Sei sulla pagina 1di 17

THOMAS N.

BRADBURY University of CaliforniaLos Angeles

FRANK D. FINCHAM State University of New YorkBuffalo

STEVEN R. H. BEACH University of Georgia

Research on the Nature and Determinants of Marital


Satisfaction: A Decade in Review

Scientific study of marital satisfaction attracted Even when compared with the high level of schol-
widespread attention in the 1990s from scholars arly output in previous decades, the 1990s wit-
representing diverse orientations and goals. This nessed a vast number of papers published on a
article highlights key conceptual and empirical wide array of topics pertaining to marital satisfac-
advances that have emerged in the past decade, tion. The sheer magnitude of this work attests to
with particular emphasis on (a) interpersonal pro- the continued importance placed on understanding
cesses that operate within marriage, including the quality of marriage, as an end in itself and as
cognition, affect, physiology, behavioral pattern- a means to understanding its effect on numerous
ing, social support, and violence; (b) the milieus other processes inside and outside the family. The
within which marriages operate, including micro- rationale for studying marital satisfaction stems
contexts (e.g., the presence of children, life stres- from its centrality in individual and family well-
sors and transitions) and macrocontexts (e.g., being (e.g., Stack & Eshleman, 1998), from the
economic factors, perceived mate availability); benefits that accrue to society when strong mar-
and (c) the conceptualization and measurement of riages are formed and maintained (e.g., desistance
marital satisfaction, including 2-dimensional, tra- from crime; Laub, Nagin, & Sampson, 1998), and
jectory-based, and social-cognitive approaches. from the need to develop empirically defensible
Notwithstanding the continued need for theoreti-
interventions for couples that prevent (e.g., Hahl-
cal progress in understanding the nature and de-
weg, Markman, Thurmaier, Engl, & Eckert, 1998)
terminants of marital satisfaction, we conclude by
or alleviate (e.g., Baucom, Shoham, Mueser, Daiu-
calling for more large-scale longitudinal research
that links marital processes with sociocultural to, & Stickle, 1998) marital distress and divorce.
contexts, for more disconfirmatory than confirm- The present analysis comes at a time when the
atory research, and for research that directly American divorce rate has declined for the eighth
guides preventive, clinical, and policy-level inter- straight year, owing, perhaps, to the sharp increase
ventions. in the age at first marriage over this same period
(U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1998). Nonetheless,
about half of all first marriages are projected to
Department of Psychology, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA end in permanent separation or divorce, the level
900951563 (bradbury@psych.ucla.edu).
of satisfaction in intact first marriages has de-
Key Words: communication, divorce, intervention, longi- clined since at least the mid-1970s (National Mar-
tudinal study, marital satisfaction. riage Project, 1999; Rogers & Amato, 1997), and

964 Journal of Marriage and the Family 62 (November 2000): 964980


Marital Satisfaction 965

there is growing recognition that marital strife pri- a topic that continues to attract attention from
or to divorce accounts, in part, for the widely pub- marital and family scholars and that has evolved
licized differences in functioning between chil- in important ways in the past 10 years. In address-
dren who do and do not come from households ing these three themes, we acknowledge and em-
marked by divorce (see Amato, this volume; phasize at the outset that we are psychologists by
Amato & Booth, 1997). Further tempering any training with strong interests in refining theory,
optimism elicited by the slowing divorce rate is collecting data, and developing interventions with
recent evidence that, on average, marital satisfac- the applied goal of bringing about stronger mar-
tion probably does not follow a U-shaped function riages and families. This probably leads us to fo-
over the marital career, as was once believed (e.g., cus more on marital processes and differences be-
Rollins & Feldman, 1970), but instead drops tween couples in marital processes than is typical
markedly over the first 10 years of marriage on of prior reviews of marital satisfaction appearing
average and then drops more gradually in the en- in this forum, and it yields an analysis that com-
suing decades (Glenn, 1998; Vaillant & Vaillant, plements rather than updates explicitly the most
1993). Systematic study of marital satisfaction recent decade review written by Glenn (1990).
therefore remains vital, and the social significance
of studying how and why marriages vary in their
INTERPERSONAL PROCESSES IN MARRIAGE
quality is matched only by the complex range of
factors that must be considered when doing so. Detailed analysis of the behaviors exchanged by
The impressive breadth and scope of work on spouses was instigated more than 25 years ago, in
marital satisfaction in the 1990s shows that re- part by Harold Raush and colleagues assertion
search on this topic is not a literature unto itself that Studying what people say about themselves
but is dispersed over several overlapping, yet gen- is no substitute for studying how they behave . . . .
erally distinct, literatures. These focus, for exam- Questionnaires and scales of marital satisfaction
ple, on psychological factors, sociodemographic and dissatisfaction have yielded very little. We
variables and trends, parenting, physical health, need to look at what people do with one another
and psychopathology, or some combination of (Raush, Barry, Hertel, & Swain, 1974, p. 5) and
these, all in relation to some aspect of marital in part by research-oriented clinicians who sought
quality. It is not possible to capture the subtleties to study how maritally discordant spouses shaped
and nuances of each of these literatures in a single each others coercive behaviors and thereby
review and, arguably, little would be gained from caused or perpetuated their discord (e.g., Stuart,
a large-scale integration of specific findings. 1969). Interest in understanding interpersonal pro-
In view of these constraints, the task we have cesses in marriage remains strong, yet research re-
set for ourselves in this article is to identify and ported in the 1990s indicated that, despite some
explore a series of key ideas and emerging trends advances, these processes are not easily studied,
that may be germane to scholars who approach and a comprehensive understanding of them is not
the study of marital satisfaction with diverse goals yet at hand.
and agendas. The article is organized around two In keeping with its applied clinical origins, re-
themes that we believe represent the sine qua non cent research on interpersonal processes in mar-
of a thorough understanding of variability in mar- riage retains a strong focus on behaviors ex-
ital satisfaction, namely, the interpersonal pro- changed during marital conflict and marital
cesses that operate within marriages and the so- problem-solving discussions. To understand this
ciocultural ecologies and contexts within which focus and the findings that accumulated in the
marriages operate. We adopt this distinction be- 1990s, it is necessary to consider research trends
cause we believe it serves well in organizing re- from earlier decades. The need to capture inter-
search conducted on marital satisfaction in the dependencies between husband and wife behavior,
1990s and because doing so draws attention to the as distinct from the raw number or proportion of
constraints to understanding that arise from anal- behaviors displayed by the husband and the wife,
yses of interpersonal processes bereft of their en- became evident early in this line of work. The
vironmental milieus and from ecological or con- resulting methodological sophistication yielded
textual analyses that fail to consider what compelling findings about the sequential patterns
transpires between spouses. A third and final of behavior that differentiated maritally distressed
theme emphasized in the article is the conceptu- and nondistressed couples. For example, Margolin
alization and measurement of marital satisfaction, and Wampold (1981) showed that, compared with
966 Journal of Marriage and the Family

those of happy couples, the interactions of dis- importance of attributions in marital functioning,
tressed couples were characterized by higher lev- a host of new and important questions now pres-
els of negative reciprocity (i.e., increased likeli- ent themselves. These include questions about at-
hood of negative behavior following negative tributions themselves, such as whether specific
behavior by the partner) and by higher levels of patterns of attributions correspond with distinct
negative reactivity (i.e., suppression of positive emotional expressions (e.g., anger versus sadness)
behaviors below base rates following negative be- and whether the manipulation of attributions can
havior by the partner). In the 1980s and 1990s, yield enduring changes in marital functioning.
researchers extended this work by focusing on less Other questions pertain more broadly to cognitive
immediately observable aspects of marital inter- variables in marriage, such as how spouses un-
action, (including interpretations of interactional derstanding of their specific negative marital in-
behaviors, emotions experienced and displayed teractions affects future interactions and how
during interaction, physiological responses to in- broader cognitive schemas (e.g., lay theories about
teraction) and on global patterns of interaction, relationships, stories that couples form about their
neglected prosocial dimensions of marital behav- marriage) organize and guide marital functioning.
ior, and marital violence. We highlight key find-
ings in each of these areas below.
Affect
Occurring largely in parallel with this work on
Cognition
cognition is a dramatic surge in research on the
The strong focus on marital cognition in the affective dimension of marital interaction (e.g.,
1980s, which was supported by longitudinal stud- Johnson & Greenberg, 1994; Matthews, Wickra-
ies of spouses maladaptive attributions or inter- ma, & Conger, 1996; Newton, Kiecolt-Glaser,
pretations for negative partner behaviors (e.g., Glaser, & Malarkey, 1995; Thomas, Fletcher, &
Fincham & Bradbury, 1987a) and their autonomic Lange, 1997). As a result of this work, there is
physiology before interaction (presumed to be an now reasonably clear evidence that this is an es-
indicator of the meaning spouses assign to their sential dimension to consider in accounting for
interactions; e.g., Levenson & Gottman, 1985), variability in the quality of marriage. Neverthe-
has carried through into the 1990s. Major devel- less, the details of this association remain to be
opments in the literature on spouses attributions clarified because some studies show, for example,
include cross-cultural evidence for the association that negative affect is detrimental for marriage,
between maladaptive explanations for marital whereas others show that it promotes marital qual-
events and marital satisfaction (Sabourin, Lussier, ity or is unrelated to it (for discussions, see Fin-
& Wright, 1991), continued elaboration of the in- cham & Beach, 1999a; Gottman & Notarius, this
ternal structure and organization of attributions volume). The lack of replication across laborato-
and other cognitive factors (e.g., Sayers & Bau- ries and even within laboratories underscores the
com, 1995), and further longitudinal data linking need for further theoretical development and the
attributions to marital deterioration (e.g., Karney low yield that is likely from further atheoretical
& Bradbury, 2000). There also is now evidence descriptive work. More specifically, definitive
that maladaptive attributions covary with elevated statements about the role of affect in eroding or
rates of negative behaviors during marital prob- supporting marital satisfaction await refinements
lem-solving discussions (e.g., Bradbury, Beach, in the conceptual underpinnings of affect-related
Fincham, & Nelson, 1996), and a series of studies constructs and in the methods used to observe
shows that key associations in this literature are emotional expressions and to discern their effects
not an artifact of such potential confounds as neu- on marriage over time.
rotic personality, self-esteem, physical aggression,
depression, or measurement procedures (see Fin-
Physiology
cham, in press). As a result, attributions now fig-
ure prominently in models of marital disruption Developing in conjunction with the increased em-
(e.g., Gottman, 1993a) and in programs designed phasis on affect in marriage is a rapidly growing
to prevent adverse marital outcomes (e.g., Mark- literature on physiological concomitants of inter-
man, Stanley, & Blumberg, 1994). action. For example, recent research addresses
Although research in the 1990s has satisfied questions about marital influence attempts and
much of the speculation in the 1980s about the blood pressure changes (Brown, Smith, & Ben-
Marital Satisfaction 967

jamin, 1998), heart rate and skin conductance tion (e.g., Christensen, 1987; Watzlawick, Beavin,
changes displayed by spouses while listening to & Jackson, 1967). Many important aspects of this
their partner talk about chronic low back pain model have been supported, using observational
(Stampler, Wall, Cassisi, & Davis, 1997), and gen- data (e.g., Klinetob & Smith, 1996), longitudinal
der differences in endocrine and immune func- designs (e.g., Heavey, Christensen, & Malamuth,
tioning during marital problem solving (Kiecolt- 1995), and cross-cultural samples (e.g., Boden-
Glaser et al., 1996). Some of these findings are mann, Kaiser, Hahlweg, & Fehm-Wolfsdorf,
intriguing; Thomsen and Gilbert (1998), for ex- 1998). At present, it appears that demand/with-
ample, found greater synchrony or correspon- drawal tendencies are at least partially responsive
dence in physiological systems among maritally to conflict structure (i.e., who wants to change;
satisfied couples than among maritally dissatisfied see Heavey, Layne, & Christensen, 1993) and that
couples. Malarkey, Kiecolt-Glaser, Pearl, and Gla- the usual gender differences may be reversed in
ser (1994) found increases in pituitary and adrenal couples characterized by violence (Babcock,
hormones as a function of increased levels of hos- Waltz, Jacobson, & Gottman, 1993). A similar
tility in newlyweds marital conflict (see Booth, shift toward macroanalytic approaches is evident
this volume, for an expanded discussion). This in Gottmans (1993b) typology, which identifies,
line of work is significant because it provides an using interaction data, three groups of couples
expanded, multisystems view of events arising who were in stable marriages over a 4-year period
within marital interaction, and it promises to de- (e.g., validators, avoiders) and two groups of un-
lineate the specific mechanisms by which physi- stable couples (i.e., hostile and hostile-detached).
ological processes mediate the widely acknowl- Although reports using sequential analysis have
edged link between marital functioning and appeared in recent years, these tend to focus on
physical well-being (see Burman & Margolin, descriptive studies of populations that have not
1992; Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996). been examined extensively using behavioral data
At the same time, this literature shows that it can (e.g., couples with a depressed wife, Nelson &
be difficult to obtain reliable physiological data Beach, 1990; couples with a violent spouse, Bur-
during spontaneous social interaction (e.g., Sand- man, Margolin, & John, 1993).
ers, Halford, & Behrens, 1999) and that, perhaps This new focus, which might be characterized
as a consequence, promising hypotheses involving as yielding relatively encompassing behavioral
physiological data (e.g., that arousal before and patterns derived at least partially from clinical or
during marital interaction would foreshadow mar- quasi-clinical observation (i.e., a top-down ap-
ital deterioration; Levenson & Gottman, 1985) proach), would seem to be a natural progression
have not been supported upon further analysis from the bottom-up approach to behavioral data
(Gottman & Levenson, 1992). In any case, the that predominated in the past. As this line of work
integration of overt behavioral data and accom- continues, it will be important to establish a rea-
panying physiology as antecedents of change in sonably exhaustive set of key macrolevel patterns,
physical and marital well-being remains an im- to demonstrate that these patterns have predictive
portant task for the future. validity beyond the specific codes that comprise
them, to establish that sampling methods do not
misrepresent systematically couples having a par-
Patterns
ticular pattern (e.g., disengaged patterns), and to
In contrast to the microanalytic studies of sequen- clarify the extent to which these patterns change
tial patterns in behavior that typified the 1980s, over key periods in the life of a marriage.
the 1990s witnessed a movement away from these
patterns and toward higher order features of in-
Social Support
teraction. Foremost among these is the demand/
withdraw pattern, whereby one spouse, typically Research on interpersonal processes in marriage
the wife, criticizes and nags the partner for focuses heavily on conflict and problem solving.
change, while the partner, typically the husband, Nonetheless, there is some ambiguity in the as-
avoids the discussion and disengages from con- sociation between problem-solving behavior and
frontation. According to this view, increased de- marital outcomes (as noted above), data suggest
mands lead to increased avoidance, which in turn that the longitudinal association between negative
leads to increased demands for engagement, with behavior and marital outcomes is moderated by
the end result being a decline in marital satisfac- spouses expressions of affection (Huston & Cho-
968 Journal of Marriage and the Family

rost, 1994), the actual frequency of overt conflict (Fincham, 1998). As the work on support contin-
in a typical marriage is proving to be surprisingly ues to develop, it will be important to recognize
low (McGonagle, Kessler, & Schilling, 1992), and that interpersonal processes within a marriage
there is growing recognition that the continued in- might be affected by the nature of support ob-
crease in dual-career couples places a premium on tained by spouses outside the marriage (Bryant &
the manner in which spouses help each other han- Conger, 1999). In an observational study of wives
dle problems that arise largely outside the mar- talking with their confidants, for example, Julien,
riage. Although support processes in marriage Markman, Leveille, Chartrand, & Begin, (1994)
have long been a topic of interest (e.g., Barker & demonstrated that wives reported relatively more
Lemle, 1984; Coyne & DeLongis, 1986), for the emotional distress and more perceived distance
reasons noted here the topic is now being ad- from their husband following the discussion to the
dressed with increased vigor (e.g., Acitelli & An- extent the confidant made more comments that in-
tonucci, 1994; Bodenmann, 1997; Coyne & terfered with or undermined the wifes marriage.
Smith, 1994; Katz, Beach, Smith, & Myers,
1997). Support processes have been reliably
Violence
linked in these studies with marital functioning
and with important health outcomes in families The final aspect of interpersonal process in mar-
(Collins, Dunkel-Schetter, Lobel, & Scrimshaw riage that we consider is physical violence (also
1993). see Johnson & Ferraro, this volume). Important
An important feature in recent studies of mar- strides in estimating the prevalence of marital vi-
ital support is the use of methods that permit more olence made in the 1980s (e.g., Straus & Gelles,
detailed investigation of potentially supportive 1986) have resulted in a large amount of research
transactions. Observational methods for assessing on marital and family violence in the 1990s (cf.
the provision and receipt of supportive behaviors Berardo, 1980). Although direct observation of
have been developed (e.g., Cutrona, 1996). The actual physical aggression in marriage typically is
resulting behaviors have been linked to marital not possible (cf. Capaldi & Crosby, 1997), a series
quality and changes in marital quality, even after of observational studies has been conducted on
controlling for behaviors observed in standard the interactional styles in violent and nonviolent
problem-solving discussions (Pasch & Bradbury, marriages. Even when compared with distressed
1998; also see Carels & Baucom, 1999; Saitzyk, couples who are not violent, for example, the in-
Floyd, & Kroll, 1997). Daily diary methods have teractions of distressed violent couples are marked
also proven to be powerful in clarifying the op- by higher levels of negative reciprocation, anger,
eration of support in marriage; for example, in a and contempt (e.g., Cordova, Jacobson, Gottman,
study of couples in which one spouse was pre- & Rushe, 1993; Holtzworth-Munroe, Smutzler, &
paring to take the bar exam, Bolger, Zuckerman, Stuart, 1998). These findings help to clarify how
& Kessler (1998) showed that the examinees dis- disagreements can escalate in violent marriages,
tress did not rise as the exam drew near to the and they also confirm that behavioral differences
extent that the partner provided increasing levels between distressed and nondistressed couples can
of support. This emerging line of work stands in exist in the absence of physical aggression. Other
sharp contrast to studies of conflict in marriage, noteworthy advances in this area include en-
and it promises to enrich our understanding of hanced measurement of aggression (e.g., Straus,
both conflict (e.g., it may be less consequential in Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, 1996), anal-
marriages characterized by higher levels of sup- ysis of the contributing role of alcohol use to vi-
port) and the determinants of marital quality. It is olent marital incidents (e.g., Quigley & Leonard,
also likely to influence the large literature on the 1999), and recognition that some form of physical
effects of marital interaction on child adjustment, aggression is present at high levels in newlywed
which has focused almost exclusively on the ef- marriage (e.g., 57% in OLeary et al., 1989).
fects of conflict on child well-being (e.g., Cum- Growing interest in domestic violence among Eu-
mings & Davies, 1994; Fincham, Grych, & Os- ropean researchers has the potential to help iden-
borne, 1994; Jouriles, Norwood, McDonald, tify cross-cultural commonalities as well as unique
Vincent, & Mahoney, 1996). These effects might cultural factors that influence the manifestation of
be weaker in families where compassionate, sup- aggression in marriage (see Klein, 1998). Like-
portive behavior is displayed routinely by the par- wise, investigations of ethnic differences in level
ents and stronger in those families where it is not of physical aggression within the United States
Marital Satisfaction 969

suggest that such differences are attributable to those that are unique to particular couples as well
differences in family income (Cazenave & as those that are common to many couplescon-
Strauss, 1990). Nonetheless, other factors, such as tribute to interpersonal processes and moderate
level of acculturation, must play a role in any links between those processes and marital out-
comprehensive explanation (Sorenson & Telles, comes. Here it must be recognized that causes of
1991). Finally, the link between physical aggres- marital dysfunction and the solutions pursued in
sion and diminished marital quality typically is the hopes of alleviating it can diverge consider-
assumed rather than demonstrated, and the low ably and that the causes of the problem can be
rate with which aggression is reported as a prob- linked to viable solutions for a problem in tenuous
lem in couples seeking therapy, even when pres- ways (see Christensen, 1998). To reason by anal-
ent, indicates that some couples may tolerate ag- ogy, variability in skin cancer across individuals
gression in their relationship (cf. Ehrensaft & is presumably due to environmental factors to
Vivian, 1996). This raises questions about whether which people are exposed or to environment by
and how aggression comes to erode marriages organism (e.g., pigmentation) interactions, but
(e.g., Leonard & Roberts, 1998; Rogge & Brad- they can be counteracted by individual-level in-
bury, 1999) and the factors controlling desistance terventions (e.g., applying sunscreen, wearing a
in aggression (Jacobson, Gottman, Cortner, Berns, hat). Thus, effective solutions that alleviate mari-
& Shortt, 1996; Quigley & Leonard, 1996). In tal dysfunction may overlap only partially with the
short, although important questions remain, re- actual causes of marital dysfunction.
search conducted in the 1990s demonstrates plain- Research on contextual or ecological factors in
ly that marriage cannot be studied or treated ef- relationship functioning has expanded dramatical-
fectively without giving due consideration to the ly in recent years, suggesting that a more balanced
possibility that spouses are or have been physi- view of interpersonal and environmental causes
cally aggressive. and solutionswill emerge in the decade ahead.
Marriages exist in highly complex, multifaceted
environments, of course, and a full understanding
MARITAL PROCESSES IN CONTEXT
of how these environments interact and impinge
Although there is widespread endorsement of the upon marriage is just beginning to develop. In the
view that the stuff and substance of an interper- sections below, we highlight a few key environ-
sonal relationship is the behavioral interaction be- ments and contexts, and we outline associated re-
tween the partners (Berscheid, 1995, p. 531), search as a way of illustrating recent progress.
many scholars adhere to the position that the Where possible, we draw attention to studies that
meaning and implications of behavioral interac- link contextual variables with specific interperson-
tion cannot be fully understood without consid- al processes rather than more global indicators of
ering the broader context in which those interac- marital functioning. We focus first on three mi-
tions occur. The ways in which couples manage crocontexts, which we define as settings and cir-
conflict may be important for the long-term qual- cumstances that are likely to be salient to couples
ity of their relationship, for example, but is a cer- and that will have relatively direct links to inter-
tain pattern of negative behavior more consequen- personal functioning in marriage, and then we
tial for blue-collar versus white-collar workers? move on to consider some macrocontexts, or
Does our understanding of social support in rela- broader social conditions and institutions likely to
tion to marital satisfaction change when we con- be less salient to couples and perhaps more indi-
sider how much stress couples experience? Does rect or subtle in their effects.
ones family background influence the meaning of
different kinds of interpersonal behaviors in mar-
Microcontexts
riage? Is marital instability less prevalent in set-
tings where there are few versus many available Children. Children figure prominently in how
mates? Answering questions such as these can marriage is experienced for many couples. Re-
sharpen our understanding of marital satisfaction, search suggests that children have the paradoxical
and indeed questions of this sort received consid- effect of increasing the stability of marriage, at
erable attention in the 1990s. least when children are relatively young, while de-
In addition to its obvious scientific merit, there creasing its quality (e.g., Belsky, 1990; Waite &
are important applied benefits to be gained from Lillard, 1991). Researchers for some time have
addressing the ways in which contextual factors turned to examine how couples negotiate the tran-
970 Journal of Marriage and the Family

sition to parenthood and the ensuing years as a enthood figures in the more general developmen-
means of understanding the putative effects of tal course of marriage, and a stronger basis for
children on marital satisfaction, and numerous intervention with at-risk couples will be estab-
studies on this topic were published in the 1990s lished (cf. Cowan & Cowan, 1995).
(e.g., Cowan & Cowan, 1992; Johnson & Huston,
1998; Levy-Shiff, Goldshmidt, & Har-Even, Spouses backgrounds and characteristics. Evi-
1991). Perhaps the most important advance in this dence that marital processes are associated with
literature has been the recognition of enormous marital satisfaction and change in marital satisfac-
variability across couples in how they change tion leads naturally to questions about antecedents
from, typically, the last trimester of pregnancy of those processes. Numerous studies on this topic
through several months or a few years postpartum. appeared in the past decade, and they were com-
Belsky and Rovine (1990) called attention to this plemented by a continuing interest in the inter-
point, noted that many couples do not change generational consequences of marital and family
much on important variables over the transition to functioning for offspring as they themselves move
parenthood, and demonstrated how differing path- into long-term committed relationships. Research
ways through this transition could be predicted on intergenerational transmission effects reported
from demographic and personality data and, in in the 1980s (e.g., McLanahan & Bumpass, 1988)
some instances, from indices of infant tempera- are now being examined with increased precision,
ment. A subsequent study of marital change pat- both with regard to those aspects of the family of
terns from the time firstborn sons were 10 to 60 origin that appear to be consequential and to the
months old indicated that spouses personality subsequent effects that they produce in families of
traits covaried with marital functioning at any one procreation (e.g., Webster, Orbuch, & House,
point in time, whereas marital dynamicspartic- 1995). There is now evidence, for example, that
ularly uncooperative coparenting behavior ob- parental divorce is associated with poorer com-
served in the homepredicted deterioration in munication observed among their offspring
marital functioning over the study period (Belsky around the time of marriage (Sanders et al., 1999)
& Hsieh, 1998). Using a continuous rather than and that the association between parental divorce
categorical measure of marital change through 2 and offspring divorce is mediated by problematic
years postpartum, with multiwave trajectories de- behaviors, such as hostility and jealousy, reported
rived for observational and self-report data, Cox, by the younger generation (Amato, 1996). Marital
Paley, Burchinal, and Payne (1999) showed that satisfaction in the parents marriage may prove to
declines in marital quality and increases in nega- be more important than their divorce in these as-
tive interaction were predicted by symptoms of sociations (Booth & Edwards, 1989). Along sim-
depression, child gender, and whether the preg- ilar lines, Marks, Wieck, Checkly, and Kumar
nancy was planned. (1996) have shown that marital processes mod-
This is an exciting line of research because it erate the effects of a history of affective disorder
is beginning to specify the individual, child, and on relapse following the birth of a child; Gotlib,
marital characteristics that render a family vulner- Lewinsohn, and Seeley (1998) have shown that
able to a difficult transition to parenthood. Iden- individuals with a history of depression during ad-
tification of marital trajectories over this important olescence are more likely to marry earlier and to
transition is likely to lead to additional questions experience higher rates of marital dissatisfaction
about how the transition to parenthood and par- than are individuals with other diagnoses or no
enting are embedded in a more encompassing de- diagnosis. Data of this kind demonstrate that a
velopmental view of marriage and marital quality. history of psychopathology is proving to be an
How does marital satisfaction figure in to couples important antecedent of marital functioning and,
decisions to have a child? Do children born at together with concurrent symptomatology, cannot
different times in marriage have different effects be overlooked in models of marital functioning
on marital satisfaction? How do marital processes (cf. Beach, in press).
predict later parenting (cf. Katz & Gottman, Perhaps the most dramatic upsurge in research
1993)? How do parenting stress and satisfaction on spousal characteristics and relationship func-
with parenting relate to marital satisfaction (cf. tioning occurred in the literature on attachment,
Kurdek, 1996; Rogers & White, 1998)? As ques- which aims to address questions about how the
tions such as these are addressed, there will be a experience of relationships early in life are man-
greater understanding of how the transition to par- ifest in individuals working models of relation-
Marital Satisfaction 971

ships and subsequent interpersonal functioning in theless, building on a series of studies that link
adulthood (Bowlby, 1969; see Simpson & Rholes, marital environments, stressors, and transitions to
1998). Although data on early parent-child func- marital outcomes, a large body of research now
tioning are typically not examined directly in this indicates that the social learning perspective may
literature, self-reports of attachment style in adult- be viewed more appropriately as one component,
hood or retrospective interview-based assessments albeit a key component, in a more inclusive model
of attachment to parents have been used to show of marital functioning.
that marital quality is greater to the extent that an At the risk of oversimplifying a large and com-
individual, and that individuals partner, report se- plex literature, research on marital environments
cure versus avoidant or anxious ambivalent at- tends to address either discrete, often traumatic
tachment styles (e.g., Feeney, Noller, & Callan, events; economic and work-related stressors; or
1994; Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Longitudinal links the total set of stressors and events to which cou-
between attachment styles and subsequent rela- ples might be exposed. In the interest of space,
tionship quality are beginning to be established we will focus on the first two lines of research
(e.g., Klohnen & Bera, 1998), and the specific in- here; examples of the third line of research can be
terpersonal behaviors that mediate this associa- found in Tesser and Beach (1998) and Whiffen
tionparticularly behaviors reflecting the regula- and Gotlib (1989). The traumatic events that have
tion of emotionare being pursued. Kobak and been studied in relation to marital functioning are
Hazan (1991), for example, showed that wives numerous and range, for example, from a hurri-
displayed more rejection during a problem-solving cane (Moore & Moore, 1996), World War II (Pa-
discussion to the extent that they described them- valko & Elder, 1990), child illness or death (e.g.,
selves as less reliant on their husband and that Hoekstra-Weebers, Jaspers, Kamps, & Klip,
they described their husband as less psychologi- 1998), and testicular cancer (Gritz, Wellisch, Siau,
cally available to them (also see Rholes, Simpson, & Wang, 1990). Many of these studies document
& Orina, 1999). Although the richness of theoriz- not only the diverse ways that couples adapt to
ing about the role of attachment in adult relation- these extreme difficulties, but also the remarkable
ships can sometimes exceed the data used to test resilience that they display when doing so. For
key hypotheses and although there is greater in- example, Gritz and colleagues, in their study of
terest in attachment among dating partners than testicular cancer and marriage, commented on
spouses, data in this area have improved rapidly how this illness strengthens marital ties for many
in a short period of time. They provide strong, couples, Schwab (1998) dispelled the myth that
conceptually guided evidence for how an over- the death of a child necessarily increases risk of
arching framework can integrate individual-level divorce, and Ward and Spitze (1998) commented
variables and interpersonal processes to clarify de- on how couples taking care of growing children
terminants of marital satisfaction. and aging parents are able to sustain a strong mar-
riage (perhaps due to selection effects; see Loomis
Life stressors and transitions. The social learning & Booth, 1995). These studies are important be-
approach, which has been influential in the study cause they often identify specific marital processes
of marriage, focuses heavily on the interior of that are affected by or that buffer the effects of
marital relationships as the generative mechanism traumatic events (e.g., Quittner et al., 1998; Um-
in marital functioning, leaving relatively little berson, 1995) and because they help to bring bal-
room for the ecological niches in which marriages ance to a portrayal of marriage that often is char-
are situated or for the intersection between interior acterized by fragility and impermanence.
processes and external factors that impinge upon Economic and work-related stressors comprise
them. This is reflected, for example, in the asser- the largest body of research on environmental in-
tion that distress, in this model, is assumed to be fluences on marriage. Adding to a long line of
a function of couples interaction patterns. Inevi- self-report studies outlining links between job
tably, couples have wants and needs that conflict. characteristics and marital quality (e.g., Hughes,
Distress results from couples aversive and inef- Galinsky, & Morris, 1992), several of these stud-
fectual responses to conflict (Koerner & Jacob- ies use observational or diary methods to specify
son, 1994, p. 208). This focus can be understood the interactional processes that are affected by fi-
in part from the clinical orientation of this model, nancial and work stress (see Menaghan, 1991).
as there is a clear need to emphasize potentially Repetti (1989), for example, used a diary proce-
changeable determinants of marital quality. None- dure with air traffic controllers and their wives to
972 Journal of Marriage and the Family

show that wives social support can increase hus- textual factors and specific marital processes are
bands social withdrawal and decrease anger in not typically addressed, in part because survey
the home following workdays marked by high methods are often used to examine them, recent
levels of air traffic volume and poor visibility. Us- work indicates that marital functioning can covary
ing observational methods, Krokoff, Gottman, and with aspects of these broader contexts.
Roy (1988) demonstrated that displays of negative The following studies help to illustrate the type
affect, but not reciprocation of negative affect, of findings obtained recently using this level of
were linked to occupational status in a sample of analysis. South and Crowder (1999), for example,
white- and blue-collar workers. And in perhaps showed that higher levels of neighborhood socio-
the most comprehensive analysis of economic economic disadvantage are associated with higher
stress and marital functioning to date, Conger, rates of premarital childbearing and earlier timing
Rueter, and Elder (1999), found support for a of first marriage. Other studies have shown that
model whereby economic pressure in a sample of mate availability, perceptions of mate availability,
predominantly rural families at Time 1 predicted and local employment rates can have far-reaching
individual distress and observed marital conflict at effects on the development and course of mar-
Time 2, which in turn predicted marital distress at riage, most notably in African American com-
Time 3; the effect of economic pressure on emo- munities (see Massey & Sibuya, 1995; Tucker &
tional distress was greater in marriages poor in Mitchell-Kernan, 1995). Recognizing that many
observed social support. spouses consider extramarital relationships before
In short, recent research on life events and divorce, South and Lloyd (1995) combined data
transitions enriches our understanding of the as- from the National Survey of Families and House-
sociation between interpersonal processes in mar- holds and census data to demonstrate that risk for
riage and marital functioning. Several researchers marital dissolution is greater in those regions char-
testify to the remarkable resilience of couples and acterized by high geographic mobility, high levels
families under stress, and the ways in which mar- of unmarried women in the labor force, and high
ital processes moderate the influence of the envi- numbers of potential mates. And, finally, there is
ronment on spouses evaluations of marriage are not only continued interest in the links between
becoming apparent. There is now a growing need various aspects of religiosity and marital function-
to map out the life events that are and are not ing (e.g., Booth, Johnson, Branaman, & Sica,
influential for different couples and for different 1995; Call & Heaton, 1997), but also in studying
stages of marriage, to clarify how individuals and how couples involvement in religious institutions
marriages may inadvertently generate stressful and practices are related to specific dyadic aspects
events, and to examine how spouses take life of marriage. Mahoney and colleagues (1999), for
events into account when making evaluations of example, presented data showing that various as-
their relationship (see Tesser & Beach, 1998). pects of marital functioning, including marital sat-
Also warranted are experimental studies designed isfaction, conflict frequency, and use of verbal ag-
to strengthen relationships by effecting change ei- gression, are predicted by joint religious activities
ther in the events that couples confront (e.g., job (e.g., praying together) and by perceptions of the
loss; see Howe, Caplan, Foster, Lockshin, & sacred qualities of ones marriage, even after con-
McGrath, 1995) or in their responses to these spe- trolling for individual religiousness and religious
cific events. homogamy.
Although their potential effects on marriage
may not be as immediately apparent as some in-
Macrocontexts
terpersonal processes (e.g., overt conflict or phys-
The final set of contextual factors we consider in- ical aggression), a host of environmental and con-
volves the broader social conditions and institu- textual variables may well influence whether and
tions that can affect individual mates and their how couples form their relationship, the obstacles
marriages. In addition to the contextual factors al- they may confront along the way, and the re-
ready notedchildren, spouses backgrounds and sources they can use to maintain their relationship.
characteristics, life stressors and transitionsit is For example, the impact of racism and accultur-
necessary to recognize that there are more encom- ation processes on marital satisfaction would seem
passing, relatively slow-changing factors that can to be especially important to understand. How
influence, to varying degrees, entire cohorts of people understand these factors and the degree to
couples. Although links between these macrocon- which they engage the relevant institutions may
Marital Satisfaction 973

be at least as important as mere exposure to them the absence of data to the contrary, we encourage
(e.g., perceptions of mate availability versus ac- researchers to administer global measures that are
tual mate availability; spiritual activity versus re- used routinely in the field (e.g., the Quality Mar-
ligious identity) and that as a result, there are like- riage Index, Norton, 1983). Second, the notion
ly to be important differences in how different that measures such as the MAT and DAS are not
individuals and couples respond to otherwise appropriate for some applications has been over-
identical milieu or the related experiences they extended to the point where they are believed to
have had. Although there appears to be a more be inappropriate for all applications. Most notably,
accepting attitude of divorce now compared with in longitudinal analyses of the association be-
20 years ago, for example, such an acceptance tween a behavioral variable and later marital sat-
might be greater among individuals whose parents isfaction, where earlier levels of marital satisfac-
divorced (see Amato, 1996). In any event, this line tion are controlled statistically, it would appear
of work underscores the value of studying the ex- that the problem does not emerge. This is because
ternal circumstances to which marriages are ex- of the statistical controlling of the variability due
posed, and it highlights the possibility that these to the behavioral items in the satisfaction measure.
circumstances can be modified to enhance marital In any case, the original arguments about the over-
functioning. lapping item content between the MAT or DAS
and other measures were made with reference to
cross-sectional data, and there is some evidence
CONCEPTUALIZING AND MEASURING MARITAL
that measures such as the MAT and DAS perform
SATISFACTION
similarly to global measures of satisfaction in lon-
Up to this point, we have provided little direct gitudinal designs (Karney & Bradbury, 1997).
analysis of the concept that is the central focus of Four other important developments in the con-
this article, marital satisfaction itself. Neverthe- ceptualization and measurement of satisfaction are
less, there have been important developments in on the horizon. First, there is growing appreciation
the conceptualization and measurement of marital for the view that a satisfying marriage is not mere-
satisfaction in recent years, and we review the ly a relationship characterized by the absence of
highlights of these developments (also see Ber- dissatisfaction, as is implied by the routine use of
scheid & Reis, 1998; Kluwer, 2000; Sternberg & the term nondistressed to describe couples who
Hojjat, 1997). are maritally satisfied. Factors that lead to marital
As a result of analyses in the 1980s by Fin- distress may not be the simple inverse of the fac-
cham and Bradbury (1987b), Huston, McHale, tors that lead to a satisfying relationship. Recent
and Crouter (1986), Norton (1983), and others, discussion of the defining features of a healthy
there is now widespread recognition that standard marriage (Halford, Kelly, & Markman, 1997),
measures of marital satisfactionsuch as Locke continuing interest in the attributes of long-term
and Wallaces (1959) Marital Adjustment Test satisfying relationships (e.g., Kaslow & Robison,
(MAT) and Spaniers (1976) Dyadic Adjustment 1996), and a growing emphasis on social support
Scale (DAS)consist of different types of items, and other positive behaviors in marriage (e.g., Cu-
including evaluative judgments about marital trona, 1996), all point to a developing conception
quality, as well as reports of specific behaviors of marriage and marital quality in which the
and general interaction patterns. As a result, the unique dimensions of dissatisfying and satisfying
use of these scales can inflate associations be- relationships are recognized.
tween marital quality and self-report measures of Second, prior efforts that conceptualize marital
interpersonal processes in marriage. This devel- satisfaction as a global evaluation of the marriage
opment had clear benefits for the interpretation of have operationally defined this concept as a single
extant findings and for the execution of much sub- dimension: Marital dissatisfaction reflects an eval-
sequent research, but it has had at least two un- uation of the marriage in which negative features
fortunate side effects. First, some researchers are are salient and positive features are relatively ab-
now more inclined to develop and employ non- sent, and marital satisfaction reflects an evaluation
standard global measures of marital satisfaction, in which positive features are salient and negative
which limits the degree to which otherwise similar features are relatively absent. Fincham and col-
studies can be integrated. We recommend against leagues have challenged this view, with the ar-
the further development and proliferation of non- gument that positive and negative evaluations in
standard measures of marital satisfaction and, in marriage can be conceptualized and measured as
974 Journal of Marriage and the Family

separate, although related, dimensions (Fincham, & Bradbury, 1997; Kurdek, 1991; Raudenbush,
Beach, & Kemp-Fincham, 1997). Data obtained Brennan, & Barnett, 1995; Wickrama, Lorenz,
with a simple measure used to capture this two- Conger, & Elder, 1997) and holds considerable
dimensional conception of marital quality indicate promise for testing refined models of marital
that the dimensions have different correlates and change.
account for unique variance in reported marital A fourth important development has been the
behaviors and attributions. Moreover, two groups application of a social-cognitive perspective to the
of wives who were indistinguishable in their MAT conceptualization of marital satisfaction. One ex-
scoresthose who were high in positivity and ample of this approach is the reconceptualization
high in negativity versus those who were low in of marital satisfaction as an attitude toward the
positivity and low in negativitydiffered reliably partner or relationship. Analyzing marital satisfac-
in their behavior and attribution scores (Fincham tion with reference to the literature on attitudes
& Linfield, 1997). This line of work is noteworthy highlights the idea that satisfaction can vary not
because it draws attention to the important but only in degree but also in the strength of the as-
largely overlooked distinction between positive sociation between the evaluation (i.e., self-report-
and negative dimensions of marriage made in pri- ed satisfaction) and the object of the evaluation
or research that incorporated reports of behavior (i.e., the partner). This association, or level of at-
in assessments of marital quality (cf. Braiker & titude accessibility, may be assessed independent
Kelley, 1979; Johnson, White, Edwards, & Booth, of the valence of the evaluation (Fazio, 1995) and
1986; Orden & Bradburn, 1968). Additionally, the thus may increase prediction of response to part-
measure derived from this view will enable more ner behavior (e.g., Fincham & Beach, 1999b).
detailed descriptions of change in marital satisfac- Such findings suggest that the correlation of mar-
tion and the factors that account for these changes. ital satisfaction with marital behavior and inter-
A third important development in the concep- pretations of marital behavior may be different for
tualization and measurement of marital satisfac- those with highly accessible attitudes compared
tion is the notion that satisfaction is appropriately with those who have less accessible attitudes (Fin-
conceptualized not simply as a judgment made by cham, Garnier, Gano-Phillips, & Osborne, 1995).
spouses at one point in time but as a trajectory They also imply that spouses whose marital sat-
that reflects fluctuations in marital evaluations isfaction is highly accessible should report more
over time. Such a trajectory is computed for in- stable satisfaction over time (they engage in top-
dividual spouses using multiple waves of data, down processing) relative to spouses whose sat-
and parameters of this trajectoryespecially its isfaction is less accessible (they engage in bottom-
slope, or rate of change over timecan be ex- up processing); data collected over 18 months of
amined in relation to other variables of theoretical marriage are also consistent with this implication
interest. According to this view, a marital satis- (Fincham et al., 1997). In short, it may be nec-
faction score assessed at one point in time cannot essary to revisit many of the correlates of marital
be fully understood without reference to earlier or satisfaction to determine whether they hold to a
later data points; a score of 95 on the MAT, for greater degree for persons with more accessible
example, has a different meaning depending on marital attitudes.
whether the individual scored 110 or 80 six Researchers in the social-cognitive tradition
months before it was obtained. The advantages of have also examined the way partners engage in
this perspective are that it encourages multiwave effortful cognitive transformations to change po-
longitudinal research on marriage (where two- tentially damaging responses to negative partner
wave longitudinal designs have predominated; see behavior into responses that are more benign (e.g.,
Karney & Bradbury, 1995), it allows researchers Yovetich & Rusbult, 1994). Because these trans-
to have direct access to the variable reflecting lon- formations are effortful, introducing a cognitive
gitudinal change in satisfaction (where two-wave load can result in more negative reactions than
longitudinal designs provide indirect access to this would have otherwise occurred. As a result, this
variable, typically by way of residualized change perspective suggests that certain stressful contexts
scores), and it encourages researchers to specify a may exert a negative effect on relationship satis-
model of marital change (where two-wave longi- faction by interfering with effortful cognitive
tudinal designs assume a simple linear model). transformations and so disrupting patterns of pro-
Use of a trajectory-based view of marital satisfac- social interaction. In addition, social-cognitive
tion is increasing (e.g., Cox et al., 1999; Karney models of assimilation and contrast effects lead to
Marital Satisfaction 975

the prediction that evaluation of the quality of and thereby overlook the numerous contributions
family relationships should be affected adversely that have been made, and we must be careful to
by a stressful life context, but only up to a point. mark progress by the degree of sophistication in
After a certain point, an increase in stress should the questions that are asked and not solely by the
result in a contrast or sharp increase in reported systematic accumulation of empirical findings.
satisfaction (e.g., Tesser & Beach, 1998). Accord- Nevertheless, the apparent increase in breadth
ingly, although not yet full articulated for the fam- without a corresponding increase in depth may be
ily, this perspective has considerable promise for part of the price that is paid for conducting re-
providing new insights regarding the correlates of search on a complex topic where research designs
satisfaction, reactions to partner behavior, and the usually preclude strong inferences of causation.
impact of various life contexts on satisfaction. The increased use of longitudinal designs advo-
cated in earlier reviews (e.g., Berardo, 1990) is an
important step in the right direction, but the in-
CONCLUSION
ferential power in these studies tends to be lower
Scientific work published in the 1990s on marital than desired, particularly because attrition tends to
satisfaction evokes both optimism and pessimism be high and nonrandom, more than two waves of
about what can be expected in the decade ahead. data are rarely collected or analyzed simulta-
The optimism derives in part from the fact that neously, and data are often collected from only
this topic is addressed with surprising vigor by one spouse (see Glenn, 1990; Karney & Bradbury,
scholars from diverse disciplinary backgrounds. 1995).
More important, it derives from the progress that The research published in the 1990s and in pri-
has been made in understanding (a) marital pro- or decades contains a wealth of ideas and infor-
cesses that covary with and may foreshadow mation about marital satisfaction. A first step to-
changes in satisfaction, (b) the complex environ- ward generating better work on this topic in the
ments to which spouses and couples adapt, and next decade may be to delve deeply into the the-
(c) how best to conceptualize and assess the qual- ories and findings in this workin those areas
ity of marriage. A central goal of this article has close to our research interests as well as those on
been to illustrate and evaluate this progress, both the periphery. Theoretical and methodological
in terms of the individual research themes we analysis of existing research is needed, and this
have highlighted and the broader notion that a can serve as a foundation for studies that clarify
complete portrayal of variability in marital quality and complement what is already known about
requires analysis of interpersonal exchanges with- marital satisfaction. We believe that these studies
in marriage, the milieus in which marriages are will be of greatest consequence to the extent they
embedded, and the interplay between these inte- meet the following three criteria. First, there is a
rior and exterior forces. Our analysis cannot be continuing need for large, well-funded intensive
considered comprehensive, of course, because re- longitudinal studies of couples, particularly those
search on marital satisfaction extends well beyond that sample marital functioning at several points
what we have presented here. Research on satis- in time. Basic research on how marriages develop
faction in relationships other than marriage (e.g., and deteriorate is deficient in several key respects,
siblings, Brody, 1998; gay and lesbian couples, and data are badly needed that will illuminate the
Kurdek, 1998; cohabiting couples, Stack & Esh- factors that account for changes in satisfaction
leman, 1998), which is important by itself and as over key periods of marital development. Second,
a complement to research on marriage, would ex- because most of the research that we can antici-
tend the scope of this analysis even further. pate in the decade ahead will be nonexperimental
Pessimism about future work in this area stems in nature, studies that rule out plausible counter-
from our perception that progress in the field is hypotheses will be particularly valuable. Most
characterized more by the adding of ideas within studies on marital quality tend to be confirmatory
a given research area than by building upon, and in their focus and emphasize convergent validity,
where appropriate, discarding existing ideas. This but studies that provide discriminant information
is perhaps inevitablethe more we look, the more and compare competing models against each other
we seeyet the tendency to supplement rather (rather than solely against the null hypothesis) will
than supplant or even integrate our hypotheses yield the most progress. Finally, it will be impor-
and ideas hinders cumulative growth in the field. tant to conduct research that directly informs and
We must be careful to not exaggerate this concern guides specific preventive, clinical, and policy-
976 Journal of Marriage and the Family

level interventions involving couples and families, transactional view of stress and coping among cou-
not simply because of the inherent value in ap- ples: Theory and empirical findings. European Re-
view of Applied Psychology, 47, 137141.
plied work and the experimental designs that are Bodenmann, G., Kaiser, A., Hahlweg, K., & Fehm-
permissible there, but also because an applied ori- Wolfsdorf, G. (1998). Communication patterns during
entationan orientation toward solving specific marital conflict: A cross-cultural replication. Personal
problems pertinent to marriages and families Relationships, 5, 343356.
Bolger, N., Zuckerman, A., & Kessler, R. (1998). Visible
will greatly focus and sharpen our basic research support, invisible support, and adjustment to stress.
efforts. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Booth, A., & Edwards, J. N. (1989). Transmission of
REFERENCES marital and family quality over the generations: The
effect of parental divorce and unhappiness. Journal
Acitelli, L. K., & Antonucci, T. C. (1994). Gender dif- of Divorce, 13, 4158.
ferences in the link between marital support and sat- Booth, A., Johnson, D. R., Branaman, A., & Sica, A.
isfaction in older couples. Journal of Personality and (1995). Belief and behavior: Does religion matter in
Social Psychology, 67, 688698. todays marriage? Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
Amato, P. R. (1996). Explaining the intergenerational ily, 57, 661671.
transmission of divorce. Journal of Marriage and the Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Volume 1. At-
Family, 58, 628640. tachment. New York: Basic.
Amato, P. R., & Booth, A. (1997). A generation at risk: Bradbury, T. N., Beach, S. R. H., Fincham, F. D., &
Growing up in an era of family upheaval. Cambridge, Nelson, G. (1996). Attributions and behavior in func-
MA: Harvard University Press. tional and dysfunctional marriages. Journal of Con-
Babcock, J. C., Waltz, J., Jacobson, N. S., & Gottman, sulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 569576.
J. M. (1993). Power and violence: The relation be- Braiker, H. B., & Kelley, H. H. (1979). Conflict in the
tween communication patterns, power discrepancies, development of close relationships. In R. L. Burgess
and domestic violence. Journal of Consulting and & T. L. Huston (Eds.), Social exchange in developing
Clinical Psychology, 61, 1627. relationships (pp. 135168). New York: Academic
Barker, C., & Lemle, R. (1984). The helping process in Press.
couples. American Journal of Community Psycholo- Brody, G. H. (1998). Sibling relationship quality: Its
gy, 12, 321336. causes and consequences. Annual Review of Psy-
Baucom, D. H., Shoham, D. H., Mueser, K. T., Daiuto, chology, 49, 124.
A. D., & Stickle, T. R. (1998). Empirically supported Brown, P. C., Smith, T. W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1998).
couple and family interventions for marital distress Perceptions of spouse dominance predict blood pres-
and adult mental health problems. Journal of Con- sure reactivity during marital interactions. Annals of
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 5388. Behavioral Medicine, 20, 286293.
Beach, S. R. H. (in press). Marital and family processes Bryant, C. M., & Conger, R. D. (1999). Marital success
in depression: A scientific foundation for clinical and domains of social support in long-term relation-
practice. Washington, DC: APA Press. ships: Does the influence of network members ever
Belsky, J. (1990). Children and marriage. In F. D. Fin- end? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 437
cham & T. N. Bradbury (Eds.), The psychology of 450.
marriage (pp. 172200). New York: Guilford Press. Burman, B., & Margolin, G. (1992). Analysis of the
Belsky, J., & Hsieh, K.-H. (1998). Patterns of marital association between marital relationships and health
change during the early childhood years: Parent per- problems: An interactional perspective. Psychologi-
sonality, coparenting, and division-of-labor corre- cal Bulletin, 112, 3963.
lates. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 511528. Burman, B., Margolin, G., & John, R. S. (1993). Amer-
Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1990). Patterns of marital icas angriest home videos: Behavioral contingencies
change across the transition to parenthood: Pregnancy observed in home reenactments of marital conflict.
to three years post-partum. Journal of Marriage and Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61,
the Family, 55, 520. 2839.
Berardo, F. M. (1980). Decade preview: Some trends Call, V. R. A., & Heaton, T. B. (1997). Religious influ-
and directions for family research and theory in the ence on marital stability. Journal for the Scientific
1980s. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 42, 723 Study of Religion, 36, 382392.
728. Capaldi, D. M., & Crosby, L. (1997). Observed and
Berardo, F. M. (1990). Trends and directions in family reported psychological and physical aggression in
research in the 1980s. Journal of Marriage and the young, at-risk couples. Social Development, 6, 184
Family, 52, 809817. 206.
Berscheid, E. (1995). Help wanted: A grand theorist of Carels, R. A., & Baucom, D. H. (1999). Support in mar-
interpersonal relationships, sociologist or anthropol- riage: Factors associated with on-line perceptions of
ogist preferred. Journal of Social and Personal Re- support helpfulness. Journal of Family Psychology,
lationships, 12, 529533. 13, 131144.
Berscheid, E., & Reis, H. T. (1998). Attraction and close Cazenave, N. A., & Straus, M. A. (1990). Race, class,
relationships. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lind- network embeddedness, and family violence. In M.
zey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in
ed., pp. 193281). Boston: McGraw-Hill. American families (pp. 321339). Brunswick, NJ:
Bodenmann, G. (1997). Dyadic coping: A systemic- Transaction.
Marital Satisfaction 977

Christensen, A. (1987). Detection of conflict patterns in of social psychology, Volume 2: Interpersonal pro-
couples. In K. Hahlweg & M. J. Goldstein (Eds.), cesses. Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell.
Understanding major mental disorders: The contri- Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. H. (1999a). Conflict in
bution of family interaction research (pp. 250265). marriage: Implications for working with couples. An-
New York: Family Process Press. nual Review of Psychology, 50, 4777.
Christensen, A. (1998). On intervention and relationship Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. H. (1999b). Marriage in
events: A marital therapist looks at longitudinal re- the new millennium: Is there a place for social cog-
search on marriage. In T. N. Bradbury (Ed.), The de- nition in marital research? Journal of Social and Per-
velopmental course of marital dysfunction (pp. 377 sonal Relationships, 16, 685704.
392). New York: Cambridge University Press. Fincham, F. D., Beach, S. R. H., & Kemp-Fincham, S.
Collins, N. L., Dunkel-Schetter, C., Lobel, M., & Scrim- (1997). Marital quality: A new theoretical perspec-
shaw, S. C. (1993). Social support in pregnancy: Psy- tive. In R. J. Sternberg & M. Hojjat (Eds.), Satisfac-
chosocial correlates of birth outcomes and postpartum tion in close relationships (pp. 275304). New York:
depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Guilford Press.
chology, 65, 12431258. Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1987a). The impact
Conger, R. D., Rueter, M. A., & Elder, G. H. Jr. (1999). of attributions in marriage: A longitudinal analysis.
Couple resilience to economic pressure. Journal of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 5471. 510517.
Cordova, J., Jacobson, N. S., Gottman, J. M., & Rushe, Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1987b). The assess-
R. (1993). Negative reciprocity and communication ment of marital quality: A reevaluation. Journal of
in couples with a violent husband. Journal of Abnor- Marriage and the Family, 49, 797809.
mal Psychology, 102, 559564. Fincham, F. D., Garnier, P. C., Gano-Phillips, S., Os-
Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (1992). When partners borne, L. N. (1995). Pre-interaction expectations,
become parents: The big life change for couples. marital satisfaction, and accessibility: A new look at
New York: Basic Books. sentiment override. Journal of Family Psychology, 9,
Cowan, C. P., & Cowan, P. A. (1995). Interventions to 314.
ease the transition to parenthood: Why they are need- Fincham, F. D., Grych, J. H., & Osborne, L. N. (1994).
ed and what they can do. Family Relations, 44, 412 Does marital conflict cause child maladjustment? Di-
423. rections and challenges for longitudinal research.
Cox, M. J., Paley, B., Burchinal, M., & Payne, C. C. Journal of Family Psychology, 8, 128140.
(1999). Marital perceptions and interactions across Fincham, F. D., & Linfield, K. J. (1997). A new look at
the transition to parenthood. Journal of Marriage and marital quality: Can spouses feel positive and nega-
the Family, 61, 611625. tive about their marriage? Journal of Family Psy-
Coyne, J. C., & DeLongis, A. (1986). Going beyond chology, 11, 489502.
social support: The role of social relationships in ad- Glenn, N. D. (1990). Quantitative research on marital
aptation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- quality in the 1980s: A critical review. Journal of
chology, 54, 454460. Marriage and the Family, 52, 818831.
Coyne, J. C., & Smith, D. A. F. (1994). Couples coping Glenn, N. D. (1998). The course of marital success and
with a myocardial infarction: Contextual perspective failure in five American 10-year marriage cohorts.
on patient self-efficacy. Journal of Family Psychol- Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 569576.
ogy, 8, 4354. Gotlib, I. H., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1998).
Consequences of depression during adolescence:
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. (1994). Children and
Marital status and marital functioning in early adult-
marital conflict: The impact of family dispute and res-
hood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 686
olution. New York: Guilford Press. 690.
Cutrona, C. (1996). Social support in couples. Thousand Gottman, J. M. (1993a). A theory of marital dissolution
Oaks, CA: Sage. and stability. Journal of Family Psychology, 7, 57
Ehrensaft, M. K., & Vivian, D. (1996). Spouses reasons 75.
for not reporting existing marital aggression as a mar- Gottman, J. M. (1993b). The roles of conflict engage-
ital problem. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 443 ment, escalation, and avoidance in marital interaction:
453. A longitudinal view of 5 types of couples. Journal of
Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluation as- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 615.
sociations: Determinants, consequences, and corre- Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1992). Marital pro-
lates of attitude accessibility. In R. E. Petty & J. A. cesses predictive of later dissolution: Behavior, phys-
Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents and iology, and health. Journal of Personality and Social
consequences (pp. 247282). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychology, 63, 221233.
Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Callan, V. J. (1994). Attach- Gritz, E. R., Wellisch, D. K., Siau, J., & Wang, H.
ment style, communication, and satisfaction in the (1990). Long-term effects of testicular cancer on mar-
early years of marriage. In K. Bartholomew & D. ital relationships. Psychosomatics, 31, 301312.
Perlman (Eds.), Attachment processes in adulthood Hahlweg, K., Markman, H. J., Thurmaier, F., Engl, J.,
(pp. 269308). London: Jessica Kingsley. & Eckert, V. (1998). Prevention of marital distress:
Fincham, F. D. (1998). Child development and marital Results of a German prospective longitudinal study.
relations. Child Development, 69, 543574. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 543556.
Fincham, F. D. (in press). Attributions in close relation- Halford, W. K., Kelly, A., & Markman, H. J. (1997).
ships: From balkanization to integration. In G. J. O The concept of a healthy marriage. In W. K. Halford
Fletcher & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Blackwell handbook & H. J. Markman (Eds.), Clinical handbook of mar-
978 Journal of Marriage and the Family

riage and couples interventions (pp. 312). New problems to confidants. Journal of Family Psychol-
York: Wiley. ogy, 8, 1631.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love concep- Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitu-
tualized as an attachment process. Journal of Person- dinal course of marital quality and stability: A review
ality and Social Psychology, 52, 511524. of theory, method, and research. Psychological Bul-
Heavey, C. L., Christensen, A., & Malamuth, N. M. letin, 118, 334.
(1995). The longitudinal impact of demand and with- Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Neuroticism,
drawal during marital conflict. Journal of Consulting marital interaction, and the trajectory of marital sat-
and Clinical Psychology, 63, 797801. isfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Heavey, C. L., Layne, C., & Christensen, A. (1993). ogy, 72, 10751092.
Gender and conflict structure in marital interaction: Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (2000). Attributions
A replication and extension. Journal of Consulting in marriage: State or trait? A growth curve analysis.
and Clinical Psychology, 61, 1627. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,
Hoekstra-Weebers, J. E. H., Jaspers, J. P. C., Kamps, W. 295309.
A., & Klip, E. C. (1998). Marital dissatisfaction, psy- Kaslow, F., & Robison, J. A. (1996). Long-term satis-
chological distress, and the coping of parents of pe- fying marriages: Perceptions of contributing factors.
diatric cancer patients. Journal of Marriage and the American Journal of Family Therapy, 24, 153170.
Family, 60, 10121021. Katz, J., Beach, S. R. H., Smith, D. A., & Myers, L. B.
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Smutzler, N., & Stuart, G. L. (1997). Personality and the marital context: The case
(1998). Demand and withdraw communication for interactive conceptualizations of needs for spousal
among couples experiencing husband violence. Jour- support. In G. R. Pierce & B. Lakey (Eds.), Source-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 731 book of social support and personality (pp. 257278).
743. New York: Plenum.
Howe, G. W., Caplan, R. D., Foster, D., Lockshin, M., Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Patterns of marital
& McGrath, C. (1995). When couples cope with job conflict predict childrens internalizing and external-
loss: A strategy for developing and testing preventive izing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 29, 940
interventions. In L. R. Murphy & J. J. Hurrell Jr. 950.
(Eds.), Job stress interventions (pp. 139157). Wash- Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Newton, T., Cacioppo, J. T.,
ington, DC: APA Press. MacCallum, R. C., Glaser, R., & Malarkey, W. B.
Hughes, D., Galinsky, E., & Morris, A. (1992). The ef- (1996). Marital conflict and endocrine function: Are
fects of job characteristics on marital quality: Speci- men really more physiologically affected than wom-
fying linking mechanisms. Journal of Marriage and en? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
the Family, 54, 3142. 64, 324332.
Huston, T. L., & Chorost, A. F. (1994). Behavioral buf- Klein, R. C. A. (Ed.). (1998). Multidisciplinary per-
fers on the effect of negativity on marital satisfaction: spectives on family violence. London: Routledge.
A longitudinal study. Personal Relationships, 1, 223 Klinetob, N. A., & Smith, D. A. (1996). Demand-with-
239. draw communication in marital interaction: Tests of
Huston, T. L., McHale, S., & Crouter, A. (1986). When interpersonal contingency and gender role hypothe-
the honeymoons over: Changes in the marriage re- ses. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 945
lationship over the first year. In R. Gilmour & S. 957.
Duck (Eds.), The emerging field of personal relation- Klohnen, E. C., & Bera, S. (1998). Behavioral and ex-
ships (pp. 109132). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. periential patterns of avoidantly and securely attached
Jacobson, N. S., Gottman, J. M., Gortner, E., Berns, S., women across adulthood: A 31-year longitudinal per-
& Shortt, J. W. (1996). Psychological factors in the spective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
longitudinal course of battering: When do couples ogy, 74, 211223.
split up? When does the abuse decrease? Violence Kluwer, E. (2000). Marital quality. In R. M. Milardo &
and Victims, 11, 371392. S. W. Duck (Eds.), Families as relationships (pp. 59
Johnson, D. R., White, L. K., Edwards, J. N., & Booth, 78). London: Wiley.
A. (1986). Dimensions of marital quality: Toward Kobak, R. R., & Hazan, C. (1991). Attachment in mar-
methodological and conceptual refinement. Journal of riage: Effects of security and accuracy in working
Family Issues, 7, 3149. models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Johnson, E., & Huston, T. L. (1998). The perils of love, ogy, 60, 861869.
or why wives adapt to husbands during the transition Koerner, K., & Jacobson, N. S. (1994). Emotion and
to parenthood. Journal of Marriage and the Family, behavioral couple therapy. In S. M. Johnson & L. S.
60, 195204. Greenberg (Eds.), The heart of the matter: Perspec-
Johnson, S. M., & Greenberg, L. S. (Eds.). (1994). The tives on emotion in marital therapy (pp. 207226).
heart of the matter: Perspectives on emotion in mar- New York: Brunner/Mazel.
ital therapy. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Krokoff, L. J., Gottman, J. M., & Roy, A. K. (1988).
Jouriles, E. N., Norwood, W. D., McDonald, R., Vin- Blue-collar and white-collar marital interaction and
cent, J. P., & Mahoney, A. (1996). Physical violence communication orientation. Journal of Social and
and other forms of marital aggression: Links with Personal Relationships, 5, 201221.
childrens behavior problems. Journal of Family Psy- Kurdek, L. A. (1991). Predictors of increases in marital
chology, 10, 223234. distress in newlywed couples: A 3-year prospective
Julien, D., Markman, H. J., Leveille, S., Chartrand, E., longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 27,
& Begin, J. (1994). Networks support and interfer- 627636.
ence with regard to marriage: Disclosures of marital Kurdek, L. A. (1996). Parenting satisfaction and marital
Marital Satisfaction 979

satisfaction in mothers and fathers with young chil- interaction processes: The long reach of the job? An-
dren. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 331342. nual Review of Psychology, 17, 419444.
Kurdek, L. A. (1998). Relationship outcomes and their Moore, D. P., & Moore, J. W. (1996). Posthurricane
predictors: Longitudinal evidence from heterosexual burnout: An island townships experience. Environ-
married, gay cohabiting, and lesbian cohabiting cou- ment and Behavior, 28, 134155.
ples. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 553 National Marriage Project (1999). The social health of
568. marriage in America. Rutgers University, New
Laub, J. H., Nagin, D. S., & Sampson, R. J. (1998). Brunswick, NJ.
Trajectories of change in criminal offending: Good Nelson, G. M., & Beach, S. R. H. (1990). Sequential
marriages and the desistance process. American So- interaction in depression: Effects of depressive be-
ciological Review, 63, 225238. havior on spousal aggression. Behavior Therapy, 21,
Leonard, K., & Roberts, L. (1998). Marital aggression, 167182.
quality, and stability: Findings from the Buffalo New- Newton, T. L., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Glaser, R., & Ma-
lywed Study. In T. N. Bradbury (Ed.), The develop- larkey, W. B. (1995). Conflict and withdrawal during
mental course of marital dysfunction (pp. 4473). marital interaction: The roles of hostility and defen-
New York: Cambridge University Press. siveness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1985). Physiolog- 21, 512524.
ical and affective predictors of change in relationship Norton, R. (1983). Measuring marital quality: A critical
satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- look at the dependent variable. Journal of Marriage
chology, 49, 8594. and the Family, 45, 141151.
Levy-Shiff, R., Goldschmidt, I., & Har-Even, D. (1991). OLeary, K. D., Barling, J., Arias, I., Rosenbaum, A.,
Transition to parenthood in adoptive families. Devel- Malone, J., & Tyree, A. (1989). Prevalence and sta-
opmental Psychology, 27, 131140. bility of physical aggression between spouses: A lon-
Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short marital gitudinal analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
adjustment prediction tests: Their reliability and va- Psychology, 57, 263268.
lidity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251255. Orden, S. R., & Bradburn, N. M. (1968). Dimensions
Loomis, L. S., & Booth, A. (1995). Multigenerational of marriage happiness. American Journal of Sociol-
caregiving and well-being: The myth of the belea- ogy, 73, 715731.
guered sandwich generation. Journal of Family Is- Pasch, L. A., & Bradbury, T. N. (1998). Social support,
sues, 16, 131148. conflict, and the development of marital dysfunction.
Mahoney, A., Pargament, K. I., Jewell, T., Swank, A. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66,
B., Scott, E., Emery, E., & Rye, M. (1999). Marriage 219230.
and the spiritual realm: The role of proximal and dis- Pavalko, E. K., & Elder, G. H. (1990). World War II
tal religious constructs in marital functioning. Journal and divorce: A life-course perspective. American
of Family Psychology, 13, 321338. Journal of Sociology, 95, 12131234.
Malarkey, W. B., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Pearl, D., & Gla- Quigley, B. M., & Leonard, K. E. (1996). Desistance of
ser, R. (1994). Hostile behavior during marital con- husband aggression in the early years of marriage.
flict alters pituitary and adrenal hormones. Psycho- Violence and Victims, 11, 355370.
somatic Medicine, 56, 4151. Quigley, B. M., & Leonard, K. E. (1999). Husband al-
Margolin, G., & Wampold, B. E. (1981). Sequential cohol expectancies, drinking, and marital conflict
analysis of conflict and accord in distressed and non- styles as predictors of severe marital violence among
newlywed couples. Psychology of Addictive Behav-
distressed marital partners. Journal of Consulting and
iors, 13, 4959.
Clinical Psychology, 49, 554567.
Quittner, A. L., Espelage, D. L., Opipari, L. C., Carter,
Markman, H. J., Stanley, S., & Blumberg, S. L. (1994). B., Eid, N., & Eigen, H. (1998). Role strain in cou-
Fighting for your marriage. New York: Jossey-Bass. ples with and without a child with a chronic illness:
Marks, M., Wieck, A., Checkly, S., & Kumar, C. (1996). Associations with marital satisfaction, intimacy, and
How does marriage protect women with histories of daily mood. Health Psychology, 17, 112124.
affective disorder from post-partum relapse? British Raudenbush, S. W., Brennan, R. T., & Barnett, R. C.
Journal of Medical Psychology, 69, 329342. (1995). A multivariate hierarchical model for study-
Massey, D. S., & Sibuya, K. (1995). Unraveling the ing psychological change within married couples.
tangle of pathology: The effect of spatially concen- Journal of Family Psychology, 9, 161174.
trated joblessness on the well-being of African-Amer- Raush, H. L., Barry, W. A., Hertel, R. K., & Swain, M.
icans. Social Science Research, 24, 352366. A. (1974). Communication, conflict and marriage.
Matthews, L. S., Wickrama, K. A. S., & Conger, R. D. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
(1996). Predicting marital instability from spouse and Repetti, R. (1989). Effects of daily workload on sub-
observer reports of marital interaction. Journal of sequent behavior during marital interaction: The roles
Marriage and the Family, 58, 641655. of social withdrawal and spouse support. Journal of
McGonagle, K. A., Kessler, R. C., & Schilling, E. A. Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 651659.
(1992). The frequency and determinants of marital Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., & Orina, M. M. (1999).
disagreements in a community sample. Journal of So- Attachment and anger in an anxiety-provoking situ-
cial and Personal Relationships, 9, 507524. ation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
McLanahan, S., & Bumpass, L. (1988). Intergeneration- 76, 940957.
al consequences of family disruption. American Jour- Rogers, S. J., & Amato, P. R. (1997). Is marital quality
nal of Sociology, 94, 130152. declining? The evidence from two generations. Social
Menaghan, E. G. (1991). Work experiences and family Forces, 75, 10891100.
980 Journal of Marriage and the Family

Rogers, S. J., & White, L. K. (1998). Satisfaction with Scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psy-
parenting: The role of marital happiness, family struc- chometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283
ture, and parents gender. Journal of Marriage and 316.
the Family, 60, 293308. Stuart, R. B. (1969). Operant-interpersonal treatment for
Rogge, R. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1999). Till violence marital discord. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
does us part: The differing roles of communication Psychology, 33, 675682.
and aggression in predicting adverse marital out- Tesser, A., & Beach, S. R. H. (1998). Life events, re-
comes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- lationship quality, and depression: An investigation
ogy, 67, 340351. of judgment discontinuity in vivo. Journal of Person-
Rollins, B. C., & Feldman, H. (1970). Marital satisfac- ality and Social Psychology, 74, 3652.
tion over the family life cycle. Journal of Marriage Thomas, G., Fletcher, G. J. O., & Lange, C. (1997). On-
and the Family, 32, 2028. line empathic accuracy in marital interaction. Journal
Sabourin, S., Lussier, Y., & Wright, J. (1991). The ef- of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 839850.
fects of measurement strategy on attributions for mar- Thomsen, D. G., & Gilbert, D. G. (1998). Factors char-
ital problems and behaviors. Journal of Applied So- acterizing marital conflict states and traits: Physiolog-
cial Psychology, 21, 734746. ical, affective, behavioral, and neurotic variable con-
Saitzyk, A. R., Floyd, F. J., & Kroll, A. B. (1997). Se- tributions to marital conflict and satisfaction.
quential analysis of autonomy-interdependence and Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 833855.
affiliation-disaffiliation in couples social support in- Tucker, M. B., & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.). (1995).
teractions. Personal Relationships, 4, 341360. The decline in marriage among African Americans.
Sanders, M. R., Halford, W. K., & Behrens, B. C. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
(1999). Parental divorce and premarital couple com- Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K.
munication. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 60 (1996). The relationship between social support and
74. physiological processes: A review with emphasis on
Sayers, S. L., & Baucom, D. H. (1995). Multidimen- underlying mechanisms and implications for health.
sional scaling of spouses attributions for marital con- Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488531.
flicts. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 667693. Umberson, D. (1995). Marriage as support or strain:
Schwab, R. (1998). A childs death and divorce: Dis- Marital quality following the death of a parent. Jour-
pelling the myth. Death Studies, 22, 445468. nal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 709723.
Simpson, J. A., & Rholes, W. S. (Eds.). (1998). Attach- U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1998, March). Marital sta-
ment theory and close relationships. New York: Guil- tus and living arrangements (update). Current Popu-
ford Press. lation Reports, (Series P20514).
Sorenson, S. B., & Telles, C. A. (1991). Self-reports of Vaillant, C. O., & Vaillant, G. E. (1993). Is the U-curve
spousal violence in a Mexican-American and non- of marital satisfaction an illusion? A 40-year study of
Hispanic White population. Violence and Victims, 6, marriage. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 55,
315. 230239.
South, S. J., & Crowder, K. D. (1999). Neighborhood Waite, L. J., & Lillard, L. A. (1991). Children and mar-
effects on family formation: Concentrated poverty ital disruption. American Journal of Sociology, 96,
and beyond. American Sociological Review, 64, 113 930953.
132. Ward, R. A., & Spitze, G. (1998). Sandwiched mar-
South, S. J., & Lloyd, K. M. (1995). Spousal alterna- riages: The implications of child and parent relations
tives and marital dissolution. American Sociological for marital quality in midlife. Social Forces, 77, 647
Review, 60, 2135. 666.
Spanier, G. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J. H., & Jackson, D. D. (1967).
scales for assessing the quality of marriage and sim- Pragmatics of human communication: A study of in-
ilar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, teractional patterns, pathologies, and paradoxes.
1528. New York: Norton.
Stack, S., & Eshleman, J. R. (1998). Marital status and Webster, P. S., Orbuch, T. L., & House, J. S. (1995).
happiness: A 17-nation study. Journal of Marriage Effects of childhood family background on adult mar-
and the Family, 60, 527536. ital quality and perceived stability. American Journal
Stampler, D. B., Wall, J. R., Cassisi, J. E., & Davis, H. of Sociology, 101, 404432.
(1997). Marital satisfaction and psychophysiological Whiffen, V. E., & Gotlib, I. H. (1989). Stress and coping
responsiveness in spouses of patients with chronic in maritally distressed and nondistressed couples.
pain. International Journal of Rehabilitation and Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6,
Health, 3, 159170. 327344.
Sternberg, R. J., & Hojjat, M. (1997). Satisfaction in Wickrama, K. A. S., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, R. D., &
close relationships. New York: Guilford Press. Elder, G. H. Jr. (1997). Marital quality and physical
Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1986). Societal change illness: A latent growth curve analysis. Journal of
and change in family violence from 1975 to 1985 as Marriage and the Family, 59, 143155.
revealed by two national surveys. Journal of Mar- Yovetich, N. A., & Rusbult, C. E. (1994). Accommo-
riage and the Family, 48, 465479. dative behavior in close relationships: Exploring
Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S., & Su- transformation of motivation. Journal of Experimen-
garman, D. B. (1996). The revised Conflict Tactics tal Social Psychology, 30, 138164.

Potrebbero piacerti anche