Sei sulla pagina 1di 15

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/223775018

A model of emotional schemas

Article in Cognitive and Behavioral Practice June 2002


DOI: 10.1016/S1077-7229(02)80048-7

CITATIONS READS

106 3,893

1 author:

Robert L Leahy
Weill Cornell Medical College
66 PUBLICATIONS 1,180 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Robert L Leahy on 02 February 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


177

A M o d e l o f Emotional S c h e m a s
R o b e r t L. Leahy, A m e r i c a n Institute f o r Cognitive Therapy, N e w York, a n d W e i l l - C o r n e l l University M e d i c a l College

Three theoretical models of the relationship between cognition and emotion are examined: (a) ventilation theory (i.e., the greater ex-
pression of emotion, the better the outcome), (b) emotionally focused therapy (i.e., activation, expression, and validation of emotion
facilitate acceptance and self-understanding), and (c) a cognitive model of emotional processing (i.e., individuals differ in their con-
ceptualization and strategies in responding to emotion). A self-report assessment of emotional schemas reflecting 14 dimensions re-
lated to cognitive processing and strategies of emotional response is presented. Fifty-three adult psychotherapy patients were assessed
and their responses on the emotional schemas evaluation were correlated with the Beck Depression Inventory and the Beck Anxiety In-
ventory. There was strong support for a cognitive model of emotional processing. Depression was related to greater guilt over emotion,
expectation of longer duration, greater rumination, and viewing one's emotions as less comprehensible, less controllable, and as dif-
ferent from the emotions others have. Anxiety was related to greater guilt over emotion, a more simplistic view of emotion, greater ru-
mination, viewing one's emotions as less comprehensible, less acceptance offeelings, viewing emotions as less controllable, and as dif-
ferent from the emotions others have. Dimensions related to the strict ventilation model--such as validation, numbness, and
expression--were not related to depression or anxiety, although acceptance of feelings was related to less anxiety. Support was found
for the emotional-focus model. Validation was related to less guilt, less simplistic ideas of emotion, expectation of shorter duration,
less rumination, and to viewing emotion as more comprehensible, more controllable, more similar to emotions of others, and more
acceptance offeelings.

THOUGH the cognitive model does imply that emo- spond (Teasdale, 1999; Wells, 1995; Wells & Carter, 1999,
A tions and cognitive schemata may be reciprocally
activated, the place of emotions in the cognitive model
2001). For example, once the individual notices that he is
anxious and worried, the next issue that arises is the in-
has not been fully developed. Several lines of work con- terpretation that the individual gives to such an experi-
verge on the importance of emotions in the cognitive ence. Thus, in Wells's meta-cognitive model, the occur-
model. First, a general cognitive model suggests that rence of worry (as an anxious emotional experience)
mood, or emotion, may affect attentional and memorial may activate beliefs about the implications of this emo-
bias and that cognitive priming may determine the emo- tional s t a t e - - f o r example, that worry will make one sick
tions and memories that are elicited (see Clark, Beck, & or crazy or that worry must be controlled, lest it go com-
Alford, 1999; Riskind, 1989). Beck (1996) has advanced pletely out of control. Similar interpretations of anxiety
the idea of modes to account for systemic organization occur in panic d i s o r d e r - - " M y emotions or sensations will
that is activated that coordinates behavior, emotion, deci- last forever and drive me crazy." Or in social p h o b i a - -
sions, and other factors. For example, the depressive "Other people will humiliate me if they knew how I felt"
mode functions as a superordinate organizing system (Wells & Papageorgiou, 1995, 1998). Related to this
that coordinates automatic thoughts, assumptions, sche- awareness of emotion as a "starting point," rational emo-
mas, rules of interpersonal conduct, behavioral activa- tive behavior therapists (REBTs) have proposed that ac-
tion, and other factors. ceptance of the emotion may be an essential aspect of
Second, empirical work on exposure with PTSD indi- t r e a t m e n t - - t h a t is, it may reflect the "given" to which the
cates that the fear structure, underlying the fear schema, patient then responds (see DiGiuseppe, 1988; Dryden,
must be fully activated in order for exposure to be effec- 1990). Similarly, Hayes's (Hayes, Jacobson, & Follette,
tive (see Ehlers & Clark, 2000; Foa & Kozak, 1986). Inhi- 1994) "acceptance" model suggests that emotional accep-
bition of the activation of these emotionally based cogni- tance may be an essential c o m p o n e n t in treatment. The
tions impairs the effect of clinical exposure. model of mindfulness in cognitive therapy also suggests
Third, meta-cognitive models propose that emotions that the experience or processing of emotions, sensa-
are experiential events to which people differentially re- tions, or feelings is an important factor in the vulnerabil-
ity to depression (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002).
Cognitive and Behavioral Practice 9, 177-190, 2002 Fourth, excessive self-focus, including ruminative think-
1077-7229/02/177-19051.00/0 ing and centering on one's t h o u g h t s and condition,
Copyright 2002 by Association for Advancement of Behavior has been associated with dysphoria and with increased
Therapy. All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
likelihood of and duration of depressive episodes as well
L~ Continuing Education Quiz located on p. 260. as anxiety (see Carver & Scheier, 1990; Nolen-Hoeksema,
178 Leahy

2000; Wells & Carter, 2001). Ruminative style, as con- berg & Safran, 1987; G r e e n b e r g , Watson, & G o l d m a n ,
trasted with an instrumental o r task-oriented style, con- 1998; Safran, 1998). G r e e n b e r g ' s m o d e l o f Emotional Fo-
fers g r e a t e r risk for depression a n d for r e p e a t e d episodes cus T h e r a p y (EFT) suggests that e m o t i o n s are a form o f
o f depression. information processing whereby the individual experi-
Fifth, research on "ironic effects" indicates that at- ences the m e a n i n g of events. For G r e e n b e r g and col-
tempts to suppress an u n w a n t e d t h o u g h t lead to r e b o u n d leagues, emotional schemas entail an organizing structure
effects such that the t h o u g h t occurs m o r e frequently at a by which an e m o t i o n contains the "meaning" o r "cogni-
later time (Wegner & Zanakos, 1994). Earlier behavioral tions." Thus, in the emotional-focused model, emotions
a p p r o a c h e s that advocated thought-stopping have now may be a "prime-mover" o f cognitions a n d contain a
b e e n discredited a n d behavioral a p p r o a c h e s to unwanted "truth" for the individual not s u b s u m e d by the m o r e
thoughts, such as p u r e obsessions or h y p o c h o n d r i a c h a l cold, rational c o n t e n t o f cognition. Thus, like physical
thoughts, include t h o u g h t exposure or flooding (Foa, pain, emotions tell us what is b o t h e r i n g us a n d that some-
Stetekee, & R o t h b a u m , 1989; Salkovskis & Kirk, 1997). thing needs to be changed. A c c o r d i n g to the EFT model,
Thus, the cognitive m o d e l o f e m o t i o n has stressed the ef- emotions allow the individual to activate the cognitive
fects o f information processing, emotion-cognition link- schemas that then provide an o p p o r t u n i t y for validation
age, meta-cognition of the plans and purposes of thoughLs by others o f meaning, self-understanding, a n d recogni-
related to u n w a n t e d e m o t i o n a l states, a n d attempts at tion o f what the individual needs. M t h o u g h EFT is a
suppression o r overcontrol of emotion. m o d e l of therapy a n d the i m p o r t a n c e o f "abreaction" o f
T h e theoretical tradition r e p r e s e n t e d by Rogerian emotions in treatment, the p r e s e n t study examines the
(Rogers, 1965) a n d psychodynamic m o d e l s stresses the role o f e m o t i o n a l e x p e r i e n c e in daily life. EFT does have
i m p o r t a n c e o f e m o t i o n a l expression. However, e m o t i o n a l implications o f how individuals will r e s p o n d to their
e x p r e s s i o n - - t h a t is, expressing u n p l e a s a n t e m o t i o n s - - e m o t i o n s - - p l a c i n g emphasis on expression, validation,
has had a m i x e d review. The expression, or catharsis, o f self-understanding, clarification, and recognition of needs
e m o t i o n s in therapy often results in an i m m e d i a t e in- that are c o m p o n e n t s o f the e m o t i o n a l experience. Thus,
crease o f arousal a n d negative affect (Kennedy-Moore & we might expect that to the d e g r e e the individual is able
Watson, 1999; P e n n e b a k e r & Beall, 1986; Smyth, 1 9 9 8 ) - to express e m o t i o n s that are validated, there should be
contrary to the ventilation hypothesis--an assumption un- an increased acceptance of those e m o t i o n s a n d an in-
derlying catharsis t r e a t m e n t models such as Freudian crease in dimensions related to e m o t i o n a l processing.
therapy or bioenergetics. In contrast to the foregoing,
longer-term effects o f e m o t i o n a l expression may be m o r e Schemas in Emotional Processing
positive. F o r example, P e n n e b a k e r a n d his colleagues P e n n e b a k e r has d e f i n e d e m o t i o n a l processing as de-
( P e n n e b a k e r & Beall; P e n n e b a k e r & Francis, 1996) have creased inhibition o f emotion, increased self-understand-
f o u n d that, whereas subjects felt worse immediately after ing, a n d e n h a n c e d positive self-reflection (Pennebaker,
writing out their negative experiences, they e n d e d u p Mayne, & Francis, 1997). A focus on e m o t i o n a l process-
feeling b e t t e r weeks a n d m o n t h s later. This may be due to ing would include factors that are operative once an emo-
the fact that simply identifying, experiencing, a n d writing tion has b e e n experienced. This would include recogni-
o u t the negative experiences resulted in greater clarifica- tion a n d labeling of an emotion, attempts to inhibit or
tion o f the issues and incidents involved a n d an increased even magnify an emotion, activation o f hypervigilance
awareness that these e m o t i o n s would not be overwhelm- a n d problem-solving strategies, expression o r ventilation,
ing. Repressive c o p i n g stTle, alexithymia, a n d overem- reliance on a receptive a n d supportive audience, distrac-
phasis on rationality or anti-emotionality are associated tion, a n d e x a m i n a t i o n of one's own t h o u g h t distortions.
with longer-term somatic problems, such as increased Consider the following two individuals: Worried Willy
risk of hypertension, cancer, asthma, a n d general somatic a n d Michael Mensch. Both learn that their desirable
complaints (see Grossarth-Maticek, Bastiaans, & Kanazir, p a r t n e r s - - b o t h , coincidentally, n a m e d M a r y - - h a v e j u s t
1985; Grossarth-Maticek, Kanazir, Schmidt, & Vetter, d u m p e d them. Worried Willy, frustrated in his goal of at-
1985; Schwartz, 1995; Taylor, Parker, Bagby, & Acklin, taining perfect r o m a n c e , notices that he has b e c o m e
1992). A l t h o u g h there is no clear evidence that overem- emotionally u n c o m f o r t a b l e with this news. H e recognizes
phasis on rationality is related to depression or anxiety, that he is upset, but he has a hard time initially labeling
these longer-term effects r e p r e s e n t an o m i n o u s conse- the feelings. H e notices that he is feeling angry, b u t he
q u e n c e o f e m o t i o n a l c o p i n g style. believes that he should n o t be angry with s o m e o n e he
T h e cognitive-interpersonal, experiential, a n d emo- presumably loves. H e is afraid o f expressing this anger,
tional expression a p p r o a c h e s to therapy stress the impor- lest Mary find out a n d closes off any chance o f reconcilia-
tance o f e m o t i o n a l e x p e r i e n c e a n d the processing o f tion. He believes that he c a n n o t share his a n g e r a n d sad-
e m o t i o n s in therapy ( G r e e n b e r g & Paivio, 1997; Green- ness with others, because they might view him as a burden.
Emotional Schemas 179

H e finds it h a r d to u n d e r s t a n d why he is so sad, since he macy a n d c o m m i t m e n t are i m p o r t a n t to h i m and, al-


has only known Mary for 2 months, a n d he feels a s h a m e d t h o u g h he will n o t have that with Missing Mary, he will
o f b e i n g so " d e p e n d e n t " o n her. H e is f u r t h e r confused, look for it with s o m e o n e else. Rather than sit at h o m e ru-
since he c a n n o t reconcile his conflicting emotions, be- m i n a t i n g a b o u t his situation, he has s c h e d u l e d a n u m b e r
lieving that you "either love s o m e o n e or you hate t h e m - - of possibly productive experiences, such as seeing friends,
b u t never both." H e is afraid that his sadness a n d a n g e r exercising, work, a n d a date with Jane. Michael likes to
m i g h t go o u t of control, so he worries a b o u t his feelings think o f himself as rational, b u t he also balances this with
a n d thinks that his u n h a p p i n e s s will last forever. H e won- the awareness that, like o t h e r people, he will feel badly
ders if he is the entire cause o f his u n h a p p i n e s s , believing after a breakup, b u t that the feelings are simply a sign o f
that he does n o t have a right to be angry. H e criticizes b e i n g a mensch.
himself for b e i n g so "needy" a n d views his desire for Mary
as a sign of his inferiority as a man. W o r r i e d Willy sits in A Cognitive Model o f Emotional Processing
his a p a r t m e n t , sipping a drink, focusing on how b a d he Most p r i o r descriptions by cognitive theorists o f the
is, r e a d i n g the Book o f Job, asking God, "Why me?" H e role o f e m o t i o n s have stressed the i m p o r t a n c e of "prim-
no l o n g e r spends time with his friends a n d he has missed ing" f a c t o r s - - f o r example, the activation o f negative
work. Sometimes he wishes that he could j u s t feel n u m b schemas o r thoughts a n d the selective attention a n d
a n d he finds that a few scotches will d o the trick. Worried m e m o r y for negative c o n t e n t (Riskind, 1989). An excep-
Willy looks at his worries as a sign o f his weakness, claim- tion to the s t a n d a r d information-processing m o d e l is the
ing to himself that he should always be rational, since he meta-cognitive m o d e l o f Wells a n d his colleagues (see
has a law d e g r e e a n d d i d quite well on the licensing P a p a g e o r g i o u & Wells, 2001a, 2001b; Wells & Carter,
exam. H e fears that his strong negative feelings will per- 2001). T h e m o d e l that I advance draws on the work by
sist into the summer, r u i n i n g his experiences at the b e a c h Wells, b u t attempts to stress various c o p i n g strategies that
house that he d r e a m e d o f sharing with Missing Mary. individuals m i g h t utilize once an e m o t i o n has b e e n acti-
W o r r i e d Willy is certain that n o o n e shares these pathetic vated. This cognitive m o d e l of emotions is d e p i c t e d in the
a n d confusing feelings and, therefore, he is reluctant to schematic in Figure 1. In this m o d e l I use the term emo-
share t h e m with others. tional schemas to refer to plans, concepts, a n d strategies
In contrast, Michael Mensch is fully aware o f his range e m p l o y e d in "response to" an emotion. This is a definition
o f f e e l i n g s - - a n g e r , anxiety, sadness, a n d even a touch of that c o m p l e m e n t s G r e e n b e r g and colleagues' ( G r e e n b e r g
hope. Initially upset with the news that Mary is gone a n d & Paivio, 1997; G r e e n b e r g & Safran, 1987) view of emo-
missing, he r e c o g n i z e d that his feelings were n e i t h e r pos- tional schemas as emotions that "contain," or give access
itive n o r negative, b u t simply " h u m a n " - - a sign that he to, the cognitions. I view b o t h kinds o f schemas as valid.
h a d the fullness o f e x p e r i e n c e that gave his family the
n a m e "Mensch" generations ago. H e is currently having
d i n n e r with his friend, U n d e r s t a n d i n g Ed, with w h o m
Michael feels c o n f i d e n t that he can express his feelings
a n d have a receptive audience. Michael finds that this ex- ~ ~ Ip[Attentiontoemotion]
pression helps him clarify his feelings, recognize that
others m i g h t feel the same way, a n d helps him see that he
has a right to feel a range o f things. H e recognizes that,
with the breakup, it makes sense to have conflicting feel- [ Emt~n
IS~ i ! i o ~ 1 gutli | Inteorpre!atins:
(Negat~ive
ings, only because life a n d relationships are complicated. |,|o lack
ofconsensus
simplistic
view withothers
Thus, he feels sad because he is losing a partner, angry n |o incomprehensible
because of h e r carelessness in telling him by e-mail, a n d |,~,ocannotacceptemotion
overlyrational
relieved because she was "high m a i n t e n a n c e " to b e g i n
with. Even t h o u g h he may feel intense sadness at times,
he knows that these feelings will n o t overwhelm him, they x~ [ Emotionispr0blematic]
can be c o n t r o l l e d to some extent, a n d that they will n o t
last forever. Consequently, a l t h o u g h he enjoys a fine
Guinness Stout with Ed, he does n o t feel a n e e d to n u m b ~.learn
himself with a d r i n k i n g binge. His view of the s i t u a t i o n - -

blame
worry
a n d his r e s p o n s e - - i s that the sadness he feels is due to
b o t h internal a n d external f a c t o r s - - t h a t is, a consequence avoidsituations
others thatelicitemotions
o f the b r e a k u p a n d his reliance on s o m e o n e who was n o t
that reliable. H e feels sad, he recognizes, because inti- Figure I. Metacognitive schematic of emotions.
180 Leahy

Let us i m a g i n e that an emotion, such as anger, has that validation might h e l p the individual c o m p r e h e n d
b e e n activated. T h e first step involves a t t e n d i n g to the a n d d e p a t h o l o g i z e an emotion.
e m o t i o n - - f o r example, labeling the e m o t i o n ("de- Comprehensibility. Do the e m o t i o n s make sense to the
pressed," "angry," or "afraid"). Two distinct pathways are sell?. This is reflected in responses to the following: "There
available, e i t h e r n o r m a l i z i n g o r pathologizing the emo- are things about myself that I just don't understand" (re-
tion. If the individual normalizes his a n g e l he can move versed), "My feelings d o n ' t make sense to me" (reversed), "I
on quickly to accepting, expressing, e x p e r i e n c i n g valida- think that my feelings are strange or weird" (reversed), "My
tion a n d l e a r n i n g from his experience. Alternatively, the feelings seem to come out of nowhere" (reversed). This di-
individual could pathologize his emotion. For example, mension reflects a cognitive appraisal o f e m o t i o n a n d
noticing that the e m o t i o n is u n c o m f o r t a b l e , she could would be viewed as an i m p o r t a n t e l e m e n t in anxiety a n d
choose the pathway o f cognitive avoidance, resulting in depression. For example, cognitive models o f anxiety
dissociative processes, bingeing, drinking, or e m o t i o n a l stress that the anxious individual often gives a catastrophic
numbness. Because the e m o t i o n (or the e x p e r i e n c e that interpretation o f his feelings ("I am going crazy"), con-
gave rise to the e m o t i o n ) has n o t b e e n adequately pro- fused interpretation ("What's h a p p e n i n g to me?"), or
cessed, she may feel a loss o f control over emotions, pathologizes his e m o t i o n s ("I s h o u l d n ' t be feeling this
thereby c o n c l u d i n g that the e m o t i o n s will last a long way" o r "This is a sign o f s o m e t h i n g deeply wrong with
time. This may, in some cases, result in r u m i n a t i o n or me"). The cognitive models o f anxiety, cited above, ad-
worry, avoidance o f p r o b l e m a t i c situations, or even blam- vanced by Clark, Salkovskis, and Wells, all suggest that the
ing others. T h e c o n s e q u e n c e o f this is a set of negative in- individual's pathological i n t e r p r e t a t i o n o f e m o t i o n a l
terpretations o f e m o t i o n s - - s u c h that e m o t i o n s may elicit arousal may increase anxiety.
guilt, a p p e a r to be d i f f e r e n t f r o m those o f others, o r Guilt. This d i m e n s i o n represents shame, guilt, a n d
a p p e a r i n c o m p r e h e n s i b l e - - i m p l y i n g that o n e c a n n o t ac- e m b a r r a s s m e n t a b o u t an e m o t i o n - - t h e belief that o n e
c e p t these e m o t i o n s a n d that o n e is helpless in c o p i n g should not have certain feelings. This is reflected by re-
with the emotion. sponses to the following: "Some feelings are wrong to
have," "I feel a s h a m e d of my feelings," "I want p e o p l e to
Emotional Schema D i m e n s i o n s believe that I am different from the way I truly feel," a n d
T h e cognitive m o d e l that I am advancing proposes "I s h o u l d n ' t have some o f the feelings that I have." For ex-
that individuals may differ as to how they conceptualize ample, some p e o p l e believe that it is wrong to have cer-
their emotions; in o t h e r words, individuals have different tain sexual or aggressive feelings, while others accept
schemas a b o u t emotions. These schemas reflect the ways e m o t i o n s a n d fantasies as part o f the h u m a n experience.
in which e m o t i o n s are e x p e r i e n c e d a n d what the individ- A m o n g self-critical depressives, there is a c o m m o n belief
ual believes are a p p r o p r i a t e plans to execute once an un- that one should n o t be depressed, f u r t h e r a d d i n g to the
pleasant e m o t i o n has arisen. In o r d e r to develop this depression. Obsessive-compulsive patients, who worry
model, I have o u t l i n e d 14 dimensions along which emo- that their thoughts will lead to immoral, irresponsible or
tional schemas may be u n d e r s t o o d . I shall p r e s e n t a b r i e f d a n g e r o u s behavior (Rachman, 1993) t h r o u g h "thought-
description of each o f these dimensions, along with the action fusion," are hypervigilant a n d a t t e m p t to suppress
implications of variations on each dimension. A self- o r divert their e m o t i o n s a n d thoughts (see Salkovskis &
r e p o r t questionnaire, the Leahy Emotional Schemas Scale Kirk, 1997).
(LESS), was developed to assess these 14 dimensions. T h e Simplistic view of emotion. The ability to u n d e r s t a n d that
items on the LESS are shown in Table 1 a n d the 14 di- one can have conflicting a n d c o m p l i c a t e d feelings a b o u t
mensions, with respective items, are shown in Table 2. self a n d o t h e r s is a sign o f h i g h e r level o f ego function-
Validation by others. T h e individual believes that there ing, cognitive differentiation, a n d cognitive complexity
is a receptive a u d i e n c e for his emotions as indicated by (Loevinger, 1976). More differentiated thinking allows
responses such as "Others u n d e r s t a n d a n d accept nay the individual the o p p o r t u n i t y to c o o r d i n a t e a p p a r e n t l y
feelings," "I d o n ' t want anyone to know a b o u t some o f my conflicting feelings, which are inevitable at times. T h e de-
feelings" (reversed), "No one really cares a b o u t my feel- m a n d for unilateral, polarized, o r simplistic thinking may
ings" (reversed). Emotionally focused therapy, drawing result in emotions a n d information b e i n g e x c l u d e d from
on the Rogerian tradition of u n c o n d i t i o n a l positive re- awareness, thereby requiring excessive attempts to ex-
gard, suggests that validation should be an i m p o r t a n t ele- clude certain emotions. As Linehan's (1993) dialectical
m e n t in the r e d u c t i o n of anxiety or depression. In addi- m o d e l suggests, the ability to c o o r d i n a t e conflicting feel-
tion, validation should assist in h e l p i n g the individual ings is an essential a t t a i n m e n t in the self-system such that
accept, c o m p r e h e n d his feeling, a n d provide interper- the failure in this c o o r d i n a t i o n may result in volatility in
sonal benefits (Rime, Mesquita, Philippot, & Boca, 1991). m o o d . Mayer a n d Salovey (1997) have suggested that
A cognitive- o r emotional-focused m o d e l would suggest "emotional intelligence" entails an ability to u n d e r s t a n d
Emotional Schemas 181

Table 1
Emotional SchemaQuestionnaire

We are i n t e r e s t e d in h o w you d e a l w i t h y o u r f e e l i n g s o r e m o t i o n s - - f o r e x a m p l e , h o w y o u d e a l with f e e l i n g s of anger, sadness, anxiety, o r


s e x u a l feelings. We all differ in h o w we d e a l with t h e s e feelings, so t h e r e are n o r i g h t o r w r o n g answers. Please r e a d e a c h s e n t e n c e c a r e f u l l y
a n d a n s w e r e a c h s e n t e n c e , u s i n g the scale below, as to h o w y o u d e a l w i t h y o u r f e e l i n g s d u r i n g t h e past m o n t h . P u t the n u m b e r o f y o u r
r e s p o n s e n e x t to t h e s e n t e n c e .

Scale: 1 = very u n t r u e o f m e
2 = somewhat untrue of me
3 = slightly u n t r u e o f m e
4 = slightly t r u e o f m e
5 = s o m e w h a t t r u e of m e
6 = very true o f m e

1. , m W h e n I feel down, I try to t h i n k a b o u t a d i f f e r e n t way to view things.


2. _ _ W h e n I have a f e e l i n g t h a t b o t h e r s me, I try to t h i n k o f why it is n o t i m p o r t a n t .
3. _ _ I often t h i n k t h a t I r e s p o n d with f e e l i n g s t h a t o t h e r s w o u l d n o t have.
4. _ _ S o m e f e e l i n g s are w r o n g to have.
5. _ _ T h e r e are t h i n g s a b o u t m y s e l f t h a t I j u s t d o n ' t u n d e r s t a n d .
6. _ _ I believe t h a t it is i m p o r t a n t to let m y s e l f cry in o r d e r to g e t m y f e e l i n g s "out."
7. _ _ If I let m y s e l f have s o m e o f t h e s e feelings, I fear I will lose c o n t r o l .
8. _ _ O t h e r s u n d e r s t a n d a n d a c c e p t slay feelings.
9. _ _ You c a n ' t allow y o u r s e l f to have c e r t a i n k i n d s o f f e e l i n g s - - l i k e f e e l i n g s a b o u t sex o r v i o l e n c e .
10. My f e e l i n g s d o n ' t m a k e s e n s e to me.
11. _ _ If o t h e r p e o p l e c h a n g e d , I w o u l d feel a lot better.
1 2 . I t h i n k t h a t t h e r e are f e e l i n g s t h a t I have t h a t I a m n o t really aware of.
13. _ _ I s o m e t i m e s fear t h a t if ! a l l o w e d m y s e l f to have a s t r o n g feeling, it w o u l d n o t g o away.
14. _ _ I feel a s h a m e d of my feelings.
15. _ _ T h i n g s t h a t b o t h e r o t h e r p e o p l e d o n ' t b o t h e r me.
16. _ _ No o n e really cares a b o u t m y feelings.
17. _ _ It is i m p o r t a n t for m e to b e r e a s o n a b l e a n d p r a c t i c a l r a t h e r t h a n sensitive a n d o p e n to nay feelings.
18. _ _ I c a n ' t s t a n d it w h e n I have c o n t r a d i c t o r y f e e l i n g s - - l i k e l i k i n g a n d d i s l i k i n g t h e s a m e p e r s o n .
19. _ _ I a m m u c h m o r e sensitive t h a n o t h e r p e o p l e .
20. _ _ I try to g e t rid o f a n u n p l e a s a n t f e e l i n g i m m e d i a t e l y .
21. _ _ W h e n I feel down, I try to t h i n k of the snore i m p o r t a n t t h i n g s in l i f e - - w h a t I value.
22. _ _ W h e n I feel d o w n o r sad, I q u e s t i o n m y values.
23. _ _ I feel t h a t I c a n e x p r e s s my f e e l i n g s openly.
24. _ _ I o f t e n say to myself, " W h a t ' s w r o n g with me?"
25. _ _ I t h i n k o f m y s e l f as a s h a l l o w person.
26. _ _ I w a n t p e o p l e to b e l i e v e t h a t I a m d i f f e r e n t f r o m t h e way I truly feel.
27. _ _ I worry t h a t I w o n ' t b e a b l e to c o n t r o l m y feelings.
28. _ _ You have to g u a r d a g a i n s t h a v i n g c e r t a i n feelings.
29. _ _ S t r o n g f e e l i n g s o n l y last a s h o r t p e r i o d o f time.
30. _ _ You c a n ' t rely o n y o u r f e e l i n g s to tell y o u w h a t is g o o d for you.
31. _ _ I s h o u l d n ' t have s o m e o f the f e e l i n g s t h a t I have.
3 2 . I often feel " n u m b " e m o t i o n a l l y - - l i k e I have n o feelings.
33. _ _ I t h i n k t h a t my f e e l i n g s are s t r a n g e o r weird.
34. _ _ O t h e r p e o p l e cause m e to have u n p l e a s a n t feelings.
35. _ _ W h e n I have c o n f l i c t i n g f e e l i n g s a b o u t s o m e o n e , I get u p s e t o r c o n f u s e d .
36. _ _ W h e n 1 have a f e e l i n g t h a t b o t h e r s m e I try to t h i n k o f s o m e t h i n g else to t h i n k a b o u t o r to do.
37. _ _ W h e n I feel down, I sit by m y s e l f a n d t h i n k a lot a b o u t h o w b a d I feel.
38. _ _ I like b e i n g a b s o l u t e l y d e f i n i t e a b o u t t h e way I feel a b o u t someone else.
39. _ _ E v e r y o n e has f e e l i n g s like m i n e .
40. _ _ I a c c e p t m y feelings.
41. _ _ I t h i n k t h a t I have the s a m e f e e l i n g s t h a t o t h e r p e o p l e have.
4 2 . T h e r e are h i g h e r values t h a t I a s p i r e to.
43. _ _ I t h i n k t h a t m y f e e l i n g s n o w h a v e nothingto d o with h o w I was b r o u g h t up.
44. _ _ I w o r r y t h a t if I have c e r t a i n f e e l i n g s I m i g h t g o crazy.
45. _ _ My f e e l i n g s s e e m to c o m e o u t of n o w h e r e .
46. _ _ I t h i n k it is i m p o r t a n t to b e r a t i o n a l a n d l o g i c a l in a l m o s t everything.
47. _ _ I like b e i n g a b s o l u t e l y d e f i n i t e a b o u t t h e way I feel a b o u t myself.
48. _ _ I tocus a lot o n m y f e e l i n g s o r my physical sensations.
49. _ _ I d o n ' t w a n t a n y o n e to k n o w a b o u t s o m e o f m y feelings.
50. _ _ I d o n ' t w a n t to a d m i t to h a v i n g c e r t a i n f e e l i n g s - - b u t I k n o w t h a t I have t h e m .
182 Leahy

Table 2
Fourteen Dimensions of the Emotional Schema Scale

Validation
Item 8. Others understand and accept my feelings.
(Item 16.) No one really cares about my feelings.
(Item 49.) I don't want anyone to know about some of my feelings.
Comprehensibility
(Item 5.) There are things about myself that I just don't understand.
(Item 10.) My feelings don't make sense to me.
(Item 33.) I think that my feelings are strange or weird.
(Item 45.) My feelings seem to come out of nowhere.
Guih
Item 4. Some feelings are wrong to have.
Item 14. I feel ashamed of my feelings.
Item 26. I want people to believe that I am different from the way I truly feel.
Item 31. I shouldn't have some of the feelings that I have.
Simplistic View of Emotion
Item 18. I can't stand it when I have contradictory feelings--like liking and disliking the same person.
Item 35. When I have conflicting feelings about someone, I get upset or confused.
Item 38. I like being absolutely definite about the way I feel about someone else.
Item 47. I like being absolutely definite about the way I feel about myself
Higher Values
Item 21. When I feel down, I try to think of the more important things in life--what I value.
(Item 25.) I think of myself as a shallow person.
Item 42. There are higher values that I aspire to.
Control
(Item 7.) If I let myself have some of these feelings, I fear I will lose control.
(Item 27.) I worry that I won't be able to control my feelings.
(Item 44.) I worlT that if I have certain feelings I might go crazy.
Numbness
Item 15. Things that bother other people don't bother me.
Item 32. I often feel "numb" emotionally--like I have no feelings.

(continued)

the c o m p l e x i t y o f o n e ' s e m o t i o n s as well as the e m o t i o n s e n c e s as p o t e n t i a l windows into value clarification. Thus,


o f others. This p e r c e p t i o n that e m o t i o n s m a y be anxiety, d e p r e s s i o n , o r a n g e r m i g h t h e l p t h e i n d i v i d u a l
c o n t r a d i c t o r y - - a n d that o n e can a c c e p t t h i s - - i s im- clarify w h a t "really matters," t h e r e b y allowing e m o t i o n a l
p a i r e d in individuals w h o have difficulty r e g u l a t i n g e m o - p r o c e s s i n g to occur.
tional e x t r e m e s (Mayer & Salovey), as well as in patients T h e s e t h o u g h t s are r e f l e c t e d by the following: " W h e n
s u f f e r i n g f r o m e a t i n g disorders, s u b s t a n c e abuse, a n d I feel down, I try to t h i n k o f t h e m o r e i m p o r t a n t things in
b o r d e r l i n e p e r s o n a l i t y (Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1991; l i f e - - w h a t I value," "1 t h i n k o f myself as a shallow p e r s o n "
W e i n b e r g e r , 1995). (reversed), o r " T h e r e are h i g h e r values that I aspire to."
Higher values. G r e e n b e r g a n d Paivio (1997) a n d G r e e n - R e c o g n i z i n g the i m p o r t a n c e o f h i g h e r values (such as in-
b e r g a n d Safran (1987) p r o p o s e d that e m o t i o n s may timacy, c o m m i t m e n t , a n d p r i d e ) affirms the l e g i t i m a c y o f
h e l p clarify o n e ' s n e e d s o r c o n s t r u c t s - - s u c h as intimacy, the p a t i e n t ' s values a n d s h o u l d assist in r e d u c i n g anxiety
r e l a t i o n s h i p , a c h i e v e m e n t - - t h a t t h e individual may wish and depression.
to c o n t i n u e to p u r s u e , e v e n with the c u r r e n t loss. In the UncontroUability. A c e n t r a l f e a t u r e o f a n u m b e r o f anx-
p r e s e n t study, I have utilized a m o r e existential c o n s t r u c t iety d i s o r d e r s is the p e r c e p t i o n that i n t e n s e n e g a t i v e
o f " h i g h e r " values that is not e q u i v a l e n t to the m o r e fun- e m o t i o n s are u n c o n t r o l l a b l e . Thus, the P T S D p a t i e n t be-
d a m e n t a l c o n s t r u c t o f u n d e r l y i n g n e e d s in the G r e e n - lieves that p o w e r f u l a n d f r i g h t e n i n g i m a g e s will over-
b e r g a n d Safran m o d e l s . T h e e m p h a s i s o n h i g h e r values w h e l m her, the obsessive-compulsive p e r s o n believes that
may be d e r i v e d f r o m an existential cognitive m o d e l o f h e will be o v e r w h e l m e d with a n x i e t y s h o u l d h e c o n t a m i -
e m o t i o n a l processing, such as that a d v a n c e d by V i c t o r n a t e his hands, the social p h o b i c believes that i n t e n s e
Frankl (1959), w h o viewed e m o t i o n a l l y evocative experi- anxiety n e e d s to b e c o n t r o l l e d lest o n e h u m i l i a t e oneself,
Emotional Schemas 183

Table 2
Continued

Rational
Item 17. It is important for me to be reasonable and practical rather than sensitive and open to my feelings.
Item 46. I think it is important to be rational and logical in almost everything.
Item 30. You can't rely on your feelings to tell you what is good for you.
Duration
Item 13. I sometimes fear that if I allowed myself to have a strong feeling, it would not go away.
(Item 29.) Strong feelings only last a short period of time.
Consensus
(Item 3.) I often think that I respond with feelings that others would not have.
(Item 19.) I am much more sensitive than other people.
Item 39. Everyone has feelings like mine.
Item 41. I think that I have the same feelings that other people have.
Acceptance of Feelings
(Item 2.) When I have a feeling that bothers me, I try to think of why it is not important.
(Item 12.) I think that there are feelings that I have that I am not really aware of.
(Item 20.) I try to get rid of an unpleasant feeling immediately.
Item 40. I accept my feelings.
(Item 50.) I don't want to admit to having certain feelings--but I know that I have them.
(Item 9.) You can't allow yourself to have certain kinds of feelings--like feelings about sex or violence.
(Item 28.) You have to guard against having certain feelings.
Rumination
(Item 1.) When I feel down, I try to think about a different way to view things.
(Item 36.) When I have a feeling that bothers me I try to think of something else to think about or to do.
Item 37. When I feel down, I sit by myself and think a lot about how bad I feel.
Item 24. I often say to myself, "What's wrong with me?"
Item 48. I focus a lot on my feelings or my physical sensations.
Expression
Item 6. I believe that it is important to let myself cry in order to get my feelings "out."
Item 23. I feel that I can express my feelings openly.
Blame
Item 11. If other people changed, I would feel a lot better.
Item 34. Other people cause me to have unpleasant feelings.

Note. Scores for items in parentheses are subtracted from total score.

and the generalized anxiety patient believes that worries e m o t i o n s , o r t h e ability to dissociate. Similarly, p a t i e n t s
will r u n o u t o f c o n t r o l a n d l e a d to s i c k n e s s o r insanity. who are extremely depressed complain of the intensity of
The depressed patient, experiencing extreme hopeless- their emotional pain, while with extreme depression
ness, b e l i e v e s t h a t his n e g a t i v e a f f e c t will o v e r w h e l m h i m t h e r e is a f e e l i n g o f flat a f f e c t a n d e m o t i o n a l d u l l i n g . A
a n d last i n d e f i n i t e l y . I t e m s r e f l e c t i n g t h e b e l i e f i n e m o - r e p r e s s i v e c o p i n g style, s o m e t i m e s c h a r a c t e r i z e d by al-
t i o n s as w i t h i n c o n t r o l a r e as follows: " I f I l e t m y s e l f h a v e e x i t h y m i a , h a s b e e n r e l a t e d to d y s p h o r i a , e a t i n g disor-
s o m e o f t h e s e f e e l i n g s , I f e a r I will lose c o n t r o l " (re- d e r s , a n d s o m a t i z a t i o n (Taylor e t al., 1991; W e i n b e r g e r ,
v e r s e d ) , "I w o r r y t h a t I w o n ' t b e a b l e to c o n t r o l m y feel- 1995). B o t h t h e c a t h a r s i s m o d e l a n d t h e e m o t i o n a l - f o c u s
i n g s " ( r e v e r s e d ) , "I w o r r y t h a t if I h a v e c e r t a i n f e e l i n g s I m o d e l w o u l d s u g g e s t t h e r e l a t i o n s h i p o f a n x i e t y to n u m b -
m i g h t g o crazy" ( r e v e r s e d ) , "You c a n ' t allow y o u r s e l f to n e s s i n t h a t e m o t i o n a l " i s o l a t i o n " o r i n h i b i t i o n is a p o o r
h a v e c e r t a i n k i n d s o f feelings, like f e e l i n g s a b o u t sex o r d e f e n s e a g a i n s t anxiety. T h e c o g n i t i v e m o d e l w o u l d n o t
v i o l e n c e " ( r e v e r s e d ) , a n d "You h a v e to g u a r d a g a i n s t hav- make any prediction about the relationship between
ing certain feelings" (reversed). numbness and anxiety or depression. Items that reflect
Numbness. T h e full r i c h n e s s a n d i n t e n s i t y o f e m o - emotional numbness include: "Things that bother other
tions, f o r s o m e , m a y b e a n e x p e r i e n c e t h a t m a k e s t h e m p e o p l e d o n ' t b o t h e r m e " a n d "I o f t e n feel ' n u m b ' e m o -
feel m o r e fully alive. W i t h o t h e r s , t h e r e is t h e s e n s e t h a t tionally, like I h a v e n o f e e l i n g s . "
i n t e n s i t y o f e m o t i o n s o n l y c a r r i e s a s e n s e o f loss o f c o n - Demand for rationality. O v e r e m p h a s i s o n r a t i o n a l i t y
trol, t r a u m a , c h a o s , a n d a s e n s e o f b e i n g o v e r w h e l m e d . a n d l o g i c - - o r " a n t i - e m o t i o n a l i t y " - - w o u l d b e v i e w e d as
M a n y p a t i e n t s w i t h a n x i e t y d i s o r d e r s m i g h t e v e n wish problematic by the catharsis and emotional-focus models,
that they could experience numbness, or a lack of strong but does not have a clear implication for a cognitive
184 Leahy

model. The emotional-focus model suggests that overra- tance of feelings leads to quicker resolution of depres-
tionality may inhibit the expression, validation, accep- sion and anxiety. Items reflecting acceptance include the
tance, and self-understanding that follow from allowing following: "When I have a feeling that bothers me, I try to
oneself emotional experiences. In prior studies, rational- think of why it is not important" (reversed), "I think that
ity and anti-emotionality have not been directly related to there are feelings that I have that I am not really aware
depression or anxiety, but have been related to higher ot" (reversed), "I try to get rid of an unpleasant feeling
risk for cancer, asthma, and cardiovascular disease (Gros- immediately" (reversed), "I accept my feelings," and "I
sarth-Maticek, Bastiaans, et al., 1985; Grossarth-Maticek, d o n ' t want to admit to having certain f e e l i n g s - - b u t I
Kanazir, et al., 1985; Schwartz, 1995). These items reflect know that I have them" (reversed).
overemphasis on rationality: "It is important for me to be Rumination. Nolen-Hoeksema (2000) and Papageor-
reasonable and practical rather than sensitive and open giou and Wells (2001a, 2001b) have shown that rumina-
to my feelings," "I think it is important to be rational and tion is related to greater depression and anxiety, with ru-
logical in almost everything," and "You can't rely on your minators often believing that their rumination prepares
feelings to tell you what is good tor you." them for the worst and helps them find a solution to their
Duration. The cognitive model of anxiety suggests problems (Wells, 1995). Although the emotional-focus
that one's predictions that anxiety will last indefinitely model does not directly address this issue, rumination
will increase or maintain current anxiety (Beck, Emery, & would be viewed as reflecting lack of acceptance of emo-
Greenberg, 1985; Clark, 1997). Similarly, the cognitive tion, with expression of emotion and rumination in-
model of hopelessness also suggests that the belief in a versely related. Items reflecting rumination include:
long duration o f depressed affect will add to c u r r e n t "When I feel down, I try to think about a different way to
depression (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979). T h e view things" (reversed), "When I have a feeling that both-
emotional-tbcus model might suggest that belief in longer ers me, I try to think of something else to think about or
duration of emotion reflects difficulty in accepting emo- to do" (reversed), "When I feel down, 1 sit by myself and
tion and, indirectly, might be related to depression or think a lot about how bad I feel," "I often say to myself,
anxiety. The following items reflect belief in longer dura- 'What's wrong with me?'" and "I focus a lot on my feel-
tion of feelings: "I sometimes fear that if I allowed myself ings or my physical sensations."
to have a strong feeling, it would not go away" and Fxpression. The willingness to experience and express
"Strong feelings only last a short period of time" (re- feelings reflects an acceptance that emotions are impor-
versed). tant and can possibly enhance change or understanding.
Consensus, Normalizing one's feelings is an important However, individuals differ considerably in this respect.
c o m p o n e n t of the cognitive treatment of anxiety. Help- The cognitive model does not emphasize expression per
ing the patient recognize that many people will have cer- se as a factor in reducing depression or anxiety, whereas
tain fantasies or feelings decreases negative labeling of the catharsis and emotional focus models stress the im-
obsessions (Salkovskis & Kirk, 1997). From the emotional- portance of expression in reducing negative affect and,
tocus model, recognizing that others have similar feel- in the case of emotionaMocus theoi); increasing compre-
ings is a form o f v a l i d a t i o n - - a process that should reduce hension and acceptance. The following items reflect ex-
depression and anxiety from this perspective. Items re- pression of emotion: "I believe that it is important to let
flecting consensus include: "I often think that I respond myself cry in order to get my feelings out" and "I feel that
with feelings that others would not have," "I am much I can express nay feelings openly."
more sensitive than other people," "Everyone has feel- Blame. The emotional-focus model does not suggest
ings like mine," and "I think that I have the same feelings that blaming others will be a useful antidote to depres-
that other people have." sion or anxiety, but the catharsis model would view blame
Acceptance of feelings. Some individuals allow them- as a displacement or projection of negative feelings about
selves to have their feelings, expending little energy try- the self, thereby leading to the prediction of an inverse
ing to inhibit these feelings. Research on citing indicates relationship between depression or anxiety and blame.
that people who try to inhibit their crying, if they feel the Although the cognitive model does not endorse the ca-
urge to cry, experience distress (Labott & Teleha, 1996). tharsis model, one could argue that blaming others is a
Similarly, research on the ironic effects of thought form of '~judgment" f o c u s - - w h e r e negative j u d g m e n t s
s u p p r e s s i o n - - t h a t is, attempts to suppress unwanted could be applied to both self and others. Thus, a cogni-
thoughts and feelings leads to a later increase in those tive model would not view blame as projection, but rather
e x p e r i e n c e s - - w o u l d suggest that acceptance of feelings as a cognitive style of j u d g i n g people, including j u d g i n g
would decrease depression and anxiety (Purdon & Clark, the self. Items reflecting blame include, "If other people
1994; Wegner & Zanakos, 1994). Similarly, emotional fo- changed, I would feel a lot better" and "Other people
cus and catharsis theories would also predict that accep- cause me to have unpleasant feelings."
Emotional Schemas 185

Method ing to Wells's meta-cognitive model, the anxious worrier


excessively focuses on his anxiety (or e m o t i o n ) , believes
Participants and Measures
that the e m o t i o n should be c o n t r o l l e d completely, a n d
Participants were 53 a d u l t psychiatric patients (21
views the e m o t i o n as d a n g e r o u s o r p a t h o l o g i c a l . This
males, 32 females, r a n g i n g in age between 23 a n d 69, with
hypervigilance a n d pathologizing o f one's internal state
a m e a n age of 40.2) seen at a cognitive behavioral clinic
t h e n results in an increase o f anxiety. T h e catharsis
in a large N o r t h A m e r i c a n city. At intake, each partici-
m o d e l o f anxiety o r depression, which would p r e d i c t that
p a n t c o m p l e t e d the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI;
anxiety a n d depression would be related to expressive-
Beck & Steer, 1987), the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI;
ness, was n o t s u p p o r t e d .
Beck & Steer, 1990), the Leahy E m o t i o n a l S c h e m a Scale
(LESS), a n d three o t h e r self-report scales: the Dyadic Ad-
Relationships Among Dimensions
j u s t m e n t Scale (Spanier, 1976); the Millon Clinical Multi-
Validation. The belief that o n e ' s e m o t i o n s are vali-
axial I n v e n t o r y - I I I (MCMI-III; Millon, Davis, & Millon,
d a t e d by others was related to less guilt, the belief that
1997); a n d the Metacognitions Q u e s t i o n n a i r e (MCQ;
o n e ' s e m o t i o n s are c o m p r e h e n s i b l e , a rejection o f a sim-
Cartwright-Hatton & Wells, 1997). A short questionnaire
plistic m o d e l o f emotion, the belief that one would n o t
was c o m p l e t e d that p r o v i d e d general d e m o g r a p h i c a n d
lose control, h i g h e r consensus, less e m o t i o n a l numbness,
life-history data. T h e LESS is a self-report questionnaire
s h o r t e r duration, a n d m o r e acceptance o f feelings. Con-
(see Table 1) that is c o m p o s e d of 50 questions i n t e n d e d
trary to a ventilation or catharsis model, validation was
to tap into the 14 d i m e n s i o n s identified above. Only the
n o t directly related to depression or anxiety, but r a t h e r
data for the BDI, BAI, a n d the LESS are r e p o r t e d here.
was related to how e m o t i o n s were conceptualized by the in-
dividual. Thus, it a p p e a r s that validation may assist in the
Emotional Schemas
cognitive processing o f emotions, m a k i n g these e m o t i o n s
In reviewing the 50 questions, 14 dimensions were
seem m o r e controllable, m o r e c o m p r e h e n s i b l e , a n d less
identified. These dimensions are shown in Table 2. Bi-
idiosyncratic.
variate correlations were c o m p u t e d between depression,
Comprehensibility. T h e cognitive m o d e l p r o p o s e s that
anxiety, a n d for all 14 dimensions. The means a n d stan-
patients may be assisted in u n d e r s t a n d i n g the reasons why
d a r d deviations for each question are shown in Table 3
they have e m o t i o n s - - t h a t is, linking their feelings to be-
a n d the results o f the correlational analysis are shown in
liefs, c u r r e n t experiences, o r even early p r i o r events. Sub-
Table 4. Preliminary analyses i n d i c a t e d that females were
jects who t h o u g h t that their e m o t i o n s m a d e sense to
less likely to believe they h a d consensus with others, - . 6 5
t h e m felt less guilty, h a d a less simplistic view o f emotions,
(males) a n d - 2 . 4 0 (females), t = 2.10, p < .05, a n d m o r e
d i d n o t fear losing control, did n o t believe that their emo-
likely to express emotions, 6.75 (males) a n d 8.46 (fe-
tions would last too long, viewed others as validating a n d
males), t = 3.22, p < .001).1
as having the same emotions, were less likely to b l a m e
others, saw themselves as accepting their emotions, h a d
Depression and Anxiety
greater validation, a n d were less likely to ruminate. In
Contrary to the view that expression of e m o t i o n
fact, "comprehensible" was correlated with ahnost every
should be related to depression a n d anxiety, there was no
dimension, indicating that the cognitive c o m p o n e n t - -
significant relationship between "expression" a n d the
making sense o f o n e ' s f e e l i n g s - - i s a central e l e m e n t in
BDI o r BAI. I n d e e d , anxiety h a d a marginal relationship
e m o t i o n a l processing. These data are also consistent with
(r - .240, ns) to expression, such that greater anxiety was
the emotional-focus m o d e l that views e m o t i o n a l valida-
related to slightly more e x p r e s s i o n - - c o n t r a r y to the venti-
tion as assisting in clarification o f meaning.
lation model. Depression was related to greater guilt over
Guilt. T h e g e n e r a l belief that one's e m o t i o n s are
emotions, the belief that one's e m o t i o n s were n o t com-
wrong or shameful was related to depression a n d anxiety.
prehensible, p e r c e p t i o n o f less control, the idea that
Subjects r e p o r t i n g greater guilt over e m o t i o n s believed
e m o t i o n s would have a long duration, less consensus with
that others would n o t validate them, were m o r e likely to
the e m o t i o n s of others, a n d g r e a t e r rumination.
b l a m e others, viewed their e m o t i o n s as less c o m p r e h e n s i -
Anxiety was related to guilt, r u m i n a t i o n , less c o m p r e -
ble, o f l o n g e r duration, were less accepting, had a simplis-
hensibility, less consensus with others on emotions, a sim-
tic view o f emotion, did n o t relate their feelings to h i g h e r
plistic view o f emotions, belief in lack o f control over
values, h a d less perceived control over emotions, e m p h a -
emotions, a n d less acceptance. These d a t a are consistent
sized being rational, e x p e c t e d l o n g e r d u r a t i o n of emo-
with a meta-cognitive m o d e l o f anxiety advanced by Wells
tions, were less accepting o f their emotions, m o r e likely
(1995; Wells & Carter, 1999, 2001). For example, accord-
to b l a m e others, viewed others as having different emo-
tions, a n d were m o r e likely to ruminate. These data are
I All t tests are two-tailed. consistent with (a) a cognitive m o d e l that ' j u d g m e n t s " o f
186 Leahy

Table 3
M e a n s and Standard Deviations of BDI, DAI, and Emotional S c h e m a Q u e s t i o n n a i r e Items

Description N Mean SD

Beck Depression Inventory, 53 16.09 11.58


Beck Anxiety Inventory 53 9.92 10.21

When 1 feel down, I try to think about a different way to view things. 53 3.92 1.43
When I have a tieeling that bothers me, I try to think of why it is not important. 53 3.09 1.68
I often think that I respond with feelings that others would not have. 53 3.66 1.90
Some feelings are wrong to have. 53 2.81 1.84
There are things about myself that I just don't understand. 53 3.79 1.72
I believe that it is important to let myself cry in order to get nay feelings "out." 53 3.58 1.79
If I let myself have some o1 these feelings, I fear I will lose control. 53 2.79 1.72
Others understand and accept my feelings. 52 4.15 1.38
You can't allow yourself to have certain kinds of feelings--like feelings about sex or violence. 52 2.21 1.53
My teelings don't make sense to me. 52 2.71 1.73
If other people changed, I would feel a lot better. 53 2.15 1.51
I think that there are feelings that I have that I am not really aware o f 53 3.23 1.61
I sometimes ti~ar that if I allowed myself to have a strong feeling, it would not go away. 53 2.34 1.63
I feel ashamed of my feelings. 53 2.87 1.59
Things that bother other people don't bother me. 51 2.55 1.47
No one really cares about nay feelings. 53 2.00 1.47
It is important for me to be reasonable and practical rather than sensitive and open to my feelings. 53 3.09 1.50
I can't stand it when 1 have contradictory feelings--like liking and disliking the same person. 53 2.64 1.62
1 am much more sensitive than other people. 53 4.38 1.80
I t O' to get rid of an unpleasant t;eeling immediately. 53 2.87 1.68

When I feel down, I try to think of the more important things in life--what I value. 53 3.15 1.76
When I feel down or sad, I question my values. 53 2.91 1.77
I feel that I can express my feelings openly. 53 4.13 1.72
I often say to myself, "What's wrong with me?" 53 4.15 1.67
I think of myself as a shallow person. 53 1.53 1.08
I want people to believe that I am difterent from the way I truly feel. 52 3.29 1.82
I worry that I won't be able to control my feelings. 53 3.04 1.60
You have to guard against having certain feelings. 53 2.62 1.66
Strong feelings last only a short period of time. 53 2.72 1.63
You can't rely on your feelings to tell you what is good tor you. 53 2.87 1.77
I shouldn't have some of the feelings that I have. 53 3.19 1.79

I often teel "numb" emotionally--like I have no ti~elings. 53 2.60 1.80


I think that nay feelings are strange or weird. 52 2.62 1.68
Other people cause me to have unpleasant feelings. 53 3.15 1.77
When I have conflicting feelings about someone, I get upset or confused. 53 3.00 1.82
When I have a feeling that bothers me, I try to think of something else to think about or to do. 53 3.11 1.46
When I feel down, I sit by myself and think a lot about how bad I feel. 53 3.57 1.86
I like being absolutely definite about the way I feel about someone else. 53 3.47 1.68
Everyone has feelings like mine. 52 2.94 1.75
I accept my feelings. 53 4.13 1.54
I think that I have the same feelings that other people have. 52 3.54 1.69

There are higher values that I aspire to. 51 4.73 1.39


I think that my feelings now have nothing to do with bow I was brought up. 53 1.96 1.51
I worry that i f l have certain feelings I might go crazy. 53 2.43 1.82
My feelings seem to come out of nowhere. 53 2.70 1.74
I think it is important to be rational and logical in almost everything. 53 3.74 1.52

I like being absolutely definite about the way 1 feel about myself. 53 3.60 1,61
I focus a lot on my feelings or my physical sensations. 53 4.13 1,69
I d o n ' t want anyone to know about some of my feelings. 53 3.77 1.85
I don't want to admit to having certain f e e l i n g s - - b u t I know that I have them. 53 3.19 1.~,5
Emotional Schemas 181

emotions will have deleterious effects on depression and


anxiety and to the perception that emotions are patho-
logical and ominous and (b) an emotional-focus model
that proposes that lack of acceptance of emotion, as in-
dexed by guilt, has deleterious effects.
Simplistic view of emotion. Increased cognitive complex-
ity in viewing one's feelings might be expected to facili- ~.~
I i~
tate emotional processing In the current study, emphasis
on a simplistic model of e m o t i o n - - t h a t one had diffi-
, ~
culty tolerating ambivalent feelings--was related to the
perception that others would not validate, that one's
emotions were not comprehensible, greater guilt, blam-
ing others, belief in less control and longer duration of
emotion, a belief that others would not share the same
emotions, less acceptance of emotion, greater rationality,
more blame, and greater rumination. Again, the dimen-

sion of simplistic conception of emotion is consistent
with a cognitive model of emotional schemas.
Higher values. Greenberg and Safran (1987) have ar-
gued that the ability to relate emotions to basic needs is a
goal of emotional processing and experiential therapy In E I I I
~5
the current study, the focus was on "higher values"--a
concept derived from Frankl's (1959) cognitive existen- ,.0
o . ~ ~
tial model However, there was no relationship between t l l I I I
emphasis on higher values and depression and anxiety.
There were significant relationships between higher
.~N
values and greater expression of emotions, less guilt, and l i t [
less rumination--findings that would be consistent with
g
the emotional processing model. Thus, emphasis on 4o
e'~
higher values may have an indirect effect on depression
II I I
and anxiety insofar as it facilitates less guilt and rumina-
tion. Thus, emphasis on higher values may moderate guilt ,..o

and rumination, since the individual may believe that the


~ . ~ ~ ~ .
intensity of emotions that derive from or reflect higher II I I
values do not necessitate guilt or obsessive rumination.
6.~"
Control. The cognitive models of Beck, Clark, Salkov- bl
skis, and Wells discussed earlier all suggest that beliefs
I I I i t I I I
that emotions will lead to loss of control may result in
greater anxiety. The current data provide some support
for this claim. Belief that o n e h a d control over emo-
tions was related to less anxiety and less depression. It I I I I I I I
was also related to greater validation, more comprehensi-
ble feelings, less guilt, less simplistic beliefs, less duration,
greater consensus with others, greater acceptance, less
II I I I I I I
blame of others, and less rumination. Thus, the percep-
tion that emotions would not go out of control was a core
predictor of most other beliefs about emotions, anxiety,
and depression. II II I I

Numbness Models of dissociation and models of emo-


tional processing argue that potentially very anxious indi- V
viduals experience emotional numbing O n e can view
this n u m b i n g as a cognitive defense against intense affect
that prevents emotional p r o c e s s i n g - - f o r example, in dis- V
sociative disorder or in PTSD. However, in the current
188 Leahy

study there was no relationship between e m o t i o n a l models emphasize accepting one's emotions. Psychody-
n u m b i n g a n d depression or anxiety. T h e r e was a relation- namic models suggest that the inability to accept emo-
ship between n u m b i n g a n d less validation of emotion, tions results in g r e a t e r anxiety while research on "ironic
g r e a t e r emphasis on rationality, less control, a n d b l a m i n g effects" suggests that attempts to inhibit e m o t i o n are
others for emotions. stressful, resulting in even greater intensity o f thoughts
Rationality. A l t h o u g h the cognitive m o d e l emphasizes a n d feelings. Consistent with these models, greater ac-
the i m p o r t a n c e o f rational t h o u g h t in testing negative be- ceptance o f e m o t i o n s was related to less anxiety. Individ-
liefs, o n e can imagine that too great an emphasis on ra- uals who accepted their e m o t i o n s r e p o r t e d that their
tionality m i g h t lead o n e to have difficulty accepting the emotions were m o r e c o m p r e h e n s i b l e , they h a d less sim-
u n p r e d i c t a b l e a n d often chaotic nature o f emotion. O n plistic views of emotions, less emphasis on rationality,
the o n e hand, one might argue that a "rational" ap- shorter duration, h a d g r e a t e r validation, less guilt, less
p r o a c h to e m o t i o n s is that emotions, by their very nature, blame of others, a n d greater control.
are n o t simplistic, predictable, or always comfortable. Rumination. G r e a t e r self-focus on feelings a n d the
The belief that o n e should always be logical a n d rational t e n d e n c y to r u m i n a t e by asking unanswerable questions
would be an irrational belief. Prior studies o f rationality a b o u t oneself has b e e n related to greater depression in
a n d anti-emotionality indicate that these individuals re- previous studies (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000). T h e p r e s e n t
p o r t less distress (such as depression or anxiety), but that study extends these findings by showing that r u m i n a t i o n
they are at g r e a t e r risk for cancer a n d cardiovascular dis- was related to greater depression a n d anxiety, as well as
ease (Grossarth-Maticek, Bastiaans, et al., 1985; Grossarth- several o t h e r factors: less comprehensibility, g r e a t e r guilt,
Maticek, Kanazii, et al., 1985). In the c u r r e n t study, em- m o r e simplistic views, less control, greater emphasis on
phasis on rationality, to the exclusion o f e m o t i o n o r rationality, l o n g e r d u r a t i o n o f negative feelings, less con-
sensitivity, was n o t related to depression or anxiety, but sensus with others, a n d less emphasis on h i g h e r values.
was related to g r e a t e r guilt, m o r e simplistic views of emo- Expressiveness. M t h o u g h catharsis m o d e l s o f anxiety
tions, less consensus with others, greater numbness, less would argue that the o p e n expression o f e m o t i o n would
acceptance o f feelings, a n d greater rumination. Thus, at- be related to less anxiety a n d depression, there was no re-
t e m p t i n g to treat one's emotions only as a rational pro- lationship between expressiveness a n d depression a n d
cess may reflect a d i m e n s i o n of rationality a n d anti- anxiety in the p r e s e n t study. In tact, the only e m o t i o n a l
e m o t i o n a l i t y that may n o t be i m m e d i a t e l y r e l a t e d to schema related to expressiveness was relationship to
depression o r anxiety, but may reflect a cognitive style of h i g h e r values, such that greater expressiveness was associ-
some generality (Grossarth-Maticek, KanaziI, et al., 1985). ated with a focus on h i g h e r values.
It should be clear, though, that emphasis on rationality Blame. Blaming others for one's feelings was related
had negative implications, a finding that offers some sup- to several e m o t i o n a l schemas that suggest a negative out-
p o r t to the emotional-focus model. c o m e of blame. Patients who b l a m e d o t h e r p e o p l e re-
Duration. A c c o r d i n g to the cognitive model, anxiety p o r t e d m o r e guilt, m o r e simplistic views o f emotions, less
a n d depression may often be related to the belief that control, m o r e numbness, l o n g e r duration, less accep-
o n e ' s e m o t i o n s will persist indefinitely. In the c u r r e n t tance o f their feelings, a n d viewed their e m o t i o n s as less
study, p e r c e p t i o n o f l o n g e r d u r a t i o n o f emotions was re- comprehensible.
lated to depression but n o t a n x i e t y - - p e r h a p s reflecting
the i m p o r t a n c e o f hopelessness a n d helplessness in de-
Discussion
pression. P e r c e p t i o n o f l o n g e r d u r a t i o n o f e m o t i o n was
related to the belief that one's e m o t i o n s were n o t coin- T h e c u r r e n t study offers some s u p p o r t for an emo-
prehensible, less validation, less consensus, greater guilt, tional-focused, o r experiential, m o d e l a n d strong sup-
b l a m i n g others, a simplistic view o f emotion, less control, p o r t for a cognitive m o d e l o f e m o t i o n a n d its relationship
less acceptance, a n d greater rumination. to depression a n d anxiety. T h e r e was little s u p p o r t for a
Consensus. Many anxious individuals label their feel- strict ventilation o r catharsis model. Contrary to a cathar-
ings as o d d o r wrong. For example, the belief that one's sis or ventilation model, expression o f e m o t i o n a n d vali-
obsessions are a sign o f insanity may lead to greater self- dation was n o t significantly related to depression a n d
focus a n d greater anxiety (Salkovskis & Kirk, 1997). In anxiety. T h e emotional-focus model, stressing e m o t i o n a l
the c u r r e n t study, the belief that others shared the same schemas that contain a n d activate cognitions, is a m o r e
feelings (consensus) was related to less anxiety a n d de- sophisticated perspective on e m o t i o n a l processing a n d
pression, m o r e comprehensibility, less guilt, less simplistic affect c o m p a r e d to the hydraulic m o d e l o f ventilation.
views o f emotions, g r e a t e r validation, less rumination, A c c o r d i n g to the emotional-focus model, expression, val-
a n d greater belief in control. idation, a n d a c c e p t a n c e o f e m o t i o n will facilitate self-
Acceptance. Emotional processing a n d experiential u n d e r s t a n d i n g a n d recognition or affirmation o f h i g h e r
Emotional S c h e m a s 189

values. T h e c u r r e n t study, a n d the c o n c e p t u a l i z a t i o n Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Manual for the Revised Beck Depression
Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
g u i d i n g it, s u p p o r t s this view o f e m o t i o n a l p r o c e s s i n g a n d
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1990). Beck Anxiety Inventory manual. San
e m o t i o n a l schemas. Thus, e x p r e s s i o n o f e m o t i o n itself Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
was only r e l a t e d to e m p h a s i s o n h i g h e r values, b u t accep- Cartwright-Hatton, S., & Wells, A. (1997). Beliefs about worry and
t a n c e a n d v a l i d a t i o n w e r e r e l a t e d to c o g n i t i v e aspects o f intrusions: The Meta-Cognitions Questionnaire and its correlates.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, i1, 279-296.
e m o t i o n a l p r o c e s s i n g - - t h a t is, viewing o n e ' s feelings as Carvel, C. S., & Scheier, M. E (1990). Origins and functions of positive
m o r e c o m p r e h e n s i b l e , f e e l i n g less guilty a b o u t e m o t i o n s , and negative affect: A control-process view. PsychologicalReview, 97,
h a v i n g a less simplistic view o f e m o t i o n s , h a v i n g a sense 19-35.
Clark, D. A. (1997). Panic disorder and social phobia. In D. M. Clark &
o f g r e a t e r c o n t r o l , viewing e m o t i o n s as h a v i n g a s h o r t e r C. G. Fairburn (Eds.), Science and practice of cognitive behaviour ther-
d u r a t i o n , a n d less r u m i n a t i o n . apy (pp. 119-154). NewYork: Oxford University Press.
These dimensions of emotion reflect the importance Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Afford, B. A. (1999). Scientific foundations of
o f a cognitive p r o c e s s i n g o f affect in the t r e a t m e n t o f de- cognitive theory and therapy of depression. New York: Wiley.
DiGiuseppe, R. (1988). Thinking what to feel. In W. Dryden & E
p r e s s i o n a n d anxiety. As P e n n e b a k e r , Mayne, a n d Francis Trower (Eds.), Developments in rational-emotive therapy (pp. 22-29).
(1997) suggested, e m o t i o n a l p r o c e s s i n g n o t only reflects Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.
t h e d i s i n h i b i t i o n o f e m o t i o n s , b u t also allows for in- Dryden, W. (1990). Dealing with anger problems: Rational-emotive therapeu-
tic interventions. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Exchange.
c r e a s e d s e l f - u n d e r s t a n d i n g a n d positive self-reflection. Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic
Simply e x p r e s s i n g e m o t i o n m a y n o t be e n o u g h . T h e de- stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 319-345.
g r e e to w h i c h e x p r e s s i o n , o r o t h e r p r o c e s s e s o r experi- Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear:
Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99,
ences, facilitates a c c e p t a n c e , u n d e r s t a n d i n g , d e c r e a s e d
20-35.
guilt, a n d g r e a t e r d i f f e r e n t i a t i o n o f e m o t i o n s , t h e m o r e Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1989). Behavioral/cogni-
t h e i m p a c t o n c o n s e q u e n t d e p r e s s i o n a n d anxiety. tive conceptualization of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behavior
T h e c u r r e n t m o d e l a t t e m p t s to i n t e g r a t e the e m o - Therapy, 20, 155-176.
Frankl, V. E. (1959). The spiritual dimension in existential analysis and
tional-focused m o d e l with a m e t a - c o g n i t i v e m o d e l . O n e logotherapy. Jou~al of Individual Psychology, 15, 157-165.
value o f this i n t e g r a t i v e m o d e l is to r e c o g n i z e t h e value o f Greenberg, L. S., & Paivio, S. (1997). Working with emotions. New York:
e m o t i o n a l a t t e n t i o n a n d e x p r e s s i o n in t h e c o n t e x t o f t h e Guilford Press.
Greenberg, L. S., & Safran,J. D. (1987). Emotion in psychotherapy: Affect,
m e a n i n g s that o n e attaches to e m o t i o n . It is a t r u i s m t h a t cognition, and the process of change. New York: Guilford Press.
e v e r y o n e e x p e r i e n c e s anger, anxiety, o r sadness, b u t o n l y Greenberg, L. S., Watson,J. C., & Goldman, R. (1998). Process-experi-
a few will e x t e n d t h e s e e m o t i o n s to a m o r e i n t e n s e o r ential therapy of depression. In L. S. Greenberg & J. C. Watson
(Eds.), Handbook of experiential psychotherapy (pp. 227-248). New
c h r o n i c affective o r anxiety disorder. It is s u g g e s t e d that
York: Guilford Press.
t h e cognitive s c h e m a s about e m o t i o n s m a y affect this om- Grossarth-Maticek, R., Bastiaans, J., & Kanazir, D. T. (1985). Psychoso-
inous development. cial factors as strong predictors of mortality from cancer,
T h e cognitive m o d e l , t o g e t h e r with an e x p e r i e n t i a l , ischaemic heart disease and stroke: The Yugoslav prospective
study.Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29, 167-176.
e m o t i o n - f o c u s e d m o d e l , suggests n u m e r o u s p o i n t s o f Grossarth-Maticek, R., Kanazir, D. T., Schmidt, E, & Vetter, H. (1985).
t h e r a p e u t i c i n t e r v e n t i o n s ; these m a y be i m p l e m e n t e d Psychosocial and organic variables as predictors of lung cancer,
singly o r in c o n j u n c t i o n with o n e a n o t h e r . T h e c u r r e n t cardiac infarct and apoplexy: Some differential predictors. Person-
ality and Individual Differences, 6, 313 - 321.
study, a n d t h e m o d e l a d v o c a t e d h e r e , is too p r e l i m i n a r y Hayes, S. C.,Jacobson, N. S., & Follette, V. M. (Eds.). (1994). Acceptance
to suggest causal d i r e c t i o n , n o r d o e s t h e c u r r e n t study al- and change: Content and context in psychotherapy. Reno, NV: Context
low for the d i f f e r e n t i a t i o n o f cognitive s c h e m a t a a b o u t Press.
d i f f e r e n t kinds o f e m o t i o n s . It is q u i t e plausible that a dif- Kennedy-Moore, E., & Watson, J. C. (1999). Expressing emotions: Myths,
realities and therapeutic strategies. New York: Guilford Press.
f e r e n t p a t t e r n o f results m i g h t b e o b t a i n e d o n c e we ex- Labott, S. M., & Teleha, M. K. (1996). Weeping propensity and the
a m i n e t h e c o n c e p t u a l i z a t i o n s o f anger, anxiety, sadness, effects of laboratory expression or inhibition. Motivation and Emo-
o r sexual feelings, However, t h e c u r r e n t c o n c e p t u a l i z a - tion, 20, 273-284.
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline person-
tion, with the d a t a r e p o r t e d h e r e , may assist the clinician ality disorder New York: Guilford Press.
in c o n s i d e r i n g h o w e m o t i o n s m a y be i n t e g r a t e d i n t o cog- Loevinger, J. (1976). Ego development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
nitive t h e o r y a n d therapy. Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, E (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P.
Salovey & D.J. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional
intelligence: Educational implications (pp. 3-34). New York: Basic-
books, Inc.
References Millon, T., Davis, R., & Millon, C. (1997). MiUon Clinical Multiaxial
Inventmy-III manual (2nd ed.). Minneapolis: NCS, Inc.
Beck, A. T. (1996). Beyond belief: A theory of modes, personality and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive dis-
psychopathology. In E Salkovskis (Ed.), Frontiers of cognitive therapy orders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms.Journal of Abnor-
(pp. 1-25). New York: Guilford Press. mal Psychology, 109, 504-511.
Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (1985). Anxiety disorders and Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2001a). Metacognitive beliefs about
phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York: Basic Books. rumination in major depression. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice,
Beck, A. T., Rush, A.J., Shaw, B. E, & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy 8, 160-163.
of depression. New York: Guilford Press. Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2001b). Positive beliefs about depres-
190 Leahy

sive rumination: Development and preliminary validation of a Taylor, G.J., Parker, J. D., Bagby, R. M., & Acklin, M. W. (1992).
self-report scale. Behavior Therap), 32, 13-26. Alexithymia and somatic complaints in psychiatric out-patients.
Pennebakel, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 36, 417-424.
event: Toward an understanding of inhibition and disease.Journal Teasdale, J. D. (1999). Metacognition, mindfulness and the modifica-
of Abnormal Psychology. 95, 274-281. tion of mood disorders. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6,
Pennebaker, J. W., & Francis, M. E. (1996). Cognitive, emotional, and 146-155.
language processes in disclosure. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 601- Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression.
626. Journal of Personality, 62, 615-640.
Pennebaker, J. W., Mayne, T.J., & Francis, M, E. (1997). Linguistic pre- Weinberger, D. A. (1995). The construct validity of the repressive cop-
dictors of adaptive bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social ing style. InJ. L. Singer (Ed.), Repressio~ and dissociation: lmplica-
Psychology, 72, 863-871. tions for personality theor); psychopathology, and health (pp. 337-386).
Purdon, C., & Clark, D. A. (1994). Obsessive intrusive thoughts in non- Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
clinical subjects: II. Cognitive appraisal, emotional response and Wells, A. (1995). Meta-cognition and worry: A cognitive model of gen-
thought control strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, eralized anxiety disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy,
403-410. 23, 301-320.
Rachman, S. (1993). Obsessions, responsibility and guilt. Behaviour Wells, A., & Carter, IL (1999). Preliminary tests of a cognitive model of
Research and Therapy, 31, 149-154. generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37,
Rime, B., Mesquita, B., Philippot, R, & Boca, S. (1991). Beyond the 585-594.
emotional event: Six studies on the social sharing of emotion. Cog- Wells, A., & Carter, K. (2001). Further tests of a cognitive model of gen-
nition and Emotion, 5, 435-465. eralized anxiety disorder: Metacognitions and worry in GAD,
Riskind, J. H. (1989). The mediating mechanisms in mood and mem- panic disorder, social phobia, depression, and nonpatients. Behav-
ory: A cognitive-priming formulation.J0urnal of SocialBehavior and ior Therapy, 32, 85-102.
Personality, 4, 173-184. Wells, A., & Papageorgiou, C. (1995). Worry and the incubation of
Rogers, C. (1965). Client centered therapy: Its current practice, implications intrusive images following stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
arid theory. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. 33, 579-583.
Sati'an, J. D. (1998). Widening the scope of cognitive therapy: The therapeu- Wells, A., & Papageorgiou, C. (1998). Social phobia: Effects of external
tic relationship, emotion and the process of change. Northvale, NJ: attention on anxiety, negative beliefs, and perspective taking.
Aronson, Behavior Therapy, 29, 357-370.
Salkovskis, E M., &Kirk, J. (1997). Obsessive-compulsive disorder. In
D. M. Clark & C. G. Fairburn (Eds.), Science and practice of cognitive
behaviour therapy (pp. 179-208). New York: Oxford University Press. I would like to thank the following colleagues at the American Institute
Schwartz, G. E. (1995). Psychobiology of repression and health: A sys- for Cognitive Therapy who provided helpful discussions about various
tems approach. In J. L. Singer (Ed.), Repression and dissociation: parts of this paper: Julie Feldman Golovscenko, Christiane Humke,
Implications for p~sonality theoo; psychopathology; and health (pp. Laura Oliff, Nicole Schaffel, Elissa Tolle, and Elizabeth Winkelman. I
405-434). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. would also like to thank Randye Semple and Anthony Papa, who were
Segal, Z. V., Williams, M . J . G . , & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness- extremely helpful in various stages of this study. Steve Holland was an
based cognitive therapy/or depression: A new approach to p~enting invaluable colleague in discnssions of all aspects of this work. Les
relapse. New York: Guilford Press. Greenberg kindly offered critical suggestions on an earlier draft of this
Smyth, J. M. (1998). Written emotional expression: Effect sizes, out-
paper. All errors in interpretation are solely my own.
come types, and moderating variables. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psycholog; 66, 174-184. Address correspondence to Robert L. Leahy, Ph.D., American
Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for Institute for Cognitive Therapy, NYC, 136 East 57th Street, Suite 1101,
assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads.Jour~tal of Mar- New York, NY 10022; e-mail: Leahy@CognitiveTherapyNYC.com.
~4age and the Family, 38, 15-28.
Taylor, G.J., Bagby, R. M., & Parkm;J. D. (1991). The alexithymia con-
struct: A potential paradigm for psychosomatic medicine. P~ch()- Received: July 9, 2001
somatics, 32, 153-164. Accepted: February 9, 2002

View publication stats

Potrebbero piacerti anche