Sei sulla pagina 1di 9

CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL, 24(23), 200207, 2012

Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC


ISSN: 1040-0419 print=1532-6934 online
DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2012.677341

How Emotional Stability and Openness to Experience Support


Invention: A Study with German Independent Inventors
Harald A. Mieg and Stephan J. Bedenk
Humboldt-Universitat zu Berlin

Anna Braun
PricewaterhouseCoopers AG, Frankfurt

Franz J. Neyer
Friedrich Schiller Universitat, Jena

How does invention depend on personality? Do inventors differ from noninventors? This
study investigated the personal factors inuencing a sample of independent inventors in
Germany (N 69). Standardized psychological questionnaires were employed to assess
the Big Five personality dimensions, willingness to take risks, self-concept, and self-
efcacy. For a comparison with noninventors, scores from reference samples were taken.
Hypotheses were formulated based on research into creativity and entrepreneurship and
the normative DABEI Stage Model of invention. As expected, the independent inventors
showed higher levels of extraversion and openness to experience. In addition, inde-
pendent inventors stood out in terms of emotional stability (low neuroticism scores). Sur-
prisingly, in this sample success as an inventor correlated negatively with openness to
experience. The interpretation is along the lines that inventors may work in a focused
manner and technical implementation of an innovative idea may be optimized with great
persistence. Discussion centers around the generalizability of the results.

From as early as Schumpeter (1911=1934), inventions have quite rare (cf. Astebro, Jeffrey & Adomdza, 2007; Hender-
been considered as the drivers of innovation and, there- son, 2004a, 2004b; Lemelson-MIT Program, 2003; Mark-
fore, of the growth of national economic health. According man, Balkin, & Baron, 2002; Weinstein & Klein, 1995).
to the United States Patent and Trademark Ofce This is surprising, as research on inventors would seem to
(USPTO), there are more than 400,000 patent applications be a fruitful addition to existing psychological research into
(U.S. and international) each year (USPTO, 2010), with up creativity, inventors representing highly creative people
to 28% of patent applications coming from independent (e.g., Weisberg, 2006; see also Weisman Deitch, 2001).
inventors (Vargas, 2007). Even though business manage- Against this background, this study followed a very
ment research has dealt in more detail with inventors (Cec- basic psychological approach: It is an empirical investi-
cagnoli, Gambardella, Giuri, Licht, & Mariani, 2006; gation of inventors personalities. It was intended to
Dahlin, Taylor, & Fichman, 2004; Ernst, Leptien, & Vitt, shed light on differences between the personalities of
2000), in-depth psychological research on inventors is still independent inventors and noninventors in Germany.

Our thanks are due to the Hans-Sauer-Stiftung, Munich, which CLASSES OF INVENTORS
supported this study. This article has been preceded by a previous pub-
lication by Braun, Meig, & Neyer (2009).
Correspondence should be sent to Harald A. Mieg, Humboldt-
Three classes of inventors can be distinguished (see Mieg,
Universitaet zu Berlin, Unter den Linden 6, D-10099 Berlin, Germany. Hoffmann, & Spars, 2010). First, employed inventors
E-mail: harald.mieg@geo.hu-berlin.de who, as a rule, work in the research and development
INVENTORS 201

departments of companies or other institutions and mainly based on self-efcacy, self-esteem, and awareness
clearly show little interest in possible independent work of capabilities and limitations. In terms of success factors,
(see Kassicieh, Radosevich, & Banbury, 1997). Second, Hellstrom et al. (2002) found a positive self-concept to be
inventor-entrepreneurs, as inventors, set up their own supportive for technological innovation. Research on the
companies and take on a management role. Third, inven- expectation of self-efcacy (Bandura, 1997) matches
tors who are independent, often shy away from setting these ndings. People with high expectation of
up their own companies and, instead, attempt to sell their self-efcacy are optimistic about mastering a difcult
inventions. This research focused on personality traits situation, whereby success is attributed to ones own
and success factors of this third group of inventors, as skills (see Beghetto, 2006; Choi, 2004; Schwarzer & Jeru-
they represent the prototypical creative and Edison-like salem, 1999). In fact, expectations of self-efcacy
type of inventor (Dahlin et al., 2004; Lettl, Rost, & von appeared to be directly relevant to success in entrepre-
Wartburg, 2009). neurial behavior or entrepreneurship (Boyd & Vozikis,
1994; Chandler & Jansen 1997; Gist & Mitchell, 1992).
One study also reported higher self-efcacy for inventors
PERSONALITY TRAITS OF INDEPENDENT who at the time of the study were actively dealing with
INVENTORS IN THE FRAMEWORK OF THE business formation (Markman et al., 2002).
DABEI STAGE MODEL
Phase 2: Creativity: The Big Five Personality Traits
For this research, three sources for the identication of
relevant personality traits were considered. The rst was Based on previous research into creativity, extraversion
psychological research into creativity and the second and openness to change were considered as important
included studies on entrepreneurial activity. As a third characteristics of inventors in phase 2 of the DABEI Stage
source, hypotheses about personality traits of inventors Model (Table 1). Together with agreeableness, neuroti-
were derived from the DABEI Stage Model of invention cism, and conscientiousness, these two traits constitute
(DABEI, 1987). the 5-Factor model of personality (see Costa & McCrae,
The DABEI Stage Model has been developed by the 1992), one of the most frequently used classication
German inventor-entrepreneur Hans Sauer and other models for personality traits (Schnabel, Asendorpf, &
prolic German inventors, such as Artur Fischer, who Ostendorf, 2002).
holds over 1,000 patents, and Ludwig Bolkow, the inven- Specic assessments of the Big Five involving real
tor of the Airbus (see DABEI, 1987). It is an experience- inventors are to date not to be found in the literature.
based and nonempirical model of psychological success Appropriate results come, instead, from research into
factors as seen by expert inventors. As a normative creativity and innovation. For instance, Biebrich and
model, the DABEI Stage Model describes the route from Kuhl (2002) generally regarded openness to experience
the idea to the product and full exploitation (see DABEI, as one of the most important personality traits for
1987, p. 2). The 10 stages of the model show how inven- creative processes (see also King, McKee Walker, &
tors should ideally progress to master the process of Broyles, 1996). On an empirical basis, Ng and Rodrigues
invention and to develop the characteristics required to (2002) found higher levels of extraversion and openness
be successful in inventing (Table 1). To allow relevant to experience in innovators (per Kirton, 1976).
personality traits to be allocated to the stages of the
model, the 10 stages have been divided into three phases
Phase 3: Entrepreneurship: Willingness to
(see Table 1).
Take Risks
Willingness to take risks is a crucial factor for both crea-
Phase 1: Self-Knowledge: Self-Concept and
tivity (Phase 2 of the DABEI Stage Model) and entrepre-
Self-Efficacy
neurship (Phase 3 of the DABEI Stage Model). As
As a central element of self-knowledge, the DABEI Stage regards creativity, Sternberg and Lubart (1996) empha-
Model emphasizes the importance of a suitable self- sized that in psychological processes responsible for cre-
concept for successful inventors (see DABEI, 1987, ative solutions, risky, rather than carefully thought-out,
p. 45). Hence, psychological research on self-concepts strategies are applied. Forster and Friedmann (2003)
was also taken into account for this study. The self- remarked, That, on many levels, creative solutions and
concept describes a persons assumptions about his or innovations are riskier rather than conservative (p. 150).
her own personal characteristics and how these assump- Willingness to take risks also characterizes entrepre-
tions affect experience and behavior (see Turner, 1968). neurial behavior (see, e.g., March & Shapira, 1987; Stew-
Hellstrom, Hellstrom, and Berglund (2002) revealed an art & Roth, 2001). The risk consists in investing
innovators reexive self-conception (p. 274), that is resources without knowing the precise output. Blanch-
202 MIEG ET AL.

TABLE 1
Stage System of the DABEI Inventors Manual

Stage Situation, What to Pay Attention to and What to Do? Relevant Personality Traits?

1. Self-knowledge It is necessary to check whether one has the Phase 1


Who am I? predisposition to be an inventor and=or Self-knowledge (DABEI Stage 1):
What can I do? entrepreneur and if so, what knowledge and skills self-concept (Hellstrom, Hellstrom, &
Psychological makeup? still need to be acquired. Berglund, 2002), self-efcacy (Bandura,
1997)
2. Knowing the system The possible effects of an innovation in systems Phase 2
What am I in society? intended for it and associated systems are to be Creativity (DABEI Stages 26): openness to
identied. experience (McCrae, 1987), extraversion
3. Identifying and classifying The prospects of success in solving problems improve (King, McKee Walker, & Broyles, 1996;
problems and formulating the with the clarity of the formulation of a task. Ng & Rodrigues, 2002)
task Evaluate several approaches to a solution.
4. Search for ideas and methods of The success of a development often depends on the
solving problems motive, motivation and intensity with which
problems are tackled.
5. Invention process, maturity of The process of invention usually begins with
the invention fortuitous information regarding the problem to
be solved. Models and samples accelerate the
process of bringing an invention to maturity.
6. Patent rights, yes? no? With a low level of protection, it often pays to
Publication? maintain secrecy and to concentrate on the
production know-how, which is where imitators
frequently fail.
7. Implementation by granting a This most difcult of stagesassociated with the Phase 3
license, setting up a rm, alone highest riskusually needs a lot of time if Stages 1 Entrepreneurship (DABEI Stages 710):
or in cooperation to 6 are still mortgaged. Good cooperation willingness to take risks (cf. Patterson,
partners reduce problems and risks! 2002; March & Shapira, 1987; Weber, Blais,
8. Commercialization only in ones Many an innovation is denied success if the sales & Betz, 2002)
own company or by granting concept is not appropriate to the product, Sales is
licenses? lacking in creativity and knowledge of the system
and its benets are not known to the user.
9. Further and=or new Complex products or systems do not achieve
developments of the technology maturity until after trials have been conducted or
and rms culture experience gained with their use. So Stages 3, 4, 5
and 6 and sometimes also 7 and 8 have to be
repeated, with some modications.
10. Market penetration, expansion With the diffusion of an innovation, the structures of
of the rm, diversication, a company set up for it change. And the market
follow-up and context have changed too. With the
reorientation of the company, follow-up and
obligations to employees and to society should also
be taken into account.

ower and Oswald (1998) and Petrakis (2004) argued Model, the rst hypothesis (H1) stated that independent
that these risks mainly include nancial resources in inventors in this sample should differ from noninventors
the way that entrepreneurs expend their own capital in ve personality traits: self-concept, self-efcacy, open-
and loans. Nevertheless, references in Patterson (2002) ness to experience, extraversion, and risk attitude.
to the discovery of a certain lack of restraint in innova- As a second hypothesis (H2), these personality traits
tors lead one to suspect that willingness to take risks were assumed to be each directly correlated with inven-
may also be relevant for ethical decisions. tors success.

METHOD
HYPOTHESES
Participants
Based on the reported research and literature on creati-
vity and entrepreneurship and assumptions made by Participants were all inventors associated with the
the eld-experienced authors of the DABEI Stage Hans-Sauer-Stiftung (a non-prot-making foundation
INVENTORS 203

that supports inventors) and members of the INSTI- by Johnson, Wilke, & Weber, 2004) on measu-
Ernderclub Berlin (www.ernderclub-berlin.de [inven- ring the risk attitude with a total of six sub-scales:
tors club]). For data assessment, all inventors were investment, ethical, recreational, health, social
visited and provided with the standardized test package. and gambling. The focus of this study was on
69 inventors (64 males, 5 females) took part in the study, the two subscales investment and ethical.
which corresponded to a return rate of 59%. 49 inven- 3. Two of the total of 11 subscales of the Frankfurt
tors (46 males, 3 females) were associated with the self-concept scales after Deusinger (1986): the
Hans-Sauer-Stiftung. The age of the participants ranged Frankfurt Selbstkonzeptskala zur allgemeinen
from 27 to 80 years (M 49.3; SD 14.2). All of them Leistungsfahigkeit (FSAL, self-concept scale on
were independent inventors. However, 20% of them general performance) and the Frankfurt Selbst-
were additionally employed. As compensation, every konzeptskala zur Verhaltens- und Entscheidungs-
participant received a book on cognitive psychology. sicherheit (FSVE, self-concept scale on certainty
in behavior and decisions).
4. The short scale on the general expectation of self-
Measures efcacy (Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1999). The scale
The test package included three parts. Part A contained contains 10 items that cover general, optimistic
standardized psychological tests in the following order: self-belief.

1. The German version of the NEO-FFI (Borkenau Reliabilities of all scales were satisfactory to good
& Ostendorf, 1993; Korner, Geyer, & Brahler, with a coefcients ranging from .66 to .86 (see Table 2).
2002) as a measure of the Big Five personality As reference values for noninventors, reported reference
factors. scores were taken from manuals and representative
2. The Domain-Specic Risk Scale (DOSPERT; studies (see Table 2 and Results section for specic
Weber, Blais, & Betz, 2002; in a German version references).

TABLE 2
Personality Characteristics of Inventors

Inventors (N 69)
Reference Samplesa
Scale a M (s) M (s) z value

NEO-FFI (Big Five)


Openness to experience 0.66 2.66 (0.46) 2.71 (0.52) 0.80
2.05 (0.47) 10.78
Extraversion 0.76 2.50 (0.50) 2.36 (0.57) 2.04
2.19 (0.51) 5.05
Neuroticism (emotionally unstable) 0.86 1.37 (0.63) 1.84 (0.70) 5.58
1.63 (0.63) 3.43
Agreeableness 0.59 2.54 (0.39) 2.44 (0.49) 1.70
2.51 (0.49) 0.51
Conscientiousness 0.78 2.77 (0.48) 2.53 (0.63) 3.16
2.68 (0.57) 1.31
DOSPERT-G (Risk attitude)
Investment 0.77 2.57 (0.96) 2.63 (0.89) 0.56
Ethical 0.82 1.98 (0.67) 2.74 (0.68) 9.28
Gambling 0.86 1.59 (0.85) 1.71 (0.76) 1.31
Health 0.73 2.43 (0.68) 2.74 (0.66) 3.90
Recreational 0.79 2.57 (0.89) 2.68 (0.77) 1.19
Social 0.47 3.53 (0.52) 3.71 (0.47) 3.18
Frankfurt self-concept scales
FSAL 0.82 Total (s2): 47.54 (40.56) Total (s2): 47.1 (34.12) 0.11
FSVE 0.70 Total (s2): 28.62 (12.74) Total (s2): 27.8 (16.16) 0.42
Scale of general self-efcacy
Self-efcacy 0.85 Total (s2): 31.16 (15.85) Total (s2): 29.5 (28.42) 0.49

Note. Bold scales are relevant to the hypothesis on differences between inventors vs. non-inventors. FSAL Frankfurt Selbstkonzeptskala zur
allgemeinen Leistungsfahigkeit. FSVE Selbstkonzeptskala zur Verhaltens- und Entscheidungssicherheit.
a
Big-Five factors: rst reference sample by the Borkenau and Ostendorf (1993) test manual; second reference sample by Korner, Geyer, and
Brahler (2002).

p < 0.05 (bilateral).  p < 0.01 (bilateral).
204 MIEG ET AL.

Part B of the test package provided information the values for the three other dimensions of the Big Five,
about personal and professional interests and socio- which were considered less relevant for inventors prior
demographic background information. Part C enquired to this study (neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientious-
about various aspects of inventive activity including suc- ness). As can be seen, inventors in this sample were
cess factors. To test the predictability of inventors found to be signicantly more emotionally stable (with
success by personality traits, success was operationalized lower neuroticism values) than noninventors.
by measuring the number of patents. German patent law
protects inventions in the form of patents, aesthetic mod-
Personality Traits as Success Factors
els and utility models (PAUMs). For this study, PAUMs
applied for, PAUMs granted, and PAUMs established in The predictability of inventors success (operationalized
the market were assessed. In the present sample, the by number of patents: PAUMS applied for, PAUMs
number of PAUMs applied for ranged from 0 to 75, granted, PAUMs established in the market) by scores
the number of PAUMs granted from 0 to 40, and on the personality scales was tested by running multiple
the number of PAUMs established in the market from regression analyses. Here, calculations of success vari-
0 to 20. ables were performed with age-free residual variables,
as age was found to be signicantly correlated with
PAUMs (e.g., PAUMS granted: r 0.40; p < 0.01).
The second hypothesis (H2), which stated that the ve
RESULTS
personality traits should each directly predict inventors
success, could not be fully conrmed with the present
Comparison of Inventors and Noninventors on
data. Only one regression for the openness to experience
Personality Traits
scale resulted in a signicant negative beta for the
Table 2 shows the results of the z-tests for the compari- PAUMs established in the market (R2 0.095, corrected
sons of inventors and noninventors on personality traits. R2 0.079; F 5.97; unstandardized b 0.64, standar-
In line with the hypothesis H1, a signicant difference in dized b 0.29; p < 0.05).
extraversion was found between independent inventors
and noninventors. As suspected, inventors in this sample
were more extrovert than the average population, as DISCUSSION
reported by Borkenau and Ostendorf (1993) and Korner
et al. (2002). In terms of openness to experience, the sup- The main aim of this study was to determine personality
posed difference did not appear in comparison to the ref- factors of independent inventors in a German sample in
erence sample from the German NEO-FFI manual comparison to reference samples of non-inventors. Based
(Borkenau & Ostendorf, 1993). However, the difference on the assumptions derived form the normative DABEI
could be found in comparison to the reference sample Stage Model that successful invention depends on an
provided by Korner et al. (2002). With willingness to optimistic self-concept, self-efcacy (Phase 1) and both
take investment risks, the participating inventors showed creative (Phase 2) and entrepreneurial (Phase 3)
no signicant difference compared to the population as personality traits, questionnaires for relevant personality
reported by Johnson et al. (2004). However, inventors models were applied.
in this sample were signicantly less willing to take risks Concerning self-concept and self-efcacy, no differ-
in the ethical area, compared to the population studied ence could be found between inventors and
by Johnson et al. (2004). No signicant differences could noninventors. In addition, scores on the self-concept
be found between the participating inventors and the ref- and self-efcacy scales failed to predict success of inven-
erence samples of noninventors on the two self-concept tors in a signicant manner. Thus, the requirement of the
scales (for reference samples, see Deusinger, 1986), as DABEI inventors manual for self-knowledge (see
well as on the short self-efcacy scale on the general Table 1) was not reected in the form of particularly
expectation of self-efcacy (for a reference sample, see marked self-concepts in real inventors.
Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1999). Taken overall, there On the entrepreneurial side, an increased willingness
was no full empirical support for the rst hypothesis to take nancial and ethical risks among inventors was
(H1), which stated that inventors and noninventors expected. However, this could not be conrmed with
should differ in ve personality traits (self concept, the present data. It is worth noting thatin comparison
self-efcacy, openness to experience, extraversion, and to noninventorsinventors showed not very marked
risk attitude). characteristics on all the risk scales taken into account.
In addition to the tests of the a priori hypothesis, In the case of health and ethical and social risk behavior,
comparisons between inventors and noninventors were inventors even showed a signicantly lower level than the
run for all the other assessed variables. Table 2 shows population average. The general nding of inventors
INVENTORS 205

willingness to take risks was, therefore, clearly disproved their goalshence presumably the negative correlation
for this sample. between success and openness to new experience. One
might come to the paradoxical conclusion that only
those inventors who are at a certain point in the inven-
Focused Cognition as an Explanation of
tion process not open to further experiences any longer,
Unexpected Emotional Stability and Doubtful
but who are strictly focused on their invention instead
Influence of Openness to Experience
are successful in the market in the long run.
With regard to the creative aspect of invention (Phase 2),
some of the presented results have not turned out as sup-
posed. Prior to the study, high levels of openness to Limitations and Future Research
experience and extraversion were expected for inventors.
One limitation of this research might be a lack of general-
This was fully conrmed for extraversion. For openness
izability, as the study focused on independent inventors
to experience it was only conrmed, when comparing
only. Hence, the results cannot be generalized to all
inventors to the representative population sample pro-
inventors. As already indicated in the introduction sec-
vided by Korner et al. (2002). However, the reason
tion, personal characteristics of independent inventors
why the comparison with the sample provided by
can differ from employed inventors and inventor
Borkenau and Ostendorf (1993) failed signicance might
entrepreneurs (Kassicieh et al., 1997; Mieg et al., 2010).
be a methodical one. Borkenau and Ostendorf surveyed
For instance, the willingness to take nancial or ethical
a relatively young sample (average age 28.7 years), which
risks is likely to be stronger among inventor
tends to go along with higher scores on openness to
entrepreneurs than among independent inventors.
experience.
Second, as the sample included German inventors only
Prior to the study, it was also expected that both
and the reference samples represent standard values for
these factors would directly predict inventors success.
the German population, conclusions may be limited with
However, the expected positive correlation could not
regard to differences between inventors and noninven-
be conrmed with this data. Surprisingly, an even nega-
tors in other countries. However, the presented ndings
tive correlation between openness to experience and suc-
t also to the view of American inventors, as research
cess as an inventor was found. That is, the inventors that
by Henderson (2004a, 2004b) shows.
were more successful in the market than others were
Another limitation of this study is that hypotheses
those who were less open to new experience. A stepwise
were mainly derived from the normative DABEI Stage
multiple regression analysis with all relevant scales
Model. This implicates that psychological factors that
showed that the openness to experience scale proved
have been found to be important for inventors (e.g., opti-
to be a stable predictor of PAUMs established in the
mism and overcondence; Astebro et al., 2007) were only
market. With regard to prior psychodiagnostic research
considered in the framework of the DABEI Stage Model
into creativity and entrepreneurship, this robust nega-
(e.g., as a marked self-concept). This approach might be
tive correlation between openness to experience and suc-
seen as a potential methodological drawback, parti-
cess in commercializing patents was not to be expected.
cularly since the DABEI Stage Model is an experience-
A second surprising result was the very low neuroticism
based, and not a scientically founded, model. However,
level of inventors. That is, inventors turned out to be
it seemed fruitful to derive hypotheses about psychologi-
much more emotionally stable than noninventors.
cal factors from a model of invention. As there is still no
Again, prior research into creativity can merely explain
comprehensive theory on the psychology of invention (cf.
these differences between inventors and non-inventors.
Lemelson-MIT Program, 2003), the DABEI Stage Model
One approach to explain our data can presumably be
was applied, instead. Nevertheless, the theory-driven
constructed by using qualitative research on inventors.
development of a broader framework and its empirical
According to this line of research, inventors are strongly
validation could be a valuable work in future research
intrinsically motivated (e.g., Henderson, 2004a, 2004b).
on the psychology of inventors.
Their personal motivation is based on mastery and
enthusiasm (Henderson, 2004a, pp. 114115) and
leads to a blend of trying things out on the one hand
and to persistence in pursuing an idea on the other (see REFERENCES
also Lemelson-MIT Program, 2003; R. J. Weber,
1996). According to this explanation, inventors work in Astebro, T., Jeffrey, S. A., & Adomdza, G. K. (2007). Inventor
a focused manner (Mieg, 2010; Wolf & Mieg, 2010). perseverance after being told to quit: The role of cognitive biases.
Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20, 253272. doi: 10.1002=
They need a certain emotional stability to not let them- bdm.554.
selves be distractedhence the low neuroticism Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efcacy: The exercise of control. New York,
levelsand perseverance and isolation in the pursuit of NY: Freeman.
206 MIEG ET AL.

Beghetto, R. A. (2006). Creative self-efcacy: Correlates in middle expected benets, and perceived-risk attitudes in German-speaking
and secondary students. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 447457. populations. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 35, 153163.
doi: 10.1207=s15326934crj1804_4. Kassicieh, S. K., Radosevich, H. R., & Banbury, C. M. (1997). Using
Biebrich, R., & Kuhl, J. (2002). Neurotizismus und Kreativitat: Struk- attitudinal, situational, and personal characteristics variables to pre-
turelle Unterschiede in der Beeinussung kreativer Leistung. [Neuroti- dict future entrepreneurs from national laboratory inventors. IEEE
cism and creativity: Structural differences in the determination of Transactions on Engineering Management, 44, 248257.
creative performance]. Zeitschrift fur Differentielle und Diagnostische King, L. A., McKee Walker, L., & Broyles, S. J. (1996). Creativity
Psychologie, 23, 171190. and the ve-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 30,
Blanchower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (1998). What makes an entrepre- 189203.
neur? Journal of Labour Economics, 16, 2660. Kirton, M. J. (1976). Adaptors and innovators: A description and a
Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1993). NEO-Funf-Faktoren-Inventar measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 622629.
(NEO-FFI) nach Costa und McCrae (Handanweisung). Gottingen, Korner, A., Geyer, M., & Brahler, E. (2002). Das NEO-Funf-Faktoren
Germany: Hogrefe. Inventar (NEO-FFI) Validierung anhand einer deutschen Bevolker-
Boyd, N. G., & Vozikis, G. S. (1994). The self-efcacy on the develop- ungsstichprobe. Diagnostica, 48, 1927.
ment of entrepreneurial intentions and actions. Entrepreneurship Lemelson-MIT Program. (2003). The architecture of invention (Report
Theory and Practice, 18, 6377. of the workshop, held in August 2003). Boston: School of Engineer-
Braun, A., Mieg, H. A., & Neyer, F. J. (2009). Sind Ernder anders als ing, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Retrieved February 14,
die Kreativitats forschung erwarten lasst? [Do inventors differ from 2009 from http://web.mit.edu/INVENT/n-pressreleases/downloads/
how psychological research on creativity perceives them?] architecture.pdf
Wtschaftpsychologie, 11, 6979. Lettl, C., Rost, K., & von Wartburg, I. (2009). Why are some inde-
Ceccagnoli, M., Gambardella, A., Giuri, P., Licht, G., & Mariani, M. pendent inventors heroes and others hobbyists? The moderating
(2006). Study on evaluating the knowledge economyWhat are role of technological diversity and specialization. Research Policy,
patents actually worth? The value of patents for todays economy 38, 243254.
and society. Report for the European Commission Brussels. March, J. G., & Shapira, Z. (1987). Managerial perspectives on risk
Chandler, G. N., & Jansen, E. (1997). Founder self-efcacy and ven- and risk taking. Management Science, 33, 14041418.
ture performance: A longitudinal study. Academy of Management Markman, G. D., Balkin, D. B., & Baron, R. A. (2002). Inventors and
Proceedings, 28, 98102. new venture formation: The effects of general self-efcacy and regretful
Choi, J. (2004). Individual and contextual predictors of creative perfor- thinking. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 27, 149166.
mance: The mediating role of psychological processes. Creativity McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to
Research Journal, 16, 187199. doi:10.1207=s15326934crj1602&3_4 experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1258
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assess- 1265.
ment in clinical practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psycho- Mieg, H. A. (2010). Focused cognition: Information integration
logical Assessment, 4, 513. and complex problem solving by top inventors. In K. L. Mosier &
Deutsche Aktionsgemeinschaft Bildung Erndung Innovation. U. M. Fischer (Eds.), Informed by knowledge: Expert perfor-
(1987). DABEIHandbuch fur Ernder und Unternehmer. Dussel- mance in complex situations (pp. 4154). London, UK: Taylor &
dorf, Germany: VDI Verlag. Francis.
Dahlin, K., Taylor, M., & Fichman, M. (2004). Todays Edisons or Mieg, H. A., Hoffmann, C., & Spars, G. (2010). Evaluierung der
weekend hobbyists: Technical merit and success of inventions by volkswirtschaftlichen Bedeutung von Einzelerndungen und deren
independent inventors. Research Policy, 33(8), 11671183. doi: Umsetzungspotential am Standort Berlin. Studie im Auftrag der
10.1016=j.respol.2004.06.003. Senatsverwaltung fur Wirtschaft, Technologie und Frauen, Bericht.
Deusinger, I. M. (1986). Die Frankfurter Selbstkonzeptskalen (FSKN). Retrieved May 25, 2011 from http://www.berlin.de/imperia/
Handanweisung [Frankfurt self-concept scales. Mannual. Gottingen, md/content/senwirtschaft/innovationspolitik/bedeutung_einzeler
Germany: Hogrefe. ndungen.pdf
Ernst, H., Leptien, C., & Vitt, J. (2000). Inventors are not alike: The Ng, A. K., & Rodrigues, D. (2002). A Big-Five personality prole of
distribution of patenting output among industrial R&D personnel. the adaptor and innovator. Journal of Creativity Behavior, 36,
IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management, 47, 184199. 254268.
Forster, J., & Friedmann, R. (2003). Kontextabhangige Kreativitat. Patterson, F. (2002). Great minds dont think alike? Person-level
[Context-depending creativity]. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie, 211(3), predictors of innovation at work. International Review of Industrial
149160. and Organizational Psychology, 17, 115144.
Gist, M. E., & Mitchell, T. R. (1992). Self-efcacy: A theoretical analy- Petrakis, P. E. (2004). Entrepreneurship and risk premium. Small
sis of its determinants and malleability. Academy of Management Business Economics, 23, 8598.
Review, 17, 183211. Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Ostendorf, F. (2002). Replicable
Hellstrom, T., Hellstrom, C., & Berglund, H. (2002). The innovation types and subtypes of personality: German NEO-PI-R versus
self: Exploring self among a group of technological innovators. NEO-FFI. European Journal of Personality, 16, 724.
Journal of Managerial Psychology, 17, 267286. doi: 10.1108= Schumpeter, J. A. (1934). The theory of economic development (R.
02683940210428083. Opie., Trans.). London, UK: Transaction. (Original German work
Henderson, S. J. (2004a). Inventors: The ordinary genius next door. In R. published 1911)
J. Sternberg, E. L. Grigorenko, & J. L. Singer (Eds.), Creativity (pp. Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1999). Skalen zur Erfassung von
103125). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Lehrer- und Schulermerkmalen. Dokumentation der psychometrischen
Henderson, S. J. (2004b). Product inventors and creativity: The ner Verfahren im Rahmen der Wissenschaftlichen Begleitung des Modell-
dimensions of enjoyment. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 293 versuchs Selbstwirksame Schulen. Berlin, Germany: Freie Universi-
312. doi: 10.1207=s15326934crj1602&3_12. tat Berlin.
Johnson, J. G., Wilke, A., & Weber, E. U. (2004). Beyond a trait view Sternberg, J. R., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity.
of risk taking: A domain-specic scale measuring risk perceptions, American Psychologist, 51, 677688.
INVENTORS 207

Turner, R. H. (1968). The self conception in social interaction. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263290. doi:
In G. Gordon & R. Gergen (Eds.), The self in social interaction 10.1002=bdm.414.
(pp. 93106). New York, NY: Wiley. Weber, R. J. (1996). Toward a language of invention and synthetic
United States Patent and Trademark Ofce. (2010). U.S. patent statistics: thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 353364.
Chart calendar years 19632008. Retrieved February 15, 2010 from Weisberg, R. W. (2006). Creativity: Understanding innovation in
http://www.uspto.gov/web/ofces/ac/ido/oeip/taf/us_stat.htm problem solving, science, invention, and the arts. Hoboken, NJ:
Vargas, T. (2007, November 18). Novel ideas pass through lter at Wiley.
U.S. patent ofce. Washington Post. Retrieved June 8, 2011 from Weinstein, N. D., & Klein, W. M. P. (1995). Resistance of personal
http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2007/11/ risk perceptions to debiasing interventions. Health Psychology, 14,
14/AR2007111400770.html 132140.
Stewart, W. H. Jr., & Roth, P. L. (2001). Risk propensity differences Weisman Deitch, J. (2001). A nation of inventors. Researching American
between entrepreneurs and managers. A meta-analytic review. Jour- history. Carlisle, MA: Discovery Enterprises.
nal of Applied Psychology, 86, 145153. Wolf, K., & Mieg, H. A. (2010). Inventors as experts in complex
Weber, E. U., Blais, A.-R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specic- problem solving. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22,
risk-attitude sale: Measuring risk perception and risk behavior. 443462. doi: 10.1080=09541440902916845.
Copyright of Creativity Research Journal is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be
copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Potrebbero piacerti anche