Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Psychology and Aging Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1990, Vol. 5, No. 1,58-67 0882-7974/90/$00.75

Tenacious Goal Pursuit and Rexible Goal Adjustment:


Explication and Age-Related Analysis of Assimilative
and Accommodative Strategies of Coping
Jochen Brandtstadter and Gerolf Renner
University of Trier
Trier, Federal Republic of Germany

Crises and critical life transitions activate 2 distinct but complementary modes of coping, (a) trans-
forming developmental circumstances in accordance with personal preferences (assimilative ten-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

dency) and (b) adjusting personal preferences to situational constraints (accommodative tendency).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Assimilative and accommodative tendencies were measured by a questionnaire comprising 2 inde-


pendent scales (Tenacious Goal Pursuit and Flexible Goal Adjustment). Both scales predict high life
satisfaction and low depression and are positively related to generalized internal control beliefs. The
scales evinced an opposite relation to age: Cross-sectional analyses on a sample of 890 Ss in the age
range from 34 to 63 years revealed a gradual shift from an assimilative to an accommodative mode
of coping. Implications for theories of depression and successful aging are discussed.

Developmental change involves gains and losses in different garded as highly aversive (cf. Hofstatter, 1986). This theoretical
functional and life domains. Efforts to keep this balance favor- point of view may also contribute to a better understanding of
able represent an essential aspect of human action and an im- the conditions and processes that help individuals maintain
portant momentum of personal development over the life span positive developmental perspectives in later phases of life, when
(cf. Baltes, 1987; Brandtstadter, 1986). Attempts to avoid or irrevocable losses and uncontrollable events cumulate.
correct developmental problems through deliberate readjust- Assimilative and accommodative modes of coping are not
ments of behavior or life-style may, of course, prove unavailing. mutually exclusive but may operate simultaneously in concrete
Problems defying intentional modification often can be over- situations. However, we expect that in the initial stage of a cop-
come only by more or less radical revisions of personal prefer- ing episode, assimilative tendencies dominate (unless the situa-
ences and life designs. Thus, we distinguish two alternative tion appears unalterable from the start). Accommodative tend-
strategies or processes of optimizing the balance of gains and encies, on the other hand, should be activated to the extent that
losses in personal development: On the one hand, discrepancies active, assimilative attempts to change the situation become in-
between the actual and the desired course of development can effective.
be eliminated by actively adjusting developmental and life cir- In this article, we begin by elaborating the theoretical back-
cumstances to personal preferences. On the other hand, these ground for these distinctions. Next, we briefly describe the con-
discrepancies may be removed by adopting the opposite strategy struction of the questionnaire used to assess assimilative and
of adjusting personal preferences and goal orientations to given accommodative strategies on a dispositional level. Finally, we
situational forces and constraints. We have designated these two present findings from a larger panel study that bear on age-re-
basic processes as assimilative coping and accommodative cop- lated changes in these modes of coping, as well as on their rela-
ing, respectively (Brandtstadter, 1989; Brandtstadter & Baltes- tionships to subjective appraisals of personal development.
Gotz, in press).1 The accommodative mode of coping should be
considered as a neutralization rather than as an active solution
Theoretical Background
of the problem. In this mode, the structure of individual cogni-
tions and valuations is modified to make the given situation ap- Both accommodative and assimilative coping processes aim
pear less negative or more acceptable. Such accommodation of at eliminating discrepancies between actual life perspectives
preferences may account for the well-known paradoxes in re- and salient concerns of personal development, which typically
search on life satisfaction, that is, the phenomenon of high satis- occur in aversive life situations and especially in later phases of
faction under circumstances of living that are commonly re- life, when individuals tend to experience an increasingly unfa-
vorable balance of developmental gains and losses (cf. Brandt-

This study was supported by a research grant from the German Re-
1
search Foundation (DFG) to Jochen Brandtstadter. To prevent misunderstanding, we emphasize that the concepts of
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to assimilation and accommodation are discussed in this article in a ge-
Jochen Brandtstadter, Fachbereich IPsychologic, Universita't Trier, neric sense, without implying meanings that these terms may have in
Postfach 3825, D-5500 Trier, Federal Republic of Germany. other (e.g., Piagetian) theoretical contexts.

58
ASSIMILATIVE AND ACCOMMODATIVE STRATEGIES 59

stadter, Krampen, & Heil, 1986; Heckhausen, Dixon, & Baltes, life themes and value orientations. The phase of depression and
1989). In Figure 1, both modes of coping have been integrated disorientation is terminated by disengaging from barren goals
into a general model of personal self-regulation of development. and building up new action orientations and perspectives for
The model delineates typical patterns of cognitive appraisals, personal development (12 and 13). Such accommodative reori-
control beliefs, and emotions that induce, moderate, or accom- entations may include adjustments of aspiration level, revision
pany assimilative and accommodative modes of coping (cf. of value priorities and evaluative standards, neutralization of
Brandtstadter, 1986,1989). aversive evaluations by selectively focusing on specific conse-
quences, and cognitive reappraisals that aim at the construction
Assimilative Phase of meaning (cf. Campbell, Converse, & Rodgers, 1976; Pearlin,
1980; Thompson, 1985). Eventually, such processes may lead
Components 1-10 of the model depict different cognitive, to radical changes in a person's system of beliefs about him- or
evaluative, and behavioral elements involved in assimilative herself and the world, changes comparable to a revolutionary
control activities, as well as associated emotional reactions. paradigm shift in the domain of scientific theories (cf. Break-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

These control activities are induced by a self-monitoring pro- well, 1986; Taylor, 1983).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cess in which the individual evaluates his or her current or antic- To put our formulation into a broader perspective, note that
ipated life situation with reference to personal plans, develop- the antagonism between active-offensive and complacent-ac-
mental goals, or life designs (1 and 2). If this appraisal signals a cepting modes of coping with life problems is a traditional topic
satisfying state of affairs (+), the tendency to change established in philosophical anthropology (cf. the distinction between
behavioral patterns or ways of living is low (3). Of special inter- offensive and defensive conceptions of happiness, Tatarkiewicz,
est, however, is the case in which actual life perspectives are neg- 1984). The problem of achieving an adequate balance between
atively evaluated (). A negative evaluation induces a motiva- these contrasting attitudes has been addressed as a key problem
tional tendency to alter the situation, the execution of which, in various philosophical conceptions of wisdom and optimal liv-
however, critically depends on subjectively available instrumen- ing (cf. Kamlah, 1973; Schopenhauer, 1851). Conceptually re-
tal resources and control capacities (4 and 5); the branching lated constructs also appear in psychological formulations of
points in the graph delineate alternative sequences of action. If the coping process; for example, the distinctions between pri-
personal resources and capabilities are considered sufficient (+) mary and secondary control (Rothbaum, Weisz, & Snyder,
to cope with the problem, corrective actions (e.g., changes in 1982), between problem-focused and emotion-focused coping
personal behavior or life-style) are prepared and tentatively exe- (Lazarus & Golden, 1981; Lazarus & Launier, 1978; Moos &
cuted (6). The implementation of behavioral goals formed dur- Billings, 1982), and between active and passive mastery (Neu-
ing the preparatory stages of such self-corrective actions may garten, 1973). Our position converges with these formulations
involve heuristic processes on different levels of planning; in in the assumption that a sense of personal efficacy and a satisfy-
this case, our model simplifies (cf. Carver & Scheier, 1981). If, ing developmental perspective may be enhanced not only by ac-
however, personal resources are considered insufficient to solve tive, instrumental efforts at control but also by the adaptation
the problem (5, -), the person may first try to improve the of personal preferences and life designs to situational con-
chances to succeed by searching for information, acquiring new straints (cf. Elster, 1983). Instrumental efforts at control, how-
skills, etc. (7 and 8). Activation of such exploratory and compe- ever, should not be confounded with reactive processes that do
tence-building tendencies in the context of crises and critical not involve personal agency. The constructs of primary versus
life events characteristically occurs when the initially perceived secondary control or of problem-focused versus emotion-fo-
lack of instrumental resources is seen not as stable or unalter- cused coping partly cut across these categories. In our formula-
able but as open to change (7, +). If this condition does not (or, tion, the notions of control and personal agency primarily apply
after repeated ineffective attempts, does no longer) hold (-), the to assimilative coping strategies. Accommodative changes in be-
individual may eventually attempt to manage the situation by liefs or preferences, by contrast, can hardly be considered as
using external help (9 and 10; cf. the concept of "proxy control," reasoned actions or as a deliberately chosen strategy; gener-
Bandura, 1977). ally, such changes simply happen to people. The common moti-
vational background of assimilative and accommodative pro-
Accommodative Phase cesses is not an overarching need to maintain control, but rather
a tendency to achieve consistency between actual and intended
According to the model, accommodative tendencies (see Fig- courses of personal development.
ure 1, Components 11, 12, and 13) are induced to the extent Some implications for a theory of depression should be
that assimilative problem-solving attempts on the different lev- briefly considered because they bear on issues of well-being and
els described above are seen as, or have become, ineffective in successful aging (see also Brandtstadter & Baltes-Gotz, in
improving the person's situation and developmental perspec- press). According to our formulation, the duration and severity
tives (7, -), that is, when assimilative tendencies and prior con- of depressive episodes depend not only on insidious attribu-
trol beliefs backing them have been sufficiently weakened. Emo- tional styles and control beliefs, as explicated in the learned
tions of hopelessness, resignation, and depression characterize helplessness approach (e.g., Peterson & Seligman, 1984), but
an intermediate stage of disorganization and disorientation just as much on the individual's readiness or capability to revise
(11). The duration of this phase depends on the importance of and readjust personal preferences. This assumption converges
the blocked goal perspectives with regard to the person's basic with observations by Coyne, Aldwin, and Lazarus (1981) indi-
60 JOCHEN BRANDTSTADTER AND GEROLF RENNER

Evaluation
of personal Is developmental Static orientation.
developmental situation low corrective
prospects satisfactory ? tendency
and conditions

(DISAPPOINTMENT^
ANNOYANCE

Appraisal
of potentials and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

limitations for
corrective action
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Development-related
Is personal Enhancement of
change efforts
(planning, control potential personal control
executive control) sufficient ? potential

Is improvement
of personal
control potential
possible ?

Involvement Are external


of external supports and
support systems resources
(proxy control) available ?

Restabllization, Accommodative
formation of Disorganization,
reappraisal, search identity crisis,
new developmental for meaning. Identity
perspectives dlsorientation
transformation

Figure 1. Assimilative and accommodative phases of coping, as related to cognitive and emotional appraisals
of personal development (from Brandtstadter, 1989). (See text for further explanations; components are
numbered for reference in text.)
ASSIMILATIVE AND ACCOMMODATIVE STRATEGIES 61

eating that depressed persons view fewer events as necessitating (1985); (0 a depression index, which was derived from self-rat-
acceptance and accommodation. A strong sense of personal ings on selected adjective scales (see Brandtstadter & Baltes-
control and efficacy may in fact reduce the individual's readi- Gotz, in press); and (g) the depression scale of the Freiburger
ness to adjust preferences and revise goal perspectives and thus Personlichkeitsinventar (Freiburg Personality Inventory, FPI,
may even aggravate depressive problems in cases in which the short version; Fahrenberg, Hampel, & Selg, 1973).
individual is faced with uncontrollable events (cf. Janoff-Bul- In exploratory factor analyses of the starting version, two ho-
man & Brickman, 1982; Perloff, 1987; Rotter, 1975). These as- mogeneous groups of items emerged that closely corresponded
sumptions and findings do not easily fit with theoretical ac- to our theoretical intentions (Brandtstadter & Renner, 1988).
counts of depression that primarily center on perceived control One group of items indicated a tendency to tenaciously pursue
and learned helplessness, but they are consistent with an ex- goals even in the face of obstacles and under high risk of failure
tended dual-process conception of coping as proposed herein. (or, at the opposite pole, the tendency to give up readily). Ac-
Our focus so far has been on functional interdependencies cording to our formulation, this tendency corresponds to an as-
between assimilative and accommodative processes in actual similative mode of coping or control. A second group of items
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

episodes of coping. Which of these processes predominates in indicated a tendency to positively reinterpret initially aversive
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

concrete episodes of coping or at which point in the coping pro- situations and to relinquish blocked goal perspectives easily (or,
cess active, assimilative efforts are relinquished and accommo- at the opposite pole, difficulty or reluctance in withdrawing
dative changes in personal preferences and beliefs are induced emotionally from barren commitments). This tendency obvi-
should depend on situational parameters, such as perceived ously corresponds to an accommodative style. From both
controllability and personal valence of the critical events, as well groups of items, two scales were compiled, each consisting of
as on generalized behavioral tendencies that reflect differences 15 items that were selected on the basis of internal consistency
in prior learning history, competence, and personality traits. As considerations: Tenacious Goal Pursuit and Flexible Goal Ad-
will be shown later, assimilative and accommodative tendencies justment.2 The internal consistency of both scales was found
can be assessed as habitualized styles or tendencies of coping on to be satisfactory (flexibility, Cronbach's a = .83, item-total
a dispositional level. correlations ranged from .35 to .60; tenacity, Cronbach's a =
.80, item-total correlations ranged from .30 to .59). Flexibility
Construction of Scales and tenacity have low variance overlap. The pattern of corre-
lations with the reference variables that were included in this
On the basis of the theoretical premises outlined earlier, we preliminary study are shown in Table 1.
developed a questionnaire to assess assimilative and accommo- In spite of the fact that tenacity and flexibility are nearly inde-
dative tendencies on a dispositional level. In this section, we pendent, both scales showed significant negative correlations
briefly describe the construction of the scales, as well as their with both indexes of depression and positive correlations with
psychometric properties and relationships to conceptually re- life satisfaction and optimism. This correlational pattern is con-
lated scales. sistent with the coping theoretical interpretation of these scales.
In generating the item pool for a starting version, we consid- However, optimistic and positive attitudes toward life (as mea-
ered different facets of accommodative processes related to dis- sured by the Life Orientation Test) are more closely related to
engagement, reorientation, and acceptance (e.g., palliative com- flexibility than to tenacity. In contrast, tenacity is more closely
parisons, selective focusing on favorable effects, construction of related to the IPC scales of generalized control beliefs. Further-
meaning, compromising and rescaling of aspiration levels, and more, both scales are positively correlated with action orienta-
flexible disengagement from blocked goals). For discriminative tion after failure, with flexibility apparently having more over-
purposes, other items were included that referred to assimila- lap with the action orientation scale.
tive tendencies (e.g., maintaining a chosen course of action even Given the convergent correlational behavior of flexibility and
under difficulty, increasing the valence of blocked goal perspec- tenacity with regard to reference measures of depression, well-
tives). Originally, 73 items were formulated and administered being, and perceived control, it is remarkable that the two scales
in a preliminary study with a sample of 97 women and 109 men showed clearly opposite regressions on the age variable (flexi-
(age, M = 34.4 years, SD = 15.2 years; range = 18-81 years). bility, r = .34, p < .01; tenacity, r = -. 18, p < .01; the difference
To investigate convergent and discriminant aspects of validity, between these correlation coefficients was significant, p< .001).
the following reference variables were included in this pilot
study: (a) the Life Orientation Test, which is used to assess opti-
2
mistic attitudes toward life (Scheier & Carver, 1985); (b) a Ger- Examples of Tenacious Goal Pursuit Scale items: "The harder a goal
man adaptation of Levenson's (1972) scales of generalized con- is to achieve, the more desirable it often appeals to me," "I can be very
trol beliefs (internality, powerful others, and chance control obstinate in pursuing my goals," "Even if everything seems hopeless, I
[IPC] scales, Krampen, 1981); (c) a scale to measure life satis- still look for a way to master the situation," and "If I run into problems,
I usually double my efforts." Examples of Flexible Goal Adjustment
faction, comprising a selection of items from scales developed
Scale items: "In general, I am not upset very long about an opportunity
by Wiendieck (1970) and by Lohr and Walter (1974); (d) a scale passed up," "I can adapt quite easily to changes in a situation," "After
to assess Type A patterns of behavior, comprising a selection of a serious disappointment, I soon turn to new tasks," "I usually recog-
items from the Jenkins Activity Survey (Jenkins, Zyzanski, & nize quite easily my own limitations," and "Even if everything goes
Rosenman, 1971); (e) a subscale denoted Action Orientation wrong, I still can find something positive about the situation." The origi-
After Failure, from the Action-Control Scale developed by Kuhl nal version of the scales can be obtained from us on request.
62 JOCHEN BRANDTSTADTER AND GEROLF RENNER

From a developmental point of view, this is an intriguing result,


clearly suggesting an age-related shift from active and tenacious
rr to a more accepting, accommodative mode of coping. In the
following section, we provide some theoretical arguments in fa-
$^oofir~r<io,^<N<sooo
$* $ : $ vor of this assumption and present some corroborating research
f>' O O < N O < N O O results from a larger, panel study covering the age range from
'i rr 'r middle to later adulthood.

i $ f .! . Age Differences in Coping Strategies^From Tenacious


8 2 2 **
Goal Pursuit to Flexible Goal Adjustment
There is only sparse and inconclusive evidence about whether
and how strategies of coping and control change with age (cf.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Fblkman, Lazarus, Pimley, & Novacek, 1987; Lazarus &


r r ' -r
) CN ^ O >
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

DeLongis, 1983; McCrae, 1982; Saup, 1987). Nevertheless,


there are some vantage points for approaching this issue from
s:. 11 s a life-span developmental perspective. Several studies indicate
that elderly people to a greater degree than younger people per-
ceive their development as heteronomously controlled, that is,
as being dependent on factors they are unable to influence, al-
S . S S 8 S though a sense of personal control may be maintained (cf.
-r t- tr^ ^ os m oo
C*-
* p cs rn Brandtstadter, Krampen, & Baltes-Gotz, 1989; Lachman,
r i' f ' ' r ' 1986; Rodin, 1987). These findings converge with the com-
monly accepted view that uncontrollable and aversive events
*rs f >r>: \o* m. oo$ and life transitions cumulate in later stages of life (cf. Seligman
& Elder, 1986). On the other hand, research on life satisfaction
i
p ts r has not revealed any general or consistent age-related change
(for a review, see Diener, 1984, and Larson, 1978). We assume
$ $$$ . that these various pieces of evidence can be brought into a co-
jqS herent picture by assuming a gradual shift from assimilative-
rr offensive to accommodative coping strategies in middle and
later adulthood.
These considerations lend theoretical substance to the sig-
nificantly opposite regressions of tenacity and flexibility on age
that were observed in the preliminary study (Table 1). To cor-
roborate this result on a broader statistical basis, we included
.:, the scales in the third wave of a larger research project centering
fi t** en
on personal self-regulation of development in adulthood. In this
rrr project, perceptions, emotional evaluations, and control beliefs
related to different facets and goal dimensions of personal devel-
opment were assessed by a broad array of variables (for details
of the research approach and theoretical rationale, see also
Brandtstadter, 1989, and Brandtstadter et al., 1986). Partici-
pants were recruited from an urban area in southwestern Ger-
many via the local bureau of population statistics. The sampling
procedure corresponded to a cross-sequential design that com-
bines cross-sectional and longitudinal comparisons (e.g.,
Schaie, 1977). More than 1,200 subjects (married couples) were
sampled over the age range from 30 to 60 years within five age
cohorts. Because the age range spanned by the study covers the
critical life transitions from early to late middle age, it seemed
broad enough to reflect long-term developmental change in
adulthood. For the third wave (1987), from which these data
were obtained, age ranges and numbers of subjects within age
cohorts were as follows: Cohort 1, 34-39 years (n = 167); Co-
hort 2,40-45 years (n = 160); Cohort 3,46-51 years (n = 196);
Cohort 4, 52-57 years (n = 186); and Cohort 5, 58-63 years
1& (n = 181). According to educational level and income, most
ASSIMILATIVE AND ACCOMMODATIVE STRATEGIES 63

Perceptions of Personal Development as a Function of


Flexible Goal Adjustment and Tenacious Goal Pursuit
A further complex of findings from the larger panel study de-
scribed earlier highlights some interesting relationships of te-
nacity and flexibility with subjective appraisals of personal de-
velopment. As part of the general assessment procedure, sub-
jects were asked to rate the perceived distance from, as well as
the perceived change toward, 17 personally valued developmen-
tal goals (Table 2; these ratings were also obtained on two earlier
occasions of measurement, in 1983 and 1985). Perceived goal
distance was rated on 9-point, Likert-type scales. Retrospec-
tively perceived change toward developmental goals (with re-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

9 spect to the past 3 years of life) was assessed on bipolar 17-point


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

<f scales. As an indirect measure of perceived change toward goals,


we also considered intraindividual changes in distance ratings
over the 4-year interval between the first (1983) and third (1987)
Cohl Coh2 Coh3 Coh4 CohS
34-39 40-45 46-51 52-57 58-63 occasions of measurement (change scores were adjusted for au-
toregressive effects by partialling first from last measurements;
Figure 2. Tenacious goal pursuit by age and sex (solid circles = female; cf. Cohen & Cohen, 1983). Table 2 summarizes the findings.
open circles = male). (Age ranges for cohorts [Coh] refer to 1987 occa-
For the third occasion of measurement, flexibility and tenacity
sion.)
correlated negatively with perceived goal distance; that is, indi-
viduals scoring high on these scales reported lower developmen-
tal deficits. Furthermore, flexibility and tenacity were associ-
ated with perceived developmental gains, as assessed by retro-
subjects belonged to the middle class. The subjects participated spective ratings and residualized longitudinal changes in
on a voluntary basis, and respondents were paid. Because perceived goal distance. For some of the goal dimensions con-
married couples were recruited for participation, the gender sidered (empathy, harmonious partnership, and friendship),
variable was approximately equally distributed within co- these relationships appeared to be more pronounced for flexi-
horts. For the total sample, the intradyadic (or intraclass) cor- bility than for tenacity. Interestingly, retrospectively perceived
relations for the target variables tenacity and flexibility were developmental gains were generally more closely associated
nonsignificant (tenacity, r-K = .03; flexibility, ric = .09), which with flexibility than with tenacity in the retrospective mode
indicates the absence of homogamy effects with regard to these (differences between respective coefficients were significant,
variables. withp < .05 for 10 of 17 developmental goal dimensions).
The cross-sectional results for tenacity and flexibility ob- The negative relation of tenacity and flexibility to perceived
tained from the third wave (1987) confirmed our expectation
that the two constructs have opposite developmental gradients
within the age range studied. For tenacity, a nonorthogonal 5 X
2 (Cohort X Sex) analysis of variance yielded significant main 57

effects for cohort, F(4,865) = 2.92, p < .05, and sex, F( 1,865) =
14.91, p < .01. With flexibility as the dependent variable, the
effect related to cohort again was significant, F(4, 862) = 6.09,
p < .01, whereas sex differences did not reach significance. The
gradients are shown in Figures 2 and 3 (the linear correlations
with age were r = -.14, p < .01, for tenacity and r = .14, p <
.01, for flexibility; the difference was significant at the .01 level).
In this larger study, tenacity and flexibility were found again
to correlate negatively with depressive tendencies as assessed by
self-ratings and questionnaire measures of depression (flexibil-
ity and depression index, r = -.33, p< .01; flexibility and FPI- 51
depression, r = -.32, p < .01; tenacity and depression index, $
Cf
r - -.25, p < .01; tenacity and FPI-depression, r = -.29, p <
.01) and positively with life satisfaction (flexibility and life satis-
faction, r = .37, p < .01; tenacity and life satisfaction, r = .29, Cohl Coh 2 CohS Coh 4 CohS
p < .01). However, the scales differed significantly in their rela- 34-39 40-45 46-51 52-57 58-63
tion to perceived marital quality as measured by the Dyadic Figure 3. Flexible goal adjustment by age and sex (solid circles = female;
Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976): flexibility, r = .35, p < .01; open circles = male). (Age ranges for cohorts [Coh] refer to 1987 occa-
tenacity, r= .13,/><.01. sion.)
64 JOCHEN BRANDTSTADTER AND GEROLF RENNER

Table 2
Correlations of Flexible Goal Adjustment and Tenacious Goal Pursuit With Perceived Distance
From, and Change Toward, Developmental Goals
Flexible goal adjustment Tenacious goal pursuit

Distance Increase of Change Distance Increase of Change


Developmental goal from goal" distance1" toward goal0 from goal distance toward goal

Health -.10** -.08* .16** -.12** -.08* .11**


Emotional stability -.29** -.26** .34** -.21** -.19** .12**
Wisdom -.16** -.16** .32** -.11** -.10** .16**
Self-esteem -.15** -.15** .30** -.18** -.15** .20**
Social recognition -.07** -.05 .26** -.16** -.14** .18**
Occupational efficiency -.08* -.04 .17** -.20** -.21** .16**
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Assertiveness -.10** -.10** .26** -.27** -.21** .26**


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Harmonious partnership -.19** -.17** .30** -.10** -.08* .07*


Empathy -.20** -.19** .32** -.05 -.02 .07*
Personal independence -.11** -.10** .26** -.12** -.08* .10**
Family security -.09** -.09** .22** -.07* -.04 .08*
Prosperity -.03 -.03 .15** -.19** -.17** .15*
Intellectual efficiency -.11** -.07* .26** -.21** -.18** .22*
Self-development -.20** -.18** .29** -.22** -.19** .21*
Physical fitness -.11** -.07* .22** -.16** -.15** .13*
Satisfying friendship -.16** -.15** .29** -.08* -.15** .12*
Commitment to ideals -.13** -.11** .19** -.07* -.05 .10**

Note. N = 779-899 (pairwise deletion of missing data).


1
Ratings of actual distance from developmental goal. b Residualized change in distance ratings within a longitudinal interval of 4 years (1983-
1987). c Retrospectively perceived developmental change toward developmental goal.
*p<.Q5. **p<.01.

developmental deficits (Table 2) may account for the reported The plotted functions for these goals relate the conditional re-
positive association of these scales with various indicators of gressions of satisfaction on distance ratings (b^) to given values
subjective well-being (life satisfaction and low depression). Ac- of the moderator variable (flexible goal adjustment). Modera-
cording to our theoretical model, emotional strain resulting tion effects for the other goals were almost identical but were
from perceived developmental losses and deficits should be alle- not plotted, in the interest of graphic clarity.
viated by accommodative processes of coping as measured by In sum, persons scoring high on flexible goal adjustment not
the Flexible Goal Adjustment. For a further test of this assump- only report lower developmental deficits but also seem less vul-
tion, we considered the relationship between perceived distance nerable to dissatisfaction and emotional strain, which may ac-
from developmental goals and goal-specific ratings of satisfac- company such deficits. As we have shown elsewhere (Brandt-
tion with personal development (which were assessed together stadter, 1989; cf. Heckhausen et al., 1989), perceived develop-
with distance ratings on the same scaling format). Not surpris- mental deficits tend to increase through middle and later
ingly, this relationship was found to be consistently negative (the adulthood. The data presented in this study indicate that ac-
average correlation for 17 goals was -.43). We hypothesized commodative modes of coping, as measured by flexible goal ad-
that the supposed palliative function of flexibility should mani- justment, may help to maintain a positive and optimistic atti-
fest itself in a moderating effect on this relationship; more spe- tude toward life despite such adverse changes.
cifically, the negative regression of satisfaction ratings on per-
ceived goal distance should be less pronounced or even vanish Discussion
for higher levels of flexibility. This effect was evaluated in a hier-
archical regression format, in which the variables of perceived We have advanced the proposition that crises and problems
distance and flexible goal adjustment were entered first into the of personal development activate two processes of coping,
multiple regression function (with satisfaction ratings as the de- termed assimilative tendency (adjusting developmental situa-
pendent variable), followed by the interaction term (Distance X tions to personal preferences) and accommodative tendency (ad-
Flexibility) on the second step (cf. Cohen & Cohen, 1983). justing personal preferences to situational constraints). These
For seven goal dimensions (social recognition, occupational processes are considered as distinct but interrelated strategies
efficiency, harmonious partnership, empathy, family security, for achieving consistency between actual and desired develop-
prosperity, and self-esteem), the predicted moderation effect mental options and thus for maintaining a satisfying perspective
reached significance. Figure 4 shows the results for family secu- on personal development. This formulation has implications for
rity, Fchange(3,813) = 4.64, p < .05; social recognition, jFCtange(3, understanding depressive reactions that often accompany the
813) = 5.21, p < .05; occupational efficiency, Fctonge(3, 813) = experience of irreversible changes and developmental losses in
4.61, p < .05; and prosperity, Fctanisl(3,813) = 13.06, p < .001. middle and later adulthood. More specifically, we assume that
ASSIMILATIVE AND ACCOMMODATIVE STRATEGIES 65

cies (defense of God's goodness in view of the existence of evil;


see particularly Leibniz, 1710). In this conceptual tradition, ac-
commodative reappraisals involving a search for hidden posi-
tive aspects of apparently unfortunate accidents appear as a
constitutive element of optimism (cf. Marquard, 1983), which
may be even more important than the presumed relation of op-
timism to generalized outcome expectancies stressed by Scheier
and Carver (1985).
Obviously, the reported relationships of tenacity and flexibil-
ity to measures of personal well-being are mediated by differ-
ences in the cognitive and emotional appraisal of personal de-
velopmental prospects that were found to be associated with
these constructs. Most noticeable in this respect is the buffering
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

effect of flexible goal adjustment on the impact of perceived


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Flexible Goal Adjustment developmental deficits on dissatisfaction with personal develop-


(standard deviation units)
ment, which indicates a lower vulnerability of flexible individu-
Figure 4. Moderating effect of flexible goal adjustment on the regression als to developmental stress. This is in line with our theoretical
of satisfaction on perceived goal distance, for different developmental formulations.
goals (1 = family security, 2 = social recognition, 3 = occupational A central concern in this study was the investigation of age-
efficiency, 4 = prosperity). (The plotted functions relate the conditional related differences in the Flexible Goal Adjustment and Tena-
regression weights ba [FLEX = x] to chosen values of the moderator cious Goal Pursuit scales. In cross-sectional comparisons, flex-
variable.)
ibility shows a clear-cut increase over the age range considered,
whereas tenacity exhibits an opposite regression on the age vari-
able. These findings need to be corroborated by longitudinal
onset, duration, and severity of depressive episodes depend not assessments that extend beyond the age range considered in this
only on the degree of perceived control over personally impor- study. We have advanced theoretical arguments about why the
tant developmental domains (as postulated by learned helpless- observed cross-sectional differences should reflect a genuine on-
ness theories) but as well on the ability or willingness to disen- togenetic age-graded trend that is not limited to middle adult-
gage from unfeasible goals and to build up new commitments hood. The gist of our argument was that a growing impact of
and developmental perspectives (cf. Klinger, 1975, 1977). The uncontrollable events on personal development favors an in-
distinction between accommodative and assimilative modes of creasing dominance of accommodative over assimilative modes
coping in fact points to possible hidden costs of perceived con- of coping (cf. Clark & Anderson, 1967; Folkman et al., 1987)
trol that may easily be overlooked from a learned helplessness and that this shift helps the aging individual to maintain a sense
perspective: Because shifting from an assimilative to an accom- of well-being and satisfaction.
modative mode of coping depends on the perceived modifiabil- The analysis of this coping pattern may be a promising van-
ity of aversive situations, and thus on the strength of situation- tage point for a theoretical explication of the wisdom concept
specific control beliefs, the necessary process of adjusting life that recently has drawn increasing attention in developmental
designs and developmental projects in the face of factually unal- psychology and gerontology (cf. Clayton, 1982; Dittmann-
terable or irreversible events may be delayed in individuals with Kohli, 1984). In sum, our research findings indicate that the
a strong sense of personal control (cf. the concept of a "pathol- emphasis on active-offensive modes of coping, which pervades
ogy of high expectations" proposed by JanofF-Bulman & Brick- psychological formulations of optimal development and suc-
man, 1982). cessful aging, may underrate the importance of accommodative
We have assessed interindividual differences in assimilative processes for coping with typical problems of aging.
and accommodative styles of coping by two independent scales,
denoted as Tenacious Goal Pursuit and Flexible Goal Adjust-
ment. The relations of tenacity and flexibility to various refer-
References
ence variables are conceptually consistent. The scales show a Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental
roughly similar pattern of external correlations with various in- psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Develop-
dicators of optimism and personal well-being. The substantial mental Psychology, 23, 611-626.
correlations with action orientation after failure and optimism, Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behav-
which were obtained in the preliminary validational study, de- ioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
serve further comment. The variance overlap between flexibil- Brandtstadter, J. (1986). Personal and social control over development:
Some implications of an action perspective in life-span developmental
ity and action orientation after failure makes sense insofar as
psychology. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.), Life-span develop-
low action orientation (high state orientation) is described as ment and behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 1-32). New 'Yfork: Academic Press.
involving a clinging to, and continual rumination about, un- Brandtstadter, J. (1989). Personal self-regulation of development: Cross-
reachable goals (cf. Kuhl, 1985,1987). In regard to the correla- sequential analyses of development-related control beliefs and emo-
tion between flexibility and optimism, note that the concept of tions. Developmental Psychology, 25, 96-108.
optimism dates back to the philosophical-theological theodi- Brandtstadter, J., & Baltes-Gotz, B. (in press). Personal control over de-
66 JOCHEN BRANDTSTADTER AND GEROLF RENNER

velopment and quality of life perspectives in adulthood. In P. B. Baltes Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement
& M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behav- from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1-25.
ioral sciences. New "York: Cambridge University Press. Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incen-
Brandtstadter, J., Krampen, G., & Baltes-Gotz, B. (1989). Kontrolliiber- tives in people's lives. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
zeugungen im Kontext personlicher Entwicklung [Control beliefs re- Krampen, G. (1981). IPC-Fragebogen zu KontroUuberzeugungen
lated to personal development]. In G. Krampen (Ed.), Diagnostik von [IPCQuestionnaire on control beliefs]. Gottingen, Federal Repub-
Attributionen und Kontmlliiberzeugungen (pp. 155-171). Gottingen, lic of Germany: Hogrefe.
Federal Republic of Germany: Hogrefe. Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional mediators of cognition-behavior consistency:
Brandtstadter, J., Krampen, G., & Heil, F. E. (1986). Personal control Self-regulatory processes and action versus state orientation. In J.
and emotional evaluation of development in partnership relations Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action control: From cognition to behav-
during adulthood. In M. M. Baltes & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), The psychol- ior(pp. 101-128). Berlin, Federal Republic of Germany: Springer.
ogy of aging and control (pp. 265-296). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kuhl, J. (1987). Action control: The maintenance of motivational
Brandtstadter, J., & Rentier, G. (1988). Hartnackige Zielverfolgung und states. In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, intention and voli-
Flexible Zielanpassung [Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal ad- tion (pp. 279-291). Berlin, Federal Republic of Germany: Springer.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

justment]. Universitat Trier: Berichte aus der Arbeitsgruppe "En- Lachman, M. E. (1986). Personal control in later life: Stability, change,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

twicklungundHandeln"(No. 28). Trier Universita'tsdruck. and cognitive correlates. In M. M. Baltes & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), The
Breakwell, G. M. (1986). Coping with threatened identities. London: psychology of control and aging (pp. 207-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
Methuen. baum.
Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. (1976). The quality of Larson, R. (1978). Thirty years of research on the subjective well-being
American life. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. of alder Americans. Journal of Gerontology, 33, 109-125.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: Lazarus, R. S., & DeLongis, A. (1983). Psychological stress and coping
A control-theory approach to human behavior. New \fark: Springer. in aging. American Psychologist, 38,245-254.
Clark, M., & Anderson, B. G. (1967). Culture and aging: An anthropo- Lazarus, R. S., & Golden, G. Y. (1981). The function of denial in stress,
logical study of older Americans. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. coping, and aging. In J. L. McGaugh & S. B. Kiesler (Eds.), Aging,
Clayton, V. (1982). Wisdom and intelligence: The nature and function biology and behavior (pp. 283-307). New \brk: Academic Press.
of knowledge in the later years. International Journal of Aging and Lazarus, R. S., & Launier, R. (1978). Stress-related transactions be-
Development, 15, 315-323. tween person and environment. In L. A. Pervin & M. Lewis (Eds.),
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation Perspectives in interactional psychology (pp. 287-327). New \brk:
analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Plenum Press.
Coyne, J. C, Aldwin, C, & Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Depression and cop- Leibniz, G. W. (1710). Essais de theodicee sur la bonte de Dieu, la li-
ing in stressful episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 439- berte de I'homme et I'origine du mal [Essays of theodicy on God's
447. goodness, the freedom of man, and the origin of evil]. Amsterdam:
Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, Isaac Troyel.
542-575. Levenson, H. (1972). Distinctions within the concept of internal-exter-
Dittmann-Kohli, F. (1984). Weisheit als mogliches Ergebnis der Intelli- nal control: Development of a new scale. Proceedings of the 80th An-
genzentwicklung im Erwachsenenalter [Wisdom as a potential out- nual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 7, 261-
come of the development of intelligence in adulthood]. Sprache & 262.
Kognition,2, 112-132. Ldhr, G., & Walter, A. (1974). Die LZ-Skala: Zur Erfassung der subjek-
Elster, J. (1983). Sour grapes: Studies in the subversion of rationality. tiven Lebenszufriedenheit im Alter [The LZ-Scale: Assessment of
London: Cambridge University Press. subjective life satisfaction in old age]. Diagnostica, 20, 83-91.
Fahrenberg, J., Hampel, R., & Selg, H. (1973). Freiburger Personlich- Marquard, O. (1983). Entlastungen: Theodizeemotive in der neuzeit-
keits-fnventar [Freiburg Personality Inventory] (F-P-J) 2nd ed. Got- lichen Philosophic [Exonerations: Motives of theodicy in modern
tingen, Federal Republic of Germany: Hogrefe. philosophy]. In P. Wapnewski (Ed.), Wissenschaftskdleg: Jahrbuch
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Pimley, S., & Novacek, J. (1987). Age 1982/83 (pp. 245-258). Berlin, Federal Republic of Germany:
differences in stress and coping processes. Psychology and Aging, 2, Siedler.
171-184. McCrae, R. R. (1982). Age differences in the use of coping mechanisms.
Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Gains and losses Journal of Gerontology, 37,454-460.
in development throughout adulthood as perceived by different adult Moos, R. H., & Billings, A. G. (1982). Conceptualizing and measuring
age groups. Developmental Psychology, 29, 109-121. coping resources and processes. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.),
Hofstatter, P. R. (1986). Bedingungen der Zufriedenheit [Determinants Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (pp. 212-230).
of satisfaction]. Zurich, Switzerland: Interfrom. New York: Free Press.
Janoff-Bulman, R., & Brickman, P. (1982). Expectations and what peo- Neugarten, B. L. (1973). Personality change in late life: A developmen-
ple learn from failure. In N. T. Feather (Ed.), Expectations and ac- tal perspective. In C. Eisdorfer & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology
tions: Expectancy-value models in psychology (pp. 207-237). Hills- of adult development and aging (pp. 311-335). Washington, DC:
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. American Psychological Association.
Jenkins, C. D., Zyzanski, S. J., & Rosenman, R. H. (1971). Progress Pearlin, L. J. (1980). Life strains and psychological distress among
toward validation of a computer-scored test for the type A coronary adults. In N. Smelser & E. H. Erikson (Eds.), Themes of work and
prone behavior pattern. Psychosomatic Medicine, 33, 193-202. love in adulthood (pp. 174-192). London: Grant Mclntyre.
Kamlah, W. (1973). Philosophische Anthropologie: Sprachkritische Perloff, L. (1987). Social comparison and illusions of invulnerability to
Grundlegung und Ethik [Philosophical anthropology: Critical foun- negative life events. In C. R. Snyder & C. E. Ford (Eds.), Coping with
dations and ethics]. Mannheim, Federal Republic of Germany: BI negative life-events (pp. 217-242). New York: Plenum Press.
Taschenbiicher. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk
ASSIMILATIVE AND ACCOMMODATIVE STRATEGIES 67

factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91, Seligman, M. E. P., & Elder, G. H. (1986). Learned helplessness and life-
347-374. span development. In A. B. Serensen, F. E. Weinert, & L. R. Sherrod
Rodin, J. (1987). Personal control through the life course. In R. P. (Eds.), Human development and the life course: Multidisciplinary per-
Abeles (Ed.), Life-span perspectives and social psychology (pp. 103- spectives (pp. 377-428). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
119). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for
Rothbaum, R, Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing the world assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Mar-
and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. riage and the Family, 38, 15-28.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 5-37. Tatarkiewicz, W. (1984). Analysis of happiness. The Hague, The Neth-
Rotter, J. B. (1975). Some problems and misconceptions related to the erlands: Martinus Nijhoff.
construct of internal vs. external control of reinforcement. Journal Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events. American Psy-
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 56-67. chologist, 38, 1161-1173.
Saup, W. (1987). Coping im Alter. Ergebnisse und Probleme psyeholo- Thompson, S. (1985). Finding positive meaning in a stressful event and
gischer Studien zum Bewaltigungsverhalten alterer Menschen [Cop- coping. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 6, 279-295.
ing in old age]. ZeitschriftfurGerontologie, 20, 345-354. Wiendieck, G. (1970). Entwicklung einer Skala zur Messung der
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Schaie, K. W. (1977). Quasi-experimental research designs in the psy- Lebenszufriedenheit im hdheren Erwachsenenalter [Construction of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

chology of aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of a scale for the assessment of life satisfaction in late adulthood]. Zeit-
the psychology of aging (pp. 39-58). New York: Van Nostrand Rein- schriftjur Gerontologie, 3, 215-224.
hold.
Scheier, M. E, & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping and health:
Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies.
Health Psychology, 4, 219-247. Received November 8,1988
Schopenhauer, A. (1851). Parerga und Paralipomena: kleine philosoph- Revision received May 12,1989
ische Schriften. Berlin, Germany: A. W. Hayn. Accepted May 19,1989

Low Publication Prices for APA Members and Affiliates


Keeping: You Up-to-Date: All APA members (Fellows; Members; and Associates, and Student Affiliates)
receive-as part of their annual dues-subscriptions to the American Psychologist and the APA Monitor,
High School Teacher and Foreign Affiliates receive subscriptions to the APA Monitor and they can subscribe
to the American Psychologist at a significantly reduced rate.
In addition, all members and affiliates are eligible for savings of up to 50% on other APA journals, as well as
significant discounts on subscriptions from cooperating societies and publishers (e.g., the British Psychological
Society, the American Sociological Association, and Human Sciences Press).
Essential Resources: APA members and affiliates receive special rates for purchases of APA books,
including the Publication Manual of the APA, the Master Lectures, and APA's Guide to Research Support.
Other Benefits of Membership: Membership in APA also provides eligibility for low-cost insurance
plans covering life; medical and income protection; hospital indemnity; accident and travel; Keogh retirement;
office overhead; and student/school, professional, and liability.
For more information, write to American Psychological Association, Membership Services,
1200 Seventeenth Street NW, Washington, DC 20036, USA.

Potrebbero piacerti anche