Sei sulla pagina 1di 7

Vol.

9 2005 Marzluff and Angell / Cultural Coevolution 69

Crib Notes

Cultural Coevolution: How the Human Bond with Crows and


Ravens Extends Theory and Raises New Questions
John M. Marzluff
Tony Angell

Introduction We illustrate our theory by detailing cultural


Human culture reflects our natural world. Caves coevolution between humans and birds of the genus
in modern day France are richly decorated with Corvus (corvids; crows and ravens). The influence of
30,000-year-old images of animals and their connec- corvids on human culture is extensive and long-last-
tion to the artists beliefs. For 10,000 years, Native ing (Armstrong 1958; Feher-Elston 1991; Marzluff
Americans organized their societies around natural and Angell 2005; Nelson 1983; Sax 2003). The po-
totems and wove the actions of animals deeply into tential for humans to change corvid culture is equally
their religion and art. Today, our art, literature, film, impressive (Marzluff and Angell 2005).
and popular culture are influenced by natures beauty,
power, and wonder. Antecedents and a Preliminary Framework
Cultural artifacts suggest ancient and intimate Humans and many social animals have a dual
coevolutionary relationships. Human genes and inheritance system whereby gene frequencies change
culture coevolve in response to natures challenges. through time in response to mutation, drift, and natu-
Living in northern environments where limited ral and cultural selection (genetic inheritance), while
sunlight reduced human ability to produce Vitamin meme frequencies change through time in response
D, for example, may have selected for a culture of to innovation, natural and cultural selection, learning,
dairying that, in turn, selected for genetic evolution and drift (cultural inheritance)(Figure 1). Culture is
of lactose absorption (Durham 1991). Plus, human composed of memes that reflect genetic, individu-
modification of nature often provides a strong impe- ally learned, and socially transmitted information
tus for genetic and cultural evolution (Odling-Smee (boxes in Figure 1)(Durham 1991; Laland et al. 2000).
et al. 2003). We follow the more mechanistic definition of cul-
We propose an additional synergy between turevariation acquired and maintained by indirect
human culture and the environmenta coevolution and direct social learning (Boyd and Richerson 1985;
of human and other species cultures. We suggest Castro and Toro 2004; Rendell and Whitehead 2001).
that when humans interact with other social species, Memes are units of information transferred among
who themselves have the ability to evolve culture, individuals by social learning (e.g., rules, songs, reli-
then simple feedbacks from a culturally evolving gions, or even specific behaviors like handshakes or
environment can stimulate rapid cultural evolution dietary choices)(Blackmore 1999).
in humans. We term the reciprocal adjustments in Humans are potent agents of natural selec-
two or more species cultures cultural coevolution tion affecting the genetic fitness of organisms and
(Marzluff and Angell 2005). Cultural coevolution modifying the configuration and functioning of the
may involve genetic fitness benefits or may depend physical environment. Environmental responses to
on migration and the diffusion of ideas, cultural drift, these effects can force cultural and natural selection on
differential modeling and role selection, or societal humans, affecting an individuals genetic fitness and the
choice and imposition, all of which are important cultural fitness of their memes, as originally postulated
to cultural evolution (Durham 1991). by Durham (1991) and Odling-Smee et al. (2003).
70 Journal of Ecological Anthropology Vol. 9 2005

Figure 1. Integration and expansion of gene-culture coevolution and niche construction models
to illustrate genetic and cultural coevolution between two interacting species (here humans and
another social animal in their environment, after Laland et al. 2000). The collection of pheno-
types within a population ( ) emerge as a joint product of genetic, individually-aquired, and
socially-learned (culture) information. Populations evolve as genetic and learned information is
transferred through time by genetic and cultural inheritance. As people interact with their environ-
ment they cause changes in other organisms inclusive fitness (natural selection) or the cultural
fitness of their memes (cultural selection). Reciprocal changes in humans caused by organisms
in their environment can lead to coevolved genes ( ) or cultures ( ).
Vol. 9 2005 Marzluff and Angell / Cultural Coevolution 71
Niche construction theory represents the environ- We expect cultural coevolution where humans
ment as an open box (Odling-Smee et al. 2003: Fig. interact closely with animals able to learn socially
6.3), but recognizes that the environment changes in (e.g., non-human primates, cetaceans, social carni-
response to human natural and cultural selection so vores, rats, parrots, and corvids) (Avital and Jablonka
that humans inherit a change in ecology as well as 2000; Emery and Clayton 2004; Rendell and White-
a change in gene and meme frequency (Laland et al. head 2001). Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)
2000). We suggest that this ecological inheritance is have fished with people at Laguna, off the coast of
not only the physical and ecological change wrought Brazil, since 1847 (Pryor et al. 1990). During this
by people, but also the cultural change in response to time dolphins developed a culture of driving fish
human activity by animals capable of social learning. into fishers nets, signaling to fishers, and feeding
Many animals acquire information through social on stunned fish. This behavior is cultural because
learning and develop traditions that meet our defini- it is learned socially (young imitate or are taught by
tion of culture (Avital and Jablonka 2000; Danchin their mothers), persistent (at Laguna for at least three
et al. 2004; Rendell and Whitehead 2001). Where hu- dolphin generations), and not found in all dolphin
man activity results in differential cultural fitness of populations. The culture of fishers includes an ability
another animals memes (Figure 1; cultural selection to interpret distinctive rolls performed by dolphins,
from humans to the environment), and the resulting which tell the fishers how many fish the dolphins are
cultural evolution in the animal affects the cultural herding and where to cast their nets.
fitness of human memes (cultural selection from the Cultural coevolution between fishers and dol-
environment to humans), human and animal memes phins is geographically restricted. In contrast, cultural
may become coevolved. This cultural coevolution is coevolution between people and corvids may be wide-
analogous to traditional genetic coevolution where spread. Most humans live in the company of at least
reciprocal natural selection between organisms drives one species of crow or raven (Marzluff and Angell
mutual change in genes (e.g., crossbill bill depth and 2005). Corvids innovate (Heinrich 1999), and learn
conifer cone structure) (Benkman 2003). from observing their parents and peers (Bugnyar and
Recent theoretical insights suggest that cultural Kotrschal 2002; Emery and Clayton 2004; Paz-y-Mio
evolution should frequently occur where long-lived, et al. 2004). Social learning and cultural inheritance
intelligent, and social animals live with people. First, may explain the differentiation of American crow
Danchin et al. (2004) recognized that observation (Corvus brachyrhynchos) and common raven (Corvus co-
of public information is adequate to allow social rax) vocal dialects (Brown 1985; Enggist-Dueblin and
learning and cultural evolution. Second, observation Pfister 2002), the geographical differences in tool use
and imitation of others is a simple form of learn- by New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) (Hunt
ing that allows culture to develop, even if imitation 2000), and the locale-specific use of automobile traffic
is not perfect. Rudimentary culture may be refined to open nuts by carrion crows (Corvus corone) (Nihei
substantially simply by parents guiding the clumsy or and Higuchi 2001).
inaccurate imitations of their offspring (Castro and
Toro 2004). Third, social learning has been shown Using the Theory
to be advantageous where the costs of individual Cultural coevolutionary theory expands our abil-
learning are high (Boyd and Richerson 1995; Rogers ity to understand human-animal interactions. Consider
1988). Fourth, models of cultural inheritance suggest maize farming and American crow exploitation of
it is advantageous when environmental change is too crops. Conventional coevolutionary theory is not ap-
fast for genetic change to track but not so fast as plicablethe selective force of corn stealing by crows
to require individual learning (Feldman et al. 1996). on people is insufficient to mold our morphology or
Human cultural evolution poses environmental chal- physiology. We are not stronger or faster because of
lenges at a pace rapid enough to favor both individual maize-stealing crows. Niche construction theory is
innovations and cultural inheritance. inadequate. It predicts that the maize environment will
72 Journal of Ecological Anthropology Vol. 9 2005
favor genetic and cultural responses by humans, but it to crows with static scarecrows. A Makah fish-drying
does not specify any details of the feedback. A theory rack, photographed 100 years ago, has a scarecrow
of cultural coevolution predicts that the culture of built into one of its supports (Stein 2000). This human
maize-stealing crows evolves in response to the culture meme would select for habituation and attentiveness
of maize-defending farmers, and vice versa. by crows and, accordingly, flexibility and innovation
Defense of maize by people takes the form of by farmers. Learning about changing form, placement,
many memes that have evolved through time: 1) no and movement of scarecrows would be facilitated by
defense; 2) defense by children; 3) defense by hanging exploitation of public information (simply flocking to
dead crows on garden fences; 4) defense by shoot- fields where other crows are safely eating) and would
ing; 5) defense by straw-filled scarecrows on sticks; drive increasingly complex scarecrow development
6) defense by automated, concussive cannons or by people (Shirota 1989). Clearly, the rapid pace of
wild-eyed kites. Curious, exploratory American crows cultural change by people is best matched by reciprocal
have likely raided American cornfields successfully cultural change by crows, and vice versa.
for a thousand years in part because of their ability In more recent times, much of the human cul-
to culturally adjust to human defense. Initial chasing tural response to crows has moved beyond the adap-
of crows by children would have favored increased tive responses of farmers trying to protect their crop.
crow flock size, persistence, and patience. Persistence Increasingly, fitness-neutral memes characterize human
and patience could represent evolving crow culture as responses to the corn-stealing abilities of crows. Few
behavioral characteristics initially learned by watching people are involved in actual crop guarding, but crows
peers or parents foil human children spread through grace logos, stimulate hunters, poets, and artists, enrich
crow populations living in close contact with people. our language and place names, and take lead roles in
Such fundamental behavioral attributes, arising from print and film. Scarecrows are now used as harvest cel-
simple mimicry (and possible shaping) of public ebration decorations, characters in movies, and inspira-
information might later become tightly restricted by tion for art and song. It appears that our perceptions of
genetic biases as consistent responses were favored crows affect culture today, just as our experience with
by life with humans (Lachlan and Feldman 2003). crows affected culture in the past.
Human cultural innovations would continue to
favor crow cultural evolution. If people responded Crows in the City
by shooting or snaring crows, the costs of individual Testing our ideas about cultural coevolution
learning and the benefits of imitating public informa- in the past requires deduction and induction, but
tion and peer or parental shaping of poor imitation our basic tenets can also be investigated in modern
would be greatly increased. In response, crow memes cities, often with a hypothetico-deductive approach.
characterized by selective avoidance and keen recog- Human settlements bring corvids and humans to-
nition would have been favored. A Mimbres bowl gether. carrion crow, common raven, and human re-
from the 11th century suggests selective, but deadly mains are commingled in ancient (4,00010,500 years
force was used to deter crows. The bowl (itself a ago) settlements of Syria, Poland, Troy, Mesopota-
cultural response to crows) details how crows were mia, and western Canada (Cavallo 2000; Dobney et al.
hung from nooses on a fence around a garden to 1999; Driver 1999; Krnneck 1995; Wyrost 1993). In
teach other crows to stay away (Brody 1977). the modern city, cultural coevolution of demeanor,
An ability to learn about dangerous situations diet, and food acquisition could be studied to better
may have a strong genetic component, but learning understand fundamental processes including: 1) the
to avoid specific places, individual people, or certain relative roles of genetic, individually-acquired, and
circumstances (e.g., people with guns) requires indi- socially-learned information to human and crow be-
vidual experience, and because of the inherent costs havior; 2) the influence of memes on genetic fitness;
of behaving incorrectly, would favor social learning 3) modes (horizontal, vertical, oblique) of acquiring
and cultural inheritance. Other humans responded social knowledge; 4) individual innovation; and 5)
Vol. 9 2005 Marzluff and Angell / Cultural Coevolution 73
occurrence of parental shaping of social learning. to as culturein humans and crows. The diversity,
We know, for example, that city crows and ravens longevity, and pervasiveness of such interactions
are more aggressive around their nests than country show us that the behavior of animals, even those
birds, in part because hazing and shooting birds is as common as crows, may profoundly stimulate hu-
rare in the city relative to the country (Knight 1984; man culture. This leads us to suggest that cultural
Knight et al. 1987). The cultural basis of persecution coevolution is an ethological service that nature
could be studied in humans and related to corvid de- provides people. But as we become an increasingly
meanor by documenting the acquisition, geographi- urban and technological species, how can we sustain
cal distribution, and covariation of human attitudes the services animal behavior provides?
and corvid nest defense behavior. An ecologist might suggest building natural fea-
Urban subsidies also offer experimental op- tures into cities and encouraging humans and nature
portunities to test cultural coevolutionary theory. to intermingle. This is certainly part of the answer,
Innovative adoption and subsequent spread of unique but we suggest a more thorough understanding of
nest materials (e.g., clothes hangers) (Kubota 2004) how human culture is stimulated by the sight, sound,
could be related to human behavior. The nature of and even culture of nature is needed. Studying the
apparent positive feedbacks between some people processes of cultural coevolution can improve this
and the crows they actively feed could be quantified, understanding. We should determine if all interac-
and the degree to which feeding and foraging are so- tions with nature are equally stimulating to human
cially-transmitted could be investigated. Exploitation culture. In our investigation, competitive interactions
of new foods can be linked to human culture. For often characterize human-corvid coevolution. Do
example, garbage use by corvids affects their diet and mutually beneficial interactions spawn more or dif-
scrounging behavior, and also causes rapid evolution ferent culture than competitive interactions? How
of human waste disposal culture (Connell 2003; Shida does human culture respond to direct versus indirect
2001), may reinforce language (to eat crow), and can natural stimulation? As we have pointed out, and as
affect culinary practices (few modern societies actu- Durham (1991) anticipated, much of the current cul-
ally eat crow). Our propensity to import fruits, pave tural stimulation provided by crows is accomplished
ground, and drive cars may be spawning unique corvid indirectly as myths, legends, and others experiences
foraging, and human driving, cultures. Carrion crows stimulate popular culture. Certainly the range of in-
in Sendai, Japan, harvest English walnuts each autumn fluence expands with indirect stimulation, but is the
and carefully place them in front of cars stopped process fundamentally different as well? Addressing
at traffic signals. When the cars move, the nuts are these questions may be a productive arena for an-
crushed, and the birds fly down to eat the nutritious thropologists, ecologists, urban planners, managers,
nutmeat (Nihei and Higuchi 2001). This behavior is policy makers, and the urban public to collaborate for
spreading slowly from the place it was first observed scientific advancement and societal benefit. Shared
20 years ago, which is consistent with social learning. knowledge could inform the design of urban natural
In accordance with cultural evolution, other popula- areas to facilitate interactions between people and
tions of carrion crows do not use cars to crack nuts, social animals for mutual cultural stimulation. Inter-
but they do drop nuts to crack them. Further investiga- pretation could educate city dwellers about the free
tion of nutcracking and driving memes (that include cultural services nature provides. Restoration might
purposefully running over nuts) (Higuchi, personal focus on cultural, in addition to ecological, function.
communication) is warranted. At the very least, it will be culturally stimulating.
Taken as a whole, the human cultures of
scavenging, hunting, farming, and urbanization John M. Marzluff, College of Forest Resources,
may have produced a complex set of closely linked University of Washington
human and corvid memes that begin to sum up to Tony Angell, 18237 40th Avenue NE, Seattle, WA
what early anthropologists like Tylor (1871) referred 98155
74 Journal of Ecological Anthropology Vol. 9 2005
References Cited Danchin, ., L.A. Giraldeau, T.J. Valone, and
Armstrong, E.A. R.H. Wagner.
1958 The folklore of birds. London: Collins. 2004 Public information: From nosy neigh-
Avital, E., and E. Jablonka. bors to cultural evolution. Science
2000 Animal traditions. Cambridge: Cambridge 305:487-491.
University Press. Dobney, K., M. Beech, and D. Jaques.
Benkman, C.W. 1999 Hunting the broad spectrum revolu-
2003 Divergent selection drives the adap- tion: The characterization of early
tive radiation of crossbills. Evolution Neolithic animal exploitation at Qer-
57:1176-1181. mez Dere, northern Mesopotamia,
Blackmore, S. in Zooarchaeology of the Pleistocene/Ho-
1999 The meme machine. Oxford: Oxford Uni- locene boundary, BAR international series
versity Press. 800. Edited by J.C. Driver, pp. 47-57.
Boyd, R., and P.J. Richerson. Oxford: John and Erica Hedges LTD.
1985 Culture and the evolutionary process. Chi- Driver, J.C.
cago: University of Chicago Press. 1999 Stratified faunas from Charlie Lake
Boyd, R., and P.J. Richerson. cave and the peopling of the western
1995 Why does culture increase human interior of Canada, in Zooarchaeology
adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology of the Pleistocene/Holocene boundary, BAR
15:125-143. international series 800. Edited by J.C.
Brody, J.J. Driver, pp. 77-83. Oxford: John and
1977 Mimbres painted pottery. Santa Fe: School Erica Hedges LTD.
of American Research. Durham, W.H.
Brown, E.D. 1991 Coevolution: Genes, culture, and human diver-
1985 The role of song and vocal imita- sity. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
tion among Common Crows (Corvus Emery, N.J., and N.S. Clayton.
brachyrhynchos). Zeitshrift fr Tierpsychologie 2004 The mentality of crows: Convergent
68:115136. evolution of intelligence in corvids and
Bugnyar, T., and K. Kotrschal. apes. Science 306:1903-1907.
2002 Observational learning and raiding of Enggist-Dueblin, P., and U. Pfister.
food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: Is 2002 Cultural transmission of vocalizations
it tactical deception? Animal Behaviour in ravens, Corvus corax. Animal Behaviour
64:185-195. 64:831841.
Castro, L., and M.A. Toro. Feher-Elston, C.
2004 The evolution of culture: From primate 1991 Ravensong: A natural and fabulous history of
social learning to human culture. Pro- ravens and crows. Flagstaff, AZ: North-
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences land.
101:10235-10240. Feldman, M.W., K. Aoki, and J. Kumm.
Cavallo, C. 1996 Individual versus social learning: Evolu-
2000 Animals in the steppe, BAR international tionary analysis in a fluctuating envi-
series 891. Oxford: John and Erica ronment. Anthropological Science 104:209-
Hedges LTD. 231.
Connell, R. Heinrich, B.
2003 City eats crow as professor caws over his 1999 Mind of the raven: Investigations and ad-
spicy sack. Mainichi Daily News, July 7. ventures with wolf-birds. New York: Cliff
Street Books.
Vol. 9 2005 Marzluff and Angell / Cultural Coevolution 75
Hunt, G.R. Odling-Smee, F.J., K.N. Laland, and M.W.
2000 Human-like, population-level specializa- Feldman.
tion in the manufacture of pandanus 2003 Niche construction: The neglected process in
tools by New Caledonian crows (Corvus evolution. Princeton: Princeton University
moneduloides). Proceedings of the Royal Society Press.
of London (B) 267:403413. Paz-y-Mio, G., A.B. Bond, A.C. Kamil, and R.P.
Knight, R.L. Balda.
1984 Responses of nesting ravens to people 2004 Pinyon jays use transitive inference
in areas of different human densities. to predict social dominance. Nature
Condor 86:345346. 430:778-781.
Knight, R.L., D.J. Grout, and S.A. Temple. Pryor, K., J. Lindbergh, S. Lindbergh, and R.
1987 Nest-defense behavior of the American Milano.
Crow in urban and rural areas. Condor 1990 A dolphin-human fishing cooperative in
89:17577. Brazil. Marine Mammal Science 6:77-82.
Krnneck, P. Rendell, L., and H. Whitehead.
1995 Bird remains from Troy, Turkey, in 2001 Cetacean culture: Still afloat after the
Archaeozoology of the Near East II. Edited first naval engagement of the cul-
by H. Buitenhuis and H.P. Uerpmann, ture wars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
pp. 109-112. Backhuys: Leiden. 24:360-382.
Kubota, H. Rogers, A.R.
2004 Crows in the city. Highlights For Children 1988 Does biology constrain culture? Ameri-
May:10-11. can Anthropologist 90:819-831.
Lachlan, R.F., and M.W. Feldman. Sax, B.
2003 Evolution of cultural communication 2003 Crow. London: Reaktion Books, LTD.
systems: The coevolution of cultural Shida, T.
signals and genes encoding learning 2001 Traps planned to corral pesky Tokyo
preferences. Journal of Evolutionary Biol- crows. Japan Times, December 15.
ogy 16:1084-1095. Shirota, Y.
Laland, K.N., J. Odling-Smee, and M.W. Feldman. 1989 A new method to scare crows Corvus
2000 Niche construction, biological evolu- macrorhynchos and C. corone. Bird Behaviour
tion, and cultural change. Behavioral and 8:1-7.
Brain Sciences 23:131-175. Stein, J.K.
Marzluff, J.M., and T. Angell. 2000 Exploring Coast Salish prehistory: The archeol-
2005 In the company of crows and ravens. New ogy of San Juan Island. Seattle: University
Haven: Yale University Press. of Washington Press.
Nelson, R.K. Tylor, E.B.
1983 Make prayers to the raven: A Koyukon view of 1871 Primitive culture: Researches into the develop-
the northern forest. Chicago: University of ment of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and
Chicago Press. custom. London: J. Murray.
Nihei, Y., and H. Higuchi. Wyrost, P.
2001 When and where did crows learn to 1993 The fauna of ancient Poland in the
use automobiles as nutcrackers? Tohoku light of archaeozoological research, in
Psychologica Folia 60:9397. Skeletons in her cupboard, Oxbow monograph
34. Edited by A. Clason, S. Payne, and
H.P. Uerpmann, pp. 251-259. Oxford:
Oxbow Books.

Potrebbero piacerti anche