Sei sulla pagina 1di 16

British Journal of Management, Vol.

21, 453468 (2010)


DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8551.2009.00664.x

Leadership Styles as Predictors of


Self-reported and Observed Workplace
Bullying
Helge Hoel, Lars Glas,1 Jrn Hetland,1 Cary L. Cooper2 and
Stale Einarsen1
Manchester Business School, University of Manchester, Booth Street West, Manchester M15 PB6, UK,
1
Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Christiesgt. 12, 5015 Bergen, Norway, and
2
Lancaster University Management School, University of Lancaster, Lancaster LA1 4VX, UK
Corresponding author email: helge.hoel@mbs.ac.uk

The connection between leadership or management style, on the one hand, and
perceptions of bullying, on the other, has received little attention within bullying
research. Hence, the aim of this study is to examine the relationship between
subordinates ratings of their immediate superiors behaviours, and both perceived
exposure and claims of observations of bullying at work. Based on a sampling process
which emphasized randomness and representativeness, the responses from 5288
respondents in Great Britain taking part in a nationwide study on psychosocial issues
at work were included in the analysis. Bullying correlated with all four leadership styles
measured. Yet, non-contingent punishment emerged as the strongest predictor of self-
perceived exposure to bullying, while autocratic leadership was the strongest predictor
of observed bullying. Hence, while observers particularly associate bullying with
autocratic or tyrannical leader behaviour, targets relate bullying more to non-
contingent punishment, i.e. an unpredictable style of leadership, where punishment is
meted out or delivered on leaders own terms, independent of the behaviour of
subordinates. In addition, laissez-faire leadership emerged as a predictor of self-
reported as well as observed bullying. Thus, leadership styles seem to play an important
but complex role in the bullying process.

Introduction public hospital employees, Niedl (1996) reported


that targets of bullying showed higher levels of
Bullying and harassment are reported to happen anxiety, depression, irritation and psychosomatic
on a regular basis in many work organizations complaints than non-victims. There is also
(e.g. Einarsen and Skogstad, 1996; Hoel, Cooper evidence emerging of a relationship between
and Faragher, 2001; Keashly and Jagatic, 2000). bullying and negative organizational outcomes,
Depending upon the methodology applied and such as absenteeism, turnover and reduced
the way the concept is operationalized, the productivity (Hoel, Einarsen and Cooper, 2003;
prevalence of bullying is estimated to be between Sheehan et al., 2001). Moreover, such eects
5% and 10% among the European workforce appear to extend beyond those directly involved
(Paoli and Merille, 2001; Zapf et al., 2003). to include also witnesses or bystanders (Hoel,
Bullying also has grave negative consequences for Faragher and Cooper, 2004; Rayner, Hoel and
targets, observers and the organization itself (e.g. Cooper, 2002; Unison, 1997). Such a ripple eect
Hoel, Faragher and Cooper, 2004; Leymann, (Hoel, Rayner and Cooper, 1999; Unison, 1997)
1996; Zapf, 1999). In a study of 368 Austrian is noted in a number of studies (e.g. Hoel and

r 2009 British Academy of Management. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford
OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA, 02148, USA.
454 H. Hoel et al.

Cooper, 2000; Unison, 1997; Vartia, 2001), with (Einarsen et al., 2003), emotional abuse (Keashly,
witnesses reporting higher levels of stress, less job 1998), mistreatment (Price Spratlen, 1995) and
satisfaction and higher turnover rates than those incivility (Pearson, Andersson and Porath,
who have not observed bullying taking place. 2005), denitions of bullying and harassment
Hence, bullying is increasingly recognized as a at work are increasingly converging, with a focus
serious problem within the working environment, on characteristics such as unwanted negative
potentially carrying a very substantial cost to the behaviour, persistency, duration and an imbal-
organization (Hoel, Einarsen and Cooper, 2003; ance of power between perpetrator and target
Sheehan et al., 2001). (Di Martino, Hoel and Cooper, 2003; Einarsen
Bullying is reported to happen at all organiza- et al., 2003). Therefore, bullying is associated
tional levels (Hoel, Cooper and Faragher, 2001). with ongoing negative relationships where targets
Yet, in many bullying cases, managers are seen as strongly resent the received treatment, seeing it as
the most typical perpetrators (Rayner, Hoel and systematic and ongoing and nding it dicult to
Cooper, 2002). In this respect, a Norwegian study defend themselves against it (Einarsen et al.,
found that more than 50% of those considering 2003). Although single acts of aggression and
themselves as targets reported having been harassment occur fairly frequently in everyday
bullied by someone in a managerial capacity interaction at work, they seem to be associated
(Einarsen, Raknes and Matthiesen, 1994), rising with severe health problems when they are
to 75% or even higher in countries such as the experienced on a regular basis (Leymann, 1996).
UK (Hoel, Cooper and Faragher, 2001; Unison, The behaviours involved may also be subtle,
1997) and Ireland (OMoore et al., 1998). devious and immensely dicult to confront
Although leaders are often considered the main (Adams, 1992, p. 17). Hence, they may even be
perpetrators of bullying, leadership and manage- dicult to recognize, at least by non-aected
ment style have received surprisingly little atten- colleagues or supervisors. Yet, as these negative
tion as antecedent of perceptions of bullying and work relationships become increasingly entrenched
harassment. The aim of this study is therefore to and personal, third parties or witnesses can seldom
investigate the relationships between the leader- remain uninvolved as the targets will tend to seek
ship style of line managers and supervisors, on support for their case (Einarsen, 1996). While
the one hand, and perceptions of bullying among some may support the target, others may not, even
self-reported targets and observers of bullying, on turning against the target (Leymann, 1996).
the other. The study adds to the growing Being aware of their own vulnerability and
literature on bullying and harassment by inves- fearful of becoming targets themselves (Rayner,
tigating how four dierent styles of leadership are Hoel and Cooper, 2002), witnesses might decide
associated with perceptions of bullying and to choose to ignore the situation rather than
harassment among subordinates, employing a intervene on behalf of targets or, in some
large-scale nationwide representative sample of instances, even collude in the bullying (Einarsen,
the UK workforce. Raknes and Matthiesen, 1994). Alternatively,
observers may suer in silence, with stress and
mental health problems, reduced job satisfaction
The nature of workplace bullying and harassment
and increased intentions to leave their job as
Bullying and harassment at work may be dened possible consequences. In other cases, non-
as repeated behaviour, actions and practices exposed colleagues may be subjected to stress
directed at one or more workers, which may be if summoned as witnesses when the case is
carried out deliberately or unconsciously, but under investigation by senior management (see
which are unwanted by the targets, causing Merchant and Hoel, 2003).
humiliation, oence and distress, and which
may interfere with job performance and/or cause
Leadership styles and perceptions of bullying
an unpleasant working environment (Einarsen
and Raknes, 1991). Although a range of terms Managers are reported to be the most frequent
are used to address the problem of psychological perpetrators from the point of view of targets
violence among organization members while at (Rayner, Hoel and Cooper, 2002). Whilst bully-
work, e.g. mobbing (Leymann, 1990), bullying ing is predominantly a downward process with

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Leadership and Bullying 455

managers seen as the key source or instigators, emotions when interacting with superiors, such as
research has shown that this phenomenon is pain, suering and despair caused by insensitive,
experienced by people at all hierarchical levels of self-serving and autocratic bosses. Similarly, a
the organization, thus aecting managers as well Norwegian study demonstrated that subordinates
as workers (Hoel, Cooper and Faragher, 2001). may experience a number of negative emotions in
Yet, as research on leadership and styles of their dealings with leaders, such as frustration,
leaders has primarily focused on leader eective- uncertainty and feelings of violation (Glas and
ness and those factors which might contribute to Einarsen, 2006). These emotions were strongly
optimal performance and output (see Northouse associated with the subordinates assessment of
(2004) for an overview), we know surprisingly the quality of the leadersubordinate relation-
little about situations where subordinates resent ship. A lack of respect and trust in the leader
the leadership style of their own leader. Further- subordinate relationship may also increase levels
more, in much of the research on leadership, of emotional regulation, e.g. suppressing or
ineective leadership has simply been considered faking ones own emotions during any interac-
to be the opposite of eective leadership (Ash- tion, which again may have short-term as well as
forth, 1994). As a result, those aspects of leader- long-term detrimental eects on recipients, such
ship which might be considered negative and even as reduced job satisfaction, increased intention to
destructive with detrimental consequences for leave and higher levels of subjective health
subordinates and followers have largely been complaints (Glas et al., 2006).
overlooked (Hoel and Cooper, 2000; Rayner and As far as the role of leaders in bullying
Cooper, 2003). scenarios is concerned, leaders might instigate
Still, in organizational climate research from perceptions of bullying unknowingly. Based on
the mid-1950s to the 1990s, 60% to 75% of all survey data, bullying has been associated with
employees typically reported that the worst lack of involvement in decision-making processes
aspect of their job was their immediate super- and with work environments where employees
visor. Several studies have also suggested that the are afraid of expressing their opinions and where
personal style of leaders may negatively inuence an authoritarian way of settling conicts prevails
the working climate and the productivity of the (Vartia, 1996). Satisfaction with supervisors
organization (Ekvall et al., 2002; Kets de Vries ability to resolve work-related conict has also
and Miller, 1984). While supportive leaders seem been found to account for the largest dierence
to increase the subordinates job satisfaction between bullied and non-bullied respondents
(Packard and Kauppi, 1999), by contrast sub- (OMoore et al., 1998). With bullying associated
ordinates who perceived themselves to be bullied with an unequal balance of power, many bullying
by their leaders experience low job satisfaction scenarios might develop from situations in which
and report many symptoms of health problems leaders attempt to control the behaviour of
(Skogstad, 1997). It is therefore not surprising subordinates by means of force. This would
that stress research has revealed that abusive and particularly be the case where such use of force
tyrannical leaders are an important source of is considered unreasonable or unjustied by the
stress for subordinates (Hogan, 1994; Hogan and recipients. From the leaders perspective, such use
Hogan, 2001). In line with these ndings, Kile of force can be instrumental in nature, motivated
(1990) reported that subordinates with an abusive by meeting organizational outcomes (Einarsen
leader may suer from anxiety, depression, et al., 2003), e.g. by increasing employee eort or
gastrointestinal and circulation problems. It is counteracting employee conduct deemed unac-
also claimed that many employees spend much of ceptable or sub-standard by the leader. In being
their time simply trying to cope with destructive directive and willing to apply force to achieve
leaders (Hogan, Raskin and Fazzini, 1990), with organizational goals, such behaviour can be
targets of such abusive supervisors reporting a associated with what is often described as an
low level of loyalty and commitment to the autocratic style of leadership. This term emerged
organization (Tepper, 2000). from the seminal work of Kurt Lewin and
Focusing on destructive aspects of the leader colleagues (Lewin, Lippitt and White, 1939) who
subordinate relationship, Frost (2004) asserts in the 1940s applied the distinction democratic
that subordinates may often experience toxic versus autocratic to decision-making processes

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


456 H. Hoel et al.

and productivity of groups within a work context The legitimacy of autocratic leadership, arising
(Burnes, 2007). These two opposing styles of from the managerial prerogative, whether actu-
leadership have frequently been applied in ally judged to be productive or not, rests on
organizational studies, although the term demo- leaders motivation for using force to ensure
cratic leadership has often been replaced by compliance, although the prevailing culture will
participative leadership, as a form of leadership impact on judgements (Hoel and Salin, 2003).
distinct from task-oriented and relationship- Accordingly, with respect to the administration
oriented leadership behaviour (Yukl, 2000). of acts or behaviour which could be construed as
Although no agreement has emerged with regard punishment by subordinates for non-compliance,
to denitions or taxonomies of these respective Podsako, Todor and Skov (1982) made a
terms (e.g. Foels et al., 2000; Johnson and Klee, distinction between contingent and non-contin-
2007), it has been usual to see autocratic leader- gent punishment. Examining the impact of social
ship as being at one end of a continuum, focusing rewards and punishment, they concluded that
on compliance, and delegation at the other (e.g. subordinate behaviour is inuenced by the
Yukl, 2000). Autocratic leadership is therefore contingencies upon which rewards and punish-
often used synonymously with a directive or even ment are administered (Peng and Peterson, 1998,
coercive style of leadership, although with its p. 70). Thus, when rewards and punishment are
potency in terms of how it is operationalized administered on the basis of, or in response to,
varying from relatively moderate to relatively tangible standards for performance and beha-
extreme levels (Foels et al., 2000), where concepts viour, they are more welcomed and satisfying to
such as abusive supervision (Tepper, 2000) or subordinates than when delivered arbitrarily or
tyrannical leadership (Einarsen, Aasland and for no good reason (Atwater et al., 1998; Peng
Skogstad, 2007) have been to used to describe the and Peterson, 1998). Hence, where the actual
latter. behaviour of the leader is considered as punish-
As autocratic leadership may render any ment for failing to comply with what is perceived
aspirations of involvement impossible, this style as unreasonable demands or unknown standards,
may be considered negative and even a source of or simply as being arbitrary, it is likely to be
bullying in itself (OMoore et al., 1998; Vartia, judged as an abuse of power and something that
1996). Managers who fail to manage their own the subordinate may nd dicult to grasp and
pressures and stress, reverting to or falling into cope with. Moreover, where managers or lead-
an autocratic or even tyrannical style of leader- ers use of force is motivated by self-interest,
ship in particularly stressful circumstances (Hoel narcissism or self-aggrandizement (Ashforth,
and Salin, 2003), where involvement and con- 1994), rather than by the perceived interest of
structive criticism may be replaced by rage and the organization, one may argue that such
shouting, exhibit behaviour which, for the sub- behaviour is more likely to be seen as illegitimate
ordinates as well for witnesses or observers, may and an abuse of power by those at the receiving
come across as both threatening and incompre- end as well as by observers. This would also
hensible. Moreover, an autocratic style could apply to situations where powerless or failing
bring about frustration and aggression among leaders attempt to gain or retain control by
subordinates, possibly giving rise to or increasing means of punitive actions (Ashforth, 1994). As
the likelihood of peer aggression (e.g. Felson, explained by Atwater et al. (1998, p. 563),
1992), and in this way act as an antecedent of Leaders may feel more powerful if they can
bullying. deliver punishment on their own terms (i.e. non-
contingent).
H1: Self-reported bullying is positively asso- Ashforth (1994) argues that destructive leaders
ciated with autocratic leadership. may foster helplessness and despair among
H2: Self-reported bullying is negatively asso- subordinates by curbing their initiative and
ciated with participative leadership. closely controlling them. Through arbitrariness
H3: Witnessed or observed bullying is posi- and non-contingent punishment, these leaders
tively associated with autocratic leadership. may render the work environment unpredictable,
H4: Witnessed or observing bullying is nega- which might cause frustration and stress for the
tively associated with participatory leadership. subordinates involved. Similarly, in an interview

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Leadership and Bullying 457

study exploring the concept of destructive leader- associated with role conict and lack of task
ship styles among Norwegian managers, Kile clarity (Einarsen, Raknes and Matthiesen, 1994),
(1990) identied a number of negative or failing to pay attention to such aspects of
destructive leader types, many of which had in leadership requirement could indirectly contri-
common that their behaviour was perceived as bute to bullying.
unpredictable and inconsistent. According to
Kile, such behaviour is likely to generate H7: Self-reported bullying is positively asso-
uncertainty, resentment, fear as well as anger ciated with a laissez-faire style of leadership.
among subordinates. H8: Witnessed or observed bullying is posi-
tively associated with a laissez-faire style of
H5: Self-reported bullying is positively asso- leadership.
ciated with a leadership style applying non-
contingent punishment (NCP leadership).
H6: Witnessed or observed bullying is posi-
tively associated with a leadership style apply-
ing non-contingent punishment (NCP Method
leadership). Sample
Another style of leadership often mentioned in This study forms part of a nationwide survey of
the management literature is laissez-faire leader- the prevalence of bullying in Great Britain (see
ship (e.g. Bass and Avolio, 1990), where leaders Hoel, Cooper and Faragher, 2001). A question-
are physically in post but fail to carry out their naire was mailed to a sample of employees
duties. But, as has been argued by Skogstad et al. identied by means of a procedure ensuring
(2007), the eect of such leadership is not a zero- randomness and acceptable representativeness,
sum game, as the abdication of responsibility fails and drawn from 70 organizations within the
to meet the expectation of subordinates and, private, public and voluntary sectors across the
indeed, the organization, with possibly detrimen- country. Together these organizations employed
tal eects on both. In this respect, it is also just under 1 million people. A total of 5288
possible that passive leadership may become questionnaires were returned, giving a response
destructive and failure to lead may be construed rate of 42.8%. Of the respondents 52.4% were
as bullying in its own right, e.g. where leaders fail men and 47.6% women. The average age for the
to respond or act in line with clear expectations. sample was 40.2 (SD 5 0.84). The respondents
The absence of adequate leadership may be identied their organizational status as workers
experienced by subordinates as an intended and (43.6%), foremen/women or supervisors (here-
systematic neglect and ignorance, even to the after referred to as supervisors) (14.9%), middle
extent that they feel socially excluded and management (21.1%), senior management
ostracized. For example, Frischer and Larsson (7.3%) and others (13.1%).
(2000) reported laissez-faire leadership to be the
main reason why Swedish PhD students drop out
of their doctoral programme.
Theoretically it has been assumed that a Measurements
laissez-faire style of leadership could also con- Exposure to workplace bullying was measured by
tribute to bullying by turning a blind eye to a self-labelling approach. The following deni-
conicts or by failure to intervene appropriately tion was presented to the respondents (see
when approached, thus indirectly condoning the Einarsen and Skogstad, 1996, p. 191):
behaviour (Einarsen et al., 2003). Besides, it may
be argued that passivity or laissez-faire behaviour We dene bullying as a situation where one or several
on the part of managers could provoke group individuals persistently over a period of time perceive
conicts and peer-bullying, hence acting as an themselves to be on the receiving end of negative
antecedent to bullying, when decisions inappro- actions from one or several persons, in a situation
priately are left to the group or when leaders where the target of bullying has diculty in defending
absences give rise to operational and co-opera- him or herself against these actions. We will not refer
tional problems. In addition, with bullying to a one-o incident as bullying.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


458 H. Hoel et al.

The denition was immediately followed by the ing on a style of leadership where there is no
question, using the above denition, please state room for employee involvement in the decision-
whether you have been bullied at work over the making process and where such involvement is
last six months, accompanied by six answer considered unnecessary (e.g. Ignores my views
alternatives with regard to frequency of experi- and opinions). Laissez-faire leadership was mea-
ence (no, rarely, now and then, monthly, weekly sured with three items which refer to indierence
and daily). and lack of involvement in tasks as well as lack of
Observed bullying was measured with the use of interest in employees (e.g. Ignores problems
the Bergen Bullying Indicator (Einarsen and when they arise). Participative leadership (seven
Raknes, 1991), a ve-item instrument which items) measures the extent to which the leader
measures the degree to which bullying is perceived values employee involvement and participation
to constitute a problem in the workplace (e.g. (e.g. Shares information with me). The items of
Bullying at my workplace reduces our e- the last three scales were designed for this study
ciency). A four-point Likert scale, ranging from by means of a focus group study. All measures of
totally agree to totally disagree, was applied. To leadership were written in behavioural terms with
obtain a pure measure of observed and witnessed no reference to motivation or intent.
bullying, we controlled in the statistical analyses The leadership styles were all measured by
for self-labelling as a victim (see Figure 1, which means of a ve-point Likert scale giving the
presents the hypothesized model). answer alternatives Never, Seldom, Occasion-
Non-contingent punishment was measured with ally, Often and Always. To avoid response
four items taken from the Leader Reward and sets, items were formulated positively as well as
Punishment Questionnaire Scale (Podsako, negatively. The Cronbach alpha levels for all
Todor and Skov, 1982). The items refer to a scales were in the range 0.730.92, suggesting
punitive management style where the punishment satisfactory internal consistency among all seven
seems to be unrelated to, or not contingent upon, scales, well above Nunnallys (1978) recommended
the behaviour of the subordinate (e.g. Repri- minimum level of 0.70. A conrmatory factor
mands me without me knowing why). Autocratic analysis of the four hypothesized leadership styles
leadership was measured with seven items, focus- showed an acceptable t to data (w2 5 4599.3;

Autocratic
leadership

NCP Self reported


leadership bullying

Laissez-faire Observed
leadership bullying

Participative
leadership

Figure 1. Leadership styles and self-reported bullying: hypothesized model

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Leadership and Bullying 459

comparative t index (CFI) 5 0.93; root-mean- 0.77 to 0.92. Positive correlations ranging from
square error of approximation (RMSEA) 5 0.46 to 0.68 were found between NCP and
0.064). In the model the leadership styles were autocratic leadership, while negative correlations
assumed to be related, and the analysis yielded that ranging from 0.48 to 0.66 were found
the three negative leadership styles were strongly between NCP and autocratic leadership, on the
and positively correlated. one hand, and participative leadership, on the
other. Between observed bullying and the leader-
ship styles, correlations in the range from 0.27
Analysis to 0.38 emerged, while the corresponding correla-
Pearson productmoment correlations and struc- tions for self-reported bullying were in the range
tural equation models were used to analyse the from 0.25 to 0.42. All correlation coecients
data. All structural equation models were tested were signicant at the 99% level.
by means of AMOS 7. Initially, the overall
measurement model was tested including the four Structural equation models
leadership styles and the two outcome measure-
ments in question. In the measurement model all Table 3 presents t indices for the estimated
the latent variables and the outcome variables structural equation models examining the relation-
were assumed to co-vary. Subsequently, the ship between leadership styles and the two bullying
hypothesized structural model was tested. In the outcomes. The initial measurement model showed
hypothesized model (Figure 1), structural paths acceptable t to data (CFI 5 0.94; RMSEA 5
from the four leadership styles to the two 0.059), and acceptable factor loadings (range 0.53
bullying outcome measures were estimated. 0.86) were found across all the four leadership
Furthermore, observed bullying was regressed styles. Furthermore, the measurement model re-
on self-reported bullying, in order to control the vealed signicant correlations between all four
prediction of observation of bullying for self- leadership styles and the two measures of bullying.
reported bullying. To evaluate the goodness of t For self-reported bullying the following correlations
of the models the RMSEA and CFI were were found: participative (r 5 0.26, po0.001),
applied.1 In the analysis, the dierent models NCP (r 5 0.46, po0.001), autocratic (r 5 0.39, po
were also compared by evaluating the change in 0.001) and laissez-faire (r 5 0.31, po0.001). The
w2 relative to the change in degrees of freedom. corresponding correlations for observed bullying
were participative (r 5 0.28, po0.001), NCP (r 5
0.39, po0.001), autocratic (r 5 0.41, po0.001) and
Results laissez-faire (r 5 0.30, po0.001) for the full sample.
When imposing the hypothesized paths from
Preliminary analyses the four leadership styles on the two measures of
Standard deviations and correlations for the bullying in the structural model, the t remained
observed variables are shown in Table 1. The acceptable. However, in the model the paths from
correlation coecients were in the range 0.10 participative and autocratic leadership to self-
0.78, and all coecients were signicant at the reported bullying, as well as the paths from
99% level. participative and NCP leadership to observed
Mean, standard deviation, alpha and inter- bullying, were not signicant. Consequently, a
correlations for the study constructs are pre- nal model constraining these paths to be zero
sented in Table 2. All scales show adequate was tested. In the nal model an insignicant
internal consistency with alphas varying from decrease in w2 by degrees of freedom (Dw2 5 6.32,
Ddf 5 4, p40.05) and a good t (CFI 5 0.94;
1
A value of RMSEA less than 0.05 indicates a good t, RMSEA 5 0.059) were found, supporting the
while values as high as 0.08 represent reasonable errors validity of the constraints. Structural paths and
of approximation in the population (Browne and factor correlations between the leadership styles
Cudeck, 1993). Originally a CFI over 0.90 was in the nal model are illustrated in Figure 2.
considered to be representative of a well-tting model
(Bentler, 1992); however, a revised cut-o value close to For self-reported bullying positive signicant
0.95 has more recently been advised (Bentler and Yuan, paths were found from both NCP and laissez-
1999). faire leadership (b 5 0.46, po0.001, b 5 0.06,

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


460

Table 1. Standard deviations and correlations between observed variables (total sample)
SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23.

1. Part1 1.163 0.621 1


2. Part2 1.245 0.610 0.645 1
3. Part3 1.208 0.590 0.643 0.633 1
4. Part4 1.242 0.519 0.569 0.554 0.564 1
5. Part5 1.196 0.527 0.555 0.576 0.612 0.659 1
6. Part6 1.052 0.644 0.637 0.640 0.683 0.584 0.614 1
7. Part7 1.219 0.625 0.777 0.656 0.685 0.596 0.604 0.713 1
8. Aut1 1.188 0.490 0.508 0.515 0.535 0.475 0.557 0.541 0.543 1
9. Aut2 1.107 0.296 0.335 0.315 0.371 0.297 0.361 0.376 0.358 0.451 1
10. Aut3 1.175 0.398 0.444 0.393 0.491 0.428 0.465 0.453 0.482 0.535 0.440 1
11. Aut4 1.172 0.274 0.282 0.319 0.300 0.302 0.417 0.336 0.319 0.455 0.373 0.365 1
12. Aut5 1.167 0.234 0.210 0.262 0.261 0.255 0.363 0.275 0.245 0.396 0.321 0.339 0.558 1
13. Aut6 1.314 0.328 0.361 0.349 0.428 0.321 0.404 0.423 0.403 0.474 0.396 0.423 0.372 0.364 1
14. Aut7 1.135 0.421 0.415 0.440 0.487 0.423 0.501 0.508 0.478 0.551 0.515 0.488 0.415 0.416 0.621 1
15. NCP1 1.292 0.273 0.262 0.307 0.361 0.245 0.361 0.350 0.307 0.452 0.338 0.317 0.387 0.373 0.416 0.429 1
16. NCP2 0.789 0.268 0.243 0.280 0.338 0.253 0.345 0.346 0.285 0.427 0.320 0.354 0.349 0.308 0.404 0.435 0.520 1
17. NCP3 0.685 0.322 0.302 0.355 0.411 0.297 0.410 0.409 0.349 0.493 0.353 0.348 0.441 0.404 0.449 0.489 0.614 0.601 1
18. NCP4 0.870 0.297 0.301 0.340 0.379 0.243 0.375 0.387 0.329 0.484 0.370 0.352 0.439 0.379 0.444 0.453 0.513 0.516 0.595 1
19. Laiz1 0.949 0.362 0.427 0.381 0.445 0.367 0.401 0.435 0.444 0.500 0.429 0.470 0.308 0.248 0.418 0.512 0.333 0.312 0.326 0.347 1
20. Laiz2 1.084 0.409 0.489 0.432 0.494 0.374 0.431 0.489 0.505 0.543 0.433 0.511 0.301 0.225 0.424 0.495 0.313 0.325 0.333 0.357 0.665 1
21. Laiz3 0.154 0.352 0.381 0.331 0.376 0.312 0.367 0.410 0.414 0.415 0.341 0.419 0.277 0.195 0.372 0.425 0.248 0.273 0.284 0.313 0.423 0.486 1
22. S.r. bul 4.748 0.117 0.101 0.134 0.130 0.091 0.136 0.152 0.115 0.185 0.142 0.128 0.128 0.125 0.170 0.183 0.198 0.252 0.202 0.207 0.162 0.141 0.109 1
23. Obs. bul 1.163 0.218 0.205 0.212 0.233 0.214 0.246 0.248 0.223 0.292 0.254 0.229 0.250 0.246 0.305 0.330 0.295 0.314 0.303 0.299 0.263 0.230 0.187 0.308

All correlations: po0.01.


H. Hoel et al.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Leadership and Bullying 461

Table 2. Correlations, means, standard deviations and alpha for study constructs

x SD a 1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

1. NCP leadership 1.51 0.67 0.83


2. Autocratic leadership 2.34 0.86 0.85 0.68*
3. Laissez-faire leadership 2.40 0.94 0.77 0.46* 0.66*
4. Participative leadership 3.11 0.97 0.92 0.48* 0.66* 0.61*
5. Observed bullying 8.48 4.75 0.92 0.37* 0.38* 0.28* 0.27*
6. Self-reported bullying 1.24 0.76 0.42* 0.35* 0.27* 0.25* 0.50*

*po0.001.

Table 3. Model t and model comparison for measurement model and structural models

Model x2 df CFI RMSEA Dx2 Ddf

Measurement model 4726.13 239 0.94 0.060


Hypothesized model 4726.13 239 0.94 0.060
Insignicant paths constrained to be zero 4732.45 243 0.94 0.061 6.32 ns 4

po0.001, respectively). Regarding observed bul- would seem that all these particular styles of
lying a positive signicant path was found from leadership are associated with perceptions of
autocratic leadership (b 5 0.31, po0.001) and a bullying, whether personally experienced and
negative signicant path from laissez-faire leader- self-reported, or as observed indirect experiences.
ship (b 5 0.08, po0.001) was revealed. Finally, Both observed bullying and self-reported bullying
a signicant positive path was found from were found to be correlated with the presence of
self-reported to observed bullying (b 5 0.40, autocratic, laissez-faire and what is referred to as
po0.001). In the nal model the explained NCP leadership, as well as the absence of
variances for self-reported and observed bullying participative leadership as embodied by ones
were 21% and 30%, respectively. immediate superior. Yet, an important nding in
our study is that observers and victims of bullying
still appear to have dierent perceptions of what
Discussion constitutes bullying. Thus, whilst non-contingent
punishment emerged as the single strongest
Bullying and harassment are often seen as predictor or risk factor of self-labelled bullying,
synonymous with abusive supervision and aggres- autocratic leadership was found to be the
sive management behaviour in Anglo-American strongest predictor for observed bullying. This
cultures. Yet, few studies have explicitly investi- suggests that targets identify bullying rst and
gated the relationship between the leadership foremost with aggression and punishment arbi-
styles of immediate superiors and perceptions of trarily meted out by managers. Where the
bullying and mistreatment among subordinates. negative and aggressive acts seemingly appear
Moreover, the lack of attention to negative out of the blue (Adams, 1992; Field, 1996;
aspects of leadership and bullying in the manage- Leymann, 1996), catching recipients o guard,
ment literature is striking, a decit which our such behaviour could be particularly dicult to
study is addressing by means of employing predict and comprehend, thus making it dicult
a representative sample of the British workforce. to protect and defend oneself. This nding also
In the study we hypothesized that workplace supports previous work by Kile (1990) and
bullying, as seen by both observers and targets, Ashforth (1994), who describe destructive leaders
is associated with the lack of participative leader- who are unpredictable and inconsistent in their
ship as well as the presence of autocratic leader- behaviour.
ship, laissez-faire leadership and non-contingent The fact that self-reported bullying was asso-
punishment. Based on the correlation analysis it ciated with an NCP leadership style does not

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


462 H. Hoel et al.

Autocratic
leadership

.80 .31

.81
NCP .46 Self reported (R2= .21)
leadership bullying
.76
.55 .40
.06

.54 Laissez-faire .08 Observed (R2= .30)


leadership bullying

.70
Significant paths
Participative Insignificant paths
leadership

Figure 2. Leadership styles and self-reported bullying: standardized regression weights, correlations and squared multiple correlations
(nal model)

necessarily preclude the possibility that the leader was at rst surprising as one might expect that
might also make use of other styles, possibly to observers would also consider such behaviour
accommodate dierent circumstances and situa- unfair and an abuse of power, particularly when
tions (Rayner, 1999), as indicated by the high acted out against someone in a subordinate
inter-correlations between the dierent styles. position with limited opportunity to defend
The view that leaders may be exible, and themselves. However, as bullying is a process
operate with a set of leadership styles in order where the protagonists share a past as well as the
to adapt to changing needs and situations that present, it is often dicult for bystanders to fully
might occur, is not new (see Hersey and make sense of the observed behaviour and the
Blanchard, 1969) and is in line with contingency meaning they might have to the actors (Einarsen
theories of leadership (e.g. Fiedler, 1978). But et al, 2003; Hoel, Rayner and Cooper, 1999). For
such exibility may also contain some potentially example, a hint or indirect reference to previous
negative aspects. For example, in the 1960s, mistakes, delivered in an overtly jovial or positive
Blake and Mouton (1985) gave a description in tone, could be perceived as a friendly act by
their managerial grid of a corresponding leader- observers, whilst the one for whom the comment
ship style (labelled 5.5) identied with leaders was meant would tend to judge it as sarcasm,
who constantly change focus and preferences, considering it a hostile or aggressive act. In
from being considerate (1.9) to uncaring (1.1), or addition, this behaviour may occur behind closed
with a focus solely on production rates (9.1) at doors, hindering any consistent observations by
the expense of well-being of subordinates. This colleagues. As social interactions between the
potential unpredictability seems to be very parties might take place in various spaces, with or
dicult for the targets of bullying in our study, without witnesses present, bystanders pictures of
as being at the receiving end of punishment the unfolding scenarios and the meaning they
delivered by leaders in a non-contingent manner attach to these events will at best be partial and
is the form of leadership most closely related to inaccurate. Therefore, where no visible intimida-
bullying and accords with reports of being in an tion or behaviour which clearly breaches ac-
abusive leadersubordinate relationship. cepted standards of behaviour is performed or
The fact that no association was found acted out, or where the behaviour is not
between observed bullying and NCP leadership considered unfair or unjust by others, observers
when controlling for the other leadership styles judgement might depart from that of targets.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Leadership and Bullying 463

This would particularly be the case where the dictable, it may be easier to guard against it
bullying is the result of planned or calculated whilst also receiving full support from colleagues.
behaviour, where the punitive and harassing This also raises the question whether observers
elements of the behaviour may be deliberately might overestimate the presence of bullying
hidden from observers who might be misled to associated with autocratic leadership.
interpret harassing behaviour as acts of concern Our ndings suggest that autocratic leaders
and interest (Adams, 1992). will not necessarily be perceived as bullies or
Several explanations may account for the fact associated with bullying by all people at all times.
that observers particularly associate bullying with But even autocratic, paternalistic leaders could be
autocratic leadership. For mature and self-con- perceived as bullies by their subordinates where
dent employees who may have an expectation of their behaviour is seen as stiing, overpowering
involvement and participation, an autocratic and overly controlling. Moreover, autocratic
style of leadership could easily be seen as over- leaders who are not themselves perceived as
controlling and inhibiting, although still possibly bullies can contribute to environments where
considered legitimate. Thus, whilst perceived as bullying becomes common.
negative and unwelcoming, such behaviour It is of course possible that the distinction
would not necessarily be seen as bullying by the between contingent and non-contingent punish-
actual target. According to Atwater et al. (1998), ment is blurred and subjective as argued by
in such circumstances, particularly when reect- Podsako and colleagues (1984), with recipients
ing the culture of the organization, such con- tending towards interpreting any repeated pun-
tingent punishment might be considered eective ishment as unfair and unreasonable and thus,
in organizational terms. A similar view is put from their point of view, non-contingent upon
forward by OReilly and Puer (1989) who argue their own behaviour. However, our results seem
that targets and observers might comply with to be more in line with the ndings of Atwater
managerial instructions in order to avoid similar et al. (1998), who argued that contingent punish-
responses in the future. However, where leaders ment and non-contingent punishment neither
ensure compliance by means of force or punish- share common predictors nor have the same
ment, their acts may be more likely to produce eects on perceptions of leader eectiveness.
perceptions of bullying, particularly where the Hence, in their view, contingent and non-
behaviour is repeated or systematically directed contingent punishment should not necessarily
towards third parties. The fact that autocratic be seen to be at opposite ends of the same
leadership in this study was operationalized or continuum.
measured by means of behavioural items, which Another problem for targets may be the fact
in terms of their potency (Foels et al., 2000) that leaders who resort to behaviour seen as
would be considered moderate by most standards bullying, in order to have their way or to ensure
and therefore likely to be judged as legitimate, compliance, do not have to be destructive in all
gives support to the idea that leader behaviour situations and in encounters with all subordi-
does not have to be viewed as tyrannical or nates. A destructive leader might also be very
downright abusive for it to be perceived as capable with regard to other organizational
bullying by observers. tasks and role fullment (Einarsen et al., 2002;
By contrast, whilst leaders application of force Skogstad, 1997). This provides a reasonable
and punishment for non-compliance is likely to explanation for the presence and even personal
be unwelcoming and unpleasant for recipients, success of such leaders in many organizations, as
such encounters might not necessarily be judged shown in a study by Rayner (1999) of university
as bullying as long as they are predictable and employees. In her study very few leaders could be
perceived as fair. Therefore, even if seen as harsh described as being either constructive or destruc-
by observers, punishment used by autocratic tive. Most leaders had subordinates who claimed
leaders may still be considered as legitimate by that they acted constructively, but at the same
those at the receiving end and their predictability time they also had subordinates who described
might make them easier to cope with. At least, as their behaviour as destructive and indeed aggres-
most colleagues will also see this behaviour as sive. Hence, for targets who perceive themselves
unacceptable and it appears to be rather pre- as being at the receiving end of unpredictable and

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


464 H. Hoel et al.

unfair behaviour of a punitive nature, such actual behaviour or action and thus less likely
factors might undermine their chance to draw that such third-party observations are labelled
attention to their case and build support from bullying.
fellow workers in order to le a successful
complaint. This would be true particularly at
Practical implications
the lower levels of the organizational hierarchy as
their organizational status may make them less This paper has shown that, in order to prevent
susceptible to fair treatment in the eyes of their workplace bullying, organizations need to ac-
colleagues, and thus more vulnerable to being the knowledge that particular styles of leadership
target of arbitrary or general aggression (Aquino, may be perceived as bullying by subordinates. In
2000; Hodson, Roscigno and Lopez, 2006). addition to clarifying managements position on
Although passive and unreceptive leadership, the issue, e.g. in a policy document, an under-
often labelled laissez-faire leadership, has fea- standing of leadership as a potential source of the
tured frequently in the leadership literature (e.g. problem needs to form part of any management
Bass, 1990; Blake and Mouton, 1985), few studies training programme. This is also a consequence
have actually investigated the possible negative of the fact that bullying only thrives when it is
eects on subordinates of this leadership style condoned, directly or indirectly, by management
(see Skogstad et al. (2007) for an exception). Yet, (Brodsky, 1976). Our study strongly suggests that
the present study shows that when leaders autocratic styles of leadership, where compliance
abdicate their responsibility for the work tasks, is secured by means of punishment and the
show little concern for their subordinates and are application of force, is likely to be deemed
never around to deal with problems, this may unacceptable and unjustied by targets as well
also bring about perceptions of bullying. Still, as observers. Yet, an important message from
our ndings indicate that this is only the case for this study is that unpredictable and unfair
targets, with a negative association between treatment is seen as a far more important
laissez-faire leadership and bullying emerging predictor of bullying by targets than by ob-
for observers, indicating that, when observers servers. This nding has a message both to those
see laissez-faire behaviour, to a lesser degree they involved with leader selection and training and to
tend to think that someone is bullied. Although those involved in complaints procedures and
the eects are modest, one may speculate about investigations. Regarding the latter, our ndings
these somewhat contradictory results for targets may indicate that many claims of bullying may
as opposed to observers. Accordingly, it is argued not be supported as observers view other leader-
that lack of response or non-responsiveness may ship styles as more problematic in this respect
also be perceived as a form of ostracism, where than do targets.
leaders punish subordinates by ignoring them In the same way as negative or destructive
and their needs (Einarsen et al., 2003). In this styles of leadership need to become a criterion in
respect, Skogstad and colleagues (2007) showed selection and promotion processes, and an
that laissez-faire leadership is actually a type of important part of the appraisal process, man-
destructive leader behaviour which demonstrates agers need to be encouraged to regularly assess
a systematic relationship with workplace stres- their own behaviour. Whilst those managers most
sors, bullying at work and psychological distress in need of changing their leadership style may be
among subordinates. unlikely to consider this a problem in the rst
By contrast, the fact that a negative associa- place, and thus need to be helped to change,
tion, albeit a modest one, emerged between this other managers would have a more open and
form of passive leadership and observed bullying self-critical approach to their own management
might be explained in a similar way as for NCP style. Some of them may be managers who are
leadership in that observers might fail to notice perceived as constructive, or who act in a
situations where leaders systematically avoid predominantly constructive way, but who revert
involvement with, or deliberately ignore the under intense pressure to destructive behaviour,
needs of, subordinates through their inaction. If with severe consequences for those directly
anything, one may argue that it would be more aected. By being aware of this problem, and
dicult to acknowledge patterns of inaction than having an understanding of the psychological

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Leadership and Bullying 465

processes associated with bullying observed in observers respectively are underestimating or


this study, organizations may also prevent overestimating levels of bullying.
accusations of bullying from being ignored or
not believed when apparently good managers are
in the ring line. Conclusions
However, there is one clear message coming
from both targets and observers alike, i.e. that By exploring the association between various
organizations must address the issue of passive or styles of leadership, on the one hand, and self-
laissez-faire styles of management, as doing reported and observed bullying, on the other, this
nothing may be potentially destructive in its study has made a contribution to an under-
own right (see also Skogstad et al., 2007). Thus, researched area of study. It has conrmed
even passive and not overtly aggressive leaders previous ndings which associate destructive
may be causing claims of bullying, or at least leadership, not only with lack of eectiveness in
laying the ground for bullying to occur among the execution of the leader role, but with
co-workers. damaging and possibly debilitating experiences
for subordinates. The study has also emphasized
the role of punishment in leadership, a key
Limitations ingredient in targets perception of being bullied
at work. Although it cannot be ruled out that
A major strength of the current study is its large, arbitrary use of punishment may reside in the
random and representative sample. However, perception of the targets, the ndings indicate
there are some important limitations. The study that managers might use punishment deliberately
is cross-sectional which militates against claims and for both personal and organizational gain.
about causality (Zapf, Dormann and Frese, Moreover, where punishment is administered
1996). We assume that the direction of the arbitrarily or in a way that is non-contingent
relationship is from leader behaviour to percep- upon the situation, its unpredictability makes it
tions of bullying, or at least that is how likely that it is labelled as bullying by the target.
subordinates perceive or construe such leader The fact that these perceptions are not
behaviour. However, it is also possible that the necessarily shared by observers must be under-
presence of bullying in the workplace might stood in the light of the nature of the bullying
inuence leader behaviour. Another possible process, where observers seldom have access to
limitation is the fact that the study is based on the same information as targets. Observers for
self-reports. Thus, this may give rise to common their part, and dierently from targets, associate
method variance which could have inated the bullying particularly with an autocratic style of
relationship between the variables (Podsako leadership, where application of force or punish-
et al., 2003). In the present study we were also ment may be deemed unacceptable and possibly
unable to separate out bullying behaviour on the as bullying. This could suggest that observers
part of leaders from similar behaviour by might draw conclusions about bullying which are
colleagues or by leaders in collusion with not necessarily shared by targets, possibly with
colleagues. However, in the context of the UK implications for their commitment to, and long-
bullies are overwhelmingly found to be leaders, term relationship with, the organization.
which was also the case in the present study, at Moreover, whilst active and consistent destruc-
least based on the targets reports. Furthermore, tive styles of leadership, such as an autocratic
although bullying seemingly is associated with leadership style, may be easier to identify and
particular styles of leadership, none of these condemn, passive destructive behaviour may at
styles should be considered as bullying per se as times be equally damaging. So far, we know
they will have dierent meanings to dierent much more about eective and constructive
people in dierent situations. Moreover, as has leadership than we do about destructive leader-
been demonstrated in this paper, targets and ship and more about active destructive leadership
observers might not share the same perspective than passive destructive leadership. Therefore,
on when particular styles of leadership are seen as destructive leadership behaviour and its out-
bullying, raising the question whether targets or comes should be examined in greater detail in

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


466 H. Hoel et al.

future studies. There is a strong need for a better mobbing pa norske arbeidsplasser. Universitetet i Bergen:
understanding of destructive leadership per se as FASH.
well as of theoretical models of leadership which Einarsen, S. and A. Skogstad (1996). Bullying at work:
epidemiological ndings in public and private organizations,
also contain destructive elements of leadership. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 5,
In order to understand how and why destructive pp. 185201.
leadership styles are used and how they are Einarsen, S., M. S. Aasland and A. Skogstad (2007).
perceived, future empirical studies must go Destructive leadership behaviour: a denition and concep-
beyond cross-sectional data, exploring these tual model, Leadership Quarterly, 18, pp. 207216.
Einarsen, S., B. I. Raknes and S. B. Matthiesen (1994).
problems as they arise and persist within the Bullying and harassment at work and their relationships to
given context. work environment quality: an exploratory study, European
Work and Organizational Psychologist, 4, pp. 381401.
Einarsen, S., A. Skogstad, M. S. Aasland and A. M. S. Bakken-
Lseth (2002). Destruktivt lederskap: Arsaker og konsek-
References venser. In A. Skogstad and S. Einarsen (eds), Ledelse pa
Godt og Vondt. Eektivitet og Trivsel, pp. 233254. Bergen:
Adams, A. (1992). Bullying at Work: How to Confront and Fagbokforlaget Bergen.
Overcome It. London: Virago. Einarsen, S., H. Hoel, D. Zapf and C. L. Cooper (2003).
Aquino, K. (2000). Structural and individual determinants of Bullying and Emotional Abuse: International Perspectives in
workplace victimization: the eects of hierarchical status and Research and Practice. London: Taylor and Francis.
conict management style, Journal of Management, 26, pp. Ekvall, G., M. Frankenhaeuser, E. Gunarsson and D. Parr
171193. (2002). Ledarstil ledarstress arbeidsklimat medarbetar-
Ashforth, B. (1994). Petty tyranny in organizations, Human reaktioner. Studier av kvinnelige och mannlige chefar och deras
Relations, 47, pp. 755778. medarbetare. Stockholm: Stochholms Universitet Faradet.
Atwater, L. E., S. D. Dionne, J. F. Camobreco, B. J. Avolio Felson, R. B. (1992). Kick em when theyre down: explana-
and A. Lau (1998). Individual attributes and leadership tions of the relationship between stress and interpersonal
style: predicting the use of punishment and its eects, aggression and violence, The Sociologist Quarterly, 33, pp.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, pp. 559576. 116.
Bass, B. M. (1990). From transactional to transformational Fiedler, F. E. (1978). The contingency model and the dynamics
leadership: learning to share a vision, Organizational of the leadership process. In L. Berkowitz (ed.), Advances in
Dynamics, 18, pp. 1932. Experimental Social Psychology, pp. 59112. New York:
Bass, B. M. and B. J. Avolio (1990). Developing transforma- Academic Press.
tional leadership: 1992 and beyond, Journal of European Field, T. (1996). Bully in Sight: How to Predict, Resist,
Industrial Training, 14, pp. 2127. Challenge and Combat Workplace Bullying. Wantage: Wessex
Bentler, P. M. (1992). On the t of models to covariances and Press.
methodology to the Bulletin, Psychological Bulletin, 112, pp. Foels, R., J. E. Driskell, B. Mullen and E. Salas (2000). The
400404. eects of demographic leadership on group member satisfac-
Bentler, P. M. and K. H. Yuan (1999). Structural equation tion, Small Group Research, 31, pp. 676701.
modelling with small samples: test statistics, Multivariate Frischer, J. and K. Larsson (2000). Laissez-faire in research
Behavioral Research, 34, pp. 181197. education an inquiry into a Swedish doctoral program,
Blake, R. R. and J. S. Mouton (1985). The Managerial Grid. Higher Education Policy, 13, pp. 131155.
Houston, TX: Gulf Publishing. Frost, P. J. (2004). Handling toxic emotions: new challenges
Brodsky, C. M. (1976). The Harassed Worker. Lexington, MA: for leaders and their organization, Organizational Dynamics,
Heath. 33, pp. 111127.
Browne, M. W. and R. Cudeck (1993). Alternative ways of Glas, L. and S. Einarsen (2006). Experienced aects in leader
assessing model t. In K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long (eds), subordinate relationships, Scandinavian Journal of Manage-
Testing Structural Equation Models, pp. 445455. Newbury ment, 22, pp. 4973.
Park, CA: Sage. Glas, L., K. Ekerholt, S. Barman and S. Einarsen (2006). The
Burnes, B. (2007). Kurt Lewin and the Harwood studies, instrumentality of emotions in leadersubordinate relation-
Journal of Applied Behavioural Science, 43, pp. 213231. ships, International Journal of Work Organization and
Di Martino, V., H. Hoel and C. L. Cooper (2003). Preventing Emotion, 1, pp. 255276.
Violence and Harassment in the Workplace. European Hersey, P. and K. H. Blanchard (1969). Management and
Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Organizational Behaviour. Englewood Clis, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Conditions, Luxembourg: Oce for Ocial Publications of Hodson, R., V. J. Roscigno and S. H. Lopez (2006). Chaos and
the European Community. the abuse of power: workplace bullying organizational and
Einarsen, S. (1996). Bullying and harassment at work interactional context, Work and Occupations, 33, pp. 382
epidemiological and psychological aspects. Unpublished 416.
PhD thesis, Department of Psychological Science, University Hoel, H. and C. L. Cooper (2000). Destructive conict and
of Bergen. bullying at work. Unpublished report, Manchester School of
Einarsen, S. and B. I. Raknes (1991). Mobbing i Arbeidslivet: En Management, University of Manchester Institute of Science
underskelse av forekomst og helsemessige konsekvenser av and Technology.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


Leadership and Bullying 467

Hoel, H. and D. Salin (2003). Organisational antecedents of Northouse, P. C. (2004). Leadership: Theory and Practice.
workplace bullying. In S. Einarsen, H. Hoel, D. Zapf and C. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
L. Cooper (eds), Bullying and Emotional Abuse in the Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric Theory. New York:
Workplace: International Perspectives in Research and Prac- McGraw-Hill.
tice, pp. 203218. London: Taylor and Francis. OMoore, M., E. Seigne, L. McGuire and M. Smith (1998).
Hoel, H., C. L. Cooper and B. Faragher (2001). The experience Victims of bullying at work in Ireland, Journal of
of bullying in Great Britain: the impact of organisational Occupational Health and Safety Australia and New Zealand,
culture, European Journal of Work and Organizational 14, pp. 569574.
Psychology, 10, pp. 443456. OReilly, C. and S. Puer (1989). The impact of reward and
Hoel, H., S. Einarsen and C. L. Cooper (2003). Organisational punishment in a social context: a laboratory and eld
eects of bullying. In S. Einarsen, H. Hoel, D. Zapf and C. experiment, Journal of Occupational Psychology, 62, pp.
L. Cooper (eds), Bullying and Emotional Abuse in the 4153.
Workplace: International Perspectives in Research and Prac- Packard, S. H. and D. R. Kauppi (1999). Rehabilitation
tice, pp. 145161. London: Taylor and Francis. agency leadership style: impact on subordinates job
Hoel, H., B. Faragher and C. L. Cooper (2004). Bullying is satisfaction, Rehabilitation Counselling Bulletin, 43, pp.
detrimental to health, but all bullying behaviours are not 511.
equally damaging, British Journal of Guidance and Counsel- Paoli, P. and D. Merille (2001). Third European Survey on
ling, 32, pp. 367387. Working Conditions 2000. European Foundation for the
Hoel, H., C. Rayner and C. L. Cooper (1999). Workplace Improvement of Living and Working Conditions, Luxem-
bullying. In C. L. Cooper and I. T. Robertson (eds), bourg: Oce for Ocial Publications of the European
International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psy- Community.
chology, pp. 195230. Chichester: Wiley. Pearson, C. M., L. M. Andersson and C. L. Porath (2005).
Hogan, R. (1994). Trouble at the top: causes and consequences Workplace incivility. In F. S. Fox and P. E. Spector (eds),
of managerial incompetence, Consulting Psychology Journal, Counterproductive Work Behaviour, pp. 177200. Washing-
46, pp. 915. ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
Hogan, R. and J. Hogan (2001). Assessing leadership: a view Peng, T. K. and M. F. Peterson (1998). Contingent and
from the dark side, International Journal of Selection and noncontingent social rewards and punishment from leaders:
Assessment, 9, pp. 4051. do US and Japanese subordinates make comparable distinc-
Hogan, R., R. Raskin and D. Fazzini (1990). The dark side of tions?, International Business Review, 7, pp. 6987.
charisma. In K. E. Clark and M. B. Clark (eds), Measures of Podsako, P. M., W. D. Todor and R. Skov (1982). Eects of
Leadership, pp. 343354. West Orange, NJ: Leadership leader performance contingent and non-contingent reward
Library of America. and punishment behaviors on subordinate performance and
Johnson, N. J. and T. Klee (2007). Passive-aggressive behavior satisfaction, Academy of Management Journal, 25, pp. 812
and leadership styles in organizations, Journal of Leadership 821.
and Organizational Studies, 14, pp. 130142. Podsako, P. M., W. D. Todor, R. A. Grover and V. L. Huber
Keashly, L. (1998). Emotional abuse in the workplace: (1984). Situational moderators of leader reward and punish-
conceptual and empirical issues, Journal of Emotional Abuse, ment behavior: fact or ction?, Organizational Behavior and
1, pp. 85117. Human Performance, 34, pp. 2163.
Keashly, L. and K. Jagatic (2000). The nature, extent and Podsako, P. M., S. B. MacKenzie, J. Y. Lee and N. Podsako
impact of emotional abuse in the workplace: results of a (2003). Common method biases in behavioural research: a
statewide survey. Paper presented at the Academy of critical review of the literature and recommended remedies,
Management Conference, Toronto, 58 August. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, pp. 879903.
Kets de Vries, M. F. R. and D. Miller (1984). The Neurotic Price Spratlen, L. (1995). Interpersonal conict which includes
Organization. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. mistreatment in a university workplace, Violence and
Kile, S. M. (1990). Helsefarlige Ledere - og Medarbeidere. Oslo: Victims, 10, pp. 285297.
Hjemmets bokforlag. Rayner, C. (1999). Bullying in the workplace. Unpublished
Lewin, K., R. Lippitt and R. White (1939). Patterns of doctoral thesis, University of Manchester Institute of Science
aggressive behaviour in experimentally created social and Technology.
climates, Journal of Social Psychology, 10, pp. 271299. Rayner, C. and C. L. Cooper (2003). The black hole in
Leymann, H. (1990). Mobbing and psychological terror at bullying at work research, International Journal of
workplaces, Violence and Victims, 5, pp. 119126. Management Decision Making, 4, pp. 4764.
Leymann, H. (1996). The content and development of mobbing Rayner, C., H. Hoel and C. L. Cooper (2002). Workplace
at work, European Journal of Work and Organizational Bullying. What We Know, Who is to Blame, and What Can
Psychology, 5, pp. 165184. We Do? London: Taylor and Francis.
Merchant, V. and H. Hoel (2003). Investigating complaints of Sheehan, M., P. McCarthy, M. Barker and M. Henderson
bullying. In S. Einarsen, H. Hoel, D. Zapf and C. L. Cooper (2001). A model for assessing the impact and costs of
(eds), Bullying and Emotional Abuse in the Workplace: workplace bullying. Paper presented at the Standing
International Perspectives in Research and Practice, pp. 259 Conference on Organizational Symbolism, Trinity College
269. London: Taylor and Francis. Dublin.
Niedl, K. (1996). Mobbing and well-being: economic and Skogstad, A. (1997). Eects of leadership behaviour on job
personnel development implications, European Journal of satisfaction, health and eciency. Doctoral thesis, Faculty
Work and Organizational Psychology, 5, pp. 203214. of Psychology, University of Bergen.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.


468 H. Hoel et al.

Skogstad, A., S. Einarsen, T. Torsheim, M. S. Aasland and Yukl, G. (2000). Leadership in Organizations, 4th edn. Engle-
H. Hetland (2007). The destructiveness of laissez-faire wood Clis, NJ: Prentice Hall.
behaviour, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12, Zapf, D. (1999). Organizational, work group related and
pp. 8092. personal causes of mobbing/bullying at work, International
Tepper, B. J. (2000). Consequences of abusive supervision, Journal of Manpower, 20, pp. 7085.
Academy of Management Journal, 43, pp. 178190. Zapf, D., C. Dormann and M. Frese (1996). Longitudinal
Unison (1997). Unisons Members Experience of Bullying at studies in organizational stress research: a review
Work. London: Unison. of the literature with reference to methodological
Vartia, M. (1996). The sources of bullying: psychological issues, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1, pp.
work environment and organisational climate, European 145169.
Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 5, pp. Zapf, D., S. Einarsen, H. Hoel and M. Vartia (2003). Empirical
203214. ndings on bullying in the workplace. In S. Einarsen, H.
Vartia, M. (2001). Consequences of workplace bullying with Hoel, D. Zapf and C. L. Cooper (eds), Bullying and
respect to well-being of its targets and the observers of Emotional Abuse in the Workplace: International Perspectives
bullying, Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and in Research and Practice, pp. 103126. London: Taylor and
Health, 27, pp. 6369. Francis.

Helge Hoel (PhD) is a lecturer at Manchester Business School, the University of Manchester, UK.
Together with Professor Cary Cooper he carried out the rst nationwide study of bullying in Britain.
He has written and contributed to a number of articles, books and reports in the area of bullying,
violence and harassment. He has also acted as adviser to the Norwegian Government in its campaign
against bullying in the workplace.

Lars Glas (PhD) is Associate Professor at the University of Bergen and a former director of the
Norwegian Institute of Group and Organizational Psychology (NORIGO). He has been a
consultant to private and public organizations in Europe for more than 15 years and has published
articles and book-chapters in the areas of leadership and emotions, leadership development,
consultancy and workplace bullying.

Jrn Hetland (PhD) is Associate Professor at the Faculty of Psychology, University of Bergen. After
completing his PhD in 1998 he has worked at the Research Centre for Health Promotion at the
University of Bergen specializing in the areas of adolescent health, smoking prevention and
workplace bullying.

Cary L. Cooper is Professor of Organizational Psychology and Health at Lancaster University


Management School, UK. He is President of the British Association for Counselling and
Psychotherapy and an Honorary Fellow of the Royal College of Physicians. In 2001 he was awarded
the CBE for his work in the eld of occupational health.

Stale Einarsen is Professor of Work and Organizational Psychology at the University of Bergen. He
has published extensively on issues related to leadership, bullying/harassment and creativity. He also
has extensive experience as an organizational consultant in the area of conict management and
leadership training. He has acted as adviser to the Norwegian Government in its campaign against
bullying in the workplace.

r 2009 British Academy of Management.

Potrebbero piacerti anche