Sei sulla pagina 1di 16

Golos, D. B., & Moses, A. M. (2013).

Developing preschool deaf children's language and literacy learning from an


educational media series. American Annals of the Deaf, 158(4), 411-425.

DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL DEAF CHILDREN'S


LANGUAGE AND LITERACY LEARNING FROM
AN EDUCATIONAL MEDIA SERIES

iTH THE INCREASE in research on multiliteracies comes greater inter-


est in exploring multiple pathways of learning for deaf children. Educa-
tional media have been increasingly examined as a tool for facilitating
the development of deaf children's language and literacy skills. The
authors investigated whether preschool deaf children (A'^ = 31) acquired
targeted American Sign Language and literacy skills after viewing one
video from an educational video series in ASL. Descriptive statistics
were gathered and a split-plot ANOVA was conducted to determine
whether targeted literacy scores increased from pretest to posttest and
whether scores varied by baseline ASL skills. A significant improvement
was found in the skills targeted in the video, which occurred regardless
of the level of baseline ASL skills. The findings support the claim that
learning ASL and literacy skills through educational media may benefit
deaf children with varied levels of exposure to ASL.
DEBBIE B . GOLOS AND
ANNIE M . MOSES Keywords: emergent literacy, deaf, Corina & Singleton, 2009; Hoffmeister,
preschool, ASL, educational media, 2000; Morford & Mayberry, 2000; Rath-
visual learning man, Wolfgang, & Morgan, 2007). With
GOLOS IS AN ASSISTANT PROFESSOR OF DEAF
the increasing amount and range of
EDUCATION, DEPARTMENT OF COMMUNICATIVE
There has been growing acknowledg- technology in the classroom, educators
DISORDERS AND DEAF EDUCATION, UTAH
ment of the use of multimedia with can also use instructional strategies,
STATE UNIVERSITY, LOGAN. MOSES IS AN
deafi children (e.g., Golos & Moses, materials, and assessments that pro-
ASSISTANT PROFESSOR OF EARLY CHILDHOOD,
2011; Loeterman, Paul, & Donahue, mote learning through a medium that
DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION AND ALLIED
2002; Mueller & Hurtig, 2010), with incorporates a variety of modalities.
STUDIES, JOHN CARROLL UNIVERSITY,
some researchers focusing on investi- While there is still much to learn about
UNIVERSITY HEIGHTS, OH.
gating and promoting learning through the development of language and liter-
multiple sources and modes of com- acy in deaf children, emerging evi-
munication. Evidence also suggests dence suggests that there are multiple
that presenting information through a pathways to growth in this domain.
visual language^ such as American Sign Access to multiple ways of presenting
Language (ASL) allows deaf children to knowledge and skills related to lan-
develop in various skills areas, such as guage and literacy is critical, particu-
language, literacy, and cognition (e.g.. larly during early childhood (Clark,

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNAJLS OF THE DEAF


DEAF CHILDREN LEARNING FROM EDUCATIONAL MEDLA.

Gilbert, & Anderson, 2011; Kyle & language and literacy skills. These and simultaneously rather than
Harris, 2010; Mayer, 2007; New London recent studies have found that media, sequentially (e.g., Sulzby & Teale, 1991;
Group, 1996). especially those that are educationally Whitehurst & Lonigan, 2001). For
Children learn a great deal about lit- oriented, can have a positive impact example, children can learn about
eracy prior to formally learning to on literacy and language skills, includ- print concepts at the same time that
read. Learning concepts such as the ing educational media presented dur- they are being exposed to print,
meaning of new words and aspects of ing the early childhood years and in through, for instance, seeing or hear-
a story (e.g., characters, setting, and American Sign Language (ASL) for deaf ing a book read aloud. Whereas previ-
main events) can be learned prior to children (e.g., Golos, 2010a, 2010b; ous notions of literacy argued that very
the acquisition of formal reading skills. Golos & Moses, 2011; Loeterman et al., young children should not be intro-
However, if development in this area is 2002). For example, deaf children who duced to reading and writing until age
to be facilitated, children must be have been exposed to educational 6.5 years (see Durkin, 1966, for discus-
exposed to fluent language models media in ASL have been found to sion on the topic of reading readiness),
from birth. Those who are not exposed demonstrate an increase in targeted the focus of the emergent literacy per-
to a fluent language model often begin vocabulary in both ASL and printed spective is on children's language and
preschool with gaps in their language English (Golos, 2006; Golos & Moses, literacy experiences that occur prior to
and literacy skills (Freel et al., 2011; 2011). Although there are differing formal schooling and that contribute
Mayberry 2007; Mayberry, del Guidice, opinions on the best routes to lan- to their ability to read and write con-
& Lieberman, 2011; Mayer 2007). guage and literacy development for ventionally as they grow.
Some have argued that the delays in deaf children, the focus of the present Language and literacy skills mutually
language and literacy already in place study is on continuing the line of reinforce each other and should both
by the time that child reaches first research that has demonstrated the be introduced into children's lives as
grade (see, e.g.. Chamberlain & May- benefits of early exposure to ASL (e.g.. soon as possible (Lonigan, Farver,
berry, 2008; Mayberry, 2010; Mayer, Chamberlain & Mayberry, 2000; Corina Phillips, & Clancy-Menchetti, 2011;
2007) are a key reason why the average & Singleton, 2009; Freel et al., 2011; Sulzby & Teale, 1991; Whitehurst &
deaf child graduates from high school Mayberry, 2007) through educational Lonigan, 2001). This can be done
reading at the fourth-grade level through a variety of face-to-face activi-
media (Golos & Moses, 2011).
(Traxler, 2000). When they go to school, ties (e.g., read-alouds, dramatic play,
many deaf children are still learning direct instruction), environments (e.g.,
Theoretical Frameworks
language at the same time that their child care, preschool and early elemen-
for Literacy Development
hearing peers are learning through lan- tary classrooms, homes, community
Educational resources aiming to affect
guage (Freel et al., 2011; Paul, 2009). sites), and "old" and "new" media tools
language and literacy skills of young
These delays often lead to problems (e.g., books, computers, television and
children, including deaf children, must
with literacy throughout schooling videos). Improving the quality and
take into account the range of skills
(Chamberlain & Mayberry, 2008; Mayer, quantity of language and literacy expe-
constituting literacy (e.g., print aware-
2007). Even with the advent of new- riences and materials in the home and
ness, vocabulary, comprehension) that
born hearing screening and an increase in early education settings is critical for
emerge over time and that predict suc-
in the availability of amplification de- all children. For deaf children, who are
cess in literacy in the long run (i.e.,
vices, some researchers suggest that at risk for language and literacy failure,
emergent literacy skills). In addition,
deaf children are still at risk (e.g., Nit- a greater quantity and higher quality of
those working to increase children's
trouer, Caldwell, Lowenstein, Tarr, & language and literacy experiences are
acquisition of skills in this domain
HoUoman, 2012). This places great sig- imperative if they are to experience
should consider the range of modes
nificance on early language and literacy early literacy development and later
often tapped in screen mediaand the
exposure (Mayberry, 2007; Mayer, 2007; literacy success (Mayer, 2007; Mussel-
variety of texts that literate individuals
Paul & Wang, 2012; Williams, 2004). man, 2000; Williams, 2004).
use and produce (i.e., multiliteracies).
With the introduction of more
advanced technologies and a wider Emergent Literacy Emergent Literacy and
range of media products, a small body The emergent literacy perspective Deaf Children
of work has begun to examine the role posits that language and literacy skills Educators and researchers concerned
of media in promoting deaf children's develop from a child's earliest years. with deaf children may agree that early

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAE


exposure to language and literacy is prebension, and language functioning. include written and expressive/recep-
critical, yet tbere are different perspec- For example, Prinz and Strong (1998) tive communication (i.e., television,
tives on how best to promote develop- found a significant positive correlation videos, computers) and witb wbicb
ment in tbese areas. Some researcbers between ASL skills and Englisb literacy cbildren (and adults) interact regularly.
bave investigated tbe connection be- scores (e.g., on measures of compre- Studies examining educational
tween sound-based language exposure bension, vocabulary, Englisb syntax, media (e.g., Fiscb, 2004; Moses, 2008)
and early literacy development, and and written narrative) after testing 155 indicate tbe great potential of tbe use
found benefits (e.g., Kyle & Harris, deaf cbildren between tbe ages of 8 of multiple modes tbat can support
2010). Otbers bave examined tbe con- and 15 years. Additional, more recent cbildren's learning, and decades of
nection between sign language (bere, evidence bas continued to sbow a empirical work bas sbown tbat educa-
ASL) and English print exposure and positive correlation between ASL skills tional media can bave a positive impact
also found benefits to early literacy and reading comprebension scores, on bearing cbildren's literacy and lan-
development (e.g., Cbamberlain & wherein ASL skills or bilingual abilities guage development, including code-
Mayberry, 2000). Altbougb questions in ASL and printed Englisb explained related (e.g., word recognition, letter
remain about bow exactly deaf cbil- up to 68% of tbe variance in reading recognition) and comprebension-
dren learn to read, as well as tbe role comprebension scores for botb cbil- related skills (e.g., vocabulary, knowl-
pbonological awareness plays (Kyle & dren and adults between tbe ages of 4 edge of story elements). Anderson and
Harris, 2010; Mayer, 2007; McQuarrie and 62 years (Allen et al., 2009; Freel et Lorcb (1983) and otber researcbers
& PariUa, 2009; Musselman, 2000; Paul, al., 2011, Mayberry et al, 2011). bave found tbat viewers' own experi-
Wang, Trezek, & Luckner, 2009; Wang, One route to language and literacy is ences and background knowledge play
Trezek, Luckner, & Paul, 2008), many tbrough face-to-face, or live, interac- a role in wbetber and bow tbey attend
agree tbat early exposure to language, tions. The studies previously men- to and comprebend screen media.
vocabulary, and comprebension skills tioned investigated exposure to ASL Altbougb based on studies of bearing
is critical for deaf cbildren wbo are and outcomes for language and literacy cbildren, tbis evidence implies tbat
considered at risk in regard to later lit- skills involved in tbese types of inter- deaf cbildren need to pay attention to
eracy acbievement (Allen et al, 2009; actions. However, anotber route is and understand wbat tbey see in order
Clark, Gilbert, & Anderson, 2011; East- tbrougb media, wbicb can include a to actively learn.
erbrooks & Huston, 2008; Freel et al., range of modalities (e.g., auditory, sign, Paul and Wang (2012) agree that the
2011; Kyle & Harris, 2006; Mayberry et print) tbat can infiuence cbildren's lan- concept of literacy must be extended
al., 2011; Miller & Clark, 2011; Nit- guage and literacy development. to incorporate multimodal formats,
trouer et al, 2012; Paul, 1996, 2003).
sucb as tbose included in tecbnology.
Wben looking specifically at tbe use Multiliteracies Tbey emphasize tbe importance of
of ASL as means of fostering early lit- In recent years, some bave argued for developing "literate tbougbt," wbicb
eracy skills in young deaf cbildren, a broader notion of literacy, called tbey define as "tbe ability to access
researcbers bave found tbat early "multiliteracies," wbicb incorporates captured, decontextualized forms of
exposure to ASL and ASL abiUty posi- multiple languages, cultures, and media information" (p. 305). By using tbis
tively relate to early literacy skills (e.g., (New London Group, 1996). Tbe belief ability, cbildren can interact witb tbe
Cbamberiain & Mayberry, 2000; Freel is tbat literacy instruction and experi- "captured" information in ways not
et al., 2011; Mayberry, 2007). Evidence ences will only be effective if tbey possible during "live" interactions.
from the work of Cbamberlain and expand on a traditional definition to One example of tbis is using sign lan-
Mayberry (2000, 2008) and Hoffmeis- include tbe varying and multiple liter- guage in video or DVD format (Paul &
ter (2000), among otbers (e.g., Clark acy-related and linguistic competen- Wang 2006a, 2006b, 2012); a cbild or
et al., 2011; Corina & Singleton, 2009; cies tbat students bring to scbool. In adult can use tbis medium to pause,
Mayberry 2007, 2010; Prinz & Strong, sbort, tbe New London Group (1996) discuss, and revisit content presented
1998), suggests tbat many deaf cbil- argues tbat literacy instruction needs on screen in order to better under-
dren wbo bave early exposure to ASL to build upon students' knowledge of stand new information.
develop literacy skills on par witb nonconventional, as well as conven- Educational materials developed in
tbose of bearing peers on an array of tional, texts witbin a sociocultural ASL for tbe deaf population sbould
literacy outcomes, including perform- framework. Second, it needs to incor- build upon deaf learners' linguistic
ance on vocabulary tasks, reading com- porate tbe variety of screen media tbat (e.g., ASL) and cultural (e.g.. Deaf)

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


DEAF CHILDREN LEARNING FROM EDUCATIONAL MEDIA

backgrounds as well as employ multi- 2001). Children's viewing of educa- on educational programs, researchers
ple modes of communication (e.g., tional programming has been linked have found that literacy-oriented edu-
pictures, ASL, print, gesture) and to positive literacy outcomes during cational programs can positively and
research-based visual strategies. All of the preschool years (Wright et al, significantly affect certain language and
these considerations can be incorpo- 2001) as well as to positive attitudes literacy skills. Positive effectsboth
rated into technological formats (e.g., toward reading, good reading habits, short- and long-term (Anderson et al.,
DVDs). Multiple modes of communica- and reading achievement in adoles- 2001)have been found for skills crit-
tion in captured format should not cence (Anderson, Huston, Schmitt, ical to comprehending texts, such as
only grab children's attention but also Linebarger, & Wright, 2001). More vocabulary knowledge and familiarity
should include content at an appropri- specifically, spending time with educa- with story elements. (Eor a summary
ate level so that they can connect with, tional media does not relate to less of research, see Fisch, 2004; Moses,
understand, and learn from what they time spent engaging in other impor- 2008; and Uchikoshi, 2009). Such
see. An expanded view of literacy, tant activities such as reading, interact- results have been found when children
which incorporates multiple modes ing with others, and playing (Huston, have viewed programming on their
of literacy, aims to meet the needs of Wright, Marquis, & Green, 1999). The own (Huston et al., 1999), with adult
the individual learner (New London same has not been found with noned- mediation (e.g., Linebarger, 2009), and
Group, 1996), in this case the deaf ucational programming, in that nega- at home or in a classroom (e.g., St. Clair
learner, and specifically deaf learners tive or no effects have been found in & Schwetz, 2003). Programs that have
whose parents support exposure the short or long term. Educational a sound curriculum and refiect current
to ASL. media that capture and maintain chil- research benefit young (hearing) view-
dren's attentionthrough formal fea- ers by increasing their vocabulary, con-
tures (i.e., pacing, visual and sound cepts of print, development of oral
Review of the Literature
effects)as well as present content at language, letter recognition, knowl-
on Media, Literacy, and
an appropriate level facilitate chil- edge of story elements, and more
Deaf Learners
dren's understanding and learning, (Fisch, 2004). For example, with regard
Research has only begun to reveal how
including their acquisition of language to narrative skills acquisition as a result
media may provide deaf children with
and literacy of viewing educational programming,
support in language and literacy acqui-
311 preschool children were randomly
sition, in addition to support during
assigned to one of three conditions:
face-to-face interactions. Yet the evi- Media's Effects on Literacy
viewing educational programming
dence that does exist is promising, and Language Development
that involved a narrative format, view-
and further work is needed to exam- In general, media have the ability to
ing educational programming that
ine how and why educational media incorporate a range of modes of com-
involved an expository format, or a no-
can benefit young deaf children and munication as well as make important
treatment condition. Participants in
can do so through various modes of connections between those modes,
the two viewing conditions watched
communication. The present study and these different modes can convey
the selected programs multiple times.
focuses specifically on how this can be new information and skills related to
To evaluate sequencing skills, children
achieved through educational media literacy. Eor example, the meaning of a
were given three pictures (already
in ASL. new word can be shown through char-
seen in literacy workbooks) and asked
acters' physical actions and facial
to put them in correct order and tell
Media's Effects on Learning expressions, can be spoken, can be
the story Children were also asked
As previously mentioned, research on conveyed through pictures or print
five comprehension questions to eval-
children and media highlights the on screen, or can be communicated
uate narrative comprehension. Results
importance of children's attention to through sign language. Neuman (1995,
showed an increase in both story
the screen as well as their ability to 2009) and others have argued that
knowledge and narrative skills, but
comprehend what they see. With an using multiple modes to represent
only for participants who viewed
appropriate level of attention and com- the same information is more benefi-
educational programming that in-
prehension, viewers can actively learn cial to learners than relying on only
volved a narrative format (Linebarger
from media, particularly educational one mode.
& Piotrowski, 2009).
media (Bickman, Wright, & Huston, Of the work that has been done

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


Deaf Children's Learning deaf 9-to-18-year-olds viewed multime- attending to text printed on the
From Educational Media dia stories on CD-ROM with print and screen. These behaviors increased
Recent evidence also supports the pictures, they had a significantly higher after the participants had watched the
potential of educational media to facil- level of story knowledge (measured video multiple times. (For example,
itate deaf children's language and liter- through story retellings) than those there was an increase of 25 signed tar-
acy skills (e.g., Loeterman et al, 2002; deaf children who viewed the stories geted vocabulary per child after the
Mueller & Hurtig, 2010). For example, in print only (Gentry Chinn, & Moul- third viewing.) This was the case for
in the Cornerstones study, research- ton, 2005). Although the researchers all of the participants, regardless of the
based literacy units were developed noted that a significant difference was type of program they attended (i.e..
for deaf and hard of hearing children not found when sign language was Total Communication, oral, ASL-Eng-
to accompany episodes o Between the added to the stories viewed on CD- lish). As in the studies mentioned ear-
Lions that were interpreted into sign ROM, this may have been due to the lier, educational videos seem effective
language. Eight teachers administered fact that the signing was computer in promoting vocabulary knowledge in
a unit to 32 deaf children in grades K-5 generated. The studies cited here deaf children. However, educational
that included exposure to the signed again highlight that multimedia mate- videos for deaf preschoolers have not
versions oBetween the Lions episodes rials can have a positive impact on been evaluated with regard to chil-
for 2 hours a day for 6-8 days. Targeted deaf children at various ages and on dren's previous exposure to ASL or the
skills included both vocabulary and another important skill (i.e., knowl- videos' impact on a broader range of
story grammar; however, vocabulary edge of story elements). key early language and literacy skills,
skills were the only aspect measured. Although research examining edu- including knowledge of stoiy elements.
Deaf children's vocabulary was en- cational media in ASL and preschool
hanced with exposure to video(s) that deaf children's literacy learning is lim- Strategies That Facilitate
provided signed support within the ited, very recent studies have shown Deaf Children's Learning
videos (Loeterman et al, 2002), and positive effects of an interactive educa- From Educational Media
the greatest gains were found for chil- tional video in ASL (with no sound). When the strategies that facilitate deaf
dren between the ages of 7 and 9 years Golos (2006, 2010a, 2010b) examined children's language and literacy skills
who were on a first-grade reading whether 25 preschool deaf children are being considered, effective strate-
level. The scores of children ages 9 and visually attended to, engaged with, and gies can be implemented during face-
above displayed an average 50% learned from an adventure-based edu- to-face interactions as well as in
increase from pretest to posttest. An cational video in ASL. The video incor- interactions recorded on video and
additional study involving 4 deaf chil- porated research-based strategies to other media. For instance, adults' use
dren between the ages of 2 and 5 elicit attention and engagement as well of strategies to interact with deaf chil-
years, indicated that videos in signed as research-based strategies used by dren during shared reading can be
language that were incorporated dur- Deaf adults to foster ASL skills and applied to media interactions, such as
ing shared reading led to increased literacy development (in written En- in technology-enhanced shared read-
vocabulary for those children (Mueller glish) in deaf children (Erting, Thu- ings. Use of e-books, for instance, was
& Hurtig, 2010). Although they are mann-Prezioso, & Bendict, 2000; Gale found to lead to greater time spent in
derived from a small body of work, & Schick, 2009; Maxwell, 1988; Pad- shared reading activities and increased
these results suggest that deaf chil- den, 1991). Results indicated that deaf sign vocabulary acquisition (Meuller
dren, like hearing children, can gain preschoolers attended to the 40- & Hurtig, 2010). Effective strategies
vocabulary knowledge through educa- minute video an average of 84% of the culled from face-to-face interactions
tionally oriented media. time and learned the vocabulary words include showing ASL and English print
The positive infiuence of educa- targeted in the video, with statistically at the same time, using "sandwiching"
tional media has also been found in significant gains found from pretest to and/or "chaining" when signing a
studies of older deaf children who posttest. Participants also engaged in a word, pointing to the English print, fin-
engaged with multimedia materials, multitude of literacy behaviors during gerspelling the word and signing it
particularly multimedia storytelling their viewings of the video, including again (Akamatsu & Andrews, 1993; Ert-
(Andrews & Jordan, 1998; Ju, 2009; sequencing, predicting, signing, fin- ing, 2001; Erting & Pfau, 1997; Padden
Snoddon, 2010). For example, when gerspelling targeted vocabulary, and & Ramsey 1998). Additional strategies

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


DEAF CHILDREN LEARNING FROM EDUCATIONAL MEDIA

Deaf adults use during "live" activities setting) targeted within the moderate to severe hearing loss, 2
include facial expression, role-play, video? were reported as having severe hear-
and group and individual eye gaze 2. Does the impact of the video ing loss, 1 was reported as having
(Blumenthal-Kelly, 1995; Erting, 2001; vary by children's baseline ASL severe to profound hearing loss, and
Erting & Pfau, 1997; Mather, 1989). skills? 11 were reported as having profound
Recent efforts have involved the hearing loss. (Hearing status was not
implementation of effective face-to- A quasi-experimental, pretest-posttest reported for 8 participants.)
face strategies in recorded inter- design was used to address the two Six of the 31 children had cochlear
actions, with the specific goal of research questions. implants, 16 used hearing aids, and 5
improving young deaf children's lan- were reported to use no amplifica-
guage and literacy skills (Golos, 2006, Method tion. Parents of 4 participants did not
2010a, 2010b; Golos & Moses, 2011). Participants complete the item on mode of ampli-
Along with educational goals, the Participants in the present study fication.
development of educational media in included a convenience sample of 3-to- Fifteen children had been identified
ASL and subsequent studies has been 6-year-old deaf students (A'^ = 31) and at birth as having some degree of hear-
framed by theories of emergent liter- their teachers (N = 7). The teachers ing loss, 5 had been identified between
acy and multiliteracies and has inte- and their students were recruited from the ages of 1 and 3 years, and 3 had
grated research-based strategies that preschool programs for the deaf and been identified at or above age 3.
promote deaf children's vocabulary hard of hearing in the United States (Information on age of identification
skills and literacy-related behaviors and Canada that incorporated ASL in was not provided for 8 children.)
during viewing. As a result of these some way into the classroom. The set- With regard to the age at which chil-
efforts, initial studies found that these tings were residential, self-contained, dren began to sign, the average was
same strategies can be used in educa- and day programs. While 37 children 2.4 years (SD = 1.2 years). Five partic-
tional media with positive results were initially recruited and had ipants had begun to sign by their first
(Golos, 2006, 2010a, 2010b; Golos & parental consent to participate, due to birthday, 13 between their first and
Moses, 2011). However, questions the hearing status of 2 of the children third birthdays, and 5 between their
remained about the effects of educa- (i.e., hearing with Deaf parents) and third and fourth birthdays.
tional media on preschool deaf chil- missing data for 4 participants, the final At the time of the present study, 7
dren's acquisition of a broader range of participant pool for this investigation children were reported to know 0-100
early literacy skills. was 31, of whom 16 were boys and 15 words, 4 were reported to know
were girls. All seven of the participat- 101-400 words, and 4 were reported
Purpose of the Study ing teachers were female. According to to know 401 words or more. (Informa-
The purpose of the present study was the surveys completed by parents of tion on 16 children was not provided.)
to investigate the extent to which deaf the participants, there were 6 partici- Finally, parents who completed the
children who are exposed to ASL in pants each who were ages 3, 4, and 5 survey reported on the type of com-
the home and/or school gain the spe- years old, and 7 participants who were munication they (as the parent) used
cific language and literacy skills tar- 6 years old. (Parents did not report an at home. Two reported using ASL (n
geted in an educational DVD series age for 6 participants.) = 2), 2 oral communication, and 19 a
created for deaf preschool-aged chil- Parents also reported on hearing combination of ASL and oral. (Infor-
dren. Two research questions guided status, use and type of amplification, mation on 8 children was not pro-
the study: age at identification, the age at which vided.) Parents varied in their
the child began to sign, and the num- (self-reported) years of signing, rang-
1. Does viewing an educational ber of signs known by the child at the ing from less than 1 year (n = 7) to
video in ASL positively impact time of the present study. 1-2 years (n = 5) to more than 2
deaf preschoolers' acquisition of With regard to hearing status, 2 chil- years of signing (n = S). (Information
vocabulary words in ASL and dren were reported to have a mild was not provided by the parents of 11
printed English as well as story hearing loss, 1 child was reported to children.) While not all parents used
elements (identifying and have mild to moderate hearing loss, 2 ASL in the home, all children were
sequencing of main events and were reported to have moderate hear- exposed to ASL in the classroom to
identifying main characters and ing loss, 4 were reported as having some extent.

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


Materials reflects the trip. Each video concludes
4)
were asked to indicate their answer by
The videos used in the present study with the main character signing the pointing to one of four pictures on the
came from an original educational newly created storybook aloud. screen.
media series called Peter's Picture, Ten vocabulary words are incorpo- Prior to taking the assessment, chil-
available on DVD (only), that features rated into each video, with five empha- dren are given an expressive/receptive
characters who go on themed-based sized for instruction, as recommended vocabulary "screener" in which 20 com-
adventures. Each video has the same by researchers (Caldern, 2005), and mon vocabulary picture cards are used.
structure, with the only differences each word is related to a theme, as pre- This portion of the assessment is
being the focal theme and the vocabu- viously mentioned. Vocabulary and administered "live"; that is, the admin-
lary words surrounding that theme. other targeted skills are repeatedly istrator shows a participant each of the
One of the videos in the series (Golos, presented in both ASL and printed 20 picture cards individually, and asks
2005) had been previously evaluated English, and several strategies are the child to provide the sign that corre-
for its effectiveness in capturing and employed to increase learning, as sponds to the picture card. If the child
maintaining the visual attention of deaf highlighted earlier (i.e., showing ASL cannot produce a sign (expressive
children ages 3-6 years (Golos, 2006, and English at the same time, asking vocabulary) for any particular card, the
2010a, 2010b), and its physical demon- viewers to sign along with the charac- cards are shuffled. Then, the adminis-
strations of engagement related to the ters, etc.). (For a further description of trator places four cards (including the
literacy and ASL content (e.g., signing the structure of each video, see Golos target card), signs the target vocabu-
and fingerspelling, along with charac- & Moses, 2011). lary word, and asks the child to point to
ters, commenting and asking ques- the correct picture (receptive vocabu-
tions about the content of the video; Measures lary). If a child misses 5 or more out of
see above under "Review of the Litera- Participants were assessed at two time the 20 words (total, including recep-
ture"). Extant evidence demonstrated pointsprior to viewing one of the tive vocabulary words), that child does
that deaf children at this age could videos (pretest) and after watching the not go on to the main, video-based part
attend to, comprehend, and learn video on two occasions (posttest). of the test.
from a video of this length and struc- Pretest assessments included a meas- This assessment has undergone two
ture. The videos in the present study ure of participants' general ASL skills rounds of pilot testing (as reported in
had similar length, structure, charac- by means of the ASL Receptive Skills Enns & Herman, 2011) to adapt and
ters, and curriculum. Test (Enns, Zimmer, Broszeit, & Rabu, revise the already standardized and
Three videos were used in the pres- 2013) and a researcher-developed normed British Sign Language: Recep-
ent study, each running approximately assessment that measured the skills tive Skills Test (Herman, Holmes &
40 minutes in ASL (with no sound) and targeted in the videos, ihc Peter's Pic- Woll, 1999), and it is currently being
targeting key early ASL and literacy ture Assessment Tool (PPAT), which is normed for deaf children (as indicated
skills (i.e., vocabulary, letter recogni- specific to the video series. During the in Enns & Herman, 2011).
tion, concepts of print, comprehen- posttest time point, participants com-
sion, and grammatical features of ASL). pleted the PPAT for a second time. Assessment of Targeted
During every live-action video in the (Program-Specific) ASL
Peter's Picture series, the main charac- Baseline ASL Receptive Skills and Early Literacy Skills
ter (a Deaf adult) and his sidekick (a The ASL Receptive Skills Test was used Participants also completed an assess-
hearing raccoon) take four deaf chil- in the present study to obtain a base- ment we had developed, the Peter's
dren on a trip; in this study, trips line measure of the children's ASL Picture Assessment TooL The PPAT is
included visits to a pizza parlor, a receptive skills. The test is video based also a video-based assessment, in
library, and a country backyard. Prior to to ensure the consistency of instruc- which a (filmed) Deaf adult fluent
the trip, the main character discusses tions and test items signed by a native in ASL introduces and administers
what they will see, including objects ASL signer. There are three practice instructions and each item in ASL on
that represent each target vocabulary sentences and 45 test items, which the video. With respect to the targeted
words (e.g., library card, librarian). assess the following grammatical areas: skills, the PPAT assesses vocabulary
Upon returning, they sequence pic- number/distribution, noun/verb dis- words and knowledge of story ele-
tures of events that took place during tinctions, spatial verbs, classifiers, role ments (including sequencing main
their trip and create a storybook that shift, and conditionals. Participants events and identifying events, plot, set-

VoLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAE


DEAF CHILDREN LEARNING FROM EDUCATIONAL MEDIA

ting, and characters) that are explicitly ments (e.g., "What happened when examine statistical differences between
introduced and repeated in the video [sidekick character] left the door open pretest and posttest scores.
series. With respect to target vocabu- and the chicken escaped?"). The story
lary knowledge, the assessment tool elements subtest has a total possible Reliability and Validity
was designed to be structured similarly score of 4. Each participant's score for of the PPAT
to other standardized language meas- this subtest was calculated by summing The PPAT was specifically created to
ures (e.g., Peabody Picture Vocabulary the number of correct answers across study the impact of the video sedes on
Test; L. M. Dunn & D. M. Dunn, 2007), the four items. deaf viewers. That is, it aims to assess
and includes items that measure partic- In the final part of the PPAT, children the ASL and early literacy skills specif-
ipants' understanding of the targeted are asked to correctly sequence events ically targeted in the video (i.e., pro-
vocabulary words in the modalities of that occur in the video using five lami- gram-specific skills). The instrument
(a) sign to picture (10 items), (b) fin- nated picture cards, each representing was not created to measure general-
gerspelling to picture (10 items), (c) a different main event from the story ized (i.e., more comprehensive) ASL
sign to print (10 items), and (d) finger- (similar to the pictures presented in the or early literacy skills, such as those
spelling to print (10 items). A practice sequencing portion of the video). The that might be measured by a standard-
question is included for each of the sequencing subtest has a total possible ized assessment of early literacy for
vocabulaiy subtests. When children score of 5 (1 point for each picture deaf children. Because deaf children
are completing each vocabulary item, being placed in the correct order). Par- have varied and often limited expres-
the (filmed) Deaf adult asks them to ticipants were scored on the number of sive skills, the PPAT was developed as
choose from one of four pictures to pictures they chose in the correct "posi- a receptive measure and designed to
identify the picture that matches the tion" (what happened first, second, align with measures previously used to
signed (and fingerspelled) concept. evaluate the effects of educational
third, fourth, and fifth in the story), and
The four choice are (a) a picture that media on narrative skills. Eor example,
the total number of correct answers was
shows the target vocabulary word in previous studies (e.g., Linebarger &
summed for their sequencing subtest
{e.g., pizza, signed "ZZa"), (b) a pic- Piotrowski, 2009), children were asked
scores.
ture that shows an item that is signed fewer than 10 total questions on
similarly to the targeted concept (e.g., sequencing and story knowledge. The
Calculating the PPAT Total Score
snake), (c) a picture that shows an PPAT has 9 questions for these areas.
The total PPAT score, which included
object that looks similar to the tar- The PPAT was reviewed by experts in
all vocabulary, story elements, and
geted concept (e.g., pie), and (d) a pic- the field, including ASL development
sequencing items, was calculated for
ture that shows a distracter item (e.g., and preschool curriculum specialists,
data analysis. Because the subtest
a drink in a glass). The vocabulaiy sub- with respect to the constructs meas-
scores varied in the possible range (i.e.,
test has a total possible score of 40, ured (vocabulary and story elements),
40 possible points for the vocabulary
and each participant's score on this the content addressed across the
subtest, 4 possible points for story ele-
subtest was included by summing the items, and the procedures through
ments subtest, etc.), each participant's
number of correct answers across the which participants' vocabulary knowl-
subtotal scores were weighted, using
four sections that measured target edge and knowledge of story elements
percent correct scores, in order to cal-
vocabulary (sign to picture, finger- were obtained. Feedback was taken
culate the PPAT total score. That is, a
spelling to picture, sign to print, and into account, and measures were mod-
participant's total score for the vocab-
ngerspelling to print). ified accordingly
ulary subtest was divided by 40 to cal-
The second part of the PPAT involves culate a percentage correct vocabulary In addition, the PPAT was piloted on
asking participants questions about ele- subtest score; the total score for the six deaf and hard of hearing preschool
ments of the story that they viewed in story elements subtest was divided by children and their parents in the
the video. This subtest includes four 4; the total score for the sequencing home. At pretest and posttest, parents
items, for which children choose one subtest was divided by 5. Then, all of administered a version of the PPAT that
correct picture from a total of four pic- the percentage correct subtest scores was not video-recorded, and parents
tures, and the items focus on children's were summed together to calculate a and their child viewed the videos three
recognition of main characters (e.g., weighted PPAT total score for each par- times within 1 week. At the end of the
"Which one of these pictures is [main ticipant. The weighted PPAT total score week, parents participated in inter-
character's name] ?") and key plot ele- for each participant was then used to views and completed surveys to pro-

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


vide feedback on the clarity and appro- dren's exposure to ASL). The parent the conclusion of the second viewing,
priateness of the test items as well as also completed items about the child's the PPAT was administered for a sec-
the clarity and ease of administration television and video viewing habits ond time, with the same procedure
procedures. The measure was modi- (average number of hours of viewing used as in the pretest administration.
fied on the basis of this feedback; for per day, favorite programs, etc.).
example, several parents commented Data Analysis Plan
that some of the pictures were too Procedure Data were entered in Microsoft Excel
small or unclear. These pictures were After gaining teacher and parent per- and then imported into SPSS 18 so that
replaced and the images made bigger missions, and after the completed descriptive and inferential statistics
and clearer. In addition, to ensure con- parent questionnaires were received, could be generated. Data were first
sistency across test administrations, participating teachers as well as analyzed descriptively to provide
the instrument was adapted into a research assistants attended a 1-hour measures of central tendency (means)
DVD version. The current PPAT (in information session to learn about the and spread (range, standard devia-
DVD format) includes the instructions, background and purpose of the proj- tions) of the main variables (see
practice items, and test items signed ect. The lead researcher demonstrated Results and Table 1) and to enable
by a Deaf adult fluent in ASL, whose how to show one of the videos to par- examination of demographic and back-
signing was recorded and played back ticipating children. Other than show ground characteristics of participants
on a computer monitor during admin- the video in its entirety on two occa- (reported earlier and in Results). Then,
istration of the PPAT. sions, teachers and research assistants a 2 (time: pretest, posttest) x 2 (ASL
Finally, statistics were calculated to were instructed not to mediate the baseline groups: low/medium, high)
examine (a) concurrent validity and viewing experience, that is, not to split-plot ANOVA was conducted, with
(b) internal consistency of the PPAT. A encourage children to interact during the dependent variable being the
correlation analysis was conducted the viewings and not to focus or expand weighted PPAT total scores, to address
between participants' scores on the upon the video content before, during, both research questions. First, the
PPAT (pretest) and on the ASL Recep- or after the viewings. (For results of a main effect of the analysis addressed
tive Skills Test (pretest) to address study involving teacher mediation of the first research question, which
concurrent validity The correlation the videos, see Golos & Moses, 2011). could be summarized as "Do partici-
coefficient was significant at .79 (p < The teachers and research assistants pants' pretest PPAT total scores differ
.001), which indicates a strong rela- also learned how to administer the ASL significant from their posttest PPAT
tionship between scores on the two Receptive Skills Test and the PPAT. total scores?" Second, the interaction
instruments. In addition, a Cronbach's One week prior to viewing the effect addressed the second research
alpha statistic was calculated to ex- video, the teachers or a member of the question, that is, whether participants'
amine the internal consistency of the research team assessed participating initial ASL skill level interacted with
PPAT. The alpha coefficient reached children individually on the ASL Recep- their PPAT total scores (e.g., did partic-
"good" consistency for PPAT at pretest tive Skills Test. Then, the day before ipants who scored low/medium on the
(a = .840) as well as posttest (a = .885). the first video viewing, the teacher or ASL Receptive Skills Test significantly
a researcher administered the PPAT to differ from pretest to posttest as com-
Parent Survey participants individually pared to participants who scored high
In addition to the child measures, a During the viewing session, teach- on the ASL Receptive Skills Test?). In
parent or guardian of each participant ers showed one video to groups of order to determine initial ASL skill
was asked to complete a questionnaire three to five participating children in a level, participants were categorized
that provided demographic informa- space separated from nonparticipating into groups by their score on the ASL
tion (i.e., child's age, parent's level of children. They explained that the par- Receptive Skills Test, which was com-
education) as well as information ticipants would be watching a video pleted at pretest. When they were sep-
about the child's hearing status and and that normal classroom behavioral arated into three approximately equal
the mode of communication used in rules should be followed. Participants groups (low = 10, medium = 9, and
the home (i.e., degree of hearing loss; watched one of the videos twice in 1 high = 11; recall that one participant
age at identification; parent's, or par- week. Two of the videos in the series did not complete the ASL Receptive
ents', signing skills; parental hearing were viewed by two groups, and one Skills Test), no significant difference
status; early intervention; and chil- video was viewed by three groups. At was found between the low and

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


DEAF CHILDREN LEARNING FROM EDUCATIONAL MEDLA.

medium groups with regard to the Table 1


total PPAT scores at pretest, t(17) = Mean Scores and Standard Deviations at Pretest and Posttest on (Weighted) PPAT
-.056,p = .956, or at posttest, (17) = Total Scores for Participants Scoring Low/Medium or High (A/= 30) on the ASL
Receptive Skills Test
-.001, p - .999. However, significant
differences were found between the ASL level Administration time point M SD Range (minimum, maximum)
low and high groups at pretest, ;(19) = Low/mediumj Pretest 0.38 0.22 0.68 (.05, .73)
-2.98,p = .008, and posttest, (19) = (n=19) Posttest 0.70 0 55 2.25 (0.05, 2 30)
-2.358, p = .029, as well as the High Pretest 0.75 0.34 1.15(0.30, 1.45)
medium and high groups at those time (1=11) Posttest 1.31 0.49 1.63(0.53,2.15)

points: pretest, i(18) = -2.761, p = Notes. PPAT = Peter's Picture Assessment Tool ASL = American Sign Language.

.013, and posttest, t(18) = -5.019,p =


.007. Therefore, the between-subjects
variable for the split-plot ANOVA for the low/medium and the high ASL key early literacy and ASL skills. Based
involved comparing two groups: skills groups (see Figure 2). on prior research related to media
low/medium (scoring between 0 and effects on hearing as well as young
11 correct on the ASL Receptive Skills Discussion deaf children, it was expected that
Test, n = 19) and high (scoring be- Early exposure to language and literacy even a small level of exposure would
tween 12 and 29 correct on the ASL is critical for all children, but particu- have a positive impact on the targeted
Receptive Skills Test, n =11). Table 1 larly for deaf children who are at risk of skills of deaf preschoolers.
includes the ranges, mean scores, and literacy failure. A variety of interven- Vocabulary, for example, is a critical
standard deviations at pretest and tions have been created to increase the skill for literacy development and suc-
posttest for the two ASL baseline language and literacy skills of this pop- cess. Previous research indicates that
groups. ulation, and certain strategies have deaf preschoolers can learn five tar-
been found to be effective. In the pres- geted vocabulary words in both ASL
Results ent study, the Peter's Picture educa- and printed English after viewing one
Results from the split-plot ANOVA tional media series was used to target video from the educational series used
showed a significant main effect: The
participants' pretest weighted PPAT
total score significantly differed from Figure 1

their posttest weighted PPAT total Means and Confidence Intervals (CIs) for Participants' Pretest and Posttest Scores
(Total) on the Peter's P/cfure Assessment Tool (PPAT)
score, i?(l, 28) = 23.32, j5 < .001, Tip^
= .454. Mean scores indicated that the
1.2-
average weighted posttest score (M =
.51, SD = .33) was higher than the
average weighted pretest score (M =
.90, SD = .60). (See Figure 1 for a dis-
play of the means and confidence
intervals at pretest and posttest.)
As mentioned above in the discus- 0.8-

sion of the data analysis plan, partici-


pants were categorized by their score
on the ASL Receptive Skills Test o.a-
(low/medium vs. high). The split-plot
ANOVA revealed no significant interac-
tion between baseline ASL scores and 0.4-
weighted PPAT total scores, F(l, 28) =
1.61, p = .215, Tip^ = .054 (note the
small and unequal subsample size). 0.2-
Figure 2 displays the weighted pretest
and posttest PPAT total mean scores PretertPPAT Posttest PPAT

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


Figure 2 hension skills, and educational videos
Pretest and Posttest (Weighted) PPAT Total Mean Scores by ASL (Baseline) Skill may provide an additional medium to
Level (Low/Medium, High)
support strategies already used in the
classroom.
Although previous research sug-
gests that children learn after viewing
1.2S-'
educational media regardless of the
communication modality used in
school programs (Golos, 2006, 2010a,
2010b; Golos & Moses, 2011), the pres-
ent study is the first that specifically
ASL Skill Level
measured children's baseline ASL
Low/Medium
skills. Although there was great vari-
0.7SH
ability in scores on the PPAT (range,
standard deviation), this is not surpris-
ing given the high variability of scores
0.SO- on the ASL Receptive Skills Test (i.e.,
participants' baseline ASL skiU level),
and it was important to consider this
variation when examining learning
Q.2S-
from the video (see research question
2 and analysis). It is also not surprising
Pretest Posttest that children with higher baseline ASL
Time Point skills scored higher on the PPAT than
children with lower baseline skills. At
Notes. PPAT = Peter's Picture Assessment Tool. ASL = American Sign Language. present it remains unknown how
adding sound to the video could affect
learning, yet it is interesting that all of
in the present investigation (Golos, before and after viewing, and results the children were able to learn tar-
2006; Golos, & Moses, 2011). Results showed a significant increase from geted skills in a format without sound
from the present study provide addi- pretest to posttest. The fact that, on and did so regardless of their baseline
tional evidence that deaf preschoolers average, children were able to identify ASL skills.
can learn other vocabulary words and key characters, settings, and story
recognize and demonstrate target events, and to sequence main events Limitations of the Study
words in a range of modesin pictures, in the correct order, indicates that they One limitation of the present study is
fingerspelling, and signing, as well as in understood the storyline of the se- that the measures did not include use
print. However, prior to this study, litfle lected video. of a standardized early literacy instru-
was known about the series' impact on To the best of our knowledge, the ment before and after viewing of a
literacy skills other than vocabulary, and present study is the first to show evi- video. As a result, the study's results
the question remained whether videos dence of media having a positive effect cannot be generalized beyond the
that use research-based strategies could on aspects of preschool deaf children's skills targeted in the videos and
enhance aspects of deaf preschoolers' literacy skills other than vocabulary. measured with the PPAT. Furthermore,
knowledge of story elements in addi- Although it has long been known that there are additional skills that are
tion to vocabulary knowledge. comprehension skills are crucial for lit- incorporated both within and across
The videos used in the present eracy development, only in the last few each video that have not yet been
study targeted a number of compo- decades have steps been taken to facil- measured (i.e., letter recognition, con-
nents related to understanding a story itate the development of comprehen- cepts of print, and grammatical con-
(i.e., characters, settings, story events, sion skills at the preschool level. It is structions in ASL). A future step could
and sequencing). Knowledge of tar- critical that early childhood educators be to expand the PPAT to include
geted story elements was assessed facilitate the development of compre- assessment of these additional skills in

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


DEAF CHILDREN LEARNING FROM EDUCATIONAL MEDL\

addition to examination of the transfer video, there was no formal check of sound-based, strategies used in the
of learning to a standardized early liter- fidelity of the implementation. videos. Future studies could also ana-
acy assessment. With regard to the skills assessed in lyze the impact of viewing this educa-
Related to the measurements, both the present study, there are also skills tional video series with and without
the PPAT and the ASL Receptive Skills embedded across videos that children sound on additional literacy skills, such
Test used a video format, which allowed might learn with greater exposure. For as letter recognition, concepts of print,
for consistency across administrations example, in addition to the five tar- and emergent writing, and include a
(i.e., all items were presented by the geted theme-related vocabulary more in-depth examination of finger-
onscreen Deaf adult fiuent in ASL). words, five additional literacy-related spelling as a bridge to English and as a
The only responsibility the teacher or words are incorporated into each decoding tool. Such research would
member of the research team had video (tide, sentence, word, page, produce much-needed information on
when administering the assessments story). Finally, although participants strategies to facilitate the development
was to document the participants' made significant improvements in the of deaf children's early literacy skills. If
responses to each item on a prepared short term (approximately 1 week), a growing body of evidence continues
scoring sheet. Although they were the design of the present investigation to support the use of multimedia
videotaped and viewed by the re- cannot speak to whether participants learning with deaf children, this could
searchers, there was no formal check retained the targeted skills after the have a direct effect on literacy instruc-
for the two items within the PPAT story experiment concluded (i.e., there was tion for this population.
elements subtest for interrater reliabil- no check for longer-term maintenance Recent studies have also shown that
ity. That is, the onscreen adult asked of learning) or whether they could children may learn more when teach-
for a signed response from the child learn skills from exposure to multiple ers scaffold their knowledge by en-
and responses ranged from one word videos from the series, as opposed to couraging interacdon with language
to one short sentence. Questions just one. Future researchers should and literacy elements during educa-
regarding scoring on these two items consider continuing this line of exper- tional media encounters (e.g., Golos &
(i.e., whether a participant received a imental work with the inclusion of a Moses, 2011; Linebarger, 2009). Based
"1," correct, or "0," incorrect) were control group and/or delayed posttests on that research, it is anticipated that
referred to us before final scoring was of the targeted skills. the growth of these skills will increase
recorded and entered. if teachers integrate viewing into the
Another set of limitations relates to Implications and Directions classroom over an extended period
the present study's design. A pretest/ for Future Research and are provided with materials to sup-
posttest design provided the ability to The results of the present study pro- plement learning during and between
compare participants' changes in scores vide a number of potential avenues for viewings. To extend this line of future
as a result of viewing the stimulus (two future research. While the study's research, it would also be worth
viewings of the video); however, the results relate only to deaf children who exploring whether and how the strate-
study did not include a control condi- have had at least some exposure to gies that are embedded within the
tion to compare viewing participants ASL, participants varied in hearing loss video series connect with and carry
to similar nonviewing participants. and use of amplification yet generally over into teachers' daily lesson plans.
This limits the generalizability of the learned the targeted skills and do so Informal data from participating teach-
results. In addition, results from this through visual strategies used in the ers' comments during previous studies
study show benefits from viewing one videos (rather than sound-based (e.g., Golos & Moses, 2011) indicate
video on two occasions in a 1-week strategies). It would be informative to the potential to further increase lan-
period. Therefore, another limitation is examine the benefits of the educa- guage and literacy learning by applying
that the effects of viewing a video more tional video series with and without strategies demonstrated in the videos.
than a handful of times and/or over a sound using a larger randomized sam- Even if program-specific words, for
longer period of time are unknown. ple of children who utilize amplifica- instance, may not directly connect
Also, although each teacher and mem- tion devices (e.g., cochlear implants) with classroom lessons or units, im-
ber of the research team was trained on in comparison to those who do not plementing the media series may
how to structure each viewing session use them. Participants in this study encourage teachers to use some of
with participants and used the same set generally learned the targeted skills, the same strategies (e.g., chaining) to
of directions to introduce and show the doing so through visual, rather than target other vocabulary words and

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


early literacy skills (e.g., during read- and bave tbe potential tofilla gap in of the Societyfor Research in Child Develop-
alouds). tbe landscape of curricular materials ment, 66(1, Serial No. 264).
Anderson, D. R., & Lorch, E. P (1983). Looking
for teacbers of tbe deaf For deaf cbil- at television: Action or reaction? InJ. Bryant
Conclusion dren witb bearing parents, wbo may & D. R. Anderson (Eds.), Children^s under-
Cbildren, bearing and deaf, are living not bave access to incidental learning standing of television: Research on atten-
tion and comprehension (pp. 1-33). New
in a world rieb witb multimedia tecb- opportunities and/or fiuent language York, NY: Academic Press.
nology, and tbe potential exists to use models, tbe results of tbe present Andrews, J., Jordan, D. (1998). Multimedia sto-
tbis tecbnology to facilitate tbe devel- study suggest that certain early liter- ries for deaf children. Teaching Exceptional
opment of language and literacy skills acy skills, including vocabulary and Children,30{5), 28-33.
Bickham, D. S., Wright, J. C , & Huston, A. C.
of young deaf cbildren, wbo are at risk knowledge of story elements, may be (2001). Attention, comprehension, and the
for later reading failure. Witb tbis learned from engaging witb multi- educational influence of television. In D. G.
potential comes a need for caution media materials. Although tbese skills Singer & J. L. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of
children and media (pp. 101-119). Thou-
and empirical evidence: Multimedia are important for future literacy suc- sand Oaks, CA: Sage.
resources for deaf cbildren sbould be cess, tbey are not tbe only skills witbin Blumenthal-Kelly,A. (1995). Fingerspelling inter-
designed to incorporate multiple tbe realm of literacy development. action: A set of deaf parents and their deaf
researcb-based visual strategies and Witb evidence tbat media can be an daughter. In C. Lucas (Ed.), Sociolinguistics
in Deaf communities (pp. 62-73). Washing-
sbould be educational and of bigb effective tool for prescbool deaf cbil- ton, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
quality. Multimedia tbat provide fluent dren, results sucb as tbose of tbe pres- Caldern, M. (2005, October). Developing lan-
language models, bave a sound cur- ent study bave tbe potential to open guage, literacy, and content knowledge for
English language learners. Paper presented
riculum witb appropriate and targeted doors for future studies examining at the Hispanic Achievement Conference,
goals and objectives, and tbat employ additional components of language Raleigh, NC.
researcb-based strategies bave tbe and literacy. Chambedain, C, & Mayberry, R. (2000). Theoriz-
greatest potential to bave a positive ing about the relationship between American
Sign Language and reading. In C. Chamber-
impact on cbildren. However, tbey Notes lain, J. Morford, & R. Mayberry (Eds.), Lan-
sbould be used to supplement ratber 1. In tbe present article, we use deaf guage acquisition by eye (pp. 221-260).
tban replace face-to-face interactions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
to refer to anyone witb bearing loss Chamberlain, C, & Mayberry, R. (2008). ASL syn-
Tbe video series we bave discussed in (including people wbo are bard of tactic and narrative comprehension in
tbe present article provides one model bearing) and Deaf to refer to someone skilled and less skilled adult readers: Bilin-
for interventions to increase tbe emerg- wbo is considered to be part of tbe gual-bimodal evidence for the linguistic
ing literacy and ASL skills of deaf basis of reading. Applied Psycholinguistics,
Deaf community. 29, 368-388.
prescboolers, regardless of tbeir initial 2. Also, we use tbe term w'^w/Zan- Clark, M. D., Gilbert, G. L., & Anderson, M. L.
ASL skills. It also provides additional guage to refer to sign languages used (2011). Morphological knowledge and
support for tbe argument tbat deaf cbil- decoding skills of deaf readers. Psychology,
tbrougbout tbe worid; in tbe present 2,109-116.
dren can make important gains wben article, we focus on ASL, however. Corina, D., & Singleton, J. (2009). Develop-
exposed to ASL and researcb-based Tberefore, our use of tbe term does mental social cognitive neuroscience:
visual strategies tbrougb researcb- not include visual codes of language
Insights from deafness. Child Development,
80{4), 952-967. doi:10.1111/i.1467-8624.2009
tested educational media programs, (e.g.. Cued Speecb) or pbonology .01310.x
and tbat tbese media materials can con- (e.g.. Visual Pbonics). Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). PPVT-4 man-
tribute to a ricber literacy and language ual. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson.
environment in the classroom and at Durkin, D. (1966). Children who read early.
References New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
bome. Akamatsu, C. T., & Andrews, J. F. (1993). It takes Easterbrooks, S., & Huston, S. (2008). Signed
two to be literate: Literacy interactions reading fluency of students who are deaf/
Researcbers bave bigbligbted tbe between parent and child. Sign Language hard of hesi'mg. Journal ofDeaf Studies and
importance of developing effective Studies, 81, 333-360. Deaf Education, 13{1), 37-54. doi:10.1093/
curricula and curricular materials for Allen, T. E., Clark, M. D., del Giudice, A, Koo, D., deafed/enmO3O
Lieberman, A., Mayberry, R., et al. (2009).
deaf cbildren, botb for tbose wbo do Enns, C , & Herman, R. (2011). Adapting the
Phonology and reading: A response to Wang, Assessing British Sign Language Develop-
and tbose wbo do not bave access to Trezek, Luckner, and Paul. American Annals ment: Receptive Skills Tesi into American
sound (e.g., Easterbrooks & Huston, of the Deaf, 154{4), 338-345. Sign Language.yowrwa/ of Deaf Studies and
2008). Multimedia materials are one Anderson, D. R., Huston, A. C , Schmitt, K., Deaf Education, 16{3), 362-374. doi:10.1093/
Linebarger, D. L., & Wright, J. C. (2001). Eady deafed/enrO04
resource teacbers can use in tbe class- childhood television viewing and adolescent Enns, C , Zimmer, K., Broszeit, C , & Rabu, S.
room to facilitate early literacy skills. behavior: The recontact study Monographs (2013). Assessing ASL development: Recep-

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


DEAF CHILDREN LEARNING FROM EDUCATIONAL MEDIA

tive Skills Test. Winnipeg, Canada: Northern S. B. (1999). How children spend their time. in deaf readers: A meta-analysis./owr/ of
Signs Research. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 912-925. Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16(2),
Erting, L. (2001). Book sharing the Deaf way: An Ju, J. M. (2009). The effects of multimedia stories 164-188. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enq049
ethnographic study in a bilingual preschool of deaf or hard-of-hearing celebrities on the Mayer, C. (2007). What really matters in the early
for deaf children. Dissertation Abstracts reading comprehension and English words literacy development of deaf children./oMr-
International, 62(12), 4031A. learning of Taiwanese students with hearing nal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education,
Erting, L., & Pfau, J. (1997). Becoming bilingual: impairment. Asian Journal of Management /2(4), 411-431.
Facilitating English literacy development and Humanity Sciences, 4(2-3), 91-105. McQuarrie, L., & Parilla, R. (2009). Phonological
using ASL in preschool [Sharing Ideas series]. Kyle, E E., & Harris, M. (2006). Concurrent cor- representations in deaf children: Rethinking
Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Pre- relates and predictors of reading and spelling the "functional equivalence" hypothesis.
College National Mission Programs. achievement in deaf and hearing school chil- foumal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Educa-
Erting, C, Thumann-Prezioso, C, & Benedict, dren.Joumal ofDeaf Studies and Deaf Edu- tion, 14(2), 137-154. doi:10.1093/deafed/
B. (2000). Bilingualism in a Deaf family: Fin- cation, 11, 273-288. ennO25
gerspelling in early childhood. In P Spencer, Kyle, E E., & Harris, M. (2010). Predictors of Miller, P, & Clark, D. (2011). Phonemic aware-
C. Erting, & M. Marschark (Eds.), The deaf reading development in deaf children: A ness is not necessary to become a skilled
child in the family and at school Mahwah, 3-year longitudinal study, foumal of Experi- deaf reader, foumal of Developmental and
NJ: Erlbaum. mental Child Psychology, 107(i), 229-243. Physical Disabilities, 23(5), 459^76. doi: 10
Eisch, S. M. (2004). Children'sleamingfivm edu- Linebarger, D. (2009). Evaluation of the .1007/S10882-011-9246-0
cational television. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Between the lions Mississippi literacy initia- Morford, J. P, & Mayberry, R. I. (2000). A reexam-
Freel, B. L. Clark, M. D., Anderson, M. L., Gilbert, tive. Retrieved from Public Broadcasting Ser- ination of "Early Exposure" and its implica-
G., Musyoka, M. M., & Huser, P C. (2011). vice website: http://pbskids.org/read/files/ tions for language acquisition by eye. In C.
Deaf inclividuals' bilingual abilities: American BTL_Mississippi_April2009.pdf Chamberlain, J. Morford, & R. Mayberry
Sign Language proficiency, reading skills, and Linebarger, D., & Piotrowski,J. T. (2009). TV as (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye (pp.
family characteristics. Psychology, 2, 18-23. storyteller: How exposure to television nar- 111-128). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
doi:10.4236/psych.2011.21003 ratives impacts at-risk preschoolers' story Moses, A. M. (2008). Impacts of television view-
Gale, E., & Schick, B. (2009). Symbol-infused joint knowledge and narrative skills. British ing on young children's literacy develop-
attention and language use in mothers with foumal of Developmental Psychology, ment in the USA: A review of the literature.
deaf and hearing toddlers. American Annals 27(1), 47-69. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 8(1),
of the Deaf 153(5), 484-503. doi:10.1353/ Loeterman, M., Paul, P, & Donahue, S. (2002, 67-102.
aad.0.0066 Eebruar)'). Reading and deaf children. Read- Mueller, V, & Hurtig, R. (2010). Technology-
Gentry, M., Chinn, K., & Moulton, R. (2005). ing Online, 5(6). Retrieved from http://www enhanced shared reading with deaf and
Effectiveness of multimedia reading materials .readingonline.org/articles/art_index.asp?HR hard-of-hearing children: The role of a fluent
when u.sed with children who are deaf. Amer- EE=loeterman/index.html signing narrator, foumal of Deaf Studies
ican Annals of the Deaf, 149(5), 394-403. Lonigan, C.J., Farver,J. M., Phillips, B., &Clancy- and Deaf Education,15(l), 72-101. doi:10
doi:10.1353/aad.2005.0012 Menchetti, J. (2011). Promoting the devel- .1093/deafed/enp023
Golos, D. (2005). Peter's picture: Our trip to the opment of preschool children's emergent Musselman, C. (2000). How do children who
farm. Boulder, CO: Signing Sun. literacy skills: A randomized evaluation of a can't hear learn to read an alphabetic script?
Golos, D. (2006). Using instructional videos literacy-focused curriculum and two profes- A review of the literature on reading and
in American Sign Language as a tool to sional development models. Reading and deafness./owm/ of Deaf Studies and Deaf
facilitate the development of emergent liter- Writing Quarterly: An Interdisciplinary Education, 5(1), 9-31. doi:10.1093/deafed/
acy skills in deaf and hard of hearing pre- foumal, 24, 305-337. doi:10.1007/slll45 5.1.9
school children (Doctoral dissertation). -009-9214-6 Neuman, S. (1995). Literacy in the television
Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Mather, S. (1989). Visually oriented teaching age: The myth of the TV effect. Norwood, NJ:
Theses database. (UMI No. 3239427) strategies with Deaf preschool children. In C. Ablex.
Golos, D. (2010a). Deaf children's engagement Lucas (Ed.), The sociolinguistics of the Deaf Neuman, S. (2009). The case for multimedia pre-
in an educational video in American Sign community (pp. 165-187). New York, NY: sentations in learning: A theory of synergy. In
Language. American Annals of the Deaf Academic Press. A. Bus & S. Neuman (Eds.),Multimedia and
155(i), 360-368. Maxwell, M. (1988). The alphabetic principle of literacy development: Improving achieve-
Golos, D. (2010b). Literacy behaviors of deaf ngerspelling. Sign Language Studies, 61, ment for young learners (pp. 4456). New
preschoolers during video viewing. Sign Lan- 377^04. York, NY: Routledge.
guage Studies, 11(1), 76-99. Mayberry, R. (2007). When timing is everything: New Ix)ndon Group. (1996). A pedagogy of mul-
Golos, D. B., & Moses, A. M. (2011). How teacher Age of first-language acquisition effects on tiliteracies: Designing social futures. Har-
mediation during video viewing facilitates lit- second-language learning. Applied Psy- vard Educational Review, 66(1), 60-92.
eracy behaviors. Sign Language Studies, cholinguistics, 28, 537-549. doi:10.1017/ Nittrouer, S., Caldwell, A., Lowenstein,J. H., Tarr,
12(1), 98-118. S0142716407070294 E., & HoUoman, C. (2012). Emergent literacy
Herman, R., Holmes, S., & WoU, B. (1999). Av^ess- Mayberry, R. (2010). Eady language acquisition in kindergartners with cochlear implants.
ing British Sign Language development: and adult language ability: What sign lan- Ear and Hearing, 33(), 683-697. Retrieved
Receptive Skills Test. Coleford, England: Eor- guage reveals about the critical period for from Wolters Kluwer website: http://dx.doi
est Bookshop. language. In M. Marschark & P. Spencer .org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e318258c98e
Hoffmeister, R. J. (2000). A piece of the puzzle: (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, lan- Padden, C. (1991). The acquisition of finger-
ASL and reading comprehension in Deaf chil- guage, andeducation (Vol. 2, pp. 281-291). spelling by deaf children. In P Siple & S.
dren. In C. Chamberlain, J. Morford, & R. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Fischer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in sign
Mayberry (Eds.), Language acquisition by Mayberry, R. L, del Guidice, A. A., & Lieberman, language research: Vol. 2. Psychology
eye (pp. 143-163). Mahwah, NJ: Hdbaum. A. M. (2011). Reading achievement in rela- (pp. 191-210). Chicago, IL: University of
Huston, A. C, Wright, J. C, Marquis, J., & Green, tion to phonological coding and awareness Chicago Press.

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


Padden, C., & Ramsey C. (1998). Reading ability rejoinder. American Annals of the Deaf, Uchikoshi, Y (2009). Effects of television on lan-
in signing Deaf children. Topics in Lan- 154(4), 346-356. guage and literacy development. In A. G. Bus
guage Disorders, 18(4), 30-46. doi:10.1097/ Prinz, P, & Strong, M. (1998). ASL proficiency and & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Multimedia and lit-
00011363-199818040-00005 English literacy within a bilingual deaf educa- eracy development: Improving achieve-
Paul, P. (1996). Reading vocabulary knowledge tion model of instruction. Topics in Lan- ment for young learners (pp. 182-195). New
and dewiness. Journal of Deaf Studies and guage Disorders, 18(4), 47-60. doi:10.1097/ York, NY: Routledge.
Deaf Education, i ( l ) , 3-15. 00011363-199818040-00006 Wang, Y, Trezek, B. J., Luckner, J., & Paul, P V
Paul, P (2003). Processes and components of Rathmann, C, Wolfgang, M., & Morgan, G. (2008). The role of phonology and phono-
reading. In M. Marschark & P Spencer (Eds.), (2007). Narrative structure and narrative logically related skills in reading instruction
Handbook of deaf students, language, and development in deaf children. Z)efwe.ssanrf for students who are deaf or hard of hear-
education (pp. 97-109). New York, NY Education International, 9(4), 187-196. ing. American Annals of the Deaf, 153(4),
Oxford University Press. Snoddon, K. (2010). Technology as a learning 396-407.
Paul, P (2009). Language and deafness (4th tool for ASL literacy. Sign Language Studies Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2001). Emer-
ed.). Sudbuiy, MA: Jones & Bartlett. 10(2), 197-213. doi:10.1353/sls.0.0039 gent literacy: Development from pre-readers
Paul, P, & Wang, Y (2006a). Literate thought and St. Clair, J., & Schwetz, L. R. (2005). Between the to readers. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickensen
deafness: A call for a new perspective and Lions as a classroom tool. The Reading (Eds.), Handbook of early literaq,' research
line of research on literacy. Punjab Univer- Teacher, 56(7), 656-(559. (pp. 11-29). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
sityfoumal of Special Education [Pakistan], Sulzby, E., & Teale, W (1991). Emergent literacy. In Williams, C. (2004). Emergent literacy of deaf
2(1), 28-37. R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P Mosenthal, & P D. Pear- chiidren. foumal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
Paul, P, & Wang, Y (2006b). Literate thought and son (Eds.), Handbook of reading research Education, 9(4), 352-365. doi:10.1093/
multiple literacies. Theory Into Practice, (Vol. 2, pp. 727-758). New York, NY Longman. deafed/enhO45
45(4), 304-310. doi:10.1207/sl5430421tip Ttaxler, C. B. (2000). Measuring up to perform- Wright, J. C, Huston, A. C, Murphy, K. C, St.
4504_3 ance standards in reading and mathematics: Peters, M., Pinon, M., Scantlin, R., et al.
Paul, P, & Wang, Y, (2012). Literate thought: Achievement of selected deaf and hard-of- (2001). The relations of early television view-
Understanding comprehension and liter- hearing students in the national norming of ing to school readiness and vocabulary of
acy. Sudbury, MA: Jones & Bartlett. the ninth edition Stanford Achievement Test. children from low-income families: The Early
Paul, P, Wang, Y, Txezek, B., & Luckner,J. (2009). foumal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, Window Project. Child Development,
Phonology is necessaiy, but not sufficient. A 5(4), 337-348. doi:10.1093/deafed/5.4.337 1347-1366.

VOLUME 158, No. 4, 2013 AMERICAN ANNALS OF THE DEAF


Copyright of American Annals of the Deaf is the property of American Annals of the Deaf
and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without
the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or
email articles for individual use.

Potrebbero piacerti anche