Sei sulla pagina 1di 9

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 56:9 (2015), pp 949957 doi:10.1111/jcpp.

12426

Early identification of ADHD risk via infant


temperament and emotion regulation: a pilot study
Elinor L. Sullivan,1,2 Kathleen F. Holton,3 Elizabeth K. Nousen,4 Ashley N. Barling,1
Ceri A. Sullivan,4 Cathi B. Propper,5 and Joel T. Nigg4
1
Department of Biology, University of Portland, Portland, OR; 2Division of Diabetes, Obesity and Metabolism, Oregon
National Primate Research Center, Beaverton, OR; 3School of Education, Teaching & Health, Center for Behavioral
Neuroscience, American University, Washington, DC; 4Departments of Psychology and Behavioral Neuroscience,
Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR; 5Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina
at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA

Background: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is theorized to have temperamental precursors early in
life. These are difficult to identify because many core features of ADHD, such as breakdowns in executive function
and self-control, involve psychological and neural systems that are too immature to reliably show dysfunction in
early life. ADHD also involves emotional dysregulation, and these temperamental features appear earlier as well.
Here, we report a first attempt to utilize indices of emotional regulation to identify ADHD-related liability in infancy.
Methods: Fifty women were recruited in the 2nd trimester of pregnancy, with overselection for high parental ADHD
symptoms. Measures of maternal body mass index, nutrition, substance use, stress, and mood were examined
during pregnancy as potential confounds. Offspring were evaluated at 6 months of age using LABTAB procedures
designed to elicit fear, anger, and regulatory behavior. Mothers completed the Infant Behavior Questionnaire about
their childs temperament. Results: After control for associated covariates, including maternal depression and
prenatal stress, family history of ADHD was associated with measures of anger/irritability, including infant negative
vocalizations during the arm restraint task (p = .004), and maternal ratings of infant distress to limitations
(p = .036). In the regulation domain, familial ADHD was associated with less parent-oriented attention seeking
during the still face procedure (p < .001), but this was not echoed in the maternal ratings of recovery from distress.
Conclusions: Affective response at 6 months of age may identify infants with familial history of ADHD, providing an
early indicator of ADHD liability. These preliminary results provide a foundation for further studies and will be
amplified by enlarging this cohort and following participants longitudinally to evaluate ADHD outcomes. Keywords:
ADHD risk, markers, early identification, maternal precursors, emotional dysregulation, infant temperament.

months and years after birth (Nigg, 2006b). These


Introduction
transactions would include early liability transmit-
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is
ted from the family as well as subsequent develop-
most commonly diagnosed in childhood but is the-
mental events. Thus, the purpose of examining early
orized to be rooted in early development, perhaps via
life markers is twofold: (a) to identify markers related
temperament (Nigg, 2006a); it is thus conceptualized
to familial liability and (b) to identify early indicators
as a neurodevelopmental disorder in DSM-5 (Amer-
of future disorder or disease.
ican Psychiatric Association, 2013). ADHD is unusu-
Identifying markers of ADHD liability before 2 or
ally heritable, with estimates well above 0.70 (Kan
3 years of age is difficult. Arnett, Macdonald, and
et al., 2013; Larsson, Chang, DOnofrio, & Lichten-
Pennington (2013) found correlations between Bay-
stein, 2013). As a result, the disorder is highly
ley scores and Child Behavior Check List behavior
familial, with substantially increased risk in off-
problems at age 15 months (girls) and 24 months
spring of parents with ADHD (Musser et al., 2014;
(boys) with ADHD symptoms in third grade (Arnett
Stawicki, Nigg, & von Eye, 2006), making parental
et al., 2013). However, they were unable to identify
ADHD an excellent proxy for offspring liability.
earlier predictors (and did not examine familial
Children with ADHD exhibit differences in brain
liability). Johnson, Gliga, Jones, and Charman
morphology by age five, which persist through ado-
(2015) reviewed the literature on correlations of
lescence (Castellanos et al., 2002) and reflect alter-
infant behavior and later ADHD (Johnson et al.,
ation in white matter development (Shaw et al.,
2015). They concluded that this literature remains
2006). This leaves open the possibility that the
sparse, although there is scattered evidence linking
developmental course of ADHD may begin very early
motor delays and isolated studies of attention but
in life, perhaps even in the perinatal period. We,
little convincing evidence of early infant markers in
along with others, have proposed that this disorder
the cognitive domain.
begins early in life and then unfolds through a series
Part of this challenge is because ADHD itself is
of developmental transactions in the ensuing
difficult to reliably characterize prior to 34 years of
age. Its behavioral symptoms (inattention, impulsiv-
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. ity, hyperactivity) are often normative in toddlers.

2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA
950 Elinor L. Sullivan et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2015; 56(9): 94957

Furthermore, higher order cognitive abilities such as The primary goal of this pilot study was to follow
executive functioning and reward discounting, up these effects and extend those findings by
important in ADHD (Barkley, 1997), are not yet including prenatal and postnatal evaluation. The
sufficiently developed to markedly shape behavior goal was to investigate differences in temperament
until the second or third year of life (Diamond, 2013). and emotional regulation of 6-month-old infants
Emotional systems, however, develop earlier and with and without ADHD liability (defined by
are a promising early life target for ADHD liability. It parental history of ADHD) in order to evaluate
is increasingly recognized that ADHD is also whether these traits serve as early life markers of
characterized by dysfunction in emotional systems liability.
(Martel, 2009), in particular in negative emotionality
and irritability (Karalunas et al., 2014; Martel &
Nigg, 2006; Musser, Galloway-Long, Frick, & Nigg,
Methods
2013; Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg, & Leibenluft, 2014).
Participants
Theorists have argued that one developmental route
to ADHD involves early negative emotionality (Nigg, Recruitment. Women were recruited from a local univer-
Goldsmith, & Sachek, 2004; Sonuga-Barke, Auer- sity-based prenatal care clinic at their 2nd trimester check-up
bach, Campbell, Daley, & Thompson, 2005). Nega- and were screened to over-represent family history of ADHD to
maximize eventual ADHD in the offspring. We enrolled 47
tive emotionality and its initial regulation (via dyadic women through 50 pregnancies (three sibling pairs). Informed
interchange with a parent as well as via orienting of consent was obtained at the time of enrollment, and all
attention) can be reliably assessed in the first years procedures complied with federal guidelines and the local IRB.
of life using well-established parental ratings and Exclusions included high-risk pregnancy (morbid obesity,
observational measures (Buss et al., 2012; Calkins, pre-eclampsia), complicating life circumstances (homeless-
ness, underage status), active substance use (tobacco, mari-
Dedmon, Gill, Lomax, & Johnson, 2002; Rothbart, juana, opioids), comorbid psychiatric conditions (bipolar
Sheese, Rueda, & Posner, 2011). Furthermore, disorder, major depressive disorder), and psychiatric medica-
emerging evidence indicates that the evolution of tion (Adderall, Wellbutrin, Prozac). Two children (one from a
effortful control depends in part on the degree to sibling pair) were excluded after enrollment due to mothers
reporting substantial alcohol or drug use during early gesta-
which negative emotionality is present in early life
tion of those pregnancies. This yielded a final N of 48 offspring
(Posner, Rothbart, Sheese, & Voelker, 2014; Roth- from 46 women.
bart et al., 2011). Thus negative emotionality in the
first year may disrupt subsequent development of Definition of familial ADHD risk. Each parents
effortful control and, by extension, could therefore genetic liability risk for offspring ADHD was coded. To do
contribute as an early marker of risk for ADHD. this, parents reported whether they had ever been diagnosed or
However, differentiation of the negative emotionality treated for ADHD and rated their childhood and current
symptoms from the Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale (BAARS)
domain is theoretically crucial. ADHD may be
screener (Barkley, 2011). The BAARS provides national norms
related, at least in important subgroups of the and percentiles for ratings of childhood and current symptoms
disorder, to alterations in positive as well as negative and impairment. The algorithm for assigning a parent to the
emotionality, but anger/irritability emerges as the risk group was as follows.
principal aspect of negative emotionality in ADHD (1) Childhood: (a) Clear recollection of being diagnosed or
treated with stimulant medication for ADHD for a sustained
(Karalunas et al., 2014; Shaw et al., 2014). More-
period or (b) childhood symptoms (recalled) exceeding 75th
over, initial studies suggest that irritability in percentile (BAARS Screen 7) and (2) Adulthood: BAARS rating
infancy may predict later ADHD (Martel, Nikolas, of 75th percentile or more (8). If no childhood ratings were
Jernigan, Friderici, & Nigg, 2012). available (see below), the adulthood BAARS scores needed to
A family risk design is optimal for evaluating exceed the 80th percentile (BAARS screen 9).
At baseline, ratings were completed by both parents on
liability markers in the early affective domain. This
themselves in 13 of the families, by mother on both herself and
approach was used to study ADHD in pioneering on the father in 10 families, and by mother on herself (with no
studies by Auerbach and colleagues (Auerbach, father data) in 25 families. To address these missing paternal
Atzaba-Poria, Berger, & Landau, 2004). They fol- assessments, paternal self-ratings on the BAARS were
lowed 66 boys from birth and then divided them into obtained at a postnatal behavioral visit. These were collected
in 20 of the 25 missing cases and used to finalize paternal and
high and low risk groups based on high and low
thus familial status. In this way, the 48 motherinfant dyads
levels of paternal symptoms of ADHD. At 7 months were grouped into 10 control pairs and 38 pairs with parental
of age, the paternal risk group showed higher levels ADHD. Of the 38 ADHD risk families, 8 were father only, 14
of anger (elicited by a LABTAB barrier task), were mother only, 11 were both, and 5 were mother ADHD,
although this was not echoed in parent ratings of father unknown in which no reliable information on the
biological father was able to be obtained.
anger [distress to limitations scale on the Infant
Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R) (Auerbach et al.,
2004)]. The same group noted parenting differences Measures
at that age, suggesting that consideration of parent
Parent ADHD symptoms. Current symptom scores
characteristics in relation to child behaviors is were computed using the BAARS-IV current symptom total
important (Landau, Amiel-Laviad, Berger, Atzaba- score for each parent. The highest score from either parent was
Poria, & Auerbach, 2009). used as the dimensional ADHD family score.

2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12426 Infant temperament as marker of ADHD liability 951

Moore, 2014; Moore et al., 2009). Infant behavior was coded


Parent ratings of child temperament. Temperament during the still face and arm restraint tasks in increments of 5-
was measured in two ways. First, mothers rated their infant on s for four mutually exclusive behavioral categories: (a) affective
the 191-item IBQ-R (Rothbart, 1981). Four scales were expression, (b) gaze, (c) vocalization, and (d) reactive/regula-
selected a priori to target negative emotions, irritability, and tory behavior. Affective expression was coded as Positive,
emotion regulation: (a) distress to limitations (anger), (b) fear, Negative, Obscure, or Neutral, and we recorded the percent of
(c) soothability, and (d) recovery from distress. In addition, a the reaction that was negative for analysis. Vocalization was
negative emotion composite score was calculated by taking the coded as None/Not Negative or Negative; we retained the
mean of distress to limitations, fear, sadness, and the inverse percent negative for analysis. Reactive/regulatory behavior
of falling reactivity/rate of recovery from distress, as seen in was coded as Escape, Attention Seeking, Exploratory, or
other studies assessing infant temperament (Clifford et al., Regulatory. We analyzed escape and attention seeking. Table 1
2013). This composite was created primarily as a check on provides operational behavioral definitions. Infant behaviors
parsimony, whereas our principal focus was on the distinction were coded through four separate viewings of the video: one for
between anger, fear, and ability to regulate (soothability and each of the behavioral categories.
recovery) as explained in the introduction.

Reliability. Inter-observer reliability was determined by


Observations of child temperament: over- analyzing percentage agreement and Cohens kappa (Cohen,
view. Motherinfant dyads completed a behavioral assess- 1960). All of the videos were assessed for agreement between
ment when the infant was approximately 6 months old (average the two independent coders. The mean percentage agreement
age 6.7 months). The session lasted about 1.5 hr and was between coders was 91.6% (range 87.4798.91) and the mean
videotaped for later coding. The testing room was equipped with Cohens kappa value was .74 (range .53.81).
a high chair, a chair for the mother, and two cameras (one
focused on the infant, the other on the mother). At the start of the
Data reduction. The percentage of each episode that the
visit, the mother and infant were given 3 min to acclimate to the
infants engaged in each behavior was determined by dividing
new environment without the tester present. Two specific
the total number of intervals when the infant engaged in each
challenge paradigms were coded for the current report.
behavior by the total number of intervals in that episode.
Behavior codings were averaged across two observers to create
Still face paradigm. The well-established still face para- final variables for analysis.
digm (Moore & Calkins, 2004; Moore et al., 2009; Tronick, Als,
Adamson, Wise, & Brazelton, 1978) was used to measure the
infants reaction to the mothers lack of emotional response, Confounds and covariates
expected to elicit fear or sadness. This paradigm consisted of
the following 2-min episodes: normal face-to-face play inter- Demographic information. Demographic information
action, still face interaction, and reunion play interaction. The (Table 2) was obtained at enrollment via maternal report.
mother placed the infant in a high chair and sat in a chair Parent education was coded from 0 to 7 based on highest grade
about an arms length away. During the normal play period, completed for each parent. Maternal and paternal ages were
the mother was instructed to play with the infant as she recorded at the time of enrollment. The birth date and sex of
normally would. The mother was then instructed to turn her the infant were obtained at sample collection on the day of
face away from the infant for 15 s and then turn back to face birth or through the medical records.
the infant for the still face period, during which the mother
maintained a neutral and expressionless face and did not Maternal body mass index (BMI). Weight and height
interact with the infant. The mother was then instructed to were obtained from medical records and used to calculate
look away again for 15 s and then return to interacting and maternal BMI prior to pregnancy and during the second
playing with the infant as she normally would. If the infant trimester.
became persistently upset, the still face period was stopped
early, and the pair moved into the reunion period. Pacifiers and
other toys were not allowed during this task. Five infants data
Maternal nutrition. Dietary data were collected using 24-
hr recalls conducted by research dietitians using the multipass
were coded as missing because the mother interacted with the
method. These 24-hr recalls were completed in the 2nd and
infant during the still face period.
3rd trimester of pregnancy over 2 or 3 days, with one of the
days being a weekend day. The 24-hr recall data were used to
Arm restraint paradigm. The arm restraint task (Cal- compute the Healthy Eating Index (HEI)-2010, as a measure of
kins et al., 2002; Stifter & Braungart, 1995) immediately overall maternal diet quality (Guenther et al., 2013). A higher
followed the still face paradigm and was designed to measure score indicates higher conformity to USDA recommendations.
the infants response to a confining situation (distress to
limitations or anger/irritability). During this task, the mother
was instructed to avert her gaze and not interact with her child
Maternal substance use. Substance use was deter-
mined via self-report on the Achenbach Adult Self-Report (ASR)
while gently holding the infants arms to his or her sides for
questionnaire (Achenback & Rescorla, 2003) during the 2nd
1 min. The mother then released the infants arms and was
and 3rd trimesters. The ASR includes questions that ask about
instructed to play and interact with her child normally for
tobacco (number of times per day), alcohol (number of days the
2 min. If the infant became persistently distressed during arm
respondent was drunk), and nonmedical drug (number of days
restraint, staff would direct the mother to move into the period
of use) usage during the past 6 months.
of normal play. Pacifiers and other comfort items were not
allowed during this task. Four infants data were coded as
missing because the mother interacted with the infant during Parental stress. The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) (Ezzati
the arm restraint period. et al., 2014) was used to obtain self-report of stress level
during the pregnancy. The average of 2nd and 3rd trimester
Behavioral coding. Two observers blinded to familial scores served as the outcome measure.
ADHD status coded each behavioral assessment video. Infants
behaviors were coded using a modified version of a published Maternal mood. Maternal depression was assessed with
coding scheme (Holochwost, Gariepy, Propper, Mills-Koonce, & the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CESD)

2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


952 Elinor L. Sullivan et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2015; 56(9): 94957

Table 1 Operational definitions for behavioral coding p < .20 in univariate analyses. These included maternal pre-
natal depression symptoms on the CESD (r = .27, p = .08),
Measure Behavior Description maternal prenatal stress level from the PSS (r = .52, p < .01),
and maternal education (r = .32, p = .046). Current maternal
Affective Positive Infants corners of mouth raised depression was also included as a covariate due to its obvious
expression and/or cheeks raised importance as a potential explanation for infant negative
Negative Infants brows may be sharply affect.
lowered and eyes may be tightly
closed. This code includes anger,
sadness, and frustration Data analysis
Obscure Infants mouth or face hidden
from view for the entire interval Statistical analyses were conducted using the MPLUS Editor
Neutral Infant displaying a relaxed face Version 7.3 (Muth en & Muth en, Los Angeles, CA). The effects of
with no obvious muscle tension family history were evaluated by computing zero order point-
Affective None/Not Infant is not vocalizing or biserial correlations with family history of ADHD. Significant
vocalization Negative vocalizations are not indicative of correlations were examined with linear regression with infant
being fussy or upset. This code behavior as the outcome and family ADHD group as the
includes silence, cooing, predictor, with covariates as discussed above. Regression
laughing, babbling, coughing, coefficients were fully standardized and are reported with their
and sneezing standard error of the mean (SEM), allowing estimation of a 95%
Negative Infant is displaying negative confidence interval, and traditional p-value. As noted earlier,
communication such as fussing, two sibling pairs were included; nonindependence was con-
crying, screaming (i.e. if upset or trolled for using the MPLUS CLUSTER procedure. All p-value
angry), and other expressions of tests are two-tailed. For correlations, we report both uncor-
mild fussiness rected p-values (due to the small sample size and preliminary
Reactive/ Escape Infant is attempting to get out of nature of the study) and Bonferroni-corrected p-values. Miss-
regulatory the chair, often accompanied by ing data varied across measures, but for all multivariate
behavior negative vocalizations. Examples models the covariance coverage matrix exceeded .50 at all
include twisting, and struggling data points and so the data matrix easily fell within the
back and forth tolerance level of the maximum likelihood procedure (Enders,
Attention Infant is trying to get the parents 2010). To maximize precision of the missing data matrix,
seeking attention when the parent is not regressions were modeled using auxiliary variables from the
engaging with the infant. dataset (i.e. all variables reported in this paper served as
Examples include exaggerated auxiliaries when not in the model), within the full information
vocalizations or limbic movement maximum likelihood environment (Graham, 2003). This pro-
while the gaze is directed toward cedure uses all available data to estimate parameter values in
the parent the presence of missing data (Enders, 2010). Auxiliary vari-
ables included in analysis were paternal education (maternal
education was a covariate); maternal and paternal age;
maternal and paternal ADHD (BAARS) scores; sex of child;
(Roberts & Vernon, 1983) scale at the 2nd and 3rd trimester, IBQ-R scales not in the dependent variable list (distress, fear,
which were averaged to form one prenatal depressed mood falling reactivity/rate of recovery from distress and negative
measure. The CESD was completed again by mothers at the emotion composite); maternal nutritional quality; mater-
6 month time point. nal prepregnancy BMI; and age of child at assessment.
For regressions, the two observational measures included in
Selection of covariates. Covariates were included in the Table 4 served as auxiliaries as well when not serving as the
model if they were associated with family history of ADHD at dependent variable.

Table 2 Demographic characteristics of control and ADHD families

Control (n = 10) Familial ADHD (n = 38)


Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p-Value

Maternal age at birth (years) 30.9 (4.7) 30.7 (5.0) .926


Paternal age at birth (years) 33.6 (7.6) 33.7 (6.4) .966
Prenatal maternal BMI (kg/m2) 27.6 (4.4) 28.5 (7.0) .747
Gestational age at delivery (days) 281.6 (8.6) 277.3 (7.0) .158
Healthy Eating Index 61.5 (7.4) 63.0 (7.6) .616
Tobacco use during pregnancy (days) 0.0 (0) 0.3 (1.0) .454
Drunkenness during pregnancy (days) 0.0 (0) 0.1 (0.4) .314
Drug use during pregnancy (days) 0.0 (0) 0.0 (0)

Percent Percent p-Value

Infant male sex 50.0 63.2 .449


Divorced families 0.0 6.1 .449
Families with a single adult 22.2 18.2 .784
Maternal bachelors degree or higher 90.0 60.5 .079
Paternal bachelors degree or higher 75.0 63.6 .543

2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12426 Infant temperament as marker of ADHD liability 953

Results Regression models with all nominally significant


The zero order point-biserial correlations for all candi- measures, along with the covariates noted earlier,
date indicators of infant liability are listed in Table 3. were then evaluated. Those results are displayed in
Uncorrected p-values are reported along with p-values Table 4. Three of the indices remained significant
that are Bonferroni-corrected within the measurement after controlling for maternal prenatal and postnatal
domain (rating or observation) and a second value that mood, prenatal stress, and maternal education level;
is corrected across all 12 correlations in the table. Five IBQ-R fear effects were explained by the correlation
specific indicators had nominal associations of of ADHD liability with maternal stress during preg-
p < .05; three of these survived the local Bonferroni nancy, and IBQ negative affect was explained by
correction: IBQ-R distress to limitations, our proxy for maternal postnatal depressed mood. The combined
anger/irritability (higher in infants with familial results suggest that the most robust indicators of
ADHD), IBQ-R fear (higher in infants with familial early risk of ADHD are increased IBQ-R scores of
ADHD), and still face attention seeking, which was our distress to limitations and increased negative vocal-
indicator of adaptive coping or regulation (lower in izations during arm restraint; these converge con-
infants with familial ADHD). Figure 1A displays the ceptually (as indices of anger/irritability).
increase in negative vocalizations during arm restraint Two sets of secondary analyses were conducted.
in infants with familial history of ADHD, and Figure 1B First, we considered parental current ADHD symp-
displays the reduction in attention seeking during still toms alone for mother, father, and combined (high-
face in infants with a familial history of ADHD to est) in simple correlations following what is in
provide an illustration of the data. Table 3. Only one correlation was reliable (increased

Table 3 Correlations of family group with candidate infant liability markers of ADHD risk at age 6 months

Bonferroni-corrected p values

Behavioral measure r SE p Local Global (*12)

IBQ-R scales (corrected locally at p value *5)


IBQ-R distress to limits (anger) .592 .215 .006* .030* .072
IBQ-R fear .454 .137 .001* .005* .012*
IBQ-R soothability .067 .248 .787
IBQ-R rate of recovery to distress .025 .290 .932
IBQ-R negative affect composite .522 .229 .023* ns ns
Observational measures (corrected locally at p value *8)
AR negative affect .220 .212 .300
SF negative affect .035 .219 .870
AR negative vocalizations .443 .224 .048* ns ns
SF negative vocalizations .026 .221 .907
Attention seeking during AR .553 .581 .341
Attention seeking during SF .631 .149 <.001* <.008* <.012*
Escape behavior during AR .179 .225 .426
Escape behavior during SF .014 .195 .941

IBQ-R, Infant Behavior Questionnaire; AR, arm restraint task; SF, still face paradigm.
Correlations were derived by computing individual point-biserial correlations one by one, in separate models. Each model included
the list of auxiliary variables: maternal and paternal age, education, and current ADHD score on the BAARS-IV, age of child at
assessment, sex of child, maternal BMI, maternal nutrition, and all IBQ-R and behavior variables shown in the table. Auxiliary
variables are not covariates. Bonferroni correction within this table was conducted in two ways: locally within measurement type,
and globally across all measures in the table.

(A) Negative vocalizations during (B) Attention seeking behavior


arm restraint during still face
Percentage of observation
Percentage of observation

80 40

*
60 30

40 20

20 10 *

0 0
Control Familial ADHD Control Familial ADHD
ADHD Status ADHD Status

Figure 1 Infants with a familial history of ADHD show increased negative vocalizations during the arm restraint paradigm (p = .004) (A),
as well as decreased attention-seeking behavior during the still face paradigm (p < .001) (B). Data are presented as the mean  SEM.

2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


954 Elinor L. Sullivan et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2015; 56(9): 94957

Table 4 Regression modeling of the association of infant


emotion/irritability behavioral markers with ADHD familial Discussion
history after controlling for covariates Identification of early precursors for ADHD is of
central importance because early intervention could
Coefficient SE p-Value be more successful than interventions after estab-
Outcome 1: IBQ-R distress to limitations (irritability/anger) lishment of ADHD. The present study examined a set
Family risk group .378 .180 .036* of conceptually relevant emotional temperament indi-
Maternal education .344 .164 .036* cators in 6-month-old infants with a parental history
Prenatal stress .211 .272 .438
of ADHD. These results should be understood as
Prenatal depression .182 .363 .616
Postnatal depression .499 .230 .030* preliminary due to the small nature of the sample and
Outcome 2: IBQ-R fear in particular the small control sample, which limits
Family risk group .261 .180 .146 power and increases the risk of nonreproducibility.
Maternal education .119 .149 .425 With that important caution, intriguing findings
Prenatal stress .945 .361 .009*
emerged that may guide further work in this area.
Prenatal depression .875 .453 .053
Postnatal depression .302 .239 .206 Laboratory observation and parental rating were
Outcome 3: IBQ-R negative affect composite used to examine measures of negative affectivity and
Family risk group .266 .194 .170 its components of anger/distress versus fear (in
Maternal education .157 .175 .371 hopes of capturing affect related to irritability as it
Prenatal stress .072 .472 .878
appears later in ADHD), as well as early indices of
Prenatal depression .286 .518 .581
Postnatal depression .590 .270 .029* emotional self-regulation in the form of attention
Outcome 4: Negative vocalizations during AR (irritability/ seeking and escape behaviors during two challenge
anger) tasks. The most robust results were replicated across
Family risk group .452 .158 .004* ratings and observations, and were not accounted for
Maternal education .345 .179 .054
by key covariates including maternal prenatal stress
Prenatal stress .426 .353 .228
Prenatal depression .213 .431 .621 and prenatal and concurrent depressed mood.
Postnatal depression .688 .287 .017* The most notable result appears to be in the domain
Outcome 5: Attention seeking during SF (positive coping/ of infant anger/irritability indicated by higher mater-
regulation) nal ratings on the distress to limitations scale of the
Family risk group .562 .142 <.001*
IBQ-R and the laboratory observation of negative
Maternal education .177 .142 .213
Prenatal stress .401 .257 .118 vocalizations during arm restraint. These measures
Prenatal depression .505 .258 .050 are conceptually similar to the clinical construct of
Postnatal depression .133 .137 .333 irritability in older children which is defined by the
Research Domain Criteria as a metric of frustration to
Family risk group 1 = control, 2 = risk, so a positive coefficient
nonreward (Leibenluft & Stoddard, 2013). This find-
means more of the behavior in the risk group. Prenatal stress is
maternal rating on the Perceived Stress Scale averaged across ing also converges with a key finding from Auerbach
2nd and 3rd trimester. Prenatal depression is maternal C-ESD and colleagues (Auerbach et al., 2004).
rating averaged across 2nd and 3rd trimester. Postnatal Infants with a familial history of risk for ADHD also
depression is maternal CESD score at the 6 month infant exhibited reduced self-regulation strategies. One
follow up visit. Auxiliary variables were maternal and paternal
indicator was a reduction in attention-seeking
age, current ADHD score on the BAARS-IV, and paternal
education (maternal education is in the model), age of child at behavior toward their mothers during the still face
assessment, sex of child, maternal BMI, maternal nutrition, paradigm. A decrease in parent-orienting behavior
and all IBQ-R and behavior variables in this table that were not during the still face paradigm was previously asso-
in the particular model. Auxiliary variables are not covariates ciated with infant negative affect (Braungart-Rieker,
but reduce the error term by improving precision of the missing
Garwood, Powers, & Notaro, 1998). Also, consistent
data matrix.
with our findings, boys with ADHD show impair-
maternal ADHD symptoms was associated with ments in recognizing and responding to facial emo-
decreased infant escape behaviors during still face; tions (Yuill & Lyon, 2007). Deficits in self-regulation
full results available on request), suggesting in are a common feature in a substantial number of
general limited effect of parental current symptoms children with ADHD, and the behavior seen here may
compared to parental lifetime ADHD status. be an early indicator in that domain (Anastopoulos
Second, we examined the effects of paternal versus et al., 2011; Martel, 2009).
maternal ADHD status separately (based on lifetime Effects for regulatory strategies, however, were not
group assignment). These effects (available on echoed in maternal report of infant behavior in the
request from the authors) were generally weak. Only IBQ-R. Maternal ratings are dependent on the childs
maternal diagnosis with lower escape behaviors home environment, which is likely to be influenced by
during still face survived Bonferroni correction; no parental ADHD diagnosis. In contrast, the testing
effects survived covariate control. This suggested procedure and environment is highly controlled in
that familial transmission was not accounted for by laboratory observations of infant temperament,
one parent but rather was emanating to some extent which may explain why differences in self-regulatory
from both parents. strategies were not detected in maternal report.

2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12426 Infant temperament as marker of ADHD liability 955

Although study groups were relatively well matched (Nigg, 2006a, 2006b). Familial transmission is likely
on most demographic variables, familial history of a complex interaction between environmental,
ADHD was associated with prenatal exposure to genetic, and epigenetic factors that contribute to
higher maternal stress and depression and marginally the heterogeneity in ADHD symptomatology (Archer,
lower maternal education. Elevated prenatal stress Oscar-Berman, & Blum, 2011).
has been documented to disrupt emotional regulation In the current study, differences between infants
and impair offspring cognition (Buss et al., 2012; with and without family history of ADHD remained
Sandman, Davis, Buss, & Glynn, 2012), so controlling after covariates such as maternal stress and mood
for this measure was extremely important. Interest- were controlled, suggesting a genetic transmission of
ingly, maternal report of stress during gestation was liability. Further, the familial status based on lifetime
associated with increased infant fear, consistent with parent assignment (including parental child and
previous findings (Davis, Glynn, Waffarn, & Sand- adult symptom reports) provided robust prediction
man, 2011; Sandman et al., 2012). It is important that of infant temperament, whereas current parental
future studies of infant behavior include additional symptoms did not. This also suggests transmission
measures of maternal stress responsivity and further of genetic liability rather than shared environment.
examine the influence of maternal stress on infant However, the present study lacked power to examine
behavior. We did not examine other maternal behav- interactions between family risk and early experience,
iors, attachment, or overall parentchild relationship such as prenatal maternal stress, nutrition or early
quality. These will be important to consider in further caregiving. These types of interactions are very likely
work as mediators and moderators of the effects seen. to be important for individual differences in outcome
The assessment of ADHD in the parents was and need to be examined in a larger population to
limited by reliance on maternal ratings of father for clarify developmental processes of transmission. The
several cases, by absence of informants who knew study also lacked power to consider sex-specific
the parents when they were children, and by absence transmission (e.g. from fathers to sons, as studied
of a structured clinical interview of parents. Thus, by Auerbach et al., 2004); this will be another inter-
these are families with high suspicion but not esting future target.
definitive ADHD. Likewise, an important caution is In conclusion, these findings, while preliminary,
that we studied ADHD liability; only some of these provide promising suggestion that measures of neg-
children will later develop ADHD. Indeed, work of ative emotionality, particularly those related to irri-
this nature in other disorders, such as autism, has tability and its regulation, may provide promising
identified apparent liability markers in infancy that indices for early detection of ADHD. Continuing to
fail to predict later disorder (Jones, Gliga, Bedford, follow these infants while increasing the sample size
Charman, & Johnson, 2014). Therefore, it is impor- is a logical next step to evaluate this proposal further.
tant to reserve judgment on the meaning of these
infant findings until we are able to evaluate children
at 3 and 4 years of age or older to determine the Acknowledgements
forward predictive utility of infant biomarkers in This original article was invited by the journal as part of
predicting emerging ADHD diagnosis. a special issue; it has undergone full, external peer
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is familial review. This work was supported by the Abracadabra
and heritable (Stawicki et al., 2006). Evidence from Foundation. The authors have declared that they have
no competing or potential conflicts of interest in relation
twin studies estimate heritability of about 70% (Burt,
to the work reported.
2009; Faraone et al., 2005). ADHD risk can be
transmitted from parents to offspring via three
fundamental mechanisms: genetics, shared environ-
Correspondence
ment, and developmental programming via epige-
Joel T. Nigg, Oregon Health and Science University,
netic modifications. It is plausible that parents 3550 SW US Veterans Hospital Road, Mail Code: DC7P,
transmit liability or susceptibility to ADHD, which Portland, OR 97239, USA; Email: niggj@ohsu.edu
is then presumably activated by early experiences

Key Points
Seeks to identify indicators of ADHD family liability at age 6 months that may predict later ADHD risk.
Parental history of probable ADHD was associated with both rated and observed distress to limitations in
infants, suggesting that an irritability-like phenotype is a marker of ADHD liability.
We provide preliminary evidence that differences in emotional regulation are detectable in infancy and may
be important biomarkers for early identification of ADHD.

2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


956 Elinor L. Sullivan et al. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2015; 56(9): 94957

Stress Scale in a community sample of older adults.


References International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 29, 645652.
Achenback, T.M., & Rescorla, L.A. (2003). Manual for the Faraone, S.V., Perlis, R.H., Doyle, A.E., Smoller, J.W.,
ASEBA adult forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Goralnick, J.J., Holmgren, M.A., & Sklar, P. (2005).
Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Anastopoulos, A.D., Smith, T.F., Garrett, M.E., Morrissey- disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 13131323.
Kane, E., Schatz, N.K., Sommer, J.L., . . . & Ashley-Koch, A. Graham, J.W. (2003). Adding missing-data relevant variables
(2011). Self-regulation of emotion, functional impairment, to FIMLbased structural equation models. Structural
and comorbidity among children with AD/HD. Journal of Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 10, 80100.
Attention Disorders, 15, 583592. Guenther, P.M., Casavale, K.O., Reedy, J., Kirkpatrick, S.I.,
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Hiza, H.A., Kuczynski, K.J., . . . & Krebs-Smith, S.M. (2013).
statistical manual of mental disorders DSM-5 (5th edn). Update of the Healthy Eating Index: HEI-2010. Journal of
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 113, 569580.
Archer, T., Oscar-Berman, M., & Blum, K. (2011). Epigenetics Holochwost, S.J., Gariepy, J.L., Propper, C.B., Mills-Koonce,
in developmental disorder: ADHD and endophenotypes. W.R., & Moore, G.A. (2014). Parenting behaviors and vagal
Journal of Genetic Syndromes & Gene Therapy, 2, pii: tone at six months predict attachment disorganization at
1000104. twelve months. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 1423
Arnett, A.B., Macdonald, B., & Pennington, B.F. (2013). 1430.
Cognitive and behavioral indicators of ADHD symptoms Johnson, M.H., Gliga, T., Jones, E., & Charman, T. (2015).
prior to school age. Journal of Child Psychology and Annual Research Review: Infant development, autism, and
Psychiatry, 54, 12841294. ADHD - early pathways to emerging disorders. Journal of
Auerbach, J.G., Atzaba-Poria, N., Berger, A., & Landau, R. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 228247.
(2004). Emerging developmental pathways to ADHD: Jones, E.J., Gliga, T., Bedford, R., Charman, T., & Johnson,
Possible path markers in early infancy. Neural Plasticity, M.H. (2014). Developmental pathways to autism: A review of
11, 2943. prospective studies of infants at risk. Neuroscience and
Barkley, R.A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained Biobehavioral Reviews, 39, 133.
attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying Kan, K.J., Dolan, C.V., Nivard, M.G., Middeldorp, C.M., van
theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 6594. Beijsterveldt, C.E., Willemsen, G., & Boomsma, D.I. (2013).
Barkley, R.A. (2011). Barkley adult ADHD rating scale-IV Genetic and environmental stability in attention problems
(BAARS-IV). New York: Guilford Press. across the lifespan: Evidence from the Netherlands twin
Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M.M., Powers, B.P., & Notaro, register. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
P.C. (1998). Infant affect and affect regulation during the still- Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 1225.
face paradigm with mothers and fathers: The role of infant Karalunas, S.L., Fair, D., Musser, E.D., Aykes, K., Iyer, S.P., &
characteristics and parental sensitivity. Developmental Nigg, J.T. (2014). Subtyping attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Psychology, 34, 14281437. disorder using temperament dimensions: Toward
Burt, S.A. (2009). Rethinking environmental contributions to biologically based nosologic criteria. JAMA Psychiatry, 71,
child and adolescent psychopathology: A meta-analysis of 10151024.
shared environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, Landau, R., Amiel-Laviad, R., Berger, A., Atzaba-Poria, N., &
135, 608637. Auerbach, J.G. (2009). Parenting of 7-month-old infants at
Buss, C., Davis, E.P., Shahbaba, B., Pruessner, J.C., Head, K., familial risk for ADHD during infants free play, with
& Sandman, C.A. (2012). Maternal cortisol over the course of restrictions on interaction. Infant Behavior & Development,
pregnancy and subsequent child amygdala and 32, 173182.
hippocampus volumes and affective problems. Proceedings Larsson, H., Chang, Z., DOnofrio, B.M., & Lichtenstein, P.
of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 109, E1312 (2013). The heritability of clinically diagnosed attention
E1319. deficit hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Psycho-
Calkins, S.D., Dedmon, S., Gill, K., Lomax, L., & Johnson, L. logical Medicine, 44, 17.
(2002). Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion Leibenluft, E., & Stoddard, J. (2013). The developmental
regulation, physiological processes, and temperament. psychopathology of irritability. Development and Psychopa-
Infancy, 3, 175198. thology, 25, 14731487.
Castellanos, F.X., Lee, P.P., Sharp, W., Jeffries, N.O., Martel, M.M. (2009). Research review: A new perspective on
Greenstein, D.K., Clasen, L.S., . . . & Rapoport, J.L. (2002). attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Emotion
Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities dysregulation and trait models. Journal of Child
in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyper- Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 10421051.
activity disorder. JAMA, 288, 17401748. Martel, M.M., & Nigg, J.T. (2006). Child ADHD and
Clifford, S.M., Hudry, K., Elsabbagh, M., Charman, T., personality/temperament traits of reactive and effortful
Johnson, M.H., & Team, T.B. (2013). Temperament in the control, resiliency, and emotionality. Journal of Child
first 2 years of life in infants at high-risk for autism Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 11751183.
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Martel, M.M., Nikolas, M., Jernigan, K., Friderici, K., & Nigg,
Disorders, 43, 673686. J.T. (2012). Diversity in pathways to common childhood
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 3746. Psychology, 40, 12231236.
Davis, E.P., Glynn, L.M., Waffarn, F., & Sandman, C.A. Moore, G.A., & Calkins, S.D. (2004). Infants vagal regulation
(2011). Prenatal maternal stress programs infant stress in the still-face paradigm is related to dyadic coordination of
regulation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, mother-infant interaction. Developmental Psychology, 40,
119129. 10681080.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Moore, G.A., Hill-Soderlund, A.L., Propper, C.B., Calkins, S.D.,
Psychology, 64, 135168. Mills-Koonce, W.R., & Cox, M.J. (2009). Mother-infant vagal
Enders, C.K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York: regulation in the face-to-face still-face paradigm is moderated
Guiford. by maternal sensitivity. Child Development, 80, 209223.
Ezzati, A., Jiang, J., Katz, M.J., Sliwinski, M.J., Zimmerman, Musser, E.D., Galloway-Long, H.S., Frick, P.J., & Nigg, J.T.
M.E., & Lipton, R.B. (2014). Validation of the Perceived (2013). Emotion regulation and heterogeneity in

2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12426 Infant temperament as marker of ADHD liability 957

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the Shaw, P., Lerch, J., Greenstein, D., Sharp, W., Clasen, L.,
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, Evans, A., . . . & Rapoport, J. (2006). Longitudinal mapping
163171 e162. of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and
Musser, E.D., Hawkey, E., Kachan-Liu, S.S., Lees, P., Roullet, adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
J.B., Goddard, K., . . . & Nigg, J.T. (2014). Shared familial Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 540549.
transmission of autism spectrum and attention-deficit/ Shaw, P., Stringaris, A., Nigg, J., & Leibenluft, E. (2014).
hyperactivity disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity
Psychiatry, 55, 819827. disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 276293.
Nigg, J.T. (2006a). Temperament and developmental Sonuga-Barke, E.J., Auerbach, J., Campbell, S.B., Daley, D.,
psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and & Thompson, M. (2005). Varieties of preschool hyperactivity:
Psychiatry, 47, 395422. Multiple pathways from risk to disorder. Developmental
Nigg, J.T. (2006b). What causes ADHD?: Understanding what Science, 8, 141150.
goes wrong and why. New York: Guilford Press. Stawicki, J.A., Nigg, J.T., & von Eye, A. (2006). Family
Nigg, J.T., Goldsmith, H.H., & Sachek, J. (2004). Temperament psychiatric history evidence on the nosological relations of
and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The DSM-IV ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes: New
development of a multiple pathway model. Journal of data and meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 4253. Psychiatry, 47, 935945.
Posner, M.I., Rothbart, M.K., Sheese, B.E., & Voelker, P. (2014). Stifter, C.A., & Braungart, J.M. (1995). The regulation of
Developing attention: Behavioral and brain mechanisms. negative reactivity in infancy: Function and development.
Advances in Neuroscience (Hindawi), 2014, 405094. Developmental Psychology, 31, 448455.
Roberts, R.E., & Vernon, S.W. (1983). The center for Tronick, E.Z., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, B.
epidemiologic studies depression scale: Its use in a (1978). The infants response to entrapment between
community sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction.
4146. American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1, 113.
Rothbart, M.K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in Yuill, N., & Lyon, J. (2007). Selective difficulty in recognising
infancy. Child Development, 52, 569578. facial expressions of emotion in boys with ADHD. General
Rothbart, M.K., Sheese, B.E., Rueda, M.R., & Posner, M.I. performance impairments or specific problems in social
(2011). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early cognition? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 16,
life. Emotion Review, 3, 207213. 398404.
Sandman, C.A., Davis, E.P., Buss, C., & Glynn, L.M. (2012).
Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts Accepted for publication: 13 April 2015
programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Published online: 12 May 2015
Neuroendocrinology, 95, 721.

2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

Potrebbero piacerti anche