Sei sulla pagina 1di 13

H. P. A. Van Dongen and G. A. Kerkhof (Eds.

)
Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 190
ISSN: 0079-6123
Copyright ! 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

CHAPTER 6

Effects of caffeine on sleep and cognition

Jan Snel{,* and Monicque M. Lorist{,}

{
Department of Psychonomics, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
{
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Groningen,
Groningen, The Netherlands
}
BCN-NeuroImaging Center, University Medical Center Groningen,
Groningen, The Netherlands

Abstract: Caffeine can be used effectively to manipulate our mental state. It is beneficial in restoring
low levels of wakefulness and in counteracting degraded cognitive task performance due to sleep
deprivation. However, caffeine may produce detrimental effects on subsequent sleep, resulting in
daytime sleepiness. This justifies a careful consideration of risks related to sleep deprivation in
combination with caffeine consumption, especially in adolescents. The efficacy of caffeine to restore
detrimental effects of sleep deprivation seems to be partly due to caffeine expectancy and to placebo
effects. The claim that stimulant effects of caffeine are related to withdrawal or withdrawal reversal
seems to be untenable.

Keywords: caffeine; modafinil; staying awake; falling asleep; expectancy; withdrawal; recovery sleep;
mental state.

Introduction people might take a nap, go for a walk, or put on


bright lights. However, for centuries, one of the
People are continuously engaged to find the opti- most popular means to manipulate our physiolog-
mum of their mental and physiological state. ical and mental state is the use of caffeine, mostly
To reach that optimum, diverse strategies are prepared as coffee. On one hand, it is deliberately
used. In the case of preparing to go to bed, for used to counteract fatigue, to stay alert, perform
example, people relax, lights are dimmed, and at satisfying levels, and postpone sleep, while on
some people take a nightcap. When sleepy at the other hand, it is intentionally avoided by
times that one is expected to be alert and wakeful, many to get a good night's sleep.
Caffeine is generally accepted to be a mild stimu-
lant. It is affordable and easily available throughout
*Corresponding author. the world and found in many products (see Table 1).
Tel.: 31 20 5256855; Fax: 31 20 6391656 After oral ingestion of caffeine, mostly in the form
E-mail: J.Snel@uva.nl

DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53817-8.00006-2 105


106

of coffee or tea, 99% of it is absorbed from the gas- brain, involved in the regulation of sleep, arousal,
trointestinal tract into the bloodstream, peaking and cognition (Ribeiro and Sebastiao, 2010).
3060 min after ingestion. Faster absorption of Therefore, it is no surprise that caffeine, as an
caffeine is found for caffeine-containing chewing adenosine receptor antagonist, can modulate
gum, with maximum levels reached between 45 physiological and mental states (Table 2). This is
and 80 min postadministration, while absorption supported by findings indicating that caffeine
rate for caffeine-containing capsules lies between indeed attenuates the buildup of sleep propensity
85 and 120 min (Kamimori et al., 2002). Caffeine associated with wakefulness (Landolt, 2008b),
diffuses throughout the entire body; it passes all although in rats, it was found (Wurts and Edgar,
biological membranes, including the bloodbrain 2000) that caffeine did not block compensatory
barrier and the placental barrier. non-rapid eye movement (non-REM) sleep and
Most of the biological effects of caffeine, at sleep continuity. Moreover, it potently attenuates
levels reached during normal human consump- EEG markers of non-REM sleep homeostasis
tion, are caused by way of antagonizing adenosine during sleep, as well as during wakefulness
receptors, in particular, A1 and A2A receptors, (Landolt, 2008b). Targeting the adenosine system
and to a lesser degree, the A2B and A3 receptors. by caffeine consumption therefore seems an
A1 and A2A adenosine receptors affect several effective tool to modulate individual vulnerability
mechanisms located in widespread areas of the to the detrimental effects of sleep deprivation on
cognitive performance, and sleep.
To be able to determine the effect of caffeine in
Table 1. Caffeine contents of common products people, individual differences have to be taken into
account. Metabolic rate and the tolerance for effects
Coffee, cup 125 ml Caffeine
of caffeine vary considerably from one person to the
(mg)
Filtered, percolated 60100 other. The half-life of caffeine is on average 3.7 h,
Drip 44 ranging from 2 to 10 h, dependent on endogenous
Instant 3550 and exogenous factors. For example, metabolic
Pads, dark regular 9095 speed of caffeine is increased by 3050% in nicotine
Pads, mild 7580
users, whereas a decrease is observed in pregnant
Cappuccino 60
Espresso cup 50 ml 5060 women and woman taking oral contraceptives. Also
Decaffeinated coffee cup 125 ml 24 there are large interindividual differences in sensi-
Tea, cup 125 ml 2045 tivity to caffeine due to genetic variations in the
Soft drinks per 100 ml adenosine A2A receptor gene (Retey et al., 2007),
Cola's general 311 the role adenosine and adenosine receptors play in
Cola light 015 non-REM sleep homeostasis, (Landolt, 2008b) and
Ice tea 312
genotype-dependent differences in sleep (Landolt,
Energy drinks per 100 ml 30
Chocolate containing drinks per 100 ml 24 2008a). However, no systematic difference in the
Chocolate/50 g metabolism of caffeine is observed between men
Milk 225 and women.
Dark 860 Besides the purposive use or avoidance of caf-
Extra dark 30210 feine, it is frequently consumed unintentionally.
Chocolate candy, bars, ice cream 210 People are not always aware that caffeine is
Alcoholic drinks or shooters per 100 ml 50120
Prescription and non-predescription 25100
added to many products to increase flavor and
medication taste. The awareness (or unawareness) of caffeine
consumption has important implications, not only
Source: Wendte et al. (2003). for our well-being, but also for caffeine research.
107

Table 2. Central adenosine receptors affected by typical caffeine exposure

Effects of
Receptor Localization Types of neurons caffeine Caffeine action

A1 Almost all brain areas, especially All types of neurons (aspecific), Antagonistic Disinhibition of
hippocampus, cerebral and cerebellar especially linked to dopamine transmitter release
cortex, certain thalamic nuclei D1 receptors
A2A Dopamine-rich regions: striatum, nucleus Colocalized with dopamine Antagonistic Increase
accumbens, tuberculum olfactorium, D2 receptors transmission via
hippocampus? dopamine D2
cortex? receptors

Source: Lorist and Tops (2003).

It is important to realize that estimating habitual Disturbances of our circadian rhythmicity may
caffeine intake is difficult and most likely results cause disruptions in sleeping patterns and influ-
in lower estimates than actually consumed. ence cognitive task performance, which can lead
Hence, the selection of participants based on their to suboptimal performance and even errors
self-reported daily consumption of caffeine is (Crochet et al., 2009; Ker et al., 2010). Jet lag
often biased and unreliable. Careful screening of and shift work are important factors that contrib-
participants, who were selected on the basis of ute to these deteriorations of performance and
their habitual intake of 100500 mg caffeine a have indeed been found to be related to an
day, by Wendte et al. (2003) revealed an actual increase in risk of injury (as discussed elsewhere
caffeine consumption of 1541285 mg (# 1.515 in this volume). There is a strong need for inter-
cups of coffee); quantities which were up to ventions to guarantee that persons who run such
250% higher than the self-reported values. In risk can do their job safely and are able to restore
addition to the underreporting of consumed caf- disturbed circadian rhythms as soon and efficiently
feine quantity, factors such as differences in as possible. Different pharmacological aids are
brewing method, used coffee blend (Arabica cof- used to counteract work-related sleep problems
fee contains 2% of caffeine, Robusta coffee 4%), and jet lag, and the question is whether caffeine
or serving size of caffeine-containing food hamper might qualify as one of them (Coste and Lagarde,
adequate estimation of caffeine intake. 2009). Evidence suggests that sleep-loss-induced
In this chapter, effects of caffeine are described; deficits in alertness and vigilance can indeed be
in particular, effects on sleepwake rhythmicity in reversed or mitigated by stimulants such as caffeine
sleep-deprived versus well-rested individuals. (Lorist and Snel, 2008; Snel et al., 2004).
Sleep deprivation has been used in different stud-
ies to elucidate the role of caffeine in offsetting these
Caffeine and sleep deprivation effects. Benitez et al. (2009), for example, used a
severe form of sleep deprivation in 14 males and 4
A 24-h economy demands individuals to operate females (mean age 25.8 years, SD 4.3), all mild
at times which are not in sync with their circa- or non-caffeine users (< 300 mg/day). Participants
dian clock. As a result, daily rhythms can become had a normal 8-h night of sleep, followed by 77 h of
disrupted and, consequently, negatively affect our continuous wakefulness. Placebo or caffeine
well-being. If the timing of sleep is not adapted to (200 mg), in the form of two sticks of Alert gum, con-
the circadian clock, this misalignment may result taining 100 mg of caffeine each, was assigned ran-
in a so-called circadian rhythm sleep disorder. domly at 1.00, 3.00, 5.00, and 7.00 a.m. during the
108

three subsequent nights. Performance was tested disadvantage that it can be used only after
periodically over the 77-h period of continuous informed consent. Participants in the Killgore
wakefulness, using the psychomotor vigilance test et al. (2009) study were administered a test to
(PVT). The PVT is a simple visual reaction task, measure the formation of abstract concepts 1 h
used to measure sustained attention. It measures after drug administration (i.e., after 45 h awake).
the time it takes to respond to a visual stimulus. Tests measuring planning time and cognitive pro-
A biomathematical model was build to describe per- cessing started 3.5 h (i.e., 47.5 h awake) and 4.5 h
formance during the period of extended wakeful- after drug administration (i.e., after 48.5 h
ness. This model identified patterns in the data that awake). The results of these studies showed that
suggested the presence of a performance inhibitor, the three stimulants differentially affected the
called fatigue, that increased and saturated over outcome of the three cognition tasks, suggesting
the 77 h of continuous wakefulness. Caffeine was that the stimulating effects of caffeine especially
found to be able to inhibit the effects of fatigue. This affected cognitive planning processes.
result confirms the findings of former research It seems clear that performance efficiency
(Balkin et al., 2004; Wesensten et al., 2002, 2004, is affected by sleep deprivation and part of the
2005) with simple psychomotor tasks and tasks of performance deteriorations can indeed be coun-
executive functions, indicating that caffeine taken teracted by caffeine or other stimulants. However,
both incidentally or with the purpose of coun- it is important to realize that task performance not
teracting drowsiness in the morning, is effective in only relies on specific task demands and related
maintaining alertness and performance after pro- cognitive abilities. The interaction between task,
longed periods of total sleep deprivation (i.e., participant, and environment actually determines
54.685 h). the quality of performance, especially during subop-
The efficacy of caffeine in restoring cognitive timal situations. It is clear that some tasks are more
processes like emotional perception, judgment, challenging than others, and related changes in
risk-taking, and planning after sleep deprivation motivation might mask fatigue effects created by
was targeted in a series of double-blind studies sleep deprivation.
by Killgore et al. (2009). They studied the effects Fatigue is a well-known and common phenome-
of caffeine in 54 participants (age 1836 years), non in sustained operations, such as long-distance
who were sleep deprived for 4550 h and subse- driving and long-term continuous work (see
quently tested. Before performing different cogni- Chapters 9 and 11 of this volume), in which a
tive tasks, they received one dose of 600 mg low information load does not promote motiva-
caffeine (n 12), 400 mg modafinil (n 12), tion to perform. In many of such real-life
20 mg dextroamphetamine (n 16), or placebo situations, a sufficient level of motivation to work
(n 14). Like caffeine, modafinil promotes alert- is required to perform adequately. Kilpelaiinen
ness and wakefulness. Modafinil is especially and colleagues studied the effect of caffeine and
FDA approved to manage fatigue in narcolepsy placebo on sustained attention and learning in
and residual fatigue in sleep apnea and shift work such a vigilance situation. They also assessed sub-
sleep disorder (Rosenthal et al., 2008). Dextroam- jective ratings of sleepiness, mood, motivation,
phetamine also has stimulant properties, known and perceived task performance in their study
to promote wakefulness. It has been provided to (Kilpelaiinen et al., 2010). Fifteen military pilot
pilots on long missions to help them remain students (age 2324 years) took part in a series
focused and alert. However, dextroamphetamine of tests in a flight simulator, in the 37 h sleep dep-
can produce some side effects like palpitations, rivation study. They received either placebo or
tachycardia, and elevated blood pressure 200 mg of caffeine twice a day (Kilpelaiinen
(Caldwell and Caldwell, 2005). Modafinil has a et al., 2010). During the experiment, vigilance
109

was assessed six times (Mackworth clock test) and one's physical and mental health. Maintaining a
learning four times. The learning task consisted of good sleep quality involves avoidance of
learning the association between Japanese hira- substances that stimulate mind and body and dis-
gana symbols and spoken syllables. Learning turb sleep habits. Caffeine, with its proven effi-
was tested 2 h later with five hiraganas from the cacy to counteract sleepiness, is one of the
previous learning session. stimulants that may produce detrimental effects
As expected, sleep deprivation decreased the on subsequent sleep, especially when sleep is
amount of correct detections and increased reaction initiated at a time when the biological clock sends
times in both the caffeine and placebo groups. The a strong waking signal as happens during daytime.
increase in number of false alarms was limited to This means a prudent use of caffeine. In other
the placebo group. Working memory, as tested in words, using caffeine at times that high mental
the learning task, remained unaffected during sleep alertness and physical activation hampers sleep
deprivation. Kilpelaiinen et al. (2010) argued that quality should be avoided, in particular, in the
the absence of performance decline in the learning hours shortly before going to sleep.
task might partly be due to the stimulating nature In people working shift hours, sleep habits have
of this task following a very exhausting and long- to be adapted to their irregular working schedule,
lasting vigilance task. With increasing sleep pres- which might cause specific problems in combina-
sure, they observed that participants even wanted tion with caffeine consumption. Pecotic et al.
to perform the learning task during task breaks to (2008) evaluated sleep habits and explored
help them stay awake, illustrating the importance whether these were influenced by caffeine con-
of taking into account the environmental factors in sumption in 130 medical students, 68 physicians
sustained performance and sleep deprivation stud- at the postgraduate study program, 162 spec-
ies. Although a clear deterioration in vigilance per- ialists, and 93 nurses. Results indicated that the
formance was observed after sleep deprivation, hours of sleep needed for feeling well rested
subjective feelings of success remained stable across depended on age, gender, work demands, and
sustained wakefulness in the caffeine group. The work schedule. However, respondents who con-
feeling of success was measured with the phrase sumed caffeine reported more trouble staying
How successful do you think you were in this awake while listening to lectures or learning and
task? which was rated on a visual analog scale run- while driving a car. Based on these results, the
ning from not at all to very much. Similarly, authors argued that caffeine consumption may
Baranski (2007) in his study on confidence in judg- impair sleep habits and quality of sleep, thereby
ment found that one night of sleep deprivation did hampering cognitive performance and wakeful-
not result in an impaired assessment of cognitive ness during nonsleep hours.
performance. It should be noted that the overconfi- Whether caffeine use is consistently the cause
dence in caffeine participants might have serious of impaired sleep in everyday situation and not
consequences in real-life work environments, like vice versa is questionable (Orbeta et al., 2006;
in aviation, because realistic self-perception is highly Roehrs and Roth, 2008; Whalen et al., 2007).
important in avoiding risks. It was found that feeling tired in the morning
induced high caffeine use which was associated
with subsequent impaired sleep, indicating that
Caffeine and recovery sleep cause and effect concerning the relation between
caffeine and impaired sleep is not always clear.
There is little doubt among laymen and health Another factor that may induce sleep disturbance
professionals about the fundamental importance in interaction with caffeine is vulnerability to
of sufficient, restorative sleep in maintaining stress (Drake et al., 2006). Drake and colleagues
110

showed that individuals with an objectively the experimental session and the experimenters
identified vulnerability to stress-induced sleep dis- monitored these individuals during the 3 days
turbance showed a stronger sleep-reactivity in before each experimental session to verify the
response to a 3-mg/kg BW caffeine challenge than absence of sleep deprivation. Effects of caffeine
nonvulnerable individuals. on sleep variables in individuals working night
The effects of caffeine on daytime recovery shifts or suffering from jet lag may be more pro-
sleep after 25 h of sleep deprivation were studied nounced because of stronger influences of circa-
by Carrier et al. (2009). Young (age 2030 years) dian rhythms and related physiological processes
and middle-aged volunteers (age 4560 years) in these individuals.
participated in a caffeine (200 mg) and placebo Effects of caffeine use on subsequent sleeping
condition, spaced 1 month apart. Three hours patterns have been related to its pharmacological
before daytime recovery sleep, the participants actions. Although it is widely accepted that the
took either the first 100 mg caffeine or placebo predominant effect of caffeine is to block specific
capsule, while the second 100 mg dose was taken adenosinergic receptors, other mechanisms may
1 hour before daytime recovery sleep. An effect play a role, as Ataka et al. (2008) presented in
of caffeine on daytime recovery sleep was their study on candidate antifatigue substances
observed in both age groups, reflected in a on mental fatigue. Caffeine, used to increase
decrease in sleep efficiency, sleep duration, slow- alertness and wakefulness, might affect sleep by
wave sleep, and REM sleep. Moreover, caffeine an effect on branched-chain amino acids.
reduced non-REM sleep EEG synchronization Branched-chain amino acids are used for the syn-
during daytime recovery sleep. These results pro- thesis of proteins and are regarded as a bio-
vide additional evidence that using caffeine to marker of mental fatigue. Ataka et al. (2008)
cope with night work and jet lag might result in examined levels of these amino acids in 17
detrimental effects on subsequent sleep. healthy participants who randomly received
However, no detrimental effects on recovery 100 mg/day caffeine or placebo twice a day for
sleep were observed in the Philip et al. (2006) 8 consecutive days. Fatigue was induced by men-
study. These authors examined nighttime driving tal task performance (Uchida-Kraepelin psycho-
performance between 2.00 and 3.30 a.m. after pla- diagnostic test and advanced trail-making test).
cebo, 30 min of napping, or 200 mg caffeine. An Task performance of the caffeine group was bet-
important difference between the Philip et al. ter than performance observed in the placebo
(2006) study and studies in which a clear effect group. However, subjective perception of fatigue,
of caffeine on recovery sleep was observed might motivation, and sleepiness did not differ between
be the length of the sleep deprivation period both groups. These results are in line with the
used. Carrier et al. (2009), for example, exposed findings of Kilpelaiinen et al. (2010), suggesting
their participants to 25 h of sleep deprivation, that administration of caffeine improves task per-
while participants in the Philip et al. (2006) study formance without decreasing the sensation of
were allowed to go to sleep immediately after the fatigue. An important observation of Ataka
nighttime driving session (i.e., after 3.30 a.m.). et al. (2008) was that plasma branched-chain
Besides the duration of the sleep deprivation amino acid levels in the caffeine group were
period, circadian rhythm might be another impor- lower than those observed in the placebo group,
tant factor that has to be taken into account after the fatigue-inducing mental tasks, as well
in explaining differential effects of caffeine as after the recovery period (as a trend). These
on recovery sleep. The participants in the results indicate that caffeine can accelerate men-
Carrier et al. (2009) study were instructed to tal fatigue through increased activation of the
maintain a regular sleepwake schedule before brain, without an accompanying sensation of
111

increased fatigue. Bartley and Chute (1947) in middle and high school students (age
argued that mental fatigue might be regarded as 1218 years) to examine the relation between
a protection mechanism warning an individual technology use (e.g., watching television, text
that he or she needs a rest in order to prevent messaging, playing video games, surfing internet),
an overload of our cognitive system. Results of caffeine intake, and quality of sleep. Calamaro
Ataka et al. (2008) imply that this warning mech- et al. hypothesized that with increased technology
anism, essential in reducing acute fatigue, is less use, especially late at night, more caffeine is con-
effective after caffeine consumption. This might sumed to stay awake. This behavior pattern, cou-
explain why caffeine used during daytime, partic- pled with the early start times for middle schools
ularly at later times of day, may have detrimental and high schools that demand earlier weekday
effects on subsequent sleep periods. rise times, was expected to result in sleep deficits.
The results showed that the hours spent with
technology activities at night were indeed related
Caffeine and self-imposed sleep deprivation to a decrease in sleep duration. Not surprisingly,
the ability to stay alert and function adequately
Sleep disturbances have been associated with an during the subsequent day was impaired by exces-
increased risk of work absenteeism, decrements sive daytime sleepiness in those students who got
in vitality, social functioning, physical and mental less sleep. In addition, caffeine consumption
health, and general quality of life (Lund et al., tended to be 76% higher in this group. Important
2010). In young people, who tend to have irregu- implications of such a strategy concern the addi-
lar life styles and consequently do not get the tional negative effects on nighttime sleep when
sleep they need, sleep problems might give rise trying to compensate daytime sleepiness by taking
to academic problems. Caffeine can be used stra- caffeine (Orbeta et al., 2006, Roehrs and Roth,
tegically to optimize the level of wakefulness, 2008; Whalen et al., 2007). Altogether, these
thereby improving daytime functioning in this results warrant a careful consideration of risks
group. It is important, though, to realize that non- related to sleep deprivation in combination with
judicious use of caffeine may intensify their sleep- caffeine consumption, especially in middle and
ing problems, since there is evidence for a greater high school age groups.
physiological need for sleep in adolescents com-
pared to other age groups. Lund et al. (2010)
examined sleep quality in college students (age Caffeine, sleepiness, and work quality
1724 years). Disturbed sleep was reported by
60% of the 1125 students, who completed a Caffeine promotes alertness during times of
cross-sectional online survey about sleep habits. desired wakefulness in persons with jet lag or shift
Students overwhelmingly stated that emotional work disorder (for a review see Lorist and Snel,
stress and academic stress were important factors 2008). An important benefit of the effects of caf-
that negatively impacted sleep and explained feine could be a reduced risk on injury and error
24% of the variance of the scores on the during these periods. Tieges et al. (2004) showed
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. It was notewor- that doses of 3 and 5 mg/kg body weight of caf-
thy that caffeine consumption, consistency of feine in well-rested habitual caffeine consumers
sleep schedule, and daily hours of television and indeed reduced the number of errors compared
video game exposure were no significant pre- to a placebo condition. Based on related changes
dictors of sleep quality (Lund et al., 2010). in brain activity shown as enlarged error-related
A different pattern of results was observed by negativity, an event-related brain component that
Calamaro et al. (2009), who performed a study reflects anterior cingulate cortex activity, they
112

concluded that coffee consumption increased the daytime driving session. Lateral deviations
monitoring of ongoing cognitive processes for have been found to be a frequent cause of sleep-
signs of erroneous outcomes. related accidents. If daytime highway driving
The observed reduction in self-consciousness (between 6.00 p.m. and 7.30 p.m.) was used as a
resulting in overconfidence after caffeine con- point of reference for nighttime driving (between
sumption (Kilpelaiinen et al., 2010) seems to con- 2.00 and 3.30 a.m.), no difference in driving per-
tradict these findings, and might have formance was observed in 75% of the participants
consequences in real-life environments. Realistic who consumed caffeine and 66% of them drove
self-perception is essential in avoiding risks. The as well after a nap, indicating that drinking coffee
important question whether under suboptimal or napping significantly reduced line crossing
conditions, for example, due to extended wakeful- errors. These results illustrate the common prac-
ness, caffeine is still able to enhance the detection tice that caffeine is used as an efficient counter-
of erroneous responses and consequently mini- measure for sleep-related accidents which are
mize the risk of errors, was extensively studied known to occur most frequently in the middle of
in recent years (e.g., Lorist and Snel, 2008). the night.
Ker et al. (2010) examined more specifically It is important to note that similar effects have
the role of caffeine in preventing errors or injuries also been found in well-rested individuals
caused by impaired alertness in individuals with (Attwood et al., 2006; Childs and de Wit, 2006;
jet lag or shift work. Their systematic review did Haskell et al., 2005; Hewlett and Smith, 2007).
not elucidate a relation between caffeine and A common comment on caffeine's ability to
injuries because none of the studies included improve performance is that little account is taken
measured injury outcomes. Concerning the occur- of the fact that caffeine withdrawal and withdrawal
rence of errors, Ker and colleagues observed that reversal might possibly obscure the net effects of
caffeine significantly reduced the number of caffeine. In a great deal of the experimental stud-
errors compared to placebo. ies on the effects of caffeine, researchers have used
One of the studies examined by Ker et al. (2010) the naturally occurring overnight caffeine absti-
was a study by Dagan and Doljansky (2006). These nence period. In addition, participants are asked
researchers evaluated the efficacy of caffeine to abstain from their usual morning caffeine con-
(200 mg) and modafinil (200 mg) in maintaining sumption prior to laboratory testing, and caffeine
cognitive performance after sleep deprivation in a consumption is delayed until the experimental ses-
flight simulation task. According to their results, sion. Improvements in performance following caf-
both caffeine and modafinil significantly decreased feine ingestion under these circumstances could
the deviation from assigned altitude and velocity reflect reversal of the adverse effects of the over-
values compared to baseline levels during the noc- night caffeine withdrawal.
turnal drop in cognitive performance, supporting It is known that due to regular caffeine intake,
that caffeine has a performance-increasing effect, the number of adenosine receptors in the central
especially under suboptimal conditions. nervous system increases. As a result of this adap-
A second study reviewed by Ker et al. (2010) tive caffeine response, individuals become more
was performed by Philip et al. (2006). These sensitive to adenosine. A subsequent reduction
authors measured nighttime highway driving per- of intake of caffeine as a blocker of adenosinergic
formance of 12 young men immediately after receptors will increase the normal physiological
200 mg of caffeine, decaffeinated coffee (con- effects of adenosine, resulting in withdrawal
taining 15 mg of caffeine), and after napping in symptoms in tolerant caffeine users. Withdrawal
the car for 30 min. An increase was found in line symptoms, including headache, irritability, and
crossings during nighttime driving compared with an inability to concentrate, usually appear within
113

1224 h after discontinuation of caffeine intake, In order to avoid confounding of caffeine effects
peaking around 48 h (Juliano and Griffiths, with tolerance or withdrawal, Michael et al. (2008)
2004). These effects last from 1 to 5 days, examined the effects of caffeine in 12 participants
representing the time required for the number (age 1829 years), who consumed either no caffeine
of adenosine receptors in the brain to revert to or very little caffeine on a regular basis. The well-
normal levels, uninfluenced by caffeine rested participants were tested on 2 separate days,
consumption. using vigilance tests scheduled at baseline (around
Keane and James (2008) examined the chronic 9.00 a.m.) and at 30, 60, 120, 180, and 240 min
effects of caffeine consumption in 15 healthy after placebo or caffeine (200 mg) administration.
individuals (age 1719 years). Participants During task performance, eye blink variables were
alternated weekly between ingesting placebo measured to assess alertness.
and caffeine (1.75 mg/kg) three times daily for 4 In contrast with the outcome of the Keane and
consecutive weeks following either usual sleep James (2008) study, the result of Michael et al.
or sleep restriction (40% of the usual amount). (2008) showed that, even though the participants
The effects of caffeine on brain activity, perfor- were well rested, caffeine was able to reduce drowsi-
mance, and mood were examined after 6 days in ness, as deduced from ocular movements and reac-
which caffeine was consumed or after 6 days in tion times, and these changes persisted for 34 h.
which participants did not use caffeine-containing The use of different ocular variables seemed to pro-
substances. The authors argued that the 6-day vide a sensitive measure to detect subtle changes in
period warrants that the effects observed in the alertness induced by caffeine. Self-reports of sleepi-
abstinence condition cannot be ascribed to with- ness were not as sensitive; differences between the
drawal-related effects, since these effects last up caffeine and placebo condition were only observed
to 5 days. Their results showed that the effects 30 min after substance administration. These
of caffeine on EEG activity were trivial and findings support the conclusion that caffeine can
inconsistent, and no clear evidence was found of have beneficial effects on performance and alertness
restorative effects of caffeine for performance (Michael et al., 2008), and that these effects do not
and mood variables. They argued that caffeine is seem to be related to withdrawal or withdrawal
of no use to enhance human function or to reversal since it is unlikely that tolerance has devel-
reverse the negative effects of sleep loss. Other oped in the individuals who did not consume caffeine
studies, however, did show increases in par- on a regular base.
ticipants who were well-rested and not deprived The general conclusion, so far, seems clear; caf-
of caffeine (e.g., Attwood et al., 2006; Childs feine can be used effectively to manipulate men-
and de Wit, 2006; Haskell et al., 2005; Hewlett tal state. It was found to be beneficial in
and Smith, 2007). The lack of clear results in the restoring low levels of wakefulness and coun-
Keane and James (2008) study might be related teracting degraded task performance. However,
to the sleep restriction protocol they used. Keane caffeine may produce detrimental effects on
and James did not observe changes in perfor- subsequent sleep, resulting in daytime sleepiness.
mance scores due to the sleep restrictions. In
addition, no main effects were observed on Remaining issues
EEG power, suggesting that the sleep restriction
manipulation might have been too weak or the Expectancy
dependent measure too insensitive to caffeine
effects to induce differential effects of caffeine Caffeine is generally regarded as a stimulant, fre-
between the usual sleep condition and the sleep quently used to make people feel more alert and
restriction condition. ready to face daily challenges and to counteract
114

sleepiness. It is assumed that these properties are suggests that this caffeine-plus-placebo regimen
the main reason for consuming caffeine in a broad could be used when work schedules demand
range of daily life situations and in research, as extended periods of alertness without sleep to
well. The stimulant effects of caffeine on physio- maximize attention but minimize negative side
logical and mental states seem to profoundly be effects.
mediated by its pharmacological actions as an Caffeine expectations were also examined in 16
adenosine receptor antagonist. Van Dongen young healthy volunteers (age 1825 years) by
et al. (2001), for example, found in their par- Anderson and Horne (2008). In their study, par-
ticipants, who were sleep deprived for 88 h and ticipants performed a three-times 30-min PVT
who received sustained low caffeine doses (separated by a 2-min break after every 30 min) after
(0.3 mg/kg BW/h) during the last 66 h, that inertia they had a light lunch; during this period, an early
was largely overcome by caffeine. They concluded afternoon dip is usually experienced. Sleepiness
that the caffeine-induced antagonism of adenosine was further enhanced by requiring participants to
receptors on the central nervous system results into limit their prior night's sleep to 5 h. Participants were
an increase of unused adenosine in the brain upon tested twice, either after they consumed a cup of
awakening which might be the cause of sleep inertia. decaffeinated coffee which was accompanied by ver-
However, expectations of caffeine (i.e., pla- bal information that the coffee was decaffeinated
cebo) effects have been found to be an important (control) or after they consumed decaffeinated cof-
additional factor to its psychostimulant effects. fee after the experimenter informed them about the
These expectations can trigger a series of physio- highly alerting effects of the super type coffee they
logical and psychological reactions, usually were going to receive (expectancy). Significantly,
related to the pharmacological effects of caffeine fewer lapses and shorter reaction times were
consumption. Sun et al. (2007), for example, stud- observed in the expectancy condition than after the
ied the effect of caffeine expectations on vigilance control condition during the first hour of task perfor-
and cognitive task performance during 28 h of mance, indicating that expectancy about consuming
sleep deprivation. They informed 10 healthy male caffeine was effective in improving performance or
participants (age 1820 years) that the capsules preventing performance decrement in moderately
they had to ingest contained caffeine and gave sleepy people. It is surprising that while caffeine
them information about the stimulating effects can take 3040 min to become pharmacologically
of caffeine to increase the expectation of caffeine effective, the effects of expectancy seemed more
effects. Participants arrived at the laboratory at rapid.
6.00 a.m. and went through 28 successive hours Anderson and Horne (2008) argued that the
of total sleep deprivation. Cognitive tests (letter effects of caffeine expectations might be related to
cancelation task, continuous addition test) were classical conditioning, that is, the expectancy effect
administered every 2 h from 12.00 to 10.00 a.m. is a conditioned response. Moreover, this effect
of the second day. Sun et al. (2007) found that might have been enhanced by knowledge of the
an initial dose of 200 mg caffeine, administered effects of caffeinated beverages. Support for a role
at 12.00 a.m., followed 4 h later by a placebo of classical conditioning in caffeine expectancy was
helped to maintain cognitive performance during provided by Attwood et al. (2008). They examined
the period without sleep to a similar extent whether the effects of caffeine could be
as the condition in which a double dose of conditioned to the context of administration in 16
caffeine was given. The placebo effect extended volunteers (age 1826 years). Four conditioning
the cognitive boost without incurring the rise in trials were followed by a test session, in which par-
blood pressure and heart rate that sometimes go ticipants received placebo before performing a sim-
together with caffeine consumption. This result ple reaction time task. Attwood et al. observed that
115

in the test session, the group who received 250 mg References


caffeine during the conditioning trials performed
significantly faster during the test session than the Anderson, C., & Horne, J. A. (2008). Placebo response to caf-
feine improves reaction time performance in sleepy people.
group who had received placebo, indicating the
Human Psychopharmacology, 23(4), 333336.
development of a conditioned response. Possible Ataka, S., Tanaka, M., Nozaki, S., Mizuma, H., Mizuno, K.,
involvement of expectancy effects in this study Tahara, T., et al. (2008). Effects of oral administration of
was reduced by presenting caffeine in a novel drink, caffeine and d-ribose on mental fatigue. Nutrition, 24(3),
and by not informing the participants about the 233238.
Attwood, A. S., Terry, P., & Higgs, S. (2006). Exploring
contents of the beverage, making the finding all
factors that mediate responsiveness to caffeine. Appetite,
the more salient. 47, 258.
Caffeine expectancy effects are not limited to Attwood, A., Terry, P., & Higgs, S. (2008). Evidence of
cognitive task performance but may be also pres- conditioned cognitive and mood effects of caffeine in
ent in physical task performance. Pollo et al. humans using a differential conditioning paradigm. Appetite,
50(23), 556.
(2008) observed across participants who thought
Balkin, T. J., Kamimori, G. H., Redmond, D. P.,
they consumed caffeine, that mean muscle work Vigneulle, R. M., Thorne, D. R., Belenky, G., et al. (2004).
which was not accompanied by a decrease of per- On the importance of countermeasures in sleep and perfor-
ceived muscle fatigue, was regarded as evidence mance models. Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine,
that caffeine expectancy could indeed counteract 75(3 Suppl.), A155A157.
Baranski, J. V. (2007). Fatigue, sleep loss, and confidence in
the symptoms of physical fatigue.
judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13
In sum, the effects elicited after administration (4), 182196.
of a placebo instead of caffeine illustrate that Bartley, S. H., & Chute, E. (1947). Fatigue and impairment in
the efficacy of caffeine to restore detrimental man. London: McGraw-Hill.
effects of sleep deprivation on performance is Benitez, P. L., Kamimori, G. H., Balkin, T. J., Greene, A., &
Johnson, M. L. (2009). Modeling fatigue over sleep depriva-
partly due to caffeine expectancy.
tion, circadian rhythm, and caffeine with a minimal perfor-
mance inhibitor model. Methods in Enzymology, 454,
405421.
Conclusions Calamaro, C. J., Mason, T. B. A., & Ratcliffe, S. J. (2009).
Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: Effects of caffeine and
Caffeine, a well-known antagonist of adenosinergic technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pedi-
atrics, 123(6), e1005e1010.
receptors, can be used effectively to modulate our Caldwell, J. A., & Caldwell, J. L. (2005). Fatigue in military
mental state. Caffeine is found to be beneficial in aviation: An overview of us military-approved pharmacolog-
restoring low levels of wakefulness and to counter- ical countermeasures. Aviation Space and Environmental
act deteriorations in task performance related to Medicine, 76(Suppl. 7), C39C51.
sleep deprivation. However, the results also indi- Carrier, J., Paquet, J., Fernandez-Bolanos, M., Girouard, L.,
Roy, J., Selmaoui, B., et al. (2009). Effects of caffeine on
cate that caffeine may produce detrimental effects daytime recovery sleep: A double challenge to the sleep-
on subsequent sleep, resulting in daytime sleepi- wake cycle in aging. Sleep Medicine, 10(9), 10161024.
ness. The efficacy of caffeine to restore mental Childs, E., & de Wit, H. (2006). Subjective, behavioral, and
performance decline in suboptimal conditions physiological effects of acute caffeine in light, nondependent
seems to be partly due to caffeine expectancy. No caffeine users. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 185(4),
514523.
support was found for the claim that stimulant Coste, O., & Lagarde, D. (2009). Clinical management of jet
effects of caffeine are related to withdrawal or lag: What can be proposed when performance is critical?
withdrawal reversal. In conclusion, caffeine pro- Travel Medicine and Infectious Disease, 7(2), 8287.
vides an adequate and common way to strategi- Crochet, P., Aggarwal, R., Misha, A., Sirimanna, P., &
cally adjust mental state, provided the effects on Darzi, A. (2009). Caffeine and taurine reverse the deteriora-
tion in laparoscopic and cognitive skill following sleep
recovery sleep are taken into consideration.
116

deprivation. Journal of the American College of Surgeons, Lorist, M. M., & Tops, M. (2003). Caffeine, fatigue, and cogni-
209(3), S111. tion. Brain and Cognition, 53(1), 8294.
Dagan, Y., & Doljansky, J. T. (2006). Cognitive performance Lund, H. G., Reider, B. D., Whiting, A. B., & Prichard, J. R.
during sustained wakefulness: A low dose of caffeine is (2010). Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in
equally effective as modafinil in alleviating the nocturnal a large population of college students. The Journal of Ado-
decline. Chronobiology International, 23(5), 973983. lescent Health, 46(2), 124132.
Drake, C. L., Jefferson, C., Roehrs, T., & Roth, T. (2006). Michael, N., Johns, M., Owen, C., & Patterson, J. (2008).
Stress-related sleep disturbance and polysomnographic Effects of caffeine on alertness as measured by infrared
response to caffeine. Sleep Medicine, 7(7), 567572. reflectance oculography. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 200
Haskell, C. F., Kennedy, D. O., Wesnes, K. A., & (2), 255260.
Scholey, A. B. (2005). Cognitive and mood improvements Orbeta, R. L., Overpeck, M. D., Ramcharran, D.,
of caffeine in habitual consumers and habitual non-con- Kogan, M. D., & Ledsky, R. (2006). High caffeine intake
sumers of caffeine. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 179(4), in adolescents: Associations with difficulty sleeping and feel-
813825. ing tired in the morning. The Journal of Adolescent Health,
Hewlett, P., & Smith, A. (2007). Effects of repeated doses of 38(4), 451453.
caffeine on performance and alertness: New data and sec- Pecotic, R., Valic, M., Kardum, G., Sevo, V., & Dogas, Z.
ondary analyses. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical (2008). Sleep habits of medical students, physicians and
and Experimental, 22(6), 339350. nurses regarding age, sex, shift work and caffein consump-
Juliano, L. M., & Griffiths, R. R. (2004). A critical review of tion. Lijecnicki Vjesnik, 130(34), 8791.
caffeine withdrawal: Empirical validation of symptoms and Philip, P., Taillard, J., Moore, N., Delord, S., Valtat, C.,
signs, incidence, severity, and associated features. Psycho- Sagaspe, P., et al. (2006). The effects of coffee and napping
pharmacology (Berlin), 176(1), 129. on nighttime highway driving: A randomized trial. Annals of
Kamimori, G. H., Karyekar, C. S., Otterstetter, R., Cox, D. S., Internal Medicine, 144(11), 785791.
Balkin, T. J., Belenky, G. L., et al. (2002). The rate of Pollo, A., Carlino, E., & Benedetti, F. (2008). The top-down
absorption and relative bioavailability of caffeine influence of ergogenic placebos on muscle work and fatigue.
administered in chewing gum versus capsules to normal The European Journal of Neuroscience, 28(2), 379388.
healthy volunteers. International Journal of Pharmaceutics, Retey, J. V., Adam, M., Khatami, R., Luhmann, U. F.,
234(12), 159167. Jung, H. H., Berger, W., et al. (2007). A genetic variation
Keane, M. A., & James, J. E. (2008). Effects of dietary caffeine in the adenosine a2a receptor gene (adora2a) contributes
on EEG, performance and mood when rested and sleep to individual sensitivity to caffeine effects on sleep. Clinical
restricted. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 81(5), 692698.
Experimental, 23(8), 669680. Ribeiro, J. A., & Sebastiao, A. M. (2010). Caffeine and aden-
Ker, K., Edwards, P. J., Felix, L. M., Blackhall, K., & osine. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 20(Suppl. 1), 315.
Roberts, I. (2010). Caffeine for the prevention of injuries Roehrs, T., & Roth, T. (2008). Caffeine: Sleep and daytime
and errors in shift workers. Cochrane Database of Systematic sleepiness. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 12(2), 153162.
Reviews, 5, CD008508. Rosenthal, T. C., Majeroni, B. A., Pretorius, R., & Malik, K.
Killgore, W. D., Kahn-Greene, E. T., Grugle, N. L., (2008). Fatigue: An overview. American Family Physician,
Killgore, D. B., & Balkin, T. J. (2009). Sustaining executive 78(10), 11731179.
functions during sleep deprivation: A comparison of caf- Snel, J., Lorist, M. M., & Tieges, Z. (2004). Coffee, caffeine
feine, dextroamphetamine, modafinil, lab. Sleep, 32(2), and cognitive performance. In A. Nehlig (Ed.), Coffee, tea,
205216. chocolate and the brain (pp. 5373). (1st ed.). Boca Raton:
Kilpelaiinen, A. A., Huttunen, K. H., Lohi, J. J., & CRC Press LLC.
Lyytinen, H. (2010). Effect of caffeine on vigilance and cog- Sun, Y., Zhang, Y., He, N., Liu, X., & Miao, D. (2007). Caffeine
nitive performance during extended wakefulness. The Inter- and placebo expectation: Effects on vigilance, cognitive per-
national Journal of Aviation Psychology, 20(2), 144159. formance, heart rate, and blood pressure during 28 hours of
Landolt, H. P. (2008a). Genotype-dependent differences in sleep deprivation. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21(2), 9199.
sleep, vigilance, and response to stimulants. Current Phar- Tieges, Z., Richard Ridderinkhof, K., Snel, J., & Kok, A.
maceutical Design, 14(32), 33963407. (2004). Caffeine strengthens action monitoring: Evidence
Landolt, H. P. (2008b). Sleep homeostasis: A role for adenosine from the error-related negativity. Brain Research. Cognitive
in humans? Biochemical Pharmacology, 75(11), 20702079. Brain Research, 21(1), 8793.
Lorist, M. M., & Snel, J. (2008). Caffeine, sleep and quality of Van Dongen, H. P., Price, N. J., Mullington, J. M.,
life. In D. Streiner, S. R. Pandi-Perumal & J. C. Verster Szuba, M. P., Kapoor, S. C., & Dinges, D. F. (2001). Caf-
(Eds.), Sleep and quality of life in clinical medicine. USA: feine eliminates psychomotor vigilance deficits from sleep
Humana Press, Springer, 978-1-60327-340-4. inertia. Sleep, 24(7), 813819.
117

Wendte, R., Snel, J., & Tieges, Z. (2003). Misclassification of Wesensten, N. J., Killgore, W. D., & Balkin, T. J. (2005). Per-
caffeine use: Causes and examples. In Fac. Psychologie, formance and alertness effects of caffeine, dextroamphet-
Programmagroep Psychonomie (pp. 120). Amsterdam: amine, and modafinil during sleep deprivation. Journal of
Universiteit van Amsterdam. Sleep Research, 14(3), 255266.
Wesensten, N. J., Belenky, G., Kautz, M. A., Thorne, D. R., Whalen, D. J., Silk, J. S., Semel, M., Forbes, E. E.,
Reichardt, R. M., & Balkin, T. J. (2002). Maintaining alert- Ryan, N. D., Axelson, D. A., et al. (2008). Caffeine con-
ness and performance during sleep deprivation: Modafinil sumption, sleep, and affect in the natural environments of
versus caffeine. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 159(3), depressed youth and healthy controls. Journal of Pediatric
238247. Psychology, 33(4), 358367.
Wesensten, N. J., Belenky, G., Thorne, D. R., Kautz, M. A., Wurts, S. W., & Edgar, D. M. (2000). Caffeine during sleep
Balkin, T. J., & Balkin, T. J. (2004). Modafinil vs caffeine: deprivation: Sleep tendency and dynamics of recovery sleep
Effects on fatigue during sleep deprivation. Aviation, Space, in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 65(1),
and Environmental Medicine, 75(6), 520525. 155162.

Potrebbero piacerti anche