Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 2016 American Psychological Association

2016, Vol. 30, No. 3, 345355 0893-164X/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/adb0000179

Emerging Adult Identity Development, Alcohol Use, and Alcohol-Related


Problems During the Transition Out of College

Jonathan R. Gates and William R. Corbin Kim Fromme


Arizona State University The University of Texas at Austin

Alcohol use generally peaks during the early 20s and declines with age. These declines, referred to as
maturing out, are presumed to result from the acquisition of adult roles (e.g., marriage, employment)
incompatible with alcohol use. Recent empirical evidence suggests that variables other than role
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

transitions (e.g., personality) may also be important in understanding this process. Changes in identity
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

that occur during emerging adulthood may also be linked to the process of maturing out of heavy
drinking, though no studies have yet addressed this possibility. Utilizing data from a large sample of
graduating college students (N 907) during senior year (Wave 1) and the 2 following years (Waves
23), the current study examined relations between aspects of emerging adult identity and drinking
outcomes (alcohol use and problems). Using time-varying covariate growth models, results indicated that
several facets of emerging adult identity conferred risk for the failure to mature out of heavy drinking and
alcohol-related problems. Experimentation/possibilities emerged as a significant risk factor for both
heavy drinking and alcohol problems, but these effects diminished considerably when accounting for
personality risk. In contrast, although small in magnitude, effects of self-focus on heavy drinking and
negativity/instability on alcohol-related problems were relatively independent of effects of other estab-
lished predictors. The effect for negativity/instability was evident only at the final wave. The findings
have important implications for theories of maturing out and may ultimately inform tailoring or
refinement of prevention/intervention approaches for emerging adults.

Keywords: emerging adulthood, alcohol, college, identity development, longitudinal

Despite widespread prevention efforts, alcohol remains a large Although a high level of alcohol use occurs during college, use
part of collegiate life and a major public health concern (Sher, generally peaks during the early twenties and declines with age
Bartholow, & Nanda, 2001; Slutske, 2005; Wechsler et al., 2002). (Jochman & Fromme, 2010). Several factors appear to facilitate
Evidence suggests that aspects of the college environment contrib- this maturing out process (e.g., marriage, parenthood), but rel-
ute to high rates of alcohol use. For example, the rate of binge atively little is known about these normative reductions in use
drinking (five or more drinks during a single occasion) among relative to the substantial literature on factors contributing to
college students in 2013 was 35% (Johnston, OMalley, Bachman, increases in drinking during the transition from adolescence into
Schulenberg, & Miech, 2014). Moreover, Wechsler and Nelson emerging adulthood. Furthermore, despite normative reductions in
(2008) demonstrated that students who binge drink are at higher alcohol use with age, a significant minority of young adults fail to
risk for alcohol-related problems including unplanned and unpro- mature out of this pattern of heavy drinking and go on to experi-
tected sexual activity, injuries, and problems with campus or local ence clinically significant problems (Jackson, Sher, Gotham, &
police. Slutske (2005) also found that college students were more Wood, 2001). Thus, it is important to identify factors that might
likely to be diagnosed with an alcohol use disorder (abuse and facilitate the maturing out process. As such, the aim of this study
dependence) relative to nonstudents. was to examine dimensions of emerging adult identity during the
transition out of college in relation to alcohol outcomes (use and
related problems).
This article was published Online First April 14, 2016. Evidence suggests that maturing out of heavy drinking is, at
Jonathan R. Gates and William R. Corbin, Department of Psychology, least in part, a result of the acquisition of adult roles and respon-
Arizona State University; Kim Fromme, Department of Psychology, The sibilities such as marriage and/or parenthood, end of formal edu-
University of Texas at Austin. cation and start of employment, change in living arrangements,
This project was supported by award number R01AA013967 from the and financial independence (Bachman et al., 2002; Eitle, Taylor, &
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). The Eitle, 2010; Fleming, White, & Catalano, 2010; Kretsch & Harden,
NIAAA played no role in manuscript writing, preparation, or interpretation
2014; Staff, Greene, Maggs, & Schoon, 2014). These relations
of the results. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and
may be a result of both role selection and role socialization. In
does not necessarily represent the views of the NIAAA or the National
Institutes of Health. support of the importance of role selection, Yamaguchi and Kandel
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to William (1985) found that marijuana users were more likely to postpone
R. Corbin, Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, 950 S marriage due to incompatible roles of marriage and substance use.
McAllister Avenue, Tempe, AZ 85281. E-mail: wcorbin@asu.edu Conversely, and consistent with role socialization, Gotham, Sher,

345
346 GATES, CORBIN, AND FROMME

and Wood (2003) found that being married reduced the likelihood Literature on identity development may also inform our under-
of a later Alcohol Use Disorder. Labouvie (1996) examined both standing of the process of maturing out of heavy drinking. Erik-
role selection and role socialization and found evidence supporting sons seminal work (Erikson, 1968) suggests that identity explo-
both mechanisms. Those who were married or became married and ration (identity vs. identity confusion) is a key aspect of adolescent
those who were parents or became parents had more friends who development, and alcohol and other drug use is a common way in
were married or parents at a later time point. At the same time, which adolescents seek to explore their identities and appear more
those who married or had children had lower levels of alcohol use adult. The tendency for exploration may also be related to
at later time points. Further, Lee, Chassin, and MacKinnon (2015) personality maturation (e.g., increases in sensation seeking) that
found that those with heavier drinking patterns premarriage had occurs during this period (Schwartz, Donnellan, Ravert, Luyckx,
the greatest decreases after marriage reflecting role-related matur- & Zamboanga, 2013). Ultimately, young people must decide
ing out processes. which behaviors to incorporate and which to move beyond (e.g.,
Employment has also been identified as a role that may be substance use) in establishing their identity. Moving beyond ado-
associated with maturing out, but research on this topic has pro- lescence, Erikson suggests that identity development shifts to a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

vided less consistent evidence relative to research on marriage and focus on intimacy versus isolation, a period during which individ-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

parenthood (Gotham et al., 2003). For example, Bachman, Wads- uals seek to develop long-term relationships. Thus, Eriksons
worth, OMalley, Johnston, and Schulenberg (1997) actually found theory also has clear implications for selection of roles that con-
small increases in alcohol use in the last 30 days among full-time tribute to the maturing out process.
employees, though this group did report small (though not signif- Marcia (1966) extended the work of Erikson in his ego identity
icant) decreases in levels of binge drinking over time. Vergs et al. status paradigm, which facilitated empirical evaluation of Erik-
(2012) found dynamic effects of employment status on risk for sons theory. Based on a combination of levels of exploration and
alcohol dependence over time. They found that becoming em- commitment, Marcia created four identity statuses; Achievement
ployed reduced the likelihood of a continued diagnosis of alcohol (high exploration, high commitment), Moratorium (high explora-
dependence among older but not younger adult males, whereas tion, low commitment), Foreclosure (low exploration, high com-
becoming employed was predictive of the emergence of alcohol mitment), and Diffusion (low exploration, low commitment).
Thus, he proposed that one must both explore possible identities
dependence for those without a previous diagnosis of alcohol
and ultimately commit to beliefs, values, and long-term goals to
dependence. In summarizing the overall pattern of findings regard-
successfully achieve desired outcomes. Supporting the potential
ing role transitions in their study, Vergs et al. (2012) concluded
link between identity development and substance use, recent stud-
that role transitions are associated with the course of alcohol
ies by Bishop, Weisgram, Holleque, Lund, and Wheeler-Anderson
disorders throughout the life course but that the effects differ as a
(2005) and Schwartz et al. (2011) found that individuals charac-
function of age, sex, and type of transition (p. 10).
terized by Marcias achievement identity status engaged in less
Although selection and socialization into adult roles is a key
substance use.
component to maturing out of heavy drinking, there is evidence
Recent influential theoretical work by Arnett (2000) builds upon
that the natural process of personality maturation may also play a
earlier identity theories and argues for a new and distinct devel-
unique role (Ashenhurst, Harden, Corbin, & Fromme, 2015; Little-
opmental period that has emerged from societal level changes.
field, Sher, & Wood, 2009, 2010a, 2010b; Steinberg et al., 2008; Arnett termed this period emerging adulthood, identifying young
Quinn & Harden, 2013). For example, impulsivity and sensation people between the ages of 18 25, though for some, this period
seeking increase during childhood and early to middle adoles- may extend through the twenties (Arnett, 2005). Arnett argues that
cence, respectively (Steinberg et al., 2008), and contribute to delays in marriage, parenthood, and other adult roles in industri-
experimentation with alcohol and other drugs. While impulsivity alized societies has extended the transition to adulthood, creating
steadily decreases starting around age 10, sensation seeking peaks a period characterized by change and exploration (Arnett, 2005,
in mid-adolescence and then declines into adulthood (Steinberg et p. 479). Consistent with this model, over the past 50 years, the
al., 2008). Recent studies suggest that changes in personality are median age of marriage has increased by 5 years to age 27 for men
linked with changes in drinking behavior. For example, Quinn and and 25 for women, while simultaneously extending the age of first
Harden (2013) found that slower decreases in impulsivity were parenthood from the early twenties to the late twenties. Given that
related to larger increases in alcohol and substance use, while the period of emerging adulthood corresponds with peak risk for
slower decreases in sensation seeking were uniquely related to heavy drinking and related problems, Arnetts theory may be of
increases in alcohol use. In another longitudinal study, Littlefield particular relevance to understanding the phenomenon of maturing
et al. (2009) examined relations between personality traits of out. In describing the period of emerging adulthood, Arnett pro-
impulsivity, neuroticism, and extraversion and problematic alcohol posed five dimensions of emerging adult identity (Arnett, 2005,
involvement between the ages of 18 and 35. They found that 2006). A brief description of each dimension and its relation to
decreases in impulsivity and neuroticism were associated with earlier theories of identity development is provided below.
decreases in problematic alcohol use, even after accounting for Identity exploration constitutes the extent to which an individual
effects of marriage and parenthood. In a recent study that exam- explores various feelings or experiences before reaching adult-
ined effects of both role socialization and personality, there was hood. Arnett (2006) explains that these experiences are the basis
evidence that effects depended upon the developmental period, for life decisions. This dimension of Arnetts theory is closely
with stronger effects of role transitions in early young adulthood aligned with the focus on identity exploration in both Eriksons
and stronger effects of personality in later young adulthood (Lee, and Marcias models of identity development. Arnett (2005) also
Ellingson, & Sher, 2015). considers emerging adulthood a time of experimentation/possibil-
EMERGING ADULT IDENTITY AND ALCOHOL USE 347

ities, when individuals have the liberty to make extensive changes found significant bivariate correlations between feeling in-
in their lives. Although similar to identity exploration in some between and substance use frequency and between negativity/
ways, Arnett clearly identifies this dimension of identity as being instability and substance related problems. Finally, Lisha et al.
of particular relevance to the period of emerging adulthood. (2014) found significant relations between a measure compris-
Whereas adolescents and children have limited choice in the ing items from experimentation and self-focus dimensions and
manner in which they live, emerging adults can generally live the cigarette, alcohol, and marijuana use. In the one longitudinal
way they see fit. Thus, they are able to explore a variety of study we were able to identify, Little et al. (2013) found that
behaviors without impediment, including high-risk behaviors like lower levels of experimentation were associated with greater
substance use. likelihood of marijuana cessation 1 year later. However, when
The dimension of negativity/instability involves the extent to controlling for baseline levels of marijuana use this effect
which young people are able to maintain confidence amid the became marginally significant, suggesting that the effect was
many changes that occur during emerging adulthood. Geographic due in part to the relation between lower experimentation and
instability is common during this period with rates of moving
less marijuana use at baseline. Although these studies provide
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

increasing rapidly beginning at age 18 and peaking in the mid-


preliminary evidence for the importance of emerging adult identity
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

twenties. For many, emerging adulthood also marks the first time
in relation to substance use, the average ages in these studies
they are able to completely govern themselves (e.g., live on their
ranged from 17 to 21 and none of the studies explored potential
own). Although Erikson identified negativity and instability that
age-related differences in effects as participants moved further into
can occur when young people fail to successfully navigate critical
stages of identity development, Arnett suggests that these experi- emerging adulthood. Given evidence for important age-related
ences are a normative part of identity development during this differences in effects of role transitions (Vergs et al., 2012) and
critical period. the relative influences of role transitions and personality matura-
Arnett also characterizes emerging adulthood as a period of tion (Lee et al., 2015), this is an important gap in the literature.
freedom and self-focus. Ultimately, emerging adults must move Thus, the current study sought to build upon prior work by
from self-focus to other focus as they take on more adult social examining relations between dimensions of emerging adult iden-
roles like marriage and full-time employment. This shift is char- tity and both heavy drinking and related problems using three
acteristic of Eriksons stage of intimacy versus isolation and aligns waves of longitudinal data collected in the fall of the fourth year of
with Marcias ideas about the importance of commitment. Finally, college (22 years of age) and the two years following (2324
Arnett suggests that emerging adults report feeling as if they are years of age). To isolate unique influences of emerging adult
not quite an adult, but not quite an adolescent, a dimension of identity, we controlled for effects of personality (sensation seeking
identify development referred to as feeling in-between. Arnett and impulsivity) and role transitions (e.g., entry into committed
(2000) demonstrated that the qualities most critical to these feel- relationships and employment). Although there is likely a trait
ings involve challenges related to accepting responsibility for component to emerging adult identity (e.g., some individuals are
ones self, making independent decisions, and becoming finan- likely to be higher than others in experimentation or negativity
cially independent (pp. 472 473). The concept of being caught in across stages of development), we were most interested in under-
between adolescence and adulthood is perhaps the most unique to standing the extent to which within-person variation (e.g., state
Arnetts theory although it clearly captures the struggle to commit aspects of emerging adult identity across time) relate to drinking
to adult roles. behavior and problems. Thus, we examined emerging adult iden-
In summary, Arnetts theories build from prior models of iden- tity dimensions as predictors of deviations from expected age-
tity development while extending them in important ways. Perhaps related declines in heavy drinking and problems. Based on results
the most novel aspects involve the ideas of extended adolescence of prior studies, we hypothesized that higher levels of experimen-
and the feeling of being caught between two important periods of tation/possibilities, self-focus, and feeling in-between would be
development. Further, while prior theories tended to focus on one
related to heavier alcohol use and that higher levels of negativity/
or two critical tasks for successful movement through stages of
instability would be related to more alcohol-related problems.
identity development, Arnett characterized multiple dimensions
Based on the finding by Lee et al. (2015) that intrapersonal
along which individuals might vary during the period of emerging
influences become more important than contextual influences as
adulthood without placing special emphasis on particular accom-
individuals move further into young adulthood, we also expected
plishments for successfully navigating this developmental transi-
tion. Given high levels of alcohol use and abuse during emerging effects of emerging adult identity to become stronger as individ-
adulthood and conceptual links between emerging adult identity uals moved toward the end of the period of emerging adulthood.
and other key determinants of maturation out of heavy drinking For example, high levels of experimentation may be less normative
(e.g., personality maturation, adult role adoption), the goal of the and therefore more indicative of a pathological process for a
current study was to examine relations between dimensions of 24-year-old who is more than a year beyond graduation than for a
emerging adult identity and alcohol-related outcomes. 22-year-old who is still enrolled in college. Although we had
Recent studies support the idea that Arnetts dimensions of specific hypotheses about only three of the five dimensions of
emerging adult identity may be important in understanding risk emerging adult identity for heavy drinking and one of five for
for substance use. For example, Allem, Lisha, Soto, Baezconde- alcohol-related problems, we examined all five dimensions as
Garbanati, and Unger (2013) found a marginally significant predictors of heavy drinking and problems given the lack of prior
relation between experimentation and past-month marijuana longitudinal studies controlling for other known predictors of
use. In another study, Smith, Bahar, Cleeland, and Davis (2014) maturing out.
348 GATES, CORBIN, AND FROMME

Method Table 1
Descriptive Statistics
Participants and Procedures Variable N M or % (SD) Range
The UT Experience! originally recruited all incoming first- Gender
time freshman at The University of Texas (UT) at Austin in the Male 316 34.80
summer before matriculation. Data for the current analyses come Female 591 65.20
Age at Wave 1 907 21.76 (.35) 20.5323.24
from participants who were assigned to a longitudinal condition
Relationship statusNot in relationship
with 10 waves of data collection from summer prior to college Wave 1 400 48.7
entry until 2 years after senior year (N 2,245; 60% female). Wave 2 298 47.2
Eligible participants were unmarried, first-time college students Wave 3 260 41.8
between the ages of 17 and 19. Of the total sample, 53.9% were Relationship statusIn a committed
relationship
Caucasian, 18.4% Asian American or Hawaiian Pacific Islander, Wave 1 385 46.9
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

15.2% Hispanic/Latino, 4.1% African American, .1% American Wave 2 267 42.2
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Indian/Alaska Native, 6.7% multiethnic, and 1.6% declined to Wave 3 266 42.8
report race/ethnicity. For more detailed information about recruit- Relationship statusMarried or
engaged
ment, see Hatzenbuehler, Corbin, and Fromme (2008).
Wave 1 36 4.4
Of the 2,245 participants who completed the initial survey, a Wave 2 67 10.6
total of 1,857 completed one of the three surveys on which the Wave 3 96 15.4
current analyses were based (senior year of high school and the fall Employment statusUnemployed
of the first and second years after graduation). These represented Wave 1 349 38.6
Wave 2 111 16.1
Waves 8 10 of the original study but we refer to them as Waves Wave 3 107 16.3
13 in the remainder of the paper given that Waves 17 of the Employment statusPart-time
original study were not included in the analyses. The sample was Wave 1 527 58.3
further restricted to those who graduated in 4 years to allow for Wave 2 148 21.5
Wave 3 78 11.9
examination of the transition out of college. Of the 1,857 partici-
Employment statusFull-time
pants who completed at least one survey across these three time Wave 1 28 3.1
points, 907 (48.9%) met inclusion criteria of completing college in Wave 2 429 62.4
4 years (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics and Table 2 for Wave 3 472 71.8
correlations among all study variables). In order to be counted as IDEAExperimentation
Wave 8 862 3.10 (.68) 14
4-year graduates, participants had to report that they were gradu- Wave 9 664 3.00 (.71) 14
ating in their fourth year (Wave 1) and confirm that they graduated Wave 10 635 2.89 (.81) 14
at Wave 2 (1 year later). Thus, 48.9% is likely an underestimate of IDEASelf-focus
the true graduation rate as those who indicated that they were Wave 8 863 3.37 (.57) 14
Wave 9 663 3.37 (.68) 14
graduating at Wave 1, but did not complete the survey at Wave 2
Wave 10 635 3.39 (.64) 14
were not counted as 4-year graduates. The average age in Year 4 IDEANegativity/Instability
of college for the final sample of 907 was 21.76 (SD .35), and Wave 8 864 3.09 (.71) 14
the average age 2 years later was 23.76 (SD .34). Wave 9 665 2.83 (.84) 14
Demographics. Basic demographics variables included gen- Wave 10 634 2.82 (.83) 14
IDEAIdentity exploration
der, ethnicity/race, and age. Wave 8 862 3.40 (.65) 14
Wave 9 662 3.24 (.76) 14
Wave 10 635 3.21 (.76) 14
Predictor Variables IDEAFeeling in-between
Dimensions of emerging adult identity. Emerging adulthood Wave 8 861 3.30 (.64) 14
Wave 9 660 3.14 (.77) 14
identity was assessed using the Inventory of the Dimensions of Wave 10 633 3.04 (.82) 14
Emerging Adulthood (IDEA; Reifman, Arnett, & Colwell, 2007). Binge drinking
The inventory includes 31 items assessing five dimensions includ- Wave 8 905 4.25 (6.79) 054
ing experimentation/possibilities, self-focus, negativity/instability, Wave 9 689 3.58 (6.58) 050
Wave 10 654 3.09 (5.37) 045
identity exploration, and feeling in-between. A sixth dimension
Drunk
(other-focused), serves as a counterpoint to self-focus but was not Wave 8 905 3.75 (6.28) 060
included in the current study (Reifman et al., 2007). All items are Wave 9 689 2.92 (5.47) 060
measured on a 4-point Likert-type scale with response options Wave 10 653 2.78 (5.01) 047
ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. In the current Alcohol-related problems
Wave 8 892 2.84 (5.55) 054
study, 15 of the 31 original items were included to capture the five Wave 9 682 1.80 (3.85) 031
original dimensions using the three highest loading items from Wave 10 649 1.83 (4.53) 043
each subscale. Sample items include: A time of learning to think
for yourself and a time of trying out new things. In the original
measurement development, internal consistency coefficients (al-
pha) ranged from .70 to .85 and testretest reliability ranged from
EMERGING ADULT IDENTITY AND ALCOHOL USE 349

Table 2
Correlations Among All Variables in the Model

Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14

1. Gender 1 .01 .02 .08 .04 .01 .02 .12 .08 .14 .14 .12 .06 .05
2. Employment status 3A 1 .59 .04 .05 .03 .08 .15 .05 .06 .01 .03 .06 .12
3. Employment status 3B 1 .07 .07 .01 .10 .23 .02 .10 .06 .04 .00 .05
4. Relationship status 3A 1 .37 .02 .06 .02 .04 .07 .03 .06 .04 .04
5. Relationship status 3B 1 .24 .17 .00 .13 .17 .15 .11 .19 .16

6. Experimentation 1 .46 .16 .47
.32
.23 .17
.17
.12
7. Self-focus 1 .08 .41 .22 .04 .01 .18 .10
8. Negativity/Instability 1 .18 .26 .11 .03 .05 .14
9. Identity exploration 1 .52 .09 .03 .08 .05
10. Feeling in-between 1 .04 .08 .05 .07
11. Sensation seeking 1 .50 .21 .18
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

12. Impulsivity 1 .15 .18


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

13. Heavy drinking 1 .61


14. Alcohol-related problems 1

p .05. p .01. p .001.

.64 to .76. Internal consistency reliability in the current study was frequency of binge drinking. Frequency of getting drunk was
good (.76 to .87 at Wave 1). assessed with the single item: During the past 3 months, how
Relationship status. Relationship status was assessed with a many times did you get drunk (not just a little high) on alcohol?
question that provided seven response options including: not dat- (Jackson et al., 2001). Binge drinking was also assessed with a
ing, dating but not exclusively, dating exclusively, engaged, mar- single item: During the past 3 months, how many times did you
ried, other, and divorced. In an attempt to avoid exclusion of have four/five (for women/men) or more drinks at a sitting?
participants, we attempted to reclassify those who provided a (Wechsler & Isaac, 1992). The mean of the two measures was
response of other into one of the six categories based on the computed as a composite measure of heavy drinking.
information provided by the participant. We were able to recode Alcohol problems. Alcohol-related problems were assessed
five of 13 at Wave 1, three of eight at Wave 2, and four of six at using the Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index (RAPI; White & Labou-
Wave 3. Participants who indicated that they were divorced or vie, 1989). The measure includes 23 items assessing the frequency
who provided a response of other that could not be recoded into of negative consequences resulting from drinking in the prior 3
one of the other categories were excluded from the analyses (n months. Example items include: relatives avoided you, missed
15). For purposes of this study, responses were reduced to three a day (or part of a day) of school or work, and not able to do your
categories: not dating, and dating but not exclusively were com- homework or study for a test. Items were scored on a 1 (0 times)
bined into a group that was not in a committed relationship; dating to 5 (more than 10 times) scale. The internal consistency reliability
exclusively comprised the second class of participants who were in for the scale in the current study was .91 at Wave 1 (Year 4).
a committed relationship but not engaged or married; engaged, and
married were combined into the final group. Two dummy codes Data Analytic Plan
were created to contrast participants who were not in a committed
relationship against those who were a) in a committed relationship Prior to conducting the primary analyses, distributions of all
but not engaged/married and b) engaged/married. variables were examined for non-normality and transformations
Employment status. Employment status was assessed with a were conducted as necessary. For the primary analyses, time
question that included three response options: no, part-time, and varying covariate growth models were conducted in Mplus 7.0
full-time. As with relationship status, two dummy codes were (Muthn & Muthn, 2013) using bootstrapping (5,000 bootstraps)
created contrasting those who were not employed against those and Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) estimation to
who were a) employed part-time, and b) employed full-time. manage missing data. Initially, we tested an unconditional growth
Sensation seeking and impulsivity. Sensation seeking and model in which loadings from the intercept latent variable to heavy
impulsivity were assessed at each wave using 19 truefalse items drinking at each wave were set to 1. The latent variable for the
from the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ; slope represented linear growth in heavy drinking over time, with
Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Joireman, Teta, & Kraft, 1993). Sensation loadings set to 0, 1, and 2 for Waves 13, respectively. In addition
seeking is characterized by the willingness to take risks for the to examining fit of the unconditional growth model, we examined
sake of varied and intense experiences and impulsivity reflects residual variances for heavy drinking at each wave to ensure that
acting without forethought. In the present study, internal consis- there was sufficient residual variability to allow for meaningful
tency reliabilities were good for both sensation seeking (alphas of prediction within our time-varying covariate (TVC) models. We
.79 to .81) and impulsivity (alphas of .74 to .79). also examined means for heavy drinking and alcohol problem
slopes to verify expected age-related decreases in these outcomes.
Next we proceeded to test the TVC models. We initially entered
Outcome Variables
gender as a time-invariant predictor of the intercept and slope, and
Heavy drinking. Heavy drinking was assessed with two the five IDEA scores as the only time-varying predictors of resid-
items: a) frequency of getting drunk (not just a little high) and b) ual variability in heavy drinking at each wave. This allowed us to
350 GATES, CORBIN, AND FROMME

examine the extent to which IDEA scores predicted time-specific .053, SE .017, p .002), with standardized coefficients ranging
deviations from the expected trajectory of heavy drinking based on from .06 to .07. Those with higher levels of self-focus reported
the underlying growth model. Next, we tested models with the heavier drinking than would be expected based on their underlying
addition of time-varying covariates including relationship status, trajectories. Effects of negativity/instability, identity exploration,
employment status, sensation seeking, and impulsivity, to deter- and feeling in between were not statistically significant (p val-
mine unique effects of emerging adult identity, accounting for ues .34).
other well established correlates of maturing out. Correlations Time-varying model with covariates. The model with the
among the time-specific predictors and between gender and the addition of covariates provided good fit to the data, 2(68 df)
time-specific predictors were freely estimated. Within both unad- 114.44; CFI .96; RMSEA .03, SRMR .02. With respect to
justed and covariate adjusted models, we first compared models the time-varying covariates, relationship status emerged as a pro-
that constrained IDEA effects to equality to models that allowed tective factor and significantly predicted residual variability in
effects to vary across time. A significant difference in model fit heavy drinking at all three waves. Relative to those not in a
indicated that the magnitude of effects differed across time. In the relationship, individuals who were in a relationship but not en-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

absence of evidence for time specific effects, we presented only gaged or married reported less drinking at all three time points
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

averaged effects across time. If model fit differed between con- (standardized coefficients .06, .07, and .14, respectively)
strained and unconstrained models, we presented the magnitude of than would be expected based on their underlying trajectory (all p
effects at all time points. The same procedures were followed for values .05). Effect sizes were similar when comparing those
the alcohol-related problems outcome variable. who were married or engaged to those not in a relationship
(standardized coefficients .09, .09, and .12; all p val-
Results ues .001). Employment status (part time and full time
employment each contrasted against no employment) was not a
significant predictor of heavy drinking at any wave (all p
Evaluation of Variable Distributions values .18). Although impulsivity did not predict residual
The heavy drinking composite measure and alcohol-related variability in heavy drinking at any wave (p values .28),
problems were positively skewed (skewness values ranging from sensation seeking was associated with residual variability in
2.53 to 4.69) and were therefore log transformed. Resulting skew- heavy drinking at all waves (standardized coefficients .13,
ness values suggested that the log-transformations were successful .12, and .08; p values .05).
in normalizing the distributions of these variables (all skewness As in the unadjusted models, tests of constrained versus uncon-
values 1.5). strained models identified no time-varying effects of the IDEA
scores (all p values .22). Thus, averaged effects of IDEA scores
are reported here. See Figure 1 for standardized coefficients for all
Growth Models for Heavy Drinking
time-specific effects from the unconstrained model. Accounting
Unconditional model. The unconditional growth model for for significant effects of relationship status and sensation seeking,
heavy drinking displayed excellent model fit, 2(1 df) .66, p experimentation remained a significant predictor (b .044, SE
.41; comparative fit index (CFI) 1.00; root mean square error .166, p .007), though the magnitude of the effect was reduced
approximation (RMSEA) 0.00, Standardized Root Mean Square (standardized coefficients of .06 to .07) relative to the unadjusted
Residual (SRMR) .01. The mean for the slope was .05 (SE model (.08 to .11). Self-focus also remained a significant risk
.008), p .001, indicating significant decreases in heavy drinking factor (b .053, SE .017, p .002), and the standardized
over time, and examination of residual variability in heavy drink- effects (.06 to .07) were not reduced with the addition of the
ing at each wave indicated that there was significant remaining covariates. Effects of negativity/instability, identity exploration,
variability that might be accounted for by the predictor variables in and feeling in between remained nonsignificant when accounting
the TVC models (all p values .001). for effects of the covariates (all p values .38).
Time-varying model without covariates. The growth model
for heavy drinking with only the IDEA scores as time-variant
Growth Models for Alcohol-Related Problems
predictors displayed excellent fit, 2(32 df) 37.12; CFI 1.0;
RMSEA .01, SRMR .03. Gender was not a significant Unconditional model. The unconditional growth model for
predictor of either intercept or slope (p values .53). Tests of alcohol-related problems displayed good model fit based on most
constrained (constraining effects of individual IDEA scores to fit indices, CFI .99, SRMR .02. However, the chi-square test
equivalence across time) versus unconstrained models found no was significant, 2(df 1) 9.54, .002, and the RMSEA
significant effects (p values .35). Thus, averaged effects across indicated marginal fit with a value of .10. The mean for the slope
time were examined. Note that ranges are provided for standard- was .053 (SE .007), p .001 indicating significant decreases
ized effects as standardized coefficients vary somewhat even in in alcohol-related problems over time, and there was significant
constrained models due to differences in the variances of the variance in alcohol-related problems at each wave that was not
predictor and outcome measures across time. Experimentation/ accounted for by the underlying trajectories.
possibilities emerged as a significant risk factor, such that those Time-varying model without covariates. The TVC growth
with higher levels of experimentation were heavier drinkers than model for alcohol-related problems without covariates displayed
would be expected based on their underlying trajectories (b excellent overall model fit, 2(32 df) 44.37, p .07; CFI .98;
.067, SE .016, p .001). Standardized coefficients ranged from RMSEA .02, SRMR .03. The time invariant covariate of
.08 to .11. Self-focus also emerged as a significant risk factor (b gender was not a significant predictor of the intercept or slope for
EMERGING ADULT IDENTITY AND ALCOHOL USE 351
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 1. Time-varying covariate model for heavy drinking. Imp Impulsivity; SS Sensation seeking; Emp
Xa Unemployed versus part-time employed; Emp Xb Unemployed versus full-time employed; Rel Xa
Not in a relationship versus committed relationship but not engaged/married; Rel Xb Not in a relationship
versus engaged/married; IE Identity exploration; SF Self-focus; EXP Experimentation/Possibilities;
FIB Feeling in-between; NEG Negativity/Instability.

alcohol-related problems (p values .52). With respect to IDEA marriage was significantly protective at Waves 2 and 3. Effects of
scores, comparison of a model that constrained effects of negativ- employment status were inconsistent, with full-time employment
ity to equality across the three time points and one that freely associated with reduced risk for alcohol problems at Wave 2 only
estimated time-specific effects yielded a significant difference in (standardized coefficient .09), and part-time employment as-
model fit, 2(2 df) 7.02, p .03. Thus, effects for negativity/ sociated with increased risk for alcohol problems at Wave 3 only
instability were examined separately at each time point. Negativ- (standardized coefficient .09). Although both sensation seeking
ity/instability emerged as a significant predictor at Wave 3 (stan- and impulsivity were associated with greater risk for alcohol-
dardized coefficient .14, p .001) but not at either Wave 1 related problems at all waves (standardized coefficients ranging
(standardized coefficient .02, p .52) or Wave 2 (standardized from .02 to .12), effects for sensation seeking were significant only
coefficient .05, p .16). at Waves 1 and 2, and effects of impulsivity were significant only
Tests of constrained versus unconstrained models did not iden- at Waves 1 and 3.
tify significant differences for any of the other IDEA scores (all p
As in the model without covariates, comparison of fit between
values .25), indicating a lack of time-specific effects. Thus
models that constrained and freely estimated the negativity/insta-
averaged effects across time are reported for these variables. See
bility paths identified a significant difference in model fit, 2 (2
Figure 2 for standardized coefficients for all time-specific effects
df) 9.39, p .01. As in the unadjusted model, the effect of
from the unconstrained model. Experimentation was a significant
negativity/instability was significant at Wave 3 (standardized co-
predictor of alcohol-related problems (b .042, SE .012, p
001), with standardized effects ranging from .07 to .09. Partici- efficient .15, p .001) but not at Wave 1 (standardized
pants with higher levels of experimentation were at heightened risk coefficient .02, p .48) or Wave 2 (standardized coefficient
for alcohol-related problems than would be expected based on the .03, p .43). Comparisons of constrained and unconstrained
underlying trajectory. Effects of self-focus, identity exploration, models for the other IDEA scores were not significant (all p
and feeling in between were not statistically significant (all p values .42), indicating a lack of time specific effects. Averaged
values .49). across time, the effect of experimentation was reduced to marginal
Time-varying model with covariates. Although the chi- significance (b .024, SE .013, p .06) with the inclusion of
square test was significant in the model including covariates, 2(68 the covariates, and the standardized coefficients were substantially
df) 108.43, p .001, all other indicators suggested good model reduced (.04 to .05) relative to the unadjusted model (.07 to .09).
fit; CFI .95; RMSEA .03, SRMR .02. Effects of relation- Effects of self-focus, identity exploration, and feeling in between
ship status were generally consistent with the heavy drinking remained nonsignificant when accounting for effects of the cova-
model, though effect sizes were smaller, and only engagement/ riates (all p values .48).
352 GATES, CORBIN, AND FROMME
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 2. Time-varying covariate model for alcohol-related problems. Imp Impulsivity; SS Sensation
seeking; Emp Xa Unemployed versus part-time employed; Emp Xb Unemployed versus full-time
employed; Rel Xa Not in a relationship versus committed relationship but not engaged/married; Rel Xb Not
in a relationship versus engaged/married; IE Identity exploration; SF Self-focus; EXP Experimentation/
Possibilities; FIB Feeling in-between; NEG Negativity/Instability.

Discussion mentation and use of alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana (Allem et al.,
2013; Lisha et al., 2014; Little et al., 2013). However, it is
The aim of this study was to examine dimensions of emerging
important to note that ours is the first to examine these relations
adult identity in relation to drinking outcomes during the transition
using longitudinal data and to examine differences in the magni-
out of college, controlling for the influences of personality and role
tude of effects across an important developmental transition. Fur-
transitions. Specifically, we predicted that experimentation, self-
ther, we controlled for other well-established predictors of matur-
focus, and feeling in-between would predict heavier drinking and
ing out including employment, relationship status, sensation
that negativity/instability would predict more alcohol-related prob-
seeking, and impulsivity, and this aspect of the study design shed
lems, with stronger effects emerging with time. The results of the
new light on findings of prior studies. Effects of experimentation
TVC growth models provided some support for study hypotheses.
were reduced by one fourth to one half with the inclusion of the
The experimentation/possibilities dimension of emerging iden-
tity was a significant predictor of both heavy drinking and alcohol- covariates, and effects on alcohol-related problems were no longer
related problems and these effects were consistent across time. significant.
However, when controlling for effects of covariates, standardized Examination of zero-order correlations suggests that the overlap
effects were reduced by 25 to 50%, and became nonsignificant was largely between experimentation and the personality variables.
(p .06) for alcohol-related problems. Significant effects of Within time-point correlations between experimentation and sen-
self-focus on alcohol use were also consistent across time. How- sation seeking ranged from .26 to .30 and correlations with im-
ever, unlike experimentation, the magnitude of the effects was pulsivity ranged from .14 to .18. Thus, prior studies demonstrating
unchanged by the inclusion of the covariates. Negativity/instability effects of experimentation likely overestimated the unique influ-
was the only facet of emerging adult identity that demonstrated ence of emerging adult identity given the overlap with other
differential effects across time, emerging as a significant predictor known personality predictors of risk. This is not to say that
of alcohol-related problems at only Wave 3. As with self-focus, the personality risk is important and emerging adult identity is not, as
magnitude of this relation was consistent across models with and there may be important interplay among these risk/protective fac-
without covariates. Whereas at least partial support was found for tors. For example, maturation in personality with brain develop-
other study hypotheses, feeling in-between was not a significant ment in emerging adulthood may set the stage for emerging adult
predictor at any wave, calling into question its relevance as a risk identity development, adoption of adult roles, and subsequent
factor for heavy drinking and related problems. maturing out of heavy drinking. Prior studies have shown that
In many ways, the findings of the current study were consistent other psychosocial risk factors like positive alcohol expectancies
with those of prior studies demonstrating links between experi- serve to mediate effects of trait personality risk on drinking out-
EMERGING ADULT IDENTITY AND ALCOHOL USE 353

comes (Corbin, Iwamoto, & Fromme, 2011). Thus, it is possible were from a single university, a large percentage of participants
that sensation seeking and impulsivity have indirect effects on were excluded from analyses because they did not graduate in 4
drinking behavior through a delayed or slower process of emerging years, and participants were roughly age 24 at the last wave. Thus,
adult identity development. This is an interesting question for it is not clear the extent to which findings will generalize to college
future research. students more broadly and to later stages of the transition out of
While small, effects of self-focus on heavy drinking were not emerging adulthood. The later emerging effects of negativity in the
impacted by other known predictors of maturing out. This is current study highlight the need for future studies that extend into
interesting as one might have expected a reduction in effects of later stages of emerging adulthood. The analyses also relied en-
self-focus after accounting for relationship status. Although the tirely on self-report data. Although self-reports of high-risk behav-
results suggest that effects of self-focus are not accounted for by iors like alcohol use may be biased, several studies have shown
role transitions, they are consistent with the idea that committed that self-reports of alcohol use are reliable (Babor, Steinberg,
relationships might indirectly contribute to reductions in alcohol Anton, & Del Boca, 2000; LaForge, Borsari, & Baer, 2005) and do
use/problems through reduced self-focus. Although testing this not generally represent underestimates of actual use.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

type of mediated effect was beyond the scope of the current study, It is also important to acknowledge that, although the analyses
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the zero-order correlation between marriage and self-focus was were longitudinal, relations between the time-varying predictors
robust (r .24), and effects of self-focus remained when ac- and residual variance in drinking and problems were cross-
counting for direct effects of marriage. Thus, prerequisites for sectional. Thus, it is quite possible that alcohol use/problems at a
testing an indirect effect were met and deserve attention in future given time point would also predict deviations away from the
studies. underlying trajectory of emerging adult identity development.
The link between negativity/instability and alcohol-related prob- Such reciprocal relations would be quite consistent with the liter-
lems is highly consistent with theoretical models of risk that ature on relations between personality variables and drinking out-
highlight the role of drinking to cope with negative emotion as a comes (Littlefield et al., 2009). Our analytic approach also did not
risk factor for alcohol-related problems (Corbin, Farmer, & Nolen- examine how changes in emerging adult identity relate to changes
Hoekesma, 2013; Cooper, Russell, Skinner, Frone, & Mudar, in drinking outcomes. If there is considerable variability in rates of
1992). Our results are also consistent with those of Smith et al. emerging adult identity development across individuals, it will be
(2014) who found that negativity/instability was significantly cor- important to understand the extent to which these differences are
related with substance-related problems. Further, Luyckx, De associated with changes in drinking outcomes. It is also possible to
Witte, and Goossens (2011) found that negativity/instability sig- model relations between growth processes and time-specific resid-
nificantly predicted depressive symptoms, which have been shown ual effects simultaneously (e.g., Autoregressive Latent Trajectory
to increase risk for alcohol-related problems (Gonzalez, Reynolds, models; Bollen & Curran, 2004), or to simultaneously model
& Skewes, 2011). The fact that negativity/instability did not separate between- and within-person processes (Latent Curve
emerge as a significant predictor until Wave 3 is also interesting. Models with Structured Residuals; Curran, Howard, Bainter, Lane,
It may be that stresses associated with the transition out of college & McGinley, 2014), which represent important directions for
(e.g., new work demands, development of new social networks) future research. Unfortunately, with only three data points and a
contribute to this association. It will be particularly important to large number of TVCs, such models were beyond the scope of the
examine effects of negativity/instability on alcohol problems in current study.
later adulthood and among individuals reporting clinically signif- It is also important to note the small magnitudes of the signif-
icant alcohol problems. icant effects observed in this study, and the fact that we examined
Although time-specific effects were demonstrated for negativi- a large number of parameters without control for multiple com-
ty/instability, we found no additional support for our hypothesis parisons. Although we tested effects of each of the five IDEA
that effects of emerging adult identity would increase with time. subscales on both alcohol use and related problems, we had a priori
The attenuation of the effects of experimentation when controlling hypotheses for only a subset of these parameters, many of which
for personality risk suggests that this aspect of emerging adult were supported. Findings for more exploratory analyses require
identity may be closely tied to personality maturation, which is future replication before we can draw firm conclusions. Regarding
typically more pronounced in adolescence (at least for sensation the small magnitude of the effects of emerging adult identity
seeking; see Steinberg et al., 2008). Thus, evidence for time- dimensions, measurement limitations may have been a contribut-
specific effects of experimentation may be more likely during the ing factor. In the context of a large battery of survey measures, we
transition into rather than out of emerging adulthood. With respect were only able to include three items for each IDEA dimension.
to self-focus, effects did not increase with time despite major role Although internal consistency reliability was good for these brief
changes (e.g., entry into committed relationships), and significant measures, effects may have been larger if the full IDEA measure
relations between these role changes and self-focus. This suggests was included. Our measure of employment status was also limited
that, although there may be normative changes in self-focus with in that we did not capture the types of jobs in which participants
the adoption of adult social roles, there may also be a more stable, were working. It seems likely that different types of jobs would be
trait-like aspect of self-focus that confers risk for alcohol use, differentially related to risk for or protection against heavy drink-
regardless of developmental stage. Of course, further studies of ing and related problems. Thus, we may have underestimated
self-focus across additional developmental periods are needed to effects of both emerging adult identity and employment status in
directly test this hypothesis. the current study.
Whereas the findings have potentially important implications, Finally, given the attenuation of relations between emerging
they must be considered in light of several limitations. Participants adult identity facets and drinking outcomes when controlling for
354 GATES, CORBIN, AND FROMME

effects of personality, it is important to recognize that we were Current and retrospective self-reports by college students. Journal of
unable to control for other personality variables of interest. Neu- Adolescence, 28, 523533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2004
roticism and negative urgency have been linked to changes in .10.007
drinking behavior during emerging adulthood (Littlefield et al., Bollen, K. A., & Curran, P. J. (2004). Autoregressive latent trajectory
2009, 2010a, 2010b; Settles, Cyders, & Smith, 2010), and it seems (ALT) models: A synthesis of two traditions. Sociological Methods &
quite possible that accounting for these personality variables might Research, 32, 336 383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0049124103260222
attenuate relations between negativity/instability and alcohol- Cooper, M. L., Russell, M., Skinner, J. B., Frone, M. R., & Mudar, P.
(1992). Stress and alcohol use: Moderating effects of gender, coping,
related problems in the same way that controlling for sensation
and alcohol expectancies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 139
seeking and impulsivity attenuated relations between experimen-
152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.101.1.139
tation and alcohol use. Including these personality variables rep-
Corbin, W. R., Farmer, N. M., & Nolen-Hoekesma, S. (2013). Relations
resents an important future direction as our results suggest that
among stress, coping strategies, coping motives, alcohol consumption
prior studies may have overestimated the unique effects of exper- and related problems: A mediated moderation model. Addictive Behav-
imentation on substance use. iors, 38, 19121919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.12.005
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Despite the aforementioned limitations, the current study adds to Corbin, W. R., Iwamoto, D. K., & Fromme, K. (2011). A comprehensive
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the literature by demonstrating complex relations between emerg- longitudinal test of the acquired preparedness model for alcohol use and
ing adult identity and drinking outcomes. The longitudinal nature related problems. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 72, 602
of the study and control for other known predictors of maturing out 610. http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2011.72.602
clarify our understanding of the unique effects of emerging adult Curran, P. J., Howard, A. L., Bainter, S. A., Lane, S. T., & McGinley, J. S.
identity across a critical developmental transition. Nonetheless, (2014). The separation of between-person and within-person compo-
longer longitudinal studies are needed to determine if effects nents of individual change over time: A latent curve model with struc-
persist or perhaps strengthen as individuals move further into tured residuals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82,
adulthood. It will also be important to examine the extent to which 879 894. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035297
changes in emerging adult identity relate to changes in drinking Eitle, D., Taylor, J., & Eitle, T. M. (2010). Heavy episodic alcohol use in
behavior. Finally, it will be important to control for additional emerging adulthood: The role of early risk factors and young adult social
personality variables (e.g., neuroticism and negative urgency) and roles. Journal of Drug Issues, 40, 295320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
to identify potential mechanisms (e.g., drinking motives) through 002204261004000203
which emerging adult identity facets contribute to drinking out- Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. Oxford, UK: W. W.
Norton.
comes. We hope the results of the current study will encourage
Fleming, C. B., White, H. R., & Catalano, R. F. (2010). Romantic rela-
others to examine these important questions.
tionships and substance use in early adulthood: An examination of the
influences of relationship type, partner substance use, and relationship
References quality. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51, 153167. http://dx
Allem, J. P., Lisha, N. E., Soto, D. W., Baezconde-Garbanati, L., & Unger, .doi.org/10.1177/0022146510368930
J. B. (2013). Emerging adulthood themes, role transitions and substance Gonzalez, V. M., Reynolds, B., & Skewes, M. C. (2011). Role of impul-
use among Hispanics in Southern California. Addictive Behaviors, 38, sivity in the relationship between depression and alcohol problems
27972800. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.08.001 among emerging adult college drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psy-
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood. A theory of development from chopharmacology, 19, 303313. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022720
the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469 Gotham, H. J., Sher, K. J., & Wood, P. K. (2003). Alcohol involvement and
480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469 developmental task completion during young adulthood. Journal of
Arnett, J. J. (2005). The developmental context of substance use in emerg- Studies on Alcohol, 64, 32 42. http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/jsa.2003
ing adulthood. Journal of Drug Issues, 35, 235254. http://dx.doi.org/ .64.32
10.1177/002204260503500202 Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Corbin, W. R., & Fromme, K. (2008). Trajectories
Arnett, J. J. (2006). Emerging adulthood: Understanding the new way of and determinants of alcohol use among LGB young adults and their
coming of age. In J. J. Arnett & J. L. Tanner (Eds.), Emerging adults in heterosexual peers: Results from a prospective study. Developmental
America: Coming of age in the 21st century (pp. 319). Washington, Psychology, 44, 8190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.81
DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Jackson, K. M., Sher, K. J., Gotham, H. J., & Wood, P. K. (2001).
11381-001 Transitioning into and out of large-effect drinking in young adulthood.
Ashenhurst, J. R., Harden, K. P., Corbin, W. R., & Fromme, K. (2015). Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 378 391. http://dx.doi.org/10
Trajectories of binge drinking and personality change across emerging
.1037/0021-843X.110.3.378
adulthood. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29, 978 991. http://dx
Jochman, K., & Fromme, K. (2010). Maturing out of substance use: The
.doi.org/10.1037/adb0000116
other side of etiology. In L. M. Scheier (Ed.), Handbook of drug use
Babor, T. F., Steinberg, K., Anton, R., & Del Boca, F. (2000). Talk is
etiology: Theory, methods, and empirical findings (pp. 565578). Wash-
cheap: Measuring drinking outcomes in clinical trials. Journal of Studies
on Alcohol, 61, 55 63. http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/jsa.2000.61.55 ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Bachman, J., OMalley, P., Schulenberg, J., Johnston, L., Bryant, A., & Johnston, L. D., OMalley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Schulenberg, J. E., &
Merline, A. (2002). The decline of substance use in young adulthood: Miech, R. A. (2014). Monitoring the Future national survey results on
Changes in social activities, roles, and beliefs. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. drug use, 19752013: Volume II, College students and adults ages
Bachman, J., Wadsworth, K., OMalley, P., Johnston, L., & Schulenberg, 19 55. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of
J. (1997). Smoking, drinking, and drug use in young adulthood: The Michigan.
impacts of new freedoms and responsibilities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Kretsch, N., & Harden, K. P. (2014). Marriage, divorce, and alcohol use in
Bishop, D. I., Weisgram, E. S., Holleque, K. M., Lund, K. E., & Wheeler- young adulthood: A longitudinal sibling-comparison study. Emerging
Anderson, J. R. (2005). Identity development and alcohol consumption: Adulthood, 2, 138 149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167696813513260
EMERGING ADULT IDENTITY AND ALCOHOL USE 355

Labouvie, E. (1996). Maturing out of substance use: Selection and self- Schwartz, S. J., Donnellan, M. B., Ravert, R. D., Luyckx, K., & Zambo-
correction. Journal of Drug Issues, 26, 457 476. http://dx.doi.org/10 anga, B. L. (2013). Handbook of psychology: Vol. 6. Identity develop-
.1177/002204269602600208 ment, personality, and well-being in adolescence and emerging adult-
Laforge, R. G., Borsari, B., & Baer, J. S. (2005). The utility of collateral hood: Theory, research, and recent advances (2nd ed., pp. 339 364).
informant assessment in college alcohol research: Results from a longi- Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
tudinal prevention trial. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66, 479 487. Settles, R. F., Cyders, M., & Smith, G. T. (2010). Longitudinal validation
http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/jsa.2005.66.479 of the acquired preparedness model of drinking risk. Psychology of
Lee, M. R., Chassin, L., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2015). Role transitions and Addictive Behaviors, 24, 198 208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017631
young adult maturing out of heavy drinking: Evidence for larger effects Sher, K. J., Bartholow, B. D., & Nanda, S. (2001). Short- and long-term
of marriage among more severe premarriage problem drinkers. Alcohol- effects of fraternity and sorority membership on heavy drinking: A
ism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39, 1064 1074. http://dx.doi social norms perspective. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 4251.
.org/10.1111/acer.12715 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.15.1.42
Lee, M. R., Ellingson, J. M., & Sher, K. J. (2015). Integrating social- Slutske, W. S. (2005). Alcohol use disorders among U.S. college students
contextual and intrapersonal mechanisms of maturing out: Joint influ- and their non-college-attending peers. Archives of General Psychiatry,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ences of familial-role transitions and personality maturation on problem- 62, 321327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.321
Smith, D. C., Bahar, O. S., Cleeland, L. R., & Davis, J. P. (2014).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

drinking reductions. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,


39, 17751787. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/acer.12816 Self-perceived emerging adult status and substance use. Psychology of
Lisha, N. E., Grana, R., Sun, P., Rohrbach, L., Spruijt-Metz, D., Reifman, Addictive Behaviors, 28, 935941. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035900
A., & Sussman, S. (2014). Evaluation of the psychometric properties of Staff, J., Greene, K. M., Maggs, J. L., & Schoon, I. (2014). Family
the revised inventory of the dimensions of emerging adulthood transitions and changes in drinking from adolescence through mid-life.
(IDEA-R) in a sample of continuation high school students. Evaluation Addiction, 109, 227236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/add.12394
& the Health Professions, 37, 156 177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., &
0163278712452664 Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity
Little, M. A., Spruijt-Metz, D., Pokhrel, P., Sun, P., Rohrbach, L. A., & as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems
Sussman, S. (2013). Predicting self-initiated marijuana use cessation model. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1764 1778. http://dx.doi.org/10
among youth at continuation high schools. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, .1037/a0012955
69. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00069 Vergs, A., Jackson, K. M., Bucholz, K. K., Grant, J. D., Trull, T. J.,
Littlefield, A. K., Sher, K. J., & Wood, P. K. (2009). Is maturing out of Wood, P. K., & Sher, K. J. (2012). Deconstructing the age-prevalence
problematic alcohol involvement related to personality change? Journal curve of alcohol dependence: Why maturing out is only a small piece
of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 360 374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ of the puzzle. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 511523. http://
a0015125 dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026027
Littlefield, A. K., Sher, K. J., & Wood, P. K. (2010a). Do changes in Wechsler, H., & Isaac, N. (1992). Binge drinkers at Massachusetts
drinking motives mediate the relation between personality change and colleges. Prevalence, drinking style, time trends, and associated prob-
maturing out of problem drinking? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, lems. Journal of the American Medical Association, 267, 2929 2931.
119, 93105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017512 http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.1992.03480210091038
Littlefield, A. K., Sher, K. J., & Wood, P. K. (2010b). A personality-based Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., Seibring, M., Nelson, T. F., & Lee, H.
description of maturing out of alcohol problems: Extension with a (2002). Trends in college binge drinking during a period of increased
five-factor model and robustness to modeling challenges. Addictive prevention efforts. Findings from 4 Harvard School of Public Health
Behaviors, 35, 948 954. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06 College Alcohol Study surveys: 19932001. Journal of American Col-
.008 lege Health, 50, 203217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/074484
Luyckx, K., De Witte, H., & Goossens, L. (2011). Perceived instability in 80209595713
emerging adulthood: The protective role of identity capital. Journal of Wechsler, H., & Nelson, T. F. (2008). What we have learned from the
Applied Developmental Psychology, 32, 137145. http://dx.doi.org/10 Harvard School of Public Health College Alcohol Study: Focusing
.1016/j.appdev.2011.02.002 attention on college student alcohol consumption and the environmental
Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego-identity status. conditions that promote it. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 69,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551558. http://dx.doi 481 490. http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2008.69.481
.org/10.1037/h0023281 White, H. R., & Labouvie, E. W. (1989). Towards the assessment of
Muthn, L. K., & Muthn, B. O. (1998 2013). Mplus users guide (6th adolescent problem drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 50, 30 37.
ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author. http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/jsa.1989.50.30
Quinn, P. D., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Differential changes in impulsivity Yamaguchi, K., & Kandel, D. B. (1985). On the resolution of role incom-
and sensation seeking and the escalation of substance use from adoles- patibility: A life event history analysis of family roles and marijuana use.
cence to early adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 223 American Journal of Sociology, 90, 1284 1325. http://dx.doi.org/10
239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000284 .1086/228211
Reifman, A., Arnett, J. J., & Colwell, M. J. (2007). Emerging adulthood: Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P., & Kraft, M.
Theory, assessment and application. Journal of Youth Development, 2 (1993). A comparison of three structural models for personality: The big
(1), Article 0701FA003. three, the big five, and the alternative five. Journal of Personality and
Schwartz, S. J., Beyers, W., Luyckx, K., Soenens, B., Zamboanga, B. L., Social Psychology, 65, 757768. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
Forthun, L. F., . . . Waterman, A. S. (2011). Examining the light and dark .65.4.757
sides of emerging adults identity: A study of identity status differences
in positive and negative psychosocial functioning. Journal of Youth and Received May 20, 2015
Adolescence, 40, 839 859. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-010- Revision received March 7, 2016
9606-6 Accepted March 7, 2016

Potrebbero piacerti anche