Sei sulla pagina 1di 15

18715-AAD159.

5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 433

Twitchell, P., Morford, J. P., & Hauser, P. C. (2015). Effects of SES on literacy development of deaf signing bilinguals.
American Annals of the Deaf, 159(5), 433446.

EFFECTS OF SES ON LITERACY DEVELOPMENT


OF DEAF SIGNING BILINGUALS

R
ESEARCH ON spoken-language monolinguals and bilinguals has shown
that socioeconomic status significantly affects literacy outcomes. One
explanation for this effect is that children in higher-SES homes have bet-
ter oral proficiency in the language of literacy instruction (Hoff, 2013;
Zhang et al., 2013). American Sign LanguageEnglish deaf bilingual chil-
dren exhibit a unique profile because they achieve L2 literacy without
prior development of spoken L2 proficiency. This provides an opportu-
nity to evaluate the role of SES in literacy development without the con-
founding effect of the influence of spoken-language proficiency. The
present study evaluated effects of SES and ASL proficiency on 135 deaf
signing bilinguals English reading proficiency. Although SES and ASL
proficiency were not correlated in this sample, both factors were signif-
icant predictors of reading proficiency. The implications of these find-
ings for educational reform in schools with deaf students are discussed.
PAUL TWITCHELL, JILL P.
MORFORD, AND PETER C. Keywords: SES, literacy develop- drens access to media that do not
HAUSER ment, sign language, deaf, bilingual encourage literacy development, such
as television and video games (Raag
TWITCHELL IS A GRADUATE INTERN AT Research on spoken-language mono- et al., 2011). Children from high-SES
DR. LAURA-ANN PETITTOS BRAIN AND linguals and bilinguals has repeatedly homes have more reading opportuni-
LANGUAGE LABORATORY FOR NEUROIMAGING shown that socioeconomic status (SES) ties and more books, and their parents
(BL2) IN THE NSF SCIENCE OF LEARNING has a significant impact on literacy are highly skilled at gauging their chil-
CENTER ON VISUAL LANGUAGE AND VISUAL outcomes (Droop & Verhoeven, 2003; drens literacy level and mediating liter-
LEARNING (VL2), AND A PHD STUDENT, Ransdell, 2012; White, 1982). Two com- acy events. Thus, the home literacy
PROGRAM IN EDUCATIONAL NEUROSCIENCE, peting explanations for this relation- environment and the amount of time
GALLAUDET UNIVERSITY, WASHINGTON, DC. ship involve the direct versus indirect children spend in literacy activities are
MORFORD IS PROFESSOR, DEPARTMENT OF impacts of SES on literacy outcomes. one explanation for the impact of SES
LINGUISTICS, UNIVERSITY OF NEW MEXICO, SES may directly affect literacy out- on literacy development (Korat, Arafat,
ALBUQUERQUE, AND AN AFFILIATED comes through characteristics of the Aram, & Klein, 2013). A second expla-
RESEARCHER, VL2. HAUSER IS A SCIENCE home literacy environment. Because nation proposes an indirect relation-
MENTORSHIP LEADER, VL2, AND PROFESSOR, higher-SES parents read more, they are ship because SES has been shown to
NATIONAL TECHNICAL INSTITUTE FOR THE DEAF, strong role models for their children in influence first-language (L1) and sec-
ROCHESTER INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY, regard to acquiring reading skills. They ond-language (L2) oral proficiency,
ROCHESTER, NY. are also more likely to restrict their chil- which affects literacy development

433

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 434

EFFECTS OF SES ON SIGNING BILINGUALS LITERACY

(Hoff, 2013; Zhang et al., 2013). Specif- comprehension of ASL-English deaf A second possibility, however, is that
ically, higher-SES parents speak to their bilingual children (ASL-English bilin- only L1 proficiency predicts reading
children more (Hart & Risley, 1995) guals) while also considering L1 profi- outcomes for deaf readers. This would
and use fewer directives in child-rear- ciency in ASL. Despite the numerous signal that the demographic context of
ing (Murray, Fees, Crowe, Murphy, & previous studies on SES, L1 proficiency, learning to read differs for deaf and
Henriksen, 2006). These characteris- and literacy skills with both monolin- hearing children, but that the actual
tics are associated with high-SES chil- gual and bilingual populations, there driver of reading achievement is pri-
drens faster rate of spoken-language have been no studies of the effect of marily linguistic in nature, and not
development, and higher levels of SES on literacy skills of ASL-English related to the home literacy environ-
phonological and metalinguistic bilinguals. Several early studies of liter- ment. The reverse pattern, showing
awareness. This second explanation acy outcomes of deaf children found effects of SES but not of L1 proficiency,
suggests that SES indirectly affects both effects and noneffects of parental is not anticipated given the wealth of
reading by promoting language skills hearing and communication choices, data linking L1 proficiency to reading
that mediate reading success. and pointed out that SES is not equiv- achievement in both hearing popula-
Because SES and L1/L2 proficiency alent in these groups (Corson, 1973; tions (e.g., Dickinson & Snow, 1987)
are confounded in research on spo- Vernon & Koh, 1970). Indeed, Vernon and those that are deaf (e.g., Cham-
ken-language readers, it is difficult to and Koh (1970) were among the first berlain & Mayberry, 2008). A final and
discern the relationship among SES, investigators to provide solid evidence intriguing possibility is that both SES
L1/L2 proficiency, and reading. Signing that early manual communication and L1 proficiency can predict reading
bilingual readers can provide an op- produced better overall educational comprehension abilities in deaf signers
portunity to look more closely and achievement, including superiority in even if they are not correlated with
more deeply at that relationship. reading skills and written language (p. each other. This finding would again
American Sign LanguageEnglish deaf 535), compared to early oral communi- point to differences in the demo-
bilingual children exhibit a unique pro- cation. More recent studies have pro- graphic context of learning to read for
file because they achieve L2 literacy vided in-depth analyses of effects of deaf signers relative to hearing read-
without prior development of spoken ASL on reading development in deaf ers, but would provide the first fully
L2 proficiency. (The term deaf is oper- individuals (Chamberlain & Mayberry, unambiguous evidence that SES and
ationally defined in the present article 2008; Hoffmeister, 2000; Padden & L1 proficiency make unique contribu-
as referring to a hearing loss of 85 dB Ramsey, 2000; Strong & Prinz, 1997). tions to reading outcomes. Before
or greater in the better ear.) Further, Evaluation of SES in greater detail, describing our own study, we first sum-
there is no widely used orthography including consideration of its effects marize a selection of the vast literature
for ASL that would make it possible for independently of parental hearing sta- on SES effects in hearing monolingual
L1 orthographic experience to have tus, is long overdue in the research on and bilingual populations.
an impact on L2 literacy. In response to reading development in deaf children.
this unique configuration of profi- Investigating whether SES affects Monolinguals: Impact of
ciency in a signed language for face-to- reading development in deaf signers SES on the Development
face communication and in the written can also help researchers address of Language Skills
form of a spoken language for reading whether deaf readers reading devel- The role of SES is a complicated and
and writing, Morford, Kroll, Piar, and opment is qualitatively different from dynamic factor that contributes to
Wilkinson (2014) have coined a term that of hearing readers. L1 proficiency both language and literacy develop-
to refer to this unique population of and SES both predict reading achieve- ment. In order to understand how SES
individuals who use both ASL and ment in hearing monolingual and bilin- specifically affects the developmental
printed English: sign-print bilinguals. gual readers, but, importantly, these stages of childrens L1, it is important
Sign-print bilingualism affords the two predictor variables are strongly to first define this construct. Family
unique opportunity to evaluate the correlated for these populations. If the SES is a measure that includes multiple
role of SES without the confound of same were true for deaf sign-print factors that may influence an individual
spoken-language proficiency influenc- bilinguals, that would signal qualitative childs growth. For example, the
ing literacy development. similarities in reading development Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of SES
The present study investigated across all groups, and the conundrum includes marital status,
whether family SES influences reading of SES effects would remain a mystery. retired/employed status, educational

434

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 435

attainment, and occupational prestige African American low-SES families in Monolinguals: Impact of
(Hollingshead, 1975). The constella- which the mothers had only 11 years SES on the Development
tion of these four factors predicts a of education, on average, to see of Literacy Skills
variety of behaviors, including the lan- whether differences in the childrens In addition to assertions about the
guage use and child-rearing practices vocabulary development could be effects of SES on early language devel-
of parents, allowing us to explore the related to the mothers input. Shimpi opment, there are also claims in the
relationship of SES and L1 proficiency et al. not only found that a correlation literature that SES contributes more
in literacy outcomes. existed between the amount of mater- directly to literacy development. While
There is broad consensus that SES nal speech input and child language the present study focuses on family
affects L1 proficiency. Hoff (2003, outcomes in these families, but that SES, the literature on SES and literacy
2006) has argued convincingly that the consistency of input affected lan- has included studies evaluating effects
these impacts are largely a factor of the guage outcomes as well. Hurtado, of family SES, as well as studies focused
quantity and complexity of maternal Marchman, and Fernald (2008) also on school and peer SES.
language input, even across different specifically tested whether maternal Beals and De Temple (1993) evalu-
cultures. For example, Hoff (2003) input influenced vocabulary develop- ated a combination of home social and
compared toddlers from 33 high-SES ment in low-SES families. They economic measures, family conversa-
families with 30 from mid-SES families recorded 27 children and their moth- tion measures, and child language
who were just beginning to form two- ers interacting in Spanish when each measures to determine the best pre-
word utterances. Mother-child natura- child was 18 months old and again dictors of early literacy success in low-
listic interaction was recorded at two when each child was 24 months old. income families. Beals and De Temple
points in time so that the childrens The amount of maternal utterances visited a total of 28 families, focusing
rate of vocabulary learning could be and each childs vocabulary size were on one parent (the mother) and the
compared to characteristics of mater- coded and assessed. Hurtado et al. child, and excluding other adults in the
nal language across the two groups. found that children who experienced home such as fathers, stepfathers, and
Hoff found that the high-SES mothers more input from their mothers had grandparents. Participants varied in
produced more word types and tokens larger vocabulary outputs at 24 months cultural and economic background.
as well as more complex utterances as than children who had less maternal Two home visits were made, one when
measured by mean length of utterance input. The overall results show that the the child was 3 years old and the sec-
than the mid-SES mothers. Each of quality and quantity of mothers input ond when the child was 4, in order to
these measures was also a significant predicts their childrens language collect measures of the home literacy
predictor of childrens rate of vocabu- understanding and vocabulary growth environment. During the home visit,
lary growth. Using multiple regression, within a low-SES environment. Hoff recordings were made of the interac-
Hoff then evaluated what proportion (2006) reviewed many studies investi- tion while the mother and child read
of variance in child vocabulary devel- gating environmental effects on lan- two books together. Mothers were also
opment could be attributed to SES guage development and concluded asked to record mealtime sessions
once the variance associated with child that variability in the rate and course of with their child. Recordings of mother-
vocabulary size at study onset and childrens language development is child book sharing and the mealtime
maternal language complexity had clearly linked to variation in opportu- conversations were subsequently ana-
been removed. The results showed nities to have access to language in the lyzed for both quantitative and quali-
that the association between SES and environment. In sum, the findings that tative measures of home language
child vocabulary disappeared once the have been documented in multiple usage, such as the use of immediate
mediating factor of maternal language languages show that mother-to-child versus displaced reference, content
was included in the model. speech, including the amount of words of child responses to questions, and
The proposal that SES effects on and utterances as well as the grammat- frequency of narrative and explana-
primary language development are ical complexity of language, has an tory talk. At the end of their kinder-
mediated by maternal language input impact on the rate of childrens devel- garten year, the children completed a
is supported by other findings that opment of vocabulary and language series of standard tests of linguistic,
show variation in language develop- overall. Oral language skills, in turn, are cognitive, and reading comprehen-
ment within a single SES level. Shimpi, a primary predictor of literacy develop- sion skills. Beals and De Temple found
Fedewa, and Hans (2012) studied ment (Dickinson & Snow, 1987). that family SES and overall childrens

435

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 436

EFFECTS OF SES ON SIGNING BILINGUALS LITERACY

language production best predicted of considering explanatory variables at from the effects of SES on oral lan-
literacy success. They also concluded all levels. Chiu and McBride-Chang guage proficiency is to look at child
that a mothers ability to mediate her concluded that, across nearly all coun- bilinguals. General research on bilin-
childs involvement in conversation at tries, literacy achievement is influenced gualism has shown that bilinguals are
home during the literacy development not only by personal family but by not like two monolinguals in one per-
stages affected the childs develop- school SES and peers SES within a son. It proves a very difficult task to
ment by transferring the responsibil- school. They suggested that further master each language at the same
ity of conversation from the mother to research be done to address the level attained by monolingual speak-
the child. specific mechanisms that explain why ers of the respective languages (Negro
While children may begin learning schools provide literacy success beyond & Genelot, 2012). Bilingualism re-
to read in the home, most make con- the individual and family characteristics searchers have examined (a) how L1
siderable gains in literacy development of the student. At the level of the fam- minority language proficiency affects
in school. Thus, in addition to evaluat- ily, number of books in the home medi- majority L2 language and literacy; (b)
ing the impact of family SES on read- ated the SES effect, whereas at the level the impact of different SES levels on
ing, it is important to consider the of the school, peers family SES was a childrens acquisition of L2 majority
possibility that school and country more influential factor. There is abun- language and literacy proficiency, and
SES may also affect literacy. Chiu and dant evidence that SES, in general, plays (c) the effects of bilingual education
McBride-Chang (2006) investigated an important role in a monolingual relative to monolingual approaches.
whether different types of SES can childs literacy development stages; One of the biggest questions in
have a crucial role in literacy develop- however, there is a need to detect bilingual education is What method of
ment. The definition of SES that Chiu whether SES directly affects literacy suc- teaching language and literacy to
and McBride-Chang used considers cess, or whether SES is merely a proxy bilinguals is most effective? The issue
not only individual families SES, but for language proficiency. is more complex than in a monolin-
also the SES of the childrens environ- Researchers have taken different gual environment, in which students
ment at the level of the school and the measures of SES for monolingual chil- are taught how to read and write in
country. Their study collected informa- dren and repeatedly found that SES one language. Many other questions
tion from 43 countries, including has a significant impact on language must be addressed: Should one lan-
measures of 193,841 fifteen-year-olds and literacy development. However, guage be taught first, and the other
gender, SES (country, school, peers language proficiency also affects liter- language later? Can children be taught
family, and childs family), number of acy development (Dickinson & Snow, two languages simultaneously? At what
books in the home, enjoyment of read- 1987; Oller & Eilers, 2002), and the stage of development can bilingual lit-
ing (as determined by survey), and majority of studies evaluating SES eracy work effectively? One way of
reading achievement (as determined effects on literacy development do not addressing this issue is to consider
by test scores). Chiu and McBride- control for this confound. There is a waiting until children achieve fluency
Chang modeled reading achievement need to distinguish effects of SES and in their L1 before introducing the L2,
using sequential sets of multilevel language proficiency on literacy out- both spoken and written. Gmez and
regressions to explain the variance of comes to determine whether SES actu- Levine (2013) looked at Spanish-Eng-
each set (gender, country SES, school ally has unique impacts on literacy that lish bilingual children in educational
SES, family SES, number of books at are independent of language profi- settings to evaluate this approach.
home, and reading enjoyment). Their ciency. Efforts to distinguish direct There were 12 participating class-
findings showed that each factor was effects of SES and effects of language rooms, with 21 teachers and 101
significantly associated with reading proficiency will contribute to under- kindergarten students from the mid-
achievement. Combined SES effects at standing of the development of liter- western United States. Gmez and
the country, school, and family levels acy skills. Levine collected data that included
accounted for 24% of the variance in specific linguistic features of teachers
reading achievement. However, varia- Bilingual Children: quality of speech in Spanish and Eng-
tion in reading level was greater within Effects of SES and L1/L2 lish and childrens spoken-language
countries than across countries, and Proficiency on Literacy assessment scores in Spanish as well
within schools than across schools, a One approach to disentangling the as their English literacy scores. The
finding that indicated the importance effects of SES on literacy achievement researchers elicited data by using

436

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 437

speech audio recordings; they tran- went to either a full English immersion studies of bilingual children. In the
scribed the recordings and coded data school or a two-way bilingual school. United States, emphasis is placed on
from both the teachers and the stu- Further, some participants only spoke English-language proficiency as a
dents in the third and fourth quarters Spanish at home, while others spoke means of achieving higher SES status.
of the school year. On the basis of both Spanish and English at home. The minority language, Spanish within
observations and recordings in the Finally, participants were assigned to this context, is less likely to influence
classroom setting, Gmez and Levine low- or high-SES groups on the basis of family SES, even though proficiency in
found that some teachers spoke only the parents educational attainment, a minority L1 language affects learning
Spanish to students. Others used both income, and occupation. Data were outcomes.
Spanish and English. The teachers collected over a 4-year period. The Students from minority-language
choice of language was related to the study was designed carefully to avoid bilingual homes can be assumed to
students test performance on words complications that occur when testing have lower levels of proficiency in the
and sentences in spoken Spanish. The procedures developed for mono- majority language targeted for literacy
researchers found that the high quality linguals are used to assess bilinguals at school. This implies that bilinguals
of Spanish-only input from the teacher performance. The test included stan- may generally lag behind their mono-
had a significant role in promoting L1 dardized assessments of spoken- lingual peers in literacy development.
Spanish proficiency. When teachers language and academic performance However, recent studies have begun
mixed languages, the childrens L1 in both Spanish and English. The to evaluate how the experience of
Spanish proficiency was not as ad- results revealed similar patterns bilingualism may affect the learning
vanced. The students were also tested between monolinguals and bilinguals, of children from majority-language
on their English literacy skills. Interest- in that high-SES students consistently homes. What exactly are some advan-
ingly, Gmez and Levine found that achieved better scores on the language tages of being bilingual and attending
children with higher L1 Spanish oral assessments than the low-SES students. bilingual schools? Kovelman, Baker,
skills outperformed those with lower Effects of SES were particularly pro- and Petitto (2008) evaluated whether
L1 Spanish oral skills. This suggests the nounced on spoken-language profi- the age of first bilingual exposure was
need to have a strong L1 foundation in ciency measures. Another key finding a factor in the outcome of literacy
order to transition to L2 literacy acqui- of the study was that when the high- skills. They studied 150 participants
sition. The researchers concluded that SES bilingual children reached fifth who were categorized into five differ-
it is important for teachers to promote grade, they had a level of proficiency in ent groups based on attendance at a
and establish a strong language foun- their English L2 reading that was equiv- Spanish-English bilingual school or an
dation for students in the early stages alent to that of monolingual English English monolingual school in grade 2
of L1 development in order for them readers. Oller and Eilers concluded or 3. Kovelman et al. evaluated various
to have successful literacy outcomes that both language exposure and SES language and literacy skills. Monolin-
in L1 and L2. The study by Gmez and have an impact on the development of gual English speakers only completed
Levine is one of several indicating that spoken- and written-language profi- English language and literacy tasks.
L2 literacy skills in bilinguals are ciency, and importantly, that the two The remaining participants were
affected by proficiency in both the L1 factors did not generate a significant tested in both English and Spanish.
and the L2 (cf. Guglielmi, 2008; Lind- interaction. However, whereas effects The researchers considered three
holm-Leary, 2014). of language exposure are reduced over areas of language and literacy skills.
Turning to the question of how the time, SES effects persist across devel- First, they assessed childrens ability to
SES of bilinguals influences their liter- opment, so that only bilinguals from manipulate the sounds within their
acy outcomes, Oller and Eilers (2002) high-SES families maintain proficiency language. Second, they looked at the
looked at SES, the language of the in their L1 in addition to developing childrens ability to read words and
home, and the method of instruction L2 literacy skills that are indistinguish- sentences. Third, they looked at the
at school to see how these factors able from monolinguals L1 literacy childrens spoken-language produc-
related to both Spanish and English skills. Further, the high SES status of tion, competence, and proficiency.
language and literacy. They elicited bilinguals families was related to par- They demonstrated that children with
data from 952 Spanish-English bilin- ents use of the L2 in the home. These the earliest exposure to both lan-
guals and 248 monolinguals in Miami, results are a reminder that language guages achieved reading levels in Eng-
FL. The Spanish-English bilinguals status is also an important factor in lish comparable to those attained by

437

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 438

EFFECTS OF SES ON SIGNING BILINGUALS LITERACY

English-dominant bilinguals and in writing systems (Cripps, 2008; Flood, attend speech therapy to support the
Spanish comparable to those of 2002; Supalla & Cripps, 2004), there is development of spoken-English skills
Spanish-dominant bilinguals. They also no widely used orthography for ASL so that they might communicate with
found that students from mono- that would enable us to assess whether parents who only know English. Sup-
lingual English-speaking homes who L1 orthographic experience could port for sign language exposure can be
attended Spanish-English bilingual have an impact on L2 literacy in our obtained through some state agencies
schools outperformed monolingual sample. This absence of a common providing early intervention, such as a
English-speaking students who orthography, however, creates an deaf mentor who makes home visits
attended monolingual schools on opportunity for a novel investigation or sign language classes for parents.
a challenging English phonological of the impact of SES and L1 language However, in no case is the provision
awareness task. By virtue of learning skills on L2 literacy achievement. of speech therapy or sign language
two languages, the bilinguals could Before L2 learning is dealt with, it is support equivalent to the continuous
attain higher levels of metalinguistic important to address some factors access to language that deaf children
awareness (i.e., the ability to reflect influencing L1 learning in deaf signers. of deaf parents experience.
on language), a capacity that allowed There is a great deal of variability of Parents also have several options for
them to outperform monolinguals on language skills in the deaf population. the education of their deaf child. Some
a task that did not directly involve L2 The process of acquiring ASL parallels deaf children attend a school where
knowledge. the acquisition of other languages in they are immersed in ASL with other
In sum, many studies provide evi- the world if exposure to ASL begins at deaf peers. Some attend private
dence of effects of SES on both the L1 birth (Newport & Meier, 1985). Unfor- schools that focus on oral speech edu-
language proficiency and L1 literacy of tunately, exposure to ASL at birth is not cation. Others attend public schools
monolinguals. Further, SES is related the experience of the majority of deaf that provide various forms of support
to language and literacy outcomes in children in the United States. The so that they might have better access
both the L1 and the L2 for bilinguals. majority of deaf children have parents to communication: ASL interpreters,
Within research on bilinguals, we see who are not aware of the importance FM systems (for amplifying CIs and
stronger effects of SES on the majority of signing with their child and who hearing aids in the classroom), real-
language than on the minority lan- are not fluent in ASL. Some children time captioning devices, and note-tak-
guage. But L1 language proficiency become deaf while still in their lan- ing resources. Some deaf children
contributes to L2 literacy even when guage and literacy development even attend public school with no
the L1 is a minority language. Thus, the stages. Hearing parents unfamiliar with support at all! Again, deaf childrens
evidence strongly implicates both SES deafness struggle to know how to exposure to ASL and English varies
and L1 proficiency in predicting L2 lit- mediate interactions involving their depending on the hearing loss, family,
eracy achievement, but these factors deaf child around language and liter- school, and other relevant factors.
are also confounded since SES predicts acy development. Indeed, Moeller One outcome of this variability in
L1 proficiency for most hearing stu- (2000) found that the degree of exposure to language in early life is
dents. Signing bilinguals can help parental involvement accounted for that defining the native language or
to clarify if both factors, SES and L1 more variation in vocabulary size of first language of a deaf signer is
language proficiency, independently deaf 5-year-olds who participated in fraught with difficulties. For the pres-
contribute to literacy development early intervention than nonverbal IQ ent study, we refer to ASL as the first
outcomes. or age of enrollment in early interven- language if the study participant relied
tion. Part of the challenge parents face on signing as the primary form of com-
The Present Study is related to the different choices avail- munication. This characterization does
ASL-English bilinguals have a unique able to address deafness at a young not mean that parents used ASL regu-
profile because L2 literacy is achieved age. Cochlear implants (CIs), hearing larly or fluently in the home. Nor does
without prior development of spoken aids, and other technology are recom- it rule out the possibility that the par-
L2 proficiency. Although there have mended to parents as means of ticipant learned some English words
been several attempts to promote a improving their childs quality of hear- prior to beginning to sign. The term
standardized orthography for ASL, and ing, but none of these technologies first language in this study refers to
deaf children have responded enthusi- can ensure that deaf children will have our best effort to ascertain the partici-
astically to opportunities to learn ASL access to language. Some deaf children pants first fully functional language.

438

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 439

The majority of the U.S. population has deaf or hearing parents. By exclud- adults ranging in age from 6 to 26
is monolingual, and resources at ing deaf individuals with hearing par- years with a hearing loss of 85 dB or
schools are based on monolingual ents from most studies, the literature greater in the better ear were re-
education. Successful communication has provided a piecemeal view of lin- cruited. Individuals with known
and employment rely to a large degree guistic and cognitive skills in the deaf learning disabilities, neurological dis-
on English-language proficiency. population. orders, or uncorrected vision difficul-
Within this broader context, the tradi- Previous studies of ASL-English ties were excluded from the sample,
tional approach to reading in the deaf bilinguals have shown that L1 language as was any participant who did not
population has been to evaluate it proficiency in ASL is related to L2 liter- complete the entire protocol. Conse-
within a monolingual framework. The acy skills, even when signing bilinguals quently, 135 participants were included
present study departs from past work do not use a writing system with in the analysis.
in two important ways. First, the ques- their L1 (Chamberlain & Mayberry,
tion of reading proficiency and the fac- 2008; Hoffmeister, 2000). Despite the Materials
tors related to it are considered within numerous previous studies on SES, L1 Parents of the participants filled out
a bilingual framework. Instead of eval- proficiency, and literacy skills of hear- a background questionnaire regard-
uating effects of SES on reading devel- ing monolinguals and bilinguals, there ing their marital status, occupation,
opment alone, this study includes a is a dearth of research on SES and how and educational experience, which
measure of ASL proficiency as well. it affects language development in ASL- was used to assign participants a
Hauser, Paludnevicience, Supalla, and English bilinguals. Thus, the goal of the score on the Hollingshead (1975)
Bavalier (2008) devised one of the first present study was to investigate this SES scale. Participants age was also
ASL tests that can be given to ASL-Eng- relationship in deaf ASL-English bilin- recorded.
lish bilingual children to directly assess guals for the first time. The research The ASLSentence Reproduction
their ASL proficiency. By including questions were: Test (ASL-SRT; Hauser, Paludnevi-
measures of both ASL and English pro- cience, et al., 2008) was the instrument
ficiency, the present study considers Does SES predict L2 reading abil- used to assess ASL proficiency. The
all the language skills of the bilingual ity in signing bilingual readers? ASL-SRT requires test takers to view 20
participants. Does L1 proficiency predict L2 sentences of increasing complexity.
Second, past studies have only reading ability in signing bilin- After each stimulus sentence, they are
included native signers (deaf children gual readers? asked to repeat the sentence verbatim
with deaf parents) because of the lack If both factors affect L2 reading (cf. the Test of Adult and Adolescent
of ASL assessment tools, which ability, are they correlated with Language; Hammill, Brown, Larsen, &
severely limited sample size and the each other? Can L2 reading abil- Weiderholt, 1994). The ASL-SRT has
generalizability of the results to the ity in signing bilingual readers be not yet been normed for different
broader deaf population. We use direct modeled with both factors? ages. In order to compare perform-
assessment of ASL skill, which allows ance across participants of different
us to include deaf individuals from Methodology and Data ages, we modified the ASL-SRT scores
both hearing and deaf families; it also To investigate the possibilities raised by into age-normed standard scores by
allows us to distinguish between dif- our research questions, we assessed calculating the mean and standard
ferent levels of language proficiency family SES, L1 ASL language proficiency, deviation of ASL-SRT scores for three
within populations of deaf individuals and L2 English reading proficiency age groups: 79120 months, 121160
who are all native signers, just as vari- in a large population of ASL-English months, and 161 months or older. We
ability in language proficiency can be bilinguals. We used mixed-effects lin- subsequently generated standard
distinguished in any population of ear modeling to explore the relation- scores for participants on the basis of
hearing native speakers of a language. ships among these three variables. their age group.
The importance of this change in Scores from the reading compre-
methods cannot be understated. As Participants hension portion of the Peabody In-
the results of the present study will The data for the present study were dividual Achievement TestRevised
demonstrate, categorical distinctions collected nationally at universities, (PIAT-R; Markwardt, 1989) were used
in ASL proficiency cannot be made on schools, and summer camps for the to assess English literacy skills. For
the basis of whether a deaf individual deaf. A total of 212 deaf children and each PIAT-R item, participants read a

439

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 440

EFFECTS OF SES ON SIGNING BILINGUALS LITERACY

sentence, then picked the picture Table 1


that matched the sentence from four Demographic Characteristics and Language Assessment Scores of the Sample
choices. Range M SD
Age, monthsa 79315 187.44 57.48
Procedure SES 11.0064.00 38.10 14.77
Participants were tested individually by ASL-SRT 0.0018.00 7.67 4.17
examiners fluent in ASL. From the total PIAT-R 55.00132.00 85.24 16.99
sample of participants, we selected a Notes. SES = socioeconomic status. ASL-SRT = ASLSentence Reproduction Test. PIAT-R =
Peabody Individual Achievement TestRevised.
subsample using the following criteria: a N = 135. Distribution of participants across age groups: 79120 months, n = 21; 121160 months,
completion of the reading assessment n =32; 161315 months, n = 82.
(PIAT-R) and intelligible writing or
signing to allow accurate scoring. A
total of 97 DoD (deaf children of deaf and language assessment values for oped signing skills are not necessarily
parents) and 38 DoH (deaf children of the sample are listed in Table 1. the parents with the highest education
hearing parents) were identified for We analyzed the data with mixed- levels and incomes.
inclusion in the study. Traditionally, effects regression using the lme4 pack- Having established the independ-
DoD and DoH have been analyzed age (Bates, Maechler & Bolker, 2012) ence of SES and ASL as factors that
separately because there have not in R (R Core Team, 2014). SES and ASL- could influence reading comprehen-
been tools available to directly assess SRT scores were treated as fixed fac- sion, we next carried out the mixed-
ASL proficiency. The assumption has tors and participant was the random effects regression including SES and
been that DoD will always have better factor; the dependent measure was ASL. Both SES (p < .01) and ASL (p <
ASL proficiency than DoH. Further, reading comprehension as measured .01) were significant factors in the
many studies do not include DoH at all by the PIAT-R. model. How can SES and ASL both
since their ASL proficiency is less pre- predict better reading scores, even
dictable. DoD account for roughly 5% Results though they are not correlated? To
of the deaf population in the United We first evaluated whether SES and help explain the complex relationship
States, with DoH representing the ASL were correlated in the sample. The between these factors, we present the
other 95% (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2002). correlation was not significant (R2 = data in a panel graph in Figure 1.
Although DoH as a group typically .020; adjusted R2 = .013; N = 135; p = Unlike most figures that display regres-
attain lower proficiency levels in ASL .097, n.s.). This is an important finding. sion data, Figure 1 does not show the
than DoD, there is a need to include SES is highly correlated with English dependent variable on the y-axis. This
data that represent the majority of the proficiency in hearing children, in figure separates the data into four sep-
deaf population. In the present study, that hearing children who experience arate but overlapping groups with
we were able to include both groups home literacy environments that are respect to the dependent variable:
because we directly assessed ASL profi- the most conducive to learning to read PIAT-low, PIAT-midlow, PIAT-midhigh,
ciency. While the DoD participants had typically also have the strongest oral and PIAT-high. The separation of the
a higher mean ASL score (8.68) than the language skills. A failure to detect a PIAT scores into four overlapping
DoH participants (5.08), the ranges of similar pattern between SES and ASL groups provides a chance to look more
scores for the two groups were highly proficiency in deaf children is the first closely at the relationship between SES
similarDoD 0 to 18, DoH 0 to 15 indicator that ASL proficiency is dis- and ASL at each level of reading profi-
differing primarily in the ranges upper tributed in the deaf population differ- ciency. In the leftmost panel of the fig-
boundary. Further, as estimated by the ently from English proficiency in the ure, one can see that there is a slightly
mean-median differential, variability in hearing population. Deaf children liv- negative correlation of SES and ASL for
ASL fluency was comparablea value ing in high-SES homes do not neces- poor readers. By contrast, in the right-
less than 1.00 for both groups (DoD sarily have the greatest L1 proficiency. most panel, one can see that SES and
.32, DoH .58). The opportunity to This may be an indication that ASL is ASL are positively correlated for good
include all 135 subjects, regardless of comparable to a minority language in readers. For the two middle groups,
parental hearing status, enabled a the English majority language environ- the regression line is flatter than for
fuller representation of the target pop- ment in the United States. Parents of the best readers, but the regression
ulation. Demographics characteristics deaf children who have well-devel- line nevertheless indicates a positive

440

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 441

Figure 1
Performance on the PIAT-R as Predicted by Both SES and ASL Proficiency in a Single-Regression Model

Notes. PIAT-R = Peabody Individual Achievement Test-Revised. SES = socioeconomic status. ASL = American Sign Language. For SES, p = .00177. For ASL
proficiency, p = .00165. N = 135. Mixed-effects regression: fixed factors = SES, ASL; random factor = participants.

correlation. The results suggest that strong portrayals of reading enjoy- holds. What we cannot conclude from
for the poorest readers, home SES and ment; they use more mediation strate- this type of study is whether the rela-
ASL proficiency are competing factors. gies, and their mediation strategies are tionship between SES and reading
By contrast, deaf signers have the more closely aligned with the reading achievement in deaf children reflects
greatest success in literacy when SES level of their children than is the case the same set of behaviors, or different
and ASL are cooperating factors pro- for low-SES parents (Korat & Haglili, behaviors, on the part of high-SES ver-
viding mutual support for develop- 2007). Some examples of mediation sus low-SES parents of deaf children.
ment. strategies include talking with children Many of the characteristics of high-
about issues that go beyond the text in SES home literacy environments in
Discussion the book. The quality of encourage- monolingual households could easily
The question, from the start, was ment high-SES parents give their chil- be transferred to the context of com-
whether SES has an impact on literacy dren to participate actively in reading municating in ASL about English print.
development of ASL-English bilinguals. activities is also greater. The style of For example, mothers from all SES lev-
The results extend the finding of prior talk of high-SES parents creates a con- els overestimate their childs literacy
studies of SES effects on monolingual nection between the texts meaning skills (Korat & Haglili, 2007). High-SES
and bilingual hearing childrens read- and the childs own experience. While mothers overestimate less and are
ing outcomes and document for the it is not surprising that these behav- more aware of the importance of using
first time that ASL-English bilinguals iors would be beneficial to deaf chil- different mediation strategies to facili-
reading proficiency also benefits from dren, the present study is the first to tate their childs literacy development.
higher levels of home SES. There are demonstrate that despite the unique In other words, the focus of educating
several reasons why high-SES parents factors influencing relationships a signing bilingual child needs to be
of hearing children are better at pro- between deaf children and their deaf on the relationship between the par-
moting literacy. They provide more or hearing parents, the association ent(s) and child, even if a parent is
reading opportunities; they exhibit between SES and literacy nevertheless learning to sign with the child. This

441

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 442

EFFECTS OF SES ON SIGNING BILINGUALS LITERACY

may be particularly critical in the case results of the present study demon- a higher level of SES in adulthood.
of deaf children who are approaching strate the need to take SES into When the advantages of bilingualism
literacy without full mastery of the lan- account as a potential factor influenc- are widely recognized by the general
guage represented by print. Deaf chil- ing parent-child behaviors in design- public, not only deaf families, but all
dren vary widely in literacy outcomes. ing studies of parental mediation of bilingual families, will benefit from this
The ability of parents to realistically literacy interactions in the future. change in societal attitudes.
assess their childs literacy perform- For deaf parents, whose children The few attempts to document
ance allows them to then respond in are fluent in their language, the rela- parental mediation of literacy activities
appropriate ways to their childs liter- tionship between SES and literacy in deaf families demonstrate that when
acy needs. The topic of literacy is may have a different explanation the home language is shared between
prioritized in the educational environ- (Beck, McKeown, & Kucan, 2013). deaf children and their signing deaf
ment for teachers and educators of Deaf families face barriers to higher parents, parents scaffold early literacy
deaf students. However, if a child is to SES levels. For instance, Schembri et al. experiences in such a way that their
obtain good literacy skills at school, (2009) collected and categorized dif- children are socialized into literate
the role and responsibility of the par- ferent SES levels of deaf individuals behaviors and become increasingly
ent(s) should be acknowledged and from Australia and New Zealand. independent participants in literacy
cultivated as well. Despite some methodological con- events (Bailes, C. N. Erting, L. C. Erting,
The next step is to consider cerns related to sampling from a & Thumann-Prezioso, 2009; Berke,
whether the behaviors of high-SES par- sparse population, Schembri et al. con- 2013). Deaf parents capitalize on visual
ents documented in previous studies cluded that deaf individuals and fami- features of signed languages to draw
can be expected in families with deaf lies SES levels are not distributed their childrens attention to features of
children. Do higher-SES parents who similarly to those of the general mono- printfor example, holding a hand-
have ASL-English bilingual children lingual population. The researchers shape next to a word in a book or sign-
accommodate their need for literacy proposed that deaf individuals face a ing different meanings associated with
success? The reason to take this into barrier to higher SES because of an English word while pointing at the
consideration is the fact that commu- restricted fluency in the majority lan- print. Chaining is a strategy first docu-
nication from parent to child is differ- guage. This account is similar to ones mented among teachers of the deaf
ent when the parent is learning to sign describing other bilingual situations (Humphries & MacDougall, 2000), but
and the child is not fluent in the par- (Oller & Eilers, 2002). Sign languages it is also observed in the behaviors of
ents language. The results ultimately are essentially minority languages and deaf parents (Berke, 2013). Deaf par-
support the idea that even when com- are not as socially esteemed as major- ents are also sensitive to the visual
munication skills between parents and ity spoken languages. Thus most DoD needs of their children; the parents
signing bilingual children are not sim- families are classified as having work- will choose how to position them-
ilar to those of other monolingual and ing and middle-class SES; by contrast, selves and a book relative to their child
bilingual families, high-SES parents DoH families can be found at all SES in a way that enables the child to view
nevertheless find ways to promote lit- levels. Yet it is not impossible for DoD the book and the parents signing
eracy. The majority of signing bilingual families to have high SES levels; it is simultaneously. While there is interest
children have parents who are learning just less typical. One type of societal in the possibility of teaching hearing
ASL as a second language or are not change that might be necessary so that parents these mediation strategies
signing bilinguals; thus, it is highly more deaf families can attain a higher (see, e.g., Schleper, 1997), there have
informative to discover that despite SES level is the recognition of bilin- not been attempts to investigate how
potential communication barriers, SES gualism as socially valuable, and the parental mediation of literacy activities
is still predictive of stronger literacy recognition of deaf individuals who might differ across SES levels of deaf
skills. There has been an attempt to use a signed language and a spoken parents.
evaluate the language behaviors of language as bilinguals. There is no rea- In addition to showing effects of
hearing parents of deaf children (Har- son to focus solely on English to the SES on reading comprehension, the
ris & Mohay, 1997; Loots, Devise, & detriment of ASL. Language attitudes results of the present study replicate
Jacquet, 2005; Spencer & Harris, influence access to educational and and extend those of prior studies
2006), but there has been no investiga- professional opportunities that allow showing that ASL proficiency predicts
tion of the role of SES as a factor. The children of lower-SES families to attain English literacy outcomes in both chil-

442

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 443

dren (Hoffmeister, 2000; Vernon & rather than its linguistic status. Inter- signs instead of creating new meaning
Koh, 1970) and adults (Chamberlain & estingly, they pointed out that their and forming representations every
Mayberry, 2008; Freel et al., 2011). results held despite the fact that 88% time they encounter a written word
Chamberlain and Mayberry (2008), for of the deaf parents they studied had they have never seen before. In other
example, assessed the proficiency in not attended school past the eighth words, signed language proficiency
ASL, Manually Coded English (MCE), grade, while 30% of the hearing par- may allow signing bilinguals to associ-
and English reading of 31 deaf adults ents had attended college. Vernon and ate meaning with written-word forms
by means of a range of assessment Koh interpreted this inverse relation- more rapidly, even though the print
tasks. Participants were separated into ship between parent education and represents words from a different lan-
two groups on the basis of their per- child success as evidence that parent- guage.
formance on the Stanford Reading child communication is essential for All parents of deaf children can pro-
Test: those reading below the grade 8 developing basic language compe- mote their childs sign language profi-
level, and those reading at grade 8 or tence. Their results may on first reflec- ciency, whether or not it is the parents
above. Deaf signers with better reading tion seem to contradict ours, but it L1. Developing strong signing skills in
abilities differed significantly from the should be noted that none of the deaf themselves and in their child gives par-
less proficient readers on an ASL gram- children of hearing parents in their ents a basis for being involved in liter-
maticality judgment task, as well as on study had access to ASL. In our popu- acy events, and for empowering their
ASL and MCE narrative comprehen- lation of deaf children, all participants children to become proficient bilin-
sion tasks. Chamberlain and Mayberry had access to ASL at some point guals. The fact that ASL skills and SES
argue that there is a linguistic basis to regardless of the hearing status of their are positively correlated in the most
reading that is independent of the spe- parents. Our study has thus allowed us proficient readers in our study sample
cific language represented by print; to discover that both accessible lan- suggests that these two factors can be
thus, sign language can provide a gen- guage and the home literacy environ- mutually beneficial. Increasingly, stud-
eral linguistic advantage to reading ment associated with high-SES families ies of bilinguals are showing that bilin-
development, even though it does not are beneficial to the development of gualism has benefits for cognitive
share phonological, lexical, or gram- literacy skills for deaf children. function, even beyond the domain of
matical features with spoken lan- How can knowledge of a sign lan- language processing. Hauser, Lukom-
guages. The results of the present guage promote literacy development? ski, and Hillman (2008) focused on
study are consistent with Chamberlain Kubus, Villwock, Morford, and Rath- executive function (EF), which is a self-
and Mayberrys findings, and add to mann (2014) found that deaf German regulatory system that has significant
those findings by showing that this bilinguals activate signs in DGS (Ger- implications for both social and aca-
relationship holds at an earlier phase man Sign Language) while processing demic development. These
of development. Chamberlain and written German words in a semantic researchers provided an overview of
Mayberry recruited adults for their similarity judgment task (cf. Morford, the ways the environment can affect
study (ages 1753 years). The present Wilkinson, Villwock, Piar, & Kroll, the development of EF. Overall, they
study demonstrates that this relation- 2011, for ASL-English bilinguals). To found better EF task outcomes for
ship between ASL and English literacy provide a potential explanation of their bilinguals compared to monolinguals,
skills is already apparent in a much findings, Kubus et al. proposed not and suggested that the evidence pro-
younger population. only that knowledge of a signed lan- vides necessary insights for parents
Vernon and Koh (1970), who docu- guage supports written-word recogni- and educators about the conse-
mented better literacy outcomes in tion by providing a general linguistic quences of sign-print bilingualism for
deaf children with deaf parents than in advantage, but that there may be lan- other cognitive functions.
deaf children with hearing parents, car- guage-specific benefits of learning a Indeed, parents and educators may
ried out their research at a time when signed language as well. Kubus et al. already have tacit knowledge of the
there was less recognition of the lin- explained that during the course of L1 potential benefits of bilingualism in
guistic status of ASL. They discussed acquisition, signers develop extensive ASL and English. Pucci, Harmon, and
their results as they related to early semantic networks. Deaf readers can Mounty (2012) interviewed 12 parents
manual communication (p. 527), thus map orthographic forms to signed and teachers who worked with deaf
emphasizing the accessibility of the phonological forms and the meaning signers to probe their emic perspec-
communication form to children representations associated with those tives on the relationship between ASL

443

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 444

EFFECTS OF SES ON SIGNING BILINGUALS LITERACY

and English proficiency in their chil- ence of qualitative differences in the mendations expressed are those of the
dren and students. Interviewees cognitive process of reading for deaf authors and do not necessarily reflect
expressed the conviction that a bidi- and hearing children. However, they the views of the National Institutes
rectionality of influence exists between do suggest important differences in of Health or the National Science
these languages. As the development the topography of learning experi- Foundation. Portions of the results
of one language increases, it supports ences for deaf and hearing children. were presented at the Seventh Annual
and facilitates development of the Deaf children may be surrounded by inter-Science of Learning Centers
other language. Much of the prior liter- books but not have sufficient language (iSLC) Conference, in Pittsburgh, PA.
ature has addressed how ASL influ- to benefit from the stories the books We extend our sincere appreciation to
ences English, since many deaf hold. They may have rich and engag- the families who participated in this
individuals become fluent signers ing conversations with their parents in study.The Authors.
before becoming proficient readers. ASL, but rarely receive encouragement
Pucci et al. found that respondents to put their stories into writing. When References
were sensitive to influences in the both factors come together to sup- Bailes, C. N., Erting, C. N., Erting, L. C., & Thu-
mann-Prezioso, C. (2009). Language and lit-
opposite direction as well, noting that port the developing deaf learner, suc- eracy acquisition through parental mediation
as English vocabulary increases, sign- cessful literacy achievement is a likely in American Sign Language. Sign Language
ers show more sensitivity to genre and outcome. Studies, 9(4), 417456.
Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2012).
lexical choice in ASL. The unique To conclude, this initial study of the lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using
insights into the language develop- effect of SES on the literacy skills of S4 Classes (R Package Version 0.999999-0)
ment of deaf bilinguals from this group ASL-English bilinguals has shown for [Computer software]. Retrieved from
rproject.org website: http://CRAN.Rproject
of stakeholders provide additional sup- the first time that SES has a direct
.org/package0lme4
port for the conclusions reached on impact on ASL-English bilinguals L2 lit- Beals, D. E., & De Temple, J. M. (1993). Home
the basis of performance-based meas- eracy success, and that this effect is contributions to early language and literacy
ures of ASL and English proficiency of independent of and additional to the development. National Reading Conference
Yearbook, 42, 207215.
deaf signing bilinguals. benefits of L1 proficiency in ASL. Since Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L.
The results of the present study can SES and ASL proficiency are not corre- (2013). Bringing words to life: Robust
also be brought to bear on the ques- lated for deaf signing bilinguals, it is vocabulary instruction. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
tion of whether reading development critical to explore a range of sensitive, Berke, M. (2013). Reading books with young
is qualitatively similar for deaf and complex linguistic and social interac- deaf children: Strategies for mediating
hearing children. On the one hand, the tions that can account for the way between American Sign Language and Eng-
lish. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Edu-
study shows that two factors that these factors contribute to the process
cation, 18(3), 299311.
influence the reading achievement of of acquiring language and literacy dur- Chamberlain, C., & Mayberry, R. I. (2008). Amer-
hearing children also influence deaf ing childhood bilingualism. Future ican Sign Language syntactic and narrative
childrens reading achievement. One research should investigate the impact comprehension in skilled and less skilled
readers: Bilingual and bimodal evidence for
of these factors, L1 proficiency, is cog- of different types of literacy mediation the linguistic basis of reading. Applied Psy-
nitive, and reflects on the importance between monolingual and bilingual cholinguistics, 29(3), 367388.
of prior language knowledge in the parents and their deaf children. Also, Chiu, M., & McBride-Chang, C. (2006). Gender,
context, and reading: A comparison of stu-
development of reading skills (Cham- research should explore the different dents in 43 countries. Scientific Studies of
berlain & Mayberry, 2008; Dickinson & roles possibly played by SES in ASL- Reading, 310, 331362.
Snow, 1987). The second factor, SES, is English bilingual childrens language Corson, H. J. (1973). Comparing deaf children
of oral deaf parents and deaf parents using
social, and reflects on the influence of and literacy outcomes.
manual communication on academic,
parents in shaping opportunities for social, and communicative functioning
learning to read (Bailes et al., 2009; Note (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Univer-
Berke, 2013; Korat et al., 2013). These The research for the present study was sity of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.
Cripps, J. H. (2008). A case study on reading
two factors often co-occur for hearing supported by the National Science processes of signing deaf children (Unpub-
children; deaf children are much less Foundation Science of Learning Cen- lished doctoral dissertation). University of
likely to benefit from both factors. ter Program, under Cooperative Agree- Arizona, Tucson.
Dickinson, D. K., & Snow, C. E. (1987). Interre-
These results do not provide a basis ment No. SBE-1041725. Any opinions, lationships among pre-reading and oral lan-
either to propose or reject the pres- findings, and conclusions or recom- guage skills in kindergartners from two social

444

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 445

classes. Early Childhood Research Quar- trajectories of children from low-SES and the United States. Sign Language Studies,
terly, 2(1), 125. language-minority homes: Implications for 4(2), 138163.
Droop, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2003). Language closing the achievement gaps. Journal of Moeller, M. P. (2000). Early intervention and lan-
proficiency and reading ability in first- and the American Psychological Association, guage development in children who are deaf
second-language learners. Reading Research 49, 414. and hard of hearing. Pediatrics, 106(3), 19.
Quarterly, 38(1), 78103. Hoffmeister, R. (2000). A piece of the puzzle: Morford, J. P., Kroll, J. F., Piar, P., & Wilkinson, E.
Flood, C. M. (2002). How do deaf and hard ASL and reading comprehension in deaf chil- (2014). Bilingual word recognition in deaf
of hearing students experience learning dren. In C. Chamberlain, J. P. Morford, & R. and hearing signers: Effects of proficiency
to write using Signwriting, a way to read I. Mayberry (Eds.), Language acquisition by and language dominance on cross-language
and write signs? (Unpublished doctoral eye (pp. 143163). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. activation. Second Language Research, 30(2),
dissertation). University of New Mexico, Hollingshead, A. A. 1975. Four-factor index of 251271.
Albuquerque. social status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale Morford, J. P., Wilkinson, E., Villwock, A., Piar, P.,
Freel, B. L., Clark, M. D., Anderson, M. L., University, New Haven, CT. & Kroll, J. F. (2011). When deaf signers read
Gilbert, G. L., Musyoka, M. M. & Hauser, P. C. Humphries, T., & MacDougall, F. (2000). Chain- English: Do written words activate their sign
(2011). Deaf individuals bilingual abilities: ing and other links making connections translations? Cognition, 118(2), 286292.
American Sign Language proficiency, read- between American Sign Language and Eng- Murray, A. D., Fees, B. S., Crowe, L. K., Murphy,
ing skills, and family characteristics. Psychol- lish in two types of school setting. Visual M. E., & Henriksen, A. L. (2006). The
ogy, 2(1), 1823. Anthropology Review, 15(2), 8494. language environment of toddlers in center-
Gmez, P. B., & Levine, S. C. (2013). Oral language Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. based care versus home settings. Early
skills of Spanish-speaking English-language (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links Childhood Education Journal, 34(3),
learners: The impact of high-quality native between maternal talk, processing speed, 233239.
language exposure. Applied Psycholinguistics, and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning chil- Negro, I., & Genelot, S. (2012). The role of
34(4), 673696. dren. Developmental Science, 11, 3139. language skills in learning to read: The case
Guglielmi, R. S. (2008). Native language Korat, O., Arafat, S. H., Aram, D., & Klein, P. of bilingualism in French overseas depart-
proficiency, English literacy, academic (2013). Book reading mediation, SES, home ments. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(4),
achievement, and occupational attainment literacy environment, and childrens literacy: 799828.
in limited-English-proficient students: Evidence from Arabic-speaking families. First Newport, E. L., & Meier, R. P. (1985). The acqui-
A latent growth modeling perspective. Jour- Language, 33(2), 132154. sition of American Sign Language. In D. I.
nal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), Korat, O., & Haglili, S. (2007). Maternal evalua- Slobin (Ed.), The cross-linguistic study of
322342. tions of childrens emergent literacy level, language acquisition (pp. 881938). Hills-
Hammill, D. D., Brown, V. L., Larsen, S. C., & Wei- maternal mediation in book reading, and dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
derholt, J. L. (1994). Test of adolescent and childrens emergent literacy level: A compar- Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E. (2002). An integrated
adult language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. ison between SES groups. Journal of Liter- approach to evaluating effects of bilingual-
Harris, M., & Mohay, H. (1997). Learning to look acy Research, 39(2), 249276. ism in Miami schoolchildren: The study
in the right place: A comparison of atten- Kovelman, J., Baker, S. A., & Petitto, L. A. (2008). design. In D. K. Oller & R. E. Eilers (Eds.),
tional behavior in deaf children with deaf and Age of first bilingual language exposure as a Language and literacy in bilingual chil-
hearing mothers. Journal of Deaf Studies new window into bilingual reading develop- dren (pp. 2240). Clevendon, England: Mul-
and Deaf Education, 2(2), 95103. ment. Bilingualism: Language and Cogni- tilingual Matters.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differ- tion, 11(2), 203223. Padden, C., & Ramsey, C. (2000). American Sign
ences in the everyday experience of young Kubus, O., Villwock, A., Morford, P., & Rathmann, Language and reading ability in deaf children.
American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. C. (2014). Word recognition in deaf readers: In C. Chamberlain, J. P. Morford, & R. I. May-
Hauser, P. C., Lukomski, J., & Hillman, T. (2008). Cross-language activation of German Sign berry (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye
Development of deaf and hard of hearing stu- Language and German. Applied Psycholin- (pp. 165189). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
dents executive function. In M. Marschark & guistics. Advance online publication. doi:10 Pucci, C., Harmon, K., & Mounty, J. (2013, Spring).
P. C. Hauser (Eds.), Deaf cognition: Founda- .1017/S0142716413000520 Emic perspectives on reading development
tions and outcomes (pp. 286308). New Lindholm-Leary, K. (2014). Bilingual and biliter- in American Sign Language/English bilingual
York, NY: Oxford University Press. acy skills in young Spanish-speaking low-SES deaf children. Deaf Studies Digital Journal.
Hauser, P. C., Paludnevicience, R., Supalla, T., & children: Impact of instructional language Retrieved from http://dsdj.gallaudet.edu
Bavalier, D. (2008). ASLSentence Reproduc- and primary-language proficiency. Interna- Raag, T., Kusiak, K., Tumilty, M., Kelemen, A.,
tion Test: Development and implications. tional Journal of Bilingual Education and Bernheimer, H., & Bond, J. (2011). Reconsid-
In R. M. de Quadros (Ed.), Sign language: Bilingualism, 17(2), 144159. ering SES and gender divides in literacy
Spinning and unraveling the past, present, Loots, G., Devise, I., & Jacquet, W. (2005). The achievement: Are the gaps across social class
and future (pp. 160172). Petrpolis, Brazil: impact of visual communication on the inter- and gender necessary? Educational Psychol-
Arara Azul. subjective development of early parent-child ogy, 31(6), 691705.
Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental interaction with 18-to-24-month-old deaf Ransdell, S. (2012). Theres still no free lunch:
influence: Socioeconomic status affects early toddlers. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Poverty as a composite of SES predicts
vocabulary development via maternal speech. Education, 10(4), 357375. school-level reading comprehension.
Child Development, 74, 13681378. Markwardt, F. C. (1989). Peabody individual American Behavioral Scientist, 56(7),
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and achievement testrevised. Circle Pines, MN: 908925.
shape language development. Developmen- American Guidance Services. R Core Team. (2014). R: A language and envi-
tal Review, 26(1), 5588. doi:10.1016/j.dr Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2002). Chas- ronment for statistical computing. Vienna,
.2005.11.002 ing the mythical 10 percent: Parental hearing Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Comput-
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language status of deaf and hard of hearing students in ing. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org

445

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


18715-AAD159.5_Winter2015 3/2/15 5:00 PM Page 446

EFFECTS OF SES ON SIGNING BILINGUALS LITERACY

Schembri, A., McKee, D., McKee, R., Pivac, S., Spencer, P. E., & Harris, M. (2006). Patterns 23, 10). Tucson: College of Education, Uni-
Johnston, T., & Goswell, D. (2009). Phono- and effects of language input to deaf versity of Arizona.
logical variation and change in Australian and infants and toddlers from deaf and hearing Vernon, M., & Koh, S. (1970). Early manual com-
New Zealand Sign Languages: The location mothers. In B. Schick, M. Marschark, & munication and deaf childrens achieve-
variable. Language Variation and Change, P. E. Spencer (Eds.), Advances in sign lan- ment. American Annals of the Deaf, 115(5),
21(2), 193231. guage development of deaf children (pp. 527536.
Schleper, D. R. (1997). Reading to deaf children: 71101). New York, NY: Oxford University White, K. R. (1982). The relation between
Learning from deaf adults. Washington, Press. socioeconomic status and academic achieve-
DC: Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Strong, M., & Prinz, P. M. (1997). A study of ment. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 461481.
Center. the relationship between American Sign Zhang, Y., Tardif, T., Shu, H., Li, H., Liu, H.,
Shimpi, P. M., Fedewa, A., & Hans, S. (2012). Language and English literacy. Journal of McBride-Chang, C., et al. (2013). Phonolog-
Social and linguistic input in low-income Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(1), ical skills and vocabulary knowledge
African American mother-child dyads from 1 3746. mediate socioeconomic status effects in
month through 2 years: Relations to vocab- Supalla, S. J., & Cripps, J. H. (2004). Learning to predicting reading outcomes for Chinese
ulary development Applied Psycholinguis- read without sound. In R. Marx (Ed.), Educa- children. Developmental Psychology, 49(4),
tics, 33(4), 781798. tional research: Practice and policy (pp. 665671.

446

VOLUME 159, NO. 5, 2015 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


Copyright of American Annals of the Deaf is the property of American Annals of the Deaf
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.

Potrebbero piacerti anche