Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

MIND, BRAIN, AND EDUCATION

SleepWake Cycle, Daytime


Sleepiness, and Attention
Components in Children
Attending Preschool
in the Morning and Afternoon
Shifts
Aline S. Belsio1 , Fernanda F. Kolodiuk1 , Fernando M. Louzada2 , Pablo Valdez3 , and
Carolina V. M. Azevedo1

ABSTRACT Children tend to sleep and wake up early and


to exhibit daytime sleep episodes. To evaluate the impact of
school start times on sleepiness and attention in preschool The sleepwake cycle plays an important role in the cog-
children, this study compared the temporal patterns of nitive processes of learning, such as attention (Gillberg
sleep, daytime sleepiness, and the components of attention & Akerstedt, 1998; Thomas et al., 2000; Valdez, Reilly, &
between children aged 46 years that study in the morning Waterhouse, 2008) and memory consolidation (Drosopou-
(n = 66) and the afternoon (n = 144) shifts. The former get los, Schulze, Fischer, & Born, 2007; Wilhelm, Diekelmann, &
up 1 hr and 30 min earlier on weekdays and show lower e- Born, 2008), in addition to contributing to restoring phys-
ciency on the sustained attention task than those who study iological and biochemical processes (Benington & Heller,
in the afternoon. Thus, the morning shift was associated with 1995; Ramm & Smith, 1990) and maintaining energy bal-
a reduction in nighttime sleep, which might have a nega- ance (Spiegel, Tasali, Penev, & Van Cauter, 2004; Taheri,
tive eect on childrens performance in the morning, causing Lin, Austin, Young, & Mignot, 2004; Taveras, Rifas-Shiman,
higher levels of daytime sleepiness and a decline in sustained Oken, Gunderson, & Gillman, 2008).
attention. Because only one parameter of one component of Sleep has an essential function in cognitive and motor
attention was negatively aected, further studies are needed development, especially in children, who are still maturing.
to conrm this eect on cognition. A number of changes inherent to this age group occur in
the sleepwake cycle as a result of the maturation process
of the nervous system. Children tend to go to bed and
wake up early; that is, they have a tendency to morningness
(Jenni & Carskadon, 2005). Total sleep duration declines
progressively with age, along with an increase in nighttime
1 Laboratrio de Cronobiologia, Programa de Ps-Graduao em Psi- sleep and a decrease in daytime sleep episodes (naps), which
cobiologia, Departmento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal do Rio are frequent in young children (Acebo et al., 2005; Jenni &
Grande do Norte
2 Departmento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal do Paran Carskadon, 2005; Louzada, Orsoni, Mello, Benedito-Silva,
3 Laboratrio de Psicosiologia, Departmento de Psicologia, Universi- & Menna-Barreto, 1996; Menna-Barreto, Isola, Louzada,
dad Autnoma de Nuevo Len Benedito-Silva, & Mello, 1996).
Address correspondence to Carolina V.M. Azevedo, Caixa Postal 1511, The sleepwake cycle is inuenced by a series of peculiari-
CEP 59078-970, Natal, RN, Brazil; e-mail: carolina@cb.ufrn.br ties in the childs social environment, such as behavior before

2016 International Mind, Brain, and Education Society and Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 1
Sleep, Sleepiness, and Attention in Children

(drinking milk, going to the bathroom, listening to stories, tonic alertness, phasic alertness, selective attention, and sus-
and use of favorite object) and during sleep (co-sleeping tained attention. Tonic alertness is the ability to respond to
or bed-sharing). These sleep patterns change when chil- changes in the environment; phasic alertness is the ability to
dren start school and the school start times modulate the respond to environmental changes preceded by a warning
sleepwake cycle, which may lead to daytime sleepiness and signal; selective attention is the ability to process a sensory
compromise academic performance (Beltramini & Hertzig, event and ignore other events; and sustained attention is the
1983; Li et al., 2008; Reimo, Souza, Medeiros, & Almiro, ability to respond to the environment for a certain interval
1998; Reimo et al., 1999; Silva et al., 2005). of time.
Despite exhibiting a tendency to morningness, children Attention may be modulated by time of day (Valdez,
between 3 and 10 years of age who study in the morning and Ramrez, Garca, Talamantes, & Cortez, 2010) and sleep
wake up and go to bed earlier sleep less during the week than deprivation (Rodrguez, 2011), which leads to a decline in
on the weekend (Adam, Snell, & Pendry, 2007; Belsio, 2010; attention levels and may compromise school learning. With
El-Sheikh, Buckhalt, Mize, & Acebo, 2006; Louzada et al., the changes in current lifestyles, children go to sleep late on
1996; Mello, Isola, Louzada, & Menna-Barreto, 1996; Silva weekdays and wake up early to go to school. This results
et al., 2005). Moreover, children aged between 7 and 10 years in partial deprivation and irregular sleeping hours and, in
who study in the morning show greater frequency of naps, in turn, daytime sleepiness, primarily in children who study in
addition to sleeping approximately 1 hr more than children the morning. However, the relationship between the com-
who study in the afternoon (Silva et al., 2005). Thus, children ponents of attention, sleep pattern, and daytime sleepiness
who study in the morning experience acute sleep deprivation in children aged between 4 and 6 years remains unknown,
on weekdays that is generally compensated for on weekends. as is the eect of school start times on these parameters. The
An increase of around 2629 min in time spent in bed during present study aimed to compare sleep habits, daytime sleepi-
the weekend was observed in children aged between 4 and ness, and the components of attention in preschool children
6 (Belsio, 2010) and 8 and 10 years (Anacleto, 2011). This as a function of school shift (morning and afternoon).
slight dierence between school days and weekends is likely
caused by a natural predisposition to go to sleep and wake
up early, characteristic of this age group, and the inuence METHODS
of parents in determining the sleepwake schedule of their
children (Iglowstein, Jenni, Molinari, & Largo, 2003; Meijer, The study was approved by the Onofre Lopes University
Habekoth, & Van Den Wittenboer, 2000). Hospital Research Ethics Committee (CEP/HUOL protocol
The shorter nighttime sleep and irregular sleep times no. 554/11). The sample consisted of 210 children of both
related to the morning shift would not be expected in chil- sexes from the morning (n = 66) and afternoon (n = 144)
dren, who naturally wake up earlier. However, in addition to shifts, aged between 4 and 6 years, from the city of Natal,
causing changes in sleep patterns, school start time may lead Brazil (Latitude: 5 47 42 S and longitude: 35 12 34
to high levels of daytime sleepiness (Silva et al., 2005) and low W). Seven private schools took part in the research. At all
attention levels (Valdez et al., 2008), which may negatively schools, morning classes start at 7 a.m. and nish at 11 a.m.,
interfere with the childs learning process. while afternoon classes are between 1 p.m. and 5 p.m.
Among the cognitive processes aected by sleep depriva- The following inclusion criteria were adopted: preschool
tion we highlight the attention, which is the ability to select children (1) of both sexes aged between 4 and 6 years, (2)
and respond specically to a stimulus in the environment for enrolled in private schools in Natal, who study in the morn-
a certain period of time. This cognitive process can be stud- ing or afternoon shift, and (3) whose parents or guardians
ied using neuropsychological models (Cohen & ODonnell, gave their informed consent.
1993; Mirsky, Anthony, Duncan, Ahearn, & Kellam, 1991; The exclusion criteria were (1) sleep disturbance or any
Posner & Rafal, 1987; Valdez et al., 2005), which makes it health problem, such as allergies, sinusitis, hypertension or
possible to analyze the brain areas involved, as well as ver- neurological disorders, and (2) incomplete questionnaires.
ify the presence of circadian rhythms in the dierent atten- Sleep habits were characterized using the sleep habits
tion components and the impact of sleep deprivation in this questionnaire applied by Wey (2002), to which were added
process. questions regarding bedtime rituals (Owens, Spirito, &
The model used in the present study was proposed by McGuinn, 2000) and information about parents schooling
Valdez et al. (2005) and is based on the Posner and Rafal level.
(1987) model, with some changes, namely in dening atten- Economic level was characterized using a questionnaire
tion components and assessing each component of the atten- adapted from the model proposed by the Brazilian Economic
tion process. In the model proposed by Valdez et al. (2005), Classication Criterion (CCEB) of the National Association
attention is divided into four components denominated of Research Companies (Associao Nacional de Empresas

2
Aline S. Belsio et al.

de Pesquisa, 2008). This classication straties the popula- computer screen for 200 ms. There were a total of 100 stim-
tion into eight economic classes (A1, A2, B1, B2, C1, C2, D, uli: 70 green hearts, 20 red circles, and 10 blue stars (each
and E), based on responses related to possession of goods blue star was preceded by an arrow). The images were dis-
and the head of the familys schooling level. Classication in played randomly. Responses were recorded on a keyboard.
points makes it possible to infer mean family income. The child had to press key 1 when a green heart appeared,
An adapted version of the sleep diary (Wey, 2002) was key 2 when a red circle appeared, and key 3 when a blue
used to characterize sleep pattern for 9 consecutive days star appeared. Each key was covered with an image sticker
(from Friday night to Monday morning). The following out- related to the requested response: key 1 with a green heart,
comes were obtained from the sleep diary: (1) sleep at night key 2 with a red circle, and key 3 with a blue star. A 100-ms
(bedtimes, wake up times, and time in bed), (2) daytime naps feedback sound (beep) was presented after each correct
(frequency of children who doze during the week and week- response.
end, duration, and times of start and end); (3) the activity According to the attention model proposed by Pos-
before bedtime (options: watching TV, playing, homework, ner and Rafal (1987) and Valdez et al. (2005), responses
or other); (4) parent regulation of bedtime (options: the par- to the green heart (a frequent stimulus) indicated tonic
ents regulated the bedtime, or the child goes to bed sponta- alertness, the red circle (a dierent, infrequent, specic
neously); (5) the way of waking up (options: spontaneously, stimulus) indicated selective attention, the blue star (a stim-
by an alarm clock, or called by someone); and (6) the rea- ulus preceded by an arrow that acted as a warning signal)
son for wake up time (options: school starting time, physical indicated phasic alertness, while changes in performance
activities or sports, travel, promenade, or other). Parents or during the task indicated sustained attention. Task duration
guardians completed the questionnaires on sleep habits, eco- was 5 min.
nomic classication, and the sleep diary with information on Training sessions were held on Monday and Tuesday,
the childrens sleep habits. during which the task was explained to the children as a
In order to assess the sleepiness levels, an adapted version game in which they would have to press certain keys corre-
(Belsio, 2014) of the Pictorial Sleepiness Scale Based on Car- sponding to the gures displayed on the screen. Data from
toon Faces of Maldonado (Maldonado, Bentley, & Mitchell, this training were excluded from the analysis. The task was
2004) was applied during 6 days, from Monday to Friday applied on three weekdays (Wednesday, Friday, and Mon-
and the next Monday, consecutively. The data collected on day) between 8 a.m. and 9 a.m. to children of the morning
the rst Monday were excluded from analysis to reduce the shift and between 2 p.m. and 3 p.m. to their afternoon coun-
eects of a new person in the school environment. Dur- terparts. The children responded to the Maldonado daytime
ing questionnaire application children were asked how they sleepiness scale before the task was applied.
were feeling at that moment, that is, whether they felt sleepy
or not. Next, the children indicated the face on the scale that
best represented how they were feeling at the time. The scale DATA ANALYSIS
was applied between 8 a.m. and 9 a.m. and 2 p.m. and 3 p.m.
to children of the morning and afternoon shifts, respectively. Sleep habits (co-sleeping, room sharing, and bedtime ritu-
A continuous performance task (CPT) was used to assess als) and economic classication of children in the morning
the components of attention in the children. The CPT and afternoon shifts were compared using the chi-square
uses frequent stimuli to track the childs attention, but test (2 ).
also requires responses to infrequent and specic stimuli The sleep parameters obtained from the sleep diary (the
that occur at random intervals (Riccio, Reynolds, Lowe, & bed and wake up times, time spent in bed at night, and total
Moore, 2002). A CPT, a simple task that continuously mon- time spent in bed every 24 hr, the latter obtained from the
itors behavior, is sensitive to sleep deprivation and circa- sum of the time spent in bed at night and the duration of
dian rhythms (Gill, Haerich, Westcott, Godenick, & Tucker, nap) were compared between shifts by analysis of variance
2006; Mullaney, Kripke, Fleck, & Johnson, 1983; Valdez et al., (ANOVA) for repeated measures and Bonferronis posttest.
2005). It has been demonstrated that this task measures each Daytime sleepiness, irregularity of bed and wake up times
component of attention and the indices of sustained atten- (calculated from the standard deviation in bed and wake
tion (Smith, Valentino, & Arruda, 2002; Valdez et al., 2005, up times, respectively), and the parameters related to naps
2010). (start time, end time, and duration) were compared between
The CPT used in the present study was an adapted ver- shifts using the MannWhitney test. Comparative analysis
sion for preschool children (Belsio, 2014) of that applied of nap-related parameters between week and weekend days
on university students by Valdez et al. (2005). In this task, was conducted using the Wilcoxon test.
three images familiar to children (a green heart, a red circle, In relation to attention task data, the MannWhitney
and a blue star) were used. Each image was displayed on a test was applied to compare the percentage and reaction

3
Sleep, Sleepiness, and Attention in Children

time for correct responses in each attention component Economic Classication


(tonic alertness, selective attention, phasic alertness, and Most of the morning shift children belonged to classes A2
sustained attentionconcentration); sequences of hits and (24%) and B1 (44%), whereas those from the afternoon
errors during the attention task (obtained by a sequence shift were in B1 (22%) and B2 (40%) (X 2 = 37.32,
of hits and errors over the course of the test; e.g., 1 hit, 2 p < .0001).
hits, 3 hits, until achieving 100 hits, which refers to the total
number of stimuli in the task), and omissions (no response
Childrens Activity Before Bedtime
to the stimulus) between children who study in the morning
A higher percentage of children who studied in the morning
and afternoon.
watch TV on Monday (51%; 2 = 30.74; p < .0001), Tuesday
Furthermore, the time on task stability, which is an index
(51%; 2 = 13.72; p = .0174), and Friday (72%; 2 = 37.13;
of sustained attention, was obtained from a linear regres- p < .0001), whereas a greater percentage of those who
sion of the percentage of correct responses and reaction studied in the afternoon watch TV on Wednesday (60%;
time in all attention components throughout the task (tonic 2 = 11.73; p = .0386), Thursday (51%; 2 = 27.47; p < .0001),
alertness, selective attention, and phasic alertness). Nega- and Saturday (50%; 2 = 31.40; p < .0001).
tive values represent a decrease in correct answers/reaction
time over the task. On the other hand, positive values
represent an increase in these parameters across the task. Parent Regulation of Childrens Bedtime
The MannWhitney test was used to compare the indices A greater percentage of morning shift students have their
between the shifts. A signicance level of 5% was set in all bedtime regulated by their parents on Tuesday (52%;
tests applied. 2 = 8.48; p = .0035), Wednesday (50%; 2 = 17.36; p < .0001),
Thursday (50%; 2 = 1107; p = .0008), and Saturday (49%;
X 2 = 10.55; p = .0011), whereas this occurred only on Friday
(43%; 2 = 6.55; p = .0104) for children who study in the
afternoon.
RESULTS

Co-Sleeping, Room Sharing and Bedtime Ritual Reason for Wake Up Time
There was no dierence in the frequency of co-sleeping, A higher percentage of parents of children who study
room sharing, or bedtime rituals between children who in the morning report school as the reason for wake up
study in the morning and afternoon (2 , p > .05) (Table 1). time on weekdays (Monday: 97%; 2 = 401.20; Tuesday:

Table 1
Frequency of Co-Sleeping, Room-Sharing, and Bedtime Rituals During the Week in Children Who Study in the Morning and Afternoon
Shifts (2 test, p > .05).

Morning Afternoon
Frequently Sometimes Rarely Frequently Sometimes Rarely
Behaviors % % % % % % 2 p
Co-sleeping and room-sharing
Co-sleeping with brother/sister 5.0 4.0 91.0 7.0 7.0 86.0 2.06 .3561
Co-sleeping with parents/guardians 41.0 27.0 33.0 41.0 27.0 32.0 0.40 .9799
Room-sharing with brother/sister 20.0 3.0 76.0 20.0 5.0 75.0 0.52 .7694
Room-sharing with parents/guardians 29.0 10.0 59.0 26.0 12.0 61.0 0.67 .7126
Sleeping alone 31.0 17.0 53.0 30.0 16.0 54.0 0.09 .9520
Bedtime rituals
Drinking milk 44.0 22.0 33.0 40.0 24.0 35.0 .73 .6914
Listening to stories 17.0 51.0 32.0 18.0 50.0 32.0 0.09 .9531
Going to the bathroom 64.0 27.0 9.0 58.0 32.0 10.0 1.26 .5319
Requires the parents in the room 57.0 25.0 18.0 61.0 23.0 17.0 0.55 .7577
Sleeping with the lights on 25.0 14.0 62.0 23.0 19.0 59.0 1.48 .4752
Using a special object 40.0 15.0 45.0 32.0 18.0 50.0 3.38 .1836
Others 29.0 19.0 50.0 38.0 15.0 46.0 3.73 .1543

Note: Frequently = behavior occurs 5 or more times during the week; Sometimes = behavior occurs 24 times during the week; rarely = behavior occurs once or
never during the week.

4
Aline S. Belsio et al.

Fig. 1. Mean and standard deviation of bedtime, wake up time, and time in bed at night, and total time in bed (night and daytime nap)
during the week in children of morning and afternoon shifts (ANOVA, *p < .05).

92%; 2 = 600.61; Wednesday: 94%; 2 = 947.41; Thurs- earlier on Monday, Tuesday, and Thursday than on Saturday
day: 86%; 2 = 342.08; Friday: 80%; 2 = 358.96; p < .0001) (F (6.462) = 8.47; Bonferroni, p < .0001) (Figure 1).
compared to those who study in the afternoon (Monday: Children from the morning shift woke up about 1 hr
18%; 2 = 401.20; Tuesday: 12%; 2 = 600.61; Wednesday: and 30 min earlier from Monday to Friday compared to
8%; 2 = 947.41; Thursday: 17%; 2 = 342.08; Friday: 14%; those who studied in the afternoon, with no dierence on
2 = 358.96; p < .0001). weekends (F (6.450) = 7.06; Bonferroni, p < .0001). Children in
the morning shift woke up earlier during the week than on
Saturday and Sunday (F (6.450) = 11.34; Bonferroni, p < .0001),
Way of Waking Up
whereas those in the afternoon shift showed no dierence
A greater number of children who study in the morn-
between weekdays and weekends (F (6.450) = 11.34; Bonfer-
ing are woken up by someone on weekdays (Monday:
roni, p < .0001) (Figure 1).
76%; 2 = 180.28; Tuesday: 76%; 2 = 128.92; Wednesday:
Morning shift students spent 1 hr less in bed on Monday
68%; 2 = 140.76; Thursday: 76%; 2 = 123.17; Friday: 63%;
and Sunday nights than their afternoon shift counterparts
2 = 193.39, p < .0001), whereas more children who study
(ANOVA F (6.456) = 7.01; Bonferroni, p < .0001). Time spent
in the afternoon wake up spontaneously on these days
in bed for children who studied in the morning was shorter
(Monday: 78%; 2 = 180.28; Tuesday: 73%; 2 = 128.92;
on Tuesday, Thursday, and Sunday than on Saturday. On
Wednesday: 79%; 2 = 140.76; Thursday (72%; 2 = 123.17;
the other hand, children who studied in the afternoon spent
Friday: 82%; 2 = 193.39, p < .0001).
more time in bed from Monday to Friday than on Saturday
(ANOVA F (6.456) = 1.10; Bonferroni, p < .0001) (Figure 1).
Temporal Sleep Patterns There was no dierence in the irregularity of bed-
Bedtimes did not dier between shifts (F (6.462) = 0.97, times (MannWhitney, p = .9588) and wake up times
p = .4438). Nevertheless, children in the morning shift usu- (MannWhitney, p = .4067) between the shifts (Table 2).
ally went to bed 40 min earlier than those who studied in Children who studied in the morning napped more fre-
the afternoon. Children who studied in the morning went quently on weekdays and weekends (2 , p < .0001) and
to bed earlier on Monday and Wednesday than on Saturday, naps were longer on weekdays (MannWhitney, p = .008)
whereas those who studied in the afternoon went to bed (Table 2). It is important to underscore that the children

5
Sleep, Sleepiness, and Attention in Children

Table 2
Irregularity in Bedtime and Wake Up Times, Frequency of Children
Who Nap, Times of Start, End, and Duration of Naps in Children
of the Morning and Afternoon Shifts (2 , MannWhitney, p < .05).

Morning Afternoon
Characteristics (n = 44) (n = 39) p
Nap
Weekdays frequency 68% 26% <.0001
of children
Start 13:40 01:18 13:38 03:05 .3121
End 15:48 01:30 15:04 03:29 .4238
Duration 02:08 00:54 01:25 00:50 .0001
Weekend frequency 44% 31% .0080
of children
Start 14:26 00:59 14:36 01:37 .4062
End 16:19 01:12 16:16 01:50 .5022 Fig. 2. Mean and standard deviation of daytime sleepiness level
Duration 01:52 00:46 01:44 00:51 .4356
during the week in children of morning and afternoon shifts. The
Irregularity
Bedtime 00:43 00:22 00:43 00:20 .9588 daytime sleepiness was measured by an adapted version of the
Wake up time 00:50 00:55 00:42 00:24 .4067 Pictorial Sleepiness Scale Based on Cartoon Faces of Maldonado
et al. (2004) (MannWhitney, p < .05).

napped at home. In children who studied in the morning,


time increased less in this group (MannWhitney, p = .05)
the naps occurred after they returned from school; while
(Figure 4). Nevertheless, there was no dierence between
those in the afternoon shift napped, for example, when they
shifts in relation to omissions (Figure 5) and the sequence
missed school to go to the dentist or to accompany their
of hits and errors across the test (MannWhitney, p > .1777)
parents to solve personal problems.
(Figure 6).

Time Spent in Bed


There was no dierence between shifts (F (6.480) = 4.05, DISCUSSION
p = .3828) or weekdays (F (6.480) = 1.62, p = .1388) for
time spent in bed every 24 hr (nighttime sleep and naps) This is a pioneering investigation on comparing the habits
(Figure 1). and temporal patterns of sleep, daytime sleepiness, and the
components of attention in preschool children enrolled in
the morning and afternoon shifts. Sleep habits did not dier
Daytime Sleepiness
between shifts, but there were dierences in temporal sleep
The level of daytime sleepiness in children who study in the
patterns, daytime sleepiness, and some components of atten-
morning was higher on Monday (MannWhitney, p = .05)
tion between children who studied in these shifts.
and Friday (MannWhitney, p = .004) compared to those
It was observed that most of the children co-slept and
who studied in the afternoon (Figure 2).
engaged in bedtime rituals regardless of the shift in which
they studied. Thus, school start time did not change these
Components of Attention behaviors, probably because they are part of family dynam-
Children in the morning shift exhibited a higher percentage ics that increase childparent interactions (Beltramini &
of correct answers for phasic alertness than those from the Hertzig, 1983; Buckley, Rigda, Mundy, & McMillen, 2002;
afternoon shift (MannWhitney, p = .05) (Figure 3). How- Silva et al., 2005).
ever, over the course of the test children who studied in the With respect to temporal sleep patterns according to
morning had fewer correct answers and a lower increase in school start time, it was observed that during the week chil-
reaction time than those who studied in the afternoon. This dren who studied in the morning woke up 1 hr and 30 min
can be visualized in the time on task stability in Figure 4. earlier than those who studied in the afternoon. Thus, they
The negative values observed represent a decrease in correct spent 1 hr less in bed at night and tended to be sleepier on
answers throughout the task. On the other hand, the positive Monday and Friday than their afternoon shift counterparts.
values represent an increase in reaction time during the task. Furthermore, a higher frequency of children from the morn-
The decrease in the percentage of correct answers across the ing shift napped with longer episodes than those who studied
task was higher in morning shift children. However, reaction in the afternoon.

6
Aline S. Belsio et al.

Fig. 3. Mean and standard error of the percentages and reaction time of correct answers for the components of attention during the
continuous performance task in children who study in the morning and the afternoon shifts (MannWhitney, p = .05).

Fig. 4. Mean and standard error of indices of time on task stability Fig. 5. Mean and standard error of omissions during the attention
in relation to the percentage and reaction time of correct answers task for children who study in the morning and the afternoon
during the performance task in children who study in the morning (MannWhitney, p > .05).
and afternoon shifts (MannWhitney, p < .05).

and the stronger inuence of parents in determining their


In more sleep deprived teenagers, this corresponds to a sleepwake times (Iglowstein et al., 2003; Meijer et al., 2000)
dierence of nearly 2 hr of sleep between school days and This dierence in temporal sleep patterns between week-
weekends (Carskadon, 2002; Carskadon, Acebo, & Jenni, days and weekend in children who studied in the morn-
2004; Sousa, Arajo, & Azevedo, 2007). The dierence ing may be due to acute sleep deprivation associated with
between children and teenagers may be due to the natural a shorter time in bed at night in consequence to the school
predisposition of children to go to bed and wake up earlier, starting time in the morning, which may contribute to a

7
Sleep, Sleepiness, and Attention in Children

selective attention, although an increase in the percentage of


correct answers for phasic alertness was observed in children
enrolled in the morning shift.
The decrease in one of the sustained attention indices
(percentage of correct answers across the task) in children
from the morning shift may have been caused by acute sleep
deprivation associated to the shorter time they spent in bed
at night due to the school start time. In the school context,
this negative eect might compromise learning.
However, both groups of children showed an increase
in reaction time across the task, but this increase was
less marked in children who studied in the morning. This
increase across the task was expected in cognitive assess-
ment tests due to the fatigue eect. The lower increase
Fig. 6. Average and standard error of the sequence of hits and
in morning shift students may be due to a motivational
error runs during the continuous performance task in children
who study in the morning and the afternoon shifts. No signicant
eect (Valdez et al., 2008), as a consequence of a higher
diferences were found between the groups in hits or error runs. activity level associated with methodological dierences
(MannWhitney, p > .05). in school tasks. Motivation and the higher activity level
might have stimulating eects on cognitive development
(Wojtczak-Jaroszowa & Jarosz, 1987).
higher level of daytime sleepiness. It is important to under- The absence of intershift dierences in some attention
score that most of the children who studied in the morning parameters may result from the level of daily sleep depri-
were woken up by their parents during the week and most vation observed in children (about 1 hr), which might not
who studied in the afternoon woke up spontaneously, reaf- have been sucient to interfere in attention levels. Further-
rming the signs of acute sleep deprivation in the former more, the time the tasks were applied could have inu-
group. Thus, the increase in sleepiness on Monday may be enced the responses obtained. Children from each shift
a consequence of the shorter time in bed from Sunday to answered the tasks at dierent circadian moments (morn-
Monday, whereas on Friday it may result from accumulated ing or afternoon). Additionally, those who studied in the
tiredness during the week. This sleepiness pattern may lead morning should have exhibited a higher homeostatic pres-
to the long nap episodes observed in the children enrolled in sure level since they had less nighttime sleep, but afternoon
the morning shift. shift students displayed a higher number of previous waking
With regard to naps and the shorter time spent in bed, hours. These factors may lead to more rapid fatigue in both
it was observed that after adding time spent in bed at night groups and might have inuenced the results obtained for
and the daytime nap, the total sleep time of morning shift attention.
students was similar to that of afternoon students. This In other studies, it was observed that university students
shows that children who studied in the morning slept less at had increased attention levels during the day and a decline
night than those from the afternoon shift, but they seemed between 4 a.m. and 6 a.m. (Valdez et al., 2005, 2010). The
to compensate this deprivation by taking daily naps. These continuous application task presented in this study was
results corroborate those observed by Silva et al. (2005), who applied between 8 a.m. and 9 a.m. to the morning group and
reported that 7- to 10-year-old children who studied in the 2 p.m. and 3 p.m. to the afternoon group. Based on the study
morning took more naps than those who studied in the with university students, it was expected that both shifts
afternoon. These sleeping patterns demonstrate acute sleep would exhibit high attention levels, while sleep-deprived
deprivation during the week that is compensated by taking children would not perform well on the task. However, chil-
daily naps. Thus, school start time inuenced the childrens dren are characterized by an advanced circadian phase in
sleeping patterns, given that they spent less time in bed and relation to college students. Thus, circadian variation in
woke up earlier to go to school in the morning. attention should be assessed considering the ontogenetic dif-
In addition, morning shift students had fewer correct ferences that could cause dierent results in each age group.
answers, even though they exhibited a shorter increase in The reduction in sustained attention eciency observed
reaction time during the CPT compared to their after- in this study is similar to the levels identied in previous
noon shift counterparts. Nevertheless, in this investigation research with children starting at 7 years of age that sought
no intershift dierences were observed in omissions, the to identify cognitive development. Shorter sleep duration
sequence of hits and errors during the test, or the number and poorer sleep quality have been associated with low per-
of correct answers and reaction time for tonic alertness and formance (Paavonen et al., 2009; Sadeh, Gruber, & Raviv,

8
Aline S. Belsio et al.

2002; Touchette et al., 2007). However, these studies mea- Psicobiologia and the Universidade Federal do Rio Grande
sured performance using scores on tests or tasks that only do Norte for nancial support.
assessed sustained attention.
This study had a number of limitations: (1) sleepwake REFERENCES
cycle analysis using questionnaires completed by parents
may have contained inaccuracies regarding their childs Acebo, C., Sadeh, A., Seifer, R., Tzischinsky, O., Hafer, A., &
habits (according to Lam, Mahone, Mason, and Scharf Carskadon, M. A. (2005). Sleep/wake patterns derived from
[2011], because parents tend to overestimate the sleep time activity monitoring and maternal report for healthy 1- to
of children, actigraphy is one of the best ways to evaluate 5-year-old children. Sleep, 28, 15681577.
sleep patterns); (2) the dierent task application times could Adam, E. K., Snell, E. K., & Pendry, P. (2007). Sleep timing and
quantity in ecological and family context: A nationally repre-
have inuenced intershift results; (3) some participants
sentative time-diary study. Journal of Family Psychology, 21,
abandoned the study at dierent stages, contributing to a 419.
decrease in the number of participants; and (4) the absence Anacleto, A. S. (2011). Ciclo viglia/sono e atividade motora em
of chronotype evaluation due to the lack of an instrument crianas de 8 a 10 anos (Doctoral dissertation). Universidade
validated for Portuguese that could have been applied to the Federal do Paran, Curitiba, Brazil.
children. Finally, an actigraphic assessment of sleeping pat- Andrade, M. (1997). Padres temporais das expresses da sonoln-
terns, using sleepiness variables, such as sleepwake times to cia em adolescentes (Doctoral thesis). Universidade de So
Paulo, So Paulo, Brazil.
classify the chronotypes of children younger than 8 years old,
Associao Nacional de Empresas de Pesquisa. (2008), Critrio de
is suggested for future studies. The attention components in
Classicao Econmica Brasil. Retrieved from http://www.
this age group should be analyzed more often throughout anep.org.br
the day to verify whether the ndings of the present investi- Belsio, A. S. (2010). Inuncia de fatores sociais sobre o ciclo sono e
gation really had an inuence on daily sleep compensation, viglia de crianas na educao infantil (Masters dissertation).
and motivation or were due to a circadian eect. Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil.
It is suggested that school start time inuences tem- Belsio, A.S. (2014). Padres temporais de sono, sonolncia diurna
poral sleep patterns and daytime sleepiness in preschool e os componentes da ateno em crianas que estudam nos
turnos da manh e da tarde na educao infantil (Doctoral
children. Changes in sleeping patterns due to the morn-
thesis). Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal
ing start time have been assessed in teenagers (Andrade, Brazil.
1997; Carskadon, 2002; Carskadon et al., 2004; Sousa et al., Beltramini, A. U., & Hertzig, M. E. (1983). Sleep and bedtime
2007) and preteenagers aged 1012 years (Epstein, Chillag, behavior in preschool-aged children. Pediatrics, 71, 153158.
& Lavie, 1998), but we observed that school start time also Benington, J. H., & Heller, H. C. (1995). Restoration of brain energy
inuences younger children, despite their natural tendency metabolism as the function of sleep. Progress in Neurobiology,
to morningness. In addition to the negative impact on sleep 45, 347360.
patterns, there were signs of negative eects on sustained Buckley, P., Rigda, R. S., Mundy, L., & McMillen, I. C. (2002). Inter-
action between bed sharing and other sleep environments dur-
attention, which may compromise learning. Since only one
ing the rst six months of life. Early Human Development, 66,
parameter of one component of attention was negatively 123132.
aected, further studies evaluating the attention compo- Carskadon, M. (2002). Factors inuencing sleep patterns of adoles-
nents in this age group at dierent times of the day are cents. In M. Carskadon (Ed.), Adolescent sleep patterns (pp.
needed to conrm this eect on cognition. Nevertheless, the 426). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
results obtained reinforce the importance of broadening the Carskadon, M. A., Acebo, C., & Jenni, O. G. (2004). Regulation of
discussion regarding changing school start times, which is adolescent sleep. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
focused on teenagers, to other age groups, such as preschool 1021, 276291.
Cohen, R. A., & ODonnell, B. F. (1993). Models and mecha-
children. To that end, it is essential to take into account the
nisms of attention. In R. A. Cohen, I. A. Sparling-Cohen, &
biological characteristics of each age group. l. F. ODonne (Eds.), The neuropsychology of attention (pp.
177186). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
AcknowledgmentsWe thank Dr. John Arajo, Dr. Luiz Drosopoulos, S., Schulze, C., Fischer, S., & Born, J. (2007). Sleeps
Menna-Barreto, and Dra. Katie Almondes for suggestions function in the spontaneous recovery and consolidation of
throughout this study. We also thank the students Diana memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136,
Jurez, Ana-hi Flores, Mariana Reyna, Gabriela Iracheta, 169183.
El-Sheikh, M., Buckhalt, J. A., Mize, J., & Acebo, C. (2006). Marital
Susana Hernandez, Galileu Borges, Maria Luiza Oliveira,
conict and disruption of childrens sleep. Child Development,
Sabinne Galina, Jackleyde Silva, Icemria Felipe, and Kar- 77, 3143.
lyne dos Anjos for their help in data collection and analysis; Epstein, R., Chillag, N., & Lavie, P. (1998). Starting times of school:
Dra. Candelria Ramrez and Dra. Aida Garca for analysis Eects on daytime functioning of fth-grade children in Israel.
support; and CAPES, Programa de Ps-Graduao em Sleep, 21, 250256.

9
Sleep, Sleepiness, and Attention in Children

Gill, M., Haerich, P., Westcott, K., Godenick, K. L., & Tucker, Ramm, P., & Smith, C. T. (1990). Rates of cerebral protein synthesis
J. A. (2006). Cognitive performance following modanil are linked to slow wave sleep in the rat. Physiology & Behavior,
versus placebo in sleep-deprived emergency physicians: 48, 749753.
A double-blind randomized crossover study. Academic Reimo, R., Souza, J. C., Gaudioso, C. E., Guerra, H. C., Alves, A. C.,
Emergency Medicine, 13, 158165. Oliveira, J. C., Silvrio, D. C. (1999). Sleep characteristics
Gillberg, M., & Akerstedt, T. (1998). Sleep loss and performance: No in children in the isolated rural African-Brazilian descendant
safe duration of a monotonous task. Physiology and Behav- community of Furnas do Dionsio, State of Mato Grosso do
ior, 64, 599604. Sul, Brazil. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 57, 556560.
Iglowstein, I., Jenni, O. G., Molinari, L., & Largo, R. H. (2003). Sleep Reimo, R., Souza, J. C., Medeiros, M. M., & Almiro, R. I. (1998).
duration from infancy to adolescence: Reference values and Sleep habits in native Brazilian Terena children in the state of
generational trends. Pediatrics, 111, 302307. Mato Grosso do Sul, Brazil. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 56,
Jenni, O. G., & Carskadon, M. A. (2005). Normal human sleep at 703707.
dierent ages: lnfants to adolescents. In Sleep Research Soci- Riccio, C. A., Reynolds, C. R., Lowe, P., & Moore, J. J. (2002).
ety (Ed.), SRS basics of sleep guide (pp. 1119). Westchester, The continuous performance test: A window on the neural
IL: Sleep Research Society. substrates for attention? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,
Lam, J. C., Mahone, E. M., Mason, T. B. A., & Scharf, S. M. (2011). 17, 235272.
Dening the roles of actigraphy and parent logs for assess- Rodrguez, D. J. (2011). Efectos de la privacin del dormir sobre
ing sleep variables in preschool children. Behavioral Sleep los componentes de la atencin (Masters thesis). Universidad
Medicine, 9, 184193. Autnoma de Nuevo Len, San Nicols de los Garza, Mxico.
Li, S., Jin, X., Yan, C., Wu, S., Jiang, F., & Shen, X. (2008). Bed- Sadeh, A., Gruber, R., & Raviv, A. (2002). Sleep, neurobehavioral
and room-sharing in Chinese school-aged children: Preva- functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children.
lence and association with sleep behaviors. Sleep Medicine, 9, Child Development, 2, 405417.
555563. Silva, T. A., Carvalho, L. B. C., Silva, L., Medeiros, M., Natale, V.
Louzada, F., Orsoni, A., Mello, L., Benedito-Silva, A. A., & B., Carvalho, J. E. C., Prado, G. F. (2005). Sleep habits
Menna-Barreto, L. (1996). Longitudinal study of the and starting time to school in Brazilian children. Arquivos de
sleep-wake cycle in children living on the same school Neuro-Psiquiatria, 63, 402406.
schedules. Biological Rhythm Research, 27, 390397. Smith, K. J., Valentino, D. A., & Arruda, J. E. (2002). Measures of
Maldonado, C. C., Bentley, A. J., & Mitchell, D. (2004). A pictorial variations in performance during a sustained attention task.
sleepiness scale based on cartoon faces. Sleep, 27, 541548. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24,
Meijer, A. M., Habekoth, H. T., & Van Den Wittenboer, G. L. H. 828839.
(2000). Time in bed, quality of sleep and school functioning of Sousa, I. C., Arajo, J. F., & Azevedo, C. V. M. (2007). The eect of
children. Journal of Sleep Research, 9, 145153. a sleep hygiene education program on the sleepwake cycle of
Mello, L., Isola, A., Louzada, F., & Menna-Barreto, L. (1996). A Brazilian adolescent students. Sleep and Biological Rhythms,
four-year follow-up study of the sleepwake cycle of an infant. 5, 251258.
Biological Rhythm Research, 27, 291298. Spiegel, K., Tasali, E., Penev, P., & Van Cauter, E. (2004). Brief com-
Menna-Barreto, L., Isola, A., Louzada, F., Benedito-Silva, A. A., & munication: Sleep curtailment in healthy young men is associ-
Mello, L. (1996). Becoming circadian: A one-year study of the ated with decreased leptin levels, elevated ghrelin levels, and
sleep/wake cycle in children. Brazilian Journal of Medical and increased hunger and appetite. Annals of Internal Medicine,
Biological Research, 29, 125129. 141, 846850.
Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., Duncan, C. C., Ahearn, M. B., & Taheri, S., Lin, L., Austin, D., Young, T., & Mignot, E. (2004).
Kellam, S. G. (1991). Analysis of the elements of attention: Short sleep duration is associated with reduced leptin, ele-
A neuropsychological approach. Neuropsychology Review, 2, vated ghrelin, and increased body mass index. PLoS Medicine,
109145. 3, 211217.
Mullaney, D. J., Kripke, D. F., Fleck, P. A., & Johnson, L. C. (1983). Taveras, E. M., Rifas-Shiman, S. L., Oken, E., Gunderson, E. P., &
Sleep loss and nap eects on sustained continuous perfor- Gillman, M. W. (2008). Short sleep duration in infancy and risk
mance. Psychophysiology, 20, 643651. of childhood overweight. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent
Owens, J. A., Spirito, A., & McGuinn, M. (2000). The Childrens Medicine, 162, 305311.
Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ): Psychometric properties Thomas, M., Sing, H., Belenky, G., Holcomb, H., Mayberg, H.,
of a survey instrument for school-aged children. Sleep, 23, Dannals, R., Redmond, D. (2000). Neural basis of alertness
10431051. and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I.
Paavonen, E. J., Rikknen, K., Lahti, J., Komsi, N., Heinonen, Eects of 24 hr of sleep deprivation on waking human regional
K., Pesonen, A. K., Porkka-Heiskanen, T. (2009). brain activity. Journal of Sleep Research, 4, 335352.
Short sleep duration and behavioral symptoms of Touchette, E., Petit, D., Sguin, J. R., Boivin, M., Tremblay, R. E., &
attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder in healthy 7- to Montplaisir, J. Y. (2007). Associations between sleep duration
8-year-old children. Pediatrics, 123, e857e864. patterns and behavioral/cognitive functioning at school entry.
Posner, M., & Rafal, R. (1987). Cognitive theories of attention Sleep, 30, 12131219.
and the rehabilitation of attentional decits. In M. J. Meier, Valdez, P., Ramrez, C., Garca, A., Talamantes, J., Armijo, P., & Bor-
A. Benton, & L. Diller (Eds.), Neuropsychological rani, J. (2005). Circadian rhythms in components of attention.
(pp. 182200) New York, NY: Churchill Livingstone. Biological Rhythm Research, 36, 5765.

10
Aline S. Belsio et al.

Valdez, P., Ramrez, C., Garca, A., Talamantes, J., & Cortez, J. Wilhelm, I., Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2008). Sleep in children
(2010). Circadian and homeostatic variation in sustained improves memory performance on declarative but not proce-
attention. Chronobiology International, 27, 393416. dural tasks. Learning and Memory, 15, 373377.
Valdez, P., Reilly, T., & Waterhouse, J. (2008). Rhythms of mental Wojtczak-Jaroszowa, J., & Jarosz, D. (1987). Chronohygienic and
performance. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2, 716. chronosocial aspects of industrial accidents. In J. E. Pauly &
Wey, D. (2002). Ciclo viglia-sono de crianas: transio da edu- L. E. Scheving (Eds.), Advances in chronobiology B
cao infantil para o ensino fundamental (Doctoral disserta- (pp. 415426). New York, NY: Alan R. Liss.
tion). Universidade de So Paulo, So Paulo, Brazil.

11

Potrebbero piacerti anche