Sei sulla pagina 1di 19

Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.

org/wiki/Disgust

Disgust
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Disgust is an emotional response of revulsion to something


considered offensive, distasteful, or unpleasant. In The
Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, Charles
Darwin wrote that disgust is a sensation that refers to something
revolting. Disgust is experienced primarily in relation to the
sense of taste (either perceived or imagined), and secondarily to
anything which causes a similar feeling by sense of smell,
touch, or vision. Musically sensitive people may even be
disgusted by the cacophony of inharmonious sounds. Research
continually has proven a relationship between disgust and
anxiety disorders such as arachnophobia, blood-injection-injury
type phobias, and contamination fear related obsessive
compulsive disorder (also known as OCD).[1]

Disgust is one of the basic emotions of Robert Plutchik's theory


Adriaen Brouwer's The Bitter
of emotions and has been studied extensively by Paul Rozin. It
Tonic, depicting a man's response
invokes a characteristic facial expression, one of Paul Ekman's
of disgust to a beverage
six universal facial expressions of emotion. Unlike the emotions
of fear, anger, and sadness, disgust is associated with a decrease
in heart rate.[2]

Contents
1 Evolutionary significance
1.1 Domains of disgust
1.2 Gender differences
2 Body language
2.1 Children's reactions to a face showing disgust
3 Cultural differences
4 Neural basis
4.1 Insula
5 Disorders
5.1 Huntington's disease
5.2 Major depressive disorder
5.3 Obsessive-compulsive disorder
6 Animal research
7 Morality
7.1 Political orientation

1 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

7.2 Self-disgust
8 Functions
9 Political and legal aspects of disgust
10 The Hydra's Tale: Imagining Disgust
11 See also
12 References
13 Bibliography
14 External links

Evolutionary significance
It is believed that the emotion of disgust has evolved as a response to offensive foods that may
cause harm to the organism.[3] A common example of this is found in human beings who show
disgust reactions to mouldy milk or contaminated meat. Disgust appears to be triggered by objects
or people who possess attributes that signify disease.[4]

Self-report and behavioural studies found that disgust elicitors include:

body products (feces, urine, vomit, sexual fluids, saliva, and mucus);
foods (spoiled foods);
animals (fleas, ticks, lice, cockroaches, worms, flies, rats, and mice);
hygiene (visible dirt and "inappropriate" acts [e.g., using an unsterilized surgical instrument]);
body envelope violations (blood, gore, and mutilation);
death (dead bodies and organic decay);
visible signs of infection[5]

The above-mentioned main disgust stimuli are similar to one another in the sense that they can all
potentially transmit infections, and are the most common referenced elicitors of disgust cross-
culturally.[6] Because of this, disgust is believed to have evolved as a component of a behavioral
immune system in which the body attempts to avoid disease-carrying pathogens in preference to
fighting them after they have entered the body. This behavioral immune system has been found to
make sweeping generalizations because "it is more costly to perceive a sick person as healthy than
to perceive a healthy person as sickly".[7] Researchers have found that sensitivity to disgust is
negatively correlated to aggression because feelings of disgust typically bring about a need to
withdraw while aggression results in a need to approach.[8] This can be explained in terms of each
of the types of disgust. For those especially sensitive to moral disgust, they would want to be less
aggressive because they want to avoid hurting others. Those especially sensitive to pathogen
disgust might be motivated by a desire to avoid the possibility of an open wound on the victim of
the aggression; however, for those sensitive to sexual disgust, some sexual object must be present
for them to be especially avoidant of aggression.[8] Based on these findings, disgust may be used as
an emotional tool to decrease aggression in individuals. Disgust may produce specific autonomic

2 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

responses, such as reduced blood pressure, lowered heart-rate and decreased skin conductance
along with changes in respiratory behaviour.[9]

Research has also found that people who are more sensitive to disgust tend to find their own
in-group more attractive and tend to have more negative attitudes toward other groups.[10] This
may be explained by assuming that people begin to associate outsiders and foreigners with disease
and danger while simultaneously associating health, freedom from disease, and safety with people
similar to themselves.

Taking a further look into hygiene, disgust was the strongest


predictor of negative attitudes toward obese individuals. A
disgust reaction to obese individuals was also connected with
views of moral values.[11]

Domains of disgust

Tybur, et al., outlines three domains of disgust: pathogen


disgust, which "motivates the avoidance of infectious A woman expressing disgust.
microorganisms"; sexual disgust, "which motivates the
avoidance of [dangerous] sexual partners and behaviors"; and
moral disgust, which motivates people to avoid breaking social norms. Disgust may have an
important role in certain forms of morality.[12]

Pathogen disgust arises from a desire to survive and, ultimately, a fear of death. He compares it to a
"behavioral immune system" that is the 'first line of defense' against potentially deadly agents such
as dead bodies, rotting food, and vomit.[13]

Sexual disgust arises from a desire to avoid "biologically costly mates" and a consideration of the
consequences of certain reproductive choices. The two primary considerations are intrinsic quality
(e.g., body symmetry, facial attractiveness, etc.) and genetic compatibility (e.g., avoidance of
inbreeding such as the incest taboo).[14]

Moral disgust "pertains to social transgressions" and includes behaviors such as lying, theft,
murder, and rape. Unlike the other two domains, moral disgust "motivates avoidance of social
relationships with norm-violating individuals" because those relationships threaten group
cohesion.[15]

Gender differences

Women generally report greater disgust than men, especially regarding sexual disgust or turn-offs
which have been argued to be consistent with women being more selective regarding sex for
evolutionary reasons.[16]

Sensitivity to disgust rises during pregnancy, along with levels of the hormone progesterone.

3 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

Scientists have conjectured that pregnancy requires the mother to "dial down" her immune system
so that the developing embryo won't be attacked. To protect the mother, this lowered immune
system is then compensated by a heightened sense of disgust.[17]

Because disgust is an emotion with physical responses to undesirable or dirty situations, studies
have proven there are cardiovascular and respiratory changes while experiencing the emotion of
disgust.[18]

As mentioned earlier, women experience disgust more prominently than men. This is reflected in a
study about dental phobia. A dental phobia comes from experiencing disgust when thinking about
the dentist and all that entails. 4.6 percent of women compared to 2.7 percent of men find the
dentist disgusting.[19]

Body language
In a series of significant studies by Paul Ekman in the 1970s, it was discovered that facial
expressions of emotion are not culturally determined, but universal across human cultures and thus
likely to be biological in origin.[20] The facial expression of disgust was found to be one of these
facial expressions. This characteristic facial expression includes slightly narrowed brows, a curled
upper lip, wrinkling of the nose and visible protrusions of the tongue, although different elicitors
may produce different forms of this expression.[21] It was found that the facial expression of disgust
is readily recognizable across cultures.[22] This facial expression is also produced in blind
individuals and is correctly interpreted by individuals born deaf.[4] This evidence indicates an
innate biological basis for the expression and recognition of disgust. The recognition of disgust is
also important among species as it has been found that when an individual sees a conspecific
looking disgusted after tasting a particular food, he or she automatically infers that the food is bad
and should not be eaten.[3] This evidence suggests that disgust is experienced and recognized
almost universally and strongly implicates its evolutionary significance.

Facial feedback has also been implicated in the expression of disgust. That is, the making of the
facial expression of disgust leads to an increased feeling of disgust. This can occur if the person just
wrinkles one's nose without awareness that they are making a disgust expression.[23]

The mirror-neuron matching system found in monkeys and humans is a proposed explanation for
such recognition, and shows that our internal representation of actions is triggered during the
observation of anothers actions.[24] It has been demonstrated that a similar mechanism may apply
to emotions. Seeing someone else's facial emotional expressions triggers the neural activity that
would relate to our own experience of the same emotion.[25] This points to the universality, as well
as survival value of the emotion of disgust.

Children's reactions to a face showing disgust

At a very young age, children are able to identify different, basic facial emotions. If a parent makes

4 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

a negative face and a positive emotional face toward two different toys, a child as young as five
months would avoid the toy associated with a negative face. Young children tend to associate a face
showing disgust with anger instead of being able to identify the difference. Adults, however, are
able to make the distinction. The age of understanding seems to be around ten years old.[26]

Cultural differences
Because disgust is partially a result of social conditioning, there are differences among different
cultures in the objects of disgust. Americans "are more likely to link feelings of disgust to actions
that limit a persons rights or degrade a persons dignity" while Japanese people "are more likely to
link feelings of disgust to actions that frustrate their integration into the social world".[27]

Another contrasting example is with mothers from the Manchu ethnic group, who used to show
affection for their children by performing fellatio on their male babies since it was not considered a
sexual act, while the Manchu regarded public kissing with revulsion, which was considered sexual.
[28][29][30][31][32]

Disgust is one of the basic emotions recognizable across multiple cultures and is a response to
something revolting typically involving taste or sight. Though different cultures find different
things disgusting, the reaction to the grotesque things remains the same throughout each culture;
people and their emotional reactions in the realm of disgust remain the same.[33]

Neural basis
The scientific attempts to map specific emotions onto underlying neural substrates dates back to the
first half of the 20th century. However, it was not until the mid-1990s when it was recognized that
six basic emotions, including disgust, were each related to a specific neural structure and therefore
considered to be a part of the clinical neurosciences.[25] Functional MRI experiments have revealed
that the anterior insula in the brain is particularly active when experiencing disgust, when being
exposed to offensive tastes, and when viewing facial expressions of disgust.[34] The research has
supported that there are independent neural systems in the brain, each handling a specific basic
emotion.[3] Specifically, f-MRI studies have provided evidence for the activation of the insula in
disgust recognition, as well as visceral changes in disgust reactions such as the feeling of nausea.[3]
The importance of disgust recognition and the visceral reaction of "feeling disgusted" is evident
when considering the survival of organisms, and the evolutionary benefit of avoiding
contamination.[3]

Insula

The insula (or insular cortex), is the main neural structure involved in the emotion of disgust.
[3][25][35] The insula has been shown by several studies to be the main neural correlate of the feeling
of disgust both in humans and in macaque monkeys. The insula is activated by unpleasant tastes,

5 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

smells, and the visual recognition of disgust in conspecific


organisms.[3]

The anterior insula is an olfactory and gustatory center that


controls visceral sensations and the related autonomic
responses.[3] It also receives visual information from the
anterior portion of the ventral superior temporal cortex, where
The insula of the left side,
cells have been found to respond to the sight of faces.[36]
exposed by removing the
The posterior insula is characterized by connections with opercula. From Henry Vandyke
auditory, somatosensory, and premotor areas, and is not related Carter - Henry Gray (1918)
Anatomy of the Human Body
to the olfactory or gustatory modalities.[3]
Bartleby.com: Gray's Anatomy,
The fact that the insula is necessary for our ability to feel and Plate 731
recognize the emotion of disgust is further supported by
neuropsychological studies. Both Calder (2000) and Adolphs (2003) showed that lesions on the
anterior insula lead to deficits in the experience of disgust and recognizing facial expressions of
disgust in others.[35][37] The patients also reported having reduced sensations of disgust themselves.
Furthermore, electrical stimulation of the anterior insula conducted during neurosurgery triggered
nausea, the feeling of wanting to throw up and uneasiness in the stomach. Finally, electrically
stimulating the anterior insula through implanted electrodes produced sensations in the throat and
mouth that were "difficult to stand".[3] These findings demonstrate the role of the insula in
transforming unpleasant sensory input into physiological reactions, and the associated feeling of
disgust.[3]

In a study by Stark & colleagues (2007), sixty-six participants took part in an event-related fMRI
analysis. 50 pictures were presented for four seconds and participants rated each picture on the
dimensions disgust and fear. The results indicated that both fear and disgust stimulus categories
resulted in activations in the occipital cortex, prefrontal cortex and in the amygdala.[38] However,
insula activation was only significantly correlated with ratings of disgust, pointing to a specific role
of this brain structure in the processing of disgust. In another intensive fMRI study by Wicker &
colleagues (2003), disgust reactions to visual and olfactory stimuli were compared. The study
consisted of four runs and in the visual runs participants viewed movies of individuals smelling the
contents of a glass (conditions: disgusting, pleasant, or neutral) and expressing the facial
expressions of the respective emotions.[3] In the olfactory runs, the same participants inhaled
disgusting or pleasant odorants. It was found that the anterior insula was activated in both the
observation of disgusted facial expressions (visual condition) and during the emotion of disgust
evoked by unpleasant odors (olfactory condition).[3] These findings demonstrate that observing
someone else's facial expression of disgust seems to automatically retrieve a neural representation
of disgust.[3] Furthermore, they emphasize the role of the insula in feelings of disgust across the
senses.

One particular neuropsychological study focused on patient NK who was diagnosed with a left

6 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

hemisphere infarction involving the insula, internal capsule, putamen and globus pallidus. NKs
neural damage included the insula and putamen and it was found that NKs overall response to
disgust-inducing stimuli was significantly lower than that of controls.[35] The patient showed a
reduction in disgust-response on eight categories including food, animals, body products, envelope
violation and death.[35] Moreover, NK incorrectly categorized disgust facial expressions as anger.
The results of this study support the idea that NK suffered damage to a system involved in
recognizing social signals of disgust, due to a damaged insula caused by neurodegeneration.[35]

Disorders
Huntington's disease

Many patients suffering from Huntington's disease, a genetically transmitted progressive


neurodegenerative disease, are unable to recognize expressions of disgust in others and also don't
show reactions of disgust to foul odors or tastes.[39] The inability to recognize expressions of
disgust appears in carriers of the Huntington gene before other symptoms appear.[40] People with
Huntington's disease are impaired at recognition of anger and fear, and experience a notably severe
problem with disgust recognition.[41]

Major depressive disorder

Patients suffering from major depression have been found to display greater brain activation to
facial expressions of disgust.[42]

Obsessive-compulsive disorder

The emotion of disgust may have an important role in understanding the neurobiology of obsessive-
compulsive disorder (OCD), particularly in those with contamination preoccupations.[43] In a study
by Shapira & colleagues (2003), eight OCD subjects with contamination preoccupations and eight
healthy volunteers viewed pictures from the International Affective Picture System during f-MRI
scans. OCD subjects showed significantly greater neural responses to disgust-invoking images,
specifically in the right insula.[44] Furthermore, Sprengelmeyer (1997) found that the brain
activation associated with disgust included the insula and part of the gustatory cortex that processes
unpleasant tastes and smells. OCD subjects and healthy volunteers showed activation patterns in
response to disgust pictures that differed significantly at the right insula. In contrast, the two groups
were similar in their response to threat-inducing pictures, with no significant group differences at
any site.[45]

Animal research
With respect to studies using rats, prior research of signs of a conditioned disgust response have

7 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

been experimentally verified by Grill and Norgren (1978) who developed a systematic test to assess
palatability. The Taste Reactivity (TR) test has thus become a standard tool in measuring disgust
response.[46] When given a stimulus intraorally which had been previously paired with a nausea-
inducing substance, rats will show conditioned disgust reactions. "Gaping" in rats is the most
dominant conditioned disgust reaction and the muscles used in this response mimic those used in
species capable of vomiting.[47] Recent studies have shown that treatments that reduced serotonin
availability or that activate the endocannabinoid system can interfere with the expression of a
conditioned disgust reaction in rats. These researchers showed that as nausea produced conditioned
disgust reactions, by administering the rats with an antinausea treatment they could prevent toxin-
induced conditioned disgust reactions. Furthermore, in looking at the different disgust and vomiting
reactions between rats and shrews the authors showed that these reactions (particularly vomiting)
play a crucial role in the associative processes that govern food selection across species.[48]

In discussing specific neural locations of disgust, research has shown that forebrain mechanisms are
necessary for rats to acquire conditioned disgust for a specific emetic (vomit-inducing) substance
(such as lithium chloride).[49] Other studies have shown that lesions to the area postrema[50] and the
parabrachial nucleus of the pons[51] but not the nucleus of the solitary tract[51] prevented
conditioned disgust. Moreover, lesions of the dorsal and medial raphe nuclei (depleting forebrain
serotonin) prevented the establishment of lithium chloride-induced conditioned disgust.[52]

Morality
Although disgust was first thought to be a motivation for humans to only physical contaminants, it
has since been expanded to apply to moral and social moral contaminants as well. The similarities
between these types of disgust can especially be seen in the way people react to the contaminants.
For example, if someone stumbles upon a pool of vomit, he/she will do whatever possible to put as
much distance between himself/herself and the vomit as possible, which can include pinching the
nose, closing the eyes, or running away. Similarly, when a group experiences someone who cheats,
murders, or rapes another member of the group, its reaction is to shun or expel that person from the
group.[53]

Jones & Fitness (2008)[53] coined the term "moral hypervigilance" to describe the phenomenon that
individuals who are prone to physical disgust will also be prone to moral disgust. The link between
physical disgust and moral disgust can be seen in the United States where criminals are often
referred to as "slime" or "scum" and criminal activity as "stinking" or being "fishy". Furthermore,
people often try to block out the stimuli of morally repulsive images in much the same way that
they would block out the stimuli of a physically repulsive image. When people see an image of rape
or murder, they often turn their heads away to inhibit the incoming visual stimuli from the
photograph just like they would if they saw a decomposing body.

Moral judgments can be traditionally defined or thought of as directed by standards such as


impartiality and respect towards others for their well-being. From more recent theoretical and
empirical information, it can be suggested that morality may be guided by basic affective processes.

8 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

Jonathan Haidt proposed that ones instant judgments about morality are experienced as a "flash of
intuition" and that these affective perceptions operate rapidly, associatively, and outside of
consciousness.[54] From this, moral intuitions are believed to be stimulated prior to conscious moral
cognitions which correlates with having a greater influence on moral judgments.[54]

Research suggests that the experience of disgust can alter moral judgments. Many studies have
focused on the average change in behavior across participants, with some studies indicating disgust
stimuli intensifies the severity of moral judgments.[55] However, additional studies have found the
reverse effect,[56] and recent studies have suggested that the average effect of disgust on moral
judgments is small or absent.[57][58] Potentially reconciling these effects, a study recently indicated
that the direction and size of the effect of disgust stimuli on moral judgment is dependent on an
individual's sensitivity to disgust.[59]

The effect also seems to be limited to a certain aspect of morality. Horberg et al. found that disgust
plays a role in the development and intensification of moral judgments of purity in particular.[60] In
other words, the feeling of disgust is often associated with a feeling that some image of what is
pure has been violated. For example, a vegetarian might feel disgust after seeing another person
eating meat because he/she has a view of vegetarianism as the pure state-of-being. When this state-
of-being is violated, the vegetarian feels disgust. Furthermore, disgust appears to be uniquely
associated with purity judgments, not with what is just/unjust or what is harmful/caregiving, while
other emotions such as fear, anger, and sadness are "unrelated to moral judgments of purity".[61]

Some other research suggests that an individuals level of disgust sensitivity is due to their
particular experience of disgust.[54] Ones disgust sensitivity can be either high or low. The higher
ones disgust sensitivity is, the greater the tendency to make stricter moral judgments.[54] Disgust
sensitivity can also relate to various aspects of moral values, which can have a negative or positive
impact. For example, Disgust sensitivity is associated with moral hypervigilance, which means
people who have higher disgust sensitivity are more likely to think that other people who are
suspects of a crime are more guilty. They also associate them as being morally evil and criminal,
thus endorsing them to harsher punishment in the setting of a court.

Disgust is also theorized as an evaluative emotion that can control moral behavior.[54] When one
experiences disgust, this emotion might signal that certain behaviors, objects, or people are to be
avoided in order to preserve their purity. Research has established that when the idea or concept of
cleanliness is made salient then people make less severe moral judgments of others.[54] From this
particular finding, it can be suggested that this reduces the experience of disgust and the ensuing
threat of psychological impurity diminishes the apparent severity of moral transgressions.[62]

Political orientation

In one study, people of differing political persuasions were shown disgusting images in a brain
scanner. In conservatives, the basal ganglia and amygdala and several other regions showed
increased activity, while in liberals other regions of the brain increased in activity. Both groups

9 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

reported similar conscious reactions to the images. The difference in activity pattern was large: the
reaction to a single image could predict a person's political leanings with 95% accuracy.[63]

Self-disgust

Although limited research has been done on self-disgust, one study found that self-disgust and
severity of moral judgments were negatively correlated.[64] This is in contrast to findings related to
disgust, which typically results in harsher judgments of transgressions. This implies that disgust
directed towards the self functions very differently from disgust directed towards other people or
objects.[64] Self-disgust "may reflect a pervasive condition of self-loathing that makes it difficult to
assign deserving punishment to others".[64] In other words, those who feel self-disgust cannot
easily condemn others to punishment because they feel that they may also be deserving of
punishment.

Functions
The emotion of disgust can be described to serve as an affective mechanism following occurrences
of negative social value, provoking repulsion, and desire for social distance.[65] The origin of
disgust can be defined by motivating the avoidance of offensive things, and in the context of a
social environment, it can become an instrument of social avoidance.[65] An example of disgust in
action can be found from the Bible in the book of Leviticus. Leviticus includes direct
commandments from God to avoid disgust causing individuals, which included people who were
sexually immoral and those who had leprosy.[65] Disgust is also known to have originally evolved
as a response to unpleasant food that may have been carriers of disease.[65] As an affective
instrument for reducing motivations for social interaction, disgust can be anticipated to interfere
with dehumanization or the maltreatment of persons as less than human.[65] Research was
performed which conducted several functional magnetic resonance images (fMRI) in which
participants viewed images of individuals from stigmatized groups that were associated with
disgust, which were drug addicts and homeless people.[65] What the study found was that people
were not inclined in making inferences about the mental conditions of these particular disgust
inducing groups.[65] Therefore, examining images of homeless people and drug addicts caused
disgust in the response of the people who participated with this study.[65] This study coincides with
disgust following the law of contagion, which explains that contact with disgusting material renders
one disgusting.[65] Disgust can be applied towards people and can function as maltreatment towards
another human being. Disgust can exclude people from being a part of a clique by leading to the
view that they are merely less than human. An example of this is if groups were to avoid people
from outside of their own particular group. Some researchers have distinguished between two
different forms of dehumanization. The first form is the denial of uniquely human traits, examples
include: products of culture and modification.[65] The second form is the denial of human nature,
examples include: emotionality and personality.[65] Failure to attribute distinctively human traits to
a group leads to animalistic dehumanization, which defines the object group or individual as

10 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

savage, crude, and similar to animals.[65] These forms of dehumanization have clear connections to
disgust.[65] Researchers have proposed that many disgust elicitors are disgusting because they are
reminders that humans are not diverse from other creatures. [65] With the aid of disgust, animalistic
dehumanization directly reduces ones moral concerns towards excluding members from the outer
group.[65] Disgust can be a cause and consequence of dehumanization.[65] Animalistic
dehumanization may generate feelings of disgust and revulsion.[65] Feelings of disgust, through
rousing social distance, may lead to dehumanization. Therefore, a person or group that is generally
connected with disgusting effects and seen as physically unclean may induce moral avoidance. [65]
Being deemed disgusting produces a variety of cognitive effects that result in exclusion from the
perceived inner group.[65]

Political and legal aspects of disgust


The emotion disgust has been noted to feature strongly in the public sphere in relation to issues and
debates, among other things, regarding anatomy, sex and bioethics. There is a range of views by
different commentators on the role, purpose and effects of disgust on public discourse.

Leon Kass, a bioethicist, has advocated that "in crucial cases...repugnance is the emotional
expression of deep wisdom, beyond reason's power fully to articulate it." in relation to bio-ethical
issues (See: Wisdom of repugnance).

Martha Nussbaum, a jurist and ethicist, explicitly rejects disgust as an appropriate guide for
legislating, arguing the "politics of disgust" is an unreliable emotional reaction with no inherent
wisdom. Furthermore, she argues this "politics of disgust" has in the past and present had the
effects of supporting bigotry in the forms of sexism, racism and antisemitism and links the emotion
of disgust to support for laws against Miscegenation and the oppressive caste system in India. In
place of this "politics of disgust", Nussbaum argues for the Harm principle from John Stuart Mill as
the proper basis for legislating. Nussbaum argues the harm principle supports the legal ideas of
consent, the Age of majority and privacy and protects citizens. She contrasts this with the "politics
of disgust" which she argues denies citizens humanity and equality before the law on no rational
grounds and cause palpable social harm. (See Martha Nussbaum, From Disgust to Humanity:
Sexual Orientation and Constitutional Law). Nussbaum published Hiding From Humanity:
Disgust, Shame, and the Law in 2004; the book examines the relationship of disgust and shame to a
society's laws. Nussbaum identifies disgust as a marker that bigoted, and often merely majoritarian,
discourse employs to "place", by diminishment and denigration, a despised minority. Removing
"disgust" from public discourse constitutes an important step in achieving humane and tolerant
democracies.

Leigh Turner (2004) has argued that "reactions of disgust are often built upon prejudices that
should be challenged and rebutted." On the other hand, writers, such as Kass, find wisdom in
adhering to one's initial feelings of disgust. A number of writers on the theory of disgust find it to
be the proto-legal foundation of human law.

11 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

Disgust has also figured prominently in the work of several other philosophers. Nietzsche became
disgusted with the music and orientation of Richard Wagner, as well as other aspects of 19th
century culture and morality. Jean-Paul Sartre wrote widely about experiences involving various
negative emotions related to disgust.[66]

The Hydra's Tale: Imagining Disgust


According to the book The Hydra's Tale: Imagining Disgust by Robert Rawdon Wilson,[67] disgust
may be further subdivided into physical disgust, associated with physical or metaphorical
uncleanliness, and moral disgust, a similar feeling related to courses of action. For example; "I am
disgusted by the hurtful things that you are saying." Moral disgust should be understood as
culturally determined; physical disgust as more universally grounded. The book also discusses
moral disgust as an aspect of the representation of disgust. Wilson does this in two ways. First, he
discusses representations of disgust in literature, film and fine art. Since there are characteristic
facial expressions (the clenched nostrils, the pursed lips)as Charles Darwin, Paul Ekman, and
others have shownthey may be represented with more or less skill in any set of circumstances
imaginable. There may even be "disgust worlds" in which disgust motifs so dominate that it may
seem that entire represented world is, in itself, disgusting. Second, since people know what disgust
is as a primary, or visceral, emotion (with characteristic gestures and expressions), they may imitate
it. Thus, Wilson argues that, for example, contempt is acted out on the basis of the visceral emotion,
disgust, but is not identical with disgust. It is a "compound affect" that entails intellectual
preparation, or formatting, and theatrical techniques. Wilson argues that there are many such
"intellectual" compound affectssuch as nostalgia and outragebut that disgust is a fundamental
and unmistakable example. Moral disgust, then, is different from visceral disgust; it is more
conscious and more layered in performance.

Wilson links shame and guilt to disgust (now transformed, wholly or partially, into self-disgust)
primarily as a consequence rooted in self-consciousness. Referring to a passage in Doris Lessing's
The Golden Notebook, Wilson writes that "the dance between disgust and shame takes place. A
slow choreography unfolds before the mind's-eye."[68]

Wilson examines the claims of several jurists and legal scholarssuch as William Ian Millerthat
disgust must underlie positive law. "In the absence of disgust", he observes, stating their claim, ". . .
there would be either total barbarism or a society ruled solely by force, violence and terror." The
moral-legal argument, he remarks, "leaves much out of account."[69] His own argument turns
largely upon the human capacity to learn how to control, even to suppress, strong and problematic
affects and, over time, for entire populations to abandon specific disgust responses.

See also
Affective neuroscience
Amygdala
Aversion Therapy

12 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

Cognitive neuroscience
Contempt
Disgusted of Tunbridge Wells
Fear
Foodborne illness
Menippean satire
Nausea
Papez Circuit
Phobia
Social neuroscience
Taboo
Vomiting

References
1. Cisler, J.M.; Olatunji, B.O.; Lohr, J.M.; 6. Curtis, V. (2007). "Dirt, disease, and disgust: A
Williams, N.L. (2009). "Attentional bias natural history of hygiene". Journal of
differences between fear and disgust: Epidemiology and Community Health. 61 (8):
Implications for the role of disgust in disgust- 660664. doi:10.1136/jech.2007.062380.
related anxiety disorders". Cognition and PMC 2652987 . PMID 17630362.
Emotion. 23 (4): 675687. 7. Schaller, M., & Duncan, L. A. (2007). The
doi:10.1080/02699930802051599. behavioral immune system: Its evolution and
PMC 2892866 . PMID 20589224. social psychological implications. In J. P.
2. Rozin P, Haidt J, & McCauley C.R. (2000) Forgas, M. G. Haselton, & W. von Hippel
Disgust In M. Lewis & J.M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Evolution and the social mind:
(Eds) Handbook of Emotions, 2nd Edition Evolutionary psychology and social cognition
(pp637- 653). New York: Guilford Press (pp. 293307). New York, NY: Psychology
3. Wicker, B.; Keysers, C.; Plailly, J.; Royet, J. P.; Press.
Gallese, V.; Rizzolatti, G. (2003). "Both of us 8. Pond, R. S.; DeWall, C. N.; Lambert, N. M.;
disgusted in my insula: the common neural Deckman, T.; Bonser, I. M.; Fincham, F. D.
basis of seeing and feeling disgust". Neuron. 40 (2012). "Repulsed by violence: Disgust
(3): 65564. sensitivity buffers trait, behavioral, and daily
doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2. aggression". Journal of Personality and Social
PMID 14642287. Psychology. 102 (1): 175188.
4. Oaten, M.; Stevenson, R. J.; Case, T. I. (2009). doi:10.1037/a0024296. PMID 21707194.
"Disgust as a Disease-Avoidance Mechanism". 9. Ritz, T.; Thons, M.; Fahrenkrug, S.; Dahme, B.
Psychological Bulletin. 135 (2): 303321. (2005). "Airways,respiration, and respiratory
doi:10.1037/a0014823. sinus arrhythmia during picture viewing".
5. Curtis, V.; Biran, A. (2001). "Dirt, disgust, and Psychophysiology. 42 (5): 568578.
disease: Is hygiene in our genes?". Perspectives doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00312.x.
in Biology and Medicine. 44 (1): 1731. PMID 16176379.
doi:10.1353/pbm.2001.0001. PMID 11253302.

13 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

10. Navarrete, Carlos David; Fessler, Daniel M.T. 18. Meissner, Karin; Muth, Eric R.; Herbert, Beate
(2006). "Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: M. (2011). "Bradygastric activity of the
The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust stomach predicts disgust sensitivity and
sensitivity on intergroup attitudes". Evolution perceived disgust intensity". Biological
and Human Behavior. 27 (4): 270282. Psychology. 86 (1): 916.
doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.12.001. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.014.
11. Vartanian, L R (2010). "Disgust and perceived PMID 20888886.
control in attitudes toward obese people". 19. Schienle, Anne; Kchel, Angelika; Leutgeb,
International Journal of Obesity. 34 (8): Verena (2011). "Frontal late positivity in dental
13027. doi:10.1038/ijo.2010.45. phobia: A study on gender differences".
PMID 20195287. Biological Psychology. 88 (23): 2639.
12. Tyber, J. M.; Lieberman, D.; Griskevicius, V. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.08.010.
(2009). "Microbes, mating, and morality: PMID 21889569.
Individual differences in three functional 20. Ward, Jamie. (2006). The Students Guide to
domains of disgust". Journal of Personality and Cognitive Neuroscience. New York, NY:
Social Psychology. 97 (1): 10322. Psychology Press.
doi:10.1037/a0015474. PMID 19586243. 21. Rozin, P.; Lowery, L.; Ebert, R. (1994).
13. Tyber, J. M.; Lieberman, D.; Griskevicius, V. "Varieties of disgust faces and the structure of
(2009). "Microbes, mating, and morality: disgust". Journal of Personality and Social
Individual differences in three functional Psychology. 66 (5): 870881.
domains of disgust". Journal of Personality and doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.5.870.
Social Psychology. 97 (1): 105. PMID 8014832.
doi:10.1037/a0015474. PMID 19586243. 22. Eckman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P.
14. Tyber, J. M.; Lieberman, D.; Griskevicius, V. (1972). Emotion in the human face: Guidelines
(2009). "Microbes, mating, and morality: for research and an integration of findings.
Individual differences in three functional New York: Pergamon
domains of disgust". Journal of Personality and 23. "Exploring the positive and negative
Social Psychology. 97 (1): 106. implications of facial feedback.". APA
doi:10.1037/a0015474. PMID 19586243. PsycNET. Retrieved 2015-11-02.
15. Tyber, J. M.; Lieberman, D.; Griskevicius, V. 24. Kohler, E; et al. (2002). "Hearing sounds,
(2009). "Microbes, mating, and morality: understanding actions: action representation in
Individual differences in three functional mirror neurons". Science. 297 (5582): 846848.
domains of disgust". Journal of Personality and doi:10.1126/science.1070311. PMID 12161656.
Social Psychology. 97 (1): 107. 25. Sprengelmeyer, R; Rausch, M; Eysel, U. T.;
doi:10.1037/a0015474. PMID 19586243. Przuntek, H. (1998). "Neural structures
16. Druschel, B. A.; Sherman, M. F. (1999). associated with recognition of facial
"Disgust sensitivity as a function of the Big expressions of basic emotions". Biological
Five and gender". Personality and Individual Sciences. 265 (1409): 19271931.
Differences. 26 (4): 739748. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0522. PMC 1689486 .
doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00196-2. PMID 9821359.
17. "Survival's Ick Factor". The New York Times. 26. Hayes, Catherine J.; Stevenson, Richard J.;
23 January 2012. Coltheart, Max (2009). "Production of
spontaneous and posed facial expressions in
patients with Huntington's disease: Impaired
communication of disgust". Cognition &
Emotion. 23: 118134.
doi:10.1080/02699930801949090.

14 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

27. Haidt, J.; Rozin, P.; McCauley, C.; Imada, S. 34. Phillips, ML; Young, AW; Senior, C; Brammer,
(1997). "Body, Psyche, and Culture: The M; Andrew, C; Calder, AJ; Bullmore, ET;
Relationship between Disgust and Morality". Perrett, DI; et al. (1997). "A specific neural
Psychology & Developing Societies. 9: substrate for perceiving facial expressions of
107131. doi:10.1177/097133369700900105. disgust". Nature. 389 (6650): 4958.
28. Clarke, John R. (2001). Looking at Lovemaking doi:10.1038/39051. PMID 9333238.
(1st paperback print ed.). Berkeley: University 35. Calder, Andrew J.; et al. (2000). "Impaired
of California Press. pp. 1516. recognition and experience of disgust following
ISBN 978-0-520-22904-4. "In the Manchu brain injury". Nature Neuroscience. 3 (11):
tribe, a mother will routinely suck her small 10771088. doi:10.1038/80586.
son's penis in public but would never kiss his PMID 11036262.
cheeks. Among adults, the Manchu believe, 36. Keysers, C.; Xiao, D. K.; Foldiak, P.; Perrett, D.
fellatio is a sexual act, but kissingeven I. (2001). "The speed of sight". Cognitive
between mother and infant sonis always a Neuroscience. 13: 90101.
sexual act, and thus fellation becomes the doi:10.1162/089892901564199.
proper display of motherly affection." 37. Adolphs, Ralph; et al. (2003). "Dissociable
29. Barre, Weston La (1975). "The Cultural Basis neural systems for recognizing emotions".
of Emotions and Gestures". In Davis, Martha. Brain Cognition. 52 (1): 6169.
Anthropological Perspectives of Movement. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00009-5.
Arno Press. p. 56. ISBN 978-0-405-06201-8. PMID 12812805.
"Manchu kissing is purely a private sexual act, 38. Stark, R.; Zimmermann, M.; Kagerer, S.;
and though husband and wife or lovers might Schienle, A.; Walter, B. (2007). "Hemodynamic
kiss each other, they would do it stealthily since brain correlates of disgust and fear ratings".
it is shameful to do ... yet Manchu mothers NeuroImage. 37 (2): 663673.
have the pattern of putting the penis of the baby doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.005.
boy into their mouths, a practice which 39. Mitchell, IJ; Heims, H; Neville, EA; Rickards,
probably shocks Westerners even more than H (2005). "Huntington's disease patients show
kissing in public shocks the Manchu." impaired perception of disgust in the gustatory
30. Barre, Weston La (1974). "The Cultural Basis and olfactory modalities". The Journal of
of Emotions and Gestures". In Starr, Jerold M. neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences. 17
Social structure and social personality. Boston: (1): 11921.
Little, Brown and Company. p. 79. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.17.1.119.
31. Halperin, David M.; Winkler, John J.; Zeitlin, 40. Sprengelmeyer, R; Schroeder, U; Young, AW;
Froma I. (1990). Before Sexuality. Princeton: Epplen, JT (2006). "Disgust in pre-clinical
Princeton University Press. p. 3. Huntington's disease: a longitudinal study".
ISBN 978-0-691-00221-7. Neuropsychologia. 44 (4): 51833.
32. Walls, Neal (2001). Desire, Discord and Death. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.003.
Boston: American Schools of Oriental PMID 16098998.
Research. p. 81. ISBN 978-0-89757-056-5. 41. Sprengelmeyer, R.; Young, A.W.; Calder, A.J.;
33. Olatunji, Bunmi O.; Haidt, Jonathan; McKay, Karnat, A.; Lange, H.; Homberg, V.; et al.
Dean; David, Bieke (2008). "Core, animal (1996). "Loss of disgust: Perception of faces
reminder, and contamination disgust: Three and emotions in Huntington's disease". Brain.
kinds of disgust with distinct personality, 119: 16471665. doi:10.1093/brain/119.5.1647.
behavioral, physiological, and clinical PMID 8931587.
correlates". Journal of Research in Personality.
42 (5): 12431259.
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.009.

15 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

42. Surguladze SA, El-Hage W, Dalgleish T, Radua 49. Grill, H.C.; Norgren, R. (1978b). "Chronically
J, Gohier B, Phillips ML (2010). "Depression is decerebrate rats demonstrate satiation but not
associated with increased sensitivity to signals bait shyness". Science. 201 (4352): 267269.
of disgust: a functional magnetic resonance doi:10.1126/science.663655. PMID 663655.
imaging study". J Psychiatr Res. 44 (1): 50. Eckel, L. A.; Ossenkopp, K. P. (1996). "Area
894902. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.02.010. Postrema mediates the formation of rapid
PMID 20307892. conditioned palatability shifts to a lithium-
43. Stein, D.J.; Liu, Y.; Shapira, N.A.; Goodman, paired solution". Behavioral Neuroscience. 110:
W.K. (2001). "The psychobiology of obsessive- 200212. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.110.1.202.
compulsive disorder: How important is 51. Flynn, F. W; Grill, H. J.; Shulkin, J.; Norgren,
disgust?". Current Psychiatry. 3 (4): 281287. R. (1991). "Central gustatory lesions: II. Effects
doi:10.1007/s11920-001-0020-3. on sodium appetite, taste aversion learning, and
44. Shapira; Liu, Y; He, A. G.; Bradley, M. M.; feeding behaviors". Behavioral Neuroscience.
Lessig, M. C.; James, G. A.; Stein, D. J.; Lang, 105 (6): 944954.
P. J.; Goodman, W. K.; et al. (2003). "Brain doi:10.1037/0735-7044.105.6.944.
activation by disgust-inducing pictures in PMID 1777107.
obsessive-compulsive disorder". Biological 52. Parker, Linda A.; Limebeer, Cheryl L.; Rana,
Psychiatry. 54 (7): 751756. Shadna A. (2008). "Conditioned nausea in rats:
doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00003-9. assessment by conditioned disgust reactions,
PMID 14512216. rather than conditioned taste avoidance".
45. Sprengelmeyer, R.; Young, A. W.; Pundt, I.; Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Sprengelmeyer, A.; Calder, A. J.; Berrios, G.; 62 (3): 198209. doi:10.1037/a0012531.
Winkel, R.; Vollmoeller, W.; Kuhn, W.; Sartory, PMID 18778149.
G.; Przuntek, H. (1997). "Disgust implicated in 53. Jones, Andrew; Fitness, Julie (2008). "Moral
obsessive-compulsive disorder". Biological hypervigilance: The influence of disgust
Sciences. 264 (1389): 17671773. sensitivity in the moral domain". Emotion. 8
doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0245. PMC 1688750 . (5): 61327. doi:10.1037/a0013435.
PMID 9447734. PMID 18837611.
46. Grill, H.C.; Norgren, R. (1978a). "The taste 54. David, B.; Olatunji, B.O. (2011). "The effect of
Reactivity Test. I: Miimetic responses to disgust conditioning and disgust sensitivity on
gustatory stimuli in neurologically normal rats". appraisals of moral transgressions". Personality
Brain Research. 143 (2): 263279. & Individual Differences. 50 (7): 11421146.
doi:10.1016/0006-8993(78)90568-1. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.004.
PMID 630409. 55. Schnall, Simone (July 1, 2008). "Disgust as
47. Travers, J. B.; Norgren, R. (1986). Embodied Moral Judgment". Personality and
"Electromyographic analysis of the ingestion Social Psychology Bulletin. 34: 10961109.
and rejection of sapid stimuli in the rat". doi:10.1177/0146167208317771.
Behavioral Neuroscience. 100 (4): 544555. 56. Cummins, DD (2012). "Emotion and
doi:10.1037/0735-7044.100.4.544. deliberative reasoning in moral judgment".
PMID 3741605. Front Psychol. 3 (328).
48. Parker, Linda A.; Limebeer, Cheryl L.; Rana, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00328.
Shadna A. (2008). "Conditioned nausea in rats: 57. May, J. (2014). "Does Disgust Influence Moral
assessment by conditioned disgust reactions, Judgment?". Australasian Journal of
rather than conditioned taste avoidance". Philosophy. 92 (1): 125141.
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. doi:10.1080/00048402.2013.797476.
62 (3): 198209. doi:10.1037/a0012531.
PMID 18778149.

16 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

58. Landy, J. "Does incidental disgust amplify 63. "Left or right-wing? Brain's disgust response
moral judgment? A meta-analytic review of tells all". New Scientist. 30 October 2014.
experimental evidence." (PDF). Perspectives on 64. Olatunji, Bunmi O.; David, Bieke; Ciesielski,
Psychological Science. 10: 518536. Bethany G. (2012). "Who am I to judge?
doi:10.1177/1745691615583128. Archived Self-disgust predicts less punishment of severe
from the original (PDF) on March 19, 2015. transgressions". Emotion. 12 (1): 16973.
59. Ong, HH (2014). "Moral judgment modulation doi:10.1037/a0024074. PMID 21707158.
by disgust is bi-directionally moderated by 65. Sherman, Gary D.; Johnathon Haidt (2011).
individual sensitivity". Frontiers in psychology. "Cuteness and Disgust: The Humanizing and
5: 194. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00194. Dehumanizing Effects of Emotion". Emotion
PMC 3944793 . PMID 24639665. Review. 3 (3): 245251.
60. Horberg, E. J.; Oveis, C.; Keltner, D.; Cohen, doi:10.1177/1754073911402396.
A. B. (2009). "Disgust and the moralization of 66. Sartre, Jean Paul. (1943) "Being and
purity". Journal of Personality and Social Nothingness. Trans. Hazel Barnes. P. 604-607.
Psychology. 97 (6): 963976. 67. Wilson RR (2007). "Wilson On Disgust: A
doi:10.1037/a0017423. PMID 19968413. Menippean Interview. Interview with Robert
61. Horberg, E. J.; Oveis, C.; Keltner, D.; Cohen, Wilson". 34 (2).
A. B. (2009). "Disgust and the moralization of 68. Wilson, Robert Rawdon (2002). The Hydras
purity". Journal of Personality and Social Tale: Imagining Disgust. U Alberta Press.
Psychology. 97 (6): 972973. p. 281.
doi:10.1037/a0017423. PMID 19968413. 69. Wilson, pp. 51-52.
62. David, B.; Olatunji, B.O. (2011). "The effect of
disgust conditioning and disgust sensitivity on
appraisals of moral transgressions". Personality
& Individual Differences. 50 (7): 11421146.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.004.

Bibliography
Cohen, William A. and Ryan Johnson, eds. Filth: Dirt, Disgust, and Modern Life. University
of Minnesota Press, 2005.
Douglas, Mary. Purity and Danger: An Analysis of Concepts of Pollution and Taboo. Praeger,
1966.
Kelly, Daniel. Yuck! The Nature and Moral Significance of Disgust. MIT Press, 2011.
Korsmeyer, Carolyn (2011) Savoring Disgust: The Foul and the Fair in Aesthetics
(https://books.google.com/books?id=NwsUasv4wmgC&printsec=frontcover&
dq=%22Savoring+Disgust:+The+Foul+and+the+Fair+in+Aesthetics%22&hl=en&
sa=X&ei=6AUGVYG8Ms3_8QXZ54EY&ved=0CBwQ6AEwAA#v=onepage&
q=%22Savoring%20Disgust
%3A%20The%20Foul%20and%20the%20Fair%20in%20Aesthetics%22&f=false) Oxford
University Press. ISBN 9780199842346.
McCorkle Jr., William W. Ritualizing the Disposal of the Deceased: From Corpse to Concept.
Peter Lang, 2010.
McGinn, Colin. The Meaning of Disgust. Oxford University Press, 2011.
Menninghaus, Winfried. Disgust: Theory and History of a Strong Sensation. Tr. Howard

17 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

Eiland and Joel Golb. SUNY Press, 2003


Miller, William Ian. The Anatomy of Disgust. Harvard University Press, 1997.
Nussbaum, Martha C. Upheavals of Thought: The Intelligence of Emotions. Cambridge
University Press, 2001.
Nussbaum, Martha C. Hiding from Humanity: Disgust, Shame, and the Law. Princeton
University Press, 2004.
Nussbaum, Martha C. From Disgust to Humanity: Sexual Orientation and Constitutional Law.
Oxford University Press, 2010.
Rindisbacher, Hans J. A Cultural History of Disgust. KulturPoetik. 5: 1. 2005. pp. 119127.
Wilson, Robert (2007). "Disgust: A Menippean Interview". Canadian Review of Comparative
Literature. 34: 203213.
Wilson, Robert Rawdon. The Hydras Tale: Imagining Disgust. University of Alberta Press,
2002.

External links
Nancy Sherman, a researcher investigating disgust Look up disgust in
(https://web.archive.org/web/20080513093810/http: Wiktionary, the free
//www14.vjc.edu/academics/faculty/sherman_nancy/) dictionary.
Jon Haidt's page about the Disgust Scale
(https://web.archive.org/web/20070524043035/http: Wikimedia Commons
//people.virginia.edu/~jdh6n/disgustscale.html) has media related to
Moral Judgment and the Social Intuitionist Model Disgust.
(https://web.archive.org/web/20071103022419/http:
//people.virginia.edu/~jdh6n/moraljudgment.html), publications by Jonathan Haidt on disgust
and its relationship with moral ideas
Hiding from Humanity: Disgust, Shame, and the Law (http://pup.princeton.edu/chapters
/i7697.html)
Shame and Group Psychotherapy (http://www.birchmore.org/index.html)
"Is repugnance wise? Visceral responses to biotechnology" Nature Biotechnology
(http://www.nature.com/cgi-taf/DynaPage.taf?file=/nbt/journal/v22/n3/full/nbt0304-269.html)
Purity and Pollution (https://web.archive.org/web/20060217145556/http:
//www.classics.ox.ac.uk/faculty/princeton/KirkpatrickPurity0310.rtf) by Jonathan Kirkpatrick
(RTF)
Paper on the economic effects of Repugnance (http://kuznets.fas.harvard.edu/~aroth/papers
/Repugnance.pdf)
Anatomy of Disgust (https://web.archive.org/web/20080513031630/http://www.channel4.com
/culture/microsites/A/anatomy_disgust/science.html), Channel 4 program
WhyFiles.org (http://whyfiles.org/shorties/278disgust/) Article written about a February 2009
study in "Science" linking moral judgments with facial expressions that indicate sensory
disgust.
Disgust: A Menippean Interview (https://ejournals.library.ualberta.ca/index.php/crcl/article
/view/10712)

18 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM
Disgust - Wikipedia https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Disgust

Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Disgust&oldid=774207763"

Categories: Emotions Concepts in aesthetics Morality

This page was last modified on 6 April 2017, at 23:05.


Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional
terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy.
Wikipedia is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit
organization.

19 of 19 4/20/2017 6:42 PM

Potrebbero piacerti anche