi-
Davidson, D. & Freudenberg, W. (1996) Gender and environmental risk ronmental Education, 18, 18.
concerns: a review and analysis of available research. Environment and Inglehart, R. (1990) Culture Shift in Advanced Industrial Society. Prince-
Behavior, 28, 302339. ton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Dixon, J. (1993) Grouping techniques. In Statistical Methods for Health- Kapelianis, D. & Strachan, S. (1996) The price premium of an environ-
care Research (ed. by B. Munro & E. Page), 2nd edn, pp. 199217. J. B. mentally friendly product. South African Journal of Business Manage-
Lippincott Co, Philadelphia, PA. ment, 27, 8995.
Dunlap, R. & Van Liere, K. (1978) The new environmental paradigm: a Kassarjian, H. (1971) Incorporating ecology into marketing strategy: the
proposed measuring instrument and preliminary results. Journal of case of air pollution. Journal of Marketing, 35, 6165.
Environmental Education, 9, 1019. Kinnear, T. & Taylor, J. (1973) The effect of ecological concern on brand
Dunlap, R., Van Liere, K., Mertig, A. & Jones, R. (2000) Measuring perception. Journal of Marketing Research, 10, 191197.
endorsement of the new ecological paradigm: a revised NEP scale. Klineberg, S. (1998) Environmental attitudes among Anglos, Blacks and
Journal of Social Issues, 56, 425444. Hispanics in Texas: has the concern gap disappeared? Race, Gender, and
Eagles, P. & Muffitt, S. (1990) An analysis of childrens attitudes toward Class, 6, 7082.
animals. Journal of Environmental Education, 21, 4144. Kotchen, M. & Reiling, S. (2000) Environmental attitudes,
Edgell, M. & Nowell, D. (1989) The new environmental paradigm scale: motivations, and contingent valuation of nonuse values: a case
wildlife and environmental beliefs in British Columbia. Society and study involving endangered species. Ecological Economics, 32,
Natural Resources, 2, 285296. 93107.
Ellen, S. (1994) Do we know what we need to know? Objective and Kristensen, K. & Grunert, S. (1991) The effect of ecological consciousness
subjective knowledge effects on pro-ecological behaviors. Journal of on the demand for organic foods. In Marketing for the New Europe:
Business Research, 30, 4352. Dealing with Complexity (ed. by J. Chias & A. Sureda), pp. 299318.
Ellen, P., Eroglu, D. & Webb, D. (1997) Consumer judgments in a chang- Proceedings of the 1991 Annual Conference of the European Marketing
ing information environment: how consumers respond to green market- Academy, Vol. 2. Barcelona.
ing claims. Working paper, Georgia State University. Lee, C. & Green, R. (1991) Cross-cultural examination of the Fishbein
Erickson, A. & Kramer-Leblanc, C. (1991) Eco-labels: the link between behavioral intentions models. Journal of International Business Studies,
environmental preferences and green practices? In Strategy and Policy 22, 289305.
in the Food System, NE 65 Conference Proceedings (ed. by J. Caswell & Luzar, E., Diagne, A., Gan, C. & Henning, B. (1995) Evaluating nature
R. Cotterill), pp. 173177. Food Marketing Policy Center, Storrs, CT. based tourism using the new environmental paradigm. Journal of Agri-
Follows, S. & Jobber, D. (1999) Environmentally responsible purchase culture and Applied Economics, 2, 544555.
behavior: a test of a consumer model. European Journal of Marketing, Lynne, G. & Rola, L. (1988) Improving attitude-behavior prediction mod-
34, 723746. els with economic variables: farmer actions towards soil conservation.
Fritzsche, D. & Dueher, R. (1982) The effects of ecological concern on Journal of Social Psychology, 128, 1928.
product attribute utility. American Marketing Association Proceedings, McCloskey, J. & Maddock, S. (1994) Environmental management: its role
pp. 364369. Chicago, IL. in corporate strategy. Management Decision, 32, 2732.
Fryxell, G. & Lo, C. (2003) The influence of environmental knowledge McDaniel, S. & Rylander, D. (1993) Strategic green marketing. Journal of
and values on managerial behaviors on behalf of the environment: an Consumer Marketing, 10, 410.
empirical examination of managers in China. Journal of Business Eth- McEachern, M. & McClean, P. (2002) Organic purchasing motivations
ics, 46, 4559. and attitudes: are they ethical? International Journal of Consumer Stud-
Furman, A. (1998) A note on environmental concern in a developing ies, 26, 8592.
country: results from an Istanbul survey. Environment and Behavior, 30, McGorr, S. (2000) Measurement in a cross-cultural environment: survey
520534. translation issues. Qualitative Market Research, 3, 7481.
Gendall, P. & Smith, T. (1995) Knowledge of scientific and environmental Maloney, M., Ward, M. & Braucht, G. (1975) Psychology in action: a
facts: a comparison of six countries. Marketing Bulletin, 6, 6573. revised scale for the measurement of ecological attitudes and knowl-
Gill, J., Lawrence, A. & Taylor, J. (1986) Ecological concern, attitude, and edge. American Psychologist, 30, 787790.
social norms in voting behavior. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50, 537 Menon, A. & Menon, A. (1997) Enviropreneurial marketing strategy: the
554. emergence of corporate environmentalism as market strategy. Journal of
Gooch, G. (1995) Environmental beliefs and attitudes in Sweden and the Marketing, 61, 5167.
Baltic states. Environment and Behavior, 27, 513539. Merchant, C. (1979) The Death of Nature: Women, Ecology, and the
Gordon, M., Stade, L. & Schmitt, N. (1987) Student guinea-pigs: porcine Scientific Revolution. Harper & Row, New York.
predictors and particularistic phenomena. Academy of Management Milfont, L. & Duckitt, J. (2004) The structure of environmental attitudes:
Review, 12, 160163. a first-and second-order confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Envi-
Greenberg, J. (1987) The college sophomore as guinea pig: setting the ronmental Psychology, 24, 289303.
score straight. Academy of Management Review, 12, 157159. Mohai, P. (1990) Black environmentalism. Social Science Quarterly, 7,
Grunert, S. (1993) Everybody seems concerned about the environment but 744765.
is this concern reflected in (Danish) consumers food choice? European Mohai, P. (1992) Men, women, and the environment: an examination of
Advances in Consumer Research, 1, 428433. the gender gap in environmental concern and activism. Society and
Gulf News (2004) In harmony with the environment. Gulf News (October Natural Resources, 5, 119.
20), 8. Mohr, L., Eroglu, D. & Ellen, P. (1998) The development and testing of a
Harzing, A. (1997) Response rates in international mail surveys: results of measure of skepticism toward environmental claims in marketers com-
a 22-country study. International Business Review, 6, 641665. munication. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 32, 3055.
Hastak, M., Horst, R. & Mazis, M. (1994) Consumer perceptions about Momsen, J. (2000) Gender differences in environmental concern and per-
and comprehension of environmental terms: evidence from survey ception. Journal of Geography, 99, 4756.

International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd 227
Gender differences in green purchase behaviour M.M. Mostafa

Napoli, J. (1994) Cairo communiqu: peoples assembly passes landmark Starch, R. (1996) Green gauge survey. In In Earths Company: Business
environmental law. Washington Report on Middle East. XIII, 5253. Environment and the Challenge of Sustainability (ed. by C. Frankel).
Nasif, E., Al-Daeaj, H., Ebrahimi, B. & Thibodeau, M. (1991) Method- New Society Publishers, New York.
ological problems in cross-cultural research: an updated review. Man- Stern, P. (1992) What psychology knows about energy conservation.
agement International Review, 31, 1332. American Psychologist, 47, 12241232.
Nooney, J., Woodrum, E., Hoban, T. & Clifford, W. (2003) Environmental Stone, G., Barnes, J. & Montgomery, C. (1995) Ecoscale: a scale for the
worldview and behavior: consequences of dimensionality in a survey of measurement of environmentally responsible consumers. Psychology
North Carolinians. Environment and Behavior, 35, 763783. and Marketing, 12, 595612.
Nunnally, J. (1978) Psychometric Theory. McGraw-Hill Book Co., New Suchard, H. & Polonsky, M. (1991) A theory of environmental buyer
York, NY. behavior and its validity: the environmental action-behavior model. In
Ottman, J. (1992) Sometimes consumers will pay more to go green. AMA Summer Educators Conference Proceedings (ed. by M. Gilly, H.
Marketing News (July 6), 16. Suchard & C. Montgomery), pp. 187201. AMA, Chicago, IL.
Phillips, L. (1999) Green attitudes. American Demographics, 21, Synodinos, N. (1991) Environmental attitudes and knowledge: a compari-
4647. son of marketing and business students with other groups. Journal of
Pierce, J., Dalton, R. & Zaitsev, A. (1999) Public perceptions of environ- Business Research, 23, 161 170.
mental conditions. In Critical Masses, Citizens, Nuclear Weapons Pro- Tabachnick, B. & Fidell, L. (2001) Using Multivariate Statistics, 4th edn.
duction, and Environmental Destruction in the United States and Russia Allyn and Bacon, Boston, MA.
(ed. by R.J. Dalton, J. Pierce & A. Zaitsev), pp. 97129. MIT Press, Taylor, S. & Todd, P. (1995) Decomposition and crossover effects in the
Cambridge. theory of planned behavior: a study of consumer adoption intentions.
Polonsky, M., Carlson, L., Grove, S. & Kangun, N. (1997) International International Journal of Research in Marketing, 12, 137155.
environmental marketing claims real changes or simple posturing? Tikka, P., Kuitunen, M. & Tynys, S. (2000) Effects of educational back-
International Marketing Review, 14, 218232. ground on students attitudes, activity levels, and knowledge concerning
Poortinga, W., Steg, L. & Vlek, C. (2004) Values, environmental concerns, the environment. Journal of Environmental Education, 31, 1219.
and environmental behavior: a study into household energy use. Envi- Walsh, J. & McGuire, J. (1992) An examination of environmental attitudes
ronment and Behavior, 36, 7093. among college students. Procedings of the 1992 Northeastern Recre-
Porter, M. & Van der Linde, L. (1995) Toward a new conception of the ational Research Symposium, General Technical Report, NE-176.
environment-competitiveness relationship. Journal of Economic Per- Weaver, A. (2002) Determinants of environmental attitudes: a five-country
spectives, 4, 97118. comparison. International Journal of Sociology, 32, 77108.
Prothero, A. & McDonald, P. (1992) Producing environmentally accept- Wehrmeyer, W. & McNeil, M. (2000) Activists, pragmatists, technophiles
able cosmetics the impact of environmentalism on the United King- and tree-huggers? Gender differences in employees environmental atti-
dom cosmetics and toiletries industry. Journal of Marketing tudes. Journal of Business Ethics, 28, 211222.
Management, 8, 147166. Widegren, O. (1998) The new environmental paradigm and personal
Riechard, D. & Peterson, S. (1998) Perception of environmental risk norms. Environment and Behavior, 30, 75100.
related to gender, community socioeconomic setting, age, and locus of
control. Journal of Environmental Education, 30, 1119.
Rolston, C. & Di Benedetto, C. (1994) Developing a greenness scale: an Appendix
exploration of behavior versus attitude. In AMA Winter Educators
Conference Proceedings (ed. by C. Park, C. Rolston & C. Di Bene- Perceived environmental knowledge scale
detto), pp. 335340. AMA, Chicago, IL.
Rugman, A. & Verbeke, A. (2000) Environmental regulations and the I know that I buy products and packages that are environmentally
global strategies of multinational enterprises. In Coping with safe.
Globalization (ed. by A. Prakash & J. Hart), pp. 7793. Routledge, I know more about recycling than the average person.
London. I know how to select products and packages that reduce the
Schahn, J. & Holzer, E. (1990) Studies of individual environmental con- amount of waste ending up in landfills.
cern: the role of knowledge, gender, and background variables. Environ- I understand the environmental phrases and symbols on product
ment and Behavior, 22, 767786. package.
Schuhwerk, M. & Lefkoff-Hagius, R. (1995) Green or non-green? Does I am very knowledgeable about environmental issues.
type of appeal matter when advertising a green product? Journal of
Advertising, 24, 4554.
Schultz, P. (2000) Empathizing with nature: the effects of perspective Environmental concern scale
taking on concern for environmental issues. Journal of Social Issues, 56,
391406. We are approaching the limit of the number of people the earth can
Schultz, P. & Zelezny, L. (1999) Values as predictors of environmental support.
attitudes: evidence for consistency across 14 countries. Journal of Envi- The earth has plenty of natural resources if we just learn to develop
ronmental Psychology, 19, 255265. them. (R)
Seguin, C., Pelletier, L. & Hunsley, J. (1998) Toward a model of environ- The earth is like a spaceship with only limited room and resources.
mental activism. Environment and Behavior, 30, 628652. Humans have the right to modify the natural environment to suit
Shamdasani, P., Chon-Lin, G. & Richmond, D. (1993) Exploring green their needs. (R)
consumers in an oriental culture: role of personal and marketing mix.
Plants and animals have as much right as humans to exit.
Advances in Consumer Research, 20, 488493.
Smith, S., Haugtvedt, C. & Petty, R. (1994) Attitudes and recycling: does
Humans were meant to rule over the rest of the nature. (R)
the measurement of affect enhance behavioral prediction? Psychology When humans interfere with nature it often produces disastrous
and Marketing, 11, 359374. consequences.
Souiden, N. (2002) Segmenting the Arab markets based on the marketing The balance of nature is strong enough to cope with the impacts of
stimuli. International Marketing Review, 19, 611636. modern industrial nations. (R)(a)

228 International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
M.M. Mostafa Gender differences in green purchase behaviour

The balance of nature is very delicate and easily upset. If things continue on their present course, we will soon experience
Human ingenuity will ensure that we do not make the earth a major ecological catastrophe.
unlivable. (R)
Despite our special abilities, humans are still subject to the laws of
Green purchase attitudes scale
nature.(a)
Humans will eventually learn enough about how nature works to I (1 = dislike; 5 = like) the idea of purchasing green products.
be able to control it. (R) Purchasing green products is a (1 = bad; 5 = good) idea.
Humans are severely abusing the environment. I have a/an (1 = unfavourable; 5 = favourable) attitude towards
The so-called ecological crisis facing humankind has been greatly purchasing a green version of a product.
exaggerated. (R)(a) *(R) = reverse-coded scores; (a) = items excluded from analysis.

International Journal of Consumer Studies 31 (2007) 220229 The Author. Journal compilation 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd 229

Potrebbero piacerti anche