Sei sulla pagina 1di 5

Computers in Human Behavior 38 (2014) 208212

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Changes in technology use and adult attachment orientation from 2002


to 2012 q
William J. Chopik , Christopher Peterson
Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: We have lived in an age of ever-increasing connection this past decade. Although technology and online
Available online 1 July 2014 social networking may not have increased psychological closeness, as some researchers suggest, it may
have increased the perceived availability of other people in our social networks. The current study
Keywords: measures changes in comfort with closeness (attachment avoidance) and perceived availability of others
Attachment (attachment anxiety) in a large Internet sample (N = 123,554) from 2002 to 2012. Attachment anxiety
Technology decreased from 2002 to 2012 and attachment avoidance exhibited no changes over the same time period.
Cohort changes
Further, these decreases were primarily driven by younger adults, presumably due to their heavy
Media
Anxiety
technology use. Attachment anxiety was negatively associated with mobile phone subscription rates
and the number of Facebook users over this time period. Results from the current study suggest that
the perceived availability of others has been increasing over timeperhaps because technology has
increased the accessibility of close others.
2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction psychological closeness, it may have increased the perceived


availability of other people in our social networks. Increases in this
We have lived in an age of ever-increasing connection this past perceived availability could have large implications for how indi-
decade. Cell phones and social networking sites have made individ- viduals interact with others on- and ofine, but few studies have
uals more accessible to each other than ever before. Time spent examined changes in psychological characteristics conceptually
exposed to new media (e.g., cell phones, texting, video games, related to technology use over this timeframe. Specically, very
TV, Internet) among children has exploded from nearly 10 h per little is known about how individuals approach toward close
week in the 1930s to 11 h per day by the 2000s (Gutnick, Robb, relationships (i.e., their attachment orientation) is related to this
Takeuchi, & Kotler, 2011). Most online social networks (e.g., Friend- recent increase in technology usage. The current study measures
ster (2002), Myspace (2003), Facebook (2004), and Twitter (2006)) changes in comfort with closeness (attachment avoidance) and
have also started in the past ten years (Lapinski, 2006). Almost half of perceived availability of others (attachment anxiety) in a large
American Internet users now have at least one online social prole Internet sample from 2002 to 2012.
(Arbitron, 2010). According to one report, time spent social network-
ing is up 82% from previous years as of 2009 (Whitney, 2010). 1.1. Societal-level changes in attachment orientation
Despite these trends, Konrath (2012) suggests that recent gen-
erations have been experiencing an empathy paradox, in which An individuals attachment orientation is generally conceptual-
technology has brought people closer together than ever before, ized as their position on two conceptually distinct dimensions:
but that this increase in technological closeness has not been anxiety and avoidance (Fraley & Waller, 1998). Attachment-related
accompanied by a sense of psychological closeness. Although anxiety reects hyperactivation of the attachment system and
technology and online social networking may not have increased preoccupation with the availability of close others (Mikulincer,
Gillath, & Shaver, 2002). For instance, individuals with higher anx-
iety scores exhibit excessive reassurance-seeking and hypervigi-
q
The author was supported by a National Science Foundation Graduate Research lance to signs of rejection and abandonment (Fraley, Niedenthal,
Fellowship.
Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology, University of Marks, Brumbaugh, & Vicary, 2006; Shaver, Schachner, &
Michigan, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109, United States. Tel.: +1 Mikulincer, 2005). The avoidance dimension is characterized by
(734) 763 5146; fax: +1 (734) 647 9440. chronic attempts to inhibit attachment-system activation in an
E-mail address: chopik@umich.edu (W.J. Chopik). effort to minimize expressions of distress (Edelstein & Shaver,

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.031
0747-5632/ 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
W.J. Chopik, C. Peterson / Computers in Human Behavior 38 (2014) 208212 209

2004; Fraley, Davis, & Shaver, 1998). For instance, individuals with who participated between September 2002 and March 2012. The
higher avoidance scores generally tend to dislike intimacy and are website is supported by researchers from the University of Penn-
less likely to provide emotional support for romantic partners sylvania. Participants self-reported their age by selecting one of
(Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998; Li & Chan, 2012). Individuals seven discrete age groups (i.e., 1820 years old, 2124, 2534,
reporting low scores on both dimensions are generally considered 3544, 4554, 5564, 65+; Mdnage category = 2534 years old). The
secure (or high on attachment security). majority of the sample (70.1%) had at least a bachelors degree.
Why might one expect attachment orientations to be changing Information on respondent-level ethnicity, sexual orientation,
in recent years? Research comparing the personality traits of dif- and relationship status was unavailable. All users were from the
ferent cohorts (e.g., Twenge, 2006) suggests that social/cultural United States. Participants created a prole on authentichappi-
conditions of a particular time period can shape an individuals ness.com and were provided with several questionnaires related
attachment orientation. One unique feature of the last decade is to the eld of positive psychology. Participation was voluntary
the rapid increase in the use of technology. Cell phones and online and participants received feedback on their attachment orientation
social networking sites presumably make it easier to reach out and following completion. Duplicate responses were ltered out prior
check on the availability of close others (Sheldon, Abad, & Hinsch, to data analysis and reporting. Use of these questionnaires for
2011). Without a means of communicating or checking in easily, online data collection has been approved by the University of
individuals may be concerned about the availability of the people Pennsylvania Institutional Review Board.
on whom they depend. Therefore, technology that enables individ-
uals to quickly contact each other (i.e., cell phones, texting, social 2.2. Adult attachment
networking sites) may afford individuals a sense of security know-
ing that close others are more readily available and therefore might The Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised Inventory (ECR-
decrease attachment anxiety. In support of this hypothesis, R; Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000) was used to assess attachment
Sheldon and colleagues (2011) discovered that higher Facebook anxiety and avoidance. The 18-item avoidance subscale (a = .91)
usage is associated with greater feelings of satisfaction with the reects an individuals discomfort with closeness and intimacy.
connection to ones social network. Further, individuals who felt The 18-item anxiety subscale (a = .90) reects an individuals con-
more disconnected after being deprived of Facebook access for a cern about abandonment and the perceived availability of close
48-h period responded with greater usage rates of social media others. Sample items include I dont feel comfortable opening up
during a second, unconstrained 48-h period. Electronic communi- to romantic partners (avoidance), and I often worry that my part-
cation is also associated with greater feelings of availability among ner doesnt really love me (anxiety). Participants rated the extent
long-distance couples (Boneva, Kraut, & Frohlich, 2001; Dainton & to which they agreed with each statement, using a 7-point Likert
Aylor, 2002; Stephen, 1986) and is associated with lower relational scale, ranging from 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly). Items
uncertainty (i.e., doubts about the status of or involvement in a were averaged to create subscales for avoidance (M = 2.87,
relationship; Jin & Pea, 2010). Further, mobile phone experiments SD = 1.25) and anxiety (M = 3.29, SD = 1.42). The correlation
suggest that texting supportive messages has the potential to boost between anxiety and avoidance was .49 (p < .001), which is consis-
attachment security (Otway, Carnelley, & Rowe, 2013). Greater tent with previous research using the ECR-R.
social networking use is also associated with greater feelings of
intimacy and support among individuals high in attachment anxi-
2.3. Mobile phone subscriptions
ety, suggesting that social networking may be associated with
lower concerns about the availability of close others (Morey,
The annual number of mobile phone subscriptions per 100 peo-
Gentzler, Creasy, Oberhauser, & Westerman, 2013).
ple in the US from 2002 to 2012 was retrieved from The World
Given that technology use may provide individuals with a sense
Banks open database of social indicators (World Bank., 2014).
of security with respect to the availability of close others by increas-
The number of mobile phone subscriptions per 100 people has
ing their perceived accessibility, attachment anxiety may have been
risen dramatically from 2002 to 2012 (2002: 49 subscriptions/
decreasing over the past decadecoinciding with increases in tech-
100, 2007: 82 subscriptions/100, 2012: 95 subscriptions/100).
nology use. Further, decreases in anxiety should be particularly pro-
nounced among younger adults, due to the heavy usage of
technology and social networking sites by this group. Thus, attach- 2.4. Use of social networking sites
ment anxiety should be lowest in years when technology use was
at its highest. Based on Konrath (2012)s observation that individu- The annual number of Facebook users was used as a proxy for
als have not experienced an increase in psychological closeness in social networking site use as it is one of the most popular social
the past decade, individuals should not be changing with respect networking sites and has existed for most of the time period of
to their comfort with closeness (attachment avoidance) over this the current study. The number of Facebook users per year was only
time period. In the current study, we examined changes in attach- available from 2004 (when the site launched) until 2012
ment anxiety and avoidance from 2002 to 2012 using a large Inter- (Facebook.com., 2013). The number of Facebook users has risen
net sample. This large sample allowed us to test the hypothesis that dramatically from 2004 to 2012 (2004: 1 million users, 2007:
attachment anxiety had been decreasing over the past decade. We 100 million users, 2012: 1.1 billion users).
also utilized publicly accessible data on the number of mobile phone
subscriptions and Facebook users (as a measure of social networking 3. Results
site usage) to test the hypothesis that higher technology usage was
associated with lower attachment anxiety. 3.1. Is attachment anxiety declining from 2002 to 2012?

2. Method To formally model the associations between year and the


attachment dimensions, we conducted multiple regression analy-
2.1. Participants and procedure ses predicting anxiety and avoidance from year of survey comple-
tion while controlling for participant age, gender, and attachment
Participants were 123,554 adult users, ranging in age from 18 to orientation (e.g., when predicting anxiety, avoidance was
65+ (73.5% female), from an Internet site (authentichappiness.com) controlled for). To assess whether decreases in anxiety were being
210 W.J. Chopik, C. Peterson / Computers in Human Behavior 38 (2014) 208212

driven by younger adults, an interaction between age and year was 3.50
also included in the regression analyses.
Regression results are presented in Table 1. As hypothesized,
3.45
year was negatively associated with attachment anxiety such that

Attachment Anxiety
anxiety decreased from 2002 to 2012. Younger adults and women
were higher in anxiety, replicating previous research (Chopik, 3.40
Edelstein, & Fraley, 2013; Del Giudice, 2011). The year  age inter-
action emerged as a signicant predictor of anxiety. To decompose 3.35
the signicant year  age interaction, we conducted separate
regression analyses predicting attachment anxiety from year, gen-
der, and avoidance among younger (34 years old and younger) and 3.30
older adults (35 years old and older). As expected, year was nega-
tively associated with anxiety among younger individuals 3.25
(b = .01, p < .001), but not so among individuals over the age of 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012
35 (b = .004, p = .12). As seen in Fig. 1, attachment anxiety Year
decreased from 2002 to 2012 among younger adults (2002 vs.
Fig. 1. Changes in attachment anxiety among younger adults from 2002 to 2012.
2012: d = .14).
Identical regression analyses were also conducted for attach-
ment avoidance (controlling for anxiety). As expected, year and 4. Discussion
the interaction between year and age were not signicant predic-
tors of avoidance. Older adults and men were higher in avoidance, The current study demonstrates that attachment anxiety has
replicating previous research (Chopik et al., 2013; Del Giudice, been decreasing at a societal-level in the last decade. We examined
2011). associations between year and attachment orientation in a sample
of 123,554 participants from 2002 to 2012. Moreover, we examined
the association between the number of mobile phone subscriptions
3.2. Is attachment anxiety related to rates of mobile phone
and Facebook users and attachment orientation over this same time
subscriptions and social networking site usage?
period. Attachment anxiety decreased over this time period, partic-
ularly among younger adultsa population that uses technology at a
To examine whether the mobile phone and Facebook usage in
high rate. Further, in the years during which mobile phone subscrip-
a particular year was associated with the attachment dimen-
tions and Facebook users were at their highest, participants reported
sions, we created two separate multilevel random-coefcient
lower attachment anxiety. The current study is the rst of its kind to
models using STATA (2013). Respondents were nested within a
examine societal-level changes in attachment orientation over this
calendar year (20022012). Participant age, gender, annual
time period. Decreases in attachment anxiety among younger adults
mobile phone subscriptions (Model 1), and annual number of
are consistent with the hypothesis that increased technology use is
Facebook users (Model 2) were treated as predictors of respon-
associated with a perceived availability of close others. No changes
dent-level scores of attachment anxiety and avoidance. As in
in attachment avoidance over time were found, consistent with
our regression analyses, each model controlled for attachment
Konraths (2012) observation that technology has not increased a
orientation when appropriate (e.g., when predicting anxiety,
sense of psychological closeness.
avoidance was controlled for). The effects of age, gender, and
These decreases in anxiety are also consistent with previous
attachment orientation in both models were identical to those
research documenting the proximityenhancing effects of tech-
reported in the regression analyses. In Model 1, the number of
nology (Morey et al., 2013; Sheldon et al., 2011). Individuals high
mobile phone subscriptions per 100 people was associated with
in attachment anxiety primarily worry about the availability of
lower attachment anxiety, b = 0.001, b = 0.02, 95% condence
close others. However, technology enhances the perceived avail-
interval = [ 0.002, 0.001], SE = 0.0003, Z = 4.58, p < .001. The
ability of others, perhaps reducing these concerns and worries.
number of mobile phone subscriptions was unrelated to attach-
Attachment anxiety has generally decreased in the past decade
ment avoidance, p = .31. In Model 2, the number of Facebook
coinciding with a dramatic increase in technology usage.
users was also associated with lower attachment anxiety,
However, greater technology use may not be universally bene-
b = 4.45  10 11, b = 0.02, 95% condence interval = [ 7.81 
cial for people, particularly those high in attachment anxiety.
10 11, 1.08  10 11], SE = 1.72  10 11, Z = 2.59, p = .01. The
Recent research suggests that greater usage of online social net-
number of Facebook users was unrelated to attachment avoid-
working is associated with more social comparisons (Chou &
ance, p = .61.

Table 1
Regressions predicting attachment dimensions from year, age, and gender.

Anxiety Avoidance
B SE (B) b t B SE (B) b t
Year .005 .001 .01 4.04** .001 .001 .002 .63
Age .10 .002 .11 41.95** .07 .002 .09 33.52**
Gender .04 .004 .02 9.80** .01 .004 .01 2.64**
Avoidance/Anxiety .56 .003 .49 201.14** .44 .002 .50 201.14**
Year  Age .003 .001 .01 3.33** .001 .001 .002 .85

Note. N = 123,554; Equation for anxiety, F (5, 123,548) = 8355.90, R = .50, p < .001; Equation for avoidance, F (5, 123,548) = 8164.84, R = .50, p < .001; Gender: 1 = men,
1 = women.
**
p < .01.
W.J. Chopik, C. Peterson / Computers in Human Behavior 38 (2014) 208212 211

Edge, 2012; Haferkamp & Krmer, 2011) and lower subjective has on individuals, particularly to see if it decreases attachment
well-being (Kross et al., 2013). Further, individuals high in attach- anxiety.
ment anxiety are more likely to be the recipient of a technology- Fourth, the results of the current study are somewhat inconsis-
mediated break-up with a romantic partner (Weisskirch & Delevi, tent with other research examining generational changes in psy-
2013). This nding suggests that, although technology may afford chological characteristics. In particular, Twenge (2000) found that
a sense of enhanced accessibility to other people, it does not nec- American college students have been increasing in (generalized)
essarily translate to more fullling relationships. These ndings anxiety from 1952 to 1993. Twenge suggests that the increase in
then leave open the question: as technology is becoming increas- generalized anxiety over this time period primarily results from
ingly integral in peoples lives and relationships, what are the dramatic increases in overall societal threat (e.g., crime rates)
psychological functions that these media are serving (Sheldon and decreases in the quality of social relationships (e.g., using
et al., 2011)? Future research can examine how these recent divorce rates as a proxy). We believe the ndings from the current
changes in attachment anxiety translate to how individuals inter- study are unique in two ways. First, although attachment anxiety is
act with one another and maintain relationships on- and ofine. moderately intercorrelated with neuroticism (Noftle & Shaver,
Despite the many strengths of the current study, some limita- 2006), attachment anxiety is more closely related to anxiety about
tions still exist. First, our large Internet sample is a convenience ones social network and the availability of close others rather than
sample comprised of volunteers. Previous studies have shown that a more general form of anxiety (and often predicts relationship
Internet-based samples can provide useful and valid data for psy- outcomes over-and-above the contributions of neuroticism). Thus,
chological research (Srivastava, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2003). examining anxiety that is domain-specic (i.e., specic to relation-
Moreover, such samples are often more diverse than traditional ships) may lead to different ndings when examining change over
undergraduate samples with respect to age, ethnicity, nationality, time than anxiety measured more globally. Further, previous
relationship status, and income (Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava, & research has linked technology usage to more positive feelings
John, 2004). However, Internet use may have become more ubiqui- about the availability of close others, particularly among individu-
tous from 2002 to 2012, and thus the population using the Internet als high in attachment anxiety (Morey et al., 2013; Sheldon et al.,
in 2002 may be very different in some special way than the popu- 2011). Second, Twenges investigation did not include measures
lation using the Internet in 2012. Researchers have examined of attachment anxiety or the frequency of technology use by
whether Internet respondents differ with respect to psychological college students over this time period. The data from her meta-
constructs compared to more traditional student samples, older analysis was also collected during a time that preceded the found-
individuals, and those less technologically skilled and found that ing of several social networking sites and the dramatic increases in
rarely do they differ (Gosling et al., 2004; Srivastava et al., 2003). Internet usage, which was the focus of the current study (e.g., par-
Worth noting is that greater familiarity and use of the Internet, ticularly in the past decade). It is also possible that other forms of
mobile phones, and social networking sites over time would also technology (e.g., digital planners, calendars, etc.) in the last ten
be consistent with our hypotheses: that individuals are relying years have decreased other domain-specic features of anxiety or
on these media at an ever-increasing rate and this reliance is lead- has led to a tailing off of the increases in anxiety observed among
ing to psychological changes in the broader population. Neverthe- younger populations. Future research can examine the temporal
less, the ndings from the current study should be interpreted with course of both general anxiety and attachment anxiety over this
caution until they are replicated in a more representative sample. time period to see how technology interacts with an individuals
Second, we were not able to assess the frequency of technology psychological characteristics to alter his or her social behavior.
use among our participants as the data collection website is pri- Despite these limitations, the current study nds support for
marily used for scale development and assessment. In a study the hypothesis that attachment anxiety has been decreasing over
examining attachment orientation and the frequency of technology the past ten years, particularly during years with high numbers
use, Morey and colleagues (2013) found that individuals high in of mobile phone subscriptions and Facebook users. This nding
anxiety were just as likely to use social networking sites and provides an important rst step in examining how the increase
texting as their less anxious counterparts. However, despite the in technology use in recent years is affecting individuals. Examin-
lack of attachment-related differences in frequency of use, individ- ing how individuals are inuenced by their experiences with tech-
uals high in anxiety could be affected by technology in ways that nology is an exciting avenue for future research (Morey et al.,
individuals low in anxiety are not. The motivations underlying 2013). Nevertheless, results from the current study suggest that
their use could also be different among individuals high or low the perceived availability of others has been increasing over
in attachment anxiety. Future experimental work can manipulate timeperhaps because technology has increased their
the frequency, depth, and duration of technology-mediated com- accessibility.
munication to see if technology use causes decreases in attach-
ment anxiety according to the mechanisms we are proposing References
(e.g., by increasing the perceived availability of others).
Third, the current study was correlational in nature and it is Arbitron (2010). Use of social media explodes almost half of americans have proles
impossible to know with certainty what has been causing these says new arbitron/edison research study. <http://arbitron.mediaroom.com/
index.php?s=43&item=682>.
decreases in attachment anxiety from 2002 to 2012. Within each Boneva, B., Kraut, R., & Frohlich, D. (2001). Using e-mail for personal relationships:
year, the median age, level of education, and gender distribution The difference gender makes. American Behavioral Scientist, 530549.
of the sample were largely consistent, making it unlikely that these Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.),
changes can be attributed to shifts in these demographic factors. Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 4676). New York, NY US: Guilford
We also controlled for these variables in our analyses. The few Press.
studies that have manipulated the use of mobile messaging or Chopik, W. J., Edelstein, R. S., & Fraley, R. C. (2013). From the cradle to the grave: Age
differences in attachment from early adulthood to old age. Journal of Personality,
social networking site usage generally nd that technology has
171183.
the potential to decrease attachment anxiety (Kross et al., 2013; Chou, H.-T. G., & Edge, N. (2012). They are happier and having better lives than i
Otway et al., 2013; Sheldon et al., 2011). Nevertheless, the current am: The impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others lives.
study is merely correlational and thus, opens the patterns in data Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 117120.
Dainton, M., & Aylor, B. (2002). Patterns of communication channel use in the
up to alternative explanations. Future research can manipulate maintenance of long-distance relationships. Communication Research Reports,
technology usage and measure the psychological effect that this 118129.
212 W.J. Chopik, C. Peterson / Computers in Human Behavior 38 (2014) 208212

Del Giudice, M. (2011). Sex differences in romantic attachment: A meta-analysis. Li, T., & Chan, D. K. S. (2012). How anxious and avoidant attachment affect romantic
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 193214. relationship quality differently: A meta-analytic review. European Journal of
Edelstein, R. S., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Avoidant attachment: Exploration of an Social Psychology, 406419.
oxymoron. In D. J. Mashek & A. P. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and intimacy Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of the attachment
(pp. 397412). Mahwah, NJ US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. system in adulthood: Threat-related primes increase the accessibility of mental
Facebook.com. (2013). Facebook statistics Retrieved October 14, 2013, from representations of attachment gures. Journal of Personality and Social
<https://www.facebook.com/press/info.php?statistics>. Psychology, 881895.
Fraley, R. C., Davis, K. E., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Dismissing-avoidance and the Morey, J. N., Gentzler, A. L., Creasy, B., Oberhauser, A. M., & Westerman, D. (2013).
defensive organization of emotion, cognition, and behavior. In J. A. Simpson & Young adults use of communication technology within their romantic
W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 249279). New relationships and associations with attachment style. Computers in Human
York, NY US: Guilford Press. Behavior, 17711778.
Fraley, R. C., Niedenthal, P. M., Marks, M., Brumbaugh, C., & Vicary, A. (2006). Adult Noftle, E. E., & Shaver, P. R. (2006). Attachment dimensions and the big ve
attachment and the perception of emotional expressions: Probing the personality traits: Associations and comparative ability to predict relationship
hyperactivating strategies underlying anxious attachment. Journal of quality. Journal of Research in Personality, 179208.
Personality, 11631190. Otway, L. J., Carnelley, K. B., & Rowe, A. C. (2013). Texting boosts felt security.
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis Attachment & Human Development, 93101.
of self-report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Shaver, P. R., Schachner, D. A., & Mikulincer, M. (2005). Attachment style, excessive
Psychology, 350365. reassurance seeking, relationship processes, and depression. Personality and
Fraley, R. C., & Waller, N. G. (1998). Adult attachment patterns: A test of the Social Psychology Bulletin, 343359.
typological model. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and Sheldon, K. M., Abad, N., & Hinsch, C. (2011). A two-process view of Facebook use
close relationships (pp. 77114). New York, NY US: Guilford Press. and relatedness need-satisfaction: Disconnection drives use, and connection
Gosling, S. D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., & John, O. P. (2004). Should we trust web- rewards it. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 766775.
based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about internet Srivastava, S., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2003). Development of
questionnaires. American Psychologist, 93104. personality in early and middle adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent
Gutnick, A., Robb, M., Takeuchi, L., & Kotler, J. (2011). Always connected: The new change? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10411053.
digital media habits of young children. New York: Sesame Workshop. STATA (2013). Stata for windows. College Station, TX: Stata Press.
Haferkamp, N., & Krmer, N. C. (2011). Social comparison 2.0: Examining the effects Stephen, T. (1986). Communication and interdependence in geographically
of online proles on social-networking sites. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and separated relationships. Human Communication Research, 191210.
Social Networking, 309314. Twenge, J. M. (2000). The age of anxiety? The birth cohort change in anxiety and
Jin, B., & Pea, J. F. (2010). Mobile communication in romantic relationships: Mobile neuroticism, 19521993. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
phone use, relational uncertainty, love, commitment, and attachment styles. 10071021.
Communication Reports, 3951. Twenge, J. M. (2006). Generation me: Why todays young Americans are more
Konrath, S. H. (2012). The empathy paradox: Increasing disconnection in the age of condent, assertive, entitledand more miserable than ever before. New York, NY
increasing connection. In R. Luppicini (Ed.), Handbook of research on technoself: US: Free Press.
Identity in a technological society. IGI Global. Weisskirch, R. S., & Delevi, R. (2013). Attachment style and conict resolution skills
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., et al. (2013). Facebook predicting technology use in relationship dissolution. Computers in Human
use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PLoS ONE. Behavior, 25302534.
e69841. Whitney, L. (2010). Twitter, Facebook us up 82 percent. <http://news.cnet.com/8301-
Lapinski, T. (2006). Myspace: The businesss of spam 2.0. <http://valleywag. gawker.com/ 1023_3-10457480-93.html>.
tech/myspace/myspace-the-business-of-spam-20-exhaustive-edition-199924. World Bank. (2014). Mobile cellular subscriptions (per 100 people). Retrieved May 10,
php>. 2014 <http://data.worldbank.org/indicator/IT.CEL.SETS.P2>.

Potrebbero piacerti anche