Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

Cognition and Emotion

ISSN: 0269-9931 (Print) 1464-0600 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcem20

Terror mismanagement: evidence that mortality


salience exacerbates attentional bias in social
anxiety

Emma C. Finch, Lisa Iverach, Ross G. Menzies & Mark Jones

To cite this article: Emma C. Finch, Lisa Iverach, Ross G. Menzies & Mark Jones (2016) Terror
mismanagement: evidence that mortality salience exacerbates attentional bias in social anxiety,
Cognition and Emotion, 30:7, 1370-1379, DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1065794

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1065794

2015 The Author(s). Published by Taylor &


Francis.

Published online: 24 Jul 2015.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 846

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 1 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=pcem20

Download by: [90.95.3.204] Date: 24 April 2017, At: 03:31


COGNITION AND EMOTION, 2016
VOL. 30, NO. 7, 13701379
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1065794

BRIEF ARTICLE

Terror mismanagement: evidence that mortality salience exacerbates


attentional bias in social anxiety
Emma C. Fincha, Lisa Iveracha, Ross G. Menziesb and Mark Jonesc
a
Centre for Emotional Health, Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, North Ryde, NSW, Australia; bDiscipline of
Behavioural and Community Health Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, The University of Sydney, Lidcombe, NSW, Australia;
c
School of Population Health, University of Queensland, Herston, QLD, Australia

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Death anxiety is a basic fear underlying a range of psychological conditions, and has Received 12 December 2014
been found to increase avoidance in social anxiety. Given that attentional bias is a core Revised 5 May 2015
feature of social anxiety, the aim of the present study was to examine the impact of Accepted 21 June 2015
mortality salience (MS) on attentional bias in social anxiety. Participants were 36
KEYWORDS
socially anxious and 37 non-socially anxious individuals, randomly allocated to a MS Mortality salience; death
or control condition. An eye-tracking procedure assessed initial bias towards, and anxiety; terror management
late-stage avoidance of, socially threatening facial expressions. As predicted, socially theory; social anxiety;
anxious participants in the MS condition demonstrated signicantly more initial bias attentional bias
to social threat than non-socially anxious participants in the MS condition and
socially anxious participants in the control condition. However, this effect was not
found for late-stage avoidance of social threat. These ndings suggest that
reminders of death may heighten initial vigilance towards social threat.

Death anxiety is regarded as a transdiagnostic con- Death as a psychological threat: terror


struct underlying a range of psychological conditions, management theory
including depressive disorders, eating disorders, post-
Terror management theory (TMT) is the leading and
traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), obsessive compulsive
most inuential theoretical approach to understand-
disorder, and anxiety disorders (Furer & Walker, 2008;
ing the impact of death anxiety on human behaviour.
Iverach, Menzies, & Menzies, 2014). Of particular note,
According to TMT, the human motivation to stay alive,
death anxiety is particularly evident across disorders
paired with the awareness that death can occur at any
characterised by anxious responding (Routledge
given moment, has the power to incite paralysing fear
et al., 2010; Strachan et al., 2007). For example, individ-
of death (Greenberg et al., 1990; Rosenblatt, Green-
uals with panic disorder frequently fear dying from a
berg, Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Lyon, 1989). TMT
heart attack (Fleet & Beitman, 1998), compulsive hand
argues that cultural worldviews and self-esteem
washers often name life-threatening illnesses as
serve an anxiety-buffering function in order to
playing a role in their anxiety (St Clare, Menzies, &
manage existential fear (Greenberg et al., 1992). Cul-
Jones, 2008), and many of the specic phobias are
tural worldviews (i.e., shared symbolic conceptions of
associated with anxiety regarding situations that have
reality) are thought to provide a sense of permanence
the potential for harm or death (e.g., snakes, spiders,
and meaning, such as believing in life after death, or
heights). This indicates that death anxiety has a perva-
identifying with personal achievements. Likewise,
sive impact across a range of anxiety disorders.

CONTACT Lisa Iverach lisa.iverach@mq.edu.au


2015 The Author(s). Published by Taylor & Francis.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not
altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.
COGNITION AND EMOTION 1371

self-esteem provides protection from death anxiety increased social avoidance in socially anxious individ-
through the belief that one is meeting the standards uals. Furthermore, Routledge et al. (2010) found that
of ones cultural worldview. MS increases social avoidance in individuals with low
As an extension of TMT, the dual process model of self-esteem. Together, these ndings conrm that
death anxiety outlines how death-related thoughts MS exacerbates anxious responding in socially
are prevented from becoming death fears. According anxious individuals.
to this model, when death-related thoughts rst
enter conscious awareness, proximal defences are
Attentional bias in social anxiety
activated in order to remove death thoughts from
focal attention. This is done by either suppressing Despite the impact of MS on anxious responding, no
death thoughts with distractions, or by denying vul- studies have examined the impact of MS on atten-
nerability to death (Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & tional bias in social anxiety. Attentional biases are con-
Solomon, 1999). When fear of death moves out of con- sidered a core feature of social anxiety. In particular,
scious awareness, the second part of the dual process Clark and Wells (1995) model of social anxiety pro-
model is activated, triggering distal defences (Green- poses that when a socially anxious individual fears
berg et al., 1992). Distal defences include strategies negative evaluation from others, attention is shifted
to reduce the accessibility of death-related thoughts away from the environment towards internal cues
and death-related fears, such as investing in cultural (e.g., negative thoughts, physiological arousal). This
worldviews, group identities, and close relationships bias in attention towards the self prevents the individ-
that provide feelings of self-worth. ual from accurately appraising audience feedback and
may elicit negative reactions from others (Clark &
Wells, 1995).
Death anxiety and anxious responding
Similarly, Rapee and Heimbergs (1997) cognitive-
From a TMT perspective, psychopathology reects behavioural model of social anxiety proposes that
maladaptive efforts to cope with awareness of death, information-processing biases and distortions in
with these fears focused instead on more manageable socially evaluative situations produce and maintain
threats (Maxeld, John, & Pyszczynski, 2014; Strachan social anxiety. According to this model, socially
et al., 2007). For instance, phobic responses to poten- anxious individuals focus attention on both internal
tially threatening but typically harmless situations or cues and external threats. That is, the socially
objects (e.g., social encounters, enclosed spaces, anxious individual will scan the environment for infor-
snakes, spiders) may develop because anxiety mation that conrms social fears, and will be vigilant
focused on a concrete situation or object is considered in detecting these negative social cues. The individual
more manageable than the thought of ones own will then attend to additional sources of information
death (Strachan et al., 2007). Anxiety Buffer Disruption regarding the likelihood of feared outcomes occurring
Theory (Pyszczynski & Kesebir, 2011) also asserts that (e.g., a mental representation of the self as seen by the
PTSD may be caused by disruption in an individuals audience, and internal cues relating to the experience
anxiety-buffering mechanisms. This disruption results of anxiety).
in the inability to defend against anxiety and death These leading cognitive-behavioural models high-
fears, and contributes to the onset and maintenance light the role that attentional processes play in social
of PTSD symptomatology (Pyszczynski & Kesebir, anxiety disorder. Accordingly, a large number of dot
2011). Hence, individuals with PTSD may not probe studies have been used to evaluate attentional
respond to mortality salience (MS) in the same defen- biases in social anxiety. Dot probe studies, however,
sive ways as psychologically healthy individuals have low reliability and only provide a snapshot of
(Pyszczynski & Kesebir, 2011). This suggests that psy- attentional processing (Cisler & Koster, 2010). This
chopathology may be associated with heightened has led researchers to utilise more advanced technol-
anxiety and fear of death. ogies, such as eye-tracking methodologies, to provide
In line with this, Strachan et al. (2007) found that a continuous measure of overt visual attention
MS was associated with increased anxious responding (Garner, Mogg, & Bradley, 2006). Eye movement
to spider-related stimuli for spider phobic individuals, studies with high ecological validity (i.e., that contain
increased time spent hand washing for individuals anticipation of a feared outcome) have found consist-
with compulsive hand washing tendencies, and ent evidence for initial bias towards, and late-stage
1372 FINCH ET AL.

avoidance of, social threat (i.e., angry and happy faces) faces) than non-socially anxious participants in the
(Chen & Guastella, 2014; Garner et al., 2006). This MS condition and socially anxious participants in the
pattern of attentional bias is in line with the vigilant- control condition.
avoidant hypothesis, which argues that socially
anxious individuals are initially vigilant towards
socially threatening stimuli, followed by strategic Method
avoidance of social threat cues in order to reduce Participants
anxiety (Garner et al., 2006). For socially anxious indi-
viduals, both happy and angry faces can be perceived Participants were rst-year psychology students
as threatening as both represent a form of evaluation enrolled at Macquarie University. Eligibility criteria
by others (Weeks, Heimberg, & Rodebaugh, 2008). for inclusion in the study were: (1) 18 years of age
and over, (2) self-reported high or low social anxiety,
and (3) functional English. In order to conrm
The present study inclusion in the socially anxious or non-socially
MS has been found to exacerbate anxious responding anxious group, participants completed a two-stage
in socially anxious individuals. In addition, MS has screening process, including the Brief Social Inter-
been found to inuence attentional bias in certain action Anxiety Scale/Social Phobia Scale (SIAS/SPS;
individuals (Kelley, Tang, & Schmeichel, 2014). For Peters, Sunderland, Andrews, Rapee, & Mattick,
instance, Kelley et al. (2014) found that MS was associ- 2012), followed by the SIAS (Mattick & Clarke, 1998).
ated with biased attention towards positive vs. nega- Inclusion in the socially anxious or non-socially
tive images for individuals higher in trait self-control, anxious group required: (1) a score falling within the
whereas individuals lower in trait self-control did not lower and upper quartile ranges of the SIAS/SPS, and
exhibit the same trend. To our knowledge, however, (2) a score above 36 (i.e., socially anxious) or below
no studies have examined the impact of MS on atten- 18 (i.e., non-socially anxious) on the SIAS.
tional biases in social anxiety. When considering that One hundred and one rst-year psychology stu-
social anxiety disorder is a prevalent, chronic, and dents met initial screening criteria for inclusion in
debilitating condition (Ruscio et al., 2008; Slade et al., the study based on brief SIAS/SPS scores. However,
2009; Stein & Kean, 2000), this is an important line of 14 participants were removed from the socially
research. In particular, understanding the mechanisms anxious group, and 4 participants were removed
that inuence or exacerbate anxious responding facili- from the non-socially anxious group, following the
tates a greater understanding of the relationship completion of the full SIAS. This resulted in a total of
between MS and anxiety, and makes a valuable contri- 83 participants, who were randomly allocated to one
bution to terror management research (Iverach et al., of the four conditions: (1) experimental (socially
2014). anxious); (2) experimental (non-socially anxious); (3)
Therefore, the purpose of the present study is to control (socially anxious); or (4) control (non-socially
investigate the effect of MS (i.e., reminders of death) anxious).
on attentional bias in social anxiety. The primary aim
is to determine whether MS increases initial bias
Materials
towards social threat (i.e., angry and happy faces),
and late-stage avoidance away from threat (i.e., Chosen materials and procedures were based on Stra-
angry and happy faces), in socially anxious individuals. chan et al. (2007) experiment. Materials were pre-
Based on previous ndings, two hypotheses are pro- sented in the following order:
posed. First, it is hypothesised that socially anxious Demographics Questionnaire. This questionnaire
participants in the MS condition will demonstrate sig- was used to obtain general information, including
nicantly more initial bias to social threat (i.e., angry visual functioning. Two participants were removed
and happy faces) than non-socially anxious partici- due to the use of thick glasses.
pants in the MS condition and socially anxious partici- SIAS (Mattick & Clarke, 1998). The SIAS is a 20-item,
pants in the control condition. Second, it is self-report measure designed to evaluate social
hypothesised that socially anxious participants in the anxiety and the tendency to fear social interactions
MS condition will demonstrate signicantly more in dyads or groups. A sample item is, I become
avoidance of social threat (i.e., angry and happy tense if I have to talk about myself or my feelings.
COGNITION AND EMOTION 1373

For the present study, mean SIAS scores were higher used in the eye-tracking procedure. For the practice
for socially anxious participants in the experimental stimuli, 16 monochrome photographs of 8 happy
and control conditions (experimental: mean = 46.72, and 8 angry Caucasian faces (4 female and 4 male)
SD = 7.52; control: mean = 44.89, SD = 2.80), than for were matched with a neutral face of the same
non-socially anxious participants in the experimental person. For the experimental stimuli, 32 monochrome
and controls conditions (experimental: mean = 9.11, photographs of 16 happy and 16 angry Caucasian
SD = 3.14; control: mean = 11.17, SD = 9.01). faces (8 female and 8 male) were matched with a
Filler Questionnaire. This was completed prior to the neutral face of the same person.
experimental manipulation in order to make the Apparatus. Eye movements recorded via the Tobii
experimental conditions appear less conspicuous. T120 eye-tracking system (Tobii Technology, 2008).
The questionnaire consists of two open-ended ques-
tions: Describe a relationship between two people
Procedure
that you saw on TV, and Jot down as specically as
you can what those two people were wearing. Participants were randomly assigned to either the
Experimental (MS) induction. Participants in the experimental (MS) or control (intense physical pain)
experimental condition completed the MS induction condition. Participants then completed paper-and-
in order to induce death-related thoughts. In order pencil, self-report measures in a private research
to conceal the true nature and purpose of the study, room on campus, with the researcher present in the
the induction was presented as a recently developed room. Calibration to the eye-tracker involved viewing
personality assessment (Projective Life Attitudes a series of dots for less than 1 min, followed by a prac-
Assessment) (Strachan et al., 2007). Participants tice trial, which involved viewing a series of 16 angry
were asked to: (1) Please briey describe the neutral and happyneutral Nimstim practice face
emotions that the thought of your own death pairs. The experimental procedure then commenced,
arouses in you and (2) Jot down as specically as with the participant viewing 32 angryneutral and
you can what you think will happen to you as you happyneutral face pairs. That is, each angry or
physically die and once you are physically dead. happy face was paired with a neutral face. All face
Control (intense physical pain) induction. Partici- pairs were counterbalanced and presented in a
pants in the control condition completed the intense random order. Upon completion of all tasks, partici-
physical pain questionnaire. Participants were asked pants were debriefed regarding the true nature of
to: (1) Please briey describe the emotions that the the study and given course credit for their time.
thought of experiencing intense physical pain
arouses in you and (2) Jot down as specically as
Preparation of eye movement data
you can what you think will happen to you as you
experience intense physical pain and once you have Eye xations were considered valid and included in
experienced intense physical pain. As in the exper- the analyses if: (1) the participants eyes were xated
imental induction, this was also presented as a on the probe before the stimulus image onset, (2) x-
recently developed personality assessment. ations fell within the facial area of the stimulus images,
Distraction/delay (word search) task. This involved a (3) eye movements occurred at least 100 ms after the
word search task. This task was completed following picture onset, and (4) eye movements did not occur
the experimental manipulation in order to allow either directly before and/or directly after blinking
death thoughts to pass from conscious to unconscious (Gamble & Rapee, 2010). These criteria resulted in
awareness, thus activating distal death defences the exclusion of 3.89% of eye movement data. Initial
(Pyszczynski et al., 1999). bias was dened as the number of xations directed
Death thought accessibility (DTA) task (experimental to the emotional face (i.e., angry or happy), as a pro-
manipulation check). In the DTA task, participants are portion of total xations for that particular face pair
asked to complete a total of 20 word fragments, type, during the rst 500 ms of the 3000 ms viewing
including six words (e.g., BURIED, DEAD, etc.) that exposure (Gamble & Rapee, 2010; Garner et al.,
can either be completed with a death-themed or 2006). Scores greater than 0.5 indicate vigilance for
neutral word. social threat, and scores less than 0.5 indicate avoid-
Nimstim set of facial expressions (Tottenham et al., ance of social threat. The 3-s stimulus exposure time
2009). The Nimstim set of facial expressions was was divided into 1-s intervals, and the proportion of
1374 FINCH ET AL.

xations to the emotional face was computed for each Manipulation check
face type and time interval (Gamble & Rapee, 2010).
An independent samples t-test revealed that partici-
pants in the experimental condition subsequently
Data analysis identied signicantly more death-related words on
the DTA task (M = 2.42, SD = 0.77) than participants
A full factorial repeated measures mixed model analy- in the control condition (M = 1.84, SD = 1.12), t(71) =
sis of variance was conducted in order to test Hypoth- 2.57, p = .012, d = .60, 95% CI [0.13, 1.03].
esis 1. Bias scores for the rst 500 ms of the 3000 ms
exposure time were entered into the model with
face valence (angryneutral, happyneutral) as a Attentional bias
within-subjects variable, and social anxiety (high vs.
Table 1 shows the scores for initial bias (0500 ms) and
low) and experimental/control condition (MS vs. phys-
late-stage avoidance (03000 ms) for socially anxious
ical pain) as between-subjects variables.
and non-socially anxious participants in the exper-
A full factorial repeated measures mixed
imental and control conditions.
model analysis of variance was conducted to test
Initial bias to social threat (Hypothesis 1). A full fac-
Hypothesis 2. Bias scores for the entire exposure time
torial three-way 2 2 (2) mixed analysis of variance
(03000 ms) were entered into the model with face
revealed a signicant interaction between social
valence (happy and angry) and time (01000 ms,
anxiety group and experimental condition for initial
10002000 ms, 20003000 ms) as within-subjects
bias towards social threat (angry or happy faces), F
variables, and social anxiety (high vs. low) and exper-
(1,69) = 4.332, p = .041, h2p = .059. As shown in
imental/control condition (MS vs. physical pain) as
Figure 1, pairwise comparisons revealed that this
between-subjects variables.
effect for initial bias towards angry and happy
All analyses assumed a .05 level of statistical signi-
faces was signicant when comparing socially
cance without adjustment for multiple comparisons.
anxious and non-socially anxious participants in the
Descriptive statistics and effect sizes were also used
experimental condition (socially anxious: M = 0.58,
to make informative judgements regarding differ-
SD = 0.08; non-socially anxious: M = 0.53, SD = 0.07),
ences between conditions. The decision not to
F(1,69) = 6.358, p = .014, h2p = .084, 95% CI [0.010,
adjust for multiple comparisons was made in order
0.084], and when comparing socially anxious partici-
to reduce the chance of reporting scientically impor-
pants in the experimental and control conditions
tant ndings as non-signicant. This approach has
(control: M = 0.54, SD = 0.08), F(1,69) = 5.404, p
been endorsed by the American Psychiatric Associ-
= .023, h2p = .073, 95% CI [0.006, 0.081]. That is,
ation (2013). With 73 participants in total, there was
when participants were presented with happy
more than 80% power to detect a large effect (.8
neutral and angryneutral face pairs, socially
based on Cohens measure), assuming a 5% level of
anxious participants in the experimental condition
signicance.
demonstrated signicantly more bias towards social
threat (angry or happy faces) than non-socially
Results anxious participants in the experimental condition
and socially anxious participants in the control con-
Participants dition, with moderate effect sizes.
Of the 83 participants who met criteria for inclusion in Late-stage avoidance of social threat (Hypothesis 2).
the present study, 10 participants were excluded A full factorial four-way 2 2 (2 3) mixed analysis
because eye movements were recorded on less than of variance revealed no signicant interaction
85% of eye-tracking trials. This resulted in a nal between social anxiety group and experimental con-
sample of 73 participants, including 17 socially dition for late-stage avoidance of social threat F
anxious and 19 non-socially anxious in the experimen- (1,69) = 0.698, p = .406, h2p = .010.
tal condition, and 19 socially anxious and 18
non-socially anxious in the control condition. Of the
Death thought accessibility
73 participants included in the present study, 60
were female and 13 were male. Participants ranged The DTA scale was originally utilised as a manipulation
in age from 18 to 50 years (M = 19.97, SD = 5.789). check to conrm the identication of signicantly
COGNITION AND EMOTION 1375

Table 1. Scores for initial bias (0500 ms) and late-stage avoidance (03000 ms) for socially anxious and non-socially anxious individuals in the
experimental and control conditions
Experimental (MS) Control (physical pain)
High SA Low SA High SA Low SA
Time interval (ms) Face valence Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
0500 Angry 0.6 0.07 0.54 0.06 0.54 0.09 0.55 0.07
0500 Happy 0.57 0.06 0.53 0.08 0.53 0.07 0.54 0.09
01000 Angry 0.62 0.05 0.57 0.03 0.57 0.05 0.56 0.04
01000 Happy 0.56 0.07 0.56 0.06 0.57 0.07 0.56 0.06
10002000 Angry 0.62 0.1 0.55 0.07 0.58 0.11 0.58 0.09
10002000 Happy 0.6 0.1 0.6 0.05 0.61 0.09 0.59 0.07
20003000 Angry 0.52 0.06 0.51 0.07 0.53 0.06 0.52 0.06
20003000 Happy 0.51 0.06 0.52 0.05 0.52 0.06 0.51 0.04
Note: SA = social anxiety; bias scores represent the proportion of total xations for that face pair type, with scores greater than 0.5 indicating
vigilance for social threat, and scores less than 0.5 indicating avoidance of social threat; each angry or happy face was always paired with
a neutral face.

more death-related words by participants in the (p < .05). Furthermore, there was no association
experimental vs. control condition (see above). between DTA and initial eye-tracking bias (p = .72).
However, additional analyses were conducted to
determine whether DTA mediated any of the results
Discussion
found in the present study. In linear regression analy-
sis there was no evidence of an interaction between To our knowledge, this is the rst study to examine the
treatment group and social anxiety status for predic- impact of MS on attentional bias in social anxiety using
tion of DTA as an outcome (p = .10). There was also eye-tracking technology. It is also one of very few
no evidence of an association between DTA and studies in the TMT literature to focus on socially
social anxiety status (p = .91), but there was strong evi- anxious vs. non-socially anxious individuals. This is a
dence of an association between DTA and experimen- considerable advance upon past research, and
tal/control condition (p = .009) consistent with the widens the territory of research investigating the
results reported above. There was no suggestion relationship between MS and psychopathology, thus
that DTA mediated the interaction effect of exper- corresponding with recent calls for more research in
imental/control condition and social anxiety status this area (Furer, Walker, & Stein, 2007; Greenberg,
on initial eye-tracking bias. After adjustment for DTA, 2012; Iverach et al., 2014). By understanding the mech-
all three signicant effects reported above remained anisms underlying anxious responding and MS, a

Figure 1. Interaction between initial bias scores for socially anxious and non-socially anxious participants in the experimental and control con-
ditions (p = .041).
1376 FINCH ET AL.

deeper understanding of the role that death anxiety either angry or happy faces) in order to reduce
may play in psychopathology is revealed, thereby death anxiety.
identifying possible treatment targets for socially This nding of vigilance to both happy and angry
anxious individuals who also report death anxiety. faces corresponds with growing evidence that social
The primary aim of this study was to evaluate initial anxiety is characterised by fear of evaluation in
bias towards social threat, and late-stage avoidance general, including fear of both negative and positive
away from social threat, for socially anxious and social evaluation (Heimberg, Brozovich, & Rapee,
non-socially anxious participants exposed to either a 2010; Weeks et al., 2008). That is, socially anxious indi-
MS or control condition. Findings from this study viduals may actually perceive positive social evalu-
conrm that MS is capable of exacerbating anxious ation (e.g., happy faces) as threatening because it
responding in socially anxious individuals, with signi- generates apprehension about increased social
cant effects found for initial bias to social threat. This demands, anticipated social failures, and possible
conrms previous evidence regarding the impact of negative evaluation (Alden, Taylor, Mellings, &
MS on anxious responding in socially anxious individ- Laposa, 2008; Heimberg et al., 2010; Weeks et al.,
uals (Routledge et al., 2010; Strachan et al., 2007), and 2008). A growing body of research using eye-tracking
corresponds with recent evidence that MS is associ- technology has found evidence that socially anxious
ated with biased attention towards positive vs. individuals are vigilant to angry and happy faces,
negative images among individuals higher in trait with vigilance to happy faces typically occurring
self-control (Kelley et al., 2014). Hence, the present within 500 ms or less of the image onset (Chen,
ndings are aligned with a wider body of research Clarke, MacLeod, & Guastella, 2012; Chen & Guastalla,
regarding the role of death anxiety across a range of 2014; Garner et al., 2006). Hence, ndings from the
psychological conditions (Iverach et al., 2014). present study correspond with this previous evidence,
As predicted, socially anxious participants in the and indicate that socially anxious participants in the
experimental condition demonstrated signicantly MS condition perceived both angry and happy faces
more initial bias to social threat, when compared as threatening.
with non-socially anxious participants in the exper- Finally, no signicant differences in late-stage
imental condition and socially anxious participants in avoidance of social threat were found for socially
the control condition. This corresponds with the rst anxious participants in the experimental condition,
stage of the vigilant-avoidant hypothesis, which when compared to non-socially anxious participants
argues that socially anxious individuals are vigilant in in the experimental condition and socially anxious
detecting socially threatening stimuli (Garner et al., participants in the control condition. This suggests
2006). In particular, socially anxious participants in that MS had a unique effect upon initial bias to
the MS condition were vigilant in detecting both social threat for socially anxious individuals, but did
angry and happy faces. This corresponds with the not impact late-stage avoidance of social threat. This
theoretical perspective that social anxiety is associated nding may be explained by the fact that eye-tracking
with fear of both negative and positive social evalu- technology only captures overt visual attention
ation (Garner et al., 2006). (Posner, Snyder, & Davidson, 1980). Although research
Furthermore, similar ndings have been reported has indicated that these movements are closely
in previous studies investigating attentional bias in aligned with the attentional system, it is possible
socially anxious individuals following priming with that attention may shift without eye movement (i.e.,
the anticipation of a stressful situation (i.e., ecologi- covert attention). In particular, covert attention is
cally valid designs). For instance, when primed with thought to precede shifts in overt attention (Posner
the anticipation of presenting a speech, socially et al., 1980). As the present study does not provide a
anxious individuals have been found to display comprehensive analysis of attentional processes, this
increased vigilance to social threat (i.e., angry and may partly explain the non-signicant effect for late-
happy faces), when compared to socially anxious indi- stage avoidance. That is, following initial bias, socially
viduals not primed with a feared situation (Chen & anxious participants may have covertly moved their
Guastella, 2014; Garner et al., 2006). This suggests attention away from social threat. Additional research
that, for socially anxious individuals in the present is required to understand overt and covert attentional
study, thinking about death may trigger distal shifts in socially anxious individuals following MS
defences to increase vigilance to social threat (i.e., induction.
COGNITION AND EMOTION 1377

Considerations experience death anxiety, and that MS may in turn


exacerbate anxious responding.
Findings from the present study elucidate the impact
In line with this, additional clinical support and
of MS on attentional processes in social anxiety.
management may be required for individuals who
Despite the signicance of these results, a number
report an underlying fear of death. Numerous
of considerations must be highlighted. First, the
approaches to the treatment of death anxiety have
present study comprised socially anxious or non-
been suggested, with the treatment of death anxiety
socially anxious participants based on self-report. It
found to improve presenting conditions. For
is possible that a clinical sample of patients diagnosed
example, existential psychotherapy and cognitive
with social anxiety disorder may have demonstrated a
behaviour therapy have been used to treat a range
different pattern of results. However, all participants
of psychological disorders and conditions related to
completed a two-stage screening process to conrm
a fear of death (Vos, Craig, & Cooper, 2015), including
inclusion in the socially anxious or non-socially
use of exposure, systematic desensitisation, and cog-
anxious group. As such, the present study may
nitive reappraisal of death anxiety (Furer & Walker,
provide a platform for investigating the impact of
2008). These approaches hold promise as transdiag-
MS on patients diagnosed with social anxiety disorder.
nostic treatments for death anxiety, and suggest the
Second, participants in the present study included
need for controlled studies to investigate the efcacy
rst-year psychology students, with a mean age of 19
of these approaches in treating death anxiety (Iverach
years, most of whom were female (82.2%). A recent
et al., 2014).
meta-analysis found that gender has the capacity
to moderate MS effects (Burke, Martens, & Faucher,
2010). That is, females may have the propensity to Conclusion
fear death more than males (Russac, Gatliff, Reece,
& Spottswood, 2007), and this may have had some The purpose of the present study was to examine the
bearing on the results of the present study. Added impact of MS on attentional bias in social anxiety. MS
to this, research has shown that death anxiety may was found to increase initial bias to social threat for
change over the lifespan (Russac et al., 2007), with socially anxious individuals. This corresponds with
ndings from the present study providing only a growing evidence that death anxiety has the potential
snapshot of MS effects in younger adults. Although to elicit anxious responding in individuals with
these ndings cannot be generalised to the wider social anxiety, compulsive tendencies, and phobias
community, they correspond with a vast array of (Menzies, Menzies, & Iverach, 2015; Strachan et al.,
MS experiments which have used rst-year psychol- 2007). As such, it is important to determine whether
ogy students to investigate MS effects (Burke et al., MS effects occur across other mental disorders, and
2010). whether the co-occurrence of death anxiety alongside
these disorders has implications for treatment success
and/or resistance. Overall, the present study provides
Implications and future directions the platform for additional research and conrms
the potential for death anxiety to signicantly
Results from this study improve our understanding of impact everyday life functioning.
the role that death anxiety may play in psychological
functioning. That is, death anxiety may have a
unique inuence on anxious responding for socially Disclosure statement
anxious individuals, with evidence of increased social
No potential conict of interest was reported by the authors.
avoidance (Strachan et al., 2007) and attentional bias
towards social threat following exposure to MS.
These ndings correspond with the evidence that
References
death anxiety may be a transdiagnostic construct
underlying a range of psychological conditions, such Alden, L. E., Taylor, C. T., Mellings, T. M., & Laposa, J. M. (2008).
as illness anxiety disorder, panic disorder, PTSD, and Social anxiety and the interpretation of positive social
events. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 577590.
obsessive compulsive disorder (Iverach et al., 2014). American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statisti-
Taken together, the present ndings suggest that cal manual of mental disorders, fth edition (DSM-5).
socially anxious individuals may be more prone to Washington, DC: Author.
1378 FINCH ET AL.

Burke, B. L., Martens, A., & Faucher, E. H. (2010). Two decades of individuals higher in trait self-control. Cognition & Emotion,
terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality sal- 28, 550559.
ience research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and vali-
155195. dation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and
Chen, N. T., Clarke, P. J., MacLeod, C., & Guastella, A. J. (2012). social interaction anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
Biased attentional processing of positive stimuli in social 36, 455470.
anxiety disorder: An eye movement study. Cognitive Maxeld, M., John, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (2014). A terror manage-
Behaviour Therapy, 41, 96107. ment perspective on the role of death-related anxiety in
Chen, N. T. M., & Guastella, A. J. (2014). Eye tracking during a psy- psychological dysfunction. The Humanistic Psychologist, 42,
chosocial stress simulation: Insights into social anxiety dis- 3553.
order. In M. Horsley, M. Eliot, B. A. Knight, & R. Reilly (Eds.), Menzies, R. G., Menzies, R. E., & Iverach, L. (2015). The role of
Current trends in eye tracking research (pp. 231236). Pune: death fears in obsessive compulsive disorder. Australian
Springer International Publishing. Journal of Psychology, 1, 611.
Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional Peters, L., Sunderland, M., Andrews, G., Rapee, R. M., & Mattick, R.
biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative P. (2012). Development of a short form Social Interaction
review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 203216. Anxiety (SIAS) and Social Phobia Scale (SPS) using nonpara-
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. metric item response theory: The SIAS-6 and the SPS-6.
In R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier Psychological Assessment, 24, 6676.
(Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment and treatment (pp. Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and
6993). New York, NY: Guilford Press. the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
Fleet, R. P., & Beitman, B. D. (1998). Cardiovascular death from 109, 160174.
panic disorder and panic-like anxiety: A critical review of the Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual-
literature. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 7180. process model of defense against conscious and unconscious
Furer, P., & Walker, J. R. (2008). Death anxiety: A cognitive-behav- death related thoughts: An extension of terror management
ioral approach. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 22, 167 theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835845.
182. Pyszczynski, T., & Kesebir, P. (2011). Anxiety buffer disruption
Furer, P., Walker, J. R., & Stein, M. B. (2007). Treating health anxiety theory: A terror management account of posttraumatic
and fear of death: A practitioners guide. New York, NY: Springer stress disorder. Anxiety, Stress and Coping: An International
Publishing. Journal, 24, 326.
Gamble, A. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2010). The time-course of attention Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioral
to emotional faces in social phobia. Journal of Behavior model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 3944. Therapy, 35, 741756.
Garner, M., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2006). Orienting and main- Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., & Lyon,
tenance of gaze to facial expressions in social anxiety. Journal D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory I: The
of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 760770. effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who
Greenberg, J. (2012). Terror management theory: From genesis to violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and
revelations. In P. R. Shaver, & M. Mikulincer (Eds.), Meaning, Social Psychology, 57, 681690.
mortality, and choice: The social psychology of existential con- Routledge, C., Ostan, B., Juhl, J., Sedikides, C., Cathey, C., & Liao,
cerns (pp. 1735). Washington, DC: American Psychological J. (2010). Adjusting to death: The effects of mortality salience
Association. and self-esteem on psychological well-being, growth motiv-
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, ation, and maladaptive behavior. Journal of Personality and
M., Kirkland, S., & Lyon, D. (1990). Evidence for terror manage- Social Psychology, 99, 897916.
ment theory II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to Ruscio, A. M., Brown, T. A., Chiu, W. T., Sareen, J., Stein, M. B., &
those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. Journal Kessler, R. C. (2008). Social fears and social phobia in the
of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 308318. USA: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication.
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Rosenblatt, A., Burling, Psychological Medicine, 38, 1528.
J., Lyon, D., Pinel, E. (1992). Assessing the terror manage- Russac, R. J., Gatliff, C., Reece, M., & Spottswood, D. (2007). Death
ment analysis of self-esteem: Converging evidence of an anxiety across the adult years: An examination of age and
anxiety-buffering function. Journal of Personality and Social gender effects. Death Studies, 31, 549561.
Psychology, 63, 913922. Slade, T., Johnston, A., Teesson, M., Whiteford, H., Burgess, P.,
Heimberg, R. G., Brozovich, F. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2010). A cogni- Pirkis, J., & Saw, S. (2009). The mental health of Australians
tive-behavioral model of social anxiety disorder: Update and 2: Report on the 2007 National Survey of Mental
extension. In S. G. Hofmann, & P. M. DiBartolo (Eds.), Social Health and Wellbeing. Canberra: Department of Health
anxiety: Clinical, developmental, and social perspectives (2nd and Ageing.
ed., pp. 395422). New York, NY: Academic. St Clare, T., Menzies, R. G., & Jones, M. K. (2008). Danger
Iverach, L., Menzies, R. G., & Menzies, R. E. (2014). Death anxiety Ideation Reduction Therapy (DIRT) for obsessivecompulsive
and its role in psychopathology: Reviewing the status of a washers: A comprehensive guide to treatment. Sydney:
transdiagnostic construct. Clinical Psychology Review, 34, Australian Academic Press.
580593. Stein, M. B., & Kean, Y. M. (2000). Disability and quality of life in
Kelley, N. J., Tang, D., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2014). Mortality salience social phobia: Epidemiological ndings. American Journal of
biases attention to positive versus negative images among Psychiatry, 157, 16061613.
COGNITION AND EMOTION 1379

Strachan, E., Schimel, J., Arndt, J., Williams, T., Solomon, S., expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants.
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (2007). Terror mismanage- Psychiatry Research, 168, 242249.
ment: Evidence that mortality salience exacerbates phobic Vos, J., Craig, M., & Cooper, M. (2015). Existential therapies: A
and compulsive behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology meta-analysis of their effects on psychological outcomes.
Bulletin, 33, 11371151. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83, 115128.
Tobii Technology. (2008). User manual: Tobii X60 and X120 eye Weeks, J. W., Heimberg, R. G., & Rodebaugh, T. L. (2008). The fear
trackers. Danderyd: Tobii Technology AB. of positive evaluation scale: Assessing a proposed cognitive
Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., component of social anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22,
Hare, T. A., & Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial 4455.

Potrebbero piacerti anche