Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

PERSPECTIVES

moral philosophy have been reviewed in detail


OPINION
elsewhere68 and are not addressed here.

The neural basis of human moral The challenge


Morality is a product of evolutionary pres-

cognition sures that have shaped social cognitive and


motivational mechanisms, which had already
developed in human ancestors, into uniquely
human forms of experience and behaviour9.
Jorge Moll, Roland Zahn, Ricardo de Oliveira-Souza, Frank Krueger Non-human primates have a vast repertoire
and Jordan Grafman of social behaviours that can be interpreted as
genuine forerunners of human morality, such
Abstract | Moral cognitive neuroscience is an contextual social knowledge, represented as caring for their peers and constantly striv-
emerging field of research that focuses on the as event knowledge in the prefrontal cortex ing for dominance10. As in humans, a sense of
neural basis of uniquely human forms of social (PFC); social semantic knowledge, stored in justice permeates their behaviour11. The evolu-
cognition and behaviour. Recent functional the anterior and posterior temporal cortex; tion of the human PFC is intimately related
imaging and clinical evidence indicates that and motivational and basic emotional states, to the emergence of human morality1215.
a remarkably consistent network of brain which depend on corticallimbic circuits. Our This has allowed motivational mechanisms
regions is involved in moral cognition. These framework offers new interpretations for social to be integrated with an exceptional power
findings are fostering new interpretations of behaviour patterns in healthy individuals and to predict outcomes, and has characterized
social behavioural impairments in patients in patients with brain dysfunction, and makes humans through their recent evolutionary
with brain dysfunction, and require new testable predictions for neuropsychological steps in the cultural explosion of the Upper
approaches to enable us to understand dissociations in moral cognition. Paleolithic period16.
the complex links between individuals The challenge for moral cognitive neuro-
and society. Here, we propose a cognitive Defining morality science is that it requires extensive cross-field
neuroscience view of how cultural and Moral (derived from the Latin moralis) and integration of neuroscience, psychology,
context-dependent knowledge, semantic ethical (from the Greek thikos) originally evolutionary biology and anthropology,
social knowledge and motivational states can referred to the consensus of manners and among other areas. In setting the goals of
be integrated to explain complex aspects of customs within a social group, or to an incli- scientific exploration in this field, some
human moral cognition. nation to behave in some ways but not in central issues should be considered. How
others4. Through the centuries, philosophical does the human moral mind emerge from
At a time of increasing awareness of the diff- theories have adopted a deductive logico the interaction of biological and cultural
erent value systems in multicultural societies verbal approach to morality that aims to factors? How can the context-dependent
and across nations, a deeper understanding identify universal principles that should nature of moral cognition be explained by
of the cognitive and brain mechanisms that guide human conduct. By contrast, a scientific neuroscience? How does moral cognition
guide human behaviour is of general interest. approach to morality is emerging from the relate to emotion and motivation, and what
Recent social cognitive neuroscience reviews documentation of changes in moral behaviour are their neural substrates? Although moral
have emphasized perceptual and emotional in patients with brain dysfunction5, which cognitive neuroscience is still in its infancy,
abilities that are shared by humans and other provides inferences that concern the major the available evidence already points to some
animals13. However, social neuroscience dimensions of moral cognition. Moral cogni- promising solutions.
has largely avoided dealing directly with the tive neuroscience, therefore, aims to elucidate
complex aspects of human moral cognition, the cognitive and neural mechanisms that The neural basis of moral cognition
including MORAL EMOTIONS and MORAL VALUES. underlie moral behaviour. Here, morality is Moral behaviour impairment. Persistent
Here, we review current theoretical accounts considered as the sets of customs and values antisocial behaviours have long been desc-
of social cognition and put forth a framework that are embraced by a cultural group to guide ribed17, yet their history in medicine is rela-
designed to overcome the main limitations social conduct, a view that does not assume tively recent. Impairment in moral sense, or
of earlier accounts. We argue that moral the existence of absolute moral values. The moral insanity, was first formally described
phenomena emerge from the integration of implications of cognitive neuroscience for as a perversion of natural feelings, affections,

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 | O CTOBER 2005 | 799


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

a b intentionality, which leads to reduced experi-


ence of pride and embarrassment1,34. Lesions
DLPFC to limbic and paralimbic structures can
impair basic motivational mechanisms, such
aPFC as sexual drive, social attachment and aggres-
STS
IOFC siveness, leading to extreme moral violations
aTL for example, unprovoked physical assaults
and paedophilia35,36. Structural and functional
Amygdala Hypothalamus Tegmentum imaging studies in psychopathic individuals
Basal forebrain Septal area have pointed to abnormalities in almost all
these regions3740.
aPFC c

IOFC mOFC
Moral emotion and judgement. Recent studies
have directly addressed the neural correlates
of moral emotions and judgements. Patients
aTL
with focal damage to the ventromedial
PFC show deficient engagement of pride,
embarrassment and regret41,42. Functional
imaging studies in healthy individuals have
involved simple MORAL JUDGEMENTS4345, moral
dilemmas46,47 and moral emotions48,4952 ,
using different tasks and stimulus presenta-
aPFC
vmPFC/ tion schemes. Overall, there is remarkable
mOFC agreement between functional imaging and
aTL clinico-anatomical evidence about the brain
areas involved in moral cognition. Activated
regions include the anterior PFC (encom-
passing the frontopolar cortex, Brodmanns
area (BA) 9/10), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC;
Figure 1 | Brain regions implicated in moral cognition and behaviour in functional imaging and especially its medial sector, BA 10/11/25), pos-
patient studies. a | Cortical regions13,99,107 include the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC), the medial and terior STS (BA 21/39), anterior temporal lobes
lateral orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC and lOFC), the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC; mostly the right hemisphere)
(BA 20/21/38), insula, precuneus (BA 7/31),
and additional ventromedial sectors of the PFC (vmPFC), the anterior temporal lobes (aTL) and the
superior temporal sulcus (STS) region. b | Subcortical structures13,36,48 include the amygdala,
anterior cingulate cortex (ACC, BA 24/32)
ventromedial hypothalamus, septal area and nuclei, basal forebrain (especially the ventral striatum/ and limbic regions. Notably, the wide range
pallidum and extended amygdala), the walls of the third ventricle and rostral brainstem tegmentum. of modalities, stimuli and task requirements
c | Brain regions that have not been consistently associated with moral cognition and behaviour in appear to have little effect on brain activation
patient studies include the parietal and occipital lobes, large areas of the frontal and temporal lobes, the patterns (FIG. 2).
brain stem, basal ganglia and additional subcortical structures. Panel b modified, with permission, from Besides the consistent patterns of brain
REF. 147 (2005) University of Iowas Virtual Hospital. Anatomical image adapted, with permission,
from REF. 148 (1996) Appleton & Lange.
activation found across studies, there were
also some differential findings. We found
activation of the anterior PFC when a moral
judgement condition was compared with
inclinations, temper, habits, moral disposi- in both moral reasoning and behaviour, non-emotional factual judgements43, but not
tions, and natural impulses18,19. Systematic indicating that moral development can be when moral judgements were compared with
evidence that specific brain regions might be arrested by early PFC damage24,25. These a socialemotional condition, during which
crucial to moral behaviour was provided by impairments in moral conduct resemble a more ventral region was activated44. Greene
early accounts of frontal lobe damage20,21 and those observed in developmental PSYCHO and colleagues used a moral judgement task
neurosurgical reports of war wounds (see, for PATHY
26,27
BOX 1. Less frequently2831, lesions that involved classic moral dilemmas (for
example, REF. 22) (FIG. 1). of the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC; typically of example, should you kill an innocent person
More recently, researchers have started to the right hemisphere) also lead to changes in order to save five other people?) and found
explicitly frame these observations within the in moral behaviour. similar activation of the anterior PFC46,47.
sphere of moral cognition, strengthening the In addition to the PFC, other brain Decision difficulty was correlated with
links between neuroscience, developmental regions are crucial for moral cognition. increased activity in the ACC. Heekeren and
neuropsychology and moral psychology. Structural changes in the anterior temporal colleagues showed that the presence of bodily
Eslinger and Damasio23 described moral lobes either acquired or developmental harm in moral violation scenarios leads to
behavioural deficits in a patient with dam- can also impair moral behaviours28,32. decreased reaction times and decreased
age to the ventromedial PFC acquired in Dysfunction of neural circuits that involve activation of the anterior temporal lobe53.
adulthood, who was remarkably unimpaired the superior temporal sulcus (STS) region Evidence is emerging that partially dissociable
in specific MORAL REASONING tasks. It was a key area for social perception33 is PFCtemporallimbic networks represent
later shown that ventromedial PFC lesions associated with the difficulty experienced distinct moral emotions, including guilt,
acquired at an early age led to impairments by individuals with autism in attributing anger and embarrassment13,4952.

800 | O CTOBER 2005 | VOLUME 6 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

Box 1 | Psychopathy and the neural organization of morality conflict monitoring would affect moral
cognition55.
The concepts of antisocial personality disorder (sociopathy) and psychopathy (a severe form of
sociopathy) originated from the need to diagnose individuals who show a pattern of behaviours Somatic marker hypothesis. Damasio and
that goes against the common good and repeatedly involves harm to others. Although social colleagues observed that patients with
norms vary among cultures and even among intracultural niches, sociopathy and psychopathy ventromedial PFC damage can detect the
cannot be reduced to cultural artefacts141 for the simple fact that their core manifestations are implications of a social situation, but cannot
stable and easily recognizable, both historically and cross-culturally. The neurobiological
make appropriate decisions in real life. They
validity of sociopathy and psychopathy is supported by increasing scientific evidence that the
suggested that such patients would be unable
brains of affected individuals differ from those of socially adjusted people: imaging studies in
to mark those implications with a signal that
psychopaths have revealed reduction of grey matter in the prefrontal cortex and abnormal brain
activation in limbic regions, as well as in the prefrontal and temporal lobes38,39.
automatically distinguishes advantageous
from pernicious actions 56 . The somatic
marker model explains why patients with
ventromedial PFC damage can still reason
Current accounts possibility that culturally shaped moral about social problems, provided the premises
Some current cognitive neuroscience values and beliefs might lead to disparate are cast verbally, but fail in natural settings.
frameworks have direct implications for our utilitarian conclusions. Finally, it is unclear The IOWA GAMBLING TASK, which was preceded
understanding of the neural basis of moral how impairment of cognitive control and by similar gambling tasks57, was put forward
cognition. The main characteristics and limi-
tations of these accounts are briefly reviewed
a b c d e
and discussed below, with an emphasis on
their relevance to moral cognition (see also
TABLE 1).

Conflict processing in moral judgement. On


the basis of functional imaging studies46,47,
Greene and colleagues have focused on the
role of cognitive control in moral judge- f g
ment. Their hypothesis was derived in part
from Miller and Cohens theoretical account STS
of PFC function54, which assumes that the
PFC is specifically involved in controlled
processing , such as in rapidly changing, ill-
The murderer was
structured situations characteristics that sent to jail
are also held by other models14. This proposal
PFC
is supported by evidence for DLPFC and
ACC activation in response to increases in aTL
attentional and conflict detection demands. Happiness
Sadness
Greenes hierarch ical processing view Disgust
assumes that cognitive control processes, Some children live Fear
Anger
afforded by the lateral PFC and ACC (cog- in the streets
nitive areas), override emotional responses Figure 2 | Functional imaging studies of moral cognition. Functional imaging studies of moral cognition
(which are attributed to the medial PFC, pos- have revealed consistent involvement of the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) and superior temporal sulcus
terior cingulate cortex and STS) to produce (STS) region, as well as the anterior temporal lobes (aTL) and limbic structures. Panels ae depict a
transverse slice showing the activation of the aPFC (frontopolar cortex, Brodmanns areas 9/10) across
UTILITARIAN responses to moral dilemmas
different studies43,46,48,45,50. Panel f shows spatially overlapping activations in the PFC, STS region and aTL,
for example, smothering a crying baby to derived from a conjunction analysis of two different studies: active moral judgements of written stimuli44 and
save more lives. By contrast, emotional areas passive viewing of pictures with moral content48. Samples of the pictorial149 and written stimuli used in these
would favour personal moral judgements studies are shown. The remarkable overlap of brain regions involved in moral cognition, regardless of a
for example, thinking that it is inappro- wide variation in task requirements and stimulus modalities, contrasts with the large variability observed in
priate to smother the baby. The theory posits brain imaging studies of less complex basic emotions depicted in panel g (REF. 150). A strong possible
explanation is the effect of familiarity and situational context, which have not been controlled in functional
mutually competitive roles of cognition and
imaging studies of basic emotions or moral cognition. The higher reproducibility of the activation patterns in
emotion in moral judgement. studies of moral cognition might, therefore, have resulted from a smaller contextual variability related to the
Greenes functional imaging findings are use of more well-defined social situations for moral judgements and moral emotions. By contrast,
in line with the cognitive control view and experimental designs of studies of basic emotion have put more effort into equating the sensory properties
demonstrate reliable task-related effects in of stimuli (such as luminance, visual complexity and frequency) at the cost of more variability in social
different types of moral judgement. However, contexts (such as fear associated with a picture of a spider or with a crime scene). Panel a reproduced,
the concepts of personal and impersonal with permission, from REF. 43 (2001) Associacao Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria. Panel b reproduced,
with permission, from REF. 46 (2001) American Association for the Advancement of Science. Panel c
violations, and of utilitarian and non- reproduced, with permission, from REF. 48 (2002) Society for Neuroscience. Panel d reproduced, with
utilitarian choices need to be broken down permission, from REF. 45 (2003) Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. Panel e reproduced, with permission,
into clear cognitive components. Further- from REF. 50 (2004) Elsevier Science. Panel f (right-hand images) from REF. 149. Panel g reproduced,
more, this account does not address the with permission, from REF. 150 (2002) Elsevier Science.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 | O CTOBER 2005 | 801


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

Table 1 | Characteristics and limitations of frameworks relevant to moral cognitive neuroscience


Theoretical Situational and cultural Relationship between Predictions from Implications for moral Key references
accounts context effects cognition and emotion brain lesions cognitive phenomena
Conflict control in No predictions Hierarchical; emotions No Task difficulty and cognitive 47,54
moral judgement inhibited by cognitive control in moral judgement
processes
Somatic marker Partially addressed Integrated; autonomic Yes Implicit assessment of 56,58,59
hypothesis signals guide cognition decision outcomes
Social response Partially addressed Hierarchical; emotional Yes Suppression of aggressive 62,69,70
reversal signals help suppress behaviour
aggression
Impairment of ToM No predictions No predictions Yes Impairments in attribution of 7678
mechanisms in mental states in sociopathy
sociopathy
Structured- Predicted Hierarchical; PFC Yes Contextual effects and 14,15,84
event-complex representations control social knowledge
framework emotional responses
Moral sensitivity Partially addressed Integrated; social Yes Moral emotions and implicit 13,43,44,48
hypothesis perception and moral moral appraisals
emotions are bound
together
Eventfeature Predicted Integrated; social Yes Binding of context- 13,48,82,87
emotion complex features, motivation, dependent social
framework emotion and contextual knowledge, features and
knowledge are bound emotion
together
PFC, prefrontal cortex; ToM, theory of mind.

as an experimental surrogate for decision- Social response reversal. The social RESPONSE normally activated by perception or expecta-
making in real life. Bechara and colleagues58 REVERSAL model, which was proposed by Blair tion of others anger69. Blair suggested that a
showed that normal individuals develop and Cipolotti to explain social behavioural different inhibitory mechanism the vio-
anticipatory galvanic skin responses when- impairments in patients with OFC dam- lence inhibition mechanism (VIM) would
ever they contemplate a risky choice, and age, was influenced by Rolls and coworkers be deficient in developmental psychopathy,
begin to choose advantageously before they response-reversal paradigm. In their pioneer- leading to instrumental aggression70. The
are consciously aware of the best strategy. ing work, Rolls and colleagues showed that VIM underscores the role of the amygdala in
Patients with ventromedial PFC damage do patients with OFC damage were impaired aversive conditioning, and is believed to have
not develop anticipatory autonomic responses in EXTINCTION and response-reversal tasks64. a key role in moral socialization.
and behave as if they are insensitive to future These impairments were correlated with These accounts can be used to make spe-
consequences, positive or negative, being measures of socially inappropriate behav- cific predictions about the role of response
primarily guided by immediate prospects iours, which led to the hypothesis that the reversals and aversive conditioning in
that ultimately lead to a net financial loss. sociopathy of these patients results from patients with OFC and amygdala lesions.
The somatic marker hypothesis has been a difficulty in modifying behavioural However, they cannot be easily extended to
influential and is considered to be a possible responses, especially when these are followed explain other types of impairment in moral
mechanism that could underlie behavioural by negative outcomes. The response-reversal behaviour that arise from damage to other
dysfunction in patients with PFC lesions. model has received extensive support from brain regions, such as the temporal lobes and
This framework is compatible with contex- electrophysiological studies in animals65 as anterior PFC. In addition, these models were
tual effects (although these are not explic- well as from human lesion and neuroimaging not designed to explain how social knowl-
itly addressed), integrates cognition and data6668. edge, on which reinforcement contingencies
emotion, makes testable predictions, and Blair and Cipolotti compared their find- operate, is represented in the brain. Finally,
has been supported by neurophysiologi- ings from a patient with OFC damage (J.S.) although bilateral amygdala lesions lead to
cal and clinical data5860. However, it does with those from a patient with DLPFC impaired perceptual judgement of facial
not explicitly address the role of different damage and five prison inmates with psy- emotions71, evidence for severe impairments
PFC subregions in moral cognition. The chopathy62. J.S. showed a drastic change in in moral behaviour following isolated amyg-
relationships between somatic markers and personality after OFC damage, becoming dala lesions acquired either in adulthood or
other cortical and limbic regions that have aggressive and callous towards other people. early childhood is still lacking.
previously been linked to moral cognition13 He was impaired in recognizing facial expres-
are also obscure. Recent evidence from both sions of anger and disgust, but was unim- Sociopathy as a failure of theory of mind.
patients with PFC lesions and healthy indi- paired in response-reversal tasks. This led the Disruptive antisocial behaviour is a hallmark
viduals has challenged the role of somatic authors to argue for a social response-reversal of early frontotemporal dementia72. These
markers in guiding decision making and mechanism an inhibitory system reliant profound changes in personality have been
social behaviours6163. on the proper functioning of the OFC that is predominantly ascribed to degeneration of

802 | O CTOBER 2005 | VOLUME 6 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

the right PFC73 or the temporal poles74,75. for goal-oriented activities is also corrobo- Limitations of current frameworks
Lough et al.76 used a battery of neuropsycho- rated by recent functional imaging studies of Some of the above frameworks point to clear-
logical and social cognition tests to assess future reward prediction88. cut singular mechanisms. These mechanisms
J.M., a 47-year-old man who presented Although this framework has clear have the potential advantage of allowing
with a decline in work performance and implications for moral cognition, these rely more specific predictions to be made about
a gross deterioration in social behaviour. on the hypothesis that the PFC stores the the workings of particular brain regions, but
Imaging studies revealed bilateral atrophy situational and temporal context of social they fall short of explaining key aspects of
of the OFC and anterior temporal lobes, knowledge. The SEC framework does not moral cognition. Some general limitations
including the amygdala. J.M. had a normal predict how PFC regions interact with lim- that apply to all of these frameworks are
IQ and fared well on standard executive bic areas and other cortical regions to give discussed below.
tests, but was otherwise severely impaired rise to a range of moral cognitive pheno-
on THEORY OF MIND (ToM) tasks that require a mena, such as moral values and moral Ecological validity of experimental designs.
degree of abstraction, with specific deficits emotions. Ecological validity is especially relevant
on first- and SECONDORDER FALSE BELIEF TASKS, for moral cognition studies, because moral
and on detection of faux pas. The authors cognition depends strongly on situational
proposed that the dissociation between Ecological validity is and cultural context 6. The experimental
the impairment in ToM mechanisms and constraints that are imposed by behavioural
normal executive performance underlies especially relevant for moral and functional imaging studies might have
the personality changes observed in some cognition studies, because an important impact on performance
cases of frontotemporal dementia. This on moral cognition tasks. Some people
account is therefore compatible with abnor- moral cognition depends might feel uncomfortable disclosing their
mal moral cognition such as difficulties
in the attribution and experience of pride
strongly on situational and opinions about sensitive issues, providing
socially desirable answers instead. On the
and embarrassment observed in autism cultural context. other hand, different people might provide
and Aspergers syndrome, which are typi- similar opinions, but rely on entirely dif-
cally associated with ToM impairments34,77. ferent moral values. The fact that moral
However, ToM abilities only account for Moral sensitivity hypothesis. A final account cognition operates to a large extent swiftly
some aspects of moral cognition, but not, is that of the moral sensitivity hypothesis13,48. and implicitly in regular social life13 makes
for example, the role of social knowledge, Using a task that engaged participants as the ecological validity issue even more cru-
contextual information and basic motiva- observers, we showed that the viewing of cial. The making of moral judgements on
tions. Noticeably, ToM is relatively intact pictures that depicted moral violations spe- extreme and unfamiliar situations, such as
in psychopathy, in line with its role in the cifically activated the anterior PFC, medial those posed by classic moral dilemmas89,
deviousness of these individuals78. OFC, STS region, brainstem and limbic offers interesting ways to probe philosophi-
structures. Scenes associated with BASIC cal points of view, but can hardly be taken
Structured-event-complex framework. The EMOTIONS (disgust and fear) activated simi- as a proxy for everyday moral reasoning.
structured-event-complex (SEC) frame- lar brainstem and limbic regions (including In addition, personal beliefs and familiarity
work 15 supports claims that executive the amygdala), but not the medial OFC and with the scenarios strongly affect behaviour
functions performed by the PFC are based STS. These findings are consistent with the and brain activation results9094.
on stored event sequence knowledge. SEC hypothesis that a network involving the
representations are long-term memories of anterior PFC, OFC, STS and limbic regions Brain processes and representations. Another
event sequences that guide the perception represents socialemotional events linked to important limitation of current accounts is
and execution of goal-oriented activities, moral sensitivity an automatic tagging the lack of specific predictions about the
such as going to a concert or giving a dinner of ordinary social events with moral values. effects of PFC lesions on moral behav-
party79. A SEC representation includes situ- This hypothesis was supported by the find- iour. PFC function has been described
ational knowledge abstracted across events ing that the medial OFC, anterior PFC, STS using two general views: the processing
(concert) and the temporal organization of and precuneus show increased coupling in approach, which holds that the cognitive
events (making a reservation, dressing up, a functional connectivity analysis48, and by function of the PFC can be described in
and so on). Activated SECs sequentially the observation that a similar set of regions terms of performance without specifying
bind representations of objects, actions is involved in moral reasoning and social a representation, and the representational
and spatial maps stored in posterior brain perception. Although we proposed that approach, which seeks to establish what type
regions. The SEC framework predicts that the OFC is more involved in automatic of information is stored in the PFC14. The
different subdivisions of the PFC store dif- socialemotional associations and that the processing view tends to regard the PFC as a
ferent types of content or domains of event anterior PFC has a role in predicting future content-free repository of processing mod-
knowledge14,80,81. Clinical and neuroimaging social outcomes, the role of the PFC in ules, such as conflict monitoring, selection
evidence supports this prediction, showing context-dependent social situations was not and inhibitory control95, and predicts task-
that different PFC regions are involved in addressed. In addition, the moral sensitiv- dependent rather than content-dependent
representing social and emotional SECs ity hypothesis makes no predictions about dissociations resulting from brain lesions.
(ventromedial PFC) 82,83 , novel or multi- specific impairments in moral cognition Therefore, moral behaviour impairments
tasking event sequences (anterior PFC)84,85 following selective damage to the anterior following PFC damage would result from a
or overlearned sequences (more posterior temporal lobes, the STS region and PFC release of limbic areas from PFC executive
PFC regions)86,87. The importance of the PFC subregions. control96. However, there is no convincing

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 | O CTOBER 2005 | 803


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

the integration of cultural and contextual


Box 2 | Culture, moral values and neuroeconomics
information during development24,106,107 .
a b Assessing the relationships between cultur-
Sukuma
Prec 60 South America ally shaped values and preferences in social
aPFC
South Pacific interactions will therefore be a logical next
Tanzania step in designing experiments with which to
50 Africa
study moral cognition BOX 2.

Percentage of mean offer


40 A new model: EFECs
Hypothalmus The evidence discussed above strongly
aPFC 30 indicates that the neural mechanisms of
moral cognition are not restricted to the
PFC, limbic areas or any other brain region.
20
We propose a new representational neural
architecture, designed to circumvent the
10
limitations of previous frameworks. In our
view, moral cognitive phenomena emerge
STS 0 from the integration of content- and context-
Ac e

M ap r
gu e
Ts ga

Au

La au

ra

bw a
ns
e
dependent representations in corticallimbic
a
h

dz

ba rm
ng
an
hu

ela

ea
Ac

ac uc

n
en

Ha
G

Sa

m O
im

m
Prec
networks.
M

hi

The structure of the framework, its

Zi
Humans often show altruistic inclinations, relying on moral values and preferences, such as properties and its predictions rely on three
equality and fairness, as well as to self-interested motivation142. Economic games provide an main components (FIG. 3a): structured event
interesting way to experimentally investigate social cooperation. In the Ultimatum Game, a knowledge, which corresponds to context-
proposer makes an offer to a responder on how to split an amount of money. If the responder dependent representations of events and
accepts, the money is split as proposed. However, if the responder rejects, both players end up event sequences in the PFC; social percep-
with nothing. Recent functional imaging studies in the new area of NEUROECONOMICS show that tual and functional features, represented as
the brain areas activated during these interactions include limbic/paralimbic regions (the context-independent knowledge in the
hypothalamus and ventral striatum), the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) and the superior anterior and posterior temporal cortex; and
temporal sulcus (STS)143, which overlap with the regions involved in moral cognition (panel a). central motive and emotional states, which
Activation of the insula, a paralimbic structure, predicted rejection of unfair offers144, and correspond to context-independent activa-
activity in the aPFC and striatum reflected decisions to punish violators of the norm145. tion in limbic and paralimbic structures.
An interesting aspect of these experimental designs is that they make it possible to measure
These components were derived from clinical
brain activation during real-time interactions among two or more individuals. Prec, precuneus.
and imaging evidence, and their relevance to
Behavioural studies clearly underscore the role of culturally shaped preferences and values in
moral cognition and behaviour is reviewed
social and economic interactions. For example, behaviour in experimental games might reflect
differences in social cooperativeness, such as proneness to engage in collective efforts. In a
below. Component representations interact
study conducted in Tanzania, the more individualistic Pimbwe group made low offers in the and give rise to eventfeatureemotion com-
Ultimatum Game, whereas the highly cooperative Sukuma group consistently made generous plexes (EFECs) through three putative BINDING
offers146. Such cultural differences are illustrated by the variability of proposals in the mechanisms: sequential binding, which has
Ultimatum Game among different social groups (panel b), although the underlying cognitive been proposed to link SECs in the PFC108;
and motivational mechanisms and their relationships to social norms and values are still temporal binding among anatomically highly
largely unknown105. Future studies could address the distinct roles of PFC subregions, limbic connected regions, also involved in PERCEPTUAL
GESTALTS in the posterior cortex ; and third-
109
areas and the temporal cortex in representing culturally shaped moral values and norms. Panel a
reproduced, with permission, from REF. 143 (2004) Elsevier Science. Panel b modified, with party binding of anatomically loosely con-
permission, from REF. 146 (2005) Sigma Xi, The Scientific Research Society. nected regions by synchronized activity,
which results in the formation of episodic
memories108,110.
evidence that PFC damage leads to universal view can better explain the role of the PFC
impairments in these processes, and it is in moral cognition. Structured event knowledge. Morality is a
hard to imagine how complex personality real-world business. It is about people navi-
and emotional changes could emerge from Culture and the brain. Finally, inferring cog- gating, interacting and making choices in
dysfunction of these all-purpose processes97. nitive and neural mechanisms from behav- an ever-changing world. Humans integrate
The finding that performance on social iours can be misleading101, especially when extensive contextual elements when assess-
reasoning tasks crucially depends on the con- cultural and situational factors are involved. ing the behaviour of others and when appre-
tent of the information being evaluated (for For instance, Westerners and East Asians ciating their own actions in a given situation.
example, social versus non-social)90,91,94,98, differ in categorization strategies when mak- The importance of the PFC in structuring
and evidence from functional imaging and ing causal attributions and predictions102, context-dependent social and non-social
brain lesion studies linking PFC subregions and moral values and social preferences are knowledge into SECs is described in terms
to content-specific dissociations in social rea- shaped by cultural codification103105. The of the SEC framework 14 . Distinct PFC
soning99, ATTITUDES82,83, beliefs92 and emotional PFC has a central role in the internaliza- regions have been postulated to be involved
signals100 indicate that a representational tion of moral values and norms through in representing event sequence knowledge.

804 | O CTOBER 2005 | VOLUME 6 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

According to the SEC model, over-learned a


event sequences, such as routine tasks, are Structured event
stored in medial and more posterior sectors knowledge Features
of the PFC, whereas less predictable event Complex, Social perceptual
long duration, features (of face, gaze,
sequences are represented in the DLPFC. multi-stage voice, body posture)
The anterior sectors of the PFC are more
important for storing long-term goals and Overlearned, Social functional
highly structured features (of social
multi-stage event complexes, such as those (medial wall) behaviours)
involved in making plans and thinking
Unfamiliar context
about the future23,84,111113, and have been (ill-structured)
implicated in integrating separate cogni- Central motive states
Socialemotional
tive operations to achieve a superordinate Aggression, anxiety, attachment,
behavioural goal84,114. Finally, the ventro- happiness, sadness, hunger,
sexual arousal
medial sectors of the PFC are preferentially
b
involved in representing social and emo-
Predicting the future of the
tional event knowledge, which is essential orphan child
for the formation of attitudes and social Perceptual features of
stereotypes115117. sadness of facial and body
expression

Social perceptual and functional features. Conceptual knowledge of


features of helplessness
When you skim your favourite newspaper,
gather at a conference or attend a family Compassion
meeting, your brain deals with a massive Experiencing attachment,
anxiety and sadness
number of perceptual signs of social signifi-
cance. Our ability to manage this burden of
information relies on complex patterns
of featural and semantic knowledge 118 . c Compassion d Embarrassment
The existence of context-independent
featural representations is supported by aPFC
a vast amount of neuropsychological and aTL
aPFC STS
functional imaging evidence119,120. Making
ACC
implicit or explicit moral appraisals when Hypothalamus
engaged in the social world requires the abil- aTL
ity to efficiently extract social perceptual and
functional features from the environment. e Indignation f Guilt
Social perceptual features are extracted from aTL
facial expression, gaze, prosody, body pos-
ACC
ture and gestures. The posterior STS is a key aPFC
region for storing these representations33,121.
In support of this view, morphological OFC Hypothalamus
abnormalities of the STS region have been
implicated in the impaired social decoding Figure 3 | The eventfeatureemotion complex framework. a | The eventfeatureemotion
observed in autism122. complex (EFEC) framework postulates that moral cognitive and behavioural phenomena arise from
Social functional features code for the binding of three main components: structured event knowledge (provided by context-dependent
representations in prefrontal subregions), social perceptual and functional features (stored in the
context-independent semantic properties
posterior and anterior sectors of the temporal cortex) and central motive or basic emotional states
that are extracted from different social (such as aggressiveness, sadness, attachment or sexual arousal, represented in limbic and paralimbic
situations. The importance of the ante- regions). b | Emergent representations predicted by the EFEC model. Relevant types of moral cognition
rior temporal cortex for semantic feature phenomenon that can be understood on the basis of the EFEC framework include moral emotions,
knowledge is underscored by supramodal moral values and long-term goals. The elements from the three main components of the EFEC
semantic impairments in semantic demen- framework interact to produce the moral emotion compassion. The prefrontal cortex provides
contextual event representations (for example, the girl is an orphan and the odds of adoption are low),
tia123. Patients with anterior temporal lobe
the superior temporal sulcus and anterior temporal cortex region contribute social perceptual (sad
resection show impairments in naming facial expression of a child) and functional (the concept of helplessness) features, and limbic/
human actions124, which indicates that this paralimbic regions underlie central motive states (feeling sadness, anxiety and attachment). These
region is involved in representing functional component representations give rise to a gestalt experience by way of temporal synchronization109.
knowledge relevant to people. The severe cf | Recent functional imaging studies show that these component representations are consistently
behavioural changes that are associated with activated by distinct moral emotions: compassion (Moll et al., unpublished observations) (c);
isolated anterior temporal atrophy in seman- embarrassment50 (d); indignation151 (e); and guilt49 (f). ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; aPFC, anterior
prefrontal cortex; aTL, anterior temporal lobes; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; STS, superior temporal
tic dementia74,75, and the finding of semantic sulcus. Anatomical image in panels a and b adapted, with permission, from REF. 148 (1996) Appleton
impairments and abnormal activity in this & Lange. Panel d reproduced, with permission, from REF. 50 (2004) Elsevier Science. Panel e
brain region in psychopathic individuals39,125 reproduced, with permission, from REF. 151 (2005) Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. Panel f
support this view. reproduced, with permission, from REF. 49 (2000) Elsevier Science.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 | O CTOBER 2005 | 805


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

Central motive states. Moral cognition pride, contempt and gratitude are prototypi- Recent functional MRI (fMRI) studies
depends on elaborated cortical mecha- cal examples of moral emotions; depending have started to shed light on these aspects.
nisms for representing and retrieving event on the context, other emotions such Attitudes that relate to sensitive issues, such
knowledge, semantic information and per- as disgust, awe and indignation or anger as war, murder and abortion, activate net-
ceptual features. However, morality would may also qualify as moral emotions131,132. works involving different PFC sectors, limbic
be reduced to a meaningless concept if it As a general rule, moral emotions result and paralimbic regions and the anterior
were stripped from its motivational and from interactions among values, norms and temporal cortex80,135. In our view, the moral
emotional aspects. Limbic and paralimbic contextual elements of social situations, values and moral emotions involved in spe-
regions126 monitor bodily homeostasis and and are elicited in response to violations or cific situations directly influence implicit
underlie elementary emotional or motiva- enforcement of social preferences and expec- and explicit moral appraisals.
tional states. The concept of central motive tations104,132. Although the contextual cues Another key aspect of moral cognition is
states 127 is an influential account of the that link moral emotions to social norms the representation of goals and the predic-
basic mechanisms of motivation. Together are variable and shaped by culture103, these tion of the utility of outcomes136 in social
with other limbic/paralimbic and brainstem emotions evolved from prototypes found in situations. Pursuing goals or foreseeing
structures (the amygdala, septal nuclei, other primates11 and can be characterized possible consequences of ones decisions
ventral striatum, medial forebrain bundle, across cultures133. in the social world requires the ability to
ventral tegmental area and para limbic estimate the likelihood of outcomes and
cortex), hypothalamic activity has a central their desirability. Functional integration
role in undirected emotionality, including ...moral emotions of information in the anterior PFC (which
sexual arousal, social attachment, hunger, represents long-term outcomes)88 and limbic
aggression and extremes of pleasantness.
result from interactions structures (which code for the reward value
Accordingly, these states can be potently among values, norms of behavioural choices) is key to our abil-
elicited or suppressed by selective lesions, ity to weigh the motivational relevance of
drugs and electrical stimulation of these and contextual elements different behavioural choices in social situ-
regions, as well as by imbalances of neuro- of social situations, and ations13. This view can be parsimoniously
transmitters or neuromodulators13,36,126129. integrated with cognitive and neurobiologi-
Central motive states must be distinguished are elicited in response to cal models of reward expectation and utility
from basic emotions, such as fear and dis- violations or enforcement estimation65,137139, and contrasts with the
gust. Basic emotions emerge by temporal interpretation that the PFC performs a cog-
binding of context representations (per- of social preferences and nitive role in abstract moral reasoning by
ceiving the feared object or situation) and expectations. suppressing emotional responses47. Our view
the central motive state itself (undirected posits a central role for the human ability to
anxiety). represent and evaluate large sets of possible
Several limbic nuclei exert a powerful Moral emotions require the integration event outcomes, which are linked to moti-
influence over a wide range of behaviours of the three components of the model. For vational salience through corticallimbic
through reciprocal connections with the example, compassion requires the integra- integration.
PFC and other cortical regions126,130. Our tion of context-independent social per-
framework underscores a key role for cen- ceptual features (for instance, a sad facial Model predictions
tral motive states in moral behaviour by way expression of a child), social functional The EFEC framework allows us to gener-
of integrated corticallimbic networks. For features (abstract conceptual knowledge ate new predictions about the patterns of
example, cortical representations allow you pointing to the features of helplessness of moral behavioural changes that result from
to notice that someone is hurt, whereas cen- an orphan child), and central motive states dysfunction of different brain regions that
tral motive states elicit anxiety and attach- (sadness, anxiety and attachment) with cannot be made using the other frameworks
ment, which encourage you to help the specific contextual event representations described above. In addition, it offers novel
suffering person. This integrative perspec- (such as her parents died in an accident, ways of interpreting functional imaging
tive contrasts with the commonly held view and the chances of adoption at her age are findings in healthy individuals. Some of
that rational cognitive mechanisms control low) (FIG. 3b). these predictions are described below.
or compete with emotional ones. Moral values (for example, being an hon- A general prediction is that different
est citizen or a caring parent) and norms neural subdivisions store distinct knowl-
Explaining complex moral phenomena (such as paying taxes and not stealing) edge or motivational states. The binding of
Although the EFEC framework can predict comprise several standards of conduct in particular neuronal groups in each of these
several possible emergent properties, we society; they enforce social conformity and areas could give rise to a particular moral
discuss three of the most relevant for moral shape attitudes and expectations in social cognitive representation (FIG. 3b).
cognition: moral emotions, moral values and situations105,134. Behaviours that deviate from A lesion of the anterior PFC would lead
long-term goals. or enforce these values elicit different moral to selective impairments in moral evalua-
Whereas basic emotions spring from judgements and emotions (for instance, tions that rely on predicting the long-term
perceptions, imagination or recollections pride when one upholds the values, or guilt outcomes of ones own actions, such as the
endowed with personal relevance, moral when one fails to do so). Despite the inti- anticipation of guilt. We predict that patients
emotions are linked to the interest or welfare mate link between moral values, norms and with damage to this area would be guided
of other individuals or society as a whole131. attitudes with moral cognition, their neural more by short-term goals because their
Guilt, compassion, embarrassment, shame, representations are still poorly understood. knowledge of long-term plans and goals, or

806 | O CTOBER 2005 | VOLUME 6 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

their binding with motivational relevance is Glossary


impaired. In our interpretation, the activa-
tion of this region during moral judgement ATTITUDES MORAL VALUES
Context-dependent, emotionally laden social concepts Culturally shaped concepts and attitudes that code for
results from representing possible outcomes and intuitions. personal and societal preferences and standards.
and how they branch into the future; this
offers a parsimonious explanation for ante- BASIC EMOTIONS NEUROECONOMICS
rior PFC activation in reflective moral rea- A collection of emotions that are shared by most An interdisciplinary field that aims to understand
mammals (for example, fear, sadness, disgust, anger, cognitive and neurobiological mechanisms that underlie
soning (moral calculus)13, and in utilitarian
happiness and surprise) that can readily be recognized choice behaviour and utility estimation.
moral judgements47. from facial expressions (mimicry), gaze direction, voice
Lesions of the DLPFC would lead to intonation, gestures and body postures. PERCEPTUAL GESTALT
behavioural impairments in unfamiliar Simultaneous perception of sensory stimuli in one or
situations, in which reliance on external BINDING more sensory modalities, experienced as a unified,
Temporal synchronization of different neuronal integrated pattern.
guidance and stimuli becomes an issue54, but assemblies, which correspond to stored neural
would leave intact well-established social representations, or codes. PSYCHOPATHY
behaviours and attitudes. By contrast, lesions A severe form of antisocial personality disorder,
of the ventral sectors of the PFC would lead EXTINCTION characterized by callousness and lack of empathy.
The mechanism by which a previously learned automatic
to severe social behavioural changes due to
behavioural response is extinguished. RESPONSE REVERSAL
disruption of socialemotional contextual A change in a learned behavioural response following a
knowledge14, with early lesions having more IOWA GAMBLING TASK change in reinforcement contingencies.
drastic effects as they impair the learning of A card-sorting task designed to probe implicit
moral values107. Lesions of the ventromedial mechanisms that govern individual choices in reward SECONDORDER FALSE BELIEF TASKS
and punishment contexts. Sophisticated mind-reading tasks that require the
PFC would tend to impair adherence to evaluation of what another person believes that a third
well-established social norms and attitudes, MORAL EMOTIONS person is thinking.
which is consistent with the often ensuing Emotions that are linked to the interest or welfare of
personality changes. Lesions of the lateral other people or society as a whole. THEORY OF MIND
A specific cognitive ability that allows one to understand
OFC are expected to impair behaviours
MORAL JUDGEMENT other people as intentional, perceptive and emotional
that rely on dynamically comparing non- A type of evaluative judgement that is based on agents, or to interpret their minds in terms of intentional,
matching socialemotional cues with stored assessments of the adequacy of ones own and others perceptual or feeling states.
representations, which is in agreement with behaviours according to socially shaped ideas of right and
the proposed role of this region in social wrong. UTILITARIANISM
A moral philosophical theory according to which the
response reversal62. MORAL REASONING best decisions are those that lead to the higher overall
Damage to the posterior STS is predicted The thinking mechanism through which moral degree of happiness or well-being for the greatest number
to disrupt the ability to recognize socially judgements are attained. of people.
relevant perceptual features of faces, body
posture and movements. This would lead to
inadequate social behaviour under circum- Dysfunction of limbic or paralimbic Conclusions and future directions
stances that depend on the perception of regions is predicted to cause exaggeration Moral cognitive neuroscience researchers
these signals, but would leave intact pre- or attenuation of basic motivational and have developed innovative paradigms for
viously established social rules, attitudes and emotional states, thereby affecting moral the scientific exploration of unique forms
outcome knowledge, as well as their integra- behaviour. Lesions of the hypothalamus, of human social behaviour. Recent stud-
tion with emotional and motivational states. septal nuclei, basal forebrain and neighbour- ies are fostering new interpretations with
Therefore, acquired lesions in adulthood are ing structures are predicted to produce gross regard to the neural bases of moral cogni-
predicted to have a relatively minor effect distortions of the valence of moral values, tion. However, they are also generating
on general social knowledge. However, attitudes and moral emotions. This is in line new conundrums that require theoretical
early developmental disorders that affect with the observation of unprovoked rage, lack frameworks to be compatible with distinc-
this region would impair the acquisition of of empathy and abnormal sexual behaviours tive characteristics of the human moral
general social knowledge, including social following isolated damage to limbic and para- condition.
rules, attitudes and outcome knowledge, limbic regions35,36,128,140. In the case of acquired We have reviewed clinical and experimen-
which depends on the perceptual integration lesions in adulthood, gross changes in the tal work and discussed the strengths and lim-
of social situations. motivational relevance of behaviours would itations of current theoretical accounts that
Lesions of the anterior temporal lobe be observed, in spite of preserved knowledge are relevant to moral cognitive neuroscience.
are expected to disrupt knowledge of social of social rules. By contrast, early developmen- We have proposed a new comprehensive
concepts and values that are more context- tal disorders that affect these regions would model the eventfeatureemotion com-
independent (such as honour and greed), cause aberrant social learning. Abnormal plex framework which integrates cultural
but to leave intact highly context-dependent behaviours in these patients do not result from and context-dependent knowledge, semantic
knowledge of sequences of social events (for impaired inhibitory mechanisms, but from social knowledge and basic motivational
example, going to a supermarket). We pre- a lack of emotional empathy, or increased states. This framework allows us to generate
dict that loss of this knowledge, as measured aggression or sexual drive, for example. These testable predictions for neuropsychological
by semantic memory tasks, would impair motivational states can be investigated with dissociations associated with selective brain
implicit and explicit evaluation of ones own functional imaging and physiological methods dysfunction, and can be used as a guideline
and others social behaviours. (such as galvanic skin responses). for designing future experiments.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 | O CTOBER 2005 | 807


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

Moral cognitive neuroscience can improve 18. Augstein, H. F. J C Prichards concept of moral insanity 45. Heekeren, H. R., Wartenburger, I., Schmidt, H.,
a medical theory of the corruption of human nature. Schwintowski, H. P. & Villringer, A. An fMRI study of
assessment, prediction and treatment of Med. Hist. 40, 311343 (1996). simple ethical decision-making. Neuroreport 14,
behavioural disorders. Understanding the 19. Prichard, J. C. in The Cyclopaedia of Practical Medicine 12151219 (2003).
(eds Forbes, J., Tweedie, A. & Conolly, J.) 1032; 847875 46. Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E.,
neural basis of moral cognition will help (Sherwood, Gilbert and Piper, London, 18331835). Darley, J. M. & Cohen, J. D. An fMRI investigation of
to shape environmental, psychological and 20. Welt, L. Ueber charackterveranderungen des menschen. emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293,
Dtsch Arch. Klin. Med. 42, 339390 (1888). 21052108 (2001).
medical intervention aimed at promoting 21. Macmillan, M. An Odd Kind of Fame: Stories of Phineas 47. Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M.
prosocial behaviours and social welfare. Gage (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA, & Cohen, J. D. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and
2000). control in moral judgment. Neuron 44, 389400 (2004).
Future studies will be needed to explore 22. Grafman, J. et al. Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and 48. Moll, J. et al. The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a
the neural basis of how different individu- aggression: a report of the Vietnam Head Injury Study. functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of
Neurology 46, 12311238 (1996). basic and moral emotions. J. Neurosci. 22, 27302736
als and social groups make use of strategies 23. Eslinger, P. J. & Damasio, A. R. Severe disturbance of (2002).
and heuristics to solve moral conflicts. The higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: 49. Shin, L. M. et al. Activation of anterior paralimbic
patient EVR. Neurology 35, 17311741 (1985). structures during guilt-related script-driven imagery. Biol.
implications of this new knowledge for how 24. Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Psychiatry 48, 4350 (2000).
societies conduct business, regulate social Damasio, A. R. Impairment of social and moral behavior 50. Takahashi, H. et al. Brain activation associated with
related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: an
behaviour and plan for their futures remain Nature Neurosci. 2, 10321037 (1999). fMRI study. Neuroimage 23, 967974 (2004).
to be seen. 25. Eslinger, P. J., Grattan, L. M., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A. R. 51. Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J. & Dolan, R. J. An
Developmental consequences of childhood frontal lobe fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing)
Jorge Moll, Roland Zahn, damage. Arch. Neurol. 49, 764769 (1992). violations of social norms. Brain 125, 16961708 (2002).
Frank Krueger and Jordan Grafman are at The 26. Cleckley, H. The Mask of Sanity (CV Mosby, St Louis, 52. Dougherty, D. D. et al. Anger in healthy men: a PET study
Missouri, USA, 1964). using script-driven imagery. Biol. Psychiatry 46, 466472
Cognitive Neuroscience Section, National Institute 27. Hare, R. D. Psychopathy: Theory and Research (John (1999).
of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, Building 10; Wiley, New York, USA, 1970). 53. Heekeren, H. R. et al. Influence of bodily harm on neural
Room 5C205; MSC 1440, NIH, Bethesda, 28. Miller, B. L., Chang, L., Mena, I., Boone, K. & Lesser, I. M. correlates of semantic and moral decision-making.
Maryland 20892-1440, USA. Progressive right frontotemporal degeneration: clinical, Neuroimage 24, 887897 (2005).
neuropsychological and SPECT characteristics. 54. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of
Ricardo de Oliveira-Souza is at the Cognitive and Dementia 4, 204213 (1993). prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24,
Behavioral Neuroscience Unit, LABS-DOr 29. Perry, R. J. et al. Hemispheric dominance for emotions, 167202 (2001).
empathy and social behaviour: evidence from right and 55. Fellows, L. K. & Farah, M. J. Is anterior cingulate cortex
Hospital Network, R. Pinheiro Guimaraes 22, left handers with frontotemporal dementia. Neurocase 7, necessary for cognitive control? Brain 128, 788796
3rd floor, Rio de Janeiro 22281-080, Brazil. 145160 (2001). (2005).
30. Tranel, D., Bechara, A. & Denburg, N. L. Asymmetric 56. Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D. & Damasio, H. Individuals with
Correspondence to J.G. functional roles of right and left ventromedial prefrontal sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to
e-mail: grafmanj@ninds.nih.gov cortices in social conduct, decision-making, and respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behav. Brain
emotional processing. Cortex 38, 589612 (2002). Res. 41, 8194 (1990).
doi:1038/nrn1768
31. Eslinger, P. J. Adolescent neuropsychological 57. Newman, J. P., Patterson, C. M. & Kosson, D. S.
development after early right prefrontal cortex damage. Response perseveration in psychopaths. J. Abnorm.
1. Blakemore, S.-J., Winston, J. & Frith, U. Social cognitive Dev. Neuropsychol. 18, 297329 (2001). Psychol. 96, 145148 (1987).
neuroscience: where are we heading? Trends Cogn. Sci. 32. Kruesi, M. J., Casanova, M. F., Mannheim, G. & 58. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A. R.
8, 216222 (2004). Johnson-Bilder, A. Reduced temporal lobe volume in early Deciding advantageously before knowing the
2. Wood, J. N. Social cognition and the prefrontal cortex. onset conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res. 132, 111 advantageous strategy. Science 275, 12931295
Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2, 97114 (2003). (2004). (1997).
3. Adolphs, R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social 33. Allison, T., Puce, A. & McCarthy, G. Social perception 59. Bechara, A., Tranel, D. & Damasio, H. Characterization
behaviour. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165178 (2003). from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn. of the decision-making deficit of patients with
4. MacIntyre, A. After Virtue (Duckworth, London, 1985). Sci. 4, 267278 (2000). ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain 123,
5. Tranel, D. Acquired sociopathy: the development of 34. Frith, C. D. & Frith, U. Interacting minds a biological 21892202 (2000).
sociopathic behavior following focal brain damage. Prog. basis. Science 286, 16921695 (1999). 60. Zahn, T. P., Grafman, J. & Tranel, D. Frontal lobe lesions
Exp. Pers. Psychopathol. Res. 285311 (1994). 35. Burns, J. M. & Swerdlow, R. H. Right orbitofrontal tumor and electrodermal activity: effects of significance.
6. Casebeer, W. D. Moral cognition and its neural with pedophilia symptom and constructional apraxia Neuropsychologia 37, 12271241 (1999).
constituents. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 840846 sign. Arch. Neurol. 60, 437440 (2003). 61. Maia, T. V. & McClelland, J. L. A reexamination of the
(2003). 36. Weissenberger, A. A. et al. Aggression and psychiatric evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: what
7. Greene, J. From neural is to moral ought: what are the comorbidity in children with hypothalamic hamartomas participants really know in the Iowa gambling task. Proc.
moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology? and their unaffected siblings. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 1607516080 (2004).
Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 846849 (2003). Psychiatry 40, 696703 (2001). 62. Blair, R. J. & Cipolotti, L. Impaired social response
8. Casebeer, W. D. Natural Ethical Facts: Evolution, 37. Muller, J. L. et al. Abnormalities in emotion processing reversal. A case of acquired sociopathy. Brain 123,
Connectionism, and Moral Cognition (MIT Press, within cortical and subcortical regions in criminal 11221141 (2000).
Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA, 2003). psychopaths: evidence from a functional magnetic 63. Fellows, L. K. & Farah, M. J. Different underlying
9. Schulkin, J. Roots of Social Sensitivity and Neural resonance imaging study using pictures with emotional impairments in decision-making following ventromedial
Function (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA, content. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 152162 (2003). and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. Cereb.
2000). 38. Soderstrom, H. et al. Reduced frontotemporal perfusion Cortex 15, 5863 (2005).
10. Hauser, M. D., Chen, M. K., Chen, F. & Chuang, E. Give in psychopathic personality. Psychiatry Res 114, 8194 64. Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., Wade, D. & McGrath, J. Emotion-
unto others: genetically unrelated cotton-top tamarin (2002). related learning in patients with social and emotional
monkeys preferentially give food to those who 39. Kiehl, K. A. et al. Limbic abnormalities in affective changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J. Neurol.
altruistically give food back. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270, processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by Neurosurg. Psychiatry 57, 15181524 (1994).
23632370 (2003). functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biol. Psychiatry 65. Rolls, E. T. The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cereb.
11. de Waal, F. B. M. Tree of Origin: What Primate Behavior 50, 677684 (2001). Cortex 10, 284294 (2000).
Can Tell Us About Human Social Evolution (Harvard Univ. 40. Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L. & Colletti, P. 66. Kringelbach, M. L. & Rolls, E. T. Neural correlates of rapid
Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA, 2001). Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced reversal learning in a simple model of human social
12. Allman, J., Hakeem, A. & Watson, K. Two phylogenetic autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Arch. interaction. Neuroimage 20, 13711383 (2003).
specializations in the human brain. Neuroscientist 8, Gen. Psychiatry 57, 119127; discussion 128129 67. ODoherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J.
335346 (2002). (2000). & Andrews, C. Abstract reward and punishment
13. Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R. & Eslinger, P. J. Morals and 41. Beer, J. S., Heerey, E. A., Keltner, D., Scabini, D. & representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature
the human brain: a working model. Neuroreport 14, Knight, R. T. The regulatory function of self-conscious Neurosci. 4, 95102 (2001).
299305 (2003). emotion: insights from patients with orbitofrontal 68. Hornak, J. et al. Reward-related reversal learning after
14. Wood, J. N. & Grafman, J. Human prefrontal cortex: damage. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 594604 (2003). surgical excisions in orbito-frontal or dorsolateral
processing and representational perspectives. Nature 42. Camille, N. et al. The involvement of the orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16,
Rev. Neurosci. 4, 139147 (2003). cortex in the experience of regret. Science 304, 463478 (2004).
15. Grafman, J. Similarities and distinctions among current 11671170 (2004). 69. Blair, R. J. Neurocognitive models of aggression, the
models of prefrontal cortical functions. Ann. NY Acad. 43. Moll, J., Eslinger, P. J. & Oliveira-Souza, R. Frontopolar antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy.
Sci. 769, 337368 (1995). and anterior temporal cortex activation in a moral J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 71, 727731 (2001).
16. Mithen, S. The Prehistory of the Mind: The Cognitive judgment task: preliminary functional MRI results in 70. Blair, R. J. The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the
Origins of Art, Religion and Science (Thames and normal subjects. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 59, 657664 modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain Cogn. 55,
Hudson, London, 1996). (2001). 198208 (2004).
17. Altschuler, E. L., Haroun, A., Ho, B. & Weimer, A. Did 44. Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E. & Grafman, J. 71. Adolphs, R., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A. R. The human
Samson have antisocial personality disorder? Arch. Gen. Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral amygdala in social judgment. Nature 393, 470474
Psychiatry 58, 202203 (2001). social judgments. Neuroimage 16, 696703 (2002). (1998).

808 | O CTOBER 2005 | VOLUME 6 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


2005 Nature Publishing Group
PERSPECTIVES

72. Miller, B. L., Darby, A., Benson, D. F., Cummings, J. L. & 99. Stone, V. E., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Kroll, N. & 128. Haugh, R. M. & Markesbery, W. R. Hypothalamic
Miller, M. H. Aggressive, socially disruptive and antisocial Knight, R. T. Selective impairment of reasoning about astrocytoma. Syndrome of hyperphagia, obesity, and
behaviour associated with fronto-temporal dementia. Br. social exchange in a patient with bilateral limbic system disturbances of behavior and endocrine and autonomic
J. Psychiatry 170, 150154 (1997). damage. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 1153111536 function. Arch. Neurol. 40, 560563 (1983).
73. Rankin, K. P. et al. Right and left medial orbitofrontal (2002). 129. Bejjani, B. P. et al. Aggressive behavior induced by
volumes show an opposite relationship to agreeableness 100. Hornak, J. et al. Changes in emotion after circumscribed intraoperative stimulation in the triangle of Sano.
in FTD. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 17, 328332 surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Neurology 59, 14251427 (2002).
(2004). Brain 126, 16911712 (2003). 130. Brodal, P. The Central Nervous System: Structure and
74. Mendez, M. F., Chow, T., Ringman, J., Twitchell, G. & 101. Wilkinson, D. & Halligan, P. The relevance of behavioural Function (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, USA, 2003).
Hinkin, C. H. Pedophilia and temporal lobe disturbances. measures for functional-imaging studies of cognition. 131. Haidt, J. in Handbook of Affective Sciences
J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 12, 7176 (2000). Nature Rev. Neurosci. 5, 6773 (2004). (eds Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R. & Goldsmith, H. H.)
75. Bozeat, S., Gregory, C. A., Ralph, M. A. & Hodges, J. R. 102. Nisbett, R. E. & Masuda, T. Culture and point of view. 852870 (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, USA, 2003).
Which neuropsychiatric and behavioural features Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 1116311170 (2003). 132. Tangney, J. P. in Self and Motivation: Emerging
distinguish frontal and temporal variants of 103. Ehrlich, P. R. Human Natures: Genes, Cultures, and the Psychological Perspectives (eds Tesser, A., Stapel, D. A.
frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimers disease? Human Prospect (Island, Washington DC, USA, 2000). & Wood, J. V.) 97117 (American Psychological
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 69, 178186 (2000). 104. Nichols, S. Norms with feeling: towards a psychological Association, Washington DC, USA, 2002).
76. Lough, S., Gregory, C. & Hodges, J. R. Dissociation of account of moral judgment. Cognition 84, 221236 133. Fessler, D. in Beyond Nature or Nurture: Biocultural
social cognition and executive function in frontal variant (2002). Approaches to the Emotions (ed. Hinton, A.) 75116
frontotemporal dementia. Neurocase 7, 123130 (2001). 105. Fehr, E. & Fischbacher, U. Social norms and human (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, USA, 1999).
77. Baron-Cohen, S. Out of sight or out of mind? Another cooperation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 185190 (2004). 134. Haidt, J. The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social
look at deception in autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 106. Grattan, L. M. & Eslinger, P. J. Long-term psychological intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol. Rev.
33, 11411155 (1992). consequences of childhood frontal lobe lesion in patient 108, 814834 (2001).
78. Richell, R. A. et al. Theory of mind and psychopathy: can DT. Brain Cogn. 20, 185195 (1992). 135. Cunningham, W. A., Nezlek, J. B. & Banaji, M. R. Implicit
psychopathic individuals read the language of the eyes? 107. Eslinger, P. J., Flaherty-Craig, C. V. & Benton, A. L. and explicit ethnocentrism: revisiting the ideologies of
Neuropsychologia 41, 523526 (2003). Developmental outcomes after early prefrontal cortex prejudice. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 13321346
79. Ruchkin, D. S., Grafman, J., Cameron, K. & Berndt, R. S. damage. Brain Cogn. 55, 84103 (2004). (2004).
Working memory retention systems: a state of activated 108. Weingartner, H., Grafman, J., Boutelle, W., Kaye, W. & 136. Schultz, W. Getting formal with dopamine and reward.
long-term memory. Behav. Brain Sci. 26, 709728; Martin, P. R. Forms of memory failure. Science 221, Neuron 36, 241263 (2002).
discussion 728777 (2003). 380382 (1983). 137. McClure, S. M., Daw, N. D. & Montague, P. R.
80. Wood, J. N., Romero, S. G., Knutson, K. M. & Grafman, J. 109. Singer, W. Consciousness and the binding problem. Ann. A computational substrate for incentive salience. Trends
Representation of attitudinal knowledge: role of prefrontal NY Acad. Sci. 929, 123146 (2001). Neurosci. 26, 423428 (2003).
cortex, amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus. 110. OReilly, R. C. & Rudy, J. W. Computational principles of 138. Schultz, W. & Dickinson, A. Neuronal coding of prediction
Neuropsychologia 43, 249259 (2005). learning in the neocortex and hippocampus. errors. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 473500 (2000).
81. Wood, J. N., Romero, S. G., Makale, M. & Grafman, J. Hippocampus 10, 389397 (2000). 139. Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. Prospect theory: an analysis
Category-specific representations of social and nonsocial 111. Okuda, J. et al. Thinking of the future and past: the roles of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263291
knowledge in the human prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. (1979).
Neurosci. 15, 236248 (2003). Neuroimage 19, 13691380 (2003). 140. Fukatsu, R., Fujii, T., Yamadori, A., Nagasawa, H. &
82. Mah, L. W., Arnold, M. C. & Grafman, J. Deficits in social 112. Eslinger, P. J. & Grattan, L. M. Altered serial position Sakurai, Y. Persisting childish behavior after bilateral
knowledge following damage to ventromedial prefrontal learning after frontal lobe lesion. Neuropsychologia 32, thalamic infarcts. Eur. Neurol. 37, 230235 (1997).
cortex. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 17, 6674 729739 (1994). 141. Murphy, J. M. Psychiatric labeling in cross-cultural
(2005). 113. Goel, V., Grafman, J., Tajik, J., Gana, S. & Danto, D. perspective. Science 191, 10191028 (1976).
83. Mah, L., Arnold, M. C. & Grafman, J. Impairment of social A study of the performance of patients with frontal lobe 142. Henrich, J. et al. (eds) Foundations of Human Sociality
perception associated with lesions of the prefrontal lesions in a financial planning task. Brain 120, 18051822 (Oxford Univ. Press, London, UK, 2004).
cortex. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 12471255 (2004). (1997). 143. Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E.
84. Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S. & 114. Ramnani, N. & Owen, A. M. Anterior prefrontal cortex: & Cohen, J. D. The neural correlates of theory of mind
Grafman, J. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. within interpersonal interactions. Neuroimage 22,
human cognition. Nature 399, 148151 (1999). Nature Rev. Neurosci. 5, 184194 (2004). 16941703 (2004).
85. Koechlin, E., Corrado, G., Pietrini, P. & Grafman, J. 115. Milne, E. & Grafman, J. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex 144. Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E.
Dissociating the role of the medial and lateral anterior lesions in humans eliminate implicit gender stereotyping. & Cohen, J. D. The neural basis of economic decision-
prefrontal cortex in human planning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. J. Neurosci. 21, RC150 (2001). making in the Ultimatum Game. Science 300,
USA 97, 76517656 (2000). 116. Pietrini, P., Guazzelli, M., Basso, G., Jaffe, K. & Grafman, J. 17551758 (2003).
86. Knutson, K. M., Wood, J. N. & Grafman, J. Brain Neural correlates of imaginal aggressive behavior 145. de Quervain, D. J. et al. The neural basis of altruistic
activation in processing temporal sequence: an fMRI assessed by positron emission tomography in healthy punishment. Science 305, 12541258 (2004).
study. Neuroimage 23, 12991307 (2004). subjects. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 17721781 (2000). 146. Paciotti, B., Hadley, C., Holmes, C. & Mulder, M. B.
87. Wood, J. N., Knutson, K. M. & Grafman, J. Psychological 117. Cunningham, W. A., Raye, C. L. & Johnson, M. K. Grass-roots justice in Tanzania: cultural evolution and
structure and neural correlates of event knowledge. Implicit and explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of game theory help to explain how a history of cooperation
Cereb. Cortex (2004). valence, emotional intensity, and control in the influences the success of social organizations.
88. Tanaka, S. C. et al. Prediction of immediate and future processing of attitudes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, Am. Scientist 93, 5865 (2005).
rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. 17171729 (2004). 147. University of Iowas Virtual Hospital [online] <http://www.
Nature Neurosci. 7, 887893 (2004). 118. McClelland, J. L. & Rogers, T. T. The parallel distributed vh.org/> (2005).
89. Thomson, J. J. Rights, Restitution, and Risk: Essays, processing approach to semantic cognition. Nature Rev. 148. Martin, J. H. Neuroanatomy: Text and Atlas 2nd edn
in Moral Theory (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Neurosci. 4, 310322 (2003). (Appleton & Lange, Stamford, Connecticut, 1996).
Massachusetts, USA, 1986). 119. Martin, A. & Chao, L. L. Semantic memory and the brain: 149. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M. & Cuthbert, B. N. International
90. Fiddick, L., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. No interpretation structure and processes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, affective picture system (IAPS): digitized photographs,
without representation: the role of domain-specific 194201 (2001). instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical Report
representations and inferences in the Wason selection 120. Caramazza, A. & Mahon, B. Z. The organization of A-6. (Univ. Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA, 2005).
task. Cognition 77, 179 (2000). conceptual knowledge: the evidence from category- 150. Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F. & Liberzon, I.
91. Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. Dissecting the computational specific semantic deficits. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 354361 Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of
architecture of social inference mechanisms. Ciba Found. (2003). emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage
Symp. 208, 132156; discussion 156161 (1997). 121. Frith, U. Mind blindness and the brain in autism. Neuron 16, 331348 (2002).
92. Goel, V. & Dolan, R. J. Explaining modulation of reasoning 32, 969979 (2001). 151. Moll, J. et al. The moral affiliations of disgust: a functional
by belief. Cognition 87, B11B22 (2003). 122. Boddaert, N. et al. Superior temporal sulcus anatomical MRI study. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 18, 6878 (2005).
93. Goel, V. & Dolan, R. J. Reciprocal neural response within abnormalities in childhood autism: a voxel-based
lateral and ventral medial prefrontal cortex during hot and morphometry MRI study. Neuroimage 23, 364369 Acknowledgements
cold reasoning. Neuroimage 20, 23142321 (2003). (2004). This research was partially supported by the LABS-DOr Hospital
94. Wason, P. C. Reasoning about a rule. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 123. Hodges, J. R., Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Network and by the Intramural Research Program of the National
20, 273281 (1968). Patterson, K. & Spatt, J. The role of conceptual Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes
95. Fuster, J. M. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, knowledge in object use evidence from semantic of Health.
and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe (Raven, New dementia. Brain 123, 19131925 (2000).
York, USA, 1997). 124. Lu, L. H. et al. Category-specific naming deficits for Competing interests statement
96. MacLean, P. A Triune Concept of the Brain and objects and actions: semantic attribute and grammatical The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Behaviour: Hincks Memorial Lecture (Univ. Toronto Press, role hypotheses. Neuropsychologia 40, 16081621
Toronto, Canada, 1973). (2002).
97. Eslinger, P. J. & Geder, L. in Behavior and Mood 125. Kiehl, K. A. et al. Temporal lobe abnormalities in semantic Online links
Disorders in Focal Frontal Lobe Lesions processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by
(eds Bogousslavsky, J. & Cummings, J. L.) 217260 functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Res. FURTHER INFORMATION
(Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 130, 2742 (2004). Cognitive Neuroscience Section, NINDS, NIH: http://intra.
USA, 2000). 126. Saper, C. B. Hypothalamic connections with the cerebral ninds.nih.gov/Lab.asp?Org_ID=83
98. Fiddick, L. Domains of deontic reasoning: resolving the cortex. Prog. Brain Res. 126, 3948 (2000). Cognitive and Behavioural Neuroscience Unit, LABS-DOr
discrepancy between the cognitive and moral reasoning 127. Stellar, E. The physiology of motivation. 1954. Psychol. Hospital Network: http://www.rededor.com.br/cbnu/
literatures. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 57, 447474 (2004). Rev. 101, 301311 (1994). Access to this interactive links box is free online.

NATURE REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 | O CTOBER 2005 | 809


2005 Nature Publishing Group

Potrebbero piacerti anche