Sei sulla pagina 1di 15

Chapter |4|

The psychology of pain: models and targets


for comprehensive assessment
Gordon J.G. Asmundson, Lydia Gomez-Perez, Ashley A. Richter and R. Nicholas Carleton

important features of a historical model that provided the


CHAPTER CONTENTS
foundation for contemporary biopsychosocial approaches
Overview 35 to understanding pain, selectively highlight important
Pain and chronic pain defined 35 cognitive constructs and pain behaviours as well as envir-
onmental influences, and conclude with a summary of
Models pertinent to understanding pain 36
important considerations in assessment planning and
Cognitive constructs 37 case conceptualization. This approach is predicated on
Behavioural constructs 39 the position that assessment and case conceptualization
Environmental influences 41 should be a conceptually driven process (Asmundson &
Hadjistavropoulos 2006; Taylor & Asmundson 2004); as
Key considerations in assessment and case
conceptualization 42 such, empirically supported theoretical constructs and
applications form the foundation upon which assessment,
Conclusion 43 case conceptualization and subsequent treatment plans
are built. Throughout we refer to current empirical findings
LEARNING OBJECTIVES and, as appropriate, incorporate examples to illustrate
salient points.
On completing this chapter readers will have an
understanding of the following:
1. The distinction between pain and chronic pain.
PAIN AND CHRONIC PAIN DEFINED
2. Gate control theory.
3. Biopsychosocial models of pain.
The traditional biomedical model of pain dates back to
4. Cognitive constructs associated with beliefs, mood, Descartes (15961650), who suggested that pain is a
anxiety and fear. sensory experience resulting from stimulation of specific
5. Behavioural constructs related to avoidance, activity noxious receptors, usually from physical damage due to
limitation, coping behaviour, pain and suicide. injury or disease. Consistent with Cartesian dualism (i.e.
6. Environmental influences of family, culture and the idea that mind and body are non-overlapping entities),
ethnicity, socioeconomic factors and work. the model is both reductionistic (i.e. all disease is directly
linked to specific physical pathology) and exclusionary
(i.e. social, psychological, behavioural mechanisms of
OVERVIEW illness are not of primary importance). Applications
of the traditional biomedical model to diagnosis, assess-
In this chapter we review the current state of knowledge ment and treatment are generally straightforward. Physical
regarding the psychology of pain as it can be applied pathology would be confirmed by data from objective tests
to assessment planning and case conceptualization. We of physical damage, and interventions would then be
provide definitions of pain and chronic pain, summarize directed toward rectifying the damage and associated

2014 Elsevier Ltd.


35
Section | 1 | Overview: what is pain?

limitations in physical functioning. The experience of pain 1965) are frequently cited as the first to integrate biological
would not be viewed as significant but, rather, as a second- and psychological mechanisms of pain within the context
ary reaction to or symptom of physical damage that would of a single model. Melzack & Wall (1965) suggested that
diminish with healing. the passage of ascending nociceptive (pain) information
Pain is now understood to involve more than a pure sen- from the body to the brain was controlled by a hypothetical
sory experience arising from physical injury or other gating mechanism within the dorsal horn of the spinal
pathology. Over the past half century a number of pain cord. The gating mechanism works as follows. Excitation
models that incorporate biological as well as psychological along the large-diameter, myelinated fibres of the spinal
(e.g. perception, cognition, affect), behavioural (e.g. avoid- cord closes the gate, whereas excitation along the small-
ance) and social (e.g. cultural) factors have been proposed. diameter, unmyelinated fibres opens the gate. Transmis-
These models, described in more detail below, have sions about current cognition and mood descending from
dramatically improved the understanding of pain and the the brain to the gating mechanism also influence whether
ability to assess and effectively intervene in cases where it the gate is closed or opened. In short, the summation of
is intense, unremitting or both intense and unremitting. information travelling along the different types of ascend-
Pain is currently defined as a complex perceptual phenom- ing fibres from the body with that travelling on descending
enon that involves a number of dimensions, including, but fibres from the brain determines whether the gate is
not limited to, intensity, quality, time course and personal open or closed and, thereby, influences the perception of
meaning (Merskey & Bogduk 1994). It is adaptive in the pain. Melzack & Casey (1968) further proposed that three
short term, facilitating the ability to identify, respond to different neural networks (i.e. sensorydiscriminative,
and resolve physical pathology or injury. Unfortunately, a motivationalaffective and cognitiveevaluative) influence
significant number of people experience pain for periods that the modulation of sensory input. The addition of these net-
substantially exceed expected times for physical healing (e.g. works to the model allowed for perceptual information
Waddell 1987). When pain persists for 3 months or longer, it regarding the location, magnitude, and spatiotemporal
is considered chronic (e.g. International Association for the properties of the noxious stimulus, motivational tendency
Study of Pain (IASP) 1986) and, while not necessarily mal- toward escape or attack, and cognitive information based
adaptive (Asmundson et al 1998; Turk & Rudy 1987), often on analysis of multimodal information, past experience,
leads to physical decline, limited functional ability and emo- and probability of outcome of different response strategies
tional distress. These pain experiences are also associated (pp 427428).
with an increased probability of experiencing comorbid Time and empirical effort have led to advances in under-
psychopathology (McWilliams et al 2003; for review, see standing the anatomy and structure of the gating mechanism
Asmundson & Katz 2009), inappropriate use of medical ser- proposed by Melzack and Wall (Price 2000; Wall 1996),
vices, reduced work performance or absenteeism, and high as well as to elaborations of the neural network model
cost insurance claims (Spengler et al 1986; Stewart et al (Melzack 1999; Melzack & Katz 2004; please refer to
2003). Translated into a dollar value, common chronic pain Chapter 6). Notwithstanding, the gate control theory chal-
conditions cost the USA and Canada, respectively, approxi- lenged the primary assumptions of the traditional biomed-
mately US$60 billion and CAN$6 billion dollars annually. ical models. Rather than being exclusively conceptualized
as sensation arising from physical damage, the experience
of pain came to be viewed as a combination of both patho-
MODELS PERTINENT TO physiology and psychological factors. As such, pain as well
UNDERSTANDING PAIN as pain-related cognitions and mood were no longer viewed
as secondary reactions to physical damage. Instead, the path-
ophysiology of pain was conceptualized as having a recipro-
There are several contemporary pain models that are impor- cal influence on cognitions and mood, and vice versa. It is
tant in the context of assessment and case conceptualiza- this foundation from which contemporary biopsychosocial
tion. These models are similar in that they all recognize models emerged.
the interplay between biological, psychological and socio-
cultural factors in the pain experience. Below we highlight
one model of considerable historical significance as well Biopsychosocial models
as primary features of various contemporary biopsychoso- The biopsychosocial approach holds that the experience of
cial models. Detailed descriptions of the social influences pain is determined by the interaction between biological,
on pain are provided by Craig in this volume (Chapter 3). psychological (e.g. cognition, behaviour, mood) and social al
number of specific biopsychosocial
(e.g. cultural) factors. A n
models have been proposed over the past 35 years, includ-
Gate control theory
ing several behaviourally based models (Fordyce 1976;
Melzack and colleagues seminal papers on the gate control Fordyce et al 1982), as well as the more comprehensive cog-
theory of pain (Melzack & Casey 1968; Melzack & Wall nitivebehavioural Glasgow (Waddell 1987; Waddell et al

36
The psychology of pain: models and targets for comprehensive assessment Chapter |4|

1984) and biobehavioural (Turk 2002; Turk & Flor 1999; (2000), and illustrated in Figure 4.1. This model can be
Turk et al 1983) models. Detailed descriptions of these summarized as follows. On perceiving pain, people make
models of pain are provided elsewhere (e.g. Asmundson an appraisal of the meaning or purpose of the pain (pain
et al 2004a; Asmundson & Wright 2004). experience). Most people appraise the pain to be unpleas-
Despite differences with respect to specificity regarding ant and discomforting but not indicative of serious threat
behavioural and cognitive influences on pain, all of the to their well-being (no fear). These people then engage
biopsychosocial models share a common focus the in appropriate behavioural restriction and graduated
focus is not on disease per se but on illness, where illness increases in activity (confrontation) until they have healed
is viewed as a type of behaviour (Parsons 1951). The con- (recovery). However, some people appraise the pain as
cept of illness behaviour (Mechanic 1962) implies that being indicative of a serious threat to their well-being (pain
individuals may differ in perception of and response to catastrophizing) and, influenced by predispositional and
bodily sensations and changes (e.g. pain, nausea, heart current psychological factors, spiral into a self-perpetuating
palpitations), and that these differences can be under- cycle characterized by fear (i.e. fear of pain, fear of (re)
stood in the context of psychological and social processes. injury), activity limitations, disability and pain. It is this
For example, whereas one person may perceive pain as latter group of people who experience pain that lasts
indicative of a potentially serious malady and solicit reas- beyond the time expected for normal healing.
surances or assistance from others in even the most rudi- This model has served as a useful platform upon which
mentary of daily activities, another person may perceive considerable amounts of empirical research and related
similar pain as discomforting but harmless and work practical applications have been based (for review of find-
through the discomfort. Illness behaviour is considered ings current to the beginning of this millennium, see
a dynamic process, with the role of biological, psycholog- Vlaeyen & Linton 2000; for review of more recent findings,
ical and social factors changing in relative importance as see Leeuw et al 2007; for review of empirically supported
the condition evolves. While a condition may be initiated treatment options, see Bailey et al 2010). In addition to
by biological factors, such as physical injury or pathology, advances in empirical findings and practical applications
the psychological and social factors may come to play a to this field of inquiry, there has been continuing refine-
primary role in maintenance and exacerbation; indeed, ment of the contemporary fear-avoidance model (e.g.
the focus often shifts from pain to significant concern Asmundson et al 2004a; Norton & Asmundson 2003).
and anxiety about personal health and well-being (see,
for example, Hadjistavropoulos et al 2001).
This focus is also shared in the more recent fear Summary
avoidance models of chronic pain (Asmundson et al
The models described above provide comprehensive con-
1999, 2004a; Vlaeyen & Linton 2000), wherein pain-
ceptualizations of pain and chronic pain, with varying
related fear and anxiety are suggested to play primary roles
degrees of emphasis on cognitive, behavioural and envi-
in the development and maintenance of disabling chronic
ronmental constructs. It is these constructs that form the
pain. The fearavoidance models are predicated on long-
basis of conceptually driven assessment and treatment
standing observations that fear and anxiety appear to be
planning. Below we provide an overview of some of the
critical elements in the pain experience. Indeed, the associ-
most important cognitive, behavioural and environmental
ation between fear and pain dates back to at least as early as
constructs and how these are assessed.
Aristotle, who, over 2000 years ago, stated Let fear, then, be
a kind of pain or disturbance resulting the imagination of
impending danger, either destructive or painful, and is an
important consideration in each of the biopsychosocial COGNITIVE CONSTRUCTS
models described above. Since fearavoidance models
have stimulated some of the most recent developments
Beliefs
with respect to assessment and case conceptualization for
individuals who have pain lasting beyond the time typical Pain beliefs are a broad category of ideations, often cata-
for physical healing, we provide a more detailed descrip- strophic in nature, held by all people, not just those with
tion here. chronic pain (van Damme et al 2002; Sullivan et al
Contemporary fear-avoidance models of chronic pain 2001). An individual forms pain beliefs, which are rela-
are based primarily on the writings of several scholars tively stable across time, based on past experiences and cul-
(Asmundson et al 1999; McCracken et al 1992; Vlaeyen tural norms (Turner et al 2004; Werner et al 2005). There
et al 1995; Waddell et al 1993), each of whom provided have been several tools designed to measure pain beliefs
slightly different conceptualizations of the role of fear, anx- (e.g. Edwards et al 1992; Waddell et al 1993; Wallston
iety, and avoidance in perpetuating pain. Subtle differences et al 1978; Williams & Thorn 1989), any of which can
aside, the primary postulates of each of these scholars are be used to provide a reliable and valid assessment of the
captured in the model proposed by Vlaeyen & Linton nature of a persons beliefs about pain.

37
Section | 1 | Overview: what is pain?

Injury

Disuse
Depression Recovery
Disability

Avoidance
hypervigilance

Confrontation
Pain experience

Pain-related
fear

Pain No fear
catastrophizing

Negative affectivity
Threatening illness information

Fig. 4.1 Fear-avoidance model.


Reprinted from Vlaeyan, J.W.S., and Linton, S.J., Fear avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. PAIN 2000,
April 85(3); 317-332. This figure has been reproduced with permission of the International Association for the study of Pain (IASP). The figure may
not be reproduced for any other purpose without permision.

Pain beliefs are commonly divided into two subcate- 2010; de Jong et al 2005; Jensen et al 2001; Walsh &
gories, organic and psychological (Edwards et al 1992). Radcliffe 2002).
Organic pain beliefs centre on the notion that pain indi-
cates immediate or imminent physical harm (e.g. Pain is
Mood
the result of damage to the tissues of my body). In contrast,
psychological pain beliefs centre on the notion that pain is Depression symptoms have been associated with increased
mediated by internal and external factors (e.g. Thinking pain behaviour (e.g. guarding, bracing, rubbing, grimacing,
about pain makes it worse). Pain beliefs have been associ- sighing), reduced individual, social and occupation activ-
ated uniquely and independently with disability and ity, as well as increased use of medical services (Arnow
depression (Turner et al 2000). et al 2009; Asmundson et al 2008; Keefe et al 1986;
There is more than two decades of evidence suggesting Ratcliffe et al 2008; Smith et al 1998; Worz 2003). In
a relationship between organic pain beliefs and physical chronic pain samples, prevalence rates of clinically signifi-
disability for patients with chronic pain (Jensen et al cant depression vary based on the criteria used for assess-
1991; Sloan et al 2007), but the relationship with pain ment (Geisser et al 1997; Pincus & Williams 1999), but
intensity has been minimal (Edwards et al 1992). In often exceed 28% (Morley et al 2002; Polatin et al 1993;
short, persons who have disabling chronic pain tend to Poole et al 2009; Worz 2003); in contrast, lifetime and
strongly endorse organic pain beliefs and are less likely 12-month community prevalence rates for depression are
than people without chronic pain to endorse psycholog- approximately 17% and 7%, respectively (Kessler et al
ical pain beliefs (Edwards et al 1992; Walsh & Radcliffe 2005a,b). Persons with one pain site are nearly twice as
2002). Because pain beliefs are learned (Turner et al likely to be depressed than persons with no pain, whereas
2004; van Damme et al 2002; Werner et al 2005), they persons with more than one pain site are nearly four times
are also modifiable. Increasing a persons belief that as likely to be depressed (Gureje et al 2008). In addition,
pain involves a significant psychological component the longer a person is in pain, or the more intense the pain,
tends to be associated with decreases in disability and, the more likely the person is to experience depressive symp-
often, reports of reduced pain intensity (Bailey et al toms (Krause et al 1994; Odegard et al 2007). Depressive

38
The psychology of pain: models and targets for comprehensive assessment Chapter |4|

symptoms do not necessarily impede treatment outcomes, painful reinjury (Kori et al 1990), or whether it is best
however, and treating depression itself may provide reduc- subsumed by a fear of somatic sensations and changes
tions in disabling pain symptoms (Glombiewski et al 2010; (Asmundson et al 1999; Greenberg & Burns 2003). Despite
Teh et al 2009). the potential utility of clearly distinguishing between pain-
Assessment of depression symptoms in persons with related anxiety and pain-related fear, additional empirical
chronic pain should be ongoing, as there is evidence that study is required; as such, in the current text, pain-related
pain is associated with fluctuations in mood over time anxiety will be used to refer to both constructs.
(Krause et al 1994; Odegard et al 2007). In addition to The structure of pain-related anxiety appears to be multi-
structured clinical interviews (e.g. First et al 1996), there dimensional (i.e. comprising cognitive, behavioural and
is a variety of standardized self-report tools that can mea- physiological components; McCracken et al 1993) and
sure ongoing depression symptoms (e.g. Beck et al 1988; continuous (i.e. occurring along a continuum ranging from
Radloff 1977). Given the probability of low mood resulting low to high; Asmundson et al 2007). Accordingly, there is
from ongoing pain, the high rates of comorbidity with also evidence that pain-related anxiety is ubiquitous in the
depression, and the associated personal and economic population (Carleton & Asmundson 2007; Carleton et al
costs, ongoing depression symptom assessment should 2009). There have been several tools designed to measure
occur for all patients with disabling chronic pain. pain-related anxiety (e.g. McCracken & Dhingra 2002;
McNeil & Rainwater 1998; Melzack 1983) and ongoing
assessment provides clinicians with information about a
key cognitive component in the maintenance of disabling
Anxiety and fear
chronic pain.
As with depression, persons with one pain site are nearly
twice as likely to have clinically significant levels of anxiety
compared to persons with no pain, whereas persons Spirituality
with more than one pain site are more than three times
Exploration of the relationship between religion, spiritual-
as likely to be anxious (Gureje et al 2008). Findings from
ity and chronic pain remains relatively novel. In a recent
community-dwelling adults in 17 countries (n 85,088)
review, religious and spiritual beliefs were found to repre-
indicate that those with back or neck pain are two to three
sent important components of the chronic pain experience
times more likely to have current (i.e. past 12 months)
(Rippentrop 2005; Unruh 2007). Many people with
panic disorder, agoraphobia or social anxiety disorder,
chronic pain report that religious and spiritual beliefs func-
and almost three times more likely to have generalized
tion as coping mechanisms that facilitate their continued
anxiety disorder (GAD) or post-traumatic stress disorder
activity (Bussing et al 2009), but the available evidence sug-
(PTSD) (Demyttenaere et al 2007). Data on lifetime prev-
gests that religious and spiritual beliefs do not directly
alence show similar patterns. For example, community-
impact pain intensity or pain interference (Rippentrop
dwelling women with fibromyalgia are four to five times
et al 2005). Research to date suggests most people with
more likely to have had a lifetime diagnosis of obses-
chronic pain reporting a religious affiliation neither feel
sivecompulsive disorder, PTSD or GAD than those with-
abandoned nor punished as a function of their religious
out (Raphael et al 2006). In people seeking treatment for
and spiritual beliefs, and that most report prayer to be an
pain, some but not all studies indicate elevated prevalence
important coping strategy (Glover-Graf et al 2007). While
of any current (2529%) and lifetime (28%) anxiety disor-
additional investigation into the impact of religious and
der relative to the general population (18%) (Kessler et al
spiritual beliefs on the pain experience is warranted, there
2005a,b), with specific elevations in the prevalence of cur-
is sufficient evidence to suggest that religious and spiritual
rent seasonal affective disorder, GAD, panic disorder and
beliefs be considered as a component of comprehensive
PTSD (for review, see Asmundson & Katz 2009).
assessment. Several general and well-established measures
Fear and anxiety related to pain are also pertinent issues.
of religious and spiritual beliefs are available (e.g.
The constructs of pain-related anxiety and pain-related fear
Cloninger et al 1994; Piedmont 1999) and more specific
are often used interchangeably, but there is theory and evi-
measures have been developed recently (e.g. Glover-Graf
dence that suggests the two are distinct (Carleton &
et al 2007).
Asmundson 2009). Specifically, pain-related anxiety repre-
sents a response to anticipated future encounters with pain-
related threats that drives avoidance behaviours; in con-
trast, pain-related fear represents a response to a current BEHAVIOURAL CONSTRUCTS
encounter with a pain-related threat that drives escape
behaviours. There are differing opinions about whether
Avoidance behaviour
pain-related fear is focused on painful sensations
(Lethem et al 1983; Vlaeyen & Linton 2000), activities asso- The cognitive constructs described above often serve to
ciated with those sensations (e.g. Waddell et al 1993) or influence the way in which a person with chronic pain

39
Section | 1 | Overview: what is pain?

behaves. Perhaps the most significant pain-related behaviour research suggests that the absence of compensation may
is that of avoidance (Asmundson et al 2004b). Indeed, avoid- be associated with better post-surgical outcomes (Atlas
ance behaviours are intrinsically appropriate components of et al 2010) and less healthcare utilization (Blyth et al
pain (Larsen et al 1997; McCracken et al 1992; Osman et al 2003); similarly, patients appear to recover faster post
1994). Relatively higher levels of pain-related anxiety facili- injury when compensation for pain and suffering is not
tate avoidance that, from the perspective of the person with available (Cassidy et al 2000). In any case, the historical
chronic pain, provides a protective function (Vlaeyen et al evidence suggests that chronic pain is associated with many
2004); in short, anticipated pain and additional harm are more losses than gains (Fishbain 1994).
skirted. The challenge associated with avoidance is that it does
serve a protective function as part of the recovery process, but
only in the short term. When anxiety drives avoidance Pain coping behaviours
beyond the time required for healing, its once protective func-
Individual beliefs, attitudes and appraisals pertaining to pain
tions become maladaptive (Asmundson et al 2004b). Mal-
impact pain perception and coping behaviours (Unruh 1996;
adaptive avoidance behaviours can be driven by pain-
Unruh et al 1999). When pain is perceived as challenging or
related beliefs (Waddell et al 1993), anxiety (McCracken &
threatening, pain behaviours often include information seek-
Dhingra 2002) or fear of re-injury (Kori et al 1990). Avoid-
ing, healthcare utilization, seeking social support, problem
ance behaviours have been associated with self-reported dis-
solving and increased or decreased use of distraction strate-
ability (Crombez et al 1999; McCracken & Gross 1995;
gies (for review, see Taylor & Asmundson 2004). Discussion
Osman et al 2001), non-specific physical complaints
of the various pain coping behaviours is beyond the scope of
(McCracken et al 1998) and reduced physical capacity
this chapter, but it is important it note that the effectiveness of
(Burns et al 2000). Consequently, assessment of avoidance
these various strategies is often dependent on situational and
behaviour warrants careful attention.
individual difference factors. To illustrate, research into the
effectiveness of cognitive control strategies, such as distrac-
tion or suppression of pain-related thoughts, has been equiv-
Activity limitation for secondary gain ocal (for review, see Asmundson et al 2010). Some studies
have supported the use of distraction, particularly for chil-
There is a lack of clarity associated with the terms malinger-
drens procedural or acute pain, but most indicate that direct-
ing and secondary gain (Craig et al 1999; Main 2003),
ing attention away from pain increases pain sensitivity and
particularly as clinicians attempt to reconcile both terms
pain-related disability. Directing attention towards the sen-
with exaggerations of legitimate symptoms as attempts to
sory quality of pain (i.e. somatic focus) may actually reduce
receive help (Rogers & Neumann 2003). Secondary gain
pain sensitivity, but the effect may depend on the sex of the
has been defined as acceptable or legitimate interpersonal
person in pain (Keogh et al 2000). The disparate findings also
advantages that result when one has the symptom of a
may relate to psychological individual difference variables,
physical disease (Fishbain 1994; Fishbain et al 1995). In
such that distraction is less effective in persons who catastro-
contrast, malingering is defined as the false or fraudulent
phize about pain (Goubert et al 2004; Hadjistavropoulos et al
exaggeration or simulation of physical or mental disease.
2000; Heyneman et al 1990; Roelofs et al 2004). These indi-
Secondary gains are necessary for malingering to occur,
viduals may instead benefit from increasing somatic focus
but secondary gains can occur independent of malingering
(Hadjistavropoulos et al 2000; Keogh & Mansoor 2001;
(Fishbain et al 2004).
Roelofs et al 2004).
Pain behaviours, particularly activity limitations, can
produce positive consequences, such as receiving attention,
decrease of responsibilities or economic compensation, all
Pain and suicide
of which constitute secondary gains that can aggravate or
maintain pain (Walker et al 2002; Worzer et al 2009). In Risk of suicide increases when chronic pain becomes over-
addition, litigation (DeBerard et al 2001; Epker & Block whelming. Relative to persons without chronic pain, those
2001; LaCaille et al 2005; Taylor et al 2000) and insurance with chronic pain are three times more likely to experience
or workers compensation (Deyo et al 2005; Epker & Block suicidal ideations and twice as likely to attempt suicide
2001; Mannion & Elfering 2006; Taylor et al 2000) are (Tang & Crane 2006). Up to half of all people with chronic
associated with poor surgical outcome (Bruns & Disorbio pain have considered suicide (Hitchcock et al 1994), but
2009). Despite the available research, meta-analytic ideation appears more frequent than forming a plan for
reviews accounting for severity of pain or disability have or attempting suicide (Smith et al 2004). Chronic pain
been mixed. Some reviews find more abnormalities and appears to play an important role in successful suicides
disability in patients who have financial incentives than (Fishbain et al 1991), particularly for older adults, who
in those without (Binder & Rohling 1996), while other are highly susceptible to both psychiatric illness and phys-
meta-analytic reviews have found minimal effects of com- ical health conditions (Manthorpe & Illife in press). That
pensation (Rohling et al 1995). Recent longitudinal said, the most common method of suicide among people

40
The psychology of pain: models and targets for comprehensive assessment Chapter |4|

with chronic pain is prescription overdose (Smith et al Culture and ethnicity


2004), which may be associated with increased access to
prescription medications. A more comprehensive discus- Culture can be understood as the knowledge through
sion of the current state of knowledge regarding suicide which groups of people interpret their lives and direct their
in people with various chronic pain conditions is provided behaviours (Turner 2005). Some aspects of the culture of
by El-Gabalawy et al (2011). an individual can influence how pain is experienced. For
example, those from the Mexican American culture are gen-
erally obligated to bear pain stoically (Villareul 1995). This
expectation may influence both their experience of pain
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES and the way they communicate pain to others. Culture
can have important influences on the way in which pain
Pain is experienced and acted on within the context of the assessment and treatment are conceptualized and prac-
environment in which a person operates on a daily basis. ticed. Mindbody dualism, focus on disease versus illness
Consequently, pain and pain-related cognitions and and biases toward cure versus care can substantively hinder
behaviours are influenced by numerous aspects of the envi- successful treatment of pain; as such, some theorists
ronment. Here we provide an overview of the influence of (Crowley-Matoka et al 2009) have called for general move-
family, culture and ethnicity, socioeconomics, and work ment towards biopsychosocial models of pain.
factors on pain. Ethnicitycultural groupings traditionally defined by a
common language, religion, nationality or heritage
(Shavers et al 2010) are also related to the way in which
Family pain is experienced and communicated. Indeed, the preva-
Family appears to influence pain experiences through both lence of chronic pain has been demonstrated to be signif-
genetic and social learning mechanisms. An association icantly higher for some ethnic groups in North America,
between some genes (i.e. the CMT and GTP ciclohydroxi- as are adverse outcomes related to pain (for review, see
lase genes) and pain perception and chronic pain has been Anderson et al 2009). For example, Day & Thorn (2010)
found (Diatchenko et al 2005; Tegeder et al 2006), but found that African Americans report significantly higher
genetic influences are, at minimum, shaped by social learn- pain intensity and pain interference compared to Cauca-
ing. Parents experiencing pain appear to be effective sian Americans. Others studies suggest that the relationship
models of illness behaviours in their children (Osborne between ethnicity and pain is better explained by socioeco-
et al 1989). This is illustrated by Goodman & McGrath nomic differences, as the association between ethnicity and
(2003), who exposed three groups of children to their pain outcomes are attenuated when socioeconomic factors
mothers behaviour during a cold pressor task. The mothers are controlled (Fuentes et al 2007; Stanaway et al 2011).
were assigned to three different conditions, including an Craig (Chapter 3) provides a comprehensive discussion
exaggerated pain condition (i.e. instructions to exaggerate of the influences of culture and ethnicity on pain. In the
pain response), a minimized pain condition (i.e. instructed context of assessment and case conceptualization, it is
to minimize pain response) and a control condition with- important that the therapist be mindful of potential cul-
out specific instructions. Children of the mothers tural and ethnic influences on pain experience and pain
instructed to exaggerate their pain response had lower pain reports. It is likewise important that he or she be mindful
thresholds and more exaggerated facial responses to cold of the way in which pain-related information from patients
pressor than did children of mothers in the control group. is received and interpreted; indeed, minorities are rated by
Spouses and significant others (e.g. family members, their providers as having less severe pain, less likely to
friends) can likewise influence the pain and pain behaviour receive opioid medications and less likely to receive com-
of the person experiencing pain (for review, see Leonard et al prehensive assessment and treatment when compared
2006). To illustrate, a spouse may unwittingly contribute to non-Hispanic Caucasian people (Shavers et al 2010).
the persistence of pain by rewarding (e.g. providing atten-
tion or sympathy) pain expression and passively sanctioning
Socioeconomics
avoidance of unwanted responsibilities or undesirable activ-
ities, by expressing negative emotion toward pain behav- Socioeconomic status (SES) represents a dynamic, multidi-
iours and by responding negatively to well behaviours mensional construct that is a robust determinant of health.
(Raichle et al 2011). However, the effects of pain on the fam- SES has a clear association with pain. Low neighbourhood
ily need not be negative. Pain might act for some as a stabi- SES (i.e. a high percentage of households below the poverty
lizing force and play a substantive role in maintaining family line, a high percentage of people 25 years old with less
homeostasis (Turk et al 1992). In the context of assessment than a high-school education and a high percentage of peo-
and case conceptualization, it is important that the therapist ple in the labour force who are unemployed) has been
be mindful of both negative and positive influences of fam- shown to be associated with increased pain, both sensory
ily on the pain experience and pain reports. and affective, as well as disability and mood disorders

41
Section | 1 | Overview: what is pain?

in adults with chronic low back pain (Fuentes et al 2007). assessment of pain. Instead, we provide some general
Similarly, recent findings indicate that those attending an guidelines to facilitate a comprehensive and multidimen-
emergency room were more likely to self-report chronic ill- sional assessment approach (see also Dworkin et al 2005;
ness and chronic pain if they were homeless, had family Tait 1999), all predicated on the notion that assessment
income less than $25 000 and perceived a lack of access is a conceptually driven process. First, we recommend that
to primary care (Hanley et al 2010). Cano et al (2006) sug- assessment include consideration of pain severity or inten-
gest that pain coping strategies may be dependent on cog- sity, pain location and distribution, as well as pain stability
nitive skills that are potentially enhanced by higher (e.g. intermittent vs persistent) and durations. Second, we
education and primary literacy levels; thus, those with recommend that some measure of physical functioning be
lower SES may be less likely to cope effectively with pain included in assessment. The experience of chronic pain is
and, as a consequence, be more prone to persistence of not homogenous and, as such, some people with chronic
their pain experience. pain function well whereas others do not. The extent of
functional limitation, which may or may not be associated
with pain severity or intensity (Turk 2002), is important for
Work directing treatment. Third, we recommend consideration of
Characteristics of work and the work environment have the various empirically supported and theoretically rele-
emerged as predictors of back pain and disability, even vant cognitive, behavioural and environment influences
after controlling for a host of other psychosocial, demo- described above. Person-specific circumstances can aid in
graphic and health-related variables (for reviews, see tailoring selection of constructs for assessment, for example
Crook et al 2002; Hoogendoorn et al 2000; Linton ones living circumstances, religious beliefs and current
2001; Shaw et al 2001, 2009). In a recent 2-year longitu- work status may dictate whether measures related to these
dinal study performed with neck pain patients, pain was areas are used. Consideration of convenience and time
predicted not only by mechanical factors (e.g. working commitments for the patient and assessor are also impor-
with arms raised) but also social factors (e.g. job tant in selecting constructs that will be assessed. Is there
demands, decision control, role conflict, empowering expertise to assess comorbid psychopathology? Is there
leadership) (Christensen & Knardahl 2010). High work time to do so? If so, there are several structured clinical
demands (e.g. long work hours, repetitive work, fatigue), interviews that a trained clinician can use for this purpose.
biomechanical factors (e.g. repetitive and sustained work If not, there are a variety of self-report and clinician-
posture), and low work support and job satisfaction have administered measures that can be used to screen for signif-
been identified as significant contributing factors to mus- icant symptoms of psychopathology which, in turn, can be
culoskeletal injury and pain (Crowther & Quayle 2010; more thoroughly evaluated as necessary. Finally, we recom-
for review, also see Macfarlane et al 2009). The Decade mend selection of a core set of measures that can be used to
of the Flag Working Group has recently identified seven gauge progress over time. This core set of measures should
workplace variables that have been shown to contribute tap the pain experience, physical functioning and psycho-
to back pain and pain-related disability: heavy physical social factors associated with pain.
demands, ability to modify work, job stress, social support,
job satisfaction, expectations for resuming work and fear
of re-injury (Shaw et al 2009). Consequently, compre- Treatment overview
hensive assessment of pain may necessitate consideration
of physical, psychological and social factors of work and Across the available treatment alternatives, the overwhelm-
the work place. ing evidence for more than two decades suggests that multi-
disciplinary treatments (i.e. those involving biological
and psychosocial interventions) result in better treatment
KEY CONSIDERATIONS IN outcomes than unimodal treatments (i.e. biological inter-
ASSESSMENT AND CASE ventions or physical therapy) for patients with chronic
pain (for review, see Flor et al 1992). Reviews of multidis-
CONCEPTUALIZATION ciplinary treatment programmes suggest that they are
not only therapeutically effective, but also cost-effective
Assessment and case (for review, see Gatchel & Okifuji 2006). Irrespective of
specific underlying causes, there is growing evidence that
conceptualization
pain-related anxiety and fear can both be reduced using
Careful and comprehensive assessment of pain is a neces- exposure-based therapies that encourage confronting
sary component of any treatment plan and any empirical anxiety- and fear-related stimuli (Asmundson et al
pursuit wherein pain is of interest, and is covered in detail 2004b; Bailey et al 2010; McCracken 1997; McCracken
in Chapter 7. It is beyond the scope of this chapter to review et al 1992). For discussion about psychological interven-
the potential complexities associated with a comprehensive tions see Chapters 8 and 9 in this text.

42
The psychology of pain: models and targets for comprehensive assessment Chapter |4|

treatment planning can be based. While each case may


CONCLUSION involve consideration of different biological, psycho-
logical and social factors in the context of the pain expe-
rience, it is careful consideration of these factors that
Contemporary biopsychosocial approaches to under-
will ultimately lead to the best outcomes for those dis-
standing pain provide a sound foundation on which
abled by pain.
comprehensive assessment, case conceptualization and

REFERENCES

Anderson, K.O., Green, C.R., Payne, R., and treating fear of pain. Oxford effects of financial incentives on
2009. Racial and ethnic disparities in University Press, Oxford. recovery after closed-head injury. Am.
pain: Causes and consequences of Asmundson, G.J.G., Collimore, K.C., J. Psychiatry 153, 710.
unequal care. J. Pain. 10, 11871204. Bernstein, A., et al., 2007. Is the latent Blyth, F.M., March, L.M., Nicholas, M.K.,
Arnow, B.A., Blasey, C.M., Lee, J., et al., structure of fear of pain continuous or et al., 2003. Chronic pain, work
2009. Relationships among discontinuous among pain patients? performance and litigation.
depression, chronic pain, chronic Taxometric analysis of the pain Pain 103, 4147.
disabling pain, and medical costs. anxiety symptoms scale. J. Pain 8, Bruns, D., Disorbio, J.M., 2009.
Psychiatr. Serv. 60, 344350. 387395. Assessment of biopsychosocial risk
Asmundson, G.J.G., Asmundson, G.J.G., Abrams, M.P., factors for medical treatment: a
Hadjistavropoulos, H.D., 2006. Collimore, K.C., 2008. Pain and collaborative approach. J. Clin.
Addressing shared vulnerability anxiety disorders. In: Zvolensky, M.J., Psychol. Med. Settings 16, 127147.
for PTSD and chronic pain: A Smits, J.A.J. (Eds.), Health behaviors Burns, J.W., Mullen, J.T., Higdon, L.J.,
cognitive-behavioral perspective. and physical illness in anxiety and its et al., 2000. Validity of the Pain
Cognitive and Behavioral Practice disorders: Contemporary theory and Anxiety Symptoms Scale (PASS):
13, 816. research. Springer, New York, Prediction of physical capacity
Asmundson, G.J.G., Katz, J., 2009. pp. 207235. variables. Pain 84, 247252.
Understanding the co-occurrence Asmundson, G.J.G., Peluso, D., Bussing, A., Michalsen, A., Balzat, H.,
of anxiety disorders and chronic Carleton, R.N., et al., 2010. et al., 2009. Are spirituality and
musculoskeletal pain: The state- Chronic Musculoskeletal Pain religiosity resources for patients with
of-the-art. Depress. Anxiety and Related Health Conditions. In: chronic pain conditions? Pain Med.
26, 888901. Zvolensky, M.J., Bernstein, A., 10, 327339.
Asmundson, G.J.G., Wright, K.D., 2004. Vujanovic, A.A. (Eds.), Cano, A., Mayo, A., Ventimiglia, M.,
Biopsychosocial approaches to pain. Distress Tolerance: Theory, 2006. Coping, pain severity,
In: Hadjistavropoulos, T., Craig, K.D. Research, and Clinical Applications. interference, and disability: the
(Eds.), Pain: Psychological Guilford Publications, New York, potential mediating and moderating
Perspectives. Erlbaum, New Jersey, pp. 221244. roles of race and education. J. Pain 7,
pp. 3557. Atlas, S.J., Tosteson, T.D., Blood, E.A., 459468.
Asmundson, G.J.G., Norton, G.R., et al., 2010. The impact of Carleton, R.N., Asmundson, G.J.G., 2007.
Allerdings, M.D., et al., 1998. workers compensation on Review of cognitive-behavioral
Posttraumatic stress disorder and outcomes of surgical and therapies for trauma, 2nd edn.
work-related injury. J. Anxiety Disord. nonoperative therapy for Canadian Psychology/Psychologie
12, 5769. patients with a lumbar disc herniation Canadienne 48, 201203.
Asmundson, G.J.G., Norton, P.J., SPORT. Spine 35, 8997.
Carleton, R.N., Asmundson, G.J.G., 2009.
Norton, G.R., 1999. Beyond pain: The Bailey, K.M., Carleton, R.N., The multidimensionality of fear of
role of fear and avoidance in Vlaeyen, J.W.S., et al., 2010. pain: Construct independence for the
chronicity. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 19, Treatments addressing pain-related Fear of Pain Questionnaire-Short
97119. fear and anxiety in patients with Form and the Pain Anxiety Symptoms
Asmundson, G.J.G., Norton, P.J., chronic musculoskeletal pain: A Scale-20. J. Pain 10, 2937.
Vlaeyen, J.W.S., 2004a. Fear- preliminary review. Cogn. Behav.
Carleton, R.N., Abrams, M.P.,
avoidance models of chronic pain: An Ther. 39, 4663.
Asmundson, G.J.G., et al., 2009.
overview. In: Asmundson, G.J.G., Beck, A.T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., et al., Pain-related anxiety and anxiety
Vlaeyen, J.W.S., Crombez, G. (Eds.), 1988. An inventory for measuring sensitivity across anxiety and
Understanding and Treating Fear of clinical anxiety: Psychometric depressive disorders. J. Anxiety
Pain. Oxford University Press, Oxford, properties. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. Disord. 23, 791798.
pp. 324. 56, 893897.
Cassidy, J.D., Carroll, L.J., Cote, P., et al.,
Asmundson, G.J.G., Vlaeyen, J.W.S., Binder, L.M., Rohling, M.L., 1996. Money 2000. Effect of eliminating
Crombez, G., 2004b. Understanding matters: A meta-analytic review of the compensation for pain and suffering

43
Section | 1 | Overview: what is pain?

on the outcome of insurance claims de Jong, J.R., Vlaeyen, J.W., Onghena, P., Fishbain, D.A., Rosomoff, H.L., Cutler, R.,
for whiplash injury. N. Engl. J. Med. et al., 2005. Fear of movement/(re) et al., 1995. Do chronic pain patients
342, 11791186. injury in chronic low back pain: perceptions about their preinjury jobs
Christensen, J.O., Knardahl, S., 2010. education or exposure in vivo as determine their intent to return to the
Work and neck pain: A prospective mediator to fear reduction? Clin. J. same type of job post-pain facility
study of psychological, social, and Pain 21, 917. treatment? Clin. J. Pain. 11, 267278.
mechanical risk factors. Pain 151, Demyttenaere, K., Bruffaerts, R., Lee, S., Fishbain, D.A., Cutler, R.B., Lewis, J.,
162173. et al., 2007. Mental disorders among et al., 2004. Do the Second-
Cloninger, R.C., Przybeck, T.R., persons with chronic back or neck Generation Atypical Neuroleptics
Svrakic, D.M., et al., 1994. The pain: results from the World Mental have analgesic properties? A
Temperament and Character Health Surveys. Pain 129, 332342. structured evidenced-based review.
Inventory (TCI): A guide to its Deyo, R.A., Mirza, S.K., Heagerty, P.J., Pain Med. 5, 359365.
development and use. Center for et al., 2005. A prospective cohort study Flor, H., Fydrich, T., Turk, D.C., 1992.
Psychobiology of Personality, of surgical treatment for back pain Efficacy of multidisciplinary pain
Washington University, St Louis, MO. with degenerated discs; study treatment centers: a meta-analytic
Craig, K.D., Hill, M.L., McMurty, B.W., protocol. BMC Musculoskelet. review. Pain 49, 221230.
1999. Detecting deception and Disord. 6, 24. Fordyce, W.E., 1976. Behavioral methods
malingering. In: Block, A.R., Diatchenko, L., Slade, G.D., for chronic pain and illness. Mosby, St
Kramer, E.F., Fernandez, E. (Eds.), Nackley, A.G., et al., 2005. Genetic Louis, MO.
Handbook of pain syndromes: basis for individual variations in pain Fordyce, W.E., Shelton, J.L.,
biopsychosocial perspectives. perception and the development of a Dundore, D.E., 1982. The
Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, New chronic pain condition. Hum. Mol. modification of avoidance learning
Jersey, pp. 4158. Genet. 14, 135143. pain behaviors. J. Behav. Med. 5,
Crombez, G., Vlaeyen, J.W.S., Dworkin, R.H., Turk, D.C., Farrar, J.T., 405414.
Heuts, P.H.T.G., et al., 1999. et al., 2005. Core outcome measures Fuentes, M., Hart-Johnson, T.,
Pain-related fear is more disabling for chronic pain clinical trials: Green, C.R., 2007. The Association
than pain itself: Evidence on the IMMPACT recommendations. Pain among Neighborhood
role of pain-related fear in chronic 113, 919. Socioeconomic Status, Race and
back pain disability. Pain 80, Edwards, L.C., Pearce, S.A., Chronic Pain in Black and White
329339. Turner-Stokes, L., et al., 1992. The Older Adults. J. Natl. Med. Assoc. 99,
Crook, J., Milner, R., Schultz, I.Z., et al., Pain Beliefs Questionnaire: an 11601169.
2002. Determinants of occupational investigation of beliefs in the causes Gatchel, R.J., Okifuji, A., 2006. Evidence-
disability following a back and consequences of pain. Pain 51, based scientific data documenting the
injury: A critical review of the 267272. treatment and cost-effectiveness of
literature. J. Occup. Rehabil. 12, El-Gabalawy, R., Asmundson, G.J.G., comprehensive pain programs for
277295. Sareen, J., 2011. Suicide and chronic chronic nonmalignant pain. J. Pain 7
Crowley-Matoka, M., Saha, S., pain. In: Pompili, M., Berman, L. (11), 779793.
Dobscha, S.K., et al., 2009. (Eds.), Suicide Risk and Physical Geisser, M.E., Roth, R.S., Robinson, M.E.,
Problems of quality and equity in Illness. American Association of 1997. Assessing depression among
pain management: exploring the Suicidology, Washington, DC, persons with chronic pain using the
role of biomedical culture. Pain Med. pp. 7586. Center for Epidemiological Studies
10, 13121324. Epker, J., Block, A.R., 2001. Presurgical Depression Scale and the Beck
Crowther, I.E., Quayle, L., 2010. psychological screening in back pain Depression Inventory: a comparative
Womens health at work program. patients: A review. Clin. J. Pain 17, analysis. Clin. J. Pain 13, 163170.
Musculoskeletal pain experience by 200205. Glombiewski, J.A., Hartwich-Tersek, J.,
women of Chinese background First, M., Spitzer, R., Gibbon, M., et al., Rief, W., 2010. Depression in chronic
working on market gardens in the 1996. Structured Clinical Interview for back pain patients: Prediction of
Sydney basin. Work 36, 129140. DSM-IV Axis I Disorders Patient pain intensity and pain disability
Day, M.A., Thorn, B.E., 2010. The edition. New York State Psychiatric in cognitive-behavioral
relationship of demographic and Institute, Biometrics Research treatment. Psychosomatics 51,
psychosocial variables to pain-related Department, New York. 130136.
outcomes in a rural chronic pain Fishbain, D.A., 1994. Secondary gain Glover-Graf, N.M., Marini, I., Baker, J.,
population. Pain. http://dx.doi.org/ concept: Definition problems and its et al., 2007. Religious and spiritual
10.1016/j.pain.2010.08.015. abuse in medical practice. APS Journal beliefs and practices of persons with
DeBerard, M.S., Masters, K.S., 3, 264273. chronic pain. Rehabilitation
Colledge, A.L., et al., 2001. Outcomes Fishbain, D.A., Goldberg, M., Counseling Bulletin 51, 2133.
of posterolateral lumbar fusion in Rosomoff, R.S., et al., 1991. Goodman, J.E., McGrath, P.J., 2003.
Utah patients receiving workers Case Reports: Completed Suicide Mothers modeling influences
compensation: A retrospective cohort in Chronic Pain. Clin. J. Pain 7, childrens pain during a cold presor
study. Spine 26, 738746. 2936. task. Pain 104, 559565.

44
The psychology of pain: models and targets for comprehensive assessment Chapter |4|

Goubert, L., Crombez, G., definitions of pain terms. Pain. with chronic pain complaints. Pain
Van Damme, S., 2004. The role of (Suppl. 3), 1222. 69, 2734.
neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and Jensen, M.P., Turner, J.A., Romano, J.M., Leeuw, M., Peters, M.L., Wiers, R.W.,
pain-related fear in vigilance to pain: a et al., 1991. Coping with chronic pain: et al., 2007. Measuring fear
structural equations approach. Pain A critical review of the literature. Pain. of movement/(re)injury in chronic
107, 234241. 47, 249283. low back pain using implicit
Greenberg, J., Burns, J.W., 2003. Pain Jensen, M.P., Turner, J.A., Romano, J.M., measures. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 36,
anxiety among chronic pain patients: 2001. Changes in beliefs, 5264.
specific phobia or manifestation of catastrophizing, and coping are Leonard, M.T., Cano, A., Johansen, A.B.,
anxiety sensitivity? Behav. Res. Ther. associated with improvement in 2006. Chronic pain in a couples
41, 223240. multidisciplinary pain treatment. context: a review and integration of
Gureje, O., Von Korff, M., Kola, L., et al., J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 69, theoretical models and empirical
2008. The relation between multiple 655662. evidence. J. Pain 7, 377390.
pains and mental disorders: Results Keefe, F.J., Wilkins, R.H., Cook Jr., W.A., Lethem, J., Slade, P.D., Troup, J.D., et al.,
from the World Mental Health et al., 1986. Depression, pain, and 1983. Outline of a fear-
Surveys. Pain 135, 8291. pain behavior. J. Consult. Clin. avoidance model of exaggerated
Hadjistavropoulos, H.D., Psychol. 54, 665669. pain perception: I. Behav. Res. Ther.
Hadjistavropoulos, T., Quine, A., Keogh, E., Mansoor, L., 2001. 21, 401408.
2000. Health anxiety moderates the Investigating the effects of anxiety Linton, S.J., 2001. Occupational
effects of distraction versus attention sensitivity and coping on the psychological factors increase the
to pain. Behav. Res. Ther. 38, perception of cold pressor pain in risk for back pain: a systematic review.
425438. healthy women. Eur. J. Pain 5, J. Occup. Rehabil. 11, 5366.
Hadjistavropoulos, H.D., Owens, K.M.B., 1122. Main, C.J., 2003. The nature of chronic
Hadjistavropoulos, T., et al., 2001. Keogh, E., Hatton, H., Ellery, D., 2000. pain: a clinical and legal challenge. In:
Hypochondriasis and health anxiety Avoidance versus focused attention Hallingan, P.W., Bass, C., Oakley, D.A.
among pain patients. In: and the perception of pain: (Eds.), Malingering and Illness
Asmundson, G.J.G., Taylor, S., differential effects for men and Deception. Oxford University Press,
Cox, B.J. (Eds.), Health Anxiety: women. Pain 85, 225230. New York, pp. 171183.
Clinical and research perspectives on Kessler, R.C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Macfarlane, G.J., Pallewatte, N.,
hypochondriasis and related et al., 2005a. Lifetime prevalence Paudyal, P., et al., 2009. Evaluation of
conditions. John Wiley & Sons, and age-of-onset distributions work-related psychosocial factors and
Toronto, pp. 298323. of DSM-IV disorders in the regional musculoskeletal pain: results
Hanley, O., Miner, J., Rockswold, E., National Comorbidity Survey from a EULAR Task Force. Annual of
et al., 2010. The relationship Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, Rheumatic Diseases 68, 885891.
between chronic illness, chronic 593602. Mannion, A.F., Elfering, A., 2006.
pain, and socioeconomic factors Kessler, R.C., Chiu, W.T., Demler, O., Predictors of surgical outcome and
in the ED. Am. J. Emerg. Med. et al., 2005b. Prevalence, severity, and their assessment. Eur. Spine J. 15,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV S93S108.
ajem.2009.10.002. disorders in the National Manthorpe, J., Illife, S., in press Suicide in
Heyneman, N.E., Fremouw, W.J., Comorbidity Survey Replication. later life: public health and
Gano, D., et al., 1990. Individual Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 617627. practitioner perspectives. Int. J.
differences and the effectiveness Kori, S.H., Miller, R.P., Todd, D.D., 1990. Geriatr. Psychiatry 25, 12301238.
of different coping strategies Kinesiophobia: A new view of chronic McCracken, L.M., 1997. Attention to
for pain. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 14, pain behavior. Pain Management 3, pain in persons with chronic pain: a
6377. 3543. behavioral approach. Behaviour
Hitchcock, L.S., Ferrell, B.R., Krause, S.J., Weiner, R.L., Tait, R.C., 1994. Therapy 28, 271284.
McCaffery, M., 1994. The Depression and pain behavior in McCracken, L.M., Dhingra, L., 2002. A
experience of chronic nonmalignant patients with chronic pain. Clin. J. short version of the pain anxiety
pain. J. Pain Symptom. Manage. 9, Pain 10, 122127. symptoms scale (PASS-20):
312318.
LaCaille, R.A., DeBerard, M.S., Preliminary development and
Hoogendoorn, W.E., van Poppel, M.N., Masters, K.S., et al., 2005. validity. Pain Res. Manag. 7, 4550.
Bongers, P.M., et al., 2000. Presurgical biopsychosocial McCracken, L.M., Gross, R.T., 1995. The
Systematic review of psychosocial factors predict multidimensional pain anxiety symptoms scale
factors at work and private life as patient outcomes of interbody (PASS) and the assessment of
risk factors for back pain. Spine. 25, cage lumbar fusion. Spine J. 5 (1), emotional responses to pain. In:
21142125. 7178. VandeCreek, L., Knapp, S.,
International Association for the Study Larsen, D.K., Taylor, S., Jackson, T.L. (Eds.), Innovations in
of Pain, 1986. Classification of Asmundson, G.J.G., 1997. clinical practice: a sourcebook.
chronic pain: Descriptions of Exploratory factor analysis of the Pain Professional Resources Press,
chronic pain syndromes and Anxiety Symptoms Scale in patients Sarasota, FL, pp. 309321.

45
Section | 1 | Overview: what is pain?

McCracken, L.M., Zayfert, C., Gross, R.T., Norton, P.J., Asmundson, G.J.G., 2003. functioning, and depression. Pain
1992. The pain anxiety symptoms Amending the fear-avoidance model 152, 8288.
scale: Development and validation of of chronic pain: What is the role of Raphael, K.G., Janal, M.N., Nayak, S.,
a scale to measure fear of pain. physiological arousal? Behav. Ther. et al., 2006. Psychiatric comorbidities
Pain 50, 6773. 34, 1730. in a community sample of women
McCracken, L.M., Zayfert, C., Gross, R.T., Odegard, S., Finset, A., Mowinckel, P., with fibromyalgia. Pain 124,
1993. The pain anxiety symptoms et al., 2007. Pain and psychology 117125.
scale (PASS): a multimodal measure health status over a 10 year period in Ratcliffe, G.E., Enns, M.W., Belik, S.L.,
of pain-specific anxiety symptoms. patients with recent onset rheumatoid et al., 2008. Chronic pain conditions
Behavior Therapist 16, 183184. arthritis. Ann. Rheum. Dis. 66, and suicidal ideation and suicide
McCracken, L.M., Faber, S.D., 11951201. attempts: An epidemiologic
Janeck, A.S., 1998. Pain-related Osborne, R.B., Hatcher, J.W., perspective. Clin. J. Pain 24,
anxiety predicts nonspecific Richtsmeier, A.J., 1989. The role of 204210.
physical complaints in persons with social modeling in unexplained Rippentrop, E.A., 2005. A review of the
chronic pain. Behav. Res. Ther. 36, pediatric pain. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 14, role of religion and spirituality in
621630. 4361. chronic pain populations.
McNeil, D.W., Rainwater, A.J., 1998. Osman, A., Barrios, F.X., Osman, J.R., Rehabilitation Psychology 50,
Development of the fear of pain et al., 1994. The Pain 278284.
questionnaire-III. J. Behav. Med. 21, Anxiety Symptoms Scale: Rippentrop, E.A., Altmaier, E.M., Chen, J.,
389410. psychometric properties in a et al., 2005. The relationship between
McWilliams, L.A., Cox, B.J., Enns, M.W., community sample. J. Behav. Med. 17 religion/spirituality and physical
2003. Mood and anxiety disorders (5), 511522. health, mental health, and pain in a
associated with chronic pain: an Osman, A., Breitenstein, J.L., Barrios, F.X., chronic pain population. Pain 116,
examination in a nationally et al., 2001. The fear of pain 311321.
representative sample. Pain 106, questionnaire-III: further reliability Roelofs, J., Peters, M.L.,
127133. and validity with nonclinical samples. van der Zijden, M., et al., 2004. Does
Mechanic, D., 1962. The concept of J. Behav. Med. 25, 155173. fear of pain moderate the effects of
illness behavior. J. Chronic Dis. Parsons, T., 1951. The Social System. Free sensory focusing and distraction on
15, 189194. Press, New York. cold pressor pain in pain-free
Melzack, R., 1983. The McGill Pain Piedmont, R.L., 1999. Does spirituality individuals? J. Pain 5, 250256.
Questionnaire. In: Melzack, R. (Ed.), represent the sixth factor of Rogers, R., Neumann, C.S., 2003.
Pain Measurement and Assessment. personality? Spiritual transcendence Conceptual issues and explanatory
Raven Press, New York, pp. 4147. and the five-factor model. J. Pers. 67, models of malingering. In:
Melzack, R., 1999. From the gate to the 9851013. Halligan, P.W., Bass, C., Oakley, D.A.
neuromatrix. Pain (Suppl. 6), Pincus, T., Williams, A., 1999. Models (Eds.), Malingering and illness
S121S126. and measurements of depression in deception: Clinical and theoretical
Merskey, H., Bogduk, N., 1994. chronic pain. J. Psychosom. Res. 47, perspectives. Oxford University Press,
Classification of chronic pain: 211219. Oxford, pp. 7182.
descriptions of chronic pain Polatin, P.B., Kinney, R.K., Gatchel, R.J., Rohling, M.L., Binder, L.M.,
syndromes and definitions of pain et al., 1993. Psychiatric illness and Langhirinrichsen-Rohling, J., 1995.
terms, second ed. IASP Press, Seattle. chronic low-back pain. The mind and Money matters: a meta-analytic review
Melzack, R., Casey, K.L., 1968. Sensory, the spine which goes first? Spine 18, of the association between financial
motivational and central control 6671. compensation and the experience and
determinants of pain. In: Poole, H., White, S., Blake, C., et al., 2009. treatment of chronic pain. Health
Kenshalo, D.R. (Ed.), The Skin Senses. Depression in Chronic Pain Patients: Psychol. 14, 537547.
CC Thomas, Springfield, IL, Prevalence and Measurement. Pain Shavers, V.L., Bakos, A., Sheppard, V.B.,
pp. 423439. Pract. 9, 173180. 2010. Race, Ethnicity, and Pain
Melzack, R., Katz, J., 2004. The gate Price, D.D., 2000. Psychological and among the US Adult Population. J.
control theory: Reaching for the Brain. neural mechanisms of the affective Health Care Poor Underserved 21,
In: Craig, K.D., Hadjistavropoulos, T. dimension of pain. Science. 288, 177220.
(Eds.), Pain: Psychological 17691772. Shaw, W.S., Pransky, G., Fitzgerald, T.E.,
Perspectives. Lawrence Erlbaum, Radloff, L.S., 1977. The CES-D Scale: 2001. Early prognosis for low back
Mahwah, NJ, pp. 303322. A self-report depression scale for disability: intervention strategies for
Melzack, R., Wall, P.D., 1965. Pain research in the general population. health care providers. Disabil.
mechanisms: a new theory. Science Applied Psychological Measurement. Rehabil. 23, 815828.
150, 971979. 1, 385401. Shaw, W.S., van der Windt, D.A.,
Morley, S., Williams, A.C., Black, S., 2002. Raichle, K.A., Romano, J.M., Jensen, M.P., Main, C.J., et al., the Decade of the
A confirmatory factor analysis of the 2011. Partner responses to patient Flags Working Group, 2009. Early
Beck Depression Inventory in chronic pain and well behaviors and their Patient Screening and Intervention to
pain. Pain 99, 289298. relationship to patient pain behavior, Address Individual-Level

46
The psychology of pain: models and targets for comprehensive assessment Chapter |4|

Occupational Factors (Blue Flags) in low back surgery: A community-based factor structure across clinical and
Back Disability. J. Occup. Rehabil. 19, study. Spine 25, 24452452. non-clinical populations. Pain 96,
6480. Tegeder, I., Costigan, M., Griffin, R.S., 319324.
Sloan, T.J., Gupta, R., Zhang, W., et al., et al., 2006. GTP cyclohydrolase and Villareul, A.M., 1995. Mexican-American
2007. Beliefs about the causes and tetrahydrobiopterin regulate pain cultural meanings, expressions,
consequences of pain in patients with sensitivity and persistence. Nat. Med. self-care and dependent-care
chronic inflammatory or 12, 12691277. actions associated with experiences
noninflammatory low back pain and Teh, C.F., Zaslavsky, A.M., Reynolds, C.F., of pain. Res. Nurs. Health 18,
in pain-free individuals. Spine 33, et al., 2009. Effect of depression 427436.
966972. treatment on chronic pain outcomes. Vlaeyen, J.W.S., Linton, S.J., 2000.
Smith, W.B., Gracely, R.H., Safer, M.A., Psychosom. Med. 72, 6167. Fear-avoidance and its consequences
1998. The meaning of pain: Cancer Turk, D.C., 2002. A diathesis-stress model in chronic musculoskeletal
patients rating and recall of pain of chronic pain and disability pain: A state of the art. Pain 85,
intensity and affect. Pain 78, following traumatic injury. Pain Res. 317332.
123129. Manag. 7, 919. Vlaeyen, J.W.S., Kole-Snijders, A.M.,
Smith, M.T., Edwards, R.R., Turk, D.C., Flor, H., 1999. The Boeren, R.G., et al., 1995. Fear of
Robinson, R.C., et al., 2004. Suicidal biobehavioral perspective of pain. In: movement/(re)injury in chronic low
ideation, plans, and attempts in Gatchel, R.J., Turk, D.C. (Eds.), back pain and its relation to
chronic pain patients: factors Psychosocial factors in pain. Clinical behavioral performance. Pain 62,
associated with increased risk. Pain perspectives. Guilford Press, New 363372.
111, 201208. York, pp. 1834. Vlaeyen, J.W.S., de Jong, J., Leeuw, M.,
Spengler, D.M., Bigos, S.J., Martin, N.A., Turk, D.C., Rudy, T.E., 1987. IASP et al., 2004. Fear reduction in chronic
1986. Back injuries in industry: A taxonomy of chronic pain syndromes: pain: graded exposure in vivo with
retrospective study. 1. Overview and Preliminary assessment of reliability. behavioral experiments. In:
cost analysis. Spine 11, 241245. Pain 30, 177189. Asmundson, G.J., Vlaeyen, J.W.S.,
Stanaway, F.F., Blyth, F.M., Turk, D.C., Meichenbaum, D., Crombez, G. (Eds.), Understanding
Cumming, R.G., et al., 2011. Back Genest, M., 1983. Pain and behavioral and treating fear of pain. Oxford
pain in older male Italian-born medicine: A cognitive-behavioral University Press, Oxford.
immigrants in. The importance of perspective. Guilford Press, Waddell, G., 1987. A new clinical model
socioeconomic factors. Eur. J. Pain, New York. for the treatment of low back pain.
Australia http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. Turk, D.C., Kerns, R.D., Rosenberg, R., Spine 12, 623644.
ejpain.2010.05.009. 1992. Effects of marital interaction on Waddell, G., Main, C.J., Morris, E.W.,
Stewart, W.F., Ricci, J.A., Chee, E., et al., chronic pain and disability: et al., 1984. Chronic low-back pain,
2003. Lost productive time and cost Examining the down side of social psychologic distress, and
due to common pain conditions in support. Rehabilitation Psychology illness behavior. Spine 9 (2),
the US workforce. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 37, 259274. 209213.
290, 24432454. Turner, L., 2005. From the local to the Waddell, G., Newton, M., Henderson, I.,
Sullivan, M.J.L., Thorn, B., global: bioethics and the concept of et al., 1993. A Fear-Avoidance Beliefs
Haythornthwaite, J.A., et al., 2001. culture. J. Med. Philos. 30, 305320. Questionnaire (FABQ) and the role of
Theoretical perspectives on the Turner, J.A., Jensen, M.P., Romano, J.M., fear-avoidance beliefs in chronic low
relation between catastrophizing and 2000. Do beliefs, coping, and back pain and disability. Pain 52,
pain. Clin. J. Pain 17, 5264. catastrophizing independently predict 157168.
Tait, R., 1999. Evaluation of treatment functioning in patients with chronic Walker, L.S., Claar, R.L., Garber, J., 2002.
effectiveness in patients with pain? Pain 85, 115125. Social consequences of childrens
intractable pain: Measures and Turner, J.A., Mancl, L., Aaron, L.A., 2004. pain: When do they encourage
methods. In: Gatchel, R.J., Turk, D.C. Pain-related catastrophizing: a daily symptom maintenance? J. Pediatr.
(Eds.), Psychosocial factors in pain: process study. Pain 110, 103111. Psychol. 27, 689698.
Critical perspectives. Guilford Press, Wall, P.D., 1996. Comments after 30
Unruh, A.M., 1996. Gender variations in
New York, pp. 457480. years of the gate control theory of
clinical pain experience. Pain 65,
Tang, N.K.Y., Crane, C., 2006. Suicidality 123167. pain. Pain Forum 5, 1222.
in chronic pain: a review of the Wallston, K.A., Wallston, B.S.,
Unruh, A.M., 2007. Spirituality, religion
prevalence, risk factors and DeVellis, R., 1978. Development of
and pain. Can. J. Nurs. Res. 39, 6686.
psychological links. Psychol. Med. 36, the multidimensional health locus of
575586. Unruh, A.M., Ritchie, J.A., Merskey, H.,
control (MHLC) scale. Health Educ.
1999. Does gender affect appraisal of
Taylor, S., Asmundson, G.J.G., 2004. Monogr. 6, 525.
pain and pain coping strategies? Clin.
Treating health anxiety: A cognitive- Walsh, D.A., Radcliffe, J.C., 2002.
J. Pain 15, 3140.
behavioral approach. Guilford Press, Pain beliefs and perceived
New York. Van Damme, S., Crombez, G.,
physical disability of patients with
Bijttebier, P., et al., 2002. A
Taylor, V.M., Deyo, R.A., Ciol, M., et al., chronic low back pain. Pain 97,
confirmatory factor analysis of the
2000. Patient-oriented outcomes from 2331.
pain catastrophizing scale: invariant

47
Section | 1 | Overview: what is pain?

Werner, E.L., Ihlebaek, C., Skouen, J.S., Williams, D.A., Thorn, B.E., 1989. An Worzer, W.E., Kishino, N.D.,
et al., 2005. Beliefs about low empirical assessment of pain beliefs. Gatchel, R.J., 2009. Primary,
back pain in the Norwegian general Pain 36, 351358. secondary, and tertiary losses
population: are they related to Worz, R., 2003. Pain in depression in chronic pain patients.
pain experiences and health depression in pain. Pain Clinical Psychology, Injury, and Law 2,
professionals? Spine. 30, Updates (IASP) XI (1), 14. 215224.
17701776.

48

Potrebbero piacerti anche