Sei sulla pagina 1di 299

Brian D.

Ostafin
Michael D. Robinson
Brian P. Meier Editors

Handbook of
Mindfulness and
Self-Regulation
Handbook of Mindfulness
and Self-Regulation
Brian D. Ostafin Michael D. Robinson
Brian P. Meier
Editors

Handbook
of Mindfulness
and Self-Regulation
Editors
Brian D. Ostan Michael D. Robinson
Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
University of Groningen North Dakota State University
Groningen, The Netherlands Fargo, ND, USA

Brian P. Meier
Department of Psychology
Gettysburg College
Gettysburg, PA, USA

ISBN 978-1-4939-2262-8 ISBN 978-1-4939-2263-5 (eBook)


DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5

Library of Congress Control Number: 2015945105

Springer New York Heidelberg Dordrecht London


Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or
part of the material is concerned, specically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of
illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microlms or in any other physical way,
and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software,
or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specic statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in
this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor
the authors or the editors give a warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material
contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made.

Printed on acid-free paper

Springer Science+Business Media LLC New York is part of Springer Science+Business Media
(www.springer.com)
Contents

1 Introduction: The Science of Mindfulness


and Self-Regulation....................................................................... 1
Brian D. Ostan, Michael D. Robinson, and Brian P. Meier

Section 1 Neuroscience and Cognitive Perspectives

2 The Emerging Neurobiology of Mindfulness


and Emotion Processing ............................................................... 9
W. Michael Sayers, J. David Creswell, and Adrienne Taren
3 Mindfulness and Training Attention ........................................... 23
Yi-Yuan Tang and Michael I. Posner
4 Mindfulness, Attention, and Working Memory ......................... 33
Alexandra B. Morrison and Amishi P. Jha
5 Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role
in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes .............................. 47
Brian D. Ostan

Section 2 Personality and Social Psychology Perspectives

6 Burning Issues in Dispositional Mindfulness Research ............. 67


Robert J. Goodman, Jordan T. Quaglia,
and Kirk Warren Brown
7 The Why, What, and How of Healthy
Self-Regulation: Mindfulness and Well-Being
from a Self-Determination Theory Perspective .......................... 81
Patricia P. Schultz and Richard M. Ryan
8 Self-Regulatory Strength and Mindfulness ................................ 95
Michael J. MacKenzie and Roy F. Baumeister
9 Mindfulness and Emotion Regulation......................................... 107
Whitney L. Heppner, Claire A. Spears, Jennifer Irvin Vidrine,
and David W. Wetter

v
vi Contents

10 Self-Compassion: What It Is, What It Does,


and How It Relates to Mindfulness ............................................. 121
Kristin D. Neff and Katie A. Dahm

Section 3 Clinical Perspectives

11 Mindfulness as a Key Construct in Modern


Psychotherapy ............................................................................... 141
Patricia Bach, Steven C. Hayes, and Michael Levin
12 How Do Mindfulness-Based Interventions Work?
Strategies for Studying Mechanisms of Change
in Clinical Research ...................................................................... 155
Tory Eisenlohr-Moul, Jessica R. Peters, and Ruth A. Baer
13 Clinical Perspectives: Mindfulness-Based Cognitive
Therapy and Mood Disorders ...................................................... 171
Brandilyn R. Willett and Mark A. Lau
14 Mindfulness and the Addictive Process: Psychological
Models and Neurobiological Mechanisms .................................. 185
Judson A. Brewer, Nicholas T. Van Dam, and Jake H. Davis
15 Mindfulness, Eating Disorders, and Food
Intake Regulation .......................................................................... 199
Jean L. Kristeller
16 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation: A Medical
Approach to the Mind and Mental Health ................................. 217
James Davis-Siegel, Moriah Gottman, and Daniel J. Siegel
17 Mindfulness as a General Ingredient
of Successful Psychotherapy ........................................................ 235
James Carmody

Section 4 Buddhist Perspectives

18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness: A Heuristic Approach............ 251


John D. Dunne
19 The Emperors Clothes: A Look Behind the Western
Mindfulness Mystique .................................................................. 271
Eleanor Rosch

Index ....................................................................................................... 293


About the Editors

Brian D. Ostafin, Ph.D. is an associate professor in the experimental


psychopathology and clinical psychology program at the University of
Groningen, the Netherlands. He received his doctorate in clinical psychology
from Boston University in 2004. His research focuses on the role of implicit
processes in psychopathology (with an emphasis on addictive behaviors) and
the usefulness of mindfulness interventions to overcome such processes. This
work has been funded by the NIH and other agencies.

Michael D. Robinson, Ph.D. is a Professor of Psychology at North Dakota


State University. He received his doctorate in social psychology from the
University of California, Davis, in 1996. Subsequently, he was trained in a
3-year NIMH-supported postdoctoral position, working during this time with
Richard J. Davidson and Gerald L. Clore. He is a prolic researcher in the
areas of personality, assessment, self-regulation, cognition, and emotion. In
addition, his work has been funded by NSF and NIH. He has been or is an
Associate Editor for Cognition and Emotion, Emotion, Journal of Personality,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and Social and Personality
Psychology Compass (emotion/motivation section). In addition, he has edited
two recent books: Handbook of Cognition and Emotion (Robinson, Watkins, &
Harmon-Jones, 2013; Guilford Press) and The Power of Metaphor (Landau,
Robinson, & Meier, 2014; American Psychological Association). He is con-
sidered an expert in implicit approaches to personality and in cognitive
approaches to emotion and self-regulation.

Brian P. Meier, Ph.D. is an associate professor of psychology at Gettysburg


College, where he teaches courses on general psychology, social psychology,
and statistics. He received his doctorate in social psychology from North
Dakota State University in 2005. His research is focused on social and per-
sonality psychology topics including mindfulness, self-regulation, embodi-
ment, emotion, aggression, and prosocial behavior. Dr. Meier is a consulting
editor for multiple journals and his research has been funded by multiple
agencies.

vii
Introduction: The Science
of Mindfulness 1
and Self-Regulation

Brian D. Ostan, Michael D. Robinson,


and Brian P. Meier

the charioteer of reason must attempt to subdue


Introduction: The Science the wild horse of appetite. Freud (1949) similarly
of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation described the manner in which a persons ego
and superego must wrestle with the instinctual
The first and best victory is to conquer self. drives of the id. Conflicts between the short-term
Plato desires of our animal nature and the long-term
goals derived from cultural values also figure
One who conquers himself is greater than another
who conquers a thousand times a thousand on the
prominently in modern theories of self-regulation
battlefield. (e.g., Vohs & Baumeister, 2011).
Buddha Despite our best intentions, reason often fails
to control our appetites. Along these lines, St.
The human being is a conflicted animal. On Paul lamented the evil which I would not do,
the one hand, we have a multitude of desires as that I do (Romans: 7:19) and Freud declared
part of our genetic birthright. Desires for sex, that the ego is not master of its own house
food, safety, certainty, and self-esteem are among (Freud, 1917, p. 143). Data support these insights.
these. On the other hand, we are expected to For example, one study examined the success of
forego our desires a good proportion of the time New Years resolutions such as losing weight,
in the service of cultural values. Observers have working on relationships, and quitting smoking
long noted that this situation is rife with the (Norcross, Ratzin, & Payne, 1989). The results
potential for conflict. Platos Phaedrus (trans. revealed that a majority of people (80 %) main-
2003) characterized a conflict of wills whereby tained their resolutions for a week but only a
minority of people (40 %) did so for 6 months.
These and similar findings underscore the diffi-
B.D. Ostafin (*) culties in regulating desires that seem part of the
Department of Psychology, University of Groningen, human condition. Further, such regulation diffi-
Groningen, The Netherlands
culties can give rise to a wide range of problem-
e-mail: b.d.ostafin@rug.nl
atic outcomes such as addiction, crime, domestic
M.D. Robinson
violence, educational underachievement, and
Department of Psychology, North Dakota State
University, Fargo, ND, USA obesity (Baumeister & Heatherton, 1996). Self-
regulation failure additionally leads to decreases
B.P. Meier
Department of Psychology, Gettysburg College, in well-being, in some cases contributing to clini-
Gettysburg, PA, USA cal disorders such as depression (Pyszczynski &

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 1


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_1
2 B.D. Ostafin et al.

Greenberg, 1987). More broadly, a number of to improve capacities to sustain attention, to


Western philosophical and spiritual traditions increase awareness of thoughts and feelings as
contend that self-control is a necessary founda- they occur, and to develop a nonjudgmental
tion for inner peace, spiritual wisdom, and con- acceptance of these thoughts and feelings are
necting with the divine (e.g., Galatians 5: 1625; proposed to free the mind from its vices. In sim-
Hadot, 1995; 2002; Merton, 1969). pler terms, the mind has some bad habits that can
Given the crucial role that self-regulation either be lessened or worked with more function-
plays in peoples lives, we need to know how to ally to the extent that one retains awareness of
facilitate it. Freuds (1917) analysis is a useful what is currently happening.
departure point in doing so. Freud metaphorically While the original purpose of mindfulness
defined the unruly part of the mind as the id and practice was to deconstruct ordinary experience
the executive part of the mind as the ego. for the sake of spiritual enlightenment (Rahula,
Consistent with the analysis above, the id and the 1959), Western psychologists have found that
ego often conflict with each other for control over mindfulness is also conducive to a variety of
behavior with the id favoring impulsive actions practical forms of self-regulation. These include
and the ego favoring more thoughtful actions (see the regulation of negative emotions (Chap. 9;
Strack & Deutsch, 2004, for a related analysis). It Hofmann, Sawyer, Witt, & Oh, 2010) and prob-
is natural to characterize the resulting tensions in lematic behaviors of multiple types (Chap. 14;
terms of a battle between the id and the ego, Zgierska et al., 2009). Mindfulness also appears
which in turn suggests particular ways of facili- to facilitate personal growth (Chap. 7) and its
tating self-regulation. The ids forces must be benefits have been touted for home (Kabat-Zinn
defeated somehow, either by weakening the id or & Kabat-Zinn, 1997), school (Rechtschaffen,
strengthening the ego and then engaging in com- 2014), and work (Carroll, 2007) settings.
bat. We note that there are fairly close parallels Independent of these potential benefits, mindful-
between this analysis and recommendations ness is a fascinating state of consciousness that
made in the self-control literature (e.g., Friese, has had a long attraction for Western psychology
Hofmann, & Wiers, 2011). Battles are costly, (James, 1902; Jung, 1964) and which warrants
however, and they are grim. Freud (1917) antici- close study in its own right. The current volume
pated this as well in his suggestion that a long- provides this close study in the form of state-of-
term solution to the self-regulation problem may the-art reviews by international experts who
depend on developing greater insight into how review what is known about whether and how
the mind works. Mindfulness similarly aims to mindfulness works while making recommenda-
increase the individuals freedom through insight. tions for future study. All chapters address the
What is mindfulness? It is a state of conscious- interface of mindfulness and self-regulation, with
ness and a way of being that can be described as chapters covering a range from basic research to
paying attention in a particular way; on pur- real-world applications in the clinic.
pose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmen-
tally (Kabat-Zinn, 1994, p. 4; also see Bishop
et al., 2004). Although this definition originates Overview of the Volume
in the Buddhist tradition (Chap. 18), similar ideas
of attending to the moment with an accepting As a general orientation, we should make two
attitude can be found in Western traditions such points. At its core, mindfulness involves paying
as the Stoic practice of delimiting the present attention to present-moment experiences in a
(Hadot, 1995). In both cases, the suggestion is nonjudgmental manner. Long-term meditation
that the untrained mind is poorly controlled, practice can help one achieve this state more reli-
prone to problematic attachments and egoism, ably, but long-term meditation practice is not
and ultimately not very conducive to harmonious necessary to achieve it. Accordingly, the volume
living in the world. Meditation practices designed reviews research in which mindfulness (a)
1 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation 3

naturally varies across people as an aspect of per- the regulation of emotions. Among other
sonality, (b) is temporarily induced among medi- findings, studies have linked mindfulness to
tation-nave participants as an experimental reduced activity in the amygdala, a structure
manipulation, (c) follows from clinical interven- implicated in emergency-based reactions, and
tions, or (d) is the result of formal meditation to increased activation in the dorsolateral pre-
training outside the context of a clinical interven- frontal cortex, a structure implicated in emo-
tion. These different ways of studying mindful- tion regulation and sustained attention.
ness often converge in their conclusions (e.g., Mindfulness seems to support a more reflec-
Chap. 2), but we should be alert to possible diver- tive, less reactive mode of brain functioning.
gences as well (Chap. 6). Tang and Posner (Chap. 3) present a model in
The second point is that although the bene- which executive attentionthe ability to
fits of mindfulness are traditionally linked to resolve mental conflicts in favor of goal-
insight (Rahula, 1959), there can be other directed processingunderlies multiple
mechanisms of action. For example, mindful- forms of self-regulation. The authors present
ness stabilizes attention, which is conducive to two categories of interventions that may ben-
purposeful behavior (Chap. 4). Many of our efit executive attention: practices that involve
problems are caused by habits of the mind and controlling mental content (e.g., working
mindfulness reduces the influence of such hab- memory training) and mindfulness medita-
its (Chap. 5). When mindful, one can become tion, which does not involve changing the con-
aware of negative thoughts and feelings sooner, tent of thoughts but rather involves a state of
which allows one to regulate them before they restful alertness. The authors review several
escalate (Chap. 13). And mindfulness allows studies in which significant behavioral, struc-
one to identify an observer self that is not syn- tural, and functional changes occur as a func-
onymous with the contents of the mind (Chap. 9). tion of mindfulness training.
There are thus a number of different, though Morrison and Jha (Chap. 4) consider the over-
perhaps interconnected, ways in which mind- lap of mindfulness with contemporary brain-
fulness can support self-regulation, as will be based models of information processing.
highlighted in this volume. For organizational Mindfulness may facilitate self-regulation by
purposes, the chapters are organized into four training executive attention, by changing
sections. working memory operations, or by increasing
the monitoring of off-task thoughts. The
authors present evidence for the benefits of
Section 1: Neuroscience mindfulness training in each of these areas
and Cognitive Perspectives and discuss ways in which mindfulness is dis-
tinct from other types of cognitive enhance-
An emerging body of work, reviewed in this sec- ment training.
tion, has revealed that mindfulness changes the Ostafin (Chap. 5) notes that many self-
way in which the brain works. Such changes in regulation failures can be traced to automatic
neural activity and cognition are likely to under- cognitive and behavioral responses. Accounts
lie some of the beneficial consequences of mind- of mindfulness suggest that the individual can
fulness. In addition, the section chapters cover learn to observe such habits without necessar-
topics such as emotion regulation, executive ily acting on them. Support for this account
attention, and addictive behavior. has been found in several studies in which
mindfulnesstrait and manipulateddecou-
Sayers, Creswell, and Taren (Chap. 2) review ples the relationship between automatic
fMRI and EEG studies on the brain mecha- processes and outcome variables such as
nisms through which mindfulness enhances rumination or alcohol consumption.
4 B.D. Ostafin et al.

Section 2: Personality and Social Heppner, Adams, Vidrine, and Wetter (Chap. 9)
Psychology Perspectives consider the multiple ways in which mindful-
ness should support emotion regulation. These
People differ in mindfulness skills independent include the ability to identify problematic
of meditation practice or experience. Research in thoughts and feelings early on, the willingness
personality and social psychology takes advan- to experience unpleasant states, and the insight
tage of this fact, thus contributing to a basic that aversive experiences naturally fade over
understanding of how mindfulness functions. time without the need to do something about
Among other topics, chapters in this section link them. Consistent with these ideas, research
mindfulness, both trait and interventions, to has shown that mindful people are less reac-
major theories of the self and psychological tive to negative events and less prone to addic-
well-being. tive relapse in the context of cravings and
stress.
Goodman, Quaglia, and Brown (Chap. 6) Neff and Dahm (Chap. 10) note that Buddhist
review the major progress that has been made practices seek to increase both mindfulness
in assessing mindfulness as a dispositional and self-compassion and that these are par-
qualitya key way in which people differ tially independent states of mind. Self-
from each other. The chapter discusses issues compassion is mindful, but also involves
related to how mindfulness should be defined bringing an attitude of caring kindness toward
and measured, including a discussion of how oneself. Dispositional and intervention
mindfulness differs from other measures of research converges on the idea that compas-
attention. The authors then present evidence sion toward the self increases well-being
for the validity of trait mindfulness measures while decreasing symptoms of anxiety and
such as an inverse relation with emotional depression.
reactivity in both neural and psychophysiolog-
ical studies.
Schultz and Ryan (Chap. 7) propose that
mindfulness may be important to living ones Section 3: Clinical Perspectives
life in a self-determined, health-promoting
way. When people are mindful, they should be The benefits of mindfulness have perhaps been
more fully aware of their deep-seated values best documented in the clinical literature. Trait
and goals and therefore in a better position to variations in mindfulness are inversely predictive
act on them. Consistent with this framework, of depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and
studies have linked mindfulness to less defen- addictive disorders. Additionally, mindfulness-
siveness, a greater focus on intrinsically moti- based treatment protocols have proven useful in
vated goals, and to higher levels of treating a number of clinical disorders.
psychological well-being.
MacKenzie and Baumeister (Chap. 8) present Bach, Hayes, and Levin (Chap. 11) introduce
evidence for a strength model of self-control the section by examining the broad questions
whereby self-control requires effort and is of how to best define mindfulness and the rea-
dependent on a limited pool of volitional sons behind the growing popularity of
resources. Support for this model has primar- mindfulness-based interventions. The authors
ily come from studies in which resources are note the potential pitfalls of tying mindfulness
depleted, but there is also evidence that self- too closely to a particular technique (i.e.,
control resources can be strengthened over Eastern contemplative practices) and instead
time. The authors suggest that mindfulness advocate an approach that is based on the pro-
training may be a useful strategy to build such cess of paying attention in a particular way
resources. and the outcomes of doing so. The authors
1 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation 5

suggest that the growing popularity of mind- also central to mindfulness, mindfulness may
fulness is due not only to persuasive research play broad roles in health promotion. The
evidence, but also to its curative properties in authors use this framework to explain the
addressing the psychological imbalances problems endemic to certain disorders of per-
caused by modern society. sonality and to impulsive forms of behavior.
Eisenlohr-Moul, Peters, and Baer (Chap. 12) Carmody (Chap. 17) discusses the challenges
document the fact that mindfulness-based and benefits of teaching mindfulness to
interventions have demonstrated their efficacy Western psychotherapy clients. Although
in a substantial number of studies. Accordingly, mindfulness and therapy both have the aim to
there is a growing need for research into the reduce suffering and increase well-being, the
mechanisms through which mindfulness idea and practice of mindfulness can seem
works. The authors provide a number of use- somewhat foreign to Western clients. Carmody
ful recommendations for conducting research shows how such issues of translation can be
of this type. circumvented through framing the goals of
Willett and Lau (Chap. 13) present an analysis mindfulness in more familiar terms.
of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy
(MBCT) for depression. The chapter describes
the MBCT intervention, presents evidence for
its value in preventing depression relapse, and Section 4: Buddhist Perspectives
outlines newer applications such as treating
bipolar disorder and acute depressive symp- As this volume demonstrates, Western psycholo-
toms. The authors also discuss alternative gists have generated a great deal of productive
delivery formats such as via phone or the research on the topic of mindfulness. The roots of
Internet. mindfulness, however, are Buddhist. The last
Brewer, Van Dam, and Davis (Chap. 14) section presents two commentaries on the rela-
explain why mindfulness may be particularly tion between Buddhism and mindfulness as prac-
useful in dealing with the dysregulated desires ticed and researched in the West.
of addiction. As the authors document, there
are striking parallels between Buddhist Dunne (Chap. 18) provides an informative
accounts of craving and modern accounts of account of the relation between contemporary
the addictive process. The chapter summa- Western mindfulness practices and two tradi-
rizes existing research on mindfulness inter- tions in Buddhism that he terms classical
ventions for addiction and discusses the and nondual. These two strands of Buddhism
potential neurological mechanisms involved. hold different theories about the causes of suf-
Kristeller (Chap. 15) discusses mindfulness fering, giving rise to distinct meditation prac-
interventions as a means to improve the self- tices. By recognizing such distinctions and by
regulation of eating among eating-disordered taking advantage of centuries of knowledge
clients. Among other processes, mindfulness accumulated by Buddhist practitioners,
may be helpful in this population by facilitat- Western scientists and clinicians can further
ing awareness of the factors that precipitate develop their mindfulness-based efforts.
binge eating and of internal cues of satiety. An Rosch (Chap. 19) suggests that there are impor-
overview of a mindfulness-based intervention tant differences between Western and Buddhist
for dysregulated eating is presented next, fol- versions of mindfulness and that we should
lowed by a review of research on the take a close look at these differences to better
intervention. understand what our science has thus far docu-
Davis-Siegel, Gottman, and Siegel (Chap. 16) mented. Among other issues, it will be impor-
propose that awareness and integration are tant to know whether there is more to our
general keys to health. As these activities are interventions than relaxation (or social support)
6 B.D. Ostafin et al.

and whether dispositional measures of Freud, S. (1917). A difficulty in the path of psycho-
analysis. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard
mindfulness capture something beyond psycho-
edition of the complete psychological works of
logical mindedness or verbal abilities. Roschs Sigmund Freud (Vol. 17, pp. 135144). London,
analysis encourages a number of directions for England: Hogarth Press.
future research, including the need for disman- Freud, S. (1949). The ego and the id. London, England:
Hogarth Press.
tling studies on mindfulness interventions.
Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Wiers, R. W. (2011). On tam-
ing horses and strengthening riders: Recent develop-
ments in research on interventions to improve
self-control in health behaviors. Self and Identity, 10,
336351.
Conclusions Hadot, P. (1995). Philosophy as a way of life: Spiritual
exercises from Socrates to Foucault. Oxford, England:
The mind seems to have a mind of its own. It can Blackwell Publishing.
want things that are not good for us, it composes Hadot, P. (2002). What is ancient philosophy? Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
narratives about the self that may or may not be
Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D.
true, and it seems to have extraordinary deficits in (2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on
staying on-task. As the opening quotes suggest, anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review.
bringing such an unruly creature under control Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78,
169183.
can be a challenge. The Buddhist tradition sug-
James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience: A
gests that mindfulness can help to tame the mind study in human nature. London, England: Longman,
and Western research has begun to provide sup- Greens, and Company.
port for this idea. When mindful, we can become Jung, C. G. (1964). Forward. [Forward]. In D. T. Suzuki
(Author), An introduction to Zen Buddhism (pp. ix
aware without becoming attached and may be bet-
xxix). New York, NY: Grove Press.
ter able to act in accordance with health-promoting Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are.
values and goals. As detailed in this volume, these New York, NY: Hyperion.
ideas are not just interesting but also practical. Kabat-Zinn, M., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (1997). Everyday bless-
ings: The inner work of mindful parenting. New York,
Mindfulness changes how we process external
NY: Hyperion.
and internal stimuli (section 1), allows us to better Merton, T. (1969). Contemplative prayer. New York, NY:
enact the goals of the self (section 2), and has Random House.
demonstrated value in dealing with problematic Norcross, J. C., Ratzin, A. C., & Payne, D. (1989).
Ringing in the new year: The change processes and
symptoms and behaviors (section 3). Such gains
reported outcomes of resolutions. Addictive Behaviors,
may be increased through a deep study of Buddhist 14, 205212.
ideas and practices (section 4). Staying aware in Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Self-regulatory
the present moment, the chapters will suggest, perseveration and the depressive self-focusing style: A
self-awareness theory of reactive depression.
benefits self-regulation in multiple ways.
Psychological Bulletin, 102, 122138.
Rahula, R. (1959). What the Buddha taught. New York,
NY: Grove.
Rechtschaffen, D. (2014). The way of mindful education:
References Cultivating well-being in teachers and students.
New York, NY: W. W. Norton and Company.
Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self- Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive
regulation failure: An overview. Psychological determinants of social behavior. Personality and
Inquiry, 7, 115. Social Psychology Review, 8, 220247.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.). (2011). Handbook
N. D., Carmody, J., . . . Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications.
A proposed operational definition. Clinical New York, NY: Guilford.
Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230241. Zgierska, A., Rabago, D., Chawla, N., Kushner, K.,
Carroll, M. (2007). The mindful leader: Awakening your Koehler, R., & Marlatt, A. (2009). Mindfulness medi-
natural management skills through mindfulness medi- tation for substance use disorders: A systematic
tation. Boston, MA: Trumpeter Books. review. Substance Abuse, 30, 266294.
Section 1
Neuroscience and Cognitive Perspectives
The Emerging Neurobiology
of Mindfulness and Emotion 2
Processing

W. Michael Sayers, J. David Creswell,


and Adrienne Taren

consider the extant mindfulness fMRI and EEG


Introduction research to better understand how the brain pro-
cesses affective stimuli in relation to trait mind-
An emerging body of research suggests that fulness, while adopting a mindful attentional
mindfulness is associated with self-reported and stance, and after mindfulness training.
clinically relevant changes in emotion process-
ing. Self-report measures of dispositional mind-
fulness are associated with reduced negative Neurobiological Models
affective states and positively associated with of Mindfulness and Emotion
positive affective states and traits (Brown, Ryan, Processing
& Creswell, 2007). Moreover, mindfulness-
based interventions reduce depressive symptom- Neurobiological models of emotion processing
atology and depression relapse in at-risk patients describe a ventral core affective system respon-
(Hofmann, Sawyer, Witt, & Oh, 2010; Teasdale sible for establishing the threat or reward value of
et al., 2000), anxiety symptoms (Hofmann et al., a stimulus, and a more dorsal affect processing
2010; Kabat-Zinn et al., 1992; Roemer, Orsillo, system responsible for appraisals and attributions
& Salters-Pedneault, 2008), and affective distur- of ones emotional state (Barrett, Mesquita,
bances in chronic pain patients (Grossman, Ochsner, & Gross, 2007; Phillips, Drevets,
Tiefenthaler-Gilmer, Raysz, & Kesper, 2007; Rauch, & Lane, 2003). While regions of affective
Kabat-Zinn, 1982). This body of work suggests processing systems overlap somewhat, the ven-
that mindfulness may be associated with changes tral system for core affect has been described as
in emotion processing, and in this chapter we including temporal lobe structures (including the
amygdala), insula, anterior cingulate cortex
(ACC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and ventro-
medial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC). It is thought
W.M. Sayers J.D. Creswell (*) that this neuroanatomically coupled ventral net-
Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, work communicates the value of affective stimuli
Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
quickly and efciently to hypothalamus and
e-mail: creswell@cmu.edu
brain stem areas for coordinating a behavioral
A. Taren
response (Barrett et al., 2007). The dorsal affect
Department of Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh,
Department of Neuroscience, Carnegie Mellon processing system is thought to be responsible
University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA for generating attributions about the cause(s) of

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 9


B.D. Ostan et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_2
10 W.M. Sayers et al.

ones core affective state, and includes the medial This review considers the evidence for
prefrontal cortex (MPFC), dorsomedial PFC mindfulness and emotional responding in light of
(DMPFC), and ACC (Barrett et al., 2007). these neurobiological models of emotion. The
Finally, neurobiological models of emotion have current research base shows that mindful atten-
also described the important role of PFC regions tion and mindfulness training are implicated in
in the regulation of emotional states (Arnsten, modulating each of these neurobiological sys-
2009; Ochsner & Gross, 2005). Specically, tems for emotional responding. Specically,
these models indicate that explicit (e.g., cognitive emerging research indicates that mindfulness can
reappraisal) and implicit (e.g., expectancies) alter ventral neural regions for generating core
emotion regulation strategies activate regions of affective responses, dorsal regions implicated in
dorsal and ventral PFC (including dorsal ACC), ones attributions about the cause of ones affec-
for modulating affect response regions (e.g., tive state, and regions related to regulating ones
amygdala) (Ochsner & Gross, 2005; see Fig. 2.1). affective responses.

Fig. 2.1 Darker shaded regions depict the three affective pro- (VMPFC); (b) the dorsal, attributional affective system
cessing systems: (a) the ventral core affect system includes includes the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), dorsomedial
the amygdala, insula, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), orbi- PFC (DMPFC), and ACC; and (c) the regulatory affective
tofrontal cortex (OFC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex processing system includes the dorsal and ventral PFC
2 The Emerging Neurobiology of Mindfulness and Emotion Processing 11

Measuring and Manipulating Trait Mindfulness and Emotion


Mindfulness Processing

Mindfulness has been described as a process Self-report measures of trait mindfulness have
of paying attention on purpose, in the present provided opportunities for investigators to relate
moment, and non-judgmentally (Kabat-Zinn, self-reported individual differences in mindful-
1994). A great deal of research has focused on ness to measures of brain activity during
operationally defining and measuring mind- affective tasks. One recent study used electro-
fulness, with varying perspectives and concep- encephalography (EEG) to assess the relation-
tualizations (for a review, see Brown et al., ship between the late positive potential (LPP)
2007; Quaglia et al. 2015). The scientic study and trait mindfulness in an undergraduate sample
of mindfulness has viewed mindfulness as both (Brown, Goodman, & Inzlicht, 2013). The LPP is
a dispositional quality that all individuals pos- a positive deection of the event-related potential
sess to varying degrees and an attentional state in the slow-wave latency range (~400500 ms
which can be fostered through training (Brown after stimulus onset), appearing most promi-
& Ryan, 2003). Trait mindfulness has been nently in the posterior and central midline scalp
measured by a variety of validated self-report regions. It is larger in response to more intense
questionnaires. In contrast to trait mindfulness, stimuli and correlates with subjective reports of
mindfulness training research entails training arousal (Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer,
meditation-nave participants to adopt a mind- & Lang, 2000). Because of these characteristics,
ful attentional stance while completing emo- some researchers consider it a sensitive marker
tion tasks, or examining how brief (4 days to of early emotional arousal (Hajcak, MacNamara,
10 weeks) mindfulness meditation training & Olvet, 2010). In this study of mindfulness and
impacts emotional responding. Finally, mind- the LPP response, researchers found that trait
fulness training effects have been explored in mindfulness as assessed by two self-report mea-
studies that compare advanced meditators sures [the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale
(with over 10 years of daily meditation prac- (Brown & Ryan, 2003) and Five Facet
tice, on average) to matched control partici- Mindfulness Questionnaire (Baer, Smith,
pants. For a recent review of scientic measures Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006)] was
and manipulations of mindfulness, see Quaglia associated with reduced LPP in response to high-
et al. (2015). arousal, unpleasant stimuli (e.g., images of
For purposes of this chapter, we describe stud- corpses). Trait mindfulness was also associated
ies that include a measure or manipulation of with reduced LPP in response to motivationally
mindfulness and a measure of brain activity while salient pleasant stimuli (e.g., erotica). These nd-
participants complete affective processing tasks. ings suggest that trait mindfulness is associated
Accordingly, we rst describe research relating with a tempered early response (~500 ms) to
dispositional (trait) mindfulness with neural mea- unpleasant and other motivationally salient stim-
sures of emotion processing. We then describe uli that occurs before a subsequent response can
research exploring how a mindful attentional arise and may indicate reduced emotional
stance can impact neural markers of emotion pro- reactivity.
cessing. In the latter case, we order the sections Another study assessed trait mindfulness with
by the amount of mindfulness training received: the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills
adopting a mindful attentional stance in (KIMS; Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004) and asked
meditation-nave participants, brief mindfulness participants to imagine personally experiencing
meditation training, and mindfulness meditation- emotional vignettes (Frewen et al., 2010). Using
trained experts. functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI),
12 W.M. Sayers et al.

the authors found that greater self-reported emotions, and interestingly, labeling and noting
individual differences in observing (on the are commonly used during mindfulness
Mindful Observing subscale) were positively meditation practices (e.g., noting the experience
associated with activation of the amygdala and of anger in the body). Building on this, Creswell
DMPFC while listening to scripts designed to and colleagues (2007) showed that dispositional
elicit experiences of rejection or social praise. mindfulness [as measured by the Mindful
The positive association between observing and Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown &
amygdala activation is opposite to research show- Ryan, 2003)] moderated neural responses to an
ing that dispositional mindfulness is associated affect-labeling task. Specically, dispositional
with reduced amygdala activation (Creswell, mindfulness was associated with greater activa-
Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007; Modinos, tion of PFC regulatory regions (including bilat-
Ormel, & Aleman, 2010) (discussed below). eral VLPFC) and greater deactivation of the
Importantly, these studies that found an associa- amygdala, suggesting that mindful individuals
tion between dispositional mindfulness and may be better able to recruit PFC regulatory
downregulated amygdala activation used regula- regions during affect labeling. A similar neural
tory instructions to modify the response to affec- affect regulation effect was observed in mindful
tive stimuli, while the Frewen et al. study did not. individuals when instructed to use a cognitive
Additionally, the observing subscale has been reappraisal regulatory strategy. Specically,
found to operate differently in meditating and Modinos and colleagues (2010) asked partici-
non-meditating samples (Baer et al., 2008). In pants to view negative images (e.g., burn victims,
meditators, the observing subscale correlated funeral scenes) and to reappraise, or reinterpret,
with psychological adjustment and well-being. their meaning so that they were no longer nega-
However, in non-meditators this subscale showed tive. The authors found that trait mindfulness
associations in the opposite direction. It may be during reappraisal (as measured by the KIMS)
that acceptance is an important moderating fac- was associated with increases in DMPFC activ-
tor; that is, an amygdala response may be higher ity, and this activity was negatively correlated
when observing internal and external experiences with amygdala activity.
without an accepting or nonjudgmental stance. In
addition, the reported DMPFC activation found
in the Frewen et al. study (associated with observ- Summary: Trait Mindfulness Research
ing) may implicate generating attributions about
ones emotional state (Barrett et al., 2007) and Trait mindfulness research involving neural pro-
may describe a potential neural underpinning of cessing of affective stimuli suggests a relationship
meta-cognitive awareness in mindful individuals between trait mindfulness and reduced emotional
(cf. Teasdale et al., 2002). reactivity. In studies using regulatory instructions,
Two studies of trait mindfulness and emotion trait mindfulness is also associated with enhanced
processing found mindfulness to be associated recruitment of emotion regulation regions. EEG
with increased PFC activity and reduced amyg- research has linked dispositional mindfulness to
dala activity in response to affective stimuli reduced early cortical emotional reactivity (Brown
(Creswell et al., 2007; Modinos et al., 2010). et al., 2013). fMRI research using regulatory
Both of these studies used regulatory experimen- experimental instructions (e.g., affect labeling,
tal instructions while participants were viewing cognitive reappraisal) has linked that trait mind-
affective stimuli. Previous studies have shown fulness to increased PFC activation and reduced
that linguistically labeling affective images acti- amygdala activation to affective images. Mindful
vates ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC), and deacti- traits were positively associated with increased
vates the amygdala (Lieberman et al., 2007). This activity in the PFC, and some functional connec-
research suggests that labeling ones feelings tivity ndings (Creswell et al., 2007; Modinos
may be a basic mechanism for regulating ones et al., 2010) suggest that PFC is inversely associated
2 The Emerging Neurobiology of Mindfulness and Emotion Processing 13

with amygdala activity. This work suggests that with a mindfulness of present moment emotions
when participants are given instructions to explic- and bodily sensations feel condition in
itly regulate affective responses (labeling, cogni- meditation-nave participants (Herwig et al.,
tive reappraisal), individual differences in 2010). The think instructions increased activation
mindfulness may activate emotion regulation in the amygdala, anterior cortical midline, and
regions in the PFC, which may in turn inhibit core posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). The feel
affective responses in regions such as the amyg- instructions deactivated the amygdala and
dala. These ndings suggesting a link between resulted in a shift toward more posteriorly bilat-
trait mindfulness and reduced emotional reactiv- eral inferior frontal and premotor regions. The
ity are important because emotional reactivity is feel condition also activated the middle insula,
central to dysfunctional emotion regulation which the authors interpreted as mindful atten-
(Linehan, 1993), and dysfunction in emotion reg- tion increasing interoception (i.e., awareness of
ulation is a core component in disorders of anxi- bodily states) (Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein,
ety, mood, substance abuse, and eating (Berking hman, & Dolan, 2004).
& Wupperman, 2012). One study examining mindfulness and cue-
induced cigarette craving in meditation-nave
participants also found that a mindful attentional
State Mindfulness Research stance reduced neural activity in regions impli-
in Meditation-Nave Participants cated in core affective reactivity (Westbrook
et al., 2013). Most craving researchers categorize
State mindfulness research can be categorized by craving as an affective state for motivating behav-
participants level of mindfulness training: medi- ior (see Skinner & Aubin, 2010). In this study on
tation nave, briey trained, and expert. Studies mindfulness and craving, mindfulness instruc-
of state mindfulness using meditation-nave par- tions led to reduced self-reported cigarette crav-
ticipants instruct participants with no previous ing and reduced neural reactivity to smoking cues
mindfulness training to adopt a mindful atten- in nicotine-deprived smokers. The ACC, includ-
tional stance. They use various experimental ing its subgenual region (sgACC), plays a central
instructions to ask participants to pay attention to role in the craving response of dependent smok-
present moment experience. Instructions may ask ers (Khn & Gallinat, 2011). Mindfully attending
participants to pay attention to present emotional to smoking cues not only reduced craving-related
experience and bodily sensations (Herwig, sgACC activation but also reduced its functional
Kaffenberger, Jncke, & Brhl, 2010), or to connectivity to other craving regions (e.g., ven-
actively monitor their responses to stimuli includ- tral striatum). The area of deactivation around the
ing thoughts, feelings, memories, and body sen- sgACC extended to the ventromedial PFC
sations with an accepting attitude (Westbrook (VMPFC), including Brodmanns area (BA) 10.
et al., 2013). BA 10 is thought to encode the subjective value
Taylor and colleagues (2011) instructed of goods such as an appetitive snack (Hare,
meditation-nave participants to mindfully attend ODoherty, Camerer, Schultz, & Rangel, 2008).
to images of differing emotional valences. These Mindfulness-related reductions in this area may
mindfulness instructions reduced self-reported indicate a shift away from the subjective self-
emotional intensity experienced in response to referential value of experience to a more objec-
the images across all valence categories (i.e., tive, non-evaluative engagement with present
positive, negative, and neutral). In these partici- moment experience.
pants, a mindful attentional stance deactivated In addition to reduction of emotional arousal,
the amygdala in response to positive and negative Farb and colleagues (2007) also showed a shift
images. Similarly, another study contrasted a nar- away from midline to lateral regions with engage-
rative, conceptual think condition (think about ment of a mindful attentional stance in meditation-
yourself, reect who you are, about your goals) nave participants. This study contrasted states of
14 W.M. Sayers et al.

narrative focus and experiential focus while dorsal and regulatory regions. As mentioned
mildly positive (e.g., charming) and negative above, diminished emotional reactivity may
(e.g., greedy) characteristics were presented to allow for improved emotion regulation and thus
meditation-nave participants undergoing fMRI. discourage the development of psychopathology.
(While we discuss results obtained prior to mind- Instructions to engage a mindful attentional
fulness training here, we note that this study and stance reduce reactivity and may therefore be
others that we review in this chapter included a protective. There is also some evidence to sup-
mindfulness training component. We present port a mindfulness-related shift away from the
neural results obtained after mindfulness training DMN and medial PFC toward a left lateralized
in our section on brief mindfulness training network when participants engage a mindful
below.) Instructions for narrative focus entailed (experiential) attentional focus (Farb et al., 2007;
judging what is occurring, trying to gure out Herwig et al., 2010). Although more research is
what that trait word means to the participant, needed to evaluate this claim, these neural nd-
whether it describes the participant, and allowing ings are consistent with the idea that a mindful
oneself to become caught up in a given train of attentional stance shifts one from a subjective,
thought. For experiential (mindfulness) focus, self-referential valuation of experience to a more
participants were to sense what is presently objective and non-evaluative perception of expe-
occurring in ones thoughts, feelings, and body rience. This non-evaluative perception of experi-
state, without a purpose or goal. Compared to ence may be a mechanism whereby a mindful
narrative focus, experiential focus resulted in attentional stance reduces emotional reactivity.
deactivation of cortical midline structures (sub- With non-evaluative perception, a stimulus loses
genual cingulate, PCC, and reduction in mPFC the self-referential valence required for strong
with lower threshold) and activation of a left- reactivity.
lateralized network (dorsolateral PFC, VLPFC,
and posterior parietal areas). The authors inter-
preted their results to represent a shift away from State Mindfulness Research After
the central default mode network (DMN) regions, Brief Mindfulness Meditation
the midline regions involved in narrative, self- Training
referential processing. They interpreted reduced
midline PFC activity as moving away from sub- In contrast to instructing meditation-nave par-
jective, self-referential valuation of experience to ticipants to adopt a mindful attentional stance,
more objective and non-evaluative engagement many studies offer participants a brief mindful-
(Farb et al., 2007). ness meditation training program. A growing
number of published studies randomly assign
participants to brief mindfulness meditation
Summary: State Mindfulness training programs (or control programs) and
Research with Meditation-Nave compare changes in neural activation patterns to
Participants affective stimuli before and after this training.
These training programs vary in length and inten-
State mindfulness research using meditation- sity, ranging from 4 days of approximately
nave participants, as well as trait mindfulness 25 min of training per day (Zeidan et al., 2011) to
ndings, indicates a mindfulness-related decrease 8 weeks of group and individual daily practice in
in core affective neural response and reported variants of the Mindfulness-Based Stress
emotion reactivity (Farb et al., 2007; Herwig Reduction (MBSR) program (Kabat-Zinn, 1994).
et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2011; Westbrook et al., One recent study examined changes in neural
2013). When engaging a mindful attentional processing of pain stimuli after a brief four-
stance, participants demonstrated deactivation of session 25-min mindfulness training program and
core affective, ventral regions and activation of compared this to an eyes-closed rest condition
2 The Emerging Neurobiology of Mindfulness and Emotion Processing 15

(Zeidan et al., 2011). Participants were exposed negative social trait adjectives before and after
to noxious heat stimuli while instructed to attend MBSR training and asked to consider if these
to the breath before and after mindfulness train- traits described them (Goldin, Ramel, & Gross,
ing. Attend to breath instructions did not reduce 2009). For positive traits, MBSR training resulted
self-reported pain ratings before mindfulness in reduced activity in self-referential DMN areas
training, but it did after training. Training also (medial PFC and DMPFC) and language pro-
reduced activity in the somatosensory cortex cessing (left inferior frontal gyrus) areas. MBSR
while monitoring breathing and during the appli- training increased activation to negative traits in
cation of the heat stimulus. Participants who visual attention areas (left inferior parietal lobule
reported the greatest meditation-related reduc- and medial precuneus), possibly indicating
tion in pain intensity had the largest meditation- reduced avoidance and increased ability to
related activation of the anterior insula and engage in negative social trait processing.
ACC. With regard to pain, mindfulness training Mindfulness training also resulted in decreased
coupled with meditation instruction during appli- reported SAD symptoms (Goldin et al., 2009).
cation of pain stimuli resulted in reduced experi- The second study exposed control and MBSR
enced pain. This reduction may be explained by trained participants to sad lm clips (Farb et al.,
the reduced activation of the primary somatosen- 2010). Mindfulness training reduced neural
sory cortex and increased activation of the ante- activity in response to sad lm clips compared to
rior insula and ACC. The authors suggested that controls, particularly in the precuneus, PCC, left
these effects may describe a neural basis for how posterior superior temporal gyrus (Wernickes
mindfulness meditation alters appraisals that area), and left frontal operculum (Brocas area).
impart signicance to salient sensory (pain) The precuneus and PCC are midline cortical
events (Zeidan et al., 2011), which is consistent structures that have been associated with autobio-
with early mindfulness research showing that graphical memory retrieval and self-referential
chronic pain patients had decoupled their pain processing (Cavanna & Trimble, 2006), and the
sensations from their cognitive-affective reactions PCC is a central node in the DMN. In addition to
after MBSR training (Kabat-Zinn, 1982). this shift away from DMN and language areas,
In contrast to this new research using quite MBSR participants showed more insula activity
brief training manipulations, the MBSR program during sad clips than controls.
consists of 8 weekly 2-h group sessions, a 1-day Another study that showed a mindfulness
silent retreat, and daily home meditation practice training-related reduction in DMN activity sug-
during the 8-week training program (Kabat-Zinn, gests that this reduction occurs with a decoupling
1994). This program is facilitated by an MBSR- of the DMN and insula (Farb et al., 2007). When
trained instructor who maintains a daily mindful- experiential and narrative focus conditions were
ness meditation practice. Mindfulness is taught contrasted during the presentation of positive and
through a progression of body-based mindfulness negative traits in participants who have com-
exercises (including guided meditations, mindful pleted MBSR, experiential focus reduced activity
stretching and yoga, and didactic exercises and along the anterior cortical midline (rostral dorsal
group discussions). Two published studies exam- and ventral mPFC) and activated right regulatory
ined differences in neural activation patterns after PFC regions, insula, and secondary somatosen-
MBSR training while participants were exposed sory cortex. Functional connectivity analyses
to emotional stimuli without any specic experi- showed that the insula was strongly correlated
mental instruction to modify their affective with VMPFC in controls but not in MBSR par-
response. Both studies found reduced activity in ticipants. Instead, the insula was coupled to
the DMN and language areas in response to DLPFC activity during experiential focus follow-
affective stimuli after mindfulness training. In the ing meditation training. The authors suggested
rst study, participants diagnosed with social that interoception may be strongly coupled with
anxiety disorder (SAD) were shown positive and narrative focus in controls but not in participants
16 W.M. Sayers et al.

with mindfulness training. The overall pattern (813 Hz) is used in this way to determine
suggests that experiential (mindful) focus may engagement of a positive, approach state or
reduce ventral core affective activity in the negative, withdrawal state. Studies that used this
VMPFC and amygdala during the presentation of methodology have yielded mixed results. Two
trait words, an effect that can be enhanced after studies (one using MBSR and another using an
mindfulness training. Moreover, mindfulness abbreviated 5-week training) found that mindful-
training increases the recruitment of right- ness training shifted -asymmetry toward the left
lateralized PFC regulatory areas (right ventro- hemisphere, suggesting that there may be a shift
and dorsolateral PFC), providing suggestive toward more approach-related positive emotion-
evidence for mindfulness training effects on reg- ality (Davidson et al., 2003; Moyer et al., 2011).
ulatory PFC regions (Cohen, Berkman, & However, one study showed no change with med-
Lieberman, 2013). itation training and a shift toward right dominant
A recent study explored how MBSR training asymmetry in controls using an 8-week
impacts neural responding in SAD patients. Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT)
When these participants were instructed to shift with participants with past suicidality (Barnhofer
attention to the breath while being exposed to et al., 2007). Finally, in another study, the whole
negative self-beliefs, they exhibited a reduction sample shifted toward right dominant
in amygdala activity following MBSR training -asymmetry regardless of whether participants
(Goldin & Gross, 2010). However, when partici- were controls or mindfulness trained using
pants were exposed to negative self-beliefs with- MBCT (Keune, Bostanov, Hautzinger, &
out instructions to direct attention to the breath, Kotchoubey, 2011). One fMRI study not designed
there was an initial increase or spike of amygdala to assess -asymmetry noted dominant left PFC
activity that quickly dissipated. Because these activity during meditation in expert meditators,
participants reported reduced experienced nega- and the authors interpreted this as indicative of a
tive emotion in response to negative self-beliefs, positive emotional state (Wang et al., 2011).
it may be that this initial spike in amygdala activ-
ity indicates that MBSR training increases initial
affective orienting or emotional processing Summary: State Mindfulness
(Goldin & Gross, 2010). Compared to baseline Research with Brief Mindfulness
reacting to negative self-beliefs, these SAD par- Meditation Training
ticipants also showed a shift away from anterior
midline cortical and other DMN regions with State mindfulness research involving brief mind-
training and breath-focused attention. Similar to fulness training and emotion processing indicates
previous ndings with SAD participants, they that training results in reduced markers of nega-
also demonstrated increased activation of visual tive affect, such as SAD symptoms, negative
attention that may indicate reduced avoidance of emotion, and pain intensity and unpleasantness
negative stimuli. to an applied thermal pain probe (Goldin et al.,
In addition to the fMRI studies of brief mind- 2009; Goldin & Gross, 2010; Zeidan et al., 2011).
fulness training, there are several studies that These effects of brief mindfulness meditation
used EEG to examine the effects of brief mind- training also co-occur with changes in specic
fulness training on prefrontal -asymmetry. It is neural activation patterns. Several studies indi-
believed that anterior hemispheric asymmetry cate a mindfulness-related downregulation of
reects motivational direction, with dominant DMN areas (particularly the VMPFC, DMPFC,
left-hemispheric activity reecting appetitive, and PCC) and language areas to a broad range of
approach responses and dominant right- affective stimuli (Farb et al., 2007, 2010; Goldin
hemispheric activity reecting aversive, with- et al., 2009; Goldin & Gross, 2010). This may
drawal responses (Davidson & Irwin, 1999). indicate that brief mindfulness training shifts par-
EEG measurement of asymmetry in the -band ticipants away from a self-referential, narrative
2 The Emerging Neurobiology of Mindfulness and Emotion Processing 17

focus and subjective valuation of experience. In ence (i.e., several years of daily practice) and
response to affective stimuli, two studies showed meditation-nave controls matched on variables
overall mindfulness-related amygdala deactiva- such as age, sex, education, and handedness
tion (Farb et al., 2007; Goldin & Gross, 2010), (Brewer et al., 2011; Hlzel et al., 2007; Ives-
and three studies found that mindfulness Deliperi, Solms, & Meintjes, 2011; Taylor et al.,
increased insula activation (Farb et al., 2007, 2011; Wang et al., 2011). One study compared
2010; Zeidan et al., 2011). It may be that this neural processing of emotionally evocative
insula activation indicates changes in interocep- images in meditation-nave participants and med-
tion and the appraisal of salient sensory events. itators with over 1,000 h of zen meditation expe-
This body of work suggests that reduced nega- rience under mindful viewing instruction and no
tive affect as a result of mindfulness training may instruction conditions (Taylor et al., 2011). When
be driven by several underlying neural mecha- looking at images without viewing instructions,
nisms: (1) deactivation of self-referential, evalua- the only difference between beginning and expe-
tive, and narrative DMN regions; (2) deactivation rienced meditators was that experts had decreased
of the amygdala likely indicating reduced reac- activity in the rostro-ventral ACC when viewing
tivity; and (3) increased insula activation indica- positive images. Under mindful viewing instruc-
tive of altered interoception and representation of tions, both beginning and experienced meditators
sensory events. These patterns indicate decreased reported reduced emotional intensity in response
activation of core affect regions both with and to the images with differing, group-specic neu-
without the recruitment of affect regulation ral correlates. Mindful instructions in beginners
regions found in subjects high in trait mindful- were associated with a deactivation of the amyg-
ness performing regulation tasks. It may be that a dala during processing of positive and negative
more objective perspective that accompanies images. In experienced meditators, mindful
movement away from self-referential DMN pro- viewing decreased activity in the medial PFC
cessing as a result of mindfulness training dimin- (BA 10) and PCC across all valence categories.
ishes core affect reactivity without engaging In another study comparing experienced medita-
regulatory processes. However, improved func- tors to matched nave controls while practicing
tioning of regulatory regions likely also accom- different types of meditation, meditation in expe-
panies mindfulness training. There are likely rienced meditators was associated with deactiva-
diverse neural pathways whereby mindfulness tion of the medial PFC and PCC (Brewer et al.,
training can reduce negative affect in response to 2011). Evidence therefore suggests that mindful-
affective stimuli, and reduction of negative affect ness in experienced meditators entails a shift
is critical to diverse clinical outcomes. EEG evi- away from the DMN, including altered activation
dence, although mixed, suggests that brief mind- in the medial PFC (also part of the dorsal affec-
fulness training may shift anterior hemispheric tive system).
dominance to the left or prevent increases in right
anterior dominance, which has been interpreted
as promoting a more positive and approach- Our Emerging Understanding
oriented mental stance (Barnhofer et al., 2007; of Mindfulness and the
Davidson et al., 2003; Moyer et al., 2011). Neurobiology of Emotion

The ndings to date indicate that mindfulness


State Mindfulness Research affects neurobiological networks implicated in
in Experienced Meditators emotion, including the ventral core affective net-
work, the dorsal emotion processing network,
Another body of research has examined func- and PFC regions implicated in the regulation of
tional neural differences between mindfulness emotion. In the ventral core affective network,
practitioners with signicant meditation experi- trait mindfulness and mindfulness training alter
18 W.M. Sayers et al.

activation in the amygdala, VMPFC, ACC, and amygdala response (Creswell et al., 2007;
insula in response to a broad range of affective Modinos et al., 2010). This suggests that
stimuli. Reductions of amygdala activation in individuals high in trait mindfulness might be bet-
response to affective stimuli have been found in ter able to recruit regulatory networks when using
trait and state mindfulness studies (Creswell a regulatory strategy. Changes in activation of the
et al., 2007; Farb et al., 2007; Herwig et al., 2010; DMPFC, part of the PFC included in the dorsal
Modinos et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2011). Along affect processing system, are also related to mind-
with other ventral affect processing regions (i.e., fulness. This region may support attributions
VMPFC and ACC), the amygdala inuences the about affective experience. Three studies of trait
visceromotor responses related to the value- mindfulness found mindfulness-related increases
based representations of an object (Barrett et al., in DMPFC activity (Creswell et al., 2007; Frewen
2007). Altered responses related to mindfulness et al., 2010; Modinos et al., 2010), and one state
in the VMPFC (Farb et al., 2007; Westbrook mindfulness study in MBSR trained participants
et al., 2013) and ACC (Farb et al., 2010; Taylor found a related decrease (Goldin et al., 2009). It
et al., 2011; Westbrook et al., 2013; Zeidan et al., may be that the DMPFC serves as part of the pre-
2011) have also been reported, further supporting frontal system associated with regulatory strate-
the possibility of changes in visceromotor value- gies in individuals high in trait mindfulness and is
based responses associated with mindfulness. downregulated during state mindfulness in the
These changes in this core affect response sys- shift away from midline DMN areas.
tem coupled with a shift from midline DMN In addition to the DMPFC activation men-
areas associated with self-referential valuation tioned above, mindfulness has also been found to
and narrative focus (i.e., VMPFC and PCC) be associated with altered activation of the MPFC
toward more lateral and posterior regions (Farb and ACC, which have been described as emotion
et al., 2007, 2010; Goldin et al., 2009; Goldin & processing regions in the dorsal affect network
Gross, 2010) may indicate a shift away from sub- (Barrett et al., 2007). In response to affective
jective valuation and narrative elaboration toward stimuli, a mindful attentional stance is associated
a more experiential and objective awareness of with reduced activation of the MPFC (Farb et al.,
present experience. Many theorists describe how 2007; Goldin et al., 2009; Taylor et al., 2011).
mindfulness is characterized by a nonjudgmental While the functional properties of the MPFC
awareness of ones moment-to-moment experi- have yet to be precisely dened (Amodio & Frith,
ence (Kabat-Zinn, 1994), and this may reduce the 2006), it is thought that this region participates in
evaluation of affective stimuli in terms of whether attributions made about the cause(s) of core
it is good or bad for me and reduce the elabora- affect (Barrett et al., 2007). In contrast to the
tion of thoughts related to that evaluation. The MPFC, the ACC has shown mindfulness-related
increase in insula activity with mindfulness (Farb activation in response to affective stimuli (Farb
et al., 2007, 2010; Herwig et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2011; Zeidan et al.,
et al., 2011), and its decoupling from the 2011), although craving-related activation of the
valuation-related VMPFC (Farb et al., 2007), sgACC in response to smoking cues is reduced
may also underlie the movement from subjective with mindfulness (Westbrook et al., 2013). The
evaluation to a bare awareness of present ACC is thought to signal the need to represent
experience. mental contents in consciousness with the aim of
Mindfulness has also been linked with reducing conict, improving understanding, or
increases in neural regulatory PFC regions when exerting greater control over them (Barrett et al.,
participants are instructed to regulate affect 2007). The pattern of increased ACC and
responses. When modifying responses to affective decreased MPFC associated with mindfulness
stimuli using a regulatory strategy (i.e., reap- may indicate increased understanding and con-
praisal or labeling), trait mindfulness is associated trol of mental contents while deemphasizing
with increased PFC activation and decreased attributions about the affect itself.
2 The Emerging Neurobiology of Mindfulness and Emotion Processing 19

Our review has focused on describing ndings How Do Mindfulness-Related


from self-report measures of mindfulness and Structural Brain Changes Inform
mindfulness training interventions, and future Our Understanding of Mindfulness
research would benet from comparing how and Emotion Processing?
these different types of measures and manipula-
tions of mindfulness relate to activation patterns Several studies document mindfulness-associated
in response to the same affective stimuli (e.g., a changes in gray matter density and volume in neu-
sad lm clip known to elicit robust sadness) (cf., ral regions implicated in emotion processing (i.e.,
Goldin & Gross, 2010; Taylor et al., 2011; Zeidan amygdala, hippocampus, and OFC). One study
et al., 2011). When including state mindfulness, reported that MBSR reduced perceived stress, and
it would also be useful to ask participants how reductions in perceived stress co-varied with
successful they felt they were in adopting a mind- decreases in gray matter density of the right amyg-
ful attentional stance, and to include such reports dala (Hlzel et al., 2010). Given that some previous
in analyses. Instructions to engage mindful atten- studies indicate that mindfulness alters amygdala
tion can be difcult to follow, and analyses using responses to affective stimuli (Creswell et al., 2007;
only subjects reporting success may further clar- Farb et al., 2007; Herwig et al., 2010; Modinos
ify mindful emotion processing patterns. et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2011), one promising
future direction is to examine the relationship
between structural and functional changes in amyg-
Promising Question for Future dalar response (cf. Gianaros et al., 2008). Also, sev-
Research eral studies indicate that mindfulness is associated
with increases in gray matter density (Hlzel et al.,
How Do Mindfulness-Related 2011) and gray matter concentration (Hlzel et al.,
Changes in Neural Emotion 2008; Luders, Toga, Lepore, & Gaser, 2009) in the
Processing Measures Relate hippocampus. The hippocampus sits adjacent to the
to Changes in Clinical Symptoms? amygdala and has been implicated as a core affec-
tive region. It is thought that the hippocampus facil-
Mindfulness-based interventions have been itates fear extinction, emotion processing, and
shown to reduce clinical symptoms of depression memory (Corcoran, Desmond, Frey, & Maren,
and anxiety (Hofmann et al., 2010; Roemer et al., 2005; Milad et al., 2007). Two studies indicate
2008; Teasdale et al., 2000) as well as affective structural changes in orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) of
disturbances in chronic pain patients (Grossman experienced meditators (Hlzel et al., 2008; Luders
et al., 2007; Kabat-Zinn, 1982; Kabat-Zinn et al., et al., 2009). Gray matter density in the medial
1992). It seems likely that these clinical changes OFC was positively associated with hours of medi-
may be mediated by basic changes in neurobio- tation practice in experienced meditators (Hlzel
logical emotion processing systems, although et al., 2008). Another study found increased gray
very little work has attempted to explore neural matter volumes in the OFC of experienced medita-
mechanisms of these clinical symptom changes tors compared to non-meditators (Luders et al.,
(Goldin et al., 2009; Goldin & Gross, 2010). We 2009). The OFC, part of the ventral affective pro-
are aware of several groups who are exploring cessing system, is thought to represent the affective
these brain-behavior links, so we hope to see value of an object in a exible, experience- or
advances in this area in the coming years. One context-dependent manner that the VMPFC uses to
challenge in advancing research on this question make choices and judgments based on this initial
is the complexity of analytic models for testing valuation (Barrett et al., 2007), suggesting that
brain-behavior relations, but advances in neuro- mindfulness training may increase processing
imaging toolboxes for mediation analysis are capacity for considering contextual factors during
now available (e.g., Wager et al., 2009). emotion valuation.
20 W.M. Sayers et al.

How Does Mindful Awareness Impact increases the specicity of our knowledge about
Neural Affective Responses the neurobiology of mindfulness and emotion
Over Time? processing.

Affective experiences are not monotonic; instead,


they are content-rich events that arise and pass References
away over time. Accordingly, mindfulness has
the potential to alter early orienting and attention Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds:
toward affective stimuli (Jha, Krompinger, & The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature
Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 268277.
Baime, 2007; Vago & Nakamura, 2011), to mod- Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that
ify early emotion processing as affective stimuli impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature
are initially perceived (Brown et al., 2013), and to Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 410422.
change how these stimuli are processed and regu- Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., &
Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment meth-
lated over time (Goldin & Gross, 2010). We ods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13,
know very little about how mindful attention can 2745.
alter these temporal parameters of emotion pro- Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D.,
cessing and its neural sequelae. For example, it is Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., . . . Williams, J. M. G.
(2008). Construct validity of the ve facet mindfulness
possible that mindful attention increases ones questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating sam-
attention to threat-related cues early in the emo- ples. Assessment, 15, 329342.
tion generation process while also promoting a Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004).
regulatory response during emotion processing. Assessment of mindfulness by self-report. Assessment,
11, 191206.
Indeed, an initial study suggests that this may be Barnhofer, T., Duggan, D., Crane, C., Hepburn, S.,
true (Vago & Nakamura, 2011), although the Fennell, M. J., & Williams, J. M. (2007). Effects of
neurobiological mechanisms are unknown. One meditation on frontal alpha-asymmetry in previously
limitation with current fMRI approaches is their suicidal individuals. Neuroreport, 18, 709712.
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J.
sluggish temporal resolution (collecting a whole (2007). The experience of emotion. Annual Review of
brain volume can take 13 s), which makes it dif- Psychology, 58, 373403.
cult to evaluate changes in emotion processing Berking, M., & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion regula-
to discrete affective events. Currently, EEG tion and mental health: Recent ndings, current chal-
lenges, and future directions. Current Opinion in
(Brown et al., 2013) and MEG (Kerr et al., 2011) Psychiatry, 25, 128134.
approaches offer the best temporal resolution for Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y. Y.,
testing these unexplored areas, which are impor- Weber, J., & Kober, H. (2011). Meditation experience
tant areas for future research. is associated with differences in default mode network
activity and connectivity. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 108, 2025420259.
Brown, K. W., Goodman, R. J., & Inzlicht, M. (2013).
Conclusion Dispositional mindfulness and the attenuation of neu-
ral responses to emotional stimuli. Social, Cognitive,
and Affective Neuroscience, 8(1), 9399.
An exciting body of research is emerging that Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benets of
identies how mindfulness changes the way the being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychologi-
brain processes affective stimuli. The body of cal well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
work we describe here represents our rst steps Psychology, 84, 822848.
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007).
in understanding the neurobiology of mindful- Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence
ness and emotion processing. Collectively, this for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18,
initial work indicates that trait mindfulness and 211237.
mindfulness training can alter ventral core affec- Cavanna, A. E., & Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus:
A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural
tive reactivity, dorsal emotion processing, and correlates. Brain, 129, 564583.
PFC regulatory neural affect regions. The coming Cohen, J. R., Berkman, E. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2013).
years will no doubt bring new research that Intentional and incidental self-control in ventrolateral
2 The Emerging Neurobiology of Mindfulness and Emotion Processing 21

PFC. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles Hajcak, G., MacNamara, A., & Olvet, D. M. (2010).
of frontal lobe function (2nd ed., pp. 417440). Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regula-
New York: Oxford University Press. tion: An integrative review. Developmental
Corcoran, K. A., Desmond, T. J., Frey, K. A., & Maren, S. Neuropsychology, 35, 129155.
(2005). Hippocampal inactivation disrupts the acquisi- Hare, T. A., ODoherty, J., Camerer, C. F., Schultz, W., &
tion and contextual encoding of fear extinction. The Rangel, A. (2008). Dissociating the role of the orbito-
Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 89788987. frontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of
Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & goal values and prediction errors. The Journal of
Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural correlates of dispo- Neuroscience, 28, 56235630.
sitional mindfulness during affect labeling. Herwig, U., Kaffenberger, T., Jncke, L., & Brhl, A. B.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 560565. (2010). Self-related awareness and emotion regula-
Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., hman, A., & tion. NeuroImage, 50(2), 734741. http://doi.org/
Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting intero- 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.089.
ceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 189195. Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D.
Cuthbert, B. N., Schupp, H. T., Bradley, M. M., Birbaumer, (2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on
N., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Brain potentials in affective anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review.
picture processing: Covariation with autonomic Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78,
arousal and affective report. Biological Psychology, 169183.
52, 95111. Hlzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Evans, K. C., Hoge, E. A.,
Davidson, R. J., & Irwin, W. (1999). The functional neu- Dusek, J. A., Morgan, L., . . . Lazar, S. W. (2010).
roanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Stress reduction correlates with structural changes in
Cognitive Sciences, 3, 1121. the amygdala. Social Cognitive and Affective
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Neuroscience, 5, 1117.
Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., . . . Hlzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C.,
Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T., & Lazar, S. W. (2011).
function produced by mindfulness meditation. Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional
Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564570. brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Research:
Farb, N. A. S., Anderson, A. K., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., Neuroimaging, 191, 3643.
McKeon, D., & Segal, Z. V. (2010). Minding ones Hlzel, B. K., Ott, U., Gard, T., Hempel, H., Weygandt,
emotions: Mindfulness training alters the neural M., Morgen, K., & Vaitl, D. (2008). Investigation of
expression of sadness. Emotion, 10, 2533. mindfulness meditation practitioners with voxel-based
Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., morphometry. Social Cognitive and Affective
McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Neuroscience, 3, 5561.
Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation Hlzel, B. K., Ott, U., Hempel, H., Hackl, A., Wolf, K.,
reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Social Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2007). Differential engagement
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 313322. of anterior cingulate and adjacent medial frontal cor-
Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J. A., Neufeld, R. W. J., Lane, tex in adept meditators and non-meditators.
R. D., Densmore, M., Stevens, T. K., & Lanius, R. A. Neuroscience Letters, 421, 1621.
(2010). Individual differences in trait mindfulness pre- Ives-Deliperi, V. L., Solms, M., & Meintjes, E. M. (2011).
dict dorsomedial prefrontal and amygdala response The neural substrates of mindfulness: An fMRI inves-
during emotional imagery: An fMRI study. Personality tigation. Social Neuroscience, 6, 231242.
and Individual Differences, 49, 479484. Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007).
Gianaros, P. J., Sheu, L. K., Matthews, K. A., Jennings, Mindfulness training modies subsystems of atten-
J. R., Manuck, S. B., & Hariri, A. R. (2008). Individual tion. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience,
differences in stressor-evoked blood pressure reactiv- 7, 109119.
ity vary with activation, volume, and functional con- Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behav-
nectivity of the amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience, ioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the
28, 990999. practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical con-
Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness- siderations and preliminary results. General Hospital
based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation Psychiatry, 4, 3347.
in social anxiety disorder. Emotion, 10, 8391. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are:
Goldin, P., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. (2009). Mindfulness Mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York,
meditation training and self-referential processing in NY: Hyperion Books.
social anxiety disorder: Behavioral and neural effects. Kabat-Zinn, J., Massion, A. O., Kristeller, J., Peterson,
Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 242257. L. G., Fletcher, K. E., Pbert, L., . . . Santorelli, S. F.
Grossman, P., Tiefenthaler-Gilmer, U., Raysz, A., & (1992). Effectiveness of a meditation-based stress
Kesper, U. (2007). Mindfulness training as an inter- reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders.
vention for bromyalgia: Evidence of postintervention The American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 936943.
and 3-year follow-up benets in well-being. Kerr, C. E., Jones, S. R., Wan, Q., Pritchett, D. L.,
Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 76, 226233. Wasserman, R. H., Wexler, A., . . . Kaptchuk, T. J.
22 W.M. Sayers et al.

(2011). Effects of mindfulness meditation training on Mindfulness: Theory, Research, and Practice. New
anticipatory alpha modulation in primary somatosen- York, NY: Guilford Publications.
sory cortex. Brain Research Bulletin, 84, 96103. Roemer, L., Orsillo, S. M., & Salters-Pedneault, K.
Keune, P. M., Bostanov, V., Hautzinger, M., & Kotchoubey, (2008). Efcacy of an acceptance-based behavior ther-
B. (2011). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy apy for generalized anxiety disorder: Evaluation in a
(MBCT), cognitive style, and the temporal dynamics of randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and
frontal EEG alpha asymmetry in recurrently depressed Clinical Psychology, 76, 10831089.
patients. Biological Psychology, 88, 243253. Skinner, M. D., & Aubin, H. J. (2010). Cravings place in
Khn, S., & Gallinat, J. (2011). Common biology of crav- addiction theory: Contributions of the major models.
ing across legal and illegal drugsa quantitative meta Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34,
analysis of cuereactivity brain response. European 606623.
Journal of Neuroscience, 33(7), 13181326. Taylor, V., Grant, J., Daneault, V., Scavone, G., Breton, E.,
Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Roffe-Vidal, S., . . . Beauregard, M. (2011). Impact of
Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M. (2007). mindfulness on the neural responses to emotional pic-
Putting feelings into words affect labeling disrupts tures in experienced and beginner meditators.
amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Neuroimage, 57, 15241533.
Psychological Science, 18, 421428. Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M.,
Linehan, M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive
borderline personality disorder. New York, NY: awareness and prevention of relapse in depression:
Guilford Press. Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and
Luders, E., Toga, A. W., Lepore, N., & Gaser, C. (2009). Clinical Psychology, 70, 275287.
The underlying anatomical correlates of long-term Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway,
meditation: Larger hippocampal and frontal volumes V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention
of gray matter. NeuroImage, 45, 672678. of relapse/recurrence in major depression by
Milad, M. R., Wright, C. I., Orr, S. P., Pitman, R. K., mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of
Quirk, G. J., & Rauch, S. L. (2007). Recall of fear Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615623.
extinction in humans activates the ventromedial pre- Vago, D. R., & Nakamura, Y. (2011). Selective attentional
frontal cortex and hippocampus in concert. Biological bias towards pain-related threat in bromyalgia:
Psychiatry, 62, 446454. Preliminary evidence for effects of mindfulness medita-
Modinos, G., Ormel, J., & Aleman, A. (2010). Individual tion training. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 114.
differences in dispositional mindfulness and brain Wager, T. D., van Ast, V. A., Hughes, B. L., Davidson,
activity involved in reappraisal of emotion. Social M. L., Lindquist, M. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2009).
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 369377. Brain mediators of cardiovascular responses to social
Moyer, C. A., Donnelly, M. P. W., Anderson, J. C., Valek, threat, part II: Prefrontal-subcortical pathways and
K. C., Huckaby, S. J., Wiederholt, D. A., . . . Rice, relationship with anxiety. NeuroImage, 47, 836851.
B. L. (2011). Frontal electroencephalographic asym- Wang, D. J. J., Rao, H., Korczykowski, M., Wintering, N.,
metry associated with positive emotion is produced by Pluta, J., Khalsa, D. S., & Newberg, A. B. (2011).
very brief meditation training. Psychological Science, Cerebral blood ow changes associated with different
22, 12771279. meditation practices and perceived depth of medita-
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive tion. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 191, 6067.
control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, Westbrook, C., Creswell, J. D., Tabibnia, G., Julson, E.,
242249. Kober, H., & Tindle, H. (2013). Mindful attention
Phillips, M. L., Drevets, W. C., Rauch, S. L., & Lane, R. reduces neural and self-reported cue-induced craving
(2003). Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The in smokers. Social, Cognitive, and Affective
neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biological Neuroscience, 8, 7384.
Psychiatry, 54, 504514. Zeidan, F., Martucci, K. T., Kraft, R. A., Gordon, N. S.,
Quaglia, J.T., Brown, K.W., Lindsay, E.K., Creswell, J.D., McHafe, J. G., & Coghill, R. C. (2011). Brain mech-
& Goodman, R.J. (2015). Current conceptualizations anisms supporting the modulation of pain by mindful-
and operationalizations of mindfulness. In K. W. ness meditation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31,
Brown & R. R. Creswell (Eds.), Handbook of 55405548.
Mindfulness and Training
Attention 3
Yi-Yuan Tang and Michael I. Posner

primarily, though not exclusively, concentrate on


Attention and Self-Regulation the executive attention network as this network
has the greatest relevance to mindfulness and to
Imaging the human brain by the use of functional self-regulation. In terms of this network, a behav-
magnetic resonance (fMRI) has revealed brain ioral criterion of efficiency refers to the speed
networks common to people when they perform with which conflict is resolved. Or, stated in other
tasks like reading, computing, or playing video terms, a person is efficient to the extent that there
games (Posner & Rothbart, 2007). One of the is a smaller difference between correct responses
most common areas of study has been tasks that when conflict is involved versus not. Behavioral
involve attentionmaintaining an alert state, ori- efficiency, defined in this manner, has been
enting to sensory stimuli, and/or resolving con- shown to correlate with the strength of anterior
flict among competing responses. Studies have cingulate cortex (ACC) activation in the execu-
revealed specific brain networks related to each tive network and with the degree of connectivity
of these attentional functions. The network between nodes of this network (Fjell et al., 2012).
involved in resolving conflict also serves as a In more specific terms, the Attention Network
means of self-regulation through control of brain Test (ANT) was devised as a means to measure
networks involved in emotion, cognition, and network efficiency (Fan, McCandliss, Sommer,
behavior (Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000; Posner, Raz, & Posner, 2002). The task, as illustrated in
Rothbart, Sheese, & Tang, 2007). Fig. 3.1, requires the person to press one key if
Although everyone has these attentional net- the central arrow points to the left and another if
works, people can differ dramatically in the effi- it points to the right. Conflict is introduced by
ciency with which they are used. We will having surrounding flankers either point in the
same (congruent) or opposite (incongruent)
direction of the central arrow. Cues presented
prior to the target provide information on when
or where the target will occur and these condi-
Y.-Y. Tang (*) tions are relevant to the alerting and orienting
Department of Psychology, Texas Tech University,
networks instead. By subtracting certain condi-
Lubbock, TX, USA
e-mail: yiyuan.tang@ttu.edu tions from other conditions, with speed as the
metric, three numbers can be assigned to the per-
M.I. Posner
Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, son that reflect the efficiency of the alerting, ori-
Eugene, OR, USA enting, and executive networks.

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 23


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_3
24 Y.-Y. Tang and M.I. Posner

Fig. 3.1 Attention network test. (a) Indicates the four cueing conditions, (b) indicates the target conditions, (c) indi-
cates the time line, (d) the three subtractions produce scores for alerting, orienting, and executive attention

Most MRI studies involve imaging the brain executive attention network develops and its
during tasks, but it has recently become common locus involves the ACC and its connections to
to study the brains of children and adults while other brain areas.
they are resting without any task as well (resting- Just as task and resting-state MRI are two
state MRI: Raichle, 2009). Resting state methods fundamentally different methods of conducting
can be applied at any age because they do not MRI experiments related to self-regulation,
require a task and the results of these studies are there are also two very different methods that
also informative concerning developmental pro- can be used to train self-regulation (Tang &
cesses. One of the brain networks active during Posner, 2009; Tang, Rothbart, & Posner, 2012b).
rest is the executive network involved in resolv- We term one of these methods network training
ing conflict and self-regulation (Dosenbach et al., and the other brain state training. In network
2007; Fair et al., 2009). During infancy and early training, specific tasks are repeatedly adminis-
childhood, most brain networks involve short tered to exercise the brain network to be trained,
connections between adjacent areas, such that the resulting in greater efficiency. This efficiency is
long connections that underlie complex forms of thought to occur through two mechanismsone
self-regulation develop slowly over childhood that tunes the neurons in each node to fit the
(Fair et al., 2009; Gao et al., 2009). Indeed, the mental operations being performed and the
brain network related to orienting to sensory other that strengthens the connection between
events seems to provide the primary source of nodes (Tang, Rothbart et al., 2012b). Conflict,
regulation prior to 23 years of age (Posner, working memory, and executive function tasks
Rothbart, Sheese, & Voelker, 2012; Rothbart, have all been used to improve self-regulation by
Sheese, Rueda, & Posner, 2011). Thereafter, an network training.
3 Mindfulness and Training Attention 25

Mindfulness meditation and aerobic exercise experimental group performed a task requiring
are two examples of efforts to improve attention them to select the larger of two arrays of items and
through the training of brain state. In technical conflict trials manipulated digit number such that
terms, brain states refer to reliable patterns of larger arrays were composed of smaller digits or
brain activity that involve the co-activation and/ vice versa. Before and after training, the children
or connectivity of multiple large-scale brain net- performed a child-based version of the ANT (Fan
works (Tang, Rothbart et al., 2012b). Training et al., 2002) while brain responses were measured
brain states is thought to improve ones ability to by scalp electrodes (Rueda et al., 2005).
switch between different states and/or to main- The effects of training were tested at ages (47
tain a state when desirable. Mindfulness training years) linked to major improvements in executive
is thought to work by facilitating the ability to attention. After training, EEG data showed clear
enter and remain in a meditative state despite the evidence of improvement in network efficiency
brains tendencies toward exiting this state (Tang, in resolving conflict as the result of training. In
Rothbart et al., 2012b). The next two sections of particular, we focused on the N2 evoked response
the chapter describe these two training methods component whose neural generator is the ACC
in greater depth. and whose occurrence is related to conflict reso-
lution (Dehaene, Posner, & Tucker, 1994; van
Veen & Carter, 2002). The N2 differences
Training Brain Networks between congruent and incongruent trials of the
ANT in trained 6-year-olds resembled differ-
When a network involves a general function such ences found among adults, whereas this was not
as attention or working memory, increased effi- the case for the control group. The trained group
ciency should produce improvements in many also showed a greater improvement in intelli-
different tasks that involve that network. Since gence compared to controls, as measured by the
working memory training may also target the K-Bit test, a child-friendly test of IQ. The
manner in which attention works (Hofmann, improvement was in overall IQ and in a matrix
Schmeichel, & Baddeley, 2012; Klingberg, portion similar to the adult Ravens test. These
2011), the range of tasks that might be affected is results are important in that they reveal general-
further increased. In addition, such training may ization to a measure of cognitive processing far
potentially extend beyond cognitive tasks to more removed from the training exercises.
social and emotional forms of self-regulation. A replication and extension of the Rueda et al.
To examine the role of training on the execu- (2005) study was carried out for 5-year-olds in a
tive attention network, we (Rueda, Rothbart, Spanish preschool (Rueda, Checa, & Combita,
McCandliss, Saccamanno, & Posner, 2005) 2012). Several additional exercises were added
adapted a method that had been used by NASA to and 10 days of training occurred. The design also
train monkeys for space travel. Children were ran- included a 2-month follow-up session. Unlike the
domly assigned to either an experimental group control group, trained children showed improve-
that was trained in executive attention or a control ment in intelligence scores, as measured by the
group that viewed child-appropriate videos for the matrices scale of the K-BIT intelligence test. The
same amount of time, a 5-day portion of the study. N2 findings of the earlier study were also concep-
Children in the experimental group first learned to tually replicated. In addition, executive attention
use a joystick to move a cat displayed on the mon- training was shown to improve emotion regula-
itor to a grass area while avoiding mud. As the tion (e.g., in a childs delay of gratification when
child progressed, the task became more difficult a reward was present). Finally, evidence for
because the grass area shrank and the mud area improved functioning was evident 2 months later
increased. The task is thought to require skills despite no intervening practice.
involving prediction, working memory, and con- Related results have been reviewed by
flict resolution. In addition, children in the Diamond and Lee (2011). Attention training has
26 Y.-Y. Tang and M.I. Posner

occurred in the context of classroom activities more optimistic note. Although we have sug-
(Diamond, Barnett, Thomas, & Munro, 2007; gested that executive attention training and WMT
Stevens, Lauinger, & Neville, 2009) or individual work on similar neural substrates, the activation
computer training (Klingberg, 2011; Rueda et al., appears to be more medial in the former case and
2012), in the latter case typically using attention lateral in the latter (Klingberg, 2011).
or working memory tasks. Usually the tasks Methodological issues may be involved, but such
increase in difficulty over time, pushing training- methodological issues have not been fully
group participants to continually improve. As an resolved. Turning to a different issue, scholars
example of this body of research, a yearlong have noted that the skills taught in preschool
curriculum-based program termed Tools of the training programs are often not retained for a
Mind, which is designed to improve executive lengthy period of time (Heckman, 2006).
function, results in rather large gains in the ability However, we suggest that are exceptions to this
to resolve conflict (Diamond & Lee, 2011). general pessimism, including in the realm of self-
Working memory training (WMT) includes regulation achievements (Moffitt et al., 2011).
procedures designed to improve executive atten- Further, there is evidence that neural assessments
tion. For this reason, a brief review of WMT of self-regulation converge with other assess-
studies is warranted. One paper reported that ments such as parental reports (Rothbart, 2011).
intensive adaptive WMT was associated with Finally, we suggest that executive attention train-
improved verbal memory span and complex ing affects functioning outside of the school con-
arithmetic ability, though puzzlingly along with text and that there is evidence that such skills can
reduced regional gray matter volume in frontal- be retained for many years (Chatty et al., 2010;
parietal brain regions (Takeuchi et al., 2011). Moffitt et al., 2011).
This study also reported another puzzle in that
WMT increased brain activity for some partici-
pants, but decreased it for others. It is possible Training Brain States
that the latter inconsistencies relate to individual
differences in learning strategies, motivation, or The approaches discussed above seek to obtain
effort (Klingberg, 2011). improvement in networks by exercising them. A
There are disputes as to whether WMT trans- rather different approach to training may be to
fers to other tasks or to more general abilities. We develop a brain state conducive to self-regulation.
do suggest, though, that there is some agreement One example is a form of mindfulness training
concerning the regional activation patterns that called Integrative Body-Mind Training (IBMT).
are likely to follow from WMT. In a representa- IBMT originates from ancient Eastern contempla-
tive study, Olesen, Westerberg, and Klingberg tive traditions including traditional Chinese medi-
(2004) found that areas of the frontal and parietal cine and Zen, but shares key components with
cortex were more strongly activated following other forms of mindfulness meditation. IBMT
the training than prior to it (also see Takeuchi teaches people to engage in less or no effort to
et al., 2010). There also appears to be involve- control their thoughts. Instead, people are coached
ment of the caudate nucleus (Olesen et al., 2004). toward a state of restful alertness that allows for a
Whether increased brain activity reflects effort or balanced (and high) degree of awareness of what
is functional may be in some dispute, however. is happening in the body, the mind, and the envi-
One study found that increased activity in a ronment. IBMT uses a qualified trainer to ensure
WMT condition correlated with improved per- that nave learners achieve the state without strong
formance (Olesen et al., 2004), but others have effort while working with trainer-group dynam-
been critical of data of this type (e.g., Buschkuehl, ics, harmony, and resonance (for a review of
Jaeggi, & Jonides, 2012). IBMT, see Tang, Yang, Leve, & Harold, 2012c).
In addition, we highlight some other issues The IBMT method leads to very rapid change
that need to be resolved while concluding on a and is therefore suited to experimental study. In
3 Mindfulness and Training Attention 27

our studies, people are randomly assigned to The tracts showing higher FA following medita-
IBMT or to a control group that receives relax- tion training included the anterior corona radiata,
ation training in accordance with standard cogni- which has previously been linked to operations of
tive behavioral treatments. Control group the executive attention network (Niogi,
participants receive more than a placebo, then, Mukherjee, Ghajar, & McCandliss, 2009).
and they also believe that they are learning impor- There are at last two ways in which white mat-
tant skills. In fact, we have found both conditions ter tracts might be changed in efficiency by train-
to be equally palatable to participants (e.g., Tang ing. One involves increases in the density of the
et al., 2010). axons that make up the connections between
In one study (Tang et al., 2007), participants brain areas and the other involves myelination
were randomly assigned to IBMT or to relaxation increases between these same brain areas. The
training, each involving 5 days of instruction and measure of FA is composed of both radial
practice. A battery of tests was administered. changes most closely related to myelination and
This included the ANT, the Profile of Mood axial changes most closely related to axon den-
States (POMS), and a stress challenge of a men- sity. Based on further considerations, Tang, Lu,
tal arithmetic task, following which cortisol and Fan, Yang, and Posner (2012a) concluded that
secretory immunoglobulin A (sIgA, an index of both sets of changes occurred as the result of
immunoreactivity) were assessed. All measures IBMT. This is interesting because attention net-
were scored blind to group condition. The IBMT work training effects have primarily been
group showed significantly greater improvement ascribed to changes in myelination alone. In
than the relaxation group on the executive atten- development, though, both changes in axon den-
tion measure. In addition, the IBMT group had sity and myelination occur. Accordingly, IBMT-
improved mood states and exhibited lesser corti- induced changes in white matter function are
sol and sIgA reactivity to the stressor task. The unlike changes due to training specific brain net-
improvements appeared to involve a change in works and more similar to what is found in brain
brain state in that there was increased brain activ- maturation due to development. Among other
ity in areas that control the parasympathetic por- implications, it may be possible to use state train-
tion of the autonomic nervous system, consistent ing by meditation to explore the behavioral con-
with a quiet, but alert state of focused attention. sequences of increases in axon density and
IBMT also altered resting-state fMRI activity myelination independent of developmental
(Tang et al., 2009). confounds.
A subsequent study (Tang et al., 2010) revealed Mindfulness training can also be carried out
additional mechanisms of change. The protocol with children. In one study (Tang, Yang et al.,
again involved 5 days of IBMT training versus 2012c), we tested the efficacy of IBMT in 60
relaxation training. Subsequently, neuroimaging Chinese preschool children between the ages of 4
was used to assay brain changes due to training. and 5. We modified the IBMT intervention by
The IBMT group had greater ACC activity as well using cartoons and stories to create an environ-
as greater functional connectivity between the ment to help the children enter a meditative state.
ACC and striatum. Moreover, parasympathetic Results of this randomized controlled trial were
function had changed more in the IBMT group consistent with the IBMT studies with adoles-
than in the relaxation control group. Diffusion cents and adults: About 10 h of IBMT signifi-
tensor imaging (DTI) analyses were also per- cantly increased self-regulation as indicated by
formed. These measured the directionality of parent ratings of their children on the Child
water molecules (fractional anisotropy or FA) due Behavior Questionnaire (Rothbart, 2011). We
to white matter and found that several white mat- also found that IBMT improved the ability of
ter tracts connecting the ACC to other brain areas children to resolve conflict on two (Stroop and
had improved their efficiency (Tang et al., 2010). go/no go) executive processing tasks in compari-
28 Y.-Y. Tang and M.I. Posner

son with an active control group (Tang, Yang Pathologies of Attention


et al., 2012c). All told, findings involving IBMT
reveal that the brain systems related to self- The ability to image the human brain has pro-
regulation can be modified in their activation and vided new perspectives for neuropsychologists in
in their connectivity. These results have several their efforts to understand, diagnose, and treat
implications and applications, some of which are damage to the human brain that might occur as
considered later in this chapter (also see Tang, the result of stroke, tumor, traumatic injury,
Rothbart et al., 2012b). degenerative disease, or errors in development. In
fact, there is convergence among neurological
and psychiatric perspectives of disorder, a con-
Does Training Last? vergence that makes sense in that both sets of dis-
orders involve dysfunctions in the efficacy of
As mentioned earlier, studies conducted on the brain networks.
role of preschool training such as Headstart have In more specific terms, attentional problems
often concluded that effects on schooling can be are a very frequent symptom of different forms
found at first, but over a few years they are of life difficulties, whether those involving
reduced or eliminated (Heckman, 2006). This learning disabilities or psychopathology. Before
may be in part because those not exposed to the there was a real understanding of the neural sub-
preschool program nevertheless receive extensive strates of attention, there was not a sufficient
elementary education and school outcomes have corpus of knowledge to remedy attentional
been emphasized. Outside of the context of problems. This is no longer the case. Viewing
schoolwork, though, long-term improvements in attention as an organ system and investigating
life outcomes can occur as a result of preschool the underlying neural networks provides a
training (Ludwig & Phillips, 2008). means of classifying disorders that differs from
Further, there is ample evidence that self- the usual internalizing (e.g., depression) versus
regulation skills, if they can be imparted early, externalizing (e.g., conduct disorder) classifica-
matter in determining life trajectories. For exam- tion applied to such disorders. In the section
ple, one study of middle school children showed below, we consider the relationship between
that a parent report measure of self-regulation attention networks and some common disor-
correlated with grades more strongly than other, ders. In general, we do not know whether the
often emphasized measures such as IQ (Moffitt attention deficits cause or result from the disor-
et al., 2011). In this same study of 1,000 children ders, but an attention-related analysis can illu-
followed for 30 years, self-regulation abilities minate the symptoms involved as well as suggest
among children were positively related to income, methods of prevention and/or remediation. A
health, and reduced criminality in adulthood. number of disorders seem to primarily involve
Although this was not a training study, those chil- the executive attention network and these
dren who, for whatever reason, showed greater include addiction, psychopathy, borderline per-
improvements in self-regulation during child- sonality, and schizophrenia.
hood most definitely benefited later on in life.
Such results, as well as more direct ones involv-
ing training (Ludwig & Phillips, 2008), advocate Substance Abuse
in favor of attempts to improve the self-regulation
abilities of children. As described above, medita- A recent test of long-term adolescent marijuana
tion training for preschool children provides abusers using conflict tasks and fMRI showed
another means of improving self-regulation, par- that in comparison to controls, abusers showed a
ticularly if the meditation skills are continually deficit in the ability to resolve conflict caused by
practiced and reinforced. an inefficient executive network (Abdullaev,
3 Mindfulness and Training Attention 29

Posner, Nunnally, & Dishion, 2010). This result emotional controls are less active in this
could either be the cause of the abuse or the result population, as psychopaths may themselves report.
of using the drug. In either case, methods that In this regard, psychopathy seems to have some-
might strengthen the activation and connectivity thing in common with borderline personality disor-
of the ACC could be useful in treatment of the der (see below) in terms of a difficulty in handling
disorder. emotion. However, the origin of borderline person-
Many other forms of addiction such as to ality disorder appears to be quite early in life
cocaine, tobacco, and alcohol involve deficits of whereas this may be less true of psychopathy.
self-regulation and it is noteworthy that studies
have linked these addictions to operations of the
ACC or to related areas of the midfrontal cortex Borderline Personality
and other parts of the executive attention network
(Goldstein & Volkow, 2011; Volkow & Fowler, Borderline personality disorder is characterized
2000). These are the brain areas that have been by a great lability of affect and difficulties in
found to be improved by the use of IBMT (Tang interpersonal relations. In some cases, patients
et al., 2009; Tang, Lu et al., 2012a), suggesting are suicidal or carry out self-mutilation. Because
that IBMT or other forms of mindfulness training this diagnosis has been studied largely by psy-
may be an effective intervention for substance choanalysts and has a very complex definition, it
abusers. might at first be thought of as a poor candidate for
a specific pathophysiology involving attentional
networks. However, by focusing on the tempera-
Psychopathy mentally based core symptoms of negative emo-
tionality and difficulty in self-regulation, patients
Psychopathy involves a failure of empathy for the can be characterized as very high in temperamen-
pain of others when taking actions that favor the tal negative affect and relatively low in effortful
self. Many believe that the initial impetus for control (Posner et al., 2002). Even when matched
empathy lies in the mirror neuron system that on these two temperament-related dimensions,
allows the pain of others to be reflected in the however, we have found that people meeting the
neuronal discharges of the self. However, studies diagnosis for borderline personality disorder dis-
suggest that psychopathic behavior may also be play executive attention deficits on the ANT rela-
linked to the degree of attention paid to environ- tive to those who are not diagnosable with a
mental cues. According to Newmans work, psy- personality disorder (Posner, 2012).
chopaths who are in prison for their behavior Imaging results further suggest overgeneral-
differ from non-psychopathic prisoners in the ization of responding to negative stimuli in the
degree to which the emotional cues of others will amygdala among borderline patients along with
influence their behavior, but only if the situation reduced responding in the ACC and related mid-
brings those cues to attention. When cues are not line frontal areas involved in self-regulation
deliberately attended, psychopaths seem to (Silbersweig et al., 2007). In the Silbersweig
behave similarly to non-psychopaths, but psy- et al. (2007) study, it was also found that lower
chopaths do not seem to monitor the environment levels of effortful control and high ANT conflict
for those cues if they are not already in the focus scores predicted poorer responses to therapy,
of attention (Zeier, Maxwell, & Newman, 2009). another feature of borderline pathology. Overall,
It is as though the pain of others is not a salient we emphasize the scientific benefits that are
enough cue for psychopaths. Consistent with this likely to accrue when methods focus on the core
analysis, imaging studies have found that the ven- deficits of patients, match patients to controls on
tral ACC is less active among psychopaths when the basis of temperament scores, and use atten-
viewing frightening faces than among normals tion performance as a guide to conducting infor-
(for a review, see Blair, 2010), suggesting that mative imaging studies.
30 Y.-Y. Tang and M.I. Posner

Schizophrenia diagnosis. The data also showed a much smaller


orienting deficit of the type that had been reported
A number of years ago, never-medicated schizo- previously in first-break patients. What we
phrenic patients were tested with a cued detection emphasize is that there is a strong executive defi-
task similar to the orienting part of the ANT and cit in chronic schizophrenia (consistent with the
were studied using positron emission tomography analysis of Benes, 1999), but that it remains to be
(PET). These patients showed a deficit in orienting determined whether this deficit exists prior to the
similar to what had been found for patients with initial symptoms or develops with the disorder.
left parietal lobe damage (Posner, Early, Reiman,
Pardo, & Dhawan, 1988). At rest, these schizo-
phrenic subjects also showed a focal decrease in
cerebral blood flow in the left globus pallidus
Conclusions and Future Research
(Posner et al., 1988), a part of the basal ganglia
Brain networks underlie aspects of attention and
with close ties to the anterior cingulate. When their
self-regulation. In recent years, two fundamen-
visual attention was engaged, they had difficulty in
tally different approaches have been reported to
shifting attention to the right visual field and they
improve attention and self-regulation. One prac-
also showed deficits in conflict tasks, particularly
tices the network through the execution of spe-
when they had to rely on a language cue. It was
cific tasks. Another involves the use of meditation
concluded that differences found in comparison to
as a means of developing a brain state that serves
a control group were consistent with difficulties in
to improve self-regulation and reduce stress.
the executive attention system with signs of mostly
Both directions of training can be advocated and
left hemisphere deficits.
there is evidence that early self-regulation ability
The deficit in orienting rightward has been
is related to more favorable later life outcomes.
replicated in first-break schizophrenics, but it
This new knowledge could lead to important
does not seem to be characteristic of later stages
improvements in treatment. To do this, we need
of the disorder (Maruff, Hay, Malone, & Currie,
to investigate how to combine methods of
1995), nor does the pattern appear to be part of
improving self-regulation that fit well with the
the genetic predisposition for schizophrenia
educational system. Methods involving training
(Pardo et al., 2000). As schizophrenia progresses,
of specific attention networks and those that
the cognitive deficits become more severe and
involve changing brain states may work at differ-
more general. Yet, there have been many reports
ent rates and on different aspects of network
of ACC and basal ganglia deficits in patients with
improvement. Imaging the brain may give us
schizophrenia (Benes, 1999). In more particular
clues as to how best to develop a combination of
terms, schizophrenic patients coming to autopsy
training strategies that may be particularly effica-
showed deficits in outflow from the ACC to a
cious. Moreover, individual differences in tem-
number of other frontal and temporal structures
perament (Rothbart, 2011) could make one
that have also been implicated in functional anal-
method more useful than others for particular
yses of the disorder (Benes, 1999).
children. Studies of self-regulation may also lead
A study using the ANT casts some light on
to more refined methods of training and instruc-
these results (Wang et al., 2005). In this study, the
tion. These could provide unusual opportunities
chronic schizophrenic patients had a much
for educators, psychologists, and neuroscientists
greater difficulty resolving conflict than did the
to work together for the common goal of improv-
similarly aged normal controls. The deficit in
ing childrens lives.
patients was also much larger than that found for
borderline personality patients. However, there
Acknowledgement This work was supported by the
was still a great deal of overlap between the Office of Naval Research N000141110034. We thank the
patients and normal subjects, indicating that the editors for insightful comments and Rongxiang for manu-
deficit is not suitable for making a differential script preparation input.
3 Mindfulness and Training Attention 31

References ings and clinical implications. Nature Reviews


Neuroscience, 12, 652669.
Heckman, J. J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics
Abdullaev, Y., Posner, M. I., Nunnally, R., & Dishion, T.
of investing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312,
(2010). Functional MRI evidence for inefficient atten-
19001902.
tional control in adolescent chronic cannabis abusers.
Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D.
Behavioral Brain Research, 215, 4557.
(2012). Executive functions and self-regulation.
Benes, F. M. (1999). Alterations of neural circuitry within
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174180.
layer II of anterior cingulate cortex in schizophrenia.
Klingberg, T. (2011). Training working memory and
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 33, 511512.
attention. In M. I. Posner (Ed.), Cognitive neurosci-
Blair, R. J. R. (2010). Psychopathy, frustration, and reac-
ence of attention (pp. 4560). New York, NY: Guilford.
tive aggression: The role of ventromedial prefrontal
Ludwig, J. C., & Phillips, D. A. (2008). The long-term
cortex. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 383399.
effects of head start on low-income children. Annals of
Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., & Jonides, J. (2012).
the New York Academy of Sciences, 40, 112.
Neuronal effects following working memory training.
Maruff, P., Hay, D., Malone, V., & Currie, J. (1995).
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(Suppl 1),
Asymmetries in the covert orienting of visual spatial
S167S179.
attention in schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 33,
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and
12051223.
emotional influences in the anterior cingulate cortex.
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N.,
Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 215222.
Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient
Chatty, R., Friedman, J. N., Hilger, N., Saez, E.,
of childhood self control predicts health, wealth, and
Schanzenbach, D., & Yagan, D. (2010). How does
public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of
your kindergarten performance affect your earnings:
Sciences of the USA, 108, 26932698.
Evidence from project STAR. On line http://obs.rc.fas.
Niogi, S., Mukherjee, P., Ghajar, J., & McCandliss, B. B.
harvard.edu/chetty/STAR_slides.pdf
(2009). Individual differences in distinct components of
Dehaene, S., Posner, M. I., & Tucker, D. M. (1994).
attention are linked to anatomical variations in distinct
Localization of a neural system for error detection and
white matter tracts. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, 4, 2.
compensation. Psychological Science, 5, 303305.
Olesen, P. J., Westerberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2004).
Diamond, A., Barnett, S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007).
Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training
Preschool improves cognitive control. Science, 30,
of working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 7579.
13871388.
Pardo, P. J., Knesevich, M. A., Vogler, J. V., Towne, B.,
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to
Cloninger, C. R., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Genetic and
aid executive function development in children 4 to 12
state variables of neurocognitive dysfunction in
years old. Science, 333, 959964.
schizophrenia: A twin study. Schizophrenia Bulletin,
Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Miezin, F. M., Cohen,
26, 459477.
A. L., Wenger, K. K., Dosenbach, R. A. T., et al.
Posner, M. I. (2012). Attention in a social world.
(2007). Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
task control in humans. Proceedings of the National
Posner, M. I., Early, T. S., Reiman, E., Pardo, P. J., &
Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 1107311078.
Dhawan, M. (1988). Asymmetries in hemispheric con-
Fair, D., Cohen, A. L., Power, J. D., Dosenbach, N. U. F.,
trol of attention in schizophrenia. Archives of General
Church, J. A., Meizin, F. M., et al. (2009). Functional
Psychiatry, 45, 814821.
brain networks develop from a local to distributed
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Educating the
organization. Public Library of Science, 5, 113.
human brain. Washington, DC: APA Books.
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, M., &
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Tang,
Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and inde-
Y. Y. (2007). The anterior cingulate gyrus and the
pendence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive
mechanisms of self-regulation. Cognitive, Affective, &
Neuroscience, 3, 340347.
Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 391395.
Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Brown, T. T., Kuperman,
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Voelker,
J. M., Chung, Y., Hagler, D. J., Jr., et al. (2012).
P. (2012). Control networks and neuromodulators of
Multimodal imaging of the self-regulating developing
early development. Developmental Psychology, 48,
brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of
827835.
Sciences USA, 109, 1962019625.
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Vizueta, N., Levy, K. N.,
Gao, W., Zhu, H., Giovanello, K. S., Smith, J. K., Shen,
Evans, D. E., Thomas, K. M., et al. (2002). Attentional
D., Gilmore, J. H., et al. (2009). Evidence on the emer-
mechanisms of the borderline personality disorder.
gence of the brains default network from 2-week-old
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects. Proceedings
the USA, 99, 1636616370.
of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106,
Raichle, M. E. (2009). A paradigm shift in functional
67906795.
imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1272912734.
Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of
Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Becoming who we are. New York,
the prefrontal cortex in addiction: Neuroimaging find-
NY: Guilford.
32 Y.-Y. Tang and M.I. Posner

Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., Rueda, M. R., & Posner, induces white-matter changes in the anterior cingulate.
M. I. (2011). Developing mechanisms of self- Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences of the
regulation in early life. Emotion Review, 3, 207213. USA, 107, 1664916652.
Rueda, M. R., Checa, P., & Combita, L. M. (2012). Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Fan, Y., Feng, H., Wang, J., Feng, S.,
Enhanced efficiency of the executive attention net- et al. (2009). Central and autonomic nervous system
work after training in preschool children: Immediate interaction is altered by short-term meditation.
and after two month effects. Developmental Cognitive Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences of the
Neuroscience, 2(Suppl 1), S192S204. USA, 106, 88658870.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., & Lu, Q.,
Saccamanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, et al. (2007). Short term meditation training improves
maturation and genetic influences on the development attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of National
of executive attention. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 104, 1715217156
Academy of Sciences of the USA, 102, 1493114936. Tang, Y. Y., & Posner, M. I. (2009). Attention training and
Silbersweig, D., Clarkin, J. F., Goldstein, M., Kernberg, attention state training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
O. F., Tuescher, O., Levy, K. N., et al. (2007). Failure of 13, 222227.
frontolimbic inhibitory function in the context of nega- Tang, Y. Y., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2012).
tive emotion in borderline personality disorder. The Neural correlates of establishing, maintaining and
American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 18321841. switching brain states. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., & Neville, H. J. (2009). 16, 330337.
Differences in neural mechanisms of selective atten- Tang, Y. Y., Yang, L., Leve, L., & Harold, G. (2012).
tion in children from different socioeconomic back- Improving executive function and its neurobiological
grounds: An event-related brain potential study. mechanisms through a mindfulness-based intervention:
Developmental Science, 12, 643646. Advances within the field of developmental neurosci-
Takeuchi, H., Sekiguchi, A., Taki, Y., Yokoyama, S., ence. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 361366.
Yomogida, Y., Komuro, N., et al. (2010). Training of van Veen, V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The timing of action-
working memory impacts structural connectivity. The monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex.
Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 32973303. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 593602.
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Volkow, N. D., & Fowler, J. S. (2000). Addiction, a dis-
Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. ease of compulsion and drive: Involvement of the orbi-
(2011). Working memory training using mental calcu- tofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 318325.
lation impacts regional gray matter of the frontal and Wang, K., Fan, J., Dong, Y., Wang, C., Lee, T. M. C., &
parietal regions. PLoS One, 6, e23175. Posner, M. I. (2005). Selective impairment of atten-
Tang, Y. Y., Lu, Q., Fan, M., Yang, Y., & Posner, M. I. tional networks of orienting and executive control in
(2012). Mechanisms of white matter changes induced schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 78, 235241.
by meditation. Proceedings of National Academy of Zeier, J. D., Maxwell, J. S., & Newman, J. P. (2009).
Sciences in the USA, 109, 1057010574. Attention moderates the processing of inhibitory
Tang, Y. Y., Lu, Q., Geng, X., Stein, E. A., Yang, Y., & information in primary psychopathy. Journal of
Posner, M. I. (2010). Short term mental training Abnormal Psychology, 118, 554563.
Mindfulness, Attention,
and Working Memory 4
Alexandra B. Morrison and Amishi P. Jha

Mindfulness is often described as attention to the Last, we introduce a broader set of hypothesized
present moment in a manner that is free from tools for cognitive enhancement and discuss the
embellishment or reactivity. Mindfulness can be uniqueness of mindfulness training among other
cultivated through a number of well-established types of cognitive training.
practices (e.g., Kabat-Zinn, 1994; Wallace,
1999), and a central feature of these practices
involves awareness and control of basic cognitive Attention and Working Memory
processes such as attention and working memory.
Accordingly, a sound understanding of mindful- At any given moment, our minds are offered a
ness requires introduction to the cognitive pro- large and varied array of information and several
cesses that support sustained attention to the potential trains of thought. Yet, the brain lacks
present moment. sufficient computational resources to fully pro-
The broad aims of this chapter are as follows. cess all that is happening. As such, cognitive pro-
First, we discuss the cognitive processesspe- cesses to select a subset of information for further
cifically, attention and working memorythat analyses are necessary to guide our moment-to-
guide our ability to select and maintain informa- moment experiences. Attention is the workhorse
tion in a given moment. Then, we discuss the for selection, and only attended information will
hypothesis that engaging in mindfulness exer- enter conscious awareness and assist in commit-
cises (both within a session and over longer peri- ment to a particular task. Failures of attention
ods of time) strengthens specific aspects of prompt mistakes like not noticing a stop sign
attention and working memory. Further, we while driving or missing a point someone else is
explore the proposal that because of bolstered making in a discussion while engaged in ones
attention and working memory, mindfulness own thoughts. Moreover, in many of our profes-
training may protect against the minds pervasive sional lives, attention is crucial. In the context of
tendencies to wander away from the present military personnel, for example, the need to
moment to task-unrelated thoughts and feelings. maintain attentive focus on all events transpiring
around ones current position (referred to as sit-
uational awareness) is critical to personal safety
and mission completion (Stanley & Jha, 2009).
A.B. Morrison A.P. Jha (*)
Attention, as described in cognitive neurosci-
Department of Psychology, University of Miami,
Miami, FL, USA ence, may be divided into two largely separable
e-mail: ajha@psy.miami.edu streams, the dorsal and the ventral. The dorsal

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 33


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_4
34 A.B. Morrison and A.P. Jha

stream (localized bilaterally in dorsal frontopari- stricter working memory capacity of four plus or
etal brain regions) is responsible for the top-down minus one item (see Cowan, 2001). In a demon-
voluntary control of attention and, thus, responds stration of the upper bound of this system, Luck
to environmental cues in a deliberate manner and Vogel (1997) presented participants with
(Corbetta & Shulman, 2002). Meanwhile, the arrays of colored squares ranging from 1 to 12
ventral stream (localized in right-lateralized ven- squares. Following a short delay, participants
tral frontoparietal brain regions) is responsible were shown another array and asked whether this
for bottom-up stimulus-driven attention and, array was the same or different than the first.
thus, is cued by less frequent and less expected While arrays of one to three squares produced
events than dorsal attention (Corbetta & Shulman, near-perfect accuracy, there was a decline in per-
2002). For example, while conversing with a formance for arrays of four squares, and an even
friend over a cup of coffee, listening to his or her larger decrement for arrays of five or more
voice would draw dorsal attention while someone squares. This discontinuity in performance for
creating a commotion by spilling coffee behind arrays exceeding four squares is interpreted as a
you would draw ventral attention. marker of a roughly four-item capacity limit.
In a theoretical model of the structure of atten- Very similar conclusions were made using the
tion, Posner and Petersen (1990) further divide same procedure and more complex stimuli (e.g.,
dorsal (voluntary) attention into the orienting and configurations of colored bars), and examina-
conflict monitoring subsystems, while ventral tions of verbal items also suggest a capacity limit
(stimulus driven) attention is encompassed by an of around four items (Cowan, 2001).
alerting subsystem. Specifically, orienting is the The size of working memory capacity differs
control of attention towards only certain inputs, between individuals and is predictive of prowess
while conflict monitoring suggests selection in measures of intelligence, reading comprehen-
between inputs that are competing in nature. sion, language acquisition, etc. (e.g., Baddeley,
Lastly, alerting is a state of preparedness that is 2003; Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Kane et al.,
onset by an external cue. 2004). Working memory is required during com-
As a core component of human cognition, plex mental operations involving selection, inte-
attention relates to and intersects with other cog- gration, and updating of information, and a low
nitive constructs like working memory (see Jha, working memory capacity corresponds to diffi-
2002). Evidence from functional magnetic reso- culty in complex and important situations in our
nance imaging (fMRI) and event-related poten- daily lives from an academic exam, to a job inter-
tial (ERP) studies suggests that working memory view, to an interpersonal altercation.
is akin to dorsal attention over time (Corbetta, In addition to differences between individuals,
Kincade, & Shulman, 2002; Jha, 2002). A quint- core cognitive capacities have been shown to dif-
essential illustration of working memory fer within an individual across situations (see
describes it as the minds workspace, where Ilkowska & Engle, 2010). A series of experi-
information is stored and processed towards any ments by Schmeichel (2007) tested the prediction
activity the mind is engaged in at present. that executive control resources (like attention
According to one theoretical account, working and working memory) are depletable and hin-
memory is dependent upon two separable cogni- dered by repeated use. Participants completed
tive processes, a controlled search through either a high-attention task (viewing a video of an
attended information and the retrieval of task- interview while ignoring text on a screen) or a
related items from longer term memory stores low-attention task (viewing the video with no
(Unsworth & Engle, 2007). instruction to ignore or remember the words on
It is well established that the capacity of work- the screen). After viewing, all participants com-
ing memory is limited. In a seminal paper, Miller pleted one of the two measures of working mem-
(1956) proposed a limit of seven plus or minus ory capacity (operation span or sentence span)
two items. More recent evidence suggests a (Conway et al., 2005; Unsworth, Heitz, Schrock,
4 Mindfulness, Attention, and Working Memory 35

& Engle, 2005). Performance on both working throughout the day. In order to gage the frequency
memory measures was worse following the high- of mind wandering, participants were asked: Are
attention task than the low-attention task. you thinking about something other than what you
Following exploration of factors such as mood are currently doing? (Killingsworth & Gilbert,
and working speed, the authors attributed the dis- 2010); 46.9 % of the time, participants reported
ruption of working memory in the second task to that their minds were wandering, in response to
the use of attentional resources in the first task. this question. In addition, individuals were less
Although this decline in performance is tempo- happy when they experienced mind wandering
rary, the length and timing of the experiment sug- episodes than at other times, and even mind wan-
gest that this deficit can persist for at least a few dering in the context of pleasant thoughts was not
minutes. accompanied by higher happiness ratings than
Working memory may also be degraded by when the mind was not wandering.
conditions of stress and negative emotional expe- In addition to a decrease in mood, mind wan-
riences (see Ilkowska & Engle, 2010). Schmeichel dering also results in poor task performance (e.g.,
(2007) showed participants video clips of an eye Franklin, Smallwood, & Schooler, 2011; Kane
surgery and of children describing upsetting et al., 2007). Schooler et al. (2011) highlight two
problems at home. One group was instructed to separate aspects of attentional fluctuations that
exaggerate their negative emotions while the accompany mind wandering. First, mind wander-
other group was instructed to simply watch the ing is characterized as a decoupling between per-
clips. After viewing, all participants completed ceptual input (or information from ones
the operation span task. Individuals asked to immediate environment) and the contents of
exaggerate their emotional responses performed attention (on something other than ones immedi-
worse on the subsequent measure of working ate environment). Second, mind wandering is
memory capacity than did the group asked only accompanied by fluctuations in the awareness of
to watch the clip. Notably, while those asked to the contents of the mind (i.e., meta-awareness),
exaggerate showed more outward emotion and and mind wandering can occur with and without
reported greater task difficulty than the other the knowledge that the mind has wandered. One
group, the groups did not differ in reported emo- study of self-reported mind wandering while
tional state. It follows that working memory per- reading a Sherlock Holmes novella found that
formance was impaired not by emotional state some participants were only intermittently aware
per se, but by task instructions that required more of whether their mind was on or off task. When
cognitive engagement relative to watching and mind wandering was divided into incidents with
naturally responding to emotionally evocative and without awareness, only mind wandering
material. In sum, repeated cognitive demandin without awareness was predictive of poorer read-
both emotionally neutral and evocative scenar- ing comprehension (Smallwood, McSpadden, &
iosis detrimental to future performance on Schooler, 2008). In sum, mind wandering is char-
tasks demanding attention and working memory. acterized by a failure to attend to task-relevant
perceptual input and, sometimes, a lack of atten-
tion to whether the mind is on or off task.
Mind Wandering In addition to a relationship between mind
wandering and attention, a growing amount of
In addition to their depletable and fragile nature, empirical evidence suggests a relationship
attention and working memory are susceptible to between ones working memory capacity and the
hijacking by competing environmental stimuli occurrence of mind wandering. During attention-
and internally generated distractions. Accordingly, ally demanding tasks, those with higher working
the mind wanders off task quite frequently. In one memory capacity report less mind wandering
large-scale study, 2,250 individuals were con- than lower capacity individuals, and this may be
tacted via phone at unpredictable intervals tied to the ability to keep task goals and
36 A.B. Morrison and A.P. Jha

task-related information in mind (Kane et al., An example of a prototypical mindfulness


2007; McVay & Kane, 2009; McVay, Kane, & activity is the direction of attention to the breath.
Kwapil, 2009). Even so, it has recently been In a mindful breathing exercise, individuals are
hypothesized that those with higher working instructed to sit in a comfortable upright posture,
memory capacity do not always wander less than select a particular sensation associated with
those with lower working memory capacity breathing, and then maintain that selected focus
(Levinson, Smallwood, & Davidson, 2012). for the duration of the practice. Instructors note
Instead, those with a high amount of working that attention will likely wander to other trains of
memory resources may be able to either inhibit thought or to external stimuli like nearby noises,
wandering during a demanding task or allow for but upon acknowledgement of these exercise-
mind wandering in a less demanding one. This unrelated thoughts, attention should be returned to
hypothesis is supported by evidence that in a sim- the breath. Thus, this and many other exercises
ple visual search task, where one is asked to press that emphasize concentrative focus require selec-
a key when he or she sees an X, those with higher tion and maintenance of information and monitor-
working memory capacity wandered more than ing of mind wandering throughout the period of
those with less capacity (rather than mind wan- practice. As such, we would expect that engaging
dering less, as they typically do in tasks highly in these practices in an ongoing fashion would not
demanding of attention). Accordingly, rather than only engage attention, working memory, and mon-
considering mind wandering a failure of con- itoring of mind wandering (as they are defined in
trolled attention, it may be a process that is either the cognitive neuroscience literature), but may
restricted or permitted according to the demands also strengthen these processes. If so, time spent
of the moment. cultivating mindfulness may be considered time
spent honing attention, working memory, and
monitoring of task-unrelated thoughts.
Mindfulness Training Mindfulness training can be understood
through the specific neural and cognitive processes
Our reliance on attention and working memory to mindfulness practices engage, and researchers
complete daily tasks and the limitations of these interested in the human mind can gain knowledge
mental abilities evoke the following question: of the extent to which essential cognitive pro-
Can we optimize the use or expand the size of cesses can be targeted and altered through deliber-
attention and working memory capacity? A ate practice. At present, the ability of cognitive
quickly developing literature tests whether train- neuroscientists to quantify mindfulness practice in
ing produces measurable benefits to cognitive cognitive and neuroscientific terms is quite lim-
performance, and while some studies demon- ited. Key issues remain, including investigating
strate gains on multiple measures (e.g., Green & the specific processes that are engaged or strength-
Bavelier, 2003; Jaeggi et al., 2010; Klingberg ened during mindfulness exercises, and the spe-
et al., 2005; Tang et al., 2007), others show cific neural and behavioral indices responsive to
changes in performance that are very limited in mindfulness training. Below, we examine mind-
scope (e.g., Chooi & Thompson, 2012; Ericsson fulness training as a vehicle for the study of atten-
& Chase, 1982; Lee et al., 2012; Redick et al., tion, working memory, and mind wandering.
2013). This mixed literature provokes extended
discourse over whether cognitive abilities like
attention and working memory are immutable or Attention
plastic (see Boot, Blakely, & Simons, 2011, and
Shipstead, Hicks, & Engle, 2012, and associated There are marked parallels between mindfulness
commentaries). Here, we detail research investi- texts illustrating mental modes and cognitive
gating mindfulness training as a hypothesized neuroscience texts examining attention. The
tool for training attention and working memory. mindfulness literature suggests that mindfulness
4 Mindfulness, Attention, and Working Memory 37

exercises target concentrative attention, where measures of attention do suggest gains in attention
attention is narrowed to a specific target like the through mindfulness practice (see Grossman,
breath, and receptive attention (also referred to as Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004). However,
open monitoring, Lutz et al., 2008), where atten- the following empirical question remains: Do the
tion more broadly encompasses the present attentional constructs identified by cognitive
moment, tracing the contents of what arises and researchers and mindfulness scholars indeed
passes in consciousness without targeted selec- overlap and, if so, how completely do they
tion of any particular aspect it (Brown, 1977; overlap?
Delmonte, 1987; Valentine & Sweet, 1999). In a Jha and others (2007) examined attentional
merging of parallel viewpoints (one based on the abilities in individuals without prior mindfulness
mindfulness training literature and contemplative experience and in experienced mindfulness prac-
practice and another based on the cognitive neu- titioners (Table 4.1). Attentional performance
roscience literature on the topic of attention), it was assessed prior to and following the training
has been hypothesized that the dorsal (or volun- using the attentional network task, which indexes
tary) attention system is targeted in concentrative orienting, conflict monitoring, and alerting (Fan,
attention, while the ventral (or stimulus driven) McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002).
system is targeted in receptive attention (Jha, Novices in this study engaged in an 8-week
Krompinger, & Baime, 2007). mindfulness training course emphasizing con-
Many traditions propose a hierarchy where centrative attention and following a prescription
prowess in concentrative attention should be similar to that described by Kabat-Zinn (1994).
established prior to honing of receptive attention. The course included a weekly 3-hour session
One account of this hierarchy explains that recep- composed of meditation, group discussion, and
tive attention requires that consciousness remain interactive mindfulness activity, and included
anchored in the present moment. In novice prac- 30 min of daily sitting practice assigned as home-
titioners or those without strong concentrative work. Experienced practitioners in the study self-
attention, the mind will likely wander away from selected to attend a 1-month residential retreat.
the present moment towards unrelated thoughts, This retreat included sitting and walking medita-
emotions, or images, making instructions to keep tions, one-on-one interviews with instructors,
attention in a receptive open state hard to follow. and 1012 h of daily formal mindfulness medita-
However, if and when one can hold attention in tion, and the cultivation of receptive attention
the present, one can then practice receptivity to was also emphasized. Both mindfulness groups
aspects of experience such as the sound of foot- were compared to a group of participants with no
steps or the emotional state that accompanies mindfulness experience and no training.
noticing this sound. According to the predictions above, improve-
The compatibility of definitions in the mind- ments in dorsal attention in those engaging in
fulness and attention literatures suggests that mindfulness training should precede strengthen-
mindfulness training should target the control of ing of involuntary attention. In line with the first
attention, and self-reported and introspective half of this prediction, at the initial cognitive

Table 4.1 Summary of Jha et al.s (2007) study of subsystems of attention and mindfulness training
System of attention Neural correlates Training emphasis (population) Results from Jha et al., 2007
Alerting Ventral frontoparietal Receptive attention After training: experienced >
network (experienced practitioners) controls and novice
Orienting Dorsal frontoparietal network Concentrative attention After training: novice >
(novice practitioners) controls and experienced
Conflict monitoring Dorsal frontoparietal network Concentrative attention Before training: Experienced
(novice practitioners) > controls and novice
38 A.B. Morrison and A.P. Jha

assessment, the retreat group of experienced gains in attention that are fragile and not easily
practitioners showed higher conflict monitoring documented by the tasks experimenters chose to
performance than the groups without mindful- administer before and after training.
ness experience. This finding is consistent with Current findings showing gains in measures of
Van den Hurk and others (2010) demonstration attention (e.g., Lutz et al., 2008) promote a level
that experienced mindfulness meditation practi- of optimism regarding mindfulness training as an
tioners exhibited better executive attention per- intervention for attention. Yet, consideration of
formance when compared to those without multiple studies highlights the need for continued
mindfulness experience. research into the precise mechanism of mindful-
Examination of performance after the training ness training, the most optimal construction of
period revealed that participants in the novice mindfulness training, and the appropriateness of
group showed stronger orienting performance mindfulness training for different populations.
after training when compared to the retreat group
and control group, but there was no difference in
conflict monitoring. Therefore, in the case of the Working Memory
dorsal subsystems, experienced practitioners
entered the study with greater conflict monitor- As working memory is a set of processes that
ing, and orienting was stronger following train- support attention over time, one might expect
ing among novice mindfulness practitioners than some commonality between working memory
the other two groups. Turning towards the invol- and the mental mode of mindfulness. When one
untary (ventral) attention system, following the engages in a mindful breathing exercise, for
retreat, alerting performance was greater in the example, working memory processes should sup-
retreat group (or experienced practitioners) than port attention on the breath over time and buffer
the other two groups. These findings provide ini- against unrelated thoughts or external stimuli.
tial support for the hypothesis that engagement in Additionally, should task goals be lost from con-
mindfulness training is accompanied by enhanced scious awareness, the retrieval component of
performance on measures of attention. Moreover, working memory could allow for a refreshing of
they advance the specific hypothesis that input task goals and a return of attention to the sensa-
level dorsal attention is targeted in early mindful- tions of the breath.
ness training while ventral attention requires Over time, a mindfulness practice might
higher levels of practice. strengthen performance under high working
It should be noted that while a growing list of memory load. This strengthening of working
studies demonstrate the attentional benefits of memory processes could occur in at least two dis-
mindfulness training (see Lutz, Slagter, Dunne, & tinct ways. First, mindfulness training might
Davidson, 2008; for a review, see Hlzel et al., limit elaboration or reactivity to information in
2011), the literature contains failure to find mea- working memory, allowing for more task-rele-
surable attentional benefits of mindfulness train- vant information to be sustained instead. Put dif-
ing as well (e.g., Anderson, Lau, Segal, & Bishop, ferently, mindfulness might allow one to use
2007). Such null findings may suggest that working memory more efficiently by limiting
mindfulness training does not produce measur- irrelevant information in working memory.
able attention enhancement as operationalized in Alternatively, mindfulness training might expand
the cognitive neuroscience literature. Alternatively, working memory capacity by allowing for
null results may indicate that the relevant mind- accommodation of a larger amount of informa-
fulness training programs were not optimal to tion. A small number of studies examine working
elicit attentional improvements or that the experi- memory capacity as an outcome variable of
ence and characteristics of the individuals were mindfulness training studies. Chambers, Lo, and
not well matched to the training administered. As Allen (2008) tested novice practitioners before
a last possibility, null findings may reflect real and after a 10-day mindfulness retreat and found
4 Mindfulness, Attention, and Working Memory 39

improvements in a measure of working memory and matched several features of a Mindfulness-


(backward digit span) that were larger than Based Stress Reduction course (Kabat-Zinn,
improvements in those who did not participate in 1994). The mindfulness training group was com-
the training. Van Vugt and Jha (2011) found that pared to pre-deployment military control and
following mindfulness training, participants civilian control groups.
reaction times were faster and less variable when The operation span task was administered to all
compared to controls. Mathematical modeling groups before and after the training interim. While
tied training-related gains to factors like a civilian controls showed stable performance over
decrease in encoding noise and a reduction of time, military controls showed degradation in their
response conservativeness. Also, Mrazek, performance. This decrement in performance sup-
Franklin, Philips, Baird, and Schooler (2013) ports claims that stress is accompanied by degraded
found an increase in an operation span task that cognitive capacity. The training group was split in
was significantly larger in those completing half into those who reported higher levels of prac-
mindfulness training than those completing nutri- tice during the intervention (an average of 634 min)
tion training. and those who reported lower levels of practice (an
In a multifaceted study, Jha, Stanley, average of 151 min). While the low-practice group
Kiyonaga, Wong, and Gelfand (2010) examined showed degradation in the operation span task
a group of US Marines and the impact of deploy- (like the military controls), the high-practice group
ment preparation on their working memory showed modest improvements that trended
capacity and emotional state. The study asked towards significance. Moreover, within the mind-
two questions about mindfulness training. fulness training group, there was a positive corre-
1) Does mindfulness training protect against lation between amount of mindfulness practice
stress-related degradation in cognitive perfor- and operation span performance. Participants also
mance, and, 2) does protection of cognitive completed a self-report mood measurethe
resources correspond with changes in affective Positive and Negative Affect Scales (Watson,
experience? The study was conducted over a pre- Clark, & Tellegen, 1988)both prior to and fol-
deployment intervali.e., the months prior to lowing the training period. Results revealed that
deployment where military service members there was a direct relationship between practice
undergo intense physical training and training in time and positive affect but that levels of negative
mission-critical skills. In addition to formal train- affect depended on working memory performance.
ing, such individuals must prepare to leave their In sum, this paper suggests protective effects of
loved ones and enter a situation that is unpredict- mindfulness training on working memory capacity
able, stressful, and potentially dangerous. While in a high-stress group of participants and affective
preparations during the pre-deployment interval benefits as well.
are meant to increase mission readiness, the pre- Together, results from the studies above
deployment interval can be marked by decreases (Chambers et al., 2008; Jha et al., 2010; Mrazek
in cognitive functioning and increases in emo- et al., 2013; Van Vugt & Jha, 2011) posit a salu-
tional disturbance (Bolton, Litz, Britt, Adler, & tary relationship between working memory and
Roemer, 2001; Maguen et al., 2008). mindfulness practice. These findings may be
In order to determine the utility of mindful- attributed to the size of working memory capac-
ness training as a tool to protect against cognitive ity or the efficiency with which working memory
and emotional degradation, a group of marines capacity is used (see Van Vugt & Jha, 2011).
completed a mindfulness course named While these possibilities are difficult to disen-
Mindfulness-Based Mind Fitness Training, tangle, future work should consider whether
which was designed and instructed by a former training impacts the total amount of information
US Army officer with mindfulness expertise. in working memory, the ratio of relevant to irrel-
Training was 8 weeks in duration, emphasized evant information, or both. Studies have begun to
mindfulness skills and their military application, gain traction on this question by administering
40 A.B. Morrison and A.P. Jha

tasks that measure capacity for task-relevant wandering than in the nutrition training group.
information but also ask participants about task- This pattern was consistent for performance in
unrelated thoughts (i.e., mind wandering) both cognitive tasks and in all three mind wan-
(Mrazek et al., 2013). dering metrics, and it provides strong evidence
that mindfulness training is of value to cognitive
performance, even when compared to an active
Mind Wandering control group taught by an expert in that field.

It has been suggested that mind wandering (inat-


tention to the task at hand) and mindfulness Fluctuations in Cognitive Processes
(attention to the present moment) are opposing During Mindfulness Practice
states of mind (Mrazek, Smallwood, & Schooler,
2012). In support of this point, a negative correla- The work described above suggests both an inter-
tion has been shown between trait mindfulness dependence of attention, working memory, and
(measured through the Mindful Awareness mind wandering and a relationship between these
Attention Scale) and four separate measures of processes and mindfulness training. Building
mind wandering (including both self-report met- upon the establishment of these initial relation-
rics and objective measures of performance in the ships, one can describe mindfulness training as
Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART): resulting in a shift in cognitive processes in order
Mrazek et al., 2012). Additionally, an 8-min to sustain or regain attention to the present
mindful breathing exercise was shown to reduce moment. A recent model describes mindfulness
indices of mind wandering (errors and reaction practice as a series of such brief cognitive states
time variability in the SART) when compared to (Hasenkamp, Wilson-Mendenhall, Duncan, &
passive relaxation and reading a newspaper. Barsalou, 2012). For example, during a mindful
More extensive mindfulness practice has also breathing exercise, attention is focused on the
been related to the occurrence of mind wander- breath (focus), the mind begins to wander (mind
ing. In a relevant study (Mrazek et al., 2013), par- wandering), one becomes aware of the off task
ticipants engaged in a 2-week mindfulness course thoughts (awareness), attention shifts back
and were compared to participants in a nutrition towards the breath (shifting), and attention is
course. Each course met for four 45-min sessions again sustained on the breath. This cycling of
per week, and the mindfulness course empha- cognitive states is thought to occur frequently as
sized focused-attention meditation with both one engages and disengages from a mindful state
physical and mental components. Before and of attention to the breath. Further, these cognitive
after training, participants completed a measure states are hypothesized to be accompanied by
of standardized test performance (subsets of the distinct patterns of brain activation.
verbal Graduate Record Exam: GRE) and a mea- According to the neural predictions of this
sure of working memory capacity (the operation model, focus, shifting, and awareness are accom-
span task). During the GRE, mind wandering panied by activity in regions associated with
was measured by asking participants at attention to a task, including the lateral prefrontal
unpredictable intervals whether their thoughts cortex, premotor cortex, lateral parietal regions,
were on or off task and also by asking partici- occipital regions, anterior cingulate cortex, and
pants to report if they noticed their mind wander- insula (Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005). In con-
ing. During the operation span task, mind trast, episodes of mind wandering should be
wandering was measured through a retrospective associated with activity in the default mode net-
report collected after completion of the working work, a group of brain regions that reveal higher
memory task. Results revealed that mindfulness signal during periods of rest than periods of task
training led to larger performance gains in the performance (Raichle et al., 2001). The default
cognitive measures and larger reductions in mind mode network includes regions of the dorsal and
4 Mindfulness, Attention, and Working Memory 41

ventral medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingu- optimistic but cautious in their conclusions due to
late cortex, posterior inferior parietal regions, lat- uncertainty around the precise timing of fluctua-
eral temporal regions, and parahippocampus tions between cognitive states and uncertainty
(Buckner, AndrewsHanna, & Schacter, 2008). over whether there are distinctly serial (rather
Activities in the attentional network and default than temporally overlapping) processes. Yet, this
mode network are shown to fluctuate in opposite work creates an intriguing stepping stone for the
fashion (Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005). description of mindfulness practice as a set of
In order to test the predictions of this model of putative cognitive and neural processes. Further
mindfulness, experienced mindfulness practitio- mindfulness research should continue to charac-
ners engaged in a 20-min mindful breathing exer- terize the neural and cognitive processes that
cise during an fMRI scan (Hasenkamp et al., support mindfulness practice and how these pro-
2012). Participants were instructed to note if their cesses differ between individuals and change
minds wandered from attention to the breath, over time.
indicate this wandering by a button press, and
then return attention back to the breath. The
authors constructed a hypothesized timeline of Mindfulness Training as a Unique
the cycling of cognitive states surrounding the Tool Targeting Cognitive
button press. Awareness was defined as the 3 s Enhancement
surrounding the button press (specifically, 2 s
prior to the button press and one after), mind The notion of cognitive enhancement garners
wandering was defined as the 3 s prior to aware- enthusiasm in both the popular press (Hurley,
ness, shifting attention was defined as the 3 s 2012) and scientific literature (Morrison & Chein,
after awareness, and focusing of attention was 2011; Slagter, Davidson, & Lutz, 2011). This
defined as a 3-s period after shifting. enthusiasm originates from a desire to overcome
Broadly, comparisons targeting each of these the inherent limitations of the human mind, the
four cognitive states revealed that while the detrimental impact of a variety factors (e.g.,
awareness, shifting attention, and focusing atten- affect, stress, age, disease) on cognition, and the
tion phases were accompanied by activity in task- failures of some techniques to increase intellectual
positive attentional networks, the mind wandering abilities. The literature contains several
phase was accompanied by activity in the default approaches to cognitive enhancement including
mode network. More specifically, the shifting skills training (Lee, Lu, & Ko, 2007), videogame
attention and focusing attention phases were training (Green & Bavelier, 2003), and neuro-
associated with subsystems of the larger atten- feedback training (Keizer, Verment, & Hommel,
tional network. When compared to the mind 2010), among others. In this final section, we
wandering period, awareness was associated with briefly detail how lessons from other training
robust activity in networks associated with efforts can be applied to the development of
salience (e.g., anterior insula and dorsal anterior mindfulness training and discuss what makes
cingulate cortex), shifting was associated with mindfulness training a distinct form of training.
areas implicated in executive attention (e.g., dor- One type of cognitive training that attracts
solateral prefrontal cortex and lateral inferior attention and discourse is computerized working
parietal cortex), and focus was accompanied by memory training (see Klingberg, 2010; Shipstead,
activity in regions associated with working mem- Redick, & Engle, 2012). Here, training requires
ory and maintenance of information over time extensive practice with computerized tasks that
(e.g., the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex). tax working memory capacity. It is hypothesized
Together, Hasenkamp and others (2012) find- that this manner of targeted training may expand
ings provide a neural and cognitive framework working memory capacity and thereby benefit
for the cycling of mental processes that occurs related cognitive constructs like cognitive control
during a mindfulness practice. The authors are and general fluid intelligence. There are now
42 A.B. Morrison and A.P. Jha

several studies in which computerized working creates a rich, meaningful, and motivating
memory training benefitted performance on experience and it is also true that mindfulness
untrained measures of attention and working training has a well-established relationship with
memory (e.g., Morrison & Chein, 2011; Klingberg aspects of well-being like mood, perceived stress,
et al., 2005; Schmiedek, Lvdn, & Lindenberger, social support, and treating of disorder symptom-
2010). In addition, some studies have shown gen- atology (see Kabat-Zinn, 1994; Shapiro, Oman,
eralized benefits to measures of fluid intelligence Thoresen, Plante, & Flinders, 2008). In this chap-
(Jaeggi et al., 2010; Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, ter, we asked whether mindfulness training leads
& Perrig, 2008; Klingberg et al., 2005). In con- to measurable improvements in attention and
trast, there are instances where training begets working memory and find that it often does.
large improvements in the practiced task but does From a standpoint of science, though, it is impor-
not lead to improvements on other tasks that are tant to determine whether increases in perfor-
not part of the training battery (Chooi & mance on cognitive assessments stem from the
Thompson, 2012; Redick et al., 2013). development of the intended cognitive skills,
There have been critiques regarding the meth- from enhanced well-being, or from some combi-
odology and conclusions of cognitive training nation of cognitive and affective elements. While
studies (Morrison & Chein, 2011; Shipstead, all types of cognitive training (e.g., computerized
Hicks & Engle, 2012; Shipstead, Redick & training, videogame training) may promote some
Engle, 2012), and these critiques can be lever- level of increase in motivation and perhaps social
aged to inform mindfulness training endeavors. support (e.g., from the experimenter), these types
One particularly nuanced notion is that improve- of benefits are especially relevant to mindfulness
ment on a singular cognitive task is not sufficient training due to the presence of instructors and co-
to claim (and should not be confounded with) participants in the training program.
broad enhancement of a cognitive construct. True In one recent paper, Slagter et al. (2011) out-
enhancement of a cognitive ability should be line particular aspects of mindfulness training
accompanied by improved performance in a that can lead to process-specific learning (or
range of untrained measures taxing this ability. training gains that generalize outside of the
Moreover, it is crucial to adjudicate between per- context of training). One of these factors is stimu-
formance changes that stem from changes in lus and task variability in that mindfulness prac-
capacity and changes attributed to strategy use, tice can have considerable variability from day to
motivation, or effects of repeated testing. day due to the ever-changing state of the mind
Currently, the gold standard methodology for and environment. Therefore, each day one prac-
studying cognitive training is the use of an active tices, the present moment will be characterized
control group tightly matched on many variables by different moods, sounds, and temperatures,
(e.g., motivation, repeated testing), but whose and this type of variability may promote transfer
training is composed of an activity not expected to novel contexts. Another aspect of mindfulness
to produce generalized gains. Put simply, demon- training highlighted by Slagter and others is
stration of true cognitive enhancement is no easy arousal. Specifically, they suggest that mindful-
task and several levels of inquiry may be required ness training exercises require awareness of ones
for bold conclusions that training A expands abil- level of arousal or drowsiness, and a mindfulness
ity B for reason C. practice may promote regulation of attention and
If several types of training are available, then emotion in an effort to maintain a balance
one might ask what makes mindfulness training between too great or too little arousal.
distinct. First, it should be restated that mindful- Lastly, we suggest the importance of the qual-
ness training is accompanied by centuries of ity of awareness of the contents of the mind to
knowledge, tradition, and a growing community mindfulness training. While completion of diffi-
of practitioners and instructors (see Wallace, cult cognitive tasks may produce some awareness
2006). From the standpoint of a practitioner, this of task difficulty and on or off task thinking,
4 Mindfulness, Attention, and Working Memory 43

awareness of cognitive states is not a central Buckner, R. L., AndrewsHanna, J. R.,., & Schacter, D. L.
(2008). The brains default network. Annals of the
component of such tasks. In contrast, a key aspect
New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 138.
of mindfulness training is meta-awareness, or Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. (2008). The
awareness of the present content of conscious- impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional
ness (see Schooler et al., 2011). The building of control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy
and Research, 32, 303322.
meta-awareness is one hypothesized mechanism
Chooi, W. T., & Thompson, L. A. (2012). Working mem-
for how mindfulness practice might improve cog- ory training does not improve intelligence in healthy
nitive performance outside of practice. For exam- young adults. Intelligence, 40, 531542.
ple, during mindfulness practice, awareness is Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick,
D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working
crucial to holding attention in the present and to
memory span tasks: A methodological review and users
returning to a mindful state following off task guide. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 769786.
thoughts. This quality of awareness seems likely Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002).
to generalize beyond mindfulness practice to Neural systems for visual orienting and their relation-
ships to spatial working memory. Journal of Cognitive
other daily responsibilities and activities.
Neuroscience, 14, 508523.
In sum, a growing body of work suggests rela- Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-
tionships between attention, working memory, and directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain.
mind wandering, and between these cognitive pro- Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 215229.
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term
cesses and those involved with mindfulness train-
memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity.
ing. Recent studies demonstrate enhancement (Jha Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87114. discus-
et al., 2007; Mrazek et al., 2013) or protection (Jha sion 114-185.
et al., 2010) of performance in measures of cogni- Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual dif-
ferences in working memory and reading. Journal of
tion following mindfulness training. It is advised
Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 19, 450466.
to note that the bar for suggesting stable increases Delmonte, M. (1987). Meditation: Contemporary theo-
in core cognitive constructs is set quite high, and retical approaches. In M. A. West (Ed.), The psychol-
the literature is continually growing with regard to ogy of meditation (pp. 3958). New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
both the measurement and cultivation of mindful-
Ericsson, K. A., & Chase, W. G. (1982). Exceptional
ness. Accordingly, the study of mindfulness pres- memory. American Scientist, 70, 607615.
ents a rich set of challenges and potentials to Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, M., &
practitioners and researchers alike and simultane- Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and inde-
pendence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive
ously informs current knowledge about core cog-
Neuroscience, 14, 340347.
nitive processes. Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M.,
Van Essen, D. C., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human
brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticor-
related functional networks. Proceedings of the
References National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 102, 96739678.
Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Bishop, S. R. Franklin, M. S., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2011).
(2007). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and atten- Catching the mind in flight: Using behavioral indices
tional control. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, to detect mindless reading in real time. Psychonomic
14(6), 449463. Bulletin & Review, 18, 992997.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An Fransson, P. (2005). Spontaneous lowfrequency BOLD
overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, signal fluctuations: An fMRI investigation of the rest-
189208. ingstate default mode of brain function hypothesis.
Bolton, E., Litz, B., Britt, T., Adler, A., & Roemer, L. Human Brain Mapping, 26, 1529.
(2001). Reports of prior exposure to potentially trau- Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video games
matic events and PTSD in troops poised for deploy- modify visual selective attention. Nature, 423,
ment. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14, 249256. 534537.
Boot, W. R., Blakely, D. P., & Simons, D. J. (2011). Do Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H.
action video games improve perception and cogni- (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health
tion? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 16. benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic
Brown, D. P. (1977). A model for the levels of concentra- Research, 57, 3543.
tive meditation. International Journal of Clinical & Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C., Duncan, E., &
Experimental Hypnosis, 25, 236273. Barsalou, L. (2012). Mind wandering and attention
44 A.B. Morrison and A.P. Jha

during focused meditation: A fine-grained temporal Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M.,
analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. NeuroImage, Gustafsson, P., Dahlstrm, K., et al. (2005).
59, 750760. Computerized training of working memory in children
Hlzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., with ADHDA randomized, controlled trial. Journal
Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action Psychiatry, 44, 177186.
from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives Lee, H., Boot, W., Basak, C., Voss, M., Prakash, R.,
on Psychological Science, 6, 537559. Neider, M., & Kramer, A.F. (2012). Performance
Hurley, D. (2012, April 18). Can you make yourself gains from directed training do not transfer to
smarter? New York Times Magazine, http://www. untrained tasks. Acta Psychologica, 139, 146158.
nytimes.com/2012/04/22/magazine/can-you-make- Lee, Y. S., Lu, M. J., & Ko, H. P. (2007). Effects of skill
yourself-smarter.html. training on working memory capacity. Learning and
Ilkowska, M., & Engle, R. W. (2010). Trait and state dif- Instruction, 17, 336344.
ferences in working memory capacity. In A. Gruszka, Levinson, D. B., Smallwood, J., & Davidson, R. J. (2012).
G. Matthews, & B. Szymura (Eds.), Handbook of The persistence of thought evidence for a role of work-
Individual Differences in Cognition. Attention, ing memory in the maintenance of task-unrelated
Memory, and Executive Control (pp. 295-320). thinking. Psychological Science, 23, 375380.
New York, NY: Springer. Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. working memory for features and conjunctions.
(2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on Nature, 390, 279281.
working memory. Proceedings of the National Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J.
Academy of Sciences, 105, 68296833. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in medi-
Jaeggi, S. M., Studer, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y.-F., Jonides, tation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163169.
J., & Perrig, W. J. (2010). On the relationship between Maguen, S., Turcotte, D. M., Peterson, A. L., Dremsa,
n-back performance and matrix reasoningimplications T. L., Garb, H. N., McNally, R. J., & Litz, B. T. (2008).
for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38, 625635. Description of risk and resilience factors among mili-
Jha, A. P. (2002). Tracking the time-course of attentional tary medical personnel before deployment to Iraq.
involvement in spatial working memory: An event- Military Medicine, 173, 19.
related potential investigation. Cognitive Brain McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train
Research, 15, 6169. of thought: Working memory capacity, goal neglect,
Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). and mind wandering in an executive-control task.
Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of atten- Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
tion. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 196.
7, 109119. McVay, J. C., Kane, M. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2009).
Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Tracking the train of thought from the laboratory into
Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective effects everyday life: An experience-sampling study of mind
of mindfulness training on working memory capacity wandering across controlled and ecological contexts.
and affective experience. Emotion, 10, 5464. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 857863.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or
Mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York, minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing
NY: Hyperion. information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197.
Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Morrison, A., & Chein, J. (2011). Does working memory
Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom training work? The promise and challenges of enhanc-
the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling ing cognition by training working memory.
study of working memory and executive control in Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 4660.
daily life. Psychological Science, 18, 614621. Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B.,
Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training
O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The general- improves working memory capacity and GRE perfor-
ity of working memory capacity: A latent variable mance while reducing mind wandering. Psychological
approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and Science, 24, 776781.
reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012).
General, 133, 189217. Mindfulness and mind wandering: Finding conver-
Keizer, A. W., Verment, R. S., & Hommel, B. (2010). gence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12, 442.
Enhancing cognitive control through neurofeedback: Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention sys-
A role of gamma-band activity in managing episodic tem of the human brain. Annual Review of
retrieval. NeuroImage, 49, 34043413. Neuroscience, 13, 2542.
Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wander- Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers,
ing mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932. W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A
Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the
memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 317324. National Academy of Sciences, 98, 676682.
4 Mindfulness, Attention, and Working Memory 45

Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Stanley, E. A., & Jha, A. P. (2009). Mind fitness: Improving
Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., ... & Engle, R. W. operational effectiveness and building warrior resil-
(2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after ience. Joint Force Quarterly, 55, 144151.
working memory training: A randomized, placebo- Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q.,
controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: & Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term meditation training
General, 142(2), 359. improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of
Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attentional control, memory the National Academy of Sciences, 104,
updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce 1715217156.
the capacity for executive control. Journal of Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of indi-
Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 241255. vidual differences in working memory capacity:
Schmiedek, F., Lvdn, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Active maintenance in primary memory and con-
Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad trolled search from secondary memory. Psychological
cognitive abilities in adulthood: Findings from the Review, 114, 104132.
COGITO study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2, Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W.
110. (2005). An automated version of the operation span
Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, task. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 498505.
T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A. (2011). Meta- Valentine, E. R., & Sweet, P. L. G. (1999). Meditation and
awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering attention: A comparison of the effects of concentrative
mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 319326. and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention.
Shapiro, S. L., Oman, D., Thoresen, C. E., Plante, T. G., & Mental Health, Religion, & Culture, 2, 5970.
Flinders, T. (2008). Cultivating mindfulness: Effects Van den Hurk, P. A., Giommi, F., Gielen, S. C., Speckens,
on wellbeing. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, A. E., & Barendregt, H. P. (2010). Greater efficiency
840862. in attentional processing related to mindfulness medi-
Shipstead, Z., Hicks, K. L., & Engle, R. W. (2012). tation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Cogmed working memory training: Does the evidence Psychology, 63, 11681180.
support the claims? Journal of Applied Research in Van Vugt, M., & Jha, A. P. (2011). Investigating the impact
Memory and Cognition, 1, 185193. of mindfulness meditation training on working mem-
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is ory: A mathematical modeling approach. Cognitive,
working memory training effective? Psychological Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 344353.
Bulletin, 138, 628654. Wallace, B. A. (1999). The Buddhist tradition of Samatha:
Slagter, H. A., Davidson, R. J., & Lutz, A. (2011). Mental Methods for refining and examining consciousness.
training as a tool in the neuroscientific study of brain Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 175187.
and cognitive plasticity. Frontiers in Human Wallace, B. A. (2006). The attention revolution: Unlocking
Neuroscience, 5, 112. the power of the focused mind. Boston, MA: Wisdom.
Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988).
When attention matters: The curious incident of the wan- Development and validation of brief measures of posi-
dering mind. Memory & Cognition, 36, 11441150. tive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070.
Taming the Wild Elephant:
Mindfulness and Its Role 5
in Overcoming Automatic Mental
Processes

Brian D. Ostan

Wild elephants are formidable creatures that can


wreak havoc on human communities. A recent The Frame Problem
account from Vietnam demonstrates the difficulty and the Problem of Framing
of controlling these animals, as bonfire barri-
cades and loud gongs failed to dissuade them Why does the human mind feel like an untamed
from destroying a villages orchards and vegeta- elephant? The approach taken here includes the
ble fields (Wild elephants raid fields, 2013). assumptions that reducing the informational
For anyone who has tried to change a behavior complexity of the world is a prerequisite to goal
such as overeating, smoking, or procrastination, pursuit and that a side effect of this reduction is
the habits of the mind can feel as powerful and inflexible responding to the environment2.
obstinately uncooperative as wild elephants. A wild To begin, the goal-oriented nature of humans
elephant metaphor of the mind is found in early can be understood as part of the strategy that we
Buddhist accounts (MN 125.23)1: use to obtain the necessities of life, such as food
Just asthe elephant tamer plants a large post in and shelter (Klinger, 1998). Unlike plants, which
the earth and binds the forest elephant to itin rely on the environment to deliver what they need
order to subdue his forest habitsand to inculcate to survive, animals, including humans, must seek
in him habits congenial to human beings, so these out the goods of life. These goods are represented
four foundations of mindfulness are the bindings
for the mind of the noble disciple in order to subdue as goals that are pursued by humans as well as
his habits. lower animals (Tolman, 1948).
An important obstacle to successful goal pur-
This chapter uses a cognitive science perspec- suit is the vast complexity of the world (Simon,
tive to review what is known about the minds 1972). This obstacle is demonstrated in the frame
unruly habits and how mindfulness may help to problem from artificial intelligence, which con-
counter them. sists of the difficulty of designing robots that can
both determine what information is relevant
(salient) for a task and ignore information that is
irrelevant (nonsalient) for the task. Dennett (1984)
1
MN is the standard abbreviation for the canonical illustrates the frame problem by describing the
Majjhima Nikya (Middle-length discourses) text. difficulties of designing a robot to use a wagon so
B.D. Ostafin (*)
Department of Psychology, University of Groningen,
2
Groningen, The Netherlands I follow Vervaekes (2011) argument regarding the
e-mail: b.d.ostafin@rug.nl necessity of frames and their costs.

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 47


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_5
48 B.D. Ostafin

that it can move a battery from one room to order to select advantageous moves. Instead,
another, in which the battery can be connected to they use heuristics such as devoting attention to
the robot. In addition to representing the intended moves that allow the opponent fewer options
consequences of its behavior (the battery will and to squares that can be influenced by many
come along when the wagon is pulled), the robot pieces (Reynolds, 1982; Simon & Simon, 1962).
must also be able to (a) discriminate between the Shortcuts such as walking to the fridge at mid-
few relevant unintended consequences of its night or attending to chess moves that constrain
action (e.g., pulling the wagon is a bad idea an opponent become habitual because they
because there is also a time-bomb on it) from the facilitate the acquisition of something good or
infinite number of irrelevant unintended conse- the elimination of something bad (Wood &
quences (e.g., the influence of pulling the wagon Neal, 2007). These experience-related frames
on air currents that contribute to weather patterns confer great adaptive advantage due to their
in South America) and (b) ignore the irrelevant speed and efficient use of limited cognitive
information so that it can act instead of being per- resources, both of which may contribute to the
petually frozen in a state of computational pro- finding that habitual behavior is associated with
cessing. Relatively complex goal pursuits such as less stress than nonhabitual behavior (Wood,
playing a game of chess have similar problems Quinn, & Kashy, 2002).
with immense quantities of information, as the It is an unfortunate fact that everything costs
number of possible chess games has been esti- something. The cost of learning-based frames
mated to be 10120 (Shannon, 1950). Given our becomes apparent when the environment
limited information processing capacity (Miller, changes. This is because frames tend to be con-
1956), we are unable to fully consider all possi- servative and are most beneficial if the environ-
ble behavioral alternatives and their conse- ment remains constant. For example, the salience
quences. Thus, we cannot play the perfect game of a midnight snack in the fridge may become
of chess or flawlessly pursue more complex goals problematic when the context changes from eat
involved in relationships, childrearing, and ones when hungry to going on a diet (Carels et al.,
career. Although it may be impossible to solve 2001). Frames also impede goal pursuit by block-
the frame problem in artificial intelligence ing the consideration of alternative options, some
(Dennett, 1984), it is clear that frames, which of which may be more efficient. For example,
demarcate the relevant from the irrelevant, are research has shown that experienced chess play-
necessary for goal pursuit. ers will select a familiar solution to achieve
Fortunately, there are a number of adapta- checkmate when a shorter but unfamiliar solution
tions that help to establish relevance. For exam- is available (Bilali, McLeod, & Gobet, 2008).
ple, innate parameters in our sensory systems These participants were only able to find the
constrain what is perceived and acted upon (von shorter solution when the board was set up so that
Uexkll, 1957). Importantly, frames are also the familiar solution was no longer an option,
derived from learning experiences (Rieskamp & indicating that the well-known solution impeded
Otto, 2006). Such learning creates mental short- access to the arguably better solution. Further,
cuts in determining what is relevant, thus allow- even though participants stated that they looked
ing decisions to be made without an exhaustive for shorter solutions after finding the familiar
search of information related to an action. For one, eye-tracking data showed that they contin-
example, previous learning means that a desire ued to primarily look at the squares relevant to
for a midnight snack does not elicit haphazard the familiar solution.
foraging through the house but instead prompts These examples show that despite intentions
a direct path toward the refrigerator. Another to the contrary, frames have a centripetal force
example is that chess players who can success- that can capture thinking and behavior.
fully plan three moves ahead need not demon- Unfortunately, the consequences are often more
strate super-human information processing in serious than a poorly played game of chess. The
5 Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes 49

problem of frames becomes apparent when con- The Habit System


sidering the difficulty of changing self-destructive
behaviors: System 1 (the habit system) is proposed to repre-
sent information in an associative network. This
Even though in the morning, you say, Im not network is the storage place for learning-based
going to drink, then you seem to hit like a blind
spot in your brain, where you go on automatic and
relevance (frames). From this perspective, the
youre going to have that drinkyoure not think- perception of a stimulus automatically activates
ing anymore, What about the kids? What about nodes in an associational network. The nodes can
the marriageYou just hit this blank spot and represent a number of things such as concepts
you go to the refrigerator, you open it, and you pull
out that bottle of wine (Moyers, 1998).
(ice creamcold), and affect (ice cream
hedonic pleasure). Additionally, the network
Treatment outcome research supports the idea includes behavioral schemata that encompass the
that alcohol and drug behavior is hard to change, contextual cues of the behavior, the behavior, and
with relapse rates of 60 % within 4 months post- consequences related to the behavior (dessert
treatment (Foster, Marshall, & Peters, 2000) and trayorder and eat desserthedonic pleasure;
70 % within the first year (Hunt, Barnett, & Strack & Deutsch, 2004). Although some asso-
Branch, 1971). Similarly bleak results are dem- ciations in System 1 are part of our genetic
onstrated by individuals making New Years res- makeup, such as loud noises eliciting an orient-
olutions, with approximately 60 % failing to ing response (Sokolov, 1963) or palatable foods
maintain their resolutions 3 months later eliciting appetitive behavior (Mennella &
(Norcross, Ratzin, & Payne, 1989). These find- Beauchamp, 1996), experience is proposed to
ings illustrate that habits of the mind can indeed play a central role in the development of associa-
act like unruly elephants. tive networks. Early behaviorist research demon-
strated that learning experiences create
associations between cues, behaviors, and
Self-Regulation and Dual-System responses as measured in post-learning actions
Models of the Mind (Skinner, 1948; Watson & Rayner, 1920). For
example, over the course of learning to read, ver-
Dual-system (process) models of the mind have bal stimuli come to automatically elicit reading
helped to provide insight into the role of habits in behavior such that it becomes more difficult to
self-regulation. Although there are a number of ignore a word in order to categorize the color in
variants of dual-system models (Epstein, 1994; which it is printed (Stanovich, Cunningham, &
Fazio, 1990; Posner & Snyder, 1975; Strack & West, 1981). More recent studies have found that
Deutsch, 2004), they overlap to a considerable conditioning strengthens mental associations as
extent in the characteristics ascribed to each sys- measured with a reaction time task (Olson &
tem. Using the terminology of Stanovich and Fazio, 2001).
West (2000), System 1 is the system of habits and Stronger associations between nodes are
involves information represented in associative reflected in the automaticity with which one node
links, automatic responses that occur quickly activates another (Strack & Deutsch, 2004).
(with the corollary that System 1 is the default Research on automaticity (and related functional
system for thinking and behavior), and little properties such as speed and efficiency) was
demand on executive control resources. In con- advanced with the development of a number of
trast, System 2 is the system of conscious reflec- implicit (indirect) measures of association, such
tion and involves information represented as as the sequential priming task. This task consists
propositions (that are operated on with logic), of the sequential presentation of two stimuli to
volition, slow response characteristics, and the examine the influence of the first (prime) on
use of executive (attentional) control. (For a cri- responses to the second (target). Early research
tique of such dual-system models, see Keren & demonstrated automaticity with a priming task
Schul, 2009.) by first creating an explicit expectancy that a
50 B.D. Ostafin

prime (e.g., the word body) would be followed The Reflective System
by an unrelated target (e.g., building-related
words, such as door). Participants were instructed Although the quick and dirty responses in System
to intentionally shift their attention away from 1 can provide effective guidance in goal pursuit,
expecting a body-related target and toward the conservative nature of System 1 means that it
expecting a building-related target when they is poorly equipped to respond to a dynamically
saw a body-related prime. The results showed changing environment. The dynamic aspect of
that when the interval between the prime and tar- the world shows itself in a variety of ways, such
get was relatively long (>500 ms), the body as novelty (e.g., an unfamiliar chessboard con-
prime facilitated recognition of the expected figuration or symptom presentation) or changes
building-related target as a word and inhibited in goals (e.g., deciding to go on a weight-loss
recognition of a target that was related to the diet). System 2 (the reflective system) provides
prime but unexpected (e.g., heart). However, greater flexibility under such circumstances.
when the interval between prime and target was The flexibility of System 2 results, in part,
brief (<500 ms), the body prime no longer facili- from the way that it represents information. In
tated recognition of the expected building- this system, information is represented as propo-
related targets but now facilitated responses to sitions with a truth value (Strack & Deutsch,
unexpected body-related targets (Neely, 1977). 2004). The representation of propositions allows
These results show that strong stimulusstimu- reasoning to be applied, such as evaluating the
lus associations are activated automatically, proposition Drinking alcohol tonight is a good
quickly, and that they are difficult to control. idea against a standard (e.g., consideration of
Similar findings have been shown with evalua- the costs and benefits of ones habit of drinking
tive priming task variants in which attitudes are on Wednesday evenings in light of a job inter-
assessed by examining the influence of primes view the next day). Such reasoning may result in
on target evaluations (Degner, 2009; Fazio, the conclusion that Drinking alcohol tonight is
Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986). not a good idea, leading to a decision/intention
An important benefit of using implicit mea- to not drink, which in turn activates appropriate
sures relates to the concept of transfer-appropriate behavioral schemata (e.g., declining a beer;
processing, which is the idea that the greater the Strack & Deutsch, 2004).
overlap between the processes that contribute to a The activity of System 2 is dependent on exec-
measure and the processes that contribute to real- utive (attentional) control, which is the ability to
life behavior, the better the measure will be in maintain attention on a goal and its object while
predicting the behavior (De Houwer, 2006; inhibiting (internal or external) distractions
Roediger, 1990). The extent to which implicit (Barrett, Tugade, & Engle, 2004; Engle, 2002).
measures assess automaticity should thus make Executive control must actively represent infor-
them valuable in understanding self-regulation mation so that logical operations can be applied.
failures, as such failures are thought to involve For example, the behavioral option of drinking
automatic processes (Hofmann, Friese, & Strack, alcohol, the standard of doing well on the job
2009). Along these lines, implicit measures interview, and the likely consequences of drink-
assessing stimulus-affect associations have been ing must all be represented in order for even
shown to predict behaviors that people often have simple reasoning to occur (e.g., If I drink my
a hard time controlling, including alcohol con- usual six beers, I will feel mentally sluggish at
sumption among heavy drinkers (Ostafin, the interview and this will negatively impact my
Marlatt, & Greenwald, 2008) and nonverbal evaluation). Reasoning will be enhanced to the
expressions of negative attitudes during innocu- degree that goal-irrelevant information (e.g., how
ous cross-racial interactions (Hofmann, good it feels to get an alcohol buzz) is inhibited.
Gschwendner, Castelli, & Schmitt, 2008). After making a decision (e.g., I will have just
5 Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes 51

one beer and then go home early), executive alcohol, and cigarettes (Diehl et al., 2010;
control must then represent the intention over the Erickson, Tiffany, Martin, & Baker, 1983;
course of the relevant time period (the short stay Hollands, Prestwich, & Marteau, 2011). Further,
at the bar) in the face of obstacles to goal comple- some research has found that aversive condition-
tion (e.g., the desire to consume a second beer). ing is mediated by changes in associative net-
works (Hollands et al., 2011; though see Mitchell,
De Houwer, & Lovibond, 2009). A recent exten-
A Dual-System Perspective sion of the conditioning method involves pairing
on Self-Regulation an incentive with avoidance behavior. For exam-
ple, instructions to respond to alcohol pictures
The classic self-regulation dilemma between with an avoidance behavior (pushing a joystick
temptation and restraint can be understood as away from the body with a concomitant zoom-
conflict between System 1 and System 2, with the out effect) have been found to result in reduced
outcome being determined by the relative alcohol use (Wiers, Eberl, Rinck, Becker, &
strength of the two systems (Hofmann et al., Lindenmeyer, 2011) and this effect is partially
2009). Stronger appetitive associations in System mediated by increased alcohol-avoidance associ-
1 will increase the likelihood of yielding to temp- ations (Eberl et al., 2013).
tation. For example, the automaticity of alcohol- Other System 1 interventions are designed to
approach associations predicts difficulty prevent the initial activation of appetitive associa-
controlling drinking behavior (Palfai & Ostafin, tions. One example consists of attention training.
2003). In addition, the ability of System 2 to Such interventions are based on the logic that
moderate the influence of automatic responses on attention toward a stimulus results in the activa-
subsequent behavior is dependent on at least two tion of appetitive responses and, therefore, that
factors (Fazio, 1990; Strack & Deutsch, 2004). training attention away from that stimulus should
The first involves the motivation to engage in facilitate self-regulation outcomes (Field & Cox,
effortful deliberation. When this motivation is 2008; MacLeod, Rutherford, Campbell,
low, behavior consistent with System 1 is more Ebsworthy, & Holker, 2002). For example, people
likely. Support for this idea has been found in the might be trained to attend to the area of space
literature on automatic associations and prejudi- occupied by neutral rather than alcohol-related
cial behavior (Olson & Fazio, 2004). Even when pictures in a visual probe task of attention.
motivation to engage in effortful deliberation is Although some studies have demonstrated bene-
high, deliberation cannot occur without sufficient fits to such attention-retraining manipulations
resources (such as time or executive control). (Schoenmakers et al., 2010), they may fail to gen-
Consistent with this idea, relations between eralize to new appetitive stimuli (Schoenmakers,
implicit associations and behavior are stronger Wiers, Jones, Bruce, & Jansen, 2007).
when the person chronically lacks such resources Other interventions have instead focused on
(Hofmann, Gschwendner, Friese, Wiers, & strengthening System 2 processes. There is evi-
Schmitt, 2008), when attention is occupied by dence that executive control can be enhanced
another task (Gibson, 2008), and when such through training (Klingberg, 2010), for example,
resources were recently used and thus in a and an initial study found that such training
depleted state (Ostafin et al., 2008). reduced alcohol use among heavy drinkers
Self-regulation interventions can be designed (Houben, Wiers, & Jansen, 2011). Also, interven-
to either weaken the operations of System 1 or tions that include elements designed to reduce
strengthen the operations of System 2. Regarding errors in reasoning have been shown to improve
System 1, there is evidence that aversive condi- the self-regulation of a number of behaviors such
tioning designed to combat appetitive associa- as alcohol use, binge eating, and gambling
tions can be effective in reducing the consumption (Darkes & Goldman, 1993; Grilo, Masheb, &
of tempting stimuli such as high-fat foods, Wilson, 2005; Ladouceur et al., 2001). Such
52 B.D. Ostafin

cognitive restructuring interventions have also chapter both because of its relation to early
been shown to alter emotional responding Buddhism (Bodhi, 2011; Gethin, 2011) and
(Shurick et al., 2012). because of its widespread usage among research-
Thus, there are several approaches that appear ers (Bishop et al., 2004) and contemporary
to work, at least somewhat, in allowing the indi- Buddhist teachers (Gunaratana, 2002), thus aid-
vidual to better resolve self-control dilemmas. ing communication among different scholarly
Nonetheless, each has important limitations. For traditions.
example, although extinction procedures may
create new associations in System 1, the previ-
ously problematic associations remain intact Mindfulness and Habit
(Bouton, 2004). This state of affairs leaves the in the Buddhist Model of Suffering
individual vulnerable when re-exposed to tempt- and Freedom3
ing cues (OBrien, Childress, Ehrman, &
Robbins, 1998) or when stress occurs (Sinha, The central concern of Buddhism is the general
Shaham, & Heilig, 2011). Stress additionally unsatisfactoriness of life, ranging from minor
limits the ability of System 2 to guide behavior, (but pervasive) irritations, boredom, and unful-
as stressors impair executive control (Qin, filled expectations to acute and intense forms of
Hermans, van Marle, Luo, & Fernndez, 2009) suffering such as the death of a loved one. An
and mitigate the effects of cognitive restructuring important cause of suffering is thought to be
on emotional response (Raio, Orederu, Palazzolo, craving and grasping (the latter of which is a
Shurick, & Phelps, 2013). more involved form of craving). From this per-
Given such limitations, other approaches to spective, the perception of external and internal
preventing self-regulation failure are desirable. stimuli is typically accompanied by positive or
One promising class of strategies aims not to negative (or neutral) feelings, which in turn trig-
alter the contents of the mind, but rather the indi- ger craving and grasping. Craving and grasping
viduals relationship to such mental contents result in thoughts, impulses, and behaviors that
(Hayes, 2004). The construct of mindfulness falls aim to obtain and maintain positive feeling states
into this category. or reduce and remove negative feeling states.
People can become trapped in a positive feed-
back loop such that thoughts, impulses, and
Mindfulness and Overcoming behaviors elicit additional feeling states, subse-
Automatic Processes: Theory quent craving and grasping, and so on.
Craving and grasping are thought to lead to
Defining Mindfulness dissatisfaction in a variety of ways. Consistently
wanting things to be different than they are
A popular definition of mindfulness is that it con- involves a chronic evaluation of the current state
sists of an awareness of immediate experience, as negative and a concomitant feeling of discon-
and a nonjudgmental, accepting attitude toward tent. Further, the difficulty of controlling the
that experience such that experiences are allowed world (both external and internal) means that
to be as they are independent of an agenda to con- attempts to obtain the pleasant and remove the
trol them (Kabat-Zinn, 1994). Scholars have sug- unpleasant are likely to lead to persistent frustra-
gested that although this definition is related to tion. Even when the desired goal is obtained, the
the original understanding of mindfulness in fleeting nature of positive experiences means that
Buddhism, it obscures other aspects of that the interlude of satisfaction will be brief. Finally,
understanding (e.g., the positive role of concepts,
evaluations and judgments in mindfulness prac- 3
This sketch is abstracted from more comprehensive
tice; Bodhi, 2011; Gethin, 2011). The nonjudg- accounts (Bodhi, 1984; Carrithers, 1983; Goldstein, 1993;
mental awareness definition will be used in this Rahula, 1959).
5 Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes 53

craving and grasping extend the natural life of a of continued mindfulness of feeling states leading
negative state through thoughts (e.g., rumination to the insight that they are short-lived. In contrast
and worry) and behaviors (e.g., acting in anger to intellectual insight, the insight derived from
elicits more conflict). mindfulness practice is understood to be an expe-
The trajectory from the perception of stimuli riential one that alters the way that stimuli are
and concomitant feeling state to craving and perceived. With extended practice, such changes
grasping is understood to be habitual (and would in perception may become automatic, thus shift-
thus involve System 1). The most usual case is ing the action of mindfulness to System 1.
that individuals are unconscious of the process. Further, such insight eliminates the imperative to
That is, the cognitions and behaviors aiming to do something about the feeling states; that is, the
increase the pleasant and decrease the unpleasant insight is that feeling states resolve themselves
are not carried out as the result of volitional without having to do anything about them. As a
intention, but instead just automatically happen. consequence, the meaning of feeling states is
A consequence of the repeated acting out of the changed in a qualitative manner. This qualitative
impulses of craving and grasping is that these change has been described as all of these
responses become reinforced and are even more changing phenomena as objects of our desire
tightly linked to perception and feeling. leave us feeling unfulfilled, while as objects of
Although the link between feeling and subse- mindfulness they become the very vehicle of
quent craving and grasping is habitual, it is not awakening (Goldstein, 2002, p. 32).
necessary. Mindfulness is proposed to lead to As evidence for the benefits of mindfulness
freedom from suffering by cutting this link. One has grown, several reviews have examined the
way that mindfulness practice is understood to do mechanisms of mindfulness from the perspective
so is by focusing awareness on the direct experi- of psychological science (Brown, Ryan, &
ence of feeling states. As this awareness is inten- Creswell, 2007; Hlzel et al., 2011; Lutz, Slagter,
tional, at least in the early phases of practice, it Dunne, & Davidson, 2008; Shapiro, Carlson,
would involve System 2. For example, the medi- Astin, & Freedman, 2006; Vago & Silbersweig,
tator may direct attention to body sensations 2012). In one recent paper, important mecha-
involved in breathing, walking, or sitting for nisms were proposed to include (1) what the
extended periods of time. This sort of bare atten- mind processes and (2) how the mind processes
tion includes a nonjudgmental element, in that this information (Teasdale & Chaskalson, 2011).
the sensations are to be observed in their most These mechanisms of mindfulness may improve
basic phenomenological form (including whether self-control outcomes through effects in both
they are pleasant, unpleasant or neutral) rather System 1 and 2.
than as states that are evaluated as good or bad
and therefore things to be sought or avoided.
Mindfulness practice also involves a concentra- Mindfulness Mechanism 1: Executive
tion element in which awareness is aimed at (Attentional) Control Lessens
either a specific object (e.g., the breath) or ongo- Problematic Content
ing experience (sometimes called choiceless
awareness). In both cases, instructions are to As mentioned, mindfulness practice often
disengage attention from distractors whenever includes a concentration element (especially
the mind has wandered. early on) and thus can be thought of as a function
Mindfulness of feeling states may lessen the of System 2 processes. Repeated practice with
likelihood of craving-related behaviors from focusing awareness should lead to an improved
just happening to the degree that that uncon- ability to resist distraction and sustain attention
sciousness plays a role in their expression. on intended objects (Lutz et al., 2008). Indeed,
However, the more central mechanism through there is accumulating evidence that mindfulness
which the feelingcraving link is broken consists training produces this benefit (Jha, Krompinger,
54 B.D. Ostafin

& Baime, 2007; Zeidan, Johnson, Diamond, pursuit. However, goals also produce frames that
David, & Goolkasian, 2010). Attentional control influence what we see and how we interpret what
should, in turn, facilitate self-regulation (Teasdale we see. A striking example can be found in the
& Chaskalson, 2011). For example, each instance inattentional blindness study of Simons and
of directing ones attention to neutral objects Chabris (1999). Among participants who had the
(e.g., the sensations of breathing or walking) is goal of counting the number of passes among a
also an instance during which one is not process- group of individuals playing catch, approxi-
ing problematic stimuli (e.g., cues of food for a mately 50 % did not perceive the unexpected
dieter). And, as already stated, less attention to event of a person in a gorilla suit walking through
problematic (e.g., tempting) stimuli means that the middle of this scene. Similarly, research using
automatically triggered associations to such stim- virtual reality methods has shown that partici-
uli are less likely to occur (Field & Cox, 2008). It pants who are looking directly at an object in
should thus be easier to control ones behavior in their hand are oblivious to changes of the objects
the absence of these System 1 thoughts and urges. size unless object size is relevant for their goal-
The ability to control attention is important, directed action (i.e., where to place the object;
but this mechanism is not unique to mindfulness. Triesch, Ballard, Hayhoe, & Sullivan, 2003).
Other interventions have been shown to improve In addition to influencing what we see, goals
self-regulation by strengthening general atten- alter our interpretation of perceived stimuli. For
tional control abilities (Houben et al., 2011) and example, a group of small children playing on the
attentional control related to specific cues of sidewalk may elicit a smile if one is out for a
temptation (Schoenmakers et al., 2010). The Sunday stroll but a curse if they are blocking the
novel element of mindfulness is represented in its quickest route to work when in a rush. That is,
focus on changing the relation with mental con- goal pursuit figures prominently in whether stim-
tent rather than changing the content itself uli are perceived positively, negatively, or as
(Hayes, 2004), as will be covered below. irrelevant (Carver & Scheier, 1990; Frijda, 1988;
Lw, Lang, Smith, & Bradley, 2008).
Given the importance of goals to what we feel,
Mindfulness Mechanism 2: changing ones goal from emotion regulation
Acceptance Breaks Maladaptive to acceptance may represent an important route
Frames to changing experience and behavior. First, an
accepting attitude may result in a stimulus no
A second mechanism of mindfulness involves longer eliciting strong automatic affective
changing the way that the content of experience is responses (i.e., craving and grasping from the
processed (Teasdale & Chaskalson, 2011). This is Buddhist perspective). As the goal to improve
done through an accepting attitude, one that can ones state weakens, stimuli may be less likely to
eventually change the framing of experience in a be perceived as something that can improve
manner conducive to self-regulation. Similar to affect state, thereby losing their motivational
learning to drive a car or other types of skill acqui- imperative. Second, an accepting attitude may
sition, an accepting attitude may initially require delink the relation between automatic affective
volition (System 2) but then become automatized responses and behavior. For example, an alco-
(System 1). Two processes through which accep- holic drink (for a problem drinker) might still be
tance may improve self-regulation include giving seen as something that could improve ones affect
up a goal to regulate emotional states and the cre- state but relinquishing the idea that one needs to
ation of experiential distance from temptation. be as happy as possible means that the stimulus is
less likely to be acted upon. Thus the impulse can
The relinquishment of an emotion regulation be allowed to arise and fall on its own accord. In
agenda. As a function of learning, we acquire this context, Marlatt (1994) has described the
frames and thus expertise in efficient goal utility of mindfulness as urge surfing, a metaphor
5 Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes 55

that contrasts with one of getting swept away by may shift from an immersed perspective in which
an impulse. it is experienced as a reality that must be made
manifest to a decentered perspective in which it is
Creation of a decentered perspective. Our normal experienced as another element of mental content
mode of experience is often characterized by an that comes and goes on its own accord. This shift
immersed, first-person perspective (see Nigro & should subsequently make it less important to act
Neisser, 1983). This means that when experienc- on the impulse. In addition, continued mindful-
ing temptation, the individual may be absorbed in ness practice may result in a decentered observa-
mental content related to acquiring the tempting tion of appetitive mental content replacing
stimulus, such as the stimulus itself (e.g., thoughts consummatory behavior as the habitual response
about how good a cold beer would taste), behav- to temptation (Breslin, Zack, & McMain, 2002).
ior that would facilitate acquisition of the stimu-
lus (e.g., walking to the fridge), etc. In this way,
our perception of the world is overshadowed by Mindfulness and Overcoming
our mental content. Such a state can be described Automatic Processes: Evidence
as looking from our thoughts, much like we look
at the world though eyeglasses (Hayes, Strosahl, As the discussion in the previous section sug-
& Wilson, 1999). Mindfulness helps individuals gests, there are a number of paths through which
to disentangle themselves from their mental con- mindfulness may allow the individual to rise
tent by directing attention toward the phenome- above the influence of System 1 habit. Recent
nology of the present-moment experience. For research supports the idea that mindfulness can
example, nonjudgmental observation would be indeed help to overcome habit.
directed toward physical sensations involved in
an appetitive state, including their location in the
body and their qualia (intensity, whether they are Mindfulness and Creativity
sharp, dull, throbbing, etc.). This observation
includes noting the dynamic nature of experi- The solution to many of lifes problems depends
ence, such as how the location and qualia of sen- upon the prior learning of logical steps designed
sations change over time or how a stimulus-related to reach a goal state (e.g., solving an algebra
thought emerges, lingers for a period of time, and problem, building a house). However, the auto-
then fades. This type of attention in which experi- matic cognitive and behavioral reactions (System
ence is treated as an object of awareness has been 1) derived from such learning can impede the
variously described as decentering (Teasdale solving of problems that require a nonhabitual
et al., 2002), re-perceiving (Shapiro et al., 2006), response (Gilhooly & Murphy, 2005; Knoblich,
or cognitive defusion (Hayes et al., 1999). The Ohlsson, Haider, & Rhenius, 1999; Luchins,
experiential distance involved in decentering rep- 1942). Because mindfulness involves a bare
resents a shift from identification with (and attentionobserving everything as if it was
immersion in) mental content (e.g., believing that occurring for the first time (Gunaratana, 2002,
one needs to eat a dessert or consume alcohol) to p. 134)it may facilitate the novel responses
dis-identification, in which the content is experi- required by creativity problems.
enced as passing thoughts and feelings that may Several recent studies have demonstrated a
or may not have some truth in them (Teasdale beneficial effect of mindfulness on creativity in a
et al., 2002, p. 276). variety of domains. For example, the Einstellung
A potential consequence of a decentered per- effect occurs when previous experience in solving
spective is that the relation between automatic similar types of problems triggers an initial idea
appetitive responses and behavior is weakened. of how to solve a current problem and this idea
For example, with mindfulness practice, the idea prevents the consideration of alternatives, even
that I need that chocolate cake (alcohol, etc.) when they are more optimal (Luchins, 1942).
56 B.D. Ostafin

Mindfulness meditation training has been shown intervention increased insight but not noninsight
to reduce the Einstellung effect, both when com- problem solving, and (c) such training effects
paring meditators and nonmeditators and follow- were partly mediated by increased state mindful-
ing participation in an 8-week mindfulness course ness (Ostafin & Kassman, 2012). These results
relative to a control group (Greenberg, Reiner, & have been extended with the finding that mind-
Meiran, 2012). fulness training outperforms a relaxation control
Another index of creativity is divergent think- condition in insight problem solving (Walsh &
ing, which is the ability to come up with novel Greaney, 2014).
ideas (e.g., listing novel uses of a common object;
Guilford, 1950). Previous research has shown
that compared to control conditions (such as Mindfulness and Attentional
20 min of resting or completing general knowl- Flexibility
edge questions), mindfulness training led partici-
pants to generate more novel exemplars of A second category of studies has examined the
categories (e.g., kitchen utensils) when asked to relation between mindfulness training and the
do so (Wenk-Sormaz, 2005). Using a within- ability to overcome the extent to which salient
subject design, a recent study similarly found that stimuli capture attention. The rapid serial visual
compared to a brief (35 min) focused-attention presentation (RSVP or attentional blink) task
training (i.e., maintaining attention on parts of represents one approach to assessing the extent to
the body), a mindfulness intervention increased which stimuli capture attention (Raymond,
the ability to generate novel uses of common Shapiro, & Arnell, 1992). The dual-target RSVP
objects but did not influence performance on a consists of presenting a rapid stream of stimuli
measure of logical thinking (Colzato, Ozturk, & with instructions to identify two targets (e.g.,
Hommel, 2012). Another recent study found that numbers) among a series of distractors (e.g., let-
compared to a brief (20 min) relaxation period, ters). Raymond et al. found that the second target
practitioners who completed a meditation inter- is more difficult to identify when it is presented
vention demonstrated greater ability to generate approximately < 500 ms after the first, indicating
higher-order categories that link groups of dispa- the time it takes to shift from automatic (System
rate stimuli (Strick, van Noorden, Ritskes, de 1) to volitional (System 2) processing.
Ruiter, & Dijksterhuis, 2012). Strick et al. further Performance on the RSVP has been shown to
noted that the meditation group responded more improve in a group after an intensive 3-month
quickly, which may suggest greater access to the meditation retreat (Slagter et al., 2007). The
correct answers generated by nonconscious results further showed that a neural indicator of
processes. attention allocation (the P3b event-related poten-
A third measure of creativity is represented by tial) was reduced in the intensive meditation
insight problems. These are problems in which group and that this change was correlated with
previous experience biases the representation of a improved RSVP performance.
problem so that it is difficult to solve. This diffi- Other research has shown that target detection
culty results in an impasse, after which the prob- can be impaired when participants are instructed
lem may be restructured, allowing an insight into to identify only a single target. For example, dis-
the solution (Knoblich et al., 1999). For example, tractors that have negative emotional content
the difficulty of solving the classic 9-dot problem have been shown to impair detection of targets
may be partly due to habitual focus on dots rather that are presented <500 ms after the distractor
than blank spaces as places to pivot (Kershaw (Most, Chun, Widders, & Zald, 2005). Brief
& Ohlsson, 2001). A recent pair of studies found mindfulness training has been shown to reduce
that (a) trait mindfulness predicts insight but not the attentional capture of negative stimuli in the
noninsight problem solving, (b) compared to a single-target RSVP paradigm (Ostafin, Verwoerd,
control condition, a brief (10 min) mindfulness & Wessel, 2014). Specifically, the results showed
5 Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes 57

that that distractors consisting of aversive pic- color-word Stroop task (Stroop, 1935). The
tures created less impairment in target detection common finding in the Stroop task is that it takes
for participants receiving two 10-min sessions of longer to name the ink color of color words when
mindfulness training compared to a control these dimensions are incongruent rather than
condition. congruent. Because word reading has become
automatic through practice, a smaller Stroop
effect is consistent with the ability to overcome
Mindfulness and Automatic (versus get stuck in) the habitual response of
Responses reading (De Houwer, 2003a). It is therefore of
interest that experienced meditators demonstrate
A third category of studies has examined the rela- less Stroop interference than nonmeditators
tion between mindfulness and the ability to over- (Moore & Malinowski, 2009). Similar findings
come automatic (System 1) responses elicited by have been shown with an intervention design. In
salient stimuli. A thought-provoking example this research, participants receiving three 20-min
consists of a case study that examined the startle sessions of mindfulness training showed less
reflex, an innate and involuntary response to sud- Stroop interference compared to two different
den loud noises, in a practitioner with over 40 control conditions (Wenk-Sormaz, 2005).
years of meditation practice (Levenson, Ekman, Although one subsequent study did not replicate
& Ricard, 2012). The results showed that indices these findings (Anderson, Lau, Segal, & Bishop,
of the startle response, including facial expres- 2007), it included modified Stroop tasks, which
sion and physiological measures, were greatly prevent direct comparison to the standard Stroop
reduced in the subject in a mindfulness medita- task (Algom, Chajut, & Lev, 2004; De Houwer,
tion period compared to a distraction period. 2003b).
Other research has shown that mindfulness Several studies have also shown that mindful-
training can help to overcome automatic ness moderates the relation between automatic
responses that have developed through previous affective responses and variables related to
learning. For example, learning experiences can behavior or conscious thought. These studies
lead to automatic approach responses to appeti- have assessed automatic associations with the
tive food cues (Van Gucht, Vansteenwegen, Van, Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald,
& Beckers, 2008). A recent series of three studies McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). The IAT consists of
examined the influence of mindfulness training categorizing stimuli from two target categories
on approach-related processes of this type (the category of interest such as alcohol and a
(Papies, Barsalou, & Custers, 2012). This comparison category such as water) and two
research used a variant of the affective Simon attribute categories (such as positive and nega-
task, in which participants are instructed to cate- tive) by pressing one of two response keys. The
gorize stimuli based on a nonaffective feature logic of the IAT is that the stronger the associa-
(e.g., frame color) while ignoring stimulus affect tion between two categories (e.g., alcohol and
(see De Houwer, Crombez, Baeyens, & Hermans, positive), the faster they can be classified when
2001). Previous research has shown that stimulus paired with the same response key. That is, the
affect influences such approach-avoidance irrelevant (affective) feature of a target (e.g., the
responses despite the irrelevance of this feature positive valence of alcohol stimuli) may automat-
to the instructions (De Houwer et al., 2001). ically activate affective responses, leading to
Papies et al. (2012) found that training partici- faster response times when the irrelevant feature
pants to allow and observe thoughts and impulses matches the extrinsic valence of the response key
related to attractive food reduced automatic food- (De Houwer, 2003b). Evidence for the IAT as a
approach responses. measure of automatic processes includes the IAT
The influence of learning on automatic placing low demand on executive control
responses has also been demonstrated in the resources, as a cognitive load does not eliminate
58 B.D. Ostafin

IAT effects (Cunningham, Preacher, & Banaji, that the ball-tossing task was web-based and that
2001), and the difficulty of controlling the IAT they would see the pictures of two other online
effect, as faking instructions have less success players. One of the other players was depicted
with the IAT compared to explicit (paper and to be Black and the other to be White. Relative
pencil) measures (Steffens, 2004). ball tosses to the two targets served as the depen-
Mindfulness has been found to moderate rela- dent measure. The results showed that the IAT
tions between alcohol IATs and drinking-related predicted a greater likelihood of tossing the ball
outcomes in several studies. For example, the to the White player in the control condition but
relation between automatic alcohol-approach not in the mindfulness condition. In sum, the
associations and problem drinking behavior was alcohol and race behavior studies suggest that
shown to be considerably weaker at higher levels mindfulness may play a general role in freeing
of trait mindfulness (Ostafin & Marlatt, 2008). individuals from automatic responses.
Parallel findings have been shown with a mind-
fulness intervention (Ostafin, Bauer, & Myxter,
2012). Participants in this study completed an Conclusions
alcohol-approach IAT, three sessions of mindful-
ness training (versus a control condition), and Without the ability to determine relevance from
then a follow-up session in which they reported irrelevance, humans could float in endless thought
their drinking over the previous week. The results about behavioral options. Fortunately, past learn-
showed that the baseline IAT predicted drinking ing allows us to automatically know what to think
at follow-up in the control condition, but not in and what to do in many situations. Unfortunately,
the mindfulness condition. Furthermore, mind- the automaticity of our thoughts and behaviors
fulness has been shown to moderate the relation means that they have a gravitational force that
between an alcohol IAT and preoccupation with can be difficult to escape when the context
alcohol-related thoughts (Ostafin, Kassman, & requires a novel response. To illustrate, craving
Wessel, 2013). The results showed that the IAT responses to food are essential to survival but
predicted preoccupation with alcohol-related become problematic for obese individuals trying
thoughts in participants with low but not high to lose weight. Mindfulness meditation was
trait mindfulness. The study also found that trait developed to counteract habitual desire for the
mindfulness was related to executive control but present to be different than it is (Rahula, 1959).
that the moderating role of mindfulness still The whole of the practice has been described as
existed when controlling for individual differ- transforming such habitual desire that contributes
ences in executive control. The latter results sug- to suffering to a more wholesome desire for well-
gest that the role of mindfulness in decoupling being (Sucitto, 2010). Evidence for the ability of
impulse from behavior is not simply due to stron- mindfulness to overcome habit is represented by
ger inhibitory control. the studies reviewed in this chapter showing
Mindfulness training may help to decouple mindfulness to influence automatic attentional
automatic affective responses from behavior in and behavioral responses related to self-
other domains. One recent study examined the regulation outcome.
influence of mindfulness on the relation between Although promising, the studies reviewed
automatic race attitudes and race behavior here represent a first step that should be furthered
(Ostafin & Friese, 2014). Participants in this with research designed to examine several press-
study completed a race IAT to assess White- ing questions. One question is whether mindful-
favoring (Black-disfavoring) attitudes, were ran- ness reduces the strength of automatic responses
domly assigned to a 10-min mindfulness or reduces the relation between these responses
intervention or a control condition, and then and downstream phenomena such as overt behav-
completed a modified cyberball task (Williams, ior (or both). Evidence for the former includes
Cheung, & Choi, 2000). Participants were told results showing that mindfulness training reduces
5 Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes 59

automatic responses (Papies et al., 2012; Wenk- Schmeichel, & Vohs, 2007). Future research
Sormaz, 2005) and attentional biases (Ostafin would benefit by examining whether amount of
et al., 2014; Slagter et al., 2007). Evidence for the practice increases the automaticity of mindful-
latter includes findings that mindfulness training ness. For example, it would be of interest to
weakens the relation between automatic affective examine whether executive control costs of
responses and overt approach behavior (Ostafin mindfulness practice decrease as one develops
et al., 2012; Ostafin & Friese, 2014), though it expertise. It would also be of interest to examine
should be noted that neither of the latter studies whether the automatization process can be
assessed the influence of mindfulness training on speeded up with strategies such as implementa-
the strength of automatic responses. tion intentions (Gollwitzer, 1999).
A second question concerns the mechanisms A final area for future research will be to
through which mindfulness influences automatic examine whether the influence of mindfulness on
processes. Executive control is an attractive can- automatic processes actually improves self-
didate given its role in moderating automatic regulation outcomes. That is, are the beneficial
appetitive processes in self-regulation (Barrett effects of mindfulness training on dysregulated
et al., 2004; Hofmann, Schmeichel, & Baddeley, appetitive behavior (Bowen et al., 2009; Brewer
2012) and the increasing body of evidence show- et al., 2011; Kristeller, Wolever, & Sheets, 2014)
ing that executive control can be strengthened by and aversive emotion (Hofmann, Sawyer, Witt,
mindfulness training (Jha et al., 2007; Zeidan & Oh, 2010) mediated by changes in automatic
et al., 2010). The benefits of mindfulness may processes? Despite these and other questions that
extend beyond executive control, however remain, the studies reviewed here provide evi-
(Ostafin et al., 2013). A central focus in mindful- dence that mindfulness may indeed help to tame
ness training is to foster an accepting attitude, the elephantine habits of the mind.
which may decrease the motivational imperative
of impulses, create the insight that impulses do
not last, or increase a decentered relation with References
impulses. These mechanisms can be viewed as
involving a shift in frames from one in which rel- Algom, D., Chajut, E., & Lev, S. (2004). A rational look
evance is represented as appetitive content to one at the emotional Stroop phenomenon: A generic slow-
down, not a Stroop effect. Journal of Experimental
in which relevance is represented as decon- Psychology: General, 133, 323338.
structed phenomenology (qualia, wave-like Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Bishop, S. R.
dynamics, etc.). Finally, a benefit of continued (2007). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and
nonjudgmental awareness may simply consist of attentional control. Clinical Psychology and
Psychotherapy, 14, 449463.
the substitution of acting on an impulse with Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004).
observing that impulse. Individual differences in working memory capacity
A third question regards the extent to which and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological
the effects of mindfulness can be shifted from Bulletin, 130, 553573.
Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. D.
System 2 to System 1. Initial practice will involve (2007). Self-regulation and the executive function:
System 2 in that volitional control of attention is The self as controlling agent. In A. W. Kruglanski &
needed in order to maintain awareness on the E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of
selected object (e.g., sensations of the breath) and basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 516539). New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
to apply an accepting attitude toward current Bilali, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2008). Why good
experience. Continued practice should shift the thoughts block better ones: The mechanism of the per-
effects of mindfulness to System 1, much like nicious einstellung (set) effect. Cognition, 108,
training automatizes cognitive and motor skills 652661.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson,
(Logan, 1988). This shift will benefit self- N. D., Carmody, J., . . . Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness:
regulation attempts, as the executive control A proposed operational definition. Clinical
resources of System 2 are limited (Baumeister, Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230241.
60 B.D. Ostafin

Bodhi, B. (1984). The noble eightfold path: Way to the end Degner, J. (2009). On the (un-)controllability of affective
of suffering. Onalaska, WA: Pariyatti Publishing. priming: Strategic manipulation is feasible but can
Bodhi, B. (2011). What does mindfulness really mean? A possibly be prevented. Cognition and Emotion, 23,
canonical perspective. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 327354.
1939. Dennett, D. (1984). Cognitive wheels: The frame problem
Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes of artificial intelligence. In C. Hookaway (Ed.), Minds,
in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 485494. machines and evolution (pp. 129151). Cambridge,
Bowen, S., Chawla, N., Collins, S. E., Witkiewitz, K., UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hsu, S., Grow, J., . . . Marlatt, A. (2009). Mindfulness- Diehl, A., Ulmer, L., Mutschler, J., Herre, H., Krumm, B.,
based relapse prevention for substance use disorders: Croissant, B., . . . Kiefer, F. (2010). Why is disulfiram
A pilot efficacy trial. Substance Abuse, 30, 295305. superior to acamprosate in the routine clinical setting?
Breslin, F. C., Zack, M., & McMain, S. (2002). An A retrospective long-term study in 353 alcohol-
information-processing analysis of mindfulness: dependent patients. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 45,
Implications for relapse prevention in the treatment of 271277.
substance abuse. Clinical Psychology: Science and Eberl, C., Wiers, R. W., Pawelczack, S., Rinck, M.,
Practice, 9, 275299. Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2013). Approach
Brewer, J. A., Mallik, S., Babuscio, T. A., Nich, C., bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical
Johnson, H. E., Deleone, C. M., . . . Rounsaville, B. J. effects replicate and for whom does it work best?
(2011). Mindfulness training for smoking cessation: Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 3851.
Results from a randomized controlled trial. Drug and Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as execu-
Alcohol Dependence, 119, 7280. tive attention. Current Directions in Psychological
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Science, 11, 1923.
Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the
for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18, psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist,
211237. 49, 709724.
Carels, R. A., Hoffman, J., Collins, A., Raber, A. C., Erickson, L. M., Tiffany, S. T., Martin, E. M., & Baker,
Cacciapaglia, H., & OBrien, W. H. (2001). Ecological T. B. (1983). Aversive smoking therapies: A condi-
momentary assessment of temptation and lapse in tioning analysis of therapeutic effectiveness.
dieting. Eating Behaviors, 2, 307321. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 595611.
Carrithers, M. (1983). Buddha: A very short introduction. Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and func- framework. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experi-
tions of positive and negative affect: A control-process mental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75109).
view. Psychological Review, 97, 1935. New York, NY: Academic.
Colzato, L. S., Ozturk, A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., &
Meditate to create: The impact of focused-attention Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of
and open-monitoring training on convergent and attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social
divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 116. Psychology, 50, 229238.
Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. Field, M., & Cox, W. M. (2008). Attentional bias in addic-
(2001). Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, sta- tive behaviors: A review of its development, causes,
bility, and convergent validity. Psychological Science, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 97,
12, 163170. 120.
Darkes, J., & Goldman, M. S. (1993). Expectancy chal- Foster, J. H., Marshall, E. J., & Peters, T. J. (2000).
lenge and drinking reduction: Experimental evidence Outcome after in-patient detoxification for alcohol
for a mediational process. Journal of Consulting and dependence: A naturalistic comparison of 7 versus 28
Clinical Psychology, 61, 344353. days stay. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 35, 580586.
De Houwer, J. (2003a). On the role of stimulus-response Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American
and stimulus-stimulus compatibility in the Stroop Psychologist, 43, 349358.
effect. Memory & Cognition, 31, 353359. Gethin, R. (2011). On some definitions of mindfulness.
De Houwer, J. (2003b). A structural analysis of indirect Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 263279.
measures of attitudes. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer Gibson, B. (2008). Can evaluative conditioning change
(Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective pro- attitudes toward mature brands? new evidence from
cesses in cognition and emotion (pp. 219244). the implicit association test. Journal of Consumer
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Research, 35, 178188.
De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why Gilhooly, K. J., & Murphy, P. (2005). Differentiating
are we using them? In R. W. Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), insight from non-insight problems. Thinking and
Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction Reasoning, 11, 279302.
(pp. 1128). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Goldstein, J. (1993). Insight meditation: The practice of
De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., Baeyens, F., & Hermans, D. freedom. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
(2001). On the generality of the affective Simon effect. Goldstein, J. (2002). One dharma. New York, NY:
Cognition and Emotion, 15, 189206. HarperCollins.
5 Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes 61

Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychological Science, 22,
Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 968975.
54, 493503. Hunt, W. A., Barnett, L. W., & Branch, L. G. (1971).
Greenberg, J., Reiner, K., & Meiran, N. (2012). Mind the Relapse rates in addiction programs. Journal of
trap: Mindfulness practice reduces cognitive rigidity. Clinical Psychology, 27, 455456.
PLoS One, 7, e36206. Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007).
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of atten-
(1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit tion. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience,
cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of 7, 109119.
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are.
Grilo, C. M., Masheb, R. M., & Wilson, G. T. (2005). New York, NY: Hyperion.
Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy and fluox- Keren, G., & Schul, Y. (2009). Two is not always better
etine for the treatment of binge eating disorder: A ran- than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories.
domized double-blind placebo-controlled comparison. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 533550.
Biological Psychiatry, 57, 301309. Kershaw, T. C., & Ohlsson, S. (2001). Training for insight:
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, The case of the nine-dot problem. In J. D. Moore &
5, 444454. K. Stenning (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-third
Gunaratana, B. H. (2002). Mindfulness in plain English. annual conference of the cognitive science society
Somerville, MA: Wisdom Publications. (pp. 489493). Mahwah, NJ: Lance Erlbaum
Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment ther- Associates.
apy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working
behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behavior Therapy, memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 317324.
35, 639665. Klinger, E. (1998). The search for meaning in evolution-
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). ary perspective and its clinical implications. In P. T.
Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential P. Wong & P. S. Fry (Eds.), The human quest for mean-
approach to behavior change. New York, NY: Guilford ing: A handbook of psychological research and clini-
Press. cal applications (pp. 2750). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D.
(2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposi-
anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. tion in insight problem solving. Journal of
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
169183. Cognition, 25, 15341555.
Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Strack, F. (2009). Impulse Kristeller, J., Wolever, R. Q., & Sheets, V. (2014).
and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Mindfulness-based eating awareness training (mb-eat)
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 162176. for binge eating: A randomized clinical trial.
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Castelli, L., & Schmitt, Mindfulness, 5, 282297.
M. (2008). Implicit and explicit attitudes and interra- Ladouceur, R., Sylvain, C., Boutin, C., Lachance, S.,
cial interaction: The moderating role of situationally Doucet, C., Leblond, J., & Jacques, C. (2001).
available control resources. Group Processes and Cognitive treatment of pathological gambling. Journal
Intergroup Relations, 11, 6987. of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 774780.
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Friese, M., Wiers, R. W., Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Ricard, M. (2012).
& Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and Meditation and the startle response: A case study.
self-regulatory behavior: Toward an individual differ- Emotion, 12, 650658.
ences perspective on behavior determination by auto- Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of autom-
matic versus controlled processes. Journal of atization. Psychological Review, 95, 492527.
Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 962977. Lw, A., Lang, P. J., Smith, J. C., & Bradley, M. M. (2008).
Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. D. Both predator and prey: Emotional arousal in threat
(2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. and reward. Psychological Science, 19, 865873.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174180. Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving:
Hollands, G. J., Prestwich, A., & Marteau, T. M. (2011). The effect of Einstellung. Psychological Monographs,
Using aversive images to enhance healthy food choices 54, 195.
and implicit attitudes: An experimental test of evalua- Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J.
tive conditioning. Health Psychology, 30, 195203. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in medi-
Hlzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., tation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163169.
Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy,
meditation work? proposing mechanisms of action G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emo-
from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives tional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their
on Psychological Science, 6, 537559. association through the experimental manipulation of
Houben, K., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. (2011). Getting a attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
grip on drinking behavior: Training working memory 111, 107123.
62 B.D. Ostafin

Marlatt, G. A. (1994). Addiction, mindfulness, and accep- alcohol-related thoughts. Psychology of Addictive
tance. In S. C. Hayes, N. S. Jacobson, V. M. Follette, Behaviors, 27, 11531158.
& M. J. Dougher (Eds.), Acceptance and change: Ostafin, B. D., & Marlatt, G. A. (2008). Surfing the urge:
Content and context in psychotherapy (pp. 175197). Experiential acceptance moderates the relation
Reno, NV: Context Press. between automatic alcohol motivation and hazardous
Mennella, J. A., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1996). The human drinking. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
infants response to vanilla flavors in mothers milk 27, 404418.
and formula. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, Ostafin, B. D., Marlatt, G. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2008).
1319. Drinking without thinking: An implicit measure of
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or alcohol motivation predicts failure to control alcohol
minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing use. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46,
information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197. 12101219.
Mitchell, C. J., De Houwer, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2009). Ostafin, B. D., Verwoerd, J., & Wessel, I. (2014). Untying
The propositional nature of human associative learn- the knot of fear: Mindfulness training weakens atten-
ing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 183198. tional bias toward trauma-film stimuli. Manuscript in
Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mind- preparation.
fulness and cognitive flexibility. Consciousness and Palfai, T. P., & Ostafin, B. D. (2003). Alcohol-related
Cognition, 18, 176186. motivational tendencies in hazardous drinkers:
Most, S. B., Chun, M. M., Widders, D. M., & Zald, D. H. Assessing implicit response tendencies using the
(2005). Attentional rubbernecking: Cognitive control modified-IAT. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41,
and personality in emotion-induced blindness. 11491162.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 654661. Papies, E. K., Barsalou, L. W., & Custers, R. (2012).
Moyers, B. (Producer). (1998). Portrait of addiction. Mindful attention prevents mindless impulses. Social
Retrieved from http://billmoyers.com/content/ Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 291299.
moyers-on-addiction-close-to-home/ Posner, M., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cog-
Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from nitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information pro-
lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading cessing and cognition: The Loyola symposium
activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of (pp. 5585). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226254. Qin, S., Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J. F., Luo, J., &
Nigro, G., & Neisser, U. (1983). Point of view in personal Fernndez, G. (2009). Acute psychological stress
memories. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 467482. reduces working memory-related activity in the dorso-
Norcross, J. C., Ratzin, A. C., & Payne, D. (1989). lateral prefrontal cortex. Biological Psychiatry, 66,
Ringing in the new year: The change processes and 2532.
reported outcomes of resolutions. Addictive Behaviors, Rahula, W. (1959). What the Buddha taught. New York,
14, 205212. NY: Grove.
OBrien, C. P., Childress, A. R., Ehrman, R., & Robbins, Raio, C. M., Orederu, T. A., Palazzolo, L., Shurick, A. A.,
S. J. (1998). Conditioning factors in drug use: Can they & Phelps, E. A. (2013). Cognitive emotion regulation
explain compulsion? Journal of Psychopharmacology, fails the stress test. PNAS Proceedings of the National
12, 1522. Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2001). Implicit attitude for- 110, 1513915144.
mation through classical conditioning. Psychological Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992).
Science, 12, 413417. Temporary suppression of visual processing in an
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Trait inferences as a RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of
function of automatically activated racial attitudes and Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Basic and Performance, 18, 849860.
Applied Social Psychology, 26, 111. Reynolds, R. I. (1982). Search heuristics of chess players
Ostafin, B. D., Bauer, C., & Myxter, P. (2012). Mindfulness of different calibers. The American Journal of
decouples the relation between automatic alcohol Psychology, 95, 383392.
motivation and heavy drinking. Journal of Social and Rieskamp, J., & Otto, P. E. (2006). SSL: A theory of how
Clinical Psychology, 31, 729745. people learn to select strategies. Journal of
Ostafin, B. D., & Friese, M. (2014). Mindfulness training Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 207236.
moderates the relation between an implicit measure of Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention with-
race attitude and interracial behavior. Manuscript in out remembering. American Psychologist, 45,
preparation. 10431056.
Ostafin, B. D., & Kassman, K. T. (2012). Stepping out of Schoenmakers, T. M., de Bruin, M., Lux, I. F. M., Goertz,
history: Mindfulness improves insight problem solv- A. G., Van Kerkhof, Dorieke H. A. T., & Wiers, R. W.
ing. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 10311036. (2010). Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modi-
Ostafin, B. D., Kassman, K. T., & Wessel, I. (2013). fication training in abstinent alcoholic patients. Drug
Breaking the cycle of desire: Mindfulness and execu- and Alcohol Dependence, 109, 3036.
tive control weaken the relation between an implicit Schoenmakers, T., Wiers, R. W., Jones, B. T., Bruce, G.,
measure of alcohol valence and preoccupation with & Jansen, A. T. M. (2007). Attentional re-training
5 Taming the Wild Elephant: Mindfulness and Its Role in Overcoming Automatic Mental Processes 63

decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M.,
generalization. Addiction, 102, 399405. Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive
Shannon, C. E. (1950). Programming a computer for play- awareness and prevention of relapse in depression:
ing chess. Philosophical Magazine, 41, 256275. Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and
Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, Clinical Psychology, 70, 275287.
B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men.
Clinical Psychology, 62, 373386. Psychological Review, 55, 189208.
Shurick, A. A., Hamilton, J. R., Harris, L. T., Roy, A. K., Triesch, J., Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M., & Sullivan,
Gross, J. J., & Phelps, E. A. (2012). Durable effects of B. T. (2003). What you see is what you need. Journal
cognitive restructuring on conditioned fear. Emotion, of Vision, 3, 8694.
12, 13931397. Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness,
Simon, H. A. (1972). Theories of bounded rationality. In self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): A
B. McGuire & R. Radner (Eds.), Decisions and orga- framework for understanding the neurobiological
nization (pp. 161176). Amsterdam: North-Holland mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in Human
Publishing. Neuroscience, 6, 296.
Simon, H. A., & Simon, P. A. (1962). Trial and error Van Gucht, D., Vansteenwegen, D., Van, D. B., & Beckers,
search in solving difficult problems: Evidence from T. (2008). Conditioned craving cues elicit an auto-
the game of chess. Behavioral Science, 7, 425429. matic approach tendency. Behaviour Research and
Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our Therapy, 46, 11601169.
midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic Vervaeke, J. (2011, March). Can foolishness lead to men-
events. Perception, 28, 10591074. tal illness? Mind matters: Towards a new understand-
Sinha, R., Shaham, Y., & Heilig, M. (2011). Translational ing of psychopathology. Lecture conducted from the
and reverse translational research on the role of stress University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada. Retrieved
in drug craving and relapse. Psychopharmacology, from http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EE5xNA3yH
218, 6982. Yc&list=PLFDA9061A3FAC3E7D&index=1
Skinner, B. F. (1948). Superstition in the pigeon. Journal Wild elephants raid fields in southern Vietnam (2013).
of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168172. Thanh Nien news. Retrieved from http://www.thanhni-
Slagter, H. A., Lutz A., Greischar, L. L., Francis, A. D., ennews.com/2010/pages/20130215-wild-elephants-
Nieuwenhuis, S., Davis, J. M., & Davidson, R. J. damage-crops-in-southern-vietnam-during-tet.aspx
(2007). Mental training affects distribution of limited von Uexkll, J. (1957). A stroll through the worlds of ani-
brain resources. PLoS Biology, 5, 138. mals and men. In C. H. Schiller (Ed.), Instinctive
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Higher nervous functions: The ori- behavior: The development of a modern concept
enting reflex. Annual Reviews of Physiology, 25, (pp. 580). New York, NY: International Universities
545580. Press.
Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & West, R. F. Walsh, M., & Greaney, J. (2014). Insight problem solving
(1981). A longitudinal study of the development of is improved by mindfulness but not by relaxation.
automatic recognition skills in first graders. Journal of Manuscript submitted for publication.
Reading Behavior, 13, 5774. Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differ- reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 114.
ences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation can reduce habitual
debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645665. responding. Advances in Mind-Body Medicine, 21,
Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the implicit association test 3349.
immune to faking? Experimental Psychology, 51, Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., &
165179. Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining automatic action
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive tendencies changes alcoholic patients approach bias
determinants of social behavior. Personality and for alcohol and improves treatment outcome.
Social Psychology Review, 8, 220247. Psychological Science, 22, 490497.
Strick, M., van Noorden, T. H. J., Ritskes, R. R., de Ruiter, Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000).
J. R., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). Zen meditation and Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the
access to information in the unconscious. internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 14761481. 79, 748762.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial ver- Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2007). A new look at habits and
bal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, the habit-goal interface. Psychological Review, 114,
643662. 843863.
Sucitto, A. (2010). Turning the wheel of truth: Wood, W., Quinn, J. M., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). Habits in
Commentary on the Buddhas first teaching. Boston, everyday life: Thought, emotion, and action. Journal
MA: Shambhala. of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 12811297.
Teasdale, J. D., & Chaskalson, M. (2011). How does Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., &
mindfulness transform suffering? II: The transforma- Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation
tion of dukkha. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, improves cognition: Evidence of brief mental training.
103124. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 597605.
Section 2
Personality and Social Psychology
Perspectives
Burning Issues in Dispositional
Mindfulness Research 6
Robert J. Goodman, Jordan T. Quaglia,
and Kirk Warren Brown

The deliberate exercise of mindful attention has populations. Despite this progress, the use of
been practiced for 2,500 years as a way to contact psychometric instruments to measure mindful-
with, and gain insight into, ongoing events and ness has sparked a number of contentious issues
experiences. In comparison, the application of regarding our understanding of the construct, its
modern scientific methods to the study of mind- measurement, and its development. In this chap-
fulness is recent, but to date this work has ter, we consider theory and review empirical
revealed manifold benefits of mindfulness train- research to address four burning issues sparked
ing and practice. In the last 10 years, the develop- by the recent interest in dispositional mindfulness:
ment and increasingly widespread use of
psychometric instruments to assess individual 1. How is mindfulness best defined and measured?
differences in mindfulness have resulted in 2. Are dispositional mindfulness measures dis-
numerous benefits to our empirical understand- sociable from other measures of attention?
ing of the construct. Likewise, emerging descrip- 3. Are measures of dispositional mindfulness
tions and operationalization of mindfulness have valid predictors of theoretically important
contributed to understanding the construct, regulatory outcomes?
revealing how mindfulness itselfapart from the 4. How does mindfulness develop?
training programs designed to enhance itis
related to numerous adaptive outcomes at
neurobiological, psychological, and behavioral
levels, and for a variety of normative and clinical How Is Mindfulness Best Defined
and Measured?

Authors Note: Portions of this chapter appear in Quaglia, Proposed definitions of mindfulness in the scien-
Brown, Lindsay, Creswell, and Goodman (2015). tific literature span a wide range, sharing both
R.J. Goodman (*) commonalities and marked differences. Most
Department of Psychological Sciences, writers and researchers trace their use of the term
Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, AZ, USA
e-mail: robert.goodman@nau.edu
mindfulness to Buddhist psychology. Thus,
establishing consensus on a definition of mind-
J.T. Quaglia K.W. Brown, Ph.D.
Department of Psychology, Virginia Commonwealth
fulness and its lived expression may begin with a
University, P.O. Box 842018, Richmond, study of Buddhist scholarship. Such scholarship
VA 23284-2018, USA is built on a deep familiarity with Buddhist source

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 67


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_6
68 R.J. Goodman et al.

texts, and the navigation of difficulties stemming stem from the fact that outside of cognitive
from translational and cultural differences that science, the terms are sometimes used inter-
have accrued over centuries. An inspection of changeably (e.g., Merikle & Joordens, 1997).
this literature reveals a range of interpretations of Some scholars have used both to describe
the meaning of mindfulness. Scholar Georges mindfulness (e.g., Bodhi, 2006, 2011), and in
Dreyfus (2011) notes that Buddhism is a plural fact, both terms may be applicable, for two rea-
tradition that has evolved over centuries to sons. First, there is a close interrelation between
include a large variety of views about mindful- attention and awareness in daily life (e.g.,
ness (p. 42). Thus it is important to emphasize Lamme, 2003); second, and more specifically,
that there is no single, authorative definition of an integration of attention and meta-awareness
mindfulness (Anlayo, 2013). helps to distinguish mindfulness from related
Limitations of space and expertise will not states. For example, attention may be concen-
permit a discussion of the various understand- trated, but only when coupled with meta-
ings of mindfulness. For the present purposes we awarenessan apprehension of the current
simply offer a brief look at two approaches to state of the mind that serves to monitor that
understanding mindfulnessthe classical or focused attentivenessdoes it become mindful
canonical accounts, and the science-based (Dreyfus, 2011).
accounts, as both have informed mindfulness Quality of attention and/or awareness is also
operationalizations for research purposes. central to contemporary scientific definitions of
Scholarly accounts of classical mindfulness are mindfulness, as the well-known definition by
based on the Pali Canon, the oldest surviving, Kabat-Zinn (1994, p. 4) illustrates: Paying
complete collection of Buddhist texts. These attention in a particular way: on purpose, in the
texts offer rich descriptions of mindfulness that present moment, and nonjudgmentally.
have informed subsequent interpretations and The role of non-judgment or acceptance in
are still widely used today. mindfulness represents a major point of differ-
Even within this approach are layered mean- ence between classical and some scientific con-
ings of mindfulness, and here we note just a few ceptions of mindfulness. Classical mindfulness,
concise, contemporary descriptions of basic as interpreted by Bodhi (e.g., 2011) and others,
mindfulness from well-regarded scholars: gives judgment or evaluation a key place in the
practice of mindfulness. Evaluation here con-
an alert but receptive equanimous observa- cerns discrimination, a reflective consideration
tion. (Anlayo, 2003, p. 60) of what is healthy and unhealthy, wise or unwise
watchfulness, the lucid awareness of each action, that comes with the conjoining of sus-
event that presents itself on the successive tained attentiveness and comprehension. Bodhi
occasions of experience. (Bodhi, 2011, p. 21) (2011, p. 26) makes the role of evaluation clear:
The minds ability to keep the object in the the practitioner of mindfulness must at times
ken [focus] of attention without losing it. evaluate mental qualities and intended deeds,
(Dreyfus, 2011, p. 47) make judgments about them, and engage in pur-
A kind of lucid holding of attention on an poseful action. In this way, classical mindful-
object, where the mind is both aware of the ness is value-laden, bringing to bear capacities
object and, in some sense, aware that it is of attention and discerning thought to regulate
aware of the object. (Gethin, 2015, p. 29) mental states and behavior. It is in the place of
thought, and evaluative thought in particular,
These descriptions highlight a sustained that represents one major difference between
attentiveness to, or awareness of perceptual classical and a number of science-based, and
events as they appear. The reference to atten- particularly clinical conceptualizations of
tion in some texts and awareness in others may mindfulness.
6 Burning Issues in Dispositional Mindfulness Research 69

Distinctions between classical Buddhist and Are Dispositional Mindfulness


science-based conceptions of mindfulness also Measures Dissociable from Other
provide a helpful lens to better understand differ- Measures of Attention?
ences between the various operationalizations of
mindfulness. Like both major approaches to con- If mindfulness is fundamentally a construct
ceptualizing mindfulness discussed here, the concerning attention, then it is important to
various self-report scale operationalizations of show both its convergence with, and divergence
mindfulness highlight quality of attention, either from related attentional constructs. Among the
as the central feature (e.g., Mindful Attention most frequently studied constructs is atten-
Awareness Scale; MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003), tional control, understood as the capacity to
or among a set of factors (e.g., Five Facet voluntarily select and focus on a specific object
Mindfulness Questionnaire; FFMQ; Baer, Smith, to the exclusion of others; this has been associ-
Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006). Beyond ated with several adaptive outcomes, such as
the attention factor, however, the various scales lower attentional bias to threatening informa-
differ widely in their inclusion of other factors tion (Derryberry & Reed, 2002). Attentional
(e.g., describing, nonjudgment). The presence of control and mindfulness appear to have several
these other factors has been strongly influenced adaptive outcomes in common. For example,
by clinical perspectives on mindfulness. For both capacities involve an increased stability
example, the FFMQ and its ancestor, the and continuity of attention toward a focal
Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills object, and both may be enhanced by training.
(KIMS; Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004) have their Evidence supports a relation between them;
conceptual origin in Dialectical Behavior two measures of mindfulness, the MAAS and
Therapy (Linehan, 1993). The Philadelphia the FFMQ Act with Awareness subscale moder-
Mindfulness Scale (Cardaciotto, Herbert, ately correlated with self-reported attentional
Forman, Moitra, & Farrow, 2008) has its concep- control in a recent study (Brown, Goodman, &
tual origins in Kabat-Zinns (1994) definition. Inzlicht, 2013). Relatedly, Mrazek, Smallwood,
Quaglia, Brown, Lindsay, Creswell, and and Schooler (2012) found modest negative
Goodman (2015) discuss the conceptual origins correlations between the MAAS and four con-
of the extant mindfulness scales in further detail. vergent measures of mind-wandering, a con-
Going forward, it is important that research- struct antithetical to attentional control. There
ers clearly state the conceptual provenance of is also evidence that mindfulness training
each mindfulness measure used in research. improves attentional control (e.g., Chiesa,
This will help to convey the particular under- Calati, & Serretti, 2011). For example, Mrazek
standing of mindfulness that is being examined et al. (2012) found behavioral indicators of
and how that operationalized perspective on reduced mind-wandering following 8 min of
mindfulness is associated with the outcomes mindfulness practice among novices, which
assessed. Addressing issues concerning the converges with outcomes from studies of
definition and operationalization of mindful- lengthier mindfulness training (Jha, Stanley,
ness will also benefit from scholarship and Kiyonaga, Wong, & Gelfand, 2010).
research to isolate the specific cognitive and Despite such expectable relations, there is
other psychological processes involved in substantial theoretical divergence between mind-
mindfulness (e.g., Davis & Thompson, 2015). fulness and attentional control. In particular,
In this and other ways there is considerable mindfulness is distinct in its interrelatedness with
opportunity for interchange between Buddhist meta-awareness and its receptive quality to ongo-
scholarship and Western science in seeking ing phenomena. The term meta-awareness has
shared, empirically grounded understandings multiple meanings, the simplest being knowledge
of mindfulness. of the state of the mind at a given moment,
70 R.J. Goodman et al.

including the quality of ones attention (Brown & face value of scale items as reflections of a
Cordon, 2009). The integration of attention and construct. Because it relies on subjective judg-
meta-awareness also helps to distinguish mind- ment, face validity is typically not a scientific cri-
fulness from concentration, which deeply informs terion for judging the value of a measure (e.g.,
the attentional control construct (Derryberry & Gravetter & Forzano, 2009; Walsh & Betz, 2001).
Reed, 2002). Thus, attention may be concen- However, we believe that self-report measures of
trated, but without meta-awareness to help pre- the mindfulness should be the subject of rigorous
serve the continuity of focused attentiveness, it is empirical scrutiny. Studies using self-report mea-
lacking in mindfulness (see Brown & Cordon, sures of mindfulness should incorporate mea-
2009; Dreyfus, 2011 for further discussion). sures of attention to assess their convergent,
Mindfulness also involves a capacity to broaden discriminant, and incremental validity. In addi-
the field of attention, or to set up awareness of tion to concerns with validity, such research will
an ever-widening domain of internal and external enhance our understanding of how mindfulness
experiences. This involves the capacity to flexi- fits into the nomological network of other, estab-
bly alter the breadth of attention, from a penetra- lished indexes of attention (e.g., Posner &
tive focus to a broad panoramic view of Rothbart, 2007).
experience (Bodhi, 2004). Thus, mindfulness
represents an attention capacity characterized by
flexibility and breadthqualities distinct from Are Measures of Dispositional
the concentrated nature of attentional control. Mindfulness Valid Predictors
From this brief theoretical overview, it follows of Regulatory Outcomes?
that measures of mindfulness should be associ-
ated with attentional controlas initial evidence Mindfulness is studied scientifically in three
reviewed earlier showedbut also predict theo- primary ways: As an outcome of formal mind-
retically relevant outcomes over and above atten- fulness training (e.g., cumulative hours of prac-
tional control. Emerging evidence supports this tice), as a state evoked by brief exercises among
incremental validity (Brown et al., 2013; Quaglia, mindfulness-nave populations, and as a dispo-
Goodman, & Brown, 2015). For example, Brown sition via psychometric instruments. Despite
et al. (2013) found both the MAAS and FFMQ common interests, researchers must guard
Act with Awareness measures to predict reduced against the assumption that these distinct modes
amplitudes of the late positive potential (LPP) of inquiry are capturing the same phenomenon
an electrocortical indicator of emotional reac- (Davidson, 2010), or that they are predicated on
tivityin response to unpleasant, high arousal the same foundational assumptions about mind-
images after controlling for attentional control fulness. Examination of the similarities and dif-
(which was not related to LPP amplitudes in this ferences among the findings from these different
study). Additionally, the MAAS and other mea- measurements of mindfulness will help address
sures of dispositional mindfulness have been many of the validity concerns regarding the use
related to specific mental health, physical health, of self-report mindfulness measures. If the three
interpersonal, and behavioral outcomes for methods show convergent results, this would
which research on attentional control has had lend credence to the claim that they are examin-
little to say. ing the same phenomenon, or at minimum, a
Taking together this theory and evidence, closely related family of phenomena. In this
there is currently little basis to conclude that trait section, we review evidence demonstrating
mindfulness measures like the MAAS are merely empirical convergence between these three
capturing experienced lapses of attention operationalizationspsychometric, induction,
(Grossman, 2011, p. 1038), a contention of mind- and interventionparticularly focusing on a
fulness scales grounded in the problematic notion widely researched outcome of interest, emotion
of face validity, the superficial appearance, or regulation.
6 Burning Issues in Dispositional Mindfulness Research 71

Eight self-report measures of trait mindfulness Neural Markers of Affective Style


for adult respondents have been published. Three Several correlational and experimental studies
of these measuresthe FFMQ (Baer et al., have examined whether mindfulness promotes
2006), its ancestor the KIMS (Baer et al., 2004), positive affective style. Affective style refers to
and the MAAS (Brown & Ryan, 2003)have individual differences in emotional reactivity and
been used most widely. Despite differences in regulation in response to emotional provocation
their conceptual foundations and intended uses, (Davidson, 1998). The most widely used mea-
the FFMQ/KIMS and the MAAS have shown sure of affective style is hemispheric brain asym-
correlations with theoretically meaningful indi- metry in electrical activity in the prefrontal
cators of well-being and psychopathology (for a cortex. Captured using electroencephalography
review see Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007). In (EEG), prefrontal asymmetry reflects the magni-
fact, a major area of convergence in findings tude of hemispheric differences in the frequency
between self-report measures of mindfulness and of alpha-band activity, where greater relative left-
mindfulness-based and mindfulness-integrated sided alpha activity has proven a reliable indica-
interventions is in mental health outcomes. The tor of generally positive affective style (but see
MAAS and FFMQ, for example, have been cor- Harmon-Jones & Harmon-Jones, 2011), and
related with lower anxiety, depression, mood dis- associated with approach motivation, higher sub-
turbance, perceived stress and other mental health jective and eudaimonic well-being (Urry et al.,
indicators. Mindfulness-based interventions, 2004), and adaptive responses to stressors
including Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction (Davidson, 2000). Conversely, greater relative
(MBSR; Kabat-Zinn, 1990) and Mindfulness- right-sided alpha activity has been associated
based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT; Segal, with avoidance motivation, withdraw-oriented
Teasdale, & Williams, 2004), have been shown to dispositions, and the experience of some negative
beneficially impact the same and similar indica- emotions (Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Senulis, &
tors (see Baer, 2003; Grossman, Niemann, Friesen, 1990).
Schmidt, & Walach, 2004; Hofmann, Sawyer, Recent research from our lab revealed that
Witt, & Oh, 2010 for reviews). higher levels of MAAS-assessed dispositional
mindfulness were associated with greater relative
left-sided alpha activity in prefrontal regions
Physiological Concomitants (Goodman, Brown, & Haver, 2012). In contrast,
of Emotion Regulation measures of depression and emotion dysregula-
tion were associated with more right-predominant
Given research showing that mindfulness and its alpha asymmetry. Mindfulness training has
enhancement are related to mental health, research shown similar results. Davidson et al. (2003)
has recently sought to examine whether this quality found that, compared to wait-list controls, par-
of attention, and training to enhance it, can improve ticipants in an 8-week MBSR program showed
emotion regulation, a key underpinning for mental significant shifts from baseline levels toward
health, behavior regulation, social relationships, greater relative left-sided cortical activation at the
and other domains of adaptive functioning (Gross, end of the program, and at 4 months after train-
1998). According to Gross (1998), Emotion regu- ing. This shift toward a more positive affective
lation refers to the processes by which individuals style was associated with improvements in
influence the emotions they have, when they have immune functioning, as measured by antibody
them, and how they are experienced and expressed response to flu virus inoculation. Similar shifts
(p. 275). While still nascent, evidence from research toward left-sided asymmetry have been found
on dispositional mindfulness, mindfulness induc- among participants with substantially less medi-
tions, and mindfulness interventions has shown tation practice. Students who practiced mindful-
convergence across neural, neuroendocrine, and ness of breathing for an average of 613 min a
other physiological levels of analysis. day over the course of a 5-week training program
72 R.J. Goodman et al.

exhibited significant shifts toward left-sided acti- Taren et al. (2013) found these scores to correlate
vation compared to controls (Moyer et al., 2011). with reduced amygdala-ACC resting state func-
Even a brief mindfulness induction, operational- tional connectivity, a potential mechanism for
ized as a 15-min guided breath meditation, pro- stress resilience. Both studies provide evidence
duced significant shifts toward left-sided for a neurobiological pathway to explain how
asymmetry among depression sufferers mindfulness may regulate emotional responses.
(Barnhofer, Chittka, Nightingale, Visser, & Neuroimaging research has also identified
Crane, 2010). In sum, dispositional mindfulness, mindfulness-related activation differences in pre-
induced mindfulness, and mindfulness training frontal cortical regions involved in emotion regu-
have each predicted prefrontal alpha asymmetry lation, such as the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex
in a manner indicative of positive affective style. (DMPFC), and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC),
which have been associated with emotional reap-
Neural Markers of Emotional Reactivity praisal (Davidson, 2000; Ochsner & Gross, 2005)
Another recent area of inquiry on mindfulness and the top-down inhibition of amygdala activity
and emotion regulation has used neuroimaging to (Quirk & Beer, 2006). Among meditators with
identify changes in brain structure and activation 110017000 h of practice, cumulative hours of
patterns tied to dampened emotional reactivity. In meditation significantly predicted gray matter
particular, this research has examined brain acti- thickness of the OFC (Hlzel et al., 2008), and
vation in regions associated with emotion- increases in OFC activation have been found in
relevant threat detection, particularly the bilateral people after as little as 4 days of mindfulness
amygdalae and areas of the prefrontal cortices. training (Zeidan et al., 2011). Evidence has also
For example, patients with social anxiety disor- shown increases in DMPFC activations follow-
der showed reduced amygdala activity following ing MBSR (Farb et al., 2010).
an 8-week MBSR intervention (Goldin & Gross, Similar patterns of activation have been pre-
2010). In a healthy stressed sample Hlzel et al. dicted using scales sensitive to individual differ-
(2010) found significant within-subject reduc- ences in mindfulness. The Observe and Act with
tions in perceived stress following an MBSR pro- Awareness subscales of the KIMS have predicted
gram, and the degree of change in perceived increases in DMPFC activity (Frewen et al., 2010),
stress was significantly correlated with decreases and total KIMS scores have predicted increases in
in gray matter density in the right amygdala. DMPFC activation, which in turn were associated
MBSR participants have also demonstrated less with reduced amygdala activation during the reap-
neural reactivity to a sadness provocation com- praisal of negative stimuli (Modinos, Ormel, &
pared to controls (Farb et al., 2010), and reduced Aleman, 2010). Additionally, the MAAS has been
activity in the right amygdala during an induced significantly related to increases in OFC activity
state of mindfulness (Farb et al., 2007). during states of rest (Way et al., 2010) and
Studies examining the functional neural cor- increases in right and life ventrolateral prefrontal
relates of dispositional mindfulness are fewer, cortex (VLPFC), ventromedial prefrontal cortex
but consistent with results from mindfulness (VMPFC), MPFC, and right dorsolateral prefron-
training. The Observe subscale of the KIMS has tal cortex (DLPFC) during socioemotional threat
been correlated with lower amygdala activity (Creswell et al., 2007). Creswell et al. (2007) also
(Frewen et al., 2010), and MAAS scores have found strong negative associations between a
been associated with less amygdala activation at number of these PFC regions and the amygdala
rest (Way, Creswell, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, among higher MAAS scorers, suggesting a more
2010) and during socioemotional threat efficient PFC down-regulation of amygdala
(Creswell, Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, responses. Thus, measures of dispositional mind-
2007). Recently Taren, Creswell, and Gianaros fulness have predicted similar patterns of neural
(2013) found that MAAS scores were associated activation in the prefrontal cortex as those attrib-
with smaller gray matter amygdala volume, and uted to formal mindfulness training.
6 Burning Issues in Dispositional Mindfulness Research 73

Neuroendocrine Markers of Stress an early phase of emotion processing, potentially


These activation patterns in the PFC and amyg- lessening or even circumventing the need for
dala have been associated with reduced secre- reappraisal, response modulation, and other,
tions of salivary cortisol (Urry et al., 2006), a downstream emotion regulatory efforts. Yet
neuroendocrine marker of stress responses that experimental research with induced and trained
can compromise healthy immune and other bio- mindfulness is needed to show whether this early
logical functioning when chronically activated. modulation of reactivity to emotionally provoca-
Emerging research (Brown, Weinstein, & tive stimuli can be attributed to mindfulness
Creswell, 2012) has shown that dispositional itself, rather than factors that may be associated
mindfulness modulates cortisol and affective with it.
responses during the Trier Social Stress Task
(TSST), a reliable induction of social evaluative
threat (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004). Similar Dispositional Mindfulness
reductions in cortisol after acute stress have been as a Moderator of Mindfulness
discovered following only 5 days of mindfulness- Training Effects
integrated training (Tang et al., 2007). Paralleling
these findings, women diagnosed with breast The convergent findings discussed here do not
cancer who completed an MBSR program definitively demonstrate that research on disposi-
showed lower cortisol levels under social evalua- tional, induced, and trained mindfulness are
tive threat compared to controls (Witek-Janusek investigating the same phenomenon. Yet a final
et al., 2008). Salivary cortisol levels and auto- body of evidence we briefly review adds further
nomic stress responses have been shown to weight to this proposition. Specifically, evidence
decrease from 6 to 14 months post-MBSR has shown that changes in dispositional mindful-
(Carlson, Speca, Faris, & Patel, 2007). ness are sensitive to participation in mindfulness
Cardiovascular responses are also subject to interventions. For example, within-subject differ-
stress, and reductions in autonomic (cardiovascu- ences in Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory disposi-
lar) stress reactivity during the TSST were found tional mindfulness (Walach, Buchheld,
to correlate with the frequency of meditation Buttenmuller, Kleinknecht, & Schmidt, 2006)
practice among participants in a mindfulness- were found among subjects following a brief
integrated training program (Kemeny et al., 4-day mindfulness training intervention (Zeidan
2012). These results have been corroborated in an et al., 2011). Significant increases in trait mind-
investigation of the role of dispositional mindful- fulness were also observed on several FFMQ
ness on TSST-related heart rate variability subscales (Act with Awareness, Observe, and
responses (Holt, 2012). Non-Judging) among participants in an MBSR
Interestingly, recent electrocortical research intervention (Hlzel et al., 2011). Further, FFMQ
on dispositional mindfulness suggests that the mindfulness was found to mediate the relation
modulating role of this quality of attention on between formal mindfulness practice and mea-
emotional reactivity may begin very soon after sures of symptom reduction and well-being
stimulus contact. As noted earlier, Brown et al. (Carmody & Baer, 2008), suggesting that the
(2013) found that MAAS and FFMQ Act with increases in dispositional mindfulness resulting
Awareness scores predicted reduced event- from mindfulness practice were at least partially
related potential amplitudes following the dis- responsible for the adaptive outcomes. Similar
play of highly arousing unpleasant images, results have been found with the MAAS.
specifically in the late positive potentialan MBSR interventions have resulted in within-
electrocortical marker of emotional arousal and between-subject improvements in
beginning 400500 ms after stimulus onset. MAAS-assessed mindfulness in several recent
These findings suggest that dispositional mind- studies (e.g., Jensen, Vangkilde, Frokjaer, &
fulness plays an emotion regulatory role during Hasselbalch, 2012; Kilpatrick et al., 2011).
74 R.J. Goodman et al.

Finally, in a study of chronically depressed emotional and biological functioning in ways


individuals, Michalak, Heidenreich, Meibert, and consistent with current theory about mindfulness,
Schulte (2008) found that higher MAAS scores mindfulness training, and their effects.
at post-MBCT significantly predicted lower risk
of depressive relapse/recurrence up to a year fol-
lowing treatment. How Does Mindfulness Develop?
It has been argued (Grossman, 2011) that the
evidence for prepost treatment mindfulness Self-report measures of mindfulness have been
score changes only suggests that trainees are commonly used in research involving partici-
likely to endorse more mindfulness scale items at pants who have little or no formal training in
the end of training than at baseline. However mindfulness. The application of self-report mea-
Brown, Ryan, Loverich, Biegel, and West (2011) sures among untrained populations raises three
responded that in the common practice of pre questions concerning the development of mind-
post training assessment, participants are not fulness: (1) Are measures of trait mindfulness
likely to know what each scale they complete actually assessing mindful capacities if, as
actually measures, being administered as part of Grossman (2011) has argued, mindfulness is
a battery of similar scales. Further, some scales, dependent on formal training for its cultivation?
such as the MAAS, have not shown evidence of (2) If, as Kabat-Zinn (2003) and others have
social desirability or other response bias (Brown argued, mindfulness is an inherent capacity of the
& Ryan, 2003). Shapiro, Brown, Thoresen, and mind, why are some people more mindful than
Plante (2011) also showed that dispositional others, as reflected in mindfulness scale score
(MAAS) improvements were maintained up to a variance? And (3) How can research on the
year after MBSR training, when any enthusiastic development of mindfulness be used to inform
flush from the training had likely dissipated. our understanding of its enhancement?
Brown et al. (2011) also note that scores on self-
report scales can change in an unexpected direc-
tion after mindfulness training, as Brown, Kasser, Are Measures of Trait Mindfulness
Ryan, Alex Linley, and Orzech (2009) found with Assessing Mindful Capacities?
the FMI. Finally, that fact that changes in scaled
mindfulness have tracked changes in mental Jon Kabat-Zinn (2003), the founder of the MBSR
health outcomes over time in expected directions program writes that, mindfulness being about
(e.g., Brown & Ryan, 2003; Shapiro et al., 2011) attention, is also of necessity universal. There is
suggests that the increases in dispositional mind- nothing particularly Buddhist about it. We are all
fulness associated with mindfulness training are mindful to one degree or another, moment by
not only veridical, but also helpful in gauging moment. It is an inherent human capacity
whether training programs produce the key out- (p. 146). Consistent with this understanding,
come they are designed fornamely, cross-situ- Brown and Ryan (2003) write, Recognizing that
ationally stable mindfulness. most everyone has the capacity to attend and to
In sum, research to date supports the criterion be aware, we nonetheless assumethat individu-
validity of several psychometric measures of trait als differ in their propensity or willingness to be
mindfulness. It reveals that there are meaningful aware and to sustain attention to what is occur-
differences in mindfulness between untrained and ring in the present (p. 822). These views stand in
trained populations, and that variance in the dis- contrast to the argument that mindfulness is only
position predicts subjective and neurophysiologi- the result of practice, a state that cannot be easily
cal indicators of adaptive emotional functioning evoked among novices, and requires gradual
in ways consistent with experimental realizations refinement by means of systematic practiceand
of mindfulness. Perhaps most importantly, dispo- is markedly different from everyday modes of
sitional mindfulness appears to predict adaptive awareness (Grossman, 2011, p. 1035). In this
6 Burning Issues in Dispositional Mindfulness Research 75

view, only those who have engaged in specific, (2012) found greater increases in dispositional
formal mental training exhibit mindfulness. mindfulness among MBSR participants com-
In parallel with this disagreement within the pared to controls.
scientific community, there are also differing Yet factors outside the context of formal mind-
views among scholars on whether qualities such fulness training may also foster the development
as mindfulness are inherent to the human mind of mindfulness (Brown & Ryan, 2004). To date,
(e.g., Olendzki, 2011). Dunne (2011) suggests little research has sought to identify the physio-
these views fall along a spectrum from Innatist to logical, psychological, and socioenvironmental
Constructivist. The former view holds that, factors that may impact levels of dispositional
because qualities such as mindfulness are innate mindfulness. But incipient research suggests that
or inherent to the person, progress is marked by dispositional mindfulness may vary according to
removing factors that obscure them. Conversely, a number of factors, from genetics to caregiver-
Constructivists tend to emphasize the acquisition child attachment style to classroom educational
and construction of these qualities. supports. For example, Murakami, Matsunaga,
Is mindfulness a rarified state open only to and Ohira (2009) recently discovered that differ-
those undergoing training? Whether mindfulness ences in a serotonin transporter gene polymor-
is accessible to regular people and beginners, phism (5HTTLPR) modulated the effects of a
or can only be experienced by those trained in it brief mindfulness induction on parasympathetic
is a point of debate that will not be resolved here. nervous system activity under conditions of emo-
But evidence that this capacity can be enhanced tional provocation, suggesting that genetics may
through training is clearly consistent with view- contribute to the capacity to enter into a mind-
ing mindfulness as a natural capacity. As Brown fully attentive state.
et al. (2011) note, when it comes to mindfulness, While many kinds of learning and skill devel-
we all start somewhere. As discussed earlier, sev- opment are considered domain-specific, in that
eral measures of dispositional mindfulness pre- training in one domain does not influence perfor-
dict theoretically meaningful outcomes consistent mance in other domains, it has been theorized
with mindfulness theory and practice, even that training in attention may generalize to a vari-
among untrained respondents (see Brown et al., ety of functional domains (Posner & Rothbart,
2011 for further discussion). 2007). Consistent with this view, differences in
cognitive processing resulting from formal medi-
tation training have been shown to generalize to
Why Are Some People More Mindful performance on a variety of tasks, presumably
Than Others? because they target core processes such as atten-
tion (Jha, Krompinger, & Baime, 2007; Slagter,
Most scholars and mindfulness researchers agree Davidson, & Lutz, 2011) and working memory
that formal mindfulness practice is the royal (Jha et al., 2010). Given the ubiquity of these
road to stabilize higher mindfulness. Research core processes across a wide variety of tasks sug-
supports the claim that such practice is indeed gests that fundamental mindful capacities may be
related to higher mindfulness. For example, trained during activities that necessitate the
Brown and Ryan (2003) found that Zen medita- deployment of attention.
tors reported significantly higher mindfulness Social developmental factors such as care-
than age-, gender- and geographic location- giverchild attachment style may also relate to
matched non-meditators. In a randomized con- the development of mindfulness (Ryan, Brown,
trolled trial, Shapiro et al. (2011) found that & Creswell, 2007; Shaver, Lavy, Saron, &
MBSR participants had higher levels of mindful- Mikulincer, 2007). Shaver et al. (2007) pointed
ness than cohort controls, measured immediately out that mindfulness and attachment security are
after the intervention and at 2 and 12 months fol- associated with similar positive outcomes, and
lowing the intervention. Similarly, Jensen et al. those authors found that insecure attachment
76 R.J. Goodman et al.

styles were related to lower dispositional mind- that do not incorporate mindfulness (e.g., Piet &
fulness. While preliminary, this research sug- Hougaard, 2011; Segal et al., 2010). Many forms
gests that supports for the development of secure of psychotherapy encourage an open, receptive
attachment may sharpen mindful capacities. attention to even challenging aspects of personal
Social Emotional Learning (SEL), typically experience (Brown & Ryan, 2004; Martin, 1997),
promoted in school-based interventions, may be but interventions specifically incorporating mind-
functionally related to mindfulness. Among fulness may also target patterns of attention that
its five central competencies are self-awareness, are biased against aversive internal experiences.
self-management, and social awareness (Colla- This correction could ultimately lead to increased
borative for Academic, Social, and Emotional mindfulness.
Learning, 2005). A recent meta-analysis of 213 Widely regarded as one of the most important
SEL studies found greater social and emotional Buddhist documents on mindfulness practice, the
skills, as well as improved academic perfor- Satipahna Sutta is clear that mindful presence
mance in participants versus controls (Durlak, can be applied in the most ordinary of day-to-day
Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor, & Schellinger, tasks (Nanamoli & Bodhi, 1995). We suggest that
2011). Social modeling may also enhance mind- while formal mindfulness training is the most
fulness through exposure to the attentiveness of clearly marked path to develop mindfulness,
others (Ryan et al., 2007). For example, an other causes and conditions may also be at play.
adaptation of MBCT for use with children Dispositionally mindful individuals may experi-
(Semple, Lee, Rosa, & Miller, 2009) incorpo- ence several factors that promote the develop-
rates parent training to model mindful inten- ment of this quality. Research is needed to
tions and behavior at home. Children randomized discover these factors.
to this adaptation of MBCT exhibited fewer
attention problems than wait-listed controls,
and these changes were still present 3 months What Is the Relation Between
later. Along the same line, Goodman, Trapp, and Informally and Formally Developed
Davis (2015) found that perceived support for Mindfulness?
autonomy and competence in classroom con-
texts facilitated mindfulness among students There may also be a beneficial synergy between
before their final examination, which then pre- informally developed mindfulness and formal
dicted better exam performance. mindfulness training. Shapiro et al. (2011) found
While investigating developmental and con- that dispositional mindfulness moderated the
textual factors that may enhance natural capaci- relation between MBSR and its numerous benefi-
ties for mindfulness is important, it is also cial outcomes in a sample of undergraduate stu-
valuable to explore factors that could inhibit its dents. Those reporting higher dispositional
development. For example, dissociation and mindfulness before the MBSR program benefited
emotional numbing, experiences common to more from the intervention, evident in larger
post-traumatic stress disorder, have been consid- increases in mindfulness, higher subjective well-
ered antithetical to mindfulness (Follette, Palm, being, empathy, and hope, and larger decreases in
& Pearson, 2006). A history of psychological stress. Revisiting the distinction between the
symptoms, such as anxiety, depression, and per- Innatist versus Constructivist views on mindful-
ceived stress may be due to underlying factors, ness, the present discussion suggests a third pos-
including attention biases, that also impede the sibility, namely that informally developed
natural development of mindfulness (Harvey, mindfulness may support the effectiveness of
Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004). This may systematic training. Thus, dispositional mindful-
help to explain why clinical interventions incor- ness could be likened to a raw material, like a
porating mindfulness have shown as good or bet- gemstone, whose inherent qualities are enhanced
ter outcomes in some populations than treatments through careful efforts.
6 Burning Issues in Dispositional Mindfulness Research 77

Conclusion Bodhi, B. (2006). The nature of mindfulness and its role in


Buddhist meditation: A correspondence between B.
Alan Wallace and the Venerable Bhikkhu Bodhi.
Research on dispositional mindfulness has Unpublished manuscript, Santa Barbara Institute for
sparked a variety of contentious issues concern- Consciousness Studies, Santa Barbara, CA.
ing the definition, operationalization, validity, Bodhi, B. (2011). What does mindfulness really mean? A
canonical perspective. Contemporary Buddhism, 12,
and development of mindfulness. Here, we have
1939.
attempted to show how several individual differ- Brown, K. W., & Cordon, S. L. (2009). Toward a phenom-
ence measures of mindfulness have contributed enology of mindfulness: Subjective experience and
to the development of theory and to a growing emotional correlates. In F. Didonna (Ed.), Clinical
handbook of mindfulness (pp. 5981). New York, NY:
body of empirical findings on mindfulness. To
Springer.
better understand the nature and expression of Brown, K. W., Goodman, R. J., & Inzlicht, M. (2013).
trait mindfulness, research would do well to Dispositional mindfulness and the attenuation of neu-
examine attentional covariates of mindfulness in ral responses to emotional stimuli. Social Cognitive
and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 9399.
addition to the relations between mindfulness
Brown, K. W., Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., Alex Linley, P., &
and neurally mediated systems of attention Orzech, K. (2009). When what one has is enough:
(Posner & Petersen, 1990). Longitudinal studies Mindfulness, financial desire discrepancy, and subjec-
examining biological and contextual factors that tive well-being. Journal of Research in Personality,
43, 727736.
influence the development of dispositional mind-
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of
fulness may foster greater understanding of how being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychologi-
to foster this quality and improve mindfulness cal well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
training programs. These and other investigations Psychology, 84, 822848.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Perils and promise
may not only help to address outstanding ques-
in defining and measuring mindfulness: Observations
tions, but also create opportunities for research- from experience. Clinical Psychology: Science and
ers to expand and better integrate our knowledge Practice, 11, 242248.
of mindfulness, and thereby promote a more rig- Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., Loverich, T. M., Biegel,
G. M., & West, A. M. (2011). Out of the armchair and
orous science on the topic.
into the streets: Measuring mindfulness advances
knowledge and improves interventions: Reply to
Grossman (2011). Psychological Assessment, 23,
10411046.
References Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007).
Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence
Anlayo, B. (2003). Satipahna: The direct path to real- for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18,
ization. Cambridge, UK: Windhorse. 211237.
Anlayo, B. (2013). Mindfulness in early Buddhism. Brown, K. W., Weinstein, N., & Creswell, J. D. (2012).
Unpublished manuscript, University of Hamburg. Trait mindfulness modulates neuroendocrine and
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical inter- affective responses to social evaluative threat.
vention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 20372041.
Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125143. Cardaciotto, L., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Moitra, E.,
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & & Farrow, V. (2008). The assessment of
Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods present-moment awareness and acceptance: The
to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 2745. Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale. Assessment, 15,
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). 204223.
Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The Carlson, L. E., Speca, M., Faris, P., & Patel, K. (2007).
Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment, One year pre-post intervention follow-up of
11(3), 191206. psychological, immune, endocrine and blood pressure
Barnhofer, T., Chittka, T., Nightingale, H., Visser, C., & outcomes of mindfulness-based stress reduction
Crane, C. (2010). State effects of two forms of medita- (MBSR) in breast and prostate cancer patients. Brain,
tion on prefrontal EEG asymmetry in previously Behavior, and Immunity, 21, 10381049.
depressed individuals. Mindfulness, 1, 2127. Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2008). Relationships between
Bodhi, B. (2004). A systematic study of the Majjhima mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medi-
Nikaya: The Satipatthana Sutta [audio files]. cal and psychological symptoms and well-being in a
Retrieved from http://www.bodhimonastery.org/a- mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal
systematic-study-of-the-majjhima-nikaya.html. of Behavioral Medicine, 31, 2333.
78 R.J. Goodman et al.

Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (2011). Does mind- Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J.,
fulness training improve cognitive abilities? A system- McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A. K. (2007).
atic review of neuropsychological findings. Clinical Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation
Psychology Review, 31, 449464. reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Social
Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 313322.
Learning. (2005). Safe and sound: An educational Follette, V., Palm, K. M., & Pearson, A. N. (2006).
leaders guide to evidence-based social and emotional Mindfulness and trauma: Implications for treatment.
learning programsIllinois edition. Chicago, IL: Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior
Author. Therapy, 24, 4561.
Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J. A., Neufeld, R. W. J., Lane,
Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural correlates of dispo- R. D., Densmore, M., Stevens, T. K., & Lanius, R. A.
sitional mindfulness during affect labeling. (2010). Individual differences in trait mindfulness pre-
Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 560565. dict dorsomedial prefrontal and amygdala response
Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disor- during emotional imagery: An fMRI study. Personality
ders: Perspectives from affective neuroscience. and Individual Differences, 49, 479484.
Cognition and Emotion, 12, 307330. Gethin, R. (2015). Buddhist conceptualizations of mind-
Davidson, R. J. (2000). Affective style, psychopathology, fulness. In K. W. Brown, J. D. Creswell, & R. M. Ryan
and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. (Eds.), Handbook of mindfulness: Theory, research,
American Psychology, 55, 11961214. and practice. New York: Guilford.
Davidson, R. J. (2010). Empirical explorations of mind- Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness-
fulness: Conceptual and methodological conundrums. based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation
Emotion, 10, 811. in social anxiety disorder. Emotion, 10, 8391.
Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C., Senulis, J., & Goodman, R. J., Brown, K. W., & Haver, D. B. (2012).
Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approach/withdrawal and cere- Electrocortical evidence for the benefits of being pres-
bral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain ent: Individual differences in mindfulness and emotion
physiology. Journal of Personality and Social regulation predict resting frontal brain asymmetry.
Psychology, 58, 330341. Poster presented at the Society for Personality and
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Social Psychology Conference, San Diego, CA.
Rosenkrantz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., Goodman, R. J., Trapp, S. K., & Davis, J. L. (2015).
Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune Opening minds by supporting motivation needs:
function produced by mindfulness meditation. Autonomy and competence facilitate mindfulness and
Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564570. academic performance. Manuscript submitted for
Davis, J. H., & Thompson, E. (2015). Developing atten- publication.
tion and decreasing affective bias: Toward a cross- Gravetter, F. J., & Forzano, L.-A. B. (2009). Research
cultural cognitive science of mindfulness. In K. W. methods for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.).
Brown, J. D. Creswell, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
of mindfulness: Theory, research, and practice. Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regula-
New York: Guilford. tion: An integrative review. Review of General
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related atten- Psychology, 2, 271299.
tional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Grossman, P. (2011). Defining mindfulness by how poorly
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 225236. I think I pay attention during everyday awareness and
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stress- other intractable problems for psychologys (re)inven-
ors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration tion of mindfulness: Comment on Brown et al. (2011).
and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Psychological Assessment, 23, 10341040.
Bulletin, 130, 355391. Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H.
Dreyfus, G. (2011). Is mindfulness present-centered and (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health
non-judgmental? A discussion of the cognitive dimen- benefits. A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic
sions of mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, Research, 57, 3543.
4154. Harmon-Jones, E., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2011). Social
Dunne, J. (2011). Toward an understanding of non-dual neuroscience of asymmetrical frontal cortical activity:
mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 7188. Considering anger and approach motivation. In
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, A. Todorov, S. Fiske, & D. Prentice (Eds.), Social
R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of neuroscience: Toward understanding the underpin-
enhancing students social and emotional learning: A nings of the social mind (pp. 173187). New York,
meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. NY: Oxford University Press.
Child Development, 82, 405432. Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R.
Farb, N. A. S., Anderson, A. K., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., (2004). Cognitive behavioral processes across psy-
McKeon, D., & Segal, Z. V. (2010). Minding ones chological disorders: A transdiagnostic approach to
emotions: Mindfulness training alters the neural research and treatment. Oxford: Oxford University
expression of sadness. Emotion, 10, 2533. Press.
6 Burning Issues in Dispositional Mindfulness Research 79

Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. Martin, J. P. (1997). Mindfulness: A proposed common
(2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on factor. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 7,
anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. 291312.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, Merikle, P. M., & Joordens, S. (1997). Parallels between
169183. perception without attention and perception without
Holt, M. P. (2012). Dispositional mindfulness and cardio- awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 6(23),
vascular functioning under stress: Predictions of 219236.
social evaluative stress reactivity and recovery. Michalak, J., Heidenreich, T., Meibert, P., & Schulte, D.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Virginia (2008). Mindfulness predicts relapse/recurrence in
Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA. major depressive disorder after mindfulness-based
Hlzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Evans, K. C., Hoge, E. A., cognitive therapy. The Journal of Nervous and Mental
Dusek, J. A., Morgan, L., Pitman, R. K., & Lazar, Disease, 196, 630633.
S. W. (2010). Stress reduction correlates with struc- Modinos, G., Ormel, J., & Aleman, A. (2010). Individual
tural changes in the amygdala. Social Cognitive and differences in dispositional mindfulness and brain
Affective Neuroscience, 5, 1117. activity involved in reappraisal of emotion. Social
Hlzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C., Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 369377.
Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T., & Lazar, S. W. (2011). Moyer, C. A., Donnelly, M. P. W., Anderson, J. C., Valek,
Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional K. C., Huckaby, S. J, Wiederholt, D. A., Rice, B. L.
brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Research: (2011). Frontal electroencephalographic asymmetry
Neuroimaging, 191, 3642. associated with positive emotion is produced by very
Hlzel, B. K., Ott, U., Gard, T., Hempel, H., Weygandt, brief meditation training. Psychological Science, 22,
M., Morgen, K., & Vaitl, D. (2008). Investigation of 12771279.
mindfulness meditation practitioners with voxel-based Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012).
morphometry. Social Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness and mind-wandering: Finding conver-
Neuroscience, 3, 5561. gence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12,
Jensen, C. G., Vangkilde, S., Frokjaer, V., & Hasselbalch, 442448.
S. G. (2012). Mindfulness training affects attention Murakami, H., Matsunaga, M., & Ohira, H. (2009).
Or is it attentional effort? Journal of Experimental Association of serotonin transporter gene polymor-
Psychology, 141, 105123. phism and emotion regulation. NeuroReport, 20,
Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). 414418.
Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of atten- Nanamoli, B., & Bodhi, B. (1995). The middle length dis-
tion. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, courses of the Buddha: The Majjhima Nikaya. Boston,
7, 109119. MA: Wisdom Publications.
Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive con-
Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective effects trol of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9,
of mindfulness training on working memory capacity 242249.
and affective experience. Emotion, 10, 5464. Olendzki, A. (2011). The construction of mindfulness.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 5570.
wisdom of your mind to face stress, pain and illness. Piet, J., & Hougaard, E. (2011). The effect of mindfulness-
New York, NY: Dell. based cognitive therapy for prevention of relapse in
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: recurrent major depressive disorder: A systematic
Mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology
Hyperion. Review, 31, 10321040.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention sys-
in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical tem of the human brain. Annual Review of
Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144156. Neuroscience, 13, 2542.
Kemeny, M. E., Foltz, C., Cavanagh, J. F., Cullen, M., Posner, M. K., & Rothbart, M. I. (2007). Research on
Giese-Davis, J., Jennings, P., Ekman, P. (2012). attention networks as a model for the integration of
Contemplative/emotion training reduces negative psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology,
emotional behavior and promotes prosocial responses. 58, 123.
Emotion, 12, 338350. Quaglia, J. T., Brown, K. W., Lindsay, E. K., Creswell, J. D.,
Kilpatrick, L. A., Suyenobu, B. Y., Smith, S. R., Bueller, & Goodman, R. J. (2015). From conceptualization to
J. A., Goodman, T., Creswell, J. D., Naliboff, B. D. operationalization of mindfulness. In K. W. Brown,
(2011). Impact of mindfulness-based stress reduction J. D. Creswell, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
training on intrinsic brain connectivity. Neuroimage, mindfulness: Theory, research, and practice. New
56, 290298. York: Guilford Press.
Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness Quaglia, J. T., Goodman, R. J., & Brown, K. W. (2015).
are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 1218. Trait mindfulness predicts efficient top-down attention
Linehan. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of bor- to and discrimination of facial expressions. Journal of
derline personality disorder. New York: Guilford. Personality. doi:10.1111/jopy.12167.
80 R.J. Goodman et al.

Quirk, G. J., & Beer, J. S. (2006). Prefrontal involvement amygdala and caudate volumes in community
in the regulation of emotion: Convergence of rat and adults. PLoS One, 8(5), e64574. doi:10.1371/journal.
human studies. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, pone.0064574.
723727. Taren, A. et al. (2013). Neural pathways of mindfulness
Ryan, R. M., Brown, K. W., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). and stress reduction: Amygdala functional connectiv-
How integrative is attachment theory? Unpacking the ity covaries with dispositional mindfulness and
meaning and significance of felt security. Psychological changes with mindfulness meditation training.
Inquiry, 18, 177182. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Segal, Z. V., Bieling, P., Young, T., MacQueen, G., Cooke, Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., Jackson, D. C.,
R., Martin, L., Levitan, R. D. (2010). Antidepressant Dalton, K. M., Mueller, C. J., Davidson, R. J.
monotherapy vs. sequential pharmacotherapy and (2004). Making a life worth living: Neural correlates
mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, or placebo, for of well-being. Psychological Science, 15, 367372.
relapse prophylaxis in recurrent depression. Archives Urry, H. L., van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Kalin,
of General Psychiatry, 67, 12561264. N. H., Thurow, M. E., Schafer, H. S., Davidson,
Segal, Z. V., Teasdale, J. D., & Williams, J. M. (2004). R. J. (2006). Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: Theoretical cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of neg-
rationale and empirical status. In S. C. Hayes, V. M. ative affect and predict diurnal pattern of cortisol
Follete, & M. M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and secretion among older adults. Journal of Neuroscience,
acceptance. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. 26, 44154425.
Semple, R. J., Lee, J., Rosa, D., & Miller, L. F. (2009). A Walach, H., Buchheld, N., Buttenmuller, V., Kleinknecht,
randomized trial of mindfulness-based cognitive ther- N., & Schmidt, S. (2006). Measuring mindful-
apy for children: Promoting mindful attention to nessThe Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory
enhance social-emotional resiliency in children. (FMI). Personality and Individual Differences, 40,
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19, 218229. 15431555.
Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K. W., Thoresen, C., & Plante, Walsh, W. B., & Betz, N. E. (2001). Tests and assessment
T. G. (2011). The moderation of mindfulness-based (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
stress reduction effects by trait mindfulness: Results Way, B. M., Creswell, J. D., Eisenberger, N. I., &
from a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Dispositional mindfulness
Psychology, 67, 267277. and depressive symptomatology: Correlations with
Shaver, P. R., Lavy, S., Saron, C. D., & Mikulincer, M. limbic and self-referential neural activity during rest.
(2007). Social foundations of the capacity for mindful- Emotion, 10, 1224.
ness: An attachment perspective. Psychological Witek-Janusek, L., Albuquerque, K., Rambo Chroniak,
Inquiry, 18, 264271. K., Chroniak, C., Durazo-Arvizu, R., & Mathews,
Slagter, H. A., Davidson, R. J., & Lutz, A. (2011). H. (2008). Effect of mindfulness based stress
Mental training as a tool in the neuroscientific study reduction on immune function, quality of life and
of brain and cognitive plasticity. Frontiers in Human coping in women newly diagnosed with early stage
Neuroscience, 5, 1217. breast cancer. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 22,
Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., 969981.
Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term meditation training Zeidan, F., Martucci, K. T., Kraft, R. A., Gordon, N. S.,
improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of McHaffie, J. G., & Coghill, R. C. (2011). Brain
the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1715217156. mechanisms supporting the modulation of pain by
Taren, A., Creswell, J. D., & Gianaros, P. (2013). mindfulness meditation. Journal of Neuroscience, 41,
Dispositional mindfulness co-varies with smaller 55405548.
The Why, What, and How
of Healthy Self-Regulation: 7
Mindfulness and Well-Being
from a Self-Determination Theory
Perspective

Patricia P. Schultz and Richard M. Ryan

To be a self-regulating being requires awareness. with their self-endorsed values and authentic
We live in a world with stimulating enticements, interests. Extensive research has shown the sig-
myriad distractions, and continuously arising and nificant physical and mental benefits that such
competing motives and desires. Letting some autonomous regulation yields. In contrast, when
pass, and acting on those that are most congruent people engage in activities based in introjections
with living well, is a formidable task. Doing so or external pressures, the regulation of their
entails a center of observation outside the fray, behavior is controlled, and such regulation is
and a capacity to make use of it. associated with diminished persistence and per-
Self-determination theory (SDT; Ryan & formance, and more impoverished experience
Deci, 2000), akin to many other philosophical and well-being (Ryan & Deci, 2000). The dis-
traditions and psychological frameworks, views tinctions between the autonomous and controlled
awareness as a critical component of healthy self- reasons underlying peoples behaviors consti-
regulation and well-being (Deci & Ryan, 1980, tutes the why approach of SDT.
1985; Ryan, Huta, & Deci, 2008). According to Additionally, within SDT, processes associated
SDT, awareness is central to the process of with awareness impact the content, or the what
healthy regulation. When people act with auton- of peoples goals. Individuals can focus on attain-
omy, they engage in behaviors that are congruent ing extrinsic goals (e.g., wealth, popularity, attrac-
tive image) or intrinsic goals (e.g., personal
growth, community contributions, close relation-
ships). The evidence reveals differential conse-
quences of both the pursuit and attainment of
P.P. Schultz (*)
Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in
extrinsic versus intrinsic goals, especially with
Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, regard to well-being (e.g., Kasser & Ryan, 1993,
NY 14627, USA 1996). Both the why and the what of behavior
e-mail: patricia.schultz@rochester.edu regulation are predictive of more optimal develop-
R.M. Ryan ment, social relationships, and wellness (Deci &
Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Ryan, 2000). Specifically, by acting autonomously
Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester,
NY 14627, USA
and pursuing intrinsic goals people can live eudai-
monically, with the rich positive experience that
Institute for Positive Psychology and Education,
Australian Catholic University, Strathfield, NSW,
attends living well (Ryan, Curren, & Deci, 2013).
Australia In this chapter we therefore address an impor-
e-mail: ryan@psych.rochester.edu tant how of eudaimonic living, namely how

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 81


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_7
82 P.P. Schultz and R.M. Ryan

both more autonomous regulation and more ment of these basic needs facilitate intrinsic
intrinsic goal selection are facilitated by the open motivation, integrated self-regulation, and wellness;
and receptive awareness that defines the construct whereas need thwarting environments, lifestyles,
of mindfulness (Brown & Ryan, 2003). To do so or activities are associated with antagonist out-
we present a brief overview of SDT, delineate the comes (Deci & Ryan, 2012).
construct of mindfulness, and review evidence of
how this quality of consciousness relates to both
the regulatory processes through which behav- A Differentiated View of Motivation:
iors are enacted, and the content of goals that The Why of Self-Regulation
individuals pursue.
Unlike many theories that conceptualize motiva-
tion as a unitary construct, SDT specifies differ-
Self-Determination Theory ent types of motivation underlying behavior
regulation. First, SDT posits that humans have
SDT is an empirically driven theory of human evolved to be liberally endowed with intrinsic
motivation and development that posits that motivation, or behavior energized by its inherent
humans are inherently active, curious, and satisfactions. Intrinsic motivation is typified by
growth-oriented creatures who naturally strive activities such as play, exploration, sport and lei-
toward both the integration of a coherent and uni- sure reading, in which people exercise capacities
fied sense of self, and the integration of the indi- and experience growth. Intrinsic motivation is
vidual within a broader social framework (Deci also a prototype of human autonomy, in that
& Ryan, 2000). Similar to other organismic theo- intrinsically motivated activities are invariantly
ries of personality development, SDT postulates experienced as self-determined, or volitional.
that people spontaneously seek challenges, pur- Yet, despite the importance of intrinsic moti-
sue interests, and strive for social connectedness vation for learning and development, most daily
(Ryan, 1995). Through the extension of their activities are not intrinsically motivated, but
inherent capacities and integrated motives, indi- rather are instrumental in nature. That is, much of
viduals more fully actualize their human poten- our behavior is extrinsically motivated. Although
tials and experience more eudaimonic lifestyles many researchers have viewed extrinsic motiva-
(Ryan, Huta, et al., 2008; Ryan et al., 2013). tion as heteronomously driven or controlled (e.g.,
Yet within the SDT perspective, this natural deCharms, 1968), SDT has long proposed that
inclination toward growth and integration is not extrinsic motivation is more complex. SDT
viewed as by any means automatic, or even a instead describes a spectrum model of extrinsic
typically smooth, developmental pathway. Just regulations spanning from highly controlled to
like a seed needs critical nutrients to flourish, the highly autonomous forms of self-regulation
integrative processes of human psychological (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000).
development require specific supports. In SDT The most controlled form of regulation in this
the most critical of these are described by the spectrum is external regulation, which pertains to
concept of basic psychological needs (Deci & behaviors that are enacted to comply with exter-
Ryan, 2000; Ryan, 1995), which represent the nal contingencies, both punishing and rewarding.
cross-developmental and culturally universal External regulation can drive behavior, but it is
necessities for growth and wellness. SDT speci- often poorly maintained, and/or accompanied by
fies three basic needs: competence (i.e., mastery negative affect. A closely related form of extrin-
and efficacy), autonomy (i.e., volition and self- sic motivation is introjected regulation.
endorsement of ones behaviors), and relatedness Introjected acts are performed to experience self-
(i.e., a sense of belonging and of being cared for). and/or other-contingent approval. Although
Substantial research shows that social contexts internally regulated, introjected behaviors are
and personal relationships that support the fulfill- experienced as controlled because the individual
7 Mindfulness and Self-Determination 83

feels pressured to avoid guilt and/or projected and more relevant to our focus in this paper,
disapproval, or conversely to garner esteem and internal processes are also important. At the core
ego-enhancement. Introjection can be a powerful of volitional and self-endorsed regulation lies the
motivator (e.g., Ryan, Koestner, & Deci, 1991), capacity to reflectively consider ones behavior
but it comes with costs such as unstable persis- and its congruency with ones personal values
tence, stress, and lower well-being. and needs (Ryan & Deci, 2006). That is, the exis-
On the more autonomous end of this spectrum tential commitment to act consistently with ones
is identified regulation, in which the individual authentic self, and be sensitive to external cir-
personally embraces and reflectively endorses his cumstances, impacts the regulatory process.
or her actions. As a result, identified regulation is When awareness of inner and outer circum-
associated with more persistence, better perfor- stances is blocked, so too is a persons ability to
mance, and more positive affect than controlled attend to prompts arising from basic needs, to
forms of regulation. The most autonomous form mobilize resilience, and to consciously self-
of extrinsic motivation is integrated regulation, organize and regulate actions (Ryan, Legate,
in which the person has fully assimilated her Niemiec, & Deci, 2012). Thus, mindfulness, con-
identified regulation into the self, coordinating it ceptualized as internal and external awareness of
with other values and goals. Like intrinsically the present moment, is a quality of consciousness
motivated actions, integrated actions are highly that has been shown to lead to greater autonomy
autonomous and wholeheartedly engaged. But (Brown & Ryan, 2003). When mindful, people
integrated regulations are still technically extrin- are aware of what is truly taking place and thus
sic because they are undertaken to attain out- better able to make purposeful decisions, engage
comes separable from the actions themselves. their self-compatibility checker (Kuhl &
Typically it is only through self-reflection and Kazen, 1994), and function in a more integrated
awareness that one is able to recognize ones own way. Yet before elaborating on this, we turn next
values and needs, and therefore able to bring new to what people are pursuing in their lives, or their
regulations into such congruence. Thus, as we goals contents, which is also affected by
shall detail, mindfulness is a crucial ingredient mindfulness.
for the integrated regulation of behavior, and
truly autonomous extrinsic self-regulation.
At this point hundreds of studies have verified: Intrinsic Versus Extrinsic Aspirations:
(a) that this spectrum of regulations forms a con- The What of Behavioral Regulation
tinuum of autonomy (e.g. Roth, Assor,
KanatMaymon, & Kaplan, 2007; Ryan & Connell, Although so far we have emphasized the why
1989); and (b) that more effective behavioral reg- of self-regulation (i.e., the regulatory processes
ulation and enhanced well-being are associated through which outcomes are pursued), SDT
with higher relative autonomy in multiple domains also concerns the what of self-regulation, or
(e.g., Blais, Sabourin, Boucher, & Vallerand, the content of the outcomes or goals pursued
1990; Gagn & Deci, 2005; Reeve & Jang, 2006; and their associated well-being consequences.
Ryan, Patrick, Deci, & Williams, 2008). Research in a number of cultures has shown
that the pursuit and attainment of intrinsic goals
Internal Promoters of Autonomous (i.e., goals such as personal growth, affiliation,
Self-Regulation or community contributions) have a positive
Taking into account the manifold positive per- relation to basic need satisfaction and a variety
sonal consequences of intrinsic regulation and of well-being outcomes. In contrast, extrinsic
more autonomous forms of extrinsic regulation, goals and aspirations (i.e., goals that reflect
considerable research within SDT has focused on extrinsic values, such as wealth, image, or
the social contextual factors that affect relative fame) are related to need thwarting and accord-
autonomy (see Ryan & Deci, 2000). Nonetheless, ingly, greater ill-being (Kasser, 2002; Kasser &
84 P.P. Schultz and R.M. Ryan

Ryan, 1993, 1996). Studies have also found that (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Awareness is the
the attainment of intrinsic goals predicts more background tracking system of consciousness,
long-lasting well-being compared to the monitoring an individuals internal and external
achievement of extrinsic goals (Vansteenkiste, happenings. Attention, on the other hand, is the
Ryan, & Deci, 2008). mechanism of focusing on a particular stimulus
Why do these two types of aspiration have present in the conscious awareness (Westen,
vastly different consequences? The explanation 1999). Therefore, an individual can be aware of a
lies in the extent to which they ultimately facili- plethora of stimuli, but it is the attention that
tate or undermine basic psychological need satis- directs what, within the ground of awareness,
faction (Ryan, Sheldon, Kasser, & Deci, 1996). is in focus.
Intrinsic goals are more closely associated with As is often the case, differing schools of
the satisfaction of fundamental needs. For exam- thought highlight various aspects of mindfulness
ple, willingly giving to others, which is associ- both theoretically and operationally (Dimidjian
ated with relationship and community goals, & Linehan, 2003; Hayes & Wilson, 2003). Brown
leaves one with a sense of autonomy (because the and Ryans definition of mindfulness as receptive
act is volitional), competence (because one is attention and awareness differs from other
having a positive impact) and relatedness approaches that include within the idea of mind-
(because one is connecting); these in turn foster fulness qualities such as acceptance (Bishop
enhanced well-being. In contrast, extrinsic aspi- et al., 2004); active cognitive operations on exter-
rations are more closely related to obtaining nal stimuli (Langer, 1989); diminished self-talk,
external approval or visible signs of worth, and non-judgment, and non-doing (Leary & Tate,
thus are generally less likely to directly provide 2007), or holding a particular set of philosophi-
need satisfaction, or may even distract from it, cal, ethical or therapeutic beliefs (Baer, Smith,
leading to greater ill-being (e.g., Niemiec, Ryan, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006). All of
& Deci, 2009; Vansteenkiste et al., 2007; these qualities are indeed associated with mind-
Weinstein & Ryan, 2010). In the following sec- ful states (Brown & Ryan, 2004; Brown, Ryan, &
tions, we will review how mindfulness fuels both Creswell, 2007a, 2007b), but from the viewpoint
the movement toward more autonomous reasons of Brown and Ryan, these other elements all fol-
for acting, and the pursuit of intrinsic goal con- low from a truly open and receptive awareness,
tents. But first, it is important to precisely define and many are consequences of mindfulness.
mindfulness, and differentiate it from concepts Regardless of the specific definition or measures,
that are sometimes mistakenly confused with it. however, research conducted over the last three
decades on dispositional mindfulness, induced
mindful states, and mindfulness training inter-
The Construct of Mindfulness ventions has demonstrated the benefits of this
attribute of consciousness for a wide range of
Within the theoretical framework of SDT we outcomes, such as psychological and physical
have drawn on the definition and measurement of health, and the quality of ones relationships and
mindfulness introduced by Brown and Ryan performance in multiple domains (e.g., Baer,
(2003). In their approach mindfulness is defined 2003, 2006; Brown et al., 2007a; Grossman,
as a receptive state of mind wherein attention, Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004).
informed by a sensitive awareness of what is
occurring at the moment, plainly observes inter-
nal (e.g., psychological and somatic experiences) Mindfulness Versus Self-Awareness
and external events that are taking place (Brown
& Ryan, 2003; Kabat-Zinn, 2003). Attention and It is important to differentiate between the con-
awareness, key elements in defining mindfulness, cept of mindfulness from the constructs of self-
are important components of consciousness awareness and reflexive consciousness, which
7 Mindfulness and Self-Determination 85

have received much attention (e.g., Buss, 1980; larly the aspect of self-reflectiveness (Trapnell
Carver & Scheier, 1981). In a nutshell, such theo- & Campbell, 1999), has been related to maladap-
ries describe self-awareness in terms of knowl- tive outcomes and poorer mental health.
edge about the self. For example, private
self-consciousness is described as a tendency to
be highly aware of internal states (Carver & Self-Regulation and Mindfulness:
Scheier, 1981). Nonetheless, this type of self- The Why
consciousness can reflect any type of internal
state, including negative reactions or evaluative We have argued that the motives underlying
dispositions toward internal or external events behavior are closely related to the quality of
rather than receptive openness. Similarly, reflex- engagement as well as to wellness consequences;
ive consciousness (e.g., Baumeister, 1999) refers such that those who pursue authentic interests
to metacognitive processes that operate within and values are overall more vital and healthy. Yet,
thoughts, emotions, and other contents of con- to pursue this trajectory of integration and auton-
sciousness, and thus can involve varied cognitive omy, SDT posits that people are aided by envi-
and intellectual operations and biases. ronments that support the fulfillment of needs for
Mindfulness is, in contrast, prereflexive and autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Support
has at its root a perceptual and non-evaluative for autonomy plays a particularly key role. This
character: it means to simply and openly observe is evident in the fact that although support for
current events or engage in bare attention. It is relatedness and competence may foster the inter-
not thoughts or cognitions, but rather the space nalization of a behavior (i.e., ones adoption of a
between them that sets the context where they regulation or value), these supports by them-
occur (Brown et al., 2007b). Drawing from a Zen selves are not enough to promote integration,
metaphor, this quality of consciousness is like a which is a process necessary for true self-
polished mirror, merely reflecting what passes regulation (e.g., Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, & Leone,
before it, without distortions or conceptual 1994; Grolnick & Ryan, 1989; Williams & Deci,
thoughts. The separation between consciousness 1996). Hence, for integration to occur, people
(context) and mental content, also referred to as need to freely process and endorse their motives
decentering and desensitization (Martin, 1997), and regulations, as well as synthesize their mean-
allows more autonomous self-regulation because ing with other aspects of self, that is, feel the
behavior is informed by authentic awareness, autonomy need satisfied.
rather than distorted self-cognitions. To be clear, Because mindfulness relates to ones capacity
this unbiased receptivity is not an aloofness or to openly attend to current internal and external
disconnection with the world, but rather a more experiences, it enables and supports the self-
alert participation in the ongoing process of liv- insight and the self-reflection necessary for
ing (Gunaratana, 2002, p. 142). ensuring ones values are in accordance with
Empirical evidence supports these distinctions ones behavior. Furthermore, it builds a frame-
between mindfulness and self-awareness. work that aids the blend of that particular behav-
Research shows at best weak, and sometimes ior with values that are already part of the self.
negative, relations of mindfulness and indicators This awareness is an important substrate of inte-
of self-awareness, including private self- gration, and therefore a critical ingredient of
consciousness, public self-consciousness, reflec- autonomy development (Deci & Ryan, 1985). In
tion, and self-monitoring (Beitel, Ferrer, & other words, one can only be highly autonomous
Cecero, 2005; Brown & Ryan, 2003). More rele- when one is clearly aware of ones values and
vant to the present discussion, whereas mindful- goals, and thus is able to engage in behaviors that
ness has been associated with adaptive outcomes are congruent with ones true self, free from
and psychological health (e.g., Baer, 2003; external pressures or internal distortions or
Brown & Ryan, 2003), self-awareness, particu- judgments.
86 P.P. Schultz and R.M. Ryan

Aiming to empirically demonstrate the a substantial part of our day-to-day cognitive,


previous argument, Brown and Ryan (2003) con- emotional, and overt behavior does not require
ducted the first studies that explicitly investigated conscious awareness and attention. In other
mindfulness and related it to the SDT framework. words, a good deal of our thoughts and actions
The authors developed the Mindful Attention occur automatically without intentional effort
Awareness Scale (MAAS) to measure this con- (Bargh, 1997; Deci & Ryan, 1980; Tart, 1994).
struct both as a disposition and as a state. They Despite the many pragmatic benefits for this
demonstrated that the MAAS was positively automaticity of behavior (e.g., ones speed in
correlated with both dispositional and state response to situational demands, or the greater
autonomy, as well as with the other two basic availability of cognitive resources for more rele-
needs described by SDT, relatedness and compe- vant tasks; Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2000; Mitchell,
tence. Moreover, to understand the role of Nosek, & Banaji, 2003), there are also costly
mindfulness in regular day-to-day living, they consequences. When acting non-consciously one
conducted additional investigations using experi- is more susceptible to engage in many habitual
ence-sampling procedures. Both student and problematic and self-defeating behaviors, which
working adult samples, after having completed a if reflected upon are not congruent with ones
measure of trait mindfulness, were assessed for self-endorsed values (e.g., Clark & Rhyno, 2005;
state mindfulness, affect, and the relative auton- Levesque & Brown, 2007; Ryan & Deci, 2006;
omy of their behavior at the receipt of a pager Verplanken & Velsvik, 2008). Mindfulness,
signal, sent three times a day on a quasi-random through awareness and attention, pulls people
basis. In both samples, higher levels of both closer to what is currently taking place, without
dispositional and state mindfulness predicted judgmental or evaluative attachments. This awak-
more autonomous activity in daily life and lower ened state allows consciousness to become clear
levels of unpleasant affects. Interestingly, the and fresh, which, in turn, acts as a liberating
effects of trait and state mindfulness on auton- agent of conditioned responses, and allows peo-
omy were independent, suggesting that even ple to better reflect upon the why of actions,
momentary experiences of mindfulness contrib- thus promoting more self-endorsed, autonomous
ute to more volitional self-regulation and emo- behavior (Brown et al., 2007a). This connects
tional well-being. with evidence showing that enhanced attention
These positive relations between autonomous and awareness can prevent the enactment of auto-
regulation and mindfulness beg the question of matic habits or reactions (e.g., Dijksterhuis &
the mechanisms and processes through which Knippenberg, 2000). For example, Gollwitzer
these relations occur. We shall now discuss pro- (1999) experimentally induced individuals to be
cesses through which they may obtain, namely: aware of their automatic stereotypic beliefs
(1) interference with automatic maladaptive toward elderly people, and found that subse-
behaviors that are incongruent with ones quently these stereotypic thoughts were less
endorsed values; (2) promotion of less ego- readily automatically triggered. In this same way,
involvement and cognitive distortion, freeing the mindfulness has been shown to be a protective
self from internal and external pressures. factor against automatization of behavior, thus
leading to more self-determined, autonomous
regulation.
De-automatizing and Integrative Levesque and Brown (2007) investigated the
Role of Mindfulness role of mindfulness as a moderator between
implicit regulation (assessed using the Implicit
One of the pathways for the salutary effects of Association TestIAT; Greenwald, McGhee, &
mindfulness is that it may decrease the likelihood Schwartz, 1998) and explicit regulation of day-
of automatic maladaptive behaviors. Research on to-day behavior using an experience-sampling
automatic and implicit processes has shown that strategy. Both implicit and explicit measures
7 Mindfulness and Self-Determination 87

assessed the degree to which the participants To add to these findings, another study by
regulation was autonomous or controlled. Results Brown and Ryan (2003) found that mindfulness
showed that implicit regulation style predicted was associated with emotional intelligence
day-to-day regulation only for those lower in dis- (Salovey, Mayer, Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai,
positional mindfulness. That is, for those high in 1995), specifically in a dimension that is closely
mindfulness, the degree of daily autonomy was associated with emotional self-knowledge: clar-
relatively high independently of implicit auton- ity of emotional experience. Such self-insight is
omy level; in other words, it was high even when thought to be a crucial result of the present-
participants implicitly associated themselves centered awareness essential for self-regulation
with pressure and control. These results under- and more integrated functioning.
score the de-automatizing role of mindfulness,
overriding maladaptative tendencies and catalyz-
ing self-endorsed behavior. This is thought to Liberating Role of Mindfulness:
occur because mindfulness acts as a brake or A Thought Is Just a Thought
redirector between salient primes and responses
to them (Deci & Ryan, 1980). Open attention and awareness also foster autono-
Focusing on this integrative aspect of mind- mous behavior because they can help free indi-
fulness, Brown and Ryan (2003) reported that viduals from the external and internal controlling
those higher in trait mindfulness showed greater forces that are alien to the authentic self (Brown
congruence between implicit or non-conscious et al., 2007a). As Hodgins and Knee (2002) put
emotional state (assessed using the IAT; it, Individuals who are functioning autono-
Greenwald et al., 1998) and explicit self-reported mouslyare responsive to reality rather than
counterpart. Given that implicit measures are not directed by ego-invested preconceived notions
controlled by consciousness, these results indi- (p. 89). Mindful individuals have an observant
cate that more mindful individuals are more stance toward experience rather than a cognitive
attuned to their implicit emotions demonstrated reflexive stance that constantly informs thoughts
by greater concordance with the analogous about the self. Stated differently, they recognize
explicit self-descriptions. Note that, in the case the self as a process of integration and assimi-
of emotions, this last study suggests that mind- lation and not as the product of self-evaluations.
fulness is related to a greater association between This raises the question of self: What is self
implicit and explicit emotional states; whereas and how can we formally define it?
when it comes to motivation, mindfulness seems Scholars from many fields, echoing the cen-
to be associated with a greater dissociation trality that the construct presents for the human
between maladaptive implicit motivational ori- experience, have been concerned with the con-
entation and adaptive behavioral motivation. cept of self (Brown, Ryan, Creswell, &
These results are not contradictory because in Niemiec, 2008). The two notions of self that pre-
both cases mindfulness appears to provide a dominate today are the I self (McAdams,
space for reflection: individuals who are more 1990), mainly studied within organismic and
attuned to their emotions are in a better position developmental theories (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 1991;
to be in touch with their true selves and behave Loevinger & Blasi, 1991); and the Me self at
genuinely; individuals who have an implicit ten- the core of social constructionist views of self.
dency toward controlled motivation, but are able The Me self or self-as-object refers to the
to stop and reframe their course of action, may creation of personal identity, and is derived from
behave more autonomously. Thus regardless of the Mead-Cooley tradition (Ryan, 1993; Ryan &
the direction of the relationship between implicit Deci, 2011). This concept of self concerns ones
and explicit measures, this open awareness identification with specific roles, attributes, group
works as a facilitator of integration and authentic memberships, and beliefs, which individuals are
self-regulation. often motivated to protect or enhance. Under this
88 P.P. Schultz and R.M. Ryan

view of self, people strive to meet the standards true self-regulation or autonomy (Brown, Ryan,
associated with these internalized and culturally et al., 2008; Martin & Erber, 2005; Ryan &
derived-self images (Ryan & Brown, 2003). Brown, 2003). Expressed differently, for highly
When maintaining such self-representations mindful people, rejections or successes do not
becomes a predominant goal, it brings with it a involve their self-worth and are not destabilizing
variety of anxieties, conflicts, and defensive reac- rather, are simply seen as part of the personal
tions (Brown, Ryan, et al., 2008). growth process, a hallmark of the I self. As a
On the other hand, the I self or self-as- result, this quieting of the ego allows them to
process view construes self not as a concept or behave volitionally and without the need to
set of self-evaluations, but rather as the inherent prove, maintain, or stay attached to conceptions
integrative tendencies of people to grow and cre- of the Me-self, which is a pathway toward inte-
ate coherence in their experiences. Behavior grated functioning (Niemiec, Ryan, & Brown,
motivated by the I self is more fully self- 2008; Ryan & Rigby, 2015).
endorsed and whole-hearted, and is more likely Several studies support the argument that
to be positively experienced, whereas behavior mindfulness, because of its grounding in reality
motivated by the Me self is more closely and detachment from ego-involved contingen-
related to controlled regulation (Ryan et al., cies and self-centered biases, attenuates reactiv-
2012). Mindfulness, with its open attention to the ity to threatening or stressful situations (see
present moment, promotes the I self regulation Baer, 2003; Brown, Ryan, et al., 2008). As a
and its synthetic tendencies by a greater allowing prime example, Niemiec and colleagues (2010)
of and interest in what is occurring, and by free- conducted several experiments informed by
ing the individual from evaluative mental con- Terror Management Theory (TMT; Greenberg,
cepts, ego-involvement, and self-centered biases, Solomon, & Pyszczynski, 1997). TMT posits
all characteristics of the Me self (Ryan & that humans respond defensively to reminders
Rigby, 2015). of death (e.g., through suppression of death
There is growing evidence linking mindful- thoughts, self-esteem enhancement, and defense
ness with both (1) less ego-involvement and of their cultural worldview). In seven experi-
defensive reactions, and lower stress appraisals ments, Niemiec et al. demonstrated that trait
and (2) more autonomous self-regulation. First, mindfulness mitigates defensive responses to
many studies using the SDT framework demon- existential threat. Moreover, these experiments
strated how ego-investment in outcomes impedes showed that this effect was due to the fact that
autonomy, increases pressure and tension, and those higher in mindfulness more fully pro-
lowers vitality (e.g., Nix, Ryan, Manly, & Deci, cessed death thoughts in an immediate sense,
1999; Ryan, 1982). In addition, other investiga- and because of that, were less likely to be hold-
tions showed how actions performed mindfully ing on to threat and behaving defensively in
are less likely to be driven by ego-concerns, and subsequent moments. These studies connect
less prone to high stress reactions (Kernis & with other research on relationship conflicts
Goldman, 2006; Niemiec et al., 2010; Ryan & (Barnes, Brown, Krusemark, Campbell, &
Brown, 2003; Weinstein, Brown, & Ryan, 2009). Rogge, 2007); social exclusion (Creswell,
As Brown, Ryan, et al. (2008) describe, mindful- Eisenberg, & Lieberman, 2008), emotional
ness entails a shift in the locus of personal sub- threat (Arch & Craske, 2006), and other ego
jectivity from conceptual representations of the threats by showing that an open, mindful pro-
self and others to awareness itself (p. 82). As a cessing of situations promotes fewer defensive
consequence, it lessens the intra- and inter- reactions to adverse situations, and, in turn,
personal pressures that the Me-self entails, and increases adaptive responses and well-being
liberates the individual from the often automatic (Brown, Ryan, et al., 2008).
cognitive distortions and defensive reactions that In another example Weinstein et al. (2009)
can disrupt the integrative process underlying demonstrated how mindfulness, by fostering less
7 Mindfulness and Self-Determination 89

defensive thinking patterns and more openness Mindfulness as a Pathway to Intrinsic


toward challenging events, promotes less nega- Goal Orientation
tive cognitive appraisals of situations, and
reduced levels of perceived stress. Across four A growing body of research suggests that mind-
studies that included experimental, longitudinal, fulness increases ones focus on intrinsic aspira-
and experience sampling designs, the authors tions, resulting in greater well-being and healthier
found that mindful individuals made more benign lifestyle decisions (Brown & Kasser, 2005).
stress appraisals, and reported more adaptive Mindfulness has also been associated with greater
stress responses, and these, in turn, fully or par- empathy and compassion for others (Beitel et al.,
tially mediated the relations between mindful- 2005; Brown et al., 2007a). For example, Shapiro,
ness and well-being. For example, in a Schwartz, and Bonner (1998) reported that medi-
longitudinal study, first-semester freshmen cal students who received mindfulness training
within a single course completed measures of displayed increases in empathy over time relative
mindfulness, cognitive appraisal, coping, and ill- to a control group, even in high-stress contexts
being at three points during the semester: at the such as finals week. Barnes et al. (2007), in an
beginning, 12 days before the mid-term, and investigation of the role of mindfulness in roman-
during final exams. The longitudinal design tic relationship stress, found that it might enhance
underscored that more mindful individuals healthy romantic relationship functioning. The
tended to appraise challenging situations as less authors suggest that these benefits stem from an
threatening, and respond in more adaptive ways inclination to other-centeredness or a greater dis-
over time. position to be present to the partner, even in chal-
In sum, it appears that open and receptive lenging situations. Mindfulness has also been
observation of internal and external events can related to the promotion of intrinsic values such
lead to positive outcomes. Moreover, when as community involvement, relationships, eco-
behavior is not driven by ego-concerns, it is more logical stewardship, and lower materialism over
likely to be congruent and authentic. An alterna- extrinsic values such as popularity and wealth
tive but not antithetical explanation is that mind- (Brown & Kasser, 2005; Brown, Kasser, Ryan, &
fulness, in its ability to enhance self-endorsed Konow, 2008).
behavior, may prompt a more selective choice of Pertinent to this discussion is a study con-
situations (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Weinstein ducted by Brown and Kasser (2005) on a diverse
et al., 2009). This higher predominance of self- national sample of adults differing in lifestyle.
endorsed volitional activity may be conducive to They compared intrinsic and extrinsic value ori-
a greater tolerance of unpleasant situations, thus entation, mindfulness, and multiple indicators of
over time reducing exposure to stressors, which subjective well-being and ecologically responsi-
in turn contributes to well-being. ble behavior. Results indicated that higher levels
of mindfulness were related to greater intrinsic
value orientation; and both variables were associ-
Self-Regulation and Mindfulness: ated with subjective well-being and more eco-
The What logically responsible behavior. Brown and Kasser
suggested that mindfulness may foster greater
So far we argued that mindfulness facilitates reflection on ones consumption and market
more autonomous self-regulation, leading to choices and their ecological impacts. In turn, they
greater satisfaction of the fundamental psycho- suggested that increased environmental and pro-
logical needs of autonomy, competence, and social behaviors supply intrinsic satisfactions
relatedness, and thus wellness. Nonetheless, also that enhance well-being (De Young, 1996, 2000).
critical to SDT is the goal content or the what In yet another relevant project, Brown, Kasser,
of authentic functioning (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Ryan, Linley, and Orzech (2009) conducted a
Ryan et al., 1996). series of studies on the role of mindfulness on
90 P.P. Schultz and R.M. Ryan

financial desire discrepancy (the difference


between what one has and what one desires) and Conclusions
subjective well-being. In an initial study of
British undergraduates, results revealed that Mindfulness is a deceptively simple concept con-
mindfulness was associated with smaller finan- cerning an open, receptive awareness to the pres-
cial discrepancy, which partially explained the ent. Yet this simple phenomenon has manifold
positive relationship between mindfulness and influences on the pathways leading toward authen-
well-being. Two more studies replicated these tic self-regulation and well-being. In this chapter
findings, controlling for financial status and we reviewed evidence concerning how this state of
showing similar findings for working adults. A consciousness permeates critical components of
final, quasi-experimental investigation was con- SDT, globally referred to as the why and the
ducted to elucidate causation pathways. what of regulation of behavior; thus, framing
Participants were attendees at residential mind- mindfulness as an essential how of living well.
fulness meditation training centers who partici- Central to autonomous self-regulation is the
pated in a 4-week-training program. Findings capacity to reflectively consider ones behavior
suggested that increases in mindfulness were and its congruency with ones personal values
related to declines in financial desire discrepancy and needs. The enhanced attention and awareness
and increases in subjective well-being. of mindfulness promotes such ability. This awak-
Furthermore, these relations were not accounted ened state also allows consciousness to acquire a
for by financial status or recent financial status clarity and freshness that act as liberating agents
changes. While these studies highlight the asso- of automatic responses, and brings people to
ciations between mindfulness, intrinsic values, reflect upon the why of actions, hence fostering
and well-being, other research within SDT sug- more self-endorsed behavior. Finally, it appears
gests that these salutary effects stem, in part, that mindfulness can function as an antidote
from the promotion of healthy self-regulation against external and internal controlling forces
(Brown & Ryan, 2003, 2004; Deci & Ryan, 2000; that frequently undermine the selection and
Kasser & Ryan, 1996). enactment of more volitional behaviors.
To summarize, the arguments above under- Mindfulness is a powerful integrative agent, and
score the idea that mindfulness increases ones SDT views integration as crucial to the develop-
valuing of intrinsic goals and life pursuits. ment of more autonomous forms of motivation.
Intrinsic aspirations are, in turn, inherently related When awareness of inner and outer circum-
to basic need satisfaction, in contrast to extrinsic stances is heightened, so are peoples abilities to
goals, which are pursued for their instrumental attend to prompts arising from basic needs, and
value, and only fulfill basic needs indirectly or to consciously self-organize and self-regulate
even distract from them. Finally, basic need satis- their actions in a manner fulfilling such needs.
faction is associated with autonomous self-regu- Consequently, one is more likely to focus on and
lation and well-being. Considering the pressured attain intrinsic life goals, living more eudaimoni-
societies we live in, where, as never before, con- cally in the process. This is supported by both
sumerist messages seductively incite the pursuit research connecting mindfulness to intrinsic over
of material goods, the display of wealth, and por- extrinsic goals, and the relations between these
tray the glories of fame, mindfulness is an impor- goals and qualities of action, vitality, and overall
tant asset that can act as a buffer, and reduce wellness.
susceptibility to such extrinsic prompts and val- Beyond all the aforementioned benefits of
ues. That is, because of their heightened aware- mindfulness, we must not overlook a valuable
ness of internal and external states, more mindful aspect of this construct: the vividness that it adds
individuals may more easily realize that material- to current experience and the moment-to-moment
istic values are distant from their most essential sensory contact with life, without dense evalua-
needs and from behaviors that represent healthy tive filtering of experience (Brown & Ryan, 2003;
self-regulation (Brown et al., 2009). Kabat-Zinn, 2005). In the chaotic, often rushed
7 Mindfulness and Self-Determination 91

environment of modern society, where people happiness. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 59, 10211031.
strive to accomplish something every minute and
Brown, K. W., & Kasser, T. (2005). Are psychological and
where multi-tasking is the normal mode of opera- ecological well-being compatible? The role of values,
tion, there is consequently little time for contem- mindfulness, and lifestyle. Social Indicators Research,
plation of ones experiences, and little inclination 74, 349368.
Brown, K. W., Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., Linley, P. A., &
to turn off the cruise control in order to reflect on
Orzech, K. (2009). When what one has is enough:
the present moment. In this lifestyle of speed and Mindfulness, financial desire discrepancy, and subjec-
production, with its increasing commercial, tive well-being. Journal of Research in Personality,
social, and political attention capturing messages 43, 727736.
Brown, K. W., Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., & Konow,
and pressures, mindfulness emerges as a pivotal
J. (2008). Materialism, acquisition, and unpleasant
tool for autonomous, vital, living. affect. Unpublished manuscript, Virginia
Commonwealth University.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of
being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychologi-
References cal well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 822848.
Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Perils and promise
structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. in defining and measuring mindfulness: Observations
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, from experience. Clinical Psychology: Science and
5363. Practice, 11, 242248.
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanism of mind- Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., Creswell, J. D., & Niemiec,
fulness: Emotion regulation following a focused C. P. (2008). Beyond me: Mindful responses to social
breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, threat. In H. A. Wayment & J. J. Bauer (Eds.),
45, 14351447. Transcending self-interest (pp. 7584). Washington,
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical DC: American Psychological Association.
intervention: A conceptual and empirical review. Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007a).
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence
125143. for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18,
Baer, R. A. (2006). Mindfulness-based treatment 211237.
approaches: Clinicians guide to evidence base and Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007b).
applications. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. Addressing fundamental questions about mindfulness.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Psychological Inquiry, 18, 272281.
Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment meth- Buss, A. H. (1980). Self-consciousness and social anxiety.
ods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
2745. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-
Bargh, J. A. (1997). Automaticity in social psychology. In regulation. New York, NY: Springer.
E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psy- Clark, D. A., & Rhyno, S. (2005). Unwanted intrusive
chology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 109183). thoughts in nonclinical individuals: Implications for
New York, NY: Guilford Press. clinical disorders. In D. A. Clark (Ed.), Intrusive
Barnes, S., Brown, K. W., Krusemark, E., Campbell, thoughts in clinical disorders: Theory, research, and
W. K., & Rogge, R. D. (2007). The role of mindful- treatment (pp. 129). New York, NY: Guilford.
ness in romantic relationship satisfaction and Creswell, J. D., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman M. D.
responses to relationship stress. Journal of Marital (2008). Neurobehavioral correlates of mindfulness
and Family Therapy, 33, 482500. during social exclusion. Unpublished manuscript.
Baumeister, R. F. (1999). The nature and structure of the De Young, R. (1996). Some psychological aspects of
self: An overview. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), The self reduced consumption behavior: The role of intrinsic
in social psychology (pp. 120). Philadelphia, PA: satisfaction and competence motivation. Environment
Psychology Press. and Behavior, 28, 358409.
Beitel, M., Ferrer, E., & Cecero, J. J. (2005). Psychological De Young, R. (2000). Expanding and evaluating motives
mindedness and awareness of self and others. Journal for environmentally responsible behavior. Journal of
of Clinical Psychology, 61, 739750. Social Issues, 56, 509526.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York, NY:
N. D., Carmody, J., Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: Academic Press.
A proposed operational definition. Clinical Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R.
Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230241. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-deter-
Blais, M. R., Sabourin, S., Boucher, C., & Vallerand, R. J. mination theory perspective. Journal of Personality,
(1990). Toward a motivational model of couple 62, 119142.
92 P.P. Schultz and R.M. Ryan

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). Self-determination Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in
theory: When mind mediates behavior. Journal of context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology:
Mind and Behavior, 1, 3344. Science and Practice, 10, 144156.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Full Catastrophe Living: Using
and self-determination in human behavior. New York, the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress,
NY: Plenum. Pain, and Illness. New York, NY, US: Delta Trade
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational Paperback/Bantam Dell; US.
approach to self: Integration in personality. In Kasser, T. (2002). The high price of materialism.
R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Perspectives on motivation Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
(Nebraska symposium on motivation, Vol. 38, pp. 237 Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the
288). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. American dream: Correlates of financial success as a
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The what and the central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and
why of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self- Social Psychology, 65, 410422.
determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the
227268. American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2012). Motivation, personality, and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology
and development within embedded social contexts: An Bulletin, 22, 280287.
overview of self-determination theory. In R. M. Ryan Kernis, M. H., & Goldman, B. M. (2006). A multicompo-
(Ed.), Oxford handbook of human motivation nent conceptualization of authenticity: Theory and
(pp. 85107). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. research. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experi-
Dijksterhuis, A., & Knippenberg, A. V. (2000). Behavioral mental social psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 283357). San
indecision: Effects of self-focus on automatic behav- Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.
ior. Social Cognition, 18, 5574. Kuhl, J., & Kazen, M. (1994). Self-discrimination and
Dimidjian, S., & Linehan, M. M. (2003). Defining an memory: State orientation and false self-ascription of
agenda for future research on the clinical application assigned activities. Journal of Personality and Social
of mindfulness practice. Clinical Psychology: Science Psychology, 66, 1103.
and Practice, 10, 166171. Langer, E. J. (1989). Mindfulness. Reading, MA: Addison-
Gagn, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination the- Wesley/Addison Wesley Longman.
ory and work motivation. Journal of Organizational Leary, M. R., & Tate, E. B. (2007). The multi-faceted
Behavior, 26, 331362. nature of mindfulness. Psychological Inquiry, 18,
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: 251255.
Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, Levesque, C., & Brown, K. W. (2007). Mindfulness as a
54(7), 493503. moderator of the effect of implicit motivational self-
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). concept on day-to-day behavioral motivation.
Terror management theory of self-esteem and social Motivation and Emotion, 31, 284299.
behavior. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experi- Loevinger, J., & Blasi, A. (1991). Development of the self
mental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 61139). San as subject. In J. Stranss & G. Goethals (Eds.), The self:
Diego, CA: Academic Press. Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 150167).
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. New York, NY: Springer.
(1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit Martin, J. R. (1997). Mindfulness: A proposed common
cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of factor. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 7,
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480. 291312.
Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles asso- Martin, L. L., & Erber, R. (2005). Commentary: The wis-
ciated with childrens self-regulation and competence dom of social psychology: Five commonalities and
in school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, one concern. Psychological Inquiry, 16, 194202.
143154. McAdams, D. P. (1990). The person. San Diego, CA:
Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
(2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health Mitchell, J. P., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003).
benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Contextual variations in implicit evaluation. Journal
Research, 57, 3543. of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 455469.
Gunaratana, B. H. (2002). Mindfulness in plain English. Niemiec, C. P., Brown, K. W., Kashdan, T. B., Cozzolino,
Boston, MA: Wisdom Publications. P. J., Breen, W. E., Levesque-Bristol, C., & Ryan,
Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (2003). Mindfulness: R. M. (2010). Being present in the face of existential
Method and process. Clinical Psychology: Science threat: The role of trait mindfulness in reducing defen-
and Practice, 10, 161165. sive responses to mortality salience. Journal of
Hodgins, H. S., & Knee, C. R. (2002). The integrating self Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 344365.
and conscious experience. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Brown, K. W. (2008). The
(Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research role of awareness and autonomy in quieting the ego: A
(pp. 87100). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester self-determination theory perspective. In H. A.
Press. Wayment & J. J. Bauer (Eds.), Transcending self-
7 Mindfulness and Self-Determination 93

interest (pp. 107116). Washington, DC: American Ryan, R. M., Koestner, R., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Ego-
Psychological Association. involved persistence: When free-choice behavior is
Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The not intrinsically motivated. Motivation and Emotion,
path taken: Consequences of attaining intrinsic and 15, 185205.
extrinsic aspirations in post-college life. Journal of Ryan, R. M., Legate, N., Niemiec, C. P., & Deci, E. L.
Research in Personality, 43, 291306. (2012). Beyond illusions and defense: Exploring the
Nix, G., Ryan, R. M., Manly, J. B., & Deci, E. L. (1999). possibilities and limits of human autonomy and
Revitalization through self-regulation: The effects of responsibility through self-determination theory. In
autonomous versus controlled motivation on happi- P. R. Shaver & M. Mikulincer (Eds.), Meaning, mor-
ness and vitality. Journal of Experimental Social tality, and choice (pp. 215233). Washington, DC:
Psychology, 35, 266284. American Psychological Association.
Reeve, J. M., & Jang, H. (2006). What teachers say and do Ryan, R. M., Patrick, H., Deci, E. L., & Williams, G. C.
to support students autonomy during a learning activ- (2008). Facilitating health behaviour change and
ity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 209218. its maintenance: Interventions based on Self-
Roth, G., Assor, A., KanatMaymon, Y., & Kaplan, H. Determination Theory. European Health Psychologist,
(2007). Autonomous motivation for teaching: How 10, 14.
self-determined teaching may lead to self-determined Ryan, R. M., & Rigby, C. S. (2015). Did the Buddha have
learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, a self? No-self, self and mindfulness in Buddhist
761774. thought and western psychologies. In K. W. Brown,
Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intra- R. M. Ryan, & J. D. Creswell (Eds.), Handbook of
personal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation mindfulness. New York, NY: Guilford.
theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L.
43, 450461. (1996). All goals are not created equal: An organismic
Ryan, R. M. (1993). Agency and organization: Intrinsic perspective on the nature of goals and their regulation.
motivation, autonomy and the self in psychological In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychol-
development. In J. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska symposium ogy of action. New York, NY: Guilford.
on motivation: Developmental perspectives on moti- Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., &
vation (Vol. 40, pp. 156). Lincoln, NE: University of Palfai, T. P. (1995). Emotional attention, clarity, and
Nebraska Press. repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the trait
Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilita- meta-mood scale. In Emotion, disclosure, & health
tion of integrative processes. Journal of Personality, (pp. 125154). Washington, DC: American
63, 397427. Psychological Association.
Ryan, R. M., & Brown, K. W. (2003). Why we dont need Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G., & Bonner, G. (1998). Effects
self-esteem: Basic needs, mindfulness, and the authen- of mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and
tic self. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 7176. premedical students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of 21, 581599.
causality and internalization: Examining reasons for Tart, C. T. (1994). Living the mindful life. Boston, MA:
acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Shambhala.
Social Psychology, 57, 749761. Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-
Ryan, R. M., Curren, R. R., & Deci, E. L. (2013). What consciousness and the five-factor model of personal-
humans need: Flourishing in Aristotelian philosophy ity: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal
and self-determination theory. In A. S. Waterman (Ed.), of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 284304.
The best within us. Washington, DC: APA Books. Vansteenkiste, M., Neyrinck, B., Niemiec, C. P., Soenens,
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination B., De Witte, H., & Van Den Broeck, A. (2007). On
theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, the relations among work value orientations, psycho-
social development and well-being. American logical need satisfaction and job outcomes: A self-
Psychologist, 55, 6878. determination theory approach. Journal of Occupational
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Self-regulation and the & Organizational Psychology, 80, 251277.
problem of human autonomy: Does psychology need Vansteenkiste, M., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008).
choice, self-determination, and will? Journal of Self-determination theory and the explanatory role of
Personality, 74, 15571585. psychological needs in human well-being. In
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2011). Multiple identities L. Bruni, F. Comim, & M. Pugno (Eds.), Capabilities
within a single self: A self-determination theory per- and happiness (pp. 181223). Oxford, UK: Oxford
spective on internalization within contexts and University Press.
cultures. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Verplanken, B., & Velsvik, R. (2008). Habitual negative
Handbook of self & identity (pp. 253274). New York, body image thinking as psychological risk factor in
NY: Guilford. adolescents. Body Image, 5, 133140.
Ryan, R. M., Huta, V., & Deci, E. L. (2008). Living well: Weinstein, N., Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). A
A self-determination theory perspective on eudai- multi-method examination of the effects of mindful-
monia. Journal of Happiness Studies, 9, 139170. ness on stress attribution, coping, and emotional well-
94 P.P. Schultz and R.M. Ryan

being. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, Westen, D. (1999). Psychology: Mind, brain, and culture
374385. (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
Weinstein, N., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). When helping Williams, G. C., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Internalization of
helps: Autonomous motivation for prosocial behavior biopsychosocial values by medical students: A test of
and its influence on well-being for the helper and self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and
recipient. Journal of Personality and Social Social Psychology, 70, 767779.
Psychology, 98, 222244.
Self-Regulatory Strength
and Mindfulness 8
Michael J. MacKenzie and Roy F. Baumeister

Self-regulation (also referred to as self-control) great degree of flexibility, far surpassing that of
has become a popular research topic in psychol- any other species. Among other benefits, self-
ogy over the past few decades. During this recent regulation enables people to pursue and achieve
time period, researchers and thinkers have recog- long-term benefits when these require short-term
nized the significance of self-regulation and its sacrifices and costs. According to some views
importance for understanding human well-being (e.g., Baumeister, 2005) the central thrust of
and human nature. Self-regulation is a pervasive human evolution was the emergence of the capac-
feature of everyday life (Hofmann, Baumeister, ity to create and sustain culture, and self-
Frster, & Vohs, 2012). A range of activities such regulation is intimately connected with this
as deciding what to wear, suppressing unwanted evolutionary advancement and the development
thoughts, inhibiting a rude (or honest) remark, of culture and civilization.
and feigning laughter all involve self-regulation In this chapter we provide a general back-
and self-control. This ability is one of humans ground on self-regulation, followed by a more
defining characteristics. Other animals may have detailed explanation of the strength model of
some ability for self-control; however, self- self-regulation, and then we discuss its relation to
control in non-human animals pales in compari- mindfulness. First, the chapter will define self-
son to humans. As will be discussed in greater regulation (and self-control). Following this, the
detail later, effective self-regulation is an impor- operation of self-regulation and its basic ele-
tant part of living a successful life. Conversely, ments are discussed, and a strength model of self-
failure at self-regulation is a central factor regulation is explicated. Some research
involved in many problems for both the individ- highlighting the benefits of successful self-
ual and society at large. Self-regulation is an regulation is outlined. Last, some aspects of
impressive evolutionary achievement and very mindfulness are looked at from the strength
likely an essential step for development of civili- model perspective of self-regulation and relations
zation and culture. Being able to restrain ones between the two are addressed.
desires and override initial responses allows a

Definition of Self-Regulation
M.J. MacKenzie (*) R.F. Baumeister
To regulate something means to change or adjust
Department of Psychology, Florida State University,
Tallahassee, FL, USA it. More specifically, to regulate means to change
e-mail: mackenzie@psy.fsu.edu in a particular sort of way: a way that is in

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 95


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_8
96 M.J. MacKenzie and R.F. Baumeister

accordance with some rule, ideal, goal or some Standards


other kind of standard. In brief, self-regulation is
regulation of the self by the self (Baumeister, Self-regulation is about changing the self, but
Heatherton, & Tice, 1994; Carver & Scheier, without standards any change would be random
1981). To self-regulate, then, means to change and without aim or purpose. Standards provide
some thought, feeling, or behavior in such a way guidance for how one should change. A standard
that it conforms to some standard. can be a rule, law, or social norm which one is
The term self-control can be used inter- motivated to follow. A standard could also be a
changeably with self-regulation. However, some personal goal, value, or ideal. The reasons behind
authors maintain an important distinction selecting or creating a particular standard can be
between the two. Self-control refers to the a complex, dynamical cognitive process influ-
active, conscious, effortful capacity of self-reg- enced by a variety of interpersonal, intrapersonal,
ulation. Self-regulation can also occur at the and situational variables (Higgins, 1987). Despite
unconscious level (see Bargh, 1990; Bargh, this complexity, humans do not often lack stan-
Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, & Trotschel, dards. In fact, rather than a lack of standards, the
2001; Chartrand & Bargh, 1996; Fitzsimons & more common problem is an overabundance of
Bargh, 2003). In other words, there may be standards. This can lead to self-regulatory break-
unconscious self-regulation but no unconscious down, especially if standards conflict (Baumeister
self-control. This chapter, however, will focus et al., 1994). Moral dilemmas typically involve
on conscious self-regulation, and so self-regula- two conflicting standards, so that adhering to one
tion and self-control can be used interchange- standard entails violating another. Standards that
ably for present purposes. are vague, ambiguous, or unclear can make self-
Self-control is the ability to override reactions regulation difficult and prone to failure. Another
to impulses, urges, and habitual responses in such common clash between standards involves the
a way that the following thoughts, feelings, or desire to feel better in the short-term versus the
behaviors will fall in line with personal or cul- desire to achieve some long-term goal. Imagine a
tural standards. Self-control has sometimes been person on a diet who is feeling upset and is con-
equated with impulse control, but impulse con- templating eating some junk food. To alleviate
trol is in a sense a misnomer. The impulse itself is his or her negative feelings, a person will prefer a
not being controlled. What is being controlled is short-term solution over a long-term solution;
how one deals with the given impulse. Thus, self- therefore, eating some unhealthy food can offer
control enables people to adjust their own behav- an immediate although temporary wave of plea-
iors and inner states, thereby enabling a high sure. But this short-term goal conflicts with the
degree of adaptability and flexibility. long-term goal of losing weight. Thus, being
upset or in a negative emotional state can com-
promise other standards involving self-control.
Elements of Self-Regulation This regulation of immediate emotional distress
has been shown to override other patterns of self-
Self-regulation can be broken down into three control related to distal goals (Tice, Bratslavsky,
main components. The first is establishing a goal & Baumeister, 2001).
or desired state. This is also generally referred to Some work on standards comes from Higgins
as having a standard. The second is monitoring and his colleagues (e.g., Higgins, 1987; Higgins,
progress toward the standard. The third is the Roney, Crowe, & Hymes, 1994; Shah, Higgins,
capacity to make the desired changes. This refers & Friedman, 1998). This research makes a dis-
to the strength or ability one has to make changes. tinction between ideal standards and ought stan-
A deficit in any one of these components can lead dards. Ideal standards represent positive strivings
to self-regulation failure. We shall now explain toward the way one would like to be. Ought stan-
each of these. dards are also based on how one would like to be,
8 Self-Regulatory Strength and Mindfulness 97

but they involve following particular duties, obli- patterns, is under question. At present it seems
gations, or laws. Higgins and colleagues suggest reasonable to conclude that violating standards
that violations of these types of standards are often brings some emotional distress, but there
accompanied by different emotions. The type of appears to be very little recently published work
negative emotion is what differs. Failure to self- that pursues the notion of specific, differentiated
regulate toward ideals produces low arousal emo- standards linked to specific emotions.
tions, such as disappointment and sadness. On In sum, standards are a necessary but not suf-
the other hand, failure to self-regulate toward ficient ingredient for successful self-regulation.
ought standards produces high arousal emotions Without standards, self-regulation would be
such as stress and worry. Simply having stan- without purpose and offer no conceivable bene-
dards is not enough to ensure that they will be fits. Sometimes, one can hold standards that con-
followed. One must be motivated to adhere to a flict with each other, which can cause certain
standard. Higgins and colleagues (e.g., Higgins self-regulatory patterns to be compromised. But
& Spiegel, 2004) suggest that there are also dif- having standards alone is not enough to success-
ferent motivational patterns used to engage in fully achieve them. One must have a desire and
self-regulation. Such patterns are commonly motivation to reach a standard. A standard that
referred to as regulatory focus. Some individuals exhorts one to eat healthy food or quit smoking
are promotion-focused: they are primarily moti- will have little effect if the person lacks motiva-
vated to reach desirable outcomes by pursuing tion to try to live up to it.
ideal standards using eager, approach-oriented
strategies. Other individuals are prevention
focused: they are primarily motivated to reach Monitoring
desirable outcomes by pursuing ought standards
using vigilant, avoidance-oriented strategies. Monitoring refers to keeping track of activities
When the regulatory standard type (ideal or that are relevant to achieving a goal or standard
ought) matches regulatory focus style (promotion and noting the extent to which progress is being
or prevention), it is referred to as regulatory fit. made. A prominent book by Carver and Scheier
Research suggests that when people experience (1981) argued that one of the main functions of
regulatory fit, self-regulatory outcomes are self-awareness is to facilitate self-regulation.
improved (Higgins, 2000). For example, research Indeed, it would be quite difficult to change a
by Keller and Bless (2006) found that individuals behavior if one were not aware of it. Self-
performed better on a cognitive task when their awareness involves more than just being aware
chronic regulatory focus style matched the situa- that one exists, or that one has brown hair. It
tionally induced self-regulatory mechanisms of involves a comparison of aspects of the self to
the task. When the task was described in a self- standards. For example, one can be aware that
regulatory prevention way (as the potential for one is overweight only by comparison to some
loss), those with a chronic prevention focus per- social or medical standard. Borrowing from
formed better compared to when the same task cybernetic theory, Carver and Scheier sug-
was described in a self-regulatory promotion way gested that self-regulation is similar to a feed-
(as the potential for gain). However, some ideas back loop. The feedback loop involves four
by Higgins and colleagues have received criti- steps called test, operate, (re)test, and exit
cism. Tangney, Niedenthal, Covert, and Barlow (TOTE; Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960).
(1998) tested some of Higgins hypotheses. A The first test phase consists of comparing ones
major finding was that violating certain types of current status on a particular thing to the rele-
standards was not significantly related to a dis- vant standard. If the standard is met, there is no
tinct emotional experience (i.e., low arousal vs. need to move on to the next phase. However, if
high arousal). Thus, the core point of the theory, the standard has not been reached, then the
linking different standards to different emotional operate phase will begin.
98 M.J. MacKenzie and R.F. Baumeister

The operate phase involves making changes a row as a way to test the energy model
and attempting to make the current status fall into (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice,
line with the relevant standard. At some point 1998; Muraven, Tice, & Baumeister, 1998). This
during or after the operate phase, another test will two-task paradigm allowed researchers to test
occur. If this test determines the standard has three competing models of self-regulation. First,
been met, then the feedback loop will be exited, if self-regulation primarily involved information
otherwise it will resume or continue the operate processing and operated as a cognitive construct,
phase. A good example of the ill effects of the recent use of self-control on the first task
reduced self-awareness (and therefore monitor- should make schemas of self-control more acces-
ing) on self-regulation is alcohol use. Alcohol sible and therefore the participant should perform
intoxication reduces self-awareness (Hull, 1981) better on the second task. Second, if self-control
and is associated with an array of self-regulatory operated as a skill, participants would display a
impairments (Baumeister et al., 1994). A major similar performance on both tasks, but would
reason appears to be that alcohol intoxication gradually get better over time. Third, if self-
stops people from keeping track of relevant control operated as the limited resource model
behaviors, so they succumb to excess. Monitoring, predicts, participants should perform worse on
then, is an important tool for successful the second task of self-control than on the first,
self-regulation. because the available energy resource would be
temporarily reduced.
Many subsequent empirical findings have sup-
The Strength Model ported the limited resource model of self-control.
When participants performed acts controlling
The third element of self-regulation is the capac- thoughts, behaviors, or feelings, the ability to
ity to make changes. For successful self- perform on subsequent self-control tasks was
regulation, it is not enough for one to have impaired. Muraven et al. (1998) had participants
standards and monitor progress toward those watch an emotionally distressing movie clip with
standards. One must also exert effort toward a specific set of instructions. Some were
achieving those standards. This aspect of self- instructed to suppress their emotional reactions,
regulation can be thought of as the operate phase others were instructed to exaggerate their reac-
of the TOTE model. tions, and others were instructed to not alter their
Recent evidence suggests that successful self- emotions. After this task, participants did a test of
control depends on a limited resource (see physical stamina using a handgrip exerciser. It
Baumeister & Tierney, 2011; Gailliot et al., 2007; was found that those who had had to alter their
Gailliot & Baumeister, 2007). Folk wisdom has emotional reactions in some way (either sup-
recognized that willpower is an important part pressing or exaggerating) performed worse on
of self-control. Scientific investigation has found the handgrip task, compared to those who did not
that such a notion of willpower is not far off the alter emotional reactions. It seems, then, that
mark. The terms limited resource model, energy those who had to regulate their emotions con-
model, and strength model will be used sumed some of the resource, which left less avail-
interchangeably. able to use in the handgrip task.
Baumeister et al. (1994) suggested that an In another study, participants began the exper-
individuals capacity for self-control was limited. iment on an empty stomach and were seated at a
They contended that self-control can be viewed table with a freshly baked pile of chocolate chip
as an energy model, where initial acts involving cookies on one side, and a bowl of radishes on the
self-control would lead to less effective self- other. Some participants were told that their task
control in subsequent tasks because of the deple- was to only eat the radishes; they were then left
tion of an energy resource. Subsequent research alone on the room with the cookies and radishes
had participants perform two self-control tasks in in front of them. Participants in the control
8 Self-Regulatory Strength and Mindfulness 99

condition were told they could eat the cookies. emotions impairs perseverance on a subsequent
The second self-control task was a series of dif- hand exercise, and resisting chocolate chip cook-
ficult (unsolvable) puzzles. The results supported ies hampers persistence on difficult puzzles. This
the limited resource model. Those who were told implies that a common resource is being drawn
to only eat the radishes gave up on the puzzles upon for a range of self-regulation tasks. These
significantly faster than those who ate the cook- studies offer support for a vital component of the
ies. The results suggest that resisting the tempta- strength model: they show that self-regulation
tion to eat the cookies drained some self-control depends on a limited resource that becomes
resource. Participants in this depleted state had depleted when one exerts self-regulatory efforts.
less self-regulatory strength to persevere on the
next, unrelated self-regulatory task (Baumeister
et al., 1998). Increasing Self-Control Strength
Another study found that participants who
completed a thought suppression exercise were Another prediction of the strength model is that
more likely to drink free beer than those who had self-control should gradually improve over time
solved math problems (Muraven, Collins, & with regular exercise. Thus, self-control may
Nienhaus, 2002). Math problems may be unpleas- resemble a muscle: after immediate usage it gets
ant but they do not require self-control and there- tired and less effective, but with continual exer-
fore do not deplete the self, whereas suppressing cise it should get stronger (Baumeister, Vohs, &
thoughts is an important form of self-regulation. Tice, 2007; Muraven & Baumeister, 2000). Some
Furthermore, a recent meta-analysis by research supports this prediction. Muraven,
Hagger, Wood, Stiff, and Chatzisarantis (2010) Baumeister, and Tice (1999) had some partici-
combined results from 83 similar self-control pants practice particular self-regulatory tasks
depletion studies and found a significant effect of (e.g., maintaining good posture) for 2 weeks.
depletion on self-control task performance. Other participants did not practice anything over
Participants who had been depleted from using the same 2-week period. Following this, partici-
self-control on an initial task exerted less self- pants returned to the laboratory and completed
control and therefore performed worse on a sec- two consecutive self-regulatory tasks (a thought
ond task, compared to those who were not suppression task followed by a persistence task).
depleted. It was also found that the effect size of It was found that those who practiced self-
depletion on self-control task performance was regulation for 2 weeks lasted longer (compared to
influenced by the duration of time between self- their previously measured baseline score) on the
control tasks. When there was less rest time persistence task than those who did not practice.
between tasks, depletion from the first task had a The 2-week exercise period apparently improved
stronger effect on performance of the second self-regulatory stamina. The self-control muscle,
task. Hagger et al. (2010) also reported that when so to speak, did not tire out as easily in the group
participants have put in some training on a self- who had exercised it for 2 weeks. Another study
control task, they perform better on that task by Oaten and Cheng (2006) found similar self-
when in a depleted state compared to those who regulatory improvements after a period of regular
have not previously trained on the task. This pat- exercise. They found that those who were
tern of depleted self-control ability is commonly assigned to a 2-month long physical exercise pro-
referred to as ego depleted. The term was gram, compared to those not in the program, dis-
selected to pay homage to Sigmund Freud, one of played reduced impairments on a second
the most prominent thinkers to use an energy self-control task and also reported an increase in
model of the self (ego). self-control behavior such as reduced cigarette
All of these studies show that the first and sec- smoking and eating healthier. A series of studies
ond act of self-control do not have to be related in by Gailliot, Plant, Butz, and Baumeister (2007)
order for depletion effects to occur. Suppressing found that participants who completed 2 weeks
100 M.J. MacKenzie and R.F. Baumeister

of self-regulation exercises (e.g., changing speak- depletion effects and thus enhance performance
ing patterns, using their nondominant hand for on subsequent self-control tasks.
common tasks) performed better on a second Some research has found that personal beliefs
self-regulatory task than those who did not do the and self-affirmation can influence typical deple-
self-regulation exercises. tion effects. Schmeichel and Vohs (2009) found
After recent use of self-control it becomes that thinking about important personal values
fatigued, tired, and less effective. The ability for counteracted the ill effects of depletion. This
one to use self-control more effectively and for study had participants rank order a list of per-
longer periods of time can be enhanced if one reg- sonal values (e.g., friends, family, aesthetics).
ularly exercises self-control. Thus, recent evidence Those in the self-affirmation condition then
seems to support the idea that self-control resem- thought about and wrote about their top ranked
bles a muscle that becomes fatigued after use and value. It was found that engaging in this self-
increases in strength with regular practice. affirmation activity between self-control tasks
significantly facilitated performance on the sec-
ond self-control task. This effect was only found
Conservation and Motivation for depleted participants. For participants who
did not engage in an initial depleting self-control
If self-regulation relies on limited resource, then task, self-affirmation had no effect on perfor-
people would be expected to allocate self- mance of the second task. Another study by Job,
regulation resources in an efficient and judi- Dweck, and Walton (2010) found that partici-
cious manner, which would entail not expending pants who believed willpower was unlimited
them on superfluous or frivolous tasks. Thus, were less impaired by ego depletion than partici-
ego depletion effects indicate conservation pants who did not believe willpower was
rather than complete expenditure of the unlimited.
resources. Indeed, Muraven, Shmueli, and A recent study by Vohs, Baumeister, and
Burkley (2006) demonstrated that people con- Schmeichel (2012) examined the extent to which
serve their self-regulatory resources as needed. beliefs and motivations influence self-control.
This research found that performance on self- They found that when depletion was mild (com-
control tasks was more likely to be impaired pleting one self-control task), particular beliefs
when participants knew they had to do another and motivations eliminated the typical depletion
self-control task later, as compared to when no effects. Specifically, those who were manipulated
further task was anticipated. Apparently, those to believe that willpower was unlimited
who were anticipating doing another self-con- (Experiment 1) or motivated by task importance
trol task had the wherewithal to conserve some (Experiment 2) did not display self-regulation
of their resources. impairment on a following self-control task.
Motivation is another factor involved in self- However, when participants had completed mul-
control. Muraven and Slessareva (2003) found tiple self-control tasks, belief and motivation did
that participants who were depleted (from com- not improve self-control performance. When ego
pleting a previous self-control task) and offered depletion was more extensive, motivation and
an incentive (e.g., getting paid for better perfor- belief did not countervail the typical depletion
mance) to complete a subsequent self-control effects.
task performed significantly better than those The contrary effects of beliefs about unlimited
who were also depleted but not offered any willpower with mild versus severe depletion help
incentive. An incentive did not influence task explain a seeming paradox. If Job et al. (2010)
performance for those who were not depleted. were correct that believing in unlimited will-
Findings from this study show that when people power could actually make willpower unlimited,
have been depleted by an initial self-control then it would be surprising if any society in the
task, an increase in motivation can override world had failed to adopt such a highly adaptive
8 Self-Regulatory Strength and Mindfulness 101

belief. Yet belief in unlimited willpower is very Benefits of High Self-Control


far from the norm. The reason, presumably, is
that such a belief is counterproductive just when Self-control is an important feature of success
willpower is most needed (i.e., when demands and well-being. Successful self-control is crucial
for self-control are high). for the optimal function of humans on the indi-
vidual and collective level. Inadequate self-
control, on the other hand, is a core feature of
Summary of the Strength Model many societal and individual ills. This section
will outline some domains in life where success-
Performance on a second self-control task is ful self-regulation is an important factor.
impaired as a result of having used some of the The importance of dispositional self-control
limited resources on an initial self-control task. was highlighted by Walter Mischel and his col-
The strength model suggests that an act of self- leagues (e.g., Mischel, Ebbesen, & Zeiss, 1972;
control consumes a limited resource. After Mischel, Shoda, & Peake, 1988; Shoda, Mischel,
engaging in self-control, availability of the & Peake, 1990) on the delay of gratification.
resource is temporarily reduced, and one will be Delay of gratification is an important form of
less effective at self-control while in this self-control because it requires one to override
depleted state. The same resource is used for a impulsive reactions in order to obtain a more
variety of self-regulatory tasks such as resisting desirable outcome at a later time. The research
temptations, suppressing thoughts, and persist- by Mischel and colleagues investigated differ-
ing on difficult tasks. If one uses self-regulatory ences in trait levels of self-regulation. In these
resources within a certain domain, all other delay of gratification studies children were pre-
domains are vulnerable to impairment. sented with a choice to have an immediate treat
According to the strength model, self-control or a more desirable treat at a later time. Some
resembles a muscle. Like a muscle, self-control children were unable to resist and nibbled away
capacity can be improved with regular practice at the treat right away, while other children were
and exercise. able to wait and ultimately received the more
Evidence has strongly supported the strength desirable treat. Follow-up studies found that the
model of self-control, and some recent research ability to delay gratification as a child predicts
has augmented certain aspects of it. Particularly, personal well-being as a young adult. Those
some research (e.g., Job et al., 2010; Muraven & who had been able to resist the immediate temp-
Slessareva, 2003) suggests that personal beliefs tation as a child were more likely to do well in
(e.g., believing willpower is unlimited), and school, to be popular, to have higher SAT scores,
motivation (such as monetary incentives) can and to exhibit better mental health than those
counteract typical depletion impairments. Other who had had poor self-control and had been
research shows that personal beliefs and motiva- unable to resist the immediate temptation
tion can offset typical self-control impairments (Mischel et al., 1988; Shoda et al., 1990).
associated with depletion, but only in cases of Poor self-control has also been recognized as
mild depletion (Vohs et al., 2012). As depletion one of the most important aspects for understand-
becomes more severe, the influence of belief and ing crime (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990; Pratt &
motivation diminishes. Hence, it appears that Cullen, 2000). People usually engage in criminal
there are variables involved with self-control behavior because they have poor self-control and
ability that are not directly related to strength or a have a hard time controlling responses to antiso-
limited resource. But it seems that these other cial impulses. Hollywood likes to portray crimi-
variables are only relevant at mild levels of deple- nals as well thought out, calculated, criminal
tion. It is likely that self-regulation ability is an masterminds (e.g., the criminal genius).
interaction between biologically based energy However these are not the characteristics of the
levels and subjective psychology factors. everyday person who engages in crime. Criminals
102 M.J. MacKenzie and R.F. Baumeister

tend to be impulsive and often fail to consider self-control is consistently exercised, ones
long-term goals or consequences. Another way, general capacity for self-control increases (gets
then, that effective self-control is beneficial for stronger). An increase in self-control ability has
both society and the individual is that it reduces been strongly associated with improvements
ones tendency for acting on antisocial impulses across a wide range of areas related to well-being.
or urges. It is possible, then, that mindfulness interven-
Tangney, Baumeister, and Boone (2004) mea- tions are a type of self-control exercise. The
sured trait levels of self-control and then exam- mindfulness interventions discussed by Brown
ined several areas of self-regulatory functioning. et al. (2007) have similar characteristics to some
Those who had higher self-control scores were of the self-control procedures used in psycholog-
better off than those with low self-control scores ical experiments. For example, the mindfulness-
on almost all of the outcomes. Individuals with based stress reduction procedure involves
higher self-control scores performed better in participants focusing their awareness on thoughts
school, had higher empathy, maintained healthier or a specific image for over an hour in a daily
relationships with friends and family, and their exercise routine. Some self-control procedures
relationships involved less conflict. Further, those involve similar attention control tasks (e.g.,
with high self-control had higher self-esteem, Gailliot, Baumeister et al., 2007; Vohs,
better psychological adjustment, better emotion Baumeister, & Ciarocco, 2005). One of these
regulation abilities, and fewer impulse control tasks requires participants to watch a silent, 6 min
problems. High self-control, then, is a valuable video of a woman speaking, which also has
trait that research suggests is beneficial to both words popping up in the corner. Some partici-
the individual and society. pants were instructing to not look at the words,
and thus to focus their attention on a specific fea-
ture. Holding attention during this 6 min task
The Strength Model of Self- despite a series of distracting stimuli was enough
Regulation and Mindfulness to induce self-control depletion. Hence, it appears
that the awareness control exercises used in
Mindfulness has been defined as a receptive atten- mindfulness interventions may be a similar, but
tion to and awareness of present events and experi- longer lasting, type of attention control task used
ence (Brown & Ryan, 2003). A host of benefits in self-control research.
have been associated with increased mindfulness Other types of mindfulness interventions
such as improved psychological well-being and require participants to consciously monitor and
physical health, better relationship quality, and control their physical movements. For example,
improved self-regulation (see Brown, Ryan, & Hanh (1976) suggests one way to increase mind-
Creswell, 2007). Overall, mindfulness has been fulness is to move in slow motion while doing
shown to facilitate healthy and adaptive human common household tasks and fully focus your
functioning across several domains. Following attention on the task. This practice is similar to
Masicampo and Baumeister (2007), we suggest exercises used to increase self-control ability that
that there are two areas where the distinction requires participants to monitor their posture
between self-regulatory processes and mindfulness (Muraven et al., 1999) or use their nondominant
requires attention. First, some conceptual overlap hand for common daily tasks such as brushing
between self-regulation exercise and mindfulness teeth or opening doors (Gailliot, Baumeister
interventions is addressed. Second, the possibility et al., 2007). The procedures proposed by Hanh
of a bidirectional relationship between self-regula- and the self-control researchers require people to
tion and mindfulness is examined. engage in common tasks in unusual ways that
As discussed previously, the strength model of require conscious control.
self-control suggests that self-control may oper- Brown et al. (2007) suggest that those with
ate like a muscle. Research shows that when higher dispositional mindfulness have increased
8 Self-Regulatory Strength and Mindfulness 103

well-being and better self-control ability. self-control. Therefore, future research should
However, it is also possible that successful self- continue to investigate the extent to which the
control leads to increased mindfulness and well- benefits of mindfulness interventions are unique
being. One route by which increased self-control to specific aspects of mindfulness such as nonat-
could lead to mindfulness is through goal regula- tachment and metacognitive insight, and the
tion. An important component of entering a extent to which they are a result of general self-
mindful state is achieving some peace of mind or regulatory processes.
clarity of thought. Unwanted worry or anxiety
deriving from uncompleted or unplanned tasks
may be a distraction and hinder such clarity of Concluding Remarks
thought. It is possible, then, that those who have
successful goal management and regulation (i.e., Successful self-control is a vital characteristic
self-control) have a greater disposition to enter that contributes to well-being in a variety of
mindful states. domains. This chapter summarized the strength
Other research has found that engaging in model of self-control, which states that the ability
mindfulness meditation can counteract self-control to self-regulate relies on limited resource.
depletion. Mindfulness meditation consists of Research has consistently shown that after com-
blocking out distractions and focusing ones atten- pleting an initial self-control task, performance
tion on the current moment. A recent study by on subsequent self-control tasks is impaired.
Friese, Messner, and Schaffner (2012) had some Another aspect of the strength model of self-
depleted participants mindfully meditate for 5 min control suggests that self-control resembles a
before a second self-control task. It was found muscle. With regular exercise and practice, ones
depleted participants who engaged in a brief ses- capacity for self-control can increase.
sion of mindfulness meditation performed just as Mindfulness is an important and useful area of
well on the second task as did those who were not research that is receiving an increasing amount of
depleted. The authors suggest that mindfulness attention. Mindfulness is associated with
meditation may counter typical depletion effects increased well-being in life across a range of
because it increases self-awareness and feelings domains (see Brown & Ryan, 2003). Self-control
relaxation. In this experimental design, depleted exercises and mindfulness interventions appear
participants who meditated (compared to those to have some features in common and there is
who did not) presumably exerted additional atten- likely a bidirectional relationship between mind-
tion control before the second task. Despite this, fulness and self-regulation. Future research on
those who meditated still outperformed their non- mindfulness should consider any possible theo-
meditating depleted counterparts. It is possible, retical overlap with self-regulation and clarify the
then, that there are unique features of mindfulness distinction between them.
that improve self-control.
Self-control and mindfulness interventions
have a number of features in common. Both have References
similar procedures, execution, and require par-
ticipation in daily exercises over extended peri- Bargh, J. A. (1990). Goal not = intent: Goal directed
thought and behavior are often unintentional.
ods of time. Both also involve regulating ones
Psychological Inquiry, 1, 248251.
thoughts or behaviors. Self-control and mindful- Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar,
ness interventions produce similar results related K., & Trotschel, R. (2001). The automated will:
to improving physical and psychological well- Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral
goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
being. Furthermore, it is not exclusively the case
81, 10141027.
that increased mindfulness causes increased self- Baumeister, R. F. (2005). The cultural animal: Human
control. Mindful states may be most accessible to nature, meaning, and social life. New York, NY:
individuals who already display a high degree of Oxford University Press.
104 M.J. MacKenzie and R.F. Baumeister

Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value
D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a lim- from fit. American Psychologist, 55, 12171230.
ited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Higgins, E. T., Roney, C. J., Crowe, E., & Hymes, C.
Psychology, 74, 12521265. (1994). Ideal versus ought predilections for approach
Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994). and avoidance distinct self-regulatory systems.
Losing control: How and why people fail at self- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66,
regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic. 276286.
Baumeister, R. F., & Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Higgins, E. T., & Spiegel, S. (2004). Promotion and pre-
Rediscovering the greatest human strength. New York, vention strategies for self-regulation: A motivated
NY: The Penguin Press. cognition perspective. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D.
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research
strength model of self-control. Current Directions in theory, and application (pp. 171187). New York, NY:
Psychological Science, 16, 351355. Guilford Press.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Frster, G., & Vohs,
being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychologi- K. D. (2012). Everyday temptations: An experience
cal well-being. Journal of Personality and Social sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control.
Psychology, 84, 822848. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102,
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). 13181335.
Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and evidence for Hull, J. G. (1981). A self-awareness model of the causes
its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 211237. and effects of alcohol consumptions. Journal of
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self- Abnormal Psychology, 90, 586600.
regulation: A control theory approach to human Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego
behavior. New York, NY: Springer. depletionis it all on your head? Implicit theories
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activa- about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological
tion of impression formation and memorization goals: Science, 21, 16861693.
Nonconscious goal priming reproduces effects of Keller, J., & Bless, H. (2006). Regulatory fit and cognitive
explicit task instructions. Journal of Personality and performance: The interactive effect of chronic and
Social Psychology, 71, 464478. situationally induced self-regulatory mechanisms on
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Thinking of test performance. Europena Journal of Social
you: Nonconscious pursuit of interpersonal goals Psychology, 36, 393405.
associated with relationship partners. Journal of Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Relating
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 148164. mindfulness and self-regulatory processes.
Friese, M., Messner, C., & Schaffner, Y. (2012). Psychological Inquiry, 18, 255258.
Mindfulness meditation counteracts self-control deple- Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960).
tion. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 10161022. Plans and the structure of behavior. New York, NY:
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiol- Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
ogy of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self- Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Zeiss, A. R. (1972).
control. Personality and Social Psychology Review, Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of grat-
11, 303327. ification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, 21, 204218.
J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., . . . Schmeichel, B. J. Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature
(2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool
energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, Social Psychology, 54, 687696.
325336. Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation
Gailliot, M. T., Plant, E. A., Butz, D. A., & Baumeister, and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control
R. F. (2007). Increasing self-regulatory strength can resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126,
reduce the depleting effect of suppressing stereotypes. 247259.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, Muraven, M., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1999).
281294. Longitudinal improvement of self-regulation through
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory practice: Building self-control strength through
of crime. Stanford, CA: University Press. repeated exercise. Journal of Social Psychology, 139,
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. 446457.
D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of Muraven, M., Collins, R. L., & Nienhaus, K. (2002). Self-
self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, control and alcohol restraint: An initial application of
136, 495525. the self-control strength model. Psychology of
Hanh, T. N. (1976). Miracle of mindfulness. Boston, MA: Addictive Behaviors, 16, 113120.
Beacon. Muraven, M., Shmueli, D., & Burkley, E. (2006).
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating Conserving self-control strength. Journal of
self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319340. Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 524537.
8 Self-Regulatory Strength and Mindfulness 105

Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanisms of diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26,
self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. 978986.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004).
894906. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less
Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success.
Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory deple- Journal of Personality, 72, 271324.
tion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Tangney, J. P., Niedenthal, P. M., Covert, M. V., & Barlow,
Psychology, 74, 774789. D. H. (1998). Are shame and guilt related to distinct
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2006). Longitudinal gains in self-discrepancies? A test of Higgins (1987) hypoth-
self-regulation from regular physical exercise. British eses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Journal of Health Psychology, 11, 717733. 75, 256268.
Pratt, T. C., & Cullen, F. T. (2000). The empirical status of Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001).
Gottfredson and Hirschis general theory of crime: A Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over
meta-analysis. Criminology, 38, 931964. impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of
Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2009). Self-affirmation Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 5367.
and self-control: Affirming core values counteracts Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Ciarocco, N. J. (2005).
ego depletion. Journal of Personality and Social Self-regulation and self-presentation: Regulatory
Psychology, 96, 770782. resource depletion impairs impression management
Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., & Friedman, R. S. (1998). and effortful self-presentation depletes regulatory
Performance incentives and means: How regulatory resources. Journal of Personality and Social
focus influences goal attainment. Journal of Psychology, 88, 632657.
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 285293. Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Schmeichel, B. J.
Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting (2012). Motivation, personal beliefs, and limited
adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies resources all contribute to self-control. Journal of
from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying Experimental Social Psychology, 4, 943947.
Mindfulness and Emotion
Regulation 9
Whitney L. Heppner, Claire A. Spears,
Jennifer Irvin Vidrine, and David W. Wetter

The exploration of mindfulness and its relation to tions of mindfulness include processes that relate
emotions and emotion regulation is a widely to emotion regulation specifically. In this chapter,
researched area of the mindfulness literature. It is we begin by noting the implications for emotions
broadly acknowledged that mindfulness has a and emotion regulation in a variety of models of
large impact on the awareness and labeling of mindfulness. Then, to highlight the wealth of
emotions, the experience of emotions, and the research linking mindfulness to emotion regula-
level of positive and negative emotions that are tion, we provide an overview of the role of mind-
experienced. As a result, varied conceptualiza- fulness in general emotional states, emotional
reactions to stimuli and events, and emotions
over time. Importantly, however, in this chapter
The preparation of this chapter was supported by the
we will go beyond emotion regulation as an out-
National Cancer Institute through grant R25-T CA57730 come of being mindful, and we will begin to link
and MD Andersons Cancer Center Support Grant the impact of mindfulness on emotion regulation
CA016672, and through a faculty fellowship from the to behavioral change. Specifically, by highlight-
University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center Duncan
Family Institute for Cancer Prevention and Risk Assessment.
ing research on smoking, alcohol use, and other
addictive behaviors, we will demonstrate that
Acknowledgement Significant portions of this chapter
emotion regulation serves as a key mechanism in
were completed while all authors were affiliated with the
Department of Health Disparities Research, University of the relationship between mindfulness and some
Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, TX, USA. domains of behavioral regulation.
W.L. Heppner (*)
Department of Psychological Science,
Georgia College & State University,
Milledgeville, GA 31061, USA Conceptualizations of Mindfulness
e-mail: whitney.heppner@gcsu.edu and Its Role in Emotion Regulation
C.A. Spears
Department of Psychology, The Catholic University Consensus about an empirical definition of mind-
of America, Washington, DC, USA fulness is still lacking. As a result, a number of
J.I. Vidrine operational definitions and conceptual mod-
Stephenson Cancer Center and Department of Family els of the broad construct of mindfulness exist.
& Preventive Medicine, The University of Oklahoma
Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK, USA Below, we describe several definitions/models of
mindfulness, highlighting the aspects of each that
D.W. Wetter
Department of Psychology, Rice University, Houston, relate to emotion regulation and models of emo-
TX, USA tion regulation.

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 107


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_9
108 W.L. Heppner et al.

Brown and Ryan (2003) have advanced a uni- concrete and present-focused. Meditation is one
dimensional notion of mindfulness as enhanced suggested avenue for cultivating hypo-egoic
attention to and awareness of current experience states, whereby conscious thoughts are observed
or present reality (p. 822). This singular and without reacting to them, and over time self-
largely cognitive conceptualization ties high lev- conscious thoughts decrease in number and
els of mindfulness to emotion regulation by com- intensity. Mindful states specifically are posited
paring mindfulness to constructs such as to reduce abstract and self-evaluative thoughts.
emotional intelligence and openness to experi- The authors state Often, people misregulate
ence and by describing a mindful mode of opera- because they resist the reality in which they find
tion as an open or receptive attention and themselves (p. 1823). In contrast, mindfulness
awareness that produces clarity of and receptivity promotes acceptance of the current reality,
toward emotions. including ones current emotional state. In addi-
Other definitions of mindfulness include this tion, mindfulness involves focus on the present in
cognitive component of attention and awareness concrete ways, which should reduce rumination
as part of a larger system, as one of several theo- and reactivity to experienced emotions. Cast in
retical subcomponents of which one or more are the light of the hypo-egoic regulation framework,
directly implicated in emotion regulation. Bishop highly mindful individuals should exhibit
et al. (2004) discuss mindfulness in terms of (1) reduced self-conscious reflection on emotion,
the self-regulation of attention, and (2) an orien- instead observing and accepting their emotions
tation to experience characterized by curiosity without attempting deliberate control of their
and openness to experience. Importantly, this emotional states; this lack of self-conscious,
second facet includes a curiosity and acceptance deliberate control should facilitate effective emo-
toward thoughts, feelings, and experiences that tion regulation.
enter conscious awareness. Shapiro, Carlson, Mindfulness has been studied as a naturally
Astin, and Freedman (2006) reflect this same ori- occurring individual difference (i.e., trait or dis-
entation to experience as a quality of attitudes, positional mindfulness), which refers to an indi-
one of three axioms (along with intention and viduals tendency to observe and accept
attention) which comprise mindfulness. The present-moment experiences in day-to-day life
attitude necessary for mindful intention and (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney,
attention includes compassion and openness, 2006; Brown & Ryan, 2003). Researchers have
such that mindful experience of emotions also examined effects of short exercises (i.e.,
becomes the capacity not to continually strive mindfulness inductions) and training programs
for pleasant experiences, or to push aversive (i.e., mindfulness-based treatment) intended to
experiences away (p. 377). From both of these increase mindfulness. Although differences
perspectives, noticing and accepting emotions in likely exist among these operation definitions of
this way over time would predict further experi- the theoretical construct of mindfulness, trait
ence of emotions (i.e., less avoidance of emo- mindfulness, mindfulness inductions, and
tions), greater tolerance of emotional states, mindfulness-based treatments appear associated
increased complexity of emotional representa- with improved emotion regulation, as will be dis-
tions (e.g., emotion differentiation), and less cussed below. In fact, mindfulness-based treat-
reactivity to emotions (i.e., lower likelihood of ments often specifically address or implicate
impulsive responses to emotions). emotion regulation.
Leary, Adams, and Tate (2006) position mind- Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction (MBSR;
fulness as a mode of hypo-egoic regulation, Kabat-Zinn, 1982, 1990) teaches mindfulness
whereby self-regulation occurs without a focus skills through mindfulness meditation exercises.
on the self. Hypo-egoic states are characterized These exercises encourage thoughts and feelings
by low self-awareness (in terms of the self as an to be observed nonjudgmentally, without becom-
object of reflection) and/or self-thoughts that are ing absorbed in their content (Kabat-Zinn, 1982).
9 Emotion Regulation 109

Through repeated nonjudgmental observation of Mindfulness-based Relapse Prevention (MBRP;


feelings, an individual learns that emotions are Witkiewitz & Marlatt, 2004) encourages
transient states. In Mindfulness-based Cognitive increased awareness of thoughts, emotions, and
Therapy (MBCT; Teasdale et al., 2000), this sensations that characterize high-risk situations
same approach of observing thoughts, emotions, for relapse to substance use, thereby allowing
and sensations nonjudgmentally is bolstered by a individuals to notice their often automatic urges
focus on decentering or detaching from ones for substance use and to choose not to use the
thoughts and emotions. In this way, depressed addictive substance. A primary motive for return-
individuals learn that negative emotions are tran- ing to substance use among those who have quit
sient states and that their negative emotions are is relief from negative affect (Baker, Brandon, &
separate from self, which over time mitigates Chassin, 2004), so mindful emotion regulation
the consuming nature of negative emotions and serves to increase the likelihood of continued
thoughts. In Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT; abstinence. (For further reference, see Baer,
Linehan, 1993) clients are taught mindfulness 2003, where many of these programs are reviewed
skills that are similar to skills gained from MBSR in greater detail and in a broader context [i.e., not
programs, including observing, describing, focused on emotion regulation].)
and nonjudgment. As a treatment for Borderline In summary, across varied theoretical concep-
Personality Disorder, which is characterized by tualizations of mindfulness as well as
extreme emotional instability, DBT targets emo- mindfulness-based programs and interventions,
tion regulation directly by encouraging the syn- emotion regulation is consistently implicated as a
thesis of dialectical forces of acceptance of consequence of fostering a mindful state.
ones current state and change toward a better However, what evidence exists linking mindful-
state. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy ness to emotion regulation? Below, we briefly
(ACT; Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, review some evidence linking mindfulness to
2006; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999) also positive and negative emotional states, as well as
encourages mindful acceptance of emotional evidence linking mindfulness to the regulation of
states, particularly emphasizing the separation of emotion following emotional stimuli, emotional
thoughts, emotions, and sensations from the self- events, and emotional states over time. Within
as-observer. This separation of thoughts from self this review, we highlight multiple avenues
allows for objective distance from emotions, through which mindfulness benefits the regula-
thereby decreasing their impact on experience. tion of emotions including: increased willingness
Notably, mindfulness can be taught and encour- to experience negative emotions, reduced reactiv-
aged through meditation (as is largely the case ity to emotional stimuli and situations, dampened
with MBSR and MBCT) as well as mindfulness self-conscious emotions and self-esteem involve-
exercises that do not necessarily involve medita- ment, increased stability in emotions over time,
tion per se (as in DBT and ACT). and activation (or deactivation) of consistent
Mindfulness-based treatments have also been brain areas early in the time course of affective
developed and applied to the treatment of sub- processing.
stance use disorders. For example, Mindfulness-
based Addiction Treatment (MBAT; Vidrine
et al., 2015), based largely on MBCT, aims to Empirical Relations of Mindfulness
help individuals to (1) become more aware of with Emotion Regulation
thoughts, feelings, and sensations from moment
to moment, (2) develop a different way of relat- An abundance of research exists linking high lev-
ing to thoughts, feelings, and sensations, and (3) els of mindfulness, or increases in mindfulness
obtain the ability to disengage attention and through training, to lower levels of negative affect
choose skillful responses to any thoughts, and higher levels of positive affect. This robust
feelings, or situations that arise. Similarly, finding occurs across varying samples (clinical
110 W.L. Heppner et al.

and non-clinical), varying ages (youth/adoles- Importantly, individuals who underwent a


cents through adults), and varying conceptualiza- focused breathing task exhibited greater willing-
tions of mindfulness (especially dispositional and ness to view highly negative pictures during an
treatment). Some examples include: in cancer optional portion of the experiment.
patients, mindfulness training predicted reduced Similarly, in another series of studies, highly
anxiety, depression, and anger, and increased mindful individuals exhibited a pattern of behav-
vigor (e.g., Speca, Carlson, Goodey, & Angen, ior consistent with greater nonjudgmental atten-
2000); across a range of clinical populations, tion and awareness of existential threat. Niemiec
mindfulness-based training programs are effec- et al. (2010) examined mindfulness in the context
tive for a number of disorders marked by emotion of terror management theory (Greenberg,
dysregulation (see Baer, 2003, for a review); in Solomon, & Pyszczynski, 1997), which posits
healthy individuals, trait mindfulness has pre- that when peoples mortality is made salient to
dicted self-reported affect (e.g., Brown & Ryan, them, a host of defensive processes ensue collec-
2003), and mindfulness meditation has predicted tively termed worldview defense. These
increased prefrontal activation asymmetries sug- defenses include proximal defenses such as sup-
gestive of temperamental shifts toward greater pression of thoughts of ones own mortality, as
positive affect (Davidson et al., 2003); and in well as distal defenses such as a cultural world-
youth/adolescent psychiatric patients, self- view and self-esteem. Niemiec et al. (2010) dem-
reported trait mindfulness has predicted less neg- onstrated that highly mindful individuals
ative affect and greater positive affect, happiness, suppressed thoughts of death to a lesser extent
and life satisfaction (Brown, West, Loverich, & following a mortality salience manipulation (i.e.,
Biegel, 2011) and mindfulness training reduced describing thoughts of their own death and what
anxiety and depression (Biegel, Brown, Shapiro, happens to them when they die) than less mindful
& Schubert, 2009). Another testament to the individuals, and that highly mindful individuals
robust nature of this relationship between mind- were willing to write about their own deaths for a
fulness and lower negative/higher positive affect longer period of time.
is that it emerges across methodologiesin Importantly, theoretical and empirical work
cross-sectional designs, short-term longitudinal from a broad literature suggests that an increased
designs, and pre-test/post-test designs. willingness to experience negative emotions will
In addition to promoting higher positive affect benefit emotion regulation. For example, cogni-
and lower negative affect, mindfulness appears to tive research on ironic suppression effects sug-
enhance the regulation of unpleasant affective gests that attempts to not think about a white
states. This evidence comes from experimental bear actually increase those same prohibited
and quasi-experimental studies that examine thoughts (Wegner et al., 1987; Wenzlaff &
links between mindfulness and responses to Wegner, 2000), thus attempting to ignore or deny
valenced stimuli and situations. Consistent with unpleasant thoughts and feelings can make them
the conceptual models including nonjudgmental more influential. Similarly, a number of effective
or non-attached awareness of negative states, therapies discussed here (e.g., DBT, MBCT), as
some studies link mindfulness to increased will- well as validated models of addictive behaviors
ingness to experience or stay with negative emo- (e.g., Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie, &
tions. In one study, a brief (15 min) mindfulness Fiore, 2004), suggest that noticing and, thus,
induction impacted responses to neutral and neg- experiencing (negative) feelings is an important
ative affective stimuli (Arch & Craske, 2006). pre-cursor to responding to situations flexibly
Specifically, participants who underwent unfo- and adaptively (e.g., choosing not to use a sub-
cused attention or worry inductions responded stance in the presence of an urge; we return to
more negatively to neutral slides post-induction, this issue below).
while those in the focused breathing induction Mindful emotion regulation appears to be
maintained moderately positive responses. characterized by a reduced reactivity to emo-
9 Emotion Regulation 111

tional stimuli and situations (Arch & Craske, powerful than negative experiences, a phenom-
2010; Britton, Shahar, Szepsenwol, & Jacobs, enon known as the negativity bias (Baumeister
2012; Raes, Dewulf, Van Heeringen, & Williams, et al., 2001), being highly mindful could also
2009). Raes et al. (2009) found that high lev- enhance ones ability to experience the rewards
els of trait mindfulness were inversely related of positive events more fully. Indeed, a study
to cognitive reactivity to sad moods, and that of the effects of MBCT on day-to-day positive
MBCT reduced cognitive reactivity from pre- experience suggested that compared to baseline
test to post-test, compared to a wait-list control levels and compared to a control group, mind-
group. Similarly, participants in an MBCT pro- fulness training led to rating pleasant experi-
gram exhibited decreased emotional reactiv- ences as more positive and to experiencing
ity following the Trier Social Stress Test, and more positive affect following pleasant experi-
a lack of anticipatory anxiety that was present ences (i.e., more rewards from pleasant expe-
in the control group (Britton et al., 2012). This riences; Geschwind et al., 2011). This aligns
reduced emotional reactivity to stress partially with findings linking mindfulness to positive
mediated improvements in depressive symp- affect and emotions in general (e.g., Brown &
toms found in the MBCT group. In a sample of Ryan, 2003; Davidson et al., 2003) and with the
clinically anxious and control participants, trait broaden-and-build theoretical model that sug-
mindfulness predicted lower negative affect and gests that fostering mindfulness encourages an
anxiety following a breathing task designed to upward spiral of positive emotions and flour-
mimic hyperventilation (Arch & Craske, 2010). ishing (Garland et al., 2010).
The contribution of mindfulness to affective Consistent with the broaden-and-build theory,
responses was over and above the contributions Catalino and Fredrickson (2011) found that flour-
of anxiety and depression levels. Further prob- ishers (i.e., those with high emotional well-being
ing revealed that the effect of trait mindfulness and regular experiences of positive emotion) expe-
on post-stressor negative affect was present for rience greater emotional boosts from pleasant
individuals high in anxiety sensitivity but not experiences in their daily lives. That is, flourishers
for those low in anxiety sensitivity. In addition, experience greater emotional reactivity to positive
reflective of the increased willingness to expe- events. Furthermore, Catalino and Fredrickson
rience negative emotions discussed above, trait found that greater reactivity to pleasant events was
mindfulness was correlated positively with dura- positively associated with dispositional mindful-
tion in the hyperventilation-breathing task when ness (particularly the observing and non-reactivity
participants were encouraged to continue as long facets). Thus, mindfulness might predict greater
as they were able (Arch & Craske, 2010). emotional reactivity to pleasant events. This notion
Of note, studies linking mindfulness to is akin to the concept of savoring, through which
reduced emotional reactivity to specific stimuli individuals maximize benefits of positive life
or events have done so almost exclusively with experiences through purposeful attention (Bryant
negative stimuli or events. It is plausible, then, & Veroff, 2007). Mindful savoring of pleasant
that highly mindful individuals are also less experiences could potentially prompt upswings in
happy, joyful, proud, etc., when they experi- positive emotion. Future research should further
ence positive events such as winning an award explore the role of mindfulness in emotional
or receiving a compliment. To our knowledge, responses to specific positive experiences and how
no studies address this point directly; however, these differ from emotional responses to negative
at least one study suggests that mindful indi- experiences.
viduals display less variability in both positive The decentered perspective (i.e., a detachment
and negative emotions in the course of their from thoughts and emotions) fostered by
daily lives (Hill & Updegraff, 2012). However, mindfulness also appears to dampen a number of
whereas the positive effects of pleasant expe- negative self-conscious emotions and emotional
riences are typically fleeting and much less responses to self-concept threatening stimuli and
112 W.L. Heppner et al.

situations. For example, social rejection has a demonstrated that the defensiveness typically
robust negative effect on individuals in labora- triggered by thoughts of ones own death was
tory studies (see Leary, Twenge, & Quinlivan, attenuated among highly mindful individuals.
2006, for a review). Heppner et al. (2008) Furthermore, Leary et al., (2007) demonstrated
explored the capacity for mindfulness to buffer that high levels of self-compassion, of which
aggressive reactions to social rejection. Sixty mindfulness is a central component, predicted
participants met in small groups to introduce reduced emotional reactivity to negative events
themselves and to receive study instructions. across a variety of contexts, including recall-
Then, they were separated and wrote self- ing and imagining negative events, reacting to
descriptive essays, which were ostensibly distrib- ambivalent feedback, and experiencing day-to-
uted to all other participants. Based on these day negative events.
essays, they voted for who they liked, respected, Importantly, experience sampling methodol-
and wanted to work with on a later task. ogies reveal similar benefits for mindful emo-
Participants were given (bogus) feedback about tion regulation over time. Experience sampling
the voting to make them feel accepted or rejected methodologies involve repeated assessments of
by their fellow participants. Then, they were the same construct within the context of the par-
given the opportunity to play an aversive noise in ticipants current experiences. Thus, experience
the ears of their opponents in a competitive reac- sampling methodologies allow for examination
tion time task. Short mindfulness instructions of moment-to-moment experience, as well as
delivered just prior to receiving the rejection trends in experience over time (e.g., instability).
feedback predicted less aggressive behavior Utilizing experience sampling with a sample of
(shorter, less intense noise blast selections) in the adults and a sample of introductory psychology
competitive task (Heppner et al., 2008). students, Brown and Ryan (2003) investigated
Similar to self-concept threats from others, moment-to-moment links between trait mind-
as in the case of social rejection, a persons own fulness, state mindfulness (in the student sam-
reflections on negative behaviors he/she has per- ple), pleasant and unpleasant affect, and
formed can threaten self-concept. Individuals autonomous self-regulation. For 21 (in the adult
vary in the degree to which they can recall and sample) or 14 (in the student sample) days, par-
discuss such negative events with awareness ticipants were sent pager messages on a quasi-
and without distortion (i.e., to make them appear random schedule which signaled them to
less negative; Barrett, Williams, & Fong, 2002). complete one paper and pencil form from a
Lakey, Kernis, Heppner, and Lance (2008) bound packet. In both samples, dispositional
explored the role of mindfulness in such reflec- mindfulness predicted day-to-day autonomy
tions on past negative behaviors. Participants and unpleasant affect (but not pleasant affect).
answered a series of 25 questions that were pro- Momentary or state mindfulness predicted
gressively more personal and imposing (e.g., greater autonomy and pleasant affect, and less
Tell me about your most enjoyable experi- negative affect, and these relationships held
ence, Tell me about a time when youve done controlling for trait mindfulness. In a more
something unethical on an assignment in the recent study (Hill & Updegraff, 2012), under-
presence of trained interviewers. Their answers graduate participants reported on their current
were later coded for verbal defensiveness, emotional experience approximately six times a
which is an assessment of the traces left by day for a week. Dispositional mindfulness pre-
defensive processes in the content and structure dicted fewer self-reported emotion regulation
of speech (Barrett et al., 2002, p. 777). They difficulties, as well as greater emotion differen-
found that individuals who were highly mind- tiation. Importantly, greater emotion differentia-
ful exhibited less verbal defensiveness when tion (positive and negative) mediated the
discussing past indiscretions. Along these same relationship between mindfulness and reduced
lines, as already discussed, Niemic et al. (2010) emotional instability (positive and negative)
9 Emotion Regulation 113

over the course of the week. In other words, (self-referential areas and the right amygdala). In
more mindful individuals evidenced lower vola- addition to differences in resting activity, this
tility or lability in their emotions. More research same study showed less amygdala activation in
is needed examining the role of mindfulness in response to emotional faces in highly mindful
everyday experiences of emotion and ongoing individuals, while depressive symptoms corre-
emotion regulation in naturally occurring lated positively with amygdala activation to emo-
contexts. tional faces. Reduced resting activation largely
accounted for the links between mindfulness and
reduced activation in response to emotional
Neurological Underpinnings stimuli.
of Mindful Emotion Regulation Interestingly, the way mindfulness influences
emotion regulation may differ as one becomes
While much of the data linking mindfulness to more experienced in being mindful. Taylor
emotional experience has been obtained using et al. (2011) compared the emotional responses
self-report methodologies, a growing body of of long-term meditators versus beginners. For
research points to the neurological underpinnings both groups, positive and negative images were
of mindful emotion regulation. Several studies rated lower in emotional intensity when viewed
point to reduced reactivity to emotional stimuli in a mindful state compared to a baseline/nor-
and illustrate the down-regulating role being mal state. Interestingly, the neural mechanisms
mindful plays in such reactions by virtue of (de-) underlying this were different for beginners
activations of consistent brain areas, and at least versus experienced meditators. In beginners,
one study suggests that mindful emotion regula- viewing emotional images in a mindful state
tion occurs early in affective processing. was related to down-regulation of the amygdala,
High levels of mindfulness are linked to while for experienced meditators, viewing emo-
increased activation in the prefrontal cortex and tional images was related to reduced activation in
decreased amygdala activation when processing right medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cin-
emotional stimuli. These patterns of brain activa- gulate cortex. This pattern suggests that higher-
tion are thought to reflect lower negative affect and order cortical regions become less involved in
enhanced emotion regulation. Creswell, Way, emotion regulation in experienced meditators.
Eisenberger, and Lieberman (2007) demonstrated In other words, when one is extensively trained
that high levels of trait mindfulness were associ- in mindfulness, experiences of reduced emo-
ated with enhanced prefrontal cortex activation tional intensity may be explained by accepting
when labeling negative affective stimuli. Similarly, and fully experiencing emotional responses in
Modinos, Ormel, and Aleman (2010) linked high the present moment without interference from
levels of mindfulness skills to increased dorsome- internal thought and self-referent processes, as
dial prefrontal cortex activation, an area involved opposed to directly controlling low-order affec-
in reappraisal. This increased activation was tive brain systems from higher-order cortical
inversely related to amygdala activation in regions (Taylor et al., 2011, p. 1531).
response to negative stimuli. Mindfulness appears to be implicated in the
Neural mechanisms of mindfulness appear to process of emotion regulation versus reactiv-
operate in stark contrast to indices of emotional ity in an early phase of emotional processing.
dysregulation such as depressive symptomatol- Brown, Goodman, and Inzlicht (2012) investi-
ogy. In one study (Way, Creswell, Eisenberger, & gated links between trait mindfulness and late
Lieberman, 2010), highly mindful individuals positive potential (LPP) responses. LPPs are
showed less resting activation in self-referential thought to index attention to emotional stimuli
processing areas and in the amygdala (bilater- and to mark emotional arousal. Highly mindful
ally), while depressive symptoms were correlated individuals showed lower LPPs to highly arous-
with greater resting activation in these areas ing positive (erotica) and negative (corpses)
114 W.L. Heppner et al.

images. In this same study, neuroticism and responsible for an estimated 443,000 deaths a
negative affectivity (trait) were both linked to year (CDC, 2008) and is linked to approximately
increased LPP responses to highly arousing 30 % of all cancers (Mokdad, Marks, Stroup, &
negative images. Gerberding, 2004) and 90 % of lung cancers
(U.S. Department of Health and Human Services,
2004). Abuse of tobacco, alcohol, and other
Mindful Emotion Regulation drugs costs our nation more than $484 billion per
as a Mechanism of Behavioral year (ONDCP, 2001). Unfortunately, drug addic-
Outcomes tion is considered a chronic relapsing condition
(Connors, Maisto, & Donovan, 1996) and relapse
As we have shown in this chapter, evidence is rates are high following treatment (McLellan,
mounting that high levels of mindfulness are Lewis, OBrien, & Kleber, 2000).
linked to more positive and less negative emo- Mindfulness may assist in cessation from
tional states, as well as less variable emotional smoking, alcohol or other drugs. Enhanced
states. Furthermore, mindfulness appears to attention to thoughts and feelings allows for the
reduce reactivity to emotional stimuli and events. early identification of problematic thoughts and
Thus, studies suggest that mindfulness plays a feelings, and promotes adaptive, flexible
substantial role in effective emotion regulation. responding to these feelings (Roemer & Orsillo,
The ways in which people regulate their emo- 2003; Teasdale, Segal, & Williams, 1995) rather
tions are thought to impact a number of behav- than automatized or reactive responses.
ioral outcomes. For example, a prominent model Furthermore, observation, exposure, and open-
of addiction posits that the avoidance of negative ness to experience are core components of mind-
affective states is a primary motive for drug use fulness (Baer et al., 2006; Bishop et al., 2004;
(Baker et al., 2004; Baker, Piper et al., 2004). Shapiro et al., 2006). In stark contrast to this
Furthermore, nonacceptance of unpleasant emo- notion, prominent models of addiction implicate
tions has been implicated in smoking and other the avoidance of negative affective states as a
substance use (Adams, Tull, & Gratz, 2012; primary motive for drug use (Baker, Brandon, &
Axelrod, Perepletchikova, Holtzman, & Sinha, Chassin, 2004; Baker, Piper et al., 2004). A
2011; Carmody, Vieten, & Astin, 2007). highly mindful individual may be more aware
Emerging evidence links trait mindfulness and and accepting of unpleasant sensations (e.g.,
mindfulness training (which explicitly involve negative affect, craving), and thus less likely to
increased awareness and acceptance of emotions) respond impulsively via substance use (Groves
to reductions in substance use and better cessa- & Farmer, 1994).
tion outcomes (e.g., Heppner et al., 2015; Emerging evidence links trait mindfulness,
Witkiewitz & Marlatt, 2004; Chap. 14), for addi- brief mindfulness exercises, and more intensive
tional discussion of mindfulness and addiction). mindfulness-based interventions to lower addic-
Therefore, more effective emotion regulation tion and enhanced cessation. One study with
provides a theoretical mechanism for the effects smokers at the time of enrollment in a smoking
of mindfulness on substance use and cessation cessation treatment program demonstrated that
outcomes. In this section we illustrate the mecha- individuals high in trait mindfulness were less
nistic role of mindful emotion regulation for the dependent on cigarettes and demonstrated less
self-regulation of behavior by reviewing evi- affective withdrawal severity and a greater sense
dence linking mindfulness to substance use dis- of agency for their upcoming quit attempt
orders, including smoking, alcohol, and other (Vidrine et al., 2009). High levels of trait mind-
drugs, and how emotion regulation mediates fulness have also been linked to less use of alco-
these relationships. hol and alcohol-related problems in samples not
Substance abuse and dependence represent dependent on alcohol (Fernandez, Wood, Stein,
significant public health problems. Smoking is & Rossi, 2010; Smith et al., 2011).
9 Emotion Regulation 115

In addition to evidence with trait mindfulness, role of emotion regulation in cessation. Negative
more controlled studies link mindfulness exer- affect is consistently implicated in relapse to sub-
cises and training to lower substance use. An stance use (e.g., Baker, Piper et al., 2004; Cooney,
experimental investigation of a brief mindfulness Litt, Morse, Bauer, & Gaupp, 1997; Hodgins,
exercise (body scan) with smokers demonstrated El-Guebaly, & Armstrong, 1995; Witkiewitz &
that compared to a control group, participants in Villarroel, 2009). Given the preponderance of evi-
the body scan condition reported significantly dence linking mindfulness to lower levels of neg-
lower desire to smoke for up to 5 min after the ative affect, it follows that mindfulness may aid in
intervention (May, Andrade, Willoughby, & cessation from smoking/substance use through its
Brown, 2011). In one randomized controlled effects on emotion regulation. Figure 9.1 (adapted
pilot trial of mindfulness training for smoking from Adams, Benitez et al., 2012; Adams, Cano
cessation (Brewer et al., 2011), individuals in the et al., 2012) illustrates potential emotional path-
eight-session mindfulness training group were ways through which mindfulness may aid in ces-
more likely to be abstinent from smoking at a sation. First, mindfulness reduces negative affect,
17-week follow up when compared to a standard which aids cessation and reduces relapse. Second,
treatment control group (Freedom From mindfulness also appears to attenuate the link
Smoking). Similarly, Bowen et al. (2009) evalu- between negative affect and substance craving/
ated MBRP as an aftercare program for individu- use. Researchers (e.g., Adams, Benitez et al.,
als who had completed intensive treatment for 2012; Adams, Cano et al., 2012; Witkiewitz &
substance use disorders (alcohol and other drugs) Bowen, 2010) have described these as mediation
and found that those in the MBRP group and moderation pathways that may occur
decreased drug use to a greater extent than those simultaneously. Evidence for each pathway is
in treatment as usual at 2 months post interven- reviewed in turn below.
tion (although this effect had decayed by 4 First, mindfulness may influence substance
months post intervention). use and cessation by reducing negative affect. In
The burgeoning literature on mindfulness, a sample of smokers attempting to quit, individu-
addiction, and substance use also illustrates the als high in trait mindfulness were more likely to

Influence on the relationship


between affect and behavior

Mindfulness

Emotional Experience
Mindfulness Substance use
(+) positive affect
and/or relapse
(-) negative affect
(-) stress/anxiety
(-) depressive
symptoms
(-) craving

Influence on affect levels

Fig. 9.1 Conceptual model of the role of mindfulness and emotion regulation in substance use and/or relapse
116 W.L. Heppner et al.

be abstinent over time, examined at follow-up leads to craving, and craving strongly predicts
time points of 3 days, 31 days, and 26 weeks relapse to drug use (for example, cigarettes;
post-quit (Heppner et al., 2013). For early smok- Shiffman et al., 2002). Therefore, mindfulness
ing abstinence (i.e., 3 days post-quit), reduced may operate on substance use behaviors not only
negative affectdepressive symptoms, sadness, by mitigating negative affect itself, but also by
and angermediated the relation between mind- weakening the link between emotional states,
fulness and early smoking abstinence. In other craving/urge, and drug use. Data from this mod-
words, highly mindful individuals had lower lev- eration perspective includes studies with trait
els of negative affect which translated into greater mindfulness, short mindfulness instructions, and
likelihood of smoking abstinence at day 3 of their mindfulness-based treatment programs.
quit attempt. Among a sample of smokers attempting to
Other studies are consistent with the notion quit, high levels of trait mindfulness were linked
that mindfulness reduces negative affect and to less perceived stress, as well as less problem-
associated substance cravings and substance use. atic drinking, indexed by quantity of alcohol
For example, May et al. (2011) found that a consumed, frequency of binge drinking, and
mindfulness exercise (the body scan) reduced likelihood of alcohol abuse/dependence (Adams,
irritability, tension, restlessness, and smoking Cano et al., 2012). Moreover, trait mindfulness
urges. In another laboratory-based study, partici- moderated the relationship between perceived
pants using both a suppression-based and a stress and quantity of alcohol use such that
mindfulness-based strategy for coping with ciga- higher perceived stress predicted increased alco-
rette cravings showed reduced smoking at 7-day hol use among participants low but not high in
follow-up; however, only participants in the trait mindfulness. In a sample of college stu-
mindfulness condition also showed reductions in dents, brief mindfulness instructions given in the
negative affect and depressive symptoms lab reduced cigarettes smoked per day over the
(Rogojanski, Vettese, & Antony, 2011). Thus, the 7-day follow up, but had no effect on reports of
mindfulness-based strategy appeared to target negative affect or urge to smoke (Bowen &
emotion regulation specifically whereas the Marlatt, 2009). However, participants given the
(here, equally effective) suppression strategy did mindfulness instructions showed a weaker rela-
not operate via an emotional pathway. In a pilot tionship between negative affect and urges to
study of an intensive mindfulness meditation- smoke than those in the control group. In another
based intervention, Mindfulness Training for laboratory study brief mindfulness instructions
Smokers, results demonstrated that highly com- attenuated the association between negative
pliant meditators achieved 100 % abstinence at 6 affect and smoking urges (Adams, Benitez et al.,
weeks post-quit (Davis, Fleming, Bonus, & 2012). Whereas negative affect was strongly
Baker, 2007). Highly compliant meditators also linked to increased smoking urges among female
showed a concomitant decrease in perceived smokers in a control condition, negative affect
stress from baseline to 1-day post quit while was not related to smoking urges among partici-
moderately compliant meditators showed an pants who had just listened to 20 min of mindful-
increase after quitting. Similarly, symptoms of ness instructions. A similar effect emerged in a
affective distress (e.g., depression, anxiety, and randomized trial of MBRP for substance use
hostility) decreased more in the highly compliant (alcohol, cocaine/crack, and methamphetamines;
group compared to the moderately compliant Witkiewitz & Bowen, 2010). MBRP participants
group from pre- to post-quit, although this differ- reduced substance use 4 months post-
ence was not significant in this small pilot study. intervention. While craving mediated the rela-
In addition to the effects of mindfulness on tionship between negative affect and substance
emotional experience, mindfulness may attenu- use in the treatment-as-usual group, in the
ate the link between unpleasant emotional states MBRP group, the link between negative affect
and substance craving/use. Negative affect often and craving was mitigated.
9 Emotion Regulation 117

Research on other behavioral outcomes (e.g., including (1) nonjudgmental awareness of


eating) also demonstrates burgeoning evidence negative states which results in increased willing-
that mindfulness aids in the self-regulation of ness to experience negative emotions, (2) a reduced
behaviors through its effects on emotion regula- reactivity to emotional stimuli and situations, (3) a
tion. For example, mindfulness-based treatment decentered perspective which dampens self-
programs have been applied to food cravings and conscious emotions and self-esteem involvement,
eating disorders with some positive results (see (4) an increased stability (i.e., reduced variability
Kristeller, Baer, & Quillian-Wolever, 2006, for a or volatility) in negative emotions over time, and
review) and aspects of trait mindfulness (5) the activation (or deactivation) of consistent
(Nonjudging and Acting with Awareness) have brain areas (e.g., amygdala) early in the time
been linked to fewer eating disorder symptoms course of affective processing. Finally, the regula-
(Adams et al., 2012). Given that a central feature tion of emotions is integral to the self-regulation of
of overeating (especially binge eating) is distur- a number of important behaviors. The highlighted
bances in affect regulation (Kristeller et al., research linking mindfulness to substance use and
2006), emotion regulation may be one mecha- cessation through its effects on emotion regulation
nism by which mindfulness promotes healthier demonstrates this important link. We hope research
eating patterns. One study of DBT adapted for continues to explore the effects of mindfulness on
Bulimia Nervosa demonstrated significant reduc- health and behavior, and that research further clar-
tions in binge eating and purging and concomi- ifies the role of emotion regulation in these links.
tant reductions in negative affect and emotional This avenue of future research will simultaneously
eating, although the effects on affect were not refine our empirical notions of the construct of
significant in this small sample (Safer, Telch, & mindfulness and will inform interventions and
Agras, 2001). Another pilot study of a meditation- treatment programs for other health and behavior
based intervention for binge-eating disorder outcomes.
yielded reductions in binge eating and binge eat-
ing severity along with reductions in depression
and anxiety from pre- to post-treatment (Kristeller References
& Hallett, 1999). Although more research is
needed, it is possible that mindfulness promotes Adams, C. E., Benitez, L., Kinsaul, J., McVay, M. A.,
Barbry, A., Thibodeaux, A., & Copeland, A. L. (2012).
more adaptive regulation of a variety of addictive
Effects of brief mindfulness instructions on reactions
behaviors (e.g., smoking, alcohol use, eating to body image stimuli among female smokers: An
behavior) through its effects on emotion experimental study. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 15,
regulation. 376384.
Adams, C. E., Cano, M. A., Heppner, W. L., Stewart,
D. W., Correa-Fernandez, V., Vidrine, J. I., Wetter,
D. W. (2012). Testing a moderated mediation model of
Conclusions mindfulness, psychosocial stress, and alcohol use
among African American smokers. Mindfulness, 6(2),
315325.
Being mindful and increasing mindfulness
Adams, C. E., McVay, J. C., Kinsaul, J., Benitez, L.,
clearly benefits emotion regulation. Emotions Vinci, C., Stewart, D. W., & Copeland, A. L. (2012).
and our relationships to emotions are sufficiently Unique relationships between facets of mindfulness
important to the concept of mindfulness that mul- and eating pathology among female smokers. Eating
Behaviors, 4, 390393.
tiple operational definitions of mindfulness
Adams, C. E., Tull, M. T., & Gratz, K. L. (2012). The role
implicate emotion regulation specifically. In of emotional nonacceptance in the relation between
addition, empirical evidence overwhelmingly depression and recent cigarette smoking. American
links mindfulness to less negative and more posi- Journal on Addictions, 21, 293301.
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mind-
tive affect. Our review of the extant literature
fulness: Emotion regulation following a focused
revealed that mindfulness appears to aid in emo- breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
tion regulation by one of several mechanisms 44, 18491858.
118 W.L. Heppner et al.

Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2010). Laboratory stressors Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of
in clinically anxious and non-anxious individuals: The being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychologi-
moderating role of mindfulness. Behaviour Research cal well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
and Therapy, 48, 495505. Psychology, 84, 822848.
Axelrod, S. R., Perepletchikova, F., Holtzman, K., & Brown, K. W., West, A. M., Loverich, T. M., & Biegel,
Sinha, R. (2011). Emotion regulation and substance G. M. (2011). Assessing adolescent mindfulness:
use frequency in women with substance dependence Validation of an adapted Mindful Attention Awareness
and borderline personality disorder receiving dialecti- Scale in adolescent normative and psychiatric popula-
cal behavior therapy. American Journal of Drug and tions. Psychological Assessment, 23, 10231033.
Alcohol Abuse, 37, 3742. Bryant, F. B., & Veroff, J. (2007). Savoring: A new model
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical inter- of positive experience. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
vention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125143. Carmody, T. P., Vieten, C., & Astin, J. A. (2007). Negative
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & affect, emotional acceptance, and smoking cessation.
Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment meth- Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 39, 499508.
ods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, Catalino, L. I., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2011). A Tuesday in
2745. the life of a flourisher: The role of positive emotional
Baker, T. B., Brandon, T. H., & Chassin, L. (2004). reactivity in optimal mental health. Emotion, 11,
Motivational influences on cigarette smoking. Annual 938950.
Review of Psychology, 55, 463491. CDC. (2008). Smoking-attributable mortality, years of
Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, potential life lost, and productivity lossesUnited
M. R., & Fiore, M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation States, 20002004. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly
reformulated: An affective processing model of nega- Report, 57, 12261228.
tive reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111, 3351. Connors, G. J., Maisto, S. A., & Donovan, D. M. (1996).
Barrett, L. F., Williams, N. L., & Fong, G. T. (2002). Conceptualizations of relapse: A summary of psycho-
Defensive verbal behavior assessment. Personality logical and psychobiological models. Addiction, 9,
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 776788. 513.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Cooney, N. L., Litt, M. D., Morse, P. A., Bauer, L. O., &
Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review Gaupp, L. (1997). Alcohol cue reactivity, negative-
of General Psychology, 5(4), 323370. mood reactivity, and relapse in treated alcoholic men.
Biegel, G. M., Brown, K. W., Shapiro, S. L., & Schubert, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 243250.
C. M. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., &
the treatment of adolescent psychiatric outpatients: A Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural correlates of dispo-
randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and sitional mindfulness during affect labeling.
Clinical Psychology, 77, 855866. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 560565.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J.,
N. D., Carmody, J., Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., .
A proposed operational definition. Clinical Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune
Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230241. function produced by mindfulness meditation.
Bowen, S., Chawla, N., Collins, S. E., Witkiewitz, K., Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564570.
Hsu, S., Grow, J., Marlatt, A. (2009). Mindfulness- Davis, J. M., Fleming, M. F., Bonus, K. A., & Baker, T. B.
based relapse prevention for substance use disorders: (2007). A pilot study on mindfulness based stress
A pilot efficacy trial. Substance Abuse, 30, 295305. reduction for smokers. BMC Complementary and
Bowen, S., & Marlatt, A. (2009). Surfing the urge: Brief Alternative Medicine, 7, 2.
mindfulness-based intervention for college student Fernandez, A. C., Wood, M. D., Stein, L., & Rossi, J. S.
smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23, (2010). Measuring mindfulness and examining its rela-
666671. tionship with alcohol use and negative consequences.
Brewer, J. A., Mallik, S., Babuscio, T. A., Nich, C., Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 24, 608616.
Johnson, H. E., Deleone, C. M., Weinstein, A. J. Garland, E. L., Fredrickson, B., Kring, A. M., Johnson, D.
(2011). Mindfulness training for smoking cessation: P., Meyer, P. S., & Penn, D. L. (2010). Upward spirals
Results from a randomized controlled trial. Drug and of positive emotions counter downward spirals of
Alcohol Dependence, 119, 7280. negativity: Insights from the broaden-and-build theory
Britton, W. B., Shahar, B., Szepsenwol, O., & Jacobs, and affective neuroscience on the treatment of emo-
W. J. (2012). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy tion dysfunctions and deficits in psychopathology.
improves emotional reactivity to social stress: Results Clinical Psychology Review, 30(7), 849864.
from a randomized controlled trial. Behavior Therapy, Geschwind, N., Peeters, F., Drukker, M., van Os, J., &
43, 365380. Wichers, M. (2011). Mindfulness training increases
Brown, K. W., Goodman, R. J., & Inzlicht, M. (2012). momentary positive emotions and reward experience
Dispositional mindfulness and the attenuation of neu- in adults vulnerable to depression: A randomized con-
ral responses to emotional stimuli. Social Cognitive trolled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 369377. Psychology, 79(5), 618.
9 Emotion Regulation 119

unpleasant self-relevant events: The implications of


Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). treating oneself kindly. Journal of Personality and
Terror management theory of self-esteem and cultural Social Psychology, 92, 887904.
worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual Leary, M. R., Twenge, J. M., & Quinlivan, E. (2006).
refinements. Advances in Experimental Social Interpersonal rejection as a determinant of anger and
Psychology, 29, 61139. aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
Groves, P., & Farmer, R. (1994). Buddhism and addic- 10(2), 111132.
tions. Addiction Research & Theory, 2, 183194. Linehan, M. M. (1993). Dialectical behavior therapy for
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & treatment of borderline personality disorder:
Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Implications for the treatment of substance abuse.
Model, processes, and outcomes. Behaviour Research NIDA Research Monograph, 137, 201216.
and Therapy, 44, 125. May, J., Andrade, J., Willoughby, K., & Brown, C. (2011).
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). An attentional control task reduces intrusive thoughts
Acceptance and commitment therapy: Understanding about smoking. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 14,
and treating human suffering. New York, NY: Guilford. 472478.
Heppner, W. L., Spears, C. A., Correa-Fernndez, V., McLellan, A. T., Lewis, D. C., OBrien, C. P., & Kleber,
Castro, Y., Li, Y., Reitzel, L. R., Vidrine, J. I., Mazas, H. D. (2000). Drug dependence, a chronic medical ill-
C. A., Cofta-Woerpel, L., Cinciripini, P. M., Ahluwalia, ness. JAMA, the Journal of the American Medical
J., & Wetter, D. W. (2015). Dispositional mindfulness Association, 284, 16891695.
predicts enhanced smoking cessation and smoking Modinos, G., Ormel, J., & Aleman, A. (2010). Individual
lapse recovery. Manuscript submitted for publication. differences in dispositional mindfulness and brain
Heppner, W. L., Kernis, M. H., Lakey, C. E., Campbell, activity involved in reappraisal of emotion. Social
W. K., Goldman, B. M., Davis, P. J., & Cascio, E. V. Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 369377.
(2008). Mindfulness as a means of reducing aggres- Mokdad, A. H., Marks, J. S., Stroup, D. F., & Gerberding,
sive behavior: Dispositional and situational evidence. J. L. (2004). Actual causes of death in the United
Aggressive Behavior, 34, 486496. States, 2000. JAMA, the Journal of the American
Hill, C. L., & Updegraff, J. A. (2012). Mindfulness and its Medical Association, 291, 12381245.
relationship to emotional regulation. Emotion, 12, Niemiec, C. P., Brown, K. W., Kashdan, T. B., Cozzolino,
8190. P. J., Breen, W. E., Levesque-Bristol, C., & Ryan,
Hodgins, D. C., El-Guebaly, N., & Armstrong, S. (1995). R. M. (2010). Being present in the face of existential
Prospective and retrospective reports of mood states threat: The role of trait mindfulness in reducing defen-
before relapse to substance use. Journal of Consulting sive responses to mortality salience. Journal of
and Clinical Psychology, 63, 400407. Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 344365.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behav- Office of National Drug Control Policy (ONDCP). (2001).
ioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the The economic costs of drug abuse in the United States
practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical con- 19921998 (NCJ-190636). National Criminal Justice
siderations and preliminary results. General Hospital Reference Service. https://www.ncjrs.gov/ondcppubs/
Psychiatry, 4, 3347. publications/pdf/economic_costs.pdf.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the Raes, F., Dewulf, D., Van Heeringen, C., & Williams,
wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and J. M. (2009). Mindfulness and reduced cognitive
illness. New York, NY: Bantam Dell. reactivity to sad mood: Evidence from a correla-
Kristeller, J. L., Baer, R. A., & Quillian-Wolever, R. tional study and a non-randomized waiting list con-
(2006). Mindfulness-based approaches to eating disor- trolled study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47,
ders. In R. A. Baer (Ed.), Mindfulness-based treatment 623627.
approaches: Clinicians guide to evidence base and Roemer, L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2003). Mindfulness: A
applications (pp. 7591). London: Elsevier. promising intervention strategy in need of further
Kristeller, J. L., & Hallett, C. B. (1999). An exploratory study. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10,
study of a meditation-based intervention for binge eat- 172178.
ing disorder. Journal of Health Psychology, 4, Rogojanski, J., Vettese, L. C., & Antony, M. M. (2011).
357364. Coping with cigarette cravings: Comparison of sup-
Lakey, C. E., Kernis, M. H., Heppner, W. L., & Lance, pression versus mindfulness-based strategies.
C. E. (2008). Individual differences in authenticity and Mindfulness, 2, 1426.
mindfulness as predictors of verbal defensiveness. Safer, D. L., Telch, C. F., & Agras, W. S. (2001).
Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 230238. Dialectical behavior therapy for bulimia nervosa.
Leary, M. R., Adams, C. E., & Tate, E. B. (2006). Hypo- American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 632634.
egoic self-regulation: Exercising self-control by Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman,
diminishing the influence of the self. Journal of B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of
Personality, 74, 18031831. Clinical Psychology, 62, 373386.
Leary, M. R., Tate, E. B., Adams, C. E., Allen, A. B., & Shiffman, S., Gwaltney, C. J., Balabanis, M. H., Liu,
Hancock, J. (2007). Self-compassion and reactions to K. S., Paty, J. A., Kassel, J. D., Gnys, M. (2002).
120 W.L. Heppner et al.

Immediate antecedents of cigarette smoking: An anal- Vidrine, J. I., Businelle, M. S., Cinciripini, P., Li, Y., Marcus,
ysis from ecological momentary assessment. Journal M. T., Waters, A. J., Wetter, D. W. (2009).
of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 531535. Associations of mindfulness with nicotine dependence,
Smith, B. W., Ortiz, J. A., Steffen, L. E., Tooley, E. M., withdrawal, and agency. Substance Abuse, 30, 318327.
Wiggins, K. T., Yeater, E. A., Bernard, M. L. Vidrine, J.I., Spears, C.A., Heppner, W.L., Reitzel, L.R.,
(2011). Mindfulness is associated with fewer PTSD Marcus, M.T., Cinciripini, P.M., Waters, A.J., Li, Y.,
symptoms, depressive symptoms, physical symptoms, Nguyen, N.T.T., Cao, Y., Tindle, H.A., Fine, M.,
and alcohol problems in urban firefighters. Journal of Safranek, L.V., & Wetter, D.W. (2015). Efficacy of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 613617. mindfulness based addiction treatment (MBAT) for
Speca, M., Carlson, L. E., Goodey, E., & Angen, M. (2000). smoking cessation: A randomized clinical trial.
A randomized, wait-list controlled clinical trial: The Manuscript submitted for publication.
effect of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduc- Way, B. M., Creswell, J. D., Eisenberger, N. I., &
tion program on mood and symptoms of stress in cancer Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Dispositional mindfulness
outpatients. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 613622. and depressive symptomatology: Correlations with
Taylor, V. A., Grant, J., Daneault, V., Scavone, G., Breton, limbic and self-referential neural activity during rest.
E., Roffe-Vidal, S., Beauregard, M. (2011). Impact Emotion, 10, 1224.
of mindfulness on the neural responses to emotional Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & Whit, T. L.
pictures in experienced and beginner meditators. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppressing.
Neuroimage, 57, 15241533. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 513.
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., Ridgeway, Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought sup-
V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention pression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 5991.
of relapse/recurrence in major depression by Witkiewitz, K., & Bowen, S. (2010). Depression, craving,
mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of and substance use following a randomized trial of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615623. mindfulness-based relapse prevention. Journal of
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., & Williams, J. M. (1995). How Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 362374.
does cognitive therapy prevent depressive relapse and Witkiewitz, K., & Marlatt, G. A. (2004). Relapse preven-
why should attentional control (mindfulness) training tion for alcohol and drug problems: That was Zen, this
help? Behavior Research and Therapy, 33, 2539. is Tao. American Psychologist, 59, 224235.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2004). Witkiewitz, K., & Villarroel, N. A. (2009). Dynamic asso-
The health consequences of smoking: A report of the ciation between negative affect and alcohol lapses fol-
Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease lowing alcohol treatment. Journal of Consulting and
Control and Prevention. Clinical Psychology, 77, 633644.
Self-Compassion: What It Is, What
It Does, and How It Relates 10
to Mindfulness

Kristin D. Neff and Katie A. Dahm

This chapter will present a conceptual account of the Buddhist point of view, compassion is given
self-compassion and review research on its ben- to our own as well as to others suffering. We
efits. It will also consider how self-compassion include ourselves in the circle of compassion
relates to mindfulness, given that these constructs because to do otherwise would construct a false
are both drawn from Buddhist psychology sense of separate self (Salzberg, 1997).
(Brach, 2003; Gunaratana, 1982; Kabat-Zinn, Compassion involves sensitivity to the
1990; Salzberg, 1997). It is important to under- experience of suffering, coupled with a deep
stand the similar and unique features of self- desire to alleviate that suffering (Goetz,
compassion and mindfulness in order to Keltner, & Simon-Thomas, 2010). This means
understand how they each relate to wellbeing, that in order to experience compassion, you
and to consider how these states of heart and must first acknowledge the presence of pain.
mind might best be developed. Rather than rushing past that homeless woman
as you are walking down the busy street, for
example, you must actually stop to consider
What Is Self-Compassion? how difficult her life must be. This involves
pausing, stepping out of your usual frame of
Self-compassion has received increased research reference, and viewing the world from the van-
attention lately, with over 200 journal articles and tage point of another. The moment you see the
dissertations examining the topic since 2003, the woman as an actual human being who is in
year that the first two articles defining and mea- pain, your heart resonates with hers (compas-
suring self-compassion were published (Neff, sion literally means to suffer with). Instead
2003a, 2003b). So what is self-compassion of ignoring her, you find that you are moved by
exactly? In order to better understand what self- her situation, and feel the urge to help in some
compassion is, it is useful to first consider what it way. And rather than looking down at the
means to feel compassion more generally. From woman or believing that she is somehow sepa-
rate and disconnected from yourself, you real-
ize that all human beings suffer and are in need
of compassionthere but for fortune go I.
K.D. Neff (*) K.A. Dahm Self-compassion is simply compassion directed
Educational Psychology Department,
inward, relating to ourselves as the object of
University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
e-mail: kneff@austin.utexas.edu; care and concern when faced with the experi-
Katherine.Dahm@va.gov ence of suffering (Neff, 2003a).

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 121


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_10
122 K.D. Neff and K.A. Dahm

The Three Components fails, makes mistakes, and gets it wrong some-
of Self-Compassion times. We do not always get what we want and
are often disappointedeither in ourselves or in
Drawing on the writings of various Buddhist our life circumstances. This is part of the human
teachers (Brach, 2003; Kornfield, 1993; Salzberg, experience, a basic fact of life shared with every-
1997), Neff (2003b) has operationalized self- one else on the planet. We are not alone in our
compassion as consisting of three main elements: imperfection. Rather, our imperfections are what
kindness, common humanity, and mindfulness. make us card-carrying members of the human
These components combine and mutually inter- race. Often, however, we feel isolated and cut off
act to create a self-compassionate frame of mind. from others when considering our struggles and
Self-compassion is relevant when considering failures, irrationally feeling that it is only ME
personal inadequacies, mistakes, and failures, as who is having such a hard time of it. We think
well as when struggling with more general life that somehow we are abnormal, that something
situations that cause us mental, emotional, or has gone wrong, and we forget that falling flat on
physical pain. our face now and then actually is normal. This
sort of tunnel vision makes us feel alone and iso-
Self-Kindness lated, making our suffering even worse (Neff,
Western culture places great emphasis on being 2011). With self-compassion, however, we take
kind to others, but not so much to ourselves. the stance of a compassionate other toward
When we make a mistake or fail in some way, we ourselves, allowing us to take a broader perspec-
often use harsh, critical internal language tive on ourselves and our lives. By remembering
Youre so stupid and lazy, Im ashamed of you! the shared human experience, we feel less iso-
We would be unlikely to say such things to a lated when we are in pain. For this reason, self-
close friend, or even a stranger for that matter. compassion is quite distinct from self-pity.
When asked directly, most people report that they Self-pity is a woe is me attitude in which peo-
are kinder to others than themselves (Neff, ple become immersed in their own problems and
2003a), and it is not unusual to encounter forget that others have similar problems. Self-
extremely kind and compassionate people who compassion recognizes that we all suffer, and
continually beat themselves up. And even when therefore fosters a connected mindset that is
our problems stem from forces beyond our con- inclusive of others.
trol, such as losing our job or getting in a car acci-
dent, we often do not give ourselves the sympathy Mindfulness
we would give to a friend in the same situation. Self-compassion entails mindful awareness of our
With self-kindness, however, we are support- negative thoughts and emotions so that they are
ive and understanding toward ourselves. Our approached with balance and equanimity. When
inner dialogs are gentle and encouraging rather we are mindful, we are experientially open to the
than harsh and belittling. This means that instead reality of the present moment without judgment,
of continually punishing ourselves for not being avoidance, or repression (Bishop et al., 2004).
good enough, we kindly acknowledge that we are Why is mindfulness an essential component of
doing the best we can. Similarly, when external self-compassion? First, we must be willing to turn
life circumstances are challenging and difficult to toward and experience our painful thoughts and
bear, we soothe and nurture ourselves. We are emotions in order to embrace ourselves with com-
moved by our own distress so that warm feelings passion. While it might seem that our pain is blin-
and the desire to ameliorate our suffering emerge. dingly obvious, many people do not acknowledge
how much pain they are in, especially when that
Common Humanity pain stems from their own inner self-critic. Or
The sense of common humanity central to self- when confronted with life challenges, people
compassion involves recognizing that everyone often get so lost in problem-solving mode that
10 Self-Compassion and Mindfulness 123

they do not pause to consider how hard it is in the or short-term interventions (e.g., Shapira &
moment. Mindfulness of our negative thoughts Mongrain, 2010) as a means of examining the
and feelings means that we do not become overi- impact of self-compassion on wellbeing.
dentified (Neff, 2003b) with them, getting caught
up and swept away by our aversive reactions
(Bishop et al., 2004). Rather than confusing our Self-Compassion and Emotional
negative self-concepts with our actual selves, we Wellbeing
can recognize that our thoughts and feelings are
just thatthoughts and feelingshelping us to One of the most consistent findings in the
drop our unquestioning belief in the storyline of research literature is that greater self-compassion
our inadequate, worthless selves. is linked to less anxiety and depression. In fact, a
Neff (2003b) proposes that the three compo- recent meta-analysis (MacBeth & Gumley, 2012)
nents of self-compassion are conceptually dis- found a large effect size when examining the link
tinct, they also overlap and tend to engender one between self-compassion and psychopathology
another. For instance, the accepting stance of across 20 studies. Of course, a key feature of self-
mindfulness helps to lessen self-judgment and compassion is the lack of self-criticism, and self-
provide insight needed to recognize our common criticism is known to be an important predictor of
humanity. Similarly, self-kindness lessens the anxiety and depression (Blatt, 1995). However,
impact of negative emotional experiences, mak- self-compassion still offers protection against
ing it easier to be mindful of them. And realizing anxiety and depression when controlling for self-
that suffering and personal failures are shared criticism (Neff, 2003a). In a study by Neff,
with others lessens the degree of self-blame, Kirkpatrick, and Rude (2007), participants were
while also helping to quell the process of over- given a mock job interview in which they were
identification. Thus, self-compassion is best asked to describe their greatest weakness. Even
understood as a single experience composed of though self-compassionate people used as many
interacting parts. negative self-descriptors as those low in self-
compassion when describing their weaknesses,
they were less likely to experience anxiety as a
What Does Self-Compassion Do? result of the task. Self-compassionate individuals
also tended to use more connected and less isolat-
An ever-increasing body of research suggests ing language when writing about their weakness,
that self-compassion enables people to suffer less using fewer first person singular pronouns such
while also helping them to thrive. So far, the as I, using more first person plural pronouns
majority of studies focusing on self-compassion such as we, and making more social references
have been correlational, using the Self- to friends, family, and other humans. This sug-
Compassion Scale (SCS; Neff, 2003a) to deter- gests that self-compassion may reduce self-
mine the association between trait self-compassion evaluative anxiety because weaknesses feel less
and psychological health. This 26-item self- threatening when considered in the light of the
report measure assesses the various thoughts, shared human experience. Self-compassionate
emotions, and behaviors that map on to the dif- people have also been found to ruminate much
ferent dimensions of self-compassionself- less than those who lack self-compassion (Neff,
kindness versus self-judgment, common 2003a), presumably because they can break the
humanity versus isolation, and mindfulness ver- cycle of negativity by accepting their human
sus over-identification. Increasingly, however, imperfection with kindness. A study by Raes
researchers are also using methods like mood (2010) found that rumination mediated the asso-
inductions (e.g., Breins & Chen, 2012; Leary, ciation between self-compassion and depression
Tate, Adams, Allen, & Hancock, 2007); behav- and anxiety, suggesting that reduced rumination
ioral observations (Sbarra, Smith, & Mehl, 2012) is one of the key benefits of self-compassion.
124 K.D. Neff and K.A. Dahm

There may be physiological processes under- compassion, instead of replacing negative feelings
lying the link between self-compassion, anxiety, with positive ones, positive emotions are gener-
and depression. Gilbert and Irons (2005) suggest ated by embracing the negative ones. For this rea-
that self-compassion deactivates the threat sys- son, it is perhaps unsurprising that self-compassion
tem (associated with feelings of insecure attach- is associated with positive psychological
ment, defensiveness and autonomic arousal) and strengths. For example, self-compassion is asso-
activates the self-soothing system (associated ciated with emotional intelligence, wisdom, life
with feelings of secure attachment, safety, and satisfaction, and feelings of social connected-
the oxytocin-opiate system). In support of this nessimportant elements of a meaningful life
proposition, Rockliff, Gilbert, McEwan, (Neff, 2003a; Neff, Pisitsungkagarn, & Hseih,
Lightman, and Glover (2008) found that giving 2008). People high in trait self-compassion or
individuals a brief self-compassion exercise (this who are induced to be in a self-compassionate
involved generating a visual image of an ideally frame of mind also tend to experience more hap-
compassionate figure sending oneself uncondi- piness, optimism, curiosity, creativity, and posi-
tional love and acceptance) lowered their levels tive emotions such as enthusiasm, inspiration,
of the stress hormone cortisol. It also increased and excitement than those who are self-critical
heart-rate variability, which is associated with a (Hollis-Walker & Colosimo, 2011; Neff et al.,
greater ability to self-soothe when stressed 2007). Shapira and Mongrain (2010) conducted
(Porges, 2007). an experiment in which individuals were asked to
Self-compassion has also been shown to miti- write a self-compassionate letter to themselves
gate the effect of negative life events on emo- every day for 7 days, and found that the brief
tional functioning in general. For instance, a intervention increased happiness levels compared
series of studies by Leary et al. (2007) investi- to a control group who wrote about early memo-
gated the way that self-compassionate people ries for the same period. Moreover, this increased
deal with negative self-relevant thoughts or life happiness was maintained at 1 month, 3 months,
events. One study used experience-sampling and 6 months follow-up. By wrapping ones pain
techniques, asking participants to report about in the warm embrace of self-compassion, positive
any difficulties they were having over a 20-day feelings are generated that help balance the nega-
period. Individuals with higher levels of self- tive ones, allowing for more joyous states of mind.
compassion had more perspective on their prob-
lems and were less likely to feel isolated by them,
e.g., they were more likely to feel that their strug- Self-Compassion Versus Self-Esteem
gles were not any worse than what lots of other
people go through. The researchers also found Although self-compassion generates positive
that priming self-compassion helped participants emotions, it does not do so by judging the self as
to take responsibility for their role in past nega- good rather than bad. In this way, self-
tive events without experiencing as much nega- compassion differs markedly from self-esteem.
tive affect as those in a control condition. Self-esteem refers to the degree to which we eval-
While self-compassion helps lessen the hold of uate ourselves positively. It represents how much
negativity, it is important to remember that self- we like or value ourselves, and is often based on
compassion does not push negative emotions comparisons with others (Harter, 1999). In
away in an aversive manner. Self-compassionate American culture, having high self-esteem means
individuals are less likely to suppress unwanted standing out in a crowdbeing special and above
thoughts and emotions than those who lack self- average (Heine, Lehman, Markus, & Kitayama,
compassion (Neff, 2003a), and more likely to 1999). There is general consensus that self-esteem
acknowledge that their emotions are valid and is essential for good mental health, while the lack
important (Leary et al., 2007; Neff, Hseih, & of self-esteem undermines wellbeing by fostering
Dejitthirat, 2005; Neff et al., 2007). With self- depression, anxiety, and other pathologies (Leary,
10 Self-Compassion and Mindfulness 125

1999). There are potential problems with high were more likely to attribute the observers
self-esteem, however, not in terms of having it, feedback to their own personality when the feed-
but in terms of getting it (Crocker & Park, 2004). back was positive rather than negative. High self-
For instance, people often put others down and compassion individuals, however, were equally
inflate their own sense of self-worth as a way to likely to attribute the feedback to their personality
feel better about themselves (Tesser, 1999), and regardless of whether the feedback was positive or
may result in narcissism, prejudice, and bullying negative. An opposite pattern was found for self-
(Aberson, Healy, & Romero, 2000; Morf & esteem. Low self-esteem individuals were equally
Rhodewalt, 2001; Salmivalli, Kaukiainen, likely to attribute the feedback to their personality
Kaistaniemi, & Lagerspetz, 1999). Self-esteem when feedback was positive or negative, but high
also tends to be contingent on success in valued self-esteem participants were more likely to attri-
life domains (Crocker, Luhtanen, Cooper, & bute the feedback to their own personality when
Bouvrette, 2003), and therefore fluctuates accord- the feedback was positive rather than negative.
ing to performance outcomes (Kernis, Paradise, This suggests that self-compassion enables people
Whitaker, Wheatman, & Goldman, 2000). As the to admit and accept that there are negative as well
Hollywood saying goes, you are only as good as as positive aspects of their personality. The main-
your latest success. tenance of high self-esteem is more dependent on
In contrast, self-compassion is not based on positive self-evaluations, and therefore may lead
positive judgments or evaluationsit is a way of to cognitive distortions in order to preserve posi-
positively relating to ourselves. People feel self- tive self-views (Swann, 1997).
compassion because they are human beings, not In a survey involving a large community sam-
because they are special or above average, so that ple in the Netherlands, self-compassion was
interconnection rather than separateness is shown to be a stronger predictor of healthy func-
emphasized. This means that with self- tioning than self-esteem (Neff & Vonk, 2009).
compassion, you do not have to feel better than Self-compassion was associated with more sta-
others to feel good about yourself. It also offers bility in state feelings of self-worth over an
more emotional stability than self-esteem because 8-month period (assessed 12 different times) than
it is always there for youwhen you are on top trait self-esteem. This may be related to the fact
of the world and when you fall flat on your face. that self-compassion was also found to be less
Leary et al. (2007) found that when considering contingent on things like physical attractiveness
hypothetical scenarios involving failure or embar- or successful performances than self-esteem.
rassment (e.g., being responsible for losing an ath- Results indicated that self-compassion was asso-
letic competition for their team), participants with ciated with lower levels of social comparison,
greater self-compassion reported less negative public self-consciousness, self-rumination,
affect (e.g., sadness or humiliation) and more anger, and need for cognitive closure, than self-
emotional equanimity (e.g., remaining calm and esteem. Also, self-esteem had a robust associa-
unflustered). In contrast, global levels of trait self- tion with narcissism while self-compassion had
esteem predicted no variance in outcomes after no association with narcissism. These findings
controlling for self-compassion levels. In another suggest that in contrast to those with high self-
study, participants were asked to give a brief intro- esteem, self-compassionate people are less
duction of themselves on video (describing inter- focused on evaluating themselves, feeling supe-
ests, future plans, etc.), and were then given rior to others, worrying about whether or not oth-
positive or negative feedback about the introduc- ers are evaluating them, defending their
tion that was ostensibly made by an observer. viewpoints, or angrily reacting against those who
Participants reactions to the feedback were then disagree with them. In sum, self-compassion is a
assessed, including their attributions for the healthier way of feeling good about oneself than
observers feedback. Individuals with low self- self-esteem that is based on the need to feel better
compassion gave defensive attributionsthey than others.
126 K.D. Neff and K.A. Dahm

Self-Compassion and Motivation Self-Compassion and Coping

Many people criticize themselves in the belief Self-compassion can be seen as an effective way to
that it will help motivate them to achieve their cope with difficult emotional experiences. For
goals. While the adage spare the rod spoil the instance, Sbarra et al. (2012) found that self-
child is rarely used in modern parenting, it compassion was key in helping people adjust after
seems to persist when relating to ourselves. To divorce. Researchers asked divorcing adults to
the extent that self-criticism does work as a complete a 4-min stream-of-consciousness record-
motivator, it is because we are driven to succeed ing about their separation experience, and inde-
in order to avoid self-judgment when we fail. pendent judges rated how self-compassionate their
But if we know that failure will be met with a dialogs were. Those who displayed greater self-
barrage of self-criticism, sometimes it can be compassion when talking about their break-up not
too frightening to even try. With self-compas- only evidenced better psychological adjustment at
sion, we strive to achieve for a very different the time, but this effect persisted over 9 months.
reasonbecause we care. If we truly want to be Findings were significant even after accounting for
kind to ourselves and do not want to suffer, we a number of competing predictors such as self-
will do things to help us be happy, such as tak- esteem. Research also indicates that self-compas-
ing on challenging new projects or learning new sion helps people cope with early childhood
skills. And because self-compassion gives us the traumas. In a youth sample, Vettese, Dyer, Li, and
safety needed to acknowledge our weaknesses, Wekerle (2011) found that self-reported levels of
we are in a better position to change them for self-compassion mediated the link between child-
the better. hood maltreatment and later emotional dysregula-
Research supports this idea. In a series of tion. This suggests that people with trauma
four experimental studies, Breines and Chen histories who have compassion for themselves are
(2012) used mood inductions to engender feel- better able to deal with upsetting events in a pro-
ings of self-compassion for personal weak- ductive manner. Self-compassion also appears to
nesses, failures, and past moral transgressions. help people cope with chronic physical pain (Costa
When compared to a self-esteem induction & Pinto-Gouveia, 2011).
(e.g., think about your positive qualities) or a
positive mood distractor (e.g., think about a
hobby you enjoy), self-compassion resulted Self-Compassion and Interpersonal
in more motivation to change for the better, try Relationships
harder to learn, and avoid repeating past mis-
takes. Other research has shown self-compas- In addition to intrapersonal benefits, self-
sion to be linked to personal initiative, compassion appears to enhance interpersonal
perceived self-efficacy, and intrinsic motiva- functioning. Neff and Beretvas (2012) found that
tion (Neff et al., 2005, 2007). Self- self-compassionate individuals were described
compassionate people have less fear of failure, by their partners as being more emotionally con-
but when they do fail they are more likely to try nected, accepting, and autonomy-supporting
again (Neely, Schallert, Mohammed, Roberts, while being less detached, controlling, and ver-
& Chen, 2009). Self-compassion also promotes bally or physically aggressive than those lacking
health-related behaviors such as sticking to self-compassion. Similarly, a study of relation-
ones diet (Adams & Leary, 2007), reducing ships between college roommates (Crocker &
smoking (Kelly, Zuroff, Foa, & Gilbert, 2009), Canevello, 2008) found that self-compassionate
seeking medical treatment when needed (Terry students provided more social support and
& Leary, 2011) and exercising (Magnus, encouraged interpersonal trust with roommates
Kowalski, & McHugh, 2010). compared to those lacking in self-compassion.
10 Self-Compassion and Mindfulness 127

An interesting question concerns whether self- why others are deserving of care are therefore
compassionate people are also more compassion- likely to be poorly integrated. The link between
ate toward others. There is some evidence that self-compassion and other-focused concern was
self-compassion stimulates parts of the brain asso- strongest among meditators, which may be the
ciated with compassion in general. Using fMRI result of practices like loving-kindness medita-
technology, Longe et al. (2009) found that instruct- tion that are designed to intentionally cultivate
ing individuals to be self-compassionate was asso- compassion for both self and others (Hofmann,
ciated with neuronal activity similar to what occurs Grossman, & Hinton, 2011).
when feelings of empathy for others are evoked.
This would suggest that the tendency to respond to
suffering with caring concern is a broad process The Origins of Self-Compassion
applied to both self and others. While research
focused directly on this topic is new, findings sug- Gilbert (2009) argues that self-compassion is an
gest that the link between self-compassion and evolved capacity that emerges from behavioral
other-compassion exists but is somewhat complex. systems involving attachment and affiliation.
Neff and Pommier (2012) examined the link Seeking proximity and soothing from caregivers
between self-compassion and compassion for in order to provide a secure base for operation in
others, empathetic concern, altruism, personal the world is a mammalian behavior. For mam-
distress, and forgiveness. Participants included mals, survival depends on the tend and befriend
college undergraduates, an older community instinct (Taylor, 2002). In times of threat or
sample, and individuals practicing Buddhist stress, animals that are protective of their off-
meditation. In all three groups, self-compassionate spring and live within cooperative groups are
people were less likely to experience personal more likely to pass their genes successfully on to
distress, meaning they were more able to con- the next generation. Among humans, the sense of
front others suffering without being over- secure attachment and belonging that emerges
whelmed. In addition, self-compassion was from the caregiving system creates feelings of
significantly associated with forgiveness. safety, of being worthy of love and care, increased
Forgiving others requires understanding the vast happiness, and reduced anxiety and depression
web of causes and conditions that lead people to (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007).
act as they do. The ability to forgive and accept For this reason, individuals who are raised in
ones flawed humanity, therefore, appears to also safe, secure environments and who experience
apply to others. Self-compassion was signifi- supportive and validating relationships with care-
cantly but weakly linked to compassion for oth- givers should be more able to relate to themselves
ers, empathetic concern, and altruism among the in a caring and compassionate manner. In contrast,
community and Buddhist samples. This associa- individuals who are raised in insecure, stressful, or
tion is probably not as robust as might be expected threatening environments are likely to have an
because of the fact that most people report being insufficiently developed self-soothing system and
much kinder to others than themselves (Neff, few internalized models of compassion to draw
2003a), attenuating the association. upon (Gilbert & Procter, 2006). Research supports
Interestingly, there was no link found between the notion that self-compassion is related to the
self-compassion and other-focused concern (i.e., caregiving system and early childhood interac-
compassion, empathetic concern, and altruism) tions. People who lack self-compassion are more
among undergraduates. This may be because likely to have critical mothers, for instance, come
young adults often struggle to recognize the from families in which there was a lot of conflict,
shared aspects of their life experience, overesti- and display insecure attachment patterns, while
mating their distinctiveness from others (Lapsley, the opposite is true for those with higher levels of
FitzGerald, Rice, & Jackson, 1989). Their self-compassion (Neff & McGeehee, 2010; Wei,
schemas for why they are deserving of care and Liao, Ku, & Shaffer, 2011).
128 K.D. Neff and K.A. Dahm

Teaching Self-Compassion humanity. Home practices are assigned at the end


of each session such as writing a compassionate
While pre-existing trait levels of self-compassion letter to oneself. Participants are asked to do
have their origins, at least in part, in early child- 40 min of self-compassion practice each day,
hood experiences, skills of self-compassion can which can be a combination of formal and infor-
also be taught. Paul Gilbert has developed a mal practices.
group-based therapy intervention for clinical Neff and Germer (2012) recently conducted a
populations called Compassionate Mind Training randomized controlled study of the MSC pro-
(CMT). CMT is designed to help people develop gram that compared outcomes for a treatment
skills of self-compassion, especially when their group (N = 24; 78 % female; M age = 51.21) to
more habitual form of self-to-self relating those who were randomized to a waitlist control
involves self-attack. In a pilot study of CMT group (N = 27; 82 % female; M age = 49.11). The
involving hospital day patients with intense large majority of participants (76 %) reported
shame and self-criticism, significant decreases in having prior experience with mindfulness medi-
depression, self-attacking, shame, and feelings of tation. Compared to controls, MSC participants
inferiority were reported after participation in the demonstrated a significant increase in their self-
CMT program (Gilbert & Procter, 2006). compassion levels (43 %), with a large effect size
Moreover, almost all of the participants felt ready indicated (d = 1.67; Cohen, 1988). Participants
to be discharged from their hospital program at also significantly increased in mindfulness
the end of the study. (19 %), compassion for others (7 %) and life sat-
Chris Germer and Kristin Neff have devel- isfaction (24 %), while decreasing in depression
oped a training program designed to teach self- (24 %), anxiety (20 %), stress (10 %), and emo-
compassion skills to the general populace called tional avoidance (16 %). All significant gains in
Mindful Self-Compassion (MSC; Neff & Germer, study outcomes were maintained at 6 months and
2012). The structure of MSC is modeled on 1-year follow-up. In fact, life satisfaction actually
Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR; increased significantly from the time of program
Kabat-Zinn, 1982), with participants meeting for completion to the 1-year follow-up, suggesting
2 h once a week over the course of the 8 weeks, that the continued practice of self-compassion
and also meeting for a half-day mini retreat. continues to enhance ones quality of life over
Formal meditation practices are taught such as time (Figs. 10.1 and 10.2).
loving-kindness meditation (LKM), an ancient Results indicated that the more MSC partici-
Buddhist practice designed to increase good will pants practiced formal meditation, the more they
for oneself and others in a general fashion by increased their self-compassion levels. Similarly,
repeating a series of phrases such as May I be the degree that participants practiced informal
safe, may I be peaceful, may I be healthy, and self-compassion techniques in daily life also pre-
may I live with ease (Grossman, Niemann, dicted gains in self-compassion. This implies that
Schmidt, & Walach, 2004). A variant of the prac- self-compassion is teachable skill that is dose-
tice is also taught that focuses on generating self- dependent. The more you practice it the more
compassioncalling to mind an emotionally you learn it. The study was limited by the lack of
difficult situation in ones life and repeating an active control group, a shortcoming that will
phrases such as May I feel safe, may I feel need to be addressed in future research. Also,
peaceful, may I be kind to myself, may I accept given that most participants had prior mindful-
myself as I am. Informal practices are taught ness meditation experience, it might be that prac-
such as placing ones hands on ones heart in tices taught in the program are only effective for
times of stress, or repeating a set of memorized those who already know how to meditate. On the
self-compassion phrases for use in daily life. other hand, the fact that MSC participants
Throughout the program, interpersonal exercises increased in wellbeing even though most had
are used to help generate feelings of common prior meditation experience suggests that MSC
10 Self-Compassion and Mindfulness 129

50% Exp.
Control

40%

30%

20%

10%

0%
SC Mind Comp Life Sat

Fig. 10.1 Percent increase in self-compassion, mindfulness, compassion, and life satisfaction

25% Exp.
Control
20%

15%

10%

5%

0%
Dep Anx Stress Avoid

Fig. 10.2 Percent decrease in depression, anxiety, stress, and emotional avoidance

offers tangible benefits over and above mindful- issue of how mindfulness and self-compassion
ness meditation alone. relate to one another will be considered in some
The term Mindful is included in the name of detail.
the program because it also teaches basic mind-
fulness skills, whichas discussed aboveare
crucial to the ability to give oneself compassion. How Does Self-Compassion Relate
However, MSC mainly focuses on teaching self- to Mindfulness?
compassion skills and includes mindfulness as a
secondary emphasis (only one session in the As discussed elsewhere in this volume, mindful-
8-week course is explicitly devoted to mindful- ness entails being aware of present moment expe-
ness). Because the distinction between mindful- rience in a clear and balanced manner (Brown &
ness and self-compassion is a complex one, the Ryan, 2003). It is a metacognitive skill involving
130 K.D. Neff and K.A. Dahm

self-regulation of attention that cultivates a qual- are inherently part of mindfulness per se (Bishop
ity of relating to ones experience with a curious, et al., 2004). Feelings of self-kindness and com-
accepting stance. Acceptance involves being mon humanity may often accompany mindful-
experientially open to whatever thoughts, emo- ness of painful experiences, of course, so that
tions, and sensations arise in awareness with an self-compassion may automatically co-arise with
attitude of non-judgment and non-resistance mindfulness itself. The two do not always co-
(Bishop et al., 2004). Both mindfulness and self- arise, however. It is possible to be mindfully
compassion are notions drawn from Buddhist aware of painful thoughts and feelings without
psychology, and mindfulness is a core compo- actively soothing and comforting oneself, or
nent of self-compassion. One might ask, there- remembering that these feelings are part of the
fore, what are the areas of overlap and shared human experience. Sometimes it takes an
distinctiveness between these constructs, and do extra intentional effort to be compassionate
they have unique implications for human toward our own suffering, especially when our
functioning? painful thoughts and emotions involve self-
There are many ways in which mindfulness, judgments and feelings of inadequacy.
as defined above, is operationally similar to self- Another distinction between mindfulness and
compassion. Both involve turning toward painful self-compassion lies in their respective targets.
experiences with an accepting stance so that Whereas mindfulness is a way relating to internal
destructive processes of reactivity are lessened, experience, self-compassion is a way of relating
as evidenced by the large research literatures to the experiencer who is suffering (Germer,
linking both mindfulness and self-compassion to 2009). Mindfulness non-judgmentally accepts
wellbeing (Keng, Smoski, & Robins, 2011; the thoughts, emotions and sensations that arise
MacBeth & Gumley, 2012). There are some dis- in present moment awareness. Compassion
tinctions worth noting, however. entails the desire for sentient beings to be happy
First, the type of mindfulness that is part of and free from suffering (Salzberg, 1997). If I am
self-compassion is narrower in scope than mind- mindful of a stabbing sensation in my knee, for
fulness more generally. The mindfulness compo- instance, it means I am aware of the hot pulsating
nent of self-compassion refers to balanced sensation without judgment or resistance, allow-
awareness of negative thoughts and feelings. For ing mental space for the sensation to be as it is.
instance, a sample item from the mindfulness When self-compassion also arises in response to
subscale of the SCS (Neff, 2003a) is When that pain, feelings of care and concern for the fact
something upsets me I try to keep my emotions in that I am experiencing this pain are conveyed,
balance. Mindfulness in general refers to the along with the motivation to soothe and comfort
ability to pay attention to any experienceposi- myself to the extent possible. Self-compassion
tive, negative, or neutralwith equanimity. involves a bit of a paradox, therefore. At the same
While it is possible to be mindful of eating a rai- time that ones present moment experience is
sin, an exercise commonly used to teach mindful- mindfully accepted without resistance, the wish
ness (Kabat-Zinn, 1990), it would not make sense for the experiencer to be free of suffering in
to give oneself compassion for eating a raisin future momentsthe motivation that lies at the
(unless perhaps you had a traumatic raisin-eating heart of compassionis also present.
experience in childhood!) One slogan of the MSC program is that we
Self-compassion as a total construct is also give ourselves compassion not to feel better, but
broader in scope than mindfulness because it because we feel bad. In other words, we learn to
includes the additional elements of self-kindness fully accept our present moment experience as it
and common humanity: actively soothing and is without resistance, while still holding our pain
comforting oneself when painful experiences in the warm embrace of compassion. Mindfulness
arise, and remembering that such experiences are is necessary to insure that compassion does not
part of being human. These are not qualities that become a slick new form of resistance (I will be
10 Self-Compassion and Mindfulness 131

kind to myself to make the pain go away), while Van Dam, Sheppard, Forsyth, and Earleywine
compassion provides the emotional safety needed (2011) examined the link between self-
to fully feel and open to ones pain. Thus, mind- compassion (using the SCS), mindfulness (using
fulness and self-compassion mutually enhance the MAAS), and various wellbeing measures in a
one another. large sample of people with moderate to severe
Ultimately, the answer to the question of how anxiety and/or depression. Results indicated that
mindfulness and self-compassion are related is individual differences in trait self-compassion, as
an empirical one. It is likely that self-compassion compared to trait mindfulness, explained signifi-
taps into differently evolved physiological sys- cantly more variance in terms of anxiety, worry,
tems than mindfulness (Gilbert, 2009). depression, and quality of life. Similarly, Baer,
Mindfulness is a form of metacognition and Lykins, and Peters (2012) compared the relative
attention regulation that has been associated with predictive utility of self-compassion (using the
increased activity in the middle prefrontal regions SCS) and mindfulness (using the FFMQ) for psy-
of the brain, and is a relatively recent evolution- chological wellbeing. Wellbeing was assessed in
ary achievement (Siegel, 2007). Compassion has terms of self-acceptance, positive relations with
been linked to the older mammalian caregiving others, autonomy, environmental mastery, pur-
system, which involves oxytocin and other hor- pose in life, and personal growth (Ryff & Keyes,
mones related to feelings of secure attachment 1995). The study found that self-compassion was
(Goetz et al., 2010), as well as specific neuronal almost twice as strong a predictor of wellbeing
networks associated with love and affiliation than mindfulness, though both were significant
(Klimecki, Leiberg, Lamm, & Singer, 2013). predictors.
These systems are likely to be differentially Another study (Neff, in preparation) exam-
recruited when one is being mindful of ones ined the relative association of self-compassion
present-moment experience versus being com- (using the SCS) and mindfulness (using the
passionate toward oneself. (It is unlikely that the MAAS) with anxiety, depression, happiness and
attachment system is activated when mindfully life-satisfaction in a non-clinical community
eating a raisin, for example.) sample (N = 338), as well as a sample of individu-
While comparisons of the physiology of als practicing Buddhist meditation (N = 174).
mindfulness versus compassion are sparse, there Among the community sample, anxiety was
is a growing research literature comparing the more powerfully predicted by mindfulness
psychological correlates of mindfulness and self- (B = 0.36) than self-compassion (B = 0.19), and
compassion using self-report methodologies. depression was more powerfully predicted by
First, it is important to note that measures of self-compassion (B = 0.48) than mindfulness
mindfulness and self-compassion are signifi- (B = 0.25), though both were still significant
cantly correlated, ranging from a low of 0.28 predictors. Self-compassion was the only signifi-
(Birnie, Speca, & Carlson, 2010) using the cant predictor of happiness (B = 0.65) and life sat-
Mindful Awareness and Attention Scale (MAAS; isfaction (B = 0.49). Among meditators, anxiety
Brown & Ryan, 2003) to a high of 0.69 (Hollis- was predicted more powerfully by self-
Walker & Colosimo, 2011) using the Five Factor compassion (B = 0.35) than mindfulness
Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer, Smith, (B = 0.15), with both predictors being signifi-
Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006). This link cant. However, self-compassion was the only sig-
is not surprising given that mindfulness is a foun- nificant predictor of depression (B = 0.59),
dational element of self-compassion, and that the happiness (B = 0.63), and life satisfaction
three elements of self-kindness, common human- (B = 0.41).
ity, and mindfulness help to mutually engender Overall, research findings so far suggest that
one another. Still, it appears that for some out- self-compassion may be a stronger predictor of
comes, self-compassion is a stronger predictor of depression, happiness, life satisfaction, and psy-
wellbeing than mindfulness alone. chological wellbeing than mindfulness alone. If
132 K.D. Neff and K.A. Dahm

so, it may be because these sorts of wellbeing vating this loving mindset is likely to translate
outcomes are more strongly impacted by the into greater self-compassion. And of course,
soothing qualities of self-kindness and the emo- given that mindfulness is a foundational element
tional safety generated by feelings of common of self-compassion, it makes sense that being
humanity (Pauley & McPherson, 2010). Findings mindful of negative thoughts and emotions also
with anxiety appear to be more mixed, and may increases ones ability to be self-compassionate.
depend on characteristics of the populations Some researchers have proposed that the
being examined such as clinical symptomology increased self-compassion associated with
or meditation experience. A great deal of further MBSR and MBCT may be a key mechanism by
research will be needed to gain a more solid which these interventions improve wellbeing
understanding of when, how, and for whom the (Baer, 2010; Hlzel et al., 2011). In support of
beneficial aspects of mindfulness and self- this idea, Shapiro, Astin, Bishop, and Cordova
compassion are realized, but results so far sug- (2005) found that healthcare professionals who
gest that the benefits are not identical. This also took an MBSR program reported significantly
holds true when considering interventions aimed increased self-compassion and reduced stress
at developing skills of mindfulness and/or compared to a waitlist control group, and that
self-compassion. self-compassion mediated the reductions in stress
associated with the program. Similarly, Kuyken
et al. (2010) examined the effect of MBCT com-
Mindfulness-Based Interventions pared to maintenance antidepressants on relapse
and Self-Compassion in depression, and found that increases in mind-
fulness and self-compassion both mediated the
Mindfulness-based interventions are widespread, link between MBCT and depressive symptoms at
the most common being the 8-week Mindfulness- 15-month follow-up. They also found that MBCT
Based Stress Reduction program (MBSR; Kabat- reduced the link between cognitive reactivity and
Zinn, 1982). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive depressive relapse, and that increased self-
Therapy (MBCT) is another popular form of compassionbut not mindfulnessmediated
MBSR that has been adapted for clinical use, par- this association, again suggesting that self-
ticularly for the treatment of depressive relapse compassion may be a more powerful predictor of
(Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2012). Both of depression than mindfulness alone.
these programs use meditation techniques such Interestingly, although self-compassion may
as the body scan, breath awareness, and yoga to be a key mechanism of action in MBCT, the
teach mindfulness skills. There is also evidence updated second edition of the MBCT training
that participation in MBSR and MBCT increases manual (Segal et al., 2012) argues that MBCT
self-compassion (e.g., Birnie et al., 2010; Kuyken instructors should not explicitly discuss or teach
et al., 2010; Rimes & Wingrove, 2011; Shapiro, self-compassion in the program. Rather, they
Brown, & Biegel, 2007). Although self- suggest that participants can learn principles of
compassion is not an explicit skill taught in self-compassion just as effectively if leaders
MBSR and MBCT, leaders of these programs implicitly embody kindness and compassion in
often convey implicit messages about the impor- the quality of their presence, their guidance of
tance of being kind and gentle with oneself. Also, meditation practice, and their responses to par-
leaders of MBSR have the option of teaching ticipants questions or comments. The authors
loving-kindness meditation (LKM) on a 1-day also advise against explicitly teaching self-
meditation retreat (Santorelli & Kabat-Zinn, compassion skills to clinically depressed indi-
2004). While LKM is designed to foster general viduals because it could reinforce their feelings
feelings of friendliness and benevolence toward of being unloved and unlovable. Research will be
self and others, rather than compassion for per- needed to determine if the explicit teaching of
sonal experiences of suffering in particular, culti- self-compassion in programs such as MBCT is
10 Self-Compassion and Mindfulness 133

beneficial or not. It seems likely, however, that (Neff, 2003a) by 43 %, and that gains in
self-compassion can be raised to a much greater self-compassion were maintained at least 1 year
extent through explicit rather than implicit meth- later. In comparison, a review of the literature
ods. Also, if programs rely on teacher warmth to revealed that five MBSR studies yielded an aver-
convey the importance of self-compassion, it is age increase of 19 % (range: 1623 %) on the
unclear how long this effect will last when indi- SCS (Birnie et al., 2010; Robins, Keng, Ekblad,
viduals have finished their program and are fac- & Brantley, 2012; Shapiro et al., 2005, 2007;
ing difficult situations in their personal lives. This Shapiro, Brown, Thoresen, & Plante, 2011),
is especially true given that the larger culture while three MBCT studies yielded an average
does not support the practice of self-compassion, increase of 9 % (range: 712 %) on the SCS
and instead tends to undermine it. (Kuyken et al., 2010; Lee & Bang, 2010; Rimes
For instance, many people in the West strug- & Wingrove, 2011). Only two of these studies
gle with being compassionate to themselves examined whether gains in self-compassion were
because our culture teaches us that self- maintained over time. Robins et al. (2012) found
compassion is weak and passive, or that it will that increases in self-compassion were main-
undermine our motivation (Gilbert, McEwan, tained for 2 months after completing
Matos, & Rivis, 2011). Western culture also MBSR. Shapiro et al. (2011) examined self-
tends to confuse self-compassion with self-pity, compassion levels both 2 months and 1 year after
self-indulgence, and the abdication of personal completing MBSR, but did not find that program
responsibility. MSC directly addresses these mis- participation significantly increased self-
conceptions in the first session of the course and compassion levels in the first place.
provides a brief overview of research on self- Research that directly compares the relative
compassion supporting its mental health benefits. impact of MBSR, MBCT, and MSC will be
This allows for conceptual buy-in that facili- needed before understanding the overlapping and
tates the learning and use of self-compassion. unique benefits of each. While MSC appears to
The self-compassion exercises taught in the pro- raise self-compassion more than mindfulness-
gram are also designed to help people bring self- based interventions, it is likely that MSC raises
compassion to actual situations with which they mindfulness levels to a lesser extent than MBSR
are currently struggling. For instance, MSC or MBCT, given that teaching mindfulness is only
teaches something called the self-compassion a secondary emphasis of the program. MBSR and
break, which involves intentionally calling to MBCT are also more likely to have a stronger
mind a current life struggle, finding a soothing impact on phenomena related to mindful aware-
physical expression of compassion such as put- ness such as attenuating reactivity to emotional
ting both hands over ones heart, then silently stimuli, decreasing emotional avoidance, as well
repeating words that convey the main elements of as enhancing cognitive flexibility, attentional
self-compassion (This is a moment of suffering, functioning, and interoception (Keng et al., 2011)
suffering is part of life, may I be kind to myself in than MSC. This suggests that the MSC program is
this moment, may I give myself the compassion I complementary to MBSR or MBCT, and that it
need.) These types of concrete tools are likely to may be an effective supplement to these MBIs,
help people learn to use self-compassion in their especially for those who are self-critical.
lives with greater efficacy. An important area for future research will be
Although no studies have yet directly com- to determine whether individual difference vari-
pared MSC with MBSR or MBCT, studies exam- ables play a role in the relative impact of each
ining the outcomes of each program independently type of program on wellbeing. MBSR and MBCT
suggest that explicitly teaching self-compassion might be more effective in improving wellbeing
does make a difference. Neff and Germer (2012) for those who have low pre-existing levels of
found that the MSC program raised participants mindfulness, for instance, while MSC may be
self-compassion levels as measured by the SCS more effective for those with lower levels of self-
134 K.D. Neff and K.A. Dahm

compassion. Research might also fruitfully compassion can be learned and maintained over
explore whether wellbeing is maximized when time. This suggests that psychological interven-
both types of programs are taken, and if so, in tions aimed at enhancing wellbeing should
what order. Intuitively, it would seem optimal to include an explicit focus on developing self-
learn mindfulness before self-compassion given compassion. Future research should explore how
that mindfulness is needed for compassion. to best accomplish this goal in a way that meets a
However, for people suffering from severe shame variety of individual needs.
or self-criticism, they might need to first cultivate
self-compassion in order to have the sense of
emotional safety needed to fully turn toward their References
pain with mindfulness.
While this last section of this chapter has Aberson, C. L., Healy, M., & Romero, V. (2000). Ingroup
bias and self-esteem: A meta-analysis. Personality &
emphasized the differential benefits of mindful-
Social Psychology Review, 4, 157173.
ness and self-compassion, this should not be Adams, C. E., & Leary, M. R. (2007). Promoting self-
taken to mean that it is possible to say one is bet- compassionate attitudes toward eating among restric-
ter than the other. This would be like trying to tive and guilty eaters. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 26, 11201144.
argue whether food is better than water, an espe-
Baer, R. A. (2010). Self-compassion as a mechanism of
cially fruitless task given that water is needed for change in mindfulness- and acceptance-based treat-
food to grow (just like mindfulness is needed for ments. In R. A. Baer (Ed.), Assessing mindfulness &
self-compassion to grow). In the Buddhist tradi- acceptance processes in clients (pp. 135154).
Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
tion mindfulness and compassion are considered
Baer, R. A., Lykins, L. B., & Peters, J. R. (2012).
to be two wings of a bird (Kraus & Sears, 2009), Mindfulness and self-compassion as predictors of psy-
and both are necessary to fly. chological wellbeing in long-term meditators and
matched nonmeditators. The Journal of Positive
Psychology, 7, 230238.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., &
Conclusion Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods
to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 2745.
Research increasingly shows that treating oneself Birnie, K., Speca, M., & Carlson, L. E. (2010). Exploring
self-compassion and empathy in the context of
with care and compassion is a powerful way to
Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction (MBSR). Stress
enhance intrapersonal and interpersonal wellbe- and Health, 26, 359371.
ing. When we are mindful of our suffering and Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson,
respond with kindness, remembering that suffer- N. D., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: A pro-
posed operational definition. Clinical Psychology:
ing is part of the shared human condition, we are
Science and Practice, 11, 191206.
able to cope with lifes struggles with greater Blatt, S. J. (1995). Representational structures in psychopa-
ease. We create a loving, connected, and bal- thology. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Rochester sym-
anced state of mind and heart that helps to reduce posium on developmental psychopathology: Emotion,
cognition, and representation (Vol. 6, pp. 134).
psychopathology while simultaneously enhanc-
Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
ing joy and meaning in life. Self-compassion is a Brach, T. (2003). Radical acceptance. New York, NY:
portable source of friendship and support that is Bantam Books.
available when we need it mostwhen we fail, Breins, J. G., & Chen, S. (2012). Self-compassion increases
achievement motivation. Personality and Social
make mistakes, or struggle in life. Unlike self-
Psychology Bulletin. doi:10.1177/0146167212445599.
esteem, which tends to be a fair-weather friend, Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of
self-compassion is an available resource in good being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychologi-
times and bad. And by combining mindful accep- cal well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 822848.
tance of our present moment experience with the
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behav-
compassionate desire to be happy and free from ioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
suffering, we maximize our ability to heal and Costa, J., & Pinto-Gouveia, J. (2011). Acceptance of pain,
reach our full potential. Fortunately, skills of self- self-compassion and psychopathology: Using the
10 Self-Compassion and Mindfulness 135

chronic pain acceptance questionnaire to identify siderations and preliminary results. General Hospital
patients subgroups. Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry, 4, 3342.
Psychotherapy, 18, 292302. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the
Crocker, J., & Canevello, A. (2008). Creating and under- wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and
mining social support in communal relationships: The illness. New York, NY: Delacorte.
role of compassionate and self-image goals. Journal of Kelly, A. C., Zuroff, D. C., Foa, C. L., & Gilbert, P.
Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 555575. (2009). Who benefits from training in self-
Crocker, J., Luhtanen, R. K., Cooper, M. L., & Bouvrette, compassionate self-regulation? A study of smoking
S. (2003). Contingencies of self-worth in college stu- reduction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
dents: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality 29, 727755.
and Social Psychology, 85, 894908. Keng, S. L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects
Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of
self-esteem. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 392414. empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31,
Germer, C. K. (2009). The mindful path to self- 10411056.
compassion. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Kernis, M. H., Paradise, A. W., Whitaker, D. J., Wheatman,
Gilbert, P. (2009). The compassionate mind. London: S. R., & Goldman, B. N. (2000). Master of ones psy-
Constable. chological domain? Not likely if ones self-esteem is
Gilbert, P., & Irons, C. (2005). Focused therapies and unstable. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
compassionate mind training for shame and self- 26, 12971305.
attacking. In P. Gilbert (Ed.), Compassion: Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C., & Singer, T.
Conceptualisations, research and use in psychother- (2013). Functional neural plasticity and associated
apy (pp. 263325). London: Routledge. changes in positive affect after compassion training.
Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Matos, M., & Rivis, A. (2011). Cerebral Cortex, 23(7), 15521561.
Fears of compassion: Development of three self-report Kornfield, J. (1993). A path with heart. New York, NY:
measures. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Bantam Books.
Research and Practice, 84, 239255. Kraus, S., & Sears, S. (2009). Measuring the immeasur-
Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind ables: Development and initial validation of the Self-
training for people with high shame and self-criticism: Other Four Immeasurables (SOFI) scale based on
Overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach. Buddhist teachings on loving kindness, compassion,
Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 13, 353379. joy, and equanimity. Social Indicators Research, 92,
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). 169181.
Compassion: An evolutionary analysis and empirical Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Taylor,
review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 351374. R. S., Byford, S., et al. (2010). How does mindfulness-
Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. based cognitive therapy work? Behavior Research and
(2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health Therapy, 48, 11051112.
benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Lapsley, D. K., FitzGerald, D. P., Rice, K. G., & Jackson,
Research, 57, 3543. S. (1989). Separation-individuation and the New
Gunaratana, B. (1982). Mindfulness in plain English. Look at the imaginary audience and personal fable: A
Somerville, MA: Wisdom Publications. test of an integrative model. Journal of Adolescent
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A develop- Research, 4, 483505.
mental perspective. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Leary, M. R. (1999). Making sense of self-esteem. Current
Heine, S. H., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 3235.
S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self- Leary, M. R., Tate, E. B., Adams, C. E., Allen, A. B., &
regard? Psychological Review, 106, 766794. Hancock, J. (2007). Self-compassion and reactions to
Hofmann, S. G., Grossman, P., & Hinton, D. E. (2011). unpleasant self-relevant events: The implications of
Loving-kindness and compassion meditation: treating oneself kindly. Journal of Personality and
Potential for psychological interventions. Clinical Social Psychology, 92, 887904.
Psychology Review, 31, 11261132. Lee, W. K., & Bang, H. L. (2010). The effects of
Hollis-Walker, L., & Colosimo, K. (2011). Mindfulness, mindfulness-based group intervention on the mental
self-compassion, and happiness in non-meditators: A health of middle-aged Korean women in community.
theoretical and empirical examination. Personality Stress and Health, 26, 341348.
and Individual Differences, 50, 222227. Longe, O., Maratos, F. A., Gilbert, P., Evans, G., Volker,
Hlzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., F., Rockliff, H., et al. (2009). Having a word with
Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness yourself: Neural correlates of self-criticism and self-
meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action reassurance. NeuroImage, 49, 18491856.
from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives MacBeth, A., & Gumley, A. (2012). Exploring compas-
on Psychological Science, 6, 537559. sion: A meta-analysis of the association between self-
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behav- compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology
ioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the Review, 32, 545552.
practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical con-
136 K.D. Neff and K.A. Dahm

Magnus, C., Kowalski, K., & McHugh, T. (2010). The Raes, F. (2010). Rumination and worry as mediators of
role of self-compassion in womens self-determined the relationship between self-compassion and depres-
motives to exercise and exercise-related outcomes. sion and anxiety. Personality and Individual
Self and Identity, 9, 363382. Differences, 48, 757761.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Boosting attach- Rimes, K. A., & Wingrove, J. (2011). Pilot study of
ment security to promote mental health, prosocial val- Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for trainee
ues, and inter-group tolerance. Psychological Inquiry, clinical psychologists. Behavioural and Cognitive
18, 139156. Psychotherapy, 39, 235241.
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the para- Robins, C. J., Keng, S., Ekblad, A. G., & Brantley, J. G.
doxes of narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory pro- (2012). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction
cessing model. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 177196. on emotional experience and expression: A random-
Neely, M. E., Schallert, D. L., Mohammed, S. S., Roberts, ized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
R. M., & Chen, Y. (2009). Self-kindness when facing 68, 117131.
stress: The role of self-compassion, goal regulation, Rockliff, H., Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Lightman, S., &
and support in college students well-being. Motivation Glover, D. (2008). A pilot exploration of heart rate
and Emotion, 33, 8897. variability and salivary cortisol responses to
Neff, K. D. (2003a). Development and validation of a compassion-focused imagery. Clinical
scale to measure self-compassion. Self and Identity, 2, Neuropsychiatry, 5, 132139.
223250. Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of
Neff, K. D. (2003b). Self-compassion: An alternative con- psychological well-being revisited. Journal of
ceptualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 719727.
Self and Identity, 2, 85102. Salmivalli, C., Kaukiainen, A., Kaistaniemi, L., &
Neff, K. D. (2011). Self-compassion. New York, NY: Lagerspetz, K. M. J. (1999). Self-evaluated self-
William Morrow. esteem, peer-evaluated self-esteem, and defensive
Neff, K. D., & Beretvas, S. N. (2012). The role of self- egotism as predictors of adolescents participation in
compassion in romantic relationships. Self and bullying situations. Personality and Social Psychology
Identity. doi:10.1080/15298868.2011.639548. Bulletin, 25, 12681278.
Neff, K. D., & Germer, C. K. (2012). A pilot study and Salzberg, S. (1997). Lovingkindness: The revolutionary
randomized controlled trial of the Mindful Self- art of happiness. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Compassion Program. Journal of Clinical Psychology. Santorelli, S., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (Eds.). (2004).
doi:10.1002/jclp.21923. Mindfulness-based stress reduction professional edu-
Neff, K. D., Hseih, Y., & Dejitthirat, K. (2005). Self- cation and training: MBSR curriculum and supporting
compassion, achievement goals, and coping with aca- materials. Unpublished manuscript, University of
demic failure. Self and Identity, 4, 263287. Massachusetts Medical School.
Neff, K. D., Kirkpatrick, K., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self- Sbarra, D. A., Smith, H. L., & Mehl, M. R. (2012). When
compassion and its link to adaptive psychological leaving your Ex, love yourself: Observational ratings
functioning. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, of self-compassion predict the course of emotional
139154. recovery following marital separation. Psychological
Neff, K. D., & McGeehee, P. (2010). Self-compassion and Science, 23, 261269.
psychological resilience among adolescents and Segal, Z., Williams, J., & Teasdale, J. (2012). Mindfulness
young adults. Self and Identity, 9, 225240. based cognitive therapy for depression: A new
Neff, K. D., Pisitsungkagarn, K., & Hseih, Y. (2008). Self- approach to preventing relapse (2nd ed.). New York,
compassion and self-construal in the United States, NY: Guildford Press.
Thailand, and Taiwan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Shapira, L. B., & Mongrain, M. (2010). The benefits of
Psychology, 39, 267285. self-compassion and optimism exercises for individu-
Neff, K. D., & Pommier, E. (2012). The relationship als vulnerable to depression. The Journal of Positive
between self-compassion and other-focused concern Psychology, 5, 377389.
among college undergraduates, community adults, and Shapiro, S. L., Astin, J. A., Bishop, S. R., & Cordova, M.
practicing meditators. Self and Identity. doi:10.1080/1 (2005). Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction for
5298868.2011.649546. health care professionals: Results from a randomized
Neff, K. D., & Vonk, R. (2009). Self-compassion versus trial. International Journal of Stress Management, 12,
global self-esteem: Two different ways of relating to 164176.
oneself. Journal of Personality, 77, 2350. Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K., Thoresen, C., & Plante, T. G.
Pauley, G., & McPherson, S. (2010). The experience and (2011). The moderation of mindfulness-based stress
meaning of compassion and self-compassion for indi- reduction effects by trait mindfulness: Results from a
viduals with depression or anxiety. Psychology and randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 83, Psychology, 67, 267277.
129143. Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K. W., & Biegel, G. M. (2007).
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Teaching self-care to caregivers: Effects of
Biological Psychology, 74, 116143. mindfulness-based stress reduction on the mental
10 Self-Compassion and Mindfulness 137

health of therapists in training. Training and Education Van Dam, N. T., Sheppard, S. C., Forsyth, J. P., &
in Professional Psychology, 1, 105115. Earleywine, M. (2011). Self-compassion is a better
Siegel, D. (2007). The mindful brain. New York, NY: predictor than mindfulness of symptom severity and
Norton. quality of life in mixed anxiety and depression.
Swann, W. B. (1997). The trouble with change: Self- Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 123130.
verification and allegiance to the self. Psychological Vettese, L. C., Dyer, C. E., Li, W. L., & Wekerle, C.
Science, 8(3), 177180. (2011). Does self-compassion mitigate the association
Taylor, S. E. (2002). The tending instinct: How nurturing between childhood maltreatment and later emotional
is essential to who we are and how we live. New York, regulation difficulties? A preliminary investigation.
NY: Holt. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction,
Terry, M. L., & Leary, M. R. (2011). Self-compassion, self- 9, 480491.
regulation, and health. Self and Identity, 10, 352362. Wei, M., Liao, K., Ku, T., & Shaffer, P. A. (2011).
Tesser, A. (1999). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance Attachment, self-compassion, empathy, and subjective
model of social behavior. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), well-being among college students and community
The self in social psychology (pp. 446460). New York, adults. Journal of Personality, 79, 191221.
NY: Psychology Press.
Section 3
Clinical Perspectives
Mindfulness as a Key Construct
in Modern Psychotherapy 11
Patricia Bach, Steven C. Hayes, and Michael Levin

Throughout the history of modern psychother- and growing database of support for the benefits
apy, therapists from traditions as disparate as of acceptance and mindfulness-based methods
behavior modification (De Silva, 1984) and psy- for improving health; health behaviors; mental
choanalysis (Fromm, Suzuki, & De Martino, health problems such as anxiety, depression, and
1960) have looked to the mindfulness traditions substance abuse; learning and concentration; as
for guidance and inspiration. Some have argued well as ones overall sense of well-being (Baer,
that mindfulness processes are inherent to virtu- 2003; Greeson, 2009; Hofmann, Grossman, &
ally any approach to psychotherapy that consid- Hinton, 2011; Hofmann, Sawyer, Witt, & Oh,
ers human suffering in depth (e.g., Deikman, 2010; Ostafin & Marlatt, 2008). Mindfulness
1982). But it is only more recently, however, that methods appear to have preventative benefits as
the concepts and methods of mindfulness in clin- well. Mindfulness skills are relatively easy and
ical practice have become both central and inexpensive to teach in individual and group for-
empirical. mats (Bishop, 2002), and are now widely avail-
As this very book attests, in the last decade able through a growing number of spiritual and
contemplative practice and other mindfulness self-help books, lectures, websites, podcasts,
methods have become common therapeutic phone apps, retreats, centers, and courses. All
interventions in their own right. There are a these developments have worked in synergy to
number of obvious reasons. There is an ample facilitate the advancement of mindfulness as a
central focus of attention from clinical practitio-
ners, clinical researchers, and indeed the public
at large. With that increase in attention has come
P. Bach (*) a similar increase in attempts to define mindful-
Department of Psychology, University of Central ness concepts, to refine their understanding, to
Florida, 4111 Pictor Lane, Orlando, FL 32828, USA measure them through self-report and neurobio-
e-mail: Patricia.Bach@ucf.edu logical means, to understand the processes that
S.C. Hayes account for their impact, and to learn how to
Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Reno, modify those processes.
1664 N. Virginia Street, Reno, Neveda 89557, USA
e-mail: stevenchayes@gmail.com In this chapter, we consider some of the views
of mindfulness that seem to apply most directly
M. Levin
Department of Psychology, Utah State University, to psychotherapy and examine some of the more
2810 Old Main Hill, Logan, UT 84322-2810, USA predominant clinical approaches that attempt to
e-mail: mike.levin@usu.edu alter mindfulness processes. We briefly review

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 141


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_11
142 P. Bach et al.

the data bearing on these methods. These meth- Our purpose is to try to understand mindful-
ods and definitions are examined in more detail ness as a central issue in psychotherapy. In that
elsewhere in this volume, so our review will be context one of the least informative uses of the
brief since our purpose is to set up a more unique term is to use it to refer to techniques and sets of
focus: why is mindfulness a key construct in techniques, disconnected from a formal process
modern psychotherapy now? We examine several of change or specific outcomes. It is common to
reasons and argue that the rise of mindfulness as see people talking of mindfulness techniques
a focus is based on the modern cultural encour- as either techniques that are directly rationalized
agement of unhealthy processes that are directly using the term or those that look similar to those
antithetical to mindfulness processes. Said more methods. For example, contemplative practices
directly, mindfulness is becoming more central in in Eastern religious traditions are mindfulness
psychotherapy now, because it is more central to techniques and only things that look like them
what people need from psychotherapy now. belong in the set. The problem is that such a use is
Finally we note features of mindfulness that our based on appearances, accidents of history, or
analysis suggests are central to experiencing their vagaries of self-description. In the absence of
full benefit in the context of the current social and information about the processes they engage,
psychological needs of people. such terms quickly become scientifically mean-
ingless once hard questions are asked. If
Vipassana style contemplative practice is agreed
Clinical Definitions of Mindfulness to be a mindfulness technique on that basis, what
about yoga, progressive muscle relaxation, chant-
Mindfulness is notoriously difficult to define in ing, or just sitting in silence at the kitchen table?
a psychologically precise way. That is not sur- There seems to be no place to draw the line.
prising given its deep roots in the spiritual and A more defensible approach is to delineate the
religious aspects of lay culture. There are almost targeted processes or the outcomes of such
no examples in psychology of terms that arise methods in broad terms. The definition offered
from everyday use later achieving widely by Kabat-Zinn is the most ubiquitous one of that
agreed-upon technical precisionas terms such kind: the awareness that emerges through paying
as thought, emotion, self, or personality will attention on purpose, in the present moment, and
readily attest. Prescientific lay terms are univer- non-judgmentally to the unfolding of experience
sally fuzzy sets. moment by moment (2003, p. 145). Note that
Given that, it is a fools errand to try to reach this defines mindfulness both as an outcome (a type
agreement on what mindfulness is and it is espe- of awareness) and as a process (that arises by
cially useless to hold up progress until such an paying attention in a particular way). Marlatt and
agreement is obtained. Definitions of mindful- Kristeller took a more purely process focus by
ness need instead to be linked to the practical referring simply to bringing ones complete
task of orienting practitioners and researchers to attention to the present experience on a moment-
an area or a domain in which various technical to-moment basis (1999, p. 68). Ellen Langer
accounts can then be explored and tested. Even at took a very different approach by considering
the gross level of domain, however, the term has response flexibility and effectiveness while
conflicting uses (Shapiro, Brown, & Biegel, addressing the inverse of mindfulness: mindless
2007). The term mindfulness is treated some- in the sense that attention is not paid precisely to
times as a technique, sometimes as a more gen- those substantive elements that are relevant for
eral method or collection of techniques, the successful resolution of the situation. It has
sometimes as a psychological process that can all the external earmarks of mindful action, but
produce outcomes, and sometimes as an outcome new information actually is not being processed.
in and of itself (Hayes & Wilson, 2003, p. 161). Instead, prior scripts, written when similar infor-
11 Mindfulness and Modern Psychotherapy 143

mation really was once new, are stereotypically primary aspect of Western culture that shared
reenacted (1978, p. 636). Buddhisms aspiration to enlightenment (Suzuki,
If you look across the many definitions of that 1948). Eric Fromm said that knowledge of Zen,
kind that are available, several features stand out and a concern with it, can have a most fertile and
as part of the fuzzy set (Fletcher & Hayes, clarifying influence on the theory and technique
2005). We will note five. First, mindfulness of psychoanalysis (1960, p. 140). In a somewhat
methods attempt to teach people to purposefully similar way, popular writers about eastern topics
direct ones attention to the present in a flexible, such as Alan Watts argued that meditation prac-
fluid, and voluntary way. Many definitions speak tices and psychotherapy have a common goal,
of mindfulness this way, for example, as the releasing the individual from attachment to the
self-regulation of attention (Bishop et al., 2004), ego and its resulting sense of isolation from oth-
paying attention in a particular way (Kabat- ers (1961), and persons who would later have a
Zinn, 1994), and being actively engaged in the large influence on psychotherapy such as Daniel
present (Langer, 2000). Second, these methods Goleman (1977) wrote extensively on mediation
are designed to help people take a nonjudgmental practices. None of these developments, however,
approach of observing, describing, and partici- profoundly altered psychotherapy itself.
pating (Dimidjian & Linehan, 2003) or decen- The widespread integration of mindfulness
tering (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002) from practices into evidence-based psychotherapy is a
psychological content. The third quality is well much more recent phenomenon. The penetration
described by Bishop et al. (2004): an orientation of mindfulness has been in part empirically
toward experience that is characterized by curi- driven, but that too in not a fully adequate expla-
osity, openness, and acceptance. The fourth is a nation. Research work on transcendental medita-
quality of effectiveness (Dimidjian & Linehan, tion (TM), beginning in the 1970s, never
2003) or flexibility (Langer, 2000). And finally penetrated mainstream clinical perspectives; that
there is the fostering of a transcendent or inter- may be because TM never achieved adequate sci-
connected quality of consciousness and perspec- entific explanation (Ospina et al., 2007), but a
tive taking as is referred to by such terms as the larger reason is revealed by work that did begin to
observing self (Deikman, 1982), or big mind penetrate the behavioral health professions.
(Merzel, 2007). We will take all five of these fea- In the mid-1970s, Herbert Benson and col-
tures as an orienting set: mindfulness involves leagues (1974) demystified the focused medita-
deliberate, nonjudgmental, and accepting atten- tion of TM into a method he termed the
tion to what is present, so as to foster more con- relaxation response. Bensons simple medita-
scious, interconnected, flexible, and effective tion method was applied to the treatment of a
styles of interacting with the internal and exter- variety of behavioral health problems (e.g.,
nal world. hypertensive patients) with good effects, similar
to those achieved with other relaxation approaches
(Kerr, 2000). Benson himself was an early advo-
The Modern Clinical History cate of the integration of psychotherapy with
of Mindfulness other forms of mindfulness meditation (Kutz,
Borysenko, & Benson, 1985). Bensons method
A focus on mindfulness is thousands of years old penetrated psychotherapy because he system-
and from the beginning of psychotherapy, major atized the methods, stripped them of unnecessary
leaders in psychotherapy have discussed the rela- spiritual and religious trappings, spoke about
tionship of psychotherapy to mindfulness meth- them in a largely naturalistic fashion, and subjected
ods (Dryden & Still, 2006). For example, Carl them to empirical test.
Jung, in a preface to D. T. Suzukis Introduction In the late 1970s, Jon Kabat-Zinn (1982) and
to Zen Buddhism, described psychotherapy as the his colleagues established Mindfulness-Based
144 P. Bach et al.

Stress Reduction (MBSR) for medical patients (Linehan, 1993 ). Linehan believed that mind-
with chronic pain, and stress and lifestyle-related fulness practice could be helpful in facilitat-
health problems. As with Benson, his work ing emotion regulation in persons with
helped simplify existing meditation traditions. borderline personality disorder (Dryden &
MBSR participants were asked to attend a series Still, 2006 ), but DBT encompassed a far wider
of short classes and a retreat (Kabat-Zinn, 1990) set of methods, with extensive emphasis on
and to commit to 4560 min of daily structured skills training.
meditation or meditation and yoga (Klatt, Linehans early outcome trials (e.g., Linehan,
Beckworth, & Malarky, 2009). Kabat-Zinn also Armstrong, Suarez, & Allmon, 1991) captured
carefully systematized his methods, and removed the attention of cognitive behavior therapy
spiritual trappings. Even the name included terms researchers around the world. Interested in find-
(stress reduction) that were humble and com- ing a better approach to deal with relapse in
mon sense, and he subjected the methods to depression, John Teasdale, Zindal Segal, and
empirical test. Mark Williams initially explored DBT as a pos-
In a separate line of inquiry beginning in the sible approach but were directed to Jon Zabat-
1980s, social scientist Ellen Langer was studying Zinn and MBSR. The result was MBCT (Segal
the applications of her flexibility-based view of et al., 2002). In MBCT, the hope was that mind-
mindfulness to changing human behavior in areas fulness practice would facilitate patients ability
as diverse as education (Langer & Weinman, to decenterto step back from thoughts and feel-
1981), gerontology (Perlmuter & Langer, 1983), ings that lead to the relapse spiral (Dryden &
and decreasing prejudice and discrimination Still, 2006).
(Langer, Bashner, & Chanowitz, 1985) and other ACT (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999, 2011)
social behaviors. Langers work impacted the was first written about conceptually by Hayes in
social sciences more broadly, rather than psycho- 1984, and technologically 3 years later (Hayes,
therapy per se, but her focus on flexibility remains 1987). The use of guided meditations and con-
a key influence in mindfulness-based psychother- templative exercises (e.g., body scans; watching
apy methods. thoughts with an open focus) was extensive in the
None of these methods were forms of psycho- early protocols, but the methods were not dis-
therapy per se, but the decision to systematize cussed using mindfulness as a term until the
them, move them out of a religious context, and actual research on ACT began to appear several
test them opened the door for additional steps in years later.
integration. That did not happen immediately In the 2000s, mindfulness virtually exploded
but it did happen, and the modern wave of mind- in psychotherapy. At the beginning of the decade
fulness in psychotherapy emerged in the 1980s a search of the psychology databases showed a
and 1990s. The trend can in part be traced to the couple of dozen articles a year on the topic. As
emergence and impact of dialectical behavior integrative texts began to appear such as
therapy (DBT), mindfulness-based cognitive Mindfulness and acceptance: Expanding the cog-
therapy (MBCT), and acceptance and commit- nitive behavioral tradition (Hayes, Follette, &
ment therapy (ACT). Linehan, 2004) or Mindfulness and psychother-
DBT was first described in written form by apy (Germer, Siegel, & Fulton, 2005) that num-
Marsha Linehan in 1987. She herself studied ber was up three times. By the end of the decade,
with Buddhist teachers and has acknowledged dozen of books on these topics were appearing
how radical the incorporation of mindfulness each year and the number of articles was up more
into therapy once seemed, joking that she had than tenfold (see Fig. 11.1). Hardly a psychother-
been tempted to call her treatment Zen apy convention could be held without numerous
Behavior Therapy and decided on DBT sessions on mindfulness and acceptance, many of
because the former sounded so unscientific them overflowing with attendees.
11 Mindfulness and Modern Psychotherapy 145

Fig. 11.1 Number of publications with the word mindfulness or meditation in the abstract in the years 2001
2012 in peer-reviewed journals in psycINFO database

Health Services Administration (www.nrepp.


Why Now? samhsa.gov/), and there is a substantial body of
evidence, both outcome and process, for the
Given the long history of mindfulness as an issue importance of mindfulness methods.
in psychotherapy, why has it exploded over the These research developments in mindfulness as
last decade? Some of these reasons have already an important process in psychotherapy occurred
been covered, but we think that this question is simultaneously with empirical difficulties else-
key to understanding the relationship of mindful- where in psychotherapy research. For instance, the
ness and modern psychotherapy itself. effect sizes for psychotherapy do not appear to be
increasing (Wampold, 2001); the processes of
change and active components of mainstream evi-
Research Developments dence-based methods often suggest that the under-
lying theories require work (Hayes, 2004;
One major reason we have already alluded to is Longmore & Worrell, 2007); the syndromal
the emergence of significant research on mind- approach championed by the DSM has failed to yet
fulness processes and methods. The key decision identify diseases with distinct etiologies and meth-
by early developers such as Kabat-Zinn and ods of intervention (Kupfer, First, & Regier, 2002)
Benson to manualize their procedures and to and the attempts to deal with that fact seem to be
eliminate religious trappings, combined with an creating greater chaos (Frances, 2010); and the
interest in research, set the stage for this process. adoption of evidence-based methods by practicing
The advent of major empirically based psycho- clinicians has been somewhat limited (Sanderson,
therapies such as MBCT, DBT, and ACT on the 2002). Thus, the field is ripe for the emergence of
stage completed that process. Both DBT and new approaches such as mindfulness.
ACT are listed as evidence-based by Division
12 of the American Psychological Association
( www.Div12.org/PsychologicalTreatments/ Strategic Changes
index.html) and by the National Registry of
Evidence-based Programs and Procedures main- In part in response to the empirical problems
tained by the US Substance Abuse and Mental listed above, there is a growing emphasis on
146 P. Bach et al.

transdiagnostic models of psychopathology (e.g., mysticism, cosmology, and the occult (Levin &
Barlow, Allen, & Choate, 2004; Harvey, Watkins, Spates, 1970) and to associated mindfulness
Mansell, & Shafran, 2004). Perhaps no goad has practices. Current academics in their late 50s and
been greater in that process than rapid decline in 60s both grew up in that era and are now the
support for a syndromal approach to knowledge senior leaders of our field including popular
development. The report of the American mindfulness authors and in addition to those
Psychiatric Association planning committee for developing treatment methods, editors, leaders in
the fifth version of the Diagnostic and Statistical grant funding organizations, magazine writers,
Manual (Kupfer et al., 2002) shows that there is a television producers, and so on. Few of these per-
lack of support for a syndromal approach. The sons would today recommend much of what they
authors point out the lack of laboratory markers experimented with as youth (no one in evidence-
for specific syndromes, high comorbidities, and a based psychotherapy would today embrace tune
tendency to pathologize ordinary human experi- in, turn on, drop out as a healthy principle of
ence. They conclude that the practice of regard- living) but mindfulness methods and concepts
ing DSM-IV diagnostic categories as equivalent capture much of what seemed truly useful.
to diseases may hinder research on etiology.
As alternatives to syndromal classification have
been sought, one of the more dominant ideas is to Technology and Cultural Changes
identify pathological processes that broadly inform
treatment decisions across a wide range of difficul- Perhaps the biggest change that may explain the
ties (Harvey et al., 2004). Mindfulness-based pro- rise of mindfulness methods, however, is social
cesses are among the most important set of such need. Human cultural changes sometimes occur
processes. For instance, Baer and colleagues (Baer, slowly, but sometimes they occur very rapidly.
Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006) The current era is clearly a time of rapid change
reported that measures of mindfulness are negatively due to the technological changes that have
correlated with measures of psychological variables occurred in communications technology. These
associated with a number of psychological problems include all of the following.
including neuroticism, thought suppression, alexi-
thymia, dissociation, and psychological symptoms. The Rise of the Exposure to Words
Further, mindfulness is positively associated with and Images
variables associated with psychological health such People today are exposed to orders of magnitude
as openness to experience and self-compassion. more words and images than were their forbear-
Findings such as these suggest that mindfulness pro- ers. It has been reported that a person can easily
cesses may be broadly applicable to the treatment of view nearly half a million words upon viewing
psychological suffering. 200 web pagesthousands more than the
460,000 words in War and Peace (Bunz, 2009).
The human mind did not evolve in such a context
Cohort Effects and it has happened so fast that cultural practices
have barely had a chance to respond to whatever
There is a joke among evidence-based therapy effects it has produced. At the very least it seems
researchers that the reason mindfulness is now likely to have produced a busier, more intensely
taking center stage is that the hippies have grown verbal human mind.
up. Although this is said in jest, there is a grain of
truth to it. The late 1960s and 1970s were a time The Rise of Graphic Exposure
of cultural upheaval. The hippie movement was to Negative Content
associated with a rejection of traditional middle- If anything truly disturbing occurs anywhere in
class values, and embraced a present moment the world you can know about it almost instantly
focus. Many in the movement were drawn to via the Internet. Even 30 years ago there were
11 Mindfulness and Modern Psychotherapy 147

restrictions against placing graphic images of Why Now: The Bottom Line
death and violence in the newspaper, never mind
television. Today such images occur live on tele- We are arguing that the rise of mindfulness meth-
vision and computer screens. ods has to do with an increased interest in trans-
As communications media have fractionated diagnostic processes, the weakening of alternative
into smaller and more discrete sectors it is easier empirical approaches, cohort effects, and greater
to present material in a more and more evaluative empirical support on the one hand, and cultural
context. Xenophobia and flame wars are ram- changes on the other. These cultural changes can
pant on the Internet. While social cliques and be summarized as a simple possibility: mindful-
modern newspapers often took particular points ness methods may be more central in psychother-
of view, today it is possible to be exposed to a apy today, because the human need for
virtually constant diet of judgmental material. mindfulness is greater today. People are dealing
Since it is hard to control the target of human with less compassion and social support, and
judgment this loose verbal cannon may also be greater encouragement of a busy, entangled,
increasing judgment of oneself, and a greater judgmental, and avoidant mode of mind. These
sense of shame and self-stigma. engage processes that are known to increase
human suffering; they also engage processes that
Decrease in Physical Community mindfulness can help alter.
Changes in physical mobility, the centrality of In a recent chapter (Hayes, Villatte, Levin, &
neighborhoods, and cultural values and shriveled Hildebrandt, 2011) we argued that modern mind-
physical social institutions have occurred over fulness and acceptance methods (what we termed
the last few decades in America and around the contextual CBT) emphasize some or all of the
world (Putnam, 2000). This is true in almost three clusters of processes and methods. One
every area imaginable, from Sunday picnics to cluster we termed openness. This refers to a
the Lions club, from church attendance to the concern with acceptance, non-attachment, meta-
League of Women Voters. Extended families cognition, defusion, emotional regulation, and
have become nuclear families that have become the like. All mindfulness methods include proce-
single-parent families. Neighbors can live next to dures designed to reduce the automatic behav-
each other for years and literally never speak. At ioral impact thoughts, feelings, memories, and
the very least these changes seem likely to have bodily sensations, but without necessarily chang-
produced a decrease in social support and reduced ing what they look like, or how often and where
a sense of cooperation and shared sacrifice. they occur. A second cluster we termed aware-
ness. This refers to attentional control, attention
Increase in Virtual Community to the now, perspective-taking skills, increased
Technology has also led, however, to the rise of compassion, greater sense of pure awareness, and
virtual communities. There is now a virtual we the like. All mindfulness methods include
that can be experienced on the Internet or on procedures designed to increase awareness, con-
social networking sites. This may act as a coun- tact with present, and an extended sense of con-
terbalance to the decrease in physical commu- sciousness. The third cluster we term
nity, but it may also support more us versus engagement. This refers to the enhancement of
them thinking which can fractionate social more flexible, pro-social, effective, compassion-
groups at a larger scale. While there are clear ate, or values-based behavior, and a greater sense
benefits to new technology, too much use of the of connection with meaning and purpose. This
Internet is associated with internet addictions, aspect is perhaps most emphasized definitionally
disruptions to social networks, neglect of work or by Langers view of mindfulness, but it is present
school, and with decreased social skills and lower in several of the current mindfulness methods in
likability (Iacovelli & Valenti, 2009). psychotherapy. Indeed, issues of right action
148 P. Bach et al.

have always been emphasized in the spiritual structured series of experiences that establish
roots of mindfulness practices. mindfulness skills.
Hayes et al. (2011) argued that Like the legs MBSR consists of a group program (generally
of a stool, when a person is open, aware, and 8 weeks) that teaches contemplative practice
active, a steady foundation is created for more skills. The methods include sitting meditation,
flexible thinking, feeling, and behaving. body scans, and use of mindfulness during every-
Metaphorically, it is as if there is greater life day activities. There are also group discussions,
space in which the person can experiment and psychoeducation, yoga, and intensive out-of-
grow, and can be moved by experiences. Thus, session practice (Kabat-Zinn, 1982). Longer
all mindfulness methods seem to be designed to MBSR meditation retreats are available and are
increase the psychological flexibility of partici- included in some programs.
pants by fostering a more open, aware, and active The focus of these methods is on creating a
approach to living (p. 160). This approach sim- focused, purposeful awareness of the present
plifies and summarizes the broader orientation moment and relating to private experiences in an
to the domain summary we developed earlier: open, nonjudgmental, and accepting manner
mindfulness involves deliberate, non- (Baer et al., 2006; Kabat-Zinn, 1994). In present
judgmental and accepting attention to what is terms, there is a great emphasis on openness and
present, so as to foster more conscious, intercon- awareness. For example, present moment aware-
nected, flexible and effective styles of interacting ness and attentional control are used in the body
with the internal and external world. scans, yoga, and contemplative practice sessions,
so as to undermine processes such as rumination,
worry, and self-criticism.
Integration of Mindfulness into MBCT bears a strong resemblance to MBSR,
Contemporary Clinical Practice but the psychoeducational elements and the con-
tent and focus of exercises are more specifically
With that simplified model in mind, we will psychotherapeutic. As originally developed,
briefly review a few forms of mindfulness-based MBCT targeted in particular the negative think-
interventions that have captured substantial ing patterns that are reactivated by and that sup-
empirical and conceptual attention. In each case port entanglement with dysphoric moods, such as
we will summarize the interventions with an eye self-criticism and rumination. A key practice in
toward these processes of openness, awareness, MBCT is decentering, which is observing
and engagement. thoughts and feelings as temporary cognitive
events that are not necessarily true about the self
(Fresco et al., 2007). Decentering from difficult
MBSR, MBCT, MBRP thinking patterns was argued to produce less
automatic reactivity and behavioral compliance.
The grandfather of many mindfulness-based Throughout each element, participants are taught
therapies, including MBCT and Mindfulness- to notice difficult thoughts, feelings, and sensa-
Based Relapse Prevention (MBRP: Witkiewitz, tions in a nonjudgmental and open manner
Marlatt, & Walker, 2005), is MBSR (Kabat-Zinn,
1990). As other methods have emerged that are Evidence
fairly direct translations of contemplative tradi- MBSR, MBCT, and related methods produce
tions, such as loving-kindness meditation (e.g., medium to large within-group effect sizes on anxi-
Carson, Keefe, Lynch, Carson, & Goli, 2005), ety and depression that persist through follow-up
Lojong meditation (Pace, Negi, Adame, Cole, & (for a recent meta-analysis of 1,140 patients from
Sivilli, 2009), and Compassionate Mind Therapy 39 studies see Hofmann et al., 2010).
(Gilbert, 2009), they have tended to follow the Similar effects are seen in a wide variety of
MBSR formula in broad terms: establishment of other problem areas including pain, coping with
11 Mindfulness and Modern Psychotherapy 149

health problems, and substance-use disorders emotion regulation, and interpersonal effectiveness
(Grossman, Niemann, Schmid, & Walach, 2004; skills. Mindfulness is trained through specific
Zgierska et al., 2009). An analysis by Greeson exercises and commonly includes homework
(2009) found evidence for efficacy for improving practice. DBT includes components that explic-
well-being and quality of life and reducing mood itly target greater openness, awareness, and
and stress symptoms in persons with stress- engagement.
related illnesses including psoriasis, type 2 diabe-
tes, fibromyalgia, rheumatoid arthritis, and Evidence
chronic low-back pain. Some of the newer spe- The empirical support for DBT is considerable,
cific varieties have less support as of yet, but the particularly in BPD (Lynch, Trost, Salsman, &
evidence so far is similar (e.g., see Bowen et al., Linehan, 2007) on outcomes such as suicidality,
2009). There is some evidence that these meth- hospitalizations, and depression. It has also
ods are more helpful with more chronic problems produced positive outcomes in the areas of eating
(Ma & Teasdale, 2004; Teasdale et al., 2000) disorders, substance use, and depression in older
although it is not yet known why. These methods adults among several other areas (Lynch et al.,
increase self-reported decentering and present 2007). Skills training appears to be a particularly
moment awareness, and reduce judgmental think- important aspect of DBT (Soler, Pascual, Tiana,
ing (e.g., Carmody, Baer, Lykins, & Olendzki, Cebria, & Barrachina, 2009) and use of these
2009) which relates to outcomes (e.g., Shapiro skills, including mindfulness skills, relates to
et al., 2007; 2008, Shapiro, Oman, Thoresen, improvements in BPD symptoms (e.g., Stepp,
Plante, & Flinders, 2008). Perhaps as a result, Epler, Jahng, & Trull, 2008). DBT is also
depressed mood evokes fewer depressive known to reduce experiential avoidance, which
thoughts (Raes, Dewulf, Heeringen, & Williams, in turn predicts later outcome changes (Berking,
2009). There are theoretical oddities in the litera- Neacsiu, Comtois, & Linehan, 2009).
ture however. For example, the amount of train-
ing or at-home meditation does not seem to
explain outcomes (Carmody & Baer, 2009; Acceptance and Commitment
Vettese, Toneatto, Stea, Nguyen, & Wang, 2009). Therapy

ACT (Hayes et al., 1999) uses acceptance and


Dialectical Behavior Therapy mindfulness techniques, and commitment and
behavioral activation techniques, to produce psy-
DBT was originally developed for borderline chological flexibility. It is one of the more broadly
personality disorder (BPD), but it has gradually focused of the mindfulness methods in part
become a method for a variety of disorders because it was not developed with a specific dis-
involving emotion dysregulation. The term dia- order in mind, and in part because it has empha-
lectical refers to the opposing forces that exist in sized more flexible, values-based behavior, in
clinical cases and that need to be managed in addition to greater openness and awareness.
treatment. For example, mindfulness, accep- Psychological flexibility is the applied model
tance, and validation strategies promote accep- that underlies ACT. It refers to the ability to con-
tance, but they need to be balanced with behavior tact the present moment, externally and inter-
change strategies that promote change. Several nally, more fully and without needless defense,
modes of intervention are included: group skills and based on what the situation affords, to persist
training, individual psychotherapy, phone coach- or change in behavior in the service of chosen
ing, and group consultation for the therapist. values. Six processes are argued to account for
The heart and soul of DBT consist of training psychological flexibility: acceptance, defusion,
specific skills, especially as part of the group pro- flexible attention to the present moment, a tran-
cesses, including mindfulness, distress tolerance, scendent sense of self, values, and committed
150 P. Bach et al.

action. The first two are openness processes; the are functionally important in producing outcomes
next two are awareness processes; the last two are (e.g., Gaudiano, Herbert, & Hayes, 2010;
engagement processes. These processes are Wicksell, Olsson, & Hayes, 2010; Zettle, Rains,
taught to clients by means of experiential exer- & Hayes, 2011). Behavioral in-session measures
cises, homework, metaphors, exploration of para- of psychological flexibility processes as early as
dox, and use of the therapeutic relationship. session two have been successful in predicting
Cognitive defusion might be facilitated by exer- positive outcomes in ACT (Hesser, Westin,
cises that encourage viewing thoughts from afar, Hayes, & Andersson, 2009). In several of these
as if they are moving vehicles on a roadway cases, more traditional cognitive measures have
(Hayes & Smith, 2005). Exercises to improve also been tested for mediation, and in all cases
contact with the present are commonly used to mindfulness and flexibility processes have proven
train flexible attention to the moment. Exercises more powerful (e.g., Wicksell et al., 2010; Zettle
are used to establish more flexible perspective et al., 2011).
taking and to decrease attachment to the concep-
tualized self. Values, which in ACT are chosen
life directions that establish reinforcers in the Summary
present that are intrinsic to patterns of action, are
addressed in detail and all of the rest of therapy is Even though mindfulness-based therapies are
linked to these values. Committed action consists relatively new, the benefits of mindfulness prac-
of traditional behavioral activation and skill tice are being widely studied in both clinical and
development techniques but the goal is to increase non-clinical samples and in disciplines outside of
behavioral flexibility linked to values in the pres- behavioral health. Outcome research is support-
ence of previously repertoire-narrowing stimuli. ive of the methods we have covered here, but the
evidence is even more supportive when looking
Evidence broadly at methods that target mindfulness pro-
As this chapter is being written there are exactly cesses (Hayes, Villatte et al., 2011). Mindfulness
116 randomized controlled trials published with allows for openness to experience and supports
ACT, over half of them in the last 3 years. acceptance and change efforts (Hofmann &
Reviews show medium- to large-group effect Asmundson, 2008). Mindfulness methods help
sizes (see Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & overcome the literal evaluative functions of lan-
Lillis, 2006; st, 2008; Powers, Vrding, & guageour tendency to judge things we encoun-
Emmelkamp, 2009; & Ruiz, 2010). What is most ter and to take those judgments as literal truths
surprising is the range of phenomena addressed: about the world (Hayes & Wilson, 2003). These
depression, coping with psychosis, substance methods are highly acceptable to patients
use, chronic pain, epilepsy, obsessive-compulsive (Finucane & Mercer, 2006) and they produce
disorder, diabetes management, reduction of good outcomes not just in traditional mental
prejudice toward people with psychological health areas but also in behavioral medicine and
problems, helping drug and alcohol counselors dealing with chronic disease (e.g., Gregg,
learn and apply evidence-based pharmacother- Callaghan, Hayes, & Glenn-Lawson, 2007;
apy, worksite stress, smoking cessation, obesity, Vowles & McCracken, 2008). They can also be
adjusting to college, eating pathology, and other applied to more subclinical problems of stress
problems. The process evidence suggests that (Chiesa & Serretti, 2009; Shapiro et al., 2008),
ACT alters psychological flexibility and its com- including workplace stress (Bond & Bunce,
ponents, such as experiential avoidance, fusion, 2000; 2003; Klatt et al., 2009; cf., Langer &
and values (Hayes et al., 2006). About two-thirds Moldoveanu, 2000).
of the published randomized controlled trials have One striking feature of mindfulness-based
included mediational analyses which have sug- treatments is that clinicians are encouraged or
gested that flexibility and mindfulness processes even required to themselves adopt mindfulness
11 Mindfulness and Modern Psychotherapy 151

practices. This is unlike any other treatment relative importance of mindfulness practice in
approach except perhaps psychoanalysis. In multicomponent treatments. Biological research
MBSR clinicians must follow strict training pro- on mechanisms of mindfulness, while relatively
cedures; DBT and ACT encouraged clinicians to new, is growing in the search to find a neurobio-
adopt mindfulness practices of their own and to logical basis of the effects of mindfulness practice.
apply these practices to themselves. In addition to Ultimately, empirical outcomes should lead to
perhaps facilitating skill in delivering treatment, greater precision in defining mindfulness and
there is a growing body of research that suggests related constructs and refining techniques to
that mindfulness practice is beneficial to the well- maximize treatment effectiveness.
being and psychological flexibility of treatment
providers. Krasner and colleagues (2009) found
that participation in a mindful communication
program was beneficial to primary care physi-
References
cians in increasing mindfulness, empathy, and Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical inter-
conscientiousness while decreasing burnout and vention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical
mood disturbance. Counseling trainees reported Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125143.
improvements in stress management and greater Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., &
Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment meth-
consciousness of themselves and their clients ods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13,
after taking a course that emphasized mindfulness 2745.
training (Christopher, Christopher, Dunnagan, & Barlow, D. H., Allen, L. B., & Choate, M. L. (2004).
Schure, 2006). There is even evidence that mind- Toward a unified treatment for emotional disorders.
Behaviour Therapy, 35, 205230.
fulness methods help clinicians learn other evi- Benson, H., Beary, J. F., & Carol, M. P. (1974). The relax-
dence-based methods (Varra, Hayes, Roget, & ation response. Psychiatry, 37(1), 3746.
Fisher, 2008). Turner (2009) nicely summarizes Berking, M., Neacsiu, A. D., Comtois, K., & Linehan,
the usefulness of cultivating mindfulness for cli- M. M. (2009). The impact of experiential avoidance
on the reduction of depression in treatment for
nicians: Mindfulness skills training builds the Borderline Personality Disorder. Behaviour Research
clinicians qualities or skills of attention, affect and Therapy, 47, 663670.
regulation, attunement, and empathy (p. 97). Bishop, S. R. (2002). What do we really know about
Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction? Psychosomatic
Medicine, 64, 7183.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson
The Future of Mindfulness Carmody, N. D. J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: A pro-
and Psychotherapy posed operational definition. Clinical Psychology:
Science and Practice, 11, 230241.
Bond, F. W., & Bunce, D. (2000). Mediators of change in
The very flexibility of mindfulness-based treat- emotion-focused and problem-focused worksite stress
ment approaches virtually guarantees that new management interventions. Journal of Occupational
developments will appear between the time this Health Psychology, 5, 156163.
chapter is written and the time it appears in print. Bond, F. W., & Bunce, D. (2003). The role of acceptance
and job control in mental health, job satisfaction, and
Given the ease with which mindfulness tech- work performance. Journal of applied psychology,
niques can be delivered to clinicians and clients 88(6), 1057.
they will remain an appealing approach. And Bowen, S., Chawla, N., Collins, S. E., Witkiewitz, K.,
given the broad applicability to decreasing suf- Hsu, S., Grow, J., et al. (2009). Mindfulness-based
relapse prevention for substance use disorders: A pilot
fering and increasing desirable outcomes efficacy trial. Substance Abuse, 30, 295305.
mindfulness-based approaches are likely to con- Bunz, M. (2009). Is the link economy of UK new sites
tinue to flourish. However, many questions still managing or making abundance? The Guardian.
remain. There are attempts to determine the best Retrieved from http://www.guardian.co.uk/media/
pda/2009/nov/02/digital-media-news-sites-
dose of mindfulness to determine how much or information-overload.
how little practice is needed for beneficial effects. Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2009). How long does a mind-
Component analyses are needed to determine the fulness-based stress reduction program need to be? A
152 P. Bach et al.

review of class contact hours and effect sizes for psy- Germer, C. K., Siegel, R. D., & Fulton, P. R. (2005).
chological distress. Journal of Clinical Psychology, Mindfulness and psychotherapy. New York, NY: The
65, 627638. Guilford Press.
Carmody, J., Baer, R. A., Lykins, E. L. B., & Olendzki, N. Gilbert, P. (2009). The compassionate mind: A new
(2009). An empirical study of the mechanisms of approach to lifes challenges. London, UK:
mindfulness in a mindfulness-based stress reduction Constable-Robinson.
program. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, Goleman, D. (1977). The varieties of the meditative expe-
613626. rience. New York, NY: Irvington Publishers.
Carson, J. W., Keefe, F. J., Lynch, T. R., Carson, K. M., & Greeson, J. M. (2009). Mindfulness research update:
Goli, V. (2005). Loving-kindness meditation for 2008. Journal of Evidence-Based Complementary and
chronic low back pain: Results from a pilot trial. Alternative Medicine, 14, 1018.
Journal of Holistic Nursing, 23, 287304. Gregg, J. A., Callaghan, G. M., Hayes, S. C., & Glenn-
Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2009). Mindfulness-based Lawson, J. L. (2007). Improving diabetes self-
stress reduction for stress management in healthy peo- management through acceptance, mindfulness, and
ple: a review and meta-analysis. The journal of alter- values: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of
native and complementary medicine, 15(5), 593600. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(2), 336343.
Christopher, J. C., Christopher, S. E., Dunnagan, T., & Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmid, S., & Walach, H.
Schure, M. (2006). Teaching self-care through mind- (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health
fulness practices: The application of yoga, meditation, benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic
and qigong to counselor training. Journal of Research, 57, 3543.
Humanistic Psychology, 46(4), 494509. Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R.
De Silva, P. (1984). Buddhism and behaviour modifica- (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes across psy-
tion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22, 661678. chological disorders: A transdiagnostic approach to
Deikman, A. J. (1982). The observing self: Mysticism and research and treatment. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford
psychotherapy. Boston, MA: Beacon. University Press.
Dimidjian, S., & Linehan, M. M. (2003). Defining an Hayes, S. C. (1984). Making sense of spirituality.
agenda for future research on the clinical application Behaviorism, 12, 99110.
of mindfulness practice. Clinical Psychology: Science Hayes, S. C. (1987). A contextual approach to therapeutic
and Practice, 10, 166171. change. In N. Jacobson (Ed.), Psychotherapists in
Dryden, W., & Still, A. (2006). Historical aspects of mind- clinical practice: Cognitive and behavioral perspec-
fulness and self-acceptance in psychotherapy. Journal tives (pp. 327387). New York, NY: Guilford.
of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive Behavior Therapy, Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment ther-
24, 328. apy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of
Finucane, A., & Mercer, S. W. (2006). An exploratory behavior therapy. Behaviour Therapy, 35, 639665.
mixed method study of the acceptability and effective- Hayes, S. C., Follette, V. M., & Linehan, M. M. (Eds.).
ness of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for (2004). Mindfulness and acceptance: Expanding the
patients with active depression and anxiety in primary cognitive behavioral tradition. New York, NY:
care. BMC Psychiatry, 6, 14. Guilford Press.
Fletcher, L., & Hayes, S. C. (2005). Relational frame the- Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., &
ory, acceptance and commitment therapy, and a func- Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy:
tional analytic definition of mindfulness. Journal of Model, processes and outcomes. Behavior Research
Rational-Emotive and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy, and Therapy, 44, 125.
23, 315336. Hayes, S. C., & Smith, S. (2005). Get out of your mind
Frances, A. (2010). DSM in philosophyland: Curiouser and into your life. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.
and curiouser. Bulletin of the Association for the Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999).
Advancement of Philosophy and Psychiatry, 17(2), Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential
37. approach to behavior change. New York, NY: Guilford
Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., Van Dulman, M. H. M., Press.
Segal, Z. V., Ma, S. H., Teasdale, J. D., et al. (2007). Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2011).
Initial psychometric properties of the experience ques- Acceptance and commitment therapy: The process and
tionnaire: validation of a self-report measure of decen- practice of mindful change (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
tering. Behaviour Therapy, 38, 234246. The Guilford Press.
Fromm, E., Suzuki, D. T., & De Martino, R. (1960). Zen Hayes, S. C., Villatte, M., Levin, M., & Hildebrandt, M.
Buddhism and psychoanalysis (viiith ed., p. 180). (2011). Open, aware, and active: Contextual
Oxford, England: Harper. approaches as an emerging trend in the behavioral and
Gaudiano, B. A., Herbert, J. D., & Hayes, S. C. (2010). Is cognitive therapies. Annual Review of Clinical
it the symptom or the relation to it? Investigating Psychology, 7, 141168.
potential mediators of change in acceptance and com- Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (2003). Mindfulness:
mitment therapy for psychosis. Behaviour Therapy, Method and process. Clinical Psychology: Science
41, 543554. and Practice, 10, 161165.
11 Mindfulness and Modern Psychotherapy 153

Hesser, H., Westin, V., Hayes, S. C., & Andersson, G. Langer, E. J., & Moldoveanu, M. C. (2000). Mindfulness
(2009). Clients in-session acceptance and cognitive research and the future. Journal of Social Issues, 56,
defusion behaviors in acceptance-based treatment of 129139.
tinnitus distress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, Langer, E. J., & Weinman, C. (1981). When thinking dis-
523528. rupts intellectual performance: mindfulness on an
Hofmann, S. G., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2008). Acceptance overlearned task. Personality and Social Psychology
and mindfulness-based therapy: New wave or old hat? Bulletin, 7, 240243.
Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 116. Levin, J., & Spates, J. L. (1970). Hippie values: an analy-
Hofmann, S. G., Grossman, P., & Hinton, D. E. (2011). sis of the underground press. Youth Society, 2, 5973.
Loving-kindness and compassion meditation: Linehan, M. M. (1987). Dialectical behavior therapy for
Potential for psychological interventions. Clinical borderline personality disorder: Theory and method.
Psychology Review, 31, 11261132. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 51, 261276.
Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, Q. A., & Oh, D. (2010). Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment
The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and of borderline personality disorder. New York, NY:
depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of The Guilford Press.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 169183. Linehan, M. M., Armstrong, H. E., Suarez, A., & Allmon,
Iacovelli, A., & Valenti, S. (2009). Internet addictions D. (1991). Cognitive behavioral treatment of chroni-
effect on likeability and rapport. Computers in human cally parasuicidal borderline patients. Archives of
behavior, 25, 439443. General Psychiatry, 48, 10601064.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behav- Longmore, R. J., & Worrell, M. (2007). Do we need to
ioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the challenge thoughts in cognitive behavior therapy?
practice of mindfulness mediation: theoretical consid- Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 173187.
erations and preliminary results. General Hospital Lynch, T. R., Trost, W. T., Salsman, N., & Linehan, M. M.
Psychiatry, 4, 3347. (2007). Dialectical behavior therapy for borderline
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living. New York, personality disorder. Annual Review of Clinical
NY: Delacorte Press. Psychology, 3, 181205.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go there you are. Ma, S. H., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Mindfulness-based
New York, NY: Hyperion. cognitive therapy for depression: Replication and
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions exploration of differential relapse prevention effects.
in context: past present and future. Clinical Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72,
Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144156. 3140.
Kerr, K. (2000). Relaxation techniques: A critical review. Marlatt, G. A., & Kristeller, J. L. (1999). Mindfulness and
Critical Reviews in Physical and Rehabilitation meditation. In W. R. Miller (Ed.), Integrating
Medicine, 12, 5189. spirituality into treatment (pp. 6784). Washington,
Klatt, M. D., Beckworth, J., & Malarky, W. B. (2009). DC: American Psychological Association.
Effects of lose-dose Mindfulness-Based Stress Merzel, D. G. (2007). Big mind big heart: Finding your
Reduction (MBSR-ld) on working adults. Health way. Salt Lake City: Big Mind Publishing.
Education & Behavior, 36, 601614. Ospina, M. B., Bond, K., Karkhaneh, M., Tjosvold, L.,
Krasner, M. S., Epstein, R. M., Beckman, H., Suchman, Vandermeer, B., Liang, Y., et al. (2007). Meditation
A. L., Chapman, B., Mooney, C. J., et al. (2009). practices for health: state of the research. Evidence
Association of an educational program in mindful Reports: Technology Assessments, 155, 1263.
communication with burnout, empathy, and attitudes st, L. G. (2008). Efficacy of the third wave of behavioral
among primary care physicians. Journal of the therapies: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
American Medical Association, 302, 12841293. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 296321.
Kupfer, D. J., First, M. B., & Regier, D. A. (Eds.). (2002). Ostafin, B. D., & Marlatt, G. A. (2008). Surfing the urge:
A research agenda for DSM-V. Washington, DC: Experiential acceptance moderates the relation
American Psychiatric Association. between automatic alcohol motivation and hazardous
Kutz, I., Borysenko, J. Z., & Benson, H. (1985). drinking. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
Meditation and psychotherapy: a rationale for the inte- 27, 404418.
gration of dynamic psychotherapy, the relaxation Pace, T. W. W., Negi, L. T., Adame, D. D., Cole, S. P., &
response, and mindfulness meditation. American Sivilli, T. I. (2009). Effect of compassion meditation on
Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 18. neuroendocrine, innate immune and behavioral responses
Langer, E. J. (2000). Mindful learning. Current Directions to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34,
in Psychological Science, 9, 220223. 8798.
Langer, E. J., Bashner, R. S., & Chanowitz, B. (1985). Perlmuter, L. C., & Langer, E. J. (1983). The effects of
Decreasing prejudice by increasing discrimination. behavioral monitoring on the perception of control.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, Clinical Gerontologist, 1(2), 3743.
113120. Powers, M. B., Vrding, M. B. Z. S., & Emmelkamp,
Langer, E., Blank, A., & Chanowitz. (1978). The mind- P. M. G. (2009). Acceptance and commitment therapy:
lessness of ostensibly thoughtful action. Journal of A meta-analytic review. Psychotherapy and
Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 635642. Psychosomatics, 78, 7380.
154 P. Bach et al.

Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of
revival of american community. New York, NY: Simon Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615623.
& Schuster. Turner, K. (2009). Mindfulness: the present moment in
Raes, F., Dewulf, D., Heeringen, C. V., & Williams, J. M. clinical social work. Clinical Social Work Journal, 37,
G. (2009). Mindfulness and reduced cognitive reactiv- 95103.
ity to sad mood: Evidence from a correlational study Varra, A. A., Hayes, S. C., Roget, N., & Fisher, G. (2008).
and a non-randomized waiting list controlled study. A randomized control trial examining the effect of
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 623627. acceptance and commitment training on clinician will-
Ruiz, F. J. (2010). A review of Acceptance and ingness to use evidence-based pharmacotherapy.
Commitment Therapy (ACT) empirical evidence: Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76,
Correlational, experimental psychopathology, com- 449458.
ponent and outcome studies. International Journal of Vettese, L. C., Toneatto, T., Stea, J. N., Nguyen, L., &
Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 10, Wang, J. J. (2009). Do mindfulness meditation partici-
125162. pants do their homework? And does it make a differ-
Sanderson, W. C. (2002). Are evidenced-based psycho- ence? A review of the empirical evidence. Journal of
logical interventions practiced by Clinicians in the Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 198225.
field? Medscape Mental Health, 7, 13. Vowles, K. E., & McCracken, L. M. (2008).
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Acceptance and values-based action in chronic
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: pain: A study of effectiveness and treatment pro-
A new approach to preventing relapse. New York, NY: cess. Journal of Clinical and Consulting
Guilford Press. Psychology, 76 , 397407.
Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K., & Biegel, G. (2007). Teaching Wampold, B. E. (2001). The great psychotherapy debate:
self-care to caregivers: Effects of mindfulness-based models, methods, and findings. Mahwah, NJ:
stress reduction on the mental health of therapists in Lawrence Erlbaum.
training. Training and Education in Professional Watts, A. W. (1961). Psychotherapy east and west.
Psychology, 1, 105115. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.
Shapiro, S. L., Oman, D., Thoresen, C. E., Plante, T. G., & Wicksell, R. K., Olsson, G. L., & Hayes, S. C. (2010).
Flinders, T. (2008). Cultivating mindfulness: Effects on Psychological flexibility as a mediator of improve-
well-being. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, 840862. ment in acceptance and commitment therapy for
Soler, J., Pascual, J. C., Tiana, T., Cebria, A., & Barrachina, patients with chronic pain following whiplash.
J. (2009). Dialectical behavior therapy skills training European Journal of Pain, 14(10), 1059e1.
compared to standard group therapy in borderline per- Witkiewitz, K., Marlatt, G. A., & Walker, D. D. (2005).
sonality disorder: A 3-month randomized controlled Mindfulness-based relapse prevention for alcohol use
clinical trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, disorders: The meditative tortoise wins the race.
353358. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 19, 211228.
Stepp, S. D., Epler, A. J., Jahng, S., & Trull, T. J. (2008). Zettle, R. D., Rains, J. C., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). Do
The effect of dialectical behavior therapy skills use on acceptance and commitment therapy and cognitive
borderline personality disorder features. Journal of therapy for depression work via the same process: A
Personality Disorders, 22, 549563. reanalysis of Zettle and Rains, 1989. Behavior
Suzuki, D. T. (1948). An Introduction to Zen Buddhism. Modification, 35, 265283.
New York, NY: Grove. Zgierska, A., Rabago, D., Chawla, N., Kushner, L.,
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, Koegler, R., & Marlatt, A. (2009). Mindfulness medi-
V., Soulsby, J., & Lau, M. (2000). Prevention of tation for substance use disorders: A systematic
relapse/recurrence in major depression by review. Substance Abuse, 30, 266294.
How Do Mindfulness-Based
Interventions Work? Strategies 12
for Studying Mechanisms
of Change in Clinical Research

Tory Eisenlohr-Moul, Jessica R. Peters,


and Ruth A. Baer

Over the past 20 years, mindfulness-based This approach allows examination of whether
interventions (MBIs) have taken hold in clini- participants learn mindfulness and related skills
cal psychology. Many MBIs now boast a foun- and whether these skills are associated with or
dation of empirical support and have garnered responsible for the observed therapeutic out-
enthusiasm from psychotherapists looking for comes. Evidence for mediation is most convinc-
novel, flexible treatment tools to augment more ing when change in the proposed mediator (e.g.,
traditional cognitive, behavioral, and interper- mindfulness skills) precedes change in the depen-
sonal approaches. Randomized controlled trials dent variable (e.g., psychological symptoms;
(RCTs) of MBIs generally demonstrate positive Kraemer, Wilson, Fairburn, & Agras, 2002).
effects of treatment; however, these results do Such studies require at least three measurement
not answer the question of whether changes in points: pretreatment, posttreatment, and follow-up,
mindfulness and related constructs are respon- or pre-, mid-, and posttreatment. These studies,
sible for positive responses to treatment (Keng, while very informative, are difficult and time
Smoski, & Robins, 2011). consuming to conduct and therefore relatively
There are several general approaches to inves- uncommon.
tigating mechanisms of change in MBIs. Among In cross-sectional studies, dispositional mind-
the most rigorous are treatment outcome studies fulness (the general tendency to be mindful in
in which levels of mindfulness and other poten- daily life) and other variables of clinical interest
tial mediators are assessed at several time points. are measured and relationships between them are
examined. Such studies sometimes compare indi-
viduals with and without a history of meditation
practice, noting if the presence or duration of
T. Eisenlohr-Moul, Ph.D. practice is related to outcomes of interest and if
Department of Psychiatry, University of North level of mindfulness statistically explains the
Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
e-mail: toryanne@gmail.com effects of meditation experience. These studies
are not true tests of mediation, because all data
J.R. Peters
Department of Psychiatry, Warren Alpert Medical are collected at the same time, making it
School, Brown University, Providence, RI, USA impossible to draw conclusions about the direc-
e-mail: jrpeters@gmail.com tion of effects. However, it is often useful to
R.A. Baer (*) examine whether the cross-sectional relationship
Department of Psychology, University of KentucKY between two variables (e.g., meditation experi-
115 Kastle Hall, Lexington, KY 40506-0044, USA ence and well-being) can be statistically accounted
e-mail: rbaer@email.uky.edu

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 155


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_12
156 T. Eisenlohr-Moul et al.

for by another variable (e.g., mindfulness skills). recommendations for testing mediation and a brief
In this way, tests of mediational models in cross- review of treatment-based mediational studies.
sectional studies can inform the treatment litera- Laboratory-based studies of mindfulness induc-
ture by providing hypotheses to be tested in tions have been reviewed recently (Levin et al.,
longitudinal studies. 2012); therefore, we consider them more briefly.
Another important approach to the study of
mechanisms of change is to manipulate mind-
fulness in laboratory experiments by guiding Design of Clinical Mindfulness
participants through a brief exercise intended to Research
induce a mindful state. Effects on outcomes of
interest, such as responses to emotional stimuli In this section, we discuss major issues in planning
or performance on various tasks, are examined. and conducting studies of how mindfulness influ-
Mindfulness inductions are often compared to ences mental health, including the importance of
alternative activities such as relaxation, rumination, theory, selection of study design and participant
or distraction. Laboratory studies of mindfulness sample, and measurement of mindfulness.
inductions usually involve only a single session
with nontreatment-seeking participants; therefore,
they cannot demonstrate the efficacy of clinical Theoretical Considerations
interventions. However, they can illuminate pro-
cesses of change by isolating treatment compo- Hypotheses about the effects of mindfulness
nents in ways that are not feasible in treatment training should be rooted in theory about how
outcome studies. mindfulness relates to other constructs or behav-
iors of interest. Especially critical are theory-
based hypotheses about mediatorsi.e., the
Overview of This Chapter pathway variables through which dispositional
mindfulness, a mindfulness induction, or mind-
Most approaches to the study of processes of fulness training is hypothesized to impact the
change in MBIs require assessment of mindful- outcome of interest.
ness and other proposed mediators. Indeed, the The most basic theoretical assumption about
importance of studying mechanisms of change in the effects of MBIs is that participation in these
MBIs has been a primary impetus for the recent interventions cultivates mindfulness skills, or the
development of instruments designed to assess ability to respond mindfully to the experiences of
mindfulness and related constructs (Baer, 2011). daily life (cognitions, emotions, sensations, etc.),
Mindfulness measures allow researchers to test and that mindfulness skills lead in turn to
important assumptions about active ingredients improvements in functioning. Several studies,
in MBIs. Although laboratory studies that manip- reviewed later, support this general model. If the
ulate mindfulness with inductions often do not development of mindfulness skills leads to
include measures of mindfulness, it has been improved mental health, then an additional medi-
argued that such studies should assess whether ational question is via what mechanisms these
the induction produced a mindful state in partici- mindfulness skills exert their effects. Recently,
pants (Levin, Hildebrandt, Lillis, & Hayes, several authors have recognized the need for
2012). Valid assessment of mindfulness is there- greater theoretical organization in clinical
fore critical to the study of mechanisms of change mindfulness research on the latter question, offer-
in MBIs, regardless of research methodology. ing a variety of theories about the mechanisms
This chapter discusses design and psychometric through which increased mindfulness may relate
considerations important to such studies, with a to outcomes of interest. In a recent review, Hlzel
strong emphasis on assessment of mindfulness and colleagues (2011) argue that most theories
and related constructs. We include statistical posit mediational roles for attention regulation,
12 How Do Mindfulness Interventions Work? 157

body awareness, emotion regulation (through intervention study and should depend on the type
reappraisal or exposure, extinction, and recon- of comparison one wishes to make. In the early
solidation), or change in perspectives on the stages of treatment development, waitlist control
self. Similarly, Vago and Silbersweig (2012) groups are often used to determine whether the
advance an integrative framework for studying new treatment is more effective than no treat-
several central psychological and neurobiological ment. If it is, later studies often compare the new
processes in both mindfulness practice and treatment to an alternative treatment. If the alter-
dispositional mindfulness, especially those pro- native treatment is well established, then a find-
cesses associated with the development of Self- ing that the new treatment performs equally well
Awareness, -Regulation, and -Transcendence or better is encouraging.
(S-ART). Rooting research efforts in such theo- For clarifying mechanisms of change, com-
ries may lead to richer studies, more ability to paring full treatment packages to interventions
compare results across studies, and faster, more from which a critical element is omitted can be
coherent accumulation of information about the very informative. For example, MacCoon et al.
way or ways in which mindfulness improves (2011) developed and validated the Health
functioning. Enhancement Program (HEP), an active control
group designed for use in longitudinal studies
of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR).
Design Considerations The HEP provides training in the same skills as
MBSR but does not use mindfulness meditation,
Depending on the nature of the hypothesis to be allowing examination of whether MBSR confers
tested, one of several study designs may be most truly unique positive effects.
appropriate. The primary strength of longitudinal studies is
the ability to determine directional links between
Longitudinal Treatment-Outcome the intervention, changes in mindfulness, and
Studies changes in outcomes of interest. Weaknesses of
The most rigorous methods for investigating the the longitudinal treatment-outcome approach are
nature of within-person change over time in few, but include trade-offs in terms of the time
MBIs are longitudinal studies in which indepen- and resources required. Finally, while studies
dent variables are manipulated. Participants are including only two assessment points (generally
randomized to an MBI condition or a comparison pre- and posttreatment) are informative, the
group (an active/alternative treatment or a wait- inclusion of three or more assessment points
list control group) while dependent variables are allows for the use of multilevel growth modeling
measured at two or more time points (preferably and other advanced statistical approaches that
at least three; see later discussion of statistical may lead to richer information about mechanisms
recommendations). Several types of longitudi- of action in MBIs. Longitudinal studies allowing
nal studies are possible; we refer here to treat- for the use of such statistical approaches may
ment outcome studies examining effects of a prove particularly informative with regard to the
mindfulness-based treatment on symptoms over study of individual-difference moderators of the
time on both potential mediators and therapeutic effects of MBIs.
outcomes. Longitudinal designs allow the
researcher to determine whether participation in Laboratory Experiments
an MBI is associated with positive changes over Laboratory experiments are often used to isolate
time, and whether changes in mindfulness and key components and identify mechanisms of
related constructs mediate treatment-related MBIs. Mindfulness is generally manipulated
improvements. across two or more conditions by varying the
The selection of an appropriate control group instructions given to participants performing lab-
is an important step in designing a longitudinal oratory tasks. Other variables are held constant,
158 T. Eisenlohr-Moul et al.

and the participants response on an outcome of mate the kinds of mindful states achieved as a
interest is measured. This type of study aims to function of mindfulness-based interventions.
provide information about the mechanisms of Therefore, another important consideration spe-
mindfulness across a shorter time course. For cific to laboratory experiments is the selection of
example, Broderick (2005) conducted a labora- a mindfulness induction. Most studies have used
tory study in which undergraduate students com- guided periods of mindful breathing, usually of
pleted a dysphoric mood induction (they listened 815 min; however, little information is available
to sad music for several minutes while reading about the degree to which such inductions
depressive sentences) and then were randomly actually result in a mindful state.
assigned to 8 min of rumination, distraction, or This last point highlights one particularly
mindfulness. The mindfulness group listened to a important area for methodological improvement
recording that guided them in mindful awareness in laboratory studies: Most mindfulness induc-
of breathing, whereas the rumination group was tion studies have not included or reported manip-
guided in thinking about how they were feeling ulation checks that assess the extent to which
and the distraction group was asked to imagine participants complied with instructions during
neutral images. Compared to the rumination and the mindfulness induction or achieved a mindful
distraction conditions, the mindfulness condition state. Studies addressing this methodological
showed significantly less negative mood after the problem by validating the brief mindfulness
8-min period, suggesting that mindfulness may inductions used in the laboratory are sorely
be effective in reducing negative emotions. needed, and reporting of manipulation check out-
As with longitudinal studies, the selection of comes should be the norm as it is in many other
an appropriate control group strengthens the con- experimental paradigms. Several relevant, vali-
clusions that can be drawn from significant find- dated state-level scales exist that may be used as
ings. Laboratory studies have compared the manipulation checks to test for the presence of
effects of various mindfulness inductions to a both compliance with instructions and achieve-
range of control conditions, such as allowing the ment of a mindful state; these are discussed
mind to wander freely (Arch & Craske, 2006), more thoroughly in the assessment consideration
listening to educational excerpts and completing section below. Ideally, future research would use
a word search puzzle (Erisman & Roemer, 2010), these or other validated measures to establish that
and thinking about neutral, externally focused changes in mindfulness as a result of inductions
thoughts (Broderick, 2005; Kuehner, Huffziger, are responsible for mindfulness induction effects.
& Liebsch, 2009). While these comparisons are Changes in affect, relaxation, and other states
important and informative, additional research is potentially related to both mindfulness and the
warranted using comparison groups that control outcomes of interest should also be monitored, as
for potential nonspecific effects of mindfulness it is important to establish that any effects of
inductions, such as relaxation. These more strin- mindfulness inductions are not merely the result
gent controls would allow researchers to make of changes in affect or other states that may be
stronger conclusions that observed effects are related but not equivalent to mindfulness.
due specifically to mindfulness.
Strengths of the laboratory experiment Cross-Sectional Studies
approach include the potential to gain greater Although cross-sectional correlational studies are
in-the-moment information about the way mind- not well suited to investigation of treatment
fulness processes unfold and the relative ease and effects, they can provide a broad understanding
low cost of data collection compared to treatment- of associations between mindfulness and clinically
outcome research. The primary weakness of this relevant variables. This information can inform
approach concerns the potentially limited exter- more specific hypotheses about the pathways
nal validity of findings; it is not clear how well through which mindfulness training may improve
mindfulness inductions in the laboratory approxi- psychological functioning. Cross-sectional studies
12 How Do Mindfulness Interventions Work? 159

are more economical than laboratory experi- beneficial. When particular clinical populations
ments and treatment outcome studies, but they are of interest, well-validated instruments (e.g., the
have major weaknesses. The information gath- Center for Epidemiological StudiesDepression
ered is correlational and cross sectional; there- scale; Radloff, 1977) or structured diagnostic
fore it is impossible to draw firm conclusions interviews (e.g., the Structured Clinical Interview
about the direction of the relationship between for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders; SCID-I; First,
mindfulness and the other variables of interest. Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1996) should be
For example, in a cross-sectional study of experi- used to quantify symptom levels or verify diag-
enced meditators and a comparison group of noses. Such participant screening improves the
demographically similar nonmeditators, Baer, clarity and replicability of the findings by making
Lykins, and Peters (2012) found that the relation- the nature of the sample as explicit as possible.
ship between years of meditation experience and
psychological well-being was statistically
accounted for by indirect effects via dispositional Assessment Considerations:
mindfulness and self-compassion. These findings Measuring Mindfulness
are consistent with the hypothesis that long-term
practice of mindfulness meditation leads to Assessment of mindfulness is essential for inves-
increased mindfulness and self-compassion, tigating whether participants in MBIs learn mind-
which in turn improves well-being; however, all fulness skills and whether these skills contribute
variables were measured at the same time, and to therapeutic outcomes. To date, mindfulness
alternative models are possible. Such studies can has been assessed almost exclusively with self-
provide only preliminary support for theories report questionnaires. Concerns about the utility
about mechanisms of change. of such measures have led to an extensive pub-
lished discussion of their strengths and weak-
nesses (e.g., Brown, Ryan, Loverich, Biegel, &
Sample Considerations West, 2011; Eisenlohr-Moul, Walsh, Charnigo,
Lynam, & Baer, 2012; Grossman, 2011; Van
Once a theory and corresponding hypotheses have Dam, Hobkirk, Danoff-Burg, & Earleywine,
been outlined, a participant population suited to 2012). Some of the issues that have been raised,
testing the empirical questions at hand must be such as the potential for deliberate or uncon-
selected. Although much research on mindful- scious response biases, apply to virtually all self-
nessand, indeed, on psychological processes in report instruments. Other concerns are more
generalhas been conducted using college stu- specific to mindfulness questionnaires. For
dents, such samples may not resemble populations example, people with no mindfulness training
likely to seek out mindfulness-based treatments or may find it difficult to report accurately on their
to take up the practice of meditation. On the other own tendencies to be mindful. Additionally,
hand, mindfulness is often conceptualized as a dis- experienced meditators may interpret some items
positional quality that varies in nonmeditating and differently than nonmeditators (Van Dam,
nontreatment-seeking populations. Student sam- Earleywine, & Danoff-Burg, 2009), though such
ples can yield useful information about disposi- issues may be remedied by using appropriate
tional mindfulness and about responses to sample matching procedures (Baer, Samuel, &
mindfulness inductions in nonmeditators. Lykins, 2011). Inconsistencies between Buddhist
Sample choice is particularly important in conceptions of mindfulness and the way mindful-
treatment-outcome studies, where the sample ness is operationalized in psychological ques-
used determines the clinical populations to which tionnaires are another source of concern (e.g.,
findings can be generalized. Therefore, samples Grossman & Van Dam, 2011). A detailed review
should be selected on the basis of a theory that of these issues is beyond the scope of this chapter
dictates for whom the treatment is expected to be (see Baer, 2011, for a comprehensive discussion).
160 T. Eisenlohr-Moul et al.

Despite these concerns, the empirical literature or subscales may be appropriate, but if the study
on mindfulness questionnaires suggests that their examines mindfulness more generally, using a nar-
psychometric properties are reasonably sound row measure may produce insignificant or mis-
and that they make important contributions to the leading results due to insufficient content coverage.
understanding of mindfulness as a psychological Measures with a unidimensional structure that
construct, its relationships with other variables, yield a single total score often have stronger psy-
and the changes that occur with mindfulness chometric properties but may not adequately cap-
training. Furthermore, it seems likely that recent ture the complex construct of mindfulness
scrutiny will result in developments and improve- (Grossman & Van Dam, 2011). The use of global
ments that could promote greater reliability and scores from multifactor scales may also be prob-
validity in self-report mindfulness measurement. lematic, at times obscuring significant relation-
Many self-report measures of mindfulness ships between outcomes of interest and specific
have been developed in recent years. Most are components of mindfulness (Baer, Smith,
questionnaires that assess the general tendency to Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006; Smith
engage in mindful behavior in daily life. Choosing et al., 2003). Another important consideration is
an appropriate measure for a particular study whether a measure has been validated for use in
depends on multiple factors. First, measures the relevant sample type; for instance, when con-
should have strong psychometric properties. High ducting research on mindfulness in nonmeditators,
internal consistency, high test-retest reliability, a measure that has been validated using meditating
and a reliable factor structure in relevant samples individuals may not perform as expected. In the
are desirable, because these qualities demonstrate following paragraphs we provide a brief overview
the capacity of a scale to relate to other variables of available mindfulness measures.
of interest (Clark & Watson, 1995; Rust &
Golombok, 2009). The number of items in a par- Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory
ticular scale directly influences reliability (with Developed with participants in intensive medita-
longer scales yielding higher reliability); there- tion retreats, the 30-item Freiburg Mindfulness
fore, short-form scales should be developed and Inventory (FMI) (Buchheld, Grossman, &
used with careful attention to the potential trade- Walach, 2001) is designed to assess openness to
offs between reliability and validity (see Smith, and nonjudgmental observation of present-
Combs, & Pearson, 2012, for discussion). Second, moment stimuli in experienced meditators. It
indicators of scale validity (content, criterion, provides a total score only, although two studies
convergent, and discriminant validity) are impor- using student samples have conducted factor
tant to establish that the scale fully captures the analyses (see Leigh & Neighbors, 2009). Items
construct of interest, rather than related but dis- include I notice how my feelings express
tinct constructs (see Clark & Watson, 1995; Rust themselves through my body, and I accept
& Golombok, 2009; and Smith, Fischer, & Fister, myself as I am. In the development sample, the
2003 for more detailed discussion). FMI demonstrated high internal consistency in
The multidimensional nature of mindfulness experienced meditators (alpha = 0.93).
presents particular challenges to the validity of Additionally, participants in meditation retreats
self-report measures. Some of the mindfulness demonstrated significant increases in FMI scores
measures presented below focus narrowly on one from pre- to post-retreat.
component of mindfulness, such as present- Walach, Buchheld, Buttenmuller, Kleinknecht,
centered attention, whereas others have multiple and Schmidt (2006) developed a shorter form of
subscales, each assessing a single component of the FMI for use in nonmeditating samples. The
the multidimensional construct. Matching the con- authors reported adequate to good internal consis-
tent of the measure to the theory examined is tency; they also found differences in the expected
keyif the theory focuses on a specific compo- directions between meditators, nonmeditators, and
nent of mindfulness, one of the narrower measures clinical groups. As expected, both forms of the
12 How Do Mindfulness Interventions Work? 161

FMI were positively correlated with private self- ful behavior: observing, describing, acting with
awareness and self-knowledge and negatively cor- awareness, and accepting without judgment.
related with dissociation and psychological Items include I notice when my moods begin to
distress in meditating and general adult samples. change (observing); Im good at finding words
The authors recommend the longer form for par- to describe my feelings (describing); When I
ticipant samples with experience with mindfulness do things, my mind wanders and I am easily dis-
or Buddhist concepts and the shorter form for tracted (acting with awareness, reverse scored);
other populations (e.g., novice meditators). and I tell myself I shouldnt be feeling the way
Im feeling (accepting without judgment,
Mindful Attention Awareness Scale reverse scored). Exploratory and confirmatory
The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) analyses supported this four-factor structure. The
(Brown & Ryan, 2003) is a 15-item, single-factor authors report internal consistencies ranging
measure assessing the general tendency to engage from 0.76 to 0.91 for the four subscales.
in present-oriented attention and awareness. Convergent and discriminant validity were sup-
Items are all reverse scored and describe the state ported by positive associations with openness to
of being preoccupied or inattentive and function- experience and emotional intelligence and nega-
ing on automatic pilot. Examples include I could tive associations with experiential avoidance.
be experiencing some emotion and not be con- Since its development, several additional studies
scious of it until some time later, and It seems have provided support for its psychometric prop-
Im running on automatic without much aware- erties (e.g., Baum et al., 2010).
ness of what Im doing. The MAAS demon-
strates good internal consistencies in Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness
undergraduate and general adult samples (Brown Scale-Revised
& Ryan, 2003; alphas = 0.82 and 0.87, respec- The Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-
tively). Zen Buddhist meditators reported higher Revised (CAMS-R) (Feldman, Hayes, Kumar,
MAAS scores than matched community controls. Greeson, & Laurenceau, 2007) is a 12-item mea-
In the validation sample, convergent validity was sure of present-focus, attention, awareness, and
evidenced by positive correlations with openness acceptance of thoughts and feelings in daily
to experience, emotional intelligence, and well- experience. Items include It is easy for me to
being, and negative correlations with rumination concentrate on what I am doing and I try to
and social anxiety. A nonsignificant relationship notice my thoughts without judging them. A
to self-monitoring provided evidence for dis- total score is calculated. In validation samples,
criminant validity. In another validation sample, internal consistencies ranged from 0.74 to 0.77,
daily fluctuations in a state version of the MAAS and confirmatory factor analyses supported a
were predictive of concurrent feelings of auton- single-factor model. The CAMS-R demonstrates
omy as well as general levels of positive and positive correlations with the FMI and MAAS
negative affect in expected directions (Brown & and with well-being, adaptive emotion regula-
Ryan, 2003). Since then, many studies have tion, cognitive flexibility, problem analysis, and
shown theory-consistent relationships between plan rehearsal, and negative correlations with
MAAS scores and a wide range of other variables symptoms of distress, worry, rumination, brood-
(see Brown et al., 2011, for a brief review). ing, thought suppression, and experiential avoid-
ance (Feldman et al., 2007).
Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills
The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills Southampton Mindfulness
(KIMS) (Baer, Smith, & Allen, 2004) is based Questionnaire
largely on the Dialectical Behavior Therapy The Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire
(DBT) conceptualization of mindfulness skills. It (SMQ) (Chadwick, Hember, Mead, Lilley, &
provides separate scores for four facets of mind- Dagnan, 2008) is a 16-item instrument that
162 T. Eisenlohr-Moul et al.

assesses the tendency to respond mindfully to Observing measures the ability to observe ones
distressing images and thoughts. The items inner experiences and responses to stimuli,
describe several aspects of mindfulness (mindful describing assesses the ability to use words to
observation, non-aversion, nonjudgment, and let- describe ones thoughts and feelings, acting with
ting go); however, the authors advise the use of a awareness measures the tendency to attend to
single total score. All items begin with Usually ongoing activity and avoid automatic pilot, non-
when I have distressing thoughts or images and judging of inner experiences measures the ten-
continue with a statement representing a dency to accept ones inner state rather than
mindfulness-related response, such as I am able judging thoughts and emotions as good or bad,
to just notice them without reacting or they and nonreactivity to inner experiences assesses
take over my mind for quite a while afterward. the tendency to allow provocative stimuli to come
The measure has demonstrated good internal and go without necessarily reacting to them. The
consistency (alpha = 0.89) and a significant posi- 39 items each has their highest loadings on their
tive correlation with the MAAS (r = 0.57). Scores respective factors and low loadings on all other
were positively correlated with pleasant mood factors. In the validation samples, factors demon-
ratings, and scores increased significantly follow- strated adequate to excellent internal consistency
ing participation in MBSR (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). (alphas ranging from 0.75 to 0.91). Correlations
In addition, regular meditators scored signifi- between facet scales and other variables were
cantly higher on this scale than nonmeditators. consistent with predictions. Most facet scores
have been shown to be higher in meditators than
Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale in nonmeditators, and several mediated the cross-
The Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale (PHLMS) sectional relationship between meditation experi-
(Cardaciatto, Herbert, Forman, Moitra, & Farrow, ence and psychological well-being (Baer et al.,
2007) is a 20-item measure assessing two sepa- 2008). These findings provided preliminary sup-
rately scored elements of mindfulness. The first port for the commonly held but relatively untested
factor, awareness, assesses the monitoring and assumption that meditation cultivates a variety of
awareness of internal and external experience mindfulness skills, which in turn contribute to
(e.g., Im aware of thoughts Im having when healthy psychological functioning. Findings also
my mood changes). The second factor, accep- suggest that a multifaceted approach to the
tance, measures a nonjudgmental attitude and assessment of mindfulness is useful, as it may
openness to experience, which includes refrain- help to clarify which aspects of mindfulness are
ing from attempts to avoid or escape ones expe- most strongly related to other psychological vari-
rience (e.g., I try to distract myself when I feel ables and to the effects of mindfulness training.
unpleasant emotions, reverse scored). Good
internal consistency (alphas ranging from .75 to Mindfulness Process Questionnaire
.91) has been established in clinical and nonclini- The 7-item Mindfulness Process Questionnaire
cal samples, and most correlations with other (MPQ) (Erisman & Roemer, 2012) is designed to
constructs were significant in expected direc- capture the extent to which mindfulness is inten-
tions. In addition, clinical samples generally pro- tionally practiced or attempted, and the tendency
duced lower scores than nonclinical samples. to bring compassionate awareness to ones experi-
ence upon noticing that the mind has wandered or
Five-Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire that ones awareness is judgmental. Respondents
In 2006, Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, and are asked to indicate how much each item is char-
Toney conducted exploratory factor analyses of acteristic of them on a scale from 1 (not at all
combined item pools from five of the mindful- characteristic of me) to 5 (entirely characteristic
ness questionnaires just summarized (all but the of me). Items include I try to be open to whatever
PHLMS, which was not yet available). These happens, as its happening, instead of having my
analyses supported a five-factor solution: mind wander to other things, and If I notice that
12 How Do Mindfulness Interventions Work? 163

Im being critical of my thoughts or feelings, I try changing the wording of items so that individuals
to be more accepting of them instead. Internal report on how they generally relate to their
consistency is acceptable (alpha = 0.71). In the thoughts and feelings rather than on how they did
first validation study, scores on the MPQ were so during a particular exercise. This version also
found to be uniquely predictive of greater concur- demonstrated good internal consistency
rent well-being and less psychopathology after (alphas = .85, .91) and positive correlations with
controlling for either the MAAS or the Five-Facet other measures of dispositional mindfulness;
Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ). In the sec- notably, such correlations were higher for the
ond validation study, changes in the MPQ as a decentering factor than for the curiosity factor.
function of participation in an MBI were associ- Scores on the decentering factor were signifi-
ated with improvements in psychological symp- cantly higher in meditators than in
toms and well-being. nonmeditators.

Toronto Mindfulness Scale Comprehensive Inventory


Most of the instruments discussed in this chapter of Mindfulness Experiences Beta
measure mindfulness as a trait-like tendency in The Comprehensive Inventory of Mindfulness
daily life. However, mindfulness can also be con- Experiences Beta (CHIME-) (Bergomi,
ceptualized as a state-like quality that occurs Tschacher, & Kupper, 2013) has 32 items
when attention is intentionally directed to present designed to capture a range of constructs thought
sensations, thoughts, and emotions, with an atti- to be central to mindfulness. In two samples of
tude of openness, curiosity, and acceptance German adults, a four-factor structure emerged:
(Bishop et al., 2004). The TMS (Lau et al., 2006) (1) accepting, nonreactive, and insightful orienta-
measures mindfulness in this way, assessing the tion; (2) present-awareness; (3) describing of
degree to which the respondent attains a mindful experiences; and (4) open, nonavoidant orienta-
state during an immediately preceding meditation tion. Convergent and discriminant validity were
session. The TMS is designed to be completed excellent, and the factor structure remained
following an exercise; in the development article, invariant across samples of novices and medita-
participants were asked to sit quietly for 15 min tors. However, factors were more highly corre-
and pay attention to your breathing and anything lated with one another in the meditation-trained
else that might arise in your experience (Lau individuals. In both samples, internal consistency
et al., 2006; p. 1450). After the exercise, partici- was acceptable (alphas .85, except for factor 4:
pants rate the extent to which they were aware and alpha = .65). Additional work on this scale is
accepting of their experiences during the exercise. needed to further establish its factor structure,
The TMS comprises two factors: curiosity and reliability, and validity, particularly in samples of
decentering. The curiosity factor assesses interest English-speaking adults.
in and curiosity about ones inner experience,
including items such as I was curious about my
reactions to things. The decentering factor Assessment Considerations:
assesses awareness of experiences without identi- Measuring Constructs Closely Related
fying with them or being carried away with them to Mindfulness
and includes items such as I was more concerned
with being open to my experiences than control- Several recently developed instruments are
ling or changing them. Both factors have demon- designed to measure constructs that are (1) highly
strated good internal consistency (alpha = .86-.87) related to mindfulness, (2) often presented to
and significant correlations with measures of self- patients alongside mindfulness training in the
awareness have been reported. context of MBIs, and (3) often considered as
A trait version of the TMS has recently been mechanisms through which mindfulness may
developed (Davis, Lau, & Cairns, 2009) by effect change. They are reviewed below.
164 T. Eisenlohr-Moul et al.

Acceptance and Action Questionnaire Self-Compassion Scale


The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire The Self-Compassion Scale (SCS; Neff, 2003) is
(AAQ) emerged from the literature on ACT a 26-item instrument measuring six aspects of
(Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999) and is avail- self-compassion: self-kindness (the tendency to
able in several versions. The most widely used extend kindness and understanding toward one-
version has nine items (Hayes et al., 2004) and self when feeling emotional pain or stress); self-
assesses elements of experiential avoidance, judgment (the tendency to be self-critical,
including negative evaluation of and tendency disapproving, and intolerant toward ones own
to avoid aversive internal stimuli (thoughts, flaws and difficult experiences); common human-
feelings, etc.) and inability to take constructive ity (the recognition that feelings of inadequacy,
action while experiencing these stimuli. When emotional pain, and failure are universal human
reverse scored, it serves as a measure of experi- experiences); isolation (feelings of aloneness,
ential acceptance. It has adequate internal con- separation, and disconnection from others at
sistency (alpha = 0.70). Experiential avoidance times of failure or distress); mindfulness (holding
is positively correlated with psychopathology negative thoughts and emotions in balanced
and has been shown to mediate the relationship awareness, with an open and accepting stance
between participation in ACT and improved toward difficult feelings and situations); and
well-being (Dalrymple & Herbert, 2007; overidentification (the tendency to become exces-
Forman, Herbert, Moitra, Yeomans, & Geller, sively immersed or consumed by negative feel-
2007). A 16-item version (Bond & Bunce, ings). Evidence supports this factor structure, and
2000) includes two subscales: willingness and the SCS has shown adequate to good internal
action. The willingness subscale measures consistency (alphas ranging from .77 to .81). The
openness to experiencing negative thoughts and SCS is positively correlated with social connect-
feelings, including items such as I try to sup- edness, life satisfaction, and emotional intelli-
press thoughts and feelings that I dont like by gence, and negatively correlated with
just not thinking about them (reverse scored). self-criticism, neurotic perfectionism, anxiety,
The action subscale reflects ability to behave and depression. Experienced meditators have
consistently with ones values even when expe- been shown to score higher than nonmeditators
riencing unpleasant thoughts and feelings and (Neff, 2003). In addition, most SCS subscales
includes items such as Despite doubts, I feel have demonstrated significant positive
as though I can set a course in my life and then correlations with components of mindfulness, as
stick to it. The most recent version (AAQ-II; assessed by the FFMQ (Baer et al., 2012).
Bond et al., 2011) has seven items and is con-
ceptualized as a measure of psychological flex- Experiences Questionnaire
ibility, the central construct in recent The Experiences Questionnaire (EQ; Fresco
descriptions of ACT. Psychological flexibility et al., 2007) measures decentering, or the ability
includes awareness and acceptance of the pres- to observe thoughts and feelings as temporary
ent moment, including thoughts and feelings, mental events rather than reflections of the self or
while either changing behavior or persisting in reality (I can observe unpleasant feelings with-
ongoing behavior, depending on what is out being drawn into them). The EQ has shown
required to pursue important goals and values strong internal consistency (alpha = .85) and sig-
(Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). nificant correlations in the expected directions
Preliminary findings suggest that the AAQ-II with variables such as rumination, emotion, and
has good internal consistency (alphas ranging depression. Depressed persons who showed
from .81 to .87), a single-factor structure, and higher decentering scores after treatment were
significant correlations with measures of men- less likely to experience a relapse during an
tal health. 18-month follow-up period, suggesting that
12 How Do Mindfulness Interventions Work? 165

decentering skills contribute to sustained recov- ANCOVA) when analyzing data in these types of
ery from depression. studies is the ability to use a set of rapidly devel-
oping techniques explicitly designed for testing
Measure of Awareness and Coping mediation. A traditional but somewhat outmoded
in Autobiographical Memory method of testing mediation involves observing
The MACAM (Moore, Hayhurst, & Teasdale, the change in the magnitude of the effect of the
1996) is a vignette-based semi-structured inter- independent variable on the dependent variable
view. Respondents are asked to imagine them- after the addition of the mediator to the model; in
selves in several mildly distressing situations and this paradigm, if the effect is significantly reduced
to describe their feelings and responses in similar when the mediator is added to the model (usually
situations they have experienced. Trained coders in a subsequent regression step), this is taken as
rate the responses for decentering, or awareness evidence for mediation (Baron & Kenny, 1986;
of thoughts and feelings as separate from the self. MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, &
Research shows that the MACAM can be scored Sheets, 2002). More modern statistical tech-
reliably and that scores are related to recovery niques instead examine the indirect effect, or the
from depression and likelihood of experiencing a effect of the independent variable on the depen-
future episode. Scores also have been shown to dent variable that occurs via the mediator. This is
change with participation in a mindfulness pro- equal to the product of the effect of the indepen-
gram (Hargus, Crane, Barnhofer, & Williams, dent variable on the mediator and the effect of the
2010). Although the MACAM has good psycho- mediator on the dependent variable. The signifi-
metric properties, it is difficult and time consum- cance of this effect can be estimated with the
ing to use. Sobel test (Preacher & Hayes, 2004; Sobel,
1982); however, due to the nonnormality of the
standard errors of the Sobel test statistic, this may
Statistical Recommendations result in underestimating findings.
for Testing Mediation The current state-of-the-science technique for
mediation is called bootstrapping, a technique
In the literature on MBIs, some of the most infor- that involves resampling from ones dataset many
mative studies use statistical tools to test for the times (e.g., 2,000) to create a large number of
presence of significant indirect effects of MBIs datasets. The indirect effect is estimated in each
on symptoms over time via one or more potential resampled dataset, creating a 95 % confidence
mediators. For example, the association between interval for the estimated effect (Preacher &
participation in an MBI and reductions in psy- Hayes, 2008). Online utilities and a variety of
chopathology over time may occur via increases macros for statistical programs are available for
in mindfulness, self-compassion, or other vari- conducting these analyses (e.g., Hayes, 2012;
ables that change over time as a function of mind- Preacher & Hayes, 2008; for macros and guides,
fulness training. Current discussions of mediation see http://afhayes.com/spss-sas-and-mplus-
emphasize the importance of temporal prece- macros-and-code.html). Bootstrapping and simi-
dence of change in the mediator; that is, evidence lar procedures for assessing the significance of
for mediation is most convincing when change in the indirect effect (Bauer, Preacher, & Gil, 2006;
the proposed mediator precedes change in the Preacher & Selig, 2012; Tofighi & MacKinnon,
dependent variable (e.g., Kraemer et al., 2002). 2011) can also be used to test mediation in the
One major advantage to using regression- context of structural equation modeling (SEM),
based approaches (e.g., multiple linear regres- which involves the creation and use of
sion, multilevel growth modeling, and structural measurement-error-free latent variables (see
equation modeling) over ANOVA-based Kline, 2010) and multilevel modeling (MLM),
approaches (e.g., repeated measures ANOVA or which is generally the most powerful and flexible
166 T. Eisenlohr-Moul et al.

statistical technique for the analysis of longitudi- versus antidepressant medication or placebo,
nal treatment-outcome data (see Tasca & Gallop, Bieling and colleagues (2012) found that decenter-
2009 and Tasca, Illing, Joyce, & Ogrodniczuk, ing skills, as measured by the EQ, mediated the
2009). Additional techniques that allow for both effects of MBCT on depressive relapse.
mediation of moderated relationships and mod- Other studies have identified mediators of the
eration of mediational models, as well as media- effects of MBSR and other meditation programs
tion in the context of multilevel SEM models, are on positive outcomes. In several RCTs compar-
also available (see Marsh et al., 2009; Edwards & ing MBSR to a waitlist control group, regression-
Lambert, 2007; Muller, Judd, & Yzerbyt, 2005; based methods have demonstrated that changes
Preacher, Zhang, & Zyphur, 2011). in self-reported mindfulness (measured using the
MAAS, KIMS, or FFMQ) account for treatment-
related improvements in perceived stress and
Review of Studies Demonstrating rumination (Shapiro, Oman, Thoresen, Plante, &
Statistical Mediation of Change Flinders, 2008), perceived stress and quality of
in MBIs life (Nyklek & Kuijpers, 2008), and perceived
stress, posttraumatic avoidance symptoms, and
Though there is a large body of work demonstrat- positive mental states in cancer patients
ing beneficial treatment effects for MBIs, fewer (Brnstrm, Kvillemo, Brandberg, & Moskowitz,
studies have provided evidence about the media- 2010). In an RCT comparing relaxation and med-
tors of such effects. In this section we describe itation training to a waitlist control group, Jain
several examples of such studies. This brief et al. (2007) used regression analyses to demon-
review is intended to be illustrative rather than strate that decreased rumination accounts for
comprehensive and focuses on studies in which treatment-related decreases in distress. In a con-
mindfulness and related constructs were mea- trolled but not randomized study of intensive
sured as outcomes in mindfulness-based inter- meditation training in prison inmates, Bowen,
vention studies. Witkiewitz, Dillworth, and Marlatt (2007) found
Several studies have identified mediators of the that reduced thought suppression mediated the
effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy effect of mindfulness training on alcohol use at
(MBCT; Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002) on 3-month follow-up.
depressive symptoms. In an RCT comparing Additionally, two studies have used regression
MBCT to antidepressant medication for the treat- analyses to test mediators of the effect of at-home
ment of depressive relapse, Kuyken et al. (2010) practice time on positive outcomes. Carmody and
used regression-based methods to demonstrate Baer (2008) reported that increases in self-
that increases in mindfulness (measured using the reported mindfulness skills (measured using the
KIMS) and self-compassion accounted for the FFMQ) accounted for the links between formal
effect of treatment on depressive symptoms at a practice time over the course of MBSR and
15-month follow-up. In an RCT comparing MBCT improvements in psychological symptoms, per-
to a waitlist control group, bootstrapping showed ceived stress, and psychological well-being. In a
significant indirect effects of MBCT on depressive study of the effects of a mindfulness training pro-
symptoms through changes in both mindfulness gram on working memory capacity in members
(measured using the MAAS) and rumination of the military, Jha, Stanley, Kiyonaga, Wong,
(Shahar, Britton, Sbarra, Figueredo, & Bootzin, and Gelfand (2010) found that treatment-related
2010). Another RCT comparing MBCT to a wait- preservation or improvement of working memory
list control group found a significant indirect effect capacity accounted for the link between mindful-
of MBCT on cognitive reactivity to sad mood ness practice time over the 8-week training and
through changes in mindfulness (measured using negative affect.
the KIMS; Raes, Ewulf, Van Heeringen, & Finally, several RCTs have found evidence
Williams, 2009). In a randomized trial of MBCT that psychological flexibility and experiential
12 How Do Mindfulness Interventions Work? 167

acceptance, which are closely related to mindful- newer methods for estimating indirect effects,
ness, mediate the effects of ACT on a variety of especially in the context of SEM or multilevel
therapeutic outcomes including chronic pain, growth models). By utilizing these recommenda-
depression, and smoking cessation (see Hayes, tions, researchers can contribute to this growing
Villatte, Levin, & Hildebrandt, 2011, for a literature and thus advance the ways MBIs can be
review). used to effect change.
Not all of these studies include clear evidence
that the mediating variable changes before the
outcome variables, although some do, and many References
do not use the most recently developed statistical
methods for testing mediation. In addition, the Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mind-
fulness: Emotion regulation following a focused
small number of studies in which mindfulness
breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
and related constructs are investigated as media- 44, 18491858.
tors suggest that greater attention to testing and Baer, R. A. (2011). Measuring mindfulness. Contemporary
refining newly developed integrative theories Buddhism, 12, 241261.
Baer, R. A., Lykins, E. L. B., & Peters, J. R. (2012).
especially in the context of MBI studiesmay
Mindfulness and self-compassion as predictors of psy-
lead to greater cohesion in the field. Notably, if chological well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology,
mindfulness and related constructs do not medi- 7, 230238.
ate the positive effects of MBIs, reporting such Baer, R. A., Samuel, D. B., & Lykins, E. L. (2011).
Differential item functioning on the Five Facet
null findings may be an important part of refining
Mindfulness Questionnaire is minimal in demographi-
both assessment methodology and mindfulness- cally matched meditators and nonmeditators.
based interventions. Despite these limitations, Assessment, 18(1), 310.
the findings presented here are consistent with Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., &
Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment meth-
the theory that mindfulness and closely related
ods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13,
constructs are at least partially responsible for the 2745.
salutary effects of MBIs. Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D.,
Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S. E., et al. (2008). Construct
validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in
meditating and nonmeditating samples. Assessment,
Conclusion 15, 329342.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004).
MBIs have proven effective for a variety of disor- Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The
Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment,
ders (Keng et al., 2011); however, evidence
11, 191206.
regarding the mechanisms by which MBIs lead to Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-
positive functioning remains somewhat sparse. mediator variable distinction in social psychological
As researchers continue to investigate the mecha- research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical consid-
erations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
nisms of change in MBIs, care should be taken to
51, 11731182.
plan and implement methodologically rigorous, Bauer, D. J., Preacher, K. J., & Gil, K. M. (2006).
theory-driven studies. As an area of study, greater Conceptualizing and testing random indirect effects
focus on the testing and refinement of recently and moderated mediation in multilevel models: New
procedures and recommendations. Psychological
developed integrative theoretical models of
Methods, 11, 142163.
mindfulness processes (e.g., Hlzel et al., 2011, Baum, C., Kuyken, W., Bohus, M., Heidenreich, T.,
Vago & Silbersweig, 2012) may lead to a more Michalak, J., & Steil, R. (2010). The psychometric
rapid and coherent accumulation of knowledge. properties of the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness
Skills in clinical populations. Assessment, 17,
Further, our review of existing mediational stud-
220229.
ies reveals that future studies may benefit from Bergomi, C., Tschacher, W., & Kupper, Z. (2013).
greater attention to the use of recent statistical Measuring mindfulness: First steps towards the devel-
advances in testing mediation of change over opment of a comprehensive mindfulness scale.
Mindfulness, 4, 1832.
time as a result of treatment (e.g., the use of
168 T. Eisenlohr-Moul et al.

Bieling, P., Hawley, L., Bloch, R., Corcoran, K., Levitan, Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity:
R., Young, L., et al. (2012). Treatment-specific Basic issues in objective scale development.
changes in decentering following mindfulness-based Psychological Assessment, 7, 309319.
cognitive therapy versus antidepressant medication of Dalrymple, K. L., & Herbert, J. D. (2007). Acceptance
placebo for prevention of depressive relapse. Journal and commitment therapy for generalized social anxi-
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 365372. ety disorder: A pilot study. Behavior Modification, 31,
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, 543568.
N. C., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: A pro- Davis, K. M., Lau, M. A., & Cairns, D. R. (2009).
posed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Development and preliminary validation of a trait ver-
Science and Practice, 11, 230241. sion of the Toronto Mindfulness Scale. Journal of
Bond, F., & Bunce, D. (2000). Mediators of change in Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 185197.
emotion-focused and problem focused worksite stress Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2007). Methods for
management interventions. Journal of Occupational integrating moderation and mediation: A general ana-
Health Psychology, 5, 156163. lytic framework using moderated path analysis.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Psychological Methods, 12, 122.
Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., et al. (2011). Preliminary Eisenlohr-Moul, T. A., Walsh, E. C., Charnigo, R. J.,
psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action Lynam, D. R., & Baer, R. A. (2012). The what and
Questionnaire - II: A revised measure of psychological the how of dispositional mindfulness: Using interac-
flexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior tions among subscales of the Five Facet Mindfulness
Therapy, 42, 676688. Questionnaire to understand its relation to substance
Bowen, S., Witkiewitz, K., Dillworth, T., & Marlatt, G. A. use. Assessment, 19(3), 276286.
(2007). The role of thought suppression in the rela- Erisman, S. M., & Roemer, L. (2010). A preliminary
tionship between mindfulness meditation and alcohol investigation of the effects of experimentally induced
use. Addictive Behaviors, 32, 23242328. mindfulness on emotional responding to film clips.
Brnstrm, R., Kvillemo, P., Brandberg, Y., & Moskowitz, Emotion, 10(1), 72.
J. T. (2010). Self-report mindfulness as a mediator of Erisman, S. M., & Roemer, L. (2012). A preliminary inves-
psychological well-being in a stress reduction inter- tigation of the process of mindfulness. Mindfulness, 3,
vention for cancer patients: A randomized study. 3043.
Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 39, 151161. Feldman, G. C., Hayes, A. M., Kumar, S. M., Greeson,
Broderick, P. C. (2005). Mindfulness and coping with dys- J. G., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2007). Mindfulness and
phoric mood: Contrasts with rumination and distrac- emotion regulation: The development and initial vali-
tion. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 501510. dation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). Journal of Psychopathology
being in the present: Mindfulness and its role in psy- and Behavioral Assessment, 29, 177190.
chological well-being. Journal of Personality and First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B.
Social Psychology, 84, 822848. W. (1996). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., Loverich, T. M., Biegel, axis I disorders, clinician version (SCID-CV).
G. M., & West, A. M. (2011). Out of the armchair and Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press.
into the streets: Measuring mindfulness advances Forman, E. M., Herbert, J. D., Moitra, E., Yeomans, P. D.,
knowledge and improves interventions: Reply to & Geller, P. A. (2007). A randomized controlled effec-
Grossman (2011). Psychological Assessment, 23(4), tiveness trial of acceptance and commitment therapy
10411046. and cognitive therapy for anxiety and depression.
Buchheld, N., Grossman, P., & Walach, H. (2001). Behavior Modification, 31, 772799.
Measuring mindfulness in insight meditation and Fresco, D. M., Moore, M., van Dulmen, M., Segal, Z. V.,
meditation-based psychotherapy: The development of Ma, S. H., Teasdale, J. D., et al. (2007). Initial psycho-
the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI). Journal metric properties of the Experiences Questionnaire:
for Meditation and Meditation Research, 1, 1134. Validation of a self-report measure of decentering.
Cardaciatto, L., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Moitra, E., Behavior Therapy, 38, 234246.
& Farrow, V. (2007). The assessment of present- Grossman, P. (2011). Defining mindfulness by how poorly
moment awareness and acceptance: The Philadelphia I think I pay attention during everyday awareness and
Mindfulness Scale. Assessment, 15, 204223. other intractable problems for psychologys (re)inven-
Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2008). Relationships between tion of mindfulness: Comment on Brown et al.
mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medi- Psychological Assessment, 23(4), 10341040.
cal and psychological symptoms and well-being in a Grossman, P., & Van Dam, N. (2011). Mindfulness, by
mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal any other name: Trials and tribulations of sati in west-
of Behavioral Medicine, 31, 2333. ern psychology and science. Contemporary Buddhism,
Chadwick, P., Hember, M., Mead, S., Lilley, B., & 12, 219239.
Dagnan, D. (2008). Responding mindfully to unpleas- Hargus, E., Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., & Williams, J. M. G.
ant thoughts and images: Reliability and validity of the (2010). Effects of mindfulness on meta-awareness and
Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire. British specificity of describing prodromal symptoms in sui-
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 451455. cidal depression. Emotion, 10, 3442.
12 How Do Mindfulness Interventions Work? 169

Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computa- Mindfulness Scale: Development and validation.
tional tool for observed variable mediation, modera- Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 14451467.
tion, and conditional process modeling. White paper. Leigh, J., & Neighbors, C. (2009). Enhancement motives
Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/public/pro- mediate the positive association between mind/body
cess2012.pdf. awareness and college student drinking. Journal of
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 650669.
Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Levin, M. E., Hildebrandt, M. J., Lillis, J., & Hayes, S. C.
Model, process, and outcomes. Behavior Research (2012). The impact of treatment components sug-
and Therapy, 44, 125. gested by the psychological flexibility model: A meta-
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., analysis of laboratory-based component studies.
Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., et al. (2004). Measuring Behavior Therapy, 43(4), 741756.
experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a MacCoon, D. G., Imel, Z. E., Rosenkranz, M. A., Sheftel,
working model. The Psychological Record, 54, J. G., Weng, H. Y., Sullivan, J. C., et al. (2011). The
553578. validation of an active control intervention for
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR).
Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50, 312.
approach to behavior change. New York, NY: MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M.,
Guilford. West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of
Hayes, S. C., Villatte, M., Levin, M., & Hildebrandt, M. methods to test mediation and other intervening vari-
(2011). Open, aware, and active: Contextual able effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83104.
approaches as an emerging trend in the behavioral and Marsh, H. W., Ldtke, O., Robitzsch, A., Trautwein, U.,
cognitive therapies. Annual Review of Clinical Asparouhov, T., Muthn, B. O., et al. (2009). Doubly-
Psychology, 7, 141168. latent models of school contextual effects: Integrating
Hlzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., multilevel and structural equation approaches to con-
Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness trol measurement and sampling errors. Multivariate
meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action Behavioral Research, 44, 764802.
from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., & Teasdale, J. D. (1996).
on Psychological Science, 6, 537559. Measure of awareness and coping in autobiographical
Jain, S., Shapiro, S. L., Swanick, S., Roesch, S. C., Mills, memory: Instruction for administering and coding.
P. J., Bell, I., et al. (2007). A randomized controlled Cambridge, UK: University of Cambridge.
trial of mindfulness meditation versus relaxation train- Unpublished manuscript.
ing: Effects on distress, positive states of mind, rumi- Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When
nation, and distraction. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, mediation is moderated and moderation is mediated.
33, 1121. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89,
Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & 852863.
Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective effects Neff, K. D. (2003). The development and validation of a
of mindfulness training on working memory capacity scale to measure self-compassion. Self and Identify, 2,
and affective experience. Emotion, 10, 5464. 223250.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living. New York, Nyklek, I., & Kuijpers, K. (2008). Effects of mindfulness-
NY: Bantam Doubleday. based stress reduction intervention on psychological
Keng, S., Smoski, J. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of well-being and quality of life: Is increased mindful-
mindfulness on psychological health: A review of ness indeed the mechanism? Annals of Behavioral
empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, Medicine, 35, 331340.
10411056. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS
Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple
equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford mediation models. Behavior Research Methods,
Press. Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717731.
Kraemer, H. C., Wilson, G. T., Fairburn, C. G., & Agras, Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and
W. S. (2002). Mediators and moderators of treatment resampling strategies for assessing and comparing
effects in randomized clinical trials. Archives of indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior
General Psychiatry, 59, 877883. Research Methods, 40, 879891.
Kuehner, C., Huffziger, S., & Liebsch, K. (2009). Preacher, K. J., & Selig, J. P. (2012). Advantages of Monte
Rumination, distraction and mindful self-focus: Carlo confidence intervals for indirect effects.
effects on mood, dysfunctional attitudes and cortisol Communication Methods and Measures, 6, 7798.
stress response. Psychological Medicine, 39(2), 219. Preacher, K. J., Zhang, Z., & Zyphur, M. J. (2011). Alternative
Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Taylor, methods for assessing mediation in multilevel data: The
R., Byford, S., et al. (2010). How does mindfulness- advantages of multilevel SEM. Structural Equation
based cognitive therapy work? Behaviour Research Modeling, 18, 161-182.
and Therapy, 48, 11051112. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depres-
Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, sion scale for research in the general population. Applied
N. D., Carlson, L., et al. (2006). The Toronto Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.
170 T. Eisenlohr-Moul et al.

Raes, F., Ewulf, D., Van Heeringen, C., & Williams, J. M. ology (pp. 290312). Washington, DC: American
G. (2009). Mindfulness and reduced cognitive reactiv- Sociological Association.
ity to sad mood: Evidence from a correlational study Tasca, G. A., & Gallop, R. (2009). Multilevel modeling of
and a non-randomized waiting list controlled study. longitudinal data for psychotherapy researchers:
Behavior Research and Therapy, 47, 623627. I. The basics. Psychotherapy Research, 19, 429437.
Rust, J., & Golombok, S. (2009). Modern psychometrics Tasca, G. A., Illing, V., Joyce, A. S., & Ogrodniczuk.
(3rd ed.). London, UK: Routledge. (2009). Three-level multilevel group models for
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). nested change data: A guide for group treatment
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: researchers. Psychotherapy Research, 19, 453461.
A new approach to preventing relapse. New York, NY: Tofighi, D., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2011). RMediation:
Guilford. An R package for mediation analysis confidence inter-
Shahar, B., Britton, W., Sbarra, D., Figueredo, A., & Bootzin, vals. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 692700.
R. (2010). Mechanisms of change of mindfulness-based Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness,
cognitive therapy for depression: Preliminary evidence self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): A
from a randomized controlled trial. International Journal framework for understanding the neurobiological
of Cognitive Therapy, 3, 402418. mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in Human
Shapiro, S. L., Oman, D., Thoresen, C. E., Plante, T. G., & Neuroscience, 6, 296.
Flinders, T. (2008). Cultivating mindfulness: Effects Van Dam, N. T., Earleywine, M., & Danoff-Burg, S.
on well-being. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, (2009). Differential item function across meditators
840862. and non-meditators on the Five Facet Mindfulness
Smith, G. T., Combs, J. L., & Pearson, C. M. (2012). Brief Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences,
instruments and short forms. In H. Cooper (Ed.), The 47(5), 516521.
handbook of research methods in psychology (pp. 395 Van Dam, N. T., Hobkirk, A. L., Danoff-Burg, S., &
409). Washington, DC: American Psychological Earleywine, M. (2012). Mind your words positive and
Association. negative items create method effects on the five facet
Smith, G. T., Fischer, S., & Fister, S. M. (2003). mindfulness questionnaire. Assessment, 19(2),
Incremental validity principles in test construction. 198204.
Psychological Assessment, 15, 467477. Walach, H., Buchheld, N., Buttenmuller, V., Kleinknecht,
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence inter- N., & Schmidt, S. (2006). Measuring mindfulness
vals for indirect effects in structural equation the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI). Personality
models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological method- and Individual Differences, 40, 15431555.
Clinical Perspectives: Mindfulness-
Based Cognitive Therapy 13
and Mood Disorders

Brandilyn R. Willett and Mark A. Lau

In the last 30 years, mindfulness-based interven- pendently, to treat psychological symptoms/


tions (MBIs) have emerged as promising acute disorders. MBIs, that evolved from MBSR and
and preventive psychosocial treatments for mood incorporate significant mindfulness training,
disorders and negative mood symptoms (e.g., include, for example, mindfulness-based cogni-
Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999; Kabat-Zinn, tive therapy (MBCT; Segal et al., 2002, 2013),
1982; Linehan, 1993; Segal, Williams, & mindfulness-based eating awareness training
Teasdale, 2002, 2013). Mindfulness-based stress (Kristeller & Hallett, 1999), and mindfulness
reduction (MBSR) was one of the first psychoso- based relapse prevention (Bowen, Chawla, &
cial interventions to use intensive mindfulness Marlatt, 2010). Other MBIs, such as acceptance
training as its primary treatment agent (Kabat- and commitment therapy (Hayes et al., 1999) and
Zinn, 1982, 1990). MBSR was first developed to dialectic behaviour therapy (DBT; Linehan,
improve the quality of life for individuals with 1993), are based on mindfulness principles but
serious medical illnesses, through the reduction do not include significant mindfulness training as
of their subjective suffering via systematic train- part of the treatment protocol.
ing in mindfulness meditation. Since then, meta- This chapter focuses on MBCT, an empiri-
analytic and systematic reviews generally support cally supported treatment for mood disorders
the clinical efficacy of MBSR in treating the psy- included in the National Institute for Clinical
chological sequelae, including depression and Excellence Guidelines (2009). We begin with an
anxiety, of many physical illnesses such as can- overview of the theoretical rationale underlying
cer, heart disease, fibromyalgia, epilepsy, and the development of MBCT for preventing depres-
chronic pain (Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, & sion relapse. We then explain how MBCT is
Walach, 2004; Hoffman, Sawyer, Witt, & Oh, thought to reduce the risk of depressive relapse
2010; Keng, Smoski, & Robins, 2011). and briefly describe the MBCT program. This is
Since MBSR was developed, several other followed by a summary of the empirical support
MBIs have evolved, either from MBSR or inde- for MBCT in the prophylaxis of major depressive
disorder (MDD) and for MBCT as a treatment for
acute and residual depressive symptoms. The
potential, and preliminary empirical support, for
B.R. Willett (*) M.A. Lau using MBCT to treat residual bipolar mood
Department of Psychiatry, University of British
Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
symptoms as well as to prevent depressive
e-mail: brandilyn.willett@gmail.com; relapse in bipolar disorder (BD) is presented.
mark.lau@vancouvercbt.ca We highlight unique patient factors that are

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 171


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_13
172 B.R. Willett and M.A. Lau

important for clinicians to consider prior to using cations, or associations, the individual practices
MBCT for mood disorders. We also discuss the meditation to cultivate a nonjudgmental, accept-
role of the therapists own personal mindfulness ing, present-moment awareness of experience
practice and training in delivering MBCT. Finally, including depression-related cognitions, sensa-
we introduce the possibility of delivering MBCT tions, and emotions (Linehan, 1993; Teasdale,
in alternative formats such as individually, via an 1999). In this way, at-risk individuals may facili-
online group and by telephone. tate a decentered perspective of thoughts and
emotions as they arise and increase their insight
into how automatic, habitual patterns of reactiv-
MBCT and Recurrent Major ity exacerbate emotional distress and can lead to
Depression depression. As a result, the individual is in a bet-
ter position to choose more flexible, deliberate,
Preventing Depressive Relapse and ultimately more adaptive responses at times
of potential relapse, producing lasting improve-
MBCT is a manualized, psychosocial group inter- ments in their emotional well-being.
vention originally designed to reduce the risk of
depressive relapse for individuals with recurrent
major depression (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, MBCT: The Program
2002). The rationale for developing MBCT is
based on a cognitive vulnerability model of MBCT, as originally developed, is an eight-
depressive relapse. This model is designed to session group intervention program for an aver-
explain why individuals with a history of multiple age of 812 participants. MBCT draws on several
depressive episodes are especially prone to depres- MBSR formal meditation practices such as the
sion relapses (Segal, Gemar, & Williams, 1999; body scan, mindful stretching, and mindfulness
Teasdale, 1988; Teasdale, Segal, & Williams, of breath/body/sounds/thoughts, but also incor-
1995). Fundamental to this model is Teasdales porates elements of cognitive therapy (Beck,
differential activation hypothesis which maintains Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979). In addition, Segal
that an important factor in whether someone will and colleagues developed the 3-Minute Breathing
experience recurrent depressive episodes is their Space, a new meditation practice, designed to
degree of cognitive reactivity, i.e., the degree to facilitate present-moment awareness of everyday
which automatic negative thinking patterns are distressing situations (Segal et al., 2002).
reactivated when in a mild dysphoric state Throughout the MBCT program, participants are
(Teasdale, 1983, 1988). These negative thought encouraged between group sessions to practice
patterns are posited to become more easily trig- formal and informal meditations as well as to
gered and accessible with each subsequent depres- complete homework exercises, totalling approxi-
sive episode. Thus, with increased experience of mately 4560 min a day. These meditation prac-
depression, even mild dysphoric moods can trig- tices and assignments are meant to assist the
ger a sufficient degree of negative, ruminative participants in incorporating awareness skills
thinking patterns that can contribute to the down- into their everyday lives.
ward spiral of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors The first half of the MBCT program focuses
that culminate into a full depressive episode (Lau, largely on developing the participants nonjudg-
Segal, & Williams, 2004). mental awareness of their moment-by-moment
Based on this model, Segal and colleagues experiences. The meditation practices introduced
(2002) designed MBCT to teach at-risk individu- in the first four sessions are designed to cultivate
als to relate differently to the mood-related reac- the individuals capacity to observe their physi-
tivation of automatic negative thinking patterns. cal, emotional, and cognitive experiences and the
Rather than engaging in ruminative, elaborative ability to deconstruct each of these into their
thinking about their experience, its origins, impli- component elements. The participants growing
13 MBCT and Mood Disorders 173

awareness is further supported by their participa- how participants can maintain long-term well-
tion in a cognitive therapy exercise designed to ness through adherence to what they have learned
teach them the cognitive model. Furthermore, in the program.
through psycho-education, participants are
guided in the development of their specific aware-
ness of depression-related experiences; in partic- Empirical Support
ular, they learn that negative automatic thoughts
are part of the package of MDD as a whole. This The efficacy of MBCT for the prevention of
knowledge facilitates the participants ability to depressive relapse has now been demonstrated in
decenter from negative automatic thoughts and to multiple randomized controlled trials and con-
identify their unique subjective indicators of firmed by systematic reviews and meta-analyses
potential relapse. (e.g., Bondolfi et al., 2010; Coelho, Canter, &
The second half of the program focuses on Ernst, 2007; Godfrin & van Heeringen, 2010;
developing the participants ability to respond Kuyken et al., 2008; Ma & Teasdale, 2004; Piet
more flexibly and deliberately when they detect a & Hougaard, 2011; Ree & Craigie, 2007;
possible relapse. Session five explicitly demon- Teasdale et al., 2000) (see Table 13.1). In the first
strates how an open, accepting, and nonjudgmen- two MBCT trials (Ma & Teasdale, 2004; Teasdale
tal awareness has the potential to be employed as et al., 2000), individuals who had recovered from
a skilful first step in relapse prevention. Session at least two major depressive episodes in the past
six introduces the possibility that thoughts are 5 years, were symptom free, and off medication
not facts. This offers participants the opportu- for at least 3 months before the study were ran-
nity to change their relationship to their thoughts domized to receive either MBCT or to continue
and helps them decenter from their thinking; that with treatment as usual (TAU). In both trials,
is, participants may begin to relate to rather than there was a significant interaction between the
from their thoughts and see these internal experi- number of previous episodes and treatment con-
ences simply as discrete mental events. Session dition on the dependent variable of depressive
six then provides participants with options as to relapse/recurrence. For participants with a his-
how they can choose to respond to thoughts, such tory of three or more depressive episodes (com-
as watching their thoughts come and go, looking prising over 75 % of participants), MBCT
at them as mental events rather than facts, and/or reduced the risk of relapse approximately by half
writing these events down to further facilitate as compared to TAU over the 1-year follow-up
their ability to decenter (Segal et al., 2013). By period. Furthermore, the benefits of MBCT could
encouraging this acceptance-based observational not be accounted for by a greater use of antide-
approach, MBCT differs from traditional cogni- pressant medication, as those in the MBCT group
tive therapy in that there are neither attempts at actually used less medication than those in the
reality testing and answering back to thoughts, TAU condition. For individuals with a history of
nor are there attempts to change the content of only two previous episodes, however, the relapse
thoughts. In session seven, participants are rates between the MBCT and TAU conditions
invited to take a 3-Minute Breathing Space were not significantly different in both studies.
(which they have been practicing since session Despite these findings, Piet and Hougaard (2011),
three) to become aware of their present-moment in their recent meta-analysis, cautioned against
experience as a first step in preventing depressive the premature exclusion of individuals with only
relapse. By bringing this deliberate awareness, two prior episodes in future studies, as further
participants are increasingly able to choose how evidence is needed to determine the differential
they will respond to the initial symptoms of effects of MBCT for these individuals.
depression. In this session participants also create More recently, MBCT was compared to main-
their own unique specific action plans for times tenance antidepressant treatment in preventing
of potential relapse. The final session focuses on relapse among remitted patients in two
174 B.R. Willett and M.A. Lau

Table 13.1 Empirical support for clinical effects of the M-ADM condition (60 %) over a 15-month
MBCT
follow-up period. Those in the MBCT group,
Empirical evidence however, reported significantly fewer residual
MBCT and depression depressive symptoms than those in the antide-
Relapse prevention pressant treatment group on the Hamilton Rating
MBCT versus TAU Teasdale et al. (2000) Scale for Depression (Hamilton, 1960) (M = 7.05
Ma and Teasdale (2004)
Bondolfi et al. (2010)
vs. 8.69) and the Beck Depression Inventory-II
Godfrin and van Heeringen (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) (M = 12.61 vs.
(2010) 17.02). Moreover, MBCT was shown to be more
MBCT versus ADM Kuyken et al. (2008) effective than maintenance pharmacotherapy in
Segal et al. (2010) improving quality of life, with no difference in
Meta-analysis/review Coelho et al. (2007) cost between these two treatments. In a more
Chiesa and Serretti (2011)
Keng et al. (2011)
recent trial, Segal et al. (2010) treated patients
Piet and Hougaard (2011) with a history of recurrent MDD to remission
Galante et al. (2012) through pharmacotherapy and then randomly
Reduced residual symptoms assigned them to one of the three conditions:
Controlled studies Crane et al. (2008) MBCT plus discontinuation of their medication;
Kuyken et al. (2008) continued M-ADM; or had their active medica-
Williams et al. (2008)
Geschwind et al. (2012) tion replaced with a placebo. Both active treat-
Reduced acute symptoms ment groups had better outcomes than the placebo
Controlled studies Barnhofer et al. (2009) group in preventing relapse for formerly
Foley et al. (2010) depressed patients, particularly for unstable
Van Aalderen et al. (2012) remitters, that is, individuals who experienced
Uncontrolled studies Finucane and Mercer (2006) periods of residual depressive symptoms during
Kenny and Williams (2007)
the remission phase.
Kingston et al. (2007)
(Mixed Methods) Overall, the conclusion of a recent meta-
Ree and Craigie (2007) analysis of MBCT randomized controlled trials is
Eisendrath et al. (2008) that MBCT is effective for preventing depressive
MBCT and bipolar disorder relapse in individuals with MDD (Piet &
Reduced residual symptoms Hougaard, 2011). This conclusion has been sup-
Controlled studies Williams et al. (2008) ported by several other reviews and meta-
Uncontrolled Miklowitz et al. (2009) analyses of both controlled and uncontrolled
Weber et al. (2010)
Deckersbach et al. (2012) studies (e.g., Chiesa & Serretti, 2011; Coelho
Meta-analysis/review Chiesa and Serretti (2010) et al., 2007; Galante, Iribarren, & Pearce, 2012;
MBCT mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, ADM antide-
Keng et al., 2011). Furthermore, recent studies
pressant medication, TAU treatment as usual have demonstrated that the benefits of MBCT
appear to be maintained, even long after patients
are no longer receiving treatment (Mathew,
Whitney, Kenny, & Denson, 2010; Munshi,
randomized controlled trials. Kuyken et al. Eisendrath, & Delucchi, 2012).
(2008) randomized participants with a history of
three or more depressive episodes and who were
taking antidepressant medication, to either MBCT: Residual and Acute
MBCT or maintenance antidepressant medica- Depression
tion (M-ADM). There were no significant differ-
ences in relapse rates between the MBCT A growing number of researchers have expanded
condition (47 %), where 75 % of patients with- on the application of the mindfulness-based cog-
drew from their antidepressant medication, and nitive vulnerability model of depressive relapse
13 MBCT and Mood Disorders 175

to acute depressive symptoms. Specifically, some reduced depressive and anxious symptoms after
have posited that teaching patients mindfulness they received the MBCT intervention (Foley
skills, which helps them distance themselves et al., 2010).
from ruminative, depressive thought patterns, In summary, the preliminary research indi-
may also be effective in reducing current depres- cates that MBCT has potential as a treatment for
sive symptoms (Kenny & Williams, 2007). both mild residual and acute depression symp-
toms, as well as for more severe cases such as
individuals with a history of suicidal ideation.
Empirical Support However, further controlled research with ade-
quate sample sizes is needed to evaluate the pos-
There is a growing body of evidence that sup- sible benefits of MBCT for these individuals.
ports the efficacy of MBCT in the treatment of
residual and acute depressive symptoms. For
example, three randomized controlled trials dem- MBCT and Bipolar Disorder
onstrated that MBCT reduced residual depressive
symptoms posttreatment for individuals with a Bipolar disorder, while less common than MDD,
history of at least three prior depressive episodes is a highly debilitating disorder characterized by
(Crane et al., 2008; Kuyken et al., 2008; Williams, recurrent episodes of depression and/or mania, as
Russell, & Russell, 2008) and a separate random- well as inter-episodic mood symptoms that cause
ized controlled trial demonstrated reduction in significant impairment and distress in the indi-
residual symptoms irrespective of the number of viduals psychosocial functioning (Angst, 1998;
previous episodes (Geschwind, Peeters, Huibers, Angst et al., 2003; Judd et al., 2003; Kessler
van Os, & Wichers, 2012). One recent, large- et al., 1994). Although individuals with BD may
scaled, randomized controlled trial showed experience periods of recovery, they often con-
MBCT to be efficacious in reducing depressive tinue to face substantial residual symptoms,
symptoms for patients with recurrent depression which can frequently lead to the recurrence of a
experiencing a current depressive episode (Van manic/depressive episode (Deckersbach et al.,
Aalderen et al., 2012). Another controlled trial 2012). Individuals with BD will experience even
evaluating depressed patients with a history of higher rates of depressive relapse than those with
suicidal ideation (Barnhofer et al., 2009) and a MDD, and will spend up to three times as many
mixed method study looking at current depres- days experiencing depressive as compared to
sive symptoms (Kingston, Dooley, Bates, Lawlor, manic or hypomanic symptoms (Angst, 1998;
& Malone, 2007) demonstrated that MBCT was Judd et al., 2003; Kupka et al., 2007).
effective in reducing acute depressive symptoms. While pharmacological interventions are cur-
This was also shown in four uncontrolled studies, rently the cornerstone treatment for managing
including individuals who failed to recover after BD, no single pharmacological agent has been
two ADM trials (Eisendrath et al., 2008; Kenny identified as a first-line therapy for the manage-
& Williams, 2007), depression and anxiety ment of acute bipolar depression (Yatham et al.,
(Finucane & Mercer, 2006), and heterogeneous 2005). Many of these medications produce unde-
psychiatric disorders (Ree & Craigie, 2007). In sirable side effects (e.g., sedation) and using anti-
another study looking at current symptoms of depressants has been associated with a greater
depression and anxiety in breast cancer patients, risk of patients switching from a depressive phase
Foley, Baillie, Huxter, Price, and Sinclair (2010) into hypomania or mania (Yatham et al., 2005).
found that there was a significant reduction in As is the case for acute treatments, limited proven
depression and anxiety symptoms, post-MBCT pharmacological options exist for long-term pre-
treatment. Furthermore, those who had initially ventive treatment. One approach to address this
been assigned to the waitlist condition and subse- problem is to use psychotherapy adjunctively to
quently received MBCT also demonstrated pharmacological treatment to enhance mood
176 B.R. Willett and M.A. Lau

stability and prevent relapse, which researchers et al. (2009) also conducted an uncontrolled pilot
have now started to investigate. study and found that, of the 22 patients who com-
pleted MBCT, there were significant reductions
in depressive symptoms and suicidal ideation, as
Empirical Support: Residual well as manic symptoms and anxiety. They con-
Symptoms cluded that MBCT appeared to be a promising
treatment alternative for BD, particularly for sub-
As a result of MBCTs success in treating MDD syndromal symptoms of depression, and that
relapse/recurrence, several studies have evalu- there is a need for future large-scale randomized
ated MBCT for treating residual mood and anxi- trials evaluating the potential of MBCT for the
ety symptoms for individuals with BD. For prevention of relapse in BD. Weber et al. (2010)
example, in a small randomized pilot study, conducted a feasibility trial exclusively with
Williams et al. (2008) assigned individuals with a bipolar patients for MBCT and concluded that
history of suicidality with unipolar and bipolar this treatment was feasible and favorably received
depression, in remission, to either an MBCT by the participants, though in need of larger, ran-
group or a waitlist. Participants from both condi- domized controlled trials. Finally, in their sys-
tions were compared on their levels of inter- tematic review of the research, Chiesa and
episodic symptoms of anxiety and depression Serretti (2010) found MBCT to be effective in
after the treatment period was over. The research- reducing anxiety symptoms for people with BD,
ers found that, in comparison to those on the confirming that this treatment provides benefit to
waitlist, participants who received MBCT dem- these patients.
onstrated decreased residual depressive symp-
toms between mood episodes. Furthermore,
patients with BD showed stable anxiety scores in Empirical Support: Relapse
the MBCT condition, whereas those in the wait- Prevention
list condition demonstrated significant increases
on anxiety measures. Although only 14 remitted Perhaps more important to inter-episodic mood
BD patients were evaluated in this study, the find- management in BD is finding treatments to
ings are encouraging and highlight the need for increase mood stability and prevent depressive
further research. relapse/recurrence. This notion is based on the
Several uncontrolled studies and systematic success of MBCT in treating relapse in unipolar
reviews have also demonstrated the benefit of depression and the promising results of alleviat-
MBCT for residual mood symptoms in BD. For ing residual symptoms. Given that no studies to
example, Deckersbach et al. (2012) used an date have directly examined the effect of MBCT
adapted version of MBCT to treat residual symp- in preventing relapse in BD, it is an important
toms of depression in individuals with BD. This area in need of investigation.
intervention consisted of 12 sessions, shorter sit-
ting meditations and body scans (to accommo-
date participants ability to concentrate), as well Patient Suitability: Important
as mindful movement exercises to help them Considerations
focus. The researchers also added a problem
solving element to assist patients in managing When working with mood disorders, it is important
potential hassles and obstacles in their lives. to consider the impact of unique patient factors on
Participants at the end of treatment, as well as at treatment course and outcome when considering
the 3-month follow-up, demonstrated higher lev- the clinical application of MBIs. Careful assess-
els of mindfulness and lower residual depressive ment and determination of patient suitability will
mood symptoms than they did prior to starting better prepare both clients and clinicians prior to
treatment (Deckersbach et al., 2012). Miklowitz commencing mindfulness treatments.
13 MBCT and Mood Disorders 177

First, whenever a psychotherapeutic technique et al., 2009). In another small controlled study
is used, it is important for the therapist to con- Langer, Cangas, Salcedo, and Fuentes (2012)
sider the patients capacity to engage with the replicated Chadwick et al.s (2009) results and
treatment. In mood disorders, particularly for found that individuals in the MBCT group were
individuals with acute depression or residual better able than those in the control group to
depressive symptoms, difficulties in focus, con- respond mindfully to stressful internal events.
centration, and motivation are very common. These results must be interpreted with caution,
Those who are unable to fully engage in the train- however, as both studies were evaluating psycho-
ing may be incapable of cultivating mindfulness sis specific to psychotic disorders (e.g., schizo-
and could even be practicing rumination. These phrenia, schizoaffective disorders) and not
psychological barriers may lead to heightened psychosis as it presents in mood disorders and
feelings of inadequacy and incapacity and could there may be important differences between these
result in treatment dropout or unsuccessful thera- groups. Mood disorder patients with psychotic
peutic intervention. Like in DBT where goals for features may require more acute, specific, or
mindfulness practices are established based on intensive interventions, such as inpatient hospi-
the capacity of the individual (Linehan, 1993), talization and/or medication. Given that there is
clinicians working with clients whose concentra- no direct evidence to support the utility of mind-
tion and focus are affected may elect to summa- fulness for patients with mood disorders with
rize the readings or cognitively complex tasks. past or current psychotic symptoms, therapists
They may focus on mindfulness practices that are must carefully screen these patients and use cau-
less demanding, or more movement oriented, tion prior to using MBCT.
until the patient is able to engage more fully. It is also important to carefully consider the use
Second, it is unclear if mindfulness training of MBCT in mood disorder patients with a history
should be applied to mood disorder patients with of or current post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
current or past psychotic features. While the Germer, Siegel, and Fulton (2005) state that mind-
prevalence rates are low (e.g., 0.35 % for MDD fulness must be skillfully applied with these
and 0.24 % for BD; Perl et al., 2007), these patients and emphasized the importance of first
cases are often more serious and currently there determining their stability of attention (p. 171).
is only indirect evidence to suggest how mindful- This is to ensure that the patient will be able to
ness may impact these patients. Limited past experience any painful thoughts, emotions, or sen-
clinical literature has advised against using medi- sations as they arise without becoming over-
tation with patients who have or are vulnerable to whelmed. Therefore it is again important to
experiencing active psychotic symptoms, claim- carefully assess a patient prior to commencing
ing that meditation may produce negative conse- mindfulness training as well as to carefully monitor
quences (Deatherage & Lethbridge, 1975; those who may be vulnerable to PTSD symptoms.
Yorston, 2001). However, recent small controlled A fourth factor to consider is the presence of
studies have indicated that MBIs may be benefi- other concurrent disorders and their potential to
cial. In a small randomized evaluation of group affect the outcome of MBIs. Some examples of
therapy for psychosis, Chadwick, Hughes, disorders frequently co-occurring with mood dis-
Russell, Russell, and Dagnan (2009) found that, orders are anxiety, substance abuse/dependence,
although the initial analysis indicated that there eating disorders, and personality disorders. While
were no significant differences between those there is evidence to suggest that mindfulness may
who received the mindfulness intervention and be helpful for the mood symptoms and even the
those who remained on the waitlist, once the primary symptoms seen in these patients, these
waitlist group received the intervention, a sec- comorbidities may impact the patients ability to
ondary analyses combining both mindfulness engage or benefit from the treatment. Therefore
groups revealed significant positive differences in therapists will need to account for these special
clinical functioning and mindfulness (Chadwick considerations.
178 B.R. Willett and M.A. Lau

Finally, while mindfulness techniques may be instructors should receive formal MBCT teacher
beneficial in the treatment of mood disorders, it is development training. Segal et al. (2013) also
important to remember that idiosyncratic patient recommend attendance at a formal mindfulness
factors may also present barriers to effective meditation retreat for a minimum of 710 days.
treatment delivery. For example, the idea of MBIs Finally, they stressed the importance of maintain-
may initially be off-putting to certain clients. ing a personal daily meditation practice. These
Although MBIs have been gaining in popularity, guidelines are based on the assumption that,
some individuals may be dissuaded due to their when teaching from their own meditation experi-
non-conventional, esoteric, or foreign nature, or ence, instructors will better embody the mindful-
even to the meditations themselves due to misper- ness qualities they are inviting their patients to
ceptions of religiosity or spirituality (Baer, 2003). practice, particularly when responding to
If a therapist is able to recognize and appropri- patients reports of their meditation experiences.
ately address these concerns and clarify miscon- Therapists with sufficient training and personal
ceptions, these barriers may be surmountable experience may be better able to coach patients
allowing the client to learn skills they may have who struggle with resistance, striving, or the
otherwise avoided. inevitable discomforts and unfamiliar subjective
Each of these factors highlights the impor- experiences that arise through mindfulness prac-
tance of appropriate screening and assessment, as tices. This may be particularly important to indi-
well as proper selection of suitable interventions viduals with mood disorders and ruminative
based on the unique needs of each patient. To thinking patterns or to those who are especially
accomplish this, therapists require adequate resistant or fearful. A therapist strongly embody-
knowledge, training, and experience. ing mindfulness may also be less inclined to try
to challenge the content of the patients thoughts,
as is emphasized in other therapies, but rather
Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapy: focus on changing the patients relationship to
Therapist Training and Practice their thoughts which is the emphasis in
MBCT. Overall, a well-trained therapist who
The therapists initial training and ongoing per- embodies mindfulness and maintains a personal
sonal practice play an important role in the effec- practice is more likely to have the necessary
tive delivery of MBCT (Segal et al., 2013). skills when using MBCT with mood disorder
Santorelli and Kabat-Zinn (2009) regarded suffi- patients.
cient mindfulness training and practice as neces-
sary for treatment fidelity. In addition, recent
studies have provided preliminary evidence for Alternative Delivery Formats
the benefits of therapists practice on patient out-
comes. For example, Grepmair et al. (2007) Investigators are now exploring patient interest
found that patients of psychotherapists-in- levels and the feasibility of alternative MBCT
training randomly assigned to a Zen-meditation delivery formats. As the support for MBCTs effi-
condition demonstrated better treatment out- cacy and clinical interest grows, it is important to
comes than patients of the therapists who did not address the possibility that the traditional struc-
practice daily meditation. tured in-person weekly group format of MBCT
Recognizing the importance of the MBCT may limit the number of patients accessing this
teachers own training in treatment outcome, treatment. First, the group format may limit access
Segal et al. (2013) provide several recommenda- in healthcare systems that favor the delivery of
tions for potential MBCT instructors. First, thera- individual versus group therapy. For example,
pists should have an adequate clinical background Australian mental healthcare is much more orien-
as well as sufficient training in leading group tated towards provision of individual therapies
psychotherapy and cognitive therapy. Second, than group therapies with the overwhelming
13 MBCT and Mood Disorders 179

majority of services being provided to individu- phone, or in the traditional group format. The
als. In addition, 5 % of potentially eligible patients delivery format chosen most frequently by par-
declined participation in an MBCT study due to ticipants was individual in-person, followed by
the group aspect (Kuyken et al., 2008). Second, in-person group, online group, and finally tele-
in-person delivery may limit access due to geo- phone delivery. Participants were also assessed
graphical constraints or physical barriers such as pre- and post-treatment on a variety of outcome
immobility or lack of transportation, or may not measures, including self-reported state (Toronto
appeal to individuals with concerns about main- Mindfulness Scale; Lau et al., 2006) and trait
taining anonymity. (Five Facets of Mindfulness Questionnaire; Baer,
As a result, investigators are exploring Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006)
whether patients would be interested in alterna- mindfulness, decentering (Experiences
tive MBCT delivery formats, as well as whether Questionnaire; Fresco et al., 2007), and rumina-
they are feasible and effective in comparison to tion (Response Styles Questionnaire; Nolen-
attending in-person groups. A recent study evalu- Hoeksema, 1991). The participants in the
ated health authority employees stated prefer- in-person group, online group, and individual
ences for receiving MBCT in the workplace in face-to-face conditions demonstrated similar pat-
four alternative delivery formats via an online terns of significant improvements on all outcome
Discreet Choice Experiment (DCE) (Lau, Colley, measures after receiving MBCT. While the pat-
Willett, & Lynd, 2012). A DCE is a technique to tern of results was similar for the telephone con-
elicit stated preferences by asking respondents to dition, the improvements were not statistically
choose between competing hypothetical but real- significant. This is likely due to the low sample
istic scenarios. One hundred and fifty-one respon- size associated with higher dropout rate for this
dents completed the DCE. They found that condition relative to the other conditions. While
respondents with a history of depression demon- larger studies with increased sample sizes (espe-
strated significant preferences as to whether they cially for telephone delivery) are needed, these
would choose to receive MBCT individually in- results provide support for new MBCT delivery
person, individually over the phone, online with a formats in that they produced similar benefits as
group, or in-person with a group. A latent class compared to the traditional in-person group
analysis suggested four classes. Class one con- delivery.
sisted of 42 % of the individuals whose primary Similarly, in a pilot study called Project
concern was the effectiveness of therapy. The UPLIFT (Thompson et al., 2010) patients with
second class (23 %) was primarily concerned epilepsy and depression were randomly assigned
about the type of interaction, preferring to receive to receive MBCT over the phone or online, or to
MBCT by telephone on their employers time. a waitlist condition. Those treated with MBCT
The third class (13 %) had the strongest prefer- showed a reduction in depressive symptoms as
ence for face-to-face delivery, whether individu- compared to the waitlist, and there was little dif-
ally or in a group. The fourth class (8 %) wished ference between telephone and online delivery.
to receive MBCT on their own time and wanted An associated study exploring these participants
to remain anonymous. impressions on the distance treatments found that
More recently, the actual preferences for, and the participants viewed them as acceptable and
feasibility of, MBCT as delivered in the same beneficial (Reisinger-Walker, Obolensky, Dini, &
four formats in employees of large healthcare Thompson, 2010).
organizations with a history of depression (Lau, Overall these studies provide preliminary sup-
Grabovac, & Willett, 2011) were evaluated. port that patients with mood disorders and other
Eligible participants with at least two previous conditions are interested in receiving MBCT and
depressive episodes and who were not currently that they have definite preference differences for
depressed were offered MBCT (if available) the method of delivery. Furthermore, each of
either online, in-person one on one, over the tele- these studies has provided preliminary support
180 B.R. Willett and M.A. Lau

for the efficacy of MBCT when delivered in alter- hypomania. Journal of Affective Disorders, 73,
123131.
native formats such as online, over the phone, or
Baer, R. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical inter-
one on one with the instructor. Additional vention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical
research to determine whether there are any dif- Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 125143.
ferences in relapse rates across the different Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., &
Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment meth-
delivery models is needed, however, to support
ods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13,
the use of alternative delivery methods in future 2745.
MBCT dissemination efforts. Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Hargus, E., Amarasinghe, M.,
Winder, R., & Williams, J. M. G. (2009). Mindfulness-
based cognitive therapy as a treatment for chronic
depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47,
Future Directions/Considerations 366373.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).
Mood disorders are a major clinical concern and Cognitive therapy of depression. New York, NY:
Guilford.
evidence is accumulating for MBCTs efficacy in
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck
preventing relapse and treating acute symptoms depression inventorySecond edition manual. San
for MDD and BD. Future larger scaled controlled Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
studies are needed to determine the effectiveness Bondolfi, G., Jermann, F., Van der Linden, M. V., Gex-
Fabry, M., Bizzini, L., Rouget, B. W., Bertschy, G.
of MBCT in treating acute depressive symptoms
(2010). Depression relapse prophylaxis with
in MDD as well as preventing episodic relapse in Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: Replication and
patients with BD. Based on the promising pre- extension in the Swiss health care system. Journal of
liminary evidence, more research is needed to Affective Disorders, 122, 224231.
Bowen, S., Chawla, N., & Marlatt, G. A. (2010).
evaluate the feasibility and efficacy of MBCT
Mindfulness-based relapse prevention for addictive
delivered in alternative formats, such as online, behaviours: A clinicians guide. New York, NY:
individually in-person, over the telephone, or an Guilford Press.
in-person group. Given the complexities of mood Chadwick, P., Hughes, S., Russell, D., Russell, I., &
Dagnan, D. (2009). Mindfulness groups for distressing
disorders and patient differences, it is important
voices and paranoia: A replication and randomized fea-
for the clinician to carefully consider the unique sibility trial. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy,
characteristics of the individual and the disorder 37, 403412.
when considering MBCT for their client. A ther- Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2010). A systematic review of
neurobiological and clinical features of mindfulness
apist should receive adequate training prior to
meditations. Psychological Medicine, 40, 12391252.
commencing MBCT treatment with their patients Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2011). Mindfulness based cog-
and consistently maintain their own personal nitive therapy for psychiatric disorders: A systematic
mindfulness practice to optimize patient treat- review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 187,
441453.
ment outcomes. As the support for MBIs grows
Coelho, H. F., Canter, P. H., & Ernst, E. (2007).
and the availability of these interventions broad- Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: Evaluating cur-
ens, so does the potential to reduce patient suffer- rent evidence and informing future research. Journal of
ing and improve the lives of individuals with Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 10001005.
Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., Duggan, D., Hepburn, S.,
mood disorders.
Fennell, M. V., & Williams, J. (2008). Mindfulness-
based cognitive therapy and self-discrepancy in recov-
ered depressed patients with a history of depression
References and suicidality. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32,
775787.
Deatherage, G., & Lethbridge, U. (1975). The clinical use of
Angst, J. (1998). The emerging epidemiology of hypoma-
mindfulness meditation techniques in short term ther-
nia and bipolar II disorder. Journal of Affective
apy. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 7, 133143.
Disorders, 50, 143151.
Deckersbach, T., Hlzel, B. K., Eisner, L. R., Stange, J. P.,
Angst, J., Gamma, A., Benazzi, F., Ajdacic, V., Eich, D.,
Peckham, A. D., Dougherty, D. D., & Nierenberg,
& Rossler, W. (2003). Toward re-definition of sub-
A. A. (2012). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for
threshold bipolarity: Epidemiology and proposed cri-
nonremitted patients with bipolar disorder. CNS
teria for bipolar-II, minor bipolar disorders and
Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 18(2), 133141.
13 MBCT and Mood Disorders 181

Eisendrath, S. J., Delucchi, K., Bitner, R., Fenimore, P., anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review.
Smit, M., & McLane, M. (2008). Mindfulness-based Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78,
cognitive therapy for treatment-resistant depression: A 169183.
pilot study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 77, Judd, L. L., Schettler, P. J., Akiskal, H. S., Maser, J., Coryell,
319320. W., Solomon, D., Keller, M. (2003). Long-term
Finucane, A., & Mercer, S. W. (2006). An exploratory symptomatic status of bipolar I vs. bipolar II disorders.
mixed methods study of the acceptability and effec- International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 6,
tiveness of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for 127137.
patients with active depression and anxiety in primary Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behav-
care. BMC Psychiatry, 6, 1428. ioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the
Foley, E., Baillie, A., Huxter, M., Price, M., & Sinclair, E. practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical con-
(2010). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for indi- siderations and preliminary results. General Hospital
viduals whose lives have been affected by cancer: A Psychiatry, 4, 3347.
randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the
Clinical Psychology, 78, 7279. wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and
Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., van Dulmen, M., Segal, illness. New York, NY: Dell.
Z. V., Teasdale, J. D., Ma, H., & Williams, J. M. G. Keng, S., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of
(2007). Initial psychometric properties of the mindfulness on psychological health: A review of
Experiences Questionnaire: Validation of a self-report empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31,
measure of decentering. Behavior Therapy, 38, 10411056.
234246. Kenny, M. A., & Williams, J. M. G. (2007). Treatment-
Galante, J., Iribarren, S. J., & Pearce, P. F. (2012). Effects resistant depressed patients show a good response to
of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on mental dis- mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Behaviour
orders: A systematic review and meta-analysis of ran- Research and Therapy, 45, 617625.
domised controlled trials. Journal of Research in Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B.,
Nursing, 18, 133155. Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., Kendler, K. S. (1994).
Germer, C. K., Siegel, R. D., & Fulton, P. R. (2005). Lifetime and twelve-month prevalence of DSM-III-R
Anxiety disorders: Befriending fear. In C. K. Germer, in psychiatric disorders in the United States: Results
R. D. Siegel, & P. R. Fulton (Eds.), Mindfulness and from the National Comorbidity Study. Archives of
psychotherapy (pp. 152172). New York, NY: General Psychiatry, 51, 819.
Guilford Press. Kingston, T., Dooley, B., Bates, A., Lawlor, E., & Malone,
Geschwind, N., Peeters, F., Huibers, M., van Os, J., & K. (2007). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for
Wichers, M. (2012). Efficacy of mindfulness-based residual depressive symptoms. Psychology and
cognitive therapy in relation to prior history of depres- Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 80,
sion: Randomized controlled trial. The British Journal 193203.
of Psychiatry, 201, 320325. Kristeller, J. L., & Hallett, C. B. (1999). An exploratory
Godfrin, K. A., & van Heeringen, C. (2010). The effects study of a meditation-based intervention for binge eat-
of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on recurrence ing disorder. Journal of Health Psychology, 4,
of depressive episodes, mental health and quality of 357363.
life: A randomized controlled study. Behaviour Kupka, R. W., Altshuler, L. L., Nolen, W. A., Suppes, T.,
Research and Therapy, 48, 738746. Luckenbaugh, D. A., Leverich, G. S., Post, R. M.
Grepmair, L., Mitterlehner, F., Loew, T., Bachler, E., (2007). Three times more days depressed than manic
Rother, W., & Nickel, M. (2007). Promoting mindful- or hypomanic in both bipolar I and bipolar II disor-
ness in psychotherapists in training influences the ders. Bipolar Disorders, 9, 531535.
treatment results of their patients: A randomized, Kuyken, W., Byford, S., Taylor, R. S., Watkins, E.,
double-blind, controlled study. Psychotherapy and Holden, E., White, K., Teasdale, J. D. (2008).
Psychosomatics, 76, 332338. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to prevent
Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. relapse in recurrent depression. Journal of Consulting
(2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health and Clinical Psychology, 76, 966978.
benefits. A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Langer, A. I., Cangas, A. J., Salcedo, E., & Fuentes, B.
Research, 57, 3543. (2012). Applying mindfulness therapy in a group of
Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. psychotic individuals: A controlled study. Behavioural
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 40, 105109.
23, 5662. Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson,
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). N. D., Carlson, L. S., Devins, G. (2006). The
Acceptance and commitment therapy. New York, NY: Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and valida-
Guilford Press. tion. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 14451467.
Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. Lau, M. A., Colley, L., Willett, B. R., & Lynd, L. D.
(2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on (2012). Employees preferences for access to
182 B.R. Willett and M.A. Lau

mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to reduce the risk ations of a CBT and mindfulness intervention for
of depressive relapse: A discrete choice experiment. people with epilepsy and depression. Epilepsy &
Mindfulness, 3, 318326. Behavior, 19, 239246.
Lau, M. A., Grabovac, A., & Willett, B. R. (2011). Santorelli, S. F., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (2009). Mindfulness-
Evaluating the feasibility of delivering mindfulness- based stress reduction professional training resource
based cognitive therapy in group over the internet or manual: Integrating mindfulness meditation into med-
individual face-to-face or via telephone in the work- icine and health care. Worcester, MA: Center for
place. Paper presented at the 45th Annual Conference Mindfulness in Medicine, Health Care, and Society.
of the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Segal, Z. V., Bieling, P., Young, T., MacQueen, G., Cooke,
Therapies, Toronto, Canada. R., Martin, L., Levitan, R. D. (2010). Antidepressant
Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Williams, M. G. (2004). monotherapy vs. sequential pharmacotherapy and
Teasdales differential activation hypothesis: Implications Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy, or placebo, for
for mechanisms of depressive relapse and suicidal behav- relapse prophylaxis in recurrent depression. Archives
iour. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 10011017. of General Psychiatry. 67, 12561264.
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment Segal, Z. V., Gemar, M., & Williams, S. (1999).
of borderline personality. New York, NY: Guilford Differential cognitive response to a mood challenge
Press. following successful cognitive therapy or pharmaco-
Ma, S. H., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Mindfulness-based therapy for unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal
cognitive therapy for depression: Replication and Psychology, 108, 310.
exploration of differential relapse prevention effects. Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002).
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression:
3140. A new approach for preventing relapse. New York,
Mathew, K. L., Whitney, H. S., Kenny, M. A., & Denson, NY: Guilford Press.
L. A. (2010). The long term effects of mindfulness- Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2013).
based cognitive therapy as a relapse prevention treat- Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression:
ment for major depressive disorder. Behavioural and A new approach for preventing relapse (2nd ed.).
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 38, 561576. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Miklowitz, D., Alatiq, Y., Goodwin, G., Geddes, J., Teasdale, J. D. (1983). Negative thinking in depression:
Dimidjian, S., Hauser, M., & Williams, M. (2009). A Cause, effect or reciprocal relationship? Advances in
pilot study of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 325.
bipolar disorder. International Journal of Cognitive Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persis-
Therapy, 2, 373382. tent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 247274.
Munshi, K., Eisendrath, S., & Delucchi, K. (2012). Teasdale, J. D. (1999). Metacognition, mindfulness and
Preliminary long-term follow-up of mindfulness- the modification of mood disorders. Clinical
based cognitive therapy induced remission of depres- Psychology & Psychotherapy, 6, 146155.
sion. Mindfulness, 4, 354361. Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., Ridgeway,
National Institute for Clinical Excellence (NICE). (2009). V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention
Management of depression in primary and secondary of relapse/recurrence in major depression by
care. London, UK: National Institute for Clinical mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of
Excellence. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615623.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995).
their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. How does cognitive therapy prevent relapse and why
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569582. should attentional control (mindfulness) help?
Perl, J., Suvisaari, J., Saarni, S. I., Kuoppasalmi, K., Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 2539.
Isomets, E., Pirkola, S., Lnnqvist, J. (2007). Thompson, N. J., Reisinger-Walker, E., Obolensky, N.,
Lifetime prevalence of psychotic and bipolar I disor- Winning, A., Barmon, C., DiIorio, C., & Compton, M.
ders in a general population. Archives of General (2010). Distance delivery of mindfulness-based cogni-
Psychiatry, 64, 1928. tive therapy for depression: Project UPLIFT. Epilepsy
Piet, J., & Hougaard, E. (2011). The effect of mindfulness- & Behavior, 19, 247254.
based cognitive therapy for prevention of relapse in Van Aalderen, J. R., Donders, A. R. T., Giommi, F.,
recurrent major depressive disorder: A systematic Spinhoven, P., Barendregt, H. P., & Speckens, A. E. M.
review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology (2012). The efficacy of mindfulness-based cognitive
Review, 31, 10321040. therapy in recurrent depressed patients with and with-
Ree, M. J., & Craigie, M. A. (2007). Outcomes following out a current depressive episode: A randomized con-
mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in a heteroge- trolled trial. Psychological Medicine, 42, 9891001.
neous sample of adult outpatients. Behaviour Change, Weber, B., Jermann, F., Gex-Fabry, M., Nallet, A.,
24, 7086. Bondolfi, G., & Aubry, J. M. (2010). Mindfulness-
Reisinger-Walker, E., Obolensky, N., Dini, S., & based cognitive therapy for bipolar disorder: A feasi-
Thompson, N. J. (2010). Formative and process evalu- bility trial. European Psychiatry, 25, 334337.
13 MBCT and Mood Disorders 183

Williams, J. M., Alatiq, Y., Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., Yatham, L. N., Kennedy, S. H., ODonovan, C., Parikh, S.,
Fennell, M. J., Duggan, D. S., Goodwin, G. M. MacQueen, G., McIntyre, R., Gorman, C. P. (2005).
(2008). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) Canadian Network for Mood and Anxiety Treatments
in bipolar disorder: Preliminary evaluation of immedi- (CANMAT) guidelines for the management of patients
ate effects on between-episode functioning. Journal of with bipolar disorder: Consensus and controversies.
Affective Disorders, 107, 275279. Bipolar Disorders, 7(Suppl 3), 569.
Williams, J. M., Russell, I., & Russell, D. (2008). Yorston, G. (2001). Mania precipitated by meditation: A
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: Further issues case report and literature review. Mental Health,
in current evidence and future research. Journal of Religion & Culture, 4, 209213.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 524529.
Mindfulness and the Addictive
Process: Psychological Models 14
and Neurobiological Mechanisms

Judson A. Brewer, Nicholas T. Van Dam,


and Jake H. Davis

Introduction In the recent psychological literature, craving


has been defined as the subjective experience of
Mindfulness meditation aims at helping people wanting [something] (Tiffany & Wray, 2012,
reduce suffering that is fed and perpetuated by p. 2). There are many components that contribute
craving. The early Buddhist texts in which this to the experience of craving; here we emphasize
practice is first articulated offer a therapeutic craving as a non-elaborative, somatic, and affec-
model explicating suffering, its cause, the possi- tive experience of grasping after a particular
bility of a cure, and the methods required to desired object (i.e., the gut feeling of wanting).
achieve that cure. This approach targets the psy- The term for craving used in the early Pali
chological roots of suffering, and in particular, Buddhist texts, tan.h, more literally means thirst.
craving, which is identified as the primary cause This apt turn of phrase indeed suggests the insa-
of suffering. It is through the relinquishment, tiable desire reported by people dealing with
release, and letting go of craving that suffering addictions, and the enormous suffering that can
is cured (SN.56.11 in Thanissaro, B. (trans.), result. Addictive behaviors cause stress and
2010a).1 Mindfulness is a central component of anguish on personal, family, and societal levels,
the method prescribed for creating release from and people with addictions use language reminis-
the cycle of craving and suffering. cent of the ancient texts we cite below. In a book
entitled Ninety Days: A Memoir of Recovery,
1
Billy Clegg wrote I can feel that old burn,
Following convention, sutta references are to collection
(e.g., Majjhima Nikya (MN), Suttanipta (Sn)), and then
that hibernating want, come awake. I imagine the
either to sutta number (in the case of DN, MN, and Iti), relief that first hit will deliver and Im suddenly
sam.yutta and sutta number (SN), nipta and sutta number up off the couch and pacing. No no no, I chant.
(AN), verse number (Dhp), vagga and sutta number (Ud, Sn). No f-king way. That craving, once it begins, is
J.A. Brewer (*) almost impossible to reverse. What my addict
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Yale mind imagines, my addict body chases. As he
University, New Haven, CT, USA
e-mail: judson.brewer@yale.edu
points out, we are literally slaves to our bodily
sensations when we do not have mental training
N.T. Van Dam
Department of Psychology, University at Albany,
tools in place to work skillfully with our sensa-
State University of New York, Albany, NY, USA tions. Not surprisingly, craving is rooted in our
J.H. Davis
most basic neural processes: positive and negative
Department of Philosophy, Graduate Center, City reinforcement. And of course, this is where mind-
University of New York, New York, NY, USA fulness training can help. Paradoxical to many

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 185


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_14
186 J.A. Brewer et al.

modern cognitive-behavioral and/or control-


based treatments, which focus on building men-
tal muscle to avoid, think through, and substitute
behaviors, mindfulness training teaches us to see
more clearly what we get from our addictions,
rather than avoiding or trying to change them. In
doing so, we naturally begin to become disen-
chanted with the cycle of craving, which begins
the process of letting go.
Addictions are one of the costliest human con-
ditions, having significant effects on mental,
physical, and economic health. Drug abuse, as a
whole in the USA, grows in economic cost by an
estimated $7 billion per year, costing approxi-
mately $180.8 billion in 2002, primarily in lost
productivity (Office of National Drug Control
Fig. 14.1 Early models of addiction: dependent origina-
Policy, 2004). Given the impact of these disor- tion. Copyright 2011 Judson Brewer. Reprinted with per-
ders, much convergent work has been done to mission of author
identify the mechanistic underpinnings of addic-
tions, and to develop effective treatments
(Goldstein et al., 2009; Kalivas & Volkow, 2005). based in automated affective reactions to percep-
In this chapter, we outline contemporary psycho- tual stimuli. For example, when environmental
logical models of addiction and highlight how cues register through the senses (thoughts are
current understanding of the addictive process considered categorically indistinct from the stan-
relates to Buddhist psychological models of dard five senses, Fig. 14.1, #1), an affective
human suffering. Also, we review studies of tone automatically arises that is typically felt as
mindfulness training for addictions and discuss pleasant or unpleasant (#2). The valence of this
insights they might provide with regard to target- affective tone is conditioned by associative mem-
ing core components of the addictive process. ories that were formed from previous experiences
Finally, we relate models of addiction and mind- (#6 + MIND). Subsequently, a desire or craving
fulness to emerging neuroimaging studies and arises as a psychological urge to act or perform a
explain how these may provide critical links behavior (#3). The craving is for the continuation
between psychological models of addiction, the of pleasant, or the cessation of unpleasant feeling
key components of the addictive process that tones, respectively. This craving motivates action
mindfulness training targets, and the neurobio- (#4) and fuels the birth of self-identity (#5),
logical mechanisms thereunder. creating a link between action and outcome that
is recorded in memory (#6). Importantly, when
this pleasant affective tone (or absence of an
Early Models of Addiction unpleasant affective tone) passes, one is left with
the pain, distress, and despair of its absence,
Buddhist psychological models distinguish five thus completing one cycle and priming the
aspects of emotional reaction to triggers (i.e., individual for the next time he/she encounters a
anything that elicits a reaction): bodily, affective, similar sensory stimulus (#7) (SN.12.2 in
cognitive, volitional, and conscious components. Thanissaro, 2010b).
The dynamic, causal relationships between these In other words, an individual learns that drug use
differentiated processes are delineated by the (action) decreases unpleasant feelings such as nega-
notion of dependent [co-]origination. In this tive affect and craving, and he/she starts forming a
process, craving is said to result from a process behavior pattern related to these affective reactions.
14 Mindfulness and the Addictive Process: Psychological Models and Neurobiological Mechanisms 187

The perception of an object is influenced by scholarship (Hamilton, 2000). Indeed, nowhere


previous experiences, and the consolidation of in the early Buddhist dialogues is the Buddha
related memories leads to habits or dispositions reported as claiming that there is no self; on the
consequently updating how perception will contrary, both the view that there is no self and
function in the future (MIND). This cycle can the view that there is a self are said to lead to suf-
build on itself in another way as well: states fering (MN.2 in n.amoli & Bodhi, 1995). The
associated with craving and aversion are them- sense of self that is born in the process of depen-
selves unpleasant; individuals often develop dent co-arising, sketched above, is constituted by
aversive reactions towards their own craving habituated reactions of clinging to pleasant
and aversion. Fortunately, the iterative nature of aspects of experience and to the absence of the
this cycle also means that it can be disrupted at unpleasant. In a moment of desperate craving for
each new round. something, it seems as if appeasing this particular
The central point of this model is that craving sense desire will bring all that one needs, even if
and aversion arise in response to an affective tone one knows better. But this is a distorted percep-
that is associated with perceptual representations tion; sense pleasures are fleeting, and incapable
of a sensory object, rather than directly in of resolving the core distress that fuels the cycle
response to the object (Grabovac, Lau, & Willett, of searching for gratification. As one discourse
2011). This provides a critical entry point for puts it, Indeed, I have long been tricked, cheated,
therapeutic interventions: through paying careful and defrauded by this mind. For when clinging, I
attention to ones own experience, the Buddhist have been clinging just to material form feel-
accounts claim that one can see that perceptions ing perception formations, consciousness
and associated affective reactions (affective tone) With my clinging as condition, being birth
are separate fromand indeed separable from ageing and death, sorrow, lamentation, pain,
craving and aversion, as well as the elaborative grief, and despair come to be. Such is the origin
thought processes these can initiate. As one of this whole mass of suffering (MN.75 in
Buddhist scholar puts it, through paying mindful n.amoli & Bodhi, 1995). The idea here is that
attention to affective reactions, one distinctly by clinging to any kind of experience, a sense of
realizes that a pleasant feeling is not identical self is born. This sense of self is very basic, being
with lust and need not be followed by it By dependent only on grasping after the objects of
doing so, he makes a definite start in cutting experience. It does not depend on clinging to an
through the chain of dependent origination at that explicit self-identity. Thus, even if one does not
decisive point where feeling becomes the condi- ruminate about ones self-identity, as long as
tion for craving It will thus become the medi- there is craving for any aspect of experience, this
tators indubitable experience that the causal affectively constructed sense of self continues.
sequence of feeling and craving is not a neces- When the sense of self is threatened, by the
sary one (Nyanaponika, 2000). It should also inability to prevent the loss of what is grasped
be noted that even when craving has already after or to prevent the presence of what is pushed
arisen, mindful awareness can prevent further away, then one suffers.
cycles of aversive reaction to the unpleasant feel- We postulate that mindfulness does not pre-
ings associated with this craving, and thus reduce vent the cognitive construction of self-identity
habitual reactions that arise in an attempt to necessary for functioning in the world, but
escape this unpleasantness. instead targets previously developed affective
Importantly, craving is the link that is targeted biases (Elliott, Zahn, Deakin, & Anderson,
here in cutting through the cycle of dependent 2011). Such affective biases prevent individuals
origination. Some traditional accounts take medi- from accurately assessing what is happening in
tation practice to be aimed at the realization that the present moment, and acting accordingly.
there is no self. However, this interpretation has Mindfulness functions to decouple pleasant and
been controversial in contemporary Buddhist unpleasant experience from habitual reactions of
188 J.A. Brewer et al.

craving and aversion. By overcoming affective she gets stressed out. At times, she may even find
biases of attention and memory, mindfulness herself at a bar before waking up to the fact that
allows individuals to feel and know more clearly something triggered her to habitually drive there
the pain of perpetuating emotional craving and and order a drink.
aversion. As we suggest below in more detail, Given that ones self-identity is largely based
being fully present with the pain of this emo- on memory, the Buddhist description of depen-
tional reactivity may be sufficient to motivate dent origination is remarkably similar to the con-
individuals not to perpetuate it. In other words, temporary model of the addictive loop. When
mindfulness does not stop one from being a per- Ethel, who has learned to associate drinking with
son, but rather from taking things personally. the reduction of stress and/or the temporary
From this perspective, mindfulness allows abatement of withdrawal (#6), encounters a
practitioners to clearly ascertain what is driving stressful situation or alcohol withdrawal symp-
their behavior, and whether or not it is moving toms such as irritability, restlessness, and agita-
them towards or away from their goals. To illus- tion (#1), her brain interprets these as unpleasant
trate this point, we will use a fictional character, (#2). She wants the unpleasant feeling to go
Ethel Knoll-Kraver throughout this chapter (see away, and consequently gets a craving to drink
Text Box). For example, mindfulness might (#3). When she drinks, she reinforces the habitu-
enable Ethel Knoll-Kraver to see clearly that ated reaction to affective experience (e.g., if I
each time she drinks in reaction to being stressed, drink, I will feel better; #46).
she only briefly avoids the stress. By seeing that While Ethel might take this personally, hav-
drug use only provides a minimal amount of ing thoughts such as I am drinker, it is not
relief, and does not address whatever led to her these particular self-related thoughts but rather
stress in the first place, she can work to fix its root the affective bias underlying the reaction of tak-
cause. By better understanding her context and ing things personally that fuels the birth of self-
the factors that have previously contributed to her identity (i.e., habituated reactions to affective
drug use, Ethel may also become more disen- experience). As the state of satisfaction from
chanted with drinking by simply seeing more feeding the craving is short-lived, the passing
clearly its effects. Ethel may know the health away of this mind state inevitably ensues, lead-
risks and financial costs of drinking but fail to ing to dissatisfaction, stress, or suffering once
give sufficient weight to these facts in her deci- again. Importantly, each time Ethel drinks, she
sions about her behavior. By attenuating emo- re-engages and reinforces this loop, resulting in
tional distortions in the decision-making process, subsequent rounds of this process (#7). Buddhist
mindfulness may function to enable Ethel to texts call this repetitive process samsara, or
weigh these factors more accurately. endless wandering, as there is no obvious way
Young Ethel Knoll-Kraver is invited to have a out of it when propagated. Ethel may even begin
drink by a group of older kids who are popular at to ruminate about drinking and start planning
school (see #1 positive cue in Fig. 14.1a). She her day around access to alcohol, which, as we
learns to associate drinking with being cool will see later, likely engages brain circuits
when shes at a party drinking with friends, she involved in self-referential processing, thus fur-
feels good (#2). Over time, she also learns that ther fueling this process. Our modern-day
having a drink after work calms her nerves (#26). equivalent of the endless wandering character-
When Ethel gets yelled at by her boss, or gets a ized by Buddhism appears remarkably similar:
bad grade in school (#1 negative cue), she the addictive loop. However, the psychological
feels stressed out (#2), gets a craving (#3), and terms and links employed in dependent origina-
stops by a bar on the way home for a drink (#4). tion will need careful refinement and empirical
The more Ethel drinks in these situations, the validation to determine their relative explana-
more she reinforces her behavior (#57), and tory and predictive power in contemporary
the more she finds herself licking her lips when models of addiction.
14 Mindfulness and the Addictive Process: Psychological Models and Neurobiological Mechanisms 189

The Birth of an Addiction human animal and human studies, suggesting an


evolutionarily conserved process. Second, it pro-
Acquisition of an addictive behavior is a complex vides some explanatory power for the relative
process based on operant conditioning: pairing strengths and weaknesses of current treatment
action with reward, which leads to modified paradigms. Third, its self-propagating nature
behavior. This process has been observed for aligns surprisingly well with Buddhist psychologi-
over a century in animals ranging from the cal models as described above regarding the causes
simple sea slug (Aplysia) to humans. Addictions of human suffering: craving and attachment.
are developed in part from the formation of asso-
ciative memories between behavior and both
positive and negative affective states (see Text The Promise of Mindfulness-Based
Box). Subsequently, cues that are judged to be Cessation Treatments
positive or negative can induce positive or nega-
tive affective states, which can then trigger The multitude of cues that can be associated with
craving to repeat this behavior (Baker, Piper, positive and negative affective states, along with
McCarthy, Majeskie, & Fiore, 2004). Additionally, drug use, creates tremendous challenges for
neutral cues that have been classically condi- successful treatment and long-term abstinence.
tioned may directly trigger craving (Lazev, Mainstay behavioral treatments have focused on
Herzog, & Brandon, 1999). Craving then leads to teaching individuals to avoid cues, foster positive
repeated behavior, whether drinking alcohol, affective states (e.g., practice relaxation or
smoking cigarettes, or eating food and results in physical exercise), divert attention from crav-
the maintenance of positive or reduction of nega- ings, substitute other activities, and develop
tive affective states (Baker et al., 2004). social support mechanisms (Fiore et al., 2008).
This process sets up positive or negative rein- Unfortunately, these have shown only modest
forcement loops, respectively, by reinforcing the success, with abstinence rates for cognitively
associative memories between these affective based treatments hovering between 20 and 30 %
states and behavior. This associative learning (Law & Tang, 1995). This may be because trig-
process may then lead to increased motivational gers are omnipresent. The omnipresence of cues
salience of future cues (in which both positive makes avoidance difficult; diversion of attention
and negative cues become more motivationally requires cognitive reserves, which are often
relevant) (Robinson & Berridge, 2008), resulting depleted after strong affective states (Muraven &
in what, building on the work of Baker, Curtin, Baumeister, 2000), and effective substitutions are
and others (Baker et al., 2004; Curtin, McCarthy, not always available. Further, these strategies
Piper, & Baker, 2006), for convenience we term may not actually target the core addictive loop
the addictive loop. Through repeated sub- (e.g., avoidance of cues decreases input to the
stance use, this loop may become habitual, lead- loop), while substitute behaviors (e.g., eating
ing to cue-induced behavior that is largely carrot sticks or candy) circumvent the loop.
outside of consciousness, let alone conscious Importantly, these strategies, at least in theory,
control (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999; Curtin et al., may not diminish the loop itself, instead leaving
2006). So strong is this cue-induced habitual it dormant to reactivate at a later time. One
behavior, that the longer an individual remains recent finding even suggests that cue-induced
abstinent from a drug of abuse, the stronger his craving may increase with length of abstinence,
or her craving for the drug becomes, making suggesting that avoiding cues or substituting
relapse more likely, and termination of the cycle behaviors may do little to target core processes
less likely (Bedi et al., 2011). fueling addictive behaviors (Bedi et al., 2011).
This addictive loop model is noteworthy for Even cue exposure that aims to decrease the con-
several reasons. First, each link in the chain is ditioned responses may not adequately disrupt
supported by convergent findings from both non- the addictive loop, instead leading to different
190 J.A. Brewer et al.

associations that are also situation specific just observe them from moment to moment. Even
(Bouton, Westbrook, Corcoran, & Maren, 2006). judgment of the craving becomes an object itself,
The experimental evidence for the core links of instead of a driving force for subsequent behav-
the addictive loop and the modest long-term effi- ior. As such, mindfulness training may specifi-
cacy of current treatments provide compelling cally target the associative learning process with
evidence for the need for innovative treatments an emphasis on the critical link between affect
that directly dismantle this loop instead of treat- and craving in the addictive loop (Nyanaponika,
ing around it (Niaura & Abrams, 2002). 2000). Through changing ones relationship to
Remarkably, the early Buddhist model of suffer- craving, via nonjudgmental awareness, one
ing does both, and the clinical therapeutic inter- begins to remove the fuel from its fire, such that
ventions it has inspired have gained increasing over time, craving and its resultant identity for-
support from recent studies. mations eventually burn out or die off.
Derived from Buddhist practices, mindfulness Mindfulness training has been incorporated
training has been adapted for use in Western cul- into several approaches for addiction treatment,
tures, taking forms such as Mindfulness-based such as Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
Stress Reduction (MBSR), Mindfulness-based (ACT) and MBRP (Bowen et al., 2009; Brewer
Cognitive Therapy (MBCT; combined with et al., 2009), and has shown preliminary success
Cognitive Therapy for depression relapse preven- therein. For example, Gifford et al. (2004) ran-
tion), and Mindfulness-based Relapse Prevention domized 76 participants to nicotine replacement
(MBRP; combined with Relapse Prevention for or ACT (seven individual + seven group sessions)
addiction treatment). Typical treatments are 8 and found 24-h abstinence of 33 % and 35 %,
weeks in duration, though alternate lengths have respectively, after treatment and 15 and 35 % at
been employed for targeted uses (Brewer, Mallik 1-year follow-up. Because mindfulness training
et al., 2011). Common features of these treat- has the advantages of teaching just a few basic
ments include the training of attention to detect techniques (awareness) that target the addictive
and modify an individuals relationship to auto- loop process, aiming both at reducing automatic-
matic thought patterns, among others. For a ity and interrupting the loop, it requires fewer and
review see Hlzel et al. (2011). less specialized sessions than other treatments.
Mindfulness trainings effectiveness has been Theoretically, this simpler, more focused
investigated for the treatment of a number of dis- approach may facilitate both conceptual and
orders including pain, anxiety, and depression behavioral skills mastery and durability of effects
among others (reviewed in Baer, 2003; Grossman, in a relatively brief treatment. Studies on the effi-
Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004; Hofmann, cacy of mindfulness training for addictions
Sawyer, Witt, & Oh, 2010). These data are prom- remain preliminary: a recent review of trials that
ising, although more confirmatory studies are included mindfulness training reported that only
needed, as many of the studies were small and/or 1 of 22 included a randomized experimental
employed wait-list or other suboptimal control design (Zgierska et al., 2009). Importantly, a
conditions. number of these studies showed no significant
Mindfulness training has only recently been differences between the mindfulness and com-
evaluated in the treatment of addictions (Bowen parison conditions. However, subsequent ran-
et al., 2009; Brewer et al., 2009; Zgierska et al., domized trials have shown some promise. For
2008). It has been operationalized to include two example, in a small pilot study of cocaine and
distinct components: (1) maintaining attention on alcohol dependence, Brewer et al. (2009) found
the immediate experience and (2) maintaining an equivalent efficacy of mindfulness training to
attitude of acceptance towards this experience that of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT, which
(Bishop et al., 2004). Here, for example, Ethel is considered a gold standard treatment for
Knoll-Kraver might bring mindful awareness to addictions) during an 8-week treatment period.
the body sensations that constitute a craving, and Importantly, in this study, participants that
14 Mindfulness and the Addictive Process: Psychological Models and Neurobiological Mechanisms 191

received mindfulness training also showed adap- become more aware of habit-linked, minimally
tive psychological and autonomic changes during conscious affective states and bodily sensations
a laboratory-based stress challenge that werent (e.g., low-level craving), thus de-automating
observed in the CBT group at the end of treat- this largely habitual process (Brewer, Bowen,
ment. This suggests that mindfulness may help to Smith, Marlatt, & Potenza, 2010). One recent
target core addictive loop features, such as nega- study showed that MT alters the way that the
tive affective states. Further, in a larger trial, brain processes interoceptive cues, showing
Bowen et al. (2009) found significantly lower greater activity in regions (anterior insula) asso-
rates of substance use up to 4 months post- ciated with integration of internal bodily sensa-
intervention in individuals receiving MBRP com- tions with external conditions or cues (Farb,
pared to those receiving treatment as usual. Segal, & Anderson, 2013). Another study showed
However, these studies should be interpreted cau- that MT, relative to a relaxation control group,
tiously, as MT has not yet been rigorously com- not only decreased emotional interference on a
pared to empirically based treatments in cognitive processing task, but also led to signifi-
large-scale head-to-head trials, and indeed may cant changes in a psychophysiological measure
not be more efficacious for these conditions than of arousal while viewing pleasant and unpleasant
standard treatment (Zgierska et al., 2009). images (Ortner, Kilner, & Zelazo, 2007). Together
With regard to smoking, mindfulness training these findings may suggest that MT leads not
has shown preliminary utility in reducing ciga- only to greater emotional stability at a physiolog-
rette cravings and withdrawal symptoms (Cropley, ical level, but also that this emotional stability is
Ussher, & Charitou, 2007), as well as in smoking paired with better neural monitoring of the body
cessation (Davis, Fleming, Bonus, & Baker, and association of its states with the external
2007). Bowen et al. (2009) provided college stu- environment, in essence, helping individuals to
dents with brief mindfulness-based instructions see things as they are.
and found that they smoked significantly fewer By decoupling pleasant and unpleasant expe-
cigarettes 1 week after the intervention compared rience from the habitual reactions of craving and
to those that did not receive instructions. Also, in aversion, careful attention to present moment
an uncontrolled trial, Davis et al. (2007) found experience can function to bring a broadening or
that 10 of 18 patients showed abstinence 6 weeks spaciousness of awareness that allows new
post-quit after receiving MBSR. More recently, appraisals of life situations. A possible result of
Brewer et al. (2011a) randomized 88 subjects to this is the ability of mindfulness to specifically
receive mindfulness training or the American facilitate positive reappraisal. For instance,
Lung Associations Freedom From Smoking Garland, Gaylord, and Fredrickson (2011) have
treatment. They found significant differences in given the example of mindfulness allowing indi-
number of cigarettes smoked as well as abstinence viduals reappraisal of a serious heart condition
rates 4 months after treatment completion (31 vs. as an opportunity to change their lifestyle and
6 % at 4 months, p = 0.01). health behaviors rather than as a catastrophe por-
tending imminent doom. Other empirical find-
ings do not support this idea. Ortner et al. (2007)
Mindfulness Training May Directly found that decreases in arousal to negative images
Target the Addictive Loop were common to both MT and relaxation training
groups, but decreases in arousal to positive
Effective implementation of mindfulness training images were unique to MT. Interestingly, tradi-
may, over time, lead to the dampening and eventual tional presentations also do not support a concep-
dismantling of the addictive loop that perpetuates tion of mindfulness as biasing subjects especially
smoking or drug use rather than just removing towards positive appraisal of life situations.
stimuli that might propagate it. For example, Rather, as Garland, Gaylord, and Park (2009)
through its attentional focus, individuals learn to acknowledge, mindfulness may function by
192 J.A. Brewer et al.

attenuating emotional distortions of stimuli per- actually painful to touch, they acquire a mistaken
ception by encouraging non-evaluative contact perception of them as pleasant (MN.75 in n.
with phenomenological experience, leading to amoli & Bodhi, 1995).
more clearly seeing things as they are. This Mindfulness counteracts both not knowing
point deserves emphasis. Explicit techniques for (lack of awareness) and knowing wrongly
positive reappraisal are taught both in contempo- (misperception), indicating an important role in
rary clinical settings and also in holistic tradi- developing what the texts call knowledge and
tional approaches to ending suffering. For vision of things as they are. The role of mindful-
example, Theravada Buddhist teachings include ness in increasing individuals ability to register
cultivation of loving-kindness (metta) as well as and report on their emotional reactions in con-
other positive or wholesome mind states such as ceptual terms may derive from a more fundamen-
appreciation/sympathetic joy at the joy of others tal function of cultivating and broadening a kind
(mudita). In traditional presentations, however, of rich, experiential feeling of emotional states
these practices are clearly delineated from the within ourselves (Davis & Thompson, 2012).
practice of mindfulness (satipatthana), which Knowing cognitively that continued substance
involves attenuating both desire and discontent in use only perpetuates ones and others suffering
regard to external objects (MN.10 in n.amoli & is often not sufficient. In their seminal article
Bodhi, 1995). On this construal, the application examining behavioral change, Prochaska,
of mindfulness in Ethel Knoll-Kravers case (see DiClemente, and Norcross (1992) stated, The
Text Box) may not result in positive appraisal, progression from contemplation to action is
but will allow her to be more aware of the various essential to beneficial outcome . Instead of
features of craving as they actually are. merely developing awareness or contemplating
As mentioned above, mindfulness may help in the problem, individuals need to feel the pain of
dispelling emotional distortions based on posi- this cycle directly, in a rich, experiential, prever-
tive as well as negative affective biases (Brewer, bal way. In the words of Bob Marley, who feels
Davis, & Goldstein, 2012), which keep us from it knows it. When one is fully and accurately
paying careful attention to the painful aspects of aware of the pain involved in the search for grati-
grasping and distorting our memory of them even fication, one does not have to positively reappraise
after we act in unskillful ways. For example, the situation or suppress emotional reactions in
women who are distracted by emotionally driven, other ways. Rather, by feeling and knowing what
self-evaluative thoughts have been shown to be brings pain, individuals can counteract motivated
much slower in registering bodily reactions to reasoning and other unconscious strategies to
emotionally charged images, an effect that is appease their craving. There is already some evi-
reversed by meditation training (Silverstein, dence that training in meditation increases this
Brown, Roth, & Britton, 2011). The textual type of subtle awareness of bodily states and their
account of the Buddhas spiritual journey high- integration with thoughts, beliefs, feelings, etc.
lights these types of cognitive distortions in com- (Farb et al., 2013). In this way, Ethel Knoll-
paring his pre-awakening times of mistaking Kraver may come to see directly for herself what
stress for happiness: In the past sensual plea- she actually gets from feeding her cravingsa
sures were painful to touch, hot, and scorching; relief that is temporary, and is motivated by a
in the future sensual pleasures will be painful to pain and destructive type of emotional reactivity.
touch, hot, and scorching; and now at present By teaching individuals to simply observe
sensual pleasures are painful to the touch, hot, aversive body and mind states (e.g., negative
and scorching. But these beings who are not free affect) rather than reacting to them, mindful-
from lust for sensual pleasures, who are devoured ness training may foster the replacement of
by craving for sensual pleasures, who burn with stress- and affect-induced, habitual reactions
fever for sensual pleasures, have faculties that are with more adaptive responses (e.g., enhanced
impaired; thus, though sensual pleasures are self-control and regulation; Curtin et al., 2006).
14 Mindfulness and the Addictive Process: Psychological Models and Neurobiological Mechanisms 193

Additionally, mindfulness training may help of these physical sensations. Each time they ride
individuals change their relationships to nega- out a cravingexperiencing its physicality with-
tive affective or physically unpleasant states out acting on itthis reinforces their insight that
and thoughts (i.e., to not take them personally). cravings will subside on their own, even if not
To be clear, we postulate that the mechanism of satisfied. In theory, this allows individuals to
action here is the attenuation of affective bias learn how to tolerate the physical sensations
underlying the reaction of taking things person- without acting on them. Cravings may continue
ally, rather than a change in self-related thoughts to arise, but learning to sit with urges, to pause
or cognitive attributions. As noted above, it is the and not immediately react, may disrupt the asso-
habitual affective bias underlying emotional ciative learning process and the automaticity of
reactivity that fuels further rounds of craving the action ordinarily taken. In other words, the
and habituation. Thus, with attenuation of this birth of an identity around an object (This is
affective bias, oxygen is slowly removed from uncomfortable for me, Id better go smoke a ciga-
the fire, ultimately leading to smoking cessation rette) is not fostered or fed. Or put another way,
(Bowen et al., 2009; Bowen & Marlatt, 2009; the fuel has not been added to the fire, such that
Brewer et al., 2010). However, studies that the fire burns out more quickly. If this is true, MT
directly test these hypotheses are needed. should affect the traditional observation that
smoking and craving are positively correlated. In
fact one might predict that it would decouple this
Is Craving an Important Target relationship.
of Mindfulness Training? A recent study suggests that this decoupling
may be true. In a follow-up to their MT for smok-
As stated above, mindfulness training may help ing cessation trial, Brewer and colleagues exam-
individuals sit with or ride out their cravings. ined the relationship between craving and
What is meant by this, and how does it fit with the smoking behavior during treatment (Elwafi,
theoretical underpinnings of mindfulness train- Witkiewitz, Mallik, Iv, & Brewer, 2012). At the
ing? First, craving is inherently unpleasant (con- start of MT, individuals showed a strong positive
sider the feeling of wanting a drink of water on an correlation between average daily cigarette use
extremely hot day), and naturally drives individu- and their self-reported craving for cigarettes, as
als to act, whether to smoke, drink, or use other measured by the Questionnaire on Smoking
drugs. The longer this craving goes unsatisfied, Urges (r = 0.58, p < 0.001). At the end of the
the more it may intensify as it becomes fueled by 4-week treatment period, this correlation was
further reactions to the unpleasantness of the reduced to the point of statistical nonsignificance
wanting itself. For example, in a study of (r = 0.13, p = 0.49). Importantly, individuals who
treatment-seeking smokers, for each standard quit smoking showed no difference in craving
deviation increase in craving scores on the target scores compared to those who continued to
quit date, the risk of lapsing rose by 43 % on that smoke at the end of treatment, but instead dem-
day, and 65 % on the following day (Ferguson, onstrated a delayed reduction in reported craving,
Shiffman, & Gwaltney, 2006). Mindfulness train- while those who did not quit reported an increase
ing teaches individuals to instead step back and in craving concomitant with increases in smok-
take a moment to explore what cravings actually ing. These results suggest that after just 4 weeks
feel like in their bodies, however uncomfortable of mindfulness training, individuals were no lon-
or unpleasant they may be. Two important ger reacting to their cravings by smoking. One
insights can be learned from this process. First, interpretation of this is that mindfulness training
individuals learn that cravings are physical sensa- may have decoupled the relationship between
tions in their bodies rather than moral impera- craving and smoking during treatment. In other
tives that must be acted upon. Second, they gain words, mindfulness practice may help individu-
first-hand experience of the impermanent nature als stop adding fuel to the fire (craving), but the
194 J.A. Brewer et al.

fire still continues to burn based on the fuel that is Neurobiological Mechanisms
already present (e.g., individuals still crave when of Mindfulness Training
they first quit). Importantly, over time, without
continued sustenance (smoking), the fire burns Brain regions that show commonality between a
out by itself. number of different maladies and importantly
The possibility of craving and smoking being have also been theoretically and functionally
decoupled by MT is further supported by the linked to mindfulness training may provide a
amount of home practice that subjects reported. logical starting point in assaying its neurobiolog-
Similar to previous studies of psychological ical mechanisms (for a more detailed review of
health and mindfulness training (Carmody & possible mechanisms, see Hlzel et al., 2011).
Baer, 2008), Brewer et al. (2011a) initially found The default mode network (DMN; a network of
that increased home practice was correlated with highly correlated brain regions that show coher-
decreased cigarette use for both formal (r = 0.44, ent activity during rest and relate to aspects of
p < 0.02) and informal practice (r = 0.48, self-referential processingsee Andrews-Hanna,
p < 0.01). In fact, the amount of mindfulness Reidler, Sepulcre, Poulin, & Buckner, 2010;
practice during treatment not only predicted Buckner, Andrews-Hanna, & Schacter, 2008;
smoking behavior at the end of treatment but Fox & Raichle, 2007) may be one of these tar-
moderated the relationship between craving and gets, given its primacy in a number of psychiatric
smoking as well: the more that individuals prac- disorders ranging from anxiety to addiction
ticed during treatment, the less craving correlated (Buckner et al., 2008). There are two primary
with the number of cigarettes individuals smoked nodes of the DMN, the medial prefrontal cortex
at the end of treatment (Elwafi et al., 2012). and the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). These
The ability of mindfulness training to attenu- have been shown to be temporally correlated with
ate the relationship between craving and sub- a number of peripheral nodes, and anticorrelated
stance use has been observed in other studies as with brain regions involved in self-monitoring,
well. Witkiewitz and Bowen (2010) examined the and cognitive control (anterior insula; AI; dorsal
relationship between depression, craving, and anterior cingulate cortex, dACC; dorsolateral
substance use following a randomized clinical prefrontal cortex, dlPFC) (Andrews-Hanna et al.,
trial of MBRP. They found that craving mediated 2010). Though self-referential processing is a
the relationship between depressive symptoms complex area of investigation in itself, on a first
and substance use in the group that received approximation, this may be where models of
conventional treatment, but not in the group that self-identity formation at least partially overlap;
received MBRP. Taken together, these results memory retrieval and the self across time are
suggest that mindfulness training may indeed linked by PCC activity (Andrews-Hanna et al.,
help individuals develop a tolerance to craving 2010; Buckner et al., 2008). As the DMN has
itself, thus over time acting to dismantle the been shown to be altered by mindfulness training
addictive loop through a dis-identification (Brewer et al., 2011b), and given its documented
with the object (or dismantling of self-identity). contributions to mind-wandering and self-
The next logical steps will be to determine how referential processing, the DMN is a biologically
these map onto current psychological models of plausible target for mindfulness training as MT
change behavior. For example, do tolerance of teaches the inverse of mind-wandering and
craving and dismantling of self-identity equate self- referential processing. Of course, the exact
to reappraisal and extinction, respectively, or to patterns and functions of the brains resting
other skills, or constitute unique entities unto state networks (of which the DMN is one) should
themselves? be interpreted with some caution as there are
14 Mindfulness and the Addictive Process: Psychological Models and Neurobiological Mechanisms 195

limitations to our current analytic methods and with caution, as this study was cross-sectional,
we are only just beginning to understand the and could be influenced by self-selection bias.
various causal factors that lead to the observed As action-monitoring/prediction (e.g., dACC)
patterns (see, e.g., Fan et al., 2012). and cognitive control regions (e.g., dlPFC) have
With regard to the effects of mindfulness been shown to be important in self-control, addic-
training on the DMN, there is evidence of tions, and treatment outcomes (Brewer,
decreased DMN activity during mindful aware- Worhunsky, Carroll, Rounsaville, & Potenza,
ness of visually presented adjectives versus 2008), these findings suggest that MT may fun-
determination of personal meaning of adjectives, damentally alter brain activity and connectivity
following 8 weeks of MBSR (Farb et al., 2007). patterns in networks important for perpetuation
Taylor et al. (2011) similarly found deactivation of addictive behaviors. In essence, mindfulness
of DMN structures in meditators practicing a may help to bring together our capacity to moni-
mindful state while viewing emotionally evoc- tor our internal and external environments (AI/
ative pictures. Extending these, Brewer and col- dACC; see Farb et al., 2013), especially when
leagues found that in very experienced meditators craving or self-referential states arise (likely acti-
(>10,000 h of practice on average), DMN deacti- vation of DMN), and to utilize self-control (likely
vation was common to three different types of activation of dlPFC) when needed. Over time, as
meditation (concentration, loving-kindness, and monitoring strengthens and the processes of crav-
choiceless awareness) (Brewer, Worhunsky et al., ing weaken due to a lack of sustenance, effortful
2011). These findings may suggest that the success self-control may not be needed as much. In the-
of MT for addictions may work via a disengage- ory, the more Ethel develops her capacity to pay
ment from self-identified habitual response patterns. attention to her internal and external environ-
By mindfully attending to cravings, the DMN ment, the less she would fuel her habitual cop-
node activity and/or connectivity may be altered, ing strategies of drinking to deal with stress and
as seen above during meditation or the viewing of withdrawal states, leading to the cooling off of
evocative pictures. Over time, these circuits may her habituated affective self-identity and its even-
even change, as the habituated sense of self tual cessation. However, prospective studies of
around smoking fades due to lack of sustenance individuals receiving MT for addictions that
or fuel. measure changes in brain activity and connectiv-
Interestingly, Brewer and colleagues found an ity over time are needed to test such hypotheses.
increase in functional connectivity between the As we focused mainly on the DMN in this
PCC, and the dACC as well as the dlPFC in expe- chapter, studies assessing other possible brain
rienced meditators compared to controls. This is regions/networks that may emerge as prominent
important, because these regions are anticorre- players in the neural mechanisms of mindfulness
lated in the vast majority of contexts, and thus will also be important.
named the task-negative (DMN) and task-
positive (dACC, dlPFC, and others) networks,
respectively (Fox & Raichle, 2007). Controls Conclusions and Future Directions
showed typical anticorrelation patterns between
these structures at baseline, which decreased dur- Over the past century, much has been discovered
ing meditation, suggesting a state-dependent about the addictive process and its underlying
connectivity pattern in untrained individuals. neurobiology (Goldstein et al., 2009; Kalivas &
However, the observed increased connectivity Volkow, 2005). From these findings, psychologi-
patterns seen in experienced meditators were cal models have been put forward that have been
present both at baseline and during meditation, instrumental in the development of novel treat-
suggesting that they may have established a ments that directly target core components of this
new default mode of intrinsic brain activity and process. These models show remarkable similari-
connectivity. These findings should be interpreted ties to ancient Buddhist psychological models
196 J.A. Brewer et al.

aimed at describing the causes of human suffering. Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson,
N. D., Carmody, J., , Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness:
Modern treatments, such as MT that are based on
A proposed operational definition. Clinical
these Buddhist models, are beginning to show Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230241.
preliminary efficacy in the treatment of addic- Bouton, M. E., Westbrook, R. F., Corcoran, K. A., &
tions, and may be doing so through changing Maren, S. (2006). Contextual and temporal modula-
tion of extinction: Behavioral and biological mecha-
ones relationship to core addictive elements such
nisms. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 352360.
as craving. Recent neuroimaging studies are con- Bowen, S., Chawla, N., Collins, S. E., Witkiewitz, K.,
verging with these concepts, suggesting that MT Hsu, S., Grow, J., , Marlatt, A. (2009). Mindfulness-
can fundamentally change basic brain processes, based relapse prevention for substance use disorders: a
pilot efficacy trial. Substance Abuse, 30, 295305.
such as the brains spontaneous activation and
Bowen, S., & Marlatt, A. (2009). Surfing the urge: Brief
intrinsic connectivity patterns. These may mani- mindfulness-based intervention for college student
fest behaviorally, in that individuals may develop smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23,
new habits such as monitoring unskillful thought 666671.
Brewer, J. A., Bowen, S., Smith, J. T., Marlatt, G. A., &
processes and automatic behaviors, and objec-
Potenza, M. N. (2010). Mindfulness-based treatments
tively observing them rather than being sucked for co-occurring depression and substance use disor-
in by them and smoking, using other drugs, or ders: What can we learn from the brain? Addiction,
engaging in other unhealthy behaviors. Perhaps 105, 16981706.
Brewer, J. A., Davis, J. H., & Goldstein, J. (2012). Why is
people can even practice becoming interested and
it so hard to pay attention, or is it? Mindfulness, the
fascinated with the bodily sensations of craving, factors of awakening and reward-based learning.
thereby co-opting the very process that perpetu- Mindfulness, 16.
ates this addictive cycle to uproot it, leading to Brewer, J. A., Mallik, S., Babuscio, T. A., Nich, C.,
Johnson, H. E., Deleone, C. M., , Rounsaville, B. J.
the later dying away or cessation of craving itself.
(2011a). Mindfulness training for smoking cessation:
Ultimately, with practice, this may lead to more Results from a randomized controlled trial. Drug and
adaptive choices with concomitant decreases in Alcohol Dependence, 119, 7280.
stress and suffering. Brewer, J. A., Sinha, R., Chen, J. A., Michalsen, R. N.,
Babuscio, T. A., Nich, C., , Rounsaville, B. J. (2009).
Mindfulness training and stress reactivity in substance
Acknowledgements We would like to thank Joseph abuse: results from a randomized, controlled stage I
Goldstein and Pat Coffey for their input on mindfulness pilot study. Substance Abuse, 30, 306317.
training techniques and theory. Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Carroll, K. M.,
Rounsaville, B. J., & Potenza, M. N. (2008).
Pretreatment brain activation during Stroop task is
associated with outcomes in cocaine-dependent
References patients. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 9981004.
Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y. Y.,
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, Weber, J., & Kober, H. (2011b). Meditation experi-
R., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-anatomic ence is associated with differences in default mode
fractionation of the brain's default network. Neuron, network activity and connectivity. Proceedings of the
65, 550562. National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical America, 108, 2025420259.
intervention: A conceptual and empirical review. Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L.
Clininical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, (2008). The brain's default network: Anatomy, function,
125143. and relevance to disease. In A. Kingstone & M. B.
Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, Miller (Eds.), The year in cognitive neuroscience 2008
M. R., & Fiore, M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation (pp. 138). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
reformulated: An affective processing model of nega- Carmody, J., & Baer, R. (2008). Relationships between
tive reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111, 3351. mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medi-
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable cal and psychological symptoms and well-being in a
automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal
462479. of Behavioral Medicine, 31, 2333.
Bedi, G., Preston, K. L., Epstein, D. H., Heishman, S. J., Cropley, M., Ussher, M., & Charitou, E. (2007). Acute
Marrone, G. F., Shaham, Y., et al. (2011). Incubation effects of a guided relaxation routine (body scan) on
of cue-induced cigarette craving during abstinence in tobacco withdrawal symptoms and cravings in absti-
human smokers. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 708711. nent smokers. Addiction, 102, 989993.
14 Mindfulness and the Addictive Process: Psychological Models and Neurobiological Mechanisms 197

Curtin, J. J., McCarthy, D. E., Piper, M. E., & Baker, T. B. Goldstein, R. Z., Craig, A. D., Bechara, A., Garavan, H.,
(2006). Implicit and explicit drug motivational pro- Childress, A. R., Paulus, M. P., et al. (2009). The neu-
cesses: A model of boundary conditions. In R. W. rocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction.
Wiers & A. W. Stacy (Eds.), Handbook of implicit Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 372380.
cognition and addiction (pp. 233250). Thousand Grabovac, A., Lau, M., & Willett, B. (2011). Mechanisms
Oaks, CA: Sage. of mindfulness: A Buddhist psychological model.
Davis, J. M., Fleming, M. F., Bonus, K. A., & Baker, T. B. Mindfulness, 2, 154166.
(2007). A pilot study on mindfulness based stress Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H.
reduction for smokers. BMC Complementary and (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health
Alternative Medicine, 7, 2. benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic
Davis, J. H., & Thompson, E. (2012). From the five aggre- Research, 57, 3543.
gates to phenomenal consciousness: Toward a cross- Hamilton, S. (2000). Early Buddhism: A new approach:
cultural cognitive science. In S. Emmanual (Ed.), A The I of the beholder (Vol. 16). London, UK:
companion to Buddhist philosophy. New York, NY: Routledge-Curzon.
Wiley-Blackwell. Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D.
Elliott, R., Zahn, R., Deakin, J. F., & Anderson, I. M. (2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on
(2011). Affective cognition and its disruption in mood anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal
disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 153182. of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 169183.
Elwafi, H. M., Witkiewitz, K., Mallik, S., Iv, T. A., & Hlzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z.,
Brewer, J. A. (2012). Mindfulness training for smok- Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness
ing cessation: Moderation of the relationship between meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action
craving and cigarette use. Drug and Alcohol from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives
Dependence, 130(13), 222229. on Psychological Science, 6, 537559.
Fan, J., Xu, P., Van Dam, N. T., Eilam, T., Gu, X., Luo, Y., Kalivas, P. W., & Volkow, N. D. (2005). The neural basis
et al. (2012). Spontaneous brain fluctuations relate to of addiction: A pathology of motivation and choice.
autonomic arousal. Journal of Neurosicence, 15, American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 14031413.
1117611186. Law, M., & Tang, J. L. (1995). An analysis of the effective-
Farb, N. A., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, ness of interventions intended to help people stop smok-
D., Fatima, Z., et al. (2007). Attending to the present: ing. Archives of Internal Medicine, 155, 19331941.
Mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes Lazev, A. B., Herzog, T. A., & Brandon, T. H. (1999).
of self-reference. Social, Cognitive, and Affective Classical conditions of environmental cues to cigarette
Neuroscience, 2, 313322. smoking.ExperimentalandClinicalPsychopharmacology,
Farb, N. A., Segal, Z. V., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). 7, 5663.
Mindfulness meditation training alters cortical repre- Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation
sentations of interoceptive attention. Social, Cognitive, and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control
and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 1526. resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126,
Ferguson, S. G., Shiffman, S., & Gwaltney, C. J. (2006). 247259.
Does reducing withdrawal severity mediate nicotine n.amoli, B., & Bodhi, B. (Eds.). (1995). The middle
patch efficacy? A randomized clinical trial. Journal of length discourses of the Buddha: A translation of the
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 11531161. Majjhima Nikya. Boston, MA: Wisdom Publications.
Fiore, M. C., Jaen, C. R., Baker, T. B., Bailey, W. C., Niaura, R., & Abrams, D. B. (2002). Smoking cessation:
Bennowitz, N. L., Curry, S. J., , Wewers, M. E. Progress, priorities, and prospectus. Journal of
(2008). Treating tobacco use and dependence: 2008 Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 494509.
Update. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health Nyanaponika, T. (2000). The vision of Dhamma: Buddhist
and Human Services. writings of Nyanponika Thera (2nd ed.). Seattle, WA:
Fox, M. D., & Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluc- Buddhist Publication Society.
tuations in brain activity observed with functional Office of National Drug Control Policy. (2004). The eco-
magnetic resonance imaging. Nature Reviews nomic costs of drug abuse in the United States.
Neuroscience, 8, 700711. Retrieved July 4, 2012, from http://www.whitehouse.
Garland, E., Gaylord, S., & Fredrickson, B. (2011). gov/ondcp
Positive reappraisal mediates the stress-reductive Ortner, C. N. M., Kilner, S. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007).
effects of mindfulness: An upward spiral process. Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional inter-
Mindfulness, 2, 5967. ference on a cognitive task. Motivation and Emotion,
Garland, E., Gaylord, S., & Park, J. (2009). The role of 31, 271283.
mindfulness in positive reappraisal. EXPLORE: The Prochaska, J. O., DiClemente, C. C., & Norcross, J. C.
Journal of Science and Healing, 5, 3744. (1992). In search of how people change. Applications
Gifford, E. V., Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., to addictive behaviors. American Psychologist, 47,
Antonuccio, D. O., Piasecki, M. M., Rasmussen-Hall, 11021114.
M. L., et al. (2004). Acceptance-based treatment for Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2008). The incentive
smoking cessation. Behavior Therapy, 35, 689705. sensitization theory of addiction: Some current issues.
198 J.A. Brewer et al.

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, (SN 12.2). Retrieved February 12, 2013, from http://
B: Biological Sciences, 363, 31373146. www.accesstoinsight.org/tipitaka/sn/sn12/sn12.002.
Silverstein, R. G., Brown, A. H., Roth, H. D., & Britton, than.html.
W. B. (2011). Effects of mindfulness training on body Tiffany, S. T., & Wray, J. M. (2012). The clinical signifi-
awareness to sexual stimuli: Implications for female cance of drug craving. Annals of the New York
sexual dysfunction. Psychosomatic Medicine, 73, Academy of Sciences, 1248, 117.
817825. Witkiewitz, K., & Bowen, S. (2010). Depression, craving,
Taylor, V. A., Grant, J., Daneault, V., Scavone, G., Breton, and substance use following a randomized trial of
E., Roffe-Vidal, S., , Beauregard, M. (2011). Impact mindfulness-based relapse prevention. Journal of
of mindfulness on the neural responses to emotional Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 362374.
pictures in experienced and beginner meditators. Zgierska, A., Rabago, D., Chawla, N., Kushner, K.,
Neuroimage, 57, 15241533. Koehler, R., & Marlatt, A. (2009). Mindfulness medi-
Thanissaro, B. (trans.) (2010a). Dhammacakkappavattana tation for substance use disorders: A systematic
Sutta: Setting in motion the wheel of truth (SN 56.11). review. Substance Abuse, 30, 266294.
Retrieved October 7, 2011, from http://www.accessto- Zgierska, A., Rabago, D., Zuelsdorff, M., Coe, C., Miller,
insight.org/tipitaka/sn/sn56/sn56.011.than.html. M., & Fleming, M. (2008). Mindfulness meditation
Thanissaro, B. (trans.) (2010b). Paticca-samuppada- for alcohol relapse prevention: A feasibility pilot
vibhanga Sutta: Analysis of dependent co-arising study. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 2, 165173.
Mindfulness, Eating Disorders,
and Food Intake Regulation 15
Jean L. Kristeller

or fluids for only a few hours before it becomes


Introduction difficult to function effectively. Yet people overeat
or undereat regularly for extended periods of
Most, if not all of us, eat mindlessly at times. We time, overriding feedback signals of physical
may overeat at a meal, choose foods without hunger and satiety, and fluctuating tremendously
reflection, too often eat to handle stress, or more in the nutritional value of their food intake.
painfully develop an eating disorder. Cultivating Overriding internal signals may be more exag-
mindfulness to create more balanced eating may gerated in individuals with eating disorders but
therefore have widespread application, both for also occurs in individuals without any eating-
disorders such as binge eating or bulimia, or to related problems. Such variation was protective
help people create more balanced eating for other from an evolutionary perspective, but with ready
reasons, including better health and a decreased access to highly palatable food has become prob-
sense of struggle. lematic (King, 2013).
Eating engages complex self-regulatory pro- The process of eating also involves virtually
cesses, most fundamentally responding to needs all the other systems that are being explored in
for food energy, nutrition, and physical satiety regard to the impact of mindfulness on self-
processes. Such basic needs are also comple- regulation, usefully understood from a multi-
mented by many other uses of foodto soothe domain model of meditation effects (Kristeller,
and comfort, to celebrate, to socialize, or to 2003, 2007). Eating is inherently a behavioral
obtain pleasure. Maintaining a balance between process, but it also involves emotions, thought
physical needs for food and the psychological processes, self-judgment and self-identity, and
value of eating often occurs flexibly and without social relationships. The meaning of food may
much awareness. Among the physiological sys- also engage spiritual experience, as beautifully
tems in the body that are necessary for survival, addressed in several books on mindful eating
eating is perhaps unique in its flexible nature. In (e.g., Altman, 2002; Bays, 2009; Hanh & Cheung,
contrast, it is possible to override needs for sleep 2010; Kabatznick, 1998). This chapter explores a
range of mindfulness practices both in regard to
the clinical development and research evidence
for the Mindfulness-Based Eating Awareness
Training (MB-EAT) program (Kristeller, 2016;
J.L. Kristeller (*)
Kristeller & wolever, 2011; Kristeller, Wolever,
Department of Psychology, Indiana State University,
Terre Haute, IN 47809, USA & Sheets, 2013; Kristeller & Wolever, 2016), and
e-mail: jkristeller@indstate.edu in regard to broader theoretical issues. The clinical
Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 199
B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_15
200 J.L. Kristeller

focus will extend from milder levels of dysregu- tion to body signals that are generally available to
lated eating, experienced at times by most individ- awareness is clearly parallel.
uals, to the extreme degree of dysfunction observed Self-regulation theory further posits that
in those with binge eating disorder (BED). even complex systems can be maintained and
re-regulated with relatively little sense of effort
or struggle (Carver & Scheier, 1982, 1998) if
Self-Regulation Theory, Mindful appropriate feedback mechanisms are engaged.
Eating, and Meditation This is in contrast to willpower models that
assume the need for ongoing vigilance for
From its inception, MB-EAT was informed by extended periods of time, or in the case of some
self-regulation theory, linked with early models addiction models, indefinitely. Within the weight
of food intake regulation that acknowledged the regulation area, willpower or abstinence-based
complex interplay of psychological and physio- models abound; while some individuals internal-
logical control processes (Rodin, 1978, 1981; ize recommended constraints, many individuals
Schachter, 1971; Schachter & Rodin, 1974). rebound in regard both to behavior and to weight
Early psychobiological formulations of self- regain. Although self-control models related to
regulation theory (Schwartz, 1975; Shapiro & weight management (e.g., Brownell, 2000;
Schwartz, 2000) focused on the importance of Cooper, Fairburn, & Hawker, 2004) have contrib-
interoceptive awareness, particularly of physio- uted substantially to understanding how individ-
logical processes, as a core element of internal uals can recondition long-standing eating
regulatory processes. These perspectives on patterns, they have tended to focus on managing
self-regulation expanded on early theories of externally or emotionally triggered impulses to
physiological homeostasis (e.g., Cannon, 1932) eat, rather than engaging use of internal feedback
and systems theory (e.g., Bertalanffy, 1968; systems. One notable exception to this is Linda
Miller, 1978). Craigheads Appetite Awareness model (Brown,
A hallmark expression of early self-regulation Smith, & Craighead, 2010; Craighead, 2006) in
theory were the biofeedback-based therapies which a core part of treatment involves cultivat-
(Schwartz, 1975; Patel, 1977). Biofeedback is ing attention to hunger and satiety signals.
effective because it facilitates interoceptive atten-
tion and awareness to assist individuals to man-
age stress-related physical symptoms. The link Regulation and Dysregulation
was then made to parallel aspects of meditation- of Food Intake
based relaxation training that also facilitates
psychobiological self-regulation by cultivating The importance of interplay between external or
processes of attention and awareness (Cuthbert, non-nutritive triggers for eating vs. internal sig-
Kristeller, Simons, Hodes, & Lang, 1981; nals related to physiological feedback systems
Davidson, Goleman, & Schwartz, 1976; has long been recognized within experimental
Kristeller, 1977; Schwartz, 1975). Although and social psychology (Schachter, 1971;
meditation practice has not been shown to be Schachter & Rodin, 1974). Decades of research
adequate to facilitate awareness of very subtle on physiological hunger and satiety mechanisms
signals, such as heart rate (Khalsa et al., 2008), indicate that such signals are easily overridden by
meditators have been shown to better track respi- non-nutritive influences, including both psycho-
ratory processes, a system more available to logical and environmental triggers (Capaldi,
interoceptive awareness (Daubenmier, Sze, Kerr, 1996). It is important to emphasize that this com-
Kemeny, & Mehling, 2013). Even though there plex interplay is part of normal food intake regu-
are no easily applied external monitors, such as lation. Wansink (2007), in his work on mindless
electrodes, to magnify subtle body signals of eating, has found that individuals make over 200
hunger and satiety, the self-regulatory value of decisions per day about food, including when to
using mindfulness techniques to cultivate atten- eat, what to eat, choices within a given meal, and
15 Mindfulness Eating Disorders 201

decisions to stop eating, increasing to about 300 larger amounts of food tolerate higher levels of
decisions for those with significant weight stomach fullness and discomfort (Geliebter &
problems (Wansink & Sobal, 2007). Given that Hashim, 2001; Geliebter, Hassid, & Hashim,
complexity, cultivating self-regulatory balance in 2001), both as a result of expanded stomach capac-
a flexible rather than constrained manner may be ity and a failure to attend to distention signals as
particularly desirable. indicators for stopping eating. By definition, indi-
The elements involved in physiological regula- viduals with BED regularly eat very large quanti-
tion of food intake and weight are surprisingly ties of food, well beyond normal feedback signals
complex. Those most available to interoceptive for satiety (Sysko, Devlin, Walsh, Zimmerli, &
awareness include hunger signals, such as low Kissileff, 2007). Individuals with BED and/or who
blood sugar and stomach growling, taste sensitiv- are heavier often note that they dont stop eating
ity, and signals to terminate eating a particular until the food is gone, or when they feel too full
food (sensory-specific satiety) or an entire meal to eat anymore. They virtually never mention
(fullness, rise in blood sugar) (Capaldi, 1996; awareness of more rapidly occurring feedback sig-
Ogden, 2010). Regardless of weight, people vary nals, such as no longer tasting or enjoying the food
considerably in their underlying patterns as much; early shifts in blood sugar that relate to a
(Drewnowski, 1996; Kristeller & Rodin, 1989) in sense of satiety; or moderate, rather than extreme,
regard to whether they initiate eating without feelings of fullness. Sensory-specific satiety or the
being physically hungry, eat in response to emo- process by which food loses its appeal as the taste
tional distress, or continue eating even when sated. buds in our mouths habituate to specific combina-
In addition, individuals who are obese and report tions of flavors is the fastest feedback system
compulsive eating patterns show marked oversen- (Remick, Polivy, & Pliner, 2009; Srensen, Mller,
sitivity to external or non-nutritive cues to eat Flint, Martens, & Raben, 2003). Evidence sug-
(social, emotional, or conditioned craving for cer- gests that it may be disrupted in obesity or binge
tain foods), and a concomitant desensitization to eating (Raynor & Epstein, 2001), but functions
internal cues, particularly related to normal sati- normally when adequate awareness is brought to
ety processes. While some models of this imbal- the process (Brondel et al., 2007). Ironically, the
ance are grounded in biological (e.g., genetic or hedonic value of food is often ignored in treatment
epigenetic) explanations (Appelhans, 2009), an of obesity and/or compulsive eating patterns,
alternative perspective is that most individuals can although it often informs addictive models of
become disconnected from internal experience, excessive food intake (Grosshans, Loeber, &
creating patterns of mindless eating. Individuals Kiefer, 2011). However, the solution offered is
with BED generally acknowledge frequently using often abstinence, rather than helping individuals
eating to manage stress (Goldfield, Adamo, cultivate more internalized self-regulation.
Rutherford, & Legg, 2008), and negative emotions Paradoxically, little comfort may actually be
(Chua, Touyz & Hill, 2004; Dunkley & Grilo, derived from the food being consumed, unlike for
2007), but this may range from relatively benign individuals without eating issues who acknowl-
use of food as a source of comfort to extreme pat- edge using food to manage negative emotions
terns in which eating facilitates dissociation from (Kristeller & Rodin, 1989).
overwhelming feelings or reflects virtually their
only coping mechanism, a significant failure of
self-regulation. Individuals with BED also exhibit Mindfulness Practice: A Self-
higher reactivity to food cues (Sobik, Hutchison, Regulation Approach
& Craighead, 2005), often expressing a sense of
incapacity to control urges to eat when appealing Recent neurocognitive models support the value
food is available. of meditation practice as a path toward re-
Deciding when to terminate food intake is also regulation (Lutz, Slagter, Dunne, & Davidson,
complex. Individuals who are obese and who eat 2008), particularly within complex emotional
202 J.L. Kristeller

systems that require higher level neuro-processing 2002), and Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention
to be brought into better balance. Ryan and (Bowen, Chawla, & Marlatt, 2011), undertake
Decis (2000) Self-Determination Theory of similar goals in regard to treatment of anxiety
mindfulness explicates the value of shifting from disorders, depression, and addictions, also utiliz-
external regulation, such as is imposed by struc- ing both mindfulness meditation training and
tured diets, to integrated regulation in which mindfulness exercises specific to the presenting
intrinsic processes meld with external factors for issues and associated therapeutic needs, such as
optimal self-regulation. As MB-EAT began to depressive thoughts or urges to drink.
develop, the role of meditation was shifting from The concepts of wisdom or insight, and
being conceptualized as a relaxation process to self-acceptance, core aspects of traditional medi-
being a powerful way to quiet the mind and culti- tation practice, are also central to the MB-EAT
vate self-awareness of internal experience. program. Wisdom, from a psychological per-
Mindfulness practice also cultivates the capacity spective, has a number of definitions (Sternberg,
to disengage undesirable reactivity, and to engage 1990), but these generally involve exercising
processes that more wisely inform behavior, good judgment in complex or uncertain situa-
also particularly relevant to issues related to eat- tions. Sternbergs balance theory of wisdom
ing and food choices (Kristeller, Baer, & Quillian- focuses on tacit rather than explicit knowl-
Wolever, 2006; Wolever & Best, 2009). edge; Baltes and Staudinger (1993) frame wis-
Meditation has become more appropriately dom in terms of exercising judgment in the
viewed as a way to cultivate a particular quality fundamental pragmatics of life. Within the
of attention and awareness, which then mediates context of meditation practice, wisdom can be
a wide range of self-regulatory processes. understood from a neuroscience perspective as
Several specific aspects of meditation practice arising from greater access to and integration of
have informed the development of the MB-EAT the complexity of experience and knowledge that
program. These include both open and guided each person already carries within (Kristeller,
meditations, and both extended practice and very 2003; Meeks, Cahn, & Jeste, 2012). Ostafin and
brief practice. All of these aspects have extensive Kassman (2012) have demonstrated that even
basis in meditative traditions and play distinct novice meditators increase in creative or insight-
roles in self-regulatory processes (Kristeller, oriented problem solving. We emphasize in the
2007). Sitting practice, whether breath awareness program that mindfulness practice can assist in
or open awareness, cultivates a more general accessing such tacit knowledge and judgment
capacity for experiencing in the moment, whether in any situation, including making apparently
that moment contains physical feelings, thoughts, trivial, but often complex, decisions regarding
or emotions. Guided practice encourages aware- food choice and eating.
ness of a specific targeted experience, yet in an From the first session of the MB-EAT pro-
observing, stable, nonjudgmental and curious gram, participants are presented with the concept
manner. Given the complexity of our relationship of wisdom as something accessible to them, in
to food, guided practices help individuals identify that wisdom emerges from their own experiences
distinct aspects of their eating experience, view- and abilities to recognize solutions to challenging
ing them with curiosity instead of judgment. situations, rather than having these proscribed
These guided practices focus on physical vs. from without. Self-acceptance is framed as an
emotional hunger, taste, fullness, and making alternative to reacting judgmentally to every
healthier and wiser food choices. Other therapeu- self-perceived slip from internalized norms, a
tic applications of mindfulness meditation, such hallmark of eating disorders. Instead, they are
as the Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction encouraged to use mindfulness first to self-
Program (Kabat-Zinn, 1990), Mindfulness-Based observe reactive or overeating episodes, then to
Cognitive Therapy (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, notice triggers, with an attitude of open curiosity,
15 Mindfulness Eating Disorders 203

and, finally, to access their own wisdom to look are assisting them to cultivate a capacity that
for alternatives. In contrast to approaches that brings this greater sense of self-control, balance,
emphasize simple solutions (i.e., highly struc- and wisdom. Figure 15.1 represents this process of
tured diets), MB-EAT acknowledges the com- engaging mindfulness in order to produce a sense
plexity of choices involved in eating, and that an of control over previously over-conditioned pat-
attitude of patience, experimentation, and self- terns of triggers and reactions. Although the exam-
compassion goes much further in developing patterns ples provided here reflect the struggles of someone
that can be sustained indefinitely. Self-acceptance with a clinically significant level of disordered eat-
and self-forgiveness are particularly relevant to ing, the processes involved apply to anyone who
interrupting dysfunctional cycles of binging, has become mindless about some aspect of his
self-recrimination, and over-restraint. or her relationship to food and eating.
At the same time, the MB-EAT model is com-
patible with other perspectives on treating dys-
functional eating patterns, including First Stage: Cultivating
cognitive-behavioral approaches (Apple & Nonjudgmental Awareness
Agras, 1997), interpersonal therapy (Wilfley
et al., 2002) for BED, and other approaches The top half (A) of the figure represents a condi-
incorporating mindfulness within acceptance- tioned cycle of dysfunctional eating. The indi-
based treatments (Safer, Telch, & Chen, 2009; vidual depicted here has many issues with
Wilson, 2004). However, similar to Appetite overeating; one of them, illustrated here, is com-
Awareness Training (Allen & Craighead, 1999), ing home from work, feeling stressed, and often
MB-EAT brings more attention to processes of overly hungry, finding a box of tempting cookies,
food intake per se. MB-EAT also incorporates and rather than eating just a few, as intended, eat-
recent work recognizing the complexity of ing the whole box. As trigger X (seeing the
hedonic drives associated with food (Appelhans, cookies; thinking I deserve a couple) leads to
2009; King, 2013). In sum, the MB-EAT pro- reaction Y (eating the whole box) over and
gram is designed to help individuals cultivate over again, increasingly experienced as out of
awareness of both internal and external triggers control and automatic, the individual begins to
to eating; interrupt dysfunctional cycles of bing- feel helpless and further distressed.
ing, self-recrimination, and over-restraint; and re- As mindfulness is introduced, the individual
engage natural physiological processes of eating becomes more aware of the triggers (1), such as
regulation. Moreover, the program emphasizes certain feelings, thoughts, or foods. These trig-
the pleasure and nurturing aspects of eating, in gers are often more complex than first perceived.
terms of both types and amount of food eaten, For example, someone may become aware that
along with healthier patterns of food choice. its not only seeing the cookies, but also being
stressed by a particular type of work issue AND
being overly hungry, combined with certain
Mindful Eating and Mindfulness thoughts (Ive blown it by eating one cookie, so
Practice: A Model I might as well keep on going.).
of De-conditioning

Cultivating a Capacity Second Stage: Cultivating Capacity


for Mindfulness to Interrupt Reactivity and Mindless
Choice
As mindfulness is increasingly recognized as a
general human capacity, rather than something The second stage (also depicted within the top
esoteric and unique to meditation practice, it half of the figure) emerges as the arrows of
becomes easier to explain to individuals how we mindfulness penetrate awareness; the individual
204 J.L. Kristeller

A Mindfulness Practice:
Cultivating Awareness and
Decreasing Reactivity
1. Awareness of
Triggers
2a. Decreasing
Reactivity

X
Y
[Trigger for
2b. Interrupting
eating: box of
ConditionedCycle [Eating reaction:
cookies; stressed;
etc.] eat entire box of
cookies]

Mindfulness Practice:
B
Enhancing Other Responses

[Trigger: box of
X Y
cookies; stressed; [Response:
hungry] binges decrease
in frequency &
3a. Other Responses size]
Become Accessible

A D [savor 2-3
C cookies]
[put box of cookies
away; rest; call
B [choose another
snack]
friend, etc.]
3b. Mindful Eating
4. Emergence of Wiser/ Responses Become
Novel Responses Available

Fig. 15.1 Mindfulness practice and mindful eating: moving from awareness (A) to wise choice (B)

begins to recognize a decreasing sense of com- the reinforcement cycle. In contrast, in our clini-
pulsion, and a weakening of the pull to react in cal experience, we have found that with
response to the set of triggers (2a). As reaction is mindfulness, as depicted in this figure, the entire
replaced by accepting observation, the reinforce- cycle weakens fairly rapidly.
ment cycle begins to weaken (2b).
Although learning theory suggests that any
decrease in pairing of stimulus and response Third Stage: Cultivating Awareness
weakens the conditioned reinforcement cycle, of Alternatives and Development
clinical evidence suggests that when access to the of Mindful Eating
object is blocked by willpower, a vow of absti-
nence, or simple self-control techniques (i.e., Several elements have shifted in the bottom half
hiding the boxes of cookies), there may be little (B) of the figure. In this next phase, the perceived
decrease in desire nor in the perceived strength of strength of both the triggers and the reaction has
15 Mindfulness Eating Disorders 205

gotten weaker/smaller. The response/reaction options may arise into consciousness. Such
(such as eating cookies just because they are insight might happen during a sitting practice or
there) has not completely disappeared, and the in the moment of engaging in the behavior itself.
triggers are generally still present (feeling upset; People in our program strongly resonate to the
seeing cookies), but their trigger strength, and the concept of cultivating the wise mind. The term
strength of the reinforcement cycle, has decreased. wisdom often implies a profound level of
The arrows of awareness connecting mindful- insight or new understanding. However, wisdom
ness to both the trigger(s) (X) and responses/ may also be simple, involving the emergence of
reactions (Y) have gotten larger/strengthened, as new choices, not offered as solutions by others
individuals learn to nonjudgmentally observe (such as structured diets), but growing out of
these overlearned patterns. ones own experience. For example, a woman in
As significantly, awareness of alternative our group realized that she was most tempted at
responses (circles A and B) to trigger situations home by a particular type of cookie (oatmeal rai-
has increased (3a). Although this can also occur sin); her previous solution had been not to buy
in response to more traditional cognitive- any cookies at all, but that left her feeling
behavioral approaches to treating overlearned deprived. She had the insight one day that she
patterns, mindfulness practice may increase the could keep a different type of cookie at home that
sense of accessibility of these alternatives, involv- she found far less tempting but which was still
ing less effort to engage them. Often these alter- satisfying when she really wanted a small, sweet
natives, such as calling a friend when stressed, snack. She then indulged her desire for oatmeal
rise into awareness after only a few moments of raisin cookies by buying one or two at a local
mindfulness. For individuals with long-standing bakeryparticularly when feeling stressed. A
eating issues, the alternatives to eating may not year later she realized she could finally have such
be clear at first, as eating has served for so long as cookies in her home without overeating on them.
a primary coping tool. Exploring alternatives,
whether in group or individual therapy, is there-
fore a valuable treatment component. Program Content: The Conceptual
Circles C and D (3b) represent ways to engage Framework
the old response systemeating for comfort
but in better balance. This involves cultivating Details of the MB-EAT program content are further
mindful awareness of food choice, physical hun- outlined in Table 15.1, organized by key theoretical
ger, taste satisfaction, and satiety. Even individu- principles and concepts. The treatment compo-
als with highly dysregulated eating patterns may nents reflecting the principles fall into five distinct
find that this can happen relatively quickly. In areas of practice: cultivating mindfulness in general;
this example, that might mean giving oneself per- the application of mindfulness to all aspects of
mission to slowly eat and savor a few cookies eating; improving emotional balance; cultivating
or to choose a different comfort food. Over outer wisdom or the wiser use of the wealth of
time, uncontrolled reactivity shifts into having a nutritional information available; and engaging
choice of responses. The sense of struggle self-acceptance and non-self-judgment.
decreases or even disappears, and the sense of
choice and self-determination increases.
Cultivating Mindfulness

Fourth Stage: Access to Emergent Mindfulness is the core capacity on which


Wise Mind MB-EAT rests. As addressed above, it is not only
central to cultivating attention and awareness, the
Finally, because mindfulness appears to act by core elements of practice (Jha, Krompinger, &
more fully engaging neural networks, novel Baime, 2007), but acts by softening and loosening
206 J.L. Kristeller

Table 15.1 MB-EAT: principles, practices, and targeted exercises


Concept/principle Practices/exercises Session
Cultivating mindfulness
Cultivate capacity to direct attention, be Mindfulness meditation practice in session. Daily 110
aware, disengage reactivity, and be practice
nonjudgmental
Cultivate ability to be mindful in daily Mini-meditation use 210
experience, including eating General use of mindfulness
Cultivating/engaging inner and outer Meditation practice/mindfulness in daily life. 110
wisdom Encouraging insight
Wisdom meditation (Session 10)
Cultivating mindful eating
Bring mindful attention and awareness to Mindfulness meditation. Mini-meditations. Wide 110
eating experience range of eating practices. Chain reaction model
Recognizing mindless eating
Cultivate taste experience/savoring and Mindfully eating a variety of food. In session: 1, 2, 4, 6, 7,
enjoying food Raisins; cheese and crackers; chocolate; fruit and 9
veggies; favorite food; potluck. All meals during
week; buffet homework
Cultivate awareness of hunger experience Hunger awareness meditation. Continuing as home 3
practice
Awareness and cultivation of taste satiety/taste Training in sensory-specific satiety, both in and out 4, 7
satisfaction/food choice of session. Taste satisfaction meter
Choice: Chips or cookies
Potluck/buffet homework
Awareness and cultivation of fullness and Fullness awareness/ratings with water. Potluck/ 16
body satiety experience buffet homework. Mindfully ending all meals
Awareness of negative self-judgment/ Eating challenging foods 26, 910
cultivating nonjudgment of eating experience Identifying cognitive distortions and black and
white thinking; surfing the urge; abstinence
violation effect
Cultivating emotional balance
Cultivate awareness of emotions and Learn to identify and tolerate emotional triggers. 35, 9, 10
emotional reactivity Chain reaction model
Meeting emotional needs in healthy ways Mindfulness practice as stress reduction. Emotional Most
eating visualization. Modifying comfort eating. sessions
Behavioral substitution
Cultivating outer wisdom
Decreasing amount eaten in a flexible and Tracking food energy/calories 26, 7
sustainable manner 500 Calorie Challenge. Mindfully attending to
caloric values of different foods
Attending to nutritional needs relative to Mindfully making food choices based on personal
better health health issues
Increasing energy output by increased Pedometers; mindful walking; exploring other
walking/gentle exercise exercises
Cultivating self-acceptance
Acceptance and non-self-judgment of body/ Improving relationship to the body. Breathe 1, 35, 8
self-regulation awareness; body scan; healing self-touch; chair yoga
Recognition of anger at self and others. Exploring feeling and thoughts toward self and 4, 5, 810
Acceptance of self/others others. Loving kindness meditation. Forgiveness
meditation. Values exercise
Recognizing/engaging capacity for growth. Cultivating and honoring wisdom in self. Wisdom All, 10
Self-empowerment meditation. Discussion throughout
15 Mindfulness Eating Disorders 207

long-standing conditioned reactions, as shown in sensation, including thoughts and feelings, and
the top half (A) of Fig. 15.1, and then extended savoring each raisin as fully as possible. Four
toward flexible self-regulation Fig. 15.1 (B), raisins are used. After receiving guidance for the
invaluable in managing the complex daily deci- first two, individuals lead themselves through
sions regarding eating. Training in mindfulness eating the third. They are given the option to eat
practice begins with breath awareness and sitting the fourth, introducing the challenge of choice
meditation. All sessions include mindfulness and for many, a surprising realization that they
meditation practice. Participants are encouraged actually dont want this last raisin. People share
to practice daily at home, initially for 10 min, amazement at the intensity of the experience, the
moving to 20 and then 30 min, gradually wean- distinct texture, and even flavor of each raisin,
ing themselves off use of the audio support. In and how it differs from mindlessly eating a
Session 2, we introduce the practice of using handful of raisins. This practice lays the founda-
mini-meditations as a way to bring mindful- tion to explore how to eat more mindfully at
ness into everyday activities, and particularly to home and in future exercises.
eating experiences. Mini-meditations are truly As the program progresses, the foods used are
that; they are intended to encompass the few increasingly challenging in terms of hedonic and
moments needed to stop, bring awareness to caloric value, including potential binge foods
the breath, notice thoughts, and observe inner with lower nutritional value. These challenges
feelings of hunger, fullness, or the range of often help individuals become more aware of
available food choices. Results from our research the distinction between liking and wanting
show that improvement in eating, emotional particular foods (Finlayson, King, & Blundell,
regulation, and weight loss occurs in direct rela- 2007). Cheese and crackers are presented in
tion to the use of practice elements, including Session 2, and in Session 4, rich chocolate cook-
sitting and guided eating meditations, and ies, along with an explicit focus on awareness of
mini-meditations. changes in taste experience. As with the raisins,
most participants express surprise at how quickly
the flavor of these foods lose their appeal when
Cultivating Mindful Eating fully and mindfully savored, engaging the experi-
ences of taste satisfaction and taste satiety.
Varied mindfulness exercises are used through- Research has extensively documented the pro-
out the program to help individuals re-balance cess by which taste buds decrease their capacity
distinct aspects of eating. In the top half of for firing as food is eaten (and as physiological
Fig. 15.1, mindfulness brings awareness to highly hunger decreases) (e.g., Heatherington & Rolls,
conditioned patterns; the bottom half of Fig. 15.1 1996). Individuals who habitually overeat tend to
focuses on cultivating alternative eating patterns ignore or override these feedback signals, chas-
that are more mindful and balanced. Exercises ing the flavor of the first few bites. Yet in our
target the following: bringing awareness to sen- experience, they are able to quickly tune into this
sations of physical hunger vs. other triggers for rapid drop in flavor when directed to do so, gen-
eating; awareness of different types of satiety, erally within a few bites. In Session 5, we focus
including taste satiety (our term for sensory- on mindfully making food choices between
specific satiety), stomach fullness, and body savory and sweet snacks: corn chips vs. butter
satiety; and awareness of making food choices, cookies. This exercise increases awareness of
both to optimize satisfaction from smaller making food choices, the pull of different
amounts of food and in regard to the nutritive foods, and again, taste.
value of food choice. Other exercises related to mindful eating
The first guided practice is borrowed from develop awareness of physical hunger and full-
MBSR: mindfully eating several raisins. The ness. Session 3 introduces the experience of
exercise focuses on noticing and amplifying each physical hunger, as distinct from emotional
208 J.L. Kristeller

hunger or other triggers for eating. Participants are report virtually no other coping approaches. At
asked to note how physically hungry they are, with the same time, they are also encouraged to iden-
10 as hungry as possible, and 1 not hungry at tify and to savor their own preferred comfort
all. They are then asked How do you know foods, with a focus on quality rather than quan-
that? a powerful question that helps them explore tity, instead of adding to their distress by worry-
their inner experience more fully. In Session 5, the ing about being bad or losing control when
experience of fullness is explored by having par- eating such foods (see Fig. 15.1-B-3b). We have
ticipants drink a large bottle of water. Using water also adapted a chain reaction exercise from Dia-
separates feelings of fullness, as also noted on a lectical Behavior Therapy for eating disorders
10-point scale, from the effects of high caloric (Wisniewski & Kelly, 2003), to help participants
intake. We also emphasize that hunger and full- explore the complexity of the patterns they have
ness are distinct physical processes, not just oppo- developed in using food to manage experiences
site ends of a scale; it is possible to eat something such as stress, anger, or anxiety. The concept of
filling (such as popcorn) and still be hungryor the chain also addresses links between harsh self-
eat something calorically dense, like ice cream, judgment, overeating, and negative affect, along
but not feel very full. By Session 7, which includes with addressing such common types of distorted
a potluck meal, individuals are able to face a very thinking as the abstinence violation effect (Ive
challenging eating situation with a new set of blown it, so I might as well keep going.) (Marlatt
skills and confidence. This meal proceeds with a & Gordon, 1985). We emphasize that the chain
set of guidelines: choosing foods for quality over reaction can be interrupted at any point, even in
quantity; returning for seconds; and leaving food the midst of a binge. Of note, our research has
on the plate. The meal is initially in silence, high- found that even when binges continued to occur,
lighting the challenge of eating mindfully after they are notably smaller (Kristeller et al., 2013).
returning to talking.
Home practices parallel the group experiences
(i.e., attention to hunger, taste, fullness, food Cultivating Outer Wisdom
choice), plus the expectation starting in Week 1
of eating one meal or snack mindfully each day, Most people come into the program with an all
increasing to eating all meals and snacks mind- or nothing attitude toward food choices. Almost
fully. Over the program, individuals are also all have been on multiple diets, of all different
encouraged more and more to attend to thoughts types, and when asked to describe a healthy
and feelings before and throughout eating. After way of eating, they can generally do sobut with
the potluck meal, they are asked to go to a com- a caloric value (e.g., approximately 1,200 calo-
mercial buffet, perceived as a singular challenge, ries) that is usually far too restrictive for a main-
but one which most individuals find, with sur- tenance level of eating. They readily acknowledge
prise, that they can now manage very well. consuming large of amounts of unhealthy
foods, such as cheese, pizza, doughnuts, or other
sweets, while generally avoiding noting the
Cultivating Emotional Balance caloric values of such foods, a task perceived as
burdensome or self-punishing. They also express
Eating meets emotional needs, more for some feeling overwhelmed by the amount of informa-
individuals than for others. We explore through- tion bombarding them regarding nutrition, and
out the program how common such patterns are, acknowledge ignoring much of it.
and that awareness of these patterns is important Throughout the program, we introduce the
to eating in a mindful, self-accepting way. value of engaging outer wisdoma healthier
Participants are also encouraged to explore alter- yet flexible attitude toward making food choices
natives to eating as ways to meet their emotional in combination with inner wisdom. Outer
needs (see Fig. 15.1-B-3a); some individuals wisdom is distinct from outer knowledge, in
15 Mindfulness Eating Disorders 209

that knowledge is familiarity with relevant better physical health and sense of well-being.
information, while wisdom is knowing how to In addition to gentle chair yoga, we emphasize
make use of that information to suit particular increasing walking, both mindfully and for exer-
needs and preferences. In the second week, we cise. In the service of self-awareness and self-
provide them with a calorie guide, and encourage regulation, we provide pedometers to all
them to check labels on packaged foods. We draw participants, first asking them to establish a base-
the analogy to financial management, asking the line, and then gradually to increase their walking
question If you were on a budget, would you by 10 % per week. Most people expand on this,
shop at a store that didnt have price tags on any- becoming quite excited about using the pedome-
thing? to emphasize that this caloric information ter to track their own improvement.
is simply a way to inform choices, given reason-
able limitations. We ask them to identify the
caloric value of typical foods eaten during the Cultivating Self-Acceptance
week, and to begin considering which they might
either take out of their regular diet or cut back on Cultivating self-acceptance is placed last in
serving size. We ask them to take on these tasks Table 15.1, not because it is least important, but
both to inform themselves and in order to find in some respects most important. Critical
ways to cut out approximately 500 calories from self-judgment is a key target for mindfulness, in
their daily pattern of eating (the 500 Calorie that self-judgment entails an emotionally
Challenge), an amount that, on average, will ini- valenced (and generally negative) reaction to
tially result in a gradual weight loss of 1 lb/week experience, and is endemic to struggling with
(500 cal 7 days = 3,500 cal). We continuously obesity and eating issues. For many individuals
emphasize the need to be flexible, to experiment throughout the weight spectrum, self-identity
with different possibilitiesand to only cut out becomes absorbed by preoccupation with eating,
foods entirely that they are willing to take out of weight, and appearance. Therefore, an important
their regular eating programs on a permanent aspect of the program is encouraging individuals
basisnot just to lose weight. to notice when such self-judgment has occurred
We also address both healthier nutritional and whether it is out of proportion to the trigger
patterns in general (i.e., introducing national (for example, feeling like a failure after eating a
guidelines such as MyPlate), and those spe- small amount of a forbidden food). In general,
cific to personal dietary needs such as diabetes the goal is to create a more accepting and flexible
or cardiac disease, but in a flexible way. For the relationship with the self, in regard both to eating
potluck meal, we ask everyone to bring two and broader self-identity.
dishes, one a favorite food that is less healthy Several exercises explicitly help individuals
but that they want to continue to eat in modera- engage self-acceptance in relation to their bodies.
tion (like macaroni and cheese), and the other A body scan exercise encourages distinguishing
that reflects healthier eating. We repeatedly between experiencing and judging the body. Both
emphasize the need for self-regulation in regard gentle chair yoga in Session 5 and mindful walk-
to meshing personal desires with knowledge- ing in Session 8 further increase awareness of the
based guidelines. body while cultivating an attitude of kindness and
Increasing exercise is another component of compassion. Chair yoga is used instead of floor
outer wisdom. Many individuals coming into yoga because heavier individuals may have diffi-
the program are extremely sedentary, avoiding culty getting down toor up fromthe floor; it is
physical activities as much as possible, related to also easier to incorporate into usual daily routines
lifelong patterns, or due to increasing issues with (such as at a desk). Finally, a healing self-touch
joint pain, lack of flexibility, or shortness of exercise1 often has profound impact; participants
breath. Although we acknowledge the contribution
of physical activity to weight management, we 1
The healing self-touch exercise was developed by Sasha
expand the discussion more broadly to cultivating Loring, MS, MEd, at Duke Integrative Medicine.
210 J.L. Kristeller

are asked to fill their hands with loving kindness, pose of the required meditation practice, in that
and place them on parts of their bodies, moving several individuals who dropped out almost
from areas they like and appreciate to areas of their immediately had shared concerns that a
body with which they struggle. Buddhist practice was incompatible with their
Self-acceptance is extended beyond the body Christian beliefs and practices. We therefore
to other aspects of the self. Throughout, the pro- emphasized that we were drawing on Buddhist
gram encourages nonjudgmental exploration of psychology rather than on Buddhism as a reli-
thoughts, feelings, and physical experiences, gion, and ended up with a completion rate of 18
including taking pleasure in eating, without out of 20 individuals.
swamping that experience with self-blame. This Results were consistent enough to analyze by
delicate balance between accountability and self- group effects. Binges per week dropped from an
blame is explored in a forgiveness meditation in average of over 4 to about 1.5, with the amount of
Session 5. It is not unusual that individuals recog- food consumed during remaining binges decreas-
nize how anger at self and others drives overeat- ing substantially. Overall, scores on the Binge
ing. As noted earlier, also core to engaging Eating Scale (BES) (Gormally, Black, Daston, &
self-acceptance is encouragement in all sessions Rardin, 1982) fell from the severe range to just
to explore their own inner wisdom, including a higher than having little or no problem with
values exercise to shift focus away from preoc- binge eating. Depression also decreased from
cupation with eating and weight as core elements clinical to subclinical levels. Individuals also
of self-identity, and culminating in Session 10 tracked their meditation practice daily, including
with a guided wisdom meditation. The emphasis both sitting meditation and guided eating medita-
is on recognizing ones own inner wisdom, creat- tion practices. The strongest predictor of
ing a path to wise choice, informed by general improvement in eating control was time spent
knowledge, but shaped by internal resources, and using eating-related meditations.
always in the service of supporting greater self- Based on these results, we obtained funding
regulation (see Fig. 15.1-B-4). from the National Center for Complementary
and Alternative Medicine at NIH for a clinical
trial with Duke University (Kristeller et al.,
Empirical Support for MB-EAT 2013), expanding the sample to include men, but
overall highly similar in weight and age to the
Although the clinical development of MB-EAT pilot study. Participants (total N = 140; 15 %
included both men and women with a range of men) were randomized to three conditions:
eating and weight issues, we focused the first sys- MB-EAT; a psycho-educational/cognitive
tematic study on obese women who met criteria behavioral (PECB) treatment based on the Duke
for BED. The original proof-of-concept study Diet and Fitness Center obesity treatment pro-
used a non-randomized, extended baseline/ gram; or a waiting list control, with follow-up at
extended follow-up design (Kristeller & Hallett, 1 and 4 months. The treatment was expanded to
1999), well suited to exploratory clinical out- nine sessions, with greater emphasis on self-
come research, in that if group results are not compassion and cultivation of self-acceptance.
consistent, changes in individual responses can The interventions were again primarily focused
be validly analyzed as in an A-B-A design. on improving relationship to eating, rather than
Treatment was delivered in a 7-session group on weight loss. As has been reported in other
program over 6 weeks. The sample was primarily studies comparing interventions for BED to
middle aged (avg. age = 46.5) with a mean BMI other active interventions, the two active inter-
of 40 (mean weight = 238 lbs). Experience with a ventions showed similar improvements in behav-
pilot group had suggested to us the need to pro- ior and on the BES. However, the MB-EAT
vide individual orientation sessions to address group improved more on other measures of self-
personal anxieties about the meaning and pur- regulated eating (e.g., the Hunger scale of the
15 Mindfulness Eating Disorders 211

Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) failed them, and whether individuals without
(Stunkard & Messick, 1985)). such compulsive eating patterns, but still moder-
Depression improved in both groups but ately to morbidly obese, would respond to
apparently for different reasons. In the PECB approaches originally designed to modulate com-
group, less depression was associated with pulsive overeating patterns.
decreased bingeing, and improvement on most Individuals were randomized to the MB-EAT
other measures of self-control, suggesting that condition or to a waiting-list control. Preliminary
improved mood may have followed from a analyses have been encouraging. Effects were
greater sense of self-efficacy. In contrast, the highly parallel between the two sets of partici-
improvement on depression in MB-EAT was pants (those with BED and those without), and
generally independent of other measures but was consistent with those observed in our previous
linked to amount of meditation practice, suggest- research. Furthermore, both groups showed a
ing a broader nonspecific value of such practice. weight loss of about 7 lbs at immediate post,
Again, amount of meditation practice, calculated largely sustained during follow-up. This was
as a total index of sitting, guided, and mini- approximately one pound per week from when
meditations, predicted improvement on other they were asked to begin cutting back 500 cal/
indicators of self-regulation, including amount of day. Also encouraging was that only one
weight lost. However, to our concern, neither individual (someone with BED) showed an
group showed any average weight loss, with increase in weight at immediate post. However,
some individuals gaining weight and others los- at the next follow-up point, she had lost this
ing. Although no predictors of weight loss could weight, noting that it had taken her a little lon-
be identified (other than meditation practice), ger to really get it. Also consistent with intro-
interviews suggested that some individuals per- ducing the outer wisdom components was
ceived the message (within either treatment) as more substantial improvement than in the previ-
being one of permissiveness around food and ous study for both those with and without BED
eating, as long as binges were decreased. on the Cognitive Restraint scale of the TFEQ,
Based on this study, we expanded MB-EAT to primarily an indicator of healthy restraint.
include an increased focus on weight loss. The Improvement on this measure was sustained
program was extended to ten sessions (as referred through the 6-month follow-up.
to in Table 15.1), with two follow-up sessions at A modified MB-EAT program is currently
1 and 2 months (Kristeller & wolever, 2011). As under investigation in an even broader range of
discussed above, we systematically added ele- individuals, with relatively lower levels of binge
ments that we refer to as cultivating outer wis- eating problems and obesity, and with more
dom: i.e., helping participants engage with focus on the stress management effects of
information about food energy (calories), nutri- mindfulness meditation-based treatment. Pilot
tion, and exercise that is flexible, targeted to their data provides preliminary evidence for this
personal needs, and likely to be sustainable. approach to this population, and for the specific
Because we wanted to evaluate the effects of value of addressing stress-related aspects of
such elements on individuals with significant obesity (Adam & Epel, 2007; Daubenmier et al.,
weight issues, independent of binge eating pat- 2011). Another group of investigators (Dalen
terns, we enrolled those with a BMI of 35 or et al., 2010) incorporated elements of MB-EAT
above, a level at which a meaningful proportion into a 6-week program linking healthier eating
of participants would meet criteria for BED. We with exercise, referred to as Mindful Eating and
wanted to address two complementary concerns: Living (MEAL), and also found significant
whether individuals with compulsive eating pat- improvement in healthy restraint, and decreases
terns might react adversely to tracking calories, in weight and other indicators of dysregulated
perhaps perceiving that we were encouraging eating. Timmerman and Brown (2012) enrolled
them in a dieting approach that had so often women who reported regularly overeating in
212 J.L. Kristeller

restaurants and focused their 6-week intervention, Conclusion


adapted from MB-EAT, solely on that context,
showing improvement in weight and nutritional The MB-EAT program combines well-understood
balance. Singh has reported several case studies principles of food intake regulation and prin-
(Singh et al., 2008, 2011) in which they very ciples of mindfulness meditation to provide a
creatively incorporated mindful eating compo- novel approach to re-regulating eating behavior.
nents, including meditation training, hunger While conceptually compatible with other effec-
awareness, and healthy nutrition, into treatment tive approaches to treating eating disorders, it
for adolescents with Prader-Willi syndrome is unique in several respects. First, training in
with classic symptoms of mild intellectual dis- mindfulness meditation serves as a foundation
ability, hyperphagia, and mild to morbid obesity. for cultivating a capacity to bring nonjudgmental
Each of them responded positively to the inter- awareness to the complex processes involved in
ventions, gradually losing substantial weight food choice, the decision to initiate eating, and
maintained over several years. Tapper (Tapper the decision to stop eating. Second, the training
et al., 2009) has evaluated a workshop-based appears to help people systematically re-engage
mindful eating intervention in a randomized innate abilities to use hunger and satiety sig-
trial for women with a wide range of weight nals. Third, the training purposefully cultivates
levels, finding significant weight loss at 6 months drawing pleasure from eating, by emphasiz-
mediated primarily by change in binge eating ing quality over quantity in doing so. Finally,
patterns. MB-EAT encourages an attitude of nonjudgmen-
MB-EAT has also been modified and adapted tal acceptance of self to daily living, body aware-
for use with an overweight/obese population ness, and emotional experiences, beginning with
with non-insulin-dependent diabetes (Miller, food-related experiences and expanding to the
Kristeller, Headings, Nagaraja, & Miser, 2012). whole self.
That study compared MB-EAT-D to a relatively Research has demonstrated the effectiveness
intensive medical nutrition group program (Smart of MB-EAT in treating compulsive eating pat-
Choices). In general, both interventions showed terns associated with BED. Ongoing research
improvement on most variables, with few differ- further suggests that MB-EAT can be adapted to
ences between them. As might be expected, the address weight loss, without losing its effective-
Smart Choices program showed more specific ness for treating the associated symptoms of
nutritional improvements, consistent with the tar- binge eating. Further work is needed to explore
geted goals. It may be that for some individuals its application to normalizing relationship to food
the Smart Choices program followed by the and eating for the both compulsive and restrictive
MB-EAT-D program would be more effective. aspects of food intake associated with bulimia
For heavier individuals with more mindless or nervosa and anorexia nervosa.
compulsive overeating patterns, it might be
advisable to provide the MB-EAT-D program
prior to Smart Choices. For example, a morbidly References
obese woman with BED enrolled in one of our
earlier trials lost little weight during the course of Adam, T. C., & Epel, E. S. (2007). Stress, eating and the
reward system. Physiology & Behavior, 91, 449458.
the MB-EAT program, but became much more
Allen, H., & Craighead, L. (1999). Appetite monitoring in
aware of the emotional and social triggers con- the treatment of binge eating disorder. Behavior
tributing to her overeating. She then rejoined Therapy, 30, 253272.
Weight Watchers, losing over 100 lbs during the Altman, D. (2002). Art of the inner meal. Los Angeles,
CA: Moon Lake Media.
following year, noting that she was now able to
Appelhans, B. M. (2009). Neurobehavioral inhibition of
use those guidelines to meet her needs more flex- reward-driven feeding: Implications for dieting and
ibly and more effectively. obesity. Obesity, 17, 640647.
15 Mindfulness Eating Disorders 213

Apple, R. A., & Agras, W. S. (1997). Overcoming eating abdominal fat among overweight and obese women:
disorders: A cognitive-behavioral treatment for bulimia An exploratory randomized controlled study. Journal
and binge-eating disorder. New York, NY: of Obesity, 2011, [vol], 651936-651936.
Psychological Corporation. Daubenmier, J., Sze, J., Kerr, C., Kemeny, M., & Mehling,
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (1993). The research W. (2013). Follow your breath: Respiratory intero-
for a psychology of wisdom. Current Directions in ceptive accuracy in experienced meditators.
Psychological Science, 2, 7580. Psychophysiology, 50, 777789.
Bays, J. (2009). Mindful eating. Boston, MA: Shambala Davidson, R. J., Goleman, D. J., & Schwartz, G. E.
Publications. (1976). Attentional and affective concomitants of
Bertalanffy, L. V. (1968). General systems theory. meditation: A cross-sectional study. Journal of
New York, NY: Braziller. Abnormal Psychology, 85, 235238.
Bowen, S., Chawla, N., & Marlatt, A. (2011). Mindfulness- Drewnowski, A. (1996). The behavioral phenotype in
based relapse prevention for addictive behavior: A human obesity. In E. D. Capaldi (Ed.), Why we eat
clinicians guide. New York, NY: Guilford Press. what we eat. Washington, D.C.: American
Brondel, L., Romer, M., Van Wymelbeke, V., Walla, P., Psychological Association.
Jiang, T., Deecke, L., et al. (2007). Sensory-specific Dunkley, D. M., & Grilo, C. M. (2007). Self-criticism,
satiety with simple foods in humans: No influence of low self-esteem, depressive symptoms, and over-
BMI? International Journal of Obesity, 31, 987995. evaluation of shape and weight in binge eating disor-
Brown, A. J., Smith, L. T., & Craighead, L. W. (2010). der patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45,
Appetite awareness as a mediator in an eating disor- 139149.
ders prevention program. Eating Disorders: The Finlayson, G., King, N., & Blundell, J. E. (2007). Liking
Journal of Treatment and Prevention, 18, 286301. vs. Wanting food: Importance for human appetite
Brownell, K. (2000). The learn program for weight man- control and weight regulation. Neuroscience and
agement. Dallas, TX: American Health. Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 9871002.
Cannon, W. (1932). The wisdom of the body. New York, Geliebter, A., & Hashim, S. A. (2001). Gastric capacity in
NY: Norton. normal, obese, and bulimic women. Physiology &
Capaldi, E. D. (Ed.). (1996). Why we eat what we eat: The Behavior, 74, 743746.
psychology of eating. Washington, D.C.: American Geliebter, A., Hassid, G., & Hashim, S. A. (2001). Test
Psychological Association. meal intake in obese binge eaters in relation to mood
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A and gender. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
useful conceptual framework for personalitysocial, 29, 488494.
clinical, and health psychology. Psychological Goldfield, G. S., Adamo, K. B., Rutherford, J., & Legg, C.
Bulletin, 92, 111135. (2008). Stress and the relative reinforcing value of
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self- food in female binge eaters. Physiology & Behavior,
regulation of behavior. New York, NY: Cambridge 93, 579587.
University Press. Gormally, J., Black, S., Daston, S., & Rardin, D. (1982).
Chua, J. L., Touyz, S., & Hill, A. J. (2004). Negative The assessment of binge eating severity among obese
mood-induced overeating in obese binge eaters: An persons. Addictive Behaviors, 7, 4755.
experimental study. International Journal of Obesity, Grosshans, M., Loeber, S., & Kiefer, F. (2011).
28, 606610. Implications from addiction research towards the
Cooper, Z., Fairburn, C., & Hawker, D. (2004). Cognitive- understanding and treatment of obesity. Addiction
behavioral treatment of obesity: A clinicians guide. Biology, 16, 189198.
New York, NY: Guilford Press. Hanh, T. N., & Cheung, L. (2010). Savor. New York, NY:
Craighead, L. W. (2006). The appetite awareness work- HarperCollins.
book. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. Heatherington, M. M., & Rolls, B. J. (1996). Sensory-
Cuthbert, B., Kristeller, J. L., Simons, R., Hodes, R., & specific satiety: Theoretical frameworks and central
Lang, P. J. (1981). Strategies of arousal control: characteristics. In E. D. Capaldi (Ed.), Why we eat
Biofeedback, meditation, and motivation. Journal of what we eat (pp. 267290). Washington, D.C.:
Experimental Psychology. General, 110, 518546. American Psychological Association.
Dalen, J., Smith, B. W., Shelley, B. M., Sloan, A. L., Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007).
Leahigh, L., & Begay, D. (2010). Pilot study: Mindful Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of atten-
eating and living (meal): Weight, eating behavior, and tion. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience,
psychological outcomes associated with a 7, 109119.
mindfulness-based intervention for people with obe- Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living. New York,
sity. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 18, NY: Delacorte Press.
260264. Kabatznick, R. (1998). The Zen of eating. New York, NY:
Daubenmier, J., Kristeller, J., Hecht, F. M., Maninger, N., Penguin Putnam.
Kuwata, M., Jhaveri, K. et al. (2011). Mindfulness Khalsa, S., Rudrauf, D., Damasio, A., Davidson, R., Lutz,
intervention for stress eating to reduce cortisol and A., & Tranel, D. (2008). Interoceptive awareness in
214 J.L. Kristeller

experienced meditators. Psychophysiology, 45, Ogden, J. (2010). The psychology of eating (2nd ed.).
671677. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
King, B. M. (2013). The modern obesity epidemic, ances- Ostafin, B., & Kassman, K. (2012). Stepping out of history:
tral hunter-gatherers, and the sensory/reward control Mindfulness improves insight problem solving.
of food intake. American Psychologist, 68, 8896. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 10311036.
Kristeller, J. (1977). Meditation and biofeedback in the Patel, C. H. (1977). Biofeedback-aided relaxation and
regulation of internal states. In S. Ajaya (Ed.), meditation in the management of hypertension.
Meditational therapy. Glenview, IL: Himalayan Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 2, 141.
International Institute Press. Raynor, H. A., & Epstein, L. H. (2001). Dietary variety,
Kristeller, J. (2016). The Joy of Half a Cookie. New York: energy regulation, and obesity. Psychological Bulletin,
Perigree/Penguin Books. 127, 325.
Kristeller, J. L. (2003). Mindfulness, wisdom, and eating: Remick, A. K., Polivy, J., & Pliner, P. (2009). Internal and
Applying a multi-domain model of meditation effects. external moderators of the effect of variety on food
Constructivism in the Human Sciences, 8, 107118. intake. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 434451.
Kristeller, J. L. (2007). Mindfulness meditation. In Rodin, J. (1978). Stimulus-bound behavior and biological
P. Lehrer, R. Wookfolk, & W. E. Simes (Eds.), self-regulation: Feeding, obesity, and external control.
Principles and practices of stress management (3rd In G. E. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness
ed.). New York, NY: Guilford. and self-regulation (Vol. 2, pp. 215239). New York,
Kristeller, J. L., Baer, R. A., & Quillian-Wolever, R. NY: Plenum.
(2006). Mindfulness-based approaches to eating disor- Rodin, J. (1981). Current status of the internal-external
ders. In R. A. Baer (Ed.), Mindfulness-based treatment hypothesis for obesity: What went wrong? American
approaches. Burlington, MA: Academic. Psychologist, 36, 361372.
Kristeller, J. L., & Hallett, C. B. (1999). An exploratory Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination
study of a meditation-based intervention for binge eat- theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,
ing disorder. Journal of Health Psychology, 4, social development and well-being. American
357363. Psychologist, 55, 6878.
Kristeller, J. L., & Rodin, J. (1989). Identifying eating pat- Safer, D. L., Telch, C. F., & Chen, E. Y. (2009). Dialectical
terns in male and female undergraduates using cluster behavior therapy for binge eating and bulimia.
analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 14, 631642. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kristeller, J. L., & Wolever, R. Q. (2011). Mindfulness- Schachter, S. (1971). Some extraordinary facts about obese
based eating awareness training for treating binge eat- humans and rats. American Psychologist, 26, 129144.
ing disorder: The conceptual foundation. Eating Schachter, S., & Rodin, J. (Eds.). (1974). Obese humans
Disorders, 19, 4961. and rats. New York, NY: John Wiley.
Kristeller, J. L., & Wolever, R. (2016). Mindfulness-based Schwartz, G. E. (1975). Biofeedback, self-regulation, and
eating awareness training: A professional guide. the patterning of physiological processes. American
New York, NY: Guilford Press. Scientist, 63, 314324.
Kristeller, J. L., Wolever, R. Q., & Sheets, V. (2013). Schwartz, G. E. (1976). Self-regulation of response pattern-
Mindfulness-based eating awareness training ing: Implications for psychophysiological research and
(MB-EAT) for binge eating: A randomized clinical therapy. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 1, 730.
trial. Mindfulness, 5(3), 282297. Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002).
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression:
(2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in medi- A new approach to preventing relapse. New York, NY:
tation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163169. Guilford Press.
Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (Eds.). (1985). Relapse Shapiro, S. L., & Schwartz, G. E. (2000). The role of
prevention: Maintenance strategies in the treatment of intention in self-regulation: Toward intentional sys-
addictive behaviors. New York, NY: Guilford Press. temic mindfulness. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, &
Meeks, T. W., Cahn, B., & Jeste, D. V. (2012). M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation
Neurobiological foundations of wisdom. In C. K. (pp. 253273). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Germer & R. D. Siegel (Eds.), Wisdom and compassion Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Singh, A. N., Winton, A. S.
in psychotherapy: Deepening mindfulness in clinical W., Singh, J., McAleavey, K. M., et al. (2008). A
practice (pp. 189201). New York, NY: Guilford Press. mindfulness-based health wellness program for an
Miller, J. (1978). Living systems. New York, NY: adolescent with Prader-Willi syndrome. Behavior
McGraw Hill. Modification, 32, 167181.
Miller, C. K., Kristeller, J. L., Headings, A., Nagaraja, H., Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Singh, A. N. A., Winton,
& Miser, F. (2012). Comparative effectiveness of a A. S. W., Singh, A. D. A., & Singh, J. (2011). A
mindful eating intervention to a diabetes self- mindfulness-based health wellness program for indi-
management intervention among adults with type 2 viduals with Prader-Willi syndrome. Journal of
diabetes: A pilot study. Journal of the Academy of Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 4,
Nutrition and Dietetics, 112, 18351842. 90106.
15 Mindfulness Eating Disorders 215

Sobik, L., Hutchison, K., & Craighead, L. (2005). Cue- Wansink, B. (2007). Mindless eating: Why we eat more
elicited craving for food: A fresh approach to the study than we think. New York, NY: Bantam.
of binge eating. Appetite, 44, 253261. Wansink, B., & Sobal, J. (2007). Mindless eating: The 200
Srensen, L. B., Mller, P., Flint, A., Martens, M., & Raben, daily food decisions we overlook. Environment and
A. (2003). Effect of sensory perception of foods on Behavior, 39, 106123.
appetite and food intake: A review of studies on humans. Wilfley, D. E., Welch, R. R., Stein, R. I., Spurrell,
International Journal of Obesity, 27, 11521166. E. B., Cohen, L. R., Saelens, B. E., et al. (2002). A
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.). (1990). Wisdom: Its nature, origins, randomized comparison of group cognitive-behav-
and development. New York, NY: Cambridge ioral therapy and group interpersonal psychotherapy
University Press. for the treatment of overweight individuals with
Stunkard, A. J., & Messick, S. (1985). The three factor binge-eating disorder. Archives of General
eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, dis- Psychiatry, 59, 713721.
inhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Wilson, G. T. (2004). Acceptance and change in the treat-
Research, 29, 7183. ment of eating disorders: The evolution of manual-
Sysko, R., Devlin, M. J., Walsh, B. T., Zimmerli, E., & based cognitive-behavioral therapy. In S. C. Hayes,
Kissileff, H. R. (2007). Satiety and test meal intake V. M. Follette, & M. M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness
among women with binge eating disorder. International and acceptance: Expanding the cognitive-behavioral
Journal of Eating Disorders, 40, 554561. tradition (pp. 243260). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Tapper, K., Shaw, C., Ilsley, J., Hill, A. J., Bond, F. W., & Wisniewski, L., & Kelly, E. (2003). The application of
Moore, L. (2009). Exploratory randomised controlled dialectical behavior therapy to the treatment of eating
trial of a mindfulness-based weight loss intervention disorders. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 10(2),
for women. Appetite, 52, 396404. 131138.
Timmerman, G., & Brown, A. (2012). The effect of a Wolever, R. Q., & Best, J. L. (2009). Mindfulness-based
mindful restaurant eating intervention on weight man- approaches to eating disorders. In F. Didonna (Ed.),
agement in women. Journal of Nutrition Education Clinical handbook of mindfulness (pp. 259288).
and Behavior, 44, 2228. New York, NY: Springer.
Mindfulness and Self-Regulation:
A Medical Approach to the Mind 16
and Mental Health

James Davis-Siegel, Moriah Gottman,


and Daniel J. Siegel

In this chapter we explore how self-regulation an integrated mind (and the opposite of a mindful
and mindfulness form two cornerstones in the state) which characteristically exhibits interper-
medical approach to the self and mental health. sonal impairments and a lack of self-regulation.
We examine how they can be applied to not only The chapter concludes by extrapolating aspects
the traditional notions of the self and mind that of self-regulation and mindfulness to offer a bio-
lie within the skin but also how they can function logical exploration of morality which can be
in the interpersonal perspective of the relational looked at as a form self-regulation on a societal
self and mind. This chapter defines the funda- scale. Throughout this chapter, we suggest that
mental psychological processes of self-regulation mindfulness facilitates the integration of both
and mindfulness and their components such as interpersonal and intrapersonal systems resulting
the self, the mind, compassion, and empathy, and in improved self-regulation and mental health
discusses how these relate to, and in fact arise regardless of whether the self is defined narrowly
from, each other. We then examine how these as limited to the body or more broadly as also
processes, when functioning optimally and har- extending between individuals.
moniously, create a more integrated mind and a
better regulated self, resulting in well-being. This
notion is expanded in a brief discussion of The Self and Self-Regulation
Antisocial Personality Disorder (formerly known
as psychopathy or sociopathy)the antithesis of Who am I? It is perhaps the simplest question we
can ask ourselves, yet the answer can be infinitely
complex. The underlying concept of self and its
J. Davis-Siegel
definition is embedded in this question, which
Department of Psychology, University of California, naturally, varies from person to person. William
Santa Cruz, CA, USA James, a pioneering psychologist in the late nine-
e-mail: jdavisiegel@gmail.com teenth century, was one of the first scientists to
M. Gottman examine this concept. He concluded there were
Department of Psychology, Reed College, three components of the self-experience: the
1626 SE Lexington St., Portland, OR, USA
e-mail: mgottman130@gmail.com
material me (the bodily self, along with sur-
rounding physical objects), the social me (your
D.J. Siegel (*)
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine,
awareness of how others view you), and the
University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA spiritual me (the self that monitors private
e-mail: info@drdansiegel.com thoughts and feelings). James believed that

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 217


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_16
218 J. Davis-Siegel et al.

everything that you associate with your identity cussed in a later section in this chapter, we have
becomes, in a sense, part of the self. (as cited in institutionalized societal self-regulation in laws,
Gerrig & Zimbardo, 2008, p. 439) based on philosophical principles of morality,
From this explanation, a curious concept of which guide human interactions. Regulation of
identity emerges: the self contains not only ones the self, is essential to create order and peace in
physical body with its neurological processes, our inner and interpersonal lives which can be
but also ones mind and ones relationships. The achieved through mindfulness, as a later section
notion of the threefold self can be applied to any- will discuss.
one we identify with or relate to, making the self
inclusive of internal experience, interpersonal
relationships, communities, and cultures. What Is the Mind?
The individuals experience of identity and
self fits within a larger society, and its rules and The mind is not often defined in the fields of
standards. In order to meet societal expectations medicine, psychology, anthropology, education,
of competency for adulthood, proper self- or even philosophy (Siegel, 2012a, 2012b).
regulation must be developed and utilized. This is Statements that the mind is simply the activity
studied in the field of psychopathology and forms of the brain do not embrace the reality that neural
a central role in approaches to psychotherapeutic firing patterns are not the same as thoughts and
intervention (Schore, 2012). Self-regulation is feelings. Just as if a blind man was told which
intertwined with achieving independence, and wavelengths in the electromagnetic field consti-
not only involves developing a sense of self tute the color purple, the perception and the expe-
and personal identity, but also is related to learn- rience of the color first hand would not be
ing social rules and roles (Lightfoot, Cole, & conveyed; the mind cannot be effectively reduced
Cole, 2009). Developing self-regulation entails to simply the biological processes of the brain.
learning to control ones thoughts, emotions, Brain functions involve networks of interacting
behaviors, and attentional states. neurons with ions flowing in and out of their
The most important aspect of self-regulation membranes and enabling communication
in cultivating the relational self is emotional con- between them with the release of neurotransmit-
trol. Emotion regulation allows us to deal with ters as a means of sending messages that propel
the disappointments, frustrations, and injured functions in the rest of the body. However, the
feelings that are inevitable in everyday life. subjective experience of the mind is not merely
Furthermore, emotion regulation is essential for the same as neural activity in the brain. Neural
functioning and flourishing in a social world firing is not the same as a mental process such as
(Lightfoot et al., 2009). Socioemotional compe- a thought or feeling, consciousness or intention.
tence and intelligence are achieved when we The mind commonly encompasses our feel-
behave well even in the face of intense emotional ings and thoughts, our dreams and hopes, and our
arousal. Developing adequate self-regulation beliefs and experiences which frame our percep-
requires increasing tolerance for emotional prov- tions of the world. Although mental processes
ocation; whereas the initial level of intensity may can be described, a definition of the mind itself is
have brought out inappropriate responses, a bet- missing from major fields of academic study. In
ter regulated self is now able to tolerate the same the interdisciplinary field of Interpersonal
emotional stress and responds appropriately. Neurobiology (IPNB) a working definition of the
Self-regulation is an important part of abiding mind and of mental health is offered. IPNB offers
by social norms and learning to be a competent an overarching view of mental health, drawing
adult. This is why in nearly every society, there is from multiple independent scientific disciplines
a higher standard of acceptable behavior for and focusing on cultivating a healthy mind
adults than for children who have not yet fully through empathic relationships and an integrated
developed the ability to self-regulate. As dis- brain. The field views the aspect of the mind
16 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation: A Medical Approach to the Mind and Mental Health 219

beyond our subjective experience and conscious example) and the objective physical state (the
awareness, and defines this facet of the mind as physical response to fear or anxiety such as
an embodied and relational process that regu- increased heart and respiratory rates). Mindfulness
lates the flow of energy and information (Siegel, practices are ancient and often involve meditation
2012a, p. 2). This definition views the mind as an and other practices, such as yoga or Tai chi. By
emergent, self-organizing process that arises practicing mindfulness, we become more attuned
from within us and regulates energy and informa- to our psychological needs and can accept things
tion flow internally and interpersonally. The self as they are. This acceptance turns to resilience
can be seen as a center of narrative gravity that and we can explore rather than react to the suffer-
forms our core awareness of information, bridg- ing we feel. This practice has numerous clinical
ing past, present, and future. Like the self, the and therapeutic benefits, such as reducing stress,
mind is both embodied in physical form (in the increasing emotional regulation, and decreasing
brain and body) and is embedded in our connec- negative emotional experiences. One example of
tions to other people. The self and mind are inti- a mindfulness practice is found in the Mindfulness
mately interdependent; one can view the mind as Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) program created
originating from the self, and a sense of self as by Jon Kabat-Zinn and studied extensively by his
being a mental process. colleague, Richard Davidson (Davidson et al.,
IPNB offers a synthesis of the universal find- 2003). Drawing on the insight-practices of
ings across independent disciplines, an analytic Buddhist traditions, now simply called mindful-
process called consilience (Wilson, 1998). ness meditation, these begin with a focus on the
Such a perspective helps offer a definition of the breath. When the practitioners attention wanders,
mind and enables us to move to the practical he then recognizes the distraction, releases it, and
issue of asking what a healthy mind is. Regulation refocuses attention on the sensations of the breath.
involves the ability to monitor something and Over and over, this practice reinforces two abili-
then to modify that thing in an adaptive manner. ties: to simply sense the breath and to monitor
By defining the mind as regulatory, we can then ones focus of attention. In this way, mindfulness
begin to examine how to improve the capacity to can be seen as including the differentiation and
monitor and modulate energy and information strengthening of these two capacities. When they
flow within and between ourselves, resulting, by are linked within the repeating practice of sensing
definition, in a healthier mind. the breath, detecting distraction, refocusing on
sensation, and on and on, a state of integration
is created in which these capacities are differenti-
Mindfulness and Self-Regulation ated and then linked, the definition of integration
(Farb et al., 2007; Siegel, 2007a, 2007b).
Although the concept of mindfulness has existed From an IPNB perspective, integration is the fun-
and been practiced in Eastern traditions for mil- damental mechanism of health. In this way, the inte-
lennia (Germer, 2004), it is just now making its grative state created with mindfulness sets the stage
way into the arena of Western scientific study, for both internal integrative states to be achieved and
modern medicine, and psychological treatment for interpersonal integration to occur (Parker,
(Coelho, Canter, & Ernst, 2007; Germer, 2004; Neslon, Epel, & Siegel, in press). Mindfulness and
Hill & Updegraff, 2012; Siegel, 2007b, 2010b). integration go hand in hand internally and interper-
As defined by Siegel (2010a), mindfulness is sonallyjust as the mind itself is emerging both
intentionally focusing attention on moment-to- from within and between individuals.
moment experience without being swept up by Western clinicians have been increasingly
judgments or preconceived ideas and expecta- drawn to the intersection of mindfulness and psy-
tions (p. XIV). Siegel goes on to define experi- chotherapy (Brown & Ryan, 2003; Germer,
ence as being made up of two aspects: the 2004) because impaired integration is at the root
subjective mental state (the feeling of fear, for of impairments to mental health. Mindfulness, as
220 J. Davis-Siegel et al.

an integrative practice, finds efficacy across a and our interpersonal relationships. Studies of
range of mental health issues (Siegel, 2010a, the epigenetic control of the genetic expression
2010b, 2012a, 2012b). Current research has regulating disease prevention (Fredrickson et al.,
focused on integrating mindfulness into psycho- 2013) have found that our eudaemonic state of
therapeutic interventions. This trend can be seen mind, that has been shown to be created with
as early as 1990 in Kabat-Zinns work with mindfulness practice (Urry et al., 2004), is
MBSR, as well as in Marsha Linehans 1993 directly optimized by how our minds function.
work treating patients with Dialectical- Body, mind, and relationships are three aspects
Behavioral Therapy. Segal, Williams, and of one reality, like the heads, tails, and sides of a
Teasdale (2002) explained how mindfulness- coin. That one reality is energy and information
based treatment was effective in preventing flow. When that flow is integrated through the
relapse in chronic depression. Siegel (2010a) dis- practice of mindfulness, body, mind, and rela-
cusses how physicians who are more mindful of tionships thrive.
themselves and others are more attuned to their
patients. Their patients recover more quickly
from common and mild illnesses and have more An Expanded View of Self
robust immune functions to fight infections and Self-Regulation
(Barrett et al., 2008). In addition to merely aug-
menting the doctor-patient connection, a clinical Defining the mind as a regulatory self-organizing
intervention study with cancer patients demon- process, allows the definitions of the mind and
strated correlations between increases in mind- the self to extend beyond the boundaries of the
fulness over time and declines in mood skin. Thus, as the mind and self extend beyond
disturbance and stress (Brown & Ryan, 2003, the individual level, self-regulation and mindful-
p. 843). This research paved the way for the study ness also have impacts on an expanded level.
of the relationship between mindfulness and This view can be embraced by a range of disci-
health (Coelho et al., 2007; Goldin & Gross, plines, enabling us to create a consilient frame-
2010). As academia has begun to embrace the work that blends insights from anthropological
benefits of mindfulness-based therapy, empirical studies of culture with neurobiological research
study on this subject continues to grow. The on brain function. Energy and information flow
research has shown that mindfulness integrates are the essential features of mental life, whether
the body, the mind, and our social relationships this life is viewed on the neurobiological, psy-
with positive effects on health. chological, relational, or societal level of
As discussed above, integration is seen as the analysis.
heart of health. Impairments to health lead to The self is an outcome of mental life which
states of chaos or rigidity, which can be seen in a occurs both within a person and between multiple
careful analysis of symptoms in each disorder people. Similarly, the minds creation of a sense
listed in the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric of self is also not constrained by the somatic
Association [APA], 2000). As the mind is both world from which it partly emanates. We, as
embodied and relational, these integrative men- selves, emerge both from within the body and
tal states shape both our bodily health (telomer- between the body and its interactions with others
ase levels (Jacobs et al., 2011) and immune and the environment. For simplicity, we shall call
function (Davidson et al., 2003)). With energy these interactions relationships. An example of
and information our fundamental element of how the self expands from the personal, inner
the system of the mind, we can see how the sepa- individual to the interactive, extended world of
ration of body from mind, or mind from rela- relationships is an expectant mother. The new life
tional world is artificial. The mind emerges from starts off as a cluster of cells completely con-
and shapes both our inner physiological states tained within the mother, which eventually grows
16 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation: A Medical Approach to the Mind and Mental Health 221

to become a baby. When the mother and child are Practicing mindfulness is one way to bring har-
separated, and the umbilical cord is cut, the sense mony to the chaos and flexibility to the rigidity.
of their unified relational self is not lost with the Integration created through mindfulness practice
physical separation. Therefore, we can see how thus provides the foundation to ease mental suf-
the self is both related to a relational reality in the fering and supports the development of healthy
womb, and later shaped by a sense of being self-regulation both within and between our-
defined by the body. An infants capacity to regu- selves. Mindfulness practice literally stimulates
late his or her physiology is dependent upon the the growth of the integrative fibers in the brain
relationship with the caregiver, and thus self- that support adaptive regulation at the heart of
regulation is a dyadic state at first, which ulti- health (Siegel, 2007a, 2007b).
mately develops an additional more autonomous
capacity as the infant matures into a young child
(Siegel, 2012a). Compassion and Mindsight
This adjustment is generally referred to as self-
regulation, the ability to control thoughts, emo- The ability of one person to sense the internal
tions and behaviors (Lightfoot et al., 2009) by experience of another and to attune to his/her
monitoring and attuning to them, and is the funda- internal state is the root of both empathy and
mental way in which we create coordination and compassion. These terms have a range of defini-
balance of the systems within and between us tions that embrace concepts such as cognitive and
from which the self emerges. These include inter- emotional understanding and caring for and feel-
nal bodily systems, such as the cardiovascular, ing with another. Studies have revealed how the
immune, endocrine, and nervous systems, as well capacity to see the mind of another has impor-
as mental and social systems that modify psycho- tant correlations with personal and relational
logical processes and intrapersonal interactions. well-being (Siegel, 2012a). Just as in practicing
Ultimately, coordination and balance within sys- mindfulness, where we attune to our individual
temsboth internal physiology and interpersonal internal experience, compassion and empathy use
relationshipsarises from integration, the link- the same principles and practices and apply them
age of different aspects of a system (Siegel, to others. In this sense, the ability to empathize
2012b). Within the body, the coordination and and practice compassion is mindfulness extended
balance of differentiated elements of these physi- beyond the individual persons inner subjective
ological and neurological systems emerge from experience and body to the relational self and
integrative processes. Just as with our interper- relational mind.
sonal relationships, our bodies combine separate In interpersonal neurobiology, the ability to
processes so that homeostasis is maintained. mindfully sense the internal subjective experi-
Integration, the linkage of differentiated parts, is ence of the self and others, to have insight into
the core mechanism beneath adaptive regulation ones own inner life, and to understand and care
and health (Siegel, 2012a, 2012b). for the internal life of another is called mind-
The placement of self and mind concep- sight. Mindsight is a learnable skill that allows
tually in both embodied and relational systems, not only the perception of the internal mental
permits us to explore the notion of health as an state of the individual as well as others, but also
example of optimally functioning integration. the regulation of the self or others towards inte-
When integration is not achieved, chaos and gration and well-being. Mindsight involves
rigidity emerge (Siegel, 2012b). In the case of insight, empathy, and integration. Mindsight has
mental illness, these two conditions can be seen been described as the core mechanism beneath
as the common ground of all symptoms of each emotional and social intelligence (Goleman,
of the major psychiatric disorders listed in the foreword: Siegel, 2010b) and embraces the way
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental we can learn to become mindfully aware of the
Disorders (DSM IV-TR) (Siegel, 2012b). present moment.
222 J. Davis-Siegel et al.

The capacity for mindsight appears to be a requires cooperation between body and mind to
part of our human legacy that is shaped by our be well regulated, a relational self can only func-
experiences (Siegel, 2010a). Mindfulness would tion optimally when there is kindness and com-
be one example of a mindsight practice as it passion between these interacting social systems.
involves insight, empathy and integration. Here is In a medical approach to self-regulation, kind-
the basic notion of how mindful awareness may ness and compassion are essential for healthy
catalyze integration in the brain. Mindfulness regulation to emerge on the relational level.
training allows an individual to develop the Mindfulness practice supports the maintenance
interoceptive monitoring capacity to be aware of of compassionate care and empathy in primary
his/her inner world (Siegel, 2007a). Within this care physicians (Krasner et al., 2009) and can
practice, two distinct neural circuits become dif- also be seen as the heart of remaining present in
ferentiated and then linked (Farb et al., 2007; interpersonal relationships (Parker et al., in
Siegel, 2007b). One is the sensing circuit; the press). Such presence has been shown to elevate
other is the observing circuit. These circuits the levels of telomerase, the enzyme that repairs
enable direct sensory experience and the capacity and maintains the ends of chromosomes to
for observation that allows us to be a witness to improve cellular health and longevity (Jacobs
and narrator of our experiences The linkage of et al., 2009). Therefore, we see that there are not
these differentiated circuits enables us to keep only psychological benefits to mindfulness prac-
track of tasks by paying attention to intentions tices but physiological ones as well.
and being aware of awareness, the two funda- Compassion emerges from the relational self.
mental processes of all mindfulness approaches It is a process that speaks to a very basic concept
(Siegel, 2007a). These aspects of self-monitoring within humanity, one of bringing out the best in
redirect attention and strengthen modifying all of us and forges intimate, healing relation-
skills, which are the two aspects of the regulatory ships, expanding and strengthening the self. Peter
function of the mind. Mindfulness essentially Hosking explained several of its modern denota-
catalyzes the integration necessary for effective tions in his 2007 essay Compassion: What is in
self-regulation. Thus, mindfulness is a form of a Word?:
self-regulation training that strengthens both The word compassion is defined in various
mind and self. but similar ways as: (a) a feeling of sorrow or pity
In many ways, an outcome of self-regulation for the pain or misfortunes of another that inclines
and the pinnacle of integration is kindness and one to help; (b) feeling deep sympathy for anoth-
compassion. When the internal state of oneself is ers suffering or misfortune accompanied by a
connected to another, compassion occurs and the desire to alleviate the pain and remove its cause;
self broadens. Accepting the vulnerability in one- (c) the feeling of emotion when one is moved by
self and others, by expressing care and concern in the suffering of another and by the need to relieve
a way that alleviates their suffering, is the core of it, and (d) sympathetic consciousness of anoth-
kindness. At the heart of these positive states is ers distress with a desire to alleviate it.
the process of integration, which is how we (Hosking, 2007, p. 2)
respect one anothers differences and promote While Hoskings comments allow us to derive
compassionate, caring linkages. Kindness and a definition of the word compassion, the process
compassion emerge from integration between goes beyond this. Compassion is something we
individuals, a process that expands the experience can do, something we can feel, and something we
of self from merely the individual, to the individ- can receive. By alleviating suffering in others, we
ual in relationship with the other. This is the are regulating our relational self: a process that
source of coordination and balance in the relation- by its very nature requires mindful awareness of
ship that is generated by self-regulation on the others emotional states.
broader level of the relational self. Just as the tra- Several studies have been conducted which
ditional experience of self as an individual address the phenomenon of compassion and
16 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation: A Medical Approach to the Mind and Mental Health 223

offer us several definitions of the concept. A


study by Wray-Lake, Flanagan, and Maggs Empathy and Neurobiology
(2011) operationalized their definition when
comparing messages communicating values to Another key aspect of regulating the relational
children. They defined compassion messages as self is empathy, which is the necessary first stage
any message that emphasized caring for others. in having compassion and understanding between
In looking at this operational definition, it is individuals. If we are to move towards alleviating
important to note that compassion requires more the suffering we see in others, we must first be
than just a feeling or a desire, as is implied by able to understand what this suffering is; this
Hoskings definitions; it requires (or at least requires empathy and at least some form of mind-
results in) constructive action. Another study, ful awareness of others emotional experiences.
by Pauley and McPherson (2010), took this idea This basic form of monitoring the different parts
further, exploring what it means to experience of the relational self is essential for mindfulness
compassion and self-compassion. Through a on the relational domain and is embedded in the
series of interviews, the authors derived a defi- neural circuitry of the brain. Thus, there is neuro-
nition of compassion that came from the partici- biological basis for empathy which enables the
pants everyday understanding of the word. It expansion of self and allows us to be mindful of
was determined that compassion is a kind and others which will be discussed in this section.
active process that has subordinate themes of Empathy has been defined as the recognition
(1) being kind towards people and (2) requiring and sharing of anothers emotional state,
action. (Preston & de Waal, 2002), which links (and thus
Both these studies suggest that compassion is expands) the self as an individual to the self as a
an action, yet it is also a relationship. Compassion relationship. There is a subtle difference between
requires us to listen to the story of another with a empathy and compassion; empathy does not
willingness to allow ourselves to be affected require action as compassion does, and is only
(Hosking, 2007, p. 8). In order to truly be the recognition and experience of emotions in
affected, we must broaden our sense of self and others. It is perceiving and experiencing the emo-
truly identify with others, not only as separate tions of those around us. When we see someone
people but as extensions (or even other parts) of crying or injured, our chest tightens and our heart
ourselves. In this way, compassion requires rate increases. We cringe when we hear a bone
empathythe ability to connect with the internal break in a movie; we flinch at the sound of a
experience of others. As we experience compas- cracking whip during a play. Even in situations
sion both for and from others, we move towards a where we know the emotions we see and hear are
mindful understanding of and integration with just dramatizations, we still feel a bodily response
that other individual. Thus, we are differentiated to portrayed fear, disgust, anger, and even happi-
and we are linked. This is integration on the inter- ness. Thus, we see that in any healthy relation-
personal level. Therefore, just as practicing ship we are mindful of the emotional states of
mindfulness results in a more attuned self, those around us and seek to soothe (or regulate)
accepting the sensing self-circuit by the observ- our relational self by understanding what the
ing self-circuit, compassion visibly engages the other part of that extended self is feeling by
relational self through action to promote interper- empathizing (Decety & Ickes, 2009).
sonal attunement and the seeing and acceptance Empathy works to expand and regulate the
of the internal state of another (Gilbert, 2009; relational self, and is an essential mechanism in
Neff, Kirkpatrick, & Rude, 2007). In many ways, psychosocial interaction; there is evidence that it
mindful awareness promotes both internal attun- is hardwired into the neural circuitry. The neuro-
ement and interpersonal attunement, each a form biological system of empathy consists of the lim-
of the integration of energy and information flow bic system (primarily the amygdala and the
within us, and between us. insula) in conjunction with the anterior cingulate
224 J. Davis-Siegel et al.

cortex and the prefrontal cortex, which are all The next important part of the empathy sys-
involved in imitation and in the activation of pain tem for us to address is oxytocin. Also known as
circuitry in oneself when observing another in the love hormone, the neuropeptide oxytocin is
pain (Jackson, Brunet, Meltzoff, & Decety, released from the posterior pituitary after orgasm
2006). The limbic system is the primary brain and induces feelings of closeness that support
region for emotional processing and the amyg- pair-bonding (Ditzen, Schaer, & Gabriel, 2009;
dala in particular is activated by fear and stress as Ross & Young, 2009). Barraza and Zak (2009)
a part of the hypothalamicpituitaryadrenal studied oxytocin levels in the context of emotion-
(HPA) axis which controls fight or flight or ally provocative videos and a philanthropic exer-
freeze response as well as mediating brain cise to see if the effect of oxytocin extends to
responses to emotional ambiguity and arousal. empathic action. They discovered a link between
The insula carries information between the cor- a self-reported empathy after watching an emo-
tex and subcortical regions and the body (Craig, tional video and higher cortisol levels as well as
2009). Besides regulating negative arousal by higher self-reported distress. Oxytocin levels
controlling information flow between the thala- increased in response to greater self-reported
mus and the limbic system, some studies suggest empathy, supporting the general notion that oxy-
that the insula also mediates negative emotional tocin is released as a part of the empathic
arousal (disgust, pain, and hunger) (Berntston, experience.
Norman, & Bechara, 2011; Gray, Harrison, & Although research in this field still continues,
Wiens, 2007). The anterior cingulate cortex is it is clear that oxytocin plays a role in enhancing
activated by social and physical distress as well empathic and compassionate functions of the
as error and conflict processing (Shirtcliff et al., brain (Bora, Yucel, & Allen, 2009). When oxyto-
2009). In fact, Lieberman and Eisenberger (2005) cin is given, generosity and empathy increase
have demonstrated that the anterior cingulate (Bartz et al., 2010). Oxytocin levels have a direct
mediates both the registration of bodily pain as correlation with trust and positive emotions and
well as the pain of social rejection. We therefore an inverse correlation to anxiety and other nega-
have the capability to respond empathetically to tive emotions (Ditzen et al., 2009). Oxytocin
both kinds of pain from the same neural circuit. given intranasally has been shown to increase the
These are the neurological mechanisms revealing amount of time participants spend gazing at faces
how our minds experience an individual bodily (Guastella, Mitchell, & Dadds, 2008); further
and a relational level of emergence. studies have shown that given intravenously, oxy-
The HPA axis also plays a role in the empa- tocin increases the ability of individuals to infer
thy network. When functioning normally, the mental states from the eyes accurately (Bartz
hypothalamus is stimulated by incoming sig- et al, 2010). Bora and colleagues (2009) state,
nals from the amygdala, which are triggered by oxytocin induces its behavioral effects, such
seeing someone in distress or experiencing dis- as promoting attachment and trust, by modulat-
tress directly (Buchanan, Bagley, & Stansfield, ing affiliation-related brain networks and inhibit-
2012). The hypothalamus, via corticotropin ing brain regions associated with aversive
releasing hormone, triggers the pituitary gland behavior (p. 322). By this, we see how the
to stimulate the adrenal cortex via adrenocorto- release of oxytocin in the brain, is a chemical
cotropic hormone (ACTH) (Kudielka, form of self-regulation which has the behavioral
Schommer, & Hellhammer, 2004). Finally, the (and relational) effects of better integrating the
adrenal cortex produces a stress-related hor- broader self and mind.
mone, cortisol, which stimulates a cascade of Much of this internalization of another per-
physiological changes, making it easier for one sons emotional state is accomplished with a
to run from danger or attack a predator neurological process called mirroring which may
(Kudielka et al., 2004). be mediated by a system of mirror neurons
16 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation: A Medical Approach to the Mind and Mental Health 225

(Pfeifer, Iacoboni, Mazziota, & Dapretto, 2008). Psychopaths and Antisocial


A mirror neuron is characterized by the ability of Personality Disorder
a single neuron to have both perceptual and motor
functions. Scientists have now discovered that In the previous sections we discussed mindfulness,
mirror neurons may play a role in the empathic self-regulation, compassion, and empathy. We turn
process. First, anterior and parietal mirror neuron now to their antithesispsychopathyin order to
regions perceive physical signals from others and further explore the processes of self-regulation and
anticipate the immediate next motion of the oth- mindful awareness. There is some debate about
ers action. This process then stimulates non-mir- what it means to be psychopathic, but what we see
ror areas in the superior temporal sulcus in in nearly all definitions of the word, is an extremely
conjunction with mirror neuron areas. This syn- limited form of self and the inability to relate and
chronous stimulation enables a neural representa- give value to the minds of others. We see in psy-
tion of the anticipated action of another to be chopaths a lack of self-regulation on the individual
mapped out (Simon, Herold, & Fekete, 2007). level as well as a complete disregard for any notion
This mapping is postulated to be the neural basis of a relational self or mind. As we explore in this
for representing anothers intention, or the mental section, the result of this type of personality is often
state of the other that drives the direction of unhealthy and dysfunctional social and emotional
motion (Simon et al., 2007). Next, mirror neu- relationships that appear to be deficient in mindful
rons engage the anterior insular cortex to drive awareness of both others and themselves.
cortical firings downward into subcortical limbic, Psychopaths are often portrayed in popular
brainstem, and bodily regions to enable us to culture as the embodiment of evil, having nearly
resonate within our bodies with what we see in superhuman abilities. This notion is supported by
others (Buk, 2009; Pfeifer et al., 2008). the etymological roots of the word which translate
This emotional resonance then moves upward, to a sickness/corruption of the soul (Walker,
by way of the right anterior insula, toward vari- 2009). The DSM-IV-TR brings these pseudo-reli-
ous aspects of the prefrontal region (Simon, et al., gious definitions into more scientific terms by
2007). The linkage of these areas to the anterior describing psychopaths as suffering from
cingulate by way of neural spindle cells enables a Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD). ASPD is
fast-acting relay between the interoceptive sig- generally defined as a pervasive pattern of disre-
nals of the insula with the complex integration of gard for, and violation of, the rights of others that
bodily input, emotion, and social interactions begins in childhood or early adolescence and con-
mediated by the cingulate region. Pfiefer and col- tinues into adulthood (APA, 2000). The overall
leagues propose (2009) that this mechanism prevalence of this disorder in the US general pop-
underlies how bodily states activated by mirror ulation is around 3 % in men and 1 % in women
neurons transmit impulses to the insula and then (APA, 2000). The degree of psychopathy a person
to the medial prefrontal regions which translate suffers from can be determined quantitatively by
what is going on in me? to the empathic query using Hares Psychopathy Checklist-Revised
of what may be going on in you? We come to which quantifies the disorder into the 20 features
feel what is happening in someone else because of a psychopath (Haycock, 2012).
of the resonant feelings we experience in our own By its very definition in the DSM-IV-TR,
worlds. Thus, we see how on a basic level, the Antisocial Personality Disorder is associated with
brain and the mind and the self all interact to give criminal and callous actions. Although only twenty
us a more mindful understanding of our sur- percent of criminals qualify as psychopathic, this
roundings and the emotional experiences of oth- group accounts for fifty percent of the most severe
ers in those surroundings; as well as providing us crimes as well as comprising over half of serial
with tools for a more integrated relational mind murderers and rapists (Hare, 1993, pp. 2530).
and a better regulated self. Psychopaths lack functioning in critical areas that
226 J. Davis-Siegel et al.

control basic empathic functions which supports cortex (PFC) generally regulates emotional
the commonly held belief that psychopathic indi- responses and mood in typical individuals and
viduals are not compassionate or empathicmak- also is the area of the brain where complex cogni-
ing them callous and egotistical. tive patterns such as making plans to solve prob-
Shirtcliff and his research team (2009) sought to lems and resolve conflicts occur. Within the PFC
explain the neurobiology of empathy by retrospec- is the orbitofrontal cortex, which is intimately tied
tively analyzing studies on psychopathy and brain to the amygdala and promotes both stimulus and
activity/anatomy. He defines psychopathy as a reinforcement learning. When the amygdala is
combination of a failure to respond to the distress hyporesponsive, as it is in psychopathic individu-
cues of others, impulsivity, and a lack of emo- als, one theory proposes that the orbitofrontal cor-
tional responsiveness (Blair et al., 2004). In his tex is also less active because it has no emotional
research, Shirtcliff looked for a lack of empathy stimulation with which to form emotional memo-
and its manifest criminal behaviors in psychopaths. ries (Blair et al., 2004). The ventromedial PFC,
The psychopathic model, he posited, cannot be another area important to the empathy system and
purely viewed as an expression of empathy shut- our capacity to have theory of mind, integrates
down but as a more complex disordered emotional emotional information from the amygdala during
functioning leading to aberrant social actions. decision-making tasks so that people respond to
While this information is helpful in determin- emotional as well as physical cues (Koenigs et al.,
ing if and to what severity people are psycho- 2007). However, these typical processes are not
pathic, it says nothing about how psychopathy the case in people with ASPD.
develops. Explanations as to why this disorder Circulating cortisol levels in psychopaths are
exists in an individual draw from a wide variety much lower than in non-psychopathic criminals,
of psychological sources ranging from biological suggesting a biological underproduction of this
to social. Recent biological studies of ASPD have distress hormone secreted from the adrenal
shown that people classified as antisocial, espe- cortex rather than an over-exposure to it in child-
cially those who are unusually hostile and rash, hood (Cima, Smeets, & Jelicic, 2008). As corti-
display lower serotonin activity than the typical sol has been linked to feelings of stress and
individual. Other studies further indicate people anxiety, lower cortisol levels may be related to
suffering from antisocial personality disorder both the psychopaths propensity toward risk-
display lower activity in their frontal lobes taking and imperviousness to the distress of oth-
(Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000). Serotonin dysfunc- ers. Normal empathy, thus, relies upon a healthy
tion is also one of the most researched possible level of stress upon seeing anothers distress
mechanisms proposed to be a part of major (Eisenberg, 2007). Psychopaths are impaired in
depression, but its exact role in ASPD is still this response and therefore lack the ability to
unclear. Other research reveals that people with experience empathy.
antisocial personality disorder respond to warn- Individuals expressing high levels of callous-
ings or expectations of stress with low brain and ness and lack of emotion display a decrease in
bodily arousal, such as slow autonomic arousal amygdala activity in conjunction with reduced
and slow EEG waves, making them unaffected anterior cingulate and insula functioning, which
by threatening or emotional situations (Comer, has been noted in studies of cooperation between
2010, p. 522). This biological difference could tested psychopaths. The amygdala in healthy
explain why antisocial individuals are more individuals tends to be active in response to ones
likely to take risks and seek thrills: they are com- own fear and to fear in others, as measured by
pensating for their under-arousal. amygdala activation when participants see
Brain abnormalities inherent to psychopathy images of fearful facial expressions. In psycho-
are located in the frontal and prefrontal cortex, paths, the amygdala responds in the same way to
where emotional signals from the limbic system neutral facial expressions as it does to fearful
are integrated and lead to behavior. The prefrontal facial expressions (Blair et al, 2004). Shirtcliff
16 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation: A Medical Approach to the Mind and Mental Health 227

et al. (2009) posits that psychopathy involves an which draws from these central concepts: moral-
inability to empathize with fear in others because ity. A sense of morality not only stems from the
psychopaths do not experience fear themselves. ability to empathize, but also relies on an indi-
Other researchers believe that much of what viduals theory of mind (ToM) (defined in a pre-
lies at the root of ASPD stems from social and vious section) which speaks to viewing the mind
cognitive problems. Comer (2010) cites Sperry as an interpersonal regulatory process and more
(2003) in his arguments that the absence of generally as a single relational mind between
parental love during infancy, leading to a lack of individuals. Theory of mind functions when a
basic trust is one social and developmental person decides on a course of action based on the
explanation for psychopathy. Comer (2010), citing perception of another persons predicted behav-
Martens (2005) and Paris (2001), then goes on: ior. This is mindfulness on a relational levelas
In this view some childrenthe ones who develop an individual becomes part of a larger self and is
antisocial personality disorderrespond to their mindful of another part of that selfs thoughts,
early inadequacies by becoming emotionally dis- feelings and state of mind before taking a course
tant, and they bond with others through the use of of action.
power and destructiveness. In support of this psy-
chodynamic explanation, researchers have found As a prosocial species, humans need ToM in
that people with this disorder are more likely than order to respond appropriately in a group setting
others to have had significant stress in their child- to both nonverbal and perceived cues. ToM also
hoods, particularly in such forms as family pov- includes applying anothers potential actions to a
erty, family violence, and parental conflict or
divorce. (p. 521) framework of cultural edicts that give context to
prediction. ToM allows us to empathize with oth-
One can sense that psychopathy is an example ers by understanding and appreciating what
of impaired self-regulation and is a limiting per- another person is feeling such that one can envi-
spective of the perceived self. Psychopaths lack sion oneself as the other person emotionally:
the ability to engage in interpersonal joining and this capacity is the essence of both empathy and
thus limit their sense of self to an identity only relational mindfulness. Empathy, combined with
defined by the boundaries of their skin. With a ToM and cultural norms of compassionate behav-
diminished arousal in response to the affective ior (stemming from mindfulness on the relational
states of others that make emotional understand- level), forms the basis for personal morality.
ing available in a healthy brain and mind, psy- Culture alone is not enough to dictate what is a
chopaths lack the ability to give meaning to the right or wrong way to treat another; one needs to
minds of others. Here we see a failing on the part be able to embody another persons emotional
of the psychopath to take a mindful perspective and logical perspectives in order to decide
of others, and of himself, and this results in whether he/she would act similarly in the same
extremely individualistic behavior. One aspect of situation. As this applies to general rules of
self-regulation that is not present in this condi- behavior across all members of a culture, or a
tion, and fundamental to how we live in our social single, distributed, cultural self, we must be able
worlds, is how we regulate our behavior. This to relate to and be mindful of potential actions of
regulation of action for the greater good in the all parts affected. Thus we see that the relational
context of societal living is called morality. mind of a society acts as a regulatory process
which governs individual actions based on moral
imagination, reasoning, decision-making, and
Self-Regulation and the Biology actions; and the only healthy way to regulate
of Morality actions on such a broad level requires mindful-
ness of the feelings and situations of all individu-
From the above discussions of empathy, compas- als that make up the extended cultural self. This
sion and the lack thereof in psychopathy, it seems is self-regulation on the cultural level that
natural that we now turn our attention to a topic embeds integrationthe honoring of differences
228 J. Davis-Siegel et al.

and the promotion of compassionate linkages When this paradigm is coupled with MRI
that enables harmony to emerge within a com- testing, the limbic system as well as the orbito-
munity or a society. frontal and the anterior cingulate cortex activate,
On an individual level, theory of mind is the indicating the importance of emotion, executive
basis for what can be developed into a relational function, and empathy in deciding what moral
mind. Theory of mind begins to develop in early action to engage in (Aoki, Funane, & Koizumi,
childhood and can be tested through a simple 2010). Furthermore, when participants read
false beliefs task (Lightfoot et al., 2009). In the descriptions in the first person of moral trans-
false belief task, a child watches a scene in which gressions committed on purpose, there are high
a character plays with a toy and then leaves the levels of activity in their amygdala. This is con-
room. While the character is absent, the location trasted by the fact that the amygdala is not acti-
of the toy is changed by a third party. The charac- vated significantly over baseline when
ter then reenters the room and the scene is paused. participants read scenarios in which the moral
At this point the child is asked where the character transgression is committed by them by accident
thinks the object is. If ToM is developed, the or when the descriptions are in third person, indi-
child will respond that the toy is in the initial loca- cating that another person is responsible in the
tion rather than the new location; if it is not how- hypothetical situation (Berthoz, Grezes, &
ever, the child will assume that the character has Armory, 2006). Amygdala involvement, in either
all the same information that he/she has and will case, leads to a strong negative emotional reac-
answer that it is in the new location. The ability to tion to the implication that one hypothetically
understand why other people act the way that they committed a crime that is known as immoral and
do in certain situations, sets the stage for us to ask non-empathetic. Here we see a self-regulating
ourselves which actions (and their accompanying response in the form of an emotional/neurologi-
preceding situational factors) are allowable and cal reaction in a single person to an action dis-
forgivable. This, when widened to the societal rupting the homeostasis of the relational self.
level, then leads to a universal code of behavior To capture a spectrum of values rather than a
among people with whom we identify and under- binary yes-no of moral decisions, scales began
stand by being part of the relational self; morality to be used for rating moral blameworthiness
is merely a very broad concept of theory of mind versus praiseworthiness of actions. Participants
where the mind in question is one of a relational read statements that are either morally upright
self, comprised of all interconnected and interde- or represent depravity and have to rate the
pendent members of that society. degree of praiseworthiness or blameworthiness
In 1985, Thomson came up with a basic para- for each statement. Praiseworthiness was
digm by which morality could be assessed. The shown to be associated with increased activity
paradigm asks participants to imagine standing in the orbitofrontal cortex and blameworthiness
next to a stranger on a bridge which is over a train with activity in the superior temporal sulcus
track. The trains brakes fail and it is careening (Takahashi et al., 2008). The orbitofrontal cor-
towards a wall; all five people on the train will be tex is involved both in facial processing and the
killed unless you push the stranger next to you pleasure response to seeing a person one con-
onto the path of the train to slow it down, killing siders attractive; its similar activation in
him/her. Participants are asked: would you kill response to a moral action may be due to a con-
one person to save five? Most participants say no ditioned social response of pleasure to admira-
even though the rational decision would be to ble behavior (Takahashi et al., 2008). This
consider one life lost worth the cost of saving region plays a key role in creating maps of oth-
five. The act of killing is so instinctually antago- ers intentions by processing social cues such as
nistic to empathic and social sensibilities that the gesture, posture, and facial expressions. These
rational application of an instrumental ToM loses regions act as agents supporting ToM process-
out to the emotional one. ing that extends beyond verbal communication
16 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation: A Medical Approach to the Mind and Mental Health 229

(Iacoboni, 2009; Simon et al., 2007) allowing (p. 546). A facet of self-regulation, impulse con-
us to make moral assessments of situations. trol is a mental function that significantly
As such, a very important part of moral judg- increases during puberty and continues to grow
ment appears to lie in the intent of an action throughout emerging adulthood. This mental
rather than only its outcome. The outcome ver- space or pause is an essential element in emo-
sus intent calculation guides the sentencing of tional intelligence (Goleman, 1996; Siegel,
many serious and petty crimes in the USA as 2010a). Impulse control has roots in both biologi-
well as in other countries (Takahashi et al., cal processes, such as brain development (mainly
2008). The use of character witnesses and psy- the frontal lobe), and social contexts, such as
chological assessments in the adjudication of interpersonally mediated expectations for soci-
crimes attests to the fact that intent and empathic etal and family obligations and interactions
potential hold the key to perceived responsibility (Lightfoot et al., 2009). As regulation entails
for immoral action. When presented with four monitoring and modifying, we can view impulse
scenarios of outcome versus intent, the temporo- control as having both internal and interper-
parietal junction of the brain, or the seat of ToM sonal levels of self-regulation. Thus, impulse
(Saxe, 2006; Young, Camprodon, Hauser, control requires mindful monitoring and modu-
Pascual-Leone, & Saxe, 2010), is activated in lating of energy and information flow in both the
the two options where guilt is unclear: when intrapersonal and interpersonal processes of
someone commits a crime without intent or selves, leading to regulation on the individual
someone intends to commit a crime and fails and relational levels.
(Young et al., 2010). When in the grey zone of In a closer examination of impulse control, let
morality, our ToM and mindfulness of others us first discuss the biological processes that are
situations and intentions puts us in the shoes of related to its development. As children progress
the alleged criminal so that we can balance intent into adolescence, there are two significant pro-
and outcome when deciding whether an action is cesses that occur with regards to emotional regu-
wrong. However, when it comes to our own lation. One is that levels of the reward
behavioral output, our ability to act morally with neurotransmitter, dopamine, shifts in ways that
self-regulation in the face of intense stress may promote a drive toward novelty and risk-
requires impulse control. taking behavior and be a part of something called
hyper-rational thinking in which the limbic
regions seem to place more evaluative weight on
Impulse Control the potential positive outcomes of an action and
de-emphasize the risks such that the reasoning
Impulse control is the psychological mechanism cortex decides to engage in risk-taking behav-
related to decision-making that balances short- iors (Siegel, 2013). The second process is the
term benefits with long-term costs. The regula- onset of brain remodeling, which continues on
tion of behavior requires people to put a temporal through late adolescence into the mid-twenties
and functional space between impulse (or urges (Pharo, Sim, Graham, Gross, & Hayne, 2011).
with high emotional payoffs) and action in order The presence of neural reconstruction in the inte-
to avoid unsatisfactory consequences in the short grative prefrontal regions responsible for impulse
or in the long run. In order to effectively control control may, in part, explain why young adoles-
ones impulses, self-regulation and mindful cents have more trouble controlling impulsive
awareness of emotions and urges are required. behaviors than adults do. This combined with
According to Lightfoot et al. (2009), Impulse shifts in the dopamine reward system and hyper-
control, inhibition, and persistence are among the rational thinking may also be reasons why even
main features of emotional regulation, and they older adolescents, no longer impulsive, engage in
underlie abilities to down-regulate negative dangerous behaviors with serious negative
emotions and up-regulate positive ones consequences.
230 J. Davis-Siegel et al.

As stated above, during adolescence the brain Giedd et al., 1999; Sowell, Thompson, Holms,
goes through its last period of intense transfor- Jernigan, & Toga, 1999) (p. 971)
mation, with the pruning and myelination that While most researchers agree that frontal lobe
ultimately create more differentiation and link- maturity plays a large role in controlling impulses
age, one that involves the prefrontal regions (Pharo et al., 2011), some also believe that the
whose integrative role is responsible for impulse brains limbic system, which is involved with
control and the emergence of more adaptive self- motivation, emotion, and memory processes is
regulation. The healthy maturation of the brain also a contributing factor (Gerrig & Zimbardo,
toward integration in the adolescent years sup- 2008) to maturity. Pharo et al. (2011) write,
ports the ability to control potentially harmful under conditions of high emotional arousal,
impulses and think in ways that lead to more bal- adolescents may be more likely to engage in risky
anced evaluative capacities to carry out less behavior because their limbic system takes over
risky behaviors. Impulse control and more inte- and they do not have the sufficient prefrontal cor-
grated thinking are vital for competent adults tical mechanisms in place to suppress their
self-regulation in any culture. The prefrontal behavior. Without the inhibitory mechanisms of
cortexs central role in coordinating and balanc- the prefrontal cortex, the adolescents are driven
ing a wide array of differentiated regions into an by the motivating and emotional signals from the
integrated whole continues to develop through limbic system, and may at times engage in risky
adolescence and is a neural capacity important behavior that is not rationally and completely
for decision-making, inhibition of thoughts and thought through. Such reasoning depends on an
behavior, and the regulation of affect or emo- engaged prefrontal cortex that enables neural
tional expressions (Bechara, Tranel, & Damasio, integration to coordinate and balance many areas
2000; Passingham, 1993; Reyna, Chapman, into a functional whole, which is not present in
Dougherty, & Confrey, 2012; Romer, 2010; early adolescence. Future research is needed to
Spear, 2010; Stuss & Knight, 2002). The orbito- support this proposal, but from a practical view-
frontal aspect of the prefrontal cortex is espe- point, teaching mindsight skills through mindful-
cially important for balancing risk and reward ness training theoretically may help support the
and shaping our response to impulses (Kelley, growth of integrative capacities that can help
Schochet, & Landry, 2004). Second only to increase self-awareness and prefrontal capacities
infancy, adolescence is the most active period of that can promote more optimal choices driven by
growth in human development, and conse- an adolescents positive values and innate but
quently a period of high vulnerability to toxic often untapped drive to move toward health
substances that can chemically impede healthy (Siegel, 2013).
development; yet adolescents commonly partici- Certainly biological processes play a signifi-
pate in high risk activities, including physically cant role in the developing adolescents situa-
dangerous behaviors and the excessive use of tional assessment and decision-making process,
psychoactive substances (Spear, 2010). but perhaps equally significant are social factors.
Pharo and colleagues (2011) expand on this Adolescence is a time of broadening capability
concept and offer a biological explanation: and autonomy, and teenagers will often test the
[The] prefrontal cortex is a critical compo- boundaries of their new-found aptitudes. Often,
nent in performing and maintaining inhibitory this testing means new opportunities for risky
behaviors and thoughts (Kelley et al., 2004; Dahl, acting on impulse.
2004; Spear, 2010). Although adolescents con- Beyond the adolescent years, examples of
ceptually understand the risks associated with impaired impulse control are common in a range
their behavior by the age of 14, the inhibitory of psychiatric conditions that are not a part of
mechanisms required to resist those risky behav- typical development. Regulation of impulses is
iors are not equivalent to that of adults until impaired in mental disorders such as pyromania
approximately 20 years of age (Giedd, 2004; (the deliberate and repeated setting of fires to
16 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation: A Medical Approach to the Mind and Mental Health 231

achieve intense pleasure or relief from tension), and integration. In our examination of the self
kleptomania (a recurrent failure to resist the and mind, we discovered that these two entities
impulse to steal), intermittent explosive disorder are in many ways a tautology: two terms that rep-
(having periodic aggressive outbursts in which resent overlapping concepts and core processes.
the individual may seriously attack people and We have defined the mind as a process that regu-
destroy property), trichotillomania (repeatedly lates the flow of information and, like the self, is
plucking hair from various parts of the individu- both embodied within the skin and extends
als bodies, particularly the scalp), and pathologi- between individuals. Ultimately, a healthy, rela-
cal gambling (the persistent and repeated tional view of the self and mind reveals how the
gambling behavior that disrupts ones life at process of integration results in both well-being
home or at work). and in the cultivation of kindness and compas-
The introduction of mindfulness training may sion. In this way, healthy self-regulation would
enhance present functioning and cultivate healthy be seen to involve self-compassion and other-
development in many people. Mindfulness prac- directed compassion as well. When such integra-
tices can be seen as a way of creating the space tive states do not arise in the course of
of the mind in which individuals learn to focus development, a constricted self emerges that is
attention on their intention and become more prone to the antithesis of compassion: psychopa-
aware of the experience of being aware (Siegel, thy. The key to cultivating a healthy and intercon-
2007a). This mind training supports the increased nected self and mind, and ultimately maximizing
ability to hold the contents of awareness in the compassion, is self-regulation. Mindfulness
form of thoughts, emotions, impulses, memories, offers one approach to creating the internal and
and images. This enhanced stability of awareness the interpersonal foundations for integration at
can permit a mental space to be created between the heart of healthy self-regulation. When behav-
impulse and action. Holding an internal sensation iors are enacted with others minds in mind,
to actan impulsein mind but inhibiting its when a sense of the greater good is embedded in
execution as a behavioral action is the heart of the intention driving our actions, we call such
impulse control. Mindfulness directly shapes the reasoning and behavior, moral. Our thesis in this
functioning of the mind to enhance this important chapter is that self-regulation is interwoven with
aspect of self-regulation. Research now indicates morality because the self and the mind are both
that impulse control and emotional regulation are embodied as well as relational processes. The
traits that can be taught and learned as a part of self and mind do not arise in isolation but are cre-
mindfulness training (Davidson & Begley, 2012; ated, in part, from our social embeddedness.
Siegel, 2010a, 2012a). Mindfulness techniques, Regulating the self in an integrated way emerges
which empower the individual to take the time to from a mind that embraces this interdependence
focus his/her attention on his/her intentions, cre- of the self as being a part of the larger wholeit
ates the important pause between impulse and is both embodied and it is relational. Thus we see
action and results in self-control through emo- how a more integrated self and mind not only
tional and behavioral regulation. benefit the individuals mental state of well-being
but serve to enhance society as a whole.

Conclusion
References
In conclusion, the topics discussed in this chap-
ter, fundamental to the field of mental health, American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.,
combine to reveal a complex relationship between
text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
self, brain, and our social world. This view also Aoki, R., Funane, T., & Koizumi, J. (2010). Brain science
provides a road map revealing the importance of of ethics: Present status and the future. Mind Brain
mindfulness in the journey toward well-being and Education, 4, 188195.
232 J. Davis-Siegel et al.

Barraza, J. A., & Zak, P. J. (2009). Empathy towards in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness
strangers triggers oxytocin release and subsequent meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(4), 564570.
generosity. Values, Empathy, and Fairness Across Davidson, R. J., & Begley, S. (2012). The emotional life of
Social Barriers: Annals of the New York Academy of your brain: How its unique patterns affect the way you
Sciences, 1167, 182189. think, feel, and liveAnd how you can change them.
Barrett, B., Rakel, D., Hoeft, T., Chewning, B., Craig, B., New York: Penguin.
& Niu, M. (2008). Practitioner empathy and duration Decety, J., & Ickes, W. J. (2009). The social neuroscience
of the common cold. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, of empathy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
22, 22. Ditzen, B., Schaer, M., & Gabriel, B. (2009). Intranasal
Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., Hollander, E., Ludwig, oxytocin increases positive communication and
N. N., Kolevzon, A., et al. (2010). Oxytocin selec- reduces cortisol levels during couple conflict.
tively improves empathic accuracy. Psychological Biological Psychiatry, 65, 728731.
Science, 21, 14261428. Eisenberg, N. (2007). Empathy-related responding: Links
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2000). with self-regulation, moral judgment, and moral
Characterization of the decision-making deficit of behavior. Herzlyia symposium on personality and
patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. social psychology (pp. 122), American Psychological
Oxford Journals, 123, 21892202. Association (2010), AZ.
Berntston, G. G., Norman, G. J., & Bechara, A. (2011). Farb, N. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon,
The insula and evaluative processes. Psychological D., Fatima, Z., et al. (2007). Attending to the present:
Science, 22, 8086. Mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes
Berthoz, S., Grezes, J., & Armory, J. L. (2006). Affective of self-reference. Social Cognitive and Affective
response to ones own moral violations. NeuroImage, Neuroscience, 2, 313322.
31, 945950. Fredrickson, B. L., Grewen, K. M., Coffeya, K. A., Algoe,
Blair, R. J. R., Mitchell, D. G. V., Peschardt, K. S., S. B., Firestine, A. M., & Ma, J. (2013). A functional
Colledge, E., Leonard, R. A., Shine, J. H., et al. (2004). genomic perspective on human well-being (B. H.
Reduced sensitivity to others fearful expressions in Singer, Ed.). Proceedings of the National Academy of
psychopathic individuals. Personality and Individual Sciences, 110(33), 1368413689. Retrieved from
Differences, 37, 11111122. http://www.pnas.org/search?author1=Barbara+L.+Fre
Bora, E., Yucel, M., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Neurobiology drickson&sortspec=date&submit=Submit
of human affiliative behaviour: Implications for psy- Germer, C. K. (2004). What is mindfulness. Insight
chiatric disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 22, Journal, 2004(Fall), 2429. Retrieved from http://
320325. www.mbcttrainingen.nl/Resources/What%20is%20
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of Mindfulness.pdf.
being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychologi- Gerrig, R. J., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2008). Understanding
cal well-being. Journal of Personality and Social human personality. In S. Frail (Ed.), Psychology and
Psychology, 84, 822848. life (pp. 439442). New York, NY: Pearson.
Buchanan, T. W., Bagley, S. L., & Stansfield, R. B. (2012). Gilbert, P. (2009). The compassionate mind. London:
The empathic, physiological resonance of stress. Constable.
Social Neuroscience, 7, 191201. Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness-
Buk, A. (2009). The mirror neuron system and embodied based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation
simulation: Clinical implications for art therapists in social anxiety disorder. Emotion, 10, 8391.
working with trauma survivors. The Arts in Goleman, D. (1996). Emotional intelligence: Why it can
Psychotherapy, 36(2), 6174. matter more than IQ. London: Bloomsbury.
Cima, M., Smeets, T., & Jelicic, M. (2008). Self-reported Gray, M. A., Harrison, N. A., & Wiens, S. (2007).
trauma, cortisol levels, and aggression in psychopathic Modulation of emotional appraisal by false physiolog-
and non-psychopathic prison inmates. Biological ical feedback during fMRI. PloS One, 2, e546.
Psychology, 78, 7586. Guastella, A. J., Mitchell, P. B., & Dadds, M. R. (2008).
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel-now? The anterior Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human
insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews faces. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 35.
Neuroscience, 10, 5970. Haycock, D. (2012). Hare psychopathy checklist.
Coelho, H. F., Canter, P. H., & Ernst, E. (2007). Encyclopedia of mental disorders. Retrieved from
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: Evaluating cur- http://www.minddisorders.com/Flu-Inv/Hare-
rent evidence and informing future research. Journal Psychopathy-Checklist.html
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, Hare, R. D. (1993). Without conscience: The disturbing
10001005. world of the psychopaths among us. New York: Pocket
Comer, R. J. (2010). Abnormal psychology. New York: Books.
Worth Publisher. Hill, C. L. M., & Updegraff, J. A. (2012). Mindfulness and
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, its relationship to emotional regulation. Emotion, 12,
M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., et al. (2003). Alterations 8190.
16 Mindfulness and Self-Regulation: A Medical Approach to the Mind and Mental Health 233

Hosking, P. (2007). Compassion: What is in a word? In Pauley, G., & McPherson, M. (2010). The experience and
M. Kostanski (Ed.), The power of compassion: An meaning of compassion and self-compassion for indi-
exploration of the psychology of compassion in the viduals with depression or anxiety. Psychology and
21st century (pp. 113). Newcastle, UK: Cambridge Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 83,
Scholars Publishing. Retrieved from http://www.c-s-p. 129143.
org/Flyers/9781847181152-sample.pdf Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziota, J. C., & Dapretto,
Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neu- M. (2008). Mirroring others emotions relates to
rons. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 653670. empathy and interpersonal competence in children.
Jacobs, T. L., Epel, E. S., Lin, J., Blackburn, E. H., NeuroImage, 39, 20762085.
Wolkowitz, O. M., Bridwell, D. A., et al. (2011). Pharo, H., Sim, C., Graham, M., Gross, J., & Hayne, H.
Intensive meditation training, immune cell telomerase (2011). Risky business: Executive function, personal-
activity, and psychological mediators. ity, and reckless behavior during adolescence and
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(5), 664681. emerging adulthood. Behavioral Neuroscience, 125,
Jackson, P. L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, 970978.
J. (2006). Empathy examined through the neural mech- Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its
anisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain
you feel pain. Neuropsychologia, 44(1), 752761. Sciences, 25(1), 172.
Kelley, A. E., Schochet, T., & Landry, C. F. (2004). Risk Reyna, V. F., Chapman, S. B., Dougherty, M. R., &
taking and novelty seeking in adolescence: Confrey, J. (2012). The adolescent brain: Learning,
Introduction to Part I. Annals of the New York reasoning, and decision making. Washington, DC:
Academy of Sciences, 1021(1), 2732. doi:10.1196/ American Psychological Association.
annals.1308.003. Romer, D. (2010). Adolescent risk taking, impulsivity,
Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., and brain development: Implications for preven-
Cushman, F., Hauser, M., et al. (2007). Damage to the tion. Developmental Psychobiology, 52(3), 263276.
prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judg- doi:10.1002/dev.20442.
ments. Nature, 446, 908911. Ross, H. E., & Young, L. J. (2009). Oxytocin and the neu-
Krasner, M. S., Epstein, R. M., Beckman, H., Suchman, ral mechanisms regulating social cognition and affilia-
A. L., Chapman, B., Mooney, C. J., et al. (2009). tive behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 30,
Association of an educational program in mindful 534547.
communication with burnout, empathy, and attitudes Saxe, R. (2006). Uniquely human social cognition.
among primary care physicians. JAMA, 302(12), Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(1), 235239.
12841293. Schore, A. N. (2012). The science of the art of psycho-
Kudielka, B. M., Schommer, N. C., & Hellhammer, D. H. therapy. New York: W.W. Norton.
(2004). Acute HPA axis responses, heart rate, and Shirtcliff, E. A., Vitacco, M. J., Graf, A. R., Gostisha,
mood changes to psychosocial stress (TSST) in A. J., Merz, J. L., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2009).
humans at different times of day. Neurobiology of empathy and callousness:
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 983992. Implications for the development of antisocial behav-
Lieberman, M. D., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2005). A pain by ior. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 27, 137171.
any other name (rejection, exclus``ion, ostracism), Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002).
still hurts the same: The role of dorsal anterior cingu- Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression:
late in social and physical pain. In J. T. Cacioppo, A new approach to preventing relapse. New York:
P. Visser, & C. Pickets (Eds.), Social neuroscience: Guilford Press.
People thinking about people (pp. 167187). Siegel, D. J. (2006). An interpersonal neurobiology
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. approach to psychotherapy. Psychiatric Annals, 36,
Lightfoot, C., Cole, M., & Cole, S. (2009). The develop- 248256.
ment of children. New York: Worth. Siegel, D. J. (2007a). The mindful brain: Reflection and
Morgan, A. B., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic attunement in the cultivation of well-being. New York:
review of the relation between antisocial behavior and W.W. Norton.
neuropsychological measures of executive function. Siegel, D. J. (2007b). Mindfulness training and neural
Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 113136. integration. Journal of Social Cognitive, and Affective
Neff, K. D., Rude, S. S., & Kirkpatrick, K. L. (2007). Self- Neuroscience, 2, 259263.
compassion and adaptive psychological functioning. Siegel, D. J. (2010a). Mindsight: The new science of per-
Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 139154. sonal transformation. New York: Random House.
Parker, S., Nelson, B., Epel, E., & Siegel, D. J. (in press). Siegel, D. J. (2010b). The mindful therapist: A clinicians
The science of presence. In K.W. Brown & C. D. guide to mindsight and neural integration. New York:
Creswell (Eds.), Handbook of mindfulness: Theory W.W. Norton.
and research. New York: Guilford Press. Siegel, D. J. (2012a). The developing mind: How relation-
Passingham, R. E. (1993). The frontal lobes and voluntary ships and the brain interact to shape who we are (2nd
action. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. ed.). New York: Guilford.
234 J. Davis-Siegel et al.

Siegel, D. J. (2012b). Pocket guide to interpersonal neuro- Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., Jackson, D. C.,
biology: An integrative handbook of the mind. Dalton, K. M., Mueller, C. J., et al. (2004). Making a
New York: W.W. Norton. life worth living: Neural correlates of well-being.
Siegel, D. J. (2013). Brainstorm: The power and purpose Psychological Science, 15, 367372.
of the teenage brain. New York: Tarcher Penguin/ Walker, I. (2009). I, psychopath [Documentary]. Australia:
Random House. Screen Australia, & New South Wales Film and
Simon, M., Herold, R., & Fekete, S. (2007). Mirror neu- Television Office.
ronsnovel data on the neurobiology of intersubjec- Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowl-
tivity. Psychiatria Hungarica, 22, 418429. edge. New York: Vintage Books.
Spear, L. P. (2010). The behavioral neuroscience of ado- Wray-Lake, L., Flanagan, C. A., & Maggs, J. L. (2011).
lescence. New York, NY: W.W. Norton. Socialization in context: Exploring longitudinal cor-
Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (2002). Principles of frontal relates of mothers value messages of compassion and
lobe function. Oxford: Oxford University Press. caution. Developmental Psychology, 48, 250256.
Takahashi, H., Kato, M., Matsuura, M., Koeda, M., Yahata, Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone,
N., & Suhara, T. (2008). Neural correlates of human A., & Saxe, R. (2010). Disruption of the right tempo-
virtue judgment. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 18861891. roparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimula-
Thomson, J. J. (1985). The trolley problem. The Yale Law tion reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments.
Journal, 94, 13951415. PNAS, 107, 67536758.
Mindfulness as a General
Ingredient of Successful 17
Psychotherapy

James Carmody

cises to cultivate insight into the moment to


Introduction moment construction of personal identity in the
mind and the degree of suffering/well-being
The Common Goal Mindfulness resulting from patterns of responding to these
Training Has with Psychotherapy constructions. Mindfulness, as well as the range
of mind-training exercises designed to facilitate
While a complete list of psychotherapys goals, it, occupies an important place in the develop-
and the means therapists have employed to facili- ment of this experiential insight.
tate these would be long and diverse, the over- Recognizing the overlapping goals of
arching themes are increasing quality of life and Buddhist and Western approaches, and particu-
reducing emotional suffering; goals that are larly the therapeutic opportunities represented by
shared by all Buddhist schools of thought, even the Buddhist mind training exercises, mindful-
as they vary in their approaches and practices. ness instruction was introduced into clinical set-
But while Buddhism and psychotherapy have tings, principally through the work of Kabat-Zinn
broadly similar ends, and even share some tech- (1982). Subsequent randomized trials have pro-
niques to foster beneficial change, historically vided solid evidence for the effectiveness of
their approaches stood in contrast. Western thera- mindfulness training in reducing stress and dis-
peutic systems focused principally on modifying tress in both clinical (Hofmann, Sawyer, Witt, &
cognitions and behavior and/or developing an Oh, 2010), and nonclinical populations (Chiesa
intellectual understanding of the dynamics of the & Serretti, 2009; Fjorback, Arendt, rnbl, Fink,
internal narrative. Buddhist approaches, while & Walach, 2011). The positive benefits that relate
including the standard religious admonishments directly to the goals of psychotherapy include:
to ethical behavior and positive cognitions in increased subjective well-being, reduced psycho-
their systematic path, offered in addition, exer- logical symptoms and emotional reactivity,
improved behavioral regulation, and improved
interpersonal coping skills (Davis & Hayes,
2011; Keng, Smoski, & Robins, 2011). Studies of
J. Carmody (*) the mechanisms of change indicate that increases
Department of Medicine, Division of Preventive and in mindfulness mediate and precede (Baer, 2009;
Behavioral Medicine, University of Massachusetts
Brnstrm, Kvillemo, Brandberg, & Moskowitz,
Medical School, 55 Lake Ave North,
Worcester, MA 01655, USA 2010; Carmody, Baer, Lykins, & Olendzki, 2009;
e-mail: James.Carmody@umassmed.edu Kuyken et al., 2010) reductions in distress

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 235


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_17
236 J. Carmody

(Baer, Carmody, & Hunsinger 2011), and longer the literature on how the construct is best under-
term trials show that improvements in well-being stood (Chiesa & Malinowski, 2011; Langer &
from the training are enduring when compared to Moldoveanu, 2000; Ludwig & Kabat-Zinn,
an educational control program (Pbert et al., 2012). 2008) and how to determine valid criterion refer-
This range of benefits to psychological well- ences for self-report measures of mindfulness
being (Hofmann et al., 2010) has led to mindful- (Davidson, 2010). Disagreements extend even to
ness training being creatively adapted to Western whether it is possible to conceptually define and
therapeutic models and successfully integrated measure mindfulness without loss of its essential
into a growing variety of clinical treatment nature; asserting that it must be experientially
modalities, both exclusively as in Mindfulness- understood within the larger Buddhist framework
Based Stress Reduction (MBSR), and in parallel from which it is derived (Grossman, 2008;
with practices such as self-reflection, journal Harrington & Pickles, 2009). More particular to
writing, skills training, affect tolerance, and the process of integrating mindfulness into psy-
reframing (Sanders, 2010) in programs such as chotherapy is the dearth of evidence related to
Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT) (Linehan, both the minimum amount of training and prac-
1993), Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy tice time required for patients to derive benefits
(MBCT) (Teasdale, Segal, & Williams, 1995), from mindfulness training (Carmody & Baer,
and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy 2009), and the amount of training and experience
(ACT) (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999). Trials in mindfulness practice therapists need to have in
of each of these programs have shown that the order to be able to represent and integrate it cor-
training does indeed improve outcomes in their rectly (Davis & Hayes, 2011).
clinical populations (Vllestad, Nielsen, & There are a number of reasons that such differ-
Nielsen, 2011). Reports of studies of the effect of ences and knowledge gaps are to be expected in
mindfulness in the more traditional psychothera- this field: First, the construct of mindfulness is
peutic process have also proliferated in recent extracted from a larger and more comprehensive
years, including its role in the therapeutic encoun- Buddhist religious system. In the original Pali
ter (Davis & Hayes, 2011). While the evidence in texts samma-sati is one of eight factors to be
this area is less solid, mindfulness does appear to cultivated in order to understand the momentary
contribute to therapists self-care (Christopher creation of mental states, and to reduce lifes suf-
et al., 2011) and to make for more compassionate fering. Mindfulness has come to be the
and empathetic therapists, although it does not accepted translation of sati, and samma-sati
necessarily translate into their having better ther- is generally understood as skillful mindfulness.
apy outcomes (Davis & Hayes, 2011). Sati can also be understood as awareness how-
ever, and in a clinical context skillful aware-
ness may be a preferable term to mindfulness
The Challenges of Integrating because awareness is a mental function within
Mindfulness into Clinical Settings patients existing realm of experience, and so
and Empirical Systems immediately conceptually accessible. It also
avoids the possible confusion that can arise by
Even as there is broad acceptance of the clinical objectifying a term like mindful that has a pre-
utility of mindfulness, there remain a number of existing conceptual meaning in Western
knowledge gaps that are important to fill if it is to literature.
find more widespread integration into psycho- A second reason for differences is the wide
therapy. These include both the lack of a testable variation that exists among different Buddhist
theoretical model to serve as an integrative foun- traditions and teachers in the understanding of
dation for comparative studies (Sedlmeier et al., mindfulness (sati), and how it is most skill-
2012) and, related to this, significant variation in fully cultivated and integrated into the broader
17 Mindfulness Psychotherapy 237

system. In Soto Zen traditions for example, Bypassing the Definition Problem:
great emphasis is placed on directing effort to Understanding Mindfulness Through
maintain correct posture during formal medi- the Training Exercises
tation (zazen) and typically refrains from
abstract instructions for the cultivation of To weave a way through these differences in
mindfulness; that it is cultivated through understanding, and to make the construct more
awareness of ones actions and surroundings. immediately accessible than the usual clinical def-
In the Theravada South Asian traditions, on inition, I describe in this chapter a conceptual
the other hand, formal meditation involves model that draws upon forty-odd years of personal
more explicit instruction in mental exercises practice in the three major Buddhist traditions;
to facilitate a more wide-ranging exploration professional experience as a therapist; clinical
of mental activity. research studying the outcomes and mechanisms
Finally, and perhaps most significantly, impor- of mindfulness training; and from teaching mind-
tant variations in emphases and understanding fulness to patients and clinicians. It is based on the
are bound to occur when an approach to the cul- skills taught in clinical mindfulness training exer-
tivation of well-being moves from conceptions of cises, and describes how those exercises support
suffering in the traditional Eastern cultures the the experiential recognition of three features of
system developed within, into the enshrined mental activity not normally discerned, and how
Western pursuit of happiness and the diversity of this recognition affects the individuals lived expe-
cultural and personal aspirations associated with rience. As such it revolves around facility in the
well-being in a market society. In the Buddhism use of attention, together with recognizing compo-
of traditional Eastern cultures an accepted truth nents of mental experience and their conditioned
was that suffering was to be expected, and that associations that the attentional facility and
efforts to escape from it will lead only to further increased awareness make possible.
suffering. It should be kept in mind also that In subsequent sections I expand on and dis-
these were typically socially static societies when cuss the features of the model concretely, includ-
compared with the aspirational culture we are ing its usefulness in psychotherapy by providing
socialized into, in which ubiquitous advertising a parsimonious and accessible conceptual frame-
offers the promise of escape from our work for presenting and discussing mindfulness
circumstances. with patients and clients (Carmody & Baer,
Even so, most descriptions of mindfulness in 2009). But briefly, and rather abstractly, they can
the clinical literature have used some variation on be described as follows:
Kabat-Zinns definition of paying attention in a
particular way: on purpose, in the present 1. Our everyday experience, while apparently
moment, and non-judgmentally (Kabat-Zinn, seamless, is in fact constructed from three phe-
1994; Shapiro, Carlson, Astin, & Freedman, nomenological components which form asso-
2006). While such a description is intriguing as ciational cycles. Those components are:
to what may follow by way of its cultivation, it is thoughts/images, impressions arriving through
unlikely on the face of it to mean much to most the senses, and the pleasant/unpleasant feeling
clients and patients not already familiar with tone associated with these. The richness of our
mindfulness practice. At worst, it may be per- experience; memory, imagination and emo-
ceived as sufficiently cryptic, challenging, and tion, are the result of the symphonic interplay
inaccessible to discourage further exploration. In of these fundamental elements.
this regard, patients are often asked to suspend 2. Because attention is normally preoccupied with
their need for a conceptual understanding in favor the content of these components, their nature
of grasping it through experience with one of the goes unrecognized, thereby sustaining their asso-
training exercises. ciational cycles. If it is a thought for example,
238 J. Carmody

attention is occupied with what the thought is to the explanation rather than lab-based neural
about, or means. With training, these compo- and biological processes. Second, by using con-
nents can be recognized as events (mind objects) structs already familiar to both therapists and
occurring in the field of awareness, thereby their clients, it provides a relatively simple con-
disrupting the associational cycle that relies on ceptual framework that can be individually
preoccupation with content and meaning. adapted both to introduce mindfulness to patients
3. Attention is adapted to highlight novelty and in psychotherapy who are nave to it, and used as
possibilities for/threats to fulfillment of needs a basis for discussing mindfulness in the ongoing
(including safety) but the movement of atten- therapeutic process. Also, taking it out of the
tion itself is not normally noticed. Rather, the commonly used descriptions and customary
object of attention is noticed, and arousal lev- mindfulness meditation language avoids the defi-
els and affects follow the degree of threat/ nition problem, and makes mindfulness seem less
delight represented by that content. These perplexing and more conceptually straightfor-
principles are explained more fully below. ward and accessible to patients. Third, it reveals
the elements of mindfulness training that have
been employed in systems such as cognitive ther-
apy for many years to make features of internal
The Utility of a Functional, experience accessible to patients. As such, it pro-
Evolutionary Approach vides a conceptually unifying foundation that
shows both the common ground, and the contrast,
While these features become evident upon reflec- mindfulness has with what patients are already
tion, in everyday life their rapidity and automatic- learning in therapy.
ity results in experience being perceived as Lastly, an evolutionary explanation for the
seamless, and so presenting to the patient no stubborn default attending mechanisms that
apparent opening for some measure of control in patients encounter when first attempting the
the face of distress. This perception results in a mindfulness attention exercises reveals an inher-
sense of powerlessness that exacerbates the stress/ ent, if dysfunctional, intelligence in those pat-
distress. The relatively simple mindfulness train- terns adapted for survival and reproductive
ing exercises (meditations) are designed to facili- success. An appreciation of the adaptive function
tate recognition of these three features in everyday of this mechanism can help patients to better
experience by developing self-regulation of atten- understand and contextualize the frustrations
tion, and cultivating curiosity about, and observa- they encounter, and that often test their resolve to
tion of, internal experience. Acquisition of this persist in endeavoring to make changes, than the
learning represents a helpful resource for patients somewhat more pejorative craving, aversion/
to approach the seamlessness of everyday felt clinging, attachment explanations derived from
experience, and represents an important feature of Buddhist conceptualizations.
the clinical utility and therapeutic value of mind-
fulness (Teasdale et al., 2002). This functional
model has a number of advantages in the psycho- The Three Principles
therapeutic setting for both therapists and patients. of the Functional Conceptual Model
First, psychotherapy, like mindfulness, relies
on a certain kind of introspection grounded in the The following sections develop and explain these
phenomenology of perception. By explicating features and illustrate their role in clinical outcomes.
the qualities of attending to experience that are
embedded in mindfulness training exercises, and First Principle
the effect those can be expected to have on the Our apparently seamless experience is con-
individuals felt experience, the model makes structed from a limited number of components
already familiar psychological processes central (thoughts/images, sensations, and their pleasant/
17 Mindfulness Psychotherapy 239

unpleasant feeling tone) which form associa- The First Principle in Mindfulness
tional cycles. The richness of experience includ- Practice: Conscious Redirection
ing memory, imagination and emotion are of Attention Affects the Cycle
symphonies of these fundamental elements. of Arousal/Distress
This three-way network linking related mental
components in an automatic and unconscious As I describe in the following section, an impor-
way to form cycles that are experienced as dis- tant function of the beginning mindfulness exer-
tress is described also by Damasio (2003). In cises is to support patients in learning to
these cycles, a thought/image gives rise to an discriminate between thoughts, sensations, and
associated bodily sensation and feeling tone their feeling tone. In association with that learn-
which through associative patterns of memory ing, the recognition that their attention is fixated
gives rise to more thoughts related to the sensa- or compelled by a cycle of associations that is
tions and feelings. The cycles can begin with any creating distress, opens an opportunity to redirect
of the components, as they form and re-form very attention to some other object available to experi-
rapidly outside of awareness in daily life, the ence that is not being noticed. Commonly used to
associations are not readily apparent. this end are the sensations associated with respi-
Attention is central to the recognition of this ration; sensations that are arousal-neutral for
principle for two reasons: First, arousal levels most people. When attention is directed toward
follow the object of attention. Thus, when atten- them, the reduced arousal attendant upon an
tion is on the content of a sequence of associated affect-neutral sensation [mental object] naturally
fearful thoughts, sensations of constriction, and follows.
unpleasant feeling tone, then the bodys arousal Breath-related sensations have the added clin-
level follows. Second, when attention is not ical utility of being immediately and continually
intentionally directed/regulated it can become available to patients as an attentional object, and
fixated on a particular sequence, forming a rela- as such, provide an ideal means for invisibly
tively closed loop that maintains the automatic reducing distress and lowering arousal levels in
cycle experienced as distress. Anxiety, for exam- the course of events in everyday life. In Fig. 17.2,
ple, results when the attention becomes fixated the closed cycle resulting in the experience of
on a fear-related cycle of components. This is distress illustrated in Fig. 17.1, is opened to the
illustrated in Fig. 17.1. Depending at least partly possibility of change as the patient learns to dis-
upon temperament, some patients will report the criminate between the components, and recalls
distress of this cycle in terms of feeling tone (I that the sensations of breathing are an arousal-
feel bad), while others will report the cognition neutral mental object available to their attention.
component (Im worried). Still others will be When, as in Fig. 17.2, attention is redirected
more aware of the sensations (I feel tense). towards the sensations of breathing, the cycle of
associations that were maintaining distress are
interrupted, with accompanying change in affect,
and reduced arousal. The principle is exemplified
in the instructions for beginning mindfulness
practitioners to deliberately redirect their
attention to the sensations of respiration (chosen
mental objectneutral stimulus) in the face of
distressing thoughts, sensations or emotions.
Attention invariably wanders away from the
breath sensations for the reasons outlined in prin-
ciple three, but as long as it is kept on, or repeat-
Fig. 17.1 Automatic maintenance of distress when atten- edly redirected back to the breath, arousal and the
tion is unregulated or compelled affect attendant upon a neutral sensation naturally
240 J. Carmody

Fig. 17.2 With mindfulness training patient recognizes breathing, and (3) redirecting attention to breath interrupts
(1) components of the cycle maintaining distress, (2) cycle maintaining distress
availability to attention of arousal-neutral sensations of

follow. Facility in the use of attention in this way, imagery, prayer, mantra, and the focus on bodily
together with the capacity to discriminate sensations in progressive muscle relaxation, tai
between the components of experience, provides chi, and yoga.
an opportunity for less emotional reactivity to
symptoms in the midst of other activities, and a Second Principle
greater sense of control and confidence in dealing The components of experience (thoughts/images,
with distress. In mindfulness training for depres- sensations, and their pleasant/unpleasant feeling
sion for example, the focused attention required tone) are referred to as mind objects in some
to sustain a negative stream of thought (with its Buddhist training traditions, and to be recognized
attendant distress) is diverted, preventing its as events occurring in the field of awareness. In
escalation into ruminative patterns which in turn current therapeutic parlance this skill is often
can lead to increases in ongoing negative psycho- referred to as re-perceiving, or metacognitive
logical states and mental distress (Teasdale et al., awareness (Safran & Segal, 1990; Teasdale et al.,
1995, 2000). 2002).
This strategy can also account for the clinical The therapeutic challenge is that attention is
benefits from mindfulness treatments for anxiety normally and naturally preoccupied with the con-
(Kabat-Zinn et al., 1992; Patel, Carmody, & tent of the components (what they are about/
Simpson, 2007). These attention regulation pro- mean) thereby sustaining their associational
cesses may also account for at least some of the cycles and attendant distress. So, having learned
demonstrated reductions in psychological dis- to distinguish between thoughts and the attendant
tress from participation in other mind-body train- sensations and feelings, mindfulness training
ing programs (Bertisch, Wee, Phillips, & instructions often include the activity of labeling
McCarthy, 2009; Manzoni, Pagnini, Castelnuovo, the component (this is a thought) to support
& Molinari, 2008) which entail some measure of this perceptual shift. With the recognition that the
attention regulation to an arousal neutral or mental components are events occurring in the
affect-positive mind object such as; focusing on mind, and can be perceived as such, the attention
breathing exercises, symptom monitoring, guided on their content required to maintain the cycle of
17 Mindfulness Psychotherapy 241

Fig. 17.3 Re-perceiving: attentional/perceptual shift from thoughts content To the thought as an event in
awareness

distress is diluted. The difference between these quality of life, perceived stress, and sleep quality,
two orientations is illustrated in Fig. 17.3. exit interviews showed that most women reported
using redirection of attention to the sensations of
the breath to help them cope in the face of their
Evidence for the Importance hot flashes and night sweats. In this way they
of the First Two Principles developed a greater sense of control and confi-
in Mindfulness Training dence in dealing with their symptoms.
The importance of re-perceiving in the reduc-
Several lines of evidence support the importance tions in distress from mindfulness training was
of attention and re-perceiving/metacognitive shown by Teasdale and colleagues, who found
awareness in the reductions in distress associated that it was a central component in reducing
with mindfulness training. Self-reports of patients relapse in patients with major depressive disorder
who have completed a mindfulness training pro- (Teasdale, 1999; Teasdale et al., 2002). In addi-
gram show that attentional redirection is one of tion, an observational study of mechanisms of
the most important skills they continue to use in change over the course of an 8-week MBSR pro-
coping with their symptoms and to increase their gram found large effect size increases in re-
well-being. In a 4-year follow-up study (Kabat- perceiving (Carmody et al., 2009); larger than the
Zinn, Lipworth, Burney, & Sellers, 1987) of par- increases in mindfulness.
ticipants in an MBSR program, nearly all reported These self-reports showing the importance of
using redirection of attention to the sensations of attention regulation to well-being are supported
breathing to some extent in their daily life situa- by laboratory studies of attentional processes that
tions, and 7494 % reported it to be moderately, relate directly to mindfulness practice instruc-
or very useful, in coping successfully with stress- tions. Jha, and others, have found improved voli-
ful situations. Similarly, in a study of MBSR for tional orienting of attention with mindfulness
women with severe hot flashes (Carmody, training that appeared to be associated with the
Crawford, & Churchill, 2006) found clinically extent of mindfulness practice (Chan &
significant improvements in menopause-related Woollacott, 2007; Jha, Krompinger, & Baime,
242 J. Carmody

2007). In addition, Valentine and colleagues evolutionary processes have resulted in default
(Valentine & Sweet, 1999) found that mindful- movements of attention that are adapted to serve
ness practitioners had superior performance on a survival and reproduction. Second, this default
sustained attention task (Wilkins counting test), movement of attention itself is not normally
especially when the stimulus was unexpected, noticed; rather the content of the object that the
and performance was related to length of experi- moving attention is highlighting is noticed,
ence with practice. together with the arousal levels and affect tone
Mindfulness practice also supports the alloca- associated with the degree of threat/delight repre-
tion of attentional resources. In the phenomenon sented by that content. This automatic process
known as attentional blink, subjects presented can result in delight in moments when things are
with two targets (pictures or numbers) imbedded going well, such as a new relationship, or oppor-
in a rapid series of distractor stimuli (non-target tunity. More commonly however, attention is in
numbers or pictures) tend to miss the second its adapted role of monitoring for threats to need
stimulus if it is presented within a half second of fulfillment.
the first. This is thought to occur because atten-
tional resources are limited, and if those resources
are consumed in processing the first stimulus, the Self-Regulation of Attention Is
second stimulus is missed. Further, Slagter and Important to Well-Being, But Getting
colleagues (Slagter et al., 2007) found that the Started in the Face of Distress Is
more experienced mindfulness meditation practi- Challenging
tioners exhibited a smaller attentional blink effect
and attributed this to their more efficient alloca- As long ago as the nineteenth century William
tion of attentional resources resulting from the James famously wrote that my experience is
training. In other words, their attention was less what I agree to attend to (James, 1890). The
caught up by the first stimulus and available for a centrality James gave to attention in shaping our
different stimulus. experience continues to find support in a number
Finally, building upon the findings of of therapeutic systems that address self-regulation
Ellenbogen and colleagues (Ellenbogen, (Baumeister & Heatherton, 1996; Kirschenbaum,
Schwartzman, Stewart, & Walker, 2006), where 1987). Further, an assumed capacity for attention
subjects trained to deliberately direct attention to self-regulation is contained in the familiar
the neutral word of a neutral-negative word pair encouragement given to children to pay atten-
reported less negative affect to a subsequent tion when learning something for which they
mental stressor, Ortner and colleagues (Ortner, have no immediate affinity. Nevertheless,
Kilner, & Zelazo, 2007) found that mindfulness although James declaration is conceptually
training was associated with more rapid disen- straightforward, and intuitively apparent, chil-
gagement from emotionally provocative stimuli. dren, and patients are not normally encouraged or
taught to attend to attention itself. Moreover,
Third Principle James own ongoing struggle with depression in
Attention highlights in awareness selected fea- his life points to the challenge of putting attention
tures of available experience, and is adapted to regulation into practice in the face of psychologi-
highlight novelty, and objects perceived even cal difficulties and distress. Even a century later,
indirectly, as leading to the satisfaction of, or psychotherapeutic systems had not developed
threats to the fulfillment of, needs and longings. systematic methods of cultivating attentional
The movement of attention itself is not normally self-regulation as a generic capacity in the ser-
noticed however, and learning to regulate atten- vice of well-being; a deficiency that was
tion to facilitate the process of recognition of successfully addressed in the mindfulness train-
these principles presents several obstacles. First, ing exercises derived from Buddhist practices.
17 Mindfulness Psychotherapy 243

Nevertheless, while these exercises build a ecology evolved to meet their most basic needs of
foundation for the well-documented opportuni- survival and reproductive success.
ties for increased self-regulation and well-being, Attentions immediate, automatic, and pre-
they require some practice (Carmody & Baer, cognitive movement toward physical threats to
2008), and patients commonly report some level safety has obvious survival value. Furthermore,
of difficulty in learning to regulate attention in its universality and automaticity suggest an
this way (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002). adapted neural mechanism responsible for it.
When first beginning the foundational mindful- Compared with our remote ancestors, however,
ness exercise of attending to the sensations of our need for the necessary sense-based scanning
respiration, the typical report is along the lines of of the environment for acute physical threats has
Gosh, I never realized how much my mind wan- become minimal. The relative physical security
dersit seems to have a mind of its own. A typi- of modern life means that when not deliberately
cal response from many instructors is to reassure or sensationally directed elsewhere, attention
the trainee that this initial frustration is to be now largely monitors for threats and opportuni-
expected, that everyone experiences the same ties for the fulfillment of other needs, particularly
challenge, and encouragement to persist in the those for power, status, and relationship; needs
face of it. A more meaningful response may be to that require considerable cognitive activity in the
give patients a conceptual introduction that clari- form of planning and strategizing. This is appar-
fies some of the embedded mental dynamics they ent in the way attention repeatedly favors the
face in getting started, and framed within the cognitive accompaniment of this monitoring
question of why this simple attempt at attention which is subjectively experienced as the all too
regulation should be associated with any diffi- familiar personal narrative. Attention defaults to
culty at all. this internal monologue of unending commentary
and evaluation in preference to the broader land-
scape of sensory experience available in the
How Evolutionary Pressures Have immediate physical surroundings, or to routine
Resulted in Attentional Defaults: This bodily sensations.
Biological Imperative Makes Given the essentially social nature of human
Attentional Self-Regulation life, this default movement of attention should
Challenging to Cultivate not be unexpected. As physical threats diminish,
there is survival and reproductive value in atten-
In a way the mind does have a mind of its own, tion now defaulting to a mental process that
and the reasons this seemingly straightforward weaves together a narrative related to appraising
attentional regulation activity, so foundational to possible threats to relationship, status, and power.
well-being, should be such a challenge are often And given the needs this internal monologue
not addressed in mindfulness instruction. As serves, it typically manifests as preoccupation
stated above, the instructor may respond with the with worries about family, work, money etc., or
assurance that everyone has difficulty, as indeed to plans to move toward satisfying these and
they do, or resort to a traditional Buddhist expla- other needs and longings, together with imagined
nation that the mind is clinging or attached, threats and allies in their fulfillment. Because the
but such explanations are thin conceptual gruel movement of attention itself typically happens
and do not place the patients difficulty in a read- largely outside of conscious awareness, we take
ily understandable framework. An evolutionary this preoccupation for granted; referring to it
approach provides a recognizable explanatory sometimes as wandering mind or more for-
frame that lets beginners appreciate that this fea- mally as stimulus-independent thought
ture of their mind is not an enemy to their desire (Buckner, Andrews Hanna, & Schacter, 2008).
for greater well-being, but an important, if some- And when attention is left to this default state
times less than functional feature, of a mental (i.e., when it is not actively directed), this cycle
244 J. Carmody

can be self-sustaining, with felt experience being tion; just noticing whatever sensation happens to
dictated by whatever is the current concern. be present in that body part, and the feeling tone
The phenomenological dilemma is that even (pleasant/unpleasant/neutral) associated with it.
as this default functions to protect and move us to Attention sooner or later inevitably wanders to
the satisfaction of important needs, its recurrent its default objects, typically the internal mono-
orientation toward threat monitoring and solu- logue, or the sensations and feelings related to it.
tions means that it is, by its nature, more or less When this wandering is noticed, the instruction is
fear-related. And since arousal levels and down- to return the attention to exploring the sensations
stream physiological processes follow the threat/ in the body. If powerful experiences emerge dur-
delight represented by the object of attention, this ing the process, the breathing sensations are
fixation on forming and re-forming threat-related available as arousal-neutral objects to which the
associational cycles, either from habit or from the attention can be returned.
present intensity of an experiential component Once some facility in noticing sensations is
such as pain, results in some elevated level of established, instruction is extended to noticing
arousal. Over time, this attentional default results cognitions, including any that may be associated
in everyday life being experienced as less than with the sensations and feelings. Intrusive cogni-
pleasant, and has a profound effect upon the tions are a good place to start since they are most
development and maintenance of mental suffer- readily identifiable and labeled as thoughts (re-
ing. It is the familiar human malaise that perceiving/meta-awareness). While initially
Buddhism refers to as the first and second noble practiced with the body lying or sitting still, these
truths; that life is characterized by suffering, or exercises can be extended to being done while
stress, resulting from craving and aversion. moving by practicing them during walking, or
It is the felt experience of these adaptive stretching as a way of supporting their integration
default processes that brings many patients to into everyday life.
therapy, and also makes the attention regulation Some facility in just these relatively simple
in mindfulness training and other mind-body skills results in reductions in distress for many
exercises the challenge that patients almost uni- patients, and with some practice the fluidity of
versally report (My mind [attention] wanders; attention that results can create a real possibility
Im bored). for greater freedom. Nevertheless, the amount of
training and practice that is required has not been
systematically studied (Carmody & Baer, 2009).
Instructions for Mindfulness Training Initial assumptions have been based on a typical
Exercises Are Designed to Facilitate traditional Buddhist monastic practice period of
Recognition of These Three Principles around 45 min. An ongoing time commitment
such as that is neither feasible nor desirable for
The initial exercise often used to begin mindful- many patients. It may also not be necessary to
ness training is one now commonly referred to as result in clinical gains; a 4-year follow-up study
the body scan; a practice from the Burmese of MBSR participants (Kabat-Zinn et al., 1987)
Buddhist tradition. In this exercise attention is found that many of the post-program clinical
initially brought to notice the sensations of respi- gains were maintained even though only 30 % of
ration. Once awareness of these as affect-neutral the respondents described themselves as regular
mind objects always available to attention is meditators. And other studies (Arch & Craske,
established, attention is then moved systemati- 2006; Zeidan, Gordon, Merchant, & Goolkasian,
cally through all parts of the body. The instruc- 2010) indicate that instruction and practice can
tion is just to notice whatever sensations happen be brief enough to integrate into a typical 15 min
to be present in each part, including noticing if no primary care appointment. The pervasive and
sensation is present. No effort is made to change ubiquitous cognitions foundational to personal
the sensations, or the pace or depth of the respira- identity that interest committed practitioners are
17 Mindfulness Psychotherapy 245

more subtle and challenging to recognize, how- The Role of the Internal Monologue,
ever, and require a particular kind of sustained Its Discontents,
curiosity that the therapist or patient may, or may and the Complementary Roles
not, have. of Mindfulness in Psychotherapy
The field of sensation is the starting place in
mindfulness training for a number of reasons. While the internal monologue is critical to our
First, in the face of the apparent seamlessness of functioning in the world, and to the satisfaction
experience, sensation is readily perceptible and of both our needs, and the needs of those we love
distinguishable for most people. Second, even and care about, the tendency for attention to
though it is the realm through which we initially default to this commentary on the experience
experience delight, in the process of socializa- arriving through our senses is observably the
tion sensation becomes neglected by attention in source of a great deal of trouble. It is character-
favor of cognitions, including thoughts experi- ized by a constant stream of judgments and com-
enced as memories and imaginings triggered by parisons as it expresses our beliefs, and attempts
the sensations. Third, for most the world of sen- among other things, to serve our needs for rela-
sation, including the senses of sight and hearing, tionship and status. But being threat-based the
is the realm through which we connect most attendant arousal results in some level of ongoing
immediately, intimately, and satisfyingly with suffering, all the while drawing attention away
others and the world around us. It is a realm of from the world of sensation with its attendant
consensus and sharing that stands in contrast opportunities for delight; a dilemma that brings
with the cognitively based internal monologue more than a few into therapy.
with which attention is so often preoccupied, and And while for some, simply learning that they
which is experienced as a place of separation, can redirect their attention to an arousal-neutral
endless explanation, alienation and worry. or positive mind object at times of stress or dis-
Cultivating an interest in attention itself and its tress will be sufficient to their needs and interest,
movement in this way, reveals to the patient just in some circumstances even this will be a chal-
how much their attention defaults to this internal lenge. For when important components of the
monologue, subverting sensory life in the pro- needs-related internal narrative are in conflict,
cess, and the delight and satisfaction that can the need for resolution results in ruminative pat-
arise with it. terns that compel attention to their themes. It is
It can be seen from these relatively simple worth noting in this regard that some practitio-
instructions that patients have an opportunity to ners attempt to use mindfulness to avoid dealing
learn and recognize a number of therapeutically with the monologues content, themes and con-
valuable things in the process of doing the exer- flicts particularly, when these appear too painful
cises. These include self-regulation of attention, or distressing to even approach; attempting to use
discriminating sensations from thoughts and the principles to dismiss these as just events in
feelings, becoming aware of the movements of the mind, and using regular practice in redirec-
attention, experiencing how feeling tone follows tion of attention to maintain some level of mental
the object of attention, recognizing that rather calm. This avoidance has been referred to as the
than being focused on their content, thoughts can spiritual bypass and leads to a restricted life in
be seen as events in awareness, and rediscover- which the practices are maintained as a way of
ing a neglected realm of experience in which keeping the lid on things. So rather than an
delight and connection are more accessible. The opportunity for liberation and greater ease, a kind
practice of the exercises also predictably of attentional vigilance is required as the need for
increases emotion regulation through exposure resolution keeps pressing attention back to the
and affect tolerance, and acceptance is implicit conflicting issues.
in the willingness to engage in these acts of But as conflicting themes and patterns are rec-
attending and perceiving. ognized in therapy, worked through, and resolved
246 J. Carmody

by the development of a more coherent narrative Baer, R. A. (2009). Self-focused attention and mecha-
nisms of change in mindfulness-based treatment.
that the patient recognizes is supporting them in
Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 38, 1520.
moving in a functional way toward satisfaction of Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-
the needs they hold most dear, attention is no lon- regulation failure: An overview. Psychological
ger compelled by their content. As a result it Inquiry, 7, 115.
Bertisch, S. M., Wee, C. C., Phillips, R. S., & McCarthy,
becomes more fluid, and the patient can choose
E. P. (2009). Alternative mind-body therapies used by
which among the constant stream of cognitions adults with medical conditions. Journal of
and sensations s/he will entertain in the interest Psychosomatic Research, 66, 511519.
of satisfying their larger needs and goals. Brnstrm, R., Kvillemo, P., Brandberg, Y., & Moskowitz,
J. T. (2010). Self-report mindfulness as a mediator of
Mindfulness can however support the role of
psychological well-being in a stress reduction inter-
psychotherapy in this exposition. For while treat- vention for cancer patientsA randomized study.
ments such as psychodynamic and cognitive psy- Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 39, 151161.
chotherapy effectively improve mood and reduce Buckner, R. L., Andrews Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L.
(2008). The brains default network. Annals of the
distress by supporting patients in gaining insight
New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 138.
into the themes of the narrative or world view, Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2008). Relationships between
and challenging and replacing self-defeating cog- mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medi-
nitions and behaviors with more functional ones, cal and psychological symptoms and well-being in a
mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal
they each to some extent rearrange the content of
of Behavioral Medicine, 31, 2333.
mental activitythe mental furniture. Without Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2009). How long does a mind-
insight into the way all experience, both func- fulness-based stress reduction program need to be? A
tional and dysfunctional, is constructed in the review of class contact hours and effect sizes for psy-
chological distress. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
mind from moment to moment, the patient
65, 627638.
remains vulnerable to a similar malaise resulting Carmody, J., Baer, R. A., Lykins, E. L. B., & Olendzki, N.
from a new set of self-referential negative judg- (2009). An empirical study of the mechanisms of mind-
ments and comparisons arising from some new fulness in a mindfulness-based stress reduction pro-
gram. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 613626.
set of life circumstances. Integrating mindfulness
Carmody, J., Crawford, S., & Churchill, L. (2006). A pilot
into the therapeutic process by whatever means study of mindfulness-based stress reduction for hot
can provide the patient with the additional wis- flashes. Menopause, 13, 760769.
dom and resilience that is gained through experi- Chan, D., & Woollacott, M. (2007). Effects of level of
meditation experience on attentional focus: Is the effi-
entially recognizing this fact. Patients and
ciency of executive or orientation networks improved?
therapists will choose the level of insight they Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine,
regard as sufficient to address their needs and cir- 13, 651658.
cumstances, but this integration is used effec- Chiesa, A., & Malinowski, P. (2011). Mindfulness based
approaches: Are they all the same? Journal of Clinical
tively in ACT, MBCT, and DBT which each in
Psychology, 67, 404424.
their own ways support these recognitions to Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2009). Mindfulness-based
good clinical effect, and inoculate the patient at a stress reduction for stress management in healthy peo-
more systemic level. ple: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alternative
and Complementary Medicine, 15, 593600.
Christopher, J. C., Chrisman, J. A., Trotter-Mathison,
M. J., Schure, M. B., Dahlen, P., & Christopher, S. B.
(2011). Perceptions of the long-term influence of
References mindfulness training on counselors and psychothera-
pists. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 5, 318349.
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mind- Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow,
fulness: Emotion regulation following a focused and the feeling brain. Orlando, FL: Harcourt.
breathing induction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, Davidson, R. J. (2010). Empirical explorations of mind-
44, 18491858. fulness: Conceptual and methodological conundrums.
Baer, R., Carmody, J., & Hunsinger, M. (2011). Weekly Emotion, 10, 811.
change in mindfulness and perceived stress in a Davis, D. M., & Hayes, J. A. (2011). What are the benefits
mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal of mindfulness? A practice review of psychotherapy-
of Clinical Psychology, 68, 755765. related research. Psychotherapy, 48, 198208.
17 Mindfulness Psychotherapy 247

Ellenbogen, M. A., Schwartzman, A. E., Stewart, J., & Langer, E. J., & Moldoveanu, M. (2000). The construct of
Walker, C. D. (2006). Automatic and effortful emo- mindfulness. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 19.
tional information processing regulates different aspects Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment
of the stress response. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, of borderline personality disorder. New York:
373387. Guilford Press.
Fjorback, L. O., Arendt, M., rnbl, E., Fink, P., & Ludwig, D. S., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (2008). Mindfulness in
Walach, H. (2011). Mindfulness based stress reduction medicine. JAMA, 300, 1350.
and mindfulness based cognitive therapyA system- Manzoni, G., Pagnini, F., Castelnuovo, G., & Molinari, E.
atic review of randomized controlled trials. Acta (2008). Relaxation training for anxiety: A ten-years
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 124, 102119. systematic review with meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry,
Grossman, P. (2008). On measuring mindfulness in psy- 8, 41.
chosomatic and psychological research. Journal of Ortner, C. N. M., Kilner, S. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007).
Psychosomatic Research, 64, 405408. Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional inter-
Harrington, N., & Pickles, C. (2009). Mindfulness and ference on a cognitive task. Motivation and Emotion,
cognitive behavioral therapy: A rebuttal. Journal of 31, 271283.
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 333335. Patel, S., Carmody, J., & Simpson, S. (2007). Adapting
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). mindfulness based stress reduction for the treatment of
Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential obsessive compulsive disorder: A case report.
approach to behaviour change. New York: Guilford Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 14, 375380.
Press. Pbert, L., Madison, J. M., Druker, S., Olendzki, N.,
Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. Magner, R., Reed, G., . . . Carmody, J. (2012). Effect
(2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on of mindfulness training on asthma quality of life and
anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. lung function: A randomised controlled trial. Thorax,
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 67, 769776.
169183. Safran, J. D., & Segal, Z. V. (1990). Interpersonal process
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. in cognitive therapy. New York: Basic Books.
New York: H. Holt. Sanders, K. M. (2010). Mindfulness and psychotherapy.
Jha, A., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Focus, 8, 1924.
Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of atten- Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D.,
tion. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., et al. (2012). The psychological
7, 109119. effects of meditation: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An out-patient program in behav- Bulletin, 138, 11391171.
ioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002).
practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical con- Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression:
siderations and preliminary results. General Hospital A new approach to preventing relapse. New York:
Psychiatry, 4, 3347. Guilford.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go there you are. Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, B.
New York: Hyperion. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of
Kabat-Zinn, J., Lipworth, L., Burney, R., & Sellers, W. Clinical Psychology, 62, 373386.
(1987). Four-year follow-up of a meditation-based Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Francis, A. D.,
program for the self-regulation of chronic pain: Nieuwenhuis, S., Davis, J. M., et al. (2007). Mental
Treatment outcomes and compliance. The Clinical training affects distribution of limited brain resources.
Journal of Pain, 2, 159173. PLoS Biology, 5, e138.
Kabat-Zinn, J., Massion, A., Kristeller, J., Peterson, L., Teasdale, J. (1999). Metacognition, mindfulness and the
Fletcher, K., Pbert, L., Santorelli, S. (1992). modification of mood disorders. Clinical Psychology
Effectiveness of a meditation-based stress reduction and Psychotherapy, 6, 146155.
program in the treatment of anxiety disorders. Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H., Pope, M.,
American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 936943. Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002). Metacognitive
Keng, S. L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects awareness and prevention of relapse in depression:
of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of Empirical evidence. Journal of Consulting and
empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, Clinical Psychology, 70, 275287.
10411056. Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995).
Kirschenbaum, D. S. (1987). Self-regulatory failure: A How does cognitive therapy prevent depressive
review with clinical implications. Clinical Psychology relapse and why should attentional control (mindful-
Review, 7, 77104. ness) training help? Behaviour Research and Therapy,
Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Taylor, 33, 2539.
R. S., Evans, A., Dalgleish, T. (2010). How does Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway,
mindfulness-based cognitive therapy work? Behaviour V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention
Research and Therapy, 48, 11051112. of relapse/recurrence in major depression by
248 J. Carmody

mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of anxiety disorders: A systematic review and meta-
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615623. analysis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49,
Valentine, E. R., & Sweet, P. L. G. (1999). Meditation and 281288.
attention: A comparison of the effects of concentrative Zeidan, F., Gordon, N. S., Merchant, J., & Goolkasian, P.
and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention. (2010). The effects of brief mindfulness meditation
Mental Health, Religion and Culture, 2, 5970. training on experimentally induced pain. Journal of
Vllestad, J., Nielsen, M. B., & Nielsen, G. H. (2011). Pain, 11, 199209.
Mindfulness- and acceptance-based interventions for
Section 4
Buddhist Perspectives
Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness:
A Heuristic Approach 18
John D. Dunne

Over the last several years, various authors have is simply that most clinical adaptations of mind-
examined contemporary conceptions of mindful- fulness are explicitly based at least partially on
ness in relation to Buddhist notions. Some authors Buddhist practices, with the most obvious case
maintain that contemporary approaches to mind- being Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction
fulness deviate signicantly from the authentic (Kabat-Zinn, 2011). Hence, to understand the
Buddhist approach, while others see more align- features of a clinical use of mindfulness that are
ment between contemporary approaches and based on Buddhist sources, it makes sense to
some traditional styles of practice. The differing examine the Buddhist practices and theories that
opinions in this regard can be confusing, and the inspired them. A second reason for examining
aim of this chapter is to lessen that confusion by Buddhist approaches to mindfulness is that the
offering an overview of key Buddhist approaches rich theoretical literature of various Buddhist tra-
to mindfulness in a manner that enables research- ditions can provide insights or suggest lines of
ers to make appropriate use of Buddhist sources. research that might not otherwise be obvious. For
In particular, this chapter presents heuristic cate- example, many contemporary accounts of mind-
gories that sort Buddhist theories and practices fulness recognize a feature of mindfulness
into two distinct styles, the Classical and the whereby one experiences a thought (such as the
Nondual, and compares them to contemporary memory of a stressful conversation) as just a
approaches to mindfulness, especially in relation mental event. When experienced this way, the
to three crucial aspects of formal practice: ethics, thought is no longer taken to be the actual event
judgment, and present-centered awareness. (the stressful conversation) that it represents.
This phenomenon is variously called decenter-
ing (Safran & Segal, 1990), reperceiving
Why Examine Buddhist Sources? (Shapiro, Carlson, Astin, & Freedman, 2006),
cognitive insight (Chambers, Gullone, &
There are three reasons why it is useful for mind- Allen, 2009), mindful awareness (Papies,
fulness researchers and clinicians to be familiar Barsalou, & Custers, 2012), defusion (Hayes,
with Buddhist accounts. The rst and most obvious 2003), and so on. As shown below, Buddhist
materials offer a detailed theoretical account of
how this phenomenon occurs, and examining that
J.D. Dunne (*)
account may suggest avenues of scientic
Department of Religion, Emory University,
Atlanta, GA, USA research into the mechanisms that underlie the
e-mail: jdunne@emory.edu phenomenon.

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 251


B.D. Ostan et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_18
252 J.D. Dunne

A third reason to examine Buddhist sources Methodological Issues


is more complex. In brief, Buddhist traditions
promote multiple approaches to mindfulness, The approach to Buddhist sources suggested here
and these approaches involve different tech- requires one to acknowledge that there is no
niques that occur with different theoretical single authoritative Buddhist account of mind-
accounts. Certain kinds of techniques and theo- fulness. Methodologically, this way of using
ries tend to occur together, and this tendency Buddhist sources stands in contrast to some
toward coherence can prove helpful in examin- recent work (for example, Rapgay & Bystrisky,
ing the coherence of ones own approach. For 2009; Wallace, 2006) that adopts what can be
example, some Buddhist styles of mindfulness called a rhetoric of authenticity whereby con-
require the suspension of all judgment, includ- temporary approaches are compared to origi-
ing ethical judgment, during formal practice. nal, authentic, or authoritative Buddhist
For these styles, if during formal practice, one is sources that allegedly provide the true account of
seeking to make judgments such as This mental mindfulness. These claims to authenticity are
state is wholesome, or This mental state is highly problematic for a number of reasons. First,
unwholesome, then one has deviated from the on the basis of Buddhist texts and observable
practice instructions (see below and also Dunne, Buddhist practices, it is clear that there is not just
2011b). In Buddhist texts, these practice styles one traditional Buddhist version of mindfulness
tend to occur with theoretical discussions about (Table 18.1) (Gethin, 2011, 2015; Sharf, 2014).
the aforementioned capacity for decentering. In Each Buddhist tradition might claim that it har-
contrast, practice styles that require explicit bors the correct version of mindfulness, but from
judgment or ethical discernment during formal the standpoint of academic scholarship on
practice do not tend to discuss decentering Buddhism, no tradition can claim that its practice
explicitly in their traditional texts. This suggests is identical to some original, authentic practice
some coherence between the suspension of ethi- taught by the Buddha. Instead, scholarly research
cal judgment in formal practice and the use of shows that Buddhist traditions necessarily change
decentering as a clearly theorized contemplative over time, largely in response to changes within
technique. In contrast, if ones own contempo- their own cultural contexts (Braun, 2013; Harvey,
rary approach requires ethical judgment during 1990; Sharf, 1995; Van Schaik, 2004). Thus, to
formal practice but also emphasizes decenter-
ing, then one is challenging the usual paradigms
in Buddhist sources. In this way, this third rea- Table 18.1 Traditional sources of mindfulness with geo-
son for examining Buddhist sources is essen- graphical origin
tially that they can help one to detect the ways in Classical
which contemporary approaches either align Vipassan (mainstream Theravda in Thailand, Burma,
with or depart from typical Buddhist practice and Sri Lanka)
styles and theoretical accounts. In cases where amatha (Tibetan; especially Gelugpa approach)
there is alignment, appreciating the varieties of Mind Training (as formal practice; Tibetan)
Buddhist approaches can help one to determine Nondual
which particular Buddhist tradition will be most Chan (China)
helpful for prompting possible avenues of fur- Zen (Japan; derived from Chan)
ther inquiry. And where there is no such align- Seon (Korea; derived from Chan)
ment, it may suggest that contemporary Mahmudr (Tibetan)
approaches are assuming some other kind of Dzogchen (Tibetan)
coherence that stands in clear contrast to typical Thai Forest Tradition (Thailand; Nondual with
Buddhist approaches. Classical features)
18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness 253

produce some single, authentic, and authoritative plative practices are rooted in careful observations
account of mindfulness in Buddhism, not only of those practices themselves. However, some
must one ignore the diversity of views across theorizing may be driven even more strongly by a
Buddhist traditions, but one must also ignore the need to present a systematic and easily defended
historical development of individual traditions account. The upshot is that one cannot assume
themselves. that a Buddhist theoretical account of mindful-
Another problem with the notion of authen- ness is just about the actual practice of mindful-
ticity is that it assumes that Buddhist practices ness itself; the need to defend a tradition against
and theories have a direct, linear relationship to critics and the urge toward scholastic systematic-
contemporary accounts of mindfulness. The real- ity may be equally strong motivations.
ity is far more complex, as Kabat-Zinn (2011) Does all this mean that we cannot resort to
has shown. Throughout history, whenever Buddhist sources if we wish to deepen our
Buddhism emerges in new cultural contexts, new understanding of mindfulness? This would be an
forms of Buddhism arise that draw in complex overreaction. We can still use these sources, but
ways on multiple Buddhist traditions (Gethin, we must do so with care. In particular, these
1998), and this certainly applies to the forms of sources are best engaged along with the practical
Buddhist practice that are emerging in Europe expertise of an actual practice community. Texts
and North America (Goldstein, 2002; McMahan, ideally should be read in relation to the living
2008; Tweed, 1992). Although MBSR, for exam- practices of such communities, and those prac-
ple, is not a Buddhist tradition, its relationship to tices should likewise be studied independently
Buddhist sources is similarly complex. Multiple of textual interpretations through methods such
contemplative traditions, some of them not as ethnography. Likewise, multiple traditions
Buddhist, have been important sources for the should participate in the conversation with texts
development of MBSR (Kabat-Zinn, 2011). and practices in dialog across languages, cul-
Similarly, multiple sources led to the emergence tures, and contexts. At this point in the develop-
of Buddhist practices such as modern forms of ment of Contemplative Studies, however, this
Buddhist mindfulness practice in Burma (Braun, type of research is not yet available; indeed,
2013) or the emergence of Chan dialogs in medi- undertaking that research will require much col-
eval China (McRae, 2003). Claiming that MBSR, laborative work across multiple disciplines.
medieval Chinese Chan, or modern Burmese Until this lacuna is lled, our examination of
Vipassan are somehow inauthentic because mindfulness in Buddhism will be problematic,
they emerge from multiple inuences requires but this chapter nevertheless attempts to embrace
one to deny the historical reality of change and the basic principle that texts, traditions, and
transformation that characterizes all contempla- actual practice stand in a complex relationship.
tive traditions.
A nal methodological issue concerns the use
of Buddhist texts. In brief, what a text says about Heuristics for Mindfulness:
a practice does not necessarily reect the way a Contemporary, Classical,
community actually engages in that practice. In and Nondual
many cases, textual accounts are meant to be nor-
mativethey do not necessarily describe what With these methodological issues in place, we
practitioners actually do; instead, they often pre- can now move on to the main goal of this chapter:
scribe what practitioners should do (see, for exam- the heuristic presentation of two overall styles of
ple, the famed Stages of Meditation discussed in Buddhist practice that alignor misalignwith
Adam, 2003). Likewise, one might assume that contemporary approaches to mindfulness, espe-
theoretical accounts about the features of contem- cially in relation to the role of ethics, judgment,
254 J.D. Dunne

and present-centered awareness in formal practice. awareness for distractions in a non-judgmental


To proceed, however, we must rst sketch the fashion; (3) non-reactively disengaging from
heuristic categories that enable this type of analy- distractors; and (4) reorienting attention to the
sis. The rst is Contemporary Mindfulness, a target object. These general features of formal
category that seeks to capture the main features training in Contemporary Mindfulness relate to
of contemporary approaches in clinical contexts. some other aspects common to programs such as
Of course, even within clinical circles, the term MBSR. Commensurate with the emphasis on
mindfulness has a broad range of application. remaining present-centered while suspending
Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR), frameworks for judgment, the MBSR approach
for example, presents a style of mindfulness that does not expect practitioners to learn evaluative
differs in important ways from Acceptance and or ethical frameworks to use as a tool of formal
Commitment Therapy and Dialectical Behavior mindfulness practice, nor are practitioners pro-
Therapy (Chambers et al., 2009). Nevertheless, vided with any normative goals, not even the
one can point to some features that are consistent simple goal, MBSR will reduce your stress
across these various contexts. We refer to this (Kabat-Zinn, 2013). These features appear to be
cluster of features with the general rubric broadly typical of Contemporary Mindfulness,
Contemporary Mindfulness. and they are especially appropriate to a compari-
The widely accepted features of Contemporary son with Buddhist traditions.
Mindfulness can be gleaned by reviewing the To compare Contemporary Mindfulness to
common elements in formal training according Buddhist approaches, we sort Buddhist practice
multiple sources (including Bishop et al., 2004; styles and theories into two heuristic categories:
Kabat-Zinn, 2013; Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, Classical and Nondual. As heuristics, these
2002; Shapiro et al., 2006). The initial stages terms are not meant to refer to single Buddhist
involve bringing the attention to an object, most traditions or lineages of practice. Rather, they
commonly the breath. The modality of this atten- point to general trends that apply across a broad
tion and the type of object selected must both be range of practices and traditions that can be
present-centered. For example, one does not take usefully distinguished in this way. The term
as ones object a past or future event, and one Classical (Cf. Rapgay & Bystrisky, 2009)
attends to the object in such a way that attention evokes the styles of contemplative practice that
remains in the present. This is commonly accom- are rooted most directly in the Abhidharma (Pali,
plished by using the sensations of breathing or Abhidhamma)1, a group of scholastic traditions
some other sensory stimulus as an object, since whose earliest texts belong to the formative
sensory stimuli occur uniquely in the present. period of Buddhist history. Among living tradi-
When one attempts to remain attentive to such an tions, the Theravda lineages and their practice
object, distractions arise, especially for novice styles (such as Vipassan) acknowledge the
practitioners. When distractions occur, one Abhidharma to contain the most precise and
notices the distraction in a non-judgmental fash- detailed accounts of meditative practices
ion that neither elaborates conceptually nor (Anlayo, 2003; Bodhi, 2011; Gethin, 2011).
judges the moment of distraction as good or bad, Within Tibetan traditions, the relevant Classical
and so on. Having non-judgmentally noticed that styles are found in the literature on Mind Training
one has been distracted, one simply disengages (Gon nu rgyal mchog & Dkon mchog rgyal
from the distractor and reorients attention to the
target object 1
Because Sanskrit has the broadest range of application,
As described above, the basic skills cultivated technical Buddhist terms are given in Sanskrit. However,
because discussions of mindfulness often involve Pali (the
through formal training in Contemporary
language used by Theravda traditions), it is occasionally
Mindfulness include: (1) holding an object in sus- cited with its Sanskrit equivalent. Terms drawn from
tained, present-centered attention; (2) monitoring Tibetan contexts are cited in Tibetan.
18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness 255

mtshan, 2006) and amatha, especially as prac- about the suffering (duh.kha) or fundamental
ticed by the Tibetan Gelugpa school (Tsong kha dissatisfaction that is understood to characterize
pa, 2002) and as presented by Alan Wallace ordinary life, and the model likewise explains
(2006). In these contexts as well, the Abhidharma how contemplative practices can address this
approach is generally considered indispensable problem.
in the analysis of meditative states, and the prac- According to the Classical model suffering
tice techniques for mindfulness overlap consider- arises primarily due to intentions (cetan) that
ably with Theravda approaches. Several induce suffering (duh.kha) because they are pro-
centuries after the formation of the Abhidharma, duced by distorted cognitions that lead to dys-
however, other styles of practice emerged in functional actions and mental states. These
India, from where they later spread to Tibet and cognitions are distorted in that they misinterpret
China (and then to other Asian cultures such as their objects to be conducive to pleasure or hap-
Korea and Japan that initially draw on Chinese piness (sukha), whereas they are actually condu-
Buddhism). Some of these later practices required cive to pain or suffering (duh.kha). Likewise,
a stance that departed in key ways from the these cognitions interpret things that are actually
Abhidharma paradigm. In particular, some tradi- impermanent (anitya) to be permanent (nitya).
tions emphasized meditative practices that were They also falsely cognize their objects to be
mean to be nondual, in that the meditator para- somehow related to an autonomous self (tman),
digmatically cultivates states without any sub- but those objects are actually seless (antman)
jectobject duality. As discussed below, this in that they are not related to or constitutive of
emphasis on nondual states required a departure any such self. These distorted intentions and cog-
from some aspects of the Classical approach of nitions also induceand are perpetuated by
the Abhidharma and led to contemplative styles, negative mental states (klea) such as attachment
including traditions still active today, that can be and aversion that also produce suffering. A pri-
collectively characterized as Nondual. mary goal of Classical Buddhist contemplative
practice is thus to put an end to distorted inten-
tions by realizing, in a state generally known as
Classical Mindfulness2 insight (vipayan), that the objects of sensory
experience and the conditioned aspects of the
Classical Mindfulness is a heuristic category that mind are, in fact, by their nature characterized by
points to certain shared features of a range of three marks: they are impermanent, seless, and
Buddhist practices that mostly closely and explic- conducive to suffering.
itly align with the Abhidharma paradigm. The The Abhidharma analysis points to several
shared features of the Classical approach are mental facets and capacities that must be culti-
especially rooted in a model of mind whereby, in vated in order to achieve insight, but two are
ordinary persons, mental moments arise with a especially important: they are smr.ti (Pali, sati)
number of different mental facets (caitasika) and samprajanya (Pali, sampajaa). The term
including affective features (such as attraction smr.ti is usually translated as mindfulness, and
and aversion) and intentions (cetan) that can although it has a wide range of application, in
themselves be expressed in subsequent mental technical Abhidharma accounts it concerns espe-
activities or vocal or physical actions. This model cially the stability and focus that are required for
is developed in the context of Buddhist concerns the practitioner to see the true nature of objects as
the three marks of impermanence and so on.
2
An abundance of sources is available for exploring what Samprajanya, although also variable in its usage,
is here called the Classical style. This section is based is usually translated in Classical contexts as
on the following: (Anlayo, 2003; Anuruddha & Bodhi,
clear comprehension so as to evoke its primary
2000; Asanga, 2001; Bodhi, 2011; Buddhaghosa &
n.amoli, 1976; Gethin, 2011, 2015; Tsong kha pa, 2002; cognitive role in the clear apprehension of the
Vasubandhu, 2012). aforementioned three marks.
256 J.D. Dunne

Ethics in Classical Practice while during informal of or between-session


contexts, that instruction set is replaced with
To achieve the requisite degrees of mindfulness some other paradigm that seeks to prepare the
and clear comprehension for insight, the practi- practitioner for the next formal session, often by
tioner must employ contemplative techniques implementing mindfulness in ordinary activities.
such as mindfulness of breathing (Pali, Paradigmatically, Buddhist traditions also gener-
npnasati) that involve focus on a specic ally seek to cultivate in practitioners the capacity
object such as the breath. These techniques, how- to sustain in all contexts the key features targeted
ever, cannot be used if the mind is chaotic, and by contemplative training, both during formal
since mental chaos arises from an abundance of practice in a meditation session and during other
negative mental states (klea) in the mind, the activities between sessions. Thus, for the
practitioner must also adopt a lifestyle that advanced practitioner, the distinction between
reduces negative mental states. This lifestyle is formal and informal practice begins to collapse.
regulated by la, an ethical code that is designed In the Classical style, both formal and infor-
to reduce the abundance and inuence of nega- mal practice require the practitioner to maintain a
tive mental states in the practitioners mind. The heedful awareness of their activities in ethical
practice thus involves an additional mental capac- terms. Two strategies for maintaining ethical
ity: apramda, the heedfulness that keeps track restraint in informal practice are common. As the
of ones ethical vows, spiritual intentions and Sanskrit author ntideva (ca. 650 C.E.) puts it, if
goals. This capacity, especially prominent in later necessary one remains like a piece of wood
Tibetan accounts in the Classical style (for exam- when one notices that one is about to engage in
ple, Tsong kha pa, 2002), requires practitioners unethical behaviors (ntideva, 2008). Here, reg-
to be vigilant in their awareness of their mental ulation of behavior essentially amounts to a
lives so as to detect when distorted intentions and veto of distorted intentions and negative men-
negative mental states are about to lead to vocal tal states that have been heedfully detected before
or physical actions that violate the ethical code. they actually result in unethical vocal or physical
This attentiveness to mental life also draws on acts. Another strategy employed by more
and enhances both mindfulness and clear com- advanced practitioners is to prevent unethical
prehension, since they are required for one to behaviors by no longer having the distorted inten-
notice and properly understand what is happen- tions that are said to motivate all unethical action.
ing in ones mind. In the context of formal practice, To do so, the practitioner must see objects and
the ethical framework provided by la also pro- conditioned mental events as conducive to suffer-
vides the means to recognize the valence of ones ing, impermanent, and seless, since intentions
mental states as either to be adopted (updeya) are distorted only when one fails to recognize
because they are wholesome or to be abandoned these three marks. For example, using the rst
(heya) because they are unwholesome. strategy, a monk might see an attractive person,
and seeing that person as an object of pleasure, he
might experience lust (a negative mental state)
Informal Practice in the Classical Style that occurs with or induces an intention to act in
a way that would violate his vows. Having heed-
The features of Classical mindfulness presented fully noticed this lustful intention before it results
thus far concern the context of formal meditation in a behavior, the monk would recall his vows
practice, but all Buddhist traditions recognize a and exercise restraint. But following the second
distinction between formal practice contexts and strategy, when the monk sees the attractive per-
informal or between session contexts. In for- son, he would no longer experience that person as
mal practice, the practitioner is meant to imple- an object of pleasure because he would recognize
ment an instruction set of specic contemplative that what he is experiencing is in fact not
techniques for the cultivation of mindfulness, conducive to pleasure, but rather to suffering.
18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness 257

Here, there is no need to exercise a veto on action styles do not train practitioners to lead their lives
because the distorted intentions that would lead in heedful adherence to an ethical code involving
to unethical or unwholesome behavior simply do specic vows, nor does one, during formal prac-
not arise. tice, use an ethical framework to assess ones
mental states so as to identify some states as to
be abandoned while others are to be adopted.
Contemporary Mindfulness The Classical Buddhist approach also differs
and Classical Buddhist Styles: Ethics, from Contemporary Mindfulness in its emphasis
Judgment, Memory on judgment. The Classical practitioner is ideally
trained in the elaborate Abhidharma typology
Some contemplative techniques used in Con- that delineates various aspects of the mind and
temporary Mindfulness align closely with categorizes them especially in ethical terms. In
Classical Buddhist approaches. For example, the both formal and informal contexts, the Classical
cultivation of mindfulness and clear comprehen- practitioner uses this typology to clearly judge
sion through formal Buddhist practices involve what is occurring in the mind. For example, when
some skills also developed by formal training in a distraction arises during the practice of
Contemporary Mindfulness. These include the Mindfulness of Breathing, Classical practitioners
cultivation of sustained attention on an object, the do not simply recognize that a distraction is
capacity to detect distractors, and the ability to occurring; additionally, they clearly identify the
drop distractors and reorient to the target object in mental state as, for example, motivated by inten-
a way that does not perturb the mind further. tions occurring with lust, and in this act of dis-
However, as Rupert Gethin, Bhikkhu Bodhi, Alan cernment, they also clearly know that lust is
Wallace, and others have noted, Contemporary something to be abandoned. Likewise, Classical
Mindfulness differs from Classical Buddhist practice trains the practitioner in other key judg-
practices in signicant ways. Perhaps most obvi- ments, namely, that any conditioned object of
ous is the prominent role played by ethics in the experience is conducive to suffering, imperma-
Classical paradigm. nent, and seless. In short, the Classical paradigm
In contrast to Classical practices, Contemporary requires the practitioner to employ judgment in a
Mindfulness does not emphasize a paradigmatic manner that explicitly connects to an ethics of
ethical framework that must be strictly adopted by what one seeks to abandon or cultivate.
each practitioner. Certainly, core values such as In contrast, Contemporary Mindfulness
loving kindness and compassion, which are also emphasizes a non-judgmental approach. For
essential to the Buddhist paradigm, are central to example, when in formal practice a distraction
Contemporary Mindfulness (Shapiro et al., 2006), occurs, practitioners are not taught to evaluate the
but these are not presented as constituting a spe- mental state as something to be abandoned or cul-
cic ethical code that each practitioner must tivated, nor does one deploy some typology to
adopt. One reason for this difference may be that analyze that mental moment. Instead, one simply
requiring a particular ethical approach would recognizes that one is distracted and returns to the
prove problematic in secular clinical contexts, focal object (such as the sensations of breathing)
where it seems far more workable to allow partici- without any further conceptual elaboration (typi-
pants to address ethical issues from their own, cal instructions are found in Kabat-Zinn, 2013;
personal perspectives (Kabat-Zinn, 2013). Segal et al., 2002). Even if one argues that there is
Likewise, certain cultural factors surrounding still some type of discernment about ones men-
religion and spirituality within modernity often tal life that is trained through Contemporary
favor an individualistic and personal approach to Mindfulness, there is clearly no attempt to guide
ethics, rather than strict adherence to an institu- practitioners toward the full-blown deployment of
tionally imposed code (McMahan, 2008). In any ethically charged judgment found in the Classical
case, it is clear that Contemporary Mindfulness Buddhist paradigm. In particular, there is no
258 J.D. Dunne

explicit attempt to foster in the practitioner a for example, to maintain the mental facet smr.ti
judgment of objects or thoughts as conducive to does not mean that one remembers the sensa-
suffering, impermanent, or seless. tions of breathing; instead, it means that one sus-
tains attention on those sensations without
becoming distracted away from them.
Smti and the Role of Memory Even though the technical Abhidharma deni-
tion of the mental facet smr.ti thus should not be
Another point of divergence between Classical understood as memory in any literal sense, the
Buddhist styles and Contemporary Mindfulness Classical Buddhist styles of practice nevertheless
is the role played by memory (Dreyfus, 2011). To clearly involve a form of memory, especially
address this question, however, it is rst impor- when this is understood in contrast to the empha-
tant to note some confusion about smr.ti, the sis on non-judgmental, present-centered aware-
Buddhist term translated as mindfulness itself. ness in Contemporary Mindfulness. As mentioned
As various authors have shown, smr.ti has a wide earlier, a central theme of formal training in
range of application, some of which bear on its Contemporary Mindfulness is the need to sustain
literal meaning, which is indeed memory. For awareness in the present in a way that does not
example, in one important early text, the stray to thoughts of the past or future, and one is
Milindapanha, the term smr.ti is at one point used likewise not to recollect any elaborate conceptual
in a manner that evokes the heedfulness men- apparatus or keep in mind any predetermined
tioned above, whereby to be mindful is to rec- goals. This emphasis on remaining in the now
ollect and keep in mind ones vows, ethical without conceptual elaboration cannot be over-
commitments, and spiritual goals (Wallace, stated. For Classical Buddhist styles of practice,
2006). Rupert Gethin (Gethin, 2015) further however, memory is crucially important, espe-
points out that smr.ti (or more precisely, its Pali cially when memory is understood in the sense of
form sati) is most frequently dened in the recollecting or keeping in mind a conceptual
Pali Nikyas with reference to someone who is apparatus and ones spiritual goals. In the context
mindful, possesses perfect mindfulness and of formal practice, the ideal Classical practitioner
understanding, one who remembers, one who must recollect the Abhidharma typology that
recollects things done and things said long enables one to monitor and recognize the various
before. Nevertheless, despite the use of smr.ti/ mental states as they arise, especially in terms of
sati in a manner that equates strongly with a those states that are to be abandoned or culti-
straightforward sense of memory, the technical vated. And this means that ones overall spiritual
Abhidharma denition of smr.ti (i.e., mindful- goalsin terms of which mental life is to be
ness) does not have this meaning. Instead, the shapedmust also be kept in mind. Thus,
sense of smr.ti as literally memory is here although the Classical styles of formal practice
apparently used in a metaphorical sense. That is, can be considered present-centered in that they
technical Abhidharma denitions of smr.ti note generally do not prompt the practitioner to focus
that it functions so as to prevent one from los- on memories of past events or future actions, they
ing (sampramos.a) the object (Anuruddha & nevertheless require the retention or recollection
Bodhi, 2000; Asanga, 2001; Bodhi, 2011; of an elaborate conceptual apparatus. And clearly,
Buddhaghosa & n. amoli, 1976; Gethin, 2011; in the context of informal practice, the Classical
Vasubandhu, 2012). The metaphor here appears Buddhist style of the mindful life requires an
to be that losing focus on an object is akin to even more intensive recollection of vows, ethics,
forgetting the object, and thus the mental and goals.
facet that prevents one from losing that focus While Contemporary Mindfulness and
can be metaphorically referred to as remem- Classical Buddhist styles clearly diverge on the
bering (smr.ti), since to remember is not to role of memory, one point of convergence is
forget. Thus, during Mindfulness of Breathing, worth noting here. Even for the practitioner of
18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness 259

Contemporary Mindfulness who cultivates a Mindfulness in the context of formal practice, and
present-centered awareness without conceptual as Jon Kabat-Zinn notes (2011), these styles have
elaboration, a kind of recollective memory must had a direct historical impact on the develop-
be operative at times. This occurs most obviously ment of MBSR, more so than any Classical style.
at the time of distraction in formal practice, when To more easily understand the overall rubric
to recognize the mental state as distracted, the of the Nondual Style, we examine the issue of
practitioner must recall at least that one is to be subjectobject duality and then survey a set of
undistracted. Or, more elaborately, one must philosophical tools that emerge along with this
recall that one intends to focus on the task at insight.
hand, such as remaining aware of the sensations
of breathing in the present moment. This form
of recollection seems akin to recollective mem- SubjectObject Duality
ory in Buddhist Classical styles, but in the latter and Suffering3
context this form of memory plays a much
broader role. Some time near the beginning of the Common
As noted previously, various scholarly works Era, various developments within Buddhism led
have discussed the ways that Contemporary to the emergence of the Mahyna or Great
Mindfulness differs from Classical Buddhist Vehicle, and this brought important changes to
approaches, and when one examines the issues of theories and practices around suffering and its
ethics, judgment, and memory, those divergences solution. By around the third or fourth century
are indeed clear. One might thus conclude that (C.E.) a new philosophical approach emerged
Contemporary Mindfulness has little in common that reconceptualized the fundamental problem
with its allegedly Buddhist roots or that it is some of suffering. This new approach still embraced
watered-down version of Buddhist meditation. many features of the Abhidharma model, espe-
Such conclusions, however, would be premature cially the insight that eliminating suffering
because other styles of Buddhist practicethose requires one to uproot the fundamental causes
that cultivate nondual experiencesalign more that make dysfunctional behavior possible.
closely with Contemporary Mindfulness, albeit Unlike the Classical Abhidharma approach, how-
only in the context of formal practice. ever, these new thinkers claimed that the root
problem lies even deeper than distorted inten-
tions and their concomitant negative mental
Nondual Buddhist Styles states. Instead, this deeper structure is what
makes it even possible to have any intentions
As Buddhism developed in India during the rst whatsoever as an agent acting on a world that,
millennium (C.E.), new philosophies and contem- from the standpoint of an agents subjectivity, is
plative approaches emerged, and some departed out there (bhya). In short, the subtler distor-
in important ways from the core Abhidharma tion in experience is this distinction between self
paradigm. These styles can be grouped under the and world, or more precisely, the structure of a
rubric Nondual because they seek to induce in distinct subjectivity standing over against distinct
the practitioner a state in which the subjectobject objects of experience. Articulated in a
structure of ordinary experiences has subsided. philosophical approach known as Yogcra
As this later form of Buddhism spreads, it strongly (Practice of Yoga) by thinkers such as Asaga
informs the Mahmudr and Dzogchen traditions and Vasubandhu (both fourth century C.E.), this
of Tibet and the Chan traditions of China; and theoretical account of suffering also promotes a
through Chinese Chan, Nondual styles appear in
Japanese Zen and Korean Seon Buddhism. As we 3
Two useful, if competing accounts of the issues dis-
shall see, several aspects of these Nondual styles cussed in this section are Gold (2014) and Lusthaus
align more favorably with Contemporary (Lusthaus, 2002).
260 J.D. Dunne

contemplative solution to this fundamental Concept Formation5


problem: the practitioner must cultivate an expe-
rience in which this false distinction between Another crucial philosophical tool that emerged
subject and object disappears in a nondual along with the nondual approach was a robust
(advaya) experience. account of concept formation known as the
Apoha theory. Developed by Buddhist epistemol-
ogists such as Dharmakrti (seventh century C.E.)
A Key Philosophical Tool: who follow the Yogcra philosophy, only three
Phenomenal Forms4 of this complex theorys details are relevant.
First, since this theory draws on the notion of
The notion of subjectobject duality as the root phenomenological forms, it presents concepts as
of suffering emerged and developed along with also involving mental representations. Thus, the
new philosophical tools (arising from the third to thought of an apple arises with a phenomeno-
seventh centuries C.E.) that were not available logical content that is ordinarily experienced as
during the early formation of the Abhidharma. somehow referring to or identical with actual
One crucial new tool was the analysis of experi- apples. However, the phenomenological form or
ence as involving phenomenal forms (kra). mental representation of an apple that arises
Specically, Buddhist epistemologists in the when one thinks apple is actually just a feature
Yogcra tradition maintained that, in ordinary of consciousness itself. Thus, as with the visual
experience, a phenomenal form of subjectivity consciousness of a color, the phenomenological
(grhakkra) always arises simultaneously with presentation that appears when one thinks
a phenomenal form of the object (grhykra). apple can be experienced as what it truly is,
On this model, a moment of visual conscious- namely, just a feature of consciousness itself.
ness, for example, is always divided into these This theory enables a contemplative method
two phenomenal forms, even though both forms whereby the disturbing thought of, for example, a
are actually just mind itself. A visual experience stressful event can be experienced not as the
of an object is thus not a direct apprehension of a object it represents (i.e., the stressful event), but
thing external to consciousness; instead, what rather as just a phenomenological form in con-
one directly contacts is a mental representation sciousness. As noted above, this is the Buddhist
(i.e., the phenomenal form of the object) that theory that underlies the contemplative technique
arises through a causal process. This model thus known in Contemporary Mindfulness by numer-
permits one to perform a type of phenomenologi- ous terms, such as decentering, cognitive insight,
cal reduction, such that the phenomenological mindful awareness, and defusion.
form of the object may be experienced not as rep- A second relevant feature of the Apoha theory
resenting what caused it, but rather as an element of concept formation is the notion that all con-
in experience itself. Thus, when one experiences cepts are necessarily formed in an approach/
the color blue, that color can be experienced not avoidance context. As Dharmakrti puts it, we do
as an object out there in the world, but rather as not use concepts simply out of some bad habit;
just a representation (vijaptimtra) that is not rather, we do so because we are organizing our
actually separate from the visual consciousness experience in terms of what we seek to obtain
itself. Likewise, the sense of subjectivity that (heya) or what we seek to avoid or eliminate
occurs with the visual experience of blue also is (updeya). If an object is not taken into this
just a phenomenal form that is in fact not distinct approach/avoidance framework, the mind will
from the phenomenal form of the object, in that not conceptualize it because it is not relevant to
both are simply features of a single moment of our actions in the world. An irrelevant object of
visual awareness. this kind might appear as a eeting sensory
impression, but it will not go through the full-
4
For resources to explore this issue, see Dreyfus (1997),
5
Dunne (2004), Arnold (2012) and Coseru (2012). The account given here is based on Dunne (2004, 2011a).
18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness 261

blown process of conceptualization described by ness, a form of nondual awareness is already


the Apoha theory. Importantly, this means that occurring. Known by the technical term reex-
concepts are always tied to a sense of oneself as a ive awareness (svasam.vitti), this aspect of con-
goal-oriented agent acting in the world. And for sciousness is nondual in the sense that when
this reasons, when one uses concepts, one is nec- information is obtained through reexive aware-
essarily operating through the dualistic self/world ness, it does not mean that a phenomenal sense of
or subject/object structure described above. subjectivity is focusing on that informations
Finally, a third relevant feature of the Apoha source as an object. Consider, for example, the
theory is that concepts necessarily involve an experience of an intensely beautiful sunset.
association of the present mental content with During the experience, one is fully focused on the
some previous experience. The thought of an visual object, yet if asked later how one felt, one
apple, for example, always draws on previous can report reliably on ones subjective sense of
experience, such that the phenomenological con- awe and so on. The claim here is that one has a
tent presented in the thought is construed as the capacity to make a reliable report without turning
same kind of thing as the phenomenological con- inward and observing the features of the experi-
tent that occurred when we saw something we ence that concerned oneself as a subject. One
called an apple yesterday. Concepts thus neces- need not make this turn because, even without
sarily draw one out of the present, at least to the having introspected in a way that makes ones
extent that present experience is being associated own subjectivity an object of observation, some
with past experiences. Likewise, concepts often aspect of consciousness was already aware of
connect to anticipated future experiences, such those subjective features. Likewise, in at least a
that the phenomenological content in the present minimal way, the sense of oneself as the subject
thought of an apple is imaginatively associated seeing that object is already presented in the
with the apples that will be bought at the store experience, even without turning inward and
tomorrow. In the psychological literature, per- observing, I am the one seeing this sunset.
haps the most striking example of this aspect of For Nondual styles of practice, reexive
conceptual thought is Mental Time Travel: the awareness is important in two ways. First, reex-
projection of oneself into the past or future dur- ive awareness does not employ a subjectobject
ing the mental simulations that constitute epi- structure, and second, it is present in every
sodic memory (Suddendorf & Corballis, 2007). moment of ordinary, dualistic consciousness. On
Thus, when I imagine myself walking through this view, it must be present because it is what
the aisles of the supermarket in search of the best accounts for the fact that dualistic experience
apples, I am engaged in a conceptually con- always includes a sense of subjectivity. In the
structed simulation that fully pulls me out of the context of contemplative practice, this means that
present moment. Likewise, when I ruminate by inducing a nondual state does not require devel-
imaginatively reliving my failure to nd good oping some new capacity of awareness. Rather, it
apples yesterday, this time traveling feature of involves enhancing an innate feature of con-
conceptual thought is operative. sciousness while also using techniques that make
the dualistic structures subside.

Reflexive Awareness6
Reflexive Awareness and Monitoring
Another crucial philosophical tool that arose in
support of Nondual contemplative approaches The theory of reexive awareness has an impor-
was the notion that, even in ordinary conscious- tant implication: namely, that one can cultivate a
capacity to monitor awareness even while one
6
is still focused on an object. It would seem that,
This section is based on the account given in the third
chapter of Dharmakrtis Pramn.avrttika as presented even for Classical approaches, some type of mon-
in Dunne (2012). See also Arnold (2012). itoring capacity is necessary so that, especially at
262 J.D. Dunne

more advanced stages of practice, one can recognize terms of direct historical inuence, the Tibetan,
the quality of awareness and make appropriate Japanese, and Korean Nondual styles are those
adjustments without losing the object of aware- that have been especially important in the devel-
ness. For example, it is acknowledged that opment of Contemporary Mindfulness. However,
advanced practitioners notice when agitation is it is crucial to note that even within Theravda
arising and can adjust accordingly even before Buddhism, whose textual lineages are depicted
mindful focus on an object is actually lost. The as rooted in the Classical Abhidharma
notion of reexive awareness provides a model approaches, Nondual features also appear. Most
whereby this monitoring can be accomplished notably, the Thai forest tradition as articulated in
without dropping ones focus on the object. the works of the twentieth century luminary
Importantly, the historical development of reex- Buddhadsa Bhikkhu, has clear elements of a
ive awareness in the Buddhist epistemological Nondual approach (see, for example, Buddhadsa
tradition occurs along with a reinterpretation of Bhikkhu, 1997).
samprajanya, a key faculty in the Classical model In India, where Nondual styles appear to have
noted above. Whereas in earlier Classical materi- originated, the clearest example of a Nondual
als samprajanya connotes a kind of clear com- contemplative style is found in Mahmudr, a
prehension, it becomes reinterpreted as precisely tradition that emerges at the end of the rst mil-
this type of monitoring function. A clear example lennium from various sources, including devel-
is found in the work of the Classical author opments within the epistemological approach to
ntideva (active c. 700 C.E.). Even though he Yogcra and tantric contemplative methods. The
explicitly rejects the Yogcra account of reex- Mahmudr literature is especially useful for the
ive awareness, he nevertheless interprets sam- way it strikes a deliberate stance in opposition to
prajanya as the moment by moment the Classical Abhidharma paradigm. This stance
examination of the state of mind and body is important because the rhetoric of Nonduality
(ntideva, 2008: 5.108, kyacittvasthyh. and some key instructions for contemplative
pratyaveks. muhurmuhuh.) that is cultivated practicecan only be understood through its
alongside mindfulness (smr.ti). From the Nondual opposition to some aspects of the Classical para-
perspective, the reexive monitoring that is digm. For example, one important rhetorical
employed during meditation on an object could theme (and an explicit instruction in formal prac-
initially be cultivated in that context. Later, one tices) is that Mahmudr does not involve any-
drops the object such that one remains in just the thing to be abandoned (heya) or anything to be
monitoring state, provided that the term moni- accomplished or adopted (updeya). This attitude
toring does not imply a subjectobject structure. does not make sense if one does not understand
that it stands against the Classical approach
whereby the practitioner deliberately seeks to
Maha mudra in Contrast abandon unwholesome or unethical mental states
to the Classical Style while cultivating or adopting virtues such as
compassion and insight.
Nondual Buddhist styles of contemplative prac- Maitrpa (eleventh century C.E., also known
tice arose historically in the context of the theo- as Advayavajra) is an especially important Indian
retical developments sketched above, and they exponent of Mahmudr whose works often
spread from India to other parts of Asia along exhibit this othering of the Classical para-
with Mahyna Buddhism. In Tibet, Nondual digm. In an especially telling example of such
approaches are found in Mahmudr and an inversion, he claims that what one is to culti-
Dzogchen (Tibetan rDzogs Chen, Great vate is not mindfulness (smr.ti), but non-mind-
Perfection), and in China, the various Chan tra- fulness (asmr.ti) (Higgins, 2008; Mathes, 2008).
ditions emerge from this style. From China, Chan Philosophically, the point here is that in the
then leads to Zen in Japan and Seon in Korea. In Abhidharma account, mindfulness is a mental
18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness 263

facet that prevents the mind from losing track of instructions and practical aphorisms (man
its object. Employing some of the philosophical ngag) that continued to be used by Tibetan teach-
tools discussed earlier, one must conclude that ers. Second, it presents these instructions in a
mindfulness in this sense could only occur when clear, extensive, and systematic way that is some-
there is subjectobject duality, since the phenom- what atypical in this tradition. In Ocean,
enal presentation of an object necessarily occurs Wangchg Dorj gathers together instructional
with the phenomenal presentation of a phenome- materials from various sources, including per-
nal subject. And since duality is the primary haps some that were previously only passed
source of delusion, this type of mindfulness is through an oral tradition, and he addresses the
still caught within it. If nondual experiences are entire scope of the Mahmudr ending in the full
what one elicits in practice, then one should not realization of nondual primordial wisdom
seek to cultivate mindfulness; practice should (gnyis su med pai ye shes). For our purposes, we
instead elicit non-mindfulness, which is often need examine only the instructions for the begin-
conceptualized as an objectless mindfulness of ner, since they are most suited to a comparison
mere non-distraction (Tibetan, ma yengs tsam with Contemporary Mindfulness.
gyi dran pa). Ocean begins with a presentation of prelimi-
While striking, this type of rhetoric can be nary practices (sngon gro) that draw on the
misunderstood, for it may seem to imply a whole- larger ethical and spiritual framework of the
sale rejection of the Classical paradigm. This is Classical approach. The beginner is thus pre-
clearly not the case, especially because the para- sumed to be thoroughly trained in that framework
digm for informal practice between sessions prior to formal Mahmudr practice. The formal
often involves the same type of ethical heedful- practice itself starts with the cultivation of
ness found in the Classical style (Wallis, 2003). amatha or Calm Abiding, which then pro-
Likewise, this type of Nondual rhetoric implies ceeds to the cultivation of the vipayan or
certain contemplative techniques, but the way Insight through which one realizes dualistic
Maitrpas rhetoric maps onto actual practice in experiences to be delusional and actually attains
India remains unclear. As with early Chan nondual experience. A peculiar feature of
sources in China (Sharf, 2014), the Indian sources Mahmudr, however, is that the basic training in
on actual practice are often laconic at best. To Calm Abiding can lead directly to Insight, and
examine the issue of practice in comparison with for this reason, even the beginners instructions
Contemporary Mindfulness, we will turn to a are couched in a nondual rhetoric that draws on
later formulation of Mahmudr in Tibet. the philosophical tools discussed earlier.

Practice in Tibetan Mah


amudra Basic Mah
amudra Instructions
for Formal Practice
This brief foray into the instructions for
Mahmudr practice aims to demonstrate how a In Ocean, the instructions for beginners are pre-
Nondual style aligns in important ways with sented as General (spyi) and then Specic
Contemporary Mindfulness, especially in terms (bye brag). Both sets of instructions seek to culti-
of the approach to judgment, memory, and ethics vate mental stability (sems gnas) through
during formal practice. To be as specic as pos- mindfulness consisting in mere non-distraction
sible, we examine a single, widely used text, The (ma yengs tsam gyi dran pa) in a way that, para-
Ocean of Definitive Meaning by the 9th Karmapa digmatically, is not focused on any object. The
Wangchg Dorj (Karma Dbang Phyug Rdo Rje, General Instructions teach this directly, such that
2006). There are two advantages to examining the beginner attempts a form of objectless medi-
this text. First, presented as a manual for medita- tation at the outset. Because the unusual begin-
tion instructors, it records many of the practice ners who fully succeed in implementing the
264 J.D. Dunne

general instructions can proceed directly to a seeks to obtain or abandon. A typical passage
nondual awareness, it is said that they have along these lines reads:
already realized Mahmudr (phyag chen rtogs Thus, do not give your mind work to do. Let it go,
pa); that is, they have already achieved a degree and without meditating on anything, rest it in a
of Insight. Others, however, must proceed relaxed, open and clear way in a state of mere non-
through the Specic Instructions that teach medi- distraction without making any adjustments at all.
Relax openly into a state without expectations or
tations on various objects and then lead the prac- judgments. In that state, do not chase the past, do
titioner to objectless practice. not invite the future. Place awareness in the present
In keeping with the rhetorical style of such without correction or expectation
traditions, the basic General Instructions are
strikingly simple: Do not chase the past; do not The overall effect of these instructions is to
invite the future; rest the awareness occurring encourage in practitioners the attitude that they
now in a clear and nonconceptual state. As these are not engaged in anything, not even meditation.
instructions are unpacked, it is clear that for One is not to hope that will obtain something
beginners, the main obstacle to advancement is laudable or fear that something undesirable will
the tendency to become caught in thoughts. Thus, not be abandoned. The task is not to evaluate
when one chases after the past, a thought of the what is occurring in the mind, nor to focus on an
past does not just occur on its own, but rather object. One simply remains undistracted in the
leads to an entire chain of thoughts. Likewise, in present, where mere non-distraction in part
inviting the future, the same tendency to means that one sustains an awareness that is not
become ensnared in a chain of thoughts pertains. caught by the goal-oriented, approach/avoidance
This ensnarement in thought keeps the practitio- structures that pull one into a chain of thoughts.
ner in a dualistic state because, as mentioned
above, conceptual consciousness is necessarily
dualistic. The practitioner is thus instructed to Self-Liberation of Thoughts
remain in the present, since as long as awareness as Decentering or Dereification
remains in the present, it cannot time travel in
the manner required for thought to operate. Another crucial tool offered to the novice practi-
tioner emerges from the previously discussed
theory of concept formation whereby the phe-
Let Go, Dont Correct, Be Free nomenal content when one thinks apple can be
of Expectation experienced just as a facet of mind, rather than as
somehow representing an actual apple. In
To aid in cultivating present-centered awareness, Mahmudr terminology, this is known as the
the novice is given other tools that also inhibit self liberation (rang grol) that occurs when one
another requirement for thought to operate, looks intently (cer gyis lta) at a thought. To do
namely, the approach/avoidance stance of an so, one must not become caught in the chain of
agent acting in the world. Three instructions are thoughts that the thought induces; instead, one
especially frequent in this regard. First, one must must remain present-centered and direct attention
let go (lhod kyis glod). Rather than direct the intently to the thought itself as an appearance in
mind toward an object or compel it to enter into a the present moment of mind. Beheld in this fash-
particular state, one releases any such deliberate ion, the thought subsides or self liberates, and it
effort. This instruction is often accompanied by thus fails to induce a chain of thoughts about past
another: do not attempt to correct, adjust, or or future. As noted previously, this closely
repair (bcos) the mind. And this admonition is approaches the notion known as decentering,
frequently amplied by noting that one should be defusion, and so on in the literature on
free of expectations especially about what one Contemporary Mindfulness.
18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness 265

Suspension of Judgment and Ethics ity to sustain awareness without becoming caught
in thoughts. Unpacking this further, in the Specic
When Wangchg Dorj moves on to the Specic Instructions Wangchg Dorj suggests an exer-
Instructions that guide practitioners who are cise that involves staring intently at a visual object
unable to initially engage with the objectless such as a small stone. He elaborates:
practice discussed by the General Instructions, he Without thinking of its features such as thickness,
explicitly addresses the suspension of the length or color, just release what is seen into its
Classical ethical typology in which some mental own place and, without distraction, make it such
facets are to be abandoned while others are to that the continuity of mindfulness is just not cut.
That focal object of meditation [i.e., the stone, etc.]
be cultivated. Discussing a practice where one is just a reminder or prompt. Hence, directing the
focuses on whatever sensory or mental impres- gaze of mere non-distraction toward it, one lets go
sion that arises, Wangchg Dorj comments: and settles awareness. It is not the case that one is
meditating on that object.
In particular, thoughts as mental objects may arise.
Some may be to be abandoned, such as the ve
poisonous mental facets that are attachment, aver- In this passage, mindfulness is not a faculty that
sion and so on. Some may be virtues to be adopted, maintains stable attention on an object; if that were
such as generosity. And some may be neutral. But
whatever thought arises, one should one-pointedly the case, one would indeed be meditating on the
attend to it and settle [awareness on it such that the stone. Instead, it is the mere non-distraction of the
thought self liberates]. Some say that one should mind that does indeed occur when the mind is
deliberately suppress thoughts to be abandoned, settled on an object, but (at least for Mahmudr)
but if one does so, then it will just increase concep-
tuality and it will be difcult for concentration can also occur in objectless states. This then relates
(samdhi) to arise. Therefore, whatever thought to another term that Wangchg Dorj uses, the
arises, one should not see the thought as a fault, spy of mindfulness (dran pai so pa).
one should just let it go and intently settle on the Wangchg Dorj uses the metaphor of the spy
thought itself. Without for a moment falling into a
scattered state, recognize each thought, one after or lookout at several places in Ocean, but the
the other. Then rest for a while. metaphor is most prominent when connected to
meditation on objects. The spy is the aspect of
Here, the typology of negative mental states to mind that observes the quality of the object-
be abandoned and virtues to be cultivated has focused state in a manner that appears to be
been set aside, since in this context judgments of similar to the monitoring function of reexive
that kind will simply proliferate and ensnare the awareness mentioned above. The exact relation-
practitioner further in thought. Nevertheless, the ship between mindfulness as a spy and mind-
rationale for setting aside the Classical typology fulness as mere non-distraction is not entirely
is not that the framework is itself somehow faulty, clear, but it appears that the capacity to monitor
but rather that in formal Mahmudr practice, ones attention is a coarser version of mere non-
any such conceptuality will be an obstacle. This distraction. If this is correct, then training through
clearly leaves a place for that ethical framework object-focused meditation can lead to an object-
in other contexts. less practice of mere non-distraction precisely
because monitoring and mere non-distraction
draw on the same objectless, reexive aspect of
Mindfulness in Mah
amudra awareness7. It is clear, in any case, that Wangchg

Another crucial feature of the instructions for the 7


This is certainly the opinion of another Mahmudr
beginner is the notion of mindfulness (Tibetan, author, Tsl Natsg Rangdrl (rTse le sna tshogs rang
dran pa) itself. In Ocean, the General Instructions grol, b. 1608), who understands mindfulness to be the
reexive monitoring aspect of amatha and who sees that
speak of a mindfulness that is mere nondistrac- mindfulness as itself becoming nondual insight (Sna
tion, and in part this clearly consists in a capac- tshogs rang grol & Kunsang, 2009).
266 J.D. Dunne

Dorj sees object-focused practice as a means to may prove useful for inquiring into the mecha-
create a state where one need only drop the object nisms of mindfulness and its features such as
so as to transition to the main practice described decentering or cognitive insight. In any case, if
in the General Instructions. In short, to cite one were to compare Contemporary Mindfulness
Wangchg Dorjs own citation of a well-known only with the Classical Buddhist paradigm, one
Sanskrit verse, Relying on object focus, the might conclude that its account of formal prac-
objectless state arises. tice departs in signicant and troubling way from
its Buddhist roots. When one turns to Nondual
styles, however, the techniques for formal prac-
mudra
Maha and Contemporary tice appear quite similar.
Mindfulness

When compared to the Classical Buddhist A Difference Between Sessions


paradigm, the basic instructions for formal
Mahmudr practice differ starkly on the issues In other ways, however, Contemporary Mind-
of judgment, memory, and ethics. Clearly, fulness still differs signicantly from the
Mahmudr formal instructions require one to be Mahmudr approach. The most crucial issue is
non-judgmental, in that one is not to engage the role of context, especially in relation to spiri-
with any conceptual evaluation during formal tuals goals and ethics. Although the instructions
practice. Instead, one releases all expectations or for formal Mahmudr practice require practitio-
evaluative paradigms, and when distracting ners to set aside any goal-oriented stance, one
thoughts occur, one does not judge them as virtu- becomes eligible for such instruction only after
ous or non-virtuous. Instead, one simply looks an intensive period of training in preliminary
intently at the thought in the present moment practices that instill, for example, an intense
and, having been experienced as just a feature of concern for the suffering of others and a strong
mind itself, the thought self liberates or dissi- motivation to become capable of relieving that
pates on its own. Since all conceptual judgment is suffering. Moreover, every meditation session
suspended, one also does not recollect any typol- begins with a rehearsal of these preliminary prac-
ogy for the evaluation of thoughts, since one tices, most especially those that refresh ones
would then be chasing after the past. Likewise, commitment to that goal. Only then does the
as shown clearly above, the ethical framework of session proceed to the actual Mahmudr prac-
the Abhidharma paradigm must also be sus- tice in which all such concerns are set aside.
pended during formal practice. Likewise, ethics play an important role in the
At rst glance, then, basic Mahmudr prac- larger context of Mahmudr practice. It is true
tice clearly aligns much more closely with some that this tradition admits room for the Madman
key features of Contemporary Mindfulness, (Tibetan, smyon pa), the highly realized practi-
including the emphasis on being present-centered tioner whose antinomian behavior transcends
and the non-judgmental stance of practice. And all ethical categories (DiValerio, 2011). Yet, in
this should come as no surprise, since Nondual social terms, the image of the Madman also
Buddhist traditions are key sources for the devel- serves to locate the novice practitioner squarely
opment of Contemporary Mindfulness. Further within the practice of the Classical Buddhist
research on contemplative theory and techniques ethical code, for as a novice, one cannot hope to
in Zen and Seon, for example, would surely enjoy the Nondual insight that is depicted in the
reveal similar parallels (see, for example, Kim, gure of the Madman. His excesses often serve
2007). The alignment of Nondual styles with to highlight the standard ethical and institu-
Contemporary Mindfulness further suggests that tional norms.
the theoretical Buddhist background on concept In the context of informal practice between
formation, monitoring, and reexivity, for example, sessions, both the Nondual and the Classical
18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness 267

approaches require the practitioner to adopt a framework of that kind. It would appear that,
paradigm of the proper Buddhist life along with similar to Mahmudr, an appeal is made to the
its ethical norms. Nondual traditions claim that emergence of innate capacities (Kabat-Zinn,
this ethical paradigm is somehow an innate 2013), but unlike Mahmudr, there is no notion
capacity that emerges naturally, but setting this that, despite the innateist rhetoric, an explicit
aside for the moment, one can instead hypothe- paradigm is still necessary.
size that the disagreement here is about the meth- If one examines the reasons for the Mahmudr
ods in formal practice that best facilitate the traditions promotion of an explicit ethical frame-
adoption of the Buddhism life paradigm between work between sessions, one possibility is that, if
sessions. The Classical traditions emphasize left simply to rely on the emergence of allegedly
techniques that reinforce and amplify the para- innate qualities, some practitioners at various
digm during formal practice. In contrast, the stages of development will not exhibit the types
Nondual approach appears to be based on an of behaviors and personal transformation that the
insight: namely, that techniques which purport to tradition seeks. By training practitioners in an
set aside all paradigms during formal practice explicit paradigm between sessions, the tradition
will more readily facilitate the adoption of the thus guarantees, in a sense, that only the alleg-
new paradigm during informal practice between edly innate qualities of wisdom, compassion and
sessions. It may further be the case that these so on emerge, rather than some other outcome
approaches both persist in many Buddhist cultural that results when the between session framework
contexts because they are found to be effective in is just left to the practitioner. In any case, a clear
producing similar behavioral results for persons assumption here is that some kind of framework
with different cognitive or affective styles. for engagement with the world must be present
Turning to Contemporary Mindfulness, it is between sessions, even if one seeks to suspend all
clear that the techniques for formal practice align such frameworks during formal practice.
signicantly with those found in Mahmudr, If rather than some kind of innate, natural
and it is possible to demonstrate a similar align- engagement with the world, practitioners instead
ment with other Nondual traditions such as necessarily deploy learned (or personally
Dzogchen and Zen (see, for example, Suzuki, invented) paradigms and frameworks for practice
2006 and Van Schaik, 2004). A signicant diver- between sessions, how would this apply to
gence emerges, however, when one examines the Contemporary Mindfulness? One possibility is
approach to contexts outside of formal practice. that, even in the absence of an explicit frame-
Here, in what is termed the between session work, an implicit one is being provided. In
context, Dorjs Ocean instructs Mahmudr MBSR, for example, that framework would
practitioners to adopt and enact the core features emerge from the use of carefully selected poems
of Classical styles paradigm, including an by Rumi, Mary Oliver, and others. Education in
emphasis on a heedful engagement with the the physiology of stress would be another com-
world in a way that avoids unethical activity and ponent. These and other aspects of MBSR train-
cultivates virtues such as generosity and compas- ing may suggest some key elements in a
sion. Nondual traditions such as Mahmudr will framework for engagement with the world that
claim that this ethical engagement emerges natu- practitioners then complete through their own
rally from the innate capacities of the practitioner creative appropriation of other sources and their
(for more on the Innateism in Nondual tradi- life histories. This does not in itself seem prob-
tions, see Dunne, 2011b), but even with this inna- lematic, but if the self-invented paradigm goes in
teist or nativist rhetoric in place, practitioners are certain directions, it does leave itself open to the
still explicitly and extensively trained in the ethi- critique of the cultural critique Slavoj iek
cal paradigm that they are to adopt between ses- (2001, 2012). In effect, he maintains that mind-
sions. Contemporary Mindfulness, in contrast, fulness has become popular because it serves to
does not usually promote any explicit ethical dampen the distress and horror of global capitalism.
268 J.D. Dunne

During sessions, one alleviates the pain, and Table 18.2 Classical and nondual: features of target
state in formal practice
between sessions, one returns to being a good
producer and consumer. Mindfulness thus Classical Nondual
becomes the opiate of the elite. While ieks Object focus Meditative states Novices may
always have an use an object,
critique is typically hyperbolic, it may not
object but eventually
entirely miss the mark. all objects are
dropped
Ethical Required Suspended
Conclusion: Using the Heuristic judgment
Conceptual Vows recollected All conceptual
schemas and Abhidharma schemas
In practical terms, the heuristic account presented categories used suspended
here can enable researchers to identify various Present- Not fully Always
styles of mindfulness and assess their coherence centeredness present-centered present-
relative to Buddhist sources. The Buddhist tradi- so as to allow for centered
tions that generally exhibit these styles are ethical judgment
and recollection
grouped according to their approach in Table 18.1, of vows and
but the reality is that sub-traditions and individ- Abhidharma
ual teachers will fall along a spectrum. Some will schema
strongly exhibit Classical tendencies; others may
be clearly Nondual, and yet others (such as the
Thai Forest tradition) may fall somewhere paradigm of the ideal Buddhist and the proper
between these poles. At the same time, as way to engage with the world. The personal
Table 18.2 illustrates, certain features of formal transformation that occurs through contemplative
practice tend to coalesce together in accord with practice is thus not just a matter of what occurs
Buddhist theoretical accounts, and from that on the cushion. It also depends heavily on the
standpoint, some approaches will appear inco- way the world is imagined before and after. How
herent. One might maintain, for example, that the does this between-session paradigm interact with
target state in formal meditation requires one to techniques in formal practice? To effect personal
be present-centered and non-judgmental, and yet transformation, are some practitioners best
one might also insist that this state retains ethical served by the Classical approach, where the para-
discernment. It is difcult to see how this digm is a prominent feature of formal practice?
approach could be theoretically coherent from a And for others, is a Nondual approach better?
Buddhist standpoint, since ethical discernment That is, do these practitioners more easily alter
would require a form of conceptuality that is not their lives to a new paradigm between sessions by
present-centered. This incoherence relative to using a formal practice that suspends all para-
Buddhist sources does not mean that such an digms? Could it be that one approach or another
approach is necessarily wrongheaded. Instead, it will be better for an individual at different points
calls for the development of a new theoretical along a developmental trajectory? These are
account that explains its coherence and thus some of the many questions that this heuristic
leads to hypotheses about mechanisms and ways engagement with Buddhist sources can suggest.
to assess outcomes empirically.
Another clear research agenda that emerges
from this heuristic is the examination of the cru- References
cial role played by the context assumed or delib-
erately created for informal practice between Adam, M. T. (2003). Meditation and the concept of insight
in Kamalalas Bhvankramas (Doctoral
sessions. In actual training, both Classical and
Dissertation). Canada: McGill University.
Nondual Buddhist styles work hard to create a Anlayo. (2003). Satipat..thna: The direct path to realiza-
lifeworld for practitioners structured around a tion. Birmingham, AL: Windhorse.
18 Buddhist Styles of Mindfulness 269

Anuruddha & Bodhi. (2000). A comprehensive manual (Eds.), Handbook of mindfulness: Theory and
of Abhidhamma: The Abhidhammattha sangaha of research. New York: Guilford Press.
cariya Anuruddha. Seattle: BPS Pariyatti Edition. Gethin, R. (1998). The foundations of Buddhism.
Arnold, D. A. (2012). Brains, Buddhas, and believing: New York: Oxford University Press.
The problem of intentionality in classical Buddhist Gethin, R. (2011). On some denitions of mindfulness.
and cognitive-scientific philosophy of mind. New York: Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 263279.
Columbia University Press. Gold, J. (2014). Paving the great way: Vasubandhus uni-
Asanga. (2001). Abhidharmasamuccaya: The compen- fying Buddhist philosophy. New York: Columbia
dium of the higher teaching. (Walpola Rahula & University Press.
S. Boin-Webb, trans.). Fremont, CA: Asian Humanities Goldstein, J. (2002). One dharma: The emerging Western
Press. Buddhism. San Francisco: Harper.
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, Gon nu rgyal mchog & Dkon mchog rgyal mtshan.
N. D., Carmody, J., , Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: (2006). Mind training: The great collection (Thupten
A proposed operational denition. Clinical Jinpa, trans.). Boston: Wisdom Publications.
Psychology: Science and Practice, 11, 230241. Harvey, P. (1990). An introduction to Buddhism:
Bodhi, B. (2011). What does mindfulness really mean? Teachings, history and practices. New York:
A canonical perspective. Contemporary Buddhism, Cambridge University Press.
12, 1939. Hayes, S. (2003). Acceptance and commitment therapy:
Braun, E. (2013). The birth of insight meditation: Modern An experiential approach to behavior change
Buddhism and the Burmese monk Ledi Sayadaw. (Paperbackth ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Higgins, D. (2008). On the development of the non-
Buddhadsa Bhikkhu. (1997). Mindfulness with breath- mentation (amanasikra) doctrine in Indo-Tibetan
ing: A manual for serious beginners (Santikaro Buddhism. Journal of the International Association of
Bhikkhu, trans.). Boston: Wisdom Publications. Buddhist Studies, 29(2), 255303.
Buddhaghosa & n.amoli. (1976). The path of purifica- Kabat-Zinn, J. (2011). Some reections on the origins of
tion: Visuddhimagga. Berkeley: Shambhala MBSR, skillful means, and the trouble with maps.
Publications. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 281306.
Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Kabat-Zinn, J. (2013). Full catastrophe living: Using the
Mindful emotion regulation: An integrative review. wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and
Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 560572. illness (Revised and updated ed.). New York: Bantam
Coseru, C. (2012). Perceiving reality: Consciousness, Books.
intentionality, and cognition in Buddhist philosophy. Karma Dbang Phyug Rdo Rje. (2006). Phyag chen rgyas
New York: Oxford University Press. pa nges don rgya mtsho, 'bring pa ma rig mun sel,
DiValerio, D. M. (2011). Subversive sainthood and tantric bsdus pa chos sku mdzub tshugs bcas so: Lhan cig
fundamentalism: An historical study of Tibets holy skyes sbyor gyi zab khrid nges don rgya mtsho'i snying
madmen (Doctoral Dissertation). United States: po phrin las od. Varanasi, India: Vajra Vidhya
University of Virginia. Institute Library.
Dreyfus, G. (1997). Recognizing reality: Dharmakrtis Kim, H.-J. (2007). Dgen on meditation and thinking: A
philosophy and its Tibetan interpretations. Albany: reflection on his view of Zen. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
State University of New York Press. Lusthaus, D. (2002). Buddhist phenomenology: A philo-
Dreyfus, G. (2011). Is mindfulness present-centered and sophical investigation of Yogcra Buddhism and the
non-judgmental? A discussion of the cognitive dimen- Cheng Wei-shih lun. New York: Routledge-Curzon.
sions of mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, Mathes, K.-D. (2008). Maitrpas Amansikrdhra
4154. (A justification of becoming mentally disengaged)
Dunne, J. D. (2004). Foundations of Dharmakrtis phi- (pp. 330). XIII: Journal of the Nepal Research Centre.
losophy. Boston: Wisdom Publications. McMahan, D. (2008). The making of Buddhist modern-
Dunne, J. D. (2011a). Key features of Dharmakrtis ism. New York: Oxford University Press.
apoha theory. In M. Siderits, T. Tillemans, & McRae, J. R. (2003). Seeing through Zen: Encounter,
A. Chakrabarti (Eds.), Apoha: Buddhist nominalism transformation, and genealogy in Chinese Chan
and human cognition. New York: Columbia University Buddhism. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Press. Papies, E. K., Barsalou, L. W., & Custers, R. (2012).
Dunne, J. D. (2011b). Toward an understanding of non- Mindful attention prevents mindless impulses. Social
dual mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 291299.
7188. doi:10.1080/14639947.2011.564820. Rapgay, L., & Bystrisky, A. (2009). Classical mindful-
Dunne, J. D. (2012). Resources for the study of svasavitti ness: An introduction to its theory and practice for
in ultimate contexts. Presented at the National Meeting clinical application. In W. C. Bushell, E. L. Olivo, &
of the American Academy of Religion. N. D. Theise (Eds.), Longevity, regeneration, and optimal
Gethin, R. (2015). Mindfulness: Buddhist conceptualiza- health: Integrating Eastern and Western perspectives
tions. In K. W. Brown, J. D. Creswell, & R. M. Ryan (pp. 148162). New York: Wiley-Blackwell.
270 J.D. Dunne

Safran, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1990). Interpersonal process in Tweed, T. A. (1992). The American encounter with
cognitive therapy. New York: Basic Books. Buddhism, 1844-1912: Victorian culture and the limits
ntideva. (2008). The Bodhicaryvatra (K. Crosby & of dissent. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
A. Skilton, trans.). New York: Oxford University Press. Van Schaik, S. (2004). Approaching the Great Perfection:
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Simultaneous and gradual approaches to Dzogchen
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: practice in Jigme Lingpas Longchen Nyingtig.
A new approach to preventing relapse. New York: Boston: Wisdom Publications.
Guilford Press. Vasubandhu. (2012). Abhidharmakosa-Bhasya of
Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A., & Freedman, Vasubandhu: The treasury of the abhidharma and its
B. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness. Journal of (auto) commentary (L. de La Valle Poussin & Lodr
Clinical Psychology, 62, 373386. Sangpo, trans.). New Delhi: Motilal Banarsidass
Sharf, R. H. (1995). Buddhist modernism and the rhetoric Publishers Private Ltd.
of meditative experience. Numen, 42(3), 228283. Wallace, B. (2006). The attention revolution: Unlocking
Sharf, R. (2014). Mindfulness and mindlessness in early the power of the focused mind. Boston: Wisdom
chan. Philosophy East and West, 64(4), 933964. Publications.
Sna tshogs rang grol, & Kunsang, E. P. (2009). Heart Wallis, G. (2003). Advayavajra on dikarma (pp. 203
lamp: Lamp of Mahamudra and the heart of the mat- 230). Pacic World: Journal of the Institute of
ter. Boudhanath: Rangjung Yeshe Publications. Buddhist Studies.
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). Mental time iek, S. (2001). On belief. New York: Routledge.
travel across the disciplines: The future looks bright. iek, S. (2012, August 10). The Buddhist ethic and the
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 335345. spirit of global capitalism. Retrieved from http://www.
Suzuki, S. (2006). Zen mind, beginners mind. Shambhala. egs.edu/faculty/slavoj-zizek/articles/the-buddhist-ethic-
Tsong kha pa. (2002). The great treatise on the stages of and-the-spirit-of-global-capitalism/
the path to enlightenment (Vol. 3). Ithaca, NY: Snow
Lion Publications.
The Emperors Clothes: A Look
Behind the Western Mindfulness 19
Mystique

Eleanor Rosch

Mindfulness is a word. It is currently a very pop- training of attention or of other virtues? What
ular word. As the other papers in this volume specific practices are taught in Mindfulness
amply document, mindfulness has become a Based Stress Reduction (MBSR, Kabat-Zinn,
magnet for research in psychology, neuroscience, 1990) and similar trainings, and what clues do we
and medicine, and it is a hot topic in clinical psy- have about how the people who go through these
chology. Many Buddhist groups have joined in programs use what they are taught? This is an
the enthusiasm, taking the research to be proof especially important question not only because
that meditation works (Shambhala Sun, MBSR is the prototype for Western therapeutic
2012) and/or that society is undergoing a mind- uses of mindfulness but also because the most
fulness revolution (Boyce, 2011). With enthusi- frequent de facto operational definition of mind-
asm, however, can come confusion. Having a fulness (verbal definitions aside) is that subjects
word may make us think there is a thing that is in an experiment have taken some form of the
the words meaning; having definitions for the 8-week MBSR program. In regard to the mea-
word (even if they are multiple and divergent) surement of mindfulness, what specific items
may lull us into thinking we (or someone) knows make up the mindfulness measurement scales;
that meaning; having trainings with that word in what do the items ask, and what are they measur-
the title reassures the researcher that the thing the ing? I believe that it is only by this kind of exami-
word means is now at hand; and once researchers nation of the specifics of what is going on that we
can design measurement instruments that vary can wake from the spell cast by the word mind-
with the training, the whole process may become fulness and gain clearer understanding of it. This
sacrosanct and largely closed to further could result in new and more grounded research
questioning. questions and simpler, more individually targeted
It is time to reopen all of this to scrutiny. Why therapiesideally perhaps even to shifts in our
are people not already mindful? Might Buddhism, understanding of body and mind.
the origin of mindfulness practices, have any- To this end, the rest of the chapter presents: (a)
thing illuminating to reveal about its Western A brief overview of mindfulness (and attention
uses? Where does mindfulness fit into Buddhist generally) in Buddhism. (b) An examination of the
contents of the 8 week MBSR program and how
participants use those contents. (c) A template of
E. Rosch (*)
the basic components that make up a therapeutic
Department of Psychology, University of California,
Berkeley, CA, USA program of this nature and an account of some
e-mail: rosch@berkeley.edu alternative ways these components could be

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 271


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5_19
272 E. Rosch

instantiated. Such a template could be used to gen- of Vietnam. I will discuss it in some detail
erate more focused research questions and to because the meditation portion of MBSR is
design interventions to fit specific individual or derived from Theravada meditations. (For more
population differences. (d) A critique of the pres- material on the early Buddhism to be covered in
ent mindfulness measurement scales. (e) this section see Buddhaghosa, 1976; Byrom,
Predictions of some major changes in our views of 1993; Kornfield, 1977; Nyanaponika 1973;
both body and mind that could occur in the future Rahula, 1959.) Theravada teachings contain the
and cast a broader perspective on all this work. basis of Buddhist psychology: an explanation of
why we are not happy when that is what we wish
for, and an account of the practices that can be
Attention and Mindfulness done to remedy that. The format in which this is
in Buddhism often presented is the Four Noble Truths: life is
marked by dukkha (suffering/unsatisfactoriness);
Western therapeutic mindfulness is not there is a cause of suffering; by extinguishing the
Buddhism. It may use some techniques borrowed cause, suffering will cease; and there is a path by
from Buddhism, but it uses them in a different which this can be accomplished. It sounds simple
manner and towards different goals. Buddhism and upbeat, but there is a catch. To see any of this
itself is not unitary. It is divided into three major personallythe pervasiveness of suffering as
world forms, called yanas (paths/vehicles) in well as its cause and curerequires looking
BuddhismTheravada, Mahayana, and deeply into ones experience. In the words of the
Vajrayanaeach of which treats the domain in Dhammapada (The Saying of the Buddha), He
which mindfulness functions, attention to the [the master practitioner] looks deeply into things/
present, rather differently. There are also a num- And sees their nature (Byrom, 1993, p. 108).
ber of newly emerging social activist forms of This is something that we dont ordinarily do and
Buddhism that at times lay claim to the status of in fact cannot do as long as we have a wild mind
a fourth yana. Psychologists should be wary of that like a monkey, leaps from branch to branch
using citations from one of the yanas (usually (common simile for the untamed mind in Eastern
Theravada) as though they represent Buddhism cultures). That is where Buddhist meditation
as a whole; not only does this obscure diversity, enters and where we find the initial introduction
but it tends to be less than appreciated by of mindfulness.
Buddhists from the non-cited yanas. The follow- There are two aspects to early Buddhist medi-
ing review emphasizes psychologically relevant tation: peaceful abiding (Pali: samatha, Sanskrit:
factors and is based on the ways Buddhism is shamatha, Tibetan: shine) and clear seeing (Pali:
taught and practiced in contemporary Buddhist vipassana, Sanskrit: vipashyana, Tibetan: lhagth-
centers as well as on historical reconstructions of ong). Shamatha, which literally means peace, is
its past. Thus the review is not in any way a sub- generally viewed as a preliminary practice whose
stitute for the detailed accounts of particular peri- purpose is to calm and stabilize the mind.
ods, trends, and texts provided by Buddhist Calming the mind includes what in the West we
Studies scholarship. call relaxation, but it is an alert relaxation. This
pacified mind provides a platform from which the
meditator can direct close enough attention to
Theravada moment-to-moment mental occurrences (breath-
ing, sensations, perceptions, feelings, thoughts)
Theravada (The Speech of the Elders) is the one to see into their nature. To make the initial sha-
surviving school of early Buddhism (that began matha practice simple, the meditator is generally
in India around 500 BCE) and the primary taught to hold his mind (more realistically to
Buddhism of Southeast Asia: Thailand, Myanmar keep returning his mind) to a single object of
(Burma), Sri Lanka, Laos, Cambodia, and parts attention, such as his breath or a visual object
19 The Emperors Clothes 273

(kasina)a task that necessitates not only close to MBSR because MBSR contains some medita-
focus on an object but an inhibition (or a release) tion, and when a person begins to meditate,
of focus on other objects. The quality of attention impermanence is usually the first thing noticed,
is not necessarily different in single object medi- albeit in the form of an active, wandering,
tation and in attention to moment-to-moment uncontrollable-seeming mind. Egolessness raises
mental phenomena, though it can be if directed in the question: who is it that observes that mind?
a probing contemplative direction. This is also relevant to the MBSR meditator
Where in all this is mindfulness? Here we because the I/me who he assumes is the observer
enter a thicket of terminological and interpretive is illusive, never in direct sight, and frustratingly
dispute. Scholars generally agree that the English never quite in control. What the more experi-
word mindfulness began life as a translation for enced Buddhist meditator also sees is that this
the Pali term sati (Sanskrit smrti), but there is dis- illusive supposed self is the center of his motiva-
agreement, both ancient and modern, about just tion, emotions, and actions. Suffering is under-
what in the meditation process (or in conscious- stood in Buddhism to be inherent in a life of
ness in general) sati was intended to include. At struggle to grasp what is felt to be good for that
issue is that meditation practices, like other imagined self, to avoid or attack what is felt bad
human activities, do not occur in a vacuum but for it, and to ignore what is judged irrelevant.
rather in meaningful contexts. In dispute is what One is trapped in the struggle because getting
kind of context and how much of it to include in what one wants increases rather than assuages
the meaning of the word mindfulness. Does desire (the basis of addiction; watch what hap-
mindfulness refer only to the power by which the pens the next time you bite into something deli-
mind can return to an object of concentration cious), and, similarly, acting out fear, aggression,
(one of a cluster of mental factors said to be or ignoring augments rather than relieves oneself
always present in a moment of experience in the of those emotions. What the beginning mindful-
Buddhist systematization of mental factors called ness meditator will see of all this is that when she
the Abhidharma) or should it be used to name a attempts to remain present with the simplest
particular kind of meditation as was done in two experience, either in meditation or daily life,
early Buddhist suttas? If the latter, does it refer to although the first moment or two might be enter-
bare attention alone (and is there such a thing?); taining, shortly the tension begins to build; bore-
or to close attention to present experience over dom, irritation, even fear, may increase; seductive
time alone; or to close attention combined with thoughts intrude, and she is again gone. In short,
memory of Buddhist teachings; or to attention there is no contentment.
plus memory plus discernment regarding ethical This constant cycling from one unsatisfactory
actions; and so on? (For an exploration of these mode of being to another is called samsara, tradi-
disputes see Contemporary Buddhism, 2011; tionally thought of as composed of actual realms
Sharf, 2015). Since the importance for psychol- into which a sentient being could be born as well
ogy of any of this is what claims were (and are) as mental states. The goal of meditation was to be
being made about different mental processes, liberated from samsara. How? In early Buddhism
those are what we will look at as we continue. the logic is that since it is desire, aggression and
When the meditator develops the capacity for ignorance in the mind that forge the chains of
close attention and clear seeing, what does he cause and effect (karma) keeping one impris-
see? Early Buddhism speaks of the Three Marks oned, those three poisons need to be eliminated
of Existence: impermanence, egolessness, and from the mind. There were two basic methods:
suffering. Impermanence points not only to obvi- (a) One could allow the impulse toward actions
ous changes such as that people die (called gross arising from both wholesome (kusala) and
impermanence) but to the subtler moment-to- unwholesome (akusala) mind states to rise and
moment arising and falling, birth and death, of fall without response. This eliminates karmic
ones perceptions and thoughts. This is relevant seeds and does not replace them with new ones.
274 E. Rosch

That was considered the only way to conclusively practitioner realizes the openness and release of
eliminate the three poisons and gain liberation. shunyata and his own inherent compassionate
But it requires renunciation, dedication, restraint good will towards others, he can live with his
of the senses, and protracted close mindfulness senses and thoughts in an enlightened way in this
meditation to implement and was thus the prov- very life. In fact, Mahayana practitioners may
ince of monastics. (b) One could counter take Bodhisattva vows to be reborn in life after
unwholesome states such as greed, hatred, and life in order to be of benefit to others.
aggression with other more wholesome mind In the Buddha nature schools appearing in
states such as nonviolence or loving kindness later Mahayana, further assertions are made; sen-
(metta). This method creates good karma, and tient beings already have an enlightened nature
was appropriate for both monastics and the lay (Buddha nature, tathagatagarbha), and the goal
community. Note that the momentary periodic of practice is to fully realize it. The path to that is
checking in with what is happening in the present seen as: a) removal of the obstacles that obscure
that is needed to be functional in everyday life the enlightened nature and b) breaking through to
and that everyone does to some extentwas the nature itself. The classic dispute between the
taken for granted and not considered mindful- gradual and sudden enlightenment schools
ness. The ultimate goal in early Buddhism and in occurred when these two steps were judged to
present Theravada is to attain nirvana (Pali: nib- conflict rather than to mutually support one
bana), a state from which, after death, one would another.
not again be reborn. What happens to mindfulness and the training
Theravada teachings in their seminal form do of attention in this context? The deliberate mind-
not transfer well to the West. The question for fulness of early Buddhism tends to be replaced by
Westerners is usually not, How can I wake from a fierce concentrationfor example, on a koan
delusion and free myself from samsara? but, (Loori, 2006)that enables the practitioner to
How can I use meditation to calm down, feel shatter his conceptual mind and emerge into the
better, and become more successful? With that vast interdependent universe in which there is no
change of context, the nature and implementation separate observer to observe or mind to be mind-
of meditation and mindfulness also change radi- ful. This is called non-duality. It directly cuts into
cally, as we will see. the sense of a self with its projects, so that, even in
the practice of just sitting (shikantaza), it is
eventually necessary to simply let go into ones
Mahayana original mind (Suzuki, 1970). Alone among
Buddhist teachers, the influential Vietnamese
Mahayana (Great Vehicle) Buddhism is esti- peace activist, poet, and Zen teacher Thich Nhat
mated to have begun around 100 CE in India and Hanh employs the English word mindfulness to
eventually spread throughout East Asia: China, refer to the entire pathits ground, path, and frui-
Korea, Japan, and parts of Vietnam. (For more tion (Nhat Hanh, 1975, 1987), a usage that can
material on the Mahayana Buddhism to be dis- potentially blur the distinctions that Nhat Hanh
cussed in this section see, Demoulin 2005; Nhat makes in other ways when that single word enters
Hanh, 1987; Santideva, 1995; Sprung, 1979; the context of science.
Suzuki, 1970; Trungpa, 1993.) Indian Mahayana
added two major teachings to early Buddhism:
emptiness (shunyata) and compassion (karuna), Vajrayana
which are said to be inseparable like the two
wings of a bird. This is a vast topic, but what is The Vajrayana (Diamond Vehicle) became prom-
relevant for present purposes is to note that in inent circa 800 CE in India. Early forms of it
Mahayana there is a type of functioning of the moved into China and Japan, later ones to Tibet.
mind beyond the motivations of samsara. When a Because it is the Tibetan forms that are most
19 The Emperors Clothes 275

known and practiced in the West, I will limit dis- ence can be experienced as brilliant, self-
cussion to those. (For more material on the knowing, and self-liberating. There is more to be
Vajrayana to be discussed in this section see, said about the practicalities of Vajrayana, but the
Fremantle 2001; Ponlop, 2003; Rosch 2008, topics that are relevant to MBSR, such as the role
Snellgrove, 1987; Sogyal, 1992; Trungpa, 1991; of the physical body, will emerge later when we
Tsoknyi, 1998.) Vajrayana adds two new teach- discuss those aspects of MBSR.
ings to the Buddhism that came before it as well What have we learned about mindfulness and
as a variety of methods originating in Indian attention to the present moment from this whirl-
Tantra and in pre-Buddhist Tibet. The new teach- wind tour of Buddhism? The minimal meaning
ing is that there is wisdom awareness (rigpa) of mindfulness appears to be close moment-to-
beyond samsara and beyond emptiness that moment attention to ones experience as it is hap-
knows the primordial ground of being: pure, pening over a period of time. The purpose is to
timeless, complete, and all-good. From that enable the mind to penetrate to the nature of
ground radiates the phenomenal world of experi- experience. People in the usual state of mind
ence. Known with pure vision (dag snang), the called samsara flee the present moment because
ordinary world of experiencesamsara with its they find it painful or boring, and it is disconfirm-
grasping, aggression, ignorance, and suffering ing of the fantasy self. As greater mindfulness
is seen to actually consist of the wisdom energies and/or awareness develop through practice and
of that radiance. (In the simpler language of a through input from the yana in which the medita-
contemporary outgrowth of Vajrayana called tor is practicing, obstacles to realization are
Shambhala (Trungpa, 1984), all this is encom- diminished or drop away; the fruition of realiza-
passed by the term basic goodness.) tion (which differs in the different yanas) begins
Where is mindfulness in this? For a beginners to manifest, and the nature of the present moment
practice, Vajrayana makes a distinction between is understood to be itself transformed in ways
mindfulness (Tibetan: trenpa), the minds simple well beyond mindfulness.
connection with an object of perception such as a
sight or ones breath, and awareness (Tibetan:
sheshin), the broader knowing that surrounds it Mindfulness Based Stress
that can, for example, recognize that one is no Reduction (MBSR)
longer attending to ones breath (Mipham, 2003).
(The closest Western analog of awareness may be We now have the background to look into some
William James concept of fringe attention in of the factors in Western therapeutic and research
contrast to focal attentionJames, 1890.) oriented uses of mindfulness. I am going to do
Various analogies may be used for the relation- this by means of an examination of MBSR, since
ship between mindfulness and awareness; for it was the first and has become the prototype for
example, mindfulness is likened to a single word such uses. In fact, most of the studies that meet
and awareness to the sentence, grammar, para- scientific standards that use eight weeks of
graph, book, or meaning in which the word is mindfulness training as their de facto opera-
embedded (Trungpa, 1976). Both mindfulness tional definition of mindfulness turn out to have
and awareness are necessary. If only a pointillist been done on MBSR or some close variant. The
mindfulness were to be developed, it would make impression given is that subjects were intensively
the practitioner a plodding ignorant animal (a practicing mindfulness meditation, or at least
tudro), not a happier or a more enlightened mindfulness, however defined, for 8 weeks. This
human; with only expanded awareness, the prac- is not the case. MBSR is a potpourri of practices
titioner could be ungrounded and imprecise. As and teaching that develop beneficial life skills
awareness is broadened and deepened with train- that include some meditation but also much else.
ing, the quality of the moment of being present is My account of what is in MBSR (although pre-
also transformed. Eventually that flash of pres- ceded by my study of written protocols for the
276 E. Rosch

training such as those in Kabat-Zinn, 1990 and friendly group atmosphere. Context and motivation
Stahl & Goldstein, 2010) is based primarily on were provided by a lecture on the destructive
participant observation (the anthropological tech- effects of stress and the benefits of reducing it,
nique that includes recordings and interviews) and participants were told that the method they
that I have carried out over the last 2 years. would learn for reducing stress was mindfulness.
Data for the following account was obtained Although mindfulness was defined as in Kabat-
as follows: I attended three complete MBSR Zinn (1990)attention to the present, on purpose,
8-week trainings. In the first training, I stayed and without judgmentthat abstract formulation
within the role of participant, taking on-site notes was not specifically referenced again in any of the
only during teacher talks and group discussions groups. (This is the usual fate of definitions in
and not conducting formal interviews. During the natural languageRosch, 2011.) The pleasures to
second two trainings, I did the same on-site be provided by enhanced attention were demon-
observations, but in addition interviewed all par- strated by an exercise consisting of the slow,
ticipants who were willing to schedule the extra guided, eating of three raisins. In closing, a work-
time to speak with me, a total of 17 people. book was given to participants with sections for
Fourteen of these people were interviewed twice, each week of the training, and Kabat-Zinn (1990)
once during the course of the training and once was recommended as optional reading. This entire
after its conclusion. Three people were inter- context differs from that in Buddhism, and we
viewed only once; two of them after the training will see how that affects participant response to
had ended. The interviews were primarily natu- the meditations.
ralistic; I posed initial topics, but subsequent
questions and conversation stemmed from the
interviewees responses. Because my intent was The Meditations
to elicit what was salient to the participants, not
all participants spoke about all of the topics. An Two guided meditations are part of the program:
exception to this was that at the end of the final a body-scan and what was called a sitting medita-
interview (when participants had already com- tion. For home practice, CDs of the guided
pleted the MBSR program), I posed specific instructions, spoken by the teacher of the group,
questions directed at their understanding and were provided to each participant. Teachers of
experience of the mindfulness aspects of the MBSR are trained and authorized by the
training. My overall purpose was to study how University of Massachusetts Center for
MBSR was presented and how participants Mindfulness, and although the three teachers had
reacted to the various exercises. different personal styles in their talks and in dis-
cussions with participants, their presentation of
the meditations were very similar.
Introductory Context and Motivation
Setting The body-scan. The body-scan appears to have
been created by a Burmese Buddhist meditation
In the first session, all three teachers in the groups teacher of the late 19th-early 20th century named
that I observed enthusiastically introduced MBSR Ledi Sayadaw, and is now taught in the West pri-
as a scientifically proven course that can improve/ marily by the Indian teacher S.N. Goenka (1987).
change peoples lives. Participants (1015 in a It is an idiosyncratic variant of Theravada
group was the norm) introduced themselves, said Buddhism. The premise of the body scan is that
why they had come to the program, and were the basic body-mind units of experience are sen-
asked to talk about an attribute that they liked sations. When we desire, fear, or abhor some-
about themselves, a technique that turns attention thing external, what we really want or do not
toward the positive, an approach used in positive want, are particular kinds of sensations. Through
psychology (Seligman, 2011). It also generated a explicit Theravada teachings and through intense,
19 The Emperors Clothes 277

prolonged, and unwavering attention to body Sitting mindfulness meditation. This is a common
sensations, including the pain that comes from mindfulness meditation, often used as a bridge to
sitting immobile for many hours at a time in vipassana exercises although not in
Goenkas intensive 10 day retreats, participants MBSR. Participants are led first to attend to their
are meant to realize that sensations are mere tran- breath, then to sensations, feelings, emotions,
sient vibrations, thereby hopefully eliminating all thoughts, and finally to awareness itself. The prac-
desire and aversion from their minds. tice is done sitting in a chair with eyes closed.
Not surprisingly, the body-scan in MBSR is Participants reactions to the practice were more
done differently. It is performed comfortably varied and less classifiable than with the body
lying down, either on a yoga mat in the MBSR scan, but it was clear that relaxation was still a
session or at home in a place of the participants strong concern. Some participants who had not
choice, usually his bed. Eyes are closed. In neu- found the body scan relaxing did relax with atten-
tral tones, the voice of the teacher instructs the tion to their breath and vice versa. Over half men-
student to notice any sensations in his left foot tioned the wildness of their minds, some with
and slowly, with pauses in between, leads the stu- surprise or interest, but others concluding from it
dent to notice points all over his body. (No men- that the meditation was something they couldnt
tion of transient vibrations or desire and aversion.) do. With probing, some participants voiced confu-
A single scan is done in a 45-min session. sion about what in their minds they were supposed
What do participants make of this practice? to look at (for example, what is the difference
Overwhelmingly participants talked about it as between a feeling and an emotion, and what does
relaxation (for 85 % it was their first comment), a looking at awareness mean?), however all could
natural assumption given that this was a stress easily identify the difference between sensations
reduction program and that the practice is done and thoughts. Three participants expressed worry
lying down with closed eyes. In this group, that the meditation, either because of negative con-
roughly half said that the body scan actually was tent or difficulty, was making them more stressed,
relaxing for them and that they did it as home- and they, as well as others who didnt like it or who
work at least part of the time. Many of these also felt they were too busy to spend 45 min on it, did
said that it helped them to sleep or actually put not to do the home practice.
them to sleep, some reporting happily that they
had never stayed awake for the entire CD. (During What participants are and are not getting out of
the group sessions, a chorus of snores typically the meditations. Participants who participated
accompanied the body scan.) Four people said actively in the program appeared to be deriving
they augmented their relaxation by adding other three main benefits from the meditations. Most
relaxation techniques they had previously learned. obvious was relaxation. The second was discrim-
The other half of the group varied from less enthu- ination of bodily sensations (and to some extent
siastic to negative, the latter finding the practice feelings) from mental thoughts and stories. While
boring, uncomfortable, or pointless. The two par- this is a distinction that functional adults can eas-
ticipants who mentioned learning to be in the ily make, at least at the cognitive level, these
present as a purpose for the exercise had the usual practices provided a platform in which the two
beginning meditators concern about drifting into could be brought into a more focal juxtaposition.
thought rather than staying with the sensations as Finally, there were precursors and perhaps
directed. Almost all the participants, upon further glimpses of mindful present moment function-
probing, reported having noticed something about ing. Relaxation and discrimination are clearly
their body or their relationship to their body that beneficial, but not of themselves present moment
they had not been previously aware of, but this presence, and the greater development of such
was at the level of information not intimacy. Not presence seemed not in the forefront of partici-
surprisingly, participants who did not like the pant concerns; e.g., only two participants men-
body scan were less likely to do it at home. tioned it without probing. By the end of the initial
278 E. Rosch

weeks of training, which was the period devoted familiar, they tried to follow their movements as
to the meditations, MBSR participants were still they did them, but, although they saw this could
confused about what constituted being present as be pleasurable, it was difficult to maintain (also
well as having difficulty doing the meditations as reported by participants who already had a yoga
instructed, even those that provided them a mea- or other movement based home practice and did
sure of relaxation. (This is what can be expected that instead). Some participants found the yoga
with an introduction to mindfulness meditation in physically difficult or painful. Over half said they
any setting.) As the course proceeded, other prac- did little or no home practice of it. Note that there
tices became the focus of the training and of par- is mounting research evidence that hatha yoga is
ticipants interest. In addition to shifting the of itself beneficial (Broad, 2012; Fields, 2012),
focus, these practices interacted with peoples so we can assume that the MBSR participants
understanding of the meditations; for example, who did the yoga were deriving those benefits
some participants reinterpreted the meditations from it whether they did it attentively or not.
as information gathering.
It should be noted that none of this would be
surprising to long-term Buddhist practitioners. Explicit Stress Reduction Practices
To develop an alert, present oriented attention
that is stable over a noticeable period of time Interruption and alleviation of stressful situations.
goes against the habits of a lifetime and disrupts These skills were central to what many participants
peoples ordinary sense of themselves. To nurture said they got out of MBSR. Participants were given a
such requires time and motivation. series of assignments. The first was to notice one or
I will return to all of these issues in later more pleasant events and record the feelings, sensa-
discussions. tions and thoughts they had during the event. This
served as preparation for the following week when
participants were to do the same for unpleasant,
Hatha Yoga stressful events. In the third week participants were
encouraged to be vigilant [my word] to detect their
Practice of simplified and relatively undemand- bodily feelings of stress, particularly changes in
ing hatha yoga movements and poses is given breathing, so that they could catch the stressful feel-
equal time with meditation in MBSR, both in the ings before they escalated out of control and thus
group sessions and in the recommendations for have a choice of what to do next. Teachers varied in
home practice. In fact some participants who how much and what kind of further advice they gave:
liked the yoga and did not like the meditations, for example, pause, breath, see what your body is
did only the yoga at home thus giving it close to doing, stay with awareness, be kind/compassionate
full time. (Guided CDs for the poses were pro- to yourself, look reasonably at what is happening,
vided for home use.) Research on MBSR might remember this situation will not last, etc. All the par-
well be enriched by a fuller consideration of this ticipants worked with these exercises to some extent,
highly physical aspect of the program. were animated in the group discussions of them, and,
The practice was described as mindful yoga in some cases, came up with their own interesting or
and the practice of moving presence, the point heartfelt solutions of what to do. Just staying with
being that it was attention to the movements, awareness was given little or no attention (another
rather than trying to perfect them, that was impor- unexpected finding to be addressed later), but the
tant. Many participants, however, reported just exercise as a whole made sense to them, and they
trying to learn what to do, a task performance went at it in full problem solving mode.
mind-set rather than an orientation to being pres-
ent. Some participants who liked the yoga Catching and correcting distortions of thought.
reported becoming caught up in trying to stretch This is a mainstay of cognitive therapy (Beck,
further. Some said that as the practice got more 2011). Distortions discussed here included: all or
19 The Emperors Clothes 279

none thinking, overgeneralization, focusing on the Teachers are screened, trained, and certified by
negative, thinking feelings are facts, perfectionism, the University of Massachusetts Center for
over-responsibility, labeling oneself, and identify- Mindfulness. (The need for a certification proce-
ing with thoughts and emotions. Participants who dure became apparent when MBSR began to be
spoke agreed that they did all of these things, some- taught by people without background, and under
times giving humorous examples. Distressing those conditions reportedly did not work.) The
emotions and the thoughts that went with them teachers I observed all had prior meditation back-
were treated in the workbooks and teacher talks as ground; two were therapists, and all had people
a combination of stressful event and distorted skills. Interaction within the group included peri-
thought and received the same instructions for alle- odic dialogs between participants and group dis-
viation as had the stressful events: notice early, cussion periods during each session. The
apply remedies. None of the (admittedly more atmosphere was pleasant and upbeat, focusing on
existential in nature) distortions of thought pointed what had been discovered or had worked for the
to in Buddhism were ever mentioned. participants when performing the exercises.
Although the sessions had some aspects of a sup-
Changing habits. Habits were given a week of port group, MBSR is not a place to discuss per-
their own, however the technique was virtually sonal pathologies. The one person I observed
the same as in working with stressful events. who appeared to be in crisis left after the third
Instructions were: be attentive to situations where week to seek individual therapy. Almost all par-
the habit comes into play; catch it early enough to ticipants, including those who reported them-
have traction; stop and do something else. selves too busy to do most of the homework, said
they liked their teacher, their group, and the
classes.
Interpersonal Relations

Metta. MBSR contains a section on metta loving Poetry


kindness practice. The essence of the practice is
to wish good for people: for example, health, Teachers periodically read poetry of their choice
safety, peacefulness, happiness, joy. Traditionally to the participants. The poetry introduced a touch
this would be done first for oneself, then a loved of imagery and feeling beyond rationality into the
one, friend, neutral, and finally an enemy. People proceedings, a dimension understandably not
easily understand what to do in this practice and, present in the training per sealso humor.
after an initial surprise at how difficult it is to
wish well to oneself, they generally reported lik-
ing it. There is research showing that an attitude The Template
of loving kindness and compassion toward one-
self and other people is beneficial (Keltner, 2009; Now that weve seen what practices and trainings
Chapter 10), and, as with hatha yoga, this aspect comprise MBSR and have at least some informa-
of MBSR would seem ripe for increased attention tion on how participants use them, it is possible
by researchers. (In two groups, a taste of another to construct a template of the factors at work in
practice of this type was added: in one the keep- this kind of therapeutic program. There are alter-
ing of a gratefulness diary, in another a brief native ways that each of the factors can be instan-
foray into the Rosenberg (2003) nonviolent com- tiated, and such alternatives could be used when
munication training.) Remember that metta is they are judged more appropriate for an individual
something different from mindfulness. or a target population. Even religions could use
the factors by substituting methods for achieving
Human interaction. MBSR participants interact them based on their own beliefs. One or more of
both with their teacher and with each other. the factors can be combined with other kinds of
280 E. Rosch

therapies or interventions. Such an analysis there were options among such practices. Even
should also bring up many research questions meditative attention to breath can be performed
that are obscured if everything is called by the in different ways with attention directed towards:
name mindfulness. breath at the nostrils, the rise and fall of the abdo-
men, whole body breathing, the outbreath only,
alternate nostril breathing, extending the out-
Origin Story breath, thinking a relaxation-inducing word such
as peace on the outbreath, and so on. Some relax-
MBSR uses stress and its evolutionary explana- ation methods can be performed quickly on the
tion as an origin story for the reversible aspects of spot, a present day virtue. When we add other
much human distress. In contrast, the origin story yogic or modern relaxation methods that do not
of dysfunction in psychoanalysis is in terms of use meditation, there are a cornucopia of possi-
childhood material that is unconscious. The ori- bilities to suit particular needs.
gin story of suffering in Buddhism is contained in With respect to research, the reader might con-
how the mind of samsara functions, of Christianity sider what role relaxation may play in the various
in sin and redemption, and so on. Simply having findings, including effects on attention, that are
an origin story satisfies peoples need for expla- reported in this volume. In addition, there may be
nation, and the belief systems in which these sto- more to relaxation of the body than is contained in
ries are embedded set the stage for how the other the opposite-of-stress model, and the maps of an
factors will be used. inner energy system in various Eastern yogic
practices may offer clues about where to look (see
next section). There may also be levels and sub-
Relaxation tleties to relaxing the mind; for example,
Vajrayana shamatha designates nine levels of
Relaxation has its own potent research history. It resting the mind, and teachings on Vajrayana
is generally defined as the absence of (or opposite awareness may use relaxation imagery such as a
of) the stress response, which, to oversimplify Brahman housewife whose work is done or the
physiologically, is carried by the action of the uncoiling of a knotted snake. And with respect to
parasympathetic, as opposed to the sympathetic, therapy, relaxation is surely the Type-O blood that
nervous system (Benson & Proctor, 2010). Books could facilitate almost anything.
and websites listing techniques for relaxation
abound, some citing research showing the effec-
tiveness of each technique (Davis, Eshelman, & Bodily Movement Practices
McKay, 2008). Virtually all of them list medita-
tion and/or mindfulness meditation, as one of Simple hatha yoga is a prominent part of
those techniques. Once one realizes that relax- MBSR. Like mindfulness, yoga has its enthusias-
ation and mindfulness are not enemies and that tic supporters and an ever-mounting research lit-
relaxation may contribute to the success of erature demonstrating its benefits (Broad, 2012;
MBSR and similar programs, it opens the door Fields, 2012). And like relaxation, once one
for creative changes. admits that yoga may be a factor in the benefits of
People differ in what does and does not relax MBSR, the door opens to variations that might be
them. Neither the body scan nor the sitting mind- more beneficial or better tailored to individual
fulness meditation of MBSR were designed for needs; for example, twists are believed helpful
relaxation, though they allowed it, but by no for alleviating depression, and there are cooling
means all of the MBSR participants found both, poses to engender relaxed calm and heating poses
or either, relaxing. In early Buddhism an alert to stimulate energy flow.
peaceful-abiding practice would generally pre- As we saw, not everybody in the MBSR groups
cede and lead into mindfulness meditation, and liked or could do the yoga or did it attentively as
19 The Emperors Clothes 281

instructed. Fortunately, there are alternatives. All Mansell, & Shafran, 2004). A number of MBSR
across Asia, the body is understood to be com- participants reported improvement in their sleep.
posed not only of flesh, blood and internal organs, The life benefits afforded by adequate sleep are
but also of an internal energy system, metaphori- of the same sort as found for MBSR and other
cally described as channelsand in some cases such programs. Thus sleep may be an amplifying
energy vortices called chakrasthrough which or mediator variable between practices such as
the energy flows. In Hindu, Buddhist and Taoist yoga and relaxation and their beneficial effects,
yoga, movement of energy in the subtle body is and, by extension, the benefits of MBSR.
seen as an origin of mental as well as physical
states; for example, wildness versus stability of
mind. Chinese exercises, such as tai chi and chi Familiarity with Body and Mind
gong, like the hatha yoga used in MBSR, are
designed to adjust the internal energy body as Any endeavor to know thyself, surely involves
well as provide exercise. There is already an becoming more familiar with ones body and
accumulation of medical and psychological mind. The emphasis of MBSR is on the body.
research showing the benefits of these systems The body scan, breathing, sensations, yoga, vigi-
(Hong, 2008). For older people or those with lim- lance for changes in the body signaling stress, the
ited movement capacity (or limited time), tai chi use of breathing or good bodily memories to
chih, a simple system designed for its medical calm stress and offset habitsall produce greater
benefits, is available and increasingly used in familiarity with the body (even in cases where it
senior centers (Stone, 1992). For Buddhist mind- is only at the conceptual level) and more ten-
fulness inspired programs, Tibetan yoga and use dency to check in periodically with the state of
of breath (Wangyal, 2011) may be particularly ones body. In this respect MBSR is closely allied
appropriate. Among Western exercises, walking with the growing field of somatic psychology and
may supply yogic as well as aerobic benefits due the upsurge of somatic therapies (Barratt, 2010).
to the bilateral continuous movement of the arms In fact, with the self improvement task oriented
and legs. attention that MBSR participants bring to the
There are research implications. It is surely body, MBSR appears closer to somatic psychol-
time to take these maps of an inner energy physi- ogy than to Buddhist mindfulness of body, one
ology as seriously as we do the effects of the of the four foundations of mindfulness taught in
yoga or acupuncture based on them, and to find early Buddhism (Silananda, 2002). MBSR also
ways to subject such conceptions of the body to offers participants the opportunity to become
scientific investigation. There are also implica- more familiar with their minds, or at least the
tions for therapy; imagine if psychotherapists and contents of their minds, but that is less promi-
teachers of therapeutic movement formed teams nent. There are other meditation derived uses of
to work simultaneously with a given patient. attention to the bodyfor example, the transfor-
mation of intractable chronic pain into sensations
of heat or light made possible by prolonged pen-
Sleep etrating concentration on the pain (Young
2004)that the interested researcher or therapist
Both relaxation and yoga have been shown to can look into.
reduce insomnia and improve the quality of sleep.
Sleep deprivation leads to a cascade of negative
effects on body and mind that are endemic in our Discrimination Between Sensations,
severely sleep deprived society (Dement & Feelings, Emotions, Thoughts
Vaughn, 1999). In addition, sleep disturbances
have been identified as comorbid factors for all One aspect of clinical disorders such as anxiety
categories of mental illness (Harvey, Watkins, or depression is the automatic leap from negative
282 E. Rosch

bodily sensations and feelings to thoughts and tion. Other strategies were to think of a favorite
stories about the meaning of those bodily states, memory or to try to reason with oneself. A num-
which then perpetuates further negative sensa- ber of people reported bringing to mind what
tions and feelings. This applies to the miseries of appeared to be the larger purpose for which they
ordinary people in nonclinical populations as were engaged in the stressful activity, usually
well. Discrimination between sensations, feel- having to do with their loved ones. It was as
ings, and thoughts is necessary if one is to develop though some participants were spontaneously
an early warning system to interrupt the automa- coming up with their higher order commitments,
ticity of this feedback loop. However, there are a fundamental aspect of Acceptance and
simpler and more explicit ways of teaching peo- Commitment Therapy (Hayes, Strosahl, &
ple such discrimination than are afforded by Wilson, 1999).
MBSR or mindfulness meditation. Carmodys
paper (this volume) provides a clear description
of the logic and implementation of this process. Identification and Correction
of Thought Distortions

Vigilance and Interruption This is a particular kind of corrective. Participants


of Habitual Patterns who identified one of the thought distortions,
usually self-criticism, as a stressor and became
Humans are already vigilant for threats and vigilant of it, then made efforts to substitute a
opportunities (however defined by the person) more positive or more reasonable alternative.
and for information relevant to present concerns. Correction of thought is important as a factor in
In fact it is the capacity for vigilance and its mis- its own right. Note that origin stories and the
use that lead to stress in the first place. To inter- belief systems in which they are embedded all
rupt the escalation of stress, negative emotions, have their own versions of true versus mistaken
and unwanted habits, vigilance is needed in order forms of thought and methods for correcting the
to catch the early stages of these patterns. MBSR mistaken.
teaches this, and any therapeutic work aimed at
explicit direct change of feelings and behavior
will probably need to incorporate some version Attention Directed to What Is Positive
of it. (Note: the term vigilance is mine and was and What Works, Not to What Doesnt
not used in MBSR.)
All of the discussions were led in a way conso-
nant with this principle, even though it was not
Correctives for Habitual Patterns explicitly taught as such. It is noteworthy as a
separate factor because it is the basic principle of
Vigilance for warning signs is not enough. Here positive psychology, another input to MBSR of a
I go again! often precedes simply going there Western therapeutic system that has growing
again. At the point of noticing, one must insert research demonstrating its effectiveness
something different. The options of just being (Seligman, 2011; Snyder, Lopez, & Pedrotti,
or of hanging out with enlightened awareness are 2011).
possible only if one has access to such states.
Likewise for not identifying with negative emo-
tions, which was largely opaque for MBSR par- Sense of Agency
ticipants. On the other hand, breathing (even if
just taking three deep breaths) made sense to Working with vigilance and finding antidotes to
them, as did looking at themselves with kindness counter stress appeared to impart a sense of
and compassionor at least having that inten- agency to people. It put them into an animated
19 The Emperors Clothes 283

problem-solving state of mind. The word mind- Finally there is the dimension of beyond the
fulness has been used for this kind of activity in known, the longing or reaching out toward
contrast to passive or automatic actions (Langer, beyondness, whether that is pictured as a depth of
1989). But the reason why Buddhist mindfulness experience, as is common in Buddhism, or as a
retreats are repetitious and stripped of complex height, as is the tendency in Western religious
activities is that complexity and mental manipu- language. The practical side of this is noted by
lation to make things better are the very activities common expressions such as outside the box
that suck a beginner away from mindful pres- and inspiration that seems to come from
ence, not those that lead to it. Agency and prob- nowhere. Lest that seem beyond the useful,
lem solving are good, even essential, but theyre notice how in Dialectical Behavior Therapy
on a different dimension of mental functioning (DBT) (Linehan, 1993), even borderline patients
than mindfulness. can be guided to a remarkable prescience that is
neither familiarly intellectual nor emotional (see
also examples in Baer, 2006).
Loving Kindness and Compassion

Humans are social animals, and metta practice is In Short


directly connected to that. People can understand
what the practice is and the instructions for how Results of this inquiry into the specifics of MBSR
to do it without a background in meditation. It might appear somewhat like Leeuwenhoeks first
relaxes and softens people and provides a ground glance through a microscope into a drop of pond
on which other practices can flourish. Some water. What had been assumed just water was
Buddhist teachers use such practices as their found teeming with life. MBSR, normally treated
basic introductory material. It is prudent to keep as just mindfulness, on closer inspection is
the practice as simple as possible and to let peo- revealed as a cornucopia of potentially beneficial
ple choose what good things they will be wishing practices, each of which has possible applications
on themselves and others. Remember that metta in research and therapy. That leaves the role of
is not a mindfulness practice. mindfulness itself as a question rather than an
assumption. This issue will be addressed further
in the final discussion.
Missing Factors

There are three known unknowns missing from Measurement of Mindfulness


MBSR. (1) First is the dimension of releasing,
giving things upwhat in religions is called The Scales
renunciation or surrender; in religious studies
kenosis; and in ordinary language change. To Five self-report scales for measuring mindfulness
change anything about oneself, one has to let go have been in common use for some time. Since
of what one was or is. This can be the unacknowl- Ruth Baer (this volume) has, with great clarity,
edged elephant in the room in any kind of ther- reviewed the scales and her own work regarding
apy, self-improvement regiment, or in MBSR. (2) their psychometric properties, I will present only
The second dimension is the experience of not a brief reminder here. The scales are: the Mindful
knowing. Openness to experience is predicated Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), the Freiburg
on openness to non-experience. Without this, the Mindfulness Inventory (FMI), the Kentucky
conscious conceptual mind thrashes anxiously in Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS), the
a sea of not knowing rather than swimming in it. Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale
Without acknowledging the unknown, we could (CAMS), and the Mindfulness Questionnaire
never learn anythingor have science. (3) (MQ). Baer and her collaborators (Baer, Smith,
284 E. Rosch

Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006), in a com- the extremes of dysfunctional spacing out,
prehensive factor analytic study of the properties most people have no idea whether theyre present
of these scales, have convincingly demonstrated or not.
them to be measuring five separate factors in the A second factor measures strong self-
construct rather than a unipolar factor. My con- criticism; a high endorser says that she makes
cern in this chapter is: what are the factors in judgments about whether her thoughts are good
these scales actually measuring? Lets look at the or bad and gets angry with herself for having dis-
bottom line definition of mindfulness and then at tressing thoughts or images. But this is a measure
the factors derived from Baers analysis. of the contents of experience not of whether a
The minimum component in definitions of person can be, or ever actually is, present with
mindfulness, past or present, is that ones mind is those contents while they occur. (To turn atten-
present with ones experience as the experience tion to the contents rather than nature of experi-
occurs over successive moments of time. ence is a natural shift that, as we have seen, was
Variations in definitions consist either of debates also made by the MBSR participants.) One might
over the subcomponents of that process or the hypothesize that being mindfully present with
addition of other attributes to the process. negative self-judgments will reduce their fre-
Mindfulness training consists of doing a practice quency of occurrence, but such a hypothesis
designed to produce such presence. If a researcher remains inherently untestable as long as the com-
wants to study the effects of mindfulness, then pleted outcome is contained in the definition.
mindfulness itself needs to be measured sepa- That is, if the dependent variable is already con-
rately from the effects; that is, the ability to main- tained in the independent variable, then a positive
tain attention to present experience with some experimental result can be nothing but a tautol-
degree of stability (at least sometimes) would be ogy. It is medieval, not scientific, logic to include
the independent variable and hypothesized out- outcomes in ones definitions (as in Molires
comes of that kind of attention would be the famous parody, Opium puts people to sleep
dependent variables. But none of the scales, or because of its dormative power). The fact that
the factors in the scales, measures that kind of MBSR may produce this kind of outcome will
attention. not help, since MBSR teaches many techniques
There is only one factor in the scales aimed at (apparently employed by participants far more
attention, a factor that loads almost entirely on vigorously than mindfulness) that are aimed
items from Brown and Ryans MAAS (Brown & explicitly at replacing negative mental contents
Ryan, 2003). It is composed of items that ask (self criticism is high on the list) with more posi-
about extremes, in fact pathologies, of attention, tive ones.
not about mindful presence. For example, a high A third factor measures moderation in emo-
endorser of these items admits to behaviors such tion, e.g., an ability to modulate affective states
as driving places on automatic pilot and then and not get carried away by thoughts; it corre-
wondering why he went there. A normal person lates positively with the first two factors (pro-
muddling through in our usual non-mindful way vided that one considers the inability to modulate
would be a relatively low endorser and be classi- affective states as the high end of the scale). All
fied as mindful. A second problem with this fac- the objections to the second factor also apply to
tor is that all of the items have to be answered in this one. In fact, if we were to actually take the
the negative to be scored as mindful; but research third factor seriously, there would be no need for
has shown that items with the same content this bookof course self-regulation and
requiring negative answers get different results mindfulness go together if they are part of the
from those phrased for positive answers (see same definition. Again appeal to the effect of
Grossman & Van Dam, 2011, for an extended MBSR is not germane. The heart of MBSR prac-
review and discussion of this issue). Finally note tical training for most participants was the set of
that this is a self-report measure, and except for exercises designed to explicitly prevent emotional,
19 The Emperors Clothes 285

even conceptual, deregulation. The guidelines: to find out what is going on in someones mind
be vigilant for signs of deregulation, stop and when that person himself doesnt know. Someone
apply a corrective. For someone already having with even occasional experience of being mind-
access to more awake states of awareness, bring- fully present is more likely to report herself hope-
ing that awareness to the situation is enough, but lessly not mindful than a person without any such
for MBSR participants what seemed meaningful experience. Long-term Buddhist practitioners
was to actively manipulate mental content reminisce with each other about times they
instead. Finally, there are practices in MBSR believed they understood or realized one or another
such as relaxation, yoga, and loving-kindness teaching but were then disabused by experience
known to calm body and mind and prevent more to the point. This is not optimal territory for
deregulation and escalation of emotions whether the use of self-report scales. Probing interviews
done mindfully or not. are highly impractical. At this point I believe it
The fourth factor is an ability to label and would be most useful for psychologists to turn
describe with words. Verbal facility is an increas- their attention away from the how-to of measure-
ingly necessary ability in our civilization, but it is ment and towards prior questions about what there
not mindfulness. In fact, a mind busy with verbal is to be measured. An extensive landscape of states
labels and descriptions is a prime obstacle for of mind related to being present lies currently
beginning meditators. One does not need an elab- uncharted by Western psychology. Some examples
orate verbal apparatus to distinguish what is the of near neighbors of close mindfulness are:
object of ones meditation from what is not.
A possible fifth factor consisting entirely of Absorptions. These are states where the mind
items taken from the FMI and KIMS, dealt with becomes sufficiently absorbed in the object on
what is usually called psychological mindedness, which it is focusing that it loses clear awareness
i.e., the observation and labeling of internal of itself and the situation. For example, some
states. It did not correlate with any of the other MBSR participants who liked the breathing
factors for people who were not experienced described themselves as relaxing or sinking into
meditators, thus offering more evidence that their breath, or as feeling their minds get blurry
mindfulness and observation mean different or fuzzy, or of feeling themselves slipping, slid-
things to those with and without training ing or falling slowly asleep. Here the mind is
(Grossman & Van Dam, 2011). present in the sense that it is not wandering else-
But these existing mindfulness scales get where yet not alert enough to be cognizant of the
research results, one might protest; what is happen- whole situation. Another kind of absorption that
ing? I believe that the scales are measuring what three MBSR participants offered as an experi-
psychology knows how to measure: factors of gen- ence of mindfulness was becoming intensely
eral mental health and wellbeing. Naturally they focused on a task and shutting out everything
correlate with each other and with other measures else, including, for one person, bodily needs.
of health versus pathology. And naturally they have Again the mind is not wandering (or not as much
brain correlates; everything a living person does as usual), but one is not present with oneself.
and is has brain correlates. The whole area of mea-
surement of mindfulness needs serious rethinking. Human realm states. This is the most important
category with respect to the Western understand-
ing of mindfulness. In Buddhism, states of mind
What to Do Instead can be classified into different aspects of the core
motivations of desire/passion, aversion/aggres-
How then should mindfulness be measured? This sion, and ignorance. Traditionally these were
is not a simple question. Buddhist teachers and taught as realms into which a sentient being could
meditation instructors (and therapists who work be born for a lifetime as well as mental states.
with sullen teenagers) well know how difficult it is Human realm is considered the most
286 E. Rosch

auspicious because (at least in early Buddhism) it felt they had gotten out of MBSR. (Relaxation
is the only realm or state of mind in which one was a close runner-up.) In short: what psychology
can hear and practice the Dharma. That is, one measures under the name mindfulness is basically
cannot practice mindfulness meditation, clear human realm functioning. Rather than quarreling
seeing, loving kindness or, some practices of the over a word, perhaps we should just distinguish it
later forms of Buddhism if ones mind is domi- as a separate species of mindfulness, naming it
nated by a hellish state of aggression, a hungry Human Realm Mindfulness or Humanist
ghost mentality of desperate neediness, the auto- Mindfulness. (And if it came to it, we could also
maticity and plodding ignorance of an animal, have Relaxation Absorption Mindfulness and Task
the competitiveness of a jealous god, or the Absorption Mindfulness.)
drugged-like, ignorant absorptions of god
realm. (For a detailed description of the realms Awareness. In awareness, the alert, stable, pres-
in terms of states of mind, see Trungpa, 1976.) ent oriented attention of mindfulness becomes
The predominant mental state of human realm increasingly expansive. Walking meditation pro-
is one of intelligent desire. The human sees that vides an example. Under Theravada mindfulness
she is not satisfied and seeks what will make instructions (minimally introduced towards the
things better. If what she does doesnt work, she end of MBSR), the practitioner walks slowly,
tries to understand why not, and experiments with barely moving, with instructions to pay close
doing or getting something else. In a human realm attention to each detail of change in pressure on
state of mind, a person intermittently checks his the foot from heel to toe, the lifting, lateral move-
environment and himself to monitor how hes ment, and lowering of the other foot, etc. Under
doing and is wary of losing that monitoring by awareness instructions, the walk is more nor-
becoming too spaced out, angry, critical, or emo- mally paced with attention on the whole body
tional. He develops skills such as language flu- moving in space including a sense of the space
ency in order to have better relationships with the surrounding the body (along with what is in it,
people, objects, and situations in his environment such as other people), the enclosure of the room
in order to better get what he wants and avoid if its taking place indoors, the functioning of
what he doesnt want. A person who spends a rea- ones senses, the vastness of the space outside the
sonable amount of time in human realm states of room, and so on. Walking in this way is consid-
mindand is able to return relatively quickly to ered a bridge to maintaining awareness during
them when she slides into extremes of ignorance, the activities of daily life.
greed, depression, aggression, or competitive-
nesswill likely be judged mentally healthy and Non-dual awareness. Here experience knows
is likely to score as mindful on the mindfulness itself without a separation of observer and
measurement scales since that is what the scales observed. One is present because there is no sepa-
are measuring. Within this framework, the basic rate mind to wander elsewhere. The senses,
aim of psychotherapies might be described as try- including mental states such as thoughts, arise
ing to help patients towards greater human realm freely in their natural brilliance. From this point
functioning. This is also the state of mind that 20 of view, judging an experience or wanting it to be
years ago and without input from Buddhism, some other way will seem irrelevant, and the labo-
Langer (1989) called mindfulness, by that mean- rious mindfulness of the beginner appears a fic-
ing using the mind actively (rather than automati- tion, an artificial and unstable exaggeration of the
cally) and with a sense of efficacy. To guide confused minds assumption of duality between
people into such a mode of functioning is also the an experiencing me and an object of experience.
predominant thrust of the second half of the Non-duality is understood to be a portal into
MBSR training; it was also what was most the further realizations in Mahayana and
described by participants when they spoke during Vajrayana, but why bring it up here? The issue is
the final class or final interview about what they that Kabat-Zinns original mindfulness instruc-
19 The Emperors Clothes 287

tion to be present without judgmentthe inspira- Why Was There Not More
tion on which MBSR is basedonly makes full Mindfulness?
sense within the context of these later teachings
where one is understood to have an original 1. Mindfulness is challenging. It goes against the
nature which, does not pick and choose, and is grain. The habits of a lifetime reinforce a mind
free from accepting and rejecting, hope and fear that moves constantly, ever building and
(common tropes in Zen and Vajrayana). The rebuilding the sense of oneself, ones past and
problem is that the typical person who comes for future, successes and failures, plans and proj-
stress reduction or therapy cannot understand or ects, whatever momentarily maintains a sense
follow instructions based on such qualities (the of meaning to ones life. Present oriented,
mind of samsara constantly judges) any more close attention cuts through all that. It is stark.
than she can follow an instruction to just be or to It requires letting go rather than acquiring. In
love herself unconditionally. (In fact without addition to these affective issues, mindfulness
judgment was interpreted as some variant of I is hard to conceptualize. One doesnt know
shouldnt criticize myself so much by most quite what it is or how to do it. It takes time
MBSR participants when asked in the final inter- and effort to develop.
view.) It takes a path that aims toward non-dual 2. Easy familiar practices supersede new chal-
awareness to uncover it, and MBSR is not that; it lenging ones. In MBSR, mindfulness is taught
is a creatively helpful path towards more and bet- in a context with other practices, such as vigi-
ter picking and choosing. This disparity confuses lance, interrupting stress, and replacing nega-
researchers and will be addressed further in the tive emotions with positive ones that are easy
final discussion. to understand and that fit into the familiar
motivations of getting what you want and
avoiding what you dont want. It is natural
Discussion, Prophecies, Buddhism, that people will replace what is difficult,
and the Emperors Clothes unknown and new with a familiar, easier, and
more understandable alternative when that is
MBSR works; it can improve peoples lives. The offered.
mindfulness measurement scales also work; they 3. Goals. Goals determine how cognition and
usably measure a relative absence of mental attention will be deployed (if youre walking
pathology as well as some positive traits. But is it to a shoe store, you may suddenly notice
mindfulness that is behind such results? If by everyones shoes). A goal also provides the
mindfulness we mean close, stable, present- rationale and motivation for performing activ-
oriented attention, the analysis in this chapter ities that will lead to that goal. Close, continu-
suggests that the role of mindfulness may not be ous attention to the present is a necessary
as obvious as previously assumed. There is no practice in early Buddhism because it enables
measure of such attention in the scales, and it is one: (a) to see impermanence, no self, and that
problematic what role it played in the minds of acting on the basis of desire, aversion, and
the MBSR participants I studied, at least in com- ignorance never lead to satisfaction, and (b) to
parison to the other aspects of the training. Why gradually empty oneself of the habits (karma)
were the participants not experiencing more of lifetimes; this is brought about by not
mindfulness by the end of the study given that responding to the content of each moment as
they had been taught two mindfulness medita- it arises. When this kind of attention is taught
tions (of the close ongoing attention genre) and a as an initial practice in later Buddhism, the
hatha yoga practice with instructions to perform further purpose is to bring the practitioner to
it mindfully? A number of factors would appear the point where she is able to uncover, awaken,
at work, each interacting synergistically with the recognize, or receive transmission of the basic
others. enlightened nature already there, in the light
288 E. Rosch

of which the present moment is transformed. opening the Pandoras box of such questions
Given such purposes and views of the mind, would have disrupted the structure, flow, and
we can see how instructions in the mindful- ambience of the program.
ness meditations to just notice, just see it, One final thought: is mindful, close attention
no need to do anything about it, just stay actually needed to achieve MBSR stress reduc-
with the experience, and so on make sense. tion or is the intermittent strategic noticing used
But if ones purpose, as in MBSR, is to reduce in human realm monitoring sufficient?
stress, thereby having fewer negative and
more positive experiences in ones life, these
instructions would be obvious only if just Limitations to This Study of MBSR
seeing it were immediately rewarding (plea-
surable, peaceful, or at least a relief). However There are a number of limiting factors. The num-
to an ordinary mind in samsara, that isnt ber of trainings I observed was small as were the
necessarily the case. number of participants interviewed. All three
trainings were commercially offered, and the par-
Purpose effects the actual mindfulness medi- ticipants were reasonably high functioning peo-
tations in many ways. For example, try doing a ple. MBSR given in a hospital setting might show
brief portion of the body scan with the intention different patterns. The one participant in the
to just notice the sensations at each point, then trainings I observed who identified herself as
a second time with the intention to use the notic- seriously ill (not one of the interviewees),
ing to relax, and a third time with the intention to remarked one day as we were leaving, These
gain information about your body. Purpose also people act like tourists visiting their bodies; wait
influences ones motivation to stick with a prac- till they get sick and have to live there. MBSR
tice. If one has a stress reduction mind-set and was originally designed for chronic pain patients
finds the meditation boring or disturbing, why judged beyond medical help at the University of
grit your teeth and stick with it? Upon realizing Massachusetts Medical Center. It is quite possi-
one is getting stressed, if one has never experi- ble that for such people the body scan, given with
enced that staying with an experience can be the original instructions to observe without judg-
transformative, why not quickly use some other ment (however they interpreted that), could have
remedy instead? been a radically new and life altering experience.
Purpose also seemed to effect how the People with problems that might require close,
MBSR classes were taught. For example, the protracted, present attention to unraveladdic-
existential questions with which Buddhism tions for example or just an inability to lose
deals (and which mindfulness meditations were weightmight have their own relationship to
designed to arouse) were never talked about in MBSR.
the discussions, even on occasions when par- A different set of issues with the study has to
ticipants brought up something that would have do with participants use of language. The medi-
triggered such discussion in a Buddhist setting. tations, in fact the domain of present oriented
To illustrate: impermanence was treated only as experience in general, were new terrain for their
a comforting reminder that painful situations vocabulary and thought processes. Were some
dont last, and participants were not steered participants saying what they did because they
away from concern for the contents of their didnt know how else to identify or express what
experience towards inquisitiveness about the they wanted or were getting from the medita-
nature of the experiencer. This is understand- tions? (The questions specifically directed at
able, even skillful. Given the place MBSR mindfulness at the end of the final interview
occupies in our society, it is important to keep it seemed more problematic for some people in this
science and medicine based and away from respect than the more naturalistic parts of the
anything interpretable as religion. Furthermore interviews.) An even deeper linguistic problem is
19 The Emperors Clothes 289

that the words we have may not be adequate to discount the role of the meditations. Participants
express what goes on for people in such medita- who did neither the meditations nor the yoga as
tions. For example, participants use of relax- homeworkstrongly predicted by their having
ation may have covered deeper and more complex named some variant of too much to do as their
processes than either they or scientists presently predominant stressor in the first classwere also
have a vocabulary to express. Finally, it is possi- the least satisfied or enthusiastic about what they
ble that the meditations were affecting partici- had gotten out of the program. However, these
pants at a level where the effects were not participants also did less of the homework for the
available to consciousness. Note that use of a stress reduction techniques, so it is difficult to
questionnaire would not help any of these untangle cause and effect. It may well be that
concerns. without at least some settling, calming, and relax-
A final obvious limitation to this study of ation from the meditations, it would be difficult
MBSR is that it was about MBSR and thus for many people to achieve even the tourist vis-
applies only suggestively and not necessarily to its to present experience needed for the stress
other mindfulness programs that present train- reduction practices. The meditations may have
ings in their own ways. made such visits more frequent and/or redirected
the sites visited. (Many participants said they
now paid more attention to their bodies.)
The Other Beneficial Factors in MBSR Buddhists might claim that introduction to the
meditations could, of itself, plant a seed of real-
The prevailing story about why MBSR works is ization that will bear fruit later. And, of course,
mindfulness. This has tended to obscure and belief in the mindfulness mystique could be a
deflect research from the array of less exotic ther- factorthus can research and its wide dissemi-
apeutic techniques that occupy the greater part of nation return home to influence that which is
the training and may have displaced, at least for studied. All of this is ripe for further research.
the people I studied, the close-attention to body
and mind potentially introduced by the medita-
tions. Look at what MBSR offers: relaxation, Prophecies and a Closing Note
physical exercise and yogic movement, improve-
ment in sleep, discrimination training, an early There are present trends that may make the future
warning system for disturbance, correctives for context for mindfulness research a different
what is negative, attention to what is positive, a world. Here are some predictions: Prediction 1:
sense of agency, correction of thought distor- Research on other forms of contemplative prac-
tions, and loving kindness. A range of therapies tice will show the same kinds of beneficial results
are invoked: somatic therapy, relaxation training, as mindfulness. This is already beginning for
sleep medicine, cognitive behavioral therapy, centering prayer in both its Christian and a secu-
behaviorist therapies, positive psychology and lar form (Wachholtz & Pargament 2005).
therapy, empowerment training, and the kindness Centering prayer requires a very different kind of
and compassion embraced by all of the worlds attention than mindfulness, a giving out and
religions and ethical systems. opening up, not a close pointillist attention.
How does this combination of techniques Prediction 2: The fact that the brain shows spe-
work in MBSR? One hypothesis is that it is a syn- cific patterns of neural firing whenever a person
ergy amongst the factorsas in the effectiveness does anything with his body or mind, including
of the AIDS cocktail versus earlier solo drugs. Or meditation, will no longer be a wow factor of
we might think of MBSR as a tea sampler or, itself; the researcher will need to show changes
more endearingly, a gift basket, of effective for- that are part of a more general theoretical formu-
mulas from which MBSR participants can select lation. Prediction 3: Biochemistry and biophysics
the one or more that work for them. Nor can we will find ways to measure increasingly subtle
290 E. Rosch

energy patterns in the body so that we can do center in their death samadhi when they are brain
serious research on inner energy bodies; for and organ dead (Rosch, 2014). Non-dual is a seri-
example, compare them across different yogic ous matter. Established traditions offer not only
systems, or relate their changing forms to physi- wise teachings but also path. In my observation
cal and mental health. Prediction 4: One of the there was nothing whatsoever said by any of the
sciences, perhaps physics, will find ways to mea- MBSR participants or said to them by their teach-
sure aspects of the mind able to function apart ers that indicated even a slight approach toward
from the brain. There are already a number of the non-dual; if anything, the meditations in
phenomena purportedly indicating this to be the MBSR evoked a heightened sense of an onlooker
case (for a compendium see Kelly & Kelly, watching oneself from a distance. If there is a
2007), and advances in resuscitation medicine deeper awareness that might allow people who
have stimulated research interest in such matters are so inclined to get out of the box altogether
(Parnia, 2013). A separation of mind and brain and perhaps remake the world for all of us, it
would open many questions and lines of research, should surely be nurtured rather than destroyed.
and could bring to center stage the claims and Back to the emperors cloths: he is naked but
meditation practices of the later forms of also resplendently arrayed. There is no contradic-
Buddhism, now largely dismissed as religion by tion. They mutually support each other as long as
psychologists. you dont think one is the other. Clarity about this
This brings us back full circle to Buddhism makes for good research, good meditation, and a
and its relation to Western therapeutic mindful- good life.
ness. No one would doubt that the therapies that
operate under the name mindfulness in the West
can be of great benefit to individuals. And any- References
thing that makes people more relaxed, reason-
able, or kindly is truly a boon to civilization. But Baer, R. A. (Ed.). (2006). Mindfulness based treatment
approaches. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
this is not the same as and does not negate the
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., &
kinds of deep mindfulness and awareness that Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment meth-
can be developed in Buddhist meditative and ods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13,
contemplative practices. That both can exist and 2745.
Barrett, B. B. (2010). The emergence of somatic psychol-
perhaps enhance one another should go without
ogy and body-mind therapy. New York: Palgrave
saying. MacMillan.
However it apparently does need a bit of say- Beck, J. S. (2011). Cognitive behavioral therapy: Basics
ing. Kabat-Zinn (2011) seems to now be arguing and beyond (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.
Benson, H., & Proctor, W. (2010). Relaxation revolution:
that his interpretation of mindfulness should and
The science and genetics of mind body healing.
will replace Buddhism. The Buddhist Studies New York: Scribner.
scholar John Dunne (2011) has supported the Boyce, B. (Ed.). (2011). The mindfulness revolution.
logic of this claim by taking Kabat-Zinns defini- Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Broad, W. J. (2012). The science of yoga: The risks and
tion of mindfulness, being present without judg-
the rewards. New York: Simon & Schuster.
ment, as a reference to Buddha nature, in fact as Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of
an evocation of the highest non-dual awareness being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological
of Vajrayana. Is this what MBSR actually con- well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84, 822848.
veys to its participants? Non-dual awareness, the
Buddhaghosa, B. (1976). The path of purification (Vols. 1
self-known arising of each moment without a and 2). Boston, MA; Shambhala.
watcher, may be vast, brilliant and glorious, but Byrom, T. (Ed.). (1993). The Dhammapada: The sayings
caution! It evaporates the sense of self, the reality of the Buddha. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Contemporary Buddhism. (2011). Mindfulness: Diverse
of time, self-referential projectsin short, life as
perspectives on its meaning, origins, and multiple
previously known. It is the kind of awareness that applications at the intersection of science and dharma
allows lamas to radiate and teach from the heart [Special issue]. 12 (1).
19 The Emperors Clothes 291

Davis, M., Eshelman, E. R., & McKay, M. (2008). The Nyanaponika, T. (1973). The heart of Buddhist meditation.
relaxation and stress reduction workbook (6th ed.). New York: Weiser Books.
Oakland, CA: New Harbinger. Parnia, S. (2013). Erasing death: The science that is
Dement, W. C., & Vaughn, C. (1999). The promise of rewriting the boundaries between life and death.
sleep. New York: Dell. New York: HarperCollins.
Demoulin, H. (2005). Zen Buddhism: A history, India & Ponlop, D. (2003). Wild awakening: The heart of
China (Vol. l). Bloomington, IN: World Wisdom. Mahamudra and Dzogchen. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Dunne, J. (2011). Toward an understanding of non-dual Rahula, W. (1959). What the Buddha taught. New York:
mindfulness. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 7188. Grove.
Fields, T. M. (2012). Yoga research. Bloomington, IN: Rosch, E. (2008). Beginners mind: Paths to the wisdom
Xlibris Corporation. that is not taught. In M. Ferrari & G. Potworowski
Fremantle, F. (2001). Luminous emptiness: Understanding (Eds.), Teaching for wisdom (pp. 135162). Hillsdale
the Tibetan book of the dead. Boston, MA: Shambhala. NJ: Erlbaum.
Goenka, S. N. (1987). The discourse summaries of Rosch, E. (2011). Slow lettuce: Categories, concepts,
S.N. Goenka. Onalaska, WA: Vipassana Research fuzzy sets, and logical deduction. In R. Belohlavek &
Publications. G. J. Klir (Eds.), Concepts and fuzzy logic
Grossman, P., & Van Dam, N. T. (2011). Mindfulness by (pp. 89120). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
any other name: Trials and tribulations of sati in Rosch, E. (2014). Tibetan dream yoga and the limits of
Western psychology and science. Contemporary western science. In R. Hurd & K. Bulkeley (Eds.),
Buddhism, 12, 219239. Lucid dreaming: New perspectives on consciousness
Harvey, A., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). in sleep (Vol. 2, pp. 122). Santa Barbara, CA:
Cognitive behavioral processes across psychological dis- Praeger.
orders: A transdiagnostic approach to research and Rosenberg, M. (2003). Nonviolent communication: A lan-
treatment. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. guage of life. Encinitas, CA: PuddleDancer.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: A visionary new
Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential understanding of happiness and well-being.
approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford. New York: Free Press.
Hong, Y. (Ed.). (2008). Tai chi chuan: State of the art in Shambhala Sun. (2012). 20 (4), whole issue.
international research (medicine and sport science). Santideva. (1995). The Bodhicaryavatara. Oxford, UK:
Basil, SZ: S. Karger. Oxford University Press.
James, W. (1890/1950). The principles of psychology. Sharf, R. H. (2015). Is mindfulness Buddhist? (and why it
New York: Dover. matters). Transcultural Psychiatry, 52(3).
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living. New York: Silananda, U. (2002). The four foundations of mindful-
Dell Publishing. ness. Boston, MA: Wisdom Publications.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2011). Some reflections on the origins of Snellgrove, D. (1987). Indo-Tibetan Buddhism: Indian
MBSR, skillful means, and the trouble with maps. Buddhists and their Tibetan successors (Vols. 1 and 2).
Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 281306. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Kelly, E., & Kelly, E. W. (2007). Irreducible mind: Snyder, C. R., Lopez, S. J., & Pedrotti, J. T. (2011).
Toward a psychology for the 21st century. Lenham, Positive psychology: The scientific and practical
MD: Rowman & Littlefield. explorations of human strengths. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Keltner, D. (2009). Born to be good: The science of a Sage.
meaningful life. New York: Norton. Sogyal, R. (1992). The Tibetan book of living and dying.
Kornfield, J. (1977). Living Buddhist masters. Santa Cruz, San Francisco, CA: Harper.
CA: Unity Press. Sprung, M. (1979). Lucid exposition of the middle way.
Langer, E. J. (1989). Mindfulness. Cambridge, MA: Boulder, CO: Prajna Press.
Perseus Books. Stahl, B., & Goldstein, E. (2010). A mindfulness-based
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of stress reduction workbook. Oakland, CA: New
borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford. Harbinger.
Loori, J. D. (2006). Sitting with koans: Essential writings Stone, J. F. (1992). Tai chi chih! Joy thru movement.
on the Zen practice of koan introspection. Somerville, Albuquerque, NM: Good Karma Publications.
MA: Dharma Communications Press. Suzuki, S. (1970). Zen mind, beginners mind. New York:
Mipham, S. (2003). Turning the mind into an ally. John Weatherhill.
New York: Riverhead Books. Trungpa, C. (1976). The myth of freedom and the way of
Nhat Hanh, T. (1975). The miracle of mindfulness! A man- meditation. Boston: Shambhala.
ual on meditation. Boston: Beacon. Trungpa, C. (1984). Shambhala: The sacred path of the
Nhat Hanh, T. (1987). Being peace. Berkeley, CA: warrior. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Parallax Press. Trungpa, C. (1991). Crazy wisdom. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
292 E. Rosch

Trungpa, C. (1993). Training the mind and cultivating relaxation on spiritual, psychological, cardiac and pain
loving-kindness. Boston, MA: Shambhala. outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 28,
Tsoknyi, D. (1998). Carefree dignity: Discourses on 369384.
training in the nature of mind. Hong Kong, HK: Wangyal, T. (2011). Awakening the sacred body: Tibetan
Ranjung Yeshe Publications. yogas of breath and movement. Carlsbad, CA: Hay House.
Wachholtz, A. B., & Pargament, K. I. (2005). Is spiritual- Young, S. (2004). Break through pain: A step-by-step
ity a critical ingredient of meditation? Comparing the mindfulness meditation program for transforming
effects of spiritual meditation, secular meditation and chronic and acute pain. Boulder, CO: Sounds True.
Index

A Automatic mental processes


AAQ. See Acceptance and action questionnaire (AAQ) frame problem, 4749
Acceptance habits (see Habits)
AAQ, 164 mindfulness (see Mindfulness)
ACT (see Acceptance and commitment therapy self-regulation and dual-system models, 4952
(ACT)) Autonomous orientation, 83
maladaptive frames, 5455
Acceptance and action questionnaire (AAQ), 164
Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT), 109, 144, B
145, 149150, 164, 167, 190, 236, 246 Binge eating disorder (BED), 200
Addiction, 114, 115 Binge Eating Scale (BES), 210
birth of, 189 Bipolar disorder and MBCT
craving (see Craving) medications, 175176
early models, 186189 relapse prevention, 176
effects, 186 residual mood and anxiety symptoms, 176
literature, 185 Borderline personality, 2830
neurobiological mechanisms, 194195 Borderline personality disorder (BPD), 149
psychological models, 185, 186 Brain network, 2325, 27, 28, 30
treatment (see Treatment of addiction) Brain state training, 24
Adrenocortocotropic hormone (ACTH), 224 Breathing exercise, 36, 38, 40, 41
Affect Buddhism
biases, 187, 188, 193 Mahayana, 274
and craving, 186, 190 nondual, 259
negative affect (see Negative affect, emotions) social activist forms, 272
positive, 189, 191, 192 suffering, 259
reactions, 187 Theravada, 272274
self-identity, 195 Vajrayana, 274275
tone, 186 yanas, 272
Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), 9, 10, 13, 15, 18 Buddhist model, 26
Antisocial personality disorder (ASPD), 225227
Attention
anxiety, 240 C
bodily sensations, 243 CAMS-R. See Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness
breath-related sensations, 239 Scale-Revised (CAMS-R)
cycle maintaining distress, 239, 240 Cessation
distress, 242243 craving, 196
facility, 240 emotion regulation, 115, 116
metacognitive awareness, 240 habituated affective self-identity, 195
mindfulness instruction, 243 mindfulness, 114
neutral sensation, 239 outcomes, 114
physiological processes, 244 smoking, 114, 115, 167, 193
self-regulation, 242243 treatments, 114, 189191
sensory experience, 243 trial, 193
and working memory, 3338 unpleasant feeling tones, 186

Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 293


B.D. Ostafin et al. (eds.), Handbook of Mindfulness and Self-Regulation,
DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-2263-5
294 Index

CHIME-. See Comprehensive inventory of mindfulness emergent wise mind, 205


experiences beta (CHIME-) interrupt reactivity, 203204
Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale (CAMS), 283 meditation practice, 203
Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised nonjudgmental awareness, 203
(CAMS-R), 161 reinforcement cycle, 205
Common humanity, 122 Depression
Comprehensive inventory of mindfulness experiences emotion regulation, 111
beta (CHIME-), 163 MB-EAT, 210
Consciousness, 37, 43 residual and acute, 174175
Contemporary mindfulness self-compassion, 123124
Abhidharma typology, 257, 266 Dialectical behavior therapy (DBT), 109, 110, 117, 149, 283
distractors, 257 Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), 27
formal and informal practice, 266 Discreet choice experiment (DCE), 179
Innateism, 267 Dispositional mindfulness research
MBSR, 267 attention, 67
meditation session, 266 classical Buddhist and science-based conception, 69
modernity, 257 definitions, 6768
nondual approach, 267 descriptions, 68
non-judgmental approach, 257, 266 dissociable measures, 6970
Controlled orientation. See Self-regulation and issues, 67, 77
mindfulness limitations, 68
Coping and self-compassion, 126 nature and expression, 77
Craving neural markers, 7172
affect, 186, 190 non-judgment/acceptance, 68
and aversion, 187, 188 operationalized perspectives, 69
Buddhist model, 190, 196 physiological concomitants, emotion regulation, 71
cessation, 196 psychometric instruments, 67
drug, 189 quality of attention and/or awareness, 68
eating disorders, 117 self-report measures, 7071
feeling, 53 training effects, 7374
and grasping, 5254 trait (see Trait mindfulness and development)
mindfulness training, 191, 193194 Dual process models of cognition
motivation, 186 habit system, 4950
negative affect, emotions, 116 reflective system, 5051
psychological model, 186 self-regulation, 5152
responses, 54
Creativity
cognitive and behavioral reactions, 55 E
divergent thinking, 56 Eating disorders
Einstellung effect, 5556 BED, 201
insight problems, 56 concomitant desensitization, 201
mindfulness intervention, 56 cultivating mindfulness, 205210
self-compassion, 124 de-conditioning, 200
Cultivating mindfulness, eating disorders dysregulation, food intake, 200201
emotional balance, 208 interoceptive awareness, 201
MBSR, 207 MB-EAT program, 205, 206
meditation practice, 207 meditation effects, 199
mini-meditations, 207 nutritional value, 199
outer wisdom, 208209 physiological feedback systems, 200
quality and quantity, 208 self-regulation theory, 199200
self-acceptance, 209210 sensory-specific satiety, 201
sensory-specific satiety, 207 Ego depletion, 99, 100
taste satisfaction, 207 Emotional state, 35, 37, 39
Emotion processing
amygdala activation, 12
D DMPFC, 11
DBT. See Dialectical behavior therapy (DBT) EEG, 11
De-conditioning, eating disorders fMRI, 11
cognitive-behavioral approach, 205 LPP, 11
comfort food, 205 VLPFC, 12
Index 295

Emotion regulation self-destructive behaviors, 4849


anxiety and depression, 111 treatment, 49
attention and awareness, 108 Freiburg mindfulness inventory (FMI),
broaden-and-build theory, 111 160161, 283
cognitive conceptualization, 108 Functional conceptual model
DBT, 109 attention, 239
differences, 108 bodily sensation, 239
experience sampling methodologies, 112113 breath-related sensations, 239
higher positive affect, 110 cognitions and behaviors, 246
hypo-ego, 108 distress automatic maintenance, 239
impact, behavioral outcomes, 114117 evolutionary processes, 242
implications, 107 major depressive disorder, 241
levels, mindfulness, 109110 MBSR, 244
literature, 107 mindfulness practice, 241
lower negative affect, 110 rapid disengagement, 242
MBCT, 109, 111 re-perceiving/metacognitive awareness, 241
MBSR, 108109 respiration and awareness, 244
mindfulness training, 110 socialization sensation, 245
neurological underpinnings, 113114 spiritual bypass, 245
operational definitions, 107
social rejection, 111112
terror management, 110 H
treatments, 109, 117 Habits
Enthusiasm, 41, 124, 155, 271 affective bias, 193
Ethics, Buddhist perspectives animals, 47
awareness, 256 attention, participants, 50
heedfulness, 256 Buddhist model, 5253
mental states, 256 consolidation of related memories, 187
Executive control coping strategies, 195
attentional, 5354 cue-induced, 189
automatic appetitive processes, 59 frames, 49
awareness, 53 information, associative network, 49
deliberation, 51 measures, 4950
distractions, 50 nodes and reflection, 49
individual differences, 58 patterns, 282
resources, 34, 49, 57 reactions, 187, 188, 191, 192
self-regulation, 54 transfer-appropriate processing, 50
stressors impair, 52
Exercises, 33, 36, 37, 42
Extrinsic goals I
awareness, 81 Impulse control
instrumental values, 90 decision-making, 230
vs. intrinsic, 8384 human development, 230
hyper-rational thinking, 229
long-term costs, 229
F mindfulness training, 231
Five-facet mindfulness questionnaire, 162 prefrontal cortex, 230
Fluid intelligence, 41, 42 psychiatric conditions, 230
FMI. See Freiburg mindfulness inventory (FMI) psychological mechanism, 229
Food cravings and eating disorders, 117 self-regulation., 229
Frames Integrative body-mind training (IBMT), 2629
adaptations, 48 Interpersonal neurobiology (IPNB), 218
complex goal, 48 Interpersonal relationships and self-compassion,
cost, 48 126127
creation, decentered perspective, 55 Intrinsic goals
experience-related, 48 autonomous, 81
goal-oriented nature, humans, 47 vs. extrinsic, 8384
information processing capacity, 48 individuals, 81
intended consequences, behavior, 48 orientation, 8990
relinquishment, emotion regulation agenda, 5455 selection, 82
296 Index

K Meditation-Nave, 1314, 17
Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills (KIMS), 161 Memory, Buddhist perspectives
centered awareness, 258
convergence, 258
M definitions, 258
MAAS. See Mindful Attention Awareness Scale divergence, 258
(MAAS) mental facet, 258
Mahamudra Milindapanha, 258
Abhidharma, 262 nondual experiences, 259
Calm Abiding, 263 recollective memory, 259
contemporary mindfulness, 262, 266268 Mental health
decentering, 264 autonomous capacity, 221
epistemological approach, 262 chronic depression, 220
expectation, 264 consilience, 219
formal practice, 263264 electromagnetic field, 218
heedfulness, 263 emotional provocation, 218
heuristics, 268 empathy and neurobiology, 223225
judgment and ethics, 265 energy and information flow, 219, 220
meditation instructors, 263 genetic expression, 220
non-distraction, 263, 265 immoral action, 229
nonduality, 262 impulse control, 229231
practical aphorisms, 263 integration, 219
reflexive awareness, 265 intrapersonal systems, 217
Major depressive disorder (MDD), 171, 173176, 180 MBSR program, 219
MBCT. See Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy neurological processes, 218
(MBCT) participants, 228
MBSR. See Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) praiseworthiness, 228
Measurement, mindfulness psychiatric disorders, 221
absorptions, 285 psychological processes, 217
awareness, 286 psychopathy/sociopathy, 217
Baers analysis, 284 regulatory process, 227
brain correlation, 285 robust immune functions, 220
deregulation, 285 societal expectations, 218
dormative power, 284 ToM, 227
human realm states, 285 Meta-awareness, 35, 43, 6870, 244
MBSR, 284 Metacognitive awareness, 240, 241
non-dual awareness, 286 Mindful attentional stance, 9, 11, 13, 14, 18, 19
psychological mindedness, 285 Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS),
psychometric properties, 283 161, 283
relaxation, 286 Mindful eating and living (MEAL), 211
self-judgments, 284 Mindfulness
stability and outcomes, 284 acceptance, maladaptive frames, 5455
Measure of awareness and coping in autobiographical and attentional flexibility, 5657
memory (MACAM), 165 automatic responses, 5758
Medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), 9, 10, 14, 15, 18 cessation treatments, 189191
Mediation analysis and creativity, 5556
changes, MBIs, 167 definition, 52, 142143
statistical recommendations, 165166 disorders (see Eating disorders)
Meditation executive control lessens, 5354
bodily sensations, 276 and habit, Buddhist model, 5253
discrimination, 277 history of (See Mindfulness and clinical practice,
information gathering, 278 history of)
mindfulness training, 11, 1417 MBIs (see Mindfulness-based
nave participants, 1314 interventions (MBIs))
practices, 25 measures of (see Mindfulness, measures of)
relaxation, 277 MT (see Mindfulness training (MT))
sensations, 277 science of, 16
Theravada Buddhism, 276 and self-regulation, 102103
transient vibrations, 277 and training attention, 2325
vipassana exercises, 277 working memory, 3343
Index 297

Mindfulness and clinical practice, history of mediation, 155


acceptance, 144145 patient suitability, 176178
ACT, 144 randomized controlled trials, 155
beginning of psychotherapy, 125 relationship, variables, 155156
Buddhisms aspiration, 125 samples, 159
DBT, 126 statistical recommendations, 165166
education, 126 theoretical assumption, 156157
integration, 125 therapist training, 178
MBCT, 126 Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR)
MBSR, late-1970s, 125126 addiction, 190
meditation practices, 125 anthropological technique, 276
mid-1970s, 125 cognitive therapy, 278
TM, 125 distortions, 278279
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) distressing emotions, 279
addiction, 190 emotion regulation, 108109
bipolar disorder (see Bipolar disorder hatha yoga, 278
and MBCT) human interaction, 279
DCE, 179 interpersonal relations, 279
development, 171 interruption and alleviation, 278
efficacy, 179180 interview responses, 276
emotion regulation, 109, 111 meditations, 275, 276
empirical support, clinical effects, 173174 motivation setting, 276
evidence, 173175, 177, 178, 180 psychotherapy, 143144, 148, 151
feasibility, 179 Mindfulness-based stress reduction course, 39
group format, 178179 Mindfulness, Buddhist perspectives
MDD, 171 coherence, 252
mood (see Mood disorders) community, 253
prevention, depressive relapse, 172 contemplative studies, 253
program description, 172173 decentering, 251
psychological imbalances, 5 distorted cognitions, 255
psychotherapy, 126, 128, 178 divergence, 258
reduction, risk, 171172 emergence, 253
residual and acute depression, 174175 ethical judgment, 252
UPLIFT project, 179 informal practices, 256257
Mindfulness based eating awareness training Mahamudra, 262267
(MB-EAT) mental facets, 255
BES, 210 nondual, 253255
clinical outcomes, 210 rhetoric of authenticity, 252
depression, 210 stressful conversation, 251
medical nutrition group program, 212 strong motivations, 253
mini-meditations, 211 suffering, 255
outer wisdom cultivation, 211 theoretical literature, 251
PECB treatment, 210 and Western versions, 56
permissiveness, 211 Mindfulness, measures of
Prader-Willi syndrome, 212 AAQ, 164
stress management, 211 CAMS-R, 161
Mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) construction, 163
assessment, 156 experiences questionnaire, 164165
changes, statistical mediation, 166167 five-facet mindfulness questionnaire, 162
cross-sectional studies, 158159 FMI, 160161
delivery format, 178180 KIMS, 161
dispositional, 155 MAAS, 161
empirical support, 155 MACAM, 165
examinations, 156 MPQ, 162163
hypotheses, 156 PHLMS, 162
laboratory experiments, 157158 SCS, 164
learning and skills, 155 SMQ, 161162
longitudinal treatment-outcomes, 157 TMS, 163
MBSR, 171 Mindfulness, mechanisms of. See Mindfulness-based
measuring (see Mindfulness, measures of) interventions (MBIs)
298 Index

Mindfulness meditation training lower levels, 109111


breath, 15 and neuroticism, 114
DMN processing, 17 patterns of brain activation, 113
MBCT, 16 and positive, 115
MBSR, 1416 post-stressor, 111
SAD symptoms, 15, 16 reduction, 115, 116
yoga, 15 smoking, 116
Mindfulness process questionnaire (MPQ), 162163 substance usage, 109, 115
Mindfulness research Network training, 24
amygdala, 13 Neural affective responses, 20
DMN, 14 Neural markers
emotional reactivity, 14 affective style, 7172
PCC, 13 emotional reactivity, 7273
rostro-ventral ACC, 17 Neural mechanisms of mindfulness, 113114
stimuli, 13 Neurobiological mechanisms, 194195
subgenual region, 13 Neurobiology
VMPFC, 13 anterior cingulate cortex, 224
Mindfulness training (MT) aversive behavior, 224
addictive loop, 191193 emotion, 19
attention, 3638 emotional experiences, 19, 223
craving, 193194 HPA axis, 224
effectiveness, 190 mirror neuron, 225
fluctuations in cognitive processes, 36, 4143 neuropeptide oxytocin, 224
fMRI, 23 psychosocial interaction, 223
literature, 36 recognition and sharing, 223
mind wandering, 40 Neurobiology of mindfulness and emotion processing,
participant experience, 276278 920
prototypical, 36 Neuroendocrine markers, stress, 73
self-regulation, 2325 Nondual, Buddhist perspectives
size of attention, 36 Abhidharma, 254
smoking, 191 Apoha theory, 260
treatment, addictions, 190191 DBT, 254
working memory, 36, 3840 distraction, 254
Mindsight, mental health formal practice, 259
energy and information flow, 223 heuristic presentation, 253
interoceptive monitoring, 222 meditative practices, 255
interpersonal neurobiology, 221 mental representation, 260
social intelligence, 221 phenomenal forms, 260
suffering, 222 reflexive awareness, 261262
telomerase, 222 suffering, 259260
Mind wandering, 3536, 40, 41, 43 Theravada approaches, 255
Mood disorders time traveling, 261
determination, patient suitability, 176 Yogacara philosophy, 260
MBI outcomes, 177
mindfulness training, 177
psychotherapeutic techniques, 177 O
PTSD, 177 Orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), 9, 10, 19
screening and assessment, 178
treatment, 178
Motivation and self-compassion, 42, 126 P
MPQ. See Mindfulness process questionnaire (MPQ) Pathologies, 28, 279, 284
Personality
ASPD, 225227
N BPD, 2830, 149
Negative affect, emotions neuroscience and cognitive perspectives, 35
avoidance, 114 SBT, 82
and craving, 116 and social psychology, 4
drugs, 114 Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale (PHLMS), 162
and emotional eating, 117 Positive affect, 110, 189, 191, 192
labeling, 113 Profile of mood states (POMS), 27
Index 299

Prophecies, mindfulness mystique S


consciousness, 289 Schizophrenia, 28, 30
hatha yoga, 287 Science of mindfulness and self-regulation
human realm monitoring, 288 disorders, 1, 4, 5
impermanence, 287 meditation, 2
MBSR, 288 neuroscience, 3
meditations, 288 personality and social psychology, 4
motivations, 287 psychological imbalances, 5
participants, 288 symptoms, 46
Psycho-educational/cognitive behavioral (PECB), 210 SCS. See Self-Compassion Scale (SCS)
Psychological needs, 82, 84, 142, 219 Self-awareness
Psychopathy, 28, 29 and attention, 84
Psychotherapeutic intervention, 218 vs. mindfulness, 8485
Psychotherapy Self-compassion
acceptance, 149150 common humanity, 122
attention, 141, 238 coping, 126
behavior modification, 141 definition, 121
BPD, 149 and emotional wellbeing, 123124
Buddhist traditions, 237 interpersonal relationship, 126127
clinical history, mindfulness, 143145 meaning, 121
cohort effects, 145146 vs. mindfulness (see Self-compassion vs.
DBT, 236 mindfulness)
definitions, mindfulness, 142143 and motivation, 126
diagnostic processes, 147 origins of, 127
effects, 148149 SCS (see Self-Compassion Scale (SCS))
emotional suffering, 235 vs. self-esteem, 124125
empirical systems, 218219 self-kindness, 122
examination, 141142 teaching, 128129
functional utility, 238 Self-Compassion Scale (SCS), 123, 130, 164
graphic exposure to negative content, 146147 Self-compassion vs. mindfulness
MBCT, 148, 178 culture teaching, 133
MBRP, 148 loving-kindness meditation, 132
MBSR, 148, 236 MBCT, 132133
mental experience, 237 MBSR, 132
mindfulness and acceptance methods, 147148 MSC program, 133
mindfulness training, 235 research, 133134
physical community, 147 Self-control. See Self-regulation
research developments, 145 Self-determination, definition, 82
strategic changes, 145146 Self-determination theory (SBT)
suffering, 235 attention and awareness, 84
therapeutic systems, 235 framework, 84
virtual community, 147 human motivation, 82
words and images, 146 mindfulness (see Self-regulation and mindfulness)
personality development, 82
perspectives, 82
R regulation, forms (see Self-regulation, forms of)
Reduce stress, 30 Self-esteem vs. self-compassion, 124125
Reinforcement, negative and positive, 185, 189, 192 Self-kindness, 122, 123, 130132, 164
Relapse prevention Self-regulation
depressive, 172, 176 definition, 9596
mindfulness based, 171 and dual-system perspectives, 5152
symptoms, 180 forms of (see Self-regulation, forms of)
Repeated testing, 42 high, 101102
Re-perceiving, 224, 240, 241 mind and mental health (see Mental health)
Research design, recommendations, 165166 and mindfulness (see Self-regulation and
Residual and acute depression mindfulness)
empirical support, 175 monitoring, 9798
reduction, 175 range of activities, 95
symptoms, 174175 recognition, 95
300 Index

Self-regulation (cont.) Strong concentrative attention, 37


and science of mindfulness, 16 Substance abuse, 2829, 114116
standards, 9697 Suffering, Buddhist perspectives
strength (see Strength model, self-regulation) Abhidharma model, 259
theory (see Self-regulation theory) Buddhism, 259
training of, 99 distorted cognitions, 255
Self-regulation and mindfulness phenomenal forms, 260
autonomous, 85, 86, 88, 9091
competence and relatedness, 85
de-automatizing and integration, 8687 T
defensive thinking patterns, 89 Terror management, 88, 110
ego-investment, 88 Theory of mind (ToM), 227229
intrinsic goal orientation, 8980 Theravada
MASS, 86 Abhidharma, 273
open and receptive observation, 89 impermanence, 273
self-evaluations, 8788 MBSR, 272
wellness consequences, 85 meditation, 272
Self-regulation, forms of monastics, 274
autonomous, 83 psychology, 272
goals, intrinsic and extrinsic, 82 relaxation, 272
identification, 83 samsara, 273
integration, 83 suffering, 273
intrinsic vs. extrinsic aspirations, 8384 TMS. See Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS)
introjected, 8283 ToM. See Theory of mind (ToM)
mindfulness, 83 Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS), 163
spectrum, 83 Training brain networks, 2526
Self-regulation theory Trait mindfulness
biofeedback, 200 and affective style, 7172
food intake regulation, 200 development (see Trait mindfulness and
fundamental pragmatics, 202 development)
hedonic drives, 203 measures of (see Trait mindfulness, measures of)
MB-EAT, 200 and stress, 7273
meditation, 202 Trait mindfulness and development
neurocognitive models, 201 assessing capacities, 7475
physiological homeostasis, 200 Buddhist documents, 76
relaxation process, 202 higher levels, 75
self-acceptance, 202, 203 informal and formal, 76
training, relaxation, 200 interventions, 76
Smoking cessation, 114116 learning and skills, 75
SMQ. See Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire mindful capacities, 7475
(SMQ) research, 75
Social rejection, 112 self-report measures, 74
Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire (SMQ), social factors, 7576
161162 Trait mindfulness, measures of
Stepping stone, 41 Buddhist psychology, 6768
Strength model, self-regulation classical, 68
conservation and motivation, 100101 conceptual provenance, 69
depletion effects, 99 contemporary descriptions, 68
development, 99100 differences, 69
energy, 98 emotional reactivity, 4
individuals capacity, 98 meditation, 14
information process and cognitive negative correlation, 40
construction, 98 neurobiological networks, 18
limited resource, 9899, 101, 103 non-judgment/acceptance, 68
and mindfulness, 102103 psychometric measures, 74
personal beliefs, 101 quality of attention and awareness, 68
skills, 98 Transcendental meditation (TM), 125
suppression exercise, 99 Treatment of addiction
task performance, 99 Buddhist practices, 190
willpower, 98 core addictive loop, 189190
Index 301

mainstay behavioral, 189 familiarity, 281


MBCT, 190 insomnia, 281
MBRP, 190 kindness and compassion, 283
MBSR, 190 measurement instruments, 271
mindfulness training, 190 meditation works, 271
physical and mental health, 290
prophecies, 287289
V relaxation, 280
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), 9, 10, 13, 15, renunciation/surrender, 283
16, 18, 19 sense of agency, 282283
sleep, 281
targeted therapies, 271
W therapeutic program, 279, 289
Western mindfulness mystique vigilance and interruption, 282
bodily movement practices, 280281 Working memory
centering prayer, 289 and attention, 3335
clinical disorders, 281 breathing, 38
contemplative practices, 290 complex mental operations, 34
design interventions, 272 ERP, 34
discrimination, 281282 fMRI, 34
distortions, 282 mind wandering, 3536
dysfunction, psychoanalysis, 280 training (see Mindfulness training)

Potrebbero piacerti anche