Sei sulla pagina 1di 9

Original Paper

Folia Phoniatr Logop 2008;60:1119 Published online: November 30, 2007


DOI: 10.1159/000111799

Living with Dysarthria: Evaluation of a


Self-Report Questionnaire
Lena Hartelius Marie Elmberg Rebecca Holm Ann-Sofie Lvberg
Stiliani Nikolaidis
Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Division of Speech and Language Pathology, and the Vrdal Institute,
Gteborg University, Gteborg, Sweden

Key Words Introduction


Dysarthria Living with Dysarthria questionnaire
Communicative difficulties Some aspects of a neurogenic speech disorder are overt
and easier to identify than others. Speech and voice pa-
rameters can be assessed and quantified in terms of num-
Abstract ber of seconds of sustained phonation, number of sylla-
The study describes an effort to evaluate the speech difficul- bles per second in an alternating motion rate task, per-
ties as perceived by individuals with dysarthria. A self-report centage intelligible words, etc. The covert aspects, the
questionnaire, Living with Neurologically Based Speech Dif- subjective consequences of living with a speech disorder
ficulties (Living with Dysarthria), was answered by 55 indi- caused by a neurological condition, are more complex
viduals with varying types and degrees of dysarthria. Results and difficult to get a comprehensive picture of. Encoun-
show that both type and degree of subjectively perceived tering speech difficulties due to a neurological disease or
communicative difficulties varied. Degree of communica- injury may involve great problems. This paper presents
tive difficulties was not related to age, gender, diagnosis, an effort to evaluate speech difficulties as perceived by
disease duration or employment status in this group. Gener- individuals with dysarthria using the Living with Dysar-
ally, the overriding problems were related to restrictions in thria (LwD) questionnaire. It also attempts to determine
communicative participation, possibilities to actively take whether this clinically relevant short questionnaire
part in work and studies and to express ones personality achieves similar results compared to the longer Commu-
were particularly affected. Communication was also affect- nication Profile for Speakers with Motor Speech Disor-
ed by emotions and by the number and familiarity of people ders.
present in communicative encounters. The dominating Dysarthria is defined as a neurological motor speech
speech difficulties were related to reduced speech rate and disorder characterized by slow, weak, imprecise and/or
a need for repetition as a consequence of misunderstand- uncoordinated movements of the speech musculature [1].
ings. A statistically nonsignificant difference was found Acquired dysarthria can be classified as progressive (as
between the higher mean of the group with moderate in degenerative diseases such as Parkinsons disease, mul-
dysarthria compared to the groups with severe and mild tiple sclerosis, motor neuron disease, Huntingtons dis-
dysarthria, indicating that severity of dysarthria does not ease, etc.) or nonprogressive (as in stroke or traumatic
necessarily predict extent of perceived communicative dif- brain injury). Dysarthria is often categorized in percep-
ficulties. It is concluded that systematic subjective reports tually based types [23] and/or according to degree of
should always be included in the assessment of individuals dysarthria (mild, moderate or severe) [4].
with acquired dysarthria. Copyright 2007 S. Karger AG, Basel
190.163.205.234 - 12/29/2016 3:12:00 AM

2007 S. Karger AG, Basel Lena Hartelius


10217762/08/06010011$24.50/0 Sahlgrenska Academy at Gteborg University, Institute of Neuroscience and
Fax +41 61 306 12 34 Physiology, Division of Speech and Language Pathology
E-Mail karger@karger.ch Accessible online at: Hlsovetenskapligt Centrum, Box 452, SE405 30 Gteborg (Sweden)
www.karger.com www.karger.com/fpl Tel. +46 31 773 6884, Fax +46 31 82 34 15, E-Mail lena.hartelius@neuro.gu.se
Downloaded by:
In recent years, the World Health Organizations In- vironment might need specific instruction or support to
ternational Classification of Functioning, Disability and enhance intelligibility.
Health (ICF) [5] have been used as a conceptual frame- Lubinski [9] listed a number of probe questions for pa-
work and basis for classification of speech and language tients with dysarthria. The questions were designed to
disorders. The structure of ICF allows any health condi- elicit information about how the person defines the com-
tion to be classified in terms of body structure or func- munication problem and its impact on different aspects
tion, activity and participation, and any disability to be of his or her life. Questions concerning motivation to im-
classified as an impairment of body structure or func- prove were also included. A corresponding set of ques-
tion, activity limitation and participation restriction. tions was posed to family members concerning demands,
Also, the biopsychosocial model of ICF includes the im- resources and definition of the problem.
pact of contextual (environmental and personal) factors In 1997, Sullivan, Beukelman, and Gaebler developed
on activity and participation. Personal factors include an assessment protocol of communicative effectiveness
age, education, other health conditions, coping styles, etc. by social context. The effectiveness in each selected con-
Environmental factors include products, technology, text can be rated on a seven-point equal-appearing inter-
natural environment but also attitudes, support and rela- val scale. The protocol was first described in Yorkston et
tionships. Today, the classification has been applied to a al. [1] and has been used to examine the relationship be-
number of communicative disorders, but Yorkston et al. tween speech intelligibility and communication effec-
[6] applied the constructs to dysarthria as a chronic con- tiveness in 25 individuals with amyotrophic lateral scle-
dition as early as 1988. Considerable research efforts have rosis (ALS) and their primary listeners [10]. Results of
been put into the development of assessment methods to this study show that perceptions of communication ef-
capture the different aspects of communicative structur- fectiveness across the 10 different social situations were
al/functional impairment, activity limitation and partic- quite similar for individuals with ALS and their listeners.
ipation restriction caused by dysarthria. Impairment of It was also noted that there was a significant positive re-
structure and/or function is often more easily assessed lationship between intelligibility of speech and self-per-
and measured compared to the other levels. One example ceived ratings of communication effectiveness.
of a structural/functional symptom of dysarthria is velo- The three questionnaires mentioned above are intend-
pharyngeal insufficiency, causing hypernasality, which ed for use in daily clinical practice as a basis for clinical
can be measured using instrumental and clinical meth- intervention to enhance functional communication. In
ods. Communicative activity can be assessed in terms of order to get a more comprehensive picture of dysarthria
intelligibility, speaking rate, naturalness and depends in from the viewpoint of individuals with dysarthria,
part on inclusion of a second person, a listener. Restric- Yorkston and Bombardier [11] developed their Commu-
tions in communicative participation need to be evalu- nication Profile for Speakers with Motor Speech Disor-
ated primarily by the person with dysarthria, in order to ders. This self-report questionnaire has been used in a
reflect the true nature of the problem. The importance of number of research projects and consists of 100 state-
the insiders perspective in obtaining a correct and com- ments concerning perceived characteristics of the disor-
prehensive picture of limitations in communicative abil- der, situational difficulty, compensatory strategies used,
ities caused by dysarthria has been emphasized by and perceived reactions of others. The person is asked to
Yorkston et al. [7] and can be explored using both quali- agree or disagree with each statement, using a five-point
tative and quantitative methods, in interviews or self- scale. The questionnaire was used in a study of 33 indi-
report questionnaires. viduals with different types and degrees of dysarthria
A few attempts have been made, to describe subjective [12]. The only significant difference found between se-
communication problems using self-report question- verity groups was perceived reactions of others. Individu-
naires intended for individuals with dysarthria. Berry als with severe dysarthria felt that others were more help-
and Sanders [8] developed a Situation Intelligibility Sur- ful, more solicitous, and more punishing than did indi-
vey, an evaluation protocol intended for the patient and/ viduals with mild or moderate dysarthria.
or primary listeners in the patients environment. Up to Antonius, Beukelman and Reid used the Communica-
25 different situations can be scored according to how tion Profile in a study of 15 patients with Parkinsons dis-
often the patient has difficulty or is difficult to under- ease and their communication partners [13]. In this study,
stand in that situation. The protocol can help clinicians no significant differences between the individuals with
identify situations where the patient or others in the en- Parkinsons disease and their primary partners were
190.163.205.234 - 12/29/2016 3:12:00 AM

12 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2008;60:1119 Hartelius/Elmberg/Holm/Lvberg/


Nikolaidis
Downloaded by:
found for the areas of situational difficulty and perceived How do the overall ratings on the Living with Neuro-
reactions of others. However, differences were found in logically Based Speech Difficulties relate to age, gen-
reports of compensatory strategies used, with the dysar- der, diagnosis, disease duration, duration and degree
thric speakers reporting that they used improved preci- of dysarthria, education and degree of professional ac-
sion and partner instruction to a higher degree than re- tivity in this group?
ported by their partners. Also, the number of dysarthric Is the information gained from Living with Neuro-
characteristics endorsed was higher in the group of indi- logically Based Speech Difficulties comparable to in-
viduals with Parkinsons disease compared to their part- formation received from an equivalent but more ex-
ners reports. The questionnaire was also used to evaluate tensive instrument, the Communication Profile for
the results of group therapy with 6 patients with Parkin- Individuals with Motor Speech Disorders [12]?
sons disease [14]. Intervention focused on improving
speech intelligibility using functional communication
strategies and was evaluated with measures of intelligi- Methods
bility, perceptual assessments and communicative effec-
tiveness (including the Communication Profile). Five of Questionnaires
the 6 participants were considered to have improved their The self-report questionnaire Living with Neurologically
speech performance, but there seemed to be no apparent Based Speech Difficulties (or LwD) has been developed within the
Vrdal Institute and its research platform of chronically ill and
relationship between the benefit in terms of increased in- disabled young adults (www.vardalinstitutet.net). The aim is to
telligibility and self-reported communicative difficul- find out how these individuals perceive themselves and their
ties. speech difficulties, as well as how they adjust to their situation.
The Communication Profile for Speakers with Motor The selection of questionnaire items was based first of all on a
Speech Disorders has been translated into Swedish and careful review of published and unpublished scales and question-
naires directed towards individuals with speech and language
was used as one of the outcome measures to evaluate the disorders. Secondly, an interview study of 18 individuals with
treatment of a group of 15 patients with hypokinetic dys- multiple sclerosis, focusing on the experiences of communication
arthria and Parkinsons disease [15]. As a result of treat- and communicative barriers and strategies perceived in daily life
ment focusing on vocal efficiency and prosody, all per- with a long-standing chronic disease also generated ideas on im-
ceived situational difficulties were significantly reduced. portant issues to include [16].
Part A includes a number of general questions concerning age,
No differences in compensatory strategies, speech char- gender, type of disease or injury (stroke, neurological disease,
acteristics or reactions of others were noted after ther- traumatic brain injury or other), disease duration, duration of
apy. speech difficulties, type and extent of education and employment.
In summary, efforts have been made to develop assess- Part B comprises 50 statements divided into ten different sections.
ment methods to capture different aspects of communi- Section 1 includes statements regarding how communication is
influenced by problems related to speech, section 2 to language/
cative structural/functional impairment, activity limita- cognition and section 3 to fatigue. Sections 46 contain state-
tion and participation restriction caused by dysarthria. ments regarding how emotions, persons, and situations affect
Previous studies indicate that what the individual speak- communication. Section 7 concerns the roles in which the patient
er with dysarthria perceives as problematic might and feels restricted, as a family member, in social and professional
might not be related to speech intelligibility, severity of situations, etc. In section 8, the patient is encouraged to think
about what contributes to their communicative changes, whether
dysarthria and the perceptions of significant others. It is it is speech, language, cognition, memory, fatigue or physical dif-
obvious that the impact of dysarthria on a persons life is ficulties. In section 9, the patient is asked to define how commu-
subjective. Consequently, the insiders perspective is an nication is affected, whether they communicate less frequently,
essential addition to the assessment of dysarthria. The with increased difficulty or with the need for assistance from oth-
self-report questionnaire used in the present study, Liv- ers. Finally, section 10 contains statements that relate to different
strategies used to increase communicative function. The patient
ing with Neurologically Based Speech Difficulties, was is instructed to indicate how well each of the 50 statements applies
developed to capture the essence of individual problems to him or her at the present time by placing an X in one of six
without being too extensive. The questionnaire is intend- boxes labeled definitely false, mostly false, partly false, some-
ed for use in a clinical setting, and it was considered im- times true, mostly true, and definitely true.
In the present study, the scale (definitely falsedefinitely true)
portant that it could be administered in a reasonably
was subsequently transformed to correspond to 05 (where 0 in-
short time. The aim of the study was to describe the per- dicated definitely false and 5 definitely true regarding statements
ceived difficulties of 55 individuals with dysarthria. The indicating a communicative problem). All answers were added up
specific questions asked were: to a sum and divided by the number of answered items to give a
190.163.205.234 - 12/29/2016 3:12:00 AM

Self-Report Questionnaire for Dysarthria Folia Phoniatr Logop 2008;60:1119 13


Downloaded by:
Table 1. Subject characteristics (n = 55) Procedure
Thirty individuals were invited to participate in the study by
Age, years 55.7 8 12.4 (2775) a speech-language pathologist at the Department of Neurology at
Sex
Sahlgrenska University Hospital. They were all patients who had
Male 25 (45%) been in contact with the department because of their dysarthria
Female 30 (55%) during the last 5-year period. Seventeen individuals were partici-
pating in an ongoing study of dysarthria in Parkinson plus syn-
Diagnosis dromes and were invited to participate as a part of that study.
Multiple sclerosis 26 These 47 individuals received both questionnaires and an infor-
Parkinson plus syndrome 19
mation letter by mail and were contacted after a week by tele-
Parkinsons disease 5
Friedreichs ataxia 1 phone, to make sure that they had been able to understand and
Motor neuron disease 1 fill out the questionnaires correctly. An additional 8 individuals
Stroke 1 were consecutive visitors to the same Department during the time
Traumatic brain injury 1 when data from the other two groups were collected. They only
Unclear progressive condition 1 answered the first questionnaire, LwD, during the visit or at home
whichever they preferred.
Mean disease duration 163.68132.7 months (12480)
13.6 years (140) The 47 individuals who received both questionnaires an-
swered them at home and could fill out the two questionnaires in
Mean duration of speech difficulties 58.5854.3 months (3276) any order and take as much time as they needed. Any questions
4.9 years (0.2523) from the respondents were answered at the follow-up telephone
Mean time of education 154839.6 months (72228) call a week after the receipt of the questionnaires and they were
12.8 years (619) then asked to send in the forms. Both questionnaires were an-
swered on paper copies. If a motor disability prevented the par-
Degree of professional activity1
ticipants to use a pen to fill out the form, they were instructed to
Not working 30 (55%)
Working part-time 11 (20%)
use a significant other as assistant. As part of the general ques-
Working full-time 7 (13%) tions included in part A, the participant was asked the following:
This questionnaire was filled out by: ( ) me alone, ( ) me with help
Degree of dysarthria2 from someone else. Four participants had received help to fill out
Mild 30 (55%) the questionnaires.
Moderate 16 (29%) Severity of dysarthria was rated on a coarse scale ranging from
Severe 8 (15%)
none, mild, moderate to severe. Dysarthria was considered mild
1 Missing data from 7 participants. when there was a detectable speech disturbance that did not affect
2 Missing data from 1 participant. intelligibility. Moderate dysarthria was distinguished by a marked
reduction in speech intelligibility. Severe dysarthria, finally, was
considered when the use of augmentative or alternative commu-
nication was needed. These definitions are in accordance with
Yorkston et al. [4]. Severity was rated during conversation with the
mean of perceived communicative problems caused by the neu- participants, either during their visit or over the telephone by the
rogenic speech disorder. Different means for the ten different sec- speech-language pathologist at the Department of Neurology.
tions were also calculated.
A translated version of the Communication Profile for Indi- Participants
viduals with Dysarthria (the Communication Profile) was used Subject characteristics are presented in table 1. The partici-
to validate LwD. The Communication Profile is a 100-item self- pants were 25 men and 30 women with a mean age of 55.7 years
report questionnaire developed by Yorkston and Bombardier [11] with a standard deviation of 12.4 and a range of 2775 years. Di-
to solicit information in four different areas: characteristics, i.e. agnoses were almost exclusively progressive neurological disor-
the patients perception of the features of the dysarthria, per- ders: 26 individuals had multiple sclerosis, 19 had a Parkinson
ceived situational difficulties (rated according to six dimensions plus syndrome, and 5 had Parkinsons disease. Mean disease du-
assumed to influence ease of communication: partner familiarity, ration was 13.6 years with a range of 140 years. Mean duration
size of audience, demand for intelligibility, demand for speed, of speech difficulties was 4.9 years with a range of 3 months to 23
emotional load and environmental adversity), compensatory years. Mean number of years in education was 12.8 years with a
strategies used (classified as improved production, environmental range of 619 years. Seven individuals were working full-time, 11
modification, avoidance, message modification or partner in- part-time and 30 were not working (due to retirement or sick-
struction) and perceived reactions of others (categorized as help- leave). Eight individuals were considered to have severe dysar-
ful, solicitous or punishing). The patient is instructed to endorse thria, while 16 had moderate and 30 mild dysarthria. Reports on
each statement by indicating strongly agree, agree, neutral, degree of professional activity were missing for 7 individuals and
disagree, strongly disagree or does not apply. To facilitate degree of dysarthria for 1 individual.
comparison with LwD, the scale was transformed to 04 (where The individuals were not selected in order to comprise a ho-
0 indicated strongly disagreeing and 4 strongly agreeing to state- mogeneous group. As a broad selection of diagnoses, dysarthria
ments indicating a communicative problem) and all items an- types and severity was desirable, the group consisted of all the
swered does not apply were excluded. patients that were in contact with the clinic at the time of study.
190.163.205.234 - 12/29/2016 3:12:00 AM

14 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2008;60:1119 Hartelius/Elmberg/Holm/Lvberg/


Nikolaidis
Downloaded by:
Statistical Methods munication problems related primarily to speech were
Results were analyzed first of all in terms of percentage indi- more commonly endorsed than language/cognition- and
viduals indicating mostly true or definitely true (4 or 5) regard-
ing all statements included in LwD. In addition, means, ranges fatigue-related problems. Emotions were reported to have
and standard deviations for the ten sections and the entire ques- effects on communication more often than different situ-
tionnaire were calculated. In order to explore relationships with ations and persons. Variability was generally high, as in-
background factors such as age, gender, diagnosis, disease dura- dicated by the large standard deviations.
tion, duration and degree of dysarthria, education and employ-
ment status, nonparametric statistics were used (Spearmans rank
correlation coefficient, Mann-Whitney U test, Kruskal-Wallis Association with Other Background Factors
one-way ANOVA test). Finally, Spearmans rank correlation coef- No significant correlations were found between mean
ficient was used to assess degree of association between subscores scores and section scores on LwD, and age or disease du-
on LwD and the Communication Profile. ration. No significant differences were found between
men and women or between degrees of professional ac-
tivity. Possible differences between diagnoses were ex-
Results plored using Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA compar-
ing the larger three groups (multiple sclerosis, Parkinson
Table 2 presents all statements included in the ques- plus syndrome and Parkinsons disease) and were found
tionnaire LwD and the percentage of individuals who in- to be nonsignificant. There was a moderate but signifi-
dicated mostly true or definitely true (45). Nine state- cant positive correlation between mean score on LwD
ments were endorsed by more than 45% of the individu- and reported duration of dysarthria (rs = 0.297, p ^ 0.05),
als. They were (in descending order of magnitude): The and a negative correlation with number of years in educa-
speech difficulties themselves (rather than language, tion (rs = 0.366, p ^ 0.01). Mean scores on LwD for the
cognition and/or physical difficulties) contribute to the three subgroups mild (n = 30), moderate (n = 16), and se-
communication problems (61.9%), My speech difficul- vere (n = 8) dysarthria are shown in figure 1. The group
ties negatively affect my self-image (52.7%), My difficul- with moderate dysarthria has a higher score, indicating
ties in communicating affect my possibilities to actively more communication problems, but the differences be-
take part in work and studies in the way I would want to tween the groups are not significant (Kruskal-Wallis one-
(52.7%), Its difficult to talk in a group of people that I way ANOVA test, p = 0.12).
dont know (50.9%), My speech difficulties get worse
when Im angry or sad (49.1%), I often need to repeat Association with Scores on the Communication Profile
what Ive said because people dont understand me Although the scales were different (LwD had a range
(47.3%), My difficulties in communicating affect my pos- of 05 and the Communication Profile 04), the correla-
sibilities to express my personality in the way I would tion was relatively high (rs = 0.683, p ^ 0.01). In addition,
want to (47.3%), I make only short remarks in discus- five different statements from three different sections
sions or conversations (45.5%), My speech is slow were selected, matched and correlated with correspond-
(45.4%). Eight statements were endorsed by 4045% of ing statements from the Communication Profile (all from
the individuals. They were: I dont communicate in the the section comprising statements on situational difficul-
way that I would want to (43.7%), I communicate like I ties). Results are presented in table 4. Correlations are
want to, but not as much or as often as I would like generally moderate to high (0.40.7) and significant.
(43.7%), Fatigue contributes to the communication prob-
lems (43.6%), I dont speak if I think that itll be hard to
make myself understood (43.6%), I worry about my Discussion
speech difficulties (43.4%), I avoid situations where Im
expected to talk (41.8%), I avoid deep or complicated In summary, the results of this questionnaire study
discussions or conversations (40.0%) and I communi- including 55 individuals with acquired dysarthria indi-
cate like I want to, but its difficult (40.0%). cated that both degree and type of subjectively perceived
Group means, ranges and standard deviations for the communicative difficulties were varying. Degree of com-
ten different sections and the entire questionnaire are municative difficulties was not related to age, gender, di-
presented in table 3. The possible range was 05, and a agnosis, disease duration or degree of professional activ-
higher mean indicates more severe communication prob- ity in this group.
lems. In this group of individuals with dysarthria, com-
190.163.205.234 - 12/29/2016 3:12:00 AM

Self-Report Questionnaire for Dysarthria Folia Phoniatr Logop 2008;60:1119 15


Downloaded by:
Table 2. Percentage of individuals indicating mostly true or definitely true (45) regarding all statements (n = 55)

Section Statement %

1 Communication problems I often run out of air when I talk 30.9


related primarily to speech I often sound hoarse 29.1
My speech is slow 45.4
My speech is slurred 32.8
I often need to repeat what Ive said because people dont understand me 47.3
2 Communication problems I have difficulty finding words when I speak 6.4
related primarily to language/ My speech is not very complex (e.g. short sentences, simple grammar) 29.1
cognition I need to think about what Im saying and how Im saying it when I speak 29.1
Understanding new information takes a long time 12.7
I dont always understand what people say to me 5.4
3 Communication problems I rarely start a conversation 16.3
related primarily to fatigue I avoid deep or complicated discussions or conversations 40.0
I avoid situations where Im expected to talk 41.8
I make only short remarks in discussions or conversations 45.5
I cannot manage to concentrate enough to follow what is being said 18.2
4 Effects of emotions My manner of communicating varies in a way thats difficult for me to predict 32.7
My speech difficulties get worse when Im angry or sad 49.1
My speech difficulties negatively affect my self-image 52.7
My mood affects how I interact with others and how I communicate 27.2
I worry about my speech difficulties 43.4
5 Effects of different persons Im treated differently by people that I communicate with 18.2
Its difficult to communicate with members of my family 7.3
Its difficult to communicate with relatives and friends 14.5
Its difficult to communicate with people I know, at work or in stores 16.4
Its difficult to communicate with people that Ive never met before 36.3
6 Effects of different situations Its difficult to talk with one or two people at home 14.6
Its difficult to talk when we have friends visiting 20.0
Its difficult to talk on the telephone 38.2
Its difficult to talk in a group of people that I dont know 50.9
Its difficult to talk about emotional things 23.7
7 My difficulties in commu- express basic need (get attention, express feelings, etc.) 25.4
nicating affect my express basic need (get attention, express feelings, etc.) 25.4
possibilities to exercise my role as a family member in the way I would want to 34.5
take part in social gatherings with relatives and friends in the way I would want to 34.5
actively take part in work and studies in the way I would want to 52.7
express my personality in the way I would want to 47.3
8 What do you think contributes The speech difficulties themselves 61.9
to the changes in the way you Difficulties with the language 9.1
communicate? Difficulties in thinking, remembering and concentrating 20.0
Fatigue 43.6
Physical difficulties 34.5
9 Communicating like I would I dont communicate in the way I would want to 43.7
want to: I communicate like I want to, but not as much or as often as I would like 43.7
I communicate like I want to, but its difficult 40.0
I communicate like I want to, but listeners often fill in words or try to help out 25.5
I have to rely on others to be able to communicate like I want to 20.0
10 How do you perceive changes I believe that my speech can be changed 36.3
and the possibility to alter your I explain my communication difficulties to other people 32.7
way of speaking? I try to express myself in another way when Im not understood 27.2
I take a break and rest a little when I notice that Im not being understood 27.3
I dont speak if I think that itll be hard to make myself understood 43.6
190.163.205.234 - 12/29/2016 3:12:00 AM

16 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2008;60:1119 Hartelius/Elmberg/Holm/Lvberg/


Nikolaidis
Downloaded by:
Table 3. Means, ranges and SDs for the ten sections of and the entire questionnaire LwD

Section Mean Range SD

1 Communication problems related primarily to speech 2.7 0.24.8 1.0


2 Communication problems related primarily to language/cognition 1.8 04.6 1.1
3 Communication problems related primarily to fatigue 2.3 05 1.5
4 Effects of emotions 2.9 05 1.2
5 Effects of different persons 2.0 04.4 1.3
6 Effects of different situations 2.4 05 1.3
7 Role restriction 2.8 05 1.4
8 Causing factors 2.5 0.65 1.0
9 Type of restriction 2.7 04.8 1.1
10 Strategies 2.5 04.6 1.2
All sections/the entire questionnaire 2.4 0.64.4 0.9

Possible range 05 (0 = definitely false, 1 = mostly false, 2 = partly false, 3 = sometimes true, 4 = mostly true,
5 = definitely true), higher mean indicated more pronounced problems (n = 55).

Table 4. Correlations (Spearmans rank correlation) between six pairs of statements from each of the two
questionnaires LwD and Communication Profile (ComPro)

Section Statement LwD Statement ComPro Correlation

Effects of emotions My speech difficulties get Its difficult for me when I am rs = 0.473
worse when Im angry or sad upset and try to get a point across p 0.001
Its difficult to talk about Its difficult for me when I am rs = 0.693
emotional things talking with a close friend about p 0.001
emotional issues
Effects of different Its difficult to communicate with Its difficult for me when I am rs = 0.584
persons members of my family talking with my family after dinner p 0.001
Its difficult to communicate with Its difficult for me when I am rs = 0.590
relatives and friends talking with a friend or family p 0.001
member in a quiet room
Effects of different Its difficult to talk on the Its difficult for me when I am rs = 0.555
situations telephone in a quiet room at home talking p 0.001
on the telephone

Generally, the overriding problems (defined as the derstandings. This is in accordance with Yorkston et al.
statements endorsed by the largest number of partici- [12], where 5080% of items related to situational diffi-
pants) were related to a negative self-image and restric- culties were endorsed by the individuals with dysarthria.
tions in communicative participation, i.e. possibilities to Emotional load and demand for intelligibility and speed,
actively take part in work and studies and to express their as well as general environmental adversity, were the situ-
personality were affected. Communication was also af- ational dimensions more frequently reported as difficult.
fected by emotions and by the number and familiarity of Size of the audience and partner familiarity was some-
people present in communicative encounters. The domi- what less influential, but the difference was not signifi-
nating speech difficulties were related to decreased speech cant. Similar results were described by Hartelius et al.
rate and a need for repetition as a consequence of misun- [15], where the most frequently perceived situational dif-
190.163.205.234 - 12/29/2016 3:12:00 AM

Self-Report Questionnaire for Dysarthria Folia Phoniatr Logop 2008;60:1119 17


Downloaded by:
the LwD mean of the group with moderate dysarthria was
4.0 slightly higher than in the severe and the mild groups, al-
3.5 though the difference was not statistically significant.
3.0
2.86 Several authors have stressed the importance of exploring
2.56 the relationship between severity of dysarthria and per-
2.5
2.2 ceived communicative difficulties, or in ICF terminology,
2.0 the relationship of functional impairment to activity lim-
1.5 itation and participation restriction, including Hustad et
1.0
al. [17] and Yorkston et al. [12]. The anticipated finding
would be that the individuals with severe dysarthria per-
0.5
ceive their difficulties as greater than the individuals with
0 moderate and mild dysarthria. However, Hustad et al. [17]
Mild dysarthria Moderate dysarthria Severe dysarthria
emphasize that the relationship between the different lev-
els (dysarthric impairment vs. communicative activity
Fig. 1. Mean scores on LwD for the three subgroups mild (n = 30), limitation and participation restriction) is not likely to be
moderate (n = 16), and severe (n = 8) dysarthria. Differences are linear and that it is important to assess each level sepa-
not significant (Kruskal-Wallis one-way ANOVA, p = 0.12). rately. Severity of dysarthria does not necessarily predict
extent of communicative participation. Yorkston et al.
[12] investigated whether speakers with mild, moderate,
and severe dysarthria differed in perceived speech char-
ficulty was related to the size of the audience. Antonius acteristics, situational difficulties, compensatory strate-
et al. [13] reported means between 53 and 58% of items gies and reactions of others. They found a significant dif-
related to situational difficulties endorsed by dysarthric ference between severity groups only in perceived reac-
participants. Statements relating to sense of effort in tions of others, in that individuals with severe dysarthria
communicating, fatigue, avoidance and worry were also felt that partners were more influential in communication
frequently endorsed. Similar findings were reported by a exchanges than individuals with mild or moderate dysar-
group of individuals with multiple sclerosis [16]. thria. The authors were somewhat surprised by the lack of
Slow rate is a distinctive feature of several types of dys- differences among the severity groups, concerning other
arthria, including spastic, ataxic, hyperkinetic and the aspects (number of speech characteristics endorsed, type
mixed dysarthrias associated with multiple sclerosis, and frequency of situations felt to be difficult, and the
ALS and the Parkinson plus syndromes [3]. It is also an number of compensatory strategies found to be benefi-
early symptom, frequently used as a compensatory mech- cial). The authors concluded that asking questions about
anism. It is therefore not surprising that slowness of perceived reactions of others might be a reasonable means
speech was the most frequently reported speech-related of assessing the degree of handicap associated with dysar-
communication problem. thria. In the present study, although not statistically sig-
The fact that age, gender, diagnosis, or disease dura- nificant, the group with moderate dysarthria reported
tion had no significant impact on perceived communica- more communicative difficulties than the other two se-
tive difficulties highlights the fact that these difficulties verity groups. A cautious interpretation of this finding is
are indeed subjective and cannot be predicted on the ba- that an individual with moderate dysarthria might be
sis of simple background factors. Furthermore, the fact maintaining a professional and social life that is more
that progression of speech deterioration in different de- communicatively demanding than a person with severe
generative diseases varies considerably might contribute dysarthria is. Severity of speech disorder in neurological
to the lack of correlation between disease duration and disease also frequently mirrors severity of disease symp-
degree of perceived communicative difficulties. For ex- toms in general, and an individual more seriously affected
ample, in Parkinson plus syndromes a speech distur- by a progressive disease might have other more pro-
bance can be one of the first signs, while individuals with nounced symptoms to handle, as well as a more restricted
Parkinsons disease usually develop speech symptoms lifestyle. In conclusion, there seems to be no unequivocal
late in the disease process. relationship between severity of dysarthria and perceived
Severity of dysarthria had no statistically significant communicative difficulties, and the issue calls for further
effect on perceived communicative difficulties. However, exploration.
190.163.205.234 - 12/29/2016 3:12:00 AM

18 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2008;60:1119 Hartelius/Elmberg/Holm/Lvberg/


Nikolaidis
Downloaded by:
The negative correlation between degree of perceived sarily reflections of severity of speech symptoms, and
communicative problems and number of years in educa- systematic protocols are needed to help identify problems
tion would seem to indicate that fewer years in education that need addressing in intervention. Systematic subjec-
imply more communicative problems in individuals with tive reports should always be included in the assessment
dysarthria. However, the correlation was moderate and of individuals with acquired dysarthria.
the number of participants relatively small, thus no con- The present study suffers from a number of limitations
clusions should be drawn. It has, in fact, been shown that that call for wariness in interpretation of results. The
physical functioning and perceived health increase sig- classification of severity of dysarthria was crude and only
nificantly with years of formal education [18]. done by one clinician. Although the selection of ques-
It is important that assessment protocols including tionnaire items was based on published scales and inter-
subjectively perceived communication problems should view data and appeared to have good ecological validity,
not be too extensive and time-consuming. One of the a few statements turned out to be problematic in the sense
aims of the present study was to establish whether the in- that they were cognitively demanding or could be related
formation gained from the present questionnaire was to several different factors. Statistical handling was lim-
comparable to information from the more extensive ited and intrapatient reliability data were not gathered.
Communication Profile. Judging from correlations be- However, as a result of the present study, the question-
tween the two different protocols in their entirety, as well naire has been revised and is presently being used in an
as between individual statements, the correspondence investigation of self-perceived communicative difficul-
was relatively good. It seems that a shorter protocol might ties in relation to dysarthria test scores and intelligibility
be able to capture the same type of perceived difficul- in a larger group of individuals. These individuals will be
ties. followed longitudinally to see how degree of dysarthria,
The fact that communicative difficulties are perceived intelligibility and self-perceived difficulties related to
very differently indicates the need for individualized communicative function, activity and participation de-
treatment planning. Subjective difficulties are not neces- velop over time in degenerative neurological conditions.

References

1 Yorkston KM, Beukelman DR, Strand EA, 9 Lubinski R: Dysarthria: A breakdown in 14 Sullivan MD, Brune PJ, Beukelman DR:
Bell KR: Management of Motor Speech Dis- interpersonal communication; in Vogel D, Maintenance of speech changes following
orders in Children and Adults. Austin, Pro- Cannito, MP (eds): Treating Disordered group treatment for hypokinetic dysarthria
Ed, 1999. Speech Motor Control. Austin, Pro-Ed, of Parkinsons disease; in Robin DA, Yorkston
2 Darley FL, Aronson AE, Brown JR: Motor 1991. KM, Beukelman DR (eds): Disorders of Mo-
Speech Disorders. Philadelphia, WB Saun- 10 Ball LJ, Beukelman DR, Pattee GL: Commu- tor Speech: Assessment, Treatment, and
ders, 1975. nication effectiveness of individuals with Clinical Characteristics. Baltimore, Brookes
3 Duffy JR: Motor Speech Disorders: Sub- amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. J Commun Publishing, 1996.
strates, Differential Diagnosis, and Manage- Disord 2004;37:197215. 15 Hartelius L, Lillvik M, Wising C: Communi-
ment. St. Louis, Elsevier Mosby, 2005. 11 Yorkston KM, Bombardier C: The Commu- cative disability before and after therapy in a
4 Yorkston KM, Miller RM, Strand, EA: Man- nication Profile for Speakers with Motor Parkinsons group; Am Speech Hear Assoc
agement of Speech and Swallowing in De- Speech Disorders. Unpublished Question- Conf Boston, 1997.
generative Diseases. Austin, Pro-Ed, 2004. naire. Seattle, University of Washington, 16 Bringfelt P-A, Hartelius L, Runmarker B:
5 ICF International Classification of Func- 1992. Communication problems and multiple
tioning, Disability and Health. http://www3. 12 Yorkston KM, Bombardier C, Hammen VL: sclerosis: 9-year follow-up. Int J MS Care
who.int/icf/icftemplate.cfm, 2003. Dysarthria from the viewpoint of individu- 2006;8:130140.
6 Yorkston KM, Beukelman DR, Bell KR: als with dysarthria; in Till JA, Yorkston KM, 17 Hustad K, Beukelman DR, Yorkston KM:
Clinical Management of Dysarthric Speak- Beukelman DR (eds): Motor Speech Disor- Functional outcome assessment in dysar-
ers. Boston, College-Hill Press, 1988. ders: Advances in Assessment and Treat- thria. Semin Speech Lang 1998;19:291302.
7 Yorkston KM, Klasner ER, Swanson KM: ment. Baltimore, Brookes Publishing, 1994. 18 Ross CE, Mirowsky J: Refining the associa-
Communication in context: a qualitative 13 Antonius K, Beukelman DR, Reid R: Com- tion between education and health: the ef-
study of the experiences of individuals with munication disability of Parkinsons disease: fects of quantity, credential, and selectivity.
multiple sclerosis. Am J Speech Lang Pathol perceptions of dysarthric speakers and their Demography 1999;36:445460.
2001;10:126137. primary communication partners; in Robin
8 Berry WR, Sanders SB: Environmental edu- DA, Yorkston KM, Beukelman DR (eds):
cation: the universal management approach Disorders of Motor Speech: Assessment,
for adults with dysarthria; in Berry WR (ed): Treatment, and Clinical Characteristics.
Clinical Dysarthria. San Diego, College-Hill Baltimore, Brookes Publishing, 1996.
Press, 1983.
190.163.205.234 - 12/29/2016 3:12:00 AM

Self-Report Questionnaire for Dysarthria Folia Phoniatr Logop 2008;60:1119 19


Downloaded by:

Potrebbero piacerti anche