Sei sulla pagina 1di 6

Research

Original Investigation

Association of the Type of Toy Used


During Play With the Quantity and
Quality of Parent-Infant Communication
Anna V. Sosa, PhD

Editorial
IMPORTANCE The early language environment of a child influences language outcome, which
in turn affects reading and academic success. It is unknown which types of everyday activities
promote the best language environment for children.

OBJECTIVE To investigate whether the type of toy used during play is associated with the
parent-infant communicative interaction.

DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS Controlled experiment in a natural environment of


parent-infant communication during play with 3 different toy sets. Participant recruitment
and data collection were conducted between February 1, 2013, and June 30, 2014. The
volunteer sample included 26 parent-infant (aged 10-16 months) dyads.

EXPOSURES Fifteen-minute in-home parent-infant play sessions with electronic toys,


traditional toys, and books.

MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Numbers of adult words, child vocalizations, conversational
turns, parent verbal responses to child utterances, and words produced by parents in 3
different semantic categories (content-specific words) per minute during play sessions.

RESULTS Among the 26 parent-infant dyads, toy type was associated with all outcome
measures. During play with electronic toys, there were fewer adult words (mean, 39.62; 95%
CI, 33.36-45.65), fewer conversational turns (mean, 1.64; 95% CI, 1.12-2.19), fewer parental
responses (mean, 1.31; 95% CI, 0.87-1.77), and fewer productions of content-specific words
(mean, 1.89; 95% CI, 1.49-2.35) than during play with traditional toys or books. Children
vocalized less during play with electronic toys (mean per minute, 2.9; 95% CI, 2.16-3.69) than
during play with books (mean per minute, 3.91; 95% CI, 3.09-4.68). Parents produced fewer
words during play with traditional toys (mean per minute, 55.56; 95% CI, 46.49-64.17) than
during play with books (mean per minute, 66.89; 95% CI, 59.93-74.19) and use of
content-specific words was lower during play with traditional toys (mean per minute, 4.09;
95% CI, 3.26-4.99) than during play with books (mean per minute, 6.96; 95% CI, 6.07-7.97).

CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Play with electronic toys is associated with decreased
quantity and quality of language input compared with play with books or traditional toys. To
promote early language development, play with electronic toys should be discouraged.
Traditional toys may be a valuable alternative for parent-infant play time if book reading is not
a preferred activity.

Author Affiliation: Department of


Communication Sciences and
Disorders, Northern Arizona
University, Flagstaff.
Corresponding Author: Anna V.
Sosa, PhD, Department of
Communication Sciences and
Disorders, Northern Arizona
University, 208 E Pine Knoll Dr,
JAMA Pediatr. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.3753 15045, Flagstaff, AZ 86011
Published online December 23, 2015. (anna.sosa@nau.edu).

(Reprinted) E1

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Central Michigan University User on 12/25/2015


Research Original Investigation Toy Type and Parent-Infant Communication

V
ariation in early language development is in part de-
termined by a childs language environment.1 The quan- At a Glance
tity of language input received from caregivers during
To promote language development, parents are encouraged to
the first few years is positively associated with a childs lan- read to their infants and spend time playing 1 on 1, but little is
guage accomplishments and this early advantage can have known about how different types of play activities affect
long-lasting implications for overall academic success.2,3 parent-infant communication.
Previous evidence suggests that in addition to the quantity of In the present study, play with books and traditional toys was
language exposure, measures of interaction quality also influ- superior to play with electronic toys in promoting high-quality
communication.
ence language development.1 For example, maternal respon-
Children vocalized less during play with electronic toys than
siveness and the number of adult-child conversations both during play with books.
positively affect language development.4,5 Beyond family- Because play with electronic toys is associated with decreased
specific factors, such as socioeconomic status and parental edu- quantity and quality of language input, it should be discouraged
cation, little is known about everyday activities that may pro- to promote early language development.
mote both the quantity and quality of communicative
interactions between parents and young children. Television
exposure, book reading, and independent and guided play are Methods
activities that have been investigated. Television exposure was
found to be associated with decreased quantity and quality of My research team and I conducted a controlled experiment
parental language input, and media viewing by children using observational measures of a volunteer community
younger than 2 years has been shown to negatively affect lan- sample of parent-infant dyads recruited through posting of fly-
guage development, likely because media use displaces other ers in public places frequented by parents of young children
more beneficial language-promoting interactions.6-8 On the in Flagstaff, Arizona, a midsized city in the southwestern United
other hand, book reading and playing together with young chil- States. Enrollment and data collection occurred over a 16-
dren are often recommended as activities that boost lan- month period between February 1, 2013, and June 30, 2014.
guage development. Clinic-based programs, such as Reach Out All procedures were approved by the institutional review board
and Read, that distribute books to impoverished families have at Northern Arizona University. Written informed consent was
been shown to improve language ability, and a variety of dif- obtained from parents.
ferent play activities, including early symbolic play and block
play, have been linked to better language ability in toddlers.9-12 Participants
Based on these and other findings, the American Academy of Pairs of parents (mother or father) and infants (aged 10-16
Pediatrics discourages media use by children younger than 2 months) were eligible. Parent-infant dyads were excluded if
years and emphasizes the importance of book reading and other the participating parent did not use English as the primary lan-
types of parent-child play time.13 However, the reality for many guage with the child. Because race/ethnicity and socioeco-
families of young children is that opportunities for direct par- nomic status may affect parent-child communication, this in-
ent-child play time is limited owing to financial, work, and formation was collected via parent report using a written
other familial factors. Thus, optimizing the quality of limited questionnaire with set categories for race/ethnicity and ma-
parent-child play time is imperative. ternal education level.
At the same time that parents are being encouraged to read
to their young children and engage in direct parent-infant play, Data Collection
they are also bombarded with advertisements for educa- To maximize the ecological validity of study findings, partici-
tional toys that claim to promote language development in pants engaged in study procedures in their own homes with-
very young children, including infants. These toys are typi- out being directly observed by researchers. On enrollment in
cally battery-operated electronic toys with buttons that pro- the study, audio recording equipment, 3 sets of toys, and study
duce lights, sounds, music, words, and phrases when acti- paperwork were delivered to participants. The recording equip-
vated. Other options parents have when selecting toys for their ment used was the LENA Pro System (Language Environ-
very young children are traditional nonelectronic toys, includ- ment Analysis; LENA Foundation). The system includes a small
ing blocks, puzzles, and stacking cups, as well as toys that en- digital recording device called a digital language processor that
courage symbolic play such as dolls and tea sets. Primary care is placed in a pocket in a vest worn by the child. The proces-
professionals and pediatricians working with families of young sor records up to 16 hours of recorded sound and is worn con-
children may be called on to provide recommendations re- tinuously by the child for at least 10 hours. The accompany-
garding selection of toys for infants and toddlers, particularly ing LENA software conducts automatic analyses of the
for those children and parents for whom reading may not be a recordings and generates estimates of the amount of speech
preferred activity. produced by adults in the childs environment, the number of
The purpose of this controlled experiment was to deter- child vocalizations, the number of adult-child conversational
mine whether the type of toy used during parent-child play turns, and the amount of exposure to electronic noise (eg, tele-
time influences the quantity and quality of the communica- vision). The algorithms used to generate these automatic analy-
tive interaction in ways that are known to be associated with ses are designed to calculate estimates over an extended re-
better language development. cording period rather than specific counts during a short period.

E2 JAMA Pediatrics Published online December 23, 2015 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Central Michigan University User on 12/25/2015


Toy Type and Parent-Infant Communication Original Investigation Research

For that reason, the automatic analyses were used to obtain a speechlike utterance consisting of, at minimum, a voiced
general information about the language environment of each vowel. Non-speechlike utterances, such as cries, grunts, and
child but were not used in data analysis for the main research laughs, were not coded as child vocalizations. A conversa-
questions. tional turn was a speechlike utterance by either the parent or
Each dyad engaged in 2 15-minute play sessions per toy set the infant that occurred within 5 seconds of an immediately
over a 3-day period, resulting in 30 minutes of play per day. preceding utterance by the other speaker. Parent response was
Participants were given a daily log indicating which toy set to a speechlike response by the parent that occurred within 5 sec-
play with each day and in which order. To minimize potential onds of an immediately preceding child vocalization. A par-
order effects, 6 different daily logs were generated, each rep- ent utterance was only counted as a parent response if it was
resenting a different presentation order. As participants were determined to be a direct response to the preceding child ut-
enrolled, they were assigned a daily log-in sequential order terance; a parent utterance that occurred within the 5-sec-
(from 1 to 6 and then starting again at 1). All but 1 participant ond window but was judged to be unrelated to the previous
followed the order presented on their daily log; this partici- child vocalization and not prompted by the child vocaliza-
pant did only 1 play session on the first day and 3 play ses- tion (eg, speaking to a pet or other adult) was not counted as a
sions on the second day. Participants were instructed to en- parent response. The conversational turn measure is a purely
gage in play sessions at their convenience during the day and quantitative measure of back-and-forth vocal interactions be-
play sessions did not need to be back to back. Parents were in- tween infant and parent while parent response measures di-
structed to play as they usually do with their child and were rect parental verbal responsiveness to a childs vocalization.
not explicitly told to minimize distractions (eg, presence of sib- Per-minute counts for each outcome variable were averaged
lings, pets, and television). Start and stop times for each play across the 2 play sessions with each toy set; this average per-
session were recorded by the parent on the daily log so that minute count was used for all analyses. Reliability coding was
they could be found easily on the audio recordings for coding conducted by a second research assistant for all of the play ses-
and analysis. sions for 15 of the participants.

Toy Sets Statistical Analysis


All toys were selected based on their potential to elicit speech Pearson-product moment correlations were calculated be-
centered on 3 semantic themes: animal names, colors, and tween coder 1 and coder 2 results for each outcome variable
shapes. The electronic toy set consisted of 3 battery-operated in each toy condition. Intercoder agreement was very high for
toys with buttons and switches that can be manipulated to pro- all outcome variables in all conditions (overall r = 0.981; range,
duce lights, words, phrases, and songs. The electronic toys in- 0.925-0.999). Coders could not be blinded to the condition be-
cluded a baby laptop, a talking farm, and a baby cell phone. cause the toy set was identifiable by listening to the play ses-
These 3 toys were selected because they are marketed as edu- sion.
cational toys that promote language development for chil- My research team and I analyzed the data using repeated
dren in this age range and are advertised as teaching animal measures analysis of variance for each of the 5 outcome vari-
names, colors, and shapes. The traditional toy set consisted ables, with the toy set as the within-participants factor. This
of 3 nonelectronic toys that also have the potential to teach ani- design allows each dyad to serve as its own control, thereby
mal names, colors, and shapes. These traditional toys in- eliminating the influence of dyad-specific factors that may in-
cluded a farm animal chunky wooden puzzle, a shape-sorter fluence the general quality of the parent-infant communica-
with 10 pieces representing 5 different shapes and 5 different tion interaction (eg, age of the child, developmental level of
colors, and a set of 10 small multicolored rubber blocks with the child, and socioeconomic status of the family). We used
pictures of animals and common objects on each side. The post hoc analyses to explore differences in outcome mea-
books toy set consisted of 5 board books: 2 with a farm ani- sures between individual toy sets.
mal theme, 2 with a shape theme, and 1 with a color theme.
Three of the books included lift-the-flap opportunities.

Results
Outcome Variables
Each play session was transcribed by a research assistant and Thirty-seven parent-infant dyads were enrolled in the study.
coded for the variables of interest: (1) number of adult words, Seven participants were not home at the scheduled material
(2) number of content-specific words, (3) number of child vo- delivery time and 4 did not complete all study procedures, re-
calizations, (4) number of conversational turns, and (5) num- sulting in useable data from 26 parent-infant dyads. Twenty-
ber of parent responses per minute. These measures were op- five parent participants were biological mothers and 1 parent
erationally defined as follows. An adult word was any word participant was the biological father. Two children were born
produced by the parent during the play session. Words pro- prematurely and were identified with moderate developmen-
duced by other adults who may have been present in the in- tal delay; parents of the other children reported no develop-
teraction were not included in the adult words count. A content- mental concerns. The sex of the child, age of the child, race/
specific word was a word produced by the parent that was in 1 ethnicity of the child, and maternal education are presented
of the 3 semantic categories that the toys were selected to elicit in Table 1. Demographic data from the 7 families who were not
(ie, animal names, colors, and shapes). Child vocalization was home at the delivery time are not available. Of the 4 families

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics Published online December 23, 2015 E3

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Central Michigan University User on 12/25/2015


Research Original Investigation Toy Type and Parent-Infant Communication

who did not complete all study procedures, 2 were Hispanic ported in Table 2 along with repeated-measures analysis of vari-
and 2 were non-Hispanic white, suggesting overrepresenta- ance P values. Main effects of condition were statistically
tion of Hispanic families in the nonparticipant group. significant for all outcome variables. My research team and I
Average per-minute counts with 95% CIs for each out- conducted post hoc analyses for each outcome variable to ex-
come variable by condition are displayed in the Figure and re- amine significant differences for the 3 possible toy set con-
trasts (ie, electronic vs traditional, traditional vs books, and
Table 1. Descriptive Data on Study Participants books vs electronic); Bonferroni-adjusted P values for each
2-way contrast are reported in Table 3.
Characteristic No. (%)
During play with electronic toys, there were fewer adult
Male 12 (46)
words (mean, 39.62; 95% CI, 33.36-45.65), fewer conversa-
Age, mean (SD), mo 13 (2.1)
tional turns (mean, 1.64; 95% CI, 1.12-2.19), fewer parental re-
Race/ethnicity
sponses (mean, 1.31; 95% CI, 0.87-1.77), and fewer produc-
Non-Hispanic white 24 (92)
tions of content-specific words (mean, 1.89; 95% CI, 1.49-
Hispanic 1 (4)
2.35) than during play with traditional toys or books (Table 2).
Native American 1 (4)
Children vocalized less during play with electronic toys (mean
Maternal education
per minute, 2.9; 95% CI, 2.16-3.69) than during play with books
High school diploma 1 (4)
(mean per minute, 3.91; 95% CI, 3.09-4.68). Parents pro-
Some college 3 (12)
duced fewer words during play with traditional toys (mean per
College degree 22 (85)
minute, 55.56; 95% CI, 46.49-64.17) than during play with
books (mean per minute, 66.89; 95% CI, 59.93-74.19) and use
Figure. Means and 95% CIs for Each Outcome Measure By Toy Set of content-specific words was lower during play with tradi-
tional toys (mean per minute, 4.09; 95% CI, 3.26-4.99) than
10
during play with books (mean per minute, 6.96; 95% CI,
Electronic 6.07-7.97).
Frequency per Minute (10 for AW)

Traditional
8 Results showed that the largest and most consistent dif-
Books
ferences were between electronic toys and books (with greater
6
values for all measures with books), followed by electronic toys
and traditional toys (with larger values in the traditional toy
condition for all measures except child vocalization), and the
4 least consistent and smallest differences were between books
and traditional toys (with larger values in the book condition
2 for adult words and content-specific words only).

0
AW CV CT RESP CSW
Discussion
AW indicates adult words; CSW, content-specific words; CT, conversational In this controlled experiment in a natural environment of
turns; CV, child vocalizations; and RESP, parent responses.
parent-infant communicative interaction during play, my

Table 2. Mean Values for Each Outcome Measure by Toy Set, With Repeated-Measures Analysis
of Variance P Value to Test for Significant Effects of the Toy Set

Mean (95% CI)


Outcome Measure Electronic Traditional Books P Value
Adult words 39.62 (33.36-45.65) 55.56 (46.49-64.17) 66.89 (59.93-74.19) <.001
Content-specific words 1.89 (1.49-2.35) 4.09 (3.26-4.99) 6.96 (6.07-7.97) <.001
Child vocalizations 2.9 (2.16-3.69) 3.74 (2.75-4.89) 3.91 (3.09-4.68) .04
Conversational turns 1.64 (1.12-2.19) 2.49 (1.79-3.34) 2.73 (2.04-3.38) <.001
Responses 1.31 (0.87-1.77) 2.09 (1.47-2.79) 2.18 (1.53-2.80) <.001

Table 3. Results of Post Hoc Analyses for Each 2-Way Comparison for Each Outcome Measure

P Valuea
Adult Content Specific Child Conversational
Comparison Words Vocabulary Vocalizations Turns Responses
Electronic vs <.01 <.001 .24 .006 .004
traditional
Traditional vs book <.001 <.001 >.99 >.99 >.99
a
P values using Bonferroni
Book vs electronic <.001 <.001 .01 <.001 <.001
adjustment.

E4 JAMA Pediatrics Published online December 23, 2015 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Central Michigan University User on 12/25/2015


Toy Type and Parent-Infant Communication Original Investigation Research

research team and I found that the type of toy used during the gaged in parent-child play and the nature of the play interac-
play session was significantly associated with both the tion, it is important to acknowledge that the effect of toy type
quantity and quality of language use. For all outcome mea- on parent-infant communication may be different for parents
sures, play with books provided a better communication from different cultural and economic backgrounds.16-18 Fur-
interaction than play with electronic toys. The effect was most thermore, the findings of this study are applicable to children
pronounced for the quantitative measures of parental lan- in a very limited age range of prelinguistic and very early lan-
guage use, suggesting that parents tend to let the toys do the guage development and effects may be different for older tod-
talking for them when their child is interacting with elec- dlers and preschoolers. Another limitation is that investiga-
tronic toys. This is particularly worrisome given that there is tors did not have complete control over the timing of the play
no evidence that children this young are able to learn vocabu- sessions and it is possible that participants may have chosen to
lary from media or other nonhuman interactions.14,15 For the play with certain toy sets when either the parent or child was
2 qualitative measures of parent-infant interaction, conversa- more tired, thereby impacting the communicative interaction.
tional turns and parental responses, play with traditional toys Despite these limitations, the results provide an empiri-
was equivalent to book reading, yet both were superior to play cal basis for primary care professionals and pediatricians for
with electronic toys. While play with books may provide a script making recommendations regarding the types of play activi-
that encourages parents to talk more and produce more words ties that promote rich language interactions.
associated with the themes in the books, it was not superior
to play with traditional toys in promoting parent-child con-
versations and parental responsiveness. For parents who may
not be inclined to read to their preverbal infants or for par-
Conclusions
ents whose children do not prefer book reading activities, play These results provide a basis for discouraging the purchase of
with traditional toys, such as blocks and shape sorters, may electronic toys that are promoted as educational and are of-
be an equally valuable use of limited parent-infant play time. ten quite expensive. These results also add to the large body
This is consistent with and may help explain results of previ- of evidence supporting the potential benefits of book reading
ous work that found that distribution of blocks to families of with very young children. They also expand on this by dem-
young children resulted in better child language ability.12 onstrating that play with traditional toys may result in com-
While the results of this study are robust given that the play municative interactions that are as rich as those that occur dur-
sessions were conducted in the participants homes rather than ing book reading. I do not claim that book reading and play with
in a controlled laboratory setting, there are important limita- traditional toys are developmentally equivalent activities; book
tions that should be taken into account. The most notable limi- reading provides numerous benefits that cannot be repli-
tations were the small sample size and the relative homoge- cated in other activities (eg, literacy socialization and expo-
neity of participants by race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status. sure to novel vocabulary and concepts) and play with toys, such
Given the robustness of the results despite the small sample as blocks and puzzles, provides developmental and cognitive
size, however, it is likely that the findings would be replicated benefits beyond the language domain.19-22 However, if the em-
with a larger group of participants. The question of whether re- phasis is on activities that promote a rich communicative in-
sults would differ for families from more diverse demo- teraction between parents and infants, both play with tradi-
graphic backgrounds is an important one that remains to be an- tional toys and book reading can be promoted as language-
swered. Given that cultural and socioeconomic factors are facilitating activities while play with electronic toys should be
known to influence both the amount of time spent en- discouraged.

ARTICLE INFORMATION Additional Contributions: The following 4. Zimmerman FJ, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, et al.
Accepted for Publication: October 18, 2015. individuals participated in data coding while Teaching by listening: the importance of adult-child
graduate students at Northern Arizona University conversations to language development. Pediatrics.
Published Online: December 23, 2015. and received hourly financial compensation as 2009;124(1):342-349.
doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.3753. student research assistants, providing statistical 5. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Baumwell L.
Author Contributions: Dr Sosa had full access to all consultation and analysis assistance for this project: Maternal responsiveness and childrens
of the data in the study and takes responsibility for Lauren Crane, MS, Brooke Santos, MS, Heather achievement of language milestones. Child Dev.
the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the Wiest, MS, and Luke Plonsky, PhD (Northern 2001;72(3):748-767.
data analysis. Arizona University).
6. Tanimura M, Okuma K, Kyoshima K. Television
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported. viewing, reduced parental utterance, and delayed
REFERENCES
Funding/Support: This study was funded by a speech development in infants and young children.
research grant from the American Speech- 1. Rowe ML. A longitudinal investigation of the role Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(6):618-619.
Language-Hearing Foundation. of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in
vocabulary development. Child Dev. 2012;83(5): 7. Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, et al.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funder had no 1762-1774. Audible television and decreased adult words,
role in the design and conduct of the study; infant vocalizations, and conversational turns:
collection, management, analysis, and 2. Hart B, Risley T. The early catastrophe. Am Educ. a population-based study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.
interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or 2003;27(4):6-9. 2009;163(6):554-558.
approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit 3. Huttenlocher J. Language input and language 8. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN.
the manuscript for publication. growth. Prev Med. 1998;27(2):195-199. Associations between media viewing and language

jamapediatrics.com (Reprinted) JAMA Pediatrics Published online December 23, 2015 E5

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Central Michigan University User on 12/25/2015


Research Original Investigation Toy Type and Parent-Infant Communication

development in children under age 2 years. J Pediatr. 14. Krcmar M, Grela B, Lin K. Can toddlers learn 19. Ginsburg KR; American Academy of Pediatrics
2007;151(4):364-368. vocabulary from television? an experimental Committee on Communications; American
9. Mendelsohn AL, Mogilner LN, Dreyer BP, et al. approach. Media Psychol. 2007;10(1):41-63. Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Psychosocial
The impact of a clinic-based literacy intervention on 15. Roseberry S, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM. Aspects of Child and Family Health. The importance
language development in inner-city preschool Skype me! socially contingent interactions help of play in promoting healthy child development and
children. Pediatrics. 2001;107(1):130-134. toddlers learn language. Child Dev. 2014;85(3):956- maintaining strong parent-child bonds. Pediatrics.
970. 2007;119(1):182-191.
10. Klass P, Neddlman R, Zuckerman B. Reach Out
and Read Program Manual. 2nd ed. Boston, MA: 16. Milteer RM, Ginsburg KR; Council on 20. Verdine BN, Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K,
Boston Medical Center; 1999. Communications and Media; Committee on Newcombe NS. Finding the missing piece: blocks,
Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health. puzzles, and shapes fuel school readiness. Trends
11. Tamis-LeMonda C, Bornstein MH. Specificity in Neurosci Educ. 2014;3(1):7-13.
mother-toddler language-play relations across the The importance of play in promoting healthy child
second year. Dev Psychol. 1994;30(2):283-292. doi: development and maintaining strong parent-child 21. High PC, Klass P; Council on Early Childhood.
10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.283. bond: focus on children in poverty. Pediatrics. 2012; Literacy promotion: an essential component of
129(1):e204-e213. primary care pediatric practice. Pediatrics. 2014;134
12. Christakis DA, Zimmerman FJ, Garrison MM. (2):404-409.
Effect of block play on language acquisition and 17. Weitzman CC, Roy L, Walls T, Tomlin R. More
attention in toddlers: a pilot randomized controlled evidence for reach out and read: a home-based 22. Fletcher KL, Reese E. Picture book reading with
trial. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(10):967-971. study. Pediatrics. 2004;113(5):1248-1253. young children: a conceptual framework. Dev Rev.
18. Hwa-Froelich DA. Play assessment for children 2005;25(1):64-103.
13. Brown A; Council on Communications and
Media. Media use by children younger than 2 years. from culturally and linguistically diverse
Pediatrics. 2011;128(5):1040-1045. backgrounds. Perspect Commun Disord Sci Cult
Linguist Diverse Popul. 2004;11(2):5-9.

E6 JAMA Pediatrics Published online December 23, 2015 (Reprinted) jamapediatrics.com

Copyright 2015 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archpedi.jamanetwork.com/ by a Central Michigan University User on 12/25/2015

Potrebbero piacerti anche