Sei sulla pagina 1di 6

Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2011 American Psychological Association

2012, Vol. 121, No. 1, 276 281 0021-843X/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0023598

BRIEF REPORT

When Are Adaptive Strategies Most Predictive of Psychopathology?

Amelia Aldao and Susan Nolen-Hoeksema


Yale University

In recent work, we showed that putatively adaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as reappraisal and
acceptance, have a weaker association with psychopathology than putatively maladaptive strategies, such
as rumination, suppression, and avoidance (e.g., Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Aldao, Nolen-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010). In this investigation, we examined the interaction between adaptive and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in the prediction of psychopathology symptoms (depression,


anxiety, and alcohol problems) concurrently and prospectively. We assessed trait emotion regulation and
psychopathology symptoms in a sample of community residents at Time 1 (N 1,317) and then
reassessed psychopathology at Time 2 (N 1,132). Cross-sectionally, we found that the relationship
between adaptive strategies and psychopathology symptoms was moderated by levels of maladaptive
strategies: adaptive strategies had a negative association with psychopathology symptoms only at high
levels of maladaptive strategies. In contrast, adaptive strategies showed no prospective relationship to
psychopathology symptoms either alone or in interaction with maladaptive strategies. We discuss the
implications of this investigation for future work on the contextual factors surrounding the deployment
of emotion regulation strategies.

Keywords: emotion regulation, adaptive regulation strategies, maladaptive regulation strategies,


flexibility, longitudinal

In the last decade, researchers have been increasingly interested ity (e.g., Campbell-Sills et al., 2006). In addition, these putatively
in examining the process by which individuals regulate emotional adaptive strategies are important components of a variety of treat-
states to respond to environmental demands (e.g., Gross, 1998; ment modalities, ranging from traditional cognitive behavioral
Kring & Sloan, 2010; Rottenberg & Gross, 2003). A substantial therapy to newer, third-wave approaches (e.g., Beck, 1976; Hayes,
amount of theoretical and empirical work has focused on the 2008; Hofmann & Asmundson, 2008; Roemer, Orsillo, & Salters-
processes by which emotion regulation strategies modify emo- Pedneault, 2008).
tional experiences and their associated expressions, behavioral On the other hand, strategies like rumination, suppression,
components, and physiological states, thereby increasing or de- avoidance, and worry generally have been associated with mal-
creasing the risk of psychopathology (e.g., Campbell-Sills, Bar- adaptive outcomes, including rebounds in negative affect follow-
low, Brown, & Hofmann, 2006; Gross, 1998; Hofmann, Heering, ing engagement with emotion-eliciting stimuli (e.g., Campbell-
Sawyer, & Asnaani, 2009). Sills et al., 2006), memory difficulties (e.g., Richards, Butler, &
The work on emotion regulation strategies has led to a func- Gross, 2003), increases in sympathetic activation (e.g., Gross &
tional differentiation of strategies based on their ability to facilitate Levenson, 1993; Wegner, Broome, & Blumberg, 1997), and di-
adaptive versus maladaptive responding. Specifically, strategies minished autonomic flexibility (e.g., Hofmann et al., 2005). Fur-
such as cognitive reappraisal, acceptance, and problem solving thermore, the self-reported use of these strategies has been asso-
generally have been associated with adaptive outcomes, including ciated with the development and maintenance of a wide range of
reductions in the experience of negative affect (e.g., Goldin, disorders, including depression (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyu-
McRae, Ramel, & Gross, 2007), increased pain tolerance (e.g., bomirsky, 2008), generalized anxiety disorder (Mennin, Holaway,
Hayes et al., 1999), effective interpersonal functioning (e.g., Rich- Fresco, Moore, & Heimberg, 2007), alcohol problems (Nolen-
ards & Gross, 2000), and diminished maladaptive cardiac reactiv- Hoeksema & Harrell, 2002), and eating disorders (Nolen-
Hoeksema, Stice, Wade, & Bohon, 2007).
Recently, we showed that putatively adaptive and maladaptive
strategies differ in their strength of association with psychopathol-
This article was published Online First May 9, 2011. ogy. Specifically, we observed that maladaptive strategies are
Amelia Aldao and Susan Nolen-Hoeksema, Department of Psychology,
consistently more strongly associated with psychopathology than
Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut.
We thank Elena Wright and Yael Levin for their thorough comments on
adaptive strategies (e.g., Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Aldao,
earlier versions of this article. Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010). It is interesting to note that
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Amelia we have seen this pattern of findings when examining both inter-
Aldao, Department of Psychology, Yale University, 2 Hillhouse Avenue, nalizing (e.g., depression, anxiety) and externalizing (e.g., alcohol,
New Haven, CT 06520. E-mail: Amelia.aldao@yale.edu eating) pathology in community samples and samples including

276
ADAPTIVE STRATEGIES AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 277

clinical and nonclinical groups, suggesting that this asymmetry in psychopathology after controlling for baseline levels of psychopa-
the strength of the relationship between adaptive and maladaptive thology (for a review, see Aldao et al., 2010). However, little is
strategies and psychopathology generalizes across the range of know about how adaptive and maladaptive strategies interact to
psychopathology (e.g., Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, predict changes in psychopathology over time. Therefore, in the
2004; Kring & Sloan, 2010). Understanding why adaptive strate- present investigation we also examined how the interaction be-
gies are relatively weak predictors of psychopathology is impor- tween adaptive and maladaptive strategies at baseline (i.e., Time 1)
tant given the centrality of such strategies to treatment and pre- predicted psychopathology symptoms one year later (i.e., Time 2),
vention efforts (e.g., Beck, 1976; Hayes, 2008; Hofmann & while controlling for baseline levels of psychopathology.
Asmundson, 2008; Roemer et al., 2008). In particular, understand-
ing when adaptive strategies are more or less useful would be
Method
critical to teaching clients how to use these strategies effectively.
Using adaptive strategies may be more difficult for people who
are also prone to using maladaptive strategies, because maladap- Participants and Procedures
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tive strategies may interfere with the use of adaptive strategies.


Participants were recruited through random-digit dialing of res-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Indirect evidence for this interference hypothesis comes from work


idential telephone numbers in San Francisco, San Jose, and Oak-
showing that maladaptive strategies may narrow attentional focus,
land, California; 1,789 individuals were initially called. Of those,
therefore preventing individuals from noticing the context around
1,317 participated in the first interview, and 1,132 of the original
them (e.g., Borkovec, Alcaine, & Behar, 2004; Richards et al.,
participants responded to a second interview 1 year later. Individ-
2003; Richards & Gross, 2000; Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000). For
uals who did not participate in the second interview scored higher
example, Joormann and colleagues (see Joormann & DAvanzato,
in psychopathology than those who did, t(1316) 3.05, p .01.1
2010) have found that rumination interferes with the ability to shift
At Time 1, the mean age of the sample was 47 years old (SD
attention away from negative stimuli. Therefore, to the extent that
15.22; range 24 82), and 52.7% of the sample identified as
maladaptive strategies narrow attentional focus, they might inter-
female. Seventy percent of the sample was Caucasian, 9.2% His-
fere with individuals ability to access and properly deploy adap-
panic, 8% African American, 6.3% Asian or Pacific Islander, and
tive regulation strategies.
6.5% identified as other or did not respond (for other demographic
On the other hand, a compensatory hypothesis suggests that
information, see Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000).
adaptive strategies may be most important in preventing psycho-
At both Time 1 and Time 2, all participants were interviewed in
pathology in people who also tend to use maladaptive strategies.
person. All measures were read aloud to participants and response
This hypothesis is consistent with models suggesting that people
options were presented on cards. Interviewers were extensively
have a repertoire of emotion regulation strategies that they use
trained in standardized responses to participants requests for clar-
flexibly depending on circumstances (e.g., Bonanno, Papa,
ification of items and probes to clarify participants answers to
ONeill, Westphal, & Coifman, 2004; Cheng, 2003; Kashdan &
questions when necessary.
Rottenberg, in press; Westphal, Seivert, & Bonanno, 2010).
Among people whose repertoire includes maladaptive strategies,
adaptive strategies may be particularly predictive of psychopathol- Measures
ogy outcomes.
Depression was assessed with the 13-item Beck Depression
In this investigation, we sought to further investigate the asym-
InventoryShort Form (BDI-SF; Beck & Beck, 1972). At Time 1,
metry in the strength of the relationship between adaptive and
internal consistency was good ( .83), and the testretest
maladaptive strategies and psychopathology. Specifically, we
correlation was .60 ( p .01). In addition, the module for major
tested whether trait maladaptive strategies moderated the relation-
depression from the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diag-
ship between trait adaptive strategies and symptoms of psychopa-
nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition
thology. Based on previous work (e.g., Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema,
(SCID; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997) was adminis-
2010; Aldao et al., 2010), we predicted that maladaptive strategies
tered. Interrater reliability on this module was 100% (see Nolen-
would be a stronger predictor of psychopathology than adaptive
Hoeksema, 2000). We created a continuous measure of depression
strategies. In addition, we predicted that adaptive and maladaptive
by summing the number of symptoms recorded for each respon-
strategies would interact with each other. Two competing possi-
dent. Internal reliability at Time 1 was .68, and the testretest
bilities were examined. First, an interference hypothesis would
reliability was .48 (p .01).
suggest that adaptive strategies would have a weaker negative
Anxiety was assessed with the 21-item Beck Anxiety Inventory
relationship with psychopathology when levels of maladaptive
(BAI; Beck & Steer, 1990). Internal reliability at Time 1 was good
strategies are high than when they are low. On the other hand, a
( .88), and the testretest correlation was .63 (p .01). We did
compensatory hypothesis would suggest that adaptive strategies
not administer the SCID modules on anxiety for this study because
would have a stronger negative relationship with psychopathology
it was designed as a study of depression, but interviewers com-
when levels of maladaptive strategies are high than when they are
pleted a one-item global rating of the anxiety levels of the respon-
low.
In addition to examining this relationship concurrently, we
evaluated the interplay between adaptive and maladaptive strate- 1
In the results section, we present analyses conducted with the full
gies and changes in psychopathology symptoms over time. Previ- sample. Analyses conducted with the smaller set of individuals who par-
ous work has shown that the use of maladaptive strategies pro- ticipated in both assessments paralleled the results obtained with the full
spectively predicts the development and maintenance of sample.
278 ALDAO AND NOLEN-HOEKSEMA

dents immediately after the interview on a scale ranging from 0 levels of distress tend to use many types of coping or emotion
(none) to 4 (disabling), using information reported by the respon- regulation in their attempts to regulate their negative affect (e.g.,
dents on the BAI, as well as information provided by the respon- Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008).
dents verbal and nonverbal behavior throughout the interview.
Testretest reliability was .32 (p .01).
Results
Alcohol-related problems were assessed with the relevant mod-
ule of the SCID (First et al., 1997). We summed the number of
problems recorded to create a continuous score from 0 to 8 (see Cross-Sectional Analyses
Nolen-Hoeksema & Harrell, 2002). The internal consistency was Zero-order correlations. We examined zero-order correla-
good ( .77). The testretest reliability was .57 (p .01). tions at Time 1 for the full sample. Consistent with previous work,
Recent work shows that emotion regulation strategies are asso- the psychopathology composite score was positively correlated
ciated with all three types of psychopathology we assessed (e.g., with the maladaptive strategies composite score (r .50, p .01)
Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010; Kring & Sloan, 2010). Further- and was not correlated with the adaptive strategies composite score
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

more, measures of the three symptom types were intercorrelated (r .03, ns).
(all ps .01). Thus, we standardized scores on each of the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Regression analyses. We predicted the psychopathology


measures (BDI, SCID depressive symptoms, BAI, anxiety rating, composite score using a stepwise regression. In the first step, we
SCID alcohol use problems) and averaged them for an overall entered adaptive and maladaptive strategies scores (which were
index of psychopathology. We note, however, that a similar pattern centered to reduce multicollinearity; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007),
of results is found whether we use this aggregated index of R2 .25, F(2, 1314) 217.06, p .01. Maladaptive strategies
psychopathology or separate indices of internalizing symptoms contributed significantly to the prediction of psychopathology,
(depression and anxiety) or alcohol problems.2 For parsimony, we t(1314) 20.81, p .01, .50. Adaptive strategies were not
report only the analyses of the overall psychopathology index. The a significant predictor, t(1314) 1.58, ns, .04. In a
testretest reliability for this index was .67 (p .01). second step, we entered the interaction term between adaptive and
The Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS; Treynor, Gonzalez, & maladaptive strategies, R2 .004, F(1, 1313) 7.34, p .01.
Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003) is a 22-item measure that assesses the Maladaptive strategies remained a significant predictor, t(1313)
tendency to engage in ruminative behavior in response to stress. 20.88, p .01, .50, and adaptive strategies became a signif-
Treynor and colleagues (2003) have removed those items with a icant predictor, t(1313) 2.05, p .05, .056. More
high content overlap with depressive symptoms. The resulting important, the interaction term between adaptive and maladaptive
brooding subscale contains 5 items and reflects the moody rumi- strategies was significant, t(1313) 2.71, p .01, .07.
nation at the core of Nolen-Hoeksemas (1991) rumination theory We followed this interaction with simple slopes analyses (Aiken
(e.g., What am I doing to deserve this). In this sample, internal & West, 1991). Both the interference hypothesis and the compen-
reliability of the brooding subscale was good ( .77). satory hypothesis entail predictions (in opposite directions) about
The COPE InventoryShort Version (Carver, 1997) is a shorter the relationship between adaptive strategies to psychopathology in
version of the original COPE inventory (Carver, Scheier, & Wein- the presence of high versus low levels of maladaptive strategies.
traub, 1989) that was adopted for this study because respondent Thus, we evaluated the role of adaptive strategies as a predictor of
burden was already high. We utilized the following scales: accep- psychopathology at high and low levels of maladaptive strategies
tance, positive reframing, behavioral disengagement, and denial. (i.e., 1 SD above/below the mean). At low levels of maladaptive
In the initial investigation (Carver, 1997), the internal consistency strategies, adaptive strategies were unrelated to psychopathology,
for these scales ranged from .57 to .64 and in the current investi- t(1313) .41, ns, .01. However, at high levels of maladaptive
gation the range was from .53 to .70. In addition, because at the strategies, greater levels of adaptive strategies were related to
time this study was conducted (mid-1990s), there was no widely lower levels of psychopathology, t(1313) 3.08, p .01,
used measure of emotional, thought, and expressive suppression, .11.
we created a 4-item subscale assessing all aspects of suppression
(i.e., expressive, thought, and emotional). These items had good
internal consistency ( .76). Longitudinal Analyses
To model the relationship between adaptive and maladaptive Correlations. We examined the relationship between strate-
strategies, we created two composite scores reflecting the use of gies at Time 1 and psychopathology symptoms at Time 2. In
each type of strategy. The composite score of adaptive strategies parallel with the cross-sectional results, maladaptive strategies at
consisted of the average between COPE acceptance and COPE Time 1 were correlated with the composite score of psychopathol-
positive reframing and the composite score of maladaptive strat- ogy at Time 2 (r .41, p .01), even after controlling for
egies consisted of the average of COPE behavioral disengagement, symptoms at Time 1 (r .12, p .01). In addition, mimicking the
COPE denial, suppression, and RRS brooding (they were all on the cross-sectional results, adaptive strategies at Time 1 were not
same scale). We submitted these subscales to a factor analysis and correlated with the psychopathology at Time 2 (r .03).
showed that the maladaptive scales loaded onto one factor (34% of Regression analyses. We predicted psychopathology symp-
the variance), and the adaptive scales loaded onto a separate factor toms at Time 2 with adaptive and maladaptive strategies at Time
(23% of the variance). The internal consistency was adequate for 1. In Step 1, we entered psychopathology symptoms at Time 1 and,
adaptive strategies ( .68) and good for maladaptive strategies
( .81). The scores were mildly correlated with each other (r
.13, p .01), likely because individuals who experience elevated 2
Available from the first author.
ADAPTIVE STRATEGIES AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 279

vated, therefore suggesting that their effectiveness might depend


on the other regulation strategies present in an individuals reper-
toire. Future empirical work should examine the process by which
individuals select one strategy over another one (e.g., Egloff,
Schmukle, Burns, & Schwerdtfeger, 2006) and can flexibly switch
between using different strategies that might be differentially
adaptive for varying contexts (e.g., Westphal et al., 2010). In
addition, it will be important to examine other contextual factors,
such as trait (e.g., emotion intensity, personality) and state (e.g.,
emotion reactivity, attention processes, physiology) components of
affective processes.
Although this study provided an important examination of the
moderating role of maladaptive strategies on adaptive strategies, it
Figure 1. Cross-sectional interaction between adaptive and maladaptive
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

had limitations. The first was our use of self-report measures of


strategies. High and low levels correspond to 1 SD above or below the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

mean, respectively. Adaptive strategies have a negative association with


psychopathology, which are subject to reporting bias (e.g., Rob-
psychopathology only at high levels of maladaptive strategies. inson & Clore, 2002). We attempted to reduce the influence of
such biases by administering these self-report measures in the
context of an interview and combining them with indices of
as expected, we found that it was a significant predictor, t(1124) psychopathology that came from structured interviews and inter-
29.87, p .01, .67, R2 .44. In Step 2, we entered adaptive viewer ratings. Still, replicating our findings with measures of
and maladaptive strategies at Time 1 centered, R2 .01, F(2, psychopathology that are all based on structured clinical inter-
1122) 8.13, p .01. Psychopathology symptoms at Time 1 views will be important.
remained a significant predictor, t(1122) 24.11, p .01, Second, our sample was a representative community sample,
.62, and the maladaptive strategies score was also a significant raising the question of whether our results would be replicated in
predictor, t(1122) 3.97, p .01, .10, but adaptive strategies comparisons of individuals with and without diagnosed disorders.
was not a significant predictor, t(1122) .02, ns, .00. In step In our previous meta analytic review (Aldao et al., 2010), we
3, we added the interaction between adaptive and maladaptive showed that maladaptive strategies were even more strongly re-
strategies at Time 1, but, unlike in the cross-sectional models, it lated to psychopathology when clinical and nonclinical samples
was not a significant predictor, t(1121) .57, ns, .01. were compared than when community samples with a range of
psychopathology were used; in contrast, adaptive strategies were
Discussion weakly and inconsistently related to psychopathology in both
community samples and comparisons of clinical and nonclinical
We examined the relationship between trait adaptive (positive
samples. Thus, our meta analysis suggests that we would find
reframing and acceptance) and maladaptive (rumination, suppres-
similar results in comparisons of clinical and nonclinical samples.
sion, behavioral disengagement, and denial) emotion regulation
strategies in the prediction of psychopathology symptoms concur- Furthermore, emotion regulation theories suggest that the relation-
rently and prospectively. At the cross-sectional level, we found ship between specific strategies and emotional distress or un-
that maladaptive strategies moderated the relationship between healthy behaviors (such as substance misuse) can be characterized
adaptive strategies and psychopathology symptoms: adaptive strat- as continuous (for reviews, see Kring & Sloan, 2010); thus, the use
egies had a negative association with psychopathology symptoms of a community sample with a representative range of psychopa-
only when levels of maladaptive strategies were elevated. This thology provide an appropriate test of these theories. Still, the
provides support for the compensatory hypothesis that adaptive generalizability of our results to comparisons of clinical versus
strategies have significant relationships to psychopathology symp- nonclinical samples remains to be tested.
toms in the presence of maladaptive strategies. On the other hand, Third, emotion regulation was measured through self-report
in prospective analyses, only maladaptive strategies predicted psy- questionnaires, although they were administered in the context of
chopathology symptoms at Time 2. a structured interview. Future studies should utilize experimental
In previous work (Aldao & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010), we argued designs to assess the multiple emotional domains (e.g., subjective,
that adaptive strategies might show a weaker association with physiological, and behavioral; Bradley & Lang, 2000). Further-
psychopathology because their usefulness might be context depen- more, we only examined 6 regulation strategies, so it will be
dent (e.g., reappraisal might be mostly adaptive when the situation important for future work to incorporate a wider range of strategies
can actually be reframed, or acceptance might be most appropriate (e.g., problem solving, seeking social support).
when there is little action an individual can take to change his or Fourth, an alternative hypothesis for the stronger association be-
her circumstances). One type of context may be the affective tween maladaptive strategies and psychopathology is that the scales
and/or regulatory state of the individual. Sheppes, Cartan, and used to measure maladaptive strategies have content overlap with
Meiran (2009) have shown that the effects of reappraisal depend psychopathology scales. In this investigation, we sought to minimize
on the point in time in the emotion process in which it is deployed. this potential confound by utilizing the brooding scale of the RRS
Similarly, in this investigation we found that adaptive strategies (Treynor et al., 2003), from which the items that most obviously
showed a negative association with concurrent levels of psycho- overlapped with distress were removed. In addition, the COPE scales
pathology only when levels of maladaptive strategies were ele- we used had minimal overlap with symptoms of negative affect.
280 ALDAO AND NOLEN-HOEKSEMA

In this investigation, we examined the relationship between First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997).
adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in the Structured Clinical Interview for DSMIV Axis I DisordersNon-patient
prediction of psychopathology symptoms concurrently and pro- edition (version 2.0). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric
spectively. When predicting psychopathology symptoms cross- Institute Biometrics Research Department.
sectionally, we found support for a context-dependent role of Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The neural
adaptive strategies, so that their association with psychopathology bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative
emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 577586. doi: 10.1016/j.bio-
symptoms was moderated by levels of maladaptive strategies.
psych.2007.05.031
Specifically, adaptive strategies had a negative association with
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integra-
psychopathology symptoms only at high levels of maladaptive tive review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271299. Retrieved from
strategies, providing support for a compensatory hypothesis. In http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/gpr/
contrast, adaptive strategies did not show an association with Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiol-
psychopathology symptoms longitudinally, suggesting their pre- ogy, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and
dictive power might be limited. Social Psychology, 64, 970 986. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

pubs/journals/psp/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

References Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regressions: Testing and behavioural processes across psychological disorders. Oxford, United
interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kingdom: Oxford University Press.
Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of cognitive emotion Hayes, S. C. (2008). Climbing our hills: A beginning conversation on the
regulation strategies: A transdiagnostic examination. Behaviour Re- comparison of acceptance and commitment therapy and traditional cog-
search and Therapy, 48, 974 983. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.002 nitive behavioral therapy. Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion regu- Reviews, 15, 286 295. Retrieved from http://www.wiley.com/bw/
lation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psy- journal.asp?ref0969 5893
chology Review, 30, 217237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004 Hayes, S. C., Bissett, R. T., Korn, Z., Zettle, R. D., Rosenfarb, I. S.,
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New Copper, L. D., & Grundt, A. M. (1999). The impact of acceptance versus
York, NY: International University Press. control rationales on pain tolerance. The Psychological Record, 49,
Beck, A. T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients in family 33 47. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/
practice: A rapid technique. Postgraduate Medicine, 52, 81 85. Re- Hofmann, S. G., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2008). Acceptance and
trieved from http://www.postgradmed.com/ mindfulness-based therapy: New wave or old hat. Clinical Psychology
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1990). Manual for the revised Beck Anxiety Review, 28, 116. Retrieved from http://www.wiley.com/bw/
Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. journal.asp?ref0969 5893
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., ONeill, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. Hofmann, S. G., Heering, S., Sawyer, A. T., & Asnaani, A. (2009). How
(2004). The importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance to handle anxiety: The effects of reappraisal, acceptance, and suppres-
and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psy- sion strategies on anxious arousal. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47,
chological Science, 15, 482 487. doi: 10.1111/j.0956 7976.2004 389 394. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.010
.00705.x Hofmann, S. G., Moscovitch, D. A., Litz, B. T., Kim, H., Davis, L. L., &
Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of Pizzagalli, D. A. (2005). The worried mind: Autonomic and prefrontal
worry and generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, activation during worrying. Emotion, 5, 464 475. doi: 10.1037/1528
& D. S. Mennin (Eds.) Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in 3542.5.4.464
research and practice (pp. 77108). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Joormann, J., & DAvanzato, C. (2010). Emotion regulation in depression:
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, O. J. (2000). Measuring emotion: Behavior, Examining the role of cognitive processes. Cognition & Emotion, 24,
feeling, and physiology. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive 913939. doi: 10.1080/02699931003784939
neuroscience of emotion (pp. 242276). New York, NY: Oxford Uni-
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (in press). Psychological flexibility as a
versity Press.
fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review. doi: 10.1016/
Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2006).
j.cpr.2010.03.001
Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of indi-
Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. S. (2010). Emotion regulation and psychopa-
viduals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and
thology. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Therapy, 44, 12511263. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001
Mennin, D. S., Holaway, R., Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., & Heimberg,
Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocols too
R. G. (2007). Delineating components of emotion and its dysregulation
long: Consider the brief COPE. International Journal of Behavioral
Medicine, 4, 92100. Retrieved from http://www.springer.com/ in anxiety and mood psychopathology. Behavior Therapy, 38, 284 302.
medicine/journal/12529 doi: 110.1016/j.brat.2006.12.004
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on
strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Social Psychology, 56, 267283. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/ 100, 569 582.
pubs/journals/psp/ Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders
Cheng, C. (2003). Cognitive and motivational processes underlying coping and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
flexibility: A dual-process model. Journal of Personality and Social ogy, 109, 504 511. doi: 101037/10021-843X.109.3.504
Psychology, 84, 425 438. doi: 10.1037/00223514.84.2.425 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Harrell, Z. A. (2002). Rumination, depression, and
Egloff, B., Schmukle, S. C., Burns, L. R., & Schwerdtfeger, A. (2006). alcohol use: Tests of gender differences. Journal of Cognitive Psycho-
Spontaneous emotion regulation during evaluated speaking tasks: Asso- therapy, 16, 391 403. Retrieved from http://www.ingentaconnect.com/
ciations with negative affect, anxiety expression, memory, and physio- content/springer/jcogp
logical responding. Emotion, 6, 356 366. doi: 10.1037/1528 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Stice, E., Wade, E., & Bohon, C. (2007). Reciprocal
3542.6.3.356 relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depres-
ADAPTIVE STRATEGIES AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY 281

sive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Sheppes, G., Catran, E., & Meiran, N. (2009). Reappraisal (but not dis-
116, 198 207. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.198 traction) is going to make you sweat: Physiological evidence for self-
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking control effort. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 9196.
rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400 424. Re- doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.06.006
trieved from http://pps.sagepub.com/ Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th
Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotion regulation in ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
romantic relationships: The cognitive consequences of concealing feel- Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination
ings. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20, 599 620. doi: reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Re-
I0.I037//00223514.79.3.41 search, 27, 248 259. Retrieved from http://www.springerlink.com/
Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: content/101589/
The cognitive costs of keeping ones cool. Personality Processes and Wegner, D. M., Broome, A., & Blumberg, S. J. (1997). Ironic effects of
Individual Differences, 79, 410 424. doi: I0.I037//00223514.79.3.41 trying to relax under stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 1121.
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Episodic and semantic knowledge Retrieved from http://journals.elsevier.com/00057967/behaviour-
in emotional self-report: Evidence for two judgment processes. Journal research-and-therapy/
Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 198 215. doi: 10.1037//0022


Review of Psychology, 51, 59 91. Retrieved from http://www.annual-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

3514.83.1.198
Roemer, L., Orsillo, S. M., & Salters-Pedneault, K. (2008). Efficacy of an reviews.org/journal/psych
acceptance-based behavior therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: Westphal, M., Seivert, N. H., & Bonanno, G. A. (2010). Expressive
Evaluation in a randomized control trial. Journal of Counseling and flexibility. Emotion, 10, 92100. doi: 10.1037/a0018420
Clinical Psychology, 76, 10831089. doi: 10.1037/a0012720
Rottenberg, J., & Gross, J. J. (2003). When emotion goes wrong: Realizing Received November 18, 2010
the promise of affective science. Clinical Psychology Science and Prac- Revision received March 4, 2011
tice, 10, 227232. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bpg012 Accepted March 8, 2011

Potrebbero piacerti anche