Sei sulla pagina 1di 12

REVIEWS

WHAT MAKES US TICK?


FUNCTIONAL AND NEURAL
MECHANISMS OE INTERVAL TIMINC
Catalin V. Buhusi and Warren H. Meek
Abstract | Time is a fundamental dimension of iife. it is crucial for decisions about quantity,
speed of movement and rate of return, as weii as for motor controi in waiking, speech,
piaying or appreciating music, and participating in sports. Traditionaliy, the way in which time
is perceived, represented and estimated has been explained using a pacemaker-
accumuiator modei that is not oniy straightforward, but also surprisingiy powerfui in
explaining behaviourai and biologicai data. However, recent advances have chaiienged this
traditional view. It is now proposed that the brain represents time in a distributed manner and
teiis the time by detecting the ooincidentai activation of different neurai popuiations.

GLOBAL POSITIONING SYSTEM Time and space are the fundamental dimensions of our number of biological mechanisms that have evolved
(GPS). A nelwork of artificial existence. Although space is gradually losing its value in a to deal with time.
salellitc tran.smitters that provide world of computer networks, ceUular phones and virtual This article reviews the rapid progress that has
highly accurate positionfixesfor libraries, time is becoming the essence of our times, as heen made in understanding tbe functional and neural
F^rth-based, portable receivers.
is reflected by ever increasing speed, rate of return and mechanisms of IN IF.RVAL TIMING. Traditionally, the manner
productivity concepts that are intrinsically related to in which durations in the seconds-to-minutes range are
time. Time is also crucial for everyday activities, from perceived, represented and estimated has been explained
our sleep-wake cycle to walking, speaking, playing and using a pacemaker-accumulator model ''. This mode!
appreciating music, and playing sports. We can engage is relatively straightforward, and provides powerful
in these activities because, like most animals, we process explanations of hoth hebavioural and physiological
and use temporal information across a wide range of data'*'"'^ However, recent advances that challenge the
intervals (l-Ui. i) in contrast to, for example, the limited traditional pacemaker-accumulator model have come
range of the light spectrum that we can see. from studies that use various modern techniques, which
Being able to tell the time is also advantageous for range from drug microinjection and ensemble recording
gathering spatial information. Just as a position in in genetically modified and wild-type rodents to func-
Duke University, tional MRI (tMRI) and positron emission tomography
Department of space can he triangulated by using distance to land-
Psychological and Brain marks, the GLOBAL POSITIONING SYSTEM (GPS) provides (PET) in neurologically impaired and control humans.
Sciences, 103 Research current position hy triangulating temporal information These data indicate that time might be represented in
Drive, GSRB-2 Buiiding, (the difference or coincidence in pbase of signals) from a distributed manner in the brain, and that telling the
Room 30 iO. time is a matter of detecting the coincidental activation
satellites. coiNciDLNCLDtiKtnoN is also used hy hats,
Durham, North Carolina of different neural populations.
27708, USA. owls and frogs to form an accurate, topographic rep-
Correspondence to W.H.M. resentation of space from INTERAURAL TIME DIFFERENCES'.
e-mail: For these species, telling space is telling time. Timing Multiple timers for multiple timescales
meck(a>psych .duke.edu and time perception are fundamental to survivai and
doi.10.l03K/nrnl764
To deal with time, organisms have developed muitiple
goal reaching in humans and other animals'', and are systems tbat are active over more than 10 orders of
Published online
15 September 2005
possible over multiple timescales"' " owing to the magnitude with various degrees of precision (FIG. U).

NATURt: REVIEWS | NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 OCTOBER 2005 75S


REVIEWS

24 h Circadian, interval and millisecond timing involve


Circadian timing different neural mechanisms'^ In mammals, the cir-
Behaviour cadian clock that drives metabolic and behavioural
Appetite rhythms is located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus
Sleep-wake cycle
Brain structures
(SCN) of the hypothalamus. This master clock coor-
10,000- Suprachiasmatic nuclei dinates tissue-specific rhythms according to light
Mechanism input" and other cues such as sociai information'''
Transcription/translation that it receives from the outside world. The circadian
1h regulatory loops
timer relies on a molecular network of transcriptional
feedback loops'^ On the other hand, interval timing
1,000-
depends on the intact striatum, but not on the intact
SCN"" or cerebellum'^'". In the interval-timing range,
Interval timing
tbe striatum and the cerebellum might both be acti-
Behaviour
vated, possibly contributing to different aspects of
1 min Foraging performance"''" as a function of the sequential stages
Decision making of motor memory consolidation^'.
Conscious time estimation
Brain structures
Corticostriatai circuits Traditional approaches to interval timing
Dopamine neurons
Mechanism
The scalar property. Three types of behavioural pro-
Coincidence detection cedure have traditionally been used to investigate
Striatal LTP/LTD interval timing in humans and other animals: estima-
tion, production and reproduction. In humans, the
-^'^^
first two protocols tend to rely on verbal instructions or
responses, requiring the participant to translate between
4 Millisecond timing
\ Behaviour
performance and a verbal representation of duration,
\
Speech, music which can lead to confounds. A more reliable approach,
0,1 -
I Motor control which can be used equally well with a wide variety of
; Brain structures anima! species'^'-, is to use a reproduction procedure,
'' * Cerebellum?
Human Mechanism in which tbe subject is presented with a given criterion
Cerebellar LTP/LTD? duration and tben required to reproduce this duration
Animal
Intrinsic properties of neural function? (FIG. 2). Typically, the participant's responses follow a
1% 10% 100% normal distribution around tbe criterion duration, and
Reiattve estimation error, log scale the width of this response distribution is proportional to
Precise * * Imprecise the criterion duration. The way in which the mean and
Figure 1 | Timing across different timescales. a | A compilation, which is by no means standard deviation of tbe response distribution covary is
exhaustive, of data from various studies"''''"'"''" that indicate the precision of humans and other usually referred to as the scalar property*', and resembles
animals in various timing tasks. Performance is precise (but less flexible) in a narrovi/ range W[:BI;R'S LAW", which is obeyed by most sensory dimen-
around 24 ti {circadian timing), less precise (but more flexible] in a wide seconds-to-minutes-to- sions. The scalar property applies not only to behavioural
hours range (intervai timing), and is of mixed precision in the sub-second range (millisecond responses, but also to neural activation as measured by
timing) in which performance is probably linked to the intrinsic properties of the neural system
ensemble recording, or by the haemodynamic response
involved^'', b-e | Circadian rhythms'' are most recognizable in nature (b). but interval and
millisecond timing also guide fundamental animal behaviours. For example, although female ring
to timed events measured with fMRP^"''' (hlG. 2).
doves use circadian-timing strategies to coordinate egg incubation, males use interval-timing
strategies" (c). Inten/al timing is involved in decision making'' (d), and millisecond timing is
central to the playing of music"' (e). LTD, long-term depression; LTP, long-term potentiation. The pacemaker-accumulator model. The established
explanation for the scalar property is based on an
internal clock model", in which pulses that are emitted
regularly by a pacemaker''""^" are temporarily stored
COINCIDENCE DETECTION \ which Operate over the range of the in an accumulator. At the time of reward or ttELiBACK,
The activation of neurons not 24-h hght-dark cycle, control sleep and wakefulness the number of pulses that have been received from tbe
by single inputs, but by the accumulator is stored in reference memory^ (FIG. 3a).
as well as tnetabolic and reproductive fitness [FIG. ib).
simultaneous activity of several
inputs. For example,
Interval timing in the seconds-to-minutes range is This information-processing (IP) model implements the
coincidental activation or involved in foraging", decision making--' IFIG. id) scalar expectancy theory^', in that the response is con-
inactivatiiin of specific dendritic and multiple-step arithmetic'^ and has been demons- trolled by the ratio comparison between the current
inputs might trigger a neuron to trated in birds"'"'", fish''', rodents'" ", primates^', and subjective time/clock reading stored in the accu-
fire, thereby Iransforminga mulator and a sample taken from the distribution of
human infants'' and adults-''^'', MILLISECOND TIMING is
time code into a rate code.
Similarly, in lhe binaural
crucial for motor control'", speech generation-" and remembered criterion durations, which are represented
auditory system, coincidental recognition^'*, playing music"' and dancing (FIG. ic). as the number of pulses from previously reinforced clock
activatiim thai results from These different timing strategies inform decision readings stored in reference memory. In this framework,
hearing a sound with a specific making in both individuals and groups; for exam- the scalar property derives from the assumption that the
interaural time difference is accumulation error is proportional to the criterion dura-
ple, to coordinate egg incubation, male ring doves
used to transform a time code
into a spatial code.
use interval-timing strategies whereas females use tion'*''''(PiG- 3b). The model has some notable advantages:
circadian-timing strategies" (FKi. ic). it is straightforward, tbereby encouraging its application

75B OGTOBER 2005 VOLUME 6 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


REVIEWS

INTERAURAI.TIME
DIFFERENCE
The difference in lhe time of
arrival ol a soudd wave at an
animal's two ears. It ranges from
100 (IS in gerbiils to about 650 |is
in humans and is one of the
sources of information used by
various species to make a
ropographic representation of
0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6
space.
Relative time (t/T')
IN TKRVAI. TIMING
Perception, estimation and fMRI: interval-timing condition fMRl; motor-timing condition
discrimination of durations in 100-1
the range of seconds to- _ Right putamen ' Right putamen -'"
minutes to hours.

CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS
Repetition of certain
phenomena in living organisms 40-
at about the same lime each day.
The most thought of circadian
rhythm is sleep, hut other
examples include body
0-
temperature, blood pressure,
and the production of hormones
and digestive secretions, 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.6 1.0 1,2 0.6 0.6 1.0 1,2
Relative time {t/T') Relative time (t-T^)
.\51LI.[SECOND TIMING
Figure 2 | The scalar property is a hallmark of interval timing at both the behavioural and neural levels, a | in a typicai
Perct'piion, estimation and
duration reproduction procedure known as the 'peak-intervai procedure', participants receive training triais, during which they
discrimination of durations in
are presented with target stimuii of specific criterion durations (8 s or 21 s in this example), and test triais, in which participants
the sub second range.
are asked to reproduce the criterion interval. In test triais the responses typioally distribute normally around the criterion intervai
with a width that is proportionai to the temporai criterion, b | When the response distributions are scaled and superimposed,
\VI:BEK'SI.AVV
they demonstrate the scalar property at the Pehaviourai ievei*''". T', test criterion, c | The scalar property also applies at tha
Formulated by lirnst Weber in
1831 to explain lhe relationship neural level for the haemodynamic response associated with a participant's 'active' reproduction of a timed criterion, but not
between the physical intensity of for 'passive' responses triggered by a cue associated with an intervai that is not timed. fMRI, functionai MRI. Panel b modified,
i stimulus and the sensory with permission, from REF. 25 (1998) American Psychoiogicai Association. Panel c reproduced, with permission from REF. 45
experience thai il causes. Weber's (2004) Eisevier Science.
Law states that the increase in a
stimulus needed to produce a
jusi noticeable difference is
constant Later. Gustav Fechner to many species and tasks^"'; it has clearly separated their affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor^'" (FIG. 3e),
(1H01-1SS7] generalized clock, memory and decision stages'*, which makes it pos- whereas cbolinergic activity in the frontal cortex is
Weber's law by proposing tbat
sible to map these components onto brain structures" proportionai to the absolute error ofa TEMPORAL MEMORY
sensation increases as tbe
logarithm of stimulus intensity: and neurotransmitter systems''; and it is surprisingly TRANSLATION C O N S T A N T ' " * (FIG. 3g,h).

S = klogi, where S = subjective successful (considering its simple structure) in terms of Despite the success of the IP model in explaining
experience, I = physical intensity, making testable predictions**. a large set of behavioural and physiological results, its
and k = constant. relevance to the brain mechanisms that are involved in
The first investigations of the biological substrates
of the clock and memory stages of the pacemaker- interval timing is unclear. For example, the idea that
FEEDBACK
To signal the end of lhe lo be- accumulator IP model used pharmacological manip- there is a direct and/or exclusive connection between the
timed duration to the ulations, and provided considerable support for a dopaminergic system and the speed of an internal clock
participant, a feedback signal is dissociation between the clock stage, which is affected has been challenged by studies in which patients with
presented. In experiments by dopaminergic manipulations, and the memory Parkinson's disease were asked to time two durations".
involving animals, the feedback When learning two criterion durations, the responses
stage, which is affected by cholinergic manipulations'*
is usually an appelilive stimulus
(for example, food) or aversive (FIG. 3d). For example, dopaminergic drugs selectively tended to migrate towards each other if the patients
stimulus (for example, affect the subjective speed of an internal clock in both were tested off their dopaminergic medication (f IG. ic).
footshock). In experiments ihal animals''"^' and humans" (FIG. 3c), whereas cholinergic The connection has also been challenged by the
involve human participants, tbe drugs aher memory storage'^'-' (FIG.30- More specifi- inconsistency between the relatively modest effects of
feedback may take various dopaminergic drugs on behaviour^' and the observed
cally, dopaminergic antagonists produce a deceleration
forms, including verbal reward,
gaining 'points', and so on. of the subjective clock speed (FIG. 3d) in proportion to levels of dopamine release in the striatum in vivo'^, and

NATUREREVIEWS NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 OCTOBER 2005 7B7


REVIEWS

Despite the number of findings that support the


r60
biological plausibility of the pacemaker-accumulator
mechanism''", alternative biological mechanisms have
been evaluated as possible substrates for interval tim-
ing. For example, to address the challenges outlined
above, a number of alternative theoretical models of
interval timing have been proposed, involving NMURAL
0 10 20 30 40 50 0 20 40 60
Objective time (s) Response rate OSCILLATORS'' '", sustained neural activation*''"''\ network
dynamics'"* or switching among behavioural states''^.
Clock pattern and dopamine Memory pattern and acetylcholine Although most ofthese theoretical models success-
Objective time (s) Objective time (s)
0 10 20 30 40 50
fully account for some aspects of the behavioural data,
0 10 20 30 40 50
validation ofthese emerging timing models will require
examination of the relevant neurobiological evidence.

Neural mechanisms of interval timing


Recent findings indicate that it might be necessary to
integrate data from several approaches to reveal the
neural mechanisms of interval timing. The evidence
supports the idea that there are two timing circuits
that can be dissociated: an automatic timing system
that works in the millisecond range, which is used in
discrete-event (discontinuous) timing and involves
the cerebellum; and a continuous-event, cognitively
controlled timing system that requires attention and
involves the basal ganglia and related cortical structures.
10 20 30 40 50 10 20 30 40 50 Because these two timing systems work in parallel,
Response peak time (s] Response peak time (s) suitable experimental controls might be required to
= 0,96 = 0.75 engage (and reveal) each system independently of the
1.5-
Prom az me other"*'-"'.
Chlorpromazine
Timing in sickness and in health. An impaired ability to
process time in the seconds-to-minutes range is found
in patients with disorders that involve dopaminergic
pathways, such as Parkinsons disease"', Huntington's
disease (HD)'' and schizophrenia'^"'^ By contrast, the
failure ofa neurological disorder such as cerebellar
injury to affect the scalar property is taken to indicate
-0.5 0 1.5 2 2.5 25
D2 high affinity D2 iow affinity that the affected structures are not essential for proper
% Absolute deviation interval timing'". Instead, the cerebellum might con-
Log K, (nM) in estimated time
Figure 3 | The pacemaker-accumulator model and dopaminergic and cholinergic
tain an internal model of the motor-effector system"',
synapses, a | Shows an information-processing (IP) model pf time perception'' implementing the so cerebellar damage could increase variability in
scalar expectancy theory''-', in the modei, a dopaminergic pacemaker sends 'pulses' to an motor and perceptual timing^*'.
accumuiator during the training period, and the number of puises is stored in reference memory
For example, Parkinson's disease, in which the
{which depends on the 'effective ievel' of acetylchoiine (ACh)). During a trial, the number of pulses
in working memory (current) is compared with that in reference memory, b | The mcdel explains
nigrostriatal dopaminergic projections degenerate,
the scalar property (Fro. 2) by assuming that the estimation error increases in proportion to the disrupts interval timing in a number of ways. Patients
criterion duration (green area), c-e | The effects of the D2 dopamine receptor antagonist show the scalar property when medicated with L-dopa
haioperidol are consistent with the slowing down of time accumuiation. Acute administration of (HIG. 4a), but not when tested off-medication (FIG. 4b).
haloperidoi results in a sudden scalar (proportional to the timed criterion) rightward shift of the Moreover, patients with Parkinson's disease are unable
estimated time, v^rtiereas its repeated administration (HI, H2 and so on) results in a gradual return to time two (or more) durations independently: the
of the estimated time to the criterion duration'^ (d). The rightward shift of the estimated time is
reproduced criteria for the two criterion durations tend
proportional to the affinity of the drug tor the D2 reoeptor'' (e). K, affinity coefficient; VEH, vehicle.
f - h I The affects of choiinergic drugs are consistent with effects on reference memory. Repeated to migrate towards each other (FIG. 4C). This migration
administration of the muscarinic choiinergic receptor antagonist atropine results in a gradual effect is eliminated, and accurate timing is reinstated,
scaiar rightward shift of Ihe estimated time'^ (g). The effect is correlated with the activity of after stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus (FIG. 5d),
cholinergic neurons in the frontal cortex as measured by sodium-dependent high-affinity choiine which is one of the relay nuciei in thalamo-cortical-
uptake (SDHACU)^(h). striatal circuits (see the section on electropbysiological
studies, below). Finally, patients with Parkinson's
disease also show poor timing of motor actions"''".
by pharmacological studies that found that, besides By contrast, the preservation of the scalar property
its involvement in the speed of an internal clock, after cerebellar lesions (FIG. 4d) supports the view that
dopamine aiso modulates the attentional processing the striatum and cerebellum are involved in different
of temporal information^^, aspects of timing and time perception. Although the

788 OCTOBER 2005 VOLUME 6 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


REVIEWS

a PD (On L-dopa) b PD (Off L-dopa) C PD (migration effect)


ffi 100-1 >, 0.15H

0.1-

- 0.05-

0.4 0.6 0.6 1 1.2 1.4 1 D.4 0.6 0.8 1 1 2 1.4 1.6 10 15 20
Relative time Relative time Time (s)
Cerebellar lesion HD
100 , 100--,

0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6 Controi HD HD


far close
Relative time
Figure 4 | Interval timing in patients with Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease and cerebellar lesions, a-c | Two
separate causes of failure to time correctly in unmedicated patients with Parkinson's disease (PD). in a peak-iniervai procedure
during which participahts time 8- and 21 -s criterion durations, patients show the scalar property when medicated with L-dopa
(a), but hot when tested off-medication, owing to increased variabiiity (iarger width function: b). Moreover, when timing the
8- and 21 -s durations, unmedicated patients aiso show inaccurate representation of time the remembered durations tend to
migrate towards each other (c). d | The scaiar property is preserved atter cerebeilar lesions. e,f | As patients with Huntington's
disease {HD) approach the age at which they are predicted to develop symptoms {HD cicse), they show a deficit in interval
timing (panel e) and decreased activation of the basal ganglia, thalamus and pre-supplementary motor area/cingulate (panel f).
TEMPORAL MEMORY HD far, onset of syptoms predicted to be a number of years away. Panels a and b redrawn trom RK 57. Pane! c reproduced,
TRANSLATION CONSTANT with permission, from REF, 57 [1998) MIT Press. Panel d redrawh from Rf. 37. Paneis e and f modified, with permission, from
A parameler in the scalar REF. 71 (2004) American Society ot Neuroradiology.
expectancy theory that is
responsible for producing scalar
transforms of sensory inpul
taken from an internal dock cerebellum is not essential for interval timing, it is of the basal ganglia and cerebellum in motor control
and stored in temporal memory.
required for correct millisecond timing". and interval timing. In the first study, participants were
It is used t(i explain systematic
discrepancies in the accuracy of Another disorder that affects dopaminergic path- required to shift their attention between two streams of
temporal memory. ways is Huntington's disease, an autosomal-dominant, events one visual and one auditory both of which
neurodegenerative disorder that involves degeneration contained target and distractor stimuli. Patients and
NEURAL OSCILLATOR of the medium spiny neurons in the caudate nucleus control participants were tested under three conditions.
Repetitive, periodical activation
and putamen. Patients who are approaching the age at The first was a double-response condition, in which they
ofa neuron. The intrinsic
mechanisms that control the which they are predicted to develop Huntington's disease were instructed to respond to a target in one dimension,
period ot the osciQator (the ('HD close'; FIG. 4f) perform worse in tests of interval then to a target in the other dimension, and so forth. The
interval between two neuronal timing than either control individuals or patients whose second was a single-response condition, in which overt
spikes] range trom fast ion predicted time of disease onset is more than 12 years responses were required only to targets in one modality,
currents (lor example, 40 H7.
away ('HD far'). In fMRI studies of interval timing while targets in the other modality served as cues for
oscillations in sparsely spiny
neufons in lhe frontal cortex) to (FK;. 4f), control participants showed activation ot the attention switching without requiring overt responses.
slow transcriptional feedback caudate-putamen, thalamus, pre-supplementary The third condition was a focused condition, in which
loops (for example, 24 h motor area (pre-SMA) and cingulate cortex. Patients participants responded only to targets in one dimension
oscillation in the SCN). in the 'HD far' group showed similar activation, but and did not attend to the other dimension"'. Relative
with possibie hyperactivation in the pre-SMA and to the focused condition, patients with both Parkinson's
ATTENTIONAL SET
caudate nucleus, which might explain their relatively disease and cerebellar lesions were impaired in the
Set of lo-be-atlended features
that are primed for use in a normal timing performance"'. By contrast, patients double-response condition, which indicates that they
specific task, such that in the 'HD close' group showed decreased activation in might have difficulty with tasks that require rapid switch-
participants would be more al! three foci. Such results complement behaviouraP", ing of both .'\TTKNTioN and MOTOR SETS. However, when
likely to attend to the features in pharmacological**' and electrophysioiogical"^ evidence the motor demands were reduced in the single-response
the attentional set than to other
that separate brain mechanisms underlie different task, patients with cerebellar lesions performed better
features of the task.
components of the interval-timing system'*'. than those with Parkinson's disease, which indicates
MOTOR SET that cerebellar lesions might cause deficits in switching
Sets of to'he-activated motor the motor set whereas Parkinson's disease might lead to
programs that are primed for Lesion studies. Traditionally, because interval timing deficits in switching the attentional set"'.
use in a specific task, such that depends on the intact striatum^^-^'^" but not on the intact
participants would be more cerebellum^"'^ the cerebellum has been charged with In another line of research, patients with cerebellar
likely to respond using one of
millisecond Eiming'** and the basal ganglia with interval damage showed deficits in producing discontinuous,
the molor programs in the
motor set than using other timing'^'' '*^ Despite this simplistic dissociation, two but not continuous, movements, indicating that the
responses. recent fmdings have shed new light on the itivotvement cerebellum might have a specific role in event-based

NATURE REVIEWSTNEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 OCTOBER 2005 T8


REVIEWS

a Number: single-unit recording b Sequences: single-unit recording

Claudate-
pLrtamer Interval timing: STn stimulation in
C Interval timing:
ensemble recording Parkinson's disease
Train on DBS
- Test off DBS
Test on DBS

I Spike rate
I Lever-press rate

- * Glutamate
- * Dopamine
12 16 20 24 2a -* Acetylcholine
Time (s)
Rgure 5 [ Electrophysiological evidence for the involvement of thalamo-cortico-striatal circuits in the representation of
time and numerosity. The central panel depicts the thalamo-cortico-striatal projections and their neurotransmitter systems. Curbed
arrows show neurai pathways; dashed arrows indicate data obtained in a specific condition or from the specific brain aiBa. a,b | The
thalamus projects to corticai areas that are involved in the processing of numerosity: the frontai cortex (a] and parietal cortex (b).
Panel a shows the representation of numerosity in the frontal cortex of primates; spike rate varies with number of items. Panel b
shows the neurai representation of the second md fifth items in a motor sequence in the primate parietal cortex, c | Corticai areas
send giutamatergic connections to the oaudate-putamen, the neurai activation of which peaks at the criterion duration in interval-
timing procedures, d | Degeneration of the dopaminergic nigrostriatal projection (as seen in Parkinson's disease) resuits in abnomiai
processing of temporal information (FIG. 4). Deep brain stimulation (DBS) of the indirect subthalamic projection eliminates the retrieval
deficit but not the encoding distortion. GA8A, y-aminobutyric aoid; GPe, external segment of globus paiiidus; GPi, internal segment
of globus paltidus; NB, nucleus basalis; PFC, prefrontai oortex; PPC, posterior parietal cortex; SMA, supplementary motor area; SNc,
substantia nigra pars compacta; SNr, substantia nigra pars reticuiata; STn, subthalamic nucieus; X, denotes degeneration of
nigrostriatai projection in Parkinson's disease. Panel a reproduced, with pemiission, from REF. 99 (2002) American Association for
the Advancement of Soienoe. Panei b reproduoed, with permission, from REF. 97 (2002) Maomiilan Magazines. Panel c drawn
using data from REE l o i . Panel d drawn using data from REK 142.

^^***. Together, these studies suggest that separate For example, neurons in the PPC were activated
timing circuits can be dissociated when continuity, motor not only during timing tasks'^, but also during tasks in
demands and attentional set are manipulated'" ^"'*', which primates were required to perform a sequence
of movements a number of times; in such tasks, PPC
Electrophysiological studies. The thalamo-cortlca!- neurons fired selectively depending on the ordinal
striatal circuits that include the basal ganglia, the pre- number (position in the sequence) in a block of trials'^
frontal cortex (PFC) and the posterior parietal cortex (FIG. 5b), The number-order-magnitude circuit of
{PPC) have been shown (for example, by brain imaging which the PPC is a part also includes areas of the PFC^,
studies'*^'*") to be activated both in interval-timing tasks which extract the quantity of visual field items'^ (FIG. 5a),
and in tasks tbat require integration of some stimulus although some recent reports have challenged the
dimension over a time interval, such as integration of hypothesis that a single parietal region underlies both
somatosensory signals and the counting of events in symbolic and nonsymbolic number representation in
numerically based behavioural tasks'". These data are humans'',
consistent with the involvement of the basal ganglia, A recent study investigated the pattern of striatal
PFC and PPC in the representation of number, sequence firing in a reproduction task in which rats were proba-
or magnitude, as well as in interval timing, thereby sup- bilistically rewarded"" at two durations, 10 s and 40
porting a mode-control model of counting and timing s. Probabilistic reward rules have been shown to
in which number and time are processed by the same have important consequences at the neural level for
neural areas^^"''\ dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra pars

www.nature.com/revlews/neuro
REVIEWS

compacta (SNc), which project to the striatum"'^ Such This definition was successfully used in a recent
a probabilistic task might, therefore, reveal the involve- study from the same group of researchers, in which
ment of nigrostriatal pathways in interval timing. participants were asked to direct their attention to the
Although rats responded reliably at both durations, duration of an event or to its colour fFlG. 6a), thereby
electrophysiological recordings revealed that two combining parametric variations in timing perform-
distinct subsets of striatal neurons were activated, ance and appropriate control conditions (time versus
thereby dissociating motor responses from temporal colour). As expected, areas in the visual cortex showed
coding in the striatum. In one set of striatal neurons a linear increase in activation when participants paid
the firing pattern peaked at about the 10-s reward more attention to the colour of the stimuli (FIG. 6c,d), In
point (FIG. 5c), whereas the activity of the other set turn, the SMA showed a linear activation when parti-
of neurons gradually increased throughout the 40-s cipants paid more attention to time, thereby identifying
interval"". Similar striatal ramp-like activity has been this area as part of the timing circuit (FIG. 6b,c). Other
observed in IJULAYED MATCHING-TO-SAMI'LL TASKS before areas, such as the putamen, were also active when
the anticipated cue'"^'"''. This indicates that sustained participants paid more attention to time (FIG. 6b).
activity over delay intervals is an important feature of Another approach that has been used to try to make
striatal activation that might be crucial for bridging sense of the many brain regions that have been reported
the interval between the moment when information is to be activated by timing procedures is the grouping
acquired and the moment when that information can of active areas by the characteristics of the timing pro-
be used in a decision. cedure used. Areas can be grouped by, for example,
Striatal neurons can code multiple durations, but the duration measured (sub-second/supra-second),
only if the PFC is intact. Lesions of the agranular frontal the use of movement to define a temporal estimate,
cortex or the nucleus basalis magnocellularis'"'"' impair and the continuity or predictability of the procedure,
rats' ability to time two stimuli simultaneously, but not under the assumption that repetitive actions require less
their ability to time each stimulus sequentially, although attention*'''. This analysis revealed two clusters of foci.
such lesions do cause small, but reliable, changes in the The 'automatic timing' cluster includes areas that were
content of reference memory'"^ A recent study inves- found to be activated by procedures that required repet-
tigated neural activity in the agranular frontal cortex itive movements and involved the timing of relatively
of rats that had been trained to time two stimuli of short durations: these areas include the SMA, primary
different durations"'\ After training, rats were tested motor cortex and primary somatosensory cortex. The
with the two separate stimuli, and with the compound 'cognitively controlled timing' cluster includes areas
stimulus. Most neurons (60%) responded only to the that were found to be activated when the durations used
compound stimulus; fewer neurons responded both were longer and the amount of movement required was
to the compound and to the separate stimuli (10%); limited: these areas include the DLPFC, intraparietal
and very few neurons responded only to one stimulus sulcus and premotor cortex. Interestingly, the brain area
(3%). That a large proportion of cells responded only to most consistently activated in neuroimaging studies of
the compound stimulus supports the hypothesis that interval timing was the SMA, and the basal ganglia and
the agranular cortex is important for divided attention, cerebellum were not identified in either cluster, possibly
for shifting attention between the two stimuli, and/or because both areas might be continuously active and
for the dynamic allocation of attention in time', their differentiation might require additional control
conditions. Together with the other approaches, the
results of neuroimaging studies indicate that interval
Functional imaging studies. In recent years, func- timing engages various neural circuits, whose roles in
tional imaging of millisecond and interval timing has temporal or other aspects of tasks are still uncertain.
received considerable interest. Two informative reviews
are noteworthy"'''"'. The first*" analysed the imaging
method (fMRI/PET), target duration (from 0.3 s to A coincidence-detection model
24 s), timing procedure (discrimination, production, The data reviewed above fail to show that the basal
reproduction, generalization, synchronization, detec- ganglia have an exclusive role in temporal processing.
tion of deviants and reproduction of sequences), Instead, the more general role of the basal ganglia might
stimulus modality (visual or auditory) and control be to monitor activity in the thalamo-cortico-striatal
DELAYED MATCHING-TO- conditions of a number of studies. The review found circuits, and to act as a coincidence detector that signals
SAMPLE TASKS that many studies report activations of areas such as particular patterns of activity in working memory"^'' '' '^
Presenialion of a stimulus is
followed by a delay, after which
the basal ganglia and cerebellum during timing tasks, Because such a role lends itself to temporal coding*^, a
a choice Is offered and the but that some or all of the supplementary motor area biologically plausible model of interval timing was
originally presented stimulus (SMA), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), ante- developed to describe timing as an emergent activity
musl be chosen. With small rior cingulate cortex and right parietal cortex are also in the thalamo-cortico-striatal loops'*''"-\ In this striatal
stimulus sels, the stimuli arc activated, and the authors questioned whether these beat-frequency (SBF) model, timing is based on the
frequently repealed, and
therefore become highly
brain areas are part of a dedicated timing circuit or are coincidental activation of medium spiny neurons in the
familiar. So. typically, such tasks simply task-specific. They proposed that a brain area basal ganglia by cortical neural oscillators''' (FIG. 7a,b).
are most readily solved by is part of the timing circuit if its activation is sensitive Synchronous cortical activity has been reported'"' and
short-lcrm or working memory to parametric aspects of the timing procedure'"""" or the mechanisms of its generation are currently being
rather than by long-lerm to variations in timing performance'""^ investigated"\ For example, neurons in the motor
memory mechanisms.

NATURE REVIEWS ^NEUROSCIENCE 781


REVIEWS

Stimulus 1
shorter/blueish

Pay more attention to the time than colour'

Pay more attention to colour than time"

X = 60 mm ^^^-j^^^iisj.

T Tc tc tC ' C
Figure 6 | Drfferential activation of the circuits involved in the processing of time and colour. An attentional cue directed
participants to allocate their attention solely to time (Tj, solely to colour (C), equally to time and colour (tc), more to time {Tc) or
more to colour (tC). Participants were presented with two circular stimuli of different durations, the colour of which flickered
randomly in time, and were asked to compare either their duration or oolour by responding with a different finger (a), b-d | When
comparing time (blue curve), a circuit involving the pre-supplementary motor area (pSfvIA), dorsal premotor cortex (PMC),
LONG-TERM POTENTIATION putamen (Put), and frontal operculum (Fop) was activated (b,c). When asked to compare coiour (red curve), activation was
(I.TP). An enduring increase In observed in visual area V4 (cd). e,f [ The activation of these circuits correlated with decreases in reaction time and response
thi' amplitude of excitatory errors. IT/fviT/ST, inferior/middle/superior temporai cortex; PMC, dorsai premotor cortex: Par, inferior parietal ccrtex. Modified,
posl.syiiaptic piitentials as a with permission, from REK 143 (2004) American Association for the Advancement of Science,
result i)f high-frequency
(tetanic) stimulation of afferent
pathways. It is measured both as
the amplitude of excitatory cortex increase their synchrony when animais are In this model, the synchronization of cortical oscil-
postsynaptic potentials and as
trained to expect a 'go' signal"^ and the synchrony of lations at trial onset and the experience-dependent
ihe magnitude of the
postsynaptic-cell population
neurons in the somatosensory"'' and visual cortices''" changes in cortico-striatal transmission are ascribed
spike. LTP is most frequently is modulated by attention. The cortical oscillators are to the dopaminergic neurons in the SNc and ventral
studied in the hippocampus and assumed to be s)Tichronized at the onset of a trial, and tegmental area (VTA). This assumption is sup-
is ollen considered to be the to oscillate at a fixed frequency throughout the criterion ported by studies that have investigated the coding
cellular basis of learning and
interval. Experience-dependent changes in cortico- of a predictive signal by dopaminergic neurons''^
memory in vertebrates,
striatal transmission are assumed to make the striatal in probabilistic reward tasks'"-. For example, under
LONG-TERM DEPRESSION neurons more likely to detect the specific pattern of conditions in which it is uncertain whether a reward
(LTD). An eiiduritig weakening activation of cortical oscillators at the time of reward will be delivered (in FIG. 7e probability of reward is
of synaptii strength that is delivery and/or feedback (PIG, 7b) through cortico-striatal p = 0.75), the activity of dopaminergic neurons shows a
thought to interact with LTP in characteristic pattern with a burst at trial onset, a burst
LONG-TERM POTENTIATION ( L T P ) a n d LONG-TERM DEPRESSION
the cellular mechanisms of
learning and memory in "'^'', Under these assumptions, the actiWty of the at the expected time of reward and sustained activity
.structures such as the stimulated striataJ neurons increases before the expected throughout the interval'"-. According to the SBF model,
hippocampus and cerebellum. time of reward, and peaks at the criterion interval the dopaminergic burst at trial onset could trigger the
Unlike LTP, which is produced synchronization of the cortical oscillators, the sus-
{FIG, 7d), a result that parallels the ensemble recordings
by brief high-frequency tained activity could reflect attentional activation of the
stimulation, LTD can bf of striatal neurons in reproduction procedures"" (FIG, 7e),
produced by long-term, low- Most importantly, simulations show that the SBF model thalamo-cortico-striatal circuits, and the burst at the
frequency stimulation. demonstrates the scalar propert/'^ (FIG, 7f,g). expected time of reward could reflect the updating of

762 ' OCrOBIiR 2005 VOLUME 6 www.nature.com/reviews/neufo


REVIEWS

1,0 1.5
Trial onset Reward Time (s)
Figure 7 | The striatal beat-frequency model. Oscillatory cortical neurons (a) project onto striatal medium spiny neurons (b),
which continuously compare the current pattern of activation of cortical cells with the pattern detected at the time ot the reward
(coincidence detection), Dopaminergic projections from the substantia nigra pars ccmpacta (SNc] and ventral tegmental area
(VTAl are prominently active at trial onset, possibly implementing a 'start-gun' that synchronizes the cortical oscillators;
throughout the interval, possibly modulating corticostriatal transmission; and at the expected time of reward, possibly coding tcr
an error in reward predicticn (c). BG, basal ganglia. Panel d shows five striatal neurcns detecting the coincidence cf the tive
cortical oscillators at the criterion duration. The histcgram of activation of a simulated striatal population (d) matches the pattern
of activation recorded in the slriatum (e), which peaks at the criterion duration. Large scale simulations indicate that the model
can represent the distribution of responses at different intervals (f), and that these distributions superimpose in relative time units,
demonstrating the scalar property of interval timing (g). Panel c reproduced, with permission, from REF, IO2 (2003) American
Association fcr the Advancement of Science. Data in panel e taken from RFF. l u i . Data in panels f and g taken from REF. 85.

cortico-striatal transmission'^" (FIG, 7a.c). Despite giving Conclusions


a comprehensive picture ol the neural circuits that are The ahilit}' to process temporal information accurately is
involved in interval timing, the current instantiation crucial for goal reaching, neuroeconomics''", and tbe sur-
of the SBF model"^ has yet to be developed to address vival of humans and other animals^'*, and requires multi-
the effect of cholinergic drugs on memory storage'^'*', ple biological mechanisms to track time over multiple
simultaneous temporal processing'"^ and the similarities timescales"'"''. In mammals, the circadian clock that drives
between counting and timing^-"''^. metabolic and behavioural rhythms is located in the
Interestingly, coincidence detection and spike count- SCN. Another timer, which is responsible for automatic
ing could be two sides of the same coin. Mathematical motor control in tbe millisecond range, relies on the cer-
studies indicate that the problem of comparing two ebellum. In contrast to these relatively localized timing
neural spike patterns could be solved by different mechanisms, a general-purpose, flexible, cognitively
strategies, depending on the acceptable discrimination controlled timer that operates in the seconds-to-minutes
error'^^. At one end of the spectrum, when the desired range involves the activation of a network of brain areas
discriminative accuracy is high for example, when that form part of the thalamo-cortico-striatal circuits,
the spike patterns code for continuous variables or notably the basal ganglia, the SMA. the PFC and the PPC.
quantities, such as time the solution involves coinci- The operation of this network of circuits as a timekeeper
dence detection. At the other end of the spectrum, can be better understood in terms of coincidental activa-
when the desired discriminative accuracy is low for tion of various brain areas. As these areas are involved in
example, when the patterns code for a discrete variable, several cognitive phenomena, it is likely that this circuit
such as number the solution involves simply count- is not limited to temporal processing, but is also involved
ing the spikes in the two spike patterns. This result sug- in other processes, such as the estimation of quantity or
gests a continuum between coincidence detection and numerosity. This neural circuit might be able to switch
spike counting, and supports a mode-control model function between coincidence detection for estimating
of counting and timing in which number and time are time, to spike counting for estimating numerosity,
processed by the same neural mechanism''^"''\ depending on the accuracy that is required to solve the

NATURE RRVIEWS NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 OCTOBER 2005 TM


REVIEWS

task''*. As such, the coincidence-detection model and the molecular foundations of the 'stopwatch''-''. Finally,
the pacemaker-accumulator model may he two sides the development of modern computational tools and
neural and hebavioural of the same coin. techniques to enable integration of information from
A crucial issue is to differentiate tbe roles of specific all these techniques will soon be crucial tor elucidating
thalamo-cortico-striatal circuits in temporal cognition, the processes that are involved in controlling working
for example, by using ensemhle recording techniques. memory and motor functions, processing time, quantity
Another important question relates to the molecular or numerosity, attending to significant events, making
bases of interval timing; in this respect, the use oftrans- decisions and calculating speed, productivity and rate
genic animal models has the potential to shed light on of return.

1, Grothe, B. New roles for synaptic inhibition in sound 25. Rakitin. B. C. ef ai. Scalar expectancy theory and peak- 49. Woodrow, H. The reproduction of tempor^ intervais. J. Exp
locaiizatbn. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 540-550 (2003) interval timing in humans. J. Exp. PsycW. Anim. Behav Rsychol. 13,473-199 (1930).
2, Richelle, M, SLejeune, H. Time in Animal Behavior ftDcess. 24.15-33(1998). 50. Hoagland, fH. The psychologicai controi of judgements of
(Pergamon, New York, 1980). 26. Penney, T. B., Gibbon, J. & UecW. W. H. Diferentiai eflects duration: evidence for a chemicai ciock. J. Gen Psychoi. 9,
3, Gallisl^, C. R. The Organization of Behavior (MIT Press, of auditory and visuai agnals on ciock speed and temporal 267-287(1933).
Cwnbndge, Massachusetts, 1990). memory. J. Exp Psychoi. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26. 51. Meek, W. H. Selective adjustment Of the speed of internal
A. Meek.'^.H.{e(i.)Functionai and Neural Mechanisms ol 1770-1787(2000). clock and memory processes. J. Exp. Psychot. Anim.
Interval Timing (CRC, Boca Raton, Fiorida, 2003). 27. Edwards, C. J., Alder, T B. 8 Rose, G. J. Auditory midbrain Behav Process. 9,171-201 (1983),
5. Pastor, M.S. Ariieda.J. (eds) Time. Wema/Cfacte and neurons that count. Nature Neurosci. 5, 934-936 (2002). 52 Mancq, A. V. & Church, R. M. The differential effects of
Movement (Eisevier. Amsierdam, 1996). 28. Schirmer, A. Timing speecii: a review of iesion and halopendol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the
6 Bradshaw. C. & SzabaOi, B. (eds) Time and Behaviour: neuroimaging findings. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 269-287 rat. Psychopharmacoiogy (Beri.) 79,1Q-15 (1983).
Psychological and Neurobehavioral Analyses (Eisevier, (2004). 53. Meek, W. H. Affnity for the tlopamine D2 receptor predicts
London, 1997). 29. Maui<, M. D. S Buonomano, D. V TTie neural basis of neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed ot an internal
7. Frajsse. P. Psycholo^ du Temps (P. U. F, Paris. France, temporal processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosd. 27, 307-340 C\oi^. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav 25. 1165-1189(1986).
1957). (2004) 54. Matell. M. S., King, G. R. & Meek, W. H. Differential
8. Gibbon, J., Church, R. M. &Meck,W. H. io Timing and 30. Shaffer, H. in Timing and Time Perceptiof] Vtol. 423 modulation of clock speed by tfie administration of
Time Perception Voi. 423 (eds Gibbon. J. & Allan, L. G.) (eds Gibbon, J. S Alian, L.) 420-428 (The New York intermitteni versus continuous cocaine. BetTav. Neurosci.
52-77 (Ttie New York Academy of Scienoes. Mew Yort<, Academy of Sciences, New Yori^. 1984). 118.150-156(2004).
1984). 31 Gibbon, J.,Morr9l, M. & Silver, R. Two itinds of timing in 55. Rammsayer, T. H. On dopaminergic modulation of temporal
9. Treisman, M. Temporal discriminalion and the indifference circadian incubation rtiythm of ring doves. Am. J. Physiol. information processing. Biol. Psychoi. 36, 209-222 (1993).
interval. Implications tor a model of the intarnaJ ciock'. Regui. Integr Comp. ffty^raf 247. R1083-R1Q87 (1984). 56. Meek, W. i-i. Choline uptake in the frontal cortex is
Psychoi. Monogr 77. 1-31 (1963) 32. Hinton. S. C. S Meek, W. H. Tlie 'intemal cloci<s' of proportional to the absolute error of a temporal memory
10. Gibbon, J.gi Allan, L. G, Timing and Tme Perception (The circadian and interval timing. Endeavour 21, 82 (1997). translation const^t in mature and aged rats. Leam. Motiv.
NewYorkAcademyof Sciences, New York, 1984) 33. Reppert, S. M. & Weaver, D. R. Coordination of circadian 33,88-104(2002).
A classic collection of papers relating to the scalar timing in mammals. Nature 416, 935-941 (2002). 57. Maiapani, C.efa/. Coupied temporal memories in
expectancy theory and other aspects of timing and 34. Levre, J. D., Funes, P, Dowse, H B.8. Hall, J. C. Resetting Parkinson's disease: a dopamine-related dysfunction.
time perception in humans and other animals. the circadian clock by social experience in Drosophila J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 316-331 (1998).
11. Gibbon, J .Malapani.C , Dale, C, L. SGaiiist^. C. R. melanogaster. Science 298, 2010-2012 (2002). 58. Holson, R. R., Bov^r, J. F., Clausing, P S Gough, B.
Toward a neurobiology ol temporai cognition; advances 35. Darlington. T K. ef al. Closing the ciroadian loop: Methamphetamine-stimutated stnatal dopamine release
and chaiienges. Curr. Opin. Neurotiol. 7, 170-184 (1997). cioci<-induced transcrtptbn of its own inhibitors per and tim. declines rapiOly over time following microdialysis probe
12. fi4eck, W. H. Neuropharmacology of timing and time Sc/ence 280.1599-1603 (1998). insertion. Brain. Res. 739, 301 -307 (1996).
perception. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 227-242 36. Lewis, R A.. Miall, R. C, Daan, S. & Kacelnik, A. Interval 59. Buhusi, C V. S Meek, W. H. Differential effects of
(1996). timing in mice does not rely upon the circadian pacen"aker methampiietamine and haloperidoi on the control of an
13. Czeisler, C. A. etai. Stability, precision, and near-24-hour Neurosd. Lett. 348,131-134(2003). intema/clock Behav Neurosd. 116.291-297(2002).
period of the human circadian pacemaker. Science 284, 37. Maiapani, C, Dubois, B., Rancurei, G. & Gibbon. J. 60. Renz, D. & Kital, S. T A basal ganglia pacemaker formed by
2177-2181 (1999). Cerebellar dysfunctions of temporal processing in the the subthaiamic nucleus and extemai globus pallidus.
14. Kacelnik, A, Timing and foraging: Gibbon's scalar seconds range in humans. NeuroreportQ, 3907-3912 (Vafure 400, 677-682(1999).
expectancy theory and optimal patch exploitation. Learn. (1998). 61. Church, R. M. 8 Broadbent, H. A. Aftemative
Motiv.33. 177-195(2002). 38. Harrington, D. L., Lee, R. R., Boyd, L A., Rapcsak, S. Z. & representations of time, number, and rate. Cognition ST,
15. Sohn, W. SCarison, R. implicit temporai tuning of working Knight, R. T Does the representation of time depend on ine 55-61 (1990).
roemory strategy during oognitive skili acquisition Am. cerebellum? Effect of cerebellar stroke. Brain 127. 561 -574 62. Miall, R.C. The storage of tirneintetvals using osoillating
J. Psyc/ro/. 116, 239-256 (2003). (2004). neurons. Neur Comp. 1, 359-371 (1989).
16. Bateson, M. S. Kaoelnik, A. Starling's preferences for 39. Spencer, R. M. C, Zelaznik, H, N,, Diedrichsen, J. S ivry, R. B. 63. Grossberg. S. 8 Schmajuk, N. A. Neural dynamics of
predictabte and unpredictable delays to food. Anim. Behav. Disrupted timing of discontinuous but rrat continuous adaptive timing and temporal discrimination during
53,1129-1142(1997). movements by cerebellar lesions. Science 300, associative learning. iVeuraWefw. 2, 79-102 (1989).
17. Ohyama, T, Gibbon. J., Deich. J. O.S Baisam, R D. 1437-1439(2003). 64. Buhusi, C. V & Schmajuk, N A. Timing in simple
Temporal control dunng maintenance and extinction of 40. Jueptner, M S Weiller, C. A review of difterartces between conditioning and occasion setting: a neural network
conditioned keypecking in ring doves Anim. Learn. Behav. basai gangiia and cerebellar controi of movements as approach. Behav Process. 45, 33-57 (1999).
27,89-98(1999). revealed by functionai imaging studies. Brain 121, 65 Machens, C. K., Romo. R. & Brody, C. D Fiexible contrd of
18. Buhusi. C. V. Sasaki. A, S Meek, W. H. Temporal integration 1437-1449(1998). mutual inhibition, a neurai modei of two-interval
as a function of signai and gap intensity in rats IRattus 41. Shadrr^ehr, R. & iHolcomb, H H. Neural correlates of motor discrimination. Science 307,1121 -1124 (2005).
norvegious) and pigeons [Coiumba livia). J. Comp. Psychoi. memoryconsolidation.Scvence 277,821-625(1997). 66. Dragoi. V, Staddon, J. E., Palmer. R. G. & Buhusi, C. V
116,381-390(2002). 42. Roberts. S. Isolation of an intemai clock. J. Exp. Psycflo/ Interval timing as an emergent iBaming property. PsyiJiol.
19. Drew, M. R., Zupan, B.. Cooke. A., Couviiion, R A. 5 Anim. Behav Process. 7,242-268(1981). Rev 110, 126-144(2003)
Baisam. P. D. Temporai control of conditioned responding in 43. Gibbon, J. Scalar expectancy ttieory and Weber's law in 67. Killeen. R R. & Fetterman, J. G. A behavioral theory of
gddf sh. J. Exp. Psychoi. Anim. Behav Proc. 31, 31 -39 animal liminc| Psychoi Rov 84. 279-325 (1977) timing. Psychoi. Rev. 95, 274-295 (1988).
(2005) This seminal paper introduced the influential scalar 68. Fraisse, P. Perception and estimation of time. Annu Rev
20. Roberts, S, S Church. R, M. Control of an internal clock. expectancy theory. f^ychoi. 35,1-37(1984),
J.Exp. Psych Anim.B^av Process. 4, 318-337(1978). 44. Weber, E. H. Arinotationes Anatomicae et Physiologicae 69. Lewis, P A. & Miall. R. C. n Functionai and t^leural
21. BuhiJsi. C. V, Perera, D. S Meek. W. iH. Memory for timing (Anatomical and Physiological Observations) (C. F. Kohler, Mechanisms of interval Timing {(xi.Ueti\.'\N.H)iyb-S3,2
visusi and auditory signais in aibino and pigmented rats. bpsiae (Leipzig), Germany 1851). (CRC, Boca Raton, Florida, USA, 2003).
J. Exp Psychoi. Ariim. Behav. Process. 31. 18-30 (2005) 45. Meek, W. H. a Maiapani, C. Neuroimaging of interval timing. 70. Maiapani, C, Deweer, B. & Gibbon. J. Separating storage
22. Gallistel, C, R., King, A. & McDonald. R. Sources of Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 21, 133-137 (2004). from retrievai dysfunctbn of temporai rnemory in Parkinson^
variat)ility and systwnatic error in mouse timing beha-vior. 46. Hinton, S. C. \n Functionai and Neural Mechanisms af disease, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 311-322 (2002).
J. Exp. Psychoi. Anim. Behav. Process 30, 3-16 (2004). Interval Timing {ea Meek, W. H.)419-438(CRC, Boca 71. Paulsen, J. S. ef al. fWRi biomarker of earfy neuronal
23. Gribova, A., Donchin, 0., Bergman, iH.. \feadia, E. & Raton, Ronda, 2003). dysfunction in presymptomatic Huntmgton's disease. Am.
Cardoso de Oliveira, S. Timing of bimanual movements in 47. Hinton, S C.& Meek, W.H.Fronlal-striatalcircuitry J. Neuroradiol. 25, 1715-1721 (2004).
human and non-human primates in relation to neuronal activated by human peak-interval timing in the supra- Proposes that the dysfunction of interval timing and
activity in primary motor cortex and supplementar/ motor seconds range. Cogn. Bra/n Res. 21,171-182(2004), the hyperactivation of SMA are early markers of
area. Exp. Brain Res. 146, 322-335 (2002). 48 Francpois, M. Contnbutions a I'etude du sens du temps; Huntington's disease.
24. Brannon, E. M., Roussel. L W., Meek, W. H, SWoldorff, M la temperature interne comme facteur de variation de 72. Rammsayer, T. Temporal discrimination in schcophrenic and
Timing in the baby brain. Cogn. Brain Res 21, 227-233 rappr6ciatbn subjective des durees. Annge Psychoi. 27, affective disorders: evidence for a dopamine-dependent
(2004J, 186-204(1927). intemalclock. W. J. /Veurosci 53,111-120(1990),

784 I OCTOBER 2005 ' VOLUME 6 www.nature.com/reviews/neuro


REVIEWS

73. Tracy. J. \.e!al. Intormalion processingctiaracteristicsot 99. Nieder, A., Freedman, D. J 8 Milier. E. K. Representation 123. Charpier, S 8 Deniau, J. M. In vivo activity-dependent
explhcrt time estimation by patients with schizophrenia and of the quantity of visual items in the primate prefronfai plasticity al cortico striatai eonneefions: evidenoe for
normal controls. Rerceof. Mot. Si-/te 86. 515-526(1998). cortex. Science 297,1708-1711 (2002). physiological long-term potentiation. Proc. NaliAcad. Sd.
74. Malz, H. P. el al. Time estimation in schLzophrenia: an fWRi 100. Shuman, M. & Kanwisher, N. Numerical magnitude in the USA 94, 703&-704G (1997).
Study at adjusted levels of diflicuity. Neurorepori 12, human parietal lobe: tests of representational generality 124. Gubellini, R, Pisani, A., Centonze, D.. Bernardi, G. 8
313-316(2001). and domain specificity. Neuron 44, 557-569 (2004). Caiabresi, P. Metabotropic glutamate receptors and striatal
75. Eiueuag, B., Brown, G. D. A., McCormack, T. Uxisden, J. I. 101. Mafell, M. S., Meek, W. H. & Nicoielis, M. A. intervai timing synaptic plasticity: implications for neurologioal diseases.
8 Goldberg, T. E. Identification of lone duration, iine length, and the encoding of signai duration by ensembles of Prog. Neurobioi. 74, 271-300 (2004).
ana letter poatbn: an experimental approach to limrng and cortical and striatal neurons. Se^av. Neurosci I t 7 , 125. Schultz, W. Neur^ coding of tMSie reward terms of animai
working memory deficits in scnizophrenia. J Abnorm. 760-773 (2003). iearnmg theory, game theory, microeconomics and
Psychoi. 113, 509-521 (2004). Dissociates motor coding from time coding in behavioural ecology Curr Opin. Neurobioi. 14,
76. Penney, T B., Week, W. H., Rolaerts, S. A , Gibixm, J. S. striatal neurons. 139-147(2004).
Erienmeyer-Kimiing, L. intervai-timing deficts in individuals 102. Fioriiio, C. D., Tobler, P. N. 8 Schultz, W. Discrete coding of 126. Sehultz, W. Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nature
at tiigh nsk for sctiizophrenia. Brain Cogn. 58, 109-118 reward probabiiity and uncertainty by dopamine rieurons. Rev. Neurosci. 1.199-207 (2000).
(2005). Sconce 299, 1898-1902(2003}, 127. van Rossum, M. 0. W. A novel spike disfanee. Neurai
77. Woiperl, D M.. Miali, R. C. & Kawato, M. internal models in Shows that in tasks that involve uncertainty, Comput 13, 751-763 (2001).
thecerebelium. Trends Cogn. So 2,338-347(1998). dopaminergic neurons burst at trial onset and at the 128. Glimcfier, R W. Decisions, Uncertainty, ana the Brain: The
78. Ivry, R. B. & Spencer, R. M. Ttie netir^ representation of expected time of reward, and show sustained Science of NeuroeconomicsiMfT Press. Cambridge,
time. Curr. Opin. Neurobioi. 14, 225-232 (2004). activation throughout the trial. Massachusetts. USA, 2004).
79 Koekkoek. S. K. et al. Cerabeiiar LTD and learning- 103. Apicelta, P, Scamati. E . Lungberg. T. & Schuftz, W. 129. Meek, W. H. Inten/al timing and genomies: what makes
dependent timing of conditioned eyelid responses. Science Neuronai activity in monkey striatum reiafed to the mutant mice tick? M. LJ. Comp. Psychoi. 14,211-231
301, 1736-1739(2003). expectation of predictable environmental events. (2001).
80. Rammsayer, T. H. & Brandler, S. Aspects of temporal J. Neurophysiol. 68, 945-960 (1992]. 130. Asehoff, J. in Timing and Time Perception Vol. 423
information processing; a dimensionai analysis. Psychoi. 104. Schultz, W., Apicella, R, Scamati, E. 8 Ljungberg, T (eds Gibbon, J. & Allan, L. G.) 442-468 (The New York
Res. 69, 115-123(2004). Neuronai activity in monkey ventral sthatum related to ftie Acadwny of Seienees, New York, 1984).
81. Ramnnsayer. T H. Neurophannacoiogical evidence for expectation of reward. J. Neurosci. 12,4595-4610(1992). 131. Rousseau, R., PoiHer, J. 8 Lemyre, L. Duration
different timing mechanisms in humans. O. J. Exp. f^ychoi. 105. Ofton,D. a , Wenk,G.L., Church, R.M SMeck, W. H. discnmination of empty time intervals marked by
52,273-2B6(1999). Attention and the frontal cortex as examined by intemiodal pulses Percept. Psychophys. 34. 541-548
82. Pfeuty, M., Ragot, R. & Pouthas, V. Processes involved m simultaneous temporal processing. Neurapsychologia 26, (1983).
tempo perception: a CNV analysis. F^ychophysiology 40, 307-318(1968) 132. Rammsayer, T. H. & Vogel, W. H. Pharmaeologic
69-76(2003). 106. Meek, W. H., Church, R.M..Wenk,G L S,Olton,D. S properties of the internal clock underlying time perception
B3, Meek, W. H. Neuropsychology of timing and time Nucleus basalis magnocelluiaris and medial septai area in Uumms. Neuropsychobiology 2&. 71-80(1992).
perception. Brain Cogn. 58,1-8 (2005), lesions differentialiy impair temporal memory. J. Neurosci. 133. Hamngton, D. L., Healand, K. Y. 8 Hemianowicz, N.
S4 Maleli, M. S. & Meek, W. H. Neuropsycndogical 7,3505-3511 (1987). Temporai processing in the basai gangiia.
mectianisms of interval timing behavior. Bioessays 22, 107. Pang, K. C , Voder, R. M. 8 Olton, D. S. Neurons in the Neuropsychology A2. 3-12(1998).
94-103(2000) lateral agranular frontal cortex have divided attmtion 134. Nagarajan, S. S., Blake. D. T., Wright, B. A., Byl, N. 8
85 Mateii.W.S AMeck, W.H.Cortico-striataicircuitsand correlates in a simultaneous temporal processing task Merzenich, M. M. Practice-related improvements in
interval timing, coincidence delection ot osciliatory Neuros.clence^03 615-628(2001) somatosensory interval discrimination are temporai iy
processes. Cogn. Brain Res. 21,139-170 (2004). A key study showing that cortical neurons are specific but generalize across skin iocafion, hemisphere,
Develops the biological assumptions of Mw SBF activated by simultaneous temporal processing. and modaiity. J. Neurosci. 18, 1559-1570(1998).
model of interval timing, and shows simulations using 108. Couli. J. T. & Nobre, A. C. Where and when tc pay 135. Karmarkar, U. R. & Buonomano, D. V Temporai specrficity
this model. attention: tne neural systems for directing attention to of perceptual learning in an audrtory discrimination task.
86. Meek, W.H. 8 Benson. A. M. Qssecling Ihe brain's intemai spatial locations and to time intervals as rEwealed by both Leam. Mem. 10, 141-147(2003).
ciock: how fronlai-striatal circuitry keeps time and shifts PETandfMRi. t36. Leieune, H. 8 Wearden, J. H. The comparative psychology
attention. Bra/n Cogn 48. 195-311 (2002). J. Neurosci. 18,7426-7435(1998). of fxed-inten/al responding: some quantitative analyses.
87. Ravizza, S. M. Alvry, R. B. Comparison of the basal ganglia 109. Rubia, K. et al. Prefrontai involvement in 'temporai Leam, Motjv. 22, 84-111 (1991).
and cerebellum in shifting attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, bridging' and timing movement. Neuropsychologia 36, 137. Church. R.M. 8 Gibbon, J. Temporai generaiization.
2B5-297 (2001). 1283-1293(1998). J. Exp. Psychoi, Anim. Behav f^ocess.8. 165-186(1982).
88. ivry.R B , Spencer, R. M.,Zelaznik, H. N. &Diednchsen,J. 110. Ferrandez, A. M, et al. Basal ganglia and supplementary 138. Gibbon, J. in The Psychoiogy of Learning and Motivation
The cerebellum and euent timing. Ann. NYAcad. Sd. 978. motor area subtend duration perception: an fMRi study, (ed. Bower, G.) 105-135 (Aeademic, New York, 1966).
302-317(2002), Neuroimage 19, 1532-1544 (2003). 139. Gibbon, J.,Fairhurst,S.S, Goldberg, B.m Tims and
89. Lewis, P. A. 8. Miall. R. C. Distinct systems for automatic and 111. Macar, F., Vidal, F. 8 Casini, L. The supplementary motor Behaviour Psychological and Neurobehaviorai Anaiyses
cognitively controlled time measurement, evidence from area in motor ar>d sensory timing: evidence from slow Voi. 120 (eds Bradshaw, C S Szabadi, E.) 329-374
neuroimaging. Curr. Opin. Neurobioi. 13. 250-255 (2003). brain potential changes Exp. Brain Res. 125, 271-280 (Eisevier, London, 1997).
An original review of the brain areas that are activated (1999). 140. Wearden, J.H., Denovan, L., Fakhri, M. 8 Hawortfi, R.
in neuroimaging studies of interval timing as a 112. Vidai, F., Bonnet, M. & Macar, F. Programming the duration Scalar timing in temporal generalization in humans with
function of the characteristics of ttie timing task. of a nwtor sequence: role of the primary and longer stimulus durations. J. Exp Rsychoi. Anim. Behav.
90. Macar, F. et al. Activation of the supplementary motor area suppiementary motor areas in man. Exp. Brain Res. 106, Process. 23, 502-511 (1997).
and ol attentional networks during temporal processing 339-350(1995} 141. Crystal, J. Orcadian time perception. J. Exp Psychoi.
& p Brain Rss 142.475-485(20021 113. Meek, W. H.&N'Diaye, K. Un mod^e neurot)ioiogiQue de Anim. Behav Process. 27, 68-78 (2001).
An excellent review of neuroimaging studies of la perception et de i'estimation du temps. Psych^ogie 142. Maiapani, C. 8 Rakitin, B.m Functona/arJdWeura/
interval timing that points out the importance of Francaise 50, 47-63 (2005). Mechanisms ofintervai Timing (ed. Meek, W. H.)
adequate control tasks. 114. Lustig, C , Mafell, M. S. 8 Meek, W. H. Not 'just' a 485-513 (CRC, Boca Raton, Flonda, 2003).
91. Nieder A. S Miller, E. K. Coding of cognitive magnitude: coincidence: frontai-striatai interaetions in working 143. Couli, J. T, Vidal, F., Nazanan, B. S. Macar, F Functional
compressed scaling of numencal information in the primate memory and inten/al timing. Memory ^3, 441-448(2005). anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation.
prefrontai cortex. Neuron 37,149-157 (20031. 115. Salinas, E. & Seinowski, T. J. Correiated neuronai activity Science 303, 1506-1508(2004).
92. Meek, W. H. & Church, R. M. A mode control model of and the flow of neural information. Nature Rev. Neurosci. A state-of-the-art neuroimaging study using a
counting and timing processes. J. Exp. F>sychol. Anim. 2,539-550(2001). ctynamic control task and parametric variation of the
Behav Process. 9, 320-334 (1983). 116. Silva, L R.,Amitai,Y.& Connors, B. W. Intrinsic interval-timing procedure.
Describes similarities between timing and counting in osciliations of neocortex generated by layer 5 pyramidal
pre verbal animals. neurons. Science 251, 432-435 (1991). Acknowledgements
93 Feigenson, L.. Dehaene, S. S. Speike, E. Core systems of 117. Gaiarreta, M. 8 Hestrin. S. Spike transmission and We wouid like to thank M. Matell for providing data for f gures 5 and
number. Trends Cogn Sci. 8, 307-314 (2004). synchrony detection in networks of GABAergic 7. This work was supported, in pan, by a grant from the National
94. Walsh. V. A theory ot magnitude: common cortical metrics mterneurons. Science 292, 2295-2299 (2001). Institute of Mental Health to G.V.B. and a JamBS McKeen Cattell
of time, space and quantity. froTc/s Cogn. Set 7. 483-488 11S. Rienie, A., Grun, S., Diesmann, M. S Aertsen, A. Spike award to W.H.M.
(2003). synchronization and rafe modulation diffefentialiy involved
95. Brannon, E.M. SRoitman, J. D. in Fundonaland Neural in motor cortical function, Soence 278, 1950-1953 Competing interests statement
Mechanisms ofintervai Timing (ed. Meek, W. H.) 143-162 (t997), The aiitfiors declare no competing financial interests.
(CRC, Boca Raton, Fbrida, USA, 2003). 119. Steinnietz, P. N. ef al. Attention modulates synchronized
96. Couli, J. T., Fnth, C. D., Buchel, C. 8 Nobre, A. C. Orienting neuronal firing in primate somatosensory eortex. Nature
attention in time: taehavioural and neuroanatomcai 404, 187-190(2000). i^y Online links
distinction between exogenous and endogenous stiifts. 120. Fries, P, Reynolds, J. H., Rone, A. E. &Desimone, R.
Neuropsychologia 38, 808-819 (2000). Modulation of osciiiatory neuronal synchronization by DATABASES
97. Sawamura, H.,Shima, K. STanji, J. Numericai selective visual attention. Science 291, 1560-1563 (2001). Tlw fallowing terms In thia artlcla are linked online te:
repfesentafion for action in the parietal cortex of the 121. Beiser,D G. 8Houk, J. C. Model of cortieal-tjasal OMIM; http ://www. rrobi.nlni.nih .gov/entrez/query. fcgi?db-OMIM
monkev. NalureAiS. 918-922 (2002). ganglionic processing: encoding the seriai order of sensory Huntington's disease | Parkinson's disease
Shows changes in the firing of PPC neurons during a evwits. Clin. Neurophysioi. 79, 3168-3168(1998).
sequence of actions, 122. Houk, J. C. \r\ Modeis of Information Processing in the FURTHER INFORMATION
'iS Nieder, A S Miller, E. K. A paneto-frontal network for visual Basa/Gang//a(edsHouk, j . C D a v i s , J.L. 8Beiser, D. G.) The Intemet Encyctopedia of Philosophy: Time: hrtp://www.
numencal information in the monkey. Proc. NatlAcad. Sa. 3-10 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA, lep uim.edu/t/t-me htn
USA 101, 7457-7162 (2004). 1995), Access to this Interactive links box it free onlliM.

NATURE Rr.VIEWS NEUROSCIENCE VOLUME 6 OCTOBER 200!^ , 768

Potrebbero piacerti anche