Sei sulla pagina 1di 14

18663-AAD159.

4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 359

Mayer, C., & Trezek, B. J. (2014). Is reading different for deaf individuals? Reexamining the role of phonology.
American Annals of the Deaf, 159(4), 359371.

IS READING DIFFERENT FOR DEAF INDIVIDUALS?


REEXAMINING THE ROLE OF PHONOLOGY

A
Q UA RT E R C E N T U R Y A G O , Hanson (1989) asked, Is reading different for
deaf individuals? (p. 85). Appealing to evidence available at the time,
she argued that skilled deaf readers, like their hearing counterparts,
relied on their knowledge of English structure, including phonological
information. This perspective on the role phonology plays in the read-
ing process for deaf learners continues to generate much debate in the
field, and little consensus exists on whether it is a necessary aspect of
learning to read for this population. The present article revisits this
question in terms of what is known about phonology and reading in typ-
ically developing learners, and in light of two reviews of the research
from the field of deafness. The authors conclude that there is stronger
empirical evidence for the argument for a relationship between phonol-
ogy and reading in the population of deaf readers than for the counter-
argument.
CONNIE MAYER AND
BEVERLY J. TREZEK Keywords: deafness, reading, edge of English structure, including
phonology, literacy, language phonological information.
In terms of the focus of the present
MAYER IS AN ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR, FACULTY
In the present article, we revisit a ques- issue of the American Annals of the
OFEDUCATION, YORK UNIVERSITY, TORONTO,
tion Hanson (1989) posed a quarter- Deaf, we will reframe this question
CANADA. TREZEK IS AN ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR,
century years ago: Is reading different in terms of the qualitative similarity
COLLEGE OF EDUCATION, DEPAUL UNIVERSITY,
for deaf individuals? (p. 85). Hanson hypothesis (Paul, Wang, & Williams,
CHICAGO, IL.
suggested that the answer to this ques- 2013), concentrating on the role of
tion appears to be both yes and no, and phonology in learning to readan
she indicated that even though deaf1 issue that continues to be hotly debated
readers may bring a different set of in the field (Allen et al., 2009; Paul,
skills and knowledge to the task of Wang, Trezek, & Luckner, 2009; Wang,
learning to read, the task itself does not Trezek, Luckner, & Paul, 2008). Our
change. Appealing to the evidence view is that the knowledge and skills
available at the time, Hanson argued required to become a proficient reader
that skilled deaf readers, like their hear- do not differ as a consequence of hear-
ing counterparts, relied on their knowl- ing loss. In other words, learning to

359

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 360

REEXAMINING THE ROLE OF PHONOLOGY

read for deaf learners is not different, fication of hearing loss and advances in communicate and think in the same
but developmentally similar, in terms hearing technologies. language used in the text they will be
of what needs to be learned (Mayer & learning to read and write (Mayer &
Trezek, 2011, in press; Trezek, Wang, & The Model of Reading Wells, 1996). In the case of bilingual
Paul, 2010). These requisites are broadly Development learners, proficiency in a first language
defined as language-related abilities The model of reading development we (L1) can be supportive of learning the
(e.g., knowledge and use of the struc- are appealing to is drawn from the second (L2), in that there is some
tures of English, including phonology, work of Chall (1996) and Mayer and cross-linguistic transfer, especially
morphology, syntax, semantics, and Wells (1996), in which a strong empha- between the written forms of L1 and
pragmatics) and code-related skills (e.g., sis is placed on the role that language L2. However, knowledge of the L1
print principles, phonological skills, the plays in the development of literacy. alone is not sufficient for learning to
alphabetic principle). While there may We further ground our argument in the read and write in L2; developing profi-
be qualitative differences with respect extensive body of research evidence ciency in the face-to-face form of the
to how deaf learners gain control of the indicating that a broadly conceived L2 is also necessary and cannot be
requisites, and it may be that instruction notion of language skills, encompass- bypassed (see Bialystok, 2004, 2011,
needs to be differentiated to support ing vocabulary, syntax, discourse, and and Cummins, 2000, for in-depth dis-
development, we believe that the abili- phonology, is fundamental to early and cussions).
ties and skills are not different and can- long-term literacy success (see Dickin- With respect to this issue, Cummins
not be bypassed. In this respect, the son, McCabe, & Essex, 2006, for an in- (2000) expressed concerns that
situation of the deaf student does not depth discussion); the interdependent notions of transfer based on a model of
differ substantially from that of other relationships between language and linguistic interdependence have been
learners who face challenges in learn- reading that have been well docu- misinterpreted as they relate to the
ing to read (Adams, 1990; Chall, 1996; mented for hearing learners (Bishop development of L2 literacy. Cummins
McGuinness, 2004; Snow, Burns, & Grif- & Snowling, 2004; Catts, 1997; Catts & cited the assertion of Bernhardt and
fin, 1998; Stanovich, 1988). Kamhi, 2005; Dickinson, Golnikoff, & Kamil (1995) that
In reexamining the issue of the role Hirsch-Pasek, 2010; Scarborough, 2001);
that phonology plays in learning to and the evidence suggesting that chil- in order to read in a second language,
read, we will focus on the following dren with stronger language capabili- a level of second-language linguistic
questions: ties tend to be more successful at the ability must be achieved . . . . Firm
onset of formal literacy instruction first-language reading skills could not
1. How have the processes of read- (see Stanovich, 1986). As such, we help readers compensate when read-
ing and learning to read been propose a model that weds reading ing in a second language. A lack of
conceptualized for deaf children development to the development of second-language linguistic knowl-
(i.e., theories of language learn- face-to-face language (spoken, signed, edge ultimately short-circuited the
ing, literacy development, bilin- or some combination), and argue that first-language reading knowledge (as
gualism), and to what extent do learning to read is dependent on this cited in Cummings, p. 177).
these conceptualizations align language foundation (see Mayer, 2007,
with what is known about hear- 2009; Mayer & Trezek, 2011, in press). In other words, L1 literacy, even when
ing readers? It is also important to emphasize it is well developed, is not sufficient
2. How has the role of phonology that, for the purpose of learning to for the development of literacy in L2
been interpreted? read, children must have an age-appro- a threshold level of proficiency in the
3. What is the available research evi- priate level of proficiency in the same L2 is required, representing a neces-
dence to support these interpre- language that is to be read and writ- sary condition for any transfer to be
tations, and to what extent does ten. While it is certainly the case that realized. This has proved to be the
the evidence support them? any language, regardless of modality, case for hearing learners (Hornberger,
can serve equally well as a language for 1989), and we would argue that there
We will conclude with a discussion of communication and cognitive devel- is no research evidence to suggest that
pedagogical and research implications opment, in terms of literacy learning it it does not hold true for deaf learners
in the current context of earlier identi- is essential that learners be able to as well (Mayer, 1999, 2009; Mayer &

360

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 361

Akamatsu, 2011; Mayer & Leigh, 2010; sensitivity to rhyme, syllable recogni- worthy that in the National Early Liter-
Mayer & Trezek, in press). What this tion) that has already been established acy Panel (NELP) meta-analysis (2008),
means in practice is that it is not pos- while acquiring the language is ex- oral language (i.e., ability to compre-
sible to circumvent the development ploited. When viewed in this way, lan- hend and produce spoken language,
of proficiency in L2 (e.g., communica- guage provides the foundation not including vocabulary and grammar)
tive competence in a face-to-face form only for the morphosyntactic and was identified as only moderately
of English) in order to learn to read it. semantic understandings needed for correlated with later measures of con-
In returning the focus to the spe- learning to read, but also for the devel- ventional literacy (e.g., decoding, com-
cific issue of phonology, we would opment of phonological awareness and prehension, spelling, writing). While
argue that for all learners who struggle other code-related abilities (Whitehurst this may be taken as a view counter to
with literacy development, there is & Lonigan, 1998; see also Dickinson, our claim, we would suggest that this
consensus that a core difficulty in McCabe, Anastasopoulos, Peisner-Fein- finding must be interpreted in terms of
learning to read manifests itself as a berg, & Poe, 2003, for a review). the broader context of learning to read
deficiency within the language system In taking this perspective, we are not in which code-related abilities (e.g.,
and, in particular, a deficiency in mas- arguing against distinguishing between phonological awareness, phonemic
tering phonological awareness skills the code- and language-related aspects awareness, and word-reading skills)
(Pugh, Sandak, Frost, Moore, & Mencl, of reading instruction, as we have made and language-related (e.g., semantic
2006, p. 65). Research has consistently use of this distinction in our own work and syntactic) abilities assume rela-
indicated that the development of (e.g., Mayer & Trezek, 2011, in press). tively greater significance at different
phonological awareness and phonics However, we want to emphasize that stages in the developmental trajectory
via systematic instruction should be in some sense this is an arbitrary dis- (see Dickinson et al., 2010, and Storch
employed as part of literacy programs tinction, as the phonological, code- & Whitehurst, 2002, for discussions).
for teaching beginning reading as well related aspects of learning to read have Specifically, the association between
as preventing and remediating reading their roots in language. In acquiring language and reading development
difficulties, and that it also needs to be the language, children are also ac- appears to weaken in the early primary
a focus in planning programs for chil- quiring the phonological understand- years, when the focus shifts to making
dren who are learning English as a ings that serve as the basis for the sense of the code, only to strengthen
second language (Ehri, Nunes, Stahl, & subsequent, explicit teaching of the again in the later grades. In other
Willows, 2001). We would also argue phonological skills that are necessary words, code-related phonological skills
that this focus on phonological aware- for learning to read. In other words, account for more of the variance in per-
ness and systematic phonics instruc- we are suggesting that language is the formance at the beginning stages of
tion also applies in the case of deaf L2 foundation for all aspects of learning learning to read than they do in the
learners, particularly as the preponder- to readeven those that researchers later years. Nevertheless, even in these
ance of the evidence suggests that deaf and educators have typically termed early years, the explicit teaching of
students using a phonological code in code-related. To be clear, we are not sug- these code-related, phonological skills
working memory tend to be better gesting that systematic attention to rests on the implicit phonological
readers than those who do not (see developing skills such as phonemic knowledge that has developed in the
Paul, 2003). awareness (i.e., the ability to identify and process of acquiring the language.
It is also important to emphasize manipulate the individual phonemes
that although educators tend to isolate within words through the air without The Role of Phonology
phonological abilities as discrete skills the aid of print) and an understanding A Clarification of Terminology
to be taught in the literacy learning of the alphabetic principle (i.e., the Before we go into detail on the role of
process, developing phonological sen- relationship between graphemes and phonology in learning to read, it is
sitivity is an aspect of acquiring a lan- phonemes) is not critical, only that this important that the terminology associ-
guage in the first place. It is only for pedagogical focus builds on the un- ated with the construct of phonology
the purposes of learning to read that derstandings that have already been be clarified, as we would suggest that
these phonological aspects need to be developed via the language acquisition inconsistencies in the use of terms has
made explicit; in the process, the process. muddied discussions of phonologys
implicit phonological knowledge (e.g., In light of this argument, it is note- place in the acquisition of reading in

361

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 362

REEXAMINING THE ROLE OF PHONOLOGY

the literature on both hearing and deaf their description of phonemic aware- cal access to the lexical store, phono-
learners. Phonological awareness is ness as a conceptual understanding of logical sensitivity has been strongly
most often viewed as an umbrella term language that also behaves like a skill. related to subsequent word-decoding
that encompasses the range of abilities Inherent in this explanation is the skills, with stable individual differences
related to detecting and manipulating notion that a conceptual understand- demonstrated beginning in preschool
the sound structures of oral lan- ing of phonology involves childrens (e.g., Burgess & Lonigan, 1998; Loni-
guagefor example, recognizing and ability to recognize that words can be gan, Burgess, & Anthony, 2000; Wag-
working with syllables, rhymes, and segmented into parts, and that parts of ner, Torgesen, Laughton, Simmons, &
individual phonemes or sounds of the language can be combined to form Roshotte, 1993; Wagner et al., 1994,
language (Bryant, MacLean, Bradley, & words. This understanding in turn 1997). Measures of phonological sen-
Crossland, 1990; Burgess, 2006; NELP, establishes the base for developing the sitivity administered in preschool reli-
2008; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, specific skills (e.g., phoneme isolation, ably predict childrens subsequent
1994). However, this term has also blending, segmenting) necessary to phonological sensitivity abilities (Bryant
been used to refer specifically to the detect and manipulate increasingly et al., 1990; Burgess, 2006; Burgess &
manipulation of individual phonemes complex linguistic elements. There- Lonigan, 1998) and later word-decod-
(e.g., Morais, Cary, Alegria, & Bertel- fore, for the sake of clarity, we would ing skills (Burgess, 2002, 2006), even
son, 1979), a construct that others argue for the use of the term phono- after controlling for variance attributa-
have termed phonemic awareness logical awareness to refer to supra- ble to age, letter knowledge, and oral
(e.g., National Reading Panel, 2000; phoneme awareness tasks that involve language abilities.
Phillips & Torgesen, 2006). Arguments detection or manipulation of larger The overwhelming majority of
for a single conceptualization and linguistic units (Lonigan, 2006, p. 81) researchers agree that these constructs
global definition of phonological such as syllables, onsets, and rhymes, are associated with the development
awareness that includes several abili- and the term phonemic awareness to of reading skills (e.g., Adams, 1994;
ties regardless of their linguistic com- describe the explicit understanding Braze, McRoberts, & McDounough,
plexity have also been mooted (e.g., that words are composed of segments 2011; Chall, 1996; NELP, 2008; National
Anthony & Lonigan, 2004; Stahl & of sound smaller than a syllable, as well Reading Panel, 2000; Storch & White-
Murray, 1994), while it has also been as knowledge, or awareness, of the hurst, 2002). While historically there
recommended that the term phono- distinctive features of individual have been divergent views as to the
logical sensitivity be used to refer to phonemes themselves (Phillips & role phonology plays in learning to
the array of abilities and skills associ- Torgesen, 2006, p. 102). In the present read, often as part of the discourse
ated with manipulation of the sound article, we will use the term phonolog- associated with the Reading Wars
structures of oral language (e.g., ical sensitivity when referring to the debate that dominated the latter half
Stanovich, 1992). In their meta-analysis overarching category that includes of the 20th century (Chall, 1967; see
of achievement in deaf readers, May- both phonological and phonemic Stanovich, 2000, for discussion), we
berry, del Giudice, and Lieberman awareness (see Mayer & Trezek, in contend that the current, widely
(2011) used the term phonological press, for an in-depth discussion). accepted, prevailing view supports
coding and awareness (PCA) to In making the case for the role of instructional approaches that combine
encompass the full range of phonolog- phonological sensitivity in learning to the strengths of whole language (e.g.,
ical abilities, while in the review by P. read, we draw on the evidence from use of authentic text, focus on compre-
Miller and Clark (2011), a range of research on typically developing learn- hension) and phonics instruction, and
terms, including phonemic awareness ers in which the importance of these in doing so, create an approach that is
(which appears in the title of their arti- constructs to the development of more than a sum of its parts (Pressley,
cle), phonological awareness, and conventional literacy is supported by 2005, p. 1).
phonological decoding, were used. studies documenting the causal role The well-documented association
We would argue that the most between these abilities and skills and between phonology and the develop-
salient distinction to be made is the development of literacy in alpha- ment of reading skills is underpinned
between characterizations of phonol- betic languages such as English (e.g., by the beginning readers need to
ogy as a conceptual understanding and Ball & Blachman, 1988; NELP, 2008; understand the systematic relationship
as a skill. Phillips and Torgensen (2006) Wagner et al., 1994). In addition to underlying the mapping of phonemes
effectively captured this difference in phonological memory and phonologi- onto graphemes. More specifically, if a

362

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 363

sequence of phonemes cannot be all phonological tasks, regardless of lin- 25 of which also measured the partic-
identified in the spoken language, the guistic complexity (e.g., onset and ipants reading ability and calculated a
basis for representing spoken words rhyme, phoneme level), reflect the statistic assessing the degree of rela-
as a sequence of corresponding same fundamental abilities (e.g., tion between PCA and reading (p.
graphemes will remain inaccessible. Anthony et al., 2002; Schatschneider, 169). In only 21 of these studies was
While there is debate with respect to Francis, Foorman, Fletcher, & Mehta, some information provided regarding
aspects of this role (e.g., the direction 1999; Wagner et al., 1997). While there participants reading abilities either as
of the causal relationship between the may be debate as to the relative impor- part of the description of background
variables, the level of phonological tance of various aspects of phonologi- characteristics or as part of the exper-
structure that must be accessed), the cal sensitivity in the process of learning imental study itself (p. 177). While
overall agreement is that phonological to read, there is no disagreement that the authors acknowledged that their
sensitivity is central to learning to read. they do play a central role. conclusion was drawn from a limited
A consideration of studies of strug- set of studies (p. 181), it is on this
gling readers lends further support to Research Evidence basis that they reported that overall
the claim that phonological sensitivity Despite the growing agreement among PCA predicted 11% of the variance in
plays a key role in the development of researchers in the field of reading, the reading proficiency in deaf partici-
early and later conventional reading consensus regarding phonological pants. Although not specifically identi-
skills, as deficits in these abilities have sensitivity has not been consistently fied, in the 7 studies in which it was
been consistently documented for extended to deaf readers, and there measured, language ability predicted
most learners in this group (see, e.g., continues to be disagreement on 35% of the variance in reading profi-
Stanovich, 1988; Wagner et al., 1994, whether it is necessary for learning to ciency. Following from this analysis,
1997). Additionally, delays in both read for this population of students. Mayberry et al. argued that PCA skills
phonological sensitivity and lexical Arguments are made that deaf indi- are a low to moderate predictor of
knowledge (e.g., vocabulary), referred viduals can bypass phonology and reading achievement in deaf individu-
to as a double deficit, have been doc- become proficient readersa position als and that other factors, most notably
umented for many poor readers (e.g., that stands in stark contrast to the con- language ability, have a greater influ-
Stanovich & Siegel, 1994), underscor- clusions that can be drawn from the ence on reading development.
ing the ongoing role that language extensive body of literature and However, we would suggest that
plays in the process of learning to read. research evidence from studies of the the evidence upon which this claim is
Some debate exists as to the pre- broader population indicating that made is less than robust. In the first
dictive reliability and significance of these abilities and skills (i.e., phono- place, as Mayberry et al. (2011) them-
various types of phonological abilities. logical and phonemic awareness) are selves noted, even for hearing learn-
For example, arguments are made that necessary to the process. We will con- ers only 12% of the variance in word
some phonemic awareness skills such sider these claims and the evidence identification skills was accounted for
as phoneme blending and segmenting presented to support them by examin- by phonological awareness in a quan-
are more highly associated with later ing two recent reviews of the literature titative meta-analysis of the effects of
reading achievement (Hatcher & that have addressed this issue (i.e., phonological awareness training on
Hulme, 1999; Hulme, 2002; Hulme et Mayberry et al., 2011; Miller & Clark, reading (Bus & van IJzendoorn, 1999).
al., 2002; Hulme, Muter, & Snowling, 2011), with a specific focus on the That said, this finding did not lead Bus
1998; Muter, Hulme, Snowling, & Tay- extent to which this research evidence and van IJzendoorn to suggest that
lor, 1997; Nation & Hulme, 1997) than indicates that deaf students have phonological sensitivity does not play
abilities such as rhyming. Others sug- learned to read in the absence of an important role in learning to read
gest that rhyming may just be contribut- phonological sensitivity. In other words, for hearing childrenrather, they con-
ing differently to the developmental does the evidence support the claim cluded that it is an important but not
process (e.g., Bryant, 1998; Goswami that deaf individuals are different in sufficient condition for early reading
& Bryant, 1990). A growing body of how they learn to read? development. Therefore, the finding
research evidence supports the posi- that 11% of the variance in the studies
tion that phonological sensitivity Literature Review 1 of deaf readers could be accounted for
should be considered a unitary con- Mayberry et al. (2011) included 57 by phonological sensitivity seems con-
struct (Burgess, 2006, p. 91), and that studies in their detailed meta-analysis, sistent with what has been found in

363

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 364

REEXAMINING THE ROLE OF PHONOLOGY

hearing learners, and thus leads to the e.g., Stanovich, 1988; Wagner et al., evidence that most of these children
conclusion that deaf readers may not 1994, 1997), the implication being that had learned to read. This begs the
be different in this regard, with phono- proficient readers have mastered these question of whether it is actually
logical sensitivity playing a similar role. phonological, code-related abilities, deficits in phonological sensitivity that
A second point is the fact that the with variance in this group being account for this poor performance.
meta-analysis by Mayberry et al. (2011), attributed to language, not code-
in contrast to that of Bus and van IJzen- related abilities. Therefore, if we may Literature Review 2
doorn (1999), included studies of the return to the previous point, it could In their review of the research, Miller
effect of phonological sensitivity across be argued that given the wide age and Clark (2011) did not provide inclu-
a wide segment of the age range (i.e., range considered in the meta-analysis sion criteria for study selection, or an
mean ages from 5 to 37 years), with by Mayberry et al. (2011), 11% of the analysis/summary chart of the included
only 4 investigations focusing exclu- variance being attributed to phonolog- studies. However, in our reading of
sively on early readersthat is, those ical sensitivity indicates that many of their article we identified three studies
through the age of 8 years (Colin, Mag- the older deaf readers were continuing that were not included in the Mayberry
nan, Ecalle, & Leybaert, 2007; Harris, & to struggle in this regard, a situation et al. (2011) meta-analysis that focused
Beech, 1998; Harris & Moreno, 2006; that may account for their poor read- specifically on phonological sensitivity
Kyle & Harris, 2006), when phonolog- ing performance. and reading performance, and that
ical, code-related abilities are most crit- Perhaps most concerning is the fact incorporated a measure of reading
ical. As we have already noted, it is well that there is no strong evidence in comprehension (McQuarrie & Parrila,
documented in the literature that the this meta-analysis that deaf individuals 2009; P. Miller, 2009, 2010). A review of
effect of phonological processing skills were able to become proficient (i.e., these study findings is provided below.
is evidenced most strongly in the early age-appropriate, at grade level) read- Based on their findings, McQuarrie
stages of learning to read, with oral ers without control of phonological and Parrila (2009) concluded that a
language skills having little or no direct sensitivity. Although calculated and dis- lack of phonological sensitivity was evi-
influence on development in first and cussed (see Mayberry et al., 2011, p. dent in the performance of both poor
second grade (Storch & Whitehurst, 177), mean reading levels were not and good readers (as cited in P. Miller
2002). reported for the 25 studies; therefore, & Clark, 2011, p. 466). In their study,
This is not to suggest that language- it is not clear how many deaf readers these researchers focused on 52 deaf
related abilities do not also play a role were actually functioning at an age- students equally divided in number
during this period, but to highlight the appropriate level in the absence of between two age groups, 8-to-13-year-
fact that phonological abilities account phonological abilities. For example, of olds and 14-to-19-year-olds. Scores
for more of the variance in the begin- the 29 participants aged 4.4 to 13.5 from the paragraph meaning subtest
ning than in the later phases of devel- years in one study, only 6 were reading of the Stanford Achievement Test (SAT-
opment (see also Frost, Madsbjerg, at grade level and the overall mean HI) and the 82-item reading com-
Niederse, Olofsson, & Srensen, reading level for the group was at the prehension subtest of the Peabody
2005). That said, it is problematic that preprimer to first-grade level (Izzo, Individual Achievement TestRevised
there are so few studies in the review 2002). Of the 2 strongest readers, 1 (PIAT-R) were used as measures of
by Mayberry et al. (2011) that investi- also had the highest phonemic aware- reading comprehension, and findings
gate reading development during the ness score, and the oldest student in related to the ability to make syllable-,
period when phonological sensitivity the study (13.5 years) was reading at a rhyme-, and phoneme-level judgments
has the most influence on learning to fifth-grade levelwell below expecta- were also reported. It is worth noting
read, as even in the hearing popula- tions for his age. Therefore, although that 31 of the 52 participants (i.e., 60%)
tion the impact of phonological sensi- it is suggested that these findings sup- had a reading age of less than 9 years,
tivity lessens in more mature readers port the premise that phonemic and only 21 read better than a 9-year-
(Storch & Whitehurst, 2002). awareness may neither facilitate nor be old. These results and the age ranges
Furthermore, it is typically only necessary for reading development of of the cohort indicate that the major-
struggling hearing readers who con- readers who are deaf (Izzo, 2002, ity of participants in McQuarrie and
tinue to face challenges as a conse- p. 26), we would submit that this claim Parrilas study did not read at an age-
quence of deficits in phonological is not warranted based on the data pre- appropriate level, and it is not clear
sensitivity beyond the early years (see, sented and that there is no compelling how many participants who lacked

364

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 365

phonological sensitivity were profi- phonological sensitivity tended to be performance and did not predict their
cient readers. Therefore, it is not better readers. In light of this finding, reading comprehension. We would
apparent how these findings provide and given the nature of the reading agree with the author in this observa-
compelling support for the view that comprehension measure that was tion, and further point out that no
phonological sensitivity is not neces- used, it is not clear how many of the direct evidence was presented that the
sary for learning to read when the deaf participants in the study were deaf participants who did not have
majority of the participants were not able to read at an age-appropriate control of phonological abilities were
proficient readers. level in the absence of phonological proficient readers.
Participants in a study conducted by abilities.
Miller (2010) included hearing (n = The same measure of reading com- Perspectives on the
85) and deaf (n = 83) cohorts from prehension is referenced in another Literature Reviews
primary school, high school, and uni- study by Miller (2009), in which he Based on this examination of the
versity settings, and involved the investigated the word-reading strate- research evidence from the reviews by
administration of two measuresone gies of 59 hearing and 31 orally edu- Mayberry et al. (2011) and Miller and
to assess phonemic and orthographic cated, prelingually deaf high school Clark (2011), we suggest that there is
awareness and one to assess reading and postgraduate students. While the not a strong empirical basis for claim-
comprehension. The reading compre- main focus of the study was on reading ing that phonology does not play a
hension task was not a standardized words in isolation, Miller did relate role in reading for deaf individuals.
measure, but rather a test that con- these findings to the reading compre- Rather, there is stronger theoretical
sisted of 36 sentences (mean length = hension measure, suggesting that the and empirical evidence to support the
6.97 words; range = 412) using only evidence fails to support a theory argument for a relationship between
very basic vocabulary (i.e., only words assigning phonological decoding a crit- phonology and reading in the popula-
with available signs in Signed Hebrew) ical role in reading comprehension tion of deaf readers than for the coun-
to prevent lexical knowledge from (p. 359). However, it is important to terargument. In addition to the older
becoming a potential discriminator point out that these deaf participants studies that are often cited in support
between participant groups. Half of exhibited drastically impoverished of our assertion (e.g., Beech & Harris,
these sentences were semantically phonological sensitivity and reading 1997; Conrad, 1964; Hanson, 1982,
plausible and half semantically implau- comprehension (relative to the test 1990; Hanson & Fowler, 1987; Han-
sible, with comprehension assessed by measure and a hearing control group), son, Goodell, & Perfetti, 1991; Han-
having participants circle a response and it is not clear how the lack of dif- son & Lichtenstein, 1990), we point to
to a short question from two or three ference found between the hearing some of the more recent work that
options. In summarizing his findings, and deaf cohorts on the word-reading has investigated phonological sensi-
Miller suggested that the failure of the task relates to reading comprehension. tivity as an aspect of early reading
deaf individuals to develop sensitivity In other words, it seems that although development in deaf childrenthe
to the phonological properties of the hearing and deaf participants were stage at which this issue is most
words may not underlie their reading using similar strategies for word read- salient (e.g., Ambrose, Fey, & Eisen-
difficulties, arguing that this weakness ing, the members of the hearing berg, 2012; Beech & Harris, 1997;
seems to reflect a processing deficit cohort were better readers and had Colin, Leybaert, Ecalle, & Magnan,
at the supra-lexical (sentence) level stronger phonological abilities. 2013; Cupples, Ching, Crowe, Day, &
where the final meaning of single As Miller (2009) acknowledged, Seeto, 2014; Dillon, de Jong, & Pisoni,
words is elaborated by its integration given that all participants had many 2012; Easterbrooks, Lederberg, E. M.
based upon syntactic (structural) years of reading experience, and the Miller, Bergeron, & Connor, 2008;
knowledge (p. 1). However, it was word pairs used in the word-reading Fagan, Pisoni, Horn, & Dillon, 2007;
also found that, overall, deaf individu- task in his study were mainly high-fre- Geers, 2003; Harris & Beech, 1998;
als at all levels exhibited markedly quency words, it might be reasonable Kyle & Harris, 2011; Spencer &
poorer reading comprehension, with to assume that the principle strategy Tomblin, 2009; Transler & Reitsma,
these scores being moderately but sig- used was orthographic (i.e., whole 2011; Vermeulen, Van Bon, Schreuder,
nificantly correlated with phonemic word recognition), which would Knoors, & Snik, 2007). Collective find-
awareness, a finding that suggests that explain why phonological sensitivity ings of these investigations suggest
those deaf individuals with increased was not indicative of the participants the following:

365

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 366

REEXAMINING THE ROLE OF PHONOLOGY

1. Deaf participants have control of Conclusion all groups of learners, deaf individuals
phonological sensitivity, although In considering the implications of our are unique in that they can become
they may score lower than their examination of the phonology debate proficient readers in the absence of
hearing peers on some measures on research and practice, we return to phonological understandings (Allen
(e.g., rhyming, reading of pseudo- the title of this article and the ques- et al., 2009; Miller & Clark, 2011;
words). tion we posed: Is reading different for Moores, 2001). An aspect of this
2. Phonological abilities among deaf individuals? Informed by the evi- debate is the pitting of language-
young deaf children correlate dence-based arguments we have pre- related abilities against code-related,
with other measures of early sented here, our response to this phonological skills to determine which
literacy (e.g., vocabulary, word question is a definitive no. What this deserves pride of place as the most
identification, passage compre- means for us in terms of practice is that important factor in learning to read
hension), as well as gains in read- instruction, including assessment, (see e.g., discussions about what
ing, in a manner similar to that needs to be differentiated (e.g., via accounts for the most variance in out-
shown by hearing readers of the accommodations) rather than differ- comes). This seems to us to be a
same age. ent, and be based on the identified debate without warrant. Even the
3. The developmental reading tra- strengths or weakness of the individual staunchest supporter of the role that
jectory of deaf learners appears learner (e.g., have a relative focus on phonological sensitivity plays in learn-
to diverge after the second code- or language-related abilities), ing to read would acknowledge that
year of reading instruction in and not on the premise that deaf read- reading fundamentally rests on a
the absence of phonological ers do not need to master the same re- language foundation. As we described
sensitivity. quisites as all other English literacy above under The Model of Reading
4. Familiarity with a natural sign learners (Mayer & Trezek, in press). We Development, phonology is an
language (e.g., British Sign Lan- would apply this model to the full aspect of language acquisition in the
guage), including fingerspelling, range of deaf learners we teach first place, and it is the implicit
was not found to correlate with whether their first language is English phonological awareness that develops
gains in reading. or ASL, whether they use spoken in the language acquisition process
5. Early exposure to a communi- or signed language (or both), and that provides the platform for the
cation system that makes the whether or not they use hearing tech- explicit teaching of the phonological
phonological aspects of the nologies, including cochlear implants, skills necessary for learning to read. It
language accessible (i.e., Cued recognizing that, despite the signifi- is these phonological skills that allow
Speech) results in age-appropri- cant positive shifts in literacy out- the reader to make the connections
ate skill development in the comes for many children using between through-the-air language
areas of phonological awareness, improved hearing technologies, some and the print on the pagepar -
reading, and spelling, although children in this group continue to face ticularly in alphabetic languages such
not necessarily vocabulary. challenges in learning to read (Arch- as English (i.e., languages that, per
bold & Mayer, 2011; Marschark, Sar- the alphabetic principle, employ
In one of the most recent studies (Cup- chet, Rhoten, & Zupan, 2010). To phoneme and grapheme correspon-
ples et al., 2014), it was found that, after elaborate on our position, we will con- dences).
numerous variables were controlled for clude by providing our perspectives on Our key point is that phonology is a
(e.g., receptive vocabulary, nonverbal three issues that we believe warrant necessary aspect of learning to read,
cognitive ability, and a range of demo- further discussion as a consequence most especially in the early years,
graphic variables, including gender, of this claim: (a) the utility of the when the primary focus is on learning
degree of hearing loss, communication debate, (b) pedagogical implications, to decode. We recognize that, in and
mode, type of sensory device, age at and (c) directions for future research. of itself, phonology is not sufficient
fitting of sensory devices, and level to allow for comprehension (i.e.,
of maternal education), phonological Utility of the Debate decoding in the absence of language);
awareness made a significant, inde- We suggest that, historically, an inordi- however, in the absence of the devel-
pendent contribution to deaf chil- nate amount of time and attention has opment of phonological sensitivity
drens early reading ability. been expended to suggest that among abilities and the ability to decode with

366

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 367

ease and automaticity, comprehension being the most expedient route for alphabetic principle with little or no
is not possible. What is abundantly learning to decode, they also suggest reliance on audition or speech.
clear from the theoretical and empiri- that this proficiency may also be With a relatively strong body of
cal evidence from hearing learners is achieved through other modalities research evidence regarding the acqui-
that phonological abilities are central (i.e., via a visual communication sys- sition of the alphabetic principle firmly
to this decoding process, and we tem such as Cued Speech or a visual- in place, we suggest that questions of
would contend that there is no robust tactile tool such as Visual Phonics) pedagogy can now shift toward a focus
body of parallel evidence to support that stand in for phonological repre- on more nuanced questions regarding
the view that this does not also hold sentations and realize the same out- phonological sensitivity instruction
true for deaf readers. As we explicated comes (i.e., mastery of sound-symbol and achievement. For example, since
in our discussion above, there are no correspondences). most studies to date have focused
data to indicate that significant num- almost exclusively on the development
bers of deaf individuals become profi- Pedagogical Implications of the alphabetic principle, it is difficult
cient readers without developing It has been suggested that all learners to surmise whether the deaf children
phonological abilities, and we could require specific instruction in order to in these investigations had acquired
identify no study that examined this develop an understanding of the phonological and/or phonemic aware-
issue directly (i.e., using standardized alphabetic principle (i.e., the existence ness abilities, the skills that research
measures of reading and phonology of relationships between phonemes evidence indicates are important pre-
and examining reading outcomes rela- and graphemes), as the relevant skills requisites for hearing learners. We
tive to age norms). do not generally develop sponta- therefore recommend that pedagogi-
We suggest that this phonology neously (see Braze et al., 2011); we cal attention be given to developing
debate has persisted in the light of would suggest that this situation also phonological sensitivity abilities prior
such a weak evidence base as a conse- holds true for deaf children. This is an to, or in conjunction with, instruction
quence of the communication dis- issue that has been largely ignored by in the alphabetic principle, and to
putes that continue to dog our field those who challenge a phonological assessing the results of these efforts.
(i.e., spoken vs. signed language, oral- perspective (e.g., Allen et al., 2009;
ism vs. manualism), with the pro- Miller & Clark, 2011), and it may be the Directions for Future
phonology camp being associated with case that some deaf children do not Research
a spoken-language, anti-sign perspec- use phonological strategies because In light of the information we have
tive. In part, such a view is understand- they have never been taught how to do provided in the present article, we
able, as mastering the phonological so. For example, several researchers advocate a research agenda that
requisites of learning to read would have suggested that direct and explicit focuses not on how deaf readers are
seem to be tied to sound and being instruction is required (e.g., Bergeron, differentas we do not think that they
able to listen and speak, and thus Lederberg, Easterbrooks, E. M. Miller, areat least when it comes to a con-
would be most obviously linked to an & Connor, 2009; Syverud, Guardino, sideration of the code- and language-
oralist model of deaf education. Such & Selznick, 2009), and that a visual-tac- related knowledge and skills that are
an interpretation seems to preclude tile tool such as Visual Phonics can be necessary for learning to read, but,
the teaching of phonology in educa- employed to augment this type of rather, on how the process of learning
tional settings that employ some form instruction (e.g., Beal-Alvarez, Leder- to read is manifested differently in deaf
of signed communication (e.g., sign berg, & Easterbrooks, 2012; Guardino, learners. In this respect, we agree with
bilingual programs). Syverud, Joyner, Nicols, & King, 2011; the conclusion of McQuarrie and
However, this is an unfortunate Smith & Wang, 2010; Trezek & Han- Parrila (2009) that it is time to focus
(and inaccurate) construal of the cock, 2013; Trezek & Malmgren, 2005; research efforts on a more detailed
issue. While it is true that those who Trezek & Wang, 2006; Trezek, Wang, investigation of what critical evidence
adopt the view that learning to read is Woods, Gampp, & Paul, 2007; Wang, must be available in the input signal to
a qualitatively or developmentally Spychala, Harris, & Oetting, 2013). The support deaf learners in the process of
similar process (i.e., the view that evidence from these investigations lexical elaboration and, in turn, the
phonology is necessary) do empha- indicates that many deaf participants construction of rich and robust word
size control of spoken language as were able to develop knowledge of the representations (p. 151).

367

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 368

REEXAMINING THE ROLE OF PHONOLOGY

Specifically, we suggest an in-depth to Wang, Trezek, Luckner, and Paul. Ameri- does predict young childrens reading: A
can Annals of the Deaf, 154, 338345. comment on Muter, Hulme, Snowling, and
examination of the relationship be-
Ambrose, S. E., Fey, M. E., & Eisenberg, L. S. Taylor (1997). Journal of Experimental
tween code- and language-related abil- (2012). Phonological awareness and print Child Psychology, 71, 2937. doi:10.1006/
ities in the development of both early knowledge of preschool children with jecp.1998.2455
and conventional reading skills as well cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Lan- Bryant, P., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Cross-
guage, and Hearing Research, 55(3), land, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration,
as an exploration of the reciprocity 811823. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11- phoneme detection, and learning to read.
between reading and writing abilities. 0086) Developmental Psychology, 26, 429438.
We also recommend that researchers Anthony, J. L., & Lonigan, C. J. (2004). The na- Burgess, S. R. (2002). The influence of speech
ture of phonological sensitivity: Converging perception, oral language ability, the home
garner insights from the literature on evidence from four studies of preschool and literacy environment, and pre-reading
hearing children, particularly on those early-grade school children. Journal of Edu- knowledge on the growth of phonological
who are struggling readers, to design a cational Psychology, 96, 4355. sensitivity: A 1-year longitudinal investiga-
Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., tion. Reading and Writing: An Interdiscipli-
wide range of investigations aimed at
Driscoll, K., Phillips, B. M., & Cantor, B. G. nary Journal, 15, 709737.
addressing similar research questions (2002). Structure of preschool phonological Burgess, S. R. (2006). The development of
germane to these types of investiga- sensitivity: Overlapping sensitivity to rhyme, phonological sensitivity. In D. K. Dickinson &
tions. Exploring the longitudinal impact words, syllables, and phonemes. Journal of S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early lit-
Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 6592. eracy research (Vol. 2, pp. 90100). New
of phonologically based interventions Archbold, S., & Mayer, C. (2012). Deaf educa- York, NY: Guilford Press.
on the conventional literacy outcomes tion: The impact of cochlear implantation? Burgess, S. R., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Bidirec-
of deaf children, particularly those out- Deafness Education International 14(1), tional relations of phonological sensitivity
215. and pre-reading abilities: Evidence from a
comes associated with the domain of Ball, E., & Blachman, B. (1988). Phonological preschool sample. Journal of Experimental
reading comprehension, would also segmentation training: Effects of reading Child Psychology, 70, 117141.
serve to better inform both research readiness. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 208225. Bus, A. G., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999).
Beal-Alvarez, J., Lederberg, A. R., & Easterbrooks, Phonological awareness and early reading: A
and practice, and perhaps bring some
S. R. (2012). Grapheme-phoneme acquisi- meta-analysis of experimental training stud-
closure to the question of whether, and tion of deaf preschoolers. Journal of Deaf ies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91,
to what extent, phonology matters in Studies and Deaf Education, 17, 3960. 403414.
reading for deaf learners. doi:10.1093/deafed/enr030 Catts, H. W. (1997). The early identification of lan-
Beech, J. R., & Harris, M. (1997). The prelin- guage-based reading disabilities. Language,
gually deaf reader: A case of reliance on Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 28,
A Note on Terminology direct lexical access? Journal of Research in 8689.
In the present article, we use the term Reading, 20, 105121. Catts, H. W., & Kamhi, A. G. (2005). The connec-
Bergeron, J., Lederberg, A. R., Easterbrooks, S. tions between language and reading dis-
deaf to refer to any individual identified R., Miller, E. M., & Connor, C. (2009). Build- abilities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
with a hearing loss, from mild to pro- ing the alphabetic principle in young chil- Chall, J. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great
found, irrespective of the use of ampli- dren who are deaf or hard of hearing. Volta debate. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Review, 109, 87119. Chall, J. S. (1996). Stages of reading develop-
fication. For instance, for the purposes
Bialystok, E. (2004). The impact of bilingualism ment (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
of the article, individuals with cochlear on language and literacy development. In T. Colin, S., Leybaert, J., Ecalle, J., & Magnan, A.
implants are regarded as deaf. We are K. Bhata & W. C. Ritchie (Eds.), The hand- (2013). The development of word recog-
not making a distinction between deaf book of bilingualism (pp. 577601). Malden, nition, sentence comprehension, word
MA: Blackwell. spelling, and vocabulary in children with deaf-
and Deaf, as we do not consider Bialystok, E. (2011). Language proficiency and its ness: A longitudinal study. Research in Devel-
this difference germane to our view implications for monolingual and bilingual opmental Disabilities, 34(5), 17811793.
of the development of reading.The children. In A. Y. Durgunnogablu & C. Gold- doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.02.001
enberg (Eds.), Language and literacy devel- Colin, S., Magnan, A., Ecalle, J., & Leybaert, J.
Authors. opment in bilingual settings (pp. 121138). (2007). Relation between deaf childrens
New York, NY: Guilford Press. phonological skills in kindergarten and word
References Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Devel- recognition performance in first grade. Jour-
Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking opmental dyslexia and specific language nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48,
and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: impairment: Same or different? Psychologi- 139146.
MIT. Press. cal Bulletin, 130, 858888. Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusion in imme-
Adams, M. (1994). Phonics and beginning read- Braze, D., McRoberts, G. W., & McDounough, C. diate memory. British Journal of Psychology,
ing instruction. In F. Lehr & J. Osborn (Eds.), (2011). Early precursors of reading-relevant 55, 7584.
Reading, language, and literacy: Instruc- phonological skills. In S. A. Brady, D. Braze, Cummins, J. (2000). Language, power, and ped-
tion for the 21st century (pp. 323). Hills- & C. A. Fowler (Eds.), Explaining individual agogy: Bilingual children in the crossfire.
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. differences in reading: Theory and evi- Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.
Allen, T., Clark, M. D., del Giudice, A., Koo, D., dence (pp. 2343). New York, NY: Taylor & Cupples, L., Ching, T., Crowe, K., Day, J., & Seeto,
Lieberman, A., Mayberry, R., & Miller, P. Francis. M. (2014). Predictors of early reading skill in
(2009). Phonology and reading: A response Bryant, P. (1998). Sensitivity to onset and rhyme 5-year-old children with hearing loss who use

368

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 369

spoken language. Reading Research Quar- Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem- Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2006). Concurrent cor-
terly 49, 85104. doi:10.1002/rrq.60 ory, and Cognition, 8, 572583. relates and predictors of reading and spelling
Dickinson, D., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. Hanson, V. (1989). Phonology and reading: Evi- achievement in deaf and hearing school chil-
(2010). Speaking out for language: Why lan- dence from profoundly deaf readers. In D. dren. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Edu-
guage is central for learning development. Shankweiler & I. Liberman (Eds.), Phonology cation, 11, 273288.
Educational Researcher, 29, 305310. and reading disability: Solving the reading Kyle, F. E., & Harris, M. (2011). Longitudinal pat-
Dickinson, D., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., puzzle (pp. 6989). Ann Arbor: University of terns of emerging literacy in beginning deaf
Peisner-Feinberg, E., & Poe, M. D. (2003). Michigan Press. and hearing readers. Journal of Deaf Studies
The comprehensive language approach to Hanson, V. (1990). Recall of order formation by and Deaf Education, 16, 289304.
early literacy: The interrelationships among deaf signers: Phonetic coding in temporal Lonigan, C. J. (2006). Conceptualizing phono-
vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and order recall. Memory and Cognition, 18, logical processing skills in pre-readers. In D.
print knowledge among preschool-aged chil- 604610. K. Dickinson & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Hand-
dren. Journal of Educational Psychology, Hanson, V., & Fowler, C. (1987). Phonological book of early literacy research (Vol. 2, pp.
95, 465481. coding in word reading: Evidence from deaf 7789). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Dickinson, D., McCabe, A., & Essex, M. (2006). A and hearing readers. Memory and Cogni- Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L.
window of opportunity we must open to all: tion, 15, 199207. (2000). Development of emergent literacy
The case for preschool with high-quality sup- Hanson, V., Goodell, E. W., & Perfetti, C. A. and early reading skills in preschool chil-
port for language and literacy. In D. Dickin- (1991). Tongue-twister effects in the silent dren: Evidence from a latent-variable longi-
son & S. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early reading of hearing and deaf college students. tudinal study. Developmental Psychology,
literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. 1128). New Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 36, 596613.
York, NY: Guilford Press. 319330. Marschark, M., Sarchet, T., Rhoten, C., & Zupan,
Dillon, C. M., de Jong, K., Pisoni, D. B. (2012). Hanson, V., & Lichtenstein, E. (1990). Short-term M. (2010). Will cochlear implants close the
Phonological awareness, reading skills, and memory coding by deaf signers: The primary reading achievement gap for deaf students?
vocabulary knowledge in children who use language coding hypothesis reconsidered. In M. Marschark & P. Spencer (Eds.), Oxford
cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies Cognitive Psychology, 22, 211224. handbook of deaf studies, language, and
and Deaf Education, 17, 205226. Harris, M., & Beech, J. R. (1998). Implicit phono- education (Vol. 2, pp. 127143). New York,
doi:10.1093/deafed/enr043 logical awareness and early reading develop- NY: Oxford University Press.
Easterbrooks, S. R., Lederberg, A. R., Miller, E. ment in prelingually deaf children. Journal Mayberry, R., del Guidice, A., & Lieberman, A.
M., Bergeron, J. P., & Connor, C. M. (2008). of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 3, (2011). Reading achievement in relation to
Emergent literacy skills during early child- 205216. phonological coding and awareness in deaf
hood in children with hearing loss: Strengths Harris, M., & Moreno, C. (2006). Speechreading readers: A meta-analysis. Journal of Deaf
and weaknesses. Volta Review, 108, 91114. and learning to read: A comparison of 8-year- Studies and Deaf Education, 16, 164188.
Ehri, L., Nunes, S., Stahl, S., & Willows, D. old profoundly deaf children with good and doi:10.1093/deafed/enq049
(2001). Systematic phonics instruction poor reading abilities. Journal of Deaf Stud- Mayer, C. (1999). Shaping at the point of utter-
helps students learn to read: Evidence from ies and Deaf Education, 11, 189201. ance: An investigation of the composing proc-
the National Reading Panels meta-analysis. Hatcher, P. J., & Hulme, C. (1999). Phonemes, esses of the deaf student writer. Journal of
Review of Educational Research, 71, 393 rhymes, and intelligence as predictors of chil- Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 4, 3749.
447. drens responsiveness to remedial reading Mayer, C. (2007). What really matters in the early
Fagan, M. K., Pisoni, D. B., Horn, D. L., & Dillon, instruction: Evidence from a longitudinal literacy development of deaf children. Jour-
C. M. (2007). Neuropsychological correlates intervention study. Journal of Experimental nal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12,
of vocabulary, reading, and working memory Child Psychology, 72, 130153. doi:10.1006/ 411431. doi:10.1093/deafed/enm020
in deaf children with cochlear implants. Jour- jecp.1998.2480 Mayer, C. (2009). Issues in second-language lit-
nal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12, Hornberger, N. (1989). Continua of biliteracy. eracy education with learners who are deaf.
461471. Review of Educational Research, 59, International Journal of Bilingual Educa-
Frost, J., Madsbjerg, S., Niederse, J., Olofsson, 271296. tion and Bilingualism, 12(3), 325334.
A., & Srensen, P. M. (2005). Semantic and Hulme, C. (2002). Phonemes, rimes, and the Mayer, C., & Akamatsu, C. T. (2011). Bilingualism
phonological skills in predicting reading mechanisms of early reading development. and literacy. In M. Marschark & P. Spencer
development: From 3 to 16 years age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies,
Dyslexia, 11(2), 7992. 82, 5864. doi:10.1006/jecp.2002.2674 language, and education (Vol. 1, 2nd ed.,
Geers, A. (2003). Predictors of reading skill Hulme, C., Hatcher, P. J., Nation, K., Brown, A., pp. 144155). New York, NY: Oxford Univer-
development in children with early cochlear Adams, J., & Stuart, G. (2002). Phoneme sity Press.
implantation. Ear and Hearing, 24, 59S68S. awareness is a better predictor of early read- Mayer, C., & Leigh, G. (2010). The changing con-
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonologi- ing skill than onset-rime awareness. Journal text for sign bilingual education programs:
cal skills and learning to read. Hillsdale, NJ: of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 228. Issues in language and the development of
Erlbaum. Hulme, C., Muter, V., & Snowling, M. J. (1998). literacy. International Journal of Bilin-
Guardino, C., Syverud, S. M., Joyner, A., Nicols, Segmentation does predict early progress in gualism and Bilingual Education, 13(2),
H., & King, S. (2011). Further evidence of learning to read better than rhyme: A reply 175186.
the effectiveness of phonological instruction to Bryant. Journal of Experimental Child Mayer, C., & Trezek, B. J. (2011). New (?) answers
with oral-deaf readers. American Annals of Psychology, 71, 3944. doi:10.1006/jecp to old questions: Literacy development in
the Deaf, 155, 562568. doi:10.1353/aad/ .1998.2456 D/HH learners. In D. F. Moores (Ed.), Part-
2011.0002 Izzo, A. (2002). Phonemic awareness and read- ners in education: Issues and trends from
Hanson, V. (1982). Short-term memory recall by ing ability: An investigation with young read- the 21st International Congress on the Edu-
deaf signers of American Sign Language: ers who are deaf. American Annals of the cation of the Deaf (pp. 6274). Washington,
Implications for order recall. Journal of Deaf, 147, 1828. DC: Gallaudet University Press.

369

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 370

REEXAMINING THE ROLE OF PHONOLOGY

Mayer, C., & Trezek, B. J. (in press). Early liter- Paul, P., Wang, Y., Trezek, B. J., & Luckner, J. L. acquisition (pp. 307342). Hillsdale, NJ:
acy development in deaf children. New (2009). Phonology is necessary, but not suf- Erlbaum.
York, NY: Oxford University Press. ficient: A rejoinder. American Annals of the Stanovich, K. E. (2000). Progress in under-
Mayer, C., & Wells, G. (1996). Can the linguistic Deaf, 154, 346356. standing reading: Scientific foundations
interdependence theory support a bilingual Paul, P., Wang, Y., & Williams, C. (2013). Deaf stu- and new frontiers. New York, NY: Guilford
model of literacy education for deaf stu- dents and the qualitative similarity hypoth- Press.
dents? Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf esis: Understanding language and literacy Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Pheno-
Education, 1, 93107. development. Washington, DC: Gallaudet typic performance profile of children with
McGuinness, D. (2004). Early reading instruc- University Press. reading disabilities: A regression-based test
tion: What science really tells us about how Phillips, B. M., & Torgesen, J. K. (2006). Phone- of the phonological-core variable-difference
to teach reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. mic awareness and reading: Beyond the model. Journal of Educational Psychology,
McQuarrie, L., & Parrila, R. (2009). Phonological growth of initial reading accuracy. In D. K. 38, 934947.
representations in deaf children: Rethinking Dickinson & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Hand- Storch, S., & Whitehurst, G. (2002). Oral lan-
the functional equivalence hypothesis. book of early literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. guage and code-related precursors to read-
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Educa- 101112). New York, NY: Guilford Press. ing: Evidence from a longitudinal structural
tion, 14, 137154. Pressley, M. (2005). Reading instruction that model. Developmental Psychology, 38,
Miller, P. (2009). The nature and efficiency of the works: The case for balanced teaching (3rd 934947.
word-reading strategies of prelingually deaf- ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Syverud, S. M., Guardino, C., & Selznick, D. N.
ened native signers. Journal of Deaf Studies Pugh, K. R., Sandak, R., Frost, S. J., Moore, D. L., (2009). Teaching phonological skills to a
and Deaf Education, 14, 344361. & Mencl, W. E. (2006). Neurobiological inves- deaf first grader: A promising strategy. Amer-
Miller, P. (2010). Phonological, orthographic, and tigations of skilled and impaired reading. In ican Annals of the Deaf, 154, 382388.
syntactic awareness and their relation to D. K. Dickinson & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), doi:10.1353/aad.0.0113
reading comprehension in prelingually deaf Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2, Transler, C., & Reitsma, P. (2011). Phonological
individuals: What can we learn from skilled pp. 6474). New York, NY: Guilford Press. coding in reading of deaf children: Pseudo-
readers? Journal of Developmental and Scarborough, H. (2001). Connecting early lan- homophone effects in lexical decision.
Physical Disabilities, 22, 549580. guage and literacy to later reading (dis)abili- British Journal of Developmental Psychol-
Miller, P., & Clark, M. D. (2011). Phonemic aware- ties: Evidence, theory, and practice. In D. K. ogy, 23(4), 525542.
ness is not necessary to become a skilled deaf Dickinson & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Hand- Trezek, B. J., & Hancock, G. R. (2013). Imple-
reader. Journal of Developmental and Phys- book of early literacy research (Vol. 1, pp. menting instruction in the alphabetic princi-
ical Disabilities, 23, 459476. doi:10.1007/ 97110). New York, NY: Guilford Press. ple within a sign bilingual setting. Journal
s10882-011-9246-0 Schatschneider, C., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 18,
Moores, D. (2001). Sign and phonology. Ameri- Fletcher, J. M., & Mehta, P. (1999). The 391408. doi:10.1093/deafed/ent016
can Annals of the Deaf, 146, 34. dimensionality of phonological awareness: Trezek, B. J., & Malmgren, K. W. (2005). The
Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. An application of item response theory. Jour- efficacy of utilizing a phonics treatment
(1979). Does awareness of speech as a nal of Educational Psychology, 91, 439449. package with middle school deaf and hard-
sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Smith, A., & Wang, Y. (2010). The impact of of-hearing students. Journal of Deaf Stud-
Cognition, 7, 323331. Visual Phonics on the phonological aware- ies and Deaf Education, 10, 257271.
Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Taylor, S. ness and speech production of a student doi:10.1093/deafed/eni028
(1997). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts who is deaf: A case study. American Annals Trezek, B. J., & Wang, Y. (2006). Implications of uti-
early progress in learning to read. Journal of the Deaf, 155, 124130. lizing a phonics-based reading curriculum
of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, Snow, C., Burns, S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Prevent- with children who are deaf or hard of hearing.
370396. doi:10.1006/jecp.1996.2365 ing reading difficulties in young children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education,
Nation, K., & Hulme, C. (1997). Phonemic seg- Washington, DC: National Academy Press. 10, 202213. doi:10.1093/deafed/enj031
mentation, not onset-rime segmentation, Spencer, L. J., & Tomblin, J. B. (2009). Evaluating Trezek, B. J., Wang, Y., & Paul, P. (2010). Reading
predicts early reading and spelling skills. phonological processing skills in children and deafness: Theory, research, and prac-
Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 154167. with prelingual deafness who use cochlear tice. Clifton Park, NY: Cengage Learning.
doi:10.1598/RRQ.32.2.2 implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Trezek, B. J., Wang, Y., Woods, D. G., Gampp,
National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing Education, 14, 121. doi:10.1093/deafed/ T. L., & Paul, P. (2007). Using Visual Phonics
early literacy: Report of the National Early enn013 to supplement beginning reading instruc-
Literacy Panel. Washington, DC: National Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining tion for students who are deaf or hard of
Institute for Literacy. Retrieved from http:// phonological awareness and its relationship hearing. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
lincs.ed.gov/publications/pdf/NELPReport09 to early reading. Journal of Educational Psy- Education, 12, 373384. doi:10.1093/deafed/
.pdf chology, 86, 221234. enm014
National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in read- Vermeulen, A. M., Van Bon, W., Schreuder, R.,
National Reading Panel: Teaching children ing: Some consequences of individual differ- Knoors, H., & Snik, A. (2007). Reading com-
to read: An evidence-based assessment of ences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading prehension of deaf children with cochlear
the scientific research literature on reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360407. implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
and its implications for reading instruc- Stanovich, K. E. (1988). The right and wrong Education, 12, 283302.
tion. Jessup, MD: National Institute for Liter- places to look for the cognitive locus of read- Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughton, P., Sim-
acy at EDPubs. ing disability. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, mons, K., & Roshotte, C. A. (1993). The
Paul, P. (2003). Processes and components of 154157. doi:10.1007/BF02648254 development of young readers phonological
reading. In M. Marschark & P. Spencer (Eds.), Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the processing abilities. Journal of Educational
Handbook of deaf studies, language, and causes and consequences of individual differ- Psychology, 85, 120.
education (pp. 97109). New York, NY: ences in reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A.
Oxford University Press. L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading (1994). Development of reading-related

370

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18663-AAD159.4_Fall2014 11/6/14 3:32 PM Page 371

phonological processing abilities: New evi- to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. Wang, Y., Trezek, B. J., Luckner, J. L., & Paul, P. V.
dence of bidirectional causality from a latent- Developmental Psychology, 33, 468479. (2008). The role of phonology and phono-
variable longitudinal study. Developmental Wang, Y., Spychala, H., Harris, R. S., & Oetting, T. logically related skills in reading instruction
Psychology, 30, 7387. L. (2013). The effectiveness of a phonics- for students who are deaf or hard of hear-
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., based early intervention for deaf and hard ing. American Annals of the Deaf, 153,
Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. of hearing preschool children and its possi- 396407.
(1997). Changing relations between phono- ble impact on reading skills in elementary Whitehurst, G., & Lonigan, C. (1998). Child
logical processing abilities and word-level school: A case study. American Annals of the development and emergent literacy. Child
reading as children develop from beginning Deaf, 158, 107120. Development, 68, 848872.

371

VOLUME 159, NO. 4, 2014 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


Copyright of American Annals of the Deaf is the property of American Annals of the Deaf
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.

Potrebbero piacerti anche