Sei sulla pagina 1di 7

Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 5056

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Experiencing the temporally extended self: Initial support for the role of
affective states, vivid mental imagery, and future self-continuity in the
prediction of academic procrastination
Eve-Marie C. Blouin-Hudon , Timothy A. Pychyl
Department of Psychology, Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, ON K1S 5B6, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Procrastination is a self-defeating behavior that occurs when present self chooses to delay a task action in favor of
Received 18 March 2015 future self taking on this task later. Given this dynamic between present and future self with procrastination, we
Received in revised form 29 May 2015 hypothesized that higher levels of perceived self-continuity would be related to lower levels of self-reported pro-
Accepted 1 June 2015
crastination, because higher self-continuity would help individuals experience future self as a direct extension of
Available online xxxx
present self. Data collected from 583 undergraduate participants in three studies revealed that individual differ-
Keywords:
ences in perceived similarity to one's future self predicts procrastination such that participants who experienced
Academic procrastination higher future self-continuity in ten years (studies 1 and 2) and in two months (study 3) reported fewer procras-
Future self-continuity tination behaviors. Furthermore, we found that high scores on Vividness of Mental Imagery and Positive Affective
Positive and Negative Affect state scales were related to future self-continuity in ten years (study 2) and in two months (study 3). We discuss
Mental imagery these results in relation to the theoretical importance of future self-continuity in an understanding of procrasti-
nation as well as our directions for future research.
2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Unfortunately, procrastination does not seem to be isolated to the


academic domain such that 20% of men and women refer to themselves
When a person is faced with challenging, unfullling, or simply bor- as being chronic procrastinators across various domains (Harriott &
ing tasks, it can be easy to feel stuck and unable to move forward with Ferrari, 1996). What is especially problematic about these numbers is
a project, a relationship, or with life in general (Blunt & Pychyl, 2000; that the tasks that are being put off involve important long-term goal
Van Eerde, 2003). This feeling is problematic because it can distract pursuits such as saving for retirement (Helman, Copeland, &
one's attention away from long-term objectives, inuence the specious Vanderhei, 2011) and adopting preventative health behaviors such as
pursuit of short-term gains, and ultimately decrease well-being. exercising, eating more healthily and going for annual physical check-
For example, students pursuing post-secondary education are often ups (Sirois, 2004, 2007; Stead, Shanahan, & Neufeld, 2010).
faced with responsibilities and deadlines that dictate when a task should This paradoxical self-defeating delay of long-term outcomes in favor
be completed, no matter how dull or aversive the task may be. Of course, of smaller, sooner rewards has been explained as present self
the long-term benets of completing these tasks likely involve the undermining future self by giving in to feel good now (Sirois & Pychyl,
successful completion of one's degree, the pursuit of graduate education 2013; Tice & Bratslavsky, 2000; Tice, Bratslavsky, & Baumeister, 2001).
or even the start of a fullling career. Paradoxically, a majority of stu- As such, present self uses the needless delay of procrastination as a
dents nevertheless report procrastinating on academic tasks (e.g., Ellis short-term emotion-focused coping strategy. Avoidance pays now for
& Knaus, 1977; O'Brien, 2002), and half of undergraduates regularly en- present self, but can lead to deleterious effects in terms of stress and com-
gage in procrastination (e.g., Day, Mensink, & O'Sullivant, 2000). promised performance for future self. Ironically, of course, the self suffers,
sooner or later.
Accordingly, this present-self/future-self discrepancy is the focus of
our research. We seek to understand how temporal aspects of self play
This research was funded by a research grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities a role in procrastination, particularly the perceived self-continuity
Research Council of Canada (Joseph Armand Bombardier Canada Graduate Scholarship- between present and future self. We might expect that individuals who
Master's) awarded to Eve-Marie Blouin-Hudon.
Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 613 520 2600x1813.
understand that the self in the future may really suffer more because of
E-mail addresses: evemarieblouinhudon@cmail.carleton.ca (E.-M.C. Blouin-Hudon), the avoidance strategy adopted by present self would be less likely to pro-
tim.pychyl@carleton.ca (T.A. Pychyl). crastinate. In the studies that follow, we have addressed the question of

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.003
0191-8869/ 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
E.-M.C. Blouin-Hudon, T.A. Pychyl / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 5056 51

whether higher levels of future self-continuity would be related to lower that procrastinators almost exclusively adopt a present-focused per-
procrastination. spective and rarely project themselves into the future (Ferrari &
Diaz-Morales, 2007; Specter & Ferrari, 2000). Since future self-
2. Conceptualizing procrastination continuity is important for guiding appropriate emotional responses
and daily goal-oriented behaviors (Chandler, 1994; Damasio, 2010;
Procrastination represents the voluntary delay of an intended action Greenwald, 1980), it follows that procrastination might be explained
and can be conceptualized as an avoidant coping strategy, which falls in part by a fragmented relationship between a person's present and fu-
under the broader family of self-regulatory failures (e.g., Sirois & ture self like it is for other self-regulation failures, such as saving for re-
Pychyl, 2013; Tice & Bratslavsky, 2000). As opposed to conscientious in- tirement. If future self-continuity can be used for adaptive temporal
dividuals dispositioned to overcome short-term pursuits by adaptively decision-making, the question then remains: how can the self achieve
regulating present behavior (i.e., delaying gratication, following continuity?
norms and rules, and prioritizing tasks; John & Srivastava, 1999; Lay & Since future self can only be accessed through one's imagination,
Brokenshire, 1997; Lay et al., 1998; Watson, 2001), procrastinators then a reduced or heightened ability to mentally create and manipulate
cope with challenging, unfullling, or boring tasks by giving in to feel vivid mental images may help explain why certain individuals feel more
good now (Blunt & Pychyl, 2000; Sirois & Pychyl, 2013; Tice & or less connected with that self (e.g., Ellis, 1995; Neisser, 1988; Part,
Bratslavsky, 2000; Tice et al., 2001; Van Eerde, 2003). Ultimately, this 1971). Furthermore, individuals who experience greater positive affec-
delay can impair long-term well-being and health by increasing anxiety, tive states may be more likely to include the patterning of future self's
depression (Ferrari, 1991; Lay, Edwards, Parker, & Endler, 1989), and goals, affect, and thoughts into present awareness, as these positive
stress (Flett, Blankstein, & Martin, 1995; Sirois, Melia-Gordon, & Pychyl, states have been found to favor mental exibility and cognitive broad-
2003; Tice & Baumeister, 1997). ening (Fredrickson, 2001; Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002). Taken together,
Sirois and Pychyl (2013) have proposed that a disconnection be- vivid mental images and positive affective states can represent an adap-
tween present and future self might explain why procrastinators pre- tive psychological tool for long-term decision making by allowing a per-
dominantly focus on present gains while failing to anticipate their son to regulate behavior within a broader cognitive-affective scope.
own affective reaction to future aversive tasks. In fact, research by An important body of research has investigated how mentally trav-
Ersner-Hersheld, Garton, Ballard, Larking, and Knutson (2009) and eling into the past or the future can inuence impulsive decision-
Hersheld, Goldstein, Sharpe, Jesse Fox, Yeykelis, Carstensen, et al. making, mostly in terms of temporal discounting (e.g., Ainslie, 2001;
(2011) provides some support for this focus on the discrepancy be- Green & Myerson, 2004; Loewenstein, Read, & Baumeister, 2003) and
tween present and future self, as Hersheld et al. found evidence that episodic memory (e.g., Suddendorf & Corballis, 2007). However, this re-
individuals who report low future self-continuity are more likely to en- search has mostly focused on a person's goal-oriented and cognitive re-
gage in self-regulation failures such as saving less money for retirement. lationship to situations and events. As such, the present research adds a
novel contribution to the literature by furthering the understanding of
3. Conceptualizing future self-continuity how self-regulation failures such as procrastination operate in relation
to the temporally extended self. As an exploratory goal, the present re-
Future self-continuity represents the extent to which a person feels search also investigates how affect and certain imaginative processes,
connected and similar to his or her future self and is central to creating more specically vivid mental imagery, may sustain and enhance the
a uid sense of identity through subjective time (Chandler, 1994). connection between present and future self.
Experiencing a sense of self that is connected and continuous over time To investigate the role of self-continuity in relation to procrastina-
guides present behavior through a broader cognitive-affective scope tion, we conducted three studies with a similar design. In each case, par-
and is benecial for goal pursuit, decision-making, well being (Bird & ticipants were asked to imagine their future self in ten years (studies 1
Reese, 2008; Damasio, 2010; Greenwald, 1980; Sani, 2008), emotion and 2) and in two months (study 3). Participants were also asked to an-
regulation (Chandler, 1994), and coping (Sadeh & Karniol, 2012). swer self-report items related to their procrastination behavior. Corre-
Although future self-continuity has many functional benets, spondingly, we extended the hypothesis that participants who
achieving a continuous sense of self may not come naturally to some, reported higher future self-continuity in ten years and at the end of
as multiple selves can be experienced throughout a lifetime. In fact, the semester would procrastinate less. As an exploratory goal founded
present self's afnity to past or future selves is contingent on the time on the idea that future self-discontinuities may be due to a failure of
that has passed between each self (Part, 1971, 1987) and can greatly the imagination (Part, 1971), studies 2 and 3 included self-report
inuence how connected and similar one feels to a future self that is questions about affective states and vividness of mental imagery. In
perceived as distant in time. these studies, we expected participants who reported highly vivid men-
Neuroscience research also supports the central role of the concep- tal images and more positive affective states to also report higher future
tion of self temporally by illustrating that certain areas of the brain acti- self-continuity in ten years (study 2) and at the end of the semester
vate differently for future self than for present self. For example, in a (study 3).
study by Ersner-Hersheld et al. (2009), participants low on future
self-continuity showed similar neural activations when they imagined 5. Study 1
their future self as when they imagined a stranger, and these were dif-
ferent neural activations than for present self. Consequently, feeling dis- In the rst study, we examined the relation of future-self-continuity
connected and different from future self has been linked to purely and procrastination. We hypothesized that future self-continuity would
present-focused decision-making such that individuals who lack per- negatively predict procrastination such that individuals with high future
sonal sameness across time (James, 1985, originally published in self-continuity would report less procrastination than individuals with
1892) are likely to make more unethical decisions (Hersheld, Cohen, low future self-continuity (Hypothesis 1).
& Thompson, 2011) and discount larger benets for future self by
accepting immediate rewards of less value (Bartels & Urminsky, 2011). 5.1. Method

4. Procrastination and the temporally extended self 5.1.1. Participants


The sample included 86 students (67.4% females, 32.6% males) who
There is limited research exploring the role of future self-continuity were aged between 18 and 42 years old (M = 22.46, SD = 4.98). Partic-
with regards to procrastination, but what ndings demonstrate so far is ipants were mainly enrolled full-time (77.9%) in their rst (17.4%),
52 E.-M.C. Blouin-Hudon, T.A. Pychyl / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 5056

second (39.5%), third (26.7%), and fourth year of study (15.1%) at a 5.3. Discussion of study 1
large, research-intensive Canadian university.
These results indicate that people who feel more connected to their
5.1.2. Procedure future self in ten years time tend to self-report lower levels of academic
Participants were recruited through an online system where they re- procrastination. Given previous research (e.g., Ersner-Hersheld et al.,
ceived grade-raising credit in an introductory psychology undergradu- 2009; Hersheld, Goldstein et al., 2011), we speculate that procrastina-
ate course for their voluntary participation. Participants were provided tion is lower for those with higher future self-continuity because these
a website link and instructed to complete a 30-minute online survey people take into account the self-defeating nature of needless delay,
on academic procrastination and future self-continuity hosted by a se- and choose to act more often in the present rather than delay action
cured website and approved by the university ethics committee. for future self.
Based on these initial ndings, it is also important to better under-
5.1.3. Procrastination stand the psychological processes that may help sustain and enhance
Procrastination was assessed using Haghbin and Pychyl's (2014) a person's sense of self through subjective time. Based on past research
Multidimensional Measure of Procrastination (MMoP). The MMoP was that recognize affect as playing an important role in mobilizing and mo-
developed to measure various aspects of academic procrastination and tivating cognitive energy (e.g., Fredrickson, 2001; Fredrickson & Joiner,
its associated emotions and cognitions. In this study, only a selection 2002; Izard, 1971, 1977; Tomkins, 1962), it follows that affective expe-
of items related to the behavior section of the MMoP were used. The be- rience might mobilize energy in ways that broaden or narrow one's cog-
havior section consists of two subscales: irrational delay (e.g., Despite nitive scope and inuence how connected and similar or disconnected
my intention to start and nish academic tasks on time, I engage in and fragmented one feels to their future self.
other unnecessary activities instead) and hedonistic delay (e.g., I am Accordingly, our second study aimed to understand how affect
not interested in starting academic tasks ahead of time because I might broaden and energize cognition to include future self. Since
would rather do more enjoyable things instead). All items are mea- cognitive-affective orientations can be quite complex (see Izard, 1977;
sured on a 1 (never) to 6 (always) Likert-type scale. A total procrastina- Tomkins, 1962), our second study was also designed to explore how
tion score is obtained by summing the item ratings. Both scales have certain higher-order cognitive processes, specically vivid mental imag-
demonstrated excellent levels of internal consistency (Irrational delay ery, may both inuence affective experience and increase future self-
study 1: = 0.95, study 2: = 0.97, study 3: = 0.97; Hedonistic continuity.
delay study 1: = 0.94, study 2: = 0.95, study 3: = 0.95).
6. Study 2
5.1.4. Future self-continuity
Ersner-Hersheld et al.'s (2009) future self-continuity scale was used It will be recalled that our investigation was guided by the proposi-
to measure how connected and similar participants felt to their future tion that future self-discontinuities may be due to a failure of the
self as imagined in ten years. This scale was based on the Inclusion of imagination (Part, 1971), as it is our unique capacity for imagination
the Other in the Self scale (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992). As such, future that allows us to create a continuous sense of self across time (Ellis,
self-continuity is measured by two items on a 7-point scale marked at 1995; Neisser, 1988).
each point by two circles that range from depicting no overlap to Of particular relevance, research by Ersner-Hersheld et al. (2009)
depicting almost complete overlap. The rst item asks participants to and Hersheld, Goldstein et al. (2011) shows that assisting participants'
select the circle pair that best describes how similar they feel to a future imagination to include more vivid representations of future self can
self in ten years on the scale ranging from 1 (not similarity at all) to 7 favor long-term decision-making. In these studies, participants were
(completely similar). Higher scores are judged to indicate more continu- instructed to enter a virtual reality environment where they were
ity with one's future self. The testretest reliability for the present scale faced with a digitally aged picture of themselves (avatar) or of another
was good over a two-week period ( = .66). research participant. Results demonstrated that participants felt more
similar to their future self following digital aging and were more willing
5.2. Results to discount present rewards (i.e., more money today) in order to in-
crease long-term gains (i.e., more money for retirement). This research
We regressed the MMoP scores on the future self-continuity scores offers a strong theoretical foundation for exploring how individual dif-
to determine if feeling connected to future self in ten years was predic- ferences in vividness of mental imagery, an important cognitive process
tive of decreases in procrastination behavior. Consistent with our hy- of the imagination, may also relate to future self-continuity.
pothesis, future self-continuity in ten years (M = 4.27, SD = 1.27) Specically, mental imagery is characterized by the ability to control
signicantly negatively predicted academic procrastination (M = 3.22, and manipulate cognitive images through all sensory modalities
SD = .780; see Table 1). (i.e., touch, smell, taste, hearing, sight; Serruya & Grant, 2009; Katz,
1983). Similar to direct perception, vivid mental images are particularly
Table 1 effective at evoking emotions as they enhance the experience of sensory
Standardized regression coefcients (standard error), statistical signicance probability details of an imaged future situation or subject (Damasio, 1999;
(P), and effect size for future self-continuity predicting academic procrastination (studies
D'Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2006; Kosslyn, Ganis, & Thompson,
1, 2, and 3).
2001). Furthermore, Positive Affect has been found to increase mental
(SE) P R2 exibility (Fredrickson, 2001; Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002). As such,
Study 1 these are likely to increase one's ability to consider different aspects or
FSC .334 (.523) .002 .111 perspectives of an object, idea, or image (e.g., Caas, Quesada, Antol,
Study 2 & Fajardo, 2003; Eslinger & Grattan, 1993; Kim, Johnson, & Gold, 2012).
FSC .175 (.048) .020 .031 Although one cannot physically meet his or her future self, positive
affective states and the ability to mentally create vivid images should
Study 3
FSC sem .173 (.041) .003 .030 allow a person to feel more connected to future self by regulating
present and future states within a broader cognitive-affective scope. In
Note. FSC = Future self-continuity in ten years, FSC sem = Future self-continuity in two
months.
order to explore these relationships, we hypothesized that Vividness of
p b .05. Mental Imagery would increase Positive Affect (Hypothesis 2a), Vividness
p b .01. of Mental Imagery would increase Negative Affect (Hypothesis 2b), and
E.-M.C. Blouin-Hudon, T.A. Pychyl / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 5056 53

that Vividness of Mental Imagery and Positive Affective states would predict Concerning hypothesis 3, results indicated that Vividness of Mental
increases in future self-continuity, while Negative Affect will predict Imagery, when included in the rst step of a hierarchical linear regres-
decreases in future self-continuity (Hypothesis 3). sion, signicantly positively predicted future self-continuity. However,
when Positive and Negative Affect were included in the model at step
6.1. Method 2, Vividness of Mental Imagery was no longer a signicant predictor of
future self-continuity. At step 2, Positive Affect signicantly positively
6.1.1. Participants predicted future self-continuity, while Negative Affect signicantly neg-
The sample included 196 students (77.2% females, 21.6% males) atively predicted future self-continuity (see Table 4 and refer to Table 2
aged between 18 and 39 years old (M = 22.05, SD = 4.14). Participants for the Pearson correlation matrix among study 2 variables).
were mainly enrolled full-time (81.9%) in their rst (10.4%), second
(35.2%), third (26.9%), and fourth (25.4%) year of study at a large, 6.3. Discussion of study 2
research-intensive Canadian University.
Our results demonstrate that positive affective states are related to
6.1.2. Procedure both the ability to construct vivid mental images and to higher levels
Participants were recruited through an online system where they re- of future self-continuity. While vividness of mental imagery was a sig-
ceived grade-raising credit in an introductory psychology undergradu- nicant predictor of future self-continuity, our results suggest that pos-
ate course for their voluntary participation. Participants were provided itive affective states explain more variance in one's connection to future
a link and instructed to complete a 60-minute online survey on academ- self in ten years than mental-image vividness.
ic procrastination in relation to emotions and imagination hosted by a In light of our ndings, positive affective states may be particularly
secure website and approved by the university ethics committee. In ad- relevant for projecting oneself to the future as they have been found
dition to the measures used to assess procrastination (Haghbin & to broaden and enhance cognitive exibility, creativity, and reectivity
Pychyl, 2014) and future self-continuity (Ersner-Hersheld et al., (Fredrickson, 2001; Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002). Of particular interest,
2009) in study 1, we also assessed affective experience and vividness research by Pyone and Isen (2011) has shown that employees who
of imagery. experienced increases in Positive Affect also reported a more future-
oriented time perspective. Extending these ndings, our results demon-
6.1.3. Positive and Negative Affect strate that positive affective states relate to the broadening of one's
The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark, & cognitive scope to include future self specically.
Tellegen, 1988) was adopted to measure the positive and negative affec- It is possible that individuals who can construct vivid mental images
tive states of participants regarding their experience over the past few do not necessarily construct images of the future or of future self per
days. Participants were asked to rate adjectives describing affect on a se. In fact, research by D'Argembeau and Van der Linden (2006) has
scale from 1 (Not at all agree) to 7 (Totally agree). Ten adjectives mea- demonstrated that people who are able to create vivid mental images
sured Negative Affect (e.g., depressed, unhappy, frustrated, worried/ are more likely to experience more details (both contextual and emo-
anxious, and angry/hostile), and ten adjectives measured Positive Affect tional) when remembering the past as well as when imagining the fu-
(e.g., happy, joyful, enjoyment/fun, and pleased). This questionnaire ture. In our sample, procrastinators who have been found to adopt a
revealed excellent internal consistency for Positive Affect (study 2: predominantly past and present-focused perspective (Ferrari &
= 0.89; study 3: = 0.87) and Negative Affect (study 2: = 0.91; Diaz-Morales, 2007; Specter & Ferrari, 2000) with high mental imag-
study 3: = 0.89). ery abilities may have projected themselves more into the past as op-
posed to the future. Although speculative, this could explain in part
6.1.4. Vividness of mental imagery why vividness of mental imagery is not a very strong unique predictor
The Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (Marks, 1973) was of future self-continuity.
used to assess individual differences in the ability to form vivid mental
images. Participants were asked to imagine 4 situations (e.g., close 7. Study 3
friend, rising sun, store, landscape), with 4 items assessing the details
of the mental image for each situation (e.g., The sun is rising above While many long-term goals can benet from projecting and
the horizon in a hazy sky). Items were rated on a scale ranging from connecting oneself to a future self ve, ten, or even twenty years
1 (no image at all, you only know that you are thinking of the object) down the road (i.e., saving for retirement, preventing health problems,
to 5 (perfectly clear and as vivid as normal vision). The numerical values graduating from college), it is often necessary for people to overcome
on the 5-point rating scale initially proposed by Marks were reversed so
that higher ratings represent greater vividness (McKelvie, 1995). Table 2
Overall, the internal consistency of the present questionnaire was Pearson correlation matrix for studies 2 and 3 variables.
excellent (study 2: = .90; study 3 = .88).
Variables 1 2 3 4

6.2. Results Study 2


1. Proc
2. FSC .175
To replicate ndings from study 1, this time with a larger sample, we 3. PA .275 .180
regressed MMop scores on future self-continuity scores. Consistent with 4. NA .240 .227 .05
our hypothesis and with ndings in study 1, results demonstrated that 5. VMI .214 .181 .277 .146
future self-continuity in ten years (M = 4.33, SD = 1.44) signicantly Study 3
negatively predicted academic procrastination (M = 2.97, SD = .940; 1. Proc
see Table 1). 2. FSC sem .173
3. PA .130 .171
Consistent with hypothesis 2a, results of a linear regression demon-
4. NA .288 .197 .108
strated that higher abilities in forming Vivid Mental Images (M = 3.70, 5. VMI .130 .142 .291 .128
SD = .642) signicantly predicted increases in Positive Affect
Note. ProcProcrastination, FSC = Future self-continuity ten years, PA = Positive Affect,
(M = 3.23, SD = .778; see Table 3). Surprisingly and contrary to hypoth- NA = Negative Affect, VMI = Vividness of Mental Imagery.
esis 2b, Vividness of mental imagery marginally predicted decreases in p b .01.
Negative Affect (M = 2.24, SD = .914; see Table 3). p b .05.
54 E.-M.C. Blouin-Hudon, T.A. Pychyl / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 5056

present temptations in order to reach shorter-term goals that are just a Table 4
few months down the road, such as completing assignments and read- Standardized regression coefcients (standard error), statistical signicance probability
(P), and Effect Sizes for Vividness of Mental Imagery and Affect predicting future self-con-
ings in order to pass a course, or ossing one's teeth daily to prevent tinuity (studies 2 and 3).
gum disease. Although a person may be more likely to feel connected
to a future self that is closer in time (e.g., Part, 1971, 1987), people (SE) P R2 R2

who procrastinate often disregard consequences for tomorrow's, next Study 2


week's, or next month's future self when needlessly delaying these Step 1 .025
VMI 157 (.173) .039
short-term goals. In order to appropriately understand procrastinators'
Step 2 .090 .066
disconnect from a future self that is much closer in time, we aimed to VMI .085 (.176) .274
replicate the ndings in studies 1 and 2, but in study 3, we measured PA .157 (.142) .042
participants' connection to their future self at the end of the semester NA .221 (.117) .003
(approximately two months' time) as opposed to the distant future as Study 3
depicted in studies 1 and 2. Step 1 .020
VMI .142 (.127) .017
7.1. Method Step 2 .077 .057
VMI .077 (.130) .207
PA .134 (.108) .026
7.1.1. Participants NA .195 (.094) .001
The total sample included 301 students (78.1% females, 20.6% males)
Note. VMI = Vividness of mental Imagery, PA = Positive Affect, NA = Negative Affect.
who were aged between 17 and 57 years old (M = 19.60, SD = 4.426). p b .016.
Participants were mainly enrolled full-time (96.3%) in their rst p b .01.
(56.8%), second (31.2%), third (9.0%), and fourth year of study (2.7%)
at a large, research-intensive Canadian university.
future self at the end of the semester, while Positive Affect was a signif-
7.1.2. Procedure icant positive predictor (see Table 4 and refer to Table 2 for the Pearson
The same procedure and measures from study 2 were used. The only correlation matrix among study 3 variables).
change was in the future self-continuity measure, where participants
were asked how similar they felt to their future self at the end of the 8. General discussion
semester using the same seven overlapping circles of the future
self-continuity scale as in studies 1 and 2. The rst study of the present research examined how future self-
continuity or feeling connected and similar to one's future self can
7.2. Results inuence self-reported academic procrastination behavior. Our results
demonstrate that students who experience a greater connection to
We regressed MMoP scores on future self-continuity scores to their future self in ten years' time also report less procrastination.
determine if feeling more connected to future self in two months was While study 2 replicated these results, study 3 demonstrated that feel-
predictive of decreases in academic procrastination behavior. Results ing connected to a future self that is much closer in time (i.e., two
demonstrated that future self-continuity at the end of the semester months) is also related to lower self-reported procrastination.
(M = 4.74, SD = 1.37) signicantly negatively predicted academic pro- Our results offer initial empirical evidence for the relationship be-
crastination (see Table 1). tween future self-continuity and a self-report measure of procrastina-
In line with ndings in study 2, analyses of a linear regression tion. Specically, individuals who report lower future self-continuity
demonstrated that higher abilities in forming Vivid Mental Images are more likely to needlessly delay tasks for future self to deal with.
(M = 3.71, SD = .628) signicantly predicted increases in Positive We argue that these individuals are more likely to engage in this need-
Affect (M = 3.17, SD = .754; see Table 3). Once again contrary to our less delay because they perceive future self as a stranger (Sirois &
hypothesis in study 2, results of a linear regression demonstrated that Pychyl, 2013) as opposed to an extension of who they are today.
Vividness of Mental Imagery signicantly predicted decreases in Our results from studies 1 and 2 also extend the ndings of
Negative Affect (M = 2.41, SD = .834; see Table 3). Ersner-Hersheld et al. (2009); Hersheld, Goldstein et al. (2011) by
Results also indicated that Vividness of Mental Imagery was a signif- demonstrating that academic procrastination, like the more long-term
icant positive predictor of future self-continuity at the end of the semes- task of saving for retirement, represents an instance of self-regulatory
ter when entered in a rst step of a hierarchical linear regression. As in failure that is partly characterized by a disconnection from future self.
study 2, when affect was entered in a second step, Vividness of Mental Unlike retirement savings however, academic procrastination can rep-
Imagery was no longer a signicant predictor. At step 2, Negative Affect resent the breakdown of action for goals that are much closer in time.
signicantly negatively predicted how similar participants felt to their Specically, our results from study 3 demonstrate that feeling connect-
ed to a future self two months down the road, as opposed to ten years
Table 3 later, is also negatively related to procrastination.
Standardized regression coefcients (standard error), statistical signicance probability As past studies have shown, delaying important tasks to future self
(P), and Effect Sizes for Vividness of Mental Imagery predicting Positive and Negative Af- can ultimately end up undermining long-term mental and physical
fect (studies 2 and 3).
health (Sirois, 2007). As such, future self-continuity may be benecial
(SE) P R2 Outcome for adaptive temporal decision-making by allowing people to relate
Study 2 and regulate present and future states into a broader cognitive-
VMI .277 (.090) .000 .077 PA affective scope.
VMI .146 (.107) .052 .021 NA In order to gain a better understanding of the cognitive and affective
Study 3
processes underlying future self-continuity, the second and third stud-
VMI .291 (.068) .000 .085 PA
VMI .130 (.080) .030 .016 NA ies of the present research were designed to explore how affect and in-
dividual differences in vividness of mental imagery can inuence a
Note. PA = Positive Affect, NA = Negative Affect, VMI = Vividness of Mental Imagery.
p b .05. person's perceived connection and similarity to future self in ten years
p b .01. (study 2) and in two months (study 3). In line with ndings that sup-

p b .10. port vivid mental imagery as a particularly efcient cognitive tool for
E.-M.C. Blouin-Hudon, T.A. Pychyl / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 5056 55

stimulating affect (Damasio, 1999; Kosslyn et al., 2001), our results Our studies also used undergraduate students as our population of
demonstrated that participants who were able to construct vivid mental interest. As such, the relationships we have observed can only truly as-
images experienced more positive affective states than participants for sist our understanding of procrastination and the temporally extended
whom this ability was not as developed. Since mental imagery has self within the academic domain. It would be important for future stud-
been found to enhance the experience of both positive and negative af- ies to explore the relations between vividness of mental imagery, affec-
fective states (Damasio, 1999; Kosslyn et al., 2001), it is surprising that tive states and future self-continuity in diverse community samples
vivid mental imagery predicted decreases in Negative Affect both in varying in age and background.
studies 2 and 3. Lastly, it is important to mention that vividness of mental imagery is
On the one hand, these ndings might be due to an artefact in our not the only facet of the imagination that could foster future self-
mental imagery measure, since imaging a close friend and a nature continuity. Imagery related to our other senses, such as touch or scent,
landscape are likely to inspire Positive Affect. However, the mental im- might also account for variance in how connected we feel to our future
agery measure was included after the affect measure in study 3, which self (e.g., Cautela & McCullough, 1978). As such, the exploration of how
indicates that there may be something more to these results. In both our imagination allows us to connect to future self by actually
studies 2 and 3, participants were asked to think about how similar experiencing that self affectively and kinesthetically could greatly
and connected they felt to their future self before they were presented benet the development of interventions aimed at preventing self-
with the other measures, which may have inuenced self-referential regulatory failures such as procrastination, and ultimately, at increasing
task-independent thoughts and images (i.e., mind wandering/ long-term health and well-being.
daydreaming). In support of this speculation, research has found that
self-referential thoughts while mind wandering are predominantly
10. Conclusion
future-oriented (Smallwood, Schooler, Turk, Cunningham, & Macrae,
2010), and that these are more likely to be related to positive images
Procrastination is a self-defeating behavior characterized by a failure
(D'Argembeau, Renaud, & Van der Linden, 2011). In light of our ndings,
in self-regulation. As such, individuals who procrastinate predominant-
it would be interesting for future research to investigate how spontane-
ly disengage from aversive tasks in order to decrease or avoid Negative
ous mental images about future self while mind wandering relate to Pos-
Affect. Ultimately, prioritizing short-term gains (sooner, smaller re-
itive versus Negative Affect.
wards) over the pursuit of long-term intentions (later, larger rewards)
Lastly, our results from studies 2 and 3 demonstrate that vividness of
can impede health and well-being because future self is increasingly
mental imagery is predictive of increases in future self-continuity, but
burdened by these tasks. In light of our ndings, we conclude that indi-
only when entered as a single predictor. When affect was entered in
viduals who procrastinate are more likely to perceive their future self as
the model, the unique variance in future self-continuity both in ten
a stranger as opposed to a direct extension of who they are today.
years (study 2) and at the end of the semester (study 3) explained by
This disconnection between present and future self may in part be
vividness of mental imagery was no longer signicant. In line with our
due to a failure of the imagination. In fact, our imaginative ability to cre-
hypotheses, Positive Affect increased future self-continuity both in ten
ate vivid mental images is particularly important for enhancing the ex-
years and at the end of the semester, while Negative Affect predicted de-
perience of affect. In turn, positive affective states are useful for
creases in one's sense of connection to future self.
energizing cognition in ways that broaden and expand the cognitive
These results indicate that vivid mental images may be most ef-
scope. Overall, we conclude that vivid mental imagery and positive af-
cient at stimulating Positive Affect, but not at directly enhancing the con-
fective states contribute to a broadened cognitive-affective scope,
nection between present and future self. As proposed by Ellis (2005),
which can be important for future self-continuity.
the self can achieve continuity when the different items in the stream
of consciousness (i.e., thoughts, images, language, emotions) are orga-
nized in a pattern that is unique to the person and sustained in time. References
Since Positive Affect has been found to energize, broaden, and motivate
Ainslie, G. (2001). Breakdown of Will. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
behavior and cognition (Fredrickson, 2001; Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002; Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the
Izard, 1971, 1977; Tomkins, 1962), it follows that these positive affec- structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
tive states can serve to unite, direct and organize conscious states into 596612.
Bartels, D., & Urminsky, O. (2011). On the intertemporal selshness: how the perceived
this unique pattern to shape the temporally-extended self. As such, for
instability of identity underlies impatient consumption. Journal of Consumer
our participants, Positive Affect likely served to broaden and guide Research, 38(1), 182198 http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/658339.
their cognitive scope to include the patterning of future states, and en- Bird, A., & Reese, E. (2008). Autobiographical memory in childhood and the development
hanced the experiencing of future self as an extension of present self, of a continuous self. In F. Sani (Ed.), Individual and Collective Self-continuity: Psycholog-
ical Perspectives. NY: Psychology Press.
as opposed to a stranger. Blunt, A., & Pychyl, T. (2000). Task aversiveness and procrastination: A multidimensional
Ultimately, it seems that ones connection to future self can benet approach to task aversiveness across stages of personal projects. Personality and
from the ability to form detailed and vivid mental images, but more im- Individual Differences, 38, 17711780.
Caas, J. J., Quesada, J. F., Antol, A., & Fajardo, I. (2003). Cognitive exibility and adaptabil-
portantly, requires a broadened cognitive scope. Accordingly, positive ity to environmentalchanges in dynamic complex problem-solving tasks. Ergonomics,
affective states, while stimulated by vivid mental images, seem general- 46(5), 482501.
ly more predictive of projecting oneself into the future and connecting Cautela, J. R., & McCullough, L. (1978). Covert conditioning. In J. L. Singer, & K. S. Pope
(Eds.), The Power of Human Imagination. New York, NY: Plenum.
to future self. Chandler, M. (1994). Self-continuity in suicidal and nonsuicidal adolescents. New
Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 5570 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.
23219946406.
D'Argembeau, A., Renaud, O., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Frequency, characteristics and
9. Limitations and future directions
functions of future-oriented thoughts in daily life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25,
96103 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.1647.
Our ndings offer a preliminary understanding of the temporally ex- Damasio, A. R. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of
Consciousness. New York, NY, USA: Harcourt Brace.
tended self in relation to procrastination and of the cognitive-affective
Damasio, A. (2010). Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain. New York, NY:
processes that serve to foster one's connection to future self. Our studies Knopf Doubleday Publishing Group.
nonetheless have clear limitations. For all three studies, the data were D'Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenol-
cross-sectional. To solidify and further elaborate our conclusions, ex- ogy of mental time travel: the effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation
strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 342350.
ploring how these constructs unfold through time in a longitudinal Day, V., Mensink, D., & O'Sullivant, M. (2000). Patterns of academic procrastination.
study is a necessary next step. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 30, 120134.
56 E.-M.C. Blouin-Hudon, T.A. Pychyl / Personality and Individual Differences 86 (2015) 5056

Ellis, R. D. (1995). Questioning Consciousness: The Interplay of Imagery, Cognition, and Lay, C. H., Kovacs, A., & Danto, D. (1998). The relation of trait procrastination to the big-
Emotion in the Human Brain. Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins Publishing Company. ve factor conscientiousness: an assessment with primary-junior school children
Ellis, R. D. (2005). Curious Emotions: Roots of Consciousness and Personality in Motivated based on self-report scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 187193.
Action. Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins Publishing Company. Loewenstein, G., Read, D., & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.). (2003). Time and Decision: Economic
Ellis, A., & Knaus, W. J. (1977). Overcoming Procrastination. New York, NY: Institute for Ra- and Psychological Perspectives of Intertemporal Choice. New York, NY: Russell Sage
tional Living. Foundation.
Ersner-Hersheld, H., Garton, M. T., Ballard, K., Larking, G. R., & Knutson, B. (2009). Don't Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. British Journal of
stop thinking about tomorrow: Individual differences in future self-continuity ac- Psychology, 64, 1724.
count for saving. Judgement and Decision Making, 4, 280286. McKelvie, S. J. (1995). The VVIQ as a psychometric test of individual differences in visual
Eslinger, P. J., & Grattan, L. M. (1993). Frontal lobe and frontal-striatal substrates for differ- imagery vividness: A critical quantitative review and plea for direction. Journal of
ent forms of human cognitive exibility. Neuropsychologia, 31(1), 1728 http://dx. Mental Imagery, 19, 1106.
doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(93)90077-D. Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1, 3539 http://
Ferrari, J. R. (1991). Compulsive procrastination: some self-reported characteristics. dx.doi.org/10.1080/09515088808572924.
Psychological Reports, 68, 455458. O'Brien, W.K. (2002). Applying the transtheoretical model to academic procrastination.
Ferrari, J. R., & Diaz-Morales, J. F. (2007). Procrastination: different time orientations re- Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Houston.
ect different motives. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 379382. Part, D. (1971). Personal identity. Philosophical Review, 80, 327.
Flett, G. L., Blankstein, K. R., & Martin, T. R. (1995). Procrastination, negative self- Part, D. (1987). Reasons and Persons. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
evaluation, and stress in depression and anxiety: a review and preliminary model. Pyone, J. S., & Isen, A. M. (2011). Positive affect, intertemporal choice, and levels of think-
In J. R. Ferrari, J. H. Johnson, & W. G. McCown (Eds.), Procrastination, and Task ing: increasing consumers' willingness to wait. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(3),
Avoidance: Theory, Research, and Treatment. New York, NY: Plenum. 532543.
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the Sadeh, N., & Karniol, R. (2012). The sense of self-continuity as a resource in adaptive cop-
broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218226 ing with job loss. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 80, 9399.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.56.3.218. Sani, F. (2008). Self Continuity: Individual and Collective Perspectives. New York, NY: Psy-
Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward chology Press.
emotional well-being. Psychological Science, 13, 172175 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ Serruya, G., & Grant, P. (2009). Cognitive-behavioral therapy of delusions: mental imagery
1467-9280.00431. within a goal directed framework. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 791802.
Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and Sirois, F. M. (2004). Procrastination and intentions to perform health behaviors: the role
probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 769792. of self-efcacy and the consideration of future consequences. Personality and
Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: fabrication and revision of personal history. Individual Differences, 37(1), 115128 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.08.005.
American Psychologist, 35, 603618. Sirois, F. M. (2007). I'll look after my health, later: a replication and extension of the
Haghbin, M., & Pychyl, T. A. (2014). Multidimensonal Measure of Procrastination. Unpub- procrastination-health model with community-dwelling adults. Personality and
lished manuscript. Individual Differences, 43, 1526 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.003.
Harriott, J. S., & Ferrari, J. R. (1996). Prevalence of procrastination among samples of Sirois, F. M., Melia-Gordon, M. L., & Pychyl, T. A. (2003). I'll look after my health, later: an
adults. Psychological Reports, 78(2), 611616. investigation of procrastination and health. Personality and Individual Differences, 35,
Helman, R., Copeland, C., & Vanderhei, J. (2011). The 2011 retirement condence survey: 11671184.
condence drops to record lows, reecting the new normal. Employee Benet Sirois, F., & Pychyl, T. (2013). Procrastination and the priority of short-term mood regula-
Residential Institute Issue Brief, 355, 140. tion: consequences for future self. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7,
Hersheld, H. E., Cohen, T. R., & Thompson, L. (2011a). Short horizons and tempting situ- 115127 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12011.
ations: lack of continuity to our future selves leads to unethical decision making and Smallwood, J., Schooler, J. W., Turk, D. J., Cunningham, P. B., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). Self-
behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117, 298310 http:// reection and the temporal focus of the wandering mind. Consciousness and
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.11.002. Cognition, 20, 11201126 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.12.017.
Hersheld, H. E., Goldstein, D. G., Sharpe, W., Jesse Fox, J., Yeykelis, L., Carstensen, L. L., Specter, M. H., & Ferrari, J. R. (2000). Time orientations of procrastinators: focusing on the
et al. (2011b). Increasing saving behavior through age-progressed renderings of the past, present, or future? Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15, 197202.
future self. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, 2337. Stead, R., Shanahan, M. J., & Neufeld, R. W. J. (2010). I'll go to therapy, eventually: pro-
Izard, C. E. (1971). The Face of Emotion. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts. crastination, stress and mental health. Personality and Individual Differences, 49,
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human Emotions. New York, NY: Plenum Press. 175180 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.028.
James, W. (1985). Psychology: The Briefer Course. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: what is mental time
Dame Press. travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299313.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big-Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance,
and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of Personal- stress, and health: the costs and benets of dawdling. Psychological Science, 8, 454458.
ity: Theory and Research. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Tice, D. M., & Bratslavsky, E. (2000). Giving in to feel good: the place of emotion regulation
Katz, A. (1983). What does it mean to be a high imager? In J. Yuille (Ed.), Imagery, memory in the context of general self-control. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 149159.
and cognition. Erbaum: Hillsdale, NJ. Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes
Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., & Gold, B. T. (2012). Common and distinct neural mechanisms of precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and
attentional switching and response conict. Brain Research, 1469(21), 92102 Social Psychology, 80, 5367.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.06.013. Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness. The Positive Affectsvol. 2, . New York,
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature NY: Springer.
Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635642. Van Eerde, W. (2003). A meta-analytically derived nomological network of procrastina-
Lay, C. H., & Brokenshire, R. (1997). Conscientiousness, procrastination, and person-task tion. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 14011418.
characteristics in job searching by unemployed adults. Current Psychology, 16, 8396. Watson, D. C. (2001). Procrastination and the ve-factor model: A facet level analysis.
Lay, C. H., Edwards, J. M., Parker, J. D., & Endler, N. S. (1989). An assessment of appraisal, Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 14011418.
anxiety, coping, and procrastination during an examination period. European Journal Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Positive and Negative Affect Schedule http://
of Personality, 3, 195208. dx.doi.org/10.1037/t03592-000.

Potrebbero piacerti anche