Sei sulla pagina 1di 7

Randomness, Rules and Compositional Structure in Design

Author(s): Michael Eckersley


Source: Leonardo, Vol. 23, No. 1 (1990), pp. 75-80
Published by: The MIT Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1578469
Accessed: 15/04/2009 13:03

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=mitpress.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with the
scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that
promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

The MIT Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Leonardo.

http://www.jstor.org
GENERAL NOTE

Randomness,Rules and
CompositionalStructure in Design

Michael Eckersley

minde'Randomness is something 'serious- dence that order lies behind


ABSTRACT
minded' people seldom seek. It usually crops up as a dis- apparent confusion and that
tracting by-product of a more interesting primary objective. more common kinds of order
A common tendency in one's ordered daily life, either need not be sought at the ex- The paperexamineshow
conceptsof randomnessand
consciously or unconsciously, is to consider randomly occur- pense of less obvious kinds of chaos impingeon the creative
ring phenomena to be merely a waste of time, uninteresting order. The writer Isaac Bash- studyandpracticeof design.A
and of no practical value. This conditioned attitude is begin- evis Singer, when asked to com- bodyof originalworkis document-
ning to change in some scientific circles as a result of ment on the messy condition of ed involvingthe transformation
of
computer-generated randomnum-
research that has shown amazing, unexpected and complex his study, responded matter-of-
berstreamsintovisuallyfascinat-
forms of order in seemingly random physical phenomena. factly that chaos is intrinsic to ingandthought-provoking graphical
Such research falls under the general umbrella term of his subject matter; the disorder arrays.Asimplecomputer-based
'chaos', and much is currently being written about it. The of his study was simply a reflec- methodforgeneratinga vast num-
berof uniquegraphicshapesis
point I attempt to establish in this paper is that investigation tion of that chaos. The com-
described.Alsoexaminedis the
of the concepts of randomness and complexity is, and has poser Antonio Vivaldi, it is said, roleof ruledevelopment indesign
been for some time, the province of the creative artist as well was inspired to create an entire andthe degreeto whicheven
as of the scientist. For the artist is, by virtue of the nature of musical composition from the simplerulescandramatically
the discipline itself, intrinsically involved in issues of order, sequence of notes resulting influencethe appearanceof com-
positionalstructure.
compositional structure and form in all its varieties. when his cat leaped unexpect-
Who would not admit to having experienced a certain edly upon a keyboard. Other
aesthetic satisfaction in the apparent randomness of cloud such examples abound in the
formations, the fluttering of leaves in the wind, the arrhyth- literature of the arts and sci-
mic monotony of raindrops, or even the imprints of scat- ences where happenstance, unpredictability and seeming
tered leaves left behind on a sidewalk after a cold autumn disorder have spawned original and creative ideas. Admit-
rain?Justas there is in all of us the instinct to organize events tedly, the artist's conception of randomness and the scien-
and space, there also seems to be a contrary urge to disor- tist's conception of randomness are colored by two different
ganize events and spaces that appear to be too rigidly struc- paradigms, but these are not necessarily incompatible.
tured or confining. This chaos/order paradox has been, Though it is natural and understandable that human beings
directly and indirectly, the subject of much literature, music find comfort in the basic clarity and dependability of law-
and visual art [1]. In nature itself an analogous paradox governed, deterministic systems, many individuals seem
exists between natural shaping systems embodied in various genuinely to prefer the surprising, the unpredictable and
types of polygons, spheres, helixes, spirals, meanders and the original.
branching patterns, and the apparently random and cur-
iously fascinating natural events described above.
Scientific activity over the last 10 years has focused on RANDOMNESS AND CHAOS
such complex phenomena as the long-term behavior of Webster's dictionary defines randomness as "lacking a def-
weather, turbulence in fluid and chaotic vibration. Whereas inite plan, purpose, or pattern"; "being or relating to a set
in years past such phenomena would have been rejected as or to an element of a set each of whose elements has equal
hopelessly complex and consigned to the periphery of sci- probability of occurrence". The classic method of generat-
entific study, this attitude appears to be changing. Such ing a random sequence is to toss a coin repeatedly, tallying
changes hint of a new validation of other forms of investiga- the occurrence of heads and tails. However, a more useful
tion involving chaotic themes and informally structured conception or definition of randomness is offered by the
processes, namely, the work of many creative artists.The fact field of algorithmic information theory [3]. This definition
that chaotic phenomena in nature are inherently interest- of randomness takes into account only the characteristics of
ing is not news to them. Many artists (e.g. Marcel Duchamp, the random sequence itself, rather than the process from
Wassily Kandinsky,Jackson Pollock) have based entire car- which the sequence was generated. Consider the following
eers on the pursuit, visualization and understanding of such two sequences, both having 20 symbols and both consisting
complex life phenomena. only of the symbols '0' and '1'.
Artists have long been considered to exhibit a high toler-
ance for ambiguity and apparent complexity. Frank Bar- Michael Eckersley (designer, educator), University of Maryland, Department of Design,
ron's [2] psychological research in the 1950s indicated that 1401 Marie Mount Hall, College Park, MD 20742, U.S.A.

the creative artist characteristically demonstrates a confi- Received 13July 1988.

? 1990 ISAST
Pergamon Press plc. Printedin Great Britain.
0024-094X/90 $3.00+0.00 LEONARDO, Vol. 23, No. 1, pp. 75-80, 1990 75
'imprint' all its own, easily identifiable
on a global scale but only truly appre-
ciated on a local scale. And it is this
imprint that students of compositional
design will find of interest.

THE STRUCTURAL
IMPRINT OF RANDOMNESS
My interest in randomness grew out of
considerable thought and practice in
teaching principles of compositional
order elaborated by Wong [ 10], Thom-
pson [11] and others and in studying
the science of statistics. Through my
work on the psychology of problem-
solving behaviors in designers, I was
introduced to random number tables
as a quick means of selecting ran-
domized groups for experimental test-
ing. From that unlikely process I no-
ticed interesting distinctions in the
makeup of groups selected randomly
and groups organized consciously. This
awareness spawned a series of informal
experiments in my design classroom. I
noticed that design students were typi-
cally unable to organize elements ran-
domly even when explicitly requested
to do so. Their conception of random-
ness was something quite different
A: 10101010101010101010 compelling. Further insights into ran- from what randomness actually looked
B: 10100011111001011001 domness and art can be found in two like. To illustrate this point, we trans-
Sequence A follows the simple rule other Leonardoarticles, one by Michael lated long streams of random numbers
of the strict alternation of the two sym- Challinor [6] and the other by Fred into graphical arraysand simply looked
bols. The sequence could be extended Whipple [7]. at the results. The issue seemed to boil
indefinitely according to that rule. But Likewise, random behavior in sys- down to structure-the graphical struc-
it can also be compressed into a more tems has interested researchers in the ture generated by random numbers was
economical algorithm: PRINT '10' (2 x fields of mathematics, physics, chemis- completely different from anything my
N) TIMES. Sequence B, in contrast, try, engineering and meteorology for students had seen. It is interesting that
does not appear to obey any rule and some time. Their work has revealed the this same observation has been made in
cannot be compressed into a more ec- pervasiveness of chaotic phenomena experimental studies involving persons
onomical formula. Therefore, it seems even in the simple systems and has led listening to sequences of random audi-
reasonable to infer its randomness. to new methods for finding hidden or- ble tones. The subjects quite often
Kuppers summarized the principle of der within apparent chaos. Pickover [8] could not distinguish the random se-
randomness in relation to the incom- describes chaos theory as the study of quence from the nonrandom one [12,
pressibility of a sequence. He stated how complicated, random behavior 13]. When individuals are confronted
that "asequence is defined as 'random' can arise in systems based on simple with the three different sequences
when the shortest algorithm needed to rules. Such forms of hidden order have A: HTHTTHHT
generate it contains about as many in- been isolated with the aid of comput- B: HTHHHHHH
formation units as the sequence itself' ers; they are called attractors,or strange C: HTHTHTHT
[4]. attractors when the chaotic behavior is sequence A tends to appear more ran-
A flurry of writings on compositional bound within certain parameters. (For dom than sequences B and C, because
order and disorder appeared in the a popular overview of the topic, see it has an equal ratio of heads and tails,
early 1970s in Leonardo.Much of this Gleick [9].) The prevailing attitude, and because it has no easily discernable
activitywas generated by Entropyand Art however, seems to be that randomness pattern. Studies shown that most
by Rudolf Arnheim [5]. Arnheim ar- as a phenomenon is only interesting people also equate symmetry and long
gued that compositional unity is ideally when some form of attractor can be runs of a single character with order,
a bipolar struggle between utter simpli- isolated within it. In other words, most and high rates of alteration and asym-
city and utter chaos. While the treatise chaos researchers do not find true or metry with randomness [14]. In reality,
met with some criticism for the loose- statistical randomness to be an attrac- however, statistical randomness often
ness of its arguments and its casual cita- tive topic for serious study. But statisti- exhibits characteristics of symmetry
tion of precise scientific terms and con- cally random behavior possesses a com- and nonalternation. In fact, the proba-
cepts, its basic premise remains plex and identifying structural bility of randomly obtaining the three

76 Eckersley,
Randomness, Rules and Compositional Structure in Design
sequences above is exactly the same! In bers, (2) a two-dimensional plane and larly occurring geometric patterns.
other words, depending on one's rela- (3) rules for insertion and rotation of a (For an in-depth discussion of periodic
tive sophistication with the concept, the predefined motif. and aperiodic tilings, see Grunbaum
actualstructure of randomness appears Already the complexity of these ran- and Shepard [17].) The subject of ran-
surprisingly nonrandom to people, dom arrays exceeds (in its own way) dom graphical arrayspresented in this
especially when viewed at a local level. that found in the famous tiling patterns paper differs considerably from the
The principle can be simply demon- of M. C. Escher and Sebastien Truchet aperiodic tilings of Penrose and others
strated by flipping a coin many times [15], where the shape topologies, how- in that random arrays do not conform
and inserting a motif sequentially into ever intricate (in Escher's case), are to the rules of tiling theory. Both sys-
a grid on each occurrence of heads. nonetheless predictably repetitive. In tems are bound by rules, however. What
Figure 1 shows an array of 10,000 ran- recent years mathematicians such as differentiates these random arrays
dom numbers generated with the use Roger Penrose [16] and Robert Am- from formal repetitive patterns is their
of a computer. Rules of insertion (e.g. man have made remarkable discoveries inherent asymmetryand their tendency
motif type, rotation, insertion coordi- in tiling theory. Tiling theory involves to result in exotic aggregate shapes.
nates) were input via a simple program. study of the geometry of shapes (e.g. It is in the astounding varietyof these
The ratio of heads to tails in the global equilateral triangles, squares or hexag- shapes that the creativity of random-
array (in this case, composed of about ons) that, when multiplied identically ness is found-not the creativity of
5,000 comma-like motifs) is approxi- and snugly adjoined, will solidly cover human self-expression but rather a nat-
mately one-to-one; the distribution is an infinitely large plane. Work by Pen- ural sort of creativity such as seen in
largely homogeneous or even. How- rose and Amman involves the concept snowflakes; the system generates an in-
ever, when areas of the same array are of aperiodictiling, and the exotic forms credible variety based on a common
viewed locally (Fig. 2) the distribution of symmetry that can arise from irregu- theme. The interplay of fixed rules
is anything but homogeneous. The ran-
dom distribution of motifs can be de-
Fig. 2. Detail view of large random distribution (comma motif), ratio 1:1. Motif rotation is
scribed as globally stable but locally un- random. Motif is a basic comma form. Special emphasis on the enormous variety of com-
predictable, asymmetrical and visually posite voids or white shapes.
quite dynamic. The notion of random
structure seems at first glance to defy
logic. However, randomness can exhi-
bit as many distinctive structural im-
prints as there are varieties and degrees
of randomness, methods of graphical
analysis and levels of fineness of analy-
sis.

CONTROLLING THE
PARAMETERS OF
RANDOMNESS
The binding of randomness through
rules or controlling parameters yields
structures that are no longer random
(in the truest sense) but rather main-
tain an ingredient or imprint of ran-
domness. The imposition of rules enor-
mously influences the graphical
appearance of randomness. In other
words, the graphical characteristics of a
random array are highly sensitive to
even the slightest change in motif type,
the density of motifs and the operating
rules. The simple manipulation of
these can lead to significant changes in
the array. Figure 3 shows just such an
instance; an array based on the identi-
cal number sequence used in Fig. 2,
with only a minor change in the motif,
yields substantially different character-
istics in the negative (white) shapes or
voids. Figure 4 shows an even more
dramatic change with the imposition of
a linear motif. Controlling parameters
for Figs 1-4 comprised three simple
factors: (1) a random sequence of num-

Randomness, Rules and Compositional Structure in Design


Eckersley, 77
composition as it is the spatial organiza-
tion of the composition itself. Examples
of dynamic asymmetry can be found in
significant works of art, architecture
and design from Mondrian's Broadway
Boogie Woogieto Frank Lloyd Wright's
Falling Waterhouse.
Randomness and design appear at
first glance to be an incongruous
match. The act of designing is usually a
highly self-conscious, goal-directed ac-
tivity, and that runs counter to most
notions of randomness. However (and
herein lies the second and most useful
aspect of the randomness principle for
designers), random graphical arrays,al-
most without exception, exhibit strik-
ing examples of dynamic asymmetry;
localized sections of large arrayscan be
isolated and appreciated as powerful
compositions. The educational impli-
cations of this phenomenon are not
trivial. A recurring project involving
randomness and the definition of rules,
motifs and colors has generated numer-
ous beautiful designs, an example of
which can be seen in Color Plate B No.
2. This project has been a useful point
of departure in discussions focusing on
issues of rule formulation and control
in the design process. These discussions
have made apparent to students and
faculty alike that design is essentially
nothing more (and nothing less) than
an activity involving the formulation,
invocation and reformulation of rules
when existing rules fail to yield satis-
factory results. In this sense the design
Fig. 3. Detail view of large random distribution (variant comma motif), ratio 1:1. Motif process can be viewed as a cybernetic
rotation is random. Motif is a slight variation on the comma. Compare the difference in machine in which the rules define the
composite voids or white shapes from those in Fig. 2. capabilities of the machine, random-
ness provides the input, and the design
(physical rules in the case of snow- negamorphs can be generated with the product is the output. Once the basics
flakes) and randomness contributes to imposition of different operating rules. of the machine have been set forth, the
the structured generation of original creative designer manipulates or rede-
forms. The odds against these shapes signs the machine in such a way as to
occurring again in exactly the same way DESIGN LESSONS get more satisfactory designs. The de-
are great, and they increase consider- FROM RANDOMNESS: signer's creativity is directly related to
ably with the size of the given void, as the intrinsic clarity, sophistication and
EDUCATIONAL inventiveness of the rules conceived
well as with the complexity of the rules
operating in their creation. Whereas IMPLICATIONS and invoked.
the same odds may operate in the crea- One of the most difficult design prin- The function of rules as determin-
tion of positive and negative aggregate ciples for instructors to teach and for ants of style are well exemplified in Fig.
shapes (the case in Figs 1-4), the nega- young designers to understand is that 5, where variables of line, shape, scale,
tive white shapes, or negamorphs, ap- of dynamic asymmetry. Ironically, this placement, type font and type style
pear somehow more compelling in most complex compositional principle (among others) operate in random
their having been created as negative is also fundamental to understanding fashion. The literal push of a button
imprints of the former. As one scans the modern notions of composition. Dy- allows such designs to be generated al-
array, anthropomorphic and zoomor- namic asymmetry involves the ordering most instantaneously. Randomness in
phic shapes seem to crystallize before of compositional elements (i.e. shapes, this case does not render uniformly sat-
one's eyes. The compelling point is that lines, colors, etc.) in a manner that isfactory results (this is not the issue),
this Noah's ark of randomly generated forges a compositional unity while pro- but one out of every seven or eight
creatures is as large as the largest array longing viewer scanning and delaying designs is quite presentable. The real
one can generate. Whole new species of perceptual closure. It is as much an power lies in the program's capability
emotion evoked in the viewer by the to allow for easy reconfiguration by its

78 Eckersley,
Randomness, Rules and Compositional Structure in Design
order. This order is somehow under-
standable on an intuitive level, reflect-
ing certain natural or man-made laws
bound together with chance. Its useful-
ness to designers compares to the les-
sons they can learn from observing
nature's own random permutations of
form.
One gain from discussions of ran-
domness in the design studio is that
naive conceptions about randomness
change as one learns to discern subtle
distinctions between random processes
and nonrandom processes. An addi-
tional payoff is familiaritywith a class of
structures uniquely different from
those self-conscious compositional
structures with which we are commonly
familiar. Random structures can be dis-
tinguished from formal, semiformal
and informal kinds of structures and
should be classified as different. As the
designer's visual language grows to ac-
commodate random structures, so too
does his or her verbal ability to differ-
entiate them from other, more tra-
ditional kinds of compositional struc-
Fig.4. Detailviewof largerandomdistribution(linearmotif), ratio 1:1.Motifrotationis
randombut at fixed degrees.The motif in this case is a line segment. tures. Just as learning is enhanced by
drawing comparisons and distinctions
between concepts, so can understand-
human counterpart to mirror the de- notions have been alluded to in the ing of customary structures be en-
signer's own universe of acceptable op- landmark research of Koenig and hanced through a learning experience
erations. As such, the computer Eizenberg [18] on the design style of with randomness.
graphic representation can serve as a Frank Lloyd Wright, and have been
powerful point of departure for design- more fully fleshed out in writings by this
Acknowledgments
ers not wishing to face the often intimi- author [19].
dating blank canvas. In other words, The author thanksJ. P. Bonta for his valuable edi-
torial advice and the Computer-Aided Design and
designers can establish upfront, in the Graphics Lab of the University of Maryland for its
form of simple algorithms, the parame- support.
ters within which they wish to work SUMMARY
graphically. Based on those parame- While some would argue that random- References
ters, if what the computer tosses up is ness is by definition without practical
1. P. Weiss, "Some Paradoxes Relating to Order",
broadly acceptable to the designer, he value, the trained eye begins to see in in P. G. Kuntz, ed., The Conceptof Order(Seattle:
or she can proceed from there. These random structures a distinguishing Univ. of Washington Press, 1968).

llI
p
Fig. 5. Example output from Design Gener-
ator program. This version of Design Gen- L12 .. .. . . x .
erator involves variables of typography as
well as shape, line width, scale and place-
ment, and variables of distortion, color in-
version,rotationand flipping.Note general
I I ro ............................
stylistic similarities to Raiuhausand De Stijl
typographical composition, specifically, to
the typography of the late Dutch designer
Piet Zwart [20].

_. ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~......
3 ||||||l||
jie ^j t

I
_

I
Pe i

Randomness, Rules and Compositional Structure in Design


Eckersley, 79
2. F. Barron, "The Psychology of Imagination", and Chaos", ComputerGraphicsFrum 6 (1987) pp. Learning,Memoyand Cognition13,
mentalPsychology:
199 (1958) pp. 150-161.
ScientificAmerican 26-34. No. 3, 392-400 (1987).
3. G.J. Chaitin, "Randomness and Mathematical 9. J. Gleick, Chaos:Makinga NewScience(New York: 15. C. S. Smith, "The Tiling Patterns of Sebastien
232 (1975) p. 47.
Proof', ScientificAmer,ican Viking, 1987). Truchet and the Topology of Structural Hierar-
4. B. Kuppers, "On the Prior Probability of the chy", Leonardo20 No. 4, 373-385 (1987).
10. W. Wong, Principlesof Two-DimensionalDesign
Existence of Life",in L. Kruger, G. Gigerenzer and (New York:Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1972). 16. R. Penrose, "Pentaplexity",Eureka39 (1978)
M. Morgan, eds., TheProbabilistic Revolution,Vol. 2: pp. 16-22.
Ideas in the Sciences(Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 11. D. Thompson, On Growthand Form (Cam-
1987) p. 362. bridge: Cambridge University Press, 1961). 17. B. Grunbaum and G. C. Shepard, Tilings and
Patterns(New York:W. H. Freeman, 1986).
5. R. Arnheim, Entropyand Art:An Essayon Disorder 12. R. Falk, "The Perception of Randomness".
and Order(Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of Paper presented at the fifth conference of the 18. H. Koenig andJ. Eizenberg, "The langauge of
California Press, 1971). International Group for Psychology of Mathemati- the Prairie: Frank Lloyd Wright's Prairie Houses",
6. M. Challinor, "Change, Chance, and Structure: cal Education, Grenoble, France, 1981. Environmentand Planning 8 (1981) pp. 295-323.
Randomness and Formalism in Art", Leonardo4, 13. K. H. Teigen, "Studiesin Subjective Probability 19. M. Eckersley, "Thoughts on a Computer-Based
No. 1, 1-11 (1971). I: Prediction of Random Events", ScandinavianPsy- Design Apprentice", in C. Pickover, ed., Computers
7. F. Whipple, "Stochastic Painting", Leonardo1, chology24 (1983) pp. 13-25. and Graphics,in press.
No. 1,81 (1968). T .^nn.-m-anti1
14A T.
tween
I'8. R
C. A.
Pick.ver adU.CA. Orli;n "Y)ictinmlikhino
l,NOd m Events, Eperi RQ 20. B. Monguzzi, "Piet Zwart",Graphis44No. 258,
8. C. A. Pickover, "Graphics, Bifurcation, Order tween Random and Nonrandom Events", Experi- 57-61 (1988).

80 Eckersley,
Randomness, Rules and Compositional Structure in Design

Potrebbero piacerti anche